Harkavy - National Humillations
Harkavy - National Humillations
Harkavy - National Humillations
Defeat, National
in International
Humiliation,
Politics
345
Motif
ROBERT E. HARKAVY
Pennsylvania State University University Park, Pennsylvania, USA
Introduction
One of the curiosities of contemporary international relations is that someof its
presumably most important dimensions remain ignored or understudied. Most
notable is the absenceof attention to the interconnection - on a comparative basisbetween defeat (usually but not always military defeat), national (or other levels of
identity) humiliation or shame,and the consequentand resultant quest for compensatory revenge.Whether this involves an outright taboo on the subject of revenge,
as is actually claimed by SusanJacoby in a recent work, is a question to which we
shallreturn.1 Further, whether this is the result of methodological or political bias, or
just becausethesesubjectsappear to be un-measurablein an empirical sense,is also
an interesting point of speculation.
Whether or not subject to actual measurementor empirical research,this subject
lends itself to an implicit model (See Figure 1 below). Amidst obvious complexity,
and begging some definitional problems that will be addressedin the following
analysis,the core of the model is fairly simple. It depicts a relationship between military defeat, the psychological absorption of such defeat by a collective body, subsequent widespreadand persistent shameand humiliation, and a resulting collective
rageand an almost ineradicableneed for vengeance.The model allows for somevariants of defeat, for the nuanced distinction between deep psychological humiliation
and mere revisionism, and the possibilities for alternative responsesother than
vengeance,i.e. withdrawal (acceptance)or internal revolution.
tt
Figure 1: A Model of Defeat, Humiliation,
Narcissistic
injury,
shame,
humiliation,
revisionism,
lowered
testosterone
at individual
level
and Revenge
/
-cl
Chronic
collective
narcissistic
rage
Vengeance
Or
Withdrawal
or
Cultural
produce
response
differences
variations
lnternrl
revolution
in
Begs questions
of
long-term
persistence
347
Most of all, the humiliation/vengeance syndrome runs against the grain of theses
that, in the modern zeitgeist, fall under the colloquial labels of endism or the obsolescence of war. The central point of endism is that bad things are coming to an
end. Huntington sees endism as manifest at three levels - the end of the Cold War
(indisputable), the proposition that wars among nation-states, or at least among
some nation-states, are coming to an end and, per Fukuyama, the end of history as
such, which results from the unabashed victory of economic and political liberalism and the exhaustion of viable systemic alternatives.3
The related obsolescence of war thesis is closely associated with political scientist
John Mueller and military historian John Keegan. 4 In their writings, one finds the
assumption that warfare among modern and relatively wealthy democracies has
become anachronistic, even unthinkable, to the extent that analogies are drawn with
the disappearance of slavery. In a related vein, the democratic peace thesis claims that,
historically and with few exceptions, democracies have not committed aggression
against each other.5 Further, low politics (trade competition) is said now permanently to have superseded anachronistic high politics (national security) as the central
focus of competition among the major contending powers.6 Additionally, the long
period dating back at least to the 1930s, in which ideological competition has defined
international bloc rivalries, is now seen to have come to a permanent end. The role of
the nation-state is said to be in decline, nibbled at from above by international organizations and multinational corporations, and from below by increased regionalism and
the strengthening of sub-national identities. 7 If these trends are permanent, then the
old tradition of balance of power politics would be dead, in practice and in theory.
Replacing it is, for instance, an image of the emerging international system that centers
on a three-bloc, neo-mercantilist economic competition between a US-led Americas
bloc, a German-led European bloc, and a Japan-led Asian bloc.8 Otherwise, a recent
and frequently reviewed work by Max Singer and Aaron Wildavsky dwells on the coming sharp bifurcation between zones of peace (peaceful economic competition
between the major democracies, singly or en bloc) and zones of turmoil (the
assumption of looming chaos and neo-Malthusian disaster - poverty, AIDS, tribal
warfare, etc.) in the developing areas.9
Endism and the obsolescence of war are not without their critics. But, to the
extent such concepts are prophetic, the revenge motif might be relegated to the
developing world or to areas characterized by Singer and Wildavsky as zones of turmoil. And, indeed, accumulated humiliations in northern zones of peace are more
likely to be worked out through economic competition (as with Japan and Germany)
or conventional and nuclear arms sales (as France and China utilize). But the jury is
still out. Richard Rosecrance has, for instance, by way of partial dissent, characterized
the present period as one of a concert of powers similar to what transpired after the
Napoleonic wars and World War I - a concert likely to break up and lead to a
renewal of big power security rivalries.10
Is the syndrome from shame and humiliation through collective narcissistic rage, to
vengeance, an anachronism rooted in the family structures and cultures of less-thanfully modern societies, of which the Arab - or more broadly, Islamic - world would
be a good example? According to this thesis, modern consumer societies and/or democracies would, by their very natures, be less prone to collective fantasies about revenge.
348
Robert E. Harkavy
349
350
Robert E. Harkavy
351
352
Robert E. Harkavy
moodsand actionsand the extent to which one can legitimately utilize concepts applicable to individuals to gauge the behavior of
national or other aggregates,i.e., the problem of isomorphism and
anthropomorphism.
.
Fifth, a significant body of writing in psychiatry, much of it years
ago, focusedon the conceptsof narcissisticrage and shame-rage
cycles, claimed to underpin neurotic individual vengeance and
vindictiveness.
. A small literature on comparative political culture and comparative cultural psychiatry is a sixth strand, raising the crucial issueof
whether some peoples or nations may be abnormally prone to
shameor humiliation - and hence to compulsions to vengeance
- in turn rooted in family and small group relations and patterns,
or historical tribal behavior.
.
Some recent work in sociobiology suggestsa seventh strand that
involves decreasesin testosteronelevelsafter individuals are defeated in sports contestsor suffer a lossof social status.This raisesan
intriguing question about the applicability of such patterns to
nations in the aftermath of defeat, although researchingsuch matters for large populations and in historical retrospective confronts
obvious impediments.
.
Finally, socio-psychologicalresearchand writing, past and present,
has dealt with nationalism, patriotism, group loyalty and the wethey phenomenon.
Each of theseis discussedin more detail below.z8
Works on Revenge
SusanJacobyswork, Wild Justice, is one extant social sciencework devoted explicitly to the study of revenge(Scheffs is the other). 29As noted, it makesonly occasional references to international relations, being largely devoted to the interplay of
revenge and justice as an historical problem largely cast in legal and religious terms.
Jacoby reviews the historical record of the revenge motif as expressedin literary
works and embeddedin legal systemsand associatedcodesof legal ethics in Ancient
Greeceand Rome, the Bible, and Europe sincethe Medieval period. Sheaddressesthe
popular fascination with revengeexpressedin literature, film and theater, referring to
the popularity of revenge as a theme in modern massentertainment.30 Indeed,
Jacoby refers to the semi-pornographic fascination with revenge themes in literature and drama, noting that 17th Century revengethemesin English tragedieswould
strike a familiar chord in connection with any number of contemporary works. A
late 1990sfilm that appearedmore than a decadeafter Jacobysbook - Sleepers, featuring Robert DeNiro - epitomizesher point.
Jacobyseemsto agreewith Karen Horney and others to the extent that vengeance
is discussedas an archaic, illegitimate and neurotic emotion and activity - . . .the
sick vestige of a more primitive stageof human development.31Vindictiveness is
seenas neurotic and, although the urge to retaliate may be universal, it is deemed
353
unhealthy. In domestic systems, retributive systems, i.e., the courts, are claimed to
remove the burden of revenge from individuals; otherwise, the religiously inclined
are advised to look to a higher authority to provide retribution,
In the interstices of her analysis, however, Jacoby makes some points interesting
and germane for this discussion. First, she concedes that, certainly in the modern
world and even when compared to recent centuries, a virtual taboo exists on the subject of revenge. In a related vein, she notes the paucity of literature on this subject in
psychoanalysis, although that discipline would appear clearly suited to the exploration and explanation of the theme of revenge. This would seem all the more so in
view of the pervasive importance of the vengeance theme in Greek mythology. In
summary, she says that a taboo has been attached to the subject of revenge in a century that has witnessed the fearful union of mass vengeance with technology, a reference to Nazi revenge against the Jews as manifested in the Holocaust.32
By extension, one might speculate that the taboo Jacoby perceives in connection
with domestic legal systems has been extended to the field of international relations.
One might argue that, to the extent that liberal analysts project the normative illegitimacy of revenge onto the field, a defacto taboo does, indeed, underlie the study of
international conflict. And, whereas Jacoby and others may decry the emotion and
practice of revenge in domestic systems where courts may provide surrogate avengers
of sorts, no such authorities exist in the anarchic international system - except, perhaps, the occasional war crimes tribunals such as Nuremberg and The Hague.
Literature on Irredentism
The recent publication of at least one major edited work on territorial irredentism
by Naomi Chazan coincided with growing interest in ethnic politics.33 This volume
provides concepts and historical context of several waves of irredentism after its
emergence as a distinct process. Such an emergence occurred when issues of state
formation and national awakening converged over the delineation of political boundaries.34 Those waves have occurred, respectively, during the latter part of the nineteenth and the early part of the twentieth century; after World War I at the time of the
Paris Peace Conference; during the decolonialization process after World War II; and,
again, after the Cold War in conflict-prone areas of the Middle East, Africa, and Asia.
Several related definitions of irredentism exist. One has to do with attempts by
existing states to annex adjacent lands and the people who inhabit them in the name of
historical, cultural, religious, linguistic, or geographic affinity? Alternatively, another
view asserts that intrinsic to the notion of irredentism is a tension between people and
territory, between politics and culture - indeed, between symbolic and instrumental
aspects of international relations.36 Such competing definitions, exhibit tension over
the relative importance of people versus territory, and over the centrality of the nationstate which demands that territory be seen as a part of national heritage.
In the Chazan volume, a number of case studies are provided: Alsace, post-World
War I boundary problems in Europe, irredentism in Germany since 1945, Turkey,
and Africa in toto in the wake of the decolonialization process. In many but not all of
these cases, defeat in war accompanying humiliation and striving for revenge all have
been involved. And, the authors in this collection stress the sentimental and subjec-
354
Robert E. Harkavy
355
sense had to be addressed even at the price of still another defeat for the Arabs, for
whom the status quo was psychologically unbearable. Failure to take into account the
revenge motif then became a prominent cause of intelligence failure (not only with
respect to the Arabs, but also the Soviets, who responded to the humiliation of their
weapons in 1967 by introducing whole new classes of weapons into the conflict).
Individual and National Identity: The Issue of Anthropomorphism
By far the most important and daunting methodological and conceptual problem
in the study of national (rather than individual) vengeance is how to apply the conceptual baggage of individual or small group psychology to nation-states, or other
collectivities. This raises the issue of anthropomorphism.
Of course, that issue can be
raised in various contexts of international relations - for instance, when preference
scales are attributed to the nation-state. But, somehow, the issue seems particularly
difficult dealing with irrational
states of mind involving vengeance, more than
when rational behavior may be attributed.
Such a methodological issue is not new in the application of social-psychological
concepts to foreign policy. Indeed, it was cited as a major obstacle to research and
understanding long ago by S. E. Perry, Herbert Kelman, and Otto Klineberg - all
pioneers in the application of psychological concepts to the study of international
relations.J2 More recently, this issue has been addressed head-on in a full-length
treatment by William Bloom.43 In it, Bloom engages in a lengthy exegesis of what he
refers to as identification theory, drawing variously upon the works of Sigmund
Freud, George Herbert Mead, Erik Erikson, Talcott Parsons, and Jurgen Habermas.
Bloom avers that, in making statements such as France declared war on England,
an implication is made that entire populations have a joint attitude. Further, he notes
that, by making such statements, academic integrity and intellectual credibility are
severely strained, and that this strain is due to the lack of theory which in a methodologically coherent way explicates the relationship between a mass national population and its state.44 He looks to the possibility of a psychological theory - what he
calls identification theory - giving the mass national population of a state just such
a theoretically coherent status.
Bloom recognizes that the lack of any theoretical status for the mass national population became more apparent with the advent of the behavioral revolution in the
study of international relations in the 1960s. In particular, he notes,
the language of anthropomorphism
in which nation-states
as
apparently coherent personalities acted and reacted on the international stage...along with such notions as national honor: national
prestige, and national character, was shown up as having little if
any explanatory power and certainly no methodologically coherent
internal logic45.
In what then becomes a lengthy and complicated analysis, devoted to a variety of key
areas of international relations theory, Bloom further attacks what he calls the individual-aggregate problematic. He draws on an essay by J. David Singer,46 in which Singer,
first having delineated certain attributes of the international system, proposes the use
of three psychological variables - personality, attitude, and opinion - said to interact
356
Robert E. Harkavy
with the system.Singeris then discussedand quoted asfollows,providing a good summary of the methodologicalissuesinvolved:
In other words I would hold that the aggregationof individual psychological properties provides a quite sufficient basefor describing
the cultural properties of the larger social entity which is Xomprised of those individuals. He repeated further on that, The
position taken here is that the cultural properties of any subnational, national or extranational system may be described in a
strictly aggregativefashion, by observing the distribution and configuration of individual psychologicalproperties.47
Bloom concludes that there is no psychological theory which precisely explains
how to argue coherently from the individual to aggregategroup or massbehavior,
which explainspolitical integration and mobilization,48 and cites political psychologistssuch as Kelman and Fred Greenstein ashaving been acutely aware of the need
for a coherent psychological theory which could be applied so as to aggregatefrom
the individual out to the group.49 Particularly germaneto our central focus here on
collective feelingsof humiliation, shame,and vengeanceis his discussionof the work
of Erik Erikson - specifically, Eriksonswork on the individual need to protect and
enhanceego identity, and the projection or aggregation of that problem to a more
collective basis.
Bloomsanalysisis echoedby Harvard psychiatrist John Mack and by the sociologist Thomas Scheff. Mack discusses
the collective psychologicalforces in the study of
history and collective myths, and further notes that there is no equivalent at a
group or collective level to the superegorestraints which can operate at an individual
level to curb hostile or violent impulses.50He refers to the difficult methodological
problem of finding a sound, conceptual balance among the relevant insights of individual and group psychology - in a field where realities are multilayered and compelling.151Scheff, on the samewavelength, admits that in claiming an isomorphism
between interpersonal and international relations, I realize that I challengean article
of faith of modern social science:that structure and processat the societal level are
fundamentally different from those at the level of persons,asDurkheim claimed, is a
reality sui generis.52
Narcissistic Ragein Psychiatric Literature
Moving back to the specific subjectof humiliation and vengeance,it is noteworthy
that in psychiatric literature, the clinical term narcissistic rage is most commonly
used. It provides the link between, on the one hand, shame and humiliation aggregatedto the collectivity of the nation - and on the other, aggressiveness
and
vengeance.According to Heinz Kohut, 53this actually involves a spectrum describing
relative degreesof such rage, culminating at the extremes to what the author seesas
the neurotic and dangerousstate of chronic narcissisticrage.54Hence, this spectrum is seento run from the deepestand most inflexible grudge of the paranoiac to
the apparently fleeting rage reaction of the narcissisticallyvulnerable after a minor
slight.55 (The author is mum, however, on whether there really is such a thing as a
narcissisticallyinvulnerable person, and whether such a person could really cope in
357
most human environments.) Thosein the grip of a narcissisticrage are said to show
total lack of empathy towards the defender.56Chronic narcissisticrage is deemed
one of the most pernicious afflictions of the human psyche - either, in its still
endogenousand preliminary form, as grudge and spite; or, externalized and acted
out, in disconnectedvengeful actsor in a cunningly plotted vendetta.57
Kohut elaboratesin this context on the metapsychologicalposition of shameand
rage,which he callsthe two principal experiential and behavioral manifestations of
disturbed narcissistic equilibrium, and notes, in this regard, narcissistic rage
belongsto the larger psychologicalfield of aggression,anger, and destructiveness.58
Anticipating one of our later-to-be discussedresearchproblems, he alsosuggeststhat
the narcissisticallyvulnerable individual respondsto actual (or anticipated) narcissistic injury either with shamefacedwithdrawal (flight) or with narcissistic rage
(fight):59 beggingthe important question of why and when either of thesetwo alternative responses
occur.
Thesequestionsare addressedby Mack, Volkan, and Scheff aswell. Mack, in discussing collective psychological forces in the study of history, stressescollective
myths, the pain of their histories, the accrued grief of the centuries, the problem
of historical grievances, and the rise and fall of national self-esteem.60Scheffs
emphasisis on unacknowledgedor bypassedshame,and on prestige asa codeword
for honor and the avoidanceof shame.61
Vengeance
Kohut joins other psychiatrists and other social scientists by asking whether
propensitiesto shameand humiliation, and to narcissisticrage,and hence to vengefulness,may be more strongly evidenced in somecultures than in others, just asthey
may be more strongly evidenced in some individuals within thesecultures. In some
writings, this is perceived as derived from deeply rooted cultural legaciesof family
structure, child-rearing, and genderrelations. The psychiatrist H.W. Glidden and the
political scientistLeonard Binder (the latter writing about Egypts political culture in
an edited volume devoted to the comparative aspects of that subject) have, for
instance, characterized Arab societies as intensely suffused with propensities to
shameand humiliation. Binder actually refers to Egypt asa shameculture, one with
a deeply rooted masstendency for conformity in relation to fear of shaming.62Glidden applied this explicitly to the Arabs hitherto incapacity or unwillingnessto make
peacewith Israel,absentthe psychiatric terminology of narcissisticrage:
Failure to conform, however,brings shame.Shameis intenselyfeared
among the Arabs, and this fear is so pervasive that Arab society has
been labeled a shame-oriented one. This contrasts sharply with
Judaismand with WesternChristian societies,which are guilt-oriented. It is to be noted, however,that in Arab termsshameis not defined
asthe commissionof an act condemnedby the value system;instead,
it meansthe discovery by outsidersthat a given individual or group
committed suchan act. Hence there is an intenseconcern with and
catering to outward appearancesand public opinion that many
observershavenoted asbeing characteristicof the Arabs.63
358
Robert E. Harkavy
Numerous writers on the interminable civil war still raging in Afghanistan have
observedthe deeply embeddedAfghan cultural traditions of retaliation and revenge.
Kohut, meanwhile,in discussingthe work of Ruth Benedict, refers to the propensity
toward narcissisticrage in the Japanese,attributed to their methods of child-rearing
through ridicule and the threat of ostracism and to the sociocultural importance
which maintaining decorum has in Japan. Benedict is quoted as noting that in
Japan, sometimespeople explode in the most aggressiveacts...They are roused to
theseaggressionsnot when their principles or their freedom is (sic) challenged...but
when they detect an insult or a detraction.e4 Benedict doesnot, apparently, attempt
to project theseclaimed national attributes from the individual to the collective level
much lessto national behavior. Seeminglymissingin the literature is a discussionof
casesat the other end of the extreme, i.e., those of cultures or nations deemed relatively lessinclined to collective narcissisticrage and vengeful behavior. Might this
apply, for instance, to more modern or liberal or democratic cultures, or to
thosewith a history of successfuldiplomatic and military endeavors?
Gliddens article, almost alone in the literature, posits a causal relationship
between family and tribal structures, culturally basedvalue systems,collective shame
and humiliation, and the compulsion to revenge.65He posits, ingroup solidarity,
stemmingoriginally from Arab tribal values,is probably the most salientcharacteristic of the mechanics of Arab society.@jFurther, this ingroup solidarity is said to
demand a high degreeof conformity and therefore imparts a strong authoritarian
tone to Arab culture and society. In that connection, Glidden refers to the prevalence
of an other-directed personality in Arab culture, said to be characteristic both of
Arab tradition and of the outlook of Islam. He then proceedsto discussthe role of
shameand conformity in Arab society, asfollows:
Conformity brings honor and social prestige,and it alsoensuresfor
the individual and his group a secureplace in society.As long as the
individual conforms, the other membersof his ingroup and its allies .
and clients are bound to help him advance his interests and to
defend him unquestioningly againstoutsideforcesand agencies...
Why is this fear of shameso powerful among the Arabs?Shame
destroys one of the key elementsin the Arab prestige system: the
ability to attract followersand clients. (Arab society is and alwayshas
beenbasedon a systemof client-patron relationships.)Sinceamong
the Arabs the identification betweenthe individual and the group is
far closerthan it is in the West (indeed, it may be saidthat the group
is the individuals alter ego). The consequences
of shameare therefore much more widespreadand complex than in Westernculture.67
Glidden relates this analysisto the matter of vengeance,specifically, the requirement for revengeagainstthe Jewsfor the history of Arab defeat. He statesthat for the
Arabs, defeat does not generatea desire for peace;instead it produces an emotional
need for revenge,and this needis deepenedrather than attenuated by each successive
defeat.68With referenceto Bloom, et al, regarding the issueof anthropomorphism,
Glidden suggeststhat the first thing to note is that sincethe Arab value systemis a
group- and not an individual-based one, it is not possiblefor the individual Arab
359
states to dissociate themselves from the Arab collectivity any more than the individual
can dissociate himself from his clan.@ Hence, he concludes that all Arabs and their
governments are driven to eliminate the shame that had been visited on them and
the other Arabs by their defeats by IsraeL70
Because Gliddens analysis is near sui generis in the literature, particularly with
respect to its focus on revenge, one wonders about its more genera1 applicability, perhaps to situations involving Arabs and/or Islam. What, for example, does it tell us
about the future of Iranian need to extract vengeance against Iraq, given that Irans
defeat in 1988 involved the large-scale use of chemical weapons against the Iranian
army? What about Pakistans compulsion for revenge against India, given the backdrop of the 1971 defeat that led to the creation of Bangladesh? Further, what is the
applicability to Bosnia, Chechnya or, for that matter, Ireland, Japan or Peru? And, in a
reversal of the standard analysis of humiliation and vengeance in the Middle East,
one writer - Jay Gonen, in his Psychohistory of Zionism -has characterized Israeli
bravado and machismo as a function of the humiliation of the Holocaust, also
requiring a kind of psychological vindication, if not outright revenge.71
Americans, even the social scientists among them, may less easily comprehend
these problems. The US collective psyche lacks a strong shame component. Even
Vietnam was a mere pinprick, and collective shame could easily be assuaged by the
knowledge that the North Vietnamese could easily have been beaten by an all-out
effort. Pearl Harbor probably caused more shock than shame, but it did give rise to a
vengeful response. But, overall, Americans have been spared this kind of deep national trauma, and they may not easily understand it elsewhere.72
Sociobiological Research and Socio-Psychological Literature on Nationalism
Approaching this subject from different disciplines and a different level of analysis, recent work in sociobiology has measured declines in testosterone levels suffered
by individuals after athletic defeats such as tennis and chess, or as a result of loss of
social status.T3 This refers back to Mazurs biosocial theory of status, which hypothesizes a feedback loop between an individuals testosterone level and his or her
assertiveness in attempting to achieve or maintain interpersonal status or dominance
rank.T4 Winning raises testosterone levels, losing decreases it; this is further claimed
to explain, in part athletic winning or losing streaks, or the alternations between hot
streaks and slumps on the part of baseball players, among others. Whether such
phenomena could be attributed or applied to international politics and victory or
defeat in war may be far from trivial.
Finally, an extensive social-psychological literature on the roots of nationalism has
long focused on individuals and small interacting groups, often involving laboratory
experiments and surveys of college students. This approach has ramifications for the
role of attachments, national and group identities in cognitive development, ingroup versus out-group loyalties, the denigration of outside groups, individual and
collective images of others, the role of reference groups in enhancing individuals
self-esteem, and negative self-identities. This literature has been surveyed by Daniel
Druckman, who dwells on the extent to which groups and nations provide security
and safety as well as status and prestige in return for loyalty and commitment.
360
Robert E. Harkavy
361
362
Robert E. Harkavy
363
ation that follows defeat. Various wars may be compared to what John Dower called
the US-Japan Pacific War: a war without mercy, with obvious racial overtones, and
between peoples of vastly different cultures as well as races.83 These factors may be
present in the Arab-Israeli conflict, just as they were, relatively speaking, absent in the
European wars of this century and also the Cold War. The vengeance factor may or
may not vary accordingly.
Finally, there is the factor of time, which may be related to some of the cultural
factors discussed above. How long does it take before shame and humiliation associated with a defeat fades away, at least to the point where it no longer requires a
vengeful response? And, how does that relate to, for instance, the magnitude and the
level of embarrassment of defeat? In the case of the Arabs narcissistic rage vis-a-vis
Israel, for instance, Glidden sees an almost open-ended time frame. Hence, according
to Glidden:
As for the element of time, the Arabs consider it to be of little account
in the quest for vengeance, which to them is an integral part of what
they conceive of as justice. There are vendettas among the Arabs
that have lasted for centuries, as all students of the Near East are
aware. In Islamic law, the question of the conduct of Islam in defeat is
regarded as an anomaly and is almost totally ignored. Those few
jurists who did deal with it maintained that the battle would be
resumed no matter how long the Muslims had to wait.84
The impact of defeat in war and the accompanying humiliation may be examined
in some related contexts. For instance, there is the long-standing generalization that
internal revolutions tend to follow military defeats. That thesis has been applied to
France after its defeat in the French and Indian wars in the 176Os, Germany and Russia after World War I, Argentina after the Falklands War, and many others.85 Some
have pointed to the connection between the Soviets debacle in Afghanistan and the
subsequent collapse of the regime. Indeed, some have also pointed to the connection
between Americas debacle in Vietnam and the accompanying domestic disarray,
even if well short of a revolutionary situation.
Heretofore, we have surmised a rough equivalence, psychologically
speaking,
between defeat in wars and resulting humiliation, and humiliation derived from
colonial domination and racial oppression as per Franz Fanon. Druckman, however, posits an alternative view in discussing Latin America and the concept of xenocentrism:87 or, the situation in which dependent countries under-value themselves
and over-value their dominators. Such an inward-turning,
negative self-identify was
also discussed by Volkan in the context of masochism and the turning of aggression
inward in anticipation of further danger and humiliation.88
One major recent work has pointed to a possible connection between defeat and
the need for revenge as a driving force behind nuclear proliferation. This may, of
course, be a merely pragmatic response to defeat, the psychological effects notwithstanding. India appears to have reacted to its defeat by China in 1962 with a drive
towards nuclear weapons.89 Ditto Iran after its defeat in 1988 in which it was on the
receiving end of chemical weapons. Concerning Pakistan, in the period after its
defeat by India in 1971, Burrows and Windrem describe the public hero worship
364
Robert E. Harkavy
devoted to scientist A.Q. Khan, father of the Pakistani bomb, who is seen as personally representing that nations transcending of defeat and humiliation via nuclear
precociousness.g0
Summary
NOTES
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
a.
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
Susan Jacoby, WildJustice: The Evolution of Revenge (New York: Harper and Row, 1983).
Hans Morgenthau,
Politics AmongNations,
3rd ed. (New York: Knopf, 1961), pp. 54-55.
Samuel Huntington,
No Exit: The Errors of Endism: The National Interest, No. 17 (Fall 1989), pp.
3-11.
John Mueller, The Retreat from Doomsday
The Obsolescence of Major War (New York: Basic Books,
1989); and John Keegan, A History of Warfare (New York: Knopf, 1993).
See, among numerous sources, Bruce M. Russett, Grasping the Democratic Peace (Princeton:
Princeton University Press, 1993).
Richard Rosecrance, The Rise ofthe Trading State State (New York: Basic Books, 1986).
Keichi Ohmae, The Rise of the Region State, Foreign Afiirs, Vol. 72, No. 2 (1993), pp. 78-89. See also
Daniel Nelson,Threats
and Capacities, Contemporary
Politics,Vol. 3, No. 4 (1997), pp. 341-363.
Edward
ttwak, The Endangered American Dream (New York: Simon and Schuster, 1993); Jeffrey
Garten, r Cold Peace (New York: Times Books, 1992); and Lester Thurow, Head to Head: The Coming
Economic Battle amongJapan,
Europe, and America (New York: Morrow,
1992).
Max Singer
and Aaron
Wildavsky,
The Real World
Order
(Chatham,
NJ: Chatham
House
Publishers,
1993).
Richard Rosecrance, A New Concert of Powers, Foreign Affairs, Vol. 71, No. 2 (1992), pp. 64-82.
See Gil Carl Alroy, Behind the Middle East Conflict: The Real Impasse Between Arab and Jew (New
York: Putnam, 1975); and Robert E. Harkavy,
After the Gulf War: The Future of Israeli Nuclear
Strategy, T!re Washington Quarrerly,Vol.
14, No. 3 (1991),PP.
161-179.
Rise of Militancy
by Moslems Threatens Stability of Egypt, The New York Times (October
27,
1981), p. Al.
This paradoxical
point is discussed in Ahmed Abdalla, Egypt and the Gulf Crisis: Short-Term
Tremor, Long-Term
Trauma, in John OLoughlin,
Tom Mayer, and Edward S. Greenberg,
eds., War
and Its Consequences: Lessons from the Persian Gulf Conflict (New York: Harper Collins, 1994), pp.
125-131.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
365
366
42.
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
48.
49.
50.
51.
52.
53.
54.
55.
56.
57.
58.
59.
60.
61.
62.
Robert E. Harkavy
S. E. Perry and A. Stanton, Personality and Political Crisis (Glencoe: Free Press, 1951); Otto Klineberg,
The Human Dimension in International
Relations (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1964);
and Herbert C. Kelman, Social-Psychological
Approaches to the Study of International
Relations,
in H.C. Kelman, ed., International
Behavior (New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,
1965); and
especially, S. E. Perry, Notes on the Role of National: A Social-Psychological
Concept for the Study
of International
Relations: Journal of Conflict Resolution, Vol. 1, No. 4 (1957), pp. 346-363. See also
Harold Lasswell, The Climate of International
Action, in Kelman, op. cit., pp. 339-353, particularly
the discussion about a Theory of Collective Mood, on pp. 344-346. According to William Bloom,
Personal Identity, National Identity, and International
Relations (Cambridge:
Cambridge
University
Press, 1990), p. 19, Kelman did grasp the nettle of the crucial issue of the psychological
link between
the individual
and the nation-state,
recognizing
its pivotal place in any attempt to work towards a
psychological
theory of international
relations.
Ibid. See, in addition, Richard Berk, Collective Behavior (New York: Richard C. Brown, 1974); Wilfred
Trotter, Instincts of the Herd in Peace and War (Oxford:
Oxford
University
Press, 1953); and Neil
Smelser, Theory ofCollective Behavior (New York: The Free Press, 1962).
Bloom, op. cit., p. 1.
Ibid., p. 2.
J. David Singer, Man and World Politics: The Psycho-Cultural
Interface, Journal ofSocial Issues, Vol.
24,No. 3 (1968),pp.
127-156.
Bloom, op. cit., p. 2 1.
Ibid., p. 22.
Ibid., pp. 22-23.
John Mack, Foreword,
in Vamik Volkan, Cyprus - War and Adaptation:
A Psychoanalytic
History of
Two Ethnic Groups in Conflict (Charlottesville,
VA: University Press of Virginia, 1979), p. XV.
Ibid., p. XIX.
Scheff, op. cit., p. 75.
Heinz Kohut, Narcissism and Narcissistic Rage, The Psychoanalytic Study ofthe Child, Vol. 27, No. 3
(1972), pp. 360-400. Among the useful pieces in this area, most devoted to the psychology
of shame
and humiliation,
are Sidney Levin, The Psychoanalysis of Shame, International
Journal of Psychoanalysis, Vol. 52, No. 4 (1971), pp. 355-361; Moshe Halevi Spero, Shame: An Object-Relational
Formulation,
Psychoanalyric Study of the Child, Vol. 39 (1984), pp. 259-282, and A. Morrison,
Shame,
Ideal Self, and Narcissism, Contemporary
Psychoanalysis, Vol. 39 (1983), pp. 295-318. The latter has a
discussion of the distinction
between the two effects of shame and guilt, and what is referred to as
the hairline distinctions
between shame, humiliation,
and mortification
(p. 261). Also discussed
are narcissistic depletion and loss, shame personalities,
and the severity of narcissistic trauma,
all relevant to our analysis of the underpinnings
of revenge. Perhaps only Blema Steinberg, heretofore, has attempted to tie these themes to political events, as in her Shame and Humiliation
in the
Cuban Missile Crisis: A Psychoanalytical
Perspective, Political Psychology, Vol. 12, No. 4 (1991), pp.
653-690. Also valuable is Christopher
Lasch, The Culture of Narcissism: American Life in an Age of
Diminishing
Expectations (New York: W.W. Norton, 1978), wherein there is a discussion of the narcissistic projection of aggressive impulses outward
(p. 57).
Kohut, op. cit., p, 396.
Ibid., p. 386.
Ibid.
Ibid., pp. 396-397.
Ibid., p. 379. The methodological
and philosophical
problems involved in utilizing a concept such as
shame are discussed in Charles Taylor, Interpretation
and the Sciences of Men, The Review of
Metaphysics,Vol.XXV,
No. 1 (1971), pp. 3-51, esp. p. 13.
Kohut, op. cit., p. 379.
Mack, op. cit., p. XIII.
Scheff, op. cit., pp. 3-4,96-97.
Leonard Binder,Egypt:
The Integrative Revolution,
in Lucian Pye, ed., Political Culture and Political
Development (Princeton:
Princeton University
Press, 1965), pp. 396-449. See also David Pryce-Jones,
The Closed Circle: An Interpretation
oftheAr&
(New York: Harper and Row, 1989).
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
71.
72.
73.
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.
81.
82.
83.
84.
85.
367
368
86.
87.
88.
89.
90.
Robert E. Harkavy
Franz Fanon, The Wretched ofthe Earth (New York: Grove Press, 1968), with a foreword
Sartre.
Druckman,
op. cit., p. 61.
Volkan, Ethnonationalistic
Rituals, op.cit.,p. 13.
Burrows and Windrem, op.cit., pp. 107- 108.
Ibid., chapter 11.
by John Paul