Neuroimaging Self-Esteem: A fMRI Study of Individual Differences in Women
Neuroimaging Self-Esteem: A fMRI Study of Individual Differences in Women
Neuroimaging Self-Esteem: A fMRI Study of Individual Differences in Women
1093/scan/nss032
Department of Psychiatry, 2Department of Psychology, 3Graduate Program in Neuroscience, 4Medical Biophysics, 5Medical Imaging,
Western University, London, ON N6A3K7, Canada 6Department of Medical Imaging, St. Josephs Health Care, and 7Imaging division,
Lawson Health Research Institute, London, ON N6A4V2, Canada
Although neuroimaging studies strongly implicate the medial prefrontal cortex (ventral and dorsal), cingulate gyrus (anterior and posterior), precuneus
and temporoparietal cortex in mediating self-referential processing (SRP), little is known about the neural bases mediating individual differences in
valenced SRP, that is, processes intrinsic to self-esteem. This study investigated the neural correlates of experimentally engendered valenced SRP via
the VisualVerbal Self-Other Referential Processing Task in 20 women with fMRI. Participants viewed pictures of themselves or unknown other women
during separate trials while covertly rehearsing I am or She is, followed by reading valenced trait adjectives, thus variably associating the self/other
with positivity/negativity. Response within dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex, cingulate cortex and left temporoparietal cortex varied with
individual differences in both pre-task rated self-descriptiveness of the words, as well as task-induced affective responses. Results are discussed as they
relate to a social cognitive and affective neuroscience view of self-esteem.
INTRODUCTION
Trait self-esteem, or the tendency to evaluate oneself positively rather
than negatively, is a robust predictor of mental health and well-being
(Baumeister et al., 2003). Neuroimaging studies strongly implicate the
medial prefrontal cortex (ventral and dorsal), cingulate gyrus (anterior
and posterior), precuneus and temporoparietal cortex (reviews by Qin
and Northoff, 2011; Qin et al., 2012a; van der Meer et al., 2010) in
mediating our ability to consciously reflect about ourselves, that is,
self-referential processing (SRP; Northoff et al., 2006). However,
little is known about the neural bases mediating individual differences
in valenced SRP, that is, why most people tend to think about themselves positively, whereas others regard themselves negatively, cognitive
and affective processes that are intrinsic to self-esteem.
Previous studies investigating valenced SRP measured neural response
while healthy participants explicitly judged the self-descriptiveness of
trait adjectives (e.g. liked vs disliked, success vs failure) (Fossati
et al., 2003; Moran et al., 2006; Yoshimura et al., 2009) or self-relevance
of valenced pictures (Phan et al., 2004; Lemogne et al., 2011), tasks not
unlike completing a self-esteem questionnaire within the scanner.
However, healthy individuals typically endorse positive stimuli (e.g. the
words liked, success) as more self-descriptive than negative stimuli
(e.g. disliked, failure) (Mezulis et al., 2004), confounding valence
with self-descriptiveness, and rendering these designs less sensitive to
detecting neural processes mediating negatively valenced SRP.
Limitations inherent to the use of direct survey-based measures of SRP
also include susceptibility to self-presentational biases and the likelihood
that not all valenced self-representations are fully accessible to conscious
reflection (e.g. Gawronski, 2009). To circumvent these concerns, priming
methodologies are increasingly used in experimental social psychology as
indirect measures of associations between valence and self-representation
(reviews by Buhrmester et al., 2011; Zeigler-Hill and Jordan, 2010).
The Author (2012). Published by Oxford University Press. For Permissions, please email: [email protected]
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/), which permits unrestricted
non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Neuroimaging self-esteem
SCAN (2013)
547
Fig. 1 Illustration of one block of the VV-SORP-T. The individual shown in the photograph is the second author. Participants posed for their own photographs in neutral expression as for a passport application.
Photographs of strangers (other-condition) were taken from the NimStim set (Tottenham et al., 2009) and matched to the participant as closely as possible for the following attributes: ethnicity, hair colour and
hair length. Participants viewed the photographs and silently rehearsed I am (for the self) or She is (for the other), and then silently read the words, thus associating the self/other with positivity/negativity on
different trials.
548
SCAN (2013)
P. A. Frewen et al.
processes. Specifically, our prior study observed that individuals with
lower trait self-esteem reported experiencing greater negative affect
during S-N trials, and lesser positive affect during S-P trials (Frewen
and Lundberg, 2012). Results for quantitative ratings collected from
the present sample are presented herein and open-ended comments are
included as Supplementary Table S1.
Procedure
All procedures were approved by the health sciences research ethics
board of Western University in London, Ontario, Canada. As noted
previously, participants were assessed for study inclusion criteria and
completed a short questionnaire battery including the adjective rating
component of the VV-SORP-T 2 weeks prior to scanning.
Participants completed a single-block practice version of the experimental component of the VV-SORP-T in an office setting 30 min
prior to fMRI scanning, and three blocks of the experimental component of the VV-SORP-T while undergoing fMRI. Participants then
rated their affective response to the VV-SORP-T immediately
post-scan. The entire experiment took 75 min to complete.
Imaging took place at the Robarts Research Institute in London,
Ontario, Canada. All imaging data were collected using a 3.0 Tesla
whole-body MRI scanner (Magnetom Tim Trio, Siemens Medical
Solutions, Erlangen, Germany) with the manufacturers 32-channel
phased array head coil. Orthogonal scout images were collected and used
to prescribe a tri-dimensional T1-weighted anatomical image of the whole
head with 1 mm isotropic resolution (MP-RAGE, TR/TE/TI 2300/2.98/
900 ms, flip angle 98, FOV (x, y, z) 256 240 192 mm, acc.
factor 4, total acq. time 3 min 12 s). The anatomical volume was
used to determine the angle of the transverse plane passing through both
the anterior and posterior commeasures mid-sagittaly and as the source
image for inter-individual spatial normalization. A set of 64 contiguous,
2 mm thick imaging planes for BOLD fMRI were prescribed parallel to the
ACPC plane and positioned to ensure coverage of the top of the brain.
BOLD fMRI images were acquired with the manufacturers standard
gradient echo EPI pulse sequence (single-shot blipped EPI) using an interleaved slice acquisition order and tri-dimensional prospective acquisition
correction (3D-PACE). EPI volumes were acquired with 2 mm isotropic
resolution and the following parameters: FOV 192 192 mm, 94 94
matrix, TR/TE 3000/20 ms, flip angle 908, 64 slices, 178 measurements.
Before completing the VV-SORP-T while undergoing fMRI a resting-state
functional scan of each participants brain was also acquired, to be
described elsewhere.
Data preparation and statistical analysis
Across blocks and runs for each of the four experimental conditions
(S-N, S-P, O-N, O-P), VV-SORP-T survey scores were summed, and
button-press RT and affect ratings were averaged. The effect of experimental condition on each of these variables was examined by ANOVA
with results reported in Table 1.
Analyses of the BOLD signal were conducted via SPM8 (Welcome
Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College, London,
UK). Standard preprocessing was conducted within SPM8, with volumes realigned to the first functional image acquired (unidirectional
movements were <4 mm from origin in all cases), normalized to a
common EPI template [rendering 2 mm3 voxels in accordance with
the coordinate system of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI)],
and data smoothed across 8 mm (FWHM). A canonical haemodynamic response function was modelled as a response to each
stimulus in individual participants (first-level), with group-averaged
results evaluated as random effects (second-level). The BOLD response
observed during each of the four task trials relative to between-block
fixation was examined via the general linear model. Planned contrasts
Neuroimaging self-esteem
SCAN (2013)
549
Table 1 Descriptive statistics and paired comparisons between conditions of the VV-SORP-T
Survey
NA
PA
RT
S-N
S-P
O-N
O-P
S-N vs S-P
M (s.d.)
M (s.d.)
M (s.d.)
M (s.d.)
d0
d0
d0
d0
3.95
15.53
14.34
758
86.79
2.84
75.26
774
8.05
15.05
6.97
732
72.79
2.32
33.55
759
28.99
3.77
10.64
0.62
6.48
0.86
2.44
0.14
2.45
0.11
2.34
1.55
0.55
0.02
0.54
0.35
4.64
0.40
5.26
0.67
1.04
0.09
1.21
0.15
13.96
4.05
4.37
1.56
3.12
0.93
1.00
0.35
(6.50)
(19.30)
(17.75)
(291)
(10.05)
(9.32)
(24.12)
(302)
(10.37)
(15.53)
(13.71)
(283)
(14.88)
(5.97)
(31.31)
(303)
S-N vs O-N
S-P vs O-P
O-N vs O-P
n 19 (one subject was missing affect rating data). Degrees of freedom are thus for multivariate ANOVA F(4,15), univariate ANOVA F(1,18) and for post hoc paired t-tests t(18). For paired comparisons between
conditions, the effect size d0 is noted. Multivariate tests were statistically significant for Reference, F(4,15) 11.56, P < 0.001, 2 < 0.76, Valence, F(4,15) 139.05, P < 0.001, 2 0.97 and
Reference-x-Valence, F(4,15) 8.14, P 0.001, 2 0.69. For survey endorsement, the results of univariate ANOVA were: Reference, F(1,18) 11.79, P 0.003, 2 0.40, Valence, F(1,18) 458.64,
P < 0.001, 2 0.96 and Reference-x-Valence, F(1,18) 20.47, P < 0.001 2 0.53. For negative affect (NA) ratings, the results of univariate ANOVA were: Reference, F(1,18) 0.04, P 0.85, 2 < 0.01,
Valence, F(1,18) 25.11, P < 0.001, 2 0.58 and Reference-x-Valence, F(1,18) 0.00, P 0.99, 2 < 0.01. For positive affect (PA) ratings, the results of univariate ANOVA were: Reference, F(1,18) 27.31,
P < 0.001, 2 0.60, Valence, F(1,18) 92.89, P < 0.001, 2 0.84 and Reference-x-Valence, F(1,18) 20.61, P < 0.001, 2 0.53. Finally, for RT (ms), the results of univariate ANOVA were: Reference,
F(1,18) 3.04, P 0.10, 2 0.14, Valence, F(1,18) 1.73, P 0.21, 2 0.09 and Reference-x-Valence, F(1,18) 0.18, P 0.68, 2 0.01.
Regions
kP<0.005
kSVC
Posterior mid-cingulate
Right superior parietal cortex
DMPFC
MPFC
Left middle frontal gyrus
Right DLPFC
Right temporal pole
Right posterior insula
Left posterior insula
Right middle frontal gyrus
Left middle frontal gyrus
Left middle frontal gyrus
Left precentral gyrus
Left precentral gyrus
Left posterior mid-cingulate
Left cuneus
Left cuneus
Posterior mid-cingulate
Right superior parietal cortex
No significant results
No significant results
No significant results
204
210
121
97
151
85
188
168
83
87
283
267
503
110
70
204
124
67
88
66a
32b
28c
132d
4.81
4.21
3.77
3.56
3.54
3.84
3.47
3.87
3.14
3.67
3.67
3.38
3.64
3.36
3.35
3.20
3.00
3.76
3.51
4
50
2
2
22
56
52
38
44
56
44
42
50
28
14
24
10
4
46
28
52
22
64
58
16
18
22
16
34
52
14
18
20
28
74
72
28
52
36
40
42
6
16
10
28
8
2
10
16
22
36
50
42
30
10
36
44
All ROIs were prescribed from Moran et al. (2006). aAt ROI 3, 19, 38, PSVC < 0.01.
ROI 3, 19, 38, PSVC < 0.01.
At ROI 50, 24, 10, PSVC 0.02.
d
At ROI 56, 15, 10, PSVC < 0.02.
b
At
c
O-P trials (d0 1.21), although negative affect was not significantly
higher during S-N than O-N trials (d0 0.02). Finally, RT was
marginally slower during S-N than O-N trials (d0 0.35), and during
S-P than O-P trials (d0 0.15).
Further replicating previous results (Frewen and Lundberg, 2012),
participants who described themselves more positively (S-P survey endorsement) experienced less negative affect during S-N trials (r 0.77,
P < 0.001), less negative affect during S-P trials (r 0.57, P 0.006)
and greater positive affect during S-P trials (r 0.52, P 0.011). In
comparison, associations between S-N survey endorsement and affective
responses were non-significant.
fMRI-BOLD response
Group-level differences between trial types
Figure 2 and Table 2 report significant responses observed as specific to
each of the four distinct trial types at the group level in comparison
with between-block fixation (in Figure 2, S-N red, S-P green,
550
SCAN (2013)
P. A. Frewen et al.
Fig. 2 BOLD response during the four conditions of the VV-SORP-T vs baseline fixation. Response during S-N trials is shown in red, during S-P trials in green, during O-N trials in magenta, during O-P trials in
yellow. Voxel-wise P < 0.005 with a cluster threshold k 67 voxels.
Predictors
Direction of correlation
Regions
kP<0.005
26b
4.10
3.51
3.40
3.14
12
22
2
42
48
54
28
26
6
2
12
10
S-N Survey
L-MPFC
Cerebellum
VACC/VMPFC
L-IFG
No significant results
S-N
NA
S-N Survey
108
82
68
81
113
67
93
61c
4.37
3.91
3.58
3.43
3.39
3.37
4.08
2
18
52
10
58
44
16
10
2
20
36
42
40
32
54
0
6
2
0
28
6
R amygdalaparahippocampal gyrus
L superior temporal gyrus
L occipital cortex
L post-central gyrus (wm)
R fusiform gyrus
81
309
70
67
87
3.93
3.85
3.78
3.68
3.60
20
62
24
22
18
4
0
48
24
72
22
8
2
34
10
S-N
NA
168
79
78
67
kSVC
O-N magenta, O-P yellow). S-N trials activated three clusters: the
posterior mid-cingulate (at ROI 0, 13, 31, Moran et al., 2006;
kSVC 66), right superior parietal cortex and dorsal ACC-MPFC (at
ROI 3, 19, 38, Moran et al., 2006; kSVC 32). S-P trials activated two
clusters: ventral MPFC and left middle frontal cortex. O-P trials activated two clusters: right DLPFC and right temporal pole. Finally,
response during O-N trials was more distributed, with the maximal
effect size observed within the right posterior insula, and additional
activations observed within the left posterior insula, right middle frontal gyrus (at ROI 50, 24, 10, Moran et al., 2006; kSVC 28), left middle
frontal gyrus (at ROI 56, 15, 10, Moran et al., 2006; kSVC 132), left
precentral gyrus, left posterior mid-cingulate and left cuneus.
Neuroimaging self-esteem
SCAN (2013)
551
Predictors
Direction of correlation
Regions
kP<0.005
kSVC
S-P Survey
R precuneus
L middle frontal gyrus (wm)
L post-central gyrus
L superior temporal gyrus
R amygdalaparahippocampal gyrus
R temporal pole
R temporal pole
85
91
143
107
148
119
102
3.92
3.60
3.44
3.26
3.67
3.53
4.51
18
22
34
48
36
58
62
42
40
32
14
0
6
6
60
18
58
34
20
18
16
DMPFC
L-TPJ (wm)
L-TPJ
L posterior insula (wm)
L post-central gyrus
L-superior frontal gyrus
No significant results
No significant results
R-DMPFC
L-TPJ
VMPFC
R-IFG
L-IFG
R-temporal pole
L-IFG
VMPFC
237
104
102
104
117
163
88
74
456
118
145
124
91
73
87a
48b
18c
22d
48e
20f
3.84
3.75
3.69
3.65
3.65
3.51
4.38
4.28
4.26
3.97
3.81
3.37
3.36
4.28
6
36
50
24
14
14
14
54
4
38
36
40
20
2
50
60
54
22
24
40
30
56
32
58
50
0
40
30
20
14
18
12
66
52
46
32
26
2
4
20
14
24
DMPFC
L-TPJ
L-IFG
R-caudate
Precuneus
L-cerebellum
VMPFC
229
108
169
159
186
75
95
35g
31h
4.10
4.08
4.03
3.75
3.70
3.34
2.93
12
52
20
10
2
16
8
32
60
24
14
60
62
52
46
16
16
4
38
34
20
+
S-P
PA
S-P Survey
S-P
PA
552
SCAN (2013)
P. A. Frewen et al.
Fig. 3 BOLD response during S-N trials vs baseline fixation (BL) and O-N trials. Within the legend, positive correlations are denoted with a plus symbol; there were no significant negative correlations. Positive
correlation between survey endorsement of negative traits and response during S-N trials (>BL) is shown in red. Positive correlation between survey endorsement of negative traits and response during S-N
trials (>O-N trials) is shown in magenta. Positive correlation between experienced negative affect and response during S-N trials (>O-N trials) is shown in blue. Voxel-wise P < 0.005 with a cluster threshold
k 67 voxels.
Fig. 4 BOLD response during S-P trials vs baseline fixation (BL) and O-P trials. Within the legend, positive correlations are denoted with a plus symbol, and negative correlations are denoted with a minus
symbol. Regarding survey endorsement of positive traits and response during S-P trials (>BL), positive correlations are shown in green and negative correlations are shown in red. Positive correlation between
experienced positive affect and response during S-P trials (>BL) is shown in yellow. Negative correlation between survey endorsement of positive traits and response during S-P trials (>O-P trials) is shown in
magenta. Positive correlation between experienced positive affect and response during S-P trials (>O-P trials) is shown in cyan. Voxel-wise P < 0.005 with a cluster threshold k 67 voxels.
Neuroimaging self-esteem
participants experienced during S-P trials, the greater was their
response within many of the same regions, specifically VMPFC (at
ROI 3, 36, 18, Phan et al., 2004; kSVC 20), DMPFC (at ROI 6,
27, 42, Lemogne et al., 2011; kSVC 35), left TPJ (within ROI 52,
56, 22, van Overwalle, 2009; kSVC 31), left inferior frontal gyrus, as
well as within the precuneus.
DISCUSSION
How people represent themselves in comparison with others, and the
role played by affective processing in such representations, are matters
of significant interest to a social cognitive and affective neuroscience of
core personality constructs including trait self-esteem. We investigated
the neural correlates of self-esteem-related processes in response to the
VV-SORP-T using an individual differences design.
Although recent meta-analyses confirm greater response within
MPFC, perigenual ACC and PCC during SRP than during ORP (van
der Meer et al., 2010; Qin and Northoff, 2011; Qin et al., 2012b),
individual studies using relatively neutral adjectives rarely observe
this effect (e.g. Ochsner et al., 2005; cf Heatherton et al., 2006; Yaoi
et al., 2009). Yoshimura et al. (2009), using valenced adjectives, similarly observed few differences between SRP and ORP. Within the
present study, the spatial maps obtained relative to fixation differed
between valenced SRP and ORP (Figure 2), while null effects were
observed when conditions were directly compared, as conducted by
Yoshimura et al. (2009). We speculate that the neural correlates of SRP
and ORP will differ principally in so far as SRP is regarded as more
affectively salient (e.g. Tacikowski et al., 2011). In other words, we
expect that trait endorsement must not only differentiate the self
from others, but this result must sufficiently matter to participants
to evoke corresponding differences in the BOLD signal. The use of
valenced adjectives as in Yoshimura et al.s (2009) study and the present one cannot assure this because, as was clearly the case in the
present study, most participants will endorse robustly positive views
of both themselves and others, leading trials requiring negative SRP
and ORP to be regarded as relatively neutral and irrelevant (Frewen
and Lundberg, 2012).
In contrast to the modal self-positivity bias, however, a certain
number of participants with lower self-esteem will endorse relatively
negative views of themselves and a corresponding range of affective
responses when such representations are primed such as via the
VV-SORP-T. Consistent with expectations, in the present study these
individual differences dovetailed considerably with between-subject
variability in the BOLD response within ROIs including MPFC,
PCC, left temporoparietal cortex and right amygdala. These potentials
for heterogeneity across subjects in experiential response to a common
stimulus pattern make them well suited to the study of the neural
correlates of personality and individual differences (Varela, 1996;
Lutz and Thompson, 2003). Further knowledge about the neural
underpinnings of negative SRP may also enlighten our understanding
of psychiatric disorders associated with maladaptive SRP including
depression (Grimm et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2009; Lemogne et al.,
2009) and post-traumatic stress disorder (Frewen et al., 2011).
The present findings are consistent with and further inform current
theorizing about the neural correlates of the emotional self (Fossati
et al., 2003). In particular, it has been posited that ventral MPFC
(inclusive of ventral ACC; Moran et al., 2006; Yoshimura et al.,
2009) may be particularly associated with SRP that is affectively (and
perhaps uniquely negatively) salient, whereas dorsal MPFC may be
particularly involved in conscious, reflective processes that are either
neutral or positive in nature (van der Meer et al., 2010; Heatherton,
2011). Consistent with previous observations, relative to fixation,
we observed ventral MPFC/ACC response during negative SRP
SCAN (2013)
553
554
SCAN (2013)
REFERENCES
Balota, D.A., Yap, M.J., Cortese, M.J., et al. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior
Research Methods, 39, 44559.
Baumeister, R.F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., Vohs, K.D. (2001). Bad is stronger than
good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 32370.
Baumeister, R.F., Campbell, J.D., Krueger, J.I., Vohs, K.D. (2003). Does high self-esteem
cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles?
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 144.
Berthoz, S., Artiges, E., Van de Moortele, P.-F., et al. (2002). Effect of impaired recognition
and expression of emotions on frontocingulate cortices: an fMRI study of men with
alexithymia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 9617.
Buhrmester, M.D., Blanton, H., Swann, W.B. (2011). Implicit self-esteem: nature, measurement, and a new way forward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(2),
36585.
Bradley, M.M., Lang, P.J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW): Instruction
Manual and affective ratings. Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in
Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
Briere, J. (2000). Cognitive Distortion Scales: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL, US:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
P. A. Frewen et al.
Britton, J.C., Phan, K.L., Taylor, S.F., Welsh, R.C., Berridge, K.C., Liberzon, I. (2006).
Neural correlates of social and nonsocial emotions: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 31,
397409.
Fossati, P., Hevenor, S.J., Graham, S.J., et al. (2003). In search of the emotional self: an
fMRI study using positive and negative emotional words. The American Journal of
Psychiatry, 160, 193845.
Frewen, P.A., Dean, J., Lanius, R.A. (2012a). Assessment of anhedonia in psychological
trauma: development of the Hedonic Deficit and Interference Scale. European Journal of
Psychotraumatology, 3, 8585.
Frewen, P.A., Dozois, D.J.A., Lanius, R.A. (2012b). Assessment of anhedonia in psychological trauma: psychometric and neuroimaging perspectives. European Journal of
Psychotraumatology, 3, 8587.
Frewen, P.A., Dozois, D.J.A., Neufeld, R.W.J., Densmore, M., Stevens, T.K., Lanius, R.
(2010). Neuroimaging social emotional processing in women: fMRI study of
script-driven imagery. Social Cognitive Affective Neuroscience, 6, 37592.
Frewen, P.A., Dozois, D.J.A., Neufeld, R.W.J., Densmore, M., Stevens, T., Lanius, R.A.
(2011). Self-referential processing in women with PTSD related to childhood abuse:
affective and neural response. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and
Policy, 3, 31728.
Frewen, P.A., Lundberg, E. (2012). VisualVerbal Self/Other-Referential Processing Task:
direct vs. indirect assessment, valence, and experiential correlates. Personality and
Individual Difference, 52(4), 50914.
Gawronski, B. (2009). Ten frequently asked questions about implicit measures and their
frequently supposed, but not entirely correct answers. Canadian Psychology, 50(3),
14150.
Grimm, S., Jutta, E., Boesiger, P., et al. (2009). Increased self-focus in major depressive
disorder is related to neural abnormalities in subcortical-cortical midline structures.
Human Brain Mapping, 30, 261727.
Harvey, P.O., Pruessner, J., Czechowska, Y., Lepage, M. (2007). Individual differences in
trait anhedonia: a structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study in
non-clinical subjects. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 76775.
Heatherton, T.F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of
Psychology, 62, 36390.
Heatherton, T.F., Wyland, C.L., Macrae, C.N., Demos, K.E., Denny, B.T., Kelley, W.M.
(2006). Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Social Cognitive
and Affective Neuroscience, 1(1), 1825.
Hyde, J.S., Mezulis, A.H., Abramson, L.Y. (2008). The ABCs of depression: integrating
affective, biological and cognitive models to explain the emergence of the gender
difference in depression. Psychological Review, 115, 291313.
Johnson, M.K., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Mitchell, K.J., Levin, Y. (2009). Medial cortex activity,
self-reflection and depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 31327.
Keedwell, P.A., Andrew, C., Williams, S.C.R., Brammer, M.J., Phillips, M.L. (2005). The
neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 58,
84353.
Kelley, W.M., Macrae, C.N., Wyland, C.L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., Heatherton, T.F. (2002).
Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5),
78594.
Lemogne, C., Gorwood, P., Bergouingnan, L., Pelissolo, A., Lehericy, S., Fossato, P. (2011).
Negative affectivity, self-referential processing and the cortical midline structures. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neurosceince, 6, 42633.
Lemogne, C., le Bastard, G., Mayberg, H., et al. (2009). In search of the depressive self:
extended medial prefrontal network during self-referential processing in major depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(3), 30512.
Lieberman, M.D., Cunningham, W.A. (2009). Type I and type II error concerns in fMRI
research: re-balancing the scale. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 4238.
Loumann, I.M., Danforth, C.M., Harris, K.D., Bliss, C.A., Dodds, P.S. (2012). Positivity of
the English language. PLoS One, 7, e29484.
Lund, K., Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical
co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 28, 2038.
Lutz, A., Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: integrating subjective experience
and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness
Studies, 910, 3152.
Mezulis, A.H., Abramson, L.Y., Hyde, J.S., Hankin, B.L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and
cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5),
71147.
Moran, J.M., Heatherton, T.F., Kelley, W.M. (2009). Modulation of cortical midline structure by implicit and explicit self-relevance evaluation. Social Neuroscience, 4(3), 197211.
Moran, J.M., Macrae, C.N., Heatherton, T.F., Wyland, C.L., Kelley, W.M. (2006).
Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective components of self.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 158694.
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., Panksepp, J.
(2006). Self-referential processing in our brain-a meta-analysis of imaging studies on
the self. NeuroImage, 31, 44057.
Northoff, G., Qin, P., Nakao, T. (2010). Rest-stimulus interaction in the brain: a review.
Trends in Neuroscience, 33, 27784.
Neuroimaging self-esteem
Ochsner, K.N., Beer, J.S., Robertson, E.R., et al. (2005). The neural correlates of direct and
reflected self-knowledge. NeuroImage, 28(4), 797814.
Phan, K.L., Taylor, S.F., Welsh, R.C., Ho, S.H., Britton, J.C., Liberzon, I. (2004). Neural
correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study.
NeuroImage, 21(2), 76880.
Qin, P., Liu, Y., Shi, J., et al. (2012a). Anterior cingulate activity and the self in disorders of
consciousness. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 19932002.
Qin, P., Liu, Y., Shi, J., et al. (2012b). Dissociation between anterior and posterior cortical
regions during self-specificity and familiarity: a combined fMRI-meta-analytic study.
Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 15464.
Qin, P., Northoff, G. (2011). How is our self related to midline regions and the
default-mode network? NeuroImage, 57(3), 122133.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Tacikowski, P., Brechmann, A., Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K., Dobrowolny, M.,
Nowicka, A. (2011). Is it about the self or the significance? An fMRI study of self-name
recognition. Social Neuroscience, 6(1), 98107.
SCAN (2013)
555
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J.W., Leon, A.C., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions:
judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 2429.
van der Meer, L., Costafreda, S., Aleman, A., David, A.S. (2010). Self-reflection and the
brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications
for schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 93546.
Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Human Brain
Mapping, 30(3), 82958.
Varela, F.J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem.
Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3(4), 33050.
Yaoi, K., Osaka, N., Osaka, M. (2009). Is the self special in the dorsomedial prefrontal
cortex? An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 4(5), 45563.
Yoshimura, S., Ueda, K., Suzuki, S., Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., Yamawaki, S. (2009).
Self-referential processing of negative stimuli within the ventral cingulate gyrus and
right amygdala. Brain and Cognition, 69, 21825.
Zeigler-Hill, V., Jordan, C.H. (2010). Two faces of self-esteem: implicit and explicit forms
of self-esteem. In: Gawronski, B., Payne, K., editors. Handbook of Implicit Social
Cognition. NewYork, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 392407.