Argumentative Ness
Argumentative Ness
Argumentative Ness
66
“
Journal of Psychological Type® Issue 7 JUL 0 6
”
Individuals who prefer Intuition (N) and Thinking (T) are the most likely
to score high on argumentativeness, with ENTJs leading the way.
Preferences
Donald A. Loffredo, Ed.D.
University of Houston–Victoria
”
contributing to the individual’s partic- is considered a and to avoid arguments. “The more
ipation and success in U.S. culture. the motivation to approach argu-
constructive communi- ments exceeds the motivation to
Infante and Rancer (1996) noted
that argumentative communication is cation trait, because avoid arguments, the more argumen-
“crucial in a democracy” (p. 320). It is the messages are tative the individual is” (Rancer,
a communication pattern that helps content-oriented, 1998, p. 153). Some people are clas-
to “support, inform, and influence” sified as high argumentative (high
the workings of governmental, pri-
not oriented toward approach/low avoid) and others
vate, and public institutions in U.S. attacking an low argumentative (high avoid/low
society. Linking argumentativeness to individual . . . approach). However, Rancer also
specific MBTI preferences, then, can pointed out that two types of moder-
provide increased understanding into ates exist. Individuals can be high
the communication behavior and approach/high avoid and, despite
possibly the social experiences of the personality types. conflicting feelings, tend to argue only when they feel
For example, because the U.S. culture is one that they can “win” an argument. Others can be low
emphasizes argumentativeness, knowing the relation- approach/low avoid and, despite feeling apathetic,
ship between that communication trait and MBTI argue only when they feel they must.
preferences may provide insight into the personality Since Infante and Rancer (1982) first conceptual-
types’ participation and comfort level in such a culture. ized argumentativeness, numerous studies in both
Rancer (1998) noted that “understanding the com- communication and psychology have explored its
munication behavior of others can be enhanced by relationship to a variety of human behaviors and
knowledge of the traits that individuals possess . . .” experiences. A review of recent literature shows that
(p. 149). An initial study revealed a slight relationship researchers have given much attention to the connec-
between argumentativeness and MBTI preferences tion between argumentativeness and verbal aggressive-
(Loffredo & Opt, 1998). The purpose of this study was ness (Ifert & Bearden, 1998; Infante & Rancer, 1996)
to extend the previous research and search for signifi- because both are viewed as subcomponents of aggres-
cant correlations between argumentativeness and MBTI sive communication. Verbal aggression, in which the
preferences. person attacks the self-concept of another rather than
just the issue, is considered a destructive communica-
BACKGROUND tion strategy, whereas argumentativeness is considered
Argumentativeness can be conceptualized as one of four constructive (Infante & Wigley, 1986). Besides verbal
communication traits that form the concept known as aggressiveness, argumentativeness also has been corre-
aggressive communication. Infante (1987) viewed lated recently with variables such as culture (Toth,
aggressive communication as being controlled by these 1999), satisfaction and cohesion in group experiences
four traits, which also interact with situational factors to (Anderson & Martin, 1999), compulsive communica-
influence communication. As Rancer (1998) explained, tion (Bostrom & Harrington, 1999; Hackman, Johnson,
“Two traits (assertiveness and argumentativeness) are & Barthel-Hackman, 1995), cognitive and communica-
considered constructive, and two traits (hostility and tion flexibility (Martin, Anderson, & Thweatt, 1998),
verbal aggressiveness) are considered destructive” and persuasion (Kazoleas, 1993; Levine & Badger,
(p. 151). Rancer further noted that although all argu- 1993). Researchers in psychology have also examined
mentative behavior is aggressive, not all aggressive communication variables, as well as looking at the rela-
behavior is argumentative. tionship between argumentativeness and personality.
J n = 127 (63.5%)
P n = 073 (36.5%)
ISTP ISFP INFP INTP
n=8 n=9 n = 13 n=4
Pairs and Temperaments
(4.0%) (4.5%) (6.5%) (2.0%) IJ n = 54 (27.0%)
++++ +++++ +++++ ++ IP n = 34 (17.0%)
++ EP n = 39 (19.5%)
EJ n = 73 (36.5%)
ST n = 64 (32.0%)
SF n = 54 (27.0%)
NF n = 42 (21.0%)
NT n = 4040 (20.0%)
ESTP ESFP ENFP ENTP
n=9 n=6 n = 15 n=9 SJ n = 86 (43.0%)
(4.5%) (3.0%) (7.5%) (4.5%) SP n = 32 (16.0%)
+++++ +++ +++++ +++++ NP n = 41 (20.5%)
+++ NJ n = 41 (20.5%)
TJ n = 74 (37.0%)
TP n = 30 (15.0%)
FP n = 43 (21.5%)
FJ n = 53 (26.5%)
ESTJ ESFJ ENFJ ENTJ
n = 25 n = 22 n = 10 n = 16 IN n = 32 (16.0%)
(12.5%) (11.0%) (5.0%) (8.0%) EN n = 50 (25.0%)
+++++ +++++ +++++ +++++ IS n = 56 (28.0%)
+++++ +++++ +++ ES n = 62 (31.0%)
+++ +
ET n = 59 (29.5%)
EF n = 53 (26.5%)
IF n = 43 (21.5%)
IT n = 45 (22.5%)
Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations for Argumentativeness by Complete MBTI® Type in
Descending Order of Argumentativeness.
MBTI Type n M SD
.195, F [2, 197] = 23.89, p < .001). The T–F preference literature that males tend to be more argumentative
pair, complete MBTI type, and gender combination was than females (Cross, 1999; Rancer, 1998), a finding
the strongest predictor of argumentativeness (R2 = .217, replicated in this study.
F [3, 196] = 18.10, p < .001). The remaining predictors The results, however, did not support an earlier
could not predict any further unique variance in the finding in which the E–I preferences were linked with
criterion variable, argumentativeness, so they were not argumentativeness. This suggests that argumentative-
entered into the analysis. ness is a communication trait that reflects a person’s
internal perceiving and judging processes as opposed to
DISCUSSION the way in which a person externalizes those processes.
The increased sample size in this study elevated the In other words, a person’s tendency to argue may be
power of the statistical tests used, which revealed more an outcome of how that person processes informa-
several differences between MBTI preferences on tion than how he or she communicates that information.
argumentativeness. As expected, participants preferring Furthermore, although the NT core preference scored
N and T scored higher on argumentativeness, thus higher in argumentativeness as would be expected, the
showing a greater tendency to approach arguments. fact that the T–F index was the strongest predictor of
Also as expected, subjects preferring S and F scored argumentativeness suggests that this communication
lower on argumentativeness, thus showing a greater trait is most connected to a person’s perception of rela-
tendency to avoid arguments. This is consistent with tionship. Argumentativeness is conceptualized as being
previous research by Williams and Bicknell-Behr a content-oriented communication strategy, which fits
(1992), which showed similar results on assertiveness. well with the “detached” nature of Ts, whereas Fs would
It has also been well documented in communication find this more difficult and might either avoid what they
Susan K. Opt, Ph.D., is Associate Professor of Communication at Salem College in Winston–Salem, NC. She holds
a doctorate in communication from The Ohio State University, a master of arts in journalism from Ohio State, and a
bachelor of fine arts degree from Wright State University. Her research interests include communication and culture,
the rhetoric of social intervention, and MBTI preferences and communication patterns. Her other MBTI research has
examined the relationship between preferences and communication apprehension, receiver apprehension, and
argumentativeness. She finds that the MBTI tool provides a useful framework for helping students understand their
similarities and differences in communication patterns.
C O N TA C T
This Journal is being made available through the collaborative efforts of Dr. Tom Carskadon, Editor of the Journal of Psychological Type, and
the Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc., CAPT, worldwide publisher. Dr. B. Michael Thorne serves as Executive Editor of the
Journal of Psychological Type.
Journal of Psychological Type is a trademark or registered trademark of Thomas G. Carskadon in the United States and other countries.
CAPT is a not-for-profit organization dedicated to the meaningful application and ethical use of psychological type as measured through the
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator instrument.
Myers-Briggs Type Indicator, Myers-Briggs, and MBTI are trademarks or registered trademarks of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator Trust in
the United States and other countries.
Center for Applications of Psychological Type, Inc. and CAPT are trademarks or registered trademarks of the Center for Applications of
Psychological Type in the United States and other countries.
ISSN 0895-8750.