Understanding Child Development
Understanding Child Development
Understanding Child Development
Sara Meadows
Contents
Acknowledgements
Preface
Note on reading
11
Note on pictures
11
13
33
69
Language development
117
Language; Methodology; Infants
perception of speech sounds; Infants
production of speech sounds; Beginning
to use words; Using words as names;
Early vocabulary; From single words to
143
6 Social relationships
173
Infantadult interaction; Describing
relationships; Parentchild relationships;
Early experience and later effects;
Sibling relationships; Peers; Sex roles,
sex differences, sex typing; Learning the
social world of school; Childrens
independent social worlds; Adolescence;
Delinquency; Overview
Further reading
209
Bibliography
212
Author index
238
Subject index
245
Acknowledgements
Preface
10
Note on reading
Note on pictures
11
Plate 1 An illuminated diagram from a Natural Science textbook compiled by the monk Byrhtferth at Ramsey Abbey
between about 1080 and 1090. It is used as illustration here because in its picture of the place of Man in the universe it links
the four ages (boyhood and infancy, adolescence, young manhood, and old age) to the influence of the signs of the Zodiac,
the four seasons, the four points of the compass, the four elements of earth, air, fire and water, and the four humours (hot,
cold, wet and dry). It might thus be seen as an early model of the developmental psychology of personality. Childhood and
infancy (bottom left dark circle), ages 014, lie between air and fire, west and north, Capricorn and Pisces, and are wet and
hot; adolescence (top left), ages 1428, between fire and earth, north and east, Aries and Gemini, and is hot and dry. (Young
manhood lasts, readers will be glad to learn, until 48, and old age to 70 or 80.) Which of the four humours dominated the
body had implications for medical treatment and for personality (see Chapter 5), and may have influenced concepts of
education and child-rearing.
12
Conceptual issues
Before we embark on either description or
explanation of the course of child development, let
alone before we consider what efforts we might make
to change its course, I must stress that neither child
nor development could be said to be simple
unproblematic concepts; in particular they are
inextricable from beliefs about how to bring up
children. There are many variations between cultures
on what they believe children naturally are and how
they should behave (Laboratory of Comparative
Human Cognition 1983). There are also historical
changes within societies. The study of the history of
western childhood is only just beginning and its
picture is controversial. Philippe Aris pioneering
study (Aris 1962) argued that strong concern and
affection for children, and a belief that childhood was
an intrinsically valuable period, were historically
recent developments associated with the rise of the
affluent household in the sixteenth and seventeenth
centuries. Previously, he claimed, there was no
concept of childhood: children were regarded with
indifference by their parents or as inferior miniature
adults to be strictly reared and severely punished. De
Mause (1976) put forward an even blacker model of
maltreatment and cruelty to children infanticide,
beatings, sexual abuse and a casual acceptance of
high mortality through infection, accident or
childrearing practices such as wet-nursing or using
opiates to quieten a crying child. Aris and De Mause
have both been accused of selecting their data without
much concern for their representativeness, and of
interpreting dubious facts in unjustified ways
21
Play
Children, like many other young mammals, spend a
considerable amount of time and energy playing.
This activity has been evaluated by adults in a wide
range of different ways. One view has been that
although childrens play may be enjoyable to the
participants and charming to fond parents observing
it, it is essentially frivolous, a pastime with no
intrinsic consequences, which is grown out of
without it having made much impact on
development. At one extreme this view was stiffened
by a sterner judgement that frivolities should be
discouraged in favour of useful activities, that
children should play less and work or pray more. It
has almost certainly been a fairly recent development
to take a contrary view and see play as making
important contributions to the childs development,
to consider it to be an important way of learning
things that the adult will need to know. This view has
had a period of considerable dominance in
psychological and educational theory: the
paradoxical slogan play is the childs work has been
implicit or even explicit in many early childhood
curricula.
It seems to be common practice in our society to
draw a distinction between work and play. Parents
do this, so do teachers: so do children, who come
home from infant school and tell their inquiring
parent that they just played, though the teacher no
doubt defined their activities in the classroom as
educationally important and as work. The work/play
distinction is conflated with the distinction between
having to work and being free to do what one wishes,
and play is seen as voluntary and not obligatory.
Voluntary activity is seen as free, absorbing,
spontaneous, enjoyable, not serious and done for
oneself not for other people (see p. 85 for discussion
of similar approaches to study). Given a model of
motivation which rests on the assumption that an
25
Gibbon
Gestation
Complete hair covering
24 weeks
During gestation
30
Ossification centres
in wrist at birth
First teeth (months)
Second teeth (years)
Growing period (years)
Life span (years)
All
34
onset during gestation,
completed after birth
23
Gorilla
Human
37
40
Never
completed
None
0.65.9
1.66.8
7
1.2?
?8.5
9
2.712.3
2.910.2
11
3.013.0
3.010.5
11
6.024.0
6.020.0
20
25
33
35
35
70
Chimpanzee
recombinations may be sought deliberately and nonplayfully, as in certain teaching programmes for
cognitive development (see Chapter 3).
Emotional and social functions of play
Y s turn
1 Hello, my name is
Mr Donkey
Hello, my name is Mr
Elephant
Hello, my name is
Mr Tiger
2 I have to go to
work
Hello, my name is
Mr Lion
Youre already at
work
No Im not
I have to go to
school
No Im not
Youre already at
school
30
Plate 3
31
Plate 4
32
Cognitive development
Studying cognitive development, we are concerned
with the child as knower; with someone who thinks,
understands, learns, remembers, and so forth. There
is still no clear, complete and valid account of what
adults do when they think, understand, etc., despite
hard work by philosophers over thousands of years,
more recently by psychologists, and very recently by
computer scientists and neurophysiologists.
Accounting for cognitive development additionally
involves describing what develops, that is, noting
what changes between different ages and explaining
how these changes come about. Quite obviously
these are extraordinarily formidable questions. We
cannot yet answer them; but currently psychologists
who have learned from Piagets partly correct and
partly incorrect answer are putting together an
exciting new account.
I will outline and discuss Piagets achievements
first, and then proceed to the post-Piagetian picture of
what and how thinking develops. Good introductions
to Piagetian theory are provided by Brainerd (1978)
and Brown and Desforges (1979); for fuller accounts
of Piagets work see Flavell (1963, 1977), Vuyk
(1981) and Gelman and Baillargeon (1983).
Piagetian theory
An anecdote which Piaget used many times gives a
good picture of the Piagetian child. A boy aged 5 was
playing with his collection of pebbles. He laid them
out in a line and counted them along the line from left
to right: there were ten. Then he counted them from
right to left, and to his great astonishment the total
was, again, ten. He put them in a circle and counted
them first clockwise and then counterclockwise: full
of enthusiasm he discovered that there were always
Piagets final factor, equilibration, was invoked to coordinate the diverse contributions of maturation and
physical,
social
and
logico-mathematical
experiences. It was a central concept in his theory,
most important in accounting for how development
occurred. Briefly, he postulated that organisms
needed to maintain a stable internal equilibrium
within the changes and uncertainties of the outside
world. Body temperature in warm-blooded animals is
a good example of the sort of process involved: the
feedback systems of thermostatically controlled
central heating are also analogous. In equilibrated
systems, of which cognition is supposed to be one, the
changes and demands of the outside world produce
small perturbations or conflicts in the system
which automatically adjusts itself to cope with them
and return either to the original steady state or in the
case of cognition to a new and better equilibrium.
There is a strong need for equilibrium: durable
disequilibria constitute pathological organic or
mental states (Piaget 1968, p. 102).
The concept of equilibration explains how
cognitive development occurs in terms of a need for
a coherently organized and consistent way of
thinking. This equilibrium is gradually constructed
as partially adequate ways of thinking conflict with
the data provided by the external world, or with their
own inconsistent processes and results, and have to
be improved. Equilibration implies that there
should be a considerable degree of organization and
coherence in cognition. It also implies that conflict
between ideas or models or ways of doing things will
be a major source of progress. Piagets account of it
Carrots
Potatoes
Boiling
Chipping
Frying
Grating/salad
Mashing
Roasting
Soup
not tried
not tried
not tried
not tried
not tried
not tried
The cook now meets parsnips for the first time. The
new vegetable is assimilated to the carrot repertory,
perhaps on the basis of similarity of shape and
texture. Parsnips boil very well, make a rather bland
soup, and although they taste quite pleasant raw are
not a visually attractive ingredient in salad. The cook
also assimilates parsnips to the potato repertoire:
parsnips are disastrous chipped or fried, mash well
and are delicious roasted. The knowledge matrix
after assimilation of parsnips would have one further
column, thus: Assimilation is always accompanied by
35
Method
Parsnips
Boiling
Chipping
Frying
Grating/salad
Mashing
Roasting
Soup
No
No
maybe
maybe
Carrots
Potatoes
Parsnips
Boiling
Chipping
Frying
Grating/salad
Mashing
Roasting
Soup
No
No
No
maybe
No
No
maybe
maybe
If p, then q
p, therefore ?
It is raining, therefore ?
not p, therefore ?
q, therefore ?
not q, therefore ?
not p
we get wet
it is not raining
Figure 1
39
Information-processing approaches
A major contribution to the study of cognitive
development has been made by informationprocessing approaches. Work in this tradition
emphasizes precise analysis of how information is
recognized, coded, stored and retrieved, and because
computer simulation techniques are often used the
structures and processes involved in handling
information are relatively tightly specified and
testable. Information-processing studies typically
present people with a problem and examine what
information they select, how they store and organize
it, what models or hypotheses are involved, and what
cognitive processes they use to reach a solution.
Capacity, processes and knowledge
41
Figure 2 Portions of the knowledge base concerning fruits. (a) The knowledge of a 5-year-old, for whom peaches and
apples are alike primarily because they are both round. (b) The knowledge of an 8-year-old, for whom peaches and
apples are similar primarily because they are both fruits.
Source: From Kail and Bisanz (1982), p. 60.
42
45
Plate 5
46
Figure 3 Cross-sectional drawings of the adult and newborn eye. The adult eye (a) is a horizontal section of the right
eye. Important structures are labelled. The visual axis is represented by the broken line and the optic axis by the solid
line. The newborn eye (b) is also a horizontal section of the right eye. It is drawn to scale to represent its size relative
to the adult eye.
Source: From Banks and Salapatek (1983).
47
Figure 4 Major pathways from the eyes to the central nervous system. Fibres of the optic nerve from the temporal halves
of each retina remain on the same side of the head; that is, they project to the ipsilateral hemisphere of the brain. Fibres
originating from the nasal halves of each retina cross at the optic chiasma and then project to the contralateral hemisphere.
The lateral geniculate nuclei are part of the thalamus. The superior colliculi are part of the mid brain. The striate cortices
are part of the cerebral cortex.
Source: From Banks and Salapatek (1983), p. 446.
48
Figure 5 Scanning patterns of newborns. The left portion shows scanning patterns when newborns were presented a
homogeneousfield.Eachdotrepresents eye position at one timesample.Thelines connectingthe dots simply connecteye
positions at adjacent time samples. The vertices of the triangles represent infra-red marker light positions. Thus, the
trianglesshown wereactually not present duringthese trials.Theright portion of this figureshowsscanningpatterns when
newborns were presented a solid black triangle on an otherwise homogeneous field. The outer triangle on each record
represents the triangles contours. The vertices of the inner triangles represent the marker light positions.
Source: From Banks and Salapatek (1983), p. 511.
Auditory perception
51
Figure 6 Cross-section of the human ear showing the three major divisions (outer, middle and inner ear), their mode of
operation, and their presumed function.
Source: From Aslin, Pisoni and Jusczyk (1983), p. 586.
new behaviours
new behaviours
Figure 7 A typical Golgi-stained pyramidal neuron whose cell body is in Layer 5 of the cortex. The dendrites (apical,
oblique, basal) receive synaptic input from other nerve cells. The axon conducts the neurons output to other nerve cells.
The enlargement presents a segment of the apical shaft as it might appear in the electron microscope. At high magnification
(30,000 to 40,000 power) the dark projections from the dendrite appear as protoplasmic extensions called spines. These
spines form synapses with the vesicle-filled axom terminals,or boutons,from other neurons.Synapses on the cell body do
not utilize spines and hence cannot be visualized with Golgi-staining techniques.
Source: From Parmelee and Sigman (1983), p. 99.
56
Cognition in infancy
I have outlined what is known about the physical
status and the functioning of infants perceptual
apparatus in the preceding section. It would seem that
they are able to get perceptual experience from birth,
and in some modalities from before birth, indeed that
perceptual experiences of various sorts are needed for
the proper development of the sensory apparatus.
However, there is a difference between the objective
physical stimulus and what is made of it in the course
of perception and cognition. Most stimuli are
interpreted by the organism which perceives them,
whether that organism be human or non-human,
infant or adult. Sometimes the stimuli need little
interpretation to be meaningful and effective,
sometimes a great deal is needed. Sometimes the
interpretation made is objective, derived from the
stimulus more than from the perceiver and likely to be
identically derived by any other perceiver:
sometimes the interpretation is subjective, a result of
the perceivers idiosyncracies not of the stimulus.
Debates about the extent and the sources of
interpretation of perception have been active in
philosophy since well before any psychology or any
60
66
Plate 6
67
Plate 7
68
Attention
Bearing this interaction in mind, then, we can look
at some of the components of cognition and how
they develop. Attention seems a good aspect to
start with since it could be taken as a necessary
condition for further cognitive activity. It is
however a somewhat vague concept, certainly
polymorphous. Taylor (1978) gives examples:
Attention can be directed, switched, captured,
distributed, divided, narrowed, sustained or
withheld. . . . Distractibility may imply that the
child is not motivated to do the tasks he is given, or
that they are too difficult for him to persist at; it may
mean that he explores all stimuli,or all prominent
Figure 8
69
Remembering
It is obvious that remembering of some sort is
necessary for virtually any human cognitive activity.
As we saw earlier it is hard to believe in the possibility
of an intelligent organism which did not use
accommodation and assimilation in its functioning,
that did not make some comparison between the
present stimulus and stimuli encountered earlier. This
would be impossible without memory. Conversely,
memory is rarely an isolated intellectual skill. Early
researchers found it necessary to concentrate on
memory for meaningless materials such as nonsense
syllables in part because this was the only way to
control for differences in subjects knowledge,
understanding and so forth (see, for example,
Baddeley 1976). It is partly because of the interaction
of remembering, understanding and acting that
developmental changes in memory are important.
It is also obvious that remembering is not one
simple activity. There have been many suggestions
about the structure of memory and the processes
which are involved at each stage. There is no clear
73
Reading
Frith (1980a) calls reading and spelling complex and
astonishing accomplishments, a description which
is obviously correct. We do not yet have a full account
of what people do when they read. Researchers agree
that very many linguistic, perceptual, attentional,
memory and cognitive skills are involved, but they
vary considerably in which they emphasize.
Research in experimental cognitive psychology
often concentrates on bottom-up analyses (see, for
example, Crowder 1982) and emphasizes the
readers use of, for example, eye movements or
pattern recognition processes. Other investigators
may assert that the readers knowledge of what is
likely to be the meaning of a word or piece of text may
be crucial in whether it can be read, and emphasize
top-down models (e.g. Smith 1978). In some cases
there has been a regrettable tendency to make the
top-down or bottom-up emphases too strong, so
that some accounts of reading as a matter of
comprehension have taken the perceptual
components as uninteresting and mechanical, and
some accounts of reading as a matter of decoding
visual information into a verbal form have excluded
anything more cognitive than word recognition.
Top-down and bottom-up have to be co-ordinated
in theories as they are in ordinary reading, where most
of the time processes at all levels are used. Recently
theories which integrate different levels have
appeared. Mortons logogen system (Morton 1969,
1980) and Rumelharts model (Rumelhart 1977; Ellis
1984) are important examples. It is clear that
reading includes many different activities at
different perceptual, linguistic and cognitive levels,
which no doubt interact in changing ways as the
reader becomes more skilled, or when the reader is
79
1
Stage
designation
2
Grade
range (age)
3
Major qualitative characteristics
and masteries by end of age
4
How acquired
5
Relationship of reading to listening
Stage 0:
Prereading,
pseudoreading
Preschool
ages 6
months6
years
Stage 1:
Initial
reading
and
decoding
Grade 1 &
beginning
Grade 2
(ages6
&7)
Stage 2:
Confirmation
and fluency
Grades2&
3(ages7
& 8)
Stage 3:
Reading for
learning
the new
Grades48
(ages9
13)
Phase A
Intermediate,
46
Phase B
Junior high
school,
79
Stage 4:
Multiple
viewpoints
High
school,
grades10
12 (ages
1517)
Reading widely from a broad range of Wide reading and study of the
complex materials, both expository and physical, biological, and social
narrative, with a variety of viewpoints. sciences and the humanities; high
quality and popular literature;
newspapers and magazines;
systematic study of words and word
parts.
Stage 5:
Construction
and
reconstruction
College
and
beyond
(age 18+)
80
Plate 8
82
Writing
I have described some of the recent work on reading
and using text. I want now to consider the second R,
writing. Like reading, this is an activity which
integrates many different processes physical,
linguistic, cognitive, even social in different ways
according to the writers age, experience and
purposes.
Scardamalia
(1981)
lists
the
interdependent skills involved, among them
questions of handwriting, spelling and punctuation,
considerations of word choice, syntax and textual
connections, and of overall purpose, organization,
clarity and euphony. Each of these is itself of course
highly complex and subject to developmental
change. I shall focus here on the more cognitive
issues, discussing the relation of writing to reading
and speaking and the production problems,
particularly the composition problems this involves.
Writing in relation to speaking
Figure11
89
cat
sat
mat
cat
sat
mat
tkoic
ttkoic
okki
kioe
cat
sat
on
the
tkoic
ttkoic
okki
kktols
kioe
cat
sat
on
the
mat
Table 3
Written text
1. tkoic
4. ttkoic
3. okki
5. kktols
2. kioe
6. tkok
90
Rereadings
13
Rereading
4
Rereadings
56
Rereading
7
cat sat
on
the
mat
eating
cat sat
on
the
mat
eating
oranges
cat
sat
on
the
mat
eating
cat sat
on
the
mat
eating
oranges
Figure 12 Relationships between amount of writing in different categories and sex, social class and education.
Source: From Griffiths and Wells (1983).
92
extinct
puncuation
intellectual
Arithmetical skills
Having sketched the development of reading and
writing, I want now to look at childrens arithmetical
skills. This is a busy research area at present, with
many fine-grained analyses of mathematical tasks.
Mayer (1985) provides a brief introduction to this
97
98
Counting
100
Ordinal numbers
Figure 14 Response time depends on the number of increments required in the min model. Number pairs represent the two
numbers to be added; e.g. 13 means 1 + 3.
Source: From Groen and Parkman (1972).
Adult
Child
Adult
Child
Metacognition
Metacognition refers to ones knowledge and
control of the domain cognition (Brown et al. 1983,
p. 106). It thus involves a range of different
psychological contents. Among them would be
awareness of cognition, of understanding or not
understanding, remembering or forgetting, at a
particular moment; knowledge about ones own
cognitive skills and about a tasks difficulties (I
wont be able to do this because I cant remember how
to do square roots); and regulation of cognition, for
example planning how to do a task, monitoring
progress on it, checking the solution. Some of this
metacognitive knowledge is stable and stateable,
though it may not be objectively true. For example, I
would know all the time that I remember epigrams
rather badly and the general meaning of a play or
novel quite well, and that in order to record the detail
I should make a note of it. Some metacognition is
much harder to describe and much harder to employ
stably and systematically; as the introspectionist
psychologists found long ago, people who reported
on their thinking while they were working on a
problem found their thinking disrupted (Valentine
1982, Russell 1984).
Recently, metacognition has taken a prominent
place in discussion of cognition and cognitive
development. This is partly because of the influence
of the information-processing models of cognition
which I briefly reviewed in the previous chapter (see
also Klahr and Wallace 1976; Siegler 1981; Sternberg
1984; Case 1985). The concept of a central executive
system which controls cognitive processes has been a
powerful metaphor for developmentalists, who see
cognition as becoming increasingly skilled and
controlled as development and education proceed.
Another reason for the rise of metacognition has been
Piagets emphasis that it is the childs awareness of a
problem in his or her thinking which is crucial for
cognitive development: a contradiction between two
of my schemes of thought forces me to develop a
resolution between them, or conflict between my
104
Social cognition
Most of the work on cognitive development deriving
from the Piagetian or information-processing
tradition centres on an individual thinker trying to
understand the objective physical world. Lately, an
interest has grown in an area which poses alternatives
to each half of this individualist model, and centres on
the thinking individual whose thinking comes from
interaction with other individuals as much as from
within himself or herself, or on individuals thinking
about the subjective social and interpersonal world. It
is this newly important area which is called social
cognition. It is a diverse field, including, for
example, the effects of social conflict on performance
of conservation tasks, childrens descriptions of
friends, the role of emotion in understanding other
people, childrens ideas about techniques for
achieving social goals such as joining groups, the use
of social scripts, and childrens theories of how
social institutions work. There is much debate about
conceptual and methodological issues, and not as yet
a unifying theory (indeed only a high level theory
could unify so wide an area): I will not attempt to do
more than discuss selected work in the field. For
reviews see Isbell and McKee 1980, Shantz 1983,
Flavell and Ross 1981, Butterworth and Light 1982,
and Forgas 1981.
Cognitive development as a social process
G1 Its morning.
G2 At morning, its lunch time!
G1 At morning, we already had breakfast. Because
at morning, its lunch time!
G2 RIGHT!
G1 Yeah, at morning, its lunch time.
G2 At morning its lunch time.
G1 But, first comes snack, then comes lunch.
G2 Right . . . Just in school, right?
G1 Yeah, right, just in school.
G2 Not at home.
G1 Well, sometimes we have snacks at home.
G2 Sometimes.
G1 Sometimes I have a snack at home.
G2 Sometimes I have a snack at my home, too.
G1 Uh-hum. Because when special children come to
visit us, we sometimes have snack. Like, like,
hotdogs, or crackers, or cookies or, something
like that.
G2 Yeah, something. Maybe cake. (Laughs)
G1 Cake.
G2 Cake. Yeah, maybe cake.
G1 Or maybe, uh, maybe, hotdog.
G2 Maybe hotdog.
G1 But, but, but, Jill and Michael dont like hotdog.
Dont you know, but, do you know Michael or
Jill?
G2 I know another Michael.
G1 I know, I know another Michael.
G2 No, I know just one Michael. I just know one
Michael.
G1 Do you know Flora?
G2 No! But you know what? Its a, its its one, its
somebodys bro . . . its somebodys brother.
G1 Are you eating your dinner? (Laughs) But not for
real.
G2 Not for real.
G1 Because at morning, its lunch time.
G2 Right, at morning it is lunch time.
G1 Right, at morning it is lunch time.
G2 Yeah.
G1 I think . . . Ill have . . . lunch. [Nelson and
Gruendel, 1979, pp. 8081]
114
89
910
1011
1112
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
1.31
3.32
0.66
3.25
1.91
2.94
1.34
2.66
2.63
4.79
0.67
5.35
1.59
2.88
0.72
1.22
1.61
2.64
0.52
1.42
Plate 9 Pas Bank from The Book of Shops, 1899, by E. V. Lucas, illustrated by Francis D. Bedford
115
Plate 10
116
4 Language development
Language
The development of language provides a good
example of epigenesis, and its later stages also
involve consideration of eco-systems. Human infants
clearly start with a great many capacities and pieces
of behaviour suited to language, but they are also born
into communities which use language and expect the
infant to use it too. All except the severely impaired
develop language in very similar ways, though at
varying rates: but the details of the language and how
it is used are heavily influenced by the childs
experience. Through development, language
functions as a means of communication, as a means
of reflecting on and re-organizing experience, and as
a way to receive and transform the accumulated
knowledge and values of the community. Using
language is thus a central part of human existence.
Methodology
117
Language development
Language development
Language development
Language development
Language development
Language development
129
Figure 16
Language development
P:
Un thermomtre.
A thermometer.
T:
Une petite phrase. Avec quoi prend-on la temprature? Vronique, une phrase.
A short sentence. What does one take a temperature
with? Veronica, a sentence.
V:
Un thermomtre.
A thermometer.
T:
V:
Avec un thermomtre.
With a thermometer.
T:
Language development
M: Maman/prend/la temprature/avec un thermomtre
(utterance is syllable-timed with each chunk forming a separate tone group).
Mother takes a temperature with a thermometer.
T:
133
Plate 11
134
Language development
135
Okay.
First by grabs.
J:
And denials.
L: No! We got the same.
By fact collecting, assertions, and inferences.
L: [Begins to count her dominoes] One, two, three, four . . .
twenty-eight, twenty-nine. [Then counts Jamies dominoes]
One, two, three, four . . . eighteen, nineteen . . . [short pause]
twenty-nine.
J:
NOOO!
Language development
Heida:
Heida:
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
M-hm.
Heida:
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
Oh.
Heida:
Dum-dum!
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
Heida:
Dan:
Oh yeah read.
Heida:
Dan:
Sure.
(Slobin 1978, pp. 523).
137
B: Lexical 1. How
C: Surface Structure 1. How would you run over a dinosaur? Id start at his tail, run up
his back, then over his neck
and Id jump off.
2. Where would you go to see a
man-eating fish? A seafood
restaurant.
D: Deep
Structure
Language development
E: Morpheme
BoundaryNo
Phonological
Distortion
F: Morpheme
Boundary
with
Distortion
Language development
141
5 Personality
Temperament
Temperament is seen as continuities of
characteristic style of behaviour which can be
observed and rated in infancy, and pervade all that a
person does. Temperament interacts with experience
to produce personality. Various temperament
dimensions have been suggested, among them
144
Personality
at
2 was positively correlated with social
participation at 71/2, with high verbal IQ and with
high field independence. High impulsivity of action
had negative implications for intellectual
development. Possibly this difference might be
related to the different demands made on children at
the two ages: a child of 71/2 is expected to be able to
control its impulses and fit them to outside
constraints such as classroom requirements of
sustained concentration and planful activity. A
cognitive style characterized by high impulsivity
has often been seen as militating against academic
achievement, and impulsivity might in excess (or in
extreme lack) also have social implications.
These subtle relationships between characteristics
over time are further complicated by the existence of
differential socialization. Parents try to encourage
ways of behaving in their child which will make the
child better able to cope with the world. One
interesting example though ethnographic rather
than the sort of work on which we are concentrating
here is found in Heaths (1983) account of language
use in different Appalachian communities (see
Chapter 4 this volume). Another, older, example in
one of the pioneering studies of temperament and
Self-concept development
Parental behaviour less explicitly specified has been
seen for some considerable time as one of the most
important sources of the self-concept. G. H. Mead in
his classic book Mind, Self and Society (Mead 1934)
says that via social interaction the young child begins
to appreciate that other people (notably, parents) have
views of him or her as good, bad, clever, a real
146
boy, big for her age and so forth. These views are
inferred from others behaviour towards him or her as
well as from their talk, and are accepted as evaluative
and categorical labels much like names. It is from
these labels applied by others that the child builds up
his or her self-concept a looking-glass self.
More recent theorists paint a slightly different
picture. Lewis and Brooks-Gunn (1979) make
Meads account part of a wider development. They
describe two rather different aspects of the self. The
subjective aspect, the existential self, is centrally
the distinction of oneself from others, the awareness
of the me who is acting, experiencing, remembering
and so forth. It probably involves the sensory self
which may have a neurological base (Konner 1982)
but it requires a sense of self-permanence which
Lewis and Brooks-Gunn see as analogous to object
permanence, and also an accumulation of learning
about the patterning of actions and outcomes. As we
have seen in discussing infancy, the rudiments of a
distinction between self and not-self seem to become
apparent increasingly clearly in the babys first six
months; by the last quarter of the first year
distinctions between self and other are becoming
independent of specific actions and contexts so that
the child could be said to have a permanent
existential self.
From here on, the second aspect of the self
develops too. This is the objective aspect, the
categorical self. This aspect refers to the
characterization of oneself in terms of categories like
gender, age, competence, attractiveness etc. The
categories used may change between cultures or
historical periods or may be universal, and may
change over an individuals lifetime or remain
constant. To give an example: my own categorical
self would include the following categories, all
relative and not in any order of importance: tall,
which appeared early, will remain constant and could
be universal; female, also early, universal and
constant, although the defining attributes and
connotations of female have undergone historical
and cultural changes; distrustful of technology
which appeared late, is clearly not historically and
culturally universal and possibly ought to be
Personality
147
Personality
The research in child rearing that stemmed from this
tradition was imposing both in conception and productivity
. . . the importance of parental nurturance and of the way
parents exercise authority have been amply demonstrated.
Yet the yield of the work with respect to the theory of
identification was disappointing . . . no consistent
relationships were found among characteristics that ought
to have been linked by their common origins in the process
of identification (Maccoby 1980, pp. 1718).
Self-efficacy
Banduras work on self-efficacy (Bandura 1977,
1981) links the cognitive and motivational
components of attribution theory to the development
of social understanding. He is concerned with how
children come to think of themselves as efficacious,
and how they act on such a judgement. He sees four
principal sources of informaion on self-efficacy. The
first is childrens own accomplishment or
performance, their history of success or failure on
tasks and their attributions of causes for achievement.
The second is vicarious experience, seeing other
peoples success or failure similarly attributed. The
third is other peoples judgement of the childs own
efficacy: credible suggestions by others that he or she
is bound to fail may persuade the child not to try and
therefore not to achieve success. This is a form of the
self-fulfilling prophecy which may operate in
schools (see, for example, Rogers 1982). Banduras
fourth source of evidence is emotional arousal; if it is
very high, arousal probably debilitates performance,
and judgements of lack of self-efficacy may increase
arousal and thereby lead to the poor performance that
was expected. What children know about how their
emotional state affects their performance is also an
important influence.
How people combine these different sorts of
information, none of which is simple, is not as yet
understood. Bandura (1981, p. 210) suggests that
judgements of self-efficacy will tend to be more
egocentric and emotionally toned than objective and
150
Aggression
In considering aggression it is necessary, as in other
areas, to point out the definitional problems. Olweus
(1979) offers this definition:
Personality
any act or behaviour that involves, might involve, and/ or to
some extent can be considered as aiming at, the infliction of
injury or discomfort; also manifestations of inner reactions
such as feelings or thoughts that can be considered to have
such aim.
Personality
154
Personality
155
Plate 13
156
Personality
Personality
Moral development
Moral development in children is a complex subject:
many different emphases are relevant to its analysis,
and because of the social implications of morality,
and the apparent importance of education and childrearing practices to its development, discussion has
often been very value-laden. As we shall see, there
are real differences as to what are and are not moral
problems, what are good or universal moral
principles, and how they develop. As in other areas of
developmental
psychology,
models
which
emphasize biology, social conditioning or
cognition are in competition: so are different views
about the natural goodness or badness of human
beings and the functions of society.
Morality and sociobiology
159
Personality
Personality
conflict. It is this sort of matter that the cognitivedevelopmental theorists of moral reasoning address.
They are concerned with how children judge good
and bad, with what sort of principles they invoke
and use in decision and justification. Piaget (1932)
made the first of the attempts to describe the
development of moral reasoning discussed here;
Kohlberg (e.g. Kohlberg 1981a,b,c) is the second
major figure, while some subsequent studies reacting
to Kohlbergs work (e.g. Mussen and Eisenberg-Berg
1977, Turiel 1983), are also touched on.
Piagets theory of moral development
Personality
165
Personality
In Europe, a woman was near death from a kind of cancer.
There was one drug that the doctors thought might save her.
It was a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had
recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but
the druggist was charging ten times what the drug cost him
to make. He paid $200 for the radium and charged $2000 for
a small dose of the drug. The sick womans husband, Heinz,
went to everyone he knew to borrow the money, but he
could only get together about $1000 which is half of what it
cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked
him to sell it cheaper or let him pay later. But the druggist
said, No, I discovered the drug and Im going to make
money from it. So Heinz gets desperate and considers
breaking into the mans store to steal the drug for his wife.
Personality
Personality
171
Plate 14
172
6 Social relationships
Infantadult interaction
It is fairly clear that babies are, and do, and can do,
things which are likely to be useful in their social
world. They have the big shiny eyes, plump cheeks,
high foreheads, fine skin, smell of milk and uncoordinated movements which evoke automatic
reactions of tenderness and nurturance in adults in our
own species as in many others (Tinbergen 1951).
They selectively attend to human faces and voices,
and astonishingly early appear to discriminate the
familiar voice of their mother from other nonfamiliar voices (Mehler et al. 1978), a preference and
173
Parenting
Social relationships
176
Social relationships
Describing relationships
We will return to the question of long-term effects of
parentchild relationships later: there are important
and delicate questions to be asked about how any
effects are caused and what cognitive or affective
systems are involved. It must also be recognized that
the mothers side of the relationship needs more
consideration than it has received and that the
ecological context is important. Variation in the
babys behaviour in different settings and towards
different people (Lamb 1978), and the approach of
defining attachment behaviour in terms of whether
they usually or predictably bring about proximity
between baby and mother figure, imply that we must
consider not just the actions of the baby but also the
actions of the person he or she is seeking proximity
with: in other words that attachment is a property of
relationships not just of individuals, and particularly
not just of the baby who is by far the more intensively
studied partner (Hinde 1979, 1982). Most children
develop multiple attachments (Rutter 1981) and
these differ somewhat in intensity, function and
content. We need to consider how we describe and
evaluate relationships.
Robert Hinde (1978, 1979) has suggested a number
of dimensions and principles which may prove useful
in constructing a science of inter-personal
Social relationships
179
180
Social relationships
Maternal deprivation
181
Social relationships
183
184
Social relationships
185
Social relationships
Sibling relationships
Most children grow up in a family which contains
other children, brothers and sisters. Often these
siblings are an important part of the childs life, both
because children may spend at least as much time
with their siblings as with their mothers, and more
time with siblings than with their fathers (Dunn
1983), and because relationships between child and
187
188
Social relationships
189
Plate 15
190
Social relationships
Peers
An enormous amount of work has been done recently
on childrens relationships with other children. It has
varied so much in research method (ethnographies,
questionnaires, sociometrics, formal experiments) in
underlying theories (neo-Piagetian, behaviourist,
sociological, etc.) and in degree of insight (ranging
from stunning banality and pomposity to an eloquent
communication of shared experience) that I shall not
attempt to summarize and integrate it. There are
major reviews or collections by Hartup (1983), Lewis
and Rosenblum (1975), Foot, Chapman and Smith
(1980), Asher and Gottman (1981), among others,
and an accessible introduction by Rubin (1980). Here
I will focus on peer relations as they shed light on a
number of developmental issues and as part of the
ecology of the childs life.
I have placed a great deal of emphasis so far on the
importance of adults for childrens development. I
must now correct that emphasis by pointing out that
Social relationships
193
Social relationships
195
196
Social relationships
197
Social relationships
200
Social relationships
201
202
Social relationships
203
Delinquency
Figure 18 Males* found guilty of, or cautioned for, indictable/triable-either-way offences per 100,000 population in the age group by
age
Notes: *Other offender, i.e. companies, public bodies, etc., are included with males aged 21 and over because separate figures are not
available before 1976; Adjusted for changes in legislation.
Source: From Rutter and Giller (1983), pp. 69; using Home Office figures.
204
Social relationships
Figure 19 Females found guilty of, or cautioned for, indictable/triable-either-way offences* per 100,000 population in the age group
by age.
Note: *Adjusted for changes in legislation.
Source: From Rutter and Giller (1983), p.70; using Home Office figures.
206
Social relationships
208
Overview
In this book I have tried to put together our current
evidence on various aspects of child development to
make a coherent picture of the patterns and sequences
that occur. I have tried to indicate what general
theories of child development might try to describe
and explain, with some detail in certain well-studied
areas. I have argued that we must try to achieve sound
causal explanations of both continuities and
discontinuities in development, not just getting age
differences but explaining in detail what has led to
what within the ecosystems that the child inhabits,
and where the sequence might have been different.
Detailed studies of restricted areas of development
are appropriate, but we must be cautious about
isolating one aspect of childrens development from
others: cognition, emotion, social relationships and
contexts, language, physical state, all co-occur and
mutually influence each other. We cannot assume that
what we see in one setting or one sample or at one time
will be representative in detail of different settings,
samples or times unless we understand very fully why
things are the way they are.
Further reading
Further reading
Bruner, J. S., Sylva, K. and Jolly, A., Play (Penguin
1976)
Davie, C. et al. , The young child at home (NFER/
Nelson 1984)
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G. and Vandenberg, B., Play,
in E. M. Hetherington (ed.) vol 4 of the Handbook
of Child Psychology (New York: Wiley 1983)
Schwartzman, H. B., Transformations the
anthropology of play (NY: Plenum Press 1978)
Smith, P. K. (ed.), Play in animals and humans
(Blackwell 1984)
Sutton-Smith, B. (ed.), Play and learning (New York:
Gardner Press 1979)
Scientific issues
209
Chapter 5 Personality
Introductory reading
Further reading
Further reading
211
Bibliography
212
Bibliography
process, in R. Glaser (ed.), Advances in Instructional
Psychology, vol. 2 (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1982)
Berger, M., Temperament and individual differences, in
M. Rutter and L. Hersov (eds.), Child and adolescent
psychiatry (Oxford: Blackwell 1985)
Berlyne, D. E., Curiosity and exploration, Science, 153
(1966), pp. 2533
Berndt, T. J., Developmental changes in conformity to
peers and parents, Developmental Psychology, 15
(1979), pp. 60816
Bernstein, B., Class, codes and control, vol. 1 (1971); vol.
2 (1973); vol. 3 (1975) (London: Routledge & Kegan
Paul)
Bertenthal, B. I. and Fischer, K. W., Development of selfrecognition in the infant, Developmental Psychology,
14 (1978), pp. 4450
Berthoud-Papandropoulou, I., An experimental study of
childrens ideas about language, in A. Sinclair, R. J.
Jarvella and W. J. M. Levett (eds.), The childs
conception of langauge (Berlin: Springer-Verlag 1978)
Best, R., Weve all got scars (Bloomington: Indiana UP
1983)
Bettelheim, B., The uses of enchantment (Harmondsworth:
Penguin 1978)
Beveridge, M. (ed.), Children thinking through language
(London: Edward Arnold 1982)
Beveridge, M. and Brierley, C., Classroom constructs: an
interpretive approach to young childrens language, in
M. Beveridge (ed.), Children thinking through language
(London: Edward Arnold 1982)
Biggs, J. B., The role of metalearning in study processes,
paper given at BPS Education Section Conference
Cambridge, September 1984 (1984/in press)
Bissex, G. L., Gnys at wrk: a child learns to write and read
(Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press)
Bjorklund, D. F. and Zeman, B. R., Childrens
organisation and metamemory awareness in their recall
of familiar information, Child Development, 53 (1982),
pp. 799810
Blinkhorn, S. F. and Hendrickson, D. E., Averaged evoked
responses and psychometric intelligence, Nature, 295
(1982), pp. 5967
Block, J., Sex-role identity and ego development (San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass 1984)
Block, J. H., Lives through time (Berkeley, California:
Bancroft Books 1971)
213
214
Bibliography
Bruner, J., Jolly, A., and Sylva, K. (eds.), Play: its role in
development and evolution (Harmondsworth: Penguin
1976)
Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. and Greenfield, P. M. (eds.) Studies
in cognitive gro wth (New York: Wiley 1966)
Bryant, B., Harris, M. and Newton, D., Children and
minders (London: Grant McIntyre 1980)
Bryant, P. E., Perception and understanding in young
children (London: Methuen 1974)
Bryant, P. E., The role of conflict and of agreement
between intellectual strategies in childrens ideas about
measurement, British Journal of Psychology, 73
(1982), pp. 24352
Bryant, P. E. and Bradley, L., Why children sometimes
write words which they do not read, in U. Frith (ed.),
Cognitive processes in spelling (London: Academic
Press 1980)
Bryant, P. E. and Bradley, L., Psychological strategies and
the development of reading and writing, in M. Martlew
(ed.), The psychology of written language (Chichester:
Wiley 1983)
Bryant, P. E. and Bradley, L., Childrens reading problems
(Oxford: Blackwell 1985)
Bryant, P. E., Jones, P., Claxton, V. and Perkins, J.,
Recognition of shapes across modalities by infants,
Nature, 240 (1972), pp. 3034
Bryant, P. E. and Trabasso, T., Transitive inferences and
memory in young children, Nature, 232 (1971), pp.
4568
Bullowa, M., Before speech: the beginnings of
interpersonal communication (Cambridge: Cambridge
UP 1979)
Burghardt, G. M., On the origins of play, in P. K. Smith
(ed.), Play in animals and humans (Oxford: Basil
Blackwell 1984)
Burnett, J. (ed.), Destiny obscure: autobiographies of
childhood, education and family from the 1820s to the
1920s (London: Allen Lane 1982)
Buss, A. H. and Plomin, R., A temperament theory of
personality development (New York: Wiley 1975)
Butterworth, G., Object identity in infancy: the interaction
of spatial location codes in determining search errors,
Child Development, 46 (1975), pp. 86670
Butterworth, G. Object disappearance and error in Piagets
Stage IV task, Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 22 (1977), pp. 391401
215
216
Bibliography
M. Gottman (eds.), The development of childrens
friendships (Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1981)
Coveney, P., The image of childhood (Harmondsworth:
Penguin 1967)
Cowie, H. (ed.), The development of childrens imaginative
writing (London: Croom Helm 1984)
Cox, M. V. (ed.), Are young children egocentric? (London:
Batsford 1980)
Crook, C. K., Taste perception in the newborn infant,
Infant behaviour and development, 1, pp. 5269
Cross, T., Mothers speech adjustments: the contribution
of selected child-listener variables, in C. Snow and C.
Ferguson (eds.), Talking to children: language input and
acquisition (Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1977)
Cross, T., Mothers speech and its association with rate of
linguistic development in young children, in N.
Waterson and C. Snow (eds.), The development of
communication (Chichester: Wiley 1978)
Crowder, R. G., The Psychology of reading: an introduction
(Oxford: OUP 1982)
Csiksentmihalyi, M., The concept of flow, in B. SuttonSmith (ed.), Play and Learning (New York: Gardner
Press 1979)
Curtiss, S., Genie. A psycholinguistic study of a modernday wild child (New York: Academic Press 1977)
Dale, P. S., Language Development (New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston 1976)
Dalton, K., Once a month: the menstrual syndrome
(London: Fontana 1983)
Damon, W. and Hart, D., The development of selfunderstanding from infancy through adolescence,
Child Development, 53 (1982), pp. 84164
Davie, C. E., Hutt, C. J., Vincent, E. and Mason, M., The
Young Child at Home (Windsor: NFER Nelson 1984)
Davies, B., Life in the classroom & playground (London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul 1979)
Dawkins, R., The selfish gene (Oxford: OUP 1976)
Decarie, T. G., A study of the mental and emotional
development of the thalidomide child, in B. M. Foss
(ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour, vol. 4 (London:
Methuen 1969)
Decasper, A. J. and Fifer, W. P., Of human bonding:
newborns prefer their mothers voices, Science, 208
(1980), pp. 11746
Delamont, S., The sociology of women (London: George
Allen & Unwin 1980)
Bibliography
Feshbach, S., Feshbach, N. D., Cohen, R. S. and Hoffman,
M., The antecedents of anger: a developmental
approach, in R. M. Kaplan, V. J. Konecni and R. W.
Novaco (eds.), Aggression in children and youth (The
Hague: Martinus Nijhoff 1984)
Field, T. M., Effects of early separation, interactive
deficits, and experimental manipulations on infant
mother face-to-face interaction, Child Development, 48
(1977), pp. 76371
Field, T. M., Sostek, A. M., Vietze, P. and Leiderman, P. H.,
Culture and early interactions (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
1981)
Fine, G. A., The natural history of preadolescent male
friendship groups, in H. C. Foot, A. J. Chapman and J.
R. Smith (eds.), Friendship and social relations in
children (London: Wiley 1980)
Bibliography
Fine, G. A., Friends, impression management, and
preadolescent behaviour, in S. R. Asher and J. M.
Gottman (eds.), The development of childrens
friendships (Cambridge: Cambrige UP 1981)
Finkelstein, N. W. and Ramey, C. T., Learning to control
the environment in infancy, Child Development, 48
(1977), pp. 80619
Fivush, R., Negotiating classroom interaction, The
Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative
Human Cognition, 5 (4) (1983), pp. 837
Fivush, R., Learning about school: the development of
kindergartners school scripts, Child Development, 55
(1984), pp. 1697709
Flavell, J. H., The developmental psychology of Jean
Piaget (Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand 1963)
Flavell, J. H., Cognitive development (Engle-wood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice Hall 1977)
Flavell, J. H., The development of knowledge about visual
perception, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1977
(Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press
1978a)
Flavell, J. H., Comments on Brown & DeLoachs paper in
R. S. Siegler (ed.), Childrens thinking: what develops?
(Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1978b)
Flavell, J. H., On cognitive development, Child
Development, 53 (1982), pp. 110
Flavell, J. H., Botkin, P. T., Fry, C. L., Wright, J. W. and
Jarvis, P. E., The development of role-taking and
communication skills in children (New York: Wiley
1968)
Bibliography
Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (vol. 18)
(Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, in press)
Goody, J., The domestication of the savage mind
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1977)
Goody, J. and Watt, I., The consequences of literacy, in J.
Goody (ed.), Literacy in traditional societies
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1968)
Gordon, J. C. B., Verbal deficit: a critique (London: Croom
Helm 1981)
Gottlieb, G., The psychobiological approach to
developmental issues, in M. M. Haith and J. J. Campos
(eds.) vol. 2 of the Handbook of Child Psychology (New
York: Wiley 1983)
Gould, S. J., Ontogeny and phylogeny (Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard UP 1977)
Gould, S. J., The mismeasure of man (Harmondsworth:
Penguin 1984)
Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C., The spandrels of San
Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the
adaptationist programme, Proceedings of the Royal
Society of London, B205 (1979), pp. 58198
Gratch, G., Appel, K. J., Evans, W. F., LeCompte, G. K. and
Wright, N. A., Piagets Stage IV object concept error:
evidence of forgetting of object conception?, Child
Development, 45 (1974), pp. 717
Graves, D., Writing: teachers and children at work
(Heinemann 1983)
Grice, H. P., Logic and conversation, in P. Cole and J. L.
Morgan (eds.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 3, Speech acts
(New York: Academic Press 1975)
Griffin, C., Typical girls? Young women from school to the
job market (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul 1985)
Griffiths, M. and Wells, G., Who writes what and why?, in
B. Kroll and G. Wells (eds.), Explorations in the
development of writing (Chichester: Wiley 1983)
Groen, G. J. and Parkman, J. M., A chronometric analysis
of simple addition, Psychological Review, 97 (1972),
pp. 32943
Grotevant, H. D. and Cooper, C. R., Patterns of interaction
in family relationships and the development of identity
exploration in adolescence, Child Development, 56
(1985), pp. 41528
Guttentag, M. and Longfellow, C., Childrens social
attributions development and change, in C. B. Keasey
(ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation vol. 25
(Lincoln, Nebraska: Univ. of Nebraska Press 1977)
221
Bibliography
Hughes, M., Carmichael, H., Pinkerton, G. and Tizard, B.,
Recording childrens conversations at home and at
nursery school, Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 20 (1979), pp. 22532
Hughes, M. and Donaldson, M., The use of hiding games
for studying the coordination of viewpoints,
Educational Review, 31 (1979), pp. 13340
Hughes, M., Mayall, B., Moss, P., Perry, J., Petrie, P. and
Pinkerton, G., Nurseries Now (Harmondsworth: Penguin
1980)
Humphreys, A. P. and Smith, P. K., Rough-andtumble in
preschool and playground, in P. K. Smith (ed.), Play in
animals and humans (Oxford: Blackwell 1984)
Huston, A., Sex-typing in E. M. Hetherington (ed.), vol. 3
of the Handbook of Child Psychology (New York: Wiley
1983)
Hutt, C., Exploration and play, in B. Sutton-Smith (ed.),
Play and learning (New York: Gardner Press 1979)
Isbell, B. J. and McKee, L., Societys Cradle: an
anthropological perspective on the socialisation of
cognition, in J. Sants (ed.), Developmental Psychology
and Society (London: Macmillan 1980)
Istomina, Z. M., The development of voluntary memory in
preschool-age children, Soviet Psychology, 13 (1975),
pp. 564
Jackson, B., Fatherhood (London: George Allen & Unwin
1984)
Jackson, B. and Jackson, S., Childminder (London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul 1979)
Jahoda, G., The construction of economic reality by some
Glaswegian children, European Journal of Social
Psychology, 9 (1979), pp. 11527
Jahoda, G., The development of thinking about
socioeconomic systems, in H. Tajfel (ed.), The social
dimension, vol. 1 (Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1984)
Jarman, C., The development of handwriting skills
(London: Basil Blackwell 1979)
Jencks, C., Inequality: a reassessment of the effect of family
and schooling in America (Harmondsworth: Penguin
1975)
Jenkins, R., Lads, citizens and ordinary kids: workingclass youth life-styles in Belfast (London: Routledge &
Kegan Paul 1983)
Jennings, K. D., Harmon, R. J., Morgan, G. A., Gaiter, J. L.
and Yarrow, L. J. Exploratory play as an index of
mastery motivation: relationships to persistence,
223
Bibliography
Kroll, B. and Wells, G., Explorations in the development of
writing (Chichester: Wiley 1983)
Kuhn, T. S., The structure of scientific revolutions
(Chicago: Chicago University Press 1962)
Kurdek, L. A., Perspective taking as the cognitive basis of
childrens moral development: a review of the
literature, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 24 (1978), pp. 3
28
Kurtines, W. and Grief, E. B., The development of moral
thought: review and evaluation of Kohlbergs work,
Psychological Bulletin, 81 (1974), pp. 45370
Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, Culture
and Intelligence, in R. J. Sternberg (ed.), Handbook of
Human Intelligence (Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1982)
Labov, W., 1969, The logic of non-standard English, in
Keddie, N. (ed.), Tinker, Tailor . . . the myth of cultural
deprivation (Harmondsworth: Penguin 1973)
Labov, W., Sociolinguistic Patterns (Philadelphia: Univ. of
Pennsylvania Press 1972)
Lamb, M. E. and Baumrind, D., Socialisation and
personality development in the preschool years, in M. E.
Lamb (ed.), Social and personality development (New
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston 1978)
Lamb, M. E. and Roopnarine, J. L., Peer influences on sexrole development in preschoolers, Child Development,
50 (1979), pp. 121922
Lamb, M. E. and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant social
cognition: empirical and theoretical considerations
(Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1981)
Lamb, M. E. and Sutton-Smith, B., Sibling relationships:
their nature and significance across the lifespan
(Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1982)
Lane, H., The wild boy of Aveyron (London: George Allen
& Unwin 1976)
Lash, J. P., Helen and Teacher (London: Allen Lane 1980)
Laslett, P., The world we have lost (London: Souvenir Press
1971)
Laszlo; J. I. and Bairstow, P. J., Perceptualmotor
behaviour: developmental assessment and therapy
(London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston 1985)
Lawton, J. T. and Coleman, M., Parents perceptions of
parenting, Infant Mental Health Journal, 4 (1983), pp.
35261
Leach, P., Who cares? A new deal for mothers and their
small children (Harmondsworth: Penguin 1979)
226
Bibliography
Martin, B., A sociology of contemporary cultural change
(Oxford: Blackwell 1981)
Martlew, M., The psychology of written language:
developmental
and
educational
perspectives
(Chichester: Wiley 1983)
Mayer, R., Mathematical ability, in R. J. Sternberg (ed.),
Human abilities: an information-processing approach
(New York: W. H. Freeman 1985)
Mead, G. H., Mind, self and society (Chicago: Univ. of
Chicago Press 1934)
Meadows, S. A. C., The development of concrete
operations: a short-term longitudinal study, (PhD
thesis, University of London 1975)
Meadows, S., An experimental investigation of Piagets
analysis of class inclusion, British Journal of
Psychology, 68 (2) (1977), pp. 22937
Meadows, S. (ed.), Developing thinking: approaches to
childrens cognitive development (London: Methuen
1983)
Meadows, S. and Cashdan, A., Teaching styles in nursery
education: final report to SSRC (1983)
Meadows, S., Philps, J., Weaver, J. and Mably, S., Adults
and childrens views on education, and their behaviour at
home and in nursery school: a report on two pilot
studies, Paper given at the British Psychological
Society Conference, London (December 1977)
Medrich, E. A., Roizen, J. A., Rubin, V. and Buckley, S.,
The serious business of growing up: a study of childrens
lives outside school (Berkeley, California: Univ. of
California Press 1982)
Mehler, J., Bertoncini, J., Barrier, J. and JassikGerschenfeld, D., Infant recognition of mothers
voice, Perception, 7 (1978) pp. 4917
Meltzoff, A. and Boston, R. W., Intermodal matching by
human neonates, Nature, 282 (1979), pp. 4034
Meltzoff, A. and Moore, M. K., Imitation of facial and
manual gestures by human neonates, Science, 198
(1977), pp. 758
Midgley, M. Beast and man: the roots of human nature
(Brighton: Harvester Press 1979)
Millar, S., The psychology of play (Harmondsworth:
Penguin 1968)
Miller, P., Theories of developmental psychology (San
Francisco: Freeman 1983)
Miller, S. A., On the generalizability of conservation: A
comparison of different kinds of transformation, British
Journal of Psychology, 73 (2) (1982), pp. 22130
228
Bibliography
Pascual-Leone J., A mathematical model for the transition
role in Piagets developmental stages, Acta
Psychologica, 32 (1970), pp. 30145
Patrick, J., A Glasgow gang observed (London: Methuen
1973)
Patterson, C, J., Cosgrove, J. M. and OBrien, R. G., Non
verbal
indicants
of
comprehension
and
noncomprehension, Developmental Psychology, 16 (1)
(1980), pp. 3848
Patterson, G. R., Families: applications of social learning
to family life (Champaign: Research Press 1975)
Patterson, G. R. and Cobb, J. A., A dyadic analysis of
aggressive behaviours, in J. P. Hill (ed.), Minnesota
symposium on child psychology, vol. 5 (Minneapolis:
Univ. of Minnesota Press 1971)
Patterson, G. R., Littman, R. A. and Bricker, W., Assertive
behaviour in children: a step toward a theory of
aggression, Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 32 (serial no. 113) (1967)
Perera, K., Childrens writing and reading: analysing
classroom language (Oxford: Blackwell 1984)
Perlmutter, M. (ed.), Parentchild interaction and parent
child relations in child development, Minnesota
Symposium on Child Psychology, vol. 17 (Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum 1984)
Perlmutter, M. (ed.), Cognitive perspectives on childrens
social and behavioural development, Minnesota
Symposium on Child Psychology, 1984, vol. 18
(Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1984)
Perlmutter, M. (ed.), Intellectual development,
Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, 1985, vol.
19 (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 1985)
Peters, A. M., The units of language acquisition
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1983)
Peters, R. S., Moral development: a plea for pluralism, in
R. S. Peters, Psychology and ethical development
(London: Allen & Unwin 1974)
Piaget, J., The childs conception of the world (London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul 1929)
Piaget, J., The childs conception of physical causality
(London: Routledge & Kegan Paul 1930)
Piaget, J., The childs conception of number (New York:
Basic Books 1952)
Piaget, J., The construction of reality in the child (New
York: Basic Books 1954)
Piaget, J., The language and thought of the child (London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul 1959)
229
Bibliography
conservation, Child Development, 45 (1974), pp. 499
502
Rosen, B., Moral dilemmas and their treatment, in B.
Munsey (ed.), Moral development, moral education and
Kohlberg (Birmingham, Alabama: Religious Education
Press 1980)
Rosenblatt, D., Developmental trends in infant play, in B.
Tizard and D. Harvey (eds.), The biology of play
(London: Heinemann Medical Books 1977)
Ross, L., The intuitive scientist formulation and its
developmental implications, in J. H. Flavell and L. Ross
(eds.), Social cognitive development (Cambridge:
Cambridge UP 1981)
Rotman, B., Jean Piaget: Psychologist of the Real
(Hassocks, Sussex: Harvester Press 1977)
Rotter, J., Chance, J. and Phares, E., Applications of a social
learning theory of personality (New York: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston 1972)
Rubin, J. Z., Provenzano, F. J. and Luria, Z., The eye of the
beholder: parents views on sex of newborns, American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 44 (1974), pp. 51219
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G. and Vandenberg, B., Play, in E.
M. Hetherington (ed.), vol. 4 of the Handbook of Child
Psychology, series ed. P. H. Mussen (New York: Wiley
1983 )
Rubin, K. H., Watson, K. S. and Jambor, T. W., Freeplay
behaviours in preschool and kindergarten children,
Child Development, 49 (1978), pp. 5346
Rubin, Z., Childrens friendships (London: Fontana 1980)
Rumelhart, D., Toward an interactive model of reading, in
S. Dornic (ed.), Attention and performance, VI
(Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1977)
Russell, J., The acquisition of knowledge (London:
Macmillan 1978)
Russell, J., Dyadic interaction in a logical reasoning
problem requiring inclusion ability,
Child
Development, 52 (1981a), pp. 13225
Russell, J., Why socio-cognitive conflict may be
impossible: the status of egocentric errors in the dyadic
performance of a spatial task, Educational Psychology,
1 (1981b), pp. 15969
Russell, J., Propositional attitudes, in M. Beveridge (ed.),
Children thinking through language (London: Edward
Arnold 1982)
Russell, J., Explaining mental life: some philosophical
issues in psychology (London: Macmillan 1984)
231
Bibliography
Shantz, C. U., Social cognition, in J. Flavell and E.
Markman (eds.), vol. 3 of the Handbook of Child
Psychology, series ed. P. H. Mussen (New York: Wiley
1983)
Shatz, M., The relationship between cognitive processes
and the development of communication skills,
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1977 (Lincoln,
Nebraska: Univ. of Nebraska Press 1978)
Shatz, M. and Gelman, R., The development of
communication skills, Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 38 (5), serial no. 152
(1973)
Shatz, M. and Gelman, R., Beyond syntax: the influence of
conversational constraints on speech modifications, in
C. E. Snow and C. A. Ferguson (eds.), Talking to children
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1977)
Shepar, R. N., Recognition memory for words, sentences
and pictures, Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal
Behaviour, 6 (1967), pp. 15663
Shields, M. M. and Duveen, G., The young childs image of
the personal and social world: some aspects of the
childs representation of persons, Paper presented at the
International Sociological Association Conference
Mexico City (August 1982)
Shultz, T. R., Play as arousal modulation, in B. SuttonSmith (ed.), Play and learning (New York: Gardner
Press 1979)
Shweder, R. A., Turiel, E. and Much, N. C., The moral
intuitions of the child, in J.H. Favell and L. Ross (eds.),
Social cognitive development (Cambridge: Cambridge
UP 1981)
Siegel, L. S., Development of the concept of seriation,
Developmental Psychology, 5 (1972), pp. 1357
Siegel, L. S., The relationship of language & thought in the
preoperational child: a reconsideration of nonverbal
alternatives to Piagetian tasks, in L. S. Siegel and C. J.
Brainerd (eds.), Alternatives to Piaget (New York:
Academic Press 1978)
Siegel, L. S., McCabe, A. E., Brand, J. and Matthews, J.,
Evidence for the understanding of class inclusion in
preschool children: linguistic factors and training
effects, Child Development, 49 (1978), pp. 68893
Siegler, R. S., Developmental sequences within and
between concepts, Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 46 (1981), pp. 174
Siegler, R. S., Information processing approaches to
development, in W. Kessen (ed.), vol. 1 of the
233
Bibliography
Thwaite, A., Edmund Gosse (London: Secker & Warburg
1984)
Tinbergen, N., Study of instinct (Oxford: Clarendon Press
1951)
Tizard, B., Adoption: a second chance (London: Open
Books 1977)
Tizard, B. and Hughes, M., Young children learning
(London: Fontana 1984)
Tizard, B., Hughes, M., Pinkerton, G. and Carmichael, H.,
Adults cognitive demands at home and at nursery
school, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 23
(2) (1982), pp. 10817
Tizard, B., Mortimore, J. and Burchell, B., Involving
parents in nursery and infant schools (London: Grant
McIntyre 1981)
Tizard, B., Philps, J. and Plewis, I., Play in preschool
centres, I and II, Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 17 (1976), pp. 25174
Tizard, J., Schofield, W., Hewison, J., Collaboration
between teachers and parents in assisting childrens
reading, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 52
(1981), pp. 15
Tolstoy, L. (translated R. Edmonds), Anna Karenina
(Harmondsworth: Penguin Classics 1954)
Tough, J., Focus on meaning: a study of childrens use of
language (London: George Allen & Unwin 1973)
Tough, J., The development of meaning (London: George
Allen & Unwin 1977)
Trabasso, T., Representation, memory and reasons: how
do we make transitive inferences?, in A. D. Pick (ed.),
Minnesota symposium on child psychology, vol. 9
(Minneapolis: University of Minnesota 1975)
Trabasso, T. and Nicholas, D. W., Memory and inferences
in the comprehension of narratives, in F. Wilkening, J.
Becker and T. Trabasso (eds.), Information integration
by children (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1980)
Trehub, S. E. and Rabinovitch, M. S., Auditory-linguistic
sensitivity in early infancy, Developmental Psychology,
6 (1972), pp. 747
Trevarthen, C., Communication and cooperation in early
infancy, in M. Bullowa (ed.), Before speech
(Cambridge: Cambridge UP 1978)
Trevarthen, C., The foundations of intersubjectivity:
development of interpersonal and co-operative
understanding in infants, in D. R. Olson (ed.), The social
foundations of language and thought (New York: W. W.
Norton & Co. 1980)
Bibliography
Witkin, H. A. and Goodenough, D. R., Cognitive Styles:
essence and origins
(New York: International
Universities Press 1981)
Witkin, H. A., Goodenough, D. R. and Oltman, P. K.,
Psychological differentiation: current status, Journal
of Personality & Social Psychology, 37 (1979), pp.
112745
Wolff, P. H., The natural history of crying and other
vocalisations in early infancy, in B. M. Foss (ed.),
Determinants of infant behaviour, vol. IV (London:
Tavistock/Methuen 1966)
Wollheim, R. Freud (London: Fontana 1971)
Wood, D. J., Teaching the young child: some relationships
between social interaction, language and thought, in D.
R. Olson (ed.), The social foundations of language and
thought (New York: Norton 1980)
Wood, D. J., Working with underfives (London: Grant
McIntyre 1981)
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S. and Ross, G., The role of tutoring
in problem solving, Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 17 (2) (1976), pp. 89100
Yarrow, L. J. and Klein, R. P., Environmental discontinuity
associated with transition from foster to adoptive
homes, International Journal of Behavioural
Development, 3 (1980), pp. 31122
Yarrow, M. R. and Waxler, C. Z., Dimensions and
correlates of prosocial behaviour in young children,
Child Development, 47 (1976), pp. 11825
237
Author index
Author index
Campione, J. J. 47, 196
Candee, D. 171
Carey, S. 140
Carpenter, T. P. 101
Case, R. 40, 41, 43, 104
Cashdan, A. 25, 29, 30, 66, 70, 154, 186, 192, 194
Cavanaugh, J. C. 106
Chall, J. S. 78, 81, 131
Chapman, A. J. 191, 193
Chapman, M. 157
Chess, S. 144
Chi, M. T. H. 43, 75, 139
Chomsky, N. 123, 124
Cicirelli, V. G. 189
Clark, E. V. 64, 75, 119, 121, 123, 137, 139, 140
Clark, H. H. 119, 121, 123, 137
Clark, M. M. 78, 81
Clark, R. 136
Clark, R. W. 161
Clarke, A. D. B. 173, 181
Clarke, A. M. 173, 181
Clarke-Stewart, A. 186
Clay, M. 81, 91
Cobb, J. A. 191
Cochran-Smith, M. 81
Coffield, F. 185
Cohen, G. 42
Cohen, L. B. 72
Cole, M. 76, 141
Coleman, J. 146, 201, 202, 203
Coleman, M. 103
Collerson, J. 93
Collins, A. 83, 84
Condon, W. S. 118
Connolly, K. J. 30
Conroy, J. S. 101, 103
Cook, M. 110, 151
Corran, G. 102
Corrigan, R. 118
Coulthard, R. M. 132
Coveney, P. 96
Covitz, S. 126
Cox, B. 139
Cox, M. V. 37, 91, 95
Croll, P. 199
Crook, C. K. 53
Cross, T. 125
Crowder, R. G. 77, 78
Csiksentmihalyi, M. 24, 27
Curtiss, S. 173
Dale, P. S. 123
Dalton, K. 182
Davie, C. E. 98, 126, 131, 186
Davies, B. 153, 192, 199, 200
Dawkins, R. 155, 173
De Casper, A. J. 118
De Loache, J. S. 75, 106
De Mause, L. 13, 15
De Villiers, J. G. 119, 137
De Villiers, P. A. 119, 137
Delamont, S. 196, 199
Delia, J. 136
Dempster, F. N. 81
Desforges, C. 33, 37
Dickinson, J. 114, 115
Dodge, K. A. 153, 195
Doise, W. 105, 108
Dominic, J. F. 93
Donaldson, M. 40, 99
Douvan, E. 202
Downing, J. 78
Doyle, A.-B. 181
Dunn, J. F. 117, 144, 147, 148, 150, 157, 174, 187, 188, 189,
192, 196
Duveen, G. 109
Dweck, C. S. 150, 194, 195
Edmonds, R. 185
Edwards, C. P. 191
Ehrhardt, A. A. 54
Eimas, P. D. 118
Eisenberg-Berg, N. 164, 170
Elliot, A. J. 210
Elliot, E. S. 150, 194, 195
Ellis, A. W. 77, 78, 79
Emerson, P. E. 188
Emler, N. 105, 108, 114, 115
Erikson, E. 19, 145, 146, 201, 202
Ervin-Tripp, S. 136
Essen, J. 185
Etaugh, C. 196
Eysenck, H. J. 143, 144
Fagan, J. F. 50, 64, 72
Fagen, R. 24
Fein, G. G. 28, 29, 30, 186
Ferrara, R. A. 41, 69
Feshbach, N. D. 153
Feshbach, S. 153
Field, T. M. 175
Fifer, W. P. 118
239
Author index
Fine, G. A. 192, 200
Finkelstein, N. W. 150
Fischer, K. W. 147
Fivush, R. 198
Flapan, D. 110
Flavell, J. H. 37, 44, 73, 74, 75, 106, 108
Foot, H. C. 191, 193
Ford, M. E. 95
Forgas, J. P. 108
Francis, H. 77, 78, 79, 93
Fransella, F. 43, 139
Frederiksen, C. H. 93
Freeman, N. 37, 64, 91
Freud, A. 148
Freud, S. 25, 28, 145, 148, 161, 162, 170, 181, 201
Frith, U. 77
Fry, C. L. 119
Funnell, E. 81, 84, 107, 109, 112
Furman, W. 189
Furth, H. 113
Fuson, K. C. 98, 99
Gallistel, C. R. 98
Gallup, G. G. 147
Galton, M. 70, 199
Garbarino, J. 20, 185
Garmezy, N. 181
Garvey, C. 23, 25, 28, 29, 135
Gelman, R. 33, 34, 37, 98, 106, 109, 124, 139, 140, 188
Gewirth, A. 163
Gibson, E. J. 61, 64
Gibson, J. J. 61, 62, 63, 64, 65
Giddens, A. 127
Giles, H. 127, 133
Giller, H. 154, 204, 205, 208
Gilligan, C. 169, 170
Ginsberg, H. 138
Gleitman, L. R. 138
Gold, D. 181, 196
Goldman, J. 20
Goldman, R. 20
Goodenough, D. R. 70
Goodnow, J. J. 44
Goody, J. 76, 141
Gordon, J. C. B. 128, 129
Gottlieb, G. 16, 160
Gottman, J. M. 29, 191, 192
Gould, S. J. 24, 25, 26, 143
Grajek, S. 188
Gratch, G. 63
Graves, D. 97
240
Green, M. 154
Grice, H. P. 117, 128, 135
Grief, E. B. 170
Griffin, P. 76, 141, 203
Griffiths, M. 92, 93, 96
Groen, G. J. 101, 102
Gruendel, H. 112
Hagen, J. W. 70, 73
Haith, M. M. 47
Hale, G. H. 70
Hall, J. W. 98, 99
Halliday, M. A. K. 93, 119, 122, 135
Halverson, C. F. 144, 145
Hamlyn, D. W. 60
Hammersley, M. 199
Hardyment, C. 181
Hargreaves, D. 150, 199
Harlow, H. F. 16, 54, 160, 173
Harlow, M. K. 16, 54, 160, 173
Harris, P. L. 50, 61, 62, 64, 65, 77
Harter, S. 150, 202
Hartup, W. W. 147, 189, 191, 192, 193, 202
Harvey, P. G. 57
Hawkins, P. R. 128, 129
Hayward, C. 81, 83
Heath, S. B. 81, 125, 128, 133, 135, 145, 153
Heider, E. R. 139
Heider, F. 110
Held, D. 127
Herbert, G. W. 185
Herbert, M. 54
Hersov, L. 186
Hetherington, E. M. 181
Hewison, J. 81, 199
Hicks, L. 49
Hinde, R. A. 160, 176, 178, 179, 180, 188, 191
Hiscock, M. 59, 60
Hofer, M. A. 15, 16
Hoffman, M. L. 155, 157
Hoggart, R. 203
Houlbrooke, R. 13, 14
Howe, M. J. A. 85
Hughes, M. 84, 91, 92, 98, 102, 103, 107, 125, 128, 129,
130, 132, 186, 189
Humphreys, A. P. 27
Huston, A. 54, 192, 195, 196
Hutt, C. J. 25, 65, 66
Inhelder, B. 34, 37, 104, 106
Isbell, B. J. 108
Author index
Istomina, Z. M. 76, 107
Kurtines, W. 170
Maccoby, E. E. 148, 149, 152, 154, 158, 171, 183, 187, 192,
195, 202
Macfarlane, J. A. 53, 54
McGlaughlin, A. 185
McGurk, H. 70
Machida, S. 139
McKee, L. 108
Mackie, D. 108
Maclean, R. 78
McRobbie, A. 203
McShane, J. 77, 120, 121
McTear, M. 136
MacWhinney, B. 135
Madge, N. 18, 19, 176, 185
Main, M. 177, 178
Maliphant, R. 205
Mandler, J. M. 83
Manning, M. 155
Maratsos, M. 123, 125, 210
Marcus, G. B. 188
Markman, E. M. 84, 94, 139
241
Author index
Martin, B. 203
Martin, J. A. 158, 171, 202
Martines, L. 195
Martlew, M. 93
Matthews, R. 78
Mayer, R. 97, 100
Mead, G. H. 146, 147
Meadows, S. 25, 29, 30, 34, 37, 66, 70, 154, 186, 192, 194,
198, 199
Medrich, E. A. 200, 201
Mehler, J. 173
Meltzoff, A. 62
Meyer-Bahlburg, H. 54
Michalson, L. 152
Midgley, M. 109, 160, 161
Millar, S. 25
Miller, P. 163
Miller, S. A. 37
Mills, M. 81, 84, 107, 109, 112
Money, J. 54
Moore, M. K. 62
Mortimore, J. 199
Morton, J. 77
Moser, J. M. 101
Moskowitz, D. S. 181
Moss, H. A. 145
Much, N. C. 170
Mueller, E. C. 189, 191, 192
Mugny, G. 105, 108
Munsey, B. 169
Mussen, P. H. 164, 170
Myers, N. A. 72
Nava, M. 203
Nelson, K. 111, 112, 118, 125
Nelson, K. E. 72
Newson, E. 96, 154, 182, 191, 197, 199, 200, 201
Newson, J. 96, 154, 182, 191, 197, 199, 200, 201
Nicholas, D. W. 83
Nicholls, J. G. 83, 150
Ninio, A. 107, 120, 175
Nottebohm, F. 120
Nowicki, S. 149
Nucci, L. P. 170
Oakley, A. 175
Ochs, E. 125, 135
OFaolain, J. 195
Olson, D. R. 76, 135, 136, 138, 141
Olweus, D. 18, 150, 153, 154
Opie, I. 29, 153, 200
242
Author index
Roopnarine, J. L. 196
Rosaldo, M. Z. 195
Rosch, E. 75, 139
Rose, P. 179
Rosen, B. 169
Rosenblatt, D. 28
Rosenblum, L. A. 191
Ross, L. 108, 110, 111
Rotman, B. 37, 106
Rotter, J. 149
Roy, C. 65, 66
Rubin, J. Z. 196
Rubin, K. H. 28, 29, 30
Rubin, Z. 191, 192
Rumain, B. 38, 39
Rumelhart, D. 77
Russell, J. 40, 45, 105, 108
Rutter, M. 18, 19, 57, 79, 144, 154, 163, 176, 178, 181, 183,
185, 186, 191, 202, 203, 204, 205, 207, 208
Salapatek, P. 47, 48, 49, 50, 51, 77
Sander, L. W. 118
Saxton, M. 81
Sayers, J. 162
Scardamalia, M. 87, 88, 94, 95, 96
Scarr, S. 16, 17, 18, 57, 188
Schachter, F. F. 182, 188
Schaffer, H. R. 188
Schank, R. C. 84, 111, 118
Schieffelin, B. 81, 125, 135
Schofield, W. 81, 199
Schwartzmann, H. 30
Scott, M. S. 71
Scribner, S. 141
Searle, J. 45, 109, 117, 135
Serbin, L. A. 192, 196
Shaffer, D. 58
Shaffer, D. R. 183
Shantz, C. V. 108, 110, 111, 193, 194
Share, D. L. 78
Shatz, M. 107, 124, 135, 188
Shepard, R. N. 71
Sherrod, L. R. 177
Shields, M. M. 109
Shultz, T. R. 27
Shweder, R. A. 170
Siegel, L. S. 99, 101
Siegler, R. S. 34, 41, 43, 98, 99, 100, 101, 104
Sigman, M. D. 55, 56, 58, 59
Simon, B. 199
Simon, T. 65
Author index
Tulving, E. 75
Turiel, E. 164, 166, 170
Turnbull, C. 160
Vaillant, G. E. 146
Valentine, E. R. 104
Valiant, G. 105, 108
Vandell, D. L. 189, 191, 192
Vandenberg, B. 28, 29, 30
Vuyk, R. 33, 37
Vygotsky, L. S. 89, 109, 125, 139, 180
Wadsworth, M. 183, 185, 206, 207
Waldrop, M. F. 144, 145
Walker, C. 149
Walkerdine, V. 102
Wallace, J. G. 45, 100, 104
Walzer, M. 155
Ward, S. 115
Wason, P. C. 38, 44
Waters, E. 178
Watson, P. 148
Watt, I. 141
Waxler, C. Z. 155, 157, 158, 171
Weaver, J. 198, 199
Wedge, P. 185
Weikart, D. 186, 208
Weinberg, R. A. 18
Weiner, B. 149, 194
Weinreich-Haste, H. 170, 171
Weisz, J. R. 149
Wellman, H. M. 74, 106
Wells, B. W. P. 152
244
Wells, C. G. 78, 81, 91, 92, 93, 96, 118, 121, 123, 124, 125,
126, 128, 129, 130, 131, 132, 133, 138, 150, 181
West, D. J. 185, 205, 206, 207
Weston, D. R. 177, 178
Wetherford, M. J. 72
Whiting, B. B. 158, 188, 191
Whiting, J. W. M. 158, 188, 191
Whittaker, S. 74, 107
Willes, M. J. 131, 198, 199
Wilson, E. O. 159
Wilson, H. 185
Wimmer, H. 83
Wingfield, J. 183
Winnicott, D. W. 175
Witkin, H. A. 70
Wolff, P. H. 118
Wollheim, R. 162
Wood, D. J. 109, 125, 132, 133, 194
Woods, P. 199
Wright, H. F. 191
Yarrow, M. R. 155, 157, 158, 171
Young, G. 192
Yule, W. 79
Yussen, S. R. 74
Zahn-Waxler, C. 155, 157, 158, 171
Zajonc, R. B. 188
Zelazo, P. R. 72
Zingg, R. M. 173
Zipes, J. 81
Subject index
245
Subject index
c cognitive processes 405, 1048; see also arithmetic;
attention;
memory;
metacognition;
reading;
remembering; writing
cognitive
structures
335,
37,
40
cognitive style 701, 145
comfort strategies 1556
communication: in infancy 174, 176, 180; as language
function 117, 120, 122, 12445, 1356; within families
182,
1834,
1856
comparing quantities 99100
competence, sense of: and moral action 1701; and
personality development 113, 14950; and school
achievement 1935, 1989
competence-performance models 37, 45
competition 160, 197
complementarity in relationships 158, 179, 180, 1889,
191
complexity: as cause of infant pattern preference 50; in
language development 124, 128, 130; in recognition
memory 72
comprehension: and metacognition 1046; in reading 77;
of text 836
concept of childhood 1314, 96; cultural variations in, and
adultchild language 125, 129, 133
concepts 42, 1223, 13940
conditioning: of altruistic behaviour 158; and memory in
infants 72; and moral development 163; and Social
Learning Theory 163
confirmation and cognitive development 434, 109
conflict: and adolescence 202; between interests at school
199; between siblings 189, 190; cognitive 346, 37, 43
5, 1045, 106; and development of aggression 1525;
and moral development 164, 166, 168, 169; and
personality stages 1456, 1612
conscience 148, 162, 163, 1701; see also superego
conservation 33, 634, 99100, 105
consistency: in cognition 36, 37, 435; in language 117,
129, 132, 133, 137; in moral behaviour 163, 166, 170; in
personality 1426
continuity: in development 1819; and early experience
144, 145; of parenting over generations 1835
conversation 118, 1246, 128, 1367
co-operation: in cognitive development 1089; and moral
development 158, 160; with peers 158, 1912; with
siblings 158, 1889
counting 989
crisis theory: and adolescence 146, 202; of personality
development 145, 146, 1612
cross-model integration 612
246
Subject index
evolution and development 1617; of attachment 175, 177;
of play 257
executive strategies in cognition 40
experience: in brain development 5860; in cognitive
development 34; in sensory development 49, 53
extraversionintroversion 1434
eye: anatomy 479; functioning 4951; movements 501,
77
faces, infants preference for 50, 1734
family environment 1807; and adolescent relationships
2023; and delinquency 19, 2057; and language
development 1246, 12732, 1334; and literacy 789,
967; and personality 144, 145, 146, 1534, 157, 158;
and sibling relationships 157, 18790; see also parent
child relations; parenting
feedback: in cognition 435; in memory 74; in
metacognition 104, 105, 106
feeling and morality 1612, 16971
feminism, moral necessity of 195
feral children 16, 173
field dependence 701
flexibility: in genetic programming 17; in play 24, 656
framing: of children by parents 1745, 180, 185, 187;
play as 27
friends 199, 200; accounts of 193; choices of 189, 1967;
skills 1913
functions of language 1357
gender: development 192, 1958; and identification 148;
and morality 170; and self-categorization 147; see also
sex differences
genes: and aggression 19, 152; and altruism 15961; and
development 15, 16, 17, 19, 54; and sex differences 195;
and sibling differences 188
grammatical rules 1224, 137, 1389
Gurin, Mme 173
habituation 64, 72
hearing, development of 513
heteronomous morality 1656
historicism 17
history of childhood 1314
home language: and reading 789; and school behaviour
1314; variation in 125,12631
hormones and behaviour 54, 151, 196
hyperactivity and attention deficits 57, 70
id 161
identification 1489; and Oedipus complex 162; and sex
differences 148, 196, 197; and sibling relationships 188;
and Social Learning Theory 163
247
Subject index
locus of control 149
locutionary force 135
logic 37, 3840
maternal deprivation 181
maternal instinct 1756
mathematics 33, 97103
maturation 34, 54
meaning: and categorization 75, 1213, 13940; in early
social interaction 174, 176; in language 1201; 1213,
125, 131, 135, 138; of text 778, 84, 88
measuring 99
memory: development 707, 107; and literacy 76, 1401;
mnemonic activities 737; working memory 405
mental space 405
metacognition 736, 789, 85, 1048, 189; metalanguage
789, 133, 1378; metamemory 736, 107
metaphor 122, 139
Middlemarch 164
modelling: of aggression 154; of altruism 158; of language
124, 125, 1301, 1323; of parenting 183, 185, 186, 189;
by parents 174; of reading 789, 81; of writing 913, 96,
1067
modulation of arousal: by parents 175; through play 27
monitoring: in cognitive development 435; of
comprehension of text 835; in language 129, 1379,
1401; of writing 88
moral development 1558, 15971
moral reasoning and judgement 160, 163, 16471
moral rules 1634, 16671
motherchild relations 12, 20, 174, 1756, 1778;
and personality development 1435, 1545
motherese 1246; use by siblings 174, 188
motivation: and moral action 1701; self-efficacy 14950,
1935, 1989; and study skills 857; and writing 91, 96
7
myelination: of neurons 55; of optic pathways 48
naming 1202
Nature 17, 1601
negative instances 44
neoteny 17, 256
nerve development 556
neuropathology and lead pollution 578
neuroticism 143, 144
neurotransmitters 58
noviceexpert differences 75, 1067
number 33, 98, 99, 100
nursery rhymes 28; and playground rhymes 192, 200
object identity 64
248
Subject index
pretend play 29
primacy effect in memory 72, 76
primary maternal preoccupation 175
pro-social behaviour 1558, 160, 1889
psychiatric disorder 19, 146, 184, 203, 2056
reading: language knowledge and 789, 131, 133, 138;
phonemic awareness and 7880, 88; processes 768;
strategies in 7984; and writing 88
recency effects in memory 72
reciprocity: dimension of relationships 179; parentchild
180; peers 1912; siblings 18991
redundancies in cognition 434
reference 120, 1212, 128, 13940; over- and underextension in 1223; referential communication 1056,
128, 129, 131
regularities in cognition 1056
regulation of cognition 435
rehearsal 735, 76
relationships: dimensions of 1789; and temperament 144;
see also complementarity; parent; peers; reciprocity;
siblings; social relationships
remembering 717; ecology of 767; and study skills 84
5; training of 737; see also memory
repertory grid analyses of concept organization 43, 139
representation: of knowledge 42, 60; in language 1201,
1212; need for social experience for 173; social 113
15; and symbolic play 29
resources for cognition 402, 44
retardation of development 26; see also neoteny revision of
text 956
role play 29, 189, 192
rough and tumble play 29, 192, 197, 200
Rousseau, Jean-Jacques 15
rules: cognitive 43; moral 1634, 169, 170; rule-governed
play 289
scaffolding 28, 1246, 1745
scanning pictures 702
school: childrens concepts of 131, 198200; and home 74,
789, 12635; language 1315; and personality
development 14950, 194, 197
scripts: in play 29; of school 198; in social cognition 111
13; and stories 83, 84, 113; in writing 93, 967
segmentation of speech sounds 77, 789, 123, 137, 140
self-categorization: by age 1478; by gender 1467, 196
8; and school 199
self-concept: in adolescence 205, 2068; development of
65, 14650; and school 857, 131, 133, 191; and study
skills 857
249
Subject index
social worlds: adolescents 2013; in Freudian theory 161
2; in moral reasoning 170; of school 198200; sex
differences in 192, 1967
socialization: and aggression 1534; of children by parents
181, 1836; and developmental change 180, 182; and
morality 158, 161, 163, 1656; of parents by children
187; and temperament 144; via books and stories 813
sociobiology and altruism 15961
sorting behaviour 139
sound-letter correspondences 7880, 91, 103
sound localization 52, 53
speech acts 11718, 120, 128, 1323, 1357
speech sounds: infants perception of 118; infants
production of 119; and reading 778, 7880
speed, cognitive development as increase in 41
spelling 77, 78, 7880, 88, 97
stage models 1920, 345, 37, 113; of moral development:
Kohlbergs 16670; Piagets 1646; neo-Piagetian 19,
113; of personality development 145, 1601; Piagets, of
cognitive development 19, 345, 37; of reading 7980
stories: childrens understanding of 813; childrens
writing of 967; as mode of socializing children 813; in
moral reasoning research 1656, 169; and reading 75,
78, 81, 84
story reading 78, 81
stress 24, 144, 2013, 2068; see also disadvantage
study skills 80, 847
subitizing 98
subtraction 101, 103
sucking 60, 174
Sullivan, Anne 121
superego 1612
supervision: of delinquents 205, 206, 207; of girls 196, 197,
200
syllogisms 3840
250
symbol manipulation 41
symbolic play 29
symbolic representation 37, 91, 1202
taste development 534
teachers: childrens accounts of teachers activities 198;
and childrens language 1315; of delinquent children
205, 207; interests in classroom 1989; judgements of
children 1315, 198; and sex typing 1967
television 196, 201, 205
temperament 1445; and aggression 19, 153
text: comprehension of 75, 835, 1056; meaning in 946
touch development 534
transfer: and memory 74; of metacognition 107
transitions between environments 21, 201, 202, 203, 207
transitive inference 378, 41
turntaking: in conversation 1357; in social interaction
174, 176
verbal communication 1056, 128, 129, 131
vigilance 70
vision 4751
visual cortex 18, 489
vocabulary 1212, 123, 127, 131, 133, 139, 140
vulnerability 144, 201, 2068
warmth/responsivity 144, 154, 158, 1834
withdrawal, as temperament dimension 144
word: coinage 140; meaning 138, 13940; order 1224;
production 119,1203
working memory 405
writig: basic skills 8891; childrens 967; composition
878, 935; functions 913; review 88, 956, 97
youth culture 203