Bookshelf NBK216032 PDF
Bookshelf NBK216032 PDF
Bookshelf NBK216032 PDF
html
We ship printed books within 1 business day; personal PDFs are available immediately.
Visit the National Academies Press online, the authoritative source for all books
from the National Academy of Sciences, the National Academy of Engineering,
the Institute of Medicine, and the National Research Council:
• Download hundreds of free books in PDF
• Read thousands of books online for free
• Explore our innovative research tools – try the “Research Dashboard” now!
• Sign up to be notified when new books are published
• Purchase printed books and selected PDF files
Thank you for downloading this PDF. If you have comments, questions or
just want more information about the books published by the National
Academies Press, you may contact our customer service department toll-
free at 888-624-8373, visit us online, or send an email to
[email protected].
NOTICE: The project that is the subject of this report was approved by the Govern-
ing Board of the National Research Council, whose members are drawn from the
councils of the National Academy of Sciences, the National Academy of Engineer-
ing, and the Institute of Medicine. The members of the committee responsible for
the report were chosen for their special competences and with regard for appropri-
ate balance.
Support for this project was provided by the American Academy of Family Physi-
cians Foundation, California HealthCare Foundation, Commonwealth Fund, W.K.
Kellogg Foundation, MetLife Foundation, National Cancer Institute, Pfizer Corpo-
ration, and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. The views presented in this
report are those of the Institute of Medicine Committee on Health Literacy and are
not necessarily those of the funding agencies.
Additional copies of this report are available from the National Academies Press,
500 Fifth Street, NW, Lockbox 285, Washington, DC 20055; (800) 624-6242 or
(202) 334-3313 (in the Washington metropolitan area); Internet, http://
www.nap.edu.
For more information about the Institute of Medicine, visit the IOM home page at:
www.iom.edu.
Copyright 2004 by the National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Printed in the United States of America.
COVER: Adapted from a design by Anne Quito, Academy for Educational Devel-
opment.
The serpent has been a symbol of long life, healing, and knowledge among almost
all cultures and religions since the beginning of recorded history. The serpent
adopted as a logotype by the Institute of Medicine is a relief carving from ancient
Greece, now held by the Staatliche Museen in Berlin.
The National Academy of Engineering was established in 1964, under the charter of
the National Academy of Sciences, as a parallel organization of outstanding engi-
neers. It is autonomous in its administration and in the selection of its members,
sharing with the National Academy of Sciences the responsibility for advising the
federal government. The National Academy of Engineering also sponsors engineer-
ing programs aimed at meeting national needs, encourages education and research,
and recognizes the superior achievements of engineers. Dr. Wm. A. Wulf is presi-
dent of the National Academy of Engineering.
The National Research Council was organized by the National Academy of Sciences
in 1916 to associate the broad community of science and technology with the
Academy’s purposes of furthering knowledge and advising the federal government.
Functioning in accordance with general policies determined by the Academy, the
Council has become the principal operating agency of both the National Academy
of Sciences and the National Academy of Engineering in providing services to the
government, the public, and the scientific and engineering communities. The Coun-
cil is administered jointly by both Academies and the Institute of Medicine. Dr.
Bruce M. Alberts and Dr. Wm. A. Wulf are chair and vice chair, respectively, of the
National Research Council.
www.national-academies.org
Study Staff
LYNN NIELSEN-BOHLMAN, Study Director
ALLISON PANZER, Research Assistant
BENJAMIN N. HAMLIN, Research Assistant
ALLISON BERGER, Program Assistant
T
his report has been reviewed in draft form by individuals chosen for
their diverse perspectives and technical expertise, in accordance with
procedures approved by the National Research Council’s (NRC’s)
Report Review Committee. The purpose of this independent review is to
provide candid and critical comments that will assist the institution in
making its published report as sound as possible and to ensure that the
report meets institutional standards for objectivity, evidence, and respon-
siveness to the study charge. The review comments and draft manuscript
remain confidential to protect the integrity of the deliberative process. We
wish to thank the following individuals for their review of this report:
vii
or recommendations nor did they see the final draft of the report before its
release. The review of this report was overseen by Daniel L. Azarnoff, D.L.
Azarnoff Associates, and Kristine Gebbie, Columbia University School of
Nursing. Appointed by the NRC and the Institute of Medicine, they were
responsible for making certain that an independent examination of this
report was carried out in accordance with institutional procedures and that
all review comments were carefully considered. Responsibility for the final
content of this report rests entirely with the authoring committee and the
institution.
Acknowledgments
T
he committee was aided in its deliberations by the testimony and
advice of many knowledgeable and experienced individuals, and the
efforts of a dedicated committee and staff. Consultants to the com-
mittee contributed ideas and report materials. The committee thanks con-
sultants Arlene Bierman, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality;
John Comings, Harvard University; Terry Davis, Louisiana State University
Health Sciences Center; Julie Gazmararian, Emory University; David
Howard, Emory University; Frank McClellan, Temple University; Scott
Ratzan, Johnson and Johnson; Dean Schillinger, University of California at
San Francisco and San Francisco General Hospital Medical Center; Steve
Somers, Center for Health Care Strategies; and Barry Weiss, University of
Arizona.
The committee acknowledges with appreciation the testimony and other
assistance of many individuals committed to improving health literacy.
These individuals are Paul S. Appelbaum, University of Massachusetts Medi-
cal School; Cynthia Baur, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services;
Nancy Berkman, RTI International; Alvin Billie, The Gathering Place; Cindy
Brach, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality; L. Natalie Carroll,
National Medical Association; Carolyn Clancy, Agency for Healthcare Re-
search and Quality; Eduardo Crespi, Centro Latino; Barbara DeBuono,
Pfizer Pharmaceuticals Group; Darren A. DeWalt, University of North
Carolina, Chapel Hill; Janice A. Drass, Centers for Medicare and Medicaid
Services, DHHS; Joyce Dubow, AARP Public Policy Institute; Lawrence J.
Fine, Office of Behavioral and Social Science Research, National Institutes
ix
x ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Foreword
C
lear communication is critical to successful health care. Patients
convey their symptoms and medical history to caregivers; health
professionals issue orders, results, and recommendations to one
another; and doctors, nurses, pharmacists, and others provide information
and instructions to patients. Health professionals are trained to observe
their patients keenly and to elicit a revealing history. Considerable effort
and money are expended to automate reporting of test results and physician
order entry so as to speed availability of clinical information and to reduce
errors. However, comparatively little attention has been devoted to en-
abling patients to comprehend their condition and treatment, to make the
best decisions for their care, and to take the right medications at the right
time in the intended dose. As this report makes clear, health literacy—
enabling patients to understand and to act in their own interest—remains a
neglected, final pathway to high-quality health care.
Tens of millions of U.S. adults are unable to read complex texts, includ-
ing many health-related materials. Arcane language and jargon that become
second nature to doctors and nurses are inscrutable to many patients. Adults
who have a problem understanding written materials are often ashamed
and devise methods to mask their difficulty. They may be reluctant to ask
questions for fear of being perceived as ignorant. If health professionals
were able to take the time to ask their patients to explain exactly what they
understand about their diagnoses, instructions, and bottle labels, the care-
givers would find many gaps in knowledge, difficulties in understanding,
and misinterpretations. These problems are exacerbated by language and
xi
xii FOREWORD
Preface
M
y understanding of the issues in health literacy was limited prior
to taking on the role of Chair of this committee. From my
expertise in defining and measuring population health and its
determinants, I appreciated the importance of the social determinants of
health. I had speculated about the role of health literacy as one pathway by
which education might exert an independent effect on health outcomes. But
until this rich and intense interaction with my colleagues from diverse fields
such as literacy, biology, health communication, anthropology, epidemiol-
ogy, medicine, nursing, and health policy, combined with poignant testi-
mony from those affected and other experts, I had no idea of the impor-
tance and complexity of this topic.
I believe that what the United States puts into practice in medicine and
health is much less than what is known. Only now do I know how pro-
foundly the gap between knowledge and practice is widened by limited
health literacy. Only now do I know why some refer to this as a “silent
epidemic”—the lack of understanding by most professionals and policy
makers of its extent and effect, and the individual shame associated with it
that keeps it even more silent and hidden.
I hope that this report will produce several outcomes:
xiii
xiv PREFACE
Contents
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 1
1 INTRODUCTION 19
Social and Economic Factors and Health, 20
Living in a Society with High Literacy Demands, 21
Health Literacy as a Public Concern, 25
Charge to the Committee, 26
Scope of the Report, 27
References, 29
xv
xvi CONTENTS
APPENDIXES
INDEX 331
TABLES
2-1 Examples of Skills Needed for Health, 42
xvii
FIGURES
ES-1 Potential points for intervention in the health literacy
framework, 5
BOXES
4-1 Sources of Health Information Reported in a Gallup Poll, 121
4-2 Selected Findings from Sex Matters, 122
4-3 Excerpt from the Introduction to “Language Access: Helping
Non-English Speakers Navigate Health and Human Services”, 135
Executive Summary
ABSTRACT
I have a very good doctor. He takes the time to explain things and break it
down to me. Sometimes, though, I do get stuff that can be hard—like when
I first came home from the hospital and I had all these forms and things I had
to read. Some words I come across I just can’t quite understand (National
Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy, 2003).1
1All vignettes in shaded text in this report represent actual stories or materials. Names were
omitted in most cases to protect the privacy of the author, and stories may have been edited
for brevity and clarity. If not otherwise attributed, vignettes were drawn from the experiences
of members of the committee.
2 HEALTH LITERACY
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 3
INTRODUCTION
M
odern health systems make complex demands on the health con-
sumer. As self-management of health care increases, individuals
are asked to assume new roles in seeking information, under-
standing rights and responsibilities, and making health decisions for them-
selves and others. Underlying these demands are assumptions about people’s
knowledge and skills.
National and international assessments of adults’ ability to use written
information suggest that these assumptions may be faulty. Current evidence
reveals a mismatch between people’s skills and the demands of health sys-
tems (Rudd et al., 2000a). Many people who deal effectively with other
aspects of their lives may find health information difficult to obtain, under-
stand, or use. While farmers may be able to use fertilizers effectively, they
may not understand the safety information provided with the fertilizer.
Chefs may create excellent dishes, but may not know how to create a
healthy diet. Indeed, health literacy can be a hidden problem—because it is
often not recognized by policy makers and health care providers, and be-
cause people with low literacy skills or who are confused about health care
may be ashamed to speak up about problems they encounter with the
increasingly complex health system (Baker et al., 1996; Parikh et al., 1996).
Without improvements in health literacy, the promise of scientific advances
for improving health outcomes will be diminished.
The Institute of Medicine (IOM) convened the Committee on Health
Literacy, composed of experts from a wide range of academic disciplines
and backgrounds, to assess the problem of limited health literacy and to
consider the next steps in this field. The committee addressed the following
charge:
4 HEALTH LITERACY
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 5
Education
System
3
FIGURE ES-1 Potential points for intervention in the health literacy framework.
Finding 2-2 The links between education and health outcomes are
strongly established. The committee concludes that health literacy may be
6 HEALTH LITERACY
Finding 2-4 While health literacy measures in current use have spurred
research initiatives and yield valuable insights, they are indicators of read-
ing skills (word recognition or reading comprehension and numeracy),
rather than measures of the full range of skills needed for health literacy
(cultural and conceptual knowledge, listening, speaking, numeracy, writ-
ing, and reading). Current assessment tools and research findings cannot
differentiate among (a) reading ability, (b) lack of background knowledge
in health-related domains, such as biology, (c) lack of familiarity with
language and types of materials, or (d) cultural differences in approaches to
health and health care. In addition, no current measures of health literacy
include oral communication skills or writing skills and none measure the
health literacy demands on individuals within different health contexts.
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 7
8 HEALTH LITERACY
Arizona. Patients with reading levels at or below third grade had mean
Medicaid charges $7,500 higher than those who read above the third grade
level.
For this report, David Howard examined the expenditure data col-
lected in association with the Baker and colleagues (2002) utilization study
(see Appendix B). He found that predicted inpatient spending for a patient
with inadequate health literacy was $993 higher than that of a patient with
adequate reading skills. A difference of $450 remained after controlling
for health status, although the causality of the associations between health
status and health-care cost could not be determined. In both analyses,
higher emergency care costs were incurred by individuals with limited
health literacy compared to those with marginal or adequate health lit-
eracy as measured by the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults
(TOFHLA), while pharmacy expenses were similar and outpatient expen-
ditures lower.
Although a robust estimate for the effect of limited health literacy on
health expenditures is lacking, the magnitudes suggested by the few studies
that are available underscore the importance of addressing limited health
literacy from a financial perspective.
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 9
The ho’ola said Mom should confess to me and before God Jehovah. She
did. She asked me to forgive her and I did. I wasn’t angry. . . . And later
Mom’s sickness left her. Of course, she still had diabetes, but the rest—being
so confused and miserable—all that left her (Shook, 1985: 109).
Culture is the shared ideas, meanings, and values that are acquired by
individuals as members of a society. Culture is socially learned, continually
evolves, and often influences us unconsciously. We learn culture through
interactions with others, as well as through the tangible products of culture
such as books and television (IOM, 2002). Culture gives significance to
health information and messages, and can shape perceptions and defini-
tions of health and illness, preferences, language and cultural barriers, care
process barriers, and stereotypes. These culturally influenced perceptions,
definitions, and barriers can affect how people interact with the health care
system and help to determine the adequacy of health literacy skills in differ-
ent settings.
The fluid nature of culture means that health-care encounters are rich
with differences that are continuously evolving. Differing cultural and
educational backgrounds between patients and providers, as well as be-
tween those who create health information and those who use it, may
contribute to problems in health literacy. Culture, cultural processes, and
cross-cultural interventions have been discussed in depth in several recent
IOM reports and represent possible nexuses of culture and health literacy
(IOM, 2002, 2003a).
It is important to understand how people obtain and use health infor-
mation in order to understand the potential impact of health literacy. Infor-
mation about health is produced by many sources, including the govern-
ment and the food and drug industries, and is distributed by the popular
media. Commercial and social marketing of health information, products,
and services is a multibillion dollar industry. People are frequently and
repeatedly exposed to quick, often contradictory bits of information. This
10 HEALTH LITERACY
Finding 4-2 More than 300 studies indicate that health-related mate-
rials far exceed the average reading ability of U.S. adults.
Finding 4-4 Health literacy efforts have not yet fully benefited from
research findings in social and commercial marketing.
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 11
Health Systems
Health systems in the United States are complex and often confusing.
Their complexity derives from the nature of health care and public health
itself, the mix of public and private financing, and the variations across
states and between types of delivery settings. An adult’s ability to navigate
these systems may reflect this systemic complexity in addition to individual
skill levels. Even highly skilled individuals may find the systems too compli-
cated to understand, especially when these individuals are made more vul-
nerable by poor health. Directions, signs, and official documents, including
informed consent forms, social services forms, public health information,
medical instructions, and health education materials, often use jargon and
technical language that make them unnecessarily difficult to use (Rudd et
al., 2000b). In addition, cultural differences may affect perceptions of
health, illness, prevention, and health care. Lack of mutual understanding
of health, illness and treatments, and risks and benefits has implications for
behavior for both providers and consumers, and legal implications for
providers and health systems. Imagine having to face this complexity if you
12 HEALTH LITERACY
are one of the 90 million American adults who lack the functional literacy
skills in English to use the U.S. health care system.2
Health literacy permeates all areas of the provider–consumer informa-
tion exchange, and provides a common pathway for the successful transfer
of information. A number of emerging areas are likely to increase the
burden of limited health literacy on those entering and using the health-care
system. These include demands inherent in chronic disease management,
increased use of new technologies, decreased time for patient/provider dis-
cussions, and legal and regulatory requirements.
Many different interventions and approaches that may hold promise
for addressing limited health literacy are being attempted across health-care
systems, professional organizations, federal and state agencies, educational
institutions, and community and advocacy groups across the United States
and in other countries. Those profiled in the report are indicators of the
creativity and promise for future improvements in countering the effects of
limited health literacy. However, few of these approaches have been for-
mally evaluated, and most are fragmented single approaches rather than
part of a systematic approach to health literacy. In order for progress to be
made, many more systematic demonstrations must be funded and rigor-
ously evaluated.
Finding 6-1 Demands for reading, writing, and numeracy skills are
intensified due to health-care systems’ complexities, advancements in scien-
tific discoveries, and new technologies. These demands exceed the health-
literacy skills of most adults in the United States.
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 13
14 HEALTH LITERACY
Recommendation 2-1 The Department of Health and Human Services and oth-
er government and private funders should support research leading to the devel-
opment of causal models explaining the relationships among health literacy, the
education system, the health system, and relevant social and cultural systems.
Recommendation 2-2 The Department of Health and Human Services and pub-
lic and private funders should support the development, testing, and use of cultur-
ally appropriate new measures of health literacy. Such measures should be devel-
oped for large ongoing population surveys, such as the National Assessment of
Adult Literacy Survey, Medical Expenditure Panel Survey, and Behavioral Risk
Factor Surveillance System, and the Medicare Beneficiaries Survey, as well as for
institutional accreditation and quality assessment activities such as those carried
out by the Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations and the
National Committee for Quality Assurance. Initially, the National Institutes of Health
should convene a national consensus conference to initiate the development of
operational measures of health literacy which would include contextual measures.
Recommendation 3-1 Given the compelling evidence noted above, funding for
health literacy research is urgently needed. The Department of Health and Human
Services, especially the National Institutes of Health, Agency for Healthcare Re-
search and Quality, Health Resources and Services Administration, the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention, Department of Defense, Veterans Administra-
tion, and other public and private funding agencies should support multidisciplinary
research on the extent, associations, and consequences of limited health literacy,
including studies on health service utilization and expenditures.
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 15
Recommendation 4-2 The Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, the
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the Indian Health Service, the Health
Resources and Services Administration, and the Substance Abuse and Mental
Health Services Administration should develop and test approaches to improve
health communication that foster healing relationships across culturally diverse
populations. This includes investigations that explore the effect of existing and
innovative communication approaches on health behaviors, and studies that ex-
amine the impact of participatory action and empowerment research strategies for
effective penetration of health information at the community level.
Recommendation 5-1 Accreditation requirements for all public and private edu-
cational institutions should require the implementation of the National Health Edu-
cation Standards.
Continued
16 HEALTH LITERACY
tive approaches to reducing the negative effects of limited health literacy. To ac-
complish this, these organizations should:
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY 17
REFERENCES
Arnold CL, Davis TC, Berkel HJ, Jackson RH, Nandy I, London S. 2001. Smoking status,
reading level, and knowledge of tobacco effects among low-income pregnant women.
Preventive Medicine. 32(4): 313–320.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Pitkin K, Parikh NS, Coates W, Imara M. 1996. The
health care experience of patients with low literacy. Archives of Family Medicine. 5(6):
329–334.
Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Scott T, Parker RM, Green D, Ren J, Peel J.
2002. Functional health literacy and the risk of hospital admission among Medicare
managed care enrollees. American Journal of Public Health. 92(8): 1278–1283.
Beers BB, McDonald VJ, Quistberg DA, Ravenell KL, Asch DA, Shea JA. 2003. Disparities in
health literacy between African American and non-African American primary care pa-
tients. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 169.
Carmona RH. 2003. Health Literacy in America: The Role of Health Care Professionals.
Prepared Remarks given at the American Medical Association House of Delegates Meet-
ing. Saturday, June 14, 2003. [Online]. Available: http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/news/
speeches/ama061403.htm [accessed: August, 2003].
Doak LG, Doak CC, Meade CD. 1996. Strategies to improve cancer education materials.
Oncology Nursing Forum. 23(8): 1305–1312.
Friedland R. 1998. New estimates of the high costs of inadequate health literacy. In: Proceed-
ings of Pfizer Conference “Promoting Health Literacy: A Call to Action.” October 7–8,
1998, Washington, DC: Pfizer, Inc. Pp. 6–10.
Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Scott T, Greemn DCFSN, Ren J,
Koplan JP. 1999. Health literacy among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organiza-
tion. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6): 545–551.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services.
Houston TK, Allison JJ. 2002. Users of Internet health information: Differences by health
status. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 4(2): E7.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2002. Speaking of Health: Assessing Health Communication
Strategies for Diverse Populations. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2003a. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic
Disparities in Health Care. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, Editors. Washington,
DC: The National Academies Press.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2003b. Priority Areas for National Action: Transforming Health-
care Quality. Adams K, Corrigan JM, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Acad-
emies Press.
Kalichman SC, Rompa D. 2000. Functional health literacy is associated with health status
and health-related knowledge in people living with HIV-AIDS. Journal of Acquired
Immune Deficiency Syndromes and Human Retrovirology. 25(4): 337–344.
Kalichman SC, Benotsch E, Suarez T, Catz S, Miller J, Rompa D. 2000. Health literacy and
health-related knowledge among persons living with HIV/AIDS. American Journal of
Preventive Medicine. 18(4): 325–331.
Kann L, Brener ND, Allensworth DD. 2001. Health education: Results from the School
Health Policies and Programs Study 2000. Journal of School Health. 71(7): 266–278.
Kim SP, Knight SJ, Tomori C, Colella KM, Schoor RA, Shih L, Kuzel TM, Nadler RB,
Bennett CL. 2001. Health literacy and shared decision making for prostate cancer pa-
tients with low socioeconomic status. Cancer Investigation. 19(7): 684–691.
Kirsch IS. 2001. The International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS): Understanding What Was
Measured. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
18 HEALTH LITERACY
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.
Lasater L. 2003. Patient literacy, adherence, and anticoagulation therapy outcomes: A pre-
liminary report. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 179.
National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy. (Harvard School of Public
Health, Department of Society, Human Development and Health). 2003. Voices from
Experience. [Online]. Available: http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/healthliteracy/voices.html
[accessed: December 4, 2003].
Parikh NS, Parker RM, Nurss JR, Baker DW, Williams MV. 1996. Shame and health literacy:
The unspoken connection. Patient Education and Counseling. 27(1): 33–39.
Parker RM, Ratzan SC, Lurie N. 2003. Health literacy: A policy challenge for advancing
high-quality health care. Health Affairs. 22(4): 147.
Ratzan SC, Parker RM. 2000. Introduction. In: National Library of Medicine Current Bibli-
ographies in Medicine: Health Literacy. NLM Pub. No. CBM 2000-1. Selden CR, Zorn
M, Ratzan SC, Parker RM, Editors. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services.
Rudd R, Moeykens BA, Colton TC. 2000a. Health and literacy. A review of medical and
public health literature. In: Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Comings J,
Garners B, Smith C, Editors. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Rudd RE, Colton T, Schacht R. 2000b. An Overview of Medical and Public Health Litera-
ture Addressing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography. Report #14. Cambridge,
MA: National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Schillinger D, Grumbach K, Piette J, Wang F, Osmond D, Daher C, Palacios J, Sullivan GaD,
Bindman AB. 2002. Association of health literacy with diabetes outcomes. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 288(4): 475–482.
Selden CR, Zorn M, Ratzan SC, Parker RM. 2000. National Library of Medicine Current
Bibliographies in Medicine: Health Literacy. NLM Pub. No. CBM 2000-1. Bethesda,
MD: National Institutes of Health, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Shook EV. 1985. Ho’oponopono: Contemporary Uses of a Hawaiian Problem-Solving Pro-
cess. Honolulu, HI: East-West Center, University of Hawaii Press.
Weiss BD, Palmer R. 2004. Relationship between health care costs and very low literacy skills
in a medically needy and indigent Medicaid population. Journal of the American Board
of Family Practice. 17(1): 44–47.
Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, Parikh NS, Pitkin K, Coates WC, Nurss JR. 1995.
Inadequate functional health literacy among patients at two public hospitals. Journal of
the American Medical Association. 274(21): 1677–1682.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Honig EG, Lee TM, Nowlan A. 1998a. Inadequate literacy is a
barrier to asthma knowledge and self-care. Chest. 114(4): 1008–1015.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, Nurss JR. 1998b. Relationship of functional health
literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. A study of patients with hyper-
tension and diabetes. Archives of Internal Medicine. 158(2): 166–172.
Win K, Schillinger D. 2003. Understanding of warfarin therapy and stroke among ethnically
diverse anticoagulation patients at a public hospital. Abstract. Journal of General Inter-
nal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 278.
Introduction
H
ealth consumers face numerous challenges as they seek health
information, including the complexity of the health systems, the
rising burden of chronic disease, the need to engage as partners in
their care, and the proliferation of consumer information available from
numerous and diverse sources. Individuals are asked to assume new roles in
seeking information, advocating for their rights and privacy, understanding
responsibilities, measuring and monitoring their own health and that of
their community, and making decisions about insurance and options for
care. Underlying these complex demands are the varying and sometimes
inadequate levels of, first, consumer knowledge and, second, skills for using
and applying a wide range of health information.
In their work, health care educators and providers make assumptions
about an individual’s ability to comprehend health information. However
over 300 studies have shown that health information cannot be understood
by most of the people for whom it was intended, suggesting that the as-
sumptions regarding the recipient’s level of health literacy made by the
19
20 HEALTH LITERACY
creators of this information are often incorrect. Over the past decade,
concerns related to literacy skills and health provided a wake-up call to
many in the health fields. Health literacy, a newly emerging field of inquiry
and practice, focuses on literacy concerns within the context of health. The
committee defines health literacy as “the degree to which individuals have
the capacity to obtain, process, and understand basic health information
and services needed to make appropriate health decisions” (Ratzan and
Parker, 2000), and views health literacy as a shared function of social and
individual factors. This chapter will provide an overview of health literacy
and this report in a broad societal context; please refer to Chapter 2 for an
in-depth discussion of the definition and conceptual basis of health literacy.
INTRODUCTION 21
22 HEALTH LITERACY
Sometimes the doctors and pharmacy use the type of words that are some-
times hard. I mean sometimes there’s some word that, it’s there, but you don’t
know what it means. They are using those fine words, those college words,
that are hard for people like me to understand and read (Rudd and DeJong,
2000).
When I started six months ago, it was a new job and it was transporting
patients. So we got a dispatcher. We had to call back to the floor and get the
orders where to take the patients. I went looking for about 30 minutes. The
patient and I were lost (Rudd and DeJong, 2000).
INTRODUCTION 23
After being diagnosed with recurrent aphthous stomatitis involving the epithelium
of the buccal mucosa, Winston did what he thought was necessary.
24 HEALTH LITERACY
INTRODUCTION 25
that people may perceive their skills to be adequate when they are not. For
example, data from the 1992 National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) show
that among those scoring in the lowest level on the prose literacy scale, only
29 percent reported they did not read well and only 34 percent reported
they did not write well. The majority of those performing at this level
perceive their reading and writing skills to be adequate. Among those in the
next highest level the results were even more surprising, as only 3 percent
said they couldn’t read well and 6 percent said they couldn’t write well
(Kirsch et al., 1993; see Chapter 2 for more information on the NALS).
To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System is a widely publicized
IOM study on the quality of health care that examined factors to medical
errors (IOM, 2000). One study cited in this report found that 10 percent of
adverse drug events were linked to errors in the use of the drug as a result of
communication failure (Leape et al., 1993). The report noted that manage-
ment of complex drug therapies, especially in elderly patients, is extremely
difficult and requires special attention to the ability of the patient to under-
stand and remember the amount and timing of dose, as well as behavioral
modifications required by the regimen (e.g., dietary restrictions) (IOM,
2000). The ability of patients and consumers to manage their own health
and medical care can be improved through better provider–patient commu-
nication and greater inclusion of the patient in treatment decisions, as
discussed in Chapter 6 (Hausman, 2001; Hayes-Bautista, 1976; Hulka et
al., 1975, 1976; Miller, 1975; Snyder et al., 1976).
26 HEALTH LITERACY
INTRODUCTION 27
28 HEALTH LITERACY
INTRODUCTION 29
guide health policy, and others. Current issues facing health care are dis-
cussed as they relate to health literacy. The chapter also reviews various
types of approaches that may help to address the problem of limited health
literacy when applied in health care settings.
Chapter 7 presents the committee’s vision for a health literate society.
REFERENCES
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Pitkin K, Parikh NS, Coates W, Imara M. 1996. The
health care experience of patients with low literacy. Archives of Family Medicine. 5(6):
329–334.
Barton D, Hamilton M. 1998. Local Literacies: Reading and Writing in One Community.
New York: Routledge.
Carrasquillo O, Orav EJ, Brennan TA, Burstin HR. 1999. Impact of language barriers on
patient satisfaction in an emergency department. Journal of General Internal Medicine.
14(2): 82–87.
Flores G, Laws MB, Mayo SJ, Zuckerman B, Abreu M, Medina L, Hardt EJ. 2003. Errors in
medical interpretation and their potential clinical consequences in pediatric encounters.
Pediatrics. 111(1): 6–14.
Grossman M, Kaestner R. 1997. The effects of education on health. In: The Social Benefits of
Education. Behermean R, Stacey N, Editors. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
Press. Pp. 69–123.
Hausman A. 2001. Taking your medicine: Relational steps to improving patient compliance.
Health Marketing Quarterly. 19(2): 49–71.
Hayes-Bautista DE. 1976. Modifying the treatment: Patient compliance, patient control and
medical care. Social Science & Medicine. 10(5): 233–238.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services.
House JS, Lepkowski JM, Kinney AM, Mero RP, Kessler RC, Herzog AR. 1994. The social
stratification of aging and health. Journal of Health & Social Behavior. 35: 213–234.
Hulka BS, Kupper LL, Cassel JC, Mayo F. 1975. Doctor-patient communication and out-
comes among diabetic patients. Journal of Community Health. 1(1): 15–27.
Hulka BS, Cassel JC, Kupper LL, Burdette JA. 1976. Communication, compliance, and con-
cordance between physicians and patients with prescribed medications. American Jour-
nal of Public Health. 66(9): 847–853.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2000. To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Wash-
ington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2001. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Wash-
ington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2002. Speaking of Health: Assessing Health Communication Strategies for Diverse
Populations. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
IOM. 2003a. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care.
Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies
Press.
IOM. 2003b. Priority Areas for National Action: Transforming Healthcare Quality. Adams
K, Corrigan JM, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Kirsch IS. 2001. The International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS): Understanding What Was
Measured. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
30 HEALTH LITERACY
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.
Leape LL, Lawthers AG, Brennan TA, Johnson WG. 1993. Preventing medical injury. Qual-
ity Review Bulletin. 19(5): 144–149.
Miller WD. 1975. Drug usage: Compliance of patients with instructions on medication. Jour-
nal of the American Osteopathic Association. 75(4): 401–404.
Pamuk E, Makuc D, Heck K, Rueben C, Lochner K. 1998. Socioeconomic Status and Health
Chartbook. Health, United States, 1998. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health
Statistics.
Parikh NS, Parker RM, Nurss JR, Baker DW, Williams MV. 1996. Shame and health literacy:
The unspoken connection. Patient Education and Counseling. 27(1): 33–39.
Parker RM, Ratzan SC, Lurie N. 2003. Health literacy: A policy challenge for advancing
high-quality health care. Health Affairs. 22(4): 147.
Purcell-Gates V, Degener S, Jacobson E, Soler M. 2000. Affecting Change in Literacy Prac-
tices of Adult Learners: Impact of Two Dimensions of Instruction. NCSALL Report No.
17. Boston, MA: National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Ratzan SC, Parker RM. 2000. Introduction. In: National Library of Medicine Current Bibli-
ographies in Medicine: Health Literacy. Selden CR, Zorn M, Ratzan SC, Parker RM,
Editors. NLM Pub. No. CBM 2000-1. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services.
Roter DL, Rudd RE, Comings J. 1998. Patient literacy: A barrier to quality of care. Journal of
General Internal Medicine. 13(12): 850–851.
Rudd, R. 2002. Health and Literacy: Recalibrating the Norm. Paper presented at the Ameri-
can Public Health Association Annual Conference, Philadelphia, PA.
Rudd R, Moeykens BA, Colton TC. 2000. Health and literacy: A review of medical and
public health literature. In: Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Comings J,
Garners B, Smith C, Editors. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Rudd RE, DeJong W. 2000. In Plain Language: The Need for Effective Communication in
Medicine and Public Health [Video]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Rudd RE, Comings JP, Hyde J. 2003. Leave no one behind: Improving health and risk
communication through attention to literacy. Journal of Health Communication, Special
Supplement on Bioterrorism. 8(Supplement 1): 104–115.
Sarver J, Baker DW. 2000. Effect of language barriers on follow-up appointments after an
emergency department visit. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 15(4): 256–264.
Snyder D, Lynch JJ, Gruss L. 1976. Doctor-patient communications in a private family prac-
tice. Journal of Family Practice. 3(3): 271–276.
Street B. 2001. Literacy and Development: Ethnographic Perspectives. New York: Routledge.
U.S. Agency for International Development. 2001. DRAFT “Communication activity autho-
rization document.”
Watters EK. 2003. Literacy for health: An interdisciplinary model. Journal of Transcultural
Nursing. 14(1): 48–54.
WHO (World Health Organization). 2000. Health Promotion. Report by the Secretariat.
World Health Organization.
31
32 HEALTH LITERACY
The degree to which individuals have the capacity to obtain, process, and
understand basic health information and services needed to make appro-
priate health decisions (Ratzan and Parker, 2000).
The capacity of the individual is a substantial contributor to health
literacy. The term “capacity” refers to both the innate potential of the
individual, as well as his or her skills. An individual’s health literacy capac-
ity is mediated by education, and its adequacy is affected by culture, lan-
guage, and the characteristics of health-related settings. In this report, the
committee has captured the range of environments and situations related to
health in the term “health context”. The health context includes the media,
the marketplace, and government agencies, as well as those individuals and
materials a person interacts with regarding health—all must be able to
provide basic health information in an appropriate manner (Rudd, 2003).
This health context is of equal importance to individuals’ health literacy
skills, as the impact of health literacy arises from the interaction of the
individual and the health context (Rudd, 2003; Rudd et al., 2003). Health
literacy, then, is a shared function of cultural, social, and individual factors.
Both the causes and the remedies for limited health literacy rest with our
cultural and social framework, the health and education systems that serve
it, and the interactions between these factors.
Health
Literacy Health
Contexts
Outcomes
and Costs
Health
Literacy
Individuals
34 HEALTH LITERACY
Education
System
3
FIGURE 2-2 Potential points for intervention in the health literacy framework.
provide opportunities for individuals who drop out of K-12 education for
academic or social reasons, for those who completed high school but did
not acquire strong skills, for elders who did not have full schooling oppor-
tunities, and for adult immigrants who may never have had access to educa-
tion and/or wish to learn to speak, read, and write English. Individuals with
college-level education or higher frequently have adequate literacy skills,
and generally are not discussed in this report. Formative and continuing
education for health professionals is also considered within the context of
education.
Finding 2-2 The links between education and health outcomes are
strongly established. The committee concludes that health literacy may be
one pathway explaining the well-established link between education and
health, and warrants further exploration.
36 HEALTH LITERACY
noted, Healthy People 2010, the document that reports the federal govern-
ment’s national health objectives, defines health literacy as “the degree to
which individuals have the capacity to obtain, process, and understand
basic health information and services needed to make appropriate health
decisions” (HHS, 2000; Ratzan and Parker, 2000). This definition is useful
because it encompasses the variety of contexts within which individuals
may confront and interact with health issues. As with a number of the other
definitions discussed above, however, it focuses attention on and appears to
limit the problem of health literacy to the capacity and competence of the
individual. This limitation is acknowledged and addressed in the action
plan for the Healthy People 2010 health literacy objective, which expands
the definition to include system-level contributions (Rudd, 2003). Recog-
nizing the limitations of this definition, the committee acknowledges the
need for future development of definitions and measures that address the
critical role that society, the health system, and the education system play in
creating a truly health-literate America.
DEFINITION OF LITERACY
Educators do not associate literacy with reading alone, but often con-
sider literacy to represent a constellation of skills including reading, writ-
ing, basic mathematical calculations, and speech and speech comprehension
skills (Kirsch, 2001a). Speech and speech comprehension are collectively
termed oral literacy, while reading and writing are referred to as print
literacy. For our discussion in this report, we further differentiate among
the following terms: basic print literacy, literacy for different types of text,
and functional literacy. Basic print literacy ability means the ability to read,
write, and understand written language that is familiar and for which one
has the requisite amount of background knowledge. Reading or text lit-
eracy is related to characteristics of the text being read such as complexity
and format. Functional literacy is the use of literacy in order to perform a
particular task. We note that health literacy has been variously defined, but
as currently used and measured, often consists of reading or text literacy
(see below for further discussion). Figure 2-3 below illustrates the relation-
ships between the different contributors to literacy.
As illustrated above in Figure 2-3, a consideration of health literacy
must include component parts directly related to the broad concept of
literacy. Literacy, as noted earlier, is context specific. For example, literacy
could be placed within the multiple health contexts noted earlier. In this
case, the construct includes cultural and conceptual knowledge that could
include an understanding of health and illness and a conceptualization of
risks and benefits. Listening and speaking skills are essential for public
health communication, the commercial sector’s advertising goals, and for
38 HEALTH LITERACY
Contexts
Literacy
Oral Print
Literacy Literacy
40 HEALTH LITERACY
The May thyroid tests showed TSH 2.794 µU/ml, which, though “normal,” is
too high for someone who has had prior thyroid carcinoma. Keeping TSH
between 0.1 – 0.3 µU/ml minimizes recurrence of thyroid cancer. Free T4
1.60 mg% is a high-normal level.
I suggest you increase L-thyroxine from 150 mcg 7 days a week to 150 mcg
5 days a week and 225 mcg (11⁄2 tablets) Wednesdays and Sundays weekly.
Have a repeat TSH, free T4 and total T3 in 8 weeks. I should also on that
occasion like you to have a serum plasma metanephrine level.
Two weeks after having those tests, please see me for a consultative office
visit.
Sincerely yours,
John Doe, M.D.
Endocrinology
42 HEALTH LITERACY
Promote and protect • read and follow guidelines for physical activity
health and prevent • read, comprehend, and make decisions based on food and
disease product labels
• make sense of air quality reports and modify behavior as
needed
• find health information on the internet or in periodicals
and books
Rep. Rosa L. DeLauro (D-Conn.) led the effort to mandate an FDA education
effort on hormone therapies. “I’m pretty well-informed about these things,
and I didn’t know what to do,” she said. “In the absence of clear information,
it can get pretty scary for women.” (Kaufman, 2003)
Literacy Surveys
Assessments of adult literacy conducted since the late 1980s have fo-
cused on functional literacy and numeracy as outlined by the National
Literacy Act of 1991.1 This act defines literacy as “the ability to read, write,
and speak in English, and compute and solve problems at levels of profi-
ciency necessary to function on the job and in society, to achieve one’s
goals, and develop one’s knowledge and potential.” This definition was
applied to the development of the national assessments of adult literacy in
the United States and other industrialized nations. The surveys measured
three of the five accepted components of literacy: reading, writing, and
mathematical calculations (or numeracy). Oral language skills, including
speaking and listening, were not assessed for the national studies, in part
because of time constraints and a possible burden on participants (Kirsch,
1P.L. 102-73, The National Literacy Act of 1991. 102nd Congress, 1st Session. H.R. 751.
44 HEALTH LITERACY
2001a). The Young Adult Literacy Survey (performed in 1985) (Kirsch and
Jungeblut, 1986), the Department of Labor Survey (1990) (Kirsch and
Jungeblut, 1992), the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS) (1992)
(Campbell et al., 1992), and the International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS)
(the initial study was performed in 1994–1998) (Kirsch, 2001a) all focus on
the ability to use print materials to accomplish a task. These task-oriented
assessments differ in complexity from basic literacy assessments that focus
on the ability to recognize or pronounce words, or to read and comprehend
text written specifically for test purposes.
Materials for these surveys were drawn from six contexts in order to
represent literacy tasks from everyday life: home and family, health and
safety, community and citizenship, consumer economics, work, and leisure
and recreation. Materials included both continuous and noncontinuous
texts. Continuous texts or prose, which is the term used in these large-scale
assessments, are typically composed of sentences that are, in turn, orga-
nized into paragraphs. These paragraphs are used to form larger structures
such as stories, newspaper or magazine articles, and even sections or chap-
ters in a book. A common way of organizing continuous texts is by their
rhetorical structure. These might include: narratives, exposition, descrip-
tion, argumentation, instructions, or a document and record. Noncontinu-
ous texts or documents as they are referred to involve the display of infor-
mation using other structures or formats. These might include tables, charts,
graphs, entry forms, maps, and diagrams. They have been described by
Mosenthal and Kirsch (1998) and Kirsch (2001b). These materials range in
both length and complexity. Some prose materials are very short such as a
brief sports article or letter. Others are more lengthy and complex such as
an editorial. Documents, too, range in length and complexity such as a
social security card on which someone has to enter their signature to a
complex table showing the results of a survey or an embedded bus schedule.
NALS scores were based on people’s ability to accomplish tasks using
printed texts. The difficulty of each task was related to three variables: type
of match, type of information, and plausibility of distracting information
(Kirsch, 2001a). Four types of matching strategies were identified: locating,
cycling, integrating, and generating.
The tasks, in ascending order of difficulty, included:
46 HEALTH LITERACY
48 HEALTH LITERACY
0.84 with the REALM, and a high reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.98;
Parker et al., 1995).
For time considerations, the TOFHLA was reduced to an abbreviated
version called the S-TOFHLA that takes 12 minutes or less to administer
(Baker et al., 1999). It consists of a reading comprehension section contain-
ing a 36-item test using the initial two passages in the reading comprehen-
sion section of the full TOFHLA—instructions for preparation for an upper
gastrointestinal series and the patient “Rights and Responsibilities” section
of a Medicaid application. It also contains a shortened 4-item measure of
numeracy. The S-TOFHLA has been shown to have good internal consis-
tency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.98 for all items combined) and
concurrent validity compared to the long version of the TOFHLA (r = 0.91)
and the REALM (r = 0.80). Both the TOFHLA and the S-TOFHLA are
available in English and in Spanish. The Spanish and English versions were
developed simultaneously and use the same standard of measurement.
Additional measures continue to be developed, including a measure of
health literacy in Veterans Administration hospital populations based on
the S-TOFHLA (Chew and Bradley, 2003), a literacy test for patients with
diabetes (Nath et al., 2001), and a functional test of ability to maintain a
medication regimen (Edelberg et al., 1998, 1999, 2001).
The use of these tests of literacy for printed material in the health
context has enabled medical researchers to explore differences among vari-
ous health-related outcomes for patients based on approximations of pa-
tients’ health literacy as indicated by patients’ reading skills for health
materials. As a result, a growing body of research has shown that limited
reading and/or numeracy skills reduce access to health information and
preventive services, reduce understanding of illness and disease, regimens
and medications, and increase outcomes such as hospitalization or decrease
outcomes such as disease management markers. This research is discussed
in detail in Chapter 3.
50 HEALTH LITERACY
yond the scope of what was feasible at the time these surveys were carried
out.
Oral language skills are of critical concern for public health and health
care. Public health and risk communication rely on a variety of channels
and media, including oral communication, to convey health promotion and
protection information, as well as for local and national alerts. Health-care
encounters rely on a dialogue between patient and provider that allow the
provider to understand symptoms, follow the course of an illness or disease
as experienced by the patient, and provide diagnosis and treatment options.
Patients are expected to tell their stories, describe their experiences, provide
explanations, and obtain help with needed action. Given the importance of
speech and speech comprehension skills to health literacy, the current mea-
sures of health literacy that are modeled on previous functional literacy
assessment tools do not tap the full scope of health literacy.
Grade-Level Scores
Grade-level scores are problematic because they require an interpreta-
tion of assessments for the level of ability for individual readers. The use of
grade level as a meaningful norm-referenced score is so problematic that the
International Reading Association recommended that it not be used for K-
12 (Joint Task Force on Assessment, 1994). Its use in adults for whom there
are no “grades” from which to extrapolate from a mean score on a norm-
referenced test is even more inaccurate.
tasks involved in the use of the materials. Although both the TOFHLA and
the S-TOFHLA include numeracy tasks, the REALM does not examine
numeracy, and none of these assessments examine writing. The contents of
the REALM and the TOFHLA focus on medical terms or materials found in
medical settings and do not represent the broad spectrum of health literacy
materials and processes that occur outside the clinical setting, limiting the
conclusions that can be drawn. As discussed above, neither considers oral
language skills.
Researchers using existing measures of health literacy have been able
to establish differences in health-related outcome measures for patients
based on differences in test scores. While the existing measures do not fully
capture the construct of health literacy, they have provided information
about vulnerable populations. Although the existing body of research is
revealing and suggestive, generalization of results to large populations is
limited. Each study focuses on a specific population, which differs from
others along relevant dimensions such as age, socioeconomic status, eth-
nicity, and primary languages. Also many studies use different measure-
ment tools and in some cases the researchers modified the scoring of these
tools. Better measures are needed if we are to be able to align our under-
standing of the distribution of health literacy with the development of
intervention strategies.
Finding 2-4 While health literacy measures in current use have spurred
research initiatives and yield valuable insights, they are indicators of read-
ing skills (word recognition or reading comprehension and numeracy),
rather than measures of the full range of skills needed for health literacy
(cultural and conceptual knowledge, listening, speaking, numeracy, writing
and reading). Current assessment tools and research findings cannot differ-
entiate among (1) reading ability, (2) lack of background knowledge in
health-related domains, such as biology, (3) lack of familiarity with lan-
guage and types of materials, and (4) cultural differences in approaches to
health and health care. In addition, no current measures of health literacy
include oral communication skills or writing skills and none measure the
health literacy demands on individuals within different health contexts.
52 HEALTH LITERACY
54 HEALTH LITERACY
Finding 2-2 The links between education and health outcomes are strongly es-
tablished. The committee concludes that health literacy may be one pathway ex-
plaining the well-established link between education and health, and warrants fur-
ther exploration.
Finding 2-3 Health literacy, as defined in this report, includes a variety of compo-
nents beyond reading and writing, including numeracy, listening, speaking, and
relies on cultural and conceptual knowledge.
Finding 2-4 While health literacy measures in current use have spurred research
initiatives and yield valuable insights, they are indicators of reading skills (word
recognition or reading comprehension and numeracy), rather than measures of
the full range of skills needed for health literacy (cultural and conceptual knowl-
edge, listening, speaking, numeracy, writing and reading). Current assessment
tools and research findings cannot differentiate among (1) reading ability, (2) lack
of background knowledge in health-related domains, such as biology, (3) lack of
familiarity with language and types of materials, and (4) cultural differences in
approaches to health and health care. In addition, no current measures of health
literacy include oral communication skills or writing skills and none measure the
health-literacy demands on individuals within different health contexts.
Recommendation 2-1 The Department of Health and Human Services and oth-
er government and private funders should support research leading to the devel-
opment of causal models explaining the relationships among health literacy, the
education system, the health system, and relevant social and cultural systems.
Recommendation 2-2 The Department of Health and Human Services and pub-
lic and private funders should support the development, testing, and use of cultur-
ally appropriate new measures of health literacy. Such measures should be devel-
oped for large ongoing population surveys, such as the NAAL Survey, the MEPS,
the BHRFSS, and the MBS, as well as for institutional accreditation and quality
assessment activities such as those carried out by the Joint Commission on Ac-
creditation of Healthcare Organizations and the National Committee for Quality
Assurance. Initially, NIH should convene a national consensus conference to ini-
tiate the development of operational measures of health literacy that would include
contextual measures.
REFERENCES
Adler NE, Ostrove JM. 1999. Socioeconomic status and health: What we know and what we
don’t. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 896: 3–15.
Alexander M. 1990. Informed consent, psychological stress and noncompliance. Humane
Medicine. 6(2): 113–119.
American Medical Association. 1999. Health literacy: Report of the Council on Scientific
Affairs. Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy for the Council on Scientific Affairs,
American Medical Association. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6):
552–557.
Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. 1999. Development of a
brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Education and Counseling. 38:
33–42.
56 HEALTH LITERACY
Berkman LF, Kawachi IO. 2000. Social Epidemiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Blanchard JS, Garcia HS, Carter RM. 1989. Instrumento Para Diagnosticar Lecturas
(Español-English): Instrument for the Diagnosis of Reading. Dubuque, IA: Kendall-
Hunt.
Campbell A, Kirsch IS, Kolstad A. 1992. Assessing Literacy: The Framework for the National
Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S.
Department of Education.
Chew LD, Bradley KA. 2003. Brief questions to detect inadequate health literacy among VA
patients. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 170.
Davis TC, Long SW, Jackson RH, Mayeaux EJ, George RB, Murphy PW, Crouch MA. 1993.
Rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine: A shortened screening instrument. Family
Medicine. 25(6): 391–395.
Davis TC, Michielutte R, Askov EN, Williams MV, Weiss BD. 1998. Practical assessment of
adult literacy in health care. Health Education and Behavior. 25(5): 613–624.
Doak CC, Doak LG, Root J. 1996. Teaching Patients with Low Literacy Skills. 2nd edition.
New York: Lippincott.
Doak CC, Doak LG, Friedell GH, Meade CD. 1998. Improving comprehension for cancer
patients with low literacy skills: Strategies for clinicians. Ca: A Cancer Journal for Clini-
cians. 48(3): 151–162.
Doak LG, Doak CC, Meade CD. 1996. Strategies to improve cancer education materials.
Oncology Nursing Forum. 23(8): 1305–1312.
Dumas RG. 1966. Utilization of stress as a therapeutic nursing measure. Utilization of a
concept of stress as a basis for nursing practice. ANA Clinical Sessions. 193–212.
Edelberg HK, Shallenberger E, Hausdorff JM, Wei JY. 1998. Application of the DRUGS tool
to assess function in ambulatory elderly. Abstract. Journal of the American Geriatrics
Society. 46(9): S103.
Edelberg HK, Shallenberger E, Wei JY. 1999. Medication management capacity in highly
functioning community-living older adults: Detection of early deficits. Journal of the
American Geriatrics Society. 47(5): 592–596.
Edelberg HK, Rubin RN, Palmieri JJ, Leipzig RM. 2001. Preliminary validation of the Drug
Regimen Unassisted Grading Scale (DRUGS) in community dwelling older adults. Jour-
nal of the American Geriatrics Society. 49(4): S65–S66.
Flesch R. 1974. The Art of Readable Writing. New York: Harper and Row.
Fry EB. 1977. Elementary Reading Instruction. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Grossman M, Kaestner R. 1997. The effects of education on health. In: The Social Benefits of
Education. Behermean R, Stacey N, Editors. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
Press. Pp. 69–123.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services.
Jastak S, Wilkinson GS. 1984. Wide Range Achievement Test–Revised (WRAT-R). Jastak
Assessment Systems.
Joint Committee on National Health Education Standards. 1995. National Health Education
Standards: Achieving Health Literacy. Atlanta, GA: American Cancer Society.
Joint Task Force on Assessment, International Reading Association and National Council of
Teachers of English. 1994. Standards for the Assessment of Reading and Writing. New-
ark, DE: International Reading Association.
Kaufman M. 2003, September. FDA Offers Guidance on Hormone Therapy. The Washington
Post.
Kerka S. 2000. Health and Adult Literacy. Practice Application Brief No. 7. Columbus, OH:
ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and Vocational Education.
Kickbusch I. 1997. Think health: What makes the difference? Health Promotion Interna-
tional. 12: 265–272.
Kirsch IS. 2001a. The framework used in developing and interpreting the International Adult
Literacy Survey (IALS). European Journal of Psychology of Education. 16(3): 335–361.
Kirsch IS. 2001b. The International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS): Understanding What Was
Measured. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A. 1986. Literacy: Profiles of America’s Young Adults. Princeton, NJ:
Educational Testing Service.
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A. 1992. Profiling the Literacy Proficiencies of JTPA and ES/UI Popula-
tions: Final Report to the Department of Labor. Washington, DC: Employment and
Training Administration.
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.
Markwardt FS. 1989. Peabody Individual Achievement Test–Revised. Circle Pines, MN:
American Guidance Service.
McLaughlin GH. 1969. SMOG grading: A new readability formula. Journal of Reading.
12(8): 639–646.
Meade CD. 2001. Community health education. In: Community Health Nursing: Promoting
the Health of Aggregates. 3rd edition. Nies M, McEwen M, Editors. Philadelphia: W.B.
Saunders Co.
Mosenthal PB, Kirsch IS. 1998. A new measure for assessing document complexity: The
PMOSE/IKIRSCH document readability formula. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Lit-
eracy. 41(8): 638–657.
Nath CR, Sylvester ST, Yasek V, Gunel E. 2001. Development and validation of a literacy
assessment tool for persons with diabetes. Diabetes Educator. 27(6): 857–864.
National Center for Education Statistics. 2003. NAAL 2003: Overview. [Online]. Available:
http://nces.ed.gov/naal/design/about02.asp [accessed: December, 2003].
Nurss JR, Baker D, Davis T, Parker R, Williams M. 1995. Difficulty in functional health
literacy screening in Spanish-speaking adults. Journal of Reading. 38: 632–637.
Pamuk E, Makuc D, Heck K, Rueben C, Lochner K. 1998. Socioeconomic Status and Health
Chartbook. Health, United States, 1998. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health
Statistics.
Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. 1995. The Test of Functional Health Lit-
eracy in Adults: A new instrument for measuring patients’ literacy skills. Journal of
General Internal Medicine. 10(10): 537–541.
Ratzan SC, Parker RM. 2000. Introduction. In: National Library of Medicine Current Bibli-
ographies in Medicine: Health Literacy. Selden CR, Zorn M, Ratzan SC, Parker RM,
Editors. NLM Pub. No. CBM 2000-1. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services.
Roter DL, Stashefsky-Margalit R, Rudd R. 2001. Current perspectives on patient education
in the U.S. Patient Education and Counseling. 44(1): 79–86.
Rudd R. 2003. Objective 11-2: Improvement of health literacy. In: Communicating Health:
Priorities and Strategies for Progress. Washington, DC: Office of Disease Prevention and
Health Promotion, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Rudd RE, Colton T, Schacht R. 2000. An Overview of Medical and Public Health Literature
Addressing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography. Report #14. Cambridge, MA:
National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Rudd RE, Comings JP, Hyde J. 2003. Leave no one behind: Improving health and risk
communication through attention to literacy. Journal of Health Communication, Special
Supplement on Bioterrorism. 8(Supplement 1): 104–115.
58 HEALTH LITERACY
Slosson RJL. 1990. Slosson Oral Reading Tests—Revised. East Aurora, NY: Slosson Educa-
tional Publishers.
Taylor WL. 1953. “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism
Quarterly. 30: 415–433.
Williams DR. 1990. Socioeconomic differentials in health: A review and redirection. Social
Psychology Quarterly. 53: 81–99.
T
his report has already noted that an individuals’ health literacy level
is the product of a complex set of skills and interactions on the part
of the individual, the health-care system, the education system, and
the cultural and societal context. It has also been noted that most individu-
als will encounter health literacy barriers at some point. High educational
attainment may not be sufficient to negotiate medical and technical lan-
guage and meanings. The following chapters discuss in more detail some of
the barriers and potential approaches to health literacy in the various con-
texts. Here, the focus is on the individual, and particularly, on the indi-
vidual who has limited literacy skills. This chapter provides an overview of
how limited health literacy may restrict an individual’s participation in
health contexts and activities. Look at Figure 3-1 on the following page and
put yourself in the shoes of a patient with limited literacy skills. How would
you feel, what would you do, where would you go, to whom would you
turn if this medication is prescribed for you by your doctor? How compli-
ant could you be about taking your medications correctly?
Navigating modern life in America with very limited literacy can be like
trying to find a hotel in another land, armed with a map of the city but
unable to decipher the letters on the street signs. Everyone—not just those
with limited literacy skills—is increasingly faced with difficult and confus-
ing text at work, at home, in institutional settings such as schools, banks,
social service organizations, and within health-care settings. People of all
literacy levels might be able to manage texts that they frequently encounter
59
60 HEALTH LITERACY
and use for everyday activities, but will often face problems with unfamiliar
types of text. For example, a woman who has never lived near a public
transportation system may find herself unable to interpret a bus schedule.
Directions for operating a particle accelerator, filing income tax returns, or
choosing between health insurance plans may be similarly indecipherable
for most adults, regardless of literacy skills in other contexts.
As discussed in detail in Chapter 2, current measures of health literacy
rely primarily on print in the health context and not on the broad array of
skills needed for true health literacy. However, since skills with the written
word are linked to skills with the spoken word, we can use information
from these measures as a starting point to make reasonable assumptions
about the average health literacy skill level of adults in the United States.
The following section examines the extent of the problem of health literacy,
estimated from existing measures of literacy based on the National Adult
Literacy Survey (NALS) and from assessments of health literacy.
LITERACY IN AMERICA
About 90 million (47 percent) U.S. adults cannot accurately and consis-
tently locate, match, and integrate information from newspapers, advertise-
ments, or forms (Kirsch et al., 1993). These adults can perform a variety of
62 HEALTH LITERACY
the numbers and operations are found in familiar and uncomplicated mate-
rials. However, adults at Level 2 find it difficult to perform these operations
in difficult text and to perform operations that are complicated by distract-
ing information and complex texts (Morse, 2002). In addition, they will
find the demands of the chart to determine dosage for children’s cold
medicine difficult and, according to studies assessing informed consent
documents, will find the process of informed consent arduous and most
likely not possible.
Most of the adults in NALS Levels 1 and 2 are “literate”; however,
adults in Level 1 are at a severe disadvantage and adults in Level 2 are
disadvantaged, in relation to the demands of twenty-first century life. These
findings have serious implications for the health sectors. Rudd, Kirsch, and
Yamamoto, in a reanalysis of the NALS with a focus on health-related tasks
only, report similar findings (Rudd et al., 2003). The 1992 NALS survey
provides the most recent nationally representative population survey data
on literacy skills of adults in the United States. The committee believes that
levels of American literacy have not improved over the past decade and that
health systems have become more complex. The committee looks forward
to the publication of the National Assessment of Adult Literacy1 (NAAL),
conducted in 2003, which contains health-related literacy tasks. The com-
mittee believes that the NAAL will significantly expand our understanding
of literacy and health literacy in America, and regrets that the data are not
yet available. In addition, a representative sample of American adults is
included in the new international Adult Literacy & Lifeskills Survey (ALL).2
Linked to the NAAL framework, the ALL also contains health-related lit-
eracy tasks. These two surveys have the potential to provide detailed infor-
mation on the extent of limited health literacy in America.
1For more information, see the NAAL on the National Center for Education Statistics web
site: http://nces.ed.gov/naal/.
2For more information, see ALL on the Educational Testing Service web site: http://www.
ets.org/all.
and western regions of the United States, those with less than a high school
degree or GED, and those who are above the age of 65. It is important to
keep in mind that the NALS was performed only in English, and that clear
differentials for literacy proficiencies can be seen within each population
group on the basis of nativity, education, and access to economic resources
(Kirsch et al., 1993). Table 3-1 displays the percentage of persons overall
and various demographic groups that have NALS Level 1 or 2 literacy
skills, the lowest of five skill levels. Each of these groups is discussed briefly
below in order to highlight those that could potentially benefit from inter-
ventions aimed at improving health literacy.3
Adults over the age of 65 have more limited literacy proficiency than
younger, working adults, according to the NALS data. However, cross-
tabulations indicate that scores for elders vary by education level and access
to financial resources (Kirsch et al., 1993). Hispanics, African Americans,
Pacific Islanders, and Native Americans are also (including Alaska Natives)
over-represented in the numbers of adults with lower literacy proficiency
scores, as indicated by the NALS data (Kirsch et al., 1993). These popula-
tions have increased in number since 1992, when the NALS was performed,
and as a group represent a larger proportion of the U.S. population (U.S.
Census Bureau, 2002). Hispanic and Asian populations in particular have
increased at a greater rate than other U.S. populations. While more recent
literacy data is not yet available, these population increases may represent
an increase in the number of individuals, and the percentage of American
adults, affected by limited health literacy.
Individuals without a high school diploma or GED have lower levels of
literacy proficiency than do those with a high school diploma or education
beyond high school. Nearly all adults who did not finish eighth grade
scored at Level 1 or 2 on the NALS, and 77 percent of these individuals
scored at Level 1. Similarly, among individuals who entered high school but
did not graduate, 81 percent scored at NALS Levels 1 or 2, while only 55
percent of high school graduates scored at those levels (Kirsch et al., 1993).
Since between 400,000 and 500,000 students drop out of high school each
year in the United States (Young, 2002), high school dropouts constitute a
large population likely living without adequate literacy skills.
Overall, more than 70 percent of immigrants tested on the NALS scored
at Levels 1 or 2. The NALS was conducted in English only. Thus the finding
that 25 percent of those scoring in the lowest levels of literacy proficiency
were immigrants to the United States might be expected, since many of
3The information in this section is drawn in part from the background paper “Outside the
Clinician-Patient Relationship: A Call to Action For Health Literacy,” commissioned by the
committee from Barry D. Weiss, M.D. The committee appreciates his contributions. The full
text of the paper can be found in Appendix B.
64 HEALTH LITERACY
Age
16–54 years 15 28 57
55–64 years 28 33 39
65 years and older 49 32 19
Racial/Ethnic Group
White 15 26 59
American Indian/Alaska Native 26 38 36
Asian/Pacific Islander 35 25 40
Black 41 36 23
Hispanic (all groups) 52 26 22
Disability
Any mental or emotional condition 48 26 27
Learning disability 59 22 19
Hearing difficulty 35 34 32
Speech disability 54 27 19
Visual difficulty 55 26 19
SOURCE: Unadjusted averages of prose and document literacy scores on the NALS as re-
ported on Tables 1.1A, 1.1B, 1.2A, 1.2B, 1.8, and Figure 1.10 in Kirsch et al. (1993) and on
Table B3.13 in the U.S. Department of Education’s report English Literacy and Language
Minorities in the United States (2001).
had not finished high school in their country of origin (Greenberg et al.,
2001).
Individuals who described themselves as having a physical or mental
condition which prevented them from participating fully in normal activi-
ties tended to score at the lowest levels of proficiency. More than half the
individuals with vision, speech, or learning disabilities performed at NALS
Level 1, as did 48 percent of individuals reporting a mental or emotional
condition. In contrast, 35 percent of individuals reporting a hearing diffi-
culty performed at NALS Level 1.
Approximately 7 of 10 prisoners surveyed by the NALS demonstrated
limited literacy proficiency. Individuals who did not finish school are over-
represented among prison inmates, so in many cases, prisoners represent
the same individuals as those with limited education. NALS investigators
studied nearly 1,150 inmates in 80 federal and state prisons that had been
randomly selected to represent penal institutions across the country (Haig-
ler, 1994).
Several other groups, not specifically studied in the NALS, are also
known to have limited literacy skills. These groups include the persons who
are poor and/or homeless, and military recruits. As discussed in Chapter 2,
social factors affect literacy. Poverty is intertwined with many sociodemo-
graphic variables (Balsa and McGuire, 2001), which, in turn, are associated
with limited literacy. Although the causal relationships are not known,
individuals with limited incomes/access to resources are also more likely
than those with higher incomes/access to resources to have lower literacy
proficiency (Kirsch et al., 1993). Homeless individuals, who may also be
affected by limited literacy and poverty, are the audience recipients of a
variety of state programs developed to enhance literacy skills (Profiles of
State Programs, 1990). Weiss and colleagues used Medicaid enrollment as a
proxy for low income and administered the Instrument for the Diagnosis of
Reading/Instrumento Para Diagnosticar Lectura (IDL) as a measure of lit-
eracy. They found that the majority of low-income individuals involved in
their study had limited reading skills, with the average score at the fifth-
grade level (Weiss et al., 1994). In addition, reading and mathematics skills
of potential military recruits are assessed, and enlistment in the military
requires passing these tests. As many recruits cannot adequately perform
the high school level reading tasks they face in the military, all branches of
the military operate educational programs to increase the literacy skills of
their members (Hegerfeld, 1999).
66 HEALTH LITERACY
can society (Comings et al., 2001; Sum et al., 2002). Basic literacy skills
that can be applied in the context of health are required for health literacy.
As discussed in Chapter 2, there are no assessment tools that fully measure
health literacy, and consequently, no population-based study to date has
directly examined the relationship of literacy to health literacy or of health
literacy, as fully defined, to health. However, correlations between mea-
sures of literacy and measures of reading in the health context such as the
Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM) and the Short Test
of Functional Health Literacy in Adults (S-TOFHLA) suggest a strong asso-
ciation (Davis et al., 1993; Parker et al., 1995).
Routine health and health-care tasks are often complicated and may
require more literacy skills than those needed to meet the demands of
everyday life. To date, no researcher has fully delineated tasks needed for
the full range of health-related activities nor has anyone fully calibrated the
levels of complexity of the various types of materials used in health con-
texts. However, a review of health literature indicates that research findings
over three decades place a wide variety of assessed materials (based prima-
rily on reading level analyses) at levels that exceed the reading skills of most
high school graduates (Rudd et al., 2000a). The substantial volume of
literature focused on assessments of health-related materials.
Health literacy may also be more reliant on domains of literacy such as
oral (speaking) ability and aural (listening) comprehension that are not
measured by NALS, or the tools currently used to measure health literacy
(discussed in Chapter 2 of this report). Recognizing that basic literacy skills
are required for health literacy, it is reasonable to conclude that individuals
with limited literacy—the 90 million individuals that scored in Levels 1 and
2 of NALS—probably also have limited health literacy. These individuals
likely lack the necessary literacy skills in English needed to effectively ob-
tain and understand much of the health-related information they will inter-
act with at home, at work, or in their communities. Furthermore, limited
health literacy probably affects more than just those with limited literacy.
Individuals with adequate literacy may be affected by the complex literacy
demands of the health-care context, and some individuals may continue to
be affected despite attempts to reduce these demands. These findings, com-
bined with the average NALS scores of U.S. adults, offer a strong argument
that 90 million U.S. adults are severely disadvantaged as they attempt to
function in health-care contexts. The committee considers health literacy to
be a reciprocal function of the health context and the individual. Therefore,
any person, no matter what literacy skills he or she possesses, may well
have limited health literacy once he or she enters complex health-care con-
texts.
68 HEALTH LITERACY
identifying people with limited skills. It is even the case that any two tests
both claiming to use grade-level scores would not identify the same students
as being below a selected standard because their norming samples most
likely were not the same. Thus the percentages falling below a particular
standard are not directly comparable, but do clearly point to where prob-
lems exist.
Studies were identified by searching the Medline (1966–2003), PsycInfo
(1974–2003), ERIC4 (1963–2003), Sociological Abstracts (1963–2003),
and CINAHL5 (1982–2003) databases through the OVID web gateway for
the indicated years with the following terms as keyword searches: “health
literacy,” “literacy and health,” and “reading and health.” To be included,
the study had to define the health or health-care population cohort, the
literacy measurement tool, and the result of the literacy screening assess-
ment. Additional studies for inclusion were identified through testimony to
the committee by experts in the field (see Appendix A), and the available
bibliographies (Greenberg, 2001; Rudd et al., 2000b; Zobel et al., 2003).
Table 3-2 summarizes identified studies containing information on the
rates of health literacy skills as currently measured among various study
populations. Results are given as percentages of the study participants with
(1) marginal and inadequate health literacy as measured by the TOFHLA
(Parker et al., 1995) or its shorter version (S-TOFHLA; Baker et al., 1999);
or (2) inadequate health literacy as indicated by a score below grade 9 on
the REALM (Davis et al., 1993); or (3) what the authors reported if a
standard measure was not used or if a standard measure was modified.
Table 3-2 also identifies the reported demographic characteristics of the
study participants that were reported to be associated with limited health
literacy.
Studies noted in Table 3-2 document the prevalence of limited health-
literacy skills as measured by the REALM or TOFHLA among patients in
general medical and pediatric clinics; specialty care clinics including those
for asthma, HIV, family planning, obstetrics, and oncology; and commu-
nity-based sites including retirement homes and social service agencies. The
studies show that limited health literacy skills, as measured by current
assessment tools, are common, with significant variations in prevalence
depending on the population sampled (Williams et al., 1998b). In many
cases, however, education was not controlled for, and in some cases differ-
ences based on health literacy assessments were no longer significant when
education was controlled for.
Two large multisite studies of convenience samples that provide esti-
70 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
Continued
72 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
Continued
74 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
South Florida
English Spanish
Inadequate: 17.3% Inadequate: 34.3%
Marginal: 11.6% Marginal: 20.3%
Adequate: 71.1% Adequate: 45.4%
Tampa, FL
English Spanish
Inadequate: 16.6% Inadequate: 60.0%
Marginal: 10.2% Marginal: 16.0%
Adequate: 73.2% Adequate: 24.0%
TOFHLA Lower education level 9.6% scored less than 80% on the
(abbreviated Health literacy levels TOFHLA
version) were not
significantly
associated with age,
race, marital status,
employment status,
and poverty status
Continued
76 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
Continued
78 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
Continued
80 HEALTH LITERACY
Participation
Citation Population Rate Study Design Setting
Reported Demographic
Associations with
Measure Low Health Literacy Prevalence of Literacy Levels
Atlanta
Inadequate: 34.7%
Marginal: 12.7%
Adequate: 52.6%
S-TOFHLA: Inadequate health literacy (score 0–53), marginal health literacy (score 54–66),
adequate health literacy (67–100).
IDL: Grade level scores from 0 to 8, 0 indicating failure at grade level 1, 8 indicating read-
ing comprehension at or above the 8th grade level.
82 HEALTH LITERACY
tal (1997) and in a Medicare managed care health plan (2002a), those with
inadequate health literacy as determined by the TOFHLA were significantly
more likely than patients with adequate health literacy scores to report their
health as poor.
Health literacy levels may influence health status by affecting care-
seeking behavior. A study by Bennett and colleagues (1998) indicated that
individuals with lower health literacy scores on the REALM may enter the
health-care system when they are sicker than those with higher health
literacy scores; among men with prostate cancer, those scoring lower on the
REALM were more likely to be initially diagnosed at a more advanced
stage of disease than those with higher health literacy. However, this differ-
ence was not statistically significant after adjustments for race, age, and
location or care. Several studies have showed that individuals with lower
health literacy scores are less likely than those with higher health literacy
scores to make use of preventive health-care services. Scott and colleagues
(2002) found that, among Medicare managed care enrollees, those with
inadequate health literacy, as measured by the S-TOFHLA, used preventive
health services (including influenza and pneumococcal vaccinations, mam-
mogram, and Pap smear) less than enrollees with higher health literacy.
Similarly, Fortenberry and colleagues (2001) administered the REALM test
to 1,035 people in four sites across the country, and found that higher
REALM scores were independently associated with gonorrhea testing in the
previous year. In contrast, Moon and colleagues (1998) found no relation-
ship between parental literacy levels, as measured by the REALM, and use
of preventive health services for pediatric patients.
84 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
86 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
88 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
90 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
92 HEALTH LITERACY
REALM (higher health literacy defined Participants with higher health literacy were
as high school level or above, and significantly more likely than those with
lower health literacy as seventh- lower health literacy to initiate and
eighth-grade level) sustain breastfeeding during the first 2
Survey months.
Continued
94 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
96 HEALTH LITERACY
Continued
98 HEALTH LITERACY
Williams
et al., 1998b N = 516 94% (Los Cohort Two urban public
402 patients Angeles) hospitals in Los
with 86% (Atlanta) Angeles and
hypertension Atlanta
and 114
patients with
diabetes
nation rates, although the numbers of literate women did not change and
there was a 12 percent increase in the number of literate men. This study
also suggests that health behaviors may improve as a result of changes in
health-care delivery without improvements in individual literacy (Northrop-
Clewes et al., 1998).
Jose, a Bolivian man in his early 30s, stayed after class one night so I could
help him understand a hospital bill. He had been having bad headaches for
some time. Thinking it was his only option for care, he had gone to the
emergency department to get treatment. There he was told the headaches
would clear up if he got glasses. He was charged $300 for this diagnosis
(Singleton, 2002).
8The information in this section is drawn from the background paper “The Relationship
Between Health Literacy and Medical Costs,” commissioned by the committee from David H.
Howard, Ph.D. The committee appreciates his contribution. The full text of the paper can be
found in Appendix B of this report.
Finding 3-1 About 90 million adults, an estimate based on the 1992 NALS, have
literacy skills that test below high school level (NALS Level 1 and 2). Of these,
about 40–44 million (NALS Level 1) have difficulty finding information in unfamiliar
or complex texts such as newspaper articles, editorials, medicine labels, forms, or
charts. Because the medical and public health literature indicates that health ma-
terials are complex and often far above high school level, the committee notes that
approximately 90 million adults may lack the needed literacy skills to effectively
use the U.S. health system. The majority of these adults are native-born English
speakers. Literacy levels are lower among the elderly, those who have lower edu-
cational levels, those who are poor, minority populations, and groups with limited
English proficiency such as recent immigrants.
Finding 3-2 On the basis of limited studies, public testimony, and committee
members’ experience, the committee concludes that the shame and stigma asso-
ciated with limited literacy skills are major barriers to improving health literacy.
equate health literacy scores were higher than in patients with adequate
literacy, while pharmacy expenses were similar in both groups and outpa-
tient expenditures were lower. Although this Medicare managed care sample
is not representative of the U.S. population as a whole, the results are
consistent with previous reports that limited-literacy individuals make
greater use of services designed to treat complications of disease and fewer
services designed to prevent complications (Baker et al., 1998, 2002; Gor-
don et al., 2002; Scott et al., 2002).
These data suggest that patients with limited literacy may interact with
a complex health-care system in ways that interfere with ideal utilization
patterns and therefore could be more expensive. However, since the causal
relationships between literacy and health-care utilization and cost have not
been discovered, it is not possible to establish a valid cost figure for the
impact of limited health literacy. If the magnitudes suggested by Howard
and Friedland approach the actual costs, they clearly underscore the impor-
tance of addressing this risk factor from a financial perspective, in addition
to health outcome implications. More research is needed to expand these
limited results and move to a clearer estimation of these effects.
Finding 3-3 Adults with limited health literacy, as measured by reading and nu-
meracy skills, have less knowledge of disease management and of health-promot-
ing behaviors, report poorer health status, and are less likely to use preventive
services.
Finding 3-4 Two recent studies demonstrate a higher rate of hospitalization and
use of emergency services among patients with limited literacy. This higher utiliza-
tion has been associated with higher health-care costs.
Recommendation 3-1 Given the compelling evidence noted above, funding for
health-literacy research is urgently needed. The Department of Health and Human
Services, especially the National Institutes of Health, the Agency for Healthcare
Research and Quality, the Health Resources and Services Administrations, and
the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; the Department of Defense; the
Veterans Administration; and other public and private funding agencies should
support multidisciplinary research on the extent, associations, and consequences
of limited health literacy, including studies on health service utilization and expen-
ditures.
REFERENCES
Arnold CL, Davis TC, Berkel HJ, Jackson RH, Nandy I, London S. 2001. Smoking status,
reading level, and knowledge of tobacco effects among low-income pregnant women.
Preventive Medicine. 32(4): 313–320.
Arozullah AM, Lee SY, Khan T, Kurup S. 2002. Low health literacy increases the risk of
preventable hospital admission. Abstract presented at the Midwest Regional Meeting of
the Society of General Internal Medicine. September 26–28, 2002: Chicago, IL.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS, Nurss J. 1997. The relationship of patient
reading ability to self-reported health and use of health services. American Journal of
Public Health. 87(6): 1027–1030.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS. 1998. Health literacy and the risk of
hospital admission. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 13(12): 791–798.
Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. 1999. Development of a
brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Education and Counseling. 38:
33–42.
Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Scott T, Parker RM, Green D, Ren J, Peel J.
2002a. Functional health literacy and the risk of hospital admission among Medicare
managed care enrollees. American Journal of Public Health. 92(8): 1278–1283.
Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Sudano J, Patterson M, Parker RM, Williams DW. 2002b.
Health literacy and performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination. Aging and
Mental Health. 6(1): 22–29.
Balsa AI, McGuire TG. 2001. Statistical discrimination in health care. Journal of Health
Economics. 20(6): 881–907.
Beers BB, McDonald VJ, Quistberg DA, Ravenell KL, Asch DA, Shea JA. 2003. Disparities in
health literacy between African American and non-African American primary care pa-
tients. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 169.
Bennett CL, Ferreira MR, Davis TC, Kaplan J, Weinberger M, Kuzel T, Seday MA, Sartor O.
1998. Relation between literacy, race, and stage of presentation among low-income
patients with prostate cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 16(9): 3101–3104.
Benson JG, Forman WB. 2002. Comprehension of written health care information in an
affluent geriatric retirement community: Use of the test of functional health literacy.
Gerontology. 48(2): 93–97.
Berk ML, Monheit AC. 2001. The concentration of health care expenditures, revisited. Health
Affairs. 20(2): 9–18.
Comings JP, Reder S, Sum A. 2001. Building a Level Playing Field: The Need to Expand and
Improve the National and State Adult Education and Literacy Systems. NCSALL Re-
port. Cambridge, MA: National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Davis TC, Long SW, Jackson RH, Mayeaux EJ, George RB, Murphy PW, Crouch MA. 1993.
Rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine: A shortened screening instrument. Family
Medicine. 25(6): 391–395.
Davis TC, Mayeaux EJ, Fredrickson D, Bocchini JA Jr, Jackson RH, Murphy PW. 1994.
Reading ability of parents compared with reading level of pediatric patient education
materials. Pediatrics. 93(3): 460–468.
Davis TC, Arnold C, Berkel HJ, Nandy I, Jackson RH, Glass J. 1996. Knowledge and attitude
on screening mammography among low-literate, low-income women. Cancer. 78(9):
1912–1920.
Doak CC, Doak LG, Root J. 1996. Teaching Patients with Low Literacy Skills. 2nd edition.
New York: Lippincott.
Fortenberry JD, McFarlane MM, Hennessy M, Bull SS, Grimley DM, St Lawrence J, Stoner
BP, VanDevanter N. 2001. Relation of health literacy to gonorrhoea related care. Sexu-
ally Transmitted Infections. 77(3): 206–211.
Friedland R. 1998. New estimates of the high costs of inadequate health literacy. In: Proceed-
ings of Pfizer Conference “Promoting Health Literacy: A Call to Action.” October 7–8,
1998, Washington, DC: Pfizer, Inc. Pp. 6–10.
Gazmararian JA, Parker RM, Baker DW. 1999a. Reading skills and family planning knowl-
edge and practices in a low-income managed-care population. Obstetrics & Gynecol-
ogy. 93(2): 239–244.
Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Scott T, Greemn DCFSN, Ren J,
Koplan JP. 1999b. Health literacy among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organi-
zation. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6): 545–551.
Gazmararian J, Baker D, Parker R, Blazer DG. 2000. A multivariate analysis of factors
associated with depression: Evaluating the role of health literacy as a potential contribu-
tor. Archives of Internal Medicine. 160(21): 3307–3314.
Glewwe P. 1999. Why does mother’s schooling raise child health in developing countries?
Evidence from Morocco. Journal of Human Resources. 34(1): 124–159.
Gonzales F, Dearden K, Jimenez W. 1999. Do multi-sectoral development programmes affect
health? A Bolivian case study. Health Policy & Planning. 14(4): 400–408.
Gordon MM, Hampson R, Capell HA, Madhok R. 2002. Illiteracy in rheumatoid arthritis
patients as determined by the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM)
score. Rheumatology. 41(7): 750–754.
Greenberg E, Macias RF, Rhodes D, Chan T. 2001. English Literacy and Language Minori-
ties in the United States. NCES 2001-464. Washington, DC: National Center for Educa-
tion Statistics. U.S. Department of Education. [Online]. Available: http://www.nces.ed.
gov/pubs2002/2002382.pdf [accessed: June 5, 2003].
Greenberg, J. 2001. An Updated Overview of Medical and Public Health Literature Address-
ing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography of Articles Published in 2000. [Online].
Available: http://www. hsph.harvard.edu/healthliteracy/literature/lit_2000.html [ac-
cessed: October, 2003].
Guerra CE, Shea JA. 2003. Functional health literacy, comorbidity and health status. Ab-
stract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 174.
Haigler KO. 1994. Literacy Behind Prison Walls. Profiles of the Prison Population from the
National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
[Online]. Available: http://www.nces.ed.gov/pubs2002/2002382.pdf [accessed: June 5,
2003].
Hegerfeld M. 1999. Reading, Writing, and the American Soldier: A Study of Literacy in the
American Armed Forces. Fort Wayne, IN: Department of Education, Indiana Univer-
sity–Purdue University at Fort Wayne.
Kalichman SC, Rompa D. 2000. Functional health literacy is associated with health status
and health-related knowledge in people living with HIV-AIDS. Journal of Acquired
Immune Deficiency Syndromes and Human Retrovirology. 25(4): 337–344.
Kalichman SC, Benotsch E, Suarez T, Catz S, Miller J, Rompa D. 2000. Health literacy and
health-related knowledge among persons living with HIV/AIDS. American Journal of
Preventive Medicine. 18(4): 325–331.
Kalichman SCP, Ramachandran BB, Catz SP. 1999. Adherence to combination antiretroviral
therapies in HIV patients of low health literacy. Journal of General Internal Medicine.
14(5): 267–273.
Kaufman H, Skipper B, Small L, Terry T, McGrew M. 2001. Effect of literacy on breast-
feeding outcomes. Southern Medical Journal. 94(3): 293–296.
Kim SP, Knight SJ, Tomori C, Colella KM, Schoor RA, Shih L, Kuzel TM, Nadler RB,
Bennett CL. 2001. Health literacy and shared decision making for prostate cancer pa-
tients with low socioeconomic status. Cancer Investigation. 19(7): 684–691.
Kirsch IS. 2001. The International Adult Literacy Survey (IALS): Understanding What Was
Measured. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.
Lasater L. 2003. Patient literacy, adherence, and anticoagulation therapy outcomes: A pre-
liminary report. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 179.
Li BDL, Brown WA, Ampil FL, Burton GV, Yu H, McDonald JC. 2000. Patient compliance is
critical for equivalent clinical outcomes for breast cancer treated by breast-conservation
therapy. Annals of Surgery. 231(6): 883–889.
Lindau ST, Tomori C, Lyons T, Langseth L, Bennett CL, Garcia P. 2002. The association of
health literacy with cervical cancer prevention knowledge and health behaviors in a
multiethnic cohort of women. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology. 186(5):
938–943.
Mayer GG. 2003. Confessions of a health illiterate. Healthcare Advances. 5(2): 2.
Montalto NJ, Spiegler GE. 2001. Functional health literacy in adults in a rural community
health center. West Virginia Medical Journal. 97(2): 111–114.
Moon RY, Cheng TL, Patel KM, Baumhaft K, Scheidt PC. 1998. Parental literacy level and
understanding of medical information. Pediatrics. 102(2): e25.
Morse L. 2002. Improving Health Literacy: An Educational Response to a Public Health
Problem. Presentation given at a workshop of the Institute of Medicine Committee on
Health Literacy. December 11, 2002, Washington, DC.
Northrop-Clewes CA, Ahmad N, Paracha PI, Thurnham DI. 1998. Impact of health service
provision on mothers and infants in a rural village in North West Frontier Province,
Pakistan. Public Health Nutrition. 1(1): 51–59.
Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development and Statistics Canada. 2000.
Literacy in the Information Age: Final Report of the International Adult Literacy Sur-
vey. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 89-571-XPE. Paris: Organization for Economic
Co-operation and Development and Ottawa: Minister of Industry.
Parikh NS, Parker RM, Nurss JR, Baker DW, Williams MV. 1996. Shame and health literacy:
The unspoken connection. Patient Education and Counseling. 27(1): 33–39.
Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. 1995. The Test of Functional Health Lit-
eracy in Adults: A new instrument for measuring patients’ literacy skills. Journal of
General Internal Medicine. 10(10): 537–541.
Parker RM, Ratzan SC, Lurie N. 2003. Health literacy: A policy challenge for advancing
high-quality health care. Health Affairs. 22(4): 147.
Profiles of State Programs. 1990. Profiles of State Programs: Adult Education for the Home-
less. Washington, DC: Division of Adult Literacy and Education, U.S. Department of
Education.
Rudd R, Moeykens BA, Colton TC. 2000a. Health and literacy. A review of medical and
public health literature. In: Annual Review of Adult Learning and Literacy. Comings J,
Garners B, Smith C, Editors. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Rudd RE, Colton T, Schacht R. 2000b. An Overview of Medical and Public Health Litera-
ture Addressing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography. Report #14. Cambridge,
MA: National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Rudd RE, Kirsch I, Yamamoto K. 2003. Literacy in health contexts: A re-analysis of the
NALS. In: Clear Health Communications: Issues and Solutions, Session 4182.1. Wash-
ington, DC: American Public Health Association Annual Conference.
Schillinger D, Grumbach K, Piette J, Wang F, Osmond D, Daher C, Palacios J, Sullivan GaD,
Bindman AB. 2002. Association of health literacy with diabetes outcomes. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 288(4): 475–482.
Schillinger D, Piette J, Grumbach K, Wang F, Wilson C, Daher C, Leong-Grotz K, Castro C,
Bindman AB. 2003. Closing the loop: Physician communication with diabetic patients
who have low health literacy. Archives of Internal Medicine. 163(1): 83–90.
Scott TL, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Baker DW. 2002. Health literacy and preventive
health care use among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organization. Medical
Care. 40(5): 395–404.
Shea JA, Guerra C, Weiner J, Aguirre A, Schaffer M, Asch DA. 2003. Health literacy and
patient satisfaction. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 187–188.
Shinagawa S, Foong HL. 2003. Health literacy challenges for Asian Americans and Native
Hawaiians and other Pacific Islanders. Supplement to oral testimony presented at a
workshop of the Institute of Medicine Committee on Health Literacy. February 13,
2003, Irvine, CA.
Singleton K. 2002. ESOL Teachers: Helpers in health care. Focus on Basics: Connecting
Research Practice. 5(C): 26–30.
Sum A, Kirsch IS, Taggart R. 2002. The twin challenges of mediocrity and inequality: Lit-
eracy in the U.S. from an international perspective. In: Policy Information Report.
Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
U.S. Census Bureau. 2002. Race and Hispanic or Latino Origin by Age and Sex for the
United States: 2000 (PHC-T-8). Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau. [Online]. Avail-
able: http://www.census.gov/population/www/cen2000/phc-t08.html [accessed: Decem-
ber 9, 2003].
U.S. Department of Education. 2001. English Literacy and Language Minorities in the United
States. Greenberg E, Macias RF, Rhodes D, Chan T, Editors. NCES 2001-464. Wash-
ington, DC: National Center for Health Statistics.
Weiss BD. 1999. How common is low literacy? In: 20 Common Problems in Primary Care.
Weiss BD, Editor. New York: McGraw-Hill. Pp. 468–481.
Weiss BD, Palmer R. 2004. Relationship between health care costs and very low literacy skills
in a medically needy and indigent Medicaid population. Journal of the American Board
of Family Practice. 17(1): 44–47.
Weiss BD, Blanchard JS, McGee DL, Hart G, Warren B, Burgoon M, Smith KJ. 1994. Illit-
eracy among Medicaid recipients and its relationship to health care costs. Journal of
Health Care for the Poor & Underserved. 5(2): 99–111.
Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, Parikh NS, Pitkin K, Coates WC, Nurss JR. 1995.
Inadequate functional health literacy among patients at two public hospitals. Journal of
the American Medical Association. 274(21): 1677–1682.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Honig EG, Lee TM, Nowlan A. 1998a. Inadequate literacy is a
barrier to asthma knowledge and self-care. Chest. 114(4): 1008–1015.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, Nurss JR. 1998b. Relationship of functional health
literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. A study of patients with hyper-
tension and diabetes. Archives of Internal Medicine. 158(2): 166–172.
Win K, Schillinger D. 2003. Understanding of warfarin therapy and stroke among ethnically
diverse anticoagulation patients at a public hospital. Abstract. Journal of General Inter-
nal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 278.
Young BA. 2002. Public High School Dropouts and Completers from the Common Core of
Data: School Years 1998–99 and 1999–2000. NCES 2002-382. Washington, DC: Na-
tional Center for Education Statistics. U.S. Department of Education. Office of Educa-
tional Research and Improvement. [Online]. Available: http://www.nces.ed.gov/
pubs2002/2002382.pdf [accessed: June 5, 2003].
Zobel E, Rowe K, Gomez-Mandic C. 2003. An Updated Overview of Medical and Public
Health Literature Addressing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography of Articles
Published in 2002. [Online]. Available: http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/health
literacy/literature/lit_2002.html [accessed: October, 2003].
O
ur understanding of health literacy gains greater depth and mean-
ing in the context of culture. This is especially important given the
ethnic and linguistic diversity of the U.S. population. In addition
to 211,460,626 Americans of European decent, the 2000 U.S. Census iden-
108
tified 69,961,280 people from 19 other ethnic and cultural groups living in
America (U.S. Census Bureau, 2000). Many of these diverse American popu-
lations have differing systems of belief about health and illness. Cultural
health beliefs affect how people think and feel about their health and health
problems, when and from whom they seek health care, and how they
respond to recommendations for lifestyle change, health-care interventions,
and treatment adherence.
Cultures also differ in their styles of communication, in the meaning of
words and gestures, and even in what can be discussed regarding the body,
health, and illness. Health literacy requires communication and mutual
understanding between patients and their families and health-care provid-
ers and staff. Culture and health literacy both influence the content and
outcomes of health-care encounters.
A definition of health literacy that does not recognize the potential
effect of cultural differences on the communication and understanding of
health information would miss much of the deeper meaning and purpose of
literacy for people (Nutbeam, 2000). Culture provides a context through
which meaning is gained from information, and provides the purpose by
which people come to understand their health status and comprehend op-
tions for diagnoses and treatments. A conceptual understanding of the
interconnections between culture and literacy through the idea of cultural
literacy can provide insights into the deeper meanings of how diverse popu-
lations in the United States come to know, comprehend, and make in-
formed decisions based on valid data regarding their health.
This intersection between culture and literacy is recognized in the U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services (HHS) National Standards for
Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services (CLAS) in Health Care.
The standard states that “health care organizations must make available
easily understood patient-related materials . . . in the languages of com-
monly encountered groups . . .” (HHS, 2001: 11). The standard goes on to
state explicitly that in addition to being culturally responsive, these materi-
als need also be responsive to the literacy levels of patients and consumers.
Issues of culture, language, and learning are interrelated, and to be effec-
tive, health education must be conducted in both culturally and linguisti-
cally appropriate formats to address the increasingly diverse multicultural
and multilingual population (AMA Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy,
1999).
Cultural, social, and family influences shape attitudes and beliefs and
therefore influence health literacy. Social determinants of health are well
documented regarding the conditions over which the individual has little or
no control but that affect his or her ability to participate fully in a health-
literate society. Native language, socioeconomic status, gender, race, and
ethnicity along with mass culture as represented by news publishing, adver-
TRADITIONAL CULTURE
Culture is the shared ideas, meanings, and values acquired by individu-
als as members of society. It is socially learned, not genetically transmitted,
and often influences us unconsciously. Human beings learn through social
means—through interactions with others as well as through the products of
culture such as books and television (IOM, 2002). Reliance on tools and
symbolic resources, notably language, is a hallmark of culture. Language is
central to social life and mediates the acquisition of much cultural knowl-
edge. Language “provides the most complex system of the classification of
experience” and is “the most flexible and most powerful tool developed by
humans” (Duranti, 1997: 49 and 47). Differences in languages and under-
lying concepts may lead to problems with health-related communication.
For example, translating the word “chemotherapy” into the Navajo lan-
guage might require pages of text. Since the Navajo language has no word
or concept for chemotherapy, the translation must start with the idea of
cancer, and include what the person might experience as a result of chemo-
therapy. This is further complicated by the fact that many Navajos believe
that if you say something will happen, it will1 (Billie, 2003).
Beyond the differences of language, culture gives significance to health
information and messages. Perceptions and definitions of health and illness,
preferences, language and cultural barriers, care process barriers, and ste-
reotypes are all strongly influenced by culture and can have a great impact
1The committee thanks Alvin Billie for his contributions to this section of the report.
Cultural Competence
The skills and knowledge of cultural competence provide a trajectory
towards the interpersonal skills for effective patient care that can be real-
ized only when comprehension, understanding, and meaning are inherent
in the process. Cultural competence has been variously defined by different
organizations.2 Speaking of Health (IOM, 2002) notes the following di-
mensions:
2For example, the HHS CLAS standards define cultural competence as “the capacity to
function effectively as an individual and as an organization within the context of cultural
beliefs, behaviors, and needs presented by consumers and their communities” and the Na-
tional Medical Association defines cultural competence as “the application of cultural knowl-
edge, behaviors, clinical and interpersonal skills that enhances a provider’s effectiveness in
patient care.”
Language
Cultural Languages
Cultural context gets transformed into cultural language that influ-
ences three critical determinants of health literacy: comprehension, under-
standing, and decision-making. Different cultural groups mobilize creative
forces to formulate unique cultural languages that must be considered in
culturally competent approaches for implementing interventions to pro-
mote health literacy. Two examples of these innovative cultural languages
can be found in the language of Aboriginal people and in the dreams of
Native American cultures.
In contrast to Westernized people whose language use is dominated by
nouns, Aboriginal people use a language dominated by verbs in deference
to their worldly vision of all existence as energy or spirit that is in constant
transformation (Ross, 1996: 116). A consequence is that there is attention
to the “relationship between things” and less focus on the “characteristic of
things” (in a noun-driven language such as English). This feature of using
“fluidity of verb-phrases” is functional in healing relationships. Thus, a re-
thinking and re-framing of the standard inquiries that drive health assess-
3Executive Order No. 13166. Benefit-Cost Report of Executive Order No. 13166: Improv-
ing Access to Services for Persons with Limited English Proficiency. August 2000.
The Hmong language has no word for cancer, or even the concept of the
disease. “We’re going to put a fire in you,” is how one inexperienced
interpreter tried to explain radiation treatment to the patient, who as a result,
refused treatment (Morse, 2003).
exacerbated in Latino patients and families since trauma has the same two
meanings in Spanish, but cultural views of illness more often focus on the
psychological meaning (Long et al., 1992). While specific meaning of the
word trauma may differ between individuals and groups, people will act
upon the meaning they understand.
Mr. G. presents to the emergency department one week later with dizziness.
His blood pressure is very low, and Mr. G. says he has been taking the
medicine just like it says to take it on the bottle. The puzzling case is dis-
cussed by multiple practitioners until one that speaks Spanish asks Mr. G.
how many pills he took each day. “22,” Mr. G. replies. The provider ex-
plains to his colleagues that “once” means “11” in Spanish.
Therefore, human illness has meaning, in both the biological sense and as a
human experience. As a result, clinical practice is inherently interpretive
and practitioners must elicit patients’ requests, elicit and decode patients’
use of language, diagnose disease and illness, and develop plans for manag-
ing problems. This includes the need to elicit explanatory modes of patients
and families, analyze conflict with the biomedical model, and negotiate
alternatives (Good and Good, 1981).
culture, meaning, and health literacy. These new measures should function
to improve the validity of current approaches and provide new knowledge
about the impact of health literacy on health outcomes in diverse popula-
tions.
MASS CULTURE
Mass culture refers to the institutions, organizations, and individuals
that produce and disseminate health messages to Americans. The quantity,
quality, and lack of quality control over these messages have exploded in
the past 10 years. Hundreds of health organizations across the country
from hospitals to advocacy groups to major government agencies like HHS,
the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), the National Li-
brary of Medicine, and the National Institutes of Health (NIH), have cre-
ated elaborate “user-friendly” information sources. These are often elec-
tronically accessible 24 hours a day to provide Americans with up-to-date
health information on the care and prevention of disease. But the informa-
tion sources available to Americans do not stop there. Major advocacy
groups such as AARP and The American Cancer Society, plus many others,
also offer detailed information on health care and disease prevention. These
approaches to providing and accessing information are in their infancy, and
must be evaluated and then modified for maximum effectiveness. When
consumer needs are at the core of information provision, whether via print,
digital media, or intrapersonal communication, the information can be
more accessible. With appropriate attention to the information needs of
health-care consumers, new technologies can offer all segments of society
greater access to health information.
In the private sector, the marketing of pharmaceutical drugs—both
over-the-counter and prescription—is now a part of every American’s
television viewing. The cost of pharmaceuticals promotions rose to $19.1
billion in 2001 (Medvantx, 2003). The industry drug packaging and con-
sumer education programs are another powerful source of health informa-
tion. Radio programs provide regular advice on both modern and herbal
medicines. Products of all kinds make health claims as part of their mar-
keting programs. Indeed, health has become a major consumer motivator
along with sex and price promotions in American marketing.
The news media has also taken health information seriously. Dozens of
major news outlets have health reporters who are increasingly skilled in
interpreting health studies. The Journal of the American Medical Associa-
tion is widely quoted and referenced in news articles in both print and
broadcast media. Finally, the Internet has provided an opportunity for any
individual to make health claims about any product or procedure with little
or no scientific basis. In sum, the American public is now faced with a
BOX 4-1
Sources of Health Information Reported in a Gallup Poll
BOX 4-2
Selected Findings from Sex Matters
Which of the following Have you ever heard this To the best of your
would you say you use information before: recollection, where did
most often as a source In addition to the typical you hear this informa-
of information about chest pain associated with tion? (asked only of
health for you: a heart attack, women may the 55% who said they
suffer from subtle symptoms had heard it)
such as indigestion,
abdominal or mid-back
pain, nausea or vomiting?
Complexity of Materials
Many materials developed to provide health information fail to take
into account the needs of the audiences for these materials. Rudd and
colleagues (2000) reviewed studies of patient information materials, and
found that disparities between the readability of education materials and
patient reading level occurred in ambulatory care settings (Cooley et al.,
1995; Davis et al., 1990), substance abuse treatment centers (Davis et al.,
1993), and pediatric care settings (Davis et al., 1994). Similar findings are
reported for patients with diabetes (Hosey et al., 1990), arthritis (Hill,
1997), and lupus (Hearth-Holmes et al., 1997). These studies found that
the reading levels of groups of patients with these chronic diseases fell
between grade levels 6 and 10, while the readability of the materials de-
signed for them fell between grade levels 7 and 13.
Rudd and colleagues (2000) also showed that several studies examined
patient education materials designed for specific ethnic groups. A substan-
tial number of studies report on both readability and comprehension assess-
ments of these documents, deeming most of them inappropriate (Austin et
al., 1995; Delp and Jones, 1996; Jolly et al., 1993, 1995; Logan et al., 1996;
Powers, 1988; Spandorfer et al., 1995; Williams et al., 1996). Hosey and
colleagues (1990) used the Wide Range Achievement Test to measure the
reading ability of a group of American Indian diabetic patients and found
that although many patients scored at a reading grade level of 5, the diabe-
tes education materials scored at a mean reading grade level of 10. Guidry,
Fagan, and Walker (1998) note that less than half of the cancer education
materials specifically targeting African Americans reflected the culture of
African Americans and that few were written at a reading grade level for
those with low literacy skills.
Finding 4-2 More than 300 studies indicate that health-related mate-
rials far exceed the average reading ability of U.S. adults.
the first nine months of 1998, visits for specific causes related to advertised
products and services increased much more dramatically during this time.
For example, visits for smoking cessation rose 263 percent, visits for impo-
tence increased 113 percent, visits for hair loss rose 30 percent, and visits
for high cholesterol rose by 19 percent (Maguire, 1999).
Commercial and social marketing of health information, products, and
services is now a multi-billion-dollar industry. According to the Institute for
Policy Innovation, direct-to-consumer advertising of prescription drugs
alone increased to $1.8 billion in 1999 (Matthews, 2001). These expendi-
tures are not unique. They represent a small portion of the dollars aimed at
providing health information to consumers and motivating specific health
behaviors. The United States invested some $1,080,000,000 in its recent 6-
year National Youth Anti-Drug Media Campaign (Eddy, 2003). The im-
portance of these expenditures goes beyond the dollar amount. These ex-
penditures are guided by an understanding of consumers and their desires.
These are not “information” campaigns, but rather targeted marketing
efforts designed to influence what people do by offering new products, new
services, lower barriers, and new motivations for changing their behavior—
to stop smoking, to avoid fast food, to get a mammogram or to delay
getting a mammogram. These expenditures are guided by years of market
research studies and experience that have shown what kind of language
works, what pictures appeal, and what messages compel people to act
(Wilke, 1994). Despite this investment of talent and resources, some pro-
grams fail. The most recent evaluation of the Office of National Drug
Control Policy’s National Youth Anti-Drug Media Campaign program
states: “There is little evidence of direct favorable Campaign effects on
youth. There is no statistically significant decline in marijuana use to date,
and some evidence for an increase in use from 2000 to 2001” (Hornick et
al., 2002).
One characteristic of the commercial and social marketing sector is that
the information is not provided objectively. The authors of these messages,
whether antismoking advocates or the cigarette industry, carefully select
facts, stories, and images that fall far short of full information for an
intelligent decision (Mazur, 2003: 6–11). This assault on the public may
have trained segments of the American public to be skeptical; to expect
short sound bites of information and to avoid equivocation in information.
As Mazur concludes:
Although we may talk about shared decision making in medical care and
the provider–patient relationship, the original goal of the decision scien-
tist, to provide a full discussion of risks and benefits among a full set of
alternatives, may or may not be attainable. And it is far from clear wheth-
er patients actually want to participate in such a fully shared decision-
making environment (Mazur, 2003: 177).
Finding 4-4 Health literacy efforts have not yet fully benefited from
research findings in social and commercial marketing.
Evidence-Based Approaches5
Within the health sciences there is emerging literature on the effective-
ness of community-based interventions with culturally diverse groups and
increasing cultural competency by health providers (IOM, 2002). The out-
5The committee would like to thank the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality for
their assistance with this segment of the report.
Promising Approaches
In addition to these published studies, many unpublished activities are
being carried out in the community to improve health literacy. Anecdotal
evidence suggests that the limited budgets of these programs, combined
with their emphasis on intervention, result in funding being used for the
approach itself to the exclusion of formal evaluation of the program out-
come. Examples of these approaches are presented below.
Community Opportunities
Community organizations provide an opportunity to address issues of
health literacy directly. An example of this is a set of programs to address
the needs of the Navajo community. Older Navajos are particularly vulner-
able to complications of medical conditions exacerbated by a mismatch in
Intervention Outcome
Continued
Intervention Outcome
Continued
the health literacy skills of health consumers and care providers. The Gath-
ering Place8 provides health and literacy programs for Navajo adults and
children in their homes and community centers. Bilingual, trained commu-
nity members provide services that include information on health, safety,
mental, and physical wellness, and preventive measures. A “Shima Yazhi”
lay health program offers information and support for new baby care,
parenting, and health concerns, in locations convenient to the client.
The Gathering Place also represents a collaborative program, as it
offers workshops to child care providers throughout Eastern Agency of
the reservation using videos, bilingual oral presentations, and hands-on
formats.
8The Gathering Place is located in Thoreau, New Mexico. For more information, see http:
//www.navajo-coop.com.
Intervention Outcome
9Asian Health Services is based in Oakland, California. For more information, see http://
www.ahschc.org.
Nongovernmental Organizations
Nongovernmental organizations provide an opportunity for informed
and effective advocates to have a role in shaping health literacy programs,
policies, and interventions. For example, The Institute for Healthcare Ad-
vancement has developed a set of books to assist consumers in deciding
what health issues can be dealt with at home (and how to best deal with
them) and what health issues should initiate a call to a health-care provider.
Most of these books, part of a series entitled What to Do for Health, are
available in English or Spanish; two are also available in Vietnamese. An
evaluation of one of the books carried out by Molina Healthcare found that
individuals and families who received the books visited the emergency de-
partment 6.7 percent less after receiving the books (Institute for Healthcare
Advancement, 2003).
Another example is provided by the Managed Care Consumer Assis-
tance Program (MCCAP). The MCCAP works to educate consumers about
the concepts of managed care and rights as consumers, and provide assis-
tance with dispute resolution procedures. MCCAP holds consumer educa-
tion workshops in the appropriate languages and provides one-on-one coun-
seling to consumers needing information or assistance with managed care.
Services are provided to low-income, multilingual, and multicultural com-
munities, through a network of 25 community-based organizations. These
community-based organizations include neighborhood centers, ethnic orga-
nizations, and social service agencies (MCCAP, 2003).
Collaborative Programs
Collaborations between government and community programs offer an
opportunity to develop interventions appropriate to the needs of the audi-
ence. An example of a collaborative approach is a set of materials on sex
education developed though a collaboration between the Program for Ap-
propriate Technology in Health, Austin/Travis County Health Department,
the Washington, DC, Center for Youth Services, Delaware (Maryland)
Delmarva Rural Ministries, the Tlingit and Haida Indian Tribes Central
Council, and the Children’s Theatre of Juneau. Entitled “Plain Talk,” these
materials were developed to educate and inform English-speaking and non-
English-speaking low-literate youth in response to a nationwide needs as-
sessment survey of 2,500 U.S. organizations that showed that teens espe-
cially need materials on AIDS, other STDs, and condom use (Program for
Appropriate Technology in Health, 2003).
Programs between for-profit or nonprofit agencies and community or-
ganizations provide an opportunity to develop appropriate interventions,
and to reach the intended audience. This type of collaboration can effec-
BOX 4-3
Excerpt from the Introduction to “Language Access: Helping
Non-English Speakers Navigate Health and Human Services”
Public and private organizations have begun to address language barriers to en-
sure effective communication between service providers and patients, particularly
in health care. The language gap can lead to delays in or denial of service, unnec-
essary tests, more costly or invasive treatment of disease, mistakes in prescribing
and using medication, and deterrence in patient compliance with treatment. Lan-
guage barriers are a contributing factor in health care disparities among racial and
ethnic minorities and in a lack of health insurance among immigrants and minori-
ties. In a series of federal guidances since 2000, federal agencies have reminded
recipients of federal funds of their obligation under civil rights law to provide mean-
ingful access to their services for limited-English proficient individuals. The Office
for Civil Rights in the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS) states
that language assistance should result in accurate and effective communication
between provider and client, at no cost to the client. Within the health and human
services field, affected organizations include state and local health and welfare
agencies, hospitals and clinics, managed care organizations, nursing homes, men-
tal health centers, senior citizen centers, Head Start programs and contractors. In
three federal programs, federal agencies have approved reimbursement for lan-
guage services to applicants and recipients who are limited English proficient.
HHS, in a November 1999 brief, approved the use of federal Temporary Assis-
tance for Needy Families (TANF) and state Maintenance-of-Effort (MOE) funds to
provide language services. In a 2000 letter to state Medicaid directors, the Centers
for Medicaid & Medicare Services confirmed that federal matching funds for the
State Children’s Health Insurance Program (SCHIP) and Medicaid are available
for state expenditures on interpretation and translation. At least nine states—Ha-
waii, Idaho, Maine, Massachusetts, Minnesota, Montana, New Hampshire, Utah
and Washington—have obtained federal matching funds for these services. Re-
cently, other states have enacted legislation requiring interpreters in emergency
departments and hospitals (Massachusetts and Rhode Island, respectively); a
health-care interpreters council (Oregon); and an office to address racial and eth-
nic disparities in health care, including language and cultural competency (New
Jersey).
Finding 4-2 More than 300 studies indicate that health-related materials far ex-
ceed the average reading ability of U.S. adults.
Finding 4-4 Health literacy efforts have not yet fully benefited from research
findings in social and commercial marketing.
Recommendation 4-2 AHRQ, the CDC, Indian Health Service, Health Resourc-
es and Services Administration, and Substance Abuse and Mental Health Servic-
es Administration should develop and test approaches to improve health commu-
nication that foster healing relationships across culturally diverse populations. This
includes investigations that explore the effect of existing and innovative communi-
cation approaches on health behaviors, and studies that examine the impact of
participatory action and empowerment research strategies for effective penetra-
tion of health information at the community level.
REFERENCES
Alzheimer’s Association of Los Angeles. 2001. Faith-Centered Partnerships: A Model for
Reaching the African-American Community. [Online]. Available: http://www.alzla.org/
resources/faithcentered/index.html [accessed: December, 2003].
AMA Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy. 1999. Health literacy: Report of the Council
on Scientific Affairs. Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy for the Council on Scientific
Affairs, American Medical Association. Journal of the American Medical Association.
281(6): 552–557.
Asian Health Services. 2003. Asian Health Services Home Page. [Online]. Available: http://
www.ahschc.org/ [accessed: October 20, 2003].
Austin PE, Matlack R 2nd, Dunn KA, Kesler C, Brown CK. 1995. Discharge instructions: Do
illustrations help our patients understand them? Annals of Emergency Medicine. 25(3):
317–320.
Benenson Strategy Group. 2001. Sex Matters Survey. Available from: Roper Center for Public
Opinion Research In: Public Opinion Online. Storrs, CT: The Roper Center for Public
Opinion Research.
Bill-Harvey D, Rippey R, Abeles M, Donald MJ, Downing D, Ingenito F, Pfeiffer CA. 1989.
Outcome of an osteoarthritis education program for low-literacy patients taught by
indigenous instructors. Patient Education and Counseling. 13(2): 133–142.
Billie A. 2003. Health and Literacy: A Native American Point of View. Presentation given at
a workshop of the Institute of Medicine Committee on Health Literacy. February 13,
2003, Irvine, CA.
Busselman KM, Holcomb CA. 1994. Reading skill and comprehension of the dietary guide-
lines by WIC participants. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 94(6): 622–
625.
Centro Latino. 2003. Centro Latino de Salud, Educación, y Cultura Home Page. [Online].
Available: http://centrolatino.missouri.org [accessed: October 20, 2003].
Collins KS, Hughes DL, Doty MM, Ives BL, Edwards JN, Tenney K. 2002. Diverse Commu-
nities, Common Concerns: Assessing Health Care Quality for Minority Americans. New
York: The Commonwealth Fund.
Cooley ME, Moriarty H, Berger MS, Selm-Orr D, Coyle B, Short T. 1995. Patient literacy
and the readability of written cancer educational materials. Oncology Nursing Forum.
22(9): 1345–1351.
Cooper LA, Roter DL. 2003. Patient-provider communication: The effect of race and ethnicity
on process and outcomes in health care. In: Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and
Ethnic Disparities in Health Care. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, Editors. Washing-
ton, DC: The National Academies Press. Pp. 336–354.
Cooper-Patrick L, Gallo JJ, Gonzales JJ, Vu HT, Powe NR, Nelson C, Ford DE. 1999. Race,
gender, and partnership in the patient-physician relationship. Journal of the American
Medical Association. 282(6): 583–589.
Davis TC, Crouch MA, Wills G, Miller S, Abdehou DM. 1990. The gap between patient
reading comprehension and the readability of patient education materials. Journal of
Family Practice. 31(5): 533–538.
Davis TC, Jackson RH, George RB, Long SW, Talley D, Murphy PW, Mayeaux EJ, Truong
T. 1993. Reading ability in patients in substance misuse treatment centers. International
Journal of the Addictions. 28(6): 571–582.
Davis TC, Mayeaux EJ, Fredrickson D, Bocchini JA Jr, Jackson RH, Murphy PW. 1994.
Reading ability of parents compared with reading level of pediatric patient education
materials. Pediatrics. 93(3): 460–468.
Delp C, Jones J. 1996. Communicating information to patients: The use of cartoon illustra-
tions to improve comprehension of instructions. Academic Emergency Medicine. 3(3):
264–270.
Duranti A. 1997. Linguistic Anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Eddy M. 2003. War on Drugs: The National Youth Anti-Drug Media Campaign. Congres-
sional Research Service, The Library of Congress.
Elkind PD, Pitts K, Ybarra SL. 2002. Theater as a mechanism for increasing farm health and
safety knowledge. American Journal of Industrial Medicine. (Supplement 2): 28–35.
Eysenbach G, Powell J, Kuss O, Sa ER. 2002. Empirical studies assessing the quality of health
information for consumers on the world-wide web: A systematic review. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 287(20): 2691–2700.
Fitzgibbon ML, Stolley MR, Avellone ME, Sugerman S, Chavez N. 1996. Involving parents in
cancer risk reduction: A program for Hispanic American families. Health Psychology.
15(6): 413–422.
Fouad MN, Kiefe CI, Bartolucci AA, Burst NM, Ulene V, Harvey MR. 1997. A hypertension
control program tailored to unskilled and minority workers. Ethnicity and Disease. 7(3):
191–199.
Friedell GH, Linville LH, Rubio A, Wagner WD, Tucker TC. 1997. What providers should
know about community cancer control. Cancer Practice. 5(6): 367–374.
Gallup Organization. 2002. Gallup Poll. Roper Center for Public Opinion Research. In:
Public Opinion Online. Storrs, CT: The Roper Center for Public Opinion Research.
Goldstein D, Flory J. 1997. Health care commerce on the net: The revolution begins. Medical
Interface. 10(8): 56–58.
Good BJ, Good MJD. 1981. The meaning of symptoms: A cultural hermeneutic model for
clinical practice. In: The Relevance of Social Science for Medicine. Eisenberg L, Kleinman
A, Editors. Dordecht, Holland: Reidel. Pp. 165–196.
Guidry JJ, Fagan P, Walker V. 1998. Cultural sensitivity and readability of breast and pros-
tate printed cancer education materials targeting African Americans. Journal of the Na-
tional Medical Association. 90(3): 165–169.
Hartman TJ, McCarthy PR, Park RJ, Schuster E, Kushi LH. 1997. Results of a community-
based low-literacy nutrition education program. Journal of Community Health. 22(5):
325–341.
Hearth-Holmes M, Murphy PW, Davis TC, Nandy I, Elder CG, Broadwell LH, Wolf RE.
1997. Literacy in patients with a chronic disease: Systemic lupus erythematosus and the
reading level of patient education materials. Journal of Rheumatology. 24(12): 2335–
2339.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services. [Online]. Available: http://www.health.gov/healthypeople [accessed: Janu-
ary 15, 2003].
HHS. 2001. National Standards for Culturally and Linguistically Appropriate Services in
Health Care. Executive Summary. Washington, DC: U.S. DHHS Office of Minority
Health.
Hill J. 1997. A practical guide to patient education and information giving. Baillieres Clinical
Rheumatology. 11(1): 109–127.
Hornick R, Maklan D, Cadell D, Barmada CH, Jacobsen L, Prado A, Romantan A, Orwin R,
Sridharan S, Zanutto E, Baskin R, Chu A, Morin C, Taylor K, Steele D. 2002. Evalua-
tion of the National Youth Anti-Drug Campaign: Fifth Semi-annual Report of Findings.
Delivered to: The National Institute on Drug Abuse. WESTAT Corporation and Annen-
berg School for Communication.
Hosey GM, Freeman WL, Stracqualursi F, Gohdes D. 1990. Designing and evaluating diabe-
tes education material for American Indians. Diabetes Educator. 16(5): 407–414.
Houston TK, Allison JJ. 2002. Users of Internet health information: Differences by health
status. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 4(2): E7.
Institute for Healthcare Advancement. 2003. Health Literacy: An Overview and Research-
Supported Solutions. [Online]. Available: http://www.iha4health.org/pdf/research_
brochures.pdf [accessed: December 9, 2003].
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2000. Promoting Health: Intervention Strategies from Social
and Behavioral Research. Smedley BD, Syme SL, Editors. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
IOM. 2001. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Wash-
ington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2002. Speaking of Health: Assessing Health Communication Strategies for Diverse
Populations. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
IOM. 2003a. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care.
Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies
Press.
IOM. 2003b. Priority Areas for National Action: Transforming Healthcare Quality. Adams
K, Corrigan JM, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Jolly BT, Scott JL, Feied CF, Sanford SM. 1993. Functional illiteracy among emergency
department patients: A preliminary study. Annals of Emergency Medicine. 22(3): 573–
578.
Jolly BT, Scott JL, Sanford SM. 1995. Simplification of emergency department discharge
instructions improves patient comprehension. Annals of Emergency Medicine. 26(4):
443–446.
Kalichman SC, Benotsch E, Weinhardt L. 2001. Quality of health information on the Internet.
Journal of the American Medical Association. 286(17): 2092–2093; author reply 2094–
2095.
Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey (NALS). Washington, DC: National
Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education.
Kumanyika SK, Adams-Campbell L, Van Horn B, Ten Have TR, Treu JA, Askov E, Williams
J, Achterberg C, Zaghloul S, Monsegu D, Bright M, Stoy DB, Malone-Jackson M,
Mooney D, Deiling S, Caulfield J. 1999. Outcomes of a cardiovascular nutrition coun-
seling program in African-Americans with elevated blood pressure or cholesterol level.
Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 99(11): 1380–1391.
Lazarus W, Mora F. 2000. Online Content for Low-income and Underserved Americans: The
Digital Divide’s New Frontier. A Strategic Audit of Activities and Opportunities. Santa
Monica, CA: The Children’s Partnership.
Lillington L, Royce J, Novak D, Ruvalcaba M, Chlebowski R. 1995. Evaluation of a smoking
cessation program for pregnant minority women. Cancer Practice. 3(3): 157–163.
Lincoln JS. 2003. The Dream in Native American and Other Primitive Cultures. New York:
Dover Publications.
Logan PD, Schwab RA, Salomone JA 3rd, Watson WA. 1996. Patient understanding of
emergency department discharge instructions. Southern Medical Journal. 89(8): 770–
774.
Long A, Scrimshaw SC, Hernandez N. 1992. Transcultural epilepsy services. In: Rapid As-
sessment Procedures: Qualitative Methodologies for Planning and Evaluation of Health
Related Programmes. Scrimshaw NS, Gleason GR, Editors. Boston, MA: International
Nutrition Foundation for Developing Countries. Pp. 205–214.
Maguire P. 1999. How direct-to-consumer advertising is putting the squeeze on physicians.
ACP-ASIM Observer. March.
Matthews M Jr. 2001. Who’s Afraid of Pharmaceutical Advertising? A Response to a Chang-
ing Health System. Policy Report 155. Lewisville, TX: Institute for Policy Innovation.
Mazur DJ. 2003. The New Medical Conversation: Media, Patients, Doctors and the Ethics of
Scientific Communication. New York: Rowman and Littlefield.
MCCAP. 2003. Who We Are. How Does MCCAP Work? [Online]. Available: http://www.
mccapny.org/about.htm [accessed: December, 2003].
Medvantx. 2003. How Can Medvantx Help You Manage Escalating Drug Costs? [Online].
Available: http://www.medvantx.com/ [accessed: October, 2003].
Morse A. 2003. Language Access: Helping Non-English Speakers Navigate Health and Hu-
man Services. National Conference of State Legislature’s Children’s Policy Initiative.
Educational Systems
. . . As young medical students, you and I learned more about the patho-
physiology of disease than we learned about answering these questions for
our future patients.”
O
verall, the U.S. educational systems offer a primary point of inter
vention to improve the quality of literacy and health literacy. The
educational systems discussed in this chapter are the K-12 system,
the adult education system, and education for health professionals. Public
educational systems in the United States are influenced by national policy
and funding, but remain under the jurisdiction of and are funded by states
and localities.
142
K-12 Education
BOX 5-1
The National Health Education Standards
In 1995, the Joint Committee on National Health Standards published the National
Health Education Standards (NHES) subtitled Achieving Health Literacy (Joint
Committee on National Health Education Standards, 1995). The standards de-
scribe the knowledge and skills essential for health literacy and detail what stu-
dents should know and be able to do in health education by the end of grades 4, 8,
and 11. The standards describe a health-literate person as a critical thinker and
problem solver, a responsible, productive citizen, a self-directed learner, and an
effective communicator.
The NHES identified obstacles that continue to impede health education programs,
including:
• Lack of appreciation for the relationship between health status and success in
academic and work performance,
• Low levels of commitment by school board members and administrators,
• Inadequately prepared teachers,
• Insufficient funding for resources and staff development,
• Overcrowded curricula with little or no time for health education (Pateman,
2002; Thackeray et al., 2002),
• Unconnected and seemingly irrelevant health instruction,
• Lack of recognition of the contribution made by health education to the achieve-
ment of the academic goals of schools,
• Failure to adequately document student performance in achievement of health
literacy.
elementary school staff from five schools in Philadelphia felt only “some-
what prepared” to teach health education (Hausman and Ruzek, 1995).
These findings highlight the importance of professional development for
teachers who provide classroom health education to young students.
Many teachers are also required by state guidelines to include specific
topics and standards within their curriculums, often in response to state-
mandated tests. Even within good health education curricula, teachers can-
not address all topics and issues at a single grade level. Although health
education may be included within the required curriculum, it might not be
included within state-mandated tests and therefore these topics will receive
less attention in the classroom (Pateman et al., 1999). A call to strengthen
school health education by health education with state assessment require-
ments was made soon after the NHES were published (Collins et al., 1995),
but a low level of grant support for health literacy assessment persists.
A national health promotion and disease prevention report recom-
mends that the United States increase the proportion of middle, junior, and
senior high schools that provide health education to prevent health prob-
lems in areas such as unintentional injuries; violence; suicide; tobacco use
and addiction; alcohol and other drug use; unintended pregnancy; HIV/
AIDS and STD infection; unhealthy dietary patterns; inadequate physical
activity; and environmental health (HHS, 2000).
The World Health Organization (1996) has described several barriers
that may impede the implementation of school health programs at local,
state, national, and international levels. First, education, health, and politi-
cal leaders, as well as the public at large, often do not possess accurate
knowledge of modern school health programs and their potential impact
on health. Second, many believe the most important function of schools to
be the improvement of language, mathematical, and scientific skills. Third,
some may not support modern school health programs because some ele-
ments of some programs may be controversial (e.g., school programs to
educate about, and prevent, HIV infection, other prevalent STDs, and
unintended pregnancy). Fourth, modern school health programs require
effective collaboration, especially among separate education and health
agencies.
Unfortunately, among 38 states that participated in the School Health
Education Profiles Study, the percentage of schools that required a health
education course decreased between 1996 and 2000, as did the percentage
of schools that taught about dietary behaviors and nutrition, and about
how HIV is transmitted. During the year 2000, only 27 percent of schools
required health education in grade 6, a number that fell to 2 percent in
grade 12 (Storch et al., 2003). A similar pattern is observed in Canadian
schools. In 1999, over 70 percent of Canadian school districts reported that
health education was mandatory in grades 3 through 5, but only 20 percent
Science Education
Science education provides a clear opportunity for implementation of
health literacy education programs and content. An example of this asso-
ciation is the Curriculum Linking Science Education and Health Literacy
program. This project transformed inner-city children’s, teachers’, and par-
ents’ or care givers’ experiences with food into an inquiry-based science
program. Guidelines for science education include content standards for
personal and community health (American Association for the Advance-
ment of Science, 1993; NRC, 1996).
Science teachers have indicated that scientist participation can
strengthen science education. A survey called “The Bayer Facts of Science
Education V,” conducted by Bayer Corporation and the National Science
Teachers Association (1999), indicated that 98 percent of the science
teachers believe that direct student–scientist interaction within classroom
was important. These findings suggest an opportunity for science and
health-care professionals to participate in school science and health educa-
tion programs to improve the health literacy of pre-college students. In
fact, more than half of 107 elementary school teachers at 31 schools re-
ported wanting classroom visits by health professionals (Thackeray et al.,
2002).
Literacy Education
The subject of literacy instruction and achievement in schools, particu-
larly reading, is more conspicuous in the political and mainstream arenas
than is health education. While health education holds promise in promot-
ing full health literacy insofar as it leads to the acquisition of the necessary
health-related knowledge, the issue of basic literacy is equally essential to
full health literacy. As detailed in previous chapters, much of the research
on health literacy documents the difficulties with printed health texts expe-
rienced by adults who are low in overall literacy skill.
Two influential reports have been issued by the National Research
Council (NRC, 1998) and the National Institute of Child Health and Hu-
man Development (National Reading Panel, 2000) addressing the failure of
schools to produce adults who are sufficiently literate to participate in an
increasingly information-driven and competitive economy. These reports,
along with a collection of Congressional mandates, helped lead to the No
Child Left Behind legislation,1 which is driving major instructional change
designed to improve the levels of achievement in the schools. The major
strategy of No Child Left Behind is to hold all schools accountable for
ensuring that students achieve certain standards in subjects that comprise
basic education, including language, science, and mathematics. It is too
soon to report any results of these changes, but increasing numbers of
schools and school districts are attending to the issues raised by the national
concern with literacy achievement.
Literacy practitioners and scholars have taken these findings and ap-
plied them to the vexing problem of why literacy skills learned in school are
often not applied to literacy tasks in life. One obvious implication of this
research is that reading and writing skills must be learned in the context of
texts and literacy purposes that readers will encounter out in the world.
Therefore, one needs to teach reading skills in those contexts. Health texts
and purposes for reading them make up one of those real-life literacy
domains. A survey conducted by Bayer and the National Science Teachers
Association (Bayer Corporation and the National Association of Science
Teachers, 1999) indicated that 98 percent of science teachers surveyed
believe that direct interaction within classroom with health professionals
was important. These findings suggest that the participation of health-care
professionals in school health education programs would improve the health
literacy of pre-college students.
Embedding health literacy instruction can be done with the two types
of literacy instruction needed to improve health literacy: basic print literacy
instruction and literacy instruction in text types common to the field of
health literacy. This latter type of instruction introduces the idea of teach-
ing functional print literacy based on using and understanding real-life text
types. Several studies, funded by the National Science Foundation and the
Interagency Educational Research Initiative, have examined the outcomes
of introducing more expository texts2 into primary-grade instruction dur-
ing years typically devoted to basic literacy learning. One study looked at
the effect on basic literacy growth of adding non-narrative texts to the
typical mix of stories used by first- and second-grade teachers (Duke, 2000).
Results indicate that students whose teachers diversified their materials
learned as much as those who did not, confirming that this approach was
not detrimental to the development of beginning reading skill and writing
abilities.
A longitudinal study, termed the TEXT study (Purcell-Gates and Duke,
2000), has shown that children as young as second and third graders can
grow in their abilities to read and to write two types of texts often found in
the health field: science informational texts and science procedural texts.
Since individual unfamiliarity with different text types often used to convey
health information is a health literacy challenge, this study is important in
that it is the only one to date that addresses the teaching of text types
specifically, and, in this case, the teaching of health literacy-significant text
types. For this study, second- and third-grade teachers were randomly as-
signed to one of two conditions: (a) The Authentic Only condition, where
teachers had their students reading science informational and science proce-
2Expository texts are statements or rhetorical discourse intended to give information about
or an explanation of difficult material.
dural texts that were constructed similar to those found in the real world
for real-world purposes of learning new science information or for actually
conducting experimental procedures. They also wrote science informational
texts and science procedural texts for real-world purposes of providing
readers with information of providing written procedures that allow read-
ers to conduct scientific investigations. (b) The Authentic-plus-Explicit con-
ditions added explicit teaching of language features associated with each
text type to the authentic reading and writing just described for condition
(a). Examples of language features include (for science informational texts)
generic nouns (whales rather than Willy the Whale) and timeless verbs
(whales eat rather than Willy the Whale ate) and (for science procedural
texts) a materials section before the ordered steps that are usually num-
bered.
Analysis of the TEXT data indicates that the children in both condi-
tions grew significantly in their abilities to read and write these science-
related text types. Although no significant differences were found due to
explicit teaching of the language features, their reading comprehension and
writing ability of these two types of texts were significantly related to how
authentic the reading and writing assignments were in each class. All of the
students learned the features of these two text types commonly employed in
the health field, and those students whose teachers used more ‘authentic’
texts and purposes for reading and writing them learned them to a greater
degree. The fact that this can occur at such an early age implies that one
does not need to wait until middle or high school to begin teaching about
the different text types so commonly used to convey health information.
This study was funded by the National Science Foundation and the Inter-
agency Education Research Initiative.
Several studies funded by the National Science Foundation and the
Interagency Educational Research Initiative have examined the outcomes of
introducing more expository texts into primary-grade instruction during
years typically devoted to basic literacy learning. A study by Duke (2000)
looked at the effect on basic literacy growth of adding non-narrative texts
to the typical mix of stories used by first- and second-grade teachers. Re-
sults are showing that students whose teachers utilized different types of
texts in the lessons grew as much as those who did not, confirming that
children this young could read and learn from non-narrative texts and that
this approach was not detrimental to the development of beginning reading
skill and writing abilities.
3These nine content areas are: alcohol and other drugs, injury prevention, nutrition, physi-
cal activity, sexual health, tobacco, mental health, personal and consumer health, and com-
munity and environmental health.
4 These six core concepts and skills are: accessing information, self-management,
internal and external influences, interpersonal communication, decision-making/goal-
setting, and advocacy.
(Matthews and Sewell, 2002). For example, the state of California has
developed a tool to aid health education curriculum development at the
local level and to promote collaborations between schools, parents, and the
community, called “Health Framework for California’s Public Schools,
Kindergarten through Grade Twelve” and the State of Alaska produced
“Healthy Reading Kits” for grades 2 through 8 (Matthews and Sewell,
2002). The state of New Jersey has implemented core curriculum content
standards for comprehensive health and physical education programs which
include health literacy. The goal of the standards is to develop citizens who
are both health-literate and physically educated. The standards for compre-
hensive health and physical education emphasize six primary areas (Morse,
2002):
5The committee thanks John Comings, Ed.D., for his contributions to this section of the
report.
6Workforce Investment Act of 1998. P.L. 105-220, 1998 H.R. 1385, enacted on August 7,
1998, 112 Stat 936. Codified as: Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act, 29 U.S.C. § 794d.
the National Institute for Literacy, and the National Center for the Study of
Adult Learning and Literacy (NCSALL). Thus, ABEL represents a collabo-
rative activity that spans government, private, and volunteer activities at
the federal, state, and community levels, which could have a large effect on
health literacy.
In fiscal year 1998, the ABEL system provided English language ser-
vices to approximately 2 million adults, high school equivalence prepara-
tion services to 800,000 adults, and basic skills services to 1,300,000 adults
(U.S. Department of Education, 1999). Each year, between 3 million and 4
million adults spend some time in an ABEL program. Though the mean
hours of participation is only 72, a significant percentage of students drop
out within the first 30 hours, and so more than half of the students are
receiving at least 100 hours of instruction. Most of these adults are in the
primary target population of health literacy programs. ABEL programs are,
therefore, an effective venue for health literacy activities. However, the
potential demand for these services was much greater and may be affected
by the fact that the ABEL system has limited resources for one-time devel-
opmental costs that produce curriculum, materials, and teacher training
designs. These efforts, and effective adult education programs to improve
health literacy, could be made available to more people through a coopera-
tive effort between the health system and the ABEL system to undertake a
research and development agenda that would lead to educational programs
that served the needs of health literacy and the needs of English language
and basic skills instruction.
7The reading and writing of more complex texts is associated with higher levels of literacy
as defined by the NALS assessment.
ment of competent physicians and other health-care providers who can help
to improve health literacy and to limit the negative effects of limited health
literacy among patients. Furthermore, research should investigate whether
increased health literacy skills in care providers such as medical assistants,
home health-care workers, and home health aides could contribute to im-
proved health-care quality and reduced medical errors. Approaches to edu-
cation for health professionals should include both curricular and continu-
ing education to reach the greatest number of providers at all stages of
career development. The approaches described below may provide a start-
ing point for increased integration of health literacy concepts and skills into
professional and continuing education programs. Further information on
the relationship of health literacy to health-care quality for all categories of
providers could help to develop future directions for such integration.
Curricular Approaches
Few official requirements or curricula address health literacy in schools
of medicine, public health, nursing, dentistry, or pharmacy. Health literacy
issues may be addressed under topics such as patient communication, but
they are generally not systematically included in these topics. Plomer and
colleagues (2001) reported on the development and implementation phases
of a project to improve medical students’ communication with limited lit-
eracy patients by incorporating literacy content into the medical student
curriculum. In this study, the use of standardized patient cases regarding
cancer screening was implemented and results revealed that group discus-
sion about literacy was prompted.
There are a few examples of courses or curriculums that should be
noted. In 1995, the Harvard School of Public Health initiated an ongoing
graduate course for students in public health that focused on health literacy
studies, research, theories, and implications (NCSALL, 2001). In addition,
the Harvard School of Public Health provides a web site (http://www.hsph.
harvard.edu/healthliteracy) about health literacy for researchers and practi-
tioners that includes a video slide show, curriculums, literature reviews,
annotated bibliographies, and policy initiatives. Another curricular ap-
proach took place at the University of Colorado Medical School in Denver
where a course on health literacy for medical students was developed and
taught during 2000 as part of a grant. This was a temporary initiative
however, and was not made a permanent part of the curriculum.
A more formal approach has been instituted at the University of Vir-
ginia School of Medicine (Dalton, 2003). This curriculum includes an
introductory lecture for first-year medical students, departments, residents,
and external institutions that request a presentation on health literacy. A
faculty development handbook is given to all faculty teaching courses in
the first and second year, which provides background information and a
list of available health literacy materials. Health literacy concepts are also
integrated into other courses in the medical school curriculum; for ex-
ample, patient case studies are presented in the second-year “Clinical Prob-
lems” course in which patients experience barriers related to communica-
tion misunderstandings and language issues. Also in the second year, the
required community preceptorship includes a health literacy component. A
fourth-year elective focusing on health literacy issues and including a ser-
vice component is currently in development and will likely be offered in
the spring of 2004. The University of Virginia (UVA) School of Medicine
also provides a web interface to help other institutions develop health
literacy curricula. It is made up of three main groups of information which
can be individually tailored to the needs of an institution: (1) an outline on
how UVA established its curriculum, with reference materials that include
a faculty development handbook and examples of written cases used at
various points of the curriculum; (2) an introductory health literacy lec-
ture, examples of illustrations, and a bibliography and resources list; (3)
standardized patient cases that illustrate work with patients with limited
literacy, that also show how to work with interpreters for the deaf and for
non-English-speaking patients; some of these case studies also integrate
cultural competency issues.
aware of its impact on patient care. In addition, the same percentage also
reported a lack of any institutional policy within their organization ad-
dressing health literacy or no assessment of the effectiveness of existing
policies (Brown et al., in press).
The American Medical Association (AMA) has developed several pro-
grams in professional continuing education in health literacy since adopting
a policy in 1998 that recognized that limited patient literacy affects medical
diagnosis and treatment. The AMA and the AMA Foundation have since
raised awareness and shared best practices about health literacy. In 2003,
the AMA Foundation, American Public Health Association, the National
Council on the Aging, and other public health organizations formed the
Partnership for Clear Health Communication, a coalition to increase aware-
ness of health literacy and its impact on the nation’s health, and introduced
a solution-oriented program that includes the “Ask Me 3” program that
promotes communication between health-care providers and patients
(AMA, 2003b; Ask Me 3, 2003). In conjunction with California Literacy,
Inc., and the California Medical Association (CMA), the AMA and AMA
Foundation developed the California Statewide Health Initiative that pro-
motes provider–patient communication as a basis for patient understanding
(AMA, 2003a). The AMA Foundation, with support from Pfizer, Inc., also
links organizations across the country through Health Literacy Coalition,
and provides grants to health literacy community service projects.
The AMA Foundation has developed and distributed educational kits
to physicians and health-care professionals. This program, “Health Lit-
eracy, Let Your Patients Understand,” includes a CD-ROM for use by
providers in a continuing education curriculum. The 2003 Health Literacy
Educational Kit is the Foundation’s primary tool for informing physicians,
health-care professionals, and patient advocates about health literacy. The
2003 Health Literacy Educational Kit is an expanded version of the kit
introduced in 2001. Included are a manual for clinicians, a new video
documentary, reprintable information, guidelines for continuing medical
education credit, and additional resources for education and involvement.
The AMA Foundation provides these kits free to AMA Alliance chapters
and state, county, and specialty medical societies that make a formal com-
mitment to launch health literacy educational programs of their own, and
to that end provide an extensive “train the trainer” program with a faculty
guide to the clinician workshop and guidelines for local implementation
planning.
Finding 5-2 Opportunities for measuring literacy skill levels required for health
knowledge and skills, and for the implementation of programs to increase learner’s
skill levels, currently exist in adult education programs and provide promising mod-
els for expanding programs. Studies indicate a desire on the part of adult learners
and adult education programs to form partnerships with health communities.
Finding 5-3 Health professionals and staff have limited education, training, con-
tinuing education, and practice opportunities to develop skills for improving health
literacy.
Recommendation 5-1 Accreditation requirements for all public and private edu-
cational institutions should require the implementation of the NHES.
REFERENCES
Academy for Educational Development. 2002. AED@work—Media Smart Youth Program.
[Online]. Available: http://www.aed.org/about/atWork/mediasmartyouth.html [accessed:
September, 2003].
ACCME (Accreditation Council for Continuing Medical Education). 2002. Definition
of CME. [Online]. Available: http://www.accme.org/incoming/pol_05_def_cme.pdf
[accessed: October, 2003].
ACCME. 2003. ACCME Annual Report Data 2002. [Online]. Available: http://www.accme.
org/incoming/156_2002_Annual_Report_Data.pdf [accessed: October, 2003].
AMA (American Medical Association). 2003a. Health Literacy Top Concern of CMA/AMA.
[Online]. Available: http://www.ama-assn.org/ama/pub/article/2403-7454.html [accessed:
October, 2003].
AMA. May 1, 2003b. AMA Foundation Teams Up to Help Fight Low Health Literacy.
[Online]. Available: http://www.ama-assn.org/ama/pub/article/2403-7627.html [accessed:
October, 2003].
American Association for the Advancement of Science. 1993. Benchmarks for Science Lit-
eracy. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Ask Me 3. 2003. Good Guestions for Your Health. [Online]. Available: http://www.askme3.
org/ [accessed: October, 2003].
Association of American Colleges and Universities. 2002. Summary of the 2001 Summer
Symposium of the Program for Health and Higher Education. College Students As a
Challenge and Opportunity for Public Health. Occasional Paper 1. Washington, DC:
Association of American Colleges and Universities.
Bandura A. 1977. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Barton AC, Hindin TJ, Contendo IR, Trudeau M, Yang K, Hagiwara S, Koch PD. 2001.
Underprivelaged urban mothers’ perspectives on science. Journal of Research in Science
Teaching. 38(6): 688–711.
Bayer Corporation and the National Association of Science Teachers. 1999. Nation’s Science
Teachers Register Concern over U.S. Science Education in New Survey. [Online]. Avail-
able: http://www.bayerus.com/msms/news/pages/factsofscience/survey99.html [accessed:
August, 2003].
Becker MH. 1974. The Health Belief Model and Personal Health Behavior. Thorofare, NJ:
Charles B. Slack.
Becker MH, Maiman LA, Kirscht JP, Haefner DP, Drachman RH. 1977. The Health Belief
Model and prediction of dietary compliance: A field experiment. Journal of Health and
Social Behavior. 18(4): 348–366.
Bereiter C. 1997. Situated cognition and how to overcome it. In: Situated Cognition: Social,
Semiotic and Psychological Perspectives. Kirschner D, Whitsun JA, Editors. Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Bigge ML. 1997. Learning Theories for Teachers. 5th edition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
Educational Publishers.
Brown DR, Ludwig R, Buck GA, Durham D, Shumard T, Graham SS. In press. Health
literacy: Universal precautions needed. Journal of Allied Health.
Burns W. 1999. Learning for Our Common Health: How an Academic Focus on HIV/AIDS
Will Improve Education and Health. Washington, DC: Association of American Col-
leges and Universities.
CCSSO (Council of Chief State School Officers; Society of State Directors of Health, Physical
Education, and Recreation; and Association of State and Territorial Health Officials).
2003. Coordinated School Health Programs Staff: 2002–2003 Directory. Washington,
DC: Council of Chief State School Officers.
CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). 2002. Youth Media Campaign: VERB
Working Together. [Online]. Available: http://www.cdc.gov/youthcampaign/working_
together/index.htm [accessed: September, 2003].
Collins J, Robin L, Wooley S, Fenley D, Hunt P, Taylor J, Haber D, Kolbe L. 2002. Pro-
grams-that-work: CDC’s guide to effective programs that reduce health-risk behavior of
youth. Journal of School Health. 72(3): 93–99.
Collins JL, Small ML, Kann L, Pateman BC, Gold RS, Kolbe LJ. 1995. School health educa-
tion. Journal of School Health. 65(8): 302–311.
Council of Chief State School Officers and State Collaborative on Assessment and Student
Standards. 1998. Assessing Health Literacy: Assessment Framework. Santa Cruz, CA:
Toucan Education.
Dalton, C (University of Virginia Health System). 2003. Building a Health Literacy Curricu-
lum. [Online]. Available: http://www.healthsystem.virginia.edu/internet/som-hlc/home.
cfm [accessed: December, 2003].
Doak LG, Doak CC, Meade CD. 1996. Strategies to improve cancer education materials.
Oncology Nursing Forum. 23(8): 1305–1312.
Douglas KA, Collins JL, Warren C, Kann L, Gold R, Clayton S, Ross JG, Kolbe LJ. 1997.
Results from the 1995 National College Health Risk Behavior Survey. Journal of Ameri-
can College Health. 46(2): 55–66.
Duke NK. 2000. 3.6 minutes a day: The scarcity of informational texts in first grade. Reading
Research Quarterly. 35: 202–224.
Freire P. 1973. Education for Critical Consciousness. New York: Seabury Press.
The Gallup Organization. 1994. Values and Opinions of Comprehensive School Health Edu-
cation in U.S. Public Schools: Adolescents, Parents, and School District Administrators.
Atlanta, GA: American Cancer Society.
Green LW, Kreuter MW. 1999. Health Promotion Planning: An Educational and Ecological
Approach. 3rd edition. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.
Grunbaum JA, Kann L, Kinchen SA, Williams B, Ross JG, Lowry R, Kolbe L. 2002. Youth
risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2001. Journal of School Health. 72(8): 313–
328.
Hausman AJ, Ruzek SB. 1995. Implementation of comprehensive school health education in
elementary schools: Focus on teacher concerns. Journal of School Health. 65(3): 81–86.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services. [Online]. Available: http://www.health.gov/healthypeople [accessed: Janu-
ary 15, 2003].
Hochbaum GM. 1958. Public Participation in Medical Screening Programs: A Sociopsycho-
logiocal Study. Public Health Service Publication No. 572. Washington, DC: Govern-
ment Printing Office.
Howard-Pitney B, Winkleby MA, Albright CL, Bruce B, Fortmann SP. 1997. The Stanford
Nutrition Action Program: A dietary fat intervention for low-literacy adults. American
Journal of Public Health. 87(12): 1971–1976.
Inflexicon. 2001. Inflexicon Products: Special Report. [Online]. Available: http://www.
inflexxion.com/inf/products/prod_special.html [accessed: September, 2003].
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 1997. Schools and Health: Our Nations Investment. Allensworth
D, Lawson E, Nicholson L, Wyche J, Editors. Washington, DC: National Academy
Press.
Jacobson E, Degener S, Purcell-Gates V. 2003. Creating Authentic Materials for the Adult
Literacy Classroom: A Handbook for Practitioners. Cambridge, MA: World Education
Inc.
Joint Committee on National Health Education Standards. 1995. National Health Education
Standards: Achieving Health Literacy. Atlanta, GA: American Cancer Society.
Kann L, Warren CW, Harris WA, Collins JL, Williams BI, Ross JG, Kolbe LJ. 1996. Youth
risk behavior surveillance—United States, 1995. Journal of School Health. 66(10): 365–
377.
Kann L, Brener ND, Allensworth DD. 2001. Health education: Results from the School
Health Policies and Programs Study 2000. Journal of School Health. 71(7): 266–278.
Keeling R. 2001. Briefing Paper: College Students As a Challenge and an Opportunity for
Public Health. Washington, DC: Association of American Colleges and Universities.
Knowles MS. 1980. The Modern Practice of Adult Education: From Pedagogy to Andragogy.
Chicago, IL: Association Press/Follet.
Kolbe L, Jones J, Birdthistle I, Whitman C. 2001. Building the capacity of schools to improve
health. In: Critical Issues in Global Health. Koop CE, Pearson C, Schwarz M, Editors.
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Kolbe LJ. 1986. Increasing the impact of school health promotion programs: Emerging re-
search perspectives. Health Education. 17(5): 47–52.
Kolbe LJ. 2002. Education reform and the goals of modern school health programs. The State
Education Standard. 3(4): 4–11.
Kolbe LJ, Collins J, Cortese P. 1997. Building the capacity of schools to improve the health of
the nation. A call for assistance from psychologists. American Psychologist. 52(3): 256–
265.
Lohrmann DK, Wooley SF. 1998. Comprehensive school health education. In: A Guide to
Coordinated School Health Programs. Marx E, Wooley SF, Northrop D, Editors. New
York: Teachers College Press. Pp. 43–66.
Marx E, Wooley S, Northrup D. 1998. Health Is Academic: A Guide to Coordinated School
Health Programs. New York: Teachers College Press.
Marzano R, Kendall J, Cicchinelli L. 1998. What Americans Believe Students Should Know:
A Survey of U.S. Adults. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of
Educational Research and Improvement.
Matthews TL, Sewell JC. 2002. State Official’s Guide to Health Literacy. Lexington, KY: The
Council of State Governments.
Mayer RE, Wittrock MC. 1996. Problem-solving transfer. In: Handbook of Educational
Psychology. Berliner DD, Calfee RC, Editors. New York: Macmillan.
McCall D, Beazley R, Doherty-Poirier M, Lovato C, MacKinnon D, Otis J, Shannon M.
1999. Schools, Public Health, Sexuality and HIV: A Status Report. Toronto: Council of
Ministers of Education, Canada.
Meade CD. 2001. Community health education. In: Community Health Nursing: Promoting
the Health of Aggregates. 3rd edition. Nies M, McEwen M, Editors. Philadelphia: W.B.
Saunders Co.
Morse L. 2002. Improving Health Literacy: An Educational Response to a Public Health
Problem. Presentation given at a workshop of the Institute of Medicine Committee on
Health Literacy. December 11, 2002, Washington, DC.
Murphy PW, Davis TC, Mayeaux EJ, Sentell T, Arnold C, Rebouche C. 1996. Teaching
nutrition education in adult learning centers: Linking literacy, health care, and the com-
munity. Journal of Community Health Nursing. 13(3): 149–158.
National Reading Panel. 2000. Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of
the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and Its Implications for Reading Instruc-
tion. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
NCSALL. 2001. Health Literacy Curricula. [Online]. Available: http://www.hsph.harvard.edu/
healthliteracy/curricula.html [accessed: September 26, 2003].
NRC (National Research Council). 1996. National Science Education Standards. Washing-
ton, DC: National Academy Press.
NRC. 1998. Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children. Snow CE, Burns MS, Griffen
P, Editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
NRC. 1999. Designing Mathematics or Science Curriculum Programs. A Guide for Using
Mathematics and Science Education Standards. Washington, DC: National Academy
Press.
Pateman B. 2002. A sharper image for school health education: Hawaii’s “seven by seven”
curriculum focus. Journal of School Health. 72(9): 381–384.
Pateman B. 2003. Healthier students, better learners. Educational Leadership. 61(4).
Pateman B, Grunbaum JA, Kann L. 1999. Voices from the field—A qualitative analysis of
classroom, school, district, and state health education policies and programs. Journal of
School Health. 69(7): 258–263.
Patrick K, Grace TW, Lovato CY. 1992. Health issues for college students. Annual Review of
Public Health. 13: 253–268.
Patterson S, Cinelli B, Sankaran G, Brey R, Nye R. 1996. Health instruction responsibilities
for elementary classroom teachers in Pennsylvania. Journal of School Health. 66(1): 13–
17.
Pender NJ. 1996. Health Promotion in Nursing Practice. 3rd edition. Stamford, CA: Appleton
and Lange.
Perkins DN. 1992. Smart Schools: From Training Memories to Educating Minds. New York:
Free Press/Macmillan.
Peterson FL, Cooper RJ, Laird JM. 2001. Enhancing teacher health literacy in school health
promotion: A vision for the new millennium. Journal of School Health. 71(4): 138–144.
Plomer K, Schneider L, Barley G, Cifuentes M, Dignan M. 2001. Improving medical students’
communication with limited-literacy patients: Project development and implementation.
Journal of Cancer Education. 16(2): 68–71.
Purcell-Gates V, Duke NK. 2000. Explicit Explanation of Genre Within Authentic Literacy
Activities in Science: Does it Facilitate Development and Achievement? Grant Proposal
funded by NSF/IERI. Grant Proposal funded by NSF/IERI.
Purcell-Gates V, Degener S, Jacobson E, Soler M. 2000. Affecting Change in Literacy Prac-
tices of Adult Learners: Impact of Two Dimensions of Instruction. NCSALL Report No.
17. Boston, MA: National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Purcell-Gates V, Degener S, Jacobson E, Soler M. 2002. Impact of authentic literacy instruc-
tion on adult literacy practices. Reading Research Quarterly. 37: 70–92.
Purtilo R, Haddad A. 1996. Health Professional and Patient Interaction. 5th edition. Phila-
delphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Co.
Rosenstock IM. 1966. Why people use health services. Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly.
44(Supplement 3): 94–127.
Rudd RE. 2002. A maturing partnership. Focus on Basics: Connecting Research and Practice.
5(3).
Rudd RE, Moeykens BA. 1999. Adult educators’ perceptions of health issues and topics in
adult basic education. NCSALL Report #8. Cambridge, MA: National Center for the
Study of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Rudd RE, Zahner L, Banh M. 1999. Findings from a National Survey of State Directors of
Adult Education. NCSALL Report #9. Cambridge, MA: National Center for the Study
of Adult Learning and Literacy.
Smith AM. 1999. Age of Risk Behavior Debut: Trends and Implications. Washington, DC:
Institute for Youth Development.
St. Leger L. 2001. Schools, health literacy and public health: Possibilities and challenges.
Health Promotion International. 16(2): 197–205.
Storch P, Grunbaum J, Kann L, Williams B, Kinchen S, Kolbe L. 2003. School Health Educa-
tion Profiles: Surveillance for Characteristics of Health Education Among Secondary
Schools (Profiles 2000). Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
Thackeray R, Neiger BLBH, Hill SC, Barnes MD. 2002. Elementary school teacher’s perspec-
tives on health instruction: Implications for health education. American Journal of Health
Education. 33: 77–82.
Ubbes VA, Cottrell RR, Ausherman JA, Black JM, Wilson P, Gill C, Snider J. 1999. Profes-
sional preparation of elementary teachers in Ohio: Status of K-6 health education. Jour-
nal of School Health. 69(1): 17–21.
U.S. Department of Education, Office of Vocational and Adult Education, Division of Adult
Education and Literacy. 1999. State-Administered Adult Education Program 1998 En-
rollment. [Online]. Available: http://www.ed.gov/offices/OVAE/98enrlbp.html [accessed:
December, 2003].
U.S. Department of Education. 2001. Digest of Education Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
WHO (World Health Organization). 1996. Improving School Health Programs: Barriers and
strategies. Geneva: World Health Organization.
WHO. 1997. Promoting Health Through Schools: Report of a WHO Expert Committee on
Comprehensive School Health Education and Promotion. Geneva: World Health Orga-
nization.
Health Systems
The fifth doctor to take a history and examine Mr. G discovered that he had
taken none of his seven chronic medications, nor the newly prescribed anti-
biotic given to him when his infection first appeared. Mr. G. explained “You
see, I already take 19 pills a day, and when I got another one I got confused
about my timing, and I was just so scared I might mess up. My daughter
usually helps me with my medicines, but she’s been sick and I didn’t want to
worry her.”
167
N
avigating the U.S. health-care and public health delivery systems is
a complex task with numerous layers of bureaucracy, procedures,
and processes. Consequently, an adult’s ability or inability to navi-
gate these systems may reflect systemic complexity as well as individual
skill levels. Patients, clients, and their family members are typically unfamil-
iar with these systems and the associated jargon. Even highly educated
individuals may find the systems too complicated to understand, especially
when people are made more vulnerable by poor health. Official documents,
including informed consent forms, social services forms, and public health
and medical instructions, as well as health information materials often use
jargon and technical language that make them unnecessarily difficult to use
(Rudd et al., 2000).
When you have medical forms and stuff, I don’t think it should be compli-
cated for a person to understand what its saying (Rudd and DeJong, 2000).
Some of the complexity of the health-care system arises from the nature
of health care and public health itself, the mix of public and private financ-
ing, and the health information and health-care delivery settings. Unlike
many other countries, the United States does not have a single organized
national health-care system. Furthermore, the United States has no national
health surveillance system, and few common norms exist for basic preven-
tive services such as immunizations. Threats of bioterrorism and new emerg-
ing diseases such as SARS continue to complicate the picture of health care.
In the past, health management was primarily the domain of the phy-
sician, but greater responsibility for health management has shifted to the
patient as health care has evolved and cost pressures on care have in-
creased. This has been called self-management, and was identified in the
Institute of Medicine (IOM) Priority Areas for National Action report
(IOM, 2003b) as one of two cross-cutting issues in improving health-care
quality that present the opportunity to improve health across the lifespan,
at all stages of health service. In order to make appropriate self-manage-
ment decisions, health information consumers must locate health informa-
tion, evaluate the information for credibility and quality, and analyze the
risks and benefits, activities that rely on health literacy skills. Consumers
must be able to express health concerns clearly by describing symptoms in
ways the providers can understand. Both patients and health-care provid-
ers must be able to ask pertinent questions and fully understand the avail-
able medical information.
Research
Employers Insurers
ti on s
u ca roup Medical Device
Ed nG
tio Biotech
d ita Health Networks
cre Health Systems
Ac Hospitals
g
tin Heart Community
ul
ns Centers Health Centers
Co
Patients/Enrolled Populations/Community
Hospices Health
Inf Departments
o
Tec rma t
hn tion
olo Cancer en
gy Centers Physician Nursing Homes r nm
Home Health Ambulatory ve
Groups ose Go
Ven Agencies Surgery Centers ipurp ca
l
tion
s
Mult ers cia
t ure
uppli Lo
Ca S sso
pita Pharmaceutical al A
l s ion
Federal Government fes
State Government Pro
Foundations
1See Finding 3-1: “About 90 million adults, an estimate based on the 1992 National Adult
Literacy Survey (NALS), have literacy skills that test below high school level (NALS Levels 1
and 2). Of these, about 40–44 million (NALS Level 1) have difficulty finding information in
unfamiliar or complex texts such as newspaper articles, editorials, medicine labels, forms, or
charts. Because the medical and public health literature indicates that health materials are
complex and often far above high school level, the committee notes that approximately 90
million adults may lack the needed literacy skills to effectively use the U.S. health system. The
majority of these adults are native-born English speakers. Literacy levels are lower among the
elderly, those who have lower educational levels, those who are poor, minority populations,
and groups with limited English proficiency such as recent immigrants.”
Finding 6-1 Demands for reading, writing, and numeracy skills are
intensified due to health-care systems’ complexities, advancements in scien-
tific discoveries, and new technologies. These demands exceed the health
literacy skills of most adults in the United States.
Each of these topics is briefly discussed in the section that follows, with
particular attention to the ways in which these issues may function as
barriers to those with limited health literacy. Other issues that are currently
salient in the health-care context include health disparities and the increas-
ingly complex health information context; these topics are discussed in
Chapter 4.
2The information in this section is drawn in part from the background paper “Improving
Chronic Disease Care for Populations with Limited Health Literacy,” commissioned by the
committee from Dean Schillinger, M.D. The committee appreciates his contribution. The full
text of the paper can be found in Appendix B.
for what, and when to call for assistance with a problem) as a particularly
daunting aspect of chronic disease management (Baker et al., 1996).
Building on the Chronic Care Model of Wagner et al. (2001),
Schillinger has developed a framework that explores ways to improve
chronic disease care and is based on health literacy and related research.
This framework is discussed in greater detail in Appendix B, but briefly,
the model represents the evidence base for chronic disease care and sup-
ports the importance of executive leadership and incentives to promote
quality, systems to track and monitor patients’ progress and support timely
provider decision-making (Piette, 2000), patient self-management training
(Lorig et al., 1999; Von Korff et al., 1997), and community-oriented care.
Since self-management practices and clinical outcomes in chronic disease
care appear to vary by patients’ level of health literacy (Kalichman and
Rompa, 2000; Schillinger et al., 2002; Williams et al., 1998b), the Chronic
Care Model, and similar comprehensive, population-based disease man-
agement approaches, may offer insights into the ways in which limited
health literacy affects chronic disease care and could identify points for
potentially successful intervention.
Patient–Provider Communication
To Err Is Human reported that communication failure was the under-
lying cause of fully 10 percent of adverse drug events (IOM, 2000b). Man-
agement of complex drug therapies, especially in elderly patients, is ex-
tremely difficult and requires special attention to the ability of the patient to
understand and remember the amount and timing of dose, as well as behav-
ioral modifications required by the regimen (e.g., dietary restrictions) (IOM,
2000b). The patient’s health literacy level as it affects the communication
process is therefore an important consideration in health outcomes.
There is some evidence of a failure of communication with patients
with limited health literacy as currently measured. Patients with chronic
diseases and limited health literacy have been shown to have poor knowl-
edge of their condition and of its management (Williams et al., 1998a, b),
often despite having received standard self-management education. Patients
with limited health literacy have greater difficulties accurately reporting
their medication regimens and describing the reasons for which their medi-
cations were prescribed (Schillinger et al., 2003a; Williams et al., 1995) and
more frequently have explanatory models3 that may interfere with adher-
3“Explanatory models are the lenses through which cultures perceive and understand ill-
ness. They consist of interpretive notions about aspects of illness and treatment including the
cause, the timing and mode of onset of the symptoms, the pathophysiological processes
involved, the natural history and severity, and the appropriate treatment” (Kleinman, 1980).
“Then the ho’ola said Mom should confess to me and before God Jehovah.
She did. She asked me to forgive her and I did. I wasn’t angry. . . . And later
Mom’s sickness left her. Of course, she still had diabetes, but the rest—being
so confused and miserable—all that left her” (Shook, 1985: 109).
To the extent that low health literacy may be more prevalent among
some racial and ethnic minority groups, these individuals may be at higher
risk for adverse events stemming from poor communication. There is a
need to understand the independent contributions of cultural competence
and health literacy to patient safety, as well as the interactions between
cultural competence and health literacy.
Patients’ varied perspectives, values, beliefs, and behaviors regarding
health and illness are consistently cited as integral to quality care in several
IOM reports (2001, 2003a). To protect people from undue harm, eliminate
errors and adverse events, prevent unnecessary human suffering, and be
more accountable to quality and cost-effective care, cultural processes
beckon attention, particularly to erode the culture of blame, reform team-
work, and redesign organizational dynamics within health-care systems. A
principle of patient safety is to include patients in safety designs and the
processes of care (IOM, 2000b). To do so, it is essential to understand
cultural nuances of what patient safety means to different people and what
beliefs, values, and actions inform people’s understanding of safe care come
into play.
The communication process plays a central role in this understanding.
For example, a recent study in California of pediatric outpatient visits that
used Spanish-language interpreters found an average of 31 interpretation
errors per pediatric clinic visit (Flores et al., 2003), two-thirds of which had
clinical consequences. The errors included errors in the dose and duration
4The committee thanks Arlene Bierman, M.D., M.S., for her contributions to this section of
the report.
I prepared all my material, cut all my pieces of Formica, and I opened the
can and I didn’t really read the label. There was a red label on the can and
even if I would have looked at it, I didn’t know what it was. I couldn’t even
read it. Come to find out my lung was poisoned from that material I was
using (Rudd and DeJong, 2000).
Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Cen-
tury (IOM, 2001) proposed that health-care processes are to be redesigned
according to 10 rules, 6 of which allude to cultural context and 5 of which
relate to health literacy via information and communications. These rules
are shown in Box 6-1. Twenty-first century health care will unravel the
complexities that arise from the interplay of cultural processes and health
literacy. Proposed changes include customizing care based on patient needs
and values and accommodating differences in patient preferences and an-
BOX 6-1
Rules for Redesigning Health-Care Processes
and the Uninsured, 2003a). Rates of uninsurance in the United States vary
by race and ethnicity at 12 percent of Caucasians, 20 percent of Asian
Americans, 22 percent of African Americans, more than 25 percent of
Native Americans, and more than 33 percent of Hispanics (Kaiser Commis-
sion on Medicaid and the Uninsured, 2003b). Uninsured patients are more
likely to have poorer health than they would if insured (IOM, 2002a).
Many African-American women who are uninsured or underinsured put
their families’ welfare ahead of their own, especially when financial re-
sources are limited. They seek treatment too late or not at all (IOM, 1999).
Further, people who are uninsured or underinsured are more likely to rely
on emergency departments for care (IOM, 2002b). An example of this can
be seen in asthma. If someone cannot afford the appropriate workup, con-
sultation, and medications to properly control asthma, they (or their chil-
dren in the case of childhood asthma) will be less likely to prevent attacks
that may lead to emergency department use (IOM, 2002b).
The problems created by the financial inability to use services may be
exacerbated by health literacy issues, including limited knowledge and in-
formation surrounding early symptoms of serious illness and the value of
prevention. Preliminary evidence discussed in Chapter 3 suggests that these
socioeconomic, cultural, and health literacy factors may be associated with
higher costs in the long run, when expensive tertiary care and emergency
department services become necessary.
Limited local health resources and services can also impede access to
care. Grocery stores and heath services are among the many resources that
are limited in low-income and remote areas. Transportation may be a ma-
jor obstacle; if services are difficult to get to or far away, people may do
without, or postpone the use of the service. For some individuals often only
limited primary care treatment services are available conveniently. So, for
example, women needing mammograms may have to travel to a facility
where this can be done, which can be a deterrent to obtaining that service.
Limitations on hours of availability can also reduce access. Some clinics
operate only or primarily during business hours. This may create problems
for many people seeking health services. In particular, low-income patients
may have a harder time getting time off from work, or finding child care, in
order to seek care or take a family member to get care. Health services that
have weekend and evening hours could potentially help prevent the unnec-
essary use of emergency departments.
Health Expenditures
Health expenditures are of concern to decision makers in both the
public and private sectors, and particularly to private employers and small
businesses. Health spending has increased as a percentage of gross domestic
product (GDP) as growth in health expenditures has outpaced growth in
the economy as a whole. The Department of Health and Human Services
(HHS)5 projects that the increase in health expenditures will fall to about 7
percent in the 2003–2007 period, and to 6.6 percent during 2008–2011
(Heffler et al., 2002). During this period health spending will continue to
5National health expenditures and future projections are reported annually by the Office of
the Actuary in the Department of Health and Human Services. It reports on total national
health expenditures, including all services such as hospitalization, physician and dentist care,
pharmaceuticals, nursing homes, home health, and other costs; these also cover all sources of
payment such as Medicare, Medicaid, different forms of private insurance and managed care
plans, and out-of-pocket payments by individuals.
outpace the overall economy by 2.5 percent per year, resulting in growth
from 13.2 percent of GDP in 2000 to 17 percent in 2011. There is no
reason to believe that these increases will moderate in the near term, and
policy attention from both public and private sectors will continue. This
attention has led to a renewed discussion of health-care policy as a na-
tional- and state-level issue that has included fundamental questions such as
whether health insurance should be tied to employment, the relative roles of
the public and private sectors, and whether consumers themselves should
assume more responsibility for health-care costs and quality.
The most recent national estimate of expenditures for 2001 is $1.4
trillion or $5,035 per capita, based on a growth rate of 8.7 percent in 2001
(Levit et al., 2003). These expenditures are the highest in the world. The
reasons for these high rates are complex. Although greater volume is often
cited as the primary reason for higher costs in the United States as com-
pared to other countries, it has been found that prices of services, rather
than volume, are responsible for higher costs in the United States. Adminis-
trative costs in the private sector are also much higher than in other coun-
tries or in Medicare. While some combination of new technology and in-
creased demand has led to increases in both prices and utilization, these
vary across the nation. A recent study indicates that variation in expendi-
tures between states is in part due to underlying demographics like age and
wage rates, and in part due to the supply of hospitals and physicians (Mar-
tin et al., 2002). In addition, states with higher managed care penetration
have lower expenditures. This is thought to be attributable to “both lower
premium rates charged by HMOs and spillover effects of competition on
non HMO premiums” (Martin et al., 2002).
Preliminary evidence on the cost implications of limited health literacy
(presented in Chapter 3), while not conclusive, give some idea of the mag-
nitude of impact of health literacy on national medical expenditures. Ef-
forts to improve health literacy, or to limit its detrimental effects, may
provide an important contribution to health-care policy addressing rising
health expenditures (Heffler et al., 2002; Levit et al., 2002; Martin et al.,
2002).
6The committee thanks Frank McClellan, J.D., for his contributions to this section of the
report. Mr. McClellan summarized and interpreted the law in relation to health literacy for
the committee.
7Title VI of the Civil Rights Act, and its implementing regulations. Americans with Disabili-
ties Act.
8A wrongful act other than a breach of contract for which relief may be obtained in the
form of damages or an injunction.
this evidence; other evidence may be found in federal and state statutes and
their implementing regulations, accreditation standards of professional as-
sociations, and by-laws of hospitals.
The treating physician or nurse is held liable for an injury only where
ordinary care in accordance with the standards of the profession would
have avoided the accident, rather than as a guarantor of a good outcome.
Therefore, in most instances the professions control the standards by which
their members are judged. There are two important qualifications to the
profession’s control of the standards. First, the provider may be found
negligent if he or she has special knowledge indicating that following the
ordinary standard in the particular circumstances involved will expose the
patient to an unreasonable risk of harm. Second, in rare instances, a court
may declare that the whole profession may be found negligent for following
a practice that new information or alternatives have shown to be unduly
dangerous.
The concept of reasonable care can be applied to considerations of
health literacy in the context of health care. The case of Incollingo v.
Ewing9 provided a court opinion reflecting both the rule that a profession
(or a large percentage of it) may be found negligent, and the rule that each
physician must use his or her own personal knowledge. This case involved
a child who died as a result of suffering aplastic anemia due to the con-
sumption of a wide-spectrum antibiotic (Chloromycetin) prescribed at first
by the child’s pediatrician for a throat infection and abdominal pains, and
then renewed by a second physician after a telephone request by the child’s
mother.
The doctor who initially prescribed Choloromycetin sought to justify
his conduct on the ground that he was aware of the risk that the drug could
cause aplastic anemia and therefore made sure not to allow for a renewal of
the prescription. In his view, the child first presented to him with a throat
infection that he regarded as a major ailment. He argued that even the
plaintiff’s expert agreed that in prescribing this antibiotic he acted in the
same manner as 95 percent of the doctors in Philadelphia where he prac-
ticed. The court noted that the drug’s package insert warned that the drug
should not be prescribed for minor ailments, and the mother produced
expert testimony at trial that the child indeed had a minor ailment when the
drug was first prescribed. Consequently, the court found that the jury was
allowed to find the first doctor negligent for not using his personal knowl-
edge appropriately.
The doctor who renewed the prescription sought to defend his action
on the ground that most doctors in Philadelphia would have prescribed the
same drug for a minor ailment at that time, based on a belief that the drug
was effective and posed a very small risk of serious harm. He acknowledged
that the package insert warned against such prescriptions of the drug, but
contended that the medical community often prescribes the drug because
the manufacturer had minimized its risks in communications directly to the
prescribing doctors. Rejecting this argument, the court emphasized that
medical custom is not always controlling, reflecting the rule that a profes-
sion (or a large percentage of it) may be found negligent. The health-care
provider must exercise reasonable care, “giving due regard to the advanced
state the profession at the time.”10
A similar view was expressed in Helling v. Carey11 (1974), in which the
appellate court declared as a matter of law that a prevailing medical custom
was negligent. A patient suffered blindness as a result of undiagnosed and
untreated glaucoma. The patient had been under the care of the physicians
for a number of years, and had never received a test of intraocular pressure.
The prevailing custom of ophthalmologists was to perform routine pressure
tests for early glaucoma only on patients who were over 40 years of age,
because the risk of glaucoma was, in their view, too small in persons under
40 to justify routine pressure tests. The court ruled that the doctors were
negligent as a matter of law, notwithstanding the evidence that they fol-
lowed the custom of their specialty at the time. Noting that a low-risk,
inexpensive test could have prevented a serious illness such as blindness in
a long-term patient who was experiencing loss of vision, the court declared
that reasonable care dictated the performance of the test.
This suggests that if future health literacy research supports the exist-
ence of associations between low-risk, inexpensive approaches to limited
health literacy and reduced morbidity or mortality, the rule that a profes-
sion (or a large percentage of it) may be found negligent could apply to the
failure to use health literacy interventions in clinical settings, including
programmatic changes in health information provision. Providers could
also be responsible when their knowledge indicated that a lack of patient
understanding would expose the patient to an unreasonable risk of harm.
Informed consent in health care and research. In most cases, consent forms
involve the use of structured and technical language to disclose subjects’
rights, roles, and responsibilities. They contain complex descriptions of
institutional practices, financial and insurance considerations, legal con-
cerns, advanced medical technologies, and potential risk/benefit consider-
ations. Cumulative research over the past two decades and across three
continents shows that consent forms for treatment and research are written
at a level beyond the skills of most patients involved in research (Criscione
et al., 2003; Freda et al., 1998; Goldstein et al., 1996; Gribble, 1999;
Grossman et al., 1994; Grundner, 1980; Hammerschmidt and Keane, 1992;
Hopper et al., 1995, 1998; Jubelirer, 1991; Lawson and Adamson, 1995;
LoVerde et al., 1989; Mader and Playe, 1997; Mathew and McGrath,
2002; McManus and Wheatley, 2003; Meade and Byrd, 1989; Ogloff and
Otto, 1991; Ordovas et al., 1999; Osuna et al., 1998; Rivera et al., 1992;
Tait et al., 2003; Tarnowski et al., 1990). Examples of consent form text at
different reading levels are provided in Table 6-1.
The readability of consent forms is often at a scientific level that con-
tributes to information overload, poor understanding, and misinformed
consent (Benson and Forman, 2002; Davis et al., 1994; Hopper et al., 1995;
Meade and Howser, 1992; Philipson et al., 1995; Raich et al., 2001; Reicken
and Rovich, 1982; Sugarman et al., 1998, 2002). In 2003, Paasch-Orlow et
al. (2003) reported results of a cross-sectional study of 114 web sites from
U.S. medical schools regarding their Institutional Review Board (IRB) read-
ability standards and informed consent templates. Specific readability stan-
dards were found on 61 web sites (54 percent) and were found to range
from a fifth-grade reading level to a tenth-grade reading level, while other
sites contained descriptive guidelines such as “simple lay language.” Results
revealed that informed consent text often falls short of the institutions’ own
readability standards and suggest that federal oversight is associated with
better readability. Figure 6-2 shows the difference between the readability
of informed consent forms and the readability required by the IRBs.
Furthermore, while patients who sign consent documents often report
their understanding of the research or treatment and satisfaction with the
consent process, they may not fully understand the consent given (Horng et
al., 2002; Pope et al., 2003; Vohra et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2003).
As pointed out in Chapter 1 of this report, all people (not just those
with low educational levels) are at risk for low health literacy. The in-
formed consent process brings with it particular challenges that may further
impede understanding. This is in part attributable to the inherent complex-
4th Grade† “You don’t have to be in this “There is no benefit to you from
research study. You can agree to being in the study. Your taking
be in the study now and change part may help patients in the
your mind later. Your decision future.”‡
will not affect your regular care.
Your doctor’s attitude toward
you will not change.”
12th Grade§ “Your participation in this study is “There may be no direct benefit to
strictly voluntary. You have the me, however, information from
right to choose not to participate this study may benefit other
or to withdraw your patients with similar medical
participation at any point in this problems in the future.”
study without prejudice to your
future health care or other
services to which you are
otherwise entitled.”
*All the examples are taken directly from medical-school Web sites unless otherwise noted.
†The readability level is based on the Flesch-Kincaid readability scale.
‡The passage was modified to present key concepts at a fourth-grade reading level.
§The readability level is based on the Fry readability formula.
SOURCE: Excerpted from Table 1 in Paasche-Orlow et al. (2003). Copyright © 2003 Massa-
chusetts Medical Society. All rights reserved. Reprinted with permission.
ity and nature of informed consent information. But, it also relates to the
multitude of psychosocial, ethical, and situational factors that may sur-
round the clinical need for informed consent, such as hospitalization, emer-
gency heart surgery, participation in Phase I cancer clinical trial, genetic
testing, new vaccine for HIV, use of surgical placebos for Parkinson’s dis-
ease, or separation of conjoined twins.
–8 –6 –4 –2 0 2 4 6 8
Difference between Grade Levels (actual minus target)
FIGURE 6-2 Difference between actual readability and target readability of in-
formed consent documents. Each bar represents 1 of the 61 institutional review
boards that indicated a specific grade-level target as a readability standard.
SOURCE: Paasche-Orlow et al. (2003). Copyright 2003. Massachusetts Medical
Society. All rights reserved.
“I was in my early 30s and having problems with my girl parts. I was bulging
in the vaginal area and I knew this was not normal. The doctor told me that
it would be an easy repair and could be done. The surgery was set up. On
the night before surgery, I remember having lots of papers pushed toward
me to sign. I signed them because I needed to do this. I had surgery the next
day and my recovery went very well. I had a large scar on my lower
abdomen. I went for my six-week follow-up visit and was asked by the nurse
how I was doing since my hysterectomy. No one had ever used those words
before, but I knew what they meant. I had never asked any questions. I made
the assumption that all doctors knew exactly what they were doing and had
better intelligence than me. I was too humiliated to reveal to the doctor and
nurse that I did not know what had been done to me. Communication had
broken down and failed me.”
“No one knew that I could not read well. Actually, I could read . . . but only
one word at a time. By the time I got to the end of a sentence—I had no
comprehension of what I had just read. I struggled to read. All that I read I
would read three times. I kept books in front of me so others would not find
out. I thought that if others found out, that they would think I was stupid. To
check on the spelling of a word, I would call the library (that way no one
could see me). There are many ways to hide poor reading from others.”
Personal story graciously provided by Toni Cordell, Adult Learner and Lit-
eracy Advocate, as told to C.D. Meade, September 2003.
surgery. She had no idea that it meant he faced a risk of loss of control of
his bladder, and thus she did not include that information when she at-
tempted to explain the risks he was confronting. In light of this testimony
the judge, sitting without a jury, ruled that informed consent was not
obtained. The court explained:
The physician is required to disclose material risks in such terms as a
reasonable doctor would believe a reasonable patient would understand.
In order for a reasonable patient to have awareness of a risk he should be
told in lay language the nature and severity of the risk and the likelihood
of its occurrence. A bland statement as to a risk of ‘loss of function of
bodily organs’ when not accompanied by any estimate of its frequency
does not amount to understandable communication of any specific real
risk.15
The case supports the concept that a written document of a patient’s
consent is evidence that an informed consent was obtained, but is not
conclusive and can be rebutted by other evidence. Thus, while it is impor-
tant to have documentary evidence of advising a patient of risks, benefits,
and alternatives, the existence of that document does not prevent the court
from considering whether the information deemed critical to making a
meaningful expression of consent to the treatment was conveyed to the
patient by a means that was likely to enable patient comprehension. Signed
consent documents are treated in this manner in most states. Evidence that
the patient could not read or comprehend the form leaves the issue of
whether an informed consent was given to evidence regarding the commu-
nication process, such as verbal conversations, picture displays, and videos.
The same is true with respect to instructions given to the patient or the
patient’s family as to monitoring physical condition, administering medica-
tion, and so forth.
cies can influence the health-care and public health systems to develop and
support integrated strategies addressing health literacy and can increase the
scientific knowledge base about health literacy by fostering research and
collaboration.
In addition, the federal government plays a central role in the produc-
tion and dissemination of health-related information and the regulation of
such information from other sources. The involvement of specific agencies
such as the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and the Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in these activities is described be-
low. One widely known regulatory action in this regard is the Health
Insurance Portability and Accountability Act of 1996. On April 14, 2003,
as mandated by this act and in accordance with the Office of Civil Rights
National Standards to Protect the Privacy of Personal Health Information,
a notice of privacy practices was disseminated to all consumers entering the
health-care system across the country (in hospitals, dental offices, pharma-
cies, and other health service locations).16 The law requires that this infor-
mation be “read and understood” by consumers. Printed information was
distributed in a variety of formats and languages to convey how medical
information may be used and disclosed including information relating to
treatment, payment and health-care operations, business associates,
fundraising, research, appointment reminders, treatment alternatives, ben-
efits and services, and persons involved in a patient’s care. However, the
committee observed that the text disseminated to convey this important
information varied widely in its nature, scope, and complexity. It is very
likely that the privacy documents were written above the reading level of
many Americans. Until formal evaluations are conducted, it remains un-
known how well consumers fully understand the federal regulations that
must be “read and understood” before care is provided.
We summarize here the activities of those agencies with important roles
relating to health literacy in the health system context. This is not an
exhaustive list of all federal agencies with related work, but is intended to
highlight those that either have ongoing activity in this area or show par-
ticular promise to influence the issue. Chapters 4 and 5 highlight some of
the health literacy-related activities of federal agencies in the contexts of the
educational system and culture and society, including those related to lan-
guage issues and interpretation in health care.
levels, from a variety of ethnic, racial, income, and health experience back-
grounds.
The Food and Drug Administration The FDA regulates and provides in-
formation about drugs, biological products, medical and radiological de-
vices, the food supply, and cosmetics. Three general areas of FDA activity
related to health literacy are advertising, outreach, and labeling.
The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention As the lead public health
agency of the United States, the CDC has a central role in successfully
communicating information on health and illness to all members of the
public. The CDC identifies “Providing credible information to enhance
health decisions” (CDC, 2003) as one of the central goals of its mission.
Related to health literacy, the CDC’s focus has encompassed efforts around
plain language including training, testing and pre-testing materials, surveys,
and the provision of health information to TV shows, networks, writers,
and producers. The CDC has addressed issues of culture in several of its
programs. For example, the National Institute for Occupational Safety and
Health added a Spanish-language section to its web site in 2001, and the
National Immunization Program developed educational material for Ameri-
can Indians and Native Alaskans in 2003. Currently, CDC is redesigning its
web site based on a CDC web evaluation completed in 2002. The evalua-
tion showed that consumers looking for basic health information regarding
disease and disease prevention are the largest segment of visitors to the
CDC web site.
18The committee thanks Patrick Weld and K. Visnawath, Ph.D., of the National Cancer
Institute for their contributions to this section of the report. Mr. Weld and Dr. Viswanath
performed this search and analysis of the CRISP database. An expanded description of the
methods of their work can be found in Appendix A.
50
40
Dollars in Millions
30
20
10
0
1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002
Year
FIGURE 6-3 National Institutes of Health grant funding over the 1997–2002
period.
of health literacy. A total of 906 grants for a 10-year period were identified,
but 229 of the grants from 1993–1996 contain missing financial data. The
figure shows the funding totals from the 565 grants that remained after
eliminating grants that were not funded, and grants for which data were
missing.
“Low-literacy” components were included in studies in cancer, child-
hood development and reading, arthritis, asthma, diabetes, HIV, mental
health, Alzheimer’s, health disparities, and studies in Spanish-speaking
populations. In addition, “low literacy” was included in seven Requests for
Applications since 2000, in studies such as Environmental Justice (spon-
sored by the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences), Adult
and Family Literacy (sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health
and Human Development), Native American Research Centers for Health,
oral health disparities, diet and physical activity assessment, and cancer
communications.
Although the increase in funding over the past decade clearly represents
a positive trend, the amount funded by NIH for programs and projects that
examine health literacy is a small segment of the NIH budget. This amount,
which is equivalent to $20 to $50 million per year, has consistently been
less than half of 1 percent of the annual grant funding by NIH.
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality The AHRQ is the part of
HHS that sponsors, carries out, and disseminates research on health-care
quality, medical errors and patient safety, health-care cost, and health dis-
parities. AHRQ recently sponsored an evidence-based review of health
literacy research to answer the following questions:
Although the results of this project were not available at the time
of publication of this report, AHRQ worked with the IOM to provide
preliminary information that was valuable in informing the work of the
Committee.
They span all races, numerous cultures, all levels of education, all socioeco-
nomic strata, and both genders, with approximately 85 percent males. The
VA patient population also shows a wide age range of 18 years and up,
with two basic spikes—20- to 30-year-old veterans with short military
stints, and those over 50 years, representing retired military. The heteroge-
neity of the population points to the need for awareness of differences in
attitudes, perceptions, and level of technological adeptness. Disabilities in
the veteran population (including blindness, deafness, and mental disor-
ders) also present barriers to access and communication. The NCP uses
various techniques to ensure that educational and health promotional ma-
terials respond to all of these issues and needs.
BOX 6-2
State Medicaid Managed Care Contracts
Information from the Center for Health Services Research and Policy’s database
on Medicaid managed care contracts (Rosenbaum et al., 2001) indicates that
guidelines and requirements for Medicaid managed care providers vary across the
states. Language in medicaid managed care contracts, valid as of 2001, shows
that although a number of states’ contracts do address some literacy- and lan-
guage-related issues, there are no specific requirements related to health literacy
in any state, and many states do not address these issues at all or do so inconsis-
tently.
Among the 42 states with risk contracts that are captured in the Medicaid Managed
Care database, 33 states and the District of Columbia included in their Medicaid
managed care contracts some type of requirement pertaining to the provision of
easily understood information. There is a great deal of variability across the states
in how this is defined, and the method of determining if the requirement is being
met. Notably, in most states, the requirement refers to the understanding of written
material only. An exception is California, which requires that both verbal and writ-
ten information be provided in a manner that can be easily understood.
In some cases, the Medicaid managed care contract includes general language
about patient comprehension such as the statement in the Massachusetts Medic-
aid Contract that requires that “written information…[be provided]…in a format and
manner that is easily readable, comprehensible to its intended audience…” (Ros-
enbaum et al., 2001). Frequently, states indicate that written information must be
readable at a specific reading grade level (see Chapter 2 for a discussion of read-
ing grade levels). The reading grade level indicated varies significantly across
states, ranging from as high as grade 8 in Montana and North Dakota, to grade 4
in a number of states, to a less-specific “suitable reading comprehension level” in
Maryland. Still other states require that “plain language” be used. Overall, a sixth-
health literacy issues that are currently part of the NCQA standards for
managed care organizations, such as:
grade reading level is the most frequently cited requirement among the states that
mention a reading level.
A related issue is that some of the information provided to patients must be provid-
ed “as written” in federal law, and Medicaid managed contracts may specifically
exclude such language from requirements to be easily understood. For example,
in Texas, patient information about advance directives is excluded from its general
readability requirement since certain language is required by federal law.
Methods to determine readability level or whether information is easily understood
also vary across states. Some states name specific programs, indices, or tests that
must be used to determine reading grade level; these include methods such as the
Fry Readability Index, Flesch Scale Analysis, or the McLaughlin Simplified Mea-
sure of Gobbledygook Index. Other states’ contracts simply state the desired read-
ing grade level. Some states require that the managed care organization itself
determine if its patient information meets the requirements, while in other states
this task is reserved for the state or its designee.
Clearly, much of this language such as that referring to the intended audience
implies that attention should be given to individuals who do not speak English or
who speak English as a second language, but less frequently is this explicitly stat-
ed. A few states do, however, require that information be provided in the other
languages when the minority-language group makes up greater than 10 percent of
the population. However, contracts more frequently include provisions for transla-
tion services and cultural competence. The Medicaid managed care contracts of
12 states do not include a requirement for services to be provided to persons
whose primary language is not English. Although 25 states include a requirement
that Medicaid managed care providers must insure cultural competence, only 10 of
these states explicitly define what is meant by cultural competence.
NOTE: The information presented in this section was obtained from Rosenbaum et al. (2001).
which asks whether the member looked for any information about how a
health plan works in written materials or on the Internet, and asks how
much of a problem, if any, it was to find or understand this information.
19The committee thanks Terry Davis, Ph.D.; Julie Gazmararian, M.P.H., Ph.D.; and Estela
Marin, M.A., for their contributions to this section of the report. Drs. Davis and Gazmararian
and Ms. Marin designed, carried out, and analyzed the survey. An expanded description of
the methods of their work can be found in Appendix A.
categories above, we have chosen not to break it out as a separate area, but
will mention particularly notable efforts in this area where appropriate.
Table 6-3 summarizes results from a sample of studies that have exam-
ined interventions taking place in, or sponsored by, health-care organiza-
tions. They are organized into the categories listed above, although we
acknowledge that there is necessarily some overlap among categories with
some types of interventions. These studies were gleaned from a review of
the database on health literacy performed by Michael Pignone and col-
leagues as a commissioned project for AHRQ,20 as well as from recently
published abstracts and testimony. This selection of studies was chosen to
represent an array of various types of interventions in different populations
and is not intended to be comprehensive. Studies included here did not have
to measure the literacy level of participants. Overall, the studies displayed
in Table 6-3 reflect the early state of the field. Pignone and colleagues
evaluated the strength of the available evidence for health literacy interven-
tions as part of the above-mentioned project for AHRQ (Pignone, 2003).
The four criteria used to determine reliability were (1) the study was ran-
domized, (2) the study design minimized confounding, (3) literacy was
measured, and (4) the result of the intervention was reported by literacy
level.
Five of the studies in Table 6-3 have been identified by Pignone (2003)
as the most methodologically reliable of those that have been published
(Davis et al., 1998b; Meade et al., 1994; Michielutte et al., 1992; Murphy
et al., 2000; Wydra, 2001). Wydra and colleagues (2001) found that their
interactive videodisc intervention increased reported self-care ability among
cancer patients, but that the literacy level of the patients had no effect on
this increase. Meade and colleagues (1994) examined the effectiveness of
conveying information about colon cancer via videotape versus a printed
booklet. Compared to controls, participants receiving any information dem-
onstrated increased knowledge, but the difference between the two modes
of communication was not significant. Davis and colleagues (1998b) found
that those with the lowest literacy levels, after receiving a simplified and
more appealing brochure, did not show a similar increase in comprehension
similar to those with higher literacy skills. In contrast, Michielutte and
colleagues (1992) also provided two different brochures—one with text
and one illustrated—and found that even while two brochures had no
influence on comprehension in the total research sample, those in the low-
est literacy levels showed increased comprehension with the illustrated for-
mat. It is possible that a larger sample would show a different effect.
20The committee would like to thank AHRQ for its assistance with this segment of the
report.
Intervention Outcome
Simplified informed consent form Participants preferred simplified form over the
versus standard form. SWOG form, and 97% thought the simplified
69 participants given Standard form was easier to read than the SWOG form
Southwestern Oncology Group (p < 0.0001). However, there was no
(SWOG) consent form (16th grade significant difference in degree of
level) and 114 participants given understanding of the two forms.
simplified form (7th grade level)
developed for study.
Continued
Intervention Outcome
Continued
Murphy et al., Sleep clinic at Controlled trial n = 192 (20 of which were
2000 Louisiana State (nonrandomized) caregivers)
University Patients at sleep clinic 18
Health Sciences years or older
Center If younger than 18,
caregiver participated
Intervention Outcome
Information about sleep apnea from Short-term knowledge about sleep apnea was
an instructional videotape or a less accurate for those with reading levels less
simple brochure. than 8th grade than for those with reading
levels at or above 9th grade. Video
intervention significantly improved only two
areas of knowledge for readers below grade 8
(p < 0.05).
Continued
Combined Approaches
Tailored Approaches
Hayes, 1998 Emergency Randomized trial n = 60
departments, Elderly emergency room
Midwestern patients
rural area
Intervention Outcome
A measure of the extent to which Higher health literacy and physician application
physicians assess patient recall of interactive communication strategy were
and comprehension in a public associated with good glycemic control (p <
hospital setting. 0.01).
Take 4 times a day, with meals and at bedtime. This medicine may make you drowsy.
BOX 6-3
Example of a Consent Form for Participation in
Smoking Cessation Study
Introduction
You are being asked to take part in a
study that looks at ways to help pregnant
women stop smoking. We want to know
if booklets can help you to stop smoking.
We also want to find out how nurses can
help you to stop smoking. We cannot be
sure that this study will help you, but it
may help others. Taking part in this study
is up to you. You can stop taking part in it
at any time. It will not get in the way of
your care at this clinic.
SOURCE: Doak et al. (1996). Reprinted with permission of the Oncology Nursing Society.
BOX 6-4
Examples of Ongoing Approaches Using Technology-Based
Communication Techniques
from other encounters with the individual (IOM, 2002c). In addition, TDIs
can be initiated by the individual through calls to helplines or services, or by
the health system, through outbound calls (IOM, 2002c). The effect of
these variable characteristics is yet to be evaluated among individuals with
low literacy and low health literacy.
Testimony to the committee has revealed a wide array of activities in
the category of technology-based communication techniques that are cur-
rently being used. Some of these are summarized in Box 6-4.
care systems, and many historically have been designed for patients with
cancer. One of the first patient navigator programs was initiated by Harold
P. Freeman in 1990 at what was then Harlem Hospital Center in New York
City. The impetus for the program came from findings by the American
Cancer Society that poor people face specific barriers when attempting to
seek diagnosis and treatment of cancer and that these barriers result in
failure to seek care, or diagnosis at a later—and less treatable—stage of
disease. This program seeks to alleviate financial barriers, communication
and information barriers, medical system barriers (such as missed appoint-
ments and lost results), and fear and emotional barriers by providing a
navigator to patients with suspicious results from initial screening tests. The
navigator acts as the patient’s advocate in the interval between the screen-
ing and further diagnosis or treatment, assisting with such practical issues
as paperwork for financial support, childcare, or transportation problems.
The navigator also may translate medical jargon into understandable lan-
guage, provide education about the disease and its treatment, assist the
patient in communicating with and asking questions of his or her doctor,
and be available to listen to fears and concerns (Health Care Association of
New York, 2002). Follow-up from the first two years of the program
showed that 87.5 percent of patients with navigators completed recom-
mended breast biopsies, while the rate in patients without a navigator was
56.6 percent. Patients with navigators also completed the biopsy in signifi-
cantly less time than those without navigators (Freeman et al., 1995). The
Patient Navigator Program continues at the newly established Ralph Lauren
Center for Cancer Care and Prevention in Harlem, and similar programs
have since been established in other settings such as Solano County, Cali-
fornia, where a Patient Navigator Program was founded in 1999 by the
Solano Coalition for Better Health–Community Cancer Task Force
(CancerActionNOW.org, 2003) and at the Washington Cancer Institute in
Washington, DC.
Combined Approaches
Some approaches to health literacy have incorporated a number of the
different types of approaches discussed above into one intervention. A
study by Davis and colleagues (1998a) at a public hospital in Louisiana
(shown in Table 6-3 above) sought to increase mammography usage in
women attending ambulatory and eye clinics. One of the interventions
provided a personal recommendation from one of the investigators, an
easy-to-read brochure, and a 12-minute education program using a video
based on focus groups. Results of this randomized controlled trial showed
that this combined approach intervention was the only significant predictor
of increased mammography usage at 6 months after controlling for literacy,
age, race, and baseline knowledge; patients who received only a recommen-
dation or only a brochure did not show increased mammography usage.
Similarly, some promising interventions that have sought to improve under-
standing for informed consent involve the combined use of simplified writ-
ten and verbal presentation to help patients better understand and concen-
trate on the information (Chan et al., 2002; Langdon et al., 2002; Muss et
al., 1979; Sorrell, 1991; Tindall et al., 1994; Young et al., 1990). Research
about the effectiveness of combining tailored print communications with
TDIs remains equivocal, with some studies supporting increased effective-
ness while others showed no effect. This research is reviewed in Speaking of
Health (IOM, 2002c). Molina Healthcare, a managed care organization
that focuses on low-income and Medicaid populations, conducted an evalu-
ation of a low-literacy program of this combined type. Families with a child
younger than six years of age were randomly assigned to either the control
group (n = 13,737 and 5,614 at 2 years) or an intervention group (n =
28,366 and 12,079 at 2 years) that received a self-help book in the mail,
plus follow-up reminders. The self-help book was appropriate for low-
literacy populations and was intended to supplement information provided
by advice nurses that are available by phone to all plan members. Results
after two years showed decreases among the intervention group in parents
taking their child to the emergency department for fever, rash, and diar-
rhea/vomiting and increases in parental self-efficacy for the same symp-
toms. The intervention group also demonstrated a decrease in calls to the
advice nurse, and were more likely to still be members of the plan after two
years than those in the control group (Ryan, 2003).
Box 6-5 describes a promising combined approach to health literacy
that uses written materials, patient educators, provider communication tech-
niques, and follow-up telephone calls.
Tailored Materials
As Chapter 4 emphasizes, culture gives significance to health informa-
tion and messages and thus approaches to health literacy should consider
the background and experiences of people. Patient viewpoints can provide
important insights about what, how, when, where, and in what manner
information can best be presented. Research on the informed consent pro-
cess that has looked at the experience from the perspective of the patient
has found that this can aid in untangling the complex dynamics associated
with the process (Albrecht et al., 2003; Cox, 2002; Ruckdeschel et al.,
1996). As Ratzan (2001) points out, the challenge in designing effective and
understandable health communications is to determine the optimal context,
channels, and content which reflect the realities of people’s everyday lives,
situations, and communication practices. In this way, messages are
BOX 6-5
Chronic Disease Management Program
• A teach-back method in which the patient teaches the content back to the ed-
ucator
• Practical skills rather than complex physiology
• Written educational materials designed for low-literacy users that the educator
reviews with the patient
• Follow-up telephone calls and quick visits by the educator when the patient
returns to the clinic that serve to reinforce the education
• A collaborative learning environment based on sensitivity to the role of literacy
in communication with patients
BOX 6-6
Description of Project Toolbox
questions, needs, and goals of the patient at that time. Research on tailored
print communications has typically shown that they can improve health
outcomes (for review, see Revere and Dunbar, 2001) but research also
suggests that they are less effective at influencing individuals who are not
thinking about making a behavior change (IOM, 2002c). Hayes (1998)
found that patients who received easily readable tailored discharge instruc-
tions subsequently performed better on a test of medication knowledge
than patients given the standard preprinted instructions (see Table 6-3).
Some current approaches that have come to the attention of the committee
also use this strategy. For example, the Veterans Administration designed
and distributes a “Flu Toolkit” that can be tailored to the educational and
cultural needs of specific locales. Another example is currently being inves-
tigated by researchers at the University of California–San Francisco. They
have developed a simple new communication tool that allows clinicians to
print for their patients an individualized, visual medicine schedule (VMS).
The VMS is a computer-generated, single page of paper that contains the
names and digital images of the prescribed doses of medication, with sym-
bols signifying the daily dosing schedule. The VMS displays the doses on a
weekly calendar to accommodate participants with medication doses that
differ from day to day. It can be updated and printed by the provider using
a color printer and can include instructions in Spanish and Chinese in
addition to English.
However, such approaches may not meet the needs of those with lim-
ited health literacy. Investigators from the Department of Obstetrics and
Gynecology at the University of California–San Francisco have developed
and evaluated a computerized, prenatal-testing, decision-assisting tool (“PT
Partnerships
The increasing complexity of health systems—including new defini-
tions of health, evolution of new media and the discovery of innovative
biologic and genomic interventions, as well as the needs of diverse audi-
ences—demands broad, interdisciplinary, multisectoral approaches. An
IOM report, Bridging Disciplines in the Brain: Behavioral and Clinical
Sciences, observes that “Solutions to existing and future health problems
will likely require drawing on a variety of disciplines and approaches in
which interdisciplinary efforts characterize not only the cutting edge of
research but also the utilization of knowledge” (IOM, 2000a: 1).
To advance health literacy, it will be necessary to develop coordinated
activities with local programs and institutions that improve quality and
access to services, strengthen systems, and formulate effective policies.
This includes fostering broad, interdisciplinary approaches to health lit-
eracy and supporting the design and management processes necessary to
make them successful. Additionally, this requires involvement beyond the
traditional health-care system with universities, training institutions, pri-
vate- and public-sector media, and governmental social service depart-
ments.
A number of promising approaches brought to the committee’s atten-
tion have featured interdisciplinary partnerships. Among the longest run-
ning and most well-known of these is the National Literacy and Health
Program in Canada which currently involves a partnership among 27 na-
tional organizations in Canada and is coordinated by the Canadian Public
Health Association. In existence for more than a decade, its objectives are
to (1) raise awareness among health professionals about the links between
literacy and health; (2) build commitment among participating national
health associations to promote literacy and health; and (3) establish links
between national health associations and the federal government, literacy
organizations, and provincial health associations in order to coordinate
action on literacy and health. The program has undertaken a number of
major projects such as producing a multimedia training resource for health
professionals, providing a literacy and health curriculum for youth, and
producing guidelines for prescription medication manufacturers. Although
the National Literacy and Health Program has not as yet been evaluated
formally (such an evaluation is currently under way), it appears to have had
a major impact on both the health professional and literacy community in
Canada.
In the United States, federal-level collaborative efforts have revolved
around Healthy People 2010 (HHS, 2000). Healthy People 2010, as previ-
ously discussed, includes an objective on health literacy which is “to im-
prove the health literacy of persons with marginal or inadequate literacy
skills.” The Office of the Secretary, ODPHP is the lead agency for this
objective, and has been working to identify and coordinate health literacy
activities across HHS, to convene HHS agencies to work collaboratively on
health literacy, and to identify external partners. In 2000, ODPHP estab-
lished a partnership with the National Center for Education Statistics, U.S.
Department of Education, to develop health literacy measures that are
included in the 2003 National Assessment of Adult Literacy. This assess-
ment will provide the first national measures of health literacy. ODPHP has
also collaborated with outside organizations on health literacy including
the AMA and the Academy of General Dentistry.
Some state- and local-level partnerships also currently exist. For ex-
ample, the Santa Clara (California) Medical Center, Santa Clara County
Library, and Plane Tree Health Library have partnered since 2001 to oper-
ate a center for health literacy on the Medical Center campus. The commu-
nity learning center provides information on a variety of medical topics and
conditions in English, Spanish, and Vietnamese in a variety of formats
(print, audio, and video) with a focus on easy-to-read materials. The Medi-
cal Center provides space readily accessible to patients, the Plane Tree
Library provides supervision and expertise in resource development, and
the Santa Clara Library recruits adult literacy students to visit the Center
and provide literacy support to patrons referred by health-care providers.
Since May 2001, more than 2,000 clients have been served.
Summary of Approaches
It can be seen that, as in the survey, most of the examples and studies
cited above fall into the category of “simplifying materials,” which includes
issues of multiple languages and translation, and can be used for enhanced
provider–patient communication as well as in other settings. Several of
them include the dimension of testing materials with the intended users for
understanding and subsequently making appropriate modifications, which
the committee has noted is very important. Some work is now in progress
to develop nonprint approaches, such as visual aids, computer instruction,
and entertainment programs. Many of the federal and some state efforts are
in the training area, most notably many of the programs in the multiple
bureaus of the HRSA. The emerging partnerships in both the public and
private sectors will be critical if sustained and systematic progress is to be
made.
When respondents to the survey discussed above were asked an open-
ended question about lessons learned from their health literacy activities,
three fairly consistent themes about lessons learned emerged:
BOX 6-7
Comments from Respondents to Survey
Finding 6-1 Demands for reading, writing, and numeracy skills are intensified
because of health-care systems’ complexities, advancements in scientific discov-
eries, and new technologies. These demands exceed the health-literacy skills of
most adults in the United States.
Finding 6-2 Health literacy is fundamental to quality care, and relates to three of
the six aims of quality improvement described in the IOM Crossing the Quality
Chasm report: safety, patient-centered care, and equitable treatment. Self-man-
agement and health literacy have been identified by IOM as cross-cutting priorities
for health-care quality and disease prevention.
Finding 6-3 The readability levels of informed consent documents (for research
and clinical practice) exceed the documented average reading levels of the major-
ity of adults in the United States. This has important ethical and legal implications
that have not been fully explored.
Recommendation 6-2 HHS should fund research to define the needed health-
literacy tasks and skills for each of the priority areas for improvement in health-care
quality. Funding priorities should include participatory research that engages the
intended populations.
Recommendation 6-4 HHS should take the lead in developing uniform stan-
dards for addressing health literacy in research, service, and training applications.
This includes addressing the appropriateness of research design and methods
and the match among the readability of instruments, the literacy level, and the
cultural and linguistic needs of study participants. In order to achieve meaningful
research outcomes in all fields:
REFERENCES
Aaronson NK, Visser-Pol E, Leenhouts GH, Muller MJ, van der Schot AC, van Dam FS, Keus
RB, Koning CC, ten Bokkel Huinink WW, van Dongen JA, Dubbelman R. 1996. Tele-
phone-based nursing intervention improves the effectiveness of the informed consent
process in cancer clinical trials. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 14(3): 984–996.
Agre P, McKee K, Gargon N, Kurtz RC. 1997. Patient satisfaction with an informed consent
process. Cancer Practice. 5(3): 162–167.
Albrecht TL, Ruckdeschel JC, Riddle DL, Blanchard CG, Penner LA, Coovert MD, Quinn G.
2003. Communication and consumer decision making about cancer clinical trials. Pa-
tient Education and Counseling. 50(1): 39–42.
Allen CE. 2001. 2000 presidential address: Eliminating health disparities. American Journal
of Public Health. 91(7): 1142–1143.
Appelbaum PS. 1997. Rethinking the conduct of psychiatric research. Archives of General
Psychiatry. 54(2): 117–120.
Baker LM, Wilson FL. 1996. Consumer health materials recommended for public libraries:
Too tough to read? Public Libraries. 35: 124–130.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Pitkin K, Parikh NS, Coates W, Imara M. 1996. The
health care experience of patients with low literacy. Archives of Family Medicine. 5(6):
329–334.
Benitez O, Devaux D, Dausset J. 2002. Audiovisual documentation of oral consent: A new
method of informed consent for illiterate populations. Lancet. 359(9315): 1406–1407.
Bennett CL, Ferreira MR, Davis TC, Kaplan J, Weinberger M, Kuzel T, Seday MA, Sartor O.
1998. Relation between literacy, race, and stage of presentation among low-income
patients with prostate cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 16(9): 3101–3104.
Benson JG, Forman WB. 2002. Comprehension of written health care information in an
affluent geriatric retirement community: Use of the test of functional health literacy.
Gerontology. 48(2): 93–97.
Berland GK, Elliott MN, Morales LS, Algazy JI, Kravitz RL, Broder MS, Kanouse DE, Munoz
JA, Puyol JA, Lara M, Watkins KE, Yang H, McGlynn EA. 2001. Health information
on the Internet: Accessibility, quality, and readability in English and Spanish. Journal of
the American Medical Association. 285(20): 2612–2621.
Bertakis KD. 1977. The communication of information from physician to patient: A method
for increasing patient retention and satisfaction. Journal of Family Practice. 5(2): 217–
222.
Edlin M. 2002. Consumer-Directed Health Care—The goals: More choice, more control.
Healthplan Magazine. 43(2): 12–17.
Elswick, J (BenefitNews.com). 2003. Consumer-Driven Health on a Roll. [Online]. Available:
http://www.benefitnews.com/detail.cfm?id=4734&terms=|elswick| [accessed: August,
2003].
FACCT (Foundation for Accountability). 2002. Who’s in the Drivers Seat? Increasing Con-
sumer Involvement in Health Care. [Online]. Available: http://www.facct.org/facct/doc
libFiles/documentFile_528.pdf [accessed: August, 2003].
Federman AD, Cook EF, Phillips RS, Puopolo AL, Haas JS, Brennan TA, Burstin HR. 2001.
Intention to discontinue care among primary care patients: Influence of physician behav-
ior and process of care. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 16(10): 668–674.
Fiscella K, Franks P, Gold MR, Clancy CM. 2000. Inequality in quality: Addressing socioeco-
nomic, racial, and ethnic disparities in health care. Journal of the American Medical
Association. 283(19): 2579–2584.
Flores G, Laws MB, Mayo SJ, Zuckerman B, Abreu M, Medina L, Hardt EJ. 2003. Errors in
medical interpretation and their potential clinical consequences in pediatric encounters.
Pediatrics. 111(1): 6–14.
Freda MC, DeVore N, Valentine-Adams N, Bombard A, Merkatz IR. 1998. Informed consent
for maternal serum alpha-fetoprotein screening in an inner city population: How in-
formed is it? Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, and Neonatal Nursing. 27(1): 99–106.
Freeman HP, Muth BJ, Kerner JF. 1995. Expanding access to cancer screening and clinical
follow-up among the medically underserved. Cancer Practice. 3(1): 19–30.
Gaba AG, Grossman SA. 2003. Braine Readability of informed consent forms at Johns
Hopkins Hospital clinical oncology research protocols (1991–1999). Proceedings of the
American Society for Clinical Oncology. 22: 524.
Gardner AW, Jones JW. 2002. An audit of the current consent practices of consultant orth-
odontists in the UK. Journal of Orthodontics. 29(4): 330–334.
Gerteis M, Edgman-Levitan S, Walker JD, Stoke DM, Cleary PD, Delbanco TL. 1993. What
patients really want. Health Management Quarterly. 15(3): 2–6.
Goldstein AO, Frasier P, Curtis P, Reid A, Kreher NE. 1996. Consent form readability in
university-sponsored research. Journal of Family Practice. 42(6): 606–611.
Good BJ, Good MJD. 1981. The meaning of symptoms: A cultural hermeneutic model for
clinical practice. In: The Relevance of Social Science for Medicine. Eisenberg L, Kleinman
A, Editors. Dordecht, Holland: Reidel. Pp. 165–196.
Graber MA, Roller CM, Kaeble B. 1999. Readability levels of patient education material on
the World Wide Web. The Journal of Family Practice. 48(1): 58–61.
Gribble JN. 1999. Informed consent documents for BRCA1 and BRCA2 screening: How
large is the readability gap? Patient Education and Counseling. 38(3): 175–183.
Grossman SA, Piantadosi S, Covahey C. 1994. Are informed consent forms that describe
clinical oncology research protocols readable by most patients and their families? Jour-
nal of Clinical Oncology. 12(10): 2211–2215.
Grundner TM. 1980. On the readability of surgical consent forms. New England Journal of
Medicine. 302(16): 900–902.
Hammerschmidt DE, Keane MA. 1992. Institutional Review Board (IRB) review lacks impact
on the readability of consent forms for research. The American Journal of the Medical
Sciences. 304(6): 348–351.
Hayes KS. 1998. Randomized trial of geragogy-based medication instruction in the emer-
gency department. Nursing Research. 47(4): 211–218.
Health Care Association of New York. 2002. HANYS Breast Cancer Demonstration Project.
[Online]. Available: www.hanys.org/quality_index/Breast_Cancer_Project/pnresourcekit.
htm [accessed: September, 2003].
Heffler S, Smith S, Won G, Clemens MK, Keehan S, Zezza M. 2002. Health spending projec-
tions for 2001–2011: The latest outlook. Faster health spending growth and a slowing
economy drive the health spending projection for 2001 up sharply. Health Affairs. 21(2):
207–218.
Hewlett S. 1996. Consent to clinical research—Adequately voluntary or substantially influ-
enced? Journal of Medical Ethics. 22(4): 232–237.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services. [Online]. Available: http://www.health.gov/healthypeople [accessed: Janu-
ary 15, 2003].
Hochhauser M. 1997. Can your HMO’s documents pass the readability test? Managed Care.
6(9): 60A, 60G–60H.
Hopper KD, TenHave TR, Hartzel J. 1995. Informed consent forms for clinical and research
imaging procedures: How much do patients understand? American Journal of Roentgen-
ology. 164(2): 493–496.
Hopper KD, TenHave TR, Tully DA, Hall TE. 1998. The readability of currently used surgi-
cal/procedure consent forms in the United States. Surgery. 123(5): 496–503.
Horng S, Emanuel EJ, Wilfond B, Rackoff J, Martz K, Grady C. 2002. Descriptions of
benefits and risks in consent forms for phase 1 oncology trials. New England Journal of
Medicine. 347(26): 2134–2140.
Hudman J. 2003. Understanding the Medicaid and Low-Income Populations: Implications
for Health Literacy. Presentation given at a workshop of the Institute of Medicine Com-
mittee on Health Literacy. April 30, 2003, Washington, DC.
Hussey LC. 1994. Minimizing effects of low literacy on medication knowledge and compli-
ance among the elderly. Clinical Nursing Research. 3(2): 132–145.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 1999. The Unequal Burden of Cancer: An Assessment of NIH
Research and Programs for Ethnic Minorities and the Medically Underserved. Haynes
MA, Smedley BD, Editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2000a. Bridging Disciplines in the Brain, Behavioral, and Clinical Sciences. Pellmar
TC, Eisenberg L, Editors. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2000b. To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health System. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
IOM. 2001. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for the 21st Century. Wash-
ington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2002a. Care Without Coverage: Too Little, Too Late. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
IOM. 2002b. Health Insurance Is a Family Matter. Washington, DC: The National Acad-
emies Press.
IOM. 2002c. Speaking of Health: Assessing Health Communication Strategies for Diverse
Populations. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
IOM. 2003a. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care.
Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies
Press.
IOM. 2003b. Priority Areas for National Action: Transforming Healthcare Quality. Adams
K, Corrigan JM, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Jacobson TA, Thomas DM, Morton FJ, Offutt G, Shevlin J, Ray S. 1999. Use of a low-
literacy patient education tool to enhance pneumococcal vaccination rates: A random-
ized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 282(7): 646–650.
JCAHO (Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations). 2003. Facts about
the Joint Commission on Accreditation of Healthcare Organizations. [Online]. Avail-
able: http://www.jcaho.org/about+us/index.htm [accessed: October, 2003].
Jubelirer SJ. 1991. Level of reading difficulty in educational pamphlets and informed consent
documents for cancer patients. The West Virginia Medical Journal. 87(12): 554–557.
Kaiser Commission on Medicaid and the Uninsured. 2003a. Access to Care for the Unin-
sured: An Update. Pub. No. 4142. Washington, DC: The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foun-
dation.
Kaiser Commission on Medicaid and the Uninsured. 2003b. The Uninsured: A Primer. Key
Facts about Americans Without Health Insurance. Washington, DC: The Henry J. Kai-
ser Family Foundation.
Kalichman SC, Rompa D. 2000. Functional health literacy is associated with health status
and health-related knowledge in people living with HIV-AIDS. Journal of Acquired
Immune Deficiency Syndromes and Human Retrovirology. 25(4): 337–344.
Kalichman SCP, Ramachandran BB, Catz SP. 1999. Adherence to combination antiretroviral
therapies in HIV patients of low health literacy. Journal of General Internal Medicine.
14(5): 267–273.
Kasper JF, Mulley AG Jr, Wennberg JE. 1992. Developing shared decision-making programs
to improve the quality of health care. Quality Review Bulletin. 18(6): 183–190.
Kaufer DS, Steinberg ER, Toney SD. 1983. Revising medical consent forms: An empirical
model and test. Law, Medicine, and Health Care. 11(4): 155–162.
Kerka S. 2000. Health and Adult Literacy. Practice Application Brief No. 7. Columbus, OH:
ERIC Clearinghouse on Adult, Career, and Vocational Education.
Kim SP, Knight SJ, Tomori C, Colella KM, Schoor RA, Shih L, Kuzel TM, Nadler RB,
Bennett CL. 2001. Health literacy and shared decision making for prostate cancer pa-
tients with low socioeconomic status. Cancer Investigation. 19(7): 684–691.
King J. 2001. Consent: The patients’ view—A summary of findings from a study of patients’
perceptions of their consent to dental care. British Dental Journal. 191(1): 36–40.
Kleinman A. 1980. Patients and Healers in the Context of Culture: An Exploration of the
Borderland Between Anthropology, Medicine, and Psychiatry. Berkeley, CA: University
of California Press.
Langdon IJ, Hardin R, Learmonth ID. 2002. Informed consent for total hip arthroplasty:
Does a written information sheet improve recall by patients? Annals of the Royal Col-
lege of Surgeons of England. 84(6): 404–408.
Lawson SL, Adamson HM. 1995. Informed consent readability: Subject understanding of 15
common consent form phrases. IRB; A Review of Human Subjects Research. 17(5–6):
16–19.
Lechter K. 2002. Patient Information Activities at the FDA. Presentation given at a workshop
of the Institute of Medicine Committee on Health Literacy. December 10, 2002, Wash-
ington, DC.
Levit K, Smith C, Cowan C, Lazenby H, Martin A. 2002. Inflation spurs health spending in
2000. Health Affairs. 21(1): 172–181.
Levit K, Smith C, Cowan C, Lazenby H, Sensenig A, Catlin A. 2003. Trends in U.S. health
care spending, 2001. Health Affairs. 22(1): 154–64.
Linzer M, Konrad TR, Douglas J, McMurray JE, Pathman DE, Williams ES, Schwartz MD,
Gerrity M, Scheckler W, Bigby JA, Rhodes E. 2000. Managed care, time pressure, and
physician job satisfaction: Results from the physician worklife study. Journal of General
Internal Medicine. 15(7): 441–450.
Lorig KR, Sobel DS, Stewart AL, Brown BW Jr, Bandura A, Ritter P, Gonzalez VM, Laurent
DD, Holman HR. 1999. Evidence suggesting that a chronic disease self-management
program can improve health status while reducing hospitalization: A randomized trial.
Medical Care. 37(1): 5–14.
LoVerde ME, Prochazka AV, Byyny RL. 1989. Research consent forms: Continued unread-
ability and increasing length. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 4(5): 410–412.
Muss HB, White DR, Michielutte R, Richards F 2nd, Cooper MR, Williams S, Stuart JJ,
Spurr CL. 1979. Written informed consent in patients with breast cancer. Cancer. 43(4):
1549–1556.
NCI (National Cancer Institute). 1998. Simplification of Informed Consent Documents. U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services. [Online]. Available: http://www.cancer.gov/
clinicaltrials/understanding/simplification-of-informed-consent-docs/ [accessed: Septem-
ber 30, 2003].
Norris SL, Nichols PJ, Caspersen CJ, Glasgow RE, Engelgau MM, Jack L, Isham G, Snyder
SR, Carande-Kulis VG, Garfield S, Briss P, McCulloch D. 2002a. The effectiveness of
disease and case management for people with diabetes. A systematic review. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine. 22(4 Supplement): 15–38.
Norris SL, Nichols PJ, Caspersen CJ, Glasgow RE, Engelgau MM, Jack L, Snyder SR,
Carande-Kulis VG, Isham G, Garfield S, Briss P, McCulloch D. 2002b. Increasing diabe-
tes self-management education in community settings. A systematic review. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine. 22(4 Supplement): 39–66.
NRC (National Research Council). 2003. Safety Is Seguridad: A Workshop Summary. Wash-
ington, DC: The National Academies Press.
O’Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, Tetroe J, Entwistle V, Llewellyn-Thomas H, Holmes-
Rovner M, Barry M, Jones J. 1999. Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or
screening decisions: Systematic review. British Medical Journal. 319(7212): 731–734.
Ogloff JR, Otto RK. 1991. Are research participants truly informed? Readability of informed
consent forms used in research. Ethics and Behavior. 1(4): 239–252.
Ordovas BJP, Lopez BE, Urbieta SE, Torregrossa S.R., Jimenez TNV. 1999. An analysis of
patient information sheets for obtaining informed consent in clinical trials. Medicina
Clinica. 112(3): 90–94.
Osborne H, Hochhauser M. 1999. Readability and comprehension of the introduction to the
Massachusetts Health Care Proxy. Hospital Topics. 77(4): 4–6.
Osuna E, Perez-Carceles MD, Perez-Moreno JA, Luna A. 1998. Informed consent. Evaluation
of the information provided to patients before anaesthesia and surgery. Medicine and
Law. 17(4): 511–518.
Osuna E, Lorenzo MD, Perez-Carceles MD, Luna A. 2001. Informed consent: Evaluation of
the information provided to elderly patients. Medicine and Law. 20(3): 379–384.
Paasche-Orlow M. 2003. Education of patients with asthma and low literacy. Abstract. Jour-
nal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1).
Paasche-Orlow MK, Taylor HA, Brancati FL. 2003. Readability standards for informed-
consent forms as compared with actual readability. New England Journal of Medicine.
348(8): 721–726.
Pepe MV, Chodzko-Zajko WJ. 1997. Impact of older adults’ reading ability on the compre-
hension and recall of cholesterol information. Journal of Health Education. 28(1): 21–
27.
Philipson SJ, Doyle MA, Gabram SG, Nightingale C, Philipson EH. 1995. Informed consent
for research: A study to evaluate readability and processability to effect change. Journal
of Investigative Medicine. 43(5): 459–467.
Philipson SJ, Doyle MA, Nightingale C, Bow L, Mather J, Philipson EH. 1999. Effectiveness
of a writing improvement intervention program on the readability of the research in-
formed consent document. Journal of Investigative Medicine. 47(9): 468–476.
Piette JD. 1999. Satisfaction with care among patients with diabetes in two public health care
systems. Medical Care. 37(6): 538–546.
Piette JD. 2000. Interactive voice response systems in the diagnosis and management of
chronic disease. The American Journal of Managed Care. 6(7): 817–827.
Pignone M. 2003. Literacy and Health: Findings from the Systematic Review for AHRQ.
Presentation at the Pfizer Health Literacy Initiative 6th National Conference. Key Ad-
vances in Health Literacy: Models for Action. September 18–19, 2003, Washington,
DC.
Pope JE, Tingey DP, Arnold JM, Hong P, Ouimet JM, Krizova A. 2003. Are subjects satisfied
with the informed consent process? A survey of research participants. [Comment]. Jour-
nal of Rheumatology. 30(4): 815–824.
Powell EC, Tanz RR, Uyeda A, Gaffney MB, Sheehan KM. 2000. Injury prevention education
using pictorial information. Pediatrics. 105(1): e16.
Price S, Raynor DK, Knapp P. 2003. Developing Effective Medicine Pictograms for the UK.
Abstract. Presentation at the Health Services Research and Pharmacy Practice Confer-
ence, Belfast. [Online]. Available: http://hsrpp.org.uk/abstracts/2003_41.shtml [accessed:
December 15, 2003].
Raich PC, Plomer KD, Coyne CA. 2001. Literacy, comprehension, and informed consent in
clinical research. Cancer Investigation. 19(4): 437–445.
Ratzan SC. 2001. Health literacy: Communication for the public good. Health Promotion
International. 16(2): 207–214.
Reicken HW, Ravich R. 1982. Informed consent to biomedical research in Veterans Adminis-
tration Hospitals. Journal of the American Medical Association. 248: 344–348.
Reid JC, Klachko DM, Kardash CAM, Robinson RD, Scholes R, Howard D. 1995. Why
people don’t learn from diabetes literature: Influence of text and reader characteristics.
Patient Education and Counseling. 25(1): 31–38.
Revere D, Dunbar PJ. 2001. Review of computer-generated outpatient health behavior inter-
ventions: Clinical encounters “in Absentia.” Journal of the American Medical Informatics
Association. 8: 62–79.
Rivera R, Reed JS, Menius D. 1992. Evaluating the readability of informed consent forms
used in contraceptive clinical trials. International Journal of Gynecology and Obstetrics.
38(3): 227–230.
Root J, Stableford S. 1999. Easy to read consumer communication: A missing link in medic-
aid managed care. Journal of Health Politics, Policy and Law. 24(1): 1–26.
Rosenbaum S, Sonosky CA, Shaw K, Zakheim MH. 2001. Negotiating the New Health
System: A Nationwide Study of Medicaid Managed Care Contracts. Shin P, Repasch L,
Managing Editors. Center for Health Services Research and Policy, George Washington
University.
Ross R. 1996. Returning to the Teachings: Exploring Aboriginal Justice. Toronto: Penguin
Books.
Rost K, Roter D. 1987. Predictors of recall of medication regimens and recommendations for
lifestyle change in elderly patients. Gerontologist. 27(4): 510–515.
Rostom A, O’Connor A, Tugwell P, Wells G. 2002. A randomized trial of a computerized
versus an audio-booklet decision aid for women considering post-menopausal hormone
replacement therapy. Patient Education and Counseling. 46(1): 67–74.
Roter DL. 2000. The outpatient medical encounter and elderly patients. Clinics in Geriatric
Medicine. 16(1): 95–107.
Roter DL, Hall JA. 1992. Doctors Talking with Patients/Patients Talking with Doctors:
Improving Communication in Medical Visits. Westport, CT: Auburn House.
Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam SM, Lipkin M Jr, Stiles W, Inui TS. 1997. Communication
patterns of primary care physicians. Journal of the American Medical Association.
277(4): 350–356.
Roter DL, Stashefsky-Margalit R, Rudd R. 2001. Current perspectives on patient education
in the US. Patient Education and Counseling. 44(1): 79–86.
Rother HA, London L, Maruping M, Miller S. 2002. Hazard Communication for Pesticide
Safety in Developing Countries: When is the Message Adequate? Bethesda, MD: Society
for Occupational and Environmental Health Conference: Pesticide Exposure and Health.
Rothman RL, Pignone M, Malone R, Bryant B, DeWalt DA, Crigler B. 2003. A longitudinal
analysis of the relationship between literacy and metabolic control in patients with
diabetes. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1).
Ruckdeschel JC, Albrecht TL, Blanchard C, Hemmick RM. 1996. Communication, accrual to
clinical trials, and the physician-patient relationship: implications for training programs.
Journal of Cancer Education. 11(2): 73–79.
Rudd RE. 2003. Objective 11-2: Improvement of Health Literacy. In: Communicating Health:
Priorities and Strategies for Progress. Washington, DC: Office of Disease Prevention and
Health Promotion, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Rudd RE, DeJong W. 2000. In Plain Language: The Need for Effective Communication in
Medicine and Public Health [Video]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Rudd RE, Colton T, Schacht R. 2000. An Overview of Medical and Public Health Literature
Addressing Literacy Issues: An Annotated Bibliography. Report #14. Cambridge, MA:
National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy
Ryan M. 2003. Demand Management. Presentation for Molina Healthcare.
Scala M. 2002. Linking Health Literacy and Medicare Education. Presentation given at a
workshop of the Institute of Medicine Committee on Health Literacy. December 10,
2002, Washington, DC.
Scandlen, G. (Galen Institute). 2003. Consumer Driven Health Care: New Tools for a New
Paradigm. [Online]. Available: http://www.galen.org/news/New_Tools.pdf [accessed:
August, 2003].
Schillinger D, Grumbach K, Piette J, Wang F, Osmond D, Daher C, Palacios J, Sullivan GaD,
Bindman AB. 2002. Association of health literacy with diabetes outcomes. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 288(4): 475–482.
Schillinger D, Machtinger E, Win K, Wang F, Chan L-L, Rodriguez ME. 2003a. Are pictures
worth a thousand words? Communication regarding medications in a public hospital
anticoagulation clinic. Abstract. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement
1): 187.
Schillinger D, Piette J, Grumbach K, Wang F, Wilson C, Daher C, Leong-Grotz K, Castro C,
Bindman AB. 2003b. Closing the loop: Physician communication with diabetic patients
who have low health literacy. Archives of Internal Medicine. 163(1): 83–90.
Schillinger D, Bindman A, Stewart A, Wang F, Piette J. 2004. Functional health literacy and
the quality of physician-patient communication among diabetes patients. Patient Educa-
tion and Counseling. 52(3): 315–323.
Schopp A, Valimaki M, Leino-Kilpi H, Dassen T, Gasull M, Lemonidou C, Scott PA, Arndt
M, Kaljonen A. 2003. Perceptions of informed consent in the care of elderly people in
five European countries. Nursing Ethics: An International Journal for Health Care Pro-
fessionals. 10(1): 48–57.
Scott TL, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Baker DW. 2002. Health literacy and preventive
health care use among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organization. Medical
Care. 40(5): 395–404.
Scrimshaw SC, Hurtado E. 1987. Rapid Assessment Procedures for Nutrition and Primary
Health Care: Anthropological Approaches to Improving Program Effectiveness. Los
Angeles, CA: University of California at Los Angeles, Latin American Center.
Shook EV. 1985. Ho’oponopono: Contemporary Uses of a Hawaiian Problem-solving Pro-
cess. Honolulu, HI: East-West Center, University of Hawaii Press.
Shortell SM, Kaluzny AD, Editors. 2000. Health Care Management: Organization, Design,
and Behavior. 4th edition. Albany, NY: Delmar Publishers.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Honig EG, Lee TM, Nowlan A. 1998a. Inadequate literacy is a
barrier to asthma knowledge and self-care. Chest. 114(4): 1008–1015.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, Nurss JR. 1998b. Relationship of functional health
literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. A study of patients with hyper-
tension and diabetes. Archives of Internal Medicine. 158(2): 166–172.
Wydra EW. 2001. The effectiveness of a self-care management interactive multimedia mod-
ule. Oncology Nursing Forum. 28(9): 1399–1407.
Young DR, Hooker DT, Freeberg FE. 1990. Informed consent documents: increasing compre-
hension by reducing reading level. IRB: A Review of Human Subjects Research. 12(3):
1–5.
A Vision for a
Health-Literate America
My best advice to others with poor reading skills is to not presume but to ask
questions. Poor readers need to take responsibility also. We need to learn
how to ask questions better. I would recommend more training for us. Re-
member, we [adult learners] want to be part of the solution.
Personal story graciously provided by Toni Cordell, Adult Learner and Lit-
eracy Advocate, as told to C.D. Meade, September 2003.
T
he evidence and judgment presented in this report indicate how
important improving heath literacy is to improving the health of
individuals and populations. This is why the Institute of Medicine
identified improving health literacy as one of two cross-cutting issues need-
ing attention in its recent Priority Areas for National Action in Quality
Improvement (IOM, 2003), and why the Surgeon General recently stated
that “health literacy can save lives, save money, and improve the health and
240
REFERENCES
Carmona RH. 2003. Health Literacy in America: The Role of Health Care Professionals.
Prepared Remarks given at the American Medical Association House of Delegates Meet-
ing. Saturday, June 14, 2003. [Online]. Available: http://www.surgeongeneral.gov/news/
speeches/ama061403.htm [accessed: August 2003].
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2003. Priority Areas for National Action: Transforming Health-
care Quality. Adams K, Corrigan JM, Editors. Washington, DC: The National Acad-
emies Press.
I
n order to respond to the study charge, several steps were undertaken to
define the scope of the problem of limited health literacy, identify ob-
stacles to creating a health-literate public, assess the approaches that
have been attempted, and identify goals for health literacy efforts. Sources
of data and information included the expertise of the committee members,
literature reviews and Internet searches of principal concepts, informal in-
terviews, commissioned works, hosting of several public workshops, and
other invited presentations.
STUDY COMMITTEE
An 11-member study committee was convened to assess available data
and respond to the study charge. The committee included members with
expertise in public health, primary medical care, health communication,
sociology, anthropology, adult literacy education, and K-12 education. The
committee convened for six 2-day meetings on October 21–22, 2002; De-
cember 10–11, 2002; February 13–14, 2003; April 29–30, 2003; June 16–
17, 2003; and September 11–12, 2003. Biographies of individual commit-
tee members appear in Appendix D.
LITERATURE REVIEW
The committee conducted extensive literature reviews and Internet
searches regarding health literacy and related topics. The literature reviewed
243
PUBLIC WORKSHOPS
The study committee hosted three 1-day public workshops in order to
obtain input from various stakeholders, consumers, and researchers. These
workshops were held in conjunction with three of the six committee meet-
ings mentioned above.
The first public workshop of the committee was held on December 10–
11, 2002 in Washington, DC. This workshop focused on health literacy-
related activities in federal government agencies, academia, and other rel-
evant organizations. Michael Pignone, M.D., M.P.H., Assistant Professor
of Medicine at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, School of
Medicine, presented information about an ongoing research project spon-
sored by the Agency for Health Care Research and Quality intended to
review the evidence base of health literacy research. Arlene S. Bierman,
M.D., M.S., from the Center of Outcomes and Effectiveness Research,
Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, spoke about health dispari-
ties and health literacy. Lawrence J. Fine, M.D., Dr. P.H., of the Office of
Behavioral and Social Science Research at the National Institutes of Health,
discussed how health literacy relates to other areas in health such as health
disparities, behavioral change, and socioeconomic determinants of health
such as education. Cynthia Baur, Ph.D., of the U.S. Department of Health
and Human Services, spoke about federal involvement in health literacy
efforts and how to leverage existing work that is relevant to the field.
Anthony Tirone, J.D., from the Joint Commission on Accreditation of
Healthcare Organizations (JCAHO), discussed some of the work of JCAHO
that has implications for how health systems respond to health literacy.
Marisa Scala, M.G.S., from the Center for Medicare Education of the
American Association of Homes and Services for the Aging, talked about
the specific needs of the Medicare population as they relate to health lit-
eracy. Karen Lechter, J.D., Ph.D., from the Center for Drug Evaluation and
Research of the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) provided an
APPENDIX A 245
overview of the activities of the FDA and their relevance to health literacy.
Lauren Schwartz, M.P.H., from the New York City Poison Control Center
shared background about and lessons from a program educating adult
learners about medications. Linda Morse, R.N., M.A., from the New Jersey
Office of Academic and Professional Standards discussed current activity in
the educational sector to improve health literacy, including efforts specific
to New Jersey, such as the New Jersey Core Curriculum Content Standards
for Comprehensive Health and Physical Education, which are currently
being implemented. Judy A. Shea, Ph.D., from the University of Pennsylva-
nia School of Medicine, shared findings about a recent research project
looking at the interplay between health literacy and patient satisfaction
with different modes of health-related communication. Finally, Tina Tucker,
M.A., M.Ed., RTC, from the National Program in Literacy, American Foun-
dation for the Blind, discussed the health needs and desires of individuals
who are visually impaired and low literate. The agenda for this workshop is
shown in Box A-1.
The third meeting of the committee was held on February 13–14, 2003,
at the Arnold and Mabel Beckman Center of the National Academies in
Irvine, California, and a public workshop was held in conjunction with this
meeting. On the afternoon of February 13, the committee heard from rep-
resentatives of consumer and advocacy groups, as well as several experts in
the areas of literacy, communication, and chronic diseases. Dean Schillinger,
M.D., from the University of California–San Francisco spoke about the
relationships between care for patients with chronic diseases, health lit-
eracy, and quality of care, with a specific emphasis on the role of patient–
physician communication in determining quality of care. Tetine Sentell,
M.A., of the University of California–Berkeley presented new analyses of
data from the National Adult Literary Survey. Susan M. Shinagawa, from
the Asian and Pacific Islander National Cancer Survivors Network, and
Heng L. Foong, of the Pacific Asian Language Services for Health, high-
lighted the most critical health literacy issues facing Asian Americans and
Pacific Islanders today, with an emphasis on issues of health and doctor–
patient communication in populations with limited English that represent
native speakers of diverse languages. Rita Hargrave, M.D., of the Univer-
sity of California–Davis and Veterans Medical Center of Northern Califor-
nia, discussed mental health care, ethnicity, and health literacy, and the
influence of physician and patient communication styles and preferences.
Alvin Billie, of the Family Learning Center and the Gathering Place in New
Mexico, discussed the unique health literacy issues facing Native Ameri-
cans, and addressed the effect of large cultural differences on doctor–
patient communication, as well as approaches to the problems created by
these cultural differences. Francis Prado and Francisco Para, from Latino
Health Access based in Santa Ana, California, described health literacy
BOX A-1
First Workshop Hosted by the Committee on Health Literacy
APPENDIX A 247
BOX A-2
Second Workshop Hosted by the
Committee on Health Literacy
3:00 p.m. Literacy and Chronic Disease Care: New Research Insights and
Reflections on Quality Standards
Dean Schillinger, M.D.
Associate Professor of Medicine, University of California–San
Francisco, San Francisco General Hospital Medical Center
3:45 p.m. Relationships Between Literacy, Race, Age, Education, and
Health in a National Sample: An Extension of NALS Analyses
Tetine Sentell, M.A.
Ph.D. Candidate, University of California, Berkeley
4:00 p.m. Health Literacy Concerns for Asian Americans & Pacific
Islanders: Challenges and Opportunities
Susan M. Shinagawa
Patient Advocate and Founder, Asian and Pacific Islander National
Cancer Survivors Network
Heng L. Foong
Patient Advocate and Program Director, Pacific Asian Language
Services for Health
4:15 p.m. The Interplay of Ethnicity and Health Literacy in Mental Health-
care
Rita Hargrave, M.D.
Assistant Professor of Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry,
University of California, Davis, Veterans Medical Center of
Northern California System of Clinics
4:30 p.m. Health and Literacy—A Native American Point of View
Alvin Billie
Program Manager, The Gathering Place, Thoreau, NM
4:45 p.m. Health Care Barriers in the Latino Community of Orange County
Francis Prado, Francisco Para
Health Educators and Promotoras, Latino Health Access
5:00 p.m. Discussion
5:30 p.m. Adjourn
APPENDIX A 249
BOX A-3
Third Workshop Hosted by the Committee on Health Literacy
APPENDIX A 251
33 respondents 62 responded
7 e-mails
undeliverable
95 Responses
APPENDIX A 253
NOTE: The same organization can be found in different categories because separate individu-
als may or may not have completed the survey.
REFERENCE
Matthews TL, Sewell JC. 2002. State Official’s Guide to Health Literacy. Lexington, KY: The
Council of State Governments.
Commissioned Papers
CONTENTS
The Relationship Between Health Literacy and Medical Costs
David H. Howard 256
255
INTRODUCTION
Past research has shown that individuals with low levels of health
literacy are more likely to be hospitalized and have worse disease outcomes
(Baker et al., 1998, 2002; Schillinger et al., 2002). The major obstacle to
extending these results by examining the relationship between health lit-
eracy and spending is the lack of data containing measures of both. Some
insight about the impact of health literacy on costs can be gleaned from
studies that examine the impact of years of schooling on medical costs, but
health literacy is a fundamentally different concept from educational attain-
ment (Davis et al., 1994; Stedman and Kaestle, 1991). At least one study
has examined the association between general literacy and costs (Weiss et
al., 1994). It found no relationship, though the sample size was small (N =
402) and not representative of the overall U.S. population (over half the
study subjects qualified for Medicaid because of disability). Furthermore,
inpatient and outpatient costs were not analyzed separately and the analysis
did not control for confounding patient characteristics. Another paper by
the same author (Weiss) shows large differences in costs by grade reading
level in a Medicaid population (Weiss, 1999), but descriptions of the meth-
ods and data are not available. This study examines the relationship be-
tween health literacy and costs using a unique dataset combining cost infor-
mation from an administrative claims file and a health literacy measure
from a beneficiary survey. Multivariate techniques are used to adjust for
underlying differences in respondents’ characteristics.
Data Description
Health literacy data were collected as part of a survey of persons enroll-
ing in a Prudential Medicare health maintenance organization between
December of 1996 and August of 1997 in one of four locations: Cleveland,
Ohio; Houston, Texas; South Florida (including Fort Lauderdale, Miami,
and nearby areas); and Tampa, Florida. New Prudential Medicare members
were contacted three months after enrollment, and those meeting the eligi-
bility criterion were asked to complete an in-person survey. In order to be
included in the study, members had to be comfortable speaking either
English or Spanish, living in the community, and possess adequate visual
and cognitive function. The survey included the Short Test of Functional
APPENDIX B 257
Schooling
12 years 34% no data 31% 0.03
>12 years 31% no data 28% 0.03
Need help
with ADLs 5% no data 9% <0.01
Need help with
IADLs 31% no data 16% <0.01
APPENDIX B 259
NOTE: Age and SF-12 score rows report mean ± standard deviation; n.s. = not significant.
sured by SF-12 scores (Ware et al., 1996) and chronic condition indicators,
were higher in those with inadequate health literacy, indicating worse health
status overall. Somewhat surprisingly, persons with inadequate health lit-
eracy are less likely to smoke or have smoked previously and less likely to
consume alcohol.
Statistical Analysis
Costs by site of service were compared between the adequate and inad-
equate health literacy groups using a two-part regression model of medical
spending. The two-part model is the standard statistical framework in em-
pirical health economics for measuring the impact an individual character-
istic on medical costs (Diehr et al., 1999). It is designed to account for the
unique distribution of medical spending found in most samples; a sizeable
minority of individuals do not use medical care, many use small amounts,
and a few individuals incur substantial medical bills that account for a large
percentage of aggregate spending. Because of the highly skewed distribu-
tion and presence of a large number of “0” values, standard statistical
methods that assume the dependent variable is normally distributed yield
inaccurate predictions of costs (Duan et al., 1983). The two-part model
attempts to more accurately mimic the empirical distribution of medical
spending by splitting the distribution into two parts and allowing the im-
pact of independent variables, such as health literacy, on the probability of
using medical care to be independent of their impact on the costs of medical
care for those who use it.
The first stage of the model measures the probability of using medical
care as a function of individual characteristics. Typically, logitistic or probit
regression is used, where the dependent variable equals one if costs are
strictly positive and zero otherwise. Probit (or probability unit) regression
was used in this case. Health literacy was included as an independent
variable, along with controls for age, sex, race, income, schooling, smok-
ing, and alcohol consumption. The second stage of the model estimates the
relationship between independent variables and costs among those who use
medical care. Parameters are estimated via least squares regression, where
the dependent variable is the logarithm of costs and the independent vari-
ables are the same as in the first stage but the sample includes only individu-
als who received care (i.e., those with strictly positive values for the relevant
cost category).
Coefficient estimates for the two-part model are difficult to interpret in
isolation, since the dependent variable of the second stage is in log, rather
than constant, dollars, and it is customary to state results in terms of
predicted spending levels. These are constructed by computing predicted
probabilities from the first stage and then multiplying these predicted prob-
abilities by the exponentiated second-stage predicted values and a “smear-
ing factor” (Duan, 1983), which is needed to transform logged dollars back
to constant dollars, and averaging the predicted spending levels over the
entire sample. Transforming log to constant dollars via this method may
produce misleading results if the variance of spending in the upper part of
the distribution differs from the variance in the lower part (Manning, 1998).
APPENDIX B 261
To address this issue, costs were also analyzed using the modified two-part
model proposed by Mullahy (1998). In this model, the first stage is the
same as in the standard two-part model, but the second stage is a nonlinear
equation where cost equals the exponentiated sum of dependent variables
and coefficients. This modified two-part model produced estimates within
5 percent of the predicted values from the standard two-part model. There-
fore, only the results from the standard two-part model are presented
below.
Two values are used to summarize the effect of a binary independent
variable on spending. The first is the average predicted spending level with
the variable indicating inadequate health literacy set equal to zero for every
respondent; the second is the average predicted spending level with the
health literacy variable set equal to one. Computed predicted values in this
manner nets out the impact of observable individual characteristics, such as
age, on spending.
Confidence intervals for predicted values were computed via simula-
tion; the first- and second-stage coefficients were drawn from their respec-
tive multivariate normal distributions and predicted values were computed
following the steps outlined above. Repeating this routine 1,000 times
produced distributions of predicted values, and the lower and upper bounds
of the confidence intervals were set equal to the 2.5th percentiles and the
97.5th percentiles of the distributions, respectively.
Two models were estimated. The first (or basic) model includes con-
trols for sex, age, income, schooling, smoking, and alcohol consumption.
The second includes additional controls for physical and mental status
(from the SF-12) and chronic conditions (high blood pressure, arthritis, and
depression). This model does not include the 66 observations for which no
physical or mental health SF-12 scores were reported, for a sample of 3,192
observations (= 3,260 – 66).
Results
Results from the two-part model are displayed in Table B-3. Inpatient
costs are the largest component of total medical spending. Predicted inpa-
tient spending for persons with inadequate health literacy is $993 higher
than that of persons with adequate health literacy (difference in raw means:
$1,859). Controlling for health status, predicted inpatient spending for
persons with inadequate health literacy is about $450 higher than that of
persons with adequate health literacy. The confidence intervals for inpa-
tient spending from the basic model overlap slightly, while the confidence
intervals for inpatient spending from the model that includes controls for
health status display a greater degree of overlap. Examining separately the
results from each stage of the two-part model helps illuminate the reasons
Basic Model
Inpatient $5,093 [$4,593 – $5,656] $6,086 [$5,424 – $6,806] $993
Outpatient $1,910 [$1,816 – $2,017] $1,795 [$1,681 – $1,914] ($115)
Emergency room $110 [$97 – $124] $174 [$154 – $196] $64
Pharmacy $700 [664 – $739] $686 [$629 – $741] ($14)
NOTE: 95% confidence intervals are in brackets. Difference column displays the mean cost in
the Inadequate column subtracted from the mean cost in the Adequate column. Negative
values are in parentheses.
for spending differences. According to the first part of the two-part model
for inpatient spending (results not shown; complete regression results are
available from the author upon request), persons with inadequate health
literacy are more likely to use inpatient services (p < 0.05), but, among
those who used inpatient care, spending did not differ by health literacy
status.
In contrast to the results for inpatient spending, the predicted outpa-
tient spending level from the basic model for persons with adequate health
literacy is higher than the predicted value for persons with inadequate
health literacy. Predicted spending on emergency room care is lower for
persons with adequate health literacy, while the predicted values for phar-
macy spending from the basic model are comparable.
These results are shown in terms of total spending by the study sample
in Table B-4. The first column shows predicted total spending under the
assumption that the proportion of individuals with adequate health literacy
is 64 percent, the actual proportion in the study sample. The second column
shows predicted total spending under the assumption that the proportion of
individuals with adequate health literacy is 100 percent, representing the
maximum attainable level of health literacy in the population.
APPENDIX B 263
NOTE: Difference column displays the mean cost in the inadequate column subtracted from
the mean cost in the adequate column. Negative values are in parentheses.
Discussion
When assessing the causality of the results presented in Tables B-3 and
B-4, it is important know whether health literacy, like ethnicity, is a con-
stant, fixed characteristic of individuals or, like income, is associated with
changes in health. Health literacy declines sharply with age in the study
cohort (Baker et al., 2000), suggesting the latter. If so, then the relationship
between health and health literacy is bidirectional; health literacy affects
health and vice versa. To take an extreme example, an individual who
experiences a severe stroke may lose the ability to read. It would be incor-
rect in such a case to attribute the costs associated with post-stroke care to
illiteracy, since the stroke caused illiteracy and not the other way around.
Controlling for health status, as is done in the extended model, removes the
effect of health on health literacy but also removes the effect of health
literacy on disease incidence, leading to estimates of the impact of health
literacy on spending that are systematically lower than the true effect.
Declines in health literacy by age are unrelated to the onset of chronic
conditions (Baker et al., 2000), suggesting that the bias due to reverse
causality is not large. Nevertheless, future studies could address this issue
by taking two or more measurements of health literacy from the same
respondent at different points in time.
Another caveat to this study is that though the analysis included fairly
extensive controls for individual characteristics, including income, educa-
tion, smoking, and alcohol consumption, there still may be unobserved
individual characteristics correlated with both health literacy and spending
that confound the results. For example, if individuals with low health lit-
eracy are also distrustful of the medical care system and are reluctant to
seek medical attention, then the results will understate the impact of health
literacy on costs.
REFERENCES
Andrus MR, Roth MT. 2002. Health literacy: A review. Pharmacotherapy. 22(3): 282–302.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS. 1998. Health literacy and the risk of
hospital admission. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 13(12): 791–798.
Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. 1999. Development of a
brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Education and Counseling. 38:
33–42.
Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Sudano J, Patterson M. 2000. The association between age and
health literacy among elderly persons. Journals of Gerontology Series B—Psychological
Sciences & Social Sciences. 55B(6): S368–S374.
Baker DW, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Scott T, Parker RM, Green D, Ren J, Peel J.
2002. Functional health literacy and the risk of hospital admission among Medicare
managed care enrollees. American Journal of Public Health. 92(8): 1278–1283.
Center for Evaluative Clinical Sciences. 1998. The Dartmouth Atlas of Health Care. Chicago,
IL: American Hospital Publishing.
APPENDIX B 265
Cohen JW, Monheit AC, Beauregard KM, Cohen SB, Lefkowitz DC, Potter DE, Sommers JP,
Taylor AK, Arnett RH 3rd. 1996–1997. The Medical Expenditure Panel Survey: A
national health information resource. Inquiry. 33(4): 373–389.
Coyte PC, Young W, Croxford R. 2000. Costs and outcomes associated with alternative
discharge strategies following joint replacement surgery: Analysis of an observational
study using a propensity score. Journal of Health Economics. 19(6): 907–929.
Davis TC, Mayeaux EJ, Fredrickson D, Bocchini JA Jr, Jackson RH, Murphy PW. 1994.
Reading ability of parents compared with reading level of pediatric patient education
materials. Pediatrics. 93(3): 460–468.
Diehr P, Yanez D, Ash A, Hornbrook M, Lin DY. 1999. Methods for analyzing health care
utilization and costs. Annual Review of Public Health. 20: 125–144.
Duan N. 1983. Smearing estimate: A nonparametric retransformation method. Journal of the
American Statistical Association. 78: 605–610.
Duan N, Manning WG Jr, Morris CN, Newhouse JP. 1983. A comparison of alternative
models for the demand for medical care. Journal of Business and Economic Statistics. 1:
115–126.
Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Scott TL, Green DC, Fehrenbach SN,
Ren J, Koplan JP. 1999. Health literacy among Medicare enrollees in a managed care
organization. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6): 545–551.
Hellinger FJ, Wong HS. 2000. Selection bias in HMOs: A review of the evidence. Medical
Care Research and Review. 57(4): 405–439.
Lindau ST, Tomori C, Lyons T, Langseth L, Bennett CL, Garcia P. 2002. The association of
health literacy with cervical cancer prevention knowledge and health behaviors in a
multiethnic cohort of women. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology. 186(5):
938–943.
Manning WG. 1998. Much ado about two: Reconsidering retransformation and the two-part
model in health economics. Journal of Health Economics. 48: 375–391.
Mullahy J. 1998. Much ado about two: Reconsidering retransformation and the two-part
model in health econometrics. Journal of Health Economics. 17(3): 247–281.
Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. 1995. The Test of Functional Health Lit-
eracy in Adults: A new instrument for measuring patients’ literacy skills. Journal of
General Internal Medicine. 10(10): 537–541.
Rubin DB. 1997. Estimating causal effects from large data sets using propensity scores. An-
nals of Internal Medicine. 127(8 Pt 2): 757–763.
Schillinger D, Grumbach K, Piette J, Wang F, Osmond D, Daher C, Palacios J, Sullivan GAD,
Bindman AB. 2002. Association of health literacy with diabetes outcomes. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 288(4): 475–482.
Scott TL, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV, Baker DW. 2002. Health literacy and preventive
health care use among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organization. Medical
Care. 40(5): 395–404.
Selden TM, Levit KR, Cohen JW, Zuvekas SH, Moeller JF, McKusick D, Arnett RH 3rd.
2001. Reconciling medical expenditure estimates from the MEPS and the NHA, 1996.
Health Care Financing Review. 23(1): 161–178.
Stedman L, Kaestle C. 1991. Literacy and Reading Performance in the United States from
1880 to Present. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Pp. 75–128.
Ware J Jr, Kosinski M, Keller SD. 1996. A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: Construction
of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care. 34(3): 220–233.
Weiss BD. 1999. How common is low literacy? In: 20 Common Problems in Primary Care.
Weiss BD, Editor. New York: McGraw-Hill. Pp. 468–481.
Weiss BD, Blanchard JS, McGee DL, Hart G, Warren B, Burgoon M, Smith KJ. 1994. Illit-
eracy among Medicaid recipients and its relationship to health care costs. Journal of
Health Care for the Poor & Underserved. 5(2): 99–111.
Wiener JM, Hanley RJ, Clark R, Van Nostrand JF. 1990. Measuring the activities of daily
living: Comparisons across national surveys. Journal of Gerontology. 45(6): S229–S237.
APPENDIX B 267
Introduction
Chronic disease management is one of the major challenges facing
health-care systems and patients in industrialized nations. Nearly three-
quarters of all health-care resources are devoted to the treatment of chronic
diseases, and nearly one-half of the U.S. population has one or more chronic
condition (Institute for Health and Aging, 1996). The collaboration be-
tween the system of care, providers, patients, and the community to provide
the best health outcomes adds a layer of complexity to the delivery of health
care to individuals with chronic disease. Effective disease management is
based on systematic, interactive communication between patients and the
providers and health system with whom they interact (Norris et al.,
2002a, b; Von Korff et al., 1997), all occurring in the context of a commu-
nity whose resources meet patients’ needs (Wagner, 1995).
This paper uses the definition of disease management provided by the
Task Force on Community Preventive Services, a nonfederal Task Force
convened in 1996 by the Department of Health and Human Services (HHS)
to provide leadership in the evaluation of community, population, and
health-care system strategies to address a variety of public health and health
promotion topics, and appointed by the Director of the Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention (Norris et al., 2002a, b). Disease management is an
organized, proactive, multicomponent approach to health-care delivery that
involves all members of a population with a specific disease, is focused on
the spectrum of the disease and its complications (including the prevention
of co-morbid conditions), and is integrated across the relevant aspects of
the delivery system.
The last few decades have seen tremendous advances in the care of
chronic conditions, including an array of new therapeutic options, risk-
factor modification for secondary prevention of co-morbid conditions, the
availability of home monitoring tools, and the growth of disease manage-
The research reported herein was supported, in part, through grants from the National
Center for Research Resources (K-23 RR16539), the Soros Open Society Institute, and The
Commonwealth Fund.
APPENDIX B 269
Informed, Prepared,
Productive Proactive
Activated Interactions
Patient Practice Team
who take an active part in their care and providers who are backed up by
resources and expertise. The Chronic Care Model integrates the published
evidence in chronic disease management, including the importance of ex-
ecutive leadership and incentives to promote quality, systems to track and
monitor patients’ progress and support timely provider decision-making
(Piette, 2000), patient self-management training (Lorig et al., 1999; Von
Korff et al., 1997), and community-oriented care. Patients and providers
prepared in these ways are likely more able to engage in productive interac-
tions that promote system efficiency and patient well-being (Norris et al.,
2002a).
A growing body of research demonstrates that self-management prac-
tices and clinical outcomes in chronic disease care vary by patients’ level of
health literacy (Kalichman and Rompa, 2000; Schillinger et al., 2002; Wil-
liams et al., 1998b). The Chronic Care Model and similar comprehensive,
population-based disease management approaches may offer insights into
the ways in which limited health literacy affects chronic disease care and
identify potential points of intervention. Preliminary evidence from a small
randomized trial suggests that disease management strategies can reduce
health literacy-related disparities in diabetes care (Rothman et al., 2003)
and that tailoring communication to those with limited health literacy might
affect outcomes in chronic anticoagulation and diabetes care (Schillinger et
al., 2002, 2003b). However, we were unable to locate published results of
any comprehensive disease management systems specifically designed to
improve chronic disease care for individuals with limited health literacy.
Developing such a system would likely benefit not only those with limited
health literacy, but all chronic disease patients, as many of the barriers
faced by those with limited health literacy are also experienced, albeit it a
somewhat lesser extent, by those with adequate health literacy.
APPENDIX B 271
and affect the availability of community resources for chronic disease pa-
tients and their families (Figure B-2). By reviewing this framework, we can
begin to generate a set of priorities for targeted study and intervention that
reflects a blending of what we have learned from the Chronic Care Model
and what is known about empowering low-literacy adults from the field of
adult education (Roter et al., 1998, 2001).
Social Policy
Health Health
Policy/Incentives Enhanced Policy/Incentives
Community
Factors
Improved • Access Expanded
Provider-Patient • health coverage Home-Based
Communication • provider availability Monitoring and
• Economic Resources Clinical Support
• Interactive, • Adult Education
Participatory • Environmental: • Patient and Family
Communication • nutrition Empowerment
• Open Discussion of • air quality • Good Self-Management
Explanatory Models • physical activity Practices
• Collaborative Goal • safety • Learning and Skill-Based
Setting • occupational risks Registries
• Patient Recall and • Trust in Health System • Decision Support and
Comprehension of • Power and Advocacy Problem-Solving Skills
Advice and Instructions • Public Health Messages/ • Ability to Navigate System
• Understanding of Health Promotion and Self-Advocate
Disease Processes and Activities • Maintaining Continuity
Treatment Options in Communication
• Trust • Self-Efficacy
FIGURE B-2 Improving chronic disease care: a framework based on health litera-
cy and related research.
APPENDIX B 273
tient health status. Without timely and reliable information about patients’
health status, symptoms, and self-care, the necessary health education, treat-
ments, or behavioral adjustments may come late or not at all. This can
compromise patients’ health and increase the likelihood of poor outcomes.
To best manage chronic disease, patients must remember any self-care
instructions they have received from their provider, be able to correctly
interpret symptoms or results of self-monitoring, and appropriately solve
problems regarding adjustments to the treatment regimen, as well as when
and how to contact the provider should the need arise. A number of studies
demonstrate that patients remember and understand as little as half of what
they are told by their physicians (Bertakis, 1977; Cole and Bird, 2000;
Crane, 1997; Rost and Roter, 1987; Roter, 2000) and the more informa-
tion provided, the less the patient is able to recall (Chow, 2003). Patients
with limited health literacy are likely to understand and remember at even
lower rates (Schillinger et al., 2004). In addition, they may be less equipped
to overcome gaps when they are at home due to knowledge deficits (Will-
iams et al., 1998a, b) and difficulties reading or interpreting instructions
(Crane, 1997; Williams et al., 1998b). Cross-sectional studies involving
patients with diabetes suggest that traditional self-management education
may not eliminate health literacy-related disparities in chronic disease out-
comes (Schillinger et al., 2002; Williams et al., 1998b).
Only a minority of clinical practices provide any form of care manage-
ment that involves outreach and support in the patient’s home (Casalino et
al., 2003). In addition, home-based disease-specific education, monitoring,
and clinical support increasingly rely on patients and providers interacting
via web-based or “e-health” interfaces (Robert Wood Johnson Foundation
and the National Cancer Institute, 2002). While these interfaces have been
suggested as a potential method of increasing interaction between patients
and providers (Robert Wood Johnson Foundation and the National Cancer
Institute, 2002), they may present overwhelming barriers to patients with
limited health literacy. Individuals with limited health literacy may have
trouble accessing the web (Fox and Fallows, 2003), difficulties reading
from web sites (Berland et al., 2001), and problems with navigation once
they get online (Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, 2002; Zarcadoolas et
al., 2002). Accreditation of health-related web sites (such as the Health on
the Net Foundation Code of Conduct) does little to improve readability of
diabetes web sites (Kusec et al., 2003).
Simply transforming text versions of disease-specific education to more
visually oriented media (i.e., CD-ROM), while associated with improve-
ments in satisfaction, does not appear to increase knowledge among pa-
tients with limited health literacy (Kim et al., 2001). Focus groups of pa-
tients with limited health literacy have identified health system navigation
(finding resources within the health system, such as knowing whom, for
APPENDIX B 275
teams, which will, in turn, lead to better outcomes (Figure B-1). In order for
this assumption to hold for populations with limited health literacy, we
need to think more critically about the ways in which we communicate
across the levels described above to ensure that interactions indeed are
productive. If we apply the Chronic Care Model without also attending to
the unique challenges to chronic disease management posed by limited
health literacy, we may improve care for many but run the risk of perpetu-
ating disparities in outcomes for those with limited health literacy.
In order to engage in more productive interactions with patients who
have limited health literacy, solutions must primarily affect the nature,
quality, and extent of communication. Previous research indicates patients
want practical, concise information focused on the identification of the
problem, what specifically the patient needs to do, why it is in their best
interest, and what outcomes they can expect (Davis et al., 2001, 2002a, b).
Communication strategies commonly employed by health professionals are
often only marginally effective for those with limited health literacy. At
present, clinicians do not have the means to uncover how patients learn
best, nor the tools to more effectively engage patients who do not appear to
be maximally benefiting from clinical interactions. In order to support
patients’ acquisition of self-management skills and increase confidence to
carry out self-management tasks, efforts should be made to develop tools to
assess how patients learn in the clinical setting, and to expand the repertoire
of options to match patients’ learning style or preferences. Some principles
derived from the field of adult education may be relevant to chronic disease
communication (Brookfield, 1986; Roter et al., 2001; Wallerstein, 1992).
Learners (patients) should be involved in developing health education mes-
sages, materials, and programs (Davis et al., 1998a, b; Rudd and Comings,
1994). Learning should be participatory; patients should be actively in-
volved in setting the agenda or curriculum and, at times, even leading it so
as to ensure relevance (relating and reflecting on experience), encourage
ongoing involvement, promote the development of behavior change through
critical thinking (thoughtful action), and support other learners to succeed.
This involvement will help ensure the education is relevant, understand-
able, culturally sensitive, and empowering. Interactions should involve ex-
ploration and problem-based learning (Cooper et al., 2003). Patient educa-
tion activities should be designed so as to engage patients in ways relevant
to their lives and their conditions and that enhance problem-solving skills
(Center for Literacy Studies, 2003). Such activities educational focus may
lead to patient-generated goal-setting, an important intermediate objective
in successful chronic disease care (Anderson, 1995; Anderson et al., 1995).
Group medical visits, an innovation in which groups of patients who share
a common condition regularly meet with a health provider, have the poten-
tial to operate via these principles. Results of a small trial with diabetes
APPENDIX B 277
ness, we need to prime the workforce to more effectively care for patients
who have chronic diseases and limited health literacy. Training of all health
professionals that focuses on communication strategies to enhance clinician
self-awareness (Frankel and Stein, 2001), mutual learning, partnership-
building, collaborative goal-setting, and behavior change for chronic dis-
ease patients is essential (Wagner, 2003; Youmans and Schillinger, 2003).
Such training should be expanded to include all members of the multidis-
ciplinary health team, including lay health educators, both as learners and
teachers. These efforts must be informed by new health communication
research that involves patients with limited health literacy, a segment often
under-represented in clinical research.
Community efforts should focus on developing relationships that foster
trust, providing resources to measure and meet community needs, and
ultimately preparing members of a community to effectively advocate for
the needs of their community (Figueroa et al., 2002). Public health mes-
sages should take into account the health literacy skills of the population to
whom the message is being targeted, involve the population from the begin-
ning, and make use of appropriate channels to convey these messages (Bird
et al., 1998).
Measuring Progress
In order to promote progress in chronic care delivery for patients with
limited health literacy, quality-of-care measures must be designed to cap-
ture health literacy-related performance. If incentives are aligned to im-
prove quality, such measures of health-care quality can, in turn, lead to the
creation of standards of care and improve practice. There are several pos-
sible approaches to measuring the extent to which health systems are
meeting the needs of patients with limited health literacy. An indirect
approach, advocated by those involved in initiatives to reduce racial and
ethnic disparities in quality of care (IOM, 2003; Sehgal, 2003) is to use
existing measures of quality, such as Medicaid Health Plan Employer Data
and Information Set indicators (that currently do not include any health
literacy-specific indicators), and stratify a system’s performance by race or
ethnicity, or, in this case, health literacy level. Such an approach would
enable a comparison of performance in process or outcome measures
among those with inadequate health literacy in comparison to those with
adequate health literacy. A health system can be considered improving if
overall performance is improving and if the extent of health literacy-
related variation in performance is narrowing over time. The main chal-
lenge to this strategy is the complexity involved in measuring health lit-
eracy. Current instruments take between 3 and 7 minutes and require in-
person administration (Baker et al., 1999). Education level is not a useful
CONCLUSIONS
Despite wide variation in literacy levels, our society places high literacy
demands on its members. It is apparent that attempts to reduce health
literacy-related disparities must revolve around either directly addressing
the problem of basic literacy and/or creating a health-care system in which
the gap between the literacy demands of the system and the literacy skills of
the patients it serves is significantly narrowed. Modern chronic disease care
requires that patients play an active role in their care, and that clinicians
and the health systems in which they work take on the challenge of partner-
ing with patients to promote successful outcomes. Meeting these goals is
APPENDIX B 279
often most challenging for those patients who have the greatest need. When
considered as a barrier to successful health communication as well as a
marker for problems with navigation and self-advocacy, it becomes clear
that the concerns related to limited health literacy are inescapably linked to
the challenges of chronic disease management. In order to ensure that the
Chronic Care Model and increasingly sophisticated chronic disease man-
agement programs can benefit patients with limited health literacy, atten-
tion must be paid to tailor design and implementation with the involvement
of patients with limited health literacy, and to expand the reach of such
programs. By promoting meaningful, collaborative communication between
patients and the providers and systems that serve them, such a reorganiza-
tion is likely to benefit all patients with chronic diseases.
REFERENCES
American Diabetes Association. 1998. Economic consequences of diabetes mellitus in the U.S.
in 1997. Diabetes Care. 21(2): 296–309.
American Medical Association. 1999. Health literacy: Report of the Council on Scientific
Affairs. Ad Hoc Committee on Health Literacy for the Council on Scientific Affairs,
American Medical Association. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6):
552–557.
Anderson RM. 1995. Patient empowerment and the traditional medical model. A case of
irreconcilable differences. Diabetes Care. 18(3): 412–415.
Anderson RM, Funnell MM, Butler PM, Arnold MS, Fitzgerald JT, Feste CC. 1995. Patient
empowerment. Results of a randomized controlled trial. Diabetes Care. 18(7): 943–949.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Pitkin K, Parikh NS, Coates W, Imara M. 1996. The
health care experience of patients with low literacy. Archives of Family Medicine. 5(6):
329–334.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS, Nurss J. 1997. The relationship of patient
reading ability to self-reported health and use of health services. American Journal of
Public Health. 87(6): 1027–1030.
Baker DW, Parker RM, Williams MV, Clark WS. 1998. Health literacy and the risk of
hospital admission. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 13(12): 791–798.
Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. 1999. Development of a
brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Education and Counseling. 38:
33–42.
Beers BB, McDonald VJ, Quistberg DA, Ravenell KL, Asch DA, Shea JA. 2003. Disparities in
health literacy between African American and non-African American primary care pa-
tients. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(S1): 169.
Berkman LF, Kawachi, IO. 2000. Social Epidemiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Berland GK, Elliott MN, Morales LS, Algazy JI, Kravitz RL, Broder MS, Kanouse DE, Munoz
JA, Puyol JA, Lara M, Watkins KE, Yang H, McGlynn EA. 2001. Health information
on the Internet: Accessibility, quality, and readability in English and Spanish. Journal of
the American Medical Association. 285(20): 2612–2621.
Bertakis KD. 1977. The communication of information from physician to patient: A method
for increasing patient retention and satisfaction. Journal of Family Practice 5(2): 217–
222.
APPENDIX B 281
Davis TC, Williams MV, Marin E, Parker RM, Glass J. 2002b. Health literacy and cancer
communication. Ca: A Cancer Journal for Clinicians. 52(3): 134–149.
Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group. 1993. The effect of intensive
treatment of diabetes on the development and progression of long-term complications in
insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. New England Journal of Medicine. 329(14): 977–
986.
Diabetes Control and Complications Trial Research Group. 1996. Lifetime benefits and costs
of intensive therapy as practiced in the diabetes control and complications trial. Journal
of the American Medical Association. 276(17): 1409–1415.
Doak CC, Doak LG, Friedell GH, Meade CD. 1998. Improving comprehension for cancer
patients with low literacy skills: Strategies for clinicians. [Review] [50 refs]. Ca: A Can-
cer Journal for Clinicians. 48(3): 151–162.
Figueroa ME, Kincaid DL, Rani M, Lewis G. 2002. Communication for Social Change Work-
ing Paper Series. Communication for Social Change: An Integrated Model for Measur-
ing the Process and Its Outcomes. New York: The Rockefeller Foundation and Johns
Hopkins University Center for Communication Programs.
Fiscella K, Franks P, Gold MR, Clancy CM. 2000. Inequality in quality: Addressing socioeco-
nomic, racial, and ethnic disparities in health care. Journal of the American Medical
Association. 283(19): 2579–2584.
Fox S, Fallows D. 2003 (July 16). Internet Health Resources: health searches and email have
become more commonplace, but there is room for improvement in searches and overall
Internet access. Washington, DC: Pew Internet & American Life Project.
Frankel RM, Stein T. 2001. Getting the most out of the clinical encounter: The four habits
model. Journal of Medical Practice Management. 16(4): 184–191.
Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Scott TL, Green DC, Fehrenbach SN,
Ren J, Koplan JP. 1999a. Health literacy among Medicare enrollees in a managed care
organization. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6): 545–551.
Gazmararian JA, Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Scott T, Greemn DCFSN, Ren J,
Koplan JP. 1999b. Health literacy among Medicare enrollees in a managed care organi-
zation. Journal of the American Medical Association. 281(6): 545–551.
Guerra CE, Shea JA. 2003. Functional health literacy, comorbidity and health status. Journal
of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1): 174.
HHS (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services). 2000. Healthy People 2010: Under-
standing and Improving Health. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Hu-
man Services.
Houts PS, Bachrach R, Witmer JT, Tringali CA, Bucher JA, Localio RA. 1998. Using picto-
graphs to enhance recall of spoken medical instructions. Patient Education and Counsel-
ing. 35(2): 83–88.
Institute for Health and Aging. 1996. Chronic Care in America: A 21st Century Challenge.
Princeton, NJ: The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation.
IOM (Institute of Medicine). 2001. Crossing the Quality Chasm: A New Health System for
the 21st Century. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
IOM. 2003. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care.
Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Jacobson TA, Thomas DM, Morton FJ, Offutt G, Shevlin J, Ray S. 1999. Use of a low-
literacy patient education tool to enhance pneumococcal vaccination rates: A random-
ized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 282(7): 646–650.
Kalichman SC, Rompa D. 2000. Functional health literacy is associated with health status
and health-related knowledge in people living with HIV-AIDS. Journal of Acquired
Immune Deficiency Syndromes & Human Retrovirology. 25(4): 337–344.
Kalichman SCP, Ramachandran BB, Catz SP. 1999. Adherence to combination antiretroviral
therapies in HIV patients of low health literacy. Journal of General Internal Medicine.
14(5): 267–273.
Kim SP, Knight SJ, Tomori C, Colella KM, Schoor RA, Shih L, Kuzel TM, Nadler RB,
Bennett CL. 2001. Health literacy and shared decision making for prostate cancer pa-
tients with low socioeconomic status. Cancer Investigation. 19(7): 684–691.
Kirsch IS (Educational Testing Service (ETS)). 1993. Adult Literacy in America: A First Look
at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, DC: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
Kusec S, Brborovic O, Schillinger D. 2003. Diabetes websites accredited by the Health on the
Net Foundation Code of Conduct: Readable or not? Studies in health technology and
informatics. Vol. 95. In: The New Navigators: From Professionals to Patients. Baud R,
Fieschi M, Le Beux P, Ruch P, Editors. Amsterdam, NE: IOS Press.
Lorig KR, Sobel DS, Stewart AL, Brown BW Jr, Bandura A, Ritter P, Gonzalez VM, Laurent
DD, Holman HR. 1999. Evidence suggesting that a chronic disease self-management
program can improve health status while reducing hospitalization: A randomized trial.
Medical Care. 37(1): 5–14.
McCulloch DK, Price MJD, Hindmarsh M, Wagner EH. 2000. Improvement in diabetes care
using an integrated population-based approach in a primary care setting. Disease Man-
agement. 3(2): 75–82.
NCQA (National Committee for Quality Assurance). 2003. Measuring the Quality of Amer-
ica’s Health Care. [Online]. Available: http://www.ncqa.org [accessed: July, 2003].
Norris SL, Nichols PJ, Caspersen CJ, Glasgow RE, Engelgau MM, Jack L, Isham G, Snyder
SR, Carande-Kulis VG, Garfield S, Briss P, McCulloch D. 2002a. The effectiveness of
disease and case management for people with diabetes. A systematic review. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine. 22(4 Supplement): 15–38.
Norris SL, Nichols PJ, Caspersen CJ, Glasgow RE, Engelgau MM, Jack L, Snyder SR,
Carande-Kulis VG, Isham G, Garfield S, Briss P, McCulloch D. 2002b. Increasing diabe-
tes self-management education in community settings. A systematic review. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine. 22(4 Supplement): 39–66.
Nurss JR, Parker RM, Williams MV, Baker DW. 1995. TOFHLA: Test of Functional Health
Literacy in Adults. Snow Camp, NC: Peppercorn Books & Press.
Nutbeam D. 2000. Health literacy as a public health goal: A challenge for contemporary
health education and communication strategies into the 21st century. Health Promotion
International. 15(3): 259–267.
Piette JD. 1999. Satisfaction with care among patients with diabetes in two public health care
systems. Medical Care. 37(6): 538–546.
Piette JD. 2000. Interactive voice response systems in the diagnosis and management of
chronic disease. American Journal of Managed Care. 6(7): 817–827.
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. 2002. Annual Report 2001. Princeton, NJ: The Robert
Wood Johnson Foundation.
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation and the National Cancer Institute. 2002. A Research
Dialogue: Online Behavior Change and Disease Management Research. [Online]. Avail-
able: http://www.rwjf.org/publications/publicationsPdfs/onlineBehaviorChange.pdf [ac-
cessed: November 10, 2003].
Rost K, Roter D. 1987. Predictors of recall of medication regimens and recommendations for
lifestyle change in elderly patients. Gerontologist. 27(4): 510–515.
Roter DL. 2000. The outpatient medical encounter and elderly patients. Clinics in Geriatric
Medicine. 16(1): 95–107.
Roter DL, Hall JA. 1992. Doctors Talking with Patients/Patients Talking with Doctors: Im-
proving Communication in Medical Visits. Westport, CT: Auburn House.
APPENDIX B 283
Roter DL, Stewart M, Putnam SM, Lipkin M Jr, Stiles W, Inui TS. 1997. Communication
patterns of primary care physicians. Journal of the American Medical Association.
277(4): 350–356.
Roter DL, Rudd RE, Comings J. 1998. Patient literacy. A barrier to quality of care. Journal of
General Internal Medicine. 13(12): 850–851.
Roter DL, Stashefsky-Margalit R, Rudd R. 2001. Current perspectives on patient education
in the US. Patient Education and Counseling. 44(1): 79–86.
Rothman R, Pignone M, Malone R, Bryant B, Crigler B. 2003. A primary care-based, phar-
macist led, disease management program improves outcomes for patients with diabetes:
A randomized controlled trial Journal of General Internal Medicine. 18(Supplement 1):
155.
Rudd RE, Comings JP. 1994. Learner developed materials: An empowering product. Health
Education Quarterly. 21(3): 313–327.
Schillinger D, Grumbach K, Piette J, Wang F, Osmond D, Daher C, Palacios J, Sullivan GaD,
Bindman AB. 2002. Association of health literacy with diabetes outcomes. Journal of the
American Medical Association. 288(4): 475–482.
Schillinger D, Machtinger E, Win K, Wang F, Chan L-L, Rodriguez ME. 2003a. Are pictures
worth a thousand words? Communication regarding medications in a public hospital
anticoagulation clinic Journal of General Internal Medicine 18(Supplement 1): 187.
Schillinger D, Piette J, Grumbach K, Wang F, Wilson C, Daher C, Leong-Grotz K, Castro C,
Bindman AB. 2003b. Closing the loop: Physician communication with diabetic patients
who have low health literacy. Archives of Internal Medicine. 163(1): 83–90.
Schillinger D, Bindman A, Stewart A, Wang F, Piette J. 2004. Functional health literacy and
the quality of physician-patient communication among diabetes patients. Patient Educa-
tion and Counseling. 52(3): 315–323.
Sehgal AR. 2003. Impact of quality improvement efforts on race and sex disparities in hemo-
dialysis. Journal of the American Medical Association. 289(8): 996–1000.
Starfield B. 1998. Primary Care: Balancing Health Needs, Services, and Technology. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Street RL Jr. 1991. Information-giving in medical consultations: The influence of patients’
communicative styles and personal characteristics. Social Science and Medicine. 32(5):
541–548.
Trento M, Passera P, Tomalino M, Bajardi M, Pomero F, Allione A, Vaccari P, Molinatti
GM, Porta M. 2001. Group visits improve metabolic control in type 2 diabetes: A 2-year
follow-up. Diabetes Care. 24(6): 995–1000.
Trento M, Passera P, Bajardi M, Tomalino M, Grassi G, Borgo E, Donnola C, Cavallo F,
Bondonio P, Porta M. 2002. Lifestyle intervention by group care prevents deterioration
of Type II diabetes: A 4-year randomized controlled clinical trial. Diabetologia. 45(9):
1231–1239.
United Kingdom Prospective Diabetes Study Group. 1998. Intensive blood-glucose control
with sulphonylureas or insulin compared with conventional treatment and risk of com-
plications in patients with type 2 diabetes (UKPDS 33). Lancet. 352(9131): 837–853.
Vinicor F, Burton B, Foster B, Eastman R. 2000. Healthy people 2010: Diabetes. Diabetes
Care. 23(6): 853–855.
Von Korff M, Gruman J, Schaefer J, Curry SJ, Wagner EH. 1997. Collaborative management
of chronic illness. Annals of Internal Medicine. 127(12): 1097–1102.
Wagner EH. 1995. Population-based management of diabetes care. Patient Education and
Counseling. 26(1–3): 225–230.
Wagner EH. 1998. Chronic disease management: What will it take to improve care for
chronic illness? Effective Clinical Practice. 1(1): 2–4.
Wagner, EH. 2003. The Chronic Care Model: Improving Chronic Illness Care. [Online].
Available: http://www.improvingchroniccare.org/change/model/components.html [ac-
cessed: March, 2003].
Wallerstein N. 1992. Powerlessness, empowerment, and health: Implications for health pro-
motion programs. American Journal of Health Promotion. 6(3): 197–205.
Wallerstein N, Bernstein E. 1988. Empowerment education: Freire’s ideas adapted to health
education. Health Education Quarterly. 15(4): 379–394.
Weiss BD, Hart G, McGee DL, D’Estelle S. 1992. Health status of illiterate adults: Relation
between literacy and health status among persons with low literacy skills. Journal of the
American Board of Family Practice. 5(3): 257–264.
Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, Coates W, Nurss J. 1995a. The impact of inadequate
functional health literacy on patients’ understanding of diagnosis, prescribed medica-
tions, and compliance. Academic Emergency Medicine. 2: 386.
Williams MV, Parker RM, Baker DW, Parikh NS, Pitkin K, Coates WC, Nurss JR. 1995b.
Inadequate functional health literacy among patients at two public hospitals. Journal of
the American Medical Association. 274(21): 1677–1682.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Honig EG, Lee TM, Nowlan A. 1998a. Inadequate literacy is a
barrier to asthma knowledge and self-care. Chest. 114(4): 1008–1015.
Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, Nurss JR. 1998b. Relationship of functional health
literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. A study of patients with hyper-
tension and diabetes. Archives of Internal Medicine. 158(2): 166–172.
Williams MV, Davis T, Parker RM, Weiss BD. 2002. The role of health literacy in patient-
physician communication. Family Medicine. 34(5): 383–389.
Win K, Schillinger D. 2003. Understanding of warfarin therapy and stroke among ethnically
diverse anticoagulation patients at a public hospital. Journal of General Internal Medi-
cine. 18(Supplement 1): 278.
Youmans S, Schillinger D. 2003. Functional health literacy and medication management: The
role of the pharmacist. Annals of Pharmacotherapy. 37(11): 1726–1729.
Zarcadoolas C, Blanco M, Boyer JF, Pleasant A. 2002. Unweaving the Web: An exploratory
study of low-literate adults’ navigation skills on the World Wide Web. Journal of Health
Communication. 7(4): 309–324.
APPENDIX B 285
Insurers
Several entities provide medical insurance coverage groups in which
low literacy is most prevalent. They include the publicly funded Medicare
and Medicaid programs and the military’s Tricare program.
Medicare
The Medicare program is a federally funded program that provides
health insurance benefits to most elderly U.S. citizens. Medicare’s costs are
heavily influenced by limited health literacy because of the high rate of
limited literacy skills among elderly individuals. According to the Centers
for Medicare and Medicaid Services, around 35 million persons over 65
years old currently receive Medicare benefits, and the number of beneficia-
ries increases annually (Figure B-3).
Federal expenditures for the Medicare program now exceed $240 bil-
lion per year for medical benefits, administrative costs, and program integ-
rity costs, representing some 20 percent of all health-care spending in the
country. These costs will all increase as the number of beneficiaries contin-
ues to grow (Figure B-4) (CMS, 2002).
Employers
• Partner with public school systems to enhance general literacy and biological science
and health education in elementary and secondary schools.
• Partner with community-based literacy enhancement programs (e.g., ProLiteracy) to
expand availability of such programs, many of which are currently oversubscribed
and cannot meet community needs.
• Expand workplace literacy education programs to include an emphasis on health
literacy.
• Fast food restaurants could provide education to employees regarding modes of
transmission of infectious disease, thus improving both employees’ health literacy
and sanitation in restaurants.
Advocacy Organizations
• Advocacy organizations for the elderly could provide health education courses and
classes on common health problems of older persons, specifically designed for
seniors with limited literacy skills.
• Professional advocacy organization could create a nationwide corps of volunteer
physicians and other health professionals to teach health education and prevention
topics in elementary and secondary schools across the nation.
• Professional health advocacy organizations could establish a well-publicized
national health literacy bee, analogous to current national spelling or geography
bees, in which children and adolescents would compete to demonstrate their
knowledge of health information.
APPENDIX B 287
Others
• Assisted living centers for the elderly could provide health education videotapes for
residents to view in their homes.
• Health education messages could be displayed on the inside or bathroom stall doors
in all public restrooms.
• Health messages, perhaps in quiz form, could be displayed on movie theater screens
for the audience to view while waiting for the movie to begin.
• Sports celebrities could appear on national media to promote important health
issues.
• Cell phone display screens could provide a “health tip of the day.”
40
30
Millions
20
10
0
1980 1984 1986 1990 1996 1999
Year
$300
$250 $241
$211
$200
Dollars in Billions
$150 $132
$100 $81
$50
$50
$23
$9
$3
$0
1967 1972 1977 1982 1987 1992 1997 2001
Fiscal Year
FIGURE B-4 Federal spending for Medicare. Note that overall spending includes
benefit dollars, administrative costs, and program integrity costs (federal spending
only).
SOURCE: Medicare Program Spending. Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Ser-
vices, Office of the Actuary, June 2002 (http://www.cms.hhs.gov/charts/default.asp).
Medicaid
The Medicaid program, funded by state and federal tax dollars, pro-
vides medical insurance benefits for about 45 million persons, most of
whom are poor (CMS, 2000). Total expenditures by the Medicaid system
were $175 billion per year in 1998, accounting for about 15 percent of all
health-care spending in the United States (Health Care Financing Adminis-
tration, 2000). The cost of the Medicaid program is expected to grow to
$444 billion by 2010 (Figure B-5).
APPENDIX B 289
500 FY 2010
Share
Total = $444
State Federal
400
Expenditures ($, billions)
300
200
100
0
1998 2000 2002 2004 2006 2008 2010
Fiscal Year
ship between literacy and costs remained strong even after a multivariable
analysis that accounted for education, gender, ethnic group, and preferred
language. Thus, to the extent that interventions to improve health literacy
(Table B-5) can reduce health-care costs, such interventions can potentially
result in substantial cost savings for Medicaid programs.
Tricare
The Tricare program provides health insurance benefits for the nation’s
military personnel (active- and non-active-duty) and their dependents. The
program covers direct medical care, prescriptions, dental care, and a variety
of other health-related benefits.
Tricare is an expensive program. Spending by the federal government
for direct medical and administrative costs totaled $8.3 billion in 1998 for
some 4 million Tricare enrollees (Stoloff et al., 2000).
As noted earlier, many military recruits have limited literacy skills.
Given the association between limited literacy and higher health-care costs,
and given the cost of the Tricare system, significant cost savings might
accrue to the U.S. military if the health literacy skills of the military recruits
were improved. Thus, the military is an entity with large potential gain
from improvement in its members’ health literacy.
As mentioned, the military currently engages in literacy-skill enhance-
ment for its recruits to enable them to function adequately in the roles as
soldiers. Incorporating health knowledge within literacy training (Table
B-5) might provide further benefit by reducing excess costs related to
limited health literacy.
Employers
Business leaders have long recognized the need for literacy enhance-
ment in the workforce, as workers’ limited literacy skills often interfere
with productivity and safety (Rockefeller Foundation Conference Proceed-
ings, 1989). This concern is of particular importance for businesses that
employ large numbers of routine service providers and production workers,
because these groups have an over-representation of undereducated indi-
viduals (Reich, 1992). Some large employers already offer literacy training
to their employees to address concerns about workplace literacy (Academy
of Human Resource Development, 2000; Askov and Van Horn, 1993;
National Institute for Literacy, 1994).
Businesses should also have an interest in health literacy because of its
relationship to health-care costs. Government agencies have reported that
nearly two-thirds of Americans under age 65 obtain health insurance
through their workplace (Monheit and Vistnes, 1997), with more than 150
APPENDIX B 291
SOURCE: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Center for Cost and Financing Stud-
ies. 2000 Medical Expenditure Panel Survey—Insurance Component (http://meps.ahrq.gov/
MEPSDATA/ic/2000/Tables_IV/TableIVA1.htm).
Advocacy Organizations
There are many national-level organizations with missions dedicated to
improving opportunities and quality of life for their constituents. Of note,
some of these organizations advocate on behalf of the groups with the
highest rates of limited literacy—the elderly, Hispanics, and African Ameri-
cans—and these organizations could implement programs to improve their
constituents’ health literacy (Table B-5). Although not discussed here, there
are also advocacy groups on both local and national levels that represent
the other high-risk groups, such as other ethnic minority groups, immi-
grants, the homeless, the poor, and prisoners.
APPENDIX B 293
SOURCE: Unadjusted averages of prose and document literacy scores on the NALS as re-
ported on Tables 1.1A, 1.1B, 1.2A, and 1.2B in Kirsch I, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A.
Adult Literacy in America: A First Look at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey.
Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, U.S. Department of Education;
September, 1993, and on Table B3.13 in U.S. Department of Education. National Center for
Education Statistics. English Literacy and Language Minorities in the United States, NCES
2001–464, by Greenberg E, Macías RF, Rhodes D, Chan T. Washington, DC: 2001.
APPENDIX B 295
NAACP
NAACP is the nation’s oldest and largest civil rights organization.
NAACP has a specific health division, with goals that include “developing
national health advocacy and education initiatives that promote equity in
health status,” “sponsoring collaborative initiatives with other national
and local health groups,” and “expanding outreach on health advocacy and
awareness in communications” (NAACP, 2003). Assuring adequate health
literacy as a component of initiatives meets all of those goals, and NAACP
may thus be an effective advocate for enhancing health literacy among
African Americans.
Prison Systems
Inmates in most prisons already have access to literacy improvement
programs. Given the large and growing size of the U.S. prison population,
however, and the costs associated with providing health care to prisoners,
incorporating health literacy content might enhance current literacy im-
provement programs.
Professional Associations
Finally, professional associations representing health-care providers
have an interest in assuring and improving health and health care for indi-
vidual patients. With evidence showing that limited literacy skills are asso-
ciated with poorer health status, all professional associations representing
APPENDIX B 297
CONCLUSION
The unique vocabulary and concepts of medicine make it difficult for
many individuals to fully understand health information provided to them
by clinicians. This lack of understanding translates into poor health lit-
eracy—i.e., a limited ability to read, understand, and use health informa-
tion to make effective health-care decisions and follow recommendations
for treatment. While limited health literacy occurs in all segments of soci-
ety, it is a particular problem for individuals with limited reading skills (i.e.,
limited general literacy).
Limited literacy is more prevalent in certain groups. These groups in-
clude the elderly, racial and ethnic minorities, persons with limited educa-
tion, immigrants, prisoners, the poor and homeless, and military recruits. In
some of these groups, such as the elderly, certain ethnic minorities, and
persons who did not complete school, the prevalence of limited literacy
exceeds 80–90 percent.
Persons with limited general and health literacy, on average, have
poorer health knowledge, poorer health status, and higher health-care costs
than do persons with higher-level literacy skills. The relationship between
limited literacy and poorer health and higher costs is strong and indepen-
dent of other socioeconomic factors.
Based on results of the NALS, about half of U.S. adults have literacy
skills that are inadequate to meet the demands of today’s health system.
Health-care systems could address this problem through processes and poli-
cies that enhance employee awareness of patients’ health literacy skills, and
by delivering information in ways that patients can understand.
A variety of public and private entities have a stake in the health
literacy problem. These include health insurers, employers, and advocacy
groups. Insurers have a stake in the problem because of the high cost of
health care for persons with limited literacy. For example, because limited
literacy skills are so common among the elderly, most of Medicare’s $240
billion annual budget goes to providing care for persons with limited
literacy.
Employers, especially those that employ large numbers of underedu-
cated service and production workers, also have a stake in health literacy.
Employers pay the high cost of their employee’s health insurance benefits,
and their businesses lose productivity due to higher rates of illness among
employees with limited literacy.
REFERENCES
AARP. 2002. AARP Facts. [Online]. Available: http://www.aarp.org/what_is.html [accessed:
August, 2003].
Academy of Human Resource Development. 2000. Workforce Development. Symposium 37
[concurrent Symposium Session at AHRD Annual Conference, March 8–12, 2000]. Ra-
leigh-Durham, NC: Academy of Human Resource Development.
AHRQ (Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Center for Cost and Financing Stud-
ies). 2000. Medical Expenditure Panel Survey—Insurance Component. [Online]. Avail-
able: http://meps.ahrq.gov/MEPSDATA/ic/2000/Tables_IV/TableIVA1.htm [accessed
August, 2003].
Askov EN, Van Horn B. 1993. Adult educators and workplace literacy: Designing customized
basic skills instruction. Adult Basic Education. 3(2): 115–125.
CMS (Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services). 2000. Medicaid Eligables—Fiscal Year
2000 by Maintenance Assistance Status and Basis of Eligability. [Online]. Available:
http://www.cms.hhs.gov/medicaid/msis/00total.pdf [accessed: August, 2003].
CMS. 2002. Medicare Program Spending. [Online]. Available: http://www.cms.hhs.gov/charts/
series/sec3-a.ppt [accessed: August, 2003].
CMS. 2003. Health Accounts: Estimates. [Online]. Available: http://hcfa.gov/stats/NHE-proj/
[accessed: August, 2003].
Community Health Partners. 2003. A Partnership for Health. [Online]. Available: http://
chphealth.org/ [accessed: August, 2003].
El Paso Community College/Community Education Program. 2001. The El Paso Collabora-
tive Health Literacy Curriculum. [Online]. Available: http://www.worlded.org/us/health/
docs/elpaso/index.htm [accessed: August, 2003].
Gabel J, Levitt L, Pickreign J, Whitmore H, Holve E, Hawkins S, Miller N. 2000. Job-based
health insurance in 2000: Premiums rise sharply while coverage grows. Health Affairs.
19(5): 144–151.
Gabel J, Levitt L, Pickreign J, Whitmore H, Holve E, Rowland D, Dhont K, Hawkins S. 2001.
Job-based health insurance in 2001: Inflation hits double digits, managed care retreats.
Health Affairs. 20(5): 180–186.
APPENDIX B 299
T
his appendix presents background and sample items from some of
the measures that have been discussed in this report. For more
information about the development and use of these measures, please
see Chapter 2.
CONTENTS
Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine 302
Excerpts from the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults 304
Excerpts from the National Adult Literacy Survey 308
301
SCORE
List 1 __________________
List 2 __________________
List 3 __________________
APPENDIX C 303
Directions:
1. Give the patient a laminated copy of the REALM and score an-
swers on an unlaminated copy that is attached to a clipboard. Hold the
clipboard at an angle so that the patient is not distracted by your scoring
procedure. Say:
“I want to hear you read as many words as you can from this list. Begin
with the first word on List 1 and read aloud. When you come to a word
you cannot read, do the best you can or say “blank” and go on to the next
word.”
2. If the patient takes more than five seconds on a word, say “blank”
and point to the next word, if necessary, to move the patient along. If the
patient begins to miss every word, have him or her pronounce only known
words.
3. Count as an error any word not attempted or mispronounced.
Score by marking a plus (+) after each correct word, a check (✓) after each
mispronounced word, and a minus (–) after words not attempted. Count as
correct any self-corrected word.
4. Count the number of correct words for each list and record the
numbers in the “SCORE’ box. Total the numbers and match the total score
with its grade equivalent in the table below (Table C-2).
Excerpts taken from: Davis TC, Crouch MA, Long SW. 1993. Rapid Estimate of Adult
Literacy in Medicine: A Shortened Screening Instrument. Louisiana State University. Re-
printed with permission.
Sample Items
At the beginning of this section, the following introduction is read:
“These are directions you or someone else might be given at the hospital.
Please read each direction to yourself. Then I will ask you some questions
about what it means.” For the first few questions in this section the patient
is given Prompt 1, a prescription bottle that has the label shown in Figure
C-1 below taped to it.
GARFIELD IM 16 Apr 93
FF941858 Dr. Lubin, Michael
PENICILLIN VK
250MG 40/0
Take one tablet by mouth four
times a day 02 (4 of 40)
FIGURE C-1 Prompt 1 for TOFHLA. Prescription label that should taped onto
an actual prescription bottle that can be handed to the patient to read.
APPENDIX C 305
At the end of the numeracy section, the patient is given Prompt 10, a
laminated card with information shown in Figure C-2 below.
FIGURE C-2 Prompt 10 for TOFHLA. Laminated card with financial informa-
tion about clinic services.
Reading Comprehension
The reading comprehension section of the TOFHLA measures a
patient’s ability to read passages using real materials from the health-care
setting using a modified Cloze procedure. Passages included come from
instructions for preparation for an upper GI series, the patient rights and
responsibilities section of a Medicaid application form, and standard hospi-
tal informed consent language.
Sample Items
At the beginning of the reading comprehension section of the TOFHLA,
the following instructions are read:
Here are some other medical instructions that you or anybody might see
around the hospital. These instructions are in sentences that have some of
the words missing. Where a word is missing, a blank line is drawn, and 4
possible words that could go in the blank appear just below it. I want you
to figure out which of those 4 words should go in the blank, which word
makes the sentence make sense. When you think you know which one it
is, circle the letter in front of that word, and go on to the next one. When
you finish the page, turn the page, and keep going until you finish all the
pages.
The reading comprehension section consists of three passages; one of these
passages is shown on the next page.
APPENDIX C 307
If you __________ AFDC for any family ____________, you will have to
a. wash a. member,
b. want b. history,
c. cover c. weight,
d. tape d. seatbelt,
Excerpts taken from: Nurss JR, Parker RM, Williams MV, Baker DW. 2001. Test of
Functional Health Literacy in Adults. Available from Peppercorn Books and Press, Inc. Re-
printed with permission.
Sample Prose Item (Level 1): Swimmer Article: Locate Fact with No
Distractor
Task: Use the article “Swimmer completes Manhattan marathon” (See Fig-
ure C-3) to answer the following question.
Underline the sentence that tells what Ms. Chanin ate during the swim.
The answer is correct if respondent underlines, circles, or puts a mark
next to the sentence beginning A Spokesman for the Swimmer, or under-
lines, circles, or puts a mark next to any part of the sentence that just lists
the foods.
This level 1 task asks respondents to read a newspaper article about a
marathon swimmer and to underline the sentence that tells what she ate
APPENDIX C 309
during a swim. Only one reference to food is contained in the passage, and
it does not use the word “ate.” Rather, the article says the swimmer “kept
up her strength with banana and honey sandwiches, hot chocolate, lots of
water and granola bars.” The reader must match the word “ate” in the
directive with the only reference to foods in the article.
Sample Prose Item (Level 2): Swimmer Article: Locate Fact with
Distractor
Task: Use the article “Swimmer completes Manhattan marathon” (see Fig-
ure C-3 above) to answer the following question.
At what age did Chanin begin swimming competitively?
Acceptable responses are underlining or circling age or the sentence con-
taining the age in the article.
This level 2 task requires the reader to locate information in the text.
The reader is asked to identify the age at which the marathon swimmer
began to swim competitively. The article first provides the swimmer’s cur-
rent age of 23, which is a plausible but incorrect answer. The correct
information, age 15, is found toward the end of the article.
This level 3 item requires the reader to read a magazine article about an
Asian-American woman and to provide two facts that support an inference
made from the text. The question directs the reader to identify what Ida
Chen did to help resolve conflicts due to discrimination.
APPENDIX C 311
APPENDIX C 313
APPENDIX C 315
or when low-level inferences were required. Tasks at this level also asked
the reader to cycle through information in a document or to integrate
information from various parts of a document.
APPENDIX C 317
APPENDIX C 319
ages, or time units (hours and minutes). These quantities appeared in both
prose and document form. Quantitative literacy refers to locating quanti-
ties, integrating information from various parts of a document, determining
the necessary arithmetic operation, and performing that operation. Quanti-
tative literacy tasks included balancing a checkbook, completing an order
form, and determining the amount of interest paid on a loan.
APPENDIX C 321
total loan payment minus the amount of the loan ($10,000) equals the total
interest charges.
One of the most difficult tasks on the quantitative scale, this item
requires readers to look at an advertisement for a home equity loan and
then, using the information given, explain how they would calculate the
total amount of the interest charges associated with the loan.
Excerpts taken from: National Center for Education and Statistics, U.S. Department of
Education. Defining Literacy and Sample Items. [Online]. Available: http://nces.ed.gov/naal/
defining/defining.asp [accessed: October, 2003].
323
APPENDIX D 325
APPENDIX D 327
National Center for the Study of Adult Learning and Literacy and is Princi-
pal Investigator (PI) for the Health and Adult Literacy studies. Dr. Rudd
also serves as PI for the Literacy in Arthritis Management: A Randomized
Controlled Trial of a Novel Patient Education Intervention with the RB
Brigham Arthritis and Musculoskeletal Diseases Clinical Research Center
and a co-PI on Pathways Linking Education to Health Study with col-
leagues at the Harvard School of Public Health. She worked with Drs.
Kirsch and Yamamoto of the Educational Testing Services (ETS) to develop
a Health Activities and Literacy Scale that provides baseline data for health
literacy assessment and discussed in a forthcoming ETS policy report. Dr.
Rudd authored the action plan for the health literacy objective in Health
People 2010.
William Smith, Ed.D., is Executive Vice President and Senior Social Scien-
tist of Development Program Services at the Academy of Educational De-
velopment (AED). Dr. Smith supervises programs of communication and
marketing for social change, and serves as senior scientist for the develop-
ment of behavior change programs at AED, publishing and speaking to
policy-making audiences around the world. He often acts as consultant to
international organizations including UNICEF and WHO, as well as na-
tional departments of health and the Centers for Disease Control and Pre-
vention. Dr. Smith is recognized as one of the leading specialists in the
application of social marketing to social change, and he is co-founder of the
Institute for Social Marketing. He has designed, supervised, and evaluated
social marketing and communication campaigns on HIV/AIDS prevention
in 22 countries, and infant and maternal health in 35 countries of the
world.
IOM Staff
Lynn T. Nielsen-Bohlman, Ph.D., is a Senior Program Officer in the Board
on Neuroscience and Behavioral Health. Dr. Nielsen-Bohlman’s research
focused on human distributed cortical networks in working memory and
attention, and their modulation by arousal, aging, cortical and subcortical
degeneration, and cortical lesion. Her studies on the differential involve-
ment of anterior and posterior cortices in working memory provided the
first evidence of a distributed working memory network in humans. Dr.
Nielsen-Bohlman received her Ph.D. in physiology from the University of
California at Davis in 1994. She was a postdoctoral fellow at the University
of California, Berkeley, and the University of California, San Francisco,
Assistant Professor of Psychiatry at Vanderbilt University, and a Psychol-
ogy Department faculty member at Belmont University and the University
of Maryland University College. She is the Study Director for the Commit-
tee on Health Literacy, and has worked in science and education outreach
for two decades.
APPENDIX D 329
School Curricula. Before joining the IOM staff, she enjoyed a 5-year tenure
as an Administrative Assistant for the American Psychological Association
(APA) where she assisted the APA Committee on Psychological Test and
Assessment, Committee on Scientific Awards, and the Committee on Ani-
mal Research and Ethics. She also worked on several funding and grant
programs sponsored by the APA Science Directorate.
Index
331
332 INDEX
INDEX 333
334 INDEX
INDEX 335
336 INDEX
Flesch Scale Analysis, 201 Health, social and economic factors in,
Florida/Caribbean AIDS Education and 20–21
Training Center, 218 Health Assessment Questionnaire, 88–89
Food and Drug Administration (FDA), Health care
192, 194–195 and informed consent in research,
advertising, 194 187–190
labeling, 194 predicted spending on, 262
outreach, 194 quality of, 176–179
Formulating literacy, 45 Health Care Financing Administration
Foundation for Accountability, 183 (HCFA), 100, 291
Foundation for Informed Medical See also Centers for Medicare &
Decision Making, 217 Medicaid Services
Friends, a popular source of health Health-care settings, interventions in,
information, 125 206–213
Fry Readability Scale, 47, 201 Health Communication Division (CDC),
Functional literacy 218
functionality of all literacy, 39–40 Health content, incorporating into adult
measures of, 49–50 education programs, 156–157
Health contexts, literacy skill demands of,
41, 43
G Health Education Assessment Project,
152
Gallup Organization polls, 121, 146 Health education programs
sources of health information reported K-12, 143–146
in, 121
opportunities for, 148–149
Gathering Place, 132 Health expenditures, 181–182
Generating literacy, 45 “Health Framework for California’s
Georgia Academy of Family Physicians,
Public Schools, Kindergarten
297 through Grade Twelve,” 153
Geriatric Depression Scale, 84–85, 88–89 Health Information National Trends
GEs. See Grade Equivalents
Survey (HINTS), 121
Governmental and agency roles, 191–201 Health information sources, 123–126
roles of regulatory agencies, 199–201 advertising and marketing, 123–124
roles of state governments, 198–199
family and friends, 125
roles of the federal government, 191– the Internet, 125–126
198 news media, 123
Grade Equivalents (GEs), 46
Health information use, 120–126
for the REALM, 303 complexity of materials, 122–123
Grade-level measures of literacy, 45–47, selected findings from Sex Matters, 122
50
sources of health information reported
grade-level ability for individual in a Gallup Poll, 121
readers, 46–47 Health Insurance Portability and
grade-level measures of materials, 47
Accountability Act of 1996, 192
Gunning Fog index, 50 Health Interview Survey (HIS), 53
“Health is Strength” Project, 133
Health knowledge, associations of limited
H health literacy with, 7, 82–100
Hablamos Juntos, 114–115 Health law and health literacy, 183–191
Harvard School of Public Health, 158 doctrine of informed consent, 186–191
HCFA. See Health Care Financing governmental and agency roles, 191–201
Administration standard of reasonable care, 184–186
INDEX 337
338 INDEX
INDEX 339
340 INDEX
INDEX 341
National Institute for Occupational Safety NICHD. See National Institute of Child
and Health, 195 Health and Human Development
National Institute of Child Health and NIFL. See National Institute for Literacy
Human Development (NICHD), NIH. See National Institutes of Health
15, 147, 161, 196 No Child Left Behind legislation, 147
National Institute of Environmental Nongovernmental organizations,
Health Sciences, 196 opportunities to improve health
National Institutes of Health (NIH), 14, literacy, 134
16, 52, 55, 103, 119, 195–196, Northwest AIDS Education and Training
229 Center, 218
Department of Cancer Control and NRC. See National Research Council
Population Science, 254 Numeracy, sample items from the Test of
grant funding over the 1997–2002 Functional Health Literacy in
period, 196 Adults, 304–305
National Library of Medicine, 4, 31, 119
National Literacy Act of 1991, 43
National Medical Association, 112n, 295 O
National Network of Health Career
Office-based clinician–patient
Programs in Two Year Colleges,
159 communication, effect on chronic
National Research Council (NRC), 147 disease outcomes, 271–272
Office of Civil Rights, 135
National Science Foundation, 15, 150–
151, 161 National Standards to Protect the
National Science Teachers Association, Privacy of Personal Health
Information, 192
146, 150
National Standards to Protect the Privacy Office of Disease Prevention and Health
of Personal Health Information, Promotion (OPHP), 192–193
Office of Minority Health, 14, 136
192
National Survey on Health Literacy Center for Linguistic and Cultural
Initiatives, 198 Competence in Health Care, 114
Office of National Drug Control Policy,
National Urban League, 295
National Youth Anti-Drug Media National Youth Anti-Drug Media
Campaign, 124 Campaign, 124
Office of the Secretary, Office of Disease
Native Americans
cultural language of, 114–115 Prevention and Health Promotion,
literacy proficiency among, 63 192–193
Office of Vocational and Adult Education,
Native Hawaiians, understanding of
healing, 175 15, 154–155
NCI. See National Cancer Institute OPHP. See Office of Disease Prevention
and Health Promotion
NCP. See National Center for Health
Promotion and Disease Prevention Opportunities to improve health literacy
NCQA. See National Committee for in health systems, 201–227
approaches to health literacy in the
Quality Assurance
NCSALL. See National Center for the health system, 204–225
Study of Adult Learning and evidence-based approaches, 126–127
other promising approaches, 127–137
Literacy
News media, a popular source of health overview of current efforts, 202–204
information, 123 recommendations, 228–229
summary of approaches, 226–227
NHES. See National Health Education
Standards: Achieving Health Organization for Economic Co-operation
Literacy and Development, 61
342 INDEX
INDEX 343
344 INDEX
State Medicaid managed care contracts, See also Short Test of Functional
200–201 Health Literacy in Adults
State Officials Guide to Health Literacy, TEXT study, 150–151
The, 152, 198 Third Workshop, hosted by the
Strategies and opportunities, in the adult Committee on Health Literacy, 249
education system, 155–157 To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health
Strategies and opportunities in K-12 and System, 25, 173, 176
university systems, 148–154 TOFHLA. See Test of Functional Health
assessment of health literacy in Literacy in Adults
educational settings, 151–152 Traditional culture, 110–119
examples of current approaches, 152– cultural languages, 114–115
154 language, 113–114
opportunities for health education language and meaning in the context
programs, 148–149 of health, 115–118
strategies for health literacy measures able to dissociate culture,
instruction, 149–151 meaning, and health literacy, 118–
Substance Abuse and Mental Health 119
Services Administration, 15, 136 Tricare, 285, 290
Survey of Income and Program
Participation, 100
Surveys U
representatives of the study sample,
257 Unequal Treatment, 136
respondents to survey, comments from, University health education, 147–148
University of Colorado Medical School, 158
227
University of Virginia (UVA) School of
Medicine, 158–159
T U.S. Census, 108
U.S. Department of Defense, 14–15, 103
TANF. See Temporary Assistance for U.S. Department of Education, 15, 154–
Needy Families 155, 161
Target readability, of informed consent National Center for Education
documents, 189 Statistics, 193
Task Force on Guidelines for Community Office of Vocational and Adult
Preventative Services, 127, 267 Education, 15, 154–155
Technology-based communication U.S. Department of Health and Human
techniques Services (HHS), 2, 14, 16, 20, 25,
examples of ongoing approaches using, 55, 103, 119, 149, 181, 193, 228
218 National Standards for Culturally and
and health literacy in the health Linguistically Appropriate Services
system, 215–218 in Health Care, 109
Temporary Assistance for Needy Families Office for Civil Rights, 135
(TANF), 135 Office of the Actuary, 181n
Test of Functional Health Literacy in Task Force on Community Preventive
Adults (TOFHLA), 8, 47–51, 68– Services, 267
71, 74–87, 90–95, 98–99, 101 U.S. Department of Labor, survey by, 44
abbreviated version, 74–75 U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs (VA),
excerpts from, 304–307 197–198
numeracy, sample items, 304–305 National Center for Health Promotion
prompts for, 304–305 and Disease Prevention, 197
reading comprehension, sample items, U.S. Pharmacopeial Convention, Inc.
305–307 (USP), 215
INDEX 345