Noi Cercetări Emoție - Cogniție

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

REVIEW ARTICLE

published: 17 February 2015


doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00058

The neurobiology of emotioncognition interactions:


fundamental questions and strategies for future research
Hadas Okon-Singer 1 * , Talma Hendler 2 , Luiz Pessoa 3 and Alexander J. Shackman 3 *
1

Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel


Functional Brain Center, Wohl Institute of Advanced Imaging, and School of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Sagol School of Neuroscience,
Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
3
Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging Center, University of Maryland, College Park, College
Park, MD, USA
2

Edited by:
Leonhard Schilbach, University
Hospital Cologne, Germany
Reviewed by:
Christian Sorg, Klinikum rechts der
Isar Technische Universitt Mnchen,
Germany
Elliot Berkman, University of Oregon,
USA
*Correspondence:
Hadas Okon-Singer, Department of
Psychology, University of Haifa,
Mount Carmel, Haifa 3498838, Israel
e-mail: [email protected];
Alexander J. Shackman, Department
of Psychology, Neuroscience and
Cognitive Science Program, and
Maryland Neuroimaging Center,
University of Maryland, 3123G
Biology-Psychology Building, College
Park, MD 20742, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
These

authors have contributed


equally to this work.

Recent years have witnessed the emergence of powerful new tools for assaying the
brain and a remarkable acceleration of research focused on the interplay of emotion and
cognition. This work has begun to yield new insights into fundamental questions about the
nature of the mind and important clues about the origins of mental illness. In particular,
this research demonstrates that stress, anxiety, and other kinds of emotion can profoundly
inuence key elements of cognition, including selective attention, working memory, and
cognitive control. Often, this inuence persists beyond the duration of transient emotional
challenges, partially reecting the slower molecular dynamics of catecholamine and hormonal neurochemistry. In turn, circuits involved in attention, executive control, and working
memory contribute to the regulation of emotion. The distinction between the emotional
and the cognitive brain is fuzzy and context-dependent. Indeed, there is compelling
evidence that brain territories and psychological processes commonly associated with
cognition, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and working memory, play a central
role in emotion. Furthermore, putatively emotional and cognitive regions inuence one
another via a complex web of connections in ways that jointly contribute to adaptive
and maladaptive behavior. This work demonstrates that emotion and cognition are deeply
interwoven in the fabric of the brain, suggesting that widely held beliefs about the key
constituents of the emotional brain and the cognitive brain are fundamentally awed. We
conclude by outlining several strategies for enhancing future research. Developing a deeper
understanding of the emotional-cognitive brain is important, not just for understanding the
mind but also for elucidating the root causes of its disorders.
Keywords: ACC, amygdala, anxiety, depression, emotion control and regulation, EEG/ERP, fMRI, PFC

Until the 20th century, the study of emotion and cognition was
largely a philosophical matter. Although modern perspectives on
the mind and its disorders remain heavily inuenced by the introspective measures that dened this earlier era of scholarship, the
last several decades have witnessed the emergence of powerful
new tools for assaying the brain and a remarkable acceleration
of research to elucidate the interplay of emotion and cognition
(Pessoa, 2013; Braver et al., 2014; Dolcos and Denkova, 2014).
The immediate goal of our Special Research Topic was to survey
recent advances in understanding how emotional and cognitive
processes interact, how they are integrated in the brain, and the
implications for understanding the mind and its disorders (OkonSinger et al., 2014b; Figure 1). Here, we consider ways in which
this rapidly growing body of work begins to address some more
fundamental questions about the nature of cognitionemotion
interactions, highlighting key points of consensus. By focusing
attention on the most important outstanding questions, we hope
to move the eld forward. First, we hope that answers provided
by our contributors will stimulate discussion. Second, we hope
that juxtaposing clear theoretical goals against the current state

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

of the science will motivate new and impactful research. Clearly,


our understanding of emotioncognition interactions remains far
from complete. Indeed, we are reminded of Ekman and Davidsons
comment: There are many promising ndings, many more leads,
[and] a variety of theoretical stances (Ekman and Davidson, 1994,
p. 3). We conclude by outlining several strategies for enhancing
future research. With continuing effort, some of the fundamental
questions will be decisively addressed. In some cases, the questions themselves will evolve, as in other areas of the biological
sciences.

HOW DOES EMOTION INFLUENCE COGNITION?


Many of our contributors highlighted evidence that the perception
of emotionally-salient stimuli and the experience of emotional
states can profoundly alter cognition.
EMOTIONAL CUES GRAB EXOGENOUS ATTENTION AND MODULATE
ENDOGENOUS ATTENTION

There is abundant evidence that emotionally-salient cues


snakes, spiders, and angry facesstrongly inuence attention

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 1

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

FIGURE 1 | The top 200 scientific terms used in the Special Research Topic. The typeface is scaled proportional to the frequency of each term. The gure
was generated using http://www.wordle.net.

(e.g., Siman-Tov et al., 2009; Lerner et al., 2012; Pourtois et al., 2013; Carreti, 2014) the ability to selectively
respond to relevant aspects of the environment while inhibiting potential sources of distraction and competing courses
of action (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Miller and Cohen,
2001). The focus of attention is determined by the pervasive competition between exogenous (often termed stimulusdriven or bottomup) and endogenous (often termed goaldirected or topdown attention) mechanisms (Egeth and Yantis,
1997).
With respect to exogenous attention, a number of contributors describe new evidence that emotionally-charged cues are
more attention-grabbing than neutral cues and highlight recent
efforts to specify the mechanisms underlying this bias (Holtmann
et al., 2013; McHugo et al., 2013; Peers et al., 2013; Stollstorff
et al., 2013). Along the way, McHugo et al. (2013) provide a
useful tutorial on methods for quantifying the capture of attention by emotional cues (e.g., dot-probe, emotional attentional
blink).
Importantly, attention can also be guided in an endogenous
fashion by internal goals (e.g., rules, instructions, and plans)
as well as moods and motivational states (e.g., feeling anxious or hungry). Mohanty and Sussman (2013) discuss evidence
demonstrating that emotion and motivation can guide attention to congruent cues (e.g., food when hungry). In particular,
they show that subcortical regions proximally involved in determining value and orchestrating emotional states (e.g., amygdala,
substantia nigra) can facilitate endogenous attentional processes
implemented in frontoparietal regions and can strengthen activation in relevant sensory regions (e.g., face-selective regions of the
fusiform gyrus when anticipating an angry face). This extended
network, encompassing sensory, attentional, and emotional circuits, facilitates the rapid detection of emotionally-signicant
information.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

ATTENTIONAL BIASES TO EMOTIONAL CUES ARE PLASTIC

Anxious individuals tend to allocate excess attention to threat and


there is evidence that this cognitive bias causally contributes to the
development and maintenance of anxiety disorders (Bar-Haim
et al., 2007; Hakamata et al., 2010; MacLeod and Mathews, 2012;
Singer et al., 2012; Van Bockstaele et al., 2013; MacLeod and Clarke,
2015). Extreme anxiety and behavioral inhibition often emerges
early in development (Fox et al., 2005a; Blackford and Pine, 2012;
Fox and Kalin, 2014), raising important questions about the degree
to which childhood attentional biases to threat are plastic and
can be inuenced by early experience (Shechner et al., 2012; BarHaim and Pine, 2013; Henderson et al., 2014; MacLeod and Clarke,
2015).
Here, Kessel et al. (2013) provide tantalizing correlative evidence that emotional biases in attention are inuenced by
caregiver style. Using an innovative combination of behavioral
and electrophysiological techniques, they show that although
temperamentally inhibited children allocate more attention to
aversive cues, this is reduced among the offspring of parents who rely on encouragement, affection, and appreciation to reinforce positive behavior. A key challenge for
future research will be to test whether targeted interventions aimed at cultivating more salubrious parenting styles
have similar consequences. Prospective designs (e.g., before
and after exposure to a negative life event or trauma)
would provide another powerful approach for understanding the plasticity of emotional attention (Admon et al., 2009,
2012).
EMOTION EXERTS PERSISTENT EFFECTS ON ATTENTION

Emotions are often conceptualized as eeting and most imaging


and psychophysiological studies of emotion focus on transient
responses to punctate emotional challenges. Yet, there is growing evidence that emotions can have lingering consequences for

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 2

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

cognition and behavior (Davidson, 2004; Suls and Martin, 2005;


Hajcak and Olvet, 2008; Qin et al., 2009).
Here, for example, Vaisvaser et al. (2013) combined serial
measures of emotional state, neuroendocrine activity, and restingstate brain activity to demonstrate that alterations in amygdala
hippocampal functional connectivity persist for more than 2 h
following exposure to intense social stress. Along conceptually
similar lines, Morriss et al. (2013) use electrophysiological techniques to show that endogenous attention is potentiated for several
seconds following brief emotional challenges (i.e., standardized
emotional images).
Several threads of evidence highlight the importance of understanding the mechanisms that govern variation in the speed
of recovery from emotional perturbation. In particular, individual differences in emotional recovery (a) strongly predict
personality traits, such as neuroticism, that confer increased
risk of developing psychopathology (e.g., Blackford et al., 2009;
Schuyler et al., 2014); and (b) are sensitive to adversity and
chronic stress exposure, two other well-established risk factors (Lapate et al., 2014). An important challenge for future
research will be to identify the neural circuitry and molecular pathways that support the enduring effects of emotion on
endogenous attention and to clarify the intermediate processes
that link variation in emotional recovery to mental health and
disease.
DISTRACTING EMOTIONAL CUES READILY PENETRATE THE GATE
PROTECTING WORKING MEMORY

Endogenous attention is tightly linked with working memory


(Postle, 2006; DEsposito and Postle, 2014; Sreenivasan et al.,
2014). The transient representation of task-sets, goals, and other
kinds of information in working memory plays a crucial role
in sustaining goal-directed attention and guiding behavior in
the face of potential distraction (Miller and Cohen, 2001). In
short, information held in working memory is a key determinant of our momentary thoughts, feelings, and behavior.
Importantly, the capacity of working memory is strongly determined by the ability to lter or gate irrelevant information
(Vogel et al., 2005; McNab and Klingberg, 2007; Awh and Vogel,
2008).
Here, Stout et al. (2013) used a well-established electrophysiological marker of working memory storage (i.e., contralateral
delay activity; Vogel and Machizawa, 2004) to show that threatrelated distractors (i.e., task-irrelevant fearful faces) are stored
in working memory and that this ltering inefciency is exaggerated in dispositionally-anxious individuals. Once in working
memory, emotional information is poised to hijack endogenous
attention and other kinds of topdown control mechanisms. From
a psychiatric perspective, this emotional gating decit may help
to explain the persistence of heightened negative affect (e.g., anxiety, sadness) among patients with emotional disorders (Grupe and
Nitschke, 2013; Cohen et al., 2014; Stout et al., 2014). An important
challenge for future studies will be to use hemodynamic imaging
techniques, such as fMRI, to clarify the neural circuitry underlying
emotional gating decits. A variety of evidence suggests that the
pulvinar may play an important role (Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010;
Arend et al., 2014).

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

DISTRACTING EMOTIONAL CUES DISRUPT COGNITIVE CONTROL AND


WORKING MEMORY

Classically, cognition and emotion have been viewed as oppositional forces (Damasio, 2005a; Okon-Singer et al., 2007, 2011;
Shackman et al., in press). From this perspective, moods and
other kinds of emotional states are responsible for short-circuiting
cognition.
Consistent with this view, Kalanthroff et al. (2013) show that
emotional distractors disrupt cognitive control. Cognitive control
encompasses the range of processes (e.g., endogenous attention,
inhibition, and learning) that are engaged when habitual responses
are not sufcient to sustain goal-directed behavior, as in stopsignal, go/no-go, Stroop, and Eriksen anker tasks (Shackman
et al., 2011b). Here, the authors demonstrate that the brief presentation of emotional images disrupts performance in the stop-signal
task, a widely used index of inhibitory control (see also Pessoa et al.,
2012).
Likewise, Iordan et al. (2013) review evidence that emotional
distractors disrupt working memory. Converging with other work
focused on emotion-related distraction (Bishop, 2007; Etkin,
2012; Bishop and Forster, 2013; Etkin et al., 2013; Okon-Singer
et al., 2014a; van Ast et al., 2014), they suggest that degraded
performance reects two processes: (a) increased engagement
of regions involved in processing socio-emotional information
and orchestrating emotional expressions (e.g., amygdala), and
(b) a reduction of delay-spanning activity in frontoparietal
cortex.
EMOTION STRENGTHENS SOME COGNITIVE PROCESSES WHILE
WEAKENING OTHERS

With the ascent of evolutionary theory in the 19th century (Darwin, 1872/2009, 1872), many scientists adopted the view that
emotions are functional and enhance tness (Susskind et al., 2008;
Todd et al., 2012; Sandi, 2013; Schwabe and Wolf, 2013; Todd
and Anderson, 2013); in short, that emotions are more adaptive
than not and that there is typically more cooperation than strife
between emotion and cognition (Levenson, 1994).
Consistent with this more nuanced perspective, the contributions from Clarke and Johnstone (2013), Morriss et al. (2013),
Robinson et al. (2013a, 2013b), Vytal et al. (2013) provide evidence that experimentally-elicited anxiety facilitates some kinds
of information processing, while degrading others. In particular,
they provide considerable evidence that anxiety: (a) enhances vigilance, potentiating early sensory cortical responses to innocuous
environmental stimuli, increasing the likelihood that emotionally
salient information will be detected; and (b) disrupts working
memory.
The molecular basis of emotions deleterious impact on working memory is reviewed by Shansky and Lipps (2013). Building
on pioneering work by Arnsten and Goldman-Rakic (1998) and
Arnsten (2009), the authors describe evidence that stress strongly
inuences catecholamine (i.e., dopamine and norepinephrine)
and glucocorticoid levels in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in ways
that degrade delay-spanning neuronal activity.
Shansky and Lipps (2013) also describe important new evidence that sex hormones, such as estrogen, can exacerbate the
impact of stress on prefrontal function. Along these lines, Sacher

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 3

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

et al. (2013) review human imaging studies showing that the


structure and function of brain circuits involved in emotion generation and regulation are strongly and dynamically modulated
by cyclic uctuations in sex hormones (see also Sacher et al.,
2012). Taken together, these observations underscore the plasticity of emotioncognition interactions and provide promising clues
about the origins of well-established sex differences in the prevalence of stress-related disorders, such as anxiety and depression
(Kessler et al., 2012; Kendler and Gardner, 2014).
EMOTIONAL STATES PROMOTE MOOD-CONGRUENT THOUGHTS AND
ACTIONS

Moods and other, more transient emotional states tend to


encourage congruent thoughts and actions (e.g., Lerner et al.,
2015), a process that is necessarily mediated by enduring
changes in brain activity and connectivity (cf. Vaisvaser et al.,
2013). Here, Van Dessel and Vogt (2012) demonstrate that
mood increases the amount of attention allocated to moodcongruent cues. Schick et al. (2013) provide evidence that individuals at risk for developing depression interpret motivationally
ambiguous cues in a less positive light. Harl et al. (2013)
describe a novel Bayesian computational framework for understanding the mechanisms underlying mood-congruency effects.
An important advantage of this framework is that it generates explicit mechanistic hypotheses. For example, the model
predicts that anxiety facilitates behavioral avoidance because
it leads to inated expectations about the need for avoidant
behavior and increased expectations of punishment or error.
Furthermore, tting model parameters to observable behavior
affords an opportunity to identify the underlying determinants
of mood-congruency effects in healthy and clinical populations.
EMOTIONAL TRAITS INFLUENCE COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE, EVEN
WHEN EMOTIONAL CUES, AND CHALLENGES ARE ABSENT

Emotional traits are often conceptualized as diatheses for emotional states (Matthews et al., 2009). Thus, individuals with high
levels of neuroticism or negative emotionality are thought to be
prone to exaggerated anxiety in the face of trait-relevant cues,
contexts, and challenges (e.g., punishment, negative feedback), as
illustrated in the contributions from Kessel et al. (2013), Moser
et al. (2013), and Proudt et al. (2013). Yet, a considerable body
of neurophysiological evidence indicates that emotional traits
are embodied in the on-going activity and connectivity of the
brain (Canli et al., 2005; Fox et al., 2008; Shackman et al., 2009;
Rohr et al., 2013; Birn et al., 2014a,b). Likewise, the sustained
levels of heightened vigilance and distress characteristic of individuals with anxiety disorders are most apparent in the absence
of clear and imminent threat (Davis et al., 2010; Lissek, 2012;
Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). These observations raise the possibility that emotional traits could inuence cognition in the absence
of explicit emotional distraction or challenge (Watson and Clark,
1984; Bolger and Schilling, 1991; Suls and Martin, 2005).
Here, Berggren et al. (2013) provide compelling evidence that
trait anxiety is associated with degraded cognitive control, indexed
using an anti-saccade task under load. This new observation
adds to a growing literature showing that hot emotional traits

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

can inuence cold cognition (Shackman et al., 2006; Eysenck


et al., 2007; Bishop, 2009; Berggren and Derakshan, 2013, 2014;
Cavanagh and Shackman, 2014), a point that we develop more fully
in the subsequent section focused on the integration of emotion
and cognition.

HOW DOES EMOTION INFLUENCE EMOTION?


An important but rarely addressed question in psychology and
psychiatry concerns the potential inuence of emotions on one
another and concomitant motivational states. For example, are we
less likely to experience excitement or joy on a day where were
feeling frazzled, depressed, or worn out (Arnsten, 1998, 2009;
Pizzagalli, 2014)?
EMOTION ALTERS REINFORCER SENSITIVITY

Building on earlier work by Bogdan and Pizzagalli (2006),


Pizzagalli et al. (2007), Bogdan et al. (2010), and Berghorst et al.
(2013) demonstrate that experimentally-elicited anxiety selectively reduces sensitivity to reward, suggesting a mechanism
that may contribute to the high rate of comorbidity between
anxiety and anhedonia (Southwick et al., 2005). Notably, this
effect was only observed in the subset of subjects who were
most responsive to the anxiety induction (i.e., threat of noxious electric shock). Given evidence that many individuals will
never experience a mood or anxiety disorder (Kessler et al.,
2012), this paradigm may provide a means of identifying those
at greatest risk. Methodologically, this observation underscores
the necessity of including independent measures of emotion
in studies of emotioncognition interactions (Shackman et al.,
2006).

HOW DOES COGNITION INFLUENCE AND REGULATE


EMOTION?
Humans frequently regulate their emotions and they do so using a
variety of implicit and explicit cognitive strategies (Gross, 1998a,b;
Gross and Thompson, 2007; Gross et al., 2011; Webb et al., 2012;
Okon-Singer et al., 2013). Implicit strategies are unintentional and
appear to occur without effort or insight. In contrast, explicit
strategies are voluntary and demand a degree of effortful control.
Several contributors to our Special Research Topic described
new insights into the mechanisms supporting the cognitive regulation of emotion and the role of emotion regulation in psychiatric
disorders, such as depression.
ATTENTION REGULATES EMOTION

Perhaps the most basic strategy for reducing distress is attentional


avoidance; that is, to simply look away from the source of distress
(Xing and Isaacowitz, 2006). Overt attentional redeployment is a
potent means of regulating the engagement of subcortical structures, such as the amygdala, that play a key role in orchestrating
emotional states (Pessoa et al., 2002; Dalton et al., 2005; Dalton
et al., 2007; van Reekum et al., 2007; Urry, 2010; Okon-Singer
et al., 2014a).
Here, Aue et al. (2013b) employed an innovative combination of eyetracking, psychophysiology, and fMRI to explore
visual avoidance in spider phobics. Taking an individual differences approach, they demonstrate that enhanced activation in

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 4

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

the amygdala and dorsal striatum to spider images was predictive


of increased visual avoidance among arachnophobes. Peripheral
measures of autonomic arousal showed a similar pattern, suggesting that arachnophobes endogenously redirect attention as a
means of regulating their extreme fear, a strategy that might be
non-adaptive in the long term (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). A key
challenge for future research will be to clarify the order of these
effects (i.e., fear attention avoidance reduced fear), perhaps by leveraging the millisecond temporal resolution afforded by
facial electromyography (e.g., Lee et al., 2009; Heller et al., 2014).
Elucidating the mechanisms supporting the recursive interplay
of emotion and attention and the mutual inuences of different
processing biases (Aue et al., 2013a) would inform our understanding of disorders, like post-traumatic stress, that are characterized
by dysregulated emotion and aberrant attention to emotionallysalient cues (e.g., Admon et al., 2013; Wald et al., 2013) and set
the stage for developing improved interventions (MacLeod and
Mathews, 2012; Bar-Haim and Pine, 2013; MacLeod and Clarke,
2015).
THE CHOICE OF COGNITIVE REGULATION STRATEGY DEPENDS ON THE
SITUATION

Sheppes and Levin (2013) emphasize that humans frequently use


effortful cognitive strategies to cope with and regulate their emotions (e.g., Egloff et al., 2006; Ehring et al., 2010). For example,
they may try to distract themselves or they may try to reappraise
the situation in a more positive light. Sheppes and Levin (2013)
provide evidence that not only do individuals have the capacity
to exibly choose emotion regulation strategies, but that they do
so in ways that are strongly inuenced by the emotional context
(e.g., choosing to reappraise when presented with mild negative pictures, and to distract themselves in face of highly aversive
stimulation).
WORKING MEMORY REGULATES EMOTION

Some strategies for regulating emotional distress, such as reappraisal, require the effortful maintenance of an explicit regulatory
goal. Rolls (2013) reviews evidence suggesting that this critically
depends on working memory. More broadly, he suggests that goals,
attentional sets, and other kinds of declarative knowledge held in
working memory play a central role in regulating the output of
emotional systems.

HOW ARE EMOTION AND COGNITION INTEGRATED?


Humans tend to experience cognition and emotion as fundamentally different. Emotion is infused with feelings of pleasure or pain and manifests in readily discerned changes in
the body, whereas cognition often appears devoid of substantial hedonic, motivational, or somatic features. These apparent
differences in phenomenological experience and peripheral physiology led many classical scholars to treat emotion and cognition
as distinct mental faculties (de Sousa, 2014; Schmitter, 2014).
But contemporary theorists have increasingly rejected the claim
that emotion and cognition are categorically different (Damasio, 2005b; Duncan and Barrett, 2007; Lindquist and Barrett,
2012; Barrett and Satpute, 2013; Pessoa, 2013), motivated in
part by recent imaging evidence demonstrating the overlap of

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

emotional and cognitive processes in the brain (e.g., Shackman


et al., 2011b; Raz et al., 2012, 2014). The neural integration of
emotion and cognition should not be surprisingafter all, the
human brain did not evolve to optimize performance on laboratory measures of cold cognition or to passively respond
to experimental manipulations of emotion, such as threat of
shock. Our brain, like that of other animals, is the product
of evolutionary pressures that demanded neural systems capable of using information about pleasure and pain, derived from
stimuli saturated with hedonic and motivational signicance,
to adaptively regulate attention, learning, somatic arousal, and
action.
A number of contributors highlighted advances in our understanding of the neural mechanisms that serve to integrate emotion
and cognition.
CANONICAL TERRITORIES OF THE COGNITIVE BRAIN REGULATE
EMOTION

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is a canonically cognitive region of the brain, well known for its critical role in reasoning
and higher cognition (e.g., endogenous attention, working memory, and cognitive control; Roberts et al., 1998; Miller and Cohen,
2001; DEsposito and Postle, 2014). Yet, there is growing evidence
that the dlPFC plays a key role in the topdown control of emotion and motivated behavior (Fox et al., 2005b; Koenigs et al., 2008;
Zaretsky et al., 2010; Buhle et al., 2013; Frank et al., 2014; Treadway
et al., 2014).
Here, Clarke and Johnstone (2013) and Iordan et al. (2013) provide tantalizing, albeit correlational, evidence that dlPFC acts to
protect the contents of working memory from emotional distraction. This converges with work by Peers et al. (2013) and Stollstorff
et al. (2013) indicating that dlPFC plays a key role in regulating the
focus of attention in the face of potentially distracting emotional
cues.
Rolls (2013) extends this perspective to decision-making, arguing that behavior reects a pervasive, dynamic competition
between two kinds of brain systems: (a) emotional systems, including circuits centered on the amygdala and ventral striatum, that
have been genetically programmed by our phylogenetic history
(e.g., fear elicited by danger, joy elicited by sweets and fat); and
(b) cognitive systems, such as the frontoparietal network, that are
informed by our ontogenetic history and governed by our declarative knowledge and explicit goals (i.e., pick the healthy orange,
not the unhealthy candy bar; cf. Hare et al., 2008, 2009). Rolls
emphasizes that the lateral PFC can override the output of emotion circuitry, biasing behavior in favor of our explicit goals. John
et al. (2013) articulate a complementary perspective, reviewing
evidence that the PFC and amygdala functionally interact via a
complex anatomical network of recurrent cortical and thalamic
projections and intra-amygdalar microcircuits (see also Pessoa and
Adolphs, 2010; Pessoa, 2012; Pessoa et al., 2012; Birn et al., 2014a,b;
Treadway et al., 2014).
Evidence linking the dlPFC to mood and anxiety disorders, as
in the papers contributed by Crocker et al. (2013) and Warren et al.
(2013), underscores the importance of developing a more sophisticated understanding of the role played by cognitive regions in
normal and disordered emotion.

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 5

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

CANONICAL TERRITORIES OF THE COGNITIVE BRAIN ARE REGULATED


BY EMOTION

Regulation is a two-way street. Just as cognitive systems (e.g.,


dlPFC) regulate emotion, emotion systems (e.g., amygdala) are
well positioned to regulate cognitive systems via their inuence over the brainstem neurotransmitter systems that govern
the quality of information processing (e.g., neuronal signal-tonoise) in cortical regions, as highlighted in the review contributed
by Shansky and Lipps (2013). Via these mechanisms, the amygdala is endowed with the capacity to transiently assume enhanced
control over attention and behavior in situations that favor immediate responses over slower, more deliberate reasoning (Davis and
Whalen, 2001; Arnsten, 2009).
ADAPTIVE AND MALADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR REFLECTS THE INTEGRATED
CONTRIBUTIONS OF EMOTION AND COGNITIVE CONTROL

Oftentimes, cognitive control is associated with laboratory tasks


that require the detection and adjudication of response conict,
as with incongruent trials of the Stroop, Eriksen Flanker, and
go/no-go tasks. Yet, it is clear that control processes are engaged
by a much broader range of cognitive and emotional challenges
(e.g., Pochon et al., 2008; Shenhav et al., 2013). In particular,
control is engaged when there is uncertainty about the optimal
course of action (e.g., probabilistic learning), when potential
actions are associated with the possibility of error or punishment, or when there is competition between alternative courses
of action (e.g., ee/freeze, go/no-go). These features are hallmarks of dangerous environments, both in the real world and
in laboratory studies of fear, anxiety, and pain. Consequently,
it has long been thought that control processes are engaged in
threatening environments in order to monitor risk, optimize
learning, and avoid potentially catastrophic actions (Norman and
Shallice, 1986; Gray and McNaughton, 2000). These theoretical
considerations raise the possibility that the neural circuitry underlying cognitive control also contributes to the negative emotions
elicited by potential threat. Indeed, there is compelling evidence
from functional imaging studies that negative affect and cognitive control paradigms consistently activate an overlapping region
of the midcingulate cortex (MCC; Shackman et al., 2011b; Lin
et al., 2014). This overlap is consistent with anatomical evidence
suggesting that the MCC represents a hub where information
about pain, threat, and other more abstract forms of potential
punishment and negative feedback are synthesized into a biasing signal that modulates regions involved in expressing fear
and anxiety, executing goal-directed behaviors, and biasing the
focus of selective attention (Shackman et al., 2011b; Cavanagh
and Shackman, 2014). Taken together, these observations suggest that anxiety and other emotions are tightly integrated with
control processes implemented in the MCC and other brain
regions.
Along these lines, Morrison et al. (2013) show that even simple, phylogenetically-ancient kinds of motivated behavior, such
as the reexive withdrawal from pain or the learned avoidance
of pain-related contexts, are dynamically shaped by complex,
hierarchically-organized networks of feedforward and feedback
connections that serve to integrate emotional (e.g., value, risk)
and cognitive computations (e.g., prediction error, attention

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

allocation, action selection) in ways that support adaptive behavior (for convergent perspectives, see the contributions from Rolls,
2013, and John et al., 2013).
Dreisbach and Fischer (2012) describe other evidence consistent with this integrative perspective. In particular, they show that
cognitive conict is aversive. This converges with a growing body
of evidence demonstrating that conict and other prompts for
increased control (e.g., errors, punishment), are experienced as
unpleasant and facilitate avoidance (Botvinick, 2007; Kool et al.,
2010; Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012; Schouppe et al., 2012; Lindstrm et al., 2013; Proudt et al., 2013; Shenhav and Buckner,
2014).
If negative emotions are indeed integrated with control processes, we would expect that anxiety and control should covary.
That is, one would expect a degree of functional convergence
between measures of anxiety and control-related activity in the
MCC or other regions (i.e., convergent validity; Campbell and
Fiske, 1959). Consistent with this possibility, Moser et al. (2013)
provide compelling meta-analytic evidence that error-related signals generated in the MCC are enhanced among anxiety patients
and individuals with heightened negative emotionality. This indicates that negative emotionality, a fundamental dimension of
childhood temperament and adult personality (Caspi et al., 2005),
involves systematic differences in the way that the brain responds
to prompts for cognitive control.
McDermott et al. (2013) describe important new evidence,
gleaned from the study of Romanian orphans, that MCC control signals are plastic. In particular, they demonstrate that
MCC-generated control signals are profoundly shaped by early
experience in ways that confer risk or resilience for later socioemotional problems. This underscores the need to clarify the
neurodevelopmental mechanisms that serve to integrate emotion
and cognition in the laboratory and in daily life.

UNDERSTANDING THE INTERPLAY OF EMOTION AND


COGNITION: STRATEGIES FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
Despite substantial progress, a number of important questions
about the interaction of emotion and cognition remain unanswered. In this nal section, we highlight three strategies for
enhancing research in the cognitive-affective sciences (for more
general recommendations about best research practices, see
Button et al., 2013a,b,c; David et al., 2013; Chalmers et al., 2014;
Ioannidis et al., 2014a,b).
UNDERSTANDING THE SIGNIFICANCE OF EMOTIONAL-COGNITION
INTERACTIONS IN THE LABORATORY REQUIRES MORE SOPHISTICATED
MEASURES OF BEHAVIOR IN THE REAL WORLD

Most investigations of emotion, cognition, and their interplay


rely on a small number of well-controlled, but highly articial
paradigms for manipulating emotion and cognition (e.g., static
aversive images and threat of shock to elicit anxiety; Coan and
Allen, 2007). Although these methods have afforded a number
of critical insights, their real-world signicance remains poorly
understood. For example, are attentional biases to threat, as
indexed by the dot-probe or other laboratory assays, predictive of
elevated behavioral inhibition or distress in daily life? Is amygdala
activation to fearful faces predictive of heightened social reticence

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 6

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

or risk avoidance outside the scanner (see Admon et al., 2009


for preliminary afrmative evidence)? Does the eliciting stimulus
(e.g., faces or aversive images) matter? Are measures of functional
connectivity or network-based metrics (e.g., node centrality; cf.
McMenamin et al., 2014) more predictive than regional activation
of behavior in the real world?
Given the limitations of ambulatory measures of brain
activitythere is no fMRI helmet as yetaddressing these fundamental questions requires pairing assays of brain and behavior
obtained in the laboratory with measures of thoughts, feelings,
and behavior obtained in the eld. Recent work combining fMRI
with ecological momentary assessment (EMA) techniques, in
which surveys are repeatedly delivered to participants mobile
devices, highlights the value of this approach for identifying the
neural systems underlying naturalistic variation in mood and
behavior, a central goal of psychology, psychiatry, and the behavioral neurosciences (Forbes et al., 2009; Berkman and Falk, 2013;
Lopez et al., 2014; Wilson et al., 2014). The widespread dissemination of smart phone technology affords additional, largely
unrealized opportunities for objectively and unobtrusively quantifying daily behavior (e.g., assessments of activity and context
based on accelerometer and geographical positioning system data
(Gosling and Mason, 2015). In short, combining EMA with laboratory assays provides a critical means of testing theoretical
validity and clinical relevance (e.g., does activation of the ventral striatum support craving and approach?), a novel strategy for
assessing and dissociating the functional signicance of new assays
and derivative measures (e.g., functional connectivity between
the striatum and PFC), and an impetus for the development
of laboratory probes that more closely resemble the challenges
we routinely encounter in life (e.g., appetitive social cues and
temptations).
UNDERSTANDING THE INTERPLAY OF EMOTION AND COGNITION
REQUIRES A DYNAMIC NETWORK PERSPECTIVE

Emotion and cognition emerge from the dynamic interactions of


large-scale brain networks. Put simply, fear, joy, attention, working
memory, and other psychological constructs cannot be mapped to
isolated brain regions because no one region is both necessary and
sufcient. Likewise, similar proles of impairment can emerge
from damage to different regions located within in the same functional network (Karnath and Smith, 2014; Oler et al., in press). This
is not a new or contentious idea; pioneers like Mesulam, GoldmanRakic, and LeDoux highlighted the importance of distributed
neural circuits more than two decades ago and there is widespread
agreement amongst basic and translational researchers (GoldmanRakic, 1988; LeDoux, 1995; Mesulam, 1998; Bullmore and Sporns,
2012; LeDoux, 2012; Uhlhaas and Singer, 2012; Anticevic et al.,
2013).
Thus, understanding the interplay of emotion and cognition requires that we accelerate the transition from localization strategies (i.e., mapping isolated brain structures to function; sometimes termed neo-phrenology) to a network-centered
approach. This will require harnessing the kinds of analytic
tools (e.g., functional connectivity ngerprinting, graph-theoretic
and machine-learning approaches) that are necessary for elucidating how adaptive and maladaptive behavior emerges from

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

functional coalitions of brain regions (Kinnison et al., 2012; Raz


et al., 2012, 2014; Anticevic et al., 2013; McMenamin et al., 2014;
Uddin et al., 2014). A key challenge for future research will
be to harness new techniques (e.g., EEG/fMRI fusions, sliding window analyses of functional connectivity, EEG source
models of connectivity) for understanding how network activity dynamically changes across the broad range of time scales
on which emotion and cognition interact (Pessoa and Adolphs,
2010; Shackman et al., 2011a; Johnson et al., 2012; Raz et al., 2012,
2014).
Computationally explicit strategies (i.e., where quantitative
parameters of an abstract computational model are t to behavioral or physiological measures), already common in the neuroeconomics literature, and information-based approaches, such as
multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA), that are increasingly common in the cognitive neuroscience literature, provide powerful
tools for discovering the functional signicance of regions and
networks associated with emotional and cognitive perturbations
and disorders (e.g., Hartley and Phelps, 2012; Montague et al.,
2012; Lewis-Peacock and Norman, 2013). For example, traditional univariate fMRI analyses use regression to predict the
activity of voxels, one-by-one, given some mental state (e.g.,
experiencing pain). While this strategy has proven enormously
generative, it does not provide strong evidence as to whether
overlapping patterns of fMRI activation (e.g., during physical
and social pain; Wager et al., 2013; Woo et al., 2014) reect
the same mental representation. MVPA provides a means of
addressing this problem. MVPA classies mental states given a
pattern of activity across voxels; in effect, treating each voxel
as a weighted source of information about mental state. This
contributes to the identication of the combinatorial code (i.e.,
pattern of activity across voxels) instantiating a particular mental state (e.g., experiencing anxiety) and to test whether that
neural signature is reinstated at other times (e.g., performing a
cognitive control task), an essential step in elucidating the functional contributions of territories that are commonly recruited by
cognitive and emotional challenges (e.g., dlPFC, MCC, anterior
insula).
Embracing a network perspective also reminds us that the functional circuitry underlying the interplay of emotion and cognition
is likely to be complex and need not recapitulate the simpler pattern of direct projections revealed by invasive anatomical tracing
techniques [cf. the contributions from John et al. (2013), Morrison et al. (2013), and Rolls (2013)]. Indeed, there is ample evidence
of robust functional connectivity between brain regions that lack
direct structural connections and increasing evidence that regulatory signals can rapidly propagate across complex, indirect
pathways in ways that enable emotion (e.g., motivational salience
or value) to be integrated with perception and other kinds of
on-going information processing (Vincent et al., 2007; Ekstrom
et al., 2008; Honey et al., 2009; Pessoa and Adolphs, 2010; Adachi
et al., 2012; Birn et al., 2014a). Deciphering the functional signicance of this connectomic complexity is likely to require more
advanced analytic approaches, such as probabilistic machinelearning techniques (Murphy, 2012). The combination of ongoing
advances in computational methods as well as developments in
brain imaging acquisition techniques (e.g., those supported by

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 7

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

the U.S. BRAIN initiative) will undoubtedly contribute to these


efforts.
UNDERSTANDING THE INTERPLAY OF EMOTION AND COGNITION
REQUIRES MECHANISTIC RESEARCH

Most of the contributors to the Special Research Topic used noninvasive techniques, such as fMRI, to trace associations between
emotion and cognition, on the one hand, and brain function
on the other. Aside from unresolved questions about the origins and signicance of the measured signals (e.g., Logothetis,
2008), the most important limitation of these techniques is that
they do not address causation. A crucial challenge for future
studies is to develop a mechanistic understanding of the distributed networks that support the interplay of emotion and
cognition. This can be achieved by combining mechanistic techniques (e.g., optogenetics) or invasive analyses of neuromolecular
pathways in animal models with the same whole-brain imaging
strategies routinely applied in humans (Borsook et al., 2006; Lerman et al., 2007; Fox et al., 2010, 2012; Lee et al., 2010; Desai
et al., 2011; Casey et al., 2013; Narayanan et al., 2013; Roseboom
et al., 2014). Similar strategies can be used with patients with
circumscribed brain damage (e.g., Nomura et al., 2010; Gratton et al., 2012; Motzkin et al., 2014). Combining fMRI or EEG
with non-invasive perturbation techniques (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation) or
pharmacological manipulations provides another opportunity for
understanding how regional changes in brain activity alter network function and, ultimately, behavior (Paulus et al., 2005;
Guller et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2013; Reinhart and Woodman,
2014). Prospective longitudinal designs represent another fruitful
approach to identifying candidate mechanisms, especially in relation to the development of neuropsychiatric disorders (Admon
et al., 2013).

CONCLUSION
The last decade has witnessed an explosion of interest in the interplay of emotion and cognition. The research embodied in this
Special Research Topic highlights the tremendous advances that
have already been made. In particular, this work demonstrates
that emotional cues, emotional states, and emotional traits can
strongly inuence key elements of on-going information processing, including selective attention, working memory, and cognitive
control. Often, this inuence persists beyond the duration of
transient emotional challenges, perhaps reecting slower changes
in neurochemistry. In turn, circuits involved in attention and
working memory contribute to the voluntary regulation of emotion. The distinction between the emotional and the cognitive
brain is blurry and context-dependent. Indeed, there is compelling evidence that territories (e.g., dlPFC, MCC) and processes
(e.g., working memory, cognitive control) conventionally associated with cognition play a central role in emotion. Furthermore,
putatively emotional and cognitive regions dynamically inuence one another via a complex web of recurrent, often indirect
anatomical connections in ways that jointly contribute to adaptive behavior. Collectively, these observations show that emotion
and cognition are deeply interwoven in the fabric of the brain,
suggesting that widely held beliefs about the key constituents of

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

the emotional brain and the cognitive brain are fundamentally


awed.
Developing a deeper understanding will require a greater
emphasis on (a) assessing the real-world relevance of laboratory assays, including measures of brain activity; (b) a network approach to characterizing the neurobiology of emotion
cognition interactions, and (c) mechanistic research. Adopting
these strategies mandates collaboration among researchers from
different disciplines, with expertise in different species, populations, measurement tools, analytic strategies, and conceptual
approaches.
Addressing the interplay of emotion and cognition is a matter of
theoretical as well as practical importance. In particular, many of
the most common and costly neuropsychiatric disordersanxiety,
depression, schizophrenia, substance abuse, chronic pain, autism,
and so oninvolve prominent disturbances of cognition and
emotion (Millan, 2013). Fundamentally, they are disorders of the
emotional-cognitive brain. Collectively, these disorders far outstrip the global burden of cancer or cardiovascular disease (Collins
et al., 2011; Whiteford et al., 2013; DiLuca and Olesen, 2014),
underscoring the importance of accelerating efforts to understand
the neural systems underlying the interaction and integration of
emotion and cognition.

GLOSSARY OF TERMS NOT DEFINED IN THE MAIN TEXT


Affect: The experience or expression of emotion (see also Barrett
et al., 2007).
Anxiety: A sustained state of heightened apprehension in response
to uncertain, distal, or diffuse threat (Davis et al., 2010).
Cognition: Cognition is a fuzzy category that conventionally
includes processes involved in knowing or thinking, including
attention, imagination, language, learning, memory, and perception (for discussion, see Duncan and Barrett, 2007).
Emotion: Like cognition, emotion is a fuzzy, contentious category that conventionally includes valenced processes (e.g., action
tendencies, attention, overt behavior, subjective feelings, and alterations in peripheral physiology) that are triggered by specic
external or internal stimuli (e.g., actual or remembered threat for
fear); often taken to include states of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, and sadness (e.g., Ekman and Davidson, 1994; Duncan and
Barrett, 2007; Gendron and Barrett, 2009; LeDoux, 2012, 2014).
Mood: A low-intensity emotional state that persists in the absence
of an explicit triggering stimulus (Ekman and Davidson, 1994).
Motivation: Internal states that are elicited by reinforcers and
serve to organize behavioral direction (i.e., approach or avoidance)
and intensity. Emotional states involve alterations in motivation
(e.g., increased avoidance in the case of fear). However, motivation can be altered by homeostatic processes, such as hunger
and satiety, that are not conventionally considered emotional
(Rolls, 1999).
Neuroticism/Negative Emotionality: A fundamental dimension
of childhood temperament and adult personality; individuals with
high levels of Neuroticism/Negative Emotionality are susceptible
to more intense or long-lasting negative emotions, including anger,

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 8

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

anxiety, fear, guilt, and sadness (Watson and Clark, 1984; Caspi
et al., 2005).
Reinforcer: Rewards and punishments; anything an organism will
work to approach or avoid (Rolls, 1999).

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
All the authors supervised the Special Research Topic. Hadas
Okon-Singer and Alexander J. Shackman wrote the paper. All the
authors edited and revised the paper.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank the many contributors and staff who made the Special
Research Topic possible. We acknowledge the assistance of L.
Friedman and support of the European Commission (Followship #334206 to Hadas Okon-Singer and Grant #602186 to Talma
Hendler), Israeli Center of Research Excellence, Israeli Science
Foundation (Grant #51/11 to Talma Hendler), National Institute
of Mental Health (MH071589 to Luiz Pessoa), and University of
Maryland (Alexander J. Shackman and Luiz Pessoa).

REFERENCES
Adachi, Y., Osada, T., Sporns, O., Watanabe, T., Matsui, T., Miyamoto, K.,
et al. (2012). Functional connectivity between anatomically unconnected areas
is shaped by collective network-level effects in the macaque cortex. Cereb. Cortex
22, 15861592. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr234
Admon, R., Lubin, G., Rosenblatt, J. D., Stern, O., Kahn, I., Assaf, M., et al. (2012).
Imbalanced neural responsivity to risk and reward indicates stress vulnerability
in humans. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2835. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr369
Admon, R., Lubin, G., Stern, O., Rosenberg, K., Sela, L., Ben-Ami, H., et al.
(2009). Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdalas predisposition and
hippocampal plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1412014125. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0903183106
Admon, R., Milad, M. R., and Hendler, T. (2013). A causal model of post-traumatic
stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities.
Trends Cogn. Sci 17, 337347. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.005
Anticevic, A., Cole, M. W., Repovs, G., Savic, A., Driesen, N. R., Yang, G., et al.
(2013). Connectivity, pharmacology, and computation: toward a mechanistic
understanding of neural system dysfunction in schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry
4:169. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00169
Arend, I., Henik, A., and Okon-Singer, H. (2014). Dissociating emotion and attention functions in the Pulvinar nucleus of the Thalamus. Neuropsychology. doi:
10.1037/neu0000139 [Epub ahead of print].
Arnsten, A. F. (1998). The biology of being frazzled. Science 280, 17111712. doi:
10.1126/science.280.5370.1711
Arnsten, A. F. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 410422. doi: 10.1038/
nrn2648
Arnsten, A. F., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1998). Noise stress impairs prefrontal
cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic
mechanism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 362368. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.
4.362
Aue, T., Guex, R., Chauvigne, L. A., and Okon-Singer, H. (2013a). Varying expectancies and attention bias in phobic and non-phobic individuals. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:418. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00418
Aue, T., Hoeppli, M. E., Piguet, C., Sterpenich, V., and Vuilleumier, P.
(2013b). Visual avoidance in phobia: particularities in neural activity, autonomic
responding, and cognitive risk evaluations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:194. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00194
Awh, E., and Vogel, E. K. (2008). The bouncer in the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 56.
doi: 10.1038/nn0108-5
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Van,
I. M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious
individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 124. doi: 10.1037/00332909.133.1.1

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

Bar-Haim, Y., and Pine, D. S. (2013). Cognitive training research and the search for a
transformative, translational, developmental cognitive neuroscience. Dev. Cogn.
Neurosci. 4, 12. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.001
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2007).
The experience of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 373. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709
Barrett, L. F., and Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and
social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 361372. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012
Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control decits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you dont. Biol. Psychol. 92, 440446. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007
Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2014). Inhibitory decits in trait anxiety: increased
stimulus-based or response-based interference? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 1339
1345. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0611-8
Berggren, N., Richards, A., Taylor, J., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Affective attention under cognitive load: reduced emotional biases but emergent
anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:188. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00188
Berghorst, L. H., Bogdan, R., Frank, M. J., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Acute
stress selectively reduces reward sensitivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:133. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00133
Berkman, E. T., and Falk, E. B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:4550. doi:
10.1177/0963721412469394
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., Mcfarlin, D. R., Rogers,
G. M., et al. (2014a). Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar dysfunction
in early-life anxiety. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 915922. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.46
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., Mcfarlin, D. R., Rogers,
G. M., et al. (2014b). Extreme early-life anxiety is associated with an evolutionarily
conserved reduction in the strength of intrinsic functional connectivity between
the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the central nucleus of the amygdala. Mol.
Psychiatry 19, 853. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.85
Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 307316. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008
Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention.
Nat. Neurosci. 12, 9298. doi: 10.1038/nn.2242
Bishop, S. J., and Forster, S. (2013). Trait anxiety, neuroticism and the brain basis
of vulnerability to affective disorder, in The Cambridge Handbook of Human
Affective Neuroscience, eds J. Armony and P. Vuilleumier (Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press), 553574. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511843716.031
Blackford, J. U., Avery, S. N., Shelton, R. C., and Zald, D. H. (2009). Amygdala temporal dynamics: temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to
familiar and novel faces. BMC Neurosci. 10:145. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-145
Blackford, J. U., and Pine, D. S. (2012). Neural substrates of childhood anxiety
disorders: a review of neuroimaging ndings. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N.
Am. 21, 501525. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2012.05.002
Bogdan, R., Perlis, R. H., Fagerness, J., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2010). The
impact of mineralocorticoid receptor ISO/VAL genotype (rs5522) and stress on
reward learning. Genes Brain Behav. 9, 658667. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.
00600.x
Bogdan, R., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 11471154. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037
Bolger, N., and Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of everyday life:
the role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. J. Pers. 59,
355386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00253.x
Borsook, D., Becerra, L., and Hargreaves, R. (2006). A role for fMRI in optimizing CNS drug development. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 5, 411424. doi: 10.1038/
nrd2027
Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conict monitoring and decision making: reconciling
two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7,
356366. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.356
Braver, T. S., Krug, M. K., Chiew, K. S., Kool, W., Westbrook, J. A., Clement, N. J.,
et al. (2014). Mechanisms of motivation-cognition interaction: challenges and
opportunities. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 443472. doi: 10.3758/s13415014-0300-0
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H.,
et al. (2013). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 9

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 24, 29812990. doi: 10.1093/cercor/


bht154
Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S.,
et al. (2013a). Condence and precision increase with high statistical power. Nat.
Rev. Neurosci. 14, 585586. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475-c4
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S.,
et al. (2013b). Empirical evidence for low reproducibility indicates low pre-study
odds. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 877. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475-c6
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S.,
et al. (2013c). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of
neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 365376. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475
Campbell, D. T., and Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol. Bull. 56, 81105. doi:
10.1037/h0046016
Canli, T., Omura, K., Haas, B. W., Fallgatter, A., Constable, R. T., and Lesch, K. P.
(2005). Beyond affect: a role for genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in
neural activation during a cognitive attention task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
102, 1222412229. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503880102
Carreti, L. (2014). Exogenous (automatic) attention to emotional stimuli: a review.
Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 1228-1258. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0270-2
Casey, B., Craddock, N., Cuthbert, B. N., Hyman, S. E., Lee, F. S., and Ressler, K. J.
(2013). DSM-5 and RDoC: progress in psychiatry research? Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
14, 810814. doi: 10.1038/nrn3621
Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., and Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: stability and change. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 453484. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913
Cavanagh, J. F., and Shackman, A. J. (2014). Frontal midline theta reects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J. Physiol. Paris. doi:
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003 [Epub ahead of print].
Chalmers, I., Bracken, M. B., Djulbegovic, B., Garattini, S., Grant, J., Gulmezoglu, A. M., et al. (2014). How to increase value and reduce waste when
research priorities are set. Lancet 383, 156165. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)
62229-1
Chen, A. C., Oathes, D. J., Chang, C., Bradley, T., Zhou, Z. -W., Williams, L. M., et al.
(2013). Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and defaultmode networks in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 1994419949. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1311772110
Clarke, R., and Johnstone, T. (2013). Prefrontal inhibition of threat processing reduces working memory interference. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:228. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00228
Coan, J. A., and Allen, J. J. B. (2007). Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, N., Daches, S., Mor, N., and Henik, A. (2014). Inhibition of negative
content-a shared process in rumination and reappraisal. Front. Psychol. 5:622.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00622
Collins, P. Y., Patel, V., Joestl, S. S., March, D., Insel, T. R., Daar, A. S., et al.
(2011). Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature 475, 2730. doi:
10.1038/475027a
Crocker, L. D., Heller, W., Warren, S. L., Ohare, A. J., Infantolino, Z. P., and Miller,
G. A. (2013). Relationships among cognition, emotion, and motivation: implications for intervention and neuroplasticity in psychopathology. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:261. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00261
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Alexander, A. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2007). Gazexation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 512520. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.
05.019
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A.,
Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze xation and the neural circuitry of face
processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 519526. doi: 10.1038/nn1421
Damasio, A. (2005a). Descartes Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain.
New York, NY: Penguin.
Damasio, A. (2005b). Human behaviour: brain trust. Nature 435, 571572. doi:
10.1038/435571a
Darwin, C. (1872/2009). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, 4th
Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Darwin, C. (1872). The Origin of Species. London: John Murray.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

David, S. P., Ware, J. J., Chu, I. M., Loftus, P. D., Fusar-Poli, P., Radua, J., et al. (2013).
Potential reporting bias in fMRI studies of the brain. PLoS ONE 8:e70104. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0070104
Davidson, R. J. (2004). Well-being and affective style: neural substrates and biobehavioural correlates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 13951411. doi:
10.1098/rstb.2004.1510
Davis, M., Walker, D. L., Miles, L., and Grillon, C. (2010). Phasic vs sustained
fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 105135. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.109
Davis, M., and Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol.
Psychiatry 6, 1334. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812
DEsposito, M., and Postle, B. R. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of working
memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 115142. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814015031
de Sousa, R. (2014). Emotion, in Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta (Stanford, CA: Stanford University). Available at:
http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/
Desai, M., Kahn, I., Knoblich, U., Bernstein, J., Atallah, H., Yang, A., et al. (2011).
Mapping brain networks in awake mice using combined optical neural control
and fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 13931405. doi: 10.1152/jn.00828.2010
Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.
001205
DiLuca, M., and Olesen, J. (2014). The cost of brain diseases: a burden or a challenge?
Neuron 82, 12051208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.044
Dolcos, F., and Denkova, E. (2014). Current emotion research in cognitive neuroscience: linking enhancing and impairing effects of emotion on cognition. Emot.
Rev. 6, 362375. doi: 10.1177/1754073914536449
Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (2012). The role of affect and reward in the conicttriggered adjustment of cognitive control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:342. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00342
Duncan, S., and Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: a neurobiological analysis. Cogn. Emot. 21, 11841211. doi: 10.1080/026999307014
37931
Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: control, representation, and time
course. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 269297. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269
Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Burns, L. R., and Schwerdtfeger, A. (2006). Spontaneous
emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: associations with negative
affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion 6,
356366. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.356
Ehring, T., Tuschen-Cafer, B., Schnulle, J., Fischer, S., and Gross, J. J.
(2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion 10, 563572.
doi: 10.1037/a0019010
Ekman, P. E., and Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of Emotion: Fundamental
Questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ekstrom, L. B., Roelfsema, P. R., Arsenault, J. T., Bonmassar, G., and Vanduffel, W.
(2008). Bottom-up dependent gating of frontal signals in early visual cortex.
Science 321, 414417. doi: 10.1126/science.1153276
Etkin, A. (2012). Neurobiology of anxiety: from neural circuits to novel solutions?
Depress Anxiety 29, 355358. doi: 10.1002/da.21957
Etkin, A., Gyurak, A., and Ohara, R. (2013). A neurobiological approach to the
cognitive decits of psychiatric disorders. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 419429.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety
and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336353. doi:
10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
Forbes, E. E., Hariri, A. R., Martin, S. L., Silk, J. S., Moyles, D. L., Fisher,
P. M., et al. (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive
affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 6473. doi:
10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336
Fox, A. S., and Kalin, N. H. (2014). A translational neuroscience approach to understanding the development of social anxiety disorder and its pathophysiology. Am.
J. Psychiatry doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040449 [Epub ahead of print].
Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shelton, S. E., Nanda, S. A., Davidson, R. J., Roseboom,
P. H., et al. (2012). Central amygdala nucleus (Ce) gene expression linked to
increased trait-like Ce metabolism and anxious temperament in young primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1810818113. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206
723109

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 10

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Oakes, T. R., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J., and
Kalin, N. H. (2010). Orbitofrontal cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity
in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. J. Neurosci. 30, 70237027. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5952-09.2010
Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Oakes, T. R., Davidson, R. J., and Kalin, N. H. (2008). Traitlike brain activity during adolescence predicts anxious temperament in primates.
PLoS ONE 3:e2570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002570
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., and
Ghera, M. M. (2005a). Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior
within a developmental framework. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 235262. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532
Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R., Shelton, S. E., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J., and Kalin,
N. H. (2005b). Calling for help is independently modulated by brain systems
underlying goal-directed behavior and threat perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A. 102, 41764179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409470102
Frank, D. W., Dewitt, M., Hudgens-Haney, M., Schaeffer, D. J., Ball, B. H., Schwarz,
N. F., et al. (2014). Emotion regulation: quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45C, 202211. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010
Gendron, M., and Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the past: a century of ideas about emotion in psychology. Emot. Rev. 1, 316339. doi:
10.1177/1754073909338877
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137156. doi:
10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033
Gosling, S. D., and Mason, W. (2015). Internet research in psychology. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 66, 877902. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015321
Gratton, C., Nomura, E. M., Perez, F., and Desposito, M. (2012). Focal brain
lesions to critical locations cause widespread disruption of the modular organization of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 12751285. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a00222
Gray, J. A., and McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry
into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford Psychology
Press.
Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
74, 224237. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
Gross, J. J. (1998b). The emerging eld of emotion regulation: an integrative review.
Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., and Urry, H. L. (2011). Cognition and emotion lecture at the 2010 spsp emotion preconference. Cogn. Emot. 25, 765781. doi:
10.1080/02699931.2011.555753
Gross, J. J., and Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual
foundations, in Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Guilford: Guilford Press.
3, 24.
Grupe, D. W., and Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety:
an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
14, 488501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524
Guller, Y., Tononi, G., and Postle, B. R. (2012). Conserved functional connectivity
but impaired effective connectivity of thalamocortical circuitry in schizophrenia.
Brain Connect. 2, 311319. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0100
Hajcak, G., and Olvet, D. M. (2008). The persistence of attention to emotion:
brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion 8, 250255. doi:
10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.250
Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E.,
et al. (2010). Attention bias modication treatment: a meta-analysis toward the
establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 982990. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making
involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646648. doi:
10.1126/science.1168450
Hare, T. A., ODoherty, J., Camerer, C. F., Schultz, W., and Rangel, A. (2008).
Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 28, 56235630. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1309-08.2008
Harl, K. M., Shenoy, P., and Paulus, M. P. (2013). The inuence of emotions on
cognitive control: feelings and beliefs-where do they meet? Front. Hum. Neurosci.
7:508. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00508

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

Hartley, C. A., and Phelps, E. A. (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biol.


Psychiatry 72, 113118. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027
Heller, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Mayer, K. E., and Davidson, R. J. (2014). The face of negative affect: trial-by-trial corrugator responses to negative pictures are positively
associated with amygdala and negatively associated with ventromedial prefrontal
cortex activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 21022110. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00622
Henderson, H. A., Pine, D. S., and Fox, N. A. (2014). Behavioral inhibition and
developmental risk: a dual-processing perspective. Neuropsychopharmacology. 40,
207224. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.189
Holtmann, J., Herbort, M. C., Wustenberg, T., Soch, J., Richter, S., Walter, H.,
et al. (2013). Trait anxiety modulates fronto-limbic processing of emotional
interference in borderline personality disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:54. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00054
Honey, C. J., Sporns, O., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Thiran, J. P., Meuli, R.,
et al. (2009). Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from
structural connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 20352040. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0811168106
Ioannidis, J. P., Greenland, S., Hlatky, M. A., Khoury, M. J., Macleod, M. R.,
Moher, D., et al. (2014a). Increasing value and reducing waste in research
design, conduct, and analysis. Lancet 383, 166175. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)
62227-8
Ioannidis, J. P., Munafo, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., and David, S. P. (2014b).
Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: detection, prevalence,
and prevention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 235241. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010
Iordan, A. D., Dolcos, S., and Dolcos, F. (2013). Neural signatures of the response
to emotional distraction: a review of evidence from brain imaging investigations.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:200. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00200
John, Y. J., Bullock, D., Zikopoulos, B., and Barbas, H. (2013). Anatomy and computational modeling of networks underlying cognitive-emotional interaction. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. 7:101. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00101
Johnson, J. S., Kundu, B., Casali, A. G., and Postle, B. R. (2012). Task-dependent
changes in cortical excitability and effective connectivity: a combined TMS-EEG
study. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 23832392. doi: 10.1152/jn.00707.2011
Kalanthroff, E., Cohen, N., and Henik, A. (2013). Stop feeling: inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:78. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00078
Karnath, H. O., and Smith, D. V. (2014). The next step in modern brain
lesion analysis: multivariate pattern analysis. Brain 137, 24052407. doi:
10.1093/brain/awu180
Kendler, K. S., and Gardner, C. O. (2014). Sex differences in the pathways to major
depression: a study of opposite-sex twin pairs. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 426435.
doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13101375
Kessel, E. M., Huselid, R. F., Decicco, J. M., and Dennis, T. A. (2013). Neurophysiological processing of emotion and parenting interact to predict inhibited
behavior: an affective-motivational framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:326. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00326
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., and Wittchen,
H. U. (2012). Twelve month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of
anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res.
21, 169184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359
Kinnison, J., Padmala, S., Choi, J.-M., and Pessoa, L. (2012). Network analysis reveals
increased integration during emotional and motivational processing. J. Neurosci.
32, 83618372. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0821-12.2012
Koenigs, M., Huey, E. D., Calamia, M., Raymont, V., Tranel, D., and Grafman, J.
(2008). Distinct regions of prefrontal cortex mediate resistance and vulnerability
to depression. J. Neurosci. 28, 1234112348. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.232408.2008
Kool, W., Mcguire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., and Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making
and the avoidance of cognitive demand. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 139, 665. doi:
10.1037/a0020198
Lapate, R. C., Van Reekum, C. M., Schaefer, S. M., Greischar, L. L., Norris,
C. J., Bachhuber, D. R., et al. (2014). Prolonged marital stress is associated
with short-lived responses to positive stimuli. Psychophysiology 51, 499509. doi:
10.1111/psyp.12203
LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 46, 209235.
doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233
LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73, 653676. doi:
10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 11

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

LeDoux, J. E. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111,
28712878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400335111
Lee, H., Shackman, A. J., Jackson, D. C., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Test-retest
reliability of voluntary emotion regulation. Psychophysiology 46, 874879. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00830.x
Lee, J. H., Durand, R., Gradinaru, V., Zhang, F., Goshen, I., Kim, D. S., et al. (2010).
Global and local fMRI signals driven by neurons dened optogenetically by type
and wiring. Nature 465, 788792. doi: 10.1038/nature09108
Lerman, C., LeSage, M. G., Perkins, K. A., OMalley, S. S., Siegel, S. J., Benowitz,
N. L., et al. (2007). Translational research in medication development for nicotine
dependence. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 6, 746762. doi: 10.1038/nrd2361
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., and Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision
making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 33.133.25.
Lerner, Y., Singer, N., Gonen, T., Weintraub, Y., Cohen, O., Rubin, N., et al. (2012).
Feeling without seeing? Engagement of ventral, but not dorsal, amygdala during unaware exposure to emotional faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 531542. doi:
10.1162/jocn-a-00165
Levenson, R. W. (1994). I. Human emotion: a functional view. II. The search for
autonomic specicity. III. Emotional control: variation and consequences, in
The Nature of Emotion. Fundamental Questions, eds P. Ekman and R. Davidson
(New York: Oxford University Press).
Lewis-Peacock, J. A., and Norman, K. A. (2013). Multi-voxel pattern analysis of
fMRI data: a review. Comput. Math. Methods Med. 2012, 114.
Lin, T., Vaisvaser, S., Fruchter, E., Admon, R., Wald, I., Pine, D., et al. (2014). A neurobehavioral account for individual differences in resilience to chronic military
stress. Psychol. Med. 113. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002013
Lindquist, K. A., and Barrett, L. F. (2012). A functional architecture of the human
brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16,
533540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.005
Lindstrm, B. R., Mattsson-Mrn, I. B., Golkar, A., and Olsson, A. (2013).
In your face: risk of punishment enhances cognitive control and errorrelated activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle. PLoS ONE 8:e65692. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0065692
Lissek, S. (2012). Toward an account of clinical anxiety predicated on basic, neurally mapped mechanisms of Pavlovian fear-learning: the case for conditioned
overgeneralization. Depress. Anxiety 29, 257263. doi: 10.1002/da.21922
Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature
453, 869878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976
Lopez, R. B., Hofmann, W., Wagner, D. D., Kelley, W. M., and Heatherton, T. F.
(2014). Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 25,
13371344. doi: 10.1177/0956797614531492
MacLeod, C., and Clarke, P. J. (2015). The attentional bias modication approach to
anxiety intervention. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 5878. doi: 10.1177/2167702614560749
MacLeod, C., and Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modication approaches
to anxiety. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8, 189217. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy032511143052
Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., and Whiteman, M. C. (2009). Personality Traits,
3rd Edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO97805118
12743
McDermott, J. M., Troller-Renfree, S., Vanderwert, R., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H.,
and Fox, N. A. (2013). Psychosocial deprivation, executive functions, and the
emergence of socio-emotional behavior problems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:167.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00167
McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., and Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink: what we know so far. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:151. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00151
McMenamin, B. W., Langeslag, S. J., Sirbu, M., Padmala, S., and Pessoa, L. (2014).
Network organization unfolds over time during periods of anxious anticipation.
J. Neurosci. 34, 1126111273. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1579-14.2014
McNab, F., and Klingberg, T. (2007). Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control
access to working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 103107. doi: 10.1038/nn2024
Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain 121, 10131052. doi:
10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
Millan, M. J. (2013). An epigenetic framework for neurodevelopmental disorders: from pathogenesis to potential therapy. Neuropharmacology 68, 282. doi:
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.11.015
Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex
function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

Mohanty, A., and Sussman, T. J. (2013). Top-down modulation of attention by


emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:102. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00102
Montague, P. R., Dolan, R. J., Friston, K. J., and Dayan, P. (2012). Computational
psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 7280. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.018
Morrison, I., Perini, I., and Dunham, J. (2013). Facets and mechanisms of adaptive
pain behavior: predictive regulation and action. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:755. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00755
Morriss, J., Taylor, A. N., Roesch, E. B., and Van Reekum, C. M. (2013). Still feeling
it: the time course of emotional recovery from an attentional perspective. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. 7:201. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00201
Moser, J. S., Moran, T. P., Schroder, H. S., Donnellan, M. B., and Yeung, N. (2013). On
the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: a meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:466. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00466
Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Wolf, R. C., Baskaya, M. K., and Koenigs, M. (2014).
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions alter neural and physiological correlates
of anticipation. J. Neurosci. 34, 1043010437. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.144614.2014
Murphy, K. P. (2012). Machine Learning: A Probabilistic Perspective. Cambridge,
MA: MIT press.
Narayanan, N. S., Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., and Laubach, M. (2013). Common
medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nat.
Neurosci. 16, 18881895. doi: 10.1038/nn.3549
Nomura, E. M., Gratton, C., Visser, R. M., Kayser, A., Perez, F., and Desposito, M.
(2010). Double dissociation of two cognitive control networks in patients
with focal brain lesions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 1201712022. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1002431107
Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. Willed and automatic
control of behavior, in Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research
and Theory, eds R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, and D. Shapiro (New York, NY:
Plenum Press), 118.
Okon-Singer, H., Alyagon, U., Kofman, O., Tzelgov, J., and Henik, A. (2011). Fearrelated pictures deteriorate the performance of university students with high fear
of snakes or spiders. Stress 14, 185193. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.527401
Okon-Singer, H., Lichtenstein-Vidne, L., and Cohen, N. (2013). Dynamic modulation of emotional processing. Biol. Psychol. 92, 480491. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.
2012.05.010
Okon-Singer, H., Mehnert, J., Hoyer, J., Hellrung, L., Schaare, H. L., Dukart, J., et al.
(2014a). Neural control of vascular reactions: impact of emotion and attention.
J. Neurosci. 34, 42514259. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0747-13.2014
Okon-Singer, H., Pessoa, L., Hendler, T., and Shackman, A. J. (2014b).
Introduction to the special research topic on the neurobiology of emotioncognition interactions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:1051. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.
01051
Okon-Singer, H., Tzelgov, J., and Henik, A. (2007). Distinguishing between automaticity and attention in the processing of emotionally signicant stimuli.
Emotion 7, 147157. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.147
Oler, J. A., Fox, A. S., Shackman, A. J., and Kalin, N. H. (in press). The central
nucleus of the amygdala is a critical substrate for individual differences in anxiety,
in Living Without an Amygdala, eds D. G. Amaral, M. Bauman, and R. Adolphs
(New York, NY: Guilford Press).
Paulus, M. P., Feinstein, J. S., Leland, D., and Simmons, A. N. (2005). Superior
temporal gyrus and insula provide response and outcome-dependent information during assessment and action selection in a decision-making situation.
Neuroimage 25, 607615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.055
Peers, P. V., Simons, J. S., and Lawrence, A. D. (2013). Prefrontal control of attention
to threat. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:24. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00024
Pessoa, L. (2012). Beyond brain regions: network perspective of cognitionemotion interactions. Behav. Brain Sci. 35, 158159. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X110
01567
Pessoa, L. (2013). The Cognitive-Emotional Brain: From Interactions to Integration. Cambridge: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262019569.
001.0001
Pessoa, L., and Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a
low road to many roads of evaluating biological signicance. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
11, 773783. doi: 10.1038/nrn2920
Pessoa, L., Mckenna, M., Gutierrez, E., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural
processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
99, 1145811463. doi: 10.1073/pnas.172403899

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 12

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., and Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between
cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion 12, 192197. doi:
10.1037/a0024109
Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and
integrated model. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 393423. doi: 10.1146/annurevclinpsy-050212-185606
Pizzagalli, D. A., Bogdan, R., Ratner, K. G., and Jahn, A. L. (2007). Increased
perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 27422753. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.013
Pochon, J. B., Riis, J., Sanfey, A. G., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2008).
Functional imaging of decision conict. J. Neurosci. 28, 34683473. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4195-07.2008
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and
brain. Neuroscience 139, 2338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005
Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional inuences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol.
Psychol. 92, 492512. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007
Proudt, G. H., Inzlicht, M., and Mennin, D. S. (2013). Anxiety and error monitoring: the importance of motivation and emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:636.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00636
Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J., Luo, J., and Fernndez, G. (2009). Acute
psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 2532. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006
Raz, G., Jacob, Y., Gonen, T., Winetraub, Y., Flash, T., Soreq, E., et al. (2014). Cry for
her or cry with her: context-dependent dissociation of two modes of cinematic
empathy reected in network cohesion dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9,
3038. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst052
Raz, G., Winetraub, Y., Jacob, Y., Kinreich, S., Maron-Katz, A., Shaham, G.,
et al. (2012). Portraying emotions at their unfolding: a multilayered approach
for probing dynamics of neural networks. Neuroimage 60, 14481461. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.084
Reinhart, R. M., and Woodman, G. F. (2014). Oscillatory coupling reveals the
dynamic reorganization of large-scale neural networks as cognitive demands
change. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 175188. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00470
Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., and Weiskrantz, L. E. (1998). The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524410.001.0001
Robinson, O. J., Krimsky, M., and Grillon, C. (2013a). The impact of
induced anxiety on response inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:69. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00069
Robinson, O. J., Vytal, K., Cornwell, B. R., and Grillon, C. (2013b). The impact of
anxiety upon cognition: perspectives from human threat of shock studies. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. 7:203. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00203
Rohr, C. S., Okon-Singer, H., Craddock, R. C., Villringer, A., and Margulies, D. S.
(2013). Affect and the brains functional organization: a resting-state connectivity
approach. PLoS ONE 8:e68015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068015
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The Brain and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rolls, E. T. (2013). A biased activation theory of the cognitive and attentional modulation of emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00074
Roseboom, P. H., Nanda, S. A., Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shackman, A. J., Shelton, S. E.,
et al. (2014). Neuropeptide Y receptor gene expression in the primate amygdala
predicts anxious temperament and brain metabolism. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 850
857. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.012
Sacher, J., Neumann, J., Okon-Singer, H., Gotowiec, S., and Villringer, A. (2012).
Sexual dimorphism in the human brain: evidence from neuroimaging. Magn.
Reson. Imaging 31, 366375. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2012.06.007
Sacher, J., Okon-Singer, H., and Villringer, A. (2013). Evidence from neuroimaging
for the role of the menstrual cycle in the interplay of emotion and cognition.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:374. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00374
Sandi, C. (2013). Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 245261.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.1222
Schick, A., Wessa, M., Vollmayr, B., Kuehner, C., and Kanske, P. (2013). Indirect
assessment of an interpretation bias in humans: neurophysiological and behavioral correlates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:272. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00272
Schmitter, A. M. (2014). 17th and 18th century theories of emotions, in Stanford
Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta (Stanford, CA: Stanford University).
Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotions-17th18th/

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

Schouppe, N., De Houwer, J., Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Notebaert, W. (2012).


Conict: run! Reduced Stroop interference with avoidance responses. Q. J. Exp.
Psychol. (Hove) 65, 10521058. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.685080
Schuyler, B. S., Kral, T. R., Jacquart, J., Burghy, C. A., Weng, H. Y., Perlman,
D. M., et al. (2014). Temporal dynamics of emotional responding: amygdala
recovery predicts emotional traits. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 176181. doi:
10.1093/scan/nss131
Schwabe, L., and Wolf, O. T. (2013). Stress and multiple memory systems: from
thinkingto doing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 6068. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.001
Shackman, A. J., Fox, A. S., and Seminowicz, D. A. (in press). The cognitiveemotional brain: opportunities and challenges for understanding neuropsychiatric disorders. Behav. Brain Sci.
Shackman, A. J., Maxwell, J. S., Mcmenamin, B. W., Greischar, L. L., and Davidson,
R. J. (2011a). Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing.
J. Neurosci. 31, 11561161. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., and
Davidson, R. J. (2011b). The integration of negative affect, pain, and cognitive
control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154167. doi: 10.1038/
nrn2994
Shackman, A. J., Mcmenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., and
Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20, 15001506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.
02476.x
Shackman, A. J., Sarinopoulos, I., Maxwell, J. S., Pizzagalli, D. A., Lavric, A., and
Davidson, R. J. (2006). Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory.
Emotion 6, 4061. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40
Shansky, R. M., and Lipps, J. (2013). Stress-induced cognitive dysfunction:
hormone-neurotransmitter interactions in the prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:123. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00123
Shechner, T., Britton, J. C., Perez-Edgar, K., Bar-Haim, Y., Ernst, M., Fox, N. A., et al.
(2012). Attention biases, anxiety, and development: toward or away from threats
or rewards? Depress Anxiety 29, 282294. doi: 10.1002/da.20914
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of
control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron 79,
217240. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
Shenhav, A., and Buckner, R. L. (2014). Neural correlates of dueling affective reactions to win-win choices. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 1097810983. doi:
10.1073/pnas.1405725111
Sheppes, G., and Levin, Z. (2013). Emotion regulation choice: selecting between
cognitive regulation strategies to control emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:179.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00179
Siman-Tov, T., Papo, D., Gadoth, N., Schonberg, T., Mendelsohn, A., Perry, D.,
et al. (2009). Mind your left: spatial bias in subcortical fear processing. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 21, 17821789. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21120
Singer, N., Eapen, M., Grillon, C., Ungerleider, L. G., and Hendler, T. (2012).
Through the eyes of anxiety: dissecting threat bias via emotional-binocular
rivalry. Emotion 12, 960. doi: 10.1037/a0027070
Southwick, S. M., Vythilingam, M., and Charney, D. S. (2005). The psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: implications for prevention and treatment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1, 255291. doi:
10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143948
Sreenivasan, K. K., Curtis, C. E., and Desposito, M. (2014). Revisiting the role of
persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 8289.
doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001
Stollstorff, M., Munakata, Y., Jensen, A. P., Guild, R. M., Smolker, H. R., Devaney,
J. M., et al. (2013). Individual differences in emotion-cognition interactions:
emotional valence interacts with serotonin transporter genotype to inuence
brain systems involved in emotional reactivity and cognitive control. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:327. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00327
Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J., Johnson, J. S., and Larson, C. L. (2014). Worry is
associated with impaired gating of threat from working memory. Emotion. doi:
10.1037/emo0000015 [Epub ahead of print].
Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J., and Larson, C. L. (2013). Failure to lter: anxious
individuals show inefcient gating of threat from working memory. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:58. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00058
Suls, J., and Martin, R. (2005). The daily life of the gardenvariety neurotic: reactivity, stressor exposure, mood spillover, and maladaptive coping. J. Pers. 73,
14851510. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00356.x

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 13

Okon-Singer et al.

Emotioncognition interactions

Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., and Anderson, A. K.
(2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 843850.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2138
Todd, R. M., and Anderson, A. K. (2013). Salience, State, and expression: the
inuence of specic aspects of emotion on attention and perception. Oxford
Handbook Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 11.
Todd, R. M., Cunningham, W. A., Anderson, A. K., and Thompson, E. (2012).
Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 365372. doi:
10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Jan, K., Asplund, C. L., Ginther,
M. R., et al. (2014). Corticolimbic gating of emotion-driven punishment. Nat.
Neurosci. 17, 12701275. doi: 10.1038/nn.3781
Uddin, L. Q., Kinnison, J., Pessoa, L., and Anderson, M. L. (2014). Beyond the
tripartite cognition-emotion-interoception model of the human insular cortex.
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1627. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00462
Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2012). Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric
disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks.
Neuron 75, 963980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004
Urry, H. L. (2010). Seeing, thinking, and feeling: emotion-regulating effects
of gaze-directed cognitive reappraisal. Emotion 10, 125135. doi: 10.1037/
a0017434
Vaisvaser, S., Lin, T., Admon, R., Podlipsky, I., Greenman, Y., Stern, N., et al.
(2013). Neural traces of stress: cortisol related sustained enhancement of
amygdala-hippocampal functional connectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:313.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00313
van Ast, V., Spicer, J., Smith, E., Schmer-Galunder, S., Liberzon, I., Abelson, J., et al.
(2014). Brain mechanisms of social threat effects on working memory. Cereb.
Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu206 [Epub ahead of print].
Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G.,
and Koster, E. H. (2013). A review of current evidence for the causal impact
of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychol. Bull. 140, 682721. doi:
10.1037/a0034834
Van Dessel, P., and Vogt, J. (2012). When does hearing laughter draw attention to happy faces? Task relevance determines the inuence of a crossmodal
affective context on emotional attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:294. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00294
van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Urry, H. L., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S.,
Alexander, A. L., et al. (2007). Gaze xations predict brain activation during the
voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. Neuroimage 36, 1041
1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.052
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Essen, D. C.,
et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain.
Nature 447, 8386. doi: 10.1038/nature05758
Vogel, E. K., and Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428, 748751. doi:
10.1038/nature02447
Vogel, E. K., Mccollough, A. W., and Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures
reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature
438, 500503. doi: 10.1038/nature04171
Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B. R., Letkiewicz, A. M., Arkin, N. E., and Grillon, C. (2013). The complex interaction between anxiety and cognition: insight
from spatial and verbal working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:93. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00093

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C. W., and Kross, E.
(2013). An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N. Engl. J. Med.
368, 13881397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471
Wald, I., Degnan, K. A., Gorodetsky, E., Charney, D. S., Fox, N. A., Fruchter, E., et al.
(2013). Attention to threats and combat-related posttraumatic stress symptoms:
prospective associations and moderation by the serotonin transporter gene. JAMA
Psychiatry 70, 401408. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.188
Warren, S. L., Crocker, L. D., Spielberg, J. M., Engels, A. S., Banich, M. T.,
Sutton, B. P., et al. (2013). Cortical organization of inhibition-related functions and modulation by psychopathology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:271. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00271
Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to
experience aversive emotional states. Psychol. Bull. 96, 465. doi: 10.1037/00332909.96.3.465
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., and Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a
meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process
model of emotion regulation. Psychol. Bull. 138, 775808. doi: 10.1037/a00
27600
Whiteford, H. A., Degenhardt, L., Rehm, J., Baxter, A. J., Ferrari, A. J., Erskine, H. E.,
et al. (2013). Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use
disorders: ndings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 382,
15751586. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6
Wilson, S. J., Smyth, J. M., and Maclean, R. R. (2014). Integrating ecological momentary assessment and functional brain imaging methods: new avenues for studying
and treating tobacco dependence. Nicotine Tob. Res. 16(Suppl. 2), S102S110.
doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt129
Woo, C. W., Koban, L., Kross, E., Lindquist, M. A., Banich, M. T., Ruzic, L., et al.
(2014). Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection.
Nat. Commun. 5, 5380. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6380
Xing, C., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Aiming at happiness: how motivation affects
attention to and memory for emotional images. Motiv. Emot. 30, 243250. doi:
10.1007/s11031-006-9032-y
Zaretsky, M., Mendelsohn, A., Mintz, M., and Hendler, T. (2010). In the eye
of the beholder: internally driven uncertainty of danger recruits the amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 22632275. doi:
10.1162/jocn.2009.21402
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted
in the absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could be construed
as a potential conict of interest.
Received: 14 November 2014; accepted: 21 January 2015; published online: 17
February 2015.
Citation: Okon-Singer H, Hendler T, Pessoa L and Shackman AJ (2015) The neurobiology of emotioncognition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for future
research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:58. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00058
This article was submitted to the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Copyright 2015 Okon-Singer, Hendler, Pessoa and Shackman. This is an openaccess article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
(CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided
the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

www.frontiersin.org

February 2015 | Volume 9 | Article 58 | 14

You might also like