Ruppe Et Al 2015 PNAS
Ruppe Et Al 2015 PNAS
Ruppe Et Al 2015 PNAS
soundscape in fishes
Laëtitia Ruppéa, Gaël Clémentb, Anthony Herrelc,d, Laurent Ballestae, Thierry Décampsc, Loïc Kévera,
and Eric Parmentiera,1
a
Laboratory of Functional and Evolutionary Morphology, Applied and Fundamental Fish Research Center, Campus du Sart Tilman Bâtiment B6c, University
of Liège, 4000 Liège 1, Belgium; bCentre de Recherches sur la Paléobiodiversité et les Paléoenvironnements (UMR 7207, CR2P), Sorbonne Universités,
Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle (MNHN)/CNRS/UPMC-Paris6, MNHN 75231 Paris Cedex 05, France; cMécanismes adaptatifs et évolution (UMR 7179,
MECADEV), MNHN/CNRS, MNHN de Paris, 75231 Paris Cedex 05, France; dEvolutionary Morphology of Vertebrates, Ghent University, B-9000 Ghent,
Belgium; and eAndromède Océanologie, 34280 Carnon-Plage, France
Edited by James H. Brown, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, and approved March 16, 2015 (received for review December 23, 2014)
The underwater environment is more and more being depicted that signal overlap is avoided (11–15). Moreover, theory predicts
as particularly noisy, and the inventory of calling fishes is con- that the competition for acoustic space should result in signal
tinuously increasing. However, it currently remains unknown how divergence, which would increase signal distinctiveness and op-
species share the soundscape and are able to communicate without portunities for correct signal discrimination (14, 15) and lead to
misinterpreting the messages. Different mechanisms of interference the avoidance of frequency overlap. This strategy predicts that
avoidance have been documented in birds, mammals, and frogs, but signals produced at a given time and in a given place can be
little is known about interference avoidance in fishes. How fish thus distinguished by their frequency. The frequency partitioning of
partition the soundscape underwater remains unknown, as acoustic the acoustic environment was first demonstrated for birds and,
communication and its organization have never been studied at the more recently, also for frogs (2, 16).
level of fish communities. In this study, passive acoustic recordings To date, there is little direct evidence of mechanisms of
ECOLOGY
were used to inventory sounds produced in a fish community signal interference avoidance in fish. At best, it has been shown
(120 m depth) in an attempt to understand how different species that calls of nearby toadfish, Opsanus beta, do not overlap.
partition the acoustic environment. We uncovered an important However, this pertains to specimens of the same species (17).
diversity of fish sounds, and 16 of the 37 different sounds recorded Moreover, O. beta increases its call rate and changes its call
were sufficiently abundant to use in a quantitative analysis. We duration during the twilight period (17). Despite the array
show that sonic activity allows a clear distinction between a diurnal of behaviors associated with sound production, fish sounds
and a nocturnal group of fishes. Moreover, frequencies of signals are mainly used during reproduction (courtship, spawning,
made during the day overlap, whereas there is a clear distinction male competition) or during behaviors related to territoriality
between the different representatives of the nocturnal callers (warning, chase, nest defense, fight). During courtship, calls
because of a lack of overlap in sound frequency. This first demon- can be used by females to assess species identity and the quality
stration, to our knowledge, of interference avoidance in a fish of potential sexual partners (18–21). During agonistic behav-
community can be understood by the way sounds are used. In iors, sound features enable the receiver to assess the fighting
diurnal species, sounds are mostly used to support visual display, capacity of the opponent because acoustic parameters can
whereas nocturnal species are generally deprived of visual cues, provide information on the size, the social status, the motiva-
resulting in acoustic constraints being more important. tion, or the physiological state of the emitter (22–25). However,
how fish sounds are organized at the level of the community
|
acoustic communication signal interference | passive acoustic recordings | remains unknown.
|
diversity of sounds frequency partitioning Passive acoustic recording methods have been developed in
recent years (26) and allow not only the detection of a larger
effect of anthropogenic noise is thought to be problematic for More and more studies stress the potential deleterious effect
many species (1–5). The main reason for this call to action is the of anthropogenic sounds on fish acoustic communication.
crucial role of sound communication in the regulation of dif- Paradoxically, how the communication between fishes in a
ferent kinds of social relationships, as has been demonstrated in community is organized remains extremely poorly known, as
numerous taxa. However, most studies with fish tend to consider studies using passive acoustic recordings are typically restricted
species in isolation, and there is a lack of data addressing to one or two species. At a single site, we were able to follow
acoustic communication of fishes living in natural communities 16 different vertebrate sounds for 15 days. We demonstrate
(6). As a consequence, how species share the soundscape with that the fish population can be distributed into two groups:
other species living in the same habitat remains unknown. This one diurnal and one nocturnal. Most interestingly, fish calling
topic has received much more attention in the air, where many at night do not show overlap at the level of the main calling
mechanisms that animals use to deal with this problem are frequency, in contrast to fish calling during the day. This shows
known (Fig. 1). The Lombard effect, for example, corresponds to that at night, in the absence of visual cues, sound communi-
an increase in signal amplitude. It was first described for humans, cation is more important.
and subsequently in birds, mammals, and fish (5, 7). However,
an increase in signal amplitude is a limited solution when the Author contributions: A.H. and E.P. designed research; L.R., G.C., L.B., T.D., and L.K. per-
background noise level is important. As a consequence, some formed research; L.R. and E.P. analyzed data; and L.R., A.H., and E.P. wrote the paper.
frogs, birds, and mammals also increase the call duration, or call The authors declare no conflict of interest.
rate, to increase the likelihood of being heard (8–10). In complex This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
acoustic environments, such as animal communities character- 1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected].
ized by an important quantity of signals, vocalizing animals cope This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.
with background noise by inserting their signals in such a way 1073/pnas.1424667112/-/DCSupplemental.
1 0.33 ± 0.12 490 ± 281 6.80 ± 2.25 23.11 ± 11.67 One pulse followed by a train of around six pulses
(pulse period: 50 ms). Sometimes, the previous pulse is absent
2 0.65 ± 0.09 480 ± 227 28.82 ± 4.49 44.75 ± 6.79 Two parts. The first one is composed of around 20–25 pulses
(pulse period: 10–15 ms). The second part is around five pulses
(pulse period: 30–35 ms)
3 0.07 ± 0.02 2,803 ± 277 2.20 ± 0.42 33.72 ± 7.83 Two or three clear pulses separated by 45–50 ms
4 0.16 ± 0.03 66 ± 5 1.00 ± 0 — Isolated boom
5 0.08 ± 0.05 158 ± 69 1.00 ± 0 — Isolated pulse
6 1.16 ± 0,31 274 ± 137 4.10 ± 0.74 3.65 ± 0.68 Coarse pulses with a small growl at the front of the pulse
(pulse period: around 200 ms)
7 0.78 ± 0.25 762 ± 122 4.50 ± 0.97 6.04 ± 1.26 Long pulse. High-frequency whistle.
8 1.14 ± 0.36 1,630 ± 401 10.30 ± 3.33 9.27 ± 1.71 Three or four groups of pulses separated by around 250 ms.
The first one is often composed of one pulse, and the next
groups of three to four pulses (pulse period: around 10–15 ms)
9 1.08 ± 0.64 2,641 ± 257 6.70 ± 3.27 6.38 ± 0.60 Clear pulses, regular
10 0.51 ± 0.12 162 ± 102 4.70 ± 1.16 9.33 ± 1.55 Coarse pulses grouped with a number of pulses, increasing
for the next one
11 0.82 ± 0.35 429 ± 267 19.80 ± 7.76 25.16 ± 6.29 Pulses separated by around 50 ms. Amplitude relative variation:
increase then decrease
12 0.35 ± 0.15 446 ± 299 16.82 ± 4.96 52.87 ± 15.44 One large pulse followed by smaller pulses with an increasing
pulse period
13 1.49 ± 0.70 1,088 ± 214 5.10 ± 2.18 3.58 ± 0.60 Series of grunts
14 1.30 ± 0.23 263 ± 114 Tonal — Long tonal sound with an increasing of the intensity.
At the end, some more isolated pulses are observable
15 0.49 ± 0.08 290 ± 198 6.90 ± 1.20 14.26 ± 2.35 One or two pulses grouped following by a train of around
eight pulses. Sometimes the first group of pulses is
absent
16 1.65 ± 0.74 427 ± 204 4.1 ± 1.20 2.75 ± 0.93 Clear pulses
17 0.24 ± 0.13 1,056 ± 151 6.4 ± 3.37 27.13 ± 2.53 Coarse pulses in regular series
Values are mean ± SD. An oscillogram is presented in Fig. S1 for each group.
ECOLOGY
group to which the sound belongs, and the vertical axis represents the as- general pattern of sound (oscillogram shape) is similar, but the
signment of a sound to a group by the model. The more yellow the color, the hours of activity are opposed (sounds of group 1 occur mainly
higher the proportion of correct assignments (Table S2).
during the day, whereas sounds of group 15 occur mainly at
night). Two hypotheses can be proposed: these sounds are pro-
of the acoustic parameters were found (Wilcoxon test, P > 0.05 in duced by two different species, one diurnal and one nocturnal,
all cases), showing each sampling is representative and does not thus avoiding acoustic interference, or these sounds are pro-
duced by a single species with a large activity period. At this
influence the results. Statistical analyses were performed to
point, we cannot distinguish between these two possibilities.
estimate the validity and consistency of these groups. In a prin-
Some sounds belonging to group 6 were assigned to group 16
cipal component analysis, ∼71.1% of the variability is explained by the model. The general pattern of these two groups is, how-
by the first two principal components (43.2% by principal com- ever, different. Sounds belonging to group 6 show long pulses
ponent 1 and 27.9% by principal component 2). Principal com- with small growls, whereas sounds belonging to group 16 are
ponent 1 describes the pulse period and the number of pulses. composed of clear and short pulses (Table 1 and Fig. S1). These
The second component describes the sound duration and the sounds cannot be produced by an identical mechanism, and thus
peak frequency. All groups are homogeneous, and Kruskal not by the same species. The same holds for group 13 (sounds of
Wallis tests were significant for each acoustic parameter be- “grunt” type), from which some sounds were assigned to group 7
tween groups (pulse period: χ2 = 161.2; df = 16; P < 0.001; (sounds of “whistle” type).
Fig. 3. Distribution of the different groups during the day. Daytime was between 05:00 and 16:30. Each group is represented by its relative percentage of
presence per hour. The longer the rectangle, the more likely the presence of the group at this hour is.
caves sheltering many species, including coelacanths. The device was placed in Statistical Analyses. A principal component analysis and a discriminant
a submarine cave (called U-cave or cave 2) localized at 113 m in depth at the function analysis were conducted to discriminate and determine the validity
head of the Jesser Canyon, about 4 km from the shoreline. of our sound groups. These tests were coupled to Kruskal Wallis tests, χ2 test,
and Tukey HSD post hoc tests. To study the emission of sounds during
Acoustic Recordings. The Digital SpectroGram Recorder system is an auton- daytime and nighttime, the same tests were used. A nonparametric test on
omous acoustic recording system. It enables users to save acoustic recordings paired data (Wilcoxon test) was used to justify the use of a raw data sample.
on a 32-GB SD memory card and is controlled by an on-board real-time clock.
All these tests were conducted with R i386 3.0.2 software.
In this study, a Digital SpectroGram Recorder system (Loggerhead In-
struments) was positioned in a crevice of the submarine cave wall. Sounds
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank the team of Gombessa 2013, in particular
were recorded for 9 min every 10 min by the hydrophone (−186 dB re 1V/μPa,
Florian Holon, Tybo Rauby, and Yannick and Cédric Gentil, for the equip-
sample rate of 20 kHz) during a total of 19 nonconsecutive days from April ment and the implementation of the device. We thank Frédéric Bertucci,
through May 2013. Files were automatically saved with temporal data. A total Bruno Frédérich, and Damien Olivier for logistical support and useful discus-
of 2,273 acoustic files were recorded during this study. Given the large number sions. We also thank to Peter Timm, Adele Stegen, and the Triton Experience
of files, only the first 9 min of each hour were analyzed (387 files). Acoustical diving team for their effective assistance during the dives. We are grateful to
analyses were conducted manually with Avisoft-SASLab Pro-5.2.07 software. the scientific teams of the South African Institute for Aquatic Biodiversity
For each distinct sound, the following acoustic parameters were measured: and of the South African National Biodiversity Institute. Thanks are also
total sound duration (s), number of pulses, pulse period (i.e., the ratio between owed to the iSimangaliso Wetland Park Authorities for permission to dive in
the park. This work was supported by the MNHN funding sources via Crédits
the pulse number and the sound duration, in pulses per second), and the peak
Recherche, Soutien pour les Expéditions scientifiques, ATM Formes, and by the
frequency in hertz (i.e., the frequency of greatest amplitude). The temporal CNRS via Labex BCdiv Biological and Cultural diversities. We also thank la
features were measured from oscillograms (44.1 kHz, 16 bit), whereas the Société des Amis du Muséum et du Jardin des Plantes, Association de Retrans-
peak frequency was obtained from the logarithmic power spectra (Hamming mission Télévisuelle Européenne France Télévision, and the Manufacture de
window, FFT Fast Fourier Transform, FFT Length 64, 689-Hz resolution). Haute Horlogerie Blancpain.
1. Codarin A, Wysocki LE, Ladich F, Picciulin M (2009) Effects of ambient and boat noise 9. Penna M, Pottstock H, Velasquez N (2005) Effect of natural and synthetic noise on evoked
on hearing and communication in three fish species living in a marine protected area vocal responses in a frog of the temperate austral forest. Anim Behav 70(3):639–651.
(Miramare, Italy). Mar Pollut Bull 58(12):1880–1887. 10. Leonard ML, Horn AG (2008) Does ambient noise affect growth and begging call
2. Halfwerk W, Slabbekoorn H (2009) A behavioural mechanism explaining noise- structure in nestling birds? Behav Ecol 19(3):502–507.
dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Anim Behav 78(6):1301–1307. 11. Brumm H, Slater P (2006) Ambient noise, motor fatigue, and serial redundancy in
3. Vasconcelos RO, Amorim MCP, Ladich F (2007) Effects of ship noise on the de- chaffinch song. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:475–481.
tectability of communication signals in the Lusitanian toadfish. J Exp Biol 210(Pt 12): 12. Egner SA, Mann DA (2005) Auditory sensitivity of sergeant major damselfish Abudefduf
2104–2112. saxatilis from post-settlement juvenile to adult. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 285:213–222.
4. Brumm H (2014) Fish struggle to be heard—but just how much fin waving is there? A 13. Grafe T (1996) The function of call alternation in the African reed frog (Hyperolius
comment on Radford et al. Behav Ecol 25(5):1033–1034. marmoratus): Precise call timing prevents auditory masking. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:
5. Brumm H, Slabbekoorn H (2005) Acoustic communication in noise. Adv Stud Behav 35: 149–158.
151–209. 14. Marler P (1960) Bird songs and mate selection. Animal Sounds and Communication,
6. Tricas T, Boyle K (2014) Acoustic behaviors in Hawaiian coral reef fish communities. eds Lanyon WE, Tavolga WN (American Institute of Biological Sciences, Washington,
Mar Ecol Prog Ser 511:1–16. DC), Vol 7, pp 348–367.
7. Holt DE, Johnston CE (2014) Evidence of the Lombard effect in fishes. Behav Ecol 15. Miller J (1982) Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals.
25(4):819–826. Cognit Psychol 14(2):247–279.
8. Brumm H, Voss K, Köllmer I, Todt D (2004) Acoustic communication in noise: Regu- 16. Villanueva-Rivera LJ (2014) Eleutherodactylus frogs show frequency but no temporal
lation of call characteristics in a New World monkey. J Exp Biol 207(Pt 3):443–448. partitioning: Implications for the acoustic niche hypothesis. PeerJ 2:e496.