(Thesis) Color Emotion in Arc
(Thesis) Color Emotion in Arc
(Thesis) Color Emotion in Arc
IN INTERIOR SPACES
A DISSERTATION
SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF
INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE AND
ENVIRONMENTAL DESIGN AND THE GRADUATE
SCHOOL OF ECONOMICS AND SOCIAL
SCIENCES OF HSAN DORAMACI BLKENT
UNIVERSITY
IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE
REQUIREMENTS
FOR THE DEGREE OF
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
IN ART, DESIGN AND ARCHITECTURE
By
Elif Helvacolu
July, 2011
To my parents
Oya & Kadir Helvacolu
COLOUR-EMOTION ASSOCIATIONS
IN INTERIOR SPACES
A DISSERTATION
SUBMITTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF
INTERIOR ARCHITECTURE AND
ENVIRONMENTAL DESIGN AND THE GRADUATE
SCHOOL OF ECONOMICS AND SOCIAL
SCIENCES OF HSAN DORAMACI BLKENT
UNIVERSITY
IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE
REQUIREMENTS
FOR THE DEGREE OF
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
IN ART, DESIGN AND ARCHITECTURE
By
Elif Helvacolu
July, 2011
ii
ABSTRACT
iii
ZET
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I am heartily thankful to my advisor Assist. Prof. Dr Nilgn Olguntrk who
introduced me into a colourful world. I would like to express my gratitude to
her for the continuous support of my Ph.D. study and research from the initial to
the final level. Without her motivation, enthusiasm, and immense knowledge,
this dissertation would not have been possible. I wish to keep up any
collaboration in our colourful world in the future.
I am honoured to thank my committee member Prof. Dr. Halime Demirkan
whose encouragement, advice and crucial contribution throughout my graduate
and Ph.D. studies I will never forget. I wish also show my appreciation to
Assoc. Prof. Dr. idem Erbu as another member of my committee for her
valuable and generous suggestions during the preparation process of this
dissertation.
I owe my deepest gratitude to Assist. Prof. Dr. Meltem Grel and Assist. Prof. Dr.
Gler Ufuk Demirba, for their critical comments regarding the finalization of
the dissertation. Besides, I would like to express my appreciation to Dr. Dilek
Gven for her suggestions throughout the statistical analyses of the thesis.
I would like to extend my heartfelt gratitude to my dearest friend Segah Sak
who patiently was with me from the very beginning of my academic adventure.
You were such a wonderful motivator even when coping seemed tough for me.
I will never forget out entertaining conversations that evoked wonderful ideas.
I owe my gratitude to nci Cantimur for patiently helping me to create the
virtual spaces for my study. In addition, special thanks go to Asl ebi, Seden
Odabaolu and Nalan nalhars for their friendship and moral support.
My deepest gratitude goes to my parents Oya and Kadir Helvacolu for their
unconditional support. I am very honoured and lucky to have you as my parents.
Thank you for giving me chances to prove and improve myself through all my
walks of life. Moreover, I would like to give my sincere thanks to my wonderful
family Cem and Didem Helvacolu, Sibel and Bahtiyar Yldz for their invaluable
support and trust.
Last but not least, I am greatly indebted to my fianc Alpaslan Gne for his
unflagging love, trust and encouragement in my life. You will always be the most
special of my life.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
2.
1.1.
1.2.
EMOTION
2.1.
2.4.
2.5.
3.
COLOUR BASICS
43
3.1.
3.2.
3.3.
4.
69
4.1.
4.2.
6.
84
5.1.
5.2.
THE EXPERIMENT
6.1.
88
6.2.3.2.
6.2.3.3.
6.2.3.4.
7. FINDINGS
7. 1.
105
7.2.
7.3.
8. DISCUSSION
123
9. CONCLUSION
131
REFERENCES
136
APPENDICES
146
ix
LIST OF TABLES
Table H.3. Statistics for gender differences for red room ..........................................173
Table H.4. Independent Samples t-Test for gender differences on emotional
reactions to red room ............................................................................................173
Table H.5. Frequency of emotions associated with red room in respect to
male gender group ..................................................................................................174
Table H.6. Frequency of emotions associated with red room in respect to
female gender group .............................................................................................174
Table H.7. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association
to red room ................................................................................................................175
Table H.8. Frequency of emotions associated with red room ..................................175
Table H.9. Statistics of the sequence of showing gray room .....................................175
Table H.10. Independent Samples t-Test results for sequence differences on
emotional reactions to gray room ....................................................................176
Table H.11. Statistics for gender differences for gray room in the first
experiment set ..........................................................................................................176
Table H.12. Independent Samples t-Test for gender differences on emotional
reactions to gray room in the first experiment set ...................................176
Table H.13. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in respect
to male gender group in the first experiment set ......................................177
Table H.14. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in respect
to female gender group in the first experiment set ..................................177
Table H.15. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association
to gray room in the first experiment set .......................................................178
Table H.16. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in the first
experiment set ..........................................................................................................178
xi
xiv
LIST OF FIGURES
xv
xvi
Figure 7.6. The distribution of emotions on the green room in respect to gender
group .........................................................................................................................114
Figure 7.7. The distribution of emotions on the green room ...................................115
Figure 7.8. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in respect to gender
group in the second experiment set ............................................................116
Figure 7.9. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in the second
experiment set .......................................................................................................117
Figure 7.10. The distribution of emotions on the blue room in respect to gender
group .......................................................................................................................119
Figure 7.11. The distribution of emotions on the blue room ...................................120
Figure 7.12. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in respect to
gender group ..........................................................................................................121
Figure 7.13. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in the third
experiment set ....................................................................................................122
Figure C.1. A view showing the public study area next to the
Multimedia Room .................................................................................................158
Figure C.2. A view showing the booths for using audio and visual
materials...................................................................................................................158
Figure D.1. A view from red wall-coloured living room .............................................160
Figure D.2. A view from blue wall-coloured living room ...........................................160
Figure D.3. A view from green wall-coloured living room .........................................161
Figure D.4. A view from gray wall-coloured living room ...........................................161
Figure G.1. The facial expressions of six basic emotions used in the study .......171
xvii
1. INTRODUCTION
Colour affects every part of our lives. It has an impact on human being
psychologically and physiologically. In addition, as a vital design element, colour
has a strong relationship with emotion. These statements are supported by
manifestations of colour not only in product design and marketing, but also in a
variety of other fields like colour therapy, colour mediation and image
consulting (Jin, Yu, Kim, Kim, & Chung, 2009).
Just as the phenomenon of colour and emotion relationship has been studied by
consumer, marketing and advertising industry and by industrial design. For the
Many colour research studies have been conducted on the relationship between
human emotions and colour. These studies mostly focused on the human
emotional reactions to a specific colour sample to form positive and negative
connotations by using defined adjectives for mood tones and emotions. In these
studies, participants simply match the adjectives with different colours without
any reference to interior space (Terwogt & Hoeksma, 1995; Zentner, 2011; Gao
& Xin, 2006; Manav, 2007; Pos & Green-Armytage, 2007).
Although there are numerous studies about both colour and emotion, there are
not enough research handling the association of emotion and interior space, and
also there is a lack of research combining both the concepts of colour and
2
1.1.
The main objective of this study is to examine the relationship between two
phenomenons of design colour and emotion in interior space. The research
questions are:
How individual colours affect human emotional reactions in interior
spaces,
Do the human emotional reactions to individual colours differ in the same
interior space?
In what way colours emotional influences are differing from each other?
The relation between colour and emotion is examined in the fourth chapter.
The symbolic meanings of chromatic and neutral colours and review of the
studies on colour emotion associations are given in detail to draw attention to
employed methodologies and emotion measurement instruments.
The fifth section explores the place of colour in interior space in the framework
of emotion is stated with some of the conducted studies in the field.
The sixth section describes the experiment with the aim, research questions and
hypotheses. The methodology of the experiment is defined with the
identification of the sample group, description of the setting, and the
explanation of the experiment procedures in detail.
The findings of the experiment that are statistically analyzed and evaluated are
given with the visual materials in the seventh section. In the eighth section the
discussion of the findings follows in relation to previous studies relevant to the
subject. The ninth chapter presents the major conclusions of the study and
suggestions for further research.
Visual and written materials that are involved in the experiment and results of
the statistical analysis which are not stated in the main body of the dissertation
are included in the appendices.
2. EMOTION
and glandular systems; or, by the expressive patterns or motor behaviours that
occurs as a result of emotions (Izard, 1977). A complete definition of emotion
must take into account the three aspects or components as:
(a) The experience or conscious feeling of emotion,
(b) The processes that occur in the brain and nervous system,
(c) The observable expressive patterns of emotions particularly those on
face (p. 4).
Feelings
Feelings and emotions are generally confederate in everyday discourse
(TenHouten, 2007). Feelings mean a persons own state of mind, especially
with reference to an evaluation of what is agreeable and disagreeable, pleasant
or unpleasant (p. 4). Emotions contain actions and movements, often in public
view, appeared in facial expression, posture, gesture, specific behaviours, and
conversation. Unlike emotions, feelings are private, playing out not in the body
but in the mind and at a higher level (TenHouten, 2007). Thus, feelings contain
some kind of pressure towards action (Levy, 1984).
Sentiments
Sentiments involve romantic love, parental love, loyalty, patriotism, trust,
friendship, happiness. A particular person or object is typically central in
sentiments. A person can have a longstanding love for a mate or parent, a
longstanding sorrow for someone who has died, and a longstanding hostility to
a rival or competitor (TenHouten, 2007, p. 6). As they relate to specific objects,
situations, and processes, they are derived and continue to exist.
Sentiments are mostly acquired on the basis of previous experience and social
learning. On the other hand, certain sentiments such as dislike for seeing blood
or of unstable surfaces may have an innate basis (Frijda, 1994).
8
Moods
Moods are distinguished from emotions in terms of time course, moods last
longer than emotion (Ekman, 1994). Moods may last for hours even for days.
However, if it states for weeks or months, it is not identified as a mood but
identified as an affective disorder.
Moods basically cite the subjects own situation. Contrary to emotions, moods
are generally of less intensity (TenHouten, 2007). Clark and Isen (as cited in
Parkinson, 1997) claimed that moods are affective states like emotions; they
have an evaluative component as feeling of good or bad. But unlike emotions,
moods do not usually take a definite object (you can just be grumpy as a result
of getting out of bed the wrong side without any particular focus to the
experience) (Parkinson, 1997, p. 3).
Another feature distinguishing moods from emotion is about having own facial
expression. Contrary to many of the emotions, moods do not have their own
unique facial expression; one infers an irritable mood by seeing many facial
expressions of anger, but there is no distinctive facial expression of irritability
itself (Ekman, 1994, p. 57).
10
On the other side, Ekman and Friesen (1971) suggested that there are six basic
biologically programmed emotions. These involve happiness, sadness, fear,
anger, surprise, and disgust, each with its own distinctive facial expression.
Higher cognitive emotions are the other category of emotions that are more
cortical than the basic emotions. They are more capable of being affected by
12
conscious thoughts; they exhibit more cultural variations (Evans, 2002). They
take longer time to build up and to die away, than the basic emotions. Higher
cognitive emotions involve love, guilt, shame, embarrassment, pride, envy and
jealousy.
the individual evaluates the relevance of the current situation to personal wellbeing, weighing up whether it has good or bad implications for pre-eminent
concerns, and implicitly asking the question: Am I in trouble or am I OK?
(Lazarus, 1968). In secondary appraisal, the concern is on the options for coping
and expectations; the individual evaluates his or her capacity for handling the
situation -coping potential-, asking, in other words, What can be done about it?
(Lazarus, 1968). The primary appraisal components are goal relevance, goal
congruency or incongruency and type of ego-involvement; the secondary
appraisal components are blame or credit, coping potential and future
expectations (Lazarus, 1991, p. 39). By the pattern of primary or secondary
appraisal component, each individual emotion is differentiated.
or someone elses; and whether the event conforms to or conflicts with your
norms (p. 6-7).
2. Bodily changes:
Lazarus (1991) maintained that Autonomic Nervous System (ANS) activity and
its end-organ effects, brain activity, and hormonal secretions are sometimes
phenomena of emotions. The responses may occur in characteristic of increase
in heart rate and blood pressure (Arnold, 1960), perspiration, and other bodily
stirrings (Dennis, 1989). Cannon (1929) argued that all the excited emotions
such as anger and fear are actually accompanied by the following set of
responses characteristically increased respiratory volume, constriction of the
blood vessels in the skin (pallor), dilation of pupils, arrest of gastro-intestinal
activity, decreased salivation (dry mouth), and increased action of the sweat
glands (as cited in Parkinson, 1997, p. 7).
Ekman (1984) reported that among emotions there are differential activities
not only in skin temperature but also in heart rate. ANS activity shows
differences both between positive and negative emotions, and in patterns of
ANS (see Figure 2.1).
15
HEART RATE
high
low
SKIN
TEMPERATURE
Happy
Disgust
Surprise
high
Anger
low
Fear
Sad
3. Emotional expression:
Expressive behaviour is one of the most obvious indicators of emotional
experience (Parkinson, 1997). Expression refers to movement and sounds made
by someone indicating the presence of emotion to someone else. These
movement and sounds are expressive to the extent that they communicate
emotional information either be deliberate or intentional. Because of face is
capable of a wide variety of subtly patterned movements, it is the most
important channel of emotional expression. Additionally, emotion can be
expressed through tone of voice, bodily posture, and gestures (Dennis, 1989).
4. Motivated action:
Emotions include the impulse to act in certain ways that is appropriate for the
particular emotion. One may feel a strong urge to hit out at someone in some
16
way when angry; to seek out for the company of your loved one and get as close
as you possibly can to him or her when in love; and may feel the strong desire to
run away, literally or metaphorically when afraid (Parkinson, 1997). In this
perspective, emotions should be seen as inherently motivational states that
serve to particular functions.
stimulus
snake
bodily response
(emotion)
tight stomach
quick hearbeat
feeling
(perception
of emotion)
fear
action
throw stone,
run
17
Factor 1: Appraisal:
The process of appraisal involves setting criteria and evaluating the outcome of
coping efforts (Leventhall, 1984). It is defined as the perception and evaluation
of the emotional event, with regard to its valence and its relevant properties for
dealing with it (Frijda, 1994, p. 61).
It is the first and most central factor in the generation of emotion (Parkinson,
1997). Appraisals theorists suggested that emotions are not always the direct
reactions to stimulus qualities; rather, what gives an object emotional impact is
its relevance to the individuals personal concerns. Smith and Lazarus (1993)
maintained an appraisal role as combining emotional responses to
environmental conditions on one side, and personal goals and beliefs on the
other side.
18
in understanding the conditions for the criterion of various emotions, but also in
distinguishing emotions from each other.
Factor 2: Arousal:
State of arousal involves diverse processes that control activation, wakefulness,
motor behaviour, and alertness. In physiological patterns, it contains
autonomic activation, hormonal events, mechanisms in the brainstem and
events in the cerebral cortex (DeCatanzaro, 1999). Arousal happens when the
body releases chemicals into the brain that act to stimulate emotions, reduce
cortical functioning and conscious control, and create physical agitation and
'readiness for action' (changingminds, para . 4).
19
Performance
efficiency
low
medium
arousal
high
Evaluative
feelings
Encounter
Appraisal
(and other
factors)
Bodily
reaction
Emotional
experience
Expressive
responses
Action
tendencies
Note: Broken lines represent linkages that are possible rather than necessary.
Figure 2.4. A four-factor theory of emotion (Parkinson, 1997, p. 17).
21
1. Emotions and the Body: As part of an integrated whole, both the face and
the body, contribute to convey the emotional state of the individual
(Shan, Gong, & McOwan, 2007). Simonov (as cited in Izard, 1977)
reported that in the electrical activity of the brain, in the circulatory
system, and the respiratory system changes occur.
23
6. Emotions and Sex: Virtually the sex drive always interacts with some
emotion (Izard, 1977). The sex drive interacting with anger and
contempt may result in sadism or rape; with guilt may produce
impotence or masochism; with excitement and joy may produce love and
marriage and produce peak experiences of sensory pleasure and
emotion.
24
Findings of Ekman and Friesens (1971) study supported that particular facial
behaviors are universally associated with particular emotions. They reported
that experience within a culture, the kinds of events that elicit particular
emotions, may act to influence the ability to discriminate particular pairs of
emotions. The reason for not differentiating the fear faces from surprise faces
may be due to fearful events are almost always also surprising in this culture
such as the sudden appearance of a hostile member of another village, the
unexpected meeting of a ghost or sorcerer, etc. Ekman (1992) reported that
emotions of happiness, surprise, fear, sadness, anger and disgust are the
consistent emotions that have universally accepted facial expressions.
25
26
28
Afraid
Hopeful
Amused
Interested
Angry
Joyful
Anxious
Nervous
Calm
Peaceful
Carefree
Regretful
Cheerful
Remorseful
Concerned
Sad
Confide
Tense
Depressed
Troubled
Edgy
Uncomfortable
Emotional
Uneasy
Guilty
Upset
Happy
Worried
29
30
Positive End:
Middle:
Strongly agree
Neutral
Negative End
Strongly disagree
There are different kinds of rating scales namely, numerical scales, graphic
scales, percentage rating, standard scales, scales of cumulated points and forced
choice scales. In addition, the response scale might be unipolar in which the
measurement is based on a single concept per scale like not at all angry to
extremely angry or bipolar in which the scale is anchored at either end by
terms with opposing meaning like unpleasant to pleasant (Schubert, 1999).
The most common manifestation of bipolar rating scale is the semantic
differential.
The Semantic Differential (SD) measures peoples reactions to stimuli for rating
on bipolar scales that specified with contrasting adjectives at each end
(Heise, 1970) (see Figure 2.9). Singh (1984) defines SD scale as a collection of
scales in which absolute ratings of concepts are done; the concept refers to the
object which is to be rated (p. 256). A concept to be differentiated is provided
to the participant in addition to a set of bipolar adjectival scales against which to
do it (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1978). The only task is to indicate for each
item, the direction of his association and its intensity on a seven-step scale. It is
important for the participant to be as representative as possible of all the ways
31
(Concept)
Happy_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____Sad
Hard _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____Soft
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(polar term X)
(5)
(6)
(7)
(polar term Y)
The position marked 4 is labelled neutral; neither X nor Y; the 3 and 5 positions
are labelled slightly X and slightly Y respectively; the 2 and 6 positions quite X
and quite Y, and the 1 and 7 positions extremely X and extremely Y (Heise,
1970; Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1978). A scale like the Figure 4 measures
directionality of a reaction like happy versus sad, hard versus soft and the
intensity from slight through extreme.
There are other various constructed instruments that are used in self-report
measurements. One of them is a questionnaire measure called the Affect Grid
which is designed to assess two dimensions of affect: Pleasure-displeasure and
arousal-sleepiness (Russell, Weiss, & Mendelson, 1989). It is composed of
32
a nine-by-nine matrix and the emotion adjectives are placed at the midpoints of
each side of the grid (Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999) (see Figure 2.10). The
research participant reads firstly the general instructions and then is given
specific instructions, such as "Please rate how you are feeling right now and
then places one checkmark somewhere in the grid (Russell, Weiss, &
Mendelson, 1989).
Stress
High
Arousal
Unpleasant Feeling
Excitement
Pleasant Feelings
Depression
Relaxation
Sleepiness
Figure 2.10. The Affect Grid. (Russell, Weiss, & Mendelson, 1989)
The other scale used in self-report is Mood Adjective Check List (MACL) in
which the participant is asked to rate how s/he felt, during when the emotion
adjective was read on a specific scale such as definitely felt it, slightly, cannot
decide, definitely not (Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999). Another important
response scale is Visual Analog Scales (VAS) in which two opposing adjectives
33
Place a vertical mark on the line below to indicate how sad you feel you are
today?
Not at all
Extremely
much
The advantages of self reports are that rating scales can be confederates to
designate any set of emotions, and can be used to measure not only individual
emotions but also mixed emotions (Desmet, 2003). They are assumed to be the
best sources of information about an individuals emotional experience
(Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999), and they are simple, straightforward and
generally quite reliable (Scherer, 2005). On the other hand, the main
disadvantage of self-reports is their difficult application between cultures due to
translation (Desmet, 2003). In emotion studies, it is difficult to translate
emotion words as straight translation is not available. Usage of colour emotion
words and their characteristics change with languages (e.g. Nakamura,
Sakolnakorn, Hansuebsai, Pungrassamee, & Sato, 2004). To overcome the
problem of between-culture comparisons, a handful of non-verbal self-report
instruments have recently been developed in which pictograms are used
34
The SAM is a non-verbal pictorial scale instrument that directly measures the
arousal, valence and dominance associated with a participants affective
reaction to various types of stimuli (Bradley & Lang, 1994; Oliveira, Fonseca,
Teixeira, & Simes, 2005). Participants point out the puppets that they think
best portray their emotions in a specific moment (see Figure 2.12). However,
there is an important limitation for its application in between-culture studies:
They do not measure distinct emotions but only generalised emotional states
(Desmet, 2003). Like SAM, there are some other non-verbal pictorial scale
instruments in which universally accepted facial expressions of emotion are
used (see Figures 2.13 and 2.14). Karadoaner (2010) generated one neutral
and six basic emotions (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, and disgust)
by using Poser Software according to the Facial Action Coding System (FACS)
descriptions and the results of the Action Units (AU) (see Appendix A2)
(see Figure 2.15).
35
36
37
Surprise
Happiness
Neutral
Disgust
Fear
Anger
Sadness
One of the most important measurement issues of emotion is the timing (Larsen
& Fredrickson, 1999). Emotions take time and they are dynamic processes that
unfold, linger, and then dissipate over time-sometimes gradually, other times
rapidly (p. 42). They consist of cascade of discrete response systems that have
its own time of onset and duration. Therefore, it is critical to capture the
38
dynamic aspects of the concept under study. Self-report measures may increase
the chance of capturing the dynamic moments of the emotional effect
(Larsen & Fredrickson, 1999).
the nose (see Appendix A). This instrument illustrates all possible
movements in the skin of the face observable to the naked eye and can
support by photographs and videotape.
41
The major advantage of the facial, vocal and physiological measures of emotions
(non-verbal instruments) is that, they are language-independent. Thus they can
be used in between-cultures studies (Desmet, 2003). Additionally, they are
unobtrusive as they do not disturb participants during the measurement.
However, the most obvious limitation of these instruments is that, they can only
reliably assess a limited set of basic emotions such as anger, fear, and surprise;
they cannot assess mixed emotions.
42
3. COLOUR BASICS
3.1. Colour: A Definition
With no doubt, colour has potency in every side of life. From birth to death, our
life scenario covers colour notionally and perceptibly. We perceive and
experience them, thus we become more aware of them. Day starts with the
sunrise colour and ends with the sunset colour, and in between we also witness
various colours with different tones and shades in the natural environment. We
design our personal environments and feel sense of belonging with the use of
favourite and suitable colours in the design process. Each colour has its own
meaning. Thus, each of them evokes different feelings. This suggests that with
its potency, it may affect human beings psychologically, physiologically and
behaviourally.
materials and surfaces including everything from light and paint to art, from
aesthetics to functionality and as an inseparable element of design
(Dalke, Little, Niemann, Camgz, Steadman, Hill, & Stott, 2005, p. 343).
Hue is the name of a colour and represents the chromatic aspect. It allows us to
differentiate one colour from another such as red from yellow, or green from
blue (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). White, gray and black as being achromatic
colours are considered to be colours without hue (Raskin, 1986).
44
which the area in which the visual stimulus presented appears to emit more or
less light proportion to that emitted area perceived as a white stimulus
(p. 494). In making contrast more effective, the attribute of brightness becomes
an important aspect. Colour values are also called shades. Raskin (1986) defined
these terms with the explanations of adding black to a colour decreases its
brightness and produces a shade ( p. 7).
Saturation also known as chroma and colourimetric purity is the strength and
vividness of a hue. Thus, it represents the amount of pigment in a colour
(Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). It allows us to distinguish a strong colour from
a weak one by representing the distance of a colour sensation from that of white
or gray (Munsell, 1988; Saunders, 1998). It is related with the intensity of
colour. Saturation is also related with tints. Raskin (1986) defines this as while
adding white to a colour decrease its saturation and produces a tint (p. 7).
45
There are ten major hues in the hue circle of the Munsell System that appear in
an order (clockwise) (Agoston, 1987; seen in Figure 3.1). Five principal hues are
red (5R), yellow (5Y), green (5G), blue (5B), and purple (5P). Five intermediate
hues are yellow-red (5YR), green-yellow (5GY), blue-green (5BG), purple-blue
(5PB), and red-purple (5RP). These five intermediate hues are after-images of
the principal hues, forming the basis for Munsells complementaries (Hunt,
1987). After-imaging is an optical reaction that occurs after staring intensely at
a hue and then shifting our eyes to a white surface; this second hue is termed
the after-image (Feishner, 2006, p. 11).
46
The hue circle is subdivided by a scale including 100 equally spaced Hue radii. A
hue range (for example R) consists of eleven hue radii; 0-10. For each hue range
there is a major hue that is located at the middle of each hue range, along hue
radius 5. The numbering of radii works clockwise from 0 to 10 in each range
(Agoston, 1987).
The divisions between 5R and 5YR are designated as; 6R, 7R, 8R, 9R, 10R, YR,
2YR, 3YR and 4YR with similar designations between other hues (Hunt, 1987).
Other divisions are represented by using decimals; 2.5 YR is intended to be
perceptually midway between samples having hues of 2YR and 3YR.
The Value (V) notation is defined on a scale from 0-10 and refers to the lightness
of perceived colour much as the luminance factor (Agoston, 1987). It includes
ten main steps, with white designated 10 and black 0, grays from 1 to 9 as they
47
Figure 3.2. A view showing hue, value and chroma scales arranged in colour
space.
(http://www.aadip9.net/flavie/2010/02/munsells-color-systemdocument.html)
48
By a colour solid or colour space, Munsell scales of hue, value and chroma can
be visualized (see Figure 3.2). The central vertical axis steps from black at the
bottom to white at the top. Chroma scales radiate in equal visual steps from the
neutral axis outward to the periphery of the colour space (Munsell Colour
Corporation, 1980). Hues are positioned on vertical axis showing values from
light to dark (Feishner, 2000).
The full Munsell notation for a chromatic colour is always given in the order of
H/V/C. The rotation for a neutral (achromatic) colour is written as NV/
(Munsell Colour Corporation, 1980). For instance, 2.5 Y 6/8 indicates that the
hue is half way between 10YR and 5Y (slightly orange to yellow) that the
lightness is slightly lighter than a medium grey and it has a fairly strong chroma
(Hunt, 1987, p. 80).
The physical samples were arranged to form an atlas and Munsell Books of
Colour were established by visual means. It displays approximately 150 colour
standards arranged in slots on charts for forty different hues. Each constant
49
hue chart is printed with a V/C grid and labelled with its hue notation (Munsell
Colour Corporation, 1980, p. 5; see Figure 3.3). The Munsell hue chips are
arranged on a square grid to show variations of Munsell value vertically and
Munsell chroma horizontally (Agoston, 1987; Hunt, 1987).
Figure 3.3. A view showing a sample page from Munsell Book of Colour.
(http://dba.med.sc.edu/price/irf/Adobe_tg/models/munsell.html)
The Munsell Colour System has various applications because of its simple use.
It enables artist to determine the components of a colour without
experimentation and provides pigment specifications that are precise
(Hunt, 1987). It is used by colour-scientists in government and industry
throughout the world. In development and specification of colour designs and
the communication of colour information between sales, engineering and
production departments, the Munsell notations and colour standards are used
(Munsell Colour Corporation, 1980).
50
According to Hering colour system the hues red, yellow, green and blue are
unique hues because they cannot be described in terms of any combinations of
other colours (Hunt, 1987). For instance, yellowish red or a reddish yellow is
one way to describe orange. However, red cannot be described as such. These
four unique hues include two pairs as red and green, and yellow and blue where
the colours in each of these pairs are opponents. Having reddish green or
a yellowish blue is impossible although yellowish reds as orange or reddish
blues as purple are possible.
Together with white and black, these four unique hues make six basic colours
that constitute one additional colour pair; white and black (Hunt, 1987). This
new colour pair is different from the unique-hue pairs in its opponency, in that
blackish white or whitish blacks are possible, experienced as the colour gray
(p. 86).
These ideas of Hering were developed and revived by the Swedish physicist
Tryggue Johansson since 1964 and produced the Natural Colour System (NCS)
(Hunt, 1987; Agoston, 1987). NCS allows everyone with normal colour vision to
make colour evaluations without the use of colour-measuring instruments or of
51
colour samples for comparison (Agoston, 1987, p. 133). It describes the formal
basic elements of the colour language and it provides the ability to identify
characteristic similarities and relations between colours (Hard and Sivik, 2001).
It is used directly to determine the perceived colour of surface. A colour is
determined in this way as an absolute measure based on colour perception.
NCS is the recognition of six psychological primaries that are six basic colours of
Hering colour system: Yellow, red, blue, green, white and black (Wyszecki,
1975; Agoston, 1987; Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). The first step in judging
a colour by the NCS is the determination of its hue. The binary compositions of
hues Y, R, B, and G, are presented systematically in NCS colour circle (see Figure
3.4). The NCS hue circle is divided into four quadrants (Y/R, R/B, B/G, and G/Y)
by unitary hues that are Y, R, B, G. The scale that is read clockwise shows
standard NCS hue designations (Agoston, 1987, p. 134). The dashed lines
separate hue ranges such as; the hues between G50Y and Y50R are the yellows;
the hues between Y50R and R50B are the reds, continuing in the same manner
around the circle. In this terminology with yellowish reds and bluish reds,
common hue terms such as orange and purple are excluded (Agoston, 1987).
52
In the NCS, colours are defined by the relative amounts of the basic colours that
are perceived presented by percentages (Hunt, 1987). For instance, a medium
grey which includes equal amounts of whiteness and blackness is defined as
having a whiteness of 50% and a blackness of 50%. A pure red colour with no
mark of yellowness or blueness or whiteness or blackness is defined as having
a redness of 100%. Therefore, the notation of Y50R in the colour circle
represents a 50/50 mixture of unitary yellow and unitary red. Similarly R50B,
B50G, and G50Y represent 50/50 mixtures (Agoston, 1987).
To judge hue, two unitary hues and the quadrant of the circle in which the hue is
located needs to be identified (Hunt, 1987). For instance, for the mixture of
yellow and red, the location is Y/R quadrant. If the hue has 10% unitary yellow
and 90% unitary red, then the NCS notation for this hue is Y90R. This notation
53
54
The Swedish Standard Institution (SIS) has adapted the NCS as a Swedish
Standard for colour notation and colour atlas. The NCS colour atlas includes 42
pages and 1750 colour samples (Swedish Standards Institution, 1996). The first
page shows the NCS colour circle with 40 colour samples of high chromaticness
(saturation), showing the hues selected for the atlas (see Figure 3.4). The
second page of the atlas includes colour samples for non-chromatic (purely
gray) and slightly-chromatic (near-gray) colours. The other 40 pages include
NCS triangles that show different hues and relationships to white and black of
a specific hue in each page (see Figure 3.6). In the NCS colour atlas the
chromatic hues are arranged in a circle with nine intermediate steps between
each, totalling to forty hues. Then, for each hue, a triangular chart is developed
showing the pure hue and its relationship to white and black
(Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000, p. 205).
3.3.3. CIELAB
In 1931, the Commission Internationale de IEclairage, (the International
Lighting Commission; referred as the CIE) an international standardizing body,
recommended a system of colourimetric (Padgham & Saunders, 1975). The
system is based on spectrophometric measurements of colour samples
illuminated by specific types of lighting and related to the visual response of a
standard observer (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000, p. 209). As an example; it is
probable for a sample to reflect green light and to look green under a northern
skylight, but it may appear slightly more yellow under artificial light.
The dimensions of colour which are hue, saturation and brightness are the CIE
tristimulus values X, Y, Z (red, green, blue) (Ylmaz, 2002) or three independent
quantities reproduced from them like x, y and Y (Agoston, 1987). CIE has a
chromaticity diagram that companion with x and y. As recognizant of the
wavelength and degree of saturation of a given colour, this diagram is used.
The spectral pure colours and non-spectral colours that are physically possible
to realize are situated inside the diagram (Grandis, 1986). In the centre, there is
white light supplied by the source (see Figure 3.7). The chromaticity diagram
allows locating the position of every colour arising from the mixture of two or
more colours within the given space. Therefore, the chromaticity, purity, and
saturation of a colour can be specific in the CIE triangle (Grandis, 1986).
56
57
58
There is a vertical metric lightness L axis in the basic structures of the CIELAB
that passing centrally through evenly spaced horizontal planes that are
subdivided into square grids containing coordinates a and b which are the
tristimulus values(Agoston, 1987, p. 107; see Figure 3.9). The mathematical
calculations that convert tristimulus values to CIELAB colour space values allow
colours to be considered as existing in an approximately uniform threedimensional space (Weatherall & Coombs, 1992). In that space, each specific
colour has a unique location defined in terms of its cartesian coordinates with
respect to the axes L, a, b (see Figure 3.10). The metric-lightness axis L shows
hueless colours ranging upwards from black, through the neutral gray to white.
Points in colour space apart from those on the metric-lightness axis shows
chromatic colours (Agoston, 1987). L represents the lightness coordinate,
a represents the red/green coordinate and b represents the yellow-blue
coordinate.
59
CIELAB was developed to accommodate a standard, uniform colour scale for the
measurement of colour differences. As it was mentioned, CIELAB was
recommended especially for matching the standards in the production of
material and objects and it is mostly used in industrial colour difference
evaluations (Grandis, 1986; Agoston, 1987; Melgosa, 2000; Feishner, 2000).
With an objective standard, it eliminates differences in human interpretation,
as well as problems caused by the fading of painted or coloured products.
Additionally, it has been used by image processing researchers (Connolly &
Fliess, 1997) and by researchers who conduct scientific experiments by using
a specified instrument for the measurement (Grandis, 1986).
The NCS and the Munsell system collections present various samplings of the
same colour (Agoston, 1987). Because of that, a simple relationship exists
between them. NCS judgments of surface colours viewed in daylight could be
translated into Munsell notation and then into CIE (x, y, Y) notation without the
use of colour measuring instruments (p. 137). The samples are measured
60
instrumentally and the CIE data can be converted into Munsell notation or
Munsell notations can be converted into CIE by conversion programs (Munsell
Colour Corporation, 1980).
Mixing red, green and blue light not only creates a white or colourless light but
also, by varying the intensities of colours almost any other colour can be
obtained (Helen, 1983) (see Figure 3.11). In Figure 3.11, red and blue are mixed
to generate magenta; red and green are mixed to generate yellow, while cyan
results from the mixture of green and blue. The lights may be mixed by placing
coloured filters in front of a projected light ray (Feisner, 2006)
(see Figure 3.12).
61
With the projection of two primary coloured light beams onto a white surface,
they will partially overlap (Pile, 1997) (see Figures 3.11 and 3.12). This
overlapping area will be brighter than the primaries and if the intensities of the
beams are equally balanced, a clear secondary colour will result. For instance,
62
when projecting red and green coloured light beams, they will overlap and this
area will be brighter than either the red or the green area. A clear yellow will
result if the intensities of these beams are equally balanced. However, if the red
beam is stronger than the green or if the green is dimmed an orange will result.
If the green is stronger than the red, than it will produce a yellow-green mix
(Pile, 1997). The white that is produced by three additive primaries can be
tinted by increasing or reducing one of the three colours.
In RGB colour model, the primary colours are red, green and blue. In between
each primary is a secondary colour that is the mixture of two primaries and two
tertiary colours that are the mixtures of a primary and a secondary colour
(http://blulob.com) (see Figure 3.14). Thus, yellow, cyan and magenta are the
secondary colours while orange, yellow-green1, cyan-green2, cyan-blue3,
(http://eportfolio.pdht.ca/w11_eportfolio/zeliger_isabella/portfolio/print/rgbcolourwheel.php).
3
63
blue-magenta4 and red magenta5 are the tertiary colours in RBG colour mode.
These hues are arranged in RGB colour circle with 12 steps as in Figure 3.13.
There are degrees around the RGB colour wheel, beginning from the top being
0 (red) and with a full rotation clockwise being 360 and each degree
represents a specific colour. Additionally, there are three values that indicate
the RGB values and can be anywhere from zero to 255 (http://blulob.com). Zero
value to red colour means that there is no red in the selected colour. On the
other side, 255 values to red colour means that red is as strong as it can possibly
be in the selected colour. This formula is the same for green and blue. Therefore,
a 255-0-0 value represents the pure red there are no other hues in the selected
colour.
64
Red Magenta
Orange
Yellow-
Blue -
Green
Magenta
Cyan -
Cyan -
Blue
Green
65
There is an ancient and widespread faith in the healing power of colour (Helen,
1983). Therefore, colour is suggested to be used as a treatment tool as regard to
66
colour healing. Colour healers believe in chakras that are evaluated as being
energy centres within the human body (OConnor, 2011). These energy centres
are formed by seven chakras. If there is too much or too little energy in each
chakra in other words no balance in the energy of chakras then emotional wellbeing and personality are influenced (Mahnke, 1996). Each chakra gets along
with one of the spectral colours that is illustrated below and proper balance in
the energy of chakras is restored through colour therapy.
1. Vertex chakra (violet) stands for wisdom, and spiritual energy.
It influences the pituitary gland.
2. Forehead chakra (indigo) stands for intuition (third eye) and influences
the pineal gland.
3. Larynx chakra (blue) stands for religious inspiration, creativity, language
and communication. It influences the thyroid gland.
4. Heart chakra (green, pink) stands for love, sympathy and harmony.
It influences the heart and the thymus gland.
5. Solar plexus chakra (yellow) stands for knowledge, intellect and is also
the seat of tension. It influences the solar plexus and the adrenal body.
6. Spleen chakra (orange) stands for energy, and it influences the spleen
and pancreas.
7. Basis chakra (red) stands for life and reproduction. It influences the sex
glands and sexual organs. (Mahnke, 1996, p. 35-36)
Kopacz (as cited in OConnor, 2011) associated colours with body functions and
dysfunctions within each chakra area, such as:
Red: Activates the circulation system and benefits the five senses; used to
treat colds, paralysis, anaemia, ailments of the bloodstream and ailments
of the lung;
Blue: Raises metabolism; is used to stabilize the heart, muscles and
bloodstream; used to treat burns, skin diseases, glaucoma, measles and
chicken pox, and throat problems;
Green: Strengthens bones and muscles, disinfects bacteria and virus, and
relieves tension; used to treat malaria, back problems, cancer, nervous
disorders, and ulcers, and to manage heart problems and blood pressure.
(OConnor, 2011, p. 231)
67
Many psychiatrists and psychologists have noted that reactions to colour are
more impulsive and emotional (Birren, 1988). Therefore, colour can have strong
influences on peoples moods and emotions. Helen (1983) explained emotional
reactions to colour as:
Light of different colours entering the eye can indirectly affect the centre of
the emotions in the hypothalamus, which in turn affects the pituitary
gland. This master gland controls the entire endocrine system, including
the thyroid and sex glands, and so controls the hormone levels of this
system and the moods consequents upon them. (p. 44)
In light of the given reasoned chain, medical circles are using colour as an
effective method of treatment by stimulating psychological emotion with colour
(Jin et al., 2009). The psychological responses to colour are generally studied
with their qualitative descriptions in the literature such as anxiety, aggression,
and happiness.
68
Every colour has a particular set of meanings. Not only physical but also
emotional characteristics can be related to any given colour (Ladau, Smith, &
Place, 1988). However, there are universally agreed meanings for red, orange,
and yellow which are associated with fire, also for blue, green, and violet which
are associated with cool oceans, deep forests, and shadows (Fehrman &
Fehrman, 2000) (to see the range of colour samples of red, green, blue, yellow,
purple, violet and orange from Munsell Book of Colour for Turkish culture,
see ahin Ekici, Yener, & Camgzs (2006) study).
Colour Analysis
RED
Red is one of the oldest colour names. It is the first colour with a defined
wavelength (Petru, 2006). It is assumed to be the colour of blood and fire. These
early associations charged red with passionate emotions. It has a primary
implication of excitement, heat, intensity, and force (Pile, 1997).
Reds positive connotation involves love (red roses, red heart), luck, passion
(red-blood), sexiness (red lipstick), importance (red carpet), dynamism,
excitement, richness, royalty and courage (Ladau, Smith, & Place, 1988; Feisner,
2006). Reds negative connotations involve war (red uniforms to disguise
blood), blood, fire, the devil, revolution and anarchy (red flag), revolution,
danger, fire and bureaucracy.
The term red contains many hues in languages that have only a few terms for
colour. Red may include hues of all reds, and hues from the range of oranges,
70
most yellows, browns, pinks, and even purples (Petru, 2006). Red becomes pink
when it is reduced to a tint and loses some of its saturation and psychological
intensity (Pile, 1997). It becomes gentle and feminine with milder warmth, with
charm and delicacy. Pink is associated generally with positive emotions: Being
in the pink means everything is fine, as when everything is rosy (Fehrman &
Fehrman, 2000). Its positive connotations contain health, sweetness, prettiness
and femininity (Feisner, 2006).
GREEN
Green has ambivalent connotations, because it is the largest colour family
distinguishable to the human eye (Feisner, 2006). It has its own meanings,
although it is made from blue and yellow (Ladau, Smith, & Place, 1988). Yellow
supplies it some pleasant characteristics while blue makes it seem more calm
(Pile, 1997). It is associated with grass, trees and other vegetations and all these
make it to be calming and restful to the eyes. This is also commonly related with
health and well-being and makes green the most restful colour.
71
BLUE
It is the coolest of the cool colours and has no inclusion of warmer tones (Pile,
1997). It symbolizes infinity and serenity in relation to the sky and the sea that
surrounds human beings (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). On the other hand, it
also symbolizes depression, sadness, and isolation. Additionally, bright blue can
be a lively colour but it can also lower the body temperature, pulse rate, and
blood pressure which stands in opposition to red in its physical effects
(Pile, 1997).
YELLOW
It has the highest luminosity after white and this makes it the most easily
perceived hue (Feisner, 2006). It can be easily seen before other colours
especially when placed with black. This combination yellow placing against
black- is used as a warning sign (to signal hazardous situations in industry).
72
tans that are some of the most popular background colours all constitute of
yellow tints.
73
ORANGE
Orange is a mixture of red and yellow and is a warm colour (Fehrman &
Fehrman, 2000). It shares some of the qualities of its neighbours in the colour
circle such as the intensity of excitement attended by red and the sense of cheer
association with yellow (Pile, 1997). The brown tones of orange are associated
with comfort and security (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000), and signifies earth,
wood, and chocolate (Feisner, 2006). Oranges positive connotations are
warmth, fruitfulness, brightness, autumn, energy, activity, excitement; while
negative emotions contain danger (fire) and brashness (Morton, 1997; Feisner,
2006).
THE NEUTRALS
Black, white and gray are the achromatics or neutral colours, constituting only
of brightness and saturation, without having the hue dimension.
BLACK
Black is a total absence of any chromatic colour (Feisner, 2006) as it absorbs
almost all the light of any colour (Ladau, Smith, & Place, 1988). By the addition
of some chromatic colours, black can be perceived as warm or cool, because it
becomes no longer pure black (Pile, 1997). Dark tones such as dark gray or very
dark blues like midnight blue can have the expressive qualities of black.
74
Most of the associations with black are negative such as death, emptiness, fear,
depression, disapproval, mystery and bad luck.
WHITE
White is the opposite of black. A white surface reflects all light in theory, thus it
represents maximum lightness (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). Although it is
defined as a non colour, it results as a combination of all chromatic colours
(Pile, 1997). In the modern movement, the colour white has been used
extensively. This has made white one of the symbolic attributes of modernism.
In addition, manufacturers use a various range of whites with blue, red, yellow
and green undertones because it is the best-selling paint (Fehrman & Fehrman,
2000). Together with black, they create a very powerful and sharp contrast of
non chromatic colours (Pile, 1997).
GRAY
Gray is the mixture of black and white. It is a neutral that can range not only
from light to dark but also from totally neutral (non chromatic) tones to warmer
tones that are the mixture of chromatic tones with white and black (Pile, 1997).
75
Light gray does not have strong associative implications as dark gray. Gray in
darker tones shares the positive and negative characteristics of black.
Gray is associated with technology, machines, aircraft, concrete, cement and the
urban environment (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). It implies confusion, loss of
distinction (gray area), intelligence, shadow and work (people in gray suits)
(Feisner, 2006). It also represents the wisdom of age (gray hair). Light gray
especially in warm toned versions are useful as background tones (Pile, 1997).
76
number of emotional responses where pink the lowest. Colour not only effects
human psychological feelings but also effects human physiological responses.
Terwogt and Hoeksma (1995) and Zentner (2001) studied colour and emotion
combinations by using matching measures with children and adults. In Terwogt
and Hoeksma (1995) study used colours were the primary colours red, blue,
yellow, and green supplemented with black and white. The emotions involved
were six basic emotions anger, happiness, sadness, fear, surprise, and disgust.
For each emotion, participants were asked to point at the colour that they
thought best fitted to the feeling given. Children aged 7-years-old combined the
colour blue with the emotion happiness, red with surprise and happiness, white
77
with surprise, fear, and sadness, black with anger; aged 11-year-old combined
the colour yellow with the emotion anger, red with happiness and surprise,
white with fear and surprise, green with aversion, and black with anger and
sadness; adults combined the colour blue with the emotion surprise, yellow
with happiness and anger, red with sadness, white with surprise, green with
happiness, and black with fear, anger, sadness, and aversion.
The effect of colour hue on emotion is studied by Odom and Sholtz (2000) by
using rating scales. The colours used were red, yellow, blue, and a lighter shade
of each with the words exciting, calm, and cheerful. Each word had a continuum
from one to ten scales. The participants were asked to circle a number on the
continuum next to each adjective representing an emotion. Primary colours
(red, yellow, blue) were seen as more cheerful and exciting than non primary
colours, while non primary colours were seen as more calming than primary
colours. In addition, yellow was found both cheerful and exciting while blue was
associated with being calm.
78
In Manavs study (2007) participants were asked to match the given adjective
list with the colour samples from the catalogue according to the emotional
response they associate. The responses indicated that the feelings of enjoyment,
79
cheerfulness, and warmness were attained to the colour samples of pink and
yellow; both green and blue samples were favoured for calmness, peacefulness,
relaxation, and modernism; green samples on the other hand were associated
with boredom, fearfulness, mystery, anxiety, annoyance; blue samples were
considered as calming but also were expressed by being cold and dull; yellow
samples were indicated as dynamic, warm and cheerful.
Kaya and Epps (2004) investigated the reason for colour emotion associations
with open-ended self-report measure. They reported that how individuals
associate colours with specific emotions are highly related with the way they
associate emotions with things, objects or the physical space. Thus, colour
related emotion is dependent on personal preference and ones past experience
with that particular colour. For instance, in their study, the colour green
attained the highest number of positive emotions, including the feelings of
relaxation, followed by happiness, comfort, peace, and hope; and it was
associated with nature and trees that create a feeling of comfort and
accompanying soothing emotions. As another example, yellow was seen to be
energetic and elicited positive emotions as it was associated with the sun and
the summer time.
Individuals colour associations with different building types were studied with
college students between ages 18 to 25 years old (Kaya & Crosby, 2006). It was
indicated that, colour associations were based on individual and emotional
aspects. It was reported that red represented energy, vitality, power, happiness
and joy, purple represented fun and creativity, and blue represented truth,
80
Gao and Xin (2006) investigated human emotional responses on colours with
a psychophysical experiment. Totally 218 colour samples that are a textile
version of the Munsell Colour Order System, covering a wide range of hue,
lightness, and chroma were used in the study. Subjects were asked to match a
more suitable word from 12 word pairs to describe the colour, after viewing a
colour sample. Contrary to previously stated studies, it was reported that hue
had less influence on basic emotion variables while people were more sensitive
to colours with low chroma and high lightness.
A new method for emotional connotations of colour was extended by Clarke and
Costall (2008). Unlike the conducted studies in the same field, the participants
were not given actual colour samples, thus they were free to imagine their own
examples. A semi-structured interview covering red, orange, yellow, green,
blue, purple, black, white, pink, brown and grey- supplied participant freedom
to respond in their own terms of descriptions. Red, orange and yellow did evoke
the more active emotions and were related to love, anger, passion (red),
warmth, sunshine, and sadness (yellow); green and blue to peacefulness,
calmness, relaxation, neutral, and sadness; purple to calming and passive
nature; pink to femininity; black to power, badness, strength, and death. Grey
was mostly regarded as lacking emotion. Most of the participant did not make
any comment about emotive qualities, but referred to un-emotive qualities.
81
Qu, Luo, Sun, Hu, and Chen (2011) examined the impact of age on colour
emotion responses. 30 single colour stimuli covered a wide range of hue,
lightness and chroma; and 190 colour pairs were used. For each colour stimuli,
participants were asked to select words that best describe the shown stimuli.
For single colours; older participants had ratings as less active, less liked, and
cooler than for younger participants. For colour pairs; light colour pairs were
rated as less active and cooler while achromatic colour pairs were rated as
cooler, less liked and less harmonious.
82
Table.4.1.
COLOUR
EMOTION
Negative emotions
Positive emotions
CHROMATIC
COLOURS
RED
happiness, surprise, energetic,
powerfulness, enjoyment,
passionate
PINK
enjoyment, cheerfulness,
warmness,
feminineness
GREEN
BLUE
YELLOW
PURPLE
happiness, cheerfulness,
excitement,
surprise, enjoyment, warmness,
dynamism, energetic
anger, sadness
NEUTRALS
BLACK
powerfulness
WHITE
surprise
GRAY
(Based on the studies by Boyatzis & Varghese (1994); Terwogt and Hoeksma
(1995); Odom and Sholtz (2000); Zentner (2001); Kaya and Epps (2004); Kaya
and Crosby (2006); Pos and Green-Armytage (2007); Manav (2007); Clarke and
Costall (2008)).
83
Colour and emotion studies investigate the affinity of these two phenomena
mostly by the use of two dimensional stimuli to draw a frame on colour emotion
associations. However, colour may also be an efficient tool in interior spaces to
cause impressions for various emotions. It is intended for any living
environment to be pleasing regarding to its colour, both psychologically and
emotionally. To analyze colour emotion associations in interior spaces, it is
necessary to identify what an interior space is and the impact of colour on its
limits.
between the space and the elements as well as among the elements themselves.
Thus, we sense shelter and enclosure upon entering a building due to the
surrounding columns, walls, floors and ceilings of an interior space that are the
major elements of interior spaces (Ching, 1987; Krier, 1992). These
architectural elements draw the boundaries of spaces. Windows and doors are
used to provide connection with the exterior. Besides the major architectural
elements, furniture is another important category of design that acts as an
intermediary between architecture and people (Ching, 1987). The selection and
arrangement of furniture within the spaces are major tasks of interior design,
while walls, floors, ceilings, windows and doors are established in the
architectural design of a building.
not override the overall atmosphere. Careful settings of colour can make warm
environments than they actually are or an appropriate usage can make cool
environments to feel warmer (Danger, 1987). Moreover, colour provides legible
spaces in terms of spatial perception (Helvacolu & Olguntrk, 2011).
Therefore, colours should be used to give the right message to people through
the built environment (Kaya & Crosby, 2006).
Even though it has an active role in the relationship between people and their
surrounded environment, colour emotion studies fail to concentrate on colour
emotion associations in indoor spaces. Studies related with spaces in the
framework of colour emotion mainly focus on influences of colour on user
performances and also preferences of colour on in environment.
In Manavs study (2007) individuals were asked to match a colour sample from
the catalogue to different areas in residences by stating the reason of
preference. They were asked to offer a colour sample from the catalogue as the
finishing coating of various areas in residences. Pink sample was preferred for
the sleeping room and childrens room as it was associated with warmth,
romance and enjoyment; blue sample was selected for the living room as it was
associated with calmness, peacefulness, being modern and relaxation; yellow
sample was selected for the dining area as it was associated with simplicity,
being classic and plainness; samples that were close to white were preferred for
kitchen, bathroom, entrance, and stair hall as they were associated with purity
and hygiene.
87
6. THE EXPERIMENT
The main objective of this study is to examine the relationship between two
crucial phenomenon of design colour and emotion in interior spaces. While
analyzing this issue, the main focuses are on:
How individual colours affect human emotional reactions in interior
spaces,
Whether human emotional reactions to individual colours differ in a
distinctive way in the same interior space,
In what way colours emotional influences are distinguished from each
other,
88
In addition to the stated focuses, the other objective of the study is to develop a
matrix between colours and emotions associations in interior spaces that would
visualize the colours and their associations with different emotions. Thus, it is
expected to find out the influence of the use of different colours in interior
spaces on human emotions.
6.1.2. Hypotheses
1. Emotional reactions shown to different colours in an interior space differ.
2. Gender effects emotional reactions to a colour in interior space.
89
90
Valid
Frequency Percent
47
26,1
Valid
Cumulative
Percent
Percent
26,1
26,1
47
26,1
26,1
52,2
11
6,1
6,1
58,3
5,0
5,0
63,3
2,2
2,2
65,6
14
4
7,8
2,2
7,8
2,2
73,3
75,6
1,7
1,7
77,2
2
2
1,1
1,1
1,1
1,1
78,3
79,4
5,0
5,0
84,4
1,7
1,7
86,1
2,2
2,2
88,3
1,7
1,7
90,0
2,8
2,8
92,8
2,2
2,2
95,0
1,1
1,1
96,1
2,8
2,8
98,9
1,1
1,1
100,0
180
100,0
100,0
91
The group needed to be balanced as to sex (ninety females and ninety males) to
explore gender differences in colour emotion association in interior spaces. The
age of the sample group varies in between 17 and 26, and the majority of the
subjects were between 19-22 in age (75,63%). The mean age of the sample
group is 21,08 (see Appendix B for the detailed information on age distribution
of the sample group).
All of the participants were familiar with the computer. 47,2% of them have
been using computer for five to ten years, while 52,8% have been using for
more than ten years. 67,2% of the participants stated that they use computer
a few times a day, 30% of them everyday and a minority 2,8% of them use
computer for every other day (see Appendix B, Table B.3 and B.4).
92
point for print, audio, and visual materials. For this reason, library is frequently
used by students who are studying in different departments. Thus, it has
a power of gathering diverse people. The sample group for this study includes
students who have not got a design past. As the library provides this
opportunity and the Multimedia Room supplies a special space for conducting
the experiment, it was decided to be used as the setting.
The permission for conducting the experiment was taken from the Directory
Department of the Library. The Multimedia Room was open between 08:3023:00 during the week and 09:00-17:00 at weekends. Thus, the study was
conducted during these hours.
The booths had both artificial and daylight illumination. To fix the variables in
the experiment setting, one booth without any daylight illumination was
selected. However, one side of the selected booth had a window pane.
Therefore, during the experiment participants might be affected or disturbed by
the outside view. In order to control the potential problem, the window pane
was made passive by covering it with a black curtain (see Figure 6.1).
93
The illuminance level was measured with Minolta Illuminance Meter T-10 to
understand the lighting situation in the booth after the black curtains were
mounted and it was kept the same during the experiment. The illuminance level
was fixed to 219 lux. A Philips Master TLD-18W/840 fluorescent lamp was used
in the coves for lighting the experiment booth.
The sitting arrangement in the booth was organized to supply the best control
of possible veiling reflections6 on the computer screen (see Figures 6.2 and 6.3).
In addition, participants were faced to an unobstructed surface by this interior
arrangement.
Veiling reflections occur when the incident light angle on the horizontal work surface is within
the observers viewing zone (Egan, 2002).
94
Figure 6.2. A view showing the interior organization of the experiment setting.
6.2.3. Procedures
6.2.3.1. Selecting the Function
In this study the main goal is to discover human emotional reactions to
individual colours in an interior space. In order to specify the kind of space, the
user requirements and human needs are analyzed carefully and translated into
spatial context.
In this research, a living space was studied. A living space satisfies belonging
needs as one has the opportunity to socialize with ones family and friends,
refers to large group of user identification, and provides a public space where
interaction is possible.
The layout of spaces as physical settings should also satisfy the stated needs in
relation with their function. All functions that are taking place in living rooms
such as conversing and watching television are analyzed and the required
furniture a bookcase, a TV unit, couches, and a coffee table are selected and
arranged (see Figures 6.4). The main concern in configuration of the living room
96
97
In the experiment the colours chosen are primary additive colours (Red, Green,
Blue) and an achromatic colour as a control tool for being lack of hue. In
experiments concerning colour, it is important to use the colours with the same
brightness (value, lightness) and saturation (chroma, chromaticness) for
controlling the variables. The aim is to change only the hues, while keeping the
perceived lightnesses and saturations of colours the same, in order to
understand the effect of hue. Therefore, the lightness (B value) and saturation
98
(S value) rates were kept the same for each four colours (see Table 6.2).
According to Illuminating Engineering Society of North America (IESNA, 2000)
guideline, 50 % is the recommended reflectance value7 for wall surfaces
(Egan, 2002). In the light of this information, lightness of colours was adjusted
to 50 as they were used for wall surfaces.
To eliminate possible effects of furniture, ceiling and floor colours within the
same interior space, these elements were also coloured in grey as being lack of
hue. To discriminate furniture, floor and ceiling from each other, they needed to
be different in their colour; therefore three different greys were used in the
space for those elements. They were chosen according to IESNA
recommendations. IESNA recommends 70% reflectance value for ceiling and
20% reflectance value for floor in indoor spaces. Thus, the brightness level for
the ceiling was adjusted to 70 and for the floor to 20. For the furniture it was
adjusted to 60 (see Table 6.2).
Reflectance is the percentage of light which is reradiated from a surface (Egan, 2002).
99
100
Second Life (SL) is a 3D virtual world imitating the real world. It provides
a space that people have an opportunity to experience real-world or SL games
that are simulations of the world, including a whole simulated ecosystem
(Helmer, 2007). The SL is populated with avatars which refer to virtual
representations of SL members. Almost all of the objects and places in SL have
been created by SL citizens (Rymaszewoski, Wallace, Winters, Ondrejka &
Batstore-Cunningham, 2007). It supplies a user-friendly interface (Hendaoui,
Limayem, & Thompson, 2008). Users are able to navigate by walking, flying and
teleporting between spaces.
After creating the four different wall coloured interior spaces in SL, videos for
all those spaces were generated by CamStudio 2.6 Beta program. Each video
lasts for 35 seconds starting from the entrance (door) of the space and ending
again at the same point.
The subjects were asked if they had any eye or vision deficiencies. Subjects who
have any vision deficiencies were asked to take the experiment with their
correction equipments such as contact lenses or eyeglasses, which they wore
101
regularly. There were no subjects with severe eye or vision damage who needed
to be excluded from the experiment.
Subjects were also given Ishiharas Tests for Colour-Blindness (Ishihara, 1975) in
the very same room with the experimental setup, under the same lighting
conditions. It is a series of plates that are designed to provide a test giving a
quick assessment of colour vision deficiency (Camgz, 2000). It is used to
designate the commonest form of colour vision disturbances among them being
red-green deficiency the most common. The test controls whether the subject
has a red-green deficiency, which may be of two types: A protan (that may be
complete (protanopia) or partial (protanomalia)) and a deutan (which may be
complete (deuteranopia) or partial (deuteranomalia)) (Camgz, 2000, p. 77).
After this test, participants were informed about the main objective and
procedure of the experiment both written and oral (see Appendix F).
In the second phase, there was a 3D living space with four versions. For each
space, all walls as one of the major element of an interior space were coloured
with four selected colours (red, green, blue, and gray) separately. Thus, there
were same 3D interior spaces with four different wall colours.
Each participant was shown the same space with two different colours. It is
critical to eliminate the possible effect of a definite sequence of seeing colours.
To control that effect, the sequences of showing the colours were changed
systematically (see Table 6.3). According to this strategy, there were six
different experiment sets and in each the sequences were different. For each set
102
there were thirty subjects (15 female, 15 male). Thus hundred and eighty
subjects were used in the study in respect to gender and different experiment
sets.
Table 6.3. Experiment sets showing the number of participants with the
sequence of colours.
Sequence of
Colour
Participants
SET 1
Gray, Red
SET 2
Red, Gray
EXPERIMENT
SET 3
Gray, Green
SETS
SET 4
Green, Gray
SET 5
Gray, Blue
SET 6
Blue, Gray
TOTAL
60
(30 female,
30 (15 female, 15 male) 30 male)
30 (15 female, 15 male)
60
(30 female,
30 (15 female, 15 male) 30 male)
30 (15 female, 15 male)
60
(30 female,
30 (15 female, 15 male) 30 male)
180 (90 female, 90
male)
Self report measure of emotion was used in the experiment to give the
participant the opportunity to express information that only s/he has access.
A matching measure in between coloured interior spaces and expressive
emotional faces was used in addition to an open-ended measure.
Coloured interior space and emotion matching task constituted of four coloured
stimuli and seven faces expressive of six basic emotions and one neutral that
include anger, disgust, surprise, happiness, fear, sadness, and representative of
103
the neutral face expression (see Appendix G, Figure G.1). Firstly, the participant
was seated in front of the computer individually and was shown one 3D interior
space video with one specific wall colour. Then, s/he was shown the seven faces
with no written name of the emotion. S/he was asked to choose a single face
representing a specific emotion that best fit the shown coloured interior space.
The age and sex of the stimulus face shown in the photographs were held
constant with the seven faces. To reduce the influence of coloured photographs
on emotion, photographs were used in black and white chosen from Ekmans
universal representativeness of basic emotions. Secondly, after choosing a face
s/he was encouraged to state the reason for choosing the specific face. No time
limit was stipulated. One participant was shown two spaces and did the
matching scale two times.
104
7. FINDINGS
To analyze the data, Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 13.00 was
used. Through the analyzing process, Independent Samples t-Test, Paired
Samples t-Test, Chi-Square Goodness-of-Fit Test and frequency tables were
used.
For each coloured room firstly, the effect of sequence of showing coloured
rooms (red, green and blue as chromatic, gray as achromatic) and secondly, the
gender influence on emotional associations to coloured room was analyzed.
Statistical analyses indicated that the showing order of the coloured rooms and
gender has no effect on emotional associations to red room and blue room.
Thus, final emotional associations to red room and blue room are given without
regarding the sequence of showing coloured rooms and gender differences. On
the other hand, contrary to red room and blue room, showing order of the green
room and gray room effects the emotional associations to green room. However,
there is no influence of gender on emotional associations to green room. Thus,
final emotional associations to green room are given without regarding gender
differences.
105
Within the scope of the stated aspects, findings from the statistical analyses are
given in respect of the experiment sets involving coloured rooms created by the
primary additive colours (red, green and blue) and gray colour sequentially. For
each coloured room, findings are beginning with the effect of sequence of
showing coloured rooms; continue with the effect of gender on emotional
associations to coloured rooms and ends with the frequencies that indicate the
distribution of emotions on coloured rooms (see Table 7.1 for the overview of
frequencies). Additionally, for each experiment set, the comparison of red, green
and blue wall coloured room with gray room is given separately in order to
understand if there is a difference in their emotional associations.
106
107
12
10
8
6
male
4
female
2
0
The findings for emotional associations to red room are given without regarding
the gender differences and sequence of showing the gray room and red room.
The findings involve 60 participants responds who experienced the red room.
more than two points on the scale (Argyrous, 2005). It is used to ascertain
whether the distribution of observed counts in the various categories of
a categorical variable matches the expected distribution of counts under
a hypothetical model for the data (Elliott & Woodward, 2007, p. 143). In this
study, the attention is on the frequency of distribution of seven emotions used
in the experiment. According to the analyses, the parameters are not equal
(=51.067, df=6, =.000) and this indicates that for the red room, the
proportion of participants with seven emotions are not the same (see Appendix
H, Table H.7). In other words, the distribution of emotions for 60 participants is
different. The figure 7.2 illustrates that the number of participants who
associate the emotions of disgust and happiness are significantly larger than the
other emotions. According to the figure, red room was associated with each six
basic emotion that is universally accepted and neutral emotion. The percentages
from the highest to lowest are as follows: Disgust (35%), happiness (33.3%),
neutral (15%), surprise (6.7%), anger (5%), fear (3.3%), and sadness (1.7%)
(see Appendix H, Table H.8).
109
110
14
12
10
8
6
male
female
2
0
111
Figure 7.4. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in the first
experiment set.
In the first experiment set emotional association to red room and gray room
was tested. Emotional associations to red room and gray room were compared
with Paired Samples t-Test. The analysis points out that there is a significant
difference between red room and gray room in terms of their associations with
emotional reactions (df=119, =.000) (see Appendix H, Tables H.17).
18
16
14
12
10
1st experience green
room
8
6
4
2
0
16
14
12
10
8
6
male
female
2
0
Figure 7.6. The distribution of emotions on the green room in respect to gender
group.
As the sequence of showing the rooms has an effect on associations, the findings
from 30 participants who first viewed green room are reported for emotion
associations. The equality of parameters of multinomial probability distribution
of seven emotions was tested. The test points out that the parameters are not
equal (=22.000, df=5, =.001) (see Appendix H, Table H.26). The number of
participants who matched the green room with the emotion of neutral,
happiness and disgust are larger than the other emotions (see Figure 7.7).
114
The percentages from the highest to lowest are as follows: Neutral (36.7%),
happiness (33.3%), disgust (20%), fear, anger and surprise (3.3% for each)
(see Appendix H, Table H.21).
12
10
8
6
4
2
0
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise happiness
fear
sadness
For establishing the effect of gender on emotional reactions to gray room in the
second experiment set, Independent Sample t-Test was conducted. The test
points out that there is not a significant difference between gender groups in
115
their emotional reactions to gray room (F=2.266, df=58, =.431) (see Appendix
H, Tables H.29 and H.30). Thus, gender has no influence on human emotional
reactions to gray coloured room in the second experiment set (see Figure 7.8,
Appendix H, Tables, H.31 and H.32).
14
12
10
8
6
male
female
2
0
Figure 7.8. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in respect to gender
group in the second experiment set.
The findings for emotional associations to gray room are given within the
responds of 60 participants involved in the second experiment set. The equality
of parameters of multinomial probability distribution of seven emotions was
tested. The test points out that the parameters are not equal (=16.400, df=3,
=.001) (see Appendix H, Table H.33). The number of participant who match the
gray room with the emotions of disgust, neutral and sadness are larger than the
other emotions (see Figure 7.9). The gray room was associated with basic
emotions that are universally accepted except anger, surprise and fear without
regarding the gender differences and sequence of showing the spaces.
116
The percentages from the highest to lowest are as follows: Disgust (38.3%),
neutral (30%), sadness (28.3%) and happiness (3.3%) (see Appendix H, Table
H.34).
Figure 7.9. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in the second
experiment set.
In the second experiment set emotional association to green room and gray
room was investigated. Emotional associations to green room and gray room
were compared with Paired Samples t-Test. The analysis points out that there is
a significant difference between green room and gray room in terms of their
associations with emotional reactions (df=119, =.000) (see Appendix H,
Table H.35).
117
118
16
14
12
10
8
6
male
female
2
0
Figure 7.10. The distribution of emotions on the blue room in respect to gender
group.
119
14
12
10
8
6
male
female
2
0
Figure 7.12. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in respect to gender
group.
121
Figure 7.13. The distribution of emotions on the gray room in the third
experiment set.
In the third experiment set emotional association to blue room and gray room
was investigated. Emotional associations to blue room and gray room were
compared with Paired Samples t-Test. The analysis points out that there is
a significant difference between blue room and gray room in terms of their
associations with emotional reactions (df=119, =.000) (see Appendix H,
Table H.52).
122
8. DISCUSSION
This study comprises three major phenomenons that are colour, emotion and
interior space. The main objective is to investigate the influence of different
colours in interior space on human emotional reactions. Emotional associations
with four different colours were tested in the study. It was hypothesized that
emotional reactions shown to different colours in an interior space differ. Thus,
an inequality on the number of people who associate a specific emotion with
a specific colour was expected. Additionally, this study investigated gender
differences on colour-emotion associations. The selected colours for the study
were red, green and blue. Gray was also tested as being an achromatic neutral
colour as a control tool. The colours and their associations with emotions were
analyzed individually.
Findings of the study were analyzed according to the experiment sets involving
coloured rooms of red, green and blue. The first experiment set embodied the
red room. The equality of parameters of multinomial distribution of seven
emotions that covered anger, disgust, neutral, surprise, happiness, fear and
sadness was tested. It was indicated that for the red room, the proportions of
participants with emotions are not the same (=.000; see Appendix H, Table
H.7). The frequency distributions pointed out that the participants who
123
associated the emotions of disgust and happiness are larger than the other
emotions.
Red is one of the powerful colours with its associations with passionate
emotions such as love, blood and fire (Pile, 1997). Furthermore, it is the colour
that creates the highest number of emotional responses (Boyatzis & Varghese,
1994). It is associated with surprise, happiness and sadness (Terwogt &
Hoeksma, 1995), energy, vitality and power (Kaya & Crosby, 2006), fear and
anger (Pos & Green-Armytage, 2007). Thus, red evokes emotions in a range
from the negatives (anger, fear and sadness) to positives (happiness, energy,
power).
In this study it was the same, as disgust and happiness were mostly associated
with red. That shows the ironic side of this colour. The number participants
who matched the red room with happiness were 20 and 14 of them found the
red interior space active and energetic. On the other hand, the ones who
associated the red room with disgust were 21 and 9 of them mentioned about
its discomforting side. Helen (1983) stated the same approach with the
explanation of red wall in a space can be reassuring and discomforting. Some
expressions recorded after the experiments regarding participants feelings for
the association of disgust with red room are as follows:
[...] I do not want to spent time in a space with red walls. It is a very
powerful colour, thus I think that after a while I may feel tired [...].
I hate the idea of a red room.
124
The second experiment set involved the green room. The equality of parameters
of multinomial probability distribution was tested and pointed out that the
parameters are not equal (=.000; see Appendix H, Table H.26). The numbers of
participant who match the green room with neutral and happiness are larger
than the other emotions. Disgust was also associated with green. The green
room was thought to be neutral when it is first experienced. It is interesting to
note that when participants experienced the green room after the gray room,
they thought by a large number that it was inducing happiness rather than
being neutral. Neutral is being allocated to the gray room. Most of the
participant who matched green room with neutral wanted to change their
responds with happiness after seeing gray room. They stated that after seeing
gray, they started to perceive the green room as being a happier environment.
Green is associated with nature and trees which creates a feeling of comfort
(Kaya & Epps, 2004). These connotations make it attain the highest number of
positive emotions such as feelings of relaxation, happiness, comfort, peace and
hope (Kaya & Epps, 2004). Similar to previously done studies, green attained
the positive emotion of happiness and neutral. 11 participants over 15 who
matched the neutral with green room mentioned that they chose it as it
represented calmness and relaxation more than the other facial expressions.
Thus neutral was thought to be an outcome of calmness and relaxation, being
125
a rather positive state. On the other hand, green room was associated with
disgust the participants who indicated that colour green was their least
favourite colour in their daily lives.
The third experiment set covered the blue room. According to the equality of
parameters of multinomial probability distribution of seven emotions, the
number of participants who matched the blue room with neutral was larger
than the other emotions. 9 participants from 25 who matched the blue room
with neutral mentioned that it represented calmness and 8 of them with
relaxation same as the green room, again neutral being a rather positive state of
emotion. This was expected from the connotations of blue with infinity and
serenity in relation to the sky and the sea that represents peacefulness and
relaxation (Fehrman & Fehrman, 2000). The previous studies that were done
without any reference to interior space reported that blue was associated with
happiness (Terwogt & Hoeksma, 1995), calmness, peacefulness, relaxation and
modernism (Manav, 2007). Thus, the blue colour is generally associated with
these emotions whether the fundamental is interior space or not. On the other
hand, in the previous studies blue was also associated with sadness (Zentner,
2001), with being cold and dull (Manav, 2007). In this study participants who
associated the room with negative emotions generally complained about the
tone of the blue. They found its brightness and saturation values high.
In each experiment set, in addition to chromatic wall coloured rooms, the same
interior space was also assessed with an achromatic wall colour. The concern
was to control the emotional reactions to chromatic wall coloured rooms. It was
126
assumed that if the approaches to chromatic room and gray room were the
same than hue (colour) of a room was not asserting any emotions. However,
emotional reactions for each chromatic room were differed from achromatic
spaces. Thus, hue (colour) was found to be significantly affecting emotions for
a space.
Emotional reactions to red room and gray room in the first experiment set were
compared and the analyses pointed out a significant difference between them
(=.000; see Appendix H, Table H.17). The number of participants who matched
gray room with disgust, neutral and sadness was larger than the other
emotions. As it is mentioned before, red room was mostly associated with
disgust and happiness. Although the number of participant who match the
interior spaces with disgust were almost the same, not any of them associated
gray room with happiness contrary to red room. In addition, sadness was
mostly associated with gray room while only one participant over sixty
associated this emotion with red room.
Emotional associations to green room and gray room were investigated and the
analyses showed a significant difference between these rooms (=.000;
see Appendix H, Table H.36). In the second experiment set, the number of
participants who matched gray room with disgust, neutral and sadness was
larger than the other emotions. Although green was mostly associated with
neutral and happiness, only two participants over sixty associated happiness
with gray room. Moreover, contrary to gray room sadness was an emotion
127
within the least associated with green room, while anger, surprise and fear were
not associated with gray room in contrast to green room.
Gray room and blue room was also compared in terms of their emotional
associations and a significant difference was found (=.000; see Appendix H,
Table H.53). According to multinomial probability distribution, the number of
participants who matched gray room with the emotions of neutral, sadness and
disgust were larger than the other emotions. Anger, surprise and fear were not
associated with gray room, while they were associated with blue room. In
addition, sadness was one of the most associated emotions with gray room,
while that was the least associated one with blue room. The association of
neutral with blue and gray room was almost the same. However, the
interpretation of neutral was different for gray and blue rooms. Participants
matched the blue room with neutral, because they felt that neutral had the most
representative facial expressions for relaxation, calmness and peacefulness. On
the other hand, for participants who matched gray room with neutral indicated
that they did not feel anything towards gray room.
An achromatic wall colour of gray, which did not have any hue in it, was
assumed to be a neutral interior with no strong emotional associations. This
study showed that in isolation the achromatic gray acted as a colour
(as in chromatic colours) on its own right. Gray was associated with neutral as
expected as having no feelings towards that colour, but it was also associated
with negative emotions that are disgust and sadness (see Table 4.5). Chromatic
colours were associated more with positive emotions as being more active
128
colours than gray (see Table 4.6). Some participants comments on gray room
were:
129
Pile (1997) claims that a living room should be in colour tones ranging from
mildly warm to neutral. Stronger tones should just be used in smaller areas or
as accents. According to Pile, although the idea of a red room, green room and
blue room may seem attractive, they should be used in a situation where there
are a number of living spaces available to give the occupants the opportunity of
choosing among the alternatives (1997). According to Helen (1983), in hospitals
blue walls have a calming effect. However, in a cafe, the same colour causes
employees to complain about a cold environment. Thus, it is fundamental to
become aware of the power of colour and design built environments a respect
for users emotional and physiological health.
130
9. CONCLUSION
Both emotion and colour have a central role in peoples lives. People are always
experiencing these phenomenons consciously or unconsciously in their daily
lives. These experiences affect people psychologically and physiologically, thus,
colour and emotion have an influence on the quality of our lives. Moreover,
colour as one of the major design tools supplies more legible interior spaces to
people. With an effective planning in the integration process of colour and
space, more sensitive and relevant designs may be achieved. These kinds of
spaces become more user friendly and they reduce the possible stress levels of
users. Therefore, in a manner, all these three important concepts colour,
interior space, emotion- are related to each other.
131
combining both the concepts of colour and emotion in the scope of interior
design.
Within the scope of the stated aspects, the main objective of this study was to
examine the association between colour and emotion in interior space.
Furthermore, it was aimed to develop a general discussion in respect to colour
and emotion associations within the framework of interior space to understand
the impacts of colours used in interior space on its inhabitants.
As in all experimental research this study also has its own limitations. These
limitations give space for future research to understand more about colour. The
limitations are due to the selection of colour, selection of function to interior
space and selection of the sample group. This study was limited with four
colours out of the available millions. Since the number of colours to be
132
investigated is increased, the required time for conducting the study would be
increased as well. However, the permission taken from the directory
department of the library for conducting the study was limited. Future studies
can continue on different colours. These approaches allow creating and
improving a guideline for designers and interior architects that may be used as
a dictionary to illustrate which colour in interior space is associated with which
specific emotion.
133
For future studies that will comprise the issues on colour and emotion in
interior spaces, it is critical to point out some suggestions for improving the
strength of the study. When studying with colour, in the selection process of
colours that are planning to be used in the experiment, at least one of the
dimensions of colour should be kept the same. Hue, brightness and saturation
generate the identity of a colour. Controlling these variables are important to
clarify and analyze whether any effect from a colour takes its source from the
hue, brightness or saturation.
Right along with the function, it is also important to decide on the layout of the
space. In order to specify the design of a layout, user requirements and human
needs for specific function should be analyzed and translated into spatial
contexts carefully. Furthermore, the spaces should be generic and conventional;
the decoration should be simple in terms of the line and style of furnitures and
textures to eliminate the effect of design in itself.
134
135
REFERENCES
Agoston, G. A. (1987). Colour theory and its application in art and design.
Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Argyrous, G. V. (2005). Statistics for research: With a guide to SPSS.
London: SAGE Publications.
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality: Neurological and physiological
aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.
Ben-Zeev, A. (2001). The subtlety of emotions. London: The MIT Press.
Birren, F. (1988). Light, colour and environment. Pennslvania: Schiffer
Publishing Ltd.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P.J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment
manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behaviour Therapy &
Experimental Psychiatry, 25 (1), 49-59.
Boyatzis, C. J., & Varghese, R. (1994). Childrens emotional associations with
colors. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155 (1), 77-85.
Camgz, N. (2001). Effects of hue, saturation, and brightness on attention and
preference. (Published doctoral dissertation). Ann Arbor: UMI Dissertation
Services, A Bell & Howell Company.
Cardinal, R. N. (2004). Emotion and motivation [PDF document]. Retrieved from
http://egret.psychol.cam.ac.uk/psychology/20034/PDF_HANDOUT_MRCPsych_2004_Emotion_Motivation.pdf
Ching, F. D. K. (1987). Interior design: Illustrated. New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold Company.
Clark, M. S., & Isen, A. M. (1982). Toward understanding the relationship
between feeling states and social behavior. In A. H. Hastorf & A. M. Isen
(Eds.), Cognitive social psychology (pp. 73-108). New York: Elseiver.
Clarke, T., & Costall, A. (2008). The emotional connotations of colour: A
qualitative investigation. Colour Research and Application, 33 (5), 406-410.
136
Connoly, C., & Fliess, T. (1997). A study of efficiency and accuracy in the
transformation from RGB to CIELAB colour space. IEEE Transactions on
Image Processing, 6 (7), 1046-1048.
Dalke, H., Little, J., Niemann, E., Camgz, N., Steadman, G., Hill, S., & Stott, L.
Colour and lighting in hospital design. Optics & Laser Technology, 38, 343365.
Danger, E. P. (1987). The colour handbook: How to use colour in commerce and
industry. England: Gower Technical Press.
DeCatanzaro, D. A. (1999). Motivation and emotion: Evolutionary, physiological,
developmental, and social perspectives. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Dennis, C. (1989). Introduction to psychology, exploration and application.
St.Paul: West Publishing Company.
Denton, H. G., McDonagh, D., Baker, S., & Wormald, P. (2004). In D. McDonagh, P.
Hekkert, Introducing the student designer to the role of emotion in design.
J. Van Erp & D. Gyi (Eds.), Design and emotion: The experience of everyday
things (pp. 415-420). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Desmet, P. M. A. (2003). Measuring of emotion: Development and application of
an instrument to measure emotional responses to products. In M. A. Blythe,
A.F. Monk, K. Overbeeke & P.C. Wright (Eds.), Funology: From usability to
enjoyment (pp. 111-123). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Egan, M.D. (2002). Architectural lighting. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ekici, . E., Yener, C., & Camgz, N. (2006). Colour naming. Optics & Laser
Technology, 38, 466-485.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and
emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17 (2), 124-129.
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial action coding system (FACS).
Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist Press.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. In K. R. Scherer &
P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to Emotion (pp. 319-343). London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99 (3),
550-553.
Ekman, P. (1994). Moods, emotions, and traits. In P. Ekman & R.J. Davidson
(Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (pp. 56-58). New
York: Oxford University Press.
137
Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R., & Meinhardt, J. (2007). Colour
and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 136 (1), 154-168.
Elliot, A. C., & Woodward, W. A. (2007). Statistical analysis: Quick reference
guidebook with SPSS examples. London: SAGE Publications.
Evans, D. (2002). Emotion: The science of sentiment. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Fehrman, K. R., & Fehrman, C. (2000). Colour: The secret influence. New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall.
Feisner, A. (2000). Colour: How to use colour in art and design. London: Laurence
King Publishing.
Feisner, A. (2006). Colour: How to use colour in art and design (2nd ed.). London:
Laurence King Publishing.
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Appraisal and beyond. Cognition and Emotion, 7 (3/4),
225-231.
Frijda, N. H. (1994). Varieties of affect: emotions and episodes, moods, and
sentiments. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion:
Fundamental Questions (pp. 59-67). New York: Oxford University Press.
Gao, X. P., & Xin, J. H. (2006). Investigation of humans emotional responses on
colours. Colour Research and Application, 31 (5), 411-417.
Gould, D., Kelly, D., Goldstone, L., & Gammon, J. (2001). Examining the validity of
pressure ulcer risk assessment scales: developing and using illustrated
patient simulations to collect the data INFORMATION POINT: Visual
Analogue Scale. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 10 (5), 697-706.
Grandis, L. D. (1986). Theory and use of colour. New York: Harry N. Abrahams,
Inc.
Hard, A., Sivik, L., & Tonnquist, G. (1996). NCS, Natural Colour System from
concept to research and applications, Part I. Colour Research and
Application, 21 (3), 180-205.
Hard, A., & Sivik, L. (2001). A Theory of colours in combination A Descriptive
model related to the NCS colour-order system. Colour Research and
Application, 26 (1), 4-28.
Heise, D. R. (1970). The semantic differential and attitude research. In F. S. Gene
(Ed.), Attitude measurement (pp. 235-253). Chicago: Rand McNally.
138
Kaya, N., & Epps, H. N. (2004). Relationship between colour and emotion:
A study of college students. College Student Journal, 38 (3), 396-405.
Kaya, N., & Crosby, M. (2006). Colour associations with different building types:
An experimental study on American College students. Colour Research and
Application, 31 (1), 67-71.
Krier, R. (1992). Elements of architecture A. C. Papadakis, (Ed.). London:
Academy Editions.
Kuehni, R. G. (2000). A comparison of five colour order systems. Colour Research
and Application, 25 (2), 123-131.
Kwallek, N., Lewis, C.M., Lin-Hsiao, J. W. D., & Woodson, H. (1998). Effects of nine
monochromatic office interior colours on clerical tasks and worker mood.
Colour Research and Application, 21 (6), 448-458.
Ladau, R. F., Smith, B. K., & Place, J. (1988). Colour in interior design and
architecture. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Larsen, R. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Measurement issues in emotion
research. In D. Kahreman & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Wellbeing: Foundations of
Hedonic Psychology (pp. 40-50). New York: Russell Sage.
Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.) Nebraska
symposium on motivation (vol.16. pp. 175-265). Lincoln, NE: University of
Nebraska Press.
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Leventhall, H. (1984). A perceptual motor theory of emotion. In K. R. Scherer &
P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 271-291). London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Levy, R. I. (1984). The emotions in comparative perspective. In K.R. Scherer & P.
Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 397-412). London: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
Luce, M. F., Bettman, J. R., & Payne, J.W. (1997). Choice processing in emotionally
difficult decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition, 23 (2), 384-405.
Lupton, D. (1998). The Emotional Self: A Sociocultural exploration. London: SAGE
Publications.
Mahnke, F. H. (1996). Colour, environment, and human response. New York:
Wiley.
140
141
Odom, A. S., & Sholtz, S. S. (2000, November 30). The reds, whites, and blues of
emotion: Examining colour hue effects on mood tones [PDF document].
Retrieved from
http://clearinghouse.missouriwestern.edu/manuscripts/184.php
Oliveira, A., Fonseca, I., Teixeira, M., & Simes, F. (2005). A functional
measurement approach to the Self-Assessment Manikin. In J. S. Monahan, S.
M. Sheffert & J. T. Townsend (Eds.). Fechner Day 2005. Proceedings of the
Twenty-First Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics
(pp. 251-256). Mt. Pleasant, MI: The International Society for
Psychophysics.
Orthony, A., Clore, G.L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions.
New York: Cambridge University Press.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1978). The measurement of
meaning. USA: University of Illinois Press.
Padgham, C. A., & Saunders, J. E. (1975). The perception of light and colour.
New York: Academic Press.
Parkinson, B. (1997). Emotion. In B. Parkinson & A.M. Colman (Eds.), Emotion
and motivation (pp. 1-21). New York: Addison Wesley Longman Publishing.
Parkinson, B., & Colman, A.M. (1997). Emotion and motivation. New York:
Addison Wesley Longman Publishing.
Petru, S. (2006). Red, black, or white?: The dawn of colour symbolism.
Documenta Praehistorica, 33, 203-208.
Pile, J. (1997). Colour in interior design. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Pos, O., & Green-Armytage, P. (2007). Facial expressions, colours and basic
emotions. Colour: Design & Creativity, 1 (1):2, 1-20.
Qu, L. C., Luo, M. R., Sun, P. L., Hu, N. C., & Chen, H. S. (2011). Age effects on
colour emotion, preference, and harmony. Colour Research and Application,
1-14. doi: 10.1002/col.20672
Raskin, R. (1986). Colour: An outline of terms and concepts. Denmark: Aarhus
University Press.
Russell, J. A., Weiss, A., & Mendelson, G. A. (1989). Affect grid: A single-item scale
of pleasure and arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 (3),
493-502.
Rymaszewoski, M., Wallace, M., Winters, C. Ondrejka, C., & BatstoreCunningham, B. (2007). Second Life: The official guide. New Jersey: John
Wiley & Sons.
142
143
http://dba.med.sc.edu/price/irf/Adobe_tg/models/ciexyz.html
http://www.iffo.nl/temp/index.php/en/technicalphotography/colourmanagem
ent/59-adobergbvssrgb.html
http://www.sapdesignguild.org/resources/glossary_colour/index1.html
http://www.redminibox.com/products/colourpicker/RGB.php
http://vladek.ehion.com/xchange/hsi/colmodels.html
http://blulob.com
144
http://infoacrs.com/nm/design.html
http://eportfolio.pdht.ca/w11_eportfolio/zeliger_isabella/portfolio/print/rgbc
olourwheel.php
http://blulob.com/2009/03/08/the-rgb-colour-wheel/
http://www.chainstyle.com/tutorials/colwheel.html
145
APPENDICES
APPENDIX A
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
Table A.2. Prototypical patterns of facial expressions (Ekman and Friesen, 1978)
Emotions
Happiness
Anger
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 10 + AU 22 + AU 23 + AU 25,26
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 10 + AU 23 + AU 25,26
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 23 + AU 25,26
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 17 + AU 23
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 17 + AU 24
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 23
AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 7 + AU 24
Sadness
Fear
Surprise
Disgust
A1 + AU 4 + AU 11 + AU 15 with or without AU 54 + AU 65
A1 + AU 4 + AU 15 with or without AU 54 + AU 65
A6 + AU 15 with or without AU 54 + AU 65
AU 25 & AU 26 may occur with all prototypes
AU 1 + AU 2 + AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 20 + AU 25 + AU 26 or AU 27
AU 1 + AU 2 + AU 4 + AU 5 + AU 25 + AU 26 or AU 27
AU 1 + AU 2 + AU 5 + AU 26
AU 1 + AU 2 + AU 5 + AU 27
AU 9
AU 9 + AU 16 + AU 25,26
AU 9 + AU 17
AU 10
AU 10 + AU 16 + AU 25,26
AU 10 + AU 17
154
APPENDIX B
DESCRIPTIONS OF THE SAMPLE GROUP
155
Table B.1. Descriptive statistics showing the mean age of the sample group.
N
Minimum
180
17,00
age
Valid N
(listwise)
Maximum
26,00
Mean
21,0833
Std.
Deviation
1,74930
180
Valid
17,00
18,00
19,00
20,00
21,00
22,00
23,00
24,00
25,00
26,00
Total
Frequency
2
8
21
39
43
33
15
12
6
1
180
Valid
Percent
1,1
4,4
11,7
21,7
23,9
18,3
8,3
6,7
3,3
,6
100,0
Percent
1,1
4,4
11,7
21,7
23,9
18,3
8,3
6,7
3,3
,6
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
1,1
5,6
17,2
38,9
62,8
81,1
89,4
96,1
99,4
100,0
Valid
Frequency
85
Percent
47,2
Valid
Percent
47,2
Cumulative
Percent
47,2
95
52,8
52,8
100,0
180
100,0
100,0
A few times a
day
Everyday
Every other day
Total
Percent
Valid
Percent
Cumulative
Percent
121
67,2
67,2
67,2
54
5
180
30,0
2,8
100,0
30,0
2,8
100,0
97,2
100,0
156
APPENDIX C
SETTING OF THE EXPERIMENT
157
Figure C.1. A view showing the public study area next to the Multimedia Room.
Figure C.2. A view showing the booths for using audio and visual materials.
158
APPENDIX D
INTERIOR SPACES USED IN THE EXPERIMENT
159
160
161
APPENDIX E
THE QUESTIONNAIRE
162
ANKET
AAIDAK SORULARI SZN N EN UYGUN EKLDE CEVAPLAYINIZ:
Kiisel Bilgiler
1. Cinsiyet: Kz
2. Ya:
Erkek
__________________________________
3. Blm: __________________________________
4. e-mail: __________________________________
Yok
163
Bilgisayar Kullanm
1. Bilgisayar kullanyor musunuz? Evet
1 yldan az
1 - 2 yl
2 - 5 yl
5 10 yl
10 yldan fazla
________________________________
164
Hayr
OUESTIONNAIRE
ANSWER THE QUESTIONS APPROPRIATE TO YOUR SITUATION:
Personal Information
1. Gender: Female
2. Age:
Male
__________________________________
3. Department: __________________________________
4. e-mail: __________________________________
2. Do you use any correction equipment (contact lenses, eyeglasses, etc.) for any
eye or vision deficiency you have? Are you wearing this equipment at the
moment?
____________________________________________________________________
Has Not
165
Computer Usage
1. Do you use a computer? Yes
No
______________________
166
APPENDIX F
INFORMATION ABOUT THE STUDY
167
TEEKKR EDERZ
168
This study is designed to gather data for a Ph.D. Dissertation entitled ColurEmotion Associations in Interior Environments prepared at Bilkent University,
Department of Interior Architecture and Environmental Design.
In this study seven faces, each representing different basic emotions, and videos
of two different interior spaces will be shown to you. Please match the shown
interior space with a single face representing a specific emotion that best fit
with each other.
THANK YOU
169
APPENDIX G
FACIAL EXPRESSIONS OF BASIC EMOTIONS USED IN THE EXPERIMENT
170
Figure G.1. The facial expressions of six basic emotions used in the study.
171
APPENDIX H
DATA ANALYSES
172
Table H.1. Statistics of the sequence of showing coloured room for red room.
SEQUENCE
RED ROOM Participants first
experience gray room
Participants first
experience red room
Mean
Std.
Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
3,6333
1,42595
,26034
30
3,2667
1,63861
,29917
F
RED
COLOUR
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
1,620
,208
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
,925
58
,359
,36667
,39659
-,42718
1,16052
,925
56,914
,359
,36667
,39659
-,42751
1,16084
RED ROOM
GENDER
TOTAL
Mean
Std. Deviation
Female
30
3,4000
1,37966
,25189
Male
30
3,5000
1,69685
,30980
F
RED
COLOUR
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
3,242
Sig.
,077
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
-,250
58
,803
-,10000
,39928
-,89925
,69925
-,250
55,682
,803
-,10000
,39928
-,89996
,69996
173
Table H.5. Frequency of emotions associated with red room in respect to male
gender group.
Valid
Missing
Total
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
2
11
3
2
9
2
1
30
30
60
Percent
3,3
18,3
5,0
3,3
15,0
3,3
1,7
50,0
50,0
100,0
Valid
Percent
6,7
36,7
10,0
6,7
30,0
6,7
3,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
6,7
43,3
53,3
60,0
90,0
96,7
100,0
Table H.6. Frequency of emotions associated with red room in respect to female
gender group.
Valid
Missing
Total
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
Total
System
Frequency
1
10
6
2
11
30
30
60
Percent
1,7
16,7
10,0
3,3
18,3
50,0
50,0
100,0
174
Valid
Percent
3,3
33,3
20,0
6,7
36,7
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
3,3
36,7
56,7
63,3
100,0
Table H.7. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association to red room.
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
Observed
N
3
21
9
4
20
2
1
60
Expected
N
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
Residual
-5,6
12,4
,4
-4,6
11,4
-6,6
-7,6
Test Statistics
RED
51,067
6
,000
Chi-Square(a)
df
Asymp. Sig.
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 8,6.
Valid
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
Frequency
3
21
9
4
20
2
1
60
Percent
Valid Percent
5,0
5,0
35,0
35,0
15,0
15,0
6,7
6,7
33,3
33,3
3,3
3,3
1,7
1,7
100,0
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
5,0
40,0
55,0
61,7
95,0
98,3
100,0
SEQUENCE
GRAY ROOM Participants first
experience gray room
Participants first
experience red room
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
3,4333
1,79431
,32759
30
3,8667
2,25501
,41171
175
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
6,363
,014
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
St d. Error
Dif f erence
-,824
58
,414
-,43333
,52614
-1,48651
,61984
-,824
55,214
,414
-,43333
,52614
-1,48764
,62098
Table H.11. Statistics for gender differences for gray room in the first
experiment set.
GENDER
TOTAL
GRAY ROOM Female
Male
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
3,9000
2,26442
,41342
30
3,4000
1,77337
,32377
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
7,863
Sig.
,007
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
St d. Error
Dif f erence
,952
58
,345
,50000
,52512
-,55113
1,55113
,952
54,849
,345
,50000
,52512
-,55242
1,55242
176
Table H.13. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in respect to male
gender group in the first experiment set.
Valid
Missing
Total
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
fear
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
1
9
13
1
2
4
30
30
60
Percent
1,7
15,0
21,7
1,7
3,3
6,7
50,0
50,0
100,0
Valid
Percent
3,3
30,0
43,3
3,3
6,7
13,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
3,3
33,3
76,7
80,0
86,7
100,0
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
13
7
10
30
30
60
Percent
21,7
11,7
16,7
50,0
50,0
100,0
177
Valid
Percent
43,3
23,3
33,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
43,3
66,7
100,0
Table H.15. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association to gray room
in the first experiment set.
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
fear
sadness
Total
Observed
N
1
22
20
1
2
14
60
Expected
N
10,0
10,0
10,0
10,0
10,0
10,0
Residual
-9,0
12,0
10,0
-9,0
-8,0
4,0
Test Statistics
Chi-Square(a)
df
Asymp. Sig.
GRAY
48,600
5
,000
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 10,0.
Table H.16. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in the first
experiment set.
Valid
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
fear
sadness
Total
Frequency
1
22
20
1
2
14
60
178
Cumulative
Percent
1,7
38,3
71,7
73,3
76,7
100,0
Table H.17. Paired Samples t-Test for emotional association differences between
red room and gray room.
Paired Differences
95% Confidence Interval of
the Difference
Pair
COLOUR EMOTION
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
Lower
Upper
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
-2,05000
1,89138
,17266
-2,39188
-1,70812
-11,873
119
,000
Table H.18. Statistics of the sequence of showing coloured rooms for green
room.
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
4,4667
1,45586
,26580
30
3,5333
1,33218
,24322
SEQUENCE
GREEN ROOM Participants first
experience gray room
Participants first
experience green room
F
GREEN
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
,019
,890
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
St d. Error
Dif f erence
2,591
58
,012
,93333
,36029
,21214
1,65453
2,591
57,549
,012
,93333
,36029
,21202
1,65465
179
Valid
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
System
Missing
Total
Frequency
1
3
4
1
17
2
2
30
30
60
Cumulative
Percent
3,3
13,3
26,7
30,0
86,7
93,3
100,0
Frequency
Valid
Percent
Valid Percent
Cumulative
Percent
anger
1,7
3,3
3,3
disgust
10,0
20,0
23,3
neutral
11
18,3
36,7
60,0
surprise
1,7
3,3
63,3
10
16,7
33,3
96,7
1,7
3,3
100,0
30
30
60
50,0
50,0
100,0
100,0
happiness
fear
Total
Missing System
Total
Mean
Std. Deviation
Female
30
4,0667
1,25762 ,22961
Male
30
3,9333
1,65952 ,30299
180
F
GREEN
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
4,513
,038
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
St d. Error
Dif f erence
,351
58
,727
,13333
,38016
-,62764
,89430
,351
54,048
,727
,13333
,38016
-,62882
,89549
Valid
Missing
Total
anger
disgust
neutral
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
2
5
7
13
1
2
30
30
60
Cumulative
Percent
6,7
23,3
46,7
90,0
93,3
100,0
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
Total
System
Frequency
4
8
2
14
2
30
30
60
181
Cumulative
Percent
13,3
40,0
46,7
93,3
100,0
1
6
11
1
10
1
30
5,0
5,0
5,0
5,0
5,0
5,0
-4,0
1,0
6,0
-4,0
5,0
-4,0
Test Statistics
GREEN
Chi-Square
22,000a
df
5
Asymp. Sig. ,001
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 15,0.
Table H.27. Statistics of the sequence of showing coloured room for gray room.
SEQUENCE
GRAY ROOM Participants first
experience gray room
Participants first
experience green room
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
3,5333
1,87052
,34151
30
4,1000
2,32453
,42440
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
8,825
Sig.
,004
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
-1,040
58
,303
-,56667
,54474
-1,65709
,52375
-1,040
55,461
,303
-,56667
,54474
-1,65815
,52482
182
Table H.29. Statistics for gender differences for gray room in the second
experiment set.
GENDER
TOTAL
GRAY ROOM Female
Male
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
4,0333
2,22033
,40538
30
3,6000
2,01032
,36703
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
2,266
,138
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
,792
58
,431
,43333
,54685
-,66130
1,52797
,792
57,437
,431
,43333
,54685
-,66153
1,52820
Table H.31. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in respect to male
gender group in the second experiment set.
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
12
10
1
7
30
30
60
183
Cumulative
Percent
40,0
73,3
76,7
100,0
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
11
8
1
10
30
30
60
Percent
Valid Percent
18,3
36,7
13,3
26,7
1,7
3,3
16,7
33,3
50,0
100,0
50,0
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
36,7
63,3
66,7
100,0
Table H.33. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association to gray room
in the second experiment set.
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
Observed N Expected N
23
15,0
18
15,0
2
15,0
17
15,0
60
Residual
8,0
3,0
-13,0
2,0
Test Statistics
GRAY
ChiSquare(a)
df
Asymp. Sig.
16,400
3
,001
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 15,0.
Table H.34. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in the second
experiment set.
Valid
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
Frequency Percent
23
38,3
18
30,0
2
3,3
17
28,3
60
100,0
184
Valid
Percent
38,3
30,0
3,3
28,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
38,3
68,3
71,7
100,0
Table H.35. Paired Samples t-Test for emotional association differences between
green room and gray room.
Paired Differences
95% Confidence Interval of
the Difference
Std.
Deviation
Mean
Pair
COLOUR - EMOTION
2,47500
Std. Error
Mean
Lower
2,02074 ,18447
Upper
-2,84026
df
-2,10974 -13,417
Sig. (2tailed)
119 ,000
Table H.36. Statistics of the sequence of showing coloured room for blue room.
SEQUENCE
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
3,7667
1,67504 ,30582
Participants first
experience blue room
30
3,8333
1,72374 ,31471
F
BLUE
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
,000
1,000
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
-,152
58
,880
-,06667
,43882
-,94507
,81174
-,152
57,952
,880
-,06667
,43882
-,94508
,81175
Mean
Std. Deviation
Female
30
4,1667
1,72374 ,31471
Male
30
3,4333
1,59056 ,29040
185
F
BLUE
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
1,870
,177
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
1,713
58
,092
,73333
,42822
-,12384
1,59051
1,713
57,629
,092
,73333
,42822
-,12396
1,59062
Table H.40. Frequency of emotions associated with blue room in respect to male
gender group.
Valid
Missing
Total
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
3
3
15
2
3
2
2
30
30
60
Percent
Valid Percent
5,0
10,0
5,0
10,0
25,0
50,0
3,3
6,7
5,0
10,0
3,3
6,7
3,3
6,7
50,0
100,0
50,0
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
10,0
20,0
70,0
76,7
86,7
93,3
100,0
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
5
10
2
5
4
4
30
30
60
Percent
8,3
16,7
3,3
8,3
6,7
6,7
50,0
50,0
100,0
186
Valid
Percent
16,7
33,3
6,7
16,7
13,3
13,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
16,7
50,0
56,7
73,3
86,7
100,0
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
Observed
N
3
8
25
4
8
6
6
60
Expected
N
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
8,6
Residual
-5,6
-,6
16,4
-4,6
-,6
-2,6
-2,6
Test Statistics
BLUE
39,167
6
,000
Chi-Square(a)
df
Asymp. Sig.
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 8,6.
Valid
anger
disgust
neutral
surprise
happiness
fear
sadness
Total
Frequency
3
8
25
4
8
6
6
60
187
Cumulative
Percent
5,0
18,3
60,0
66,7
80,0
90,0
100,0
Table H.44. Statistics of the sequence of showing coloured room for gray room
in the third experiment set.
SEQUENCE
Mean
Std. Error
Mean
Std. Deviation
30
3,8000
2,00688 ,36641
Participants first
experience blue room
30
4,5000
2,19325 ,40043
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
2,765
,102
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
-1,290
58
,202
-,70000
,54277
-1,78647
,38647
-1,290
57,549
,202
-,70000
,54277
-1,78665
,38665
Table H.46. Statistics for gender differences for gray room in the third set.
GENDER
TOTAL
GRAY ROOM Female
Male
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
30
4,4000
2,12700
,38834
30
3,9000
2,10664
,38462
F
GRAY
Equal v ariances
assumed
Equal v ariances
not assumed
Sig.
,370
,545
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Dif f erence
Std. Error
Dif f erence
,915
58
,364
,50000
,54657
-,59407
1,59407
,915
57,995
,364
,50000
,54657
-,59408
1,59408
188
Table H.48. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in respect to male
participants in the third experiment set.
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
9
12
9
30
30
60
Percent
15,0
20,0
15,0
50,0
50,0
100,0
Valid
Percent
30,0
40,0
30,0
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
30,0
70,0
100,0
Valid
Missing
Total
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
System
Frequency
6
11
2
11
30
30
60
Percent
10,0
18,3
3,3
18,3
50,0
50,0
100,0
Valid
Percent
20,0
36,7
6,7
36,7
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
20,0
56,7
63,3
100,0
Table H.50. Chi-square goodness-of-fit test for emotion association to gray room
in the third experiment set.
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
Observed N Expected N
15
15,0
23
15,0
2
15,0
20
15,0
60
Residual
,0
8,0
-13,0
5,0
Test Statistics
Chi-Square(a)
df
Asymp. Sig.
GRAY
17,200
3
,001
a 0 cells (,0%) have expected frequencies less than 5. The minimum expected cell
frequency is 15,0.
189
Table H.51. Frequency of emotions associated with gray room in the third
experiment set.
Valid
disgust
neutral
happiness
sadness
Total
Frequency
15
23
2
20
60
Percent
25,0
38,3
3,3
33,3
100,0
Valid
Percent
25,0
38,3
3,3
33,3
100,0
Cumulative
Percent
25,0
63,3
66,7
100,0
Table H.52. Paired Samples t-Test for emotional association differences between
blue room and gray room.
Paired Differences
Mean
Pair
COLOUR - EMOTION
2,40833
Std.
Deviation
Std. Error
Mean
1,85389 ,16924
190
Upper
-2,07323 -14,231
df
Sig. (2tailed)
119 ,000