A Multidimensional Physical Self-Concept and Its Relations To Multiple Components of Physical Fitness
A Multidimensional Physical Self-Concept and Its Relations To Multiple Components of Physical Fitness
A Multidimensional Physical Self-Concept and Its Relations To Multiple Components of Physical Fitness
(PSPP; Fox, 1990; Fox & Corbii, 1989) and the unpublished Sutherland and Marsh
(1982) Physical Ability and Self Description Scale, which is the basis of the present
investigation.
The PSPP (Fox & Corbin, 1989; Sonstroem, Speliotis, & Fava, 1992)
measures four physical subdomains (Bodily Attractiveness, Sports Competence,
Physical Strength, and Physical ConditioninglExercise)as well as a global Physi-
cal Self-Worth scale. Factor analyses identified the four subdomains (although
the global physical scale was not included in these factor analyses) and the self-
concept responses predicted degree and type of physical activity involvement.
Also, the pattern of relations among the self-concept scales supported their
hierarchical model of physical self-concept and provided further support for the
Shavelson et al. (1976) model. Fox and Corbin (1989) noted, however, that the
Physical Attractiveness scale was not substantially related to physical activity,
although it was more strongly correlated with both their Physical Self-Worth
scale and a general Self-Esteem scale (see also Marsh, 1990). Fox and Corbin
focused primarily on the internal structure of physical self-concept, and tests of
the divergent validity were not emphasized.
Potentially useful directions for future research with the PSPP and other
multidimensional physical self-concept scales include stronger tests of (a) the hierar-
chical model based on hierarchical confiiatory factor analyses (e.g., Marsh &
Hocevar, 1985), (b) the divergent validity of the physical selfconcept responses
based on relations to specific criteria logically related to each scale, and (c) relations
between their physical self-concept hierarchy and a hierarchy of physical fitness
measures. The purpose of the present investigation is to pursue these issues by
examining relations between responses to the Sutherland and Marsh physical self-
concept instrument and multiple dimensions of physical fitness.
Methods
Subjects
Subjects (Ss) were 105 girls attending one of two private girls' schools in
metropolitan Sydney. All participants were in the eighth grade and were either
13 or 14 years of age. The girls came from primarily middle- and upper-middle-
class backgrounds. After obtaining consent from the school and parents, the
physical fitness tests and the physical self-concept instrument were administered
during physical education classes. The average performances on the physical
fitness tests by these girls were well below the normative average performances
of 14-year-old girls published by Fleishman (1964).
Measures
The physical fitness tests included four tests from Fleishman's Basic Fitness
Tests (1964) and the 12-Minute Run Test recommended by Cooper (1968). The
tests, administered according to instructions presented by Fleishman and by
Cooper, were Static Strength using a hand grip dynamometer, Balance (gross
body equilibrium) based on ability to remain balanced on a 1.91 cm rail, using
their preferred foot, with hands on hips and their eyes closed; shuttle run test
(Explosive Strength/Power) in which Ss ran back and forth five times between
46 / Marsh and Redmayne
two lines 18.29 metres apart (i.e., a total of 91.44 metres); dynamic Flexibility
in which Ss bent forward without bending their knees to touch an X marked on
the floor, straightened up, and without moving their feet, twisted to the left and
touched an X located about shoulder height directly behind them for as many
cycles as possible in 20 seconds; and 12-minute run (Endurance), which was
substituted for the 600 yard runlwalk test originally recommended by Fleishman
(1964) because it was deemed a more valid test of endurance (Cooper, 1968).
The physical self-concept items that comprise the Sutherland and Marsh
(1982) instrument and its a priori scales are presented in Figure 1. Instructions,
format, and the 6-point response scale were based on the SDQII (Marsh, 1990),
as were the general Physical Ability and Physical Appearance scales. The Balance,
Flexibility, and Endurance self-concept scales were designed to match and be
most highly correlated with the corresponding fitness tests of Balance, Flexibility,
and Endurance, whereas the Strength self-concept factor was designed to corre-
spond to and be most highly correlated with the Static Strength and shuttle run
(Explosive Strength) tests. The general Physical Ability scale from the SDQII
was expected to be substantially correlated with all the physical fitness compo-
nents, but to be most highly correlated with Endurance-the most general compo-
nent of fitness. The Physical Appearance self-concept scale based on the SDQII
was not predicted to be substantially correlated to any of the fitness tests. Coeffi-
cient alpha estimates of reliability were consistently high for all six physical self-
concept scales (.84 to .92). An exploratory factor analysis-a principal axis factor
extraction with a Kaiser normalization followed by an oblique rotation--clearly
identified each of the six a priori scales in that each measured variable loaded
more substantially on its intended scale than on any other scale.
Statistical Analyses
As recommended for analyses of SDQ responses (e.g., Marsh, 1990, 1992,
1993a), all factor analyses were conducted on item-pair responses in which the
first two items in each scale (as shown in Figure 1) were averaged to form the
first item pair, the second two items were averaged to form the second item pair,
and so forth. The use of item pairs is recommended because item-pair scores are
more reliable and contain less idiosyncratic variance and because the ratio of the
number of measured variables to the number of Ss is doubled. This is particularly
important in the present investigation in which the number of Ss (N = 105) is
small in relation to the number of items (50), so that even the ratio of Ss to item-
pairs (105125) is marginal.
Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA), performed with LISREL 7 (Jore-
skog & Sorbom, 1988), were used to test the a priori factor structure underlying
the self-concept responses and physical fitness tests. In CFA the researcher posits
an a priori structure and tests the ability of a solution based on this structure to
fit the data by demonstrating that (a) the solution is well defined, (b) parameter
estimates are consistent with theory and a priori predictions, and (c) the x2
and subjective indices of fit are reasonable (Marsh, Balla, & McDonald, 1988;
McDonald & Marsh, 1990). For present purposes the Relative Noncentrality
Index (RNI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) recommended by McDonald and
Marsh (1990) are considered. Both indices vary along a 0-to-1 continuum in
which values greater than .9 are typically taken to reflect an acceptable fit. The
Multidimensional Physical Self-concept / 47
Physical Appearancea
I am attractive for my age.
I believe I have a nicely shaped body.
*I am ugly.
I am good looking.
I have a nice looking face.
*Nobody thinks that I'm good looking.
My body looks nice.
I'm better looking than most of my friends.
Physical Abilitya
*I am awkward at things like sport, gym, and dance.
*I hate things like sport, gym, and dance.
I can run a long way without stopping.
I enjoy things like sport, gym, and dance.
I am good at things like sport, gym, and dance.
*I try to get out of sports and physical education classes whenever I can.
*I am lazy when it comes to sports and hard physical exercise.
I am better than most of my friends at things like sport, gym, and dance.
Strength
I am stronger than most girls my age.b
*I lack the strength needed to run fast.
I am good at lifting heavy objects.
*I am weak in physical tasks.
I would do well in a test of strength.
*I am weak and have no muscles.
*I have always been weak in physical activities.
I have a lot of power in my body.
Balance
I would be successful on a test of balance.
*I fall over often when trying to balance.
I think I could hold a balanced position for a long time.
I rarely lose my balance.
*I am not a very good balancer.
When necessary I think I could keep my body in a stable position.
*I have little balancing ability.
I have a good sense of balance.
Flexibility
*I have never been able to bend, twist, or tum my body easily.
I think I am flexible enough for most sports.
*It is difficult for me to bend and move quickly in different directions.
Figure 1 -The Sutherland and Marsh (1982) Instrument: 50 items and 6 a priori
scales. Note. Asterisks denote negatively worded items that should be reverse scored.
"Thesetwo scales were from the SDQII instrument. bFor more general use, the word
girl should be replaced with people. (Continued on next page)
48 1 Marsh and Redmayne
Flexibility (Continued)
I am quite good at bending, twisting, and turning my body.
*My body is stiff and inflexible.
My body parts bend and move in most directions well.
I think I would perform well on a test measuring flexibility.
I can bend and turn my body easily in games and sports.
Endurance
*I never last long in physical activities.
I am fit and can last a long time during physical activities.
*I have poor endurance in physical tasks.
I can last a long time in most physical activities.
*I am exhausted quickly by physical tasks.
I think I could run a long way without getting tired.
I can exert myself for a long period of time if I have to.
*I have very little stamina.
I do not tire easily in physical activities.
I get tired quickly if I start to run.
Figure 1 -(Continued)
RNI contains no penalty for a lack of parsimony so that the addition of new
parameters automatically leads to an improved fit that may reflect capitalization
on chance, whereas the TLI contains a penalty for a lack of parsimony.
Results
The first-order factor structure underlying the six components of physical
self-concept and the five components of physical fitness (Table 1) was well-
defined, the goodness of fit was reasonable (RNI = .911), and all the factor
loadings relating the measured variables to their latent constructs were statistically
significant and substantial (see Table 1).
Correlations among the six components of physical self-concept were sub-
stantial, varying from .448 to 399 (Table 1). Not surprisingly,the general Physical
Ability scale from the SDQII was substantiallycorrelated with the specific compo-
nents of physical self-concept. Among the specific components, correlations
involving Endurance tended to be the highest, whereas those involving Physical
Appearance were lowest. Correlations among the five components of physical
fitness were substantially smaller, varying from .024 to .394. The largest correla-
tions involved the Endurance component of physical fimess, whereas the smallest
involved the Balance component.
Correlations between the physical self-concept and physical fitness factors
varied from -.052 to 543 (Table 1). Consistent with a priori predictions, Endur-
ance self-concept was most highly correlated with Endurance fitness (r = .643),
and Strength self-concept was most highly correlated with the Static Strength
(r = 4 2 ) and Shuttle Run (r = .441) tests. Although Flexibility self-concept
was significantly correlated with the Flexibility test (r = .21 I), it was more highly
Multidimensional Physical Self-concept / 49
SC Appear1
SC Appear2
SC Appear3
SC Appear4
SC Physicall
SC Physical2
SC Physical3
SC Physical4
SC Strong1
SC Strong2
SC Strong3
SC Strong4
SC Balance1
SC Balance2
SC Balance3
SC Balance4
SC Flexible1
SC Flexible2
SC Flexible3
SC Flexible4
SC Endure1
SC Endure2
SC Endure3
SC Endure4
SC Endure5
SC Fit Strongb
SC Fit Balanceb
SC Fit Shuttleb
SC Fit Flexibleb
SC Fit Endureb
Factor correlations
SC Appear -
SC Physical 455 -
SC Strong 538 825 -
SC Balance 529 449 481 -
SC Flexible 549 786 775 723 -
(Continued)
50 / Marsh and Redrnayne
Table 1 (Continued)
Note. All parameter estimates are presented in standardized form without decimal
points. All estimated factor loadings, uniquenesses, and residual covariances are
statistically significant ( p < .05), as are factor correlations greater than .20. All
nontarget factor loadings were fixed at zero and were not estimated. The ability of this
model to fit the data was good, ~'(353)= 545.8, RNI = .911, TLI = 290. All
indicators are scored so that higher scores reflect higher levels of fitness and self-
concept.
"Residual covariances were fit relating the SC Appear 1 and 4, and relating SC
Strength 2 and 3. "The five physical fitness tests were treated as single-item factors
and were assigned an arbitrary, but plausible, reliability estimate of .90 by fixing the
uniqueness values to be .10 (see Joreskog & Sorbom, 1988).
correlated with the Endurance, Shuttle Run, and Static Strength tests. Balance
self-concept was not significantly related to the Balance test (r = .097), although
the Balance test was not significantly related to any other measure of fitness or
self-concept. The general Physical Ability scale was significantly correlated with
all the fitness tests except Balance, but the largest correlation was with Endurance
(r = .594). Physical Appearance self-concept was not significantly correlated
with any of the fitness tests.
The first hierarchicalmodel (Model A in Figure 2) posited a single hierarchi-
cal component of physical self-concept and a single hierarchical component of
physical fitness. The very high correlation between the general fitness and general
physical self-concept factors (r = .76) offered strong support for the convergent
validity of the physical self concept responses in relation to physical fitness.
Model A did not fit the data as well as the first-order factor (RNIs of .911 vs.
.883), but it did surprisingly well given its parsimony.
Inspection of the first-order factor suggested that there was a clear relation
between the two strength components, in addition to a general agreement based
particularly on the endurance components. In Model B (Figure 2), higher order
strength factors were posited for both the self-concept and fitness sides of the model.
Although this model was able to fit the data better than Model A (RNIs of .892 vs.
.883), inspection of LISREL's modification indices (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1988)
suggested the need for a correlation between the residual variances associated with
the Balance and Flexibility self-concept factors, as shown in Figure 2. This model
provided an acceptable fit to the data (RNI = .902), and the TLI, which took into
account model parsimony, was actually better for this model (391) than for the
first-order factor (.890). Although the correlation between the general physical self-
concept and physical fitness factors was still substantial (r = .77), the correlation
between the two strength factors (r = .45) was also large.
Multidimensional Physical Self-concept 1 51
both instruments and, perhaps, a range of external criteria that are logically
related to the specific scales from each instrument (e.g., the Marsh, Byme, &
Shavelson, 1988, study of the academic self-concept domain).
References
Bentler, P.M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin,
107, 238-246.
Cooper, K. (1968). Aerobics. New York: Evans.
Fleishman, F.A. (1964). The structure and measurement physical fitness. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Fox, K.R. (1990). The Physical SeZf-F'erceptio Profile manual. DeKalb, IL: Northern
Illinois University, Office for Health Promotion.
Fox, K.R., & Corbin, C.B. (1989). The Physical Self-Perception Profile: Development
and preliminary validation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 11,408-430.
Joreskog, K.G., & Sorbom, D. (1988). LISREL 7: A guide to the program and applications.
Chicago: SPSS.
Jackson, S., & Marsh, H.W. (1986). Athletic or antisocial: The female sport experience.
Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 198-211.
Marsh, H.W. (1990). A multidimensional,hierarchical self-concept: Theoretical and empir-
ical justification. Educational Psychology Review, 2, 77-171.
Marsh, H.W. (1992). SelfDescription Questionnaire II: A Theoretical and empirical basis
for the measurement of multiple dimensions of adolescent self-concept. A test manual
and a research monograph. Sydney, Australia: University of Western Sydney,
Faculty of Education.
Marsh, H.W. (1993a). Academic self-concept: Theory measurement and research. In J.
Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 4, pp. 59-98). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Marsh, H.W. (1993b). Physical fitness self-concept: Relations to field and technical indica-
tors of physical fitness for boys and girls aged 9-15. Journal of Sports & Exercise
Psychology, 15, 184-206.
Marsh, H.W., Balla, J.R., & McDonald, R.P. (1988). Goodness-of-fit indices in confirma-
tory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 391-
410.
Marsh, H.W., Byrne, B.M., & Shavelson, R. (1988). A multifaceted academic self-concept:
Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 80, 366-380.
Marsh, H.W., Hocevar, D. (1985). The application of confirmatory factor analysis to the
study of self-concept: First and higher order factor structures and their invariance
across age groups. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 562-582.
Marsh, H.W., & Peart, N. (1988). Competitive and cooperative physical fitness training
programs for girls: Effects on physical fitness and on multidimensional self-con-
cepts. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 10, 390-407.
Marsh, H.W., Richards, G., & Barnes, J. (1986a). Multidimensional self-concepts: A
long term follow-up of the effect of participation in an Outward Bound program.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 475-492.
Marsh, H.W., Richards, B., & Barnes, J. (1986b). Multidimensional self-concepts: The
effect of participation in an Outward Bound program. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 45, 173-187.
.LaupAs JO Ll!sla~!unaqL Aq pueIlaqlnS uAqoa 01 papnme SVM ley] uo!lmnpg 1e3!sAqd
u! ( s ~ o u o ~uo!le3npa
) jo lolaq3eg jo aasap aql ~ o smaura1!nbal
j aql jo luawIIglnj
1e!md se pa~l!urqns s!saql paqs!lqndun ue urorj a m 3 Lpnls s!yl u! pasn elea