Fecal Matters: Habitus, Embodiments, and Deviance: Martin S. Weinberg, Colin J. Williams
Fecal Matters: Habitus, Embodiments, and Deviance: Martin S. Weinberg, Colin J. Williams
Fecal Matters: Habitus, Embodiments, and Deviance: Martin S. Weinberg, Colin J. Williams
and Deviance
MARTIN S. WEINBERG, Indiana University
COLIN J. WILLIAMS, Indiana University–Purdue University
at Indianapolis
This article examines fecal matters—namely, the social concerns that can accompany defecation and flatu-
lence. Researching 172 university students, we show how aspects of the socio-cultural context as “embodied” in
four groups of participants (heterosexual women and men and non-heterosexual women and men) mediate the
operation of the “fecal habitus”—that part of culture that interprets and organizes fecal events (Inglis 2000).
The study finds that the heterosexual women and the non-heterosexual men show the greatest commitment to
the habitus and the heterosexual men the least. It provides some evidence that the non-heterosexual women also
The authors thank Leah Van Wey, Cher Jamison, and Patricia McManus for help with the data analysis and presen-
tation, and Brian Powell, Elizabeth Armstrong, and the editor and anonymous referees for their helpful suggestions with
regard to the write-up. Direct correspondence to: Martin S. Weinberg, Department of Sociology, Indiana University, 1020
E. Kirkwood Ave., Bloomington, IN 47405. E-mail: [email protected].
Social Problems, Vol. 52, Issue 3, pp. 315–336, ISSN 0037-7791, electronic ISSN 1533-8533.
© 2005 by Society for the Study of Social Problems, Inc. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photo-
copy or reproduce article content through the University of California Press’s Rights and Permissions website, at http://www.
ucpress.edu/journals/rights.htm.
316 WEINBERG/WILLIAMS
activity sustained the interaction order; for Goffman this was “a skillful accomplishment for
the human agent” (Williams and Bendelow 1998:56). Thus, as he says, “the individual is
always in jeopardy because of the adventitious linking of events, the vulnerability of his [or
her] body, and the need in social situations to maintain the proprieties” (Goffman 1967:169).
Persons in everyday social interaction then must constantly pay attention to their bodies
through what we can call “social problems prevention work” (compare to Holstein and Miller
2003).
Shortcomings or failures at such work can lead to another of Goffman’s concerns:
embarrassment. Signified by embodied emotional signs such as blushing, fumbling, sweating,
etc., embarrassment occurs when we fail to project an acceptable self before others in the
social situation (Goffman 1967; Gross and Stone 1964; Weinberg 1968). The threat to charac-
ter is especially severe with regard to defecation, as failure to control the disposition of fecal
outputs in an appropriate way can project a self that is incompatible with a person’s identity
as a competent, mature adult (see Cahill et al. 1985). Such failures, furthermore, can be
experienced on a range of minor to major happenings. Events such as unintentionally
“breaking wind” in public violate conventions and are productive of embarrassment. Those
men’s and women’s embodiment are the same among persons who do not define themselves
as traditionally heterosexual (e.g., as gay, lesbian, bisexual). Specifically, do non-heterosexual
women tend to scrutinize their body and bodily processes in the same way as straight
women? Do non-heterosexual men tend to be as blasé about bodily functions as straight men
may be? Do any differences related to sexual identity affect the operation of the fecal habitus?
Recent work suggests that gay and lesbian embodiment is both conditioned by and
resists the dominant culture. For example, Ritch Savin-Williams (2001) found that many gay
men “. . . [are] repelled by . . . [straight men’s] behavior, their standard of dress and cleanli-
ness, and their barbarian nature” (p. 121). Moreover, Marc E. Mishkind and associates (2001)
say that gay subcultural norms place “an elevated importance on all aspects of a man’s physi-
cal self—body build, grooming, dress, [and] handsomeness” (p. 113). It seems that a new
phenomenon, the “queer body”—muscled, buffed, and tanned—has become a widespread
ideal among young gay men (Kiley 1998). According to Travis Kong Shiu-Ki (2004), having
such a “fit” body is part of gay men’s “embodied cultural capital” (p. 9). Many bisexual men
also are involved in gay subcultures to some extent (Weinberg, Williams, and Pryor 1994)
and may be similar to the gay men who reject certain aspects of “hegemonic masculinity”
sought to embarrass the offender and provide amusement to others. Or it can signify solidarity
based on flouting authority or middle-class adult norms of propriety.
We examine the above topics and questions with our research. First, we describe the
nature of the study and those who participated in it. Then, we discuss the embodied concerns
study participants had with regard to fecal matters and their fears of being labeled deviant.
We examine embarrassing and shameful experiences that ensued from breaches of the fecal
habitus and the projection of incompatible selves. We consider the role of the various senses
(sound, smell, and sight), the gender of the audience, and the role of the social relationship in
providing a context for one’s concerns. We look at social problems prevention work—the
strategies used to avoid the awkward interactions that can result from being designated devi-
ant in this way. And we look at how the intersection of one’s gender and sexual identity may
enter into these matters.
The Study
10 percent in another way. Ages ranged from 18 to 34 with two-thirds being 18 to 22 years
old. The mean age was 21.3. Finally, using education of father and mother as a crude indica-
tor of social class background, over half of the mothers and fathers had a college degree. Al-
though there are two significant differences in the above social characteristics among the
gender-sexual identity groups (i.e., a greater proportion of African Americans among the het-
erosexual women and a slightly older average age for the non-heterosexual men), neither of
these characteristics proved to be significantly related to the answers given to our questions.
The questions asked investigated their concerns and experiences surrounding fecal mat-
ters. One set of questions referred to the sound involved in having a bowel movement and
asked how uncomfortable the study participants would feel if this sound was overheard by
different people (e.g., a new date, a spouse/partner, a good friend, a work associate, a
stranger). Where appropriate, we varied the individuals posited in each relationship by gen-
der (e.g., a woman who was a good friend, a man who was a good friend). Later, if the study
participant noted differences in discomfort by gender and/or relationship, they were asked to
describe what was involved in creating such a difference. We investigated their concern with
the smell and sight of defecation and how their concerns were related to the particular sense
We begin by describing two common ways in which fecal matters are experienced and
how they create the anticipation of embarrassment, shame, and disgust. One is “body
betrayal”; the other is “proximity/distance.” Body betrayal (Featherstone and Hepworth 1991)
is part of a more general characteristic of embodiment. Body boundaries are highlighted by
efforts to maintain the integrity of inside/outside borders against the pressures of fluids, gasses,
and the like. Failing to do this can result in embarrassment when the projection of oneself as
a mature and poised adult is called into question. Thus, the experience of involuntary flatu-
lence is described by one heterosexual man as follows:
It’s not so much the action itself, as it is the reaction that passing gas gets from people. I guess the
most embarrassing thing about it is the loss of control in holding your gas. It just seems like in social
situations . . . that you’d be able to hold your gas [our emphasis].
Body betrayal also casts into relief the social expectation that these boundaries will
be controlled. Thus, people feel unease over “upset stomachs” and other gastro-intestinal
Fecal Matters 321
ailments. They are aware of their bodies as both being a container and being contained
(see Falk 1994; Johnson 1987) and from which substances can demand escape. Experi-
encing the body this way, although a private matter, nonetheless must be articulated with
the public fecal habitus. One of life’s most embarrassing moments can be “having to go”
in a situation either where toilet facilities are unavailable or in which one feels the
facilities can not be reached in time. Digestive trauma is one of the things that compro-
mises the body container and throws into relief the needs of the body. One heterosexual
man related his attempt to bring the physical and the social into alignment under these
conditions:
My [digestive] system has a problem with Italian food, and for my sister’s last birthday we went to
Olive Garden. I went walking in the mall afterwards, and I had to run to the toilet and I almost did
not make it. I actually sprayed a little poop just as I sat down. My family knew I was trying to run,
but I kept my cheeks clenched to avoid having an accident.
Clenching the sphincter muscles was the way he tried to control the body container. To
feel literally driven by the body in an attempt to appropriately meet its dictates is also
A second way in which the embodiment of the fecal habitus can be experienced is
through the failure to clearly distance oneself from one’s fecal outputs. Routinely this is done
through use of the flush toilet, the mechanical manifestation of the habitus. This does not
always occur efficiently, resulting in a disconcerting proximity to one’s fecal outputs. This can
produce feelings of disgust, defined by Paul Rozin, Jonathan Haidt, and Clark R. McCauley
(1993) as “a reaction to unwanted intimacy” (p. 576), especially to waste products of the
body. Failure to clearly separate oneself from one’s fecal outputs can occur in the following
cases: the fecal bolus will not flush away or flushing will not remove all fecal debris—in New
Zealand called “floaters” and “stripes” respectively (Longhurst 2001); a fecal smell remains for
others to experience; fecal matter adheres to the body or ends up in one’s clothing; fecal
efforts can be overheard; flatulence can be heard and/or smelled. One heterosexual man
reported the apprehension such a situation caused him:
[Embarrassing?] Leaving a skid mark in someone else’s bowl and not being able to do anything
about it. Like a piece of shit that is stuck to the inside of the bowl that the flushing doesn’t get rid of.
It was out of my control . . . They know they didn’t leave it and they could narrow it down to who
it might be.
Another described the following experience as potentially shameful even though the
audience was members of his family:
I was at my aunt’s house. This sucks. I took the most horrible shit, and the toilet wouldn’t flush, and
I tried several times to flush it, and pushed so hard that I broke the handle. My aunt, she didn’t find
out it was me, but she saw the shit and the broken handle. Because I was there, I was still a suspect,
still scared of being called out in front of my entire family.
With regard to the experience of proximity, the closer the distance between the person
and his or her fecal output, the greater the blow to self presentation, and the greater the
stigma. One non-heterosexual man recounted:
322 WEINBERG/WILLIAMS
I thought I was going to fart while I was working [he does large landscaping projects] and when I
did, shit started coming out. When it came out, it just kept coming so I ran into the cornfield, pulled
my pants down and finished in my pants. I had to take them off. I left them in the field and made a
pair of shorts with my shirt. And besides getting made fun of by my co-workers, I just finished my
day. Well, it was only two hours . . . To this day, they call me “diaper boy.”
It is worth mentioning that it is the sense of sight that is involved in these examples.
Although an obvious medium for gauging distance between a person and their fecal prod-
ucts, other senses can be involved too, notably smell. Producing a fecal smell through flatu-
lence or leaving one behind after defecating can also bring others into contact too close to
one’s fecal output. A non-heterosexual woman recalled not being able to adequately distance
herself:
Just having to go at work one time. It smelled afterwards, and we didn’t have any air freshener.
They made fun of me for, like, two hours afterwards. It was, like, pretty embarrassing at 15. You
know, I worked with them and had to go to school with them the next day.
Having one’s feces seen is a situation that, in addition to creating embarrassment, can
produce shame, in that many participants felt such an occurrence had implications for assess-
ing their character. One heterosexual man expressed this as follows:
If someone just hears or smells you, it is still an abstract concept of you defecating. But once they
see it, the image is imprinted in their minds.
This seems to be because such deviance can be directly attributed to a person—a concrete
reminder that they have breached the fecal habitus and have done so in a way that produces
a high degree of disgust. A non-heterosexual man said:
The visual impact is more powerful than the audible impact. When you’ve seen something, you
have a visual memory and that’s harder to deal with than the sound.
Out of sight, out of mind. It’s a personal thing for you to hear or smell someone going to the bath-
room. It’s not affecting you, but to see it face to face, it’s a very different thing. You are entering
someone’s personal zone.
The participants saw the sight of their excrement as more destructive of self than were
fecal sounds and odors. The seemingly universal nature of these reactions probably accounts
for the fact that we found no related differences between the four gender-sexual identity
groups. This was not because such experiences and anxieties were rare. Seventy percent of
the participants said they had experienced a situation where their stools would not flush
away. These experiences produced a great deal of concern over the discovery that they were
the source of the excreta.
The most common strategies to remedy such a situation were reported (with about equal
mention) to be continuing to flush (or putting extra water in the toilet), using a toilet plunger
(if available) or some other object as a tool, or such practices as blaming others or fleeing the
scene. These situations were reported to be stressful, as efforts at distancing were not always
immediately successful. A non-heterosexual woman reported:
Putting space between the self and the fecal product was evident in the case of this heterosex-
ual woman:
I pretended they [the feces] weren’t mine. I lied and said they were someone else’s.
And a non-heterosexual man described fleeing the scene of a fecal mess with an even greater
sense of urgency:
I was in the airport . . . and I couldn’t do anything, so I just washed my hands and left . . . [My boy-
friend] was waiting outside and I said, “We just need to leave.” One of the airport attendants was
walking into the bathroom at the time.
The above reactions flow from the anxiety felt from being unable to easily distance one’s
bodily products from the gaze of others. Such strategies are aimed at eliminating their associ-
ation with what the fecal habitus evaluates as polluting: human excrement.
The fecal habitus also strongly enjoins us to suppress our fecal odors. We found discom-
fort over breaching this rule to be less than for having one’s feces seen, but more than for
having one’s fecal efforts overheard. Thus, while almost half of the participants reported
anticipating the same level of discomfort over others smelling their bowel movements as they
did over being overheard having them, twice as many of the remaining participants thought
that it would be worse to have someone smell their fecal outputs. Again, the almost universal
disgust associated with the smell of fecal odors led to no differences in this regard according
to gender-sexual identity group.
Most of study participants anticipated that other persons would feel disgust over the
smell of their bowel movement. The greater negative anticipation regarding smell than sound
by many of the participants was attributed to the fact that smell can produce nausea, a corre-
late of disgust. And no one wanted to think of themselves as causing another person to feel
sick. One heterosexual woman described this anticipated effect on an audience:
[Smell] is more intense and more disgusting. It can be more embarrassing depending on how it
smells. The worse it stinks, the nastier they think I am.
Another factor entering into how participants felt their self would be affected was that
allowing fecal smells to escape called into question their self control. A heterosexual
woman commented:
324 WEINBERG/WILLIAMS
[They think] that it stinks . . . like I made the area smell bad . . . like it’s my fault.
Both the failure to contain fecal smells and the inability to distance oneself from one’s fecal
outputs are shown in the shame recalled by one heterosexual man:
In a public restroom . . . I was leaving, and the other person was walking in and there was a strong
smell. As I left, I made eye contact with the opposing person, and there was an awkward feeling,
knowing that they will be put in a situation of discomfort because of my bowel movement. The per-
son walking in had a look of disgust and I had a look of remorse . . . knowing that I was responsible.
I could tell that they acknowledged the smell and were not pleased.
Such anticipations may be widespread, but because we have argued that the fecal habi-
tus is mediated by socio-cultural factors there may be variations in what is anticipated.
Thus, whether disgust or humor was the more anticipated reaction was said by some to
relate to the gender of the person overhearing. A heterosexual man said:
Fecal Matters 325
For guys it’s kind of like a joke, like “dude, that’s nasty.” Women would probably think it was gross.
It’s a social norm that women should find that disgusting, or at least should never outwardly say
that they accept it or that it is not gross.
Sexual identity also mediates such expectations. Thus, we found about half of the het-
erosexual women and the non-heterosexual men anticipating that the hearer would feel
disgusted as compared to over a quarter of the heterosexual men and non-heterosexual
women (Table 1). A non-heterosexual man attributed his concerns to the influence of his
more effeminate friends (“divas”):
Only around people that I’m regularly naked with would I be comfortable with them knowing what
I was doing in the bathroom. I’m on the self-prescribed “pretty pill”—where you don’t fart, sweat,
burp, or use the bathroom. [“Pretty pill”?] That’s just a social construction between some friends
Non-heterosexual men who adopt such a stance not only distance themselves from their
own body products, but also from the attitude of heterosexual men.
With regard to humor, a different pattern appears. Both heterosexual and non-hetero-
sexual men were at least twice as likely as either group of women to think that others
would find their fecal sounds humorous, which was the next most common expectation.
The anticipation of humor when fecal sounds breach body boundaries, thus, seems more
Table 1 • Differences among the Gender-Sexual Identity Groups over Fecal Matters a
closely linked to gender than to sexual identity. In contrast, as shown, the anticipation of
disgust reflects the intersection of gender and sexual identity. On the one hand, the non-
heterosexual women were less likely to anticipate disgust than the heterosexual women;
on the other hand, the non-heterosexual men were more likely to anticipate disgust than
the heterosexual men.
In addition to the embodied nature of gender and sexual identity mediating the effects
of the fecal habitus, the anticipation of embarrassment also is affected by thinking of how
our gaffs and faux pas may be constructed by various audiences (see Weinberg 1968).
Referring to this, one non-heterosexual woman stratified her male audiences by sexual
identity:
If it’s a straight guy, there’s always that tension—some kind of male-female tension. With gay guy
friends, I am comfortable.
Heterosexual women were the most likely to anticipate that such an event could affect a
relationship (Table 1). This concern over incompatible images of themselves reflects the par-
ticular image that heterosexual women have been measured against, one that emphasizes
purity, restraint, and “femininity.” And this incompatibility was experienced as especially
salient when the audience was men. Further, in response to the question asking about the
gender of the hearer, over a quarter more of the heterosexuals (both the women and the men)
said the gender of the person would make a difference. In addition, the greater discomfort
was with the “other gender” among all of the groups except the non-heterosexual men who
were evenly divided into feeling more discomfort with men or women (Table 1). When we
analyzed why this was the case, men and women from the different groups often gave different
answers. The heterosexual women’s explanations were more likely to refer to a loss of attrac-
tiveness reflecting a deviation from gender ideals (Table 1). One heterosexual woman said,
With males, you feel they have this image of women that they just aren’t like that. You know the
“women don’t sweat, they glow” thing.
In the quotations that follow, gender norms were addressed by one heterosexual woman,
Some of the heterosexual men validated these women’s concerns. One said:
Women in my mind are beautiful, perfect creatures that are the object of desire . . . I don’t want the
image of them tainted in my mind.
Heterosexual men were more likely than the other groups to frame concerns about
themselves in terms of “bad manners” in the presence of the other gender (Table 1). As one
heterosexual man put it:
[It’s] compromising . . . I’m a guy and I’m a pig, and a lot of girls don’t like that, so I try to control
myself a bit. Compromising what I would normally do, like keep the door open when I’m pooping.
We have argued that heterosexual women’s self consciousness over fecal matters reflects
the salience of “attractiveness norms.” Non-heterosexual men, like the heterosexual women,
were also more concerned about a loss of attractiveness (Table 1). Their lack of ease stemmed
from a similar emphasis on the body beautiful, but in this case, in the eyes of other non-
heterosexual men. Additionally, non-heterosexual men of the type involved in this study
seem less likely to be integrated into networks of hegemonic masculinity (Connell 2002a)
and thus heterosexual men are less likely to be their reference group.
It is also interesting that men of any sex identity who avoid this form of masculine
expression may be labeled deviant because they are “not acting like men.” As described by a
heterosexual woman:
We had a guy who lived with us two years ago, and he went to great lengths for people not to hear
. . . him. He would run the water so we couldn’t hear him. He went to extreme lengths. But since
we’d hear the water running, we knew what he was doing. We were like: “He’s worse than a girl.”
The heterosexual men and the non-heterosexual women were less concerned with hiding
that they were defecating beings. On average, they worried less about the loss of attractiveness.
It appears that the greater embodiment concerns of heterosexual women and non-heterosexual
men reflect a dependence on men for their personal validation as sexually attractive.
328 WEINBERG/WILLIAMS
The lesser concern of heterosexual men over the embodiment of attractiveness is rein-
forced by the complicit masculinity (Connell 2002b) that local communities of heterosexual
men can sustain. Their greater indifference to body boundaries is most clearly seen when the
audience is other men. Thus, the heterosexual men stand out in having less concern over
their fecal sounds than do any of the other groups, especially with regard to good male
friends, men they know well, and male strangers (Table 3). Findings are not parallel for either
women’s group (i.e., women do not show a greater comfort than men do when women are
the audience—women who are good friends, whom they know well, or who are strangers).
Furthermore, it is not surprising that the heterosexual men also showed the least concern
about having a bowel movement in a public restroom and were more likely to report being
intentionally flatulent in the presence of other people (Table 1). As one heterosexual man
replied when asked what he thought other people would feel if they smelled his fecal gas:
Guys would say it’s raunchy and then say “Nice one,” because if it’s strong it’s more manly. You
know, because women would not try to clear a room with a fart.
Table 3 • Discomfort over Bowel Movement being Overheard, as Mediated by Gender and
Sexual Identitya
Group
The longer the relationship, the more it was believed to routinize the experience of the
other’s bodily functions. Embarrassment and shame were least likely to be reported in rela-
tionships that had become routinized or where a single event (like a fecal mishap) would
probably not outweigh other aspects of a primary relationship. In the beginnings of a rela-
tionship, however, unruly bodies were seen to draw the most unwelcome attention and make
incompatible selves the most evident.
We again asked participants how they complied with the fecal habitus so as to protect
their body boundaries. To the question, “What things do you do, or what strategies do you
engage in to avoid people from hearing you [defecate],” the strategy they mentioned most
often was to produce distance by waiting until there was no audience to overhear them. A
non-heterosexual woman said:
I go to the bathroom when no one’s there. I go in another room if I have to fart.
Of the strategies reported, waiting until no one was in a public restroom was followed in fre-
quency by running the water [in the sink or bathtub] of a private bathroom to mask the
sound. Flushing the toilet and/or waiting until other people in the public lavatory flushed
The strength of the norms of the habitus is also reflected in accounts of how embodiment
can be manipulated. For example, some persons controlled their sphincter muscles to let out
gas or excrement slowly, thus decreasing the sound of their bowel movement. One hetero-
sexual man stated:
If it is going to be loud, I would stop and go, meaning let it out in intervals so it would not be a big
kerplunk sound.
Other techniques to prevent people from hearing included having a bowel movement early
in the morning or late at night, going upstairs if people are downstairs (or vice versa), doing it
as fast as you can, pulling out the toilet paper roll to make a cover-up noise, turning the fan
on, and not using public restrooms at all.
Compliance with this aspect of the habitus was also shown to be mediated by the inter-
section of gender and sexual identity. The most common strategy of waiting until others were
not around was mentioned by at least two-thirds of the heterosexual women and the non-
heterosexual men. They mentioned this about twice as often as the heterosexual men and
the non-heterosexual women (Table 1). The strategy of using toilet flushing to mask the
sound was most likely to be practiced by the heterosexual women (Table 1). These results are
consistent with the findings reported earlier that show elevated concerns over fecal events
among heterosexual women and non-heterosexual men.
This article has explored what has been called the “fecal habitus” (Inglis 2000), that
aspect of culture that organizes the body in reference to fecal outputs. We investigated the
everyday concerns and social problems prevention work of a group of university students as
they go about actualizing the fecal habitus. Everyday social order is based on appropriate
body conduct, and our analysis reveals how such order is sustained through the threats posed
to one’s identity as a competent adult. We also looked at how these concerns and strategies
were shaped by socio-cultural factors, especially as embodied in four different gender-sexual
identity groups. Further, we hoped to show how, using the idea of embodiment, a sociology
330 WEINBERG/WILLIAMS
of everyday social problems could be enriched by reconciling the materiality and the social
construction of bodies.
To study the operation of the fecal habitus we began by examining two important fea-
tures of bodily experience: how corporality is contained and how people distance themselves
from bodily outputs. We then asked questions that posed a breach in the taken-for-granted
status of the habitus as the body comes into focus when fuelled by fears of body betrayal and
proximity to fecal matters. We examined how the body is activated, how one takes into
account different senses, how the effort to align the body and the habitus plays out before
particular audiences, and how the emotions of embarrassment, shame, and disgust make them-
selves felt.
We proposed that, given the dominant cultural heritage surrounding women’s bodies,
heterosexual women would be more concerned about social reactions than other groups
with regard to fecal events. The data from replies to both open-ended and closed-ended ques-
tions support this expectation. The heterosexual women were likely to anticipate more devi-
ant labeling, more disgust from other people, and more negative effects on relationships and
to report more worry about the loss of attractiveness. They were also more likely to report
the state we did find older residents to be less concerned over these breaches.) Also, our sex-
ual identity categorization suffers from the conflation of gay and bisexual identities into a
simple non-heterosexual status. The small number of participants with such identities pro-
hibited our keeping them in separate groups. At the same time, those who identified as bisex-
ual versus gay/lesbian did not differ significantly from each other in their answers to the
questions in the study. However, even combining the gay and bisexual men only resulted in a
group of 19 non-heterosexual men.
Theoretical Issues
Our results may help to revitalize deviance theory (see Best 2004; Goode 2002, 2003).
The findings illustrate how a universal physical act can become socially complex through the
anticipations of deviant attribution. Deviance theory may be elaborated by linking it to cur-
rent sociological interest in the body (see Crossley 2001; Evans and Lee 2002; Gubrium and
Holstein 2003; Williams and Bendelow 1998). An important theme in this recent work
1990:168). Such experiences, moreover, may outlast the situation that produced them and
have continuing emotional and interactional effects.
Three, we also found that vigilance concerning breaches of body boundaries was of
greater concern for heterosexual women whose body image was mediated by cultural notions
of “feminine” demeanor. Clearly, we can see how gender may provide a context for the gen-
eration of incompatible selves as it prescribes even stricter applications of the fecal habitus for
some persons. Gender, then, is far from merely a surface-level phenomenon, but is embodied
even in connection with the most intimate bodily functions. Thus, the habitus can reinforce
gender inequality through the anxiety, embarrassment, and shame it engenders in many
women (see Martin 1998). This is also shown through one concrete expression of the fecal
habitus, the public toilet. Women more often have to queue to use such facilities than do
men. This is because men ignore women’s different physiological needs and trivialize the
issue of toilet provision, such barriers reflecting “an embodiment of male power within cul-
tural and political institutions” (Edwards and McKie 1997:148). Such discrimination is physi-
cally felt by women as they must delay the need to relieve themselves which can result in the
fear of getting to the toilet in time. Even when the urge is less intense, as they stand in line
Five, our findings also have relevance for the study of stigma. The universality of bodily
phenomena like fecal matters can confuse the distinction between “normals” and “deviants,”
the essential base for much deviance theory. There will never be a time for anyone when
fecal mishaps fade away or become of no salience at all (in fact, with old age they can
worsen). In other words, the ever present possibility of body betrayal means that there are no
“timeouts” where not being stigmatized in this way can be guaranteed. Further, for embodied
deviance of this type it is difficult to identify typical deviant careers. This is not to say that
fecal events do not create more stable forms of deviance. Organized groups exist to deal with
problems like colostomies and such. And, there may be subcultures that routinely breach the
habitus. For example, in pre-AIDS San Francisco there existed a subgroup of non-heterosexual
men devoted to “shit play”—where participants played with feces and defecated on each
other in what was defined as a sexual context (Weinberg et al. 1994). (These “shit clubs”
show how local cultures can also affect the embodiment of the fecal habitus, another unex-
plored mediating factor.) In this case, it showed non-heterosexual men distancing themselves
more from the habitus than did heterosexual men—the opposite of what we found at the
References
Adler, Patricia A. and Peter Adler. 2003. Constructions of Deviance: Social Power, Context, and Interaction. Bel-
mont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.
Barry, Dave. 2004. “It’s Time to Plunge into the World Toilet Problem.” Indianapolis Star, November 7,
p. J7.
Barry, Kevin. 2000. “Are You Ready for Unisex Toilets?” (London) Times Educational Supplement, November
20, pp. 6, 2p, 3c.
Best, Joel. 2004. Deviance: Career of a Concept. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.
Blinde, Elaine M. and Diane E. Taub. 1992. “Women Athletes as Falsely Accused Deviants: Managing the
Lesbian Stigma.” Sociological Quarterly 33:521–33.
Brekhus, Wayne H. 2003. Peacocks, Chameleons, Centaurs: Gay Suburbia and the Grammar of Social Identity.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Brook, Barbara. 1999. Feminist Perspectives on the Body. London: Longman.
Cahill, Spencer, William Distler, Cynthia Lachowetz, Andrea Meaney, Royn Tarallo, and Teena Willard.
1985. “Meanwhile Backstage: Public Bathrooms and the Interaction Order.” Urban Life 14:33–58.
Callero, Peter L. 2003. “The Sociology of the Self.” Annual Review of Sociology 29:115–33.
Gubrium, Jaber F. and James A. Holstein. 2003. “The Everyday Visibility of the Aging Body.” Pp. 205–28
in Aging Bodies: Images of Everyday Experience, edited by Christopher A. Faircloth. Walnut Creek, CA:
Altamira Press.
Holstein, James A. and Gale Miller. 2003. “Social Constructionism and Social Problems Work.” Pp. 70–91
in Challenge and Choices: Constructionist Perspectives on Social Problems, edited by James A. Holstein and
Gale Miller. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Inglis, David. 2000. A Sociological History of Excretory Experience: Defecatory Manners and Toiletry Technologies.
Lewiston, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press.
Inglis, David and Mary Holmes. 2000. “Toiletry Time: Defecation, Temporal Strategies and the Dilemmas
of Modernity.” Time and Society 9:507–33.
Johnson, Mark. 1987. The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination and Reason. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Karp, David A., William C. Yoels, and Barbara H. Vann. 2004. Sociology in Everyday Life. Long Grove, IL:
Waveland Press.
Katz, Jack. 1999. How Emotions Work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kiley, Dean. 1998. “Queer Crash Test Dummies: Theory, Aging, and Embodiment Problematics.” Pp.
327–44 in Looking Queer: Body Image and Identity in Lesbians, Gay, and Transgender Communities, edited
Savin-Williams, Ritch C. 2001. “Memories of Same-Sex Attractions.” Pp. 117–33 in Men’s Lives, edited by
Michael S. Kimmel and Michael Messner. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy and Margaret Lock. 1987. “The Mindful Body: A Prologemon to Future Work in
Medical Anthropology.” Medical Anthropology Quarterly 1:6–41.
Schudson, Michael. 1984. “Embarrassment and Erving Goffman’s Idea of Human Nature.” Theory and
Society 13:633–48.
Shildrick, Margrit. 1997. Leaky Bodies and Boundaries: Feminism, Postmodernism and (Bio)ethics. New York:
Routledge.
Shiu-Ki, Travis Kong. 2004. “Queer at Your Own Risk: Marginality, Community and Hong Kong Gay
Male Bodies.” Sexualities 7:5–30.
St. John, Warren. 2003. “Metrosexuals Come Out.” New York Times, June 22, Section 9, p. 1.
Synnott, Anthony. 1993. The Body Social: Symbolism, Self and Society. London: Routledge.
Terry, Charles M. 2005. “The Function of Humor for Prison Inmates.” Pp. 337–47 in Social Deviance: Read-
ings in Theory and Research, fifth edition, edited by Henry N. Pontell. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Terry, Jennifer and Jaqueline Urla. 1995. Deviant Bodies: Critical Perspectives on Difference in Science and Popu-
lar Culture. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.