Tensiones Inducidas
Tensiones Inducidas
Tensiones Inducidas
doi:10.1093/beheco/arr169
Advance Access publication 11 October 2011
Original Article
Systematic deviations in sex ratio may be adaptive in the face of the prevailing environmental or physiological cues experienced
by the mother; yet some theoretical and mechanistic hypotheses remain at odds and are rarely examined noninvasively under
natural conditions. Conventional interpretations of the Trivers and Willard (Trivers RL, Willard DE. 1973. Natural selection of
parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 179:9092) hypothesis (TWH) predict that higher stress should be
associated with female-biased litters; yet, several mechanistic hypotheses predict the opposite. We tested the predictions of the
TWH and 2 stress-related mechanistic hypotheses in a free-living polygynandrous sciurid, Richardsons ground squirrel, Urocitellus
richardsonii. We examined the relationship between litter sex ratio and indicators of maternal condition and investment. These
included litter size, juvenile mass at emergence, as well as maternal age, changes in maternal body mass, and maternal fecal
glucocorticoid (cortisol and corticosterone) levels during gestation, lactation, and post-weaning. Males born of small litters were
significantly heavier at emergence than males from larger litters, which were female biased, whereas females showed no significant change in mass with litter size. Mothers with higher fecal cortisol levels during the gestation period (but not during
lactation or post-weaning) were more likely to produce male-biased litters, whereas females producing larger litters showed
significantly higher cortisol levels during lactation (but not gestation or post-weaning) than mothers producing smaller litters.
Our results provide support for both the proximate glucose-mediated mechanistic model of Cameron (Cameron EZ. 2004.
Facultative adjustment of mammalian sex ratios in support of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: evidence for a mechanism. Proc R
Soc Ser B Biol Sci. 271:17231728) and the broad-sense TWH, where social and life-history characteristics play an integral role.
Key words: advantaged matriline, glucocorticoids, litter size, maternal condition, sex ratio, stress, Urocitellus richardsonii. [Behav
Ecol 23:160167 (2012)]
INTRODUCTION
kewed sex ratios at birth, weaning, or fledging in vertebrates
have been attributed to the differential costs of successfully
rearing offspring of a given sex and to condition-dependent
differences in the relative reproductive value of each sex
(Hardy 1997). In polygynous mammals, the fitness returns
associated with high-quality male offspring may exceed those
derived from producing high-quality female offspring because
of the huge variability in male reproductive success. As a result,
females with the greatest capacity for investment are predicted
to bear more male offspring, whereas the opposite should be
true for poorer quality females (Trivers and Willard 1973).
Among mammals, the costs for producing male versus female
offspring in any given species are often difficult to quantify
(Frank 1990), but the immediate energetic investment into
male offspring is ordinarily greater because of their higher
developmental rates, larger size, and greater nutritional demands (Gomendio et al. 1990; Redondo et al. 1992; but see
Koskela et al. 2009).
Despite evidence for sex ratio manipulation in mammals,
the study of adaptive sex allocation (ASA) is plagued by incon-
Ryan et al.
2003) and can orchestrate entire physiological systems, influencing the secretion and activity of other hormones (Ketterson
et al. 1991).
Baseline glucocorticoid (or GC, cortisol, and corticosterone) levels can provide an indication of individual state or
condition by quantifying the physiological effort to sustain
homeostatic balance in the face of social or environmental
perturbations (Palme et al. 2005). Stressors associated with
increases in baseline GC levels (such as overcrowding, resource limitation, or intraspecific competition) also match
the conditions predicted to lead to the adaptive manipulation
of offspring sex ratio (Cameron 2004). Furthermore, GCs
demonstrate marked inhibitory effects on reproductive physiology and behavior, as well as parental investment (Wingfield
and Sapolsky 2003), providing a critical link between the conditions favoring ASA and the physiological and behavioral
mechanism for accomplishing sex ratio manipulation. Indeed, measures of social or environmental stress have been
linked to ASA (Pratt and Lisk 1989; Gotz et al. 2008; Love and
Williams 2008; Ideta et al. 2009); however, the actual role of
GCs themselves in sex ratio manipulation remains poorly understood, particularly in free-living mammalian populations.
Among the proposed mechanisms for sex ratio manipulation in mammals (likely to differ from those operating in birds
due to fundamental differences in sex determination), 2 in
particular suggest an indirect role for the stress axis and GCs
(Cameron 2004; Grant 2007). Both models make predictions
that appear to conflict with those most commonly associated
with the Trivers and Willard hypothesis (TWH; i.e., that
stressed females in poor condition with high GC levels should
produce a greater proportion of females). The question that
remains, therefore, is to what extent these mechanistic models
are empirically supported and if they can be reconciled in the
face of what appear to be the contrary predictions of conventional sex ratio theory.
To address these questions, we quantified the relationships
between measures of maternal quality (age, spring mass, and
gain in mass over active season), maternal investment (litter
size, sex ratio, and offspring size), and stress or allostatic
load (in the form of fecal GC levels) in free-living Richardsons ground squirrels (Urocitellus richardsonii). Traditional
interpretations of sex ratio manipulation (so called narrowsense TWH; Cockburn et al. 2002) predict that poor quality
females with the lowest capacity to invest (i.e., the lightest,
most stressed, or the youngest) should produce female-biased litters. Under these conditions, high GC levels would also
be predicted to be associated with female-biased litters and
potentially with other aspects of female condition (e.g., age,
mass; Clutton-Brock and Iason 1986).
On the other hand, support for both mechanistic hypotheses
requires that GC levels be negatively associated with sex ratio
(i.e., the ratio of males to the total number of offspring produced within a litter). The mechanism proposed by Cameron
(2004) predicts that sex ratio will be positively associated with
litter size, due to the differential mortality of developing female embryos, whereas the model of Grant and Irwin (2005)
does not predict this relationship. The novelty of this study is
that it tests the predictions of both mechanistic (proximate)
and adaptive (ultimate) hypotheses using noninvasive techniques under ecologically relevant conditions.
Our study species, Richardsons ground squirrels, are semifossorial, hibernating colonial rodents found in the northern
plains and intermontane valleys of North America (Michener
and Koeppl 1985). Following emergence from hibernation,
males compete intensely, often fatally, for mating opportunities (Michener 1983b). Although all females are typically inseminated in a given year, many males do not successfully
reproduce (Michener and Locklear 1990). This single annual
161
162
Behavioral Ecology
Ryan et al.
163
RESULTS
Age-effects on maternal condition and offspring production
To examine the potential for age-related effects on maternal
condition, we compared age classes separately for spring emergence mass and weight gain over the summer. We were also interested if another measure of maternal condition, GC levels
are affected by the age of the female. Yearling females
emerged significantly lighter (259.5 6 12.5 g) than two 2year-olds or females 3 years and older (325.0 6 15.3 g and
329.9 6 11.5 g, respectively; F2,14 9.85, P 0.002), but there
were no age-related differences in weight gain from spring to
prehibernation (F2,12 0.481, P 0.629).
We assessed potential age-related effects on investment in
the form of litter size, sex ratio, and the mean of juvenile
masses. Neither litter size (F2,18 0.974, P 0.397), sex ratio
(F2,18 0.0145, P 0.986) nor mean juvenile mass (F2,17
0.0747, P 0.928) differed significantly among the 3 age
classes. Furthermore, age class had no significant effect on
cortisol or corticosterone levels for any of the 3 periods (gestation, lactation, or post-weaning; P . 0.190 in all cases).
Age-independent maternal condition and offspring
production
We considered the associations between parameters of maternal investment, including litter size and sex ratio, juvenile mass,
and maternal seasonal changes in mass, to examine the effects
of maternal condition on litter production and vice versa.
Independent of age class, spring emergence mass was significantly correlated with subsequent litter size, with heavier mothers tending to produce larger litters (P 0.019; Table 1).
Although mothers producing larger litters exhibited neither
greater nor lesser gains in mass over the active season (P
0.276; Table 1), weight gain was significantly related to litter
sex ratio; mothers that produced more males gained more
mass over the summer (P 0.038; Table 1).
Individual litter sex ratio varied considerably and was inversely correlated with the number of offspring in the litter
(P 0.0496; Table 1). A pooled analysis of juvenile masses at
emergence (2931 days old) revealed significant differences
between males and females (MannWhitney U 1543, nm
73, nf 60, P 0.003), with males significantly heavier than
juvenile females at emergence (96.5 6 2.2 g versus 88.9 6
2.2 g). Male mass decreased significantly (Spearmans
q 20.359, df 54, P 0.007) with increases in litter size,
whereas female mass did not (Figure 1).
GCs, maternal condition, and offspring production
To test the predictions of both the mechanistic and the theoretical hypotheses, we quantified the association between ma-
Figure 1
Relationship between litter size and emergence mass for Urocitellus
richardsonii juveniles. Emergence mass was not related to litter size for
females (degrees of freedom [df] 64, P 0.366; grey circles, solid
grey line), but males from larger litters were significantly lighter than
those from smaller litters (Spearmans q 20.359, df 54, P
0.007; black diamonds, broken black line). Number of observations
for each sex represented by symbol size, between 1 and 5
observations. Least-squares regression lines are based on parametric
correlations (not used due to the data distribution) but are provided
for illustrative purposes.
Table 1
Relationship between indicators of maternal condition and litter composition in Urocitellus richardsonii
Variable
By variable
Correlation (r)
Lower 95%
Upper 95%
Probability
Mass gain
Litter size
Litter size
Sex ratio
Sex ratio
Sex ratio
Spring mass
Spring mass
Mass gain
Spring mass
Mass gain
Litter size
16
18
16
18
16
22
20.290
0.548
20.286
20.339
0.522
20.423
20.687
0.109
20.685
20.696
0.036
20.717
0.240
0.808
0.244
0.152
0.809
20.002
0.2756
*0.0186
0.2822
0.1690
*0.0381
*0.0496
Measures of maternal condition approximate the time of conception (spring mass) and the gain in mass between spring mass and descent into
hibernation (mass gain). Correlations estimated by residual maximum likelihood method, with significance (P , 0.05) indicated by an asterisk (*).
Behavioral Ecology
164
Figure 2
Relationship between fecal cortisol levels of gestating Urocitellus
richardsonii females (n 21) and the sex ratio of the subsequent litter
emerging from the maternal burrow. Generalized log-transformed
(Glog) data fit a normal distribution and showed a significant
relationship with litter sex ratio (r2 0.19, P 0.049; y 0.16[GlogMeanCortisol] 1 0.56).
Figure 3
Relationship between fecal cortisol levels of female Urocitellus
richardsonii (n 22) during lactation and the number of offspring in
the litter at juvenile emergence. Generalized log-transformed (Glog)
cortisol values showed a significant relationship with litter size (r2
0.27, P 0.014; y 1.84[Glog-MeanCortisol] 1 6.00).
Table 2
Relationship between cortisol and corticosterone levels during distinct reproductive phases (gestation, lactation, and post-weaning) and
reproductive allocation (sex ratio and litter size) in Urocitellus richardsonii
Variable
Gestation
Mean (Glog CORT)
Sex ratio
Sex ratio
Litter size
Litter size
Lactation
Mean (Glog CORT)
Sex ratio
Sex ratio
Litter size
Litter size
Post-weaning
Mean (Glog CORT)
Sex ratio
Sex ratio
Litter size
Litter size
By variable
Correlation (r)
Lower 95%
Upper 95%
Probability
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
Cortisol)
Cortisol)
CORT)
Cortisol)
CORT)
21
21
21
21
21
0.607
0.435
0.174
20.184
0.132
0.238
0.004
20.278
20.570
20.318
0.823
0.729
0.564
0.269
0.534
*0.0035
*0.0490
0.4502
0.4249
0.5678
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
Cortisol)
Cortisol)
CORT)
Cortisol)
CORT)
22
22
22
22
22
0.845
20.200
20.226
0.549
0.266
0.657
20.574
20.592
0.166
20.175
0.934
0.242
0.216
0.788
0.619
*,.0001
0.4339
0.3269
*0.0143
0.1186
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
(Glog
Cortisol)
Cortisol)
CORT)
Cortisol)
CORT)
21
21
21
21
21
0.555
20.179
0.064
0.299
20.098
0.162
20.567
20.378
20.153
20.508
0.796
0.274
0.482
0.647
0.348
*0.0090
0.4367
0.7832
0.1884
0.6719
Correlations were determined by residual maximum likelihood method using generalized log-transformed (Glog) hormonal data (Cortisol
cortisol and CORT corticosterone) from fecal GC extractions. Significance (P , 0.050) is indicated by an asterisk (*).
Ryan et al.
165
Behavioral Ecology
166
FUNDING
Discovery Grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering
Research Council of Canada (#311909 to W.G.A., #154271 to
J.F.H.).
The authors thank Ffion Cassidy and Lindsay Skyner for their contributions in the field and lab, and Steve Whyard, Spencer Sealy, and Tony
Williams for helpful comments on the manuscript. This manuscript was
also greatly improved by the insightful comments of the editor, Candy
Rowe, and one anonymous reviewer. Graphical assistance from Kristen
B. Gorman and Sebastian A. Pardo were greatly appreciated. We also
thank the management and staff of the Assiniboine Park Zoo for allowing access to the squirrels, and Mr Scott Gray of the Zoological Society
of Manitoba for providing access to a freezer and space for equipment
storage at the Zoo.
REFERENCES
Altmann J, Hausfater G, Altmann S. 1988. Determinants of reproductive
success in savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus. In: Clutton-Brock
T, editor. Reproductive success: studies of individual variation in
contrasting breeding systems. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago
Press. p. 403418.
Altmann M, Altmann J. 1991. Models of status-correlated bias in offspring sex ratio. Am Nat. 137:542555.
Austad SN, Sunquist ME. 1986. Sex-ratio manipulation in the common opossum. Nature. 324:5860.
Bauman DE, Currie BW. 1980. Partitioning of nutrients during pregnancy and lactation: a review of mechanisms involving homeostasis
and homeorhesis. J Dairy Sci. 63:15141529.
Boonstra R, Barker JM, Castillo J, Fletcher QE. 2007. The role of the
stress axis in life-history adaptations. In: Wolff J, Sherman PW, editors. Rodent societies: an ecological & evolutionary perspective.
Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press. p. 139149.
Broussard DR, Michener GR, Dobson FS. 2006. Age-specific resource
investment strategies: evidence from female Richardsons ground
squirrels (Spermophilus richardsonii). J Zool. 268:389394.
Broussard DR, Michener GR, Risch TS, Dobson FS. 2005. Somatic
senescence: evidence from Richardsons ground squirrels. Oikos.
108:591601.
Cameron EZ. 2004. Facultative adjustment of mammalian sex ratios in
support of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: evidence for a mechanism.
Proc R Soc Ser B Biol Sci. 271:17231728.
Cameron EZ, Lemons PR, Bateman PW, Bennett NC. 2008. Experimental alteration of litter sex ratios in a mammal. Proc R Soc Ser B
Biol Sci. 275:323327.
Charnov EL. 1982. The theory of sex allocation. Princeton (NJ):
Princeton University Press.
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE. 1984. Maternal dominance, breeding success and birth sex ratios in red deer. Nature.
308:358360.
Clutton-Brock TH, Iason GR. 1986. Sex ratio variation in mammals.
Q Rev Biol. 61:339374.
Cockburn A, Legge S, Double MC. 2002. Sex ratios in birds and
mammals: can the hypotheses be disentangled? In: Hardy IC, editor. Sex ratios: concepts and research methods. Cambridge (UK):
Cambridge University Press. p. 267286.
Dama MS, Singh NMP, Rajender S. 2011. High fat diet prevents overcrowding induced decrease of sex ratio in mice. PLoS One. 6:e16296.
Frank SA. 1990. Sex allocation theory for birds and mammals. Annu
Rev Ecol Syst. 21:1355.
Gittleman JL, Thompson SD. 1988. Energy allocation in mammalian
reproduction. Am Zool. 28:863875.
Gomendio M, Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE, Simpson
MJ. 1990. Mammalian sex ratios and variation in costs of rearing
sons and daughters. Nature. 343:261263.
Gotz AA, Wolf M, Stefanski V. 2008. Psychosocial maternal stress during pregnancy: effects on reproduction for F0 and F1 generation
laboratory rats. Physiol Behav. 93:10551060.
Grant VJ. 2007. Could maternal testosterone levels govern mammalian
sex ratio deviations? J Theor Biol. 246:708719.
Grant VJ, Irwin RJ. 2005. Follicular fluid steroid levels and subsequent
sex of bovine embryos. J Exp Zool A Comp Exp Biol. 303:
11201125.
Ryan et al.
167