Noi Cercetări Emoție - Cogniție
Noi Cercetări Emoție - Cogniție
Noi Cercetări Emoție - Cogniție
Edited by:
Leonhard Schilbach, University
Hospital Cologne, Germany
Reviewed by:
Christian Sorg, Klinikum rechts der
Isar Technische Universitt Mnchen,
Germany
Elliot Berkman, University of Oregon,
USA
*Correspondence:
Hadas Okon-Singer, Department of
Psychology, University of Haifa,
Mount Carmel, Haifa 3498838, Israel
e-mail: [email protected];
Alexander J. Shackman, Department
of Psychology, Neuroscience and
Cognitive Science Program, and
Maryland Neuroimaging Center,
University of Maryland, 3123G
Biology-Psychology Building, College
Park, MD 20742, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
These
Recent years have witnessed the emergence of powerful new tools for assaying the
brain and a remarkable acceleration of research focused on the interplay of emotion and
cognition. This work has begun to yield new insights into fundamental questions about the
nature of the mind and important clues about the origins of mental illness. In particular,
this research demonstrates that stress, anxiety, and other kinds of emotion can profoundly
inuence key elements of cognition, including selective attention, working memory, and
cognitive control. Often, this inuence persists beyond the duration of transient emotional
challenges, partially reecting the slower molecular dynamics of catecholamine and hormonal neurochemistry. In turn, circuits involved in attention, executive control, and working
memory contribute to the regulation of emotion. The distinction between the emotional
and the cognitive brain is fuzzy and context-dependent. Indeed, there is compelling
evidence that brain territories and psychological processes commonly associated with
cognition, such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and working memory, play a central
role in emotion. Furthermore, putatively emotional and cognitive regions inuence one
another via a complex web of connections in ways that jointly contribute to adaptive
and maladaptive behavior. This work demonstrates that emotion and cognition are deeply
interwoven in the fabric of the brain, suggesting that widely held beliefs about the key
constituents of the emotional brain and the cognitive brain are fundamentally awed. We
conclude by outlining several strategies for enhancing future research. Developing a deeper
understanding of the emotional-cognitive brain is important, not just for understanding the
mind but also for elucidating the root causes of its disorders.
Keywords: ACC, amygdala, anxiety, depression, emotion control and regulation, EEG/ERP, fMRI, PFC
Until the 20th century, the study of emotion and cognition was
largely a philosophical matter. Although modern perspectives on
the mind and its disorders remain heavily inuenced by the introspective measures that dened this earlier era of scholarship, the
last several decades have witnessed the emergence of powerful
new tools for assaying the brain and a remarkable acceleration
of research to elucidate the interplay of emotion and cognition
(Pessoa, 2013; Braver et al., 2014; Dolcos and Denkova, 2014).
The immediate goal of our Special Research Topic was to survey
recent advances in understanding how emotional and cognitive
processes interact, how they are integrated in the brain, and the
implications for understanding the mind and its disorders (OkonSinger et al., 2014b; Figure 1). Here, we consider ways in which
this rapidly growing body of work begins to address some more
fundamental questions about the nature of cognitionemotion
interactions, highlighting key points of consensus. By focusing
attention on the most important outstanding questions, we hope
to move the eld forward. First, we hope that answers provided
by our contributors will stimulate discussion. Second, we hope
that juxtaposing clear theoretical goals against the current state
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
FIGURE 1 | The top 200 scientific terms used in the Special Research Topic. The typeface is scaled proportional to the frequency of each term. The gure
was generated using http://www.wordle.net.
(e.g., Siman-Tov et al., 2009; Lerner et al., 2012; Pourtois et al., 2013; Carreti, 2014) the ability to selectively
respond to relevant aspects of the environment while inhibiting potential sources of distraction and competing courses
of action (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Miller and Cohen,
2001). The focus of attention is determined by the pervasive competition between exogenous (often termed stimulusdriven or bottomup) and endogenous (often termed goaldirected or topdown attention) mechanisms (Egeth and Yantis,
1997).
With respect to exogenous attention, a number of contributors describe new evidence that emotionally-charged cues are
more attention-grabbing than neutral cues and highlight recent
efforts to specify the mechanisms underlying this bias (Holtmann
et al., 2013; McHugo et al., 2013; Peers et al., 2013; Stollstorff
et al., 2013). Along the way, McHugo et al. (2013) provide a
useful tutorial on methods for quantifying the capture of attention by emotional cues (e.g., dot-probe, emotional attentional
blink).
Importantly, attention can also be guided in an endogenous
fashion by internal goals (e.g., rules, instructions, and plans)
as well as moods and motivational states (e.g., feeling anxious or hungry). Mohanty and Sussman (2013) discuss evidence
demonstrating that emotion and motivation can guide attention to congruent cues (e.g., food when hungry). In particular,
they show that subcortical regions proximally involved in determining value and orchestrating emotional states (e.g., amygdala,
substantia nigra) can facilitate endogenous attentional processes
implemented in frontoparietal regions and can strengthen activation in relevant sensory regions (e.g., face-selective regions of the
fusiform gyrus when anticipating an angry face). This extended
network, encompassing sensory, attentional, and emotional circuits, facilitates the rapid detection of emotionally-signicant
information.
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Classically, cognition and emotion have been viewed as oppositional forces (Damasio, 2005a; Okon-Singer et al., 2007, 2011;
Shackman et al., in press). From this perspective, moods and
other kinds of emotional states are responsible for short-circuiting
cognition.
Consistent with this view, Kalanthroff et al. (2013) show that
emotional distractors disrupt cognitive control. Cognitive control
encompasses the range of processes (e.g., endogenous attention,
inhibition, and learning) that are engaged when habitual responses
are not sufcient to sustain goal-directed behavior, as in stopsignal, go/no-go, Stroop, and Eriksen anker tasks (Shackman
et al., 2011b). Here, the authors demonstrate that the brief presentation of emotional images disrupts performance in the stop-signal
task, a widely used index of inhibitory control (see also Pessoa et al.,
2012).
Likewise, Iordan et al. (2013) review evidence that emotional
distractors disrupt working memory. Converging with other work
focused on emotion-related distraction (Bishop, 2007; Etkin,
2012; Bishop and Forster, 2013; Etkin et al., 2013; Okon-Singer
et al., 2014a; van Ast et al., 2014), they suggest that degraded
performance reects two processes: (a) increased engagement
of regions involved in processing socio-emotional information
and orchestrating emotional expressions (e.g., amygdala), and
(b) a reduction of delay-spanning activity in frontoparietal
cortex.
EMOTION STRENGTHENS SOME COGNITIVE PROCESSES WHILE
WEAKENING OTHERS
With the ascent of evolutionary theory in the 19th century (Darwin, 1872/2009, 1872), many scientists adopted the view that
emotions are functional and enhance tness (Susskind et al., 2008;
Todd et al., 2012; Sandi, 2013; Schwabe and Wolf, 2013; Todd
and Anderson, 2013); in short, that emotions are more adaptive
than not and that there is typically more cooperation than strife
between emotion and cognition (Levenson, 1994).
Consistent with this more nuanced perspective, the contributions from Clarke and Johnstone (2013), Morriss et al. (2013),
Robinson et al. (2013a, 2013b), Vytal et al. (2013) provide evidence that experimentally-elicited anxiety facilitates some kinds
of information processing, while degrading others. In particular,
they provide considerable evidence that anxiety: (a) enhances vigilance, potentiating early sensory cortical responses to innocuous
environmental stimuli, increasing the likelihood that emotionally
salient information will be detected; and (b) disrupts working
memory.
The molecular basis of emotions deleterious impact on working memory is reviewed by Shansky and Lipps (2013). Building
on pioneering work by Arnsten and Goldman-Rakic (1998) and
Arnsten (2009), the authors describe evidence that stress strongly
inuences catecholamine (i.e., dopamine and norepinephrine)
and glucocorticoid levels in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in ways
that degrade delay-spanning neuronal activity.
Shansky and Lipps (2013) also describe important new evidence that sex hormones, such as estrogen, can exacerbate the
impact of stress on prefrontal function. Along these lines, Sacher
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Emotional traits are often conceptualized as diatheses for emotional states (Matthews et al., 2009). Thus, individuals with high
levels of neuroticism or negative emotionality are thought to be
prone to exaggerated anxiety in the face of trait-relevant cues,
contexts, and challenges (e.g., punishment, negative feedback), as
illustrated in the contributions from Kessel et al. (2013), Moser
et al. (2013), and Proudt et al. (2013). Yet, a considerable body
of neurophysiological evidence indicates that emotional traits
are embodied in the on-going activity and connectivity of the
brain (Canli et al., 2005; Fox et al., 2008; Shackman et al., 2009;
Rohr et al., 2013; Birn et al., 2014a,b). Likewise, the sustained
levels of heightened vigilance and distress characteristic of individuals with anxiety disorders are most apparent in the absence
of clear and imminent threat (Davis et al., 2010; Lissek, 2012;
Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). These observations raise the possibility that emotional traits could inuence cognition in the absence
of explicit emotional distraction or challenge (Watson and Clark,
1984; Bolger and Schilling, 1991; Suls and Martin, 2005).
Here, Berggren et al. (2013) provide compelling evidence that
trait anxiety is associated with degraded cognitive control, indexed
using an anti-saccade task under load. This new observation
adds to a growing literature showing that hot emotional traits
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Some strategies for regulating emotional distress, such as reappraisal, require the effortful maintenance of an explicit regulatory
goal. Rolls (2013) reviews evidence suggesting that this critically
depends on working memory. More broadly, he suggests that goals,
attentional sets, and other kinds of declarative knowledge held in
working memory play a central role in regulating the output of
emotional systems.
The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is a canonically cognitive region of the brain, well known for its critical role in reasoning
and higher cognition (e.g., endogenous attention, working memory, and cognitive control; Roberts et al., 1998; Miller and Cohen,
2001; DEsposito and Postle, 2014). Yet, there is growing evidence
that the dlPFC plays a key role in the topdown control of emotion and motivated behavior (Fox et al., 2005b; Koenigs et al., 2008;
Zaretsky et al., 2010; Buhle et al., 2013; Frank et al., 2014; Treadway
et al., 2014).
Here, Clarke and Johnstone (2013) and Iordan et al. (2013) provide tantalizing, albeit correlational, evidence that dlPFC acts to
protect the contents of working memory from emotional distraction. This converges with work by Peers et al. (2013) and Stollstorff
et al. (2013) indicating that dlPFC plays a key role in regulating the
focus of attention in the face of potentially distracting emotional
cues.
Rolls (2013) extends this perspective to decision-making, arguing that behavior reects a pervasive, dynamic competition
between two kinds of brain systems: (a) emotional systems, including circuits centered on the amygdala and ventral striatum, that
have been genetically programmed by our phylogenetic history
(e.g., fear elicited by danger, joy elicited by sweets and fat); and
(b) cognitive systems, such as the frontoparietal network, that are
informed by our ontogenetic history and governed by our declarative knowledge and explicit goals (i.e., pick the healthy orange,
not the unhealthy candy bar; cf. Hare et al., 2008, 2009). Rolls
emphasizes that the lateral PFC can override the output of emotion circuitry, biasing behavior in favor of our explicit goals. John
et al. (2013) articulate a complementary perspective, reviewing
evidence that the PFC and amygdala functionally interact via a
complex anatomical network of recurrent cortical and thalamic
projections and intra-amygdalar microcircuits (see also Pessoa and
Adolphs, 2010; Pessoa, 2012; Pessoa et al., 2012; Birn et al., 2014a,b;
Treadway et al., 2014).
Evidence linking the dlPFC to mood and anxiety disorders, as
in the papers contributed by Crocker et al. (2013) and Warren et al.
(2013), underscores the importance of developing a more sophisticated understanding of the role played by cognitive regions in
normal and disordered emotion.
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
allocation, action selection) in ways that support adaptive behavior (for convergent perspectives, see the contributions from Rolls,
2013, and John et al., 2013).
Dreisbach and Fischer (2012) describe other evidence consistent with this integrative perspective. In particular, they show that
cognitive conict is aversive. This converges with a growing body
of evidence demonstrating that conict and other prompts for
increased control (e.g., errors, punishment), are experienced as
unpleasant and facilitate avoidance (Botvinick, 2007; Kool et al.,
2010; Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012; Schouppe et al., 2012; Lindstrm et al., 2013; Proudt et al., 2013; Shenhav and Buckner,
2014).
If negative emotions are indeed integrated with control processes, we would expect that anxiety and control should covary.
That is, one would expect a degree of functional convergence
between measures of anxiety and control-related activity in the
MCC or other regions (i.e., convergent validity; Campbell and
Fiske, 1959). Consistent with this possibility, Moser et al. (2013)
provide compelling meta-analytic evidence that error-related signals generated in the MCC are enhanced among anxiety patients
and individuals with heightened negative emotionality. This indicates that negative emotionality, a fundamental dimension of
childhood temperament and adult personality (Caspi et al., 2005),
involves systematic differences in the way that the brain responds
to prompts for cognitive control.
McDermott et al. (2013) describe important new evidence,
gleaned from the study of Romanian orphans, that MCC control signals are plastic. In particular, they demonstrate that
MCC-generated control signals are profoundly shaped by early
experience in ways that confer risk or resilience for later socioemotional problems. This underscores the need to clarify the
neurodevelopmental mechanisms that serve to integrate emotion
and cognition in the laboratory and in daily life.
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Most of the contributors to the Special Research Topic used noninvasive techniques, such as fMRI, to trace associations between
emotion and cognition, on the one hand, and brain function
on the other. Aside from unresolved questions about the origins and signicance of the measured signals (e.g., Logothetis,
2008), the most important limitation of these techniques is that
they do not address causation. A crucial challenge for future
studies is to develop a mechanistic understanding of the distributed networks that support the interplay of emotion and
cognition. This can be achieved by combining mechanistic techniques (e.g., optogenetics) or invasive analyses of neuromolecular
pathways in animal models with the same whole-brain imaging
strategies routinely applied in humans (Borsook et al., 2006; Lerman et al., 2007; Fox et al., 2010, 2012; Lee et al., 2010; Desai
et al., 2011; Casey et al., 2013; Narayanan et al., 2013; Roseboom
et al., 2014). Similar strategies can be used with patients with
circumscribed brain damage (e.g., Nomura et al., 2010; Gratton et al., 2012; Motzkin et al., 2014). Combining fMRI or EEG
with non-invasive perturbation techniques (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation) or
pharmacological manipulations provides another opportunity for
understanding how regional changes in brain activity alter network function and, ultimately, behavior (Paulus et al., 2005;
Guller et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2013; Reinhart and Woodman,
2014). Prospective longitudinal designs represent another fruitful
approach to identifying candidate mechanisms, especially in relation to the development of neuropsychiatric disorders (Admon
et al., 2013).
CONCLUSION
The last decade has witnessed an explosion of interest in the interplay of emotion and cognition. The research embodied in this
Special Research Topic highlights the tremendous advances that
have already been made. In particular, this work demonstrates
that emotional cues, emotional states, and emotional traits can
strongly inuence key elements of on-going information processing, including selective attention, working memory, and cognitive
control. Often, this inuence persists beyond the duration of
transient emotional challenges, perhaps reecting slower changes
in neurochemistry. In turn, circuits involved in attention and
working memory contribute to the voluntary regulation of emotion. The distinction between the emotional and the cognitive
brain is blurry and context-dependent. Indeed, there is compelling evidence that territories (e.g., dlPFC, MCC) and processes
(e.g., working memory, cognitive control) conventionally associated with cognition play a central role in emotion. Furthermore,
putatively emotional and cognitive regions dynamically inuence one another via a complex web of recurrent, often indirect
anatomical connections in ways that jointly contribute to adaptive behavior. Collectively, these observations show that emotion
and cognition are deeply interwoven in the fabric of the brain,
suggesting that widely held beliefs about the key constituents of
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
anxiety, fear, guilt, and sadness (Watson and Clark, 1984; Caspi
et al., 2005).
Reinforcer: Rewards and punishments; anything an organism will
work to approach or avoid (Rolls, 1999).
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
All the authors supervised the Special Research Topic. Hadas
Okon-Singer and Alexander J. Shackman wrote the paper. All the
authors edited and revised the paper.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank the many contributors and staff who made the Special
Research Topic possible. We acknowledge the assistance of L.
Friedman and support of the European Commission (Followship #334206 to Hadas Okon-Singer and Grant #602186 to Talma
Hendler), Israeli Center of Research Excellence, Israeli Science
Foundation (Grant #51/11 to Talma Hendler), National Institute
of Mental Health (MH071589 to Luiz Pessoa), and University of
Maryland (Alexander J. Shackman and Luiz Pessoa).
REFERENCES
Adachi, Y., Osada, T., Sporns, O., Watanabe, T., Matsui, T., Miyamoto, K.,
et al. (2012). Functional connectivity between anatomically unconnected areas
is shaped by collective network-level effects in the macaque cortex. Cereb. Cortex
22, 15861592. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr234
Admon, R., Lubin, G., Rosenblatt, J. D., Stern, O., Kahn, I., Assaf, M., et al. (2012).
Imbalanced neural responsivity to risk and reward indicates stress vulnerability
in humans. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2835. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr369
Admon, R., Lubin, G., Stern, O., Rosenberg, K., Sela, L., Ben-Ami, H., et al.
(2009). Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdalas predisposition and
hippocampal plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1412014125. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0903183106
Admon, R., Milad, M. R., and Hendler, T. (2013). A causal model of post-traumatic
stress disorder: disentangling predisposed from acquired neural abnormalities.
Trends Cogn. Sci 17, 337347. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.005
Anticevic, A., Cole, M. W., Repovs, G., Savic, A., Driesen, N. R., Yang, G., et al.
(2013). Connectivity, pharmacology, and computation: toward a mechanistic
understanding of neural system dysfunction in schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry
4:169. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00169
Arend, I., Henik, A., and Okon-Singer, H. (2014). Dissociating emotion and attention functions in the Pulvinar nucleus of the Thalamus. Neuropsychology. doi:
10.1037/neu0000139 [Epub ahead of print].
Arnsten, A. F. (1998). The biology of being frazzled. Science 280, 17111712. doi:
10.1126/science.280.5370.1711
Arnsten, A. F. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 410422. doi: 10.1038/
nrn2648
Arnsten, A. F., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1998). Noise stress impairs prefrontal
cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic
mechanism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 362368. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.
4.362
Aue, T., Guex, R., Chauvigne, L. A., and Okon-Singer, H. (2013a). Varying expectancies and attention bias in phobic and non-phobic individuals. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 7:418. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00418
Aue, T., Hoeppli, M. E., Piguet, C., Sterpenich, V., and Vuilleumier, P.
(2013b). Visual avoidance in phobia: particularities in neural activity, autonomic
responding, and cognitive risk evaluations. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:194. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00194
Awh, E., and Vogel, E. K. (2008). The bouncer in the brain. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 56.
doi: 10.1038/nn0108-5
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Van,
I. M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious
individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 124. doi: 10.1037/00332909.133.1.1
Bar-Haim, Y., and Pine, D. S. (2013). Cognitive training research and the search for a
transformative, translational, developmental cognitive neuroscience. Dev. Cogn.
Neurosci. 4, 12. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.001
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2007).
The experience of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 373. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709
Barrett, L. F., and Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and
social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 361372. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012
Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control decits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you dont. Biol. Psychol. 92, 440446. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007
Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2014). Inhibitory decits in trait anxiety: increased
stimulus-based or response-based interference? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 21, 1339
1345. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0611-8
Berggren, N., Richards, A., Taylor, J., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Affective attention under cognitive load: reduced emotional biases but emergent
anxiety-related costs to inhibitory control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:188. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00188
Berghorst, L. H., Bogdan, R., Frank, M. J., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Acute
stress selectively reduces reward sensitivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:133. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00133
Berkman, E. T., and Falk, E. B. (2013). Beyond brain mapping: using neural measures to predict real-world outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 22:4550. doi:
10.1177/0963721412469394
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., Mcfarlin, D. R., Rogers,
G. M., et al. (2014a). Evolutionarily conserved prefrontal-amygdalar dysfunction
in early-life anxiety. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 915922. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.46
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., Mcfarlin, D. R., Rogers,
G. M., et al. (2014b). Extreme early-life anxiety is associated with an evolutionarily
conserved reduction in the strength of intrinsic functional connectivity between
the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the central nucleus of the amygdala. Mol.
Psychiatry 19, 853. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.85
Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account.
Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 307316. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008
Bishop, S. J. (2009). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention.
Nat. Neurosci. 12, 9298. doi: 10.1038/nn.2242
Bishop, S. J., and Forster, S. (2013). Trait anxiety, neuroticism and the brain basis
of vulnerability to affective disorder, in The Cambridge Handbook of Human
Affective Neuroscience, eds J. Armony and P. Vuilleumier (Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press), 553574. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511843716.031
Blackford, J. U., Avery, S. N., Shelton, R. C., and Zald, D. H. (2009). Amygdala temporal dynamics: temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to
familiar and novel faces. BMC Neurosci. 10:145. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-145
Blackford, J. U., and Pine, D. S. (2012). Neural substrates of childhood anxiety
disorders: a review of neuroimaging ndings. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N.
Am. 21, 501525. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2012.05.002
Bogdan, R., Perlis, R. H., Fagerness, J., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2010). The
impact of mineralocorticoid receptor ISO/VAL genotype (rs5522) and stress on
reward learning. Genes Brain Behav. 9, 658667. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.
00600.x
Bogdan, R., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 11471154. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.037
Bolger, N., and Schilling, E. A. (1991). Personality and the problems of everyday life:
the role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. J. Pers. 59,
355386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1991.tb00253.x
Borsook, D., Becerra, L., and Hargreaves, R. (2006). A role for fMRI in optimizing CNS drug development. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 5, 411424. doi: 10.1038/
nrd2027
Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conict monitoring and decision making: reconciling
two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7,
356366. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.356
Braver, T. S., Krug, M. K., Chiew, K. S., Kool, W., Westbrook, J. A., Clement, N. J.,
et al. (2014). Mechanisms of motivation-cognition interaction: challenges and
opportunities. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 443472. doi: 10.3758/s13415014-0300-0
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H.,
et al. (2013). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
David, S. P., Ware, J. J., Chu, I. M., Loftus, P. D., Fusar-Poli, P., Radua, J., et al. (2013).
Potential reporting bias in fMRI studies of the brain. PLoS ONE 8:e70104. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0070104
Davidson, R. J. (2004). Well-being and affective style: neural substrates and biobehavioural correlates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 13951411. doi:
10.1098/rstb.2004.1510
Davis, M., Walker, D. L., Miles, L., and Grillon, C. (2010). Phasic vs sustained
fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 105135. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.109
Davis, M., and Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol.
Psychiatry 6, 1334. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812
DEsposito, M., and Postle, B. R. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of working
memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 115142. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814015031
de Sousa, R. (2014). Emotion, in Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta (Stanford, CA: Stanford University). Available at:
http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/
Desai, M., Kahn, I., Knoblich, U., Bernstein, J., Atallah, H., Yang, A., et al. (2011).
Mapping brain networks in awake mice using combined optical neural control
and fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 13931405. doi: 10.1152/jn.00828.2010
Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.
001205
DiLuca, M., and Olesen, J. (2014). The cost of brain diseases: a burden or a challenge?
Neuron 82, 12051208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.044
Dolcos, F., and Denkova, E. (2014). Current emotion research in cognitive neuroscience: linking enhancing and impairing effects of emotion on cognition. Emot.
Rev. 6, 362375. doi: 10.1177/1754073914536449
Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (2012). The role of affect and reward in the conicttriggered adjustment of cognitive control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:342. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00342
Duncan, S., and Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: a neurobiological analysis. Cogn. Emot. 21, 11841211. doi: 10.1080/026999307014
37931
Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: control, representation, and time
course. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 269297. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.269
Egloff, B., Schmukle, S. C., Burns, L. R., and Schwerdtfeger, A. (2006). Spontaneous
emotion regulation during evaluated speaking tasks: associations with negative
affect, anxiety expression, memory, and physiological responding. Emotion 6,
356366. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.356
Ehring, T., Tuschen-Cafer, B., Schnulle, J., Fischer, S., and Gross, J. J.
(2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion 10, 563572.
doi: 10.1037/a0019010
Ekman, P. E., and Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of Emotion: Fundamental
Questions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ekstrom, L. B., Roelfsema, P. R., Arsenault, J. T., Bonmassar, G., and Vanduffel, W.
(2008). Bottom-up dependent gating of frontal signals in early visual cortex.
Science 321, 414417. doi: 10.1126/science.1153276
Etkin, A. (2012). Neurobiology of anxiety: from neural circuits to novel solutions?
Depress Anxiety 29, 355358. doi: 10.1002/da.21957
Etkin, A., Gyurak, A., and Ohara, R. (2013). A neurobiological approach to the
cognitive decits of psychiatric disorders. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15, 419429.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety
and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336353. doi:
10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336
Forbes, E. E., Hariri, A. R., Martin, S. L., Silk, J. S., Moyles, D. L., Fisher,
P. M., et al. (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world positive
affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 6473. doi:
10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081336
Fox, A. S., and Kalin, N. H. (2014). A translational neuroscience approach to understanding the development of social anxiety disorder and its pathophysiology. Am.
J. Psychiatry doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040449 [Epub ahead of print].
Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shelton, S. E., Nanda, S. A., Davidson, R. J., Roseboom,
P. H., et al. (2012). Central amygdala nucleus (Ce) gene expression linked to
increased trait-like Ce metabolism and anxious temperament in young primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 1810818113. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206
723109
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Oakes, T. R., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J., and
Kalin, N. H. (2010). Orbitofrontal cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity
in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. J. Neurosci. 30, 70237027. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5952-09.2010
Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Oakes, T. R., Davidson, R. J., and Kalin, N. H. (2008). Traitlike brain activity during adolescence predicts anxious temperament in primates.
PLoS ONE 3:e2570. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002570
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., and
Ghera, M. M. (2005a). Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior
within a developmental framework. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 235262. doi:
10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532
Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R., Shelton, S. E., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J., and Kalin,
N. H. (2005b). Calling for help is independently modulated by brain systems
underlying goal-directed behavior and threat perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A. 102, 41764179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409470102
Frank, D. W., Dewitt, M., Hudgens-Haney, M., Schaeffer, D. J., Ball, B. H., Schwarz,
N. F., et al. (2014). Emotion regulation: quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45C, 202211. doi:
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010
Gendron, M., and Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the past: a century of ideas about emotion in psychology. Emot. Rev. 1, 316339. doi:
10.1177/1754073909338877
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137156. doi:
10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033
Gosling, S. D., and Mason, W. (2015). Internet research in psychology. Annu. Rev.
Psychol. 66, 877902. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015321
Gratton, C., Nomura, E. M., Perez, F., and Desposito, M. (2012). Focal brain
lesions to critical locations cause widespread disruption of the modular organization of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 12751285. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a00222
Gray, J. A., and McNaughton, N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry
into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford Psychology
Press.
Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.
74, 224237. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
Gross, J. J. (1998b). The emerging eld of emotion regulation: an integrative review.
Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2, 271. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., and Urry, H. L. (2011). Cognition and emotion lecture at the 2010 spsp emotion preconference. Cogn. Emot. 25, 765781. doi:
10.1080/02699931.2011.555753
Gross, J. J., and Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual
foundations, in Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Guilford: Guilford Press.
3, 24.
Grupe, D. W., and Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety:
an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.
14, 488501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524
Guller, Y., Tononi, G., and Postle, B. R. (2012). Conserved functional connectivity
but impaired effective connectivity of thalamocortical circuitry in schizophrenia.
Brain Connect. 2, 311319. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0100
Hajcak, G., and Olvet, D. M. (2008). The persistence of attention to emotion:
brain potentials during and after picture presentation. Emotion 8, 250255. doi:
10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.250
Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E.,
et al. (2010). Attention bias modication treatment: a meta-analysis toward the
establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 982990. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-control in decision-making
involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324, 646648. doi:
10.1126/science.1168450
Hare, T. A., ODoherty, J., Camerer, C. F., Schultz, W., and Rangel, A. (2008).
Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 28, 56235630. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1309-08.2008
Harl, K. M., Shenoy, P., and Paulus, M. P. (2013). The inuence of emotions on
cognitive control: feelings and beliefs-where do they meet? Front. Hum. Neurosci.
7:508. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00508
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
LeDoux, J. E. (2014). Coming to terms with fear. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111,
28712878. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400335111
Lee, H., Shackman, A. J., Jackson, D. C., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Test-retest
reliability of voluntary emotion regulation. Psychophysiology 46, 874879. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00830.x
Lee, J. H., Durand, R., Gradinaru, V., Zhang, F., Goshen, I., Kim, D. S., et al. (2010).
Global and local fMRI signals driven by neurons dened optogenetically by type
and wiring. Nature 465, 788792. doi: 10.1038/nature09108
Lerman, C., LeSage, M. G., Perkins, K. A., OMalley, S. S., Siegel, S. J., Benowitz,
N. L., et al. (2007). Translational research in medication development for nicotine
dependence. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 6, 746762. doi: 10.1038/nrd2361
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., and Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision
making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 33.133.25.
Lerner, Y., Singer, N., Gonen, T., Weintraub, Y., Cohen, O., Rubin, N., et al. (2012).
Feeling without seeing? Engagement of ventral, but not dorsal, amygdala during unaware exposure to emotional faces. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 531542. doi:
10.1162/jocn-a-00165
Levenson, R. W. (1994). I. Human emotion: a functional view. II. The search for
autonomic specicity. III. Emotional control: variation and consequences, in
The Nature of Emotion. Fundamental Questions, eds P. Ekman and R. Davidson
(New York: Oxford University Press).
Lewis-Peacock, J. A., and Norman, K. A. (2013). Multi-voxel pattern analysis of
fMRI data: a review. Comput. Math. Methods Med. 2012, 114.
Lin, T., Vaisvaser, S., Fruchter, E., Admon, R., Wald, I., Pine, D., et al. (2014). A neurobehavioral account for individual differences in resilience to chronic military
stress. Psychol. Med. 113. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002013
Lindquist, K. A., and Barrett, L. F. (2012). A functional architecture of the human
brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16,
533540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.005
Lindstrm, B. R., Mattsson-Mrn, I. B., Golkar, A., and Olsson, A. (2013).
In your face: risk of punishment enhances cognitive control and errorrelated activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle. PLoS ONE 8:e65692. doi:
10.1371/journal.pone.0065692
Lissek, S. (2012). Toward an account of clinical anxiety predicated on basic, neurally mapped mechanisms of Pavlovian fear-learning: the case for conditioned
overgeneralization. Depress. Anxiety 29, 257263. doi: 10.1002/da.21922
Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature
453, 869878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976
Lopez, R. B., Hofmann, W., Wagner, D. D., Kelley, W. M., and Heatherton, T. F.
(2014). Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 25,
13371344. doi: 10.1177/0956797614531492
MacLeod, C., and Clarke, P. J. (2015). The attentional bias modication approach to
anxiety intervention. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 5878. doi: 10.1177/2167702614560749
MacLeod, C., and Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modication approaches
to anxiety. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 8, 189217. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy032511143052
Matthews, G., Deary, I. J., and Whiteman, M. C. (2009). Personality Traits,
3rd Edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO97805118
12743
McDermott, J. M., Troller-Renfree, S., Vanderwert, R., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H.,
and Fox, N. A. (2013). Psychosocial deprivation, executive functions, and the
emergence of socio-emotional behavior problems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:167.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00167
McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., and Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink: what we know so far. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:151. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00151
McMenamin, B. W., Langeslag, S. J., Sirbu, M., Padmala, S., and Pessoa, L. (2014).
Network organization unfolds over time during periods of anxious anticipation.
J. Neurosci. 34, 1126111273. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1579-14.2014
McNab, F., and Klingberg, T. (2007). Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control
access to working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 103107. doi: 10.1038/nn2024
Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain 121, 10131052. doi:
10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
Millan, M. J. (2013). An epigenetic framework for neurodevelopmental disorders: from pathogenesis to potential therapy. Neuropharmacology 68, 282. doi:
10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.11.015
Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex
function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., Kenzer, A., and Bauer, A. (2012). Interactions between
cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion 12, 192197. doi:
10.1037/a0024109
Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and
integrated model. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 393423. doi: 10.1146/annurevclinpsy-050212-185606
Pizzagalli, D. A., Bogdan, R., Ratner, K. G., and Jahn, A. L. (2007). Increased
perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 27422753. doi:
10.1016/j.brat.2007.07.013
Pochon, J. B., Riis, J., Sanfey, A. G., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2008).
Functional imaging of decision conict. J. Neurosci. 28, 34683473. doi:
10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4195-07.2008
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and
brain. Neuroscience 139, 2338. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005
Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., and Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional inuences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol.
Psychol. 92, 492512. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007
Proudt, G. H., Inzlicht, M., and Mennin, D. S. (2013). Anxiety and error monitoring: the importance of motivation and emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:636.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00636
Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J., Luo, J., and Fernndez, G. (2009). Acute
psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral
prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 2532. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006
Raz, G., Jacob, Y., Gonen, T., Winetraub, Y., Flash, T., Soreq, E., et al. (2014). Cry for
her or cry with her: context-dependent dissociation of two modes of cinematic
empathy reected in network cohesion dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9,
3038. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst052
Raz, G., Winetraub, Y., Jacob, Y., Kinreich, S., Maron-Katz, A., Shaham, G.,
et al. (2012). Portraying emotions at their unfolding: a multilayered approach
for probing dynamics of neural networks. Neuroimage 60, 14481461. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.084
Reinhart, R. M., and Woodman, G. F. (2014). Oscillatory coupling reveals the
dynamic reorganization of large-scale neural networks as cognitive demands
change. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 175188. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00470
Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W., and Weiskrantz, L. E. (1998). The Prefrontal Cortex: Executive and Cognitive Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524410.001.0001
Robinson, O. J., Krimsky, M., and Grillon, C. (2013a). The impact of
induced anxiety on response inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:69. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00069
Robinson, O. J., Vytal, K., Cornwell, B. R., and Grillon, C. (2013b). The impact of
anxiety upon cognition: perspectives from human threat of shock studies. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. 7:203. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00203
Rohr, C. S., Okon-Singer, H., Craddock, R. C., Villringer, A., and Margulies, D. S.
(2013). Affect and the brains functional organization: a resting-state connectivity
approach. PLoS ONE 8:e68015. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068015
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The Brain and Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rolls, E. T. (2013). A biased activation theory of the cognitive and attentional modulation of emotion. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00074
Roseboom, P. H., Nanda, S. A., Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shackman, A. J., Shelton, S. E.,
et al. (2014). Neuropeptide Y receptor gene expression in the primate amygdala
predicts anxious temperament and brain metabolism. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 850
857. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.012
Sacher, J., Neumann, J., Okon-Singer, H., Gotowiec, S., and Villringer, A. (2012).
Sexual dimorphism in the human brain: evidence from neuroimaging. Magn.
Reson. Imaging 31, 366375. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2012.06.007
Sacher, J., Okon-Singer, H., and Villringer, A. (2013). Evidence from neuroimaging
for the role of the menstrual cycle in the interplay of emotion and cognition.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:374. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00374
Sandi, C. (2013). Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 245261.
doi: 10.1002/wcs.1222
Schick, A., Wessa, M., Vollmayr, B., Kuehner, C., and Kanske, P. (2013). Indirect
assessment of an interpretation bias in humans: neurophysiological and behavioral correlates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:272. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00272
Schmitter, A. M. (2014). 17th and 18th century theories of emotions, in Stanford
Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta (Stanford, CA: Stanford University).
Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotions-17th18th/
www.frontiersin.org
Okon-Singer et al.
Emotioncognition interactions
Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., and Anderson, A. K.
(2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 843850.
doi: 10.1038/nn.2138
Todd, R. M., and Anderson, A. K. (2013). Salience, State, and expression: the
inuence of specic aspects of emotion on attention and perception. Oxford
Handbook Cogn. Neurosci. 2, 11.
Todd, R. M., Cunningham, W. A., Anderson, A. K., and Thompson, E. (2012).
Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 365372. doi:
10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.003
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Jan, K., Asplund, C. L., Ginther,
M. R., et al. (2014). Corticolimbic gating of emotion-driven punishment. Nat.
Neurosci. 17, 12701275. doi: 10.1038/nn.3781
Uddin, L. Q., Kinnison, J., Pessoa, L., and Anderson, M. L. (2014). Beyond the
tripartite cognition-emotion-interoception model of the human insular cortex.
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1627. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00462
Uhlhaas, P. J., and Singer, W. (2012). Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric
disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks.
Neuron 75, 963980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004
Urry, H. L. (2010). Seeing, thinking, and feeling: emotion-regulating effects
of gaze-directed cognitive reappraisal. Emotion 10, 125135. doi: 10.1037/
a0017434
Vaisvaser, S., Lin, T., Admon, R., Podlipsky, I., Greenman, Y., Stern, N., et al.
(2013). Neural traces of stress: cortisol related sustained enhancement of
amygdala-hippocampal functional connectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:313.
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00313
van Ast, V., Spicer, J., Smith, E., Schmer-Galunder, S., Liberzon, I., Abelson, J., et al.
(2014). Brain mechanisms of social threat effects on working memory. Cereb.
Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu206 [Epub ahead of print].
Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G.,
and Koster, E. H. (2013). A review of current evidence for the causal impact
of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychol. Bull. 140, 682721. doi:
10.1037/a0034834
Van Dessel, P., and Vogt, J. (2012). When does hearing laughter draw attention to happy faces? Task relevance determines the inuence of a crossmodal
affective context on emotional attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:294. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00294
van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Urry, H. L., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S.,
Alexander, A. L., et al. (2007). Gaze xations predict brain activation during the
voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. Neuroimage 36, 1041
1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.052
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Essen, D. C.,
et al. (2007). Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain.
Nature 447, 8386. doi: 10.1038/nature05758
Vogel, E. K., and Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity. Nature 428, 748751. doi:
10.1038/nature02447
Vogel, E. K., Mccollough, A. W., and Machizawa, M. G. (2005). Neural measures
reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature
438, 500503. doi: 10.1038/nature04171
Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B. R., Letkiewicz, A. M., Arkin, N. E., and Grillon, C. (2013). The complex interaction between anxiety and cognition: insight
from spatial and verbal working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:93. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00093
Wager, T. D., Atlas, L. Y., Lindquist, M. A., Roy, M., Woo, C. W., and Kross, E.
(2013). An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N. Engl. J. Med.
368, 13881397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471
Wald, I., Degnan, K. A., Gorodetsky, E., Charney, D. S., Fox, N. A., Fruchter, E., et al.
(2013). Attention to threats and combat-related posttraumatic stress symptoms:
prospective associations and moderation by the serotonin transporter gene. JAMA
Psychiatry 70, 401408. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.188
Warren, S. L., Crocker, L. D., Spielberg, J. M., Engels, A. S., Banich, M. T.,
Sutton, B. P., et al. (2013). Cortical organization of inhibition-related functions and modulation by psychopathology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:271. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2013.00271
Watson, D., and Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: the disposition to
experience aversive emotional states. Psychol. Bull. 96, 465. doi: 10.1037/00332909.96.3.465
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., and Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling: a
meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process
model of emotion regulation. Psychol. Bull. 138, 775808. doi: 10.1037/a00
27600
Whiteford, H. A., Degenhardt, L., Rehm, J., Baxter, A. J., Ferrari, A. J., Erskine, H. E.,
et al. (2013). Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use
disorders: ndings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 382,
15751586. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6
Wilson, S. J., Smyth, J. M., and Maclean, R. R. (2014). Integrating ecological momentary assessment and functional brain imaging methods: new avenues for studying
and treating tobacco dependence. Nicotine Tob. Res. 16(Suppl. 2), S102S110.
doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt129
Woo, C. W., Koban, L., Kross, E., Lindquist, M. A., Banich, M. T., Ruzic, L., et al.
(2014). Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection.
Nat. Commun. 5, 5380. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6380
Xing, C., and Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Aiming at happiness: how motivation affects
attention to and memory for emotional images. Motiv. Emot. 30, 243250. doi:
10.1007/s11031-006-9032-y
Zaretsky, M., Mendelsohn, A., Mintz, M., and Hendler, T. (2010). In the eye
of the beholder: internally driven uncertainty of danger recruits the amygdala and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 22632275. doi:
10.1162/jocn.2009.21402
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was conducted
in the absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could be construed
as a potential conict of interest.
Received: 14 November 2014; accepted: 21 January 2015; published online: 17
February 2015.
Citation: Okon-Singer H, Hendler T, Pessoa L and Shackman AJ (2015) The neurobiology of emotioncognition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for future
research. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:58. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00058
This article was submitted to the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Copyright 2015 Okon-Singer, Hendler, Pessoa and Shackman. This is an openaccess article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
(CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided
the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
www.frontiersin.org