Abstracts Word2007
Abstracts Word2007
Abstracts Word2007
Juliette Blevins
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology
[email protected]
Prosodic domains are central to synchronic descriptions of sound patterns. Whether one
is determining the inventory of contrastive features, identifying phonotactics, or
analysing alternations, reference must be made to a specific prosodic domain, most
commonly, the phonological word. In contrast, historical descriptions of sound patterns
make little reference to prosodic domains. Though words are central units of comparison,
and regular sound change is assumed to apply within such units, there has been relatively
little study of the role of prosodic domains in determining the nature and direction of
sound change. This talk presents an overview of two ways prosodic domains can
influence sound change, working primarily with historical data from Austronesian and
neighboring languages. In the first case, an inherited prosodic domain is self-reinforcing
over time. In the second case, a prosodic domain is superimposed on a language via
contact, leading to a range of sound changes that are unattested in non-contact situations,
but which serve to satisfy the imposed prosodic domain. A range of hypotheses are
suggested by this study, from restrictive contact origins of sesquisyllabic domains, to
accounts of prosodically determined allomorphy.
The Optimal Word in Hindi
Gyanam Mahajan
UCLA
This paper analyzes the phonological word in some Indo Aryan languages. While it is
true that it is difficult to find evidence for a phonological word in Indo Aryan
languages or South Asian languages in general, it is striking that there is an optimal
word in these languages. The basic claim is that the optimal Hindi word is a
predictable, preferred form of bi-syllabic, with each syllable being heavy. While this is
not an expected unit since it is not a definable foot, this paper will show several word
formation strategies that are prosodically motivated to yield the bi-syllabic, each syllable
bi-moraic form. First, the processes that are considered are related to naming in Indo-
Aryan languages. Specifically, this paper looks at the following naming processes: (i)
assigned nicknames, kinship terms, other common names for objects (which are all
overwhelmingly bisyllabic, bimoraic) (ii) hypocoristics or nickname formation (e.g. long
names are reduced to two syllables, each syllable heavy, while one syllable names are
added onto to yield bisyllabic, bimoraic syllables). Additonal data is provided from
assimilation of borrowed words into Indo Aryan languages (lengthening in borrowed
Arabic words or gemination in borrowed English words) . Secondly, the paper analyzes
verb forms and provides an analysis with schwa epenthesis, which is again
overwhelmingly constrained by a preference for bisyllabic, each syllable heavy shape of
a word (e.g. verb stems are overwhelmingly monosyllabic and affixes attach to yield the
optimal word shape). Additional data is discussed from heritage language learners.
While most of the data is drawn from Hindi, the paper considers other closely related
languages to look at the similarities and differences with Hindi. We observe similar data
for Gujarati and Punjabi. We also pursue the idea of India as a Linguistic Area to
consider data from Tamil, which is a Dravidian language.
THE PHONOLOGICAL WORD AS THE DOMAIN OF RULE APPLICATION:
THE CASE OF BANGLA, TAMIL AND AO
Paroma Sanyal
([email protected])
T.Temsunungsang
([email protected])
K.G.Vijayakrishnan
([email protected])
In this paper we look at three languages from different language families of South
Asia to argue in favour of positing the prosodic category phonological word, between the
foot/prosodic word and the phonological phrase. We show that certain phonological
generalisations are best stated at this level of prosodic organization.
In Bangla, which belongs to the Indo-Aryan family, iterative feet are built from the
left edge of the stem in the domain of the phonological word. Importantly, excluding the
prefix, derivational and inflectional suffixes including the genitive marker (clitic?) is parsed
and re-syllabified along with the stem to form a phonological word.
1. (a). [(akor)( n)] pr.wd + er => [(akor)(ner)]ph wd
attraction GEN
(b). [(pori)(brton)]pr.wd + er => [(pori)(brto)(ner)]ph wd
change GEN
In the above examples, the suffix/clitic gets incorporated into the prosodic domain of the
stem. This is reiterated by the licensing of the vowel // and vowel length observations in the
language. However, this domain is smaller than a phonological phrase which can include
more than one phonological word.
In the Dravidian language Tamil, the vowels /u/ and // (unrounded, high, back vowel)
are in complementary distribution. While the former occurs in morpheme-initial position, the
latter is rounded elsewhere in the environment of labial consonants. The domain for this rule
of labial harmony is the phonological word (stem + suffix + clitic) as the data in 2 shows.
2. (a). perkk sweep
(b). perkku-m sweep. F.neut
Chungli, one of the major dialects of Ao, a Tibeto-Burman tone language with three
contrastive tones, attested on every syllable also provides evidence for the phonological word
between the foot and the phrase. The tonal sequence M-L does not occur within words but is
attested across word boundaries.
3. [[(an) (tukln)-i]ph.wd [asm-a]ph.wd ]PP run to the east
sun rise -LOC run-imp
In the above, the M in /an/ changes to H when followed by a L tone. However, the M in /-i /
when followed by L of the second phonological word, undergoes no change.
Martine Mazaudon
Lacito-CNRS, Paris
[email protected]
The languages of the Tamang branch of Bodish, or TGTM languages, are at a stage in
their evolution where tone is still in the process of creation by transphonologisation from
segmental material, while tonal oppositions are now threatened by reduction or loss, at least in
the bilingual speakers of some of these languages, under the influence of increased contact
with non tonal languages.
In these languages, the domain of the lexical assignment of tone is the morpheme,
whether monosyllabic or polysyllabic, and the domain of its phonetic implementation is the
word, comprising a lexical root and a (possible) string of affixes. Word-tone is a characteristic
of this group of languages and of (?most) languages of its sister branch, the Tibetan branch.
But some Qiangic languages are also reported with word-tone and it may be more an areal
character than a character of a branch of Tibeto-Burman. We will look at the different
structures in Risiangku Tamang, Standard Tibetan and Dzongkha.
Secondly, tones in Tamang are defined by a bundle of features or cues, not all based on
F0, like voicing, open quotient -- breathiness -- which we have studied instrumentally, and
others which we have not.
Finally we will look at other prosodies (in the Firthian sense), like restrictions on vowel
length, and intervocalic voicing which help define the word-domain or smaller domains in
Tamang.
The Prosodic Structure of Prinmi
Prinmi is a Tibeto-Burman language unique to southwestern China. It falls within the linguistic
area of South-East Asia (Matisoff 2001). Like many languages of this area, Prinmi morphemes
are predominantly monosyllabic and the language has developed a tonal system, where all
lexemes are assigned with a particular tonal category. Unlike the typical tonal language of
South-East Asia, the domain for lexical tones in Prinmi is not based on the syllable; it also
differs from word-tone languages such as Tibetan.
Tonal patterns in Prinmi are generated with two parameters: specification of the locus of H (the
high tone) in an underlying domain independent of the grammatical word, and the potential
spreading of H to the next syllable. Such characteristics of the tonal system of Prinmi are akin
to those found in the pitch-accent system of Japanese. Consequently, Prinmi has been analyzed
as a pitch-accent language in Ding (2001; 2006). To avoid unnecessary confusion from the
rubric pitch-accent (cf. Hyman 2006), the kind of tonal system found in Prinmi and Japanese
will be referred to as melody-tone in this paper. Accordingly, the underlying domain for
lexical tones will be labeled as the melody.
The prosodic structure of Prinmi involves three types of domain at various level, viz. the
melody, the phonological word, and the grammatical word. Minimal triplets that contrast solely
in terms of tonal categories occur in many monosyllabic words, e.g. (1). For the sake of
convenience, all syllables in Prinmi data are marked for tone (with H for high, F for falling
R for rising, and L for low.
(1) Tone A [H.L.L]melody Tone B [H.H.L]melody Tone C [L.H.L]melody
a iF louse iH hundred iR new
b bjF urine bjH busy bjR to fly
The grammatical words in (1), being monosyllabic, has a great impact on the realization of
these tonal categories: it neutralizes the contrast between Tone A and B. However, when the
words occur in a larger domain of phonological word after an enclitic is attached, the different
patterns become transparent:
(2) Tone A [H.L.L]melody Tone B [H.H.L]melody
a iH gjeL louse iH gjeH hundred
~ ~
b mH gjeL hair mH gjeH name
c bi-H gjeL honey bi-H gjeH sun
In compounding a complex grammatical word often extends into a larger phonological word,
where the second formative loses its original tone, e.g.
~
(3)a oH sheep + F meat o~ H H mutton
b tsh-i F goat + F meat tsh-i H L goat meat
However, it is possible for a compound word longer than two syllables to have more than one
melody. Similarly, the phonological word may also contain more than one melody.
References
Ding, P. S. 2001. The pitch-accent system of Niuwozi Prinmi. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 24: 57-83.
. 2006. A typological study of tonal systems of Japanese and Prinmi: Towards a definition of pitch-accent
languages. Journal of Universal Language 7: 135.
Hyman, L. M. 2006. Word-prosodic typology. Phonology 23: 225257.
Matisoff , J. A. 2001. Genetic versus contact relationship: prosodic diffusibility in South-East Asian languages,
291327 of Dixon & Aikhenvald (eds), Areal Diffusion and Genetic Inheritance. Oxford: OUP.
Observations on the Production and Perception of Tone in Manang-
Gurung
Kristine Hildebrandt (University of Manchester) and David Hnlich
(University of Leipzig)
Shengli Feng
Harvard University
The notion of MinWd is derived from the interactions of Prosodic Hierarchy and Foot
Binarity (M & P 1990, 1993, 1998). The Prosodic Hierarchy impinges on every prosodic
word to contain at least one foot, while the Foot Binarity demands that every foot be
bimoraic or disyllabic. As a result, a prosodic word must contain at least two moras or
syllables according to the transitivity of the Prosodic Hierarchy. The Minimal Word is
therefore a single PrWd in the system. As we will see in this paper, the Minimal Word is of
singular importance in characterizing a wide range of prosodic-morphological phenomena
not only in languages known in previous studies, but also in Mandarin Chinese. It is
demonstrated, first, that among all VO forms in the language, only the ones that meet the
minimal word requirement exhibit word properties while longer forms are all on a par with
phrases. Secondly, it is shown that a process of category changing from a [Aux V] verbal
expression to a [Aux-V]adjective compound is conditioned strictly on whether or not the [Aux
V] is a minimal word. Finally, there is a clear distinction, in Mandarin Chinese, between
MinWd and non-MinWd [A+N] forms both morphologically and syntactically.
This paper raises interesting questions as to why and how the Templatic Constraint
could control not only morphology but also syntax, questions that are extremely important for
theoretical as well as empirical inquiries in future research.
Address:
Shengli Feng
Email: [email protected]
Gta bo, Khmu tmluy, Khasi kdor, Ksingmul pta:, Santali daletmetahkanae:
an overview of Munda and Austroasiatic word structure
Gregory D. S. Anderson
Living Tongues Institute for Endangered Languages
[email protected]
In this talk I offer an overview of some of the issues in Munda and Austroasiatic [AA] word
structure. The internal classification of the AA language phylum remains an open question.
It is clear that Munda is divergent from other AA languages. However, it has not yet been
demonstrated how exactly Proto-Munda differs from other putative proto-languages within
AA nor what exactly the relationship[s] is[/are] between the different identifiable subgroups
of AA (with notable exceptions, e.g. Katuic and Bahnaric, which seem to form some higher-
level group together). The term Mon-Khmer [MK] for example has been used for numerous
and as yet inadequately justified subgroupings such as core MK: Monic, Khmeric,
Bahnaric, Katuic, Pearic, Mangic, Palaung-Waic, Khmuic, Khasic, and Viet-Muong; a
mainland MK that includes all of the above and Aslian (and the as yet unclassified
Chinese AA languages as well as Palyu languages) and a macro-MK which further includes
Nicobarese.
Austroasiatic is an old phylum and the subgroups are not closely connected; most
likely, there were additional branches that have died out unrecorded. In addition, AA
languages fall into several different areal linguistic spheres (Sino-, Indo-, and Malayo-
sphere) which have altered the nature of the languages from their original structure. But a
detailed comparative analysis can begin to reveal what the original kernel of AA-ness
was; despite their obvious differences, all languages have a basic AA component.
In this presentation I offer an overview of the nature of AA languages from the
perspective of word structure. This addresses in brief both the nature of the phonemic
systems found in these languages and the broader word-based phonotactics that, for MK
at least (but arguably elsewhere in AA as well), are highly characteristic and indicative of
the family as a whole. Said in other terms, there is a core AA word structure that is found
more or less across the languages of the family. Once the basics of the phonemic systems,
and their combinatorial possibilities are introduced, I give a short overview of
suprasegmentals as well as basic word structure. In all discussions, I move from Munda
to other AA languages in a roughly West to East trajectory.
After an overview of the synchronic nature of AA languages from the perspective
of the phonological structure of the word is offered, I then mention some of the major
overarching issues in AA diachronic linguistics. A study of AA word structure is of
course in part morphological and due to the peculiar history of the family and probable
original proto-AA structure, comparative AA phonology and morphology are
inextricable. Preliminary reconstructions of word forms in AA subgroups are offered and
the issues they elucidate discussed in brief. Whether or how categorization into parts of
speech is relevant to the phonology (or morphophonology) of the word in Munda and
other AA languages is also examined in brief. In addition to the possible differences (or
lack thereof) between nominals and verbals, the nature of expressives and the role of
reduplication in AA word structure are briefly discussed and exemplified. The talk is
summed up by offering thoughts on fruitful areas for future research in comparative
Munda and AA word structure.
John Peterson, [email protected]
In my proposed talk, I will deal with these and other issues in more detail, highlighting the
difficulties involved in any attempt to find a single definition of the "word" in Kharia.
The Structural Identity of the Phonological Word in Vietnamese:
constituency as domain
Shanti lfsbjrninn
The School of Oriental and African Studies
Vietnamese has been described as lacking a phonological word, a minor syllable and minimal word
constraints (Schiering et al. 2007). It will be shown in this essay that Vietnamese phonology viewed
from a Government Phonology or Minimalist Phonology perspective does have minimal word
constraints and a differentiation between phonological words and clitics (Pham 2003) and prefixes
(Thompson 1959) which in Government Phonology (contra Optimality Theory) are also lexically
endowed with syllabic structure. What this paper will also illustrate is the correlation between
minimal word sizes and viability of holding stress.
In part one, it will be shown that in a good number of languages (Mandarin, Kammu, Khmer,
Italian, English, Turkish, Thai, Mongolian) we can see how, in order to gain primary stress and
therefore wordship, the syllabic structures in question must conform to a particular configuration,
namely: the Charette foot (Charette in press.). The Charette foot is a binary relationship between
two nuclei. One, the head, is obligatorily filled, and another, the dependent, obligatorily silent. In
this model, the head of the foot is head of the domain and therefore able to (A-)license its fringes
(Kaye et al. 1990; Harris 1997). All the while, being an obligatory binary relationship between two
nuclei automatically creates what have been called bi-moraic or ONON templates. The paper
will show evidence that if the minor syllable of Austro-Asiatic is large enough ONON it can hold
stress/tone (Kammu) while if it cannot be fitted with a Charette foot, ie. ON it may not bear stress
(Svantesson and Karlsson 2004; Schiering et al. ms.).
Part two will then show how one can motivate a templatic analysis for the phonological word of
Vietnamese; its minimal size, and most importantly its behaviour, contrasts with that of the prefixes
and clitics which attach themselves to these phonological words for stress. This is contra Schiering
et al. (2007), although, some of their other observations do make the Vietnamese phonological
domain a strange one.
Part three will show that, even within GP, Vietnamese has a strange phonological domain, the
templatic analysis of part two which shows Vietnamese having some sort of phonological word is
argued to be arbitrary and flawed in terms of licensing. More importantly it misses certain
phonological observations made in Vietnamese grammars and studies (particularly Pham 2003).
What is salvaged is the following: there is a structural correlation between the minimal word and
the ability to hold stress.
Part four will conclude the paper by illustrating that Phams (2003) observations that the nucleus
and the rhyme form a constituent is the key to understanding the true extend of the correlation
between minimal word size and stress. The observation that the Vietnamese rhyme manifests trade
offs of length between the vowel and the rhyme [ta:m] vs. [tam:] (Pham 2003) is exactly what
Minimalist Phonology (Pchtrager 2006) would expect and mirrors the better known Italian stress
domain: [fat:o] vs. [fa:to] (Lowenstamm 1996). It will be argued that Vietnamese and other South
East Asian languages limit projecting their phonological words above this stress domain while
European languages phonological words may project further, although, all the while, maintaining
this stress domain as a constituent which behaves identically to SE Asian phonological words. This
is revealed in European languages when study is turned to their minimal words.
On the notion of a Cerberean node
Nozomi Kodama
Kumamoto University, Japan
Tonal features demarcate three tiers in Japanese prosodic structure. An intonational phrase, with an
edge tone, and optionally, a tone bearing segment at the rightmost end, consists of one or more
phonological phrases. A phonological phrase, demarcated by a tone rise from the beginning, has
other various prosodic as well as segmental features such as a mora timing domain, compulsory
devoicing of high vowels between two voiceless consonants and the domain of catathesis of
accented words as claimed by Pierrehumbert and Beckman(1988), which may be better explained
simply by the absence of the distinctive tone rise within. (See Kawakami 1956). Phonological words
that make up the phonological phrase have their respective lexically specified tonal feature that
realizes as one pitch fall (accent) at most within or at the rightmost edge. Phonological words and
phrases generally correspond to syntactic phrases and words (or clitic groups in Southwestern
dialects where postpositions and clitics are not lexically specified for tonal features), although a
single syntactic phrase or word may be broken into two or more phonological phrases. Examination
of such cases suggest that syntactic structural nodes at various (morpho-)syntactic levels are divided
into two types: one that phonologically unifies (either as a phonological word or phrase) and the
other that phonologically realize as divided by the phonological phrase boundary, the latter of which
I call a Cerberean node, a non-endocentric node that does not have a single head constituent.
Examples: {azia no buNka} Asian cultures {azia to}{ahurika no buNka} Asian and African
cultures; aziasyokoku //Asian country// {azia}{ahurikasyokoku} Asian and African countries
{azia ahurikasyokoku}{rekihoL} a visit of Asian and African countries
Applied to other prosodically contrastive pairs such as {beLtoLbeN no}{uNmeL} Fate, the
symphony, by Beethoven vs. {beLtoLbeN no uNmeL} Beethovens fate or {hayaku}{kite}
Come, hurry vs. {hayaku kite} Come early, this departure from the unified treatment of
syntactic nodes may have further implications.
Note. I understand that Japanese is not included in South or Southeast Asian languages. So I am
currently working on similar Telugu constructions. At least morphologically similar constructions
abound in Telugu (such as Dvandva compounds that bear plural marking only on the latter
constituent). But as for prododic features, I could make only tentative analyses by September. Rather
I expect to gain insights from other presentations.
Wordhood in Riau Indonesian
David Gil
Isolating languages pose a particular problem for the notion of word. In languages
with a substantial amount of morphology it is generally possible to identify a set of
criterial features, some universal, others language-specific, distinguishing word-external
syntactic structure resulting from the concatenation of words and phrases, from word-
internal morphological structure resulting from processes such as affixation,
compounding and the like. Such criteria form the basis for the existence, within
linguistic theory, of autonomous disciplines of syntax and morphology. However, in
isolating languages, characterized by a paucity of morphological structure, there may not
be enough morphology to support a robust and systematic distinction between
morphological and syntactic structure. Accordingly, in isolating languages, there may be
relatively little evidence for the existence of words as a viable unit of linguistic structure,
as distinct from morphemes. Moreover, what little evidence is available tends to come
from the domain of phonology.
This paper examines the notion of word in one extreme exemplar of the isolating
language type: Riau Indonesian. In general, it is argued that, compared to other, non-
isolating languages, the word plays a much smaller role in the grammar of Riau
Indonesian. Nevertheless, it is still possible to support a distinction between
morphological and syntactic structure in Riau Indonesian.
This paper argues for the following morphological structure underlying words in
Riau Indonesian:
outer word
inner word
syllable syllable
Evidence for the above structure is derived from a number of different sources of
evidence, as summarized in the following table:
CORE FOOT INNER WORD INNER WORD OUTER WORD
(terminal) (non-terminal)
focus intonation X
no reduction X
epenthesis X
loanword expansion X
obligatory si- X
N- realized as nge- X
Warasa ludling X X
final k realized as ? X
Sabaha ludling X
Bahasisa ludling X
Pantun rhythm X
reduplication X
spelling X X X
Evidence for Word Structure Categories
In total, the above phenomena provide convincing evidence for the posited underlying
word structure, but hardly overwhelming evidence: the extent to which the grammar of
Riau Indonesian makes reference to the various categories constituting word structure is
still much less than in most other languages.