Stuttering ThiaminProtocol
Stuttering ThiaminProtocol
Stuttering ThiaminProtocol
IMPORTANT NOTICE
This report is intended as a reference work only, not as a medical guide
or a manual for self treatment. One should always seek competent
medical advice before trying or even suggesting the supplements
described in this report
Acknowledgements
For my wife,
Judith
and to the memory of my mother-in-law,
Jeane Stockheim
Table of Contents
Acknowledgments..
Table of contents..
Foreword..
1.
2.
3.
13
4.
20
5.
25
6.
54
7.
62
8.
67
9.
Intent Therapy..
73
85
Afterword..
86
Appendix..
88
Foreword
In this report I demonstrate that thiamin (vitamin B1) taken daily and in
sufficient quantities can put stuttering into remission for almost a third of
young adult males who stutter - regardless of how long they have stuttered or how severely.
It has now been five years since the original investigation and those who
became fluent during that first study and continued to take the vitamin
have remained fluent.
Chapter 1
Background and the accidental discovery
It was 1960 and I was a graduate student in speech science at Ohio State
University, preparing to take an examination that, if passed, would allow
me to start writing a doctoral dissertation. As part of my preparation, I decided to read the literature in speech pathology from its beginning. I was
interested in learning more about the history of the profession and felt if I
devoted some time each day to reading early journal articles, I would get a
better understanding of the growth and direction the profession had taken
since its inception some forty years earlier.
One day, during my reading, I happened upon a study that, in spite of its
many flaws, seemed quite remarkable. It had to do with the effect of thiamin on the speech of children who stuttered.
As I read the study, I imagined the pages of the journal filled, in subsequent issues, with many discussions of the results of this study. But no,
nothing. It was if the study hadnt been published.
Perhaps it had to do with the extremely poor quality of its design. There
were many mistakes. But still, nothing ever appeared in the journal, not
even a criticism of it. I was amazed but since I had, at that time, just a
passing interest in stuttering and other things to do, I simply noted this
piece of information and continued my reading of the early literature in
speech pathology in preparation for taking the doctoral qualifying exams in
speech science.
Chapter 2
NYU Medical School and
another accidental discovery
We move now to 1974, some 12 years after receiving my doctorate. During that time I had been doing basic research, moving up the academic
ladder, and publishing articles that, while interesting in an abstract sense,
had no clinical relevance. Then, in 1974, I decided to shift my focus to clinical issues. I reexamined the speech pathology literature and decided that
the area of stuttering, for several reasons, would be fertile ground for investigation.
First, in spite of the fact that it had a huge research literature, no one
seemed to have a good understanding of what was going on. And second, the enigmatic nature of the disorder intrigued me. (For example,
people who stutter often stop stuttering immediately and completely when
they go into an empty room and speak to themselves out loud alone).
Then there was a very excellent text written by an eminent expert in stuttering named Charles Van Riper. The text was called The Nature of Stuttering and it had well over a thousand references to research studies that
had been conducted over the years on all aspects of stuttering. I decided
to examine the book in great detail to see if I could use the findings to
generate a model of stuttering that would account for most, if not all, of the
features described by Van Riper.
10
Coinciding in time with this came an offer to join the faculty at New York
University Medical School as a research professor. This was quite appealing but I had one proviso: In addition to doing research on the speech of
people born with diverse cranio-facial anomalies (such as cleft lip and
palate), I also wanted time to do research into stuttering. They agreed
and I took the position.
My appointment at the Medical School was at the Institute of Reconstructive Plastic Surgery. I began to examine a whole host of speech problems
arising from a variety of genetically-based malformations of the head. A
previous interest in the anatomy and physiology of the speech mechanism
stood me in good stead as I began to collaborate with several surgeons on
the design of new operations to improve the speech of people born with
cleft palate.
One of the cleft palate patients stuttered and when the ultrasound-generating device was applied to the side of the neck, something happened as
the patient spoke that was not expected. The throat seemed to constrict
just before and during most stutter events. The behavior was relatively
consistent and perked my attention. So I contacted the speech clinic at the
medical school and over the next several months obtained five more individuals who stuttered (but with no cleft palates) and, again using ultrasound, made similar observations.
11
Further research revealed that the center of this activity was at the base of
the throat in a structure called the larynx and, within that structure, the vocal cords. The thought occurred that these activities, since they not only
coincided in time with the stutter but also often preceded it, might somehow have been provoking or triggering it.
It is well known that the vocal cords are, in part, controlled by the breathing centers of the brain. So it seemed logical that some sort of breathing
maneuver performed just before speaking might be capable of exerting a
positive influence on reducing the apparent effort associated with these
constricting vocal cord movements. After experimenting with a variety of
breathing techniques, one was settled upon that reduced the frequency of
stuttering by almost 70%.
Later it was discovered that a combination of this new breathing technique
coupled with a slowed first word of each sentence could further reduce the
frequency of stuttering to almost 94%. I wrote a book about this combined
approach and this led eventually to the establishment of The National
Center For Stuttering.
The functions of The Center were to treat clients and train speech pathologists. During the 1980s and 90s over eight hundred speech therapists
were trained in what was later called the Airflow Technique. In the
process, over fifteen thousand individuals who stuttered were seen for
treatment, making The National Center For Stuttering, at that time, the
largest treatment and training facility of its kind in the world.
12
Chapter 3
Thiamin and stuttering
The Center, during the peak of its operation, maintained a hotline number
for the general public. The hotline was in existence from 1977 until 2010.
When parents called requesting guidance for young children who had recently begun to stutter, along with the usual stress-reducing recommendations, the Center recommended that the child be given 30mg of thiamin
daily - provided the child was between the ages of two and four and the
parent had first obtained permission from the pediatrician.
In no instance was more than 30mg of the vitamin per day recommended.
13
specialist are shown in parentheses). Later, we will discuss the enhancements that have subsequently been made to it to increase the percentage
of those showing positive results.
The study
Excluded was anyone with a medical condition that would interfere with thiamin absorption or any medication or dietary
habits that could negate the effects of ingesting the vitamin.
The experiment lasted for two weeks. This time period was
selected because prior clinical experience and the earlier 1951
report had indicated that positive speech effects of thiamin, if
they were to occur, would take place within the first two weeks.
In addition, prior clinical experience had also shown that
300mg daily of the vitamin was the level at which positive
speech effects for adult males might be expected to occur.
14
15
In all instances, judgments of stutter events were made independently from video recordings by two clinicians with a total
of 47 years working with individuals who stutter. Video presentation was considered essential since, for some of the subjects, a number of the stutter events were silent struggles.
Results
Inter- and intra-judge reliability correlation coefficients (a statistical test) for repeated judgments were greater than 94%,
indicating that the two raters agreed well with themselves and
each other.
The average stuttered syllables for the two groups prior to the
start of the study were not significantly different (meaning that
both randomly selected groups probably came from the same
population). The average stuttered syllables for the control
group (the ones taking the placebo) pre- and post-trial were
also not significantly different. However, the average stuttered
syllables for the experimental group (the ones taking the vitamin) pre- and post-trial were significantly different (p<.01).
(This means that the difference before and after taking the vitamin was so great that it could have occurred by chance less
than once in a hundred repeats of the experiment.)
A closer examination of the data revealed that the switch effect was present for 6 of the 19 subjects receiving the supplement. Pre- supplement, the average percent syllables for
this group of six had been 9.1%. Post-supplement, it was less
16
Discussion
Within the limits of this study, one may conclude that for slightly less than a third of young adult males who stutter between
the ages of 21 and 37, 300mg of thiamin, taken daily, in the
absence of precluding medical conditions, may be of significant value in either substantially reducing or eliminating their
stuttering. This finding, if substantiated by others, would seem
to have profound implications for the approximately 1% of the
worlds population that stutters.
The six subjects showing the switch effect have now been
followed for five years.
17
There is another reason a cohort study is important. The results and conclusion derived in the original study were based
on a very small number of subjects. While some may object to
this, saying that a small number can easily result in an unrecognized sampling error, the positive outcome of a cohort study
can be used to demonstrate that the finding and conclusion
derived from the original study are probably correct.
Also, bear in mind that many statistical tests have been developed for the express purpose of examining data from small
numbers of experimental subjects. These tests, called small
score statistics, have been shown to yield valid results.
Of
The cohort follow-up was conducted by telephone at approximate two-week intervals, now over a five-year period.
The
18
There is one additional piece of evidence that lends credence to this finding - even though it is anecdotal. There is a blog on the internet called:
stuttersense (at http://blogspot.com) that is largely devoted to the Airflow
Technique that has, at my suggestion, started the discussion of thiamin
and stuttering. Since the discussion began, the blog conducted two surveys among its readers who elected to try thiamin and found that among
74 respondents who indicated they had taken the specified amount of thiamin (300mg per day), 29 (38%) reported dramatic improvements in their
speech.
19
Chapter 4
More about thiamin
The recommended dietary allowance for adult males for thiamin is 1.5mg
per day. The positive effects reported in the experiment required an
amount 200 times greater. One might logically raise the issue of toxicity.
Fortunately, the issue has been addressed repeatedly and the answer
provided by virtually all experts in such matters is that in the amounts taken orally, there have been virtually no adverse reactions recorded. Thiamin is one of the water-soluble vitamins and the common belief is that
any excess is simply urinated out. The Food and Nutrition Board of the
National Academy of Sciences has indicated that there is no upper limit of
toxicity for this vitamin. The Food and Drug Administration lists thiamin
hydrochloride among its GRAS (Generally Recognized As Safe) supplements. A number of studies, investigating a variety of disorders, have
used as much as 9000 milligrams (9 grams) per day for prolonged periods
of time (months), with no negative consequences. Various international
organizations have generally agreed that vitamin B1, when taken orally, in
amounts up to and including 500mg per day poses no threat to the health
of the general public although, when pressed, they admit that this number is arbitrary and could just as well have been higher.
20
There is one additional recommendation: be sure to take the form of thiamin called thiamin hydrochloride. Thiamin Hydrochloride was the compound used in the 1951 study and the Centers study with adults. There
are a number of other forms of thiamin, but none of the opinions offered in
this book can relate to them because they have not been studied.
It has been demonstrated that the maximum amount of thiamin that can
be used by the body at any one time is 5mg. Indeed, the total amount of
thiamin that typically can be found in the body is 30mg. So the question
remains: why 300mg? The answer probably lies somewhere in the extremely complex process of absorbing thiamin and getting it past a structure called the blood-brain barrier.
But there are two processes that can be brought to bear to bring thiamin
into the brain. One makes use of chemical carriers that transport a small
amount of thiamin across the barrier. But that source is saturable (essentially limited in its capacity). There is another process that is much less
saturable. It is called diffusion and it occurs when chemicals in high concentration on one side of a membrane are passively driven across that
membrane to equalize the concentrations on both sides of the membrane.
21
It has been shown that if one takes 100mg of thiamin three times a day,
its density in the blood will continue to rise over the first 7 10 days, finally
reaching a steady state after that time. Interestingly, it would appear that
total levels of 100 or 200mg of thiamin per day taken orally simply do not
produce sufficient blood densities to diffuse enough of the vitamin across
the barrier to produce much of an effect upon stuttering, while at 300mg
per day, for almost a third of adult males, it does.
Thus choosing 300mg a day as the level for the adults in the Centers
study came about as a result of trying different levels of the vitamin clinically and choosing the lowest level that would appear to make a difference
in stuttered speech for a significant number of individuals.
Interestingly, if one visits a vitamin shop and looks at the levels of thiamin
available, one may be surprised to see a large number of bottles marked
500mg. These are routinely recommended by physicians for alcoholics.
Alcohol gravely depletes the body of thiamin by interacting with it in many
ways thereby effectively eliminating it.
There are other issues, besides alcohol, that can have negative impacts
as well. Here, for example, is an enumeration of just a few of the many
conditions that have been shown to interfere with thiamin absorption:
Alzheimers Disease
Anorexia
Diabetes
Hyperthyroidism
Infection
Malabsorption syndrome
22
Inadequate thiamine intake can also occur with diets containing a lot of the
following:
Acid blockers
Antacids
Antibiotics
Anticonvulsants
Antivirals
Aromatase inhibitors
Blood pressure drugs
Bronchodilators
Cardiac glycosides
Diuretics
Hormone replacement therapy
Oral contraceptives
Sulfonamides
23
The modern food industry has responded to the fragility of thiamin by moving toward thiamin enrichment of grain products. In a sense, this has been
effective for without it, thiamin deficiency would be a much more common phenomenon.
People with heart failure and gastrointestinal disease have an increased
risk of thiamin deficiency. In each of these groups, restoring normal thiamin levels may prevent some of the worst complications of the disease.
Even in the absence of any of these diseases, elderly people are at increased risk of thiamin deficiency. This is, at least in part, due to a reduction in the ability to absorb dietary thiamin that occurs as we age.
Popular fad diets that greatly reduce intake of grains and legumes make
thiamin deficiency more likely. For instance, after eight weeks on the
Atkins diet, people following this popular low-carbohydrate strategy saw
their thiamin intake drop by 40%.
As can be seen from the above, even the recommended dietary allowance
(RDA) of 1.2mg of thiamin per day, given the minefield of obstacles standing in its way, may be difficult to achieve. Some nutritionists consider thiamin to be the most deficient vitamin in the diet.
24
Chapter 5
The trigger for stuttering
This chapter describes the trigger for stuttering, how it arises and develops in childhood, the stresses that provoke it and the ways individuals
eventually learn to cope with it. This sets the stage for the later explanation of the role of thiamin in producing fluent speech. It also explains why
a technique (Intent Therapy), also to be described later, works as well as it
does.
Abstract
The behaviors that characterize stuttering are learned attempts to prevent or release these speech-arresting levels of
vocal cord tension. The learned behaviors can be diverse.
For example, at the onset of stuttering in children, the most
typical immediate response to a sudden vocal cord immobilization is a syllable repetition.
with varying degrees of tension and frequency and often become habits.
After a period of time, which can vary greatly, the child may
become aware of and react to these repetitions by engaging
in one or more of a number of behaviors.
These can be
avoidance activities such as changing words, forceful confrontational attacks such as violent head shaking, or more
subtle pre-speech maneuvers such as swallowing, inhaling
deeply or speaking on expiratory reserve volume (the very
last bit of air left in the lungs). All of these behaviors can become learned because all have the power to either prevent
the unique pattern of sensory impulses (the trigger) from occurring or to extricate from it once it has occurred and thus
are rewarding.
26
27
stuttering(8) and part as a result of the successful use of behavior modification techniques in treatment.(9) (Behavior modification techniques are used to change learned behaviors.)
All the things that people who stutter do when they get stuck
in their speech and/or all the things that people who stutter do
to avoid getting stuck in their speech.
This definition may appear simplistic, but notice what it accomplishes. First, it takes the point of view of the person who
stutters, not the point of view of an observer. Second, it takes
an exclusive orientation away from a catalog of overt struggle
behaviors and provides for the very real possibility of important cognitive components. Finally, it strongly emphasizes
that overt stuttering, scanning and avoidance behaviors are
responses to something else - the getting stuck or, what is
hypothesized in this chapter to be the event that elicits the
trigger for stuttering.
28
ger for stuttering occur more often among males than females, why does it tend to run in families, why is it absent or
markedly reduced when a person who stutters talks to themselves out loud alone, or when they sing, or whisper, or speak
to the rhythm of a metronome, or speak chorally, or against a
masking noise, or when using a foreign accent or talking to
pets?(10) And why does the trigger typically not occur when, in
the course of speech development, a child is using single
words to express himself - only to appear suddenly months or
years later when the child begins using sentences?(11) And
when it does appear, why does it typically manifest itself at
the beginnings of sentences rather than being evenly distributed throughout?(12)
And then there are the interesting issues raised by the cognitive component.
And, later in life, if this child, as an adult, masters word substitution and situation avoidance to the point where he never
stutters overtly but still lives in constant fear of inadvertently
stuttering - are we not to call him a person who stutters?
These are some of the clinical questions that arise when a
traditional overt-symptom-response-only approach is taken
29
If overt stuttering, scanning and avoiding are learned responses to the occurrence or anticipation of a trigger,
wouldnt it be better to ignore these learned responses and
treat the trigger instead?
30
One might argue about the validity of the above example, but
it still retains its power to explain what is here contended to
be a common mistake in therapy (attempting to treat stuttering instead of its trigger). It also explains a similar and common mistake made by researchers who try to uncover the
core brain mechanisms underlying stuttering by exhaustively
mapping the brains equivalents to what are essentially
learned responses.
For example, a
But both psychotherapy and speech therapy have long histories of not being very successful in the treatment of chronic
stuttering.(13, 14) Both disciplines often rationalize their relative
lack of success by saying that since stuttering is a complex,
multi-factorial problem, starting early in life and of long-standing duration, it is highly resistant to permanent extinction.
Rarely, if ever, is there mention of an intervening step, such
as a triggering event, that might be going unaddressed.
31
In our quest for the origin of the trigger, we examine the beginnings of stuttering. We do so for one reason: unencumbered by the overlay of years of conditioning, important clues
to the trigger reveal themselves more readily at this early
stage.
ages of two and five, with a major peak of onset of occurrence between two and a half and three and a half years of
age.(15) The second is that most children who stutter have
been speaking fluently for a period of time before their stuttering begins. The average length of time the child has been
speaking fluently before the onset of stuttering is seventeen
months, but it can vary widely.(16) Third, many children who
stutter can have periods when they are totally fluent. These
periods, sometimes called remissions, can last for days,
weeks, months, years or, in the case of outgrowth, forever.(17)
Fourth, at the onset, most stuttering appears at the beginnings of phrases or sentences.(18) And fifth, by far, the most
32
Given these observations, it is obvious that the trigger, whatever it is, is not occurring all or even most of the time. And if
the trigger is a response to stress, then the stress apparently
comes and goes quickly, and often without apparent rhyme or
reason. It is strange to think, for example, that a stress and
its associated trigger could be present on the first word of a
sentence and not on the rest of the sentence, or that Johnny
could be overheard talking fluently with his action figures
when playing alone in the family room, only to stutter instantly
when someone comes into the room and asks him a question.
For the purposes of this chapter, the threshold for the trigger
is defined as: A level of physiological activity capable of eliciting an overt stutter or of provoking an avoidance response.
When the magnitude of the physiological activity crosses the
threshold, an overt stutter response occurs; when it ap-
33
And since,
without scanning, the trigger events are essentially all unanticipated, they evoke what are probably the most basic of responses.
It is a contention of this chapter that the most basic of responses are the ones that occur whenever there is a sudden,
unanticipated stoppage of a learned and generally smoothly
functioning forward progression of preprogrammed neuromuscular events.
So, for example, both speech and walking are forwardmovement processes. Both have beginnings, midpoints, and
endings.
34
Call
these actions repetitions. The second group, much less numerous than the first, were those who applied pressure continuously to the bar in the effort to open the door. Call these
actions prolongations. And the third group were those who
paused, seemingly impatiently waiting for the door to open so
they could continue walking. Call these inactions (or hesitations) blocks.
35
er, children had been speaking for a while prior to the onset of
stuttering. The learning process had progressed without undue incident. Speech was becoming smoother and more automatic each day.
movement process, the progress was abruptly and unexpectedly halted. (The equivalent to the getting stuck part of the
definition).
Interestingly, re-
search on the earliest stuttering signs show this to be precisely the case, with the onset of signs typically being mostly repetitions, with occasional prolongations and/or hesitations. (20)
36
Children
37
So swallowing, when it occurs, is usually a conscious anticipatory behavior designed to either disable or prevent a trigger
from occurring.
38
Since the trigger occurs in response to stress and both apparently occur in the silence before speech, it would seem
reasonable to consider what events might be taking place
during this time period that could be interpreted as stressful.
There are several obvious candidates. To begin, if the child is
speaking in sentences, as they typically are when stuttering
begins, there are a number of choices that have to be made
before speech can begin. The vocabulary, grammar, syntax,
intonation and linguistic stress of the entire sentence have to
be selected, integrated and sequenced.(24) And these activities, and the neurological processes that sub-serve them,
have to take place in fractions of a second and in a brain that,
on average, is two and a half to three and a half years old.
It is a contention of this chapter that these complex and diverse activities, occurring quickly and simultaneously in a relatively immature central nervous system, constitute multiple
stresses that express themselves as heightened levels of
muscle tension in the vocal tract during the silence before
speech begins.
39
forward progression of preprogrammed neuromuscular activities. They are the repeated pressing of the bar required to
open the stuck door at the end of the railway car.
It has been
shown, for example, that given the correct phonetic environment, the effect of the articulatory (speech) requirements for
the last word of a sentence can sometimes be detected on
the first word of that sentence.(26) This means that continuous
speech is not merely a concatenation of one word independently following another in time, but instead, prior to the onset
of the sentence, that is, during the silence that precedes it, a
large part of the articulatory organization for the entire sentence can take place.
40
The second not-so-obvious event occurring in the silence before speech is the stress associated with speaking too rapidly. More specifically, it is the speed of the first word. It is
called speed stress and it manifests itself in the following
manner: As the length of a sentence is increased, the duration of each of the syllables in the sentence decreases.(27)
This means, that as the sentence gets longer, the speaker
talks more rapidly. And although this is true throughout the
sentence, nowhere is it apparently more prominent than on
the first word of the sentence.(28) So the speed of the first
word is particularly sensitive to the number of words in the
sentence. This is a normal feature of speech production.
41
speed extension.
The basic principle of physiology demonstrated is that in order to have progressively greater initial velocities, one must
recruit (involve) progressively more muscle fibers before the
movement begins and that this recruitment is reflected in the
greater average tensions recorded prior to the onset of the
faster extensions.
42
Were pre-
The second type is the position required of the vocal cords for
the first sound in an utterance. As indicated earlier, it is not
always the case that the vocal cords adduct (come together
and touch one another) just before speech. Quite often the
opposite is true. A little less than half of the consonants in
English are voiceless, that is, they require the vocal cords to
be apart. So, for example, the voiceless p, t, and k sounds
are produced with the vocal cords apart, while their voiced
articulatory equivalents, b, d and g, are produced with the
vocal cords together.
43
The are many sources for this tension. Some can be said to
be systemic. These might be: fatigue, nutritional deficiencies,
allergies, illnesses, and hormone fluctuations. Others might
be reactions to environmental conditions such as extremes of
temperature, humidity - or positive ions, drugs, alcohol, pollutants, etc.
44
There are thus at least ten separate events taking place simultaneously in the silence before phrases or sentences in
normal speech production. They are: the selection of vocabulary, grammar, syntax, intonation and linguistic stress, the
effects of co-articulation, speed of the first word, and the two
forms of pre-phonatory tuning of the larynx: correct pitch and
loudness at onset and correct voiced/voiceless distinction at
onset - and the base level tension. Add to this an eleventh
event, the role of anticipatory stress, scanning - in the case of
the individual who has learned to look ahead for feared
sounds, words and speaking situations - and the relatively
complete picture of stress is painted.
Managing these events correctly represents a substantial undertaking when viewed within the constraints of the time
available and the immaturity of the nervous system at the typical age of the onset of stuttering. Clearly this creates a multiple-stress situation and, as indicated earlier, a common expression of stress is an increase in muscle tension.
The question remains, where is the most likely site of the tension? The purpose of this chapter is to suggest that the answer lies at the narrowest aperture along the vocal tract from
lungs to lips: the space between the two vocal cords. A small
increase in muscle tension at this location can, given its crucial role in so many of these activities, create more difficulty in
45
speech than an increase in muscle tension at any other location. Furthermore, if the critical source of tension were elsewhere, let us say, the lips, the tongue, or the soft palate, the
primary initial signs of stuttering, repetitions, would show articulation and/or resonance defects indicative of struggle in
these areas, but they do not. On the contrary, most clinicians
often describe the very early onset repetitions as seemingly
effortless.
Swallowing, rapid, deep inspirations, and speaking on supplemental air - all prevent repetitions because they interfere
with the buildup of tension leading to the threshold of laryngeal blocks, but they do so in different ways. For example, in
the case of an abrupt, deep, inspiratory gesture, a rapid and
large laryngeal patency (opening) is produced by a reflexive
contraction of two muscles, the posterior cricoarytenoid and
cricothyroid(33) to accommodate the greater inflow of air. This
neural command to the laryngeal abductors (muscles that
open the vocal cords) supersedes the intent to speak, with
46
Speaking on supplemental air (just the opposite of the previous approach) also accomplishes the same thing, but in a
more involved way.
47
flex, like the others, interferes with the preparations that must
be made to get ready for speech.
There are a whole host of other observations that further implicate the larynx as the site of the trigger. Here are just a
few: Occasionally, in the effort to speak without stuttering, a
child will resort to whispering. The altered physiology of the
larynx during whispering to a more abducted state of the vocal folds would support the role of the larynx as the site of the
trigger.(37) The use of the electro larynx - a device used by laryngectomees (individuals who have had their voice boxes
removed - usually because of cancer) to produce voice has
elicited fluent speech when used by non-laryngectomized individuals who stutter.(38) Changing the pitch of the voice, and
48
thus the internal and external laryngeal tensions and postures, has produced similar effects.(39) Mouthing words fluently with no attempt at phonation further supports the contention.(40) Fluent esophageal speech (speech made by swallowing air into the esophagus and releasing it in a sort of controlled burp) following total laryngectomy with individuals who
had stuttered preoperatively has been reported.(41) The therapeutic use of gentle onsets of phonation to ease into vocal
fold vibration also is corroborative.(42) And the research that
shows, in a similar vein, that individuals who stutter have
longer Voice Onset Times (take longer to get their vocal cords
vibrating) than people who dont suggests an inherent state of
neural-based inertia at the vocal cords.(42) All of this admittedly circumstantial, but all, nevertheless, suggestive of a laryngeal involvement.
common activities for lessening or interfering with the prephonatory tensions in and around the larynx is the use of verbal starters. A starter may be a sound, a syllable, a word or
words (real or nonsense) or a phrase that has no contextual
linguistic function and is used before the onset of the feared
utterance to get the vocal cords vibrating in the expectation
that they will continue to vibrate directly into and through the
feared utterance. Verbal starters have a reputation for working initially and then failing. The reason this happens is that
after a while, the starter becomes so automatic it becomes
incorporated into the overall plan for the sentence.
49
Said
Change the afferent array and the conditioned stutter responses cease. The most efficient way for making changes
to the array is to subtract laryngeal tensions or to create new
ones, ones that have no cue value. So while Pavlovs dogs
were conditioned to respond to the acoustic spectral array of
a bell by salivating, no such response occurred to the
acoustic spectral array of hand clapping since only the bells
spectra had been the recipient of conditioning.
50
We do know, however, that since the basal ganglia are critically involved in memory, cognition, learning, sequencing of
complex motor commands and have direct connections with
51
52
All of these behaviors, with the exception of the core behavior, the laryngeal block, are learned.
53
Chapter 6
Laryngeal reciprocal inhibition and GABA
We now understand that to prevent the trigger for stuttering from occurring
one must somehow prevent the buildup of tension that occurs at the vocal
cords under conditions of stress. In the previous chapter we specified
eleven stresses that have the potential of adding to the buildup of that tension. In this chapter, we start by taking a closer look at the behavior of the
vocal cords during normal speech as well as during moments of stuttering.
In part 5 of the previous chapter we pointed out that some sounds are
made without voicing (that is, the vocal cords are apart and not making
any sound) and other sounds are made with voicing (that is, the vocal
cords are together and vibrating to making sound). In most languages of
the world, this voiceless/voiced distinction is often the only characteristic
separating two sounds that are, in all other respects, identical.
For example, in English, the [s] sound is voiceless, the sound heard as an
[s] is generated in the mouth as air passes through a narrow channel between the tongue and anterior hard palate and becomes turbulent. No
sound is made at the vocal cords. Its voiced equivalent [z] is the same as
the [s] in terms of how its made but, in addition, the vocal cords are vibrating, thus adding a second source of sound.
54
the consonant is voiced or voiceless (the vowels are all voiced). Also note
that this is happening with great speed (in thousandths of a second) and
with exquisite precision. This important requirement for speech has been
almost entirely neglected by researchers and clinicians working with people who stutter. However, as we will discover, it is of utmost importance in
understanding the mechanism underlying the trigger for stuttering.
The muscles that oppose each other in opening and closing the cords are
said to be antagonistic to one another. Most muscles or muscle groups
have their antagonist muscles or muscle groups.
So, for example, I bend my arm at the elbow by contracting the biceps
muscle and extend it by contracting the triceps muscle. The biceps and
triceps are said to be antagonists. But also, and very important, while one
muscle is actively contracting to produce the desired movement the other,
its antagonist, is actively relaxing. This means that if one looks closely at
my triceps when my biceps is actively contracting, not only is my triceps
not opposing the contraction, but the normal subtle background contracting present to keep my triceps warmed up, so to speak, so that it is ready
to work is suddenly absent.
55
Furthermore, while the basal ganglia receives information from most of the
cerebrum, it sends almost all of its information back only to the frontal cortex. As such, the frontal cortex and basal ganglia can be said to constitute
an integrated neural loop. The frontal cortex and basal ganglia appear to
specialize in different, but related aspects of learning. The frontal cortex
acts when new behaviors have to be learned while the basal ganglia take
over when the behaviors have become learned. It is quite clear that the
obvious struggle behaviors which characterize the disorder are learned,
but it is also quite likely that most of the tensions developed at the vocal
cords which trigger the disorder are not.
GABA (gamma-amino butyric acid), is the most extensive inhibitory neurotransmitter in the brain - and it is mostly concentrated in the basal ganglia.
GABA is also found in the frontal cortex. As indicated earlier, the neurons
in the frontal cortex receive inhibitory commands from the basal ganglia.
These commands, under normal conditions, are then communicated to the
56
muscles of the vocal cords via a set of pathways between the frontal cortex and the vocal cords. The fact that thiamin produces a remission of
symptoms for almost of third of adult males who stutter would suggest that
these individuals are GABA-function deficient. If this is true and if stress
causes a further decrease in GABA function and a commensurate increase in excitatory neurotransmitter function, especially neurotransmitters
called dopamine and glutamate, then the stage is set, not only for a loss of
the normal laryngeal reciprocal inhibitions required for speech, but beyond
that, to a spasm of the vocal cords.
Direct evidence of a vocal cord spasm as the likely core of stuttering has
been provided by a widely known and respected study published in 1978
in the Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. Investigators, using electrodes implanted in the vocal cords of individuals who stutter, found that
just before and during moments of stuttering the muscles that both open
and close the vocal cords during speech were both being contracted
forcefully and simultaneously. In other words, there was clearly a loss of
the reciprocal inhibition at the vocal cords that one would expect during
speech.
57
Laryngeal muscle activity during fluent and stuttered utterances was investigated via electromyography. Analysis revealed that stuttering was accompanied by high levels of
laryngeal muscle activity and disruption of normal reciprocity between abductor and adductor muscle groups.
Results are interpreted as demonstrating the existence of a
laryngeal component in stuttering and showing a strong
correlation between abnormal laryngeal muscle activity and
moments of stuttering.
Notice that the authors had not quite gotten their interpretation of their
findings correct. They viewed the aberrant laryngeal behavior as a laryngeal component in stuttering, not as the core behavior of the disorder.
Others have made this mistake, even to this day, failing to fully grasp the
overwhelming evidence that demonstrates that the obvious supra (above)
and infra (below) - laryngeal struggle behaviors that so often characterize
the disorder are learned.
Investigators have tried giving GABA to people who stutter. But GABA, in
its pure form, apparently does not cross the blood-brain barrier and thus
has little effect. So various pharmaceutical derivatives of it have been developed that cross the barrier and have been tried with people who stutter.
In the main, the results of these studies have been positive, with statistically significant enhancements of fluency demonstrated. But the problem
with most of these pharmaceutical agents is that they often come with unacceptable side effects and none has ever been able to produce the kind
58
If one carefully examines these studies, it becomes obvious that the experimenters, while purportedly looking at a speech problem were, in fact,
unaware that the overt struggle behaviors they were studying were
learned. Instead, had they been aware of the core of the disorder (the laryngeal spasm) and had they known about the behavior of the larynx during speech, they would have fully understood that what they should have
been examining is the laryngeal reciprocal inhibition required for the rapid
alternation between voiced and voiceless consonants in ongoing speech.
There is, however, one study where the investigator was aware of this.
Martin Adams, in an article published in the Journal of Speech and Hearing Research in 1971, made an important contribution to our understanding of stuttering.
In his study, Adams created two distinctly different reading passages. The
first was composed entirely of voiced consonants. The second contained
a combination of both voiced and voiceless consonants. So the first passage did not require any laryngeal reciprocal inhibition because the vibration was continuous throughout. The second, on the other hand, required
the usual reciprocal inhibition that occurs in normal, ongoing speech. Said
somewhat differently, in the first passage, once the vocal cords started to
vibrate, they continued to do so in uninterrupted fashion until the end of
the passage; while in the reading of the second passage, there was the
typical closed/open alternation between voiced and voiceless consonants
- and its associated requirement for reciprocal inhibition.
Adams noted that apart from this difference, the two passages were care-
59
Please note, however, that had his subjects been instructed, prior to reading the passages for recording, to rehearse reading them out loud alone in
an empty room, with no one listening, many of them would have been fluent. For these individuals, without an audience, presumed or real, with its
attendant conditioned stresses, there would be no stuttering.
60
There are direct connections from the basal ganglia to and from a laryngeal area on the motor cortex. There are connections from this laryngeal
area on the motor cortex (via a pathway called the corticobulbar tract) to
the nucleus ambiguus in the medulla. A majority of laryngeal motoneurons
in the nucleus ambiguus are GABA sensitive. The nucleus ambiguus contains the cell bodies of nerves that innervate not only the muscles of the
larynx but also muscles that are activated during the pharyngeal phase of
swallowing.
Since GABA is present in the basal ganglia, on the motor cortex, and there
are GABA-sensitive motor neurons in the nucleus ambiguus, it is tempting
to speculate that this linkage may represent the neural pathway responsible for the laryngeal spasm that generates the trigger for stuttering - especially when there is the presumption of a deficiency of GABA function.
61
Chapter 7
The magnesium connection
I decided to look at this elixir in more detail because since it was was
taken internally it might have systemic implications. I found that drinking
the juice of half a lemon in a glass of water upon arising was a frequently
recommended suggestion for good health. I further discovered that lemon
juice, although obviously containing citric acid, actually produces an alkaline condition in the body. Some health professionals suggest that an alkaline condition of the body (tested from saliva or urine using commercially available test strips) is more resistant to infection and other diseases
than an acidic condition. There have also been anecdotal reports about
62
individuals whose stuttering has been helped by eating an alkaline-producing diet (fruits, vegetables, nuts) as opposed to an acid-producing one
(meat, processed foods, refined sugar).
The second component, molasses, appears to have also been recommended more than once. I thought originally it was just to counter the
tartness of the lemon juice. But sometimes it was recommended by itself.
I discovered that molasses, a product of sugar refining, is essentially a distillate of much of the nutrition in the plant. The roots of sugar cane grow
deep, some 10 - 15 feet down, well below the depleted surface soils and
gather much of the vitamins and minerals found at these subsurface levels. Most of these are in small amounts - with one exception: magnesium.
The amount of magnesium in one tablespoon of unsulphured blackstrap
molasses turns out to be 200 percent greater than the RDA for magnesium for children ages six to eight.
My research further revealed that vitamin C aids the absorption of magnesium and that magnesium, in turn, is required to convert thiamin into several forms, one of which is required for absorption. So the vitamin C in the
lemon juice was facilitating the absorption of the magnesium and the
63
The significance of all of this became clear in 1991 with a study reported
in the Polish Journal of Otolaryngology. Fifty-three children who stuttered
and twenty-two who did not were examined in terms of levels of magnesium in their bloodstream. In 47% of the stuttering children, there was a
deficiency of magnesium; virtually none was found in the non-stuttering
children.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
64
7.
8.
Some foods - like raw nuts and seeds, soybeans, spinach, and
chard - contain compounds called phytic acid and oxalic acid which
cause magnesium to be eliminated from the body.
In view of the above, and the often-quoted statistic that eighty percent of
Americans have diets that are magnesium deficient, we began our investigation into magnesium by recommending its RDA (recommended dietary
allowance). The RDA represents the average daily dietary intake level
sufficient to meet the nutrient requirement of nearly all - 97 to 98% of healthy individuals in a group.
For the typical adult male we decided to use, as a standard recommendation, 400mg of magnesium glycinate per day. 130mg tablets are available
and our recommendation is that one 130mg tablet of magnesium glycinate
accompany one 100mg tablet of thiamin hydrochloride at breakfast, lunch
65
and dinner. It is appropriate to take them at the same time since thiamin
and magnesium are both absorbed largely in the small intestine.
We have been evaluating magnesium clinically for some time now. Our
goal is to ferret out features that might compromise a randomized, doubleblind study. Gaining clinical experience as a precursor to doing formal research has proven to be a productive way of gaining new knowledge.
66
Chapter 8
The Thiamin Protocol
In this chapter, we outline a three-week test that one can take to determine if thiamin can benefit their stuttering. We call the combination of
300mg of thiamin hydrochloride with 400mg of magnesium glycinate taken
daily in a specific way: THE THIAMIN PROTOCOL. The Protocol is for
adult males. Adult females and children require lesser amounts. Also, before taking, remember again to obtain the OK from your health care
provider.
So what follows must be done to create the supportive environment for the
three-week test.
For one week prior to and two weeks during the test:
67
Almonds
Apples
Apricots
Asparagus
Bananas
Broccoli
Carrots
Cauliflower
Cherries
Green beans
Hazelnuts
Honey
Mineral water
Potatoes
Radishes
Spinach
Tomatoes
Watermelon
Zucchini
68
American/cheddar cheese
Brown rice
Butter
Chicken
Cooked salmon
Canned sardines
Cooked turkey bacon
Dry roasted peanuts
Hard boiled egg
Hot dogs
Lean beef
Lentils
Low-fat yogurt
Mozzarella
Oats
Parmesan cheese
Pepperoni
Plain bagel
Walnuts
White bread
White canned tuna fish
Whole Milk
Whole-wheat spaghetti
Wild cooked trout
For a complete listing of both alkaline and acid-forming foods, the reader
is advised to go to the internet and search under the term: alkaline/acid-
69
forming foods. Basically, if one can elect an alkaline-forming diet for the
three-week period, one would have a much better idea of the true impact
The Thiamin Protocol will have on ones stuttering. As an aside, the taking
of a magnesium supplement has been shown to have an alkalizing effect
on the body, something that is desirable for general health reasons. Also
note that the taking of a magnesium supplement is often recommended to
reduce or eliminate muscle spasms.
The goal is not to radically change your diet. The goal is to create the optimum conditions, just prior to and during the test, for The Thiamin Protocol to reduce or eliminate your stuttering. If you experience a positive outcome, you may then slowly add back acid-forming foods to determine the
optimum acid/alkaline balance for you.
Since our main focus is upon thiamin, let us now move on to a bit more
detail about it. There are many types of thiamin; some cross the bloodbrain barrier, others do not. Although thiamin hydrochloride does not
cross the barrier it has a long history of safety and it has been scientifically
tested for efficacy for people who stutter. It may very well be that some of
the other forms of thiamin may turn out to be more effective (and in some
clinical reports this has been suggested) but, in the absence of scientific
data, they cannot be supported. Also, some of these newer forms of thiamin may have the potential of engendering side effects that are less than
desirable.
70
cals used to add bulk to a pill, enhance its bioavailability, permit it to retain
its strength over extended storage periods, etc.) and consistency from
batch to batch are to be considered. One brand of thiamin may therefore
have a different combination of excipients than another and their effects
upon the efficacy of thiamin have yet to be studied.
While there are a number of good brands in the marketplace, the brand
used in our studies was Solgar. The reasons had to do with the reputation
of the company and its products and the products ready availability in
many health food stores, not only across the United States, but throughout
the world.
The reason one is not advised to take all 300mg of thiamin at once is that,
as indicated earlier, too much at any one time will be quickly excreted
through the urine. Furthermore, it has been shown that continuous ingestion of thiamin in a dispersed manner, as indicated earlier, enables blood
levels to rise continuously for between seven and ten days before leveling
off, thereby enabling progressively greater diffusion of the vitamin across
the blood-brain barrier. Interestingly, this agrees with the frequent clinical
71
If, however, after two weeks there has been no noticeable improvement
taking the supplements, the odds are they will not work for you, even at
higher levels. However, this does not necessarily mean that there is no
hope. Instead, one can initiate a new and different approach, an approach
that will be described in the next chapter.
Finally, as usual, do not start the Thiamin Protocol without the express
consent of your physician.
72
Chapter 9
Intent Therapy
In this chapter we propose a solution for those for whom the supplement
approach is ineffective, as well as for those for whom it is effective but
want eventually to be weaned off it. The solution combines the use of a
very simple, but unique, way to get set to speak together with a contemporary version of an electronic device that enhances the speed of acquisition
of new behaviors. We begin with the new way to get set to speak.
Let us start by reiterating some of the stresses that are associated with
the production of a sentence. These stresses occur during the time one
gets set to speak. They are stressful not because they are inherently so,
but because there are so many of them occurring simultaneously and with
such great rapidity.
73
is spoken more rapidly suggesting that the longer the sentence, the
greater the pre-speech tension (because faster movements require more
tension before the movement begins). And the third event of importance,
the pre-phonatory tuning of the larynx - which we noted was comprised of
two types: In one, if speech is to start with the correct pitch and loudness,
the vocal cords must assume specific postures and tensions before
speech. And two, the open or closed position required of the vocal cords
for the first sound in an utterance depending upon whether the first sound
is voiceless or voiced.
These pre-speech sources of tension are present when an individual intends to say a sentence. Many of them are not present when there is no
intent to say a sentence. This is an extremely important point. It bears
repeating: without the intent to say a sentence many of the pre-speech
sources of tension cannot be there.
The goal is to reduce the pre-speech tensions to a level below the trigger
threshold for stuttering by altering how much a person intends to say before they speak. The technique that accomplishes this is called: Intent
Therapy.
But first, some additional observations. If a person says just one word, it
is because they have intended to say just one word. In that circumstance,
many of the stresses associated with saying sentences are not present.
So while vocabulary stress remains, grammar, syntax, intonation and linguistic stress do not. And while pre-phonatory tuning of the larynx remains, co-articulation between words and much of speed stress do not.
74
What you are going to learn is a sport that has four rules.You are going to
take pride in how well, how smoothly, how slickly, how undetectably you
follow these rules as you play your sport. And, as a byproduct of playing
your sport correctly, you will not stutter.
We start with sentences that begin with one-syllable words. We are going
to slow the first word of each sentence. But this will not be ordinary slowing. It is going to be very different. Here are the four rules:
1.
The first rule is that when you say a sentence, you are to put a mental comma, a pause, between that first word and the rest of the sentence. You don't have to slow the first word; all you have to do is put
the comma in and your brain will do it for you automatically.
2.
Here's the second rule. You are to say the first word as if it were the
only word you were going to say, with the part that comes after the
comma, the rest of the sentence, spoken as an afterthought.
3.
Heres the third rule. There shall be no influence of the second word
on the ending of the first word. The comma is a real wall separating
the first word from the rest of the sentence.
75
4.
Rule number four. Never inhale during the comma because if you
do, your brain will think you have begun a new sentence without
technique and then you may stutter.
If you closely examine these four rules, you will notice that the first three
say the same thing - but in different ways. What has to be learned is that
you must intend to say just one word. Once you have successfully accomplished this, then and only then can you shift your intent and say the
rest of the sentence. Do this correctly and you have stripped most of the
pre-speech sources of tension from the vocal cords that normally occur
before sentences - while still retaining the sentence form.
If you have trouble doing this, take 50 file cards (3x5) and write a single
one-syllable word on each. Shuffle them, turn them face down. and read
them one at a time, adding a different made up rest of the sentence to
each - as an afterthought. With no foreknowledge of the word to come,
the temptation to create the sentence in advance is reduced.
Practice this several times a day, for weeks at a time, until it becomes
second nature to you. What you are practicing is not reading single words
and then adding made-up sentences. What you are practicing is shifting
from saying a word with the intent to only say that word to saying the rest
76
If you ask 100 people who stutter to think of a difficult word, a word they
have trouble saying, with the exception of their name, more often than not
the word will be multi-syllabic. As a matter of fact, at least nine times out
of ten it will be. The question is: why are multi-syllable words reported as
difficult nine times more often than mono-syllable words? What is so special about them? Here is the answer:
It would appear that the basic unit of speech, in so far as the brain is concerned, is the one-syllable word - that we were originally meant to speak
only in monosyllables. But when one does it sounds unnatural. For example, read the following four sentences out loud and see for yourself: I
would like to tell you how to buy a car. If you go to a street near my house
you will find a man there who sells used cars. He has been there for a
long time. You can trust him. Most unnatural, wouldnt you say? It has
almost a metronomic quality to it.
Interestingly, when you ask people who stutter to speak in synchrony with
a metronome they rarely stutter.
77
spoken. You may have missed it. Read it out loud again. The speeding
of the syllables in Mississippi occurs completely automatically so there is
no reason to pay any attention to it.
As indicated, most people are totally unaware of doing this. It is completely automatic. But acoustic phoneticians, who have measured speech durations, have long known that the speed of the syllables in a multi-syllable
word in a sentence is always faster that the speed of a one-syllable word
in a sentence. As a matter of fact, even when a person is asked to say a
multi-syllable word by itself, a similar, but lesser degree, of speeding occurs.
Remember the arm extension example in Chapter 5? Faster arm extensions require more tension prior to the movement than slower arm extensions. Equally, faster speech requires more tension before speaking than
slower speech. So it is fair to say that both in sentences and out of sentences, syllables in multi-syllable words, since they are almost always
spoken more rapidly than monosyllable words are almost always spoken
with more tension. And since it is tension that triggers the threshold for
stuttering, it is far more likely one will hit ones threshold on a long word
78
than a short one. And thats why long words are so much more often reported as the difficult ones.
79
After several weeks doing this, making certain your rate does not increase
beyond 108 syllables per minute and you do not sound like a robot, go
back to two-syllable words, each on a separate 3 x 5 file card, shuffle the
cards, and start reading them spontaneously adding words to each twosyllable word to create a new sentence for each. Remember, this is Intent
Therapy, and the first word, regardless of its length, is to be spoken as if it
were the only word you were going to say, with the rest of the sentence
spoken at normal rates but as if it were an afterthought. Thinking of the
sentence as a whole before you say it adds back all of the pre-speech
tensions and this, more than anything else, leads to stuttering.
80
cation. You might mark such locations on the page to prompt reapplications. Some of these mid-sentence reapplications should be before monosyllable words; others should be before multi-syllable words.
There are three reasons a person will stutter in the middle of a sentence.
Either they have taken a breath in mid sentence, thus effectively making
two sentences and are not aware of it; or the stuttered word is multi-syllabic producing a sudden increase in muscle tension; or it is a feared word
producing its own increased muscle tensions. In the latter instance, the
feared word is also likely to be multi-syllabic - at least 90% of the time it is
likely to be. One would be well advised to practice saying feared words at
a rate of 108 beats (syllables) per minute. Make a list of them and practice
Intent Therapy for each. Practice hundreds of repetitions at a rate of 108
syllables per minute for each word. Do this for weeks. The response
has to be so automatic it can withstand the distracting effect of stress.
When you are certain you can can consistently produce perfect examples
of intent therapy when alone, begin trying to use it in low-stress situations.
If you succeed in low-stress situations, move slowly to situations of progressively higher stress. This is a process called systematic desensitization. As you progress, if you move slowly and experience success you will
build your confidence.
We fully recognize, however, that life and its stresses are not nicely
arranged in a neat hierarchy and you will often be in high-stress situations
sooner than you would like. In those circumstances do not be disappointed if you do not succeed. Just continue with your daily practice and your
persistence will eventually pay off. Remember, Intent Therapy, like all
sports, needs a lot of practice.
81
In my experience, if you separate your practice into the steps and sequences just outlined, you should, within a few months, be able to handle
stressful situations with perfect examples of Intent Therapy.
To facilitate automatic use of technique in conversation, a private reminding device called the MotivAider (www.motiv-aider.com) is used. The device is worn on a belt or in a pocket. Periodically, at pre-set intervals, it
emits silent, pulsing signals (inaudible vibrations) that have been programmed in advance.
After going through the Protocol, whenever the pulsing signal is felt, the
word INTENT is automatically remembered, followed by the desire to use
Intent Therapy on the next sentence. The MotivAider is generally set to go
off once every fifteen minutes. So four times an hour, throughout the day
the wearer is privately reminded to use technique.
82
One final word about Intent Therapy. A slowed first word is a byproduct of
good technique. The rest of the sentence, the afterthought, is always to
be spoken at a normal rate. But what is a normal rate? Or, said somewhat differently, what is the average duration of a syllable in conversation?
It turns out that average depends on a number of factors, the most important of which is whether the syllable is in a stressed or unstressed word.
The average duration of a syllable in an unstressed word is about 125 milliseconds - in a stressed word, about 300 milliseconds. This means that
the average rate of an unstressed syllable is about 8 syllables per second,
and the average rate of a stressed syllable is about 3.3 syllables per second.
Apparently interesting things happen in the brain when we speak at different rates. In a study published in 2001, researchers, using functional
magnetic resonance imaging to study the extent and magnitude of blood
flows in the brain, had speakers repeat a syllable at three different rates:
2.5 syllables per second, 4 syllables per second and 5.5 syllables per
second.
They found that different rates of speech activated different parts of the
brain. In the cerebellum, there was a direct correlation between the syllable rates of 4.0 and 5.5 per second and the spatial extent and magnitude
of the blood flows. The cerebellum seemed unresponsive to the slowest
83
rate. But in the basal ganglia, the area of particular interest in stuttering,
there were proportional spatial extents and blood flows for the 2.5 and 4.0
rates, but not for the 5.5 rate. In other words, the basal ganglia were not
responding to faster rates, rates even remotely approaching the rates for
unstressed syllables in ongoing speech. And remember, first words of
sentences are typically spoken at unstressed rates (a lot of stuttering occurs at the beginning of sentences) as are the syllables of multi-syllable
words spoken anywhere in a sentence (most feared words are multisyllables).
84
Chapter 10
The sequence
We have several tools that can be used to deal with stuttering and there is
a preferred sequence to follow when using them. But first, before beginning, remember to consult with your physician to get clearance. Also,
should any undesirable side effects occur, cease taking all supplements
immediately and contact your physician.
With that in mind, begin by taking The Thiamin Protocol Test. Remember,
the test includes a week of dietary preparation followed by two weeks of
supplement ingestion while continuing to maintain the dietary restrictions.
Be sure to take the specific compounds and amounts of thiamin and magnesium recommended.
If you are among the 38% or so whose stuttering goes into remission it will
be clear and unmistakable.
If you do not observe clear-cut evidence of enhanced fluency during the
three-week test period, stop taking the supplements. Also, terminate the
diet restrictions and consider moving instead to Intent Therapy.
85
Afterword
The Thiamin Protocol does not work for everyone. The brain is an exquisitely complex structure requiring a number of approaches to the solution of
issues. In this report we have presented what we consider to be a simple
and effective solution for slightly more than a third of the worlds young
adult males who stutter.
But much more needs to be done. The goal must be to achieve higher
remission rates. The original remission rates using thiamin alone were on
the order of 30 percent, with magnesium added it appears to be on the order of about 38 percent. But more research is required to further clarify
the optimum thiamin/magnesium ratios for fluency.
In addition, there are complex interactions between the derivatives of thiamin and magnesium, derivatives that cross the blood-brain barrier, that
need to be explored. There are other minerals, such as calcium, other vitamins, such as B6 and herbs, such as valerian, that require examination.
Even an optimal diet for fluency is probably an area worthy of investigation.
In addition, apart from The Thiamin Protocol, we have also used this report as the vehicle for introducing Intent Therapy. We view Intent Therapy
as a viable alternative for those for whom The Thiamin Protocol is ineffective or for those who wish, eventually, to wean themselves off The Protocol. Intent Therapy is a powerful tool for subtracting pre-speech laryngeal
tensions, an area that has heretofore not been fully explored.
86
Some may say that the conclusions drawn in this report regarding the efficacy of The Thiamin Protocol have not been adequately supported by scientific research. I would tend to agree. Obviously much more research
needs to be done. But the results of these few studies when combined
with the extensive clinical experience gained from a variety of sources
tends to lend credence to the conclusions offered.
The Thiamin Protocol is, hopefully, the first of a long line of productive outcomes to emanate from an ongoing series of observations that represent
a new genre of inquiry. We hope eventually to be able to expand upon the
findings described in this report, looking continuously to improve upon
them as we learn more about the core of stuttering, its nervous-system
underpinnings and how best to study and treat them.
87
Appendix
Reference List For Chapter 5
1.Van Riper, C. The Nature of Stuttering - 2nd ed. Waveland Press, Prospect
Heights, IL, 1992.
2.Williams, D.E., & Kent, L.R. Listeners evaluations of speech interruptions. J.
Speech and Hearing Res., 1958, 1, 124-136.
3.Bosshardt, H.G. Cognitive processing load as a determinant of stuttering:
summary of a research programme. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 2006, 20,
371-385.
4.Ward, D. Stuttering and Cluttering:Frameworks for Understanding and Treatment. Psychology Press, New York, NY, 2006.
5.Ham, R. Therapy of Stuttering. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, 1990.
6.Van Riper, C. The Nature of Stuttering - 2nd ed. Waveland Press, Prospect
Heights, IL, 1992.
7.Ingham, R.J., Stuttering and Behavior Therapy: Current Status and Experimental Foundations, College Hill Press, San Diego, 1984.
8.Flanagan, B., Goldiamond, I. & Azrin, N., Operant stuttering: the control of stuttering behavior through response-contingent consequences. J. Exper. Anal. Behav., 1958, 1,173-177
9.Onslow, M., Andrews, C., & Lincoln, M., A control experimental trial of an operant treatment for early stuttering J. Speech and Hearing Res., 1994, 37,
1244-1259.
10.Bloodstein, O. Conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent: a review of the literature, J. Speech Hearing Dis., 1949, 14, 295-302.
11.Ratner, N. Stuttering: A Psycholinguistic Perspective. In: Curlee, R. & Siegel,
G., (Eds.) Nature and Treatment of Stuttering: New Directions, Allyn & Bacon,
Boston, 1997.
88
12.Schwartz, M. F., Stutter No More, Simon & Schuster, New York, 1991.
13.Webster, L. M., A clinical note on psychotherapy for stuttering, J. of Fluency
Dis., 1977, 2, 253-255.
14.Bobrick, B., Tangled Tongues, Simon & Schuster, New York, 1995.
15.Yairi, E. And Ambrose, N. Early Childhood Stuttering For Clinicians by Clinicians, PRO-ED, Austin, 2004.
16.Johnson, W., A study of the onset and development of stuttering. In: Stuttering
and Adults, Univ. of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, 1955.
17.Van Riper, C. The Nature of Stuttering - 2nd ed. Waveland Press, Prospect
Heights, IL, 1992.
18.Bloodstein, O., The development of stuttering: II development phases, 1960,
25, J. Speech and Hear. Dis., 366-76.
19.Froeschels, E., The significance of symptomatology for the understanding of
the essence of stuttering, 1952, 4, Folia Phoniatrica, 217-230.
20.Voelker, C. H., A preliminary investigation for a normative study of fluency: a
clinical index to the severity of stuttering, Amer. J. Of Orthopsychiat., 1944, 14,
285-94.
21.Fraser, M., Self Therapy For The Stutterer, Stuttering Foundation of America,
Memphis, 2007.
22.Van Riper, C. The Nature of Stuttering - 2nd ed. Waveland Press, Prospect
Heights, IL, 1992.
23.Ramig, P. R, & Dodge, D. M., The Child and Adolescent Stutterer: Treatment
and Activity Resource Guide, Delmar Language Learning, Independence, KY,
2009.
24.Zackheim, C. T., & Conture, E., Childhood stuttering and speech disfluencies
in relation to childrens mean length of utterance: a preliminary study, 2003. 28, J.
Fluency Dis., 28, 115-142.
89
90
38.Scripture, E., Stuttering and Lisping, 2nd ed., Macmillan, New York, 1923.
39.Ramig, P. & Adams, M., Rate reduction strategies used by stutterers and nonstutterers during high-and low-pitched speech, 1980, 5, J. of fluency Dis., 27-41.
40.Perkins, W. et al., Discoordination of phonation with articulation and respiration, In: Stuttering Then and Now, Shames, G. and Rubin, H. Eds., Charles E.
Merrill, Columbus, 1986.
41.Bloodstein, O., A Handbook On Stuttering (5th ed.), Singular Publishing
Group, San Diego, 1995.
42.De Nil, L. & Brutten, G., Voice onset time of stuttering and nonstuttering children: the influence of externally and linguistically imposed time pressure, J. of
Fluency Dis., 1991, 16, 154-158.
43.Guitar, B., Stuttering: An Integrated Approach to its Nature and Treatment, 3rd
ed., Lippincott Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia, 2005.
44.Alm, P., Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits; a critical review of possible
relations, J. of Communic. Dis., 2004, 4, 325-369.
45.Giraud, A. et al., Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia in persistent developmental stuttering, 104, Brain and Language, 2008, 104, 190-199.
46.Herrero, M. T, Barcia, C., & Navarro, J. M., Functional anatomy of thalamus
and basal ganglia, Childs Nervous System, 2002, 18, 386-404.
47.Brown, S., Ngan, E. & Liotti, M., A larynx area in the human motor cortex,
Cerebral Cortex, 2008, 18, 837-845.
91
92