VOL 3 - Layout 1
VOL 3 - Layout 1
VOL 3 - Layout 1
Fu n c t i o n a l
Neuroanatomy
of the Brain
Third Part
To Alexandra
CONTENTS
Foreword ........................................................................................................................................XXXI
FIRST PART
Chapter 1
HISTORY OF THE BRAIN AND MIND
Introduction .................................................................................................................................1
Egyptian physician Imhotep ........................................................................................................9
The Ebers Papyrus ....................................................................................................................11
Three faces of Greece and Hippocrates ...................................................................................12
1. Early Greece .........................................................................................................................12
2. The Golden Age of Greece ...................................................................................................13
ALCMAEON OF CROTON .......................................................................................................14
HIPPOCRATES .........................................................................................................................17
3. The Hellenistic Era ................................................................................................................20
PLATO .......................................................................................................................................22
ARISTOTLE ..............................................................................................................................22
GALEN ......................................................................................................................................23
The Middle Ages .......................................................................................................................26
RHAZES ....................................................................................................................................26
ESMAIL JORJANI .....................................................................................................................26
AVICENNA ................................................................................................................................28
MONDINO DE’ LIUZZI ..............................................................................................................31
The Renaissance .......................................................................................................................31
LEONARDO DA VINCI...............................................................................................................32
JACOPO BERENGARIO DA CARPI..........................................................................................32
VESALIUS..................................................................................................................................32
GABRIELE FALLOPIO...............................................................................................................35
GIULIO CASSERIO ..................................................................................................................35
JOHANN VESLING ...................................................................................................................36
JOHANN JAKOB WEPFER ......................................................................................................36
RENÉ DESCARTES .................................................................................................................36
PACCHIONI ..............................................................................................................................38
LANCISI ....................................................................................................................................39
WILLIS ......................................................................................................................................41
SWEDENBORG ........................................................................................................................44
THE 4 GENERATIONS OF THE MECKEL FAMILY ..................................................................45
GALVANI ...................................................................................................................................54
VOLTA .......................................................................................................................................55
GALL (PHRENOLOGY) ............................................................................................................56
FLOURENS ...............................................................................................................................60
DARWIN ....................................................................................................................................61
JEAN-BAPTISTE BOUILLAUD .................................................................................................62
V
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 2
INTRODUCTION IN THE NERVOUS SYSTEM
Basic division of the brain stem ...............................................................................................142
Medulla oblongata ...................................................................................................................142
Pons ........................................................................................................................................143
Cerebellum ..............................................................................................................................143
Midbrain ..................................................................................................................................143
Forebrain .................................................................................................................................144
Diencephalon ..........................................................................................................................144
The basal nuclei or basal ganglia ............................................................................................146
Cerebral hemispheres .............................................................................................................150
Cerebral cortex ........................................................................................................................151
White matter of the cerebral hemispheres ..............................................................................153
Chapter 3
MEDULLA OBLONGATA (OR BULB)
External structure ....................................................................................................................156
Internal structure .....................................................................................................................158
I. Level of motor (pyramidal) decussation ...............................................................................160
Dorsal column nuclei ...............................................................................................................160
Spinal trigeminal nucleus and its tracts ...................................................................................166
Other tracts .............................................................................................................................168
II. Level of sensory (lemniscal) decussation ...........................................................................169
Medial lemniscus .....................................................................................................................169
Medial longitudinal fasciculus (MLF) .......................................................................................170
Accessory cuneate nucleus ....................................................................................................171
Arcuate nuclei .........................................................................................................................171
Area postrema .........................................................................................................................172
VI
CONTENTS
Chapter 4
PONS
The ventral surface of the pons ...............................................................................................210
The dorsal surface of the pons ................................................................................................211
Internal structure of the pons ..................................................................................................212
Ventral (basilar) pons ..............................................................................................................212
Dorsal pons (pontine tegmentum) ...........................................................................................214
Vestibular system ....................................................................................................................223
Vestibular apparatus ...............................................................................................................224
Bony labyrinth .........................................................................................................................224
Membranous labyrinth .............................................................................................................225
Microstructure of the vestibular system ...................................................................................226
Utricle ......................................................................................................................................229
Saccule ...................................................................................................................................230
Vestibulocochlear nuclei and nerve .........................................................................................231
The vestibular nerve and nuclei ..............................................................................................231
Central pathways ....................................................................................................................232
Vestibular nuclear complex .....................................................................................................233
Afferent projections (input) to the vestibular nuclei .................................................................234
Efferent projections (output) from the vestibular nuclei ...........................................................238
The cochlear nerve and nuclei ................................................................................................252
Anatomy of hearing .................................................................................................................255
Central transmission ................................................................................................................258
The cochlear nuclei .................................................................................................................259
Auditory pathways ...................................................................................................................261
Superior olivary nuclei .............................................................................................................264
Lateral lemniscus ....................................................................................................................264
Inferior colliculus .....................................................................................................................265
Medial geniculate nucleus .......................................................................................................266
Auditory cortex ........................................................................................................................268
The auditory pathway through the reticular formation .............................................................270
Descending auditory projections .............................................................................................270
Sound reflex ............................................................................................................................272
Sections of the pons ................................................................................................................272
VII
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 5
MIDBRAIN
Pretectal area ..........................................................................................................................336
Tectum .....................................................................................................................................337
The superior colliculi ............................................................................................................337
Efferent connections ...............................................................................................................338
Afferent connections ...............................................................................................................340
The inferior colliculi ..............................................................................................................342
Afferent connections ...............................................................................................................342
Efferent connections ...............................................................................................................343
Tegmentum .............................................................................................................................344
Nuclear groups ........................................................................................................................345
The red nucleus ......................................................................................................................351
The posterior commissure .......................................................................................................355
Basal portion of the midbrain ..................................................................................................357
Cerebral peduncle ...................................................................................................................357
Substantia nigra ......................................................................................................................358
Mesencephalic dopaminergic cell groups ...............................................................................362
Pupillary reflexes .....................................................................................................................364
Supranuclear pathways for accommodation ...........................................................................367
Pupillodilation ..........................................................................................................................368
Vertical gaze ............................................................................................................................369
Midbrain clinical correlates ......................................................................................................370
Syndrome of Weber ................................................................................................................371
Syndrome of Benedikt .............................................................................................................372
Claude’s syndrome .................................................................................................................372
Nothnagel’s syndrome ............................................................................................................373
Plus-minus lid syndrome .........................................................................................................373
Top of the basilar syndrome ....................................................................................................373
Walleyed syndrome .................................................................................................................374
VIII
CONTENTS
Chapter 6
RETICULAR FORMATION
Introduction .............................................................................................................................385
Organization of the RF ............................................................................................................386
Neurons of the RF ...................................................................................................................386
Reticulospinal tracts ................................................................................................................388
Medullary reticular formation ...................................................................................................389
Afferent fibres to the medullary RF .........................................................................................391
Efferent fibres from the medullary RF .....................................................................................392
The pontine reticular formation ...............................................................................................394
Mesencephalic reticular formation ..........................................................................................398
Chemical specified systems ....................................................................................................400
Monoaminergic system ...........................................................................................................401
Zones of the RF ......................................................................................................................402
Afferents to the RF ..................................................................................................................404
Efferents from the RF ..............................................................................................................404
Nuclei associated with the RF .................................................................................................404
Functions of the RF .................................................................................................................405
Somatic motor function ...........................................................................................................405
The descending influences .....................................................................................................405
Pontine (medial) reticulospinal tract ........................................................................................407
Medullary (lateral) reticulospinal tract .....................................................................................408
Corticoreticular fibres ..............................................................................................................409
Control of eye movements and other cranial nerve function ...................................................410
Somatic sensory function ........................................................................................................412
Control of visceral functions ....................................................................................................413
Respiration ..............................................................................................................................414
Cardiovascular activity ............................................................................................................415
Influence on the endocrine system .........................................................................................417
Ascending reticular activating system .....................................................................................417
Projections from the mesopontine tegmentum to the forebrain ..............................................419
Ascending reticular inhibitory system ......................................................................................426
Chapter 7
CEREBELLUM
Organization of the cerebellum ...............................................................................................439
Morphological approach of the cerebellum .............................................................................440
Functional approach of the cerebellum ...................................................................................444
IX
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 8
DIENCEPHALON
EPITHALAMUS .......................................................................................................................542
Stria medullaris thalami ...........................................................................................................542
Habenular nuclei .....................................................................................................................542
The Pineal gland .....................................................................................................................542
Functions .................................................................................................................................544
Posterior commissure .............................................................................................................546
THALAMUS .............................................................................................................................546
X
CONTENTS
Anatomy ..................................................................................................................................547
Borders ....................................................................................................................................549
Lateral and external medullary laminae ..................................................................................550
Nuclear groups of the thalamus ..............................................................................................551
Anterior nuclei .........................................................................................................................554
Intralaminar nuclei ...................................................................................................................556
Medial nuclei ...........................................................................................................................559
Median nuclei ..........................................................................................................................561
Metathalamic nuclei ................................................................................................................562
Posterior nuclear complex .......................................................................................................566
Pulvinar nuclei and lateral posterior nucleus ...........................................................................566
Reticular nucleus .....................................................................................................................568
Ventral nuclei ...........................................................................................................................570
Neuronal circuitry ....................................................................................................................582
Functions of the thalamus .......................................................................................................582
Clinical considerations ............................................................................................................584
SUBTHALAMUS .....................................................................................................................586
Subthalamic nucleus ...............................................................................................................587
Forel’s field H (prerubral field) .................................................................................................589
The zona incerta .....................................................................................................................590
HYPOTHALAMUS ..................................................................................................................591
Hypothalamic regions ..............................................................................................................593
Nuclei of the hypothalamic regions .........................................................................................594
Preoptic region ........................................................................................................................597
Suprachiasmatic (supraoptic) region .......................................................................................599
The anterior hypothalamic nucleus .........................................................................................600
Interstitial nuclei of the anterior hypothalamus ........................................................................600
The paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei ..............................................................................601
Tuberal region .........................................................................................................................604
The dorsomedial nucleus ........................................................................................................605
The ventromedial nucleus .......................................................................................................605
The lateral hypothalamic nucleus ............................................................................................605
The arcuate (infundibular) nucleus ..........................................................................................606
Mammillary region ...................................................................................................................607
Hypothalamic zones ................................................................................................................608
Periventricular zone ................................................................................................................609
Medial zone .............................................................................................................................610
Lateral zone ............................................................................................................................610
Ventrolateral preoptic nucleus .................................................................................................613
Connections of the hypothalamus ...........................................................................................613
Local connections ...................................................................................................................613
Extrinsic connections ..............................................................................................................614
Afferent pathways to the hypothalamus ..................................................................................614
Fornix ......................................................................................................................................614
Amygdalohypothalamic tract ...................................................................................................616
Stria terminalis ........................................................................................................................616
XI
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
SECOND PART
Chapter 9
THE BASAL GANGLIA
Basal ganglia anatomical organization ....................................................................................732
The striatum ............................................................................................................................735
Caudate nucleus .....................................................................................................................736
Nucleus accumbens ................................................................................................................739
Putamen ..................................................................................................................................740
Globus pallidus ........................................................................................................................743
Subthalamic nucleus (nucleus of Luys) ...................................................................................747
Substantia nigra ......................................................................................................................750
Nuclei associated with the basal ganglia ................................................................................751
The claustrum .........................................................................................................................751
Amygdaloid nucleus (amygdala) .............................................................................................751
Thalamic nuclei .......................................................................................................................751
Connections of the basal ganglia ............................................................................................752
Striatum ...................................................................................................................................753
Afferent fibres (input) ...........................................................................................................753
Corticostriate fibres .................................................................................................................753
Thalamostriate fibres ...............................................................................................................755
Nigrostriate fibres ....................................................................................................................755
Fibres from the ventral tegmental area ...................................................................................756
Fibres from the brainstem pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus ............................................756
Efferent fibres (output) .........................................................................................................756
The ansa system .....................................................................................................................757
The lenticular fasciculus ..........................................................................................................757
The thalamic fasciculus ...........................................................................................................759
Striatopallidal fibres .................................................................................................................760
Striatonigral fibres ...................................................................................................................761
Ventral (limbic) striatum ...........................................................................................................762
Globus pallidus pathway .........................................................................................................763
Afferent fibres (input) ...........................................................................................................763
Striatopallidal fibres .................................................................................................................764
Subthalamopallidal fibres ........................................................................................................764
XIII
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Tremor .....................................................................................................................................804
Chorea ....................................................................................................................................804
Athetosis .................................................................................................................................805
Ballisme ...................................................................................................................................805
Huntington’s disease ...............................................................................................................806
Dystonia ..................................................................................................................................807
Tic disorder .............................................................................................................................808
Tourette’s syndrome ................................................................................................................809
Hypokinetic disorders ..............................................................................................................810
Parkinson’s disease ................................................................................................................811
Wilson’s disease .....................................................................................................................821
Chapter 10
LIMBIC LOBE AND LIMBIC SYSTEM
Limbic lobe ..............................................................................................................................843
Limbic system .........................................................................................................................847
Parts of the limbic system .......................................................................................................849
Olfactory system .....................................................................................................................849
Septum ....................................................................................................................................852
Cingulate gyrus and cingulum .................................................................................................855
Alveus, fimbria, and fornix .......................................................................................................858
Thalamus ................................................................................................................................861
Hypothalamus .........................................................................................................................862
Locus ceruleus and dorsal raphe nucleus................................................................................862
Habenular nuclei .....................................................................................................................863
Ventral tegmental area ............................................................................................................863
Dorsal longitudinal fasciculus ..................................................................................................863
Indusium griseum ....................................................................................................................863
Fasciola cinerea ......................................................................................................................865
Limbic functions ......................................................................................................................865
Limbic system and emotion .....................................................................................................866
Negative and positive reinforcement .......................................................................................870
Limbic system and memory ....................................................................................................870
Limbic system disorder ...........................................................................................................873
Chapter 11
HIPPOCAMPAL FORMATION
Introduction .............................................................................................................................883
Anatomy ..................................................................................................................................883
Structure ..................................................................................................................................886
Cornu Ammonis (hippocampus proper) ..................................................................................886
Regional variations ..................................................................................................................891
The cytoarchitectonic organization of the hippocampus .........................................................893
The pyramidal layer .................................................................................................................893
Molecular layer ........................................................................................................................895
Polymorphic layer ....................................................................................................................896
XV
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Interneurons ............................................................................................................................896
Hippocampal connections .......................................................................................................899
Afferent fibres ..........................................................................................................................899
Efferent fibres ..........................................................................................................................901
Intrinsic circuitry of the hippocampal formation .......................................................................902
Commissural connections of the hippocampus .......................................................................903
Dentate gyrus ..........................................................................................................................904
Afferents to the dentate gyrus .................................................................................................908
Efferents from the dentate gyrus .............................................................................................910
Subiculum ................................................................................................................................911
Intrinsic connections ................................................................................................................912
Subicular efferents and afferents ............................................................................................912
Presubiculum and parasubiculum ...........................................................................................912
Intrinsic connections ................................................................................................................914
Commissural connections .......................................................................................................914
Extrinsic connections ..............................................................................................................914
Presubiculum projections to layer III of the entorhinal cortex ..................................................914
Intrahippocampal connections ................................................................................................915
Entorhinal cortex .....................................................................................................................915
Cytoarchitectonic organization ................................................................................................915
Associational connections .......................................................................................................917
Commissural connections .......................................................................................................917
Perforant and alvear pathway projections ...............................................................................917
Extrinsic connections ..............................................................................................................917
Transmitters and receptors of the hippocampal formation ......................................................920
Functional role of the human hippocampus ............................................................................921
Learning and memory .............................................................................................................921
Motor control ...........................................................................................................................934
Emotional behavior .................................................................................................................934
Regulation of the hypothalamo-hypophysial axis ....................................................................935
There is an inverted U-shape function between the level of stress and memory.....................936
Neurogenesis ..........................................................................................................................937
Fast synaptic subcortical control of hippocampal circuits .......................................................939
The hippocampus and human diseases .................................................................................940
Encephalitis and the hippocampus .........................................................................................941
Ischaemic damage to the hippocampus ..................................................................................942
Schizophrenia and the hippocampus ......................................................................................942
Hippocampal sclerosis ............................................................................................................942
Neurotransmitter systems .......................................................................................................948
GABAergic mechanisms .........................................................................................................948
Adenosine ...............................................................................................................................949
Opiod .......................................................................................................................................949
NPY (neuropeptide Y) .............................................................................................................949
Galanin ....................................................................................................................................950
Alzheimer’s disease .................................................................................................................951
XVI
CONTENTS
Chapter 12
AMYGDALA
Introduction .............................................................................................................................989
Anatomical organization of the amygdala ...............................................................................990
Connections ............................................................................................................................993
Major pathways .......................................................................................................................994
Subcortical connectivity ...........................................................................................................995
Afferent connections ............................................................................................................995
Efferent connections ............................................................................................................997
Connections with the olfactory system ..................................................................................1000
Connections with the hippocampal formation .......................................................................1000
Connections with neocortex ..................................................................................................1000
Neurochemical modulation ....................................................................................................1005
Functions of the amygdala ....................................................................................................1006
The fear system and the seeking system ..............................................................................1012
Erasing fear memories ..........................................................................................................1016
The seeking system ..............................................................................................................1018
The amygdala in normal aging ..............................................................................................1019
The human amygdala and fear .............................................................................................1020
The amygdala and learning ...................................................................................................1024
The human amygdala and memory ......................................................................................1024
The role of the human amygdala in perception and attention ...............................................1027
The human amygdala in social function ................................................................................1033
The human amygdala dysfunction ........................................................................................1037
The human amygdala in autism ............................................................................................1040
The human amygdala in schizophrenia ................................................................................1044
The human amygdala in Alzheimer’s disease .......................................................................1045
Chapter 13
OLFACTORY SYSTEM
Olfactory receptors ................................................................................................................1078
Olfactory transduction ...........................................................................................................1082
Olfactory bulb – secondary olfactory neurons........................................................................1084
Olfactory tract ........................................................................................................................1089
Olfactory striae ......................................................................................................................1090
Thalamic neurons ..................................................................................................................1094
Cortical neurons ....................................................................................................................1095
Olfactory cortex projections ...................................................................................................1095
Efferent olfactory connections ...............................................................................................1096
Disorders of the olfactory system ..........................................................................................1096
Chapter 14
GUSTATORY SYSTEM
Taste receptors ......................................................................................................................1103
Lingual taste buds .................................................................................................................1105
XVII
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 15
FRONTAL LOBES
Introduction ............................................................................................................................1122
PRECENTRAL DIVISION ......................................................................................................1125
Functional organization .........................................................................................................1128
Disorders due to lesions of the upper motor neurons ...........................................................1134
Pathological hyperreflexia .....................................................................................................1135
Spasticity ...............................................................................................................................1135
Loss of dexterity ....................................................................................................................1135
Weakness ..............................................................................................................................1136
Pseudobulbar (spastic bulbar) palsy .....................................................................................1136
PREMOTOR DIVISION .........................................................................................................1138
Broca’s aphasia .....................................................................................................................1144
Frontal-subcortical aphasias .................................................................................................1146
Brodmann’s area 8 .................................................................................................................1147
Supplementary motor area (SMA) .........................................................................................1150
Cingulate motor cortex ..........................................................................................................1153
Motor functions of somatosensory cortex ..............................................................................1154
PREFRONTAL DIVISION ......................................................................................................1155
Cytoarchitecture ....................................................................................................................1156
Connections ..........................................................................................................................1158
Neuropsychological functions of the prefrontal cortex ...........................................................1162
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) .................................................................................1162
Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex ...............................................................................................1163
Orbitofrontal cortex ................................................................................................................1165
Medial prefrontal cortex .........................................................................................................1169
Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex .......................................................1169
Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex ...........................................1175
Executive function, actions and plans ...................................................................................1176
Executive control ...................................................................................................................1179
Rostro-caudal axis of PFC ....................................................................................................1182
Response inhibition ...............................................................................................................1184
Monitoring ..............................................................................................................................1185
Attention ................................................................................................................................1185
Mental flexibility .....................................................................................................................1187
Affect .....................................................................................................................................1188
Depression ............................................................................................................................1188
XVIII
CONTENTS
Euphoria ................................................................................................................................1188
Hypokinesia ...........................................................................................................................1189
Ambiguity and decision .........................................................................................................1190
Planning ................................................................................................................................1193
Theory of mind: empathy .......................................................................................................1196
Memory of the future .............................................................................................................1199
Novelty and routine ...............................................................................................................1199
Memory .................................................................................................................................1200
Retrieval ................................................................................................................................1201
Temporal sequencing ............................................................................................................1201
Source of memory .................................................................................................................1201
Autobiographical memory .....................................................................................................1202
Working memory ...................................................................................................................1203
Social behavior ......................................................................................................................1206
Prefrontal cortex and intelligence ..........................................................................................1206
Prefrontal dysfunction ...........................................................................................................1209
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1209
Frontal lobe syndromes .........................................................................................................1211
Dorsolateral frontal syndrome ...............................................................................................1211
Orbitofrontal syndrome ..........................................................................................................1213
Medial syndrome ...................................................................................................................1216
Reticulofrontal disconnection syndrome ...............................................................................1218
Attention deficit / hyperactive disorder (ADHD) .....................................................................1218
Disturbance in self-awareness ..............................................................................................1219
Lesions in Broca’s area .........................................................................................................1225
Lesions in the frontal eye field ...............................................................................................1226
Lesions in the motor cortex ...................................................................................................1226
Chapter 16
PARIETAL LOBE
Primary somatosensory cortex ..............................................................................................1256
Subdivisions of the Parietal Cortex .......................................................................................1258
Somatosensory association cortices .....................................................................................1259
High-order cortical representation .........................................................................................1263
Gustatory cortex ....................................................................................................................1264
Spatial information ................................................................................................................1265
Object recognition .................................................................................................................1265
Guidance of movement .........................................................................................................1265
Movement intention ...............................................................................................................1268
Human volition ......................................................................................................................1270
Brain circuits ..........................................................................................................................1271
Early gamma oscillations synchronize developing thalamus and cortex ..............................1273
Voluntary action and decision making ...................................................................................1273
Voluntary action as exploratory behavior ..............................................................................1276
Voluntary action as random behavioral noise .......................................................................1277
Voluntary action as conditioned responding ..........................................................................1277
XIX
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 17
TEMPORAL LOBES
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1319
Anatomy ................................................................................................................................1321
The central auditory system ..................................................................................................1323
Auditory cortex ......................................................................................................................1325
Auditory cortical anatomy ......................................................................................................1326
Cytoarchitecture of auditory cortex .......................................................................................1328
Connectivity ...........................................................................................................................1329
Auditory association cortex ...................................................................................................1330
Anatomy ................................................................................................................................1330
Functional studies in monkeys ..............................................................................................1330
XX
CONTENTS
THIRD PART
Chapter 18
OCCIPITAL LOBE
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1409
VISUAL PATHWAYS .............................................................................................................1409
Visual field topography ..........................................................................................................1412
Anatomy ................................................................................................................................1414
Cytoarchitecture ....................................................................................................................1415
Primary visual cortex .............................................................................................................1417
Extrastriate visual areas – outside of V1 ...............................................................................1419
The functional organization of extrastriate visual areas ........................................................1421
Connectivity ...........................................................................................................................1424
Visual input ............................................................................................................................1425
Intrinsic circuitry ....................................................................................................................1428
Corticocortical connectivity ....................................................................................................1431
Patterns of organization within the sensory cortices: Brain modules ....................................1432
The columnar organization of the striate cortex ....................................................................1434
Functional subsystem ...........................................................................................................1436
Motion perception ..................................................................................................................1436
Color ......................................................................................................................................1436
Form ......................................................................................................................................1437
XXII
CONTENTS
Chapter 19
WHITE MATTER OF CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE
Association fibres ..................................................................................................................1480
The short arcuate fibres ........................................................................................................1481
The long arcuate fibres .........................................................................................................1481
The uncinate fasciculus .........................................................................................................1481
The cingulum .........................................................................................................................1481
The superior longitudinal fasciculus ......................................................................................1482
The superior fronto-occipital fasciculus (subcalosal bundle) .................................................1482
The inferior longitudinal fasciculus ........................................................................................1482
The external capsule .............................................................................................................1482
Projection fibres ....................................................................................................................1482
Corticopetal fibres .................................................................................................................1483
XXIII
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 20
CORPUS CALLOSUM
Anatomy of the corpus callosum ...........................................................................................1493
The rostrum ...........................................................................................................................1495
The genu ...............................................................................................................................1495
The body ...............................................................................................................................1495
The isthmus ...........................................................................................................................1497
The splenium .........................................................................................................................1498
The callous fibres ..................................................................................................................1499
The function of the corpus callosum .....................................................................................1504
The clinic of the corpus callosum ..........................................................................................1506
Specificity of callosal fibres ...................................................................................................1511
The disconnection syndrome ................................................................................................1511
The effects of complete disconnection ..................................................................................1513
The effects of partial disconnection .......................................................................................1513
The acute disconnection syndrome ......................................................................................1514
The chronic disconnection syndrome ....................................................................................1515
The transfer of sensorial information .....................................................................................1516
Visual effects .........................................................................................................................1516
Language ..............................................................................................................................1521
Postcallosotomy mutism .......................................................................................................1523
Corpus callosum and reading ...............................................................................................1526
Corpus callosum and the visual-spatial aptitudes .................................................................1527
Somesthesis ..........................................................................................................................1527
Audition .................................................................................................................................1530
Olfaction ................................................................................................................................1531
The transfer of motor information ..........................................................................................1532
Movement .............................................................................................................................1532
Praxic and graphometric activities ........................................................................................1534
Agnosia and alexia ................................................................................................................1537
Transfer of complex asymmetrically organized information ..................................................1537
Unilateral apraxia ..................................................................................................................1537
Alien hand syndrome ............................................................................................................1538
Corpus callosum and memory ..............................................................................................1540
Corpus callosum and attention ..............................................................................................1541
Corpus callosum and the emotional function ........................................................................1542
The interhemispheric transfer of elaborate behavior .............................................................1542
The median fusion .................................................................................................................1543
XXIV
CONTENTS
Chapter 21
CEREBRAL CORTEX
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1592
Fissures, sulci, and gyri .........................................................................................................1597
Lateral organization ...............................................................................................................1597
Longitudinal organization ......................................................................................................1598
Point-to-point representation on the cortex ...........................................................................1599
Association areas of the cortex .............................................................................................1600
Connections between cortical areas and binding problems ..................................................1604
Histology of the cerebral cortex .............................................................................................1605
Inhibitory interneurons in the neocortex ................................................................................1609
Cortical layers ........................................................................................................................1611
Cortical connectivity ..............................................................................................................1614
Afferent connections .............................................................................................................1614
The thalamocortical fibres .....................................................................................................1614
The thalamocortical relationship ...........................................................................................1615
Efferent connections .............................................................................................................1616
Myeloarchitectonics ...............................................................................................................1617
Cortical columns spots and stripes (microarchitecture) ........................................................1619
The organization of the cells of the cortex ............................................................................1624
Cortical layers, afferents and efferents ..................................................................................1625
Mapping the human cortex ....................................................................................................1627
Neurotransmitters of the cerebral cortex ...............................................................................1630
Cortical areas ........................................................................................................................1632
Functional aspects of the cerebral cortex .............................................................................1634
The functional areas of the cerebral cortex ...........................................................................1639
Summary ...............................................................................................................................1640
Cortical areas controlling motor activity ................................................................................1640
THE PRIMARY MOTOR AREA .............................................................................................1641
The corticospinal tract ...........................................................................................................1645
Secondary motor cortex ........................................................................................................1648
XXV
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Automatisms .........................................................................................................................1718
Anomic aphasia .....................................................................................................................1720
Primary gustatory cortex .......................................................................................................1721
Chapter 22
VENTRICULAR SYSTEM AND MENINGES
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1748
Lateral ventricle .....................................................................................................................1749
The frontal horn .....................................................................................................................1749
The body or the central portion .............................................................................................1749
The posterior (occipital) horn ................................................................................................1749
The inferior (temporal) horn ..................................................................................................1750
The choroid plexus ................................................................................................................1750
Third ventricle ........................................................................................................................1750
Anterior wall ..........................................................................................................................1751
The lamina terminalis ............................................................................................................1751
Roof .......................................................................................................................................1751
Floor ......................................................................................................................................1752
Posterior wall .........................................................................................................................1752
Lateral wall ............................................................................................................................1753
Aqueduct of Sylvius ...............................................................................................................1753
Fourth ventricle .....................................................................................................................1753
Floor of the fourth ventricle ...................................................................................................1754
Roof of the fourth ventricle ....................................................................................................1755
Production of the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) ..........................................................................1756
CSF absorption .....................................................................................................................1757
CSF function .........................................................................................................................1758
Tanycites ...............................................................................................................................1758
Composition of CSF ..............................................................................................................1758
Meninges ...............................................................................................................................1759
Cranial dura mater ................................................................................................................1760
Reflections of the meningeal layer of the dura mater ............................................................1760
Falx cerebri ...........................................................................................................................1761
Tentorium cerebelli ................................................................................................................1761
Falx cerebelli .........................................................................................................................1762
Diaphragma sella ..................................................................................................................1762
Cavum trigeminale (Meckel’s cave) ......................................................................................1762
Vascular and nerve supply ....................................................................................................1762
Sinus pericranii ......................................................................................................................1763
Emissary veins ......................................................................................................................1763
Innervation of the cranial dura mater ....................................................................................1763
Leptomeninges and CSF space ............................................................................................1763
Cranial arachnoid ..................................................................................................................1764
Arachnoid cisternae ..............................................................................................................1765
Arachnoid granulations and villi ............................................................................................1766
Pia mater ...............................................................................................................................1770
XXVII
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 23
CEREBRAL ASYMMETRY in nonhumans
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1776
Asymmetry in rats .................................................................................................................1779
Asymmetry in birds ................................................................................................................1781
Asymmetry in nonhuman primates ........................................................................................1782
The evolution of handedness ................................................................................................1786
Theories of hand preference .................................................................................................1787
Environmental theories .........................................................................................................1787
Culture and language ............................................................................................................1788
Sensory or environmental deficits .........................................................................................1789
Brain organization in nonhearing people ...............................................................................1789
Environmental deprivation .....................................................................................................1790
Environmental accident .........................................................................................................1790
Functional cerebral organization of left-handers ...................................................................1790
Anatomical theories ...............................................................................................................1791
Hormonal theories .................................................................................................................1792
Genetic theories ....................................................................................................................1793
Sex differences in cerebral organization ...............................................................................1795
Sex differences in behavior ...................................................................................................1795
Motor skills ............................................................................................................................1796
Spatial analysis .....................................................................................................................1796
Mathematical aptitude ...........................................................................................................1796
Perception .............................................................................................................................1797
Verbal ability ..........................................................................................................................1797
Sex difference in the brain structure .....................................................................................1797
The influence of sex hormones .............................................................................................1798
Explanation of sex differences ..............................................................................................1802
Hormonal effects ...................................................................................................................1802
Genetic sex linkage ...............................................................................................................1803
Maturation rate ......................................................................................................................1803
Environment ..........................................................................................................................1804
Preferred cognitive mode ......................................................................................................1804
Anatomical studies ................................................................................................................1804
Handedness and the longevity ..............................................................................................1807
Effects of hemispherectomy ..................................................................................................1809
Ontogeny of asymmetry ........................................................................................................1811
ASYMMETRY BETWEEN THE HEMISPHERES in humans ................................................1814
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1814
Historical aspects ..................................................................................................................1817
Bouillaud ...............................................................................................................................1817
Marc Dax ...............................................................................................................................1818
Gustav Dax ...........................................................................................................................1819
Broca .....................................................................................................................................1820
Wernicke ...............................................................................................................................1821
Pitres and amnesic aphasia ..................................................................................................1825
XXVIII
CONTENTS
Geschwind ............................................................................................................................1826
Aphasia and thought .............................................................................................................1826
ASYMMETRIES OF THE HUMAN FRONTAL LOBES .........................................................1827
Asymmetries in language ......................................................................................................1828
Morphological asymmetries ..................................................................................................1830
Functional imaging ................................................................................................................1831
Asymmetry of prosody and emotion ......................................................................................1833
Prefrontal cortex ....................................................................................................................1836
Premotor cortex .....................................................................................................................1839
Primary motor cortex .............................................................................................................1840
Medial wall (anterior cingulate cortex and SMA) ...................................................................1842
Anterior cingulate cortex .......................................................................................................1842
Supplementary motor area ....................................................................................................1844
Asymmetry in the motor system ............................................................................................1846
Direct observation .................................................................................................................1847
Interference tasks ..................................................................................................................1847
The left hemisphere as interpreter ........................................................................................1848
Asymmetries for the language in humans .............................................................................1848
Asymmetry in visuospatial functions .....................................................................................1852
Hemispheric isolation of visual and tactile information ..........................................................1852
Sharing of attention control ...................................................................................................1853
Asymmetries in processing spatial informations ...................................................................1854
Asymmetry in memory and learning ......................................................................................1856
Asymmetry in the information process ..................................................................................1858
Asymmetry in the attention ....................................................................................................1858
Asymmetry in the music ........................................................................................................1860
Musical perception ................................................................................................................1862
Asymmetry in the visual perception and language of temporal-lobe lesions ........................1863
Language ..............................................................................................................................1864
Asymmetry in the auditory system ........................................................................................1864
Asymmetry in the somatosensory system of parietal-lobe lesions ........................................1866
Somatosensory symptoms of parietal-lobe lesions ...............................................................1868
Blind touch ............................................................................................................................1869
Object recognition .................................................................................................................1870
Pseudocerebellar syndrome .................................................................................................1870
Somatosensory agnosias ......................................................................................................1870
Astereognosis .......................................................................................................................1870
Asomatognosia .....................................................................................................................1871
Left parietal syndrome ...........................................................................................................1873
Apraxia and the parietal lobe ................................................................................................1874
Ideomotor apraxia (IMA) .......................................................................................................1874
Ideational apraxia ..................................................................................................................1875
Conceptual apraxia ...............................................................................................................1875
Limb kinetic apraxia ..............................................................................................................1876
Conduction apraxia ...............................................................................................................1877
Dissociation apraxia ..............................................................................................................1877
XXIX
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Chapter 24
THE NEURAL BASIS OF CONSCIOUSNESS
Introduction ...........................................................................................................................1928
Patients and method .............................................................................................................1928
Results ..................................................................................................................................1931
BRAINSTEM .........................................................................................................................1931
The reticular formation ..........................................................................................................1932
Ascending (reticular) activating system (AAS) ......................................................................1932
Ascending (reticular) inhibitory system (AIS) ........................................................................1936
Clinical aspects .....................................................................................................................1939
Coma .....................................................................................................................................1939
Persistent vegetative state ....................................................................................................1939
Diffuse axonal injury (loss of consciousness in concussion).....................................................1939
Logorrhea syndrome with hyperkinesia ................................................................................1940
MIDBRAIN (Mesencephalon) ................................................................................................1941
Dorsal midbrain syndrome ....................................................................................................1941
DIENCEPHALON ..................................................................................................................1942
The hypothalamus .................................................................................................................1943
The thalamus ........................................................................................................................1944
The epithalamus ....................................................................................................................1946
LIMBIC SYSTEM AND HIPPOCAMPUS ..............................................................................1947
CEREBRAL CORTEX ...........................................................................................................1949
Unilateral and diffuse, bilateral cortical destruction ...............................................................1949
Discussion .............................................................................................................................1952
Conclusions ...........................................................................................................................1953
.............................................................................................................................................. i
XXX
Foreword
As a neurosurgeon who has performed over 40 000 surgeries on the central and
peripheral nervous system during my 50 years of continuous neurosurgical activity, I can
comprehend the structural and functional complexity of the brain.
In order not to disturb the highly functional areas of the central nervous system, I
was forced to get familiar with the details of the brain map, which, taking into
consideration my experience, varies from individual to individual, and I can say that each
person, healthy or sick, is unique.
I have been an assiduous reader of many books and papers in order to have a better
documentation in this area, but I could not find any manual or book to contain relatively
complete and up-to-date information on the anatomy and physiology of the brain. The
existing neuroanatomy textbooks are not thorough enough, in my opinion, as they do not
explain the morphological and neurophysiological complexity of white and grey matter.
That is why – and also because of practical reasons – I decided to gather information
from books and recent papers and make a precise and complete synthesis of the structure
and functions of the brain, the most complex system in the universe, without claiming
originality. I carefully and respectfully quoted from the works of great neuroanatomists
and I mentioned them in references.
So, this book is not written by a neuroanatomist, but it represents a textbook
assembled by me as a neurosurgeon, as I have often had to deal with a variety of known
or still unknown, normal or abnormal activities of the brain during my numerous years
of activity.
To keep up with the vast literature in this research field, and with the investigations
of the brain as a whole has been for me a real challenge or better said an impossible task,
an unreachable goal.
The clinical pre- and post-op information has been of great help in understanding
the basic scientific concepts and the way in which the central nervous system, especially
the brain, operates and interacts in the presence of various internal and external harmful
factors, or in abnormal, pathological situations.
The turmoil of my life as a neurosurgeon came with sacrifices, sleepless nights, but
also with the great satisfaction of saving many lives, which has helped keeping my
physical and moral tonus.
The struggle with the mentalities, with the hostility of people and especially that one
of my colleagues sometimes brought me down, but never made me give up.
I have lived desolation and bitterness of painful defeats, but I have also won
extremely tough battles with the diseases I fought. I considered neurosurgery to be a true
XXXI
LEON DăNăILă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
religion to which I dedicated my entire life. I was selfish with myself but an offering
person to the ones around me. I have worked a lot without receiving help from anyone
and I became a well known name forgetting about my own life. I have always struggled
to conquer centimetre by centimetre the way I had chosen in life. But, in order to study
and write, I had to isolate myself from the surrounding reality and carry on with my
existence in a rather symbolic way.
Patients, beginning with the adult, paediatric or elderly population admitted into the
hospital with various cognitive disorders caused by cerebral tumours, vascular diseases,
parasites, traumatisms, developmental or psychiatric disorders and degenerative diseases
have always been thoroughly investigated and helped. The evaluation and diagnosis of
their cognitive topography represented the starting point for an early rehabilitating
treatment. In order to achieve this I carefully studied the neuropathology of different
morbid entities as well as their consequences upon the highly functional activities of the
cerebral cortex.
When I decided how to operate on a patient I have always considered the post-
surgical quality of life. The main goal of neurosurgical operations was to re-establish
patients’ possibility of going back to school, to work, and their ability to carry on their
own business, to drive and thus to have their own independence.
Publishing this book concurs with an enormous explosion of knowledge about the
morphology and physiology of the central nervous system and its vast reciprocal
connections and plasticity. Consequently, I found it hard to keep up with the multitude
of works published during the past ten years about functional neuroimaging,
neuropharmacology, computational modulation, rehabilitation methods, theories of
thinking, of memory, attention, frontal functions, language etc., as well as the structures
and the immense number of neural connections and columns that build them.
First, as a graduate of the Faculty of Psychology and then as a Professor in
neuropsychology at the same university, I had to prove accurate knowledge about the
central nervous system, its macroscopic, cellular and chemical architecture, primary and
associative areas for sight, hearing, taste, motion, sensibility, visceral, endocrine and
speech functions.
While psychology is the science that studies behavior, neuropsychology deals with
its neural and neuronal determinants. Modern neuroimagistics make the scanning of
healthy persons possible, clarifying the brain - behavior interrelationships. It also helps
us to comprehend the normal cerebral processes which concern language, recognizing
objects, attention, memory, thinking and control of movements.
These were the reasons that determined me for a period of 7 years, day by day, to
gather the most significant and up-to-date information regarding the activity of all areas,
structures, regions and cerebral connections – realizing, however, that I cannot exhaust
all subjects.
This book, which includes an immense material, starts with the history of
neuroscience, data and ideas referring to soul, mind and brain, the way they have been
imagined and conceived by healers, witches and philosophers since old times.
XXXII
FOREwORD
Nevertheless, the book aims at revealing some basic and recent data about mind and
brain, making them accessible to students, doctors, psychologists, biologists and all those
interested in this vast topic and research field – the brain – who are studying by
themselves.
The information included in the entire content of this book which comes from studies
on animals, primates and humans is non-exhaustive. Critics, comments, corrections and
suggestions from neuroanatomists, neurophysiologists, colleagues and all those who
come in contact and use this book are welcome.
No matter how much effort I made writing this book, from various reasons, objective
or subjective, I keep the doors open to corrections, additions and novelty and, why not,
to reinterpretation.
It’s me who will do it or maybe others will do it better than I did.
I wish to gratefully acknowledge the kind assistance of Mrs. Stela Georgescu in
typing and correcting the text on the computer and her great effort in arranging the
illustrations and the ample bibliography.
I also thank to Dorel Arsene, MD, for his generosity and attention during typing a
large part of the text on the computer, and to Mrs. Olga Dumitru, editorial assistant, for
her care and accuracy in correcting the entire text and references.
XXXIII
OCCIPITAL LOBE
Introduction
On the lateral surface of the brain, there are no definite boundaries between the
occipital lobes and the parietal and temporal lobes.
The occipital lobe encompasses the most posterior portion of the human cerebral
cortex and is primarily responsible for vision. The surface area of the human occipital
lobe encompasses approximately 12% of the total surface area of the neocortex of the
human brain. Vision begins with the spatial, temporal, and chromatic pattern of light
falling onto photoreceptors of the retina and culminates in the perception of the properties
of the objects and surfaces within the world around us (Deyoe, 2002). To permit these
more cognitive functions, the occipital lobe is heavily interconnected with other lobes of
the brain, especially the parietal and temporal lobes, as well as with an array of subcortical
structures. Through these connections, the results of visual processing in the occipital
lobe enter into and can be influenced by more general processes involved in goal-directed,
motivated behavior and “thinking” (Kaas, 1997).
Traditionally, occipital cortex has been subdivided into anatomically distinct regions
based on cytoarchitecture. Primary visual cortex is the most distinct region of the occipital
lobe. It is also known as striate cortex due to the stria of Gennari, a band of myelinated
axons running horizontally in layer 4B which demarcates the extent of striate cortex
within and adjacent to the calcarine sulcus.
VISUAL PATHWAYS
The retina arises as an evaginated portion of the brain, the optic pouch, which
secondarily is invaginated to form the two layered optic cup. The axons of ganglion cells,
at first unmyelinated, are arranged in fine radiating bundles which run parallel to the
retinal surface and converge at the optic disk (or optic papilla) to form the optic nerve.
This region of the retina contains no photoreceptors and, because it is insensible to light,
produces the perceptual phenomenon known as the blind spot. It is the site from which
the ophtalmic artery and veins enter (or leave) the eye. The subarachnoid space
1409
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
surrounding the optic nerve is continuous with that of the brain. As a result, increases in
intracranial pressure can be detected as papilledema, a swelling of the optic disk.
Optic nerves enter the cranial cavity through the optic foramina and unite to form
optic chiasma, beyond which they are continued as the optic tracts. In humans, about
60% of these fibres cross in the chiasma, while the other 40% continue toward the
thalamus and midbrain targets on the same side. So, within the chiasma a partial
deccussation occurs, the fibres from the nasal halves of the retina crossing to the opposite
side, and those from the temporal halves of the retina remaining uncrossed. In binocular
vision each visual field, right or left, is projected upon portion of both retinae. Thus the
images of objects in the right field of vision are projected on the right nasal and the left
temporal half of the retina. In the chiasma the fibres from those two retinal portions are
combined to form the left optic tract, which represents the complete right field of vision.
By this arrangement the whole right field of vision is projected upon the left hemisphere,
and the left visual field upon the right hemisphere. Once past of the chiasma, the ganglion
cell axons on each side form the optic tract. Thus, the optic tract, unlike the optic nerve,
contains fibres from both eyes. The partial crossing of ganglion cell axons at the optic
chiasma allows information from corresponding points on the two retinas to be processed
by approximately the same cortical site in each hemisphere.
On each side the optic tract sweeps outward and backward, encircling the
hypothalamus and the rostral portion of the crus cerebri (Hubel, 1988).
The ganglion cell axons in the optic tract reach a number of structures in the
diencephalon and midbrain. The major target in the diencephalon is the dorsal lateral
geniculate nucleus of the thalamus. The lateral geniculate nucleus gives rise to the
geniculocalcarine tract which forms the last relay to the visual cortex. Neurons in the
lateral geniculate nucleus, like their counterparts in the thalamic relays of other sensory
systems, send their axons to the cerebral cortex via the internal capsule (Hubel and
Wiesel, 1968; Hubel, 1988).
The superior colliculus is concerned with detection of movement with the visual
fields and with the coordination of eye and head movements. The pretectal region is
concerned with the pupillary light reflex (Fig. 18.1).
Geniculocalcarine tract arises from the lateral geniculate nucleus, passes through
the retrolenticular portion of the internal capsule and forms the optic radiations, which
end in the striate cortex (also referred to as Brodmann’s area 17 or V1), which lies largely
along and within the calcarine fissure in the occipital lobe.
The retinogeniculostriate pathway, or primary visual pathway, convey information
that is essential for most of what is thought of seeing.
Thus, damage anywhere along this route results in serious visual impairment.
A second major target of the ganglion cell axons is a collection of neurons that lies
between the thalamus and the midbrain in a region known as the pretectum. The
pretectum is important as the coordinating center for the pupillary light reflex. The initial
component of the pupillary light reflex pathway is a bilateral projection from the retina
to the pretectum.
1410
18. Occipital lobe
Fig. 18.2. Pathways in the visual system: from the eye to V1. A schematic drawing of the pathways in binocular vision,
showing the visual input from left and right visual fields (top of figure) through the optic nerve and optic tract (center of
figure), continuing on through the LGN and onto V1 in the cortex (bottom of the figure). (Source: Standring, 2005).
Most of the rest of the field of view can be seen by both eyes; i.e., individual points
in visual space lie in the nasal visual field of one eye and the temporal visual field of the
other. However, the inferior nasal fields are less extensive than the superior nasal field,
1413
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
and consequently the binocular field of view is smaller in the lower visual field than in
the upper.
The topographic arrangement of photoreceptors in the retina is maintained in the central
connection. Neurons representing the vertical midline of the visual field are represented in
both hemispheres and are functionally linked by interhemispheric, callosal connections.
Anyhow, visual space is systematically distorted in the cortical representation.
Ganglion cells that lie in the nasal division of each retina give rise to axons that cross
in the chiasma, while those that lie in the temporal retina give rise to axons that remain
on the same side. Images of objects in the left visual hemifield fall on the nasal retina of
the left eye and the temporal retina of the right eye, and the axons from ganglion cells in
these regions of the two retinas project through the right optic tract. Thus, unlike the optic
nerve, the optic tract contains the axons of ganglion cells that originate in both eyes and
represent the contralateral field of view.
For the primary visual pathway, the map of the contralateral hemifield that is
established in the lateral geniculate nucleus is maintained in the projection of the lateral
geniculate nucleus to the striate cortex.
Functional neuroimaging (fMRI) has been used to chart the retinotopic organization of
visual areas in the human occipital lobe (Zilles and Clarke, 1997). The resulting flat maps
show bands of activation corresponding to alternating representations of quadrants of the
horizontal and vertical meridian. On the medial wall of the hemisphere, these bands are
oriented roughly parallel to the calcarine sulcus (Deyoe and van Essen, 1988; Deyoe, 2002).
From maps of the meridian representations, it is possible to estimate the locations
of the borders between different visual areas.
Thus the fovea is represented in the posterior part of the striate cortex, whereas the
more peripheral regions of the retina are represented in progressively more anterior parts
of the striate cortex. The upper visual field is mapped below the calcarine sulcus, and the
lower visual field above it. Like the representation of the hand region in the somatic
sensory cortex, the representation of the macula is therefore disproportionately large,
occupying most of the caudal pole of the occipital lobe.
Anatomy
The occipital lobes form the posterior pole of the cerebral hemispheres, lying under
the occipital bone at the back of the skull. Viewed from the medial surface of the cerebral
hemisphere, the occipital lobe is distinguished from the parietal lobe by the parieto-
occipital sulcus (Fig. 18.3). No clear landmarks separate the occipital cortex from the
temporal or parietal cortex on the lateral surface of the hemisphere, however, because
the occipital tissue merges with the other regions. For practical purposes, an imaginary
line running laterally from the dorsal tip of the parieto-occipital sulcus to the preoccipital
notch is considered the effective boundary separating the occipital lobe from the parietal
and temporal lobes.
The lack of clear landmarks makes it difficult to define the extent of the occipital
areas precisely and has led to much confusion about the exact boundaries.
1414
18. Occipital lobe
Within the visual cortex, there are three clear landmarks. The most prominent is the
calcarine sulcus, which is flanked above by the cuneus and below by the lingula, and
which contains much of the primary cortex. The calcarine fissure divides the upper and
lower halves of visual world. On the ventral surface of the hemisphere there are two gyri
(lingual and fusiform). The lingual gyrus, separated from the more laterally placed
fusiform gyrus by the collateral sulcus, includes part of visual cortical regions V2 and
VP, whereas V4 is in the fusiform gyrus.
The fusiform gyrus is then bounded laterally by the occipitotemporal sulcus,
although this sulcus tends to be variable and interrupted as it extends posteriorly toward
the occipital pole.
The lateral surface of the occipital lobe is especially variable from individual to
individual.
Consequently, the lateral occipital gyrus can be irregular and can be split by the
lateral occipital sulcus, which has several important variants, sometimes appearing as
one, two, or even three small sulci running approximately in the anterior-posterior
direction. Most dorsally on the lateral surface there is the transverse occipital sulcus,
which often forms the most posterior end of the intraparietal sulcus.
Cytoarchitecture
As for all neocortex, the occipital gray matter is laminated, though the criteria for
identifying and naming different layers have varied considerably. The monkey occipital
cortex was first divided by Brodmann into three regions (areas 17, 18, and 19), but studies
using imaging, physiological, and even anatomical techniques have produced much finer
subdivisions.
Although the map is still not complete, the consensus is that the occipital cortex
contains at least nine different visual areas (V1, V2, V3, VP, V3a, V4d, V4v, DP, and
MT also known as V5) (Tootell and Hadjikhani, 2001) (Fig. 18.4).
The precise locations of the human homologues are still not settled. In a map
constructed by Tootell and Hadjikhani there are included all the monkey areas as well as
an additional color-sensitive area (V8) (Fig. 18.5).
1415
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
1416
18. Occipital lobe
Some of the areas contain a complete visual field, whereas others have only an upper
or lower visual field. This distinction is curious, and Previc (1990) has suggested that the
upper and lower fields may have different functions, with the upper more specialized for
visual search and recognition and the lower specialized for visuomotor guidance.
Primary visual cortex is the most cytoarchitecturally distinct region of the occipital
lobe. It is also known as striate cortex, due to the stria of Gennari, which can be identified
even in a cross section of freshly cut tissue (Fig. 18.6).
Fig. 18.7. Cytoarchitectural map of human cortex. A, Convex surface; B, medial surface (after Brodmann, 1909).
So, from lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), neurons send their signals to the primary
visual cortex, called V1, because it is the first cortical visual area. The left LGN projects to
V1 in the left hemisphere which contains a retinotopic map of the entire right visual field.
The right LGN project to the right V1 which contains a map of the entire left visual field.
Its complex laminar organization is probably the most distinct of all cortical areas.
Although we usually say that the cortex has six layers, it is possible to see many more in area
V1, partly because layer IV alone features four distinct layers (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
1418
18. Occipital lobe
fail to recall colors from before their injuries or even to imagine colors. The loss of area V4
results in the loss of color cognition, or the ability to think about color (Meadows, 1974;
Sacks and Wasserman, 1987; Tanaka et al., 2001; Tootell and Hadjikhani, 2001).
Similarly, a lesion in area V5 produces an inability to perceive objects in motion.
Objects at rest are perceived but, when the objects begin to move, they vanish. In
principle, a lesion in area V3 will affect from perception but, because area V4 also
processes form, a rather large lesion of both V3 and V4 would be required to eliminate
form perception (Zihl et al., 1983; Servas et al., 1993; Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994).
An important constraint of the function of areas V3, V4, and V5 is that all these areas
receive major input from area V1. People with lesions in area V1, act as though they are
blind, but visual input can still get through to higher levels-partly through small projections
of the lateral geniculate nucleus to area V2 partly through projections from the colliculus
to the thalamus (the pulvinar) to the cortex. This is the tectopulvinar pathway.
So, projections to the superior colliculus can reach the cortex through relays in the
lateral posterior-pulvinar complex of the thalamus. Because this so-called tectopulvinar
pathway constitutes the visual system in fish, amphibians and reptiles, we can expect it
to be capable of a reasonably sophisticated vision. Because there are two visual pathways
to the neocortex, complete destruction of the main pathway, the geniculostriate pathway,
does not render a subject completely blind (Poggio, 1968; Kaas, 1987; Livingston and
Hubel, 1988).
People with V1 lesions seem not to be aware of visual input and can be shown to
have some aspects of vision only by special testing.
Area V1 thus appears to be primary for vision in yet another sense: V1 must function
for the brain to make sense out of what the more specialized visual areas are processing.
So, people with significant V1 damage are capable of some awareness of visual
information, such as motion. Barbur and colleagues (1993) suggested that the integrity
of area V3 may allow this conscious awareness, but this suggestion remains a hypothesis.
Fig. 18.8. Subdivisions of the extrastriate cortex in the macaque monkey. (A) Each of the subdivisions indicated contains
neurons that respond to visual stimulation. Many are buried in sulci, and the overlying cortex must be removed in order to
expose them. Some of more extensively studied extrastriate areas are specifically identified (V2, V3, V4 and MT).
V1 is the primary visual cortex; MT is the middle temporal area. (B) The arrangement of extrastriate and other areas of
neocortex in a flattened view of the monkey neocortex. There are at least 25 areas that are predominantly or exclusively
visual in function, plus 7 other areas suspected to play a role in visual processing.
(A after Maunsell and Newson, 1987; B after Felleman and Van Essen, 1991).
Functional MRI studies have indicated a similar arrangement of visual areas within
human extrastriate cortex (Sereno et al., 1995). Using retinotopically restricted stimuli it has
been possible to localize at least 10 separate representations of the visual field (Fig. 18.9).
One of these areas exhibits a large motion-selective signal, suggesting that it is the
homologue of the motion-selective middle temporal area described in monkeys. Another
area exhibits color-selective responses, suggesting that it may be similar to V4 in non-
human primates.
So, in clinical description, a well-studied patient, who suffered a stroke that damaged
the extrastriate region thought to be comparable to area MT in the monkey, was unable
to appreciate the motion of objects. The neurologist who treated her noted that she had
difficulty in pouring tea into a cup because the fluid seemed to be “frozen”. In addition,
she could not stop pouring at the right time because she was unable to perceive when the
fluid level had risen to the rim. The patient also had trouble following a dialogue because
she could not follow the movement of the speaker’s mouth. Crossing the street was
potentially terrifying because she could not judge the movement of approaching cars.
1422
18. Occipital lobe
Fig. 18.9. Localization of multiple visual areas in the human brain using fMRI. (A, B) Lateral and medial views
(respectively) or the human brain, illustrating the location of primary visual cortex (V1) and additional visual areas V2, V3,
VP (ventral posterior area), V4, MT (middle temporal area), and MST (medial superior temporal area). (C) Unfolded and
flattened view of retinotopically defined visual areas in the occipital lobe. Dark grey areas correspond to cortical regions
that were buried in sulci; light regions correspond to regions that were located on the surface of gyri. Visual areas in
humans show a close resemblance to visual areas originally defined in monkeys (after Sereno et al., 1995).
Her ability to perceive other features of the visual scene, such as color and form, was
intact (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982; Zihl et al., 1983; Sereno et al., 1995; Tootell et
al., 1996; Courtney and Ungerleider, 1997; Purves et al., 2004).
Another example of a specific visual deficit as a result of damage to extrastriate
cortex is cerebral achromatopsia. These patients lose the ability to see the world in color,
although other aspects of vision remain in good working order. The normal colors of a
visual scene are described as being replaced by “dirty” shades of gray, much like looking
at a poor quality black-and-white movie (Purves et al., 2004).
When achromatopsic individuals are asked to draw objects from memory, they have
no difficulty with shapes, but are unable to appropriately color the objects they have
represented (Purves et al., 2004).
It is important to distinguish this condition from the color blindness that arises from
the congenital absence of one or more cone pigments in the retina. The discovery of the
1423
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
three human cone types and their different absorption spectra is correctly regarded,
therefore, as the basis for human color vision. Nevertheless, how these human cone types
and higher-order neurons they contact produce the sensation of color is still unclear.
A fundamental problem has been that, although the relative activities of three cone
types can more or less explain the colors perceived in color-matching experiments
performed in the laboratory, the perception of color is strongly influenced by the context.
The phenomena of color contrast and color constancy led to a heated, modern debate
about how color percepts are generated that now spans several decades. For Land (1986),
the answer lay in a series of ratiometric equations that could integrate the spectral returns
of different regions over the entire scene. In achromatopsia, the three types of cones are
functioning normally; it is damage to specific extrastriate cortical areas that renders the
patient unable to use the information supplied by the retina (Purves et al., 2004).
Extrastriate cortical areas are organized into two largely separate systems that eventually
feed information into cortical association areas in the temporal and parietal lobes.
One system, called the ventral stream, includes area V4 and leads from the striate
cortex into the inferior part of the temporal lobe. This system is responsible for high-
resolution form vision and object recognition. Neurons in the ventral stream exhibit
properties that are important for object recognition, such as selectivity for shape, color,
texture, and faces.
Lesions of the inferotemporal cortex produce profound impairments in the ability
to perform recognition tasks but no impairment in spatial tasks.
The dorsal stream, which includes the middle temporal area, leads from striate cortex
into the parietal lobe. This system is responsible for spatial aspects if vision such analysis of
motion and positional relationships between objects in the visual scene (Purves et al., 2004).
Neurons in the dorsal stream show selectively for direction, and speed of movement.
Lesions of the parietal cortex severely impair an animal’s ability to distinguish
objects on the basis of their position, while having little effect on its ability to perform
object recognition tasks.
These effects are remarkably similar to the syndrome associated with damage to the
parietal and temporal lobes in humans.
These discoveries led to the concept that individual cortical visual areas can contain
multiple, distinct processing pathways or streams, thereby extending (though not
necessarily reiterating) the organizational principle of multiple processing pathway found
in the retina and lateral geniculate nucleus.
Connectivity
By the late 1960s, the consensus was that the visual cortex is hierarchically
organized, with visual information proceeding from area 17 to areas 18 and 19.
Each visual area was thought to provide some sort of elaboration on the processing
of preceding area. This strictly hierarchical view is now considered too simple, and has
been replaced by the notion of a distributed hierarchical process, with multiple parallel
and interconnecting pathways at each level (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
1424
18. Occipital lobe
So, the primary visual pathway from the retina to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus
in the thalamus and on the primary visual cortex is the most important and certainly the
most thoroughly studied. Different classes of neurons within this pathway encode the
varieties of visual information-luminance, spectral differences, orientation, and motion.
The parallel processing of different categories of visual information continues in
cortical pathways that extend beyond primary visual cortex, supplying a variety of visual
areas in the occipital, parietal and temporal lobes.
Visual areas in the temporal lobe are primarily involved in object recognition,
whereas those in the parietal lobe are concerned with motion. Normal vision depends on
the integration of information in all these cortical areas.
So, distinct population of retinal ganglion cells sends their axons to a number of
central visual structures that serve different functions. The most important projections
are to the pretectum for mediating the pupillary light reflex, to the hypothalamus for the
regulation of circadian rhythms, to the superior colliculus for the regulation of eye and
head movements, and – most important at all – to the lateral geniculate nucleus for
mediating vision and visual perception.
The primary visual pathway (retinogeniculostriate projection) is arranged topo-
graphically such that central visual structures contain an organized map of the
contralateral visual field.
Visual input
According to Deyoe (2002) visual input from the retina is relayed through the lateral
geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus to terminate in the primary visual cortex
(striate, cortex, area 17, V1).
A second major pathway arises from retinal projections that bypass the LGN and
project to the superior colliculus. The superior colliculus then projects to the thalamic
pulvinar nucleus, which in turn distributes widely to the cortex of the occipital lobe. The
layers in the lateral geniculate are distinguished on the basis of cell size. The
geniculocortical pathways are subdivided into three main components associated with
different neuronal subclasses in the retina and LGN (Maunssell, 1992).
Two ventral layers are composed of large neurons and are referred to as the
magnocellular layers, while more dorsal layers are composed of small neurons and are
referred to as the parvocellular layers. The axons of the relay cells in the magno- and
parvocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus terminate on distinct populations of
neurons located in separate strata within layer 4 of striate cortex.
One pathway originates from small P-cells in the retina and is relayed through the
parvocellular layers of the LGN to terminate in the layer 4Cβ of V1 and more sparsely
in layers 4A and 6. A second pathway to striate cortex begins with large M-type retinal
ganglion cells and is relayed through the magnocellular layers of the LGN to terminate
in layer 4Cα accompanied by a light projection to layer 6.
The response properties of P and M ganglion cells provide important clues about
the contributions of the magno- and parvocellular streams to visual perception.
1425
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
M ganglion cells have larger receptive fields than P cells, and their axons have faster
conduction velocities.
M and P ganglion cells also differ in ways that are not so obviously related to their
morphology. M cells respond transiently to the presentation of visual stimuli, while P
cells respond in a sustained fashion. Moreover, P ganglion cells can transmit information
about color, whereas M cells cannot. P cells convey color information because of their
receptive field centers and surroundings are driven by different classes of cones (i.e.,
cones responding with greatest sensitivity to short-, medium-, or long-wavelength light).
For example, some P ganglion cells have centers that receive inputs from long wavelength
(“red”) sensitive cones and surrounds that receive inputs from medium-wavelength
(“green”) cones. Others have centers that receive inputs from “green cones” and surrounds
from “red cones”. As a result, P cells are sensitive to differences in the wavelengths of light
striking their receptive field center and surroundings (Ungerleder and Mishkin, 1982; Purves
and Lotto, 2003). Although M ganglion cells also receive inputs from cones, there is no
difference in the type of cone input to the receptive field center and surroundings; the center
and surroundings of each M cell receptive field is driven by all cone types.
A third source of projections to V1 comes from a class of small cells within the
koniocellular (K) or intercalar layers of the LGN.
These neurons typically receive input from both the retina and the superior colliculus
and terminate in the supragranular layers (above layer 4) of striate cortex. These afferents
tend to cluster into “pufflike” regions of layers 2-3 that are also unique for their high
levels of metabolic enzyme cytochrome oxidase (co).
Afferents to striate cortex via the alternate tectopulvinar pathway terminate most
heavily in laminae 1 and upper 2-3 (Horton, 1992; Kaas, 1997; Zilles and Clarke, 1997).
So, neurons contributing to the K-cell pathway reside in the interlaminar zones that
separate lateral geniculate layers. These neurons receive inputs from fine-caliber retinal
axons and project in a patchy fashion to the superficial layers (layers II and III) of striate
cortex. Although the contribution of the K-cell pathway to perception is not understood,
it appears that some aspects of color vision, especially information derived from short-
wavelength-sensitive cones, may be transmitted via the K-cell rather the P-cell pathway
(Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Hubel, 1988; Ungerleider and Mishikin, 1982; Berson, 2003;
Chalupa and Werner, 2004).
Neurons of the P, M, and K pathways differ in their visual response properties. Neurons
of the parvocellular path have smaller receptive fields, are often color opponents, and are
well-suited for conveying information about fine spatial detail and color. Magnocellular
neurons tend to have larger receptive fields, prefer low spatial frequencies, and have more
transient responses. They tend to have the greatest sensitivity to luminance contrast,
especially at low spatial frequencies and low luminance levels. They are optimized for
processing rapid temporal changes such as flicker and movement (Fig. 18.10).
The function of K-pathway neurons has appeared to be a major determinant of co-
puff cell properties and may play a key role in the modulation of responses evoked by
the M and P pathways (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Casagrande and Kaas, 1994).
1426
18. Occipital lobe
Fig. 18.10 (A) Distribution of thalamic inputs to the occipital lobe and nearby portions of the parietal and temporal
lobes. (From Zilles and Clarke, 1997). (B) Concurrent input to V1 from K, M, and P retinogeniculate pathways
with schematic of intrinsic circuitry and cytochrome oxidase defined puffs (dashed ellipses). V2 receives segregated
input from different sets of output neurons in V1 and distributes projections to the different sets of extrastriate visual
areas via distinct populations of output neurons in different cytocrome oxidase defined compartments. Such circuitry
forms the anatomical basis of multiple concurrent processing pathways within and among occipital visual areas
(Adapted from Casagrande and Kaas, 1994). (C) Functional interpretation of V1 circuitry
(Adapted from Callaway, 1989). Abbreviations: LGN, lateral geniculate nucleus; sc, superior colliculus;
K, konicellular; M, magnocellular; P, parvocellular.
So, although these three pathways are distinct, their functional capabilities can
overlap significantly. It is primarily at the extremes of spatial and temporal frequencies
that the M- and P-cell capabilities are most different.
Both M and P pathways may contribute to processing intermediate spatial and
temporal frequencies, but P-cell will tend to dominate for high spatial frequencies that
are slowly varying. Conversely, magnocellular neurons will respond better than P-cells
at low spatial frequencies that are rapidly varying. Chromatically, P-cell color opponency
appears to be critical for hue discrimination, but M-cells can respond more strongly to
some wavelengths than others, though they are not opponent and their tuning is typically
broader than that of P-cells. Under natural viewing conditions, all three inputs to the
visual cortex are likely to be concurrently active. Under extreme visual conditions, the
specialized capabilities of one or another of the pathways may take over and thereby
extend the range of useful sight (Horton, 1992; Callaway, 1998; Deyoe, 2002).
Thus the retinogeniculate pathway is composed of parallel magnocellular and
parvocellular streams that convey distinct types of information to the initial stages of
cortical processing. Damage anywhere along the primary visual pathway which includes
the optic nerve, optic tract, lateral geniculate nucleus, optic radiation, radiation, and striate
cortex, results in a loss of vision confined to the predictible region of visual space.
1427
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Damage to the magnocellular layers has little effect on visual acuity or color vision,
but sharply reduces the ability to perceive rapidly changing stimuli. In contrast, damage
to the parvocellular layers has no effect on motion perception, but severely impairs visual
acuity and color perception.
Visual information conveyed by the parvocellular stream is particularly important
for high spatial resolution vision – the detailed analysis of the shape, size, and color of
objects. The magnocellular stream, on the other hand, appears critical for tasks that
require high temporal resolution, such as evaluating the location, speed and direction of
a rapidly moving object (Zihl et al., 1983; Rodieck, 1998; Purves et al., 2004).
– In sum, primary visual pathway is composed of separate functional streams that
convey information from different types of retinal ganglion cells to the initial stages of
cortical processing. The magnocellular stream conveys information that is critical for
detection of rapidly changing stimuli, the parvocellular stream mediates high acuity vision
and appears to share responsibility for color vision with the konicellular stream. Beyond
the striate cortex, parcellation of function continues in the ventral and dorsal streams that
lead to the extrastriate and association areas in temporal and parietal lobes, respectively.
Areas in the inferotemporal cortex are especially important in object recognition, whereas
areas in the parietal lobe are critical for understanding the spatial relations between objects
in the visual field.
Intrinsic circuitry
The retinal information relayed through the LGN is processed further by the intrinsic
circuitry of V1 and then distributed to other cortical areas via output neurons in layers 2-3
(Callaway, 1998) (Fig. 18.11). Subcortical projections to the superior colliculus and
feedback projection to LGN originate from layers 5 and 6 respectively.
It is through this selective distribution of visual information combined with the
characteristics of local processing that different visual areas achieve their functional
uniqueness.
Hubel and Wiesel (1962) used microelectrode recording to examine the properties of
neurons in more central visual structures. The responses of neurons in the lateral geniculate
nucleus were found to be remarkably similar to those in the retina, with a center-surrounded
receptive field organization and selectivity for luminance increases or decreases. However,
the small spots of light that were so effective at stimulating neurons in the retina and LGN
were largely ineffective in the visual cortex. Instead, most cortical neurons in cats and
monkeys responded vigorously to light-dark bars or edges, and only if the bars were
presented at a particular range of orientations within the cell’s receptive field.
The responses of a large number of single cells, Hubel and Wiesel (1962),
demonstrated that all edge orientation was roughly equally represented in visual cortex.
As a result, a given orientation in a visual scene appears to be “encoded” in the activity
of a distinct population of orientation-selective neurons.
Hubel and Wiesel (1962) also found that there are subtly different subtypes within
a class of neurons that preferred the same orientation. For example, the receptive field of
1428
18. Occipital lobe
some cortical cells, which they called simple cells, were composed of spatially separate
“on” and “off” response zones, as if the “on” and “off” centers of lateral geniculate cells
that supplied these neurons were arrayed in separate parallel bands. Other neurons,
referred to as complex cells, exhibited mixed “on” and “off” responses throughout their
receptive fields as if they received their inputs from a number of simple cells. Further
analysis uncovered cortical neurons sensitive to the length of the bar of light, decreasing
their rate of response when the bar exceeded a certain length. Still other cells responded
selectively to the direction in which an edge moved across their receptive field. Anyhow,
there is little doubt that the specificity of the receptive field properties of neurons in the
striate cortex (and beyond) play an important role in determining the basic attributes of
visual scenes (Purves et al., 2004).
Fig. 18.11. Central projections of retinal ganglion cells. Ganglion cell axons terminate in the lateral geniculate nucleus
of the thalamus, the superior colliculus, the pretectum, and the hypothalamus. For clarity, only the crossing axons of
the right eye are shown (view is looking up at the inferior surface of the brain) (after Purves et al., 2004).
Another feature is the binocularity. Most cortical neurons have binocular receptive
field, and these fields are almost identical, having the same size, shape, preferred
orientation, and roughly the same position in the visual field of each eye.
Bringing together the inputs from the two eyes at the level of the striate cortex
provides a basis for stereopsis, the special sensation of depth that arises from viewing
nearby objects with two eyes instead of one. Because the two eyes look at the world from
slightly different angles, objects that lie in front of or behind the plane of fixation project
to noncorresponding points on the two retinas. For a small disturbance on either side of
1429
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the plane of fixation, where the disparity between the two views of the world remains
modest, a single image is perceived; the disparity between the two eye views of objects
nearer or farther than the point of fixation is interpreted as depth (Purves et al., 2004).
An important advance in studies of stereopsis was made in 1959 when Julesz (1971,
1995) discovered an ingenious way of showing that stereoscopy depends on matching
information seen by the two eyes without any prior recognition of what object(s) such
matching might generate.
Although the neurophysiological basis of stereopsis is not understood, some neurons
in the striate cortex and in other visual cortical areas have receptive field properties that
make them good candidates for extracting information about binocular disparity.
Unlike many binocular cells whose monocular receptive fields sample the same
region of visual space, these neurons have monocular fields that are slightly displaced
(or perhaps differ in their internal organization) so that the cell is maximally activated
by stimuli that fall on noncorresponding parts of the retinas (Purves et al., 2004).
Some of these neurons (so-called far cells) discharge to disparities beyond the plane
of fixation, while others (near cells) respond to disparities in front of the plane of fixation.
The pattern of activity in these different classes of neurons seems likely to contribute to
the sensation of stereoscopic depth (Julesz, 1995; Tootell et al., 1996).
So, the intrinsic processing of V1 can be divided into two major levels. Input from
LGN terminates at the first level in the different subdivisions of layer 4C. Information is
then distributed primarily to level 2 output neurons within layers 2-3, and 4B. The latter
output signals are modified by neurons in layers 5 and 6 that combine information about
the inputs and outputs of each processing level and feed it back onto neurons at the same
level (Zilles and Clarke, 1997; Kaas, 1997).
The laminar organization of V1 circuitry is complemented by horizontal connections
that tend to connect functionally similar zones distributed within specific laminae.
In layers 2-3, long horizontal projections preferentially interconnect puff-like zones
containing cells that have similar visual response properties and that are rich in the
metabolic enzyme cytochrome oxidase.
Likewise, interpuff zones tend to be interconnected most strongly with other interpuff
zones. In V2, a similar pattern of specific horizontal connections link co-defined
compartments termed the thick-, thin-, and interstripe zones (Kaas, 1997; Callaway, 1998).
Together, the laminar specificity of vertical intrinsic connections and the functional
specificity of horizontal connections provide an anatomical substrate for creating different
subsets of output neurons whose visual response properties reflect different combinations
of the P, M, and K afferent pathways, as well as modulatory effects of cortical feedback
pathways and other subcortical inputs (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Zilles and Clarke,
1997; Kaas, 1997; Callaway, 1998).
Output neurons in layer 4B of V1 are dominated by M-pathway characteristics, as
are the responses of the extrastriate visual areas, such as V2 thick stripes and area MT,
to which the 4B cells project.
In contrast, output neurons in the interpuff regions of V1 tend to be strongly biased
by P-cell characteristics, which are subsequently imparted to downstream visual areas
1430
18. Occipital lobe
such as V2 (thin- and interstripe compartments), VP, and V4. Output neurons in co-puff
subdivisions tend to exhibit characteristics that appear to reflect a mixture of influences.
As a result, later processing stages, such as V4, can also be shown to exhibit a
mixture of influences.
Overall, then the intrinsic circuitry of V1 acts to process, combine, and redistribute
the visual information available in the different afferent pathways, thereby creating a new
set of output signals that reflects the afferent inputs, but that also encodes more complex
visual properties or features. Intrinsic circuitry within subsequent cortical processing
stages, though less well-studied, presumably functions in a similar manner (Callaway,
1998; Deyoe, 2002).
Corticocortical connectivity
The output neurons of layers 2-3 and 4B of V1 selectively distribute different types of
visual information to subsequent processing stage in extrastriate visual cortex. Each
extrastriate visual area itself has unique feedforward and feedback connections within and,
often, outside the occipital lobe. These patterns of selective connectivity are a primary
determinant of the functional specificity of each visual area. Finally, interhemispheric
projections through the corpus callosum connect the left and right halves of each visual
area into a functionally integrated whole (Kaas, 1997; Callaway, 1998; Deyoe, 2002).
In monkey, connections among cortical visual areas tend to follow a consistent
pattern of laminar distribution that differentiates forward, backward, and lateral types of
connectivity (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991).
Generally, forward connections arise from neurons in layers 2-3 of the lower area
and distribute to layer 4, tapering off into the lower reaches of layer 3 in the higher visual
area. In some visual areas, however, forward projections can also originate in subgranular
layers (below layer 4), though the terminal distribution remains targeted at layer 4 of the
recipient area (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Callaway, 1998; Deyoe, 2002).
In contrast, backward-type projections tend to originate in layer 6 of the higher area
and distribute outside the layer of the lower area, though some feedback projections can
have a bilaminar origin. Finally, some visual areas have interconnections, whose terminal
fields engage nearly all laminae. This distribution pattern is thought to characterize lateral
connections between visual areas at approximately the same processing level. On the
basis of these patterns of connectivity, it is possible to arrange the various occipital visual
areas into a processing hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991).
The extent to which this plan applies to human visual cortex is not known, though
the overall scheme is likely to be similar (Deyoe and Van Essen, 1988).
This connectional hierarchy incorporates several important organizational features.
First, it is generally consistent with the concept that successively more complex visual
response properties are represented at successive processing stages, though a detailed
comparison of the response properties of efferent neurons at one stage with the properties of
recipient neurons at the next stage are generally lacking. Another key aspect of this cortico-
cortical network is that nearly every forward connection is matched by a corresponding
backward connection, thereby establishing reciprocity between visual areas.
1431
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
E
Fig. 18.13. Columnar organization of orientation selectivity in the monkey
striate cortex. Vertical electrode penetrations encounter neurons with the
same preferred orientations, whereas oblique penetrations show a
systematic change in orientation across the cortical surface. The circles
denote the lack of orientation-selective cells in layer IV. (after Purves et F
al., 2004).
1433
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
This patterned circuit has led to conclude that modules are a fundamental feature of
the cerebral cortex, essential for perception, cognition, and perhaps even consciousness.
Although modular circuits of a given class are readily seen in the brains of some
species, they have not been found in the same brain regions of other, sometimes closely
related, animals. Second, not all regions of the mammillian cortex are organized in a
modular fashion. And third, no clear function of such modules has been discerned (Purves
et al., 2004).
1993; Obermeyer and Blasdel, 1993). This approach has now been applied to both striate
and extrastriate areas in both experimental animals and human patients undergoing
neurosurgery (Purves et al., 2004). The results emphasize that maps of stimulus features
are general principle of cortical organization.
This powerful technique can also be used to determine how maps for different
stimulus properties are arranged relative to one another, and to detect additional maps
such as that for direction of motion. A comparison of ocular dominance hands and
orientation preference maps shows that pinwheel centers are generally located in the
center of ocular dominance bands, and that the iso-orientation contours that emanate from
the pinwheel centers run orthogonal to the border of ocular dominance bands (Bonhoeffer
and Grinvald, 1993; Obermeyer and Blasdel, 1993).
These systematic relationships between the functional maps that coexist within
primary visual cortex are thought to ensure that all combinations of stimulus features
(orientation, direction, ocular dominance, and spatial frequency) are analyzed for all
regions of visual space (Bonhoeffer and Grinvald, 1993; Obermeyer and Blasdel, 1993).
Although most neurons in the striate cortex respond to stimulation of both eyes, the
relative strength of the inputs from the two eyes varies from neuron to neuron. At the
extremes of this continuum are neurons that respond almost exclusively to the left or
right eye; in the middle are those that respond equally well to both eyes. As in the case
of orientation preference, vertical electrode penetrations tend to encounter neurons with
similar ocular preference (or ocular dominance), whereas tangential penetrations show
gradual shifts in ocular dominance (Tootell et al., 1996; Hubel and Wiesel, 1977; Tootell
et al., 1996; Chalupa and Werner, 2004; Purves et al., 2004). These shifts in ocular
dominance result from the ocular segregation of the inputs from lateral geniculate nucleus
within cortical layer IV (Horton, 1992).
In short, the striate cortex is composed of repeating units, or modules, that contain
all the neuronal machinery necessary to analyze a small region of visual space for variety
of different stimulus attributes.
A number of other cortical regions show a similar columnar arrangement of their
processing circuitry.
Compared to retinal ganglion cells, neurons at higher levels of the visual pathway
become increasingly selective in their stimulus requirements. Thus, most neurons in the
striate cortex respond to light-dark edges only if they are presented at a certain orientation;
some are selective for the length of the edge, and others to movement of the edge in a
specific direction. Indeed, a point in visual space is related to a set of cortical neurons,
each of which is specialized for processing a limited set of the attributes in the visual
stimulus. The neural circuitry in the striate cortex also brings together information from
the two eyes; most cortical neurons (other than those in layer IV, which are segregated
into eye-specific columns) have binocular responses. Binocular convergence is
presumably essential for the detection of binocular disparity, an important component of
depth perception (Purves et al., 2004).
1435
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Functional subsystem
The incoming visual information is relayed from the retina through the lateral
geniculate nucleus to V1 and then to V2 and other extrastriate visual areas.
So, V1 (the striate cortex) is the primary vision area: it receives the largest input
from the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus and it projects to all other occipital
regions. V1 is the first processing level in the hierarchy.
V2 also projects to all other occipital regions. V2 is the second level.
After V2, three distinct parallel pathways emerge on route to the parietal cortex,
superior temporal sulcus, and inferior temporal cortex, for further processing.
As we shall see in more detail shortly, the parietal pathway, or dorsal stream, has a
role in the visual guidance of movement, and the inferior temporal pathway, or ventral
stream, is concerned with object perception (including color).
The middle pathway along the superior temporal sulcus is probably important in
visuospatial functions.
At hierarchical levels beyond V1 and V2, part of the visual information goes to areas
V3 and VP (Horton, 1992).
Motion perception
Human hMTS+ respond selectively to moving vs. stationary stimuli, but also responds
to other aspects of motion stimuli in ways that appear to parallel reported single-unit
response properties or perceptual effects. Motion produces a significant change in activation
within hMT+, yet it can result in little or no change in activation V1-V2 (Deyoe, 2002).
Though retinal image motion often indicates the movement of objects (or self),
motion cues can also be used to identify the boundaries and 3-dimensional shape of
objects rather than their trajectories.
In summary, the middle-temporal area, or what is commonly called area MT, is
important for motion perception. Almost all of the neurons in area MT are direction-
selective, meaning that they respond selectively to a certain range of motion directions
and do not respond to directions beyond that range (Zeki, 1974; Albright, 1984).
Moreover, some of these neurons respond well to the pattern of motion (Albright, 1992),
meaning that these neurons can integrate many different motion directions and calculate
what the overall direction of an object might be. The activity in this region seems to be
closely related to motion perception and, when activity in this region is disrupted, motion
perception may be severely impaired (Tong and Pearson, 2007).
Color
The most studies have identified several distinct foci within human visual areas VP
and V4 that are activated somewhat more strongly by chromatic than by archromatic
stimuli. Anyhow, greater selectivity and task specificity are found anterior to V4v. Such
selectivity has been shown to be associated with visual area V8 and, in some tasks, with
a more anterior site. V8 is retinotopically distinct from V4v and contains a representation
of both superior and inferior visual fields.
1436
18. Occipital lobe
Color-selective activation sites in dorsal occipital cortex have also been observed
on occasion, but their location and identity remain poorly defined.
The presence of occipitotemporal color-related sites is consistent with human lesion
studies in which damage to ventral cortex results in diminished or absent color
discrimination (achromatopsia) (Zeki, 1990). Often, color perception losses in both upper
and lower visual fields, even though the lesion appears to be confined to ventral cortex.
In contrast, area V8, which is located within the lesion-sensitive zone, does have both
upper and lower field representations, thereby making it a prime candidate for the
achromatopsia site (Zeki, 1990). Moreover, V8 is located adjacent to the fusiform face area
(FFA). This juxtaposition of V8 and FFA is likely to account for the fact that prosopagnosia
(inability to recognize face) is commonly associated with cerebral achromatopsia.
In humans, cerebral achromatopsia does not arise from lesions of V4v but from more
anterior lesions encompassing V8 and / or additional neighboring areas. This does not
necessarily imply that V8 or any single visual area is solely responsible for color vision
or for all color-related deficits. In fact, moving stimuli consisting of pure chromatic
contrast can readily activate human hMT+.
So, to summarize, area V4 is known to be especially important for the perception of
color (Zeki, 1997) and some neurons in this area respond well to more complex features
or combination of features (Pasupathy and Connor, 2002). For example, some V4 neurons
are sensitive to curvature of two lines that meet at a specific angle. These neurons might
signal the presence of a curving contour or of a corner (Tong and Pearson, 2007).
Area V4 sends many outputs to higher visual areas in the ventral visual pathway,
which is important for object recognition (Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982). The anterior
part of ventral visual pathway consists of the ventral temporal cortex, which is especially
important for object recognition.
Form
There is general agreement that a major portion of the form processing system is
located in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex.
Passive viewing of luminance-based contour and edges readily activates retinotopic
visual areas of the medial and ventral occipital cortex in humans. The size of a detectable
increment in the contrast of a plaid grating was quite precisely tracked by a proportional
increment in fMRI response of visual area V1, V2d, V3 and V4A. In area hMT+, the
fMRI response tends to saturate above a few percent contrast, consistent with the high
contrast sensitivity of MT neurons studied in animals (Kaas, 1997).
Single-cell recording in animals has shown that neurons in V2, but less in V1, are
capable of responding selectively to these contours, thereby suggesting a hierarchical
processing mechanism. Neuroimaging studies with human subjects have shown that some
types of illusory contours may preferentially activate later processing stages including
V3A, V4v, V7, V8 and LO (lateral occipital visual complex).
The most selective responses to motion-defined contours and shapes have been
associated with a region termed the kinetic-occipital area (KO).
It is located approximately 1.5 cm posterior to hMT+ in the lateral occipital cortex.
1437
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Cue invariance
In humans, several neuroimaging studies have tested for cue-invariant responses to
contours, edges, or outline figures defined by luminance, color, texture, binocular
disparity, or motion. In the most comprehensive study, similar responses were observed
for luminance-, texture-, and motion-defined figures in lateral occipital cortex posterior
to hMT+ (labeled LO) and in, or near, area V3A. The cue-invariant representation of
contour information in LO and V3A may represent an important stage of abstraction in
influence of object attributes from the available retinal information (Deyoe, 2002).
Figure-ground segmentation
It has been proposed that a swath of cortex located posterior to area hMT+ forms a
complex specialized for the segmentation and processing of object information. This area,
labeled LO, is more strongly activated by discrete objects than by gratings, textures, or
motion fields (Deyoe, 2002).
In summary, lateral occipital (LO) cortex seems to have a general role in object
recognition and responds strongly to a variety of shapes and objects (Malach et al., 1995).
LO prefers intact shapes and objects more than scrambled visual features.
This region seems to represent the particular shapes of objects.
Presumably, different neurons in this region respond best to different kinds of
objects. A method to test object selectivity is to measure neural adaptation to a particular
shape. This is exactly what LO does, even when the repeated object is presented in a new
location or in a different format so that retinotopic visual areas (V1-V4) will not adapt to
the image (Grill-Spector et al., 1999; Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001).
Parahippocampal place area (PPA) is another strongly category-selective region that
responds best to houses, landmarks, indoor and outdoor scenes (Epstein and Konwisher,
1998).
In comparison, this brain area responds more weakly to other types of stimuli such
as faces, bodies or inanimate objects. Because this region responds to very different
stimuli than the fusiform face area, many studies have taken advantage of the different
response properties of the FFA and PPA to study the neural correlates of visual awareness
(Tong and Pearson, 2007).
Complex forms
Regions of cortex in the collateral sulcus and adjacent lingual gyrus that is similar to
retinotopically defined V4 and / or VP, the fusiform gyrus or adjacent inferior temporal
gyrus, sometimes extending into area LO, encompass a swath of cortex that extends from
the occipital lobe into ventral temporal cortex. This swath not functionally homogeneous
is perhaps most consistently apparent for the analysis and recognition of face. The posterior
extent of this region is lateral to the area activated during color processing (V8) or during
the presentation of “scrambled” faces and has been termed the fusiform face area (FFA).
Face stimuli and face discrimination have been studied extensively. In the human
studies it has been shown that the fusiform gyrus from the occipital junction forward to
midtemporal lobe is activated by tasks requiring face recognition.
1438
18. Occipital lobe
This region responds more to human, animal, and cartoon faces than to a variety of
non-face stimuli, including hands, bodies, eyes shown alone, back views of heads,
flowers, buildings and inanimate objects (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Mc Carthy et al., 1997;
Tong et al., 2000; Schwarzlose et al., 2005).
In a recent study, Tsao et al. (2006) recorded the activity of single neurons in this
face area and discovered that 97 per cent of the neurons in this region responsed more to
face than to other kinds of objects.
This region seems to be important for the conscious perception of face.
Lesions that cause prosopagnosia tend to have common involvement of the fusiform
gyrus within the general vicinity of the FFA (Fig. 18.4).
In summary, single unit recordings in monkeys have revealed that many neurons in
the ventral temporal cortex respond best to contour, simple shapes, or complex objects.
Same neurons in this region are highly selective and respond to only a particular kind of
object, such as a hand, a face shown from a particular viewpoint, a particular animal, a
familiar toy or an object that the monkey has learned to recognize and so forth (Desimone
et al., 1984; Gross, 1992; Logothetis et al., 1995; Tanaka, 1996).
Human neuroimaging studies have revealed many brain areas involved in processing
objects. These object-sensitive areas, which lie just anterior to early visual areas V1-V4,
respond more strongly to coherent shapes and objects, as compared to scrambled,
meaningless stimuli.
The lateral occipital complex lies on the lateral surface of the occipital lobe, just
posterior to area MT. Because this region is strongly involved in object recognition, Tong
and Pearson (2007) consider it as part of the ventral pathway.
The fusiform face area lies on the fusiform gyrus, on the ventral surface of the
temporal lobe. The parahippocampal place area lies on the parahippocampal gyrus, which
lies medial to the fusiform gyrus on the ventral surface of the temporal lobe.
So, the two distinct visual streams have evolved to use visual information in two
fundamentally different ways: the dorsal stream for guiding movements and the ventral
for identifying objects.
Anyhow, according to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), the third stream of visual
processing, which originates from structures associated with both the parietal and the
temporal pathways and flow to a region of the temporal lobe is buried in the superior
1441
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
prefrontal cortex, where information from each pathway can also be reunited (Tong and
Pearson, 2007).
Visual consciousness
In the brain there are a lot of areas responsible for consciousness. These areas work
together to achieve this remarkable feat. Primary visual cortex seems to be important for
our ability consciously to perceive any visual feature, while higher visual areas may have
a more specialized role in perceiving certain visual features or objects (Tong, 2003).
Different cortical visual areas and neurons involved in processing a specific kind of
visual stimuli (color, orientation, motion, faces, objects, etc.), together with other cortical
areas and subcortical structures (thalamus, hypothalamus, reticular formation, etc.) seems
to play different roles in our conscious visual experience.
According to one theory of visual consciousness, called hierarchical theory (Crick
and Koch, 1995; Rees et al., 2002), higher visual areas respond to more complex stimuli,
such as entire objects, and can integrate information about many visual features, which
are processed in early visual areas. The interactive theory of visual consciousness
emphasizes a different idea. It turns out that the signals entering the brain do not simply
travel up the visual hierarchy: higher visual areas send feedback signals back down to
early visual areas, especially to area V1 (Bullier, 2001). There are as many feedback
projections as feedforward projections in the visual system, neurons projecting from
higher visual areas to lower visual areas.
According to the interactive theory, once a stimulus is presented, feedforward signals
travel up the visual hierarchy, activating many neurons in its path, but this feedforward
activity is not enough for consciousness. Instead, high-level areas must send feedback
signals back to lower-level areas where the feedforward signals come from, so that neural
activity returns full circle, forming a neural circuit (Pollen, 1999; Lamme and Roclfsema,
2000).
This combination of feedforward - feedback signals is important for awareness,
because higher areas need to check the signals in nearly areas and confirm if they are
getting the right message, or perhaps to link neural representation of an object to the
specific features that make up the object (Tong and Pearson, 2007).
Unconscious perception
According to Tong and Pearson (2007) the term unconscious perception refers to
situations when subjects report not seeing a given stimulus, but their behavior or brain
activity suggests that specific information about the unperceived stimulus was indeed
processed by the brain.
Neurons may fire in a stimulus-specific way at many levels of the brain, but this
activity may not be strong enough, last long enough, or involve enough neurons or brain
areas to lead to awareness. One of the best examples of this comes from neural recordings
in animals under anesthesia; visual neurons in many brain areas still show strong
stimulus-selective responses (Tong and Pearson, 2007). If you become conscious of a
1443
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
particular stimulus, then neurons in your brain that represent that pattern will become
highly active.
Anyhow, without enough activity in the right brain areas, awareness may simply
fail: the result is unconscious perception. When two different stimuli are flashed briefly
enough in quick succession, the visual system can no longer separate the two stimuli.
Instead, what people perceive is a mix, or a fused blend of the two images.
For example, if we were to expose you to a red square, then quickly follow it with
a green square, what you might experience is a combination of the two – a yellow square
(Tong and Pearson, 2007). This method can be used to present invisible patterns, such as
a face or a house, by flashing red contours on a green background to one eye and the
opposite colors to the other eye.
When subjects were presented with such images in the fMRI scanner, the fusiform
face area responded more strongly to faces and the parahippocampal place area responded
more to house, even though subjects were no longer aware of the images (Moutoussis
and Zeki, 2002). This is an example of unconscious perception. The activity in ventral
temporal parts of the brain could differentiate, to some degree, whether a face or house
was presented, even though the subject could not verbally report a difference.
Area MT, a brain area specialized for processing motion, also responds to
unperceived motion. When moving stimulus is presented far out in the periphery of the
visual field and is crowded by other stimuli, area MT still responds to motion, even when
subjects are not aware of the motion (Moutoussis and Zeki, 2006). In this experiment,
the motion stimulus was flanked on two sides by flickering stimuli, making the visual
space so busy and cluttered that subjects could not see the motion in the display. Although
MT responded somewhat to the perceived motion stimulus, it responded much more
strongly when the surrounding flickering stimuli were removed and motion stimulus was
clearly perceived.
In other studies, researchers used binocular rivalry to render pictures of fearful faces
invisible.
The expression of the faces carried emotional content, although the subjects were
never aware of seeing the faces. The amygdala that normally responds to emotional
stimuli also responded to the invisible fearful faces (Pasley et al., 2004).
It is clear that many brain areas may continue to show stimuli-specific activity
despite the fact that we are unaware of that stimulus.
In most of these studies, greater activity was found when subjects were aware of a
stimulus than when they were not, suggesting that a minimum level of activity may be
needed to make the difference between no awareness and awareness. A low level of
neural activity below a specific threshold might not be adequate to result in being aware
of a stimulus (Tong and Pearson, 2007).
In sum, Tong and Pearson (2007) can conclude that at least two things are needed
for visual awareness: (1) activity in the right neurons or brain areas; and (2) activity that
exceeds a critical threshold.
1444
18. Occipital lobe
Visual attention
Visual attention refers to the ability to prepare for, select, and maintain awareness
of specific locations, objects, or attributes of the visual scene (or an imagined scene)
(Deyoe, 2002).
The center of gaze typically follows the focus of attention, but the observer can
intentionally dissociate the two, thereby demonstrating that the neural systems controlling
eye movements and attention are at least partially distinct (Deyoe, 2002). Some visual
attributes are:
– enhance the detection of subtle changes in brightness, color, or virtually any other
attribute;
– require focused attention to be perceived correctly. In a crowded visual
environment directed attention may be required if different attributes of the same object
are to be correctly associated. Certain behaviors can be performed with little, if any,
focused attention, whereas others are highly sensitive to the allocation of attention.
Automatic processes direct behavior that occurs without intention, involuntarily, without
conscious awareness, and without producing interference with ongoing activities.
Automatic processing may be an innate property of the way in which sensory information
is processed or it can be produced by extended training (Logan, 1992, Bargh and
Ferguson, 2000).
Operations that are not automatic have been referred to by various terms, including
controlled, effortful, attentive, and conscious. Conscious operations differ fundamentally
from automatic processing in that they require focused attention. A person stopping at a
red light is an example of bottom-up processing, whereas a person actively searching for
a street at which to turn is an example of top-down processing (Treisman, 1986).
Bottom-up processing is data driven; that is, it relies almost exclusively on the
stimulus information being presented in the environment. In contrast, top-down
processing is conceptually driven. It relies on the use of information already in memory,
including whatever expectation might exist regarding the task at hand. So, automatic and
conscious processing can reasonably be presumed to require at least some different
cortical circuits (Logan, 1992; Naatanen, 1992; Neibur, 2002).
Treisman (1986) and Treisman and Gormican (1988) concluded that, although
practice can speed up feature processing, it remains dependent on specific automatic
neural associations between features and a serial-processing pathway. Feature processing
appears to be innate to the visual system.
Posner and Raichle (1993) suggest that, in a sense, the attentional process provides
the “glue” that integrates features into a unitary object. When features have been put
together, the object can be perceived and held in memory as a unit. A clear prediction
from Treisman’s theory is that neurons in the visual areas outside area V1 and probably
outside area V2 should respond differentially, depending on whether attention is focused
on the corresponding receptive field. Treisman (1986) presumes that features are the
properties that cells in the visual system are coded to detect. But features may perhaps
be biologically significant stimuli, as illustrated in an experiment by Eastwood and
1445
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
colleagues (2001). The task was to identify the odd face, which could be a happy face in
a sea of sad ones or vice versa. Before you try to do so, turn your book upside down.
Subjects were faster at detecting sad faces, whereas they were upside down or right
side up. Furthermore, when Eastwood and colleagues redid the experiment with abstract
targets that signified either a happy or sad state, subjects still found the sad-related feature
faster.
Given that the features should be equally conspicuous in the happy and sad
conditions, it follows that there is something biologically more important to detecting
sad (negative) than happy (positive) stimuli.
It appears that negative stimuli (potentially dangerous or threatening features as well
as sad ones) are attended to very efficiently and demand attention more than to targets
for positive features.
From an evolutionary perspective, favoring the nervous system that attends to stimuli
that can make a difference to an animal’s survival makes sense. The evolution of
biological targets is likely more important to survival than are the simplest targets detected
by cells in area V1.
How and where attention exerts its effects over the processing of incoming visual
information are becoming clearer. Ample evidence from animals and humans now shows
that attention can modulate the responses of neurons in occipital visual cortex.
Moran and Desimone (1985) trained a monkey to hold a bar while it gazed at a
fixation point on a screen. A sample stimulus (for instance, a vertical blue bar) appeared
briefly at one location in the receptive field, followed about 500 ms later by two stimuli:
one at the same location and another in a separate one. The key point is that both targets
were in the cell’s receptive field, but only one target was in the correct location. When
the test stimulus was identical with the sample and in the same location, the animal was
rewarded if it released the bar immediately. In this way, the same visual stimulus could
be presented to different regions of the neurons’ receptive field, but the importance of
the information varied with its location.
– As the animal performed the task, the researchers recorded the firing of cells in
the area V4. Cells in area V4 are color and form sensitive; so, different neurons responded
to different conjunctions of features. Thus, a given cell might respond to one stimulus
(for example, a horizontal green bar) and not to another (for example, a vertical red bar).
These stimuli were presented either in the correct location or in an incorrect location for
predicting reward.
So, when effective stimulus was presented in the correct location, the cell was highly
active. When the same stimulus was presented in an incorrect location, however, the cell
was not responsive. It appears that, when attention is focused on a place in the visual
world, the cell responds only to stimuli appropriate to that place.
Moran and Desimone (1985) considered the possibility that visual area activated
earlier (V1) or later (TE) in visual processing might also show attentional effects.
Presumably the features detected in area V1 were too pimple to direct attention, whereas
those in area TE could.
1446
18. Occipital lobe
So, the cells showing constraints in spatial attention were in the areas V4 and TE,
both parts of the object-recognition stream. Neurons in this system are coding spatial
location.
Moran and Desimone’s monkeys did not make a movement in space. If they did,
we would predict that cells in the posterior parietal cortex would be significant to
attentional demands.
In fact, Mountcastle (1995) reported just such results. These cells are responding
not to the features of the stimuli but rather to the movements needed to get to them. Thus,
there appear to be two types of visual attention, one related to the selection of stimuli
and the other to the selection and direction of movements.
Kahneman (1973) noted that perceptual system does not always work at peak
efficiency. He proposed that the capacity to perform mental activity is limited and that
this limited capacity must be allocated among concurrent activities. Thus, for Kahneman,
one aspect of attention is the amount of effort that is directed toward a particular task.
When you attempt a difficult maneuver with a car, such as parking in a tight spot,
you turn down your car radio.
Spitzer and colleagues (1988) wandered if cells in area V4 might vary their firing
characteristics in accord with the amount of effort to solve a particular visual problem.
Spitzer and colleagues (1988) reasoned that it should be easy for cell to discriminate
between a stimulus within its preferred orientation or color and a stimulus outside this
orientation of color. Animals’ performance confirmed that the finer discrimination was
more difficult: 93% of their responses were correct under the easy conditions as compared
with 73% under the difficult conditions.
The change in response characteristics of the area V4 cells is intriguing. First, under
the difficult conditions, the cells increased their firing rate by an average of about 20%.
Second, the tuning characteristics of the cells changed.
The more difficult task appears to have required increased attention to the
differences between the stimuli, as manifested in a change in the stimulus selectivity of
neurons in area V4.
It is not known how this attentional effect can alter the cell’s activity. The result of
studies using monkeys have led to the idea that the critical region may be the pulvinar,
which is a thalamic nucleus that projects to secondary visual areas in the tectopulvinar
system.
Petersen and his colleagues (1987) found that neurons in the pulvinar respond more
vigorously to stimuli that are targets of behavior than they do to the same stimuli when
they are not targets of behavior. That is, when a visual stimulus is present but has no
meaning for the animal, the cells have a low firing rate. When the same stimulus now
signifies a reward, the cells become more active.
Because the pulvinar complex projects to the posterior parietal cortex, temporal
cortex, and prefrontal cortex, it may play same role in directing Treisman’s “spotlight”
to different parts of space.
Petersen and his colleagues tested this prediction by finding that disrupting the
pulvinar does disrupt spatial attention. The pulvinar receives visual inputs from the
1447
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Observable changes in the optic disc are, therefore, of particular importance. They
may reflect the presence of raised intracranial pressure (papilledema or “choked disc”),
infarction of the optic nerve head (disc edema), congenital defects of the optic nerves
(optic pits or colobomas), hypoplasia and atrophy of the optic nerves, and glaucoma.
Lesions of the chiasma, optic tract and geniculocalcarine pathway cause bitemporal
hemianopsia, homonymous hemianopsia or superior homonymous quadrantanopsia
(contralateral temporal and ipsilateral nasal quadrants) (Meyer’s loop).
Parietal lobe lesions are said to affect more the inferior quadrants of the visual field
than the superior ones, but this is difficult to document; with a lesion of the right parietal
lobe, the patient ignores the left half of space, and with a left parietal lesion, the patient
is often aphasic.
Damage in the region of the optic chiasma results in specific types of deficits that
involve the visual field of both eyes. For example, damage to the middle portion of the
optic chiasma can affect the fibres that are crossing from the nasal retina of each eye,
leaving the uncrossed fibres from the temporal retinas intact. The resulting loss of vision
is confined to the temporal visual field of each eye and is known as bitemporal
hemianopsia. It is also called heteronomous hemianopsia to emphasize that the parts of
the visual field that are lost in each eye do not overlap. Individuals with this condition
are able to see in both left and right visual fields, provided both eyes are open (Fig. 18.15).
Damage to structures that are central to the optic chiasma, including the optic tract, lateral
geniculate nucleus, optic radiation, and visual cortex, results in deficits that are limited
to the contralateral visual hemifield.
Because such damage affects the corresponding parts of the visual field in each eye,
there is a complete loss of vision in the affected region of the binocular visual field, and
the deficit is referred to as a homonymous hemianopsia.
Damage along the optic radiation in its course from the lateral geniculate nucleus to
the striate cortex is rarely complete. Some of the optic radiation axons run out into the
temporal lobe on their route to the striate cortex, a branch called Meyer’s loop. Meyer’s
loop carries information from the superior portion of the contralateral visual field. More
medial parts of the optic radiation, which pass under the cortex of the parietal lobe, carry
information from the interior portion of the contralateral visual field. So, superior fibres
that leave the lateral geniculate body go straight back to the primary visual cortex; inferior
fibres loop anteriorly around the superior temporal horn of the lateral ventricle (Meyer’s
loop). Because these fibres are approximately 5 cm from the top of the temporal lobe,
they are sometimes damaged during temporal lobectomy, can thus result in a superior
homonymous quadrantanopsia. Damage to the optic radiation underlying the parietal
cortex results in an inferior homonymous quadrantanopsia.
Macular vision is represented most posteriorly at the occipital tips. Each fovea appears
to project in both occipital lobes. Injury to central visual structures can lead to a phenomenon
called macular sparing. Macular sparing is commonly found with damage to the cortex,
but can be a feature of damage anywhere along the length of the visual pathway.
The nonoverlapping part of the most peripheral temporal field (monocular temporal
crescent) represents unpaired crossed axons from the nasal retina, which project to the
most anteromedial part of the visual cortex. The primary visual cortex has inter-
connections with numerous visual association areas (Zeki, 1993).
Visual agnosia
The disorder of vision and higher visual functions are common in many patients
with neurological disease. The visual system is the most widely researched and best
understood higher order sensory system in the brain.
Object agnosia is a rare condition first described by Lissauer in 1890. It consists of
a failure to name and indicate the use of a seen object by spoken or written words or by
gesture. The patient cannot even tell the generic class of the seen object. Visual acuity is
intact, the mind is clear, and the patient is not aphasic – conditions requisite for the
diagnosis of agnosia. If the object is palpated, it is recognized at once, and it can also be
identified by smell or sound if it has an odor or makes noise. Moving the object or placing
it in its customary surroundings facilitates recognition. Lissauer conceived the visual
object recognition as consisting of two distinct processes – the construction of a
perceptual representation from vision (apperception) and the mapping of this perceptual
representation onto stored percepts or engrams of the object’s functions and associations.
Lissauer proposed that impairment of either of these processes could give rise to a defect
in visual object recognition.
Teuber was credited in 1968 with the classic definition of agnosia – that is, a normal
percept stripped of its meaning. In the setting of visual agnosia, such a definition implies
normal or near normal sensory perception in the context of impaired recognition of the
object being perceived. Most patients with visual agnosia can be shown to have
abnormality of perception.
1450
18. Occipital lobe
Associative agnosia
Associative agnosia corresponds to a “pure” visual agnosia, as reflected in Teuber’s
definition, and means the ability to recognize an object despite any apparent perception
of the object. Thus, the associative agnostic can copy a drawing rather accurately,
indicating a coherent percept, but he cannot identify it.
Failure of object recognition is a defect in memory, that affects not only past
knowledge about the object, but also the acquisition of new knowledge. Associative
agnosias are more likely with damage to region in the ventral stream that are farther up
the processing hierarchy, such as the anterior temporal lobe. So, associative agnosia
usually results from damage to the ventral temporal cortex.
Apperceptive agnosia
Apperceptive visual agnosia refers to an abnormality of visual recognition in which
perceptual features are so prominent as to dominate the clinical picture. Basic visual
functions (acuity, color, motion) are preserved. The fundamental deficit is an inability to
develop a percept of the structure of an object or objects. In the simplest case, patients
are simply unable to recognize, copy, or match simple shapes.
Many patients have another unusual symptom, too-often referred to as simultan-
agnosia. In this case patients can perceive the basic shape of an object, but they are unable
to perceive more than one object at a time. Such patients often act as though they were
blind, possibly because they are simply overwhelmed by the task at hand. Such patients
can still recognize objects by using other senses such as touch, hearing or smell, so the
loss of function is strictly visual.
Apperceptive agnosia does not result from a restricted lesion but usually follows
gross bilateral damage to the lateral parts of the occipital lobes including regions sending
outputs to the ventral stream. Such injuries are probably most commonly associated with
carbon monoxide poisoning, which appears to produce neuronal death in “watershed”
region – that is, regions lying in the border areas between territories of a different arterial
system (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
In the course of assessing patients with possible agnosia, it is important to distinguish
between defects in naming and defects in recognition. This is a distinction that is
commonly either blurred or completely unappreciated in the scientific and clinical
literature on agnosia. The key is to understand that an object can be recognized without
being named but cannot be named without being recognized. Thus, if a patient is asked
to identify an object and fails to produce its name, it is important to probe the patient
regarding features of the object to establish that a true recognition defect exists.
Visual agnosia does not appear to result from damage to the dorsal stream. The most
affected region is the tissue at the occipitotemporal border, which is part of the ventral
visual pathway.
1451
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
James et al. (2003) propose that the dorsal system is not only responsible for
processing “where” objects are, but also “how” action can be performed toward a
particular object, such as pointing at or reaching for that object. Apparently, visual
processing in the dorsal system is not accessible to consciousness – the patient cannot
report the orientation of the slot – yet the dorsal system can guide the right action.
Complementary studies of patients with optic ataxia have revealed the opposite pattern
of visual deficits, indicating a double dissociation between conscious visual perception
and visually guided action. Optic ataxia typically results from damage to the parietal lobe,
which is part of the dorsal pathway. These patients can perceive visual orientations and
recognize objects well, but have great difficulty in performing visually guided actions.
Simultanagnosia
Wolpert (1924) originally described a patient who demonstrated a “spelling
dyslexia” (an inability to read all but the shortest words, spelled out letter by letter) and
a failure to perceive simultaneously all the elements of a scene and to properly interpret
the scene. In the framework of gestalt psychology, the patient could recognize the parts
but not the whole. A cognitive defect of synthesis of the visual impressions was thought
to be the basis of this condition, which Wolpert called simultanagnosia. Some of the
patients with this disorder have a right homonymous hemianopsia; in others, the visual
field is full. This is part of the Balint syndrome, the other components of which are faulty
visual scanning (ocular apraxia) and visual reaching (optic ataxia), suggesting that a fault
in ocular scanning might underlie all the defects.
Through tachistoscopic testing, Kinsbourne and Warrington (1963) have noted that
reducing the time of stimulus exposure permits single objects to be perceived in an instant,
but not two objects. Rizzo and Robin (1990) have proposed that the primary defect is in
sustained attention to incoming visual-spatial information. Nielsen (1946) has attributed
this disorder to a lesion of the inferolateral part of the dominant occipital lobe (area 18).
In a patient who presented with an isolated “spelling dyslexia” and simultanagnosia,
Kinsbourne and Warrington (1963) found a localized lesion within the inferior part of
the left occipital lobe. In other instance, the lesions have been bilateral in the superior
part of the occipital association cortices.
Prosopagnosia
The earliest mention of the symptom was made by Quaglino, an Italian
ophthalmologist, who, in 1867, reported a patient suffering from left hemianopsia,
achromatopsia, and inability to recognize familiar faces – problems caused by a
cerebrovascular accident.
The symptom had to await Bodamer’s report in 1947 to be identified as a distinct
form of agnosia, deserving a distinctive name (prosopagnosia, from the Greek word
prosopon, meaning “face”).
Since the 1940’s, more than a hundred case reports (Farah, 1990) have been
published, and some patients have been extensively and repeatedly investigated.
1452
18. Occipital lobe
A study of a very unusual object agnosic patient, who was very impaired at recognizing
objects could recognize upright faces just as well as normal subjects (Moscovitch et al.,
1997). Remarkably, if the face were turned upside-down, the patient became severely
impaired, performing six times worse than normal participants under these conditions.
Apparently the patient has an intact system for processing upright faces, but the system
responsible for processing objects and upside-down faces is badly damaged (Valentine,
1988). Taken together, evidence from that patient and prosopagnostic patients provide
evidence of a double dissociation between the visual processing of upright faces as compared
to objects and upside-down faces (Tong and Pearson, 2007). Prosopagnosics may not accept
the fact that they cannot recognize their own faces, probably because they know who must
be in the mirror and thus see themselves. According to Damasio and colleagues, most facial
agnosics can tell human from nonhuman faces and can recognize facial expression normally
(Damasio et al., 1982; 1989).
Scientists have also described a third form of prosopagnosia termed “developmental
prosopagnosia” (De Haan and Campbell, 1991; Kracke, 1994; De Haan, 1999; Jones and
Tranel, 2001). Developmental prosopagnosia refers to a selective face recognition defect
that begins in childhood and is not associated with any identifiable structural lesion. In
general, such cases have shown significant apperceptive features, although one reported
case was believed to be an essentially associative prosopagnosia. Similar to cases of
acquired prosopagnosia, such patients recognize individuals by gait, voice, or other
nonface features. In one case there were features of a broader visual object agnosia. In
those cases in which neuroimaging has been completed, no structural brain abnormalities
have been identified. Functional imaging has not been done in such cases. These patients
appear to manifest the face recognition disability over many years. Although first
identified in childhood, in at least one case the disorder persisted into adulthood.
Similar to cases of acquired prosopagnosia, in at least one case nonconscious
electrodermal recognition of familiar faces was demonstrated.
Prosopagnosia can result from bilateral damage around the regions of the lateral
occipital cortex, inferior temporal cortex and the fusiform gyrus (Meadows, 1974;
Bouvier and Engel, 2006). In some cases, unilateral damage to the right hemisphere may
lead to this impairment. Because lesions are usually quite large and might damage fibre
tracts leading to a critical brain region, it is difficult to identify a precise site. Nonetheless,
the brain lesion sites associated with prosopagnosia appear to encompass the fusiform
face area and extend much more posteriorly (Tong and Pearson, 2007).
All postmortem studies on facial agnosias have found bilateral damage, and the results
of imaging studies in living patients confirm the bilateral nature of the injury in most
patients, with the damage centered in the region below the calcarine fissure at the temporal
junction. These results imply that the process of facial recognition is probably bilateral, but
asymmetrical (Kolb et al., 1983; Boussaud et al., 1990; Hancock et al., 2000).
recognition defect at the level of nonunique objects rather than at the level of specific
members of a category (Damasio et al., 2000). For example, a patient with visual agnosia
may not know that a violin is a violin, that a dog is a dog, or that a car is a car (Jones and
Tranel, 2002). Memory is normal, and there is defect in judgment, basic verbal intellectual
skills, or language.
Associated conditions often include defects in color recognition and naming and
defects in reading. Lesions associated with visual object agnosia are typically more
extensive than those in case of prosopagnosia, including occipitotemporal cortex
bilaterally, extending dorsally into visual association cortex, and almost always involving
underlying white matter. There is evidence that the left hemisphere plays a more critical
role than the right in the development of visual object agnosia (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
Disorder of reading
The study of alexia dates at least to the contributions of Déjérine, who in 1891 and
1892 described two patients with quite different patterns of reading impairment.
Déjérine’s first patient (Déjérine, 1891) developed an impairment in reading and
writing subsequent to an infarction involving the left parietal lobe. Déjérine termed this
disorder “alexia with agraphia” and attributed the disturbance to a disruption of the
“optical image for words”, which he thought to be supported by the left angular gyrus.
Déjérine concluded that reading and writing required the activation of these “optical
images” and that the loss of the images resulted in an inability to recognize or write
familiar words.
Déjérine’s second patient (Déjérine, 1892) was quite different. This patient was
unable to read aloud or for comprehension but could write, a disorder that Déjérine
designated “alexia without agraphia” (also known as agnosic alexia and pure alexia). The
patient had a right homonymous hemianopsia from a left occipital lesion, which included
the fibres carrying visual information from the right to the left hemisphere. Déjérine
explained alexia without agraphia in terms of a “disconnection” between visual
information confined to the right hemisphere and the left angular gyrus, which he
assumed to be critical for the recognition of words.
The study of acquired dyslexia was revitalized by Marshall and Newcombe (1973).
These investigators described a patient (GR) who read approximately 50 percent of
concrete nouns but was severely impaired in the reading of abstract nouns and all other
parts of speech. The most striking aspect of GR’s performance, however, was his
tendency to produce errors that appeared to be semantically related to the target word
(e.g., speak read as “talk”). Marshall and Newcombe (1973) designated this disorder
“deep dyslexia”. These investigators also described two patients whose primary deficit
appeared to be an inability to derive the pronunciation of irregularly spelled words, such
as “yacht”. This disorder was designated “surface dyslexia”.
Pure alexia refers to the inability to read with preserved ability to write. These
patients are not aphasic, nor is there a defect in memory, orientation, basic intellectual
skills, or verbal arithmetic skills. The neurologic exam is normal apart from a typical
finding of a right homonymous hemianopsia.
1455
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
An inability to read has often been seen as the complementary symptom to facial-
recognition deficits.
Alexia is most likely to result from damage to the left fusiform and lingual areas.
Either hemisphere can read letters, but only the left hemisphere appears able to combine
the letters to form lexical representations (that is, words). The lesion typically abuts the
splenium. Thus, visual information is regarded only by the right occipital lobe, and such
information is blocked from passing to the left hemisphere by virtue of the retrosplenial
lesion. This information thus cannot be decoded into lexically meaningful words by
association areas in the left parietal cortex, specifically surrounding the left angular gyrus
(Jones and Tranel, 2002). In the instance, when a patient is asked to trace letters with his
or her hand, there is no such disconnection between sensorimotor cortex on the left and
the left angular gyrus. The key in this case is that the visual system is bypassed (Jones
and Tranel, 2002).
Alexia can be conceived as a form of object agnosia in which there is an inability to
construct perceptual wholes from parts or to be a form of associative agnosia, in which
case word memory (the lexical store) is either damaged or inaccessible (Farah, 1990;
1998; Behrmann et al., 1992).
In evaluating patients with pure alexia, it is useful to ask them to copy printed
material, to write a sentence by dictation, and to write a sentence spontaneously.
However, such patients will have difficulty in copying sentences, given their inability to
read. If the examiner asks the patient to trace letters with the patient’s finger, the patient
may be able to discern the meaning of the written word (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
Achromatopsia
Diseases may affect color vision by abolishing it completely (achromatopsia) or
partially by quantitatively reducing one or more of the three attributes of color –
brightness, hue and saturation. Or, only one of the complementary pairs of color may be
lost, usually red-green. The disorder may be congenital and hereditary or acquired. The
commonest form, and the one to which the term color blindness is usually applied, is a
male sex-linked inability to see red and green while normal visual acuity is retained.
Patients with achromatopsia complain that colors appear to be black and white,
washed out, gray or dirty. Such patients may also demonstrate some color desaturation
and dyschromatopsia rather than complete lack of color.
Lesions associated with achromatopsia are characteristically found in the
infracalcarine medial occipital lobe, typically involving the lingual and fusiform gyri.
When the lesion is unilateral, the color defect is in the contralateral visual space. When
the lesion is bilateral, the color defect subsumes the entire visual field. There may well
be visual field cuts, typically involving superior quadrants.
Damasio (1985) has drawn attention to a group of acquired deficits of color
perception with preservation of form vision, the result of focal damage (usually,
infarction) of the visual association cortex and subjacent white matter. Color vision may
be lost in a quadrant, half of the visual field, or the entire field. The latter, or full-field
achromatopsia, is the result of bilateral occipitotemporal lesions involving the fusiform
and lingual gyri, a localization that accounts for its frequent association with visual
agnosia (especially, prosopagnosia), and some degree of visual field defect. A lesion
restricted to the inferior part of the right occipitotemporal region, sparing both the optic
radiation and striate cortex, causes the purest form of achromatopsia (left hemiachro-
matopsia). With a similar left-sided lesion, alexia may be associated with the right
hemiachromatopsia.
In addition to the losses of perception of form, movement, and color, lesions of the
visual system may also give rise to a variety of positive sensory visual experiences. The
simplest of these are called phosphenes, i.e., flashes of light and colored spots in the
absence of luminous stimuli. Mechanical pressure on the normal eyeball may induce
them at the retinal level, as every child discovers.
In patients with migraine, ischemia of nerve cells in the occipital lobe gives rise to
the bright zigzag lines of a fortification spectrum.
Formed or complex visual hallucinations are observed in a variety of conditions,
notably in the withdrawal state following chronic intoxication with alcohol and other
sedative-hypnotic drugs, in Alzheimer disease, and in disease of the occipitoparietal or
occipitotemporal regions or the diencephalon (“peduncular hallucinosis”). In color
blindness a genetic abnormality of cone pigments is postulated, but the defect cannot be
seen by inspecting the retina. A failure of the cones to develop or a degeneration of cones
may cause a loss of color vision, but in the latter condition visual acuity is often
diminished, a central scotoma may be present, and, although the macula appears to be
normal ophthalmoscopically, fluorescein angiography shows the pigment epithelium to
1457
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
be defective. Congenital color visions are usually protan (red) or detan (green), leaving
yellow-blue color vision intact; acquired lesions may affect all colors.
Lesion of the optic nerves usually affects red-green more than blue-yellow; the
opposite is true of retinal lesions. An exception is a dominantly inherited optic atrophy,
in which the scotoma mapped by a large blue target is larger than that for red.
Color anomia
The term color anomia refers to an inability to name colors in the context of normal
performances on tests of matching color or of color generation. This disorder is usually
associated with pure alexia and right homonymous hemianopsia.
Color naming can be affected independently of color perception, imagery, and
recognition (Geschwind and Fusillo, 1966; Damasio et al., 1979; Rizzo and Damasio,
1989; Rizzo et al., 1993).
Color anomia is relatively rare, and, in fact, naming of color is often relatively spared
in patients who have severe naming defects for other classes of stimuli. Goodglass and
coworkers (1986) found that color naming was very infrequently impaired in various
aphasic patients and was, in fact, one of the categories that tended to stand out as being
especially intact – a finding consistent with an older case study (Yamadori and Albert,
1973). Such patients are able to match same-color chips and associate colors with objects.
When shown a specific color, such patients are unable to name it with appropriate verbal
tag. This defect seems to be exacerbated when less contextual information is available.
Given the site of the lesion in color anomia, there is some suggestion that this may
represent a disconnection syndrome. That is, in the left visual field (ipsilateral to the
lesion) color perception is normal, but perception is limited to primary visual cortex in
the right hemisphere only. This elementary perceptual information is blocked by a
retrosplenial lesion in the mesial left occipital lobe, thereby rendering such information
unavailable to the left hemisphere (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
Color agnosia
Following the Teuber (1968) definition of agnosia as a “normal percept stripped of
its meaning”, color agnosia refers to the loss of the ability to retrieve color knowledge
pertinent to a given stimulus that is not caused by deficient color perception. So, color
agnosia is a rare condition defined as an inability to retrieve color information in the
context of normal perception and language. Color agnosia is often associated with visual
object agnosia and may be associated with category-related recognition defects,
particularly for living entities as opposed to artifactual entities. The defect should also
be distinguished from color anomia and from color imagery impairment. In the latter
condition, the patient cannot bring into mind’s eye a particular color, but the patient still
knows what the color is and can give verbal or nonverbal testimony to that knowledge –
for instance, by performing normally on tasks requiring matching of color or names with
objects. A patient with color agnosia, by contrast, has lost the “knowing” of colors and
will not perform such tasks normally (Tranel, 1997). For example, a patient with color
agnosia will be unable to remember the color of common entities (e.g., “what color is a
1458
18. Occipital lobe
banana?”) and will be unable to provide a list of objects that come in various colors (e.g.,
“name things that are red”) (Jones and Tranel, 2002). Some authors have argued that
especially in cases in which the defect is “two-ways” – i.e., when the patient cannot name
colors from color stimuli and cannot point the colors given the color names – the
designation of agnosia is accurate. Color agnosia is rare. Only a few well-studied cases
have been reported (Kinsbourne and Warrington, 1964; Farah et al., 1988; Schnider et
al., 1992; Luzzatti and Davidoff, 1994, etc.). Based on a handful of well-studied patients,
the neuroanatomical correlates of color agnosia include the occipital-temporal region
either unilaterally on the left or bilaterally.
Functional imaging studies suggest that areas associated with color knowledge are
probably anterior and lateral to areas associated with color perception.
Visual neglect
Neglect can be defined as “the failure to report, respond, or orient to novel or
meaningful stimuli, presented to the side opposite a brain lesion, when this failure cannot
be attributed to either sensory or motor defects”. In the visual modality, the most common
form of neglect is hemispatial neglect. The essential feature of spatial neglect is that the
patient ignores objects in the visual hemifield contralateral to the lesion, in the absence
of a visual field cut.
Such neglect is commonly associated with right hemisphere lesions resulting in left
hemispatial visual neglect.
Right hemispatial visual neglect, though rare, can be seen during the acute period in
some cases of left hemisphere lesions. The typical lesion is in the occipital-temporal-
parietal border zone (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
So, damage to the dorsal pathway can lead to optic ataxia (impairments in visual
guided actions) or visual neglect. Damage to the ventral temporal cortex can lead to
impairments in visual perception, object recognition or face recognition. Patients with
brain injuries in the ventral and dorsal pathways reveal a dissociation between the
conscious perception of basic shapes and orientations and the ability to perform visually
guided actions.
The finding of neglect is often not restricted to the visual modality and may be
demonstrated in both sensorimotor and auditory systems concurrently with visual neglect.
One of the ways to evaluate hemispatial visual neglect in affected patients is to
perform a double simultaneous stimulation task, in which the examiner introduces a visual
stimulus to the left visual field, to the right visual field, and then to both fields
simultaneously. The most clear evidence of neglect from this procedure is that the patient
will identify each stimulus individually, but when presented simultaneously the patient
will identify only the stimulus presented ipsilateral to the lesion.
Furthermore, methods of detecting hemispatial neglect include drawing tasks, line
bisection-tasks, or line cancellation tasks (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
So, material in one visual field – usually the left – can be seen but remain unnoticed
unless the patient’s attention is drawn to it (Chaves and Caplan, 2001). This form of
1459
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
visual inattention, also known as unilateral sensory or spatial neglect, typically occurs
when there is right parietal lobe involvement as well as occipital lobe damage. Right
occipital lesions are less likely to give rise to inattention. The so-called “visual
inattention” associated with occipital lobe damage is similar to simultaneous agnosia in
that the patient spontaneously perceives only one thing at a time. It differs from
simultaneous agnosia in that the patient will see more than one object if others are pointed
out: this is not the case in a true simultaneous agnosia.
In addition to demonstrating neglect in other modalities (sensorimotor and auditory),
associated signs observed in neuropsychological testing of patients with neglect include
constructional dyspraxia, anosodiaphoria, and anosognosia.
exceptions, no cortical potential can be evoked in the occipital lobes by light flashes or
pattern changes (visual evoked response), and the alpha rhythm is lost in the EEG. There
may also be visual hallucinations of either elementary or complex types. The usual cause
of cortical blindness is occlusion of the posterior cerebral arteries (embolic or thrombotic).
The infarction may also involve the medial temporal regions and thalami with a resulting
Korsakoff amnesic defect and a variety of other neurologic deficits referable to the high
midbrain and diencephalon. Hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy, Schilder disease and
other leukodystrophies, Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease, progressive multifocal leukoence-
phalopathy, and bilateral gliomas are other causes of cortical blindness.
A commonly associated phenomenon with cortical blindness is Anton’s syndrome,
in which the patient with cortical blindness denies all visual difficulties. Anton’s
syndrome is considered a special form of anosognosia and is frequently seen only in the
acute epoch (within 2 or 3 months following the event).
The main characteristic is denial of blindness by a patient who obviously cannot see.
The patient acts as though he could see, and when attempting to walk, he collides with
objects, even to the point of injury. He may offer excuses for his difficulties: “I lost my
glasses”, “The light is dim”, etc., or there may be only an indifference to the loss of sight.
The lesion in cases of negation of blindness extend beyond the striate cortex to
involve the visual association areas.
Cortical blindness and Anton’s syndrome may also result from subcortical lesions
to optic radiations. Pupillary responses are preserved, but visual evoked potential cannot
be demonstrated (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
Bilateral lesions of primary visual cortex result in cortical blindness, a condition
commonly caused by bilateral infarction of the posterior cerebral arteries.
Rarely, the opposite condition may arise: a patient can see small objects but claims
to be blind. This individual walks about avoiding obstacles, picks up crumbs or pills from
the table, and catches a small ball thrown from a distance. Damasio et al. (1980) suggests
that this might be a type of visual disorientation but with sufficient residual visual
information to guide the hand, and that the lesion will be in the visual association areas
superior to the calcarine cortex.
Topographical disorientation
The term topographical disorientation refers to an acquired inability to navigate the
environment in daily life (Tranel et al., 1997; Barrash, 1998; Damasio et al., 2000).
The most striking manifestation of spatial memory deficit is topographic
disorientation – namely, the inability to find one’s way in a familiar environment and to
learn new paths, in the absence of global amnesia, severe mental deterioration, or
disorders of visual perception and exploration. Förster (1890) and Meyer (1900) must be
credited with having provided the first exhaustive description of its features. With
restitution of function or in milder cases from the beginning, navigation is impaired only
in environments that the patient has first experienced since the onset of disease (Habib
and Sirigu, 1987).
1461
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Two broad types of topographical disorientation can be discerned. The first may be
termed anterograde topographical disorientation, which involves a defect in new learning.
Patients with this disorder will complain that they become lost in new places, cannot
learn new routes, or cannot retrace their path in real life. Associated clinical findings may
include left hemispatial neglect, left visual field cuts, and constructional dyspraxia. The
site of lesion associated with the disorder is the medial occipitotemporal area, particularly
on the right and including the posterior parahippocampal gyrus and lingual gyrus.
Anterograde topographical agnosia has been described in cases of right parietal
dysfunction, and of left mesial occipitotemporal dysfunction but in these cases the clinical
phenomenon appears to be more transient and less severe.
Retrograde topographical disorientation refers to the inability to recognize previously
well-known spatial / topographical routes. The patient with retrograde topographical
disorientation will be unable to recognize either previously known visual scene (e.g.,
rooms in their home and the street on which they lived) or landmarks (e.g., their house
and the local grocery). Patients with retrograde topographical disorientation are unable
to describe in verbal terms from memory familiar scenes or routes. The lesion most
commonly associated with this disorder is in the mesial occipitotemporal cortices of the
right hemisphere, thus disconnecting primary visual cortex from higher order association
cortices (Jones and Tranel, 2002).
It is apparent that the basic deficit underlying the patient’s difficulty is the inability
to retrieve and abstract map of the route, which specifies the spatial relationships defining
the position of a place with respect to other places and the subject and to transform them
in guidelines for walking.
Landmark recognition and spatial map construction are the main mental operations
that assist navigation through familiar surroundings and their discrete disruption underlies
two forms of route-finding disability, topographic agnosia and topographic amnesia
(Paterson and Zangwill, 1945). The failure to identify a specific building might result
from perceptual impairment that prevents the appreciation of the small distinctive features
identifying an exemplar of a category whose elements are similar or from the inability to
match the perceived building with its representation stored in memory. Prosopagnosia is
a frequent, but not necessarily concomitant, topographic agnosia. Landis and coworkers
(1986) found prosopagnosia in only 7 of 16 patients suffering from what they called
“environmental agnosia”.
Failure to recognize familiar environments is by no means a constant feature of
patients with topographic disorientation. De Renzi and Scotti (1969) who were unable to
identify famous building did not improve in taking his bearing when their names were
provided by the examiner because he could not remember their position with respect to
other sites of the city. The amnestic nature of this form of topographic disorientation is
confirmed by the patient’s failure to give a verbal description of the route, to trace it on
a road map, and to draw a map of a familiar place.
The dissociation between topographic amnesia and topographic agnosia is
exemplified by patient 5 of Aimard et al. (1981) who was unable to find a room in his
apartment but, when he eventually opened its door, immediately recognized it. Of course,
1462
18. Occipital lobe
agnosic and amnesic disorders can coexist in the same patient, as both are dependent on
right hemispheric damage.
The study of the anatomic correlates of topographic disorientation has pointed out
the crucial role played by the posterior regions of the brain, especially of the right
hemisphere. In some cases damage is bilateral (Förster, 1890; Wildbrand, 1892; Dunn,
1895; Peters, 1896; Meyer, 1900), but in others it is confined to the right side, where the
areas more frequently involved are the parietal lobe (Paterson and Zangwill, 1945; Hécaen
et al., 1956; Assal, 1969; Aimard et al., 1981; Newcombe and Ratcliff, 1990) and the
medial occipitotemporal cortex (Gloning, 1965; Whitty and Newcombe, 1973; Hécaen
et al., 1980; Aimard et al., 1981; Landis et al., 1986; Habib and Sirigu, 1987). The most
frequent etiology is an infarction in the territory of the right posterior cerebral artery,
which supplies blood to the medial surface of the occipital and temporal lobe. Habib and
Sirigu (1987) argued that the crucial lesion is located in the right parahippocampal gyrus
(posterior to the uncus and anterior to the subsplenial region), but it is difficult to reconcile
this assumption with the lack of topographic disorientation found in patients submitted
to right temporal ablation, although they showed psychometric signs of spatial learning
impairment (Milner, 1965; Milner, 1971; Smith and Milner, 1981).
In the animal, parietal area 7, corresponding to the inferior parietal lobule in humans,
has strong connections with the posterior parahippocampal gyrus, but the evidence that
its damage results in route-finding impairment is meager and based only on the report
that monkeys are hesitant and slow in finding their way to the cage after bilateral
(Ettlinger and Wegener, 1958; Bates and Ettlinger, 1960) and also unilateral ablation
(Sugishita et al., 1978) of areas 5 and 7.
Episodes of topographic disorientation are frequently seen in the advanced stages
of degenerative dementia of Alzheimer type.
Eleven cases have been reported (Moretti et al., 1983; Stracciari, 1992; Stracciari et
al., 1994). During the attack which lasted from 5 to 40 min, the patients were perfectly
aware of what was happening to them and retained the ability to recognize places, shops,
streets, and so on, but they could not find their way out. None of them showed signs of
mental deterioration, or of verbal and spatial dysmnesia when the episode was over. This
reflects a transient dysfunction of unknown origin of the right occipitotemporal region.
disabilities can be observed in left-sided lesions in these same sites, but such patients
often have a fluent aphasia, a lesser or more transient constructional defect, and the defect
is more likely to show a lack of appreciation of spatial relationships and preserved
visuoperception (Jones and Tranel, 2002). Since the introduction of imaging techniques
in clinical practice, it has been increasingly recognized that subcortical structures
contribute to cognitive functions, and disorder of language, praxis, lateralized attention,
and so on, have been reported following damage to basal nuclei.
Visual hallucinations
A hallucination is a perception in the absence of an adequate peripheral stimulus; it
must be distinguished from an illusion, which is a misinterpretation of a perception.
Illusions occur in normal people, especially when they are tired, inattentive, or in states
of high expectation.
Visual hallucinations are less frequent in schizophrenia than auditory ones, but when
they are present, they often occur in association with other hallucinations. Visual hallu-
cinations of small animals are noted in delirium, especially delirium tremens, and
fornication, the sensation that animals are crawling under the skin or over the body, have
been associated with cocaine psychosis.
Traditionally it has been taught that the lesions responsible for visual hallucinations,
if identifiable, are usually situated in the occipital lobe or posterior part of the temporal
lobe, and that elementary hallucinations have their origin in the occipital cortex, and
complex ones, in the temporal cortex. However, the complexity of visual hallucinations
has only limited diagnostic value; in some cases, formed hallucinations are localizing
with lesions of the occipital lobe and unformed ones with lesions of the temporal lobe
(Weinberger and Grant, 1941).
Also, as emphasized by these authors, lesions that give rise to visual hallucinations,
simple or elaborate, need not be confined to central nervous structures but may be caused
by lesions at every level of the neuro-optic apparatus (retina, optic nerve, chiasma, etc.).
So, visual hallucinations caused by a neurological disease are attributable to lesions
anywhere in the visual system from the retina (macular degeneration, cataracts,
enucleations), optic nerve and tract, midbrain, geniculocalcarine radiation (multiple
sclerosis, ischemia, compression, stroke, tumors), to the occipital or temporal cortex
(stroke, tumors, seizures). They sometimes reflect sensory deprivation.
Other visual hallucinations appear in migraine, narcolepsy, Alzheimer’s disease,
diffuse Lewy body disease, Parkinson’s disease with dopaminergic treatment, drug
intoxication or withdrawal, metabolic encephalopathies (delirium), schizophrenia, and
depression or mania (mood congruent). The clinical setting for the occurrence of visual
hallucinations varies. The simplest black and white moving scintillations are part of
migraine. Others, some colored, can occur as a seizure aura. Often they are associated
with a homonymous hemianopia, as indicated earlier. Frequently they occur as part of a
confusional state or delirium. In the “peduncular hallucinosis” of Lhermitte (1971), the
hallucinations are purely visual, appear natural in form and color, sometimes in pastels,
move about as in an animated cartoon, and are usually considered to be unreal, abnormal
phenomena (preserved insight). Similar phenomena may occur as part of hypnagogic
hallucinations in the narcolepsy-cataplexy syndrome.
In their material, McKee and associates (1990) emphasize that so-called peduncular
hallucinosis has been associated mainly with high mesencephalic lesions and, more
particularly in one case, with lesions of the pars reticulata of the substantia nigra. The
hallucinations in this disorder are purely visual; if hallucinations are polymodal, the lesion
is always in the occipitotemporal parts of the cerebrum.
1466
18. Occipital lobe
Visual hallucinations may be elementary or complex, and both types have sensory
as well as cognitive aspects.
Elementary (or unformed) hallucinations include flashes of light, colors, luminous
points, stars, multiple lights (like candles), and geometric forms (circles, squares, and
hexagons). They may be stationary or moving (zigzag, oscillations, vibrations, or
pulsations). They are much the same as the effects that Foerster and Penfield (1930)
obtained by stimulating the calcarine cortex in a conscious person. Complex, or formed,
hallucinations include objects, persons or animals. They are indicative of lesions in the
visual association areas or their connections with the temporal lobe. They may be of
natural size, lilliputian, or grossly enlarged.
With hemianopsia, they may appear in the defective field or move from the intact
field toward the hemianopsic one. The patient may realize that the hallucinations are false
experiences or may be convinced of their reality. Patient’s response is usually in accord
with the nature of the hallucination or he may react with fear to a threatening vision.
A special syndrome of ophthalmopathic hallucinations occurs in the blind person.
The visual images may be of elementary or complex type, usually of people or animals,
and are polychromic (vivid colors). The hallucinations may occupy all of the visual field,
the field of one eye, or corresponding blind fields in the patient with homonymous
hemianopia (hemianopsia). Moving the eyes or closing the affected eye has variable
effects, sometimes abolishing the hallucinations. A similar phenomenon in the elderly
(with preserved vision) has been called the “syndrome of Bonnet”, following the report
by this author of visual hallucinations occurring in a “sane” person. The topic of senile
hallucinations has been extensively reviewed by Gold and Robin (1989).
The hallucinatory phenomena of delirium are nonlocalizable, but sometimes the
evidence points to an origin in the temporal lobe. In ophthalmopathic hallucinations, there
is visual loss from retinal, optic nerve or tract, or occipital lobe lesions, and, with the
latter locations, a slight impairment of mental function as well.
Visual imagery
Visual mental imagery refers to the evocation of visual images in the “mind’s eye”
in the absence of sensation through the retina.
Early studies demonstrated that patients with various types of deficits in higher order
visual functions (e.g., achromatopsia and spatial neglect) tended to show similar deficits
in mental imagery. For example, when asked to imagine walking down a familiar street,
a patient with left hemispatial visual neglect would describe only the buildings on the right
side of the street. When asked to imagine turning around and walking the other direction
up the street, the same patient would describe buildings on the other side of the street that
had previously been neglected (Heilman et al., 1993; Farah, 2001; Tranel, 2001).
Similarly, patients with deficits in color perception due to cerebral lesions have been
shown to have deficits in imaging colors of objects (Temple, 1992; Farah, 2001). So,
visual perceptual deficits in patients can often be demonstrated in some way by similar
deficits in visual imagery.
1467
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
(area 17) may be more actively involved in mental imagery than there had previously
been suspected.
Summary
Lesions in the primary visual area
A unilateral lesion in the primary visual area (Brodmann’s area 17) results in
blindness of the contralateral visual field, referred to as a contralateral homonymous
hemianopsia. If distruction of the primary visual cortex is the result of a vascular lesion,
macular sparing may occurs, that is, central vision is unaffected.
It is believed that this due to the presence of collateral anastomotic channels between
the middle and posterior cerebral arteries.
A bilateral lesion in the primary visual area results in complete blindness, also
referred to as cortical blindness.
The usual cause of cortical blindness occlusion of the posterior cerebral arteries
(embolic and thrombotic). A transitory form of cortical blindness may occur with head
injury, migraine, or the antiphospholipid antibody syndrome of lupus erythematous, and
as a consequence of intravascular dye injection and administration of a number of drugs
such as interferon-alpha or cyclosporine.
REFERENCES
Adams RD, Victor M, Ropper AH, Neurologic disorders caused by lesions in particular parts of
the cerebrum. In: RD Adams et al. (eds.). Principles of Neurology. Sixth Edition, chap 22;
New York, St Louis, San Francisco, etc., McGraw-Hill, 435-471, 1997.
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Impaired recognition of emotion in facial
expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 372; 669-672, 1994.
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Fear and the human amygdala. J Neurosci 15;
5879-5891, 1995.
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR, The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393; 470-
474, 1998
Aimard G, Vighetto A, Confavreux C, Devic M, La désorientation spatiale. Rev Neurol 137; 97-
111, 1981.
Ajuriaguerra J, Hecaen H, Angelergues R, Les apraxies: Variétés cliniques et latéralisation
lésionnelle. Rev Neurol 102; 566-594, 1960.
Albright TD, Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of macaque. J.
Neurophysiol 52; 1106-1130, 1984.
Albright TD, Form-cue invariant motion processing in primate visual cortex. Science 255; 11141-
1143, 1992.
Assal G, Régression des troubles de la reconnaissance des physionomies et de la mémoire
topographique chez un malade opéré d’un hématome intracérébral pariétotemporal droite.
Rev Neurol 121; 184-185, 1969.
Barbur JL, Watson JDG, Franckowiak RSJ, Zeki S, Conscious visual perception without V1.
Brain 116; 1293-1302, 1993.
Bargh JA, Ferguson MJ, Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes.
Psychological Bulletin 126; 925-945, 2000.
Barlow HB, Blakemore C, Pettigrew JD, The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination.
J. Physiol 193; 327-342, 1967.
Bates JAV, Ettlinger G, Posterior biparietal ablation in the monkey. Arch Neurol 3; 177-192,
1960.
Barrash J, A historical review of topographical disorientation and its neuroanatomical correlates.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20; 807-827, 1998.
Barton JJ, Caplan LR, Cerebral visual dysfunction. In: J Bogousslavsky and LR Caplan (eds).
Stroke Syndromes. (2nd ed) Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
Behrmann M, Winocur G, Moscovitch M, Dissociation between mental imagery and object
recognition in a brain-damaged patient. Nature 359; 636-637, 1992.
Bender MB, Krieger HP, Visual function in perimetric blind field. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 65;
72, 1951.
Benson DF, Disorders of visual gnosis. In: JW Brown (ed), Neuropsychology of Visual
Perception. New York: IRBN Press, 1989.
Benton AL, Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia 6; 53-60,
1968.
Berson DM, Strange vision: ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci 26; 314-
320, 2003.
Blardel GG, Salama G, Voltage-sensitive dyes reveal a modular organization in monkey striate
cortex. Nature 321; 579-585, 1986.
1470
18. Occipital lobe
Bonhoffer T, Grinvald A, Optical imaging based on intrinsic signals: The methodology. In: A
Tage (ed), Brain Mapping: The Methods. New York: Academic Press, 1996.
Bonhoffer T, Grinvald A, The layout of iso-orientation domains in area 18 of the cat visual cortex:
Optical imaging reveals a pinwheel-like organization. J Neurosci 13; 4157-4180, 1993.
Bodamer J, Die Prosopagnosie. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkrank 179; 6-53, 1947.
Boussaud D, Ungerleider GL, Desimone R, Pathways for motion analysis: Cortical connections
of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the
macaque. Journal of Comparative Neurology 296; 462-495, 1990.
Bouvier SE, Engel SA, Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia.
Cereb Cortex 16; 183-191, 2006.
Bullier J, Feedback connections and conscious vision. Trend Cogn. Sci. 5; 369-370, 2001.
Burton H, Abend NS, MacLeod A-MK, et al., Tactile attention tasks enhance activation in
somatosensory regions of parietal cortex: A positron emission tomography study. Cerebral
Cortex 9; 662-674, 1999.
Callaway EM, Local circuits in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Ann Rev Neurosci
21; 47-74, 1998.
Campbell R, Landis T, Regard M, Face recognition and lip reading: A neurological dissociation.
Brain 108; 509-521, 1986.
Casagrande VA, Kaas JH, The afferent, intrinsic, and efferent connections of primary visual cortex
in primates. In: A Peters and K Rockland (eds), Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 10, Plenum Press, New
York; 201-259, 1994.
Chaves CJ, Caplan LR, Posterior cerebral artery. In: J Bogousslavsky and LR Caplan (eds). Stroke
Syndromes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
Clarke S, Miklossy J, Occipital cortex in man. Organization of callosal connections, related myelo-
and cytoarchitecture, and putative boundaries of functional visual areas. Journal of
Comparative Neurology 298; 188-214, 1990.
Corbetta M, Mierzin FM, Dobmeyer S, et al., Selective and divided attention during visual
discrimination of shape, color, and speed: Functional anatomy by positron emission
tomography. Journal of Neuroscience 11; 2383-2402, 1991.
Corbetta M, Mierzin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE, A PET study of visuospatial attention.
Journal of Neuroscience 13; 1202-1226, 1993.
Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, What fMRI has taught us about human vision. Curr Op Neurobiol
7; 554-561, 1997.
Chalupa LM, Werner JS (eds), The Visual Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2004.
Crick E, Koch C, Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature 375; 121-123,
1995.
Cumming BC, An unexpected specialization for horizontal disparity in primate primary visual
cortex. Nature 418; 633-636, 2002.
Damasio AR, Disorder of complex visual processing: Agnosia, achromatopsia, Balint’s syndrome
and related difficulties of orientation and construction. In: Mesulam MM (ed), Principles of
Behavioral Neurology. Philadelphia, Davis; 259-288, 1985.
Damasio AR, Regional diagnosis of cerebral disorders. In: JB Wyngaarden and LH Smith, Jr
(eds), Textbook of Medicine (18th ed). Philadelphia: Saunders, 1988.
Damasio AR, McKee J, Damasio H, Determinants of performance in color anomia. Brain Land
7; 74-85, 1979.
Damasio A, Yamada T, Damasio H, et al., Central achromatopsia: Behavioral, anatomic, and
psychologic aspects. Neurology 30; 1064, 1980.
1471
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Damasio AR, Damasio H, Van Hoesen GW, Prosopagnosia: Anatomical basis and behavioral
mechanisms. Neurology 32; 331-341, 1982.
Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H, Disorders of visual recognition. In: F Boller and J Grafman
(eds), Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 2, Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1989.
Damasio AR, Tranel D, Rizzo M, Disorders of complex visual processing. In: MM Mesulam (ed),
Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. Oxford Univ Press, New York, 332-372,
2000.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Tratat de Neuropsihologie, Vol 2, editura Medicală Bucureşti; 261-319, 2006.
De Haan EHF, A familial factor in the development of face recognition deficits. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol 21; 312-315, 1999.
De Haan EHF, Campbell R, A fifteen year follow-up of a case of developmental prosopagnosia.
Cortex 27; 489-509, 1991.
De Renzi E, Scotti G, The influence of spatial disorders in impairing tactual discrimination of
shapes. Cortex 5; 53-62, 1969.
De Renzi E, Bonacini MG, Faglioni P, Right posterior patients are poor assessing the age of a
face. Neuropsychologia 27; 839-848, 1989.
Déjérine J, Sur un cas de cécité verbale avec agraphie, suivi d’autopsie. CK Séances Soc Biol 3;
197-201, 1891.
Déjérine J, Contribution à l’étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différents variétés de
cécité verbale. CR Séances Soc Biol 4; 61-90, 1892.
Desimone R, Albright TD, Gross CG, Bruce C, Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal
neurons in the macaque. J. Neurosci 4; 2051-2062, 1984.
Deyoe EA, Occipital lobe. In: VS Ramachandran (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain,
Vol. 3, Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston, London, etc.; 677-7115, 2002.
Deyoe EA, van Essen DC, Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends
Neurosci 11; 219-226, 1988.
Dunn D, Double hemiplegia with double hemianopsia and loss of a geographical center. Trans
Coll Phys Philadelphia 17; 45-55, 1895.
D’Esposito M, Detre JA, Aguirre GK, et al., A functional MRI study of mental image generation.
Neuropsychologia 35; 725-730, 1997.
Ettlinger G, Wegener J, Somaesthetic alternation, discrimination and orientation after frontal, and
parietal lesions in the monkey. Q J Exp Psychol 10; 177-186, 1958.
Eastwood JD, Smilek D, Merikle PM, Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces
expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception and Psychophysics 63; 1004-1013,
2001.
Epstein R, Konwisher N, Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital
complex. Science 293, 1506-1509, 1998.
Farah MJ, The neurological basis of mental imagery. A componential analysis. Cognition 18; 245-
272, 1984.
Farah M, Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1990.
Farah MJ, The neuropsychology of mental imagery. In: F Boller and J Grafman (eds.), Handbook
of Neuropsychology. Vol. 2, Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1990.
Farah MJ What is “special” about face perception? Psychological Review 105; 482-498, 1998.
Farah MJ, The neural basis of mental imagery. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive
Neurosciences. 2nd ed. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 965-974, 2000.
1472
18. Occipital lobe
Farah MJ, The neuropsychology of mental imagery. In: F Boller and J Grafman (eds), Handbook
of Neuropsychology. 2nd ed. Vol 4, Elsevier, Amsterdam; 239-248, 2001.
Farah MJ, Levine DN, Calvanio R, A case study of mental imagery deficit. Brain Cog 8; 147-
164, 1988.
Farah MJ, Tanaka JW, Drain HM, What causes the face inversion effect? J Psychol Hum Percept
Perform 21; 628-634, 1995.
Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC, Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex.
Cerebral Cortex 1; 1-47, 1991.
Förster R, Ueber Rindenblindheit. Albrecht v. Graefe Arch Ophtalmol 38; 94-108, 1890.
Foerster O, Penfield W, The structural basis of traumatic epilepsy and results of radical operation.
Brain 53; 99-119, 1930.
Geschwind N, Fusillo M, Color naming defectes in association with alexia. Arch Neurol. 15; 137-
146, 1966.
Gloning K, Die Cerebralen Bedingten Stoerungen de Raumlichen Sehens und des Raumerlebens.
Vienna, Maudrig, 1965.
Gold K, Robin PV, Isolated visual hallucinations and the Charles Bonnet syndrome: A review of
the literature and presentations of six cases. Comp Psychiatry 30; 90, 1989.
Goodale MA, Visual pathways supporting perception and action in the primate cerebral cortex.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 3; 578-585, 1993.
Goodale MA, Perception and action in the human visual system. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed), The New
Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd ed. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 365-377, 2000.
Goodale MA, Humphrey GK, The objects of action and perception. Cognition 67; 181-207, 1998.
Goodale MA, Millner DA, Jakobson LS, Carey JDP, A neurological dissociation between
perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature 349; 154-156, 1991.
Goodglass H, Wingfield A, Hyde MR, Theukrauf JC, Category specific dissociations in naming
and recognition by aphasic patients. Cortex 22; 87-102, 1986.
Grill-Spector K, Kushner T, Edelman S, et al., Differential processing of objects under various
viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron 24; 187-203, 1999.
Gross CG, Representation of visual stimuli in inferior temporal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond
B Biol Sci 335, 3-10, 1992.
Habib A, Sirigu A, Pure topographical disorientation: A definition and anatomical basis. Cortex
23; 73-85, 1987.
Hancock PJB, Bruce V, Burton M, Recognition of unfamiliar faces. Trends in cognitive Science
4; 330-337, 2000.
Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, et al., Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: Architecture,
projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science 295; 1065-1070, 2002.
Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG, Clark VP, et al., The effect of face inversion on activity in human
neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22; 189-199, 1999.
Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI, The distributed human neural system for face perception.
Trends in Cognitive Science 4; 223-233, 2000.
Hécaen H, Penfield W, Bertrand C, Malno R, The syndrome of apractognosia due to lesions of
the minor cerebral hemisphere. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 75; 400-434, 1956.
Hécaen H, Clinical symptomatology in right and left hemispheric lesions. In: VB Mountcastle
(ed) Interhemispheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance. Baltimore, Johns Hopkins.
Ch. 10; 215-263, 1962.
Hécaen H, Tzortzis C, Randot P, Loss of topographical memory with learning deficits. Cortex
16; 525-542, 1980.
1473
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E, Neglect and related disorders. In: KM Heilman and E
Valenstein (eds), Clinical Neuropsychology. 3rd ed. Oxford Univ Press, New York; 279-336,
1993.
Hendry SH, Reid RC, The koniocellular pathway in primate vision. Anu Rev Neurosci 23; 127-
153, 2000.
Hoff H, Potzl O, Zür diagnostischen Bedeutung der Polyopie bei Tumoren des Okzipithalhirns.
Z Gesante Neurol Psychiatr 152; 433, 1935.
Horton JC, The central visual pathways. In: WM Hart (ed.), Alder’s Physiology of the Eye. St
Louis: Mosby Yearbook, 1992.
Hubel DH, Eye, Brain and Vision. New York: Scientific American Library, 1988.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the
cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 160; 106-154, 1962.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J
Physiol (Lond) 195; 215-243, 1968.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex. Proc R Soc
(Lond) 198; 1-59, 1977.
Jakobson LS, Archibald YM, Carey DP, Goodale MA, A kinematic analysis of reaching and grasping
movements in a patient recovering from optic ataxia. Neuropsychologia 2; 803-809, 1991.
Jones R, Tranel D, Severe “associative” developmental prosopagnosia in a child with superior
intellect. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 23; 265-273, 2001.
Jones RD, Tranel D, Visual disorder. In: VS Ramachandran (ed), Encyclopedia of the Human
Brain, Vol 4. Amsterdam, Boston, London etc., Academic Press; 775-789, 2002.
Julesz B, Foundations of Cyclopean Perception. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1971.
Julesz, B, Dialogues on Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995.
James TW, Culham J, Humphrey GK, et al., Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition
but not object-directed grasping. An fMRI study. Brain 126; 2463-2475, 2003.
Kaas JH, The organization and evolution of neocortex. In: SP Wise (ed), Ed. Higher Brain
Function. New York: Wiley, 1987.
Kaas J, The organization of visual cortex in primates: Problems, conclusions, and the use of
comparative studies in understanding the human brain. In: K Rockland (ed), Cerebral Cortex,
Vol. 12, Plenum Press, New York, 1997.
Kahneman D, Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1973.
Kanwisher N, Mc Dermott J, Chun MM, The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate
cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302-4311, 1997.
Kingdom F, Li H-CO, Mac Aulay EJ, The role of chromatic contrast and luminance polarity in
stereoscopic segmentation. Vision Research 41; 375-383, 2001.
Kinsbourne M, Warrington EK, The localizing significance of limited simultaneous visual form
perception. Brain 86; 697, 1963.
Kinsbourne M, Warrington EK, Observations on color agnosia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
27; 296-299, 1964.
Kleist K, Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig, Barth, 1934.
Kolb B, Whishaw JQ, The occipitals lobes. In: B Kolb and JQ Whishaw (eds), Fundamentals of
Human Neuropsychology Fifth Edition, Worth Publishers, New York; 318-344, 2003.
Kolb B, Milner B, Taylor L, Perception of faces by patients with localized cortical excisions.
Canadian Journal of Psychology 37; 8-18, 1983.
Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Shared mechanisms in visual imagery and visual perception: Insight
from cognitive neuroscience. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd
ed. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 975-986, 2000.
1474
18. Occipital lobe
Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N, Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital
complex. Science 293, 1506-1509, 2001.
Kracke J, Developmental prosopagnosia in Asperger syndrome: Presentation and discussion of
an individual case. Dev Med Child Neurol 36; 873-886, 1994.
Kuffler SW, Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammallian retina. J Neurophysiol
16; 37-68, 1953.
Kurucz J, Feldmar G, Prosopo-affective agnosia as a symptom of cerebral organic disease. J Am
Geriatric Soc 23; 225-230, 1979.
Lamme VA, Roclfsema PR, The distinct models of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent
processing. Trends Neurosci 23, 571-579, 2000.
Land E, Recent advances in Retinex theory. Vis Res 26; 7-21, 1986.
Landis T, Cummings JL, Benson DF, Palmer EP, Loss of topographical familiarity. An
environmental agnosia. Arch Neurol. 43; 132-136, 1986.
Lhermitte F, Chain F, Escourolle, et al., Etude des troubles perceptifs auditifs dans les lésions
temporales bilatérales. Revue Neurol. (Paris) 124; 329-351, 1971.
Lissauer H, A case of visual agnosia with a contribution to theory. Cognitive Neuropsychology
5; 157-192, 1888.
Lissauer H, Ein Fall von Seelenblindheit nebst eine Beitrag zur Theorie des selben. Arch für
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 21; 222-270, 1890.
Livingston M, Hubel D, Segregation of form, color, movement and depth: Anatomy, physiology,
and perception. Science 240; 740-749, 1988.
Luria AR, Tsvetkova LS, The programming of constructive activity in local brain injuries.
Neuropsychologia 2; 95-108, 1964.
Logan GD, Attention and preattention in theories of automaticity. American Journal of Psychology
105; 317-340, 1992.
Logothetis NK, Paulus J, Poggio T, Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of
monkeys. Curr Biol 5, 552-563, 1995.
Lorente de No R, The structure of the cerebral cortex. Physiology of the Nervous System, 3rd ed.
New York: Oxford University Press, 1949.
Luzzatti C, Davidoff J, Impaired retrieval of object-color knowledge with preserved color naming.
Neuropsychologia 32; 933-950, 1994.
Marshall JC, Newcombe F, Patterns of paralexia : A psycholinguistic approach. H Psycholing
Res 2; 175-199, 1973.
Marshall RS, Lazar RM, Binder JR, et al., Intrahemispheric localization of drawing dysfunction.
Neuropsychologia 32; 493-501, 1994.
Maunsell JHK, Functional visual streams. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2; 506-510, 1992.
Mc Carthy G, Puce A, Gore JC, Allison T, Face-specific processing in human fusiform gyrus. J.
Cogn Neurosci 9, 605-610, 1997.
McKee AC, Levine D, Kowall NW, Richardson EP, Peduncular hallucinations associated with
isolated infarction of the substantia nigra pars reticulata. Ann Neurol 27; 500, 1990.
McNeil JE, Warrington EK, Prosopagnosia: a face-specific disorder. Q J Exp Psychol 46; 1-10, 1993.
Meadows JC, The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 37; 489-501,
1974.
Meadows JC, Disturbed perception of color associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain 97;
615-632, 1974.
Meyer O, Ein- und doppelseitige homonyme Hemianopsie mit Orientierungstoerungen. Monatschr
Psychiatr Neurol 8; 440-456, 1900.
1475
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Milner B, Visually-guided maze learning in man: Effects of bilateral hippocampal, bilateral frontal
and unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia 3; 317-338, 1965.
Milner B, Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Brit
Med Bull 27; 272-277, 1971.
Milner AD, Goodale MA, The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1995.
Moran J, Desimone R, Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science
229; 782-784, 1985.
Moretti G, Cafarra P, Parma M, Transient topographical amnesia. Ital J Neurol Sci 3; 361, 1983.
Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Behrmann M, What is special about face recognition? Nineteen
experiments on a person with visual object agnosia and dyslexia but normal face recognition.
J Cogn Neurosci 9; 555-604, 1997.
Mountcastle VB, Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat’s somatic sensory
cortex. J Neurophysiol 20; 408-434, 1957.
Mountcastle VB, Perceptual Neuroscience: The Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge: Harvard University
Press, 1988.
Mountcastle VB, The parietal system and some higher brain functions. Cerebral Cortex 5; 377-
390, 1995.
Moutoussis K, Zeki S, The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated
with invisible stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99; 9527-9532, 2002.
Moutoussis K, Zeki S, Seeing invisible motion: a human fMRI study. Curr Biol 16; 574-579,
2006.
Naatanen T, Attention and Brain Function. Hillsdale NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1992.
Newcombe F, Ratcliff G, Disorders of visuospatial analysis. In: F Boller, J Grafman (eds):
Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam, Elsevier, vol 2; 333-356, 1990.
Niebur E, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO, Synchrony: A neuron mechanism for attentional selection?,
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 12; 190-194, 2002.
Nielsen JM, Agnosia, Apraxia, Aphasia: Their Value in Cerebral Localization, 2nd ed, New York,
Hoeber, 1946
Obermayer K, Blasdel GG, Geometry of orientation and ocular dominance columns in monkey
striate cortex. J Neurosci 13; 4114-4129, 1993.
Ogden JA, Visual object agnosia, prosopagnosia, achromatopsia, loss of visual imagery, and
autobiographical amnesia following recovery from cortical blindness. Case MH.
Neuropsychologia 31; 571-58, 1993.
Paterson A, Zangwill OL, A case of topographical disorientation associated with a unilateral
cerebral lesion. Brain 68; 188-221, 1945.
Pasley BN, Mayes LC, Schultz RT, Subcortical discrimination of unperceived objects during
binocular rivalry. Neuron 42; 163-172, 2004.
Pasupathy A, Connor CE, Population coding of shape in area V4. Nat Neurosci 5; 1332-1338,
2002.
Peters A, Ueber die Beziehungenzwieschen Orientierungsstoerungen und ein- und doppelseitiger
Hemianopsie. Arch Augenheilk 32; 175-187, 1896.
Petersen SE, Robinson DL, Morris JD, Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial orientation.
Neuropsychologia 25; 97-106, 1987.
Pillon B, Troubles visuoconstructifs et methodes de compensation: Resultats de 85 patients atteints
de lésions cerebrales. Neuropsychologia 19; 375-383, 1981.
Posner MI, Raichle ME, Images of Mind. New York: Scientific American Library, 1993.
1476
18. Occipital lobe
Poggio GF, Central neural mechanisms in vision. In: VB Mountcastle (ed), Medical Physiology.
St Louis: Mosby, 1968.
Pollen DA, On the neural correlates of visual perception. Cereb. Cortex 9; 4-19, 1999.
Previc FH, Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological
origins and neurophysiological implications. Behavioral and Brain Science 13; 519-575, 1990.
Purves D, Lotto RB, Why We See What We Do: An Empirical Theory of Vision, Chapter 3 and 4.
Sunderland MA: Sinauer Associates; 41-87, 2003.
Purves D, Riddle DR, La Mantia AS, Iterated patterns of brain circuitry (or how the brain gets its
spots). Trends in Neuroscience 15; 362-368, 1992.
Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, et al., Central visual pathways. In: D Purves, Augustine
GJ, Fitzpatrick D et al., (eds), Neuroscience. Third Edition. Chapter 11, Sinauer Associates
Inc Publishers. Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA; 259-282, 2004.
Quaglino A, Emiplegia sinistra con amaurosi – Guarigione. Perdita totale della percezione dei
colori e della memoria della configurazione degli oggetti. Annotazione alla medecima di GB
Borelli. Giorn Oftalmol Ital 10; 106-117, 1867.
Rees G, Kreiman G, Koch C, Natural correlates of consciousness in human, Nat Rev Neurosci in
human, Nat Rev Neurosci 3, 261-270, 2002.
Rizzo M, Damasio AR, Acquired central achromatopsia, In: JJ Kulikowski, CM Dickinson, KK
Murray (eds), Seeing Contour and Color, England: Pergamon Press; 758-763, 1989.
Rizzo M, Robin DA, Simultanagnosia: A defect of sustained attention yields insights on visual
information processing. Neurology 40; 447, 1990.
Rizzo M, Smith V, Pokorny J, Damasio AR, Color perception profiles in central achromatopsia.
Neurology 43; 995-1001, 1993.
Rodieck RW, The First Step in Seeing. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, 1998.
Sacks O, Wasserman R, The case of the colorblind painter. New York: Review of Books 34, 25-
33, 1987.
Schiller PH, Logothetis NK, The color-opponent and broad-band channels of the primate visual
system. Trends Neurosci 13; 392-398, 1990.
Schnider A, Landis T, Regard M, Benson DF, Dissociation of color from object in amnesia. Arch
Neurol 49; 82-985, 1992.
Schwarzlose RF, Baker CJ, Kanwisher N, Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform
gyrus. J. Neurosci. 25, 11055-11059, 2005.
Sereno MI, Sale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belieweau JW, Brody TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB,
Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance
imaging. Science 268; 889-893, 1995.
Servas P, Goodale MA, Humphrey GK, The drawing of objects by a visual form agnosic:
Contribution of surface properties and memorial representations. Neuropsychologia 31; 251-
259, 1993.
Smith ML, Milner B, The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location.
Neuropsychologia 19; 781-793, 1981.
Spitzer H, Desimone R, Moran J, Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal
performance. Science 240; 338-340, 1988.
Stracciari A, Transient topographical amnesia. Ital J Neurol Sci 13; 593-596, 1992.
Stracciari A, Lorusso S, Pazzaglia P, Transient topographical amnesia. J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry 57; 1423-1425, 1994.
Sugishita M, Ettlinger G, Ridley RM, Disturbance of cage-finding in the monkey. Cortex 14; 431-
438, 1978.
1477
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Sumer P, Mollon JD, Catarrhine photopigments are optimized for detecting targets against a
foliage background. Journal of Experimental Biology 203; 1963-1986, 2000.
Tanaka J, Weiskopf D, Williams P, The role of color in high-level vision. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences 5; 211-215, 2001.
Tanaka K, Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 19, 109-139, 1996.
Temple CM, Developmental memory impairment: Faces and patterns. In: R Campbell (ed), Mental
Lives: Case Studies in Cognition. Blackwell, Oxford; 199-215, 1992.
Teuber HL, Alteration of perception and memory in man. In: L Weiskrantz (ed.). Analysis of
Behavioral Change. New York: Harper and Row; 269-375, 1968.
Tong F, Primary visual cortex and visual awareness. Nat Rev. Neurosci 4; 219-229, 2003.
Tong F, Nakayama K, Moscovitch M, et al., Response properties of the human fusiform face area.
Cogn Neuropsychol 17; 257-279, 2000.
Tong F, Pearson J, Vision. In: BJ Baars and NM Gace (eds). Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness.
Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. Chapter 6. Amsterdam-Boston-Heidelberg etc.
Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier; 149-181, 2007.
Tootell RBH, Hadjikhani N, Where is “dorsal V4” in human visual cortex? Retinotopic
topographic and functional evidence. Cerebral Cortex 11; 298-311, 2001.
Tootell RB, Dale AM, Sereno MI, Malach R, New images from human visual cortex. Trends
Neurosci 19; 481-48, 1996.
Tsao DY, Freiwold WA, Tootell RB, Livingstone MS, A cortical region consisting entirely of
face-selective cells. Science 311; 670-674, 2006.
Tranel D, Disorders of color processing (perception, imagery, recognition, and naming). In: TE
Feinberg and JM Farah (eds), Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. New York, St
Louis, San Francisco etc., McGraw-Hill, Health Profession Division; 257-265, 1997.
Tranel D, Central color processing and its disorders. In: F Boller and J Grafman (eds), Handbook
of Neuropsychology. 2nd ed., Vol. 4, Elsevier, Amsterdam; 1-14, 2001.
Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR, A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge.
Neuropsychologia 35; 1319-1324, 1997.
Treisman A, Features and objects in visual processing. Scientific American 254; 14-124, 1986.
Treisman A, Gormican S, Feature analysis in early vision. Psychological Review 95; 15-30, 1988.
Ungerleder JG, Mishkin M, Two cortical visual systems. In: DJ Ingle, MA Goodale and RJ
Mansfield (eds). Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 549-586, 1982.
Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV, “What” and “where” in the human brain, Current Opinion in
Neurobiology 4; 15-165, 1994.
Valentine T, Upside-down faces: A review of the effects of inversion upon face recognition, British
Journal of Psychology 79; 471-491, 1988.
Van Buren JM, Trans-synaptic retrograde degeneration in the visual system of primates. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 26; 402, 1963.
Weinberger LM, Grant FC, Visual hallucinations and their neuro-optical correlates, Arch
Ophthalmol 23; 166, 1941.
Weliky M, Bosking WH, Fitzpatrick D, A systematic map of direction preference in primary
visual cortex. Nature 379; 725-728, 1996.
Whitty CWM, Newcombe F, Oldfield’s study of visual and topographical disturbances in a right
occipito-parietal lesion after 30 years duration. Neuropsychologia 11; 471-475, 1973.
Wildbrand H, Ein Fall von Seelenblindheit und hemianopsie mit Sections-Befund. Dtsch Z
Nervenheilk 2; 361-387, 1892.
1478
18. Occipital lobe
1479
WHITE MATTER
OF CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE
The central nervous system (CNS) is composed of white and gray matter.
White matter consists mostly of nerve cell axons, whereas gray matter consists
mostly of nerve cell bodies.
Gray matter is arranged into nuclei or cortex. The cerebral cortex consists of 50-
100 billion nerve cell bodies arranged into a three to six layered sheet that laminates the
brain surface.
All information entering (afferent fibres) or leaving the cerebral cortex (efferent
fibres) or connecting one part of the cortex with another must pass through the subcortical
white matter. Thus, the cerebral cortex receives the following afferent (input) fibres:
corticocortical (association and commissural) excitatory fibres (which release glutamate
or aspartate) from ipsilateral and contralateral areas, thalamocortical excitatory fibres
from thalamus nuclei, cholinergic excitatory fibres from the basal forebrain nuclei,
noradrenergic inhibitory fibres from the brain stem locus ceruleus, and serotonergic
inhibitory fibres from the brain stem raphe nuclei (Patestas and Gartner, 2008). Cortical
efferent fibres arise from the output neurons of the cerebral cortex: the pyramidal and
fusiform cells. Their myelinated axons course deep, pass into the subcortical white matter,
and then are distributed to widespread regions of the CNS.
The central core white matter of the cerebral hemispheres is composed of myelinated
nerve fibres of varied sizes and their supporting neuroglia. These nerve fibres which make
up the white matter of the cerebral hemispheres are either association fibres which link
different cortical areas in the same hemisphere; commissural fibres, which link
corresponding cortical areas in the two hemispheres; or projection fibres, which connect
the cerebral cortex with the corpus striatum, diencephalon, brain stem and the spinal cord.
Association fibres
The association fibres interconnect various areas of the cortex within the same
hemisphere. Association fibres also known as arcuate fibres are axons of small pyramidal
cells and fusiform neurons primarily from cortical layers II and III. Association fibres that
1480
19. White matter of cerebral hemisphere
connect various cortical areas make up most of subcortical white matter. These fibres gather
to form fasciculi that connect different lobes, but like a two-way highway, fibres merge into
and exit these fasciculi all along their course. These arcuate fibres restricted to a single
hemisphere are subdivided into two major categories, short and long fibres (Fig. 19.1).
The short arcuate fibres connect adjacent gyri (U-shaped fibres). These fibres leave
the cortex of one gyrus, enter the underlying white matter, and then loop around the pit
of a sulcus and project into the cortex of an adjacent gyrus. Thus, many pass subcortically
between adjacent gyri, some merely pass from one wall of sulcus to the other, and most
of them are confined to cortical gray matter.
The long arcuate fibres which connect distant nonadjacent gyri, and different lobes
of the same hemisphere consist of the following fibre tracts; the uncinate fasciculus,
cingulum, superior longitudinal fasciculus, inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and the
superior and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus.
The fibres follow a sharply curved course across the stem of the lateral sulcus, near
the anteroinferior part of the insula.
The uncinate fasciculus which extends from the anterior temporal to motor speech
(Broca’s) area and orbital gyri, forms fronto-temporal interconnections.
The cingulum which extends from medial cortex below the rostrum to
parahippocampal gyrus and parts of the temporal lobe is located internal to the cingulate
gyrus continuing into the parahippocampal gyrus. The cingulum is a prominent
association bundle in the cingulate gyrus. This bundle of long and short association fibres
carries impulses to and from the parahippocampal gyrus.
Fig. 19.1. The organization of the principal association fibres projected upon a sagittal section of the left cerebral
hemisphere. (after Crossman, 2008).
1481
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The superior longitudinal fasciculus extends from anterior frontal lobe to occipital,
parietal, and temporal lobes and is located in the core of the hemisphere. It is the largest
of long association fasciculi. It starts in the anterior frontal region and arches back, above
the insular area contributing fibres to the occipital cortex (areas 18 and 19). It curves
down and forwards, behind the insular area, to spread out in the temporal lobe.
The superior longitudinal fasciculus contains a group of fibres, the arcuate fasciculus
that forms a distinct arch posterior to the insula. This fasciculus links Broca’s area (the
frontal lobe motor area for speech) with Wernicke’s area (the temporal lobe area for
language comprehension).
The superior fronto-occipital fasciculus (subcalosal bundle) interconnects the
frontal lobe with the occipital lobe.
The fronto-occipital fasciculus starts at the frontal pole. It passes back deep to the
superior longitudinal fasciculus, separated from it by the projection fibres in the corona
radiata. It lies lateral to the caudate nucleus near the central part of the lateral ventricle.
Posteriorly, it fans out into the occipital and temporal lobes, lateral to the posterior and
inferior horns of the lateral ventricle (Crossman, 2008).
The inferior longitudinal fasciculus extends from posterior region of the parietal
and temporal lobes to anterior region of occipital lobe. Its fibres, probably derived mostly
from areas 18 and 19, sweep forward, separated from the posterior horn of the lateral
ventricle by the optic radiation and tapetal commissural fibres, and are distributed
throughout the temporal lobe (Crossman, 2008).
The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, which like their superior counterpart also
interconnects the frontal and occipital lobes, radiates in the frontal lobe, passes through
the temporal lobe inferior to the insula, to also radiate in the occipital lobe. Some of its
fibres curve around on the deep aspect of the lateral sulcus, interconnecting the frontal
and temporal lobes, as a result of their hook-like path are referred to as the uncinate
fasciculus (Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
The external capsule is insinuated between the claustrum and putamen, and the
extreme capsule is located between the claustrum and the insular cortex (Haines and
Mihailoff, 2006).
Projection fibres
Projection fibres include ascending fibres from lower centers to the neocortex, such
as projections from the thalamus, and descending fibres from the neocortex to the brain
stem and spinal cord.
Projection fibres are restricted to a single hemisphere and connect the cerebral cortex
with lower levels, namely the corpus striatum, diencephalon, brain stem, and spinal cord.
The majority of these fibres are axons of pyramidal cells and fusiform neurons.
These fibres are component part of the internal capsule, which is subdivided into
the anterior limb (Fig. 19.2), genu (Fig. 19.3), posterior limb (Fig. 19.4), restrolentiform,
and sublentiform regions. The projection fibres may be subdivided into corticopetal and
corticofugal fibres.
1482
19. White matter of cerebral hemisphere
Corticopetal fibres are afferent fibres that bring information from the thalamus to
the cerebral cortex. They consist of thalamocortical fibres.
Thus corticopetal fibres include: (1) thalamocortical fibres arising from the VPL,
VPM, ventral lateral (VL), and ventral anterior (VA) nuclei of the thalamus relaying
sensory information such as touch, pressure, conscious proprioception, two-point tactile
1483
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
sensation, as well as pain and temperature sensation from the body and head to the sensory
cortex; (2) the MGN (medial geniculate nuclei) of the thalamus relaying auditory
information via the auditory radiation to the auditory cortex; and (3) the LGN (lateral
geniculate nuclei) of the thalamus relaying visual information via the optic radiation to
the visual cortex. Although these afferent fibres terminate in layers I-IV of the cerebral
cortex, most terminate in layers IV. The cholinergic and serotonergic afferents of the
cerebral cortex function in the control of the sleep and arousal.
Corticofugal fibres are efferent fibres that transmit information from the cerebral
cortex to lower centers of the brain and spinal cord.
These fibres consist of axons arising from large pyramidal cells that course in the
corona radiata and internal capsule to terminate in the basal ganglia, the brain stem, and
the spinal cord.
Corticofugal fibres consist of the corticothalamic, corticoreticular, corticorubral,
corticotegmental, corticopontine, corticobulbar and corticospinal tracts.
Commissural fibres
The commissural fibres ensure the anatomical and functional connections of the two
cerebral hemispheres. Therefore, it is possible to coordinate their integrative-reflex
actions.
Commissural fibres arise in one cerebral hemisphere and cross the midline to
terminate in the corresponding cortical area of the contralateral hemisphere. These fibres
provide an avenue of communication between corresponding cortical areas of the two
hemispheres which is essential in the integration of information in the two sides of the
brain. Commissural fibres consist of the myelinated axons of medium-sized pyramidal
neurons residing in various layers of the cerebral cortex. Cortical commissural fibres
emerge mainly from the pyramidal cells residing in the cortical superficial layers and the
external granular (layer II) and external pyramidal (layer III) layers.
In the adult brain there are four major hemispheric commissures: the anterior
commissure, the posterior commissures that bridge the temporal lobes, the hippocampal
commissure (commissure of the fornix) connecting hippocampal formation of two
hemispheres, and the corpus callosum.
Anterior commissure
The anterior commissure is a compact bundle of myelinated nerve fibres located
caudal to the rostrum of the corpus callosum but rostral to the main part of the fornix.
Here it is embedded in the lamina terminalis where it is part of the anterior wall of the
third ventricle (Fig. 19.5).
In sagittal suction it is oval. Its long (vertical) diameter is approximately 1.5 mm.
Laterally it splits into anterior and posterior bundles. The smaller anterior bundle curves
forwards on each side to the anterior perforated substance and olfactory tract. The
posterior bundle curves posterolaterally on each side in a deep grove on the anteroinferior
aspect of the lentiform complex, and subsequently fans out into the anterior part of the
1484
19. White matter of cerebral hemisphere
temporal lobe, including the parahippocampal gyrus (Crossman, 2008). It connects the
right and left amygdala, the olfactory bulb and anterior olfactory nucleus, the anterior
perforated substance, olfactory tubercle and diagonal band of Broca; the prepyriform
cortex; the entorhinal area and adjacent parts of the parahippocampal gyrus; part of the
amygdoid complex (especially the nucleus of the lateral olfactory stria); the bed nucleus
of the stria terminalis and the nucleus accumbens; the anterior regions of the middle and
inferior temporal gyri.
Thus, anterior commissure is a relatively small one in the basal forebrain lying above
the optic chiasma and anterior to the main columns of the fornix that connects
homologous areas of the middle and inferior temporal gyri, including parts of the
olfactory cortices (Mendoza, 2011).
Posterior commissure
The posterior commissure which is of unknown constitution in man is a small
fasciculus that crosses the midline at the base of the pineal gland and just posterior
(dorsal) to the cerebral aqueduct.
Various small nuclei are associated with it. Among these are the interstitial nuclei
of the posterior commissure, the nucleus of Darkschewitscz in the periaqueductal grey
matter, and the interstitial nucleus of Cajal near the upper end of the oculomotor complex,
closely linked with the medial longitudinal fasciculus. Fibres from all these nuclei and
the fasciculus cross in the posterior commissure. It also contains fibres from thalamic
and pretectal nuclei and the superior colliculi, together with fibres that connect the tectal
and habenular nuclei. The destinations and functions of many of these fibres are obscure
(Crossman, 2008).
Thus, the posterior commissure connects the right and left pretectal region and
related cells group of the mesencephalon.
1485
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The habenular commissure which is a small fascicle running along the upper aspect
of the posterior commissure, interconnected the habenular nuclei. However, the pretectal
area and pretectal nuclei are parts of the epithalamus, that are three fibre bundles: the
posterior commissure, habenular commissure, and the habenulo-interpeduncular tract.
Hippocampal commissure
The hippocampal commissure (commissure of the fornix) is formed by fibres that
originate in the hippocampal formation and cross the midline as a thin layer inferior to
the splenium of the corpus callosum. Thus, this commissure joins the right and left
hippocampi to one another.
Corpus callosum
The corpus callosum is the most massive commissure in the entire nervous system.
It is the largest axonal tract of the adult brain that provides symmetrical connections
between the two hemispheres. It provides a bridge for the passing of information from
one cerebral hemisphere to the other by 200-300 million myelinated and unmyelinated
axons (Dupree, 2011).
Although the corpus callosum is the largest white matter tract of the brain, it is not
essential for life.
The corpus callosum begins development around the 11th week of gestation and
continues through adolescence. Initially, the corpus callosum is composed of astrocytic
processes which serve as conduits for growing axons extending to the contralateral
hemisphere (Paul et al., 2007).
Corpus callosum is composed of four parts: the rostrum, the genu, the body (also
known as the trunk), and the splenium. Each portion of the corpus callosum is responsible
for connecting symmetrical regions of the two hemispheres, with the rostrum and genu
connecting portions of the prefrontal and premotor cortices, the body interconnecting the
premotor, motor, supplementary motor, and the posterior parietal cortices, while the
splenium connects portions of the temporal, occipital, and parietal lobes.
Fig. 19.6. Schematic diagram of the right internal capsule as seen in a horizontal section (after Carpenter, 1979).
Although the radiations connect with practically all parts of the cortex, the richness
of connections varies considerably for specific cortical areas. Most abundant are the
projections of the frontal granular cortex, the precentral and postcentral gyri, the calcarine
area and the gyrus of Heschl. The posterior parietal region and adjacent portions of the
temporal lobe also have rich thalamic connections, but relatively scanty radiations go to
other cortical areas (Walker, 1938).
The thalamic radiation are grouped into four subradiations designated as the thalamic
peduncles.
The anterior or frontal peduncle connects the frontal lobe with the medial and
anterior thalamic nuclei.
The superior or centroparietal peduncle connects the Rolandic area and adjacent
portion of the frontal and parietal lobes with the ventral tier thalamic nuclei. The fibres,
1487
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
carrying general sensory impulses from the body and head, form part of this radiation
and terminate in the postcentral gyrus.
The posterior or occipital peduncle connects the occipital and posterior parietal
convolutions with the caudal portions of the thalamus. It included the optic radiations
(geniculocalcarine) from the lateral geniculate body to the calcarine cortex (striate area).
The inferior or temporal peduncle is small and includes the scanty connections of
the thalamus with the temporal lobe and the insula. Includes in this are the auditory
radiations (geniculotemporal) from the medial geniculate body to the transverse temporal
gyrus of Heschl (Carpenter, 1979).
Thalamocortical and corticofugal fibres within the internal capsule occupy a
comparatively small, compact area.
The cerebral hemisphere is connected with the brain stem and spinal cord by the
extensive projection system of the internal capsule. Thus, descending motor fibres arising
from the motor cortex destined for the brain stem and the spinal cord also descend in the
internal capsule.
Fibres of the internal capsule flare out into the hemisphere as they pass distal to the
caudate and putamen. This abrupt divergence of internal capsule fibres forms the corona
radiata (“radiating crown”), which contains converging corticofugal fibres, as well as
diverging corticopetal fibres (Haines, 2006). Thus, fibres arise from the whole extent of
the cortex, enter the while substance of the hemisphere and appear as a radiating mass of
fibres, the corona radiata, which converges toward the brain stem (Fig. 19.7). On
reaching the latter they form a broad compact fibre band, the internal capsule, flanked
medially by the thalamus and caudate nucleus and the lateral by the lenticular nucleus.
Thus, the internal capsule is composed of all the fibres, afferent and efferent, which
go to, come from, the cerebral cortex. A large part of the capsule is obviously composed
of the thalamic radiation described above. The rest is composed mainly of corticofugal
fibre systems (efferent cortical fibres) which descend to the lower portion of the brain
stem and the spinal cord.
In an axial plane through the hemisphere the internal capsule can be divided into an
anterior limb, a genu (L “knee”) and a posterior limb. The two limbs converge toward
the genu, the angle between the anterior and posterior limb.
The anterior limb is interposed between the head of the caudate nucleus and the
lenticular nucleus. This anterior limb contains the anterior thalamic radiation (thalamocortical
/ corticothalamic fibres) or peduncle, and the prefrontal corticopontine tract.
These frontopontine fibres, which arise from the cortex in the frontal lobe, synapse with
cells of the pontine nuclei. Axons of these cells enter the opposite cerebellar hemisphere
through the middle cerebellar peduncle. Anterior thalamic radiations interconnect the medial
and anterior thalamic nuclei and limbic structures with the frontal cortex.
The genu, located at the intersection of the anterior and posterior limbs, is situated
approximately at the level of the interventricular foramen. The genu abuts the third ventricle
medially and the globus pallidus forms its lateral border. The genu of internal capsule
contains corticonuclear (corticobulbar) fibres that arise in the frontal cortex just rostral to
1488
19. White matter of cerebral hemisphere
precentral sulcus (area 4), form the precentral gyrus (primary motor cortex) and terminate
mostly in the contralateral motor nuclei of cranial nerves. Anterior fibres of the superior
thalamic radiation, between the thalamus and cortex, also extend into the genu.
Lesions of these fibres give rise to motor deficits of cranial nerves, most notably
deficits related to the facial and hypoglossal nerves (Haines, 2006).
The posterior limb of the internal capsule is larger, more complex and includes the
corticospinal tract. The fibres concerned with the upper limb are anterior. More posterior
regions contain fibres representing the trunk and lower limbs.
Other descending axons include frontopontine fibres, particularly from areas 4 and
6 and corticorubral fibres, which connect the frontal lobe to the red nucleus. Most of the
posterior limb also contains fibres of the superior thalamic radiation (the somaesthetic
radiation) ascending to the postcentral gyrus (Crossman, 2008).
This posterior limb is divided into a thalamolenticular part (which lies between
the lenticular nucleus and the dorsal thalamus), a sublenticular part (fibres passing
ventral to the lenticular nucleus) and a retrolenticular part (fibres extend caudally for
a short distance behind the lenticular nucleus). In this caudal region a number of fibres
passing beneath the lenticular nucleus to reach the temporal lobe collectively form the
sublenticular portion of internal capsule.
Fig. 19.7. Diagram of the blood supply of the internal capsule and corpus striatum. The putamen and globus pallidus
are shown rotated ventrally away from their normal position adjacent to the internal capsule. Regions supplied by
branches of the middle and anterior cerebral arteries are shown in black and portions of the internal capsule
and corpus striatum supplied by the anterior choroidal artery. Direct branches of the internal carotid artery supply
the genu of internal capsule (Alexander, 1942).
1489
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The anterior limb of the internal capsule contains axons to and from frontal
association cortex (Martin, 2003; Fix, 2008; De Piero, 2011).
Pathology. The posterior limb of the internal capsule is a frequent site for lacunar
strokes. The most common syndrome is that of pure motor hemiplegia. The lenticulo-
striate arteries, penetrating branches of the middle cerebral artery, are frequently damaged
by hypertension, diabetes, and aging, and can be occluded, causing strokes.
Less commonly small aneurysms (Charcot-Bouchard aneurysms) may develop, and
cause intracerebral hemorrhage, sometimes referred to as intraparenchymal hemorrhage
(De Piero, 2011).
Thrombosis or hemorrhage of the anterior choroidal, striate or capsular branches of
the middle cerebral arteries are responsible for most injuries to the internal capsule.
Vascular lesions in the posterior limb of the internal capsule may result in
contralateral hemianesthesia of the head, trunk, and limbs due to injury of thalamocortical
fibres en route to the sensory cortex. There is also a contralateral hemiplegia due to injury
of the corticospinal tract (Aitken, 1909; Alexander, 1942).
If the genu of the internal capsule is involved in the injury, corticobulbar fibres are
also destroyed. Lesions in the posterior third of the posterior limb may include the optic
and auditory radiations.
In such instances there may be a contralateral triad consisting of hemianesthesia,
hemianopsia and hemihypoacusia. More extensive vascular lesions may include the
thalamus or corpus striatum, so that affective changes and symptoms due to injury of the
basal ganglia may be added to those characteristic of injury to the internal capsule
(Alexander, 1942; Dănăilă and Păiş, 1988, 2004). Thus, lesions in this area produce more
widespread disability than lesions in any other region of the nervous system.
Lesions in cerebral white matter sever connections between lower and higher centers
or between cortical areas. White matter lesions are found in many dementing disorders
and appear to be specifically associated with attentional impairments (Junkué et al., 1990;
Filley, 2001).
1491
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
REFERENCES
Aitken HF, A report on the circulation of the lobar ganglia: made to Dr. James B. Ayer. Boston
Med Surg J Suppl 160; 25, 1909.
Alexander L, The vascular supply of the striopallidum. A Res Nerv & Ment Dis Proc 21; 77-132,
1942.
Augustine JR, Human Neuroanatomy. An Introduction. Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston,
Heidelberg etc.; 316-318, 2008.
Carpenter MB, Human Neuroanatomy. Seventh Edition. The Williams and Wilkins Company,
Baltimore; 162-165, 1979.
Crossman AR, Basal ganglia. In: S Standring (ed.), Gray’s Anatomy. Fortieth Edition. Churchill
Livingstone, Elsevier; 354-359, 2008.
Dănăilă L, Păiş V, Ateroscleroza cerebrală din sistemul carotidian, ed. Ştiintifică şi enciclopedică,
Bucureşti, 1988.
Dănăilă L, Păiş V, Ateroscleroza cerebrală ischemică. editura: editura Medicala, Bucureşti, 2004.
De Piero TJ, Internal Capsule. In: JS Kreutzer, J De Luca, B Caplan (eds) Encyclopedia of Clinical
Neuropsychology. Vol. 2, Springer, New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London; 1343, 2011.
Dupree J, Corpus Callosum. In: JS Kreutzer, J De Luca, B Caplan (eds.), Encyclopedia of Clinical
Neuropsychology. Vol. 1, Springer , New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London; 709, 2011.
Filley CM, The Behavioural Neurology of White Matter. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2001.
Fix JD, Neuroanatomy (3rd ed.), Baltimore, MD: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins, 2008.
Haines DE, Fundamental neuroscience for basic and clinical applications. 3rd ed, Philadelphia,
PA: Churchill Livingstone, Elsevier; 2006.
Haines DE, Mihailoff GA, The telencephalon. In: DE Haines (ed.), Fundamental neuroscience
for basic and clinical applications. Philadelphia. Churchill-Livingstone-Elsevir; 244-259,
2006.
Junqué, C, Pujol, J, Vendrell P, et al., Leuko-araiosis on magnetic resonance imaging and speed
of mental processing. Arch Neurol 47; 151-156, 1990.
Mendoza JE, Anterior Commissure. In: JS Kreutzer, J De Luca, B Caplan (eds) Encyclopedia of
Clinical Neuropsychology. Vol. 1, Springer, New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, London;
187, 2011.
Martin J, Neuroanatomy: Text and atlas (3rd ed). New York, Mc Graw-Hill, 2003.
Patestas AM, Gartner LP, A Textbook of Neuroanatomy. Blackwell Publishing, USA, 2009.
Paul LK, Brown WS, Adolphs R, et al., Agenesis of the corpus callosum: genetic, developmental
and functional aspects of connectivity. Nature Review Neuroscience 8; 287-299, 2007.
Walker AE, The thalamus of chimpanzee. IV. Thalamic projection of the cerebral cortex. J Anat
73; 37-93, 1938.
1492
CORPUS CALLOSUM
The corpus callosum has fibres that project between the hemispheres in an orderly
way, with regions in the anterior portion connecting similar brain areas in the frontal lobes
and regions in the posterior portion connecting similar brain areas in the occipital lobe.
A variety of individual differences in callosal size and pattern are suggested to exist.
For example, Witelson (1986) reported that the corpus callosum is larger in left-handers
than in right-handers and in the women than in the man.
Thus, the size and the shape of the corpus callosum vary greatly from individual to
individual, and there are sex differences in the shape of the callosum. The splenium is
more bulbous shaped in women and has a greater maximum width but it is more tubular
shaped in men. There are age related changes in the corpus callosum during childhood
and adulthood. In some cases, the corpus callosum is lacking, a condition termed agenesis
of the corpus callosum.
The corpus callosum originates from the area of the lamina terminalis as a structure
initially composed of astrocytic processes. Axons from developing neurons in each
hemisphere traverse this glial structure across to the contralateral side. As this takes place
the corpus callosum enlarges in a caudal direction to form the prominent structure found
in adults.
The corpus callosum is located superior to the diencephalon and forms the roof of
much of the lateral ventricles, and the floor of the longitudinal fissure.
Its antero-posterior length is of 96 mm in men and 98 in women when measured
through by projecting the farthest anterior and posterior points on a horizontal line (Velut
et al., 1998). The anterior end is located 4 cm away from the frontal pole, and the posterior
end is found at 6 cm from the occipital pole. However, the corpus callosum is organised
with a great deal of regularity (Brodal, 1981; Colonier, 1986; Witelson, 1995; Clarke et
al., 1998)
This huge bundle of commissural fibres, the corpus callosum, consists, from rostral
to caudal, of five segments: the anteriormost region, the rostrum, the curved genu, the
relatively flattened body (also called the trunk) forming the roof of the lateral ventricle,
the isthmus, and its posteriormost region, the splenium (Fig. 20.1).
The rostrum represents the most anterior part and it forms a curve downwards. It
continues towards the posterior with a lamina of white matter of various widths. This
horizontal lamina descends in a beak-like shape that reaches the superior margin of the
white anterior commissure.
The peaked posterior extremity of the rostrum is located between the subcallous area
downward and the septum pellucidum upwards. Consequently, the posterior elongation of
this white lamina separated, from the subcallous area by the posterior paraolfactory sulcus,
locates itself above the paraterminal gyrus which covers the septal nuclei (Velut et al., 1998).
The genu of the corpus callosum which presents a significant anterior convexity is
situated anterior to the rostrum. Thus, the anterior extent of the corpus callosum, known
as the genu, bends inferiorly and turns posteriorly where it forms a slender connection,
the rostrum, with the anterior commissure.
Inferior to the genu and rostrum is the subcallosal area – a collective term including
the subcallosal gyrus and, inferior to it, the septal area. The term, rhinencephalon (Greek,
nose brain) refers to cerebral structures related to olfaction, particularly in lower animals.
In humans, this area of the brain consists of many structures on the medial and inferior
surfaces of each cerebral hemisphere.
One, the anterior perforated substance, is posterior to the olfactory trigone and best
visualised on the inferior surface of the brain.
Another, the subcallosal area, is on the medial surface of each cerebral hemisphere
inferior to the genu and rostrum of the corpus callosum. It includes a dorsally located
subcallosal gyrus and a ventrally located septal area. As the cingulate gyrus turns into
the subcallosal area, it is continued with the subcallosal gyrus that is inferior to the
rostrum of the corpus callosum.
The virtual separation of the genu from the rostrum and body consists of a vertical
line that crosses through the most posterior area of the genu. The genu is located between
the septum pellucidum, which is inserted in a posterior medial line and the gyrus cinguli
which is located anteriorly.
The insertion of the septum pellucidum is frequently doubled (in 83% of cases
according to Winkler et al., 1997) resulting in a double septum and septal cavity (Fig. 20.2).
The septum pellucidum is bilaminar with a potential space between its layers (cavum
septi pellucidi) that is normally absent at birth but may remain open as a median cleft.
There is an enormous variability in the reported incidence of cavum septi pellucidi (from
2% to 71.5%).
Enlargement of the cavum, though rare in healthy individuals, has been reported in
patients with schizophrenia (Augustine, 2008).
Many of the fibres passing through the rostrum and genu arch rostrally to
interconnect the anterior cortical areas of the frontal lobes; these form the minor (or
frontal) forceps.
The body (trunk) of the corpus callosum is continued behind the genu and consists
of most of the commissural fibres that interconnect the remaining frontal lobe, parietal
lobe, and part of the temporal lobe. Thus, midcallosal areas contain a mixture of fibres
coming from both anterior and posterior regions.
1495
1496
Fig. 20.2. Diagram of the medial view of a sagital section of the brain.
20. Corpus callosum
For Winkler et al. (1997) the distance between the M line (the line that unites the
point located 5 cm anterior from the central sulcus with the superior margin of the
interventricular foramen) and the most anterior point of the adhesion of the corpus
callosum to the fornix varies between 10 and 40 mm (the average is 23.79 mm). One
must also take notice of the fact that the antero-posterior length of this adhesion is highly
variable. Its maximum is greater than 10 mm.
Finally, Winkler et al. (1997) highlights the fact that the separation of the corpus
callosum from the fornix is very easy in 77% of cases and difficult or impossible in the
remaining cases. These variations highlight the importance on an MRI scan before every
callosotomy.
The splenium is the region located in the most posterior area of the corpus callosum,
and closes the “callous C“. The fibres interconnecting the occipital lobes, loop through
the splenium of the corpus callosum forming the major (or occipital) forceps. Fibres from
the visual cortex at the posterior pole of the cerebrum occupy the posterior end portion
of the callosum.
The tapetum, which is located in the lateral wall of the atrium and posterior horn of
the lateral ventricle, is also composed of fibre bundles that cross in the splenium and
separate the ventricle from the optic radiation.
Thus, the posterior extent of the corpus callosum, known as the splenium, is bullous
in shape. This portion of the corpus callosum consists of commissural fibres
interconnecting the posterior parietal, posterior temporal and occipital cortices. Lesions
in this area are associated with the well-known neurobehavioral disconnection syndrome
of alexia without agraphia.
Contrary to the rest of the corpus callosum that is hidden between the two
hemispheres, the splenium emerges in the ambient cistern or the superior cerebellar lake
surrounded in its posterior region by the falco-tentorial junction. The splenium forms a
thicker area which is the superior limit of this cistern through its inferior convexity.
The anterior limit is given by the medial pineal body and the lateral pulvinar, and
downwards and forwards there is the mesencephalic tectum. Downwards, the cistern is
limited by the culmen of the vermis, and in its posterior area it is limited by the free
margin of the cerebellar tentorium, which curves upwards and internally in order to bond
with the free margin of the cerebral falx. This falco-tentorial junction forms the original
triangle of the right sinus. At this level the width of the cingular gyrus, which moulds on
the posterior region of the splenium, decreases and forms the isthmus. This isthmus forms
away from the median line and is continued interiorly by the parahippocampal gyrus.
The subsplenial gyrus, which is between the isthmus and the splenium, is continued
with the fasciolar gyrus. Above and ahead of the splenial formation, there is the body of
the fornix which contains in its posterior area the interhyppocampal fibres of its
commissure. It is called the psalterium. In the lateral area, each lateral margin of this
body is continued by the fornix cross which becomes the fimbria that roles around the
pulvinar area.
Then, the fimbria emerges above the fasciolar gyrus and away from the splenium,
which remains above these structures.
1498
20. Corpus callosum
In sum, in humans, the different regions of the corpus callosum described on the
medial sagittal section are: the rostrum, the genu, the body or the trunk, the isthmus and
the splenium.
Callosal fibres radiating to the frontal lobe (radiations of the corpus callosum) form
the genu from the frontal forceps; those radiating from the splenium in the occipital lobe
form the occipital forceps. Attached to the inferior border of the corpus callosum is the
thin septum pellucidum – a transparent, membranous partition that separates the right
and left cerebral ventricles and forms their medial wall.
Fig. 20.3. (A) Horizontal schematic representation of the brain showing the fibre connections of the genu and
splenium of the corpus callosum. (B) Coronal schematic representation of the brain showing the fibres of the anterior
comissure and the body of the corpus callosum. (Modified from Young PA and Young PH, 1997.
Basic Clinical Neuroanatomy. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore).
1500
20. Corpus callosum
Fig. 20.4. Patterns of commissural connections. (A) The areas shaded show regions of the cortex of a rhesus monkey
that receive projection from the contralateral hemisphere through the corpus callosum.
(B) Regions of the corpus callosum showing zones through which a radioactive label was transported
after injections into specific locations in the cortex (after Pandya and Seltzer, 1986).
The fibres of the anterior half of the callous body connect to the premotor
dorsolateral cortex and the median supplementary motor area. The middle area of the
body connects to the motor area M1, the insula and the anterior cingulate cortex (Pandya
and Seltzer, 1986; Habib and Pelletier, 1994).
More towards its posterior region, the callous areas connect to the somatosensory
areas S1 and S2. The posterior area of the body connects to the associative parietal cortex,
and more towards the posterior it connects to the superior temporal areas (including the
hearing area), the insula and the posterior cingulum. The areas M1, S1 and S2 related to
the foot and hand are not connected. Thus, the motor and sensory areas for distal parts of
the limbs (mainly the hands and feet) lack connections. It could be argued that, because
their essential function is to work independently of one another, connections are not
necessary. Among the areas that do receive interhemispheric connections, the density of
projections is not homogeneous (Fig. 20.4 A) (Pandya and Seltzer, 1986).
Areas of the cortex that represent the midline of the body – such as the central
meridian of the visual fields, auditory fields, and trunk of the body on the somatosensory
and motor cortex – have the densest connections. The functional utility of this
arrangement is that movements of the body or actions in central space require
interhemispheric cooperation.
Thus, the corpus callosum bonds together the representations of the midpoints of
the body and space that are divided by the longitudinal fissure. The corpus callosum
1501
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
connects identical points in the contralateral hemisphere. One group of projections goes
to areas to which the homotopic areas on the contralateral side project. Another group of
projections has a diffuse terminal distribution. The pattern of connections between the
hemisphere in the rhesus monkey is illustrated in figure 20.4B (Pandya and Seltzer, 1986).
Thus the fibres passing through the body of the corpus callosum are proceeding from
front to back, from the premotor, motor, somatosensory, and posterior parietal cortex.
Many studies proved that the transfer of somato-sensitive information needs to
integrate the trunk of the corpus callosum (Diamond et al., 1977; Bogen, 1987). In
particular, the fibres that pass through the posterior area of the trunk are crucial to
finishing the tasks that need the crisscross location of a tactile stimulus. Geffen et al.
(1985) showed that if the trunk of the corpus callosum is the regular path for the sensitive
information, the splenium can maintain up to 50% of this function.
The fibres of the splenium form a posterior concavity that is larger than the one of
the genu which is called the major forceps (Young and Young, 1997).
Outside and at a distance from the median line, the splenium is arched by the isthmus
of the cingulate gyrus. The fibres from the superior area of the splenium connect to the
inferior temporal cortex and the parahippocampal gyrus. In its anterior area (and the
superior one) the fibres connect to the superior parietal lobules and the juxtastriate region
(Pandya and Seltzer, 1986; Habib and Pelletier, 1994) (Fig. 20.5).
The dissection of the fibres located in the splenium really demonstrates that the
inferior fibres that are located in the splenial area go towards the striate regions and form
the major forceps. Areas 17 are not connected. The more these fibres are located in the
posterior region of the splenium, the closer they are to the median line of the occipital
lobe. So, fibres in the posterior part, or splenium, are from the superior temporal, inferior
temporal, and visual cortex (Pandya and Seltzer, 1986).
The superior splenial fibres go towards the occipito-temporal and temporal regions
(Velut et al., 1998). This location of the special fibres forms the tapetum.
Initially, the fibres are located on the roof of the ventricular atrium. Afterwards they
form into a fan like shape from back to front, and spread in an anterior direction below
the lateral ependymal area of the atrium and the inferior horn of the lateral ventricle.
Thus, the tapetum is located between this ependyma and the optic radiations.
In order to reveal the optic radiations at the level of the roof of the inferior horn in
the lateral ventricle it is necessary to perform the ablation of the fibres located in the
tapetum. Finally, the fibres located mostly towards the anterior area of the anterior
tapetum are next to the tail of the caudate nucleus, which is located above the stria
terminalis. Thus, according to Pandya and Seltzer (1986), most of the primary visual
cortex (area V1) is devoid of interhemispheric connections, except for that part
representing the midline of the visual world, the visual meridian. The lack of such
connections has been explained in functional terms: this cortex represents the visual world
topographically, and there is no need for one half of the representation to be connected
to one other.
1502
20. Corpus callosum
Fig. 20.5. Functions associated with the dominant and nondominant cerebral hemispheres. (Modified from Noback,
CR et al. (1996), The Human Nervous System, 5th edn. Williams and Willkins, Baltimore).
Klingler’s method allows the dissection of fibres in each of these areas and the
establishment of their connections to the corona radiata and the optic radiations.
Finally each segment of the corpus callosum takes part in forming the walls of the
lateral ventricle.
Despite the size and the large number of commissural fibres, the information
regarding the functional role of the corpus callosum is limited. In humans the total number
of fibres is approximately 200 million. However, there is detailed data missing regarding
1503
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the callous connections of many regions. The first investigations have been done in the
visual area. The somatic and sensitive areas are connected to the contralateral ones, but
the right and left areas related to the hand and foot lack commissural callous
connections (Powell, 1981).
It is not well defined if there are callous connections between the visual cortex and
the two hemispheres. They exist only to represent the vertical meridian (or visual area)
of the retina. It can be claimed that the connected cortical areas are in relation with a
medium retinal region which is very narrow and from which the ganglion cells send axons
that spread and send collaterals in the optic tract on both sides. The commissural bonds
are specific because they gather together cortical columnar units in a bilateral function.
Fig. 20.6. A top view of the two hemispheres. Schematic drawing of the two halves of the cerebral cortex,
showing some major functions of the right and left hemispheres. Note the massive bridge of the corpus callosum
connecting the two sides. The eyes on top focus on converging lines in order to enable stereoscopic depth
perception. (Standring, 2005).
Following the section of the optic chiasma and corpus callosum, cats trained with one
eye masked were unable to remember simple visual discriminations learned with the first eye.
The untrained eye could be trained to make reverse-type discrimination without
interfering with the patterned discrimination learned on the opposite side. This functional
independence of the surgically separated cerebral hemispheres with respect to learning,
memory and other gnostic activities has been the stimulus for considerable investigations
(Mountcastle, 1962).
Thus, interhemispheric connections function to allow each hemisphere to work as a
coordinated unit.
Callosal disconnections of either inter- or intrahemispheric connections can produce
a variety of neurological syndromes including apraxia, aphasia, agnosia, alexia, and
unilateral agraphia.
The most systematic investigation of hemispheric specialisation and hemispheric
integration in cases of callosal disconnection has been carried out with patients who have
undergone callosotomy in order to control their intractable epilepsy.
1505
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the command. Thus, although the subject would comprehend the command, the left hand
would be unable to obey it. Necessarily then, callosal interruption would result in an
inability to follow verbal commands with the left hand although there would be no loss
of comprehension (as expected from a left hemisphere lesion) and there would be no
weakness or incoordination of the left hand (as expected from a right hemisphere lesion).
We now recognise the notion that spatial or pictorial instructions understood by the right
hemisphere may require a callosally mediated interhemispheric communication for
correct left hand execution.
In humans, the highlighting of many callous related symptoms was done in patients
with tumors, degenerative disease, partial vascular lesions and agenesis of the corpus
callosum.
The study of epileptic patients, where the ablation of a commissure (anterior,
posterior, hippocampal) or a callosotomy (sectioning of corpus callosum) had been
performed as therapy, allowed the description of the main components of the callous
disconnection syndrome.
One should keep in mind that callosal lesions, surgical as well as naturally occurring,
are often accompanied by damage to or pressure upon neighbouring structures, resulting
in several distinct types of signs: (a) signs of hemisphere disconnection, (b) neigh-
bourhood signs, and (c) nonlocalising signs such as meningismus or signs of increased
intracranial pressure. Here, attention is given mainly to signs of the first type.
Thus, the earliest clinical report of a surgical division of commissural pathways to
prevent the spread of an epileptic attack was made by Van Wagenen and Herren in 1940,
coincident with Erickson’s report of the experimental observations in primates of seizure
spread via the corpus callosum (Erickson, 1940). Van Wagenen and Herren (1940) were
inspired to perform the surgery on patients with severe intractable epilepsy after observing
that one of their epileptic patients experienced considerable relief after developing a tumor
in his corpus callosum. Epileptic seizures are caused by abnormal electrical discharges
that reverberate across the brain from one hemisphere to the other. Severing all or part
of the corpus callosum can reduce seizure activity by 60-70% in 80% of the patients. Van
Wagenen and his colleagues feared that one of the side effects of the surgery would be
that it might produce a split personality, or a dual consciousness.
However, after 20 of the surgeries were conducted in the 1940’s, researchers like
Akelaitis never found psychological side effects (Akelaitis, 1941, 1945). The surgery did
not appear to create two minds, each with its own personality and consciousness, fighting
over the control of the body. The absence of a dual consciousness continued to be
observed when the surgery was revived by Bogen, in the early 1960’s, and Sperry.
In 1965, Bogen advocated this approach as a surgical treatment for epilepsy, and
further reports appeared in the 1970’s (Luessenhop, 1970; Wilson et al., 1975). However,
despite the phenomenal academic interest generated by the function of the corpus
callosum and lateralised hemispheric function in humans, this therapeutic approach did
not arouse much clinical interest until several reports appeared in the mid-1980’s. The
procedure was medically beneficial, leaving some patients virtually seizure-free after-
wards, with minimal effects on their everyday behavior.
1507
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Mitchel (1987) found that human infants younger than about 1 year are like split-brain
patients in that they are unable to transfer information about objects obtained by touch.
Rudy and Stadler-Morris (1987) found that rats trained on spatial-navigation tasks
learned the task with one hemisphere at 22 days of age but could not learn it with the
other.
By the time they were 25 days old, they did display interocular equivalence. The
researchers suggest that the 22-day-old rat behaves like a split-brain animal. After the
end of myelinisation no transfer can be performed. In cats, the period of myelinisation in
callous fibres begins around the age of 28 days (Elberger, 1982), and in humans it extends
until the end of the tenth year of existence (Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967).
Due to this reason, a callosotomy performed before the age of 10 should remain
almost asymptomatic.
Transfer tests in children with a callosotomy performed before the age of ten,
revealed minor or no deficit (Geoffroy et al., 1983, 1986; Lassonde et al., 1986). The
reaction time, much longer than in normal persons, confirms the limits of the
compensation mechanisms in these subjects (Sauerwein and Lassonde, 1983; Lassonde
et al., 1991, 1996).
A commissurotomy performed between age ten and fifteen leads to not very severe
signs of callous disconnection (ideomotor apraxia, unilateral anomia, etc.), similar to the
ones in adults. It is possible that this mildness is based on certain forms of plasticity that
determines a cerebral reorganisation. Its quality and efficacy depends on how early the
lesion was produced (Lassonde et al., 1996).
Related to different aspects of the interhemispheric disconnection syndrome in
children, youth and adults, Ramackers and Nijokiktjien (1991) released theirs concept of
development.
Young children may be considered subjects with a physiological split-brain. In the
first decade, the transfer capacity of tactile information is limited, but it increases with
age (Quinn and Geffen, 1986).
The fact that the physiological disconnection syndrome disappeared is an ontogenetic
phenomenon (Galin et al., 1977) that corresponds to a degree of myelinisation (Ptito and
Lepore, 1983).
Thus, some authors stress about the hypothesis of a dysfunction in the corpus
callosum in some dyslexic subjects (Ramackers and Nijokiktjien, 1991). This
phenomenon of plasticity in subjects with no corpus callosum or subjects, who underwent
an early callosotomy, was related to the critical period of development that coincides
with synaptic hyperproduction (Hommet and Billard, 1998).
However, the precise relation between the callous malformation and the
interhemispheric dysfunction is suspected but not yet proved in the development
pathology.
We performed callosotomies (Fig. 20.7) for intractable seizures in four patients.
They were 17, 19, 25 and 27 year-old. After surgery we obtained remission of 8-43
months and a decrease in the number and severity of the epileptic seizures. The routine
1509
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
A B
Fig. 20.7. Postoperative CT-scan shows a partial disconnection (A + B) in a patient with contractable seizures.
Control clips have been fixed in order to see the location and extension of the colostomy.
The postoperative results were very good.
neurological examination did not record any significant change in walking, orthostatism,
cutaneous and myotatic reflexes, cranial nerves, sight, hearing and smell. The extraocular
movements of the face, tongue, mandible and those of each limb were normal. However,
the coordination of the hands was affected if a task that needed both hands was performed
without seeing.
When studied under more stringent experiment condition where sensory input was
limited to one hemisphere, the consequences of callosal disconnection readily became
apparent. Thus, when deprived of visual feedback, the commissurotomised participant
was unable to name an object held in the left hand because the left (naming) hemisphere
no longer had access to information in the right hemisphere. For the same reason, when
deprived of visual input, one hand was unable to select an object held by the other. While
the left hand was unable to write an intelligible sentence, it may surpass the efforts of the
right in copying a complex geometric design. If emotionally charged images are restricted
to the left visual field, a split-brain patient may evidence an appropriate affective
response; however, because the speaking left hemisphere is disconnected from the
hemisphere that saw the image, he or she may be unable to explain their reaction.
On rare occasions, patients have been dismayed that one hand (usually the left) might
be carrying out some activity of which the left (speaking hemisphere) is unaware or find
that one hand is working in a counter-productive fashion to the other (e.g., one hand
pulling pants up and the other pushing them down) (Mendoza, 2011).
However, according to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), for dualists who hold that the
brain has a separate corresponding mental representation (the mind), these are compelling
reasons to consider that a split-brain person possesses two brains and two minds. For
materialists, who hold that behavior is explained as a function of the nervous system,
without recourse to mind, the philosophical implications are not so weighty. But for
1510
20. Corpus callosum
Thus, the complete section of the corpus callosum, which has come to be used
increasingly as a treatment for medically intractable epilepsy (Reeves, 1985) is regularly
followed by a wide variety of neurological and neuropsychological deficits. The deficits
that are most apparent immediately after operation make up the acute disconnection
syndrome (Wilson et al., 1977). Those deficits that persist make up the stabilised
syndrome of hemisphere disconnection (Sperry et al., 1969; Bogen, 1978; 1985 a, b).
A partial section of the corpus callosum may produce a fraction of the full syndrome,
depending upon the part of the corpus callosum that has been cut as well as the amount
and nature of any associated extracallosal damage.
In general, most of the disconnection deficits, both acute and chronic, are not present
as long as the splenium is spared (Apuzzo et al., 1982; Gordon et al., 1971; Dănăilă and
Golu, 2002). But certain partial sections do entail a few inevitable deficits, as well as
some common complications, whose recognition is important for the surgeon utilising
transcallosal approaches.
Sperry, Gazzaniga and others have extensively studied the effects of hemispheric
disconnection on behaviours related to both motor and sensory systems.
The patients with divided brain (split-brain) operated by Akelaitis (1943 and 1944)
and Bogen and Vogel (1962) for epileptic seizures, have been examined by Sperry et al.
(1969) and Sperry (1986). Thus, with the exception of several transitory postoperatory
phenomena observed just in some of the patients (mutism, left hemiapraxia, etc.), the
respective patients did not seen to stand out in any way compared to the ones who did
not undergo surgery. However, when these patients went through adequate psychological
tests, they showed some particular signs and symptoms.
Afterwards, they have been grouped under the name “callous disconnection
syndrome“ (Bogen and Vogel, 1962; Bogen and Gazzaniga, 1965; Gazzaniga, 1970;
Hécaen and Assal, 1973; LeDoux and Gazzaniga, 1978; Bogen, 1985; Sperry, 1986).
The signs of callous disconnection are based upon the principle of a specialised left
hemisphere involved in the verbal behavior and a right hemisphere involved in spatial
and visual-perceptive abilities.
The communication between the two hemispheres is based upon the registration of
sensorial or motor information coming from the callous commissure in each of them. Or,
in some cases, it is based upon the bilateral transport of information.
Thus, signs of disconnection may be observed when the callous commissure is
sectioned, and the motor or sensorial information is limited to a single hemisphere
(Lassonde et al., 1996).
There are four conditions in which the hemispheres become completely or partial
separated.
First, in humans, the interhemispheric fibres are sometimes cut as a therapy for
intractable epilepsy.
Second, people are born with congenitally reduced or completely missing
interhemispheric connections.
Third, in animals, disconnections are performed to trace functional systems, to model
human conditions and to answer basic questions about interhemispheric development.
Four, a stroke involving the anterior and / or posterior cerebral artery.
1512
20. Corpus callosum
Sperry (1974) and Sperry et al. (1979) have found that patients with partial
disconnection are significantly better at motor tasks such as those needed to use the
Etch-a-Sketch.
The results of research on monkeys with partial commissurotomies suggest that the
posterior part of the corpus callosum (splenium) subserves visual transfer, whereas the region
just in front of the splenium affects somatosensory transfer (Pandya and Seltzer, 1986).
Van Gijn, 1977). In any event, the Babinski signs rapidly subside bilaterally and are not
present over a long term (Botez and Bogen, 1976).
As the severity of the acute disconnection syndrome subsides in a week or so, a
phenomenon variously called “intermanual conflict” or “the alien hand” appears (Bogen,
1987). Almost all complete commissurotomy patients manifest some degree of
intermanual conflict during the early postoperative period.
Intermanual conflict was observed after commissurotomy by Wilson et al. (1977)
and by Akelaitis (1944-1945), who called it “diagnostic dyspraxia”.
While doing the block design test unimanually with his right hand, his left hand
came up from beneath the table and was reaching for the blocks when he slapped it with
his right hand and said, ”That will keep it quiet for a while”. Examples of intermanual
conflict have been reported in detail by Rayport et al. (1983) and Ferguson (1985). In
rare instances, the intermanual conflict may reappear even years later.
One of Bogen’s patients remained seizure-free for 8 years and then had a status
epilepticus a few months after her family physician discontinued her anticonvulsant
medication (Bogen, 1985). Her seizures were readily controlled when her medication
was reinstituted. Her left-handed anomia was still readily demonstrable 21 years postope-
ratively. This persistence of unilateral tactile anomia was also true of all their other
patients (Bogen et al., 1981).
were obtained. Not only could the animal not carry out a discrimination task previously learned
by one eye (cerebral hemisphere) when only the opposite eye was left uncovered, but each
eye (cerebral hemisphere) could learn conflicting tasks, depending on which eye was used.
In other classic series in the 1960s, Gazzaniga, Boegen and Sperry clearly
demonstrated the effects of split-brain preparations in human subjects (Mendoza, 2011).
Following commisurotomy, behavioral deficits were not always obvious. This is
probably due to the extensive cross-cueing that normally takes place, especially through
vision where the right hand sees what the left one is doing and vice-versa.
Although in cases of naturally occurring callosal lesion in humans, or even more
surprisingly in cases of callosal agenesis, there is typically a failure to evidence the signs
of a disconnection syndrome (Mendoza, 2011).
The psychological examination emphasised discrete elements of unilateral agraphia
on the left side, unilateral constructive apraxia on the right side, tactile anomia of the left
hand, disorder in naming objects presented in the left visual area and amnestic elements.
In addition to providing insights into how the two hemispheres normally interact
and their respective strengths, studies of split-brain patients have raised some intriguing
questions about the presumed unitary nature of consciousness.
Mishkin (1979) studied area TE, thinking of it as the final step in the neocortical
visual system. He later studied the problem of how visual stimuli might gain what he
calls “motivational” or “emotional” significance. Monkeys with bilateral temporal
lobotomies including the amygdala attach no significance to visual stimuli. That is, they
will repeatedly eat nasty-tasting objects or place inedible objects in the mouth.
In 1965, Geschwind proposed that this symptom represents a disconnection of the
amygdala from the visual system. Area TE connects with the amygdala on the same side
and the amygdala on the opposite side through the anterior commissure.
The result of Mishkin (1979) and Nakamura and Mishkin (1980) point to an
important role of the nonvisual cortex in visual perception and demonstrate the
importance of studying the connections to a functional system as well as its areas. If an
animal cannot move in response to visual information, it appears to be essentially blind,
even though the visual system may be processing the sensory input.
We can speculate at this point that it is an extreme example of a disconnection
syndrome because the brain is disconnected from both its inputs and outputs. It seems
1517
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
likely that, although the brain could still function, it would be unconscious in the absence
of inputs or outputs (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
In human beings, visual material can be presented selectively to a single hemisphere
by having the patient fix his gaze on a projection screen onto which pictures of objects
or symbols are projected to the right, left or both visual half-fields using exposure times
of 0.1 second or less (Bogen, 1987).
In short, the visual system is crossed, so information flashed to one visual field
travels selectively to the contralateral hemisphere. By using this fact, researchers have
demonstrated left- and right-visual-field superiority for different types of input.
For example, verbal material is perceived more accurately when presented to the
right visual field, presumably because the input travels to the left, linguistic hemisphere.
On the other hand visuospatial input (such as a map) produces a left-visual-field
superiority, because the right hemisphere appears to have a more important role in
analysing spatial information (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
However, words presented to the left visual field, and hence right hemisphere, are
sometimes perceived, not as accurately or consistently as when they are presented to the
right visual field. These relative effects occur because either hemisphere potentially has
access to input to the opposite hemisphere through the corpus callosum, which connects
the visual areas. Thus, the visual-field superiority observed in normal subjects is relative
(Myers, 1956; Sperry, 1974; Nakamura and Mishkin, 1980; Lepore et al., 1986; Lassonde
1986; Lassonde et al., 1986; Fantie and Kolb, 1989).
A commissurotomy patient no longer has such access, because the connection is
severed. Given that speech is usually housed in the left hemisphere of right-handed
patients, visual information presented to the left visual field will be disconnected from
verbal associations because the input goes to the right, nonlinguistic hemisphere.
Similarly, complex visual material presented to the right visual field will be
inadequately processed, because it will not have access to the visuospatial abilities of the
right hemisphere. It follows that, if material is appropriately presented, it will be possible
to demonstrate aphasia, agnosia, alexia, and acopia (the inability to copy a geometric
design) in a patient who ordinarily exhibits none of these symptoms, as Kolb and
Whishaw (2003) demonstrated.
If verbal material is presented to the left visual field, the commissurotomy patient
will be unable to read it or to answer questions about it verbally, because the input is
disconnected from the speech zones of the left hemisphere.
Presentation of some verbal material to the right visual field presents no difficulties,
because the visual input projects to the verbal left hemisphere (Downer, 1961; Geschwind,
1965; Gazzaniga, 1970; Schrift et al., 1986; Fabri et al., 2001; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Similarly, if an object is presented to the left visual field, the patient will be unable
to name it and thus will appear agnosic and aphasic. If presented to the right visual field,
this same object will be correctly named, because the left visual cortex perceives the
object and has access to the speech zones. Thus, we can see that the split-brain patient is
aphasic, alexic, and agnosic if verbal material or an object requiring a verbal response is
1518
20. Corpus callosum
presented visually to the right hemisphere, but this person appears normal if material is
presented to the left hemisphere (Downer, 1961; Geschwind, 1965; Gazzaniga, 1970;
Schrift et al., 1986; Fabri et al., 2001; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
A further deficit can be seen if the patients are asked to copy a complex visual figure.
Because the right hemisphere controls the left hand, we might predict that the left hand
will be able to copy the figure but right hand being deprived of the expertise of the right
hemisphere, will be severely impaired. This result is indeed the case: the left hand draws
the figure well, whereas the right hand cannot and is, thus, acopic (Kolb and Whishaw,
2003).
The complete splenial sections are exceptional. There are only few documented
observations of spontaneous lesions that led to splenial destruction with no previous
lesions (Damasio et al., 1980; Degas et al., 1987; Habib et al., 1990). The typical behavior
of subjects with callous disconnection, in the case of a short tachistoscopic projection of
a stimulus representing an image, a letter, a word etc., in the lateral region of this visual
area refers only to reporting a light or a “flash” in the left side. The same stimulus
presented on the right side leads to its immediate recognising. In case of subjects with
cerebral division, a simultaneous bilateral projection of stimuli in each visual hemifield
shows that it is difficult to distinguish them and that the images cannot overlap. Each
hemisphere responds independently to its respective stimulus (Lassonde et al., 1996). In
the case of chimerical figures (faces of people, shapes etc.), formed by two unpaired
halves, each of the elements in a compound pair was projected in a single hemisphere.
Under these conditions, if a verbal answer is required, the subject communicates only
the stimulus directed towards the left hemisphere. When he is asked to point with the left
hand, the subject indicates the stimulus analysed by the right hemisphere (Levy et al.,
1972).
With the help of this test it is possible to confirm the difference between specific
functions for each hemisphere. So, the stimuli presented in the right hemifield which is
controlled by the left hemisphere can be read and described verbally. The right
hemisphere could not name, neither verbally, nor in writing, the visual material presented
in the left hemifield.
With the left hand the subject can indicate with no difficulty, in a collection of things,
the object that is presented to him as an image or verbally.
The failure of the right hand is due to the fact that the left hemisphere, on which it
depends, is not informed about the stimulation exerted exclusively upon its homologue
(LeDoux and Gazzaniga, 1978).
Thereinafter, it was proved that patients with callosotomy, too, have a superior
visuospatial function of their hemisphere, while their left hemisphere uses electively
visual imagery that consists of letters.
The use of tactile information in order to form representations of abstract shapes is
better developed at the level of the parietal lobe in the right hemisphere. The analysis of
information from unfamiliar facial figures, too, belongs to the right hemisphere, while
the left hemisphere is more able to generate voluntary expressions. However the facial
1519
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
When they were asked to select with their hand an object that corresponds to the
object that was presented in their left visual field, they selected the matching object. This
showed that the visual system is functioning properly, and that it is the language function
that is not.
Although the above studies support the finding that the left cerebral hemisphere is
the “dominant” hemisphere in the processing of language, other studies indicate that the
right hemisphere may also be involved to some extent in language comprehension
(Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
Language
The most striking observation regarding split-brain subjects is the presence of
complex generative language in only one hemisphere. Nonetheless, the series of patients
operated on by Bogen demonstrated a well-developed right hemispheric lexicon in the
majority of patients examined, although that lexicon appeared to be limited to simple
auditory and visual comprehension and some written output (Gazzaniga et al., 1962;
Sperry et al., 1969; Levy et al., 1971).
Assessing the scientific significance of the data obtained after the examination of the
four left-handed patients with face localised in the right hemisphere, we may state that:
a. they confirm other studies of this kind which pointed out the poorer lateralisation
of the speech structure in left-handed patients as compared with right-handed ones
(Gazzaniga, 1983).
b. they demonstrated the role of the right hemisphere in the integration and
production of speech in left-handed patients. It extends its competence in performing
impressive speech. On the other hand it begins to directly control the articulating
apparatus and, implicitly, to participate in the production of expressive speech;
c. correlated with the data supplied by the analysis of right-handed patients with
lesions in the right hemisphere, they show that the linguistic dominance of the left
hemisphere does not completely eliminate but only limits the participation of the right
hemisphere in the production of verbal behavior (Dănăilă and Golu, 1987).
The Wilson-Roberts series, demonstrated much less frequent right hemispheric
participation in even rudimentary language processing. By 1983, of the 28 completed
callosotomies, only 3 patients had a documented right hemispheric lexicon (Gazzaniga,
1983). Moreover, there was considerable variation in the quality and sophistication of
the language available to the right hemisphere (Sidtis et al., 1981).
The visual and auditory lexicons of the right hemisphere appear to be similar, albeit
somewhat smaller than the corresponding left hemispheric lexicons (Gazzaniga et al., 1984).
Both hemispheres can make a variety of semantic judgments, recognising cate-
gorical, functional and associative relations. The ability to discriminate words from
nonword letter strings is limited but possible, which suggest that the visual word form is
represented in the right hemisphere of the patients (Reuter-Lorenz and Baynes, 1992).
Phonologic information is difficult for the right hemisphere to manipulate, although
it may possess limited phonologic competence and be able to produce speech. Sidtis et
1521
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
al. (1981) assessed discrimination of phonemes (such as “ba” versus “pa”) in two
callosotomy patients. The right hemisphere of one patient was able to discriminate but
not identify phoneme contrasts and was also able to identify rhyming words. However,
this patient was able to produce some verbal responses to left visual field stimuli within
a year of her completed surgery (Gazzaniga et al., 1984).
The other patient remained mute until more than 10 years after the surgery; he has now
gained rudimentary control of speech within the right hemisphere (Baynes et al., 1995).
Although he remained unable to make accurate judgments that require moving from
letter to sound, he was able to integrate visual and auditory phonologic information within
his right hemisphere (Baynes et al., 1994, 1995). This remarkable development has
implications for the limits of functional plasticity and for the role of the right hemisphere
in long-term recovery from aphasia (Baynes and Gazzaniga, 1997).
The linguistic prowess of the right hemisphere does not appear to extend to the use
of grammatical rules for comprehension or the production of sentences.
In a left-handed patient with right hemispheric language dominance, comprehension
of grammatical relations appears to be possible only for the right hemisphere (Lutsep et
al., 1995).
Right hemispheric reading proceeds more slowly than the left hemispheric reading
and may use a different mode of processing as has been reported for some deep dyslexic
patients (Reuter-Lorenz and Baynes, 1992).
Likewise, a right hemispheric lexicon with a more diffuse or associative organisation
than that of the left hemisphere has been suggested as the source of certain reading errors
in deep dyslexic (Coltheart, 1980; Schweinger et al., 1989) and pure alexic patients
(Coslett and Saffran, 1989).
The language profile seen in callosotomy patients is more consistent with the profile
reported by Coslett and Saffran (1989) for the preserved reading of their pure alexic
patient than with that reported for deep dyslexic patients (Baynes and Gazzaniga, 1997).
Thus, auditory comprehension of words by the disconnected right hemisphere is
suggested by the subject’s ability to retrieve with the left hand various objects if they are
named aloud by the examiner. Visual comprehension of printed words by the right
hemisphere is often present: after a printed word is flashed to the left visual half-field,
the subject is often able to retrieve with the left hand the designated item from among an
array of hidden objects. Control by the left hemisphere is excluded in these tests because
incorrect verbal descriptions given immediately after a correct response by the left hand
show that only the right hemisphere knew the answer.
While the disconnected right hemisphere’s receptive vocabulary can increase
considerably over the years, single word comprehension is rarely accompanied by speech.
The most extreme cases of right hemisphere language ability in right-handed (and left
hemisphere-speaking) split-brain subjects include two patients with right hemisphere
speech, both with an intact anterior commissure (Sidtis et al., 1981; McKeever et al., 1982).
Right hemisphere language in the split-brain subject has other limitations, with the
syntactic ability being rudimentary at best. Whereas phonetic and syntactic analysis seems
1522
20. Corpus callosum
to specialise heavily in the left hemisphere, there is a rich lexical structure in the right
hemisphere (Zaidel, 1978).
However, after a commissurotomy, each hemisphere can be tested separately,
demonstrating in a positive way those things that each hemisphere can do better than the
other, rather than inferring from its loss of function what a hemisphere is able to do if injured.
Representative reviews are included in the references (Bogen and Bogen, 1969;
Levy, 1974; Nebes, 1974; Sperry, 1974; Zaidel, 1983; Bradshaw and Nettleton, 1983;
Trevarthen, 1984).
Right-handers can write legibly, albeit not fluently, with the left hand. This ability
is commonly lost with callosal lesions, especially those that cause unilateral apraxia. An
inability to write to dictation is common with left hemisphere lesions, almost always
affecting both hands. The left hand may be dysgraphic if affected by a right hemispheric
lesion, such as a frontal lesion causing forced grasping (Bogen, 1978).
That the left dysgraphia after callosal sectioning is not simply attributable to an
incoordination or paresis can be established if one can demonstrate other abilities in the
left hand requiring as much control as would be required for writing. The left hand may
spontaneously doodle or it may copy various designs or diagrams. It is not so much the
presence of a deficit but rather the contrast between certain deficits and certain retained
abilities that is most informative (Bogen, 1987).
Simple or even complex geometric figures previously made with the patient’s own
right hand can often be copied by a left hand that cannot write or even copy writing
(Bogen and Gazzaniga, 1965; Bogen, 1969; Kumar, 1977; Zaidel and Sperry, 1977).
Postcallosotomy mutism
After a complete section of the cerebral commissures, there was in almost every case
a postoperative period of mutism, of varying duration, during which speech was absent
or extremely sparse, although comprehension and writing were retained.
In these cases, there was little if any paraphasia: when the ability to talk returned
there was no nominal amnesia; in some cases there was a definite lack of bodily
spontaneity and motor initiative for a time, but only partially correlated in duration with
the loss of speech.
As the mutism subsided, there was a stage of partial recovery that usually included
hoarseness or whispering, but without paraphasia, anomia, or novel semantic or syntactic
errors, except for 1 right-hander (corpus callosum was operated from the left) (Bogen, 1987).
However, postcommissurotomy deficits depend mainly upon the nature and amount
of preexisting extracallosal damage (Sperry et al., 1969; Gordon et al., 1971; Bogen, 1976).
Postcommissurotomy mutism was originally considered a simple neighbourhood
sign, a partial akinetic mutism from the retraction affecting the anterior end of the third
ventricle (Cairns, 1952; Ross and Stewart, 1981). In contrast to the cases of complete
commissurotomy in other patients all of the same structures (including the massa
intermedia and the anterior commissure) were severed, except the splenium, and there
was no immediate postoperative mutism (Gordon et al., 1971).
1523
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Such cases are evidence not only against a third ventricle origin for mutism, but also
against a right supplementary cortex origin. On the other hand, Ross et al. (1984) reported
mutism after anterior callosal sections but not after posterior callosal sections.
After a complete callosotomy in two stages, Rayport et al. (1985) observed in three
of eight cases a marked decrease in spontaneous speech unaccompanied by paraphasia
or a comprehension deficit or inability to sing. The authors suggested that, in these most
affected cases, mixed hand dominance may have been an important consideration.
Of particular importance was the absence of any language or speech problems after
the first stage of callosotomy (rostrum, genu, and most of the trunk). The mutism appeared
only after the second stage of the callosotomy (the splenium and remainder of the trunk).
This result does not totally eliminate the retraction on the supplementary motor
cortex as a partial contributory cause.
Rayport et al. (1985) have suggested that mutism could result from an interhe-
mispheric conflict. But they emphasize more the aspect of mixed dominance, which might
indicate a greater role than usual of the corpus callosum in the production of speech.
A diaschisis secondary to differentiation of speech areas accounts for deficits in
terms of left hemisphere dysfunctions even when the left hemisphere in unmolested.
So, the diaschisis of the left hemisphere (from a complete section) must affect the
speech “centres” or “circuits” more than it affects writing “centres“ or “circuits”. This
implies, in turn, that the writing function is more robust or resistant in some sense than
that for speech, in spite of having developed later in both phylogeny and ontogenesis
(Bogen, 1978).
A more speculative possibility is that speech usually requires interhemispheric
integration in order to control the larynx and other midline structures in the following
sense: One can suppose that left hemisphere speech ordinarily includes a corollary
discharge (Sperry, 1950) to the other hemisphere.
When the commissures are completely severed a downstream interhemispheric
conflict occurs at the level of the motor nuclei for the larynx, which results in dysphonia
(Bogen, 1987).
Those concerned with the normal physiology of speech will be more interested in the
temporary (several weeks to months) mutism after splenial sections (either as part of a total
callosotomy or as a second stage) than the long-lasting mutism (many months to years) of
those whose anomalous laterality is associated with long-standing cortical lesions. But for
the surgeon using a transcallosal approach to the third ventricle, neither of these is apt to be
as important as transient mutism (several days to weeks) after the section of callosal
segments (such as the trunk) well anterior to the splenium (Bogen, 1987).
However, the lack of speech could be considered a mutism of the type often
associated with akinesia of either cingulate or subfrontal origin. Ross et al. (1984) think
of the postcommissurotomy state as a sort of “forme fruste“ of akinetic mutism, in which
mutism is much more evident than is akinesia.
The occurrence of mutism in callosotomy cases without entry into the third ventricle
could be explained as the result of a subfrontal retraction while sectioning the rostrum.
1524
20. Corpus callosum
Arguing against this is the paucity of postoperative mutism in four of Ross et al. (1984)
patients whose splenium was spared but who underwent a section of both the rostrum
and the anterior commissure under direct vision. Thus, transitory mutism (for a few days
or months) immediately after surgery (commissurotomy) is classic but not completely
explained.
For Bogen (1987), keeping an intact splenium of the corpus callosum was sufficient
in order to avoid such a disorder.
In case of sectioning the intermediary area between the genu and the trunk of the
corpus callosum, the transitory mutism would be due to the inclusion of interhemispheric
fibres from the cingular area 24, which are associated, to the elemental mechanisms of
speech (Ross et al., 1984). According to Reeves (1991), an anterior callosotomy produces
a speech disorder which evokes an “adynamic” aphasia of variable gravity (from a simple
bradyarthria to complete mutism). The reasons for this type of disorder still remain
unknown but there probably are multiple ones: preparatory associated lesions, the
phenomenon called diaschisis of the hemisphere that dominates speech, a functional
imbalance between the two hemispheres in relation to the brutal disruption of an
assembly of cortical and cortico-subcortical connections.
Reeves (1991) shows that these disorders are associated with a certain number of
symptoms which evoke a dysfunction of the medial regions in the non-dominant frontal
cortex.
They manifest through a deficit of the inferior left limb and grasping of the left hand
which indicates a lesion of the right SMA.
In such situations the author invokes a surgical injury of the right internal frontal
region.
Long-term dysphasic disorders after a callosotomy are very rare and debatable. After
a complete callosotomy, Fergusson et al. (1985) and Spencer et al. (1988) reported
multiple cases of an important and long-term lack of variability in spontaneous speech,
with no disorders in understanding or repetition. The respective authors suggest that such
a disorder occurs mainly in patients whose speech depends on the interhemispheric
interaction and very large bilateral representations. This is why before every
commissurotomy there is a mandatory test with amobarbital. We can probably be
reassured with respect to the risk of severe postoperative mutism by a favorable response
to carotid amobarbital testing. This test can give ambiguous results, but it can be used to
exclude critical dependence upon the commissures. That is, if the patient continues to
speak intelligibly when the hemisphere minor for speech is narcotised, then the
disconnection of this minor hemisphere will probably not deprive the patient of an
essential resource with respect to speech production.
Except these particular cases, the callous disconnection is considered to be an
intervention that does not affect speech.
However, in “split-brain” cases, there are some very subtle disorders of speech as
for instance an influence on very elaborate or pragmatic speech. Zaidel (1990) shows
that the most obvious pragmatic deficits appear in subjects whose right hemisphere has
1525
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
a lesion of the left occipital region, lesion which is frequently vascular, are incapable of
understanding or reading words.
However, after a period of total alexia they can decipher the words by splitting them
into syllables (“letter by letter dyslexia”). Messages coming from the right hemisphere
(the only one receiving information due to a right homonymous hemianopsia) are
interrupted by a lesion of the splenial fibres at the level they enter the left hemisphere,
consequently to an occipital lesion, spread the forward and into depth.
Therefore, the absence of interhemispheric transfer leads to some speech disorder
directly related to the dominance of the left hemisphere.
In right-handed “split-brain” patients, the deficits manifest through left tactile
anomia, left lateral homonymous pseudohemianopsia, left ideomotor apraxia to verbal
orders and agraphia of the left hand.
The linguistic potential of the right hemisphere in a patient with cerebral division,
seems to be limited to verbal, heard or read understanding but only for certain categories
of words, and his capacity of expressing himself is extremely limited, but not quite
inexistent (Bogen, 1985).
The analysis of subjects with callous lesions consists of a privileged pattern for
understanding the role of the right hemisphere in reading (Michel et al., 1996).
Somesthesis
The somatosensory system is completely crossed. Thus, an object placed in the left
hand can be named because the tactile information projects to the right hemisphere,
crosses to the left, and subsequently has access to the speech zones. Similarly, if a subject
is blindfolded and the right hand is moulded to form a particular shape, the left hand is
able to copy the shape. The tactile information goes from the right hand to the left
1527
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
hemisphere and then across the corpus callosum to the right hemisphere, and the left hand
forms the same shape (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The lack of interhemispheric transfer after hemisphere disconnection can be
demonstrated with respect to somesthesis (including touch, pressure, and proprioception)
in a variety of ways. The somatosensory functions of the left and right parts of the body
become independent.
Consequently, unseen objects in the right hand are handled, named, and described
in a normal fashion but the subjects with total callosotomy cannot do so if the object is
placed in the left hand, because the sensory input is disconnected from the left (speech)
hemisphere.
Thus, the most useful single sign of hemisphere disconnection is unilateral tactile
anomia: this is an inability to name or describe an object when it is left by one hand
whereas it is readily named (or well described if the name is unknown) when it is placed
into the other hand or when it is presented either to vision or to audition.
This unilateral tactile anomia was present in every patient with complete
commissurotomy studied by Gazzaniga and LeDoux (1978), McKeever et al. (1981) and
Bogen (1978), despite sparing the anterior commissure. Although there are many signs
of brain bisection, one of the most convincing ways to demonstrate hemisphere
disconnection is to ask the patient to feel with one hand then name various small, common
objects such as a button, coin, safety pin, paper clip, pencil stub, a rubber band or key.
Vision must be excluded (Bogen, 1987).
The patient with a hemisphere disconnection is generally unable to name or describe
an object in the left hand although he readily names objects in the right hand. Sometimes
the patient will give a vague description of the object although unable to name it, but
there is a contrast with the ability to name the object readily when it is placed into the
right hand.
The most certain proof that the object has been identified is for the subject to retrieve
it correctly from a collection of similar objects.
However, astereognosis can be reasonably excluded.
Specific posture impressed on one (unseen) hand by the examiner cannot be mimicked
in the opposite hand. A convenient way to test for lack of interhemispheric transfer of
proprioceptive information is as follows: the patient extends both hands beneath the opaque
screen (or vision is otherwise excluded) and the examiner impresses a particular posture
on one hand. For example, one can put the tip of the thumb against the tip of the little finger
and have the other three fingers fully extended and separated. The split-brain patient cannot
mimic with the other hand a posture being held by the first hand.
This procedure should be repeated with various postures and in both directions. After
complete commissurotomy there is a partial loss of the ability to name exact points
stimulated on the left side of the body.
This defect is least apparent, if at all, on the face and it is most apparent on the distal
parts, especially the finger tip. This deficit is not dependent upon language; it can be
shown in a nonverbal (picture identification) fashion, in which case the deficit is present
in both directions (right-to-left and vice versa) (Bogen, 1987).
1528
20. Corpus callosum
According to Bentin et al. (1984) and Gaffen et al. (1985), one of the most sensitive
tests for evaluating the callous disconnection is that of crisscross tactile localisation. It
consists of applying light pressure upon the extremity of the finger, outside the visual
area of the patient. With the help of the thumb of the same hand, the patient indicates
without difficulty which of the fingers had been stimulated. If the tests are performed
separately with the fingers from the opposite hand the patient will be constantly wrong.
Audition
The auditory system is more complex than the other sensory system because it has
both crossed and uncrossed connections.
A naturally occurring lesion near the callosal trunk can result in the suppression of
one ear when tested dichotically. Hence, one might accept similar changes after a section
of the callosal trunk. However, several authors showed the place of the callosal transfer
of audible information.
Springer and Gazzaniga (1975) were the first to report a left dichotic extinction in a
case of a parietal callosotomy in which the splenium had been preserved. Musier and
Reeves (1986) noticed that complete callosotomies lead to an alteration of performance
of the left ear when undergoing dichotic tests. This fact explains the disruption of transfer
for verbal information that was initially received from the left ear / right hemisphere.
On the other hand, the two ears have a deficit in tests of pattern recognition for sound
frequency. This suggests that the test needs both hemispheres (Habib, 1998).
However, the performance of subjects with an anterior callosotomy are very variable,
therefore the conclusion that pertinent fibres for audible information transfer are located
in the posterior half of the commissurotomy. This fact had been confirmed in some cases
with spontaneous parietal lesions of the corpus callosum (Damasio et al., 1980; Degas et
al., 1987; Habib et al., 1990).
According to Damasio et al. (1980) and Degas et al. (1987), the splenial lesion had
been accompanied by an alteration of the dichotic hearing test. This fact suggests that
the passage of pertinent intertemporal fibres for audible transfer is located in the splenium.
Alexander and Warren (1988) came to the conclusion that the interhemispheric
passage of audible information is located near the somatosensorial information from the
posterior area of the trunk, i.e., the isthmus. Damasio and Damasio (1979), Poncet et al.
(1987) point out that in order to finish the extinction of the left ear it is necessary for the
callous fibres to be touched in general in the interior of the left hemisphere and not at the
level of the corpus callosum itself.
This “paradoxical extinction” of the left ear accompanied by a moderate aphasia
became the classic sign of a lesion of the profound white matter in the left hemisphere,
similar to the sensitive differentiation or ideomotor apraxia to which it is associated
(Poncet et al., 1987).
Therefore, the audible attributes are bilateral, but each sound stimulus is analysed
by both hemispheres due to the interhemispheric transfer through the corpus callosum.
Because the contralateral tracts are more important due to the number of fibres they
1530
20. Corpus callosum
contain, they are dominant over the information directed through the ipsilateral tracts.
Nevertheless, the patient with a split-brain can report with no difficulty the verbal or
nonverbal stimuli that are present in one ear only.
However, the verbal stimuli perceived by the right ear are analysed mainly by the
left temporal region (linguistic). Due to the fact that the right hemisphere is responsible
for musicality, the musical material is analysed with less difficulty in the left ear, from
where it is massively directed towards the right temporal area. However, after a cerebral
commissurotomy, the patient readily identifies single words (and other sounds) if they
are presented to one ear at a time.
But if different words are presented to the two ears simultaneously (“dichotic
listening”), only the words presented to the right ear will be reliably reported (Milner et
al., 1968; Sparks and Geschwind, 1968; Efron et al., 1977). Therefore, words played into
the left ear can travel directly to the hemisphere or can go to the right hemisphere and
then to the left through the corpus callosum. However, the direct access does not appear
to exist when the hemispheres are disconnected.
This large advantage of the right ear is usually considered the result of two concurrent
circumstances: (a) the ipsilateral pathway (from the left ear to the left hemisphere) is
suppressed by the presence of simultaneous but differing inputs, as it is in intact individuals
during dichotic listening (Kimura, 1967; Teng, 1981). (b) the contralateral pathway from
the left ear to the right hemisphere has now been severed and conveys information that
ordinarily reaches the left (speaking) hemisphere by the callosal pathway. Although left
ear words are rarely reported, their perception by the right hemisphere is occasionally
evidenced by appropriate actions of the left ear (Gordon, 1973).
Contralateral ear suppression commonly appears after hemispherectomy or the
creation of another large hemispheric lesion. Because there is usually suppressed by left
hemisphere lesions, the suppression of the left ear by a left hemisphere lesion has been
called “paradoxical ipsilateral extinction”. Further observations have led to the conclusion
that, whether the lesion is in the left or the right hemisphere, or if it is close to the midline,
the suppression of left ear stimuli is probably attributable to an interruption of
interhemispheric pathways (Sparks et al., 1970; Michel and Peronnet, 1975; Damasio
and Damasio, 1979).
In any case, the auditory effects are unlikely to be of clinical importance, although
one can anticipate certain patients in whom slight auditory alterations might be quite
important.
Olfaction
The olfactory tract is not essentially crisscrossed. The interhemispheric commu-
nication between the cortical areas responsible for the olfactory analysis is ensured rather
through the anterior commissure, than thorough the corpus callosum.
According to Bouchet and Cuilleret (1983), areas 23 and 24 of the anterior and
posterior region of the cingular circumvolution could be responsible for olfaction. Their
interhemispheric communication fibres transit, at least in monkeys, through the dorsal
facet of the corpus callosum.
1531
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 20.9. Anosmia. (A) In the normal condition, olfactory input to the right nostril transvers directly back into the right
hemisphere and crosses the anterior commissure, thus gaining access to the left (speech) hemisphere. (B) Anosmia
results from section of the anterior commissure. (The jagged line indicates the lesion.) When the pathway is severed.,
the information is blocked, and the left hemisphere has no way of knowing what odor the right hemisphere perceived.
(after Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The most obvious change consists of the impossibility to perform two simultaneous
actions with each hand (Gazzaniga et al., 1967).
In fact, the most certain functional correlation in matter of the topographic anatomy
of the corpus callosum refers to the existence of important difficulties in bimanual
coordination consequent to sectioning the anterior half of the callous commissure.
Zaidel and Sperry (1977) observed that in 5-10 years after surgery an important
quantitative and qualitative alteration of bimanual coordination still persists during tasks
that involve either a rapid alternative movement of both hands, or a complex bimanual
coordination.
Kreuter et al. (1972) also demonstrated that during the synchronised or alternant
bimanual tapping test, the “split-brain” subjects cannot perform the task unless they
significantly slow down the rhythm of motor act.
Preilowski (1990), who studied the role of commissural fibres in bimanual
coordinated movements, believes one needs an unharmed anterior area of the corpus
callosum in order to complete these actions.
The information coming from a hemisphere needs to be transferred to the con-
tralateral hemisphere as an interhemispheric exchange of “motor corollary discharges”.
This role normally belongs to the premotor frontal cortex or more specifically to the
supplementary motor area (SMA) or the medial premotor system (Goldberg, 1985).
Through the genu of the corpus callosum, in the anterior area of the body, the SMA
is richly connected to the contralateral SMA as well as to the primary motor area. The
connections between the two motor and premotor systems of the corpus callosum are
crucial for performing coordinate actions with the two superior limbs.
However, a total callous section alters the synchronisation of the two hands more
severely than an anterior section. This fact suggests the partially compensatory role of
regions located more posterior which transmit proprioceptive information through the
presplenial region (Ellenberg and Sperry, 1979).
If a hand performs rapid alternative movements of pronation-supination one can
notice, particularly in children, the natural tendency of performing mirror movements
with the other hand (Njiokiktjien et al., 1986) when the subject is involved in a bimanual
activity. This tendency is found in subjects with callous agenesis, but is more severe.
Dennis (1976) supposes that this tendency of performing mirror gestures generally means
the callous transfer systems are immature.
Here, the corpus callosum has an inhibitory action that suppresses mirror involuntary
movements which disturb the coordinated bimanual movements.
For Dennis (1976), these inhibitory systems borrow the posterior region of the corpus
callosum. The callous connections may also play an important role in some motor
activities such as moving a hand during learning a particular motor action (Habib, 1998).
Therefore, patients with callosotomy present many disorders of interhemispheric
disconnections when a monomanual answer is expected. Thus, in right-handed patients,
writing with the left hand results in a unilateral left agraphia which is characterised by
drawing illegible letters of the alphabet, paragraphia, and the impossibility of copying
words if not written in printed form.
1533
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
These symptoms presented by the left hand (in right-handed patients) are
consequent to the fact that the right hemisphere is essentially aphasic, while writing
with the right hand (which depends on the left hemisphere) is normal. In the preoperatory
phase a unilateral left ideomotric apraxia appears, but only for verbal orders (Lassonde
et al., 1996).
However, according to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), because the motor system is
largely crossed, we might predict that the disconnection of the hemispheres will induce
a form of apraxia and agraphia, because the left hand would not receive instructions from
the left hemisphere. These disabilities would not be seen in the right hand because it has
access to the speech hemisphere.
So, if a patient was asked to use the right hand to copy a geometric design, it might
be impaired (acopia) because it is disconnected from the right hemisphere, which
ordinarily has a preferred role in rendering.
Preilowski (1975) and later Zaidel and Sperry (1977) stated that the severity of the
deficit declines significantly with the passage of time after surgery, possibly because the
left hemisphere’s ipsilateral control of movements is being used.
A second situation that might produce severe motor deficits in commissurotomy
patients is one in which the two arms must be used in cooperation.
Thus, patients were severely impaired at alternating tapping movements of the index
finger.
A high degree of manual cooperation is required to trace a diagonal line smoothly.
If the hemispheres have been disconnected, the cooperation is severely retarded, because
the left and right motor systems cannot gain information about what the opposite side is
doing, except indirectly by the patient’s watching them.
Dramatic illustrations of conflict between hands abound (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
It is of interest to note that while inhibiting these episodes of intermanual conflict
were able to use their left hands in a purposeful and cooperative manner when “not
thinking of what they were doing (Preilowski, 1975).
in the posterior, at the level of the callous isthmus, engrams through the disconnection of
the left hemispheric motor (Heilman, 1979).
Therefore, one can distinguish two types of callous agraphies of the left hand: one
related to the disruption of premotor fibres from the anterior area of the corpus callosum,
characterised not only by affecting the handwriting but also the capacity of typewriting
or the use of letters on a mobile phone with the left hand.
The other one is related to the presplenial lesion which disconnects the left angular
gyrus and affects only handwriting.
Thus, the left hemisphere, which lacks the spatial support of the right hemisphere,
becomes apraxo-agnosic. The drawing or copying of a drawing which had been well
executed with the right hand before surgery becomes unrecognisable after operation. The
left hand may however reproduce the drawing. In this case, Bogen and Gazzaniga (1965)
speak about a constructive apraxia, and Preilowski (1975) and Zaidel and Sperry (1977)
speak about a dysfunction of the ability of bimanual motor coordination. The situation
becomes more dramatic when a task must be performed without seeing.
According to Watson and Heilman (1983), at the level of the left hemisphere, in
right handed people, there are two types of programs or engrams: a verbal-motor one,
that controls the linguistic nature of the written message, and another one named video-
kinaesthetic, that controls the graphic aspects that allow a spatial-temporal structure.
Finally, Sugishita et al. (1980) and Gersh and Damasio (1981) insist upon the
dissociated character of callous apraxia and agraphia. Degas et al. (1987) consider that a
lesion of the splenium, and of the posterior fourth of the body of the corpus callosum,
provoked agraphia without left apraxia.
Kazuis and Sawada (1993) had a patient with a lesion located in the posterior half
of the body of the corpus callosum leaving the juxta-splenial area of the isthmus intact.
They observed this patient showed left apraxia without agraphia.
These data show that the interhemispheric fibres that allow the transfer of graphic
information from the left to the right hemisphere are located in the posterior area of the
body of the corpus callosum and those that control the gestures of the left hand are located
in its anterior region.
Thus, if a lesion of the corpus callosum disconnects the left hand from the left
hemisphere, that hand is unable to respond to verbal commands and is considered apraxic.
Suppose, however, that the right hand is unable to respond to verbal commands.
Geschwind (1965) speculated that this deficit results from a lesion in the left hemisphere
that disconnects its motor cortex (which controls the right hand) from the speech zone
(Fig. 20.10). Thus, the right hand cannot respond to verbal commands and is considered
apraxic.
Although Geschwind’s model can explain bilateral apraxia in some patients, it must be
emphasized that disconnection is not the only cause of apraxia. Because the posterior cortex
has direct access to the subcortical neural mechanisms of arm and body movements, parietal
input need not go through the motor cortex, except for the control of finger movements.
Further, patients with sections of the corpus callosum are initially apraxic but show substantial
recovery despite a disconnection of the motor cortex of the left and right hemisphere.
1535
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 20.10. Frontal commissurotomy: Sketched here are the structures sectioned during a frontal commissurotomy.
This includes most of the corpus callosum (sparing the splenium), the ventral hippocampal commissure
(between the fornices), and the anterior commissure (after Bogen, 1987).
Unilateral apraxia
In 1907, Liepmann and Maas described a patient with a right hemiparesis from a
lesion of the pons. The patient also had a lesion of the corpus callosum. This patient was
unable to correctly pantomime to command with his left arm. Because he had a right
hemiparesis, his right hand could not be tested.
Since the work of Broca, it has been known that right-handers’ left hemisphere is
dominant for language. Liepmann and Maas could have attributed their patient’s inability
to pantomime to a disconnection between language and motor areas so that the left
hemisphere that mediates comprehension of verbal commands could not influence the
right hemisphere’s motor areas that are responsible for controlling the left hand.
However, this patient also could not initiate gestures or correctly use actual tools or
objects, therefore, a language-motor disconnection could not account for these findings.
Liepmann and Maas posited that the left hemisphere of right-handers contains movement
formulas and that the callosal lesion in this patient disconnected these movement formulas
from the right hemisphere’s motor areas.
Geschwind and Kaplan in 1962, Geschwind in 1965, and Gazzaniga et al. (1967)
also found that their patients with callosal disconnection could not correctly pantomime
to command with the left hand. Unlike the patient reported by Liepmann and Maas,
however, their patients could imitate and correctly use actual tools and objects with the
left hand. The preserved ability to imitate and use actual tools and objects suggests that
the inability to gesture to command in these patients with callosal lesions was induced
by a language-motor disconnection rather than a movement formula-motor disconnection.
In addition, a disconnection between the movement formula and motor areas should
produce spatial and temporal errors, but many of the errors made by Liepmann and Maas’
patient appeared to be content errors. In 1983, Watson and Heilman described a patient
with an infarction limited to the body of the corpus callosum. Their patient had no
1537
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
weakness in her right hand and performed all tasks flawlessly with her right hand. With
her left hand, however, she could not correctly pantomime to command, imitate, or use
actual tools. Immediately after her cerebral infarction she made some content errors, but
later on, she made primarily spatial and temporal errors.
Her performance indicated that not only language but also movement representations
were stored in her left hemisphere and that her callosal lesion disconnected these
movement formulas from the right hemisphere.
Thus, left limb dispraxia can be attributed to the simultaneous presence of two
deficits: poor comprehension by the right hemisphere (which has good control over the
left hand) and poor ipsilateral control by the left hemisphere (which understands the
commands).
Failure to correctly position a limb, to move the limb correctly in space, and to
properly orient the limb is called ideomotor apraxia. Apraxia has been subclassified
into ideational apraxia, and limb-kinetic apraxia (Liepmann, 1900).
Ideational apraxia is a loss of the conception of a gesture or skilled movement. In
this form, the patient does not seem to know what to do, and the motor activity is not
facilitated by the use of actual objects.
Ideomotor apraxia affects the implementation of movement, producing spatial and
timing errors. The patient seems to know what to do but cannot carry movement out
properly.
Limb-kinetic apraxia refers to clumsiness, awkwardness of a limb in the perfor-
mance of a skilled act that cannot be accounted for by paresis, ataxia, or sensory loss.
This apraxia manifested by the inability to make finely graded, precise limb movements.
Apraxia may be differentiated by the body elements involved in the impaired
movement, using the term limb apraxia, oral or buccofacial apraxia, trunk or axial apraxia.
The terms dressing apraxia and constructional apraxia are sometimes used to
describe certain symptoms of unilateral extinction or neglect (amorphosynthesis) that
characterize partial lobe lesions.
These patients also make temporal errors and may fail to correctly imitate
movements, but some patients imitate worse than they gesture in response to command.
In right-handed individuals, ideomotor apraxia is almost always associated with left
hemisphere lesions, but in left-handers, ideomotor apraxia is usually associated with right
hemisphere lesions. Ideomotor apraxia is associated with lesions in a variety of structures,
including the corpus callosum, the inferior parietal lobe, and the premotor areas.
Ideomotor apraxia has also been reported with subcortical lesions that involve the
basal ganglia and white matter.
Alien hand syndrome. Symptoms: anarchic hand, callosal apraxia, diagnostic
dyspraxia, Dr. Strangelove syndrome, intermanual conflict, magnetic apraxia, wayward hand.
Alien hand syndrome is a relatively rare manifestation of damage to specific brain
regions involved in voluntary movement (Goldberg and Goodwin, 2011). The core
observation is the patient’s report that one of his / her hand is displaying purposeful,
coordinated, and goal-directed behavior over which the patient feels he / she has no
1538
20. Corpus callosum
voluntary control. The hand seems to possess the capability of acting autonomously,
independent of their conscious voluntary control.
The patient fails to recognize the action of one of his hands as his own. The hand,
effectively, appears to manifest a “will of its own” (Goldberg and Goodwin, 2011).
This definition excludes disordered, non-purposeful, and dyskinetic movements
associated with other involuntary movement disorders.
Three forms of alien hand syndrome have been described: frontal, callosal, and
posterior.
The frontal form is associated with damage to the medial surface of the cerebral
hemisphere in the frontal region. The frontal variant involves the medial aspect of the
premotor cortex anterior to the primary motor cortex including the supplementary motor
area and anterior cingulate cortex. Generally, these alien behaviours appear in the hand
contralateral to the damaged hemisphere regardless of hemispheric dominance.
The posterior or “sensory” form appears most often with a parietal or parieto-
occipital focus of circumscribed damage (Pack et al., 2002). As in the frontal variant, the
alien behavior appears in the hand contralateral to the damaged hemisphere. The
movement of the affected alien limb is typically less organized and often has an ataxic
instability particularly with visually guided reaching. The limb may also show pro-
prioceptive sensory impairments with hypaesthesia, so that the kinaesthetic impairment
limits the monitoring of limb position. Visual field deficits as well as hemi-inattention
may be seen on the same side as the alien hand (Goldberg and Goodwin, 2011).
Alien hand behavior has also been reported in association with a subcortical thalamic
infarction.
The fMRI study of cortical activation patterns associated with alien and non-alien
movement has demonstrated that alien movement is in fact characterized by the isolated
activation of the primary motor cortex in concert with the activation of interhemispheric
premotor regions (Assal et al., 2007).
The callosal form is seen with an isolated lesion of the corpus callosum.
The voluntary motor systems of the two hemispheres are isolated from each other
due to lost interhemispheric communication following callosotomy.
In the “callosal” variant of alien hand syndrome, the appearance of “intermanual
conflict” or “self-oppositional” behaviours is the predominant feature (Goldberg and
Goodwin, 2011). In the callosal variant, the problematic alien hand is consistently the
nondominant hand, while the dominant hand is the identified “good”, controlled hand.
The patient may express frustration and bewilderment at the conflicting and disruptive
behavior of the alien hand whose motivations remain inaccessible to consciousness
(Goldberg and Goodwin, 2011).
There may be an attentional component that modulates the appearance of these episodes
of self-oppositional behavior since intermanual conflict is observed more frequently when
the patient is fatigued, stressed, or is engaged in effortful multitasking activity.
1539
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Occasionally, the two hands are observed to be engaged in two different and entirely
unrelated activities as if being guided by completely separate and independent intentions
(Goldberg and Goodwin, 2011).
The callosal and frontal variants are often seen in combination with a corresponding
overlap of observed behavior. However, a clear differentiation between an apparent
intermanual conflict due to attempts to restrain alien behaviours associated with the
frontal variant, and a true intermanual conflict, in which the two hands are directed
towards an independently contradictory purpose, may be difficult to make (Biran and
Chatterjee, 2004).
The patient may become fearful that they will be held accountable for consequences
of an action of the alien hand over which they do not feel control (Giovannetti et al., 2005).
The patient may display “auto-criticism” complaining that the alien hand is not doing
what it has been “told to do” and is therefore characterized as disobedient, wayward, or
“evil”. Given the predicament created, the patients may develop depersonalization and
dissociate themselves from the unintended actions of the hand (Biran and Chatterjee,
2004; Giovannetti et al., 2005; Assal et al., 2007; Sumner and Husain, 2008; Goldberg
and Goodwin, 2011).
In alien hand syndrome, different regions of the brain are able to command purposeful
limb movements, without generating conscious feeling or self-control over these
movements. This process, impaired in alien hand syndrome, normally produces the
conscious sensation that movement is being internally initiated and produced by an active
self (Frith et al., 2000; Scepkowski and Cronin-Golomb, 2003; Sumner and Husain, 2008).
However, an evaluation of callosal apraxia and impairment of interhemispheric
transfer of information should be included. There is no definitive treatment for the alien
syndrome but a number of different rehabilitative approaches have been described.
however, the elaborative processing involved has a deleterious effect on the accuracy of
perceptual recognition.
This result extended to include verbal material (Metcalfe et al., 1995).
However, this predilection has an important role in memory accuracy (Baynes and
Gazzaniga, 1997). Thus, the left hemisphere interprets the theoretical constructions by
assimilating information in a comprehensible whole with the help of verbal material.
hemi-body that is opposite to the lesion, in which the superior limb has the tendency of
performing movements without voluntary control of the subject.
Thus, most often appear movements of attraction to a certain visual target with the
purpose of grabbing the objects that are close. Goldberg associates this behavior to an
excessive addiction of the motor cortex to the environment, because the motor cortex is
freed of the premotor median cortical control and left under the influence of only the
lateral system (its role is to control movements oriented as a response to stimuli from the
environment).
Goldberg calls this “foreign hand sign” behavior, which is often misinterpreted.
This is why Poncet et al. (1987) replaces the term with “stubborn hand“. This
symptom must be differentiated from the motor behavior observed after a callous lesion,
which is called apraxia.
In practice, it is important to make a difference between the stubborn hand sign and
the apraxia. The first is related to a double lesion, both median frontal and callous, which
affects the hand opposite to the harmed frontal lobe, and the latter appears only after a
callous lesion that affects exclusively the superior left limb (Habib, 1998).
to the witnesses, both for sounds transmitted for the pericentral region and for sounds
transmitted for the lateral region. These data do not seem to support the hypothesis of a
privileged relation of the corpus callosum with the median line (Poirier et al., 1993).
Consequently, the entire pathology of the callous system should affect the median fusion
process because it is certain that the corpus callosum participates in the making of this
median fusion.
However, participation is more important for the systems that are certainly contra-
lateralized (visual, and lemniscal somatosensorial (Lepore et al., 1994; Lassonde et al., 1996).
but led to an interruption of the callous tract between the motor centres in the right
hemisphere which controls the left hand and the speech areas located near the left
hemisphere.
Gersh and Damasio (1981) reported cases of people with apraxia but no agraphia of
the left hand that followed the sectioning of the anterior region of the trunk in the corpus
callosum. The authors came to the conclusion that praxic impulses use fronto-frontal
tracts, thus the apraxia, but no agraphia of the left hand, appear when the section affects
exclusively the anterior area of the trunk in the corpus callosum.
Risse et al. (1989) noticed that the sectioning of the two-thirds located in the anterior
area of the corpus callosum leads to small or no changes. Here an example (Fig. 20.11).
Fig. 20.11. Anterior callosotomy. This sketch shows how anterior callosotomy differs from frontal commissurotomy.
There are two principal differences: the anterior commissure is spared, and third ventricle is not entered
by separating the fornices (after Bogen, 1987).
In the case of a larger anterior section there appears a disruption of the ability to
transfer sensorial and position information from one hand to the other. On the contrary,
a section of the splenium disrupts the transfer of visual information between hemispheres
and isolates the lateral visual inputs.
Thus, a section of the corpus callosum that spares the splenium (as well as the
anterior commissure) but is otherwise complete can be readily accomplished via an
1545
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
exposure anterior to the rolandic bridging vein (Bogen, 1987). This procedure which
avoids entry into the third ventricle has come to be the most popular version of
commissural section for epilepsy or tumors. After this operation one does not observe
most of the acute disconnection syndrome. This smoother postoperative course is not
surprising because the acute disconnection syndrome does not usually occur after frontal
commissurotomy, which is essentially the same procedure plus a section of the anterior
commissure and massa intermedia.
Absence of significant mutism with callosotomy sparing the splenium has been
reported by Rayport et al. (1985) and Bogen (1987).
Ross et al. (1984) reported transient (1- to 10- day) mutism with this procedure. This
difference could depend upon the amount of retraction particularly if there has been a
retraction of the left hemisphere below the falx as well as the usual right hemisphere
retraction. Even a lesser callosal section may be followed by mutism when it is associated
with a concurrent thalamic lesion (Bogen, 1987).
Midcallosal section
A midcallosal section is an incision through the trunk sparing not only the splenium
but also most of the genu. Such an incision affords ready access to the foramina of Monro
with practically no obligatory physiological cost as presently assessable (Gordon et al.,
1971; Ozgur et al., 1977; Shucart and Stein, 1978; Winston et al., 1979; Long and Leibrock,
1980; Antunes et al., 1980; Apuzzo et al., 1982). I operated on 115 cases of tumors of the
third ventricle. The clinical picture was dominated by the signs of intracranial hypertension.
Neuropsychological signs (40%) were sometimes under-estimated, revealing the disease
in only 17% of cases. Ophthalmologic signs and endocrine disorders have been found in
32% of these cases. In planning the surgical approach, we took into account the localization
and size of the tumors: transcallosal approach for the third ventricle, primary tumors (58,2%)
(Figures 20.12, 20.13, 20.14, 20.15).
Microsurgery has been used in all cases – Laser with CO2 and Nd: YAG was used
in 12 (10,4%) cases.
The somesthetic nontransfer has not been detected in most cases of midcallosal
section. But if the task is made more difficult, so that normal individuals commonly make
mistakes, some deficits (as compared with normal subjects) have been elicited.
Splenial section
In the case of a section of the posterior half of the trunk, the agraphia of the left hand
with no associated apraxia is due to the fact that the graphic impulses transit over the
posterior area, through parietal tracts.
The visual interhemispheric transfer is done especially through the splenium. A
lesion at this level leads to alexia without agraphia (Figures 20.16, 20.17, 20.18, 20.19,
20.20, 20.21).
1546
20. Corpus callosum
A B
C D
Fig. 20.12. (A and B) Preoperative coronal and sagittal T1-weighted gadolinium-enhanced MRI demonstrating details
of an anterior giant cystic craniopharyngioma with sellar enlargement, and suprasellar component and cystic
extension into the third ventricle. (C and D) images were obtained after almost complete removal of tumor with very
good results. Excision was accomplished through a midline anterior transcallosal and interfornicial corridor.
Thus, whereas more anterior section of the corpus callosum entails minimal long
term physiological cost, a section of the splenium commonly causes a left hemialexia
(Trescher and Ford, 1937; Maspes, 1948; Wechsler, 1972; Iwata et al., 1974; Damasio
et al., 1980). This inability to read in the left visual half-field may be accompanied by a
so called colour anomia, an inability to give the names of colours presented to the
patient’s view although the colours can be matched and the patient can give (speak or
write) the colour names of objects, for example “yellow“ when asked the colour of a
banana (Geschwind and Fusillo, 1966).
When the hemialexia and associated deficits are more severe they can be mistaken
for a left homonymous hemianopsia.
1547
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
A B
Fig. 20.13. (A) A contrast enhanced axial CT-scan of a 32-year-old man with a large third ventricle tumor
(craniopharyngioma). (B) Axial CT-scan, 6 months, postoperatively, confirming total removal of the tumor with
excellent results. Excision was accomplished through a midline anterior transcallosal corridor.
A B
Fig. 20.14. (A) Sagittal MRI demonstrating details of a big third ventricular tumor (dermoid cyst) who was excized
transcallosal with very good results. (B) Sagittal MRI after complete removal of the tumor.
A B
C D
Fig. 20.15. (A and B) Preoperative coronal and sagittal T1-weighted gadolinium-enhanced MRI demonstrating of an
anterior third ventricle tumor (astrocytoma). (C and D). Images were obtained after complete removal of the tumor
with excellent results. Excision was accomplished through a midline transcallosal approach.
In splenial lesions the right occipital cortex is disconnected from the left hemisphere.
Thus, the left hemisphere retains competence to write to dictation but no longer has access
to information arriving in the right occipital lobe from the left visual half-field.
Generally, alexia is a cortical disorder of reception in which there is a loss of the
ability to comprehend written material manifested as an impairment of reading. Those
afflicted with alexia have at one time the ability to read but no longer comprehend written
material.
1549
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
A B
Fig. 20.16. (A) Contrast enhanced axial CT-scan in a 39-year-old woman shows a large, well-delimitated pineal
region tumor (meningioma). (B) Postoperative CT-scan after total removal of the tumor.
A B
Fig. 20.17. (A) CT-scan showing a large pineal region tumor with involvement of the third and lateral ventricle.
(B) Postoperative CT-scan after the excision of that pineal region tumor (germinoma).
Hemialexia is the loss of reading ability in the nondominant visual field as the result
of a surgical section of the corpus callosum.
Alexia without agraphia (visual verbal agnosia) is a syndrome, in which a literate
person loses the ability to read aloud, to understand written script, and, often, to name
colours, i.e., to match a seen colour to its spoken name – visual verbal colour anomia.
Such a person can no longer name or point on command to words, although he is
sometimes able to read letters or numbers (Adams et al., 1997).
1550
20. Corpus callosum
A B
C D
Fig. 20.18. (A and B) Sagittal and coronal T1-weight gadolinium-enhanced MRI, demonstrating a medium-sized
pineocytoma. (C and D) Postoperative MRI after total removal of the tumor.
A B
Fig. 20.19. (A) Contrast enhanced axial CT-scan shows a large germinoma of the pineal region with involvement of
the third ventricle. (B) Postoperative CT-scan (five month after operation) shows total removal of the tumor.
A B
Fig. 20.20. (A) Sagittal T1-weight gadolinium-enhanced MRI in a 31-year-old male patient, shows a pineal tumor.
(B) Postoperative MRI after removal of a germinoma. The tumor was removed by the infratentorial
supracerebellar approach.
Autopsies of such cases have usually demonstrated a lesion that destroys the left
visual cortex, and underlining white matter, particularly the geniculocalcarine tract, as
well as the connection of the right visual cortex with the intact language areas of the
dominant hemisphere.
In the case originally described by Déjérine (1892), the disconnection occurred in
the posterior part (splenium) of the corpus callosum, wherein lie the connection between
the visual association areas of the two hemispheres.
1552
20. Corpus callosum
A B
Fig. 20.21. (A) Contrast enhanced axial CT-scan in a 27-year-old woman shows a medium-sized pineoblastoma.
(B) Postoperative CT-scan after total removal of the tumor.
More often, the callosal pathways are interrupted in the forceps major or in the
paraventricular region (Damasio and Damasio, 1983). In either event, the patient is blind
in the right half of each visual field by virtue of the left occipital lesion, and visual
information reaches only the right occipital lobe; however, this information cannot be
transferred, via the callosal pathways, to the cingular gyrus of the left (dominant)
hemisphere.
A lesion deep in the white matter of the left occipital lobe at its junction with parietal
lobe interrupts the projection from the intact visual cortex to the language areas but spares
the geniculocalcarine pathway (Greenblatt, 1973).
This lesion, coupled with the one in the splenium, prevents all visual information
from reaching the language areas, including the angular gyrus and Wernicke’s area.
In yet other cases, the lesion is confined to the cingular gyrus, or the subjacent white
matter. In such cases a right homonymous hemianopsia will also be absent but the alexia
may be combined with agraphia, with anomic aphasia and other elements of the
Gerstmann syndrome, i.e., right-left confusion, acalculia, and finger agnosia. The entire
constellation of symptoms is referred to as the syndrome of the angular gyrus.
Alexia, with agraphia, is an acquired inability to read and write. The lesion is
probably localised in the left angular gyrus of the parietal lobe (the left cerebral
hemisphere is dominant for language in 95% of humans), corresponding to Brodmann’s
area 39. Such patients can comprehend spoken words. Alexia without agraphia is a rare
and uncommon disorder, in which patients write fluently, but cannot read what they have
written. They cannot read words or letters, but can read numbers. The injury usually
involves the dominant medial occipital lobe. The intact right primary visual cortex has
probably lost its connection with the intact dominant visual language area, particularly
1553
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the angular gyrus. Patients see words, but cannot make the associations necessary for
their identification. Some patients are able to learn to trace letters with their fingers or
use ocular movements to identify these letters, and thus read, though slowly and
inefficiently. Thus, the visual interhemispheric transfer is done especially through the
splenium. A lesion of the splenium leads to alexia without agraphia (Geschwind, 1965).
After the posterior sectioning of the corpus callosum, although naming the stimuli
in the left visual field is not possible, there are many high-degree informational transfers
(Sidtis et al., 1981) as well as the possibility to integrate audio and visual information.
Cerebral vascular strokes that produce partial callous lesions can lead to a tactile anomia
and agraphia, which reveals the importance of fibres in the corpus callosum during
language transfer between the two hemispheres (Baynes and Gazzaniga, 1997).
Pandya and Seltzer (1986) highlighted in monkeys a tract for interparietal fibres
located just before the splenium (the isthmus of the corpus callosum), which, if sectioned,
is enough to lead to disorders of a crisscross tactile localisation (Volpe et al., 1982). This
area would also be critical for transferring audible information (Gazzaniga et al., 1975).
Complete callosotomy
When the corpus callosum is entirely sectioned at one sitting, the acute disconnection
syndrome appears in about all respects as it is seen after a complete cerebral commissurotomy
including the anterior commissure and massa intermedia (Bogen, 1987). This state after
callosotomy includes transient mutism, hence, when this symptom follows a complete section
it is not reasonably ascribed to the molestation of third ventricular structures.
Moreover, mutism does not necessarily follow a frontal commissurotomy that
includes molestation of the third ventricular structures but spares the splenium. Thus,
postcommissurotomy mutism requires an explanation on some other parts than third
ventricle retraction. Staying out of the third ventricle does avoid same problems, including
transient diabetes insipidus.
Experiments with monkeys have shown that if the anterior commissure is left intact
it can compensate for the loss of the splenium with respect to interhemispheric transfer
of certain kinds of visual information (Hamilton, 1982).
But the anterior commissure cannot compensate completely for splenial loss in
humans because hemialexia usually is present after splenial section.
Indeed, most of the stabilized syndromes seen after a complete cerebral commis-
surotomy are also seen (i.e., have not been compensated) after a callosotomy sparing the
anterior commissure (Gazzaniga and LeDoux, 1978; McKeever et al., 1981).
This is perhaps not surprising, because the anterior commissure is only 1/100 the
size of the corpus callosum. On the other hand, we can appreciate how significant it might
be when we consider the wealth of information that is conveyed over one optic nerve –
the diameter of which is about the same as that of the anterior commissure.
This question is complicated by the fact that the size of the anterior commissure is quite
variable; a diameter difference of 3 or 4 times has been reported (Yamamoto et al., 1981).
1554
20. Corpus callosum
With double field stimulation, only spots of light falling in the right visual field were
reported. In these subjects, the visual fields stopped exactly in the midline and no macular
sparing was evident.
Thus, it would appear that no visual information can be transferred from one
hemisphere to the other after a section of the corpus callosum.
Certain lesions involving portions of the corpus callosum, or association areas of
the cortex which give rise to commissural fibres, produce disturbances of higher brain
functions collectively recognized as the disconnection syndrome (Geschwind, 1965,
1965a, 1970).
Word blindness without agraphia presumably results from lesions which interrupt
fibres from the visual association area which cross in the splenium of the corpus callosum
and project to the left angular gyrus.
Pure word deafness may result from subcortical lesions located in the left temporal
lobe and which interrupt the left auditory radiation as well as callosal fibres from the
contralateral auditory region. A similar syndrome manifested by various forms of agnosia
or apraxia may result from lesions involving portions of the corpus callosum and the
association fibre system (Carpenter, 1979).
Thus, after sectioning the corpus callosum, the two hemispheres are independent;
each receives sensory input from all sensory systems, and each can control the muscles
of the body, but the two hemispheres can no longer communicate with each other.
Because the functions in these separate cortices, or “split brain“, are thus isolated,
sensory information can be presented to one hemisphere and its function can be studied
without the other hemisphere having access to the information.
So, information seen in a particular part of the visual world by both eyes is sent to
only one hemisphere: input from the left side of the world (the left visual field) goes to
the right hemisphere, whereas input from the right side of the world (the right visual
field) goes to the left hemisphere. The two sides of the world are joined by a connection
thorough the corpus callosum.
With the corpus callosum severed, the brain cannot relate the different views of the
left and right hemispheres.
When the left hemisphere of a split-brain patient has access to information, it can
initiate speech and hence communicate about the information. The right hemisphere
apparently has reasonably good recognition abilities but is unable to initiate speech,
because it lacks access to the speech mechanisms of the left hemisphere (Kolb and
Whishaw, 2003).
The special capacities of the right hemisphere in facial recognition can also be
demonstrated in split-brain patients.
Levy et al. (1972) devised the chimerical figures test, which consists of pictures of
faces and other patterns that have been split down the centre and recombined in
improbable ways. When the recombined faces were presented selectively to each
hemisphere, the patients appear to be unaware of gross discordance between the two sides
of the pictures. When asked to pick out the picture that they had seen, they chose the face
1556
20. Corpus callosum
seen in the left visual field (that is, by their right hemisphere), demonstrating that the
right hemisphere has a special role in the recognition of face.
Thus, a surgical section of the corpus callosum cuts off direct interhemispheric
communication (Bogen, 1985; Seymour et al., 1984; Baynes and Gazzaniga, 2000). When
examined by special neuropsychological techniques (Zaidel et al., 1990), patients who
have undergone a section of commissural fibres (commissurotomy) exhibit profound
behavioral discontinuities between perception, comprehension, and response, which
reflect significant functional differences between the hemispheres.
Probably because direct communication between two cortical points occurs less
frequently than indirect communication relayed through lower brain centres, especially
the thalamus and the basal ganglia, these patients generally manage to perform everyday
activities quite well, including tasks involving interhemispheric information transfer
(Myers and Sperry, 1985; Zaidel et al., 1990; Sergent, 1990, 1991) and emotional and
conceptual information not dependent on language or complex visuospatial processes
(Cronin-Golomb, 1986). In noting that alertness remains unaffected by commissurotomy
and that emotional tone is consistent between the hemispheres, Sperry (1990) suggested
that both phenomena rely on bilateral projections through the intact brain stem.
In sum, the results of careful studies of patients with commissurotomies provide
clear evidence of the complementary specialization of the two cerebral hemispheres. It
must be recognized, however, that, as interesting as these patients are, they represent only
a very small population and their two hemispheres are by no means normal.
The right hand is superior in drawing fine, detailed featural information, and the left
for the overall gestaltic interrelationships and general outline, in a manner analogous to
the left - right dissociations noted in a constructional apraxia.
Similar hand differences appeared with respect to strategies adapted in tactually matching
a viewed, unfolded, three-dimensional object: this led to the general conclusion that the right
hemisphere has the synthetic ability to integrate unrelated parts into a meaningful whole,
whereas the left hemisphere specializes in the analysis of component features.
Levy and Trevarthen (1976) concluded that each hemisphere has its own cognitive
strategy, the right hemisphere synthesizing over space, the left hemisphere analyzing over time.
Callosal agenesis
Occasionally, individuals may be born with partial or complete absence of the corpus
callosum. More and more instances of such “experiments of nature” are emerging with
the development of new imaging techniques (Jeeves, 1990).
In large autopsy series their incidence is of 5.5% - 26 % (Jellinger et al., 1981).
However, its clinics and pathogenicity remain partially obscure. Rohmer et al. (1960)
claim that, although the agenesis of the corpus callosum had been described for the first
time by Reil in 1812, its pathogenicity is little known.
Callosal deficits in such cases vary in degree, depending on the time after conception
that the presumed causative insult occurred.
1557
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 20.22. Sagittal MRI, shows the posterior half agenesia of the corpus callosum.
Andermann et al. (1994) and Wisniewski and Jeret (1994) support the importance of
the age-gestation factor, because the size of the malformation seems to be related to the
moment in which the damaging factors enter in the course of foetal development. Thus,
interrupting the development itself between the end of the first gestation month and the
beginning of the forth month lead to a total absence of all neocortical commissures.
Interrupting the development during the fourth month leads to the agenesis of the
corpus callosum with preservation of the anterior commissure. On the other hand, the
development anomalies that appear after the forth month determine a partial agenesis,
which especially affects the splenium (Fig. 20.22).
The splenium or rostrum are more likely to be absent, and the anterior commissure
is usually still present, and is often hypertrophied due to the enclosure of heterotopic
callosal fibres. The anterior commissure in fact is important in the extensive functional
reorganization that seems to occur in such cases (Bradshaw and Mattingley, 1995).
During the development, there can only occur an imperfect commissure and not a real
agenesis, as the fibres of the corpus callosum are present, but instead of having a
transversal projection they form the margins of the internal facets of the two hemispheres.
Therefore, from an anatomical point of view, the agenesis of the corpus callosum
may be partial or total associated or not to craniofacial, either tumoural or constitutional
(hydrocephalus, lack of septum pellucidum, microgyria etc.) (Lassonde, 1994).
Clinical manifestations. Ten percent of such cases are quite asymptomatic, coming
to light by chance at necropsy, and other being recognized because of other neurological
problems.
The symptoms in a callous agenesis may be discovered in a patient with normal
intelligence or may be included in a major polimalformation syndrome. Because of this
1558
20. Corpus callosum
reason, different psychic disorders, mental debility, epilepsy etc. are related to the
simultaneous presence of an extracallous pathology. Totally asymptomatic cases are
discovered only in autopsy and are debatable.
However, many scientists tried to check for the presence of a disconnection syndrome,
but the results were negative (Rohmer et al., 1960; Jeeves, 1979; Saul and Sperry, 1968;
Sauerwein and Lassonde, 1983; Lassonde et al., 1981, 1984; Lassonde, 1994).
The absence of disconnection deficits led to more interpretation based on the use of
some compensatory mechanisms (Jeeves, 1986, 1991, 1994) such as: (1) bilateralization
of cerebral functions; (2) the use of subtle behavioral strategies (cross-cueing), that allow
the non-stimulated hemisphere to use proprioceptive markers (a movement) derived from
the answer provided by the other hemisphere. This ensures a bilateral distribution of
information (Gazzaniga, 1970); (3) the overuse of ipsilateral sensitive and motor
projection tracts, which allow each hemisphere to have a bimanual representation; (4)
the maximum use of residual commissures (anterior and subcortical) in order to ensure
an interhemispheric transfer.
The philogenetic, morphologic and functional characteristics of the anterior
commissure show that it represents a single possible alternative for an interhemispheric
transfer in case of a callous agenesis. In all situations in which this commissure is present
it forms the main functional system capable of compensating the deficits induced by the
absence of the corpus callosum (Guénot, 1998).
Moreover, there is a degree of anomalies in tests of interhemispheric transfer which
correlates in case of a callous agenesis with the presence or absence of the anterior
commissure which can sometimes be hypertrophic. Whichever the nature of the
compensatory mechanisms used by subjects with an agenesis of the corpus callosum,
they do not pass certain limits because the interhemispheric communication for these
people has decreased (Lassonde et al., 1990, 1996).
According to Jeeves (1979), Sauerwein et al. (1994), Sylver and Jeeves (1994), and
Lassonde et al. (1996), this decrease of interhemispheric commutation includes the
following: slower motor tests, if bimanual coordination is needed; mostly severe difficulty
of grabbing, well observed at the beginning of school age; slowness in performing certain
visual tactile transfer tasks; difficulty in a crisscross localizing test for a tactile stimulus;
difficulty of unimanual transfer (Geffen et al., 1994); deficit of median fusion observed at
a discrimination test of two tactile stimulation points (Schiavetto et al., 1993); and deficit
in the evaluation of distance between visual stimuli (Lassonde et al., 1981; Jeeves, 1991);
and disorders of the ability to locate sound within the entire audio-field (Poirier et al., 1993).
Problems of phonologic analysis and video-constructive deficits have also been
observed in children with normal intelligence (Temple and Ilsley, 1994). These deficits
observed in persons with an agenesis of the corpus callosum are also seen in normal little
children, because of the myelinization processes in the corpus callosum, which for them
is not yet finished (Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967). As the growth process in the
commissural system is ending, these aptitudes improve progressively (Lassonde et al.,
1996).
1559
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Failure of the corpus callosum to develop (agenesis of the corpus callosum) may be
accompanied by an absence of the anterior and hippocampal commissures.
Although some patients with this condition may experience focal seizures and have
mental retardation, others live for many years with few or no obvious neurologic deficits.
These individuals frequently have developmental abnormalities in other parts of the
nervous system.
Some persons with agenesis of the corpus callosum (a rare congenital condition in which
the corpus callosum is insufficiently developed or absent altogether) are identified only when
some other condition brings them to a neurologist’s attention, as they normally display no
neurological or neuropsychological defects (Harris, 1995; Zaidel et al., 2003) other than
slowed motor performances, particularly of bimanual tasks (Lassonde et al., 1991).
Thus, persons who are born without a corpus callosum can perform interhemispheric
comparisons of visual and tactile information. The interpretation of these results is that
the patients have enhanced conduction in the remaining commissures (for example, for
vision) and that they develop enhanced abilities to use their few uncrossed projections
(for example, for tactile information) (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003)
There are a number of reports of poor transfer of information if stimuli are complex.
Furthermore, nonspecific deficits in task performance have been reported in these
patients.
Lassonde (1986) and Lassonde et al. (1991) presented pairs of stimuli to six patients
with agenesis of the corpus callosum, asking them if the pairs were the same or different.
Letters, numbers, colours or forms were used, either the pair was presented one on top
of the other in one visual field (interahemispheric task) or one stimulus was presented in
one visual field and the other stimulus in the other visual field (interhemispheric task).
The acallosal group was equally accurate in identifying same - different pairs under
both conditions.
Their reactions, however, were very slow for both forms of presentation. Lassonde
(1986) suggested that the callosum participates in hemispheric activation as well as in
the transfer of information.
Thus, the acallosal group has alternative ways of obtaining the interhemispheric
transfer of information but not of activation.
According to Jeeves (1986, 1990), one explanation of why language is lateralized
to one hemisphere is that it gets a start there and then that hemisphere actively inhibits
its development in the other hemisphere. In people with callosal agenesis, the opportunity
for such an inhibitory process to work is much reduced. Yet the lateralization of language
and other functions in most of these people is similar to that in the general population.
Thus, the corpus callosum and other commissures are not necessary for the development
of asymmetries.
They are clearly not indispensable, but may play the following roles (Jeeves, 1990):
1. Interhemispheric integration, updating, and information transfer;
2. Inhibition of ipsilateral pathways to avoid unnecessary competition;
3. Inhibition during development so as to facilitate hemispheric specialization.
1560
20. Corpus callosum
Plotkin, 1982; Hori, 1986). However, Zettner and Netsky (1960) believe that their
pathogenesis lies in faulty differentiation of primitive meninx tissue in the
intrahemispheric fissure, which may later interfere with the development of midline
structures. More than 50% of patients with intracranial lipomas present with seizures.
Almost 20% of patients have mentation defects (Clarici and Heppner, 1979). The
symptoms are usually due to obstructive hydrocephalus.
The management of an intracranial lipoma should generally be conservative. Only
in unusual cases does the tumor require debulking or removal. The tenacious attachments
to surrounding structures may account for the poor results.
The gliomas of the corpus callosum (glioblastomas, oligodendrogliomas and
astrocytomas) have a different symptomatology, which can be expressed even during the
first days through headaches, epileptic seizures, neurologic disorders (lesions of cranial
nerve, hemiparesis, walking disorders, and dysfunctions of the autonomous nervous
system) and psychological changes (impulsivity, apathy, spatial or temporal disorien-
tation, memory disorders etc.).
In this context, when the assembly in the callous territory and its neighbouring
regions are invaded by the tumoural process, occurs a syndrome of callous disconnection.
When there is a partial invasion of the corpus callosum, we find only some components
of the disconnection syndrome (Fig. 20.23). Frontal lobe gliomas tend to invade the
frontal lobe, crossing through the corpus callosum. Gliomas arising in the regions below
the corpus callosum are largely confined and tend to invade the basal structures, such as
the thalamus and peduncles along the corticospinal tracts.
T2-weight studies suggested that a larger proportion of tumors located close to the
corpus callosum invade this white matter tract.
The spread of malignant gliomas through all the white matter tracts (i.e., corpus
callosum, uncinate fasciculus, fasciculus longitudinalis and occipitofrontalis, auditive
and visual bundle, and corona radiata) has been documented (Matsukado et al., 1961).
The corpus callosum is the main white matter tract that leads to bilateral growth (Scherer,
1940; Salazar and Rubin, 1976). Grafted human glioma cells migrate along the glia
limitans, the basement membrane-lined blood vessels and Virchow-Robin space, the
subpial and subependymal space and the white matter fascicles of anatomic pathways,
including the corpus callosum, internal capsule, optic tract, and other parallel and
intersecting fibre tracts.
The intact vascular basement membrane is not penetrated by glioma cells (Bernstein
and Woodard, 1995).
Marchiafava-Bignami disease
In 1903, Marchiafava and Bignami described a curious syndrome of selective
demyelinisation of the corpus callosum in alcoholic Italians who indulged in large
quantities of red wine.
The disease seems to affect severe and chronic alcoholics in their middle or late life,
with a peak incidence between 40 and 60 years of age. A toxic cause, such as direct
toxicity of ethanol or other constituents, seems equally plausible. Clinical observations
of that three alcoholic patients were few and incomplete; in two of them, a chronic ill-
defined psychosis had been present, and, in all three, seizures and coma had occurred
terminally. In 1907, Bignami described a case in which the corpus callosum lesion was
accompanied by a similar lesion in the central portion of the anterior commissure.
These early reports were followed by a spate of articles that confirmed and amplified
the original clinical and pathological findings. About 40 cases of this disorder had been
described in the Italian literature (Mingazzini, 1922). A clinical diagnosis is often
difficult, because neurological presentation varies considerably. Many patients present
in terminal coma, which often preludes a premortem diagnosis. Mental and motor
slowing, other personality and behavior changes, incontinence, dysarthria, seizures, and
hemiparesis occur to a varying extent. The most common picture on neurological
evaluation is probably that of a frontal lobe or dementia syndrome.
A B
C D
Fig. 20.24. Conventional anteroposterior and lateral angiographic view (A, B) show a large arteriovenous
malformation that involve the anterior part of the corpus callosum, the cingulate girus and the right ventricular
ependyma. Postoperative (C, D) carotid angiography demonstrates complete resection of that malformation
with very good results.
from medial lenticulostriate arteries (Fig. 20.24). Blood supply also comes from
perforators of the anterior cerebral arteries and anterior communicating artery (Dănăilă
et al., 2010).
The approach uses a unilateral frontal craniotomy extending to the midline. The
subfrontal approach allows identification and division of the feeding vessels from the
anterior communicating complex and the anterior cerebral arteries. This can be followed
by an anterior interhemispheric approach to follow A2 vessels distally.
Thus, these lesions can be resected by skeletonizing the pericallosal arteries as they
pass through the lesion and taking only the side branches to the malformation. The
dissection of the medial and intraventricular plane of the lesion is completed using an
approach through the corpus callosum (Fig. 20.25).
Fig. 20.25. Preoperative (A, B, C) carotid angiography of a large interhemispheric arteriovenous malformation. This
was approached through a large right frontoparietal craniotomy. Postoperative (D, E, F) carotid angiography
demonstrates complete resection of that arteriovenous malformation. Postoperative evolution was excellent. ►
1564
20. Corpus callosum
A B
E F
1565
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The AVMs of the splenium are supplied primarily by pericallosal branches of the
posterior cerebral artery and distal pericallosal branches of the anterior cerebral artery as
well as by medial and lateral choroid branches of the posterior cerebral artery. Drainage
is usually into the internal cerebral vein and the vein of Galen (Fig. 20.26).
The surgical approach is parasagittal interhemispheric from the opposite side with
division of the falx to have a more direct access to the lateral aspect of the lesion.
The lateral extent of the lesion is defined by splitting the splenium in the direction
of the fibres.
A B
C D
1566
20. Corpus callosum
E F
Fig. 20.26. Preoperative (A, B, C) carotid angiography of a splenial arteriovenous malformation. These AVMs of the
splenium were supplied primarily by distal pericallosal branches of the anterior cerebral artery as well as by medial
and lateral choroidal branches of the posterior cerebral artery. Drainage is into the internal cerebral vein and vein of
Galen. The surgical approach was parasagittal interhemispheric from the right (opposite side with division of the falx
to have more direct access to the left aspect of the lesion. Postoperative (D, E, F), carotid angiography demonstrates
complete resection of the malformation. The evolution of the patient was excellent.
Summary
The loss of the ability to transfer information from the left hemisphere to the right
hemisphere and vice-versa seems to have no impact on their overall psychological state.
However the question of the perceived unity of the world continues to interest scientists.
The most spectacular finding in that respect has been the discovery that the corpus callosum
can be cut in humans without changing the perceived unity of the world and of the self.
According to Baars (2007), the role of the two hemispheres in human cognition and
mind-brain identity has been the subject of extensive studies, and we are still unfolding
the subtle and not so subtle differences in the roles that the mirror-image hemispheres
play in perception, language, thought, and consciousness.
For many years such callosotomies were performed to improve intractable epilepsy.
This surgical procedure has proved to be effective in preventing seizures from being
transferred from one cerebral hemisphere to the other (Fig. 20.27).
It was the introduction to lateralized procedures in early studies that led to that unique
research opportunity presented by split-brain patients. Thus, more careful studies have
provided evidence that a complete callosotomy does have subtle but long lasting effects.
Because of this, a partial resection (callosectomy) is preferred.
There are some hemispheric differences that are fairly well understood, such as
crossover wiring. Many aspects of sensory and motor processing entail the crossing over
of input (sensory) or output (motor) information from the left side to the right, and vice-
versa (Fig. 20.28).
1567
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 20.27. The symptoms of callous disconnection and the location of the responsible lesions: 1. The fibres that pass
through the rostrum and through the ventral area of the genu come from the ventral and medial region of the
prefrontal cortex; 2. The arched fibres of the genu and the cingulate fibres that pass underneath the cortex of the
cingulate gyrus are connected in the posterior area with the septal nucleus; 3. The fibres of the anterior half of the
corpus callosum are destined to the fronto-frontal tracts; their lesion leads to the apraxia of the left hand without
agraphia; 4. Fibres that connect the somato-sensorial cortex; their lesions lead to the left tactile anomia; 5. Fibres that
connect the supramarginal gyrus; their lesions leads to the left ideomotor apraxia; 6. Fibres that connect the lateral
temporal cortex; their lesions leads to a dichotic extinction in the left ear; 7. Fibres that connect the superior parietal
lobe; their lesions leads to a diagnostic dyspraxia; 8. Fibres that connect the superior parietal lobe; their lesions leads
to constructive disorders of the right hand and tactile anomia of the left hand; 9. Fibres that connect the angular gyrus;
their lesions leads to agraphia of the left hand; 10. Fibres that connect the prestriate cortex; their lesions leads
to left visual anomia and left hemialexia.
Only the olfactory nerve, which is a very ancient sensory system, stays on the same
side of the brain on its way to the cortex.
The time lag between the two hemispheres working on the same task may be as short
as 10 ms, or one-hundredth of a second (Handy et al., 2003).
Therefore, when the great information highway of the corpus callosum is intact, the
differences between the hemispheres are not very obvious. But when it is cut, and the
proper experimental controls are used to separate the input of the right and left half of
each eye’s visual field, suddenly major hemispheric differences become observable
(Baars, 2007).
However, tactual testing of the right hand showed no significant impairment of the
dominant but disconnected left hemisphere. Similar testing of the left hand indicated
severe agnosia, anomia, and agraphia. Also, in tachistoscopic presentations, the results
showed no abnormality in response to stimuli presented to the right visual field, but the
patient was severely impaired with stimuli presented to the left visual field (Gazzaniga
and Miller, 2009).
Severing the entire callosum blocks, the interhemispheric transfer of sensory, motor,
perceptual, gnostic, and other forms of information in such a way that it allows us to
study hemispheric differences and the unique ways in which the hemispheres interact
with each other (Zaidel, 1990; Gazzaniga, 2000; 2005).
The most obvious hemispheric difference is the capacity to learn language. It had
been known prior to split-brain research that the left hemisphere is dominant for language
and, particularly, speech production (Broca, 1865; Wernicke, 1874).
1568
20. Corpus callosum
The left hemisphere is also specialized in written language. And, while the right
hemisphere does have a limited capacity for reading and is able to read whole words
(ideographic lexical / semantic access), it is unable to convert graphemes to phonemes as
can the language-dominant left hemisphere (Zaidel and Peters, 1981; Zaidel et al., 1985).
Gazzaniga and Miller (2009) have a patient who generates spoken language
exclusively from the left hemisphere, and the written language exclusively from the right
hemisphere.
Previously, it had been assumed that spoken and written language shared the same
neural mechanism but this patient demonstrates that the output of these two functions
can be operated by independent modules (Baynes et al., 1998).
As much as cognitive styles and personal tastes and habits might seem to reflect the
processing characteristics of one or the other hemisphere, these qualities appear to be
integral to both hemispheres (Arndt and Berger, 1978; Sperry et al., 1979). “In the normal
intact state, the conscious activity is typically a unified and coherent bilateral process
that spans both hemispheres through the commissures“ (Sperry, 1976).
1569
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
However, very few tasks rely exclusively on one hemisphere. The interaction between
hemispheres has important mutually enhancing effects. Complex mental tasks such as
reading, arithmetic, and word and object learning are performing best when both hemispheres
can be actively engaged (Rey, 1959; Moscovitch, 1979; Huettner et al., 1989; Gaillard, 1990;
Belger and Banich, 1998; Weissman and Banich, 2000; Dănăilă and Golu, 2006).
Other mutually enhancing effects of bilateral processing show up in the superior
memorizing and retrieval of both verbal and configurational material when
simultaneously processed (encoded) by the verbal and configurational system (Milner,
1978; Moscovitch, 1979); in enhancing cognitive efficiency of normal subjects when
hemispheric activation is bilateral rather than unilateral (Berger and Perret, 1986; Berger
et al., 1987); and in better performances of visual tasks by commissurotomized patients
when both hemispheres participate than when vision is restricted to either hemisphere
(Zaidel, 1979; Sergent, 1991 a, b).
The cerebral processing of music illuminates the differences in how each hemisphere
contributes, the complexities of hemispheric interactions, and how experience can alter
hemispheric roles.
The left hemisphere tends to predominate in the processing of sequential and
discrete tonal components of music (Gaede et al., 1978; Breitling et al., 1987; Botez
and Botez, 1996).
The inability to use both hands to play a musical instrument (bimanual instrument
apraxia) has been reported with left hemisphere lesions that spare motor functions
(Benton, 1977).
The right hemisphere predominates in melody recognition and in melodic singing
(Gordon and Bogen, 1974; Yamadori et al., 1977; Samson and Zatorre, 1988; Kumkova,
1990; Dănăilă and Golu, 2006 ). Its involvement with chord analysis is generally greatest
in musically untrained persons (Gaede et al., 1978). Training can alter these hemispheric
biases so that, for musicians, the left hemisphere predominates for melody recognition
(Bever and Chiarello, 1974; Messerli et al., 1995), tone discrimination (Shanon, 1981;
Mazziota et al., 1982), and musical judgments (Shanon, 1980, 1984).
Moreover, intact, untrained persons tend most to show lateralised effects for tone
discrimination or musical judgments (Shanon, 1980, 1981, 1984). Taken altogether, these
findings suggest that while cerebral processing of different components of music is
lateralized with each hemisphere predominating in certain aspects, both hemispheres are
needed for musical appreciation and performance (Bauer and McDonald, 2003).
The bilateral integration of cerebral function is most clearly exhibited by creative
artists, who typically have intact brains. Excepting singing, harmonica playing, and the
small repertoire of piano pieces written for one hand, making music is a two-handed activity.
Moreover, for instruments such as guitars and the entire violin family, the right hand
performs those aspects of music that are mediated predominantly by the right hemisphere,
such as expression and tonality, while the left hand interprets the linear sequence of notes
best deciphered by the left hemisphere.
Thus, by its very nature, the artist’s performance involves the smoothly integrated
activity of both hemispheres (Lezak et al., 2004).
1570
20. Corpus callosum
recognize faces and it can perform just as well as the right with familiar faces, but the
right hemisphere is superior in its ability to perceive and recognize unfamiliar faces.
However, while both hemispheres can generate spontaneous facial expression, only
the dominant left hemisphere can generate voluntary facial expression (Gazzaniga and
Smylie, 1990).
The expected right-hemispheric superiority in visuospatial functions has been
demonstrated in callosotomy patients. The use of tactile information to built spatial
representation of abstract shapes also appears to be better developed in the right
hemisphere (Milner and Taylor, 1972).
Although the right hemisphere demonstrates superior levels of performance on a
variety of perceptual and spatial tasks, the left hemisphere appears to have at least some
competence in most areas and in some cases is essential for the solution of complex visual
problems.
Verbal IQ appears to be stable following callosotomy, although performance IQ may
decline (Zaidel, 1990).
However, the cognitive functioning of each hemisphere is quite redundant.
Both hemispheres are quite capable of encoding and retrieving of past events, despite
a significant theory in the 1990s on the brain region underlying episodic memory function
based on neuroimaging works. The theory, called HERA (hemispheric encoding /
retrieval asymmetry), stipulated that episodic encoding was predominantly a left
hemisphere process while episodic retrieval was predominantly a right hemisphere
process (Tulving et al., 1994). The asymmetries observed in neuroimaging studies were
based on the fact that most studies at the time used verbal material. Miller et al. (2002)
found in split-brain patients that manipulating the encoding of words led to greater effects
in the left hemisphere than the right, and that manipulating the encoding of faces led to
greater effects in the right hemisphere than in the left.
Since that split-brain study, recent neuroimaging studies have confirmed that
encoding and retrieval asymmetries are primarily due to the type of stimuli used and not
the general hemispheric differences in processing (Wig et al., 2004).
Despite some unique specialization in the right hemisphere, breaking up the right
hemisphere is not hard to do for the left hemisphere.
Although some effects due to the callosotomy surgery have been observed (e.g.,
Phelps et al., 1991, found impairments in free recall but not in recognition performance
on some memory tasks that may be due to limitations in strategic and search processes
imposed by the severing of the corpus callosum), most functions remain intact after the
right hemisphere is disconnected from the left, including verbal IQ (Nass and Gazzaniga,
1987; Zaidel, 1990), and many problem-solving skills (LeDoux et al., 1977). However,
in some cases, problems with higher order cognitive processes such as concept formation,
reasoning, and problem solving with limited social insight have been observed (Brown
and Paul, 2000).
The disconnection does not seem to affect another critical component of our
conscious system, the “interpreter” (Gazzaniga and Miller, 2009).
1572
20. Corpus callosum
Conclusions
Some authors consider the activity of the corpus callosum to have an inhibitory role
(Eidelberg, 1969; Jeeves, 1986). It allows each hemisphere to apply an inhibiting action
on its homologous, with the purpose of controlling a given function. This reciprocal
inhibition avoids the duplication of functions (Dennenberg, 1981). The inhibiting model
was suggested in order to explain the development of a domination of the contralateral
tract over the ipsilateral fibres in motor and sensitive systems (Dennis, 1976).
However, the inhibiting role of the corpus callosum in the ontogenesis of hemi-
spheric specialization has not yet been confirmed (Lassonde et al., 1990; Jeeves, 1994).
In the context of epilepsy pathogenesis, it was common to mention that the callous
tract had a facilitating action (Bremer, 1966; Geoffroy et al., 1986). This affirmation
supports the fact that a section of the corpus callosum determines not only the abolishment
of bilateral propagation, but also a decrease or ceasing of epileptic discharge in the initial
focal (Bogen and Vogel, 1962; Geoffroy et al., 1986; Lassonde et al., 1996). This is due
to the fact that influxes from the intact hemisphere would moderate the activity of
abnormal cells and the propagation of epileptic discharge to the healthy hemisphere
through retroaction (Bremer, 1966, 1967; Lassonde, 1986).
On the other hand, clinical observations show that the corpus callosum is
significantly involved in a functional reorganizing after a cerebral aggression. Thus,
speech recovery, observed in patients with left hemispheric lesions is severely limited if
produced at the same time as a lesion of the corpus callosum (Goldstein, 1948; Russel,
1963). As an assembly, these data suggest that the intact hemisphere could facilitate a
better function of the damaged hemisphere through the effect of the corpus callosum
(Lassonde et al., 1996).
In the case of patients with unilateral lesions and bilateral symptoms, the reciprocal
transcallous facilitation may disrupt a well functioning intact hemisphere.
However, it is highly probable that the normal interhemispheric regulating process
depends both on the inhibiting influences and on the stimulating ones (Lassonde et al.,
1996). In this context, Berlucchi (1983) claims that the function of the corpus callosum
is to equilibrate the activity of the two hemispheres and to allow an optimal integration
of cortical activity.
The callous disconnection has an important role in the explanation of syndromes
such as apraxia of limbs, pure alexia and unilateral agraphia.
The consequences of a callous lesion in adults are at the same time complex but
relatively discrete. At first sight, they are not a major handicap and are compatible with
a normal life and daily activity, if not associated with hemispheric lesions (Habib, 1996),
ideomotor apraxia, left unilateral anomia and agraphia, right constructive apraxia,
hemialexia and other manifestations given by a deficit of informational transfer between
hemispheres that can pass unnoticed if not specifically searched for.
Consequently, even though after a callosotomy the behavior is almost unchanged,
investigations revealed the capacity of each hemisphere and confirmed the hypothesis
based on studies conducted on normal patients as well as those based on studies conducted
on patients with focal lesions.
1573
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
1574
20. Corpus callosum
REFERENCES
Aboitiz F, Scheibel AB, Fisher RS, Zaidel E, Fiber composition of the human corpus callosum.
Brain Res 598; 143-153, 1992.
Adams RD, Victor M, Ropper AH, Principles of Neurology. Sixth Edition. McGraw-Hill, New
York, St Louis, San Francisco etc.; 483, 1997.
Akelaitis AJ, Studies on the corpus callosum: Higher visual functions in each homonymous field
following complete section of the corpus callosum. Arch Neurol Psychiatr 45; 788, 1941.
Akelaitis AJ, Studies on the corpus callosum. VII. Study of language function (tactile and visual
lexia and graphia) unilaterally following section of the corpus callosum. Journal of
Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 2; 226-262, 1943.
Akelaitis AJ, A study of gnosis, praxis, and language following section of the corpus callosum
and anterior commissure. J Neurosurg 7; 94-102, 1944.
Akelaitis AJ, Studies of the corpus callosum. IV. Diagnostic dyspraxia in epileptics following
partial and complete section of the corpus callosum. Am J Psychiatr 101; 594-599, 1945.
Alexander MP, Warren RL, Localization of callosal auditory pathways: a CT case study.
Neurology 38; 802-804, 1988.
Alpers BJ, Grant FC, The clinical syndromes of the corpus callosum. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 25;
67-86, 1931.
Andermann E, Andermann F, Nagy R, et al., Genetic studies of the Andermann syndrome. In:
Lassonde, M, Jeeves MA (eds), Callosal Agenesis – A Natural Split Brain? Advances in
Behavioral Biology. New York, London: Plenium Press; 31-38, 1994.
Anderson RC, Ghaton S, Feldstein NA, Surgical approaches to tumors of the lateral ventricle.
Neurosurg. Clin N Am 14; 509-525, 2003.
Antunes JL, Louis, Ganti SR, Coloid cysts of the third ventricle. Neurosurgery 7; 450-455, 1980.
Apuzzo MLJ, Chikovani OK, Gott PS, et al., Transcallosal, interfornicial approaches for lesions
affecting the third ventricle: Surgical considerations and consequences. Neurosurgery 10;
547-554 1982.
Arndt S, Berger DE, Cognitive mode and asymmetry in cerebral functioning. Cortex 14; 78-86,
1978.
Assal F, Schwartz S, Vuilleumier P, Moving with or without will: functional neural correlates of
alien hand syndrome. Ann Neurol. 62; 301-307, 2007.
Augustine JR, Human Neuroanatomy. An introduction. Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston,
Heidelberg, etc., 2008.
Baars BJ, The brain. In: VJ Baars and NM Gage (eds). Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness.
Introduction to cognitive Neuroscience. Academic Press. Elsevier. Amsterdam, Boston
Heidelberg etc., 121-146, 2007.
Barbizet J, Duizabo P, Les syndromes de dysconexion interhemispheriques. Dans : Abrégé de
Neuropychologia. 3e édition révisés. Paris: Masson: 115-126, 1985.
Bauer RM, McDonald, Auditory agnosia and amusia. In: TE Feinberg and MJ Farah (eds),
Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 2003.
Baynes K, Gazzaniga MS, Callosal disconnection. In: TE Feinberg and MJ Farah (eds), Behavioral
Neurology and Neuropsychology McGraw Hill. New York, St Louis, San Francisco, etc. 419-
425, 1997.
1575
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Baynes K, Gazzaniga MS, Consciousness, introspection, and the split brain: the two minds / one
body problem. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed), Cambridge;
MA; MIT Press, 2000.
Baynes K, Tramo MJ, Gazzaniga MS, Reading with a limited lexicon in the right hemisphere of
a callosotomy patient. Neuropsychologia 30; 187-200, 1992.
Baynes K, Funnell MG, Fowler CA, Hemispheric contributions to the integration of visual and
auditory information in speech perception. Percept Psychophys 55; 633-641, 1994.
Baynes K, Wessinger CM, Fendrich R, Gazzaniga MS, The emergence of the capacity to name
left visual field stimuli in a callosotomy patient implications for functional plasticity.
Neuropsychologia 33; 1225-1242, 1995.
Baynes K, Eliassen JC, Lutsep H, Gazzaniga MS, Modular organization of cognitive systems
masked by interhemispheric integration. Science 280; 902-905, 1998.
Belger A, Banich MT, Costs and benefits of integrating information between the cerebral
hemispheres : A computational perspective. Neuropsychology 12; 380-398, 1998.
Benson DF, Alexia. In: Vinken PJ, Bruyn CW (eds). Handbook of Clinical Neurology.
Amsterdam, Elsevier – Biomedical, 1985.
Bentin S, Sahar A, Moscovitch M, Intermanual information transfer in patients with lesions in
the trunk of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychology 22; 601-611, 1984.
Benton AL, The amusias, In: M Critchley and RA Henson (eds), Music and the brain. London:
William Heinemann, 1977.
Benton A, Exploring the History of Neuropsychology. Selected Papers. Oxford University Press, 2000.
Berger JM, Perret E, Interhemispheric integration in a surface estimation task. Neuropsychologia
24; 743-746, 1986.
Berger JM, Perret E, Zimmermann A, Interhemispheric integration of compound nouns: Effects
of stimulus arrangement and mode of presentation. Perceptual and Motor Skills 65; 663-671,
1987.
Berlucchi G, Anatomical and physiological aspects of visual functions of the corpus callosum.
Brain Res 37; 371-392, 1972.
Berlucchi G, Two hemispheres but one brain, Behav Brain Sci 6; 173-179, 1983.
Berlucchi G, Commissurotomy studies in animal. In: Boller F and Grafman J (eds), Handbook of
Neuropsychology. Vol 4. Elsevier. Amsterdam; 9-49, 1990.
Berlucchi G, Filipo C, Di Stefano M, Tassinari G, Influence of spatial stimulus response
compatibility on reaction time of ipsilateral and contralateral hand to lateralized light stimuli.
Human perception and performance. J Exper Psychol 3; 505-517, 1977.
Bernstein JJ, Woodard CA, Glioblastoma cells do not introvasate into blood vessels. Neurosurgery
36; 124-132, 1995.
Bever TG, Chiarello RJ, Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians. Science 185; 537-
539, 1974.
Bignami A, Sulle alteragione del corpo calloso e della commissura anteriore ritrovate in un
alcoolista. Policlinico (sei part) 14; 460, 1907.
Biran I, Chatterjee A, Alien hand syndrome. Arch Neurol 61; 292-294, 2004.
Bodamer J, Die Prosopagnosie. Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr 179; 6, 1947.
Bogen J, Cerebral commissurotomy: a second case report. JAMA 194; 1328-1329, 1965.
Bogen JE, The other side of the brain: I. Dysgraphia and dyscopia following cerebral
commissurotomy. Bull Los Angeles Neurp Soc 34; 73-105, 1969.
Bogen JE, Hemispherectomy and the placing reactions in cats. In: Kinsbourne M, Smith WL (eds);
Hemispheric Disconnection and Cerebral Function. Springfield, IL, Charles C Thomas, 1974.
1576
20. Corpus callosum
Bogen JE, Linguistic function in the short term following cerebral commissurotomy. In: Avakian
-Whitaker H, Whitaker HA (eds), Studies in Neurolinguistics. Vol. 2. New York. Academic
Press; 193-224, 1976.
Bogen JE, The callosal syndrome. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E (eds), Clinical Neuropsychology.
New York, Oxford University Press; 308-359, 1978.
Bogen JE, Split-brain syndrome. In: PJ Vinken, GW Bruyn, and HI Klawans (eds), Handbook of
clinical neurology. New York : Elsevier, 1985 a.
Bogen JE, The stabilized syndrome of hemisphere disconnection. In: Benson DF, Zaidel E (eds);
The Dual Brain: Hemispheric Specialization in the Human. New York. Guilford Press, 1985 b.
Bogen JE, Les callosal syndrome. In: KM Heilman, and E Valetein (eds), Clinical
Neuropsychology. New York: University Press; 308-359, 1985.
Bogen JE, Psychological Consequences of complete or partial commissural section. In: Apuzzo
LMI (ed.), Surgery of the Third Ventricle. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore; 175-194, 1987.
Bogen JE, Vogel PJ, Cerebral commissurotomy in man. Bull Los Angeles Neurol Soc 27; 169-
172, 1962.
Bogen JE, Gazzaniga MS, Cerebral commissurotomy in man: Minor hemisphere dominance for
certain visuospatial functions. J Neurosurg 23; 394-399, 1965.
Bogen JF, Bogen GM, The other side of the brain: The corpus callosum and creativity. Bull Los
Angeles Neurol Soc 34; 191-220, 1969.
Bogen JE, Campbell A, Thompson A, Long-term persistence of unilateral anomia following
complete cerebral commissurotomy. Proc Soc Neurosci 7; 945, 1981.
Botez M, Bogen JE, The grasp reflex of the foot and related phenomena in the absence of other
reflex abnormalities following cerebral commissurotomy. Acta Neurol Scand 54; 453-463,
1976.
Botez MI, Botez T, Les amusies. In : MI Botez (ed.), Neuropsychologie clinique et neurologie du
comportement (2eme ed). Montreal: Les Presses de l’Université du Montréal, 1996.
Bouchet A, Cuilleret J, Anatomice topographique descriptive et fonctionnelle. I. Le système
nerveux controle la face, la tête et les organes de sens. Lyon, Villeurbaine, Paris: Simep 194-
199, 1983.
Bradshaw JL, Nettleton NC, Human Cerebral Asymmetry. Englewood, Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall,
1983.
Bradshaw JL, Mattingley JB, Clinical Neuropsychology. Behavioral and Brain Science. Academic
Press. San Diego, Boston, New York etc. 187-205, 1995.
Breitling D, Guenther W, Rondot P, Auditory perception of music measured by brain electrical
activity mapping. Neuropsychologia 25; 765-774, 1987.
Bremer F, Étude électrophysiologique d’un transfert interhémisphérique callosal. Arch Ital Biol
104; 1-29, 1966.
Bremer F, La physiologie du corps calleux à la lumière de travaux recents. Laval Medical 38;
835-843, 1967.
Brion S, Jedynak CP, Sémiologie calleuse dans les tumeurs et les malformations vasculaire. Dans:
Michel F, Schott B (ed.), Les syndromes de disconexion calleuse chez l’homme. Lyon: Presses
de l’imprimérie JP: 253-265, 1975.
Brion S, Jedynak CP, Les troubles du transfer interhémisphérique. Congrès de Psychiatrie et de
Neurologie. Nîmes: Masson, Paris, 1975.
Broca P, Sur le siège de la faculté du language articulé. Bull Soc Anthrop (Paris) 6; 377-393,
1865.
1577
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Brodal A, Neurological Anatomy (3rd ed.) New York: Oxford University Press, 1981.
Brown WS, Paul LK, Cognitive and psychosocial deficits in agenesis of the corpus callosum with
normal intelligence. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 5; 135-137, 2000.
Cairns HR, Disturbances of consciousness with lesions of the brain stem and diencephalon. Brain
75; 109-146, 1952.
Campbell AL, Bogen JE, Smith A, Disorganization and reorganization of cognitive and
sensorimotor functions in cerebral commissurotomy: Compensatory roles of the forebrain
commissures. Brain 104; 493-511, 1980.
Carpenter MB, Human Neuroanatomy. Seventh Edition. The Williams and Wilkins Company.
Baltimore, 1979.
Clarici C, Heppner F, The operative approach to lipoma of the corpus callosum. Neurochirurgia
22; 77-81, 1979.
Clark CR, Geffen GM, Corpus callosum surgery and recent memory. Brain 112; 165-175, 1989.
Clarke JM, McCann CM, Zaidel E, The corpus callosum and language. Anatomical - behavioral
relationships. In: M Beeman and Chiarello (eds), Right hemisphere language comprehension.
Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 1998.
Colonier M, Notes on the early history of the corpus callosum with an introduction to the
morphological papers published in this festschrift. In: F Lepore, M Ptito and H H Jasper (eds).
Two Hemispheres – One Brain. New York : Liss, 1986.
Coltheart M, Deep Dyslexia: A right hemisphere hypothesis. In: Coltheart M, Patterson KF,
Marshall JC (eds): Deep Dyslexia. London; Routledge, 1980.
Corballis MC, Sergent J, Imagery in a commissurotomized patient. Neuropsychologia 26; 13-26,
1988.
Corballis PM, Funnell MG, Gazzaniga MS, A dissociation between spatial and identity matching
in callosotomy patients. Neuroreport 10; 2183-2187, 1999.
Corballis PM, Fendrich R, Shapley R, Gazzaniga MS, Illusory contours and amodal completion:
Evidence for a functional dissociation in callosotomy patients. J Cogn Neurosci 11; 459-466,
1999.
Coslett HB, Saffran EM, Evidence for presumed reading in “pure alexia”. Brain 112; 327-259, 1989.
Cronin-Golomb A, Subcortical transfer of cognitive information in subjects with complete
forebrain commissurotomy. Cortex 22; 499-519, 1986.
Cusik CG, Kaas JH, Interhemispheric connections of cortical sensory and motor representations
in primates. In: Lepore F, Ptito M, Jasper HH (eds), Two hemispheres: One Brain: Functions
of the corpus callosum. Alen R Liss. New York; 83-102, 1986.
Damasio A, Pure alexia. Trends Neurosci 6; 93-96, 1983.
Damasio H, Damasio AR, Paradoxic ear extinction in dichotic listening: possible anatomic
significance. Neurology 29; 644-653, 1979.
Damasio AR, Damasio H, The anatomic basis of pure alexia. Neurology 33; 1573-1583, 1983.
Damasio AR, Chui HC, Corbet J, Kassel N, Posterior callosal section in a nonepileptic patient. J
Neurosurg Psychiatr 43; 351-356, 1980.
Damasio AR, Damasio H, van Goesen GW, Prosopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavioral
mechanisms. Neurology 32; 331, 1982.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. II (Tratat de neuropsihologie) Editura
Medicala, Bucharest, 2006.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Tratat de neuropsihologie, Vol. 1, Bucureşti, editura Medicală, 2002.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Role of the interaction of the two cerebral hemispheres in the integration of
the language system. Rev Roum Sci Sociales - Série de Psychologie 31; 87-99 1987.
1578
20. Corpus callosum
Dănăilă L, Petrescu AD, Rădoi MP, Tumors of the third ventricle. The 7th national Congress of
Romanian Society of neurosurgery. Cluj-napoca, 28 Septembrie - 2 octombrie Abstracts PC
13, 2010.
Degas JD, Gray F, Louarn T, et al., Posterior callosal infarction. Clinico-pathological correlation.
Brain 110; 1155-1171, 1987.
Déjérine J, Contribution à l’étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de
cécité verbale. Mémoires de la Société de Biologie 4, 61-90, 1892.
Déjérine J, Roussy G, Le syndrome thalamique. Rev Neurol 12; 521-532, 1906.
Dennenberg VH, Hemispheric laterality in animals and the effects early experience. Behav Brain
Sci 4; 1-21, 1981.
Dennis M, Impaired sensory and motor differentiation with corpus callosum agenesis: a lack of
callosal inhibition during ontogeny? Neuropsychologia 14; 455-469 1976.
Desai KI, Nadkarni TD, Muzumdar DP, Goel AH, Surgical management of colloid cyst of the
third ventricle – a study of 105 cases. Surgical Neurology 57; 295-302, 2002.
Diamond SJ, Scammel RE, Brouwers EYM, Functions of the centre section (trunk) of corpus
callosum in man. Brain 100; 543-562, 1977.
Di Stefano MR, Sauerwein HC, Lassonde M, Influence of anatomical factors and spatial
compatibility on the stimulus response relationship in the absence of the corpus callosum.
Neuropsychologia 302; 177-185, 1992.
Downer JL, Changes in visual gnostic functions and emotional behavior following unilateral
temporal pole damage in the “split-brain” monkey. Nature 191; 50-51, 1961.
Duvernoy HM, The human hippocampus. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. Heidelberg, 1998.
Eidelberg E, Calosal and non-callosal connections between the sensory-motor cortices in cat and
monkey. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 26; 557-563, 1969.
Elberger AJ, The functional role of the corpus callosum in the developing visual system. A review.
Prog Neurobiol 18; 15-79, 1982.
Efron R, Bogen JE, Yung EW, Perception of dichotic chords by normal and commissurotomized
human subjects. Cortex 13; 137-149, 1977.
Ellenberg L, Sperry RW, Capacity for holding sustained attention following commissurotomy.
Cortex 15; 421-438, 1979.
Erickson TC, “Spread of Epileptic Discharge”, Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 43; 429-
452, 1940.
Eviator Z, Zaidel E, The effects of words length and emotionality on hemispheric contribution to
lexical discution. Neuropsychologia 29; 415-428, 1991.
Fabri M, Polonara M, De Pesce A, et al., Posterior corpus callosum and interhemispheric transfer
of somatosensory information: An fMRI and neuropsychological study of a partially
callosotomized patient. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13; 1071-1079, 2001.
Fantie B, Kolb B, Tasks traditionally used to assess callosal function reveal performance deficits
associated with mild head injury. Neuroscience Abstracts 15; 132, 1989.
Ferguson SM, Some neuropsychiatric observations on behavioral consequences of corpus callosum
section. In: Reeves A (ed): Epilepsy and the corpus callosum. New York Plenum, 1985.
Fergusson SM, Rayport M, Corrie WS, Neuropsychiatric observations on behavioral consequences
of corpus callosum section for seizure control. In: Reeves AG (ed), Epilepsy and the corpus
callosum. Plenum Press, New York; 501-514, 1985.
Forster BA, Corballis PM, Corballis MC, Effect of luminance on successiveness discrimination
in the absence of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia 38; 441-450, 2000.
1579
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Gazzaniga MS, Kutas M, Van Patten C, Fendrich R, Human callosal function: MRI – verified
neuropsychological functions. Neurology 39; 942-946, 1989.
Geffen G, Nilsson Quinn K, Teng EL, The effect of lesions of the corpus callosum on finger
localization. Neuropsychologia 23; 497-514, 1985.
Geffen GM, Nilsson J, Simpson DA, Jeeves MA, The development of interhemispheric transfer
of tactile information in cases of callosal agenesis. In: Lassonde M, Jeeves MA (eds), Callosal
Agenesis – A Natural Split Brain? Advances in Behavioral Biology. New York, London:
Plenium Press; 185-197, 1994.
Geoffroy G, Lassonde M, Jelisle F, Decarie M, Corpus callosotomy for control of intractable
seizures in children. Neurology 33; 891-897, 1983.
Geoffroy G, Lassonde M, Saurwein H, Decarie M, Ellectivenes of corpus callosotomy for control
of intractable epilepsy in children. In: Lepore F, Ptito M, Jasper HH (eds), Two Hemispheres:
One Brain. New York: Alan Less; 361-368, 1986.
Gerber SS, Platkin B, Lipoma of the corpus callosum. J Neurosurg 57; 281-285, 1982.
Gersh F, Damasio AR, Praxis and writing of the left hand may be served by different callosal
pathways. Arch Neurol 38; 634-636, 1981.
Geschwind N, Disconnection syndrome in animals and man. I. Brain 88; 237-294, 1965.
Geschwind N, Disconnection syndrome in animals and man. II. Brain 88; 585-644, 1965 a.
Geschwind N, The organization of language and the brain. Science 170; 940-944, 1970.
Geschwind N, Kaplan E, A human cerebral disconnection syndrome. Neurology 12; 675-685,
1962.
Geschwin N, Fusillo M, Color-naming defects in association with alexia. Arch Neurol 15; 137-
146, 1966.
Giovannetti T, Buxbaum L J, Biran I, Chatterjee A, Reduced endogenous control in alien hand
syndrome: Evidence from naturalistic action, Neuropsychologia 43; 75-88, 2005.
Goldberg G, Supplementary motor area structure and function. Review and hypotheses. Behav
Brain Sci 8; 567-616, 1985.
Goldberg G, Goodwin ME, Alien hand syndrome. In: JS Kreutzer, J DeLuca, B Caplan (eds),
Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer. New York, Dordrecht, Heidelberg,
London, Vol. 1; 84-91, 2011.
Goldstein K, Language and Language Disturbances. New York: Grüne and Straton, 1948.
Gordon HW, Sperry RW, Lateralization of olfactory perception in the surgically separated
hemispheres of man. Neuropsychologia 7; 111-120, 1968.
Gordon HW, Bogen JE, Sperry RW, Absence of disconnection syndrome in two patients with
partial section of the neocommissures. Brain 94; 327-336, 1971.
Gordon HW, Verbal and Nonverbal Cerebral Processing in Man for Audition. Thesis. California
Institute of Tehnology, 1973.
Gordon HW, Bogen JE, Hemispheric lateralization of singing after intracarotid sodium
amylobarbitone. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 37; 727-738, 1974.
Greenblatt SH, Alexia without agraphia or hemianopsia. Brain 96; 307-316, 1973.
Greenblatt SH, Saunders RL, Culver CM, Bogdanowicz W, Normal interhemispheric transfer
with incomplete section of the splenium. Arch Neurol 37; 567-571, 1980.
Guénot M, Transfer interhémisphérique et agénesie du corps calleux. Capacitées et limites de la
commissure antérieure. Neurochirurgie 1 (suppl 44); 113-115, 1998.
Guillemot JP, Richer L, Prevost L, et al., Receptive field properties of somatosensory callosal
fibers in the monkey. Brain Res 402; 293-302, 1987.
1581
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Guillemot JP, Lepore F, Prevost L, et al., Somatosensory receptive fields properties of fibres in
the rostral corpus callosum of the cat. Brain Research 441; 221-232, 1988.
Haaland KY, Delaney HD, Motor deficit after left or right hemisphere damage due to stroke or
tumor. Neuropsychologia 19; 17-27, 1981.
Habib M, Syndrome de déconexion calleuse et organisation fonctionnelle de corpus calleux chez
l’adulte. Neurochirurgie 1 (suppl 44) 102-109, 1998.
Habib M, Pelletier J, Neuroanatomie functionnelle des relations interhémisphériques. Aspects
théoriques et perspectives cliniques. I. organisation anatomo-fonctionnelle des connexions
calleuses. Rev Neuropsychol 4; 69-112, 1994.
Habib M, Ceccaldi M, Poncet M, Syndrome de deconnexion calleuse par infarctus jonctionnel
hémisphérique gauche. Revue Neurologique (Paris) 146; 19-24, 1990.
Hamilton CR, Mechanisms of interocular equivalence. In: Ingle D, Goodale M, Mansfield R (eds):
Advances in the Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press; 693-717, 1982.
Hampson E, Kimura D, Hand movement asymmetries during verbal and nonverbal tasks. Can J
Psychol 38; 102-125, 1984.
Handy TC, Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Cortical and subcortical contribution to the representation
of temporal information. Neuropsychologia 41; 1461-1473, 2003.
Hannequin D, Goulet P, Joanette Y, La contribution de l’hémisphère droit à la communication
verbale. Raport de Neurologie, LXXXVe Congrès de Psychiatrie et Neurologie de Langue
Française. Masson, Paris, 1987.
Harris LJ, The corpus callosum and hemispheric communication. An historical survey of theory
and research. In: FL Kitterle (ed.), Hemispheric communication; Mechanisms and models.
Hillsdale, NJ, Erlboaum, 1995.
Hécaen H, Assal G, Revues Critiques: les relations interhémisphériques et le problème de la
dominance cérebrale d’après les recherches sur les sections calleuses chez l’animal et chez
l’homme. Année Psychol 73; 491-522, 1973.
Heilman KM, Apraxia. In: Heilman KM and Valenstein (eds), Clinical Neuropsychology. Oxford
University Press. New York; 159-185, 1979.
Hommet CD, Billard C, Le syndrome calleux chez l’enfant. Neurochirurgie 1 (supl 44); 110-112,
1998.
Hoppe KD, Bogen JE, Alexinthymia in twelve commissurotomized patients. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics 28, 148-155, 1977.
Hori A, Lipoma of the quadrigeminal plate region with evidence of congenital origin. Arch Pathol
Lab Med 110; 851-951, 1986.
Huettner MIS, Rosenthal BL, Hynd GW, Regional cerebral blood flow ( fCBF ) in normal readers.
Bilateral activation with narrative text. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 4; 71-78, 1989.
Imig TJ, Reale RA, Brugge JE, et al., Topography of cortico-cortical connections related to
tonotopic and binaural maps of cat auditory cortex. In. Lepore F, Ptito M, Jasper HH (eds),
Two hemispheres: One Brain. New York, Alen Liss; 75-81, 1986.
Innocenti GM, What is so special about callosal connections? In Lepore F, Ptito M, Jasper HH
(eds), Two hemispheres: One Brain. New York, Alen Liss; 171-188, 1986.
Iwata M, Sugishita M, Toyokura Y, et al., Étude sur le syndrome de disconnexion visuo-linguale
après la transection du splenium du corps calleux. J Neurol Sci 23; 421-432, 1974.
Jason GW, Hemispheric asymmetries in motor function: I. Left-hemisphere specialization for
memory but not performance. Neuropsychologie 21; 35-45, 1983.
Jeeves MA, Some limits to interhemispheric integration in cases of callosal agenesis and partial
commissurotomy. In: Russel IG, Von Hof MW, Berlucchi G (eds), Structure and Functions
of Cerebral Commissures. Baltimore, University Park Press; 449-474, 1979.
1582
20. Corpus callosum
Jeeves MA, Callosal agenesis: Neuronal and developmental adaptations. In: Lepore F, Ptito M,
Jasper HH (eds), Two hemispheres: One brain. New York, Alen Liss, 403-422, 1986.
Jeeves MA, Agenesis of the corpus callosum. In: F Boller and J Grafman (eds), Handbook of
Clinical Neuropsychology. Vol. 4, North Holland. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 9-47, 1990.
Jeeves MA, Stereoperception in callosal agenesis and partial callosotomy. Neuropsychologia 29;
19-34, 1991.
Jeeves MA, Callosal agenesis: A natural Split Brain? Overview. In: Lassonde M, Jeeves MA
(eds), Advances in Behavioral Biology. New York, London: Plenum Press; 285-299, 1994.
Jellinger K, Gross H, Kaltenback E, Grisold W, Holoprosencephaly and agenesis of the corpus
callosum frequency of associated malformations. Octa Neuropathol 55; 1-10, 1981.
Kazuis S, Sawanda T, Callosal apraxia without agraphia. Ann Neurol 33; 401-403, 1993.
Kihlstrom JF, Klein SB, Self-knowledge and self-awareness. In: Snodgrass JD and Thompson RL
(eds). The Self Across Psychology: Self Recognition, Self Awareness, and the Self Concept. An
NY Acad Sci 818, New York: New York Academy of science; 5-17, 1997.
Kimura D, Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex 3; 163-178, 1967.
Kimura D, Archibold Y, Motor function of the left hemisphere. Brain 97; 337-350, 1974.
Kinsbourne M, The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychol 33; 193-201,
1970.
Kinsbourne M, The control of attention by interaction between the hemispheres. In: S Konblum
(ed), Attention and Performance. IV. Academic Press, London, 1973.
Klingler EL, Atlas cerebri humani. S. Karger (Suisse). New York, 1956.
Kolb B, Whishaw IQ, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Fifth Edition. Worth Publishers.
United stated of America, 2003.
Kreuter S, Kinsbournt M, Trevarthen C, Are deconnected cerebral hemispheres independent
channales? A preliminary study of the effect of unilateral loading on bilateral finger topping.
Neuropsychologia 10; 453-461, 1972.
Kumkova E, Memory for birds’ voices. Hemispheric specialization (abstract). Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Neuropsychology 12; 42, 1990.
Kumar S, Short-term memory for a non-verbal tactual task after cerebral commissurotomy. Cortex
13; 55-61, 1977.
Lashley KS, In search of the enfram. Symposium of the Society of Experimental Biology. 4; 454-
482, 1950.
Lassonde M, The facilitatory influence of the corpus callosum on intrahemispheric processing.
In: F Lepore, M Ptito, HH Jasper (eds), Two Hemispheres – One Brain. New York, Alen
Liss, 1986.
Lassonde M, Disconnection syndrome in callosal agenesis. In: Lassonde M, Jeeves MA (eds),
Callosal agenesis. A natural Split Brain? Advances in Behavioral Biology. New York,
London, Plenum Press, 275-284, 1994.
Lassonde M, Lortie J, Ptito M, Geoffroy G, Hemispheric asymmetry in callosal agenesis as
revealed by dichotic listening performance. Neuropsychologia 445-448, 1981.
Lassonde M, Ptito M, Laurencelle L, Étude tachistoscopique de la spécialization hémisphérique
chez l’agenésie du corpus callosum. Rev Can Psychol 35; 527-536, 1984.
Lassonde M, Sauerwein H, Geoffroy G, Decarie M, Effects of early and late transection of the
corpus callosum in children. Brain 109; 953-967, 1986.
Lassonde M, Ptito M, Lepore F, La plasticité du système calleux. Rev Can Psychol 44; 166-179,
1990.
1583
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Powell TPS, Certain aspects of the intrinsic organization of the cerebral cortex. In: O. Pompeiano
and CA Marson (eds). Brain mechanism of perceptual awareness and purposeful behavior.
Raven Press, New York; 1-19, 1981.
Preilowski BFB, Possible contribution of the anterior forebrain commissures to bilateral motor
coordination. Neuropsychologie 10; 267-277, 1972.
Preilowski B, Bilateral Motor interactions: Perceptual motor performance of partial and complete
„split-brain” patients. In: KJ Zulch, O Creutzfeldt, and GC Galbraith (eds), Cerebral
localization. Berlin and New York: Springer, 1975
Preilowski B, Intermanual transfer. Interhemispheric interaction and handedness in man and
monkey. In: Trevarthen CB (eds), Brain Circuits and Functions of the Mind. University Press.
Cambridge; 168-180, 1990.
Ptito M, Lepore F, Interocular transfer in cats with early callosal transection. Nature 301; 513-
515, 1983.
Purves SJ, Woadhurst WB et al., Result of anterior corpus callosum section in 24 patients with
medically intractable seizures. Neurology 38; 1194-1201, 1988.
Quin K, Geffen G, The development of tactile transfer of information. Neuropsychologia 24; 793-
804, 1986.
Ramackers G, Nijokiktjien C, Pediatric Behavioral. Neurology. Vol. 3: The child corpus callosum.
Amsterdam, Duyi Publication, 1991
Rayport M, Ferguson SM, Corrie WS, Outcomes and indications of corpus callosum section for
intractable seizure control. Appl Neurophysiol 46; 47-51, 1983.
Rayport M, Carrie WS, Ferguson SM, Results of two-stage corpus callosum section for seizure
control in clinically and electroencephalographically defined cases. In: Reeves A (ed);
Epilepsy and the Corpus Callosum. New York. Plenum, 1985.
Reeves A (ed), Epilepsy and the corpus callosum. New York, Plenum, 1985.
Reeves AG, Behavioral changes following corpus callosotomy. In Smith D, Treiman D, Trimble
M (eds). Advances in Neurology, Vol. 55, Raven Press, New York, 1991.
Reil JC, Mangel des Mittleren, Freyen. Theils des Balkens in Menschengehirn. Arch Physiol 11;
314-344, 1812.
Reuter-Lorenz PA, Baynes K, Modes of lexical access in the callosotomized brain. J Cogn
Neurososci 4; 155-164, 1992.
Rey A, Sollicitation de la mémoire de fixation par des mots et des objects presentés simultanément.
Archives de Psychologie 37; 126-139, 1959.
Risse GL, LeDoux JC, Springer SP, et al., The anterior commissure in man: Functional variation
in a multisensory system. Neuropsychologia 16; 23-31, 1978.
Risse GL, Gates J, Lund G, et al., Interhemispheric transfer in patients with incomplete section of
the corpus callosum: Anatomic verification with magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol
46; 437-443, 1989.
Rivest J, Mitchell T, Intriligator J, Perceptual learning in the visual and auditory systems with and
without attention. The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, 37, 180, 1996.
Rohmer F, Wackenheim A, Vrousos G, Les agénésies du corps calleux. In: Cossa P (ed), Comptes
rendus du Congrès de Psychiatrie et de Neurologie de langue française. LVII session. Tours:
Masson, 1960.
Roser ME, Fugelsang JA, Dunbar KN, et al., Dissociating causal perception and causal inference
in the brain. Neuropsychology 19; 591-602, 2005.
Ross ED, Stewart RM, Akinetic mutism from hypothalamic damage: Successful treatment with
1587
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Sidtis JJ, Volpe BT, Hotzman JD, et al., Cognitive interaction after staged callosal section:
Evidence for transfer of semantic activation. Science 212; 344-346, 1981.
Sparks R, Geschwind N, Dichotic listening in man after section of neocortical commissures.
Cortex 4; 3-16, 1968.
Sparks R, Goodglass H, Niskel B, Ipsilateral versus contralateral extinction in dichotic listening
from hemispheric lesions. Cortex 6; 249-260, 1970.
Spencer SS, Spencer DD, Williamson PD, et al., Corpus callosum for epilepsy. II. Neuro-
psychologicial outcome. Neurology 38; 24-28, 1988.
Sperry RW, Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion;
I. Camp Physiol Psychol 43; 482-489, 1950.
Sperry RW, Cerebral organization and behavior. Science 1933; 1949 -1757, 1961.
Sperry RW, Some general aspects of interhemispheric integration. In: VB Mountcastle (ed.).
Interhemispheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance. John Hopkins Press. Baltimore. Ch 3,
43-49, 1962.
Sperry RW, Lateral specialization in the surgically separated hemispheres. In: FO Schmitt and
FG Worden (eds), Neurosciences: Third Study Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1974.
Sperry RW, Changing concepts of consciousness and free will. Perspectives in Biology and
Medicine 20; 9-12, 1976.
Sperry RW, Consciousness, personal identity, and the divided brain. In: F Lepore, M Ptito, HH
Jasper (eds) Two hemispheres: One brain. New York, Alen Liss; 3-20, 1986.
Sperry RW, Forebrain commissurotomy and conscious awareness. In: CB Trevarthen and RW
Sperry (eds), Brain circuits and functions of the mind. Cambridge : Cambridge University
Press, 1990.
Sperry RW, Gazzaniga MS, Bogen JE, Interhemispheric relationship: The neocortical commissure:
syndrome of hemisphere disconnection. In: Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW (eds), Handbook of
Clinical Neurology. Vol. 4. Amsterdam. Elsevier; 273-290, 1969.
Sperry RW, Zaidel E, Zaidel D, Self – recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor
hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 17; 153-166, 1979.
Springer S, Gazzaniga MS, Dichotic testing of partial and complete commissurotomized patients.
Neuropsychologia 13, 341-346, 1975.
Sugishita M, Toyokura Y, Yoshioka M, Unilateral agraphia after section of the posterior half of
the truncus of the corpus callosum. Brain Lang 9; 215-225, 1980.
Sugishita M, Shinohara A, Shimoji T, Does a posterior lesion of the corpus callosum cause hemialexia?
In: Reeves A (ed.), Epilepsy and the Corpus Callosum. New York, Plenum Press, 1985.
Sumner P, Husain M, At the edge of consciousness: automatic motor activation and voluntary
control. The Neuroscientist 14, 474-486, 2008.
Sylver PH, Jeeves MA, Motor coordination in callosal agenesis. In: Lassonde M, Jeeves MA (eds).
Callosal agenesis – A natural split-brain? Advances in Behavioral Biology. New York.
London: Plenum Press, 207-219, 1994.
Tanaka Y, Yoshida A, Kawahata N, Hashimoto R, Obayashi T, Diagnostic dyspraxia. Clinical
characteristics, responsible lesion and possible underlying mechanism. Brain 119; 859-873, 1996.
Temple CM, Ilsley J, Sounds and shapes: Language and spatial cognition in callosal agenesis. In:
Lassonde M, Jeeves MA (eds). Callosal agenesis – A natural split-brain? Advances in
Behavioral Biology. New York. London: Plenum Press; 261-273, 1994.
Teng EL, Dichotic ear difference is a poor index for the functional asymmetry between the cerebral
hemispheres. Neuropsychologia 19; 235-240, 1981.
1589
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Witelson SF, Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of human corpus callosum: A
postmortem morphological study. Brain 112; 799-835, 1989.
Witelson SF, Neuroanatomical bases of hemispheric functional specialization in the human brain:
Possible developmental factors. In: FL Kitterle (ed), Hemispheric communication:
Mechanisms and models. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum, 1995.
Yakovlev PI, Lecours AR, The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In: A
Minkowski (ed.), Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. London: Blackwell 3-65,
1967.
Yamamoto J, Rhoton AL, Peace DA, Microsurgery of the third ventricle: Part I. Neurosurgery 8;
334-356, 1981.
Yamadori A, Osumi Y, Masuhara S, Okubo M, Preservation of singing in Broca’s aphasia. Journal
of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 40; 221-224, 1977.
Young PA, Young PH, Basic Clinical Neuroanatomy. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore:
Lippincott, 1997.
Zaidel E, Lexical organization in the right hemisphere. In: Buser, Rougeul – Buser A (eds),
Cerebral Correlates of Conscious Experience. Amsterdam. Elsevier, 1978.
Zaidel E, Performance on the ITPA following cerebral commissurotomy and hemispherectomy.
Neuropsychologia 17; 259-280, 1979.
Zaidel E, Disconnection syndrome as a model for laterality effects in the normal brain. In: Hellinge
J (ed), Cerebral Hemisphere Asymmetry Methods. Theory and Application. New York:
Praeger, 1983.
Zaidel E, Language functions in the two hemispheres following complete commissurotomy and
hemispherectomy. In: Boller F, Grafman G (eds.); Handbook of Neuropsychology. New York:
Elsevier. Vol. 4; 115-150, 1990.
Zaidel D, Sperry RW, Memory impairment after commissurotomy in man. Brain 97; 263-276,
1974.
Zaidel D, Sperry RW, Some long-term motor effects of cerebral commissurotomy in man.
Neuropsychologia 15; 193-204, 1977.
Zaidel E, Peters AM, Phonological encoding and ideographic reading by the disconnected right
hemisphere. Two cases studies. Brain Lang 14; 205-234, 1981.
Zaidel E, Benson DF, Zaidel E. The Dual Brain, 205-231. New York Guilford, 1985.
Zaidel E, Zaidel DW, Bogen JE, Testing the commissurotomy patient. Neuromethods 17; 147-
201, 1990.
Zaidel E, Clarke J, Suyenobu B, Hemispheric independence: a paradigm case for cognitive
neuroscience. In: AB Scheibel and AF Wechsler (eds), Neurobiology of higher cognitive
function. New York: Guilford; 297-352, 1990.
Zaidel E, Iacoboni M, Zaidel DW, Bogen JE, The callosal syndromes. In: Heilman and E
Valenstein (eds) Clinical neuropsychology (4th ed), New York Oxford University Press, 2003.
Zettner A, Netsky MC, Lipoma of the corpus callosum. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 19; 305-319,
1960.
1591
CEREBRAL CORTEX
Introduction
Anatomists use the term cortex (from the Latin for “bark”, as in a tree’s bark) to
refer to any outer layer of cells. In neuroscience, the terms cortex and neocortex (new
cortex) are often used interchangeably to refer to the outer part of the forebrain, and so
by convention “cortex” refers to “neocortex” unless otherwise indicated. So, most of the
cortex that covers the cerebral hemispheres is neocortex, defined as the cortex that has
six cellular layers, or laminae. Each layer comprises more or less distinctive populations
of cells based on their different densities, sizes, shapes, inputs, and outputs. Despite an
overall uniformity, regional differences based on these laminar features have long been
apparent, allowing investigators to identify numerous subdivisions of the cerebral cortex.
The cortex is the part of the brain that has expanded the most in the course of evolution;
it comprises 80% by volume of the human brain (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The human neocortex has an area as large as 2 500 cm2 but a thickness of only 1,5
to 3,0 mm. It consists of four to six layers of cells (gray matter) and is heavily wrinkled.
This wrinkling is nature’s solution to the problem of confining the huge neocortical
surface within a skull that is still small enough to pass through the birth canal. Just as
crumpling a sheet of paper enables it to fit into a smaller box than it could when flat, the
folding of the neocortex permits the human brain to fit comfortably within the relatively
fixed volume of the skull (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
According to Baars (2007), we usually see the brain from the outside, so that the
cortex is the most visible structure. But the brain grew and evolved from the inside out,
very much like a tree, beginning from a single seed, then turning into a thin shoot, and
then mushrooming in three directions: upward, forward and outward from the axis of
growth. That point applies both to phylogenesis – how species evolved – and ontogenesis
– how the human brain grows from the foetus onward.
The mature brain reveals that pattern of growth and evolution. It means, for example,
that lower regions like the brain stem are generally more ancient than higher regions,
such as the frontal cortex. Basic survival functions like breathing are controlled by neural
centres in the lower brain stem, while the large prefrontal cortex in humans is a late
1592
21. Cerebral cortex
addition to the basic mammallian brain plan. It is located the farthest upward and forward
in the neural axis. Thus, local damage to the prefrontal cortex has little impact on basic
survival functions, but it can impair sophisticated abilities like decision-making, self-
control and even personality (Baars, 2007).
The cerebral cortex of the cerebral hemispheres, the convoluted outer layer of gray
matter composed of tens of billions of neurons and their synaptic connections, (Fig. 21.1)
is the most highly organized correlation centre of the brain, but the specificity of cortical
structures in mediating behavior is neither clear-cut nor circumscribed (Collins, 1990;
Franckowiak et al., 1997). The bulk of the cerebral cortex is comprised of the neocortex.
The phylogenetically older parts of the cortex include the paleocortex (olfactory cortex,
entorhinal and periamygdaloid areas) and the archicortex (the hippocampal formation).
The tens of billions of neurons send, in all directions, a large number of axons, covered
by supportive myelin. These form the white matter of the cortex that fills the large
subcortical space.
Partial or total lesions of same Brodmann specialized areas or one of the lobes leads
to a modular loss of consciousness. When all the cerebral cortex is destroyed as well as
the white matter, the patient becomes unconsciousness.
The majority of the human cerebral cortex is devoted to tasks that transcend encoding
primary sensations or commanding motor actions.
Collectively, the association cortices mediate these cognitive functions of the brain,
broadly defined as the ability to attend to, identify, and act meaningfully in response to
complex external or internal stimuli.
Descriptions of patients with cortical lesions, functional brain imaging of normal
subjects, and behavioral and electrophysiological studies of non-human primates have
established the general purpose of the major association areas.
Subsequently, techniques such as single-cell recording and electrical stimulation of
cells in the cerebral cortex have been used in animals, non-human primates, as well as
humans undergoing surgery for diseases such as epilepsy and Parkinson’s disease to map
out functional areas of the brain (Jones, 2000).
Functional neuroimaging techniques such as positron emission tomography (PET),
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and magnetoencephalography (MEG)
have been used to confirm previous knowledge about the localization of functions within
the cerebral cortex as well as to conduct studies in healthy human subjects that were
previously not possible.
Even those functions that are subserved by cells located within relatively well-
defined cortical areas have a significant number of components distributed outside the
local cortical centre (Brodal, 1981; Paulescu et al., 1997).
In this way, effortful tasks show a wider spread of brain activity, even beyond the
executive regions of the frontal cortex. For example, in a classic study, Smith and Jonides
(1998) found dramatically expanded cortical activity as a function of memory load. The
difficulty level rises very quickly with the number of items to be kept in mind.
What we cannot see yet, even with advanced brain recording methods, is the strength
of connections between brain cells. Mental effort changes connection strengths, the neural
signalling density between cortical localizations. Cognitive efforts is one of the biggest
factors in brain functioning.
The neural firing or brain metabolism is not a direct measure of the complexity of
some mental processes. It seems to indicate the recruitment of neuronal resources that
are needed to work together to perform a task that is new or unpredictable. Once even
very complex processes are learned, they seem to require less cortical activity.
Automaticity also involves a loss of conscious access and voluntary control as assessed
by behavioral measures (Schneider, 1995; Baars, 2002).
Cortical involvement appears to be a prerequisite for awareness of experience
(Fuster, 1995; Kohler and Moscovitch, 1997; Roth, 2000).
Patterns of functional localization in the cerebral cortex are broadly organized along
two spatial planes. The lateral plane cuts through homologous areas of the right and left
hemispheres. The longitudinal plane runs from the front to the back of the cortex, with a
1595
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
relatively sharp demarcation between functions that are primarily localized in the forward
portion of the cortex and those whose primary localization is behind the central sulcus or
fissure of Rolando (Lezak et al., 2004).
In the newborn, the two cerebral hemispheres weigh approximately 402 g in the
male and 380 g in the female. By the end of the first year, the weight of the brain has
doubled, and by the end of the sixth year, it has tripled to at least 90% of its adult weigh
at that time. This increase in weight is due mainly to an increase in the number of blood
vessels, myelin layers, and supporting or glial elements (Schmitt et al., 1981; Roland and
Zilles, 1996; Steinmetz, 1996; Zilles, 2004).
The role of glia, in among other examples, synapse formation, synapse maturation
and plasticity, and rapid conduction of action potentials, as well as their immunological
functions in the nervous system, have by now been unequivocally established (Stern,
2010). The molecular mechanisms underlying astrocyte specifications and growth and
the interaction with other cell types to assemble the nervous system are still not
completely understood. Another type of glial cell, oligodendrocytes, is central to the
brain’s myelination, which ensures the efficiency and speed of action potentials (Emery,
2010). Microglia are another fascinating subpopulation of glial cells. They sense
pathological tissue alternation and they can develop into brain macrophages and perform
immunological functions (Graeber, 2010).
Lee at al. (2010) report that tonic inhibition in the cerebellum is due to GABA being
released from glial cells by permeation through the Bestrophin 1 anion channel. Ginhoux
et al. (2010) investigate the developmental origin of microglia and show that adult microglia
derive from primitive myeloid progenitor cells that arise before embryonic day 8.
In a perspective, Fields (2010) presents his hypothesis that white matter, which
consists mostly of myelinated axons, may play a role in brain plasticity and learning.
In adults, the cerebral hemispheres weight about 1 450 g in males with a normal
range of 1 200 to 1 600 g. In females, the cerebral hemispheres weight about 1 350 g,
with a normal range between 1 100 and 1 500 g. The brain of an elephant weighs about
5 000 g and that of the blue whale weighs about 6 800 g.
The surface of the brain is folded such that two-thirds of its surface area lies buried
and out of view in the depths of the cerebral fissures or sulci.
When one considers the surface area of the brain per lobe, about 41% of the human
brain consists of the frontal lobe, 17% consists of the occipital lobe, 21% consists of the
temporal lobe, and the parietal and insular lobes make up about 21% of the surface area
of the brain (Roland and Ziels, 1996; Steinmetz, 1996; Augustine, 2008).
The surface gray matter of the cerebral hemispheres, or cerebral cortex, average
about 2.5 mm in thickness, with variations from one brain region to another. The primary
visual cortex (area 17 of Brodmann) is about 1.5 mm in depth whereas the primary motor
cortex (area 4 of Brodmann) is about 4.5 mm. There is a variation in the thickness of the
cerebral cortex, with it being thinner in the depths of the fissures than at the upper margins
of the fissures. Some 1012 neurons may be present in both cerebral hemispheres (1012 =
= 1 trillion or 1 million ´ 1 million) (Augustine, 2008).
The total area of the cerebral cortex is about 2 500 cm2.
1596
21. Cerebral cortex
Lateral organization
The two mirror-image halves of the cortex have puzzled people for centuries. Why
are there two hemispheres? If we have but one mind, why do we have two hemispheres?
The two cerebral hemispheres are nearly symmetrical. The primary sensory and
motor centres are homologously pozitioned within the cerebral cortex of each hemisphere
in a mirror-image relationship. With certain exceptions, such as the visual and auditory
systems, the centres in each cerebral hemisphere predominate in mediating the activities
of the contralateral (other side) half of the body.
Thus, an injury to the primary somesthetic or somatosensory area of the right
hemisphere results in decreased or absent sensations in the corresponding left-sided body
part; an injury affecting the left motor cortex results in a right-sided weakness or paralysis
(Lezak et al., 2004).
The hemispheres are completely separate, divided by the longitudinal fissure that
runs between the two hemispheres from the anterior to the posterior part of the brain.
But, how do the two hemispheres “talk” to each other? The answer lies in the fibre
tract (corpus callosum), a large arch of white matter that runs from the front to the back
of the brain, linking the two hemispheres.
Although the two hemispheres are similar in appearance and structure, they are not
biologically identical and they have different information processing abilities and
propensities.
The corpus callosum, with at least 200 million nerve fibres, is the largest tract that
connects the two cerebral hemispheres. Although there are no cortical landmarks that
divide the corpus callosum, it is generally the cause that different regions of the corpus
callosum contain fibres originating in different cortical areas. That is, the anterior portion
of the corpus callosum contains primarily fibres that originate in promotor and frontal
regions of the cortex, middle portion contain fibres that originate in motor and
somatosensory regions and so forth. In addition, many callosal fibres are homotopic; that
is they connect homologous areas of the two hemispheres. The corpus callosum also
1597
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
contains fibres that originate in a specific region of one hemisphere and terminate in a
completely different region of the opposite hemisphere.
Comparisons of split-brain patients with intact individuals provide a clear indication
that the corpus callosum is critical to normal interhemispheric interactions, especially
interactions that require the transfer of information about the identity or name of a
stimulus.
The corpus callosum has been hypothesized to play two important but very different
roles: transferring information between the hemispheres and creating a kind of inhibitory
barrier that minimized maladaptive cross talk between the complementary processes for
which each hemisphere is dominant. Anyhow, same types of information can be
transferred subcortically, and subcortical structures can also play a role in producing
unified behavioral responses. Studies of perceptual processing in normal individuals
provide important insights about the factors that determine when it is more efficient for
the two hemispheres to operate collaboratively than to operate independently (Ivry and
Roberts, 1998).
A major difference in the sources of innervation of the association cortices is the
enrichment in direct projections from other cortical areas called corticocortical
connections. These connections form the majority of the input to the association cortices.
Ipsilateral corticocortical connections arise from primary and secondary sensory and
motor cortices, and from other association cortices within the same hemisphere.
Corticocortical connections also arise from both corresponding and noncorresponding
cortical regions in the opposite hemisphere via the corpus callosum and anterior and
posterior commissure, which together are referred to as interhemispheric connections.
In the association cortices of humans and other primates, corticocortical connections
often form segregates bands or columns in which interhemispheric projection bands are
interdigitated with bands of ipsilateral corticocortical projections (De Felipe and Jones,
1988; Garey, 1994; Posner and Raiche, 1994; Purves et al., 2004).
One general conclusion suggested by this research is that distributing information
across both hemispheres becomes more beneficial as the task becomes more demanding of
attentional resources. That is, when the processing demands are minimal, there is often a
within-hemisphere advantage. However, when the processing demands are increased (as
much as indicating whether two letters of different cases have the same name or whether
both are vowels), there is typically an across-hemisphere advantage (Gazzaniga, 2000).
Interhemispheric collaborations can have emergent properties that are impossible to
deduce from the sum of the parts provided by the two individual hemispheres.
Longitudinal organization
Although no two human brains are exactly alike in their structure, all normally
developed brains share the same major distinguishing features. The external surface of
each half of the cerebral cortex is wrinkled into a complex of ridges or convolutions
called gyri (sing.: gyrus), which are separated by two deep fissures and many shallow
clefts, the sulci (sing.: sulcus).
1598
21. Cerebral cortex
The two prominent fissures and certain of the major sulci divide each hemisphere
into four lobes, the occipital, parietal, temporal and frontal lobes (Brodal, 1981; Mesulam,
2000; Dănăilă and Golu, 2006).
cortex, with a cortical representation arranged according to pitch, from high tones to low
ones. So, these areas that receive projections from structures outside the neocortex or
send projections to it are called primary projection areas. Nevertheless, the primary
projection areas of the neocortex are small relative to the total size of the cortex.
Destruction of a primary cortical sensory or motor area results in specific sensory
or motor deficits but generally has little effect on the higher cortical functions. Some
mild decrements in movement speed and strength of the hand on the same side as the
lesions in the motor cortex have been reported (Smutok et al., 1989; Cramer et al., 1999).
So, signals coming into the association cortices via the thalamus reflect sensory and
motor information that has already been processed in the primary sensory and motor
areas of the cerebral cortex.
Unlike the thalamic nuclei that receive peripheral sensory information and project
to primary sensory cortices, the input to these association cortex-projecting nuclei comes
from other regions of the cortex. The primary sensory cortices receive thalamic
information that is more directly related to peripheral sense organs and to the basal ganglia
and cerebellum.
In any case, the primary sensory areas send projections into the areas adjacent to
them, and the motor areas receive fibres from areas adjacent to them. These adjacent
areas, less connected with the sensory receptors and motor neurons, are referred to as
secondary areas. The secondary areas are thought to be more engaged in interpreting
perceptions or organising movements than are the primary areas.
Neurons in these secondary cortical areas integrate and refine raw percepts or simple
motor responses.
Tertiary association or overlap zones are areas peripheral to the functional centres
where the neuronal components of two or more different functions or modalities are
interspersed. The posterior association cortex, in which the supramodal integration of
perceptual functions takes place, has also been called the multimodal (Pandya and
Yeterian, 1990) or heteromodal (Strub and Black, 1988; Mesulam, 2000) cortex.
So, the areas that lie between the various secondary areas are referred to as tertiary
areas. Often referred to as association areas, tertiary areas serve to connect and coordinate
the functions of the secondary areas. Tertiary areas mediate complex activities such as
language, planning, memory, and attention. These processing areas are connected in a
“stepwise” manner so that information-bearing stimuli reach the cortex first in the primary
sensory centres. They then pass through the cortical association areas in order of
increasing complexity, interconnecting with other cortical subcortical structures along
the way to the frontal and limbic system association areas and finally comes the
expression through action, thought, and feelings (Pandya and Yeterian, 1998; Mesulam,
2000; Arciniegas and Beresford, 2001).
Another important source of innervation to the association areas is subcortical,
arising from the dopaminergic nuclei in the midbrain, the noradrenergic and serotonergic
nuclei in the brain stem reticular formation, and cholinergic nuclei in the brain stem and
basal forebrain.
This diffuse input projects to different cortical layers and, among other functions,
determines the mental state along a continuum that ranges from deep sleep to high alert
(Mc Cormick, 1992; Mc Carley, 1995; Provencio et al., 2000; Willie et al., 2003).
Generally, each association cortex is defined by a distinct subset of thalamic, cortico-
cortical, and subcortical connections. As a result, inferences about the function of human
association areas continue to depend critically on the observation of patients with cortical
lesions. Damage to the association cortices in the parietal, temporal, and frontal lobes,
respectively, results in specific cognitive deficits; this indicates much about the operations
and purposes of each of these regions (Purves et al., 2004).
1601
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
These projection systems have both forward and reciprocal connections at each step
in the progression to the frontal lobes; and each sensory association area makes specific
frontal lobe connections which, too, send their reciprocal connections back to the
association areas of the posterior cortex (Rolls, 1998).
Generally, there are identified areas that function in more than one modality (for
example, vision and touch). These areas, known as the multimodal or polymodal cortex,
presumably function to combine characteristics of stimuli across different modalities.
For example, we can visually identify objects that we have only touched, which implies
some common perceptual system linking the visual and somatic system (Kolb and
Whishaw, 2003). Insight into the function of these cortical regions has come primarily
from the observations of human patients with damage to one or another of these areas.
Noninvasive brain imaging of normal subjects, functional mapping during neurosurgery,
and electrophysiological analysis of comparable brain regions in non-human primates
have generally confirmed the clinical deduction. There are four distinct regions of
multimodal cortex, one in each of the occipital, parietal, temporal and frontal lobes. These
areas imply that more than one process requires polymodal information.
Different regions could take part in different memory processes, object perception,
emotion, movement control, and so on.
Anyhow, it is now clear that virtually the entire cortex behind the central fissure has
some kind of sensory function.
The multimodal cortex appears to be of two general types, one type related to the
recognition and related processing of information and the other type controlling
movement related to the information in some manner (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). This
concept suggests that we have parallel cortical systems: one system functions to
understand the world, whereas the other system functions to move us around in the world
and allows us to manipulate our world. According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), the
cortex is fundamentally an organ of sensory perception and related motor processes.
Jerison (1991) suggested that our knowledge of reality is related directly to the structure
and number of our cortical maps. As the number of maps possessed by an animal brain
increases, more of the external world is known to the animal and more behavioral options
are available to it. For instance, animals such as rats and dogs, whose brain does not have
a cortical region analysing colour, perceive the world in black and white. This lack must
limit their behavioral options, at least with respect to colour.
Jerison (1991) suggested that cortical maps determine reality for a given species.
Furthermore, he noted that, the more maps a species has, the more complex the internal
representation of the external world must be.
Thus, if humans have more maps than dogs, then our representation of reality must
be more complex than that of a dog. Similarly, if dogs have more maps than mice then a
dog’s understanding of the world is more complex than that of a mouse. This viewpoint
suggests that the relative intelligence of different mammallian species may be related to
the number of maps that the cortex uses to represent the world.
Dogs would have more olfactory maps than people and would thus be more
intelligent about smells, but the total number of maps in all sensory regions taken together
is far greater in humans than in dogs.
1602
21. Cerebral cortex
This referred to the cognition, which is the process by which we came to know the
world.
More specifically, cognition refers to the ability to attend to external stimuli or
internal motivation: to identify the significance of such stimuli and to make meaningful
responses. So, the association cortices receive and integrate information from a variety
of sources, and they influence a broad range of cortical and subcortical targets.
Unlike damage to primary cortical areas, a lesion involving association areas and
overlap zones typically does not results in specific sensory or motor defects; rather, the
behavioral effects of such damage will more likely appear as a pattern of deficits running
through related functions or as impairment of a general capacity (Goldberg et al., 1989;
Goldberg, 1995).
Thus, certain lesions that are implicated in drawing distortions also tend to affect
the ability to do computations on paper; lesions of the auditory association cortex do not
interfere with hearing acuity per se but with the appreciation of patterned sounds.
In sum, the association cortex includes most of the cerebral surface of the human
brain and is largely responsible for the complex processing that goes on between the
arrival of input in the primary sensory cortices and the generation of behavior.
The diverse functions of the association cortices are loosely referred to as cognition,
the process by which we came to know the world (Mountcastle et al., 1975; Platt and
Glimcher, 1999).
Inputs to the association cortices include projections from the primary and secondary
sensory and motor cortices, the thalamus, and the brain stem. Outputs from the association
cortices reach the hippocampus, the basal ganglia and cerebellum, the thalamus, and other
association cortices.
So, different regions of the neocortex have different functions. Some regions receive
information from sensory systems, other regions command movements, and yet other
regions are the sites of connections between the sensory and the motor areas, enabling them
to work in concert (Penfield and Boldrey, 1958; Truex and Cerpenter, 1969; Elliott, 1969).
Overall, the neocortex can be conceptualized as consisting of a number of fields:
visual, auditory, body senses, and motor. Because vision, audition and body senses are
functions of the posterior cortex, this region of the brain (parietal, temporal, and occipital
lobes) is considered to be largely sensory; and because the motor cortex is located in the
frontal neocortex, that lobe is considered to be largely motor.
Finally, because each lobe contains one of the primary projection areas, it can
roughly be associated with a function (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Frontal lobes: motor
Parietal lobes: body senses
Temporal lobes: auditory function
Occipital lobes: visual functions
On the other hand, all studies indicate that, among other functions, the frontal
association cortex is especially important for the planning of appropriate behavioral
responses, the parietal association cortex is especially important for attending to stimuli
1603
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
in the external and internal environment, and the temporal association cortex is especially
important for identifying the nature of such stimuli.
Anyhow, these parts of the brain are responsible for encoding sensory information
(primary sensory cortices), and commanding movements (primary motor cortex). But
these regions account for only a fraction (perhaps a fifth) of the cerebral cortex. Much of
the remaining cortex is concerned with attending to complex stimuli, identifying the
relevant features of such stimuli, recognizing related objects, and planning appropriate
responses as well as storing aspects of this information. Collectively, these integrative
abilities are referred to as cognition, and it is evidently the association cortices in the
frontal, parietal and temporal lobes that make cognition possible.
The extrastriate cortex of the occipital lobe is equally important in cognition; its
functions, however, are largely concerned with vision. These other areas of the cerebral
cortex are referred to collectively as the association cortices (Tanji and Shima, 1994;
Garey, 1994; Glimcher, 2003).
In reality, only about 40% of the possible intercortical connections within a sensory
modality are actually found, which leads us to the flowing solution: intracortical networks
of connections among subsets of cortical regions. This idea has considerable appeal.
First, all cortical areas have internal connections among units with similar properties.
These connections link neurons that are neighbours and synchronise their activity.
Second, through a mechanism called re-entry, any cortical area can influence the
area from which it receives input. This remarkable, interactive aspect of cortical
connectivity means that, when area A sends information to area B, area B reciprocates
and sends a return message to area A (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). Zeki suggested that an
area could actually modify its inputs from another area before it even receives them. An
important point is that the connections from area A and B do not originate from the same
layers, suggesting they play different roles in influencing each other’s activity. Computer
modelling suggests that the primary function of neural connections is to coordinate
activity within and between areas in order to produce a globally coherent pattern, known
as integration, over all areas of the perceptual system (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). This
concept of cortical organisation is likely to be foreign to many readers.
Jerison (1991) related the binding problem to his analogy of multiple cortical maps.
The evolutionary expansion of the cortex in area has implications for the brain with
multiple neurosensory channels that are trying to integrate information into a single
reality. Because so many different kinds of sensory information reach the cortex, it is
necessary somehow discriminate equivalent features in the external world.
Suppose that the brain creates labels to designate objects and a coordinate system to
locate objects in the external world – that is, in space and time. Suppose also that sensory
information must be tagged to persist through time and must be categorized to be retrieved
(remembered) when needed.
Labels, coordinates, and categories are products of cognition (knowledge and
thought). Viewed in this way, jerison’s analogy of multiple cortical maps provides a basis
for thinking about how the information that is arriving to the cortex is integrated into
perception and organized as knowledge and thought.
It should not be a surprise that injuries to discrete cortical areas alter the way people
perceive the self and the environment and the way they think about them.
One form of sensory deficit is agnosia. It renders a partial or complete inability to
recognize sensory stimuli.
The pattern of distribution of neuron cell bodies, is, generally, called cyto-
architecture.
There are five neuronal types in the human cerebral cortex. These include the
characteristic neuron of the cerebral cortex, the pyramidal neuron which measures some
10 to 70 µm in diameter. The majority of neurons in the human cerebral cortex are
pyramidal neurons. These excitatory neurons project to other pyramidal neurons forming
networks among themselves. A single pyramidal cell may have up to 200 synapses from
other neurons on its cell body but upwards of 40,000 synapses on its axon and dendrites.
Pyramidal cells are found in all layers of the cortex with the exception of molecular
layer I, and they are the predominant cell type in layers II, III and V.
Pyramidal cells are characterized by a roughly triangular cell body, a single large
apical dendrite that arises from the apex of the cell body and usually extends towards the
molecular layers, giving off branches along the way, an array of basal dendrites that run
in a predominantly horizontal direction, and an axon that originates from the base of the
soma leaves the cortex, and passes through the white matter.
The largest pyramidal neurons, called the giant pyramidal cell of Betz, are found
almost exclusively in the primary motor cortex, which is located in the precentral and
anterior paracentral gyri. Betz cells are most common in the region of the motor cortex
that projects to the anterior horn of the lumbar spinal cord and hence are concerned with
the control of leg movement.
Apical and basal dendrites of pyramidal cells are characterized by membrane
specialization called dendritic spines that give the impression of thorns on a rose bush.
Pyramidal neurons represent virtually the only output pathway for the cerebral cortex.
Axons of pyramidal cells may terminate in another region of the cortex in the same
hemisphere (association fibres), decussate in the corpus callosum to terminate in the
cerebral cortex of the opposite hemispheres (callosal fibres), or course through the white
matter to any of the numerous subcortical targets in the forebrain, brain stem, or spinal
cord (projection fibres) (Lynch, 2006).
Pyramidal cells display a laminar organization, with the cell bodies in a given layer
projecting to specific neural targets (Fig 21.2). Generally, pyramidal neurons in layers II
and III give rise to association and callosal fibres. Pyramidal cell in layers V project to
many subcortical structures, including the spinal cord, as projection fibres. The neurons
in layer VI send their axons to a variety of locations, including the thalamic nuclei and
other regions of the cortex. Within the cortex, the axons of pyramidal cells send off an
extensive and relatively dense array of axon collaterals. These collaterals terminate in all
cortical layers and extend through a horizontal area covering several millimetres around
the cell body (Fig. 21.3). Pyramidal cells use an excitatory aminoacid, either glutamate
or aspartate, as their neurotransmitter.
The stellate or granule neuron is another neuronal type found in the cerebral cortex.
These cells are small, polygonal or triangular in shape and have dark-staining nuclei and
scanty cytoplasm. These neurons, ranging from 4 to 8 µm, have a number of dendrites passing
in all directions and a short axon which ramifies close to the cell body (Golgi type II).
1606
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.2. Representative cell types in the cerebral cortex and the layers in which their cell bodies and dendrites are
found. Dendrites of pyramidal cells (Py) of layers II, III, and V extended into layer I, whereas those of modified
pyramidal cells (mPy) in layer VI extend only to about layer IV. Chandelier cells (Ch) are restricted almost entirely to
layer III. The somata of aspiny and spiny stellate neurons (Asp, Sp) are in layer IV, although their processes extend
into other layers. Basket cells (Bas) have processes that collectively extend into all cortical layers from cell bodies
located mainly in layers III and V. (Adapted from Hendry SHC, Jones EG: Size and distributions of intrinsic neurons
incorporating tritiated GABA in monkey sensory motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 1; 390-408, 1981, and from Jones EG:
Laminar distribution of cortical efferent cells. In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 1, New York, Plenum Press, 1984, pp. 521-553.
Other larger stellate cells have longer axons which may enter the medullary
substance. Some resemble the pyramidal cells in that they have an apical dendrite which
extends to the surface. These cells are known as stellate or star pyramidal cells (Lorente
de Nó, 1949). Stellate cells are found throughout all layers of the cortex but are especially
numerous in layer IV.
This neuronal type has a wide distribution and is probably correlative in function,
interrelating different cortical layers.
Spiny stellate cells are the second most numerous cell type in the neocortex. Several
primary dendrites, profusely covered in spines, radiate for a variable distance from the
cell body. Their axons ramify within the grey matter predominantly in the vertical plane.
Spiny stellate cells probably use glutamate as their neurotransmitter.
The smallest group comprises the heterogeneous non-spiny or sparsely spinous
stellate cells. All are interneurones and their axons are confined to the grey matter.
However, there is not a single class of cells, but a multitude of different forms, including
basket, chandelier, double bouquet, neurogliaform, bipolar / fusiform and horizontal cells.
1607
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 21.3. Diagram showing some of the intracortical circuits. Sinaptic junction are indicated by loops; Afferent
thalamocortical fibres; efferent cortical neurons, intracortical neurons; G, granule cell; H, horizontal cell; M, Martinotti
cell; P, pyramidal cell; S, stellate cell; B, mode of termination of afferent cortical fibres.
(Based on data by Lorente de Nó, 1949).
These types may have horizontally, vertically or radially ramifying axons. The principal
recognisable neuronal type is the neurogliaform or spiderweb cell. These small spherical
cells, 10 to 12 µm in diameter, are found mainly in laminae II to IV, depending on cortical
area. Seven to ten dendrites typically radiate out from the cell soma, some branching
once or twice to form a spherical dendritic field of approximately 100 to 150 µm in
diameter. The slender axon arises from the cell body or a proximal dendrite. It branches
profusely within the vicinity of the dendritic field to give a spherical axonal arbour up to
350 µm in diameter (Crossman, 2008).
The horizontal cells of Cajal are located in the superficial cortical layers. These
small neurons correlate adjoining areas with each cerebral hemisphere and have axons
and dendrites that run parallel to the cortical surface.
The cells of Martinotti, present in practically all cortical layers, are small triangular
cells whose axons are directed towards the surface. Some fibres arborize in the same
layer; others send collaterals to a number of layers.
The fifth cortical neuronal type is the polymorph, multiform, or pleomorphic
neuron. These essentially modified pyramidal neurons are associative in function and
occur in the innermost cortical layers (Augustine, 2008).
1608
21. Cerebral cortex
to a lesser extent, horizontal arrays in the mature neocortex (Rakic, 1988; Price and
Thurlow, 1988; Kornack and Rakic, 1995; Yu et al., 2009). Moreover, specific chemical
synapses preferentially form between vertically aligned sister excitatory neurons,
suggesting that spatially organized ontogenetic clones of excitatory neurons contribute
to the formation of functional columnar microcircuits in the neocortex (Yu et al., 2009).
Whether inhibitory interneurons are spatially organized with respect to the formation
of repetitive functional columns in the neocortex remains an outstanding question (Brown
et al., 2011).
However, neocortical interneurons are remarkably diverse and often classified by
distinctive morphologies, expression of neurochemical markers, firing pattern, and
synaptic connectivity (Markram et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2007; Ascoli et al., 2008).
Proper production of a correct number of each of these different subgroups of neocortical
interneurons is essential for constructing a functional neocortex.
Brown et al. (2011) found that neocortical inhibitory interneurons were produced as
spatially organized clonal units in the developing ventral telencephalon.
Furthermore, clonally related interneurons did not randomly disperse but formed
spatially isolated clusters in the neocortex. Individual clonal clusters consisting of
interneurons expressing the same or distinct neurochemical markers exhibited clear
vertical or horizontal organization.
These results suggest that the lineage relationship plays a pivotal role in the
organization of inhibitory interneurons in the neocortex (Brown et al., 2011).
Proper production of each of these different subgroups of neocortical interneurons is
essential for constructing a functional neocortex. Radial migration of daughter cells along
the mother radial glial cell has been extensively characterized in the dorsal telencephalon
during excitatory neuron neurogenesis and migration. Brown et al. (2011) showed that before
tangential migration differentiating daughter cells in the medial GE (MGE) and the PoA,
which include neocortical interneurons, migrate radially along the mother radial glial
progenitor cell. The physiological importance of this initial radial migration is unclear (Brown
et al., 2011). It may allow proper neuronal differentiation of the daughter cells before they
begin migrating tangentially to reach the neocortex (Brown et al., 2011).
It has been suggested that direct contact with the radial glia promotes GABAergic
interneuron differentiation (Li et al., 2008). Consistent with this, Brown et al. (2011)
found that cells within individual clones with the most pronounced neuronal
characteristics are located furthest away from the ventricular zone. They possess the
typical bipolar morphology of a tangentially migrating interneuron, indicating that they
are poised for tangential migration. Cells with less pronounced neuronal characteristics
are located close to the ventricular zone (Brown et al., 2011).
Neocortical interneurons generated in the MGE and PoA undertake complex
migration routes to reach their final destination in the neocortex (Corbin et al., 2001;
Mariss and Rubenstein, 2003).
They migrate tangentially over long distances to enter the neocortex through the
marginal zone or the intermediate and subventricular zones before turning radially to
1610
21. Cerebral cortex
reach their ultimate location in the neocortex. Similar to excitatory neurons, inhibitory
interneurons generated in the MGE and PoA display a birth date-dependent laminar
distribution in the neocortex (Ang et al., 2003; Batista-Brito and Fishell, 2009; Miyoshi
and Fishell, 2011), thereby arguing for a regulated process of interneuron migration.
However, the long-distance tangential migration of interneurons has been considered to
be mostly random (Ang et al., 2003; Tanaka et al., 2009).
Brown et al. (2011) found that clonally related interneurons do not randomly disperse
but form spatially organized vertical and horizontal clusters in the neocortex.
Nearly all neocortical neuronal circuits are composed of excitatory neurons and
inhibitory interneurons.
Similar to excitatory neurons, inhibitory interneurons in the neocortex develop highly
specific synaptic connections for the assembly of functional circuits (Gupta et al., 2000;
Yoshimura and Callaway, 2005; Thomson and Lamy, 2007; Fino and Yuste, 2011).
The synaptic connections from local inhibitory interneurons to excitatory neurons
exhibit a stereotypic spatial pattern (Kätzel et al., 2011), suggesting a high degree of
spatial and functional organization of neocortical interneurons (Brown et al., 2011).
Cortical layers
Although the cerebral cortex contains an enormous number of cells, the number of
cell types is small (Colonnier, 1967).
Based on cell size and packing density (cytoarchitectonics) each cortical region is
divisible into six cellular layers, with three of these layers being subdivided based on
their architectural features.
This six-layered cellular arrangement is characteristic of the entire neopallial cortex,
which is referred to as neocortex, isocortex (Vogt and Vogt, 1919) or homogenetic cortex
(Brodmann, 1909).
Two regions of the cerebral cortex have fewer than six layers. The first contains
only three layers, is classified as archicortex, and includes the hippocampal formation.
The second contains from three to five layers, is classified as paleocortex, and includes
the olfactory sensory area and the nearby entorhinal and periamygdaloid cortices.
On the other hand, those cortical areas with six definite layers are termed homotypic
areas.
Homotypical variants, in which all six laminae are found, are called frontal, parietal
and polar.
Those cortical areas in which six layers are obscure, such as in the primary motor
cortex (where there appears to be only five layers) or where seven layers are identifiable,
such as in the primary visual cortex, are termed heterotypic areas.
The two types of cortex, granular and agranular, are regarded as virtually lacking
certain laminae, and are referred to as heterotypical.
The agranular type is considered to have diminished, or absent, granular laminae
(II and IV), but always contains scattered stellate somata. Large pyramidal neurones are
found in the greatest densities in the agranular cortex, which is typified by the numerous
1611
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
efferent projections of pyramidal cell axons. Agranular cortex occurs in areas 4, 6, 8, and
44 and parts of the limbic system.
In the granular type of cortex the granular layers are maximally developed, and it
contains densely packed stellate cells, among which small pyramidal neurons are
dispersed.
Layers III and IV are poorly developed or unidentifiable. This type of cortex is
particularly associated with afferent projections.
However, it does receive efferent fibres, derived from the scattered pyramidal cells,
although they are less numerous than elsewhere.
Granular cortex occurs in the postcentral gyrus (somatosensory area), striate area
(visual area) and superior temporal gyrus (acoustic area), and in small areas of the
parahippocampal gyrus. Despite its very high density of stellate cells, it is almost the
thinnest of the five main types.
The other three types of cortex are intermediate forms. In the frontal type, large
numbers of small- and medium-sized pyramidal neurons appear in laminae III and V,
and granular layers (II and IV) are less prominent.
The parietal type of cortex contains pyramidal cells, which are mostly smaller in
size than those found in the frontal type.
In marked contrast, the granular laminae are wider and contain more stellate cells:
this kind of cortex occupies large areas in the parietal and temporal lobes.
The polar type is classically identified with small areas near the frontal and occipital
poles and is the thinnest form of cortex. All six layers are represented, but the pyramidal
layers (III) are reduced in thickness and not as extensively invaded by stellate cells as is
the granular type of cortex. The multiform layers (VI) are more highly organised than
other types.
However, these six cortical layers include the: (I) molecular layer, (II) external
granular layer, (III) external pyramidal layer, (IV) internal granular layer, (V) internal
pyramidal layer, and (VI) multiform layer.
Layer (I) – the molecular layer contains very few neuron cell bodies and consists
primarily of axons running parallel (horizontal) to the surface of the cortex. Within it are
found the terminal dendritic ramifications of the pyramidal and fusiform cells from the
deeper layers, and the axonal endings of the Martinotti cells.
Layer (II) – the external granular layers are composed of a mixture of small neurons
called granule cells and slightly larger neurons, which are called pyramidal cells based
on the shape of their cell body.
The apical dendrites of these pyramidal cells extend into layer I and their axons
descend into, and through the deeper cortical layers. This layer is poor in myelinated
fibres. Myelin fibre stains show mainly vertically arranged processes traversing the layer.
Layer (III) – the external pyramidal layers are composed mainly of well formed
pyramidal neurons. The size of the pyramidal cells is smallest in the most superficial part
of the layer and greatest in the deepest part. Two sublayers are recognized: a superficial
layer of medium-sized pyramidal cells, along with some neurons of other types, and a
1612
21. Cerebral cortex
deeper layer of larger ones. Their apical dendrites go to the first layer, while most of their
axons enter the white matter, chiefly as association or commissural fibres. Intermingled
with the pyramidal neurons are granules and Martinotti cells. In the most superficial part
of the layer, a number of horizontal myelinated fibres constitute the band of Kaes-
Becherew.
Layer (IV) – the internal granular layer is usually the narrowest of the cellular
laminae and is composed almost exclusively of smooth (aspiny) stellate neurons and
spiny stellate neurons many of which have short axons ramifying within the layer. By
the 1950s it had become customary to refer to virtually all intrinsic cortical neurons as
stellate cells, even though many were not actually star shaped.
Now, a number of distinct morphologic types are recognized. Some of the more
important are the spiny and aspiny stellate cells, basket cells, and chandelier cells.
Three types of intrinsic neurons receive thalamocortical axon terminals in layer IV:
the small spiny cells, the aspiny stellate cells, and dendrites of the large basket cells. Of
these, the spiny cells are believed to be excitatory, whereas basket cells and aspiny stellate
cells use the neurotransmitter GABA and are thus considered to be inhibitory interneurons.
Most other intrinsic neurons are presumed to be inhibitory (Lynch and Tian, 2005).
This layer is free of pyramidal-shaped cells. It can be divided into an outer (IV a)
and inner (IV b) portion in many neocortical areas and into three portions (IV a, IV b, IV
c) in the primary visual cortex.
Layer IV is the primary target for ascending sensory information from the thalamus
(Lynch, 2006).
Layer (V) – the internal pyramidal (ganglionic) layer, consists of medium-sized and
large pyramidal neurons intermingled with granule and Martinotti cells. The apical
dendrites of the larger pyramids ascend to the molecular layer; dendrites of the smaller
pyramids ascend only to layer IV.
The large pyramidal cells of this layer are a major source of cortical efferent fibres
including axons to the basal nuclei, brain stem, and spinal cord. Some corticocortical
axons also originate in layer V. These are probably collateral branches of axons that are
projecting to some subcortical targets. A considerable number of callosal fibres is
furnished by the smaller pyramidal cells. The horizontal fibre plexus in the deeper portion
of this layer constitutes the internal band of Baillarger.
Layer VI – the multiform (or fusiform / pleomorphic) layer, contains an assortment
of neuron types including some with pyramidal, ovoid and fusiform cell bodies. These
spindle-shaped cells have their axes perpendicular to the cortical surface. Like the pyramidal
neurons of layer V, the spindle cells vary in size; the longer ones send a dendrite into the
molecular layer, while the dendrites of the smaller ones ascend to layer IV, or arborize
within the fusiform layer. Thus, the dendrites of many pyramidal and spindle cells from
layers V and VI come into direct relation with the endings of sensory thalamocortical fibres,
which ramify chiefly in the internal granular layer (Mountcastle, 1997).
The axons of the cells of this layer project to subcortical targets, such as the thalamus,
and to other cortical regions as corticocortical connections.
1613
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Many of the short arcuate association fibres connecting adjacent convolutions are
furnished by the deep stellate cells of layer VI (Lorente de Nó, 1949).
Cortical connectivity
Different cortical areas have widely different afferent and efferent connections.
Afferent connections
The widely separated but functionally interconnected areas of cortex share common
patterns of connections with subcortical nuclei and within the neocortex. Thus, contiguous
zones of the striatum, thalamus, claustrum, cholinergic basal forebrain, superior colliculus
and pontine nuclei connect with anatomically wide areas of the prefrontal and parietal
cortex, which are themselves interconnected.
In contrast, other functionally distinct regions, e.g., areas in the temporal and parietal
cortex, do not share such contiguity in their subcortical connections.
All the various nonpyramidal neurons of the cerebral cortex function as cortical
interneurons, because their axons do not leave the immediate region of the cell body
(Jones 1975; Lynch and Tian 2005).
With the help of the Golgi technique, the distribution of dendritic and axonal
terminals has been worked out by a number of investigators, notably Cajal (1909-1911).
Lorente de Nó (1949) has given a detailed account for the elementary pattern of
cortical organization that is applicable to the parietal, temporal and occipital isocortex.
According to this investigator, the arrangement of the axonal and dendritic branchings
forms the most constant feature of cortical structure.
The afferent fibres to the cortex include projection fibres from the thalamus,
association fibres from other cortical areas of the same side and commissural fibres from
the opposite side.
The thalamocortical fibres, especially the specific afferent ones from the ventral
tier thalamic nuclei and the geniculate bodies, pass unbranched to layer IV. Here, the
axons form a dense terminal plexus (Colonnier, 1967); some of these fibres extend to
layer III where they arborize. Specific afferent fibres in layer IV establish both
axodendritic and axosomatic synapses upon stellate neurons which are fantastically
profuse on some cells (Colonnier, 1968).
Fibres of the so-called nonspecific thalamocortical system, related to the intralaminar
thalamic nuclei and indirectly to the ascending reticular activating system, also reach the
cerebral cortex. Histological data concerning the origin, course and termination of these
afferent fibres, have been meagre (Hanberry and Jasper, 1953; Nauta and Whitlock, 1954;
Bowsher, 1966).
Jones and Leavit (1974) and Jones (1984) have been shown that the intralaminar
thalamic nuclei, which project mainly to the striatum, project collateral fibres diffusely
to broad regions of the cerebral cortex.
Thus, the cerebral cortex receives inputs, called diffuse inputs, and it consists of
fibres that branch extensively and end diffusely over a wide area of cortex without respect
1614
21. Cerebral cortex
for the cytoarchitectural boundaries. These inputs arise from the nonspecific nuclei of
the thalamus (for example, the ventral anterior, central lateral, and midline nuclei), the
locus ceruleus, and the basal nucleus (of Meynert). These structures are generally
concerned with regulating overall levels of cortical excitability and the associated
phenomena of arousal, sleep, and wakefulness (Lynch, 2006).
According to Jasper (1960), the synaptic termination of fibres of this nonspecific
system in the cortex is chiefly axodendritic and widely distributed in all layers, but the
principal physiological effects appear to be within the superficial layers.
The association of callosal fibres give off some collaterals to layer V and VI and
ramify mainly in layer II and III, and to a lesser extent in layer IV (Carpenter, 1979).
Thus, thalamocortical axons terminate primarily in layer IV and to a lesser extent in layers
III and VI. In layer IV, they terminate on excitatory and inhibitory interneurons as well
as on dendrites from neurons in other layers. The axons of interneurons, in turn, may end
on dendrites of pyramidal cells or of other interneurons.
Generally, the details of intrinsic circuitry of the cerebral cortex are very complex.
A single neuron may also receive synaptic contacts from thousands of other neurons. The
basic framework of cortical circuitry consists of afferent fibres, local circuits for the
processing of this afferent information, and efferent fibres that convey the processed
information to another site.
The local processing of information culminates in connections to pyramidal cells,
which carry the information to other cortical and subcortical regions. A copy of the
information also goes to neurons in the immediate vicinity via axon collaterals. Generally,
the termination of corticocortical axons is quite different from that of thalamocortical
axons. Corticocortical axons branch repeatedly and make synaptic contacts on neurons
in all layers of the cortex (Lynch, 2006).
Thalamocortical relationship
According to Lynch (2006) the cortex of the frontal lobe encompasses Brodmann’s
areas 4, 6, 8 to 12, 32 and 44 to 47. The primary somatomotor cortex (area 4) and the
premotor and supplementary motor cortices (area 6) receive input mainly from the ventral
lateral nucleus of the thalamus and subserve important motor functions.
The lateral, medial, and orbital aspects of the frontal lobe receive thalamocortical
fibres mainly from the dorsomedial and anterior nuclei of the thalamus.
These latter cortical areas relate primarily to functions of the limbic system through
a variety of direct and indirect connections. Of particular note are the pars orbitalis and
pars triangularis of the inferior frontal gyrus, damage to which results in Broca aphasia.
Areas 3, 1, 2, 5, 7, 39, 40 and 43 are located in the parietal lobe. The primary
somatosensory cortex (areas 3, 1 and 2) receives inputs from the ventral posterolateral
and ventral posteromedial nuclei.
These thalamic nuclei receive a full range of somatosensory inputs through synaptic
relays in the spinal cord and brain stem and transmit this information to the cerebral
cortex. The inferior parietal lobule comprises, in general, areas 39 and 40. Along with
area 22, these areas are the cortical regions associated with Wernicke aphasia.
1615
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The occipital and temporal lobe encompass areas 17 to 22, 36 to 38, and 41 and 42.
These areas of the cortex, plus portions of the parietal lobe, have extensive connections
with the pulvinar nucleus of the thalamus and are involved in the processing of visual
and auditory information at several different functional levels. In this area are the primary
sensory cortices for vision and hearing. Area 17, on the bank of the calcarine sulcus, is
the primary visual cortex; areas 41 and 42, in the depth of the lateral fissure in the
transverse temporal gyri, constitute the primary auditory cortex.
These cortical areas receive inputs from the lateral and medial geniculate nuclei of
the thalamus, respectively.
The limbic lobe, which forms the most medial edge of the hemisphere, contains
areas 23 to 31 and 33 to 35. The cingulate cortex receives fibres primarily from the
anterior nucleus of the thalamus but also from the lateral dorsal nucleus.
Other regions of the limbic lobe have some connections with the dorsomedial
nucleus. However, many of the subcortical targets of the parahippocampal and uncal
cortices are structures such as the hippocampal formation. These are, in turn, projects to
a variety of thalamic and basal forebrain targets (Lynch, 2006).
Major afferents to the cortical area tend to terminate in layers I, IV and VI.
Quantitatively lesser projections end either in the intervening laminae II / III and V, or
sparsely throughout the depth of the cortex. Numerically, the largest input to a cortical
area tends to terminate mainly in layer IV. This pattern of termination is seen in the major
thalamic inputs to visual and somatic sensory cortex.
Generally, non-thalamic subcortical afferents to the neocortex, which are shared by
wide spread areas, tend to terminate throughout all cortical layers, but the laminar pattern
of their endings still varies considerably from area to area (Crossman, 2008).
Generally, all cortical areas receive topographically organized cholinergic
projections from the basal forebrain, noradrenergic fibres from the locus ceruleus,
serotoninergic fibres from the midbrain raphe nuclei, dopaminergic fibres from the ventral
midbrain, and histaminergic fibres from the posterior hypothalamus.
Efferent connections
All neocortical areas have axonal connections with other cortical areas on the same
side (association fibres), the opposite side (commissural fibres), and with subcortical
structures (projection fibres). The pyramidal cells are the efferent neurons of the cerebral
cortex.
Thus, all neocortical areas are connected with subcortical regions although their
density varies between areas. First among these are connections with the thalamus. All
areas of the neocortex receive afferents from more than one thalamic nucleus, and all
such connections are reciprocal.
The vast majority, if not all, of the cortical areas project to the striatum, tectum, pons
and brain stem reticular formation. Additionally, all cortical areas are reciprocally
connected with the claustrum; the frontal cortex connects with the anterior part and the
occipital lobe with the posterior part (Crossman, 2008).
1616
21. Cerebral cortex
The primary somatosensory, visual and auditory cortices give rise to ipsilateral
corticocortical connections to the association areas of the parietal, occipital and temporal
lobes, respectively, which then progressively project towards the medial temporal limbic
areas, notably the parahippocampal gyrus, entorhinal cortex and hippocampus.
Thus, the first (primary) somatosensory area (SI) projects to the superior parietal
cortex (Brodmann’s area 5), which in turn projects to the inferior parietal cortex (area
7). From here, connections pass to the cortex in the walls of the superior temporal sulcus,
and so on to the posterior parahippocampal gyrus, and into the limbic cortex.
Similarly, for the visual system, the primary visual cortex (area 17) projects to the
parastriate cortex (area 18), which in turn projects to the peristriate region (area 19).
Information then flows to the inferotemporal cortex (area 20), to the cortex in the walls
of the superior temporal sulcus, then to the medial temporal cortex in the posterior
parahippocampal gyrus, and so to limbic areas. The auditory system shows a similar
progression from the primary auditory cortex to the temporal association cortex and so
to the medial temporal lobe (Crossman, 2008).
The primary somatosensory cortex (SI) in the postcentral gyrus is reciprocally
connected with the primary motor cortex (area 4) in the precentral gyrus.
The superior parietal lobule (area 5) is interconnected with the premotor cortex (area
6), that in turn is connected with area 7 in the inferior parietal lobule. This has reciprocal
connections with the prefrontal association cortex on the lateral surface of the hemisphere
(areas 9 and 46), and the temporal association areas, which connect with more anterior
prefrontal association areas, and, ultimately in the sequence, with the orbitofrontal cortex.
Similar stepwise links exist between areas on the visual and auditory association pathways
in the occipitotemporal lobe and areas of the frontal association cortex.
The connections between sensory and association areas are reciprocal (Crossman,
2008). Generally, short corticocortical fibres arise more superficially, and long
corticocortical (both association and commissural) axons come from cells in the deeper
parts of the layer III.
The internal pyramidal lamina, layer V, gives rise to cortical projection fibres, most
notably corticostriate, corticopontine, corticobulbar, and corticospinal axons. In addition,
a significant proportion of feedback corticocortical axons arise from cells in this layer,
as do some corticothalamic fibres. Layer VI, the multiform lamina, is the major source
of corticothalamic fibres.
Supragranular pyramidal cells, predominantly from layer III, but also lamina II, give
rise primarily to both association and commissural corticocortical pathways.
Myeloarchitectonics
Based on the types of fibres present (radial, oblique, and horizontal fibres), their
distribution, layering, and amount of myelin, collectively termed myeloarchitectonics,
six major cortical fibre layers are identifiable in the human cerebral cortex.
Related to layer II is the stria of the external granular layer. This layer of fibres
is associative in function with many association and commissural fibres. The stria of the
1617
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
internal granular layer in layer IV, receives inputs from various specific thalamic
nuclei.
Finally, the stria of the internal pyramidal layer is located in the deep part of layer
V (Fig. 21.4).
The infragranular cortical layers have their output through the stria in layer V (Jones,
1984; Braitenburg and Schüz, 1998; Zielles and Palomero-Gallagher, 2001; Augustine,
2008).
Thus, two features of the myelinated fibres in the neocortex are noteworthy. First,
there are prominent plexuses of horizontally running myelinated fibres in layers IV and
V. These are called the outer and inner bands of Baillarger, respectively. In the primary
visual cortex, bordering on the calcarine sulcus the outer band of Baillarger is greatly
expanded. This band can be seen with the naked eye in fresh and stained sections and is
called the stria (line) of Gennari.
Second, in most regions of the neocortex, there are many radially oriented bundles
of axons passing between the subcortical white matter and various parts of the cortex or
between the inner and outer cortical layers (Fig. 21.5).
Fig. 21.4. Layers of the human cerebral cortex (from Standring, 2005).
1618
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.5. Nissl (A) and myelin (B) stains of adjacent sections of the human cerebral cortex and a Golgi impregnation
(C) of a pyramidal neuron in the primate neocortex. (A and B by courtesy of Drs. Grazyna Rajkowska and Patricia
Goldman-Rakic, University of Mississippi Medical Center and Yale University; C by courtesy of Dr. José Rafols,
Wayne State University).
The convergence of specific afferents upon specific cells in the columnar units
appears to imprint a specific modality which is relayed by intracortical connections to
other cells in the column. The complex axonal branching suggests that intracortical
circuits involve cells in all parts of the column. These vertical circuits are interconnected
by short neuronal links represented primarily by the short axons of granule cells whose
processes arborize within a single layer.
Through these short links, cortical excitation may spread horizontally and involve a
progressively larger number of vertical units.
Thus, in one cortical region, all neurons might have receptive fields for a finger,
whereas, in a nearby region, the neurons might have receptive fields for the wrist. Within
a cortical region in which all neurons had about the same receptive field, the neurons
responded to different sensory submodalities. Some neurons are activated by light touch
on the skin, others by joint rotation, and still others by strong pressure on deep tissue
(Lynch, 2006).
So, neurons in the somatosensory cortex are located in vertical columns at right
angles to the surface and extend through all cortical layers; they often share similar
receptive fields and have specific functions. The width of these cortical columns has an
upper limit in primates of about 5 mm. Columns in SI are functionally interrelated by
intrinsic corticocortical connections and by direct horizontal connections between
columns. These intrinsic connections serve to connect parts of the cerebral cortex having
different response properties, yet lying in regions of similar regional representation
(Augustine, 2008). Thus, a specific afferent fibre may not only fire vertical columns of
cells in its immediate vicinity, but may reach other units through Golgi type II cell relays
(Lorente de Nó, 1949).
According to Cajal (1909, 1911) the unique morphological feature of the human
cortex is the enormous number of Golgi type II cells considered to interrelate vertical
cell columns.
The topographical pattern present on the cortical surface extends throughout its
depth. Studies of the visual (striate) cortex demonstrate similar discrete functional
columns extending from the pial surface to the white matter that are responsive to a
specific kind of retinal stimulation in the form of long narrow rectangles of light (“slits”),
dark bars against a light background, or straight-line borders, all of which must have a
particular axis of orientation (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962, 1963).
Thus, in the visual cortex, narrow (50 µm) vertical strips of neurons respond to a bar
stimulus of the same orientation (orientation columns) and wider strips (500 µm) respond
preferentially to stimuli detected by one eye (ocular dominance columns). Adjacent
orientation columns aggregate within an ocular dominance column to form a hypercolumn,
responding to all orientations of a stimulus for both eyes for one point in the visual field
(Lynch, 1980; Peters and Jones, 1999; Casanova Manuel, 2005; Crossman, 2008).
Thus, in the visual cortex, at least three types of regularly repeating features are
superimposed on the laminar patterns of neurons: the stimulus orientation columns, the
ocular dominance columns, and the cytochrome oxidaze-rich blobs.
1620
21. Cerebral cortex
The ocular dominance columns in the visual cortex provide a clear example of the
role of thalamic inputs in columnar organization.
Neurons in the layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus that receive inputs from the
right eye send their axons to layer IV of the right eye-dominant columns. Here, the axons
terminate predominantly on spiny and aspiny stellate cells, which, in turn, project to
pyramidal cells. Collaterals of pyramidal cell axons provide one pathway by which neural
signals can spread from one column to influence activity in adjacent columns.
This influence may be either excitatory via direct connections, or inhibitory via
interneurons.
The right eye has a direct and strong influence on neurons in right-eye-dominant
columns and an indirect and weaker influence on neurons in the adjacent left-eye-
dominant columns (Lynch, 2006).
When a microelectrode was inserted at right angles to the surface of the cortex, all of
the neurons encountered were activated by only one of these submodalities (Fig 21.6, B).
In contrast, when a microelectrode was moved parallel or obliquely relative to the
surface of the cortex, it encountered neurons of different submodalities as it moved from
one functionally related group of neurons to another (Fig. 21.6, A) (Lynch, 2006).
If all of the cells in one column respond to maintained pressure on the skin, the cells
in an adjacent column respond to joint position.
Similar columnar organization has been described in widespread areas of the
neocortex, including the motor cortex and association areas.
According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), most interactions between the layers of
the cortex take place within the cells directly above or below the adjacent layers.
There have been many terms for the vertical organization of the cortex, two of the
most common being column and module. Although these terms are not always
interchangeable, the underlying idea is that groups of 150 to 300 neurons form little
circuits ranging from about 0.5 to 2.0 mm wide, depending on the cortical region.
Evidence for some kind of modular unit comes from two principal sources: staining and
probing (Purves et al., 1992).
If a radioactive aminoacid is injected into one eye of a monkey, the radioactivity is
transported across synapses to the primary visual cortex. The radioactivity is not evenly
distributed across the cortex, however, in that it travels only to places that connect with
the affected eye. Thus, the pattern of radioactivity seen in the primary visual cortex (area
17) is a series of stripes, much like those on a zebra (Purves et al., 1992).
A different pattern is seen in the same visual cortex when a different technique is used.
If the cortex is stained with cytochrome oxidaze, which shows areas of high metabolic
activity by staining mitochondria, the visual cortex appears spotted. These spots, known as
“blobs”, have a role in colour perception (Fig. 21.7, A and B) (Purves et al., 1992).
Curiously, if the same stain is applied to area 18, which is an adjacent visual region, the
staining pattern looks more like stripes (Fig. 21.7, C) than like spots (Purves et al., 1992).
Finally, if the primary somatosensory cortex of a rat is stained with succinic dehydro-
genaze, the cortex shows a pattern of spots that are known as “barrels” (Fig. 21.7, D). Each
barrel corresponds to one of the vibrissae on the face of the rat (Purves et al., 1992).
Fig. 21.7. Spots and stripes in the cortex, obtained by staining (after Purves et al., 1992).
1622
21. Cerebral cortex
patterns in the cortex may well correspond to secondary functions of cortical organization.
One suggested that the cortex forms its intrinsic connections to process information; one
efficient pattern of connectivity is the vertical module.
The module certainly conforms to an important aspect of cortical connectivity, but
it does not cause cortical connectivity. There must be an alternative way (or ways) of
organizing complex neural activity that does not require a constant module. Although a
cortical organization with columns is a useful arrangement, it is not the only way to
organize a brain (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Fig. 21.9. A schematic drawing of the six layers of cortex, the gray matter. Note that some cortical neurons
send axons to the thalamus, while other receive input from thalamic neurons. Ipsilateral = same side
of the cortex; Contralateral = opposite side.
two layers. Specific afferents include projections from the thalamus as well as from the
amygdala. Most of these projections terminate in layer IV, although projections from the
amygdala and certain thalamic nuclei may terminate in more superficial layers.
2) Nonspecific afferents presumably serve general functions, such as maintaining a
level of tone or arousal, so that the cortex can process information. They terminate
diffusely over large regions of the cortex – in some cases, over all of it. Nonspecific
afferents even release their transmitter substances into the extracellular space. The
norepinephrinergic projections from the brain stem, the cholinergic projections from the
basal forebrain, and the projections from certain thalamic nuclei are examples of
nonspecific afferents (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Despite significant variations among different cytoarchitectonic areas, the circuitry
of all cortical regions has some common features. First, each cortical layer has a primary
source of inputs and a primary output target. Second, each area has connections in the
vertical axis (called columnar or radial connections) and connections in the horizontal
1626
21. Cerebral cortex
axis (called lateral or horizontal connections). Third, cells with similar functions tend to
be arrayed in radially aligned groups that span all of the cortical layers and receive inputs
that are often segregated into radial or columnar bands. Finally, interneurons within
specific cortical layers give rise to extensive local axons that extend horizontally in the
cortex, often linking functionally similar groups of cells. The particular circuitry of any
cortical region is a variation on this canonical pattern of inputs, outputs and vertical and
horizontal patterns of connectivity (Purves et al., 2004).
Fig. 21.10. Cerebral hemispheres of the human with Brodmann’s areas applied. From Vergleichende
Lokalisationsliehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund der Zellenbaues.
1628
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.11. Cytoarchitectural map showing Brodmann areas on the lateral (A) and medial (B) surface of the
hemisphere. (Modified after Brodmann K from Carpenter MB, Sutin J: Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore,
Williams and Wilkins, 1983.)
Brodmann’s map is very useful because the regions depicted in it correspond quite
closely with regions discovered with the use of noncytoarchitectonic techniques. Figure
21.11 summarises some of the relations between areas on Brodmann’s map and areas
that have been mapped according to their known functions (Brodmann, 1909; Everett
1965; Elliot, 1969). For example, area 17 corresponds to the primary visual projection
area, whereas areas 18 and 19 correspond to the secondary visual projection areas. Area
1629
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
4 is the primary motor cortex. Broca’s area related to the articulation of words is area 44,
areas 6 and 8 (eye movement) correspond to secondary areas. The motor territory
corresponds to areas 9, 10, 11, 45, 46 and 47.
Areas 1, 2 and 3 correspond to the primary body senses projection areas, areas 5 and
7 correspond to the secondary body senses projection areas, and areas 7, 22, 37, 39, and
40 correspond to the territory sensory projection areas.
One of the problems with Brodmann’s map is that new, more powerful analytical
techniques have shown that many Brodmann’s areas actually consist of two or more
architectonically distinct areas. For this reason, the map is continually being updated and now
consists of an unwieldy mixture of numbers, letters, and names (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Anyhow, Brodmann’s areas (BA) are distinct areas of the cerebral cortex based on
the organization of cells or the cytoarchitecture of the human brain. A functional
distinction of brain regions correlates remarkably well with this neuroanatomic
differentiation.
Some of these are so important that we will call them modules (high functional areas).
These modules are widely used today when discussing the results of neuroimaging studies.
At a microscopic level of description, we have the Brodmann areas.
When the surface layers of the cortex are carefully studied under a microscope, small
regional differences can be seen in the appearance of cells in the layers and their
connections (Baars, 2007).
However, about 70% of the human cerebral cortex is buried in sulci which
complicate our ability to visualize its extent from a surface view or topographic map. A
surface view of the brain thus hides the source of the majority of activation in imaging
studies. This problem is solved by the flat map, which allows images to visualize the
entire surface area of a hemisphere in a single view.
Van Essen and Drury (1997) used the MRI analysis of the Visible Man, a digital
atlas of the human body, to generate flat maps of the human cortex. Figure 21.12, A
shows a surface view of the Visible Man’s two hemispheres with the lobes, identified by
different shadings. Flat maps display the sulci and gyri in an alternative format
(Fig. 21.12, B), in which buried cortex (not visible from the exterior of the hemisphere)
is shown in darker shades.
The location of brain areas in a whole brain can be calculated by using a three-
dimensional atlas. Sections are taken at regular intervals (typically 4 mm in the human
brain). So, van Essen and Drury were able to identify the coordinates for the regions in
their flat map. Their coordinate system makes it possible to identify the location of
activations in imaging studies with respect to cortical surface.
Fig. 21.12. Cortical maps. (A) A digital surface map. (B) The cortex has been “flattened” so that all tissues hidden in
sulci are visible. Flat maps provide a perspective on the relative size of various cortical regions and on the amount of
tissue dedicated to different functions, as detailed in the table (after van Essen and Drury, 1997).
The pyramidal cells of the cortex, and therefore the output of the cortex, are
modulated by a variety of cortical afferents. The influence of these afferent fibres is to
act on pyramidal cells either directly or via interneurons. Most interneurons within the
cortex are GABAergic and inhibitory.
A variety of neuropeptides (monoamines) are also found in the cerebral cortex; they
influence not only populations of neurons but also local metabolic activity and vascular
smooth muscle. The most important monoamines in the cortex are (1) norepinephrine,
which originates from the locus ceruleus of the pons and distributes sparsely to all cortical
layers, (2) dopamine, which arises from the substantia nigra – pars compacta and the
adjacent ventral tegmental area and is found in moderate amounts in layers I and VI and
sparsely in II-V, and (3) serotonin, which arises from the raphe nuclei and distributes heavily
to all cortical layers (Vogt et al., 1997; Mesulam, 2004; Lynch, 2006).
1631
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Cortical areas
The cerebral cortex does not have a uniform structure. It has been mapped and
divided into a number of distinctive areas that differ from each other in total thickness,
in the thickness and density of individual layers and in the arrangement and number of
cells and fibres.
In 1905, Campbell defined some 20 cortical fields in the human cerebral cortex.
In 1909, Brodmann published a monograph that continues to guide the study of
neuroscience even today. The cerebral cortex was originally classified by Brodmann into
52 different cytoachitectural areas (Fig. 21.13), but he did not assign a function to each
area at that time. He numbered them 1 to 52, which reflects the order in which he
examined and mapped them. Additional studies have provided information correlating a
function to many of these areas.
The anatomical / functional correlation, however, is not as accurate as was thought
in the past.
Some of these areas are commonly referred to by both name and number, and should
be known by the student. These are: the primary somatosensory cortex (Brodmann’s areas
3, 1, and 2), motor cortex (Brodmann’s areas 4, 6, and 8), secondary sensory cortex
1632
21. Cerebral cortex
(Brodmann’s areas 5 and 7), visual cortex (Brodmann’s areas 17, 18 and 19), and auditory
cortex (Brodmann’s area 41 and 42). The functional areas of the cerebral cortex are shown
in figures 21.14 and 21.15.
Vogt and Vogt (1919) parcelled the human cerebral cortex into more than 200 areas
based on cellular patterns. The Vogts were leaders in this field, defining some 229 cortical
areas. Von Economo (1929) divided it into 109. Thus, according to von Economo (1929),
all cortical structure is reducible to five fundamental types (Fig. 21.16), based primarily
on the relative development of granule and pyramidal cells. Types 2, 3 and 4, known
respectively as the frontal, parietal, and polar types, are homotypical and constitute by
far the largest part of the cortex.
Type 1 (agranular) and 5 (granular) are heterotypical and limited to smaller
specialized regions (Fig. 21.17).
Even the last numbers are apparently insufficient, since other investigators have
found a number of distinctive cytoarchitectural fields in regions previously considered
homogeneous (Beck, 1929; Rose, 1935).
The brain map of Bailey and von Bonin (1951) utilizes various colours to distinguish
distinctive cytoarchitectural features. These authors felt that the concept of absolutely
sharp areal boundaries has been carried to absurd lengths and that most brain maps failed
to properly represent transitional areas.
Fig. 21.14. Lateral view of the cerebral hemisphere showing the functional areas of the cerebral cortex.
(Modified from Young PA, Young PH (1997), Basic Clinical Neuroanatomy. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore.)
1633
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The posterior part of the cortex is the sensory unit. It receives sensations, processes
them, and stores them as information.
The anterior cortex (the frontal lobe) is the motor unit. It formulates intentions,
organizes them into programs of action, and executes the programs. Both cortical units
have a hierarchical structure with three cortical zones arranged functionally one above
the other. The first zone corresponds to Flechsig’s primary cortex; the second corresponds
to the slower-developing cortex bordering the primary cortex, which Luria labelled
secondary cortex; and the third, the slowest-developing cortex, which Luria labelled
tertiary cortex.
Luria conceives of the cortical units as working in concert along zonal pathways.
Sensory input enters the primary sensory zones, is elaborated in the secondary zones,
and is integrated in the tertiary zones of the posterior unit. To execute an action, activation
is sent from the posterior tertiary sensory zones to the tertiary motor zone for formulation,
to the secondary motor zone for elaboration, and then to the primary frontal zone for
execution.
Information in the tertiary sensory zone activates the paralimbic cortex for memory
processing and the amygdala for emotional assessment. A lesion in the primary visual
1635
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
zone would produce a blind spot in some part of the visual field. A lesion in the secondary
visual zone might produce a perceptual deficit, making the person unable to recognize
the activity. A lesion in the tertiary sensory zone might make it impossible to recognize
the significance of the activity in its abstract form (somebody lost his money).
Damage to the paralimbic cortex would leave no memory of the event, and damage
to the amygdala would render the person unresponsive to the event’s emotional
significance. A lesion in the tertiary motor area might prevent the formation of the
intention. A lesion in the secondary motor zone might make it difficult to execute the
sequences of movements required in an activity. A lesion in the primary zone might make
it difficult to execute a discrete movement required in an action.
Luria (1973) based his theory on three assumptions:
1) The brain processes information is serially – that is, one step at a time. Thus,
information from sensory receptors goes to the thalamus, then to the primary cortex, then
to the secondary cortex, and finally to the tertiary sensory cortex. Similarly, the output
goes from tertiary sensory to tertiary motor, then to secondary motor, and finally to
primary motor.
2) Serial processing is hierarchical; that is, each level of processing adds complexity
that is qualitatively different from the processing in the preceding levels. The tertiary
cortex could be considered a “terminal station” in so far as it receives input from the
sensorimotor and perceptual areas and performs higher cognitive processes on that input.
3) Our perceptions of the world are unified and coherent entities. So, the same active
process creates each percept, and naturally the simplest way to do so is to form it in the
tertiary cortex.
According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), a strictly hierarchical processing model
requires that all cortical areas be linked serially, but this serial linkage is not the case.
All critical areas have reciprocal (reentrant) connections with the regions to which they
connect, which means that there is no simple “feed forward” system. Only about 40% of
the possible connections among different areas in a sensory modality are actually found.
Then, Zeki (1993) made the interesting point that, because a zone of cortex has
connections with many cortical areas, it follows that each cortical zone is probably
undertaking more than one operation, which is subsequently relayed to different cortical
areas. In addition, the results of the same operation are likely to be of interest to more
than one cortical area, which would account for multiple connections.
These principles can be seen in the primary visual cortex, which appear to make
calculations related to colour, motion, and form. These calculations are relayed to specific
cortical regions for these processes. And the same calculation may be sent to cortical as
well as to subcortical regions. This implies that cortical processing can bypass Luria’s
motor hierarchy and go directly to subcortical motor structures.
So, an area such as the primary visual cortex, which is processing colour, form, and
movement, might be considered more complex than an area that processes only colour
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Finally, Luria assumed that his introspection about perception being a unitary
phenomenon was correct.
1636
21. Cerebral cortex
There are two logical possibilities to put this knowledge together in a meaningful
way to see organization in the cortex. According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), one
possibility is that there is no hierarchical organization but rather some sort of nonordered
neural network. As individual organisms gain experiences, this network becomes ordered
in some way and so produces perceptions, cognitions, and memories. The results of a
wealth of perceptual research suggest that the brain filters and orders sensory information
in a species-typical fashion.
Felleman and van Essen (1991) suggested that cortical areas are hierarchically
organized in some well-defined sense, with each area occupying a specific position
relative to other areas, but with more than one area being allowed to occupy a given
hierarchical level. Felleman and van Essen (1991) proposed that the pattern of forward
and backward connections could be used to determine hierarchical position.
Thus, ascending (or forward) connections terminate in layer IV whereas descending
(or feedback) connections do not enter layer IV, usually terminating in the superficial
and deep layers (Fig. 21.18).
Fig. 21.18. Inter- and intraareal connections. (A) The flow of information to and from the cortex. Information from the
thalamus goes to the primary cortex, which then projects to the association cortex. Note the reciprocal connections at
each level, representing feedback loops. (B) The principle of the reentry. When one cortical area sends information to
another, the projection arises from layers II and III and terminates in layer IV. The receiver returns a connection (reentry)
from layers V and VI to layers I and VI of the first area. In this way, a receiving cortical area can modify the inputs that it is
getting. This reentry principle holds for all levels of cortical connectivity (after Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
1637
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
They also recognize a third type of connection, which is columnar in its distribution
and terminating in all layers. This type of connection is uncommon but provides a basis
for placing areas in the same location in the hierarchy.
By analysing the patterns of connectivity among the visual, auditory, and
somatosensory areas, Felleman and van Essen (1991) found evidence of what they call a
distributed hierarchical system.
The cerebral cortex is the organ of thought. More than any other part of the nervous
system, the cerebral cortex is the site of the intellectual functions that make us human
and that make each of us a unique individual. These intellectual functions include the
ability to use language and logic, and to exercise imagination and judgment (Lynch,
2006). Although other brain structures, including the thalamus, corpus striatum,
claustrum, and cerebellum contribute to these functions, the multimodal association
cortex is closely linked to the most complex intellectual functions, such as logical
analysis, judgment, language and imagination.
Neurons in the cortex receive input from many subcortical structures by way of the
thalamus and also from other regions of the cortex via association fibres.
Cortical neurons, in turn, project to a wide range of neural structures, including other
areas of the cerebral cortex, the thalamus, the basal nuclei, the cerebellum via the pontine
nuclei, many of the brain stem nuclei, and the spinal cord.
The cerebral cortex is divided into distinct functional areas, some of which are
devoted to the processing of incoming sensory information, others to the organization of
motor activity, and still others primarily to what are considered “higher intellectual
functions”.
These functions include memory, judgment, the planning of complex activities,
processing of language, mathematical calculations, and the construction of an internal
image of an individual’s surroundings. Thus, in discussing functions of the cortical areas
in humans, it is essential to understand that we are not dealing with properties inherent
in the cerebral cortex itself, but we are dealing with complete neuronal arcs or chains of
neurons that project their impulses from specific areas of the nervous system to their
terminations.
The functional features of any cortical area are dependent on the connections to and
from that area and the interrelations that exist between the various primary and secondary
sensory receptive areas, the motor projection areas and the primary, secondary, and
tertiary association areas (Augustine, 2008).
Cerebral dominance refers to the observation that one cerebral hemisphere of the
brain is more involved in certain functions such as language, handedness, musical talents,
visual-spatial abilities, attention, and emotion. (Amunts et al., 1996; Braitenburg and
Schuz, 1998; Augustine, 2008).
In some cases, there is a functional lateralization and, in other cases, there is an
accompanying structural asymmetry in the brain. The early studies of Broca and
Wernicke demonstrated the lateralization of language function to the left hemisphere.
1638
21. Cerebral cortex
Summary
Knowledge of the world is constructed by the brain. To Jerison, this knowledge is
mind. The levels of function in the brain are hierarchically organized and then focused
on the structure and functional organization of the cortex.
The cortex comprises spiny and aspiny neurons organized into about six layers
(sensory, motor and associational). The vertical organization of the cortex is referred to
as columns or modules. There are many cortical maps. As cortical maps develop, the
brain must also develop the mind to organize the maps in such a way as to produce
knowledge of the external world. The next step in mental development is language. After
all, language is the way of representing knowledge. Multiple representations of sensory
and motor functions exist in the cortex. One evolutionary change in mammals has been
an increase in the number of representations. The cortex processes information about the
world, in multiple representations, and these representations are not formally connected;
yet we perceive the world as a unified whole.
This conundrum is the “binding problem” (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The connections among cortical areas in a sensory system constitute only a part of
all cortical connections. The four other principal connections in the cortical hierarchy are
with the frontal lobe, paralimbic cortex, (phylogenetically the older cortex), multimodal
cortex, and subcortical connections and loops.
Sensory regions do not connect directly with the motor cortex but may project to
either the premotor or prefrontal cortex. Connections to the premotor cortex participate
in ordering movements in time and controlling hand, limb, or eye movements with respect
to specific sensory stimuli. Projections to the prefrontal cortex take part in the control of
movements in time and short-term memories of sensory information.
The paralimbic cortex is adjacent and directly connected to the limbic structures and it
comprises roughly three layers. It can be seen in two places: 1) on the medial surface of the
temporal lobe, where it is known as the piriform cortex, entorhinal cortex, and para-
hippocampal cortex; and 2) just above the corpus callosum, were it is referred to as the
cingulate cortex. The paralimbic cortex plays a role in the formation of long-term memories.
Sensory inputs from subcortical structures are received by the cortex, from the
thalamus or indirectly through midbrain structures, such as the subcortical loops.
Each level interacts and is integrated with higher and lower levels by ascending and
descending connections. Subcortical loops connect the cortex, thalamus, amygdala, and
hippocampus; an indirect loop with the striatum connects with the thalamus (Kolb and
Whishaw, 2003).
Subcortical loops are presumed to play some role in amplifying or modulating
cortical activity. The amygdala adds affective tone to visual input. In absence of the
amygdala, laboratory animals display absolutely no fear of threatening objects.
activity, modulation of sensory input and alterations of awareness and of the state of
consciousness (Carpenter, 1979).
Corticofugal fibres originating largely from the deeper layers of the cerebral cortex,
are projected to parts of the corpus striatum, the brain stem nuclei at all levels, and the
spinal cord.
Cortical efferent fibres projecting to other cortical areas of the same hemisphere are
designated as associational, while those projecting to cortical areas of the opposite
hemisphere are commissural. The frontal lobe is the rostral region of the hemisphere,
anterior to the central sulcus and above the lateral fissure. The precentral gyrus runs
parallel to the central sulcus on the superolateral surface, extends into the medial surface,
and is limited anteriorly by the precentral sulcus.
The motor activity of the entire opposite side of the body is controlled by the motor
areas of the cerebral cortex (area 4) that are located anterior to the central sulcus. Fibres
arise from the primary motor cortex (MI), the secondary motor cortex (MII), and the
primary somatosensory (somesthetic) cortex (SI) of the frontal and parietal lobes to
terminate in the motor nuclei of the brain stem and spinal cord. Cytoarchitecturally area
4 represents a modification of the typical six-layered isocortex in which the pyramidal
cells in layers III and V are increased in number and the internal granular layer is
obscured. For this reason this cortex is called agranular.
Thus, nerve cells in the primary motor cortex are organized into groups, each group
sending its axons to the cranial nerve motor nuclei, or the reticular formation, or the gray
matter of the spinal cord, where they control the motor activity of a single muscle.
The rostral border of area 4 has been distinguished physiologically as a distinct
subdivision, referred to as area 4S (Hines, 1936, 1937). Ablation of this narrow strip of
cortex along the rostral border of area 4 in monkeys was said to produce a transient spastic
paralysis, and stimulation of this area was reported to inhibit extensor muscle tone.
Subsequent studies indicated that area 4S was one of a number of cortical areas from
which suppressor effects could be obtained in response to stimulation (Dusser de Barenne
and Mc Culloch, 1939).
Thus, on the lateral surface of the brain, the precentral sulcus bounds the precentral
gyrus anteriorly and the central sulcus bounds it posteriorly. However, on the medial
surface of the brain there is no reliable anatomical landmark for the anterior border of
area 4.
The primary motor cortex (Brodmann’s area 4) plays an important role in the
execution of distinct, well defined, voluntary movements. The precentral gyrus of the
right cerebral hemisphere controls movement of the left side of the head and body,
whereas that of the left cerebral hemisphere controls the right side of the head and body
(Passingham, 1993).
Studies in primates have demonstrated two spatially separate motor representations
of the arm and hand in area 4 (Amunts et al., 1996; Augustine, 2008). The long axes of
these representations are oriented approximately parallel to the central sulcus. Stimulation
in both areas causes the same movements and activates the same muscles at similar
strengths. This phenomenon is termed the double representation hypothesis. These two
different areas or subareas of area 4 differ with regard to their somatosensory input. The
caudal area relates to movements that use tactile feedback for their execution whereas
the rostral area relates to movements that use proprioceptive feedback for their execution.
These findings along with cytoarchitectural observations and quantitative distri-
butions of transmitter-binding sites have led to the suggestion that area 4 in humans
includes area 4 anterior (4a) and area 4 posterior (4p) (Augustine, 2008).
In addition to the motor representation across area 4, tactile and proprioceptive maps
occur in 4a and 4p as well. Thus, the primary motor area in humans can be subdivided
based on anatomy, neurochemistry, and function.
The thumb, index, and middle fingers have a representation in both 4a and 4p
(Augustine, 2008).
The primary motor cortex (MI) corresponds to the precentral gyrus (area 4), and is
the area of cortex with the lowest threshold for eliciting contralateral muscle contraction
by electrical stimulation.
It contains a detailed topographically organized map (motor homunculus) of the
opposite body half, with the head represented most laterally, and the leg and foot
represented on the medial surface of the hemisphere in the paracentral lobule (Fig. 21.19)
(Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950).
1642
21. Cerebral cortex
Electrical stimulation of the motor area evokes discrete isolated movements on the
opposite side of the body. Usually, the contraction involves the functional muscle groups
concerned with a specific movement, but individual muscles, even a single interosseous,
may be contracted separately.
Flexion or extension at a single finger joint, twitchings at the corners of the mouth,
elevation of the palate, protrusion of the tongue and even vocalization, expressed in
involuntary cries or exclamation, all may be evoked by careful stimulation of the proper
areas. Charts of motor representation, which are in substantial agreement, have been
furnished by a number of investigators (Foerster 1936 a, 1936 b; Penfield and Boldrey,
1937; Scarff, 1940; Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950; Penfield and Jasper, 1954).
The location of centre specific movements may vary from individual to individual,
but the sequence of motor representation appears constant. Ipsilateral movements have
not been observed in men, but bilateral responses occur in muscles of the eyes, face,
tongue, jaw, larynx and pharynx. According to Penfield and Boldrey (1937), the centre
for the pharynx (swallowing) lies in the most inferior opercular portion of the precentral
gyrus; it is followed, from below upward, by centres for the tongue, jaw, lips, larynx,
eyelid and brow, in the order named. Next come the extensive areas for finger
movements, the thumb being lowest and the little finger highest; these are followed by
areas for the hand, wrist, elbow and shoulder. Finally, in the most superior part, are the
centres for the hip, knee, ankle and toes. The last named are situated at the medial border
1643
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
of the hemisphere and extend into the paracentral lobule, which also contains the centres
for the anal and vesicle sphincters.
There has been considerable controversy regarding the location of representation of
the lower extremity, due mainly to difficulties in stimulating the medial surface.
According to Foerster (1936 a), the paracentral lobule contains foci related to the foot,
the toes, the bladder and the rectum. Penfield and Boldrey (1937) reported leg movements
in 23 cases produced by the stimulation of superior portions of the precentral gyrus, but
Scarff (1940) was unable to elicit any leg movements by stimulating the lateral surface
of the hemisphere. According to Scarff (1940), the leg, as a rule, is represented only on
the medial surface of the hemisphere (i.e., in the paracentral lobule). The threshold in
different topographical parts of area 4 varies. The region of the thumb appears to have
the lowest threshold, while the face area has the highest threshold.
Motor cortical neurons are active in relation to the force of contraction of agonist
muscles; their relation to amplitude of movement is less clear. Their activity precedes
the onset of electromyographic activity by 50 to 100 milliseconds, suggesting a role for
cortical activation in generating rather than monitoring movement (Passingham, 1993).
A striking feature is the disproportionate representation of body parts in relation to
their physical size: large areas represent the muscle of the face and hand, which are
capable of finely controlled or fractionated movements. The hand area is on the middle
third of the human precentral gyrus. The size of its representation reflects its extraordinary
capabilities to sense temperatures, to participate in the active sense of touch in exploring
the world around us, as well as carry out complex fine skilled movements including
communicating with our hands. In sum, the representation of the knee, leg, ankle, foot
and bladder begin on the edge of each cerebral hemisphere and pass over onto the anterior
paracentral lobule.
The bladder region is just anterior to the region representing the ankle and the toes.
The part of the precentral gyrus controlling movement of the toes is located near its
superior aspect, whereas the part of the precentral gyrus controlling movement of the
tongue, mouth, and larynx is located near its inferior aspect (bordering the lateral fissure).
Half of it is associated with motor activity of the hands, tongue, lips, and larynx –
reflecting the manual dexterity and ability for speech that humans possess.
Prior to the onset of movement, the primary motor cortex receives instructions and
information about the pattern of the intended movement from the other motor areas of
the cortex.
The ipsilateral somatosensory cortex (SI) projects in a topographically organized
way to area 4, and the connection is reciprocal. The projection to the motor cortex arises
in areas 1 and 2, with little or no contribution from area 3b. SI fibres terminate in layers
II and III of area 4, where they contact mainly pyramidal neurons. Evidence suggests
that neurons activated, monosynaptically, fibres from SI, as well as those activated
polysynaptically, make contact with layer V pyramidal cells, including Betz cells, which
give rise to corticospinal fibres (Passingham, 1993; Bodegard et al., 2000; Geyer et al.,
2000; Kaas, 2004). Movement-related neurons in the motor cortex which can be activated
1644
21. Cerebral cortex
from SI tend to have a late onset activity, mainly during the execution of movement. It
has been suggested that this pathway plays a role primarily in making motor adjustments
during a movement. Additional ipsilateral corticocortical fibres to area 4 from behind the
central sulcus come from the second somatic sensory area (S II) (Burton et al., 1995;
Amunts et al., 1996; Geyer et al., 1999; Disbrow et al., 2000; Bodegord et al., 2000;
Parsons et al., 2005).
The motor cortex receives major frontal lobe association fibres form the premotor
cortex and the supplementary motor area and also fibres from the insula (Augustine,
1985; 1996; Geyer et al., 1996; Kaas, 2004).
It is probable that these pathways modulate motor cortical activity in relation to the
preparation, guidance and temporal organization of movements. Area 4 sends fibres to,
and receives fibres from its contralateral counterpart, and also projects to the contralateral
supplementary motor cortex (Crossman, 2008).
Apart from its contribution to the corticospinal tract, the motor cortex has diverse
subcortical projections. The connections with the thalamus, striatum, pontine nuclei and
the subthalamic nucleus are strong.
Thus, the major thalamic connections of area 4 are with the ventral posterolateral
nucleus (VPL), which in turn receives afferents from the deep cerebellar nuclei. The VPL
nucleus also contains a topographic representation of the contralateral body, which is
preserved in its point-to-point projection to area 4, where it terminates largely in lamina IV.
Other thalamic connections of area 4 are with the centromedian and parafascicular
nuclei. These appear to provide the only route through which output from the basal
ganglia, routed via the thalamus, reaches the primary motor cortex, since the projection
of the internal segment of the globus pallidus to the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus
is confined to the anterior division, and there is no overlap with cerebellothalamic
territory.
The anterior part of the ventrolateral nucleus projects to the premotor and
supplementary motor areas of the cortex with no projection to area 4 (Crossman, 2008).
Corticospinal tract. The corticospinal tract, which is considered to transmit direct
impulses for highly skilled volitional movements to lower motor neurons, arises mainly
from area 4. The larger corticospinal fibres are probably the axons of giant pyramidal
cells, for these cells undergo chromatolysis following a section of the pyramid (Holmes
and May, 1909; Levin and Bradford, 1938).
Since the number of fibres in the human corticospinal tract at the level of the pyramid
is approximately 1,000,000, axons of the giant cells of Betz could account for only a little
over 3% of these fibres, assuming that each cell gives rise to a single corticospinal fibre
(Lassek, 1940). Studies of the fibre spectrum of the human corticospinal tract, indicating
about 30,000 fibres with diameters between 9 and 22 µm (Lassek and Rasmussen, 1939,
1940; Lassek, 1954), strongly support the view that these fibres are the parent axons of
the giant pyramidal cells.
Approximately 90% of the fibres of the corticospinal tract range from 1 to 4 µm in
diameter. Of the total number of fibres in the tract, about 40% are poorly myelinated.
1645
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The more numerous small fibres of the corticospinal tract are considered to arise from
smaller cells in this and other cortical regions.
Interruption of the corticospinal tract also gives rise to chromatolytic cell changes
in small pyramidal cells in layers III and V, not only in area 4, but also in areas 3, 1, 2
and 5 (Levin and Bradford, 1938). Ablations of area 4 in monkeys cause degeneration of
27 to 40% of the corticospinal tract, including virtually all of the large myelinated fibres
(Lassek, 1942, 1954). Other authors have reported smaller percentages of degenerated
fibres after similar ablations (Häggqvist, 1937), but more reported complete degeneration
of the corticospinal tract (Mettler 1944 a; Welch and Kennard, 1944).
Ablations of the parietal cortex (areas 3, 1, 2, 5 and 7) also produce degeneration of
myelinated fibres in the corticospinal tract (Minkowski, 1923-1924; Peele, 1942).
Combined ablations of the precentral and postcentral gyri cause degeneration of 50
to 60% of the fibres in the corticospinal tract (Lassek, 1942, 1942 a; Russell and De
Myer, 1961).
A quantitative study of the origin of corticospinal fibres in monkeys based on silver
staining methods (Russell and De Myer, 1961) indicates that virtually all fibres of the
corticospinal tract arise from area 4, area 6 and parts of the parietal lobe.
Approximate percentages of corticospinal fibres arising from these areas are as follows:
(1) area 4, 31%; (2) area 6, 29%; and (3) parietal lobe, 40%. Complete decortication, or
hemispherectomy, causes all fibres of the corticospinal tract to degenerate in men (Lassek
and Evans, 1945) and in monkeys (Mattler, 1944 a; Russel and De Myer, 1961).
Thus, the corticospinal fibres arise from the pyramidal cells in layer V and give rise
to the largest corticospinal axons in diameter. There is also a widespread origin from
other parts of the frontal lobe, including the premotor cortex and the supplementary motor
area. Many axons from the frontal cortex, notably the motor cortex, terminate in the
ventral horn of the spinal cord. In cord segments mediating dexterous hand and finger
movements they terminate in the lateral part of the ventral horn, in close relationship to
the motor neuronal group. A small percentage establishes direct monosynaptic connec-
tions with alpha motor neurons.
Between 40 and 60% of pyramidal tract axons arise from parietal areas, including
area 3a, area 5 of the superior parietal lobe, and S II in the parietal operculum. The
majority of parietal fibres to the spinal cord terminate in the deeper layers of the dorsal
horn.
The motor cortex also projects to the subthalamic nucleus. The motor cortex sends
projections to all nuclei in the brain stem, which are themselves the origin of descending
pathways to the spinal cord, namely the reticular formation, the red nucleus, the superior
colliculus, the vestibular nuclei and the inferior olivary nucleus.
Ablation of the motor cortex in mammals produces increasingly greater
neurological deficits at progressively higher levels of the phylogenetic scale (Walker and
Fulton, 1938).
In cats, the removal of the motor cortex, or even hemidecortication, do not impair
the animal’s ability to walk upon recovery from anesthesia.
1646
21. Cerebral cortex
According to Bucy (1949), the spasticity is not severe and is less intense than that
commonly associated with hemiplegias resulting from large capsular lesions.
Although there is considerable restitution of function in proximal muscle groups,
relatively little recovery of skilled motor function occurs in the smaller distal muscles of
the extremities.
However, immediately after complete or partial lesions of the precentral gyrus, there
is a flaccid paralysis of the contralateral limbs or limb, marked hypotonia and loss of
superficial and myotatic reflexes. Within a relatively short time, the Babinski sign can
be elicited. The myotatic reflexes generally return early in an exaggerated form.
The total cortical area that mediates motor activity of a particular body region is
proportional to the complexity of the movements produced in that region.
1648
21. Cerebral cortex
The primary motor cortex then translates this information into execution of
movement, and relays it mainly to the brain stem or spinal cord. Most of the nerve signals
that arise in the secondary motor cortex mediate complex movements produced by groups
of muscles performing a particular task, unlike the discrete muscle contractions elicited
by the stimulation of the primary motor cortex (Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
PREMOTOR CORTEX
Immediately in front of the primary motor cortex lies Brodmann’s area 6. It extends
onto the medial surface, where it becomes contiguous with area 24 in the cingulate gyrus,
anterior and inferior to the paracentral lobule. Near the superior border it is quite broad
and includes the caudal portion of the superior frontal gyrus. The rostral border of area
6 on both medial and lateral surfaces of the human brain is unclear. On the medial surface
of the brain, area 6 extends inferiorly to the cingulate sulcus where its caudal border is
Brodmann’s area 24, part of the anterior cingulate cortex.
Inferiorly the premotor area narrows, and near the operculum, it is limited to the
precentral gyrus. Its histological structure resembles that of the motor area; it is composed
principally of large well formed pyramidal cells, but there are no giant cells of Betz. The
presence of pyramidal cells in layers III and V and the narrowness of layer V make it
difficult to distinguish an internal granular layer (Campbell, 1905).
For this reason, area 6, like area 4, is referred to as the agranular frontal cortex.
Area 6 has been subdivided into various portions (Fig. 21.21) (Foerster, 1936 b).
According to this parcellation, area 6aα lies immediately rostral to area 4 along the
convexity of the hemisphere, while area 6aβ occupies the region of the superior frontal
gyrus on both the lateral and medial surfaces of the hemisphere. A small area called 6b
lies in front of the face area.
Fig. 21.21. The areas of electrically excitable cortex on the lateral surface of the human brain.
The motor area is shown in black, and the so-called “extrapyramidal areas” are hatched except for the eye fields,
which are stippled (after Foerster, 1936 b).
1649
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The dorsal premotor area also receives fibres from the posterior temporal cortex and
projects to the supplementary motor cortex.
The frontal eye field (area 8) projects to the dorsal subdivision. Perhaps the greatest
functionally significant difference in connectivity between the two premotor area
subdivisions is that the dorsal premotor area receives fibres from the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex, whereas the ventral subdivision receives fibres from the ventrolateral
cortex. All of these association connections are likely to be or are known to be, reciprocal
(Crossman, 2008).
The main function of this area is the motor control of axial and proximal limb
musculature. It also plays a role in orienting the body and upper limbs in the direction of
a target. Once a motor task has been initiated, activity in the premotor cortex diminishes,
reflecting its key function in the planning phase of motor activity.
A motor pattern exists across the premotor cortex with responses that are
contralateral, but not as discrete as the responses that follow stimulation of the primary
motor cortex.
Neuronal activity in the premotor cortex in relation to both preparation for movement
and movement itself has been extensively studied experimentally. Thus, according to
Crossman (2008) direction selectivity for movements is a common feature of many
premotor neurons. In behavioral tasks, neurons in the dorsal premotor cortex show
anticipatory activity and task-related discharge as well as direction selectivity, but little
or no stimulus-related changes.
The dorsal premotor cortex is probably important in establishing a motor set or
intention, contributing to motor preparation in relation to internally guided movements.
In contrast, the ventral premotor cortex is more related to the execution of externally
(especially visually) guided movements in relation to a specific external stimulus
(Crossman, 2008).
According to Roland and Zilles (1996), Zilles (2004), and Augustine (2008), the
premotor cortex is an extrapyramidal motor area in that it supplements the activity of the
primary motor cortex. The premotor cortex is also termed the frontal aversive field in
that stimulation of this area causes rotation of the head, eyes, and trunk to the opposite
side with complex synergistic movements of flexion and extension of the contralateral
arm and leg. In addition to the motor representation across area 6, a tactile map occurs
across this area as well.
Injury to the premotor cortex produces a motor apraxia. This purely cortical
concept refers to the inability to carry out a sequence of motor activities in the presence
of intact motor and sensory paths. Motor apraxia is evident through clumsiness in writing
or drawing (Augustine, 2008).
A grasp reflex is likely to appear as well. Following damage to the primary motor
cortex, the premotor cortex is able to substitute to some degree.
This region also participates in cortical automatic associated movements that
accompany fine skilled voluntary movements. The grasp reflex (grasping of the
examiner’s hand) is elicited by firmly stroking the palm fingers of the patient’s hand in
1651
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
an outward direction. The grasp tends to persist, and the more force exerted, the greater
the intensity of the grasp. Unlike a normal grasp, the thumb usually remains extended.
A grasp reflex appearing in newborns and disappearing at two to four months is
likely to be marked by suspending a child by his grasp. In older individuals, the primary
motor cortex inhibits this reflex.
The SMA contains a representation of the body in which the leg is posterior and the
face anterior, with the upper limb between them. According to Augustine (2008), SMA
is somatotopically organized in humans with index finger responses anterior and dorsal
to shoulder responses.
Unilateral ablations of the SMA in men produce no permanent deficit in the
maintenance of posture or the capacity of movement (Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950;
Penfield and Welch, 1951).
According to Travis (1955), unilateral ablations of the supplementary motor area in
monkeys produce weak transient grasp reflexes in the contralateral limbs, and moderate
bilateral hypertonia of the shoulder muscles, but no paresis.
Bilateral simultaneous ablations of this area result in disturbances of posture and
tonus, but produce no paresis. Gradually increasing hypertonia, resulting in muscle
contracture, develops in a period of 2 to 4 weeks.
The hypertonia is mainly in flexor muscles. Myotatic reflexes are hyperactive, and
clonus can be demonstrated. Ablations of the SMA and the precentral motor area on the
same side result in an immediate contralateral hypotonic paresis with impaired myotatic
reflexes. Within a period of 2 weeks, the hypotonus changes to hypertonus and the
myotatic reflexes become exaggerated.
All evidence indicates that the SMA is a bilaterally functioning entity concerned
primarily with mechanisms of posture and movement. Following bilateral simultaneous
ablations of the SMA in monkeys indicate that such lesions produce a milder increase in
muscle tone, inconsistent reflex changes and no evidence of joint contracture (Coxe and
Landau, 1965).
According to Bertrand (1956), ablations of the SMA do not produce degeneration
passing to the spinal cord, but electrical stimulation of the SMA in monkeys evokes
potentials bilaterally in the region of the corticospinal tracts at spinal cord levels. The
bilateral features of this system are striking and appear to explain why unilateral lesions
of the SMA cause only minor deficits.
Penfield and Jasper (1954) report that motor responses from the SMA can be elicited
after the removal of the precentral gyrus. Stimulation of this motor area in men, following
hemispherectomy, induces ipsilateral movements similar to those which can be produced
voluntarily (Bates, 1953).
According to Augustine (2008), complex motor tasks activate the rostral SMA
whereas simple motor tasks activate the caudal SMA proper. Pre-SMA is involved in the
acquisition of new motor sequences whereas SMA proper participates in the acquisition
of motor sequences that are essentially automatic.
Activation of SMA occurs during imagined movements whereas regional cerebral
blood flow increases in this area during complex sequential movements (Gelnar, 1998;
Bodegård et al., 2000; Fuster, 2001; Kaas, 2004).
According to Crossman (2008), the role of the SMA in the control of movement is
primarily in complex tasks which require temporal organization of sequential movements
and in the retrieval of motor memory. Input from the basal ganglia assists the SMA in its
1653
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
role in the programming phase of the patterns and sequences of complex movements,
and the coordination of bilateral movements. The SMA mediates muscle contractions of
the axial (trunk) and proximal limb (girdle) musculature (i.e., the muscle controlling
movements of the arm and thigh). Stimulation of the supplementary motor area in
conscious patients has been reported to elicit the sensation of an urge to move, or of
anticipation that a movement is about to occur. A region anterior to the SMA for face
representation (area 44, 45) is important in vocalization and speech production.
In sum, the traditionally defined supplementary motor area includes two separate
regions: a caudal region (SMA proper) that has reciprocal connections with the primary
motor area and project to the spinal cord, and a rostral region (presupplementary motor
area) that receives projections from the prefrontal and cingulate cortices. Basal ganglia
input reaches the caudal region, whereas cerebellar input reaches the rostral region.
Neuronal responses to visual stimuli prevail in the rostral region, whereas somatosensory
responses prevail in the caudal region. The urge to initiate movement in humans is elicited
only from the rostral region (Wise, 1985; Tanji and Kurata, 1989; Tanji, 1994; Gallese
et al., 1996).
Smooth pursuit allows for continuous, undistorted vision of objects moving across
the visual field by matching the velocity of eye movements with the velocity of a target.
Smooth pursuit likely requires a more complicated neural network, and more processing
in cortical, basal ganglia, brain stem, and cerebellar regions for proper functioning
(Krauzlis, 2004).
Because the generation of saccades can be largely automatic and independent of
cortical structures, study of “top-down” control of saccades can be used to model
cognitive aspects of motor control (Stuphorn and Schall, 2002).
Multiple regions within the frontal cortex and subcortical nuclei are involved in the
generation of eye movements. For saccades, it is believed that the frontal lobe oculomotor
regions are most important for voluntary saccades, whereas accurate reflexive saccades
can be generated with little input from the frontal lobes (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2004).
Data from both monkeys and humans support the roles for the frontal lobes in both
movement perception and representation (Rizzolati and Luppino, 2001) as well as
planning and execution of movements.
The first intraoperative stimulation studies in humans resulted in a broad region for
the frontal eye fields because eye movements were elicited from various areas
encompassing Brodmann’s areas 6, 8, 9 and 46 (Foerster, 1936; Penfield and Boldrey,
1937; Smith, 1944). The latest studies, in which smaller current intensities have been
used, restricted the frontal eye fields to the anterior portion of the motor strip, in the upper
portion of the facial area (Woolsey et al., 1979; Lüders et al., 1988; Godoy et al., 1990).
However, the sulcal cortex could not be investigated with large subdural electrodes, and
the mediolateral position of frontal eye fields remains imprecise. Positron emission
tomography studies also have been utilised to improve our knowledge of the localization
and function of the frontal eye fields (Fox et al., 1985; Anderson et al., 1994; O’Sullivan
et al., 1995; Petit et al., 1996; Sweeney et al., 1996).
The results of these studies have indicated that human frontal eye fields are located
more posteriorly in the frontal lobe than expected by a comparison with monkey frontal
eye fields.
In humans, fMRI imaging, with its increased spatial resolution, has provided new
insights into the functional anatomy of frontal eye fields (Darby et al., 1996; Petit et al.,
1997; Luna et al., 1998; Corbetta et al., 1998).
Contrary to studies performed using subdural electrodes, intracerebral electrical
stimulation enables localized stimulation of almost any part of the cortex, including the
sulcal cortex (Munari et al., 1993, 1994).
Lobel et al. (2001) used intracerebral electrical stimulation in 38 patients with
epilepsy prior to surgery, and frontal regions where stimulation induced versive eye
movement were identified. These studies showed that two distinct oculomotor areas could
be individualized in the region classically corresponding to the frontal eye fields. One
oculomotor area was consistently at the intersection of the superior frontal sulcus with
the fundus of the superior portion of the precentral sulcus. The second oculomotor area
was located more laterally and close to the surface of the precentral gyrus. Lobel et al.
1655
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
(2001) compared results obtained from fMRI imaging with those obtained using
intracerebral electrical stimulation.
These two areas are anatomically distinct (mean distance from each other is 19 mm).
The deep oculomotor areas are the regions of the human frontal lobe where eye
movements can be most easily elicited by electrical stimulation. It is located more
posteriorly in the frontal lobe, apparently within Brodmann’s area 6. The most remarkable
feature is the specific anatomical link between the deep oculomotor areas and the
intersection of the superior frontal sulcus.
Anatomy. The frontal eye field in men occupies principally the caudal part of the
middle frontal gyrus (corresponding to parts of area 8) and extends into contiguous
portions of the inferior frontal gyrus (Carpenter, 1979).
According to Boxer (2007), in the frontal lobes, three regions are most involved in
eye movement control: the frontal eye field, the supplementary eye fields, and the
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Fig. 21.22). These frontal eye movement control regions
are strongly connected to regions in the dorsal parietal lobe involved in eye movement
control, particularly in the vicinity of the lateral intraparietal sulcus, the basal ganglia,
and brain stem structures.
Fig. 21.22. Brain regions involved in saccade control. In the frontal lobes, frontal eye field (FEF) is a critical node for
initiation of voluntary saccades. It receives input from the parietal eye fields (PEFs) and thalamus, which provide
visual information. The supplementary eye field (SEF) monitors and supervises FEF activity, and the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) likely exerts a direct modulatory influence on the FEF, as well as on downstream structures
in the brain stem. Other regions in the medial frontal lobes, including the pre-supplementary motor area (pSMA) and
anterior cingulate (cing), may also exert modulatory influences on the FEF via the SEF and DLPFC. There are three
main pathways from the FEF to the primary and secondary motor neurons in the brain stem nuclei that generate
saccades: (1) a direct connection between the FEF and the brain stem oculomotor nuclei; (2) a pathway through the
caudate eye field and substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr), which helps to suppress saccades; and (3) a direct
connection between the FEF and superior colliculus (SC), which is finely tuned for saccade programming and can
generate reflexive saccades independently of the FEF (after Boxer, 2007).
1656
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.23. Lateral surface of the left cerebral hemisphere showing the frontal eye field (parts of areas 6, 8, 9), the motor
speech (Broca’s) area (areas 44, 45) and Wernicke’s area. The perimeter of these areas is delineated by an interrupted
line to indicate uncertainly as to their precise extent. Wernicke’s area is variously depicted by different authorities as
encompassing a large parietotemporal area which includes areas 39 and 40. Areas 22 and 37 are considered by some to
be respectively auditory and visuo-auditory areas associated with speech and language (after Crossman, 2008).
According to Augustine (2008) and Crossman (2008), there are primary and
secondary eye fields that occupy parts of Brodmann’s areas 6, 8 and 9 (Fig. 21.23). The
primary frontal eye field is in the posterior part of the middle frontal gyrus. The secondary
frontal eye field is in the dorsal part of the inferior frontal gyrus. Cytoarchitecturally,
area 8 is a typical six layered isocortex of the frontal type in which the granular layers
are distinct.
Processes of neurons in the eye fields project their fibres through the corticobulbar
path to nuclei of the ocular muscle. The primary eye fields project fibres to agonist
muscles, whereas neurons in the secondary eye fields project fibres to antagonist muscles.
Voluntary ocular movements obviously involve the cooperative action of eye field in
both cerebral hemispheres (Paus, 1996; Lebel et al., 2001).
Frontal eye field. The frontal eye field is located in the rostral bank of the arcuate
sulcus in macaque monkeys (Stuphorn and Schall, 2002). In humans, high-resolution
fMRI studies have localized the frontal eye field to the anterior wall of the precentral
sulcus, near the caudal end of the superior frontal sulcus, a region homologous to the
location of the frontal eye field in monkeys (Rosano et al., 2002).
1657
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The frontal eye field can be further subdivided into two regions, one involved in the
generation of saccades, which is located in the upper portion of the anterior wall of the
precentral sulcus, and a smooth pursuit-related area, which is found deeper along the anterior
wall, extending in some subjects to the fundus or deep posterior wall. Using histopathological
markers from postmortem specimens, these regions have been demonstrated to be structurally
similar to other regions of the motor cortex (Rosano et al., 2003).
Electrical stimulation of the frontal eye field in men causes conjugate deviation of
the eyes, usually to the opposite side (Foerster, 1931; Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950).
Stimulation here causes occasionally upward movements, convergence, divergence of
the eyes and pupillary changes. Movements of the head and pupillary changes usually
accompany movements of the eyes.
Unilateral stimulation of the frontal eye fields in humans usually produces bilateral
opening or closing of the lids (Paus, 1996; Lobel et al., 2001; Milea et al., 2002).
Based on electrical stimulation studies of human subjects prior to brain surgery, the
saccade-generating portion of the frontal eye field is found 1-2 cm anterior to the primary
motor cortical regions involved in finger and hand movements, a region in close
agreement to that identified in fMRI studies (Yamamoto et al., 2004).
Similarly, cortical stimulation experiments in humans confirm that the smooth pursuit
region is located more posteriorly than the saccade region (Blanke and Seeck, 2003).
The frontal eye field receives connections that convey both visuosensory information
and motor information to its neurons. Importantly, regions within the frontal eye field
receive information from both the ventral visual stream (the “what” pathway) and the
dorsal visual stream (the “where” pathway; Schall et al., 1995). The neurons of the frontal
eye field that receive connections from the ventral stream generate shorter saccades than
those innervated by dorsal stream neurons. This is consistent with the expected size of
saccades necessary to explore visually a small object of the face (in detail) versus those
necessary to characterize distant aspects of a subject’s spatial environment.
The frontal eye field also receives information from the supplementary eye fields,
the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the thalamus, and the substantia nigra pars reticulata.
According to Crossman (2008), the frontal eye field receives its major thalamic projection
from the parvocellular mediodorsal nucleus, with additional afferents from the medial
pulvinar, the ventral anterior nucleus and the suprageniculate-limitans complex, and
connects with the paracentral nucleus of the intralaminar group.
The thalamocortical pathways to the frontal eye field form part of a pathway from
the superior colliculus, the substantia nigra and the dentate nucleus of the cerebellum.
Neurons in the cerebellum are critical for maintaining smooth pursuit eye movements
and may help to refine certain types of saccades.
The frontal eye field has extensive ipsilateral corticocortical connections, receiving
fibres from several visual areas in the occipital, parietal and temporal lobes, including
the medial temporal area (V5) and area 7a.
There are also projections from the superior temporal gyrus, which is auditory rather
than visual in function. From within the frontal lobe, the frontal eye field receives fibres
from the ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices.
1658
21. Cerebral cortex
These inputs can positively and negatively modulate the activity of neurons within
the frontal eye field.
From the frontal eye fields fibres descend in the corticobulbar path, enter the genu
of the internal capsule, the cerebral crus at the base of the midbrain, and descend into the
brain stem. Along the way, a few fibres turn off oculomotor nuclear levels to supply the
oculomotor nucleus as the remaining fibres descend and end in the abducens nucleus.
This area contains cell bodies whose axons join the corticonuclear tract and descend to
terminate in the eye movement control centres located in the midbrain reticular formation
and the paramedian pontine formation. In turn, they transmit information to the motor nuclei
of cranial nerves III, IV, and VI, which control voluntary eye movements.
The frontal eye field projects to the dorsal and ventral premotor cortices and to the
medial motor area, probably, to the supplementary eye field adjacent to the supplementary
motor area proper. It projects prominently to the deeper layer of the superior colliculus
via a transtegmental projection (Kuypers and Lawrence, 1967). However, the most
substantial and highly organized projections to the superior colliculus arise from the
visual cortex (Garey et al., 1968). These fibres mainly enter the stratum opticum via the
brachium of the superior colliculus. In the brain stem, the superior colliculus plays a
dominant role in organizing many aspects of eye movement control through its
connections with other nuclei in the dorsal midbrain (for vertical eye movements) and
the pons (for horizontal eye movements). Although the superior colliculus does not
project direct fibres to the nuclei of the extraocular muscles, it has projections to both
the reticular formation and the accessory oculomotor nuclei (Carpenter, 1971).
Thus, the frontal eye field projects to the pontine gaze centre within the pontine
reticular formation, and to other oculomotor related nuclei in the brain stem.
Studies in nonhuman primates suggest that in normal subjects most of the control
of saccades by the frontal eye movement regions is mediated by the superior colliculus
(Sparks, 2002).
Under conditions of superior colliculus damage, other cortical and brain stem regions
can partially compensate for the lost superior colliculus functions; however, saccades are
much less accurate in time and space.
Neurons that have saccade-related activity in the intermediate and deep layers of the
superior colliculus are arranged topographically and fire bursts of activity before an eye
movement, be it a saccade or smooth pursuit (Krauzlis and Dill, 2002).
Thus, the activation of rostral cells generates small-amplitude eye movements, and
the activation of caudal cells generates larger-amplitude saccades. Electrical stimulation
of the rostral pole of the superior colliculus inhibits saccades, consistent with an important
role for maintaining fixation on a target.
These neurons have monosynaptic excitatory inputs neurons in the brain stem that
inhibit eye movements (Leigh and Kennard, 2004). Upward saccades are represented
medially, and downward saccades, laterally, within the superior colliculus (Sparks, 2002).
Neurons that are located downstream of these signals in midbrain and pontine
oculomotor nuclei, have firing rates that directly correlate with the speed and amplitude
1659
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
of saccades. Via a basal ganglia circuit, which includes the oculomotor portions of the
caudate nucleus, with projections to the substantia nigra pars reticulata, the superior
colliculus integrates information regarding the reward value of saccades to specific
locations (Hikosaka et al., 2000).
Impairments in memory-guided and predictive saccades in neurodegenerative
disease with basal ganglia damage may in part arise from damage to this circuit (Leigh
and Kennard, 2004).
Nicotinic cholinergic inputs from the pedunculopontine nucleus to the superior
colliculus may also have important effects on attention and motivation for saccades
through a stimulatory effect on the superior colliculus (Kobayashi et al., 2001). Decreased
attention and visual search efficiency (Doffner et al., 1992; Mosimann et al., 2004) and
difficulties with saccade programming (Shafiq-Antonacci et al., 2003) in Alzheimer’s
disease may be in part explained by damage to this cholinergic circuit.
In addition to, and as part of, their roles in generating saccades, the neurons of the
frontal eye field represent the behavioral relevance of objects within the visual field; thus,
the activity over portions of the frontal eye field can be said to represent a visual saliency
map (Thompson and Bichot, 2005).
After the presentation of a visual stimulus, over time, activity within the frontal eye
field evolves to represent the behavioral significance of the stimulus. This behavioral
significance can be read out as a saccade to the novel stimulus, but it may be represented
as a covert shift of attention without an actual eye movement (Boxer, 2007).
However, this cortical field is believed to be a centre for voluntary eye movements
not dependent upon visual stimuli. The conjugate eye movements commonly are called
“movements of command”, since they can be elicited by instructing the patient to look
to the right or left (Cogan, 1956).
Some studies in men and primates suggest a double representation of specific eye
movements in each frontal eye field (Crosby, 1953; Lemmen et al., 1959; Crosby et al., 1962).
Unlike the frontal eye field, the occipital eye centre is not localized to a small area.
Eye responses can be obtained from a wide region of the occipital lobe in monkeys, but
the lowest threshold is found in area 17 (Walker and Weaver, 1940). The occipital eye
centres are presumed to subserve movements of eyes induced by visual stimuli, such as
following moving objects. These pursuit movements of the eyes are largely involuntary,
although they are not present in young infants.
Electrophysiological recordings in monkeys and lesion studies in humans have shown
that brain stem structures, the basal ganglia, thalamus, cerebellum, and within the cortex,
the parietal eye fields, supplementary eye field, and frontal eye fields are involved in the
execution and control of saccades (Leigh and Zee, 1991; Pierot-Deseiligny et al., 1995).
However, two regions of the cerebral cortex in humans influence ocular movements.
At the posterior end of the middle frontal gyrus and corresponding to Brodmann’s areas
8 and 9 is a region responsible for voluntary eye deviation called the primary eye field.
There is also a secondary motor eye field at the posterior end of the inferior frontal
gyrus on its dorsal part.
1660
21. Cerebral cortex
Stimulation of the human primary eye fields yields deviations of the eyes, including
divergence, horizontal conjugate deviation to the contralateral side, and deviation up and
to the contralateral side.
The pattern on the secondary eye field is a mirror image of that on the primary eye
field. The secondary eye fields permit relaxation of the antagonistic muscles during
voluntary eye deviations.
Thus, the frontal eye field plays a role in the coordination of eye movements,
particularly those mediating voluntary visual tracking of a moving object via conjugate
deviation of the eyes.
Supplementary eye fields. According to Boxer (2007), the supplementary eye field
is located on the medial surface of the superior frontal gyrus, in the dorsal aspect of the
paracentral sulcus. An oculomotor area in the frontal cortex, separated from the frontal eye
field, was defined by Schlag and Schlag-Rey in 1987. It is located rostrally to the
supplementary motor area (M II) on the medial surface of the hemisphere. The
supplementary eye fields receive connections form the frontal eye field, the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex and the posterior part of the cerebral
hemisphere. The supplementary eye field projects to the frontal eye field and to the
subcortical nuclei involved in eye movements (superior colliculus and reticular formation).
The supplementary eye field is involved in programming sequences of saccades or
combined saccade-body movements and likely has important preparatory roles in more
complex eye movements (spatiotopic saccades), (Isoda and Tanji, 2003).
Functional imaging studies in humans suggest that the supplementary eye field is
activated prior to both simple and more complex saccade tasks (Gagnon et al., 2002),
suggesting that it may have a supervisory role over neurons in the frontal eye field (Schall
et al., 2002).
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in
multiple aspects of saccade programming, including making decisions to initiate
voluntary saccades and maintaining spatial working memory for target locations
(Funahashi et al., 1993; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2002, 2003; Cisek and Kalaska, 2004).
The regions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that are most important for the control
of eye movements are located in the middle frontal gyrus in Brodmann’s areas 46 and 9.
The suppression of unwanted reflexive saccades likely involves a direct inhibitory
projection from dorsolateral prefrontal cortex neurons to the brain stem saccade-
generating circuitry, including the superior colliculus (Condy et al., 2004).
Experiments with monkeys suggest that there are neurons in the vicinity of the
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex whose activity signals the suppression of a reflexive saccade
or the command “Don’t look” (Hasegawa et al., 2004). Activity from these neurons may
be crucial for the ability of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ability to help select one
behaviorally relevant saccade over a range of other visually salient targets.
Nyffeler et al. (2002) confirmed the roles of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in
spatial memory, first identified in nonhuman primates by Constantinidis et al. (2001).
Thus, studies of the voluntary control of eye movements, particularly saccades, are
revealing important principles about the mechanisms of motor control by the frontal lobes.
1661
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Thus, three cortical areas not involved directly in triggering of saccades play
important roles in planning, integration, and chronologic ordering of saccades.
The prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area 46) plays a role in planning saccades to
remembered target locations. The inferior parietal lobe is involved in visuospatial
integration. Bilateral lesions in this area result in Balint’s syndrome (optic ataxia, ocular
apraxia, psychic paralysis of the visual fixation).
The hippocampus appears to control the temporal working memory required for
chronologic order of saccade sequences (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
Smooth-pursuit movements are slow eye movements initiated by a moving object.
Unlike saccades, smooth-pursuit movements cannot occur in darkness.
Cortical areas involved in smooth pursuit include the temporooccipital region and
the frontal eye field. Each of these areas has direct projections to brain stem neurons that
drive pursuit. The temporooccipital region is driven by input from the primary visual
cortex. The posterior parietal and the superior temporal cortices may also contribute to
smooth pursuit indirectly through visual attention.
Lesions in the temporooccipital cortex in humans associated with smooth-pursuit
deficits correspond to Brodmann’s areas 19, 37, and 39. Lesions in the frontal eye field
also have been associated with deficits in smooth pursuit.
A corticofugal pathway for smooth-pursuit courses from the temporooccipital cortex
through the posterior limb of the internal capsule to the dorsolateral pontine nucleus in
the mid-pons. The second courses from the frontal eye field to the dorsolateral pontine
nucleus and the nucleus reticularis tegmenti pontis (Afifi and Bergman 2005).
Cortical areas for smooth pursuit also project to the flocculus of the cerebellum after
relaying in the dorsolateral pontine nucleus. The flocculus, in turn, projects on the
vestibular nuclei, which project to the cranial nerve nuclei of extraocular movement (CN
III, IV, IV) (Leichnetz et al., 1981; Morrow and Sharpe, 1990; Tijsen et al., 1991;
Gaymard et al., 1993; Sharp et al., 1995; Lekwuuwa and Barnes, 1996; Deseilligny et
al., 1996; Morecraft and Yeterian, 2000; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1995; Pryse-Phillip,
2003; Afifi and Bergman, 2005). Cerebral hemisphere lesions impair ocular pursuit
ipsilaterally, or laterally whereas posterior fossa lesions impair ocular pursuit either
contralaterally or ipsilaterally.
The antisaccade task. According to Boxer (2007), the antisaccade task has two
components: suppression of a reflexive saccade towards a target, and programming of a new
saccade in the opposite direction. Due to its ease of performance, this task has been used
extensively to investigate aspects of voluntary motor control. The ability to perform
antisaccades accurately develops in late childhood (Klein, 2001) and decline in normal elderly
subjects in parallel to other cognitive measures of frontal lobe function (Klein et al., 2000).
Early studies in patients with frontal lobe lesions showed that the antisaccade task
is exquisitely sensitive to frontal lobe damage (Guitton et al., 1985).
The anatomy of antisaccade dysfunction has been elicited in subjects with focal brain
lesions and shown to involve a dorsolateral prefrontal cortex internal capsule – the
superior colliculus brain stem circuit – in which damage to any component of the circuit
1662
21. Cerebral cortex
the same brain region and patterns of activity are involved in deciding to initiate or cancel
an eye movement (Boxer, 2007). Data from human subjects with local brain lesions or
neurodegenerative syndromes suggest that the integrity of these circuits is necessary for
accurate voluntary saccade initiation (Boxer, 2007).
the muscles related to speech, there is a loss of coordination of those muscles leading to
a difficulty in expressing oneself.
Broca’s aphasia, also termed expressive aphasia or nonfluent aphasia, consists of
a loss of the ability to speak fluently.
Patients with the most severe form of Broca’s aphasia are unable to speak (mutism),
although they are able to swallow and breathe normally and make guttural sounds. Here
is a difficulty in turning a concept or thought into a sequence of meaningful sounds. In
less severe cases, or in patients during the recovery process, limited speech is possible.
In Broca’s aphasia, there is a low fluency associated with relatively well-preserved
comprehension.
The patient is likely to be unable to speak or continually repeats the last word said
before the injury occurred. There may be difficulty with certain sounds or letters.
However, speech is slow and laboured, enunciation is poor, and nonessential words are
commonly omitted (telegraphic speech).
Broca’s area is involved in the planning of motor activity that is necessary for words
to be produced. Broca’s aphasia also impairs an individual’s ability to express a thought
or concept by writing it down. So, affected persons typically have as much difficulty with
writing (agraphia) as with speaking. Although the patient is able to understand spoken
or written language and can communicate verbally to some degree, the extremely
laborious nature of the process of communication causes considerable frustration. Under
particular emotional stress, patients may use inappropriate or vulgar words or phrases to
express their distress.
Mild aphasia without other deficits indicates that the damage affects only cortical
areas. However, full-blown Broca’s aphasia indicates that the damage extends beyond
the Broca area of the cortex to include insular cortex and subjacent white matter.
Patients typically have contralateral motor signs and symptoms, such as weakness
(paresis of the lower part of the face, lateral deviation of the tongue when protruded, and
weakness of the arm (Lynch, 2006).
The resulting deficit depends on the size, depth, severity, timing, and whether the
cerebral hemisphere is dominant for language (the left cerebral hemisphere in
approximately 95% of humans).
Most patients with Broca’s aphasia are able to sing, although they do not speak well.
It is likely that the right cerebral hemisphere is dominant over the left for singing capacity.
If injury leading to aphasia takes place at an early age, it is possible to build up
speech patterns in the other cerebral hemisphere. This is an example of the plasticity of
the nervous system and its ability to compensate when injured (Grodzinsky and Amunts,
2006).
The most common causes of Broca’s aphasia are tumors and occlusions of frontal
M4 branches of the middle cerebral artery.
Aphasia plus motor problems suggest an occlusion of branches from the proximal
parts of the middle cerebral artery (M1), including the lenticulostriate arteries, which
serve the internal capsule.
1665
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
In one of the original reports (Gallese et al., 1996), some interesting laterality patterns
were observed. About 40% of the mirror neurons tested demonstrated a discharge
influenced by the laterality of the observed hand.
The majority of these neurons responded more strongly when the observed hand
was ipsilateral to the recorded hemisphere (i.e., a left-hand action triggered stronger
discharge in mirror neurons in the left ventral premotor cortex, compared to a right-hand
action).
Another finding was related to the direction of the observed reach-and-grasp action.
About two-thirds of the mirror neurons tested demonstrated directional preference. More
than 80% of these neurons preferred the direction towards the recorded side, that is, from
right to left for mirror neurons in the left hemisphere (Gallese et al., 1996).
Iacoboni et al. (1996) described that the human left inferior frontal area responded to
action observation, action execution, and even more during action imitation. In light of the
evolutionary hypothesis of a link between mirror neurons and language (Rizzolatti and Arbib,
1998), the left lateralization report in this study has been often taken as suggesting that the
whole mirror neuron system in humans is left-lateralized (Corballis, 2003).
In that study, right-hand actions imitated left-hand observed actions. Even the
monkey data would have predicted a left-lateralized pattern in this case (Iacoboni et al.,
1999).
In fact, a reanalysis of seven fMRI studies of imitation adapting various forms of
hand imitation has shown a fairly bilateral activation pattern in the posterior inferior
frontal cortex (Molnar-Szakacs et al., 2005).
A highly reproducible effect that is likely due to the mirror neuron system is the
increase in the excitability of the motor corticospinal system during action observation.
When a transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) pulse is applied over the primary motor
hand area, a motor response is evoked in the contralateral hand. If subjects are also
watching somebody else’s actions, this motor evoked response is much stronger when
compared to watching some visual control stimuli (Fadiga et al., 1995; Aziz-Zadeh et
al., 2002; Fadiga et al., 2005).
This effect is likely due to the activation of mirror neurons in the premotor cortex
that feed directly onto primary motor regions (Cerri et al., 2003; Shimazu et al., 2004).
When subjects are listening to action sounds, their corticospinal excitability is
facilitated (Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2004).
This effect, likely mediated by mirror neurons that respond to the sound of an action
(Kohler et al., 2002), is completely lateralized to the left hemisphere in humans (Aziz-
Zadeh et al., 2004).
This strong left-hemisphere lateralization suggests that a fundamental step in the
hypothesized evolutionary progression from action recognition to language may be the
multimodal representation of action that only the left hemisphere appears to have in
humans (Geschwind and Iacoboni, 2007).
Right-hemisphere lateralization was observed during imitation and observation of
facial emotional expression (Carr et al., 2003).
1667
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
PREFRONTAL CORTEX
The prefrontal cortex makes up about a third of the human neocortex and
corresponds to three major regions on the various surfaces of the frontal lobe. Collectively
these three regions forming the prefrontal cortex constitute a complex association area
that provides the structural basis for the most complex intellectual and moral functions
of which humans are capable (Markowitsch et al., 1979; Morecraft and Yeterian, 2000)
The lateral prefrontal cortex
The prefrontal cortex on the lateral surface of the hemisphere (the lateral prefrontal
cortex) which involves the superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri anterior to the
premotor cortex, comprises Brodmann’s areas 8, 9, 10, 11, 45, 46, and 47 (Fuster, 2001).
Areas 44 and 45 are particularly notable in men since, in the dominant hemisphere, they
constitute the motor speech area (Broca’s area).
In nonhuman primates, two subdivisions of the lateral prefrontal cortex are
recognized, a dorsal area equivalent to area 9, and perhaps including the superior part of
area 46, and a ventral area, consisting of the inferior part of area 46 and area 45.
According to Crossman (2008), both the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal
areas receive their major thalamic afferents from the mediodorsal nucleus, and there are
additional contributions from the medial pulvinar nucleus, the ventral anterior nucleus,
and from the paracentral nucleus of the anterior intralaminar group.
The dorsolateral area receives long association fibres from the posterior and middle
superior temporal gyrus (including auditory association areas), from parietal area 7a, and
from much of the middle temporal cortex. From within the frontal lobe it also receives
projections from the frontal pole (area 10), the medial prefrontal cortex (area 32) and the
medial surface of the hemisphere. It projects to the supplementary motor area, the dorsal
premotor cortex and the frontal eye field. All these thalamic and corticocortical
connections are reciprocal. Commissural connections are with the homologous area, and
with the contralateral inferior parietal cortex (Crossman, 2008).
The ventrolateral prefrontal area receives long association fibres from both area
7a and area 7b of the parietal lobe, from auditory association areas of the temporal
operculum, from the insula and from the anterior part of the lower bank of the superior
temporal sulcus. From within the frontal lobe it receives fibres from the anterior
orbitofrontal cortex and projects to the frontal eye field and the ventral premotor cortex.
1668
21. Cerebral cortex
It connects with the contralateral homologous area via the corpus callosum. These
connections are probably all reciprocal (Crossman, 2008).
The cortex of the frontal pole (area 10) receives thalamic inputs from the
mediodorsal nucleus, the medial pulvinar nucleus and the paracentral nucleus. It is
reciprocally connected with the cortex of the temporal pole, the anterior orbitofrontal
cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Fig. 21.24. Human prefrontal areas numbered according to Brodmann (from Fuster, 2001).
It receives fibres from the anterior cortex of the superior temporal gyrus. Within the
frontal lobe, it has connections with the orbitofrontal cortex, and the medial motor areas
of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Crossman, 2008).
Area 1 is on the crown of the postcentral gyrus, area 2 intrasulcal in position forming
the anterior wall of the postcentral sulcus, area 3 is divisible into areas 3a and 3b in
nonhuman primates and humans, with area 3b being intrasulcal but forming the posterior
wall of the central sulcus. Area 3a, also intrasulcal, forms the floor of the central sulcus,
is a transition region between primary motor area 4 along the precentral gyrus and area
3b on the posterior bank of the central sulcus. Thus, there are four strep-like
somatosensory areas in the human primary somatosensory area of the postcentral gyrus
of the parietal lobe (Geyer et al., 1999, 2000; Augustine, 2008).
The primary somatosensory cortex contains within it a topographical map of the
contralateral half of the body. The face, tongue and lips are represented inferiorly, the
trunk and upper limb are represented on the superolateral aspect, and the lower limb on
the medial aspect of the hemisphere, giving rise to the familiar “homunculus” map
(Fig. 21.26). Thus, the body is represented on the somatosensory cortex by an upside-
down homunculus. The foot is represented on the superior medial surface of the
hemisphere, the leg, thigh, trunk, shoulder, arm, and forearm on the superior surface of
the postcentral gyrus, whereas the hand, head, teeth, tongue, larynx, and pharynx are on
the lateral surface of the postcentral gyrus. The total cortical area representing a body
part is proportional to the discriminative capability of the body part.
Fig. 21.26. The sensory homunculus across the postcentral gyrus and paracentral lobule
(after Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950).
1671
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
and short association fibres from the adjacent primary motor cortex. Callosal fibres in
S I arise mainly from the deep part of layer III and terminate in layer I to IV.
Thus, efferents from the primary somesthetic cortex project to the motor cortex, the
opposite primary somesthetic cortex, and the association somatosensory cortex (area 5
and 7) in the posterior parietal cortex (Fig. 21.27).
Fig. 21.27. The lateral (A) and medial (B) surfaces of the left cerebral hemisphere
depicting Brodmann’s areas (after Crossman, 2008).
of visceral pain. Electrical stimulation reveals that visceral sensory areas in the cerebral
cortex in humans include the lower end of the primary somatosensory cortex and part of
the insular lobe (Augustine, 2008).
In a study of the speed of moving stimuli across the skin of the hand, there was
primarily activation in area 1 and secondary in area 3b of the somatosensory cortex. In
another study, the discrimination of caricatures or objects rich in curvatures led to an
increase in regional cerebral blood flow outside of area 1 but perhaps in area 2 on the
posterior bank of the postcentral sulcus.
Positron emission tomography, cytoarchitectonic mapping, and a combination of
sensory stimuli (passive or active touch, brush velocity, edge length, curvature, and
roughness) reveal the probable hierarchical processing of tactile shape in sensory areas
of the human brain. This involves areas 3b and 1 as the first steps in the process, area 2
being the next step, and the anterior part of the cortex lining the intraparietal sulcus and
the adjacent part of the supramarginal gyrus being the final step. The anterior part of the
human intraparietal sulcus is involved in visually guided grasping (Augustine, 2008).
Lastly, in a fourth study, a pleasant and soft touch to the hand elicited by using velvet in
comparison to a neutral stimulus produces activation in the orbitofrontal cortex. This
suggest that the pleasant or rewarding aspects of touch (“that feels good”) as well as the
pleasant rewarding aspect of taste (“that tastes good”) or smell (“that smells good”) all
of which are represented in different parts of the orbitofrontal cortex, collectively
contribute to our state of emotion (Badegård et al., 2000 a, b, 2001; Augustine, 2008).
Functional imaging studies of the human S I show that multiple digit representations
occur in the four subdivisions (3a, 3b, 1 and 2) resembling a multiple representation
hypothesis that is present in nonhuman primates (Kaas et al., 1999, 2004).
Neurophysiologic studies of the somesthetic cortex have revealed the following
information: (1) the functional cortical unit appears to be associated with a vertical
column of cells that is modality specific. Neurons within a cortical unit are activated by
the same peripheral stimulus and are related to the same peripheral receptive field. (2)
Area 3b is activated by cutaneous stimuli and areas 2 and 3a by proprioceptive impulses,
whereas area 1 is activated by either cutaneous or proprioceptive impulses. Area 2 appears
to represent predominantly deep receptors, including those signalling the position of joints
and other deep body sensations, whereas area 3a represents sensory input from muscles
(Burton et al., 1995).
(3) Somatosensory neurons responding to joint movement show a marked degree of
specificity in that they respond to displacement in one direction. (4) Fast- and slow-
adapting neuronal pools have been identified in response to hair displacement or
cutaneous deformation. (5) Fibres mediating cutaneous sensations terminate rostrally,
whereas those mediating proprioceptive sensations terminate more caudally in the
somesthetic area (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
The cortical representation along the postcentral gyrus reflects a composite of
somatotopically organized areas instead of a continuous homunculus.
1675
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
nuclei of the thalamus, are referred to as the secondary sensory areas (Carpenter, 1979).
These areas have been defined and mapped in experimental animals by recording evoked
potentials in response to peripheral stimulation (Adrian, 1940, 1941; Woolsey and Walzl,
1942; Woolsey and Fairman, 1946; Thompson et al., 1950). Studies of these secondary
sensory areas indicate that sequential representation of parts of the body, or of the
tonotopic pattern in the case of the auditory areas, is not the same as in the primary areas
(Woolsey, 1958; Rose and Woolsey, 1958).
Adjoining the primary somatosensory cortex or S I is a smaller region, the secondary
somatosensory cortex or S II, on the superior bank of the lateral sulcus, continuing onto
the parietal operculum of the human brain and corresponding to Brodmann’s area 43.
(Eickhoff et al., 2006 a, b). S II contains a somatotopic representation of the body, with
the head and face most anteriorly, adjacent to S I, and the sacral regions most posteriorly.
Thus, body representation in this area is bilateral, with contralateral predominance, and
is reverse of that in the primary area so that the two face areas are adjacent to each other.
The secondary somatosensory area in animals appears to coincide with the so-called
secondary motor area (Welker et al., 1957; Disbrow et al., 2000).
The secondary sensory area contains neurons with receptive fields that are large,
poorly demarcated, overlap extensively, and often have bilateral representation.
Representation of the body form in the secondary somatosensory area (II) is nearly
as detailed as that in the postcentral gyrus (Woolsey, 1958), and single unit analysis
(Carreras and Levitt, 1959) has revealed many cells in this area to both mode and place
specific. Although a detailed somatotopic organization exists in S II, with the exception
of parts of the digit representation, neighbouring neurons in S II do not form a precise
body map comparable to that in S II of nonhuman primates.
In S II, there is a split in the representation for the face, thumb, and foot causing a
dual presentation of the same body part in the composite map of S II (Krubitzer et al.,
1995; Kaas, 2004).
Within the cortex, S II is reciprocally connected with S I in a topographically
organized manner, and projects to the primary motor cortex. S II also projects in a
topographically organized way to the lateral part of area 7 (area 7b) in the superior parietal
lobe, and makes connections with the posterior cingulate gyrus. Both right and left S II
areas are interconnected across the corpus callosum, although distal limb representations
are probably excluded. There are projections to S II and area 7b (Crossman, 2008).
The secondary somesthetic area contains no cells sensitive to join movement or join
position.
The S II is reciprocally connected with the ventral posterior nucleus of the thalamus in
a topographically organized fashion. Some thalamic neurons probably project to both S I
and S II via collaterals. Other thalamic connections of the S II are with posterior groups of
nuclei and with the intralaminar central lateral nucleus. S II also projects to laminae IV to
VII of the dorsal horn of the cervical and thoracic spinal cord, the dorsal column nuclei, the
principal trigeminal nucleus, and the periaqueductal grey matter of the midbrain.
S II in nonhuman primates receives fibres from the ventral posterior medial nucleus
and the caudal part of the ventral posterior lateral nucleus. Reciprocal corticothalamic
1677
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
fibres occur from S II in nonhuman primates to the ventral posterior nucleus. Output from
S II is to S I, to the primary motor cortex (Brodmann’s area 4), and the premotor cortex
(Brodmann’s area 6).
Lesions interrupting connections between the secondary somesthetic area, posterior
parietal cortex, and ventral posteromedial and centrolateral thalamic nuclei have been
associated with pseudothalamic pain syndrome. The pain is spontaneous and
characterized as burning or icelike and is associated with impairment of pain, and
temperature appreciation (Déjérine 1900; Schmahmann and Leifer, 1992).
The unilateral or bilateral removal of area S II in monkeys causes a profound defect
of tasks requiring tactile learning and retention. Even in the face of injury to the primary
association cortex in the parietal lobe, patients are still able to recognize two points as
two points and to identify the position of the parts of their body.
However, the patient will be unable to name an object placed in the affected hand,
based on somatosensory information. There are disturbances mainly of the discriminative
sensory function of the opposite leg, arm, and side of face without impairment of the
primary modalities of sensation. The lack of recognition of shape and form is frequently
a manifestation of cortical disease. Thus, astereognosis, connotes an inability to identify
an object by palpation, even though the primary sense data (touch, pain, temperature, and
vibration) are intact. Astereognosis is either right-, or left-sided, and is the product of a
lesion in the opposite hemisphere, involving the sensory cortex, particularly S II, or
thalamoparietal projections. With this deficit, (cortical astereognosis) patients lose the
ability to use the somatosensory information.
Part of the parietal lobe including the primary (area 3, 1, and 2) and secondary
somatosensory cortex (area 43) is often termed the anterior parietal cortex. The entire
parietal cortex behind S I and S II is often termed the posterior parietal cortex. Posterior
parietal cortex, in turn, is divisible in the superior parietal lobule and the inferior parietal
lobule.
The superior parietal lobule lies superior to the intraparietal sulcus and includes
Brodmann’s areas 5 and 7a and 7b, whereas the inferior parietal lobule lies inferior to
the intraparietal sulcus and includes Brodmann’s areas 39 and 40.
Based on the serial processing of sensory information from secondary sensory areas,
the superior parietal lobule is likely to be a tertiary somatosensory association area in
terms of its higher order processing.
Area 5 receives a dense feedforward projection from all cytoarchitectonic areas of
S I in a topographically organized manner. The thalamic afferents to this area come from
the lateral posterior nucleus and from the central lateral nucleus of the intralaminar group.
Ipsilateral corticocortical fibres from area 5 go to area 7, the premotor and supplementary
motor cortices, the posterior cingulate gyrus, and the insular granular cortex.
Commissural connections between both sides of area 5 tend to avoid the areas of
representation of the distal limb. The response properties of cells in area 5 are more
complex than in S I, with larger receptive fields and evidence of submodality
convergence. Area 5 contributes to the corticospinal tract (Crossman, 2008).
1678
21. Cerebral cortex
Areas 5 and 7 receive their inputs mainly from the primary somatosensory areas but
also have reciprocal connections with the pulvinar and lateral posterior nucleus of the
thalamus. Such connections permit the reinforcement, correlation, and integration of
sensory information. The superior parietal lobule is involved in spatial orientation.
In monkeys area 7a is not related to the cortical pathway for somatosensory
processing but instead forms part of a dorsal cortical pathway for spatial vision. The
major ipsilateral corticortical connections to area 7a are derived from visual areas in the
occipital and temporal lobes. In the ipsilateral hemisphere, area 7a has connections with
the posterior cingulate cortex (area 24) and with area 8 and 46 of the frontal lobe.
Commissural connections are with its contralateral homologue. Area 7a is connected with
the medial pulvinar and intralaminar paracentral nuclei of the thalamus.
In experimental studies, neurons within area 7a are visually responsive: they relate
largely to the peripheral vision, respond to stimulus movement, and are modulated by
eye movement (Crossman, 2008).
In monkeys, area 7b receives somatosensory inputs from area 5 and S II. Connections
pass to the posterior cingulate gyrus (area 23), insula and temporal cortex. Area 7b is
reciprocally connected with area 46 in the prefrontal cortex and the lateral part of the
premotor cortex. Commissural connections of area 7b are with the contralateral
homologous area, and with area S II, the insular granular cortex and area 5. Thalamic
connections are with the medial pulvinar nucleus and the intralaminar paracentral nucleus
(Crossman, 2008).
Neuronal responses in the tertiary somatosensory association areas involve the
integration of a number of cortical and thalamic inputs. The processing of multisensory
somatosensory inputs in these areas allows for the perception of shape, size and texture,
and the identification of objects by contact (stereognosis).
The tertiary somatosensory association areas project to multimodal nonprimary
association areas (areas 39 and 40) in the inferior parietal lobule that receive inputs from
more than one sensory modality and serve intermodal integration and multisensory
perception.
Single-cell recording in area 5 in monkeys suggests that this area is essential for the
proper use of somatosensory information, for goal-directed voluntary movements, and
for the manipulation of objects (Lüders et al., 1985).
Single-cell recording in area 7 indicates that this area plays an important role in the
integration of visual and somatosensory stimuli, which is essential for the coordination
of the eyes and hands in visually guided movements (Godoy et al., 1990; Pierrot-
Deseilligny and Gaymard, 1990; Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1995).
Injuries to the parietal tertiary somatosensory association areas often lead to rare
visual illusions that are modifications of the normal position of objects. The world appears
upside down and the chair, table or other objects also appear upside down.
Such phenomena do not have any emotional component to them, as do such illusions
that occur after injury to the temporal lobe.
Loss of the body schema often results because such patients do not recognize a
body part as belonging to themselves. This agnosia can occur for a digit, hand, or half of
1679
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the face. This condition, most frequently results from an extensive lesion in the superior
aspect of the nondominant parietal lobe (areas 5, 7, 40, and 39). The individual does not
recognize certain parts of the body as being his and show unilateral unawareness of denial
defects on one side of the body (anosognosia).
In milder cases, patients may show an inappropriate lack of concern for their deficits
(anosodiaphoria). Tests such as line bisection, drawing, copying, and visual search tasks
may provide useful clinical screening procedures (Robertson and Marshall, 1993).
Apraxia is a deficit in the voluntary use of the limbs to carry out a series of
movements in which each step depends on the preceding movement. Injuries to the
dominant parietal lobe often result in the phenomena of ideational apraxia. Ideational
apraxia is impairment of familiar and well-practiced movements (pick up a cup, pour
water in it, drink the water etc.).
Sensory extinction is a subtle symptom of parietal lobe injury due to perceptual
rivalry so that the injured area loses out, and the sensation coming from that side of the
body is extinguished.
The inferior parietal lobule includes the supramarginal gyrus and the angular gyrus
corresponding to Brodmann’s areas 40 and 39. The intraparietal sulcus forms the dorsal
border of the inferior parietal lobule. The supramarginal gyrus caps the posterior tip of
the lateral sulcus (Fig. 21.28), whereas the angular gyrus caps the posterior tip of the
superior temporal sulcus. The angular gyrus, corresponding to Brodmann’s area 39, is
functionally involved in language.
Fig. 21.28. Superolateral surface of the left cerebral hemisphere including the boundaries, major sulci,
and gyri of the parietal lobe (after Augustine, 2008).
A unilateral lesion involving Brodmann’s areas 39, 40, 22 and 37, results in
Wernicke’s (receptive) aphasia. Because this area plays a role in the comprehension and
formulation of language, an individual with a severe form of this condition has difficulty
in comprehending the spoken words. These patients cannot name an object they are able
1680
21. Cerebral cortex
to see, although they know how to use it, and have a condition termed alexia without
agraphia or the inability to read, although the ability to write remains.
Sentence comprehension impairments are usually observed in the context of tasks
requiring a precise understanding of the grammatical relationships among words in a
sentence. In 1906, Pierre Marie first noted that this type of test was very sensitive to mild
degrees of language deficit.
Injury to the supramarginal gyrus, particularly in the left hemisphere corresponding
to Brodmann’s area 40, causes the loss of the ability to express oneself in language, a
condition called conduction apraxia. Speech is broken but comprehension is good. In
classic conduction aphasia, phonemic errors are more common in repetition tasks than
in spontaneous speech. In transcortical motor aphasia, repetition may be spared, but
spontaneous speech is contaminated by phonemic errors.
The major association cortex is connected with all the sensory cortical areas and,
thus, functions in higher-order and complex multisensory perception. The major
association cortex refers to supramarginal and angular gyri in the inferior parietal lobule
which corresponds to areas 39 and 40 of Brodmann. Its relation to the speech areas in
the temporal and frontal lobes gives it an important role in communication skills. Patients
with lesions in the major association cortex of the dominant hemisphere present a
conglomerate of manifestations that include receptive and expressive aphasia, inability
to write (agraphia), inability to synthesize, correlate, and recognize multisensory
perceptions (agnosia), left - right confusion, difficulty in recognizing different fingers
(finger agnosia), and the inability to calculate (acalculia). These symptoms and signs are
grouped together under the term Gerstmann’s syndrome (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
Involvement of the major association cortex in the nondominant hemisphere is
usually manifested by disturbances in drawing (constructional apraxia) and in the
awareness of body image. Such patients have difficulty in drawing a square or circle or
copying a complex figure. They often are unaware of a body part and thus neglect to
shave half of face or dress half the body (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
Fig. 21.29. Path for vestibular impulses from receptors in the peripheral vestibular apparatus to the primary vestibular
cortex of the parietal lobe. This path is presumably bilateral in that there is never any direction of movement of the head
that does not stimulate both sets of vestibular receptors. The thalamic termination of this path includes the oral part of the
ventral posterior lateral nucleus (VPLo), caudal part of the ventral lateral nucleus (vLc) and dorsal part of the ventral
posterior inferior nucleus (VPId). Some authors include VPI in the borders of the ventral posterior lateral nucleus (VPL) of
the dorsal thalamus. The few primary fibres that reach the cerebellum are not shown (after Augustine, 2008).
1682
21. Cerebral cortex
Thalamic VPL and posterior nucleus neurons constitute separate, parallel pathways
transmitting vestibular information from the brain stem to the cortex, because their
connections with cortical areas are distinct. In addition, vestibular signals indirectly
project to the anterior thalamic nuclei, where cells respond only when facing a preferred
heading direction. These neurons are thought to be involved in spatial navigation and
lose their directional selectivity through vestibular ablation (Baloh, 1990; Yates and
Miller, 1996; Guldin and Grusser, 1998).
According to Dickman (2006), two cortical areas respond to vestibular stimulation
(Fig. 21.30). One region, area 2v, lies at the base of the intraparietal sulcus just posterior
to the hand and mouth representation in the postcentral gyrus. This region functions in
vestibular consciousness – the awareness of position or movement of our body caused
by the effects of gravity and acceleration and deceleration. Other than vertigo (the
abnormal sensation of self-motion or of the motion of external objects often described as
“dizziness”), there is no easily definable, discrete vestibular sensation. Cells in area 2v
receive visual and proprioceptive inputs. Thus, area 2v is probably involved with motion
perception and spatial orientation, because it has reciprocal connections with other
parietal regions involved in similar functions (e.g.: area 5 and 7) (Kahane et al., 2003;
Dickman, 2006).
Fig. 21.30. The vestibulo-thalamo-cortical pathway. Vestibular input arises from the vestibular nuclei as
vestibulothalamic fibres and is relayed to the cortex as thalamocortical fibres. (A) Areas 3a and 2v (B) are the main
cortical regions that receive this input (after Dickman, 2006).
1683
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
It appears that vestibular stimuli integrate with visual and somatosensory modalities.
In the process of this integration, vestibular stimuli seem to lose their original, pure
character. This overlap of vestibular and visual with other somatosensory modalities often
allows each to compensate for a deficiency in the other (Leigh, 1994; Brand and
Dieterich, 1999; Augustine, 2008).
Electrical stimulation of area 2v in humans produces sensations of moving, spinning,
or dizziness. Area 2v neurons respond to head movements and receive projections from
the posterior thalamic nucleus.
Lesions of parietal cortical areas result in confusion in spatial awareness. Injuries
along the intraparietal sulcus lead to dizziness and a subjective feeling that the world is
whirling about the patient. There is the illusion of rotation or of flouting.
The second cortical area responding to vestibular stimulation, area 3a, lies at the
base of the central sulcus, adjacent to the motor cortex, and receives input from the VPL
or VPI thalamic nuclear neurons. In addition, area 3a cell receives inputs from the
somatosensory system. Because these cells project to areas of the motor cortex, it is
believed that one of their function is to integrate motor control of the head and body.
(Leigh, 1994; Kahane et al., 2003).
Visual disorientation and the disorder of spatial (topographic) localization is
another syndrome. Spatial orientation depends upon visual, tactile, and kinesthetic
perceptions. There are instances where the deficit in visual perception predominates.
Patients with this disorder, in distinction to those with environmental agnosia, are unable
to orient themselves in an abstract spatial setting (topographagnosia). Such patients cannot
draw the floor plan of their house or a map of their town, and cannot describe a familiar
route, from their home to their place of work, or find their way in familiar surroundings.
In brief, they have lost topographic memory. This disorder is almost invariably caused
by lesions in the dorsal convexity of the right parietal lobe, which is of interest, since it
suggests that there are two separate processes for spatial orientation – one for the actual
space (environmental) and one for the abstract topography of space.
et al., 1996, 2004), indicating that this region is involved in generating conscious motor
images. According to some reports (Wolpert et al., 1995; Kawato, 1999; Blakemore et
al., 2001), another brain region, the cerebellum, is also involved in predicting the future
state of a motor act.
For example, when lifting an object, subjects anticipate the increase in load force
and accordingly adapt their grip force to hold the object (Flanagan and Wing, 1997).
However, these processes may not be available to conscious awareness. Sirigu et al.
(2004) found that cerebellar patients, similar to normal control subjects, were able to
report when they first intended to move, whereas patients with parietal damage could
not. These results suggest that the parietal cortex is involved in monitoring awareness of
one’s movements.
Sirigu et al. (2004) show that patients with parietal lesions could report when they
started moving, but not when they first became aware of their intention to move. This
stands in contrast with the performance of cerebellar patients who behaved as normal
subjects. Thus, Sirigu et al. (2004) propose that when a movement is planned, activity in
the parietal cortex, as part of a corticocortical sensorymotor processing loop, generates a
predictive internal model of the upcoming movement. This model might form the neural
correlate of motor awareness.
Sirigu et al. (2004) suggest that the brain may contain several internal models used for
predictive control of voluntary action. An implicit module in the cerebellum would provide
fast processing for execution of action and predicting their sensory consequences (Blakemore
et al., 2001; Wolpert et al., 2001). A second system, in the parietal cortex, would monitor
intentions and motor plans at a higher level, detecting when actions match their desired goals.
These processes typically involve conscious experience (Sirigu et al., 2004).
1782. This band of Gennari represents a thickened external band of Baillarger in layer
IV of the cortex.
In myelin preparations, the band of Gennari appears as a prominent dark band in the
visual cortex, also known as the striate cortex. The term striate refers to the presence in
unstained preparations of the thick white band of Gennari.
It occupies the upper and lower lips and depths of the posterior part of the calcarine
sulcus and extends into the cuneus and lingual gyrus.
The primary visual cortex receives afferent fibres from the lateral geniculate nucleus
via the optic radiation. The latter curves posteriorly and spreads through the white matter
of the occipital lobe. Its fibres terminate in strict point-to-point fashion in the striate area.
The superior lip of the calcarine sulcus receives fibres projecting from the superior
retinal quadrants whereas the inferior lip of the calcarine sulcus receives projections from
the inferior retinal quadrants.
Thus, the lateral geniculate nucleus projects visual information to the primary visual
cortex (V1) (Brodmann’s area 17). This cortical area receives input arising from the
contralateral visual field: the temporal half of the ipsilateral retina and the nasal half of
the contralateral retina. The cortex of each hemisphere receives impulses from two
hemiretinas, which represent the contralateral half of the binocular visual field.
The superior and inferior retinal quadrants are connected with corresponding areas
of the striate cortex. Thus, the superior retinal quadrants (representing the inferior half
of the visual field) are connected with the visual cortex above the calcarine sulcus, and
the inferior retinal quadrants (representing the upper half of the visual field) are connected
with the visual cortex below the calcarine sulcus. Thus, fibres originating from the
superior half of the retina terminate in the superior part of the visual cortex; those from
the inferior half of the retina terminate in the inferior part. The peripheral parts of the
retina activate most anterior parts of the visual cortex.
Geniculocalcarine fibres pass in the external sagittal stratum which is separated from
the wall of the inferior and posterior horns of the lateral ventricle by the internal sagittal
stratum and by fibres of the corpus callosum designed as the tapetum. Fibres of the
internal sagittal stratum are corticofugal fibres passing from the occipital lobe to the
superior colliculus and the lateral geniculate body (Altman, 1962; Garey et al., 1968).
The macular fibres project most posteriorly along the calcarine sulcus around the
occipital pole, followed by fibres from the paramacular areas. The macular representation
in the primary visual cortex is much larger than that of other areas of the retina, reflecting
the high visual acuity of the macula.
Unilateral injury to the calcarine sulcus will lead to a contralateral homonymous
hemianopsia in which there is blindness in the ipsilateral nasal field and the contralateral
temporal field.
Lesions involving portions of the visual cortex, such as the inferior calcarine cortex,
produce a homonymous quadrantanopsia, in which blindness results in the superior half
of the visual field contralaterally. Similarly, lesions limited to the upper calcarine cortex
produce a lower contralateral quadrantanopsia in which blindness is limited to the
contralateral lower quadrant of the visual field (Fig. 21.31).
1686
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.31. Visual field deficits caused by interruption or transection of fibres at certain points along the visual path.
A. Section of the optic chiasma with a resulting bitemporal hemianopia (loss of vision in the temporal parts of both right
and left visual fields). B. Section of the left optic nerve with blindness in the left visual field and a normal right visual field.
C. Section of the optic tract causing a contralateral homonymous hemianopia. D. Section of the optic radiations in the
temporal lobe with an incongruous visual field defect. The temporal part of the right visual field is affected, as is the
superior nasal quadrant of the left visual field – a superior quadrantanopia. E. Section of the optic radiations in the
parietal lobe with a resulting contralateral homonymous hemianopia (modified from Harrington, 1981).
A bilateral destruction of the striate areas causes total blindness in men, but other
mammals, such as dogs and monkeys, retain the ability to distinguish light intensities
after ablations of the visual cortex (Klüver, 1942; Glees, 1961; Snyder et al., 1966). If
the destructive lesion is of vascular origin as occurs in occlusions of the posterior cerebral
artery, central (macular) vision in the affected visual field is spared. This phenomenon is
known clinically as macular sparing and is attributed to the contralateral arterial supply
of the posterior visual cortex (macular area) from the patent middle cerebral artery.
The striate cortex is granular. Layer IV, bearing the stria of Gennari, is commonly
divided into three sublayers. Passing from superficial to deep, these are IV A, IV B (which
contains the stria) and IV C. The densely cellular IV C is further subdivided into a
superficial IV Cα and a deep IV Cβ. Layer IV B contains only sparse mainly non-
pyramidal neurons.
Thus, the primary visual cortex occupies about 21 cm2 in each cerebral hemisphere.
Area 17 in young adults has about 35,000 neurons per mm3.
The primary visual cortex (Area 17 / V1) is thin, averages 1,8 mm in thickness, and
sums up to about 3% (range 2-4%) of the entire cerebral cortex. Although it resembles
other cortical areas, being arranged in six layers (I to VI), extensive quantitative analyses
and correlation studies in humans have identified at least ten layers in the primary visual
1687
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
cortex: layer I, II, III, IVa, IVb, IVc, Va, Vb, VIa and VIb. Layer IVc, in turn, is divisible
into IVc-α and IVc-β. Neurons in each layer have a distinctive size, shape, density, and
response to visual stimuli (Zilles et al., 1986; Glickstein, 1988; Zeki et al., 1991; Zilles,
1995, 2004). The input to area 17 from the lateral geniculate nucleus terminates
predominantly in IV A and IV C (Paxinos, 1990; Lee et al., 2000).
Other thalamic afferents from the inferior pulvinar nucleus and the intralaminar
group pass to layers I and IV. Geniculocortical fibres terminate in alternating bands.
Axons from the geniculate lamina which receives information from the ipsilateral eye
(laminae 2, 3 and 5) are segregated from those of laminae which receive input from the
contralateral eye (laminae 1, 4 and 6).
Neurons within layer IV C are monocular, i.e., they respond to stimulation of either
the ipsilateral or contralateral eye, but not both.
This horizontal segregation forms the anatomical basis of the ocular dominance
column in that neurons encountered in a vertical strip from below the pia to the white
matter, although binocular outside layer IV, exhibit a preference for stimulation of one
or the other eye (Lee et al., 2000; Crossman, 2008).
Thus, the primary visual cortex is arranged into columns oriented perpendicularly
to the cortical surface, extending from the cortical surface (under the pia mater) to the
subcortical white matter. Nerve cells, in a particular cortical column, are functionally
similar. Columns in the primary visual cortex include the ocular dominance columns,
the site where fibres relaying information from the ipsilateral or the contralateral eye
terminate, and the orientation columns, where nerve cells are responsive to visual stimuli
that have a comparable spatial orientation.
Layers II and III of the primary visual cortex also contain vertically oriented
aggregates of nerve cells that are responsive to colour.
The striate cortex, anatomically far more complex than the retina or lateral geniculate
body, does not have cells with concentric receptive fields. Cells of the striate cortex show
marked specificity in their response to restricted retinal stimulation (Hubel and Wiesel,
1959, 1963). A given cell responds vigorously when an appropriate stimulus is shone on
its receptive field, or moves across it, provided the stimulus is presented in a specific
orientation.
Thus, all cells within each column have the same receptive field axis of orientation.
The receptive field axis of orientation differs from one cell column to the next, and may
be vertical, horizontal or oblique. Thus for each stimulated area of the retina, each line
and each orientation of the stimulus, there is a particular set of “simple” striate cortical
cells that respond.
Changing any of the stimulus arrangements will cause an entirely new and different
population of “simple” striate cells to respond (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962).
“Complex” type cells, like “simple” cells, respond best to “slits”, “bars”, or “edges”,
provided the orientation is suitable. Unlike “simple” cells, these cells respond with
sustained firing as the slits of light are moved across the retina, preserving the same
receptive field axis of orientation (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). There is a relationship
1688
21. Cerebral cortex
between the complexity of response and the position of cells in relation to the cortical
laminae. Simple cells are mainly in layer IV and complex and hypercomplex cells
predominate in either layers II and III or layers V and VI.
Since columns of cells constitute the fundamental functional units of the cortex, each
small region of the visual field must be represented in the striate cortex many times, in
column after column of cells with different receptive field orientation.
The receptive fields of all binocularly influenced cortical cells occupy corresponding
sites in the two retinas. About 80% of the cells in the striate cortex are influenced
independently by the two eyes (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962).
Considerable evidence indicates that early visual experience can change the
distribution of the selective orientation of striate cortical units (Hirsch and Spinelli, 1970;
Blakemore and Cooper, 1970).
Cells of the striate cortex in kittens with controlled visual deprivation appear to adapt
functionally to the restricted visual orientations to which they have exposed during the
early critical period (Pettigrew et al., 1973).
Studies of controlled, visually deprived kittens, that permitted normal binocular
viewing after the critical period, show a dramatic increase in the number of striate cells
which are binocularly activated (Spinelli et al., 1972).
The other major functional basis for visual cortical columnar organization is the
orientation column. Thus, an electrode passing through the depth of the cortex at right
angles to the plane from below the pia to the white matter, encounters neurons that
respond preferentially to either a stationary or a moving straight line of a given orientation
within the visual field. Cells with simple, complex and hypercomplex receptive fields
occur in area 17. Simple cells respond optimally to lines in a narrowly defined position.
Complex cells respond to lines anywhere within a receptive field, but with a specific
orientation. Hypercomplex cells are similar to complex cells except that the length of
the line or bar stimulus is also critical for an optimal response. Thus, the visual cortex
units are of two varieties: simple and complex. Simple units respond only to stimuli in
corresponding fixed retinal receptive fields. Complex and hypercomplex units are
connected to several simple cortical units. It is presumed that the complex units represent
an advanced stage in cortical integration.
Thus, the visual cortex is organized into units, and, for each unit, a particular
orientation of the stimulus is most effective. Some units respond only to vertically
oriented stripes, while others respond only to horizontally oriented stripes. Some units
respond at onset of illumination, while others respond at cessation of illumination.
Units that respond to the same stimulus and orientation are grouped together in
repeating units referred to as columns. Two general varieties of functional columns have
been described: ocular dominance and orientation columns. Thus, the striate cortex is
organized into vertical and horizontal columns. The vertical columnar system is
concerned with retinal position, line orientation, and ocular dominance. The horizontal
system segregates cells of different orders of complexity. Simple cells located in layer
IV are driven monocularly, while complex and hypercomplex cells, located in other
layers, are driven by impulses from both eyes (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
1689
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
located in the inferior occipitotemporal area, in the region of the lingual or fusiform gyrus.
V5 in humans is probably located in area 19 of Brodmann. V2 like V1 is retinotopically
organized. V3 is associated with form, V4 with color, and V5 with motion.
Area 18, also termed the parastriate area, has connections with the primary visual
cortex and with area 19 in the occipital lobe through short association fibres. Area 18 has
connections with areas 18 and 19 in the contralateral cerebral hemisphere by way of
commissural fibres. Long association fibres connect area 18 with the frontal lobe
permitting this region to receive sensory, motor, and auditory impulses as well as those
from the insular and temporal lobes (Augustine, 2008). The importance of the existence
of long association fibres, that interrelate the frontal lobe with the occipital lobe, is evident
in a patient who has an injury in the middle frontal gyrus and saw stars and flashes of
light. This suggests that the occipital lobe was being stimulated when in fact the lesion
irritated fibres in the frontal lobe that interrelate the frontal and the occipital lobes
(Augustine, 2008).
Thus, the primary visual cortex projects to the visual association area where
information is processed and subsequently relayed to the tertiary visual areas (areas V3,
V4, and the middle temporal area) of the cortex. So, the major ipsilateral corticocortical
feedforward projection to V2 comes from V1. Areas 18 and 19 do not have a visible
stripe of fibres in layer IV. Area 18, the secondary visual cortex or area V2, surrounds
V1, connects with it, and lacks a specialized layer IV. More than 30 extrastriate
association areas are identifiable in the temporal, parietal and occipital lobes of primates
(Huck et al., 2002; Zilles, 2004).
Feedforward projections from V2 pass to several other visual areas (and are
reciprocated by feedback connections) including the third visual area (V3) and its various
subdivisions (V3 / V3 d; VP / V3v; V3a), the fourth visual area (V4), areas in the temporal
and parietal association cortices, and the frontal eye field (Crossman, 2008).
Thus, primary visual area 17 sends many fibres to the extrastriate visual area 18 and
area 19 that have an especially well-differentiated system of intracortical and myelinated
fibres. Area 18, in turn, has reciprocal connections with other extrastriate areas.
Thalamic afferents to V2 come from the lateral geniculate nucleus, the inferior and
lateral pulvinar nuclei and parts of the intralaminar group of nuclei. Subcortical efferents
arise predominantly in layers V and VI. They pass to the thalamus, claustrum, superior
colliculus, pretectum, brain stem reticular formation, striatum and pons.
As for area 17, the callosal connections of V2 are restricted predominantly to the
cortex, which contains the representation of the vertical meridian (Crossman, 2008).
Area 18 interprets, associates, and facilitates the understanding of impulses that reach
area 17. Information from area 18 in the dominant cerebral hemisphere correlates with
that from area 18 in the nondominant cerebral hemisphere. Area 18 participates in colour
vision in humans (Hurlbert, 2003). Area 18 and 19 in the parietal lobe are involved with
following of automatic eye movements. Stimulation of area 18 in humans yields
unformed images, wheels, flashing lights, streaks, flickering light, and coloured spots
(Fox et al., 1987; Zeki et al., 1991; Huck et al., 2002; Goodale and Westwood, 2004).
1691
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Injury to area 18 will result in the loss of following eye movements towards the
contralateral side. Although the patient is still able to see because the primary visual
cortex is intact, they often present with a visual agnosia – the loss of power to recognize
the import of visual stimuli in the absence of visual defects.
Connections of the secondary visual cortex to the angular gyrus (area 39) play a role
in recognition of visual stimuli. Lesions interrupting this connection result in visual
agnosia (De Renzi, 2000).
Cerebral achromatopsia results from bilateral damage to the V4/V4 α region of the
colour centre. If patients experience complete ablation of V4, they lose colour vision in
the entire visual field. Thus, cerebral achromatopsia arises following brain damage to
V4/V4 α located in the ventral medial region of the occipital lobe, typically caused by a
tumor, haemorrhage, or some sort of brain trauma. Because V4 is located in the vicinity
of the fusiform gyrus and the lingual gyrus, known to process faces, the comorbidity
between achromatopsia and prosopagnosia is extremely high (Kanwisher et al., 1997;
Bouvier and Engel, 2006).
In less extreme cases, known as dyschromatopsia, patients lose the ability to
perceive selective colour and / or colour constancy (Glickstein and Whitteridge, 1987;
Goodale and Milner, 1992; Bartels and Zeki, 2000; Bouvier and Engel, 2006).
The first cases of cerebral achromatopsia were reported by Verry in 1888. In
response to these patients, Verry introduced the concept of a “colour centre” in the brain.
Continued research confirmed the existence of a cortical region devoted to colour
processing. Thus, Meadows (1974) demonstrated a correlation between the cortical region
sensitive to colour and the damaged cortical regions in achromatopsic patients.
The dorsal stream (“where” path) or occipito-parieto-prefrontal path participates
in the appreciation of the spatial relations among objects (spatial vision) as well as for
the visual guidance of movements towards objects in visual space (Ungerleider and
Haxby, 1994).
The occipitoparietal cortex includes parts of Brodmann’s area 19 and area 7 from
the superior parietal lobule. The prefrontal part of these paths includes parts of the inferior
frontal gyrus corresponding to Brodmann’s areas 45 and 47 as well as the dorsal part of
premotor area 6.
There seems to be some left hemisphere specialization or dominance for visual form
in the ventral stream. Evidence from neuropsychology, electrophysiology, and
neuroimaging suggests that there are specialized processing regions for specific categories
of visual objects (i.e., faces).
Face recognition is a biological necessity and unique behavioral capacity that
requires a dedicated neuronal system that can be selectively damaged (Kanwisher and
Yovel, 2006).
Prosopagnosia is a deficit in the general capacity to discriminate between very
similar exemplars in a class of objects due to damage to neural representations of objects
that are widely distributed and overlapping (Haxby et al., 2001). Prosopagnosia is the
result of injury to a specialized neural system that develops with experience and is
required when distinguishing between subtle differences within a category of complex
visual material (Gauthier et al., 1999; Riesenhuber and Poggio, 2000; Serre et al., 2007).
Loss of face recognition and colour vision usually coexist because of the proximity of
the areas responsible for them (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2001; Hadjikhani and de Gelder,
2002; Hubel et al., 2003; Schiltz et al., 2006).
1694
21. Cerebral cortex
different cortical fields (the frontal eye fields for voluntary ocular movement and
preoccipital areas for following movements of the eyes) as well as two separate
extrapyramidal paths for their production. (Paus, 1996; Pandya and Yeterian, 1996; Milea
et al., 2002).
Cortical stimulation of parietal area 19 and occipital area 18 yields deviation of the
eyes upward, downward, or horizontal deviations. If the upper part of area 19, or the
lower part of area 18, is stimulated, upward deviation of the eyes will occur. Presumably,
there are connections from these two regions of the parietal and occipital lobes,
respectively, to the tegmentum of the midbrain by way of the internal corticotectal fibres.
In particular, these fibres pass the rostromedial part of the superior colliculus (Lobel
et al., 2001; Schüz and Miller, 2002; Zilles, 2004; Augustine, 2008).
From the rostromedial superior colliculus, tecto-oculomotor fibres pass to the
oculomotor and trochlear nuclear complexes of the midbrain. They end on neurons that
supply muscles that raise the lids and lift the eyes in an upward direction (Jampel, 1975;
King et al., 1976; Bortolami et al., 1977; Augustine et al., 1981; Leigh and Zee, 2006;
Augustine, 2008).
If the upper part of area 18, or the lower part of area 19, is stimulated, downward
deviation of the eyes will occur. The anatomic basis is by way of internal corticotectal
fibres that pass from the parietal and occipital lobes to the caudolateral part of the superior
colliculus. From this region, tecto-oculomotor fibres pass to the oculomotor nucleus,
particularly to these neurons that supply muscles that lower the eyes (Tijssen et al., 1991;
Augustine, 2008).
If the middle part of parietal area 18 and the middle part of occipital area 19 are
stimulated, horizontal deviation of the eyes will occur. In this situation, corticotegmental
paths pass from the cerebral cortex to the tegmentum of the midbrain. From there,
connection fibres project to the contralateral abducens nucleus and the abducens reticular
gray matter in order to initiate a horizontal deviation of the eyes (King et al., 1976;
Augustine et al., 1981; Leigh and Zee, 2006).
Thus, projections from area 18 and 19 also reach the frontal eye fields (area 8 of
Brodmann) in the frontal lobe, as well as the superior colliculus and motor nuclei of the
extraocular muscles. These projections play a key role in conjugate eye movement
induced by visual stimuli (visual pursuit). However, the connections and functional
aspects of ocular movements are exceedingly complex.
Patients with lesions in the posterior parietal eye field lose reflexive visually guided
saccades but are able to move their eyes in response to command (intentional saccades).
Saccadic movements are fast eye movements with rapid fixation of vision from
point to point with no interest in the points in between. Positron emission tomography
(PET) scans and lesion studies indicate that the cerebral areas most important for saccadic
control are: (1) the posterior parietal cortex; and (2) the frontal premotor cortex (Pierrot-
Deseilligny et al., 1995; Lekwuwa and Barnes, 1996).
Frontal eye field. Three areas in the frontal lobe participate in saccadic processing:
(1) the frontal eye field (area 8 of Brodmann); (2) the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (area
1696
21. Cerebral cortex
46 of Brodmann); and (3) the supplementary eye field (anterior part of the supplementary
motor area). The frontal and supplementary eye fields are activated during all types of
saccadic movements. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is activated during fixation.
The frontal eye field located in the middle frontal gyrus, anterior to or in the anterior
portion of the motor strip, corresponds to area 8 of Brodmann and the immediately
adjacent cortex. This field triggers intentional (voluntary) saccades to visible targets in
the visual environment, and subserves intentional exploration of the visual environment
(Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1995; Lekwuwa and Barnes, 1996). The frontal eye field
receives multiple cortical inputs, from the parietooccipital cortex, supplementary eye
fields, and the prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s area, 46). The frontal eye field elicits
intentional (voluntary) saccades through connections to the nuclei of the extraocular
muscles in the brain stem.
The pathway from the frontal eye field to the nuclei of extraocular movements is
not direct but involves multiple brain stem reticular nuclei, including the superior
colliculus, the interstitial nucleus of the longitudinal fasciculus, and the paramedian
pontine reticular formation.
This frontal eye field can be functionally subdivided into different sectors depending
on the direction of the eye movements that can be elicited from it.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation in humans has permitted the delimitation of the
frontal eye field within an area that comprises portions of the dorsolateral prefrontal and
premotor cortex (Chouinard et al., 2003; Pierrot-Deseiligny et al., 2004).
Auditory cortex
Fig. 21.32. Superolateral surface of the left cerebral hemisphere including the boundaries,
major sulci and gyri of the temporal and occipital lobes.
The primary auditory cortex (Brodmann’s areas 41 and 42) resides deep in the
lateral fissure of Sylvius, in the hidden superior surface of the superior temporal gyrus,
containing the transverse temporal gyri of Heschl, and in the floor of the lateral fissure.
Thus, two transversely-oriented gyri, the anterior and posterior transverse temporal
gyri are continuous with the superior temporal gyrus but folded and hidden from view
when one observes the lateral surface of the cerebral hemisphere. The anterior transverse
temporal gyrus (of Heschl), lying in depth of the lateral sulcus and corresponding to
Brodmann’s area 41 (Fig. 21.33) is the primary auditory cortex or A I. Recordings of
primary evoked responses to auditory stimuli during surgery for epilepsy provide
evidence for a restricted portion of Heschl’s gyrus as the primary auditory area (Heffner,
1987; Hocherman and Yirimiya, 1990; Liegeois-Chauvel et al., 1994).
However, the two transverse temporal gyri are buried in the lateral sylvian sulcus,
covered by parts of the frontal and parietal opercula, and continuous with the superior
temporal gyrus. Caudal to the transverse temporal gyri is a smooth area, the planum
temporale, which is usually larger on the left side than on the right (Fig. 21.34).
The temporal operculum houses the primary auditory cortex. This is coextensive
with the granular area 41 in the transverse temporal gyri. Surrounding areas constitute
the auditory association cortex (Fig. 21.35).
The length and surface area of the left primary auditory cortex is greater than the
right in newborns and adults.
The circular insular sulcus limits area 41 medially while laterally area 43 does not
reach the temporal operculum.
Thus, the primary auditory cortex (Brodmann’s area 41 or A I) is located in the first
(anterior) transverse temporal gyrus but many extend into the second (posterior) gyrus.
1698
21. Cerebral cortex
Fig. 21.33. Cytoarchitectural map showing Brodmann areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere (modified after
Brodmann K, from Carpenter MB, Sutin J: Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins, 1983).
Fig. 21.34. The organization of auditory cortical areas. Location and interconnections of auditory cortical areas
(A) and of the granular cortex in area 41 (B), and the orthogonal isofrequency and binaural responses columns in the
primary auditory cortex (detail from A) (after Henkel, 2006).
1699
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
frequency columns. The cells in each frequency column respond to an auditory stimulus
of a particular frequency. So, cells responding to low frequencies reside in the frequency
columns located in the rostral extent of the transverse temporal gyri of Heschl, whereas
frequency columns containing cells responding to gradually higher frequencies are lined
up in sequence towards the caudal extent of the primary auditory cortex.
The other dimension of the auditory cortex is composed of alternating binaural
columns. There are two types of binaural columns: summation columns and suppression
columns. The neurons residing in the summation columns respond to an auditory stimulus
that stimulates both ears simultaneously.
In contrast, the neurons in the suppression columns respond maximally to an auditory
stimulus that stimulates only one ear, but respond minimally when the auditory stimulus
stimulates both ears (Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
The primary auditory cortex of each side receives information from both ears by way
of the medial geniculate body of the thalamus, and then sends projections to the contralateral
side via the corpus callosum. Area 41 is also reciprocally connected with the anterior division,
and area 42 with the posterior division of the medial geniculate body.
The primary auditory cortex receives fibres predominantly from the contralateral side.
Orderly projections from neurons of the medial geniculate body to the primary
auditory cortex are termed thalamotemporal projections or acoustic radiations that
reach the temporal lobe through the sublenticular part of the internal capsule. Auditory
fibres originate in the peripheral organ of Corti (Fig. 21.36) and establish neural synapses
in the neuraxis, both homolateral and contralateral to their side of origin before reaching
the medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus (Altschuler et al., 1986, 1991; Pickles,
1988; Webster et al., 1992).
Physiologic studies of the primary auditory cortex have revealed that it does not
play a major role in sound frequency discrimination but rather in the temporal pattern of
acoustic stimuli. Sounds are heard in the primary auditory cortex and pattern for tones of
different pitch exist here in nonhuman primates and, presumably, also in human.
Frequency discrimination of sound is a function of subcortical acoustic structures.
The optimal stimulus that fires auditory cortical units seems to be a changing frequency
of sound stimuli rather than a steady-frequency stimulus (Webster et al., 1992; Yost,
1994).
One of the characteristic features of the auditory system is its tonotopic localization.
The tonotopic localization present in the cochlea appears to be preserved through all of
the relay nuclei of the auditory system to levels of the inferior colliculus (Neff, 1961;
Rose et al., 1963; Whitfield, 1977). At higher levels of the auditory system there is an
increasing proportion of neural elements not directly concerned with the parameter of
stimulus frequency, even though they respond to complex sounds (i.e., noise click)
covering broad bands of the audible spectrum (Ades, 1959).
The tonotopic organization of constituent cells of the cortical layers and incoming
afferent fibres from a series of orderly isofrequency columns extend through the primary
auditory cortex as long stripes. High frequencies are represented medially and low
frequencies laterally.
1701
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 21.36. Ascending central auditory pathways. Monoaural pathways , binaural pathways, and other connections. A I
and A II, primary and secondary auditory cortices; H, high frequencies; L, low frequencies (after Henkel, 2006).
Single-cell responses are to single tones of a narrow frequency band. Cells in single
vertical electrode penetrations share an optimum frequency response.
The series of stripes so formed have one subcomponent composed of cells excited
by stimulation of both ears alternating with a subcomponent composed of cells excited
by the contralateral ear and inhibited by the ipsilateral ear (Webster et al., 1992; Yost,
1994; Henkel, 2006).
In the cerebral cortex two areas showing tonotopic localization have been defined
in cats by determining the loci of potentials evoked in response to stimulation of nerve
1702
21. Cerebral cortex
fibres in the cochlea, or different sound frequencies (Woolsey and Walzl, 1942; Ades,
1943, 1959). In the more dorsal area, referred to as auditory area I (A I), the basal coils
of the cochlea (high frequencies) are represented rostrally, and the apical region of the
cochlea (low frequencies) caudally. In the more ventral auditory area (A II) the tonotopic
localization is reversed. A third cortical zone designated PE (i.e., posterior ectosylvian
area) lies posterior to auditory areas I and II and appears to be related functionally to
auditory area I.
Anatomical studies of ablations of these auditory areas have provided information
concerning the origin of afferent fibres from the medial geniculate body (Rose and
Woolsey, 1949, 1958).
Auditory areas I and II have also been identified in monkeys (Ades and Felder, 1942;
Pribram et al., 1954). In chimpanzees, tones of low frequency are represented
anterolaterally, while tones of high frequency are represented posteromedially (Bailey et
al., 1943).
Stimulation of the cortical areas in the temporal lobe near the primary auditory area
(i.e., areas 42 and 22), in men, produces sounds described as the noise of a cricket, a bell
or a whistle. These sounds are elementary tones which may be high or low pitched,
continuous or interrupted, but are always devoid of complicated or changing qualities
(Penfiled and Rasmussen, 1950). Most of these auditory responses are referred to the
contralateral ear. The largest and most important fibre crossing is in the trapezoid body
at the level of the cochlear nuclei, but others also are present, including fibres from
auditory cortical areas that cross in the corpus callosum (Mettler, 1932).
Stimulation of the primary auditory cortex produces crude auditory sensations such
as buzzing, humming, or knocking. Such sensations are clinically referred to as tinnitus.
Physiological studies (Woolsey and Walzl, 1942; Ades and Brookhart, 1950;
Rosenzweig, 1954) indicate that each cochlea is represented bilaterally in the auditory
cortex, although some slight differences exist between the two sides. Thus, when the
sound is presented on one side, the cortical response is greatest in the contralateral
hemisphere. If the sound is presented in a median plane, the cortical activity in the two
hemispheres is equal.
Because audition is represented bilaterally at a cortical level, unilateral lesions of
the auditory cortex cause impairment in sound localization in space and diminution of
hearing bilaterally, but more often contralaterally.
According to Penfield and Evans (1934), the removal of one temporal lobe impairs
sound localization on the opposite side, especially judgment of distance of sound.
Clinically, unilateral lesions of the auditory cortex are difficult to recognize.
Experimental studies indicate that bilateral ablations of auditory areas I, II and PE
in cats do not abolish auditory localization of sound in space, although discriminations
of this kind are impaired (Neff et al., 1956; Neff and Diamond, 1958). Sound localization
in space is most critically impaired by bilateral ablations of A I (Strominger, 1969).
Bilateral ablations of the auditory cortical areas have little or no effect on the ability
of cats to discriminate changes in frequency (Meyer and Woolsey, 1952; Butler et al.,
1957; Neff and Diamond, 1958).
1703
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Meyer and Woolsey (1952) reported that following bilateral ablations of auditory
areas I, II and PE, and somatic area II, cats could not relearn to discriminate changes in
frequency, but could discriminate changes in sound intensity. The ability to localize sound
in space is not affected by a section of the corpus callosum, is affected very little by a
section of the commissure of the inferior colliculus and is severely affected by a section
of the trapezoid body (Neff and Diamond, 1958; Jerger, 1960).
Neurons in each medial geniculate body project fibres to layers IIIb and IV of the
auditory cortex. Area 41 in sensorial area of the koniocortex with layers II-IV of densely
arranged small neurons surrounded by fields with large and medium-sized IIIc pyramidal
neurons.
The primary auditory cortex is connected with the primary (unimodal) association
auditory cortex. Other important connections include the auditory cortex of the
contralateral hemisphere, the primary somesthetic cortex, frontal eye fields, Broca’s area
of speech in the frontal lobe, and the medial geniculate nucleus. Thus, the auditory cortex
interconnects with the prefrontal cortex, though the projections from A I are small.
Generally, posterior parts of the operculum project to areas 8 and 9. Central parts project
to areas 8, 9 and 46. More anterior regions project to areas 9 and 46, to area 12 on the
orbital surface of the hemisphere, and to the anterior cingulate gyrus on the medial
surface. Contralateral corticocortical connections are with the same and adjacent regions
in the other hemisphere.
Onward connections of the auditory association pathway converge with those of the
other sensory association pathways in cortical regions within the superior temporal sulcus
(Pickles, 1988; Webster et al., 1992; Yost, 1994; Crossman, 2008).
Via its projection to the medial geniculate body in the thalamus, the primary auditory
cortex controls its own input by changing the excitability of medial geniculate neurons.
faces (Altschuler et al., 1991; Webster et al., 1992; Yost, 1994; Crossman, 2008). Lesions
of area 22 and part of area 21 of Brodmann have no effect on the perception of sound
and pure tone.
Area 20 is the higher visual association area. The posterior inferior temporal cortex
receives major ipsilateral corticocortical fibres from occipitotemporal visual areas,
notably V4.
The anterior inferior temporal cortex projects onto the temporal pole and to
paralimbic areas on the medial surface of the temporal pole.
Additional ipsilateral connections of the inferior temporal cortex are with the anterior
middle temporal cortex in the walls of the superior temporal gyrus, and with visual areas
of the parietotemporal cortex.
Unlike this, these are frontal lobe connections with area 46, with the orbitofrontal
cortex, and with the frontal eye field. Reciprocal thalamic connections are with the
pulvinar nuclei. Intralaminar connections are with the paracentral and central medial
nuclei. Callosal connections are between corresponding areas and the adjacent visual
association areas of each hemisphere.
The cortex of the temporal pole receives feedforward projections from widespread
areas of the temporal association cortex which are immediately posterior to it. The dorsal
part receives predominantly auditory inputs from the anterior part of the superior temporal
gyrus. The inferior part receives visual inputs from the anterior area of the inferior
temporal cortex (Crossman, 2008).
Other ipsilateral connections are with the anterior insular, the posterior and medial
orbitofrontal, and the medial prefrontal cortices.
The temporal pole projects onwards into limbic and paralimbic areas.
Thalamic connections are mainly with the medial pulvinar nucleus, and with
intralaminar and midline nuclei.
Physiological responses of cells in this and more medial temporal cortices
correspond particularly to behavioral performance and to other recognition of high-level
aspects of social stimuli (Crossman, 2008).
Descending auditory pathways. Descending projections make reciprocal con-
nections throughout the auditory pathway. They form feedback loops that provide circuits
to modulate information processing from the peripheral level to the cortex (Yost, 1994;
Henkel, 2006). Thus, the auditory cortex projects to the medial geniculate nucleus and
nuclei of the inferior colliculus. The inferior colliculus projects to the periolivary nuclei,
which, in turn, send olivocochlear efferents to the cochlea. There are also descending
projections from the periolivary nuclei to the cochlear nuclei (Fig. 21.37).
Acoustic startle reflex, orientation, and attention. Reflexive and learned responses
to sound require sensory-motor integration. In addition to corticocortical interconnections
for the dissemination of auditory information, there is also an integration of auditory sensory
inputs with motor pathways in the brain stem (Henkel, 2006). Reticulospinal neurons in the
region of the lateral lemniscus have dendrites that sample lemniscal activity and are involved
in rapid acoustic startle reflex pathways (Webster et al., 1992; Henkel, 2006).
1706
21. Cerebral cortex
In addition, the deep layers of the superior colliculus receive auditory information
from the inferior colliculus and auditory areas (Fig. 21.38). The deep layer of the superior
colliculus integrate auditory, visual and somesthetic information and project to the brain
stem and cervical spinal cord nuclei via tectobulbospinal fibres, which are involved in
controlling the orientation of the head, eyes, and body to sound (Altschuler et al., 1986,
1991; Yost, 1994; Henkel, 2006).
In sum, bilateral destructions of the primary auditory cortex, the bordering area 22,
or both will result in cortical deafness (bilateral loss of hearing caused by bilateral cerebral
injuries).
1707
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 21.38. The pathways that subserve auditory-motor integration involved in simple orientation to a novel auditory
stimulus. RF, reticular formation; PPRF, paramedian pontine reticular formation (after Henkel, 2006).
Although such patients have bilateral cerebral injury, others often have a reasonably
normal audiogram because tones of different pitch came to consciousness at a thalamic
level. Destruction of only one primary auditory cortex will lead to subtle auditory deficits.
Such patients have difficulty in localizing sounds in the contralateral auditory field and
perhaps a loss of discrimination of distorted, interrupted, or accelerated speech
(Augustine, 2008).
Destruction of the auditory association cortex (Wernicke’s region) in the left cerebral
hemisphere often fields a handicapping auditory receptive aphasia. Such patients have
normal hearing and a normal production of words (fluent). Yet they are unable to interpret
spoken sounds related to language or use words appropriately. Patients with injuries
limited to the parietal operculum are often indifferent to loud and unpleasant noise
(Eickhoff et al., 2006; Augustine, 2008).
The medial geniculate nucleus forms a small protuberance on the lower caudal
surface of the thalamus between the lateral geniculate body and the pulvinar. Low
frequencies are represented laterally and higher frequencies are represented medially.
Thus, the anterior division of the medial geniculate nucleus receives afferents from the
central nucleus of the inferior colliculus and projects to the primary auditory cortex.
The posterior division receives inputs from the pericentral nucleus of the inferior
colliculus and projects to secondary auditory cortex. This pathway may convey
information about moving or novel stimuli that direct auditory attention.
The medial (magnocellular) division receives afferents from the external nucleus
of the inferior colliculus and projects to association areas of the auditory cortex. The
1708
21. Cerebral cortex
medial division projects to temporal and parietal association areas and to the amygdala,
putamen and pallidum. So, this pathway may be a part of the reticular activating system
(Pickles, 1988; Webster et al., 1992; Henkel, 2006).
INSULA
Vicq d’Azyr was the first, in 1781, to express interest in the insula. He referred to it
as the „convolutions” situated between the sylvian fissure and the corpus callosum. In
1809 (a, b), Reil was the first to describe the insula.
In order to view the insula, a highly developed structure in the depth of the sylvian
fissure, the frontal, temporal, and parietal opercula have to be pulled apart, since this lobe
is submerged within and forms the floor of the lateral sulcus (Eickhoff et al., 2006).
The sulcus separating the insula from the operculae is referred to by different authors
as the periinsular sulcus, limiting sulcus, circuminsular sulcus, insular sulcus, or circular
sulcus.
The frontal operculum is between the anterior and ascending rami of the lateral
fissure, forming a triangular division of the inferior frontal gyrus.
The frontoparietal operculum, between the ascending and posterior rami of the lateral
fissure, consists of the posterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus, the lower ends of the
precentral and postcentral gyri, and the lower end of the anterior part of the inferior
parietal lobule (Varnavas and Grand, 1999).
The temporal operculum (Fig 21.39), below the posterior ramus of the lateral fissure,
is formed by the superior temporal and transverse temporal gyri.
Fig. 21.39 Coronal representation showing the somatotopy of the body in the primary motor cortex, which is located in
the precentral and anterior paracentral gyri (adapted from Penfield and Rasmussen, 1968).
1709
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 21.40. Lateral view of the left cerebral hemisphere. The frontal and parietal opercula are removed, and the
temporal operculum is retracted to expose the insula and transverse temporal gyri (after Hains and Mihailoff, 2006).
The term “operculum” (Latin: lid, or covering) refers to the fact that these parts of
the respective lobes form a lid or cover for the insular lobe that forms the floor of the
lateral sulcus. Anteriorly, the inferior region of the insula adjoins the orbital part of the
inferior frontal gyrus. The insula is shaped like a pyramid. The summit of the pyramid is
in the insular apex. Its apex is beneath and near the anterior perforated substance where
the circular sulcus is deficient. The medial part of the apex is termed the limen insulae
(gyrus ambiens). The limen insulae (threshold to the insula) is the area in which the
inferior surface of the hemisphere is continuous with the insular cortex.
The insula is traversed by an obliquely directed central insular sulcus which divides
the insular surface into a large anterior and a small posterior part. The anterior part
exhibits transverse and accessory insular gyri and three or four short gyri (anterior,
middle, and posterior). The transverse and accessory insular gyri form the insular pole
located at the most anterior inferior aspect of the insula (Fig. 21.40).
The posterior part, located caudally to the central insular sulcus, is composed of the
anterior and posterior long gyri, separated by the postcentral insular sulcus (Ture et al.,
1999). The cortex of the insula is continuous with that of its opercula in the circular
sulcus. The insula is approximately coextensive with the subjacent claustrum and
putamen.
The insula is surrounded by the arcuate fasciculus which interconnects the frontal
and temporal lobes. The superior longitudinal fasciculus, located in the core of the
hemisphere, interconnects the frontal, parietal and occipital cortices. In the white matter
of the temporal lobe, fibres passing between the frontal and occipital areas make up the
inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus.
1710
21. Cerebral cortex
The insular cortex of the primates shows a sequential transition from allo- to meso-
and finally to isocortex (Brackhaus, 1940; Stephan, 1975; Jones and Burton, 1976 b;
Mesulam and Mufson, 1982 a, 1985). Its most retro-ventro-mesial portion located right
at the limen insulae is allocortical and forms the insular segment of the prepyriform
olfactory cortex. Around this allocortical core are arranged three concentric, approxi-
mately semicircular belts. The first, the agranular insula, bordering rostroventrally on the
allocortical central segment of the prepyriform cortex, is part of the lateral mesocortical
belt that runs along the lateral edge of the prepyriform-periamygdaloid allocortex; it is
bordered along its dorsal and caudal periphery by the dysgranular insular proisocortex,
which at its periphery dorsally and caudally is followed by the isocortical granular insula.
Thus, cytoarchitectonically, three zones are recognized within the insula. Anteriorly, and
extending caudally into the central insula, the cortex is agranular. It is surrounded by a
belt of dysgranular cortex, in which the laminae II and III can be recognized, and this, in
turn, is surrounded by an outer zone of homotypical granular cortex which extends to the
caudal limit of the insula.
Although the function of the insula is still somewhat unclear, it is known that the
insular cortex receives nociceptive and viscerosensory inputs.
Connections. The insula of primates is reciprocally connected to many surrounding
and to some more distal cortical areas and to the amygdala (Mesulam and Mufson, 1982
b, 1985; Mufson and Mesulam, 1982; Seleman and Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Cavada and
Goldman-Rakic, 1989 a). The connections of the postero-dorsal granular and adjacent
portion of the dysagranular insula are part of the transinsular somatosensory association
path (Mufson and Mesulam, 1982; Mesulam and Mufson, 1985; Friedman et al., 1986).
This region also seems to receive inputs from the auditory association cortex
(Mufson and Mesulam, 1982; Mesulam and Mufson, 1985). It is also connected to a
segment of the premotor cortex in area 6 representing the face, to the prefrontal cortex
below the principal sulcus (areas 45 and 46), the isocortical portion of the orbitofrontal
cortex, the cingulate cortex, and the precentral nucleus of the amygdala (Mesulam and
Mufson, 1982 b, 1985; Mufson and Mesulam, 1982).
Rostral to this somatosensory association area in the posterior insula there may be
in the dysagranular insula an area of multimodal convergence between gustatory and
other afferents emanating from the somatosensory and premotor areas concerned with
the representation of the face and mouth region (Sudakov et al., 1871; Mesulam and
Mufson, 1971). In monkeys, this dysgranular and agranular area receives afferents from
the parvocelullar portion of the ventroposterior medial thalamic nucleus, the thalamic
gustatory relay nucleus (Mufson and Mesulam, 1984; Mesulam and Mufson, 1985;
Pritchard et al., 1986), and its neurons respond to gustatory stimuli (Sudakov et al., 1971;
Yaxley et al., 1990).
The parvocelullar portion of the ventroposterior thalamic nucleus, also receives
viscerosensory afferents that, as they ascend from the brain stem through the ventromedial
thalamus, are difficult to disentangle from ascending taste pathways (Beckstead et al., 1980).
1711
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
In rodents, the insula receives afferents from the same thalamic relay nucleus as well
as more diverse gustatory and viscerosensory inputs from gustatory-viscerosensory relay
nuclei in the brain stem, the parabrachial nuclei (Saper, 1982 a, b; Shipley and Saunders,
1982; Cechetto and Saper, 1987; Krushel and van der Kooy, 1988; Allen et al., 1991).
The rostro-ventral dysgranular-agranular insular cortex of primates is reciprocally
connected with the gustatory opercular cortex, the mesocortical orbitofrontal cortex
(especially its caudolateral portion), the cingulate cortex, the olfactory prepyriform
allcortex, the temporopolar cortex, most of the amygdaloid nuclei, and the peri- and
entorhinal cortices (Mesulam and Mufson, 1982 b, 1985; Mufson and Mesulam, 1982;
Rolls, 1989 a).
In primates the postero-dorsal granular-dysgranular sector may predominantly
function as a unimodal somatosensory association area. This is supported by the fact that
this region is reciprocally connected with the somatosensory subdivision (area 7 b) of
the parietal association cortex (Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989 a; Neal et al., 1990 b).
Conversely, the anteroventral dysgranular-agranular sector is likely to be multimodal
with emphasis on gustatory, viscerosensory, and olfactory modalities together with some
visceromotor representations.
Thus, the insular lobe in primates including humans has connection with the cerebral
cortex of the frontal lobe (orbital cortex, frontal operculum, lateral premotor cortex,
ventral granular cortex and medial area 6) and the parietal lobe (second somatosensory
area and retroinsular area of the parietal lobe). Ipsilateral cortical connections of the insula
are diverse. Somatosensory connections are with S I, S II and surrounding areas, area 5
of the superior parietal lobe and area 7b of the inferior parietal lobe.
There are also insular connections with the temporal lobe (temporal pole and superior
temporal sulcus) and the insular lobe (an abundance of local intra-insular connections)
(Augustine, 1985, 1996, 2008).
There are insular projections to subdivisions of the cingulate gyrus and to the lateral,
lateral basal, central, cortical and medial amygdaloid nuclei. There are also insular
connections with nonamygdaloid areas such as the perirhinal cortex, entorhinal cortex,
and the periamygdaloid cortex. The thalamic taste area projects fibres to the ipsilateral
insular-opercular cortex (Augustine, 1996; Ture et al., 1999; Augustine, 2008).
The insular cortex projects to almost all the nuclei of the amygdala (Amaral and
Insausti, 1992; Carmichael and Price, 1995; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2002).
Most of these projections originate in the rostral insular cortices, specifically the
agranular (Ia) and rostral aspects of the dysgranular (Id) division (Amaral and Insausti,
1992; Carmichael and Price, 1995; Mufson et al., 1997; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2000,
2002).
Projections from more caudal division of the insular cortex (caudal divisions of Id
and the granular insular cortex Ig) are less dense and less widespread (Amaral and
Insausti, 1992; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2000, 2002).
The parainsular cortex sends projections to the lateral nucleus, the basal nucleus, and
the accessory basal nucleus (Amaral and Insausti, 1992; Stefanacci and Amaral, 2000).
1712
21. Cerebral cortex
Most of the insular projections are directed towards the middle to caudal aspects of
the amygdala and originate predominantly in layer II and III, with a lesser contribution
from layer V (Stefanacci and Amaral, 2002).
The amygdaloidal complex returns projections throughout the insular cortex and to
the superficial and deep layers (Carmichael and Price, 1995). These connections are
generated by the lateral nucleus, the basal nucleus, the accessory basal nucleus, the medial
nucleus, the anterior cortical nucleus, the periamygdaloid cortex, and the anterior
amygdaloid area (Amaral and Price, 1984; Carmichael and Price, 1995).
Caudal regions of area Id and area Ig receive fewer projections from the amygdaloid
complex (Amaral and Price, 1984).
Thalamic afferents to the insula come from subdivisions of the ventral posterior
nucleus and of the medial geniculate body, from the oral and medial parts of the pulvinar,
the suprageniculate / nucleus limitans complex, the mediodorsal nucleus and the nuclei
of the intralaminar and midline groups. It appears that the anterior (agranular) cortex is
connected predominantly with the mediodorsal and ventroposterior nuclei, while the
posterior (granular) cortex is connected predominantly with the pulvinar and the ventral
posterior nuclei.
The other nuclear groups appear to connect with all areas (Crossman, 2008).
Thus, the anterior insular part connects with the frontal lobe, and the posterior part
connects with the parietal and temporal lobes. Lesion-based analysis has shown that
distructions of the left anterior part impair coordination of articulation and speech
production.
Functional aspects
In rats, single-cell recording in the insular cortex reveals a „viscerotopic”
organization. Thus, the taste and gastric representation lies next to the somatosensory
representation of the tongue and that for cardiopulmonary receptors next to that of the
trunk (Cechetto and Saper, 1987).
In primates and rodents, the anteroventral portion of the insula is concerned with
alimentary and related visceral functions (Hoffman and Rasmussen, 1953; Penfield and
Faulk, 1955).
There is also a visceromotor representation in the insula. In various animal species
including primates, stimulation of the insula affects a number of autonomic functions
such as gastric peristalsis, salivation, blood pressure, and heart rate as well as respiration
(Kwada, 1951; Hoffman and Rasmussen, 1953; Oppenheimer and Cechetto, 1990).
In humans, insular stimulation affects gastric peristalsis (Penfield and Faulk, 1955), as
well as heart rate and blood pressure. Bradycardia and falls in diastolic blood pressure was
more often elicited from left insular stimulations whereas tachycardic and pressure responses
occured more commonly with stimulation of the right insula (Oppenheimer et al., 1992).
Thus, the insular cortex has been implicated in the mediation of heart rate, in taste,
and in gustatory processing. Given the role of the amygdaloid complex in identifying
danger in the environment, connections with insular cortex may be a route for gustatory
and autonomic information to be processed by the amygdala.
1713
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The insular cortex receives nociceptive and viscerosensory inputs. Thus, it has
visceral sensory and somatic sensory roles as well as visceral motor and somatic motor
roles.
According to Mufson et al. (1997) and Augustine (1996, 2008), the insula partici-
pates in the autonomic regulation of the gastrointestinal tract and the heart and is as well
functioning as a motor association area. This motor function includes a role in the
recovery of motor functions after stroke and in ocular movements.
The insular lobe also serves as a vestibular area and as a language area including
memory tasks related to language and auditory processing underlying speech. Other
insular functions include a possible role in the verbal component of memory and its role
in selective visual attention.
Wernicke’s area
Language is an arbitrary and abstract way to represent thought processes by means
of sentences and to present concepts or ideas by means of words.
Most components of the language system are located in the left hemisphere, which
is the dominant hemisphere for language in approximately 95% of humans.
Nearly all right-handers and about two-thirds of left-handers have such dominance.
Wernicke’s area, comprises an extensive region that includes the posterior part of
the superior temporal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 22) including planum temporale in the
floor of the sylvian fissure, and the parietooccipitotemporal junction area including the
angular gyrus (Brodmann’s area 39).
According to Augustine (2008), those parts of the superior temporal gyrus cor-
responding to Brodmann’s areas 42 and 22 constitute Wernicke’s region, an auditory
association region.
The upper surface of area 22, the planum temporale, is distinctly longer on the left
side (dominant hemisphere for language) in most people. Together, Heschl’s gyrus
(primary auditory cortex) and the anterior and posterior temporal planes are sometimes
referred to as the planum temporale.
Brodmann’s areas 42 and 22 constitute Wernicke’s region, an auditory association
region. In this region, sounds are appreciated, they become meaningful for understanding
language, and are interpreted as words. Thus, Wernicke’s area is concerned with the
comprehension of language. The superior temporal gyrus component of Wernicke’s area
(area 22) is concerned with comprehension of spoken language, whereas the angular
gyrus (area 39) and adjacent regions are concerned with comprehension of written
language. According to Afifi and Bergman (2005), spoken language is perceived in the
primary auditory area (Heschl’s gyrus, area 41 and 42) in the superior temporal gyrus
and transmitted to the adjacent located Wernicke’s area, where it is comprehended.
According to Haines and Mihailoff (2006), the gyri that are part of the Wernicke’s
area are the supramarginal gyrus – Brodmann’s area 40 – and the angular gyrus –
1714
21. Cerebral cortex
Brodmann’s area 39. Clinically, the Wernicke area is regarded as extending into the
temporal lobe and encompasses portions of Brodmann’s area 22 and some of area 21.
According to Patestas and Gartner (2008), Wernicke’s area consists of auditory
association cortex (part of Brodmann’s area 22), as well as parts of the supramarginal
and angular gyri (Brodmann’s areas 37, 39, and 40, usually in the left, dominant
hemisphere). Wernicke’s area is also referred to as the receptive (sensory, language area,
general interpretative area, or the gnostic area. It plays an important role in the
comprehension and formulation of language. When an individual reads and says the
words, the visual pathway relays the sensory visual input to the primary visual cortex
where the words are “seen”. The primary visual cortex transmits this input to the
association visual cortex where the significance of the words is determined. The visual
information is then relayed to Wernicke’s area where the words are comprehended, and
the creation of the words takes place. Wernicke’s area then projects to Broca’s area,
which dictates the motor activity necessary to produce the words.
Reading requires the perception of visual language stimuli by the occipital cortex,
followed by correlation with auditory language information, via the intermodal
association cortex of the angular gyrus. Writing involves the activation of motor neurons
projecting to the arm and hand.
In the right cerebral hemisphere this cortical area is an auditory association area
that serves to determine the emotional undertones of language. When we hear someone
speak, the person’s voice often reveals if he / she is happy, upset, angry, etc.
Although several areas in the left hemisphere are dominant in the reception,
programming and production of language function, corresponding areas in the right
hemisphere are metabolically active during speech. These areas are believed to be
concerned with the melodic function of speech (prosody). Lesions in such areas of the
right hemisphere render speech amelodic (aprosodic). Lesions in area 44 on the right
side result in a dull monotonic speech. Lesions in area 22 on the right side, on the other
hand, may lead to an inability of the patient to detect inflection of speech. Such patients
may be unable to differentiate whether a particular remark is intended as a statement or
as a question (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
In right-handed people and in a majority of left-handers as well, clinical syndromes
of aphasia result from left hemisphere lesions. Rarely, aphasia may result from a right
hemisphere lesion in a right-handed patient, a phenomenon called crossed aphasia
(Bakar et al., 1996).
Broca’s area
Gall and Spurzheim (1809), based on their observations of skull shape, placed the
ability to speech and recall words in the inferior aspect of the frontal lobes. Bouillaud
(1825) hypothesized that the anterior lobes of the brain contained a centre for speech
production. Bouillaud even offered a prize to the first individual who reported a case with
loss of speech without lesion to the frontal lobes.
However, there were a number of studies that were real contributions to the
knowledge of the clinical phenomenology of aphasia. One is the description by Osborne,
1715
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
in 1833, of a highly educated patient with severe jargon aphasia who understood spoken
speech quite well and could read with understanding. His writing was only mildly
affected.
Lordat’s case, reported in 1843, is of a priest who after a stroke showed an almost
complete motor aphasia but steadily improved.
There is also Marcé’s paper, in 1856, dealing specifically with impairment in writing.
Marcé described a number of cases of agraphia within the setting of oral language
disturbances of varying severity and different types, and he postulated the existence of a
cerebral centre for writing distinct from the coordinating centre for oral speech.
There is, also, an important reference to receptive aphasia in the 1843 monograph
of Lordat.
The association of aphasia with right hemiplegia is usually ascribed by Broca (1861,
1863), but this distinction is often dimmed by the accompanying mention of Marc Dax’s
earlier contribution in 1836 (1865), a quarter of a century before Broca’s first observation.
In 1836, Marc Dax presented a work indicating that disturbances in language production
were due to lesions of the left hemisphere. Dax’s work remained largely unknown until
his son, Gustav Dax (1865, 1877), presented and published his deceased father’s work
in the years after 1863 (see Benton, 2000 and Buckingham, 2006).
Dax’s memoir is not only interesting in that it is the first mention of the role of the
left hemisphere in the function of speech but also because it raises once again the problem
of “priority” in scientific observations (Benton, 2000).
Broca called the inability to produce language in the context of intact comprehension,
“aphenie”. Trousseau subsequently renamed such disturbances “aphasia” in 1864.
However, in 1863, Broca published a series of eight cases showing primarily left-frontal
lesions with language production deficit. In an 1865 and 1869 manuscript, Broca firmly
asserted that the left hemisphere is the dominant seat for language production.
Broca’s area is equivalent to areas 45 and 44 located in the opercular and triangular
portions of the left inferior frontal gyrus. This area of the cortex not only “programs” the
neurons in the adjacent motor cortex subserving the mouth and larynx from which
descending axons travel to the brain stem cranial nerve nuclei in the imitation of a
sequence of complex movements, but also has expressive language capacities (e.g.,
grammar).
Within Broca’s area, a coordination program for vocalization is formulated. The
elements of the program are transmitted to the face, tongue, vocal cords, and pharynx
area of the motor cortex for execution of speech.
The supplementary motor area is also necessary in the production of language for
the initiation of speech. Broca’s area receives input from Wernicke’s area via the arcuate
fasciculus (Wise et al., 1999; Sherwood et al., 2003).
Broca’s area is involved in planning the motor activity that is necessary for the word
to be produced. Broca’s area projects this information to the premotor cortex where motor
activity is planned and relayed to the primary motor cortex, which initiated the
movements necessary to produce speech. Thus, when a word is heard, the output from
1716
21. Cerebral cortex
the primary auditory area (Heschl’s gyrus) is conveyed to the adjacent Wernicke’s area,
where the word is comprehended. If the word is to be spoken, the comprehended pattern
is transmitted via the arcuate fasciculus from Wernicke’s area to Broca’s area in the
inferior frontal gyrus. The arcuate fasciculus is a long associated fibre bundle that links
Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas of speech. Damage to the arcuate fasciculus is associated
with impairment of repetition of spoken language. If the word is to be read,
representations visualized as words or images are conveyed from the visual cortex (areas
17, 18 and 19) to the angular gyrus (area 39), which in turn arouses the corresponding
auditory form of the word in Wernicke’s area. From Wernicke’s area, the information is
relayed via the arcuate fasciculus to Broca’s area (Afifi and Bergman, 2005).
These pathways, and doubtless others, constitute the cortical circuitry for language
comprehension and expression. In addition, other cortical centres involved in cognitive
processes project into the primary language cortex, influencing the content of language.
Finally, subcortical structures play increasingly recognized roles in language function
(the thalamus, the reticular activating system, the nuclei of the basal ganglia etc.).
Lesions in Broca’s area are associated with a type of aphasia (motor, anterior,
expressive, nonfluent) characterized by the inability of a patient to express himself by
speech. Such patients are able to comprehend language when Wernicke’s area is intact.
Autopsy data and neuroimaging studies have shown both the absence of Broca’s aphasia
in individuals with lesions in this area and also the reverse (Yang et al., 2008).
Depression is a common psychological consequence of aphasia, and is significantly
more common after anterior left-hemisphere lesions (including those associated with
nonfluent aphasia) than lesions in other areas (Carson et al., 2000).
biological roots. Although this conclusion seems obvious for language, it is less obvious
for music, which has often been perceived as an artefact of culture.
in prosopagnosia, the inability to recognize faces (Hadjikhani and Gelder, 2002; Schiltz
et al., 2006).
Thus, a severe deficit in discriminating and identifying faces with the inability to
recognize one’s own pictures or mirror images is likely to occur with injury to both
fusiform and lingual gyri. The visual association properties of these regions include the
synthesis of specific faces, which requires the complex synthesis of visual input, memory,
and other sensory phenomena. Individuals with prosopagnosia or face blindness may
learn to recognize people by their clothes, mannerisms, gait, hairstyle or voice. While
they connot recognize a person by their face, they are able to recognize facially expressed
emotions like happy, sad, and angry (Bodamer, 1947; Damasio et al., 1982; Landis et
al., 1986; Schiltz et al., 2006).
Medial regions of the temporal lobe are essential for learning and memory. Bilateral
damage results in the inability to learn new information or make new memories
(anterograde amnesia). These patients are unable to make new memories, learn new facts,
or recall new experiences. Their memory leading up to the surgery is largely intact and
general intelligence is also unaffected (French et al., 1993; Engel and Weiser, 1997;
Blumer et al., 1998; Engel, 2001).
Anomic aphasia
Anomia can occur in healthy individuals who occasionally experience difficulty in
thinking of an intended word during conversation, also known as the tip-of-the-tongue
state (Biedermann et al., 2008). It is a frequent occurence in individuals with left
hemisphere brain damage and aphasia (Raymer, 2005). Typically associated with
difficulties for nouns, anomia also can affect the ability to retrieve other classes of words,
such as verbs and adjectives. It is important to note that anomia and the anomic aphasia
are not synonymous. Anomia is the primary symptom of anomic aphasia and can also
be observed in virtually all other forms of aphasia, both as initial and residual signs when
other signs and symptoms of aphasia have revolved (Raymer and Rothy, 2008).
Anomia leads to a complete inability to retrieve a word and other times an
inappropriate word is retrieved known as a paraphasia. In severe forms of anomia,
neologisms may occur in which the uttered word may not be recognizable at all.
Anomic aphasia is the language impairment that involves only word finding
difficulties or pure anomia in contrast to other forms of aphasia (Goodglass et al., 2001).
In the left temporal / occipital region, particularly in parts of the fusiform and
probably the lingual gyrus corresponding to Brodmann’s areas 37 and 29, is a region
connected by the inferior occipitofrontal fasciculus with the inferior and perhaps
midtemporal regions and with other cortical areas along its course.
This region connects with the inferolateral and part of the lateral of the frontal lobe.
Acutely, anomic aphasia has been described following left temporal / occipital
lesions (e.g., area 37) and left thalamic lesion (Raymer et al., 1997 a, b).
The grammatical characteristics of the sentence remain intact. Common in anomic
aphasia is circumlocution, in which the speaker cannot think of the intended word and instead
describes or provides associated information about the word (Laine and Martin, 2006).
1720
21. Cerebral cortex
1722
21. Cerebral cortex
REFERENCES
Abel LA, Unverzagt F, Yee RD, Effects of stimulus predictability and interstimulus gap on
saccades in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Congnitive Disorders 13; 235-243,
2002.
Abeles M, Goldstein MH, Functional architecture in cat primary auditory cortex: Columnar
organization and organization according to depth. J Neurophysiol 33; 172-187, 1970.
Ades HW, A secondary acoustic area in the cerebral cortex of the cat. J Neurophysiol 6; 59-64,
1943.
Ades HW, Central auditory mechanisms. In: J Field (ed.), Handbook of Physiology, Section I,
Vol I. American Physiological Society, Washington, DC, Ch. 24, 585-613, 1959.
Ades HW, Felder R, The acoustic area of the monkey (Macaca mulatta). J Neurophysiol 5; 49-
54, 1942.
Ades HW, Brookhart HM, The central auditory pathway. J Neurophysiol 13; 189-205, 1950.
Adrian ED, Double representation of the feet in the sensory cortex of the cat. J Physiol 98; 16-18,
1940.
Adrian ED, Afferent discharges to the cerebral cortex from peripheral sense organ. J Physiol 100;
159-191, 1941.
Afifi AK, Bergman RA, Functional Neuroanatomy. Text and atlas. Lange Medical Books / Mc
Graw-Hill. New York, Chicago, San Francisco etc., 246-257, 2005.
Allen GV, Saper CB, Hurley KM, Cechetto DF, Organization of visceral and limbic connections
in the insular cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 311; 1-16, 1991.
Altman J, Some fiber projections to the superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol, 119; 77-95,
1962.
Altschuler RA, Bobbin RP, Hoffman DW, Neurobiology of Hearing: the Cochlea. New York.
Raven Press, 1986.
Altschuler RA, Bobbin RP, Clapton BM, Hoffman DW, Neurobiology of Hearing. The Central
Auditory System. New York, Raven Press, 1991.
Amaral DG, Price JL, Amygdala-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). Journal
of Comparative Neurology 230; 465-496, 1984.
Amaral DG and Insausti R, Retrograde transport of D [3H]-aspartate injected into the monkey
amygdaloid complex. Experimental Brain Research 88; 375-388, 1992.
Amunts K, Schlaug G, Schleicher A, et al., Asymmetry in the human motor cortex and handedness.
NeuroImage 4; 216-222, 1996.
Anderson TJ, Jenkins IH, Brooks DJ et al., Cortical control of saccades fixation in man. A PET
study. Brain 117; 1073-1084, 1994.
Andersson B, and Jewell PA, Studies on the thalamic relay for taste in the goat. J Physiol 139;
191-197, 1957.
Ang Jr. ES, Haydar TF, Gluncic V, Rakic P, Four-dimensional migratory coordinates of
GABAergic interneurons in the developing mouse cortex. J. Neurosci 23; 5805-5815, 2003.
Arciniegas DB, Beresford TP, Neuropsychiatry. An introductory approach. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 2001.
Ascoli GA et al., Pelilla terminology: nomenclature of features of GABAergic interneurons of
cerebral cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 9; 557-568, 2008.
1723
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Bertrand G, Spinal efferent pathways from the supplementary motor area. Brain 79; 461-473,
1956.
Biedermann B, Ruh N, Nickels L, Coltheart M, Information retrieval in tip of the tongue: New
data and methodological advances. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 37; 171-198, 2008.
Blakemore C, Cooper GF, Development of the brain depends on visual experience. Nature 228;
478, 1970.
Blakemore SJ, Frith CD, Wolpert DM, The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory
consequences of action. Neuroreport 12; 1879-1884, 2001.
Blanke O, Seeck M, Direction of saccadic and smooth eye movements induced by electrical
stimulation of the human frontal eye field: effect of orbital position. Experimental Brain
Research 150; 174-183, 2003.
Blomquist AJ, Benjamin RM, Emmers R, Thalamic localization of afferents from the tongue in
squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). J Comp Neurol 118; 77-78, 1962.
Blum M, Walker AE, Ruch TC, Localization of taste in the thalamus of Macaca mulatta. Yale J
Biol and Med 16; 175-192, 1943.
Blumer D, Wakhlu S, Davies K, Herman B, Psychiatric outcome of temporal lobectomy for
epilepsy: incidence and treatment of psychiatric complications. Epilepsia 39; 478-486, 1998.
Bodegård A, Geyer S, Naito E et al., Somatosensory areas in man activated by moving stimuli:
cytoarchitectonic mapping and PET. NeuroReport 11; 187-191, 2000.
Bodegård A, Ledberg A, Geyer S et al., Object shape differences reflected by somatosensory
cortical activation, J Neurosci 20; 1-5, 2000.
Bodegård A, Geyer S, Naito E et al., Somatosensory area in man activated by moving stimuli:
cytoarchitectonic mapping and PET. NeuroReport 11; 187-191 2000 a.
Bodegård A, Ledberg A, Geyer S et al., Object shape differences reflected by somatosensory
cortical activation. J Neurosci 20; 1-5, 2000 b.
Bodegård A, Geyer S, Grefkes C et al., Hierarchical processing of tactile shape in the human
brain. Neuron 31; 317-328, 2001.
Börnstein WS, Cortical representation of taste in man and in monkey. II. Localization of cortical
taste area in man and method of measuring impairment of taste in man. Yale J Biol and Med
13; 133-156, 1940 a.
Börnstein WS, Cortical representation of taste in man and monkey. I. Functional and anatomical
relations of taste, olfaction and somatic sensibility. Yale J Biol and Med 12; 719-736, 1940.
Bortolami R, Veggetti A, Callegari E et al., Afferent fibers and sensory ganglion cells within the
oculomotor nerve in some mammals and man. I. Anatomical investigations. Arch Ital Biol
115; 355-385, 1977.
Bouillaud JB, Traité Clinique et Physiologique de l’Encéphalite. Paris: Baillière, 1825.
Bouvier SE, Engel SA, Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia.
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY: 1991) 16; 183-191, 2006.
Bowsher D, Some afferent and efferent connections of the parafascicular – center median complex.
In: DP Purpura and MD Yahr (eds.), The Thalamus. University Press, New York, 99-108,
1966.
Brandt T, Dietrich M, The vestibular cortex. Its locations, functions, and disorders. Ann NY Acad
Sci 871; 293-312, 1999.
Broca P, Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation
d’aphémie. Bull Soc Anat (Paris) 6; 330-357, 1861.
1725
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Broca P, Localization des fonctions cérébrales. Siège du langage articulé. Bull Soc Anthrop (Paris)
4; 200-203, 1863.
Broca P, Sur la faculté du langage articulé. Bul Soc Antropol 6; 337-393 1865.
Broca P, Sur le siège de la faculté de langage articulé. Tribune Médicale 3; 254-256, 265-269,
1869.
Brockhaus H, Die Cyto- und Myeloarchitektonik des Cortex claustralis und des Claustrums beim
Menschen. J Psychol Neurol (Leizig) 49; 248-249, 1940.
Brodal A, Neurobiological anatomy (3rd ed) New York, Oxford University Press, 1981.
Brodmann K, Vergleichende Lokalisationlehr der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargesteltt
auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig: JA Barth, 1909.
Brown KN, Chen S, Han Z, et al., Clonal production and organization of inhibitory interneurons
in the neocortex. Science 334; 480-486, 2011.
Buccino G, Vogt S, Ritzl A et al., Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions:
An event-related fMRI study. Neuron 42; 323-334, 2004.
Buckingham HW, The Marc Dax (1770-1837) / Paul Broca (1824-1880) controversy over priority
in science: Left hemisphere specificity for seat of articulate langage and for lesions that cause
aphemia. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 20; 613-619, 2006.
Burkhalter A, Bernardo KL, Organization of corticocortical connections in human visual cortex.
Proc Natl Acad Sci 80; 1071-1075, 1989.
Butler RA, Diamond IT, Neff WD, Role of auditory cortex in discrimination of changes in
frequency. J Neurophysiol 20; 108-120, 1957.
Butt et al., The temporal and spatial origins of cortical interneurons predict their physiological
subtype. Neuron 27; 9682-9695, 2005.
Boxer AL, Principles of motor control by the frontal lobes as revealed by the study of voluntary
eye movements. In BL Miller and JL Cummings (Eds.), The Human Frontal Lobe. Second
Edition. The Guilford Press. New York, London, Ch. 17, 262-276, 2007.
Bradley WE, Farrell DF, Ojeman GA, Human cerebrocortical potentials evoked by stimulation
of the dorsal nerve of the penis. Somatosens Mot Res 15; 118-127, 1998.
Braitenburg V, Schüz A, Cortex Statistics and Geometry of Neuronal Connectivity. 2nd ed, Springer
Verlag, Berlin, 1998.
Brodmann K, Vergleichende localizationslehre der groshirnrinde. Leipzig: Borth, 1909.
Brouwer B, Ashby P, Altered corticospinal projections to lower limf motoneurons in subjects with
cerebral palsy. Brain 114; 1395-1407, 1991.
Bucy PC, Effects of extirpation in man. In: PC Bucy (ed.), The precentral Motor Cortex, ed. 2.
University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ch. 14, 353-294, 1949.
Bucy PC, The surgical treatment of abnormal involuntary movements. Neurologia 1; 1-15, 1959.
Burton H, Fabri M, Alloway K, Cortical areas within the lateral sulcus connected to cutaneous
representation in area 3b and 1: a revised interpretation of the second somatosensory area in
macaque monkeys. J Camp Neurol 355; 539-562, 1995.
Cajal S. Ramón y, Histologie du système nerveux de l’homme et des vertébrés. Norbert Maloine,
Paris. 2 vols, 1909, 1911.
Campbell AW, Histological Studies on the Localization of Cerebral Function. Cambridge
University Press, New York, 360, 1905.
Carmichael ST, Price JL, Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in
macaque monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology 363; 615-641, 1995.
Carpenter MB, Human Neuroanatomy. Seventh Edition. The Williams and Wilkins Company,
Baltimore, Maryland, USA, 547-599, 1979.
1726
21. Cerebral cortex
Carpenter MB, Upper and lower motor neurons. In: J.A. Downey, and RC Darling (Eds.),
Physiological Basis of Rehabilitation Medicine. WB Saunders Company, Philadelphia, Ch.
1; 3-27, 1971.
Carpenter MB, Sutin J, Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins, 1983.
Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC et al., Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from
neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceeding of the National Academy of Science
of the United States of America 100; 5497-5502, 2003.
Carreras M, Levitt M, Microelectrode analysis of the second somatosensory cortical area in the
cat. Fed. Proc, 18; 24, 1959.
Carson AJ, MacHale S, Allen K et al., Depression after stroke and lesion location. A systematic
review. Lancet 356; 122-126, 2000.
Casanova Manuel F, Neocortical Modularity and the Cell Minicolumns. New York, Nova Science
Publisher, 2005.
Cavada C, Goldman-Rakic PS, Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I – Parcelation of areas
based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections. J Comp Neurol 287; 393-
421, 1989 a.
Cechetto DF, Saper CB, Evidence for a vinerotropic sensory representation in the cortex and
thalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 262; 27-45, 1987.
Cerri G, Shimazu H, Maier MA, Lemon RN, Facilitation from ventral premotor cortex of primary
motor cortex outputs to macaque hand muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology 90; 832-842,
2003.
Chouinard PA, Van Der Werf YD, Leonard G, Paus T, Modulating neural networks with
transcranial magnetic stimulation applied over the dorsal premotor and primary motor
cortices. J Neurophysiol 90; 1071-1083, 2003.
Cisek P, Kalaska JF, Neural correlates of mental rehearsal in dorsal premotor cortex. Nature 431;
993-996, 2004.
Cogan DG, Neurology of the Ocular Muscles, (Ed. 2), Charles C Thomas, Publisher, Springfield,
Ill., 1956.
Cohen MJ, Landgren S, Strom L, Zottermann Y, Cortical reception of touch and taste in the cat.
Acta physiol scandinav 40: Suppl 135; 50, 1957.
Collins RC, Cerebral cortex. In: AL Pearlman and RC Collins (eds). Neurobiology of disease.
New York Oxford University Press, 1990.
Colonnier M, The fine structural arrangement of the cortex. Arch Neurol 16; 651-657, 1967.
Colonnier M, Synaptic patterns and different cells types in the different aminae of the cat visual
cortex. An electron microscopic study. Brain Res 9; 268-287, 1968.
Condy C, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Ostendorf F et al. Neural substrate of antisaccades: Role of
subcortical structures. Neurology 63; 1571-1578, 2004.
Constantinidis C, Franowicz MN, Goldman-Rakic PS, The sensory nature of mnemonic
representation in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 4; 311-316, 2001.
Corballis MC, From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness.
Behavioral Brain Sciences 26; 199-208, 2003.
Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Cantoro TE et al., A common network of functional areas for attention
eye movements. Neuron 21; 761-773, 1998.
Corbin JG, Nery S, Fishell G, Telencephalic cells take a tangent: non-radial migration in the
mammalian forebrain. Nat Neurosci 4 (suppl), 1177-1182, 2001.
Coxe WS, Landau WM, Observations upon the effect of supplementary motor cortex ablation in
the monkey. Brain 88; 763-772, 1965.
1727
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Cramer SC, Finkelstein SP, Schaechter JD, et al., Activation of distinct motor cortex regions
during ipsilateral and contralateral finger movements. Journal of Neuropsychology 81; 383-
387, 1999.
Crosby EC, Relations of brain centers to normal and abnormal eye movement in the horizontal
plane. J Comp Neurol 99; 437-480, 1953.
Crosby EC, Humphrey T, Lauer EW, Correlative Anatomy of the Nervous System. Macmillan
Company, New York, 731, 1962.
Crossman AR, Cerebral hemisphere. In: bray’s Anatomy. First Edition, Chapter 23, Churchill
Livingstone. Elsevie, London, 335-359, 2008.
Curtis BA, Jacobson S, Marcus EM, An Introduction to the Neurosciences. Philadelphia: Saunders,
1972.
Daffner KR, Scinto LF, Weintraub S et al., Diminished curiosity in patients with probable Alzheimer’s
disease as measured by exploratory eye movements. Neurology 42; 320-328, 1992.
Damasio AR, Damasio H, van Hoesen GW-Prasopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavioral
mechanisms. Neurology 32; 331, 1982.
Darby DG, Nobre AC, Thangaray V et al., Cortical activation in the human brain during lateral
saccades using EPISTAR functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 3; 53-62,
1996.
Dax G, Observations tendant á prouver la coincidence constante de dérangements de la parole
avec une lésione de l’hémisphere gauche de cerveau. Montpellier Méd. 38; 313-340 and 508-
529; 39; 112-130, 226-237, and 413-421, 1877.
Dax M, Lésions de la moitié gauche de l’encephale coincident avec l’oublie de signes de la pensée.
Gax. Hbd. Méd. Chir (Paris) 2; 259-262, 1865.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Tratat de Neuropsihologie, Vol 2. editura Medicală Bucureşti, 261-319, 2006.
De Felipe J, Jones EC, Cajal on the cerebral Cortex: An Annotated Translation of the Complete
Writings. New York: Oxford University Press, 1988.
Denny-Brown D, Botterell EH, The motor functions of the agranular frontal cortex. A Rev Nerv
and Ment Dis Proc 27; 235-345, 1948.
Denny-Brown D, Motor mechanisms – Introduction: the general principles of motor integration.
In: J Field (ed.), Handbook of Physiology, Section I. Vol II, American Physiological Society,
Washington, DC Ch. 32; 781-796, 1960.
De Renzi E, Disorders of visual recognition. Sem Neurol 20; 479-485, 2000.
De Yoe EA, Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC, McClendon E, Multiple processing streams in
occipitotemporal visual cortex. Nature 371; 151-154, 1994.
Déjérine J, Sémiologié du système nerveux. In : C Bouchard (ed), Traité de Pathologie Générale.
Vol 5. Paris. Masson, 1900.
Desmurget M, Grfton S, Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements.
Trends Cogn Sci 4; 423, 2000.
Di Pellgrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L et al., Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological
study. Experimental Brain Research 9; 176-180, 1992.
Dickman JD, The vestibular system. In: DE Haines (ed.), Fundamental Neuroscience for Basic
and Clinical Applications. Third Edition. Churchill Livingstone. Elsevier. Philadelpia, 351-
365, 2006.
Dickman JD, Byer M, Hess BJ, Three-dimensional organization of vestibular related eye
movements to rotational motion in pigens. Vision Res 40; 2831-2844, 2000.
Disbrowe-Roberts T, Krubitzer L, Somatotopic organization of cortical field in the lateral sulcus
of Human sapiens: evidence for S II and PV. J Comp Neurol 418; 1-21, 2000.
1728
21. Cerebral cortex
Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B et al., Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention
understanding. Science 308; 662-667, 2005.
Foerster O, Motor cortex in man in the light of Hughlings Jackson’s doctrine. Brain 59; 135-159,
1936 a.
Foerster O, Symptomatologie der Erkrankungen des Grosshirns. Motorische Felder und Bahmen.
In: Neurologie. Vol 5. Julius Springer, Berlin, 1-357, 1936 b.
Fox PT, Fox JM, Raichle ME, et al., The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary
saccades: a positron emission tomographic study. J Neurophysiol 54; 348-369, 1985.
Fox PT, Miezin FM, Allman JM et al., Retinotopic organization of human visual cortex mapped
with positron-emission tomography. J Neurosci 7; 913-922 1987.
Franckowiak RSJ, Fristan KJ, Frith CD, et al., Human brain function. San Diego Academic Press,
1997.
French JA, Williamson PD, Thadani VM et al., Characteristics of medial temporal lobe epilepsy:
I. Result of history and physical examination: Annals of Neurology 43; 774-780, 1993.
Friedman DP, Murray EA, O’Neill JB, Mishkin M, Cortical connections of the somatosensory
fields of the lateral sulcus of macaques: evidence for a corticolimbic pathway for touch. J
Comp Neurol 252; 323-247, 1986.
Fulton JF, Keller AD, The Sign of Babinski. A study of the Evolution of Cortical Dominance in
Primates. Charles C Thomas, Springfield, Ill., 165, 1932.
Fulton JF, Kennard MA, A study of flaccid and spastic paralysis produced by lesions of the
cerebral cortex in primates. A Res Nerv and Ment Dis Proc 13; 158-210, 1934.
Funahashi S, Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS, Prefrontal neuronal activity in rhesus monkeys
performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature 365; 753-756, 1993.
Fuster JM, Memory in the cerebral cortex: An empirical approach to neural metworks in the
human and nonhuman primate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995.
Fuster JM, The prefrontal cortex – an update: time is of the essence. Neuron 30; 319-333, 2001.
Gabrieli JDE, Poldraqck RA, Desmond JE, The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and
memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95; 906-913, 1998.
Gagnon D, O’Driscoll GA, Petrides M, Pike G, The effect of spatial and temporal information on
saccades and neural activity in oculomotor structures. Brain 125; 123-139, 2002.
Gall FJ, Spurzheim G, Recherche sur le système. Nerveux en Général et sur Celui du Cerveau en
Particulier. Paris: F Schoell, 1809 (Reprint. Amsterdam: Bonsel, 1967).
Gallese V, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Rizzolotti G, Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain
119; 593-609, 1996.
Gastant H, Clinical and electroencephalographic classification of epileptic seizures. Epilepsia 11;
102, 1970.
Gastant H, Definitions. In: Dictionary of Epilepsy. Part I. World Health Organization, Geneva, 1973.
Garey LJ, (Trans), Brodmann’s “Localization in the Cerebral Cortex”. Smith-Gordon, London 1994.
Glimcher PW, Decisions, Uncertainly and the Brain: The Science of Neuroeconomics, Cambridge,
MA: MIT, Press 2003.
Garey LJ, Jones EG, Powell TPS, Interrelationships of striate and extrastriate cortex with the
primary relay sites of the visual pathway. J Neurol Neurosurg and Psychiat 31; 135-157, 1968.
Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Andersson et al., Activation of the middle fusifom “face area” increases with
expertize in recognizing novel objects. Nature Neuroscience 2; 568-573, 1999.
Gazzaniga MS, Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication. Does the corpus
callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123; 1293-1326, 2000.
1730
21. Cerebral cortex
Gelman DM, Marin O, Generation of interneuron diversity in the mouse cerebral cortex. Eur. J
Neurosci 31; 2136-2141, 2010.
Gelnar PA, Krauss BR, Szeverenyi NM, Apkarian AV, Fingertip representation in the human
somatosensory cortex: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 7; 261-283, 1998.
Gennari F, De peculiari structura cerebri, nonnullisque ejus morbis. Parma: ex Regio Typographia,
1782.
Geschwind N, Behavioral changes in temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychological Medicine 9; 217-219,
1979.
Geschwind DH, Iacoboni M, Structural and functional asymmetries of the human frontal lobes.
In: BL Miller and JL Cummings (eds.), The Human Frontal Lobes. Second Edition. The
Guilford Press. New York, London, 68-91, 2007.
Geyer S, Ledberg A, Schleicher A et al., Two different areas within the primary motor cortex of
man. Nature 382; 805-807 1996.
Geyer S, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of human primary somatosensory cortex. 1.
Microstructural organization and interindividual variability. NeuroImage 10; 63-83, 1999.
Geyer S, Schorman T, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, Areas 3a, 3b, and 1 of human somatosensory cortex.
Part 2, Spatial normalization to standard anatomical space. NeuroImage 11; 684-696, 2000.
Gibbs FA, Ictal and non-ictal psychiatric disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease 113; 522-528, 1951.
Ginhoux F, Greter M, Leboeuf M et al., Fate mapping analysis reveals that adult microglia derive
from primitive macrophages. Science 330; 841-845, 2010.
Glickstein M, Whitteridge D, Tatsuji Inoue and the mapping of the visual on the human cerebral
cortex. TINS 10; 350-353, 1987.
Godoy J, Lüders H, Dinner DS et al., Versive eye movements elicited by cortical stimulation of
the human brain. Neurology 40; 296-299, 1990.
Goldberg E, Gradient approach to neocortical functional organization. Journal of clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology 11; 1489-1517, 1989.
Goldberg E, Higher cortical functions in humans: The gradiental approach. In: E Goldberg (ed).
Contemporary neuropsychology and the legacy of Luria. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1990.
Goldberg E, Rise and fall of modular orthodoxy. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology 17; 193-2008, 1995.
Goldberg E, Bilder RM, Hughes JE et al., A reticulofrontal disconnection syndrome. Cortex 25;
687-695, 1989.
Goodale MA, Milner AD, Separate visual pathways for perception of action. Trends Neurosci 15;
20-25, 1992.
Goodale MA, Westwood DA, An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical
pathways for perception and action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14; 203-211, 2004.
Gorno-Tempini et al., Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: An fMRI study.
NeuroImage 14; 465-473, 2001.
Graeber MB, Changing face of microglia. Science 330; 783, 2010.
Grodzinsky Y, Amunts K, Broca’s Region. Oxford University Press, New York, 2006.
Guitton D, Buchtel HA, Douglas RM, Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing
reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Experimental Brain Research 58;
455-472, 1985.
Gupta A, Wang Y, Markram H, Organizing principles for a diversity of GABAergic interneurons
and synapses in the neocortex. Science 287; 273-278, 2000.
1731
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Glickstein M, The discovery of the visual cortex. Sci Am 256; 118-127, 1988.
Gless P, Experimental Neurology. Oxford University, Press, London, 1961.
Guldin WO, Grüsser OF, Is there a vestibular cortex? TINS 21; 254-259, 1998.
Hadjikhani N, De Gelder B, Neural basis of prosopagnosia: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 16;
176-182, 2002.
Häggvist G, Faserabalytische Studien über die Pyramidenbahn. Acta Psychiat et Neurol 12; 457-
466, 1937.
Haines DE, Michailoff GA, The telencephalon. In: DE Haines (Ed.), Fundamental Neuroscience
for Basic and Clinical Applications. Third Edition. Churchill, Livingstone. Elsevier,
Philadelphia, 2006.
Hanbery J, Jasper H, Independence of diffuse thalamo-cortical projection system shown by
specific nuclear destructions. J Neurophysiol 16; 252-271, 1953.
Happe F, Brownell H, Winner E, Acquired “theory of mind” impairments following stroke.
Cognition 70; 211-240, 1999.
Hasegawa RP, Peterson BW, Goldberg ME, Prefrontal neurons coding suppression of specific
saccades. Neuron 43; 415-425, 2004.
Haxby JV, Gobbini Mi, Furey ML et al., Distributed and overlapping representation of faces and
objects in ventral temporal cortex. Science 293; 2425-2430, 2001.
Heffner HE, Ferrier and the study of auditory cortex. Arch Neurol 44; 218-221, 1987.
Henkel CK, The auditory system. In: DE Haines (ed.), Fundamental Neuroscience for Basic and
Clinical Applications. Churchill Livingstone Elsevier, Philadelphia, 2006.
Highstein SM, Cohen B, Buttner-Ennerver JA, New directions in vestibular research. Ann NY
Acad Sci 781; 1-739, 1996.
Hikosaka O, Takikawa Y, Kawagoe R, Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive
saccadic eye movements. Physiological Reviews 80; 953-978, 2000.
Hillis AE, Language and frontal cortex. In: BL Miller and JL Cummings (eds.), The Human
Frontal Lobes Functions and Disorders, Second Edition. The Guilford Press, New York,
London. Ch. 20; 306-316, 2007.
Hines M, The anterior border of the monkey’s (Macaca mulatta) motor cortex and the production
of spasticity. Am J Physiol 116; 76, 1936.
Hines M, The “motor” cortex. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 60; 313-336, 1937.
Hirsch HVB, Spinelli DN, Visual experience modifies distribution of horizontally and verically
oriented receptive fields in cat. Science 168; 869-871, 1970.
Hocherman S, Yirimya R, Neuronal activity in the medial geniculate nucleus and in the auditory
cortex of the rhesus monkey reflects signal anticipation. Brain 113; 1707-1720, 1990.
Hoffman BL, Rasmussen T, Stimulation studies of insular cortex of Macaca mulatta. J
Neurophysiol 16; 343-351, 1953.
Holmes G, May WP, On the exact origin of the pyramidal tract in man and other mammals. Brain
32; 1-43, 1909.
Huang ZL, Di Cristo G, Ango F, Development of GABA inervation in the cerebral and cerebellar
cortices. Nat Rev Neurosci 8; 673-686, 2007.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat’s striate cortex. J Physiol 148;
547-591, 1959.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the
cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol 160; 106-154, 1962.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN, Shape and arrangement of columns in cat’s striate cortex. J Physiol 165;
559-558, 1963.
1732
21. Cerebral cortex
Hubel D et al., Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicate alternative face processing
strategies in autism. Neurology 61; 1232-1237, 2003.
Huck AC, Dougherty RF, Heeger DJ, Retinotopy and functional subdivision of human areas MT
and MST. J Neurosci 22; 7195-7205, 2002.
Hurlbert A, Color vision: primary visual cortex shows its influence. Curr Biol 13; 270-272, 2003.
Hutton S, Kennard C, Oculomotor abnormalities in schizophrenia: A critical review. Neurology
50; 604-609, 1998.
Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Mazziotta JC, Blood flow increases in left dorsal premotor cortex during
sensorimotor integration learning. Society for Neuroscience. Abstract 22; 720, 1996.
Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M et al., Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286;
2526-2528, 1999.
Iacoboni M, Molna-Szakacs I, Gallese I, Gallese V et al., Grasping the intentions of others with
one’s own mirror neuron system. PloS Biology 3; e79, 2005.
Iamel RS, Ocular torsion and the function of the vertical extraocular muscles. J Comp Ophtalmol
79; 292-304, 1975.
Ishai A, Ungerleider LG, Matin A, Haxby JV, The representation of objects in the human occipital
and temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 12 Suppl 2; 35-51, 2000.
Isoda M, Tanji J, Contrasting neuronal activity in the supplementary and frontal eye fields during
temporal organization of multiple saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology 90; 3054-3065, 2003.
Ivry RB., Robertson LC, The two sides of perception. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1998.
Iwata M., Modular organization of visual thinking. Behavioral Neurology 2, 153-166, 1989.
Jasper HH, Unspecific thalamocortical relations. In: J Field (ed), Handbook of Physiology. Sec-
tion 1, Vol. II. American Physiological Society. Washington DC. Ch. 53; 1307-1321, 1960.
Jerger JF, Observations on auditory behavior in lesions of the central auditory pathways. AMA
Arch Otolaryng 71; 797-806, 1960.
Jerison NJ, Brain Size and the Evolution of Mind. New York, American Museum of Natura
History, 1991.
Johnston JL, Miller JD, Nath A, Ocular motor dysfunction in HIV-1-infected subjects: A
quantitative oculographic analysis. Neurology 46; 451-457, 1996.
Jones EG, Varieties and distribution of non-pyramidal cells in the somatic sensory cortex of the
squirrel monkey. J Comp Neurol 160; 205-268, 1975.
Jones EG, Laminar distribution of cortical efferent cells. In: Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 1. New York,
Plenum Press, 521-553, 1984.
Jones EG, Cortical and subcortical contributions to activity-dependent plasticity in primate
somatosensory cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 23; 1, 2000.
Jones EG, The thalamus, Second Edition, Vol. I, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2007.
Jones EG, Leavitt RY, Retrograde axonal transport and the demonstration of non-specific
projections to the cerebral cortex and striatum from thalamic intralaminar nuclei in the rat
and monkey. J Comp Neurol 154; 349-378, 1974.
Jones EG, Burton H, Areal differences in the laminar distribution of thalamic afferents in cortical
fields of the insular parietal and temporal regions of primates. J Comp Neurol 168; 197-247,
1976 b.
Kaada BR, Somato-motor, autonomic and electrocorticographic responses to electrical stimulation
of “rhinencephalic” and other structures in primates, cat and dog. Acta physiol Sand 24 (Suppl
83); 285, 1951.
Kaas JH, The transformation of association cortex into sensory cortex. Brain Res Bull 50; 425,
1999.
1733
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Kaas JH, Evolution of somatosensory and motor cortex in primates. Anat Rec A Discov Mol Cell
Evol Biol 281; 1148-1156, 2004.
Kahane P et al., Reappraisal of the human vestibular cortex by cortical electrical stimulation study.
Ann Neurol 54; 615-624, 2003.
Kanwisher N, Dermott J, Chun MM, The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate
cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience 17; 4302-4311, 1997.
Kätzel D, Zemelman BV, Buetfering C, et al., The columnar and laminar organization of inhibitory
connections to neocortical excitatory cells. Nat Neuosci 14; 100-107, 2011.
Kawato Y, Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobil 9;
718-727, 1999.
Kanwisher N, Yovel G, The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception
of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences
361; 2109-2128, 2006.
Kennard MA, Somatic functions. In: PC Bucy (ed.), The Precentral Motor Cortex. Ed. 2.
University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ch. 9; 243-276, 1949.
Kennard MA, Fulton JF, The localizing significance of spasticity, reflex grasping and the signs
of Babinski and Rossolino. Brain 56; 213-225, 1933.
Kennard MA, Viets HR, Fulton JF, The syndrome of the premotor cortex in man: Impairment of
skilled movement, forced grasping, spasticity and vasomotor disturbances. Brain 57; 69-84,
1934.
Keubitzer L, Clarey J, Tweedale R et al., Redefinition of somatosensory areas in the lateral sulcus
of macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 15; 3821-3839, 1995.
King WN, Lisberger SG, Fuchs AF, Responses of fibers in medial longitudinal fasciculus (MLF)
of alert monkeys during horizontal and vertical conjugate eye movements evoked by
vestibular or visual stimuli. J Neurophysiol 39; 1135-1149, 1976.
Klein C, Developmental functions for saccadic eye movement parameters derived form pro- and
antisaccade tasks. Experimental Brain Research 139; 1-17, 2001.
Klein C, Fischer B, Hartnegg K et al., Optomotor and neuropsychological performance in old age.
Experimental Brain Research 135; 141-154, 2000.
Kobayashi Y, Saito Y, Isa T, Facilitation of saccade initiation by brain stem cholinergic system.
Brain and Development 23 (Suppl. 1); 24-27, 2001.
Kohler S, Moscovitch M, Unconscious visual processing in neuropsychological syndromes: A
survey of the literature and evaluation of models of consciousness. In: MD Rugg (ed).
Cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1997.
Kohler E, Keysers C, Umiltà MA et al., Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action
representation in mirror neurons. Science 297; 846-848, 2002.
Kolb B, Whishaw QI, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Fifth Edition. World Publishers.
New York, 2003.
Kormack DR, Rakic P, Radial and horizontal deployment of clonally related cells in the primate
neocortex: relationship to distinct mitotic lineages. Neuron 15; 311-321, 1995.
Krauzlis R and Dill N, Neural correlated of target choice for pursuit and saccades in the primate
superior colliculus. Neuron 35; 355-363, 2002.
1734
21. Cerebral cortex
Krauzlis RJ, Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. Journal of Neurophysiology
91; 591-603, 2004.
Krushel LA, Van Der Kooy D, Visceral cortex: integration of the mucosal senses with limbic
information in the rat agranular insular cortex. J Comp Neurol 270; 39-54, 1988.
Küver H, Functional significance of the geniculo-striate system. Biological Sympasia 7; 253-299,
1942.
Kuyspers HGJM, Lawrence DG, Cortical projections to the red nucleus and the brain stem in the
rhesus monkey. Brain Res 4; 151-188, 1967.
Laine M, Martin N, Anomia: Theoretical and clinical aspects. New York: Psychology Press, 2006
Lanadis T, Cummings JL, Benson F, Palmer EP, Loss of topographic familiarity: An
environmental agnosia. Arch Neurol 43; 132, 1986.
Landgren S, The response of thalamic and cortical neurons to electrical and physiological
stimulation of the cat’s tongue. In: WA Rosenblith (ed.), Sensory Communication.
Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, Cambrige, 437-453, 1961.
Lassek AM, The human pyramidal tract. II. A numerical investigation of the Bets cells of the
motor area. Arch Neurol and Psychiat 44; 718-724, 1940.
Lassek AM, The pyramidal tract. The effect of pre- and postcentral cortical lesions on the fiber
components of the pyramids in monkey. J Nerv and Ment Dis 95; 721-729, 1942 a.
Lassek AM, The human pyramidal tract. IV. A Study of the mature, myelinated fibers of the
pyramid. J Comp Neurol 76; 217-225, 1942.
Lassek AM, The pyramidal tract: Basic considerations of corticospinal neurons. A Res Nerv and
Ment Dis Proc 27; 106-128, 1947.
Lassek AM, The Pyramidal Tract: Its Status in Medicine. Charles C Thomas, Publisher,
Springfield, III, 1-66, 1954.
Lassek AM, Rasmussen GL, The human pyramidal tract. A fiber and numerical analysis. Arch
Neurol and Psychiat 42; 872-876,1939.
Lassek AM, Rasmussen GL, A comparative fiber and numerical analysis of the pyramidal tract.
J Comp Neurol 72; 417-428, 1940.
Lassek AM, And Evand JP, The human pyramidal tract. XII. The effect of hemispherectomies on
the fiber components of the pyramids. J Comp Neurol 83; 113-119, 1945.
Lawrence DG, Kuypers HGJM, The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey.
The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain 91; 1-14, 1968.
Lee HW et al., Mapping of functional organization in human visual cortex. Electrical cortical
stimulation. Neurology 54; 849-854, 2000.
Lee S, Yoon Bo-Eun, Berglund K et al., Channel-mediated tonic GABA release from glia. Science
330; 790-796, 2010.
Leichnetz GR et al., The prefrontal corticotectal projection in the monkey. An anterograde and
retrograde horseradish peroxidase study. Neuroscience 6; 1023-1041, 1981.
Leigh RJ, Zee DS, The neurology of Eye Movements. Ed 2. Philadelphia: FA Davis, 1991.
Leigh RJ, Human vestibular cortex. Ann Neurol 35; 383-384, 1994.
Leigh RJ, Kennard C, Using saccades as a research tool in the clinical neurosciences. Brain 127;
460-477, 2004.
Leigh RJ, Zee DS, The neurology of eye movements. In: Contemporary Neurology Series. 4th ed.
Vol. 70. Oxford University Press, New York, 2006.
Lekwuwa GU, Barnes GR, Cerebral control of eye movements: I. The relationship between
cerebral lesion sites and smooth pursuit deficits. Brain 119; 473-490, 1996.
1735
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Lemmen LJ, Davis, and Radnor LL, Observation on stimulation of the human frontal eye field.
J Comp Neurol 112; 163-168, 1959.
Lennie P, Receptive fields. Curr Biol 13; 216-219, 2003.
Levin PM, Bradford FK, The exact origin of the corticospinal tract in the monkey. J Comp Neurol
68; 411-422, 1939.
Lezak MD, Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd Ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, The behavioral geography of the brain. In: MD Lezak,
DB Howieson, DW Loring (eds) Neuropsychological Assessment. Fourth Edition. Oxford
University Press, New York, 39-85, 2004.
Li H, et al. Functional differentiation of a clone resembling embriogenic cortical interneuron
progenitors. Neurobiol. 68; 1549-1564, 2008.
Liégeois-Chauvel C et al., Localization of the primary auditory area in man. Brain 114; 139-153,
1994.
Liégeois-Chauvel C, Peretz I, Babai M et al., Contribution of different cortical areas in the
temporal lobes to music processing. Brain 121; 1853-1867, 1998.
Livingstone MS, Hubel DH, Specificity of intrinsic connections in primate primary visual cortex.
J Neurosci 4; 2830-2835, 1984.
Lloyd D, Virtual lesions and the not-so-modular brain. J. Intern. Neuropsychol. Society 6; 627-
635, 2000.
Lobel E, Kahane P, Leonards U et al., Localization of human frontal eye fields: anatomical and
functional findings of functional magnetic resonance imaging and intracerebral electrical
stimulation. J Neurosurg 95; 804-815, 2001.
Lordat J, Analyse de la Parole pour Servir à la Théorie de Divers Cas d’Alalie et de Paralalie.
Paris: JB Baillière. 1843.
Lorente de Nó, The structure of the cerebral cortex. In: JF Dulton (ed.), Physiology of the Nervous
System. Ed. 3. Oxford University Press, New York, 288-330, 1949.
Lüders H, et al., The second sensory area in humans: Evoked potential and electrical stimulation
studies. Ann Neurol 17; 177-184, 1985.
Lüders H, Lesser RP, Dinner DS et al., Localization of cortical function: new information from
extraoperative monitoring of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia 29 (Suppl 2); 56-65, 1988.
Luna B, Thulborn KR, Strojawas MH, et al., Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided
saccades in humans: a fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 8; 40-47, 1998.
Luria AR, Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Book, 1966.
Luria AR, Working Brain. Harmondsworth, England, Penguin, 1973.
Lynch JC, The cerebral cortex. In: DE Haines (ed.), Fundamental Neuroscience for Basic and
Clinical Applications. Churchill Livingstone Elsevier, Philadelphia, 512-526, 2006.
Lynch JC, Tian JR, Corticocortical networks and corticosubcortical loops for the higher control
of eye movement. In: Buttner-Ennever JH (ed.): Neuroanatomy of the Oculomotor System.
Amsterdam, Elsevier, Vol. 151, 467-508, 2005.
Lynch JC, Columnar organization of the cerebral cortex (cortical columns). In: Neuroscience Year
(Supplement to Encyclopedia of Neuroscience). Boston, Birkhauser, 37-40, 1989.
Marcé LV, Sur quelques observations de physiologie pathologique tendant à démontrer l’existence
d’un principe coordinateur de l’écriture. Mémoires de la Société de Biologie 3; 93-115, 1856.
Marie P, Révision de la question de l’aphasie. Semaine Médicale 26; 241-247, 1906.
1736
21. Cerebral cortex
Mariss O, Rubenstein JL, Cell migration in the forebrain. Annu Rev Neurosci 26; 441-483, 2003.
Markowitsch H, et al., Cortical afferents to the prefrontal cortex of the cat: A Study with the
horseradish peroxidase technique. Neurosci Lett 11; 115-120, 1979.
Markram H, et al. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci 5; 793-
807, 2004.
Matelli M, Luppino G, Rizzolatti F, Patterns of cytochrome oxidase activity in the frontal agranular
cortex of the macaque monkey. Behavioural Brain Research 18; 125-136, 1985.
Mc Carley RW, Sleep, dreams and states of consciousness. In: PM Conn (ed), Neuroscience in
Medicine. Philadelphia: JB., Lippincott, 535-554, 1995.
Mc Cormick DA, Neurotransmitter actions in the thalamus and cerebral cortex. J. Clin.
Neurophysiol 9; 212-223, 1992.
Meadows JC, Disturbed perception of colors associated with localized cerebral lesions. Brain: A
Journal of Neurology 97; 615-632, 1974.
Mesulam MM, Behavioral neuroanatomy. In: MM Mesulam (ed), Principles of behavioral and
cognitive neurology (2nd ed). New York, Oxford University Press, 2000.
Merzenich MM, Brugge JF, Representation of the cochlear partition on the superior temporal
plane of the Macaque monkey. Brain Res. 50; 275-296, 1973.
Merzenich MM, Knight PL, Roth G, Cochleotopic organization of primary auditory cortex in the
cat. Brain Res 63; 343-346, 1973.
Mesulam M, Mufson EJ, Insula of old world monkey. 1. Architectonics in the insula-orbito-
temporal components of the paralimbic brain. J Comp Neurol 212; 1-22, 1982 a.
Mesulam M, Mufson EJ, Insula of the old world monkey. III. Efferent cortical output and
comments on function. J Comp Neurol 212; 38-52, 1982 b.
Mesulam M, Mufson EJ, The insula of Reil in man and monkey. Architectonics, connectivity and
function. In: A Peters and EG Jones (eds.), Cortex, Vol 4, Association and Auditory Cortices.
Plenum Press, New York, London, 179-226, 1985.
Mesulam MM, The cholinergic innervations of the human cerebral cortex. Prig Brain Res 145;
67-78, 2004.
Mettler FA, Connections of the auditory cortex of the cat. J Comp Neurol 55; 139-183, 1932.
Mettler FA, On the origin of the fibers in the pyramid of the primate brain. Proc Soc Exper Biol
and Med 57; 111-113, 1944 a.
Meyer DR, Woolsey CN, Effects of localized cortical destruction upon auditory discriminative
conditioning in the cat. J Neurophysiol 15; 149-162, 1952.
Milea D, Lobel E, Lehéricy S et al., Intraoperative frontal eye field stimulation elicits ocular
deviation and saccade suppression. NeuroReport 13; 1359-1364, 2002.
Milner B, Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Arch Neural 9; 900-1000, 1963.
Minkowski M, Étude sur les connections anatomiques des circonvolutions rolandiques, pariétales
et frontales. Schweiz Arch Neurol in Psychiat 12; 71-104 and 227-268; 14; 255-278; 15; 97-
132. 1923-1924.
Miyoshi G, Fishell G, GABAergic interneuron lineage selectively sort into specific cortical layers
during early postnatal development. Cereb Cortex 21; 845-852, 2011.
Miyoshi G, Butt SJ, Takebayashi N, Fishell G, Physiologically distinct temporal cohorts of cortical
interneurons arise from telencephalic Olig 2 expressing precursors. J. Neurosci 27; 7786-
7798, 2007.
1737
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Miyoshi et al., Genetic fate mapping reveals that the caudal ganglionic eminence produces a large
and diverse population of superficial cortical interneurons. J Neurosci 30; 1582-1594, 2010.
2010,
Molnar-Szakacs I, Iacoboni M, Koski L, Mazziotta JC, Functional segregation within pars
opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus: Evidence from fMRI studies of imitation and action
observation. Cerebral cortex 1; 986-994, 2005.
Moore CJ, Stern CE, Corkin S et al., Segregation of somatosensory activation in the human
Rolandic cortex using fMRI. J Neurophysiol 84; 558-569, 2000.
Morecraft RJ, Yeterian E, Prefrontal cortex. Enciclopedia of Human brain 4; 11-26, 2000.
Morrow MJ, Sharpe JA, Cerebral hemispheric localization of smooth pursuit asymmetry.
Neurology 40; 284-292, 1990.
Mosimann UP, Felblinger J, Bollinari P et al., Visual exploration behavior during clock reading
in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 127; 431-438, 2004.
Mosimann UP, Muri RM, Burn DJ et al., Saccadic eye movement changes in Parkinson’s disease
dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain 128; 1267-1267, 2005.
Mostofsky SH, Lasker AG, Gutting LE et al., Oculomotor abnormalities in attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder: A preliminary study. Neurology 57; 423-450, 2001.
Mountcastle VB, Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat’s somatic sensory
cortex. J Neurophysiol 20; 408-434, 1957.
Mountcastle VB, The cortical organization of the neocortex. Brain 120; 701-722, 1997.
Mountcastle VB, Lynch JC, Georgopoulous A, et al., Posterior parietal association cortex of the
monkey. Command function from operations within extrapersonal space. J. Neurophys. 38;
871-908, 1975.
Mufson EJ, Mesulam M, Insula of the old world monkey. II. Afferent cortical input and comments
on the claustrum. J Comp Neurol 212; 23-37, 1982.
Mufson EJ, Mesulam M, Thalamic connections of the insula in the rhesus monkey and connects on
the paralimbic connectivity of the medial pulvinar nucleus. J Comp Neurol 227; 109-120, 1984.
Mufson EJ, Sobreviela T, Kordower JH, Chemical neuroanatomy of the primate insular cortex:
relationship to cytoarchitectonics, connectivity, function and neurodegeneration. In: Björklund
T Hökfelt (eds.), Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy. Vol 13: The Primate Nervous
System, Part I, 377-454. Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1997.
Munari C, Kahane P, Tassi L et al., Intracerebral low frequency electrical stimulation: a new tool
for the definition of the “epileptogenic area”? Acta Neurochir Suppl 58; 181-185, 1993.
Munari C, Hoffman D, Francione S et al., Stereo-electroencephalography methodology:
advantages and limits. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl 152; 56-69, 1994.
Munoz DP, Le Vasseur AL, Flanagan JR, Control of volitional and reflexive saccades in Tourette’s
syndrome. Progress in Brain Research 140; 467-481, 2002.
Mungus D, Interictal behavior abnormality in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of General
Psychiatry 39; 108-111, 1982.
Müri R, Iba- Zizen MT, Derosier C et al., Location of the human posterior eye field with functional
magnetic resonance imaging. J Comp Neurosurg Psychiatry 60; 445-448, 1996.
Nauta WJH, Whitlock DG, An anatomical analysis of the non-specific thalamic projection system.
In: JF Delafresnaye (ed), Brain Mechanism and Consciousness (Symposium). Blackwell
Scientific Publications, Oxford, 81-98, 1954.
Neal JW, Pearson RCA, Powell TPS, The ipsilateral cortico-cortical connections of area 7b, PF,
in the parietal and temporal lobes of the monkey. Brain Res 524; 119-132, 1990 b.
1738
21. Cerebral cortex
Neff WD, Neural mechanisms of auditory discrimination. In: WA Rosenblith (ed), Sensory
Communications, Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press and John Wiley and Sons,
New York, 1961.
Neff WD, Diamond IT, The neural basis of auditory discrimination. In: HF Harlow and CN
Woolsey (eds), Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior. University of Wisconsin Press,
Madison, 101-126, 1958.
Neff WD, Fisher JF, Diamond IT, Yela M, Role of auditory cortex in discrimination requiring
localization of sound in space. J Neurophysiol 19; 500-512, 1956.
Nieman DH, Bour LJ, Linszen DH et al., Neuropsychological and clinical correlates of antisaccade
task performance in schizophrenia. Neurology 54; 866-871, 2000.
Noctor SC, Flint AC, Weissman TA, et al., Neurons derived from radial units in neocortex. Nature
409; 714-720, 2001.
O’Rourke NA, Sullivan DP, Kaznowski CE et al., Tangential migration of neurons in the
developing cerebral cortex. Development 121; 2165-2176, 1995.
O’Sullivan EP, Jenkins IH, Henderson L et al., The functional anatomy of remembered saccades:
a PET study. Neuroreport 6; 2141-2144, 1995.
Oppenheimer SM, Cechetto DF, The cardiac chronotopic organization of the rat insular cortex.
Brain Res 533; 66-72, 1990.
Oppenheimer SM, Gelb A, Girvin JP, Hachinski VC, Cardiovascular effects of human insular
cortex stimulation. Neurology 42; 1727-1732, 1992.
Osborne J, On the loss of the faculty of speech depending on forget fulness of the art of using the
vocal organs, Dublin J Med Chem Sci 4; 157-170, 1833.
Pacia SV, Devinsky O, Perrine K et al., Clinical features of neocortical epilepsy. Annals of
Neurology 40; 724-730, 1996.
Pandya DN, Yeterian EH, Architecture and connections of cortical association areas. In: A Peters
and EG Jones (eds). Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 4, New York: Plenum Press, 1985.
Pandya DN, Yeterian EH, Architecture and connections of cerebral cortex: Implications for brain
evolution and function. In: AB Scheibel and AF Wechsler (eds), Neurobiology of higher
cognitive function. New York: Guilford Press, 1990.
Pandya DN, Yeterian EH, Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. Philos Trans R
Soc Lond. B Biol Sci 351; 1423-1432, 1996.
Pandya DN, and Yeterian EH, Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. In: AC
Roberts, TW Robbins and L Weiskrantz (eds), The prefrontal cortex. Executive and cognitive
functions. New York Oxford University Press, 1998.
Parsons LM, Sergent J, Hodges DA, Fox PT, The brain basis of piano performance.
Neuropsychologia 43; 199-215, 2005.
Passingham RE, Brain size and inteligence in man. Brain Behavior and Evolution 16, 253-270,
1979.
Passingham RE, The Frontal Lobes and voluntary Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1993.
Passingham R, Functional organization of the motor system. In: RSJ Franckowiak et al., (eds)
Human brain function. San Diego, Academic Press, 1997.
Patton HD, Ruch TC, The relation of the foot of the pre- and postcentral gyrus to taste in the
monkey and chimpanzee. Fed Proc 5; 79, 1946.
Patton HD, Ruch TC, and Walker AE, Experimental hypogeusia from Horsley-Clarke lesions of
the thalamus in Macaca mulatta. J Neurophysiol 7; 171-184, 1944.
Paulescu E, Franckowiak RSJ, and Bottini G, Maps of somatosensory system. In: RSJ
Franckowiak et al., (eds) Human brain function. San Diego, Academic Press, 1997.
1739
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Paus T, Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review. Neuropsychologia
34; 475-483, 1996.
Paxinos G (ed.), The Human Nervous System. San Diego, Academic Press, 439-755, 1990.
Peters A, Jones EG, Cerebral cortex. New York, Plenum Press, Vol. 1-14, 1984-1999.
Peele TL, Cytoarchitecture of individual parietal areas in monkey (Macaca mulatta) and
distribution of the efferent fibers. J Comp Neurol 77; 693-737, 1942.
Penfield W, Evans J, Functional defects produced by cerebral lobectomies. A Res Nerv and Ment
Dis Proc 13; 352-377, 1934.
Penfield W, Boldrey E, Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man
as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain 60; 389-443, 1937.
Penfield W, Rasmussen T, The Cerebral Cortex of Man. A clinical Study of Localization of
Function. Macmillan Company. New York, 248, 1950.
Penfield W, Welch K, The supplementary motor area of the cerebral cortex. A clinical and
experimental study. A.M.A. Arch. Neurol and Psychiat 66; 289-317, 1951.
Penfield W, Jasper HH, Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain. Little Brown
and Company, Boston, 896, 1954.
Penfield W, Faulk ME, The insula: further observations on its function. Brain 78; 445-470, 1955.
Penfield W, Mathieson G, Memory: autopsy findings and comments on the role of the
hippocampus in experimental recall. Arch Neurol 31; 145-154, 1974.
Penfield W, Boldrey E, Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex as studied
by electrical stimulation. Brain 60; 389-443, 1958.
Peters A, and Jones EG, Cerebral Cortex, Vol. 1-14. New York, Plenum Press, 1984-1999.
Petit L, Orssaud C, Tzourio N et al., PET study of voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans:
basal ganglia - thalamocortical system and cingulate cortex involvemenet. J Neurophysiol
69; 1009-1017, 1993.
Petit L, Clark VP, Ingeholm J et al., Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in
human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI imaging. J Neurophysiol 77; 3386-3390, 1997.
Pettigrew JD, Olson and Hirsch HUB, Cortical effects of selective visual experience: Degeneration
or reorganization? Brain Res 51; 354-351, 1973.
Pickles JO, An introduction to the Physiology of Hearing, 2nd ed. London. Academic Press, 1988.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Gaymard B, Eye movement disorders and ocular motor organization. Curr
Opin Neurol Neurosurg 3; 796-801, 1990.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C et al., Cortical control of saccades. And Neurol 37; 557-567, 1995.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Rivaud-Pechoux S et al., Cortical control of spatial memory in
humans: The visuooculomotor model. Annals of Neurology 52; 10-19, 2002.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Muri RM, Ploner CJ et al., Decisional role of the dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex in ocular motor behavior. Brain 126; 1460-1473, 2003.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Milea D, Muri RM, Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Current
Opinion in Neurology 17; 17-25, 2004.
Platt ML, and Glimcher PW, Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature
400; 233-238, 1999.
Polyakov GJ, Modern data on the structural organization of the cerebral cortex. In: AP Luria (ed),
Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books, 1966.
Posner MI, Raiche ME, Images of Mind. New York: Scientific Library, 1994.
Powell TPS, Mountcastle VB, Some aspects of the functional organization of the cortex of the
postcentral gyrus of the monkey: A correlation of findings obtained in a single unit analysis
with cytoarchitecture. Bull. Johns Hopkins Hosp 105; 133-162, 1959 a.
1740
21. Cerebral cortex
Pribram KH, Rosner BS, Rosenblith WA, Electrical response to acoustic clicks in monkey: Extent
of neocortex activated. J Neurophysiol 17; 336-344, 1954.
Price J, Thurlow L, Cell lineage in the rat cerebral cortex: a study using retroviral-mediated gene
transfer. Development 104, 473-482, 1988.
Pritchard TC, Hamilton RB, Morse JB, Norgren R, Projections of the thalamic gustatory and
lingual areas in the monkey. Macaca fascicularis. J Comp Neurol 244; 213-228, 1986.
Provencio I, and 5 others, A novel human opsin in the inner retina. J. Neurosci 20; 600-605, 2000.
Pryse-Phillips W, Companion to Clinical Neurology. 2nd ed., Oxford, Oxford University Press,
2003.
Purves D, Riddle DR, La Mantia AS, Iterated patterns of brain circuitry (or how the brain gets its
spots). Trends in Neuroscience 15; 362-368, 1992.
Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, et al., (eds), The Association Cortices. In: D. Neuroscience
Third Edition. Sinauer Associates Inc., Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts USA, 613-
636, 2004.
Rakic P, Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241; 170-176, 1988.
Raymer AM, Moberg P, Crosson B et al., Lexical-remantic deficits in two patients with dominant
thalamic infarction. Neuropsychologia 35; 211-219, 1997 a.
Raymer AM, Foundas A, Maher L et al., Cognitive neuropsychological analysis and neuro-
anatomic correlates in a case of acute anomia. Brain and Language 58; 137-156, 1997 b.
Raymer AM, Naming and word retrieval problems. In: LL La Pointe (ed.), Aphasia and related
neurogenic language disorders. 3rd ed., New York; Thieme, 72-86, 2005.
Raymer AM, Rothi LJ, Cognitive neuropsychological approaches to assessment and treatment:
Impairments of lexical comprehension and production. In: R Chapey (ed). Language
intervention strategies in adult aphasia. 5th ed., Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins,
607-631, 2008.
Reid CB, Liang I, Walsh C, Sistemic widespread clonal organization in cerebral cortex. Neuron
15; 299-310, 1995.
Reil JC, Das Hirnschenkel-System oder die Hirnschenkel-Organisation im grossen Gehirn. Arch
Physiol Halle 9; 147-171, 1809 a.
Reil JC, Die sylvische Grüße Arch Physiol Hall 9; 195-208, 1809 b.
Richter CP, Hines M, Experimental production of the grasp reflex in adult monkeys by lesions of
the frontal lobe. Am J Physiol 101; 87-88, 1932.
Riesenhuber M, Poggio T, Models of object recognition. Nature Neuroscience 3 (Suppl.) 1199-
1204, 2000.
Rizzolatti G, and Arbib M, Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 2; 188-194, 1998.
Robert TS and Akert K, Insular and opercular cortex and its thalamic projection in Macaca mulatta.
Schweiz Arch Neurol Neurochir Psychiat 92; 1-43, 1963.
Robertson IH, Marshall JC (eds), Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. Have,
UK: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1993.
Roland Ep, Zilles K, Functions and structures of the motor cortices in humans. Curr Opin
Neurobiol 6; 773-781, 1996.
Rolls ET, Information processing in the taste system of primates. J EXP Biol 146; 141-164, 1989 a.
Rolls et. al., The orbitofrontal cortex: In AC Roberts et al., The prefrontal cortex: Executive and
cognitive functions. Oxford University Press, 1998.
Romansky KV et al., Corticosubthalamic projection in the cat: An electron microscopic study.
Brain Res 163; 319-322, 1979.
1741
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Rosano C, Krisky CM, Velling JS et al., Pursuit and saccadic eye movement subregions in human
frontal eye field: A High-Resolution fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex 12; 107-115, 2002.
Rose M, Cytoarchitektonic und Myeloarchitektonik der Grosshirnrinde. In: O Bunke and O
Foerster (eds.), Handbuch der Neurologie. Vol. 1. Springer, Berlin, 588-778, 1935.
Rose JE, Woolsey CN, The relation of thalamic connections, cellular structure and evocable
electrical activity in the auditory region of the cat. J Comp. Neurol 91; 441-466, 1949.
Rose JE, Woolsey CN, Cortical connections and functional organization of the thalamic auditory
system of the cat. In: HF Harlow and CN Woolsey (eds), Biological and Biochemical Bases
of Behavior. University of Wisconsin Press, Madison, 127-150, 1958.
Rose JE, Greenwood DB, Goldberg JM, Hind JE, Some discharge characteristics of single neurons
in the inferior colliculus of the cat. I. Tonotopical organization, relation of spike-counts to
tone intensity, and firing patterns of single elements. J Neurophysiol 26; 294-320, 1963.
Rosenzweig MR, Cortical correlates of auditory localization and of related perceptual phenomena.
J Comp Physiol Psychol 47; 269-276, 1954.
Roth G, The evolution and ontogeny of consciousness. In: T Metzinger (ed), Neural correlates of
consciousness. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Russell JR, De Myer W, The quantitative cortical origin of pyramidal axons of Macaca rhesus.
Neurology 11; 96-108, 1961.
Saper CB, Convergence of anatomic and limbic connections in the insular cortex of the rat. J
Comp Neurol 210; 162-173 1982 a.
Saper CB, Reciprocal parabrachial-cortical connections in the rat. Brain Res 242; 33-40, 1982 b.
Scarff JE, Primary cortical centers for movement of upper and lower limbs in man. Arch Neurol
and Psychiat 44; 243-299, 1940.
Scarff JE, Unilateral prefrontal lobotomy for relief of intractable pain and termination of narcotic
addition. Surg Gynec and Obst 89; 385-392, 1949.
Schall JD, Morel A, King DJ, Bullier J, Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye
field in macaque: Convergence and segregation of processing streams. Journal of
Neuroscience 15; 4464-4487, 1995.
Schall JD, Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Monitoring and control of action by the frontal lobes. Neuron
36; 309-322, 2002.
Scheibel ME, Scheibel AB, Elementary processes in selected thalamic and cortical subsystems –
the structural substrates. In: The Neurosciences, Second Study Programs, Vol. 2. New York,
Rockefeller University Press, 443-457, 1970.
Schiltz C, Sorger B, Caldara R et al., Impaired face discrimination in acquired prosopagnosia is
associated with abnormal response to individual faces in the right middle fusiform gyrus.
Cereb Cortex 16; 574-586, 2006.
Schmahmann JD, Leifer D, Parietal pseudothalamic pain syndrome: Clinical features and anatomic
correlates. Arch Neurol 49; 1032-1037, 1992.
Schlag J, Schlag-Rey M, Evidence for a supplementary eye field. J Neurophysiol 57; 179-200,
1987.
Schmitt FO, Woden FG, Adelman G, Dennis SG, The Organization of the Cerebral Cortex. MIT
Press, Cambridge, 1981.
Schneider WX, Vam: A neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object
recognition and space-based motor action. Visual Cogn 2; 331, 1995.
Schneider KA, Richter MC, Kastner S, Retinotopic organization and functional subdivision of
the human lateral geniculate nucleus: a high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging
study. J Neurosci 24; 8975-8985, 2004.
1742
21. Cerebral cortex
Schüz A, Miller R, Cortical Areas: Unity and Diversity. Taylor and Francis, London and New
York, 2002.
Seleman LD, Goldman-Rakic PS, Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral
prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the Rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed
neural network subserving spatially guided behavior. J Neurosci 8; 4049-4068, 1988.
Serre T, Kreiman G, Kouch M et al., A quantitative theory of immediate visual recognition. In: P
Cisek, T Drew and FJ Kalaska (Eds.), Progress in brain research (Vol 164), Amsterdam:
Elsevier, 33-56, 2007.
Shafiq-Antonacci R, Maruff P, Masters C, Currie J, Spectrum of saccade system function in
Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology 60; 1272-1278, 2003.
Sharp J, et al., Disturbance of ocular motility. Continuum 1; 41-91, 1995.
Shaywitz SE, Dyslexia. NEJM 338; 307-312, 1998.
Shenkin HA, Lewey FH, Taste area preceding convulsions in a lesion of the parietal operculum.
J Nerv. And Ment Dis 100; 352-354, 1944.
Sherwood CC et al., Variability of Broca’s area homologue in African great apes: Implications
for language evolution. Anat Rec Part A 27; 276-285, 2003.
Shimazu H, Maier MA, Cerri G et al., Macaque ventral premotor cortex exerts powerful facilitation
of motor cortex outputs to upper limb motoneurons. Journal of Neuroscience 24; 1200-1211,
2004.
Shipley MT, Saunders MS, Special senses are really special: evidence for a reciprocal bilateral
pathway between insular cortex and nucleus parabrachialis. Brain Res Bull 12; 221-226, 1982.
Shmuelof L, Zachary E, A mirror representation of others’ actions in the human anterior parietal
cortex. J Neurosci 26; 9736-9742, 2006.
Sirigu A et al., The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science
273; 1564-1568, 1996.
Sirigu A, Daprati E, Ciancia S et al., Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the
parietal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 7; 80-84, 2004.
Smith WK, The frontal eye fields. In: PC Bucy (ed), The Precentral Motor Cortex. Urbana, IL:
University of Illinois Press, 308-342, 1944.
Smith EE, and Jonides J, Neuroimaging analysis of human working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci USA 95; 12061-12068, 1998.
Smutok MA, Grafman J, Salazar AM, et al., The effects of unilateral brain damage on contralateral
and ipsilateral upper extremity function in hemiplegia. Physical Therapy 69; 195-203, 1989.
Snyder M, Hall WC, Diamond IT, Vision in tree shrews after removal of striate cortex.
Psychoneurol Sc. 6; 243-244, 1966.
Sparks DL, The brain stem control of saccadic eye movements. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience
3; 952-964, 2002.
Spinelli DN, Hirsch HVB, Phelps RW, Metzler J, Visual experience as a determinant of response
characteristics of cortical receptive fields in cat. Exper Brain Res 15; 289-304, 1972.
Stefanacci L, Amaral DG, Topographic organization of cortical inputs to the lateral sulcus of the
macaque monkey amygdala: A retrograde tracing study. Journal of Comparative Neurology
421; 52-79, 2000.
1743
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Stefanacci L, Amaral DG, Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala:
An anterograde tracing study. Journal of Comparative Neurology 451; 301-323, 2002.
Steinmetz H, Structure, functional and cerebral asymmetry: in vivo morphometry of the planum
temporale. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 20; 587-591, 1996.
Sterling P, Retina. In GM Shepherd (ed), The synaptic organization of the brain (4th ed). New
York, Oxford University Press, 1998.
Stern P, Introduction. Glee for Glia. Science 330; 473, 2010.
Strominger NL, Subdivisions of auditory cortex and their role in localization of sound in space.
Exper Neurol 24; 348-362, 1969.
Stephan H, Allocortex. In: W Bergmann (ed.), Handbuch der mikroskopischen Anatomie des
Menschen. Vol. 4, Nervensystem, Part 9, Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York,
998 pp, 1975.
Stuphorn V, Schall JD, Neuronal control and monitoring of initiation of movements. Muscle and
Nerve 26; 326-339, 2002.
Sturb RL, Black FW, Neurobehavioral disorders. A clinical approach. Philadelphia: Davis, 1988.
Sudokov K, Mac Lean PD, Reeves A, Marino R, Unit study of exteroceptive input to
claustrocortex in awake, sitting, squirrel monkey. Brain Res 28; 19-34, 1971.
Sugar O, French JD, Chusid JG, Corticocortical connections of the superior surface of the temporal
operculum in the monkey (Macaca mulatta). J Neurophysiol 11; 175-184, 1948.
Sugar O, Amador LV, Griponissiotes B, Corticocortical connections of the walls of the superior
temporal sulcus in the monkey (Macaca mulatta). J Neuropath and Exper Neurol 9; 179-185,
1950.
Sutherling WW et al., Cortical sensory representation of the human hand: size of finger regions
and nonoverlapping digit somatotopy. Neurology 42; 1020-1028, 1992.
Sweeney JA, Mintun MA, Kwee S, et al., Positron emission tomography study of voluntary
saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. J Neurophysiol 75; 454-468, 1996.
Tanaka DH, et al. Random walk behavior of migrating cortical interneurons in the marginal zone:
time lapse analysis in flat-mount cortex. J. Neurosci 29; 1300-1311, 2009.
Tanji J, Shima K, Role of supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead.
Nature 371; 413-416, 1994.
Thaker GK, Ross DE, Cassady SL et al., Saccadic eye movement abnormalities in relatives of
patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 45; 253-264, 2000.
Thompson KG, Bichot NP, A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Progress in
Brain Research 147; 251-262, 2005.
Thompson AM, Lamy C, Functional maps of neocortical local circuitry. Front Neurosci 1; 19-
42, 2007.
Thompson JM, Woolsey CN, Talbot SA, Visual areas I and II of cerebral cortex of rabbit. J
Neurophysiol 13; 277-288, 1950.
Tijssen CC et al., Conjugate eye deviation: Side, site, and size of hemispheric lesion. Neurology
41; 846-850, 1991.
Toga AW, Thompson PM, Mori S et al., Toward multimodal atlases of the human brain. Nature
Rev Neurosci 7; 952-966, 2006.
Tonji J, Kurata K, Changing concepts of motor areas of the cerebral cortex Brain Dev 11; 374-
377, 1989.
Tower SS, Pyramidal lesion in the monkey. Brain 63; 36-90, 1940.
Tower SS, The pyramidal tract. In: PC Bucy (ed), The Precentral Motor Cortex, Ed. 2. University
of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ch. 6; 149-172, 1949.
1744
21. Cerebral cortex
Travis AM, Neurological deficiencies after ablation of the precentral motor area in Macaca
mulatta. Brain 78; 155-173, 1955.
Travis AM, Neurological deficiencies following supplementary motor area lesions in Macaca
mulatta. Brain 78; 174-198, 1955 a.
Trousseau A, De aphasie, maladie décrite récemment sous le nom impropre d’aphémie. Gazettes
des Hopitaux 37; 13-14, 25-26, 37-39, 48-50, 1964.
Truex RC, Carpenter MB, Human Neuroanatomy. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1969.
Ture U et al., Topographic anatomy of the insular region. J Neurosurg 90; 720-733, 1999.
Umiltà MA, Kohler E, Gallese V et al., “I know what you are doing”: A neurophysiological study.
Neuron 31; 155-165, 2001.
Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV, “What” and “where” in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4;
157-165, 1994.
Urasaki E et al., Cortical tongue area studied by chronically implanted subdural electrodes: With
special reference to parietal motor and frontal sensory responses. Brain 117; 117-132, 1994.
Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Structural and functional analyses of human cerebral cortex using a
surface-based atlas. Journal of Neuroscience 17; 7092-7102, 1997.
Varnas GG, Grand W, The insular cortex: Morphological and vascular anatomic characteristics.
Neurosurgery 44; 127-138, 1999.
Verrey D, Hémiachromatopsie Droite Absolue. Conservation Partielle De La Perception
Lumineuse Et Des Formes. Ancien Kyste Hémorrhagique De La Partie Inférieure Du Lobe
Occipital Gauche. Archives d’Ophtalmologie 8; 289-300, 1888.
Vicq d’Azyr F, Recherches sur la structure du cerveau, du cervelet, du la moelle elongeé, de la
moelle épinière; et sur l’origine des nerfs de l’homme et des animaux. Hist Acad Royale Sci:
495-622, 1781.
Vogt C, Vogt O, Allgemeine Ergebnisse Unserer Hirnforschung. Vierte Mitteilung: Die
physiologische Bedeutung der architektonischen Rindenreizungen. J Psychol. Neurol 25; 279-
462, 1919.
Von Economo CF, The cytoarchitectonics of the human Cerebral Cortex. Oxford Medical
Publications, London, 1986, 1929.
Walker AE, Fulton JF, Hemidecortication in chimpanzee, baboon, macaque, potto, cat and coati:
A study in encephalization. J Nerv and Ment Dis 87; 677-700 1938.
Walker AE, Weaver TA Jr, Ocular movements from the occipital lobe in the monkey. J
Neurophysiol 3; 353-357, 1940.
Walsh C, Cepko CL, Widespread clonal dispersion in proliferative layers of cerebral cortex. Nature
362; 632-635, 1993.
Wanadi J, Adami H, Sherr J et al., Naltrexone treatment of tardive dyskinesia in patients with
schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 24; 441-445, 2004.
Webster D, Fay RR, Popper AN, Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, Vol I. The Auditory
Pathway: Neuroanatomy. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1992.
Weiskrantz L, Blindsight. Oxford, UK, Clarendon Press, 1986.
Welch WK, Kennard MA, Relation of cerebral cortex to spasticity and flaccidity. J Neurophysiol
7; 255-268, 1944.
Welker WT, Benjamin RM, Miles RC, Woolsey CN, Motor effects of cortical stimulation in
squirrel monkey. (Saimiri sciureus). J Neurophysiol 20; 347-364, 1957.
Williamson PD, French JA, Thadani VM et al., Characteristics of medial temporal lobe epilepsy:
II. Interictal and ictal scalp electroencephalography, neuropsychological testing,
neuroimaging, surgical results, and pathology. Annals of Neurology 34; 781-787, 1993.
1745
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Willie JT and 13 others, Distinct narcolepsy syndromes in orexin receptor-2 and orexin null mice:
Molecular genetic dissection of non-REM and REM sleep regulatory processes. Neuron 38;
715-730; 2003.
Winner E, Brownell H, Happe F et al., Distinguishing lies from jokes: Theory of mind deficits
and discourse interpretation in right hemisphere brain-damage patients. Brain and Language
62; 89-106, 1998.
Wise SP, The primate premotor cortex: Past, present and preparatory. Ann Rev Neurosci 8; 1-19,
1985.
Wise RJ et al., Brain regions involved in articulation. Lancet 353; 1057-1061, 1999.
Whittfield IC, The Auditory Pathway. Williams and Wilkins Company. Baltimore, 1967.
Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI, An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science
269; 1880-1882, 1995.
Wolpert DM, Gharhamani Z, Competitional principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci
3; 1212-1217, 2000.
Wolpert DM, Gharhamani Z, Flanagan JR, Perspectives and problems in motor learning. Trends
Cogn Sci 1; 487-494, 2001.
Wonders CP, Anderson SA, The origin and specification of cortical interneurons. Nat Rev
Neurosci 7; 687-696, 2006.
Wonders CP, et al., A spatial bias for the origins of interneuron subgroups within the medial
ganglionic eminence. Dev. Biol 314; 127-136, 2008.
Woolsey CN, Walzl EM, Topical projection of nerve fibers from local regions of the cochlea to
the cerebral cortex of the cat. Bull. John Hopkins Hosp 71; 315-344, 1942.
Woolsey CN, Fairman D, Contralateral, epsilateral and bilateral representation of cutaneous
receptors in somatic area I and II of the cerebral cortex of pigs, sheep and other mammals.
Surgery 19; 684-702, 1946.
Woolsey CN, Settlage PH, Meyer DR et al., Patterns of localization in precentral and
“suplementary” motor areas and their relation to the concept of a premotor area. A Res Nerv
Ment Dis Proc 30; 238-264, 1951.
Woolsey CN, Organization of somatic sensory and motor area of the cerebral cortex. In: HF
Harlow and CN Woolsey (eds.) Biological and Biochemical Bases of Behavior. University
of Wisconsin, Madison, 63-81, 1958.
Woolsey CN, Erickson TC, Gilson WE, Localization in somatic sensory and motor areas of human
cerebral cortex as determined by direct recording of evoked potentials and electrical
stimulation. J Neurosurg 51; 476-506, 1979.
Xu Q, et al. Sonic hedgehog signaling confers ventral telencephalic progenitors with distinct
cortical interneuron fates. Neuron 65; 240-328, 2010.
Yu YC, Bultje RS, Wang X, Shi SH, Specific synapses develop preferentially among sister
excitatory neurons in neocortex. Nature 458, 501-504, 2009.
Zatorre RJ, Musical perception and cerebral functions: A critical review. Music Perception 2;
196-221, 1984.
Zatorre RJ, Neural specializations for tonal processing. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences 930; 193-210, 2001.
Zatorre RJ, Belin P, Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cerebral Cortex
11; 946-953, 2001.
Zatorre RJ, Belin P, Penhume V, Structure and function of the auditory cortex. Music and speech.
Trends in Cognitive Science 6; 37-46, 2002.
1746
21. Cerebral cortex
Zeki S, Watson JD, Lueck et al., A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human
visual cortex. J Neurosci 11; 641-649, 1991.
Zihl J, Von Cramon D, Mai N, Selective disturbance of moment vision after bilateral brain
damage. Brain 106; 313-340, 1983.
Zilles K, Werners R, Büsching U, Schleicher A, Ontogenesis of the laminar structure in areas 17
and 18 of the human visual cortex. A quantitative study. Anat Embrieol 174; 339-353, 1986.
Zilles KO, Is the length of the calcarine sulcus associated with the size of the human visual cortex?
A Morphometric study with magnetic resonance tomography. J Hirnforsch 36; 451-459, 1995.
Zilles K, Architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Regional and laminar organization. In:
Paxinos G, Mai JK (eds.), The Human Nervous System. 2nd ed, Chapter 27, 997-1055, Elsevier,
Academic Press, Amsterdam, 2004.
Zilles K, Palomero-Gallagher N, Cytomyelo-, and receptor architectonics of the human parietal
cortex. NeuroImage 14; 8-20, 2001.
Yang ZH, Zhao XQ, Wang CX et al., Neuroanatomic correlation of the post-stroke aphasias
studied with imaging. Neurological Research 30; 356-360, 2008.
Yamamoto J, Ikeda A, Satow T et al., Human eye fields in the frontal lobe as studied by epicortical
recording of movement-related cortical potentials. Brain 127; 873-887, 2004.
Yates BJ, Miller AD, Vestibular Autonomic Regulation. New York, CRC Press, 1996.
Yaxley S, Rolls ET, Sienkiewicz ZJ, Gustatory responses of single neurons in the insula of the
macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 63; 689-700, 1990.
Yoshimura Y, Callaway EM, Fine-scale specificity of cortical networks depends on inhibitory
cell type and conectivity. Nat Neurosci. 8; 1552-1559, 2005.
Yost WA, Fundamentals of Hearing: An introduction. San Diego, Academic Press, 1994.
1747
VENTRICULAR SYSTEM
AND MENINGES
Introduction
The cerebral ventricular system which consists of the four ventricles and the central
canal of the spinal cord are the remnants of the lumen of the embryonic neuronal tube.
The cells of the nervous system, both neurons and glia, originated from germinal
matrix adjacent to the lining of this tube. The cells multiply and migrate away from the
walls of the neural tube, forming the nuclei and cortex. As the nervous system develops,
the mass of tissue grows and the size of the tube diminishes, leaving various spaces in
different parts of the nervous system.
The parts of the tube that remain in the hemispheres are called the cerebral ventricles,
or the lateral ventricles. The ventricles are described in the order in which they are
numbered from rostral to caudal.
Thus, each cerebral hemisphere contains a large lateral ventricle that communicates
near its rostral end with the third ventricle via the interventricular foramen (foramen of
Monro) on either side of the median plane.
The third ventricle communicates with the forth ventricle by way of the aqueduct of
the midbrain, a wide tent-shaped cavity lying between the brain stem and cerebellum.
The fourth ventricle becomes continuous with the central canal of the medulla
oblongata and spinal cord and open by means of apertures into the subarachnoid space.
The ventricular system contains cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), which is mostly secreted
by the choroid plexuses located within the lateral, third and fourth ventricle. The total
volume of the fluid is about 100 to 140 ml and its pressure is about 150 mm of saline
(normal range: 70-180 mm) in the lateral recumbent position. The pressure is several
times higher in the lumbar region when sitting, but is about atmospheric at the foramen
magnum and is negative in the ventricles.
CSF flows from the lateral to the third ventricle, then through the cerebral aqueduct
into the fourth ventricle. It leaves the fourth ventricle through the foramen of Magendie
and the foramina of Luschka to reach the subarachnoid space surrounding the brain.
1748
22. Ventricular system and meninges
The neuroglial cells that line the ventricles of the brain and the central canal of the
spinal cord are termed ependymal cells. The ependyma is nonciliated in adult.
Lateral ventricle
The largest ventricles, the paired ventricles, separated from one another by the
septum pellucidum, are located in the right and left cerebral hemispheres. Thus, each
of the paired lateral ventricle is a C-shaped profile cavity that wraps around the thalamus
and is situated deep within the cerebrum. Thus, each lateral ventricle wraps around the
superior, inferior, and posterior surface of the thalamus. The superior surface of the
thalamus forms the floor of the body, the posterior surface of the thalamus forms the
anterior wall of the atrium, and the inferior surface of the thalamus forms the medial part
of the roof of the temporal horn.
The caudate nucleus is an arched, C-shaped cellular mass that wraps around the
thalamus and constitutes an important part of the wall of the lateral ventricle (Rhoton, 1987).
Each lateral ventricle possesses a body and three horns, the anterior, posterior, and
inferior horns. Thus, its various parts are the anterior horn, which lies deep to the frontal
lobes; the central portion or body, which lies deep to the parietal lobes; the atrium or
trigone where the ventricle widens and curves into the inferior horn, which goes into the
temporal lobes. In addition, there may be an extension into the occipital lobes, called the
occipital or posterior horn.
The frontal horn of the lateral ventricle has as its inferior boundary the rostrum, its
rostral boundary the genu, and its superior boundary the trunk, of the corpus callosum.
The anterior horns of the two ventricles are separated by the septum pellucidum.
Laterally the frontal horn of the lateral ventricle is limited by the bulging head of
the caudate nucleus. The anterior horn extends back as far as the interventricular foramen.
The narrow interventricular foramen is located immediately posterior to the column of
the fornix and separates the fornix from the anterior nucleus of the thalamus.
The body or the central portion of the lateral ventricle lies within the frontal and
parietal lobes and extends from the interventricular foramen to the splenium of the corpus
callosum. It is inferior to the trunk of the corpus callosum. The body is also on the medial
aspect of the thalamus (inferiorly) and body of the caudate nucleus (superiorly). The
bodies of the lateral ventricles are separated by the septum pellucidum, which contains
the columns of the fornices in its lower edge (Born et al., 2004). The inferior limit of the
body of the ventricle and its medial wall are formed by the body of the fornix. In the
central part of the lateral ventricle is the choroid plexus. The body of the lateral ventricle
widens posteriorly to become continuous with the posterior and inferior horn at the
collateral trigone or atrium (Rhoton, 2002; Born et al., 2004; Kawashima et al., 2006).
The posterior (occipital) horn of the lateral ventricle tapers into the occipital
lobe of each cerebral hemispheres. It is usually diamond-shaped or square in outline,
and the two sides are often asymmetrical. Fibres of the tapetum of the corpus callosum
separate the ventricle from the optic radiation and form the roof and lateral wall of the
posterior horn.
1749
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Forceps major pass medially as they sweep back into the occipital lobe, and produce
a rounded elevation in the upper medial wall of the posterior horn. Lower down, a second
elevation, the calcar avis, corresponds to the deeply infolded cortex of the anterior part
of the calcarine sulcus (Mc Lone, 2004; Crossman, 2008). The occipital horns are usually
asymmetrical – the back part of one horn may appear as a separate entity.
The inferior (temporal) horn of the lateral ventricle extends into the temporal lobe
from the fourth part of the central part of the lateral ventricle. This is the largest
compartment of the lateral ventricle. It curves round the posterior aspect of the thalamus
(pulvinar), passes downwards and posterolaterally and then curves anteriorly to end
within 2.5 cm of the temporal pole, near the uncus (Switka et al., 1999; Crossman, 2008).
It corresponds to the superior temporal sulcus. The floor of the ventricle consists of
the hippocampus medially and the collateral eminence, formed by the infolding of the
collateral sulcus, laterally.
Superiorly, the inferior horn is formed mainly by the tapetum of the corpus callosum,
the tail of the caudate nucleus, and the stria terminalis which runs forward to end in the
amygdaloid body (Switkaetal, 1999; Mc Lone, 2004).
The temporal extension of the choroid plexus fills this fissure and covers the outer
surface of the hippocampus.
The choroid plexus of each lateral ventricle is invaginated along a curve line known
as the choroid fissure.
The fissure extends from the interventricular foramen in front and arches over the
posterior end of the dorsal thalamus, as far as the end of the temporal horn. The choroid
plexus of the lateral ventricle is only in the central part of the lateral ventricle and the
temporal horn (Strazielle and Ghersi-Egea, 2000).
At the junction of the central part with the temporal horn it is best developed and is
there known as the glomus choroideum.
Calcified areas (corpora amylacea) are frequent in the glomus.
The vessels of the plexus originate from the internal carotid (anterior choroidal
artery) and from the posterior cerebral (posterior choroidal arteries).
At the interventricular foramina, the choroid plexuses of both lateral ventricles
become continuous and then join that of the third ventricle.
Third ventricle
The third ventricle is a narrow cleft unilocular, located in the center of head, below
the corpus callosum and the body of the lateral ventricle; above the sella turcica, pituitary
gland, and midbrain; and between the cerebral hemispheres, thalami, and the walls of the
hypothalamus (Rhoton, 1987). Over a variable area, the thalami are frequently adherent
to each other at the interthalamic adhesion (massa intermedia). This interthalamic
adhesion varies in size, and the thalamus and hypothalamus create minor differences in
the lateral walls of the third ventricle.
It communicates at its anterosuperior margin with each lateral ventricle through the
foramen of Monro and posteriorly with the forth ventricle through the aqueduct of
Sylvius. It has an anterior, a roof, a floor, a posterior, and two lateral walls.
1750
22. Ventricular system and meninges
Anterior wall
The anterior part of the third ventricle extends from the foramina of Monro, above,
to the optic chiasma below, which crosses its floor.
The part of the anterior wall visible on the surface is formed by the optic chiasma
and the lamina terminalis. The optic chiasma is a major modifier of the floor of the third
ventricle because its size and location affect the size and configuration of the optic and
infundibular recesses. The optic recess seems to be a residual space on the floor of the
third ventricle of which formation, shape, and size are the result of the varying indentation
of the ventricular floor by the chiasma (Yamamoto et al., 1981).
The lamina terminalis is a thin sheet of gray matter and pia mater that attaches to
the upper surface of the chiasma, to the anterior commissure and to the rostrum of the
corpus callosum.
The lamina terminalis forms the roof of the small virtual cavity lying immediately
below the ventricle called the cistern of the lamina terminalis.
Above this, the anterior wall is formed by the diverging columns of the fornices and
the anterior commissure.
When viewed from within, the boundaries of the anterior wall from superiorly to
inferiorly are formed by the columns of the fornix, foramina of Monro, anterior com-
missure, lamina terminalis, optic recess, and optic chiasma. The anterior commissure is
a compact bundle of myelinated nerve fibres that cross the midline in front of the columns
of the fornix. The diameter of the anterior commissure varies from 1.5 to 6.0 mm. The
distance from posterior end of the anterior commissure to the anterior border of the
foramen of Monro range from 1.0 to 3.5 mm (Yamamoto et al., 1981). The foramen of
Monro on each side is located at the junction of the roof and the anterior wall of the third
ventricle. The foramen is a ductlike canal that opens between the fornix and the thalamus
into the lateral ventricle and extends inferiorly below the fornix into the third ventricle
as a single channel. The foramen of Monro is bounded anteriorly by the junction of the
body and the columns of the fornix and posteriorly by the anterior pole of the thalamus
(Rhoton, 1987). The structures that pass through the foramen are the choroid plexus, the
distal branches of the medial posterior choroidal arteries, and the internal cerebral,
thalamostriate, superior choroidal, and septal vein.
Roof. A thin layer of ependyma covered by pia mater forms the thin roof of the third
ventricle. Above the roof is the body of the fornix. The body forms a gentle arch located
between the roof of the third ventricle and the floor of the body of each lateral ventricle.
The upper layer of the anterior part of the roof of the third ventricle is formed by the
body of the fornix, and the posterior part of the roof is formed by the crura and the
hippocampal commissure. The septum pellucidum is attached to the upper surface of the
body of the fornix.
According to Yamamoto et al. (1981), anteroposterior length of the septum
pellucidum varied from 28 to 50 mm. The tela choroidea forms two of the three layers in
the roof below the layer that is formed by the fornix.
1751
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The roof has four layers: one neural layer formed by the fornix, two thin
membranous layers of tela choroidea, and a layer of blood vessels between the sheets of
the tela choroidea (Rhoton, 1987; Rhoton, 2002).
The final layer in the roof is a vascular layer located between the two layers of tela
choroidea. The vascular layer consists of the medial posterior choroidal arteries and their
branches and the internal cerebral veins. The internal cerebral veins arise in the anterior
part of the velum interpositum, just behind the foramen of Monro, and they exit the velum
interpositum above the pineal body to enter the quadrigeminal cistern and join the great
vein (Rhoton, 1987).
The velum interpositum is the space between the two layers of the tela choroidea.
The cleft between the lateral edge of the fornix and the superomedial surface of the
thalamus is called the choroidal fissure. The choroid plexus of the lateral ventricle is
attached along this fissure.
Floor. The floor of the third ventricle extends from the optic chiasma anteriorly, to
the orifice of the aqueduct of Sylvius posteriorly.
When viewed from inferiorly, the structures forming the floor from anterior to
posterior include the optic chiasma, the infundibulum of the hypothalamus, the tuber
cinereum, the mammillary bodies, the posterior perforated substance, and (most
posteriorly) the part of the tegmentum of the midbrain located above the medial aspect
of the cerebral peduncle (Rhoton, 1987; Rhoton, 2002).
There is a small angular, optic recess at the base of the lamina terminalis, just dorsal
to and extending into the optic chiasma. Behind it, the anterior part of the floor of the
third ventricle is formed mainly by hypothalamic structures. Immediately behind the optic
chiasma lies a thin infundibular recess, which extends into the pituitary stalk. The
posterior part of the floor extends posterior and superior to the medial part of the cerebral
peduncles and superior to the tegmentum of the midbrain.
Posterior wall. The posterior wall of the third ventricle extends from the suprapineal
recess above, to the aqueduct of Sylvius below. Thus, the posterior boundary of the third
ventricle is marked by a suprapineal recess above the pineal gland, which extends into
the pineal stalk, by the habenular commissure, by the pineal gland, by the posterior
commissure, and by the aqueduct of Sylvius. The habenulae are small eminences on the
dorsomedial surfaces of the thalamus just in front of the pineal gland. The habenulae are
connected across the midline in the rostral half of the stalk of the pineal gland by the
habenular commissure (Rhoton, 2002).
The suprapineal recess projects posteriorly between the upper surface of the pineal
gland and the lower layer of tela choroidea in the roof.
The shape and size of the suprapineal recess are related to the size of the corpus
callosum. The increasing size of the splenium, as seen from the cat and monkey, is
associated with a concomitant reduction of the suprapineal recess. The habenular
commissure, which interconnects the habenulae, crosses the midline in the cranial lamina,
and the posterior commissure crosses in the caudal lamina.
Because of the development of the neocortex and the relative unimportance of
olfaction, the habenulae are the smallest.
1752
22. Ventricular system and meninges
The position of the anterior and posterior commissures are used as the basic reference
points for stereotaxic surgical procedures.
Lateral wall. The walls of the third ventricle are hidden between the cerebral
hemispheres. The upper part of the lateral wall is formed by the medial surface of the
anterior two-third of the thalamus, and the lower part is formed by the hypothalamus
anteriorly and the subthalamus posteriorly.
Hypothalamic sulcus between the interventricular foramen and the cerebral aqueduct
marks the boundary between the thalamus and hypothalamus. The superior limit of the
thalamic surfaces of the third ventricle is marked by narrow, raised ridges, known as the
striae medullaris thalami.
The massa intermedia projects into the upper one-half of the third ventricle and often
connects the opposing surfaces of the thalamus. It was present in 76% of the brains
examined and was located 2.5 to 6.0 mm posterior to the foramen of Monro (Yamamoto
et al., 1981; Rhoton 1987, 2002).
The columns of the fornix form distinct prominences in the lateral walls of the third
ventricle just below the foramen of Monro, but inferiorly they sink below the surface.
The third ventricle presents several recesses: (1) the optic recess superior to the
chiasma; (2) the infundibular recess in the infundibulum of the neurohypophysis; (3) a
less well-defined recess rostral to the mammillary bodies; (4) the pineal recess in the
stalk of the pineal body; (5) the suprapineal recess (Augustine, 2008).
Aqueduct of Sylvius
The aqueduct of Sylvius is a small tube in the midbrain, 1-2 mm in diameter and
about 1 cm in length. It extends throughout the dorsal quarter of the midbrain in the
midline, is widest in its central part, is surrounded by the gray matter and connects the
third and fourth ventricle. The superior and inferior colliculi are dorsal to aqueduct and
the midbrain tegmentum is ventral. Rostrally it commences immediately behind and
below the posterior commissure.
Caudally, it is continuous with the lumen of the fourth ventricle at the junction of
the midbrain and pons.
Fourth ventricle
The fourth ventricle is a romboid shaped space in the hindbrain who lies between the
brain stem and the cerebellum. Rostrally (superiorly) it is narrow becoming continuous
with the cerebral aqueduct (of Sylvius), and caudally (inferiorly), it narrows into the central
canal of the medulla oblongata (obex), which, in turn, is continuous with the central canal
of the spinal cord (Fig. 22.1). The ventricle is, at its widest, at the level of the
pontomedullary junction, where a lateral recess on both sides extends to the lateral border
of the brain stem. In the widest part of the ventricle, on either side, it has two openings, the
right and left foramina of Luschka and a single median foramen of Magendie.
1753
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 21.1. Hemisected skull demonstrating the flow of cerebrospinal fluid in the ventricle of the brain
and the subarachnoid spaces (after Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
All three foramina permits cerebrospinal fluid to leave the ventricular system and
enter the subarachnoid space. The foramina of Luscha lead to the interpeduncular cistern,
whereas the foramen of Magendie delivers the CSF into the cerebellomedullary cistern.
Floor of the fourth ventricle. The floor of the fourth ventricle is a romboid cavity
on the dorsal surfaces of the pons and the rostral half of the medulla. It consists largely
of grey matter and contains important cranial nerve nuclei.
The superior part of the ventricular floor is triangular in shape and is limited laterally
by the superior cerebellar peduncles as they converge toward the cerebral aqueduct.
The inferior part of the ventricular floor is also triangular in shape and is bounded
caudally by the gracile and cuneate tubercles, which contain the dorsal column nuclei,
and more rostrally, by the diverging inferior cerebellar peduncles (Rhoton, 2010;
Crossman, 2008).
A dorsal median sulcus divides the floor into right and left halves. Each half is it-
self divided, by an often indistinct sulcus limitans, into a medial region known as the
medial eminence and a lateral region known as the vestibular area. The vestibular nuclei
lie beneath the vestibular area.
1754
22. Ventricular system and meninges
The medial eminence, an elevation on the floor of the fourth ventricle, extends the
length of the pons and into the open medulla. In the caudal part of the pons, the medial
eminence presents a swelling called the facial colliculus, a small elevation produced by
an underlying loop of efferent fibres from the facial nucleus which covers the abducens
nucleus. Between the facial colliculus and the vestibular area the sulcus limitans widens
into a small depression, the superior fovea. In its upper part, a small region of bluish-
grey pigmentation denotes the presence of the subjacent locus ceruleus.
Inferior to the facial colliculus, at the level of the lateral recess of the ventricle is the
striae medullaris, which runs transversely across the ventricular floor (Rhoton, 2000).
The hypoglossal and vagal trigone are caudal to the facial colliculus and form the
caudal part of the medial eminence at medullary levels.
Thus, in the inferior area of the floor of the fourth ventricle, the medial eminence is
represented by the hypoglossal triangle (trigone), which lies over the hypoglossal nucleus.
Laterally, the sulcus limitans widens to produce an indistinct inferior fovea. Caudal to it,
between the hypoglossal triangle and the vestibular area, is the vagal triangle, which
covers the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus. The triangle is crossed below by a narrow
translucent ridge, the funiculus separans, which is separated from the gracile tubercle by
the small area postrema. They are both covered by ependyma, containing tanycytes and
neurons (only area postrema) (Rhoton, 2000; Crossman, 2008).
The lowermost part of the floor of the fourth ventricle has the shape of the point of
a pen (calamus scriptorius). Here are nuclei related to respiration, cardiovascular activity
and the act of swallowing (deglutition) (Matsushima et al., 1982; Augustine, 2008).
Roof of the fourth ventricle. Sheets of white matter (the superior and inferior
medullary vela) which are lined by ependyma and which stretch between both superior
and inferior cerebellar peduncles form the roof of the fourth ventricle. The superior
medullary velum is continuous with the cerebelar white matter and is covered dorsally
by the lingula of the superior vernis (Rhoton, 2000).
The inferior medullary velum formed by ventricular ependyma and the pia mater of
the tela choroidea, presents a deficiency, the median aperture (foramen Magendie), just
inferior to the nodule of the cerebellum.
CSF flows through this foramen into the cisterna magna (Matsushima et al., 1982;
Barshes et al., 2005). The ends of the lateral recesses have similar openings, the lateral
apertures. These, are not well-defined openings but rather gaps between the cerebellum
and medulla oblongata with choroid plexus protruding through them. By turning fila of
the glossopharyngeal and vagal nerves medially, one is able to see the choroid plexus
projecting through the lateral aperture immediately inferior to the flocculus (Barshes et
al., 2005; Augustine, 2008). The choroid plexus of the fourth ventricle invaginates into
the roof of the ventricle on either side of the median plane. A prolongation of each plexus
protrudes through the corresponding lateral aperture.
The vessels to the plexus arise from cerebellar branches of the vertebral and basilar
arteries. The choroid plexus of the fourth ventricle is T-shaped, with vertical and
horizontal limbs but the precise form varies widely. The vertical (longitudinal) limb is
double, flanks the midline and is adherent to the roof of the ventricle.
1755
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The horizontal limbs of the plexus project into the lateral recesses of the ventricle.
Small tufts of plexus pass through the foramina of Luschka and emerge, still covered by
the ependyma, in the subarachnoid space of the cerebellopontine angle (Rhoton, 2000;
Mc Lone, 2004; Crossman, 2008). The blood supply of the fourth ventricular choroid
plexus is from the inferior cerebellar arteries.
Capillaries drain into a rich venous plexus served by a single choroidal vein.
The vascular pia mater in the roofs of the third and fourth ventricles and in the medial
wall of the lateral ventricle along the line of the choroid fissure, is closely opposed to the
ependymal lining of the ventricles, without any intervening brain tissue.
It forms the tela choroidea, which gives rise to the highly vascularized choroid
plexuses from which CSF is secreted into the lateral, third and fourth ventricles.
The body or stroma of the choroid plexus consists of many capillaries, separated
from the subarachnoid space by the pia mater and choroid ependymal cells (Strazielle
and Ghersi-Egea, 2000; Crossman, 2008).
Many different routes are possible once the CSF has reached the cisterna magna.
The fluid may travel: superiorly toward the cerebellar hemispheres to the ambient cistern;
anterosuperiorly toward the interpenduncular and interchiasmatic cistern; anteriorly
toward the premedullary, prepontine, and cerebellopontine cistern; or inferiorly toward
the spinal subarachnoid space. CSF in the spinal subarachnoid space posterior to the
spinal cord and dentate ligaments is directed in the caudal direction. The overall direction
of fluid ventral to the spinal cord is in the cephalad direction; therefore, returning the
CSF to the basilar cisterns (Milhorat, 1975).
The movement of CSF in the subarachnoid space is complex and is characterized
by a fast flow component and a much slower bulk flow component. During systole, the
major arteries lying in the basal cisterns and other subarachnoid spaces dilate significantly
and exert pressure effects on the CSF, causing rapid CSF flow around the brain and out
of the cranial cavity into the upper cervical vertebral canal.
The flow of CSF is propagated by the cardiac cycle. The pulsation of the arterial
system transmits pulsation to the brain parenchyma, the choroid plexus, and the large
arteries at the skull base (Ohara et al., 1988). The volumetric displacement of the CSF
increases with low diastolic pressure and low systolic pressure (Barshes et al., 2005).
Perhaps the smallest contribution is from the outpouring of new CSF and the ciliary
beating of the ventricular ependyma. The pressure gradient across the arachnoid villi also
contributes to the bulk flow of CSF via the creation of a pressure gradient. The mean
CSF pressure in the brain is 150 mm saline while the pressure in the superior sagittal
sinus in 90 mm saline (Milhorat, 1975).
The amplitude of the CSF pulsations is also affected by the respiratory cycle, the
resistance of outflow created by the arachnoid villi, the mean intracranial pressure, and the
compliance of the cranial and spinal cord cavities (Fujii et al., 1980; Ohara et al., 1988).
Pulsations of 10-30 mm H2O and 20-30 mm H2O in amplitude are seen at particular
points in the respiratory and cardiac cycles, respectively, with isovolumetric measure-
ments in the lumbar CSF (Fujii et al., 1980).
The amplitude of pulsations in the cisterna magna is 50 mm H2O while that of the
lumbar fluid is 30 mm H2O) (Fishman, 1992).
According to Duboulay et al. (1972), an average of 0.1 mL CSF was displaced from
the third ventricle during each systole; in comparison to 1.0 mL in the basal cisterns and
0.64 mL in the cisterna magna.
Thus, radiographic studies in humans have shown that pulsations throughout the
neuraxis lead to the “pumping” of CSF and that various areas of the brain and spinal
cord provide varying contribution to the pumping activity (Ohara et al., 1988; Stoodley
et al., 1997).
CSF absorption. CSF is absorbed into the venous system by active diffusion into
cerebral capillaries which occurs as a result of the interstitial pressure differential.
Other routes of absorption exist, including the ependyma, the leptomeninges, and
the lymphatics of the spine (Milhorat, 1975). The driving forces for absorption of CSF
have been attributed to the gradient in both hydrostatic and colloid osmotic pressures
1757
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
between the protein-free CSF in the arachnoid villi and the venous spaces (Barshes et
al., 2005).
CSF function. The CSF does act to support as cushion elements in the central
nervous system. The CSF acts to lessen the apparent weight of the brain to approximately
4% of this mass (Burt, 1993).
The CSF appears to have a function at least partially analogous to that of lymphatics
in other organs – namely, removing fat-soluble and toxic substances from the brain’s
extracellular fluid. Many fat-insoluble molecules are also removed from the brain
extracellular fluid by the circulation of CSF including urea, albumin, homovanillic acid,
and norepinephrine (Milhorat, 1975).
The “internal milieu” of the brain may be regulated by exclusion of large and polar
molecules from the CSF and also by modification of the CSF by capillary-glial com-
plexes, epithelia, and neurons themselves (Fishman, 1992).
The CSF may also function as a mechanism of intracerebral transport for biogenic
amines which initiate the secretion of pituitary hormone release factors. Tanycytes appear
to have a role in this function (Milhorat, 1975).
Tanycites are found in clusters in the walls of the third ventricle and cerebral aqueduct,
in floor of the fourth ventricle, and in the cervical spinal canal. Clusters of tanycytes are
often associated with circumventricular organs, namely the median eminence, the area
postrema, the subcommissural organ, and the pineal gland (Peters et al., 1991).
In contrast to the ependymal cells, tanycytes have many microvilli and few cilia.
These cells have three portions: a somatic portion, a neck portion, and a tail portion with
end-feet which course through the hypothalamus to contact fenestrated blood vessels or
pial surfaces (Fishman, 1992).
Fixed macrophages are also present in the arachnoid border layer. These cells are
sometimes referred to as Kolmer or epiplexus cells when associated with the choroid
plexus. They contain many membrane-bound inclusions and variable vacuoles; they lack
cytoplasmic processes (Peters et al., 1991).
Composition of CSF. The fact that the CSF is isosmotic in comparison to the plasma
suggests that water freely equilibrates between the two fluid compartments (Fishman,
1992). The composition of CSF compared to that of plasma is presented in Table 22.1.
Thus, CSF is not simply a protein-free dialysate of the plasma but rather a true
secretion requiring energy for its production.
According to Barshes et al. (2005), the secretion of CSF is dependent upon the active
transport of sodium which is performed by a choroid epithelial sodium-potassium
activated ATPase. The in vivo inhibition of choroid plexus fluid formation by ouabain,
an inhibitor of this ATPase, support the idea that CSF is a secretion. This ATPase and
other transport enzymes are responsible for the transport of other ions and micronutrients
into the CSF. Small amounts of protein are transported into CSF mainly by pinocytosis
(Barshes et al., 2005).
1758
22. Ventricular system and meninges
Table 22.1
Normal Composition of Cerebrospinal Fluid and Serum
(adapted from Fishman, 1992)
CSF Serum
(arterial)
Meninges
Three connective membranes that envelop the central nervous system are collectively
known as the meninges. They provide support and protection for the delicate tissue they
surround. Although the dura mater surrounding the brain is continuous with the dura
mater surrounding the spinal cord at the level of the foramen magnum, it is customary to
discuss the two separately.
Thus, separating the brain and spinal cord from the calcified tissue are three more
or less concentric membranes. The outermost, dense, irregular collagenous connective
tissue is the dura mater or pachimeninx.
The innermost connective tissue membrane is the pia mater, the thin translucent
membrane, adherent to the surface of the brain and spinal cord, which accurately follows
every contour. The middle spiderweb-like is a delicate layer of reticular fibres, the
arachnoid.
The pia mater and arachnoid collectively are called the leptomeninges and are
separated from one another by the subarachnoid space.
1759
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Thus, the meningeal layer of the dura is reflected inwards to form three folds, namely
the falx cerebri, falx cerebelli, and tentorium cerebelli that partially divide the cranial
cavity into compartments.
Additionally, this meningeal layer forms a diaphragm over the hypophysial fossa,
known as the diaphragm sella and Meckel’s cave (cavum trigeminale).
Falx cerebri. The largest of these folds is the sickle-shaped fax cerebri which
extends in the midline from the crista galli to the internal occipital protuberance. Falx
cerebri occupies the longitudinal fissure between the two cerebral hemispheres. The
crescent is narrow in front, and broad behind where it is attached to the superiors surface
of the tentorium cerebelli. The superior, convex surface of the falx cerebri is attached to
the periosteal layer of the dura, leaving only a narrow, endothelial lined channel, the
superior sagittal sinus. The inferior border that is free but concave in shape follows the
shape of the corpus callosum.
The layers of the falx along the convex border divide to accommodate the superior
sagittal sinus whereas those along the concave border divide to accommodate the inferior
sagittal sinus.
The superior sagittal sinus begins just behind the crista galli, at the foramen cecum,
and terminates posteriorly at the confluence of sinuses. At the junction where the falx
cerebri joins and fuses with the tentorium cerebelli is another endothelial lined space, the
straight sinus, that receives blood from the inferior sagittal sinus and the great cerebral
vein. Blood from the straight sinus also enters the confluence of sinuses.
Tentorium cerebelli. The tentorium cerebelli, a horizontal reflection of the menin-
geal layer of the dura mater, is interposed between the cerebellum and the occipital lobes
of the cerebrum. It divides the cranial cavity into supratentorial and infratentorial
compartments, which contain the forebrain and hindbrain respectively. Anteriorly, the
lateral aspect of the tentorium is attached to the superior surface of the petrous portion of
the temporal bone and forms endothelially lined spaces, the right and left superior petrosal
sinuses, and continues anteriorly to attach to the posterior clinoid processes of the
sphenoid bone. The tentorium forms a large part of the floor of the middle cranial fossa,
and fills much of the gap between the ridges of the petrous temporal bones. On both sides,
the rim of the tentorial incisures is attached to the apex of the petrous temporal bone and
continues forward as a ridge of dura mater to attach to the anterior clinoid process.
This ridge marks the junction of the roof and the lateral part of the cavernous sinus.
The periphery of the tentorium cerebelli (attached to the superior border of the
petrous temporal bone), crosses under the free border of the tentorial incisures at the apex
of the petrous temporal bone, and continues forward to the posterior clinoid processes as
a rounded, indefinite ridge of the dura mater.
The angular depression between the anterior parts of the peripheral attachment of
the tentorium and the free border of the tentorial incisure is part of the roof of the
cavernous sinus (Standring, 2008).
The free and concave anterior edge of the tentorium forms the tentorial incisure,
the only opening between the supratentorial and infratentorial components. This tentorial
1761
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
incisure or match is filled by the midbrain and the anterior part of the superior aspect of
the cerebellar vermis and permits the ascent of the posterior cerebral arteries to reach the
cerebral hemisphere. The convex outer limit of the tentorium is attached posteriorly to
the lips of the transverse sulci of the occipital bone and to the posterior-inferior angles of
the parietal bones, where it encloses the transverse sinuses.
Laterally, it is attached to the superior borders of the petrous parts of the temporal
bones, where it contains the superior petrosal sinuses.
The lateral borders of the tentorium cerebelli extend much further anteriorly than
does its midline.
The superior surface of the central region is convex. The layers of the tentorium
along the convex border divide to accommodate the transverse sinus.
The compartment inferior to the tentorium contains not only the cerebellum but also
the pons and medulla oblongata.
Falx cerebelli. The falx cerebelli, a relatively small reflection of the meningeal layer
of the dura lies below the tentorium and attaches to the inferior aspect of the tentorium
in the median plane and to the internal occipital crest. This small midsagittal septum
partially separates the cerebellar hemispheres.
The posterior margin of the falx cerebelli contains the occipital sinus and is attached
to the internal occipital crest. The apex of the falx cerebelli frequently divides into two
small folds, which disappear at the sides of the foramen magnum.
Diaphragma sella. The diaphragma sella is a thin reflection of the meningeal layer
of the dura mater over the pituitary fossa, which is perforated by the infundibulum
(Rhoton, 2002).
This fourth dural projection is circular in shape, forming a roof over the sella turcica
and covering its content.
The lateral aspect is attached to the clinoid processes. The anterior and posterior
edges of the diaphragma sella house the anterior and posterior intercavernous sinuses.
The infundibulum and pituitary stalk pass into the pituitary fossa through a central
opening in the diaphragma.
Cavum trigeminale (Meckel’s cave). The cavum trigeminale is a narrow, slit-like
region interposed between the periosteal and meningeal layers of the dura mater
positioned on the trigeminal impression of the petrous portion of the temporal bone. It is
occupied by the trigeminal ganglion as well as by the sensory and motor roots of the
trigeminal nerve (Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
Vascular and nerve supply. The periosteal layer of the dura mater is richly supplied
by blood vessel, whereas the meningeal layer has no vascular supply. The blood vessels
of the periosteal layer include the middle meningeal and accessory meningeal arteries of
the middle cranial fossa, as well as the meningeal branches of the anterior and posterior
etmoidal arteries and meningeal branches of the internal carotid artery of the anterior
cranial fossa. Meningeal branches of the vertebral, occipital, middle meningeal, and
ascending pharyngeal arteries serve the periosteal dura of the posterior cranial fossa.
Blood is drained from the dura by meningeal veins that empty their blood into several of
the sinuses as well as into nearby emissary veins and diploic veins. The diploic veins are
1762
22. Ventricular system and meninges
large, thin-walled vessels that occupy chanels in the diploe of the cranial bones. Four
main trunks are usually described; these are the frontal, anterior or posterior temporal,
and occipital diploic veins.
Sinus pericranii is a rare condition involving congenital or acquired anomalous
connections between an extracranial blood-filled nodule and an intracranial dural venous
sinus via dilated diploic and / or emissary veins of the skull (Sheu et al., 2002).
Emissary veins traverse cranial apertures and make connections between intra-
cranial venous sinuses and extracranial veins. These connections are of clinical
significance in determing the spread of injection from extracranial foci to venous sinuses,
for example, the spread of infection from the mastoid to the venous sinuses or from the
paranasal sinuses to the cavernous sinus (Browder and Kaplan, 1976; Kaplan and
Browder, 1976; Ozveren et al., 2002).
The prefix lepto-, denoting “fine” or “thin” in Greek, contrast of the pachymeningeal
layer called the dura mater (Nolte, 1993).
Whereas the dura mater and pia mater have been described since the time of the
Egyptians some 3000 years ago, the arachnoid mater was not clearly distinguished as a
separate layer until the work of the Dutch anatomist Gerardus Blaes in 1666 (Bakay, 1991).
The term arachnoid was applied by the Dutch anatomist Frederick Ruysch (1638-
1731), the name roughly meaning “spider-like” and referring to the web-like structure of
these layers (Sanan, van Loveren, 1999).
Key and Retzius (1875) made a landmark contribution to the anatomy of these layers.
Cranial arachnoid
The arachnoid is a delicate nonvascular membrane between the dura and pia mater
which passes over the sulci without following their contours. Ultramicroscopic exami-
nation of the arachnoid has revealed two components making up this layer: an outer layer,
often referred to as the arachnoid barrier cell layer; and an inner layer, often referred
to as the arachnoid trabeculae.
The arachnoid barrier cell layer is a layer of two to three tiers of flattened cells. These
cells have a large, oval- to spindle-shaped nucleus, multiple cytoplasmic processes, scant
mitochondria, small rough endoplasmic reticulum and a poorly developed Golgi apparatus
(Nabeshima et al., 1975; Oba and Nakanishi, 1984; Haines and Frederickson, 1991).
A basement membrane underlies the arachnoid barrier cell layer and separated this
layer from the underlying subarachnoid space (Haines and Frederikson, 1991). Numerous
zonulae occludens (tight junctions), zonulae adherens and macula adherens (desmosomes)
are found interconnecting cells of this layers. These connections function as the meningeal
barrier, which excludes proteins and other large molecules from diffusing from the blood
to the CSF in the subarachnoid space (Nabeshima et al., 1975; Haines and Frederikson,
1991, Peters et al., 1991).
Thus, the intravascular introduction of dyes will stain the dura but not the underlying
meningeal layers, the CSF or the brain parenchyma (Nabeshima et al., 1975).
Occasional intercellular connections (viz. desmosomes) also exist between the cell
of the arachnoid barrier cell layer in the cranium and the overlying dura.
From the arachnoid surface trabeculae, known as arachnoid trabeculae, extends
toward and attach to the external surface of the pia mater.
Trabeculae, traverse the subarachnoid space from the deep layer of the arachnoid
mater to the pia mater, as thin, web-like chordae. Each trabecula has a core of collagen
which is coated by leptomeningeal cells. In the meshes of the arachnoid trabeculae
(subarachnoid space), cerebrospinal fluid circulates. The cells of the trabecular layer have
smaller nuclei, abundant mitochondria, and well-developed Golgi apparatuses and rough
endoplasmatic reticulum (Oba and Nakanishi, 1984). Extracellular collagen fibrils are
found outside of the cells in this layer (Nabeshima et al., 1975). Gap junctions often
connect cells within the arachnoid trabecular layer. The extensive gap junction allow the
arachnoid cells to function together to allow the passage of small molecules from cell to
cell (Peters et al., 1991).
1764
22. Ventricular system and meninges
The trabeculae that cross the subarachnoid space may form compartments,
particularly in the perivascular region, which may facilitate directional flow of CSF
throughout the subarachnoid space.
The subarachnoid space therefore contains CSF, the larger arteries and veins which
traverse the surface of the brain. These arteries and veins in the subarachnoid space are
coated by a thin layer of leptomeninges, often one cell thick.
Cranial and spinal nerves that traverse the subarachnoid space, to pass out of cranial
or intervertebral foramina, are coated by a thin layer of leptomeninges which fuses with
the arachnoid at the exit foramina. Blood vessels from the vascular meninges perforate the
arachnoid to reach the pia mater. However, each vessel is completely surrounded by
arachnoid fibroblasts and, therefore, the vessels never actually enter the subarachnoid space.
Scanning electron microscopy, however, has revealed that the pia actually surrounds
the vessels as it travels through the subarachnoid space but does not accompany the vessel
as it descends into the brain parenchima. Instead, the pia surrounding the vessel spreads
out over the pia which is covering the surface of the brain, effectively occluding the
perivascular space from the subarachnoid space (Hutchings and Weller, 1985). Thus, the
Virchow-Robin space communicates with the brain extracellular space rather than the
subarachnoid space.
The subarachnoid space who lies between the arachnoid and pia mater is connected
with the fourth ventricle of the brain by the median aperture (foramen of Magendie), and
by the paired lateral apertures (foramina of Luschka). Foramen of Magendie provides
communication with the cisterna magna, and foramina of Luschka open into the
subarachnoid space at the cerebellopontine angle, behind the upper roots of the
glossopharyngeal nerves.
Structurally and functionally, arachnoid barrier layer prevents cerebrospinal fluid in
the subarachnoid space from reaching the dura.
Arachnoid cisternae
In certain areas of the brain, the arachnoid completely diverges from the pia mater,
forming expanded subarachnoid spaces known as subarachnoid cistern. These more
expansive spaces identified as subarachnoid cisterns, are continuous with each other
through the general subarachnoid space.
As CSF percolates through the subarachnoid spaces it also enters the subarachnoid
cisterns, filling them.
The largest cistern, the cisterna magna is formed between the caudal aspect of the
cerebellum and the dorsal surface of the medulla oblongata, as the arachnoid stretches
across these two structures rather than following the contour of the brain. The cistern is
continuous above with the lumen of the fourth ventricle through its median aperture, the
foramen of Magendie, and below with the subarachnoid space of the spinal cord.
The pontine cistern is a much smaller space than the cisterna magna. It is located
along the ventral surface of the pons and communicates with the subarachnoid space of
the spinal cord caudally, and rostral to the pons, with the interpeduncular cistern.
1765
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The basilar artery runs through the pontine cistern into the interpeduncular cistern.
The interpeduncular cistern is located between the right and left cerebral peduncles
and receives CSF by way of the two lateral foramina of Luschka, from fourth ventricle.
Anteriorly, the interpeduncular cistern extends to the chiasmatic cistern, frequently, the
two are considered to be a single cistern, the cisterna basalis, even though the optic
chiasma is interjected between them.
The cistern of the lateral fossa is formed by the arachnoid as it bridges the lateral
sulcus between the frontal, parietal, and temporal opercula. It contains the middle cerebral
artery.
The cisterna ambiens (superior cistern) or the cistern of the great cerebral vein is
located in the vicinity of the superior aspect of the cerebellum, the corpora quadrigemina,
and the pineal body. This superior cistern lies posterior to the brain stem and third
ventricle, and occupies the interval between the splenium of the corpus callosum and the
superior cerebellar surface.
The great cerebral vein traverse the superior cistern, and the pineal gland protrudes
into it.
Several smaller cisterns have been described, including the prechiasmatic and
postchiasmatic cistern, which are related to the optic chiasma, and the cistern of the
lamina terminalis and the supracallosal cistern, all of which are extension of the
interpeduncular cistern and contain the anterior cerebral arteries.
The subarachnoid space also extends through the optic foramen into orbit where it
is bounded by the optic nerve sheath. The latter is formed by fusion of the arachnoid and
dura mater, and surrounds the orbital position of each optic nerve as far as the back of
the globe, where the dura fuses with the sclera of the eye.
For the carotid bifurcation aneurysms, the entire carotid-ophthalmic and carotid
cisterns must be opened, as well as the cisterns surrounding the proximal A1 and M1
arteries.
when they are known as Pacchionian bodies. The name Pacchionian granulations has
been used to refer to large, elaborate arachnoid granulations in horses and in man
(Wolpow and Schaumburg, 1972).
These outpushing of the arachnoid mater and subarachnoid space through the wall
of dural venous sinuses, very often are located close to points where veins enter the sinus.
Thus, most of the arachnoid granulations are associated with lacunae lateralis,
diverticula of the superior sagittal sinus, although some just into the lumen of the sinus.
At the base of each arachnoid granulation a thin neck of arachnoid mater projects
through an aperture in the dural lining of the venous sinus and expands to form a core of
collagenous trabeculae and interwoven channels.
Fig. 22.2. An arachnoid granulation (Modified). Morphology of CSF drainage pathways in man
(Raimond, ed, Kida and Weller, 1994).
1767
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Fig. 22.3. Coronal section through the vertex of the skull to show the relationships between the superior
sagittal sinus, meninges and arachnoid granulations (adapted from Drake, Vogl and Mitchell, 2005).
1769
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Studies of Kida and Weller (1994), Greitz (2004) and Zakharov et al. (2004), have
shown that CSF may also be resorbed throughout the ventricles and subarachnoid space,
through the walls of pial and subarachnoid vessels. Moreover, there is a growing body
of experimental evidence that significant volumes of CSF may be absorbed into
extracranial lymphatics in animals. It has been suggested that extracranial lymphatics
play a major role in CSF reabsorption at relatively low pressures, but that other routes,
including arachnoid granulations, become increasingly important as CSF pressure rises.
A connection between parenchymal interstitial fluid and extracranial lymphatics in
humans has yet to be convincingly demonstrated (Crossman, 2008).
Therefore, the function of the arachnoid granulations is transporting CSF manu-
factured by the choroid plexuses of the ventricles of the brain into the vascular system.
Cerebrospinal fluid acts as a fluid buffer for the protection of the central nervous
system. Cerebrospinal fluid also compensate for changes in blood volume in the cranium,
allowing the cranial contents to remain at a constant volume.
No one element of the cranial contents (brain, blood or cerebrospinal fluid) can
increase, except at the expense of the other (Monroe-Kellie doctrine).
It is important to note that although the arachnoid granulations protrude into the
venous sinuses, they are always separated from the blood by the endothelial lining of the
dural sinus / lacuna lateralis (Kida and Weller, 1994) (Fig. 22.4).
Pia mater
This vascular membrane is composed of: (1) an inner membranous layer, the intima
pia (Key and Retzius, 1875), and (2) a more superficial epipial layer. The intima pia,
adherent to underlying nervous tissue, follows its contours closely and is composed of
fine reticular and elastic fibres. Where blood vessels enter and leave the central nervous
system, the intima pia is invaginated forming a perivascular space.
The intima pia, like the arachnoid, is avascular and derives its nutrients by diffusion
from the cerebrospinal fluid and the underlying nervous tissue (Millen and Woollam,
1961, 1962). The epipial layer is formed by a meshwork of collagenous fibre bundles
continuous the arachnoid trabeculae. Cerebral vessels lie on the surface of the intima pia
within the subarachnoid space.
However, the cranial pia mater is the most delicate layer of the cranial meninges,
which closely invests the surface of the brain, from which it is separated by a microscopic
subpial space. It follows the contours of the brain into concavities and the depths of
fissures and sulci. The cranial pia also lines the basal cistern.
The cranial pia mater is composed of a single layer (or, occasionally, two layers) of
attenuated fibroblasts that form a transparent epithelioid membrane that closely invests
the contours of the brain (Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
Since the pia mater is vascular, it has numerous blood vessels associated with it;
however, because this is so, thin vessels are in part surrounded by cells derived from the
arachnoid trabecular and, in part, by cells of the pia mater. Deep to the epithelioid sheath
is a thin, discontinuous layer of collagen and elastic fibres. The end-feet of the astrocytes
1770
22. Ventricular system and meninges
form a protective layer that underlies this subpial extracellular matrix, separating it from
the neural tissue (Kida and Weller, 1994; Patestas and Gartner, 2008). The cell of the pia
mater are modified fibroblasts similar to the cells of arachnoid membrane. Their
morphology is often undistinguishable from that of the arachnoid cells (Cloyd and Low,
1974; Peters et al., 1991).
According to Kida and Weller (1994) and Crossman (2008), pia mater is formed
from a layer of leptomeningeal cells, often only 1 to 2 cells thick, in which the cells are
joined by desmosomes and gap junctions but few, if any, tight junctions. The cells are
continuous with the coating of the subarachnoid trabeculae and separated from the basal
lamina of the glia limitans by bundles of collagen and fibroblasts-like cells, and the
arteries and vein that lie in the subpial space (Fig. 22.5).
The intimal layer of pia is an avascular layer. In contrast to the overlying epipial
layer, it is adherent to the brain throughout all its contours. It has been proposed that the
vascular epipial layer represents the contribution of mesenchyme to the pia, while the
avascular intimal layer represents the contribution of the neural crest (Millen and
Woollam, 1961).
Fig. 22.5 The relationships between the dura, the leptomeninges and the blood vessels that enter and leave
the cerebral cortex. The subarachnoid space is divided by trabeculae (after Crossman, 2008).
1771
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The cranial arachnoid trabeculae join the pia to the arachnoid mater. Although very
thin and apparently fragile, cranial pia nonetheless exhibits a high level of stiffness, thus
influencing the mechanical properties of the brain. The cerebral vessels that are in the
subarachnoid space have a superficial covering of the cranial pia. Smaller vessels ramify
on the pia before penetrating the brain (Barshes et al., 2005). Thus, pia mater is reflected
from the surface of the brain onto the surface of the blood vessels in the subarachnoid
space, which means that the subarachnoid space is separated by a layer of pia from the
subpial and perivascular space of the brain (Nicholas and Weller, 1988).
Interrelationships of the pia mater and the perivascular (Virchow-Robin) spaces in
the human cerebrum was studied by Zhang et al. (1990).
In addition to separating the subarachnoid space from the brain tissue, the pia mater
also serves to degrade neurotransmitter substances and to prevent material in the
subarachnoid space from entering the nervous tissue, as evidenced by the inability of
erythrocytes to cross the pia mater in case of subarachnoid hemorrhage (Barshes et al.,
2005; Patestas and Gartner, 2008).
Thus, despite its delicate nature, the pia mater appears to form a regulatory interface
between the subarachnoid space and the brain. Pial cells contain enzymes such as catechol
-O- methyltransferaze and glutamine synthetaze (which degrade neurotransmitters), they
also exhibit pinocytotic activity and ingest particles up to 1 in diameter.
A subpial space of variable thickness exists between the pia and the basement
membrane of the glial limitans (outer glial layer of the brain and spinal cord). This space
contains collagen fibrils (Cloyd and Low, 1974). Pial cells are often joined to arachnoid
trabecular cells with desmosomes (Haines and Frederikson, 1991).
1772
22. Ventricular system and meninges
REFERENCES
Kida S, Weller RO, Morphology of CSF drainage pathways in man. In: A Raimondi (ed),
Principles of Pediatric Neurosurgery. Vol 4. Berlin: Springer, 1994.
Matsushima T, Rhoton AL Jr, Lenkey C, Microsurgery of the fourth ventricle: Part 1.
Microsurgical anatomy. Neurosurgery 11; 631-667, 1982.
Mc Lone DG, The anatomy of the ventricular system. Neurosurg Clin N Am 15; 33-38, 2004.
Millen JW, Woollam DHM, Observations on the nature of the pia mater. Brain 84; 514-520, 1961.
Millen J, Woollam DHM, The Anatomy of the Cerebrospinal Fluid. Oxford University Press,
New York, 90-102, 1962.
Milhorat TH, The third circulation revisited. J Neurosurg 42; 628-645, 1975.
Nabeshima S, Reese TS, Landis DM, Brightman MW, Junctions in the meninges and marginal
glia. J Comp Neur 164; 127-134, 1975.
Nicholas DS, Weller RO, The fine anatomy of the human spinal meninges. J Neurosurg 69; 276-
282, 1988.
Nolte J, The Human Brain: An introduction to its functional anatomy. St. Louis: Mosby Year
Book 33-47, 1993.
Oba Y, Nakanishi J, Ultrastructure of the mouse leptomeninx. J Comp Neur 225; 448-457, 1984.
Ohara S, Nagain H, Matsumoto T, Banno T, MR imaging of the CSF pulsatory flow and its
relation to intracranial pressure. J Neurosurg 69; 675-682, 1988.
Ozveren MF, Uchida K, Aiso S, Kawase T, Meningovenous structures of the petroclival region:
clinical importance for surgery and intravascular surgery. Neurosurgery 50; 829-836, 2002.
Pacchioni A, Dissertatio epistolaris de glandulis conglobatis durae meningis humanae, indeque ortis
lymphaticis ad piam meningem productis Romae: typis Io. Francisci Buagni, Dissertatio 1705.
Patestas MA, Gartner LP, A text book of Neuroanatomy. Blackwell Publishing USA, 84-98, 2008.
Peters A, Palay SL, Webster H, The Fine Structure of the Nervous System. New York: Oxford
University Press, 1991.
Rhoton AL Jr, The sellar region. Neurosurgery 51 (Suppl 1); 335-374, 2002.
Rhoton AL Jr, Microsurgical anatomy of the third ventricular region. In: MLJ Apuzzo (ed),
Surgery of the Third Ventricle. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore. London. Los Angeles.
Sydney, 92-166, 1987.
Rhoton AL Jr, Cerebellum and fourth ventricle. Neurosurgery 17 (Suppl 3); 7-27, 2000.
Rhoton AL Jr, The foramen magnum. Neurosurgery 47 (Suppl 3); 155-193, 2000.
Rhoton AL Jr, The lateral and third ventricles. Neurosurgery 51 (Suppl 1); 207-271, 2002.
Sanan A, van Loveren HR, The arachnoid and the myth of Arachne. Neurosurgery 45; 152, 1999.
Sheu M, Fauteux G, Chang H et al., Sinus pericranii: dermatologic considerations and literature
review. J Am Acad Dermatol 46; 934-941, 2002.
Standring S, Head and Neck. In S Standring (ed.), Gray’s Anatomy. Fortieth Edition, Section 4.
London UK, Churchill Livingstone Elsevier, 423-434, 2008.
Stoodley MA, Brown SA, Brown CJ, Jones NR, Arterial pulsation-dependent perivascular cere-
brospinal fluid flow into the central canal in sheep spinal cord. J Neurosurg 86; 689-693, 1997.
Strazielle N, Gherzi-Egea JF, Choroid plexus in the central nervous system: biology and
physiopathology. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 59; 561-574, 2000.
1774
22. Ventricular system and meninges
Switka A, Narkiewicz O, Dziewiatkowski J, Morys J, The shape of the inferior horn of the lateral
ventricle in relation to collateral and occipitotemporal sulci. Folia Morphol (Warsz) 58; 69-
80, 1999.
Vesalius A, De humani corporis fabrica libri septem. Basel: Johannes Oporinus, 1543.
Wolpow ER, Schaumburg HH, Structure of the human arachnoid granulation. J Neurosurg 37;
724-727, 1972.
Yamamoto I, Rhoton AL Jr, Peace DA, Microsurgery of the third ventricle: Part I. Microsurgical
anatomy. Neurosurgery 8, 334-356, 1981.
Yamashima T, Functional ultrastructure of cerebrospinal fluid drainage channel in human
arachnoid villi. Neurosurgery 22; 633-641, 1988.
Zakharov A, Papaiconomou C, Koh L, et al., Integrating the roles of extracranial lymphatics and
intracranial veins in cerebrospinal fluid absorption in sheep. Microvasc Res 67; 96-104, 2004.
Zhang ET, Inman CBE, Weller RO, Interrelationships of the pia mater and the perivascular
(Virchow-Robin) spaces in human cerebrum. J Anat 170; 111-123, 1990.
1775
CEREBRAL ASYMMETRY
in nonhumans
Introduction
For many years it has been generally believed that lateralization of cerebral function
was uniquely human characteristic and in fact arose on response to specifically human
selective processes (Levy, 1969).
However, evidence of lateralization of functions has been documented in the brain
of several animal species (Le May, 1977; Webster and Thurber, 1978; Nottebohm, 1981;
Glick et al., 1982), and some studies (Denenberg, 1981; Glick et al., 1982) have brought
into question the view (Warren, 1977) that the human pattern of cerebral laterality is
“species unique”.
In 1885, Ernest Mach postulated a mechanism of spatial behavior that applied to
both man and beast. He recounted: “The idea that the distinction between right and left
depends upon an asymmetry, and possibly in the last resort upon a chemical difference,
is one which has been present to me from my earliest years”. It was not until about nine
decades later that the implications of Mach’s idea were investigated. In 1873, Nothnagel
found that injections of chromic acid into the striatum caused a bending of the trunk to
the side of lesion (Wilson, 1914).
Ferrier (1876) stated that “irritation of the corpus striatum causes general muscular
contraction on the opposite side of the body. The head and body are strongly flexed to
the opposite side, so that the head and tail become approximated.
In 1921, Lashley described a rotation syndrome after combined unilateral destruction
of caudate nucleus and the motor cortex above it.
“Circus” movements after “parietal and unsymmetrical injuries to the striatum” in
rats were reported by Herrick in 1926.
Thus, when subjected to repeated testing with the same dose of a given drug, some
animals rotate consistently to the left whereas others rotate to the right. It was further
shown that nonlesioned, untreated rats also rotate at night (the more active half of their
circadian cycle) and that this rotation is in the same direction as that induced by
amphetamine (Glick and Cox, 1978).
1776
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Mach (1885) noted that “human being and animals that have lost their direction
move, almost without exception, nearly in circle”.
Behavioral and biological laterality is also ubiquitous in many nonhuman species,
with many instances of asymmetry being at least analogous to asymmetries found in
humans. At least some may also be homologous, in the sense of sharing common
structures and developmental origins.
Motor asymmetries have been discovered for a number of species, with individuals
sometimes showing very strong left-right preference. Furthermore, preference seems to
depend on variables such as age and sex and on specific task demands. Despite these
caveats, in several species of primates there tends to be left-hand preference for reaching
and maintaining postural control but a right hand preference for manipulation and other
skilled activities (Hellige, 2002).
According to Corballis (1997), in freely moving animals, there is strong evolutionary
pressure for bilateral symmetry, in the placement of sense organs and limbs and those
parts of the brain that are associated to them. This is so because such animals must be
equally attentive to both sides of space and be able to move in a straight line.
For other systems, asymmetry may be favored in such cases as a way of packaging
organs more efficiently into the body cavity or of packaging cognitive functions more
efficiently into a brain whose size is limited.
Once an organism’s brain becomes functionally asymmetric, additional asymmetries
are likely to arise via the same kind of snow ball mechanism (Christman, 1997; Banich
and Heller, 1998).
That is, some of the additional evolutionary adaptations are favored by the
environment, with the result that those adaptations also become asymmetric.
This would occur regardless of whether the environment favored hemispheric
asymmetry for a new adaptation. As this process is repeated, the extent of functional
brain asymmetry increases (Hellige, 1993, 2002).
One of the most fundamental divisions of the human brain is that of the left and right
cerebral hemispheres.
Numerous studies have revealed the consistent presence of both behavioral and
anatomical asymmetries that reflect the specialized capacities of each hemisphere
(Gazzaniga, 1970; Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985; Annett, 1985; Galaburda, 1991;
Bogen, 1993). Apparently, the left and right hemisphere appear to be anatomically and
biologically identical, leading one to wonder why there are so many functional
asymmetries.
However, postmortem studies of anatomy and structural imaging studies of the living
brain have documented a number of consistent physical asymmetries.
The ability to measure structure within the normal, living brain has made it possible
to search for relationships between structural asymmetry and a variety of other
nonmotoric behavioral and perceptual asymmetries, but no clear-cut picture has emerged.
Perhaps those structural characteristics revealed by contemporary imaging
techniques are still relatively gross.
1777
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
years ago (mya). This left-hand advantage extends to some species of Old World
monkeys. In addition, some species of Old World monkeys show right hemisphere
dominance for processing monkey faces and left hemisphere dominance for processing
communicatively relevant vocalizations (Corballis, 1997; Christman, 1997; Banich and
Heller, 1998; Hellige, 2002).
The similarity of these asymmetries to those of humans suggests that their precursors
were present in our last common ancestors, approximately 40 mya (Corballis, 1997).
Asymmetry in rats
The phenomenon of rotation in normal rats was one of the first indications that
normal animals may have an intrinsic asymmetry in nigrostriatal function that is
accentuated by some drugs. Several experiments have been concerned with the general
functional significance of nigrostriatal asymmetry revealed by the rotation studies. All
results suggested that rotation is a stereotyped form of spatial behavior and that spatial
tendencies are derived, at least in part, from a nigrostriatal asymmetry.
Comparison of rats having stronger or weaker directional biases showed that the
strength of the biases was related generally to overall learning ability, as well as
specifically to the ability to discriminate between left and right (Zimmerberg et al., 1978).
Rats lacking clear spatial biases were hyperactive, had difficulty in learning a variety of
tasks, and were unable to distinguish left from right.
Cerebral lateralization in the rat is clearly present at birth, changes during
development, and is sexually dimorphic.
However, it is apparent that left- and right-sided rats differ behaviorally as well as
neurochemically (Valdes et al., 1981). Behavioral and cerebral differences between left-
and right-handed human beings are well documented. Whereas right-handed individuals
almost always have left cerebral dominance, left-handed individuals have either right
cerebral dominance, left cerebral dominance, or ambivalent dominance (Kolb et al., 1982).
It was generally observed that approximately 50% of rats rotated to the right and
50% to the left.
Because left frontal cortex was usually more active than right frontal cortex, the
implication was that, in a large population, more rats should have right side preferences
than left side preferences and right preferences should be greater than left preferences
(Glick and Ross, 1981). These scientists reexamined the data accumulated for the past
several years and determined that, in a group of 602 rats, there was small (54.8%) but
significant (p < 0.025) right population bias; right-sided rats were also found to be both
more active and have greater side preferences than left-sided rats (Glick and Ross, 1981).
Research with rats and chicks has also demonstrated asymmetry for emotional
behaviors. For example, in both handled rats and chicks the right hemisphere tends to
produce emotional activity, whereas the left hemisphere tends to inhibit emotional
activity. In addition to providing interesting instances of laterality, effects such as these
also illustrate the importance of reciprocal activity between the left and right sides of the
brain (Rogers, 1997; Vallortigara, 2000).
1779
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Ross and Glick (1981) speculated that the increase in population bias in human being
as compared with rodents is perhaps related to the evolution and growth of the cortex in
relation to subcortical structures. The population bias would be expected to increase in
parallel with phylogeny if the left - right asymmetry in cortex increased as the cortex
became more convoluted in conjunction with an enhanced modulation of subcortical
structures (e.g., striatum).
Differences in the neurotransmitters found in each hemisphere have also been
associated with differences in hemisphere function (Glick et al., 1982; Direnfeld et al.,
1984; Robinson and Starkstein, 2002; Berridge et al., 2003) and sex (Arato et al., 1991).
It has been hypothesized that the distribution of two important neurotransmitters is
asymmetric in the human brain, with dopamine being more prevalent in the left
hemisphere and norepinephrine being more prevalent in the right hemisphere.
These differences may have an evolutionary foundation, for they have been found
in primates and other animals (Nottebohm, 1979; Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985;
Corballis, 1991).
The lateralized size differential in primates is paralleled in some species by left
lateralization for vocal communication (Mac Neilage, 1987).
Rossor et al. (1980) investigated directly the possibility of neurochemical symmetry
in postmortem human brain. They measured concentration of three to five neuro-
transmitters in nine structures, both left and right sides, of normal brain; their primary
goal was to learn whether left - right differences need consideration in comparisons of
abnormal and normal brain. Because only one substance (GABA-gamma-aminobutyric
acid) in one structure (substantia nigra) showed a left - right difference by a t test, it was
concluded that neurochemical laterality, although still possibly a substrate for functional
asymmetry, was unlikely to be an important consideration in such comparisons. After-
wards, data were eventually subjected to correlational analysis.
The results showed that human brain indeed has lateral asymmetries in several
structures and transmitter systems (Glick et al., 1982).
There were several similarities between the human data and those previously
reported in rats.
Thus, even though information on cerebral dominance was lacking in human beings
it was clearly demonstrated that levels of ChAT (choline acetyltransferaze) and DA
(dopamine) were higher in the left globus pallidus than in the right globus pallidus. Since
most of the patients (N = 14) should have been right-handed pallidal ChAT and DA levels
were obviously higher on the side contralateral to hand preference (Glick et al., 1982).
Similarly, in rats striatal DA levels were higher on the side contralateral to side
preferences (Zimmerberg et al., 1974).
The similarities between rat and human brains demonstrated that studies in the rat
may reveal mechanisms and functions of brain asymmetry that are relevant to human
beings.
In sum, studies in rats and mice focus on two rather different types of asymmetry.
1780
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
The first emphasizes postural and motor asymmetries such as paw preference and
direction of movement. Unlike handedness in humans, most postural asymmetries in
rodents are random across the population.
The second type of lateralization in rodents is more interesting in the current context
because it is seen in the population, much as we saw in birds.
As in humans, there is an asymmetry in the anatomy of the two cerebral hemispheres,
with the right hemisphere being larger. In addition, the cortex of the right hemisphere is
thicker, especially in the visual and posterior parietal regions, and this difference is
modulated by hormones.
Male rodents have a greater difference than females.
Both prenatal stress and postnatal castration were shown to abolish the anatomical
asymmetry in males, presumably owing to alterations in the normal hormonal
environment (Stewart and Kolb, 1988, 1994).
Evidence is accumulating to support the conclusion that the hemisphere is
specialized for the processing of species-specific calls.
The auditory asymmetry may represent a left-hemisphere advantage in the
processing of rapidly changing acoustic stimuli. Claims that the right hemisphere is
specialized for controlling emotional and spatial behavior remain speculative (Kolb and
Whishaw, 2003).
The results of studies on nonhuman primates and rodents show that the increased
aggression in males is probably a result of the male hormone testosterone both pre- and
postnatally. Castrating infant male rats or monkeys decreases aggression and treating
female with testosterone increases aggression (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). Generally,
men are physically more aggressive than women.
An intriguing asymmetry in rodents is an apparent lateralization of immune
responses. So, lesions in the left hemisphere of mice, but not in the right, suppress T-cell
function. The T-cell suppression may be due to lateralized control of the secretion of the
specific hormones, such as prolactin. Whether similar asymmetries exist in the control
of immune functions in humans is not yet known, although such asymmetry has been
hypothesized by Kang et al. (1991).
Stress may alter anatomical asymmetries in the rat cortex, and many researchers
have suggested that stress may also alter functional asymmetries.
Thus, Denenberg (1981) hypothesized that specific experiences might alter the two
hemispheres differentially during development. So, Denenberg’s results could be
explained by the lateralized responses to various stress-related hormones, such as
glucocorticoids.
Asymmetry in birds
In 1981, Nottebohm severed the hypoglossal nerve in canaries and found a severe
disruption in the bird’s song after left lesions but not after right ones. Subsequent work
showed anatomical differences in the structure controlling birdsong in the two avian
hemispheres and identified many song-related regions as sexually dimorphic.
1781
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Although a left hemisphere dominance for song has been shown in many species of
songbirds (and even in chickens), it is not characteristic of all songbirds. Apparently, the
zebra finch has little anatomical and functional asymmetry, even though it sings. It may
be that the lateralization is not for singing itself but for some other still-unrecognized
feature of bird vocalizations (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Nottebohm’s discovery led to interest in the possibility of asymmetry in the visual
system of birds because the optic nerve of the most birds cross over almost completely
at the optic chiasma. Thus, each hemisphere receives most input from a single eye.
Furthermore, birds have no corpus callosum and, although other small commissures
connect the hemisphere, there is less interhemispheric transfer in birds than in mammals.
According to Bradshaw and Rogers (1993), the right-eye system is specialized for
categorizing objects, such as food versus nonfood items, whereas the left-eye system is
specialized for responding to the unique properties of each stimulus (color, size, shape,
and so forth), including topographical information. Thus, the left hemisphere of birds
appears to be specialized for categorizing objects and the right for processing
topographical information.
The result of research by Horn (1990) showed an asymmetry for memory formation
in the chicken brain.
One curious asymmetry is in sleep. Birds spend much of their sleep time with only
one hemisphere asleep, which presumably allows them to monitor the environment. On
the other hand, it also means that there is a transient asymmetry in sensory processing,
which might have significant implications for the animals. Kolb and Whishaw (2003)
note parenthetically that unilateral sleep is also characteristic of cetaceans (whales,
dolphins) and seals, a sensible adaptation in mammals that can drown.
In sum, lateralization takes many forms in the brain and is not a unique property of
mammals.
forms of hemispheric asymmetry were already present in our ancestral line before the
first hominids emerged approximately 4 mya (Hellige, 2002).
Tool making and language are both forms of praxis and involve properties such as
recursion, embeddedness, and generativity. Both gestural communication and vocali-
zation also require sequences of precisely time movements. So, there may have been
pressure for the same brain hemisphere to become dominant for those aspects of
vocalization and language that are shared with tool making and gestural communication.
Thus, the increased development of language-related structures in the posterior part of
the brain is suggested to have occurred after the advent of hominids.
Studies in the 1977 by Warren unequivocally failed to find any systematic hand
preference in rhesus monkeys, and he concluded that observed preferences are task
dependent and are strongly affected by learning.
Afterwards, Mac Neilage et al. (1987) argued that, because earlier studies
concentrated on particular types of movements, a hand preference in monkeys has been
overlooked. Their basic premise is that primates evolved a preference for reaching with
one limb (the left) while supporting the body with the other (the right). As the prehensile
hand developed and primates began to adopt a more upright posture, the need for a hand
used primarily for postural support diminished and, because this hand was free, it became
specialized for manipulating objects. They later proposed that the hand specialization
was accompanied by hemispheric specializations: a right-hemisphere (left-hand)
perceptuomotor specialization for unimanual predation (grasping fast-moving insects or
small animals) and a left-hemisphere specialization for whole-body movements.
A significant difficulty of this hypothesis is that studies of limb use in primates are
hampered by poor control of myriad confounding factors including species, age, sex, task
difficulty, learning, and sample size (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). One objection to the
theory is that cerebral asymmetry must precede handedness, and whether this asymmetry
did indeed precede handedness and why it might have done so is not clear.
Hoster and Ettlinger (1985) trained 155 rhesus monkeys to make a tactile response
in a task in which the subjects had to discriminate a cylinder from a sphere. The results
showed that the 78 monkeys spontaneously using the left hand outperformed the 77 using
the right hand. Thus, as in the humans, the right hemisphere outperformed the left one,
suggesting an asymmetry.
Hamilton and Vermeire (1988) took a different approach. They taught 25 split-brain
monkeys to discriminate two types of visual stimuli that have shown lateralization in
humans. In one task, the animals had to discriminate between pairs of lines differing in
slope by 15º (15º versus 30º, for example, or 105º versus 120º). For each pair, the more
vertical line was designated as positive, and the monkey received a food reward for
choosing it.
Most monkeys learned the line-orientation discriminations faster with the left
hemisphere.
The second task required the animals to discriminate different monkey faces. The
right hemisphere of most animals showed better discrimination and memory of the faces.
1783
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Thus, approximately 66-71% of the population is right-eyed, and 58-60% are right-
eared.
Today, terms such as southpaw in baseball suggest an evolution of tolerance toward
left-handers and in professional sports, even admiration (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The most common cited statistics for left handedness in the general population is
10%, referring to the percentage of people who write with the left hand. But, when
broader criteria are used, estimates range from 10% to 30%. The problem is that
handedness is not absolute; some people are nearly totally left- or right-handed, whereas
others are ambidextrous (that is, they use either hand with equal facility) (Annett, 1970).
The evidence of left handedness on Annett’s task varies from a low of about 6% when
cutting with scissors to a high of about 17% when dealing cards.
Thus, tool use and language seem to have interacted in a synergistic fashion to
produce dramatic changes in human skull in the period from 10,000 to 30,000 years ago
(Hellige, 2002).
Paleontologists have examined the wear patterns on stone tools and the grinding
marks on devices used to grind grains.
These tools and implements date back between 8 000 and 35 000 BC and involve
the early humanoid Homo habilis, one of the earliest tool makers. The wear patterns of
these artefacts confirm that, even at that early date, there appeared to be a consistent
predominance of right-handers. Perhaps the most astounding evidence derives from more
than 1.5 million years ago, involving one of our very early hominid ancestors, Australo-
pithecus (Corballis, 1991; Coren, 2002).
Although Australopitheciens were not tool makers, they were tool users and would
pick up an appropriately sized and shaped rock or stick and use it for a weapon.
Examination of the skulls of baboons hunted by this early precursor to humans shows
that the vast majority of these hominids were already right-handed (Corballis, 1991;
Iacoboni and Zaidel, 2002; Coren, 2002).
All language is probably located in the left hemisphere of bilingual people, but the
possibility that their left-hemisphere language zones are enlarged or slightly different in
microorganization from those who speak only a single language cannot be ruled out (Kolb
and Whishaw, 2003).
However, the major effects of language and environment on the brain heavily depend
on culture rather than on changes in cerebral asymmetry.
Exposure to multiple languages does not change the normal pattern of brain
organization.
The results of PET studies by Klein et al. (1995, 1999) showed that no difference
appears in the cerebral activation for various language tasks performed in English and
French or English and Chinese by bilingual subjects. In particular, no activation of the
right hemisphere was recorded for any task in either language. There may, however, be
subtle differences in the cerebral representation of different languages within the left
hemisphere.
Using fMRI, Kim et al. (1997) showed that languages acquired later in life may
activate different, although adjacent, frontal regions from those activated by first
languages or second languages acquired early in life.
Hickock et al. (2001) studied 34 congenitally deaf patients who had unilateral brain
injury. Left-hemisphere patients performed poorly on all measures of language use,
whereas right-hemisphere patients performed poorly on visuospatial tasks, exactly what
would be expected in hearing people.
Environmental deprivation. According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), an adolescent
girl endured nearly 12 years of extreme social and experimental deprivation and
malnutrition. She was discovered at the age of 13½ , after having spent most of her life
isolated in a small closed room during which time she was punished for making any
noise. After her rescue, Genie’s cognitive development was rapid, although her language
lagged behind other abilities.
Results of her dichotic listening tests showed a strong left-ear (hence right-
hemisphere) effect for both verbal and nonverbal environmental sounds. The right ear
was nearly totally suppressed. Genie’s right hemisphere appeared to be processing both
verbal and nonverbal acoustical stimuli, as would be the case in people with a left
hemispherectomy in childhood (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Probably, in the absence of appropriate auditory stimulation, the left hemisphere lost
the ability to process linguistic stimuli. Another explanation is that Genie’s left
hemisphere was either being inhibited by the right hemisphere or by some other structure
or it was performing other functions (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Environmental accident. This theory postulates a genetically determinated bias
toward being right handed.
Left-handedness develops through a cerebral deficit caused by accident. This idea
comes from correlating statistics on the incidence of the left-handedness and neurological
disorders in twins. About 18% of twins are left handed, close to twice the occurrence in
the population at large. Twins also show a higher incidence of neurological disorders,
which are suspected to result overwhelmingly from intrauterine crowding during fetal
development and stress during delivery (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Bakan et al. (1973) argued for a high probability of stressful birth among left-
handers, which increases the risk of brain damage to the infant and so maintains the
statistical incidence of left handedness.
So, most deviation from the expected pattern of fetal development occurs because
something has gone wrong during pregnancy or the birth process. For this reason,
researchers began to focus on the handedness and birth stressors or pregnancy
complications. Such pathological factors are associated with an increased likelihood of
left-handedness.
These factors are premature birth, prolonged labor, Rh incompatibility, breech delivery,
multiple birth, anoxia, cesarian delivery, and forceps or other instruments to assist the
delivery. Thus, it is not surprising that older mothers (aged 40 and older), who are prime
candidates for pregnancy and birth complications, are more than twice as likely to produce
left-handed children (Bishop, 1990; Corballis, 1991; Springer and Deutsch, 1995).
nonfamilial left-handers, who have no such family history. According to Hécaen and
Sauguet (1971), the performance of nonfamilial left-handed patients with unilateral
lesions is like that of right-handed patients on neuropsychological tests. In contrast,
familial left-handers perform much differently, suggesting to Hécaen and Sauguet that
they have a different pattern of cerebral organization.
Rasmussen and Milner (1977) found that, in left-handers, language is represented
in the left hemisphere in 70%, in the right hemisphere in 15%, and bilaterally in 15%.
Kimura (1999) reported the incidence of aphasia and apraxia in a constitutive series of
520 patients selected for unilateral brain damage only. The frequency of left-handedness
in this population was within the expected range, and these patients did not have a higher
incidence of either aphasia or apraxia than right-handers did. In fact, the incidence of
aphasia in left-handed patients was approximately 70% of the incidence in right-handers,
exactly what would be predicted from the sodium amobarbital studies. Thus, although a
small proportion of left-handers have bilateral speech or right-hemisphere speech, the
majority of left-handers do not (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
However, there is larger incidence of left-handedness among mentally defective
children and children with various neurological disorders than it is found in the general
population. Thus, it can be excepted by probability alone that right-handed children with
left-hemisphere damage would switch to right-hemisphere dominance than in the reverse
direction.
Anatomical theories
One anatomical theory attributes right-handedness to enhanced maturation and
ultimately greater development of the left hemisphere.
Generalizing from assumption, the theory predicts that nonfamilial left-handers will
show an asymmetry mirroring the right-handers, whereas familial left-handers will show
no anatomical asymmetry (Kolb et al., 1982; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
However, no study has specifically considered anatomical asymmetry with respect
to handedness or to familial history and handedness (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Another theory have emphasized that many animals have a left-sided developmental
advantage that is not genetically coded (Morgan, 1977).
For example, there is a left-sided bias for location of the heart, the size of the ovaries
in birds, the control of birdsong, the size of left temporal cortex in humans, the size of
the left side of the skull in the great apes, and so on (Morgan, 1977; Moscovitch, 1977).
This predominance of left-favoring asymmetries puts the celebrated left-hemisphere
speech dominance in the more general, structural perspective of all anatomical asym-
metries (Moscovitch, 1977).
Because neither genetic evidence nor genetic theory accurately predicts these human
asymmetries, Morgan (1977) assumes that they all result from some fundamental
asymmetries in human body chemistry. Thus, Morgan’s theory fails to explain left-
handedness in the presence of other normal asymmetries such as the location of the heart
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
1791
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Hormonal theories
Geschwind and Galaburda (1987) proposed that brain plasticity can modify cerebral
asymmetry significantly during early life, leading to anomalous patterns of hemispheric
organization. A central factor in their theory is the action of the sex linked male hormone
testosterone in altering cerebral organization during development.
Testosterone does affect cerebral organization; so it is reasonable to suggest that
differences in testosterone level might influence cerebral asymmetry (Mc Glone, 1977, 1980).
Thus, while in the uterus, if the fetus is exposed to an elevated concentration of the
hormone testosterone, this fact could produce left-handedness by altering the relative
rate at which the two hemispheres of the brain develop. Since testosterone is also the
male hormone, this could also explain the observation that men are more likely to
become left-handed than are women (approximately 12% left-handedness for men versus
8% for women).
Geschwind and Galaburda (1987) suggested that testosterone’s effect is largely
inhibitory, meaning that higher-than-normal levels of testosterone will slow development,
possibly acting directly on the brain or indirectly through an action on genes. Thus,
testosterone’s inhibitory action is largely in the left hemisphere, allowing the right
hemisphere to grow more rapidly, which leads to altered cerebral organization and, in
some people, to left-handedness.
Testosterone also affects the immune system, leading to more diseases related to a
malfunctioning immune system (Gualtieri and Hicks, 1985).
Unfortunately, the bulk of available evidence does not support the model of
Geschwind-Galaburda theory (Grimshaw et al., 1993; Bryden et al., 1994).
Although there is sufficient homogeneity there is also sufficiently reliable heterogeneity
to warrant consideration of individual variation. Of particular interest has been the possible
relationship of functional hemispheric asymmetry to a number of other between-subject
factors including handedness, sex, intellectual ability and psychopathology.
Thus, hand dominance is determined by a variety of genetic and environmental
factors, both before and after birth.
Environmental influences include pre- and postnatal trauma, prenatal levels of
testosterone and other hormones (higher prenatal levels of testosterone are associated
with greater incidence of left-handedness), asymmetric positioning of the fetus in the
utero, as well as the biases of the postnatal world.
There are many effects of fetal hormone levels on brain development in other species
and in humans there are clear relationships between biological sex and cognitive ability.
For example, women tend to outscore men on tests of verbal fluency, and men tend to
outscore women on tests of spatial ability. Thus, the incidence of left-handedness is
slightly higher in men than in women, consistent with the hypothesis that higher levels
of fetal testosterone promote development of the right hemisphere relative to the left
hemisphere (Corballis, 1997; Halpen, 2000), so, deficits in patients with unilateral brain
damage, finding evidence of greater functional hemispheric asymmetry in men than in
women.
1792
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Behavioral laterality studies using neurologically intact men and women provide, at
best, weak support for the hypothesis of greater functional laterality in men because the
results have been quite variable.
There are indications that individual variations in brain laterality may be related to
individual differences in cognitive ability. Relative performance on tests of verbal and
visuospatial ability are related to handedness. There is also evidence that extreme
intellectual precocity, especially for mathematical reasoning, is related to advanced
development of the right hemisphere relative to the left, perhaps as a result of increased
levels during fetal development (Hellige, 2000, 2002).
Genetic theories
Given the evidence that handedness pattern in humans extends far back into
evolutionary history it is not surprising that the vast majority of theories of handedness
have included the suggestion of a genetic factor.
Most genetic models for handedness postulate a dominant gene or genes for right-
handedness and a recessive gene or genes for left-handedness (Hardyck and Petrovich
1977). Annett (1970) rejects this idea in favor of a dominant gene (rs+) responsible for
the development of speech in the left hemisphere.
Annett (1970) hypothesizes further that the processes necessary for left-hemisphere
speech also confer advantage on motor control in the right hand. The recessive form of
the gene (rs– ) result in no systematic bias either for speech or for handedness. If both
alleles occurred equally often statistically, then 50% of the population would be (rs+– )
and the rest would be equally divided, 25% (rs++) and 25% (rs– –). People in the rs+– and
rs++ groups, constituting 75% of the population, would show a left-for-speech and right-
handedness shift. The remaining 25%, people in the rs– – group, would show no bias; so
half would, by chance, be left-handed (Annett, 1970).
Thus, Annett’s model predicts about 12.5% left-handers, which is pretty close to
what we see in the population. Unfortunately, her theory neither predicts the number of
left-handers with right-hemisphere speech nor attempts to differentiate between familial
and nonfamilial left-handers (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Unfortunately, the empirical evidence on handedness does not strongly support
genetic theories. Some theorists are still trying to work out more sophisticated
mathematical descriptions that might show that handedness is inherited; however, there
still is a strong element of doubt in the data (Coren, 2002).
One way to investigate the contributions of genes and experience to cerebral
organization is to analyze MRIs from normal brain and to vary the genetic relationships
among the subjects.
Thompson et al. (2001) varied genetic relatedness by comparing the MRIs of unrelated
people, dizygotic twins, and monozygotic twins. The results were striking, because the
quantity of gray matter, especially in frontal, sensorimotor, and posterior language cortex
was dissimilar in unrelated people but almost identical in monozygotic twins.
Because monozygotic twins are genetically identical, we can presume that any
differences must be attributable to environmental effects. Curiously, there was an
1793
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
This raises one very important issue – the relevance of animal models to human
development and function. Thus, what happens in the mouse is relevant to humans, but
many human diseases have proven hard to model in the mouse.
According to Abu-Khalil et al. (2003), there are few human studies of important
patterning molecules or signaling centers involved in mouse brain patterning.
Similarly, given those various diseases, such as schizophrenia and some dementias,
witch affect the integrity of the cortex, the high correlation between the brain structures
of identical twins could account for the strong genetic component of these diseases
(Thompson et al., 2001; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Motor skills
One obvious difference in motor skills is that, on average, men are superior in
throwing objects, such as balls or darts at targets, and are superior at intercepting objects
thrown toward them. These differences are present in children as young as 3 years of age
(Kimura, 1999). In contrast, women have superior fine motor control and surpass men in
executing sequential and intricate hand movement. However, young girls are superior to
young boys at each of these skills (Mc Coby and Jacklin, 1974; Majeres, 1983; Hall,
1984; Hall and Kimura, 1995).
Spatial analysis
According to Kimura (1999), although men are generally believed to be superior to
women at spatial analysis, this belief applies to only some types of spatial behaviors.
Men are superior at tasks requiring the mental rotation of objects, and they are superior
at spatial navigation tasks (Collins and Kimura, 1997).
Subjects are given a tabletop map on which they must learn a designated route. On
average, men learn such tasks faster and with fewer errors than women do (Kimura,
1999). Although this map test is not a real-world test of spatial navigation, the findings
are consistent with those of studies showing that the men have better overall map
knowledge than women (Corsi-Cabrera et al., 1997).
Male superiority in spatial-navigation tasks contrasts with female superiority on test
of spatial memory. Women are better than men at identifying which objects have been
moved. In the map test women have better recall for landmarks along the route. Thus,
the spatial information itself is not the critical factor in this sex difference; rather the
critical factor is required behavior, that is the way in which the spatial information is to
be used (Kimura, 1999; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
In accounting for findings that the spatial performance of right hemisphere damaged
patients is adversely affected by lesions occurring anywhere in a fairly wide area while
only those left hemisphere damaged patients with relatively severe damage to a well
defined area show impaired performance on spatial tasks, De Renzi and Faglioni (1967),
too, hypothesized more diffuse representation of functions in the right hemisphere and
more focal representation in the left.
Mathematical aptitude
Stanley founded The Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth (SMPY) in
1971. The project primarily included holding talent search with the intent of identifying
gifted youth, particularly in the area of mathematics.
Within a 15-year period, thousands of 12-year-old children were given the Scholastic
Aptitude Test Mathematics exam. Stanley was particularly interested in the children with
the highest scores because they might be assumed to be least affected by extraneous
environmental factors such as social pressures. Benbow and Stanley (1980) and Benbow
(1988) have shown that the sex difference increased as the scores increased: 12 times as
1796
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
many “gifted” boys than girls at the top. Furthermore, this ratio was found worldwise
across different cultures, although the absolute scores varied with the educational system.
Benbow and Stanley (1980) have searched for support for a primarily environmental
explanation of their data but they have not found it.
On average, men get better scores on tests of mathematical reasoning, whereas
women do better at tests of computation.
Perception
According to Kimura (1999), perception refers to the recognition and interpretation of
the sensory information that we take in from the external world (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
There would appear to be no a priory reason to expect a sex difference, but the
evidence suggests that women are more sensitive to all forms of sensory stimulation,
except for vision. However, women are more sensitive than men to facial expressions
and body postures. They also detect sensory stimuli faster.
Males may have one perceptual advantage, however, in that their drawing of
mechanical things such as bicycles are superior (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Verbal ability
According to Kimura (1999), women are superior to men on tests of verbal fluency,
on average, and they have superior verbal memory. The sex differences in verbal ability
have long been known, in part because girls begin talking before boys and appear to be
more fluent throughout life.
For example, the Chicago Word-Fluency Test asks subjects to write as many words
beginning with “s” as possible in 5 minutes and as many four-letter words beginning with
“c” as possible in 4 minutes. Girls performed better – at some ages, by as many as 10
words – in a broad study that Kolb and Whishaw (2003) did with children.
It is often argued that sex-related differences are related to life experience, but
Kimura (1999) argues compellingly that this relation is unlikely for the cognitive
behaviors. In particular, most if not all these differences, as well as differences in
aggression are found in both children and adults, and the differences are largely
unaffected by training. There are certainly training effects on most tests, but the effects
tend to be of similar magnitude in both sexes (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
The same sex differences seem unrelated to life experience.
Lynn (1994) and Alexopoulos (1996) have concluded that men have small (4 point)
advantage in IQ scores, on average.
When different cerebral regions are examined separately, with results being
correlated for relative size of the cerebrum of different brains, the findings of many studies
show areal-dependent sex differences, as summarized in Table 23.2.
Table 23.2
Summary of sex differences in gross brain anatomy
(after Kolb and Whishaw, 2003)
Sex-related differences in brain volume are not diffusely spread across the cerebral
hemispheres. Generally, female brain appear to have larger volumes in region associated
with language functions, in medial paralimbic regions, and in lateral frontal areas (Filipek
et al., 1994; Schlaepfer et al., 1995; Harasty et al., 1997). In addition, women have a
greater relative amount of matter and in the temporal lobe, they have more densely packed
neurons (Witelson et al., 1995; Gur et al., 1999).
Conversely, men have a larger medial frontal (Goldstein et al., 2001), and cingulate
areas (Paus et al., 1996), a larger amygdala and hypothalamus (Swaab and Fliers, 1995),
a larger overall white matter volume (Gur et al., 1999), a large cerebral ventricle (Murphy
et al., 1996), and a larger right planum parietale (Jäncke et al., 1994).
However, male brains tend to have more neurons (Pakkenberg and Gundersen, 1997)
and female brain more neuropils (that is dendrites and axons) per neuron (Kolb and
Whishaw, 2003).
et al., 1996; Frost et al., 1999) studies show more asymmetrical activity in men than in
women, particularly in language-related activities (Table 23.3).
Table 23.3
Sex differences in imaging studies
(after Kolb and Whishaw, 2003)
Measures of the blood flow, including those obtained with the use of PET, show
that women have more rapid overall blood exchange than men have, possibly due to the
difference in the density of neurons or the distribution of gray matter and white matter
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
From these results there are differences in anatomical organization and functional
activity of the male and female brain. Presumably, the anatomical differences correspond
to the functional differences that researchers have found (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Because of these anatomical and functional differences, two types of lesion-related
differences are possible in the neurological patients.
Such difference might exist if the two hemispheres were more similar functionally
in one sex than in the other sex. Indeed, the greater asymmetry observed in EEG (Corsi-
Cabrera et al., 1997), MEG (Reite et al., 1995) and fMRI (Pugh et al., 1996, Frost et al.,
1999) studies in men suggests that men might show more asymmetry effects of unilateral
lesions than women.
Injury to the frontal lobe might have greater effects in one sex than in the other, a
difference that would be consistent with a greater relative volume of much of the frontal
lobes in women (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
One way to assess the asymmetry of left- or right-hemisphere lesions is to look at
the effects of lesions on general tests of verbal and nonverbal abilities.
So, Inglis and Lawson (1982) showed that, although left- and right-hemisphere
lesions in men affected the verbal and performance subscales of the WAIS (Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale) differently, left-hemisphere lesions in women depressed both
1800
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
IQ scores equally, and right-hemisphere lesions in women failed to depress either score.
A clear sex difference emerged in which males with left-hemisphere lesions exhibited a
depression in verbal IQ, whereas males with right-hemisphere exhibited a complementary
deficit in performance IQ. In contrast, females with right-hemisphere lesions showed no
significant depression in either IQ scale.
Thus, Inglis and Lawson (1982) found an equivalent effect of left-hemisphere lesions
on verbal IQ in both sexes, but men with right-hemisphere lesions were more disrupted
than woman were.
Work of Kimura (1999) shows that the pattern of cerebral organization within each
hemisphere also differs between the sexes. Men and women are almost equally likely to
be aphasic subsequent to left-hemisphere lesion. But apraxia is associated with frontal
damage to the left-hemisphere in women and with posterior damage in men. Aphasia
develops most often when damage in to the front of the brain in women but to the rear of
the brain in men (Kimura, 1999).
Thus, men are likely to be aphasic and apraxic after damage to the left posterior
cortex, whereas women are far more likely to experience speech disorders and apraxia
after anterior lesion.
Kimura (1999) also suggests an analogous sex-related difference subsequent to right-
hemisphere lesions.
Anterior, but not posterior lesions in women impaired their performance of the block-
design and object-assembly subtests of WAIS, whereas men were equally affected on
these tests by either anterior or posterior lesions.
Kolb and Stewart (1991) have found parallel results in their studies of rats with
prefrontal lesions: male rats with these lesions have much smaller deficits in various tests
of spatial navigation than do female rats with similar lesions. This finding may suggest
a fundamental difference between the sexes in the intracerebral organization in mammals.
Kolb and Stewart (1991), in an anatomical study, show a large difference between
male and female rats in the dendritic organization of neurons in the prefrontal cortex.
This sex difference was affected by treatment that either increased or decreased
testosterone levels during development (Kolb and Stewart, 1991).
Strauss et al. (1992) gave sodium amobarbital to 94 epileptic patients who were
being considered for elective surgery after infant brain damage. Left-hemisphere injury
in infancy leads to the shifting of language to the right hemisphere: so Strauss expected
this shift in those patients with left-hemisphere injury. The unexpected result was a sex
difference in the likelihood of cerebral reorganization subsequent to left-hemisphere
injury after one year of age: girls were unlikely to show reorganization, whereas boys
appeared likely to shift language, perhaps as late as puberty. This suggests that the male
brain may be more plastic after cortical injury.
According to Kolb and Stewart (1991), males showed a better recovery and more
synaptic changes than females did.
Thus, unilateral cortical lesions have different effects on male and female brain.
1801
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Because the girls mature faster than boys, superior spatial abilities in boys may be
directly related to their relatively slow development (Weber, 1976).
Environment. In regard to spatial ability, boys are expected to exhibit greater
independence than girls and thus to engage in activities such as exploring and
manipulating the environment.
Harris (1978) concluded that, although a few studies can be found to support the
environmental view, the bulk of the evidence fails to do so.
In the study, by Thomas et al. (1973), on the horizontality of a liquid, women who
failed the task were repeatedly shown a bottle half-filled with red water that was tilted at
various angles. Women stated that “water is level when the bottle is upright but inclined
when the bottle is tilted”.
Men and women who performed correctly stated that “water is always level”.
Thus, although environmental theories may be appealing, there is no evidence that
environmental or social factors can solely account for the observed sex differences in
verbal and spatial behaviors (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Preferred cognitive mode. According to Kolb and Whishaw (2003), the difference
in strategies used by men and women to solve problems may be at least partly responsible
for the observed sex differences in behavior.
Genetic maturational and environmental factors may predispose men and women to
prefer different modes of cognitive analysis.
Women solve problems primarily by using a verbal mode. Because this cognitive
mode is less efficient in solving spatial problems, women exhibit an apparent deficit
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
In sum, at least six significant behavioral differences are related: verbal ability,
visuospatial analysis, mathematical ability, perception, motor skills, and aggression (Kolb
and Whishaw, 2003).
Harshman et al. (1983), in a very ambitious study of the interaction of sex and
handedness in cognitive abilities, found a significant interaction between sex and
handedness; that is, sex-related differences in verbal and visuospatial behavior vary as a
function of handedness.
Witelson (1985, 1989) found that callosal size also varies by sex and handedness.
Anatomical studies
Generally, the left-hemisphere of the brain controls the right side of the body and
the right hemisphere of the brain controls the left side of the body.
Broca (1861) discovered that the major language production center in the brain is
located in the left hemisphere of the brain. Another speech center called Wernicke’s area
(Wernicke, 1874), that is associated with speech comprehension, is also located in the
left hemisphere of the brain.
The fact that the right-hand and primary speech functions are controlled by the same
half of the brain has fuelled speculations attempting to link brain organization language
functions, and handedness. Thus, Broca introduced the notion of the “dominant
1804
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
hemisphere”, by which he meant the hemisphere that directs not only the movements of
writing, drawing, and other fine movements but also language and perhaps even major
aspects of logical thought. He then suggested that left-handers might: have a brain that
is organized in a mirror image to that of right-handers, with language control on the right
side of the brain.
The data, however, suggest that this relationship is unfounded. So, acquired a
pathology that damages the left side of the brain, or an injection of sodium amytal into
the left side of the brain results in the development of aphasia.
Although virtually all right-handers have language area in their left hemisphere,
Coren (2002) did not find the mirror imaged brain that we expected in left-handers.
The data show that the vast majority of left-handers have the same brain organization
as right-handers, with their language control exclusively confined to the left hemisphere.
According to Coren (2002), only about 2 of 10 left-handers have language exclusively
in the right hemisphere, as had been predicted by Broca.
There is also a small group of individuals who seem to have their language control
split between both sides of the brain or duplicated on both sides of the brain (Springer
and Deutsch, 1995; Coren, 2002).
The overwhelming number of studies with the new technology (positron emission
tomography, functional magnetic resonance imaging, magnetoencephalography) have
found that there is a remarkable similarity between brains, regardless of handedness, with
the only major difference being that hand control regions are somewhat larger and more
active on the side opposite to the dominant hand.
However, right-handers seem to have brains that are slightly more asymmetrical, in
terms of both their structure and their functional specialization.
A larger proportion of left-handers seem to have more symmetrical brains and are
more likely to have functions equally represented in both hemispheres (Corballis, 1991;
Springer and Deutsch, 1995; Toga et al., 2006).
Certain cerebral anatomical asymmetries are apparent at both the macroscopic and
histological levels.
Hand preference is correlated with differential patterns of right - left asymmetry in
the parietal operculum, frontal cortex, occipital region, vascular patterns, and cerebral
blood flow (Carmon et al., 1972; Hochberg and Le May, 1975; Le May, 1977).
Thus, in comparison with right-handers, a higher proportion of left-handers show
no asymmetry.
Ratcliffe et al. (1980) correlates the asymmetry in the course of the sylvian (lateral)
fissure, as revealed by carotid angiography, and with the results of carotid sodium
amobarbital speech testing. They found that left- and right-handers with speech in the
left hemisphere had a mean right - left difference of 27º in the angle formed by the vessels
leaving the posterior end of sylvian fissure. In the left- and right-handers with speech in
the right hemisphere or with bilaterally represented speech, the mean difference shrank
to 0º. Thus, the anatomical asymmetry in their population was related to speech
representation and not necessarily to handedness.
1805
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
She found that the cross-sectional area was 11% greater in left-handed and
ambidextrous people than in the right-handed people.
Although no two human brains are exactly alike in their structure, in most people
the right frontal area is wider than the left and the right frontal pole protrudes beyond the
left, while the reverse is true of the occipital pole: the left occipital pole is frequently
wider and protrudes further posteriorly than the right but the central portion of the right
hemisphere is frequently wider than the left (Damasio and Geschwind, 1984; Jäncke and
Steinmetz, 2003).
Men show greater degree of frontal and occipital asymmetry than women (Bear et
al., 1986). These asymmetries exist in fetal brains (de Lacoste et al., 1991; Witelson, 1995).
The left sylvian fissure, the fold between the temporal and frontal lobes, is larger
than the right in most people (Witelson, 1995), even in newborns (Seidenwurm et al.,
1985). Most attention has focused on asymmetry of the posterior portion of the superior
surface of the temporal lobe, the planum temporale.
This region, which is involved in auditory processing, is larger on the left side in
most right-handers (Strauss et al., 1985; Beaton 1997).
Thus, for the right-handed individuals the planum temporale is larger in the left
hemisphere in approximately 65% of the cases, larger in the right hemisphere in
approximately 10% of the cases and approximately equal in 25% of the cases. The
corresponding percentages for non-right-handers are approximately 25, 10, and 65%
respectively. Similar distributions characterize asymmetry of the sylvian fissure (Hellige,
2000; Zaidel and Iacobini, 2002).
In sum, one of the most notable anatomical asymmetries is in the planum temporale,
which is usually larger on the left than on the right side.
Subtle asymmetries in the superior temporal lobe have been demonstrated in terms
of overall size and shape, sulcal pattern, cytoarchitecture, and at the neuronal level.
It seems reasonable to assume that these differences underlie some of the functional
asymmetries for language representation (Toga et al., 2006).
Thus, the left-handers suffer injuries while playing sports, during the traffic, working
with tools and motorized equipment, working with kitchen and home implements and
devices, and even while engaged in military activities (Coren, 2002).
Left-handedness might be a marker (at least, statistically) for the possible existence
of some form of neural pathology, psychological deviance, or developmental abnormality.
Thus, the same pathology that caused the left-handedness might have also caused some
form of collateral damage that can reduce the individual’s physiological or psychological
fitness through direct or secondary mechanisms (Coren, 1993, 2002). So, a substantial
body of research found that left-handedness is frequently associated with a variety of
symptoms or syndromes. However, the number of findings in which left-handedness is
associated with positive outcomes in the research literature is much rarer.
A review of the literature indicated at least 60 negative pathological or undesirable
conditions associated with left-handedness as opposed to only 4 positive or desirable
1808
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
conditions. Table 23.4 provides an idea of the positive and negative conditions related to
an increased proportion of left-handers (Coren, 2002).
The research literature is repleted with suggestions that left-handedness is
predominantly associated with negative conditions, and with a range of pathological
antecedents. Example of rare traits include animals that are colored differently from the
vast majority of their species. For instance, a “blue-marl” collie, a white dog, or an albino
human often have major sensory deficits affecting their vision or hearing. These rare
traits could include rare palm crease patterns, rare fingerprint patterns, or rare distribution
of toe lengths, and even unusual ear shapes are often found to be associated with cognitive
or physical deficits.
Left-handedness, which affects only about 10% of the population, would also be
qualified as a rare trait (Coren, 2002).
Thus, the rare versus the common trait can be influenced by pathological factors.
According to Coren (2002), half of the resulting population of left-handers are
pathological, whereas only 1.2% of the right-handers are pathological. This means that
the relative risk of left-hander being pathological is approximately 41% greater than that
of right-hander being pathological.
Left-handedness might be seen as a soft sign indicating increased risk for many
problems: however, which specific problems an individual might suffer from, or which
neural loci are involved, is not determinable (Coren, 2002).
From the standpoint of behavioral medicine, we may view left handedness as a risk
factor that may well predict susceptibility to illness, physiological deficits, psychological
problems, and perhaps even a shortened life span (Coren, 2002).
Effects of hemispherectomy
The catastrophic epilepsies, in which panhemispheric syndromes are associated with
intractable seizures, include Rasmussen’s encephalitis, developmental syndromes (i.e.,
hemimegaencephaly, tuberous sclerosis, hamartomas, Sturge-Weber syndrome), and
congenital hemiplegia or porencephaly (Carson et al., 1996).
Although the original surgical approach of anatomic complete, en bloc hemi-
spherectomy with spearing of basal ganglia, hypothalamus, and diencephalon (Krynaw,
1950; Rasmussen, 1975) was successful from the standpoint of seizure control, the
immediate and delayed complications were daunting (Pilcher and Ojeman, 1993).
Postoperative complications were significantly reduced with the introduction, by
Rasmussen (1975), of the technique of modified or functional hemispherectomy, in which
a generous central and temporal resection is juxtaposed with deafferentation of the frontal
and occipital lobes (Rasmussen, 1987). With deafferentation rather than removal of the
frontal and occipital lobes, the volume of intracranial dead space is reduced, and in
7.3-year follow-up study of 14 patients, no hemosiderosis or hydrocephalus occurred,
and 10 of 14 patients were seizure free. Villemure and Mascott (1995) introduced the
peri-insular hemispherectomy, in which a smaller craniotomy and a much reduced peri-
insular (i.e., opercular frontal, parietal, and temporal) resection are combined with
1809
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
complex tests such as negotiating a maze and reading a map (Kohn and Dennis, 1974;
Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
In sum, each hemisphere can assume some of its opposite’s functions if the opposite
hemisphere is removed in the course of development, but neither hemisphere is totally
capable of mediating all of the missing hemisphere’s function (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
There is convincing evidence against equipotentiality: both hemisphere appear to have a
processing capacity that probably has an innate structural basis (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
With few exception, patients undergoing hemispherectomy are of below-average or, at
least, average intelligence, and their proficiency at tests of the intact hemisphere’s function
is often less than normal (Dennis, 1980; Dennis et al., 1981; Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Although such children have severe behavioral difficulties after hemispherectomy,
they often compensate remarkably, communicating freely and, in some cases, showing
considerable motor control over the limbs opposite the excised hemisphere.
Using both fMRI and SEPs (somatosensory evoked potentials), Holloway et al.
(2000) investigated the sensorimotor functions of 17 hemispherectomy patients.
Ten patients showed SEPs in the normal hemisphere when the nerves of the limb
opposite the excised hemisphere were stimulated. Similarly, fMRI shows that, for at least
some of the patients, passive movement of the same limb produced activation in a region
of somatosensory cortex that normally responds to the opposite hand.
The Holloway’s team concluded that the responses to the hand ipsilateral to the
normal hemisphere must occur because direct ipsilateral pathways run from the normal
hemisphere to the affected limb.
However, the novel ipsilateral responses were found in patients with both congenital
and acquired disease, suggesting that although age at injury may be important, the ability
of nervous system to alter its organization to compensate for injury may influence the
cerebral reorganization.
Ontogeny of asymmetry
Generally, adultlike cerebral asymmetries are present before birth, a result that
supports an innate predisposition for cerebral asymmetry in humans.
In a MRI study, Sowell et al. (2002 a and b) confirm this general impression.
Thus, a basic template for cortical development appears to lay down an asymmetrical
organization prenatally, and the pattern progresses after birth.
The results of ERP studies by Dennis (1980), and Molfese and Molfese (1988)
confirm a functional asymmetry in which the left hemisphere showed a greater response
to speech stimuli as early as one week of age. There is apparently little change in this
difference during development.
However, initially it permits some flexibility or equipotentiality. The cognitive
functions of each hemisphere can be conceived as hierarchical. Simple, lower-level
functions are represented at the base of the hierarchy, corresponding to functions in the
primary sensor, motor, language, or visuospatial areas. More complex, higher-level
functions ascend the hierarchy, the most complex being at the top. These advanced
functions are the most lateralized (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
1811
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
At birth, the two hemispheres overlap functionally because each is processing low-
level behaviors.
By age of 5, the newly developing higher-order cognitive processes have very little
overlap, and each hemisphere becomes increasingly specialized. By puberty, each
hemisphere has developed its own unique functions (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Thus, Moscovitch (1977) emphasized the possibility that one hemisphere actively
inhibits the other, thus preventing the contralateral hemisphere from developing similar
functions. This active inhibition presumably develops at about age of 5, as the corpus
callosum becomes functional. According to Moscovitch (1977), this inhibitory process not
only prevents the subsequent development of language processes in the right hemisphere
but also inhibits the expression of the language processes already in the right hemisphere.
The right hemisphere of commissurotomy patients appears to have greater language
abilities than expected from the study of normal patients, presumably because the right
hemisphere is no longer subject to inhibition by the left. Netley (1977) showed that people
born with no corpus callosum demonstrate little or no functional asymmetry as inferred
from dichotic listening, suggesting that the absence of interhemispheric connection results
in attenuated hemispheric asymmetry.
Popularized accounts of laterality have suggested that people can be classified as
“right-brained” or left-brained”, depending on whether they prefer to use strategies and
models of cognition associated with one hemisphere or another.
Thus, left-brained people are said to be rational and analytic, whereas right-brained
people are said to be intuitive, artistic, and creative.
However, there is individual variation on the various dimensions of laterality, but
there is no evidence that any neurologically intact individuals are functionally half-
brained in the manner referred to as hemisphericity. Individuals do differ reliably in
cognitive style, personality, creativity and so forth (Hellige, 2002).
However, hemispheric asymmetries are subtle, with no indication that aspects such
as rationality and creativity are the exclusive product of one brain hemisphere. Instead,
both hemispheres contribute to virtually everything we do (Hellige, 2000, 2002).
The discovery that both of disconnected hemispheres of split-brain patients have a
good deal of competence with respect to perception and motor control has led to
speculation about whether or not the surgery has produced a doubling of consciousness
or resulted in people with two minds.
Sperry (1986) believed that this was the case. In part, he based his conclusion on
the fact that the disconnected right hemisphere has its own perceptions, cognitions, and
memories of which the disconnected left hemisphere is completely unaware.
Others, however, have questioned whether the right hemisphere can truly think and
whether its limited abilities include the same level of awareness and consciousness that
seems typical on the left hemisphere.
Certainly, the disconnected right hemisphere is usually incapable of speech and other
forms of verbal communication that are uniquely human and that some have argued, are
essential for the concept of “mind”. There is much debate among philosophers and
1812
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
cognitive scientists about the extent to which language is essential for thinking, conscious
reflection, or mental life.
Le Doux and Gazzaniga (1978) hypothesized that an important function of an
individual’s verbal left hemisphere is the construction of internal, subjective reality or
ongoing narrative based on its observations of his or her own overt behavior.
In this view, observation of one’s own behavior is necessary because many processes
that influence behavior are not open to conscious experience. Le Doux and Gazzaniga
(1978) emphasize this with respect to behavior and bodily sensations produced by the
right hemisphere, of which the left hemisphere would have no direct knowledge.
However, even within the left hemisphere there appear to be a great many modules
whose processes may influence behavior but that are not themselves open to conscious
awareness.
Thus, the need to create an ongoing personal narrative by making inferences about
one’s own behavior is likely to extend to covert processes performed by both
hemispheres. Although, neither hemisphere is uniformly superior for processing
nonverbal perceptual information. Instead, both hemispheres contribute to perceptual
processing and do so in ways that could be described as complementary.
This chapter is not meant to solve all of the mysteries of lateralized functions, but
merely to highlight anatomical and functional asymmetries as they relate to language,
complex motor behaviors, and sensorimotor integration.
It is often assumed that anatomical asymmetries invariably reflect functional
asymmetries. However, physiological asymmetries, asymmetries in gene expression, or
subtle differences in neuronal cytoskeletal architecture may play a more significant role
in hemispheric specialization than gross anatomical or cytoarchitectonic asymmetries
(Geschwind and Iacoboni, 2007).
The intensification of morphological asymmetries associated with language is
important because of a wealth of evidence that these asymmetries are functionally
relevant (Witelson, 1977, 1992; Galaburda et al., 1978; Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985).
Furthermore, a number of studies support the general notion that the amount of
cerebral cortex dedicated to a particular function may reflect the brain capabilities in that
area (Eccles, 1977; Jerison, 1977; Garraghty and Kaas, 1992).
However, the size of a brain region is not always positively correlated with its
capabilities. Often, a larger cerebral hemisphere can be observed due to neuronal
migration abnormalities or other cortical malformations. In the domain of language more
specifically, the brains of individuals with dyslexia appear to be more symmetrical, with
a larger than usual planum temporale on the right, rather than a smaller planum temporale
on the left (Galaburda, 1993; Kushch et al., 1993).
Thus, anatomical asymmetries, whether gross or fine, cannot be viewed in isolation
and must eventually be considered in the context of physiology of the neuronal systems
to which they contribute (Geschwind and Iacoboni, 2007).
Handedness is of particular interest because it is a behavioral manifestation of brain
laterality for certain manual activities and it is also related to other, more cognitive
aspects of laterality.
1813
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Complementarity refers to the fact that, in many domains, the role for which each
hemisphere is dominant can be described as complementary. Consequently, both
hemispheres normally play a role in virtually all complex activities, such as understanding
language or identifying faces, with their contributions fitting together like two pieces of
a puzzle (Hellige, 2000).
Individual differences in the behavior of normal subjects result, at least in part, from
individual differences in how the cerebral hemispheres are organized and how functions
are lateralized.
At one extreme, people who are logical, analytical, verbal, and meticulous are
assumed to rely more on their left hemispheres to solve problems in everyday life,
whereas people who are visual, intuitive and synthesizer are more concerned with
organizing concepts and visualizing meaningful wholes are assumed to rely more on their
right hemisphere.
Anyhow, factors other than brain organization probably contribute to preferred
cognitive mode.
On the one hand, strategies used by subjects can significantly influence tests of
lateralization, and, on the other hand, cognitive mode may be due to biases in socialization
or environmental factors in addition to neuronal, genetic, or constitutional biases.
Nevertheless, the idea that individual differences in behavior and cognition result
in part from individual differences in brain organization is a provocative assumption
worthy of serious study.
Clinical and neuroimaging studies, including PET, fMRI, ERP, and MEG, allowed
researchers to map cerebral activity as it takes place in normal subjects, and performances
related to left and right hemisphere functions.
Because both hemispheres are scanned, however, it is possible to assess left - right
differences in cerebral activation during a large range of behavioral measures.
Anyhow, clinical study has limitations of functional localization because “symptoms
must be viewed as expressions of disturbances in a system, not as direct expressions of
focal loss of neuronal tissue” (Benton, 1981).
Aphasics with similar symptoms may present lesions which vary in site or size. On
the other hand, cortical mapping by electrode stimulation (Ojemann, 1979) and neuro-
imaging techniques (Mazziota et al., 1997) demonstrates a great deal of interindividual
variability in cortical patterning. Examples from functional imaging studies show that
many different areas of the brain may be engaged during a cognitive task (Franckowiak
et al., 1997; Gazzaniga, 2000). For even the relatively simple task of telling whether
words represent a pleasant or unpleasant concept, the following areas of the brain showed
increased activation: left superior temporal cortex, posterior cingulate, left para-
hippocampal gyrus, and left inferior frontal gyrus (McDermott et al., 1999).
It is important to recognize that normal behaviors are functions of the whole brain
with important contributions from both hemispheres entering into every activity and
emotional state. Only laboratory studies on intact or split brain subjects or studies of
persons with lateralized brain damage demonstrate the differences in hemisphere function.
1815
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Tradition and common sense favor verbal (left hemisphere) versus nonverbal (right
hemisphere) dichotomy. However, some theories see this dichotomy as unsatisfactory
because a number of facts have been brought forth to support their position. There is the
centuries-old observation that some aphasic patients, if primed, can recite familiar prayers
faultlessly (Benton and Joynt, 1960).
Some aphasic patients can sing a song (with its words) while some nonaphasic right
hemisphere-damaged patients cannot (Benton, 1977; Burkland and Smith, 1977). An
aphasic patient who does not understand the propositional content of an utterance may
appreciate the import of its prosodic component, i.e., he will know whether the examiner
is making a statement, asking a question, or issuing a command.
Conversely, the verbal functions of many right hemisphere-damaged patients are
not fully intact, with examination disclosing and at least modest decline in performance
level as measured by controlled word association and taken tests.
Another point that is brought up is the sizable number of subjects who show a left
ear (i.e., right hemisphere) superiority when processing verbal information in dichotic
listening studies.
Observations such as these have impelled some theories to look for a more basic
dichotomy. The leading candidates are the “serial-parallel” and “analytic-holistic” pairs.
The two human cerebral hemispheres are not simply mirror images of each other.
Much information on the lateralization of cerebral function has come from studying
patients in whom the corpus callosum had been divided (commissurotomy) as a treatment
for intractable epilepsy, and from those rare individuals who lack part, or all, of their
corpus callosum.
Commissurotomy produces the “split-brain” syndrome, which has provided evidence
supporting the notion that abilities or functions are predominantly associated with one
or another hemisphere.
Thus, patients with chrome epilepsy, who have undergone surgical section of the
corpus callosum in order to relieve their seizures, portray few difficulties under normal
circumstances. However, when these “split brain” patients undergo psychological testing,
the two halves of the brain appear to behave relatively autonomously, e.g., visual
information directed to the right cerebral hemisphere alone does not evoke verbal
response, and consequently individuals cannot name objects or real words solely
presented to the left visual field. Destruction of the splenium of the corpus callosum and
its connections to the left occipital cortex, either by stroke or tumor, leads to the posterior
disconnection syndrome of “alexia without agraphia”. Such individuals speak and write
without difficulty but cannot understand written material (alexia). Disconnection of visual
processes in the right hemisphere from the verbal processes of the dominant left cerebral
hemisphere is thought to explain the syndrome.
Knowledge of such lateralization of function has been advanced more recently by
functional brain imaging techniques.
In sum, in the human brain, the left cerebral hemisphere is usually the dominant
hemisphere (in absolute 95% of the population), whereas the right hemisphere is the
nondominant hemisphere.
1816
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
The left hemisphere usually prevails for verbal and linguistic function, for problem
solving, for mathematical skills, and for analytical thinking.
The right hemisphere is mostly non-verbal. It is more involved in spatial and holistic
or “Gestalt” thinking, in recognition of faces, and in many aspects of musical and poetry
skills, and in some emotions (Table 23.5).
Table 23.5
Summary of data on cerebral lateralization
(after Kolb and Whishaw, 2003)
Note: Functions of the respective hemispheres that are predominantly mediated by one hemisphere in right-handed people.
Historical aspects
Bouillaud
Jean Baptiste Bouillaud (1796-1881) read a paper before the Royal Academy of
Medicine in France in which he argued from clinical studies that certain functions are
localized in the neocortex and, specifically, that speech is localized in the frontal lobes,
in accordance with Gall’s beliefs and opposed to Flourens’s beliefs. Beginning in 1825
1817
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
and for a half-century thereafter, Bouillaud was the great champion of Gall’s localization
of the centers of speech and language in the frontal lobes and he argued repeatedly,
vigorously and, at times, rancorously that aphasic disorders resulted only from lesions in
this territory. In his Traité … de l’encephalite (1825), he presented 29 cases with and
without aphasia and with and without lesions in the anterior, middle, and posterior lobes.
All of the aphasic patients had lesions that were in, or close to the anterior lobes.
Inspection of Bouillaud’s 29 cases shows that 25 had lesions confined to a single
hemisphere, 11 in the left hemisphere and 14 in the right. Eight (73%) of the 11 left
hemisphere cases were aphasic. Four (29%) of the right hemisphere cases were aphasic.
Reviewing his own autopsied cases as well as those in the literature, he typically presented
just a few words on the loci of the lesions and sometimes these few words were very
imprecise indeed. In some cases, the flat statement that the lesion was in one or the other
anterior lobes (or both) is made. However, in other cases, the locus of the lesions is
described as being in the “anterior” part of the hemispheres.
Perhaps one reason why Bouillaud did not perceive this trend toward a higher
frequency of aphasic disorder in his left hemisphere patients is that not only he was
obsessed with the frontal lobe localization of aphasic disorder but he also accepted Gall’s
dual localization of the centers of speech and language in both hemispheres.
Observing that acts such as writing, drawing, painting, and fencing are carried out
with the right hand, Bouillaud also suggested that a part of the brain that controls them
might possibly be the left hemisphere.
Marc Dax
Marc Dax was born in 1770 and died in 1837. He studied medicine in Montpellier,
his graduate thesis being an interesting survey of the incidence and nature of the diseases
occurring over a 5-year period in the small town of Aigues-Mortes. As Gibson (1962)
has pointed out, in this thesis Dax called attention to the occasional occurrence of post-
seizure focal paralysis in some children. This observation preceded by many years, those
of Bravais and Todd, who are usually credited with the first descriptions of the
phenomenon.
Dax had a keen interest in the study of language, and this may account for the special
attention which he paid to language disturbances. In 1836, 1 year before his death, he
wrote a paper (Dax, 1836, 1865) on the association between aphasic disorders and lesions
of the left hemisphere for presentation at a regional medical congress at Montpellier. He
was, thus, about 66 year old when he wrote his famous mémoire. It is a remarkable
document. Dax describes successive observations that led him gradually to the conviction
that aphasia was a product of left hemisphere disease. He collected cases over the ensuing
20 years, so that at the time of writing his paper he reported having a series of over 40
cases in which the diagnosis of left hemisphere disease had been made, primarily on
clinical ground, without pathological confirmation.
The paper was not published and, as it will be seen, there is no evidence that Marc
Dax actually presented it at the congress. When it was, belatedly, submitted for publication,
1818
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
in 1865, by his physician-son, Gustav Dax, 4 years after Broca’s initial observation (Broca,
1865), it initiated a controversy about priority which has persisted up to the present day. At
the same time, together with his father’s memoir, Gustav Dax (Dax, 1865) published a
summary of his own observations and views on aphasia (Joynt and Benton, 2000).
From a scientific standpoint, Marc Dax’s generalization on the role of the left
hemisphere in speech was hardly derived from well documented observations. The
clinical descriptions were scanty, and the pathological confirmation was entirely lacking.
Dax made no mention of handedness nor did he, in any way, link hand preference to
hemispheric localization of speech function. Nonetheless, Marc Dax did make, and for
the first time, a clinical observation of the highest importance.
Gustav Dax
Dax’s paper (M. Dax, 1865) was published in 1865 by his son, Gustav Dax (G. Dax,
1865), who stated that it had been read at a regional medical meeting in Montpellier in
1836. In fact, there is no evidence that he did present the paper on that occasion (Joynt
and Benton, 1964). It is not mentioned in account of the meeting, nor could anyone be
found who remembered having heard it. It seems almost certain that, if the paper had
been presented, it would not have been totally neglected and would have had some
repercussions (Benton, 2000).
The tone of Dax’s paper is personally modest but firm in conviction. Its style
indicates that it was meant to be a communication to his peer. He was quite aware of the
importance of his discovery and he made one or two copies which he sent to professional
friends. Why he did not make his discovery known at the time through publication or
oral presentation is not clear (Benton, 2000).
Anyhow, there is suggestive evidence that Gustav Dax, the son, did know of the
preferential involvement of the left hemisphere in aphasia prior Broca’s 1863 report.
Presumably, this knowledge was gained from his father – for Gustav Dax had prepared an
extensive treatise of his own on aphasia (Dax, 1877) entitled: “Observations intended to
prove the constant coincidence of the derangements of speech with a lesion of the left
cerebral hemisphere”. In the introduction, Gustav Dax pays respect to his father’s memory
and mentions his father’s views on the localization of lesions responsible for the loss of
speech. This work of Gustav Dax was received by the Academy of Medicine of Paris on
March 24, 1863. Broca’s statement on the eight cases of aphasia with left hemisphere lesions
was delivered on April 2, 1863. Unfortunately, the commission appointed by the Academy
to examine Gustav Dax’s essay did not publish a report on it until 2 years later. Additional
support for Dax’s claim was provided in 1879 by Caizergues, who reported he had
discovered a copy of Marc Dax’s memoir when classifying the papers of his grandfather
who had been dean of the faculty of Montpellier (Caizergues, 1879). There is, therefore,
excellent evidence that Marc Dax did make his observation prior to Broca.
In short, Marc Dax had made an observation for which he was unwilling to take
public responsibility, and hence he recorded it in the form of an essentially private
communication (Benton, 2000).
1819
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Broca
In 1861, Pierre Paul Broca (1824-1880) reported two instances of aphasia with
lesions in the left third frontal convolution (Broca, 1861).
He made no mention at that time of the significance of both these lesions occurring
on the left side. Later, at a meeting of the Anthropological Society of Paris on April 2,
1863, Broca reported on eight autopsies (Broca, 1863) of patients with aphasia and noted
that they all had lesions in the third frontal convolution. In 1865, Broca expanded his
views of the special role of the left hemisphere in speech and also discussed his priority
in making these observations (Broca, 1865).
In consequence Broca located speech in the third convolution of the frontal lobe on
the left side of the brain. Thus, he discovered that functions could be localized to a side
of the brain, a property that is referred to as lateralization. Because speech is thought to
be central in human consciousness, the left hemisphere is frequently referred to as the
dominant hemisphere, to recognize its special role in language. Thus, the concept of
hemispheric cerebral dominance was born when Broca (1865) was led to conclude from
his observations that “we speak with the left hemisphere”. He had read Marc Dax’s paper
as published by his son in 1865 and, naturally, wished to read the original presentation
along with any discussion. He searched in vain throughout the medical literature for the
original report or even for a reference to the 1836 paper. He then asked the librarian of
the Montpellier Faculty of Medicine to make personal inquiries regarding the 1836
presentation. The librarian reported that he had interviewed 20 physicians who had
attended the 1836 congress at Montpellier, and none could recall such a presentation.
Hence Broca was, with good reason, quite skeptical that Marc Dax had actually presented
his paper at the congress.
The discussion on priority was reopened by Broca in 1877 (Broca, 1877). He stated
that he had obtained a manuscript of the Marc Dax paper, purportedly presented in 1836,
and compared it with the Gustav Dax manuscript on aphasia. Broca admits that the style,
the mode of expression and the discussion all demonstrated no difference in origin.
Therefore, he did not doubt that the paper was written in 1836 by Marc Dax for
presentation, but he did not believe that it was ever presented. Broca speculated that Marc
Dax probably felt ensure of his ground and did not have the courage to face a discussion
as there was no confirmation of his cases by autopsy findings (Benton, 2000).
However, Broca’s anatomical analysis was criticized by French anatomist Pierre
Marie, who reexamined the brains of Broca’s first two patients, Tan, and a Monsieur
Lelong, 25 years after Broca’s death. Marie pointed out in his article titled “The Third
Left Frontal Convolution Plays No Particular Role in the Function of Language” that
Lelong’s brain showed general nonspecific atrophy, common in senility, and that Tan
had additional extensive damage in his posterior cortex that may have accounted for his
aphasia. Broca had been aware of Tan’s posterior damage but concluded that, whereas
the posterior damage contributed to his death, the anterior damage had occurred earlier,
producing aphasia.
Anyhow, a lesion in Broca’s area resulted in a loss of motor memory-images and
hence produced a primarily expressive aphasia with preservation of the capacity to
1820
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
understand speech. The speech pattern is nonfluent: on beside examination, the patient
speaks hesitantly, after producing the principal, meaning-containing nouns and verbs but
omitting small grammatical words and morphemes. This pattern is called agrammatism
or telegraphic speech. Patients with acute Broca’s aphasia may be mute or may produce
single words, often with dysarthria and apraxia of speech, naming is deficient, but the
patient often manifests a “tip of the tongue” phenomenon, getting out the first letter or
phoneme of the correct name.
Paraphasic errors in naming are more frequently of literal than verbal type (Kirshner,
2000).
Auditory comprehension seems intact, but detailed testing usually reveals some
deficiency, particularly in the comprehension of complex syntax. Repetition is hesitant
in these patients, resembling their spontaneous speech. Reading is often impaired. Broca’s
aphasics may have difficulty with syntax in reading, just as in auditory comprehension
and speech. Writing is virtually always deficient in Broca’s aphasias. Most patients have
a right hemiparesis necessitating use of nondominant, left hand for writing. Many patients
can scrawl only a few letters.
Associated neurological deficits of Broca’s aphasics include right hemiparesis,
hemisensory loss, and apraxia of the oral apparatus and the nonparalyzed left limbs.
An important clinical feature of Broca’s aphasia is its frequent association with
depression (Robinson, 1997).
The lesions responsible for Broca’s aphasia usually include the traditional Broca’s
area in the posterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus, along with damage to adjacent
cortex and subcortical white matter.
Lesions involving the traditional Broca’s area (Brodmann’s areas 44 and 45) resulted
in difficulty initiating speech, and lesions combining Broca’s area, the lower precentral
gyrus, and subcortical white matter yielded the full syndrome of Broca’s aphasia.
Aphemia, a rare variant of Broca’s aphasia, is a nonfluent syndrome in which the
patient is initially mute and then able to speak with phoneme substitutions and pauses.
Aphemia may be equivalent to pure apraxia of speech (Kirshner, 2000).
Wernicke
Broca himself was not greatly concerned with the neurological mechanisms
underlying speech and its disturbance. It was left to a younger physician, German ana-
tomist Carl Wernicke (1848-1904), to develop a mature theory of the nature of aphasic
disorders. In a monograph appeared in 1874, Wernicke demonstrated that a fluent aphasic
disorder, characterized by impaired understanding of speech and disordered expressive
speech, was specifically associated with disease in the territory of the posterior temporal
lobe of the left hemisphere (Wernicke, 1874). Wernicke was aware that the part of the
cortex that receives the sensory pathway, or projection, from the ear – and this is called
the auditory cortex – is located in the temporal lobe, behind Broca’s area. He, therefore,
suspected a relation between the functioning of hearing and speech, and he described cases
of aphasic patients with lesions in this auditory projection area that differed from those
1821
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
described by Broca. Like Broca, he conceived aphasia as a disorder of the sign function
of language. He denied that aphasic patients were necessarily impaired in intellect even
though, as a clinician, he knew that many aphasics, perhaps a majority, did in fact show
cognitive defects that extended beyond the realm of language. But he insisted that
“nothing could be worse for the study of aphasia than to consider the intellectual
disturbance associated with aphasia as an essential part of the disease picture”.
However, Wernicke went beyond his empirical discovery and his restriction of
aphasia to a disorder of the sign function of language to create a model of the neurological
mechanisms, derangements of which produced aphasic disorder. His model, and the
revision of it, developed by other neurologists, postulated the existence of interconnected
cerebral centers of speech in which memory-images of the different modalities of speech
were stored. A center for memory-images of the movement patterns of expressive speech
was located in Broca’s area in the posterior frontal region. A center for auditory memory-
images of words was located in Wernicke’s area in the posterior temporal lobe. A center
for visual memory-images of words was located further back in the angular gyrus.
Whether or not there was a specific center for memory-images of the movements patterns
of writing was a subject of debate. Those who believed in the existence of such a center
placed it either in the second frontal gyrus above Broca’s area or in the supramarginal
gyrus close to the center for visual memory-images of words (Benton, 2000).
In any case, Wernicke provided the first model for how language is organized in the
left hemisphere. It hypothesizes a programmed sequence of activities in Wernicke’s and
Broca’s language areas. Wernicke proposed that auditory information is sent to the
temporal lobes from the ear. In Wernicke’s area, sounds are turned into sound images or
ideas of objects and stored. From Wernicke’s area, the ideas can be sent through a
pathway called the arcuate fasciculus to Broca’s area, where the representations of speech
movements are retained. From Broca’s area, instructions are sent to muscles that control
movements of the mouth to produce the appropriate sound.
If the temporal lobe were damaged, speech movements could still be mediated by
Broca’s area, but the speech would make no sense, because the person would be unable
to monitor the words.
Because damage to Broca’s area produces loss of speech movements without the
loss of sound images, Broca’s aphasia is not accompanied by a loss of understanding
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Wernicke’s aphasia may be considered a syndrome opposite to Broca’s aphasia, in
that expressive speech is fluent but comprehension is impaired. The speech pattern is
effortless and sometimes even excessively fluent (logorrhea). A speaker of a foreign
language would notice nothing amiss, but a listener who shares the patient’s language
detects speech empty of meaning, containing verbal paraphasias, neologisms, and jargon
productions. Neurologists refer to this pattern as paragrammatism. In milder cases, the
intended meaning of an utterance may be discerned, but the sentence goes away with
paraphasic substitutions (Kirshner, 2000). Naming in Wernicke’s aphasia is deficient,
often bizarre with paraphasic substitutions for the correct name. Auditory comprehension
1822
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
is impaired, sometimes even for the simple nonsense questions. Auditory perception of
phonemes is deficient in Wernicke’s aphasia, but deficient semantics is the major cause
of the comprehension disturbance; disturbed access to semantics and to the internal
lexicon is central to the deficit of Wernicke’s aphasia. Although the patients were able
to hear, they could not understand or repeat what was said to them. Reading compre-
hension is usually affected, but occasional patients show greater deficit in one modality.
Writing is also impaired, but in a manner quite different from that of Broca’s aphasia
(Kirshner, 2000).
The diverse symptom-pictures of aphasia encountered in clinical practice were
explained in terms of either a lesion in one or more centers (i.e., the “central” aphasia) or
a lesion in the connections between them (i.e., the “conduction” aphasia). Temporal lobe
aphasia is sometimes called fluent aphasia, to emphasize that the person can say words.
It is more frequently called Wernicke’s aphasia, however, in honor of Wernicke’s
description. The region of the temporal lobe associated with the aphasia is called
Wernicke’s area.
Most patients have no elementary motor or sensory deficits, although a partial or
complete right homonymous hemianopsia may be present.
The psychiatric manifestations of Wernicke’s aphasia are quite different from those
of Broca’s aphasia.
Depression is less common; many Wernicke’s aphasics seem unaware of, or
unconcerned about their communicative deficits. With time, some patients become angry
or paranoid about the inability of family members and medical staff to understand them.
The lesions of patients with Wernicke’s aphasia are usually in the posterior portion
of the superior temporal gyrus, sometimes extending into the inferior parietal lobe.
Damage to Wernicke’s area (Brodmann’s area 22) has been reported to correlate
most closely with persistent loss of comprehension of single words, although others
(Kertesz et al., 1993) have found only larger temporoparietal lesions in patients with
lasting Wernicke’s aphasia.
A receptive speech area in the left inferior temporal gyrus has been suggested by
electrical stimulation studies and by a few descriptions of patients with seizures involving
this area (Kirshner et al., 1995), but aphasia has not been recognized with destructive
lesions of this area.
Extension of the lesion into the inferior parietal region may predict greater
involvement of reading comprehension.
Wernicke also predicted a new language disorder although he never saw such a case.
He suggested that, if the arcuate fibres connecting the two speech areas (Wernicke‘s and
Broca’s area) were cut, disconnecting the areas but without inflicting damage on either
one, a speech deficit that Wernicke described as conduction aphasia would result.
Conductive aphasia has been advanced as a classical disconnection syndrome. Geschwind
later pointed to the arcuate fasciculus, a white matter tract traveling from the deep
temporal lobe, around the sylvian fissure to the frontal lobe, as the site of disconnection
(Geschwind, 1965). Conduction aphasia is an uncommon but theoretically important
1823
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
syndrome that can be remembered by its striking deficit of repetition. Most patients have
relatively normal spontaneous speech, although some make literal paraphasic errors and
hesitate frequently for self-correction. Naming may be impaired, but auditory
comprehension is preserved. A patient who can express himself at a sentence level and
comprehend conversation may be unable to repeat even single words. Reading and
writing are somewhat variable, but reading aloud may share some of the same difficulty
as repeating.
Associated deficits include hemianopsia in some patients; right-sided sensory loss
may be present, but right hemiparesis is usually mild or absent (Kirshner, 2000).
The lesions of conduction aphasia are usually in either the superior temporal or
inferior parietal regions. Patients with limb apraxia have parietal lesions (supramarginal
gyrus). The supramarginal gyrus appears to be involved in auditory immediate memory
and in phoneme generation. Lesions in this area are associated with conduction aphasia
and phonemic paraphasic errors.
Wernicke proposed that, although different regions of the brain have different
function they are interdependent in that to work, because they must receive information
from one another. Using this same reasoning, French neurologist Joseph Déjérine (1849-
1917), in 1892, described a case in which the loss of the ability to read (alexia, meaning
“word blindness”, from Greek lexia, for “word”) resulted from a disconnection between
the visual area of the brain and Wernicke’s area. So, Déjérine (1892) elucidated the
neuroanatomic substrate of pure alexia on the basis of autopsy study of a patient with
this disorder. His patient, an educated man and an accomplished musician, suddenly lost
the ability to read musical scores as well as conventional written material. Yet he could
write, perform music from memory, and showed no difficulty in the expression or
understanding of oral speech. He had a right visual field defect – in all probability a
hemiachromatopsia, not a hemianopsia. Autopsy study disclosed infarctions in the
territory of the left posterior cerebral artery, specifically, the mesial occipital area and
the splenium of the corpus callosum. Déjérine inferred that the lesions had the effects of
preventing the transmission of visual information to the language centers of the left
hemisphere, thus making reading impossible while leaving the interpretation of nonverbal
visual stimuli intact.
Déjérine and others accept the proposition that aphasia involves a defect in
intelligence.
Neurolinguists and cognitive psychologists have divided alexias according to
breakdowns in specific stages of the reading process. The linguistic concepts of surface
structure versus the deep meanings of words have been instrumental in these new
classifications. Four patterns of alexia (or dyslexia, in British usage) have been
recognized: letter-by-letter, deep, phonological, and surface dyslexia.
Like reading, writing may be affected either in isolation (pure agraphia) or in
association with aphasia (aphasic agraphia). In addition, writing can be impaired by motor
disorders, by apraxia, and by visuospatial deficits. Isolated agraphia has been described
with left frontal or parietal lesions.
1824
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Geschwind
Norman Geschwind (1926-1984) presented his ideas in a now classic paper,
“Disconnexion Syndromes in Animals and Man”, which was published in 1965. This
comprehensive analysis, some 100 pages in length, presented the concept of discon-
nection and illustrated its far-reaching implications. Its impact was immediate and far-
reaching. In a real sense, it brought neuroanatomy back into the field of aphasia, apraxia,
and agnosia, and it provided a fruitful approach to the understanding of these types of
disorders, as well as other behavioral manifestations of brain disease.
Today, when we undertake to analyze the mechanisms underlying aphasic disorders,
amnesic syndromes, perceptual defects, and visuomotor disabilities, we think in terms
of disconnection – not simple disconnection, to be sure, but rather of disturbances of
progression and interaction in information processing that are based on the destruction
of specific pathways (Benton, 2000).
Wernicke’s speech model was updated by American neurologist Norman Geschwind
in the 1960s and is now sometimes referred to as the Wernicke-Geschwind model.
reasoning or symbolic thinking. They are generally more impaired than are nonaphasic
patients with left-hemisphere disease.
Among aphasic patients, those with significant defects in oral verbal comprehension
are most likely to show defective nonverbal test performance.
Moreover, certain nonverbal tasks, such as the identification of environmental
sounds and pantomime recognition, are failed only by aphasics (and perhaps severely
demented patients) but not nonaphasics with brain disease (Vignolo, 1969; Duffy et al.,
1975; Varney, 1978; Varney and Benton, 1982; Dănăilă and Golu, 2006).
However, the overriding finding (often overlooked in intergroup comparisons) is
one of extreme interindividual variability among aphasic patients, even among those with
severe disturbances in comprehension. This variability indicates that in virtually every
sample of aphasics some will perform normally, a fact which has been convincingly
documented in case reports (Alajouanine and Lhermitte, 1964; Zangwill, 1964).
In addition, there is no evidence for a significant relationship between the degree of
deficit in nonverbal task performance and the severity of aphasic disorder (Basso et al.,
1973), in part because stroke-produced aphasia is such an important cause of socio-
economic disability. At the same time, neurodiagnostic procedures, such as CT, magnetic
resonance imaging, functional magnetic resonance imaging, magnetoencephalography,
and positron-emission tomography, permit investigators to collect a substantial amount
of clinicopathologic data fairly quickly and new discoveries have modified concepts
about classification and lesional localization.
functionally, although each is involved more or less directly with behavior output (Stuss
and Benson, 1986; Pandya and Barnes, 1987; Goldberg, 1990; Damasio, 1991; Stuss et
al., 1994; Pandya and Yeterian, 1998).
Asymmetries in language
In the last two decades, functional imaging has provided a revealing view of areas
involved in healthy and neurologically impaired subjects (Roland, 1984; Petersen et al.,
1988; Binder et al., 1995; Klein et al., 1995; Warburton et al., 1996). Most observant
clinicians have remarked on the variability and overlap of aphasic syndromes, especially
in the immediate period following brain injury, as well as the individual differences in
symptoms between patients with apparently similar lesions (Benson, 1986; Galaburda et
al., 1990). In light of the individual variability in gross morphology in humans
(Andrianov, 1979; Rajkowska and Goldman-Rakic, 1995) there is a left-hemisphere
superiority and proficiency for the majority of vocal, motor, and language function
(Geschwind, 1970; Benson, 1986).
However, some language capacity exists in most right hemispheres (Dănăilă and
Golu, 1987; Iacoboni and Zaidel, 1996). Especially, is the predominance of the right
frontal lobe in the production of the melodic components that contribute to prosody, as
well as the expression of the emotional content of language.
Because spoken and written language are human specializations, detailed animal
models of the role of different frontal subregions serving language are not available
(Geschwind and Iacoboni, 2007).
This is in contrast to frontal lobe participation in other cognitive functions, such as
working memory and sensorimotor integration, in which studies in primates have vastly
accelerated our knowledge of regional subspecializations and provided models that can
be tested in humans (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Funahashi et al., 1989; Wilson et al., 1993;
Petrides, 1994).
Broca’s original belief that lesions confine to the posterior portion of the third left
frontal gyrus caused loss of articulatory function (aphémie) occurred on the background
of conviction (Broca, 1861 a, b, c; 1864 a, b; 1865), shared by his contemporaries, that
the left and right frontal lobes were identical in size and anatomy (Flourens, 1824; Broca,
1865; Berker et al., 1986).
The recovery of language function occurred through the compensatory effort of
homologous, essentially equipotential regions in the right frontal lobe (Geschwind and
Iacoboni, 2007). Following Broca, numerous cases supported the left hemispheric loca-
lization of language in right-handers, while expanding the cerebral territory responsible
for language functions (Wernicke, 1874; Jackson, 1880, 1915; Broca, 1888).
Jackson (1868) presented the first case of the left-handed man with aphasia and right-
sided lesions, further supporting between hand dominance and language lateralization.
Afterwards studies have confirmed the functional localization of language to the left
hemisphere in 99% of right-handers (Hécaen et al., 1981; Annett, 1985; Benson, 1986).
However, this relationship is less certain in left-handers, with most demonstrating either
1828
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
have also demonstrated that lesions localized to Broca’s area lead to nonfluent aphasia
or nonmotor articulatory disturbance, as well as persistent apraxia and dysprosody, but
not frank agrammatism, as is paradigmatic in many current formulations of nonfluent
(Broca’s) aphasia. Those with typical Broca’s aphasia have larger lesions that encom-
passed deeper white matter structures, anterior insula, and adjacent perisylvian regions
(Mohr et al., 1978; Alexander et al., 1990).
Morphological asymmetries. Comparison of the weights of both hemispheres
yielded variable and inconclusive results (Thurman, 1866; Broca, 1875; Aresu, 1914;
Von Bonin, 1962). However, the left hemisphere is greater than the right, suggesting
more cortical surface area overall on the left (Von Bonin, 1962). Indirect measurements
taken of indentations in the skull, called petalias, reflect outgrowth of the adjacent cerebral
hemisphere. Thus, the presence of marked left occipital petalia, the nature of frontal lobe
asymmetries has been less obvious (Tinley, 1927; Hadziselmovic and Cus, 1966).
However, most careful quantitative studies in adequate numbers of cases show a predo-
minance of the right frontal petalia (Hadziselmovic and Cus, 1966; Geschwind and
Galaburda, 1985).
Le May and Kido (1978) made direct measurements of the frontal lobes and
demonstrated that the width of the right frontal region was greater in 58% of right-
handed patients and extended further in 31%, as opposed to only 14% that extend
further forward on the left. This gross structural asymmetry has been consistently
observed in more recent studies (Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985; Bear et al., 1986;
Glickson and Myslobodsky, 1993).
However, the meaning of these observations is unclear, because they do not reflect
the total extent of cortical surface area because much of cortical surface area is contained
in the sulcal folds. This explanation is likely to hold for the studies of Wada et al. (1975),
that demonstrated a right-side size advantage when only the lateral cortical surfaces of
areas 44 and 45 were measured (Fig. 23.1).
Functional imaging
Positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging
(fMRI) have supported the functional specialization of the left frontal cortex in language
and language-related tasks (Frith et al., 1991; Mc Carthy et al., 1993; Binder et al., 1995;
Klein et al., 1995; Just et al., 1996). Indeed, widespread areas of lateral frontal
hypometabolism are even seen in patients with aphasia and lesions in parietal and
temporal cortex, further implicating the lateral left frontal cortex in language function
and recovery (Metter, 1991).
1831
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Even simple language tasks, although highly lateralized, activate a network of widely
distributed left-hemisphere cortical areas (Petersen et al., 1988; Binder et al., 1995; Just
et al., 1996). In most careful PET of fMRI studies of language, homologous regions are
often activated on the right side, although typically at far lower levels than those on the
left (Habib et al., 1996; Just et al. 1996; Warburton et al., 1996). The left-hemisphere
activations are highly variable and extend beyond Broca’s region, including the supple-
mentary motor area, and cingulate medially, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and
the premotor area laterally.
One of the factors confounding the interpretation of PET language data is the
variability in activated areas across different studies. In spite of this variability, left
perisylvian regions in general, and Broca’s area in particular show consistent activation
in language tasks. So, the Broca’s area is a critical structure dedicated solely to language
output. Since Broca’s region comprises cytoarchitectonically and physiologically diverse
areas, it may serve several language-related functions (Poppel 1996).
Thus, lesions studies, intraoperative electrical stimulation, and PET imaging studies
confirm its role in phonological process (Lecours and Lhermitte, 1970; Denny-Brown
1975; Ojemann and Mateer, 1979; Demonet et al., 1994; Zatorre et al., 1996).
PET data indicate that Broca’s region is activated in a wide variety of non-output-
related language tasks, including listening tasks (Roland, 1984).
Phonological discrimination tasks often engage verbal working memory functions,
which are typically associated with left frontal lobe predominance as well (Milner and
Petrides, 1984; Paulescu et al., 1993; Petrides et al., 1993).
Thus, Broca’s region comprises contiguous area serving separate functions that can
be simultaneously engaged in the same task.
Denny-Brown (1965, 1975) emphasize the importance of visual input in language
acquisition and visual influences on aphasia caused by lesions of Broca’s area.
Thus, disruption of the integration of these visual inputs is a component of the lateral
alexia that can sometimes be observed in patients with Broca’s area lesions (Benson,
1977; Boccardi et al., 1984).
In sum, the functional supremacy of one cerebral hemisphere is crucial to language
function. There are many ways of determining that the left side of the brain is dominant:
(1) by the loss of speech that occurs with disease in certain parts of the left hemisphere
and its preservation with lesions involving corresponding parts of the right hemisphere;
(2) by preference, for greater facility in the use of the right hand, foot, and eye; (3) by
the arrest of speech with magnetic cortical stimulation or a focal seizure or with electrical
stimulation of the anterior language area during surgical procedure; (4) by the injection
of sodium amytal into the left internal carotid artery (Wada test – a procedure that
produces mutism for a minute or two, followed by misnaming, including preservation
and substitution; misreading; and paraphasic speech – effects lasting 8 to 9 min in all);
(5) by the dichotic listening test, in which different words or phonemes are presented
simultaneously to two ears (yielding a right ear – left hemisphere advantage); (6) by
observing increases in cerebral blood flow during language processing: (7) by
1832
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
In the foreign accent syndrome, which can result from left frontal lesions involving
Broca’s area, and neighboring cortical and subcortical regions, inappropriate syllable
stress, phoneme misproduction, rhythm, and pauses occur, changing a patient’s accent
often without chronically altering other aspects of language (Monrad-Krohn, 1947).
Exaggerated prosody, sometimes observed in nonfluent aphasia, reflects the
speaker’s attempts to communicate using retained right frontal abilities in the face of
minimal linguistic capabilities (Geschwind and Iacoboni, 2007).
The role of orbital frontal lobes in the regulation of emotion and mood has been well
established in studies of patients with brain injury (Benson and Stuss, 1986) and tumors.
The orbitofrontal cortex is the cortex of the ventral aspect of the frontal lobe. It
comprises mainly areas 11 and 13. The orbital prefrontal syndrome can ensue from a
variety of disease processes, including tumors (Fig. 23.2), and aneurisms of the anterior
communicating artery (Fig. 23.3).
Attention is disturbed mainly in its exclusionary aspect. The patient is unable to
suppress interference from external stimuli or internal tendencies.
Imitation of other and utilization behavior – the compulsion to utilize objects or tools
prompted simply by their presence – may be symptoms related to that lack of interference
control (Lhermitte et al., 1986)
Orbitofrontal hypermotility is the opposite of the hypomotility and aspontaneity of
the apathetic syndrome from lateral and medial lesions. In a substantial number of patients
the prevalent effect is euphoria, often accompanied by irritability and a contentious,
paranoid stance (Cummings, 1985).
A B
Fig. 23.2. A preoperative contrast enhanced computed tomographic (CT) scan of a 56-year-old man shows a giant
size olfactory grove meningioma and displacement of anterior and fronto-basal frontal lobes (A). The patient presents
an orbitofrontal syndrome. Postoperative contrast-enhanced CT-scan showing no residual tumor (B) and orbitofrontal
syndrome disappeared.
1834
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
A B
Fig. 23.3 (A) The left carotid angiogram demonstrates an ACoA aneurysm which projects inferiorly.
(B) Postoperative angiography demonstrated complete obliteration of the aneurysm with a clip.
Instincts are disinhibited and normal judgement impaired. These patients may show
by their behavior a blatant disregard for even the most elementary ethical principles.
Thus, orbitofrontal syndrome is oftentimes undistinguishable from mania.
Criminal sociopathy is another psychiatric condition analogous in some respect to
the orbitofrontal syndrome (Gorenstein, 1982; Lapierre et al., 1995).
Left frontal damage, especially damage to the anterior frontal lobes, is far more likely
to cause depression than similar lesions on the right (Gainotti, 1972; Sackeim et al., 1982;
Robinson et al., 1984; Starkstein et al., 1991). Lesions on the right more frequently lead
to mania (Jorge et al., 1993), especially regions of the orbitofrontal cortex (Starkstein et
al., 1987, 1989). In healthy subjects, left prefrontal cortex cerebral blood flow increases
when patients induce a state of dysphoria by thinking sad thoughts (Pardo et al., 1993;
George et al., 1995).
A transcranial magnetic stimulation (causing transient hypofunctioning) of the left,
but not right prefrontal cortex, resulted in decreased self-report of happiness and a
significant increase in sadness rating (Pascual-Leone et al., 1996).
Electrophysiological evidence suggests that the left frontal lobe is more specialized
for positive emotions related to approach and exploratory mechanisms and the right for
negative avoidance-related reactions (Davidson, 1992; Davidson and Sutton, 1995).
Prefrontal cortex
The lateral prefrontal cortex is the prefrontal cortex of the lateral convexity of frontal
lobe. It comprises part or entirely of areas 8, 9, 10 and 46. It is known to be primarily
involved in working memory processes (Goldman-Rakic, 1987). Working memory is the
ability to retain an item of information for the prospective execution of an action that is
dependent on that information. It is an essential cognitive function for the mediation of
cross-temporal contingencies in the temporal integration of reasoning, speech, and goal-
directed behavior (Fuster, 2009). Working memory, however, can fail in many patho-
logical conditions of the brain. The reason why is failure especially evident and
consistently found in the frontal patient is because that kind of memory is necessary for
prospective action, whether the action is a motor act, a mental operation, or a piece of
spoken language (Fuster, 2009).
Judging from the effects of a prefrontal lesion, working-memory of all modalities
seems distributed through lateral cortex, without anatomical compartmentalization for
spatial working memory (Müller et al., 2002; Müller and Knight, 2006).
However, the reversible transient lesions by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
have been reported to segregate spatial and non-spatial deficits to the dorsal and ventral
lateral cortex, respectively (Mottaghy et al., 2002).
A frontal eye-field (area 8) lesion leads to deficit of working memory for ocular
saccades (Ploner et al. 1999). Left-frontal patients have the most difficulty with working
memory of verbal items, and right-frontal patients with non-verbal areas.
Right-visuospatial specialization, in patients with right frontal lesions is more likely
to demonstrate poor use and representation of visuospatial data in a variety of tasks that
require working memory, and those with left frontal lesions are more likely to have
disordered memory for episodic information (Kolb and Whishaw, 1985; Milner, 1995).
Of patients with unilateral frontal damage, those with right frontal damage show the
poorest performance in design fluency tasks (Benson and Stuss, 1986).
Frontal patients, unlike temporal patients, have been noted to perform poorly in
spatial as well as nonspatial conditional association tests probably because of the working
memory component that those tests contain (Petrides, 1985).
Interference and the failure to control interference clearly play a role in the memory
deficit of the frontal patients.
This has been demonstrated by use of memory tasks with proper control of
interference factor (Oscar-Berman et al., 1991; Stuss, 1991; Chao and Knight, 1995; Ptito
et al., 1995).
So, in patients with brain injury, left frontal lobe damage typically leads to more
profound verbal recall deficits than right-sided damage, whereas right frontal damage
causes deficits primarily in categorization (Milner and Petrides, 1984; Incisa della
Rocchetta, 1986; Incisa della Rocchetta and Milner, 1993).
Deficits in the retrieval of verbal material in patients with left frontal damage may
be specific to certain lexical categories, in that injury to the left, but not to the right,
premotor areas produced a specific deficit in verb, but not noun retrieval (Damasio and
Tranel, 1993).
1836
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Later, even conventional delay tasks were shown to be impaired after a frontal lesion
(Milner et al., 1985; Freedman and Oscar-Berman, 1986; Oscar-Berman et al., 1991;
Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1991; Verin et al., 1993; Dubois et al., 1995).
Other neuropsychological studies, further substrate the prefrontal working-memory
deficits in digit span tests (Vidor, 1951; Hamlin, 1970, Janowsky et al. 1989 a; Stuss,
1991), in certain recognition tests (Milner and Teuber, 1968), and in temporal order and
recency tests (Milner, 1971, 1982; Milner et al., 1985; Shimamura et al., 1990; Kesner
et al., 1994; Jurado et al., 1997, 1998; Marshuetz, 2005).
PET studies have shown that listening to digits activates the left dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex in normal subjects when active monitoring and manipulation of external
information held in memory are required only to make judgements about the same stimuli,
and no active manipulation is required (Petrides et al., 1993). Similarly, visuospatial
information activates the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in normal subjects when
active manipulation and monitoring of information are required.
The same visuospatial information activates only the right ventrolateral prefrontal
cortex when only “reproduction” of information without active manipulation and
monitoring is demanded by the task (Owen et al., 1996).
The general patterns of lateralization of verbal working memory function to the left
frontal lobe and of visuospatial working memory function to the right frontal lobe, are
consistent with the clinical lesion data (Smith et al., 1996).
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex seems to be a critical structure in a number of
delayed response and conditional sensorimotor learning tasks in nonhuman primates
(Goldman-Rakic, 1987).
Neurophysiological evidence showed that the neuronal discharge in dorsolateral
prefrontal neurons of monkeys performing conditional sensorimotor tasks is dependent
upon the learning component of the task (Fuster, 1995).
Learning related rCBF (regional cerebral blood flow) increases seem to be lateralized
to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex even when learning effects in conditional motor
tasks are largely parallel in both hands (Iacoboni et al., 1996 b). This suggests that transfer
of learning might occur through the anterior regions of the corpus callosum, intercon-
necting the prefrontal cortex of the two cerebral hemispheres (Iacoboni and Zaidel, 1995).
Working memory can be characterized as sustained attention to an internal
representation (Fuster, 2009).
Working memory is subject to destructibility and interference, which are likely after
prefrontal damage.
In a “rich” environment, with plentiful stimuli and distractors the frontal patient’s
memory is more likely to fail than in a quiet and simple environment. Just as critically,
interference may come from whithin – that is from the reservoir of memories and alter-
natives that the subject has experienced or is likely to experience in that particular context.
The greater the similarity between these and the memory currently “on line“, the
greater is the probability that they will interfere with it (Fuster, 2009).
1838
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Premotor cortex
Situated just anterior to the precentral area, the premotor (area 6) and supplementary
motor areas have been identified as the site in which the integration of motor skills and
learned action sequences takes place (Kolb and Whishaw, 1996; Nilson et al., 2000;
Eslinger and Geddes, 2001; Damasio and Anderson, 2003). The concept of a premotor
cortex was first proposed in 1905 by Campbell, who called it the intermediate precentral
cortex. The term premotor cortex was first used by Hines in 1949.
Independent anatomical and physiological evidence in nonhuman primates (Matelli
et al., 1985; Fogassi et al., 1996; Fujii et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1996) and PET data
in humans (Iacoboni et al., 1996 a; Rizzolatti et al., 1996) support the division of premotor
cortex into four fields: a rostral (PMdr) and a caudal (PMdc ) field in the dorsal premotor
cortex, and a rostral (PMvr) and a caudal (PMvc) field in the ventral premotor cortex.
Neurophysiological evidence from studies of nonhuman primates suggests that PMdr
is associated with saccade-, arm-, and eye-, eye position-, and stimulus-related activity,
whereas PMdc is associated with arm motor preparation- and arm movement-related activity
(Fujii et al., 1996). The ventral premotor cortex seems to be associated with grasp
representations and action recognition in PMvr (Rizzolatti et al., 1996), and with peripersonal
space coding of somatosensory and visual stimuli in PMvc (Fogassi et al., 1996).
The medial premotor cortex is the so-called supplementary motor area (SMA) of
Woolsey et al. (1952). In both premotor areas, arcuate (prearcuate area 8 and postarcuate
area 6) and SMA, there is a degree of somatotopical organization – that is, differential
representation of the body (Woolsey et al., 1952; Muakkassa and Strick ,1979). Premotor
areas participate in afferent / efferent loop with the basal ganglia and the thalamus: the
looped interconnections are probably targeted to specific sites on both cortical and
subcortical structures (Passingham, 1997; Middleton and Strick, 2000).
The dorsal premotor cortex is traditionally associated with neglect in extrapersonal
space with selection and preparation of movements guided by external sensory stimuli,
and with the retrieval of responses associated with specific sensory stimuli (Halsband
and Freund, 1990; Passingham, 1993).
1839
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The primary motor cortex is the site of origin of about 30% to 40% of the fibres in
the pyramidal tract.
Furthermore, all the large-diameter axons (approximately 3% of pyramidal fibres)
originate from the giant motor neurons (of Betz) in the primary motor cortex.
Most of the neurons contributing fibres to corticospinal tract have glutamate or
aspartate as their excitatory neurotransmitter (Holmes and May, 1909; Levin and
Bradford, 1938; Lassek, 1940, 1954).
Stimulation of the motor cortex in conscious humans gives rise to discrete and
isolated contralateral movement limited to a single joint or a single muscle.
Bilateral responses are seen in extraocular muscles and muscles of the face, tongue,
jaw, larynx, and pharynx.
Thus, the primary motor area corresponds to the precentral gyrus (area 4 of
Brodmann). On the medial surface of the hemisphere, the primary motor area comprises
the anterior part of the paracentral lobule.
The contralateral half of the body is represented in the primary motor area in a
precise but disproportionate manner, giving rise to the motor homunculus in the same
way as that described for the primary somesthetic cortex.
The primary motor cortex functions in the initiation of highly skilled fine movements.
The motor area receives fibres from the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus, the
main projection area of the cerebellum. The motor area also receives fibres from the
somesthetic cortex (areas 1, 2 and 5) and the supplementary motor cortex.
The connections between the primary motor and somesthetic cortices are reciprocal.
Although the primary motor cortex is not the sole area from which movement can
be elicited, it is nevertheless characterized by initiating highly skilled movement at a
lower threshold of stimulation than the other motor areas.
One of the most striking lateralized behaviors in humans is hand preference, which
is typically associated with manual skill (Annett, 1985). Fine manual coordination is
lateralized to the left motor cortex in most right-handed individuals (Liepmann and Mass,
1907; Annett, 1985; Goldberg, 1985).
Some fMRI studies in human demonstrate asymmetric activation of primary motor
cortex during volitional fine movements of the hand (Kawashima et al., 1993; Kim et
al., 1993).
Stepniewska et al. (1993) and Geyer et al. (1996) demonstrated that the primary
motor cortex in human and nonhuman primates is divided functionally into a rostral sector
and a caudal sector. However, the functional asymmetries of the primary motor cortex
do not differentiate between rostral and caudal sectors (Fuster, 2007).
Hemispatial neglect is typically associated with right temporal-parietal lesions, but
it is observed with right frontal lobe lesions as well (Heilman and Valenstein, 1972;
Heilman et al., 1993).
Patients with right pre-Rolandic lesions, often encompassing primary motor areas,
tend not to move the hand ipsilateral to the lesion in the contralateral hemispace (Bisiach
et al., 1990) and exhibit motor impersistences well, which is thought to reflect an
1841
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
attentional deficit (Benson and Stuss, 1986). Paradoxically, these patients tend to neglect
lines seen on the left under free vision, and lines seen on the right under mirror-reversed
vision (Bisiach et al., 1995). Furthermore, a double dissociation in patients with unilateral
neglect is often seen.
Some patients have unilateral neglect only for near space; others have unilateral
neglect only for far space (Halligan and Marshall, 1991). This double dissociation
suggests that the representations of extrapersonal and personal space are differentiated
and segregated in the human brain.
In the nonhuman primate, there is evidence for two parietal-frontal circuits sub-
serving extrapersonal and personal space. A dorsal parietal-frontal circuit comprises area
7a, lateral intraparietal area, and dorsal premotor cortex, and codes extrapersonal space.
A ventral parietal-frontal circuit comprises area 7b, anterior intraparietal area, and
ventral premotor cortex, and codes personal space.
These two circuits are anatomically largely independent, but both input to primary
motor cortex (Passingham, 1993).
A PET observation has suggested that the right motor cortex is a critical structure in
mapping extrapersonal onto personal space (Iacoboni et al., 1997).
A number of fMRI studies have suggested a major role for the primary motor cortex
in motor learning (Grafton, 1995). Most of these studies have resulted in lateralized
activation of the left primary motor cortex. The only fMRI studies of which Iacoboni et
al. (1996) are aware that have used an unbiased learning paradigm in which left- and
right-hand motor activity was completely counterbalanced (Iacoboni et al., 1996 a, b;
Iacoboni et al., 1997) have resulted in blood flow increases consistently lateralized to the
left frontal lobe. These blood flow increases occurred mainly in dorsal premotor cortex
and in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and only sporadically in primary motor cortex
(Iacoboni et al., 1996 b).
In the macaque, three areas, buried in the cingulate sulcus, seem to have significance
in sensorimotor processes: the rostral cingulate motor area, located anterior to the genu
of the arcuate sulcus, and the dorsal and the ventral cingulate motor areas, located caudal
to the genu of the arcuate sulcus (Picard and Strick, 1996).
PET findings have suggested at least two cortical fields in the human anterior
cingulate cortex: a large rostral one, anterior to the anterior commissure, associated with
complex sensorimotor tasks and with a somatotopic arrangement; and a small caudate
one, posterior to the anterior commissure, associated with simple tasks and not showing
a clear somatotopy (Picard and Strick, 1996).
Area 24 is at the crossroads of pathways linking the limbic system with the frontal
lobe, and, at the same time, is one of the so-called “suppressor areas”, which upon
stimulation induce general muscular hypotonia (Smith, 1945).
The anterior cingulate has been associated with attentional functions (Bench et al.,
1993; Posner and Dehaene, 1994). However, asymmetries in the anterior cingulate in
attentional mechanisms have not been systematically described (Pardo et al., 1991).
Furthermore, widespread areas of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex are preferentially
activated during a variety of attentional tasks (Bench et al., 1993; Vendrell et al., 1995;
Lewin et al., 1996).
Picard and Strick (1996) observed a slower learning in right hand in subjects
practicing in a random fashion, associated with blood flow increases in the left SMA-
proper and the left rostral anterior cingulate area, in a region overlapping with the arm
representation in the human anterior cingulate cortex. Thus, behavioral data and rCBF
findings suggest a greater sensitivity of the left rostral cingulate region to contextual cues.
This is in line with a general role of the cingulate cortex in context-specific learning in
other mammals (Freeman et al., 1996).
The most striking asymmetry in the anterior cingulate region is at morphological
level. In the left hemisphere, there are often two cerebral sulci in the cingulate region,
the cingulate sulcus and the paracingulate sulcus, whereas in the right hemisphere, there
1843
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
is generally only one sulcus, the cingulate sulcus. It has been speculated that this
asymmetry might be related to certain aspects of effortful versus automatic vocalization
(Paus et al., 1996).
Indeed, the PET study that compared reversed speech (effortful) with overpracticed
speech, foci of activation were largely observed overlapping with the paracingulate sulcus
in the left hemisphere (Paus et al., 1993).
Lesions of the anterior cingulate region generally lead to hypokinesia or akinesia,
depending on their size (Kreindler, Macovei, Cardas and Dănăilă, 1966; Meador et al.,
1986; Verfaellie and Heilman, 1987). They are also frequently accompanied by defective
self-monitoring of behavior and of the ability to correct errors.
Akinetic mutism often results from massive bilateral lesions (Kreindler, Macovei
Cardas and Dănăilă, 1966). It is usually accompanied by severe neurovegetative
deterioration (Fuster, 2009). Some patients with lesions of the anterior cingulate region
have been noted to suffer from cataplexy (Ethelberg, 1950) – that is the paroxysmal and
general loss of muscle tonus commonly induced by strong emotion (Levin, 1953).
Once, the global adynamia results from irritation of area 24 (Fuster, 1955).
Mutism and apathy are the most prevalent disorders of medial frontal damage,
especially if that damage is large. Thus, subjects with a large medial lesion appear
characteristically unaware of their own condition (Nielsen and Jacobs, 1951; Barris and
Schuman, 1953).
Supplementary motor area. The supplementary motor area (SMA) is located on
the medial surface of the frontal lobe, anterior to the medial extension of the primary
motor cortex (area 4). It corresponds roughly to the medial extension of area 6 of
Brodmann. Although the existence of a motor area in the medial aspect of the frontal
cortex rostral to the precentral area of primates has long been known.
Penfield and Welch (1951) were the first to call this portion of the cortex the
supplementary motor area.
A homunculus has been defined for the supplementary motor area in which face and
upper limbs are represented rostral to the lower limb and trunk. Stimulation in humans
give risk to complex movement in preparation for the assumption of characteristic
postures (Brinkman and Porter, 1979; Tanji, 1994).
Two distinct areas can be differentiated in SMA: a rostral area called pre-SMA and
a caudal area called SMA-proper. In macaques, the pre-SMA is located mainly anterior
to the genu of the arcuate sulcus, whereas SMA-proper is located posterior to the genu
of the arcuate sulcus. In the human brain, the pre-SMA is located rostral to the level of
the anterior commissure, and the SMA-proper is located caudal to the level of the anterior
commissure. Anatomical and neurophysiological evidence suggest that the pre-SMA is
related to selection and preparation of movements, whereas the SMA-proper is more
related to aspects of motor execution (Picard and Strick, 1996). Although simple motor
tasks are elicited from stimulation of the supplementary motor area, the role of this area
in simple motor tasks is much less significant and is likely to be subsidiary to that of the
primary motor area. On the other hand, the supplementary motor area assumes more
1844
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Anyhow, two different types of experiments have been devised to assess motor
asymmetries: 1) direct observation of motor asymmetry; and 2) interference tasks (Kolb
and Whishaw, 2003).
1) Direct observation
If asymmetry in the control of movement is inherent, this asymmetry might be
observable when people are engaged in other behavior.
For example, perhaps the right hand is more active during the performance of verbal
tasks, which do not require a manual response, and the left hand is more active during
the performance of nonverbal tasks, such as listening to music, which also do not require
a manual response. To examine this possibility, Kimura (1964 and 1973) videotaped
subjects talking or humming. They found that right-handed people tend to gesture with
their right hands when talking but are equally likely to scratch, rub their noses, or touch
their bodies with either hand. Kimura interpreted the observed gesturing with the limb
contralateral to the speaking hemisphere to indicate a relation between speech and certain
manual activities.
Differences in gesturing, which favor the right hand in right-handed subjects, could
simply be due to a difference in preferred hand rather than to functional asymmetry in
motor control.
A second observed motor asymmetry was reported in the performance of complex
movements of the mouth. Wolf and Goodale (1987) did single-frame analyses of
videotaped mouth movements produced when people make verbal or nonverbal sounds.
These observations support the idea that the left hemisphere has a special role in the
selection, programming, and production of verbal and nonverbal oral movements.
Considerable evidence shows that the left side of the face displays emotions more
strongly than the right side, and Wolf and Goodale (1987) showed that the onset facial
expressions occur sooner on the left side of the face. Thus, it is not the control of
movement itself that is asymmetrical but rather movement for a particular purpose.
2) Interference tasks
A variety of interference tasks examine a well-known phenomenon that most people
manifest: the difficulty of doing two complex tasks at the same time. Perhaps the most
interesting interference study known to us is an unpublished experiment by Robert Hicks
and Marcel Kinsbourne (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003). They persuaded several unemployed
musicians to come to their laboratory daily to play the piano. The task was to learn a
different piece of music with each hand so that the two pieces could be played simul-
taneously. When the musicians had mastered this very difficult task, the experimenters
then asked them to speak or to hum while playing.
Speaking disrupted playing with the right hand, and humming disrupted playing
with the left.
Interference studies provide a useful way to study the roles of two hemispheres in
controlling movement, but much more work is needed before researchers can identify
1847
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
the complementary roles of the two hemispheres (see reviews by Murphy and Peters,
1994, and by Carosseli et al., 1997). It will be necessary to identify which types of
movements each hemisphere is especially good at controlling, because these movements
will probably be resilient to interference effects.
Studies of interference effects are intriguing because they may be sources of fresh
insights into the cortical organization of the motor system, but interference effects are
poorly understood and appear to be capricious. In addition, as we become proficient at
motor tasks, we are less prone to interference effects (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Introduction
The most obvious functional difference between the hemispheres is that, for most
people, the left hemisphere is the primary mediator of verbal function, including reading
and writing, understanding and speaking, verbal ideation, verbal memory, numerical
symbol system, and even comprehension of verbal symbols traced on the skin. Processing
the linear and rapidly changing acoustic information needed for speech comprehension
is better with the left than with the right hemisphere (Schwartz and Tallal, 1980; Haward
1997; Beeman and Chiarello, 1998).
Males show a stronger left hemisphere lateralization for phenological processing
than females (Shaywitz et al., 1995; Zaidel et al., 1995).
So, left hemisphere dominance for many aspects of language is the most obvious
and cited asymmetry outside of the motor domain. From clinical neurological data as
well as other sources, it is estimated that speech production is limited to the left
hemisphere in approximately 95% of right-handed individuals. Left hemisphere is also
dominant for many aspects of language perception and for the verbal processing of
stimulus material.
1848
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
emotional tone. So, the right hemisphere is superior to the left in using the intonation
cues of speech to identify emotional tone of voice. The right hemisphere is also involved
in processing narrative-level linguistic information (Hellige, 2001). Some of these
complementary language-related asymmetries may be related to hemispheric difference
in the efficiency of processing different aspects of acoustic signals.
For example, identification of many spoken phonemes requires efficient processing
of rapid changes in the acoustic signal over brief periods of time, a type of processing
for which the left hemisphere is hypothesized to be superior.
In contrast, identification of the emotional tone of voice requires efficient processing
of much slower modulations of the acoustic signal over longer periods of time, a type of
processing for which the right hemisphere is hypothesized to be superior (Heilman, 1997;
Christman, 1997; Hellige, 2000).
So, patients with right hemisphere damage may be quite fluent, even verbose
(Brookshire, 1978; Rivers and Love, 1980; Cutting, 1990), but illogical and given to lose
generalization and bad judgment (Stemmer and Joanette, 1998). They have difficulty in
ordering, organizing and making sense out of complex stimuli or situations, and thus
many display planning defects (Lezak et al., 2004). Verbal comprehension may be
compromised by confusion of the elements of what is heard by personalized intrusions,
by literal interpretations, and by generalized loss of gist in a morass of details (Beeman
and Chiarello, 1998). Their speech may be uninflected and aprosodic, paralleling their
difficulty in comprehending speech intonations (Ross, 2003).
These patients are vulnerable to difficulty in maintaining a high level of alertness
(Ladavas et al., 1989), which may be akin to the association of right hemisphere lesions
with impersistence – the inability to sustain facial or limb posture (Pimental and
Kingsbury, 1989).
Dănăilă et al. (1990) evaluated speech disorders at 15 patients with tumors and
strokes localized in the right hemisphere. They noted there main categories of speech
impairments: disturbances of contents of “semantic structure”, disturbances of forms of
the logical-grammatical structure, and disturbances of dynamics – volume, output,
fluency, rhythm, intonation and writing.
The two hemispheres also appear to access word meanings in complementary ways.
When a word is present, the left hemisphere restricts processing very quickly to one
possible meaning, usually the dominant meaning or the meaning most consistent with
the present context, whereas the right hemisphere maintains activation of multiple
meanings and remote associated words for a more expended period of time (Springer and
Deutsch, 1998).
Anyhow, viewed in retrospect and taken in their totality these clinical contributions
would seem to have produced at least suggestive evidence for the view that the right
hemisphere should not be considered simply as a minor hemisphere with no distinctive
functional properties (Benton, 2000).
1851
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
It appears that this observation is also true for the human species in an area no more
than 2º from the vertical meridian ( Fendrich and Gazzaniga, 1989).
Although represented the visual information in this area has little utility, as neither
detailed shape comparisons nor brief displays could be reliably compared across the
meridian ( Fendrich and Gazzaniga, 1989).
The same networks that favored coordinate spatial processing also favored coding the
identify of specific shapes, whereas the same networks that favored categorical spatial
processing also favored the assignment of shapes to categories. Interestingly, there is
evidence of right hemisphere superiority for processing the sort of specific shape information
that would be needed to distinguish among the exemplars of single category, but left
hemisphere superiority for classifying the prototypes used to define different categories.
Arithmetic calculations (involving spatial organization of the problem elements or
distinct from left hemisphere – mediated linear arithmetic problems involving) have a
significant right hemisphere component (Grafman and Rickard, 1997; Dehaene, 2000).
Arithmetic failures are most likely to appear in written calculations that require
spatial organization of the problems’ elements (Grafman and Rickard, 1997; Dănăilă and
Golu, 2006).
Visuospatial and other perceptual deficits show up in these patients’ difficulty
copying designs, making constructions, and matching or discriminating patterns or faces.
Prosopagnosia (inability to recognize faces) occurs only when the cortex on the
undersides of the occipital and temporal lobes is damaged bilaterally (Mesulam, 2000;
Dănăilă and Golu, 2006). Patients with right hemisphere damage may have particular
problems with spatial orientation and visuospatial memory such that they get lost, even
in familiar surroundings, and can be slow to learn their way around a new area. Visual
functions have to do with dorsal-ventral distinction. Two now well-identified visual
systems have separate pathways with different cortical loci (Mesulam, 2000; Goodale,
2000; Dănăilă and Golu, 2006).
Their constructional disabilities may reflect both their spatial disorientation and
defective capacity for perceptual or conceptual organization. Stereoscopic vision may be
affected (Benton and Hécaen, 1970).
The distinctive processing qualities of each hemisphere become evident in the
mediation of spatial relations. In the right hemisphere the tendency is to deal with the
same visual stimuli as a spatially related whole.
Patients with left hemisphere damage may make defective constructions largely
because of tendencies toward simplification and difficulties in drawing angles, but
they also may display deficits in visuospatial orientation and short-term recall (Mehta
et al., 1989).
The ability to perform complex manual – as well as oral – motor sequences may be
impaired (Harrington and Haaland, 1992; Meador et al., 1999; Schluter et al., 2001).
The distinctive processing qualities of each hemisphere became evident in the
mediation of spatial relations. Left hemisphere processing tends to break the visual
percept into details that can be identified and conceptualized verbally in terms of number
or length of lines, size and direction of angles, etc. Together, the two processing systems
of the left and the right hemisphere provide recognition, storage and comprehension of
discrete and continuous, serial and simultaneous, detailed and holistic aspects of
experience across at least the major sensory modalities of vision, audition and touch
(Lezak et al., 2004).
Thus, model has clinical value, because loss of tissue in a hemisphere tends to impair
its particular processing capacity. A diminished contribution from one hemisphere may
1855
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
to the side of the lesion (Milner, 1972; Lee et al., 1989; Smith, 1989; Morris et al., 1995,
Pillon et al., 1999).
Impaired verbal memory appears with surgical resection of the left temporal lobe
(Seidenberg et al., 1998), and nonverbal (auditory, tactile, visual) memory disturbances
accompany right temporal lobe resection (Lezak et al., 2004). With left temporal
lobectomies, deficits have been found for different kinds of verbal memory, including
episodic (both short-term and learning), semantic and remote memory (Smith, 1989; Frisk
and Milner, 1990; Loring and Meador, 2003).
These patients also lag behind normal controls in learning designs, although once
learned their retention is good unlike patients with right temporal lesions who fail both
aspects of this memory task (Jones-Gotman, 1986).
Same patients with cortical lesions have shown selective deficits in retrieving highly
specific types of information, such as items in certain categories, but not others (Martin
et al., 1997; Gabrieli, 1998). This finding suggests that cortical representation of
knowledge is highly organized.
Reduced access to verbal labeling may explain the left temporal patients’ slowed
learning. Learning manual sequences becomes more difficult following left but not right
lobectomy (Jason, 1987).
Cortical stimulation of the anterior left temporal cortex interferes with verbal
learning without affecting speech, while stimulation of the posterior left temporal cortex
is more likely to result in retrieval (word finding) problems and anomia (literally, no
words) (Fedio and Van Buren, 1974; Ojemann, 1978).
Lesions in different areas of the left temporal lobe differentially affect the degree
and nature of impairment in immediate auditory recall of tones or digits (Gordon, 1983).
Memory deficits documented for patients with right temporal lobectomies and other
temporal lobe lesions involve designs, faces, melodies, and spatial formats such as those
used in maze learning (Abrahams et al., 1997).
In short, these patients display memory impairments when perceptions or knowledge
cannot be readily put into words (Shapiro et al., 1981; Smith, 1989). This left - right
difference has been found in brain activation studies comparing the effect of stimulus
material on temporal lobes (Ojemann, 1978; Dolan et al., 1997), and on the prefrontal
cortex’s role in memory (Wagner et al., 1998; Mc Dermott et al., 1999; Lee et al., 2000).
However, current evidence suggests that the relationship between the type of material
(verbal vs. nonverbalizable) to be learned or modality of stimulus input and hemisphere
involvement is not simple. Functional neuroimaging data demonstrate that both
hemispheres may be activated by a verbal memory task (Buckner et al., 1998; Johanson
et al., 2001).
This finding suggests that cortical representation of knowledge is highly organized.
Loss of facts, knowledge of objects, and meaning of words have been reported with
selective damage to the inferolateral temporal gyri of one or both temporal lobes, with
sparing of the hippocampal and parahippocampal gyri (Graham and Hodges, 1997).
1857
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Thus, while the hippocampus and medial limbic structures are involved in the
processing of newly learned information that has not been consolidated yet, the temporal
cortex appears to house old learned information.
Fig. 23.5. Functions associated with the dominant and nondominant cerebral hemispheres.
(Modified from Noback CR et al. (1996), The Human Nervous System, 5th edn., Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore).
However, other studies suggest that neither hemisphere has an attentional advantage,
but rather each hemisphere directs attention contralaterally (Mirsky, 1989; Posner, 1990),
1859
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
and that they are equally capable of detecting stimuli (Prather et al., 1992). The right
hemisphere directs attention to far space while the left hemisphere directs attention to
near space (Heilman et al., 1995). The appearance of right hemisphere superiority for
attention in some situations may stem from its ability to integrate complex, nonlinear
information rapidly.
activated the same areas through to be involved in the execution of movement sequences
generally (i.e., primary and premotor cortex, SMA, basal ganglia, and the cerebellum).
Musical perception
Musical perception may be considered in terms of the reception of those perceptual-
acoustic characteristics that define the basic elements of musical sounds, such as
frequency, pitch, consonance, timbre, duration, intensity and spatial localization.
Disturbance in receptive musical capacities in combination with aphasic disorder
have been found to be associated with lesions involving the middle and posterior parts
of the first and second temporal gyri, the transverse temporal gyri, and the anterior
temporal region of the left hemisphere.
The three surgically explored cases of Dorgeuille (1966) may be cited to illustrate
this point.
They showed a fluent aphasia, impairment in the discrimination of tones, expressive
musical defects, defective reproduction of sounds and rhythmic patterns and defects in
singing melodies with which thay had been quite familiar.
Receptive amusia without concomitant aphasic disorder has been found to be
associated with temporal lobe disease of either hemisphere or of both hemispheres
(Schuster and Taterka, 1926; Pötzl and Uiberall, 1937; Pötzl, 1939, 1943; Spreen et
al., 1965).
A plausible interpretation is that some individuals show dissociated dominance, i.e.,
thay are left hemisphere dominant for language but right hemisphere dominant for music.
Henschen (1920) localized a center for singing in the upper part of the third frontal gyrus,
a venter for oral musical comprehension in the left temporal pole and a center for reading
music in the angular gyrus.
The idea that each hemisphere makes a distinctive contribution to receptive and
expressive musical performance has been proposed. For example, Barbizet et al. (1969)
and Barbizet (1972) have advanced the concept that the right hemisphere participates
primarily on the perceptual and executive levels of musical activity while the left
hemisphere mediates the recognition and memory of musical structures, the symbolic
processes in reading and writing music, and the higher level integrative functions involved
in musical composition. It is clear that there can be a dissociation between expressive
and receptive functions.
But why one aphasic patient with focal lesion will show concomitant disturbances
in musical function while another patient with a similar type of disorder and lesion will
show no musical disabilities is a complete mystery (Benton, 2000). It is possible that the
contribution which each hemisphere makes to the mediation of musical function varies
quantitatively from one individual to another.
Reliable knowledge about interhemispheric relations in this respect no doubt would
go far toward helping us understand a cognitive problem, namely, why some patients are
rendered amusic as a consequence of disease of the right hemisphere while the majority
of patients in this category are not affected (Benton, 2000).
1862
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
accurately, they are impaired at recognizing the anomalous aspects of this picture and
others. So, when the damage is on the right, the person may be slow and erratic at
recognizing faces but not profoundly prosopagnosic.
Language. Electrical stimulation of the superior temporal sulcus (especially in the
posterior half) in the left hemisphere of typical right-handers often causes speech arrest
or other alterations of language abilities. PET findings show bilateral activation of
Heschel’s gyrus and unilateral activation of Wernicke’s area (left hemisphere only) when
typical right-handed subjects listen to a verbal material such as a narrative.
Speech-related areas are occasionally found in the middle temporal gyrus, although
it is possible that such unusual patterns of speech representation might be found
predominantly in patients with preexisting brain damage.
Damage to the anterior parts of the inferior and lateral portions of the temporal lobe
of the speech hemisphere causes problems with confrontational naming, but repetition
and grammar are usually unaffected.
An inferior speech area has been seen in some patients (speech arrest following
electrical stimulation) and surgeons have usually assumed that these areas can be removed
without significant loss of language ability.
Some patients have isolated naming deficits (e.g., proper names) with unilateral
lesions of the anterior and lateral tip of the temporal lobe.
Damage to the anterior parts of the inferior and lateral portions of the temporal lobe
of the right hemisphere has been associated with memory deficits for autobiographical
episodes, although other retrograde memory functions may remain intact.
Using functional activation with PET or fMRI has confirmed the hypothesis that
visual analysis extends into the inferior portions of the temporal lobes. The activation
appears to be bilateral in most cases, but there is a predominance of left-sided activation
if the object’s name is being sought.
Anyhow, it is worth mentioning that one of the disadvantages of functional brain
scans is that patterns of activation reveal what is activated not what must be activated for
successful completion of the task.
The following symptoms are associated with disease of temporal lobes: disturbance
of auditory sensation and perception; impaired long-term memory; altered personality
and affective behavior: disorders of music perception; disorders of visual perception;
disturbance in the selection of visual and auditory input; inability to use contextual
information; impaired organization and categorization of sensory input; and altered sexual
behavior (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
We can divide the temporal regions on the lateral surface into those that are auditory
(Brodmann’s areas 41, 42, and 22) and those that form the ventral visual stream on the
lateral temporal lobe (areas 20, 21, 37 and 38). The visual regions are often referred to
as inferotemporal cortex.
The medial temporal region (limbic cortex) includes the amygdala and adjacent
cortex (uncus), the hippocampus and surrounding cortex (subiculum, entorhinal cortex,
perirhinal cortex), and the fusiform gyrus. The entorhinal cortex is Brodmann’s area 28,
and the perirhinal cortex comprises Brodmann’ areas 35 and 36.
In contrast with the visual-system, pathways, the projections of the auditory system
provide both ipsilateral and contralateral inputs to the cortex; so there is bilateral
representation of each cochlear nucleus in both hemispheres.
In the early 1960s, Doreen Kimura (1961 and 1967) studied neurological patients while
they performed dichotic listening tasks. Pairs of spoken digits (say, “two” and “six”) were
presented simultaneously through headphones, but one digit only was heard in each ear. The
subjects heard three pairs of digits and then were asked to recall as many of the six digits as
possible, in any order. Kimura and Folb (1968) noticed that subjects recalled more digits
that had been presented to the right ear than had been presented to the left.
This result led Kimura to propose that, when different stimuli are presented
simultaneously to each ear, the pathway from the right ear to the speaking hemisphere
has preferred access and the ipsilateral pathway from the left ear is relatively suppressed.
Thus, during a dichotic task, the stimulus to the left ear must travel to the right
hemisphere and then across the cerebral commissures to the left hemisphere. This longer
route puts the left ear at a disadvantage, and words played to the right ear are recalled
more accurately.
Having found a right ear advantage for the perception of dichotically presented
speech stimuli, an obvious next step was to search for tasks that gave a left-ear superiority.
In 1964, Kimura reported just such an effect in the perception of melodies. Two excerpts
of instrumental chamber music were played simultaneously through headphone, one to
each ear. After each pair, four excerpts (including the two that had been played
dichotically) were presented binaurally (to both ear), and the subject’s task was to identify
the two that had been heard previously. Amazingly, Kimura (1964) found a left ear
advantage on this task.
Not all normal subjects show the expected ear advantages in dichotic studies, the
effects are not large when they occur (seldom exceeding a twofold difference in accuracy
in the two ears), and dichotic results are apparently affected by various contextual and
practice effects.
Patients with damage to the corpus callosum exhibit an almost complete inhibition
of words presented to the left ear, even though they can recall words presented to this ear
if there is no competing stimulus to the right ear.
The Kimura’s experiments imply that the left hemisphere is specialized for
processing language related sounds, whereas the right hemisphere processes music-
related sounds.
1865
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Papcun et al. (1974) showed that Morse-code operators have a right-ear superiority
for the perception of the code, even though the sounds are distinguished only by their
temporal structure. Thus, the results of this study might be taken as evidence that the left
hemisphere is not specialized for language so much as for “something else”.
the inconsistency of responses to painful and tactile stimuli; the difficulty in distin-
guishing more than one contact at a time; disregard of stimuli on the affected side when
the healthy side is stimulated simultaneously (tactile inattention or extinction); the
tendency of superficial pain sensation to outlast the stimulus and to be hyperpathic; and
the occurrence of hallucinations of touch.
More precise testing is possible by using a von Frey hair. By this method, a stimulus
of constant strength can be applied and the threshold for tactile sensation determined by
measuring the force required to bend a hair of known length.
Thus, the defect is essentially one of sensory discrimination, i.e., an impairment or
loss of the sense of position and passive movement and the ability to localize tactile,
thermal and noxious stimuli applied to the body surface; to recognize figures written on
the skin; to distinguish between single and double contacts (two point discrimination);
and to detect the direction of movement of a tactile stimulus. In contrast, the perception
of pain, touch, pressure, vibratory, and thermal stimuli is relatively intact. This type of
sensory defect is sometimes referred to as “cortical”, although it can be produced just as
well by lesions of the subcortical connections.
In tests of pressure sensibility, two points discrimination, point localization, position
of sense, and tactile object recognition, Semmes and Turner (1977) and Corkin et al.
(1970) found bilateral disturbance in nearly half of their patients with unilateral lesions,
but the deficits were always more severe contralaterally and mainly in the hand.
A pseudothalamic pain syndrome on the side deprived of sensation has also been
described by Biemond (1956). The discomfort (burning or constrictive pain, identical to
the thalamic pain syndrome) resulted from vascular lesions restricted to the cortex.
Another somatosensory symptom is simultaneous extinction that can be
demonstrated only by special testing procedures. The logic of this test is that a person is
ordinarily confronted by an environment in which many sensory stimuli impinge
simultaneously, yet the person is able to distinguish and perceive each of these individual
sensory impressions.
To offer more-complicated sensory stimulation, two tactile stimuli are presented
simultaneously to the same or different body parts. The objective of such a double
simultaneous stimulation is to uncover those situations in which both stimuli would be
reported if applied single, but only one would be reported if both were applied together. A
failure to report one stimulus is usually called extinction and is most commonly associated
with damage to the somatic secondary cortex, especially in the right parietal lobe.
Blind touch
Paillard et al. (1983), reported the case of a woman who appears to have a tactile
analogue of blindsight. This woman had a large lesion of areas PF, PE (area PE is
equivalent to area 5 and PF to area 7b in Felleman and van Essen’s flat map of cortical
areas in the macaque) and some of PG (area PG in the monkey includes areas 7a, VIP,
LIP, IPJ, PP, MSTc, and MSTp), resulting in a complete anesthesia of the right side of
the body so severe that she was likely to cut or burn herself without being aware of it.
1869
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Nevertheless, she was able to point with her left hand to location on her right hand where
she had been touched, even though she failed to report feeling the touch.
Anyhow, the PG areas are primarily visual.
The presence of a tactile analogue of blindsight is important because it suggests the
existence of two tactile systems – one specialized for detection and an other for
localization. Such specialization may be a general feature of sensory system organization
(Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Object recognition
Warrington and Taylor (1973) described a common symptom of right-parietal-lobe
lesion, in which patients having these lesions badly impaired at recognizing objects shown
in unfamiliar view. They wrote that the deficit is not in forming a gestalt, or concept, but
rather in perceptual classification.
Such allocation can be seen as a type of spatial matching in which the common view
of an object must be rotated spatially to match the novel view.
Warrington and Taylor suggested that the focus for this deficit is roughly the right
inferior parietal lobule, the same region proposed as the locus of contralateral neglect.
Pseudocerebellar syndrome
With anterior parietal lobe lesions there is often an associated hemiparesis, since the
position of the parietal lobe contributes witch a considerable number of fibres to the
corticospinal tract. Or, there is only a poverty of movement of the opposite side as part
of the somatic neglect. The affected limbs, if weakened, tend to remain hypotonic and
the musculature undergoes atrophy of a degree not explained by inactivity alone.
Exceptionally, at some phase in recovery from the hemisensory deficit, there is
incoordination of movement and intention of tremor of the contralateral arm and leg,
simulating a cerebellar deficit (pseudocerebellar syndrome) (Adams et al., 1997).
Somatosensory agnosias
There are two major types of somatosensory agnosias: astereognosis and
asomatognosia.
Astereognosis
Astereognosis (Greek: a, priv.; + stereos, solid; + gnosis, knowledge) refers to the
loss of the power of judging the form, shape and size of an object by touch, based on the
impressions from deeper receptors.
Ahylognosia (Greek: a, priv.; + hyle, matter; + gnosis, recognition) is the inability
to recognize differences of density, weight, texture and roughness of objects by touch. It
depends mainly on cutaneous impressions.
The lack of recognition of texture, shape and form is frequently a manifestation of
cortical disease but a similar clinical defect will occur if tracts that transmit tactile and
proprioceptive sensation are interrupted by lesions of the spinal cord and brain stem (and,
1870
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
of course, of the peripheral nerves). The latter type of sensory defect is called
stereoanesthesia and must be distinguished from astereognosis, which connotes an
inability to identify an object by palpation, even though the primary sense data (touch,
pain, temperature and vibration) are intact. In practice, a pure astereognosis is rarely
encountered, and the term is employed when the impairment of superficial and vibratory
sensation in the hand seems to be of insufficient severity to account for the defect. Defined
in the way, astereognosis is either right- or left-sided, and, with the qualifications
mentioned below, is the product of a lesion in the opposite hemisphere, involving the
sensory cortex, particularly S 2, or the thalamoparietal projections.
Finally, there is a distinction to be made between astereognosis and tactile agnosia.
Casselli (1991) defined tactile agnosia as a strictly unilateral disorder, right or left, in
which the impairment of tactile object recognition is unencumbered by a disturbance of
the primary sensory modalities. Such a disorder would be designated by other as a pure
form of astereognosis. In accordance with Adams et al. (1997), tactile agnosia is a
disturbance in which a one-sided lesion lying posterior to the postcentral gyrus of the
dominant parietal lobe results in an inability to recognize an object by touch in both
hands.
According to this view, tactile agnosia is a disorder of apperception of stimuli and
of translating them into symbols akin to the defect in naming parts of the body, visualizing
a plan or a route, or understanding the meaning of the printed or spoken words (visual or
auditory verbal agnosia).
Asomatognosia
The idea that visual and tactile sensory information are synthesized during
development into a body schema or image (perception of one’s body and of the relations
of bodily parts to one another) was first clearly formulated by Pick (1908) and extensively
elaborated by Brain (1941).
The formation of the body schema is believed to be based on the constant influx and
storage of sensations from our bodies as we move about; hence, motor activity is
important in its development. Always, however, a sense of extrapersonal space is
involved as well, and this dependents upon visual and labyrinthine impulses. The
mechanisms upon which these perceptions depend are best appreciated by studying their
derangements in the course of neurological disease (Adams et al., 1997).
Asomatognosia refers to the loss of knowledge or sense of one’s own body and
bodily condition. Unilateral asomatognosia (Anton-Babinsky syndrome) include:
anosognosia (the unawareness or denial of illness); anosodiaphoria (indifference to
illness); autotopagnosia (inability to localize and name body parts or inability to recognize
any part of the body, a condition resulting from a lesion of the minor hemisphere);
allocheiria (one-sided stimuli are felt on the other side); and asymbolia for pain (the
absence of normal reaction to pain, such as reflexive withdrawal from a painful stimulus).
These conditions resulting from a lesion of the minor hemisphere in which are included
right parietal lobes. Anyhow, unilateral asomatognosia is seven times as frequent with
1871
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
right (nondominant) parietal lesions as with the left-sided ones, according to Hécaen’s
statistics (Hécaen, 1962). The apparent infrequency of right sided symptoms is
attributable in part to their obscuration by an associated aphasia.
The observation that a patient with a dense left hemiplegia may be indifferent to, or
unaware of the paralysis was first made by Anton. Babinsky named this disorder
anosognosia. It may express itself in several ways: The patient may act as if nothing were
the matter. If asked to raise the paralyzed arm, the patient may raise the intact one or do
nothing at all. If asked whether the paralyzed arm has been moved, the patient may say
yes. If the failure to do so is pointed out, the patient may admit that the arm is slightly
weak. If told it is paralyzed, the patient may deny that this is so or offer an excuse: “My
shoulder hurt” (Adams et al., 1997).
Some patients report that they feel as though their left side had disappeared, and
when shown their paralyzed arm, they may deny it is theirs and assert that it belongs to
someone else or even take hold of it and fling it aside.
This mental derangement, obviously includes a somatosensory defect, and loss of
the stored engrams of the body schema as well as a conceptual negation and neglect of
half of the body.
Anosognosia is usually associated with a blunted emotionality. The patient looks
dull, is inattentative and apathetic, and shows varying degrees of general confusion. There
may be an indifference to failure, a feeling that something is missing, visual and tactile
illusions when sensing the paralyzed part, hallucinations of movement, and allocheiria
(one sided stimuli are felt on the other side).
Weintraub and Mesulam (1987) indicated that patients with right parietal lesions
show elements of ipsilateral neglect in addition to the striking degree of contralateral
neglect, suggesting that, in respect to spatial attention, the right parietal lobe is truly
dominant.
Neglect presents obstacles to understand. Heilman et al. (1993), examined 13 patients
with neglect and noted that the area of lesions was the right inferior parietal lobule.
Anyhow, they wrote that contralateral neglect is occasionally observed subsequent
to lesions to the frontal lobe and cingulate cortex, as well as to subcortical structures
including the superior colliculus and lateral hypothalamus. Neglect is caused by either
defective sensation or perception, or defective attention or orientation. The strongest
argument favoring the theory of defective sensation or perception is that a lesion to the
parietal lobes, which receive input from all the sensory regions, can disturb the integration
of sensation into perception. Denny-Brown and Chambers (1976) termed this function
morphosynthesis and its disruption amorphosynthesis. A current elaboration of this theory
proposes that neglect follows a right parietal lesion because the integration of the spatial
properties of stimuli becomes disturbed. As a result, although stimuli are perceived, their
location is uncertain to the nervous system and they are consequently ignored.
In the absence of right hemisphere function, the left hemisphere is assumed to be
capable of some rudimentary spatial synthesis that prevents neglect of the right side of
1872
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
the world. This rudimentary spatial ability cannot compensate, however, for the many
other behavioral deficits resulting from right parietal lesions (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003).
Critchley (1953) and others suggested that neglect results from defective attention
or orientation.
Heilman et al. (1993), propose that neglect is manifested by a defect in orienting to
stimuli: the defect results from the disruption of a system whose function is to “arise”
the person when new sensory stimulation is present.
The lesion responsible for the various forms of unilateral asomatognosia lies in the
cortex and white matter of the superior parietal lobe but may extend variability into the
postcentral gyrus, frontal motor areas, and temporal and occipital lobes, accounting for
some of the associated abnormalities.
Rarely, a deep lesion of the ventrolateral thalamus and juxtaposed white matter of
the parietal lobe will produce contralateral neglect.
Another aspect of parietal lobe physiology, revealed by human disease, is the loss
of exploratory and orienting behavior with the contralateral arm and even a tendency to
avoid tactile stimuli.
Mori and Yamadori (1989) call this “rejection behavior”. Denny-Brown and
Chambers (1976) attribute the released grasping and exploring that follow frontal lobe
lesions to a disinhibition of parietal lobe automatism.
Ideational apraxia
The inability to carry out a series of acts or sequence of action – an ideational plan
that leads to goal – has been called ideational apraxia.
When performing a task that requires a series of acts, these patients have difficulty
sequencing the acts in the proper orders. To determine its presence, patients should be
tested for their ability to perform multistep sequential tasks. Failure to perform each step
in the correct order suggests ideational apraxia.
Patients who select the wrong movement (content errors) have also been diagnosed
as having ideational apraxia, but in order to avoid confusion these errors may be classified
as conceptual apraxia (Heilman et al., 1997).
Ideational apraxia may occur with more diffuse brain diseases such as those
associated with degenerative disorders or with focal lesions of the left hemisphere,
particularly in the parietal lobe. In general, however, patients with frontal lesions have
the most difficulty with sequencing.
Conceptual apraxia
To perform a skilled act, two types of knowledge are needed: conceptual knowledge
and production knowledge.
Whereas dysfunction of praxis production system induces IMA, defects in the
knowledge needed to successfully select and use the tools and objects are called
conceptual apraxia.
Therefore, patients with IMA make production errors (e.g., spatial and temporal
errors), and patients with conceptual apraxia make content and tool selection errors.
Patients with conceptual apraxia may not recall the type of actions associated with
specific tools, utensils, or objects (tool-object action knowledge) and therefore make
content errors (Heilman et al., 1997). For example, when asked to demonstrate the use
of a screw driver by pantomiming as by using the tool, the patient with the loss of tool-
1875
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
object action knowledge may pantomime a hammering movement or use the screw driver
as if it were a hammer.
Unfortunately, use of the term ideational apraxia has been confusing; with the term
erroneously used to label other disorders (Heilman et al., 1997). The term ideational
apraxia has been used to describe patients who make conceptual errors.
Patients with conceptual apraxia may be unable to recall which tool is associated
with a specific object (tool-object association knowledge).
For example, when shown a partially driven nail, they may select a screw driver
rather than a hammer from an array of tools. This conceptual defect also occurs in the
verbal domain, which has these results (Heilman et al., 1997): when a tool is shown, the
patient may be able to name it (e.g., hammer), but when the patient is asked to name or
point to a tool when its function is described, he cannot. The patient may also be unable
to describe the functions of tools.
Patients with conceptual apraxia may also have impaired mechanical knowledge
(Heilman et al., 1997). Mechanical knowledge is also important for tool development,
and patients with conceptual apraxia may be unable to correctly develop tools.
In 1920, Liepmann thought that conceptual knowledge was located in the caudal
parietal lobe, but in 1989, Ochipa et al., placed it in the temporal-parietal junction.
Later, a patient was reported with left-handed and rendered conceptual apraxia by
lesion in the right hemisphere, suggesting that both production and conceptual knowledge
have lateralized representation and that such representations are contralateral to the
preferred hand. Further evidence that these conceptual representations stored in the
hemisphere that is contralateral to the preferred hand drives from the observations of a
patient who had a callosal disconnection and demonstrated conceptual apraxia of the
nonpreferred (left) hand (Ochipa et al., 1992).
Researchers studying right-handed patients who had either right or left hemisphere
cerebral information found that conceptual apraxia was more commonly associated with
left than right hemisphere injury (Heilman et al., 1997). However, they did not find any
anatomic region that appeared to be critical, suggesting that mechanical knowledge may
be widely disturbed in the left hemisphere of right-handed people. Although conceptual
apraxia may be associated with focal brain damage, it is perhaps most commonly seen in
degenerative dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. It was also noted that the severity of
conceptual and IMA did not always correlate.
The observation that patients with IMA may not demonstrate conceptual apraxia
and patients with conceptual apraxia may not demonstrate IMA provides support for the
postulate that the praxis production and praxis conceptual systems are independent.
However, for normal function these two systems must interact (Heilman et al., 1997).
Limb kinetic apraxia usually affects the hand that is contralateral to a hemispheric
lesion.
Liepmann (1920) thought that limb kinetic apraxia was induced by lesions that injured
the primary motor and sensory cortex. In 1986, it was demonstrated that monkeys with
lesions confined to the corticospinal system show similar errors (Watson et al., 1986).
Conduction apraxia
Patients with IMA are generally able to imitate better than they can pantomime to
command. However, one patient was reported who was more impaired when imitating
than when pantomiming to command. Because this patient was similar to the conduction
aphasic who repeats poorly, this disorder was termed conduction apraxia (Heilman and
Rothi, 1985).
Whereas the lesions that induce conduction aphasia are usually in the supramarginal
gyrus or Wernicke’s area, the lesions that induce conduction apraxia are unknown.
The mechanism of conduction apraxia may be similar to that of conduction aphasia,
such that there is a disconnection between the portion of the left hemisphere that contains
the movement representation (input praxicon) and the part of the left hemisphere that are
important for programming movements (output praxicon) (Heilman, 1997).
Dissociation apraxia
In 1973, Heilman described patients who, when asked to pantomime to command,
looked at their hand but failed to perform any recognizable actions. Unlike patients with
ideomotor and conduction apraxia, these patients’ imitation and use of objects were
flawless. In addition, when they saw the tool or object they were to pantomime, their
performance was also flawless.
When asked to pantomime in response to visual or tactile stimuli, some patients were
unable to do so, but these patients could pantomime to verbal command.
Patients with dissociation apraxia may have callosal lesions. In those patients with
callosal lesions, it has been proposed that whereas language was mediated by their left
hemisphere, the movement representations (praxicon) were stored bilaterally (Heilman,
1997). Therefore, their callosal lesion induced a dissociation apraxia only of the left hand
because the verbal command could not get access to the right hemisphere’s movement
representations.
Not only can callosal lesions be associated with an IMA, but also callosal
disconnection can cause dissociation apraxia. The subjects of Gazzaniga and associates
(1967) and the patient described by Geschwind and Kaplan (1962) had disassociation
apraxia of the left hand.
With the left hand, they could not gesture normally to command but performed well
with imitation and actual tools, because these tasks do not need verbal mediation and the
movement representations stored in their right hemisphere can be activated by tactile or
visual input.
1877
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Right-handed patients who have both language and movement formulas represented
in their left hemisphere may show combination of dissociation and IMA with callosal
lesions.
When asked to pantomime with the left hand they perform no recognizable
movement (dissociation apraxia), but when imitating or using actual tools, they may
demonstrate the spatial and temporal errors seen with IMA (Heilman, 1997).
Not all callosal lesions cause IMA of the left hand, which suggests that not all right-
handers have movement formulas entirely represented in their left hemisphere. Apraxia
from right hemisphere lesions in a right-hander is rare but has been reported, suggesting
that hand preference is not entirely determined by the laterality of movement
representation and may be multifactorial (Heilman et al., 1997).
Left-handers may demonstrate an IMA without aphasia from a right hemisphere
lesion. These left-handers are apraxic because their movement representations are stored
in their right hemisphere and their lesions destroyed these representations. These left-
handers were not aphasic because their left hemisphere mediated language (as in the
majority of left-handers).
If these left-handers had callosal lesions, they may have demonstrated a dissociation
apraxia of the left arm and on IMA of the right arm.
The dissociation apraxia from left hemisphere lesions that Heilman (1973) described
was, unfortunately, incorrectly termed ideational apraxia. These patients probably had
an intrahemispheric language-movement formula, visual-movement formula, or
somesthetic-movement formula dissociation (Heilman, 1973). The location of lesions
that cause intrahemispheric dissociation apraxia is not known.
Facial-oral apraxia
Facial-oral apraxia is probably the most common of all apraxias (Adams et al., 1997).
It may occur with lesions that undercut the left supramarginal gyrus or the left motor
association cortex and may be associated with apraxia of the limbs.
Such patients are unable to carry out facial movement to command (lick the lips,
blow out a match, etc.), although they may do better when asked to imitate the examiner
or when holding a lighted match.
With lesions that are restricted to the facial area of the left motor cortex, the apraxia
will be limited to the facial musculature and may be associated with a verbal apraxia or
cortical dysarthria.
Dressing apraxia
A particularly common group of parietal symptoms easily tested at the bedside,
consists of neglect of one side of the body in dressing and grooming (“dressing apraxia”),
recognition only on the intact side of bilaterally and simultaneously presented stimuli
(“sensory extinction”), deviation of head and eyes to the side of the lesion and torsion of
the body in the same direction (failure of directed attention to the body and to
extrapersonal space on the side opposite the lesion).
1878
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
The patient may fail to shave one side of the face, apply lipstick or comb the hair
only on one side, or find it impossible to put on eyeglasses, insert denture, or put on a
dressing gown when one of its sleeves has been turned inside out.
Unilateral spatial neglect is brought out by having the patient bisect a line, draw a
daisy or a clock, or name all the objects in the room.
Homonymous hemianopsia and varying degrees of hemiparesis may or may not be
present (Adams et al., 1997).
Constructional apraxia
Disturbance of the perception of space, other than language-related ones, are most
evident in patients with lesions of the right, nondominant parietal lobe. These include
disorders of topographic (extrapersonal) orientation and topographic and geographic
memory, with resulting difficulties in route finding and an inability to reproduce
geometric figures (“constructional apraxia”).
A number of tests have been designed to elicit these disturbances, such as indicating
the time by placement of the hands of a clock, drawing a map, spontaneous (free)
drawing, copying a complex figure, reproducing stick-pattern constructions and block
designs, three-dimensional constructions, and reconstructions of puzzles (Adams et al.,
1997). In the majority of cases, as remarked above, the lesions responsible for these
deficits prove to be in right hemisphere, though the unilateral dominance is not as striking
as that of language and language-associated functions. In the most severe disturbances
of visual-spatial perception, described by Holmes and Horrax in 1919, both parietal lobes
were affected.
The terms dressing apraxia and constructional apraxia are sometimes used to
describe certain symptoms of unilateral extinction or neglect (amorphosynthesis) that
characterize parietal lobe lesions. These abnormalities are not apraxias in the strict sense
as defined above but an imperception of the body.
In sum, from the above description it is evident that the left and right parietal lobes
function differently. The most obvious difference, of course, is that language and
arithmetical functions are centered in the left hemisphere. It is hardly surprising, therefore,
that verbally mediated or verbally associated spatial functions are more affected with
left-sided than with right-sided lesions. It must also be realized that language function
involves cross-modal connections and is central to all cognitive functions.
Hence, cross-modal matching tasks (auditory-visual, visual-auditory, visual-tactile,
tactile-visual, auditory-tactile, etc.) are most clearly impaired with lesions of the dominant
hemisphere. Indeed, this is what Butters and Brody (1968) have found. Similarly, faults
in what Luria (1973) has called logicogrammatical and syntactic aspects of language
(which he considers to be quasispatial) occur with left parietal lesions. Such patients can
read and understand spoken words, but cannot grasp the meaning of a sentence if it
contains elements of relationship (e.g., “the mother’s daughter” versus “the daughter’s
mother”; “the father’s brother’s son”). There are similar difficulties with calculation, as
just described. The recognition and naming of parts of the body and the distinction of
1879
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
right from left and up from down are other learned, verbally mediated spatial concepts
that are disturbed by lesions in the dominant parietal lobe.
Damage to the somatosensory regions of the parietal lobe produces deficits in tactile
functions, ranging from simple somatosensation to the recognition of objects by touch.
Posterior parietal-lobe injury interferes with the visual guidance of hand and limb
movements. Thus, effects of unilateral disease of the dominant parietal lobe (left
hemisphere in right-handed patients) include: disorders of language (especially, alexia);
Gerstmann syndrome; tactile agnosia (bimanual astereognosis); bilateral ideomotor,
ideational, and limb apraxia; deficits in arithmetic and writing.
Effects of unilateral disease of the nondominant parietal lobe (right hemisphere in
right-handed patients) includes: deficits in visual-spatial cognition topographic memory
loss; Anton-Babinski syndrome, dressing and constructional apraxia; and contralateral
neglect. These disorders may occur with lesions of either hemisphere but have been
observed more frequently with lesions of the nondominant one.
Effects of unilateral disease of the parietal lobe, right or left, include: cortical sensory
syndrome and sensory extinction (or total hemianesthesia with large acute lesions of
white matter); mild hemiparesis; unilateral muscular atrophy in children; hypotonia;
poverty of movement; hemiataxia; visual inattention; and sometimes anosognosia; neglect
of one-half of the body and of extrapersonal space (observed more frequently with right
than with left parietal lesions).
Effects of bilateral disease of the parietal lobes include: visual spatial imperception,
optic ataxia, spatial disorientation, severe forms of constructional apraxia and confusion.
Generally, posterior parietal-lobe injury interferes with the visual guidance of hand
and limb movements.
al., 1998). These findings are generally interrupted as indicating right hemisphere
superiority for affective expression.
Recognition of emotional facial expressions can be selectively impaired following
damage to the right temporoparietal areas, and both PET and neuronal recordings
corroborate the importance of this region for processing facial expressions of emotions.
For example, lesions restricted to right somatosensory cortex result in impaired
recognition of emotion from visual presentation of face stimuli. These findings are
consistent with a model that proposes that we internally stimulate body state in order to
recognize emotions (Adolphs, 2002).
This is disagreement as to whether right hemisphere damaged patients experience
emotions any less than other people. Same studies have found reduced autonomic
responses to what would normally be an emotional stimulus (Gainotti, 2003).
Lezak and associates (2004) and others have observed strong – but not necessarily
appropriate – emotional reactions in patients with right-lateralized damage, leading to
the hypothesis that their experience of emotional communications and their capacity to
transmit the nuances and subtleties of their own feeling states differ from normal affective
processing (Barbizet, 1974; Morrow et al., 1981; Ross and Rush, 1981; Lezak, 1994),
leaving them out of joint with those around them.
Patients whose injuries involve the right hemisphere are less likely to be dissatisfied
with themselves or their performances than are those with left hemisphere lesions (Keppel
and Crowe, 2000) and less likely to be aware of their mistakes (Mc Glynn and Schacter,
1989). They are more likely to be apathetic (Andersson et al., 1999), to be risk takers
(Miller and Milner, 1985), and to have poorer social functioning (Brozgold et al., 1998).
At least in the acute or early stages of their condition, they may display an
indifference reaction tending to deny or make light of the extent of their disabilities
(Gainotti, 1972; Pimental and Kingsbury, 1989).
In extreme cases, patients are unaware of such seemingly obvious defects as
crippling left-sided paralysis or slurred and poorly articulated speech. These patients tend
to have difficulty making satisfactory psychological adaptations, with those whose lesions
are anterior being most maladjusted in all areas of psychosocial functioning (Tellier et
al., 1990).
Lesions in right temporal and parietal cortices have been shown to impair emotional
experience, arousal, and imagery. It has been proposed that the right hemisphere contains
systems specialized for computing effect from nonverbal information; these may have
evolved to subserve aspects of social cognition.
Lesions and functional imaging studies have corroborated the role of the right
hemisphere in emotion recognition from facial expressions and from prosody. There is
currently controversy regarding the extent to which the right hemisphere participates in
emotion: Is it specialized to process all emotions (the right hemisphere hypothesis), or is
it specialized only for processing emotions of negative valence while the left hemisphere
is specialized for processing emotions of positive valence (the valence hypothesis)? It
may well be that an answer to this question will depend on more precise specification of
1883
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
which components of emotion are under consideration (Panksepp, 1998; Rolls, 1999;
Adolphs, 2002).
In contrast to recognition of emotional stimuli, emotional experience appears to be
lateralized in a pattern supporting the valence hypothesis, in which the left hemisphere
is more involved in positive emotions and the right hemisphere is more involved in
negative emotions.
So, is reported a higher incidence of depression in patients with anterior lesions
2-4 months poststroke (Kim and Choi-Kwan, 2000; Singh et al., 2000) and with right
hemisphere lesions at six months poststroke (Mac Hale et al., 1998).
Gainotti (1993) suggests that the emotional processing tendencies of the two
hemispheres are complementary: “The right hemisphere seems to be involved
preferentially in functions of emotional arousal, intimately linked to the generation of
the autonomic components of the emotional response, whereas the left hemisphere seems
to play a more important role in functions of intentional control of the emotional
expressive apparatus”. These authors hypothesize that language development tends to
override the left hemisphere’s capacity for emotional immediacy, while, in contrast, the
more spontaneous and pronounced affective display characteristic of right hemisphere
emotionality gives that hemisphere the appearance of superior emotional endowment.
As awareness of deficit is often muted or lacking with right hemisphere lesions (Carpenter
et al., 1995; Meador et al., 2000), these patients tend to be spared of the agony of severe
depression particularly early in the course of their condition.
When the lesion is on the right, the emotional disturbance does not seem to arise
from awareness of defects so much as from the secondary effects of the patient’s
diminished self-awareness and social intensivity (Lezak et al., 2004). Patients with right
hemisphere lesions who do not appreciate the nature or extent of their disability tend to
set unrealistic goals for themselves or to maintain previous goals without taking their
new limitations into account. As a result, they frequently fail to realize their expectations
(Lezak et al., 2004). Depression in patients with right-sided cortical damage may take
longer to develop than it does in patients with left hemisphere involvement since it is less
likely to be an emotional response to immediately perceived disabilities than to a more
slowly evolving reaction to their secondary consequences. When depression does develop
in patients with right-sided disease, however, it can be more chronic, more debilitating,
and more resistive to intervention (Lezak et al., 2004).
Inappropriate euphoria and self-satisfaction may accompany lesions involving other
than right hemisphere areas of the cortex (Mc Glynn and Schacter, 1989). Further,
premorbid personality colors the quality of patients’ responses to their disabilities. Thus,
the clinician should never be tempted to predict the site of damage from the patient’s
mood alone.
expressions and voice intonation, and most are normally responsive to uncaptioned
cartoons but do as poorly as right hemisphere patients when the stimulus is verbal
(Heilman et al., 1995).
Hemispheric differences have been reported for the emotional and even personality
changes that may accompany brain injury (Sackeim et al., 1982; Prigatano, 1987;
Gainotti, 1993).
Patients with left hemisphere lesions can exhibit a catastrophic reaction (extreme
and disruptive transient emotional disturbance). The catastrophic reaction may appear as
acute – often disorganizing – anxiety, agitation, or tearfulness, disrupting the activity that
provoked it. Typically, it occurs when patients are confronted with their limitations, as
when taking a test (Prigatano, 1987; Robinson and Starkstein, 2002).
They tend to regain their composure as soon as the source of frustration is
removed. Anxiety is also a common feature of left hemisphere involvement (Gainotti,
1972; Galin, 1974). It may show up as under cautiousness (Jones-Gotman and Milner,
1977) or oversensitivity to impairments and a tendency to exaggerate disabilities
(Keppel and Crowe, 2000). Yet, despite tendencies to be overly sensitive to their
disabilities, many patients with left hemisphere lesions ultimately compensate for them
well enough to make a satisfactory adjustment to their disabilities and living situations
(Tellier et al., 1990).
Davidson and Irwin (2002) and Davidson and Henriques (2000) posited an approach
/ withdrawal dimension, correlating increased right hemisphere activity with increases
in withdrawal behaviors (including feelings such as fear or sadness, as well as depressive
tendencies) and left hemisphere with increases in approach behaviors (including feelings
such as happiness).
Patients whose left hemisphere has been inactivated from use of the Wada technique
(intracarotid injections of sodium amytal for pharmacological inactivation of one side of
the brain to evaluate lateralization of function before surgical treatment of epilepsy)
(Wada and Rasmussen, 1960) are tearful and tell of feeling of depression more often than
their right hemisphere counterparts, who are more apt to laugh and feel euphoric. In the
same vein, Regard and Landis (1988) found that pictures exposed to the left visual field
were disliked and those to the right were liked.
Since the emotional alterations seen with some stroke patients and in lateralized
pharmacological inactivation have been interpreted as representing the tendencies of the
disinhibited intact hemisphere, some investigators have hypothesized that each
hemisphere is specialized for positive (the left) or negative (the right) emotions, sugges-
ting a relationship between the lateralized affective phenomena and psychiatric disorders
(Flor-Henry, 1986; Lee et al., 1990).
However, studies of depression in stroke patients have produced inconsistent results
(Sato et al., 1999; Carson et al., 2000; Singh et al., 2000).
Shimoda and Robinson (1999) found that hospitalized stroke patients with the
greatest incidence of depression were those with left anterior hemisphere lesions. At
short-term follow-up (3-6 months), proximity of the lesion to the frontal pole and lesion
1885
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
volume correlated with depression in both right and left hemisphere stroke patients. At
long-term follow-up (1-2 years), depression was significantly associated with right
hemisphere lesion volume and proximity of the lesion to the occipital pole.
Moreover, the incidence of depression in patients with left hemisphere disease
dropped over the course of the first year (Robinson and Manes, 2000).
Impaired social function was most evident in those patients who remained depressed.
Women are more likely to be depressed in the acute stages of left hemisphere stroke
than men (Paradiso and Robinson, 1998). With left hemisphere damaged patients,
depression seems to reflect awareness of deficit; the more severe the deficit and acute
the patient’s capacity for awareness, the more likely is that the patient will be depressed.
The occurrence of deviant emotional reaction in the course of disease is associated
with lesions in some parts of the nervous system more regularly than in others. These
have been grouped in the term limbic and are among the most complex and least
understood parts of the nervous system. As defined by Broca (1878) and later by Papez
(1937), the limbic system generally consists of the group of structures around the medical
edge of the cerebral hemisphere.
These structures include the amygdala, the hippocampus, the parahipocampal gyrus,
and related structures, such as the orbital / medial (orbitofrontal) cortex, cingulate cortex,
anterior and mediodorsal thalamic nuclei, the ventromedial corpus striatum (i.e., the
accumbens and medial caudate nucleus), and the nucleus basalis of Meynert. These
structures have important roles in emotion, motivation, and memory.
Next we discuss those structures for which the most data are available from humans;
the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex.
This constellation of behavioral changes has been sought in human beings, but the
complete syndrome has been described only infrequently (Marlowe et al., 1975). Pillieri
(1967), has collected some of the cases that come closest to reproducing syndrome.
Orbitofrontal cortex
The importance of the frontal lobes in social and emotional behavior was
demonstrated in the mid-1800s by the famous case of Phineas Gage. In 1850, Bigellow
published this famous “crowbar case”, originally described by Harlow (1848).
Gage, a railroad construction foreman, had sustained the passage of an iron bar
(1.25 inches in diameter at the larger end, 3.5 feet in length and 13.5 pound in weight),
which entered the left frontal lobe, emerged from the right frontal bone near the sagittal
suture, leaving a circular opening of about 3.5 inches in diameter and destroying the
left frontal lobe and anterior temporal pole, as well, in all probability, some right frontal
tissue.
Phineas Gage did not appear to have suffered intellectual impairment in the narrow
sense of the term. Four years after his injury, he went to South America where he worked
for 8 years before returning to the United States, a year before his death. However, be-
ginning about one month after his injury, he exhibited a remarkable change in personality.
Before the injury, Gage was considered a honest, reliable, polite, deliberate person
and a good businessman.
Subsequently, he became uncaring, profane, childish, capricious, obstinate, showed
poor judgment, used profane language, socially inappropriate in his conduct and was
inconsiderate of others.
In short, he showed a distinctive type of personality change that later authors, such
as Welt (1888), associated with prefrontal lobe disease.
The studies of Jastrowitz (1888), Welt (1888) and Oppenheim (1890) established
that distinctive changes in personality and behavior could be related to disease of the
prefrontal region, and Welt specifically implicated involvement of the orbital and mesial
sectors of the region. The terms moria (turpidity) and Witzelsucht (addiction to joking)
are linguistic residues of their observations.
In discussing frontal lobe symptomatology, Moritz Jastrowitz (1888) stated that he
had seen a specific form of dementia, characterized by an oddly cheerful agitation, in
patients with tumors of the frontal lobe, which he referred to as “so-called moria”.
Leonore Welt (1888) wrote a paper in which she reported a personally observed case
and presented a detailed review of earlier literature. Her patient, a 37-year-old man, had
sustained a severe penetrating frontal fracture after a fall from a fourth storey window.
Physical recovery was swift and uneventful after an operation for removal of bone from
the brain. Beginning about 5 days after the traumatic event, the patient showed a
remarkable change in personality. He was aggressive and malicious and given to making
bad jokes. He teased other patients unmercyfully and played mean tricks on the hospital
personnel. He showed no respect for the physicians and threatened to “expose” them in
the daily press. He exhibited this objectionable behavior for about a month, at which time
his behavior gradually improved.
1888
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Some months later, he died from a pleuritic infection. Autopsy disclosed destruction
of the gyrus rectus in both hemispheres as well as the mesial sector of the right inferior
frontal gyrus.
Analyzing the eight autopsied cases showing personality change, she found that
invariably there was involvement of the orbital gyri.
In 1890, Hermann Oppenheim wrote a comprehensive paper on the clinical
manifestations of brain tumors. Four patients who exhibited Witzelsucht (addiction to
joking) proved to have tumors of the right frontal lobe, three of which had invaded the
mesial and basal area.
Extensive study of modern-day patients with similar anatomical profile (i.e., bilateral
damage to ventromedial frontal lobe) has shed more light on this case. These patients
show a severely impaired ability to function in society, even with normal IQ, language,
perception, and memory.
Damasio (1994) and others have illuminated the importance of ventromedial frontal
cortices in linking stimuli with their emotional and social significance.
So, orbitofrontal cortex plays a key role in impulse control and regulation and
maintenance of set and of ongoing behavior (Stuss et al., 1983; Malloy et al., 1993). In
healthy persons, this region is involved in the expression of aggressive behavior (Pietrini
et al., 2000). Damage here can give rise to disinhibition and impulsivity, with such
associated behavior problems as aggressive outbursts and sexual promiscuity (Grafman
et al., 1996; Eslinger, 1999).
Lesions here also can disrupt a patient’s ability to be guided by future consequences
of their actions (Bechara et al., 1994) and lead to poor decisions (Bechara et al., 1999).
Left-sided traumatic damage to this area has been associated with prolonged
unconsciousness (Salazar et al., 1987). Frontal lobe disturbances thus tend to have
repercussions throughout the behavioral repertoire.
Emotional and social functions bear same resemblance to that of the amygdala, but
with two important differences. First, it is clear that the ventromedial frontal cortices play
an equally important role in processing stimuli with either rewarding or aversive
contingencies, whereas the amygdala’s role, at least in humans, is the clearest for aversive
contingencies. Second, reward-related representations in ventromedial frontal cortex are
less stimulus driven than in the amygdala and thus can play a role in more flexible
computations regarding punishing or rewarding contingencies (Adolphs, 2002).
Temporal lobe connections to the orbitobasal forebrain are further implicated in
cognitive functioning. Patients with lesions here are similar to patients with focal temporal
lobe damage in displaying prominent modality, specific learning problems along with
some less severe diminution in reasoning abilities (Salazar et al., 1986).
Because the structures involved in the primary processing of olfactory stimuli are
situated at the base of the frontal lobe, odor discrimination is affected by orbitofrontal
lesions – in both nostrils, when the lesions is on the right, but only in the left nostril, with
left-sided lesions (Eslinger et al., 1982; Zatorre and Jones-Gotman, 1991).
1889
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
1890
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
REFERENCES
Aboitiz F, Scheibel AB, Zaidel E, Morphometry of the Sylvian fissure and the corpus callosum,
with emphasis on sex differences. Brain 11; 1521-1541, 1992.
Abrahams S, Pickering A, Polkey CE, Morris RG, Spatial memory deficits in patients with
unilateral damage to the right hippocampal formation. Neuropsychologia 35; 11-24, 1997.
Abu-Khalil A, Fu L, Grove EA, et al., Wnt genes define distinct boundaries in the developing
human patterning. Journal of Comparative Neurology 474; 276-288, 2003.
Adams RD, Victor M, Ropper AA (eds), Principles of Neurology. Sixth Ed. Chapter 5, McGraw-
Hill, New York etc., 84-93, 1997.
Adolphs R, Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological
mechanisms. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 1; 21-61, 2002.
Adolphs R, Damasio AR, Neurobiology of emotions at the systems level. In: JC Borod (ed) The
Neuropsychology of Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press, 2000.
Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio AR, The human amygdala in social judgment. Nature 393; 470-
474, 1998.
Aggleton JP, The contribution of the amygdala to normal and abnormal emotional states. Trends
in Neurosciences 16; 328-333, 1993.
Alajouanine T, Lhermitte F, Non-verbal communication in aphasia. In: Reuck AVS, O’Conner
M (eds), Disorders of Language. Boston: Little Brown and Co., 168-177, 1964.
Albanese E, Merlo A, Albanese A, Gomez E, Anterior speech region: Asymmetry and weight-
surface correlation. Archives of Neurology 46; 307-310, 1989.
Albert ML, Goodglass H, Helm NA, et al., Clinical Aspects of Dysphasia. New York, Springer
Verlag, 1981.
Alexander MP, Benson DF, The aphasias and related disturbances. In: RJ Joynt (ed): Clinical
Neurology, Vol. 1, New York, Lippincott, Chap. 10, 1991.
Alexander MP, Naser MA, Palumbo C, Broca’s area aphasias: Aphasia after lesions including the
frontal operculum. Neurology 40; 353-362, 1990.
Alexopoulos DS, Sex differences and IQ. Personality and Individual Differences 20; 445-450,
1996.
Allen LS, Gorki RA, Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human
brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 312; 97-104, 1991.
Anderson B, Southern BD, Powers RE, Anatomic asymmetries of the posterior superior temporal
lobes: A post-mortem study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 12; 247-254,
1999.
Andersson R, Cognitive changes after amygdalectomy. Neuropsychologia 16; 439-451, 1978.
Andersson S, Krogstad JM, Finset A, Apathy and depressed mood in acquired brain damage:
Relationship to lesion localization and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychological
Medicine 29, 447-456, 1999.
Andrianov OS, Structural basis for functional interhemispheric brain asymmetry. Human
Physiology 5; 359-363, 1979.
Ankney CD, Sex differences in relative brain size: The mismeasure of woman, too? Intelligence,
16; 329-336, 1992.
1891
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Annett M, Left, Right, Hand and Brain: The Right Shift Theory. London: Erlbaum, 1985.
Annett MA, A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of
Psychology 61; 303-321, 1970.
Anzola GP, Bertoloni G, Buchtel HA, Rizzolatti G, Spatial compatibility and anatomical factors
in simple and choice reaction time. Neuropsychologia 15; 295-302, 1977.
Arato M, Frecska E, Tekes K, MacCrimmon DJ, Serotonergic inter-hemispheric asymmetry:
Gender differences in the orbital cortex. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 84; 110-111, 1991
Aresu M, La superficie cerebrale nell uomo. Archives italiano di anatomia e di embriologia (Italian
Journal of Anatomy and Embriology) 12; 380-433, 1914.
Babinski J, Contribution à l’étude des troubles mentaux dans l’hémiplégie organique cérébrale
(anasognosie). Rev. Neurobiologique 22; 845-848, 1914.
Bakan P, Dibb G, Reed P, Handedness and birth stress. Neuropsychologia 11; 363-366, 1973.
Banich MT, Interhemispheric interaction: Mechanisms of unified processing. In: FL Kitterle (ed),
Hemispheric Communication. Mechanisms and Models. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1995.
Banich MT, Heller W, Evolving perspectives on lateralization of function (Special issue). Current
Directions in Psychological Science 7; 1-2, 1998.
Banich MT, Nicholas CD, Integration of processing between the hemispheres in word recognition.
In: M Beeman and C Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension.
Perspectives from Cognitive euroscience. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Banti G, Afasia e sue forme. Firenze (Florence): Tipografia Cenniniana, 1886.
Barbizet J, Role de l’hémisphère droit dans les perceptions auditives. In: Barbizet M, Ben Hamida
and Ph Duizabo (eds), Le monde de l’hémiplégique gauche. Paris: Masson, 1972.
Barbizet J, Role de l’hémisphère droit dans les perceptions auditives. In: Barbizet M, Ben Hamida
and Ph Duizabo (eds), Le monde de l’hémiplégique gauche. Paris: Masson, 1974.
Barbizet J, Duizabo P, Enos G, Fuchs D, Reconnaissance de messages sonores: bruits familiers et
airs musicaux familiers lors des lésions cérébrales unilatérales. Rev. Neurol. 121; 624, 1969.
Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, et al., The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience and
Biobehavioral Reviews 24; 355-364, 2000.
Barris RW, Schuman HR, Bilateral anterior cingulate gyrus lesions: syndrome of the anterior
cingulate gyri. Neurology 3; 44-52, 1953.
Basso A, De Renzi E, Faglioni P et al., Neurophysiological evidence for the existence of cerebral
areas critical to the performance of intelligence tests. Brain 96, 715-728, 1973.
Batteman F, On Aphasia or Loss of Speech. London: John Churchill and Sons, 1870.
Baynes K, Gazzaniga MS, Callosal disconnection. In: TE Feinberg, and MJ Farah (eds),
Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. McGraw Hill. New York, St. Louis, San
Francisco, etc, 419-425, 1997.
Baynes K, Eliassen JC, The visual lexicon: Its access and organization in commissurotomy
patients. In: M Beeman and C Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension.
Perspective from Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Baynes HK and Gazzaniga MS, Consciousness, introspection, and the split brain: The two minds
– one body problem. In: Gazzaniga (ed). The Two Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed).
Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Bear D, Schiff D, Saver J, et al., Quantitative analysis of cerebral asymmetries: Fronto-occipital
correlation, sexual dimorphism and association with handedness. Archives of Neurology 43;
598-603, 1986.
1892
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Beaton AA, The relationship of the planum temporale asymmetry and morphology of the corpus
callosum to handedness, gender, and dyslexia: a review of the evidence. Brain and Language
60; 255-322, 1997.
Beaumont JG, Future research directions in laterality. Neuropsychology Review 7; 107-126, 1997.
Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW, Insensitivity to future consequences
following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50; 7-15, 1994.
Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP, Different contributions of the human amygdala
and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision making. Journal of Neuroscience 19; 5473-
5481, 1999.
Beeman M, Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. In: M Beeman and C Chiarello
(eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives from Cognitive Neuro-
science. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Beeman M, Chiarello C, Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives from
Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Bellugi U, Poizner H, Klima ES, Brain organization for language: Clues from sign aphasia. Human
Neurobiology 2; 155-170, 1983.
Benbow CP, Stanley JC, Sex differences in mathematical ability: fact or artefact. Science 210;
1262-1264, 1980.
Benbow CP, Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability in intellectually talented
préadolescents: Their nature, effects, and possibble causes. Behavior and Brain Sciences 11;
169-232, 1988.
Bench CJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM et al., Investigations of the functional anatomy of attention using
the stroop test. Neuropsychologia 31; 907-922, 1993.
Benowitz LJ, Bear DM, Rosenthal R, et al., Hemispheric specialization in nonverbal
communication. Cortex 19; 5-11, 1983.
Benson DF, The third alexia. Archives of Neurology 34; 327-331, 1977.
Benson DF, Aphasia and the lateralization of language. Cortex 22; 71-86, 1986.
Benson DF, Stuss DT, The Frontal Lobes. New York: Raven, 1986.
Benton AL, The problem of cerebral dominance. Canad. Psychol. 6; 332, 1965.
Benton AL, The amusias. In: Critchley M and Henson RA (eds). Music and the Brain. London;
William Heinemann, 1977.
Benton AL, Reflexions on the Gerstmann syndrome. Brain and Language 4; 45-62, 1977.
Benton AL, Focal brain damage and the concept of localization of function. In: C Loeb (ed),
Studies in Cerebrovascular Disease. Milan: Masson Italia Editore, 1981; reprinted in: L Costa
and O Spren (eds). Studies in Neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press, 1985.
Benton A, Aphasia (1800-1860). In: Benton A (ed). Exploring the History of Neuropsychology.
Selected Papers. Oxford, University Press, 161-173, 2000.
Benton AL, Joynt RJ, Early descriptions of aphasia. Arch. Neurol. 3; 205-222, 1960.
Benton AL, Hecaen H, Stereoscopic vision in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Neurology
20; 1084-1088, 1970.
Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB, Multilingual Aphasia Examination (3rd ed). Iowa City: AJA,
1994.
Berker EA, Berker AH, Smith A. Translation of Broca’s 1865 report. Localization of speech in
the third left frontal convolution. Arch Neurol. 43; 1065-1072, 1986.
Berridge CW, Espana RA, Stalnaker TA, Stress and coping: Asymmetry of dopamine efferents
within the prefrontal cortex. In: K Hugdahl and RJ Davidson (eds) The Asymmetrical Brain.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 69-103, 2003.
1893
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Biemond A, The conduction of pain above the level of the thalamus opticus. Arch Neurol Psychiat
75; 231, 1956.
Bigellow HJ, Dr Harlow’s case of recovery from the passage of an iron bar through the head. Am.
J. Med. Sci 39; 13-22, 1850.
Binder JR, Rao SM, Hammeke TA, et al., Lateralized human brain language systems demonstrated
by task subtraction functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of Neurology 52; 593-
601, 1995.
Bishop DVM, Handedness and developmental disorder. Lippincott, Philadelphia, 1990.
Bisiach E, Gemineani G, Berti A, Rusconi ML, Perceptual and premotor factors in unilateral
neglect. Neurology 40; 1278-1281, 1990.
Bisiach E, Tegner R, Ladavas E, at al., Dissociation of ophtalmokinetic and melokinetic attention
in unilateral neglect. Cerebral Cortex 5; 439-447, 1995.
Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain
regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98; 11818-11823,
2001.
Blumstein S, Cooper WE, Hemispheric processing of intonation contours. Cortex 10; 146-158,
1974.
Boccardi E, Bruzzone MG, Vignola LA, Alexia in recent and late Broca’s aphasia. Neuro-
psychologia 22; 745-754, 1984.
Bogen JE, The other side of the brain. I. Disgraphia and dyscopia following cerebral
commissurotomy. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Societies 34; 73-105, 1969 (a).
Bogen JE, The other side of the brain. II. An oppositional mind. Bulletin of the Los Angeles
Neurological Societies 34; 135-162, 1969 (b).
Bogen JE, The callosal syndromes. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E (eds): Clinical Neuro-
psychology, 3rd ed, New York: Oxford University Press, 337-407, 1993.
Bogen JE, Memory: A neurosurgeon’s perspective. In: MLJ Apuzzo (ed), Surgery of the Third
Ventricle (2nd ed), Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins, 1997.
Bogen JE, Gazzaniga MS, Cerebral commissurotomy in man: Minor hemisphere dominance for
certain visuo-spatial functions. J Neurosurg 23; 394, 1965.
Boles DB, X-linkage of spatial ability: A critical review. Child Development 51; 625-635, 1980.
Borod JC, Carpenter M, Naeser M, Goodglass H, Left-handed and right-handed aphasics with left
hemisphere lesions compared on nonverbal performance measures. Cortex 21; 81-90, 1985.
Borod JC, St. Clair J, Koff E, Alpert M, Perceive and poser asymmetries in processing facial
emotion. Brain Cognition 13; 167-177, 1990.
Borod JC, Cerebral mechanisms underlying facial, prosodic and lexical emotional expression: A
review of neuropsychological studies and methodological issue. Neuropsychology 7; 445-
463, 1993.
Borod JC, Haywood CS, Koff E, Neuropsychological aspects of facial asymmetry during
emotional expression: A review of the normal adult literature. Neuropsychology Review 7;
41-60, 1997.
Borod JC, Cicero BA, Obler LK, et al., Verbal aspects of emotional communication. In: M Beeman
and Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives from
Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum,1998.
Borod J, Bloom RL, Santschi-Haywood C, Verbal aspects of emotional communication. In: M.
Beeman and Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives from
Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
1894
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Botez MJ, Wertheim N, Expressive aphasia and amusia. Brain 82; 186, 1959.
Bottini G, Corcoran R, Sterzi R, et al., The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of
figurative aspects of language. A positron emission tomography activation study. Brain 117;
1214-1253, 1994.
Bouillaud JB, Sur la faculté du langage articulé. Arch Gen Méd. 1; 575, 1865.
Bouma A, Lateral Asymmetries and Hemispheric Specialization. Amsterdam: Swets and
Zeitlinger, 1990.
Bowers D, Bauer RM, Helman K, The nonverbal affect lexicon: Theoretical perspectives from
neuropsychological studies of affect perception. Neuropsychology 71; 433-444, 1993.
Brain WR, Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere.
Brain 64; 244-272, 1941.
Brinkman C, Porter R, Supplementary motor area in the monkey: Activity of neurons during
performance of a learning motor task. J Psychol 42; 681-709, 1979.
Broadbend WM, A case of peculiar affection of speech. Brain 1; 484-503, 1878.
Broca P, Nouvelle observation d’aphémie produite par une lésion de la moitié posteriori des
deuxième et troisième circonvolutions frontales gauches. Bull Soc Anat 36; 398-407, 1861 a.
Broca P, Perte de la parole: Ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lobe antérieur
gauche du cerveau. Bull. Soc. Anthrop. 2; 235-238, 1861 b.
Broca P, Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé suivies d’une observation
d’aphémie. Bulletin de la Société d’anatomie 6 ; 350-357, 1861 c.
Broca P, Localisations des fonctions cérébrales: siége du langage articule. Bull. Soc Anthrop. 4;
2000-2004, 1863.
Broca P, Deux cas d’aphémie traumatique, produite par lésions du troisième circonvolution
frontale gauche. Bull Soc Chir. 5; 51-54, 1864 a.
Broca P, Sur le siège du langage articulé : aphémie traumatique; lésion troisième circonvolutions
frontale. Bull Soc Anthropol 5; 362-365, 1864 b.
Broca P, Sur le siège de la faculté du language articulé. Bull Soc Anthrop (Paris) 6; 377-393, 1865.
Broca P, Instructions craniologiques et craniométriques. Mémoires de la Société d’Anthropologie
(Paris), 1875.
Broca P, Rapport sur un mémoir de M. Armand de Fleury intitule: De l’inégalité dynamique des
deux hémisphères cérébraux. Bull Acad. Méd. (Paris) (Series 2) 6; 508-539, 1877.
Broca P, Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales. Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure
limbique dans la série des mammifères. Rev. Anthropol. 1; 385-398, 1878.
Broca P, Mémoires sur le cerveau de l’homme et des primates. Paris: Reinwald, 1888.
Brodal P, The corticopontine projection in the rhesus monkey: Origin and principles of
organization. Brain 101; 251-283, 1978.
Brodal A, Nerurological Anatomy (3rd ed) New York: Oxford University Press, 1981.
Brookshire RH, An Introduction to Aphasia (2nd ed). Minneapolis: BRK, 1978.
Brozgold AZ, Borod JC, Martin CC, et al., Social functioning and emotional expression in
neurological and psychiatric disorders. Applied Neuropsychology 5; 15-23, 1998.
Brownell H, Martino G, Deficits in inference and social cognition. In: M Beenman and C Chiarello
(eds). Right Hemisphere Language. Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah NJ,
Erlbaum, 1998.
Brunia CHM, Van Boxel GJM, Motor preparation. In: JT Cacciopo, LG Tassinary, and GG
Berntson (eds), Handbook of Psychophysiology (2nd ed). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press, 2000.
1895
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Bryden MP, Strategy effects in the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. In: G Underwood (ed),
Strategies of Information Processing. New York: Academic Press, 1978.
Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Schaster DL, et al., Functional anatomic study of episodic retrieval.
II. Selective averaging of event-related fMRI trials to test the retrieval success hypothesis.
Neuroimaging 7; 163-175, 1998.
Burgess C, Lund K, Modeling cerebral asymmetries in high-dimensional space. In: M Beeman
and C Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language. Perspectives from Cognitive Neuro-
science Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Burkland CW, Smith A, Language and the cerebral hemisphere. Neurology 27; 627-633, 1977.
Bradshaw J, Rogers L, The evolution of lateral asymmetries, language, tool-use, and intellect.
New York: Academic Press, 1993.
Bryden MP, McManus IC, Bulman-Fleming MB, Evaluating the empirical support for the
Geschwind-Behan-Galaburda model of cerebral lateralization. Brain and Cognition 26; 103-
167, 1994.
Butters N, Brody BA, The role of the left parietal lobe in the meditation of intra- and cross-modal
association. Cortex 4; 328-343, 1968.
Cahill L, Mc Gaugh JL, Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends
in Neurosciences 21; 294-299, 1998.
Caizergues R, Notes pour servir à l’histoire de l’aphasie. Montpellier Méd. 42; 178-180, 1879.
Campbell AW, Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral Function. Cambridge
University Press. New York; 360, 1905.
Cancelliere AE, Kertesz A, Lesion localization in acquired deficits of emotional expression and
comprehension. Brain and Cognition 13; 133-147, 1990.
Caplan D, Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1987.
Carlesimo GA, Caltagirone C, Components in the visual processing of known and unknown faces.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 17; 691-705, 1995.
Carmon A, Disturbance of tactile sensitivity in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Cortex 7;
83, 1971.
Carmon A, Benton AL, Tactile perception of direction and number in patients with unilateral
cerebral disease. Neurology 19; 525-532, 1969.
Carmon A, Harishanu Y, Lowinger E, Lavy S, Asymmetries in hemispheric blood volume and
cerebral dominance. Behav. Biol. 7; 853-859, 1972.
Carosseli JS, Hiscock M, and Roebuck T, Asymmetric interference between concurrent tasks: An
evaluation of competing explanatory models. Neuropsychologia 35; 457-469, 1997.
Carpenter K, Bert A, Oxbury S, et al., Awareness of and memory for arm weakness during
intracarotid sodium amytal testing. Brain 118; 243-251, 1995.
Carson B, Javedan S, Freeman J, et al., Hemispherectomy: A hemidecortication approach and
review of 52 cases. J Neurosurg 84; 903-911, 1996.
Carson AJ, Mac Hale S, Allen K, et al., Depression after stroke and lesion location: a systemic
review: Lancet 336; 122-126, 2000.
Casseli RJ, Rediscovering tactile agnosia. Mayo Clin Proc 66; 129-142, 1991.
Cavada C, Goldman-Rakic PS, Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for
segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe.
Journal of Comparative Neurology 287; 422-445, 1989.
Chao LL, Knight RT, Human prefrontal lesions increase distractibility to irrelevant sensory inputs.
NeuroReport 6; 1605-1610, 1995.
1896
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Chelazzi L, Corbetta M, Cortical mechanisms of visuospatial attention in the primate brain. In:
MS Gazzaniga (ed), The Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed) Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Chiarello C, Lateralization of lexical process in the normal brain; A review of visual half-field
research. In: HA Whitaker (ed), Contemporary Reviews in Neurophysiology. New York:
Springer-Verlag, 1988.
Chorover S, Cole M, Delayed alteration performance in patients with cerebral lesions.
Neuropsychologia 4; 1-7, 1966.
Christman S, Cerebral Asymmetries in Sensory and Perceptual Processing. Elsevier, Amsterdam,
1997.
Cicone M, Wapner W, Gardner H, Sensitivity to emotional expressions and situations in organic
patients. Cortex 16; 145-158, 1980.
Cohen JD, Botvinick M, Carter CS, Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: Who’s in control?
Nature Neuroscience 3; 421-423, 2000.
Collins DW, Kimura D, A large sex difference on a two-dimensional mental rotation task. Behav
Neurosci 111; 845-849, 1997.
Corballis MC, The lopsided ape: Evolution of the generative mind. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, 1991.
Corballis MC, Can commissurotomized subjects compare digits between the visual fields?
Neuropsychologia 32 (12); 1475-1486, 1994.
Corballis MC, The genetics and evolution of handedness. Psychol Rev. 104; 714-727, 1997.
Coren S, Left-handedness: Behavioral Implications and Anomalies. Advances in Psychology, No.
67. North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1990.
Coren S, The Left-Hander Syndrome. The Causes and Consequences of Left-Handedness. Random
House, New York, 1993.
Coren S, Left-handedness. In: VS Ramachandran (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain.
Vol. 2, Academic Press, An imprint of Elsevier Science. Amsterdam, Boston, London, New
York, etc., 685-694, 2002.
Corkin S, Tactually guided maze learning in man: Effects of unilateral cortical excision and
bilateral hippocampal lesions. Neuropsychologia 3; 339-351, 1965.
Corkin S, Milner B, Rasmussen T, Effects of different cortical excisions on sensory thresholds in
man. Trans. Am Neurol Assoc. 89; 112-116, 1964.
Corkin S, Milner B, Rasmussen T, Somatosensory thresholds. Archives of Neurology 23; 41-58,
1970.
Corsi-Cabrera M, Arce C, Ramos J, Guevara MA, Effect of spatial ability and sex on inter- and
intrahemispheric correlation of EEC activity. Neurophysiology 102; 5-11, 1997.
Coslett HB, Saffran EM, Reading and the right hemisphere: Evidence from acquired dyslexia. In:
M Beeman and C Chiarello (eds), Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives
from Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Critchley M, The Parietal Lobes, London, 1953.
Critchley M, Personification of paralysed limbs in hemiplegics. Br Med J 30; 284-286, 1955.
Critchley M, Observations on anasodiaphoria. Encephale 46; 540-546, 1957.
Crossman AR, Cerebral hemisphere. In: Standring S (ed), Gray’s Anatomy. Fortieth edition.
Churchill, Livingstone, Elsevier. London, UK, Ch. 23; 335-359, 2008.
Cummings JL, Clinical Neuropsychiatry. Orlando, FL: Grune and Stratton, 1985.
Cummings JL, Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 50; 873-880, 1993.
Cutting J, The Right Cerebral Hemisphere and Psychiatric Disorders. Oxford: University Press,
1990.
1897
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Dalin O, Berattelse om en dumbe, son Kan siunga. K Swenska Wetensk. Acad Handlingen
Stockholm 6; 114-115, 1745.
Damasio HC, Neuroanatomy of frontal lobe in vivo: A comment on methodology. In: HS Levin,
HM Eisenberg, and AL Benton (Eds.), Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction. New York:
Oxford University Press, 1991.
Damasio AR, Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. Grosset Putnam, New
York, 1994.
Damasio AR, Geschwind N, The Neural Basis of Language. Annual Review of Neuroscience 7;
127-147, 1984.
Damasio AR, Tranel D, Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 90; 4957-
4960, 1993.
Damasio AR, Anderson SW, The frontal lobes. In: KM Heilman and Valenstein (eds), Clinical
Neuropsychology (4th ed). New York: Oxford University Press, 2003.
Danly M, Shapiro B, Speech prosody in Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language 16; 171-290, 1982.
Davidson RJ, Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition 20;
125-151, 1992.
Davidson RJ, Sutton SK, Affective neuroscience: The emergence of a discipline. Current Opinion
in Neurobiology 5; 217-224, 1995.
Davidson RJ, Irwin W, The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends
Cognitive Sci. 3; 11-22, 1999.
Davidson RJ, Irwin W, The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. In: JT
Cacioppo et al., (eds). Foundations in Social Neuroscience. Cambridge MA: MIT Press, 2002.
Davidson RJ and Henriques J, Regional brain functions in sadness and depression. In: J Borod
(ed), The Neuropsychology of Emotion. New York: Oxford University Press, 2000.
Dax G, Notes sur le même sujet. Gaz. Hebd. Méd. Chir. 2; 262, 1865.
Dax M, Lésions de la moitié gauche de l’encéphale coïncidant avec l’oubli des signes de la pensée.
Gaz. Hebd. Méd. Chir. 2; 259- 262, 1865.
Dax G, Observations tendant à prouver la coïncidence constante de dérangements de la parole
avec une lésion de l’hémisphère gauche du cerveau. Montpellier Méd, 1877.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Role of the interaction of the two cerebral hemispheres in the integration of
the language system. Rev. Roum. Sci. Sociales – Serie de Psychologie 31; 87-99, 1987.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Psychiatric Surgery. Posibilities and limbs. editura Academiei Republicii
Socialiste România, Bucureşti; 270, 1988.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Tratat de Neuropsihologie, Vol 2. editura Medicală Bucureşti; 261-319, 2006.
Dănăilă L, Golu M, Iulia Iufu, The interaction of the two cerebral hemispheres in the integration
of the language system. Rev. Roum. Neurol. Psychiat. 28; 97-107, 1990.
De Lacoste MC, Horvath DS, Woodward DJ, Possible sex differences in the developing of human
fetal brain. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 13; 831-846, 1991.
De Renzi E, Faglioni P, The relationship between visuo-spatial impairment and constructional
apraxia. Cortex 3; 327-342, 1967.
Direnfeld LK, Albert ML, Volicer L, et al., Parkinson’s disease. The possible relationship of
laterality to dementia and neurochemical findings. Archive of Neurology 41; 935-941, 1984.
Dehaene S, Cerebral bases of number processing and calculation. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed), The
New Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.
Déjérine J, Contribution à l’étude anatomopathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de
cécité verbale. Mém. Soc. Biol. 4; 61-90, 1892.
1898
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Delazer M, Bartha L, Transcoding and calculation in aphasia. Aphasiology 15; 64-679, 2001.
Delis DC, Wapner W, Gardner H, Moses JA, The contribution of the right hemisphere to the
organization of paragraphs. Cortex 19; 43-50, 1983.
Delis DC, Robertson LC, Efron R, Hemispheric specialization of memory for visual hierarchical
stimuli. Neuropsychology 30; 683-697, 1986.
Delis DC, Kramer J, Kaplan E, California Proverb Test. Lexington, MA: Boston Neuro-
psychological Foundation 1988.
Demonet JF, Price C, Wise R, Franckowiak RS, A PET study of cognitive strategies in normal
subjects during language tasks: influence of phonetic ambiguity and sequence processing a
phoneme monitoring. Brain 117; 671-682, 1994.
Denenberg VH, Hemispheric laterality in animals and the effects of early experience. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 4; 1-50, 1981.
Dennis M, Whitaker HA, Language acquisition following hemidecortication: Linguistic
superiority of the left over the right hemisphere. Brain and Language 3; 404-433, 1976.
Dennis M, Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication.
Brain and Language 10; 287-317, 1980.
Dennis M, Lovett M, Wiegel-Crump CA, Written language acquisition after left or right
hemidecortication in infancy. Brain and Language 12; 54-91, 1981.
Denny-Brown D, Cerebral dominance. In: KJ Zulch, O Creutzfeldt, and GC Galbraith (eds),
Cerebral Localization. New York: Springer-Verlag, 306-307, 1975.
Denny-Brown D, Chambers RA, Psychologic aspects of visual perception. 1. Functional aspects
of visual cortex. Arch Neurol 33; 219, 1976.
Dewson JH, Preliminary evidence of hemispheric asymmetry of auditory function in monkeys.
In: S Harnad, RW Doty, L Goldstein, J Jaynes, and G Krauthamer (eds), Lateralization in
the Nervous System. New York: Academic Press, 1977.
Dolan RG, Paulescu E, Fletcher P, Human memory system. In: RSJ Franckowiak et al. (eds).
Human Brain Function. San Diego, Academic, Press, 1997.
Dopson WG, Beckwith BF, Tucker DM, Bullard-Bates, Asymmetry of facial expression in
spontaneous emotion. Cortex 20; 243-251, 1984.
Dordain M, Degos JD, Dordain G, Voice disorders in left hemiplegia. Revue de Laryngologie
Otologie Rhinologie 92; 178-188, 1971.
Dorgeuille C, Introduction à l’Étude des Amusies. Thèse. Paris, 1966.
Dronkers NF, Redfern BB, Knight RT, The neural architecture of language disorder. In: MS
Gazzaniga (ed), The New Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed), Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press,
2000.
Dubois B, Levy R, Verin M, et al., Experimental approach to prefrontal functions in humans. In:
J Grafman, KJ Holyoak and F Boller (eds), Structure and Functions of the Human Prefrontal
Cortex. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences, 41-60, 1995.
Duffy R, Duffy J, Pearson K, Pantomime recognition in aphasic patients. J. Speech Hear Res 18;
115-132, 1975.
Duncan J, Saitz RJ, Kolodny J, et al., A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 289; 457-
460, 2000.
Esposito G, Van Horn JD, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Gender differences in cerebral blood flow
as a function of cognitive state with PET. Journal of Nuclear Medicine 37; 559-564, 1996.
1899
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Eacott MJ, Gaffan D, Interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor conditional learning via the anterior
corpus callosum of monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research 38; 109-116, 1990.
Eslinger PJ, Orbital frontal cortex: Historical and contemporary views about its behavioral and
physiological significance. Neurocase 5; 225-229, 1999.
Eslinger PJ, Geddes L, Behavioral and emotional changes after focal frontal lobe damage. In: J.
Bogousslavsky and JL Cummings (eds), Disorders of Behavior and Mood in Focal Brain
Lesions. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
Eslinger PJ, Reichwein RK, Frontal lobe stroke syndromes. In: J Bogousslavsky and LR Caplan
(eds), Stroke Syndromes (2nd ed). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.
Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR, Van Hoesen GW, Olfactory dysfunction in man: Anatomical and
behavioral aspects. Brain and Cognition 1; 259-285, 1982.
Etcoff NL, The neuropsychology of emotional expression. In: G Goldstein and RE Tarter (eds).
Advances in Clinical Neuropsychology (Vol. 3). New York: Plenum Press, 1986.
Ethelberg S, Symptomatic “cataplexy” or callostic fits in cortical lesion of the frontal lobe. Brain
73; 499-512, 1950.
Eccles JC, Evolution of the brain in relation to the development of the self-conscious mind. Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences 299; 161-178, 1977.
Falzi G, Perrone P, Vignolo LA, Right-left asymmetry in anterior speech region. Archives of
Neurology 39; 239-240, 1982.
Farah MJ, Gazzaniga MS, Holtzman JD, Kosslyn SM, A left hemisphere basis for visual imagery?
Neuropsychologia, 23; 115-118, 1985.
Fedio P, Van Buren, Memory deficits during electrical stimulation of the speech cortex in
conscious man. Brain and Language 1; 29-42, 1974.
Fendrich R, Gazzaniga MS, Evidence of foveal splitting in a commissurotomy patient.
Neuropsychologia 27; 273-281, 1989.
Ferrier D, The Functions of the Brain. London: Smith Elder, 1876.
Filipek PA, Richelme C, Kennedy DN, Caviness VSJ, The young adult human brain: an MRI-
based morphometric analysis. Cereb Cortex 4; 344-360, 1994.
Filley CM, Neurobehavioral Anatomy. Niwot, CO, University Press of Colorado, 1995.
Finkelnburg FC, Asymbolie. Berlin Klin Wschr 7; 449, 1870.
Fiore SM, Schooler JW, Right hemisphere contributions to creative problem solving: Converging
evidence for divergent thinking. In: M Beenman and C Chiarello (eds). Right Hemisphere
Language Comprehension. Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ,
Erlbaum, 1998.
Flor-Henry P, Observations, reflexions and speculations on the cerebral determinants of mood
and on the bilaterally asymmetrical distributions of the major neurotransmitter systems. Acta
Neurol Scand 74 (Suppl 109); 75-89, 1986.
Flourens MJP, Recherches expérimentales sur les propriétés et les fonctions du système nerveux
dans les animaux vertebrates. Paris, Ballière, 1824.
Fogassi L, Gallese V, Fadiga L, et al., Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex.
Journal of Neuropsychology 76; 144-157, 1996.
Fogassi L, Ferrari PF, Gesierich B, et al., Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention
understanding. Science 308; 662-667, 2005.
Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Heilman KM, Morphologic cerebral asymmetry and handedness: The
pars triangularis and planum temporale. Archives of Neurology 52; 501-508, 1995.
1900
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Foundas AL, Leonard CM, Gilmore RL, et al., Pars triangularis asymmetry and language
dominance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
93; 719-722, 1996.
Franckowiak RSJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, et al., Human Brain Function. San Diego Academic
Press, 1997.
Freedman M, Alexander MP, Naeser MA, Anatomic basis of transcortical motor aphasia.
Neurology 34; 409-417, 1984.
Freedman M, Oscar-Berman M, Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response
deficits in humans. Behav. Neurosci 100; 337-342, 1986.
Freedman JM Jr, Cuppernell C, Flannery K, Gabriel M, Cortex-specific multi-site cingulate
cortical, limbic thalamic, and hippocampal neuronal activity during concurrent discriminative
approach and avoidance training in rabbits. Journal of Neuroscience 16; 1538-1549, 1996.
Frisk V, Milner B, The relationship of working memory to the immediate recall stories following
unilateral temporal or frontal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia 28; 121-135, 1990.
Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Franckowiak RS, A PET study of word finding. Neuro-
psychologia 29; 1137-1148, 1991.
Fritsch G, Hitzig E, Uber die elektrische Erregborkeit des Grosshirns. Arch. Anat. Physiol. 37;
300-332, 1870.
Frost JA, Binder JR, Springer JA, et al., Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both
sexes. Evidence from functional MRI. Brain 122; 199-208, 1999.
Fujii N, Muskiake H, Tanji J, Rostrocaudal differentiation of dorsal premotor cortex with
physiological criteria (Abstract). Society for Neuroscience 22; 2014, 1996.
Fukuda Y, Sawai H, Watanabe M, et al., Nasotemporal overlap of crossed and uncrossed retinal
ganglion cell projections in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata). J Neurosci 9; 2353-2373,
1989.
Funahashi S, Bruce CJ, Goldman-Rakic PS, Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s
dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol 61; 331-349, 1989.
Funahashi S, Chafee MV, Goldman-Rakic PS, Prefrontal neuronal activity in Rhesus monkeys
performing a delayed anti-saccade task. Nature 365; 753-756, 1993.
Fuster JM, Die Physiopathologie der Kataplexie. Confirm Neurol 15; 360-368, 1955.
Fuster JM, Memory in the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995.
Fuster JM, Cognitive functions of the frontal lobes. In: BL Miller and JL Cummings (eds), The
human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. New York, Guilford Press, 1999.
Fuster JM, The Prefrontal Cortex. Fourth Edition. Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston,
Heidelberg, etc., 185-186, 2009.
Gur RC, Gur RE, Obrist WD, et al., Sex and handedness differences in cerebral blood flow during
rest and cognitive activity. Science 217; 659-661, 1982.
Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Yan M, et al., Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy
young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. Journal of Neuroscience 19; 4065-
4072, 1999.
Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Horton NJ, et al., Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain
assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex 11; 490-497, 2001.
Glick SD, Cox RD, Nocturnal rotation in normal rats: correlation with amphetamine-induced
rotation and effects of nigrostriatal lesions. Brain Res 150; 149-161, 1978.
Glick SD, Ross DA, Lateralization of function in the rat brain: Basic mechanisms may be operative
in humans. Trends Neurosci 4; 196-199, 1981.
1901
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Glick SD, Ross DA, Hough LB, Lateral asymmetry of neurotransmitters in human brain. Brain
Research 234; 53-63, 1982.
Gualtieri T, Hicks RE, An immunoreactive theory of selective male affliction. Behavioural and
Brain Sciences 8; 427-441, 1985.
Grimshaw GM, Bryden MP, Finegan JK, Relations between prenatal testosterone and cerebral
lateralization at age 10. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 15; 39-40, 1993.
Gabrieli JDE, Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology 49; 87-
115, 1998.
Gaillard F, Converso G, Lecture et latéralisation. Le retour de L’homme calleux. Bulletin d’Audio-
phonologie. Annales Scientifique de l’Université de Franche-Comté 4; 497-508, 1988.
Gainotti G, Emotional behavior and hemispheric side of one lesion. Cortex 8; 41-55, 1972.
Gainotti G, Emotional and psychosocial problems after brain injury. Neuropsychological
Rehabilitation 3; 259-277, 1993.
Gainotti G, Emotional disorders in relation to unilateral brain disorders. In TE Feinberg and MJ
Farah (eds), Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 2003.
Galaburda AM, Broca’s region: Anatomic remarks made a century after the death of its discoverer.
Revue Neurologique (Paris) 136; 609-616, 1980.
Galaburda AM, Asymmetries of cerebral neuroanatomy. In: Marsh J, Bock MJ (eds), Biological
Asymmetry and Handedness, New York, Wiley, 219-226, 1991.
Galaburda AM, Rosen GD, Sherman GF, Individual variability in cortical organization: its
relationship to brain laterality and implications it function. Neuropsychologia 28; 529-546,
1990.
Galin D, Implications for psychiatry of left and right cerebral specialization. Archives of General
Psychiatry 31; 572-583, 1974.
Gardner H, Ling PK, Flamm L, Silverman J, Comprehension and appreciation of humorous
material following brain damage. Brain 98; 399-412, 1975.
Gardner H, The stories of the right hemisphere. In: Spaulding (ed), Forty-first Nebraska
symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1994.
Gaymard B, Rivaud S, Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Role of the left and right supplementary motor areas
in memory-guided saccade sequences. Annals of Neurology 34; 404-406, 1993.
Gazzaniga MS, The bisected brain. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1970.
Gazzaniga MS, Organization of the human brain. Science 245; 947-952, 1989.
Gazzaniga MS, Neuroscience. Regional differences in cortical organization. Science 289; 1887-
1888, 2000.
Gazzaniga MS, Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication. Does the corpus
callosum enable the human condition? Brain 123; 1293-1326, 2000.
Gazzaniga MS, The new cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.
Gazzaniga MS, Bogen J, Sperry R, Dyspraxia following diversion of the cerebral commissures.
Arch Neurol. 16; 606-612, 1967.
Gazzaniga MS, Consciousness and the Cerebral Hemispheres. The Cognitive Neurosciences.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1391-1400, 1995.
Gazzaniga MS, Principles of human brain organization derived from split-brain studies. Neuron
14; 217-228, 1995.
Gazzaniga MS, Smylie CS, Facial recognition and brain asymmetries: Clues to underlying
mechanisms. Annals of Neurology 13; 536-540, 1983.
Gazzaniga MS, Smylie CS, Dissociation of language and cognition: a psychological profile of
two disconnected right hemispheres. Brain 107; 145-153, 1984.
1902
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Gazzaniga MS, Smylie CS, Hemispheric mechanisms controlling voluntary and spontaneous facial
expressions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2; 239-245, 1990.
Gehring WJ, Knight RT, Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature
Neuroscience 3; 516-520, 2000.
Gemba H, Miki N, and Sasaki K, Field potential change in the prefrontal cortex of the left
hemisphere during learning processes of reaction time hand movement with complex tone in
the monkey. Neuroscience Letters 190; 93-96, 1995.
George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PJ, et al., Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in
healthy women. American Journal of Psychiatry 152; 341-351, 1995.
George MS, Parekh PJ, Rosinsky N, et al., Understanding emotional prosody activates right
hemisphere regions. Archives of Neurology 53; 665-670, 1996.
Gerstmann J, Fingeragnosie: eine umschriebene Störungder Orientierung am eigenen Korper.
Wien Klin Wschr 37; 1010-1012, 1924.
Gerstmann J, Fingeragnosie und isolierte Agraphie ein neues Syndrom. Z. Ges Neurol Psychiat
108; 152-177, 1927.
Geschwind N, Disconnection syndromes in animals and man. Brain 88; 237-294, 1965.
Geschwind DH, The organization of language and the brain. Sience 170; 940-944, 1970.
Geschwind N, Kaplan E, A Human cerebral disconnection syndromes. Neurology 12; 675-685,
1962.
Geschwind N, Galaburda A, Cerebral lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations, and
pathology. Archives of Neurology 42; 428-458, 521-552, 1985.
Geschwind N, Galaburda AM, Cerebral Lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations and
pathology. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, 1987.
Geschwind DH, Iacoboni M. Structural and functional asymmetries of the human frontal lobes.
In: Miller BL, Cummings JL, editors. The human frontal lobes: functions and disorders. New
York, USA: Guilford Press, 45-70, 1999.
Geschwind DH, Iacoboni M, Structural and functional asymmetries of the human frontal lobes.
In: BL Miller and JL Cummings (eds). The Human Frontal Lobes Functions and Disorders.
Second Edition. The Guilford Press, New York, London, 68-91, 2007.
Geschwind DH, Iacoboni M, Mega MS, et al., Alien hand syndrome: Interhemispheric motor
disconnection due to a lesion in the midbody of the corpus callosum. Neurology 45; 802-
808, 1995.
Geyer S, Ledberg A, Schleicher A, et al., Two different areas within the primary motor cortex of
man. Nature 389; 805-807, 1996.
Ghent L, Mishkin M, Teuber HL, Short-term memory after frontal lobe injury in man. J. Comp.
Psychol 55; 705-709, 1962.
Gibson WC, Pioneers in localization of function in the brain. JAMA 180; 944-951, 1962.
Gibson C, Bryden MP, Dichaptic recognition of shapes and letters in children. Canadian Journal
of Psychology 37; 132-142, 1983.
Glickson J, Myslobodsky MS, The representation of pattern of structural brain asymmetry in
normal individuals. Neuropsychologia 31; 145-159, 1993.
Gloor R, Olivier A, Quesney LF, et al., The role of the limbic system in experimental phenomena
of temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of Neurology 12; 129-144, 1982.
Gold BT, Kertesz A, Right hemisphere semantic processing of visual words in an aphasic patient:
An fMRI study. Brain and Language 73; 456-465, 2000.
Goldberg G, Supplementary motor area structure and function: Review and hypotheses. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 8; 567-616, 1985.
1903
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Goldberg E, Higher cortical functions in humans: The gradiental approach. In: E Goldberg (ed).
Contemporary Neuropsychology and the Legacy of Luria. Hillsdale NJ, Erlbaum, 1990.
Goldberg E, Podell K, Lateralization in the frontal lobes. In: HH Jasper, S Riggio and P Goldman-
Rakic (eds), Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Frontal Lobe. New York: Raven,
85-96, 1995.
Goldberg E, Costa LD, Hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive system.
Brain and Language 14; 144-173, 1981.
Goldman-Rakic PS, Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by
representational memory. In: Mountcastle VB, (ed), Handbook of Physiology – The Nervous
System, 373-417, 1987.
Gonzalez-Rothi LG, Raade AS, Heilman KM, Localization of lesions in limb and bucofacial
apraxia. In: A. Kertesz (ed), Localization and Neuroimaging in Neuropsychology. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press, 407-428, 1994.
Goodale MA, Perception and action in human visual system. In: MA Gazzaniga (ed) The New
Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd. ed), Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Goodale MA, Milner AD, Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in
Neurosciences 15; 20-25, 1992.
Gordon DP, The influence of sex on the development of lateralization in speech. Neuropsychologia
21; 139-146, 1983.
Gorenstein EE, Frontal lobe functions in psychopaths. J. Abnormal Psychol 91; 368-379, 1982.
Gorman DG, Cummings JL, Hypersexuality following septal injury. Archives of Neurology 49;
308-310, 1992.
Grafman J, Rickard T, Acalculia. In: TE Feinberg and MJ Farah (eds). Behavioral Neurology and
Neuropsychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1997.
Graham KS, Hodges JR, Differentiating the role of the hippocampal complex and the neocortex
in long-term memory storage: Evidence from the study of semantic dementia and Alzheimer’s
disease. Neuropsychology 11, 77-89, 1997.
Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, et al., Frontal lobe injuries, violence and aggression: A report
of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology 46; 1231-1238, 1996.
Grafton ST, Mapping memory systems in the human brain. Seminars in Neuroscience 7; 157-163,
1995.
Grafton ST, Arbib MA, Fadiga L, Rizzolatti G, Localization of grasp representations in humans
by positron emission topography: Observation compared with imagination. Experimental
Brain Research 112; 103-111, 1996.
Galaburda AM, Le May M, Kemper TL, Geschwind N, Right-left asymmetries in the brain.
Science 199; 852-856, 1978.
Galaburda AM, Neurology of developmental dyslexia. Current Opinions in Neurobiology 3; 237-
242, 1993.
Galuske RA, Schlote W, Bratzke H, Singer W, Interhemispheric asymmetries of the modular
structure in human temporal cortex. Science 289; 1946-1949, 2000.
Garraghty PE, Kaas JH, Dynamic features of sensory and motor maps. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 2;
522- 527, 1992.
Galaburda AM, Le May M, Kemper TL, Geschwind N, Right-left asymmetries in the brain.
Science, 199; 852-856, 1978.
Galaburda AM, Neurology of developmental dyslexia. Current Opinions in Neurobiology 3, 237-
242, 1993.
1904
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Hayes TL, Lewis DA, Anatomical specialization of the anterior motor speech area: Hemispheric
differences in magnopyramidal neurons. Brain and Language 49; 289-308, 1995.
Head H, Holmes G, Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain 34; 102, 1911.
Hebb DO, Intelligence in man after large removals of cerebral tissue: report of four left frontal
lobe cases. Journal of General Psychology 21; 73-87, 1939.
Hécaen H, Clinical symptomatology in right and left hemispheric lesions. In: Mountcastle VB,
(ed), Interhemispheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance. Baltimore, John Hopkins, Chap
10; 215-263, 1962.
Hécaen H, Introduction à la Neuropsychologie. Paris, Larousse, 1972.
Hécaen H, Angelergues R, Le Cécité Psychique. Masson: Paris, 1963.
Hécaen H, Sauguet J, Cerebral dominance in left-handed subjects. Cortex 7; 19-48, 1971.
Hécaen H, Consoli S, Analyse des troubles de language au cours des lésions de l’aire de Broca
(Analysis of language disorders in lesions of Broca’s area). Neuropsychologia 11; 377-388,
1973.
Hécaen H, De Agostini M, Monzon-Montes A, Cerebral organization in lefthanders. Brain Lang
12; 261-284, 1981.
Heffner HE, Heffner RS, Temporal lobe lesions and perception of species-specific vocalizations
by macaques. Science 226; 75-76, 1984.
Heilman KM, Ideational apraxia – A redefinition. Brain 96; 861-864, 1973.
Heilman KM, The neurobiology of emotional experience. J. Neuropsychiatr. Clin Neurosci 9;
439-448, 1997.
Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Frontal lobe neglect in man. Neurology 22; 660-664, 1972.
Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T, Right hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism
underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 30; 327-330, 1980.
Heilman KM, Rothi LJ, Apraxia. In: Clinical Neuropsychology (KM Heilman and E Valenstein
(eds). Oxford University Press, New York, 1985.
Heilman KM, Maher LM, Greenwald ML, Rothi LJ, Conceptual apraxia from lateralized lesions.
Neurology 49; 457-464, 1997.
Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E, Neglect and related disorders. In: KM Heilman and E
Valenstein (eds). Clinical Neuropsychology. (3rd ed), New York, Oxford University Press,
1993.
Heilman KM, Chartter J, Doty LC, Hemispheric asymmetries of the near-far spatial attention.
Neuropsychology 9; 58-61, 1995.
Heilman KM, Rothi LJ, Apraxia. In: KM Heilman and E Valenstein (eds). Clinical
Neuropsychology (4th ed). New York: Oxford University Press, 2003.
Heiss WD, Kessler J, Thiel A, et al., Differential capacity of left and right hemispheric areas for
comprehension of post-stroke aphasia. Annals of Neurology 45; 430-438, 1999.
Helliard RD, Hemispheric laterality effects on a facial recognition task in normal subjects. Cortex
9; 246-258, 1973.
Hellige JB, Hemispheric differences for processing spatial information: Categorization versus
distance (abstract). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 10; 330, 1988.
Hellige JB, Cerebral Hemispheric asymmetry: What’s right and what’s left. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993.
Hellige JB, Coordinating the different processing biases of the left and right cerebral hemispheres.
In: FL Kitterle (ed), Hemispheric Communication: Mechanisms and Models. Hillsdale, NJ,
Erlbaum, 1995.
1906
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Incisa della Rocchetta AI, Classification and recall of pictures after unilateral frontal or temporal
lobectomy. Cortex 22; 189-211, 1986.
Incisa della Rocchetta AI, and Milner B, Strategic search and retrieval inhibition: the role of frontal
lobes. Neuropsychologia 31; 503-524, 1993.
Indefrey P, Levelt WJM, The neural correlates of language production. In: Gazzaniga (ed). The
Two Cognitive Neurosciences (2nd ed). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Inglis J, Lawson JS, A meta-analysis of sex differences in the effects of unilateral brain damage
on intelligence test results. Canadian Journal of Psychology 36; 670-683, 1982.
Ivry RB, Lebby PC, The neurology of consonant perception: Specialized module or distributed
processor? In: M Beeman and C Chiarello (eds), Right hemisphere language comprehension.
Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
Iwatsubo T, et al., Corticofugal projections to the motor nuclei of the brain stem and spinal cord
in humans. Neurology 40; 309-312, 1990.
Jackson JH, Deficit of intellectual expression (aphasia) with left hemiplegia. Lancet 1; 457, 1868.
Jackson JH, On aphasia, with left hemiplegia. Lancet 1; 637-638, 1880.
Jackson JH, Hughlings Jackson on aphasia and kindred affections of speech, together with a
complete bibliography of his publications on speech and a reprint of some of the more
important papers. Brain 38; 1-190, 1915.
Jäncke L, Steinmetz H, Anatomical brain asymmetries and their relevance for functional asym-
metries. In: K Hugadahl and RJ Davidson (eds) The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge,
MA:MIT Press, 187-230, 2003.
Jäncke L, Schlang G, Huang Y, Steinmetz H, Asymmetry of the planum parietale. Neuroreport
5; 1161-1163, 1994.
Janowsky JS, Shimura AP, Kritchevsky M, Squire LR, Cognitive impairment following frontal
lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behav Neurosci 103; 548-560, 1989 a.
Janowsky JS, Oviatt SK, Orwoll ES, Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behav
Neurosci 108; 325-332, 1994.
Jason GW, Studies of manual learning and performance after surgical excision for the control of
epilepsy. In: J Enge Jr. (ed), Fundamental Mechanisms of Human Brain Function. New York:
Raven Press, 1987.
Jastrowitz M, Beitrage zur Localisation im Grosshirn und uber deren praktische Verwerthung.
Dtsch Med Wochenschr 14; 81-83, 1888.
Jeannerod M, Aribib MA, Rizzolatti G, Sakata H, Grasping objects: The cortical mechanisms of
visuomotor transformation. Trends in Neurosciences 18; 314-320, 1995.
Jerison HJ, The theory of encephalization. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 299;
146-160, 1977.
Jinnai K, Matsuda Y, Neurons of the motor cortex projecting commonly in the caudate nucleus
and the lower brain stem in the cat. Neurosci Lett 13; 121-126, 1979.
Jonas S, The supplementary motor region and speech. In: E Percman (ed), The Frontal Lobe
Revisited. New York, IRBN Press, 1987.
Joanette Y, Goulet P, Hammequin D, Right Hemisphere and Verbal Communication. New York,
Springer-Verlag, 1990.
Johanson SC, Saykin AJ, Flashman LA, et al., Brain activation on fMRI and verbal memory
ability: Functional neuroanatomic correlates of CVLT performance. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society 7; 55-62, 2001.
1908
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Jones-Gotman M, Milner B, Design fluency: The innervation of nonsense drawing after focal
cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia 15; 653-674, 1977.
Jones-Gotman M, Right hippocampal excision impairs learning and recall of list of abstract
designs. Neuropsychologia 24; 659-670, 1986.
Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Olivier A, et al., Learning and retention of words and designs
following excision from medial lateral temporal-lobe structures. Neuropsychologia 35; 963-
973, 1997.
Jorge RE, Robinson RG, Starkstein SE, et al., Secondary mania following traumatic brain injury.
American Journal of Psychiatry 150; 916-921, 1993.
Jossmann P, Motrische Amusie (Demonstration). Zbl. Neurol. Psychiat 44; 260, 1926.
Jossmann P, Die Beziehungen der motorischen Amusie zu den apraktischen Storungen Mschr.
Psychiat Neurol. 63; 239, 1927.
Joynt RJ, Benton AL, The memoir of Marc Dax on aphasia. Neurology 14; 851-854, 1964.
Jurado MA, Junké C, Pujol J, et al., Impaired estimations of word occurrence frequency in frontal
lobe patients. Neuropsychologia 35; 635-641, 1997.
Jurado MA, Junké C, Vendrell P, et al., Overestimation and unreliability in «feeling-of-doing
judgments» about temporal ordering performance: impaired self-awareness following frontal
lobe damage. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20; 353-364, 1998.
Juraska JM, Sex differences in developmental plasticity of behavior and the brain. In: Greenough
WT, Juraska JM (eds), Developmental Neuropsychobiology, New York: Academic Press,
409-422, 1986.
Just MA, Carpenter PA, Keller TA, et al., Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension.
Science 274; 114-116, 1996.
Kaas JH, Stepniewska I, Motor cortex. In: VS Ramachandran (ed), Encyclopedia of the Human
Brain. Volume 3. Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston, London etc.; 159-169, 2002.
Kang DH, Davidson RJ, Coe CL. et al., Frontal brain asymmetry and immune function. Behavior
Neuroscience 105; 860-869, 1991.
Kawashima R, Yamada K, Kinomura S, et al., Regional cerebral blood flow changes of cortical
motor areas and prefrontal areas in humans related to ipsilateral and contralateral hand
movement. Brain Research 623; 33-40, 1993.
Keeman JP, Wheeler MA, Gallup GG Jr., Pascual-Leone A, Self recognition and the right
prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4; 338-344, 2000.
Keppel CC, Crowe SF, Changes to body image and selfesteem following stroke in young adults.
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 10; 15-31, 2000.
Kertesz A, Lau WK, Palk M, The structural determinants of recovery in Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain
Long 44; 153-164, 1993.
Kesner RP, Hopkins RO, Fineman B, Item and order dissociation in humans with prefrontal cortex
damage. Neuropsychologia 32; 881-891, 1994.
Kiehl KA, Liddle PF, Smith AM, et al., Neural pathways involved in the processing of concrete
and abstract words. Human Brain Mapping 7; 225-233, 1999.
Killcross S, The amygdala, emotion, and learning. Psychologist 13; 502-507, 2000.
Kim JS, Choi-Kwan S, Post-stroke depression and emotional incontinence: Correlation with lesion
location. Neurology 54; 1805-1810, 2000.
Kim Y, Morrow L, Passafiume D, Boller F, Visuoperceptual and visuomotor abilities and locus
of lesion. Neuropsychologia 22; 177-185, 1984.
1909
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Kim SG, Ashe J, Hendrich K, Ellerman JM, Functional magnetic imaging of motor cortex:
Hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. Science 261; 615-616, 1993.
Kim KHS, Relkin NR, Lee KM, Hirsch J, Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second
languages, Nature 388; 171-174, 1997.
Kimura D, Sex differences in cerebral organization for speech and praxis functions. Canadian
Journal of Psychology 37; 19-35, 1983.
Kimura D, Sex and Cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, 1999.
Kimura D, Some effects of temporal lobe damage on auditory perception. Canadian Journal of
Psychology 15; 156-165, 1961.
Kimura D, Left-right differences in the perception of melodies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology 16; 355-358, 1964.
Kimura D, Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex 3; 163-178, 1967.
Kimura D, The asymmetry of the human brain. Scientific American 228; 70-78, 1973.
Kimura D, Acquisition of a motor skill after left hemisphere damage. Brain 100; 527-542, 1977.
Kimura D, Folb S, Neural processing of background sound. Science 161; 395-396, 1968.
Kingstone A and Gazzaniga MS, Subcortical transfer of higher-order information: More illusory
than real? Neuropsychologia 9; 321-328, 1995.
Kircher TT, Senior C, Phillips ML, et al., Recognizing one’s own face. Cognition 78; 1-15, 2001.
Kirshner HS, Language Disorders. Aphasia. In: Bradley WG, Daroff RB, Fenichel GM and
Marsden CD (eds) Neurology in Clinical Practice. Vol. 1, Third Edition. Boston, Oxford,
Singapore, Butterworth-Heinemann, 141-159, 2000.
Kirshner HS, Hughes T, Farkovarz T, Abou-Khalil B, Aphasia secondary to partial status
epilepticus of the basal temporal language area. Neurology 45; 1616-1618, 1995.
Klebanoff SG, Psychological changes in organic brain lesions and ablations. Psychological
Bulletin 42; 585-623, 1945.
Klein D, Milner B, Zatorre R, Meyer E, Evans A, The neural substrates underlying word
generation: a bilingual functional-imaging study. Proceedings National Academy of Sciences
USA 92; 2899-2903, 1995.
Klein D, Milner BA, Zatorre RJ, Zhao V, Nikelski EJ, Cerebral organization in bilinguals: A PET
study of Chinese-English verb generation. NeuroReport 10; 2841-2846, 1999.
Kohn B, and Dennis M, Selective impairments of visuo-spatial abilities in infantile hemiplegies
after right cerebral hemidecortication. Neuropsychologia 12; 505-512, 1974.
Kolb B, Stewart J, Sex-related differences in dendritic branching of cells in the prefrontal cortex
of rats. J Neuroendocrinol 3; 95-99, 1991.
Kolb B, Whishaw IQ, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Fifth Edition. Worth Publishers
USA, Chapter 11 and 12; 250-317, 2003.
Kolb B, Sutherland RJ, Nonneman AJ, Whishaw IQ, Asymmetry with cerebral hemisphere of the
rat, mouse, rabbit and cat: The right hemisphere is larger. Experimental Neurology 78; 348-
359, 1982.
Kolb B, Whishaw Q, Fundamentals of Neuropsychology (2nd ed). San Francisco: Freeman, 1985.
Kolb B, Whishaw Q, Fundamentals of Neuropsychology (4th ed). New York, Freeman, 1996.
Kolb B, Whishaw QI, Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Fifth Edition. World Publishers,
New York, 2003.
Kononova EP, Structural variability of the cortex cerebri. Inferior frontal gyrus in adults (Russian).
In: “Annals of the Brain Research Institute”. In: SA Sarkisov and IN Filimonov (eds), Vol.
1; 49-118, State Press for Biological and Medical Literature, Moscow, Leningrad, 1935.
1910
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Kononova EP, The frontal lobe (Russian). In: SA Sarkisov, IN Filimon and NS Preobrashenskaya
(eds), The Cytoarchitecture of the Human Cortex Cerebri. Medgiz, Moscow, 309-343, 1949.
Konorski J, A new method of physiological investigation of recent memory in animals. Bull.
Acad. Pol. Sci. 7; 115-117, 1959.
Kosslyn SM, Image and Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1996.
Kreindler A, Macovei M, Cardaş M, Dănăilă L, Mutismul akinetic. Consideraţii anatomo-clinice.
St. Cert. Neurol. 11; 349-360, 1966.
Krynaw RA, Infantile hemiplegia treated by removing one’s cerebral hemisphere. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatr. 13; 243-267, 1950.
Kuhl PK, Language, mind, and brain. Experience alters perception. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed), The
New Cognitive Neuroscience (2nd ed), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2000.
Kulynych JJ, Vladar K, Jones DW, Weinberger DR, Gender differences in the normal
lateralization of the supratemporal cortex: MRI surface-rendering morphometry of Heschl’s
gyrus and planum temporale. Cerebral Cortex 4; 107-118, 1994.
Kushch A, Gross-Glenn K, Jallad B et al., Temporal lobe surface area measurements on MRI in
normal and dyslexic readers. Neuropsychologia 31; 811-821, 1993.
Kussmaul A, Die Stoerungen der Sprache. Leipzig: Vogel, 1876.
Ladavas E, del Pesce M, Provinciali L, Unilateral attention deficits and hemispheric asymmetries.
Neuropsychologia 27; 353-366, 1989.
Landis T, Regard M, Graves R, Goodglass H, Semantic paralexia: A release of right hemispheric
from left hemispheric control? Neuropsychologia 21; 359-364, 1983.
Landis T, Regard M, The right hemisphere’s access to lexical meaning: A function of its release
from left-hemisphere control? In: Chiarello (ed). Right Hemisphere Contribution to Lexical
Semantics. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1988.
Lange J, Fingernosie und Agraphie. Monatsschrift fuer Psychiatrie und Neurologie 76; 129-188,
1930.
Lapierre D, Braun CM, Hodgins S, Ventral frontal deficits in psychopathy: neuropsychological
test findings. Neuropsychologia 33; 139-151, 1995.
Lashley KS, Studies of Cerebral Function in Learning. II. The Effects of Long Continued Practice
Upon Cerebral Localization. Journal of Comparative Psychology 6; 453-468, 1921.
Lassek AM, The human pyramidal tract. II. A numerical investigation of the Betz cells of the
motor area. Arch Neurol and Psychiatr. 44; 718-724, 1940.
Lassek AM, The Pyramidal Tract: Its Status in Medicine. Charles C Thomas, Publisher,
Springfield, Ill; 166, 1954.
Le Doux J, The Emotional Brain. The Mysterious Underpinning of Emotional Life. New York:
Simon and Schuster, 1996.
Le Doux JE, Gazzaniga MS, The Integrated Mind. New York, Plenum Press, 1978.
Le May M, Asymmetries of the skull and handedness: Phrenology revisited. Journal of
Neurological Sciences 32; 243-253, 1977.
Le May M, Kido DK, Asymmetries of the cerebral hemispheres on computed tomograms. Journal
of Computer Assisted Tomography 2; 471-476, 1978.
Lecours AR, Lhermitte F, L’Aphasic. Paris, Flammarion, 1970.
Lee GP, Meador KJ, Smith JR, et al., Preserved cross modal association following bilateral
amygdalotomy in man. International Journal of Neuroscience 40; 47-55, 1988.
Lee GP, Loring DW, Thompson JL, Construct validity of material specific memory measures
following unilateral temporal lobe ablations. Psychological Assessment 1; 192-197, 1989.
1911
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Lee GP, Loring DW, Meador KJ, Brooks BB, Hemispheric specialization for emotional
expression: A reexamination of results from intracarotid administration of sodium
amobarbital. Brain and Cognition 12; 267-280, 1990.
Lee AC, Rubins TW, Pickard JD, Owen AM, Asymmetric frontal activation during episodic
memory: The effects of stimulus type on encoding and retrieval. Neuropsychologia 38; 677-
692, 2000.
Levin PM, Bradford FK, The exact origin of the corticospinal tract in the monkey. J Comp Neurol
68; 411-422, 1938.
Levin M, Aggression, guilt, and cataplexy. Arch Neurol Psychiat 69; 224-235, 1953.
Levine SC, Individual differences in characteristics arousal asymmetry: Implications for cognitive
functioning. In: FL Kitterle (ed). Hemispheric Communication: Mechanisms and Models.
Hillsdale NJ, Erlbaum, 1995.
Levy J, Possible basis for the evolution of lateral specialization of the human brain. Nature 224;
164-165, 1969.
Levy J, Language, cognition, and the right hemisphere. A response to Gazzaniga. American
Psychologist 38; 538-541, 1983.
Levy J, Trevarthen CB, Sperry RW, Perception of bilateral chimeric figures following hemispheric
deconnection. Brain 95; 61-78, 1972.
Lewin J, Friedman L, Wu D, et al., Cortical localization of human sustained attention: Detection
with functional MR using a visual vigilance paradigm. Journal of computer Assisted
Tomography 20; 695-701, 1996.
Lewinsohn PM, Zieler RE, Libet J, et al., Short term memory: comparison between frontal and
nonfrontal right- and left-hemisphere brain damage patients. J Comp. Psychol 81; 248-255,
1972.
Ley RG, Bryden MP, A dissociation of right and left hemispheric effects for recognizing emotional
tone and verbal content. Brain and Cognition 1; 3-9, 1982.
Lezak MD, Domains of behavior from a neuropsychological perspective: The whole story. In: W
Spaulding (ed), 41st Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1992-1993. Lincoln: University of
Nebraska Press, 1994.
Lezak MD, Newman SP, Verbosity and right hemisphere damage. Paper presented in the
2nd European Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society Noordvijkerhout,
Holland, 1979.
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Neuropsychological Assessment. Forth Edition. Oxford
University Press, Oxford, 76-77, 2004.
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Neuropsychological assessment. Fourth Edition. Oxford
University Press, 30-33, 2004.
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, The behavioral geography of the brain. In: MD Lezak,
DB Howieson, DW Loring (eds) Neuropsychological Assessment. Fourth Edition. Oxford
University Press, New York, 39-85, 2004.
Lhermitte J, Le lobe frontal. L’encéphale 24; 87-118, 1929.
Lhermitte F, Pillon B, Serdaru M, Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and
utilization behavior: a neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Ann Neurol 19; 236-334,
1986.
Liepmann H, Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol 8; 15-44, 102-132, 182-
197, 1900.
Liepmann H, Apraxia. Erbgn der ges Med 1; 516-543, 1920.
1912
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Liepmann H, Mass O, Fall von linksseitiger agraphie und apraxie bei techtsseitiger lahmung.
Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie 10; 214-227, 1907.
Loring DW, Meador KJ, Neuropsychological aspects of temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. In: TE
Feinberg and MJ Farah (eds), Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychiatry (2nd ed), New York:
McGraw-Hill, 2003.
Luck SJ, Hillyard SA, Mangun GR, Gazzaniga MS, Independent hemispheric attentional systems
mediate visual search in split-brain patients. Nature 342; 543-545, 1989.
Luria AR, The frontal lobes and the regulation of behavior. In: KH Pribram and AR Luria (eds),
Psychology of the Frontal Lobes. New York, Academic Press, 1973.
Luria AR, Traumatic Aphasia. The Hague: Mouton, 1970.
Luria AR, The Working Brain. Harmondworth, England, Penguin, 1973.
Lynn R, Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: A paradox resolved. Personality and
Individual Differences 17; 257-271, 1994.
Mac Coby E, Jacklin C, The psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford, Calif: Stanford University
Press, 1974.
Mac Hale SM, O’Rourke SJ, Wardlaw JM, Dennis MS, Depression and its relation to lesion
location after stroke. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 64; 371-374, 1998.
Mach E, The Analysis of Sensations, and the Relation of the Physical to the Psychical, 1st German
edition, 1885.
MacNeilage P, The evolution of hemispheric specialization for manual function and language.
In: SP Wise (ed.), Higher Brain Functions. New York: Wiley, 1987.
MacNeilage PF, Studdert-Kennedy MG, Lindblom B, Primate handedness reconsidered. The
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10; 247-263, 1987.
MacNeilage PF, Studdert-Kennedy MG, Lindblom B, Primate handedness: A foot in the door.
The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11; 737-744, 1988.
Majeres RL, Sex differences in symbol-digit substitution and speeded matching. Intelligence 7;
313-327, 1983.
Malloy PF, Bihrale A, Duffy J, Cimino C, The orbitomedial frontal syndrome. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology 8; 185-201, 1993.
Mangun GR, Luck SJ, Plager R, Loftus W, et al., Monitoring the visual world: Hemispheric
asymmetries and subcortical processes in attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6;
267-275, 1994.
Mann L, Casuistische Betrage zur Hirnschirurgie und Hirnlokalisation. Mschr. Psychiat. Neurol.
4; 369, 1898.
Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P, Wurker M, et al., The amygdala’s contribution to memory – a study
on two patients with Urbach-Wiethe disease. Neuroreport 5; 1349-1352, 1994.
Marlowe WB, Mancall EL, Thomas JJ, Complete Kluver-Bucy syndrome in man. Cortex 11; 53,
1975.
Marshuetz C, Order information in working memory: an integrative review of evidence from brain
and behavior. Psychol Bull 131; 323-339, 2005.
Martin A, Wiggs CL, Weisberg J, Modulation of human medial temporal lobe activity by form,
meaning, and experience. Hippocampus 7; 587-593, 1997.
Martinez BA, Cain WS, de Wiji RA et al., Olfactory functioning before and after temporal lobe
resection for intractable seizures. Neuropsychology 7; 351-363, 1993.
Masdeu JC, Schoene WC, Funkenstein H, Aphasia following infarction of the left supplementary
motor area: A clinicopathological study. Neurology 28; 1220-1223, 1978.
1913
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Milner B, Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In: KH Pribram and DE Broadlent
(eds). Biology of memory New York: Academic Press, 1970.
Milner B, Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Br
Med. Bull 27; 272-277, 1971.
Milner B, Disorders of learning and memory after temporal lobe lesions in man. Clinical
Neurosurgery 19; 421-446, 1972.
Milner B, Some cognitive effects of frontal lobe lesions in man. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 298;
211-226, 1982.
Milner B, Aspects of human frontal lobe function. Advances in Neurology 66; 67-81, 1995.
Milner B, and Teuber HL, Alteration of perception and memory in man: reflections on methods.
In: L Weiskrantz (ed), Analysis of Behavioral Change. New York, NY: Harper and Row;
268-275, 1968.
Milner B, Taylor L, Right hemisphere superiority in tactile pattern recognition after cerebral
comissurotomy. Neuropsychologia 10; 1-15, 1972.
Milner B, Petrides M, Behavioral effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Journal of Neuroscience
7; 403-407, 1984.
Milner AD, Goodale MA, The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.
Milner B, Petrides M, Smith ML, Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of memory. Human
Neurobiol 4; 137-142, 1985.
Milner B, Carey DP, Harvey M, Visuality guided action and the “need to know”. Behavioral and
Brain Sciences 17; 213-214, 1994.
Milles CK, Mc Connell JW, The naming centre, with the report of a case indicating its location
in the temporal lobe. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 22; 1-7, 1895.
Mirsky AF, The neuropsychology of attention: Elements of a complex behavior. In: E Perecman
(ed), Integrating theory and practice in clinical neuropsychology. Hillsdale NJ, Erbaum,
1989.
Mohr JP, Pessin MS, Finkelstein S, et al., Broca aphasia: Pathologic and clinical. Neurology 28;
311-324, 1978.
Molfese DL, Molfese VJ, Right hemisphere responses from preschool children to temporal cues
contained in speech and nonspeech materials: Electrophysiological correlates. Brain and
Language 33; 245-259, 1988.
Monrad-Krohn GH, Dysprosody or attention in “melody of language”. Brain 70; 405-415, 1947.
Montreys CR, Borod JC, A preliminary evaluation of emotional experience and expression
following unilateral brain damage. International Journal of Science 96; 269-283, 1998.
Moreno CR, Borod JC, Welkowitz I, Alpert M, Lateralization for the expression and perception
of facial emotion as a function of age. Neuropsychologia 28; 199-209, 1990.
Morgan M, Embryology and inheritance of asymmetry. In: S Harnad, RW Doty, L Goldstein, J
Jaynes, G Krauthamer (Eds.), Lateralization in the Nervous System. New York, Academic
Press, 1977.
Mori E, Yamadori A, Rejection behavior: A human analogue of the abnormal behavior of Denny-
Brown and Chambers’ monkey with bilateral parietal ablation. J. Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
52; 1260, 1989.
Morris RG, Abrahams S, Polkey CE, Recognition memory for words and faces following
unilateral temporal lobectomy. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 34; 571-576, 1995.
1915
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Morrow LA, Vertunski PB, Kim Y, Boller F, Arousal responses to emotional stimuli and laterality
of lesions. Neuropsychologia 19; 65-71, 1981.
Moscovitch M, The development of lateralization of language functions and its relation to
cognitive and linguistic development: A review and some theoretical speculations. In: SJ
Segalowitz and FA Gruber (eds). Language Development and Neurological Theory. New
York: Academic Press, 1977.
Mottaghy FM, Gangitano M, Sparing R, et al., Segregation of areas related to visual working
memory in the prefrontal cortex revealed by rTMS. Cerebral Cortex 12; 369-375, 2002.
Muakkassa KF and Strick PL, Frontal lobe inputs to primate motor cortex: evidence for four
somatotopically organized “premotor” areas. Brain Res. 177; 176-182, 1979.
Müller NG, Knight RT, The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: contributions of human
brain lesion studies. Neuroscience 139; 51-58, 2006.
Müller NG, Machado L, Knight RT, Contributions of subregions of the prefrontal cortex to
working memory: evidence from brain lesions in humans. J Cogn Neurosci 14; 673-686,
2002.
Murdoch GE, Acquired speech and language disorders: A neuroanatomical and functional
neurological approach. New York: Chapman and Hall, 1990.
Murphy DG, DeCarli C, McIntosh AR, et al., Sex differences in human brain morphometry and
metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission
tomography study on the effect of aging. Archives of General Psychiatry 53; 585-598, 1996.
Murphy K, Peters M, Right-handers and left-handers show differences and important similarities
in task integration when performing manual and vocal tasks concurrently. Neuropsychologia
32; 663-674, 1994.
Nauta WJH, Some brain structures and functions related to memory. Neurosciences Research
Progress Bulletin II; 1-20, 1964.
Netley C, Dichotic listening of callosal agenesis and Turner’s syndrome patients. In: SJ Segalowitz
and FA Gruber (eds), Language Development and Neurological Theory, New York,
Academic Press, 376, 1977.
Neville H, Electroencephalographs testing of cerebral specialization in normal and congenitally
deaf children: A preliminary report. In: Segalowitz SJ, Gruber FA (eds) Language
development and neurological theory. New York, Acad. Press, 122-134, 1977.
Nichelli P, Grafman J, Pietrini P, et al., Where the brain appreciates the moral of a story. Neuro
Report 6; 2309-2313, 1995.
Nielsen JM, Jacobs LL, Bilateral lesions of the anterior cingulate gyri: report of case. Bull LA
Neurol Soc 16; 231-234, 1951.
Nilsson LG, Nyberg L, Klingberg T, et. al., Activity in motor areas while remembering action
events. Neuroreport 11; 2199-2201, 2000.
Nothnagel H, Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die Funktionen DES Gehirns. Archiv für
pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie und für Clinische Medizin 57; 184-214, 1873.
Nottebohm F, Origins and mechanisms in the establishment of cerebral dominance. In: MS
Gazzaniga (ed), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Neuropsychology (Vol. 2). New
York: Plenum Press, 1979.
Nottebohm F, Brain pathways for vocal learning in birds: a review of the first 10 years, Prog.
Psychobiol. Physiol. Psychol. 9; 85-124, 1981.
Novelly RA, Augustine EA, Mattson RH, et al., Selective memory improvement and impairment
in temporal lobectomy for epilepsy. Annals of Neurology 15; 64-67, 1984.
1916
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Obler LK, Zatorre RJ, Galloway L, Vaid J, Cerebral lateralization in bilinguals: methodological
issues. Brain and Language 15; 40-54, 1982.
Ochipa C, Rothi LJG, Heilman KM, Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection and use Ann
Neurol 25; 190-193, 1989.
Ochipa C, Rothi LJG, Heilman KM, Conceptual apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 114; 2593-
2603, 1992.
Ojemann GA, Organization of short-term verbal memory in language areas of human cortex:
Evidence from electrical stimulation. Brain and Language 5; 331-340, 1978.
Ojemann GA, Individual variability in cortical localization of language. Journal of Neurosurgery
50; 164-169, 1979.
Ojemann GA, Mateer C, Human language cortex: Localization of memory, syntax, and sequential
motor-phoneme identification systems. Science 205; 1401-1403, 1979.
Ornstein R, Herron J, Johnstone J, Swencionis C, Differential right hemisphere involvement in
two reading tasks. Psychology 16; 398-401, 1979.
Oppenheim H, Lehrbuch der Nervenkrankheiten für Aerzte und Studirende, II Aufl, Berlin,
Karger, 1898.
Oppenheim H, Zur Pathologie der Gehirngeschwulste. Archiv für Psychiatrie 21; 560-578, 1890.
Oscar-Berman M, McNamara P, Freedman M, Delayed-response tasks: Parallels between
experimental ablation studies and findings in patients with frontal lesions. In: HS Levin, HM
Eisenberg and AL Benton (eds), Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press, 230-255, 1991.
Owen AM, Evans AC, Petrides M, Evidence for a two stage model of spatial working memory
processing within the lateral frontal cortex: A positron emission tomography study. Cerebral
Cortex 6; 31-38, 1996.
Paillard J, Michel F, Stelmach G, Localization without content: A tactile analogue of “blindsight”.
Archives of Neurology 40, 548-551, 1983.
Pakkenberg B, Gundersen HJ, Neocortical neuron number in humans: effect of sex and age. J
Comp Neurol. 384; 312-320, 1997.
Pandya DN, Barnes CL, Architecture and connections of the frontal lobe. In: Perecman E, The
Frontal Lobes Revised. New York: IRBN Press, 1987.
Pandya DN, Yeterian EH, Comparison of prefrontal architecture and connections. In: Roberts
AC, Robbins TW, Weiskrantz L, (eds) The Prefrontal Cortex. Executive and Cognitive
Functions. New York: Oxford University Press, 1998.
Panksepp J, Affective Neuroscience: The Foundation of Human and Animal Emotions. Oxford
Univ. Press, New York, 1998.
Papanicolaou AC, Moore BD, Deutsch G, et al., Evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in
recovery from aphasia. Archives of Neurology 45; 1025-1029, 1988.
Papcun G, Krashen D, Terbeek R, et al., Is the left hemisphere organized for speech, language
and / or something else? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 55; 319-327, 1974.
Papez JW, A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 38; 725-743, 1937.
Paradiso S, Robinson RG, Gender differences in post-stroke depression. Journal of Neuro-
psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 10; 41-47, 1998.
Pardo JV, Fox PT, Raichle ME, Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron
emission tomography. Nature 34; 61-64, 1991.
Pardo JV, Pardo TP, Raichle ME, Natural correlates of self-induced dysphoria. American Journal
of Psychiatry 150; 713-719, 1993.
1917
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Pick A, Über Störungen der Orientierung am einigen Körper. Berlin, Karger, 1908.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C, Rivaud S, Gaymard B, Agid Y, Corectal control of memory-guided
saccades in man. Exp Brain Res 83; 607-617, 1991.
Pietrini P, Guazzelli M, Gasso G, et al., Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed
by positron emission tomography in healthy subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry 157;
1772-1781, 2000.
Pilcher WH, Ojemann GA, Presurgical evaluation and epilepsy surgery. In: Apuzzo MLJ (ed)
Brain Surgery: Complications, Avoidance and Management. New York, Raven Press, 417-
424, 1993.
Pillieri G, The Kluver-Bucy syndrome in man. Psychiatr, Neurol 152; 65, 1967.
Pillon B, Bazin B, Deweer B, et al., Specificity of memory deficits after right or left temporal
lobectomy. Cortex 35; 561-571, 1999.
Pimental PA, Kingsbury NA, The injured right hemisphere: Classification of related disorders.
In: Neuropsychological aspects of right brain injury. Austin TX: Pro-Ed, 1989.
Pincus JH, Tucker GJ, Behavioral Neurology (4th ed). New York: Oxford University Press, 2003.
Pizzamiglio L, Mammucari A, Disturbance of facial emotional expression in brain-damaged
subjects. In: G Gainotti and C Caltagirone (eds), Emotions and the Dual Brain. Berlin:
Springer-Verlag, 1989.
Ploner CJ, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Gaymard BM, et al., Errors of memory-guided saccades in humans
with lesions of the frontal eye field and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neuropsychol
82; 1986-1090, 1999.
Poizner H, Bellugi U, Klima ES, Biological foundations of language. Clue from sign language.
annual Review in Neuroscience 13; 283-307, 1990.Poppel D, Some remaining questions
about studying phonological processing with PET: Response to Demonet, Fiez, Paulescu,
Patersen, and Zatorre. Brain and Language 55; 380-385, 1996.
Posner MI, Dehaene S, Attentional networks. Trend in Neuroscience 2; 75-79, 1994.
Posner MI, Hierarchical distributed networks in the neuropsychology of selective attention. In:
Caramazza (ed), Cognitive Neuropsychology and Neurolinguistics: Advances in Models of
Cognitive Function and Impairment. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1990.
Pötzl O, Uiberall H, Zur Pathologie der Amusie. Wiener Klin Wschr 50; 770, 1937.
Pötzl O, Zur Pathologie der Amusie. Z. Neurol. Psychiat. 165; 187, 1939.
Pötzl O, Bemerkungen zum Problem der Kortikalen Vorgänge bei der akustisken Wahrnehmung.
Mschr. Ohrenhlk 77; 422, 1943.
Prather P, Jarmulowicz L, Barwnell H, Gardner H, Selective attention and the right hemisphere:
A failure in integration, not detection (abstract). Journal of clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology 14, 35, 1992.
Preuss TM, Caceres M, Oldham MC, Geschwind DH, Human brain evolution: Insight from
microscopy. Nature Reviews: Genetics 5; 850-860, 2004.
Price CJ, Functional anatomy of reading. In: RSJ Franckowiak et al., (eds), Human Brain
Function. San Diego: Academic Press, 1997.
Prigatano GP, Personality and psychosocial consequences after brain injury. In: MJ Meier et al.
(eds) Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone, 1987.
Proust A, Arch Gen Méd. Cited by Ballet (1866) and Henschen (1920).
Proust A, De l’Aphasie. Arch. Gen. Méd. 1, 147, 1872.
Provins KA, Handedness and speech: A critical reappraisal of the role of genetic and environ-
mental factors in the cerebral lateralization of function. Psycholo Rev 104; 554-571, 1997.
1919
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Ptito A, Crane J, Leonard G, et al, Visual-spatial localization by patients with frontal lobe lesions
invading or sparing area 46. NeuroReport 6; 1781-1784, 1995.
Pugh KR, Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, et al. Cerebral organization of component processes in
reading. Brain 119; 1221-1238, 1996.
Raichle ME, Fiez JA, Videen TO et al., Practice-related changes in human brain functional
anatomy during nonmotor learning. Cerebral Cortex 4; 8-26, 1994.
Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic P, Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal areas in the normal
human cortex: II. Variability in locations of areas 9 and 46 and relationship to the Talairach
coordinate system. Cerebral Cortex 5; 323-337, 1995.
Rapport RL, Tan CT, Whitaker HA, Language function and dysfunction among Chinese- and
English-speaking polyglots: cortical stimulation, Wada testing and clinical studies. Brain and
Language 18; 342-366, 1983.
Rasmussen T, Surgery of frontal lobe epilepsy. In: Purpura DP, Penry JK, Walter RD, (eds)
Advances in Neurology, Vol. 8. New York: Raven Press, 1975.
Rasmussen T, Commentary: Extratemporal cortical excisions and hemispherectomy. In: Engel J
Jr (ed): Surgical Treatment of the Epilepsies. New York: Raven Press, 417-424, 1987.
Rasmussen T, Milner B, The role of early left-brain injury in determining lateralization of cerebral
speech functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 299; 355-369, 1977.
Ratcliff G, Dila C, Taylor L, Milner B, The morphological asymmetry of the hemispheres and
cerebral dominance for speech: A possible relationship. Brain and Language 11; 87-98, 1980.
Regard M, and Landis, The Procedure vs. content learning: effects of emotionality and repetition
in a new clinical memory test (Abstract. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
psychology 10; 86, 1988.
Reite M, Sheeder J, Teale P, et al., MEG based brain laterality: sex differences in normal adults.
Neuropsychologia 33; 1607-1617, 1995.
Rivers DL, Love RL, Language performance on visual processing tasks in right hemisphere lesion
cases. Brain and Language 10; 348-366, 1980.
Rizzolatti G, Matelli M, Pavesi G, Deficits in attention and movement following the removal of
postarcuate (area 6) and prearcuate (area 8) cortex in macaque monkeys. Brain 106; 655-673,
1983.
Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Matelli M, et al., Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET:
1. Observation versus execution. Experimental Brain Research 111; 246-252, 1996 a.
Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L, Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions.
Cognitive Brain Research 3; 131-141, 1996 b.
Robertson LC, Rafal R, Disorders of visual attention. In: Gazzaniga (ed), The New Cognitive
Neuroscience (2nd ed), 633-649. Cambridge: MIT Press, 2000.
Robinson RG, Neuropsychiatric consequences of stroke. Ann Rev Med 48; 217-229, 1997.
Robinson RG, Manes F, Elation, mania, and mood disorders. Evidence from neurological disease.
In: JC Borod (ed), The Neuropsychology of Emotion. New York: Oxford, University Press,
2000.
Robinson RG, Starkstein SE, Neuropsychiatric aspects of cerebrovascular disorders. In: SC
Yudofsky and RE Hales (eds). Essentials of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences.
Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press, 2002.
Robinson RG, Kubas KL, Starr LB, et al., Mood disorders in stroke patients : Importance of
location of lesion. Brain 107; 81-93, 1984.
1920
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Rogers LJ, Early experimental effects on laterality: Research on chicks has relevance to other
species. Laterality 2; 199-219, 1997.
Roland PE, Metabolic measurements of the working frontal cortex in man. Trends Neurosci. 7;
430-435, 1984.
Rolls ET, The Brain and Emotion. Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1999.
Ross DA, Glick SD, Right-sided population bias and lateralization of activity in normal rats. Brain
Res. 205; 222-225, 1981.
Rossor M, Fahrenkrug J, Emson P, et al., Reduced cortical choline acetyltransferase activity in
senile dementia of Alzheimer type is not accompanied by changes in vasoactive intestinal
polypeptide. Brain Res 201; 249-253, 1980.
Ross DA, Affective prosody and the aprosodias. In: MM Mesulam (ed), Principles of Behavioral
and Cognitive Neurology. New York, Oxford University Press, 2000.
Ross ED, The aprosodias. In: TE Feinberg and MJ Farah (eds), Behavioral Neurology and
Neuropsychology (2nd ed). New York: McGraw-Hill, 2003.
Ross ED, Mesulam MM, Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and
emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology 36; 144-148, 1979.
Ross ED, Rush AJ, Diagnosis and neuroanatomical correlates of depression in brain damaged
patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 38; 1344-1354, 1981.
Rossion B, Dicot L, Devolder A, et al., Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based
face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12; 793-
802, 2000.
Ruckdeschel-Hibbard M, Gordon WA, Diller L, Affective disturbances associated with brain
damage. In: SB Filskov and TJ Boll (eds), Handbook of clinical neuropsychology (Vol. 2).
New York: Wiley, 1986.
Rudel RG, Denckla MB, Spalten E, The functional asymmetry of Braille letter learning in normal
sighted children. Neurology 24; 733-738, 1974.
Sackeim HA, Gur RC, Saucy MC, Emotions are expressed more intensely on the left side of the
face. Science 202; 434-436, 1978.
Sackeim HA, Greenberg MS, Weiman AL, et al., Hemisphere asymmetry in the expression of
positive and negative emotions. Archives of Neurology 39; 210-218, 1982.
Salazar AM, Grafman J, Jabbari B, et al., Epilepsy and cognitive loss after penetrating head injury.
Advances in Epidemiology 16; 627-631, 1987.
Salazar AM, Grafman J, Schlesselman S, et al., Penetrating war injuries of the basal forebrain:
Neurology and Cognition. Neurology 36; 459-465, 1986.
Sass KJ, Buchanan CP, Kraemer S, et al., Verbal memory impairment resulting from hippocampal
neuron loss among epileptic patients with structure lesions. Neurology 45; 2154-2158, 1995.
Sato R, Bryan RN, Fried LP, Neuroanatomic and functional correlates of depresses of depressed
mood: The Cardiovascular Health Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 150; 919-929,
1999.
Scheibel AB, Paul LA, Fried J, et al., Dendritic organization of the anterior speech area.
Experimental Neurobiology 87; 109-117, 1985.
Schilder P, The Image and Appearance of the Human Body. K Paul, London, 1935.
Schlaepfer TE, Harris GJ, Tien AY, et al., Structural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy
female and male subjects: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research 61;
129-135, 1995.
1921
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Schluter ND, Krams M, Rushorth MF, Passingham RE, Cerebral dominance for action in the
human brain: the selection of action. Neuropsychologia 39; 105-113, 2001.
Schramm J, Behrens E, Entzian W, Hemispherical deafferentation: An alternative to functional
hemispherectomy. Neurosurgery 36; 509-516, 1995.
Schramm J, Kral T, Clusmann H, Transsylvian keyhole functional hemispherectomy. Neuro-
surgery 49; 891-901, 2001.
Schuster P, Taterka H, Beitrag zur Anatomie und Klinik der reinen worttaubheit. Z. Neurol
Psychiat 105; 494, 1926.
Schwartz J, Tallal P, Rate of acoustic change may underlie hemispheric specialization for speech
perception. Science 207; 1380-1381, 1980.
Searleman A, A review of right hemisphere linguistic capabilities. Psychological Bulletin 84;
503-528, 1977.
Seidenberg M, Hermann B, Wyler AR, et al., Neuropsychological outcome following anterior
temporal lobectomy in patients with and without the syndrome of musical temporal lobe
epilepsy. Neuropsychology 12; 303-316, 1998.
Seidenwurm D, Bird CR, Enzmann DR, Marshall WH, Left-right temporal region asymmetry in
infants and children. AJNR 6; 777-779, 1985.
Semmes J, Weinstein L, Ghent L, Teuber HL, Somatosensory changes after penetrating brain
wounds in man. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press, 1960.
Semmes J, Weinstein L, Ghent L, Teuber HL, Correlates of impaired orientation in personal and
extrapersonal space. Brain 86; 747-772, 1963.
Semmes J, Turner B, Effects of cortical lesions on somatosensory task. Journal of Investigations
in Dermatology 69; 181-189, 1977.
Sergent J, Unified response to bilateral hemispheric stimulation by a split-brain patient. Nature
305; 800-802, 1983.
Sergent J, Inferences from unilateral brain damage about normal hemispheric functions in visual
pattern recognition. Psychological Bulletin 96; 99-115, 1984.
Sergent J, Face perception and the right hemisphere. In: L Weiskrantz (ed), Through without
Language. Oxford Clarendon Press, 1988.
Sergent J, Furtive incursions into bicameral minds. Brain 113; 537-568, 1990.
Sergent J, Processing of spatial relations within and between the disconnected cerebral
hemispheres. Brain 114; 1025-1043, 1991.
Sergent J, Judgment of relative position and distance on representations of spatial relations. Journal
of Exp. Psychol: Human Perception and Performance 91; 762-780, 1991.
Seymour SE, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Gazzaniga MS, The disconnection syndrome: Basic findings
reaffirmed. Brain 117; 105-115, 1994.
Shapiro BE, Danly M, The role of the right hemisphere in the control of speech prosody in
positional and affective contents. Brain and Language 25; 19-36, 1985.
Shapiro BE, Grossman M, Gardner H, Selective musical processing deficits in brain damage
populations. Neuropsychologia 19; 161-169, 1981.
Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, et al., Sex differences in the functional organization of the
brain for language. Nature 373; 607-609, 1995.
Shepherd GM, and Green CA, Olfactory bulb. In: GM, Shepherd (ed), The Synaptic Organization
of the Brain (4th ed). New York: Oxford University Press, 1998.
Shimamura AP, Janowsky JS, Squire LR, Memory for the temporal order of events in patients
with frontal lobe lesions and amnestic patients. Neuropsychologia 28; 803-813, 1990.
1922
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Shimizu T, Nariai T, Maehara T, et al., Enhanced motor cortical excitability in the unaffected
hemisphere after hemispherectomy. Neuroreport 11; 3077-3084, 2000.
Shimoda K, Robinson RG, The relationship between poststroke depression and lesion location
on long-term follow-up. Radiological Psychiatry 45; 187-192, 1999.
Simonds RJ, Scheibel AB, The postnatal development of the motor speech area: A preliminary
study. Brain and Language 37; 42-58, 1989.
Singh A, Blake SE, Herrman N, et al., Functional and neuroanatomic correlation in poststroke
depression. The Sunnybrook Stroke study. Stroke 31; 637-644, 2000.
Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA, Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET.
Cortex Cerebral 6; 11-20, 1996.
Smith ML, Memory disorders associated with temporal-lobe lesions. In: F. Boller and J Grafman
(eds). Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 3, Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1989.
Smith WK, The functional significance of the rostral cingular cortex by its response to electrical
excitation. J Neurophisiol 8; 241-255, 1945.
Sowell ER, Trauner DA, Gamst A, Jernigan TL, Development of cortical and subcortical brain
structures in childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol.
44; 4-16, 2002 a.
Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Rex D, et al., Mapping sulcal pattern asymmetry and local cortical
surface gray matter distribution in vivo: maturation in perisylvian cortices. Cerebral Cortex
12; 17-26, 2002 b.
Sperry RW, Consciousness, personal identity and the divided brain. In: Lepore, F, Ptito, M., and
Jasper, H (Eds), Two Hemispheres: One Brain. New York, Alen Liss; 3-20, 1986.
Spreen O, Benton AL., and Fincham RW, Auditory agnosia without aphasia. Arch. Neurol. 13,
84, 1965.
Springer SP, Deutch G, Left Brain, Right brain. Freeman, New York, 1995
Springer SP, Deutsch G, Left Brain / Right Brain; Perceptives from Cognitive Neuroscience.
Freeman, New York 1998.
St. John RC, Lateral asymmetry in face perception. Can J, Psychol 35; 213-223, 1981.
Starkstein SE, Bryer JB, Bethier ML, et al., Depression after stroke: The importance of cerebral
hemisphere asymmetries. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 3; 276-285,
1991.
Starkstein SE, Federoff JP, Price TR, et al., Neuropsychological and neuroradiologic correlates
of emotional prosody comprehension. Neurology 44; 515-522, 1994.
Starkstein SE, Robinson RG, Mechanism of disinhibition after brain lesions. Journal of Nervous
and Mental Diseases 185, 108-114, 1997.
Starkstein SE, Pearlson GD, Boston J, Robinson RG, Mania after brain injury: A controlled study
of causative factors. Archives of Neurology 44; 1069-1973, 1987.
Starkstein SE, Robinson RG, Honing MA, et al., Mood changes after right-hemisphere lesions.
British Journal of Psychiatry 155; 79-85, 1989.
Stelmach GE, Motor learning: Toward understanding acquired representations. In: JR Bloedel,
TJ Ebner and SP Wise (eds), The Acquisition of Motor Behavior in Vertebrates. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press, 391-408, 1996.
Stemmer B, Joanette Y, The interpretation of narrative discourse of brain-damaged individuals
within the framework of multilevel discourse model. In: M Beeman and C Chiarello (eds),
Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension. Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience.
Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum, 1998.
1923
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Stengel E, Loss of spatial orientation, constructional apraxia and Gerstmann’s syndrome. J Ment.
Sci. 90; 753-760, 1944.
Stepniewska I, Preuss TM, Kaas JH, Architectonics, somatotopic organization, and ipsilateral
cortical connections of the primary motor area (M1) of owl monkey. Journal of Comparative
Neurology. 330; 238-271, 1993.
Stewart J, Kolb B, The effects of neonatal gonadectomy and prenatal stress on cortical thickness
and asymmetry in rats. Behav. Neural Biol. 49; 344-360, 1988.
Stewart J, Kolb B, Dendritic branching in cortical pyramidal cells in response to ovariectomy in
adult female rats: suppression by neonatal exposure to testosterone. Brain Research 654; 149-
154, 1994.
Stone J, The naso-temporal division of the cat’s retina. J. Comp. Neur. 126; 585-600, 1966.
Strauss E, Lapointe JS, Wada JA, et al., Language dominance: correlation of radiological and
functional data. Neuropsychologia 23; 415-420, 1985.
Strauss E, Wada J, Hunter M, Sex-related differences in the cognitive consequences of early left-
hemisphere lesions. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 14; 738–748, 1992.
Stuss DT, Assessment of neuropsychological dysfunction in frontal lobe degeneration. Dementia
4; 220-225, 1993.
Stuss DT, Interference effects on memory functions in postleukotomy patients: an attentional
perspective. In: HS Levin, HM Eisenberg and AL Benton (eds), Frontal Lobe Function and
Dysfunction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 157-172, 1991.
Stuss DT, Benson DF, The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press, 1986.
Stuss DT, Benson DF, The frontal lobes and language. In: E Goldberg (ed). Contemporary
Neuropsychology and the Legacy of Luria. Hillsdale NJ, Erlbaum, 1990.
Stuss DT, Benson DF, Kaplan EF, et al., The involvement of orbitofrontal cerebrum in cognitive
tasks. Neuropsychologia 21; 235-248, 1983.
Stuss DT, Eskes GA, Foater JK, Experimental neuropsychological studies of frontal functions.
In: F Boller and J Grafman (eds), Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 9), Amsterdam:
Elsevier, 1994.
Subirana A, Handedness and cerebral dominance. In: PJ Vinken, GW Bruyn, M Critchley and
JAM Frederiks (eds), Handbook of Clincal Neurology, Vol. 4, Amsterdam; North-Holland
Publishing Co., 1969.
Sun T, Patione C, Abu-Khalil A, et al., Early asymmetry of gene transcription in embryonic human
left and right cerebral cortex. Science 308; 1794-1798, 2005.
Swaab DF, Fliers E, A sexually dimorphic nucleus in the human brain. Science 228; 1112-1115,
1995.
Swithenby SJ, Bailey AJ, Brautigam S, et al., Neural processing of human faces: A magneto-
encephalographic study. Experimental Brain Research 118; 501-510, 1998.
Tanji J, The supplementary motor area in the cerebral cortex. Neurosci Res 19; 251-268, 1994.
Tanji J, Shima K, Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead.
Nature 371; 413-416, 1994.
Tellier A, Adams KM, Walker AE, Rourke BP, Long-term effects of severe penetrating head
injury on psychosocial adjustment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 58; 531-
537, 1990.
Thompson PM, Cannon TD, Narr KL, et al., Genetic influences on brain structure. Nature
Neuroscience 4; 1253-1258, 2001.
Thomas H, Jamison W, Hummel DD, Observation is insufficient for discovering that the surface
of still water is invariantly horizontal. Science 191; 173-174, 1973.
1924
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Thurnam J, On the weight of the brain and the circumstances affecting it. Journal of mental science,
1866.
Tinley F, The brain of prehistoric man. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry 17; 289-299, 1927.
Toga AW, Thompson PM, Mori S, Amunts K, Zilles K, Towards multimodal atlases of the human
brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7; 952-966, 2006.
Tognola G, Vignola LA, Brain lesions associated with oral apraxia in stroke patients: A clinico-
neuroradiological investigation with the CT scan. Neuropsychologia 18; 257-272, 1980.
Tonkonogy J and Goodglass H, Language function, foot of the third frontal gyrus, and Rolandic
operculum. Archives of Neurology 38; 486-490, 1981.
Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M, Two cortical visual system. In: DJ Ingle and RJF Mansfield (eds),
Analysis of Visual Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1982.
Uyehara JM, Cooper WC, Hemispheric differences for verbal and nonverbal stimuli in Japanese-
and English-speaking subjects assessed by Tsunoda’s method. Brain and Language 10; 405-
417, 1980.
Valdes JJ, Mactutus CF, Cory RN, Cameron WR. Lateralization of norepinephrine, serotonin and
choline uptake into hippocampal synaptosomes of sinistral rats. Physiol Behav 27; 381-383,
1981.
Vallortigara G, Comparative neuropsychology of the dural brain: A stroll through animals’ left
and right perceptual worlds. Brain and Language 73, 189-219, 2000.
Van Lancker D, Nicklay CKH, Comprehension of personally relevant (PERL) versus novel
language in two globally aphasic patients. Aphasiology 6; 37-61, 1992.
Van Lancker DR, Sidtis JJ, The identification of affective-prosodic stimuli by left- and right-
hemisphere-damaged subjects: all errors are not created equal. J. Speech and Hearing
Research 35 (5); 963-970, 1992.
Van Lancker DR, Kreiman J, Cummings J, Voice perception deficits: Neuroanatomical correlates
of phonoagnosia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 11; 665-674, 1989.
Varfaellie M, Heilman KM, Response preparation and response inhibition after lesions of the
medial frontal lobe. Archives of Neurology 44; 1265-1271, 1987.
Varney NR, Linguistic correlates of pantomime recognition. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 41;
564-568, 1978.
Varney NR, Benton AL, Qualitative aspects of pantomime recognition defects in aphasia. Brain
Cogn. 1; 132-139, 1982.
Varney NR, Menefee L, Psychosocial and executive deficits following closed head injury:
Implications for orbital frontal cortex. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation 8; 32-44, 1993.
Vendrell P, Junque C, Pujol J, et al., The role of prefrontal regions in the Strop task.
Neuropsychologia 33; 341-352, 1995.
Verin M, Partiot A, Pillon B, et al., Delayed response tasks and prefrontal lesions in man-evidence
for self-generated patterns of behavior with poor environmental modulation.
Neuropsychologia 31; 1379-1396, 1993.
Vidor M, Personality changes following prefrontal leucotomy as referred by the Minnesota
Multiphasic Personality Inventory and the resulty of psychometric testing. J Ment Sci 97;
159-173, 1951.
Vignolo LA, Auditory agnosia: a review and report of recent evidence. In: Benton AL (ed),
Contributions to Clinical Neuropsychology. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Co., Ch. 7; 172-208,
1969.
Villemure JG, Mascott CR: Peri-insular hemispherotomy: surgical principles and anatomy.
Neurosurgery 37; 975–981, 1995.
1925
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Von Bonin G, Anatomical asymmetries of the cerebral hemispheres. In: VB Mountcastle (ed),
Interhemispheric Relations and Cerebral Dominance. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
Press, 1-6, 1962.
Wada J, Rasmussen T, Intra-carotid injection of sodium amytal for the lateralization of cerebral
speech dominance. Journal of Neurosurgery 17; 266-282, 1960.
Wada JA, Clarke R, Hamm A, Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in humans. Archives of
Neurology 32; 239-246, 1975.
Wagner AD, Poldrack RA, Eldridge LL, et al., Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal
activation during episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroreport 9; 3711-3717, 1998.
Warburton E, Wise RJ, Price CJ, Weiller C, et al., Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects.
Studies with PET. Brain 119, 159-179, 1996.
Warren JM, Functional Lateralization of the Brain. In: Evolution and the Lateralization of the
Brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 299; 273-280, 1977.
Warrington EK, Taylor AM, The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition.
Cortex 9; 152-164, 1973.
Wasserstein J, Zappulla R, Rosen J, et al., In search of closure: Subjective contour illusions, gestalt
completion tests, and implications. Brain and Cognition 6; 1-14, 1987.
Watson RT, Shepherd Fleet W, Gonzales-Rothi L, Heilman KM, Apraxia and supplementary
motor area. Arch. Neurol 43; 787-792, 1986.
Weber DP, Sex differences in cognition: A function of the maturation rate? Science 192; 572-
574, 1976.
Webster WG, Thurber AD, “Problem-solving strategies and manifest brain asymmetry”. Cortex
14; 474-484, 1978.
Weintraub S, Mesulam M-M, Right cerebral dominance in spatial attention. Arch Neurol 44; 621,
1987.
Weintraub S, Mesulam M-M, and Kramer L, Disturbances in prosody: a right-hemisphere
contribution to language. Archves of Neurology 38; 742-744, 1981.
Welt L, Über Charakterveranderungen des Menschen infologe von Läsionen des Stirnhirns,
Deutsche Archiv für Kliniche Medizin 42; 339-390, 1888.
Wernicke C, Der aphasische Symptomencomplex: Breslau, Cohn and Weigert, Breslau, 1874.
Wilson SAK, An experimental research into the anatomy and physiology of the corpus striatum.
Brain 36; 427-492, 1914.
Wilson FA, Scalaidhe SP, Goldman-Rakic PS, Dissociation of object and spatial processing
domains in primate prefrontal cortex. Science 260; 1955-1958, 1993.
Witelson SF, Early hemisphere specialization and interhemispheric plasticity: An empirical and
theoretical review. In: SJ Segalowitz and FA Gruber (eds), Language Development and
Neurological Theory. New York: Academic Press, 1977.
Witelson SF, Anatomic asymmetry in the temporal lobes: its documentation, phylogenesis, and
relationship to functional asymmetry. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 299;
328-354, 1977.
Witelson SF, The brain connection: The corpus callosum is larger in left-handers. Science 229;
665-668, 1985.
Witelson SF, Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of the human corpus callosum: A
postmortem morphological study. Brain 112; 799-835, 1989.
Witelson SF, Cognitive neuroanatomy: a new era. Neurology 42 (4); 709-13, 1992.
1926
23. Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans
Witelson SF, Neuroanatomical bases of hemispheric functional specialization in the human brain:
Possible developmental factors. In: FL Kitterle (ed), Hemispheric Communication:
Mechanisms and Models Hillsdale NJ, Elbaum, 1995.
Witelson SF, Goldsmith CH, The relationship of hand preference to anatomy of the corpus
callosum in men. Brain Research 545; 175-182, 1991.
Witelson SF, Kigar DL, Sylvian fissure morphology and asymmetry in men and women: bilateral
differences in relation to handedness in men. Journal of Comparative Neurology 323; 326-
340, 1992.
Witelson SF, Lezner II, Kigar DL, Women have greater density of neurons in posterior temporal
cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 15; 3418–3428, 1995.
Wolf ME, and Goodale MA, Oral asymmetries during verbal and non-verbal movements of the
mouth. Neuropsychologia 25; 375-396, 1987.
Woolley CS, Gould E, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS, Naturally occurring fluctuations in dendritic
spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci; 10; 4035-4039, 1990.
Woolsey CN, Settlage PH, Meyer DR, et al., Patterns of localization in precentral and
“supplementary” motor areas and their relation to the concept of a premotor area. Res Publ
Ass Nerv Ment Dis 30; 238-262, 1952.
Yakovlev PL, Rakic P, Patterns of decussation of bulbar pyramids and distribution of pyramidal
tracts on two sides of the spinal cord. Trans American Neurological Association 91; 366-367,
1966.
Yamadori A, Osumi Y, Masuhara S, Okubo M, Preservation of singing in Broca’s aphasia. Journal
of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 40; 221-224, 1977.
Yamadori A, Mori E, Tabuchi M, et al., Hypergraphia: a right hemisphere syndrome. Journal of
Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 49, 1160-1164, 1986.
Zaidel E, Lexical organization in the right hemisphere. In: PA Buser and Rougeul-Buser (eds).
Cerebral Correlates of Conscious Experience. INSERM Symposium 6. Amsterdam Elsevier
/ North-Holland, 1978.
Zaidel E, Language functions in the two hemispheres following cerebral commissurotomy and
hemispherectomy. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (eds) Handbook of Neuropsychology. New York
Elsevier. Vol. 4; 115-150, 1990.
Zaidel E, Iacobini M, The parallel brain. Cambridge: Mit Press, 2002.
Zaidel E, Aboitiz F, Clarke J et al., Sex differences in interhemispheric relation for language. In:
FL Kitterle (ed), Hemispheric Communication: Mechanisms and Models. Hillsdale NJ,
Erlbaum, 1995.
Zangwill OL, Intelligence in aphasia. In: Reuck AVS, O’Conner M (eds), Disorders of Language.
Boston: Little Brown and Co.; 261-274, 1964.
Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M, Human olfactory discrimination after unilateral frontal or temporal
lobectomy. Brain 114; 71-84, 1991.
Zatorre RJ, Binder RJ, Functional and structural imaging of the human auditory system. In: Brain
Mapping: The Systems. A Toga and J Mazziotta (eds). Academic Press, San Diego; 365-402,
2000.
Zatorre RJ, Peretz I (eds), The biological foundations of music. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 930; 2001.
Zatorre RJ, Meyer E, Gjedde A, Evans AC, PET studies of phonetic processing of speech: Review,
replication, and reanalysis. Cerebral Cortex 6; 21-30, 1996.
Zimmerberg B, Glick SD, Serussi TP, Neurochemical correlate of a spatial preference in rats.
Science 185; 623-625, 1974.
Zimmerberg B, Strumpf AJ, Glick SD, Cerebral asymmetry and left-right discrimination, Brain
Research 140; 194-196, 1978.
1927
THE NEURAL BASIS
OF CONSCIOUSNESS
Introduction
Consciousness is an universal set of neurologic and mental or spiritual processes
produced by the brain in awake state, which allows people to understand the mind of
others (mind reading); it also allows an optimal social living to perform any kind of high
normal behavior activity (social, political, professional, moral, ethic, religious, etc.)
related to the self and the environment (family, society).
So, the two separate forms of consciousness are neurologic and mental or spiritual.
The neurologic form of consciousness is based on interactions between the brain stem,
midbrain, diencephalon, limbic system and the cerebral cortex. There is the idea of being
conscious versus unconscious.
Consciousness may be also defined as our awareness of our environment, our bodies
and ourselves (Hobson, 2007). Awareness of ourselves implies an awareness of
awareness, that is the conscious recognition that we are a conscious being. Awareness of
ourselves implies meta-cognition (Hobson, 2007).
Although many theorists treat consciousness as single, all-or-nothing phenomenon,
others distinguish between first-order consciousness and a meta-level of consciousness.
For example, they may distinguish between consciousness and metaconsciousness
(Schooler, 2002), primary consciousness and higher-order consciousness (Edelman and
Tononi, 2000), or core consciousness and extended consciousness (Damasio, 1999).
Animals possess primary (core) consciousness which comprises sensory awareness,
attention, perception, memory (or learning), emotion and action (Edelman, 1992).
According to Hobson (2007), what differentiates humans from their fellow mammals and
gives humans what Edelman defines as secondary consciousness, depends upon language
and the associated enrichment of cognition that allow humans to develop and to use verbal
and numeric abstraction. Spoken and written language are human specializations. These
mental capacities contribute to our sense of self as agents and as creative beings. It also
determines the awareness of awareness that we assume our animal “collaborators” do
not possess (Hobson, 2007).
Many factors are involved in the establishing levels of consciousness commonly
referred to as wakefulness and sleep. Such factors include the enormous driving of the
cerebral cortex over the ascending activating and inhibiting systems and the influence of
1928
24. The neural basis of consciousness
cyclic limbic activation of cerebral cortical areas. There are complex interconnections
between these areas of the nervous system: the hypothalamus, thalamus and hippocampal
formation. Our state of consciousness includes accompanying autonomic responses such
as changes in respiration, heart rate, or body temperature. Injuries that involve a
considerable part of tegmentum of the midbrain result in a profound coma because of the
interruption of ascending multisynaptic activating system (AAS) as well as descending
hypothalamotegmental and dorsal longitudinal fasciculus path.
In humans, the complex system of mental and spiritual processes depends on and is
produced by the highest psychical activities, i.e., it depends on and is produced by the
brain, making people: use symbolic representation and language; reflect on the past and
anticipate and plan for the future; transform thought into speech and action; logically
record the personal experience and transmit it orally by writing and / or by drawing;
participate in the progress and civilization; read through other people’s thoughts, judge
correctly their intentions and act consequently; use abstractization and generalization to
make new discoveries; know, spread and protect the ethical, moral and religious standards
in order to live an optimal social life; organize behavior and extrapolate it in time; deal
with cognitive novelty.
The prediction and comprehension of others’ behavior are evidently very important
aspects of social functioning.
The consciousness processes belong, in essence, to people who act by control
mechanisms of psychological activities, generalization and abstractization mechanisms,
exploring and handling of mental images to solve all the problems man is facing with.
In humans, the level of consciousness depends on the complexity of the brain
ontogenetic evolution; the human brain makes culture and technology possible. We believe
that the cerebral cortex is absolutely necessary for this function; machines that are
responsive to sensory events and are capable of complex movements are not conscious.
According to this view, the brain stem occupies the bottom of the totem pole, providing
the basic arousal mechanism without which the higher brain regions cannot operate.
On the other hand, consciousness must be a function of numerous interacting
systems. The major structures supposed to play a key role in the neural correlates of
consciousness are: the brain stem, the midbrain, the diencephalon (especially the
hypothalamus and thalamus), the limbic system, and the cerebral cortex.
Thus, the brain “language” can be conceptualized as the transmission of neural
signals (Yamazaki and Tanaka, 2007). The “grammar” of the brain’s language system
concerns the proper timing of neuronal impulses. Neuronal impulse timing is based upon
proper integration and balance of excitatory and inhibitory processes (Hatta et al., 2004).
been diagnosed with brain tumors, and one patient with encephalitis; patient mean age
was 42,88 years, the youngest being 21 and the oldest 73 years.
Six (66,66%) of the patients were males and three (33,33) females. In 8 patients, the
main examination was computed tomography (CT) and magnetic resonance imaging
(MRI). In the patient presenting multiple brain metastases, the brain was subject to
examination after death.
In this section we will present the clinical symptomatology of the above patients.
Case 1. A 21-year-old man presented with a history of sudden onset of coma.
MRI-scan revealed a bilateral ponto-mesencephalic hemorrhage triggered by a
cavernoma. After total resection of the cavernoma and hematoma, patient’s status has
improved significantly, presenting only a remaining minimal right side weakness and
hemisensory deficit. Now the patient is a student, and has an excellent state.
Case 2. A 38-year-old woman presented with logorrhea syndrome, with hyper-
kinesia, hyperwakefulness and hyperprosexia. The patient could sleep for only a short
time, and then awake and was unable to fall back asleep. MRI-scan revealed a petroclival
meningioma which significantly compresses the superior part of the brain stem.
Case 3. A 36-year-old man presented with intermittent increased intracranial
pressure, paralysis of the conjugate upward gaze (Parinaud’s sign) and pseudo-Argyll
Robertson pupil. A contrast-enhanced CT-scan revealed a tumor of the pineal area. Only
15% of the patients with tumors of the pineal area, which are compressing the dorsal area
of the mesencephalon, also experience arousal disorders.
Case 4. A 29-year-old woman presented with left hemiparesis by compression of the
adjacent internal capsule, palsies of vertical and lateral gaze, absence of convergence,
retraction nystagmus, and mild sensory deficit in the opposite side of the body, including
the trunk. Pain and thermal sensation was more affected than touch, vibration and position.
Involvement of ventral posterolateral and posteromedial nuclei of the thalamus
causes loss or diminution of all forms of sensation on the opposite side of the body.
Contrast medium-enhanced coronal and axial MRI shows a big right thalamic tumor.
The tumor was fully resected and after the surgery the patient remained with a mild
sensory deficit and a hemiparesis, but she returned to an independent life.
Case 5. A 56-year-old man presented with a sudden onset of coma and fever.
Coronal and axial Tl-weighted MRI-scans revealed edematous, demyelination
symmetrical changes infra- and supratentorial which involved the entire midbrain, both
thalamic formations, bilateral basal ganglia, two-side temporo-occipital convolution and
hippocampus, determined by encephalitis (limbic encephalitis). After 9 days of coma the
patient died.
Case 6. A 56-year-old man with a history of severe headaches, presented with taste
and smell disorder, excessive euphoria accompanied by peculiar kind of compulsive,
shallow and childish humor (moria), irritability, hypomania and puerilism.
The patient also shows disorders of attention and motility, distractibility, hyper-
reactivity, hyperkinesia, perseveration, emotional lability, cognitive dysfunction that
impede the initiation and temporal organization of actions and lacks of initiative, wrong
decision and instinctual disinhibition.
1930
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Contrast enhanced computed tomography (CT) scan shows a giant size olfactory
groove meningioma and displacement of anterior brain.
Post surgery, after complete resection of the tumor, the orbitofrontal syndrome
disappeared almost completely.
Case 7. A 35-year-old woman was admitted to our department of neurosurgery with
a bilateral meningioma of the anterior falx who compressed dorsolateral premotor and
prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s areas 6, 8, 9, 10, and 46).
As symptoms, she had attention disorder directed to a particular item of sensorium
or inner experience and incapacity to suppress from inner experience items that can
interfere with what is currently on focus.
She was apathetic, disinterested in herself and the world around her. Visuospatial
neglect along with gaze abnormalities were also present because the lesion encroaches
on area 8. According to our experience, the apathy is present in all lateral-damage
conditions, and is mostly apparent after large bilateral lesions of the frontal convexity.
Perseveration, dysexecutive syndrome with attention, working-memory and planning
disorders were among the symptoms. Language disorders were directly linked to failure
of temporal integration.
In this patient, depression was secondary to cognitive disorder.
Case 8. A 46-year-old man was admitted to our department of neurosurgery with
olfactory hallucination (“uncinate fit”) often accompanied by a dreamy state of mind,
auditory elementary hallucination, when the patient heard the sound of running water,
impaired recognition of melodies in the absence of words, the usual tendency for the
patient to report the current date as an earlier one and prosopagnosia.
Coronal Tl-weighted, gadolinium-enhanced MRI prior to surgery showed a large
right-sided temporobasal tumor (a).
Post surgery coronal Tl-weighted, gadolinium-enhanced MRI (b) obtained after
complete removal of the tumor (astrocytoma). The patient improved very much and was
discharged after 2 weeks.
Case 9. Patient, aged 73, was admitted to our neurosurgery department with a coma
and deceased within 18 hours. On autopsy, more than 110 bilateral cortical and
intracerebral metastases have been discovered, originating from a melanocarcinoma
situated in the right abdominal area, previously subject to surgery 7 months earlier.
Results
Starting from clinical neuropsychology, we present our results, along with the
comments made on the main cerebral formation, which play an important role in
understanding this extremely complex problem, which is the consciousness.
BRAINSTEM
The brain stem is the portion of the central nervous system rostral to the spinal cord
and caudal to the cerebral hemispheres.
1931
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The net-like appearance of the brain stem neurons led to the designation “reticular
formation”, a term that was originally used in a purely descriptive anatomical sense.
synchronized, or high-voltage, slow-wave pattern indicative of deep sleep and the animals
were behaviorally unresponsive. Bremer concluded that the forebrain fell asleep due to
the lack of somatosensory and auditory sensory inputs.
The reticular activating system obtained this designation in 1949, when Moruzzi
and Magoun stimulated it electrically in anesthetized cats and found that the stimulation
produced a waking pattern of electrical activity in cat’s cortex. When Moruzzi and
Magoun placed lesions in the paramedian reticular formation of the midbrain, the animals
showed cortical-evoked responses to somatosensory or auditory stimuli, but the back-
ground EEG was synchronized and the animals were behaviorally unresponsive. These
observations emphasized the midbrain reticular core as relaying important arousing
influences to the cerebral cortex and this pathway was labeled the ascending reticular
activating system (initially called ARAS, but today named AAS).
The most important reticular nuclei for arousal and consciousness are the raphe
nuclei and the central nuclei. These groups receive significant converging sensory input
from all sensory modalities and project to the thalamus (i.e., intralaminar nuclei),
cholinergic basal forebrain nuclei, and the entire cerebral cortex. An important component
of the central reticular activating system is thought to be the noradrenergic nuclei,
particularly the locus ceruleus, at the pontomesencephalic junction.
The centromedian and parafascicular nuclei, two of the intralaminar nuclei of the
thalamus representing the rostral extent of the AAS receive inputs from the spinothalamic,
trigeminothalamic and multisynaptic ascending pathways (of the reticular formation)
relaying pain sensation. As a result of their diffuse cortical connections, they are involved
in the maintenance of arousal.
The neurons of the locus ceruleus project to the thalamus, hypothalamus, basal
cholinergic nuclei, and the neocortex (Moore and Bloom, 1979).
Immediate coma results from the destruction of the central reticular nuclei at or
above the upper pontine level (Fig. 24.1, case 1).
Anyhow, AAS acts on the cerebral cortex through the thalamus, directly, and through
the arousal caudal hypothalamic neurons (tuberomammillary nucleus) which are
connected with suprachiasmatic nuclei (Fig. 24.2).
As a result, the reticular formation comes to be known as the reticular activating
system to maintain general arousal, and as the reticular inhibitory system for sleeping.
However, an exact physiologic role of the reticular activating system in
consciousness is unclear. The awake condition like the sleep has many phases: a quick
short phase which is determined by the direct action of AAS on the cerebral cortex, a
longer phase during the 24 hours, determined by indirect action of AAS on the cortex
through the thalamus; and a rhythmical phase determined by the AAS action on the
cerebral cortex through the hypothalamus awaking system under the influence of the
suprachiasmatic nucleus. These nuclei are serially interconnected with AAS not only in
the forward, but also in the reverse direction (backward).
1933
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
A B
C D
E F
1934
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Fig. 24.2. The Ascending Reticular Activating System (ARAS) is found in the brain stem (1), and sends projections
throughout the cortex: directly (2), through the thalamus (3), or through the hypothalamus (4), tuberomammillary
neurons (5), which receive influence from suprachiasmatic nucleus (6).
◄ Fig. 24.1. A 21-year old man who presented with a history of sudden onset of coma.
Sagittal (A), axial (B) and coronal (C) T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans revealed a gross
pontine hemorrhage (1.9 cm) from a cavernous malformation that reached the surface of the floor of the fourth
ventricle and in the cerebelopontine angle.
The lesion was resected through a suboccipital approach. Sagittal (D), axial (E), and coronal (F) MRI scans,
one year later, reveal no recurrence. Two months after surgery, the patient presented with minimal right sided
weakness and hemisensory deficits. Now the patient is a student, and has an excellent state.
1935
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
directly, through the thalamus, through the ventrolateral preoptic (VLPO) nucleus of the
hypothalamus, which in its turn is under the influence of the suprachiasmatic nucleus,
and through the basal ganglia.
The lateral group of the reticular formation, localized in the pons and rostral part of
the brain stem, gives origin to AIS. When AIS is activated, the cerebral cortex becomes
inactive and the person asleep. This system receives inhibitory signals from the
cerebellum and sends output signals to the thalamus, to the hypothalamic sleeping center,
and directly to the cerebral cortex (Fig. 24.3).
Fig. 24.3. The Ascending Reticular Inhibitory System (ARIS) is found in the brain stem, and sends projections
throughout the cortex: directly (1), through the thalamus (2), or through the hypothalamus;
ventrolateral preoptic nucleus-VLPO, (3), which receives influence from suprachiasmatic nucleus.
The raphe nuclei, in the midline of the brain stem, use serotonin as their primary
neurotransmitter and have diffuse connections to the cerebral cortex and subcortical gray
matter (Moore et al., 1978).
Thus, the correlated activity between reticular neurons leads to a strengthened
connection, both excitatory and inhibitory. In the absence of inhibition, any external input,
weak or strong, would generate more or less the same one-way pattern, an avalanche of
1937
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
excitatory stimuli involving the whole population (Hopfield and Tank, 1986). Cortical
networks gain their nonlinearity and functional complexity primarily from the inhibitory
interneuronal system (Pouille and Scanziani, 2001). The specific firing patterns of
principal cells in a network depend on the temporal and spatial distribution of inhibition.
Without inhibition and dedicated neural formation, excitatory circuits cannot accomplish
anything useful (Buzsaki, 2006). Fast coupling of the excitatory and inhibitory influences
can bring about a submillisecond precision of spike timing (Pouille and Scanziani, 2001).
Anyhow, reticular nucleus efferents terminate in the immediately underlying
thalamic nuclei and the reticular nuclei efferents are GABA-ergic (the neurons in reticular
nuclei are exclusively inhibitory, using GABA as their transmitter) (Bogen, 1997, 2001).
Thus, thalamocortical communication can be simultaneously inhibited by this reticular
nucleus efferents that terminate in the underlying thalamic nuclei. In the brain and
particularly in the cerebral cortex, there are multiple influences, both inhibitory and
facilitatory. When a performance has been lost because the competence has lost some
facilitation, re-emergence of the performance can result simply from the subsidence of
inhibition (Sherrington, 1932). The same suggestion was made by von Monakow in 1911.
The main point is that a loss of performance is not necessarily the result of damage to
the competence for that performance; it may result from unbalanced or excessive
inhibition of the competence (Bogen, 2001).
Generally, the reticular formation of the brain stem is, in turn, influenced by
circadian clocks located in the suprachiasmatic nuclei and the arousal (tuberomammillary
nucleus) and sleeping (ventrolateral preoptic nucleus) centers of the hypothalamus. The
clock adjusts periods of sleep and wakefulness to appropriate duration during the 24-
hours cycle of light and darkness. So, in all structures of the nervous system, inhibition
plays a pivotal role.
Thus, brain uses not only excitation, but also inhibition during its normal operations
and behaviors. With no inhibitory cells in a network, depending on the temporal and spatial
distribution and dedicated interneurons, excitatory circuits cannot accomplish anything
useful (Buzsaki, 2006). Excitatory potentials dominate on the dendrites of principal cells,
whereas only inhibitory postsynaptic potentials impinge upon the cell body (soma).
Interneurons provide autonomy and independence to neighboring principal cells but they
offer, at the same time, useful temporal coordination. The functional diversity of principal
cells is enhanced by the domain-specific actions of GABA-ergic interneurons which can
dynamically alter the qualities of the principal cells (Buzsaki, 2006). The separation of
inputs in a network with only excitatory connections and circuits is not possible. Like all
somatic functions at all levels of the system, executive functions, beginning with attention,
make use of inhibition for focus, contrast suppression of interference, order, and timeliness
(Fuster, 2009). Inhibition enhance saliency and contrast. Inhibition appears essential for
the control of impulsivity and a wide array of instinctual drives.
So, there are two opposing active processes that could summate, algebraically, a
control excitatory reticular system and a control inhibitory one. Thus, the brain uses not
only excitation, but also inhibition during its normal function.
1938
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Ascending output of the brain stem reticular formation not only subserves arousal
and sleep but also contains information about other bodily states and other neural
formation outside the reticular formation. This is why ARAS and ARIS might be named
ascending activatory system (AAS) and, respectively, ascending inhibitory system (AIS).
Clinical aspects
It is important to distinguish between alertness and impairment of the wakeful state.
It is possible to be awake and not conscious, but it is impossible to be conscious and not
awake. A combination of clinical lesion studies and animal data has identified the
following major mechanisms through which alterations in consciousness are produced:
disturbance of the ascending reticular system; bilateral lesions of the midbrain and
diencephalon; and bilateral involvement of the cerebral cortex (hemispheres). To what
degree same damage will render unconsciousness of a person remains to be clarified.
Coma
Destructive lesions of the brain stem may occur as a result of vascular disease, tumor,
infection, or trauma. Unlike compressive lesions, which can often be reversed by
removing a mass, destructive lesions cannot be reversed. Between the conscious state of
mind and coma there are multiple intermediary stages that manifest through: confusion
or lethargy, drowsiness, stupor, semicoma (light coma), locked-in syndrome persistent
vegetative state, loss of conscious in concussion (diffuse axonal injury). We will shortly
discuss below the most important of them.
A B
Fig. 24.4. MRI studies of a petroclival meningioma (A) which compresses from outside the brain stem provoke
logorrhea syndrome with hyperkinesia and hyperwakefulness. Postsurgery (B) this syndrome disappears.
1940
24. The neural basis of consciousness
MIDBRAIN (Mesencephalon)
The midbrain is the short portion of the brain between the pons and the cerebral
hemispheres. It consists of tectum, contains the four corpora quadrigemina and two
cerebral peduncles with tegmentum and crus cerebri.
The cerebral aqueduct, surrounded by the central gray matter, separates the tectum
from the tegmentum. The cerebral peduncle consists of two parts: 1) a dorsal part, the
tegmentum; and 2) a ventral part, the crus cerebri. These two parts are separated from
each other by substantia nigra.
The midbrain tegmentum contains the trochlear and oculomotor nuclei, neural
structures concerned with ocular and visual reflexes, the mesencephalic reticular
formation, the red nuclei and many scattered collections of cells.
A B
Fig. 24.5. A) A contrast-enhanced axial CT-scan of a 36-year-old man with hydrocephalus and a large pineal region
tumor, that was totally resected. Postsurgery, the patient remains clinically intact. B) Axial CT-scan, 3 months
postsurgery confirming total removal of the tumor (germinoma).
Pressure from this direction produces the characteristic dorsal midbrain syndrome,
manifested first by limited upgaze. In severe cases, the eyes may be fixed in forced,
downward position. There may also be a deficit of convergent eye movements and
associated pupilloconstriction. The presence of retractory nystagmus, in which all of the
eye muscle contracts simultaneously to pull the globe back into the orbit, is characteristic.
Motor responses are difficult to obtain or result in extensor posturing. Motor tone and
tendon reflexes may be heightened, and plantar responses are in extension.
If the cerebral aqueduct is compressed sufficiently to cause acute hydrocephalus,
however, an acute increase in supratentorial pressure may ensue.
This may cause an acute increase in downward pressure on the midbrain, resulting
in sudden lapse into deep coma (Posner et al., 2007).
Most patients in whom the herniation can be reversed suffer chronic neurologic
disability (Brendler and Selverstone, 1970; Zervas and Hedley-Whyte, 1972).
After the midbrain stage becomes complete, it is rare for patients to recover fully.
DIENCEPHALON
The diencephalon contains the hypothalamus, thalamus, subthalamus (substantia
nigra, the zona incerta, the nucleus of the tegmental fields of Forel, ansa lenticularis,
Forel’s field H1 – thalamic fasciculus – Forel’s field H2 – lenticular fasciculus-, and
subthalamic fasciculus), metathalamus (medial geniculate body and lateral geniculate
body), and epithalamus (pineal body, habenular trigones, stria medullaris, and roof of
the third ventricle).
In the following, we study only the role of the hypothalamus and thalamus in sleep,
arousal, and circadian rhythm.
1942
24. The neural basis of consciousness
The hypothalamus
The hypothalamus is composed of about 22 small nuclei, the fibre system that passes
through it and the pituitary gland. Although the hypothalamus comprises only about 0.3%
of the brain weight, it takes part in nearly all aspects of motivated behavior, including
sleeping, arousal, temperature regulation, emotional behavior, endocrine function,
metabolism, sexual behavior, and movement.
From our point of view, the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (sleeping system), the tubero-
mammillary nucleus (arousal system), and the suprachiasmstic nucleus (day - night cycle
system) are important.
The sleep is a circadian function, and although the suprachiasmatic nuclei are not
essential for its generation, they are responsible for consolidation of sleep in cycles that
occur within a circadian framework.
According to Hobson (2007), when humans go to sleep they rapidly become less
conscious. The initial loss of awareness of the external world that may occur when we
are reading in bed is associated with the slowing of the EEG that is called Stage I. At
sleep onset, although awareness of the outside world is lost, subject may continue to have
visual imagery and associated reflective consciousness. Even in the depths of stage IV
non-REM sleep, when consciousness appears to be largely obliterated, the brain remains
highly active and it is still capable of processing its own information. From PET and
single neuron studies, it can safely be concluded that the brain remains about 80% active
in the depths of sleep. Most of the brain activity is not associated with consciousness.
Non-REM, Stage IV is characterized by low-frequency, high-amplitude EEG in which
subjects may report not only some thought-like mentation but also movie-like dreams
(Bosinelli, 1995).
The circuitry through which AIS influences the sleep, being localized in the upper
pons and rostral parts of the brain stem, includes the hypothalamic ventrolateral preoptic
nuclei, suprachiasmatic nuclei, the thalamus, and the cerebral cortex. Saper et al. (2005)
provide very good arguments regarding the sleep and arousal. Nevertheless, in our
opinion, the explanation of the sleep / wakefulness given by them as due to a flip-flop
switch, to the influence of suprachiasmatic nucleus, to the homeostatic mechanism and
to the allostatic mechanism is not enough. It is our opinion that the explanation should
also include the existence of the AAS and AIS which are working also under the influence
of the suprachiasmatic nucleus, homeostatic mechanisms and allostatic mechanism and
which control the sleep.
The arousal, like sleep, exhibits more steps: a rapid one, which has a short lifetime
and which is determined by the direct action of the ascending activating system on the
cerebral cortex; another, with a longer lifetime, within the 24 hours, which is caused by
indirect action of AAS on the cerebral cortex via thalamus; and the third, which is
rhythmic and it is determined by the AAS action on the cerebral cortex via the
hypothalamic arousal system which, in its turn, is found under the influence of the
suprachiasmatic nucleus.
Some studies have demonstrated that the influence of the hypothalamus on arousal
is not restricted to the tuberomammillary neurons in caudal hypothalamus. In particular,
1943
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
a prominent group of neurons confined to the lateral hypothalamus has been implicated
in the sleep disorder known as narcolepsy (Card et al., 1999).
These neurons express novel neuropeptides known as hypocretins or orexins and are
differentially concentrated within the perifornical nucleus that surrounds the fornix in the
tuberal hypothalamus. Mapping studies have shown that hypocretin (orexin) neurons are
similar to tuberomammillary neurons in that they are confined to the hypothalamus and give
rise to extensive projections throughout the neuraxis (Parent, 1997).
Human sleep occurs with circadian periodicity. Thus, humans have an internal “free-
running clock” that operates even in the absence of information about the period of 24
hours (Aschoff, 1965; Hobson, 1989; Colwell and Michel, 2003). This clock is controlled
by the suprachiasmatic nucleus.
So, circadian rhythms provide temporal organization and coordination for
physiological, biochemical, and behavioral variables in all eukaryotic organisms and in
some prokaryotes. Circadian rhythms that are genetically determined, not learned (Hall,
1990, Rosato et al., 1997; von Schantz and Archer, 2003), are generated by an
endogenous self-sustained pacemaker.
The pineal body synthesizes the sleep-promoting neurohormone melatonin and
secretes it into the bloodstream where it modulates the sleep.
The thalamus
The physiological understanding of the human thalamus is limited. The fundamental
function of the thalamus is that of relay and it modulates peripheral information to the
cerebral cortex and to the basal ganglia, keeping the somatosensory, mental, and
emotional activity of a living individual in harmony.
With the exception of the thalamic reticular nucleus, all thalamic subnuclei possess
thalamic projection neurons that relay processed information to the cerebral cortex. In
addition, the thalamic subnuclei also have inhibitory GABA-ergic interneurons whose
cell bodies and processes are confined to a single subnucleus. The reticular nucleus of
the thalamus is a continuation into the diencephalon of the reticular formation of the brain
stem. It receive inputs from the cerebral cortex and thalamic nuclei. The former are
collaterals of corticothalamic projections and the latter are collateral of thalamocortical
projections. The reticular nucleus projects to other thalamic nuclei. The inhibitory
neurotransmitter of this projection is GABA. The reticular nucleus is unique amongst the
thalamic nuclei because its axons do not leave the thalamus. Based on its connections,
the reticular nucleus plays a role in integrating and gating activities of the thalamic nuclei.
As the termination site for the reticular ascending system is considered, it is not
surprising that the thalamus has an important arousal and sleep-producing function (Green
1987; Steriade et al., 1990; La Berge, 2000; Jones, 2007) and that it alerts, activates or
inhibits a specific processing and response system. Its involvement in attention shows
up in diminished awareness of stimuli impinging on the side opposite to the lesion
(unilateral inattention) (Dănăilă, 1972; Arseni and Dănăilă, 1977; Ojemann, 1984; Posner,
1988; Heilman et al., 2003).
1944
24. The neural basis of consciousness
The ascending input to intralaminar nuclei can help explain consciousness of primitive
percepts (non-cognitive component). So, ascending output of the brain stem reticular
formation not only subserves arousal but also contains information about other states. Thus,
other input to reticular formation comes from the spinothalamic system, and trigeminal
complex, and from dentate nuclei in the cerebellum conveying proprioceptive signals. There
are also ascending inputs to intralaminar nuclei from deep layers of the periaqueductal gray,
substantia nigra and amygdala with affective information, and from the vestibular nuclei
with information about body position (McGuiness and Krauthamer, 1980; Kaufman and
Rosenquist, 1985; Royce et al., 1991; Jones, 2007).
Intralaminar nuclei. These nuclei, embedded in the internal medullary lamina,
consist of centralis, lateralis, paracentralis, central medial nuclei (anterior group), and
centromedial and parafascicular nuclei (posterior group) (Ohye, 2002). The latter are
often called the centromedian-parafascicular complex.
The anterior group receives different projections from the spinothalamic tract, deep
cerebellar nucleus, brain stem reticular formation, etc.
The posterior group has a reciprocal connection with the basal ganglia. The efferent
connection with the cerebral cortex is very wide and was thought to be a diffuse
projection. The intralaminar nuclei were classified as representatives of the “nonspecific
system” rather than of the “specific system”, such as the thalamic station for the visual,
auditory, or somatosensory system with definite modality-specific peripheral input.
Reticular nucleus. This nucleus is considered to be related to arousal, attention,
cognitive function, etc. As discussed later, it plays a role in maintaining cortical activity
in a disease state of epilepsy (Ohye, 1990; Ohye, 1998; Jones, 2007).
Ohye (2002) studied the human thalamus using microrecordings during stereotactic
thalamotomy for dyskinesia and found verbal command neurons in this nucleus and
adjacent area.
Surround-type inhibition mediated by thalamic reticular nuclei may selectively gate
out extraneous stimuli while allowing focused relay important sensory data to the
thalamocortical circuits, which endow a given neural activity pattern with the property
of conscious perception (Ames and Marshall, 2003). But how is this neurophysiologic
activity coordinated in time to produce a somewhat unified conscious stream? Data
suggest the answer may lie in the acquisition of gamma synchrony, most commonly at
approximately 40 Hz (Gray and Viana di Prisco, 1997).
Gamma synchrony has also been hypothesized to “bind” disparate features of a given
object, such as color, size, texture, and motion, into a temporally unified sensory stimulus
(Singer and Gray, 1995).
On the other hand, thalamocortical neurons receive ascending projections from the
locus ceruleus (noradrenergic), raphe nuclei (serotonergic), reticular junction (cholin-
ergic), TMN (histaminergic) and project to cortical pyramidal cells.
In the tonic firing state, thalamocortical neurons transmit information to the cortex
that is correlated with the spike trains encoding peripheral stimuli (Steriade, 1992, 1999).
In brief, the control of sleep and wakefulness depends on the brain stem and
hypothalamic modulation of the thalamus and cortex.
1945
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
The epithalamus
The function of the epithalamus is not well understood.
Production of the pineal hormone melatonin is cyclic, with high levels of synthesis
occurring at night and low levels during the day.
A B
C D
Fig. 24.6. Contrast medium-enhanced coronal (A) and axial (B) magnetic resonance imaging showing a big right
thalamic tumor that proved to be an astrocytoma. Thirteen months following resection and radiotherapy, coronal (C)
and axial (D) magnetic resonance imaging demonstrates the absence of tumor.
The patient was conscious and in good state.
1946
24. The neural basis of consciousness
A B
Fig. 24.7. A 56-year-old man who presented with a sudden onset of coma and fever. Coronal (A) and axial (B) T1-
weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans revealed edematous, demyelination symmetrical changes infra-
and supratentorial, which involve entire midbrain, both thalamic formations, bilateral basal ganglia, two-side
temporooccipital convolution and hippocampus, determined by encephalitis. After 9 days of coma, the patient died.
hypothalamus. Nauta (1958) developed this concept further, insisting on the functional
importance of certain regions of the neural axis, such as the septum, cingulate gyrus,
orbitofrontal cortex, preoptic area, “limbic striatum” (including the nucleus accumbens,
mesolimbic dopaminergic tract), nonspecific thalamic nuclei, hypothalamus and midbrain
tegmental area, regions closely related to the amygdala and hippocampus. These regions
form a ring, or “limbus”, around the base of the brain. Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)
is part of a neural circuit that mediates outcome-contingent changes in behavior (Ito et
al., 2003; Kerns et al., 2004; Kennerley et al., 2006) and processes fictive information in
human (Chiu et al., 2008). The ACC is interconnected with the orbitofrontal cortex which
mediates fictive thinking in humans (Camille et al., 2004; Ursu and Carter, 2005).
Hayden et al. (2009) hypothesized that neurons in the ACC, which monitors the
consequences of actions and mediates subsequent changes in behavior, would respond
to fictive reward information.
Generally, the hypothalamus makes a link between the limbic and endocrine system
reasonable. The limbic system is now considered to be a functional unit. Areas around
the limbic system are called paralimbic and have a more complex histologic structure.
Anyhow, the limbic system makes a link between external and internal world.
The Hippocampus. The hippocampus occupies the medial part of the floor of the
temporal horn and is divided into three parts: head, body and tail.
Structure. The hippocampus is bilaminar, consisting of the cornu Ammonis (or
hippocampus proper) and the gyrus dentatus (or fascia dentata), with one lamina rolled
up inside the other.
Functions and connections: the possible functions of the hippocampus are divided
into four categories: (1) learning and memory, (2) regulation of emotional behavior,
(3) certain aspects of motor control; and (4) regulation of hypothalamic functions
(Duvernoy, 2005). The hippocampus and related diencephalic structures form and
consolidate declarative memories that are ultimately stored elsewhere.
The hippocampus is also involved in the regulation of the hypothalamo-hypophysial
axis. Through its projections to the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus, it may inhibit
the hypophysial secretion of adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH) (Jacobs et al., 1979;
Teyler et al., 1980; Herman et al., 1989; Diamond et al., 1996).
The amygdala: amygdala is a complex mass of gray matter buried in the anterior-
medial portion of the temporal lobe, just rostral to the hippocampus.
The amygdala and its interconnections with an array of neocortical areas in the
prefrontal cortex and anterior temporal lobe, as well as several subcortical structures,
appear to be especially important in the higher order processing of emotion.
The amygdala links cortical regions that process sensory information with
hypothalamic and brain stem effector systems. In a review of the role of the amygdala in
emotional processing, Phelps and Le Doux (2005) identified five areas in which there is
evidence from studies of cognition-emotion interactions involving the amygdala:
1) implicit emotional learning and memory;
2) emotional modulation of memory;
1948
24. The neural basis of consciousness
CEREBRAL CORTEX
The cerebral cortex of the cerebral hemispheres, the convoluted outer layer of gray
matter composed of tens billions of neurons and their synaptic connections, is the most
highly organized correlation center of the brain, but the specific of cortical structures in
mediating behavior is neither clear-cut nor circumscribed (Collins, 1990; Franckowiak
et al., 1997). This multitude of neurons send a large number of axons in all directions,
covered by supportive myelin. This forms the white matter of the cortex which fills the
large subcortical space.
The cerebral cortex receives sensory information from internal / external environ-
ment of the organism, processes this information and then decides on and carries out the
response to it.
Generally, the hemispheres supply much of the content and registration function of
consciousness, including language, abstract reasoning, somatosensory visual and spatial
abilities, map of the physical dimensions of the self, executive function, complex emotion,
feelings, memory and ability to read other’s mind.
While the cortex is vital for cognitive functions, it interacts constantly with major
satellite organs, notably the thalamus, basal ganglia, hypothalamus, cerebellum, brain-
stem and limbic regions, among others.
In order to be conscious, to operate at normal parameters, to record and potentiate
the internal and external sensory data, and to correctly process them based on the previous
individual experience, and to answer adequately, it is necessary that the cerebral cortex
should be integer and aroused by the ascending activating system.
These considerations suggest that there might be multiple conscious awareness
systems, each supporting conscious awareness in different mental domains.
A B
Fig. 24.8. A presurgery contrast enhanced computed tomographic (CT) scan of a 56-year-old man shows a giant size
olfactory groove meningioma and displacement of anterior brain (A). The patient presents an orbitofrontal syndrome.
Postsurgery contrast-enhanced CT scan showing no residual tumor (B) and orbitofrontal syndrome disappeared.
A B
Fig. 24.9. A presurgery contrast enhanced computed tomographic (CT) scan of a 35-year-old woman shows a bilobed
meningioma of the anterior falx. She has a dorsolateral frontal syndrome (A). Postsurgery contrast-enhanced
computed tomography scan showing no residual tumor (B) and frontal syndrome disappeared.
1950
24. The neural basis of consciousness
We want to stress the existence of the same discrete modules in the brain for each
possible neuropsychological capacity. Adjacent modules communicate with each other
more than do non-adjacent modules. So, the term of modular or functional localization
of consciousness is used to indicate that certain functions can be localized to particular
areas of the cerebral cortex. The mapping of cortical function began with inference made
from the deficits produced by cortical lesions in humans.
A B
Fig. 24.10. (A) Preoperative coronal T1-weighted, gadolinium-enhanced magnetic resonance images obtained for a
46-year-old man who presented prosopagnosia and memory disturbance. It demonstrated a large right-sided
temporo-basal astrocytoma. (B) Image was obtained after complete removal of the tumor.
After two months the symptoms disappeared.
A B
Fig. 24.11. Bilateral, cortical and subcortical, more than 110 cerebral metastases (A and B).
The primitive tumor was a melanocarcinoma.
1951
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Discussion
The consciousness processes belong to people who act by control mechanisms of
psychological activities, generalization and abstractization mechanisms, as well as by
exploring and handling of mental images to solve all the problems man is facing with.
The consciousness level depends on the complexity of the brain ontogenetic evolution.
It must be a function of numerous interacting systems. Data based on our experience
support this statement.
According to Tononi and Laureys, consciousness can be dissociated from other brain
function, such as responsiveness to sensorial inputs, motor control, attention, language,
memory, reflection, spatial frames of reference, the body and perhaps even the self
(Tononi and Laureys, 2009). We consider this point of view to be incorrect because
consciousness cannot appear without these function. It cannot be dissociated from them.
The respective functions represent modules of the consciousness. The consciousness
results from the respective cerebral activities. Lesions of some functions lead to modular
disorder of the consciousness.
The major structures supposed to play a key role in the neural correlates of
consciousness are: the brain stem, the diencephalon (the hypothalamus and thalamus),
the limbic system (especially the hippocampus and amygdala), basal ganglia, cerebellum,
and the cerebral cortex. The brain stem is the source of massive reticular formation
pathways that activate or inhibit higher and lower brain centers. They are the core of the
basic arousal and sleeping cycle.
The hypothalamus, the thalamus and the cerebral cortex are likely closely intertwined
with RF which plays a key role in consciousness. Generally, there are an ascending
activating system (AAS) and an ascending inhibitory system (AIS).
However, AAS which appears to be responsible for maintaining cortical arousal is
not the same with consciousness.
Sleep is based on ascending reticular inhibitory system. The two reticular systems
(AAS and AIS) are under the influence of the suprachiasmatic nucleus and of the awake
and sleep centers in the hypothalamus. So, as much as awake is determined by AAS,
sleep, considered the most profound natural alteration of consciousness, is determined
by AIS. AAS and AIS are not neurological basis of consciousness but they rather
constitute the necessary substrate for consciousness to emerge.
1952
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Conclusions
– Bilateral destruction of the reticular activating nuclei at the rostral pons and
midbrain lead to loss of consciousness and the induction of coma.
– The damage of the ascending reticular inhibitory system lead to the appearance of
the logorrhea syndrome with hyperkinesia, hyperwakefulness, and hyperprosexia.
– The ascending inhibitory system is very important in explaining the sleep and
many other behaviors.
– AAS and AIS reach the cerebral cortex by three distinct ways: directly, through
the thalamus and through the hypothalamus.
– The sleep is controlled by action of the AAS-AIS dipole.
– With respect to its functioning, the cerebral cortex may be compared to a
continuous chess game between of the two systems, AAS and AIS, which act in perfect
equilibrium in order to perform all functions and behaviors of the individual.
– The cerebral cortex and consciousness have a modular structure.
1953
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
REFERENCES
Adams JH, Graham D, Jennett B, The neuropathology of the vegetative state after an acute brain
insult. Brain 123; 1327-1338, 2000.
Ames C, Marshall L, Differential diagnosis of altered states of consciousness. In: HR Winn (ed),
Youmans Neurological Surgery. Vol. 1. Fifth Edition. Saunders, Philadelphia. Pennsylvania,
277-299, 2003.
Arseni C, Dănăilă L, Logorrhea syndrome with hyperkinesia. Eur Neurol 15; 183-187, 1977.
Aschoff J, Circadian rhythms in man. Science 148; 1427-1432, 1965.
Berger H, Ueber das electroenkephalogramm des menschen. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. 87; 527-
570, 1929.
Bly M, Faymonville ME, Peigneux P, Auditory processing in severely brain injured patient:
differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state.
Archives of Neurology 61; 233-238, 2004.
Bogen JE, Some neurophysiologic aspects of consciousness. Semin Neurol. 17; 95-103, 1997.
Bogen JE, The thalamic intralaminar nuclei and the property of consciousness. In: P.D. Zelazov,
M Moscovitch, E Thompson (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, Cambridge
Univ Press, 775-807, 2001.
Bosinelli M, Mind and consciousness during sleep. Behavioural Brain Research 69; 195-201, 1995.
Bremer F, Cerveau isolé et physiologie du sommeil. Comp. Rend. Soc. Biol 118; 1235-1242, 1935.
Bremer F, Nouvelles recherches sur le mécanisme du sommeil. Comp. Rend. Soc. Biol. 122; 460-
464, 1936.
Brendler SJ, Selverstone B, Recovery from decerebration. Brain 93; 381-392, 1970.
Broca P, Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales. Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure
limbique dans la série des mammifères. Rev Anthropol 1; 385-398, 1878.
Brodal A, The hippocampus and the sense of smell. A review. Brain 70; 179- 222, 1947.
Brooks WM, Friedman SD, Gasparovic C, Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in traumatic brain
injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil 16; 149-164, 2001.
Buzsaki G, Diversity of cortical functions is provided by inhibition. In: G. Buzsaki (ed), Rhythms
of the Brain, Oxford University Press, Cycle 3; 61-79, 2006.
Camille N, Coricelli G, Sallet J, Pradat-Diehl P, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, The involvement of the
orbitofrontal cortex in the experience of regret. Science, 1167-1170, 2004.
Card JP, Swanson LW, Moore RY,. The hypothalamus: An overview of regulatory system. In:
Fundamental Neuroscience (M. Zigmond, FE Bloom, SC Landis, L Roberts, LR Squire, Eds),
Academic Press, San Diego, 1013-1026, 1999.
Chiu PH, Lohrent TM, Montague PR, Smokers’ brains compute, but ignore, a fictive error signal
in a sequential investment task. Nature Neuroscience 11; 514-520, 2008
Collins RC, Cerebral Cortex. In: AL Pearlman and RC Collins (eds.), Neurobiology of disease,
New York: Oxford University Press, 1990.
Colwell CS, Michel S, Sleep and circadian rhythms: Do sleep centers talk to the clock?, Nature
Neurosci 10; 1005-1006, 2003.
Damasio AR, Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam, 1994.
Damasio AR, The feeling of what happens. New York: Harcourt Press, 1999.
Dănăilă L, Clinical and experimental study on the reticular substance psychopathology (in
Romanian language), 110 pp. Graduating thesis, Faculty of Philosophy, University of
Bucharest, 1972.
Dănăilă L, Pascu ML, Lasers in Neurosurgery. Ed. Acad. Române, 2001.
Dănăilă L, Pascu ML, Second International Symposium on Coma and Consciousness: Clinical,
Social and Ethical Implications, Berlin, 4-5 June. Abst. 21, 2009.
Della Porta P, Maiolo AT, Negri VU, Cerebral blood flow and metabolism in therapeutic insulin
coma. Metabolism 13; 131-140, 1964.
1954
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Denes-Ray V, Epstein S, Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave
against their bitter judgment. Journal Personality and Social Psychology 66; 819-829, 1994.
Diamond DM, Fleshner M, Ingersoll N, Rose GM, Psychological stress impairs spatial working
memory: relevance to electronophysiological studies of hippocampal function. Behav.
Neurosci 110; 661-672, 1996.
Duvernoy HM, The Human Hippocampus. Third Ed., Springer Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2005.
Edelman GM, Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books, 1992.
Edelman GM, Tononi G, A universe of consciousness. New York: Basic Books, 2000.
Franckowiak RSJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Mazziota JC, Human brain function. San
Diego, Academic Press, 1997.
Fuster JM, The Prefrontal Cortex. Fourth Edition. Amsterdam, Boston, Heidelberg, etc. Academic
Press Elsevier, 2009.
Gaus SE, Strecker RE, Tate BA, Parker RA, Saper CB, Ventrolateral preoptic nucleus contains
sleep-active, galaninergic neurons in multiple mammallian species. Neuroscience 115; 285-
294, 2002.
Giza CC, Hovda DA, The neurometabolic cascade of concussion. J. Athl. Train 36; 228-235, 2001.
Gray CM, Viana di Prisco G, Stimulus dependent neuronal oscillations and local synchronization
in striate cortex of the alert cat. J. Neurosci 17; 3239-3253, 1997.
Green S, Physiological psychology. New York, Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1987.
Hall JC, Genetics of circadian rhythms. Ann. Rev Genet 24; 659-694, 1990.
Hatta T, Masui T, Ito E, Hasegawa Y, Matsuyama Y, Relation between the prefrontal cortex and
cerebro-cerebellar functions: evidence from the results of stabilometrical indexes. Apply.
Neuropsychology 11; 153-160, 2004.
Hayden BY, Pearson JM, Platt ML, Fictive reward signals in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science
324; 948-950, 2009.
Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E, Neglect and related disorders. In: KM Heilman and E
Valenstein (eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (4th ed). New York University Press, 2003.
Herman JP, Schäfer MKH, Young EA, Thompson R, Douglass J, Akil H, Watson SJ, Evidence
for hippocampal regulation of neuroendocrine neurons of the hypothalamo-pituitary-
adrenocortical axis. J. Neurosci. 9; 3072-3082, 1989.
Hobson JA, Sleep. New York: Scientific American Library, 1989.
Hobson JA, State of consciousness: normal and abnormal variation. In: PD Zelazov, M.
Marcovitch and E Thompson (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness, Chapter
16. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, New York; 435-444, 2007.
Hopfield, JJ, Tank DW, Computing with neural circuits: A model. Science 233; 625-633, 1986.
Ito S, Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Schall JD, Performance monitoring the anterior cingulate cortex
during saccade countermanding. Science 303; 120, 2003.
Jannett B, Plum F, Persistent vegetative state after brain damage: A syndrome in research of a
name. Lancet 1; 734-737, 1972.
Jacobs MS, Mc Farland WL, Morgane PJ, The anatomy of the brain of the bottlenose dolphin
(Tursiops truncatus). Rhinic lobe (rhinencephalon): the archicortex. Brain Res Bull 4
(Suppl.1); 1-108, 1979.
Jones GJE, The Thalamus. Second edition. Vol. I and II. Cambridge, New York, Melbourne etc.,
Cambridge University Press, 2007.
Kagan J, The second year: The emergence of self-awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1981.
1955
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain (iii)
Kennerley SW, Walton ME, Behrens EJ, Buckley M., Rushworth MSF, Optimal decision making
and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience 9; 940-947, 2006.
Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho YR, Stenger AV, Carter SC, Anterior cingulate
conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303; 1023-1026, 2004.
La Berge D, Networks of attention. In: MS Gazzaniga (ed). The new cognitive neuroscience (2nd
ed). Cambridge MA, MIT Press, 2000.
Levy DE, Sidtis JJ, Rottenberg DA, Jarden JO, Differences in cerebral blood flow and glucose
utilization in vegetative versus locked-in patients, 1987. Annals of Neurology 22; 673-682, 1987.
Lewis M, The development of self-consciousness. In: J Roessler and N. Eilan (eds). Agency and
self-awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 275-295, 2003.
Marrocco RT, Field BA, Arousal. In: V.S. Ramachandran (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain.
Amsterdam, Boston, London etc., Academic Press, 223-236, 2002.
McGuiness CM, Krauthamer GM, The afferent projections to the centrum medianum of the cat
as demonstrated by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase. Brain Research 184; 255-
269, 1980.
Minzenberg MJ, Watrous AJ, Yoon JH, Ursu S, Carter C, Modafinil shifts human locus ceruleus
to low-tonic, high-phasic activity during functional MRI. Science 322; 1700-1702, 2008.
Moore RY, Bloom FE, Central catecholamine system: Anatomy and physiology of the
norepinephrine and epinephrine systems. Ann. Rev Neurosci 2; 113-168, 1979.
Moore RY, Halaris AE, Jones BE, Serotonin neurons of the midbrain raphe: Ascending
projections. J. Comp. Neurol. 180; 417-438, 1978.
Moruzzi G, Magoun WH, Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1; 455-473, 1949.
Nauta WJH, Hippocampal projections and related neural pathways to the midbrain in the cat.
Brain 81; 319-340, 1958.
Ohye C, Thalamus. In: The Human Nervous System (G. Paxinos ed.) Academic Press, San Diego,
439-468, 1990.
Ohye C, Thalamotomy for Parkinson’s disease and other types of tumor. Part 1: Historical
background and technique. In: Textbook of Stereotactic and Functional Neurosurgery (PL
Gildenberg and RR Tasker Eds), 1167-1178, McGraw-Hill, New York, 1998.
Ohye C, Thalamus and thalamic damage. In: V.S. Ramachandran (ed), Encyclopedia of the human
brain Vol. 4. Academic Press, Amsterdam, Boston, London; 575-597, 2002.
Ojemann GA,. Common cortical and thalamic mechanisms for language and motor functions.
American Journal of Physiology 246; 901-903, 1984.
Papez JW, A proposed mechanism of emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 38; 725-743, 1937.
Parent A, Hypothalamus. In: Carpenter’s Human Neuroanatomy, 9th ed, Williams and Wilkins,
Baltimore, 706-743, 1997.
Phelps EA, Le Doux JE, Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing from animal models
to human behavior. Neuron 48; 175-187, 2005.
Posner MI, Structures and functions of selective attention. In: Boll and B.K. Bryant (eds), Clinical
neuropsychology and brain function: Research, measurement, and practice. Washington DC,
American Psychological Association, 1988.
Posner JB, Saper CB, Schiff ND, Plum F, Plum and Posner’s Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma.
Fourth Edition, Oxford University Press, 110-112, 2007.
Posner JB, Saper CB, Schiff ND, Plum F, (eds). Plum and Posner’s Diagnosis of Stupor and Coma.
Chapter 3. Structural causes of stupor and coma. Oxford University Press, 88-118, 2007.
1956
24. The neural basis of consciousness
Auditory object recognition and scene analysis Ascending (reticular) activating system (AAS)
1342 1932
Auditory processing 1338 Ascending (reticular) inhibitory system (AIS)
Binaural properties 1340 1936
Complex response properties 1339 Clinical aspects 1939
Functional studies in humans 1331 Coma 1939
Functional studies in monkeys 1330 Diffuse axonal injury (loss of consciousness in
Neurophysiology 1336 concussion) 1939
Sound localization 1340 Logorrhea syndrome with hyperkinesia 1940
Spatial attention 1341 Persistent vegetative state 1939
Spatial localization 1338
Reticular formation 1932
The left temporal lobe and verbal memory 1335
Brain structures studied in emotional behavior in hu-
The right temporal lobe, nonverbal memory, and
musical perception 1335 mans 1882
The role of the planum temporale in sound de- Broca 6, 33, 61, 62, 65-76, 80, 120, 122-124, 128,
coding 1340 129, 136, 509, 585, 676, 739, 843, 849, 851-
The role of the primary auditory cortex 1338 853, 877, 908, 915, 967, 998, 1086, 1089,
Tonotopy 1339 1122, 1141-1147, 1182, 1225, 1226, 1229,
Unilateral lesions 1333 1236, 1239, 1248, 1303, 1349, 1352, 1365,
Auditory cortex 268, 1697 1366, 1383, 1481, 1482, 1485, 1497, 1537,
Acoustic startle reflex, orientation, and attention 1568, 1577, 1591, 1615, 1630, 1638, 1648,
1706 1657, 1664, 1665, 1668, 1704, 1705, 1715-
Area 20 1706 1717, 1725, 1726, 1731, 1743, 1804-1806,
Area 21 1705 1819-1823, 1828-1830, 1832-1834, 1840,
Auditory association cortex 1704 1846, 1861, 1886, 1891, 1893-1895, 1898,
Descending auditory pathways 1706 1902, 1906, 1915, 1927, 1947, 1954
Primary auditory cortex 1697 Brodmann 88, 90-93, 123, 125, 128, 129, 132, 134,
Auditory pathway through the reticular formation
135, 139, 152, 154, 242-244, 268, 300, 309,
270
328, 357, 367, 448, 557, 563, 565, 567, 571,
Auditory pathways 261
575, 576, 753, 780, 844, 851, 852, 857, 892,
Ballisme 805
893, 911, 914, 915, 919, 966, 1001, 1114,
1124, 1125, 1128, 1137, 1139, 1142, 1144,
B
1147, 1152, 1154, 1155, 1157, 1163, 1165,
Basal ganglia 731
1181, 1186, 1205, 1225, 1226, 1229, 1234,
Basal ganglia – anatomical organization 732
1255-1259, 1261, 1262, 1282, 1299, 1300-
Basal ganglia – subcortical loop 782
1302, 1313, 1321, 1323, 1325, 1326, 1328,
Basal ganglia and automatic processing 798
1330, 1332, 1364, 1384, 1410, 1415, 1417,
Basal ganglia and cerebellum in motor function 801 1418, 1448, 1469, 1553, 1594-1596, 1611,
Basal ganglia and learning 792 1615, 1617, 1627, 1628-1630, 1632, 1633,
Basal ganglia circuitry 773 1639, 1641, 1642, 1648, 1649, 1654-1657,
Basal ganglia functions 788 1660-1662, 1664, 1668-1670, 1672, 1674,
Cognitive function 790 1677, 1678, 1680, 1681, 1685, 1686, 1690-
Gating function 791 1700, 1704-1706, 1714, 1715, 1720-1722,
Motor function 789 1726, 1730, 1806, 1821, 1823, 1829-1831,
Basal nuclei or basal ganglia 146 1840, 1841, 1843, 1844, 1846, 1865, 1867,
Basal portion of the midbrain 357 1931, 1949, 1952
Basic division of the brain stem 142
Blind touch 1869 C
Bony labyrinth 224
Bouillaud 62, 63, 64, 67, 68, 73, 75, 122, 138 Callosal agenesis 1557
Brain modules 1432 Callous fibres 1499
Brain organization in nonhearing people 1789 Cardiovascular activity 415
Brain stem 1931 Caudal pons 272
ii
INDEX
Caudate nucleus 736 Circuits that modulate activity of the basal ganglia
Central auditory system 1323 786
Auditory cortex 1325 Claude’s syndrome 372
Auditory cortical anatomy 1326 Climbing fibres 468
Connectivity 1329 Clinical effects and symptoms 1448
Cytoarchitecture of auditory cortex 1328 Achromatopsia 1457
Central nervous system 1480, 1586, 1722, 1758, Apperceptive agnosia 1451
1759, 1770, 1774, 1931, 1936 Associative agnosia 1451
Central pathways 232 Color agnosia 1458
Cerebellar circuitry 479 Color anomia 1458
Cerebellar cortex 484 Cortical blindness and Anton’s syndrome 1460
Granular layer 493 Defects in constructional skills 1463
Molecular layer 486 Disorder of color processing 1456
Purkinje cell layer 488 Disorder of reading 1455
Cerebellar cortex – pharmacologic considerations Lesions in the association with visual areas 1469
495 Lesions in the primary visual area 1469
Cerebellar cortex – physiology 496 Other visuospatial anomalies 1460
Cerebellar peduncles 450 Prosopagnosia 1452
Internal structure 452 Simultanagnosia 1452
Cerebellum 143, 439 Topographical disorientation 1461
Cerebellum and behavioral syndrome 523 Visual agnosia 1450
Visual field defects 1448
Cerebellum and cognition 504
Visual hallucinations 1466
Cerebellum and language 506
Visual illusions (metamorphopsias) 1465
Cerebellum and psychiatric disorders 522
Visual imagery 1467
Cerebellum and writing 508
Visual neglect 1459
Cerebellum, working memory, creativity and gifted-
Visual object agnosia 1454
ness 508
Clinical effects of temporal lobe lesions 1382
Clinical aspects 511
Apperceptive deficits 1384
Cerebral asymmetry in nonhumans 1776
Associative deficits 1384
Asymmetry in birds 1781
Auditory agnosia 1384
Asymmetry in nonhuman primates 1782 Auditory hallucinations 1386
Asymmetry in rats 1779 Auditory illusions 1386
Handedness and functional asymmetry 1785 Behavioral aspects of auditory agnosia 1385
The evolution of handedness 1786 Cortical deafness 1383
Cerebral cortex 151 Disorder of memory, emotion and behavior 1387
Cerebral cortex 1592, 1949 Disturbances of time perception 1389
Discussion 1952 Neurological substrate of the disorder 1385
Unilateral and diffuse, bilateral cortical destruc- Nonauditory syndromes 1389
tion 1949 Smell and taste 1388
Cerebral hemispheres 150, 1414, 1480, 1483-1485, Vestibular disturbances 1388
1493, 1503, 1505, 1508, 1511, 1557, 1576, Visual disorders 1382
1578, 1580, 1583, 1588-1590, 1592, 1593, Word deafness (auditory verbal agnosia) 1385
1596, 1597, 1628, 1634, 1657, 1749, 1750, Cochlear nerve and nuclei 252
1753, 1761, 1777, 1781, 1798, 1814-1816, Cochlear nuclei 259
1838, 1850, 1859, 1875, 1880, 1898, 1902, Cognitive affective syndrome 520
1906, 1911, 1914, 1922, 1926, 1931, 1941, Columnar organization of the striate cortex 1434
1949 Color 1436
Cerebral peduncle 357 Cue invariance 1438
Cerebro-cerebellar circuit 482 Form 1437
Chemical specified systems 400 Functional subsystem 1436
Chorea 804 Motion perception 1436
iii
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Commissural connections of the hippocampus 903 Cortical layers, afferents and efferents 1625
Complete callosotomy 1554 Cortical localization of music 1717
Conceptual apraxia 1875 Cortical-striatal-pallidal-thalamo-cortical circuits
Conduction apraxia 1877 774
Connections between cortical areas and binding Corticocortical connectivity 1431
problems 1604 Corticoreticular fibres 409
Connections of the basal ganglia 752 Corticoreticulocerebellar pathway 465
Corticostriate fibres 753 Cranial arachnoid 1764
Fibres from the brain stem pedunculopontine Cranial nerve nuclei of the medulla 178
tegmental nucleus 756 Clinical considerations 181
Fibres from the ventral tegmental area 756 Cytoarchitectonic organization of the hippocampus
Globus pallidus pathway 763 893
Nigrostriate fibres 755 Interneurons 896
Pallidohabenular fibres 767 Molecular layer 895
Pallidonigral fibres 766 Polymorphic layer 896
Pallidosubthalamic fibres 764 The pyramidal layer 893
Pallidotegmental fibres 767 Cytoarchitecture of the temporal lobe cortex 1364
Pallidothalamic fibres 766 Asymmetry of temporal lobes 1366
Striatonigral fibres 761 Connections of the temporal cortex 1365
Striatopallidal fibres 760, 764
Striatum 753 D
Subthalamopallidal fibres 764
Thalamostriate fibres 755 Dale 8, 100, 101, 102, 103, 104, 105, 107, 122, 123,
The ansa system 757 124, 125, 643, 711, 1478
The lenticular fasciculus 757 Darwin 61, 126
The thalamic fasciculus 759 Decerebrate rigidity 376
Ventral (limbic) striatum 762 Dentate gyrus 904
Constructional apraxia 1879 Afferents to the dentate gyrus 908
Control of eye movements and other cranial nerve Efferents from the dentate gyrus 910
functions 410 Depth and the analysis of space 1439
Control of visceral functions 413 Descartes, René 36, 47, 94, 95, 130, 544, 1898, 1954
Cornu Ammonis (hippocampus proper) 886 Descending auditory projections 270
Regional variations 891 Details of cerebellar circuitry 481
Corpus callosum 1431, 1484-1486, 1492-1495, Diencephalon 144, 540
1497-1509, 1511-1516, 1518, 1520, 1523- Direct and indirect pathways (loops) 783
1528, 1530-1547, 1550, 1552, 1554, 1555- Dissociation apraxia 1877
1564, 1567, 1568, 1571-1573, 1575 Dopamine-releasing neurons 769
Corpus callosum and attention 1541 Dorsal pons (pontine tegmentum) 214
Corpus callosum and its visuospatial aptitudes 1527 Dorsal surface of the pons 211
Corpus callosum and memory 1540 Dressing apraxia 1878
Corpus callosum and reading 1526 Dystonia 807
Corpus callosum and the emotional function 1542 E
Cortical areas 1632
Cortical areas controlling motor activity 1640 Efferent connections of the diencephalon 620
Cortical columns spots and stripes (microarchitec- Adenohypophysis 631
ture) 1619 Diencephalic periventricular system 621
Cortical connectivity 1614 Dorsal longitudinal fasciculus 622
Afferent connections 1614 Fasciculus mammillary princeps 620
Cortical language areas 1714 Fornix 621
Broca’s area 1715 Hypophysial portal system 634
Wernicke’s area 1714 Hypothalamocerebellar fibres 624
Cortical layers 1611 Hypothalamocortical fibres 624
iv
INDEX
Hypothalamoprefrontal fibres 624 Hitzig 62, 78-80, 82, 83, 85, 127, 129,1128,
Hypothalamospinal tract 623 1233
Hypothalamothalamic fibres 624 Frontal commissurotomy 1544
Mammillointerpeduncular tract 621 Frontal lobes 1122
Affect 1188
Mammillotegmental tract 621
Ambiguity and decision 1190
Mammillothalamic tract 620
Attention 1185
Medial forebrain bundle 622 Attention deficit / hyperactive disorder (ADHD)
Neurohypophysis 628 1218
Periventricular bundle 624 Autobiographical memory 1202
Pituitary gland 626 Broca’s aphasia 1144
Supraopticohypophysial tract 625 Brodmann’s area 8 1147
Tuberohypophysial tract 625 Cingulate motor cortex 1153
Efferent fibres from the medullary RF 392 Connections 1158
Corticostriatal connections 1158
Efferent projections (output) from the vestibular nu-
Cytoarchitecture 1156
clei 238
Depression 1188
Efferents from the cerebellum 471 Disorders due to lesions of the upper motor
Efferents from the RF 404 neurons 1134
Egyptian physician Imhotep 9 Disturbance in self-awareness 1219
Emotion and motivation function of the basal gan- Dorsolateral frontal syndrome 1211
glia 793 Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) 1162
Encephalitis and the hippocampus 941 Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex 1163
Environmental accident 1790 Empathy 1198
Environmental deprivation 1790 Euphoria 1188
Executive control 1179
Epithalamus 542
Executive function, actions and plans 1176
Functions 544 Frontal lobe syndromes 1211
Habenular nuclei 542 Frontal-subcortical aphasias 1146
Posterior commissure 546 Function and localization within rostral pre-
Stria medullaris thalami 542 frontal cortex 1169
The pineal gland 542 Functional organization 1128
Extrastriate visual areas – outside of V1 1419 Hypokinesia 1189
Lesions in Broca’s area 1225
F Lesions in the frontal eye field 1226
Lesions in the motor cortex 1226
Loss of dexterity 1135
Facial nucleus and nerve (CN VII) 277
Medial prefrontal cortex 1169
Facial-oral apraxia 1878 Medial syndrome 1216
Fast synaptic subcortical control of hippocampal cir- Memory 1200
cuits 939 Memory of the future 1199
Ferrier 7, 62, 82-86, 93, 121, 127-129, 1057, 1697, Mental flexibility 1187
1729, 1732, 1776, 1900 Monitoring 1185
Figure-ground segmentation 1438 Motivation and cognitive control in the human
Complex forms 1438 prefrontal cortex 1175
Motor functions of somatosensory cortex 1154
Fissures, sulci, and gyri 1597
Neuropsychological functions of the prefrontal
Flourens 60, 79, 80, 127
cortex 1162
Forebrain 144, 1480, 1485, 1578, 1586, 1587, 1589, Novelty and routine 1199
1592, 1601, 1606, 1614, 1616, 1626, 1727, Orbitofrontal cortex 1165
1737, 1761, 1802, 1889, 1921, 1933, 1936 Orbitofrontal syndrome 1213
Fritsch and Hitzig 62, 78-80, 82, 83 Pathological hyperreflexia 1135
v
Leon DănăiLă – Functional neuroanatomy of the brain
Visual perception 1376 Hippocrates 1, 2, 8, 12, 15, 17-21, 24, 25, 27, 29,
Thalamocortical fibres 1614 42, 129, 130, 138
Thalamocortical relationship 1615 Plato 2, 8, 15, 16, 22, 25, 41, 127, 134, 135
Efferent connections 1616 The Golden Age of Greece 13
Thalamus 546 The Hellenistic Era 20
Anatomy 547 Tic disorder 808
Anterior nuclei and lateral dorsal nucleus 554 Top of the basilar syndrome 373
Borders 549 Tourette’s syndrome 809
Clinical considerations 584 Transfer of complex asymmetrically organized in-
Functions of the thalamus 582 formation 1537
Intralaminar nuclei 556 Tremor 804
Lateral and external medullary laminae 550 Trigeminal nerve (CN V) 283
Medial nuclei 559 Trigeminal neuralgia 315
Median nuclei 561 Trigeminal reflexes 311
Metathalamic nuclei 562 Trigeminal tracts 297
Neuronal circuitry 582 Tumors and degenerative diseases of corpus callo-
Nuclear groups of the thalamus 551 sum 1561
Posterior nuclear complex 566
Pulvinar nuclei and lateral posterior nucleus 566 U
Reticular nucleus 568
Ventral nuclei 570 Unconscious perception 1443
Unilateral apraxia 1537
The 4 generations of the Meckel family
Unilateral MLF lesion: internuclear ophthalmoplegia
Johann Friedrich Meckel The Elder 45
276
August Albrecht Meckel 52
Utricle 229
Johan Friedrich Meckel The Younger 50
Johann Heinrich Meckel Von Hemsbach 53
V
Philipp Friedrich Theodor Meckel 48
The clinic of the corpus callosum 1506
Ventral (basilar) pons 212
The Ebers Papyrus 11
Ventral and dorsal pathways 1440
The neural basis of consciousness 1928
Ventral surface and midtemporal cortical areas 1718
Patients and method 1929 Anomic aphasia 1720
Results 1931 Automatisms 1719
The superior temporal sulcus and biological motion Hallucinations 1718
1379 Illusions 1718
Theories of cerebellar function 500 Ventral surface of the pons 210
Theories of hand preference 1787 Ventricular system and meninges 1748
Culture and language 1788 Anterior wall 1751
Environmental theories 1787 Aqueduct of Sylvius 1753
Sensory or environmental deficits 1789 Cavum trigeminale 1762
The left hemisphere 1884 Composition of CSF 1758
The right hemisphere 1882 Cranial dura mater 1760
There is an inverted U-shape function between the CSF absorption 1757
level of stress and memory 936 CSF function 1758
Three faces of Greece and Hippocrates 12 Diaphragma sella 1762
Alcmaeon of Croton 14 Emissary veins 1763
Aristotle 2, 4, 8, 14, 17, 22-24, 31, 37, 42, 43, Falx cerebelli 1762
121, 138 Falx cerebri 1761
Early Greece 12 Floor 1752
Galen 3, 4, 8, 20, 21, 23-27, 30, 31-34, 37, 42, Floor of the fourth ventricle 1754
121, 128, 129, 132, 135, 137, 138, 140, 544, Fourth ventricle 1753
546 Frontal horn 1749
xii
INDEX
xiii