Journal of Linguistics: Vol. I (Studies in Language Companion Series 129) - Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John
Journal of Linguistics: Vol. I (Studies in Language Companion Series 129) - Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John
Journal of Linguistics: Vol. I (Studies in Language Companion Series 129) - Amsterdam & Philadelphia, PA: John
http://journals.cambridge.org/LIN
710
REVIEWS
711
JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS
three volumes, then the ensuing three chapters are devoted to SemR, the
starting point of linguistic synthesis, or the input to a MTM.
Three further chapters constitute Part II (‘ Semantic representation in
a meaning–text linguistic model ’). Chapter 4 (167–241) concentrates on
the notion and requirements of SemR, presenting a feature of semantics
in SMT as distinct from other semantic approaches in that it is at the core
of a complete theory MTT, and for a formalizable model MTM. The
representation SemR=nSemS ; Sem-Comm(unicative)S ; Rhet(orical)S;
Ref(erential)Sm indicates that SemR is composed of four structures, with
SemS expressing the propositional meaning of U, Sem-CommS(U) express-
ing its communicative organization, RhetS(U) presenting its ‘artistic’ or
expressive effects, and the RefS(U) linking semantemes in the SemS(U) to
their referents. Structured in the same way in any language, the SemS is
cross-linguistically universal. Its characterization is networks whose labels
on the nodes are semantemes and whose arcs are numbers. Semantemes are
classified into predicates and (semantic) names, which Mel’čuk calls two
semantic ‘parts of speech ’. A predicate has argument slots, which is the
combinatorial property of genuine predicates. All semantic (and syntactic
and morphological) links between the elements of utterances are expressed in
terms of dependency relations. Using numeral labels as mere distinguishers
rather than semantic roles constitutes a major difference from other semantic
approaches.
Chapter 5 (243–288) is about semantemes of causation, with ‘ cause1 ’
(=‘be the cause of ’) referring to non-volitional causation, and ‘ cause2 ’
(=‘be the causer of ’) referring to volitional or agentive causation. Employed
in a great proportion in semantic decompositions, causation maintains its
crucial place in semantic description.
Chapter 6 (289–394) focuses on Sem-CommS, the first peripheral structure
of SemR, but its significance is by no means peripheral. Being the ‘framing ’
or ‘packaging ’ of meaning, it concerns communicative organization of the
meaning expressed by U, rather than the communicative organization of U
itself. The latter is specified in the MTT framework by DSynt-CommS. This
novelty of MTT sets it apart from other approaches. Unlike the SemS, which
is conveniently and naturally represented by using genuine linguistic
units, Sem-CommS’s formal description must be based on an artificial
metalanguage, which makes use of eight oppositions, namely Thematicity,
Givenness, Focalization, Perspective, Emphasis, Assertivity, Unitariness,
and Locutionality, each with two or three values. The most universal one,
Thematicity, has three semantic values, Thematicity={Theme, Rheme,
Specifier}, and can be illustrated by the sentence ‘[In 2005]Sem-Spec [this
student]Sem-T [passed his final exam]Sem-R ’.
In volume 2 of SMT (Studies in Language Companion Series 135,
published too late for inclusion in this review), the author introduces Deep-
Syntactic Representation (DSyntR), or the endpoint of semantic operations
712
REVIEWS
(Part III), and describes the semantic module itself by detailing the major
types of semantic rules that perform the {SemRi} , {DSyntRj} transition,
i.e. semantic and deep-syntactic paraphrasing (Part IV). We look forward to
the publication of volume 3 of SMT, which – in the final Part V, entitled
‘Linguistic excursuses ’ – is to address several important semantic phenom-
ena and discuss in depth some corresponding concepts including actants,
Government Patterns, and phraseology (xxi).
As SMT came out after three decades since its conception, we can
expect the whole of this multi-volume ‘crowning achievement of a long
and illustrious career ’ (xiii) to be insightful and well presented, but not
necessarily abstruse. Judging by our reading of the first volume of the work,
the vast and deep topic is in fact very clearly structured and presented, with
the contents and structures for each part and each chapter clearly indicated
at the outset. All the key ideas are highlighted and each key concept is well
defined, exemplified and illustrated. These make it easy for readers to closely
follow the logical thread. With some very basic ideas of semantics, one can
start reading, move quickly and happily with the contents, and really enjoy
the novel ideas presented in the highly readable text. Sharply dissimilar from
other semantic approaches, this theoretically-complete formalizable model
of human language is unique in many ways.
In providing an overview of MTT, we note first that it has deep roots
in many of the schools of early 20th-century linguistics, such as transfor-
mational grammar, generative semantics, stratificational linguistics,
case grammar and rational grammar, whose influences are evident in MTT.
Secondly, as a theory of the fundamental question of the nature of language,
MTT is a unified framework, whose models operate on the idea that
language is a mapping (correspondence) from the meaning (semantics) of
U to its form or text (phonetics), with intermediate levels of syntax and
morphology. Thus, MTT has a stratificational and modular organization,
providing a coherent theoretical framework for semantics, and making it an
essential part of an integrated whole instead of being a stand-alone, distinct
component of grammar. In most other semantic approaches, semantics is
self-contained and is often independently studied with no connection with
other levels. Thirdly, relations (rather than classes) are the main organizing
factor in language and the concept of dependency (rather than constituency)
is used extensively in MTT. The representations at the different levels are
non-linearized, like the unordered network of SemR(U) indicating relations
between the semantemes. In the international context, MTT has established
a very successful model in studying dependency grammar. For instance,
a SyntR of U is a dependency tree-structure. And then the relationships
between representations are translations or mappings, rather than transfor-
mations. Sets of rules, or components ensure transitions between levels.
While many of the current trends treat various levels of language in a more
heterogeneous way, MTT uses different formal languages for different levels
713
JOURNAL OF LINGUISTICS
R EF ERE NCES
Cruse, Alan. 2004. Meaning in language: An introduction of semantics and pragmatics. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Davis, Steven & Brendan S Gillon (eds.). 2004. Semantics: A reader. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Lappin, Shalom (ed.). 2001. The handbook of contemporary semantic theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
Mel’čuk, Igor A. 1988. Dependency syntax: Theory and practice. Albany, NY : State University
Press of New York.
714
REVIEWS
715