Ardila - Ostrosky Solis - (Ed) - 1989 PDF
Ardila - Ostrosky Solis - (Ed) - 1989 PDF
Ardila - Ostrosky Solis - (Ed) - 1989 PDF
of Language and
Cognitive Processes
Critical Issues in Neuropsychology
Series Editors:
Cecil R. Reynolds Antonio E. Puente
Texas A&M University University of North Carolina, Wilmington
ALFREDO ARDILA
Miami Institute of Psychology
Miami, Florida
and
FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS
National Autonomous University of Mexico
Mexico D.F., Mexico
ix
x PREFACE
rizes the findings of the relatively new science of in vivo localization. The
author considers traditional (autopsy, head injury, surgical procedure,
cortical stimulation, EEG) and modern (isotope localization, comput-
erized axial tomography, cerebral blood flow, positron emission tomogra-
phy, single-photon computerized tomography, magnetic resonance imag-
ing) techniques for localizing lesions in an in-depth analysis focusing on
the issue of the extent to which a function is localizable and the variables
that influence its symptomatology.
Segalowitz ("ERPs and Advances in Neurolinguistics") presents a
review of some event-related potentials (ERP) techniques and technical
contributions directly related to questions of research in neurolinguistics.
He examines the application of ERP in the area of lexical semantics,
syntaxis, reading, and primary and secondary language acquisition, and in
the study of patients with disorders of language resulting from brain
damage. Segalowitz emphasizes the advantages of the latter paradigm as an
alternative technique to supplement the traditional behavioral measures.
Part II deals with language development and dissolution. Eslava-
Cobos and Mejia ("Disorders in Language Acquisition and Cerebral Matu-
ration: A Neurophysiological Perspective") present a historical review of
concepts of disorders in language acquisition. They discuss the embryo-
logic development of the nervous system with regard to normal language
acquisition. In addition, they review Latin American studies, especially
those carried out by Quiros and Azcoaga, emphasizing Pavlov's theory of
analyzers as applied to language by Bechtereva. The authors also discuss
abnormal language organization, trying to support a system of neu-
rophysiological classification.
Cummings and Benson ("Speech and Language Alterations in De-
mentia Syndromes") present a broad clinical study focusing on speech
and language in Alzheimer's dementia, Parkinson's disease dementia, and
multi-infarction dementia. They analyze distinguishing features for each
dementia and discuss differential profiles of preserved and impaired
functions in each dementia syndrome.
Part III deals with oral and written language disorders. Buckingham
("Mechanisms Underlying Aphasic Transformations") outlines a lan-
guage production model based on normality, underlining the fact that,
without some notion of how a normal linguistic productive system oper-
ates, there is little hope of understanding aphasic errors. He points out
that aphasic breakdowns arise through derailments of computation errors
that are corrected in normal language production but augmented and
sustained in aphasics. The author emphasizes that only through models of
normal language production is it possible to characterize mechanisms
underlying aphasic transformations.
Ardila, Rosselli, and Pinzon ("Alexia and Agraphia in Spanish
Speakers: CAT Correlations and Interlinguistic Analysis") present a large-
PREFACE xi
Alfredo Ardila
Feggy Ostrosky-Solis
Contents
Chapter 1
Microgenesis: Historical Review and Current Studies 3
Robert E. Hanlon and Jason W. Brown
Chapter 2
Fractionation of Spatial Cognition following Focal and Diffuse
Brain Damage ............................................... 17
Dean C. Delis and Amy M. Bihrle
xiii
xiv CONTENTS
Conclusion 32
References 32
Chapter 3
Anatomical and Physiological Correlations and Neuroimaging
Techniques in Language Disorders ............................ 37
Andrew Kertesz
Autopsy Location. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37
Localization through Head Injuries ............................ 40
Surgical Localization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40
Cortical Stimulation ......................................... 41
Electroencephalography and Evoked Potentials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Isotope Localization of Lesions ............................... 44
Computerized Tomographic (CT) Lesion Localization ........... 45
Cerebral Blood Flow (CBF) Localization ....................... 48
Positron Emission Tomography (PET) ......................... 49
Single-Photon Computerized Tomography (SPECT) ............. 50
Magnetic Resonance Imager (MRI) .. . . .. . .. . .. . . ... . . .. . . . ..... 50
Discussion and Conclusion ................................... 52
References .................................................. 56
Chapter 4
ERPs and Advances in Neurolinguistics 61
Sidney J. Segalowitz
Summary................................................... 77
References .................................................. 77
Chapter 5
Disorders in Language Acquisition and Cerebral Maturation: A
Neurophysiological Perspective ............................... 85
Lyda Mejia and Jorge Eslava-Cobos
Chapter 6
Speech and Language Alterations in Dementia Syndromes 107
Jeffrey 1. Cummings and D. Frank Benson
Chapter 7
Mechanisms Underlying Aphasic Transformations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 123
Hugh W. Buckingham
Chapter 8
Alexia and Agraphia in Spanish Speakers: CAT Correlations and
Interlinguistic Analysis. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 147
Alfredo Ardila, Monica Rosselli, and Oscar Pinzon
Method..................................................... 150
Subjects. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 150
Procedure ................................................. 150
Instrument ................................................ 154
Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154
Population with Cerebral Damage and Normal Population... .. 155
Patients with Right-Hemisphere Lesions and Patients
with Left-Hemisphere Lesions ............................ 155
Intrahemispheric Comparisons. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 155
Quantification of the Type of Errors. . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 160
Scores on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure ................ 162
Superimposition of Scans .................................. 163
Discussion .................................................. 168
Patients with Right and Left Lesions ........................ 168
Intrahemispheric Comparisons. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . 168
Topography of the Clinical Findings ........................ 172
An Interlinguistic Consideration ............................ 173
References .................................................. 174
CONTENTS xvii
Chapter 9
Semantic Aphasia Reconsidered 177
Alfredo Ardila, Maria Victoria Lopez, and Eugenia Solano
Chapter 10
Cognitive Effects of Adrenal Autografting
in Parkinson's Disease ....................................... 197
Feggy Ostrosky-Solis, Ignacio Madrazo, Rene Drucker-Colin,
and Luis Quintanar
Chapter 11
The Striatum as a Temporary Memory Store 219
Roberto A. Prado-Alcala
Chapter 12
Development and Plasticity in the Central Nervous System:
Organismic and Environmental Influences ..................... 229
Donald G. Stein
Index........................................................ 253
Brain Organization
of Language and
Cognitive Processes
I
Microgenesis
Historical Review and Current Studies
ROBERT E. HANLON and JASON W. BROWN
EARLY STUDIES
3
4 ROBERT E. HANLON and JASON W. BROWN
PERCEPTGENESIS
CLINICAL APPLICATIONS
tions, which may take the form of images or ideas, then function to aid in
the process of constructing the thought.
Though not explicit as to the manner in which some presentations
influence and are integrated in the process of thought development while
others are discarded, Schilder maintained that the dual processes of sim-
ilarity and contiguity playa governing role-that is, "a presentation
evolves through other presentations which are associated with it by sim-
ilarity and contiguity, primarily by clang and external associations" (p.
499). The idea of inclusion versus exclusion entails that presentations
holding a logical relevance to the developing thought will be incorporated
into its structure, while those without a logical relationship fade into the
background or periphery.
One of the more intriguing aspects of Schilder's work is the conten-
tion that these early, primitive presentations that contribute to the devel-
oping thought are symbolic images that aid in the apprehension of mean-
ing. Moreover, these preparatory presentations have a tendency to fuse
with associated presentations, as well as the capacity for alteration and
transformation in response to emotional influences.
The direction of this process is a progressive evolvement toward ob-
ject-directed, reality-oriented thought. Two major phases were differenti-
ated within this unidirectional process, along with the primary determi-
nants of the progression within each phase. Subjective, wish-determined
symbolic images of the preparatory stages were referred to as the sphere.
The thought structures that arise from this sphere show a progressive
striving for objectivity in a manner that increasingly conforms to the rules
of logic and the nature of reality. In this way the structure finally achieves
the form of a concept. This conceptual phase represents the elaboration
and structuration of the symbolic images of the sphere toward objectivity,
resulting in the clear realization of the meaning content of the thought.
As mentioned, Schilder's theory of microgenesis is vast and encom-
passing, especially with regard to the interpretation of different forms of
impaired cognitive processing in various pathological conditions. A fun-
damental idea in his writings is that the process of thought development
recapitulates the phyloontogeny of cognition. In addition, and in keeping
with the Jacksonian idea of hierarchical organization, Schilder (1953)
concludes that this developmental progression occurs through the psy-
chic "energy-exchanges" between the neural levels in the hierarchy.
Schizophrenic thought disorders, expressive and receptive aphasia, the
cognitive impairments such as dementia, agnosia, apraxia-essentially
the entire realm of neuropsychology-reflects an incomplete develop-
ment through the micro genetic levels of cognition.
Another major microgenetic theorist was Werner (1956), who set
forth two basic assumptions: "First, the functions underlying abnormal
behavior are in their essence not different from those underlying normal
8 ROBERT E. HANLON and JASON W. BROWN
EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
CONCLUSION
world around us. This is no less true for cognitivist or AI theories of mind
than for the micro genetic theory. Thus, microgenesis entails that objects
in the world are the end products of a chain of events in the mind, not a
starting point in a piecemeal constructive process. In this sense, the mi-
crogenetic idea seems to be bound up with an idealist stand on the nature
of externality. Microgenesis takes a strong position with regard to this
question, so that the data supporting micro genetic theory can be taken as
evidence for the correctness of its epistemological basis.
Similarly, the notion that representations develop out of subsurface
stages, levels beneath awareness, has implications for our understanding
of what a mental representation is. What it definitely is not is its proposi-
tional or perceptual content, for this is only the tip of its structure. On the
micro genetic view, conscious representations are not the instigators of
behavior but are rather outcomes of the processing rising from below. This
clearly has profound consequences for our views on volition and agency.
In fact, current physiological work lends support to the idea that actions
develop prior to our awareness of them, and that the sense of volition does
not stand behind and direct behavior but is given to awareness on the
heels of the action development. These philosophical issues, and their
admittedly somewhat unpalatable solutions, are deeply woven into the
fabric of micro genetic thinking, perhaps accounting for some of the re-
sistance to this concept. But the very fact that our idea of what the world
is depends so importantly on our theory of mind and brain is precisely the
reason why this theory is worth a closer look.
REFERENCES
Ach, N. (1905). Uber die Willenstatigheit und das Denken. Gottingen: Vandenhoeck and
Ruprecht.
Brown. J. W. (1972). Aphasia. apraxia and agnosia: Clinical and theoretical aspects.
Springfield. IL: Thomas.
Brown. J. W. (1977). Mind, brain. and consciousness. New York: Academic Press.
Brown, J. W. (1988a). The life of the mind: Selected papers. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brown. J. W. (1988b). Classics in neuropsychology: Selected papers on agnosia and apraxia.
Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum.
Cegalis. J. A. (1973). Prism distortion and accommodtive change. Perception and Psycho-
physics, 13, 494-498.
Cegalis, J. A. (1984). On the role of conflict in microgenesis. In W. Froehlich, G. Smith. J.
Dr':lguns, & U. Hentschel (Eds.), Psychological processes in cognition and personality.
Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Cegalis, J. A., & Leen, D. (1977). Individual differences in responses to induced perceptual
conflict. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 991-998.
Cegalis, J. A, & Young, R. (1974). The effect of inversion-induced conflict on field-depen-
dence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 373-379.
Conrad. K. (1947). Dber den Begriff der Vorgestalt und seine Bedeutung fur die Hirnpathol-
ogie. Nervenarzt, 18, 289-293.
14 ROBERT E. HANLON and JASON W. BROWN
DEAN C. DELIS· San Diego Veterans Administration Medical Center, Department of Psy-
chiatry, University of California-San Diego, School of Medicine, San Diego, California
92161. AMY M. BIHRLE • The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, University of Cal-
ifornia-San Diego, and San Diego State University, San Diego, California 92138.
17
18 DEAN C. DELIS and AMY M. BIHRLE
HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE
VISUAL ATTENTION
o 0 0 0 Q Q Q Q
0 Q
0 Q
o 0 Q Q
0 Q
0 Q
o 0 o 0 Q
o 0 0 0 Q Q Q Q
0 Q
0 Q
o 0 Q Q
0 Q
0 Q
0 Q Q Q Q
r
r
r
c c c c c c r r r r r r
r
r
r
r
r
c C t r r r
r
FIGURE 1. Examples of (a) linguistic
r
hierarchical stimulus set and (b) non-
linguistic hierarchical stimulus set
b used in the experiment.
.,
~
E
~ .80
E
~ .60
.,
~
~ .40
c
...J
~ .20
~
0
~
E -.20
~
E -.40
~ FIGURE 2. Error analysis in recognizing
-.60 larger and smaller forms for the three
..!!!
-0 subject groups. The LHD patients made
E -.80
en
more errors in recognizing smaller forms
I!?
0
:::;: relative to larger forms, whereas the RHD
Left- Control Right-
Hemisphere Subjects Hemisphere patients made more errors in recognizing
Patients Patients larger forms relative to smaller forms.
FRACTIONATION OF SPATIAL COGNITION 23
J
J J t! t! t!
J t! t!
J t! t!
J t! t!
J J t! t!
J t! " " " t!
a
S b
FIGURE 3. Examples of (a) pair of hier-
archical stimuli, (b) drawing by an LHD
patient illustrating correct construction
of higher-level form presented in left
hemispace, and (c) drawing by an RHD
patient showing correct construction of
lower-level forms presented in right
c hemispace.
primarily by his ltlft hemisphere) and only global forms with his left hand
(controlled primarily by his right hemisphere) (see Figures 5 and 6). His
performance on a forced-choice recognition task was consistent with his
drawing results: He was more accurate in recognizing global forms when
pointing with his left hand and local forms when pointing with his right
hand. These results strongly suggest that components of visuospatial pro-
cessing can be disconnected in the same person following resection of the
corpus callosum.
Up to this point, all of the procedures involved a memory or construc-
tional component. We thus conducted a study to test whether the global-
local dissociation is revealed on a purely perceptual task without memory
or constructional demands (Robertson & Delis, 1986). In this experiment,
a task developed by Palmer (1980) was presented to unilateral brain-
damaged patients and normal control subjects. The subjects were asked to
indicate in which direction an equilateral triangle appeared to point when
it was either presented individually or aligned within other equilateral
triangles or circles. This task enables an analysis of the influence of a
more global reference frame (i.e., the aligned triangles) on the perception
of more local elements (i.e., a single triangle). The procedure is well
suited for brain-damaged populations because there is no time limit and it
places minimal demand on motor manipulation and memory skills. The
results of this experiment were consistent with our other findings: The
RHD patients were influenced significantly less by the global alignment
than the normal control subjects, whereas the LHD patients were influ-
FRACTIONATION OF SPATIAL COGNITION 25
7 o LHD Patients
o RHD Patients
~
6
j
0 ,,
,, /0
"I ,
-..>--
/
/' ,
/
CY
/
-- -- , "-
"-
3 "" ""
'0 '0
.."8 2
<II
>-
a c
..'"
U
:l
U Copy
«
U
10
o, ,
8
, /0
, /
" / ,/
/
0---------:8
7
/ , ------
/ / "0 0--
/
/
/
6 0
jL-____ ~ ________L __ _ _ __ L_ _ _ _ ~ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ __ L_ _ _ __ _
b d
Higher Lower Left Right
Level Level Hemispace Hemispace
Forms Forms Forms Forms
FIGURE 4. Drawing accuracy for (a and b) forms at higher and lower levels of hierarchical
stimuli, and (c and d) forms presented in left and right hemispace, for controls (e), LHD
patients (0), and RHD patients (0).
enced significantly more than the normal control subjects. These findings,
in concert with other data, suggest that differential processing of levels of
visual hierarchical structure has a biological basis in the brain.
yyyyyyyyy
y
y
y
y
y
y
8000008
o 0
c--------)
rIGURE 6. Examples of (a) target hierarchical stimulus.
:h) drawing done with the patient's right hand postcom-
nissurotomy illustrating accurate representation of
mly the lower-level form. and (c) drawing done with
iJ.is left hand postcommissurotomy illustrating accurate
representation of only the higher-level form.
FRACTIONATION OF SPATIAL COGNITION 27
pairment occurs primarily after RHD (Arrigoni & De Renzi, 1964; Benton,
1967; Benton & Fogel, 1962; De Renzi and Faglioni, 1967; Hannay et al.,
1976; Piercy & Smyth, 1962), whereas others have concluded that damage
to either hemisphere results in equivalent visuospatial dysfunction (Ben-
ton, 1973; Benson & Barton, 1970; Black & Strub, 1976; Colombo, De
Renzi, & Faglioni, 1976). These contradictory findings have raised the
question of whether visuospatial functioning is subserved primarily by
the right hemisphere or both hemispheres (Benton, 1985; De Renzi, 1982).
Delis, Kiefner, and Fridlund (1988) investigated severity of drawing
impairment and found that RHD and LHD patients were equally impaired
when the results were collapsed across global and local level forms pre-
sented in right and left hemispace. However, when drawing performance
was analyzed in terms of hierarchical levels and sides of hemispace, the
results clearly indicated that both hemispheres contribute qualitatively
different and complementary processes to visuospatial functioning (see
also Kaplan, 1983; Levy, 1974; Nebes, 1973; Sergent, 1982). At least two
reasons may account for past reports of greater visuospatial dysfunction
in RHD patients: (1) Impaired global processing may be manifested more
saliently than deficient local processing, especially when studies employ
visual stimuli that have minimal or no internal details (e.g., the outline of
a geometric shape); and (2) RHD is associated with defective processing in
both sides of hemispace, whereas LHD is associated with impaired pro-
cessing primarily in contralateral hemispace (Heilman et al., 1985).
Further evidence indicating that both hemispheres mediate qualita-
tively different visuospatial processes comes from studies of facial per-
ception. Traditionally it was thought that the right hemisphere is superior
for facial processing (Benton & Van Allen, 1968). It has recently been
demonstrated that the right hemisphere is superior only when holistic
aspects of faces must be considered. When individual featural aspects of
faces are important to the task, the left hemisphere is dominant (Parkin &
Williamson, 1987; Patterson & Bradshaw, 1975; Sergent, 1982). Thus it
appears that facial processing may be either primarily a right- or a left-
hemisphere function, depending on the task demands.
either the global or local levels. Stimuli were presented in three experi-
mental conditions: a No Bias condition in which there was an equal
probability of the target's appearing at either the global or the local level, a
Local Bias condition in which there was a 75% probability of the target's
appearing at the local level, and a Global Bias condition in which there
was a 75% probability of the target's appearing at the global level. Predict-
ably, subjects who are able to distribute attention between the global and
local levels should show improved performance in the biasing conditions.
The unilateral brain-damaged patients included a group with damage
restricted to the right temporal-parietal region (RTP) and two groups of
left-hemisphere-damaged patients with lesions in either the superior tem-
poral gyrus (LSTG) or the rostral inferior parietal lobule (LIPL) as deter-
mined by CT scan.
The authors report three important findings. First, attentional pro-
cesses in the LIPL patients were disrupted; that is, their performance did
not vary as a function of biasing condition. In contrast, the LSTG group
did not demonstrate an attentional disturbance. This finding is consonant
with studies indicating that the parietal region of higher primates and
humans plays a special role in attention (Bushnell, Goldberg, & Robinson,
1981; Mountcastle, 1978; Posner et a1., 1984). More specifically, the pre-
sent study suggests that the inferior parietal region is critical for the con-
trolled distribution of attention over the visual field and between hier-
archical levels. Second, overall response times were significantly lower
for both global and local forms in the LTPG group than for the LIPL group
and normal control group, suggesting a deficit in visual discrimination
associated with temporal damage. Such an interpretation is in accord
with the functional roles attributed to the two visual pathways. Third,
there was a global-local dissociation between LSTG and RTP groups, but
not between LIPL and RTP groups. The LSTG group showed a strong
baseline advantage in identifying targets at the global level, whereas the
RTP group demonstrated the opposite pattern. This finding suggests that
the glocal-Iocal dissociation previously reported with unilateral brain-
damaged patients is related to perceptual, rather than attentional, mecha-
nisms. It is also now clear that the hemispheric asymmetry observed in
hierarchical processing occurs in the first few hundred milliseconds of
analyzing a stimulus.
FIGURE 7. Examples of drawings of (a) letter and (b) shape hierarchical stimuli by normal control, Down syndrome, and Williams
syndrome subjects in the memory condition. w
....
32 DEAN C. DELIS and AMY M. BIHRLE
CONCLUSION
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
REFERENCES
Arrigoni, G., & De Renzi, E. (1964). Constructional apraxia and hemispheric locus of lesion.
Cortex, 1, 180-197.
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A., & Sabo, H. (in press). Dissociation between language and
cognitive functions in Williams syndrome. In K. Mogford & D. Bishop (Eds.), Language
development in exceptional circumstances. London: Churchill Livingstone.
Bennett, F. c., LaVeck, B., & Sells, C. J. (1978). The Williams elfin facies syndrome: The
psychological profile as an aid in syndrome identification. Pediatrics, 61, 303-305.
Benson, D. F., & Barton, M. 1. (1970). Disturbances in constructional ability. Cortex, 6, 19-46.
FRACTIONATION OF SPATIAL COGNITION 33
Benton, A. 1. (1967). Constructional apraxia and the minor hemisphere. Confinia Neu-
rologica, 19, 1-16.
Benton, A. 1. (1973). Visuoconstructive disability in patients with cerebral disease: Its rela-
tionship to side of lesion and aphasic disorder. Documenta Opthal. 34, 67-706.
Benton, A. 1. (1982). Spatial thinking in neurological patients: Historical aspects. In M.
Potegal (Ed.), Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations. New York:
Academic Press.
Benton, A. L. (1985). Visuoperceptual, visuospatial, and visuoconstructive disorders. In K.
M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford.
Benton, A. 1., & Fogel, M. L. (1962). Three-dimensional constructional praxis. Archives of
Neurology, 7, 347-354.
Benton, A. L., Hannay, J., & Varney, N. R. (1975). Visual perception of line direc;tion in
patients with unilateral brain disease. Neurology, 25, 907-910.
Benton, A. 1., Levin, H. S., & Van Allen, M. W. (1974). Geographic orientation in patients
with unilateral cerebral disease. Neuropsychologia, 12, 183-191.
Benton, A. 1., & Van Allen, M. W. (1968). Impairment in facial recognition in patients with
cerebral disease. Cortex, 4, 344-358.
Bihrle, A. M., Bellugi, U., Delis, D. C., & Marks, S. (in press). Seeing either the forest or the
trees: Dissociations in visuospatial dysfunction. Brain and cognition.
Black, F. W., & Strub, R. 1. (1976). Constructional apraxia in patients with discrete missile
wounds of the brain. Cortex, 12, 212-220.
Bradshaw, J. 1., & Nettleton, N. C. (1981). The nature of hemispheric specialization in man.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 51-91.
Brownell, H. H., & Gardner, H. (1981). Hemispheric specialization: Definitions, not incanta-
tions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 64-65.
Bushnell, M. c., Goldberg, M. E., & Robinson, D. 1. (1981). Behavioral enhancement of visual
responses in monkey cerebral cortex. L Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to
selected visual attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 46, 755-772.
Cermak,1. S., & Butters, N. (1972). The role of interference and encoding in the short-term
memory deficits of Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia, 10, 89-95.
Chomsky, N. A. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Colombo, A., De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1976). The occurrence of visual neglect in patients
with unilateral cerebral disease. Cortex, 12, 221-231.
Critcheley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. London: Edward Arnold.
Delis, D. C., Kiefner, M., & Fridlund, A. J. (1988). Visuospatial dysfunction following uni-
lateral brain damage: Dissociations in hierarchical and hemispatial analysis. Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 421-431.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., & Kiefner, M. G. (1988). Visuospatial functioning before and after
commissurotomy: Disconnection in hierarchical processing. Archives of Neurology, 45,
123-130.
Delis, D. C., Robertson, 1. C., & Efron, R. (1986). Hemispheric specialization of memory for
visual hierarchical stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 24, 205-214.
De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of spatial exploration and cognition. New York: Wiley.
De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1967). The relationship between visuospatial impairment and
constructional apraxia. Cortex, 3, 327-342.
De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., & Scotti, G. (1970). Hemispheric contribution to exploration of
space through the visual tactile modality. Cortex, 6, 191-203.
Fillmore, C. J. (1971). Types of lexical information, In D. D. Steinberg & 1. A. Jakobouits
(Eds.), Semantics: An interdisciplinary reader in philosophy, linguistics, and psychol-
ogy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fowler, A. E. (in press). The development of language structure in children with Down
syndrome. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.). Down syndrome: The developmental
perspective. New York: Cambridge University Press.
34 DEAN C. DELIS and AMY M. BIHRLE
Gardner, H., Brownell, H. H., Wapner, W., & Michelow, D. (1983). Missing the point: The
role of the right hemisphere in the processing of complex linguistic materials. In E.
Perecman (Ed.). Cognitive processes in the right hemisphere. (pp. 169-191). New York:
Academic Press.
Geschwind, N. (1970). The organization of language and the brain. Science, 170,940-944.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders.
Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Hannay, H. J., Varney, N. R., & Benton, A. L. (1976). Visual localization in patients with
unilateral brain disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 39, 307-
313.
Haxby, J. V., Duara, R., Grady, C. L., Cutler, N. R., & Rapoport, S. 1. (1985). Relations between
neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic asymmetries in early Alzheimer's disease.
Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 5, 193-200.
Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T., & Valenstein, E. (1985). Neglect and related disorders. In K.
M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.). Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Jackson, J. H. (1876). Case of large cerebral tumour without optic neuritis and with left
hemiplegia and imperception. Royal Opthalmological Hospital Reports, B, 434-444.
Jernigan, T., Tallal, P., & Bellugi, U. (1988). Cerebral morphology of magnetic resonance
imaging (MRI) in developmental cognitive disorders. Paper presented at the Interna-
tional Neuropsychological Society annual meeting, New Orleans, LA.
Kaplan, E. (1983). Process and achievement revisited. In S. Wapner & B. Kaplan (Eds.),
Toward a holistic developmental psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kinchla, R. A., Solis-Macias, V., & Hoffman, J. (1983). Attending to different levels of struc-
ture in a visual image. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 1-10.
Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: Top-down, bottom-up,
or middle-out. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 225-231.
Kleist, K. (1934). Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth.
Levy, J. (1974). Cerebral asymmetries as manifested in split-brain man. In M. Kinsbourne &
W. 1. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, IL:
Thomas.
Levy-Agristi, J., & Sperry, R. W. (1968). Differential perceptual capacities in major and minor
hemispheres. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 61, 1151.
Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.
Martin, M. (1979). Local and global processing: The role of sparsity. Memory and Cognition,
6,476-484.
Mattis, S. (1976). Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly
patient. In 1. Bellack & T. B. Karusu (Eds.), Geriatric psychiatry. New York: Grune &
Stratton.
Moscovitch, M. (1979). Information processing and the cerebral hemispheres. In M. S.
Gazzaniga (Ed.). The handbook of behavioral neurobiology. New York: Plenum Press.
Mountcastle, V. B. (1978). Brain mechanisms for directed attention. Journal of the Royal
Society of Medicine, 71, 14-28.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception.
Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.
Nebes, R. D. (1973). Perception of spatial relationships by the right and left hemispheres of
commisurotomized man. Neuropsychologia, 11, 285-289.
Newcombe, F. (1985). Neuropsychology qua interface. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 7, 663-681.
Newcombe, F., Ratcliff, G., & Damasio, H. (1987). Dissociable impairments of visual and
spatial processing following right posterior cerebral lesions: Clinical, neuropsycholo-
gical, and anatomic evidence. Neuropsychologia, 25, 149-161.
FRACTIONATION OF SPATIAL COGNITION 35
Newcombe, F., & Russell, W. R (1969). Dissociated visual perceptual and spatial deficits in
focal lesions of the right hemisphere. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychia-
try, 32, 73-81.
Palmer, S. E. (1980). What makes triangles point: Local and global effects in configuration of
ambiguous triangles. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 285-305.
Parkin, A. J., & Williamson, P. (1987). Cerebral lateralisation at different stages of facial
processing. Cortex, 23, 99-110.
Patterson, K., & Bradshaw, J. 1. (1975J. Differential hemispheric mediation of nonverbal
stimuli. Journal of Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1,246-252.
Patterson, A., & Zangwill, O. 1. (1944). Disorders of visual space perception associated with
lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain, 67, 331-358.
Piercy, M., & Smith, V. O. G. (1962). Right hemisphere dominance for certain nonverbal
intellectual skills. Brain, 85, 775-790.
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
32,3-25.
Posner, M.I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bowhuis
(Eds.), Attention and performance. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafa!' RD. (1984J. Effects of parietal injury on
covert orienting of visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863-1874.
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafa!, R D. (1987J. How do the parietal lobes
direct covert attention? Neuropsychologia, 25, 135-147.
Robertson, L. C., & Delis, D. C. (1986J. "Part-whole" processing in unilateral brain damaged
patients: Dysfunction of hierarchical organization. Neuropsychologia, 24, 363-370.
Robertson, 1. C., Lamb, M. R, & Knight, R T. (in press). Effects of lesions of temporal-
parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neu-
roscience,
Samuels, I., Butters, N., & Goodglass, H. (1970). Visual memory deficits following cortical
and limbic lesions: Effect of field of presentation. Neuropsychologia, 6, 447-452.
Sergent, J. (1982). The cerebral balance of power; Confrontation or cooperation? Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 253-272.
Sergent, J., & Bindra, D. (1982). Differential hemispheric processing of faces: Methodological
considerations and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 541-554.
Udwin, 0., Yule, W., & Martin, N. D. T. (1986). Age at diagnosis and abilities in idiopathic
hypercalcemia. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 61, 1164-1167.
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual pathways. In D. J. Ingle, M. A.
Goodale, & R J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior, (pp. 549-585).
Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Von Armin, G., & Enge!, P. (1964). Mental retardation related to hypercalcemia. Develop-
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 6, 366-377.
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1967). An experimental investigation of facial recognition in
patients with unilateral lesions. Cortex, 3, 317-326.
Warrington, E. K., James, M., & Kinsbourne, M. (1966). Drawing disability in relation to
laterality of cerebral lesion. Brain, 89, 53-82.
Warrington, E. K., & Rabin, P. (1970). Perceptual matching in patients with cerebral lesions.
Neuropsychologia, 8, 475-487.
3
AUTOPSY LOCALIZATION
ANDREW KERTESZ· University of Western Ontario, St. Joseph's Health Centre, London,
Ontario N6A 4V2, Canada.
37
38 ANDREW KERTESZ
random and rare opportunity for single cases and precludes the collection
of groups for generalizations to be made. Attempts to collect a large
number of postmortem examinations correlated with clinical observa-
tions have been made in the early 20th century, but they are plagued with
the heterogenity of the material and the lack of reliable detail in the
neurological and psychological examinations. Especially prominent are
the works of Moutier (1908), who wrote a monograph on Broca's aphasia
and associated syndromes. He described a systematic aphasia examina-
tion as an appendix to his book. He borrowed many cases, especially from
Bernheim (1900), and this method of collecting cases from the literature to
augment a few new descriptions became popular and is widely used even
today. It is not unusual to trace a case description through several series
(sometimes losing quite a bit in translation). Marie and Moutier (his
pupil) entered a vigorous debate concerning the localization of lesions in
Broca's aphasia and singled out the "quadrilateral space" central to the
insula as crucial for language, opposing Dejerine and his anatomist wife,
Klumpke, who favored the frontal operculum, also on the basis of autopsy
evidence. The most monumental collection of autopsy material is that of
Henschen (1922), who in seven large volumes collected 1,337 cases with
descriptions and illustrated more than 300 of them, often including mye-
lin-stained sections and photographs of specimens. The turn of the cen-
tury autopsy studies not only described the anatomy of lesions but drew
elaborate diagrams (resembling at times the more recent processing mod-
els) to illustrate their particular theory of language production as inferred
from the lesion evidence. This activity was dampened by the criticism of
Henry Head, who derided the "diagram makers." Von Monakow's (1914)
influential work pointed out the problems of distant effects of tumors and
the effects of depression of surrounding or functionally connected struc-
tures with acute stroke diaschisis. Nielson (1946) and Kleist (1962) wrote
more recent monographs on postmortem examinations and clinical patho-
logical correlation with language function and cognition. They espoused
rather extreme views of equating specific anatomy with function. Since
then, mostly single case reports have appeared with a variable quality of
clinical and psychological detail.
Improvements in autopsy studies may come about by the systematic
examination of stroke populations with the more sophisticated cytoarchi-
tectonic and connectivity techniques described above. Stroke registries
established at several centers allow more careful follow-up of patients.
The topographical analysis of the brain regions has been limited largely to
sulcal and gyral patterns, but cytoarchitectonic methods may correspond
more to functional differentiation. Our knowledge of functional dif-
ferences between children and adults, left and right hemispheres, sexes,
handedness, and cultural influences on cerebral organization may further
improve the sophistication of interpretations.
40 ANDREW KERTESZ
SURGICAL LOCALIZATION
CORTICAL STIMULATION
sory, motor, and language areas can be spared using this technique. Pen-
field and Roberts (1959) mapped the language areas and Van Buren,
Fedio, and Frederick (1978), Ojemann and Whitaker (1978), and Ojemann
and Mateer (1979) have added further information concerning cortical
localization.
The advantage of the method is that it can be applied in the awake,
cooperative patient who can perform selected functions during stimula-
tion. In this respect, it resembles other so-called functional methods of
localization, such as cerebral blood flow (CBF) and positron emission
tomography (PET). However, stimulation interferes with functions, inter-
rupts or alters them, and, less frequently, elicits positive phenomena. In
this respect, it is more like the lesion method, but stimulation involves a
much smaller area, allowing a greater resolution for mapping. Several
functions can be examined by repeated stimulation of the same area.
Limitations of the technique include the logistics of a lengthy surgical
procedure, difficulty reproducing the stimulation on the same spot, and
the brevity of tasks that can be employed during the short period of stim-
ulation (maximum about 12 seconds). The subjects usually have epilepsy
or tumors with reorganized brains, and the conclusions may not always be
generalizable.
The results of cortical stimulation reveal a striking discreteness of
function that is different from the conclusions drawn from lesion studies.
Naming, for example, may be affected over one gyrus, but not a few milli-
meters next to it. On the other hand, naming errors could be elicited from
a wide variety of cortical areas. The most consistent area to produce
speech arrest was in Broca's area, the posterior inferior frontal cortex, in
front of the face motor cortex (Ojemann & Whitaker, 1978). The posterior
superior temporal cortex seemed to be the next most important area in-
volved in language. Cortical mapping in bilingual patients showed sites
where one language was impaired but not the other. Naming in the less
competent and more recently acquired language was disrupted from a
wider area of stimulation. Some dissociations were also observed between
oral and sign language. When multiple language functions are tested over
the same site, only one may be involved. Grammar, semantics, and artic-
ulation seem to be dissociated in a highly variable fashion across patients.
Grammatical errors often occurred in the frontal lobe without articulatory
deticit. Semantic errors were from more widely distributed sites of stim-
ulation. Memory-related sites surround the perisylvian (usually parietal)
cortex at some distance from it. Individual variability may be related to
anatomical and educational differences and IQ (Mateer, 1983).
The localization of nonverbal functions in the nondominant hemi-
sphere was also studied with cortical stimulation using line orientation,
face-matching, and facial recognition tasks. Perceptual errors were ob-
served at the parietooccipital and also at the frontal sites. Recognition
ANATOMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS 43
errors occured in the posterior portion of the superior temporal gyrus and
parietal lobe. The different spatial tasks, such as face matching and line
orientation, were affected at similar sites, but dissociation was observed
between the sites for perception and recognition (Mateer, 1983). The ten-
tative conclusion reached was that visuospatial functions in the nondomi-
nant hemisphere appeared to be as discretely localized as verbal functions
in the dominant hemisphere. This is somewhat contrary to the conclu-
sions drawn from lesion studies by Semmes, Weinstein, Ghent, and Teu-
ber (1960) and Kertesz and Dobrowolski (1981).
with the method is the time restriction on the stimuli. The stimulation has
to be very short and have a definite onset, duration, and offset, in order to
be connected with cerebral event. In a way, it is the opposite to cerebral
blood flow studies (see below), which require more sustained cerebral
activity to be analyzed. The major advantage of the technique is that it
reflects a physiological event connected with actual cerebral processing,
although the resolution of localization is poor and the potentials are usu-
ally over a large area of the scalp and the brain. The origin and the genera-
tion of evoked potentials are often obscure, and their interpretation is
often subject to argument and to change over the course of years. For
instance, P300 potentials, which were initially interpreted as reflecting
cerebral perceptual processing, are now considered a postdecisional po-
tential, signifying the closure of the perceptual process and target selec-
tion. Since the method is noninvasive and is becoming relatively less
expensive, future expansion of the technique can be expected. Its major
requirement and, at the same time, limiting factor is the necessity of
having a trained electrophysiologist who devotes full time to this activity.
gyrus and the posterior perisylvian region, and conduction aphasia in-
volved a smaller and somewhat more anterior region between Wernicke's
and Broca's area. Lesions causing conduction aphasia appeared to cluster
bimodally into anterior and posterior groups. Lesions of anomic aphasics
were widely scattered, and transcortical aphasics were outside the per-
isylvian speech area. Transcortical motor aphasics overlapped in the sup-
plementary motor area in the superior frontal lobe, and transcortical sen-
sory aphasics were posterior to Wernicke's area. The severity of aphasia as
measured with a standardized test correlated inversely with lesion size.
The major disadvantage of isotope scanning is that the normal land-
marks are few, so that accurate anatomical localization is difficult and
depends on inference. The edges of the lesions are somewhat indistinct
and can be subject to misinterpretation. A certain portion of the lesions is
missed because the scan is undertaken either too early or too late. This
restriction in time is often a disadvantage and excludes localization in
many patients studied chronically. However, as discussed above, the spe-
cific interval during which the scan will be positive makes it useful as a
diagnostic tool. This method has been eclipsed by the CT scan and subse-
quently by MRI scanning. However, there are still conditions where this
method is superior to all others, such as in the localization of herpes
simplex encephalitis, where, in several examples, the CT scan was nega-
tive while the radioisotope study showed a clear-cut uptake in both tem-
porallobes. Another continuing advantage of the method is its relatively
high availability in just about every hospital and its relatively low ex-
pense.
which all other localizing methods are compared. Because of its availabil-
ity and easy administration, it will likely retain this position for consider-
able time to come. Much neuropsychological work has been done with CT
and will continue to be relevant.
MRI is the latest imaging modality, and it shows the greatest promise
in accurate localization of lesions without invasive radiation. The tech-
nique uses the inherent magnetic properties of spinning atomic nuclei by
placing the structure to be imaged in a large magnet and applying short-
wave radiofrequency pulses to produce a resonance signal that can be
quantified and computerized (Doyle et a1., 1981). Superior anatomical
ANATOMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS 51
detail can be achieved with excellent gray and white matter differentia-
tion and an accurate outline of the edge of the brain from CSF spaces. The
brain can be imaged in coronal and sagittal sections, in addition to the
horizontal ones, which is the usual plane obtained in other modalities.
This imaging flexibility, combined with anatomical accuracy, has already
established MRI as a useful clinical and research tool. The apparent lack
of biohazard allows it to be used to study normals without clinical indica-
tions, as well as a more frequent repetition of imaging in patients than is
possible in other modalities that use ionizing radiation.
Various pulse sequences can be used to probe the metabolic and
molecular environment of various regions of the brain, and this makes the
technique much more dynamic than the CT scan. Currently, two major
pulse sequences are used. One called "inversion recovery" emphasizes
gray and white matter differences and provides excellent anatomical de-
tail. The second, called "spin echo," is utilized to detect edema and other
metabolic changes associated with lesions. Spin echo sequences are often
superior to CT scanning in the early detection of cerebral infarct, thus
reducing the false negative rate obtained on CT in the first 2 or 3 days of a
stroke. The technique is particularly suited to detect demyelinating
plaques and some other degenerative diseases in addition to early strokes.
The evolution of an infarct follows a regular pattern, with a gradual clear-
ing of the central portion on the spin echo image that leaves a ring of
increased signal intensity around the infarct in the chronic state. This ring
appears to extend beyond the hypo density seen on CT. The selection of
various repetition times and pulse sequences permits a differentiated vi-
sualization of brain tissues, such as the CSF versus the brain, by altering
the influence of various tissue properties. The inversion recovery se-
quence emphasizes the T1 or longitudinal relaxation time, which is the
return of the spinning nuclei to their original axis in the large magnetic
field. The spin echo sequences are weighted for the T2 relaxation time,
which depends on the interaction of the spinning nuclei with each other.
MRI is a new technology that is currently in a relatively unstable state
of development. The expense is somewhat greater than that of scanners
for most prototype equipment. Recently, this modality has become avail-
able to most major centers. It will likely not displace, but will comple-
ment CT scanning. Clinical imaging can be accomplished in 15 minutes,
but for research purposes the pulse sequences can take an hour. Lesions
are well outlined, and the dynamic changes around or in the lesions can
be followed over time. Variations in the images are dependent on the
molecular environment and water density of the tissues. Actual func-
tional changes or psychological states cannot be examined with the cur-
rent equipment.
Neuropsychological studies with MRI include the localization of le-
sions in aphasia and a study of anatomical asymmetries utilizing the
52 ANDREW KERTESZ
Autopsy The ultimate in Restricted to Indirect re- Only in the Postmortem Moderate Accuracy Retrospecti ve
accuracy some cases mote past
Skull wounds Poor, only an ap- Restricted group Indirect Unlimited af- Penetrating Minor In vivo exam Inaccurate
proximation ter lesion injury
Neurosurgery Accurate Restricted group Indirect Unlimited af- Neurosurgery Moderate In vivo exam Altered tissue
ter lesion
Cortical stim- Accurate surface Restricted to a Indirect 12 sec and Neurosurgery Moderate to Small areas Limited test-
ulation only few control expensive ing
EEG, evoked Poor (lobar only) Limited to phys- Expecta- Minutes and Noninvasive Moderate Physiological Low specif-
potentials iologists tions, at- millise- icity
tention conds
Isotope scans Poor (outline Everywhere Indirect Unlimited Noninvasive Minor In vivo exam Poor resolu-
only) acute stage tion
CT scans Accurate to a de- Almost every- Indirect Unlimited af- Minor radia- Moderate In vivo exam Limited land-
gree where ter lesion tion marks
CBF scans Poor, better ar- Restricted to Closely re- 5 min Minor (more Moderate to Related to Poor resolu-
terially stroke centers lated if arterial) expensive function tion
PET scans Poor but improv- Only few Direct (met- 20-60 min Minor radia- Veryexpen- Directly func- Resolution,
ing abolic) <1 min in tion sive tional expense
future
SPECT scans Poor but improv- Restricted to Closely re- 5-20 min Minor radia- Moderate to CBF in slices Resolution,
ing few centers lated tion expensive expense
MRI scans Accurate Expanding ma- Indirect Unlimited af- Noninvasive Moderate to Best resolution Not yet avail-
jor centers ter lesion expensive able
56 ANDREW KERTESZ
REFERENCES
Benson, D. F. (1967). Fluency in aphasia: Correlation with radioactive scan localization.
Cortex, 3, 373-394.
Benson, D. F., Metter, E. J., Kuhl, D. E., & Phelps, M. (1983). Positron-computed tomography
in neurobehavioral problems. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology. New
York: Academic Press.
Bernheim, F. (1900). De l'aphasie motrice. Paris: Carre et Naud.
Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth.
Brown, W. S., Marsh, J. T., & Smith, J. C. (1973). Contextual meaning effects on speech-
evoked potentials. Behavioral Biology, 9, 755-761.
Buchsbaum, M., & Fedio, P. (1970). Hemispheric differences in evoked potentials to verbal
and nonverbal stimuli in the left and right visual fields. Physiology and Behavior,S,
207-210.
Burkhardt, G. (1891). Ueber Rindenexcisionen, als Beitrag zur operativen Therapie der Psy-
chosen. Allgemeine Zeitschrift fiir Psychiatrie, 47, 463-548.
Callaway, E., & Harris, P. R. (1974). Coupling between cortical potentials from different
areas. Science, 183, 873-875.
Cappa, S. F., & Vignolo, L. A. (1979). "Transcortical" features of aphasia following left
thalamic hemorrhage. Cortex, 15, 121-130.
Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1983). Localization of lesions in achromatopsia and pros-
opagnosia. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology. New York: Academic
Press.
Dandy, W. (1931). The effect of total removal of the left temporal lobe in a right handed
individual: Localization of areas of the brain concerned with speech. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease, 74, 739-742.
ANATOMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS 57
Levine, D., & Mohr, J. (1979). Language after bilateral cerebral infarctions: Role of the minor
hemisphere in speech. Neurology, 29, 297-338.
Low, W., Wada, J. A., & Fox, M. (1973). Electroencephalographic localization of the conative
aspects of language production in the human brain. Transactions of American Neu-
rological Association, 98, 129-133.
Luria, A. R. (1970). Traumatic aphasia. The Hague: Mouton.
Marie, 1"., & Foix, C. (1917). Les aphasies de guerre. Revue Neurologique, 24, 53-87.
Marinesco, G., Sager, 0., & Kreindler, A. (1936). Etudes electroencephalographiques; elec-
troencephalogrammes dans l'aphasie. Bulletin of the Academy of Medicine of Paris,
116, 182.
Mateer, C. (1983). Localization of language and visuospatial functions by electrical stimula-
tion. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology. New York: Academic Press.
Mazziotta, J. C., & Phelps, M. E. (1984). Human sensory stimulation and· deprivation:
Positron emission tomographic results and strategies. Neurology, 14, 50-60.
Metter, E. J., Wasterlain, C. G., Kuhl, D. E., Hanson, W. R., & Phelps, M. E. (1981). 18 FDG
positron emission computed tomography in a study of aphasia. Annals of Neurology,
10, 173-183.
Mettler, R. (1949). Selective partial ablation of the frontal cortex: A correlative study of the
effects of the human psychotic subjects. New York: Hoeber.
Mohr, J. P. (1976). Broca's area and Broca's aphasia. In H. Whitaker & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.),
Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.
Mohr, J. P., Watters, W. C., & Duncan, G. W. (1975). Thalamic hemorrhage and aphasia. Brain
and Language, 2, 3-17.
Moutier, F. (1908). L'aphasie de Broca. Paris: Steinheil.
Neville, H. J., Kutas, M., & Schmidt, A. (1982). Event-related potential studies of cerebral
specialization during reading. 1. Studies of normal adults. Brain and Language, 16, 300-
315.
Nielsen, J. M. (1946). Agnosia, apraxia, aphasia. New York: Hoeber.
Ojemann, G. A., & Mateer, C. (1979). Human language cortex: Localization of memory,
syntax and sequential motor-phoneme identification systems. Science, 250, 1401-1403.
Ojemann, G. A., & Whitaker, H. A. (1978). Language localization and variability. Brain and
Language, 6, 239-260.
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Raichle, M. E., Herscovitch, P., Mintun, A. M., Martin, W. R. W., & Power, W. (1984).
Dynamic measurements of local blood flow and metabolism in the study of higher
cortical function in humans with positron emission tomography. Neurology, 14,48-49.
Rei!, J. C. (1809). Untersuchungen tiber den Bau des grossen Gehirns im Menschen. Archiv
fiir Anatomie und Physiologie, 9, 136-524.
Rubens, A. B. (1976). Transcortical motor aphasia. In H. Whitaker & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.),
Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.
Russell, W. R., & Espir, M. L. E. (1961). Traumatic aphasia. London: Oxford University
Press.
Sanides, F. (1962). Die architektonik des menschlichen Stirnhirns. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Schiller, F. (1947). Aphasia studied in patients with missile wounds. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 10, 183.
Semmes, J., Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., & Teuber, H. 1. (1960). Somatosensory changes after
penetrating brain wounds in man. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Shelburne, S. A. (1972). Visual evoked responses to word and nonsense syllable stimuli.
Cortex, 12, 325-336.
Smith, A. (1966). Speech and other functions after left (dominant) hemispherectomy.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 29, 467-471.
ANATOMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS 59
Sperry, R. W., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Bogen, J. E. (1969). Interhemispheric relationship: The
neocortical commissures; syndromes of hemispheric disconnection. In P. J. Vinken & G.
W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 4). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Talairach, J., & Szikla, G. (1967). Atlas of stereotaxic anatomy of the telencephalon. Paris:
Masson.
Ter-Pogossian, M. M., Ficke, D. C., Mintun, M. A., Herscovitch, P., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M.
E. (1984). Dynamic cerebral positron emission tomographic studies. Neurology, 15, 46-
47.
Teyler, T. J., Roemer, R. A., Harrison, T. F., et al. (1973). Human scalp-recorded evoked-
potential correlates of linguistic stimuli. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1, 333-
324.
Tikofsky, R. S., Collier, B. D., Saxena, V. K., Hellman, R. S., Zielonka, J. A., & Palmer, D. W.
(1984). 1-123 iodoamphetamine (IMP) single photon emission computerized tomogra-
phy (SPECT) imaging in normals and aphasics. Paper presented at the Academy of
Aphasia meeting, Los Angeles.
Tikofsky, R. S., Kooi, K. A., & Thomas, M. H. (1960). Electroencephalographic findings and
recovery from aphasia. Neurology, 10, 154-156.
Van Buren, J., Fedio, P., & Frederick, G. (1978). Mechanism and localization of speech in the
parietotemporal cortex. Neurosurgery, 2, 233-239.
Von Keyserlingk, D. G., De Bleser, R., & Poeck, K. (1983). Stereographic reconstruction of
human brain CT series. Acta Anatomique, 115, 336-344.
Von Monakow, C. (1914). Die Lokalisation im Grosshirn und der Abbau der Funktionen
durch corticale Herde. Wiesbaden: Bergmann.
Wood, F. (1980). Theoretical, methodological, and statistical implications of the inhalation
rCBF technique for the study of brain-behavior relationships. Brain and Language, 9, 1-
8.
Yarnell, P. R., Monroe, M. A., & Sobel, 1. (1976). Aphasia outcome in stroke: A clinical and
neuroradiological correlation. Stroke, 7, 516-522.
4
There are a great many reviews of ERP techniques for those intending
to set up an ERP laboratory (e.g., Callaway, Tueting, & Koslow, 1978;
Gaillard & Ritter, 1983). In this section, rather than outline these details, I
61
62 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
Methods of Analysis
Rather than outline the evidence in favor of and against interpreta-
tions of the various wave peaks that have been propounded, I would like
to focus on the aspects of wave analysis that pertain to neurolinguistics
specifically. The first question we must address in cognitive electrophysi-
ology is whether the effect we find is attributable to the specific cognitive
processes of interest or to general attentional processes. For example, the
early negative (Nl00 and sometimes N200), late positive (P300), and later
negative (N400) components seem to be sensitive to attentional aspects of
the typical cognitive ERP paradigm, and the peaks have become quite
famous in the ERP literature (see Figure 1 for a simple example). The size
and latency of these components may vary depending on the stimuli or
the processing task, allowing us to conclude that there are processing
differentiations being made. For example, reading high-frequency words
may produce values quite different from reading low-frequency words. As
useful as these components are in cognitive electrophysiology (see Fried-
man, Simson, Ritter, & Rapin, 1975; Gaillard & Ritter, 1983; Kutas & Van
Petten, 1988, for reviews), we can never be sure that the cognitive compo-
nent we are interested in is reflected in these famous peaks, since the
wave contains much information beyond these particular points.
An alternate approach is to compare the entire wave shape across the
experimental conditions. This is done usually with one of two methods,
either with what is known as the peA-ANOVA technique (Donchin, 1966)
or by using Pearson correlations in particular time frames across the wave.
The former is a highly controversial technique that derives time-clustered
components by which to parse the wave shape, and then compares the
factor scores for these components across the independent variables. The
factor scores essentially reflect amplitude differences associated with the
independent variables. The controversy surrounds the issue of whether or
64 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
Factor 1
Factor 2 FIGURE 2. An example of a PCA out-
put from an ERP experiment
Factor 3 (Segalowitz & Cohen, 1988). The cen-
Factor 4 troid is the averaged response over
frontal, parietal, and temporal sites to a
Factor 5 series of consonant-vowel speech
Factor 6 sounds. The eight factors represent sta-
tistically orthogonal time clusters that
Factor 7 together account for 93% of the vari-
ance in the raw data. See text for further
Factor 8
discussion.
not the time-based components are true underlying factors in the makeup
of the aggregate averaged ERP (Mocks, 1986; Mocks & Verleger, 1986;
Wood & McCarthy, 1984). The true test of this technique is, like any other
technique, in replication and generalization, which has been occasionally
done (e.g., Molfese, 1978, vs. Segalowitz & Cohen, 1989), but much less
often than for the more traditional analysis of wave peaks. This situation
may be as much a product of the relatively few laboratories utilizing this
complex technique as of the inherent complications in using it (Kramer &
Donchin, 1987).
For example, one of the facets of this technique that deserves caution
is that in a single study, the averaged ERPs are subjected to a principal
components analysis that derives several factors, each representing some
time clustering (see Figure 2). For each factor, the analysis produces factor
scores for each combination of independent variables for each averaged
ERP entering the analysis. These factor scores indicate the relative weight
for the dependent measures-Leo the cell in the design-leading to an
analysis of variance for each factor. One may question the true Type I
error when, say, 10 such ANOVAs are performed, not exactly on the same
data set, but on scores derived from the same data set. We do not know
whether this should be a concern, and perhaps the test of time will be an
empirical one. For example, Segalowitz and Cohen (1988) tried to repli-
cate Molfese's (1978) study of cerebral asymmetries in ERPs produced to
certain phonological contrasts. The effect seemed to generalize well to the
new stimuli, contexts, and electrode sites used by Segalowitz and Cohen,
but the nagging problem of true alpha level remained. To test for this, they
performed 100 repeated ANOV As on the factor scores for the critical
ERPs AND ADVANCES IN NEUROLINGUISTICS 65
condition that replicated Molfese for each of eight factors, each time ran-
domizing the factor score set. There were in this way nine chance replica-
tions of the original result. They concluded that the .025 level was more
than safe as a level of Type I error to accept for the replication.
It may be that the strength of the PCA-ANDV A technique is not sim-
ply in finding differentiations in the ERPs that are very difficult to make
from a visual scan of the data, but is to be found in the focusing of
attention to a particular portion of the wave. As long as we accept the idea
that the ERP is highly sensitive to all sorts of processing, we will be left
with the problem of deciding which subtle fluctuation in the waves are
reflections of our experimental design. To return to the example given
from Segalowitz and Cohen, after verifying that the effect replicating Mol-
fese was not a statistical quirk, they returned to the raw ERPs and exam-
ined the wave in the section indicated by the factor in question. If the
factor had been an emergent statistical product, there should be no corre-
spondence in the raw data. They found that the raw data produced the
same interaction. It was unlikely, though, that they would have thought to
look at that part of the wave if the PCA had not pointed to it, since it did
not represent one of the famous peaks. Similarly, Brown, Marsh, and Smith
(1979) found the same results using a PCA-ANOVA treatment of data
reported earlier using ERP correlations (Brown, Marsh, & Smith, 1976).
A simpler method of comparing the overall wave shape that would be
useful for neurolinguistic investigations is a straight correlational metric.
For example, Brown, Marsh, and Smith (1973, 1976) found that the Pear-
son correlation of the ERPs for noun and verb forms of ambiguous words
was higher over the right hemisphere than over the left hemisphere, and
that this difference was especially found for the anterior sites. The beauty
of this technique is that we do not have to interpret the specifics of the
wave characteristics, only that the waves resemble each other across con-
ditions over some sites and not over others. Refinement of this method
involves restricting the time frame for the calculation of the ERP.
that the latency of the P300 peak is not correlated with the RT response,
and thus could not be simply due to some preparation to respond. This is
important because, without being assured of this separation, we could
conclude that the ERP did not reflect cognitive processes at all, but only
motor responses. This would have been easy to accept since movement
effects are very strong in the EEG record. Since, however, the P300 seems
to reflect something in the decision-making process, we can entertain the
possibility that the peaks in general reflect important steps in the cog-
nitive requirements of the tasks.
Now, if we find that a change in processing requirement causes a
prominent peak to lag or lead, we may wish to conclude that the process-
ing change occurs at this point in the task. This is a reasonable hypothesis
to form from such data, but are the data sufficient to conclude this? Clear-
ly the processing must occur at some time before the evidence of it ap-
pears in the ERP. The difficulty is that we do not know, and perhaps
cannot know, how much before. Thus, a mental chronometry is possible
only in terms of "at least by the point" constructs.
More elaborate mental chronometries can be constructed from the
PCA-ANDVA technique, since many research questions can be asked at
the same time of the same data, each with a timing figure. For example,
Segalowitz, Menna, and MacGregor (1987) presented the results of adult
subjects' making matching judgments on pairs of words, which either (1)
look similar and rhyme (e.g., look, book), (2) look similar and do not
rhyme (e.g., paid, said), (3) rhyme but do not look similar (e.g., make,
ache), or (4) do not rhyme or look similar (e.g., mind, wall), a paradigm
used by Polich, McCarthy, Wang, and Donchin (1983). A factor peaking at
272 msec indicates that the left hemisphere has reacted differentially to
the rhyme confusion list (list 3) compared with the two control lists (lists
1 and 4), whereas the right hemisphere does not. A factor at 370 msec
shows that the left hemisphere is clearly differentiating both confusion
lists (2 and 3) from the controls, while the right hemisphere is only differ-
entiating the visual confusion list (list 2). By 490 msec, both hemispheres
are sensitive to the two confusion lists. This simple story would be fine if
we could be confident that these times really reflected the decision point
in the mental progression in the task. Because of the difficulties men-
tioned earlier in verifying the objective reality of the component factors, it
is important to replicate these sorts of specific findings. The use of the
ERP technique to create a mental chronometry would certainly be a break-
through for cognitive electrophysiology. Whether the techniques outlined
will prove useful in the end is an empirical question.
There are, however, built-in limitations that should be acknowledged
at the outset. First of all, the generalizability of the results across laborato-
ries will always be difficult not because of instability of the results but
because of differing amplification and filtering systems. Each set of ampli-
ERPs AND ADVANCES IN NEUROLINGUISTICS 67
fiers has to have some setting for the time constant (Duncan-Johnson &
Donchin, 1981), and this time constant effectively sets the lower limit of
the frequency response. Similarly, each laboratory must decide on the set
of signal filters that it will use, these usually being analogue filters built
into the amplifier system. Analogue filters, however, cause phase shifting.
For example, the peaks of slow waves can be delayed by half a cycle with
standard 30-Hz low pass filters, which can translate into as much as 50
msec or more. This shift can be easily measured for a pure signal, say a 10-
Hz sine wave, but is unpredictable for an irregular wave such as an EEG
signal. Since most laboratories use the upper cutoff of 30 Hz and a lower
cutoff of under 1 Hz, there may be fairly good correspondence in the
literature, but it does not mean that this will hold for all laboratories (cf.
Kramer & Donchin, 1987).
A second difficulty with obtaining a fine chronometry is the inherent
limitation of the filtered ERP signal. The usual 30-Hz band pass is used to
reduce the higher frequencies that stem from muscle artifacts, and indeed,
EEG signals filtered at 30 Hz are considerably cleaner to view. The diffi-
culty is that at 30-Hz filtering, defining a peak takes 33 msec, which
means that peaks cannot be fully resolved in a time period shorter than
this, although with averaging, apparently higher frequencies can be ob-
tained. Is 33 msec a long or a short time for cognitive processing? This
clearly depends on the processing we are concerned with, but we can
easily think of cases where much can be done in this time period. It is not
surprising, then, that the major ERP peaks that have been tied to cognitive
processing are separated by at least 100 msec. The technique as it is
currently used limits the temporal resolution.
to the left hemisphere via the right visual half-field will be greater over the
right hemisphere, especially if recorded at the 01 and 02 sites (Wood,
1982). This is because the visual signal registers in the mesial section of
the occipital lobe, and the more centrally located the stimulus, the strong-
er this effect. One solution is to display stimuli laterally enough to have
them represented on the lateral occipital surface; another is not to record
occipitally when using visual stimuli. For example, Neville, Kutas, and
Schmidt (1982a) find ERP asymmetries in temporal and frontal regions
that are independent of the visual field of stimulus presentation. There
have been no suggestions, though, that this problem of hemisphere differ-
entiation arises from any cognitive activity outside of visual perception,
although presumably mesial primary sensory cortex of the parietal lobe
would produce similar results.
A similar situation holds for the N100 component of the ERP ob-
tained in an auditory attention task. This negative wave is found max-
imally over the frontocentral areas of the scalp. Yet it has been shown to
be generated in the auditory cortex of the temporal lobe (Scherg & Von
Craman, 1986). Notice that once again we are dealing with primary senso-
ry cortex.
Lexical Semantics
There has been a considerable increase in interest in the last few
years on the issue of lexical semantics, which for the purposes of this
chapter will be defined as the network of meaning relationships that is
mediated through the linguistic system (d. Job & Sartori, 1988). The in-
crease in interest stems no doubt from both cognitive and neuropsycholo-
gical sources. The development of semantic network systems over the
past two decades (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975; Johnson-Laird, Hermann, &
Chaffin, 1984) has spurred hope of understanding the structure of the web
of meaning in the human mind. Until recently, however, the neurolin-
guistic issue remained fairly simple: Linguistic semantics is entirely a
function of the left hemisphere, and the primary issue was whether or not
there were semantic-syntactic disruptions in anterior versus posterior
forms of aphasia (Zurif & Caramazza, 1976). Brown et a1. (1973, 1976), for
example, as mentioned earlier found that when subjects listen to a word
that could be interpreted as a noun or a verb depending on the context
(Le., fire, duck, 1ead/led), ERPs over the left hemisphere, and the anterior
presentation especially, differentiated the two grammatical forms.
More recently, however, patients with right-hemisphere damage have
70 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
Reading in Normals
Can we discern component processes in reading and use ERPs to
define them, time-track them, and localize them cerebrally? Current theo-
ries of reading focus on English, probably because most of the researchers
in the field work in English-speaking countries (e.g., Coltheart, 1987; Colt-
heart, Patterson, & Marshall, 1980; Henderson, 1984). The models of read-
ing English almost invariably include two routes: a phonologically based
route, which involves the grapheme-phoneme correspondence (GPC)
rules, and a so-called visual route, which makes use of visual information.
The need for the two routes in English is clear (Patterson, 1982). The
reading of new words is clearly first attempted by "sounding out," if the
spelling seems to be regular, and often pronunciation errors are based on
spelling-pronunciations. Visual information is also clearly used since En-
glish has many homophones that have distinct spellings (e.g., seas, seize,
sees). Both these situations obtain in French as well (Le., new words
pronounced by GPC analogies to known words, and the existence of hom-
onyms such as il LIE, le lit, je lis). The mapping of spelling onto sound and
vice versa differs in interesting ways, however, and so the psycholinguis-
tic processes in reading may differ across the two languages (N. S.
Segalowitz, 1986). Languages with more regular GPC, such as Spanish,
Italian, or Estonian, again may involve the reader with a different set of
cognitive strategies. These interlanguage differences are only beginning to
be studied (Patterson, Marshall, & Coltheart, 1985).
In any case, the existence of these two routes for English is well
72 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
RIGHT HEMISPHERE
,
FIGURE 3. Averaged ERPs from left- and right- ,,
hemisphere sites taken to the second of a pair of LEFT HEM I SPHERE
letter strings (Segalowitz, Menna, & MacGregor,
1987). Subjects had to indicate by button press
whether the two letter strings, which were either
pronounceable (---) or un-pronounceable (-J, were
the same or different. These averages are for the
"same" trials only, and illustrate the slight but con-
sistent hemisphere by stimulus list interaction. 1;---+----+---+-----+---;j,5bo MSEC
ERP variables onto reading performance scores at each age group. The
finding relevant to the present discussion is that as the children got older,
the amount of variance accounted for by the left temporal site increased
dramatically while that for the parietal sites, especially the right, de-
creased. Thus, there is support, circumstantially at least, for the neu-
rolinguistic right-to-left shift for reading. By the hypothesis given here,
this relationship may not hold for those languages where phonics is the
sole strategy used.
These researchers, however, suggest that the important issue here is
not the application of GPC rules but of automaticity. Many theorists (Ellis,
1984) suggest that beginning readers first decode words element by ele-
ment and gradually automatize the process so that larger groupings are
apprehended. This chunking is necessary, of course, for rapid, fluent
reading. Remaining research questions include, however, whether the
advanced reader truly abandons the more elemental process and whether
the automatized process represents a shift in cerebral functioning. Bakker
and Licht (1986), for instance, report that normally, beginning reading
children show a shift in greater ERP amplitude to reading words from the
right hemisphere to the left hemisphere. Poor readers who remain slow at
decoding seem to be stuck at the first element-by-element stage and do not
show such a shift. Poor readers who read rapidly with many mistakes-
that is, who seem to move to automatized reading before they have built
up the processing chunks adequately-make this ERP amplitude shift
sooner than normal. If these results are reliable, the neurolinguistic im-
plications for reading and for automatized processes in general are con-
siderable with pedagogical implications (Bakker, 1984).
Language Development
We expect that language acquisition, as a phonological guessing
game, is primarily left-hemisphere-based. Molfese, Freeman, and Palermo
(1975), in their classic study, found ERP hemisphere differentiation of
speech sounds versus musical sounds in young infants. Many similar
findings (Molfese & Betz, 1988; S. J. Segalowitz, 1983) have since rein-
forced the view that hemisphere specialization is congenital in most chil-
dren and is not gradually developed during childhood, as was formerly
thought (Lenneberg, 1967). ERPs have proved very useful for cognitive
studies with young infants because there are few behavioral paradigms
that will tap processing at such an early stage, especially in the auditory
modality. Molfese and Betz list 11 ERP studies with infants demonstrating
cerebral asymmetries. They then go on to describe their line of studies
that focus on the mechanisms of such early functional asymmetries, again
illustrating how the ERP paradigm is well suited for this research ques-
tion. For example, they have shown not only that certain stop consonant
ERPs AND ADVANCES IN NEUROLINGUISTICS 75
distinctions are made from birth onward but that such data can be used to
predict later delay in language acquisition (Molfese & Molfese, 1985). As
well, they are using the ERP paradigm to examine the brain correlates of
word acquisition by comparing the ERP response to arbitrarily paired
nonsense syllables with a new object (Molfese et 01., 1985) and to real
known versus unknown words (Molfese, n.d.).
There are many other developmental neurolinguistic hypotheses
worthy of investigation using this technique. For example, the develop-
ment of lexical semantics is an area that has been well studied behav-
iorally over the last two decades. Meanwhile, neurolinguistic investiga-
tors have suggested that word acquisition requiring a new category
distinction may especially entail right-hemisphere mechanisms (Gross-
man, 1981; Grossman & Carey, 1978). From a developmental neu-
rolinguistic viewpoint, this is an important question, since we are still
exploring the relationship between lexical and nonlexical semantics.
Given that Molfese has shown that the ERP paradigm is sensitive to at
least some aspect of word acquisition, it would be interesting to examine
developmental data on these same parameters that were of interest in the
adult lexical semantic studies.
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Albert, M., & abler, 1. (1978). The bilingual brain. New York: Academic Press.
Bakker, D. J. (1984). The brain as a dependent variable. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology,
6, 1-16.
Bakker, D. J., & Licht, R. (1986). Learning to read: Changing horses in midstream. In G. Th.
Pavlides & D. F. Fisher (Eds.), Dyslexia: Its neuropsychology and treatment. New York:
Riley.
Beaumont, J. G. (Ed.). (1982). Divided visual field studies of cerebral organization. London:
Academic Press.
Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical
reading-spelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15,663-687.
Brown, W. S., Marsh, J. T., & Smith, J. C. (1973). Contextual meaning effects on speech-
evoked potentials. Behavioral Biology, 9, 755-761.
Brown, W. S., Marsh, J. T., & Smith, J. C. (1976). Evoked potential wave form differences
produced by the perception of different meanings of an ambiguous phrase. Electroen-
cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 41, 113-123.
Brown, W. S., Marsh, J. T., & Smith, J. C. (1979). Principal component analysis of ERP
differences related to the meaning of an ambiguous word. Electroencephalography and
Clinical Neurophysiology, 46, 709-714.
Brownell, H. H., Michel, D., Powelson, J., & Gardner, H. (1983). Surprise but not coherence:
Sensitivity to verbal humor in right-hemisphere patients. Brain and Language, 18, 20-
27.
Brownell, H. H., Potter, H. H., Michelow, D., & Gardner, H. (1984). Sensitivity to lexical
denotation and connotation in brain-damaged patients: A double dissociation? Brain
and Language, 22, 253-265.
Bryden, M. P. (1982). Laterality: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York:
Academic Press.
Burgess, C., & Simpson, G. B. (1988). Cerebral hemisphere mechanisms in the retrieval of
ambiguous word meanings. Brain and Language, 33, 86-103.
Callaway, E., Tueting, P., & Koslow, S. H. (1978). Event-related brain potentials in man. New
York: Academic Press.
Campbell, K, Deacon-Elliott, D., & Proulx, G. (1986). Electrophysiological monitoring of
78 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
closed head injury, I. Basic principles and techniques. Cognitive Rehabilitation, Sep-
tember-October, 26-32.
Campbell, K, Deacon-Elliott, D., Suffield, J. B., & Proulx, G. B. (1987). Electrophysiological
monitoring of closed head injury, II. Multimodal sensory evoked potentials. Cognitive
Rehabilitation, January-February, 26-33.
Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology: An introduction. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Caramazza, A., Gordon, J., Zurif, E. B., & DeLuca, D. (1976). Right-hemispheric damage and
verbal problem solving behavior. Brain and Language, 3, 41-46.
Chapman, R M., Bragdon, H. R, Chapman, J. A., & McCrary, J. W. (1977). Semantic meaning
of words and averaged evoked potentials. In J. E. Desmedt (Ed.), Recent developments in
the psychobiology of language: The cerebral evoked potential approach. London: Ox-
ford University Press.
Chiarello, C. (1985). Hemisphere dynamics in lexical access: Automatic and controlled
priming. Brain and Language, 26, 146-172.
Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.
Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
Coltheart, M. (1980). Deep dyslexia: A right-hemisphere hypothesis. In M. Coltheart, K.
Patterson, & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Coltheart, M. (1987). The psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Coltheart, M., Patterson, K, & Marshall, J. C. (1980). Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Crystal, D. (1988). Linguistic levels in aphasia. In F. C. Rose, R Whurr, & M. Ryke (Eds.),
Aphasia (pp. 23-45). London: Whurr.
Deacan-Eliott, D., Campbell, K. B., Suffield, J. B., & Proulx, G. B. (1987). Electrophysiological
monitoring of closed head injury. III. Cognitive evoked potentials. Cognitive Rehabilita-
tion, 12-21.
Delis, D. C., Wapner, W., Gardner, H., & Moses, J. A. (1983). The contribution of the right
hemisphere to the organization of paragraphs. Cortex, 19, 43-50.
Donchin, E. (1966). A multivariate approach to the analysis of averaged evoked potentials.
IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, BME-13(3), 131-139.
Duffy, F. H., Burchfiel, J. L., & Lambroso, C. T. (1979). Brain electrical activity mapping
(BEAM): A method for extending the clinical utility of EEG and evoked potential data.
Annals of Neurology, 5, 309-321.
Duncan-Johnson, C. c., & Donchin, E. (1981). The time constant in P300 recording. Psycho-
physiology, 16, 53-55.
D'Urso, V., Denes, G., Testa, S., & Semenza, C. (1986). The role of the right hemisphere in
processing negative sentences in context. Neuropsychologia, 24, 289-292.
Dywan, J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1986). The role of the case study in neuropsychological
research. In J. Val siner (Ed.), The individual subject and scientific psychology. New
York: Plenum Press.
Ellis, A. W. (1984). Reading, writing and dyslexia: A cognitive analysis. London: Erlbaum.
Friedman, D., Simson, R, Ritter, W., & Rapin, I. (1975). The late positive component (P300)
and information processing in sentences. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neu-
rophysiology, 38, 255-262.
Gaillard, A. W. K, & Ritter, W. (Eds.). (1983). Tutorials in ERP research: Endogenous compo-
nents. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Garnsey, S. (1985). Function words and context words: Reaction time and evoked potential
measures of word recognition. Cognitive Science Technical Report URCS-29, University
of Rochester, Rochester, New York.
Gevins, A. S. (1986). Quantitative human neurophysiology. In H. J. Hannay (Ed.), Experi-
ERPs AND ADVANCES IN NEUROLINGUISTICS 79
mental techniques in human neuropsychology (pp. 419-456). New York: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Gevins, A. S., Bressler, S. L., & Illes, J. (1988, January 27-30). Effects of incipient fatigue on
higher cortical function. Paper presented at the 16th Annual International Neuropsy-
chological Society meeting, New Orleans.
Gibson, D. (1988). The impact of early developmental history on cerebral asymmetries:
Implications for reading ability in deaf children. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz
(Eds.), Developmental implications of brain lateralization. New York: Guilford Press.
Gibson, C., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1986). The impact of visual-spatial information on the devel-
opment of reading proficiency in deaf children. In J. L. Nespoulous, P. Perron, & A. R.
Lecours (Eds.), The biological foundations of gestures: Motor and semiotic aspects (pp.
215-227). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grossman, M. (1981). A bird is a bird is a bird: Making reference within and .without
superordinate categories. Brain and Language, 12, 313-331.
Grossman, M., & Carey, S. (1978, October). Word-learning after brain damage. Paper present-
ed to the Academy of Aphasia, Chicago.
Hannay, J. H. (1986). Experimental techniques in human neuropsychology. New York: Ox-
ford University Press.
Hatta, T. (1977). Recognition of Japanese kaiji in the left and right visual fields. Neuropsy-
chologia, 15, 605-688.
Hatta, T. (1981). Differential processing of kaiji and kana stimuli in Japanese people: Some
implications from Stroop-test results. Neuropsychologia, 19, 87-93.
Henderson, L. (1984). Orthographies and readings; Perspectives from cognitive psychology.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hier, D. B., & Kaplan, J. (1980). Verbal comprehension deficits after RH damage. Applied
Psycholinguistics, 1, 279-294.
Hjorth, B. (1982). An adaptive EEG derivation technique. Electroencephalography and
Clinical Neurophysiology, 54, 654-661.
Job, R., & Sartori, G. (1988). The cognitive neuropsychology of visual and semantic process-
ing of concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5(1).
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Hermann, D. J., & Chaffin, R. (1984). Only connections: A critique of
semantic networks. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 292-315.
Katznelson, R. D. (1981). EEG recording, electrode placement, and aspects of generator
localization. In P. L. Nunez, Electrical fields of the brain. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Kramer, A. F., & Donchin, E. (1987). Brain potentials as indices of orthographiC and pho-
nological interaction during word matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology;
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 76-86.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1983). Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and
semantic anomolies. Memory and Cognition, 11, 539-550.
Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy
and semantic association. Nature, 307, 161-163.
Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. (1988). Event-related brain potential studies of language. In P. K.
Ackles, J. R. Jennings, & M. G. H. Close (Eds.), Advances in psychophysiology (139-187).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Lecours, A. R., Lhermitte, F., & Bryans, B. (1983). Aphasiology. London: Balliere Tindall.
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
Leong, C. K., Wong, S., Wong, A., & Hiscock, M. (1985). Differential cerebral involvement in
perceiving Chinese characters: Levels of processing approach. Brain and language, 26,
131-145.
Licht, R., Bakker, D. J., Kok, A., & Bouma, A. (1988). The development of lateral event-related
80 SIDNEY J. SEGALOWITZ
potentials (ERPs) related to word meaning: A four year longitudinal study. Neuropsy-
chologia, 26, 327-340.
Marshall, J. C. (1985). On some relationships between acquired and developmental dyslexia.
In F. H. Duffy & N. Geschwind (Eds.), Dyslexia: A neuroscientific approach to clinical
evaluation. Boston: Little, Brown.
Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175-199.
McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1981). A metric for thought: A comparison of P300 latency and
reaction time. Science, 211, 77-80.
Mitterer, J. O. (1982). There are at least two kinds of poor readers: Whole-word poor readers
and recoding poor readers. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 445-461.
Mocks, J. (1986). The influence of latency jitter in principal component analysis of event-
related potentials. Psychophysiology, 23, 480-484.
Mocks, J., & Verleger, R. (1986). Principal component analysis of event-related potentials: A
note on misallocation of variance. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysi-
ology, 65, 393-398.
Molfese, D. L. (1978). Left and right hemisphere involvement in speech perception: Elec-
trophysiological correlates. Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 237-243.
Molfese, D. M. (1983). Event related potentials and language processes. In A. W. K. Gaillard
& W. Ritter (Eds.), Tutorials in ERP research: Endogenous components (pp. 345-367).
Amsterdam: North Holland.
Molfese, D. M. (n.d.). Electrophysiological correlates of word meanings in 14-month-old
human infants. (Mimeo)
Molfese, D. M., & Betz, J. C. (1988). Electrophysiological indices of the early development of
lateralization for language and cognition and their implications for predicting later
development. In D. M. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Developmental implications of
brain lateralization (pp. 17-190). New York: Guilford Press.
Molfese, D., Freeman, R. B., & Palermo, D. S. (1975). The ontogeny of brain lateralization for
speech and non-speech stimuli. Brain and Language, 2, 356-368.
Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1985). Electrophysiological indices of auditory discrimina-
tion in newborn infants: The bases for predicting later language development? Infant
Behavior and Development, 8, 197-211.
Molfese, D. M., Wetzel, W. F., Linnville, S. E., Imbasciate, C., Leicht, D., Courtney, C.,
Baldwin, K., & Adams, C. A. (1985). Word recognition in 16-month-old infants: EJec-
trophysiological indices. Paper presented at the 57th annual meetings of the Mid-
western Psychological Association.
Neville, H. J., Kutas, M., & Schmidt, A. (1982a). Event-related potential studies of cerebral
specialization during reading. I. Studies of normal adults. Brain and Language, 16, 300-
315.
Neville, H. J., Kutas, M., & Schmidt, A. (1982b). Event-related potential studies of cerebral
specialization during reading. II. Studies of congenitally deaf adults. Brain and Lan-
guage, 16, 316-337.
Nunez, P. L. (1981). Electric fields of the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
Papanicolaou, A. C., Levin, H. S., & Eisenbeg, H. (1984). Evoked potential correlates of
recovery from aphasia after focal left hemisphere injury in adults. Neurosurgery, 14,
412-415.
Paradis, M. (Ed.). (1983). Readings on aphasia in bilinguals and polyglots. Montreal: Didier.
Patterson, K. E. (1982). The relation between reading and phonological coding: Further
neuropsychological observations. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cog-
nitive functions (pp. 77-111). London: Academic.
Patterson, K. E., Marshall, J. C., & Coltheart, M. (1985). Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological
and cognitive studies of phonological reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ERPs AND ADVANCES IN NEUROLINGUISTICS 81
Polich, J., McCarthy, G., Wang, W. S., & Donchin, K (1983). When words collide: Ortho-
graphic and phonological interference during word processing. Biological Psychology,
16, 155-180.
Posner, M. 1. (1986). Chronometric explorations of mind. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Sasanuma, S., Hoh, M., Kobayashi, Y., & Mori, K. (1980). The nature of the task-stimulus
interaction in the tachistoscopic recognition of kana and kanji words. Brain and Lan-
guage, 9, 298-306.
Sasanuma, S., Hoh, M., Mori, K., & Kobayashi, Y. (1977). Tachistoscopic recognition of kane
and kanji words. Neuropsychologia, 15, 547-553.
Scherg, M., & Von Cramon, D. (1986). Evoked dipole source potentials of the human auditory
cortex. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 65, 344-360.
Segalowitz, N. S. (1986). Skilled reading in the second language. In J. Vaid (Ed.), Language
processing in bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and neuropsychological perspectives. Hills-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Segalowitz, S. J. (1983). Cerebral asymmetries for speech in infancy. In S. J. Segalowitz (Ed.),
Language functions and brain organization. New York: Academic Press.
Segalowitz, S. J., & Cohen, H. (1989). Right hemisphere EEG sensitivity to speech. Brain and
Language. (in press).
Segalowitz, S. J., & MacGregor, L. (1987, February 19-21). ERP correlates of reading pho-
nologically regular and non-regular English words. Paper presented at the International
Neuropsychological Society meeting, Washington, DC.
Segalowitz, S. J., Menna, R., & MacGregor, L. (1987, July 1-4). Left and right hemispheric
participation in normal adults' reading: Evidence from ERPs. Paper presented at the
European meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Barcelona, Spain.
Taylor, 1., & Taylor, M. M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic Press.
Vaid, J. (1983). Bilingualism and brain lateralization. In S. J. Segalowitz (Ed.), Language
functions and brain organization. New York: Academic Press.
Vaid, J., & Genesee, F. (1980). Neuropsychological approaches to bilingualism: A critical
review. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 417-445.
Van Petten, C., & Kutas, M. (1987). Ambiguous words in context: An event-related potential
analysis of the time course of meaning activation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26,
188-208.
Wood, C. C. (1982). Application of dipole localization methods to source identification of
human evoked potentials. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 388, 139-155.
Wood, C. C., & McCarthy, G. (1984). Principal component analysis of event-related poten-
tials: Simulation studies demonstrate misallocation of variance across components.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 59, 249-260.
Zurif, K, & Caramazza, A. (1976). Psycho linguistic structures in aphasia: Studies in syntax
and semantics. In H. Whitaker & H. A. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol.
1). New York: Academic Press.
II
85
86 LYDA MEJIA and JORGE ESLAVA-COBOS
pointed out how these children usually had difficulties in learning certain
skills.
Ley (1930) defined two types of difficulties in children's language.
The first, which he considered a delay in the praxic functions in language,
is one in which the child can pronounce sounds and syllables but has
great difficulties in the synthesis of these elements, which nevertheless
still allow an adequate expression of language. Interestingly enough, he
described in this problem an alteration of abstract concepts, which he
explained as due to the fact that they are acquired through the spoken
word. The other clinical entity he called "hearing agnosia of evolution,"
characterized by impairment in the comprehension of words and of writ-
ten material, which he considered due to difficulties in abstract thinking.
Worster-Drought (1943, 1968) returned to his analysis of "congenital
auditory imperception" and further defined its characteristics as impos-
sibility for the comprehension of language, poor expression (frequently as
idioglossia), normal nonverbal IQ, and normal audiogram; some of these
patients have a discrete hearing loss but not sufficiently severe as to
explain their language difficulties. He defined four types of imperception,
ranging from difficulty to comprehension of words to imperception of all
sounds-a situation extremely difficult to differentiate from deafness. At
the same time, he described delays in development of spoken language
without any difficulty in comprehension, called by him "developmental
executive aphasia."
Separating cases in which expression was impaired from those that
affected comprehension, Morley (1957) described "developmental apha-
sia," distinguishing three subtypes of expression impairment: expression
aphasia of evolution, evolutive dysarthria, and evolutive apraxia of
expression.
In the early 1950s, Landau, Goldstein, and Kleffner (1954) described
autopsy findings in a child whose language development started at age 6
and by the moment of his death-4 years later-was quite functional. They
found extensive bilateral destruction of the perisylvian cortex in the area of
the central sulcus and retrograde degeneration in both medial geniculate
nuclei, and concluded that his language had developed through different
connections from the usual primary thalamocortical projections. In a com-
mentaryto this case, Lenneberg (1967) says: "I cite this case to illustrate the
enormous plasticity of the human brain (or the lack of cortical specializa-
tion) with respect to language during the first years of life" (p. 183).
In the mid-1950s and 1960s, the discussion frequently centered on
terminology. Many rejected the term aphasia, because they felt it implied
loss of something that children had not acquired, and preferred dyspha-
sia. Others, Ajuriaguerra et a1. (1969) among them, preferred the term
aphasia for the most serious symtoms and dysphasia for "a population
who presents a disturbance in the integration of language without sensory
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CEREBRAL MATURATION 87
the child until 4 years of age, when he will initiate intuitive thinking (still
in the preoperational stage). In spite of his having organized important
linguistic abilities, they are not yet sufficient to decentrate thinking,
which must then rely on perceptual clues. Reversibility is still absent,
hence preventing the child from understanding the relation of parts to the
totality, explaining conservations after a perceptual transformation, or
having categorical criteria for classifications.
The coincidence of this first linguistic level with preconceptual
thinking is responsible for the enrichment of semantic aspects of language
in this child, busy searching for different ways in which to represent the
world he is exploring through symbolic play. This latter is accompanied
by egocentric language, first expressed as repetition or echolalia, then as
monologue, and finally as collective monologue (Piaget & Inhelder, 1981).
The importance of this egocentric language cannot be sufficiently empha-
sized because it plays the role that interior language accomplishes in the
adult. "It not only escorts the activity of the child but serves his mental
orientation and conscious comprehension, and helps him in the difficul-
ties that continuously surge; it is the language for himself, intimately and
fruitfully linked with infantile thinking .... At the end it will transform
itself into interior language" (Vigotsky, 1962).
It is important to realize that the gradual enrichment of significances
contributes directly to syntactic progress. The handling of complex struc-
tures will in tum permit the expression of more abstract situations, pro-
gressively separated from purely perceptual aspects-as will be seen in
the third stage of communication. At the same time, that handling of
complex structures reveals to us a greater conceptual enrichment of the
individual. From the phonologic perspective, a gradual acquisition of
phonemes from his native language can be seen, but some with greater
difficulty than others, thus appearing later. While some explain this delay
through the need for finer articulatory movements that require greater
maturity of muscles involved (for those that appear later), others believe
that "the articulatory delay is rather due to an imperfect bond between the
acoustic and the motor image" (Alarcos Llorach, 1976)-that is, to an
instability of the kinesthetic motor verbal analyzer, whose functional ex-
ternal expressions are phonematic stereotypes.
By the end of this first linguistic level the child is in possession of all
the sounds of his native language, even though he sometimes fails in their
synthesis to form polysyllables with symphones. From the syntactic per-
spective he initiates his first complex sentences-basically of the coordi-
nated type-and has a verbal repertoire whose richness depends greatly
on his own peculiar environment. The connections and relations among
those significances from his repertoire that the child progressively makes
will allow the appearance of interior language.
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CEREBRAL MATURATION 97
Verbal Analyzer
"Simple" retardations, equivalent to the one previously described for
the kinesthetic motor verbal analyzer, are infrequent in verbal analyzer
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CEREBRAL MATURATION 101
CONCLUSIONS
The study of disorders in language acquisition in child~en is hin-
dered by the changes that maturation imposes on the clinical manifesta-
tions, thereby forcing their interpretation within a dynamic frame. They
must then be considered from the perspective of functional states-either
physiologic or pathologic-that progressively stabilize until a stable state
is reached. This evolution has its origins in neural embryogenesis, a com-
plex phenomenon, determined by both biologic and environmental fac-
tors, which probably extends itself at least until the third decade of life.
Language derangements can be grouped in two large categories: diffi-
culties in the phonologic-syntactic code or in the semantic code, depend-
ing on whether the kinesthetic motor verbal or the verbal analyzer is
involved. Conditions characterized by difficulties in the organization of
analyzers must be differentiated from those where involution is the basic
phenomenon, even though the process of stabilization of their stereotypes
is still in its initial stages. Derangements of both analyzers or associations
with difficulties in other cerebral functions can be frequently seen, lead-
ing to more complex pathologic conditions.
REFERENCES
Ajuriaguerra. J., Guignard. F .• Jaeggi, A., Kocher, F., Maquard. M .• Paunier. A .• Quinodoz. D .•
& Siotis. E. (1969). Organizacion psicologica y perturbaciones del desarrollo del
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CEREBRAL MATURATION 103
Hadden, W. (1891). On certain defects of articulation with cases ilustrating the results of
education on the oral system. Journal of Mental Sciences, 37, 96.
Horwitz, B. (1981). Neuronal plasticity: How changes in dendritic architecture can affect the
spread of postsynaptic potentials. Brain Rsearch, 224, 412-418.
Kaye, M., Mitchell, D., & Cynader, M. (1982). Depth perception, eye alignment and cortical
ocular dominance of dark reared cats. Developmental Brain Research, 2, 37-53.
Kerr, J. (1897). School hygiene in its mental, moral and physical aspects. Journal of the Royal
Statistics Society, 60, 613.
Kiessling, L., Denckla, M., & Carlton, M. (1983). Evidence for differential hemispheric func-
tion in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 25, 727-734.
Landau, W., Goldstein, R, & Kleffner, F. (1954). Congenital aphasia: A clinic pathologic
study. Neurology, 10, 915-921.
Landau, W., & Kleffner, F. (1957). Syndrome of acquired aphasia with convulsive disorder in
children. Neurology, 7, 523-530.
Launay, C. L. (1979). Graves en la adquisicion de lenguaje. In C. Launay & S. Burel-Maison-
ny, Trastornes del lenguaje la palabre y la voz en el nino (pp. 128-129). Barcelona:
Toray Masson.
Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
Levi-Montalcini, R (1964). Events in the developing nervous system. Progress in Brain
Research, 4, 1-29.
Ley, J. (1930). Les troubles du developpement du langage. Journal du Neurologie et Psychi-
atTie, 30, 78-90.
Luria, A. (1974). Cerebro y lenguaje. Barcelona: Fontanella.
Luria, A. (1979). El papel del lenguaje en el desarrollo de la conducta. Buenos Aires:
Cartago.
McLennan, 1., & Hendry, 1. (1981). Effects of peripheral deprivation on mammalian motoneu-
rone development. Developmental Brain Research, 1, 440-443.
Meisami, E., & Mousavi, R. (1982). Lasting effects of early olfactory deprivation on the
growth, DNA, RNA, and protein content, and Na- K-ATPase and ache activity of the rat
olfactory bulb. Developmental Brain Research, 2, 217-229.
Morley, M. (1957). The development and disorders of speech in childhood. Edinburgh:
Livingstone.
Mower, G., Burchfield, J., & Duffy, F. (1981). The effects of dark-rearing on the development
and plasticity of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Developmental Brain Research, 1,418-
424.
Orton, S. (1930). Familial occurrence of disorders in acquisition of language. Eugenics, 3,
140-147.
Owens, R (1984). Language development. Columbus: Merrill.
Pfaendler, V. (1960). Les vices de la parole dans l'optique du geneticien. Akt Problem der
Phoniatriks und Logopaedie, 1,35-40.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1981). Psicologia del nino. Madrid: Morata.
Popper, K. (1977). Busqueda sin termino: Una autobiografia intelectual. Madrid: Tecnos.
Purpura, D. (1976). Structure dysfunction relations in the visual cortex of preterm infants. In
C. Brazier, (Ed.), Brain dysfunction in infantile febrile convulsions (pp. 223-240). New
York: Raven Press.
Quiros, J., D'Elia, N., Feldman, J., Galian, M., Lira, E., Lujan Marelli, L., Mercado, M.,
Pericoli, B., Rincon, M., Rosental, C., Schrager, 0., Soto, R, & Tormakh, E. (1971). Las
llamadas afasias infantiles. Buenos Aires: C.E.M.1.F.A.
Quiros, J., & Della Cella, M. (1971). La dislexia en la ninez. Buenos Aires: Paidos.
Rabinowicz, T. (1976). Morphological features of the developing brain. In C. Brazier (Ed.),
Brain dysfunction in infantile febrile convulsions (pp. 1-23). New York: Raven Press.
LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND CEREBRAL MATURATION 105
Sidman, R, & Rakic, P. (1973). Neuronal migration with special reference to developing
human brain: A review. Brain Research, 62, 1.
Siguan Soler, M. (1978). De la comunicacion gestual al lenguaje verbal. In J. Bronckart, P.
Malrieu, M. Siguan Soler, H. Sinclair, T. Slama-Cazacu, & A. Tabouret La genesis del
lenguaje (pp. 23-50). Madrid: Pablo del Rio.
Swaimann, D., & Wright, F. (1982). The practice of pediatric neurology. St. Louis: C. V.
Mosby.
Ucherman, V. (1891). The case of dumbness (aphasia) without deafness, paralysis or mental
debility. Archives of Otolaringology, 20, 321.
Vigotsky, 1. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Volpe, J. (1977). Neurologia neonatal. Clinicas de Perinatologia, 4, 3-30.
Worster-Drought, C. (1943). Congenital auditory imperception (congenital word deafness)
and its relation to idioglossia and allied speech defects. Medical Press and Circu~ar, 210,
5460.
Worster-Drought, C. (1968). Speech disorders in children. Developmental Medicine and
Child Neurology, 10, 427-440.
Worster-Drought, C., & Allen, L (1929). Congenital auditory imperception. Journal of Neu-
rology and Psycopathology, 10, 193.
Wyllie, J. (1894). The disorders of speech. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.
Yakovlev, P., & Lecours, A. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the
brain. In Minkowski, A. (Ed.) Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 3-70).
Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
6
JEFFREY L. CUMMINGS and D. FRANK BENSON' The Neurobehavior Unit, West Los
Angeles Veterans Administration Medical Center, Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry
and Behavioral Sciences, UCLA School of Medicine, Los Angeles, California 90024.
107
108 JEFFREY L. CUMMINGS and D. FRANK BENSON
METHODOLOGY
Assessment
The battery of speech and language tests used in these studies was
derived from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass &
Kaplan, 1976) and the Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1979). The battery
included 37 operationalized subtests and assessed elements of spon-
taneous speech, auditory comprehension, repetition, naming, paraphasia,
reading aloud, reading comprehension, automatic speech, sentence com-
pletion, word list generation (number of animals named in 1 minute),
writing, dysarthria, and reiterative speech disturbances. Each subtest re-
ceived a scale score of 0 (normal) to 6 (most abnormal) based on responses
to six (or a multiple of six) scorable questions (e.g., "Is your name Green?")
or graded by operationalized anchor points. Table 1 lists the elements of the
battery. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, &
McHugh, 1975) was used to determine the severity of the dementia syn-
SPEECH AND LANGUAGE ALTERATIONS IN DEMENTIA SYNDROMES 109
dromes. BMDP statistical software (Dixon, Brown, Engleman, & Hill, 1985)
was used for all statistical analyses; the Bonferroni adjustment for multiple
testing has been applied to all results reported as significant.
Participants
All participants had at least an eighth-grade education and spoke
English as their native language. Seventy normal control subjects were
tested and all had MMSE scores in the normal range.
All dementia patients were thoroughly evaluated to establish an ac-
curate clinical diagnosis. Each had a thorough historical review, physical
examination, neurologic examination, and mental status testing. Standard
laboratory studies, including complete blood count, erythrocyte sedimen-
tation rate, serum electrolytes, serum calcium and phosphorus, serum
vitamin B12 and folate levels, thyroid and liver function tests, and a
serological test for syphilis, were obtained and determined to be normal.
An electroencephalogram and computerized tomographic (CT) scan of the
110 JEFFREY 1. CUMMINGS and D. FRANK BENSON
RESULTS
Sample characteristics
Age (mean with SEM) 71.33 (1.64)
MMSE 10.50 (1.3)
N 30
Score
Most impaired features (scale score> 3/6)
Narrative writing 4.63 (0.39)
Information content of speech 3.83 (0.29)
Word list generation (animals/min) 3.37 (0.59)
Nursery rhyme completion 3.17 (0.42)
Comprehension of complex commands 3.09 (0.43)
Naming 3.03 (0.34)
Least impaired abilities (scale score < 2/6)
Reading sentences aloud 1.97 (0.47)
Phrase repetition 1.87 (0.40)
Sentence completion 1.83 (0.46)
Phrase length 1.80 (0.38)
Grammatical complexity 1.53 (0.38)
Reading words aloud 1.47 (0.45)
Melodic line 1.43 (0.34)
Verbal paraphasia 1.43 (0.32)
Neologistic paraphasia 1.40 (4.40)
Echolalia 1.33 (0.25)
Loudness 1.28 (0.29)
Intelligibility 1.17 (0.27)
Literal paraphasia 1.10 (0.26)
Counting 1.07 (0.38)
Articulation 1.07 (0.24)
Pitch 1.00 (0.23)
Number repetition 0.97 (0.38)
Rate 0.90 (0.24)
Word repetition 0.80 (0.32)
Palilalia 0.70 (0.27)
Logoclonia 0.70 (0.31)
Stuttering 0.33 (0.14)
tinguished the patient from the control population. After Bonferroni ad-
justment for multiple testing, six subtests no longer differed from the
control group: word and number repetition, reading words aloud, stutter-
ing, palilalia, and logoclonia. The-most marked disturbances (scale scores
> 3/6) involved information content of spontaneous speech, comprehen-
sion of complex commands, naming, word list generation, writing to dic-
tation, narrative writing, and completion of nursery rhymes (Table 2).
Relatively preserved abilities (scale scores of < 2/6) included counting,
sentence completion, phrase length, grammatical complexity, melodic
line, paraphasias, oral reading, repetition, dysarthria measures, and re-
112 JEFFREY L. CUMMINGS and D. FRAl'\)K BENSON
Parkinson's Disease
There was no difference between the DAT and PD patient groups in
age or dementia severity (Table 3). DAT and PD could be distinguished by
SPEECH AND LANGUAGE ALTERATIONS IN DEMENTIA SYNDROMES 113
Bonferroni
PD score DAT score significance
(SEM) (SEM) level
Sample characteristics
N 16 10
Age 71.81 (0.96) 73.20 (3.30) n.s.
MMSE 20.25 (0.78) 17.90 (0.84) n.s.
Distinguishing features
Phrase length 2.31 (0.31) 0.40 (0.10) .001
Information content 1.18 (0.24) 2.80 (0.24) .001
Melody 2.56 (0.15) 0.40 (0.16) .001
Writing mechanics 3.62 (0.37) 0.60 (0.40J .001
Naming 0.43 (0.20) 2.10 (0.54) .05
Grammatical complexity 1.06 (0.28) 0.20 (0.13) .05
Animal naming (total/min) 10.02 (0.91) 5.50 (1.19) .01
Loudness 3.81 (0.29) 0.50 (0.22) .001
Pitch 2.18 (0.31) 0.20 (0.13) .001
Articulation 2.75 (0.32) 0.50 (0.22) .001
Rate 2.81 (0.27) 0.30 (0.15) .001
Intelligibility 2.62 (0.28) 0.30 (0.15) .001
both speech and language parameters. All five dysarthria measures, writ-
ing mechanics, and speech melody were all significantly more abnormal
in PD. Phrase length and grammatical complexity were also more dis-
turbed in PD than in DAT. Linguistic compromise, on the other hand, was
more evident in DAT patients, who had significantly poorer scores on
information content of spontaneous speech, confrontation naming, and
word list generation.
Multi-Infarct Dementia
MID and DAT patient groups did not differ significantly in age or
dementia severity (Table 4). Two language and four speech parameters
were significantly different in the two groups. Information content of
spontaneous speech and confrontation naming were more abnormal in
DAT, whereas speech pitch, melody, articulation, and rate were more
disturbed in MID.
The most abnormal language parameters in MID (scale score> 1.5/6)
were narrative writing, writing to dictation, writing mechanics, comple-
tion of nursery rhymes, and comprehension of complex auditory com-
mands.
MID patients were also compared with PD patients with overt demen-
tia. The two groups were not significantly different in age or MMSE scores
114 JEFFREY L. CUMMINGS and D. FRANK BENSON
Bonferroni
MID score DAT score significance
(SEM) (SEM) level
Patient characteristics
N 18 14
Age 67.38 (2.45) 73.14 (2.61) n.s.
MMSE 15.71 (1.31) 18.77 (0.95) n.s.
Distinguishing features
Information content 0.72 (0.25) 2.92 (0.24) .001
Naming 0.77 (0.27) 2.28 (0.45) .05
Pitch 1.83 (0.39) 0.35 (0.16) .01
Melody 2.67 (0.45) 0.57 (0.17) .05
Articulation 1.88 (0.40) 0.50 (0.17) .05
Rate 1.72 (0.27) 0.28 (0.12) .05
Bonferroni
MID score PD score significance
(SEM) (SEM) level
Patient characteristics
N 18 17
Age 67.38 (2.95) 71.94 (0.91) n.s.
MMSE 18.77 (0.95) 19.70 (0.91) n.s.
Distinguishing features
Phrase length 1.05 (0.36) 2.29 (0.29) .05
Nursery rhyme completion 2.44 (0.47) 0.81 (0.39) .05
Loudness 1.77 (0.47) 3.64 (0.32) .01
Rate 1.22 (0.23) 2.76 (0.26) .001
Stuttering 0.00 (0.00) 0.70 (0.25) .05
Writing mechanics 1.83 (0.44) 3.70 (0.36) .01
SPEECH AND LANGUAGE ALTERATIONS IN DEMENTIA SYNDROMES 115
DISCUSSION
CONCLUSIONS
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Ph.D., for her aid with the statistical analyses, and Bonita Porch and
Wendy Kashitani for preparing the manuscript.
REFERENCES
Adams, R. D., & Victor, M. (1977). Principles of neurology. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Albert, M. L. (1978). Subcortical dementia. In R. Katzman, R. D. Terry, & K. L. Bick (Eds.),
Alzheimer's disease: Senile dementia and related disorders (pp. 173-180). New York:
Raven Press.
Alexander, M. P., LoVerme, & S. R., Jr. (1980). Aphasia after left hemisphere intracerebral
hemorrhage. Neurology, 35, 1193-1202.
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis-
orders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Appell, J., Kertesz, A., & Fisman, M. (1982). A study of language functioning in Alzheimer
patients. Brain and Language, 17,73-91.
Arendt, T., Bigl. V., Arendt, A., & Tennstedt, A. (1983). Loss of neurons in the nucleus
basalis of Meynert in Alzheimer's disease, paralysis agitans, and Korsakoff's disease.
Acta Neuropathologica, 61, 101-108.
Bayles, K. A., & Tomoeda, C. K. (1983). Confrontation naming impairment in dementia.
Brain and Language, 19, 98-114.
Benson, D. F. (1978). Amnesia. Southern Medical Journal, 71, 1221-1228.
Benson, D. F. (1979). Aphasia, alexia, and agraphia. New York: Churchill Livingstone.
Benson, D. F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). The aphasias and related disturbances. In A. B. Baker
& R. J. Joynt (Eds.), Clinical neurology (Vol. 1). Philadelphia: Harper & Row.
Boller, F., Mizutani, T., Roessmann, U., & Gambetti, P. (1980). Parkinson disease, dementia,
and Alzheimer disease: Clinicopathological correlations. Annals of Neurology, 7, 329-
335.
Brun, A., & Gustafson, L. (1978). Limbic lobe involvement in presenile dementia. Arch
Psychiatr Nervenkr, 226, 79-93.
Brunner, R. J., Kornhuber, H. H., Seemuller, E., Sugar, G., & Wellesch, C. W. (1982). Basal
ganglia participation in language pathology. Brain and Language, 16, 281-299.
Caine, E. D., Ebert, M. H., & Weingartner, H. (1977). An outline for the analysis of dementia.
The memory disorder of Huntington's disease. Neurology, 27, 1087-1092.
Cummings, J. L. (1986). Subcortical dementia: Neuropsychology, neuropsychiatry, and pa-
thophysiology. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 682-697.
Cummings, J. L., & Benson, D. F. (1983). Dementia: A clinical approach. Boston: Butter-
worths.
Cummings, J. L., & Benson, D. F. (1986). Dementia of the Alzheimer type. An inventory of
diagnostic clinical features. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 34, 12-19.
Cummings, J. L., & Benson, D. F. (1984). Subcortical dementia: Review of an emerging
concept. Archives of Neurology, 41, 874-879.
Cummings, J. L., Benson, D. F., Hill, M. A., & Read, S. (1985). Aphasia in dementia of the
Alzheimer type. Neurology, 35, 394-397.
Cummings, J. L., Houlihan, J. P., & Hill, M. A. (1986). The pattern of reading deterioration in
dementia of the Alzheimer type: Observation and implications. Brain and Language, 19,
315-323.
Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., Rizzo, M., Varney, M., & Gersh, F. (1982). Aphasia with
nonhemorrhagic lesions of the basal ganglia and internal capsule. Archives of Neu-
rology, 39, 15-20.
120 JEFFREY 1. CUMMINGS and D. FRANK BENSON
Damasio, A. R., Graff-Radford, N. R, Eslinger, P. J., Damasio, H., & Kassell, N. (1985).
Amnesia following basal forebrain lesions. Archives of Neurology, 42, 263-271.
Dixon, W. J., Brown, M. B., Engleman, 1., & Hill, M. A. (1985). BMDP statistical software
(2nd ed.). Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Emery, O. B., & Emery, P. E. (1983). Language in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type.
Psychiatry Journal of the University of Ottawa, 8, 169-178.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R (1975). Mini-mental state. Journal of Psychi-
atric Research, 12, 189-198.
Freedman, M., Rivoira, P., Butters, N., Sax, D. S., & Feldman, R G. (1984). Retrograde
amnesia in Parkinson's disease. Canadian Journal of Neurological Science, 11, 297-
301.
Gaspar, P., & Gray, F. (1984). Dementia in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Acta Neu-
ropathologica, 64, 43-52.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1976). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders.
Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.
Hachinski, V. C., Iliff, 1. D., Zilkha, E., Du Boulay, G. H., McAllister, V. 1., Marshall, J.,
Russell, R W. R, & Symon, L. (1975). Cerebral blood flow in dementia. Archives of
Neurology, 32, 632-637.
Hachinski, V. C., Lassen, N. A., & Marshall, J. (1974). Multi-infarct dementia. Lancet, 2, 207-
210.
Hakim, A. M., & Mathieson, G. (1979). Dementia in Parkinson disease: A neuropathologic
study. Neurology, 29, 1209-1214.
Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, K., & Shindler, A. G. (1985). Language disintegration in dementia:
Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language, 25, 117-133.
Huber, S. J., Shuttleworth, E. c., & Paulson, G. W. (1986). Dementia in Parkinson's disease.
Archives of Neurology, 43, 987-990.
Hughes, W., Dodgson, M. C. H., & MacLennan, D. C. (1954). Chronic cerebral hypertensive
disease. Lancet, 2, 770-774.
Kertesz, A. (1979). Aphasia and associated disorders: Taxonomy, localization, and recov-
ery. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Landis, T., Graves, R, Benson, D. F., & Hebben, N. (1982). Visual recognition through
kinaesthetic mediation. Psychological Medicine, 12, 4515-531.
Martin, A., & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer's disease:
The breakdown of semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19, 124-141.
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R, Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984).
Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group
under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alz-
heimer's Disease. Neurology, 34, 939-944.
Nelson, H. E., O'Connell, A. (1978). Dementia: The estimation of premorbid intelligence
using the new Adult Reading Test. Cortex, 14, 234-244.
Roman, G. C. (1985). Lacunar dementia. In J. T. Hutton & A. D. Kenny (Eds.), Senile dementia
of the Alzheimer type (pp. 131-151). New York: Alan R Liss.
Whitehouse, P. J., Hedreen, J. c., White, C. 1., III, & Price, D. 1. (1983). Basal forebrain
neurons in the dementia of Parkinson disease. Annals of Neurology, 13, 243-248.
Wilson, R S., Kaszniak, A. W., & Fox, J. H. (1981). Remote memory in senile dementia.
Cortex, 17,41-48.
III
This chapter treats the various "mechanisms" that may be said to under-
lie aphasic transformations. I have quoted the word mechanisms for a
reason, since that term implies some sort of device (actually, another
metaphor) or internal agent that does things. Whatever these mechanisms
are in their neurophysiological instantiations, they must be understood
psychologically as manipulating some types of elements in some domain
of operation. Without first attempting to characterize what the mecha-
nisms might be at a psycholinguistic level of explanation and what they
might do, we will not be in any position to begin to characterize aphasic
transformations. That is, in order to start I must layout a production
model of some sort that can justifiably be said to form part of the cognitive
system for language-a model constructed from evidence external to the
data from aphasia.
The justification for the type of model I intend to use comes from
modern studies in the philosophy of psychology (e.g., Fodor, 1983),
where the claim (p. 29} is made that "contemporary cognitive theory takes
it for granted that the paradigmatic psychological process is a sequence of
transformations of mental representations and that the paradigmatic cog-
nitive system is one which effects such transformations." Like Fodor, I
will assume for the purposes of the present chapter that if I am going to
speak of psychological mechanisms, then I must assume that these mech-
anisms will be computational systems of one sort or another. Concurring
further with Fodor (1982, p. 279), I will assume that these computational
processes are both symbolic and formal. The processes are termed sym-
123
124 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
bolic because they are defined over (operate upon) representations that
consist of symbolic elements. The processes are termed formal because
they apply to the various representations in virtue of the configuration or
syntax of those representations. The formal operations apply, as it were,
to various types of symbols without "understanding" anything about
what those symbols mayor may not mean. Or, to put it another way, the
operations manipulate the symbols in terms of their shapes, not their
meanings.
Once we spell out the levels of representation and the various types
of computations that map one level onto another, we will be in a position
to consider what types of derailments of the computations could occur to
account for the production of aphasic transformations.
Over the past 10 years or so, Merrill Garrett (1975, 1976, 1980) has
been developing a model for sentence production based on data he and
others have collected from the slips of the tongue of normal, non-brain-
damaged subjects. More recently Garrett (1982, 1984) has shown how his
model may be used to characterize aphasic syndromes. Schwartz (1987)
and Buckingham (1980, 1985, 1986, 1987) have incorporated and ex-
tended Garrett's model in explaining their aphasic data. Garrett's publica-
tions (1982, 1984), however, have ensured that his model will playa
major role in psycho linguistic accounts of the aphasias for some time to
come. That there is, indeed, a clear relation between slips of the tongue
and many types of aphasic errors has been indicated in Cutler and Fay's
(1978) introduction to the reissue of the landmark study of Meringer and
Mayer (1895) and in Buckingham (1980). It is therefore not without prece-
dent that I utilize a model such as Garrett's to characterize the computa-
tional and representational breakdowns seen in aphasic language.
The overall model of Garrett (Garrett, 1984, p. 174) consists of in-
ferential processes that map conceptual structures onto a message level
representation, which is viewed by Garrett as a real-time construct that
determines all subsequent sentence level production. It is in this sense
that all subsequent mental processes are "executive driven" (Fodor, 1986,
p. 81). At these higher levels, which, parenthetically, have not been ar-
rived at through data from slips of the tongue but rather through philo-
sophical argument (e.g., Searle, 1983), intentionality is the key, and since
"executive control" is established at the message level, it may be said that
"intentionality is the key to the mental" (Fodor, 1986, p. 81). Again, the
message level is based upon the speaker's current perceptual and affective
states of mind and on his general knowledge of how the world is. So-
called scripts and plans as well as other schemata are crucial in the me-
APHASIC TRANSFORMATIONS 125
morial structures pertaining to knowledge of the world, and are the sine
qua non for characterizing message level representations. In addition,
representations of the message level must be composed of a basic sym-
bology of simple concepts and some sort of syntax. That the representa-
tions are composed of symbols in an arrangement follows from the re-
quirement that computational processes be both symbolic and formal. It is
also important to emphasize that there is a syntax at every level of
representation.
Mapping from the message level to the functional level in Garrett's
model (e.g., Garrett, 1984, p. 176) is realized through what he calls logical
and syntactic processes. There are three principal computations here.
First, there is a determination of functional level structures. Although
they are not the same, as Garrett points out, these functional level struc-
tures are strikingly similar to the "deep case" structures of Fillmore
(1968), and they are couched in f(x) notation consisting of atomic predi-
cates and arguments. The second computation is that of a meaning-based
lexical identification. The computations that operate here access the lex-
icon solely in terms of meaning,l but a "linking address is set up here that
establishes an association with the shape of the word in question. The
shape here, however, may be very abstract (see Caplan, 1987, pp. 246-
247). Moreover, it should be pointed out that these computations are very
fast and may initially access all the meanings of some word or perhaps
initially all meaning-related words. The meaning "sphere" or semantic
field may be what is pointed to, and initially the linking addresses of all
members of the sphere apparently present themselves in a rapid, and
almost reflexlike, fashion (e.g., Swinney, 1982). Derailments here would
ultimately cause incorrect selections of the shapes of words that were
nevertheless in the semantic sphere of the target, and consequently it is
within these computations that one would locate the mechanism of se-
mantic paraphasia. Meaning-related, lexical slips of the tongue are lo-
cated precisely here by Garrett. The third computation in the message to
functional mapping is the assignment of the semantically selected lexical
items to functional structures. The Functional Level structure is thereby
computed and the representation is a syntactic one but, again, a very
IGiven Fodor's stipulation that computations manipulate symbols only in terms of their
shapes, we may question the status of the operations involved in the first, or meaning-
based, lexical lookup at the functional level. Is there any essential computational difference
between "appreciating" the meaning of a word and "selecting" that word from the mental
lexicon on the basis of its phonological shape? It is certainly logical that a word can be
"selected" on the basis of its meaning, but how is this carried off as a computation if that
selection does not involve symbol manipulation. "Symbol" implies form as well as mean-
ing. So, it would appear that even at the functional level where the selection processes do
not have access to the phonological shape of a word, those processes do have access to the
word in terms of its semantic symbolic form. Phonological form is connected to the first
lexical lookup computations through the "linking address."
126 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
actual motor commands. The computations that run off of the phonetic
level take into consideration all other nonlinguistic contextual factors of
articulation that exist in each speaking situation, factors such as tempo-
rary obstructions in speech cavities (such as bite blocks and chewing
gum), attentional states, and other factors that do not playa role in the
underlying linguistic system but which must be accounted for on each
speaking occasion, since the information is nevertheless relevant for
motor production.
For the purposes of this chapter, however, we need only focus upon
the functional and positional levels of representations and the computa-
tions that construct them. I will now turn to the various additions to the
Garrett model that I feel are necessary for a more complete picture of the
computations between the functional and positional levels of that model.
structure of the syllable. The model I incorporate goes back to the work of
Pike and Pike (1947) and is further discussed in Bell and Hooper (1978).
To begin with, the syllable bifurcates into an "onset" and a "core," as
illustrated in Figure 1. The onset consists of any initial consonants of the
syllable. The core bifurcates into a "peak" and a "coda." The peak is the
syllabic element that may carry stress, and is thus usually the slot for the
vowels, although certain of the sonorant consonants in English may func-
tion as syllabics. The core consists of any terminal consonants assigned to
the syllable. Like the onset, the core may be empty. The only unit of the
syllable that must be filled is, of course, the peak. On this view, the syntax
of syllables is essentially binary. Coda consonants are more strongly glued
to the peak than are onset consonants, and there is much data to support
this (e.g., Shattuck-Hufnagel, 1987). We can now return to the scan copier.
It scans the segmental elements of the phonological forms in the
buffer by syllable position. That is, it scans onsets and cores, but within
the core there is some evidence that the scanner is sensitive to whether it
is manipulating a peak item or a coda. Slips of the tongue have instructed
us here. The literature on them is replete with examples where the slip
involves the movement of a segment to some other location in another
syllable. However, it has been noted that in practically every case, the
segment that switches syllables ends up in the same position as it oc-
cupied in its original syllable. That is, onsets move to onset positions,
S (yllable)
o (nset) C (ore)
c
~
012
c c .......... c
n
~
P (eak) C (ada)
(+ syllabic)
(+ stress) ~
C C C ...... C
o 1 2 n
peaks go only to positions for peaks, and codas move to coda positions.
The only recent finding is that ambisyllabic consonants have a greater
range of choice as to where they may end up, since by their very nature
they are ambivalent as to whether they are the coda of one syllable or the
onset of another (Buckingham, 1980; Stemberger, 1982). Consonants that
are unambiguously assigned to a syllable are called "tautosyllabic." The
scanner, then, scans and copies segments by syllable position. If it mis-
scans, then some onset that occurs later on in the form will be copied in
an earlier onset position. At times there will be an exchange of segments,
and at other times either the original segment will be deleted by the
checkoff monitor or it will remain in its original position-the checkoff
monitor failing to check the misscanned segment off from its original slot.
In this latter case, the so-called doublet error will be the result (see
Lecours & Lhermitte, 1969). In the end, all of the processes suggested by
Shattuck-Hufnagel will have to interact with the computations suggested
by Garrett at the positional level, since phonological shape and utterance
order are crucial at this point in the production process.
The next augmentation to the positional level processes in the Garrett
model is the so-called random generator suggested by Butterworth (1979).
Butterworth suggested this device on the basis of data from neologistic
jargonaphasia. That is, there appeared to be a need for getting some sort of
phonological material into the buffer in those cases where it seemed that
the speaker could not retrieve target words on the basis of their form. The
neologisms Butterworth analyzed consisted of phoneme aggregates that
did not follow the normal distributional characteristics of words in En-
glish. In fact, his statistical analysis showed them to be randomly dis-
tributed, not in terms of phonotactic arrangement but in terms of expec-
tancy. He therefore posited a "random generator" that would go into
operation at the point of blocked access to phonological shapes. Again, by
"random" Butterworth did not mean haphazard ordering of phonemes
but rather that the frequency-based expectancies for phoneme use were
not followed in the construction of the neologisms. The ability assumed
by this random generator is not so strange and should not be taken to be
some recondite capacity that results from brain damage (Buckingham,
1987). Merrill Garrett (1982, p. 46) discusses Butterworth's proposal in
the following manner: "Butterworth hypothesizes that the neologistic
forms are generated by jargon aphasics in response to a word retrieval
difficulty-rather than halt, the speaker generates a phonetically accept-
able slot filler using the phonological and morphological structure build-
ing systems normally at one's disposal."
In a footnote supporting the assumption that this random generator is
not some de novo creation by the brain lesion but rather part of the normal
cognitive system, Garrett (1982, p. 72) writes: "Any speaker can generate
nonsense forms on request, and everyone on occasion produces nonce
APHASIC TRANSFORMATIONS 131
LEXICAL SUBSTITUTIONS
~EANING
LEXICON
FORM
SCAN COPIER
} ~SITIONAL ::YEL REP.
RANDOM
GENERATOR
PRODUCTIVE ORDER
SYLLABIC SLOTS I PHONETIC LEVEL REPRESEN)
FIGURE 2. An augmented schematic diagram of the Garrett model for sentence production
demonstrating the additions of the scan copier, the checkoff monitor, the random generator,
the working memory buffer for the serial ordering of segments, and the productive-order
syllabic slots, The diagram highlights the fact that these processes enter into the computa-
tions that operate between the functional level and the positional level, and as such they
feed into the positional leveL (From Buckingham, 1987, p, 382.)
132 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
the production process, which is to say that they occur in different com-
putational domains in a model such as Garrett's.
Meaning-based lexical errors take place owing to derailments of the
functional level computations of meaning-based selection (or apprecia-
tion). These so-called semantic paraphasias have been catalogued and
described in numerous publications, such as Buckingham (1981), Buck-
ingham and Rekart (1979), and Rinnert and Whitaker (1973). Recently,
disturbances of lexical semantic representations have been summarized
in Caplan (1987, chap. 12). The semantically related substitutive errors all
indicate that the subject nevertheless correctly arrives at the semantic
sphere of the word sought, so, in a sense, there really is no semahtic field
disturbance-at the level of the sphere itself. These spheres are many and
are discussed fully in Buckingham and Rekart (1979), in Rinnert and
Whitaker (1973), and in the references found in those works. For these
kinds of lexical substitutions, the linking address serves only to hold onto
the semantic field of the target word, to ensure that at the point of form-
based retrieval, the phonological shape found will be from the necessary
semantic area. Form-based retrieval need not be disrupted, and in fact
cannot be, if there is to be a semantic substitute for the target, since in an
obvious sense the error must nevertheless be accessed in terms of its
shape. Consequently, there may be a disruption with the linking address.
Anomias that are characterized by numerous semantic substitutions are
quite different in nature from anomias where no forms are accessed what-
soever. In addition, the semantic paraphasia does not necessarily take into
account phonological structure, since a typical error would be to sub-
stitute Thanksgiving for Easter, the two forms having little in common in
terms of their shapes. Caplan (1987, p. 212), however, observes that there
is often formal similarity between target words and their semantically
related substitutes. This need not be the case, however.
The network of connections in the lexicon is a web-shaped structure
that stretches in the horizontal as well as in the vertical plane. There are
vertical (paradigmatic, hyponymic relations as well as horizontal (syntag-
matic) modificationallattributive relations. These are semantic relations;
but there are form relations as well, since in some obvious sense words
with similar phonological makeup must be linked (see Fay & Cutler,
1977). And this in turn allows for the form-based selection errors. Form-
based selection derailments, in the Garrett scheme, take place at the posi-
tionallevel, well after meaning-based selection has been computed. This
does not mean, of course, that it would not be possible in some instances
to claim that some failure to appreciate semantic connectiveness could
give rise to form-based selection difficulties. It only means that form-
based selection disturbances can be dissociated from meaning-based ap-
preciation of words. There is ample evidence of this type of dissociation
(e.g., Buckingham, 1985).
APHASIC TRANSFORMATIONS 133
LEXICAL BLENDS
PHONEMIC SUBSTITUTION
unit that differs from the original by that one feature. In that event, the
substitution is really simply the reversal of some feature + or - marking.
Shattuck-Hufnagel and Klatt (1979) have argued that, at least for slips of
the tongue, accounting for phonemic substitutions in terms of distinctive
feature switches buys one very little, and that the proper way of viewing
these kinds of transformations is in terms of whole phonemic units sub-
stituting for whole phonemic units. In any event, usually only phonemes
that are similar in terms of their distinctive feature makeup substitute for
one another. Practically every investigator of phonemic substitutions has
pointed this out (e.g., Blumstein, 1973). The ambiguity in analysis (feature
vs. whole phoneme unit) is still a serious problem for the substitution of
phonemes that differ minimally in their feature array; however, it repre-
sents less of a problem for the substitution where the two phonemes
involved differ in several features, which is apparently the case for many
of the so-called conduction aphasics (see Caplan, 1987, p. 220). For sub-
stitutions involving two or three features, it would seem more likely that
the transformation involves whole phonemic units. The issue, however, is
still unresolved, and in many instances we simply cannot be sure whether
the substitution involves whole segments or switches in feature markings.
We now ask ourselves where in the production process could
phonemic substitutions take place. In terms of the combined model pre-
sented in this chapter, errors in the selection of unit phonemes would
appear minimally to occur somewhere during the manipulation of the
phonological structure of words and the copying of representative forms
onto levels more in line with articulatory production. It must be admitted,
however, that the investigator who proposed the scan-copier model of
phonological production (Le., Shattuck-Hufnagel) did not observe many
exclusively paradigmatic phonemic substitutions in her slip of the tongue
data and, accordingly, did not set up her model to readily account for
them. In fact, Shattuck-Hufnagel's findings lead her to the conclusion that
few if any strict phonemic substitutions occur-that is, phonemic sub-
stitutions that do not implicate linear ordering disruptions. Be that as it
may, the scanner could on occasion somehow select a closely related
segment (or perhaps not so closely related) and copy it onto the productive
level. Just how this may come about is not well defined from the analyses
provided by Shattuck-Hufnagel, since, again, she does not think that pure
phonemic substitutions account for much of her error data. This finding
from slip data is rather puzzling, given the fact that one of the most
detailed studies of phonemic paraphasias to date (Blumstein, 1973) as-
sumed that most of the phonemic paraphasias across syndromes (Broca,
Conduction, and Wernicke) were, in fact, phonemic substitutions.
To get around the puzzling "no-source" (purely paradigmatic)
phonemic substitution, Shattuck-Hufnagel (1979, pp. 317-318) offers sev-
eral suggestions. First, it may be the case that the source of the substitut-
136 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
ing phoneme was in part of the utterance not recorded or written down by
the investigator. Here we must recall that Shattuck-Hufnagel is talking
about data collection for slips of the tongue, not aphasia. Consequently,
this suggestion is bound to the research methodology. Most investigators,
given the very nature of the data they are collecting, do not accumulate
slip of the tongue samples through lengthy tape recordings, but rather in
one setting or another they overhear a slip and quickly try to write it down
with paper and pencil. They may, of course, fail to write down enough of
the utterance that surrounded the slip to spot a possible linear source of a
slip recorded as a pure phonemic substitution originally. For this reason,
Shattuck-Hufnagel's first alternative is a real possibility for slips of the
tongue. The aphasia researcher, on the other hand, is more likely to at
least audiotape-record the speech output of the patient. That being the
case, a fairly distant linear source for a phonemic substitution may be
somewhat easier to ascertain.
Second, it may happen that the information specifying a target seg-
ment is incompletely or incorrectly copied onto its syllabic slot by the
scanning mechanism. How this might occur is left undescribed by Shat-
tuck-Hufnagel. This account may simply reduce to the claim that the
phonemic units that get to the buffer are underspecified in their featural
makeup, and the error occurs during the computations that fill in the rest
of the feature matrix, as suggested by Beland and Nespoulous (1985).
A third possibility suggested by Shattuck-Hufnagel is that some com-
ponent of a segment (Le., a feature) in an analogous syllable slot down line
interferes with (contaminates) the segmental array currently being copied.
Admittedly, though, Shattuck-Hufnagel pays little attention to the feature
level of explanation for her error corpus.
A fourth alternative account considered by Shattuck-Hufnagel is a
more intriguing one. It is that the substituting segment's source is in a
word that somehow mistakenly presents itself as a competing or alter-
native element into the set of ongoing planning units. Competing plan
errors are discussed in detail in Baars (1980). This type of lexical account
for phonemic substitution errors is also considered by MacNeilage,
Hutchinson, and Lasater (1981).
Although I cannot go into much detail in this chapter, an additional
possible confounding factor involved in the observation of so many sup-
posedly nonsource phonemic substitutions could be that subtle artic-
ulatory asynchronies, which are not generally perceived without tech-
nical measurement, give rise to shifts in the acoustic spectrum. These
acoustic shifts may in some cases be affecting crucial perceptual cues for
the hearers. The altered acoustic cue may be such that the perceiver
"hears" another phonological unit. In that case, the hearer would make
the claim that the speaker substituted the phonemic unit that was actually
APHASIC TRANSFORMA nONS 137
constructed in the perceiver's own mind. This process has been referred
to as "phonemic false evaluation," and the clinical and theoretical reper-
cussions of it have been discussed in Buckingham and Yule (1987). Suf-
fice it to say that, consequently, in many cases, what have been tagged as
phonemic substitutions may in reality be substitutions in the minds of
hearers only-the speaker having selected the correct target phoneme
only to produce it with some sort of articulatory aberration, thereby shift-
ing the acoustic quality such that the hearer constructs another phoneme.
In this case, it would be incorrect to claim that the speaker selected the
wrong segment, and thus it would not be a case of phonemic substitution.
To the extent that Shattuck-Hufnagel's alternative accounts have some
feasibility and in fact could represent what is actually going on, then
those processes, together with presumed instances of phonemic false
evaluation, may be robust enough to rule out pure phonemic substitutions
altogether, for aphasia and for slips of the tongue.
PHONEMIC TRANSPOSITION
disappearing from that slot a permissible sequence still obtains, the antic-
ipated segment may delete from its original position in a word. Buck-
ingham (1987, 1989) details the constraints involved in whether doublets
are produced or not. We will briefly consider the nature of these con-
straints later on in this chapter.
Now it should be clear that the above transformations would most
likely occur as derailments with the scan copier and checkoff mecha-
nisms postulated earlier in this chapter. The scanner would erroneously
scan ahead a syllable or two down line and copy, for example, some
syllable onset too early. That onset could then be added to the stock of
segments of the word by filling an empty slot (see Beland & Nespoulous,
1985) or it could substitute for the original consonant onset. Moreover, the
anticipating segment mayor may not be deleted from its original position
by the checkoff monitor. That is, if there is a disruption of the checkoff
monitor, it will fail and the anticipated item will not be checked off. So, in
a sense, the checkoff monitor's operation appears to be constrained by
principles of syllable structure (again, see Buckingham, 1987, 1989, for
more details).
Virtually everything that has been said about anticipatory transposi-
tions can be said of perserverative transpositions, except in this case the
process works from left to right, whereas the anticipatory shifts went from
right to left. Perseverated segments may be added to the total stock of
segments of some word or they may substitute for some segment that is
down line. Furthermore, the perseverated segment mayor may not delete
from its original slot, again depending upon the sequence that would
remain. Here again, the scan copier would copy, for instance, an initial
onset consonant onto an onset position somewhere down line in the
word, or phrase, either adding another segment by filling an empty slot
(see Beland & Nespoulous, 1985) or by substituting for some other conso-
nant originally in that position. Once again, it appears that the derail-
ments that result in perseverative phonemic transpositions take place
during the operation of the scan copier mechanism.
Since practically all of the phonemic transpositions take place in the
segmental structuring of content words, it would appear that these errors
occur during the manipulation of the contentives. Furthermore, the ma-
nipulation must involve the form of the content words, since form is
crucially involved with phonemic errors. The forms of content words are
being uniquely manipulated as they are inserted into the phrasal frame
slots at the positional level in the Garrett model. It may very well be
during this phrasal frame placement that the scan copier operates as well
(perhaps in parallel) on the segments of the contentives. Consequently, I
would assign the location of phonemic paraphasia to the stage at which
lexical phonological representations of individual content words are in-
serted into the developing phrasal matrices at the positional level.
APHASIC TRANSFORMA nONS 139
recalcitrant segment on the sonority scale. The liquids Irl and III are
between the nasals and glides in terms of sonority. The glides Iyl and Iwl
are quite close to the vowels in sonority, but note that glides may never be
the peaks since they are never stressed. The liquids, on the other hand,
may serve as syllabic elements in English. A typically acceptable syllable
on this scale would be the word gland, where the outermost elements are
oral stops Igl and Idl (the least sonorous), the internal consonants III and
Inl being more sonorous, and the peak a being the most sonorous. Conse-
quently, a syllable is composed of an increasing degree of sonority ipward
toward the stressed elements and from there a decreasing degree of
sonority to the final item of the coda. A syllable such as put would,consist
of a steep incline and decline in terms of sonority, whereas a syllable such
as ground would have a more gradual incline and decline: oral stop,
liquid, vowel, glide, nasal, oral stop. It is a universal fact of syllable
structure (Clements, 1988) that the most preferred, least complex, least
marked, or simplest type of CVC syllable involves sharp inclines and
gradual declines in sonority (very sharp crescendos and less sharp decres-
cendos). Syllables such as Ibal are simpler or more preferred than sylla-
bles such as Iwa/. The unmarked final VC syllable has a more gradual
decrescendo. In this way, we can set up a complexity metric for syllable
types and in a sense for sequences of two segments. Simple, two-segment
sequences are those pairs where the segments involved are far apart on
sonority hierarchy. Accordingly, there would be an enhancement of a
contrast in prominence, or what Donnegan and Stampe (1978) refer to as
an "attraction of opposites." This is why geminate consonant clusters and
sequences of two vowels are so rare in languages. It also explains why
children's earliest syllables involve segments far apart on the sonority
scale (Le., CVs, where the Cs are oral stops). Onset consonant clusters
composed of two oral stops are rare as well, as are onset clusters with
sequences of segments that are adjacent on the sonority hierarchy (e.g.,
Harris, 1983, p. 21, for Spanish). Again, the attraction of opposites holds
more strongly in initial demisyllables, where sharp crescendos are
preferred.
If we turn our attention back to doublet creating phonemic para-
phasias, we note that in order to produce the doublet, the checkoff
monitor must not work, leaving the moved segment (having been either
anticipated or perseverated) in its original position as well. In Buck-
ingham (1989), several examples are cited that demonstrate the operation
of the sonority principle. When some segment is erroneously transposed
by the scan copier, it is generally the case that it will not be checked off
from its original position, if by so doing a highly marked sequence of
segments would be set up. I should note that, heretofore, doublets have
been described but not explained. The sonority principle attempts to offer
an explanation of why we get doublets in some instances and not in
142 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
others. Take the doublet creation of two /p/'s in the paraphasia papple for
the word apple. If the doublet had not been created-that is, if the antici-
pated /p/ had been correctly checked off by the monitor-that would have
left a highly marked cross-syllabic sequence of two vowels: lre/ and /:}/.
Most linear transpositions of the doublet-creating sort come from syllable
peripheries and cross syllable boundaries. Consequently, the so-called
"syllable contact law," which follows from sonority principles (Clements,
1988), provides a partial account here. In the syllable contact sequence
X$Y (where $ indicates a syllable boundary), X is usually more sonorous
than Y. Therefore, a situation in which X and Y are both vowels would be
highly undesirable. It turns out that on close inspection, doublet errors are
most often created when the misscanned consonant that crosses syllable
boundaries comes from an intervocalic position. The principle of sonority
and the syllable contact law neatly explain why this should be the case.
Again, the computations carried out by the scan copier and by the check-
off monitor must be sensitive to the syllabic structure.
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Baars, B. J. (1980). On eliciting predictable speech errors in the laboratory. In V. A. Fromkin
(Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New
York: Academic Press.
Beland, R., & Nespoulous, J. L. (1985). Recent phonological models and the study of aphasic
errors. Paper presented at the Academy of Aphasia, Pittsburgh.
Bell, A., & Hooper, J. B. (Eds.). (1978). Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Blumstein, S. E. (1973). A phonological investigation of aphasic speech. The Hague:
Mouton.
Buckingham, H. W. (1980). On correlating aphasic errors with slips-of-the-tongue. Applied
Psycholinguistics, I, 199-220.
Buckingham, H. W. (1981). Lexical and semantic aspects of aphasia. In M. T. Sarno (Ed.),
Acquired aphasia. New York: Academic Press.
Buckingham, H. W. (1985). Perseveration in aphasia. In S. Newman & R. Epstein (Eds.),
Current perspectives in dysphasia. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
Buckingham, H. W. (1986). The scan-copier mechanism and the positional level of language
production: Evidence from aphasia. Cognitive Science, 10, 195-217.
Buckingham, H. W. (1987). Phonemic paraphasias and psycholinguistic production models
for neologistic jargon. Aphasiology, I, 381-400.
Buckingham, H. W. (1989). The principle of sonority, doublet creation, and the checkoff
monitor. In J. L. Nespoulous & P. Villiard (Eds.), Morphology, phonology and aphasia.
New York: Springer-Verlag. (in press).
Buckingham, H. W., & Kertesz, A. (1976). Neologistic jargon aphasia. Amsterdam: Swets &
Zeitlinger.
Buckingham, H. W., & Rekart, D. M. (1979). Semantic paraphasia. Journal of Communication
Disorders, 12, 197-209.
Buckingham, H. W., Whitaker, H., & Whitaker, H. A. (1978). Alliteration and assonance in
neologistic jargon aphasia. Cortex, 14, 365-380.
Buckingham, H. W., & Yule, G. (1987). Phonemic false evaluation: Theoretical and clinical
aspects. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, I, 113-125.
Butterworth, B. (1979). Hesitation and the production of verbal paraphasias and neologisms
in jargon aphasia. Brain and Language, 18, 133-161.
Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology: An introduction. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.
Clements, G. N. (1988). The role of the sonority cycle in core syllabification. Working Papers
of the Cornell Phonetics Laboratory, No.2, April.
Cutler, A., & Fay, D. (1978). Introduction. In R. Meringer & C. Mayer (Eds.), Versprechen und
Verlesen. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Donegan, P. J., & Stampe, D. (1978). The syllable in phonological and prosodic structure. In
A. Bell & J. B. Hooper (Eds.), Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Ellis, A. W. (1980). On the Freudian theory of speech errors. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in
linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. New York: Academic
Press.
144 HUGH W. BUCKINGHAM
Fay, D. (1981). Substitutions and splices: A study of sentence blends. In A. Cutler (Ed.), Slips
of the tongue and language production. Amsterdam: Mouton.
Fay, D., & Cutler, A. (1977). Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon. Linguistic
Inquiry, 8, 505-520.
Fillmore, C. J. (1968). The case for case. In E. Bach & R. T. Harms (Eds.)' Universals in
linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Fodor, J. A. (1982). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive
psychology. In H. 1. Dreyfus (Ed., with H. Hall), Husserl, intentionality, and cognitive
science (a Bradford book). Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology (a Bradford
book). Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Fodor, J. A. (1986). Information and association. In M. Brand & R. M. Harnish (Eds.)' The
representation of knowledge and belief. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47,
27-52.
Garrett, M. F. (1975). The analysis of sentence production. In B. Gordon (Ed.), The psychol-
ogy of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory. New York: Academic
Press.
Garrett, M. F. (1976). Syntactic processes in sentence production. In R. J. Wales & E. Walker
(Eds.), New approaches to language mechanisms. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Garrett, M. F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),
Sentence production 1: Speech and talk. London: Academic Press.
Garrett, M. F. (1982). Production of speech: Observations from normal and pathological
language use. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive function.
London: Academic Press.
Garrett, M. F. (1984). The organization of processing structure for language production:
Applications to aphasic speech. In D. Caplan, A. R. Lecours, & A. Smith (Eds.),
Biological perspectives on language. Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Harris, J. W. (1983). Syllable structure and stress in Spanish: A nonlinear analysis.
Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
Kohn, S. E. (1984). The nature of the phonological disorder in conduction aphasia. Brain and
Language, 23, 97-115.
Kohn, S. E. (1985). Phonological breakdown in aphasia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
Boston: Tufts University.
Lecours, A. R., & Lhermitte, F. (1969). Phonemic paraphasias: Linguistic structures and
tentative hypotheses. Cortex, 5, 193-228.
Lenneberg, E. H. (1968). Review of Speech and brain mechanisms by W. Penfield & 1.
Roberts. In R C. Oldfield & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Language. Middlesex, England:
Penguin.
MacNeilage, P. F., Hutchinson, J. A., & Lasater, S. A. (1981). The production of speech:
Development and dissolution of motoric and premotoric processes. In J. Long & A.
Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. 9). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meringer, R, & Mayer, C. (1895). Versprechen und verlesen, Eine psychologisch-lin-
guistische Studie. Stuttgart: Goschense Verlagsbuchhandlung.
Pike, K. 1., & Pike, E. (1947). Immediate constituents of Mazateco syllables. International
Journal of American Linguistics, 13, 78-91.
Rinnert, C., & Whitaker, H. A. (1973). Semantic confusions by aphasic patients. Cortex, 9,
56-81.
Schwartz, M. F. (1987). Patterns of speech production deficit within and across aphasia
syndromes: Application of a psycholinguistic model. In M. Coltheart, G. Sartori, & R. Job
(Eds.), The cognitive neuropsychology of language. London: Erlbaum.
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
APHASIC TRANSFORMATIONS 145
Interest in reading alterations arose toward of the last century when De-
jerine described two types of alexia: alexia with agraphia and alexia with-
out agraphia. The first type was correlated with a lesion in the angular
region and the second with a lesion of the occipital lobe in which the
corpus callosum was involved. The relationship between the angular re-
gion and alterations in both reading and writing has been repeatedly
confirmed in the literature (e.g., Benson, 1979; Geschwind, 1965; Hecaen,
1962). The topography of alexia without agraphia has, however, been
debated by several authors (Damasio & Damasio, 1983; Greenblatt, 1983),
and the presence of this reading disturbance has been accepted in cases of
exclusive occipital lesions (Damasio & Damasio, 1983).
Almost a century after Dejerine described the two cases of alexia
classically known in the literature, Benson in 1977 proposed the term
frontal alexia to describe the reading alterations observed in patients with
Broca's aphasia.
Although reading seems to be a strongly lateralized function, since
alterations are usually a product of left-hemisphere lesions, right-hem i-
147
148 ALFREDO ARDILA et al.
Phoneme Grapheme
Ibl b,v
lsi c (before e, i), s, z
ItSI ch
Idl d
If I f
Igl g (before a, 0, u) and gu (before e, i)
Iii i, y (as a conjunction and in diphthongs)
Ihl j, g (before e, i)
Ikl c (before a, 0, u) and qu (before e, i)
III I
IAI or Ijl II
Iml m
Inl n
IJlI fl
Ipl p
Irl r
Ii:I rr, r (at the beginning of a word)
It I t
Ikl + lsi x
Ijl y
h
lal a
lei e
101 o
lui u
In 1982 Bub and Kertesz suggested the term deep dysgraphia to de-
scribe the profile characterized by the semantic substitutions in writing
that one of their patients presented. Similarly, Roeltgen, Sevush, and
Heilman (1983) gave the name phonological agraphia to the inability
another patient presented to write nonsense words to dictation in spite of
a good ability to write known words. Roeltgen, Rothi-Gonzalez, and
Heilman (1986) have proposed the existence of a semantic agraphia in
five patients who could spell irregular words but had difficulties in giving
them their meaning.
Little is known about the relationship between writing alterations
and cerebral damage in reading-writing systems like the one used in
Spanish. The classifications used are very vague and do not sufficiently
satisfy observations made in clinical practice. Furthermore, no studies
have been carried out to correlate reading-writing alterations in Spanish-
speaking patients with lesions in particular areas of the brain.
One of the methods that has probably allowed us to advance furthest
in our knowledge about cerebral lesion topography is the brain scan or
150 ALFREDO ARDILA et a1.
METHOD
Subjects
Sixty-two patients with cerebral damage were studied (27 women, 35
men; average age = 41.43, SD = 13.92, age range = 16-65). These subjects
presented various etiologies (vascular = 44, tumoral = 14, traumatic = 4).
Sixty-two normal subjects matched for age, sex, and schooling with the
first group were also studied. The cerebral damage had evolved in a peri-
od varying from 1 to 4 months. Patients had no background of previous
neurological or psychiatric illnesses. Average schooling was 8.14 years
(range = 4-19, SD = 4.54). Table 2 shows the general characteristics of
the population studied. All lesions were confirmed by means of comput-
erized axial tomography, and the analysis of damage topography was
carried out by a neuroradiologist.
Procedure
The patients included in the sample were taken from the neuropsy-
chological services of the San Juan de Dios Hospital and the Colombian
Institute of Neurology. Each patient was administered a neuropsycholo-
gical battery consisting of the following tests: (1) Boston Diagnostic Ap-
hasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1979), (2) Token Test (De Renzi &
Faglioni, 1978), (3) a reading and writing test specifically designed for
Spanish-speaking subjects and standardized in a population of normal
subjects (Rosselli & Ardila, 1987), (4) the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure
(Osterrieth, 1944). The specific language tests (Boston Diagnostic Aphasia
Examination and the Token Test) were applied only to patients with left-
cerebral damage. Normal subjects were given only the reading and writing
test.
ALEXIA AND AGRAPHIA IN SPANISH SPEAKERS 151
Educational
Sex Age level Topography Hemisphere Etiology Token Category
Instrument
A reading and writing test designed for Spanish-speaking subjects
was used (Rosselli & Ardila, 1987). The test consisted of the following
subtests:
Reading
Reading of letters (20; 15 consonants, 5 vowels)
Reading of syllables (12; e.g., po, li, elus, trans)
Reading of logo tomes (11; e.g., tala, fasaja)
Reading of words (13; e.g., coso, libra, ventana, bicieleta)
Reading of sentences (5; e.g., La cantina es de Juan)
Understanding orders (9; e.g., close your eyes)
Reading and comprehension of texts (9; 108-word text, 4 questions;
and 185-word text, 5 comprehension questions)
Ideographic reading (7; e.g., Coca-Cola)
Writing
Writing of letters (dictation and copy) (14; 9 consonants, 5 vowels)
Writing of syllables (dictation and copy) (10; e.g., po, ela)
Writing of words (dictation and copy) (6; e.g., coso, caballo)
Writing of sentences (dictation and copy) (4; e.g., La gente se reune)
Written description of a picture (cookie theft)
The test uses a scoring system based on the number of errors; the
possible number of errors is indicated in brackets. The written description
of a picture was taken from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination
(Goodglass & Kaplan, 1979) and the proposed scoring method was used.
RESULTS
Intrahemispheric Comparisons
After comparing the results obtained by patients with right and left
lesions, intrahemispheric comparisons were then carried out. For this
purpose, the patients in each group were divided into two subgroups:
prerolandic and retrorolandic, depending on whether the scanner image
was found in front of or behind the central fissure.
The left retrorolandic group showed a significantly greater number of
errors in reading [X = 41.65 (43% errors)], and in writing [X = 15.81
(47%)], than the prerolandic group [reading: X = 23.55 (25%); writing: X
= 9.18 (27%)]. The group of patients with right retrorolandic lesions pre-
sented an almost equivalent number of errors to the right prerolandic
TABLE 3. t Test of Means Obtained by Patients with Right and Left Cerebral Lesions in the Reading Subtests a
Subtest
group LTRb SLB LGT WOR SEN ORD TXT IDG
Right hemi- X = 0.5 X = 1.00 X = 2.72 X = 2.00 X = 1.44 X = 1.21 X= 3.05 X = 0.22
sphere 2.5% 8% 25% 15% 29% 13% 34% 3%
n = 18
Left hemi- X= 2.8 X= 3.64 X = 5.89 X = 5.10 X = 2.24 X= 3.91 X = 5.78 X = 1.29
sphere 14% 30% 54% 39% 45% 43% 64% 18%
n = 37
df 53 53 53 53 53 53 53 53
2.27 2.11 2.006 2.15 1.08 2.33 1.71 2.18
TABLE 4. t Test of Means Obtained by Patients with Right and Left Cerebral
Lesions on the Writing Subtests Q
Subtests
groups Letters Syllables Words Sentences
Prefrontal
n=6 0 7 15 14 2 8 4 0
Broca
n=5 15 38 49 29 60 47 47 37
Conduction
n=6 29 28 64 48 40 42 48 14
Wernicke
n = 13 17 28 55 42 48 57 84 14
Anomic
n=4 13 25 73 42 50 50 89 16
Alexia without agraphia
n = 3 30 50 82 69 86 100 100 51
Right prerolandic
n=6 7 4 11 6 6 11 12 3
Right retrorolandic
n = 12 25 18 36 25 43 22 50 6
aReading of letters (LTR). reading of syllables (SYL). reading of logotomes (LGT). reading of words (WaR).
reading of sentences (SEN). comprehension of written orders (ORD). comprehension of a text (TXT).
ideographic reading (IDE).
reading subtests. The greatest number of errors was made in the reading of
letters and words by the CA and A WA groups. The AW A, AA, and CA
group showed the greatest number of errors in the reading of logotomes; in
reading comprehension A WA, AA, and WA were most impaired. In the
case of ideographic reading, the greatest numbers of errors was observed
in patients with AW A. Patients with PF presented the lowest mean for
errors. Table 6 shows the percentage of patients presenting errors in each
reading subtest.
In Table 7, we can appreciate the percentage of errors obtained by
each subgroup of patients in each subtest of the writing section. After the
GA group (79% errors), the patients in the WA (36%), CA (30%), and AWA
(30%) groups presented the largest number of errors in the writing of
letters. In the writing of syllables, the greatest number of errors was made
by groups AA, A WA, CA, and WA, and in the writing of words, the groups
A WA, W A, and CA presented a higher average number of errors in rela-
tion to the other groups. The AA and AWA groups presented the greatest
number of errors in the writing of sentences in spite of the fact that the
number of errors was very similar in all groups except for the groups with
right lesions and the PF left group, for which the average of errors was
lower. Table 8 shows the percentage of patients presenting errors in each
writing subtest.
ALEXIA AND AGRAPHIA IN SPANISH SPEAKERS 159
Prefrontal 17 33 83 67 17 33 67 0
Broca 80 80 100 100 80 100 80 60
Conduction 50 83 100 100 83 100 83 67
Wernicke 69 69 100 92 62 100 100 38
Anomie 75 75 100 100 100 75 100 75
Alexia without agraphia 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
Right prerolandic 17 33 50 50 33 66 33 17
Right retrorolandic 33 41 83 91 66 83 109 25
"Letters (LTR), syllables (SYL), logotomes (LGT) , words (WOR), sentences (SEN), comprehension of
orders (ORD), comprehension of a text (TXT), ideographic reading (IDE).
Prefrontal 4 26 22 58
Broca 20 42 43 75
Conduction 30 48 55 83
Wernicke 36 48 55 75
Anomie 18 57 33 88
Alexia without agraphia 30 53 60 100
Right prerolandic 7 13 18 38
Right retrorolandic 14 24 27 55
Prefrontal 33 50 66 83
Broca 40 80 60 100
Conduction 83 83 83 100
Wernicke 77 92 100 100
Anomie 75 75 50 100
Alexia without agraphia 100 100 100 100
Right prerolandic 33 50 50 50
Right retrorolandic 75 75 41 75
160 ALFREDO ARDILA et 01.
LSU NED VMS ANT PER AND LRE SLM 10M NOM LAD NEG CON
Prefrontal 1.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 4.0 2.0 0.0 0.5 0.0 0.0
Broca 5.0 0.0 0.0 5.0 6.5 0.0 0.0 0.5 8.0 0.0 2.0 0.0 0.0
Conduction 11.5 1.5 2.0 3.5 1.5 0.0 3.5 3.5 3.5 0.0 3.5 0.0 0.0
Wernicke 11.5 8.0 2.5 1.0 1.0 0.0 2.5 3.0 5.0 2.0 2.0 0.0 0.0
Anomie 9.0 6.0 1.0 0.0 0.5 6.5 6.5 1.5 4.0 10.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
Alexia without agraphia 15.0 5.0 11.0 0.0 9.0 0.0 15.0 2.5 1.5 1.5 4.0 0.0 0.0
Right prerolandic 0.5 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 1.0 0.0 0.0 0.5 0.5 3.0
Right retrorolandic 1.5 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 3.0 3.0 0.0 1.5 5.5 8.5
oEach cell represents the average number of errors. Literal substitutions (LSU). neologisms (NED). verbal morphological substitutions (VMS). anticipations (ANT).
perseverations (PER). anomie errors (AND). literal reading (LRE). substitution of logotomes for meaningful words (SLM). letter omissions (LOM). noun omissions (NOM).
letter additions (LAD). neglect (NEG). confabulation (CON).
162 ALFREDO ARDILA et at
PER LIT NEO LAD LOM FAD FOM ANT GEL COP
Prefrontal 3.2 2.0 0.0 0.0 1.6 0.0 0.0 0.4 0.0 0.0
Broca 3.2 5.6 0.0 1.2 0.8 0.0 0.0 5.2 0.0 8.8
Conduction 1.6 7.2 6.0 1.6 6.4 0.0 0.0 2.0 0.0 4.0
Wernicke 3.2 8.8 9.6 2.8 6.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.4
Anomie 0.0 5.2 12.0 2.0 1.2 0.0 0.0 2.4 0.0 2.0
Alexia without agraphia 3.2 7.6 4.0 3.6 6.4 0.0 0.0 1.2 0.0 37.2
Right prerolandic 0.0 1.2 0.0 2.8 2.4 5.2 0.8 0.0 1.2 0.0
Right retrorolandic 0.0 1.2 0.0 2.4 8.0 3.2 2.4 0.0 12.0 0.0
aEach cell represents the average of errors. Perseverations (PER). literal substitutions (LIT), neologisms
(NEO),letter additions (LAD), letter omissions (LaM). feature additions (FAD). feature omissions (FOM),
anticipations (ANT), grouping of elements (GEL), copying errors (COP).
Superimposition of Scans
The scans of the patients from each group were superimposed in
accordance with the steps explained in procedure. In this way, templates
were obtained corresponding to the groups studied, seven of which are
shown jn Figures 1 to 7. Owing to the small sample in the AWA and GA
groups, the superimposition of their scans did not produce important
data. The templates of the figures indicate the areas that had a certain
level of superimposition: 20-40%, 50-60%, 70-90%, and 100%.
Figure 1 shows the regions of the left hemisphere that were involved
in the PF group (n = 6). The anterior prefrontal cortex and the paramedian
and parasagittal region of the left frontal lobe are those principally in-
volved. Figure 2 shows how in patients with BA there is an extensive
damage of the motor and premotor cortex and also of the anterior portion
of the insular cortex. In the CA group (n = 6), a damage principally of the
anterior portion of the insula appeared, involving a small portion of the
sensitive cortex in the middle part and extending to the beginning of the
supramarginal region (Fig. 3).
The topographical findings obtained from the superimposition of
scans of patients with WA (n = 13) are shown in Figure 4. The posterior
third of the insular cortex was the site of the lesions in all the patients.
164 ALFREDO ARDILA et 01 .
SQ(Dw ~
\ /... ~
~
_
I!lIID
~
100\
70\ - 90~
SO~ - 60~
--...... CJ 20~ - 40~
. 40~
The posterior temporal cortex, particularly the first gyrus, was also ob-
served to be involved and, to a lesser degree, the posterior parietal cortex
(angular gyrus). The scanner results of the patients with AA (n = 4) can be
seen in Figure 5. Deep involvement of the posterior temporal lobe and
angular gyri predominated. There were very few patients with AWA (n =
3), and it was therefore not possible to produce an adequate superimposi-
tion of the lesions; the posterior portion of the occipital lobe was observed
to be involved in all the patients, and 1 patient also presented a certain
posterior parietal damage. The patients with global aphasia (n = 4) pre-
sented an extensive cortical and subcortical damage that extended from
the premotor regions of the frontal lobe to the parieto-temporo-occipital
junction.
The superimposition of scans of the lesions presented by the patients
with right-hemisphere lesions is shown in Figures 6 and 7. The patients
with prerolandic lesions (n = 6) had involved the superior region of the
right prefrontal area, and, on occasions, this damage reached the premo tor
area. The cerebral compromise observed in the patients from the retro-
rolandic group was located principally in the deep cortical portion of the
parietal lobe. In some patients, however, a deep posterior temporal and
anterior occipital damage was also observed.
so~
:!o~
DISCUSSION
Intrahemispheric Comparisons
If the left prerolandic and retrorolandic groups are compared, a great-
er number of errors can be observed in the retrorolandic group on both the
reading and writing tests, while no important differences were observed
in the two groups of patients with right-hemisphere lesions. Several as-
pects could be related to these findings: (1) The posterior areas of the left
cerebral cortex have classically been considered to be principally respon-
sible for the symbolic aspects of reading and writing, while the anterior
areas have been related to the motor aspects of the same. (2) Several
authors (e.g., Bradshaw & Nettleton, 1981) have spoken of the homoge-
neousness of the right-hemisphere areas, with a supposedly more diffuse,
less localized organization of functions, in contrast to the heterogeneity of
the left hemisphere areas, with a supposedly more defined organization of
its functions.
ALEXIA AND AGRAPHIA IN SPANISH SPEAKERS 169
FIGURE 8. Sample of writing of a patient with right hemisphere lesion. "Cascade" phe-
nomenon: Margins increase each time a new line is begun.
172 ALFREDO ARDILA et a1.
An Interlinguistic Consideration
Finally, it is worthy to mention that the psycholinguistic models of
the alexias and agraphias developed in other languages, particularly in
English, do not seem to be applicable to Spanish. In this work (and in all
the authors' personal experience) no semantic paralexia appears in Span-
ish-speaking aphasic patients. Morphological verbal paralexias do, how-
ever, frequently appear: One part of the word is read correctly (usually the
first morpheme) and the rest is deduced (e.g., Ikonsiderandol - .lkon-
siderasyonl; "considering", "consideration"). Moreover, given the writ-
ing system of Spanish, there are no appreciable differences between the
reading of logotomes and the reading of low-frequency words. The writing
system of the Spanish allows the correct reading of even completely un-
known words. Spelling is a task with a low level of difficulty.
The existence of a double system of reading (phonological and se-
mantic) would not then seem to be acceptable, as has been proposed for
English. This seems to be a fundamental point of view: while Spanish
presents an almost complete phonological writing system, the English
writing system is partially phonological and partially logographic (Samp-
son, 1985).
For Spanish speakers, the underlying cognitive operation during
reading is to convert graphemes into phonemes. This is not totally true in
English: Reading is achieved at a more morphological level. For Spanish
speakers it is just surprising to approach the English writing system, as it
is perhaps for English speakers to approach an ideographic system. A
Latin-American woman living in the United States for many years ex-
pressed this feeling in a very descriptive way: "The problem with English
is that you read one thing, but you have to say another." For her, of course,
reading could be thought of only as converting graphemes into phonemes.
Our point, simply, is that reading in English and reading in Spanish
represent quite different cognitive activities. Consequently, brain repre-
sentation of written language and models for alexias and agraphias have
to be somehow different. Many more studies are needed in this regard.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
gela Flores, Catalina Ramirez, and Lissy Sperber helped in the collection
of the data. The English version was prepared by Jenny Lewis. To all of
them our gratitude.
REFERENCES
Ardila, A., Montanes, P., Bernal, B., & Serpa, A. (1986). Partial psychic seizures and brain
organization. International Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 23-32.
Ardila, A., & Ostrosky, F. (1984). The right hemisphere: Neurology and neuropsychology.
London: Gordon and Breach.
Benson, D. F. (1977). The third alexia. Archives of Neurology, 34, 327-331.
Benson, D. F. (1979). Aphasia, alexia and agraphio. New York: Churchill Livingstone.
Benson, D. F., & Cummings, J. L. (1985). Agraphia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of
clinical neurology, Vol. 45: Clinical neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Bradshaw, J. L., & Nettleton, N. C. (1981). The nature of the hemispheric specialization.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 146-165.
Bub, D., & Kertesz, A. (1982). Deep agraphia. Brain and Language, 17, 146-165.
Damasio, A., & Damasio, H. (1983). The anatomical basis of pure alexia. Neurology, 33,
1573-1583.
De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened
version of the Token Test. Cortex, 13, 424-433.
Edwards, A. L. (1968). Experimental design in psychological research. New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnection syndromes in animals and man. Brain, BB, 237-294.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1979). Evaluacion de las afasias y de transtornos similares.
Buenos Aires: Editorial Medica Panamericana.
Greenblatt, S. H. (1983). Localization of lesions in alexia. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in
neuropsychology. New York: Academic Press.
Hecaen, H. (1962). Clinical symptomatology in right and left hemisphere lesions. In V. B.
Mountcastle (Ed.), Interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance. Baltimore:
Johns Hopkins.
Hecaen, H., & Albert, M. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.
Kertesz, A. (1983). Localization of lesions in Wernicke's aphasia. In A. Kertesz (Ed.),
Localization of neuropsychology. New York: Academic Press.
Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
Luria, A. R. (1977). Las funciones corti cales superiores en el hombre. La Habana: Editorial
Orbe.
Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach.
Journal of Psycholinguistics, 2, 175-199.
Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1980). The conceptual status of deep dyslexia. In M.
Colheart, K. Patterson, & J. V. Marshall (Eds.), Deep dyslexia. London: Routledge and
Kegan Paul.
Miceli, G., Silveri, M. C., & Caramazza, A. (1985). Cognitive analysis of a case of pure
dysgraphia. Brain and Language, 25, 187-212.
Naeser, M. A. (1983). CT scan lesion size and lesion locus in cortical and subcortical ap-
hasias. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology, New York: Academic
Press.
Novoa, O. P., & Ardila, A. (1987). Linguistic abilities in patients with prefrontal damage.
Brain and Language, 30, 206-225.
ALEXIA AND AGRAPHIA IN SPANISH SPEAKERS 175
Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d'une figure complexe. Revue de Psychologie, 30,
206-256.
Poeck, K., Bleser, R., & Keyserlingk, T. (1984). Computed tomography localization and
standard aphasic syndromes. In J. J. C. Rosen (Ed.), Advances in neurology: Progress in
aphasiology (Vol. 42). New York: Raven Press.
Roeltgen, D. P., Rothi-Gonzalez, E., & Heilman, K. M. (1986). Linguistic semantic agraphia: A
dissociation of the lexical spelling system from semantics. Brain and Language, 27,
257-280.
Roeltgen, D. P., Sevush, S., & Heilman, K. M. (1983). Phonological agraphia, writing by the
lexical-semantic route. Neurology, 33, 755-765.
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1987). Alteraciones de la lectura, la escritura y el calculo. In J.
Bustamante & F. Lopera (Eds.), E1 lenguaje: Principios de neurolinguistica. Medellin:
Prensa Creativa.
Rosselli, M., Rosselli, A., Vergara, I., & Ardila, A. (1985). Topography ofthe hemi-inattention
syndrome. International Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 165-172.
Sampson, G. (1985). Writing systems. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
9
ALFREDO ARDILA • Konrad Lorenz Foundation University, Bogota, Colombia, and Miami
Institute of Psychology, Miami, Florida 33166-6612. MARIA VICTORIA LOPEZ. Co-
lombian Association of Neuropsychology, Bogota, Colombia. EUGENIA SOLANO'
Neurologic Institute of Colombia, Bogota, Colombia.
177
178 ALFREDO ARDILA et a1.
~
--- ....... ~ ~
CASE I ~ LUR IA 1~101 CASE :t (I.UR IA 1970 ] CASE 1 (LUR IA "70)
~ -;::"' ....
~ ~....... ~
---- .......
~ ~ ~:'." ~
~""
HIEA. ct al . 11980) HIER et .at. ~ "801 ARO I L A {UNPUBLISHED)
~ ~ ~
PATIENT 1 PATIENT 2 SU PER IMPOS I T ON
I
CASE REPORTS
Patient 1
This is a 21-year-old right-handed woman with 12 years of schooling,
monolingual (Spanish), who in November 1983 presented involuntary
contraction of her right arm with subsequent loss of consciousness.
The only important fact in her history is a bilateral fronto-parieto-
temporal pulsatile headache from the age of 13. Her examination on
admittance to the Neurological Institute of Colombia proved to be
normal except for a discrete hypoesthesia in the right hemibody. As a
result of the scans she was taken to the operating room, where a left
posterior par~etal glioma was removed. She subsequently received co-
balt therapy. During the following years, she remained apparently
symptom-free, suffering occasionally from paresthesias; on one occa-
sion she had a right hemibody focal seizure. However, she stopped
studying and worked sporadically as a manicurist. In June 1986 a
control scan was taken (Figure 2) that recorded the presence of a
mixed-density lesion. This clearly showed the contrasting medium
with well-defined borders and an irregular shape with no great con-
tralateral deviation from the structures of the middle line.
Her neurological examination showed her to be an alert, cooper-
ative patient, with adequate affect, fluent conversational language, and
no evidence of agrammatism or dysprosody. She exhibited occasional
verbal paraphasias and word-finding difficulties. No comprehension
defect was evident in casual conversation. There was extinction in the
presence of simultaneous double tactile stimulation, moderate diffi-
Patient 2
This is a 28-year-old right-handed man with 14 years of schooling,
monolingual (Spanish). He was diagnosed as having a left parietal
astrocytoma, which was removed in 1981 after an episode of right
hemibody partial motor seizure with subsequent generalization. The
patient subsequently returned to his work in the government, but only
on a part-time basis, owing to what he called his "tiredness and slow-
ness." He still had a four-fifths right hemiparesis and moderate hypo-
esthesia. Since then he has had frequent partial somatosensory and
motor seizures on his right side, some of them with subsequent gener-
alization. In May 1986 a control scan was taken (Figure 3) that showed
a zone of lesser density in the operative region, multiple calcifications
that do not exert any apparent effect, and dilatation of the left occipital
horns.
His neurological examination showed him to be an alert, cooper-
ative patient, depressed in affect, with fluent language, no evidence of
agrammatism, dysprosody, or articulatory defects, and with occasion-
al verbal paraphasias and word-finding difficulties. A right hemi-
hypoesthesia and a four-fifths hemiparesis was observed. There was
tactile extinction, right agraphesthesia and astereognosia, finger ag-
nosia, and apraxia. The rest of the examination proved to be within
normal limits.
Testing Procedure
Each of the patients was given a battery of tests that lasted approx-
imately 10 hours and was divided into l-hour sessions. The battery in-
cluded the following:
1. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, 1955).
2. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE; Goodglass & Ka-
plan, 1972).
3. Token Test-shortened version (De Renzi & Faglioni, 1978).
4. Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure.
5. Right-left orientation: The patient indicates on his body and on the
examiner's body simple and crossed instructions, recognition of right and
left on a drawing, reproduction of the examiner's movements with the
appropriate hand (Head's test).
6. Finger gnosis: The patient names/shows the fingers on his and the
examiner's hand; shows, on his hand, the finger that corresponds to the
one indicated by the examiner; indicates that one or two of his fingers are
being touched; says how many fingers there are between the two touched
by the examiner.
In addition to these tests, the following were designed for the purpose
of this research:
7. Reading: letters, syllables, meaningless sequences, words of differ-
ent levels of difficulty, sentences, comprehension of written instructions,
reading of text aloud, reading of ideograms (e.g., Coca-Cola), recognition
of symbols (e.g., danger).
8. Writing: letters, syllables, words with different levels of difficulty,
copying words, dictation and copying sentences, changes in the type of
writing.
9. Calculation: reading of numerals, writing of numbers, transcription
of numbers to letters and vice versa, pointing out the largest number in a
pair, arithmetical operations (mental and written), reading of arithmetical
signs, successive operations (100 - 13; 1,4,7 ... ), counting forward and
backward, ordering numbers in columns, arithmetical problems, appre-
ciation of quantities (e.g., how much does an egg weigh?) and of time (e.g.,
how long does it take a person to walk around a block?).
10. Grammar: conversational language, completion of sentences
(verbs, adverbs, prepositions), changing the tense, article-noun agree-
ment (Spanish has masculine/feminine, singular/plural forms of nouns
and articles), indicating the verb that corresponds to a noun (e.g.,
thought-to think) and vice versa, antonyms (with the use of affixes and
with a change in the stem), saying what action is being performed in a
drawing, ordering the parts of a sentence, understanding of passive sen-
tences, understanding of comparative sentences, understanding of coordi-
nation (e.g., the secretary you sent with your cousin is a friend of Peter),
SEMANTIC APHASIA RECONSIDERED 183
RESULTS
Intelligence. Table 1 shows the two patients' scores on the WAIS.
Patient l's IQ was 95 and Patient 2's was 91. In both cases, the lower
scores were obtained on the Arithmetic, Digit Span, and Digit Symbol
subtests. Both had higher Verbal than Performance IQs.
Patient 1 Patient 2
Full scale IQ 93 91
Verbal IQ 95 93
Performance IQ 91 90
Information 10 10
Comprehension 10 13
Arithmetic 7 4
Similarities 10 9
Vocabulary 10 11
Digit span 7 6
Digit symbol 7 5
Picture completion 10 8
Block design 8 10
Picture arrangement 10 8
Object assembly 8 9
....00
2 3 4 5 6 7 "'"
MELODIC LINE I I I 1 I,
intonational contour Absent limited to runs through
short phrases and entire sentence
stereotyped
expressions
1
PHRASE LENGTH
longest occasional (1 /10) word 4 words 7 words
uninterrupted word runs
---
ARTICULATORY AGILITY
facility at phonemic and syllable always impaired normal only in " never impaired
level or impossible familiar words
and phrases ""
GRAMMATICAL FORM
"
variety of grammatical none available limited to simple normal range
'"
construction (even if incomplete) declaratives and
./
stereotypes /'
./
PARAPHASIA IN _ J
RUNNING SPEECH present in every once per minute of ~ absent
utterance conversation~
WORD FINDING t~
informational content in fluent without ~information speech exclusively
relation to fluency
::>
information proportional content words t""
"rj
to fluency
..... G=l
~_ .......... 1 t:l
AUDITORY COMPREHENSION o
converted from objective absent (z= -1.5) (z= -1) (z= -.5) (z= 0) (z= +.5) normal ::>
z-score mean (z= +1) §
F
FIGURE 4. Rating scale profile of speech characteristics (continuous line = patient 1, dotted line = patient 2). ::>
~
2..
SEMANTIC APHASIA RECONSIDERED 185
pected, the profile is close to that for anomic aphasia (and, indeed, both
patients presented associated anomic defects). However, auditory com-
prehension has been fairly well retained, being almost normal in Patient
1, with a low quantity of paraphasias in discursive speech.
Finger Gnosis: Neither of the patients was able to perform all the
tests of recognition and naming of fingers. A certain difference was ob-
served in Patient 1, where performance with the left hand was discretely
better.
Writing. Both patients used their right hand to write. Their writing
was slow, hesitant, and poorly legible, with incorrect spacing, distortions
of letters, and some literal paragraphias (Figure 5). In the case of the 10-
word dictation in the BDAE, correct oral spelling scores were higher than
in the written spelling scores, suggesting that the writing defect is apraxic
or spatial rather than linguistic. The same difficulties were found in
copying.
tions with just one figure (e.g., 3 + 5) were possible but impossible with
two digits (e.g., 93 - 13). The same results were found with written
operations. Recognition of arithmetical symbols (+, -, x, :) was correct.
The patients could count adequately forward and backward but could not
perform successive calculations (e.g., 1, 4, 7 ... ). The ordering of num-
bers in columns was adequate. Both made mistakes in solving simple
arithmetic problems and in the appreciation of quantities and time.
DISCUSSION
Our two patients and all the other patients reported in the literature
presented acalculia in association with the difficulties in the comprehen-
sion of different elements within language. Also, to a certain extent, spa-
tial, praxic, and gnosic defects would point to the proposal that they form
a unified syndrome (Luria, 1976). According to the results of the BDAE,
both patients exhibited mistakes in auditory comprehension (minimal in
case 1, moderate in case 2), some naming defects (particularly with regard
SEMANTIC APHASIA RECONSIDERED 187
aphasic type, and certain spatial confusions appear in reading. Both pa-
tients present finger agnosia-moderate left-right disorientation in Pa-
tient 1 and severe disorientation in Patient 2; severe acalculia; and diffi-
culties in the use and comprehension of adverbs, prepositions, passive
sentences, comparative sentences, and verbal elements with a spatial
content.
The similarity between our cases and those reported by Hier et al.
(1980) is striking. The WAIS, for example, shows, in these authors' results
and in our own, a Verbal IQ higher than a Performance IQ, with decreases
in Arithmetic, Digit Span, and Digit Symbol. The associated disorders
(right-left disorientation, finger agnosia, acalculia, and apraxic agraphia)
are the same. However, despite the apparently similar lesion location and
lesion volume in our patients, the performance in our two patients is
somehow different in some tasks. For example, Patient 1 has a near-
normal language comprehension but Patient 2 has moderately severe
comprehension deficits, based on the Token Test; right-left disorientation
and agraphia were more evident in Patient 2; and the performance on the
grammar test shows some differences, especially in the use of preposi-
tions.
The defects found in our patients typifies the left posterior parietal
syndrome and illustrates the semantic aphasia described by Head (1920)
and analyzed by Luria (1970, 1976). Conversational language is adequate
and fluent, with only a discrete tendency to word forgetfulness. There are
no defects in articulation, phonological recognition, prosody, or the com-
prehension of isolated words, and agrammatism was not observed. How-
ever, language comprehension does not go beyond the limits of isolated
elements or simple sentences, and comprehension of logicogrammatical
structures is seriously compromised. Comprehension of the existing rela-
tionship between the different parts of a sentence is difficult or impossi-
ble, as is the simultaneous synthesis of the different parts of a sentence.
Thus, there is, to say, a "relationship agrammatism" or "impressive
agrammatism" (Luria, 1970). Aspects of a spatial or quasi-spatial nature
(preposition-e.g., to, from; passive sentences-e.g., from whom and to
whom the action is directed), in particular, show alterations.
It is interesting to compare the effects of posterior parietal lesions and
temporal-parietal-occipital lesions in both hemispheres. Above all, it
should be pointed out that in subhuman primates damage in this region in
either of the two hemispheres produces similar deficits: defects in the
perception of simultaneous stimuli, disorders in the visual control of
movements and in occulomotor control in general, errors in spatial orien-
tation and in going through labyrinths and such (Lynch, 1980). In man,
the effects of equivalent lesions (as happens in the case of damage to any
area of cortical association) produce asymmetric defects that are in some
way parallel. Right posterior parietal lesions imply constructional aprax-
SEtv1ANTIC APHASIA RECONSIDERED 189
lWe should remember that the word digit comes from digitus. the Latin for "finger." Digits
can be taken to be the fingers of the hand. and we can see that the act of calculating stems
from counting the fingers of the hand. This association between fingers and counting seems
to go back to the very origin of calculation (Cauty. 1984). The association between finger
agnosia and acalculia would not then be fortuitous as far as cerebral organization is con-
cerned. nor would the fact that both can be included simultaneously within the same
syndrome.
190 ALFREDO ARDILA et oJ.
aphasia the difficulty would depend more exactly on the semantics of the
spatial relationships underlying passive sentences (from whom and to
whom the action is directed), locative statements, and transitive verbs.
It would seem, then, that it is possible to distinguish basic cognitive
defects that in some way could underlie the diversity of neuropsycholo-
gical alterations found in the case of left posterior parietal and parietal-
temporal-occipital damage: defects in spatial conceptualization, spatial
through which language intervenes, knowledge of the external spatial
environment (and the spatial dimensions of one's own body and in the
spatial organization and sequencing of its movements) by means of verbal
symbols and such. In other words, Gerstmann syndrome (left angular
syndrome) and the so-called semantic aphasia are manifestations of the
same underlying cognitive deficits. They would conform a unified neuro-
psychological syndrome.
REFERENCES
Ardila, A. (1981). Las afasias. Bogota: Instituto Neurologico de Colombia.
Ardila, A. (1984). Neurolinguistica. Mexico: Trillas.
Ardila, A., & Ostrosky-Solis, F. (1984). The right hemisphere and behavior. In A. Ardila & F.
Ostrosky-Solis (Eds.), The right hemisphere: Neurology and neuropsychology pp. 3-
50). New York: Gordon and Breach.
Benson, B. F. (1979). Aphasia, alexia and agraphia. New York: Churchill Livingstone.
Benson, B. F., & Cummings, J. 1. (1985). Agraphia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of
clinical neurology, Vol. 45: Clinical Neuropsychology (pp. 457-472). Amsterdam:
Elsevier.
Benton, A. 1. (1977). Reflection on the Gerstmann syndrome. Brain and Language, 4, 45-62.
Boller, F., & Grafman, J. (1983). Acalculia: Historical development and current significance.
Brain and Cognition, 2, 205-223.
Boller, F., & Grafman, J. (1985). Acalculia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical
neurology, Vol. 45: Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 473-482). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Botez, M. I. (1985). Parietal lobe syndromes. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical
neurology, Vol. 45: Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 63-86). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Brown, J. W. (1972). Aphasia, apraxia and agnosia. Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Cauty, A. (1984). Taxonomie, syntaxe et economie des numerations parlees. Amerindia, 9,
111-146.
Conrad, K. (1932). Versuch einer psychologischen Analyse des parietal syndromes.
Monatschrift fiir Psychiatrie und Neurologie, 34.
Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. London: Arnold.
Dahmen, W., Hartje, W., Bussing, A., & Sturm, W. (1982). Disorders of calculation in aphasic
patients-Spatial and verbal components. Neuropsychologia, 20, 145-153.
Deloche, G., & Seron, X. (1984). Some linguistic components of acalculia. In F. C. Rose (Ed.),
Advances in neurology, Vol. 42: Progress in aphasiology (pp. 215-222). New York:
Raven Press.
De Renzi, E., & Faglioni, P. (1978). Normative data and screening power of a shortened
version of the Token Test. Cortex, 14,41-49.
SEMANTIC APHASIA RECONSIDERED 193
Gerstmann, J. (1940). The syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left,
agraphia and acalculia. Archives of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 44, 398-
408.
Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disorders. New York: Grune & Stratton.
Goodglass, H., Gleason, J. G., & Hyde, M. R (1970). Some dimensions of language com-
prehension in aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 13, 595-606.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1972). Assessment of aphasia and related disorders.
Philadelphia: Lea Febiger.
Grewel, F. (1969). The acalculia. In P. J. Vinken & G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical
neurology (pp. 181-196). (Vol. 4). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Head, H. (1920). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. Brain, 43, 87-165.
Hecaen, H., & Albert, M. L. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.
Hecaen, H., Angelergues, T., & Houillier, S. (1961). Les varietes cliniques des acalculies au
cours des lesions retrorolandiques: Approche statistique du probleme. Revue Neurologi-
que, 105, 85-103.
Hier, D. B., Mogil, S. I., Rubin, N. P., & Kornros, G. R (1980). Semantic aphasia: A neglected
entity. Brain and Language, 10, 120-131.
Kertesz, A. (1979). Aphasia and associated disorders. New York: Grune and Stratton.
Kertesz, A. (1985). Aphasia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol.
45: Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 287-332). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1962). A study of finger agnosia. Brain, 85, 47-66.
LeDoux, J. E. (1982). Neuroevolutionary mechanism of several asymmetry. Brain, Behavior
and Evolution, 20, 197-213.
LeDoux, J. E. (1984). Cognitive evolution: Clues from brain asymmetry. In A. Ardila & F.
Ostrosky-Solis (Eds.), The right hemisphere: Neurology and neuropsychology pp. 51-
60). New York: Gordon and Breach.
Levin, H., & Spiers, P. A. (1985). Acalculia. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical
neuropsychology (2nd ed., pp. 97-115). New York: Oxford University Press.
Luria, A. R (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.
Luria, A. R (1970). Traumatic aphasia. The Hague: Mouton.
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books.
Luria, A. R (1976). Basic problems of neurolinguistics. The Hague: Mouton.
Lynch, J. (1980). The functional organization of posterior parietal association cortex.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 485-634.
Morris, H. H., Luders, H., Lesser, R P., Dinner, D. S., & Hahn, J. (1984). Transient neuropsy-
chological abnormalities (including Gerstmann's syndrome) during cortical stimulation.
Neurology, 34, 877-883.
Poeck, K., & Orgass, B. (1966). Gerstmann's syndrome and aphasia. Cortex, 2, 421-437.
Roeltgen, D. P., Sevush, S., & Heilman, K. M. (1983). Pure Gerstmann's syndrome from a
focal lesion. Archives of Neurology, 40, 46-47.
Schwartz, M. F., Saffran, E. M., & Marin, O. S. M. (1980). The word order problem in
agrammatism: Comprehension. Brain and Language, 10, 249-262.
Strub, R, & Geschwind, N. (1974). Gerstmann's syndrome without aphasia. Cortex, 10, 378-
387.
Strub, R, & Geschwind, N. (1983). Localization in Gerstmann syndrome. In A. Kertesz (Ed.),
L~calization in neuropsychology (pp. 295-322). New York: Academic Press.
Varney, N. R. (1984). Gerstmann syndrome without aphasia: A longitudinal study. Brain and
Cognition, 3, 1-9.
Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: Manual. New York: Psychological
Corporation.
Zucker, K. (1934). An analysis of disturbed function in aphasia. Brain, 57.
IV
197
198 FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS et al.
""(MOTOR
CORTEx
TENPO~A L
COllTEX
, ,
,, .
, ,
,, ,,
. ,
,
,, ,,, ,,
,
, .
//
..
VENTRAL VENTRAL
ANTERIOR ANTERIO R
rotuCLEuS NUCL EUS ,,/
VENTt:'lAL
L ATERAL
l<UCLEUS
-----_ ......,>
CENTRONE OIA,... "
THAL ANUS ,, THALAMUS , ~Cl..EUS "
,, , , , "
,
, , ,
",
SUBSTANTIA
fIIlGRA
·· "'t!) ~~:::Ar<TIA
...... - ...
··
SUPERIOR
..
COLL ICULUS
FIGURE 1. Schematic diagram of the motor and complex anatomical circuits. Motor loop: cortico-putamino-pallido-thalamo-cortical (motor
cortex). Complex loop: cortico-caudato-nigro-thalamo-cortical (premotor and prefrontal cortex). (Adapted from DeLong, Georgopulos, & Crutcher,
1983.)
ADRENAL AUTOGRAFTING IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE 201
1986; Brozoski, Brown, Rosvold, & Goldman, 1979; Lees & Smith, 1983;
Stern & Langston, 1985; Taylor et 01., 1986, 1987).
In spite of the fact that the primary pathology of PD is the degenera-
tion of the dopaminergic projection to the striatum, not all these patients'
symptoms can be attributed to loss of nigrostriatal dopamine. There are
other neurochemical systems that are found to be affected in PD, such as
the noradrenergic cells in the locus coeruleus, serotoninergic neurons in
the dorsal raphe nucleus, and acethylcholine due to lesions in the sep-
tohippocampal system and the substantia innominata. At the cortical
level, a reduction in somatostatine has been reported (see review in Agid
et 01., 1987).
The reduction of acethylcholine and its enzymes in the nucleus
basalis of Meynert has been associated with demential disturbances (Gas-
par & Gray, 1984). As for the other neurotransmitters, no clear relation has
yet been established between the biochemical changes and the clinical
symptomatology. However, in light of data obtained from animal re-
search, it has been hypothesized that the selective alteration in the nor-
adrenergic systems could cause attentional disturbances; the reduction of
serotoninergic metabolism has been associated with depression, and the
decrease in somatostatine at the cortical level has been correlated with
intellectual deterioration (Agid et 01., 1987).
It would appear that the lesions in the different neuronal systems do
not evolve in parallel but may be additives or potentiate one another in
terms of functional expression. From the biochemical point of view, it has
been found that the cholinergic activity in striatum and cortical areas is
related to the quantity of dopamine receptors. In PD patients with a low
number of dopamine receptors, labeling of cholinergic muscarinic recep-
tors revealed decreased numbers in both the caudate nucleus and the
cortex. Individuals with an increased number of dopamine receptors dis-
played evidence of a corresponding increase in cholinergic receptors
(Rinne, 1982). The variety in the extension and the degree of lesions that
has been found among PD patients could be the pathological substratum
for the wide variety of motor and cognitive symptoms that have been
observed.
TREATMENT
Neurosurgical Treatment
The tremor and rigidity of Parkinson's disease have been attributed to
a loss of an inhibitory influence within the basal ganglia, which leads to
the release of the inhibition and to an abnormal outflow of the internal
portion of the globus pallidus to the ventral anterior and lateral nuclei of
the thalamus and finally to the motor cortex. Neurosurgical treatment of
PD began in 1930 (Selby, 1967) and involves stereotaxic lesions in the
globus pallidus or in the ventro-lateral thalamus contralateral to the side
of the body that is most affected. This technique apparently decreases the
abnormal activity (disinhibited) and relieves the tremor and rigidity. The
bradykinesia of the patients does not improve, however (possibly because
it is a primary deficit), and for this reason the performance of daily ac-
tivities remains affected. It has been reported that the tremor and rigidity
reappear 1 to 3 years after surgery. In the cognitive area, different distur-
bances appear as a result of the surgery (especially when performed bilat-
erally), such as deterioration in language and in visuoperceptual func-
tions, and severe conceptual and emotional disturbances (Darley, Brown,
& Swenson, 1975; Riklan & Levita, 1970). The success of pharmacological
therapy has decreased the need for this type of surgery.
Pharmacological Treatment
The cornerstone of pharmacological treatment in PD is the use of the
precursor of dopamine, levodopa, a substance that is able to cross the
hematoencephalic barrier and be transformed into dopamine by the
dopaminergic system and, more recently, the use of dopaminergic agonist
agents.
When PD patients are treated with levodopa, an initial improvement
is observed, and then gradually, over the years, the improvement de-
creases. A large number of patients present secondary effects to the action
of the drug, including dyskinesias or abnormal involuntary movements,
"on-off" phenomena characterized by the presence of severe akinesia and
periods of relative mobility, "wearing-off" or "end of dose" phenomena
characterized by an accelerated deterioration in the beneficial effects, and
psychiatric disorders. These side effects are on occasion more severe than
the disease itself.
The conventional treatments used do not alter the course of the ill-
ness since they produce only a temporal symptomatic relief. Currently,
the need exists to develop new therapeutic approaches that will produce
the medication or appropriate procedure that can supply dopamine in
ADRENAL AUTOGRAFTING IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE 203
Tissue Transplants
It is in this context that a line of research arose within the basic
neurosciences in which the treatment of experimental models of Parkin-
son's disease with transplanted dopaminergic neurons has been explored.
That is, an attempt has been made to fulfill all the theoretical assumptions
of the ideal drug, infusing dopamine through the neurons or cells that
synthesize it (Le., the substantia nigra or adrenal medulla) into portions of
the brain deficient in this neurotransmitter.
The capacity of several tissues to survive and grow when they are
transplanted within the central nervous system has been known since
Ram6n y Cajal (1938). The central nervous system is an immunologically
privileged organ with a low capacity for rejection and an ideal place to
transplant autologous or fetal tissue (Barker & Billingham, 1977). It is
thought that in the brain the access to the immune system to foreign
tissues is limited for two reasons: (1) The brain lacks lymphatic vessels
and lymph nodes from which many of the cells of the immune system are
deployed, and (2) the walls of the blood vessels in the central nervous
system are specialized in creating a "blood-brain barrier." The absence of
rejection in the neuronal transplants can also reflect the suitable charac-
teristics possessed by the nervous cells. Many cells on their surface bear
large molecules known as Class I major histocompatibility antigens. Dif-
ferent in each animal, the antigens are molecules that the immune system
recognizes as foreign when it rejects grafted tissue. Few of these antigens
are found in neurons.
The development of experimental models of Parkinson's disease and
the verification of the viability of the monoaminergic neurons on being
transplanted within the central nervous system produced a wide field for
research in neurobiology. In 1971 Ungerstedt (1971a, b) described an ex-
perimental model for Parkinson's disease that consists in the destruc-
tion of neurons from the nigrostriatal system with a selective-6-hydrox-
idopamine neurotoxin. This substance selectively destroys the neurons,
fibers, and terminals that contain catecholamines (for example, dopa-
mine). When the dopamine pathways on both sides of the brain are de-
stroyed, a generalized akinetic syndrome is produced that is reminiscent
of the hypokinesia of Parkinsonism and includes interruption of eating
and drinking habits and death of the animal if it is not given intensive
care. Unilateral lesions of the nigrostriatal pathway result in turning
movements away from the side of the lesions that can be measured quan-
titatively by a rotometer. Following unilateral lesions of the substantia
nigra, the ipsilateral striatum responds to denervation by becoming super-
204 FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS et aI.
Surgical Technique
The surgical procedure involved simultaneous adrenalectomy and
frontal craniotomy. Upon extraction of the adrenal gland under a dissect-
ing microscope, six to eight fragments of adrenal tissue were obtained (0.8
g in total approximately) and placed on a wet surface. Simultaneously, the
caudate nucleus was approached, with the aid of a surgical microscope,
through the lateral ventricle by means of a nontraumatic transcortical
(second frontal gyrus F2). In the head of the caudate nucleus, a 3 x 3 x 3-
mm bed was constructed and the adrenal medullary fragments were
placed within the cavity. Initially, six small fragments were inserted in
the cavity, and then the last two fragments, which were slightly larger,
were anchored to the ependyma of the caudate nucleus with a couple of
stainless-steel miniature staples. In this manner, the inner fragments can-
not dislodge themselves from the cavity, but the cerebrospinal fluid can
bathe all the grafted tissue. In both patients, clinical improvement was
noted at 15 and 6 days (respectively) after implantation and has been
maintained 24 months in the first patient and 18 months in the second.
Rigidity and akinesia were significantly reduced, and functionally both
patients are completely independent in their daily activities and are
working, the first as a farmer and the second as a civil engineer.
Since these results appeared, the autologous graft of adrenal medulla
to the caudate nucleus has been carried out in a larger number of PD
patients who presented severe symptoms of rigidity, tremor, akinesia, and
pronounced on-off effects. Patients have been studied with video, spec-
trophotography, electromyography, neurophysiological, and neuropsy-
chological studies.
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL FINDINGS
aInciudes 95 items from which 195 scores can be obtained, and emphasizes two aspects: (1) quality of the
mistakes-each item is scored according to one or several criteria and not simply according to whether
the subject performed the task or not, and (2) a simple quantification is carried out under three categories
for each criterion-namely, normal performance; regular performance, moderately anomalous; and im-
possible performance. Hence, the poorer the subject's performance, the higher his score.
Sample
Tests
a 90 b ------------------90
80------------------ --------------------80
U) -. .~.
1,&.1 70~ C>. l'"
~ '<7-...... ~ ~A\\
~ 60 7_", (
-
~....<;",.-d-- 3--
I
~ 50----------------------
40~'~--~~--~~~--~~ , , 40
I II III IV V VI VII VIII IX II III IV V \,1 VII VIII IX
--TEST - PREOPERATIVE
•••••• RETEST •••••• POSTOPERATIVE 'Tl
trl
C"l
* P < 0.05 C"l
>-<:
FIGURE 3. Mean scores of the neuropsychological diagnostic scheme obtained by (a) the control unoperated PD oen
group (n = 10) in the test-retest condition and by (b) the operated PO group (n = 16) in the pre- and postopera-
tive evaluation. The dark lines show the mean and the limits of 2 standard deviations. Higher scores reflect ~
en
greater number of errors than average and lower scores reflect fewer numbers of errors. Level of significance of 7<:
>-<:
postoperative improvement is indicated, as well as mean values and standard deviation"s for the descriptive cJ:,
characteristics for the two groups. I, Motor functions; II, somatosensory knowledge; III, visuoperceptual and o
visuospatial recognition; IV, auditory knowledge and language; V, cognitive processes; VI, oral language; VII, C
en
reading; VIII, writing; IX, calculus. ~
e..
ADRENAL AUTOGRAFTING IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE 211
X
SAMPLE
m1EB]
mm
TESTS
V ISU AL
MEMORY
TASK
t \ '\ I
I7
PRE -OP
? I
<::
POST - OP
(91 days)
the surgery. One of the patients had permanent damage in the septum,
causing an accentuation of cognitive difficulties, and the patient's disori-
entation, inattention inertia, and lack of initiative increased. In another
case, after several postsurgical complications, the patient developed an
encephalopathy that severely affected all cognitive functions .
In the immediate postoperative period, six patients presented visual
and one auditory hallucinations that cleared spontaneously within 72
hours after the operation. Patients were aware of these hallucinations, in
general were not frightened by them, and could speak about them without
anxiety. The content of the hallucination varied in each patient. For ex-
ample, one patient with hallucinations saw insects on the wall and an-
other often heard the telephone ringing. Mental confusion was observed
in five cases and perseveration in motor and verbal tasks in six. This
behavior disappeared gradually within 2 to 4 weeks after surgery.
212 FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS et 01.
GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS
striatum and premotor and prefrontal cortex (complex loop) have been
described.
A variable involvement of the dopaminergic system could be the
basis of the heterogeneity in the motor signs and the cognitive profile
obtained during the preoperative evaluation, as well as in the differential
response to the autografting procedure.
Several investigators have reported a higher frequency of cognitive
impairment in patients with more bradykinesia and rigidity. Mortimer et
al. (1982) proposed two clinical forms of idiopathic PD-one with pre-
dominant bradykinesia and cognitive impairment, and the other with
predominant tremor and relatively intact function. Lieberman (1974) re-
ported less prominent tremor in PD patients with dementia, and Zetusky,
Jankovic, and Pirozzolo (1985) reported a significant association between
deterioration in mental status with bradykinesia, postural instability, and
gait difficulty, whereas tremor was associated with a relative preservation
of mental status and less functional impairment. Bernheimer, Birkmayer,
Hornykiewiez, Jellinger, and Seitelberger (1973) found that regional
chemical changes were correlated with the type and degree of clinical
manifestations. For example, severity of akinesia correlates best with
dopamine and HVA deficiency in the caudate nucleus, whereas the de-
gree of tremor paralleled severity of HVA deficiency in the pallidum, and
rigidity was not related to any specific regional distribution of dopamine
or HVA deficiency.
Within the caudate, different cognitive functions are subserved by
different regions. In PD, the maximal dopamine reduction is observed in
the anterodorsal head of the caudate, which is the area that has greatest
connection with the frontal cortex (Rosvold, 1972). In subhuman pri-
mates, experimental lesion in the anterodorsal and ventral part of the
head of the caudate causes difficulties in tasks that require response-
inhibition and produce perseveration, difficulty in shifting responses,
and deficits in behavioral programming (Cools, 1980; Johnston, Rosvold,
& Mishkin, 1968; Rosvold, 1972; Teuber & Proctor, 1964), and, in contrast,
lesions in the tail of the caudate produced deficits in visual discrimina-
tion (Divac, Rosvold, & Szwarcbart, 1967). It could be that in our study, in
the patients who show abnormal preoperative profiles, the disease af-
fected the caudate nucleus, and thus the dopamine concentration in this
structure is decreased. It is worth noting that the adrenal medullary frag-
ments are grafted (within the lateral ventricle with partial inclusion) with-
in the head of the caudate, and therefore the supply of dopamine to this
region could explain the positive cognitive effects after the autograft. The
possibility also exists that behavioral recovery is induced through the
regeneration of fibers and dopamine-containing cells of the meso-cortical
dopaminergic system.
214 FEGGY OSTROSKY -SOLIS et 01.
REFERENCES
Agid, Y., Ruberg, M., Dubois, B., Pillon, 8., Cusimano, G., Raisman, R, Cash, R, Lhermitte,
F., & Javoy-Agid, F. (1986). Parkinson's disease and dementia. Clinical Neurophar-
macology, 9, 522-536.
Agid, Y., Javoy-Agid, F., & Ruberg, M. (1987). Biochemistry of neurotransmitters in Parkin-
son's disease. In C. D. Marsden & S. Fhan (Eds.), Movement disorders (Vol. 2, pp. 166-
230). London: Butterworths.
Albert, M. 1., Feldman, R G., & Willis, A. L. (1974). The subcortical dementia of progressive
supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 371, 121-130.
Backlund, E., Granberg, P., Hamberger, B., Knutson, E., Martenson, A., Sedvall, G., Seiger,
A., & Olson, 1. (1985). Transplantation of adrenal medullary tissue to the striatum in
Parkinsonism. Journal of Neurosurgery, 62, 169-173.
Barker, C. F., & Billingham, R E. (1977). Immunologically privileged sites. Advances in
Immunology, 25, 1-54.
Bernheimer, H., Birkmayer, W., Hornykiewicz, 0., Jellinger, K., & Seitelberger, F. (1973).
ADRENAL AUTOGRAFTING IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE 215
Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington: Clinical, mor-
phological and neurochemical correlations. Journal of Neurological Science, 20, 415-
455.
Bjorklund, A., Dunnett, S. 8., Stenevi, U., Lewis, M. E., & Iversen, S. D. (1980). Reinnervation
of the denervated striatum by substantia nigra transplants: Functional consequences as
revealed by pharmacological and sensoriomotor testing. Brain Research, 199,307-333.
Bjorklund, A., & Lindwall, O. (1978). The meso-telencephalic dopamine system: A review of
its anatomy. In K. Livingston & O. Hornykiewicz (Eds.), Limbic mechanisms (pp. 307-
331). New York: Plenum Press.
Bohn, M. C., Marciano, F., Cupit, L., & Gash, D. M. (1987). Adrenal medulla grafts promote
recovery of striatal dopaminergic fibers in MPTP treated mice. Science, 247, 913-915.
Boller, F., Passafiume, D., Keefe, N. C., Rogers, K., Morrow, 1., & Kim, Y. (1980). Visuospatial
impairment in Parkinson's disease. Archives of Neurology, 41, 485-490.
Brozoski, T. J., Brown, R. M., Rosvold, H. E., & Goldman, P. S. (1979). Cognitive deficit
caused by regional depletion of dopamina in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey.
Science, 205, 929-932.
Carlsson, A., Lindquest, M., & Magnusson, T. (1957). 3-4-dihydroxyphenylalanine and 5-
hydroxytroptophan as reserpine antagonists. Nature, 180, 1200.
Cools, A. (1980). Role of the neostriatal dopaminergic activity in sequencing and selecting
behavioral strategies: Facilitation of processes involved in selecting the best strategy in a
stressful situation. Behavioral Brain Research, 1, 361-378.
Cotzias, G. C., Papavasiliov, P. S., & Gellene, R. (1969). Modification of Parkinson chronic
treatment with L-dopa. New England Journal of Medicine, 280, 337-345.
Darley, F., Brown, J., & Swenson, W. (1975). Language changes after neurosurgery for Parkin-
sonism. Brain and Language, 2, 65-69.
DeLong, M. (1974). Motor functions of the basal ganglia single unit activity during move-
ment. In F. D. Schmitt & F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences (pp. 319-325). Third
study program. Cambridge, MA: M.LT. Press.
DeLong, M. R., Georgopulos, A. P., & Crutcher, M. D. (1983). Corti co-basal ganglia relations
and coding of motor performance. In Neural coding of motor performance. Experimen-
tal brain research (pp. 30-40). Berlin: Springer.
Divac, L, Rosvold, H., & Szwarcbart, M. (1967). Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the
caudate nucleus. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 63, 184-190.
Drucker-Colin, R., Madrazo, L, Ostrosky-Solis, F., Shkurovich, M., Franco, R., & Torres, C.
(1988). Adrenal medullary tissue transplants in the caudate nucleus of Parkinson pa-
tients. In J. Sladek & D. Gash (Eds.), Transplants in the central nervous system. Program
in brain research. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Dubois, B., Danze, F., Pillon, B., Cusimano, J., Lhermitte, F., Agid, Y. (1987). Cholinergic
dependent cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Annals of Neurology, 22, 26-30.
Dubois, B., Ruberg, M., JavoY-Agid, F., Pluska, A., & Agid, Y. (1983). A subcortical cho-
linergic system is affected in Parkinson's disease. Brain Research, 288, 213-218.
Dunnett, S. B., Bjorklund, A., & Stenevi, U. (1983). Dopamine-rich transplants in experimen-
tal parkinsonism. Trends in Neuroscience, 6, 266-270.
Dunnett, S. B., Bjorklund, A., Stenevi, U., & Iversen, S. D. (1981a). Behavioral recovery
following transplantation of substantia nigra in rats subjected to 6-0HDA lesions of the
nigrostriatal pathway. Brain Research, 215, 147-161.
Dunnett, S. B., Bjorklund, A., Stenevi, U., & Iversen, S. D. (1981b). Grafts of embryonic
substantia nigra reinnervating the ventrolateral striatum ameliorate sensoriomotor im-
pairments and akinesia in rats with 6-0HDA lesions of the nigrostriatal pathway. Brain
Research, 229, 209-207.
Ehringer, H., Hornykiewicz, O. (1960). Verteilung, Von. Noradrenalin und Dopamin (3-
Hydroxytyramin) im Gehirn des Menschen und ihr Verhalten bei Erkrankungen des
Extrapyramidalen Systems. Klinische Wochenschrift, 38, 1236-1239.
216 FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS et al.
Elizan, T. S., Sroka, H., Maker, H., Smith, H., & Yahr, M. D. (1986). Dementia in idiophatic
Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neural Transmission, 65, 285-302.
Farley, L J., Price, K. S., & Hornykiewicz, O. (1978). Monominergic systems in the human
limbic brain. In K. Livingston & O. Hornykiewicz (Eds.)' Limbic mechanisms (pp. 333-
349). New York: Plenum Press.
Forno, L. S., Langston, J. W., Delanney, 1. E., Irwin, L, & Ricaurte, G. A. (1986). Locus
ceruleus lesions and eosinophilic inclusions in MPTP-treated monkeys. Annals of Neu-
rology, 20, 449-455.
Freed, W. J. (1983). Functional brain tissue transplantation: Reversal of lesion-induced rota-
tion by intraventricular substantia nigra and adrenal medulla grafts with a note on
intracranial retinal grafts. Biology and Psychiatry, 18, 1205-1267.
Freed, W. J., Morihisa, J. M., Spoor, E., Hoffer, B. J., Olson, L., Seiger, A, & Wyatt, R J. (1981).
Transplanted adrenal chromaffin cells in rat brain reduce lesion-indm:ed rotational
behavior. Nature, 292, 351-352.
Freeman, M., & Albert, M. 1. (1985). Subcortical dementia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.),
Handbook of clinical neurology. Vol. 46: Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 1049-1052).
Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Gash, D., Bohn, M., Jiao, S., Fiandaca, M., Okawara, S., Kordower, J., Hansen, J., Notter, M.,
Snyder, J., Marciano, F., Schwarz, H., & Shoulson, L (1987, October 18). Adrenal
medullary implantation promotes recovery of tyrosine hydroxylase immunoreactivity
in host striatum of MPTP animal models of parkinsonism. Paper presented at the Sym-
posium on Etiology, Pathogenesis and Prevention of Parkinson's Disease, San Francisco.
Gasar, P., & Gray, F. (1984). Dementia in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Acta Neu-
ropathologica, 64, 43-54.
Hakim, A H., & Mathieson, G. (1979). Dementia in Parkinson's disease: A neuropathological
study. Neurology, 29, 1209-1214.
Javoy-Agid, F., & Agid, Y. (1980). Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in
Parkinson disease? Neurology, 30, 1326-1330.
Javoy-Agid, F., Ruberg, M., Taquet, H., Bukobza, B., Agid, Y., & Gaspar, P. (1984). Bio-
chemical neuropathology of Parkinson's disease. Advances in Neurology, 40,189-198.
Johnston, T. N., Rosvold, H. E., & Mishkin, M. (1968). Proyections from behaviorally defined
sectors of the prefrontal cortex to the basal ganglia, septum and diencephalon of the
monkeys. Experimental Neurology, 21, 20-30.
Langston, J. W., Ballard, P., Tetrud, J. W., & Irwin, L (1983). Chronic parkinsonism in human
due to a product of meperidine-analog synthesis. Science, 219, 979-980.
Lees, A. J., & Smith, E. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the early stages of Parkinson's disease.
Brain, 106,257-270.
Lieberman, A. N. (1974). Parkinson's disease: A clinical review. American Journal of Medi-
cal Science, 267, 66-80.
Lieberman, A., Dziatolowski, M., Kupersmith, M., Serby, M., Goodgold, A., Korein, J., &
Goldstein, M. (1979). Dementia in Parkinson's disease. Annals of Neurology, 6, 355-
359.
Lindvall, 0., Backlund, E., Farde, L., Freedman, R, Hoffer, B., & Seiger, A. (1987). Transplan-
tation in Parkinson's disease: Two cases of adrenal medullary grafts to the putamen.
Annals of Neurology, 22, 457-468.
Luria, A. R. (1977). Las funciones corticales superiores en el hombre. La Habana: Orbe.
Madrazo, L, Drucker-Colin, R, Diaz, V., Martinez-Mata, J., Torres, C., & Becerril, J. (1987).
Open microsurgical autograft of adrenal medulla to the right caudate nucleus in two
patients with intractable Parkinson's disease. New England Journal of Medicine, 316,
831-834.
Matison, R, Mayeux, R, Rosen, J., & Fahn, S. (1982). Tip of the tongue phenomenon in
Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 32, 567-570.
ADRENAL AUTOGRAFTING IN PARKINSON'S DISEASE 217
Mortimer, J. A., Pirozzolo, F. J., Hansch, E. C., & Webster, D. D. (1982). Relationship of motor
symptoms to intellectual deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 32, 133-137.
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Canseco, E., Quintanar, L., Navarro, E., Meneses, S., & Ardila, A. (1985).
Sociocultural effects in neuropsychological assessment. International Journal of Neu-
roscience, 27, 53-66.
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Quintanar, L., Madrazo, I., Drucker-Colin, R, Franco-Bourland, R, &
Leon-Meza, V. (1988). Neuropsychological effects of brain autograft of adrenal medull-
ary tissue for the treatment of Parkinson's disease. Neurology, 38, 1442-1450.
Ostrosky-Solis, F., Quintanar, L., Meneses, S., Canseco, E., Navarro, E., & Ardila, A. (1986).
Actividad cognoscitiva y nivel sociocultural. Revista de Investigacion Clinica, 38, 37-
42.
Perlow, M. J. (1987). Brain grafting as a treatment for Parkinson's disease. Neurosurgery, 20,
335-342.
Perlow, M. J., Freed, W. J., Haffer, B. J., Seiger, A., Olson, L., & Wyatt, R J. (1979). Brain grafts
reduce motor abnormalities produced by destruction of nigrostriatal dopamine system.
Science, 204, 635-645.
Pirozzolo, F. J., Hansch, C., & Mortimer, J. A. (1982). Dementia in Parkinson's disease:
Neuropsychological analysis. Brain and Cognition, 1, 71-83.
Ramon y CajaI, S. (1938). Degeneration and regeneration of the nervous system. London:
Oxford University Press.
Riklan, M., & Levita, E. (1970). Psychological studies of thalamic lesions in humans. Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 150, 251-265.
Rinne, U. K. (1982). Brain neurotransmitter receptors in Parkinson's disease. In C. D. Mar-
sden & S. Pahn (Eds.), Movement disorders (pp. 59-74). London: Butterworths.
Rosvold, H. E. (1972). The frontal lobe system: Cortical subcortical interrelationships. Acta
Neurobiologica Experimentalis, Warsaw, 32, 439-460.
Scatton, B., Rouquier, L., Javoy-Agid, F., & Agid, Y. (1982). Dopamine deficiency in the
cerebral cortex in Parkinson disease. Neurology, 32, 1039-1040.
Selby, G. (1967). Stereotactic surgery for the relief of Parkinson's disease. Part I. A critical
review. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 5, 315-342.
Stern, Y., & Langston, J. W. (1985). Intellectual changes in patients with MPTP induced
Parkinsonism. Neurology, 35, 1506-1509.
Taylor, A. E., Saint-Cyr, J. A., & Lang, A. E. (1986). Frontal lobe dysfunction in Parkinson's
disease. The cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. Brain, 109, 845-883.
Taylor, A. E., Saint-Cyr, J. A., & Lang, A. E. (1987). Parkinson's disease: Cognitive changes in
relation to treatment response. Brain, 110,35-51.
Teuber, H., & Proctor, F. (1964). Some effects of basal ganglia lesions in subhuman primates
and man. Neuropsychologia, 2, 85-93.
UhI, G. R, Hedreen, J. C., & Price, D. L. (1985). Parkinson's disease: Loss of neurons from the
ventral tegmental area contralateral to therapeutic surgical lesions. Neurology, 35,
1215-1218.
Ungerstedt, U. (1971a). Adipsia and aphagia after 6-hydroxydopamine induced degeneration
of the nigro-striatal dopamine system. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Supplementum,
367, 95-122.
Ungerstedt, U. (1971b). Striatal dopamine release after amphetamine or nerve degeneration
revealed by rotational behavior. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica Supplementum, 367,
49-68.
Unsicker, K., Rieffert, B., & Ziegler, W. (1980). Effects of cell culture condition, nerve growth
factor, dexamethasone and cyclic AMP on adrenal chromaffin cells in vitro. In O. Eran-
ko, S. Soinila, & H. Paivarento (Eds.), Histochemistry and cell biology of autonomic
neurons, SIF cells, and paraneurons (pp. 51-59). New York: Raven Press.
Villardita, T., Smirni, P., Le Pera, F., Zappala, G., & Nicoletti. F. (1982). Mental deterioration,
218 FEGGY OSTROSKY-SOLIS et a1.
vi suo perceptive disability and constructional apraxia in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neu-
ro1ogica Scandinavica, 66, 112-120.
Wurtman, R. J., Pohorecky, 1. A., & Baliga, B. S. (1972). Adrenocortical control of the
biosynthesis of epinephrine and protein in the adrenal medulla. Pharmacological Re-
view, 24, 411-426.
Zetusky, W., Jankovic, J., & Pirozzolo, F. (1985). The heterogeneity of Parkinson's disease:
Clinical and prognostic implications. Neurology, 35, 522-526.
11
The search for the mechanisms involved in memory storage has yielded
an enormous amount of experimental data. To date, however, we are far
from understanding how the nervous system integrates the relevant infor-
mation derived from experience, and how that information is channeled
to effector systems when the same, or a similar, experience occurs.
Recent work has provided evidence strongly suggesting that memory
storage depends not upon the workings of a single neuroanatomicallneu-
rochemical system but, rather, upon sequential activation of different sys-
tems (for a review, see Prado-Alcala, 1985). What follows is a brief ac-
count of the experimental data that have given support to this hypothesis.
219
220 ROBERTO A. PRADO-ALCALA
Winocur, 1974), as well as other methods that interfere with its neuronal
activity, such as electric stimulation and topical application of potassium
chloride (Le Piane & Phillips, 1978; Prado-Alcala & Cobos-Zapiain, 1979;
Prado-Alcala, Grinberg, Alvarez-Leefmans, & Brust-Cammona, 1973;
Prado-Alcala et a1., 1975; Prado-Alcala, Kaufmann, & Moscona, 1980;
Wyers & Deadwyler, 1971; Wyers, Deadwyler, Hirasuna, & Montgomery,
1973; Wyers, Peeke, Elliston, & Herz, 1968).
Although those studies indicated that normal functioning of the CN is
important for the establishment of memory, they did not provide clues
about the nature of the mechanisms implicated in such function, but were
instrumental in the designing of experiments aimed at defining the neu-
rochemical events that may occur during learning and performance of
conditioned behaviors. Thus, it was shown that alterations in the activity
of striatal dopamine (Kim & Routtenberg, 1976a; Phillips & Clouston,
1978; Stabuli & Huston, 1978), GABA (Salado-Castillo & Prado-Alcala,
1987), and acetylcholine (Haycock, Deadwyler, Sideroff, & McGaugh,
1973; Neill & Grossman, 1970; Prado-Alcala et a1., 1972) also produce
significant memory impairments.
Since dopamine, GABA, and acetylcholine are the main neurotrans-
mitters regulating the activity of the nigro-neostriatal system, the data
suggested that the nigro-neostriatal system is critically involved in memo-
ry functions. Several lines of evidence give support to this idea: Blockade
of GABAergic activity of the substantia nigra (Cobos-Zapiain & Prado-
Alcala, 1986; Kim & Routtenberg, 1976b) and combined treatment of a
dopaminergic blocker with intrastriatal atropine (Rivas-Arancibia &
Prado-Alcala, 1986) result in amnesia.
In related studies it has been shown that when rats and cats are
trained to bar-press until they reach asymptotic performance and then
injected into the CN with a high concentration of potassium chloride, they
show a significant retention deficit. Again, when training is extended, the
same treatment does not produce retention deficits (Prado-Alcala &
Cobos-Zapiain, 1979; Prado-Alcala, Kaufmann, & Moscona, 1980).
The results reported above strongly indicated that the CN is critically
involved in the acquisition and early maintenance stages of positively
reinforced instrumental conditioning, and that this structure is not en-
gaged in memory after overtraining. It was reasoned that if this represents
a general way of functioning of the striatum, equivalent effects should be
produced when animals are trained in other types of tasks.
As stated above, the striatum plays a very important role in the con-
solidation of memory of one-trial passive avoidance, a negatively rein-
forced task. This task was used to determine whether the overtraining
effect could be generalized further.
Overtraining, as studied in prior experiments, involved multiple
training sessions, a high number of positive reinforcers, and a prolonged
exposure to the experimental situation. In the case of one-trial passive
avoidance there is only one training session and application of only one
reinforcer, and the duration of the trial is brief. For the designing of the
experiments to be described below, it was decided to vary the magnitude
of only one of these parameters (the reinforcer), testing different inten-
sities of the foot shock. This manipulation is equivalent to having differ-
ent amounts of positive reinforcers.
In one experiment, the effects of posttraining injection of atropine
into the anterodorsal aspect of the striatum on retention of passive avoid-
ance were assessed. Independent groups of rats were given different levels
of foot shock (0.25, 0.50, or 1.00 mAl during training. As expected, retro-
grade amnesia was seen only in the 0.25-mA group (Giordano & Prado-
Alcala, 1986).
In order to determine whether some striatal neurochemical system,
other than cholinergic, became engaged in consolidating memory after
overtraining, groups of rats were trained, using also low, medium, and
high foot shock intensities, and then given intra striatal injections of Xilo-
caine, thus arresting all normal neural actiyity of the injected area. It was
reasoned that if consolidation was dependent upon the integrative func-
tions of the striatum, an amnesic state would be produced, regardless of
the intensity of the foot shock-Le., regardless of overtraining. In agree-
ment with the studies reported above, those rats that were submitted to
overtraining had as good retention scores as intact animals (Perez-Ruiz &
Prado-Alcala, 1986).
With regard to active avoidance, there are only a few relevant studies
that could bear on this problem; however, these studies were not designed
to study the overtraining effect, and interpretation of these results should
THE STRIATUM AS A MEMORY STORE 223
CONCLUSIONS
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Phillips, A. G., & Clouston, R (1978). Disruption of one-trial appetitive learning and passive
avoidance following stimulation of the substantia nigra pars compacta. Behavioral Biol-
ogy, 23, 388-394.
Polgar, S., Sanberg, P. R, & Kirkby, R J. (1981). Is the striatum involved in passive avoidance
behavior? A commentary. Physiological Psychology, 9, 354-358.
Prado-Alcala, R A. (1985). Is cholinergic activity of the caudate nucleus involved in memo-
ry? Life Sciences, 37, 2135-2142.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Bermudez-RaUoni, F., Velazquez-Martinez, D., & Bacha, M. G. (1978).
Cholinergic blockade of the caudate nucleus and spatial alternation performance in rats:
Overtraining-induced protection against behavioral deficits. Life Sciences, 23, 889-896.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Cepeda- G., Verduzco, L., Jimenez, A., & Vargas-Ortega, E. (1984). Effects
of cholinergic stimulation of the caudate nucleus on active avoidance. Neuroscience
Letters, 51, 31-36.
Prado-Alcala, R A., & Cobos-Zapiain, G. G. (1977). Learning deficits induced by cholinergic
blockade of the caudate nudeus as a function of experience. Brain Research, 138, 190-
196.
Prado-Alcala, R A., & Cobos Zapiain, G. G. (1979). Interference with caudate nucleus ac-
tivity by potassium chloride. Evidence for a "moving" engram. Brain Research, 172,
577-583.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Cruz-Morales, S. E., & L6pez-Miro, F. A. (1980). Differential effects of
cholinergic blockade of anterior and posterior caudate nucleus on avoidance behaviors.
Neuroscience Letters 18, 339-345.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Fernandez-Samblancat, M., & Solodkin-Herrera, M. (1985). Injections of
atropine into the caudate nucleus impair the acquisition and the maintenance of passive
avoidance. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 22, 243-247.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Grinberg, Z. J., Alvarez-Leefmans, F. J., & Brust-Carmona, H. (1973).
Suppression of motor conditioning by the injection of 3M KCl in the caudate nuclei of
cats. Physiology and Behavior, 10, 59-64.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Grinberg, Z. J., Alvarez-Leefmans, F. J., G6mez, A., Singer, S., & Brust-
Carmona, H. (1972). A possible caudate-cholinergic mechanism in two instrumental
conditioned responses. Psychopharmacologia (Berlin), 25, 339-346.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Grinberg, Z. J., Arditii, Z. L., Garcia, M. M., Prieto, H. G., & Brust-
Carmona, H. (1975). Learning deficits produced by chronic and reversible lesions of the
corpus striatum in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 15, 283-287.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Kaufmann, P., & Moscona, R. (1980). Scopolamine and KCl injections
into the caudate-putamen. Overtraining-induced protection against deficits of learning.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 12, 249-253.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Signoret, L., & Figueroa, M. (1981). Time-dependent retention deficits
induced by post-training injections of atropine into the caudate nucleus. Pharmacology,
Biochemistry and Behavior, 15, 633-636.
Prado-Alcala, R A., Signoret-Edward, L., Figueroa, M., & Barrientos, M. A. (1984). Post-trial
injection of atropine into the caudate nucleus interferes with long-term, but not with
short-term retention of passive avoidance. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 42, 81-84.
Rivas-Arancibia, S., & Prado-Alcala, R A. (1986). Nucleo caudado y aprendizaje. XXN.
Interacci6n entre los sistemas dopaminergico y colinergico en procesos de memoria.
XxIX Congreso Nacional de Ciencias Fisiol6gicas, Mexico.
Salado-Castillo, R., & Prado-Alcala, R A. (1987). Effects of picrotoxin injections into differ-
ent regions of the striatum on retention of passive avoidance. Society for Neuroscience
Abstracts, 13, 657.
Sanberg, P. R, Lehmann, ]., & Fibiger, H. C. (1978). Impaired learning and memory after
kainic acid lesions of the striatum: A behavioral model of Huntington's disease. Brain
Research, 149, 546-551.
226 ROBERTO A. PRADO-ALCALA
Sanberg, P. R., Pisa, M., & Fibiger, H. C. (1979). Avoidance, operant and locomotor behavior
in rats with neostriatal injections of kainic acid. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Be-
havior, 10, 137-144.
Stabuli, U., & Huston, J. P. (1978). Effects of post-trial reinforcing vs. subreinforcing stimula-
tion of the substantia nigra on passive avoidance learning. Brain Research Bulletin, 3,
519-524.
Winocur, G. J. (1974). Functional dissociation within the caudate nucleus of rats. Journal ot
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 432-439.
Wyers, E. J., & Deadwyler, S. A. (1971). Duration and nature of retrograde amnesia produced
by stimulation of caudate nucleus. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 97-103.
Wyers, E. J., Deadwyler, S. A., Hirasuna, N., & Montgomery, D. (1973). Passive avoidance
retention and caudate stimulation. Physiology and Behavior, 11,809-819.
Wyers, E., J., Peeke, H. V. S., Elliston, J. S., & Herz, M. J. (1968). Retroactive impairment of
passive avoidance learning by stimulation of the caudate nucleus. Experimental Neu-
rology, 22, 350-366.
v
DONALD G. STEIN· Dean of the Graduate School and Associate Provost for Research.
Rutgers University, Newark, New Jersey 07102.
229
230 DONALD C. STEIN
Given this set of beliefs and attitudes, it makes sense that, in many
laboratories, experimental lesions are created, employed, and controlled
in such a way as to guarantee that an observable deficit will occur. In fact,
this would be the most reasonable approach for those concerned with the
mapping of behavioral functions onto increasingly discrete brain regions,
zones, or even specific "command or control" neurons. But what if ex-
perimental conditions can be manipulated in such a way that the same
extent of injury, created in a different way, leads to behavioral sparing or
marked reduction in the impairment? Thus, if such outcomes were possi-
ble, the "constancy" of the lesion effect would be challenged and a new
set of "contextual" factors would have to be considered in determining
the development of events that will follow the injury.
Workers with a heavily vested interest in cerebral mapping and neu-
ronal specificity are not happy with such a conceptual (or contextual)
approach to the study of cerebral organization, and one can readily under-
stand why. If, as we shall try to show, the cerebral circuitry underlying
behavioral events can shift as a function of environmental, hormonal, and
other organismic events, map making becomes very difficult at best. Pic-
ture what it might be like to have a state map where the locations of cities
and towns (and the highways to and from them) would shift from day to
day as a function of economic conditions, the weather, population shifts,
famine, and other factors. Yet, in geography, such realities do occur, albeit
perhaps more slowly. Nonetheless, following a catastrophe (earthquake,
war, flooding), rapid and massive change could occur as new "centers"
and paths develop to replace those that were lost. Can such contextually
induced plasticity occur at the level of the individual central nervous
system following the equivalent catastrophe of stroke, trauma, or pen-
etrating injury? Is the hard-wired nature of the nervous system an inher-
ent characteristic of the brain itself, or is it more a characteristic of the
particular beliefs and attitudes we have developed to study it? We will
need to return to this point many times as we proceed with this essay.
When we talk about brain injury or plasticity, we must keep in mind
that these events (or processes) do not occur in a static system. Injury and
subsequent change can be seen as a dynamic series of events in which
many different variables playa role in determining the outcome of injury.
Table 1, taken from a recent paper by Geschwind (1985), shows a number
of specific and nonspecific changes that occur after lesions of the nervous
system. Anyone of these parameters could be the subject of a lifetime of
research. It is easy to understand why, in the face of so many variables,
one would be tempted to focus upon one or two factors and ignore the
others.
Most research in neuroscience takes the approach that given parts of
the central nervous system have highly specific structure-function rela-
tionships. In this view, each neuron, each organ of the brain is presumed
232 DONALD G. STEIN
1psychologists often criticize their colleagues in the neurosciences for what sometimes
seems to be a highly demonstrable lack of interest in what the nervous system does with
respect to behavior. However, in all fairness to those in the neurosciences, there are many
psychologists who feel there is nothing to be learned by developing a better understanding
of how the nervous system works. This "black box" approach to the brain is still very much
with us in academic and clinical psychology. Neither level of parochialism can ultimately
benefit our understanding and treatment of neural and behavioral pathologies.
236 DONALD G. STEIN
the presence of so many "free" terminals are not compatible with the
view that the frontal circuitry is fixed and static. Instead they suggested
that in living tissue there is an ongoing process of synaptic shifting and
relocation that cannot be seen when tissue is examined postmortem.
If this type of plasticity does occur in the adult eNS, then it is possi-
ble to think that synaptic morphology and function will be altered by the
organism's experience. 2
If we can agree that behavioral manipulation can have a direct effect
on eNS morphology, can such manipulations alter the outcome of brain
injury? If, for example, environmental "enrichment" can enhance recov-
ery or sparing of function after injury, under what conditions, 'then, is it
appropriate to "specify" structure-function relationships in the central
nervous system? Here one might see environment or training as part of the
developmental process contributing to the dynamic reorganization of
eNS morphology and function. Anatomical measurements taken from
animals exposed to different environments have shown that enriched
conditions can result in greater cortical thickness and more complex syn-
aptic profiles in comparison with those raised under isolated conditions.
Kelche and Will (1982), in a follow-up to their observations of behavioral
recovery, demonstrated that hippocampal lesions can produce a reduc-
tion in the number of dendritic spines of pyramidal neurons in the oc-
cipital cortex. However, the number of spines was increased if the oper-
ated rats were maintained in complex environments. A thorough review
of the issues surrounding the effects of differential housing and outcome
of brain injury has been prepared by Dalyrimple-Alford and Kelche
(1985).
Here again, the summary point to be made is that the context in
which injury (or any other ongoing event) occurs cannot be overlooked in
developing a theory of nervous system specificity of plasticity.
look at one fixed period of time to examine (and predict) the outcome of
brain injury. In one of the last papers of Norman Geschwind (1985) dis-
cussing the mechanisms of change after brain injury, he said that "there
must be many cases in which the capacity for recovery is latent and
revealed only by some further manipulation, but experimenters have only
rarely been zealous in their search for the right maneuver" (emphasis
added). Geschwind argued that the time needed for recovery in adults
may be much longer than in infants. He went on to say that "most neu-
rologists are gloomy about the prognosis of severe adult aphasia after a
few weeks, and pessimism is reinforced by lack of prolonged follow-up in
most cases." He then said: "I have, however, seen patients severely ap-
hasic for over a year who then made excellent recoveries: one patient
returning to work as a salesman, the other as a psychiatrist. Furthermore,
there are patients who continue to improve over many years. For example,
a patient whose aphasia was still quite evident six years after onset clear-
ed substantially by eighteen years. Change after damage may show great
individual variation" (p. 3). The concept of delayed recovery is yet an-
other example of the "plasticity" that needs to be explained in developing
an appropriate conceptual model of nervous system organization. There is
no rule saying that, if recovery is to occur, it has to be immediate! One of
the tasks of our laboratory and of the many students who have worked in
it with me is to find a way to promote recovery as rapidly as possible, to
get it to occur in a shorter period of time than the 6 or 18 years mentioned
by Geschwind. Very often, as Geschwind indicated, we tend not to look
for recovery if it has not occurred for a very long period of time. Thus, we
can often miss the fact that the processes that underlie central nervous
system plasticity may take far longer than we have previously assumed.
We should also not overlook the fact that the "potential for plasticity"
may be present but blocked by ongoing processes inimical to its man-
ifestation. The focus in research, then, is to unlock those neural events
that can lead to functional recovery even though the time course may be
far longer than we would like to imagine.
functional recovery after spinal cord damage in newborn and adult cats.
Over the last several years, Barbara Bregman and Michael Goldberger
(1983a,b) have been examining both the behavioral and anatomical corre-
lates of recovery in cats with spinal cord injuries inflicted early in life or
at maturity. First, Bregman and Goldberger carefully analyzed motor pat-
tern recovery after hemisections of the spinal cord in newborn or adult
animals. The cats' movements were filmed and quantified, and, although
both groups showed recovery of function, the results indicated that the
recovery was not "uniform"; some motor behaviors were better after adult
injury, while some recovered better when the damage was inflicted early
in life (Bregman & Goldberger, 1983b).
For instance, tactile placing was completely abolished after spinal
cord hemisection in adults, but no loss of hind limb placing occurred
when the lesions were made in newborn animals. In contrast, accurate
foot placement during locomotion was much better in adult operated
animals, as was hopping with the foot opposite to the side of the lesion.
Bregman and Goldberger suggested that recovery or sparing of function
occur when the lesions are made prior to the time that the motor reflexes
begin to develop in normal animals. The better recovery in cats damaged
as adults is thought to be due to the possibility that there may be more
(neural) sources (spinal and supraspinal) that could participate in the
adjustments needed for good locomotion.
To support these speculations, Bregman and Goldberger (1982,
1983a,b) carefully examined the anatomical changes that might account
for the sparing or failure of motor function they observed in their injured
cats. As in the visual system, lesions in the immature spinal cord lead to
the development of aberrant neuronal fiber paths, which could mediate
some degree of recovery (or maladaptive behavior).
Using retrograde neuronal marker techniques (HRP), the authors ex-
amined histopathology in cats with spinal lesions at 1 day of birth or as
adults. The anatomical results supported the complexity of their behav-
ioral data. First, whether lesions were made in neonates or adults had no
effect on sparing of brainstem-spinal fibers-there was no "plasticity"
here. In addition, neonatal lesions led to much greater retrograde degener-
ation in this path than did similar lesions created in adult cats. In con-
trast, corticospinal projections were dramatically spared, but "only in the
neonatal group." There were also anomalous projections in the latter
group that were not seen in the former (corticospinal neurons reached
lumbar segments of the cord by an alternate course). Bregman and Gold-
berger emphasize that tactile placing is completely abolished after adult
lesions, but completely spared after neonatal injury. One can infer, then,
that the functional sparing is the result of the "anatomical plasticity of
(this new) corticospinal projection." The sparing and development of the
anomalous path is likely due to the relatively late arrival of this pathway
DEVELOPMENT AND PLASTICITY IN THE CNS 243
during the normal course of development. Thus, since the fibers are not
really present at the site at the time of the injury, they can grow past the
site and reroute themselves to their appropriate targets (Bregman & Gold-
berger, 1982).
In summary, the work on early versus late injury and anomalous
growth highlights yet again the importance of the context; in this case,
stage of maturation can determine the outcome of traumatic brain injury.
The factors that lead to survival and growth on the one hand, or loss of
neurons on the other, are not well understood. Nonetheless, there is con-
siderable progress being made to unlock the appropriate conditions or
factors that lead to consistent recovery of function where none is ex-
pected.
Especially when attempts are made to facilitate recovery with drugs
or experimental agents, the specific context, experience, or "background"
of the subject will interact with the "treatment" to determine the outcome
of the brain injury. In adult organisms, the potential for plasticity in the
form of reorganization (or deblocking) of function is also present, but
different rehabilitative strategies may be required to activate the mecha-
nisms underlying the recovery itself.
One last example taken from the work of Dennis Feeney and his
students will suffice. Feeney, Gonzalez, and Law (1982) found that injec-
tions of amphetamine can enhance the levels of the neurotransmitter nor-
epinephrine in adult animals who have been depleted of this substance by
lesions of the catecholaminergic system. Injuries in the motor cortex often
block or eliminate visual, postural, proprioceptive, righting, and tactile
placing reflexes. Amphetamine administration can apparently restore
these reflexes and restore motor and visual performance, even after acute
bilateral injury. In the face of a considerable amount of literature, it would
seem that such a finding would lead to the development of an effective
clinical treatment for brain injury, but the question is more complex.
First, in reviewing the literature, Feeney and Sutton (1987) found that
recovery could not be maintained if animals did not receive repeated
testing (general visual experience in the home cage was not sufficient). In
cats, many tests of the specific placing response had to be administered
for the drug to be effective in inducing recovery. To test the importance of
experience in amphetamine-induced recovery from brain injury, Feeney
and his students restrained rats with motor cortex lesions in a small box
during amphetamine intoxication and compared them with counterparts
allowed to roam freely or who were given beam-walking experience.
Blocking ballistic movements eliminated the beneficial amphetamine ef-
fects entirely (Hovda & Feeney, 1984).
In reviewing their work, Feeney and Sutton (1987) suggest that "drug
therapy alone may not be efficacious for inducing recovery. Rather a com-
bination of drug plus appropriate behavioral training and testing
244 DONALD G. STEIN
For the most part, the prognosis for recovery from brain damage in
humans is poor. When it does occur, it is often characterized as an in-
teresting anomaly, and there is little in the way of systematic inquiry into
either the historical or present conditions that led to the patients' unique
outcome in comparison with those who seem to be less fortunate. Yet
individual differences in response to the same type or extent of injury
could provide important clues as to some of the mechanisms underlying
functional recovery. As early as 1902, two developmental neurologists,
Baldwin and Polton, said that "apart from plasticity in general, the facts
of individual accommodation make the nature and limits of brain plas-
ticity a matter of great interest." They may have been among the first to
talk about individual differences in response to brain injury.
Unfortunately, in the laboratory setting, our own empiricist orienta-
tions and the social conditions in neuroscience do not often permit the
thorough study of individual accommodation to trauma. We are forced to
employ statistical averaging methods to seek the mean case, and we are
taught that deviation from the average is due to error in measurement. In
the final analysis, we will need to address the question of whether there
are effective tools to pursue our understanding of why there is so much
individual variability in response to brain injury and what that variability
can tell us about mechanisms of eNS plasticity.
Let me illustrate this point further by referring to work by R.
Thompson (1978). In A Behavioral Atlas of the Rat Brain Thompson
plotted all of the different areas reported by researchers to affect different
behavioral functions. The ostensible purpose of the atlas was to provide
the reader with a visually easy way to determine where one could create a
specific lesion to produce a specific deficit. In illustrations, Thompson
showed where damage to the brain results in maze-learning deficits. In
examining all of the areas that appear to produce deficits in learning
ability, it is hard for me to conceive of the fact that behavior as complex as
maze learning could be localized in a single given structure or pathway in
the central nervous system. Yet this atlas represents the results of many
studies that have attempted to do just this. For the sake of argument one
could say that with respect to a phenomenon as amorphous as learning,
the processes involved should be relatively distributed throughout "high-
DEVELOPMENT AND PLASTICITY IN THE CNS 245
In summary, the examples that I have provided show that there is still
turmoil in neuroscience that is being generated by dramatically different
ways of looking at nervous system functions. It is an important conflict
that spills over even into the more clinical areas of human neuropsychol-
ogy. Let me again repeat the quote I gave earlier from the neuroscience
texts of Kandel and Schwartz (1981), here describing the clinical psycho-
pathology of temporal lobe function. They claim that both cognitive func-
tions and character traits are anatomically localizable. Thus, "their
clinical studies and their counterparts in experimental animals suggest
that all behavior, including higher mental functions, is localizable to
specific regions or constellations of regions within the brain. The role of
descriptive neuroanatomy is, therefore, to provide us with a functional
guide to the localization within the three-dimensional neurospace, a map
for behavior" (p. 11). Given the data presented so far, perhaps the reader
should draw his or her own conclusions about which of the competing
paradigms might be most appropriate for the study of physiological and
organismic plasticity.
Finally, I present here the summary statements taken from a recent
book on cortical localization in clinical neuropsychology (Kertesz, 1983).
Kertesz argues that "the function of an area is related to the lesion if: (1)
The same functional deficit always follows the lesion. (2) The same deficit
DEVELOPMENT AND PLASTICITY IN THE CNS 249
As we come to the end of this essay, I think it is safe to say that the
neurosciences are undergoing a dramatic shift in ideas about the "limits
of plasticity" in the central nervous system. We have seen that there are a
variety of developmental processes leading to recovery or change that
must be considered in describing or predicting the outcome of brain inju-
ry. It is no longer possible to describe or report symptoms simply in terms
of damage to a specific path or circuit because those paths or circuits may
not be constant. The conditions under which damage occurs at a given
moment in the cumulative history of the organism must now be taken into
account in our attempts to define structure-function relationships in the
CNS. In this context, we must also be more aware of the fact that environ-
mental influences can also modulate and sculpt neuronal morphology,
such that the functional demands made on morphology may determine
the output of a given neural area; thus, function (environment) may play
some role in determining structure.
Mapping of neuronal circuits in fixed tissue is an important and
useful endeavor in that it may give us the conditions by which we can
begin to explore the more dynamic aspects of nervous system function.
However, when such mapping becomes an end in itself, or when it be-
comes merely the demonstration of technique without any reference to
behavior, then the science of understanding the function of the nervous
system will not really progress. The fashion now is toward an ever-in-
creasing, mechanical and ideological "reductionism" where behavior is
250 DONALD G. STEIN
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
REFERENCES
Attella, M., Nattinville, A., & Stein, D. G. (1987). Hormonal state affects recovery from frontal
cortex lesions in adults females rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 48, 352-367.
Baudry, M., & Lynch, G. W. (1984). Glutamate receptor regulation and the substrates of
memory. In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of
learning and memory (pp. 431-450). New York: Guilford Press.
Bregman, B., & Goldberger, M. (1982). Anatomical plasticity and sparing of function after
spinal cord damage in neonatal cats. Science, 237, 553-555.
Bregman, E., & Goldberger, M. (1983a). Infant lesion effect: II. Sparing and recovery of
function after spinal cord damage in newborn and adult cats. Developmental Brain
Research, 8, 119-135.
Bregman, B. S., & Goldberger, M. E. (1983b). Infant lesion effect: III. Anatomical correlates of
sparing and recovery of function after spinal cord damage in newborn and adult cats.
Developmental Brain Research, 9, 137-154.
DEVELOPMENT AND PLASTICITY IN THE CNS 251
Cajal, S. (1928). Degeneration and regeneration of the central nervous system (R. M. May,
Trans.). London: Oxford University Press.
Dalyrimple-Alford, J. D., & Kelche, C. (1985). Behavioral effects of pre-operative-post-
operative differential housing in rats with brain lesions: A review. In B. E. Will. P.
Schmitt, D. C. Dalyrimple-Alford (Eds.), Brain plasticity, learning and memory. Ad-
vances in behavioral biology (Vol. 28, pp. 441-458). New York: Plenum Press.
Descarries, L., Baudet, A., & Watkins, K. C. (1975). Serotonin nerve terminals in adult rat
neocortex. Brain Research, 100, 563-588.
Earle, E. (1987). Lesion size and recovery of function: Some new perspectives. Brain Re-
search Reviews, 12, 307-320.
Feeney, D. M., Gonzalez, A., & Law, W. A. (1982). Amphetamine, haloperidal and experience
interact to affect rate of recovery after motor cortex injury. Science, 217, 885-857.
Feeney, D. M., & Sutton, R. (1987). Pharmacotherapy for recovery of function after brain
injury. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology, 3, 135-197.
Gazzaniga, M. (1974). Determinants of cerebral recovery. In D. G. Stein, J. J. Rosen, & N.
Butters (Eds.), Plasticity and recovery offunction in the eNS (pp. 203-216). New York:
Academic Press.
Geschwind, N. (1985). Mechanisms of change after brain lesions. In R. Nottebohm (Ed.),
Hope for a new neurology, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 457, 1-11.
Gollin, E. S. (1981). Development and plasticity. In B. S. Gollin (Ed.), Developmental plas-
ticity: Behavioral and biological aspects of variation in development. New York: Aca-
demic Press.
Greenough, W., & Chang, P. F. (1985). Synaptic structural correlates of information storage in
mammalian nervous system. In C. W. Cotman (Ed.), Synaptic plasticity (pp. 335-374).
New York: Guilford Press.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley Press.
Hovda, D. A., & Feeney D. M. (1984). Amphetamine with experience promotes recovery of
locomotor function after unilateral frontal cortex injury in the cat. Brain Research, 298,
358.
John, E. R., Tang, Y., Brill, A. B., Young, R., & Ono, H. (1986). Double-labeled metabolic maps
of memory. Science, 233, 1167-1175.
Kalil, R. E., & Schneider, G. E. (1975). Abnormal synaptic connections of the optic tract in the
thalamus after midbrain lesions in newborn hamsters. Brain Research, 100, 690-698.
Kandel. E. R., & Schwartz, J. H. (1981). Principles of neuroscience. Amsterdam: Elsevier
North-Holland.
Kaplan, B. (1983). A trio of trials. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Developmental psychology: Histor-
ical and philosophical perspectives (pp. 185-228). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kelche, C., & Will, B. (1982). Effects of postoperative environments following dorsal hippo-
campallesion on dendritic branching and spines in rat occipital cortex. Brain Research,
245, 107-115.
Kertesz, A. (1983). Localization in neuropsychology (p. 18). New York: Academic Press.
Kuhn, T. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Nieto-Sampedro, M. (1988). Growth factor induction and order of events in CNS repair. In D.
G. Stein & B. A. Sabel (Eds.)' Pharmacological approaches to the treatment of brain and
spinal cord injury (pp. 301-338). New York: Plenum Press.
Nottebohm, F. (1985). Neuronal replacement in adulthood. In F. Nottebohm (Ed.), Hope for a
new neurology (pp. 143-162). Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 457.
Raisman, G. (1978). What hope for repair of the brain? Annals of Neurology, 3, 101-106.
Scheff, S. W., Hoff, S. F., & Anderson, N. J. (1986). Altered regulation of lesion-induced
synaptogenesis by adrenalectomy and corticosterone in young adult rats. Experimental
Neurology, 93, 456-470.
252 DONALD G. STEIN
Schneider, G. (1979). Is it really better to have your brain damage early? A revision of the
"Kennard Principle." Neuropsychologia, 17, 557-583.
Schneider, G., & ]haveri, S. (1974). Neuroanatomical correlates of spared or altered function
after brain lesions in the newborn hamster. In D. G. Stein,].]. Rosen, & N. Butters (Eds.)'
Plasticity and recovery of function in the eNS (pp. 65-110). New York: Academic Press.
Slavin, M. D., Laurence, S., & Stein, D. G. (1988). Another look at vicariation. In S. Finger, T.
E. LeVere, C. R. Almli, & D. G. Stein (Eds.), Brain injury and recovery: Theoretical and
controversial issues (pp. 165-180). New York: Plenum Press.
Thompson, R. F. (1986). The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science, 223, 941-947.
Thompson, R. (1978). A behavioral atlas of the rat brain. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Valenstein, E. S., Cox, V. c., & Kakolewski,]. W. (1968). Modification of motivated behavior
elicited by electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus. Science, 158, 1118-1121.
Index
Acalculia, 177-178, 186, 189, 190 Anarithmia, 189-190; see also Acalculia
Accommodation, 244 Anarthria, 101
Accumbens (nucleus), 199 Anarthric retardation, 87, 100
Acetylcholine, 201, 220, 222, 223 Angular gyrus, 41, 147, 153, 189
Acoustic cue, 136 Anomia, 102, 112, 132; see also Aphasia
Adipsia, 245, 246 Anosognosia, 47, 170
Adrenalectomy, 206 Anterograde degeneration, 38
Adrenaline, 204 Anticipatory errors, 137
After-image, 11 Anticipatory transposition, 138
Agnosia, 7, 8, 56, 86, 115, 178, 189 Aphasia, 6-8, 17, 39-41, 45, 51, 56, 71,
auditive, 87 76, 85-86, 88, 102, 107-108, 123-124,
visual, 166 126-127, 134, 136-137, 142-143, 148,
Agraphesthesia, 181 239, 245
Agraphia, 112, 147-149, 173, 187, 189- anomic, 15, 45, 153, 158-162, 166, 169,
190 172, 189
aphasic, 148 Broca's, 46, 135, 147, 151-153, 159-164,
apraxic, 188 169, 170, 190, 191
deep, 149 conduction, 46, 135, 151, 153, 158-163,
phonological, 149 165, 169, 170, 172, 177
semantic, 149 congenital, 85
spatial, 171 cortical motor, 46
Agrammatism, 52, 160, 170, 172, 180, 181, developmental, 86
188, 191 global, 141, 153, 163-166, 169
Akinesia, 199, 202, 206 motor, 46, 87
Alexia, 73, 147-148, 190 semantic, 177-192
frontal, 147 transcortical motor, 44-45
literal, 169 transcortical sensory, 45-47, 54, 112,
spatial, 148 115
verbal, 169 Wernicke's, 44, 54, 112, 115, 135, 151,
with agraphia, 147 153, 158-163, 165, 169, 170, 172, 190
without agraphia, 147, 151-153, 157- Aphasic retardation, 87
163, 169-170 Apperceptive process, 5
Alpha rhythm, 43 Apractagnosia, 87
Alzheimer's disease, 32, 50, 54, 108, 214; Apraxia, 7, 56, 177-178, 181, 183, 187
see also Dementia constructional, 18,56, 151, 171, 178, 188
Amnesia, 56, 107-108 expression, 86, 162
Amphetamine, 204, 243 orofacial, 97
Amygdala, 199 verbal, 46
Analyzers, 91-92, 94, 98-100 Astereognosia, 181
253
254 INDEX