Opportunities For Prevention
Opportunities For Prevention
Opportunities For Prevention
DETAILS
CONTRIBUTORS
GET THIS BOOK Deepali Patel, Rapporteur; Forum on Global Violence Prevention; Board on Global
Health; Health and Medicine Division; National Academies of Sciences,
Engineering, and Medicine
FIND RELATED TITLES
SUGGESTED CITATION
Visit the National Academies Press at NAP.edu and login or register to get:
Distribution, posting, or copying of this PDF is strictly prohibited without written permission of the National Academies Press.
(Request Permission) Unless otherwise indicated, all materials in this PDF are copyrighted by the National Academy of Sciences.
THE NATIONAL ACADEMIES PRESS 500 Fifth Street, NW Washington, DC 20001
Additional copies of this publication are available for sale from the National Acad-
emies Press, 500 Fifth Street, NW, Keck 360, Washington, DC 20001; (800) 624-
6242 or (202) 334-3313; http://www.nap.edu.
The National Academy of Engineering was established in 1964 under the char-
ter of the National Academy of Sciences to bring the practices of engineering
to advising the nation. Members are elected by their peers for extraordinary
contributions to engineering. Dr. C. D. Mote, Jr., is president.
For information about other products and activities of the National Academies,
please visit www.nationalacademies.org/about/whatwedo.
1 The National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine’s planning committees are
solely responsible for organizing the workshop, identifying topics, and choosing s peakers. The
responsibility for this published Proceedings of a Workshop rests with the workshop rapporteur
and the institution.
1 The National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine’s forums and roundtables
do not issue, review, or approve individual documents. The responsibility for this published
Proceedings of a Workshop rests with the workshop rapporteur and the institution.
vii
viii
Reviewers
T
his Proceedings of a Workshop was reviewed in draft form by indi
viduals chosen for their diverse perspectives and technical expertise.
The purpose of this independent review is to provide candid and criti-
cal comments that will assist the National Academies of Sciences, Engineer-
ing, and Medicine in making each published proceedings as sound as possible
and to ensure that it meets the institutional standards for quality, objectivity,
evidence, and responsiveness to the charge. The review comments and draft
manuscript remain confidential to protect the integrity of the process.
We thank the following individuals for their review of this proceedings:
ix
Contents
1 OVERVIEW 1
Forum on Global Violence Prevention, 1
Workshop Objectives, 2
Organization of the Proceedings of a Workshop, 3
2 FRAMING THE PARADIGM 5
Violence and Mental Illness: What Do We Know?
What Do We Need? What Can We Do?, 5
Operational Definitions for the Workshop, 8
Ecological Framework, 10
Relationship Between Mental Illness and Violence, 15
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Violent Behavior, 17
References, 19
xi
xii CONTENTS
APPENDIXES
A Workshop-Related Discussion Papers 75
B Workshop Agenda 129
C Workshop Speaker Biographies 139
BOXES
1-1 Statement of Task, 3
FIGURES
A-1 Mental health expenditures, 101
A-2 The impact of being bullied on functioning in adulthood, 121
A-3 Adjusted mean young adult CRP levels (mg/L) based on childhood/
adolescent bullying status, 123
TABLES
A-1 Mental Health Professionals in LAC, 102
A-2 Number of Users Attending Mental Health Facilities, 103
xiii
Overview1
O
n February 26–27, 2014, the National Academies of Sciences,
Engineering, and Medicine’s Forum on Global Violence Prevention
convened a workshop titled Mental Health and Violence: Oppor
tunities for Prevention and Early Intervention. The workshop brought
together advocates and experts in public health and mental health, anthro-
pology, biomedical science, criminal justice, global health and development,
and neuroscience to examine experience, evidence, and practice at the
intersection of mental health and violence. Participants explored how vio-
lence impacts mental health and how mental health influences violence and
discussed approaches to improve research and practice in both domains.
1 The planning committee’s role was limited to planning the workshop. The Proceedings
of a Workshop was prepared by the rapporteur as a factual account of what occurred at the
workshop. Statements, recommendations, and opinions expressed are those of individual
presenters and participants and are not necessarily endorsed or verified by the National Acad-
emies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. They should not be construed as reflecting any
group consensus.
WORKSHOP OBJECTIVES
In her introductory comments, planning committee co-chair Peggy
urray of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism ex-
M
plained that this workshop on mental health and violence prevention
emerged from discussions held during previous workshops, as well as cur-
rent events and media reports. She noted that what is known about mental
health and violence prevention is complicated, and what is not known is
vast. Law enforcement officials, in particular, are burdened by the number
of people with mental illness they encounter and are not well equipped to
deal with these numbers outside of traditional corrective means. Planning
committee co-chair Mark Rosenberg of The Task Force for Global Health
further explained that the intersection of mental health and violence is
confusing. Is the relationship unidirectional, and in which way, or is it
bidirectional? The workshop planning committee sought to shed light on
this issue to gain a clearer picture of this interaction.
Because the relationship between mental health and violence is com-
plex, complicated, and of interest to numerous stakeholders, the planning
committee acknowledged that it was not feasible to conduct an exhaustive
review in a 2-day workshop (see Box 1-1 for the Statement of Task). Thus,
the committee identified the following topics as important to address:
OVERVIEW 3
BOX 1-1
Statement of Task
An ad hoc committee will plan a 2-day public workshop to explore the rela-
tionship between mental health and violence. The workshop will feature invited
presentations and discussions with the goal of laying the foundation for progress
in improving outcomes with respect to mental health and violence embodied in
research, policy change, and program development.
Workshop speakers and participants will explore a continuum of approaches
to improving both mental health and violence prevention with these objectives:
T
he opening panels of the workshop set the stage for the subsequent
discussions. The keynote address provided an overview of the evi-
dence for and the policy involving mental illness and its relation-
ship to violence. Speakers presented on operational definitions, ecological
frameworks, cultural context, risk and protective factors, and neurobiology.
They noted the common misperception that mental illness plays a greater
role in the risk of violence than it actually does. Although, under certain
circumstances, persons with mental illness are indeed at a greater risk of
violence to others and, in general, are at greater risk for suicide.
1 This section summarizes information presented by Tom Insel, National Institute of Mental
Health.
domain of criminal justice and point to social inequities. Public health tools
are e ssential in reducing violence, Insel asserted.
At the highest levels of the U.S. government, there is both a desire to
address the recent shootings in schools and public places and a hesitancy
to directly address gun violence. This desire has translated into transform-
ing mental health care to reduce additional violence. Furthermore, Insel
remarked that the framing of mental health and violence on the same axis—
though done with good intentions—has resulted in more misconceptions.
To highlight this point, he shared the following data:
• Most people with mental illness are not violent, and most acts of
violence are not committed by people with mental illness.
• Some people with mental illness are a danger to themselves and
others.
• Fear of those with mental illness confounds the assessment of risk
(i.e., people with mental illness are more likely to be victims than
perpetrators).
• Early detection and early treatment can reduce risk.
Los Angeles.
part due to the ambiguity around terms. Vickie Mays noted that even the
term “mental health” is confusing; some equate it with mental illness,
while others place it on the side of well-being. To create a foundation for
workshop discussions, Mays presented a series of operational definitions
for common terminology in the field.
Mental health is defined as “a state of well-being in which the indi-
vidual realizes his or her own abilities, can cope with the normal stresses of
life, can work productively and fruitfully, and is able to make a contribution
to his or her community” (WHO, 2001).
On mental illness, Mays acknowledged there is no one encompassing
definition because perspectives, such as health care assessment and justice,
often have different aims. However, despite this difficulty, stakeholders
share a common goal of developing research that produces better predic-
tors, interventions, and treatments.
On the other hand, severe mental illness (SMI) has a greater consensus
in definition and comprises several disorders including bipolar disorder,
depression, obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD), panic disorder, post-
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and schizophrenia. SMI is disruptive, not
only for individuals, but also for families, communities, and sometimes in
the broader system. On the positive side, there are treatments, including
not just medication but also therapies. One important policy direction is
ensuring these treatments reach the people who need them. “In terms of
serious mental illness, we need to remember that recovery is possible when
we can get these treatments to people in an effective manner and in a timely
manner,” she stated.
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edi-
tion (DSM-5), defines a mental disorder as “a syndrome characterized
by clinically significant disturbance in an individual’s cognition, emotion
regulation, or behavior that reflects a dysfunction in the psychological,
biological, or developmental processes underlying mental functioning”
(APA, 2013). While Mays acknowledges some level of controversy around
the DSM-5 system, she highlighted the key elements of the definition of a
syndrome that is characterized by “clinically significant disturbance” in the
areas of cognition, emotional regulation, and behavior (APA, 2013).
Violence, as defined by the World Health Organization (WHO), is the
“intentional use of physical force or power, threatened or actual, against
oneself, another person, or against a group or community, that either results
in or has a high likelihood of resulting in injury, death, psychological harm,
maldevelopment, or deprivation” (WHO, 1996). Mays emphasized the
broad definition in thinking about interventions.
Conduct disorder refers to a group of behavioral and emotional prob-
lems that usually begin during childhood or teenage years. Children with
the disorder have “long-term, continual patterns of behavior” that tend
ECOLOGICAL FRAMEWORK
The ecological framework session included an overview and discussions
of risk and protective factors and intervention points related to mental health
and violence at the individual, relationship, community, and societal levels.
Medical Center.
San Diego.
she observed that culture overlaps sectors of the ecological framework and
is central to mental health and illness. She described several fundamental
aspects of mental illness that are shaped by culture:
Discussion
Following the presentations, speakers delved into the concepts and
themes they raised. They spoke of the failure of detecting mental illness
related to violence before the occurrence of such violence, particularly
suicide. Caine remarked that instead of focusing on individual risk, going
“upstream” at the population level means examining life circumstances
in the community and the family. The need to address the “bottom of the
pyramid” is felt around the world, and there is “tremendous commonality
around community engagement,” he continued. However, the focus is too
often on suicide or homicide as an individual problem, partly because of the
stigma around mental health. Caine also emphasized the importance of
assessing the continuum of the problem rather than the event itself, which
would include considering morbidity and disability when assessing the
burden of violence, and not just at mortality. Jenkins added that addressing
these societal issues would mean rethinking the concept of resource scarcity
and instead generating political will to build the needed capacity.
5 This section summarizes information presented by Mark Rosenberg, Task Force for Global
Mental Health.
of value in the ventral medial prefrontal cortex. These issues are also seen
in people with substance use disorders, ADHD, and externalizing disorders.
In summary, Blair noted that the three neurocognitive systems he dis-
cussed might have a relationship with certain disorders, but are not disorder
specific. The acute threat response, if overly responsive, is more likely
to have an episode of reactive aggression. If an individual has empathic
problems, then he or she will not be as responsive to the distress or pain
of others and is less likely to be inhibited in causing harm. And those with
problems in the reward-and-punishment circuitry have issues with external-
izing disorders.
He speculated that there are additional factors that affect brain pro-
cesses, such as poverty, which modulates decision making, and impover-
ished diet, which affects the development of brain structures such as the
amygdala. Genetics, too, might play a role in increased responsiveness in
the acute threat circuitry, and possibly other systems. And finally, he men-
tioned the role of alcohol, which in healthy individuals reduces response to
distress of others and affects reward–punishment decision making.
In the discussion following the presentation, Blair addressed a question
regarding how suicide plays out in these neurocognitive systems by noting
that it is difficult to determine because the brain architecture explored does
not generate self-harm behavior in mammalian species. Impulsivity plays
a role, but the process is not necessarily within one of the three systems
he described. While reactive and instrumental aggression are different pro-
cesses, what is reactive is subjective and lies within the perception of the
perpetrator and his or her social milieu. He also discussed the implication
of this research on treatment; presumably, treatments that teach pro-social
behavior should recalibrate these systems in individuals with mental health
conditions. This seems to be true in several cases, but he noted that conduct
disorders, for example, might also require pharmacology.
REFERENCES
APA (American Psychiatric Association). 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention) National Center for Injury Prevention and
Control. 2005. Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS™).
https://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisqars/index.html (accessed November 11, 2017).
Edgerton, R. 1966. Conceptions of psychosis in four East African societies. American Anthro-
pologist 68(2):408–425.
Farmer, P. 2004. An anthropology of structural violence. Current Anthropology 5(3):307.
Hawton, K., and K. van Heeringen. 2009. Suicide. The Lancet 373(9672):1372–1381.
Jenkins, J. H. 1991. The state construction of affect: Political ethos and mental health among
Salvadoran refugees. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 15(2):139–165.
NIAAA (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism). 2005. National epidemiologic
survey on alcohol and related conditions. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
Nielssen, O. B., and M. M. Large. 2010. Rates of homicide during the first episode of psycho-
sis and after treatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin
36(4):702–712.
Nielssen, O. B., G. S. Malhi, P. D. McGorry, and M. M. Large. 2012. Overview of violence
to self and others during the first episode of psychosis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
73(5):e580–e587.
NIMH (National Institute of Mental Health). 1991. Epidemiologic catchment area survey
of mental disorders, wave I (household), 1980–1985. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, National Institute of Mental Health.
Richardson, E. G., and D. Hemenway. 2011. Homicide, suicide, and unintentional firearm
fatality: Comparing the United States with other high-income countries. Journal of
Trauma and Acute Care Surgery 70(1):238–243.
Short, T., S. Thomas, P. Mullen, and J. R. Ogloff. 2013. Comparing violence in schizophrenia
patients with and without comorbid substance-use disorders to community controls. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica 128(4):306–313.
Smith, T. W., P. V. Marsden, and M. Hout. 2013. General social survey, 1972–2012. Chicago,
IL: National Opinion Research Center.
USSC (U.S. Sentencing Commission). 2011. Guidelines manual. Washington, DC: U.S.
Sentencing Commission.
WHO (World Health Organization). 1996. Violence: A public health priority. Geneva,
Switzerland: World Health Organization.
WHO. 2001. Strengthening mental health promotion. Fact sheet no. 220. Geneva, S witzerland:
World Health Organization.
At the Intersection of
Mental Health and Violence
T
he relationship between mental health and violence is complex and
often misunderstood, with a number of misperceptions around risk
of violence and victimization. Speakers discussed the stigma and
discrimination that people with mental illness experience, particularly in
the media. They also discussed the need for additional research on the
intersection of mental health and violence, noting that the detection and
the assessment of risk of violence are imprecise. Speakers also examined
how a better understanding of the pathways for and the risk factors of
violence could yield more effective interventions.
21
Panelists further explored these issues within the context of the use of
mechanical restraints, misperceptions around violence and its association
with mental illness, as well as the history of deinstitutionalization and the
failure to transition to community-based care.
Use of Restraints1
Because there is a misperception that people with mental illness are
more prone to violence, it is a common practice to use mechanical restraints
in institutional settings. But Elyn Saks asserted that the use of restraints,
though well intentioned, is itself a violent act. Restraints when used over
a length of time are extremely painful and degrading and cause feelings of
helplessness. They can also be retraumatizing for those with posttraumatic
stress disorder (PTSD) or for other survivors of trauma. She noted that,
in her personal experience, the use of restraints was not necessarily due
to her own behavior. In fact, the literature supports the idea that the use
of r estraints has more to do with the institutional ethos than other factors,
such as p atient characteristics or patient–staff ratio.
In exploring why restraints are used, Saks noted that there are studies
that indicate that restraints help those being restrained feel safer. However,
she observed that in her experience, she had never heard anyone express
that sentiment, and that emergency fatalities do not lessen with the use of
restraints. A second, more legitimate reason is that restraints can be pro-
tective for health care and service providers. For those patients who might
become imminently violent, there is legitimate justification for restraints.
However, she noted four reasons why the use of restraints as protection
might be problematic:
California.
Saks suggested that restraints might cause more deaths than lives they
save. In a series of articles in the Hartford Courant, a Harvard University
statistician estimated that one to three people die each week in restraints—
aspirating in their own vomit, strangling, or having heart attacks. She stated
that since there are other means of protecting people, it is not clear whether
restraints cost or save lives. Restraint-reduction efforts have resulted in
lowered use of restraints without increased violence in Philadelphia and
Massachusetts. The United Kingdom by and large does not use extreme
restraints, and has not done so for 20 years.
In cases where the use of restraints might be justified (e.g., transport-
ing a violent person or when a medical professional needs to be in close
quarters), Saks recommended several enhanced procedural steps:
2 This section summarizes information presented by Harvey Rosenthal, New York Associa-
Discussion
Following the presentations, panelists and workshop participants dis-
cussed additional issues raised, including challenges outside the United
States. In Latin America and the Caribbean, the movement toward recovery
and integration is not nearly as robust. Fisher and Rosenthal both noted
that the recovery movement has its roots in the United States, and it is im-
portant that, even while expanding it outside the United States, continued
work and sustained commitment is maintained at its origin.
Additionally, Eric Caine of the University of Rochester Medical Center
expanded on the issue of community mental health, observing that one of
the reasons for its lack of prioritization and funding was a change in the
way mental health was structured and treated. Caine went on to note that,
previously, community mental health fell under the purview of the National
Institute of Mental Health, but currently the Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) provides block grants to states,
with individual counties developing systems and allocating funds. At this
level of granularity, he postulated, grassroots and peer-led organizations
have an important role in shaping community mental health.
United Kingdom.
5 A PPV of 0.41 indicated that of those in the high-risk group, only 41 percent went on to
commit violence. However, a PPV of 0.91 indicated that 91 percent of the time, the test was
correct in determining an individual was in the low-risk group (Fazel et al., 2012).
outcomes from the meta-analysis, Fazel noted there was some evidence that
the instruments could inform treatment and management plans and could
be used to screen out low-risk individuals. In comparing them to other
tools, Fazel observed that they fared poorly compared with diagnostic tools
but were more similar to existing prognostic tools from other medical dis-
ciplines. However, the consequences of moderately useful tools in violence
prevention are different: There are costs in terms of extended detention, as
well as costs of staff training and time.
In a second review, Fazel and his colleagues looked more closely at
different tools specifically designed for populations with mental illness. He
noted that they were disappointing due to wide variation in their predictive
ability. Additionally, only two studies looked at schizophrenia, which would
normally be considered a risk factor for violence. In looking at the content
of the tools, he and his team determined there is a wide variation in what
is included; for example, the instruments included a wide variety of factors
related to criminal history, failing to converge on what that entailed. And in
another recent study by Jeremy Coid and his colleagues, these instruments
were found to fare even more poorly with psychopathy than they do with
mental illness (Coid et al., 2013).
Fazel concluded his remarks with a summary of his findings. The
risk assessment tools he examined had limited value in predicting risk of
reoffending but could be useful in identifying different risk groups for man-
agement. More importantly, he argued that the tools should be used dif-
ferently: to screen out low-risk people as a means of focusing resources on
the remainder. He also felt that the research could be better improved—by
independent funding, validation by impartial experts, and higher standards
of evidence—toward the development of better assessment tools.
groups, but would be more frequent the scarcer the resources. In a recent
meta-analysis, Wolke and his colleague concluded that, indeed, bullying is
found in all classes and segments in society (Tippett and Wolke, 2014). In
another meta-analysis, researchers discovered that bullying is more preva-
lent in more unequal societies, so that inequality as a proxy for scarcity is
in fact correlated with bullying occurrence (Elgar et al., 2009).
Adverse consequences of bullying have been explored in the litera-
ture, and Wolke shared some examples. In one study in primary school,
Wolke and his colleagues looked at physical and emotional health prob-
lems in the four previously mentioned bully groups. He noted that the
most strongly affected group is the bully-victims, who are somewhat
socially defeated. Those with the lowest problems are the pure bullies,
who are not victims of bullying themselves (Wolke et al., 2001). In a
longitudinal study on bullying history, researchers found that incidence
of bullying is not the only factor—chronic bullying has add-on effects.
Those who are currently being bullied fare worse than those who were
bullied in the past, but those bullied currently and in the past do worst
of all (Bogart et al., 2014).
In another study with far-reaching implications, researchers in B ritain
discovered that bullying in elementary school was associated with self-harm
with intent to commit suicide at age 17, with a population-attributable frac-
tion of 20 percent (Lereya et al., 2013). This means that if bullying were
eliminated, Wolke explained, 20 percent of adolescent self-harm cases could
be prevented. He emphasized the importance of this by noting that, by
comparison, obesity, which commands significant resources for its preven-
tion, only accounts for 3 percent of heart attacks. Other research supports
similar findings; another study found that chronic bullying before age 11
increases risk of psychotic experiences threefold (Wolke et al., 2014).
In studies done in adults who experienced bullying as children, re-
searchers again found health problems, particularly psychological condi-
tions, in those who were pure victims, but also bully-victims. They had
poorer psychosocial outcomes, as well, including difficulty maintaining
employment and relationships (Copeland et al., 2013; Wolke et al., 2014).
In another study looking at inflammatory responses to C-reactive protein,
the stronger or more chronic the bullying, the higher the response. The
largest change was for victims, followed by bully-victims, but the lowest
was in pure bullies (Copeland et al., 2014).
Wolke closed his remarks by summarizing the findings from the lit-
erature: being bullied has wide-ranging effects on mental health, from
increasing risk for psychopathology to adverse psychosocial and social out-
comes. The chronically bullied and bully-victims have the worst long-term
outcomes. Bullies do not experience these adverse outcomes but do tend to
show lower empathy and higher rates of manipulation as adults.
Discussion
In response to questions regarding mental illness risk factors for youth
violence, Pardini noted that the main driving predictors are conduct dis-
orders and oppositional defiant disorder; others, such as depression and
anxiety, do not have a strong relationship. Additionally, when he and his
colleagues analyzed other potential factors, such as trauma and physical
abuse and neglect at home, they were also not as significant in predicting
future violent behavior.
A related topic raised in discussion between panelists and the audience
was the role of the family in bullying. In response to one such question,
Wolke noted that over time, children spend more time with their peers than
their family members, emphasizing the importance of peer acceptance. He
went on to explain that while violence by parents is detrimental to a child’s
well-being, most violence experienced is by peers and siblings. However,
violence among siblings is rarely considered abuse or bullying. But Wolke
noted that sibling violence has adverse effects, particularly in regard to
bullying—those who are victimized in their own home by a sibling are 4 to
12 times more likely to be a victim at school, as well. And those who bully
their siblings are three times more likely to bully others.
Participants at the workshop also further explored challenges raised by
screening, including the important distinction between a diagnosis and a
positive screen, the latter of which has been shown to decrease productivity.
Additionally, screening in schools raises issues around data protection. The
combination of a false-positive screen and potential privacy concerns has
profound negative implications for individuals.
Finally, in a discussion around criminalization of sibling abuse, bully-
ing, and other violent youth behavior, panelists and several audience par-
ticipants raised skepticism around the effectiveness of criminalizing people.
While some bullying and family violence would under other circumstances
be considered crimes, families and schools are often reluctant to report
such incidents. In addition, several participants noted that rehabilitation
and treatment have a greater positive impact than criminalization in both
other-directed and self-directed violence.
REFERENCES
Bogart, L., M. Elliott, D. Klein, S. Tortolero, S. Mrug, M. Peskin, S. Davies, E. Schink, and
M. Schuster. 2014. Peer victimization in fifth grade and health in tenth grade. Pediatrics
133(3):440–447.
Coid, J., S. Ullrich, and C. Kallis. 2013. Predicting future violence among individuals with
psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry 203(5):387–388.
Copeland, W. E., S. Wolke, and A. Angold. 2013. Adult psychiatric and suicide outcomes
of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry
70(4):419–426.
Copeland, W. E., D. Wolke, S. T. Lereya, L. Shanahan, C. Worthman, and E. J. Costello.
2014. Childhood bullying involvement predicts low-grade systemic inflammation into
adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 111(21):7570–7575.
Elgar, F. J., W. Craig, W. Boyce, A. Morgan, and R. Vella-Zarb. 2009. Income inequality and
school bullying: Multilevel study of adolescents in 37 countries. Journal of Adolescent
Health 45(4):351–359.
Fazel, S., J. P. Singh, H. Doll, and M. Grann. 2012. Use of risk assessment instruments to pre-
dict violence and antisocial behavior in 73 samples involving 24,827 people: Systematic
review and meta-analysis. British Medical Journal 345:e4692.
Fox, J. 2015. Extreme killing. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Lereya, S. T., C. Winsper, J. Heron, G. Lewis, D. Gunnell, H. Fisher, and D. Wolke. 2013. Being
bullied during childhood and the prospective pathways to self-harm in late adolescence.
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 52(6):608–618.
Swanson, J. 2015. Mental illness and reduction of gun violence and suicide: Bringing epide-
miologic research to policy. Annals of Epidemiology 25(5):366–376.
Tippett, N., and D. Wolke. 2014. Socioeconomic status and bullying: A meta-analysis. Ameri-
can Journal of Public Health 104(6):e48–e59.
van Wijk, A., R. Loeber, R. Vermeiren, D. Pardini, R. Bullens, and T. Doreleijers. 2005. Violent
juvenile sex offenders compared with violence juvenile nonsex offenders: Explorative
findings from the Pittsburgh Youth Study. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and
Treatment 17(3):333–352.
Wilson, S. J., M. W. Lipsey, and J. H. Derzon. 2003. The effects of school-based intervention
programs on aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology 71(1):136–149.
Wolke, D., S. Woods, K. Stanford, and H. Schulz. 2001. Bullying and victimization of primary
school children in England and Germany: Prevalence and school factors. British Journal
of Psychology 92:673–696.
Wolke, D., S.T. Lereya, H.L. Fisher, G. Lewis, and S. Zammit. 2014. Bullying in elementary
school and psychotic experiences at 18 years: A longitudinal, population-based cohort
study. Psychological Medicine 44(10):2199–2211.
D
espite advances in the science of violence prevention, several gaps
and challenges remain. The impact of violence is mediated through
several means, such as firearms and pesticides, and modifiers, such
as alcohol. Specifically, the lethality of firearms and commonly used pesti-
cides result in higher fatalities, while alcohol reduces inhibitions that might
otherwise be a barrier to violence. Speakers presented on how these means
and modifiers affect violence and violence prevention, and how reducing
access to these means can reduce violence.
35
Other studies, both cross-sectional and longitudinal, that look at the popu-
lation level have found a positive association with prevalence of firearms
ownership and risk of suicide, with higher risks at younger ages2 (Miller
et al., 2007; Stevens et al., 2006). On a similar note, an analysis of gun
ownership rates in the 1990s and youth suicide rates showed that suicides
decreased dramatically as household gun rates dropped.
Regarding restricting firearms access to youth, the laws that require
owners to lock guns away reduced suicide risk among 14- to 17-year-olds
by 8 percent overall. Additional laws related to restricting firearms did not
have an effect, though, nor did they have an effect on older youth aged 22
to 24 years3 (Webster et al., 2004). Other studies support these findings,
showing protective effects for older individuals with the Brady Handgun
Violence Prevention Act and some state laws that require permits for pur-
chasing (Andrés and Hempstead, 2011).
5 This section summarizes information presented by Mike Luo, The New York Times.
violence events, such as one in which a disturbed young man opened fire on
a crowd, killing several people, he questioned what it meant for people with
mental illness to have access to firearms. He noted that the current federal
standard stipulates that one cannot purchase or possess firearms if one has
been involuntarily committed or adjudicated as “mentally defective.” The
vast majority of those with mental illness, even severe mental illness, will
never get to this point. Luo and his colleagues wanted to explore this area
further to learn the stories of people with diagnosed mental illness who
possessed firearms. However, he noted, there was a big privacy challenge in
this area, dealing with both mental health and gun ownership.
In most states, he observed, records of purchases of concealed handgun
permits are not publicly available. Because he was unable to obtain such
records via public inquiries, he instead inquired at police departments and
courts for records of people from whom firearms were confiscated for
mental health reasons. Such calls are not enough to disqualify someone
from possessing firearms but are usually grounds for temporary confiscation
because a person is a danger to himself or herself or to others. However,
the circumstances under which it is legal to confiscate a firearm are not
straightforward. Luo noted that while taking away a firearm on someone’s
person is usually allowable, the situation is less clear when the firearm is
in another location. Most police departments would require a warrant to
confiscate the weapon in these circumstances; for example, Connecticut and
Indiana passed laws giving police more leeway on this.
In the past year in Connecticut, there were 180 instances when police
removed firearms from people they deemed to pose a risk of imminent
danger, 40 percent of which involved serious mental illness. In 2012, Luo
and his colleagues found that in Marion County, Indiana, there were 30
instances of confiscation, with about 40 percent of those involving mental
illness. Most people were placed under observation, but not involuntarily
committed, and in most instances, the firearms were returned shortly.
Luo cited a few examples of these policies at work. In Indianapolis,
before the law giving police greater jurisdiction was passed, an individual
with a diagnosis of schizophrenia retook possession of his firearm and
was later involved in a police shooting. In Hillsborough County, Florida,
there was another instance in which a veteran with a history of treatment
for depression, anxiety, and paranoia made violence-related comments
to his psychiatrist and subsequently had his firearms confiscated. He was
involuntarily hospitalized but not committed, and a few months later had
his firearms returned. In a third situation in Colorado, an individual with
prior suicide attempts, who also had not been committed, had successfully
requested his firearms be returned.
In the context of these cases, Luo raised a series of questions about the
intersection of privacy, regulation, public health, science, and rights. Given
Discussion
In the subsequent discussion, workshop participants shared their per-
spectives on issues raised during the presentations, particularly around
predicting future violence and restricting lethal means. Webster noted
that, rather than focusing on diagnosis or involuntary commitment to dis
qualify an individual from owning a lethal weapon, the focus should be on
potential danger. For example, indicators of substance abuse, such as mul-
tiple violations for driving under the influence of alcohol (DUIs), magnify
risk associated with severe mental illness. Mark Rosenberg of The Task
Force for Global Health agreed, and he suggested that stress and distress
are triggers for suicide and could be assessed.
Phillips echoed comments made earlier, stating that predicting indi
vidual behavior is very difficult, and current instruments to do so are impre-
cise. Luo concurred, mentioning that most assessments of risk are conducted
by psychiatrists using unstructured criteria.
Eric Caine of the University of Rochester Medical Center considered
whether community-based approaches, in which all members of the com-
munity feel invested, might be a more effective means of reducing violence.
He spoke about a program in King County, Washington, in which a coali-
tion was built among public health officials, injury prevention stakeholders,
and firearms retailers. The program involves incentivizing firearms buyers
to purchase gun locks by offering discounts. It does not ask anyone to
serve as a gatekeeper, but instead builds a community of safety. Caine sug-
gested that such an approach might help bridge differences among different
stakeholders.
Miczek closed by emphasizing the important role that the mouse model
played in teasing apart the various pathways in the brain that result in
aggressive behavior, particularly in relation to alcohol consumption, in a
manner not possible in human research.
on Addictions.
1998). In a similar study, researchers also found that the association be-
tween alcohol and negativity was highest among those who also showed
antisocial tendencies (Jacob et al., 2001).
Leonard proposed a cognitive disruption model to explain alcohol’s
impact on aggression. Intoxication leads to some level of cognitive impair-
ment, to which people adapt by focusing on salient cues and missing subtle
context. This theory suggests that alcohol should exacerbate overt emo-
tions in certain settings—that is, a setting that evokes aggression would be
heightened with alcohol, whereas a situation that evokes sadness would
have low risk of aggression. Alcohol exacerbates a person’s reaction to
the most dominant cues and hides those that are peripheral and could be
inhibiting (Parrott and Giancola, 2004). To the extent that mental illness
is associated with negative affect and impaired self-control, Leonard pos-
tulated, alcohol might interact with psychopathology to create a high risk
of violence. He showed one study that suggested a synergistic effect for
substance use disorders and mental illness (Van Dorn et al., 2012).
Finally, successful treatment of alcohol use disorders results in a reduc-
tion of aggression. If sobriety levels are maintained, then both verbal and
physical violence is reduced. If there is relapse, then the rate of violence
increases again.
approach that aims to change behavior and the conditions that shape that
behavior. Often, these two approaches are seen as oppositional, when they
should be complementary. Nelson spoke of a theory developed by Alex
Wagenaar and Cheryl Perry that looked at the relationship of high-risk in-
dividuals embedded within a community. While much research has focused
on drinking and alcohol-related problems or individual risk factors that
increase drinking behavior, Wagenaar and Perry considered that, within
the community, there is a wide range of availability of alcohol. Problems
of alcohol at the individual level are a function of the economic, legal, or
physical availability of alcohol in those communities. This availability, in
turn, is shaped by policies and norms around how alcohol is provided or
restricted. Nelson and his colleagues examined the efficacy of some of these
policies around the United States and created a list of the top 10:
Regarding taxes, Nelson noted the evidence that price strategies are
inversely related to violence: the higher the price of alcohol, the lower the
rates of alcohol-related violence (Wagenaar et al., 2010). On alcohol retail
density, studies show more violence and violent crime where there is greater
density, and time-series data show increases in violence when alcohol out-
lets privatize and proliferate. Despite evidence for these types of programs
being effective in reducing harm related to alcohol consumption, there has
not been increased uptake of them, Nelson noted. Instead, policies judged
to be less effective are on the rise in the United States.
eral de São Paulo and National Institute on Alcohol and Drug Policy, Brazil.
unregulated alcohol market and close ties between the alcohol industry and
politicians are challenges for the alcohol control policy landscape. There
is little awareness among policymakers and little community involvement
in alcohol policy, and there are few good examples of successful alcohol
policies and programs within Brazil.
Ronaldo Laranjeira further described the unregulated market in Brazil:
There is no licensing requirement to sell alcohol, resulting in nearly 1 alco-
hol outlet per 200 people. In addition, an estimated 30 percent of drivers
on weekends are intoxicated, and there is little restriction on adolescent
purchase of alcohol. The price of alcohol is inexpensive as well. Laranjeira
noted that one can of beer costs 30 cents (by comparison, a liter of orange
juice costs $3.50). Alcohol is also marketed heavily in Brazil. Laranjeira
stated that normally, sale of alcohol in sports stadiums is forbidden by law.
However, for the World Cup in 2014, lobbying from both the International
Federation of Associated Football (FIFA) and the alcohol industry suc-
ceeded in changing that law.
Despite these and other challenges, Laranjeira shared an example of a
successful program in the city of Diadema, in São Paolo state. Diadema has
around 350,000 inhabitants, most of whom are low to middle class. In the
1990s, there was a high rate of homicides—102 per 100,000 people—with
50 percent occurring between 9 p.m. and 6 a.m. There was also a high rate
of violence against women during that time, as well as a high incidence
of gang activity and car crashes. In 2002, a municipal law was passed
prohibiting the sale of alcohol between 11 p.m. and 6 a.m. The law was
enforced, with local police verifying compliance every night. The first viola-
tion resulted in a warning, the second in a fine, and the third in a fine and
the working permit license suspended. Despite the previously mentioned
lack of licensing, enforcement on this law was active, and there was high
approval among the community.
Ten years of homicide data were examined, from 1995 to 2005, includ-
ing homicide data both before and after the law was enacted. The results
showed that 528 lives were saved, with a 46 percent reduction in homicides.
Additional years of data have reinforced this reduction in violence, with a
homicide rate of less than 20 per 100,000 people in the past few years and a
decrease in violence against women, as well (Duailibi et al., 2007). Laranjeira
closed by noting that one important factor in the success of this program was
the continued enforcement; success of program replications in other cities has
decreased because of a failure to sustain the nightly police checks.
Discussion
In the ensuing discussion, speakers and workshop participants fur-
ther explored some of the main themes raised, notably the importance
REFERENCES
Andrés, R., and K. Hempstead. 2011. Gun control and suicide: The impact of state firearm
regulations in the United States. Health Policy 101:95–103.
Bushman, B. J. 1997. Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Validity of proposed explana-
tions. Recent Developments in Alcoholism 13:227–243.
Duailibi, S., W. Ponicki, J. Grube, I. Pinsky, R. Laranjeira, and M. Raw. 2007. The effect of
restricting opening hours on alcohol-related violence. American Journal of Public Health
97(12):2276–2280.
Gunnell, D., M. Eddleston, M. Phillips, and F. Konradsen. 2007. The global distribution of
fatal pesticide self-poisoning: Systematic review. BMC Public Health 7:357.
Ito, T. A., N. Miller, and V. E. Pollock. 1996. Alcohol and aggression: A meta-analysis on
the moderating effects of inhibitory cues, triggering events, and self-focused attention.
Psychological Bulletin 120(1):60–82.
Jacob, T., K. E. Leonard, and J. R. Haber. 2001. Family interactions of alcoholics as related
to alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25:834–843.
Leonard, K. E., and B. M. Quigley. 1999. Drinking and marital aggression in newlyweds:
An event-based analysis of drinking and the occurrence of husband marital aggression.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 60(4):537–545.
Leonard, K. E., and L. J. Roberts. 1998. The effects of alcohol on the marital interactions of
aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology
107(4):602–615.
Leonard, K. E., and M. Senchak. 1996. Prospective prediction of husband marital aggression
within newlywed couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105(3):369–380.
Miller, M., S. J. Lippmann, D. Azrael, and D. Hemenway. 2007. Household firearm owner-
ship and rates of suicide across the 50 United States. Journal of Trauma and Acute Care
Surgery 62(4):1029–1034.
Murphy, C. M., J. Winters, T. J. O’Farrell, W. Fals-Stewart, and M. Murphy. 2005. Alcohol
consumption and intimate partner violence by alcoholic men: Comparing violent and
nonviolent conflicts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 19(1):35–42.
Parrott, D. J., and P. R. Giancola. 2004. A further examination of the relation between trait
anger and alcohol-related aggression: The role of anger control. Alcoholism: Clinical and
Experimental Research 28(6):855–864.
Stevens, J. A., P. S. Corso, E. A. Finkelstein, and T. R. Miller. 2006. The costs of fatal and
non-fatal falls among older adults. Injury Prevention 12(5):290–295.
Swanson, J. W., L. K. Frisman, A. G. Robertson, H. J. Lin, R. L. Trestman, D. A. Shelton, K.
Parr, E. Rodis, A. Buchanan, and M. S. Swartz. 2013. Costs of criminal justice involve-
ment among persons with serious mental illness in Connecticut. Psychiatric Services
64(7):630–637.
Van Dorn, R., J. Volavka, and N. Johnson. 2012. Mental disorder and violence: Is there
a relationship beyond substance use? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology
47(3):487–503.
Vigdor, E. R., and J. A. Mercy. 2006. Do laws restricting access to firearms by domestic vio-
lence offenders prevent intimate partner homicide? Evaluation Review 30(3):313–346.
Wagenaar, A. C., A. L. Toblet, and K. A. Komro. 2010. Effects of alcohol tax and price poli-
cies on morbidity and mortality: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health
100(11):2270–2278.
Webster, D. W., J. S. Vernick, A. M. Zeoli, and J. A. Manganello. 2004. Association between
youth-focused firearm laws and youth suicides. JAMA 292(5):594–601.
Wiebe, D. J. 2003. Homicide and suicide risks associated with firearms in the home: A national
case-control study. Annals of Emergency Medicine 41(6):771–782.
S
everal systems, particularly mental health services and the justice
system, play crucial roles in addressing mental illness and violence.
If not established as supportive structures, they can cause harm and
trauma and possibly increase the risk of violence. Speakers explored how
these systems can protect and heal, by building positive environments and
providing treatment and redress.
51
a study that found 46 percent of those surveyed believe that people with
serious mental illness are much more dangerous than those without; 29 per-
cent were willing to work closely with someone with a mental illness, and
33 percent were willing to have a neighbor with a mental illness. These per-
ceptions are also affected by whether the respondents had experience, either
directly or through a family member or close friend, with mental illness.
This context, Barry argued, is important for considering the connection
between public attitudes and broader support for mental health services.
Many people experience mental illness, and seeking care is common;
one in five seeks care yearly, and one in three over the lifetime. Broadly
speaking, she asserted, treatment history or diagnosis is not a specific or
useful predictor of violence. Most people with mental illness do not com-
mit acts of violence, and most violent acts are not committed by people
with a diagnosis of mental disorder. Less than 2 percent of the population
meets the diagnostic criteria for severe and persistent mental illness, and it
is a subgroup of those—adults with conduct disorders in childhood—that
has the strongest association with violence. But even among that sub-
group, the majority is not violent but instead is more likely to be victims
of violence.
Given this background, Barry asked, can access to services impact
violence? She described two types of services most often discussed: broad
institutionalized care and universal screening. On the first, she observed
that there is no clear association between institutionalized care and patterns
of violence among people with severe and persistent mental illness. Addi
tionally, there are several civil rights challenges with institutionalization
and its history of practice. On universal screening, she pointed out there is
both low specificity for screening instruments and a lack of capacity in the
system for the additional individuals who might be identified in screening.
She concluded that broad approaches are not likely to be effective in reduc-
ing violence related to severe mental health disorders.
There is a role, she noted, for targeted interventions that improve access
and treatment for adolescents with conduct disorders, particularly interven-
tions that address co-occurring mental health and substance use disorders
or that are oriented toward suicide prevention. Yet, many of them have not
been well implemented. She emphasized, however, that there are reasons
beyond violence to improve behavioral health systems in the United States,
such as dealing with undertreatment and inappropriate treatment, quality
of care, and even measuring and tracking quality. This has implications for
payment and insurance, particularly in the context of performance-based
metrics. In contrast to the overall health care system, Medicaid plays a
much larger role in covering the costs of care and treatment, while private
insurance is limited. Even within insurance schemes, historically, mental
health services have been underprovisioned and underfunded. However,
people with severe mental illnesses are uninsured at a much higher rate than
people with no mental diagnosis.
The Paul Wellstone and Pete Domenici Mental Health Parity and
Addiction Equity Act, enacted in 2008, was intended to equalize coverage
for mental health and substance use services, as comparable to other health
services within an insurance program, including not only coinsurance, but
deductibles and copayments, as well. It also required that insurance pro-
gram designs, including elements such as prior authorization and provider
networks, had to be equal. Its impact is significant, especially in providing
out-of-pocket financial protection.
The Patient Protection and Affordable Care Act of 2010, designed
to expand access to and affordability of all health care services, has also
resulted in increased coverage of mental health services. Barry explained
that this increase is due to an expansion of public programs, reform and
redesign of insurance markets, and delivery system and payment reform.
While the state health insurance exchanges account for some of the expan-
sion, the bulk of it is a result of Medicaid expansion, particularly the new
Medicaid Health Home option, which allows for different types of services
that have not been traditionally financed but are important for coordinat-
ing care.
The new health care provisions also have implications for criminal jus-
tice. People in prisons have the option to enroll in or maintain M edicaid,
which provides continuity of coverage. For those on antipsychotics, for
example, this means continued medication access and could result in low-
ered recidivism. Barry closed with a reflection about stigma and mental
health, citing a recent study of her own in which, when presented with
information about the recovery and treatment of people with mental illness,
survey respondents responded more favorably when asked if they would
be willing to work closely with or live next to a person with mental illness.
Such v ignettes, she observed, could dramatically alter public perception on
mental illness and improve mental health services and access.
Stephan stated that promising evidence suggests that there are benefits
to in-school mental health services. Around social and emotional learning
and universal mental health promotion, there are improvements in student
social competency and behavioral and emotional functioning. Additionally,
improvements are seen in academic indicators, such as grades, test scores,
attendance, and teacher retention. There is also evidence of cost savings to
schools and communities.
In looking at violence, Stephan remarked that youth are exposed to
violence in a variety of settings, including school and home, and more
than 60 percent of them report lifetime exposure to traumatic events
(McLaughlin et al., 2013). One in five youth report being physically as-
saulted by peers; a similar proportion report emotional violence by peers,
as well. The school is a common setting for physical intimidation, assault,
and emotional violence, with more than half of all incidents occurring in
school (Turner et al., 2011). During the 2009 school year, 1 in 10 schools
reported a serious violent incident that required the presence of criminal
law enforcement (Robers et al., 2012). Moreover, 16 percent of students
3 This section summarizes information presented by Dévora Kestel, Pan American Health
Organization.
Discussion
Several themes raised by the speakers were further explored by audience
participants following the presentations. In the absence of a robust com-
munity mental health system, and with limited capacity of mental health
professionals, prisons in the United States have served the role of mental
health care providers—a situation that participants felt was not necessarily
one to emulate in other parts of the world. In particular, as children leave
school for various mental health or violence issues, many of them end up in
prison. Michael Phillips of the Shanghai Mental Health Center noted that
in China the transition of mental health care from involuntary commitment
to voluntary has resulted in more people with mental illness ending up in
prison. He commented that training community health w orkers in mental
health could be one way to address the personnel shortage, while others
noted that integrating mental health and general health could address issues
in both domains.
BOX 5-1
Issues Across the Criminal Justice System
The Courts: Civil rights, societal values, equal protection, competency to pro-
ceed, competency to be a witness, competency to accept a plea, appropriate
punishment, protection of public, risk of recidivism, capacity to serve probation,
access to alternatives, cost, and training.
He further observed that police officers think that their work is mis-
understood by researchers and other social service providers. For example,
about 70 percent of police work does not involve law enforcement, and
much of a police officer’s interaction with the public is not recorded. Many
police officers would like to do more to serve people with mental illness,
but barriers of time and resources stand in the way (Cooper et al., 2004).
The justice system in the United States is one of the most fragmented
professional systems in the country, Greenberg remarked. There are about
18,000 state and local law enforcement agencies in the United States; how-
ever, if a department has fewer than 10 officers, it is not required to report
within the federal system. Across the world, police agencies are sometimes
military or quasi-military operations or they fall under the purview of
a national police system. Similarly, the court system is fragmented, and
sound data do not exist on numbers because many courts are temporary.
Incarceration is not well integrated either: prisons, jails, and lockups are
distinct places and fall under different jurisdictions, police departments,
sheriff departments, departments of corrections, county jails, and state and
federal penitentiaries.
In addition to the fragmentation, Greenberg observed that the conversa-
tion around deinstitutionalization and community-based services occurred
before the majority of today’s police departments were in service, and
there is still miscommunication around mental health issues. Additionally,
policing is primarily a reactive profession—that is, police officers respond
to calls and attempt to resolve them on the spot. Greenberg stated that, on
BOX 5-2
What Police Patrol Officers Want
Mental Health Practitioners to Know
Sample of comments:
• I function alone.
• I am the only officer on duty this shift.
• My immediate focus is on safety. I need to know about the crisis at hand,
who is hurt or in danger, the environment, and access to weapons. Talk
about mental illness, what happened in the past, and everything else will
occur later.
• The closest hospital to my beat is 25 miles away.
• People with mental illness and their families don’t know what to expect
when I arrive.
• At 3:30 a.m. on a Sunday, I don’t have access to mental health workers or
county attorneys . . . if I get hold of them they don’t want to come out.
• The family calls and wants help . . . then when I explain what I can or have
to do, they turn on me. It’s tough. I understand. They want intervention
without consequence. They want help I’m not able to give.
• You need to know how we approach potentially life-threatening situations.
• We’re criticized for using force. We follow a use-of-force continuum. We
can’t take chances. It is better to be judged by 12 than carried by 6.
misunderstanding. Police were better trained, but it was not until there was
outreach in the deaf community that the shootings were reduced.
Greenberg closed by noting that rather than focusing on top-level goals,
such as policy, procedure, or funding, interventions primarily should focus
on point of entry. He noted two specific groups that could benefit from
interventions focused on the point of entry: emergency dispatchers, who
gather and disseminate information and thereby create the foundation for
potential encounters, and the police, who interact with the affected family
or the environment in a direct way. Better training and support for emer-
gency dispatchers and police is particularly crucial.
Therapeutic Jurisprudence7
Therapeutic jurisprudence is a healing approach to the law, with the
intention of “rehabilitation, compliance with the law, and helping victims
to cope with the impact of crime on their lives,” David Wexler stated. He
explained that therapeutic jurisprudence is best known in special problem-
solving or solution-focused courts, such as drug treatment court, mental
health court, and domestic violence court. The law has an effect on well-
being. This effect has been largely ignored in administration of the law, but
Wexler argued that it should be studied and factored into law reform. The
Hague Institute for Innovation of Law is exploring options to maintain
therapeutic jurisprudence, particularly in criminal law and juvenile justice.
It is an interdisciplinary approach that involves psychology, criminology,
and social work, as well as working with offenders and victims.
While it has mostly been used in specialized courts, the therapeutic
jurisprudence approach has broader implications for those who fail to meet
the qualifications of those special courts. Attempts to expand the special
courts often encounter budget obstacles, so a second option is to apply the
skills and insights elsewhere in the criminal justice system, in which people
with mental illness or drug or alcohol problems might find themselves.
Wexler suggested that there are several elements that could be incorpo-
rated with the wider system, such as early diversion, bail hearings, plea
negotiations, judicial settlement conferences, non-incarcerative sentences,
and conditional release. Wexler noted that his project also examines police
interrogation and newer, more humanistic methods of investigative inter-
viewing, even before a person’s entry into the court system. In this way, he
remarked, the project looks at both the law in action and the roles of legal
actors, including judges, lawyers, and therapists.
In exploring how such elements might be included in criminal justice,
Wexler noted that he and his colleagues examined which practices are in
place already, which are not, why they are not, and how they could be
of Therapeutic Jurisprudence.
REFERENCES
Beck, A. J., and L. M. Maruschak. 2001. Mental health treatment in state prisons, 2000.
Bureau of Justice Statistics Special Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). 2008. Youth risk behavior survey data.
www.cdc.gov/yrbs (accessed November 30, 2017).
Cooper, V. G., A. M. Mclearen, and P. A. Zapf. 2004. Dispositional decisions with the men-
tally ill: Police perceptions and characteristics. Police Quarterly 7(3):295–310.
Kohn, R., S. Saxena, I. Levav, and B. Saraceno. 2004. The treatment gap in mental health care.
Bulletin of the World Health Organization 82(11):858–866.
McKay, M. M., C. J. Lynn, and W. M. Bannon. 2005. Understanding inner city child mental
health need and trauma exposure: Implications for preparing urban service providers.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 75(2):201–210.
McLaughlin, K. A., K. C. Koenen, E. D. Hill, M. Petukhova, N. A. Sampson, A. M. Zaslavsky,
and R. C. Kessler. 2013. Trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder in a na-
tional sample of adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 52(8):815–830.
Metzner, J. L. 2002. Class action litigation in correctional psychiatry. Journal of the American
Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 30(1):19–29.
Milam, A. J., C. D. M. Furr-Holden, and P. J. Leaf. 2010. Perceived school and neighborhood
safety, neighborhood violence, and academic achievement in urban school children.
Urban Review 42(5):458–467.
Robers, S., J. Kemp, J. Truman, and T. D. Snyder. 2012. Indicators of school crime and safety:
2011. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of
Education and Bureau of Justice Statistics, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department
of Justice. http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2012/2012002.pdf (accessed November 30, 2017).
Turner, H. A., D. Finkelhor, S. L. Hamby, A. Shattuck, and R. K. Ormrod. 2011. Specifying
type and location of peer victimization in a national sample of children and youth. Jour-
nal of Youth and Adolescence 40(8):1052–1067.
T
hroughout the workshop, participants shared a myriad of experi-
ences, evidence, and practice in multiple domains related to mental
health and violence prevention. In the last panel of the workshop,
speakers and participants communicated knowledge and best practices on
inexpensive and more nimble program evaluation. They also discussed
final thoughts and raised additional questions on advancing the science
and practice.
University.
67
first year and the first three quarters of the second year always maintained
a control group (i.e., un-enrolled) for comparison. One of the advantages
of this methodology is that, when the whole community has agreed to the
intervention, there is still a process for incorporating everyone. Because
all the schools received the intervention, there was no delay or associated
costs. At the same time, there was a benefit to the schools that received
the intervention first because often school districts are eager to imple-
ment a program sooner rather than later. However, those who receive the
intervention later have the advantage of more efficient implementation.
Finally, an important benefit of roll-out design is that evaluation is built
into implementation—meaning that cost issues around evaluation may be
averted and that accountability is naturally integrated with the process.
Once a program has been deemed effective, the final step in evalua-
tion is making a program work. Brown noted that there are two areas for
this step. On the research side, implementation science gathers generalized
knowledge of program design; while on the practice side, quality improve-
ment is an ongoing local evaluation. He shared the RE-AIM perspective for
ensuring program success:
Brown further noted that these measurable elements determine whether the
implementation will be successful.
In the discussion following the presentation, participants queried
Brown regarding issues of sustainability, such as funding evaluations and
ensuring continuity and consistency as personnel change at program sites.
Participants discussed requiring a measure of evaluation in grant proposals
and the importance of building partnerships with the entire community.
They also considered alternate methods of gathering data for evaluation,
such as practice-based evidence and ongoing data collection in mobile
health (mHealth) programs, that could integrate with traditional methods.
School of Education.
fear could assist in developing more trust with professionals and countering
misinformation in the media and the general public.
He closed by asserting that cross-disciplinary collaboration for policy,
coupled with mandates for providing better education and training in all
fields, could build an institutional culture of understanding across all dis-
ciplines. In parallel to this work in professional fields, people with mental
illness and their families, he argued, should be better equipped to engage
the system at any point of contact.
San Diego.
5 This section summarizes information presented by Dévora Kestel, Pan American Health
Organization.
6 This section summarizes information presented by James Mercy, Centers for Disease
Discussion
To close the workshop, participants shared their perspectives on the
discussions over the 2 days and presented their thoughts on research,
program implementation, and policy. Important questions were raised on:
Mental Health.
REFERENCES
Kessler, R. C., K. A. McLaughlin, J. G. Green, M. J. Gruber, N. A. Sampson, A. M. Zaslavsky,
S. Aguilar-Gaxiola, A. O. Alhamzawi, J. Alonso, M. Angermeyer, C. Benjet, E. Bromet,
S. Chatterji, G. de Girolamo, K. Demyttenaere, J. Fayyad, S. Florescu, G. Gal, O. Gureje,
J. M. Haro, C. Hu, E. B. Üstün, S. Vassilev, M. C. Viana, and D. R. Williams. 2010.
Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health
Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry 197(5):378–385.
NRC and IOM (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine). 2009. Preventing men-
tal, emotional, and behavioral disorders among young people: Progress and possibilities.
Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Appendix A
CONTENTS
75
Figures
A-1 Mental health expenditures, 101
A-2 The impact of being bullied on functioning in adulthood, 121
A-3 Adjusted mean young adult CRP levels (mg/L) based on childhood/
adolescent bullying status, 123
Tables
A-1 Mental Health Professionals in LAC, 102
A-2 Number of Users Attending Mental Health Facilities, 103
APPENDIX A 77
A.1
Correspondence to:
R. James R. Blair, PhD
9000 Rockville Pike
Bldg. 15k, Room 205, MSC 2670
Bethesda, MD 20892
[email protected]
Introduction:
Distinguishing Forms of Violence
The goal of this brief review is to consider neurocognitive mechanisms
that, when dysfunctional, have been suggested to increase the risk for
violence. However, before this can be considered it is worth noting that,
from a neuroscience perspective, there appears to be more than one form
of violence (Blair, 2001). Specifically, a distinction should be drawn be-
tween reactive (affective/defensive/impulsive) and instrumental (proactive/
planned) aggression (Crick and Dodge, 1996).
Reactive aggression is unplanned and can be characterized as impul-
sive. It is explosive, involves the active confrontation of the victim and
is typically accompanied by negative affect (anger, sadness, frustration,
and irritation). One notable feature distinguishing reactive aggression in
humans from that studied in animals is that in humans, it is often associated
with frustration (Berkowitz, 1993). Frustration occurs when an individual
continues to do an action in the expectation of a reward but does not actu-
ally receive that reward (Berkowitz, 1993).
Instrumental aggression in contrast is planned. It involves the selection
of a behavior (a covert or overt aggressive response) in anticipation of a
positive outcome (e.g., acquisition of territory or goods, improvement of
APPENDIX A 79
APPENDIX A 81
Conclusions
In conclusion, this review outlines three neurocognitive systems
that, when dysfunctional, increase the risk for aggression. These are (1)
the acute threat response implicating the amygdala, hypothalamus, and
periaqueductal gray; (2) empathic responding (instrumental) implicating
the amygdala and, in the context of decision making influenced by em-
pathy for potential victims, ventromedial frontal cortex; and (3) reward–
punishment-based decision making implicating striatum and ventromedial
frontal cortex. Importantly, identifying these systems provides treatment
targets. Interventions can be designed to address the functioning of these
systems and their efficacy, indexed by their impact on the systems them-
selves, as well as downstream consequences of reduced aggression.
References
Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann N Y Acad
Sci, 1191, 42-61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x.
Aharoni, E., Antonenko, O., and Kiehl, K. A. (2011). Disparities in the moral intuitions of
criminal offenders: The role of psychopathy. J Res Pers, 45(3), 322-327. doi: 10.1016/j.
jrp.2011.02.005.
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. Philadelphia, PA:
Temple University Press.
Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated associa-
tions with reinforcements for aggression. Psychological Review, 81, 165-176.
Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the
psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1-29.
Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality dis-
orders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71(6),
727-731.
Blair, R. J. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci,
14(11), 786-799. doi: 10.1038/nrn3577.
Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies.
Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135-145.
Blanchard, R. J., Blanchard, D. C., and Takahashi, L. K. (1977). Attack and defensive
behaviour in the albino rat. Animal Behavior, 25, 197-224.
Blanchard, R. J., Takahashi, L. K., and Blanchard, D. C. (1977). The development of intruder
attack in colonies of laboratory rats. Animal Learning and Behavior, 5(4), 365-369.
Coccaro, E. F., McCloskey, M. S., Fitzgerald, D. A., and Phan, K. L. (2007). Amygdala and
orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biologi-
cal Psychiatry, 62(2), 168-178.
Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., and Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy
in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 64, 783-790.
Crick, N. R., and Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social information-processing mechanisms in reactive
and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67(3), 993-1002.
Crowley, T. J., Dalwani, M. S., Mikulich-Gilbertson, S. K., Du, Y. P., Lejuez, C. W., Raymond,
K. M., and Banich, M. T. (2010). Risky decisions and their consequences: Neural process-
ing by boys with Antisocial Substance Disorder. PLoS ONE, 5(9), e12835. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0012835.
de Wied, M., van Boxtel, A., Matthys, W., and Meeus, W. (2012). Verbal, facial and autonomic
responses to empathy-eliciting film clips by disruptive male adolescents with high versus
low callous-unemotional traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol, 40(2), 211-223. doi: 10.1007/
s10802-011-9557-8.
Finger, E. C., Marsh, A. A., Mitchell, D. G. V., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Budhani, S., . . . Blair,
R. J. R. (2008). Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psy-
chopathic traits during reversal learning. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(5), 586-594.
Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D. M., Farrell, J. M., and Kimonis, E. R. (2005).
Callous-unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems
and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 33(4), 471-487.
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., and Schug, R. A. (2008). The neural correlates of moral decision-
making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5-6.
Gregg, T. R., and Siegel, A. (2001). Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggres-
sion in cats: Implications for human aggression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biological
Psychiatry, 25(1), 91-140.
Jeon, D., and Shin, H. S. (2011). A mouse model for observational fear learning and the empa-
thetic response. Curr Protoc Neurosci, Chapter 8, Unit 8 27. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.
ns0827s57.
Lee, T. M. C., Chan, S. C., and Raine, A. (2008). Strong limbic and weak frontal activation to
aggressive stimuli in spouse abusers. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(7), 655-656.
Marsh, A. A., Finger, E. C., Mitchell, D. G. V., Reid, M. E., Sims, C., Kosson, D. S., . . .
Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and
adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 712-720.
McCrory, E. J., De Brito, S. A., Sebastian, C. L., Mechelli, A., Bird, G., Kelly, P. A., and V
iding,
E. (2011). Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Curr
Biol, 21(23), R947-948. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.015.
Miller, P. A., and Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and external-
izing/ antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324-344.
New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A., Newmark, R. E., Zhang, J., Triebwasser, J., Meyerson, D., Laza-
rus, S., Trisdorfer, R., Goldstein, K. E., Goodman, M., Koenigsberg, H. W., Flory, J. D.,
Siever, L. J., and Buchsbaum, M. S. (2009). Laboratory induced aggression: A positron
emission tomography study of aggressive individuals with borderline personality disorder.
Biological Psychiatry, 66(12), 1107-1114.
APPENDIX A 83
Nucci, L. P., and Nucci, M. (1982). Children’s social interactions in the context of moral and
conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53, 403-412.
O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Beyond simple reinforcement learning: The computational neuro
biology of reward-learning and valuation. Eur J Neurosci, 35(7), 987-990. doi:
10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08074.x.
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Perry, D. G., and Perry, L. C. (1974). Denial of suffering in the victim as a stimulus to violence
in aggressive boys. Child Development, 45, 55-62.
Plichta, M. M., Vasic, N., Wolf, R. C., Lesch, K. P., Brummer, D., Jacob, C., . . . Gron,
G. (2009). Neural hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness during immediate
and delayed reward processing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol
Psychiatry, 65(1), 7-14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.07.008.
Scheres, A., Milham, M. P., Knutson, B., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Ventral striatal hypo-
responsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol
Psychiatry, 61(5), 720-724. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.042.
Strohle, A., Stoy, M., Wrase, J., Schwarzer, S., Schlagenhauf, F., Huss, M., . . . Heinz, A. (2008).
Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
order. Neuroimage, 39(3), 966-972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.044.
Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Millner, A., Gilhooly, T., Zevin, J. D., and Casey, B. J. (2011).
Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Dev Sci, 14(2), 190-
204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00971.x.
Viding, E., Sebastian, C. L., Dadds, M. R., Lockwood, P. L., Cecil, C. A., De Brito, S. A., and
McCrory, E. J. (2012). Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with
conduct problems: The role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry, 169(10),
1109-1116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191.
White, S. F., Marsh, A. A., Fowler, K. A., Schechter, J. C., Adalio, C., Pope, K., . . . Blair, R.
J. R. (2012). Reduced amygdala responding in youth with disruptive behavior disorder
and psychopathic traits reflects a reduced emotional response not increased top-down
attention to non-emotional features. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(7), 750-758.
White, S. F., Pope, K., Sinclair, S., Fowler, K. A., Brislin, S. J., Williams, W. C., . . . Blair, R. J.
R. (2013). Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youth with disrup-
tive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. American Journal of Psychiatry.
Yau, W. Y., Zubieta, J. K., Weiland, B. J., Samudra, P. G., Zucker, R. A., and Heitzeg, M. M.
(2012). Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alco-
holics: Relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use. J Neurosci,
32(7), 2544-2551. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1390-11.2012.
A.2
The very title of this conference saddens me, and makes me angry.
Clearly the gun lobby has been effective in changing the narrative from con-
trolling guns to controlling those of us who have been labeled mentally ill.
This narrative is based on false information equating persons with mental
health disorders with increased violence.
Introduction
In my 20s I was diagnosed with schizophrenia and was involuntarily
hospitalized on three occasions. Ironically, I was studying the possible bio-
chemical bases of mental illnesses at the National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) at the time. (That was in the late ’60s. In May of last year, 45 years
later, Dr. Thomas Insel of NIMH stated that NIMH will not use the Diag-
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition [DSM-5]
because its diagnostic categories are not based on biological m arkers.) I
was researching the factors controlling the biosynthesis of dopamine and
serotonin. I so reduced human experiences to chemistry that I became
convinced that we all were merely chemical machines, and that we lacked
meaning or agency. I lost the meaning of human relationships and emo-
tional expression. Being unable to understand communication left me out
of touch. I believe this empty view of life and the loneliness it produced left
me in despair and caused me to depart from everyday reality. I indeed did
fit the DSM definition of schizophrenia. I recovered from schizophrenia by
finding meaning through emotionally connecting with others and myself. I
concluded that we have a self that supersedes all we can write in a formula.
This understanding brought me back to everyday life with people. I decided
I could learn more about this human dimension by becoming a psychiatrist,
APPENDIX A 85
and have practiced in clinical settings for the last 35 years. I also founded
and run the National Empowerment Center, a Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration (SAMHSA)-funded, technical assistance
center dedicated to bringing hope and recovery to the mental health system
and society.
Recovery depends on people with mental illness finding meaningful
relationships and working in the community. The misinformation perpetu-
ated by our media is interfering with recovery. This incorrect coupling of
mental health issues with violence increases prejudice and discrimination
among all members of society. (Advocates are rejecting the use of the term
“stigma” because the term itself has produced increased prejudice.)
I will summarize the evidence that people diagnosed with mental health
conditions are no more violent than matched community members without
such a label. Although there is a lack of association of violence and mental
health conditions, the increased attention on mental health can be an op-
portunity to improve that system. Therefore, I will point out problems in
our mental health system and society that are barriers to recovery. Finally,
I will recommend ways that our mental health system needs to continue
its transformation from a maintenance-based to a recovery-based system.
1. Persons with mental illness are no more likely than the matched
controls in the community [to perform violent acts].
2. Persons with a substance abuse disorder carry a substantial risk of
increased violence.
APPENDIX A 87
in distress and listening to his or her voice, believing in them, and giving
them hope and humility. Peers can greatly enhance these qualities when
their roles as recovery coaches are valued and understood.
Recommendations
1. Broaden the community dialogue on mental health, and ensure
that persons with lived experience of mental health conditions are
included in the planning and participation of these dialogues.
2. NIMH and SAMHSA should promote the training and evaluation
of Open Dialogue (Seikkula, 2006) in the United States, to reach
people where they live, and while they are still connected to their
natural supports. This approach, developed in Finland, is the most
successful approach in the world for helping young people who
have experienced their first psychotic experience to recover a full
life in the community.
3. Hiring peers in valued roles as crisis workers and in peer-run
respites; peers are capable of reaching persons whom non-peers
cannot reach. This is true because when you have experienced delu-
sions and voices, you know how to reach and connect with other
persons going through a similar experience. Peer-run respites are a
good example of the application of this capacity of peers in divert-
ing persons from hospitalization (see the section on crisis respites
at www.power2u.org).
4. Training first responders, peers, and families in Emotional CPR.
This is a preventative public health program, enabling anyone to
help another person through an emotional crisis. eCPR, therefore,
represents the type of primary prevention that would reach a much
greater proportion of persons than present programs that focus on
persons labeled with mental health disorders (see www.emotional-
cpr.org).
References
Appelbaum, P.S., Robbins, P.C., Monahan, J. 2000.Violence and delusions: Data from the
MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. Am. J. Psychiatry 157:566-572.
Consortium for Risk-Based Firearm Policy. 2013. Guns, public health and mental illness:
An evidence-based approach for state policy. John Hopkins University: Baltimore, MD.
Corrigan, P. 2005. On the stigma of mental illness: Implications for research and social
change, fifth edition. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.
Elbogen, E., and Johnson, S. 2009. Intricate link between violence and mental disorder results
from national epidemiological survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives Gen-
eral Psychiatry 66:152-161.
APPENDIX A 89
Fazel, S., Gulati, G., Linsell, L., Geddes, J.R., and Grann, M. 2009. Schizophrenia and vio-
lence: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med 6(8): e1000120. doi:10.1371/
journal.pmed.1000120.
Fazel, S., Låndström, N., Hjern, A., Grann, M., and Lichtenstein, P. 2009b. Schizophrenia,
substance abuse, and violent crime. JAMA 301(19):2016-2023.
Fisher, D.B. 2008. Promoting recovery. Learning about mental health practice. Eds. T. Stickley
and T. Basset. John Wiley and Sons: Chichester, U.K. pp. 119-139.
McGinty, E., et al. 2013. Gun policy and serious mental illness: Priorities for future research
and policy. Psychiatric Services, epub ahead of print, doi:10.1176/appi. Ps.201300141.
Monahan, J., Steadman, H.J., Silver, E., et al. 2001. Risk assessment: the MacArthur Study of
Mental Disorder and Violence. Oxford University Press: Oxford, U.K.
New Freedom Commission on Mental Health. 2003. Achieving the promise: Transforming
mental health care in America final report. DHHS Pub. No. SMA-03-3832. U.S. Depart-
ment of Health and Human Services: Rockville, MD.
Seikkula, J., and Trimble, D. 2005. Healing elements of therapeutic conversation: Dialogue as
an embodiment of love. Family Process 44(4):461-475.
Seikkula, J., Aaltonen, K., Alakare, B., Haarakanga, K., Keranen, J., and Lehtinen, K. 2006.
Five-year experience of first-episode nonaffective psychosis in open-dialogue approach:
Treatment principles, follow-up outcomes, and two case studies. Psychotherapy Research
16(2):214-228.
Steadman, H.J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P.S., et al. 2000. A classification tree
approach to the development of actuarial violence risk-assessment tools. Law Humanity
Behavior 24:83-100.
Steadman, H.J., Mulvy, E.P., Monahan, J., et al. 1998. Violence by people discharged from
acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 55:393-404.
Stuart, H. 2003. Violence and mental illness: An overview. World Psychiatric Assoc 2:121-124.
Swanson, J. et al. 2013. “Preventing gun violence involving people with serious mental ill-
ness.” In Reducing gun violence in America: Informing policy with evidence and analysis.
Webster, D. and Vernick, J. eds. Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD. pp.
33-51.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 1999. Mental health: A report of the Surgeon
General. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, National Institutes
of Health, National Institute of Mental Health: Rockville, MD.
Van Dorn, R., Volavka, J., and Johnson, N. 2012. Mental disorder and violence: Is there a
relationship beyond substance abuse? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology
47:487-503.
Weigel, D. 2013. CPAC Diary: Wayne LaPierre’s “Mental Health” Chutzpah. Slate Magazine,
March 15.
A.3
Despite the long-term call by criminal justice and mental health profes-
sionals, advocates, and political officials to work together more effectively
in providing services to people with mental illness, progress in many juris-
dictions (cities, counties, towns, states, tribal land) and nations has been
slow. This paper presents and expands on a portion of the information
presented during the workshop titled Mental Health and Violence: Oppor
tunities for Prevention and Early Intervention. It offers information on
the interface between the criminal justice and mental health communities,
addresses some of the reasons for measured progress, and suggests ways in
which the two professions might advance success. While this paper briefly
addresses public safety, including corrections, it focuses primarily on inter
action among the police, mental health providers, and people who have
mental illness and their families, friends, and other advocates.
For decades, mental health professionals, advocacy organizations, fam-
ily members, political leaders, and others have sought to have the criminal
justice system or criminal justice community (generally perceived as consist-
ing of the police, courts, and corrections) end the unnecessary “criminaliza-
tion” of mental illness. This continues to be a priority.
Generally, an arrest is defined as the taking of a person into custody by
a legal authority, usually in response to a criminal charge. The criminaliza-
tion hypothesis is based on the assumption that police inappropriately use
arrest to resolve encounters with people who have mental illness (Engel and
Silver, 2001). Criminalization that can affect the future of a person with
mental illness goes beyond traditional arrest and may include criminal cita-
tions (usually issued for minor offenses that do not require taking a person
into custody) and inclusion of names in criminal incident reports.
Terms such as “criminal justice system” and “criminal justice com-
munity” suggest that there is a common foundation of standards, policies,
practices, and other linkages across the profession. In fact, criminal justice
in the United States and in many other nations is a highly fragmented,
parochial, and compartmentalized and, at times, is a competitive collection
of local, state, tribal, and federal agencies. There is no central organiza-
tion, professional association, or other body with the authority to mandate
policy, practice, or change. As of 2011, there were approximately 15,000
local police departments in the United States. The majority of these agencies
APPENDIX A 91
APPENDIX A 93
Officers assigned to CITs receive training that surpasses the norm for
their peers. A survey of 33 state law enforcement certification agencies
(Police Officer Standards and Training Commissions [POSTs]), which exist
in almost every state in the United States, showed that the average train-
ing for police officers on “mental illness” was 9.1 hours. The majority of
training courses ranged from 2 to 4 hours, with the shortest course being
50 minutes. These courses included content on awareness and process
(policies and procedures for arrest, safety, and commitment to a hospital,
shelter, or other care facility). Some of the courses described by the states
incorporated mental illness with training on all “special populations,” with
no information on time allotted specifically to mental illness. Two states
reported that they have no requirement for training on mental illness. Little
research is available on training provided to specialized police agencies,
such as school police, campus police and security personnel, transporta-
tion police, and tribal police. The inconsistency in and minimal amount of
training provided to police on service to people who have mental illness is
a global issue (Psarra et al., 2008).
Calls are made to the police by people with mental illness who have
been victimized, as well as by spouses, other family members, neighbors,
mental health professionals, and people who simply observe behavior or
an incident but have no relationship to the person with mental illness. The
importance of the initial point of contact—often a police or other govern-
ment call taker/dispatcher—in obtaining critical information and relaying
it to the responding police patrol officer cannot be overstated. Despite
improved police call-taking protocols, information provided to the police
call taker by the victim or observer (witness) is too often brief, panicked,
incomplete, and inaccurate. They may only report the immediate need,
threat, or danger and fail to mention that a person who has mental illness
is involved. As such, the initial information a police officer receives may
make no reference to mental illness or contain any details about risk, ex-
isting injury or illness, medication use, illicit substance abuse, presence of
weapons, or precise location. Officers need, but often lack, information on
the individual’s medical or criminal history, cause of the crisis or hostility,
prior suicide or self-injury attempts, and attending physicians, among other
information (James, 1990). Information provided to a responding police
officer may be described as and limited to the following:
• Man injured
• Woman acting out
• Doctor has trouble with patient
• Unknown trouble
• Suspicious circumstance
• Assault
APPENDIX A 95
• Threat of assault
• Threat of suicide
• Parent cannot control child
• Disorderly conduct
• Assist with a commitment
Officer safety and the safety of others are paramount when police
officers receive a call for service or personally observe an unusual behav-
ior. When information about mental illness is conveyed, no matter how
detailed, police officers make assumptions about the potential for danger
(Watson et al., 2004).
APPENDIX A 97
References
Baltic, S. E. (Ed.). (2011). Crime in the United States 2011. Bernan Press.
Borum, R. (2000). Improving high-risk encounters between people with mental illness and the
police. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28(3), 332-337.
Compton, M. T., Bahora, M., Watson, A. C., and Oliva, J. R. (2008). A comprehensive review
of extant research on Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) programs. Journal of the American
Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 36(1), 47-55.
Compton, M. T., Neubert, B. N. D., Broussard, B., McGriff, J. A., Morgan, R., and Oliva, J.
R. (2009). Use of force preferences and perceived effectiveness of actions among Crisis
Intervention Team (CIT) police officers and non-CIT officers in an escalating psychiatric
crisis involving a subject with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbp146.
Cordner, G. (2006). People with mental illness. Problem-oriented guides for police, Problem-
Specific Guides Series (40).
Cunningham, P., McKenzie, K., and Taylor, E. F. (2006). The struggle to provide community-
based care to low-income people with serious mental illnesses. Health Affairs, 25(3),
694-705.
Deane, M. W., Steadman, H. J., Borum, R., Veysey, B. M., and Morrissey. J. P. (1999). Emerg-
ing partnerships between mental health and law enforcement. Psychiatric Services, 50(1),
99-101.
Division of Public Safety Leadership. (2013). Roundtable on Police response to People with
Mental Illness. Johns Hopkins University, School of Education, Division of Public Safety
Leadership, March 19, 2013, Columbia, Maryland.
Engel, R. S., and Silver, E. (2001). Policing mentally disordered suspects: A reexamination of
the criminalization hypothesis. Criminology, 39(2), 225-252.
Fakhoury, W., and Priebe, S. (2002). The process of deinstitutionalization: An international
overview. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 15(2), 187-192.
Frontline. (2005). Deinstitutionalization: A Psychiatric “Titanic.” Retrieved from http://www.
pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/asylums/special/excerpt.html.
Hanafi, S., Bahora, M., Demir, B. N., and Compton, M. T. (2008). Incorporating crisis inter-
vention team (CIT) knowledge and skills into the daily work of police officers: A focus
group study. Community Mental Health Journal, 44(6), 427-432.
James, R. (1990). What do police officers really want from the mental health system? Hospital
and Community Psychiatry, 41(6), 663.
Lamb, H. R., Weinberger, L. E., and DeCuir, W. J. (2002). The police and mental health.
Psychiatric Services, 53(10), 1266-1271.
Lord, V. B., Bjerregaard, B., Blevins, K. R., and Whisman, H. (2011). Factors influencing the
responses of crisis intervention team–certified law enforcement officers. Police Quarterly,
14(4), 388-406.
Lurigio, A. J., and Swartz, J. A. (2000). Changing the contours of the criminal justice system
to meet the needs of persons with serious mental illness. Criminal Justice, 3, 45-108.
Nagel, T. (2005). The problem of global justice. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 33(2), 113-147.
Novak, K. J., and Engel, R. S. (2005). Disentangling the influence of suspects’ demeanor and
mental disorder on arrest. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and
Management, 28(3), 493-512.
Peternelj-Taylor, C. (2008). Criminalization of the mentally ill. Journal of Forensic Nursing,
4(4), 185-187.
Psarra, V., Sestrini, M., Santa, Z., Petsas, D., Gerontas, A., Garnetas, C., and Kontis, K.
(2008). Greek police officers’ attitudes towards the mentally ill. International Journal of
Law and Psychiatry, 31(1), 77-85.
Teplin, L. A. (2000). Keeping the peace: Police discretion and mentally ill persons. National
Institute of Justice Journal, 244, 8-15.
Teplin, L. A., McClelland, G. M., Abram, K. M., and Weiner, D. A. (2005). Crime victimization
in adults with severe mental illness: Comparison with the National Crime Victimization
Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(8), 911-921.
Tucker, A. S., Van Hasselt, V. B., and Russell, S. A. (2008). Law enforcement response to the
mentally ill: An evaluative review. Brief Treatment and Crisis Intervention, 8(3), 236.
Vermette, H. S., Pinals, D. A., and Appelbaum, P. S. (2005). Mental health training for law
enforcement professionals. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law
Online, 33(1), 42-46.
Watson, A. C., Corrigan, P. W., and Ottati, V. (2004). Police officers’ attitudes toward and
decisions about persons with mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 55(1), 49-53.
Wells, W., and Schafer, J. A. (2006). Officer perceptions of police responses to persons with a
mental illness. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management,
29(4), 578-601.
A.4
APPENDIX A 99
Although the general data available from LAC present a situation that
could be viewed as dismal, it is important to highlight that there are many
good examples in the region worth replicating. There are countries that
have been reforming—a continuously ongoing process—their mental health
system for decades; there are also regions or towns within countries that
use their autonomy to move the mental health agenda forward, even when
the national situation is not as advanced.
This brief article intends to highlight some of the most salient features
of mental health systems in LAC.
Resources Availability
One partial explanation for this significant gap is the inadequacy of
funds available to develop appropriate services for those suffering from
mental and neurological disorders. The world median of the health budget
allocated to mental health is 2.82 percent (6).
Mental health expenditures at the regional level are not that different
from global levels; the median health budget allocated to mental health is
2.3 percent, with differences linked to sub-regional characteristics.
In the context of the existing limited budget environment, it is very
important to understand how those resources are used. The principal part
of that budget goes to outdated, custodial style, psychiatric hospitals, with
very limited funds made available for the development of community-
based mental health services. Specifically, in the English-speaking Caribbean
countries, the mental health budget is 3.5 percent of the health budget, and
84 percent of that budget goes to mental hospitals. In Central America, the
mental health budget is even lower, at 1.5 percent of the health budget, with
75 percent of it spent in mental hospitals. In South America, the budget
represents 2 percent of the health budget, with 66 percent going to mental
hospitals (7).
Figure A-1 illustrates the median percentage of the government health
budget allocated to mental health and to psychiatric hospitals, by subregion
and total.
* A few islands with a small population and a relatively high number of general nurses (all
involved with mental health patients) create this high number.
SOURCE: Presented by Dévora Kestel on October 30, 2015.
APPENDIX A 103
APPENDIX A 105
Final Considerations
Although countries are moving toward the development of community-
based mental health services that are decentralized and closer to people’s
realities, there is still much to do to have the region ready to answer to
situations related to violence or other specific needs, such as the appropriate
response to populations affected by disasters (natural or man-made) and
the needs of vulnerable groups.
Countries in the region will be ready to answer to these situations
when an appropriate range of mental health services, from promotion and
prevention to rehabilitation and recovery, based in the community will
be available and when mental health will fully be integrated with general
health services.
When discussing violence and mental health, mental health profes-
sionals should be aware that most LACs’ mental health systems create and
direct violence toward individuals with mental disorders and their families.
Until changes occur to their mental health systems, this violence will con-
tinue to neglect to provide patients with the attention and services they need
and will maintain the idea that the mentally ill are violent.
Integrating mental health with general health services is a good strategy
to ensure that the health system as a whole will offer adequate care to the
people who need it.
References
1. Murray, C. J. L. et al. Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries
in 21 regions, 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study.
2010. Lancet, 2012; 380: 2197–2223.
2. Ferrari, A. J., Charlson, F. J., Norman, R. E., Patten, S. B., Freedman G., et al. 2013.
Burden of depressive disorders by country, sex, age and year: Findings from the Global
Burden of Disease Study 2010. PLoS Med 10(11): e1001547. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pmed.1001547.
3. Kohn, R., Saxena, S., Levav, I., and Saraceno, B. 2004. The treatment gap in mental
health care. Bull World Health Organ 82: 858-66 pmid: 15640922.
4. World Health Organization. World Mental Health Survey Consortium. 2004. Preva-
lence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health
Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAVA, http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
pubmed/15173149.
5. Kohn, R. 2013. Treatment gap in the Americas, http://www.paho.org/hq/index.
php?option=com_contentandview=articleandid=9408andItemid=99999.
6. World Health Organization. 2011. Mental Health ATLAS 2011, http://www.who.int/
mental_health/publications/mental_health_atlas_2011/en.
7. Pan American Health Organization. 2013. WHO-AIMS: Report on mental health
systems in Latin America and the Caribbean, http://www.paho.org/hq/index.
php?option=com_contentandview=articleandid=935andItemid=1106andlang=enandlimi
tstart=7.
8. World Health Organization. Comprehensive mental health action plan 2013–2020,
http://www.who.int/mental_health/publications/action_plan/en.
A.5
APPENDIX A 107
The relationship between alcohol and IPV was comparable across clinical
and community samples as well. Not included in moderation a nalyses, the
small-to-medium relationship between heavy alcohol use and IPV has been
reported within incarcerated (e.g., Logan et al., 2001; White et al., 2001),
emergency department ( Lipsky et al., 2005), and military (Pan et al., 1994)
samples, and nascent literature indicates an association among gay and
lesbian samples, as well (Klostermann et al., 2011). Furthermore, research
indicates that the link between heavy alcohol use and IPV is a cross-cultural
phenomenon extending across ethnic groups (Caetano et al., 2001) and
beyond Western nations with varying alcohol use and violence norms,
including countries in South America (Kishor and Johnson, 2004), Asia
(Rao, 1997), and Africa (Yigzaw et al., 2005). Most recently, problematic
male alcohol use was determined to increase the odds of male-to-female
IPV perpetration in 12 of 14 regions (N = 24,097 across 10 countries) that
participated in the World Health Organization Multi-Country Study on
Women’s Health and Domestic Violence population-based survey study
between the years of 2000 and 2003 (Abramsky et al., 2011). Thus, the
existing literature provides consistent evidence for an association between
heavy alcohol use and IPV that can be observed widely.
There have been fewer longitudinal investigations addressing the asso
ciation between heavy alcohol use and IPV. However, the available evi-
dence suggests that heavy alcohol use is longitudinally predictive of IPV
perpetration over short follow-up periods, but the evidence is less sup-
portive over long follow-up periods. Although not a longitudinal study,
Leonard et al. (1985) found that alcohol disorders within the past 3 years
were predictive of husband IPV, while alcohol disorders that occurred
before the last 3 years were not. This observation is consistent with longi-
tudinal research involving premarital (Heyman et al., 1995) and newlywed
(Quigley and L eonard, 1999) couples in which alcohol problems were
predictive of IPV at the earliest, but not subsequent, follow-ups. L eonard
and Senchack (1996) reported that prospective reports of premarital
problematic alcohol use were predictive of physical IPV perpetration
among husbands during the subsequent first year of marriage, even after
controlling for premarital IPV. Similarly, Keller and colleagues (2009) re-
ported that husband, but not wife, alcohol problems predicted increased
physical IPV at a 2-year follow-up. The composite longitudinal evidence
indicates that problematic alcohol use may be longitudinally associated
with subsequent IPV and that this relationship may attenuate over time,
possibly in response to shifting patterns of alcohol use as well as dynamic
dyadic coping and adjustment strategies.
These studies demonstrate a general association between alcohol and
IPV, but lack the temporal element necessary to establish causation. This
proximal relationship can be best established through event-based as well as
APPENDIX A 109
APPENDIX A 111
Treatment
It is important to recognize that alcohol is neither a necessary nor a
sufficient cause of intimate partner violence but instead, it contributes, in
concert with other factors, to an increase in the occurrence and severity of
such violence. Hence, in some groups, alcohol may have a minimal impact
on IPV, while in other groups, its impact may be quite substantial. This is
most apparent when we examine treatment populations. These popula-
tions offer a unique opportunity to observe both the association between
problematic alcohol use and IPV as well as mutual changes in these condi-
tions over time. We see strikingly high rates of comorbidity regardless of
the identified treatment sample. Alcohol problems are detected in upwards
of 50 percent of mandated and voluntary male IPV treatment seekers (e.g.,
Brown et al., 1999; Dalton, 2001; Gondolf, 1999; Stuart, et al., 2003).
High rates of hazardous drinking are also detected among female IPV of-
fenders (Stuart et al., 2003). Rates of IPV among substance abuse treatment
seeking samples also routinely exceed 50 percent in both male and female
samples (e.g., Chase et al., 2003; Gondolf and Foster, 1991; Murphy and
O’Farrell, 1994; Murphy et al., 2001).
Despite the high co-occurrence of problematic alcohol use and IPV, the
behaviors are not routinely assessed unless included among the initial refer-
ral questions (Easton et al., 2007). Little evidence supports the effectiveness
of IPV treatment programs at preventing subsequent acts of violence. Meta-
analytic reviews have revealed small or non-significant effects, suggesting
that treatment may reduce the risk of IPV by as little as 5 percent beyond
legal intervention alone (e.g., Babcock et al., 2004; Feder and Wilson, 2005).
Eckhardt and colleagues (2013) recently collected all case controlled studies
of IPV treatment programs to report that the existing research, though meth-
odologically flawed, contained roughly equivocal support for and against the
effectiveness of IPV interventions. The failure of IPV treatment programs
has been attributed, in part, to poor rates of attendance and high attrition
(Babcock and Steiner, 1999; Gondolf, 2000). Heavy alcohol use may also
impact the poor outcomes of IPV treatment inasmuch as IPV perpetrators
with alcohol problems attend fewer sessions and drop out of treatment at a
greater rate than perpetrators without drinking problems (for a review, see
Daly and Pelowski, 2000; Olver et al., 2011).
Indeed, substance abuse treatment success has been associated with
reductions in IPV. One investigation found that intensive treatment for
alcohol dependence, in the absence of IPV-specific content, resulted in
significant reductions not only in alcohol use but also physical and psy-
chological IPV perpetration at 6- and 12-month follow-ups, according to
male clients and their wives (Stuart et al., 2003). Similar results were found
among a sample of females seeking treatment for alcohol dependence; treat-
ment resulted in reductions in alcohol, physical violence, and psychological
IPV perpetration at 6- and 12-month follow ups (Stuart et al., 2002). In a
larger treatment evaluation study, alcohol treatment resulted in reductions
in the prevalence of IPV from baseline assessment to 1-year follow-up and
a greater increase in IPV among relapsed clients relative to remitted clients
at 2 year follow-up (O’Farrell et al., 2003).
When detected, however, dual alcohol problems and IPV most often
result in assignment to separate treatment programs that fail to coordinate
efforts to minimize client burden, reducing the likelihood of completing
either program (Bennett and Lawson, 1994; Schumacher et al., 2003).
Initial evidence suggests that integrated alcohol and IPV treatments may be
more effective than treatment as usual. Evaluations of behavioral couples
therapy for alcohol and IPV problems have evidenced reductions in alcohol
use as well as the prevalence of IPV among both male and female partners
APPENDIX A 113
Future Directions
Although we have briefly reviewed the literature that has contributed
to our considerable understanding of the effects of acute heavy alcohol
consumption on IPV, there is a dearth of research into the effects of chronic
heavy alcohol use on the risk of perpetrating IPV. The processes by which
chronic heavy alcohol consumption may affect the ability to interpret and se-
lect prosocial responses to incoming social stimuli remain unclear. One model
describes indirect effects, positing that chronic alcohol use increases the risk
of IPV through gradual changes to interpersonal dynamics that r educe rela-
tionship satisfaction, increase relationship stress, and decrease reliance upon
non-violent conflict resolution tactics designed to accommodate a partner or
improve the partnership (Quigley and Leonard, 1999). Alternatively, neuro-
biological evidence reveals that chronic alcohol use is associated with neu-
ronal death and widespread deficits in executive cognitive functioning (e.g.,
Sullivan et al., 2002). Easton and colleagues (2008) extended this research
to report greater cognitive impairment (e.g., attention, concentration, flex-
ibility) among alcohol-dependent men who reported IPV perpetration when
compared to alcohol-dependent men who reported no IPV perpetration. As
previously stated, much of the existing research provides evidence for the
proximal effects of heavy alcohol consumption on IPV, even among individu-
als with chronic alcohol abuse problems. Additional long-term longitudinal
and neuropsychological research beginning in adolescence and extending
through early adulthood is required to further develop our understanding of
the biopsychosocial effects of chronic alcohol use on IPV.
The role of psychopathology, as well as its interaction with alcohol, in
IPV perpetration represents another underdeveloped area of research. Simi-
lar to alcohol, early IPV models conceptualized mental illness as an excuse
References
Abbey, A. (2011). Alcohol’s role in sexual violence perpetration: Theoretical explanations,
existing evidence and future directions. Drug and Alcohol Review, 30, 481-489.
Abramsky, T., Watts, C. H., Garcia-Moreno, C., Devries, K., Kiss, L., Ellsberg, M., Hansen,
H. A., and Heise, L. (2011). What factors are associated with recent intimate partner
violence? Findings from the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic
violence. BMC Public Health, 11, 109-125.
Babcock, J. C., Green, C. E., and Robie, C. (2004). Does batterers’ treatment work? A
meta-analytic review of domestic violence treatment. Clinical Psychology Review, 23,
1023-1053.
Babcock, J. C., and Steiner, R. (1999). The relationship between treatment, incarceration,
and recidivism of battering: A program evaluation of Seattle’s coordinated community
response to domestic violence. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 46-59.
Bennett, L., and Lawson, M. (1994). Barriers to cooperation between domestic-violence and
substance-abuse programs. Families in Society, 75, 277-286.
Brown, T. G., Werk, A., Caplan, T., and Seraganian, P. (1999). Violent substance abusers in
domestic violence treatment. Violence and Victims, 14, 179-190.
Bushman, B. J. (1997). Effects of alcohol on human aggression. Validity of proposed explana-
tions. Recent Developments in Alcoholism, 13, 227-243.
APPENDIX A 115
Caetano, R., Schafer, J., and Cunradi, C. B. (2001). Alcohol-related intimate partner violence
among White, Black, and Hispanic couples in the United States. Alcohol Research and
Health, 25, 58-65.
Campbell, J. C., Webster, D., Koziol-McLain, J., Block, C., Campbell, D., Curry, M. A., ...
and Laughon, K. (2003). Risk factors for femicide in abusive relationships: Results from
a multisite case control study. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 1089-1097.
Chase, K. A., O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2003).
Factors associated with partner violence among female alcoholic patients and their male
partners. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 64, 137-149.
Crane, C. A., Hawes, S. W., Devine, S., and Easton, C. J. (2014). Axis I psychopathology
and the perpetration of intimate partner violence. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70,
238-247.
Dalton, B. (2001). Batterer characteristics and treatment completion. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 16, 1223-1238.
Daly, J. E., and Pelowski, S. (2000). Predictors of dropout among men who batter: A review
of studies with implications for research and practice. Violence and Victims, 15, 137-160.
Desmarais, S. L., Reeves, K. A., Nicholls, T. L., Telford, R. P., and Fiebert, M. S. (2012).
Prevalence of physical violence in intimate relationships, part 2: Rates of male and female
perpetration. Partner Abuse, 3, 170-198.
Dutton, D. G., and Corvo, K. (2007). The Duluth Model: A data-impervious paradigm and a
failed strategy. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12, 658-667.
Easton, C. J., Mandel, D. L., Hunkele, K. A., Nich, C., Rounsaville, B. J., and Carroll,
K. M. (2007). A cognitive behavioral therapy for alcohol-dependent domestic vio-
lence o ffenders: An integrated Substance Abuse–Domestic Violence treatment approach
(SADV). The American Journal on Addictions, 16, 24-31.
Easton, C. J., Sacco, K. A., Neavins, T. M., Wupperman, P., and George, T. P. (2008). Neuro-
cognitive performance among alcohol dependent men with and without physical violence
toward their partners: A preliminary report. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol
Abuse, 34, 29-37.
Eckhardt, C. I. (2007). Effects of alcohol intoxication on anger experience and expression
among partner assaultive men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 61-71.
Eckhardt, C. I., and Crane, C. (2008). Effects of alcohol intoxication and aggressivity on
aggressive verbalizations during anger arousal. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 428-436.
Eckhardt, C. I., Murphy, C. M., Whitaker, D. J., Sprunger, J., Dykstra, R., and Woodard,
K. (2013). The effectiveness of intervention programs for perpetrators and victims of
intimate partner violence. Partner Abuse, 4, 196-231.
Exum, M. L. (2006). Alcohol and aggression: An integration of findings from experimental
studies. Journal of Criminal Justice, 34, 131-145.
Fals-Stewart, W., Leonard, K. E., and Birchler, G. R. (2005). The occurrence of male-to-female
intimate partner violence on days of men’s drinking: The moderating effects of antisocial
personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 239-248.
Feder, L., and Wilson, D. B. (2005). A meta-analytic review of court-mandated batterer
intervention programs: Can courts affect abusers’ behavior? Journal of Experimental
Criminology, 1, 239-262.
Ferrer, V., Bosch, E., Garcia, E., Manassero, M. A., and Gili, M. (2004). Meta-analytic study
of differential characteristics between batterers and non-batterers: The case of psycho
pathology and consumption of alcohol and drugs. Psykhe, 13, 141-156.
Foran, H. M., and O’Leary, K. D. (2008). Alcohol and intimate partner violence: A meta-
analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1222-1234.
Giancola, P. R., Josephs, R. A., Parrott, D. J., and Duke, A. A. (2010). Alcohol myopia
revisited clarifying aggression and other acts of disinhibition through a distorted lens.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 265-278.
Gondolf, E. W. (1999). A comparison of four batterer intervention systems: Do court refer-
ral, program length, and services matter? Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 14, 41-61.
Gondolf, E. W. (2000). How batterer program participants avoid reassault. Violence against
Women, 6, 1204-1222.
Gondolf, E. W., and Foster, R. A. (1991). Wife assault among VA alcohol rehabilitation
patients. Psychiatric Services, 42, 74-79.
Graham, K., Bernards, S., Wilsnack, S. C., and Gmel, G. (2011). Alcohol may not cause
partner violence but it seems to make it worse: A cross national comparison of the
relationship between alcohol and severity of partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 26, 1503-1523.
Haber, J. R., and Jacob, T. (1997). Marital interactions of male versus female alcoholics.
Family Process, 36, 385-402.
Hatters-Friedman, S. H., and Loue, S. (2007). Incidence and prevalence of intimate partner
violence by and against women with severe mental illness. Journal of Women’s Health,
16, 471-480.
Heyman, R. E., O’Leary, K. D., and Jouriles, E. N. (1995). Alcohol and aggressive personal-
ity styles: Potentiators of serious physical aggression against wives? Journal of Family
Psychology, 9, 44.
Jacob, T., and Krahn, G. L. (1988). Marital interactions of alcoholic couples: Comparison
with depressed and nondistressed couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
ogy, 56, 73-79.
Jacob, T., and Leonard, K. E. (1988). Alcoholic-spouse interaction as a function of alcohol-
ism subtype and alcohol consumption interaction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97,
231-237.
Jacob, T., Leonard, K. E., and Randolph Haber, J. (2001). Family interactions of alcoholics as
related to alcoholism type and drinking condition. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Research, 25, 835-843.
Keller, P. S., El-Sheikh, M., Keiley, M., and Liao, P. (2009). Longitudinal relations between
marital aggression and alcohol problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 2-13.
Kishor, S., and Johnson, K. (2004). Profiling domestic violence: A multi-country study.
Calverton, MD: ORC Macro.
Klostermann, K., Kelley, M. L., Milletich, R. J., and Mignone, T. (2011). Alcoholism and
partner aggression among gay and lesbian couples. Aggression and Violent Behavior,
16, 115-119.
Lawrence, E., Orengo-Aguayo, R., Langer, A., and Brock, R. L. (2012). The impact and con-
sequences of partner abuse on partners. Partner Abuse, 3, 406-428.
Leonard, K. E. (2005). Alcohol and intimate partner violence: When can we say that heavy
drinking is a contributing cause of violence? Addiction, 100, 422-425.
Leonard, K. E., Bromet, E. J., Parkinson, D. K., Day, N. L., and Ryan, C. M. (1985). Patterns
of alcohol use and physically aggressive behavior in men. Journal of Studies on Alcohol
and Drugs, 46, 279-282.
Leonard, K. E., and Quigley, B. M. (1999). Drinking and marital aggression in newlyweds:
An event-based analysis of drinking and the occurrence of husband marital aggression.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 60, 537-545.
Leonard, K. E., and Roberts, L. J. (1998). The effects of alcohol on the marital interactions of
aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy, 107, 602-615.
APPENDIX A 117
Leonard, K. E., and Senchak, M. (1996). Prospective prediction of husband marital aggression
within newlywed couples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 369-380.
Lipsey, M. W., Wilson, D. B., Cohen, M. A., and Derzon, J. H. (1997). Is there a causal rela-
tionship between alcohol use and violence? In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments
in alcoholism: Vol. 13. Alcohol and violence – Epidemiology, neurobiology, psychology,
family issues (First ed., pp. 245-282). Springer: US.
Lipsky, S., Caetano, R., Field, C. A., and Larkin, G. L. (2005). Psychosocial and substance-
use risk factors for intimate partner violence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 78, 39-47.
Logan, T. K., Walker, R., Staton, M., and Leukefeld, C. (2001). Substance use and intimate
violence among incarcerated males. Journal of Family Violence, 16, 93-114.
Murphy, C. M., and O’Farrell, T. J. (1994). Factors associated with marital aggression in male
alcoholics. Journal of Family Psychology, 8, 321-335.
Murphy, C. M., O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., and Feehan, M. (2001). Correlates of inti-
mate partner violence among male alcoholic patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 69, 528-540.
Murphy, C. M., Winters, J., O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2005). Alcohol
consumption and intimate partner violence by alcoholic men: Comparing violent and
nonviolent conflicts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 35-42.
Norlander, B., and Eckhardt, C. (2005). Anger, hostility, and male perpetrators of intimate
partner violence: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 119-152.
O’Farrell, T. J., Fals-Stewart, W., Murphy, M., and Murphy, C. M. (2003). Partner violence
before and after individually based alcoholism treatment for male alcoholic patients.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 92-102.
O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Neavins, T. M., and Van Hutton, V. (2000). Verbal aggression
among male alcoholic patients and their wives in the year before and two years after
alcoholism treatment. Journal of Family Violence, 15, 295-310.
O’Farrell, T. J., Murphy, C. M., Stephan, S. H., Fals-Stewart, W., and Murphy, M. (2004).
Partner violence before and after couples-based alcoholism treatment for male alcoholic
patients: The role of treatment involvement and abstinence. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 72, 202-217.
Olver, M. E., Stockdale, K. C., and Wormith, J. S. (2011). A meta-analysis of predictors of
offender treatment attrition and its relationship to recidivism. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 79, 6-21.
Pan, H. S., Neidig, P. H., and O’Leary, K. D. (1994). Predicting mild and severe husband-to-
wife physical aggression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 975-981.
Pence, E., and Paymar, M. (1993). Education groups for men who batter: The Duluth Model.
New York: Springer Publishing Company.
Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (1999). Husband alcohol expectancies, drinking, and
marital-conflict styles as predictors of severe marital violence among newlywed couples.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13, 49-59.
Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (2006). Alcohol expectancies and intoxicated aggression.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 484-496.
Rao, V. (1997). Wife-beating in rural South India: A qualitative and econometric analysis.
Social Science and Medicine, 44, 1169-1180.
Rehm, J., Shield, K. D., Joharchi, N., and Shuper, P. A. (2012). Alcohol consumption and the
intention to engage in unprotected sex: Systematic review and meta-analysis of experi-
mental studies. Addiction, 107, 51-59.
Schumacher, J. A., Fals-Stewart, W., and Leonard, K. E. (2003). Domestic violence treatment
referrals for men seeking alcohol treatment. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 24,
279-283.
Schumacher, J. A., Homish, G. G., Leonard, K. E., Quigley, B. M., and Kearns-Bodkin, J.
N. (2008). Longitudinal moderators of the relationship between excessive drinking and
intimate partner violence in the early years of marriage. Journal of Family Psychology,
22, 894-904.
Sharps, W., Campbell, J., Campbell, D., Gary, F., and Webster, D. P. (2001). The role of alcohol
use in intimate partner femicide. American Journal on Addictions, 10, 122-135.
Steele, C. M., and Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects.
American Psychologist, 45, 921-933.
Stith, S. M., Smith, D. B., Penn, C. E., Ward, D. B., and Tritt, D. (2004). Intimate part-
ner physical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta-analytic review.
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 65-98.
Stuart, G. L., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., and Ramsey, S. E. (2003). Substance abuse and
relationship violence among men court-referred to batterers’ intervention programs.
Substance Abuse, 24, 107-122.
Stuart, G. L., Moore, T. M., Ramsey, S. E., and Kahler, C. W. (2003). Relationship aggres-
sion and substance use among women court-referred to domestic violence intervention
programs. Addictive Behaviors, 28, 1603-1610.
Stuart, G. L., Ramsey, S. E., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., Farrell, L. E., Recupero, P. R., and
Brown, R. A. (2002). Marital violence victimization and perpetration among women
substance abusers: A descriptive study. Violence Against Women, 8, 934-952.
Stuart, G. L., Ramsey, S. E., Moore, T. M., Kahler, C. W., Farrell, L. E., Recupero, P. R., and
Brown, R. A. (2003). Reductions in marital violence following treatment for alcohol
dependence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 1113-1131.
Sullivan, E. V., Fama, R., Rosenbloom, M. J., and Pfefferbaum, A. (2002). A profile of neuro-
psychological deficits in alcoholic women. Neuropsychology, 16, 74-83.
Taylor, S. P., and Leonard, K. E. (1983). Alcohol and human physical aggression. Aggression:
Theoretical and Empirical Reviews, 2, 77-101.
Tedeschi, J. T., and Quigley, B. M. (1996). Limitations of laboratory paradigms for studying
aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 1, 163-177.
Testa, M., Crane, C. A., Quigley, B. M., Levitt, A., and Leonard, K. E. (2014). Effects of
administered alcohol on intimate partner interactions in a conflict resolution paradigm.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75, 249-258.
Testa, M., and Derrick, J. L. (2014). A daily process examination of the temporal association
between alcohol use and verbal and physical aggression in community couples. Psychol-
ogy of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 127-138.
Testa, M., Quigley, B. M., and Leonard, K. E. (2003). Does alcohol make a difference?
Within-participants comparison of incidents of partner violence. Journal of Interpersonal
Violence, 18, 735-743.
Thompson, M. P., and Kingree, J. B. (2006). The roles of victim and perpetrator alcohol use
in intimate partner violence outcomes. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 163-177.
Tjaden, P., and Thoennes, N. (2000). Prevalence and consequences of male-to-female and
female-to-male intimate partner violence as measured by the National Violence Against
Women Survey. Violence Against Women, 6, 142-161.
Van Dorn, R., Volavka, J., and Johnson, N. (2012). Mental disorder and violence: Is there a
relationship beyond substance use? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47,
487-503.
White, R. J., Ackerman, R. J., and Caraveo, L. E. (2001). Self-identified alcohol abusers in
a low-security federal prison: Characteristics and treatment implications. International
Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 45, 214-227.
Yigzaw, T., Yibric, A., and Kebede, Y. (2005). Domestic violence around Gondar in northwest
Ethiopia. Ethiopian Journal of Health Development, 18, 133-139.
APPENDIX A 119
A.6
University of Warwick,
Department of Psychology (Lifespan Health and Wellbeing Group) and
Division of Mental Health and Well-being (Warwick Medical School),
Coventry, UK
Bullying
Bullying is the systematic abuse of power and is defined as aggres-
sive behavior or intentional harm doing among peers that is carried out
repeatedly, and involves an imbalance of power, either actual or perceived,
between the victim and the bully.1 Bullying can take the form of direct bul-
lying, which includes physical and verbal acts of aggression such as hitting,
stealing, name calling, or indirect bullying, which is characterised by social
exclusion and rumor spreading.2,3,4 Children can be involved in bullying
as victims and as bullies, but also as bully/victims, a subgroup of victims
who display bullying behavior.5,6 Recently there has been much interest
in cyberbullying, which can be broadly defined as any bullying which is
performed via electronic means, such as mobile phones or the Internet. Ap-
proximately 50 percent of children report having been bullied at some point
in their lives, and 10 to 14 percent experience chronic bullying lasting for
more than 6 months.7,8 Between 2 and 5 percent are bullies, and a similar
number are bully/victims in childhood/adolescence.9 Rates of cyberbullying
are substantially lower, around 4.5 percent for victims and 2.8 percent for
perpetrators (bullies; bully/victims), with up to 90 percent also traditionally
(face to face) bullied.10 Being bullied by peers is the most frequent form of
abuse encountered by children, much higher than abuse by parents or other
adult perpetrators.11
APPENDIX A 121
Victimization
being previously bullied at school age still show an increased risk for their
health, self-worth, and quality of life years later.24 Fourthly, where victims
and bully/victims have been considered separately, bully/victims seem to
show the poorest outcome ranging from mental health, to economic adap-
tation, social relationships to early parenthood.9,27,32,35 Fifthly, studies that
did distinguish between bullies and bully/victims found no adverse effects
of being a pure bully on adverse adult outcomes. This is consistent with
a view that bullies are highly sophisticated social manipulators who show
little empathy and are callous.36
Processes
There are a variety of potential routes by which being victimized may
affect later life outcomes. Being bullied may alter physiological responses
to stress,37 interact with a genetic vulnerability such as variation in the se-
rotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene,38 or affect telomere length (aging) or the
epigenome.39 Altered HPA-axis activity and altered cortisol responses may
not only increase the risk for developing mental health problems40 but also
increase susceptibility to illness by interfering with immune responses.41
A recent study found that bullied children may experience higher than
normal chronic inflammation and associated health problems that can per-
sist into adulthood.42 Blood tests for C-reactive protein (CRP), a marker
of low-grade systemic inflammation in the body often associated with
cardiovascular disease, metabolic syndrome, and psychological disorders,
revealed that CRP levels in the blood of bullied children increased with the
number of times they were bullied. Additional blood tests carried out on
the children after they had reached 19 and 21 years old revealed that those
who were bullied as children had CRP levels more than twice as high as
bullies, whereas bullies had CRP levels lower than those who were neither
bullies nor victims (see Figure A-3). Thus, bullying others appears to have
a protective effect in reducing the general rise in chronic inflammation
from childhood to early adulthood. This is consistent with studies showing
lower inflammation for individuals with higher socioeconomic status43 and
studies with non-human primates showing health benefits for those higher
in the social hierarchy.44 The clear implication of these findings is that both
ends of the continuum of social status in peer relationships are important
for inflammation levels and health status.
Furthermore, experiences of threat by peers may alter cognitive re-
sponses to threatening situations45 or affect school performance. Both al-
tered stress responses and altered social cognition (e.g., being hypervigilant
to hostile cues46) and neuro-circuitry47 related to bullying exposure may
affect social relationships with parents, friends, and co-workers. Finally,
victimization, in particular of bully-victims, has been found to be associated
APPENDIX A 123
FIGURE A-3 Adjusted mean young adult CRP levels (mg/L) based on childhood/
adolescent bullying status.
NOTE: These values are adjusted for baseline CRP levels as well as other CRP-
related covariates. All analyses used robust standard errors to account for repeated
observations.
SOURCE: Reprinted with permission from Copeland et al., 2014.
Considering this evidence of ill effects of being bullied and the fact that
children will have spent much more time with their peers than their parents
by the time they reach 18 years of age, it is more than surprising that child-
hood bullying is not at the forefront as a major public health concern.53
Children are hardly ever asked about their peer relationships by health
professionals. This is because health professionals are poorly educated
about bullying and find it difficult to raise the subject or deal with it.54 To
prevent violence against oneself (e.g., self-harm) and reduce mental health
problems, it is imperative to address bullying!
Key Messages
• Childhood bullying is a significant risk factor leading to harmful
physical, psychological, and social effects that can last a lifetime.
• It affects children from all socioeconomic backgrounds and ethnic
groups and requires universal intervention.
• There is a need for greater awareness and responsiveness in pri-
mary and secondary health care as part of a communitywide,
integrated approach to stemming the effects of childhood bullying.
• Evidence-based guidance needs to be developed on how best to
identify affected children in health care, provide support to chil-
dren and their parents, and, where necessary, make referrals to
appropriate agencies for associated physical and mental health
problems.
• Effective interventions that can be delivered in primary care to
minimize the consequences of being bullied are needed. These may
include innovative online interventions.55
• New approaches are needed to channel the considerable leadership
abilities and need for social recognition of bullies into socially ac-
ceptable and prosocial activities.
References
1. Olweus D. Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Wiley-Blackwell; 1993.
2. Bjorkqvist K, Lagerspetz KM, Kaukiainen A. Do girls manipulate and boys fight?
Developmental trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior.
1992;18(2):117-127.
3. Wolke D, Woods S, Bloomfield L, Karstadt L. The association between direct and rela-
tional bullying and behaviour problems among primary school children. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry. 2000;41(8):989-1002.
4. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK. Children’s treatment by peers: Victims of relational and overt
aggression. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8(02):367-380.
5 . Haynie DL, Nansel T, Eitel P, et al. Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups of
at-risk youth. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2001;21(1):29-49.
APPENDIX A 125
6. Boulton MJ, Smith PK. Bully/victim problems in middle-school children: Stability, self-
perceived competence, peer perceptions and peer acceptance. British Journal of Devel-
opmental Psychology. 1994;12(3):315-329.
7. Analitis F, Velderman M, Ravens-Sieberer U. Being bullied: Associated factors
in children and adolescents 8 to 18 years old in 11 European countries. Pediatrics.
2009;123(2):569-577.
8. Wolke D, Lereya ST, Fisher HL, Lewis G, Zammit S. Bullying in elementary school and
psychotic experiences at 18 years: A longitudinal, population-based cohort study. Psy-
chological Medicine. 2013;FirstView:1-13.
9. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Angold A, Costello E. Adult psychiatric outcomes of bul-
lying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry.
2013;70(4):419-426.
10. Olweus D. Cyberbullying: An overrated phenomenon? European Journal of Develop-
mental Psychology. 2012/09/01 2012;9(5):520-538.
11. Radford L, Corral S, Bradley C, Fisher H. The prevalence and impact of child maltreat-
ment and other types of victimization in the UK: Findings from a population survey of
caregivers, children and young people and young adults. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2013.
12. Volk AA, Camilleri JA, Dane AV, Marini ZA. Is Adolescent Bullying an Evolutionary
Adaptation? Aggressive Behavior. 2012;38:222-238.
13. Olthof T, Goossens FA, Vermande MM, Aleva EA, van der Meulen M. Bullying as stra-
tegic behavior: Relations with desired and acquired dominance in the peer group. Journal
of School Psychology. 2011;49(3):339-359.
14. Hawley PH, Little TD, Card NA. The myth of the alpha male: A new look at dominance-
related beliefs and behaviors among adolescent males and females. International Journal
of Behavioral Development. January 1, 2008 2008;32(1):76-88.
15. Woods S, Wolke D, Novicki S, Hall L. Emotion recognition abilities and empathy of
victims of bullying. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2009;33(5):307-311.
16. Tippett N, Wolke D. Socioeconomic Status and Bullying: A Meta-Analysis. American
Journal of Public Health. 2014:e1-e12.
17. Tippett N, Wolke D, Platt L. Ethnicity and bullying involvement in a national UK youth
sample. Journal of Adolescence. 2013;36(4):639-649.
18. Elgar FJ, Craig W, Boyce W, Morgan A, Vella-Zarb R. Income Inequality and School Bully
ing: Multilevel Study of Adolescents in 37 Countries. The Journal of Adolescent Health:
Official publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine. 10/01 2009;45(4):351-359.
19. Garandeau C, Lee I, Salmivalli C. Inequality Matters: Classroom Status Hierarchy and
Adolescents’ Bullying. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013/10/16 2013:1-11.
20. Wolke D, Skew AJ. Bullying among siblings. International Journal of Adolescent Medi-
cine and Health. 2012;24(1):17-25.
21. Garandeau CF, Lee IA, Salmivalli C. Differential effects of the KiVa anti-bullying pro-
gram on popular and unpopular bullies. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.
2014;35(1):44-50.
22. Camodeca M, Goossens FA, Schuengel C, Terwogt MM. Links between social informa-
tive processing in middle childhood and involvement in bullying. Aggressive Behavior.
2003;29(2):116-127.
23. Wolke D, Skew A. Family factors, bullying victimisation and well-being in adolescents.
Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2012;3(1):101-119.
24. Bogart LM, Elliott MN, Klein DJ, et al. Peer victimization in fifth grade and health in
tenth grade. Pediatrics. February 17, 2014.
25. Zwierzynska K, Wolke D, Lereya TS. Peer victimization in childhood and internalizing
problems in adolescence: a prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology. 2013/02/01 2013;41(2):309-323.
26. Lereya ST, Winsper C, Heron J, et al. Being bullied during childhood and the prospective
pathways to self-harm in late adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(6):608-618.e602.
27. Sourander A, Jensen P, Ronning JA, et al. What is the early adulthood outcome of boys
who bully or are bullied in childhood? The Finnish “From a Boy to a Man” study. Pedi-
atrics. August 1, 2007;120(2):397-404.
28. Klomek AB, Sourander A, Niemelä S, et al. Childhood bullying behaviors as a risk for
suicide attempts and completed suicides: A population-based birth cohort study. Journal
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48(3):254-261.
29. Takizawa R, Maughan B, Arseneault L. Adult Health outcomes of childhood bullying
victimization: Evidence from a five-decade longitudinal british birth cohort. American
Journal of Psychiatry. 2014:online first.
30. Wolke D, Schreier A, Zanarini MC, Winsper C. Bullied by peers in childhood and
borderline personality symptoms at 11 years of age: A prospective study. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):846-855.
31. Arseneault L, Milne BJ, Taylor A, et al. Being bullied as an environmentally mediated
contributing factor to children’s internalizing problems: A study of twins discordant for
victimization. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. February 1, 2008 2008;162(2):145-150.
32. Wolke D, Copeland WE, Angold A, Costello EJ. Impact of bullying in childhood on
adult health, wealth, crime, and social outcomes. Psychological Science. August 19, 2013
2013;24(10):1958-1970.
33. Arseneault L, Bowes L, Shakoor S. Bullying victimization in youths and mental health
problems: “Much ado about nothing”? Psychological Medicine. 2010;40(5):717-729.
34. Brown S, Taylor K. Bullying, education and earnings: Evidence from the National Child
Development Study. Economics of Education Review. 2008;27(4):387-401.
35. Lehti V, Klomek AB, Tamminen T, et al. Childhood bullying and becoming a young
father in a national cohort of Finnish boys. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology.
2012;53(6):461-466.
36. Sutton J, Smith PK, Swettenham J. Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or
skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1999;17:435-450.
37. Ouellet-Morin I, Danese A, Bowes L, et al. A discordant monozygotic twin design shows
blunted cortisol reactivity among bullied children. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2011;50(6):574-582.e573.
38. Sugden K, Arseneault L, Harrington H, Moffitt TE, Williams B, Caspi A. Serotonin trans-
porter gene moderates the development of emotional problems among children follow
ing bullying victimization. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry. 2010;49(8):830-840.
39. Shalev I, Moffitt TE, Sugden K, et al. Exposure to violence during childhood is associated
with telomere erosion from 5 to 10 years of age: A longitudinal study. Mol Psychiatry.
2012;18:576-581.
40. Harkness KL, Stewart JG, Wynne-Edwards KE. Cortisol reactivity to social stress in
adolescents: Role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrino.
2011;36(2):173-181.
41. Segerstrom SC, Miller GE. Psychological Stress and the human immune system: A meta-
analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;30(4):601-630.
42. Copeland WE, Wolke D, Lereya ST, Shanahan L, Worthman C, Costello EJ. Childhood
bullying involvement predicts low-grade systemic inflammation into adulthood. 2014.
PNAS: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
111(21):7570-7575.
APPENDIX A 127
Appendix B
Workshop Agenda
129
APPENDIX B 131
10:30 AM BREAK
• Mark Rosenberg
12:00 PM LUNCH
APPENDIX B 133
3:30 PM BREAK
• Peggy Murray
APPENDIX B 135
• Peggy Murray
• Mark Rosenberg
11:00 AM BREAK
12:45 PM LUNCH
APPENDIX B 137
Appendix C
Albert J. Allen, M.D., Ph.D., is the senior medical fellow with responsibility
for bioethics and pediatric capabilities at Lilly Research Labs, Eli Lilly and
Company, Indianapolis, Indiana. Dr. Allen received a B.S. in chemistry and
an M.S. in biochemistry from The University of Chicago and an M.D. and
a Ph.D. from the University of Iowa. In 1995, Dr. Allen and his mentors,
Dr. Susan Swedo and Dr. Henrietta Leonard, shared the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry’s Norbert and Charlotte Rieger Award
for Scientific Achievement for their research on possible infection-triggered
cases of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and tics. In the same year,
he joined the Institute for Juvenile Research at the University of Illinois
at Chicago, where he was an assistant professor in child and adolescent
psychiatry. In Chicago, he established and ran a pediatric OCD and tic
disorders clinic. He joined Eli Lilly in April 2000 and, in late 2003, he
became global medical director of the Strattera Product Team. In October
2004, he was made global medical director of the Neuroscience Platform
Team. In the past few years, he was the senior medical director globally
for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and related disorders.
He was also extensively involved with several activities related to pediatric
studies and global regulatory activities across Lilly’s neuroscience products,
and has participated in pharmaceutical industry activities in pediatric drug
development and the assessment of drugs in development for human abuse
liability. He chairs Lilly’s Bioethics Advisory Committee and co-chairs
Lilly’s Pediatric Steering Committee, and he is the past chair of Lilly’s Drug
Abuse Liability and Dependence Advisory Committee. Dr. Allen is a mem-
ber of the American Psychiatric Association and the American Academy
139
APPENDIX C 141
James Blair, M.D., is the chief of the Unit on Affective Cognitive Neuro
science at the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). Dr. Blair
received a doctoral degree in psychology from University College L ondon
in 1993 under the supervision of Professor John Morton. Following
graduation, he was awarded a Wellcome Trust Mental Health Research
Fellowship, which he held at the Medical Research Council Cognitive
Development Unit for 3 years. Subsequently, he moved to the Institute
of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London. There, with Uta
Frith, he helped form and co-lead the Developmental Disorders group,
and was ultimately appointed senior lecturer. He joined the NIMH Intra
mural Research Program in 2002. Dr. Blair’s primary research interest
involves understanding the neurocognitive systems mediating affect in
humans and how these become dysfunctional in mood and anxiety dis-
orders. His primary clinical focus is on understanding the dysfunction of
affect-related systems in youth with specific forms of conduct disorder.
Eric Caine, M.D., has investigated factors that contribute to suicide, with
a focus on links to unemployment, choice of specific methods, burdens of
suicide, and attempts during young and middle adulthood. Past research
has focused on military personnel and their families in the areas of inti
mate partner and family violence and suicide. Currently, his work has
addressed public health approaches to prevention that complement indi
vidually oriented treatments. He has been the principal investigator of
multiple N ational Institutes of Health (NIH) research and training grants
related to suicide research and prevention. Since 2001, he has led a series
of collaborative initiatives in China that deal with suicide prevention, the
delivery of mental health services in developing countries, and the potential
for public health approaches to reduce injuries and deaths.
APPENDIX C 143
Patrick Fox, M.D., completed his residency training in general adult and
forensic psychiatry at the Yale School of Medicine in 1999. He is board
certified in Adult General and Forensic Psychiatry. Additionally, he currently
serves on the Forensic Examination Committee for the American Board of
Psychiatry and Neurology. He has presented nationally and internationally
on seclusion and restraint reform, physician-assisted suicide, mental health
reform, sex offender management, violence risk management, outpatient civil
commitment, and jail diversion programs. He has also served on state panels
addressing access to care for and management of youth with psychiatric dis-
abilities, sex offender registration, sexually violent predator statutes, and civil
commitment. Following his completion of residency and fellowship training,
Dr. Fox remained on the faculty at Yale as an assistant professor, working
initially as a consulting forensic psychiatrist for the Connecticut Department
of Mental Health and Addiction Services, and later serving as director for
the Whiting Forensic Division, Connecticut’s maximum security forensic
hospital. Additionally, he was deputy director for Yale’s Forensic Psychiatry
Training Program from 2007 to 2012. In 2012, he took a position as at-
tending psychiatrist for the Denver County Sheriffs’ Department, managing
the city jail’s most acutely ill inmates. In April 2013, Dr. Fox was appointed
deputy director of clinical services for the Colorado Department of Human
Services’ Office of Behavioral Health. He has been serving as acting director
for the Office of Behavioral Health since October 2013. In this capacity, he
is responsible for overseeing all administrative and clinical services related to
the provision of mental health and substance abuse treatment for the office.
APPENDIX C 145
Janis Jenkins, Ph.D., received her Ph.D. from the University of California,
Los Angeles, and completed her postdoctoral training in clinically relevant
medical anthropology at Harvard Medical School. She is internationally
recognized for her expertise on cultural and mental health. Her principal
interests include the course and outcome of major mental illness, psy-
chopharmacology, ethnicity, violence, adolescence, resilience, and quali-
tative methods. Her research has been conducted with Latino and Latin
American immigrants and refugees, along with Euro-American, African
American, and Native American populations. As co-principal investigator
for the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)-funded study “South-
west Youth and the Experience of Psychiatric Treatment,” Dr. Jenkins
and her team have investigated psychiatric disorders, cultural meaning,
and violence among adolescents who have received inpatient treatment
in New Mexico. Dr. Jenkins has been on faculty at Harvard University,
Case Western Reserve University, and University of California, San Diego,
where she is professor of anthropology and adjunct professor of psychiatry.
She has been principal investigator for a series of NIMH-funded studies
on culture and mental health. She has also been awarded funding by the
School for Advanced Research and the National Alliance for Research on
Schizophrenia and Depression. Dr. Jenkins has served as a member of three
Scientific Review Groups at NIMH. She is a member of the Institute for
Advanced Studies in Princeton, New Jersey (in residence during academic
year 2011–2012). She has been visiting scholar-in-residence at the Russell
Sage Foundation in New York City, the Institute of Social Medicine in Rio
de Janeiro, and Distinguished Visiting Faculty at Monash University in
Melbourne, Australia. She has published widely in scientific journals, in-
cluding American Journal of Psychiatry, British Journal of Psychiatry, and
Medical Anthropology Quarterly. She has published two edited volumes:
Schizophrenia, Culture, and Subjectivity: The Edge of Experience (with
R. J. Barrett) by Cambridge University Press (2004) and Pharmaceutical
Self: The Global Shaping of Experience in an Age of Psychopharmacology
(School for Advanced Research, 2011).
Dévora Kestel, M.Sc., M.P.H., is a mental health regional adviser at the Pan
American Health Organization (PAHO). She is Argentinean and obtained
her M.Sc. in psychology (Universidad Nacional de La Plata). She later
earned an M.Sc. in public health at the London School of Hygiene and
Tropical Medicine. After completing her university studies in Argentina,
she moved to Italy, where she worked for 10 years in the development
and supervision of community-based mental health services in Trieste and
other cities of the region. In 2000, she joined the World Health Organiza-
tion (WHO) in Kosovo as a mental health officer. In 2001, she moved to
Albania, holding the same position until 2006, when she was appointed
WHO Representative to Albania. In both countries, she worked closely with
the Ministries of Health to help establish comprehensive community-based
APPENDIX C 147
Institute of Alcohol and Drugs Policies, Brazil. He finished his Ph.D. at the
London University, National Addiction Center, with Professor Griffith Ed-
wards in 1995. He returned to São Paulo, Brazil, and set up an Addiction
Research Unit. His work at the National Addiction center has focused on
several areas: organizing for the first time in Brazil two national household
surveys on alcohol and drugs, in collaboration with Dr. Raul Caetano from
Texas University; working on the study and implementation of alcohol and
drug policies in the community, such as the closing of bars in the city of
Diadema, zero blood alcohol concentration for drivers, partner violence
related to alcohol, and violence and mortality related to “crack/cocaine”
use; implementing an alcohol and drug treatment system in the State of São
Paulo. Dr. Laranjeira is also a member of the Department of Addiction of
the Brazilian Psychiatric Association.
Michael Luo has worked at The New York Times since 2003. He became
deputy metropolitan editor in 2014, helping to oversee coverage of New
York City and the surrounding region and directing a team of reporters
APPENDIX C 149
APPENDIX C 151
these problems. Much of this work has focused on the development and
treatment of conduct problems among youth exhibiting callous and un-
emotional (CU) traits. Over the past 8 years, this has involved analyzing
data from two of the most extensive longitudinal studies ever conducted
within the United States: the Pittsburgh Youth Study (co-director) and the
Pittsburgh Girls Study. Dr. Pardini’s innovative research directly influences
the adoption of the new conduct disorder specifier based on the presence
of CU traits (called limited prosocial emotions) in the Diagnostic and Sta-
tistical Manual, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), and earned him the Early Career
Contribution Award from the Society of the Scientific Study of Psychopathy
(2013). He is currently serving as a consultant to members of the Conduct
Disorders Research Committee for the eleventh revision of the International
Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). He has also been involved in research
designed to evaluate the effectiveness of interventions for children exhibit-
ing early aggression, including the Stop Now and Plan (SNAP) and the
Resources to Enhance the Adjustment of Children (REACH) programs.
APPENDIX C 153
in violence prevention and later became the first permanent director of the
National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. He also held the posi-
tion of special assistant for behavioral science in the Office of the Deputy
Director (HIV/AIDS). Dr. Rosenberg is board certified in both psychiatry
and internal medicine with training in public policy. He is on the faculty
at Morehouse Medical School, Emory Medical School, and the R ollins
School of Public Health at Emory University. Dr. Rosenberg’s research and
programmatic interests are concentrated on injury control and violence
prevention, HIV/AIDS, and child well-being, with special attention to be-
havioral sciences, evaluation, and health communications. He has a uthored
more than 120 publications and recently co-authored the book Real Col-
laboration: What It Takes for Global Health to Succeed (University of
California Press, 2010). Dr. Rosenberg has received numerous awards
including the Surgeon General’s Exemplary Service Medal. He is a member
of the N ational Academy of Medicine. Dr. Rosenberg’s organization, the
Task Force for Global Health, participated in the National Academies of
Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine–sponsored workshop Violence Preven-
tion in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: Finding a Place on the Global
Agenda, and the Task Force remains interested in helping to continue the
momentum of the workshop through the Forum on Global Violence Pre-
vention. The Task Force is heavily involved in the delivery of a number of
global health programs and sees many ways in which interpersonal violence
and conflict exacerbate serious health problems and inequities.
Elyn R. Saks, Ph.D., J.D., specializes in mental health law, criminal law, and
children and the law. Her recent research focused on ethical dimensions of
psychiatric research and forced treatment of the mentally ill. She teaches
Mental Health Law, Mental Health Law and the Criminal Justice System,
and Advanced Family Law: The Rights and Interests of Children. She served
as the University of Southern California (USC) Law’s associate dean for
research from 2005 to 2010 and also teaches at the Institute of Psychiatry
and the Law at the Keck School of Medicine at USC and is an adjunct pro-
fessor of psychiatry at the University of California, San Diego. Professor
Saks was a 2009 recipient of a MacArthur Foundation fellowship and in
fall 2010 announced she is using funds from the “Genius Grant” to create
the Saks Institute for Mental Health Law, Policy, and Ethics. The Institute
spotlights one important mental health issue per academic year and is a
collaborative effort that includes faculty from seven USC departments: law,
psychiatry, psychology, social work, gerontology, philosophy, and engi
neering. Professor Saks recently published The Center Cannot Hold: My
Journey Through Madness (Hyperion, 2007), a memoir about her struggles
and successes with schizophrenia and acute psychosis. Other publications
include Refusing Care: Forced Treatment and the Rights of the Mentally Ill
(University of Chicago Press, 2002), Interpreting Interpretation: The Limits
of Hermeneutic Psychoanalysis (Yale University Press, 1999), and Jekyll on
Trial: Multiple Personality Disorder and Criminal Law (with Stephen H.
Behnke, New York University Press, 1997). Before joining the USC Law
faculty in 1989, Professor Saks was an attorney in Connecticut and an
instructor at the University of Bridgeport School of Law. She graduated
summa cum laude from Vanderbilt University before earning her master of
letters from Oxford University and her J.D. from Yale Law School, where
she also edited the Yale Law Journal. She holds a Ph.D. in psychoanalytic
science from the New Center for Psychoanalysis. Professor Saks is a mem-
ber of Phi Beta Kappa; an affiliate member of the American Psychoanalytic
Association; a board member of Mental Health Advocacy Services; and a
member of the Los Angeles Psychoanalytic Foundation, Robert J. Stoller
Foundation, and American Law Institute. Professor Saks won both the
Associate’s Award for Creativity in Research and Scholarship and the Phi
Kappa Phi Faculty Recognition Award in 2004.
APPENDIX C 155
2005. She currently directs two forums: one on Global Violence Prevention
and the other on Public–Private Partnerships for Global Health and Safety.
She is also co-directing a workshop on Evaluation Methods for Large-Scale,
Complex, Multinational Global Health Initiatives. From 2009 to 2013, she
was the study co-director for the outcome and impact evaluation of the U.S.
global HIV/AIDS initiative (i.e., the President’s Emergency Plan for AIDS
Relief, or PEPFAR). Her portfolio of work for the National Academies also
includes a mix of consensus studies, workshops, and other activities, includ-
ing the Evaluation of the Implementation of PEPFAR; Preventing Violence
in Low- and Middle-Income Countries; the Assessment of the Role of Inter-
mittent Preventive Treatment for Malaria in Infants; Depression, Parenting
Practices, and the Health Development of Children; and Achieving Global
Sustainable Surveillance for Zoonotic Diseases. Before joining the National
Academies, she was an analyst on the health care team at the U.S. Gov-
ernment Accountability Office. Before returning to graduate school, she
coordinated a foundation-funded program at Duke University’s Center
for Health Policy, Law, and Management to integrate public and private
mental health services with the continuum of care for people living with and
affected by HIV/AIDS in 54 counties in North Carolina. For 6 years, she
served as the executive director of a Ryan White–funded HIV/AIDS con-
sortium, developing a comprehensive ambulatory care system for 21 mostly
rural counties in North Carolina. Previous North Carolina health-related
committee service includes several advisory committees to the governor of
North Carolina and to the secretary of the North Carolina Department
of Health and Human Services for programmatic and policy issues related
to HIV care, prevention, and treatment, as well as substance abuse preven-
tion and treatment. She received an M.S.P.H. in health policy analysis from
the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill. As an Echols Scholar, she
completed her undergraduate studies at the University of Virginia.
APPENDIX C 157
Dieter Wolke, Ph.D., studied at the University of Kiel in Germany and ob-
tained his Ph.D. from the University of London Faculty of Science. He has
worked at different colleges of the University of London (i.e., Institute of
Education; King’s College; and the Institute of Child Health, Hospital for
Sick Children) and the Universities of Munich, Hertfordshire (chair), Bristol
(chair in lifespan psychology, and deputy director of the Avon Longitudinal
Study [ALSPAC]), and was guest professor of the University of Zurich and