Waimiri Atroari Grammar
Waimiri Atroari Grammar
Waimiri Atroari Grammar
by
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
2003
UMI Number: 3106972
Copyright 2004 by
Bruno, Ana Caria Dos Santos
UMI
UMI Microform 3106972
Copyright 2004 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.
jane Hill
erry Langend
g. 2^03
Ofelia Zep Date
Date
Date
^ Ajt) 3
Dissertation DirectorWill/Terry' Lange^idoen Date
3
Brief quotations from this dissertation are allowable without special permission,
provided that accurate acknowledgment of source is made. Requests for permission for
extended quotation from or reproduction of this manuscript in whole or in part may be
granted by the copyright holder.
SIGNED:
4
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Many people have carried this project forward to its completion. I am grateful for
the non-academic and academic support from those here and in Brazil.
My first THANKS go to the Waimiri Atroari people who shared their language
and culture with me. Thanks Warakaxi, Dauna, Ewepe, Damixiri, and Tuwadja for telling
me stories, repeating verbal paradigms, and patiently repeating what I could not hear, and
explaining things that I did not understand.
I would like to thank all whom I met in the Waimiri Atroari Program and who
made my stay in the field unforgettable and my research possible, particularly Parazinho,
Paulino Dantas, Goreti, Kissia Daniela, Trissiana Santos, Sara Moraes, Katia Nunes,
Mara Santos, Carla Yamane, Edith Lacerda, Antonio Carlos, Carmen do Vale, Marcilio
Dias and Porfirio Carvalho.
I also would like to say thanks to all my teachers and classmates from The
University of Arizona. Principally, thank you Heidi and Tania not only for sharing your
knowledge with me, but also for your friendship and support during the difficult
moments.
Thank you, Du, Gessi, and Sergiofrom the Museu Emilio Goeldifor helping
me with your ideas, friendship and support. Du, I will never forget when we shared the
apartment in Belem (laughing and fighting about Linguistics, dancing reggae, listening to
brega, redoing many times drafts of our projects and reports to Dermy, and eating 'unha
de caranguejo' because we did not have money to buy other stuff).
Thanks to my Brazilian friends in Tucson, Alzira and Daniel, Claudio and Sandra,
Lu and Marcal, Ana e Guto for your friendship, psychological and emotional support. I
am very grateful to you for entertaining my husband and taking care of my daughters
when I needed to have peace to study.
Thanks to Aldir, Stellinha, Carla, and Odileiz for helping me with your friendship.
Thank you to my mother who taught me to be strong even in the worst times.
Thank you, God, for giving me strength, serenity, and patience during the most
difficult moments of my life.
And finally THANK you Adair Palacio (my first advisor in Linguistics), Denny
Moore, Ofelia Zepeda, Dr. Langendoen, and Dr. Hill for being the most supportive
advisors that I could ever have hoped for through my graduate career and personal life. I
hope that one day I will be able to do the same good job for someone else as you have
done for me.
The fieldwork was largely funded and backed by the Programa Waimiri Atroari, which
provided transport, food, and accommodation.
DEDICATION
and
and
TABLE OF CONTENTS
CHAPTER 1- INTRODUCTION 12
CHAPTER 2 - PHONOLOGY 32
2. Phonology 32
2.1 Segments 32
2.1 Consonants 33
2.1.1 Stops 33
2.1.2 Palatals 34
2.1.3 Fricatives 35
2.1.4 Nasals 36
2.1.5 Glottal 37
2.1.6 Liquid 37
2.1.7 Approximant 38
2.1.8 Contrast 38
2.2 Vowels 39
2.2.1 High vowels 40
2.2.2 Mid vowels 40
2.2.3 Low vowels 41
2.2.4 Vowel Length 41
2.2.5 Contrast 41
2.3 Stress 42
2.4 Syllable Structure 43
2.5 Morphonological Process 45
2.5.1 Vowel replacement 45
2.5.2 Vowel deletion 46
2.5.3 Vowel harmony 47
2.5.4 Reduplication 47
2.6 Theorethical Discussion 48
2.6.1 Syllable structure 48
7
ABBREVIATIONS 159
APPENDICES 160
A1 Tyiyry ikaa 160
A2 Kyrywaky weriri ikaa 161
A3 lanana ikaa 163
A4 Kaapy tahkome karykapa 168
A5 Meie ikaa 172
A6 Wyty ikaa 173
B Kara mo pyky wenpatypy 175
C More verbal paradigms 176
D List of animals and plants found in this sketch with their scientific names 178
REFERENCES 180
9
ABSTRACT
The Waimiri Atroari people, who call themselves kinja 'people' and whose
language belongs to the Carib family, live today in an area in the northern part of the
State of Amazonas and in the southern part of the State of Roraima. Like many other
There are few linguistics studies about Waimiri Atroari, most of them being phonological
Taking this situation into consideration, this dissertation intends to describe some
First, in the introductory chapter I provide some information about their language and
culture, and I discuss their experience with formal education. Second, I describe the
segmental phonology and analyze the syllable structure and reduplication process under
Optimality Theory. Next, I present the word classes and a description of their
morphology. Then, I investigate the system of case marking. Finally, in syntax I analyze
the phrase structure and the word order under the framework of X-bar theory. The
"enough" data means waiting forever. Although I still have to understand and explain
many aspects of the Waimiri Atroari language, I did not want to wait forever to write this
grammatical sketch.
12
CHAPTER 1INTRODUCTION
The Waimiri Atroari people, who call themselves kinja 'people' and whose
language belongs to the Carib family, live today in an area in the northern part of the
State of Amazonas and in the southern part of the State of Roraima (see Figure 1 below).
They form 19 villages, located along the Camanau/Curiau, Alalaii, Jauaperi, and Rio
Branquinho rivers. The total population is 970 individuals (PWA, Waimiri Atroari
The Waimiri Atroari people have suffered a long history of violent and
the international market, such as the hides of forest animals, wood, Brazilian nuts, and
latex. According to Barbosa Rodrigues, the first contacts between the Waimiri Atroari
(whom he called Crichands) and non-indigenous Brazilians date from the 1P''' century in
the lower Rio Negro River (Rodrigues 1885:10). These contacts were not welcomed by
the Waimiri Atroari people. In the early 1900s, Alipio Bandeira^who worked for the
Indian Protection Servicetried to establish new contacts with the Waimiri Atroari
Until the latter half of the 20'^ century, invasions undertaken by local populations
and those organized by local governments were sporadic. However, toward the end of
13
the 1960s, the Federal government began massive occupation of the Waimiri Atroari
territory through the establishment of regional development projects. Figure 1 shows the
Vai tiara
Bd.'l Ui-qt;} ^ y /
/y A Kisiwc
/ .A V
J,.,. J ' \ f
\
A^ anahkwa'
ANAHKWA ' ! 1 '
! A ; /
i<. .^AIKON .
'r"% a"" a\ Rorat-J^a
WvrI ALALAU
a- f
-"WfiyslnUij
. A-J i " '"Will.UkJ r-illuiH -
' \
AM
B R AS:( L
In the early 1970s, the BR-174 highway linking Manaus (Amazonas State) to Boa
Vista (Roraima State) was pushed through the middle of the Waimiri Atroari territory
' In this map, each triangle is a village. This big line crossing circle 01 and circle 02 is the BR-174 road that
links Manaus (Amazonas State) to Boa Vista (Roraima State). As mentioned in Section 1.3, each circle is
analyzed as a speech community (Gumperz 1962)
(Bruno 1999). The construction of this road marked, for the Waimiri Atroari people, "the
beginning of a period of escalating contact with the outside world." (Milleken and Miller
1992:07).
Waimiri Atroari population was 1,500. Because of the construction of the road and
resulting innumerable health problems brought to the community, in 1986 the population
was reduced to 374. Moreover, in 1974, a project to identify all mineral resources of
Amazonia (Radam-Brasil) was established. Then, the Parapanema mine company, based
on the results of this project, solicited permission to mine in the Pitinga River. The
Brazilian Bureau of Amerindian Affairs) endorsement. Legally, this was not possible,
because the land it was located on was within the bounds of the Waimiri Atroari
reservation. Since 1981, this mining company has extracted minerals in that region.
Another historic event central to the current situation of the Waimiri Atroari
community is the construction of the Balbina hydroelectric plant. In the 1980s, one
hydroelectric plant was built to supply the city of Manaus with power, flooding part of
the Waimiri Atroari reservation. Consequently, two villages were relocated to different
locales, causing political problems to the extracted groups (Silva 1993). Moreover, as a
result of this construction, in 1987, a contract was established between FUNAI and the
Eletronorte (the company responsible for providing electricity in the northern part of
Brazil). This contract stated that the company would be responsible for providing
education, health, and environmental consultation to the Waimiri Atroari villages for the
next 25 years, in return for being given permission to build a hydroelectric plant on the
Waimiri Atroari reservation. As a result, the Programa Waimiri Atroari (The Waimiri
Atroari Program) was created, and FUNAI was designated to administer the money
provided by Eletronorte.
The Waimiri Atroari Program strives to minimize the direct and indirect impact of
the changes in the ecosystem of the Waimiri Atroari reservation. It has as a principal
goal to prepare the Waimiri Atroari people to become self-sufficient through helping
them to develop the necessary skills to manage their economic and environmental
resources and to make political decisions important for their cultural survival. Many
Brazilian anthropologists do not agree with the political actions developed by the
Waimiri Atroari Program. They classify the actions of the program as paternalistic and
oppressive. Baines (1992, 1994, 1997, 1999), an anthropologist who did fieldwork with
the Waimiri Atroari people from 1982 to 1985, classifies the indigenism developed by the
to the indigenous communities and at the same time establishing large projects in
indigenous territories.
Baines (1994:27) claims that in spite of the image of the Waimiri Atroari people
constructed by the media, in fact "it makes up the asymmetric relations established by an
indigenist administration that excludes the natives of the decisive process of planning and
evaluation, but incorporates them in its politic marketing." I agree that the attitudes and
behaviors of some of the Waimiri Atroari today reflect the interethnic experiences
explored by Baines (1991). However, the situation in the Waimiri Atroari area today is
very different. Baines did fieldwork in this community when the area was administered
by the FAWA (Waimiri Atroari Attraction Front 1970-1987). FAWA was constituted by
the old group of FUNAI employees, who utilized the traditional policies which
encouraged the dependence of indigenous people on State resources and patronage; this is
a different historical and political situation than the current one. Since the time Baines
was expelled from the area (1984), he has been unable to come back. Having done
fieldwork more recently (1995-1998, 2000), I observed that the Waimiri Atroari people
have been active in the decision-making process of planning and evaluating the program,
distinguish three Carib "types:" 1) Northwest Amazon groups; 2) a second group located
basically at the "Guiana area;" and 3) a third group found in the Upper Xingu Basin.
Using the criteria for classification established in Carib Speaking Indian: Culture,
Society, and Language (Basso 1977), the Waimiri Atroari people are best identified as
They live in single round houses, forming a communal family household that
constitutes a political group as well as a kin group (the whole village lives in a single
round house). The men move in with wife's natal family. The villages are usually small,
averaging about 20-40 kinja. For sustenance, they depend on bitter manioc, which is
utilized to make manioc flour with which to prepare large tortillas on a ceramic griddle.
Unlike other peoples of this second Carib group, the Waimiri Atroari people do not
use of hallucinogenic drugs. The shamans not only have the power to act as healers, but
also they name children and have knowledge of the sacred and divine.
The Waimiri Atroari people still live essentially according to their traditional
ways. They inhabit remote villages and subsist by slash and bum horticulture, hunting,
fishing, and collecting forest fruits. In their gardens, they plant mainly bitter manioc (to
make manioc flour and tortillas), banana, sugar cane, pineapple, and different species of
sweet potato. The villages are located close to small and big rivers, each village has
political and economic autonomy, and there is no centralized power. The distribution of
work among men and women is distinct, but sometimes they can work together. Men
hunt, prepare the gardens in order to plant together with women, and make basketry and
artifacts for fishing and hunting, such as arrows and bows. The women take care of the
domestic space, but they also collect fruits and can fish. They make hammocks and the
aruma^ bracelets.
Like the majority of Carib groups of that region, the Waimiri Atroari group has a
strong tendency toward local group endogamy, uxorilocality, and small local groups.
Dravidian type, based on the distinction between "consanguineal group" and "affinal
ties", from genealogical level, age, sex, and lineage. He also explains that
^ Bracelets make with a kind of native liana (vine) called aruma {Ischnosiphon arouma.Marantaceae).
18
The ideal marriage for a man is with a weriky 'cousin' category that
includes an ambilateral cross-cousin. It is possible for other kinds of
marriage, such as of a man with his pasky 'sister's daughter', or his
imekyky 'brother's daughter' ... The single men can have sexual access to
their brother's wives... According to the Waimiri Atroari's perception of
conception, a child will have as many fathers as possible depending on the
number of sexual partners that his/her mother had in the time of
conception. As a result, a person can have more than one apapa
'biological father.' Silva 1995:216
Linguistically, using Gildea's classification (1998), which is based on
morphosyntactic properties of each verbal system, the Waimiri Atroari language is in the
the Waimiri Atroari language has not suffered dramatic changes. All members of the
community (children, adults, and elders) speak the language. Generally, only the male
leaders of the villages and the native teachers speak Portuguese, and occasionally some of
a unique ethnic group. They speak a language which presents some phonetic and lexical
In the Waimiri Atroari area (Figure 1 above), it is possible to identify three speech
communities. A speech community is "a social group that shares a set of verbal signs"
(Gumperz 1962). The first speech community can be observed in the region of Alalau
River; all the villages in this area share the same verbal signs in speaking the Atroari
dialect. The second speech community is constituted by what I call the Samauma group
(the Mynawa, Karebi Syna, Pardo, and Arine villages), and the third is composed of the
Curiaii, Cacau, and Mare. The second and third speech communities both speak the
Waimiri dialect. Silva (1993:44) noted that among this aglomerado 'cluster', as he calls
it, the linguistic unity among the groups is mentioned more often than the disparities.
Interestingly, each speech community makes fun of "the way of speaking" of the others.
Until recently, little linguistic work had been done to document this language
family. Although today there is an increase in linguistic studies related to the Carib
languages (Meira 1999, Gildea 1998, Campetela 1997, Pacheco 1997, Hall 1988, Hill and
Hill 1985, Tavares 1993, Payne 1990, Derbyshire 1985, 1999, and Hoff 1986), these
studies are restricted to linguistic aspects, such as phonology, morphology, and syntax,
Like the majority of languages in the Carib family, there are few linguistic studies
of the Waimiri Atroari language, most of them being phonological sketches (Hill and Hill
1985, Lacerda 1991, Bruno 1995, 1999, 2000, and 2002). Taking this situation into
syntactic features.
Since Basso's edited collection (1977), few ethnographic studies have been done.
Studies relating to the languages and cultures of the Carib family are limited (Basso
1985, 1987, 1995; Francheto 1986; Kloos 1971; Baines 1991; and Mattel Muller 1991).
the Waimiri Atroari people (Baines 1999, 1997, 1994, 1993, 1992, 1991; Bandeira 1926;
Barbosa Rodrigues 1885; Carvalho 1982; Monte 1992; Milleken et al 1992; Silva 1993,
1995; Do Vale 1994, 2002; and Espindola 1995). Baines's work explores the interethnic
experience among the FUNAJ employees and the Waimiri Atroari people; Silva's work
discusses the kinship system, and Do Vale (2002) has a very interesting ethnographic
Until recently, the Waimiri Atroari language was not written. In 1985, one village
had contact with a national group of Catholic missionaries known as CIMI (Indigenous
linguistic studies. They taught about twenty village men to read. Later, the couple was
refused access by FUNAI, but the village people wanted to continue to study.
21
Amazonian)Joseph and Tamara Hill^who had worked and done linguistic research
with the Wai-Wai people (another Carib group), tried to continue to work in the same
village. They also did a preliminary description of the phonology and established another
orthography that diverged from the Schwade's orthography in the following aspects (see
Table 1.2 below). Later on, this couple was expelled as well.
22
/JT/ ii n
/y/ y to represent the glide J
/i/ i y
They did not register long vowels They registered long vowels as VV
In 1988, with the contract established between FUNAI and Eletronorte, the
Waimiri Atroari people decided that any educational experience from now on would be
realized for all the villages (no more restricting education to one village). In 1987, Marcio
Camanau/Curiaii area. He wanted to collect data about the kinship system, and as a
condition for his research the Waimiri Atroari asked him to teach them to read and write.
Silva utilized Schwade's orthography in his classes. From 1988 to 1993, FUNAI hired
non-native teachers to go to the villages to teach the Waimiri Atroari people their
In May 1988, Joao das Letras, responsible for the educational sub-program,
analyzed the different orthographic systems and chose particular aspects of each of them.
In 1989, the linguist Ruth Monserrat started to provide linguistic advice to the project.
Under her supervision and with the help of Edith Lacerda (the teacher) and Edilberto
Fonseca (the coordinator of educational sub-program), the educational group (the non-
native teacher together with the linguist) started to have meetings to discuss linguistic
^ Lacerda, having a natural talent, developed by herself some linguistic research describing many aspects of
the Waimiri Atroari language. I learned a lot from her.
23
The problem with this system was that the Waimiri Atroari did not attend the
discussions. Although the non-native teachers discussed the issues with Waimiri Atroari
students in the villages, in the meetings with the linguist no kinja were there. Moreover,
Monserrat was very busy giving linguistic advice to different indigenous groups.
Consequently, she did not develop any profound linguistic study of the language. This
system generated a climate of uncertainty among the community in relation to the way of
writing the language. Besides, the non-native teachers did not stay very long in the
villages. Then, every time that a village got a new teacher, they needed time to leam the
language and adapt to the village life style (no electricity, no running water, and two
months in the jungle in order to get eight days of vacation in the city).'^
'' My first contact with the Waimiri Atroari people happened in 1991. At that time, I also was a non-native
teacher. I worked with them until 1993. At this time, I did not have any intention of doing linguistic
In April 1992, the non-native teacher of Mynawa village became the new
educational coordinator of the project. Together with the community, she decided to not
change anything in the orthography until someone could provide a close and permanent
linguistic study. In 1994, the Waimiri Atroari community decided to invest in forming
native teachers inasmuch as the non-native teachers had problems with the Waimiri
Atroari language and did not stay very long in the villages. At the end of 1994, I
proposed to the community to do linguistic research with them. Tired of linguistic and
anthropological research, they only accepted me because I had lived with them before.
In 1995, the educational sub-program and the community decided that some
modifications to the orthography would be made. At that time, people from different
villages used different orthographies; even in the same groups, people wrote sometimes
with a different orthography than the one they were exposed to. The Waimiri Atroari
teachers wanted to produce literacy material, but they were worried that these materials
would not take into consideration dialectal differences or would not work at all because
Then the community, the educational group and I decided to rethink the
orthography. I went to different villages and asked many questions, such as what they
thought would change or not change in the orthography; and, from the different
orthographies, what aspects we could continue to use. I did not want to make drastic
changes because many kinja already knew how to write. Later on, the educational sub-
research. However, I had the opportunity to work in a village that had the tradition of producing a lot of
mythological stories and different kinds of texts, continuing the work established by the teacher, Carlos
Augusto Queiroz, who had needed to leave the village because of health problems.
program organized a big meeting on the reservation where I and 35 Waimiri Atroari
people selected by the community discussed and made the necessary changes, such as
covering the dialectal differences, marking the long vowels because the non-native
teachers could not hear them; the kinja described things were missing when writing some
words. Non-native people did not hear the differences between kymy 'bacaba berry' and
kymyy 'hand', for example, and the orthography needed to change to record this
distinction.
I believe that any orthography should represent the sounds of the language in
question, not only being phonologically well based, but also taking into consideration the
speakers' beliefs and perspectives. An idea of what is going on in the language and how
the sounds can be represented must be given by linguists, explaining the consequences,
but only the speakers must decide. The idea that indigenous people 'are not able to
by the kinja teachers about phonology). Besides, language is not only an instrument of
communication, language carries symbolic value that conditions social, political, and
economic spheres.
26
Table 1.4Differences between the old orthographies and the one utilized today
In fact, I would have preferred to use [y] to represent the glide sound, but the
registered as [i]. If they used [j] to register the glide, it could make things more confusing
because the letter [j] in Portuguese is used in a different way. Because the Waimiri
Atroari schools are bilingual, I think we need to keep this in mind when elaborating
orthographies.
27
The new role and figure of being a teacher brought some changes and problems to
the routine of the community. One of the problems was the power struggle among the
traditional leaders and the native teachers regarding changing the notion of leadership.
The first changes in the concept of leadership were situated during the opening
the Waimiri Atroari culture was changed by FUNAI's intervention in the area. He
explains that in order to establish contact with the community, FUNAI "created" a system
of capitaes 'captains' (military view) for each village. The capitaes were selected by their
ability (in that time extremely limited) to speak Portuguese, lack of resistance to the
construction of the road, and easy manipulability. To sustain this manipulative tactic,
FUNAI "offered" some privileges to the capitaes, such as food, clothes, radios, lanterns,
authority and leadership was modified. In order to sustain this change, not only the
FUNAI employees, but also the new leadersthe captainsexplained that the elders (the
traditional leaders) died with the contact; therefore, they needed new leaders. However,
Baines mentions that not all of the elders died; in reality, the "elders that survived were
deprived of their authority by FUNAFs workers and the Waimiri Atroari captains"
(Baines 1991:282).
The other problem is related to how the Waimiri Atroari teachers can reconcile
both types of knowledge; the problem that they are having is that they do not yet know
how to find the time for both forms of knowledge. Usually, they say that;
It is hard to find time to hunt, fish, leam how to make basketry, learn
traditional songs, and at the same time to prepare classes, study, and attend
Today, there are two native teachers in each of the seventeen schools. The school
classes average from five to fifteen students. Generally, the women have classes separate
from the men, but there are a few exceptions, where men and women study together.
Students range in age from eight to forty. Currently, the younger children are not
formally enrolled to attend on a regular basis (however, they are welcomed at the schools
(taking the Waimiri Atroari conceptions and knowledge of the world to study Science
Due to the fact that there is currently 100% use of the language, the Waimiri
Atroari community has not developed a concern to sustain language use. On the other
hand, the Waimiri Atroari teachers are very concerned about the elaboration of material
for use in the school setting to assure use and retention of the language and culture.
Therefore, in 1997, the educational program and I decided to improve the list of words
organized by Lacerda and Queiroz (1991), and create an illustrated dictionary that could
This description is based on data collected through fieldwork with native speakers
of the language. Waimiri Atroari consultants of four different villages and two speech
communities ranging in age from fifteen to fifty years helped me to obtain specific
EwepeMarcelo, and DamixiriRenato. The elicitation was done in three stages. The
first one occurred when I was a teacher in the Alalau and Paryry villages in 1991. As I
did not have the intention of conducting professional linguistic research, I just collected
some verbal paradigms and mythological stories with Dauna (Shaman and storyteller of
the Alalau village). In the second stage (1995-1998), I collected more focused
30
marking, causatives, and word order. Natural texts in the form of stories and m34:hs were
tape recorded and then transcribed with the assistance of the Waimiri Atroari consultants.
Discussion of the grammaticality of some materials was divided into categories; for
and syllable structure. I also utilized this time to make some revisions to the dictionary
material.
The present work is organized as follows. Chapter 1 discusses some historical and
cultural aspects of the Waimiri Atroari people. It also provides a discussion of the
segmental units in Waimiri Atroari and analyzes some aspects such as syllable structure
morphology of each lexical class. It also provides an analysis of case marking in this
language. Chapter 4 describes the syntax of phrases, clauses, and simple and some
complex sentences. It analyzes the possible types of word order in this language and
(?) - interrogative symbol in the glosses line means that I do not know the
meaning;
utterance in the first line, glosses and morphemic analysis in the second line, and a free
CHAPTER 2 - PHONOLOGY
2. Phonology
This chapter is organized as follows. Sections 2.1 and 2.2 present a structural
description of the segmental units in Waimiri Atroari. Section 2.3 analyzes the stress
pattern in this language. Section 2.4 provides a description of the syllable structure.
vowel deletion, vowel harmony, and reduplication. Finally, section 2.6 analyzes some
and twelve vowel phonemes, seven short and five long. The consonant phonemes are /p/,
Pol, /t/, /d/, /k/, /?/, /s/, /J/, /h/, /tj"/, /d3/, /m/, /n/, /jV, /r/, /w/, /j/, and the vowel phonemes
are III, Id, Id, /a/, /i/, /u/, /o/, /e:/, /a:/, /e;/, /i:/, /o:/.
2.1 Segments
Table 2.0 and Table 2.2 show all 29 distinctive segments of Waimiri Atroari
represented together with the orthographic symbols used in the literacy project (see
Fricative s I(x) h
Nasal m n Ji(nj)
Liquid r
Approximant w j(i)
33
2.1 Consonants
2.1.1 Stops
Phonemes /p/, III, fk! are voiceless occlusives at bilabial, apico-alveolar, and
dorso-velar points of articulation. They can occur initially and intervocalically. Phonemes
/b/ and /d/ are voiced occlusives at bilabial and apico-alveolar points of articulation. They
can only occur in the beginning of the word and intervocalically. Waimiri Atroari does
not allow stops in coda position or at the end of the word.^ Note that /p/ and /k/ appear in
(l)/p/ {2)1X1
(3)/k/
Sometimes /p/ occurs in free variation with the voiced stop /b/ and the nasal /m/.
Like the phoneme /p/, the phoneme /b/ also occurs in variation with the nasal /m/.
Although the examples below show this free variation, /p/, /b/, and /m/ are distinctive
segments as shown in Table 2.1, which shows minimal or near minimal pairs.
(8) [b]~[m]
babyka~bamyka 'wide'
benry-menry 'design, drawing'
The examples in (6) and (7) show that Waimiri Atroari does have nasal
assimilation: for example, impa 'then, after.' However, it is also possible to find
examples where nasal assimilation does not occur, such as nytynpa 'he/she went' and
sakenpa 'angry'. The cases where nasal assimilation does not occur involve a morpheme
2.1.2 Palatals
The phonemes /tx/, and /dj/ are respectively voiceless and voiced phonemic
Although /dj/ occurs more frequently before /i/ than /d/, this does not mean that
/d/+/i/ always results in the palatalization of /d/. See the examples in (11)-(13) where /d/
2.1.3 Fricatives
The phonemes /s/, /x/, and /h/ are voiceless fricatives at apico-alveolar,
alveopalatal, and glottal points of articulation, respectively. They can occur in the
beginning and in the middle of the word. In this group, /s/ and /h/ can appear in word-
(16)/h/
In some words, it is possible to find free variation among [x]~[h], [s]~[h], and
the Carib family, the most striking of these being the occurrence of a consonantal system
36
which includes five segments produced with friction (three fricatives, /x/, fhl, and /si, and
two affricates, /tx/ and /dj/). The existence of both the glottal fricative /h/ and the glottal
stop /'/ is a rare phenomenon in the family. According to Gildea (1995), these consonants
originated from a historical process of syllabic reduction: in some languages, this process
results in a stop; in other languages, the result is a Ixicative. In Waimiri Atroari, though,
2.1.4 Nasals
The phonemes /ml, /n/ and /nj/ are nasals produced at bilabial, apico-alveolar,
palatal points of articulation, respectively. They can occur in the beginning and middle of
the word. The phonemes im/ and /n/ can occur in the coda position. However, they do not
appear at the end of the word. The only exceptions are found with the second position
particle ram and the word takrehen 'delay, linger.'^ Although the phoneme /nj/ usually
(17)/m/ (18)/n/
a.ba.ma [a.ba.ma] 'blind' na.na [na.na] 'pineapple'
ma.ma [ma.ma]'mother' ia.na.na [ja.na.na]'mythological pers'
sam.ka [sam.ka] 'hanrniock' ny.tyn.pa [ni.tin.pa] 'she/he went'
(19)/nj/
2.1.5 Glottal
The occlusive glottal /'/ occurs between vowels and between a vowel and a
consonant. When a glottal appears in an intervocalic envirormient, the two vowels are
always identical. It can appear in the middle of the word, and in the end of the word as
2.1.6 Liquid
The phoneme /r/ is the only liquid segment in this language. It is a flap with some
lateral release. In Waimiri Atroari, with the exception of the second particle position ram.
^ In the text 'kaapy tahkome karykepa' in the appendix, it is possible to see that the underlying
representation of takrehen is takrehene; therefore, this coda at the end of the word is not common.
38
there are no words beginning with /r/. Moreover, /r/ can form clusters /kr/ and /pr/ in
CCV syllables.
(24)
wa.ra.ra [wa.ra.ra] 'turtle' te.me.re [te.me.re] 'jaguar'
py.ry.ry [pi.ri.ri]'frog' pa.na.ry [pa.na.ri]'long time ago'
ma.kry.kry [ma.kri.kri] 'pernilong' kra.txi.ni [kra.tji.ni] 'proper name
we.ri [we.ri] 'woman' me.pry [me.pri] 'tapir'
2.1.7 Approximant
Phonemes /w/ and /y/ are a bilabial glide and a palatal glide, respectively. Hill
sketch, /w/ and /j/^ are interpreted as a consonant in initial position of the syllable, but in
the nucleus the same segments are interpreted as the vowels /u/ and I'll.
2.1.8 Contrast
This section provides examples that show contrast among the consonant
^ Today, in the orthography used in the literacy project, the community does not use [y] to represent the
glide. For the glide, they use jj] as established by Hill and Hill (1986). Now /i/ is used both in onset and
nuclear position, such as hi.ri 'cajun fruit' and ma.ba.ia [ma.ba.ya] 'papaya.' In order to address the
community request and not do radical changes, we used the symbol [y] for the /i/.
39
2.2 Vowels
From this group of high vowels, only the /i/ high front vowel and the /i/ high
central unrounded vowel can occur in the beginning of the word as a V syllable. On the
other hand, only the high central unrounded /i/ can be lengthened. The high round vowel
IvJ has a very restricted distribution. It only occurs preceding or following bilabials.
(29) IvJ
In this group, both vowels can be lengthened. The middle, front unround vowel Id
ranges from closed to open: /e/ occurs in closed syllables, and is glottalized or lengthened
in an open syllable followed by a consonant. On the other hand, the middle back rounded
In Waimiri Atroari, vowel length occurs only in open syllables. There are no
considered phonemes contrasting with short vowels; they are found among nouns, verbs,
(34)
2.2.5 Contrast
This section provides examples that show contrast among the vowel phonemes in
2.3 Stress
falls on the final position syllable. Words with more than two syllables may possibly
have primary and secondary stress. The length of the vowel influences the stress pattern.
There are a lot of questions that I was not able to answer in relation to the stress
The syllable can also include a consonant element in onset position and another in final
position as a coda. Onsetless syllables (V, VV, VC) only occur at the begirming of the
word. Any consonant may appear in syllable-initial position, but only nasals /m/ /n/,
fricatives /s/, and glottals /V /h/ may appear in coda position. When CC.C occurs, the
Onset Coda
Left edge of the intervocalically Left edge and Right
word middle of word edge of
the word
/p/ + + - -
/b/ + + - -
/t/ + + - -
/d/ + + - -
/k/ + + - -
/'/ + +
/h/ + + + -
/s/ + + + -
/x/ + + - -
/tx/ + + - -
/dj/ + + - -
/m/ + + + -
/n/ + + + -
/nj/ + + - -
/r/ - + - -
/w/ + + - -
/y/ + + -
In Waimiri Atroari, eight different syllable patterns are observed: V, VV, VC,
V, VV, and VC syllables occur only at the leftmost edge of the word. In the V
syllables, all the vowels can fill this slot, except the high, back unrounded /u/. On the
other hand, VV syllables only appear as the prefix that marks 1' possessive and first
object.
(36) /V/
(37) N N !
(38) INa
CVC, CCV, and CVV syllables, like CV syllables, can occur in different
environments. CVC and CCV syllables can occur initially, medially, and finally;
(39) /CVC/
(40) /CCV/
(41)/CVV/
appears at the end of the verb. Then, when syllabifying the /h/, it will fill the coda
position.
(42) /CCVC/
2.5.1.1 In Waimiri Atroari, word-final vowels /e/, /y/, and III are replaced by /a/ when
(43)
2.5.1.2 In interrogative phrases, the final vowel of the last word is usually replaced by a
glottalized /eV.
(44)
In texts and also in normal speech, between two morphological words, the word-
final vowels of the first word delete, and almost always after [h], one of the consonants
(45)
Moreover, when adding the suffixes valuative -e 'me and the devaluative -erne the
last vowel of the word is deleted.
wykyry 'man'^ wykyr-eme
weri 'woman'> wer-eme
b a h i n j a 'c h i l d b a h i n j - e m e
47
2.5.3.1 When the phoneme /y/ precedes Ci, /y/ is replaced by /i/.
(46)
2.5.3.2 Vowel harmony also occurs between the verb stem and the suffix marking
tense/aspect
2.5.4 Reduplication
meaning of repetition (to do many times) or continuation (to keep doing). Waimiri
Atroari has a case of partial reduplication: the reduplicant copies part of a segment of the
to give the possible verb stem shape in this language, inasmuch as the reduplicant copies
(Prince & Smolensky 1993). For example, the patterns V, VC, and VV violate the onset
constraint by which syllables prefer to begin with consonants. However, these patterns
only occur at the left edge of the word. Although in this language the only VV syllable is
found in the prefix that marks 1^' possessive or 1' object, I will consider it to be a syllable
because this pattern undergoes the same constraints established for VC and V syllables.
Another interesting aspect related to this pattern is the sequence of VV and V syllables;
for example, when i.myy ' somebody's hand' gets the possessive marker, it will be
syllabified aa.i.myy 'my hand.' The two syllables do not violate left-edge alignment.
morpheme edges.
On the other hand, although Waimiri Atroari allows syllables without onsets in
the leftmost syllable of the word, this is not true for the reduplicant, which is unmarked
relative to the language as a whole. Waimiri Atroari has a typical case of emergence of
the unmarked structure (McCarthy & Prince 1994) when selecting the shape of the
reduplicant (RED); as a result RED must have an onset, thus it must be unmarked (Bruno
2000).
Moreover, we can observe that the CCV and CCVC syllables also violate another
constraint related to onset: * Complex Onset. Above in the syllable structure section, I
explained that CCVC syllables occur only in word-medial position. One of the reasons
morpheme -hkypa~hpa 'after something,' which appears at the end of the verb. As a result,
when syllabifying, the /h/ will be the coda, contributing to the structure of the syllable.
On the other hand, the patterns VC, CVC, and CCVC violate the constraint, which states
that codas are not allowed. In Waimiri Atroari, codas are allowed; however, they can
only be nasal, fricative, or glottal and cannot occur at the end of the word, as was
to account for the data set in section 2.4: Align onsetless syllable left, *Complex Onset,
Parse and Fill as suggested by Kager (1999), and MAX-BR to cover the emergence of the
unmarked syllable in the reduplicant case. I decided to use the align-onset left instead of
just onset because Kager (1999:110) has a very interesting discussion about the "naive
notion that syllables must have onset except word-initially." He argues that redundancy
can be avoided by using the alignment constraint. Below I provide the definition of each
constraint.
Align onsetless SYL/Left - Syllables without onset must be aligned to the left
edge of the word;
50
b)'a.ma.na *!
*
c) a.man *!
d) a.ma.na
candidates (b) and (c) violate DEP I-O and MAX I-O, respectively. Candidate (a) does
not violate any faithfulness constraints; however, it does violate the constraint align
which syllabifications which create onsets are favored over syllabifications which create
Tableau 2
**
b) ma.kry.kry
c) ma.kry.y.kry *! **
*!
As Waimiri Atroari allows syllables with complex onsets, this constraint will
always be violated. Candidates (a) and (c) violate the constraint DEP I-O; however,
candidate (c) is the worst candidate because it violates the align onsetless/Syl/Left
constraint, as well. As a result, candidate (b) is the winner because it only violates
*complex onset.
Tableau 3
b) a.ma.kra
c) a.ma ky.ra *!
candidate because it not only violates the alignment constraint, it also contradicts the
Candidate (c) is the worst candidate because it violates the faithfulness constraint, DEP
Tableau 4
Candidate MAX DEP Coda Align *Complex
/nybixikwahpa/ I-O I-O Condition Onsetless/Syl/ onset
'after injured' (Nasal/ Left
Fricative/
Glottal)
*
a) nyb.i.xi.kwah.pa *!
b) nyb.xi.kwah.pa *! *!
c) ny.bi.xi.kwah.pa
faithfulness constraint, MAX I-O, and the coda condition, as well. Candidate (a) does not
violate the undominated constraints, but it does violate the coda condition and the align
Tableau 5
b) no.sa *!
c) no'.sa
In Tableau 5, all candidates satisfy the coda condition constraint. Candidates (a)
and (b) cannot be the optimal candidates because they violate the faithfulness constraints
MAX 10 and DEP 10. However, candidate (a) is the worst candidate because it violates
Tableau 6
b) ar. v.-na.ry.ma.pa * *
c) ma.rv.-na.ry.ma.pa *! - * -
*
d) na.ry-na.ry.ma.pa
Because in Waimiri Atroari, the leftmost edge of the word can have an onsetless
syllable, we would assume that the reduplicant shape in Tableau 6 could have an
onsetless structure, inasmuch as it was aligned to the left edge. However, in this
language, RED behaves differently: it must have an onset. To account for the emergence
of this unmarked syllable, I ranked the MAX-BR constraint below the markedness
constraint, as suggested by McCarthy and Prince (1994). Candidate (c) has an onset but
constraint MAX-BR and does not violate the undominated faithfulness constraints,
Tableau 7
b) ow-woo.sa *! *
SSlSiJIIIMiSiliiS
c) kwoo-woo.sa *!
WMMSSiMMUM
d) woo-woo.sa
alignment constraint and the MAX-BR constraint, established to account for the
reduplicant case. Candidate (b) cannot compete either because it violates the markedness
constraint, coda condition. Candidate (d) violates one of the undominated constraints,
DEP-IO. As a result, candidate (d) is the optimal candidate because it does not violate the
faithfulness constraints, and obeys the emergence of the unmarked structure, where the
delete (deletion is not permitted) or insert (epenthesis is not allowed either) segments to
avoid an ill-formed syllable. Therefore, I assume that faithfulness constraints prevent any
attempt to create 'perfect syllables.' Therefore, MAX-IO and DEP-IO are ranked above
markedness constraints. However, this is not true in the case of the reduplicant structure
2.6.2 Reduplication
reduplicant must be bimoraic. It copies the first two moras of the left edge of the verb
stem and includes the prefix (marking person) if the verb stem starts with a vowel. This
inclusion of the prefix is not relevant for the bimoraic pattern inasmuch as the simple
consonantal onsets are prosodically irrelevant. In no case was the temporal suffix taken
into account. Therefore, I consider the base only the verb stem as demonstrated in
(48) (49)
Meira (1999:98), which lengthens the vowel to substitute for the coda, Waimiri Atroari
does not fail to include the coda of the first syllable in the reduplicated segment.
The coda is counted as the second mora; therefore, we must assume that the
number of moras determines the shape of the reduplicant. This modification has not only
made the analysis more elegant, but it has also unified the pattern.
Therefore, when the verb stem begins with a vowel, the prefix marking person has to
count as an onset. For example, in the verb stem -aryma-, if we have an input as n- aryma
-pa, it is not possible to reduplicate it as *ary-narymapa 'They come back many times or
56
repeatedly.' The prefix must become part of the reduplicant; as a result, the reduplication
occurs as narv-narymapa.
(52)
Problem: Onsetless RED Solution: Prefix as part of RED
*ary-narymapa nary-narymapa
However, this example creates two problems. The first one is related to the
alignment of the reduplicant. In Waimiri Atroari, RED has to be aligned to the left edge
of the verb stem n-aryma-pa; however, in this case RED was aligned before the prefix n-
pa. The second problem is related to the need for an onset. Why does the prefix need to
serve as the onset? Mester (1988:202) observed that in Chumash, a Southern California
language, if the stem starts with a vowel, the reduplicant would include any consonant
immediately preceding the stem because the RED would mainly have the invariant CVC
However, this is not the case in Waimiri Atroari because the only condition required for
As a result, the only coherent explanation for this case must be based on the
emergence of the unmarked approach (McCarthy & Prince 1994). This approach claims
preferred more over marked elements (Archangeli 2000; Kager 1999). Although Waimiri
Atroari allows onsetless syllables, I must assume that the structure of the reduplicant is
unmarked relative to the language as a whole, so it must have an onset. However, this
57
only happens in the cases of onsetless stems because this does not work for such marked
"the reduplicant copies the base." However, they claim that perfect identity cannot
correspondence, which is a relation between two structures, such as base reduplicant (B-
R) or input and output (I-O). Correspondence theory assumes that given two strings Si
Waimiri Atroari, I claimed that the weight of the syllable determined the shape of the
reduplicant. As a result, Waimiri Atroari would have two patterns of reduplication (one
(53) When the first two syllables are light, the reduplicant copies the two initial
syllables of the verb stem CVCV (disyllabic reduplicant) and prefixes this material to the
base.
a)/iakyby-ia/ iaky-iakvby-ia 'Make porridge many times.'
cv.cv.cv-cv cv.cv
b) /myryka-ky/ myry-myryka-kv 'Mix well many times.'
cv.cv.cv-cv cv.cy
c) /n-aryme-pa/ nary-naryme-pa 'They came back many times.'
cv.cv.cv-cv cv.cv
(54) When the first syllable is heavy, it copies the initial heavy syllable sequence of
the verb stem (CVC CVV, VV, and CCV) and prefixes this material to the base.
58
However, the problem with this analysis was that I needed to classify the CCV
according to the heavy pattern. According to Hayes (1989, 1995), the distinction between
light and heavy syllables is based on moraic structure. He claims that a heavy syllable
contains two moras; however, a light syllable contains only one mora. On the other hand,
the moraic structure of languages can vary (McCarthy & Prince 1986). In the
phonological inventory of Waimiri Atroari, I classify the palatal glide phoneme /j/ as an
porridge many times' and (54c) iaa-iaa-pa 'Took many times,' if the phoneme /j/ is not
taken as a consonant, I would have a trimoraic RED. Therefore, to solve this problem I
have decided to factor the complex onset into the mora calculation. However, as this is
the only example that I have at this moment, I consider this aspect a remaining issue to be
59
reduplicant is a prefixing process and it normally preserves the linear order of elements of
the base, and the copied segments in the base and reduplicant are identical in features.
(3) Align RED L, Stem, L - The reduplicant has to be aligned with the left edge
of the stem.
(c) MAX B-R - Every element of the base has a correspondent in the reduplicant.
(d) IDENT B-R - Reduplicant correspondent of a base [aF] segment is also [aF].
the emergence of the unmarked: 1) the onset is required for the reduplicant, but not for
' Originally, Keiichiro Suzuki and Tania Granadillo suggested explaining my data based on the notion of
the foot; however, after reviewing my fieldwork notes and my first analysis, I decided to use moras.
60
Tableau 8
**
b. iia2k3e4R-iia2k3y4b5y6B -ia *!
* *
C.iia2k3b5y4 R.iia2k3y4b5y6 b -ia *!
** * ****
d. iia2k3k4y5R-iia2k3y4b5yB -ia *!
****
e. iia2k3y4 R-iia2k3y4b5y6B -ia
In tableau 8, the worst candidates are candidates (a), (c), and (d) because they
process. Candidate (c) is ruled out because it not only violates the undominated
Although candidate (b) could possibly be a well-formed candidate because it does not
violate the RED=Bimoraic constraint, it is worse than candidate (e) because it violates the
constraint IDENT B-R (F). Therefore, the optimal candidate is candidate (e) because it
only violates the constraint MAX B-R. Inasmuch as Waimiri Atroari has partial
reduplication, the constraint MAX B-R will always be violated. As result, the ranking of
Tableau 9
Input: RED-t-myryka-ky RED=BI IDENT B-R(F) DEP B-R MAX B-R
mix-IMP MORAIC
'Mix well many times'
***
a. myrka-myryka b -ky *!
b. myre-myryka b -ky *! **
He*
c.myry-myrykaB -ky
d. myryy-myryka B-ky *! *! *
does not violate the RED=Bimoraic constraint; therefore, candidate (c) wins. Candidate
(b) could be the winning candidate; however, it violates the IDENT B-R constraint and as
observed in Tableau 8, the change of features between the reduplicant and the base is not
allowed in the reduplication process. Candidate (a) is bad because it violates the
undominated constraint. Candidate (d) not only violates RED=Bimoraic, it also violates
Tableau 10
b. iae-iaas-pa *!
*c.iaa-iaa b -pa
In Tableau 10, the whole verb stem is copied; therefore, MAX B-R is not violated
for any of the three candidates. Candidate (a) is the worst candidate because it violates
not only the Bimoraic constraint, but also the DEP B-R constraint. Candidate (b) could
62
have been considered as an optimal candidate; however, it violates the IDENT B-R
Tableau 11
b. nu-win-wenta b -pa **
*!
c. nu-weny-wenta b -pa *
**
d. nu-wen-wenta b -pa
e. wen-nu-wentaB -pa **
In Tableau 11, candidate (a) is the worst candidate because it violates not only the
well-formedness constraint RED=Bimoraic, but also the B-R constraint DEP B-R.
Candidates (b) and (c) do not violate the undominated constraint, but they do violate two
important B-R constraints, IDENT B-R, and DEP B-R. Candidate (e) does not violate the
undominated constraint; however, it does violate the Align RED, L, Stem, L constraint.
Tableau 12
He**
b. ny-tah-tahkwa B -ky
d. ny-taah-tahkwa b -ky *! * **
Although candidate (a) in Tableau 12 has fewer violations than candidates (c), (d),
and (e), it cannot be an optimal candidate because it does violate the Bimoraic condition
established for the RED structure. Candidate (c) does not violate Red=Bimoraic, but it
does violate IDENT B-R (F). Candidates (d) and (e) are completely out because they not
only violate the undominated constraint, they also violate the DEP B-R constraint.
However, in this case candidate (e) is the worst candidate because it violates the
alignment constraint, as well. Candidate (b) is the optimal candidate because it does not
Tableau 13
b. ny-kyn-kynky b -pia **
c. ny-ken-kynky b -pia *! **
Although candidate (d) does not violate the bimoraic condition in Tableau 13, it
cannot be the optimal candidate because it violates the alignment constraint in which the
reduplicant has to be aligned with the left edge of the verb stem. Candidate (a) violates
the undominated constraint and candidate (c) violates the IDENT B-R constraint. As a
result, candidate (b) is the optimal candidate because it only violates MAX B-R.
Tableau 14
a. ary-naryme b -pa *! * **
b. na-naryme b -pa *!
v. * ****
*
c. nyry'-naryme b -pa *! *
i , ,1 **
d. nary-naryme b -pa
Atroari allows syllables without onsets, the structure of the reduplicant must be
65
unmarked: it has to have an onset. As a result, when the verb stem starts with a vowel, the
prefix is copied, as well. Consequently, candidate (a) is not an optimal candidate because
it violates the well-formedness constraint (Kager, 1999) and the emergence of the
unmarked form required for the RED. Therefore, candidate (d) is the optimal candidate.
(c) is the worst candidate because it violates three important constraints. In this tableau,
Tableau 15
a. oo-w-oo B -se *! *
b. woo'-w-oo B - se *! *
c. oa-w-oo B -se *! * *
* **
d. WOO -W-OOB -se
undominated; consequently candidates (a) and (c) are completely out of the competition
because they do not obey the requirement. Candidate (b) cannot be the optimal candidate
because it violates the RED=Bimoraic constraint. Although candidate (d) violates the
alignment constraint, it is the optimal candidate because it does not violate the
The template is not just segmental (or skeletal as assumed by Marantz 1982, Mester
1988), but it is a prosodic entity as demonstrated in the Waimiri Atraori case. This
analysis shows that the well-formedness constraints are above the Base-Reduplicant
DEP B-RMAX B-R). The superiority of the well-formedness constraints reinforces the
analysis that Waimiri Atroari has a typical case of emergence of the unmarked structure
when selecting the shape of the reduplicant; consequently, RED must have an onset.
According to the data collected thus far, reduplication examples with V and
CVCVC verb stem have not yet been found. However, it can be predicted that in a verb
with V stem shape, the reduplicant would copy the prefix to avoid the onset constraint
and lengthen the vowel to satisfy the bimoraic condition (as explained for Tiriyo in
section 2.5.2). For example, n-y-sa, 'he goes' would reduplicate as nyy-ny-sa, obeying
the bimoraic condition. On the other hand, in the case of a CVCVC verb stem, the coda
of the second syllable would be deleted to avoid violating the RED-bimoraic constraint.
In this case, one more violation would account for MAX B-R, without breaking the
semantic, morphological, and syntactic criteria. According to Meira (2003), few studies
of the Carib languages discuss and define criteria for word class (Hoff 1968 on Carib;
of the criteria that were used to define the parts of speech in Waimiri Atroari and to
describe each lexical class. Section 3.2 analyzes case marking in this language.
Waimiri Atroari has a complex and rich morphology typical of the Carib family.
It presents both prefixes and suffixes. The prefixes mark person and the suffixes mark
morphological and syntactic grounds, Waimiri Atroari stems can be divided into six
lexical classes: (1) nouns including pronouns; (2) adjectives; (3) verbs; (4) adverbs; (5)
As already noted for other Carib languages, in Waimiri Atroari the nouns and the
verbs are unproblematic and are easy to identify (see Table 3.1 below). However, the
remaining classes, such as adjectives, adverbs, and postpositions, are much more difficult
to characterize.
68
Nouns Verbs
Can take inflectional affixes Can take a large set of inflectional
marking person (possession); affixes, such as prefixes marking
Can take derivational affixes such person (A, S, 0), and suffixes
as verbalizers and adverbalizers; marking tense, aspect, mood;
Occur as the head of a simple Noun Can take a set of specific
Phrase; derivational affixes used to form
Occur together with a transitive nouns {-typy) and adverbs {-esa,
verb to form a VP; -pesa)-.
Can occupy core argument position
in a clause, such as A, S, 0.
3.1.1 Nouns
The classic definition of nouns argues that nouns are said to represent 'persons,
places, and things;' that is, nouns give names to, or identify, objects. In Waimiri Atroari,
the nouns take inflectional affixes to indicate possession. Nouns also take derivational
affixes like the verbalizer -ta, the absentive -my, the devaluative -erne, and the valuative
-e'me (this will be discussed below). The nouns in this language do not inflect for
different words for men and women, through male and female speech (principally on
kinship terms).
occur as the head of a simple noun phrase. Together with transitive verbs, nouns form
verb phrases (see discussion in chapter 4). The noun can be formed by one root or by two
(1)
3.1.1.1 Possession
Certain nouns, such as body parts and kinship terms, must have a possessor. There are
some nouns which are never possessed and so never take person markers (See Table 3.4
below), such as nature elements (sun, moon, star). Interestingly, some nouns that could be
possessed, for example, dog, bird, banana, or fish, are possessed with a generic
expression for pet ieky and food wyty. For example, aa-ieky can mean 'my dog'. I also
can say aa-iehy naminja 'my pet dog'. Possessor-possessed order is obligatory, and the
In Waimiri Atroari, a number of vowel-initial noun and verb stems take a 'linking
prefix' i- when immediately preceded by their determiners (that is, the possessor, with
nouns, and the object, with transitive verbs). Besides its occurrence with transitive verb
stems, such as akyna 'to sweep' (11), this prefix generally occurs with obligatorily
possessed nouns (body-part and kinship terms, etc.), such as eba 'eye' (12):
72
On the other hand, consonant-initial stems, such as pana 'ear' and xiky 'to cut'
Similar morphological devices are also found in other Carib languages, such as
Atroari, the prefix i- can also co-occur with 2"'' and 1' plural inclusive possessors:
relational prefix. Although rarely described as such for Carib languages (where they
have been traditionally analyzed as 3'^'' person markers), relational prefixes are very
common in languages of the Tupi and Macro-Je stocks, a fact that has been pointed out as
evidence for the genetic relationship between Carib and those two language groupings
(Rodrigues 1994).
73
Besides providing further evidence for the existence of relational prefixes in the
Carib family, the consequences of such an analysis muddies the question of how to
distinguish pronominal prefixes from clitics in Waimiri Atroari (and maybe other Carib
languages as well). The hypothesis to be investigated in future work is that both noun
and verb stems in Waimiri Atroari present only one slot for prefixation. This slot can be
occupied either by a personal prefix or by the relational prefix i-. Since personal prefixes
and the relational prefix cannot co-occur, the presence or absence of a relational prefix
morpheme preceding a noun or a verb stem is a prefix or a clitic. This distinction would
explain the differences in morphological behavior between the 1' person plural inclusive
morpheme k(y)- (15a, 16a) and the 3'^'' person morpheme kyy= (15b, 16b), for example.
only the elements here analyzed as clitics seem to have argument status when attached to
The nouns also occur with 3 different classes of suffixes: the verbalizers -to, a
suffix that indicates absentive -my, and a group of suffixes that I called valuative -e 'me
Nouns like wena 'vomit' and myny 'blood' appear suffixed with -ta form verbs;
In Waimiri Atroari, some nouns can take a suffix that indicates absence,
Interestingly, I tried to put this suffix in words like xiwia 'beautiful' to see if we
could have xiwia-my 'without beauty' or tykomia 'cold' then tykomia-my 'without cold',
and etypa 'sick', then etypa-my 'without sickness'. But they are not allowed. This could
be because those words can take the suffix -pa 'emphatic' that only adjectives can take.
take these two different morphemes. The devaluative -eme can indicate that a person or
an animal is dead or sick, and that an object is no good anymore, old, or no longer usable.
On the other hand, the valuative -e 'me, when used for persons and animals, indicates that
they are alive; when used with objects, it indicates that they are still good to use.
75
These morphemes can occtar with subjects of intransitive and transitive verbs,
example (22), and objects of transitive verbs (23). I haven't seen a situation in which both
of them can occur in the same sentence. This may be because only one is necessary
inasmuch as they are not required to disambiguate the sentence. Moreover, -e 'me and -
erne are not obligatory, and the valuative occurs less often than the devaluative.
In this sentence, the valuative morpheme is marked in the word for clay, to indicate that this is a good
clay to make a ceramic pan.
76
3.1.1.3 Pronouns
characteristics used to describe them are the existence of animacy and deictic distinction.
In Waimiri Atroari, pronouns can occur in subject and object position, and like nouns,
they can also take the valuative and devaluative morphemes. The Waimiri Atroari
Waimiri Atroari has the typical Carib system for pronouns: first person (1),
second person (2), first person dual inclusive (1+2), first person exclusive (1+3), and the
third person pronouns, including the demonstratives (proximal, medial, and distal). The
third-person pronouns in Waimiri Atroari are sensitive to aspects such as visibility and
proximity. The terms proximal, medial, and distal are used to label the demonstrative
categories representing the degree of proximity (closeness) to the reference point, which
3rd
Personal pronouns identify people and things on the basis of their relationship to
the discourse in which they are referred to, such as their conversational role. For
example, the pronouns awy, kara, aa T are used by anyone who takes the role of speaker
in a discourse. Note that the first-person pronoun has three different forms. The form
kara T', with its alternate kra, occurs when somebody is answering a question or
emphasizing that he did the action, or that he wants something. It is the only pronoun that
can occur in OSV order, separating the verb phrase (see discussion on word order in
chapter 4). The forms aa and awy T are used without any specific distinction (see
below).
Observe that the free independent pronouns aa '1 pro' and a'a '1+3 pro' are
identical to the person prefixes for possessed nominals. The 1+3 possessive mark loses
As illustrated in Table 3.6 above, the other non-third-person pronouns do not have
different forms from the first person singular. The pronoun amyry (a) 'you' is used to
Atroari makes a distinction between inclusive and exclusive. First-person plural inclusive
kyky includes the person addressed, meaning something like 'weyou and I'. The first
person plural exclusive a'a excludes the person or persons addressed, meaning 'we, that
is, I and others, but not you.' The examples below illustrate sentences with second-
In Waimiri Atroari, as for other languages in the Carib family, the third-person
pronouns are sensitive to features such of proximity, visibility, and animacy. The
anaphoric class has three kinds of third-person pronouns; mykyky 'he, she, it, they, who
are close, proximal attending the conversation;' mykyka'a, ka 'he, she, it, they, who are
not close, but whom we can see, distal;' and iry 'he, she, they, it, who are not close or
Both mykyky and ka can occur in the subject and object position, but in my data
ka seems to be preferred when occupying an object position. On the other hand, I did not
The demonstrative or deictic forms are marked for proximity and animacy. In the
proximal group, the (h)anji, kanji, anjinji forms mean 'this, these,' but in some situations,
they can be translated as 'here.' In this group, only byby, by is used for animate objects.
It means 'this,' but in some examples it means 'he/she' (see examples below).
The medial form myry also means 'this,' but like the proximal forms anji, kanji it
Examples (65), (66), and (67) illustrate examples with animate entities; therefore,
the pronoun byby/by is selected. Unlike other Carib languages, which have an elaborate
In the distal group, the forms mo 'o and mymo' mean 'that, those, there.' They are
used with inanimate objects. The other distal form, myky, also means 'that,' but it only
An interesting aspect related to the form mo 'o is that when translated as 'there,' it
seems that in Waimiri Atroari there are two kinds of 'there.' The first is a 'there' that
gives an idea of a permanent state or remaining there; in this context the morpheme ipy is
utilized. The second 'there' has a temporary notion; in this case the form mo 'o is used
3.1.2 Verbs
Verbs are words which signify actions, events, or temporary states in relation to
beings and things in the world. They are the core, the binding element in most sentences
in any language. Verbs can serve as head of verb phrase, predicates of clauses, and they
code events in text. In this section, I discuss primarily the word-structure (morphology)
of verbs. I discuss the various grammatical affixes (prefixes and suffixes) that can be
The structure of the verb in Waimiri Atroari is basically prefix-stem-suffix. Of all lexical
classes, the verb is the richest in morphological possibilities. It can take a large set of
Table 3.8 below illustrates the morphological possibilities with some examples of
Vomit See
1. -wen-ta 1. -ini-
vomit-VERBL see
2. hu-wen-ta-pa 2. h-ini-pia
1S-VOmit-VERBL-REM.P lA-see-lM.P
'I vomited' 'I saw'
3. wen-ta-ha 3. ni-huwa
vomit-VERBL-NEG see-NEG
'not vomit' 'not see'
4. wen-ta-ky 4. ni-ky
vomit-VERBL-iMP see-iMP
'Vomit!' 'Look!'
5. hu-wen-tah-py-pia 5. h-ini-py-pia
1A-vomit-VERBL-CAUS-IM.P lA-see-CAUS-iM.p
'I made him vomit.' 'I made him see.'
6. aa=wen-tah-py-pia 6. aa=ini-py-pia
10-VOmit-VERBL-CAUS-IM.P 10-see-CAUS-iM.P
'He made me vomit.' 'He made me see.'
Verb stems in this language can have different shapes, including V, VV, VC,
VCV, CV, CVC, and CVCVCCV. Tense/aspect markers distinguish non-past, remote,
recent, and imminent past, and future; and a modal suffix marks the manner of the action.
There are three subclasses of verbs in Waimiri Atroari: transitive, intransitive, and
copular. The transitive verbs are traditionally defined as the group of verbs which
subcategorize for a direct object. Transitive verb stems can take both A and O prefixes
(see Table 3.10 below). The intransitive verbs are often referred to as the group of verbs
which do not subcategorize for a direct object. Here, I use the term more narrowly, to
mean verbs that have only a subject, not an object. Intransitive verbs can only take one
set of person markers. Intransitive verbs change valence when taking causative suffixes;
as a result, they become transitive verbs taking direct objects. Transitive verb stems make
implicit reference to two participants, A and O, while intransitive verbs denote only one
participant, S.
The forms w-, m-, n-, and h- occurs with verbal stems beginning with vowels, wy-
, my-, ny, hy- are used with verbal stems beginning with consonants. The forms, wu-, mu-,
nu, and hu- only appear with verbal stems beginning with bilabials.^' Paradigms of
transitive and intransitive verb forms with the prefixes marking person follow (more
Transitive Intransitive
The most important and indispensable elements attached to the verbal stem are the
tense-aspect suffixes. They always appear finally in the verb construction. They serve to
indicate a wide range of functions and meanings around the time value of the
considered remote. However, in Waimiri Atroari, it is possible to make events in the past
seem more immediate or recent; therefore, in this language there are suffixes that indicate
remote past -pa, recent past -piany (events that could be occurred in the same day,but
also in the day before), immediate past -pia (an event that just happened), and a non-
89
specified past -ky. The suffix -pa is most coraumonly used in narratives and in stories
about remote events in the past, things that are finished and done with.
Formally, the future means talking about an event/action/state that has not yet
begun, but is said to begin sometime in the future. The Waimiri Atroari language divides
the future into imminent -teZ-txe and remote -tapeZ-txape. In this grammatical sketch, the
suffixes -e, -ia, -sa, -sapa, -pysa (this last suffix indicates iterative) are classified as
tense/aspect markers that denote a non-past notion. The difference among them is not
clear. It requires futher research. Waimiri Atroari has other suffixes that denote aspects,
but they will be not discussed in this grammatical sketch. Paradigms of intransitive and
wy-synehka-pa m-ini-Jja
IS-disappear-REM.P 2A-see-rem.p
'I disappeared.' 'You saw it.'
wy-sjoiehky-piany m-ini-piany
IS-disappear-REC.P 2A-see-REC.P
'I disappeared.' 'You saw it.'
wy-synehky-pia m-ini-pia
1S-disappear-IM.P 2A-see-IM.P
'I disappeared.' 'You saw it.'
Apparently, there are no distinction among them, examples (a), (b), and (c) above
(c) aa h-eni-te
Ipro lA-see-iMM.F
'I will see it.'
3.1.2.2 Mood
Speech aspects in human languages may be used for many purposes, but the three
main ones seem to be: to give information (declarative sentences), to ask for information
3.1.2.2.1 Imperatives
The purpose of the imperative sentence/clause form is for the speaker to get the
hearer to act. This can be done in several ways, such as subtle and indirect, and direct.
Here, I discuss just the direct form. There are three types of imperative suffixes in
91
Waimiri Atroari: -ky, -hne, -kwapy. The suffixes -hne and -kwapy have a more restricted
distribution; they occur principally with the verbs speak, hear, and look. However, they
(84) wen-ta-hne
Vomit-VERBL-IMP
'vomit!'
(86) kyta-ky
yell-iMP
'Yell!'
(87) ee-ky
drink-iMP
'Drink!'
In the negative imperative form, the form used to command people not to do
something, which involves the negative suffix, the verb remains in its bare form, but now
In Waimiri Atroari, two negation suffixes occur with the verb form: -huwa, and
-ha (V-stem/_a). They are always positioned after the verb root.
As shown in table 3.7 in the template for verbs, the interrogative clitic is attached
Questions are a request for information, when the speaker misses some
information, and thinks that the hearer knows it. There are two general types of
93
information questions: (a) yes/no questions in which the speaker wants the hearer to say
whether it is true (yes) or false (no); and (b) Wh- questions in which the speaker knows
most of the information, but is missing one elementas a result, the speaker asks the
hearer to identify that missing element, for example the subject, direct or indirect object,
Interrogative forms are used when asking questions regarding who, what, whose,
how, where. In Waimiri Atroari there are two forms bypa 'who, whose' and apia 'what'.
The first form is used with animate entities and the second form is used with inanimate
entities.
In Waimiri Atroari, even with a Wh- word occurring in the first position, we have
the interrogative clitic =e' in the last word of the sentence. However, sometimes the
interrogative clitic does not appear. In the examples below, I illustrate where the
The cases where the interrogative clitic sometimes does not appear seem to be
related to questions of the Yes/No type. However, that optionality does not occur with
Wh- questions.
Interrogative clitics attach to verbs
(110) Temeh-eme m-itxiky-pian=e'
Jaguar-DEV 2A-shoot-REC.P-RNT
'Did you shoot the jaguar with an arrow?'
(111) Epe ka my-pyny-p=e'
where ? 2A-take-REM.P-INT
'Where did you take it?'
(121) M-yty-pi=e'
2s-understand-IM.P-INT
'Did you understand?'
96
To assure that this is a clitic, not just a verb suffix, I demonstrate that it can occurs
with all the word classes in this language. In verbs, the last vowel of the suffix that marks
tense/aspect is replaced by the interrogative clitic: for example, -piany (recent past)
becomes -pian^^e', -pa (remote past) becomes -p=e', -pia (immediate past) becomes -
added to the word karyka 'chicken,' the last vowel of the word is not deleted.
On the other hand, examples (123) and (124) show the two possibilities. In (123),
the last vowel of the word naminja 'dog' is deleted, but in (124), the interrogative suffix
Examples (135) to (138) show the flexibility of the interrogative suffix with
Yes/No questions. As you can see above in examples (110) to (121), the clitic occurs in
the verb. However, it can occur in the last word of the sentence, such as nouns and
pronouns (examples 122-128), adverbs (example 129), adjectives (130 and 131),
locatives (example 132), and copulas (examples 133-138). Phonologically, sometimes the
last vowel of the verb or noun is replaced by the clitic =e' (examples 110-121) and 124);
on the other hand, note that sometimes there is no replacement of the last vowel. In that
case, the clitic =e' is added at the end of word (examples 124-126^ and 129. The second
Observe that in this kind of sentence, the interrogative clitic could occur in
different situations. For example, in (139), it occurred in the first particle that marks
disjunction, but in the example (140), it occurred in the verb; however in the last example
(141), the interrogative clitic occurred not only in the first particle, but also it appeared in
the last disjunctive particle. This shows that the interrogative clitic always appears as the
last element of the sentence. Previously, I had considered the interrogative clitic to be a
suffix. However, two factors, distribution (it can occur in different parts of speech) and
Atroari seems to behave as a 'special clitic', which is located with a phrase that
constitutes its scope, and it may appear at least initially, finally, or post-finally. In the
case of the Waimiri Atroari language, the interrogative clitic occurs in the final position.
Anderson says that clitics appear to be somewhat freer than affixes. They are much less
there is one in which causativization is morphologically marked with the suffix -py,
generally with the 'made' reading, having the semantic meaning 'make somebody do
something' or 'cause something without resistance.' The other kind has the 'let' reading,
In this type of construction, Waimiri Atroari allows two possible structures: one
structure in which the causative morpheme appears on the lexicalized made/cause verb,
as observed in examples (142) to (145); and the other structure in which this lexicalized
(149) Ka k-yeepitxah-py-pia.
3PRO l+2o-laugh-CAUS-IM.p
'She/he made us laugh.'
101
In the cases described above, we can note that when an intransitive verb, such as
shout, rest, wake up, laugh, jump, throw up, and bleed, takes the causative, it changes its
valence, behaving as a transitive verb with the following structure: V[Intr +Caus [A O]].
Moreover, it was shown in example (155) that optionally when we have a CAUSEE, it is
kind of control structure: Kaina can control the subject PRO of the complement. As a
102
result, the old subject Kaina becomes an indirect object. However, as in Japanese, in
Waimiri Atroari the causers (as initiators of the events) are generated in the SPEC of the
event Phrase (Harley 1995). To illustrate this assumption, I decided to use the tree Event
(157)
a) EyentP b) EyentP
NP Event' Aa Event'
A A
Cause EventP Kaina EventP
NP V Kyrywu inise
In this structure, Aa is the CAUSER, having the highest position in the tree, or
being generated in the first Event Phrase; Kaina is the CAUSEE; the lexicalized
cause/made verb was put in the second Event Phrase; and finally the VP kiriwu inise is
One of the differences between the 'make' and 'let' causative is that in the 'let'
causative construction people are not forced to do something; as a result, this does not
particle tre 'me that is used when we permit or order somebody to do something, but we
do not know if the person will do it. Levin (2000) argues that "causative and
true that the person will learn how to make a ceramic pan or if in (159), the person will
leave to hunt.
The other difference between the two types of constructions is that in the 'let'
reading construction, we do not have the causative morpheme -pi. On the other hand, I
cannot assume that the particle tre 'me is particular to the 'let' reading causative because
In this sentence, as in the sentences in (158) and (159), it is not necessarily true
In Waimiri Atroari, constructions that denote desire are marked with the suffixes
intransitive verb stems, resulting in a noun. This noun denotes an instrument used for the
3.1.3 Adjectives
states that are relatively stable over time. In the Waimiri Atroari language, adjectives are
free morphemes formed by only one root. They usually precede the noun. They are the
only word class that can take the suffix -pa 'emphatic.'
Adjectives in this language can occur in subject and object position, as arguments.
English or Portuguese. As is evident in the list below, in Waimiri Atroari some adjectives
Abemyhpa puffy
Aberymyhy-abemyhy round
Anykyxi thin
Ase new
Awinihe-awinini, awinjehe, awenjahky one, alone
Ba'xiri-be'xiri small
Bahinja-baxinja small, little, child
Bakyma sour, salt
Byryryma twisted
Djapyma straight, correct
Etypa hot
Inama weak
Kareme, karany good, beautiful
Kyby short for animals and plants
Kybyma bitter
Kykyryhpa burned
Mady-madyma cripple
Maiwu~maiy fat, thick
Masara lazy, apprentice
Mixopy long, lengthy
Nerimy brave, courageous
Nyryny alive
Panapy hardworking
Panaxi headstrong
Pine short
Pitymy single
Sakra white
Sakyna angry
Saweny-sewuna light
Sehe tall
Ta'kwa short for person
Taha big, large, leader
Tamxa soft, lazy
Tapany~typany~tipyna hard
Tapyryma black, dark
Temyna dry
Texiba sad
Teximy bad, not good taste
Tirika strong, potent
Trewine~trewuna agile, fast
Tuwaka, happy, content
Tuwera ripe
Txamyry old, elder
Txanpa pregnant
Txika sharp
Txipikia ashamed
Txitymy single
Tybyska smooth
Tykomia cold
Tykoxinja dirty
Typyra stink, bad smell
107
The following sentence examples illustrate the difference in meaning for the
This symbolis indication of free variation; we can fmd both forms. It is not necessarily related to
dialectal difference.
108
Moreover, the words bahinja and txamyry can take the suffix -pesa esa that
means 'in time.' Two examples are bahinja-pesa 'in time of child-childhood' and
txamyry-pesa 'in time of elders, old age' (see examples 182 and 183 below). However,
the words taha, awinini, and xiwia cannot take this suffix. Therefore, I cannot say *xiwia-
provided in the list, for example, the word tamxa 'soft, lazy'. Originally, this word only
meant soft. However, it seems that because of non-native influence, this word has
acquired a new meaning, 'lazy'. Today, the Waimiri Atroari use the word tamxa to
indicate a person who is not hardworking and is lazy. Not only does it mean a quality of
material, but it is now also a characteristic of a person. Finally, the other word in the list
that has a very curious semantic feature is the word xiwia. This word can mean good,
beautiful, yellow, or red (warm colors). Interestingly, the Waimiri Atroari people love
anything yellow and red. Therefore, it makes sense that xiwia also means beautiful.
3.1.4 Adverbs
adjectives, and clauses, typically expressing notions of time, location, maimer, degree,
and circumstances. Adverbs do not inflect for person or tense/aspect. They cannot be the
possessor nor the possessed in possessive constructions. They cannot occupy subject or
object positions. Another characteristic that makes adverbs different from other word
classes is its mobility in the clause. Syntactically, adverbs function as adjxmcts in any
type of clause.
Waimiri Atroari has many words fitting into this category. For example, time
adverbs are most commonly independent words referring to the time when the events/
actions occurred: araky 'today, now,' pana 'yesterday, long ago,' aminjaky 'tomorrow,'
110
kyky 'at night,' and kokyny 'early.' Location adverbs illustrate where the action/event
takes place: mie 'far away,' kypy 'near, close,' ipyna 'there,' iky 'under.' A few adverbs,
such kokyny 'early' and waha 'many,' can take the emphatic suffix -pa that occurs in
adjectives. For example, it is possible to say kokyny-pa 'very early.' The following
baka-paiky
kill-T/A
'Many elders climbed in the kamakaxi trees in order to kill the parakeets.'
3.1.5 Postpositions
phrases. They also appear as part of PPs as adjuncts in non-copular clauses. Unlike other
languages of the family, such as Tiriyo, Macuxi, and Carib, in Waimiri Atroari, except
Ill
for the dative, the words classified as postpositions do not take person-marking prefixes.
Therefore, in this grammatical sketch I call the postpositions that do not take the person-
Allative.
(191) Locatives - The main use of this postposition is to indicate where the action/event
is taking place or to indicate the position (inside, on, above) of the event.
Observe that all the locatives begin with the phonemes /k/ or III. The locative ta
seems to be used only on surfaces or places that do not have water. On the other hand, the
locatives ka and kaka in my data-base only occur with situations that involve water.
(192) Directionals - The directional postpositions indicate the motion toward their
objects or indicate the goal. Here, I divided them into two types; the allative taka and the
ablative tany.
t-aryma-hkypa
3REFLX-come back-after that
'We will rest after we return from the party.'
(193) Dative - This postposition is used to indicate the indirect object or the
beneficiary. As mentioned at the beginning of this section, the dative postposition is the
only one that takes the same affixes that mark case and possession in Waimiri Atroari
3.1.6 Particles
Waimiri Atroari the particles can be divided into various subclasses. For example, the
second-position particle ram is defined by its position after the first syntactic constituent.
Subordinate particles such as impa 'then,' imka 'when, if,'and the lexical negative
particle constitutes by themselves utterance , for example, ie, iahe 'no,' wan 'no,' kap,
114
kapy 'neg,' ie'xeme 'all.' The agentivity particle that marks the most agentive person in
In this section, I illustrate examples of the evidential particle ka, the second-
position particle ram, the negation particle kap, kapy, and the conjunction particle many
This particle is used to demonstrate and express that the participants of the
event/actions have evidence for what they saying. They do not have any doubt about the
(195) iakere ka by
alligator EVID this
'This is an alligator.'
Like the ram particle, this particle only occurs after the first constituent of the
utterance. These are characteristics observed by Hoff (1985) for evidentials in the Carib
language.
115
In Waimiri Atroari, the particle ram can be utilized to identify constituents. Here,
in this grammatical sketch, I will not discuss this function; rather, I will illustrate its
occurrence. As you can see in the examples below, ram can appear after NP, VP, and PP
structures.
In this section, I discuss non-verbal negation. These are negation sentences that
are not marked in the verb, but rather marked by particles. In Waimiri Atroari, kap ~ kapy
~ kapa and \van ate the particles that indicate negation. These particles are usually used
to negate existence.
tyruwa many
ceramic pan too
'The tapir's foot touched the stone griddle and broke, the ceramic pan too
This section describes and analyzes case marking in Waimiri Atroari. This
analysis explores whether Waimiri Atroari has the typical inverse split-S system
117
background on the Waimiri Atroari case marking system. Section 3.2.2 provides a
morphosyntactic properties of the verbal system from a group of languages in the Cariban
family. Moreover, this section provides some examples of the ditransitive construction. In
section 3.2.3, a discussion about the agentivity particle ia is provided in order to verify if
3.2.1 Background
I am using Blake's definition of case (2000:12) in which case in its most central
manifestation is a system of marking dependent nouns for the type of relationship that
they bear to their head. However, it is not the only grammatical mechanism for marking
head-modifier relation. One important type of alternative is the principle of marking the
head rather than any dependent. As it is analyzed, this is the case of Waimiri Atroari. By
this 1 mean that in this language, nouns do not get any affixes that can be identified as
For Waimiri Atroari transitive verbs, both A and O marking prefixes occur (see
Table 1 in Chapter 1). Their distribution is conditioned by a person hierarchy that ranks
non-third persons (first person '1,' second person '2,' and first person dual '1+2') as
n-prefix or a zero-prefix (see discussion below). The higher person will be marked
When first person, second person, and first person plural inclusive act on the third
person, the subject prefixes will be chosen, agreeing with the first or second person.
However, when third person acts on first and second person, the object prefixes
will be selected, still agreeing with the first and second person.
independent clause verbal systems, such as Set I (inverse/split-S), Full Set II (ergative).
119
"the seven systems are identified by means of six distinct, but interrelated,
Gildea claims that the nominative languages have retained the original system and
main verbs.
such as personal prefix set and the collective number suffixes; word order is generally
nominative, in that the OV unit is clear; there are no auxiliaries and A and O nominals are
not case-marked. Gildea (1998:59) says that in all Set 1 systems for which we have
evidence, the A may occur either preceding or following the OV unit. As with Carib of
Surinam (Hoff 1978), in Waimiri Atroari (Bruno 2001) the more neutral order (in terms
the Set I system identify the subject of an intransitive verb (S) and both the subject (A)
and (O) of a transitive verb. In addition, this set has the inverse prefixes (all those in
which 3A acts on SAP-Speech Act Participants '1,' '2,' and '1+2' O) that are identical to
120
the subset of the intransitive subject prefixes that he calls So; the direct prefixes (those in
which SAP acts on 30) are most similar to the Sa subset of the intransitive prefixes.
Although I agree with Gildea's classification in many aspects and I can see that
Waimiri Atroari has some of the characteristics of the Set I system, I still have to verify
more accurately whether Waimiri Atroari follows this inverse split-S pattern. Van Valin
(1990), using the Role and Reference Grammar (RRG) fi"amework, proposed a semantic
analysis for the intransitive verb classes of Italian and Georgian, according to which
'activity' intransitive verbs belong to the Sa category while 'non-activity' ones are in the
So category. Meira (1995) observed that the intransitive verb case marking in some
Cariban languages seems to follow Van Valin's assumption, dividing the verb class into
ones that denote activities versus those which do not denote activities.
Particularly, I need to look with more detail into this possibility for Waimiri
Atroari. The data that I have do not clarify the distinction between intransitive verbs that
versus non-activity). As can be seen from the examples in table 3.12, the distinction
between verbs denoting activities versus non-activities does not seem to work for
Waimiri Atroari. For example, the first person in the paradigm with the verb 'shout' takes
the prefix fi-om the object set. Here, we have two problems: this is supposed to take an A
prefix instead of an O set because it is classified in the Sa set; the other problem is related
to the fact that we expected that the second person in this paradigm would take the prefix
from the O set, as well. Therefore, before I claim that Waimiri Atroari has the same
Inverse Split-S system demonstrated by Gildea (1998) for the Set I, I need to collect more
121
paradigms with intransitive verbs. On the other hand, according to Dixon (1979:83), there
are languages of the Split-S-marking where the two intransitive subclasses do not have as
good a semantic basis, such as Mandan and Guarani. This could be an explanation for the
situation in Waimiri Atroari. In this language, the tense/aspect device seems to not
Activities - Sa Non-activities - So
Go '-y-' Bleed'^ '-myn-ta'
w-y-sa 'I go' wu-myn-ta-pa 'I bled'
m-y-sa 'you go' mu-myn-ta-pa 'you bled'
n-y-sa 'she/he goes' nu-myn-ta-pa 'she/he bled'
h-y-sa 'we go (1+2)' hu-myn-ta-pa 'we bled (1+2)'
n-y-sa 'we go (1+3) nu-myn-ta-pa 'we bled (1+3)
/hu-. The first set is used with strictly intransitive verbs such as rymy 'die', y 'go', tahkwa
The word for blood is myny, in order to get the verb 'bleed,' we add the verbalizer -ta.
122
'jump', and mynta 'bleed'. The second set is used with transitive verbs with personal
objects such as ini 'see' and aape' 'push', and transitive verbs with impersonal objects
such as py 'know', ee 'drink', and wenta 'vomit'. (The Classification of certain verbs
such as mynta 'bleed' (intransitive) and wenta 'vomit' (transitive) is not explained.) This
distinction is not made for any other person and number combination.
In addition, at least one intransitive verb kyta 'shout' takes the first person
singular object clitic prefix aa=, for which I also have no explanation.
The examples below illustrate more clearly the hierarchy discussed above and
mark the subject's grammatical relation on the verb, but they can mark the object as well
in some languages. This happens in Waimiri Atroari when the objects are first and second
person.
123
We can observe that the full pronouns of first, second, plural inclusive and
exclusive do not occur when they behave as objects. Preferentially, the free
speaker/hearer pronouns are subjects only. This demonstrates that the first-person plural
inclusive and exclusive are also above the third person in the hierarchy.
124
When in Waimiri Atroari a third subject acts on a third object (3S30), it is not
totally clear whether the zero marks NOM or ACC; therefore, in the table of prefixes, I
hesitated to put a zero there. Usually, it is observed cross-linguistically that if the case-
marking paradigm includes a zero, it will normally be the NOM, but zero ACC is also
attested. In addition, if A aligns with S, then it will have to be O that is marked. Waimiri
closed system, with dative as the most highly marked case, accusative
They observed that is possible that semantic case and structural case compete with
each other, principally "if the number of arguments to be realized exceeds the number of
distinct structural cases. This happens with ditransitive verbs if the language has only one
object case available, and with causativized ditransitives in general because no languages
have three different structural object cases" (2001:379). As we can see in example (211),
part of their assumption seems to be confirmed, but here what is marked is the ACC.
125
t-aska i-epa-se.
SREFLX-relatives REL-invite-in order to
'He took the ceremonial calendar to his brother to invite his relatives.'
On the other hand, example (211) is problematic because the two arguments of
the verb 'tell' are not both NPs. It shows one NP and one clausal argument, and clause
does not show case. Unfortunately, I could not find more examples of ditransitive verbs
to see what happens in this case. Example (213) is interesting because as we are dealing
with 3 A and 30, it seems that it does not get any marking.
below, I confront a problem: that of assuming that the recipient must become the direct
object as in Lummi (Jelinek 1983). When the indirect object is a first, second, or first
plural exclusive, it is the dative that receives the prefixes instead of the verb. The verb in
this situation is marked with the relational prefix. In Waimiri Atroari, the dative is the
only postposition that gets the prefixes that mark person. This indicates a separate
*Observe that the dative can occur in the end of the sentence, but also in other
environments.
3A30 Mykyka xiba i-ry-pia mykyka inaka.
3PRO fish REL-give-iM.p 3PRO DAT
'She/he gave fish for him'
1+2A30 Kyka ram ka inaka xiba hv-rv-pia.
1+2PRO 2PART 3PRO DAT fish l+2A-give-LM.P
'We gave fish for him.'
or
Kyka xiba h^-ry-pia mykyka inaka.
1+2PRO fish l+2A-give-IM.P 3PRO DAT
2A10, the dative is marked with the object marker, but the verbs are marked with the
subject marker. Although we have observed these new cases, we can see that the
hierarchy still works. Moreover, the examples in Table 3.16 demonstrate that some
nominal expressions such as xiba 'fish' and the person who receives the fish do not need
to have a fixed order. The dative also does not have a fixed order; this made me rethink
the idea that the dative could be a core argument. Other aspects observed are that Waimiri
Atroari requires an external argument acting as subject; we cannot omit any of the
explains that there is a distinction between pronominal clitics and nominal expressions
where independent pronouns are included. According to her, nominal expressions can be
adjuncts do not need to have fixed order. I wonder whether this is true for Waimiri
Atroari inasmuch as we can note in the examples that the subject has a fixed order and
Interestingly, Waimiri Atroari has a particle ia that tells which argument is the
participant that performs the action in the sentence. In the examples below, we can
observe that this particle occurs in different kinds of sentences and it is not necessarily
related to the causative construction. It can appear when it is necessary to identify the
128
CAUSEE as the agent of the structure (see example 217), but it is not obligatory. As we
According to Gildea (1998) and Meira (1999), in the Carib family there is a
postposition ya (in the Proto-Carib w+ya) that marks different kinds of participants;
directionals, datives, causees, ergative, and agent markers. Meira (1999:512) explains
that the various kinds of participants that ya marks do seems to show some 'common
semantic threads'they are all human or sentient. In Tiriyo, the language described by
Meira, we can see in example (226) below that the distinction is not always clear.
organized, with the sole genitive relationship to the nominalized verb being claimed by
the verb's notional absolutive argument (S and O), and the notional ergative argument
(A) being therefore forced into oblique status." According to him, in the northern Full Set
II dominant languages, such as Makuxi and Kapon, the goal/dative function of this
morpheme has been lost, leaving only the ergative agent-marking fianction (see examples
morpheme ia does not mark datives and directionals. It clearly marks agents and causees.
In two clauses with two different actors, ia appears after the actor who denotes more
would prefer to explore and get more data before claiming that in this situation ia marks
ergativity.
Another interesting aspect of the particle ia that can be a topic for further research
is related to the notion of agency. According to Mithun (1991:516), the prototypical agent
is considered the "participant which performs, instigates, or controls the situation denoted
by the predicate," In this sense, this assumption seems to demonstrate that agentiveness
Atroari.
The notion of semantic agency is a complex one, as explored in Foley & Van
Valin (1984) and DeLancey (1985), among many others. Foley & Van Valin (1984:29)
characterize their general category 'actor' as "the participant which performs, effects,
participant which does not perform, initiate, or control any situation but rather is alfected
by it some way" (Foley & Van Valin 1984:29), features shared by prototypical patients.
participant who instigates and controls the action and another who performs the action.
The participant who performs or does the action will be followed by the ia particle.
Interestingly, the participants who will perform the action do not necessarily have
S/0 as ABS (Blake 2001). Although ergativity is not a unified phenomenon, I prefer to
do more research in order to claim that Waimiri Atroari shows any feature of ergativity.
In Waimiri Atroari, S and O share no formal features, as we would expect if they were
"absolutive" elements, so I do not treat them as the same. Moreover, even when this
Moreover, it is necessary to collect more data in order to verify whether Waimiri Atroari
follows the Inverse Split-S system observed by Gildea (1998). The selection of the
prefixes in intransitive verbs do not seem to be conditioned by tense or aspect where the
ergative is always found in either past tense or perfect tense (Dixon 1979:95). It also does
In the Waimiri Atroari language, first, second, and first plural inclusive and
exclusive person are ranked higher than third person. However, when second person acts
on first or first acts on second, it was observed that in some cases we have subject
132
agreement, while other cases show object agreement. Therefore, subject and object
marking must follow this hierarchy: l=2,l+2/l+3>3. Although we have observed that full
internal pronouns as object were not obligatory, this is not true for the external
arguments. A remaining issue that needs to be studied in more detail is the behavior of
the ditransitive verbs in order to better understand the role of the indirect object. Clearly,
This chapter is organized as follows. Section 4.1 describes and analyzes phrase
structure in Waimiri Atroari. Section 4.2 provides a discussion about Waimiri Atroari
clausal order. Section 4.3 analyzes the topicalization construction in this language, and
finally section 4.4 provides a brief analysis of two types of subordinated clauses.
In this grammatical sketch, I argue that Waimiri Atroari is a head-right (or head-
final) language. However, there are cases in which the head is apparently allowed to
have variable position (either left or right side). As I intend to show, all these examples
adverbial quantifiers, and numeral words. It has been observed that adjuncts are different
fi-om complements in that they have a higher degree of positional fi"eedom so that this
many, and two do not belong to the functional category of determiners. Therefore, the
similarity in distribution of the head among the phrases containing adjectives, adverbial
language.
As I have already mentioned, I claim that S(0)V is the basic word order, based on
kinds of order are also attested, such as (0)VS, SVO, and (0)SV. Again, the occurrence
of word orders like SVO seems to constitute a counterexample to the claim that Waimiri
Atroari is a head-right language. Although I will not be dealing with such examples in
this paper, I suggest that a possible explanation could be related to the influence of
Portuguese in the speech of the younger speakers. The OSV and OVS word orders will
be analyzed as being the result, respectively, of the movement of the object and the whole
VP to a topic position,
Here, I assume that phrases are built around an element whose head is instantiated
by a major lexical class, such as N, V, or A. Second, I assume that there are at most two
projections of each class, an intermediate projection X' and a maximal projection XP,
and we can add adjuncts at any level. Since X-Bar Theory allows "Parameters''^^ (Travis,
1989:264) on the position of heads, complements, and adjuncts, I use it to explain the
phrase structure in this language. According to Greenberg (1963), there is a general word
order tendency in natural languages that tends to place modifier elements either before or
after the head. On the other hand, it is observed that the position of heads and
complements in different kinds of phrases seems not to be limited to the binary choice
where all heads must be either left or right. In other words, there could be some 'mixed
In Waimiri Atroari, the head of the phrase occurs predominantly at the right edge
of the constituent in noun, verb, and postposition phrases. However, in the case of noun
According to Travis (1989;264), "[LJanguage variation is allowed through parameters which introduce a
limited flexibility to the system. Parameters represent the range of variation that can be found in natural
languages as well as what has to be learned by the children."
135
phrases containing a numeral, quantifier word, or adjective, this does not seem to always
be the case. As will be demonstrated below, the fact that the head can be positioned
either to the left or to the right in this kind of phrase is probably related to the fact that
numerals, adverbial quantifiers, and adjectives are adjuncts, and, as such, can occupy
variable positions.
In Waimiri Atroari, the simplest case of noun phrases can have a single noun (1)
or a pronoun (2). The clearest cases of NPs involving two nouns are examples of
Moreover, the examples involving noun phrases seem to have examples of both
adjunction and complements. In the trees in (7b) and (8b), the SPEC N is a possessor and
the head is the N'. Unlike English, where the possessive clitic '5 is attached to the
possessor NP, in Waimiri Atroari it is the possessed noun that receives the morphological
typical possessive phrase (i.e., 'the village that belongs to lawara') or a 'naming'
construction ('the village whose name is lawara'). Moreover, possessives will be always
136
on the left because they will be either complement or specifier (Comp for inalienable and
b. NP b. NP
NP N' NP N'
A 1 A 1
temere N
A
L 1
N
flexible in their distribution, in a noun phrase containing adjectives, the adjectives can be
Many Carib languages do not have 'adjective' as a syntactic class (part of speech). Words corresponding
semantically to adjectives are classified as nouns. As discussed in Chapter 3,1 claim that Waimiri Atroari
does have adjectives. Syntactically, adjectives can, like nouns, occur in subject or object position.
However, unlike nouns, adjectives cannot take the suffix -mi that indicates 'absence' (e.g. ety-my
'nameless'). Furthermore, only adjectives can take the emphatic suffix -pa (e.g. tamkwa-pa 'very short').
On the other hand, it is not clear whether one can use the second-position particle ram as a boundaiy
constituent in phrases of the type <Adj N>. I have to do more tests because it is not always the case that
the Waimiri Atroari consultants allow this kind of construction:
either to the left or to the right of the head noun. At this moment, I cannot determine
whether this variation is purely stylistic or whether it entails any semantic difference.
c. d.
N' N'
xiwia N N xrma
'beautifiil'
mydy tydy
'house' 'house'
warara bi pipe-se
turtle eggs look.for-in. order, to
'Yesterday, we went in the big canoe to look for turtle eggs.'
also present a certain degree of positional variation. As shown in examples (13) to (17)
below, adverbial quantifiers can occur either to the left or to the right of the head noun.
138
As the syntactic trees in (13c) and (13d) demonstrate, I consider such quantifiers as
c. d.
AdvP AdvP
waha X X waha
'many' i | 'many'
xiba xiba 'fish'
xirikiki baka-paiky
parakeet kill-T/A
'Many elders climbed trees to kill parakeets.'
I provide them to show that other kinds of adverbs behave in the same way, presenting
and wapi 'many, a lot' seems to be free when they modify a noun phrase (examples 13-
position when modifying a verb phrase (examples 16 and 17). Waimiri Atroari lacks
determiners that correspond to each, every, most, and some, a fact that suggests the
in Waimiri Atroari quantifiers such as all, many, and two do not belong to the functional
According to Bach et al (1995), D-quantifier is associated with determiner-like elements where the
scope is restricted to NPs in specific positions.
140
The native lexicon of Waimiri Atroari has only three numeral words,18 whose
awinini -awinihe -awynihe' means 'alone' and also 'one;' the term typytyna means 'a
couple,' 'a pair,' or 'two;' the word for 'three' is takynynapa. Thus, traditionally the
kinja counted only up to three; amounts higher than three were referred to simply as
'several, many'. Today, with the modern necessity for handling money and the
introduction of western mathematical concepts through the village schools, the kinja
started using Portuguese loanwords to refer to numbers higher than three. These
borrowed numerals occur in the same position as the native words meaning 'one,' 'two,'
or 'three.' Less commonly, Portuguese numerals for 'one,' 'two,' or 'three' may also be
used instead of the native words, especially by the younger speakers (25). As shown in
the examples below, numeral words can occur before a noun (21-23, 26), after a noun
'^.I am using the term 'niuneral words' instead of 'numerals' because I have no evidence for the existence
of numerals as an independent part-of-speech in this language.
This sentence was t^en from a text narrating a fight between the kinja and the non-natives. The three
non-Indian characters mentioned in tliis sentence have already been introduced in an earlier passage of the
141
It is not totally clear what motivates this variation in the position of the numeral
words. However, from the examples shown above, it seems that the variation may be
related to issues of specificity and definiteness. That is, if the speaker thinks the listener
already knows and can identify the particular referent which will be talked about, the
speaker will codify such referents as definite and specific. According to Diesing (1992)
and Diesing & Jelinek (1995), there is a mapping between argument structure and
organization of the clause with respect to presuppositional (familiar) vs. information new
to the discourse." The Waimiri Atroari language clearly follows this mapping when
topicalizing some arguments of the clause and also seems to make an association between
definiteness and old information versus indefiniteness and new information through the
relative position of the numeral word in a noun phrase. When in specific and definite
text. Therefore, the noun phrase taMnini pahki kamijia 'only [the] three White men' is clearly definite in
this context, as shown by the English translation provided above.
This is an example of reduplication in Waimiri Atroari. Reduplication in tliis language is bimoraic,
occurring with verb stems to indicate repetition or continuation; See section 2.5.4 and 2.6.2.
142
contexts, the numeral word seems to prefer the left side of the noun (21-23, 26).
However, in unspecific contexts, the numeral word is positioned either post-verbally (25)
or at the right side of the noun (24, 26). In this sense, the numeral words in (24) and (26)
behave like the adverbial quantifier in example (15), inasmuch as the speaker is not
talking about a specific group of people or hammocks. It is true that examples (21) and
(25) are potentially problematic for my assumptions, since it is not clear if the consultant
is talking about two specific chickens or a specific group of seventeen kinja. Therefore,
preceded by a NP (27). The VP can move to before the subject NP through topicalization
(27) and cannot have its components separated, except in the OSV context when the
object moves alone to a topic position (as will be discussed below in example 30). In
criterion to test the constituency of a given phrase (See section 3.1.6.2). The particle ram
can never intervene between two elements of the same phrase (27c). Furthermore, since
According to the syntactic framework I am adopting here, based on HaJpem & Zwicky (1996), the first
element is the first immediate constituent of the clause, such as a complement or argument of the verb, an
adverbial modifier, or other clausal constituent.
143
.ikome i-iny-pia
(28) ka-ky/
speak-IMP
'Speak!'
Example (30) above illustrates the only context where the VP is separated by the
subject, when the object undergoes topicalization. (For more details, see section 2.1 on
In Waimiri Atroari, some postpositions can inflect for person, taking the same
series of markers used to indicate the possessor on nouns and the object on transitive
verbs (See table 3.10). The syntactic link between a postposition and its noun phrase
144
object is as strong as that between the elements of the noun and verb phrases: nothing can
(31) PostP
Post'
NP Post
A
syna ke
'with water'
The tree above demonstrates that the head is always to the right in postpositional
phrases, just as with noun and verb phrases. The examples below reinforce my claim that
Based on the different types of phrases shown above, I argue that Wairoiri Atroari
postposition phrases, Waimiri Atroari presents a typical case of head right. However, in
phrases with adjuncts, such as noun phrases containing adjectives, adverbial quantifiers,
and numeral words, the relative position of the head seems to vary; a) depending on the
kind of information that the speaker intends to convey, such as specificity and
I have shown in the first section of this chapter that Waimiri Atroari is a head-
right language. In the next section, I intend to extend the analysis to show that this
notion specifier is central to much contemporary work in syntax, it is hard to identify any
common set of properties which all specifiers share because of the disagreement among
linguists of what is its fLinction." Here, I assume the view that "specifier position is used
illustrated in the trees below, Waimiri Atroari allows different kinds of clausal order,
such as SOV, SVO, SV, VS, OSV, and OVS. However, as demonstrated in the tree in
In relation to this issue, I have to do more tests and collect more data to check this hypothesis.
146
(39), in the majority of cases the head of the clause is on the right and the specifier is in
(39) a. VP b. VP c. VP
Spec V Spec V
V Comp Comp V V
d.VP e. VP
V Spec V Spec
V Comp V
mostly positioned before the head. The only exceptions are in the OSV and VS orders.
In the OSV context, topicalization destroys the order of the SPEC in relation to the head,
leaving behind a trace. However, at this point in the analysis, it is not clear to me what
motivates the VS order.^^ It is possible that verbal topics offer an area of exploration for
In this grammatical sketch, I claim that SOV is the basic order based on three
distinctions between old and new information, etc.). Waimiri Atroari tends to put old
information on the left side of the sentence. Waimiri Atroari shows what Mithun
(1992:31) call 'newsworthiness:' even when both arguments (subjects and objects) are
equally provided at the same time or both are new (none of them was presented in
previous discourse or context), the speakers of Waimiri Atroari tend to prefer the subject
reported stories, and others), I analyzed all sentences that presented both one- and two-
place predicates with overt non-pronominal arguments and pronominal arguments. From
a limited sample of ninety-five sentences, the proportion of word order variation found
SOV 42
sv 25
OVS 10
svo 8
osv 5
VS 5
Taking the position of the specifier in relation to the head, even with the
may not be sufficient to establish the basic word order of a language, as pointed out by
Derbyshire (1977), this criterion is also corroborated by other facts of the Waimiri
Atroari language. Here, I demonstrate that at surface structure the basic word order
appears in different types of constructions, reinforcing the assumption that SOV shows
the 'descriptive simplicity' pointed out by Chomsky (1965). The examples below also
Transitive context
(40) aa ram ka h-ini-pia
1 2PART 3 1-see-lM
'I saw him.'
As you can observe, examples (40) to (43) were taken from elicitation data and
Quotative context
(45) mawa njy i-tee-pa, n-ootxi-pa ke-pa, apia myre?
Mawa noise REL-hear-REM.P 3-go.down-REM.P say-REM.P what that
'Mawa heard a noise, waited, and said: "What's that?"'
Since X-Bar Theory does not allow or cannot generate sentences of the type
'OSV inasmuch as the SPEC of IP cannot intervene between the verb (head) and the
complement object, in this grammatical sketch I show that the OSV and OVS orders
result respectively from the object and the VP movement to a topic position.
149
4.3 Topicalization
According to Payne (1997:270), "Topic is what the sentence is about, it is the old,
initial position, a fact that is very common cross-linguistically. The topic construction in
Waimiri Atroari is used to turn the attention to a definite object in order to avoid being
Aissen (1992:43), analyzing the position of topic and focus in Mayan, claims that
sentences with two NPs before the verb, (SOV) and (OSV), involve the focus of one NP
and the topicalization of the other. She shows that while SOV represents subject
topicalization and object focus (topic is S-initial), OSV order must represent object
topicalization and subject focus (focus position is preverbal). Taking part of this
approach into consideration, I claim that the OVS order in Waimiri Atroari results from a
left movement of the VP in order to reach topic position, and unlike Mayan, the OSV
which moves the subject to the initial position in order to express topic, in Waimiri
Atroari OVS is not neutral. Instead, it is a marked order where what is moved to the
topic position is the whole VP. In this paper, I argue that the element to be topicalized
moves into the SPEC position within a TopP (Topic Phrase) constituent headed by a topic
tapiwutape ta n-o'm-pa
Tapiwutape LOC 3S-dive-REM.P
'In the shallow beach of the Tapiwutape lake Tyiyry dived.'
topicalized probably because it contains old informationthat is, the noun tyiyry
'Tyiyry' is introduced earlier in the text (46a). As a result of the topicalization of the VP,
the subject NP xiriminja 'm3 'thological entity,' which is new information, is introduced at
A similar case occurs in the examples in (47) below. In (47a), the subject NP
ianana 'lanana' and the object NP tahkome 'elder(s)'are both introduced for the first time
in the unmarked, SOV order. In (47b), the NP tahkome occurs again as the subject of an
intransitive verb. In the following two sentences, (47c) and (47d), the NP tahkome is
xirikiki baka-paiky
parakeet shoot/kill-T/A
'Many elders climbed the kamakaxi tree in order to kill the parakeet.'
[ V P ] Top' [VP]
p IP Tbp JP
0 xiriminja 0 lanana
0 V
DP V
[tyiyry i-yhia i-erekytypa] [tahkome bakepa]
As X-bar theory cannot deal with OSV order, I claim that this order results from
the dislocation of the object to topic position. Diesing (1995:126) claims that in many
languages, object shift depends on information structure, in particular something like the
objects move for configurational or interpretational reasons rather than for case-checking.
According to my Waimiri Atroari consultants, this order happens when they want to
152
emphasize who did the action. Therefore, as proposed by Diesing (1995), I assume that
in the OSV order, the object undergoes topicalization. The subject position in this order
is restricted to (taking only the kra ~ kara form) and 2"'^ person pronouns. The only
exception for this restriction happens with quotative sentences, where the subject can be a
proper noun.
I leave aside these quotative sentences since they are well known for exhibiting a
marked order (Branigan and Collins 1993). I assume that OSV order in the standard case
can best be explained by Wackernagel's Law, which claims that clitics, particles, and
pronouns must appear in second position in a clause (Halpem & Zwicky 1996).
According to Kaisse (1981), recent research on clitics shows that languages may vary
those languages, such as Bulgarian, Luiseno, and Serbo-Croatian, that allow either the
first word or the first constituent of the clause to define 'second position' (see footnote
9)26
As illustrated above in examples (50) and (51), the pronoun indicating first person singular kra can occur
after a word or a constituent. On the other hand, second position particle ram can appear not only after a
word and a constituent, but also after a whole clause.
imka typotxe a=wutoty m-itxirikw-epa ipaikyparam tymeri a=w-iapa
if fast 2-run 2-stop-T/A after 2PART jaguar 2=kill-T/A
'If you stop running fast, the jaguar will catch you.'
153
In the tree below, I demonstrate that the heavy element moves to the left and the
light element stays in the right, in the second position. The pronoun kra is in fact in the
second position, inasmuch as the second-position particle ram cannot co-occur in this
example (see example 52). They compete for the same slot in the tree, the Top slot.
TopP
[ O ] Top'
Top
kra
aryma h-yni-any
clauses are those which serve an 'adverbial function:' they modify a verb phrase or a
simply add some extra information to the clauses. Thompson & Longacre (1985:172)
154
explain that there are three types of subordinated clauses: those which function as noun
and those which function as modifiers of verb phrases or entire propositions (adverbial
clauses).
In this section, I briefly direct the analysis to two types of subordinated clauses:
clauses that indicate time and location, and what Meira (1999) and Gildea (1998) call
'nominalized clauses.' Here, I want to demonstrate that these clauses work exactly like
This type of clause can occur either at the beginning or at the end of the sentence.
Thompson & Longacre {op. cit.) claim that in the world's languages, there are typically
three devices for marking subordinated clauses: subordinating morphemes, special verb
forms, and word order. Waimiri Atroari uses only the first two devices. As shown in the
example below, temporal adverbial clauses in Waimiri Atroari are introduced by the
word imka 'when, if As Thompson & Longacre {op. cit, 193) mention, there are
several languages, such as Indonesian and certain languages of Papua New Guinea,
which do not make a distinction between if clauses and when clauses. Waimiri Atroari
seems to be one of these languages, since, as we have mentioned, imka can be either
In this paper, as you note in the trees below, I am not dealing with INFL (I am not representing it in the
trees).
155
As you can observe in the tree below, in the temporal adverbial clause, the VP
undergoes topicalization; it does not show the basic order SOV, but the marked OVS.
This example was given after I had asked about other examples: kerne 'he' is old
information. Moreover, you can note that the C slot can be empty or filled by the word
b CP
CP(AdvP; CP
[VP] Top'
Top
ndminja
[kerne ikipia]
epiapi yaki, can only occur at the end of the sentence, as shown by examples (55) and
(56) below:
156
(56)
a. ka ram ase mydy iam-ia apiapy iaky temer-eme wu-pa
3 2PART new house build-T/A where jaguar-DEV kill-REM
'He builds the new village where he killed the jaguar.'
b. IP
ase mydy ia
apiapyyaky
temereme
wupa
157
Nominalized clauses present the same parameter position of the head shown by
the more typical NPs described above. As shown by examples (57) and (58) below, the
head in these nominalized clauses is marked by the nominalizer sufiBx -o, while the NP
corresponding to the subject of a transitive clause is marked by the particle ia. Unlike
independent clauses, which may present word-order variations due to phenomena such as
topicalization, nominalized clauses present a strict SOV order, a fact that gives flirther
support to the postulation of SOV as the basic word order in Waimiri Atroari.
amyra
C minipa
aapapa w-o
The root of the verb 'to kill' is -wm and the root of the verb 'to make' is -aJdmi'. The last vowel of the
root is dropped with the suffixation of the nominalizer -o. The nominalizer -o is also observed in other
Carib languages (Gildea 1998).
I have provided an analysis of the Waimiri Atroari phrase structure and word
order, as well as a brief overview of the distribution of the heads in some subordinated
and nominalized clauses, accounting for the head and specifier parameter. I have argued
that Waimiri Atroari belongs to a parametric class of languages that exhibits the
following characteristics at spell-out: (a) head-right when not involving adjuncts, (b)
Less usual word orders, such as OVS and SOV, are accounted for by postulating a
respectively. Although I do not analyze cases of SV and SVO orders in this paper, I
speculate that SVO order could be a result of Portuguese influence, inasmuch as its
occurrence is very reduced, being more common among the younger male speakers, who
present a higher degree of bilingualism than elders and women. Therefore, this, as well
as the factors underlying the occurrence of SV word order, is one of the hypotheses to be
fiarther investigated.
ABBREVIATIONS
APPENDICES
tapiwutape ta n-o'm-pa.
Tapiwutape LOG 3s-dive-REM.P
'In the shallow beach of the Tapiwutape lake Tyiyry dived.'
am pa kinja many
other people also
'After Paryna shot Xiriminja, Maiahka also shot him, and other people did too.'
These stories, traditional tales of the Kinja people, were narrated by Dauna, a male shaman and
storyteller today in his late fifties, in the village of Kaminjanyty in 1990/1993. I collected lanana story,
kaapy tahkome karykapa, wyty ikaa (written by Waraie), and meie ikaa (written by the women of Alalaii's
village). The other stories were collected by Carlos Augusto Queiroz (ex-professor of the Alalaii's village)
161
n-ipykwapy-sapa pyruwa ke
3S-kill-T/A arrow INSTR
' She did not want to come back then Paxe killed her with arrows.'
xirikiki baka-paiky
parakeet shoot/kill-T/A
'Many elders climbed the kamakaxi tree in order to kill the parakeets.'
lanana is a mythological entity, the owner of tlie forest. He has as a pet parakeet {firikiki in Waimiri
Atroari).
164
ianana
lanana
'Then the lucky person heard lanana happily saying to the dead elders:-"hepe!
hepe! hepe! hebe.'"
i-apremy
REL-owner
'Then the lucky person took the door's measurement from lanana's house to the
village.'
i-tapia-pa
REL-close-REM.P
'Then the elder took the wood plank and make a door to close ianana's entrance.'
24. -hensin hensin mensi taba ieni piky many taba iene. ..
hensin hensin mensi taba ieni piky many taba iene
'Hensin, hensin mensi taba iene piky many taba iene said lanana's son.'
Note: I tried to get the translation fijr this song, but even my consultants could not
translate.
ny-tytapah-ky
3S-held-PAST
'Then the elders took lanana's son who was hold in the tree hole to rear.'
taka
AL
'Then after he was grown up, he hunted a tapir and wrapped it with pataua leaves
to look like agouti.'
amyry m-inehetxa axiwi sybary myra kapy inime mepri inehty taha
2PRO 2A-bring agouti bad ? NEG husband tapir bring big
taham-pa-ry
big-EMPH-?
'Then when she cut the string that tied the bundle, the bundle spread open
showing the real size of the tapir.'
kaapy ia tybekia-paiky.
heaven to tell-after
'Many animals came close to the elder's place,(somebody telling to other that this
happened before.'
Note; The elders perceived it because the armadillo's teeth are small, and they
were big as the wild pig's teeth.
Note: The villages were very distant from each other, but a big fire had made a
big clearing in the jungle that made it possible to see from a village to another.'
Note: The house's central pole made a hole in the sky. Through this hole the
people who were saved exited, went above the old sky and built a new land.
Today the Waimiri Atraori people live on the top of sky.
'Then the elders (the brother and sister that survived) took a long time to build
their own house and make their garden plantation too, his wife also helped him to
make the garden plantation.'
n-yme-pa.
3s-born-REM.P
'Then many sons were born to help them.'
pykyry-maty
tell-?
'After that Mawa took that couple who survived said my father.'
172
Note: The elders said that the sky had fallen three times already. The last time was
when this pair of siblings survived.
aa-pap-eme
iPOS-fkther-DEV
'That fallen sky was substituted by this sky that we have today... said my father.'
Note: This heaven that we have today will fall as well said the kinja
kaapa taka
garden AL
'Early in the morning, we went to the garden to collect manioc tubers'
wyiepe ta
jamaxi(kind of basketery) Loc
'Then we came back to the house with the jamaxi full of manioc.'
9. impa ny-tyky-pia.
then l+3S-finish-LM.P
'Then we finished.'
bixuwa taka
bowl AL
'Then, they took the meat from the pan and put it in a bowl.
174
8 ny-tyky-pia
3S-finish-LM.P
'Finished.'
175
KYNYRY (tongue)
Kynyry nata (tip of the tongue)
Kynyry sopry (center of the tongue)
Kynyry iee kysa (laterals of the tongue)
Kynyry kadyhyry (back of the tongue)
BILABIAL- kypyta bixi nytybaske sypyky naka. (The two lips get together);
P , b, kapaiky kypj^a bixi nybaske imany kynyraty tuwaha na kiee iepry pyky naka
wotyrykwaharyky na.
" When we say /p/ and Ihl our lips will get together and our tongue will be in the
middle, it will not touch the palate"
ALVEOLAR- Kynyry nata notyrykwe kiee pyny wotyka pyky naka - The tip of the
tongue touch behind of the teeth, on the alveolar ridge)
176
C.7
1A30 Aa ram ka h-irima-py-piany.
IPRO 2PART 3PR0 lA-rest-CAUS-REC.P
'I made him/her rest.'
2A30 Am5Ta ram ka m-irima-py-piany.
2PR0 2PART 3PRO 2A-rest-CAUS-REC.P
'You made him/her rest.'
3A30 Ka ram ka 0-irima-py-piany.
3PR0 2PART 3PRO 0-rest-caus-REC.P
'She/he made him rest.'
3A20 Ka ram a-irima-py-piany.
3PRO 2PART 20-rest-CAUS-REC.P
'She/he made you rest.'
1A20 Aa ram k-irima-py-piany.'
IPRO 2PART 20-rest-CAUS-REC.P
'I made you rest.
Animals:
REFERENCES
ARCHANGELI, Diana. 2000. What Does OT tell us about language? Talk given at 12"'
SCIL at University of Arizona, Tucson. (Handout).
BAINES, S.G. 1991. a FUNAI quern sabe Frente de atragdo Waimiri AtroarL Belem:
Museu Paraense Emilio Goeldi/CNPq/SCT/PR.
. 1994. Epidemics, the Waimiri Atroari Indians and the politics of demography.
SerieAntropologia, 162, Brasilia: Departamento de Antropologia, Universidade de
Brasilia.
BASSO, Ellen. 1977. Carib speaking Indians: culture, society, and language. In
Anthropological Papers of The University of Arizona, 28. Tucson: The University
of Arizona Press.
BASSO, Ellen. 1985. A musical view of the universe. Philadelphia: University of Pennsyl
vania. Press.
. 1995. The last cannibals: a South America oral history. Austin: University Press.
BRANIGAN, Philips and COLLINS, Chris. 1993. Verb movement and the quotative
construction in English. In Bobaljik, Jonathan and Philips Branigan (editors), MIT
Working Papers in Linguistics 18. Cambridge: MIT.
BRUNO, Ana Carla. 1995a. Proposta ortografica para lingua Waimiri Atroari. Manaus:
PWA.
. 1999. The causative construction in Waimiri Atroari. Final paper for the Lexical
Semantics class. Tucson: University of Arizona.
CARNIE, Andrew and E GUILFOYLE, Eithne (editors). 2000. The Syntax of verb initial
languages. New York: Oxford University Press.
CARVALHO, Jose P. F. 1982. Waimiri Atroari: a hitdria que ainda nao foi contada.
Brasilia.Edifao do autor.
CHOMSKY, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.
. 1977. Word order universals and the existence of OVS languages. Linguistic
Inquiry 8.590-99.
DELANCEY, Scott. 1985. On active typology and nature of agentivity. Relational typo
logy, ed. by Frans Plank, 47-60. Berlin:Mouton.
DffiSiNG, Molly and JELINEK, Eloise. 1995. Distributing arguments. Natural Languages
Semantics 3.123-176.
FOLEY, William A., and VAN VALIN, Robert D., Jr. 1984. Functional and Universal
Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
HALPERN, Aaron. 1992. Topics in the placement and morphology of clitics. PhD
dissertation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.
HALPERN, Aaron and ZWICKY, Arnold. 1996. Approaching second: second position
clitics and related phenomena. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
. 1995. Metrical stress theory: principle and case of studies. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
HARLEY, Heidi. 1995.SASE Bizarre; The structure of Japanese causative. MIT. 13p. ms.
183
HILL, Joseph and HILL, Tamara, 1985.Phonological sketch of Waimiri Atroari (Cariban).
Unpublished manuscript. Missao Evangdica da Amazonia.
HOFF, Berend J. 1978. The relative order of the Carib finite verb and its nominal depen
dents. Studies on Fronting. Frank Jansen,ed., pp 11-27. Dordrecht, Netherlands;
Foris.
JACKENDOFF, Ray. 1977. X' Syntax: A Study of Phrase Structure. Linguistic Inquiry
Monograph. Cambridge; MIT Press.
KAISSE, Ellen M. 1981. Luiseno particles and the universal behavior of clitics. Linguistic
Lnquiry 12;424-34.
KLOOS, Peter 1971. The Maroni River Caribs. Assen. The Netherlands; Gorcum.
LACERDA, Edith and QUEIROZ, Carlos Augusto. 1991. Waimiri Atroari preliminary
dictionary. Manaus; PWA.
LEVIN, Beth. 2000. Aspects, lexical semantic representation, and argument expression.
Talk given at the Linguistic Colloquium at the University of Arizona,
Tucson.(Handout).
MCCARTHY, John and PRINCE, Alan. 1994. The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality
Theory. University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and Rutgers. 43p.ms.
MEIRA, Sergio. 1995a. The Accidental intransitive split in the Cariban family, ms
MESTCR, R.Armin. 1988. Studies in Tier Stj'ucture. New York: Garland Publishing.
MILLEKEN, W.; MILLER, R. P. et al. 1992. Ethnobotany of the Waimiri Atroari Indians of
Brazil. Great Britain: The Royal Botanic Garden, Kew, Whistable Litho,
MITHUN, Marianne. 1991. Active/agentive case marking and its motivations. Language
61: 510-546,
PAYNE, Doris and Marianne Mithun (editors). 1992. Pragmatics of Word Order
Flexibility. Typological Studies in Language 22. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
PRINCE, Alan and SMOLENSKY, Paul. 1993. Optimality Theory. Constraint interaction in
generative grammar. Ms., Rutgers University, New Brunswick and University of
Colorado, Boulder.
RADFORD, Andrew. 1997. Syntactic Theoiy and the structure of English: A minimalist
approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
RODRIGUES, Aryon. 1994. Grammatical affinities among Tupi, Carib, and Macro-Je.
Unpublished manuscript. Brasilia; Universidade de Brasilia.
SUVA, Marcio F. da. 1993. "O parentesco Waimiri Atroari: algumas observa9oes prelimi-
nares" In Viveiros de Castro, Eduardo de & Cunha, Manuela Carneiro de
{Or^s.)Amazonia etnologia e historia indigena. Sao Paulo, Nucleo de Historia
Indigenae de Indigenismo USP/FAPESP. p.211-228.
TRAVIS, Lisa. 1989. Parameters of phrase structure. In Baltin, Mark and Kroch, Anthony
(editors). Alternative Conceptions of Phrase Structure. Chicago; Chicago
University Press.
VAN VALIN, Robert D., Jr. 1990. Semantic parameters of Split Intransitivity. Language
66, 221-260.
WUNDERLICH, Dieter and LAKAMPER, Renate. 2001. On the interaction of structural and
semantic case. Lingua. ILL; 277-418.