Zuur Protocol 2010

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Methods in Ecology and Evolution 2010, 1, 314

doi: 10.1111/j.2041-210X.2009.00001.x

A protocol for data exploration to avoid common


statistical problems
Alain F. Zuur*1,2, Elena N. Ieno1,2 and Chris S. Elphick3
1

Highland Statistics Ltd, Newburgh, UK; 2Oceanlab, University of Aberdeen, Newburgh, UK; and 3Department of
Ecology and Evolutionary Biology and Center for Conservation Biology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA

Summary
1. While teaching statistics to ecologists, the lead authors of this paper have noticed common statistical problems. If a random sample of their work (including scientic papers) produced before doing
these courses were selected, half would probably contain violations of the underlying assumptions
of the statistical techniques employed.
2. Some violations have little impact on the results or ecological conclusions; yet others increase
type I or type II errors, potentially resulting in wrong ecological conclusions. Most of these violations can be avoided by applying better data exploration. These problems are especially troublesome in applied ecology, where management and policy decisions are often at stake.
3. Here, we provide a protocol for data exploration; discuss current tools to detect outliers, heterogeneity of variance, collinearity, dependence of observations, problems with interactions, double
zeros in multivariate analysis, zero ination in generalized linear modelling, and the correct type of
relationships between dependent and independent variables; and provide advice on how to address
these problems when they arise. We also address misconceptions about normality, and provide
advice on data transformations.
4. Data exploration avoids type I and type II errors, among other problems, thereby reducing the
chance of making wrong ecological conclusions and poor recommendations. It is therefore essential
for good quality management and policy based on statistical analyses.
Key-words: collinearity, data exploration, independence, transformations, type I and II
errors, zero ination
Introduction
The last three decades have seen an enormous expansion of the
statistical tools available to applied ecologists. A short list
of available techniques includes linear regression, generalized
linear (mixed) modelling, generalized additive (mixed) modelling, regression and classication trees, survival analysis, neural networks, multivariate analysis with all its many methods
such as principal component analysis (PCA), canonical correspondence analysis (CCA), (non-)metric multidimensional
scaling (NMDS), various time series and spatial techniques,
etc. Although some of these techniques have been around for
some time, the development of fast computers and freely available software such as R (R Development Core Team 2009)
makes it possible to routinely apply sophisticated statistical
techniques on any type of data. This paper is not about these
methods. Instead, it is about the vital step that should, but
frequently does not, precede their application.

*Correspondence author. E-mail: [email protected]


Correspondence site: http://www.respond2articles.com/MEE/

All statistical techniques have in common the problem of


rubbish in, rubbish out. In some methods, for example, a single outlier may determine the nal results and conclusions.
Heterogeneity (dierences in variation) may cause serious
trouble in linear regression and analysis of variance models
(Fox 2008), and with certain multivariate methods (Huberty
1994).
When the underlying question is to determine which covariates are driving a system, then the most dicult aspect of the
analysis is probably how to deal with collinearity (correlation
between covariates), which increases type II errors (i.e. failure
to reject the null hypothesis when it is untrue). In multivariate
analysis applied to data on ecological communities, the presence of double zeros (e.g. two species being jointly absent at
various sites) contributes towards similarity in some techniques
(e.g. PCA), but not others. Yet other multivariate techniques
are sensitive to species with clumped distributions and low
abundance (e.g. CCA). In univariate analysis techniques like
generalized linear modelling (GLM) for count data, zero
ination of the response variable may cause biased parameter
estimates (Cameron & Trivedi 1998). When multivariate techniques use permutation methods to obtain P-values, for exam-

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society

4 A. F. Zuur et al.
ple in CCA and redundancy analysis (RDA, ter Braak & Verdonschot 1995), or the Mantel test (Legendre & Legendre
1998), temporal or spatial correlation between observations
can increase type I errors (rejecting the null hypothesis when it
is true).
The same holds with regression-type techniques applied on
temporally or spatially correlated observations. One of the
most used, and misused, techniques is without doubt linear
regression. Often, this technique is associated with linear patterns and normality; both concepts are often misunderstood.
Linear regression is more than capable of tting nonlinear relationships, e.g. by using interactions or quadratic terms (Montgomery & Peck 1992). The term linear in linear regression
refers to the way parameters are used in the model and not to
the type of relationships that are modelled. Knowing whether
we have linear or nonlinear patterns between response and
explanatory variables is crucial for how we apply linear regression and related techniques. We also need to know whether the
data are balanced before including interactions. For example,
Zuur, Ieno & Smith (2007) used the covariates sex, location
and month to model the gonadosomatic index (the weight of
the gonads relative to total body weight) of squid. However,
both sexes were not measured at every location in each month
due to unbalanced sampling. In fact, the data were so unbalanced that it made more sense to analyse only a subset of the
data, and refrain from including certain interactions.
With this wealth of potential pitfalls, ensuring that the scientist does not discover a false covariate eect (type I error),
wrongly dismiss a model with a particular covariate (type II
error) or produce results determined by only a few inuential
observations, requires that detailed data exploration be applied
before any statistical analysis. The aim of this paper is to provide a protocol for data exploration that identies potential
problems (Fig. 1). In our experience, data exploration can take
up to 50% of the time spent on analysis.
Although data exploration is an important part of any analysis, it is important that it be clearly separated from hypothesis
testing. Decisions about what models to test should be made
a priori based on the researchers biological understanding of
the system (Burnham & Anderson 2002). When that understanding is very limited, data exploration can be used as a
hypothesis-generating exercise, but this is fundamentally different from the process that we advocate in this paper. Using
aspects of a data exploration to search out patterns (data
dredging) can provide guidance for future work, but the
results should be viewed very cautiously and inferences about
the broader population avoided. Instead, new data should be
collected based on the hypotheses generated and independent
tests conducted. When data exploration is used in this manner,
both the process used and the limitations of any inferences
should be clearly stated.
Throughout the paper we focus on the use of graphical tools
(Chateld 1998; Gelman, Pasarica & Dodhia 2002), but in
some cases it is also possible to apply tests for normality or
homogeneity. The statistical literature, however, warns against
certain tests and advocates graphical tools (Montgomery &
Peck 1992; Draper & Smith 1998, Quinn & Keough 2002).

Fig. 1. Protocol for data exploration.

Laara (2009) gives seven reasons for not applying preliminary


tests for normality, including: most statistical techniques based
on normality are robust against violation; for larger data sets
the central limit theory implies approximate normality; for
small samples the power of the tests is low; and for larger data
sets the tests are sensitive to small deviations (contradicting the
central limit theory).
All graphs were produced using the software package R
(R Development Core Team 2008). All R code and data used
in this paper are available in Appendix S1 (Supporting Information) and from http://www.highstat.com.

Step 1: Are there outliers in Y and X?


In some statistical techniques the results are dominated by outliers; other techniques treat them like any other value. For
example, outliers may cause overdispersion in a Poisson GLM
or binomial GLM when the outcome is not binary (Hilbe
2007). In contrast, in NMDS using the Jaccard index (Legendre & Legendre 1998), observations are essentially viewed as
presences and absences, hence an outlier does not inuence the
outcome of the analysis in any special way. Consequently, it is
important that the researcher understands how a particular
technique responds to the presence of outliers. For the
moment, we dene an outlier as an observation that has a
relatively large or small value compared to the majority of
observations.
A graphical tool that is typically used for outlier detection is
the boxplot. It visualizes the median and the spread of the data.
Depending on the software used, the median is typically presented as a horizontal line with the 25% and 75% quartiles
forming a box around the median that contains half of the
observations. Lines are then drawn from the boxes, and any

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Data exploration 5
points beyond these lines are labelled as outliers. Some
researchers routinely (but wrongly) remove these observations.
Figure 2a shows an example of such a graph using 1295
observations of a morphometric variable (wing length of the
saltmarsh sparrow Ammodramus caudacutus; Gjerdrum, Elphick & Rubega 2008). The graph leads one to believe (perhaps
wrongly, as we will see in a moment) that there are seven
outliers.
Another very useful, but highly neglected, graphical tool to
visualize outliers is the Cleveland dotplot (Cleveland 1993).
This is a graph in which the row number of an observation is
plotted vs. the observation value, thereby providing much
more detailed information than a boxplot. Points that stick out
on the right-hand side, or on the left-hand side, are observed
values that are considerable larger, or smaller, than the majority of the observations, and require further investigation. If
such observations exist, it is important to check the raw data
for errors and assess whether the observed values are reasonable. Figure 2b shows a Cleveland dotplot for the sparrow
wing length data; note that the observations identied by the
boxplot are not especially extreme after all. The upward trend
in Fig. 2b simply arises because the data in the spreadsheet
were sorted by weight. There is one observation of a wing
length of about 68 mm that stands out to the left about half
way up the graph. This value is not considerably larger than
the other values, so we cannot say yet that it is an outlier.
Figure 3 shows a multi-panel Cleveland dotplot for all of
the morphometric variables measured; note that some variables have a few relatively large values. Such extreme values
could indicate true measurement errors (e.g. some t the characteristics of observer distraction sensu Morgan 2004,
whereby the observers eye is drawn to the wrong number on a
measurement scale). Note that one should not try to argue that
such large values could have occurred by chance. If they were,
then intermediate values should also have been generated by

(b)

Order of the data

60
55

Wing length (mm)

65

(a)

chance, but none were. (A useful exercise is to generate, repeatedly, an equivalent number of random observations from an
appropriate distribution, e.g. the Normal distribution, and
determine how the number of extreme points compares to the
empirical data.) When the most likely explanation is that the
extreme observations are measurement (observer) errors, they
should be dropped because their presence is likely to dominate
the analysis. For example, we applied a discriminant analysis
on the full sparrow data set to see whether observations differed among observers, and found that the rst two axes were
mainly determined by the outliers.
So far, we have loosely dened an outlier as an observation
that sticks out from the rest. A more rigorous approach is to
consider whether unusual observations exert undue inuence
on an analysis (e.g. on estimated parameters). We make a distinction between inuential observations in the response variable and in the covariates. An example of the latter is when
species abundances are modelled as a function of temperature,
with nearly all temperature values between 15 and 20 C, but
one of 25 C. In general, this is not an ideal sampling design
because the range 2025 C is inadequately sampled. In a eld
study, however, there may have been only one opportunity to
sample the higher temperature. With a large sample size, such
observations may be dropped, but with relative small data sets
the consequent reduction in sample size may be undesirable,
especially if other observations have outliers for other explanatory variables. If omitting such observations is not an option,
then consider transforming the explanatory variables.
In regression-type techniques, outliers in the response variables are more complicated to deal with. Transforming the
data is an option, but as the response variable is of primary
interest, it is better to choose a statistical method that uses a
probability distribution that allows greater variation for large
mean values (e.g. gamma for continuous data; Poisson or negative binomial for count data) because doing this allows us to

55

60
65
Wing length (mm)

Fig. 2. (a) Boxplot of wing length for 1295 saltmarsh sparrows. The line in the middle of the box represents the median, and the lower and upper
ends of the box are the 25% and 75% quartiles respectively. The lines indicate 1.5 times the size of the hinge, which is the 75% minus 25% quartiles. (Note that the interval dened by these lines is not a condence interval.) Points beyond these lines are (often wrongly) considered to be outliers. In some cases it may be helpful to rotate the boxplot 90 to match the Cleveland dotplot. (b) Cleveland dotplot of the same data. The
horizontal axis represents the value of wing length, and the vertical axis corresponds to the order of the data, as imported from the data le (in this
case sorted by the birds weight).
 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Order of the data from text file

6 A. F. Zuur et al.
Culmen length

Nalospi to bill tip

Weight

Wing length

Tarsus length

Head length

Value of the variable

work with the original data. For multivariate analyses, this


approach is not an option because these methods are not based
on probability distributions. Instead, we can use a dierent
measure of association. For example, the Euclidean distance is
rather sensitive to large values because it is based on Pythagoras theorem, whereas the Chord distance down-weights large
values (Legendre & Legendre 1998).
Some statistical packages come with a whole series of diagnostic tools to identify inuential observations. For example,
the Cook statistic in linear regression (Fox 2008) gives information on the change in regression parameters as each observation is sequentially, and individually, omitted. The problem
with such tools is that when there are multiple outliers with
similar values, they will not be detected. Hence, one should
investigate the presence of such observations using the graphical tools discussed in this paper, before applying a statistical
analysis.
Ultimately, it is up to the ecologist to decide what to
do with outliers. Outliers in a covariate may arise due to
poor experimental design, in which case dropping the
observation or transforming the covariate are sensible
options. Observer and measurement errors are a valid justication for dropping observations. But outliers in the
response variable may require a more rened approach,
especially when they represent genuine variation in the variable being measured. Taking detailed eld or experiment
notes can be especially helpful for documenting when unusual events occur, and thus providing objective information
with which to re-examine outliers. Regardless of how the
issue is addressed, it is important to know whether there
are outliers and to report how they were handled; data
exploration allows this to be done.

Step 2: Do we have homogeneity of variance?


Homogeneity of variance is an important assumption in analysis of variance (ANOVA), other regression-related models and
in multivariate techniques like discriminant analysis. Figure 4
shows conditional boxplots of the food intake rates of Hudso-

Fig. 3. Multi-panel Cleveland dotplot for six


morphometric variables taken from the sparrow data, after sorting the observations from
heaviest to lightest (hence the shape of the
weight graph). Axis labels were suppressed to
improve visual presentation. Note that some
variables have a few unusually small or large
values. Observations also can be plotted, or
mean values superimposed, by subgroup (e.g.
observer or sex) to see whether there are differences among subsets of the data.

nian godwits (Limosa haemastica), a long-distance migrant


shorebird, on a mudat in Argentina (E. Ieno, unpublished
data). To apply an ANOVA on these data to test whether
mean intake rates dier by sex, time period or a combination
of these two variables (i.e. an interaction), we have to assume
that (i) variation in the observations from the sexes is similar;
(ii) variation in observations from the three time periods is similar; and (iii) variation between the three time periods within
the sexes is similar. In this case, there seems to be slightly less
variation in the winter data for males and more variation in the
male data from the summer. However, such small dierences
in variation are not something to worry about. More serious
examples of violation can be found in Zuur et al. (2009a). Fox
(2008) shows that for a simplistic linear regression model heterogeneity seriously degrades the least-square estimators when
the ratio between the largest and smallest variance is 4 (conservative) or more.
In regression-type models, verication of homogeneity
should be done using the residuals of the model; i.e. by plotting
residuals vs. tted values, and making a similar set of conditional boxplots for the residuals. In all these graphs the residual
variation should be similar. The solution to heterogeneity of
variance is either a transformation of the response variable to
stabilize the variance, or applying statistical techniques that
do not require homogeneity (e.g. generalized least squares;
Pinheiro & Bates 2000; Zuur et al. 2009a).

Step 3: Are the data normally distributed?


Various statistical techniques assume normality, and this has
led many of our postgraduate course participants to produce
histogram after histogram of their data (e.g. Fig. 5a). It is
important, however, to know whether the statistical technique
to be used does assume normality, and what exactly is assumed
to be normally distributed? For example, a PCA does not
require normality (Jollie 2002). Linear regression does
assume normality, but is reasonably robust against violation
of the assumption (Fitzmaurice, Laird & Ware 2004). If you
want to apply a statistical test to determine whether there is sig-

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Data exploration 7
Female

Male

10

Intake rate

08

Fig. 4. Multi-panel conditional boxplots for


the godwit foraging data. The three boxplots
in each panel correspond to three time periods. We are interested in whether the mean
values change between sexes and time periods, but need to assume that variation is similar in each group.

06

04

02

00
Summer

Pre-migration

Winter

Pre-migration

Winter

Migration period

as dierences among months may be made smaller, and more


dicult to detect.

Step 4: Are there lots of zeros in the data?

(b)

100
80
60
40
20
0

0
14

16

18

20
22
Weight (g)

24

26

28

100
80
60
40
20
0

July

Frequency

Frequency
50
100

150

(a)

August

Elphick & Oring (1998, 2003) investigated the eects of straw


management on waterbird abundance in ooded rice elds.
One possible statistical analysis is to model the number of birds
as a function of time, water depth, farm, eld management
method, temperature, etc. Because this analysis involves modelling a count, GLM is the appropriate analysis. Figure 7
shows a frequency plot illustrating how often each value for
total waterbird abundance occurred. The extremely high number of zeros tells us that we should not apply an ordinary Poisson or negative binomial GLM as these would produce biased
parameter estimates and standard errors. Instead one should
consider zero inated GLMs (Cameron & Trivedi 1998; Zuur
et al. 2009a).
One can also analyse data for multiple species simultaneously using multivariate techniques. For such analyses, we
need to consider what it means when two species are jointly
absent. This result could say something important about the
ecological characteristics of a site, for example that it contains
conditions that are unfavourable to both species. By extension,

100
80
60
40
20
0

June

nicant group separation in a discriminant analysis, however,


normality of observations of a particular variable within each
group is important (Huberty 1994). Simple t-tests also assume
that the observations in each group are normally distributed;
hence histograms for the raw data of every group should be
examined.
In linear regression, we actually assume normality of all the
replicate observations at a particular covariate value (Fig. 6;
Montgomery & Peck 1992), an assumption that cannot be veried unless one has many replicates at each sampled covariate
value. However, normality of the raw data implies normality
of the residuals. Therefore, we can make histograms of residuals to get some impression of normality (Quinn & Keough
2002; Zuur et al. 2007), even though we cannot fully test the
assumption.
Even when the normality assumption is apparently violated,
the situation may be more complicated than it seems. The
shape of the histogram in Fig. 5a, for example, indicates skewness, which may suggest to one that data transformation is
needed. Figure 5b shows a multi-panel histogram for the same
variable except that the data are plotted by month; this lets us
see that the skewness of the original histogram is probably
caused by sparrow weight changes over time. Under these
circumstances, it would not be advisable to transform the data

Fig. 5. (a) Histogram of the weight of 1193


sparrows (only the June, July and August
data were used). Note that the distribution is
skewed. (b) Histograms for the weight of the
sparrows, broken down by month. Note that
the centre of the distribution is shifting, and
this is causing the skewed distributed for the
aggregated data shown in (a).

Summer

15

20

Weight (g)

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

25

8 A. F. Zuur et al.

Covariate
Fig. 6. Visualization of two underlying assumptions in linear regression: normality and homogeneity. The dots represent observed values
and a regression line is added. At each covariate value, we assume
that observations are normally distributed with the same spread
(homogeneity). Normality and homogeneity at each covariate value
cannot be veried unless many (>25) replicates per covariate value
are taken, which is seldom the case in ecological studies. In practice, a
histogram of pooled residuals should be made, but this does not provide conclusive evidence for normality. The same limitations holds if
residuals are plotted vs. tted values to verify homogeneity.

06

07

08

09

10

RBGU
LESA
DUNL
SNIP
LBDO
GRYE
LBCU
KILL
GREG
SNEG
GBHE
AMBI
COOT
UNDU
NOSH
NOPI
AMWI
GWTE
GADW
MALL
MALL
GADW
GWTE
AMWI
NOPI
NOSH
UNDU
COOT
AMBI
GBHE
SNEG
GREG
KILL
LBCU
GRYE
LBDO
SNIP
DUNL
LESA
RBGU

Response variable

05

Frequency
100 200 300 400 500 600 700

Fig. 8. A corrgram showing the frequency with which pairs of waterbird species both have zero abundance. The colour and the amount
that a circle has been lled correspond to the proportion of observations with double zeros. The diagonal running from bottom left to
top right represents the percentage of observations of a variable equal
to zero. Four-letter acronyms represent dierent waterbird species.
The top bar relates the colours in the graph to the proportion of
zeros.

0 4 8 13 19 25 31 37 43 49 55 61 67 73 79 85 91 97
Observed values

Fig. 7. Frequency plot showing the number of observations with a


certain number of waterbirds for the rice eld data; 718 of 2035 observations equal zero. Plotting data for individual species would result in
even higher frequencies of zeros.

when two sites both have the same joint absences, this might
mean that the sites are ecologically similar. On the other hand,
if a species has a highly clumped distribution, or is simply rare,
then joint absences might arise through chance and say
nothing about the suitability of a given site for a species, the
similarity among the habitat needs of species or the ecological
similarity of sites. A high frequency of zeros, thus, can greatly
complicate interpretation of such analyses. Irrespective of our
attitude to joint absences, we need to know whether there are
double zeros in the data. This means that for each species-pair,
we need to calculate how often both had zero abundance for
the same observation (e.g. site). We can either present this
information in a table, or use advanced graphical tools like a
corrgram (Fig. 8; Sarkar 2008). In our waterbird example, the
frequency of double zeros is very high. All the blue circles cor-

respond to species that have more than 80% of their observations jointly zero. This result is consistent with the biology of
the species studied, most of which form large ocks and have
highly clumped distributions. A PCA would label such species
as similar, although their ecological use of habitats is often
quite dierent (e.g. Elphick & Oring 1998). Alternative multivariate analyses that ignore double zeros are discussed in
Legendre & Legendre (1998) and Zuur et al. (2007).

Step 5: Is there collinearity among the


covariates?
If the underlying question in a study is which covariates are
driving the response variable(s), then the biggest problem to
overcome is often collinearity. Collinearity is the existence of
correlation between covariates. Common examples are covariates like weight and length, or water depth and distance to the
shoreline. If collinearity is ignored, one is likely to end up with
a confusing statistical analysis in which nothing is signicant,
but where dropping one covariate can make the others signicant, or even change the sign of estimated parameters. The
eect of collinearity is illustrated in the context of multiple
linear regression, but similar problems exist in analysis of
variance, mixed eects models, RDA, CCA, GLMs or GAMs.
Table 1 gives the results of a multiple linear regression in which

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Data exploration 9
the number of saltmarsh sparrows captured in a study plot is
modelled as a function of covariates that describe the relative
abundance of various plant species (for details, see Gjerdrum,
Elphick & Rubega 2005; Gjerdrum et al. 2008). The second
column of the table gives the estimated P-values of the t-statistics for each regression parameter when all covariates are
included in the model. Note that only one covariate, that for
the per cent cover of the rush Juncus gerardii, is weakly signicant at the 5% level.
In linear regression, an expression for the variances of the
parameters bj is given by (Draper & Smith 1998; Fox 2008):
Variancebj

1
r2

2
1  Rj n  1S2j

The term Sj depends on covariate values, n is the sample size


and r2 is the variance of the residuals, but these terms are not
relevant to the current discussion (and therefore their mathematical formulation is not given here). It is the rst expression
that is important. The term Rj2 is the R2 from a linear regression model in which covariate Xj is used as a response variable,
and all other covariates as explanatory variables. A high R2 in
such a model means that most of the variation in covariate Xj
is explained by all other covariates, which means that there is
collinearity. The price one pays for this situation is that the
standard errors of the parameters are inated with the square
root of 1 (1 ) Rj2), also called the variance ination factor
(VIF), which means that the P-values get larger making it more
dicult to detect an eect. This phenomenon is illustrated in
Table 1; the third column of the table gives the VIF values for
all covariates and shows that there is a high level of collinearity.
One strategy for addressing this problem is to sequentially
drop the covariate with the highest VIF, recalculate the VIFs
and repeat this process until all VIFs are smaller than a preselected threshold. Montgomery & Peck (1992) used a value of

10, but a more stringent approach is to use values as low as 3 as


we did here. High, or even moderate, collinearity is especially
problematic when ecological signals are weak. In that case,
even a VIF of 2 may cause nonsignicant parameter estimates,
compared to the situation without collinearity. Following this
process caused three variables to be dropped from our analysis:
the tall Spartina alterniora, and those for plant height and
stem density. With the collinearity problem removed, the
Juncus variable is shown to be highly signicant (Table 1).
Sequentially dropping further nonsignicant terms one at a
time gives a model with only the Juncus and Shrub variables,
but with little further change in P-values, showing how dropping collinear variables can have a bigger impact on P-values
than dropping nonsignicant covariates.
Other ways to detect collinearity include pairwise scatterplots comparing covariates, correlation coecients or a PCA
biplot (Jollie 2002) applied on all covariates. Collinearity can
also be expected if temporal (e.g. month, year) or spatial variables (e.g. latitude, longitude) are used together with covariates
like temperature, rainfall, etc. Therefore, one should always
plot all covariates against temporal and spatial covariates. The
easiest way to solve collinearity is by dropping collinear covariates. The choice of which covariates to drop can be based on
the VIFs, or perhaps better, on common sense or biological
knowledge. An alternative consideration, especially when
future work on the topic will be done, is how easy alternative
covariates are to measure in terms of eort and cost. Whenever
two covariates X and Z are collinear, and Z is used in the statistical analysis, then the biological discussion in which the eect
of Z is explained should include mention of the collinearity,
and recognize that it might well be X that is driving the system
(cf. Gjerdrum et al. 2008). For a discussion of collinearity in
combination with measurement errors on the covariates, see
Carroll et al. (2006).

Table 1. P-values of the t-statistic for three linear regression models and variance ination factor (VIF) values for the full model. In the full
model, the number of banded sparrows, which is a measure of how many birds were present, is modelled as a function of the covariates listed in
the rst column. In the second and third columns, the P-values and VIF values for the full model are presented (note that no variables have been
removed yet). In the fourth column P-values are presented for the model after collinearity has been removed by sequentially deleting each
variable for which the VIF value was highest until all remaining VIFs were below 3. In the last column, only variables with signicant P-values
remain, giving the most parsimonious explanation for the number of sparrows in a plot
Covariate

P-value (full model)

VIF

P-value (collinearity removed)

P-value (reduced model)

% Juncus gerardii
% Shrub
Height of thatch
% Spartina patens
% Distichlis spicata
% Bare ground
% Other vegetation
% Phragmites australis
% Tall sedge
% Water
% Spartina alterniora (short)
% Spartina alterniora (tall)
Maximum vegetation height
Vegetation stem density

00203
09600
09989
00640
00527
00666
00730
00715
02160
00568
00549
00960
02432
07219

449953
27818
16712
1593506
537545
120586
58170
37490
44093
170677
1214637
1593828
61200
32064

00001
00568
08263
03312
02538
08908
09462
02734
04313
06942
02949

000004
00727

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

10 A. F. Zuur et al.
shows a multi-panel scatterplot (also called a pair plot) for the
1295 saltmarsh sparrows for which we have morphological
data. Any observation that sticks out from the black cloud
needs further investigation; these may be dierent species,
measurement errors, typing mistakes or they may be correct
values after all. Note that the large wing length observation
that we picked up with the Cleveland dotplot in Fig. 2b has
average values for all other variables, suggesting that it is
indeed something that should be checked. The lower panels in
Fig. 10 contain Pearson correlation coecients, which can be
inuenced by outliers meaning that outliers can even contribute to collinearity.

Step 6: What are the relationships between Y


and X variables?
Another essential part of data exploration, especially in
univariate analysis, is plotting the response variable vs. each
covariate (Fig. 9). Note that the variable for the per cent of tall
sedge in a plot (%Tall sedge) should be dropped from any
analysis as it has only one non-zero value. This result shows
that the boxplots and Cleveland dotplots should not only be
applied on the response variable but also on covariates (i.e. we
should not have calculated the VIFs with %Tall sedge included
in the previous section). There are no clear patterns in Fig. 9
between the response and explanatory variables, except perhaps for the amount of Juncus (see also Table 1). Note that the
absence of clear patterns does not mean that there are no relationships; it just means that there are no clear two-way relationships. A model with multiple explanatory variables may
still provide a good t.
Besides visualizing relationships between variables, scatterplots are also useful to detect observations that do not comply
with the general pattern between two variables. Figure 10
Maximum vegetation height

Banded

Vegetation stem density

8 10 12

20

40

% Tall sedge

50
40
30
20
10
0
0

10

15

10 20 30 40 50

10

20

80

30

% Spartina alterniflora (short) % Spartina alterniflora (tall)

15

20

% Bare ground

% Juncus gerardii

10

10

20

15

40

60

% Other vegetation

20 0

% Shrub

40

Staying with the sparrow morphometric data, suppose that


one asks whether the relationship between wing length and
weight changes over the months and diers between sexes. A
common approach to this analysis is to apply a linear regression model in which weight is the response variable and wing
length (continuous), sex (categorical) and month (categorical)

50
40
30
20
10
0

% Water

% Distichlis

50
40
30
20
10
0

60

Step 7: Should we consider interactions?

8 10 12 0

Height of thatch

30

40

20 40

50

60

60 80 100

% Phragmites australis

50
40
30
20
10
0

10

% Spartina patens

20

40

60

Fig. 9. Multi-panel scatterplots between the


number of banded sparrows and each covariate. A LOESS smoother was added to aid
visual interpretation.

80

Covariates

10 12 14 16

10 15 20 25
65

20 24 28 32

05

55
Tarsus length

05

Head length

04

05

07

25

05

04

05

07

07

Nalospi to bill tip

06

05

06

06

05

12 18

Culmen length

10 20

10 14

35

20 26 32

Wing chord

55 60 65

25

30

35

6 8

12

Weight
16

Fig. 10. Multi-panel scatterplot of morphometric data for the 1295 saltmarsh sparrows.
The upper right panels show pairwise scatterplots between each variable, and the lower left panels contain Pearson correlation
coecients. The font size of the correlation
coecient is proportional to its value. Note
that there are various outliers.

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Data exploration 11
Given : month
Sep
Aug
Jul
Jun
May

60

52

56

60

52

56

60

are covariates. Results showed that the three-way interaction is


signicant, indicating that the relationship between weight and
wing length is indeed changing over the months and between
sexes. However, there is a problem with this analysis. Figure 11
shows the data in a coplot, which is an excellent graphical tool
to visualize the potential presence of interactions. The graph
contains multiple scatterplots of wing length and weight; one
for each month and sex combination. A bivariate linear regression line is added to each scatterplot; if all lines are parallel,
then there is probably no signicant interaction (although only
the regression analysis can tell us whether this is indeed the
case). In our example, lines have dierent slopes, indicating the
potential presence of interactions. In some months, however,
the number of observations is very small, and there are no data
at all from males in September. A sensible approach would be
to repeat the analysis for only the JuneAugust period.

Step 8: Are observations of the response


variable independent?
A crucial assumption of most statistical techniques is that
observations are independent of one another (Hurlbert 1984),
meaning that information from any one observation should
not provide information on another after the eects of other
variables have been accounted for. This concept is best
explained with examples.
The observations from the sparrow abundance data set were
taken at multiple locations. If birds at locations close to each
other have characteristics that are more similar to each other
than to birds from locations separated by larger distances, then
we would violate the independence assumption. Another
example is when multiple individuals of the same family (e.g.
all of the young from one nest) are sampled; these individuals
might be more similar to each other than random individuals
in the population, because they share a similar genetic makeup and similar parental provisioning history.
When such dependence arises, the statistical model used to
analyse the data needs to account for it. For example, by mod-

Male

Given : sex

Female

16 18 20 22 24

56

16 18 20 22 24

Fig. 11. Coplot for the sparrow data. The


lower left panel shows a scatterplot between
wing length and weight for males in May,
and the upper right panel for females in
September. On each panel, a bivariate linear
regression model was tted to aid visual
interpretation.

Weight (g)

52

52

56

60

52

56

60

Wing length (mm)

elling any spatial or temporal relationships, or by nesting data


in a hierarchical structure (e.g. nestlings could be nested within
nests). Testing for independence, however, is not always easy.
In Zuur et al. (2009a) a large number of data sets were analysed in which dependence among observations played a role.
Examples include the amount of bioluminescence at sites along
an oceanic depth gradient, nitrogen isotope ratios in whale
teeth as a function of age, pH values in Irish rivers, the number
of amphibians killed by cars at various locations along a road,
feeding behaviour of dierent godwits on a beach, the number
of disease-causing spores aecting larval honey bees from multiple hives and the number of calls from owl chicks upon arrival of a parent. Another commonly encountered situation
where non-independence must be addressed is when there is
phylogenetic structure (i.e. dependence due to shared ancestry)
within a data set.
There are many ways to include a temporal or spatial dependence structure in a model for analysis. These include using
lagged response variables as covariates (Brockwell & Davis
2002), mixed eects modelling (Pinheiro & Bates 2000), imposing a residual correlation structure using generalized least
squares (Zuur et al. 2009a) or allowing regression parameters
to change over time (Harvey 1989). It is also possible to t a
model with and without a correlation structure, and compare
the models using a selection criterion or hypothesis test
(Pinheiro & Bates 2000). The presence of a dependence structure in the raw data may be modelled with a covariate such as
month or temperature, or the inclusion of a smoothing function of time or a two-dimensional smoother of spatial coordinates (Wood 2006). Regardless of the method used, the model
residuals should not contain any dependence structure. Quite
often a residual correlation structure is caused by an important
covariate that was not measured. If this is the case, it may not
be possible to resolve the problem.
When using regression techniques, the independence
assumption is rather important and violation may increase the
type I error. For example, Ostrom (1990) showed that ignoring
auto-correlation may give P-values that are 400% inated.

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

12 A. F. Zuur et al.
(b)

C. fuscicollis ACF
ACF
04 02 08

C. fuscicollis abundance
0
400 800

(a)

10
15
20
Time (2 weeks)

25

6
8
Lag

10

12

(d)

L. dominicanus ACF
ACF
04 02 08

L. dominicanus abundance
0
4
8 12

(c)

10
15
20
Time (2 weeks)

25

Hence, it is important to check whether there is dependence in


the raw data before doing the analysis, and also the residuals
afterwards. These checks can be made by plotting the response
variable vs. time or spatial coordinates. Any clear pattern is a
sign of dependence. This approach is more dicult if there is
no clear sequence to the observations (e.g. multiple observations on the same object), but in this case one can include a
dependence structure using random eects (Pinheiro & Bates
2000; Fitzmaurice et al. 2004; Brown & Prescott 2006; Zuur
et al. 2009a). Figure 12a,c shows a short time series illustrating
the observed abundance of two bird species on a mudat in
Argentina over a 52 week period (E. Ieno, unpublished data).
The rst time series shows high numbers of white-rumped
sandpipers Calidris fuscicollis during the rst 20 weeks, followed by zeros (because the species migrates), and then an
abundance increase again after 38 weeks. The second time series does not show a clear pattern in the abundance of kelp gulls
(Larus dominicanus).
A more formal way to assess the presence of temporal
dependence is to plot auto-correlation functions (ACF) for
regularly spaced time series, or variograms for irregularly
spaced time series and spatial data (Schabenberger & Pierce
2002). An ACF calculates the Pearson correlation between a
time series and the same time series shifted by k time units.
Figures 12b,d show the auto-correlation of the time series in
panels (a) and (c). Panel (b) shows a signicant correlation with
a time lag of k = 1 and k = 2. This means that abundances at
time t depend on abundances at time t ) 1 and t ) 2, and any
of the methods mentioned above could be applied. For
the L. dominicanus time series, there is no signicant autocorrelation.

Discussion
All of the problems described in this paper, and the strategies
to address them, apply throughout ecological research, but

6
8
Lag

10

12

Fig. 12. (a) Number of Calidris fuscicollis


plotted vs. time (1 unit = 2 weeks). (b)
Auto-correlation function for the C. fuscicollis time series showing a signicant
correlation at time lags of 2 and 4 weeks
(1 time lag = 2 weeks). (c) Number of Larus
dominicanus vs. time. (d) Auto-correlation
function for L. dominicanus showing no
signicant correlation. Dotted lines in panels
(b) and (d) are c. 95% condence bands.
The auto-correlation with time lag 0 is, by
denition, equal to 1.

they are particularly relevant when results are to be used to


guide management decisions or public policy because of the
repercussions of making a mistake. Increasing attention has
been paid in recent years to the body of data supporting particular management practices (Roberts, Stewart & Pullin 2006;
Pullin & Knight 2009), and applied ecologists have become
increasingly sophisticated in the statistical methods that they
use (e.g. Ellison 2004; Stephens et al. 2005; Robinson & Hamann 2008; Koper & Manseau 2009; Law et al. 2009; Sonderegger et al. 2009). But more fundamental questions about the
appropriateness of the underlying data for a given analysis can
be just as important to ensuring that the best policies are
derived from ecological studies.
In this paper, we have discussed a series of pitfalls that can
seriously inuence the results of an analysis. Some of these
problems are well known, some less so, but even the wellknown assumptions continue to be violated frequently in the
ecological literature. In all cases, the problems can lead to statistical models that are wrong. Such problems can be avoided
only by applying a systematic data exploration before embarking on the analysis (Fig. 1).
Although we have presented our protocol as a linear
sequence, it should be used exibly. Not every data set requires
each step. For example, some statistical techniques do not
require normality (e.g. PCA), and therefore there is no point in
making histograms. The best order to apply the steps may also
depend on the specic data set. And for some techniques,
assumptions can be veried only by applying data explorations
steps after the analysis has been performed. For example, in linear regression, normality and homogeneity should be veried
using the residuals produced by the model. Rather than simplistically following through the protocol, ticking o each
point in order, we would encourage users to treat it as a series of
questions to be asked of the data. Once satised that each issue
has been adequately addressed in a way that makes biological
sense, the data set should be ready for the main analysis.

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

Data exploration 13
Ecological eld data tend to be noisy, eld conditions
unpredictable and prior knowledge often limited. In the
applied realm, changes in funding, policy, and research priorities further complicate matters. This situation is especially so
for long-term studies, where the initial goals often change
with circumstances (e.g. the use of many data sets to examine
species responses to climate change). For all these reasons,
the idealized situation whereby an ecologist carefully designs
their analysis a priori and then collects data may be compromised or irrelevant. Having the analytical exibility to adjust
ones analyses to such circumstance is an important skill for
an applied ecologist, but it requires a thorough understanding of the constraining assumptions imposed by a given data
set.
When problems arise, the best solutions vary. Frequently,
however, ecologists simply transform data to avoid assumption violations. There are three main reasons for a transformation; to reduce the eect of outliers (especially in covariates), to
stabilize the variance and to linearize relationships. However,
using more advanced techniques like GLS and GAMs, heterogeneity and nonlinearity problems can be solved, making
transformation less important. Zuur et al. (2009a) showed
how the use of a data transformation resulted in dierent conclusions about long-term trends compared to an appropriate
analysis using untransformed data; hence it may be best to
avoid transforming response variables. If a transformation is
used, automatic selection tools such as Mosteller and Tukeys
bulging rule (Mosteller & Tukey 1977) should be used with
great caution because these methods ignore the eects of covariates. Another argument against transformations is the need
to subsequently back-transform values to make predictions; it
may not always be clear how to do this and still be able to interpret results on the original scale of the response variable. It is
also important to ensure that the transformation actually
solves the problem at hand; even commonly recommended
transformations do not always work. The bottom line is that
the choice of a specic transformation is a matter of trial and
error.
It is a given fact that data exploration should not be used to
dene the questions that a study sets out to test. Every step of
the exploration should be reported, and any outlier removed
should be justied and mentioned. Reasons for data transformations need to be justied based on the exploratory analysis
(e.g. evidence that model assumptions were violated and that
the transformation rectied the situation).
Applying data exploration (e.g. scatterplots to visualize relationships between response and explanatory variables) to create hypotheses and then using the same data to test these
hypotheses should be avoided. If one has limited a priori
knowledge, then a valid approach is to create two data sets;
apply data exploration on the rst data set to create hypotheses
and use the second data set to test the hypotheses. Such a process, however, is only practical for larger data sets. Regardless
of the specic situation, the routine use and transparent reporting of systematic data exploration would improve the quality
of ecological research and any applied recommendations that
it produces.

Acknowledgements
We thank Anatoly Saveliev, and two anonymous reviewers for comments on
an earlier draft.

References
Brockwell, P.J. & Davis, R.A. (2002) Introduction to Time Series and Forecasting, 2nd edn. Springer-Verlag, New York.
Brown, H. & Prescott, R. (2006) Applied Mixed Models in Medicine, 2nd edn.
John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Burnham, K.P. & Anderson, D.R. (2002) Model Selection and Multimodel
Inference. A Practical InformationTheoretic Approach, 2nd edn. Springer,
New York.
Cameron, A.C. & Trivedi, P.K. (1998) Regression Analysis of Count Data.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Carroll, R.J., Ruppert, D., Stefanski, L.A. & Crainiceanu, C.M. (2006)
Measurement Error in Nonlinear Models: A Modern Perspective, 2nd edn.
Chapman & Hall, Boca Raton, FL.
Chateld, C. (1998) Problem Solving: A Statisticians Guide. Chapman & Hall,
Boca Raton, FL.
Cleveland, W.S. (1993) Visualizing Data. Hobart Press, Summit, NJ.
Draper, N.R. & Smith, H. (1998) Applied Regression Analysis, 3rd edn. John
Wiley and Sons, New York.
Ellison, A.M. (2004) Bayesian inference in ecology. Ecology Letters, 7, 509
520.
Elphick, C.S. & Oring, L.W. (1998) Winter management of Californian rice
elds for waterbirds. Journal of Applied Ecology, 35, 95108.
Elphick, C.S. & Oring, L.W. (2003) Conservation implications of ooding rice
elds on winter waterbird communities. Agriculture, Ecosystems and
Environment, 94, 1729.
Fitzmaurice, G.M., Laird, N.M. & Ware, J.H. (2004) Applied Longitudinal
Analysis. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ.
Fox, J. (2008) Applied Regression Analysis and Generalized Linear Models, 2nd
edn. Sage Publications, CA.
Gelman, A., Pasarica, C. & Dodhia, R. (2002) Lets practice what we preach:
turning tables into graphs in statistic research. The American Statistician, 56,
121130.
Gjerdrum, C., Elphick, C.S. & Rubega, M. (2005) What determines nest site
selection and nesting success in saltmarsh breeding sparrows? Condor, 107,
849862.
Gjerdrum, C., Elphick, C.S. & Rubega, M.A. (2008) How well can we model
numbers and productivity of saltmarsh sharp-tailed sparrows (Ammodramus
caudacutus) using habitat features? Auk, 125, 608617.
Harvey, A.C. (1989) Forecasting, Structural Time Series Models and the Kalman
Filter. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Hilbe, J.M. (2007) Negative Binomial Regression. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge, UK.
Hurlbert, S.H. (1984) Pseudoreplication and the design of ecological eld
experiments. Ecological Monographs, 54, 187211.
Jollie, I.T. (2002) Principal Component Analysis, 2nd edn. Springer, New York.
Koper, N. & Manseau, M. (2009) Generalized estimating equations and generalized linear mixed-eects models for modelling resources selection. Journal
of Applied Ecology, 46, 590599.
Laara, E. (2009) Statistics: reasoning on uncertainty, and the insignicance of
testing null. Annales Zoologici Fennici, 46, 138157.
Law, R., Illian, J., Burslem, D.F.R.P., Gratzer, G., Gunatilleke, C.V.S. &
Gunatilleke, I.A.U.N. (2009) Ecological information from spatial patterns of
plants: insights from point process theory. Journal of Ecology, 97, 616628.
Legendre, P. & Legendre, L. (1998) Numerical Ecology. Second English
Edition. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Montgomery, D.C. & Peck, E.A. (1992) Introduction to Linear Regression
Analysis. Wiley, New York.
Morgan, J.H. (2004) Remarks on the taking and recording of biometric
measurements in bird ringing. The Ring, 26, 7178.
Mosteller, F. & Tukey, J.W. (1977) Data Analysis and Regression: A Second
Course in Statistics. Addison Wesley, Reading, MA.
Ostrom, C.W. (1990) Time Series Analysis: Regression Techniques, 2nd edn.
Sage Publications Inc, Thousand Oaks Newbury Park, CA.
Pinheiro, J. & Bates, D. (2000) Mixed Eects Models in S and S-Plus. SpringerVerlag, New York.
Pullin, A.S. & Knight, T.M. (2009) Doing more good than harm building an
evidence-based for conservation and environmental management. Biological
Conservation, 142, 931934.

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

14 A. F. Zuur et al.
Quinn, G.P. & Keough, M.J. (2002) Experimental Design and Data Analysis for
Biologists. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
R Development Core Team (2009) R: A Language and Environment for
Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
ISBN 3-900051-07-0. URL http://www.R-project.org.
Roberts, P.D., Stewart, G.B. & Pullin, A.S. (2006) Are review articles a
reliable source of evidence to support conservation and environmental
management? A comparison with medicine. Biological Conservation, 132,
409423.
Robinson, A.P. & Hamann, J.D. (2008) Correcting for spatial autocorrelation in sequential sampling. Journal of Applied Ecology, 45, 1221
1227.
Sarkar, D. (2008) Lattice: Multivariate Data Visualization with R. Springer,
New York.
Schabenberger, O. & Pierce, F.J. (2002) Contemporary Statistical Models for
the Plant and Soil Sciences. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL.
Sonderegger, D.L., Wang, H., Clements, W.H. & Noon, B.R. (2009) Using
SiZer to detect thresholds in ecological data. Frontiers in Ecology and the
Environment, 7, 190195.
Stephens, P.A., Buskirk, S.W., Hayward, G.D. & Mart nez del Rio, C. (2005)
Information theory and hypothesis testing: a call for pluralism. Journal of
Applied Ecology, 42, 412.
ter Braak, C.J.F. & Verdonschot, P.F.M. (1995) Canonical correspondence
analysis and related multivariate methods in aquatic ecology. Aquatic
Science, 57, 225289.
Wood, S.N. (2006) Generalized Additive Models. An Introdcution with R.
Chapman Hall CRC, Boca Raton, FL. Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Walker,

N.J., Saveliev, A.A. & Smith, G. (2009a) Mixed Eects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R. Springer, New York.
Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N. & Smith, G.M. (2007) Analysing Ecological Data.
Springer, New York.
Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N. & Meesters, E.H.W.G. (2009b) A Beginners Guide to R.
Springer, New York.
Received 13 August 2009; accepted 8 October 2009
Handling Editor: Robert P. Frecklenton

Supporting Information
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online
version of this article:
Appendix S1. Data sets and R code used for analysis.
As a service to our authors and readers, this journal provides
support ing information supplied by the authors. Such materials may
be re-organized for online delivery, but are not copy-edited or typeset.
Technical support issues arising from supporting information (other
than missing les) should be addressed to the authors.

 2009 The Authors. Journal compilation  2009 British Ecological Society, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1, 314

You might also like