tmp4633 TMP
tmp4633 TMP
tmp4633 TMP
To cite this article: Mauro Maldonato, Silvia Dell'Orco & Anna Esposito (2016): The Emergence
of Creativity, World Futures, DOI: 10.1080/02604027.2016.1262641
Anna Esposito
Seconda Universit di Napoli, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Caserta, Italy
INTRODUCTION
The human capacity to create is a central dimension of our existence. The word
creativity is often being used in everyday life and its meaning would appear to
be clear, at least superficially. However, from the scientific point of view, creativity
can be intended in different ways. It is, in fact, an issue that has been studied from
many different disciplines: from philosophy to neuroscience, from economic to
sociology. Over time, divergent interpretations and theories have given rise to an
extensive literature (Kaufman & Sternberg, 2010).
However, beyond the wide variety of definitions, there seems to be broad agree-
ment with the following statement: a psychological process that facilitates the birth
of new and useful ideas (theoretical or practical), connections, and problem solu-
tions. In any case, the main feature of creative thinking is that it is guided by
heuristic processes rather than algorithmic (Amabile, Barsade, Mueller, & Staw,
2005). As heuristics, in fact, creative thinking eludes formal logic of the ratio-
nal mind and manifests itself so quickly, that it does not cross the threshold of
consciousness (Maldonato, Montuori, & DellOrco, 2013).
1
2 MAURO MALDONATO ET AL.
Consider a poem. When an individual reads it, his left hemisphere analyzes the
sequence of letters and integrates them into words and sentences, following the
logical laws governing written language. It checks for grammatical and morpho-
logical meaning and grasps the factual content. But the right hemisphere inter-
prets a poem as more than a string of words. It integrates the information with
its own prior ideas and imagination, allows images to well up, and recognizes
overarching metaphorical meaning. (Kraft, 2005, p. 20)
Dacey and Lennon (1998) also observed that cerebral activity during tasks of
divergent thinking resembles what happens during a relaxed state of mind. This
would support the importance of an incubation stage (unconscious) that precedes
the conscious production of new ideas and insights (Claxton, 2000).Despite that
the importance of incubation is amply recognized, there is no much understanding
of how it works because it occurs not consciously and cannot be verbalized.
The idea that creative thinking is the result of a process that goes through several
phases (characterized by different degrees of consciousness) has been confirmed
by observations and experiences reported by famous inventors and empirical sci-
entists. Wallas (1926) summarizes this process into four stages of thought:
I then began to study arithmetical questions, apparently without any great result,
and without suspecting that they could have the least connection with my previous
investigations. Disgusted by my lack of success, I went away to spend a few
days at the seaside, and thought about entirely different things. One day, as I was
walking along the edge of the cliff, the idea came to me, again with the same
characteristics of brevity, suddenness, and immediate certainty, that arithmetical
transformations of indefinite ternary quadratic forms are identical with those of
non-Euclidian geometry. (cited in Patrick, 1955, p. 14)
CONCLUSION
Creativity can be inscribed within the broader framework of the evolution of the
brain and social life. During the natural history of mammals, higher cognitive
activities have emerged for a progressive expansion of the cortical areas (especially
frontal and prefrontal cortex), which played a decisive role compared to other sub-
cortical structures such as the basal ganglia. However, the variation-selection pro-
cesses, implicit and explicit, leading to innovation and creativity, would not be
unimaginable without the enormous work of consciousness, with its huge variety
of discriminations (qualia and internal and external sensory phenomena) involved
in the distributed and dynamics activity of the thalamocortical system. The future
research programs on a so crucial function of human evolution are inextricably
linked to the clarification of the relationship between brain and consciousness.
REFERENCES
Amabile, T. M., Barsade, S. G., Mueller, J. S., & Staw, B. M. (2005). Affect and creativity
at work. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50(3), 367403.
Barron, F. (1958). The psychology of imagination. Scientific American, 199, 150166.
Beeman, M. J., & Bowden, E. M. (2000). The right hemisphere maintains solution-related
activation for yet-to-be-solved problems. Memory and Cognition, 28(1231), 1231
1241.
Caan, W., Perrett, D. I., & Rolls, E. T. (1984). Responses of striatal neurons in the behaving
monkey. 2. Visual processing in the caudal neostriatum. Brain Research, 290(1), 53
65.
Claxton, G. (2000). The anatomy of intuition. In T. Atkinson & G. Claxton (Eds.), The
intuitive practitioner: On the value of not always knowing what one is doing (pp. 32
52). Buckingham, England: Open University Press.
Coccurello, R., Adriani, W., Oliverio, A., & Mele, A. (2000). Effect of intra-accumbens
dopamine receptor agents on reactivity to spatial and non-spatial changes in mice.
Psychopharmacology, 152(2), 189199.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2008). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, NY:
Harper and Row.
8 MAURO MALDONATO ET AL.
Dacey, J. S., & Lenno, K. H. (1998). Understanding creativity: The interplay of biological,
social and psychological factors. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
de Bono, E. (1970). Lateral thinking: creativity step by step. New York, NY: Harper and
Row.
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of
creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822848.
Doron, K. W., Bassett, D. S., & Gazzaniga, M. (2012). Dynamic network structure of inter-
hemispheric coordination. PNAS, 109(46), 1862718628.
Fiore, S. M., & Schooler, J. W. (1998). In M. Beeman & C. Chiarello (Eds.), Right hemi-
sphere language comprehension (pp. 349371). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Graybiel, A. M. (1997). The basal ganglia and cognitive pattern generators. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 23(3), 459469.
Guilford, J. P. (1959). Traits of creativity. In H. H. Anderson & M. S. Anderson (Eds.),
Creativity and its cultivation, addresses presented at the Interdisciplinary Symposia
on Creativity (pp. 142161). New York, Harper, East Lansing, NY: Michigan State
University.
Holm-Hadulla, R. M. (2013). The dialectic of creativity: A synthesis of neurobiologi-
cal, psychological, cultural and practical aspects of the creative process. Creativity
Research Journal, 25(3), 293299.
Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (2010). The Cambridge handbook of creativity. Cam-
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Kraft, U. (2005). Unleashing creativity. Scientific American Mind, 16(1), 1623.
Maldonato, M., Montuori, A., & DellOrco, S. (2013). The exploring mind. natural logic
and intelligence of the unconscious. Kindle Edition.
Montuori, A., & Donnelly, G. (2013). Creativity at the opening of the 21st century. Creative
Nursing, 19(2), 5863.
Mumford, M. D., Medeiros, K. E., & Partlow, P. J. (2012). Creative thinking: Processes,
strategies, and knowledge. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(1), 3047.
Oliverio, A. (2008). Brain and creativity. Progress of Theoretical Physics, Suppl., 173, 66
78.
Oliverio, A., & Maldonato, M. (2014, November). The creative brain. CogInfoCom, 5th
IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom),
Vietri sul Mare, Italy.
Patrick, C. (1955). What is creative thinking? Philosophical Library.
Posner, M. I. (2012). Cognitive neuroscience of attention. New York, NY: Guilford.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., Saccamanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, matura-
tion, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 1493114936.
Shena, W., Liua, C., Zhanga, X., Zhaob, X., Zhanga, J., Yuana, Y., & Chena, Y. (2013).
Right hemispheric dominance of creative insight: An event-related potential study.
Creativity Research Journal, 25(1), 4858.
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: The inte-
gration of product, person, and process perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4),
475494.
Sperry, R. W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. Ameri-
can Psychologist, 23(10), 723.
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.