Genius Prodigies and Savants

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 61

Geniuses,

Prodigies &
Savants
Extraordinary people What
makes them tick
6-7 December, 1999
WP Young Room
University of Sydney

This event was conceived and organised by the


Centre for the Mind, Joint Venture - Australian
National University & University of Sydney

www.centreforthemind.com
The Centre for the Mind gratefully acknowledges all those who
contributed to this event, in particular David Helfgott, the Tao family
and the many speakers who presented such inspirational material. We
would also like to acknowledge the support of New Scientist.

Contents

Page
v-vi
Foreword Professor Allan Snyder
Event Programme
Background on Speakers
Introduction
Phillip Adams
First Impressions
Robyn Williams
Unveiling the Savant Mind
Dr Robyn Young
Normal Potential: What is it?
Dr Glenison Alsop
Insight: Creative Genius
Fiona Hall
A Visual Feast
Dr Darold Treffert

vii-ix
x-xv
1-4
5-8
9-12

Do the Brains of Extraordinary People


Differ from Normal People
Professor Michael OBoyle

45-47

How Do They Do It?


A Savant Prime Number Calculator
Dr Mike Anderson

48

Perspectives from a Psychological


Theory of Intelligence
Dr Mike Anderson

49

How Do They Do It? Savants


Professor John Mitchell

50-51

13-14

A Teachers Perspective
Lynne Kelly

52-54

15-17

Education: Help or Hindrance?


Trevor Clark

55-57

18-22

Genius + Madness. Is There a Connection?


Professor Jack Pettigrew

58-59

23-24

Journal and Newspaper articles about research from


the Centre for the Mind

Insights from a Savant Calculator


25-33

Is Integer Arithmetic Fundamental to Mental


Processing?: The Mind's Secret Arithmetic
Professor Allan Snyder and Professor John Mitchell
Proceedings of the Royal Society, London

34-35
36-40

60-81
82-93
94-99
100-105

41-44
ii

iii

Foreword
Professor Allan Snyder, FRS
Director, Centre for the Mind
One of the enduring mysteries of the mind is that so much is
done unconsciously. Great ideas seem to pop up from
nowhere.
Clearly, a world of unconscious information is sifted through,
by mechanisms of which we are totally unaware, to arrive at
our perception and judgements.
One way to get insight into the fascinating workings of the
unconscious is to understand unusual minds. These minds
seem to have privileged access to the unconscious, as I discuss
in the last two articles of this book.
Art, music and mathematics are often presumed to be the
supreme expression of human achievement. Yet, they are
mysteriously the very skills that seem to appear effortlessly
in the case of child prodigies or even spontaneously in the
case of individuals who suffer from rare forms of brain
damage. We are compelled to understand this phenomon.
Professor Allan Snyder recently completed filming of a BBC
documentary and a radio documentary on the Centre for the
Minds research into Geniuses, Prodigies and Savants. Professor Snyder is the Director of the Centre.

iv

Two days in December 1999 marked an historic occasion in

Perhaps what stands foremost in my mind is the depth of


compassion and understanding by everyone in their reaching
out to understand unusual minds.
Reading about the feats of extraordinary minds and scientific
research at the frontier of knowledge is in itself exciting. But,
nothing can equal the exhilaration of witnessing these
breathtaking minds in action right before our eyes.

Event Programme
Monday 6th December 1999

Opening Address
The occasion was one of a continuous high. Many of the
moments were captured by unprecedented media interest
including TV coverage from the BBC, the ABC and all major
print media, and extensive radio coverage. A number of radio
and TV documentaries are now in preparation. And, the event
in its entirety was filmed by a visual anthropologist.
This collection of summaries encapsulates the main concepts
discussed at the event. I will not attempt to precis them further.

Phillip Adams

First Impressions Snapshots


Phillip Adams, Dr Glenison Alsop, Professor
Bruce Miller, Professor Jack Pettigrew,
9:10-10:30
Robyn Williams, Dr Robyn Young
Morning Tea

The impetus for this unique occasion originated with research


that Professor John Mitchell and I published in the
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London on the minds
secret arithmetic. The global debate resulting from this
research inspired the event.

9:00-9:10

10:30-11:20

Unveiling the Savant Mind


Dr Robyn Young

11:20-11:40

Normal Potential What is it?


We concluded that the extraordinary skills of savants, such

Dr Glenison Alsop

11:40-12:00

Insight Creative Genius


Fiona Hall, Visual Artist
Lunch

12:00-12:20
12:20-1:30

Visual Feast Short Film


1:30-1:45
vi

vii

Event Programme

Event Programme

Monday 6th December 1999

Tuesday 7th December 1999

What Do Unusual Minds Tell Us About


Ourselves

Welcome

Dr Robyn Young, Professor Allan Snyder,


Dr Mike Anderson

Professor Allan Snyder


1:45-3:00

Insight The Savant Mind


Dr Joan Curtis
Afternoon Tea

3:00-3:15
3:15-3:45

9:00-9:10

Wired to Create: Geniuses, Prodigies &


Savants The Brain
Professor Bruce Miller, Professor
Michael OBoyle
Morning Tea

9:10-10:30
10:30-11:20

How Do They Do it ? Savants


Insight Family Perspective
Dr Billy Tao

3:45-4:00

Performance Trevor Tao

Transition From Prodigy & Savant to


Adulthood
Dr Joan Curtis, Gillian Helfgott, Dr Billy
Tao, Terence Tao, Nigel Tao, Trevor
Tao, Simon Tedeschi, Dr Robyn Young

Lunch
4:00-5:00

Close

Childs Play Hypothetical with Kids


Lynne Kelly

11:20-12:30
12:30-1:00
1:00-2:00
2:00-2:30

Education: Its Role Help or Hindrance

Insight Personal Viewpoint


Gillian Helfgott

Dr Mike Anderson, Professor Allan


Snyder, Professor John Mitchell

5:00-5:20
5:30

Lounge Refreshments

5:30-7:00

Concert David Helfgott

7:00-8:00
viii

Dr Glenison Alsop, Lynne Kelly, Trevor


Clark, Dr Robyn Young, Dr Mike Anderson
Afternoon Tea

Genius + Madness: Is There a


Connection?

2:30-3:45
3:45-4:15

4:15-5:30
ix

Trevor Clark
Background on Speakers
Phillip Adams
Writer and compere of the influential Australian Broadcasting
Corporation Radio National program Late Night Live. Phillip
was the foundation chairman of the Commission for the
Future, Chairman of the Australian Film Commission, the
Australian Film Institute, the National Australia Day Council
and President of the Victorian Council for the Arts. His most
recent books include Retreat from Tolerance, Talk Back and
A Billion Voices.

Acting Director of Services at the Autism Association of New


South Wales, and the Principal for the Vern-Barnett School
of Children with Autism. Trevor is the winner of the 1995
Hollingworth International Research Award. He is a
community member of the NSW Association for Gifted and
Talented Children, the Gifted Learning Disabled Support
Group and the Australian Special Education Association.

Dr Joan Curtis

A recognised authority on profoundly gifted children.


Glenison is a member of the CHIP Foundation which was
created to develop children with high intellectual potential,
and is currently writing a book Demonstrating where
Development and Social Psychology leads.

Founder of the Mansfield Autistic Centre and the Mansfield


Adult Autistic Services in Victoria. The Mansfield Autistic
Centre provides travelling teachers who can visit families with
autistic children living at home, full-time remedial training
and education and adult residence for autistic men to promote
independence. Joan has now retired as Director of Services
at Mansfield Autistic Centre but continues as Co-ordinator
of MACCRO.

Dr Mike Anderson

Fiona Hall

Dr Glenison Alsop

A remarkable South Australian visual artist, creative genius


and winner of the inaugural Contempora Award.

xi

Lynne Kelly
Principal, Virtual School for the Gifted, a school which has
no physical location and offers over 25 courses to gifted
students all over the world. Lynne is also the Co-ordinator of
the Gifted and Talented Program at the Methodist Ladies
College which is a prestigious girls school. Lynne has a
Masters in Education, a Graduate Diploma in Computing, a
Diploma of Education and a Bachelor of Engineering. Lynne
is the author of eight education texts and a CD-ROM.

Professor Bruce Miller


Bruce Miller is an AW Clausen Professor of Neurology at the
University of California, San Francisco Medical School.
Bruce discovered that a form of dementia triggered amazing
artistic prowess in patients who previously had no interest or
ability in art.

Professor John Mitchell


John is a mathematical genius who has been involved in
various aspects of physics and biology for much of his early
career. His current work at the Centre focuses on unravelling
the phenomenon behind our innate mathematical abilities. He
is a founding member of the Centre and is the co-author of
the internationally acclaimed paper The Minds Secret
Arithmetic.

xii

researcher on how intellectual ability is mediated by the


structure and circuitry of the human brain. His area of
expertise is Cognitive Neuropsychology. He has won various
awards including the Outstanding Educator Award, The
National Honor Society, Iowa State University.

Professor Jack Pettigrew


Professor of Physiology and Director at the Vision, Touch
and Hearing Research Centre at the University of Queensland.
Jack is a fiercely original researcher who has generated
immense international interest on his break-through work on
bipolar disorder and its links to creativity. He has 159
publications in refereed journals and collectively receives over
300 citations a year. Jack is the only Fellow of the Royal
Society in Queensland and is in high demand to speak at
international conferences. Jack won the first Commonwealth
Special Research Centre award in 1988 at the University of
Queensland.

Professor Allan W Snyder


Director, Centre for the Mind, a joint venture of the Australian
National University and the University of Sydney. Allans
discovery with John Mitchell that savant skills are within us
all received world-wide attention. He was awarded the
International Australia Prize from the Prime Minister in 1997
and was featured as one of Australias 10 most creative minds
in the Bulletin/Newsweek magazine in 1998. Allan holds the
150th anniversary chair of Science and the Mind at the
University of Sydney and the Peter Karmel Chair of Science
and the Mind at the Australian National University. He is a
fellow of the Royal Society of London.
xiii

in 1996. Robyn has published many books including Here


Comes the Philistines and And Now for Something Completely
Different.

The Tao Family


Billy Tao is a Paediatrician. His wife Grace taught high school
mathematics. Their three remarkable children, Nigel, Terry
and Trevor, are now young adults. Terry was a child prodigy
who became a UCLA Assistant Professor of Mathematics at
age twenty. Terry made front-page news when he started high
school (doing Year 11 Mathematics and Physics) at age seven.
Trevor was diagnosed autistic when he was two but went on
to win many prizes in music, chess and mathematics
competitions. Nigel is now a Science/Economics student and
tutor at the Australian National University.

Simon Tedeschi
Simon Tedeschi was born a musical prodigy and is the 1998
Young Performer of the Year. Simon is a musical genius.

Dr Darold Treffert
Darold Treffert is celebrated internationally for his work with
autistic savants and is the author of Extraordinary People.

Robyn Williams
Author and host of the popular ABC Radio National Science
Show, Robyn is the President of the Australian Science
Communicators and is a visiting Professor at the University
of New South Wales. Robyn has won various awards
including the Michael Daley Award of Science in 1996 and
the Individual Award, Centre for Australian Cultural Studies

xiv

xv

Introduction*
Phillip Adams, AO
Recent election outcomes have intensified the hate part of
my love/hate relationship with Australia. Add to this the glass
ceiling that presses down on fellow broadcasters like Robyn
Williams who have discovered that the size of audiences is
inversely proportional to the quality of the broadcast, Ive
come to the conclusion that my old impression that Australia
is an anti-intellectual realm is correct. I used to see Australia
as looking at a great pair of buttocks squatting in the South
Pacific with Sydney and Perth at the hips and Adelaide
approximately at the sphincter. This image was echoed in
the iconography of the coat of arms because here are two
creatures which are huge of bum and small of head.
The Centre for the Mind is such a big idea, it is too big for
any one university, so it is shared by the Australian National
University and the University of Sydney.
How did this come about? While many of you were attending
university, I was reading comics. It was only through comics
that I educated myself and I discovered that when the planet

we realise the dream by investing in groundbreaking research


which shatters mindsets by stage-managing spectacular
initiatives which challenge and inspire and by acting as a nexus
for the great minds of our world.
The Centre is a nexus for neurons, a safe house for synapses
and right from day one, it has been provocative and
fascinating. We only have to look at the calibre of people the
Centre has bought to this country, the likes of Oliver Sacks
and Daniel Dennett.
Typical of the Centre, we are focusing on the greatest mystery
of all, the greatest mystery of creation, which is the strange
mollusc we have between our ears. The snail-like creature
beneath the calipash of the skull.
Night after night, I sit in the radio studio listening to people
talking about this organ and disputing its significance or its
nature. The study of consciousness is probably the hottest of
all; not only among scientists, but amongst philosophers and
those who study artificial intelligence. There is no more
fascinating or awesome task than the study of what makes
the human mind tick, and that is the purpose of the event that
follows.

with him. Im always astonished at what happens between


his ears. Hes not actually interested in scenery unless theres
a ruin in it so he finds himself being bounced around my
property in a four wheel drive tightly disinterested in it, but I
was fascinated by his first encounter with a farm gate. Now
it is an Australian tradition that if youre sitting in the front
with the driver, youve got to open the gates. So we stop at
the first gate. This is a gate that cows can open, they come
over with their big wet noses and push the wire off the post
and the gate swings open. My seven-year-old could open it.
Barry couldnt. He spent about 15 minutes tugging at the
wire and looking at it at various angles, trying to conceive
the wire holistically. Finally he opened the gate. I said Barry,
we have a problem, youre on the wrong side.
Now I was telling this story in the middle of the desert to
Professor Paul Davies with whom I was making a television
series called The Big Questions and for some reason the
producers decided to put this English academic into RM
Williams clobber, so he was being dressed up as a cowboy.
He looked exceedingly odd. He was in the process of dressing
himself when I was telling him the Barry Jones story. He
laughed and I noticed he couldnt do his belt up. He had one
of those belts with two loops through which you snake the
leather and it completely defeated him and I found great
comfort in this; that two of the most intelligent people I knew
couldnt do their trousers up or open gates. I think this sets
up the theme we are looking at because the human brain is a
profoundly odd organ.
* The previous three pages are a transcript from the event and not written specifically for this
publication.

Phillip Adams - a national treasure.

First Impressions Snapshots*


Robyn Williams

deconstructive; I think you can turn that on its head and show
that their capacities are in fact very human ones.

When I was 15 or 16, I was going to a special concert at the


Royal Festival Hall in London and it was completely packed.
I raced up the wrong staircase with my Australian friend and
we blundered straight into the royal box where your Queen
sits on special occasions and there was Bertrand Russell, with
the head of his family the Duke of Bedford. Bertrand Russell
had been a hero of mine for many years, for as long as I can
remember. He revolutionised maths, he wrote the Second
Principia at the turn of the century, he revolutionised
philosophy. He won a Nobel Prize for Literature and he was
an amazingly radical protestor. This was in fact his 90th
birthday concert, only a few months after and a few months
before he was actually being arrested by the police in Trafalgar
Square.

I have broadcast a number of programmes recently about


aspects of the brain which show how extraordinarily
developed ideas show how little you know. The other day, I
was interviewing Tim Crowe. Tim is a Professor of Psychiatry
at Oxford. He describes schizophrenia as not a straightforward
disease - but the price you pay for having language, and many
of the symptoms - like voices and the strange bits of
information that flow through, are in fact, he suggests, the
way that language has gone somewhat out of control.

I went up to this man in the box and noticed a pimple on his


nose. This genius has a pimple and the most extraordinary
thing is that I have found that if ever I come across someone
who is supposed to be a genius or a prodigy, my meeting
them can in some way devalue them because they turn out to
be human beings. I noted that practically every genius and
hero through history has in some ways been deconstructed.

Then theres Sarah Blakemore who is doing research on


tickling. The reason she is doing this is to find out whether
the brains understanding of self is different in certain people,
indeed against schizophrenics. In fact, you find if they put
them in one of those big machines you can tell where the
chemical work is going on, that people who have symptoms
turn out to be rather like self ticklers which go to different
parts of the brain. It is awareness of self.

I love them being human and thats why Bertrand Russell


grew in stature in my eyes. But you can see that people write
off Mozart because a lot of people say that Mozarts work is
80% predictable. When he was young he was writing amazing
music, but it was music for young people, unlike Mendelssohn
who was writing grown-up music. And so people get

John Bradshaw, who is a Professor at Monash, talked about


synaethesia where you actually see colours associated with
words and he described that growing up is involved in the
paring away of function. We become more and more efficient
at things, you have an immense capacity of doing things at
the beginning, things are too linked and he likens it to a statue
being a huge block of stone. You remove the excess stone
and end up with material in your brain that you can actually
use effectively, except it doesnt happen completely in some

people. So you get various folk who say the words John
Howard which to them represent a dirty brown colour. I
wonder what colour Jeff Kennett would be.
I suggest that what this Centre has done has shown very
effectively that genius is a matter of supreme concentration
and it is also a matter of learning randomlessly. Jack Pettigrew
is interested in the way the mind can run free. Well when I
arrived, I started talking in German to Allan Snyder, for no
reason. My mother had a very free brain and people couldnt
understand what the hell she was talking about because when
she was making a joke, she missed out the middle two lines
and quite often you do that. Spike Milligan has just been on
the radio talking about bipolar disorder which is why he is
devoted to lithium and Milligan is a quintessential guy. Half
the time his randomness and jokes really worked, the other
half, we had no idea what he was talking about. He looked
mad and in some ways he was mad. It seems to me there are
ways in which human beings can in fact catch up with
Robyn Williams - the familiar voice of the ABC Science show.

Unveiling the Savant Mind


Dr Robyn Young
A savant is an individual demonstrating exceptional skills
despite an overall low level of general functioning. Savant
Syndrome refers to observable behavioural characteristics
rather than to a diagnostic classification and the term therefore
incorporates all types of intellectual disability or mental
retardation including autism.
There are three types based on level of skill:
1. Prodigious savant - skills not only remarkable in
contrast to their low general level of functioning, but also
to levels beyond the accomplishments of most people in the
general population.
2. Talented savant - skills are beyond the range predicted
by a generally low level of intelligence, but are not
exceptional in the prodigious sense because they might be
expected to occur at similar levels among non-disabled
persons.
3. Splinter skills - levels of interest and competence
appreciably above the general level of functioning and
which are far more common among the autistic population.

highly developed sensory discriminations


artistic ability
mechanical dexterity
memory for facts.

I conducted three main studies:


Study 1 The Examination of a Musical Savant and his
Family.
Findings:
musical ability among available members of the
savants family
perfect pitch in all musical savants and some
family members
memory for music was well organised and
structurally based
some cognitive functions may be independent
from a general capacity.
Study 2 Calendar Calculation
Findings:
savants were aware of the 14 calendar

Despite the range of abilities available to all humans,


savant-type skills fall into a discrete range of skills:
musical precocity

10

Study 3 Psychometric Evaluation


Findings:
Significant correlations between skill level and IQ
suggest that the extent to which the ability is developed
reflects more sophisticated cognitive processes and is
IQ dependent
Mean IQ of immediate family members supports the
predisposition of savants toward higher levels of
intelligence
Presence of similar skills in family members
Presence of as many as nine skills (M=4.4, SD=2.2)
suggesting that the same underlying processes may be
involved in the development of subsequent skills.

In summary, my studies suggest that a savant is a


neurologically impaired individual with idiosyncratic and
divergent profiles of intellectual ability and language and intellectual impairments consistent with autism, who has an
intense interest and preoccupation with a particular area of
skill. These circumstance, together with the necessary preserved neurological capacity to process information in a manner relevant to their skill (probably sequential), a well-developed memory (probably declarative), a familial predisposition toward high achievement (possibly innate), and adequate
support, encouragement and reinforcement, provide the necessary climate for savant skills to develop.

It is suggested here that savant skills are suited to the


individuals who develop them because
(i)
(ii)
(iii)
(iv)

they are highly dependent on some preserved


neurological ability (perhaps declarative memory)
they require little higher order manipulation of
cognitive stimuli
they can occur in the absence of a high general
level of intellectual functioning
the processes involved can be developed further
with practice.

The existence of savants is consistent with a theory that some


skills are based on relatively well-differentiated neurological
capacities; the skills developed by savants are generally rulebased, rigid and highly structured, lacking critical aspects of
creativity and cognitive flexibility abilities generally
considered to reflect intelligence.
11

Coffee break at the art display during the Geniuses, Prodigies


and Savants event.
12

Normal Potential What Is It?


Dr Glenison Alsop

validation of the highest learning children, the most important


experiences on which their adaptive behaviour and selflearning is based will be attenuated.

In the quest to better understand the most extraordinary, we


are left to wonder about the source from whence they come.
In other words, is there something about normal intellectual
potential which can in any way predict extraordinary potential
that associated with genius, prodigy, savant and the
realisation of that potential into extraordinary achievement.

It becomes possible to hypothesise therefore that there is a


link between the experience of learning and brain function.
The question becomes: could learning behaviour become a
variable that shapes brain function, mediated by the
individuals understanding of his or her level of competence?

Extraordinary intelligence certainly has its place in the


emergence of the genius and the prodigy. Like the prodigy,
the savant too is a performer. But unlike the inclusive
creativity of the genius, the savant is characteristically rigid
and exclusive. That is, savantism is associated with an unusual
form of behaviour or with its unusual expression in a highly
particularised domain.

Should this be the case, as a society we will need to change


our values if we wish exceptional intelligence to be a
determinant in the future.

Although we recognise consistencies in the way all three


function in their personal attributes, and the source of their
achievements, we cannot as yet replicate the conditions where
we can predict one will emerge from among us. Very high
intelligence is a pre-condition of the prodigy and the genius,
but both also rely on other aptitudes and personal traits.
We do have a very good understanding of the conditions which
nurture extraordinary intellectual potential we know them,
but as a society we do little to respond to them. Yet social
values are critical to how children of high intellectual potential
(CHIP) learn to manage and respond to their achievement.
Arguably self-knowledge is a necessary determinant of
achievement. Without appropriate social support and
13

Dr Glenison Alsop, Professor Bruce Miller and Professor Jack


Pettigrew.

14

Insight
Fiona Hall, Visual Artist
Through the layering of ideas in my work, I try to show the
multifaceted nature of environmental issues and the
interconnectedness of concepts and materials that might at
first appear quite alien to each other.
At the very early stages of thinking about a work, I have on
the one hand an idea about the subject I intend to work with
and on the other hand, an emotional response to that subject,
and also a strong attraction to a material which I may
incorporate into the work. At first the subject matter and the
material generally appear quite disparate even to me, and its
a struggle to amalgamate the two, but I usually find that the
disparity pays higher dividends in the final work than if I
started with materials and subject matter that seemed much
more appropriate to each other, but would in the end be too
predictable.
I have learned to trust my peripheral mind and to bring quick
or fleeting perceptions more sharply into focus. If I sit in a
chair and try concertedly to come up with a good idea, nothing
very exciting will happen. I think its important to have an
awareness of the territory which interests you, but to avoid
the potential constraints inherent in this kind of focus.

the mind might be in the future, enabling it to wander quite


freely over a variety of issues related to other works. I find
myself working on a number of projects simultaneously, each
requiring different mediums and different kinds of problem
solving.
Recently I have been working on a couple of landscape
architecture commissions which have even further extended
and challenged the materials and scale I work with. I now
find that I am working simultaneously on very small intricate
things and on work of landscape proportions both of which
may be closely aligned conceptually.
For example, the work Paradisis Terrestris Entitled (sardine
can sculptures of Australian native plants which are titled with
Aboriginal language names of these species) has close ties
with the garden commission at the National Gallery of
Australia, where all of the surviving Aboriginal names of the
plants species used in the garden are written in the pathways.
What might be seen as distinctly different methodologies
actually come from the same conceptual base. More and more
it is the multifaceted rendering of a concept into several vastly
different mediums and modes of practice that interests me.
The work is enriched by these different permutations, that
then come back in altered forms.

My work generally necessitates an intense amount of labour


in its production. Ive now become accustomed to the idea
that a work may take several months to complete. At times
this does seem a bit too arduous, but I do derive much pleasure
and satisfaction from the process. In the process of making,

15

16

A Visual Feast
Dr Darold Treffert
(During the Geniuses, Prodigies and Savant event videos of
Dr Trefferts autistic patients and their art works were
screened. He describes these here.)
To merely describe Leslies music or Alonzos sculptures is
to do justice to neither. The spectacular expertise of the
prodigious savant must be seen and heard to fully appreciate
the rarity and uniqueness of this astonishing juxtaposition of
ability and disability the islands of genius in these
remarkable people. These videos demonstrate the nature and
scope of the extraordinary abilities in four prodigious savants,
and explore as well the vast implications that savant syndrome
has for understanding brain function over-all, and hidden
potential, perhaps, in each of us.
Alonzo
Alonzo now lives in a condominium in Boulder, Colorado
and has a part-time job at the local YMCA. His remarkable
sculpting ability has been his conduit toward normalisation
and greater independence. Alonzo sculpts animals
magnificently, using crystalline clay as his medium. Horses
are his favourites but any animal can be copied exactly from
just a brief glimpse at a two-dimensional picture or from a
stroll through the zoo. He can complete such a horse or any
other animal in about forty-five minutes. He has also done
life-size pieces, some of which have sold for as much as
$45,000. His work is sought and appreciated around the
world.

Plumeria acutisolia: frangipani; araliya; malliya poo, 1999


aluminium and tin by Fiona Hall.
The Centre for the Mind would like to thank Fiona Hall for allowing the
use of her art work in this publication.

17

The film illustrates Alonzos remarkable, innate talent as a


18

sculptor. He has never had an art lesson in his life. His case
is even more rare because he is what is called an acquired
savant, that is, a person whose savant skill emerged following
a CNS injury or illness. It was following a fall as an infant
that Alonzos incredible sculpting talent appeared. As with
other savants, Alonzos family has been a loving and powerful
force in training the talent through encouragement and
reinforcement. Their unconditional acceptance of Alonzo,
and their focus on ability rather than the disability, has been
instrumental as well in furthering his art which is his
language and in so doing furthering Alonzos social and
daily living skills as well.
In the acquired savant particularly there is evidence that
damage to the left hemisphere of the brain results in right
hemisphere compensation producing then, the right brain
skills art, music, lightning calculating and other
mathematical skills, calendar calculating and spatial and
mechanical skills so typical and characteristic of savants
overall. These right brain skills are always coupled with
prodigious memory exceedingly narrow but exceptionally
deep probably because of corresponding damage to higher
level cognitive memory circuitry permitting access to, but
also the limitation of, lower level habit memory circuitry. The
combination of right brain skills linked to habit memory is
savant syndrome. In the male foetus circulating testosterone
can be one source of neuronal damage to the later developing
left hemisphere, with subsequent right hemisphere
compensation, which may account for the approximately 6:1
male:female ratio in savant syndrome.

music. Instead some genetic or inherited factors so called


ancestral memory must additionally be present to account
for this vast, inborn store of knowledge, talent and skills. This
film explores that possibility suggesting such buried potential
may well reside within all of us. Our task now is to learn
how to tap such buried potential. Meanwhile Alonzo
continues to sculpt, to create, to enjoy and to grow toward
even more independence in his infectiously mellow manner.
His is a most inspirational story.
Tony
Tony is an autistic savant (approximately 50% of savants are
autistic; the other 50% have some other form of developmental
disability or brain injury). Tony is blind. And Tony is a
musical genius. This film documents that prodigious musical
skill. I first met Tony in 1989 when he was awarded a summer
scholarship at the prestigious Berklee College of Music in
Boston, Massachusetts. Tony so impressed the staff at Berklee
with his astounding musical talent that summer, that he was
admitted there as a full time student and graduated magna
cum laude in 1996. That is a striking achievement for someone
both blind and autistic. Tony is a tremendous jazz musician
and improvises freely and impressively.

In the prodigious savant, such as Alonzo, repetition and


practice alone would not be sufficient to account for the innate
access that these remarkable people have to what have been
called the rules the vast syntax of art, mathematics or

Tonys spectacular jazz ability extends to other musical styles


as well, ranging from country to classics. He plays fourteen
musical instruments, twelve of them proficiently. Like many
other musical savants, his ability surfaced at an astounding
level when his mother, Janice, bought him a chord organ at a
sale. Tony was two years old at the time. On the film his
mother recounts Tonys remarkable progress since then,
including his repertoire of over 7000 songs even before we
stopped counting 8 years ago. The film shows Tony receiving
his diploma from Berklee, a highlight in his life and a tribute

19

20

again to the usefulness of training the talent. There has


been no dreaded trade-off of musical skills for language
acquisition and growth in social skills. Quite the contrary,
those skills have provided, again, a conduit toward
normalisation, an impressive transition indeed. Tony
maintains a web site at www.ccmw.org/deblois/.
Richard
Richard is an autistic savant who has continued to excel as an
artist of international renown. He uses the unique medium of
Swiss oil crayons and concentrates on landscape scenes done
with breathtaking fidelity and rich, deep colours. His
preoccupation with, and phenomenal skill at depicting light
remains striking and amazing. As his repertoire has grown
however, so have his language and social skills, again with
no dreaded trade-off of special abilities. This film is a brief
glimpse at some of his work, and a brief introduction to him
as a person. The best way to describe Richard and his
phenomenal talent though, is to view his art, which can be
accessed at http://wawro.org. His art speaks for itself and
requires no elaboration.
Leslie
When Dustin Hoffman watched Leslie play the piano on a
1983 60 Minutes program he was moved to tears. That
program inspired Dustin Hoffman to play the part of the savant
when the script for the movie Rainman came to Hollywood
in 1986. That movie made autistic savant a household word.
Leslies story of his incredible talent, nurtured by the untiring
effort, faith and belief of his foster-mother, May, continues to
inspire still wherever Leslie plays. That story was told in yet
another movie The Woman Who Willed a Miracle which
depicts so vividly the remarkable moment in which Leslie at
21

age 14, played flawlessly from beginning to end


Tchaikovskys Piano Concerto No. 1, having heard it for the
first time as a theme song to a television movie that evening.
Leslie is blind, mentally handicapped and has cerebral palsy,
yet his music is magnificent. His repertoire seems bottomless
as well and professional musicians marvel at his innate grasp
of the rules of music. He has never had a music lesson in
his life. Imaging studies have documented the type of left
hemisphere brain damage cited above. Music is Leslies
language. And it has been his conduit toward normalisation
as well and this film amply documents that progress and
growth as with the other savants. His story, like so many
savants, is better seen than told and these brief film clips, I
hope, exemplify that.
The real significance of the savant is our ability to understand
him or her, yet no model of brain function will be complete
until it can fully incorporate and explain this amazing
circumstance of incredible islands of ability existing in a sea
of disability. My thoughts to better understand that
circumstance, and to better appreciate the vast implications
savant syndrome has for understanding and eventually tapping
hidden potential that I am convinced resides in each of us,
are contained in much greater detail in my book,
Extraordinary People: Understanding Savant Syndrome,
which will be re-released soon with an epilogue update.
Newer information about Savant Syndrome, incorporating
some of those recent research advances, can also be found on
my savant syndrome website at http://wismed.com/
foundation/savant.htm. There is a recent article entitled The
Savant Syndrome and Autistic Disorder in CSN Spectrums:
The International Journal of Neuropsychiatric Medicine,
volume 4, no. 12, pp. 57-60, 1999.

22

3.

Insights from a Savant Calculator


Dr Joan Curtis

An autistic young man whose memory for names and


dates is phenomenal.

I am presenting 3 examples of autistic prodigies.


1. An exhibition of 30 paintings by six autistic young men.
2. Two electrical models invented by an autistic young
man who has no literacy or numeracy skills, and no
training in electricity.

Tim Mallows, an autistic savant, and Dr Joan Curtis.

While the unusual talents of some autistic people are so


dramatic that they catch everyones attention, it is important
to realise that even low functioning autistic people have
islands of ability and if we search for them some remarkable
gifts come to light.

An electronic dog and music box invented and constructed by


autistic savant Guy Burton.
23

24

Living with Exceptional Children


Dr Billy Tao

BACKGROUND:
I shall begin by saying something about the background of
my family. I am a paediatrician and my wife Grace was a
high school mathematics teacher. We have three children:
Terry, Trevor and Nigel, all born in Adelaide.
Terry is now 24. He started school at five, but by seven he
was studying part-time in primary school and part-time in a
nearby high school, doing Year 11 and 12 Pure Math and
Year 11 Physics. He did his matriculation exams in Math I
and II when he was eight, and was allowed to study as a parttime student in first and second year Mathematical courses at
Flinders University when he was nine. He won a bronze medal
in the International Mathematical Olympiad Competition
when he was 10, a silver medal in the following year, and a
gold medal the year after. At eight he scored 760 out of 800
in the American University entrance SAT-Math test. At 17
he completed his Masters degree at Flinders University, and
at 20 his Ph.D. at Princeton University. He has been an
Assistant Professor in Mathematics at UCLA since 1996, and
earlier this year he was the recipient of a Sloan Fellowship,
one of the most prestigious post-doctoral awards in America.
He is currently a visiting Fellow at the University of NSW.

composition competitions. He was also interested in chess,


and at 14 he was the Australian Junior Chess Champion. In
the following year he won the (Australian) National Chess
Player of the Year award. He was one of the six members
in the Australian Mens team in the International Chess
Olympiad held in Moscow in 1994. His ability in the
mathematical field actually came as a surprise. When he was
in primary and junior high school he was doing OK in statebased mathematical competitions, getting distinctions and
credits and so forth, but then from 1994 onwards he started to
win a series of prizes in national mathematical competitions.
The wins were slightly unexpected because unlike Terry and
Nigel he had not been reading mathematical books or
attending special tutorial lessons. We thought he already had
too much on his plate, in music, chess, and normal school
activities, and because of his autism we were not confident
about his writing and presentation skills.

Trevor is 22. He was diagnosed autistic when he was two.


He started to show his musical ability at an early age, and has
won prizes or medals in many piano performance and music

The ultimate surprise came in 1995, when he won a Westpac


Medal and then a place (together with Nigel) in the Australian
team for the International Mathematical Olympiad, which was
held in Toronto. He won a bronze medal on that occasion.
He matriculated with five merits, earning him an Australian
Students Prize for Excellence, and entered the University of
Adelaide in 1996 studying two degrees concurrently, in
Mathematical Science and Music. He was also the first student
allowed to major in both Music Performance and Composition
at the Elder Conservatorium. He was awarded the University
of Adelaides Deans Certificate of Merit for both Level I
and Level II math subjects. He is currently doing Level III
subjects.

25

26

His mathematical insight was quite unusual for an autistic


savant, and I sometimes wondered if he should still be labelled
as a savant or something else. So too was his creativity in
music composition, for he was not just copying or memorising
other composers works. His pieces were often quite original,
with his own distinctive style.

plays four musical instruments and has been active in several


different sports at ANU. His latest award in sports was a
best-and-fairest trophy in netball. He is also a tutor in residence at his university dorm. Despite his diverse interests
and busy social life he still managed to win two major prizes
in final year Economics earlier this year.

He has been winning prizes in composition since he was in


primary school, and one of his earliest winning pieces, a String
Quartet, was played by the Australian String Quartet in a music
workshop held in Adelaide in 1991, and broadcast on 5UV,
an Adelaide-based classical music radio station. Last year
his quintet entitled Abstractions was chosen for performance
in the Elder Conservatorium Centenary Concert, and has
received good reviews from music critics in both The
Australian and The Advertiser newspapers. He currently holds
a performance scholarship at the Elder Conservatorium, and
is the winner of the Conservatorium composition prize two
years in a row.

REFLECTIONS ON THEIR EDUCATIONAL


MODELS:

Nigel is 20 and is more of an all-rounder. He skipped one


grade in primary school, and then won a high school
scholarship. He continued to win prizes in many state and
national science and mathematics competitions, and competed
in the International Mathematical Olympiad at age 14 and
15, winning bronze medals on both occasions. He won a
1994 and a 1995 Australian Students Prize for Excellence,
and a national undergraduate scholarship in 1996 to study for
a combined degree in Science and Economics at the Australian
National University. In addition to Math, Computer and
Economics subjects, he has also taken Psychology, Biology,
Politics and Law courses at ANU in the past two years. He
27

I would now like to spend some time on their educational


models, which were slightly different from each other, because
the three of them had different personalities, interests and
strengths, and in Trevors case, the effect of autism. The same
can be said for other gifted children and their education in
general.
At the time when Terry and Trevor were discovered to be
unusual students it was almost 20 years ago, and special
educational programs for either gifted or high-functioning
autistic children were not in place as they are today. There
were no set models for us to follow, so we had to improvise
from the start. Ironically it was a good thing, because had we
not had the same degree of freedom to experiment, the
outcome could well have been quite different.
Terrys model could best be described as one of radical
acceleration, starting high school at seven, university at nine,
and becoming an Assistant Professor at 20. For it to succeed,
we had to have several pre-conditions. Firstly, we must not
have a failed precedent, which would certainly mean the end
of any proposal to put a seven-year-old student in high school

28

at that time. Secondly, we must have open-minded school


teachers and university staff who were willing to try the
unknown and take up the challenge. Thirdly, the primary
and high schools and the university must be fairly close to
each other so that travelling from one place to another was
still practical between lessons. Lastly, there must not be any
premature and adverse publicity at the start of the programme,
or else some egalitarian bureaucrat from the Education
Department could easily become alarmed and want to nip
the whole thing in the bud. Fortunately for Terry, by the
time his story appeared on the front page of the local
newspaper it was already fait accompli, so no one intervened,
even though the general policy at that time was against
accelerated education.
Trevors model was one of early intervention, which meant
finding out the diagnosis early, and getting professional help
early. Again at that time it was relatively uncharted territory
in teaching high functioning autistic children, and there was
not much experience from either the Education Department
or the Autism Association of South Australia. So the
Association assigned a special-Ed teacher by the name of Jean
Bryant to work with us, and explore how best to educate him
and integrate him into a normal school. Nowadays integration
of autistic children into normal schools is commonplace, but
in those days we had to find a school which was prepared to
try it first.

the same time we had also discovered his savant abilities in


music and chess, and arranged lessons for him with several
music teachers and chess players. Jean Bryant has since
written a book on her teaching experience with Trevor, which
is called The Opening Door.
Nigels model was a more traditional one, which was that of
a broad-based high-quality education with emphasis more on
diversity and lateral extension rather than vertical acceleration.
Even his university education followed the same mode. For
example, he has completed many of his university subjects
ahead of time and could have finished his combined degree
in three years, but instead of graduating early he has chosen
to spend another year studying non-core subjects such as
Psychology, Politics, Biology and Law. The choices of his
university subjects were all made by himself, but even at an
early age we were convinced that this model suited him better.
If we had implemented Terrys model onto him purely because
it has worked well for Terry, we probably would have a
mismatch and a different outcome for Nigel.

We did find a nearby school which seemed to suit Trevor


perfectly. It was a small school with little more than one
hundred students, and the principal was not daunted by the
idea of having an autistic child among his pupils. At about

One important aspect about Nigels education is also worth


mentioning. Being the youngest in the family and less well
known than either Terry or Trevor, he was somewhat
overshadowed by his two elder brothers, although by all
accounts he was a tremendously gifted child with an I.Q.
greater than 180. We sensed that he was not at his best when
he was living with us, and was probably held back because of
self-doubt and lack of motivation. Then an opportunity came
when he finished high school, and we actively encouraged
him to leave Adelaide to study at the Australian National
University. It proved to be a good move and had a very

29

30

positive effect on him. He has emerged from the shadow of


his brothers and become a wonderfully helpful and considerate
person. He started to achieve his full academic potential,
and has become a very popular person at his dorm. Again, I
want to emphasise that the same approach for Nigel may not
suit somebody else, who may not like the idea of leaving
home and venturing into a totally new and unfamiliar
environment.
My description of Terrys educational model as one of
acceleration and Nigels model as one of extension was made
deliberately to highlight their differences. In reality, the two
were just two slightly different versions of a universal model
of gifted education, which is best visualised as a pyramid.
The breadth of the pyramid is the extension part, and the height
of the pyramid is the acceleration part. The ultimate shape of
the pyramid, whether it is tall or squat, depends very much
on the characteristics of the childs ability and personality.
In Terrys case, the extension part of the model was provided
by allowing him to study in different grades on different
subjects at high school. So between the age of seven and 13,
while still attending university part-time in Math and Physics,
he was allowed to complete high school education in subjects
such as Chemistry, English, Latin, and Geography. In Nigels
case, acceleration was provided by allowing him to study high
school math when he was in primary school, and attending
math and physics lessons several grades ahead of his home
class when he was in high school. He finished his
matriculation Math I and II and Physics exams at age 13 and
14 respectively. However, unlike Terry he did not attend
university part-time when he was still in high school. Instead
31

he took up Music and Japanese, and spent more time in


extracurricular activities such as chess and school band.
I believe the major advantage of a pyramid model is the
stability factor. A purely vertical acceleration model is like a
column, which becomes more and more unstable as the height
increases. As bright children make the transition into
adulthood, it is important that they have a broad knowledge
base, and are emotionally mature to be able to withstand the
shocks of reality.

A CAUTIONARY NOTE ON THE POSSIBLE


OVERUSE OF THE WORD GENIUS:
Before I finish, I would like to make a plea to everyone here
not to overuse the word Genius. Genius is the highest level
of attainment in any particular field of human endeavour.
When compared with prodigies, geniuses imply much greater
depth and calibre in knowledge, wisdom, creativity,
experience, personalities, maturity of thought, and a host of
other valued human qualities and dimensions. Not only do
they have overwhelming talents and abilities, their greatness
and achievements must also pass the test of time. In contrast
the achievements of prodigies can often be described as
superficial dazzling speed of learning or high levels of
technical skills etc. They are often relatively young people
and still inexperienced in life. Like apprentices trying to
become masters, prodigies have a long way to go before they
can be called geniuses. In history, only a handful of geniuses
were ex-prodigies (e.g. Mozart), while many were little known
during childhood (e.g. Einstein).

32

It is human nature to overstate a persons ability or weakness,


particularly from people with a bias, such as admirers or
critics. We often call someone a genius or idiot more than
the person deserves. We have a tendency to call above-average
children bright, bright children gifted, gifted children
prodigies, and prodigies geniuses, without realising that
this can put severe pressure on them and actually hinder their
progress. The word genius is a very big hat to wear, and has
to be handed out cautiously. Just ask the Nobel Prize winners
of our time: how many of them will feel comfortable if they
are called geniuses, which may evoke the thought of a
Shakespeare or Einstein? Yet they are all very bright and
successful people, the highest achievers in their fields. Some
of them may well have been prodigies when they were young.
The fact that they may not like to be called geniuses does not
mean that they see themselves as failures. So I think we should
refrain from overusing the word genius on prodigies, and
avoid over-expectation on these very bright people.

Transition of the Prodigy and Savant


Simon Tedeschi
I was always different both as a baby and young child
(according to my mother) but, unlike other gifted people, I
showed no signs of prodigious ability until around five or six
years old, before which my life was quite normal.
Once I heard a piano my life was irrevocably altered. It
became a vehicle for my emotions, my intellect, my drive
and my spirit. My need to make beautiful music was
overwhelming. It took the place of normal childhood pursuits
and when I was very young, I didnt really feel the sacrifice.
Only now as an adult I can look back and understand that to
devote myself to music, other aspects of life, usually so
integral to normal development, were omitted. I spent very
little time in the classroom, although nominally I attended
school. A lot of that time was spent in the school hall with
my best mate, the piano. Thus, my being different was
cultivated. In the afternoons instead of games or sport or
homework, I practised while others played soccer and cricket.
I was already, from the age of eight or nine, up on a concert
platform with all its associated challenges.
Adolescence somehow didnt have time to emerge childhood
merged into adulthood without the normal experiences and
rebellions of teenage angst. The gaining of maturity was
unusually rapid, driven by overseas tours (alone from the age
of 14), by earnings, tax returns, an agent and signing contracts
heady stuff for a teen.
It has not been easy, but if I had my time again, I suspect
history would repeat itself.

Terrence Tao, Trevor Tao and Nigel Tao.

33

34

Living with David


Gillian Helfgott
The topic of my comments today is Living With David
Helfgott. I wonder how many of you have seen the movie
Shine. The film, whilst never setting out to be a documentary,
was inspired by Davids life and gave a wonderful insight
into his personality. Whilst some scenes were dramatised
and certain characters had to be combined, the director Scott
Hicks made a film that is not only one of great integrity but
also truly inspirational.
David was born into a Jewish/Polish family, who had little
prosperity. In 1947 and by the age of five David was showing
a great interest in the piano and by nine performing major
works by Chopin and Liszt. By the age of 12 he was declared
a prodigy, and the ensuing publicity kept his name to the fore
in Western Australia. He won the ABC Concerto Competition
in that State six times and had a great ambition to study
overseas.

A panel session of Geniuses, Prodigies & Savants (Transition


from Prodigy & Savant to Adulthood, 4-5pm Monday).

35

In direct contrast to Dr and Mrs Tao, who appear to have so


wisely guided their three outstanding children in their
education, David, whose father had a leaning more to
communism than capitalism, sent David to the local State
schools. He did not even attend the special music school in
Perth and did not apply for any scholarships to colleges or
music schools. When he was forced to leave home by his
father in his late teens, he then applied for the Royal College
of Music in London and gained acceptance and a scholarship.
The years in London he found very fruitful, until ill health
intervened in his last year at the College and he returned to
Australia and over a decade in and out of mental institutions.
36

After twelve years away from the professional concert


platform, David returned to performing in 1984 an
outstanding achievement considering his traumatic past.
I met David when he just started playing in the Wine Bar in
Perth in 1983. The impression he made on me was immediate,
overwhelming and lasting. His hyperactivity was very
apparent, but the warmth of his personality and desire for
acceptance were extremely appealing. I guess I fell in love
with him on the spot. The next day he saw me and asked me
to marry him and I nearly said yes! Why?
David is an exceptional person. Whilst he has many
mannerisms and certain nervous anxiety at times, he is full of
love. He wants everyone to be kind as indeed he is. He does
not know meanness, jealousy, resentment, sustained anger or
bitterness. And this is despite twelve years on and off in
mental institutions and that was in the seventies which was
not the most enlightened time in mental health treatment.
Shock therapy was given, heavy medication, he was denied
access to a piano in the first instance and unfortunately there
was not a great deal of empathy between him and his
psychiatrist, until he was fortunate enough to meet Dr Wyn
Warren in 1988. She helped him open new doors of
confidence, personal love and a sense of peace about his very
troubled relationship with his late father. I attended sessions
with David, at Dr Warrens request, and this strengthened even
further our bond of love and respect.

25 cups of coffee a day, about 500 grams of sugar, smoking


125 cigarettes a day, taking about quarter of a bottle of aspirin
and chewing gum! One does not need to have a vivid
imagination to guess the level of hyperactivity from that
consumption. His medication was Serance, which he had
been on since 1970.
For those who have been in a mental wilderness, love and
support in a personal sense seems so important. I was able to
devote 24 hours a day to David and at times it felt like 48
hours in one day. But I was there by choice, unlike parents
and families. But soon the results started to manifest. With
the decrease in coffee, cigarettes, etc., a greater calm
descended and taking David swimming for an hour twice daily
(this of course helped cut down the cigarette intake) his
physical health started to improve dramatically. His
untidiness, as the scenes in Shine show, was evident every
day. It was not until we built our own home in 1991 that he
realised that the property was his, and his attitude changed.
He then took great care not to litter the house, and indeed
now helps daily, and with such amazing willingness. I dont
think I have ever asked him to do anything and he has hesitated
even getting me a glass of water in the middle of the night!

Challenging, funny, exciting, frustrating, loving and


exhausting are all words describing my experience of life with
David. But never boring! When we met he was drinking

In my book Love You to Bits and Pieces, Life with David


Helfgott, I describe in detail our life together the blending
of his creative abilities and my practicality. We make a good
team complete opposites, but that has its benefits as we
both have our own interests he practices and I do some
astrology and deal with all the business, and yet we share our
love of books, nature, travel and friendships.

37

38

One of the things I found sad when I went to live with David
was the lack of public awareness to mental illness and the
fear of it. As about 20% of the population will require some
therapy at sometime in their lives, it is essential that we gain
a great tolerance to those who appear different, and, an
understanding of the need for medication at certain times in a
persons life. Why do we not feel a diabetic is failing if they
need insulin, and yet anyone on medication for a psychiatric
disorder is deemed a failure by many and this can result in
the person withdrawing their medication, sometimes with
disastrous results. I also deal with this matter in my book.
The years away from performing did not dim Davids passion
for his music and his comeback in 1984 revealed that his great
virtuoso ability had not left him it had been dimmed, but
never died. His music fills his life, his head, his soul. He is
indeed blessed that he can earn his living now from his
obsession and passion few have that opportunity. His
suffering has added a new dimension to his music his Liszt
performances of the Dante and Sonata in B Minor reflect his
journey through life and speak more eloquently than words.
For those with exceptional talent, to use it is paramount to
their fulfillment. As David says away from the stage I am a
mouse, but when I walk on to the stage and play the piano I
become a leaping lion.

39

David Helfgott, Professor Allan Snyder and Gillian Helfgott.

40

Emerging Genius at the Onset of


Dementia
Professor Bruce L. Miller
Art burst forth on the planet earth approximately 40,000 years
ago. Remarkably, it appeared in both Australia and Europe
at the same historical moment, produced in different climates
and habitats by peoples separated culturally and physically.
Not crude scribblings, these works were breathtakingly
beautiful and their production required advanced technical
skills. Beyond demonstrating abilities never previously
evident on this planet, the art mirrored a culture with spiritual
ideas and musical passions. These sculptures, cave-paintings
and petroglyphs still serve as a window into the cognitive
processing and brain capacity of the humans who lived in
these ancient societies. The capacity, perhaps even necessity,
to produce art suggested that a profound change had occurred
in the human brain. Soon after the appearance of art, pictorial
and then symbolic language emerged and a written record
began to complement the visual history left from pictures and
sculptures. Like the language organ elegantly described
by Chomsky, humans had evolved an art organ pre-wired
genetically and ready to express visual images of the external
and internal world. The study of patients with focal brain
lesions offers valuable insights into what parts of the brain
are required to produce art. Ultimately, it tells us about the
brain regions that evolved to allow the presence of art and
language.
The ability to precisely copy internal images, objects or
drawings is profoundly altered by injury to the right parietal
lobe. Realistic reproduction is lost with right parietal injury,
41

even in previously accomplished artists. This loss is due to a


variety of factors relating to spatial cognition. Neglect of the
contralateral visual space will lead to drawings where the left
half of the space is unused, or only sketchily drawn.
Additionally, the simple process of copying or putting together
two three-dimensional structures is impaired. The right
hemisphere, but in particular, the right posterior parietal lobe,
is required for the art that was produced for the first time
40,000 years ago. Clearly, right posterior parietal cortex is
required for realistic reproduction. Less is known regarding
the function of the left hemisphere in the production of art
and it is possible to suffer a left hemisphere infarct and
continue to produce precise copies of objects or paintings.
However, there does appear to be an important left hemisphere
contribution to art, and in one report, a conceptual artist who
suffered a left frontotemporal stroke lost the ability to produce
symbolic paintings. In contrast, this man continued to produce
beautiful realistic reproductions. The left hemisphere remains
important for the conceptual and verbal aspects of visual art.
Two degenerative dementias, Alzheimers disease and
frontotemporal dementia, reveal contrasting ways through
which focal brain injury can influence artistic expression.
With Alzheimers disease there is a relentless loss of
visuoconstructive ability. Drawings become distorted with
diminished accuracy and spatial perspective. These losses
coincide with dysfunction in the right parietal lobe. In
frontotemporal dementia the ability to copy is relatively
preserved. Even though creativity disappears in most patients
with frontotemporal dementia, the ability to draw remains
late into the course of the illness. This suggests that evolution

42

of the frontal lobes, by itself, is unlikely to explain the


production of art in prehistoric times. Furthermore, in one
anatomical subtype of frontotemporal dementia, patients with
left anterior temporal degeneration, visual creativity remains
or develops. The art in these individuals is reminiscent of the
non-symbolic reproductions of the conceptual artist who lost
the ability to produce symbolic art with left hemisphere stroke.
Yet, despite the absence of symbolism, the work is visually
appealing and successful. We have hypothesised that both
visual perception and visual interest is enhanced by injury to
the left anterior temporal lobe, the brain region responsible
for access to semantic information.
Clearly, various brain regions contribute to visual creativity.
The right parietal lobe is dominant for the ability to actively
reproduce images while the left anterior temporal and frontal
lobes contribute to the conceptual component of art. Most
artistic productivity uses both the left and right hemispheres
although autistic savants and patients with semantic dementia
produce an art that is primarily visual and non-conceptual.
The appearance of art 40,000 years ago reflects a change in
the human brain. The likely site of this change is posterior
parietal cortex.

Professor Bruce Miller - discoverer of artistic genius at the onset


of a form of Alzheimers disease.
43

44

Do The Brains of Extraordinary


People Differ From Normal People
Professor Michael W OBoyle
By way of introduction, my name is Professor Michael W.
OBoyle and I have recently been appointed Morgan Chair
in Psychology and Director of the Morgan Centre for the Study
of High Intellectual Potential at the University of Melbourne.
The following is a very brief overview of past work and future
directions in my ongoing investigation of intellectual
giftedness.
The expressed goal of the newly formed Morgan Centre is to
conduct empirical research into the biological, cognitive and
developmental mechanisms that underlie gifted intellectual
ability, and in particular, the investigation of prodigious
mathematical talent (i.e., those who are 12-15 years of age
and classified in the top 5% of performers based upon
standardised tests of math abilities). While much has been
said and done with regard to psychometrically identifying
individuals who are (or are not) mathematically gifted, very
little has been done in developing a scientific model of how
such prodigious talent has arisen, seemingly in the absence
of any formal training or instruction.
Over the last decade, my work has focused on the functional
organisation of the gifted brain and how it is qualitatively
(not just quantitatively) different from those of their average
ability cohorts (OBoyle, Benbow, & Alexander, 1995). In
particular, using a variety of neuropsychological methods, I
have demonstrated that enhanced development of the right
cerebral hemisphere and an unusual reliance upon it when
processing information for the purposes of learning, are key
45

characteristics of the mathematically gifted brain. In addition,


the ability to switch (and adjust) activation levels in an
orchestrated and coordinated manner between the left and
right hemispheres when processing new information, may be
a unique characteristic of gifted brain functioning.
Interestingly, in a recent Nature paper, Canadian researchers
(Witelson, et al., 1999) have examined the brain of Albert
Einstein and found that his brain was characterised by
enhanced development of the parietal lobes, particularly in
the right cerebral hemisphere. This, by the way, is the very
area that has been implicated in my research as being related
to mathematical precocity in these gifted adolescents.
Supporting evidence for such theorising has come primarily
from electrophysiological data collected in my lab via the
electroencephalogram (EEG). The latter is used to monitor
brain wave activity in gifted subjects when performing a
variety of mental tasks (e.g., mental rotation, chimeric face
processing, visually matching circles to arcs, etc.). My future
projects, however, will investigate underlying brain
morphology and functional organisation in the mathematically
gifted using far more sophisticated and state-of-the-art brain
imaging techniques. Specifically, these studies will involve
the use of functional magnetic imaging (FMRI) and positron
emission tomography (PET) technology to investigate how
brain structure and functional organisation relate to prodigious
mathematical talent.

46

How Do They Do It? Savants


A Savant Prime Number Calculator
Dr Mike Anderson
Prime numbers have a mystical quality that has intrigued us
since at least the beginning of Greek Philosophy. Perhaps
this makes it even more astonishing that a young man
(Michael) with a very low measured IQ, and who can barely
communicate, can recognise a prime number when he sees
one.
How he recognises whether a number is, or is not, prime is
certainly not through any mystical process. We have
conducted a series of experiments that demonstrate that he
recognises a prime number using a method first described by
Eratosthenes an astronomer in ancient Greece.
By comparing Michaels response times with those of
mathematics students and using computer simulations we
discovered that he divides a target number by every integer
up to the square root of that number. If none of the integers
divide leaving no remainder then he knows that the target
number is prime. What we do not know is how he came to
discover this method.
Dr Billy Tao and Professor Michael OBoyle.

Many have wondered whether prime numbers have a


transcendent quality that derives from the very physical
structure of the universe itself, or on the contrary whether
they are merely constructions of the human brain. Maybe
this isnt an either or. Maybe both human brains and prime
numbers reflect fundamental principles of the physical
universe. And maybe this can be seen most clearly in this
extraordinary case.

47

48

Education Help or Hindrance?


Perspectives from a Psychological Theory
of Intelligence
Dr Mike Anderson
In my presentation I will argue that a viable theory of human
intelligence provides some hypotheses about whether geniuses
and prodigies are born or made and what role, if any, education
should have in the fostering of talent.
What we know about the likely cause of savant skills indicates
the central role of general intelligence in fostering genius,
for there are no savant geniuses. True genius probably requires
an inborn talent that arises largely through a rare assortment
of genes. Eminence on the other hand is likely to be a product
of an interaction between high intelligence, personality and
circumstance. Real genius is likely to be relatively immune
from educational intervention. The development of talent,
on the other hand, may be best accomplished when children
are taught alongside peers of similar abilities.

How Do They Do It? Savants


Professor John Mitchell
Professor Snyder and I have a theory. How do we come to
such a strange theory when there are probably more obvious
theories suggested by mainstream psychology? We came at
it from an unusual angle, we didnt come at it from studying
savants, we came at it from the vision side of things. We
were interested in how the visual system worked. We were
interested in such questions as why do we have colour vision, why do we have optical illusions and so forth.
And so for us the interesting question was not why a mentally
retarded three-year-old could draw like Leonardo Da Vinci
but why the rest of us couldnt. And the conclusion that we
came to was that we are not normally interested in the details
of the pictures we see. Were interested in identifying objects
and deciding how to respond to them. We had a pixel map of
whats out there in our brain and if we were conscious of it,
we should have little trouble drawing out that pixel map on a
piece of paper but we dont do that for the reasons just
outlined. For example, if you see your friend, you recognise
him straight away. You dont look and say hes got grey
hair, blue eyes, big nose and moustache, that must be John,
you just recognise him straight away.
Then it came as a bit of a surprise to find that a supposedly
mentally retarded three-year-old girl could draw so well. But
of course, we learned that she was autistic and this meant that
she is somehow conceptually deficient.
She didnt think in terms of concepts like we do so maybe
she can get access to these details and reproduce them readily.

49

50

So this led us to put forward this bold hypothesis that maybe


all savant abilities came about in this particular way. I think
we do tend to have a bit of a distorted idea as to what is genius.
We thought for a long time that being able to play chess like
a grand master was genius but we all know now that chess
doesnt require intelligence. But we do not consider walking
across a room without falling over to be genius because
anyone can do that and we dont consider being able to
recognise voices or to be able to understand language as
genius. But these are extraordinary abilities, were all walking
geniuses. I dont think it is far fetched to say that perhaps,
there are hidden facilities that dont normally manifest
themselves because we are not really interested in identifying
primes or multiplying numbers together. We are not normally
interested in doing mathematics and we are not normally
interested in drawing. We evolved for the purpose of
identifying whats out there and responding should we run
away from it should we chase after it. Thats the way our
brains evolved.
It seems to us that the simplest hypothesis for someone like
Nadia was to simply suggest that for some reason, she has
lost the ability or the desire or the interest in conceptualising
and this somehow opens up access to other parts of the brain.
There doesnt seem to be any reason until proven otherwise
to suggest that she has some other part of the brain that is
different to the rest of us.

Geniuses, Prodigies and Savants A


Teachers Perspective
Lynne Kelly
The normal school, by definition and statistics, has mostly
pretty normal ability children racing around its corridors.
They will not meet a savant, but may, very occasionally, come
across a prodigy or genius. Both are poorly defined terms,
and we teachers cant sit around until the debate is done about
what they are and what they need. So we can only do what
we can with the information we have available today, and
programme accordingly.
We use the term gifted and acknowledge varying degrees of
giftedness the further from the norm being the profoundly
gifted child probably what is best described as genius. There
is no homogeneous group of gifted children, and the idea that
they can be grouped together for a single special program is
outdated now. There is just too much variation between them.
The key to dealing with such variation is recognising the
abilities may not be consistent over subject domains. A
profoundly gifted mathematician may be average in language
skills. A gifted artistic child may not be so far ahead in
mathematics. So they need to be placed according to their
ability level in the different domains. This requires flexibility
within the school timetable, so a child based in year 6, say,
can do year 9 maths, but be in enriched year 6 for the rest.
Research, such as that by Prof. Michael OBoyle and others,
is indicating a different brain behaviour for prodigious
mathematics students. This probably moves across all
domains. They think and learn differently. This matches

51

52

many of the instincts of teachers dealing with these kids. It is


essential we move beyond just doing things faster into doing
it faster and adding things which are different curriculum
material which enables them to use the different thinking
patterns. For example, in mathematics, many children can
calculate well and get the answers to problems they know
how to do quickly. It is those who can visualise approaches
to problems they dont know how to do who are the true
mathematicians. It is those who pose further problems, to
whom getting the right answer is only the first step in playing
with the numbers, who challenge us to make sure there is
work which encourages this skill.

on them are continually increasing. Very few schools can


provide such specialisation yet, although the trend towards
such provisions is definitely clear. I am very fortunate to
have such a position at Methodist Ladies College in
Melbourne, where programming for gifted students is my sole
task. I work there part-time, and the rest is spent as Principal
of the Virtual School for the Gifted, which provides such
specialist courses into the normal school via the Internet.
These children are different to the normal student and need
different educational provisions. The bad news is, they are
very different to each other, and the more gifted they are, the
more individual their program needs to be.

While talking about skill, can I mention my other pet hate.


The expression of superiority when the child knows more
than the teacher. Little Johnny (this poor kid has a lot to
answer for) might be able to name every dinosaur since the
Big Bang, but that is not what modern education is about.
This is the information age information is available at the
touch of a finger or two to the keyboard. We teach the higher
level skills (according to Benjamin Bloom); comprehension,
application, analysis, synthesis and evaluation. The more able
the child, the higher the proportion of the latter level skills.
An individual program is needed, slotting in and out of the
regular program as required, with the ability to move faster
and to access material designated to these higher levels,
problem posing, individualising, but with a framework. I
believe trained educators are needed to develop such
frameworks, knowing the future needs of the student in upper
secondary and tertiary level.
But that takes time. Teachers have very little and expectations
53

54

Education: Help or Hindrance?


Trevor Clark
Among the underserved minority of gifted children with
disabilities are a group of children referred to as autistic
savants. These gifted children, despite suffering from often
incapacitating disturbances in communication, social skills
and intellectual development, often display remarkable gifts
or splinter skills in one or several domains. They are possibly
the least recognised, and potentially productive group of gifted
underachievers.
Savant gifts or splinter skills may be exhibited in the following
skill areas or domains; memory, hyperlexia, art, music,
mechanical or spatial skill, calendar calculation, mathematical
calculation, sensory sensitivity, athletic performance, and
computer ability. These skills may be remarkable in contrast
with the disability of autism (Savant 1), or may be in fact
prodigious when viewed in relation to the non-disabled person
(Savant 2).

Skills In The Autistic Population Through Curriculum Design.


The project aims to apply the often unproductive but
prodigious savant skills in a functional and meaningful
manner, through the design and implementation of an
educational curriculum. The curriculum incorporates several
programme models currently in use in the education of both
the gifted and the autistic child. The curriculum is based
upon the savant skills or gifts, on the strength and interests of
the students, and not on their deficits. The intrinsic motivation
displayed by the savant whilst engaged in their areas of savant
interests, is in essence being borrowed to help teach functional
living skills.
Although it is important to increase our understanding and
knowledge of savant syndrome, and how it is they are able to
engage in often prodigious performances in certain skill
domains, it is now time to assist the savant to make use of
their savant skills, to increase their chances of leading more
functional and rewarding lives. By doing so, we may in fact
be able to reduce the impact of autism on the lives of these
students with autism, who also display savant skills or gifts.

The often prodigious gifts or talents of the majority of autistic


savants are exhibited in obsessive or unproductive behaviours.
These skills appear to have little meaning or functional
application and are often referred to as anomalous talents or
skills. It is my view that education can assist the autistic savant
to use his or her often anomalous savant skills to lead a more
independent and functional life.
Under the supervision of Professor Miraca Gross, at the
University of New South Wales, I am currently involved in a
research project entitled, The Application of Savant/Splinter
55

56

Genius + Madness: Is There a Connection?


Professor Jack Pettigrew
The link between human creativity and bipolar disorder is
well recognised, despite two symmetrical difficulties:- 1.
measuring creativity in the present, when the bipolar diagnosis
is more secure; 2. the difficulties of diagnosis in an historical
genius whose creativity has stood the test of time. I would
like to try to go beyond the obvious link between creative
output and the hypomanic state to suggest a role for two other
universal mental traits that seem to be quantitatively altered
in bipolar subjects:- neural clocks that govern switch rates
and intraneuronal signalling pathways. One neural rhythm,
interhemispheric switch rate, varies by two orders of
magnitude in the general population but is slower in bipolar
subjects.
Slower interhemispheric switching would provide greater
divergence in the complementary points of view adopted
successively by each hemisphere and therefore a greater
creative clash when they meet (note that I think that both
hemispheres are involved in the creative process, not just the
right as popular writings have it).
Intracellular signalling pathways have greater amplification
in bipolar brains. While there is no available data on a possible
link between clock rate and intraneuronal signal amplification,
I predict a correlation based on the likelihood that gene
products used to make the clock (e.g. ion channels) will also
be used elsewhere in other parts of the brain. Sensitivity to
external stimuli of all kinds is of obvious relevance to
creativity, since creation of new thought patterns has to
precede their selection and execution.

57

58

Professor Jack Pettigrew - speaking of his personal journey and


scientific enquiry about bi-polar disorder.

59

The following text is the cover story from the prestigious scientific journal, Proceedings of the Royal Society, London. Professors Snyder and
Mitchells paper created enormous interest world-wide on the minds secret powers. Media coverage ranged from The Times of London to the
Russian popular press, plus radio, TV and documentaries. This research
was also the impetus for the Geniuses, Prodigies and Savants conference.
60

Is Integer Arithmetic Fundamental to


Mental Processing?: The Mind's Secret Arithmetic
Professor Allan Snyder and Professor
John Mitchell
Unlike the ability to acquire our native language, we struggle to learn multiplication and division. It may then come as
a surprise that the mental machinery for performing lightning fast integer arithmetic calculations could be within us
all even though it can not be readily accessed, nor do we
have any idea of its primary function. We are led to this provocative hypothesis by analysing the extraordinary skills of
autistic savants. In our view such individuals have privileged access to lower levels of information not normally available through introspection.
1. Introduction
We are largely unaware of the ways in which our brains process information. For example, we are not conscious that shape
is computed from object shading or that perspective is derived (among various ways) by the gradient of texture
(Hemholtz 1910, Snyder and Barlow 1988). And why should
we be? It is the object itself that is of ultimate interest rather
than the manner in which we derive its label. Indeed, this
specific example explains why it is so difficult to draw naturally occurring scenes, as has been elaborated elsewhere
(Snyder and Barlow 1988, Snyder and Thomas 1997). Analogously, the foundations of our most fundamental beliefs, our
mindsets, are not normally available for introspection. We
are highly concept driven (Bartlett 1932; Snyder 1998). Presumably, this confers advantage by allowing us to operate
61

automatically. Essentially, a world of unconscious information is sifted through, by mechanisms of which we are unaware, to arrive at our final judgements. We might therefore
be in for some genuine surprises if we had access to the mental processes used to construct our mindsets.
To gain insight into this fundamental problem we turn to a
rare group of individuals, savants with early infantile autism,
because they appear significantly less concept driven than
normal individuals. Furthermore, this group has been a subject of scrutiny, so we can borrow from comprehensive empirical studies and much theoretical discussion (Kanner 1943;
Asperger 1944; Frith 1989; Treffert 1989; Baron-Cohen 1994)
in order to build a framework from which predictions can be
made about unconscious mental processes. This leads to our
hypothesis that the mental machinery for performing lightning fast integer arithmetic (lengthy multiplication, division,
factorization and prime identification) is within us all, although it can not normally be accessed, nor do we know what
primary function it serves.
2. Savants - minds with privileged access to lower levels
of information?
Building on the pioneering work of Kanner 1943, Asperger
1944, Frith 1989, O'Connor 1989 and Hermelin and O'Connor
1990, we surmise that children with early infantile autism
give insight into a mind with limited mindsets, a mind that is
not concept driven (Snyder and Thomas 1997; Snyder 1998).
In our view such a mind can tap into lower level details not
readily available to introspection by normal individuals. This
is consistent with the constellation of traits associated with
early infantile autism, especially those of savants (Hill 1978;
62

Treffert 1989; O'Connor 1989; Howe 1989; Nettlebeck and


Young in press) who seem to be aware of information in some
raw or interim state prior to it being formed into the ultimate
picture. For example, it explains (Snyder and Thomas 1997)
how it is that Nadia (Selfe 1977), a mentally retarded three
and a half year old, can draw natural scenes like that of Fig. 1
from memory, with astonishing life-like perspective and to
do so spontaneously without any training or without even
passing through the usual scribble stage.
Now, it is a surprising fact that normal individuals cannot
draw naturalistic scenes unless they are taught the tricks and
schema to do so (Gombrich 1960). The reason why this is so
unexpected is that our brains obviously possess all of the necessary visual information required to draw, but we are apparently unable to access it for the purpose of drawing. For
example, our brain performs the calculations necessary to label
three-dimensional objects. Yet the difficulties of drawing even
a simple sphere are legion. We are not consciously aware of
how our brains derive shape from shading, perspective from
gradients of texture, size invariance with distance and so on.

Fig 1 Autistic child's drawing at about three and a half years (Selfe
1977).
63

Clearly, it is the object label or symbolic identification that is


of ultimate importance and not the actual attributes processed
by the brain to formulate the label (Snyder and Barlow 1988).
Indeed, normal preschool children draw, not so much what
they see, but rather from what can be called their mental
schema. And, these schema, like those of Fig. 2, tend to be
invariant across cultures. The horse is conventionally drawn
side-on, head to the left, and in bold outline form as is typical
of late preschool art. Somehow, the autistic savant Nadia can
directly tap the way in which our brain derives perspective,
whereas normal individuals can not. Yet these autistic savant
artists often struggle to recognise familiar faces (Selfe 1977).
So we believe that artistic savants have direct access to lower
levels of neural information prior to it being integrated into
the holistic picture - the ultimate label. All of us possess this
same lower level information, but we can not normally access it. Ramachandran (1998 p. 287) in his remarkable new
book enriches this possibility by suggesting neurobiological
mechanisms.
In our opinion, all savant skills can be explained as analogous to those of drawing (Snyder, 1998). Put simply, savants
have privileged access to lower levels of raw information.
Take the ability of perfect pitch as an example. Our mechanism for hearing consists of discrete frequency analysers
which allow for the possibility of perfect pitch. But, surprisingly only one in 10,000 persons possess absolute pitch
(Profita and Bidder 1988) and it is debatable whether or not
absolute pitch can be taught (Takeuchi and Hulse 1993). So,
in analogy to our discussion above on vision, it is the holistic
information content that is important for hearing and not the
component attributes from which this is derived (Miller 1989;
64

Heaton, Hermelin and Pring 1998). Yet, all musical savants


possess absolute pitch (Miller 1989). They apparently have
access to lower levels of auditory information while we do
not.
These and other savant skills (Treffert 1989; Rimland and
Fein 1988), including the extraordinary ability to recall seemingly meaningless detail as opposed to recall of concepts, unusual sensory discrimination of smell and touch, and even time
keeping abilities, reinforce our view that savants are able to
tap something that is in us all, but which is not normally accessible. And this is consistent with numerous observations
as captured by O'Connor's (1989, page 4) statements that their
"gift springs so to speak from the ground, unbidden, apparently untrained and at the age of somewhere between 5 and 8
years of age. There is often no family history of the talent"
and it "is apparently not improved by practice." Also, the
talents are "chiefly in the direction of imitation and there is
little capacity for originality or for creativity" (Treffert 1989,
page 9).

To our knowledge no young savant (when the skill first


emerges) has ever given any insight into the methods used,
nor can they learn or be taught. With maturity the occasionally offered insights are suspect, possibly being contaminated
by expectations or the acquisition of concepts concerning the
particular skill. Furthermore, savant skills often recede or
are lost altogether with the onset of maturity (Selfe, 1977;
Treffert 1989; Barnes and Earnshaw 1995).
All of this suggests that the unusual skills of savants can be
used as a diagnostic tool to probe information from lower
level mechanisms which are not available to introspection of
the normal mind. But, the savant has not revealed unknown
or unexpected mechanisms in the case of drawing or perfect
pitch. The physics of natural scenes already tells us how
perspective must be computed by the brain and discrete frequency analysers are already known to be the primary auditory receptors. Nor, in this vein, should the savants' astonishing feats of recall for detail reveal anything new about
mental processing, since much evidence supports the view
that we all store an enormous amount of information, with
only a minute subset available for recall (Treffert 1989,
Penfield and Roberts 1966). Indeed, our recall like our drawing skills appear to be concept oriented (Bartlet 1932).

Fig 2 Representative drawings of normal children, each at age

So the extraordinary drawing skills of savants, their astonishing recall of detail and their ability of perfect pitch do not
reveal unexpected mental processing. We all have the same
raw information but just can not directly access it, at least on
call. But what does the existence of savant lightning calculators tell us about mental processing in the normal mind?

four years and two months. (Emma and Teneal, Parents on Campus
Preschool, Australian National University).

3. Savant lightning calculators

65

66

Because normal children struggle to learn multiplication and


division, it is surprising that some savants perform integer
arithmetic calculations mentally at lightning speeds (Treffert
1989, Myers 1903, Hill 1978, Smith 1983, Sacks 1985,
Hermelin and O'Connor 1990, Welling 1994, Sullivan 1992).
They do so unconsciously, without any apparent training, typically without being able to report on their methods, and often
at an age when the normal child is struggling with elementary arithmetic concepts (O'Connor 1989). Examples include
multiplying, factoring, dividing and identifying primes of six
(and more) digits in a matter of seconds as well as specifying
the number of objects (more than one hundred) at a glance.
For example, one savant (Hill 1978) could give the cube root
of a six figure number in 5 seconds and he could double
8,388,628 twenty four times to obtain 140,737,488,355,328
in several seconds. Joseph (Sullivan 1992), the inspiration
for the film Rain Man about an autistic savant, could spontaneously answer "what number times what number gives
1234567890" by stating "9 times 137,174,210". Sacks (1985)
observed autistic twins who could exchange prime numbers
in excess of eight figures, possibly even 20 figures, and who
could see the number of many objects at a glance. When a
box of 111 matches fell to the floor the twins cried out 111
and 37, 37, 37. Similar skills were reported as early as 1801
about a child named Dase, who was also "singularly devoid
of mathematical insight" and of low general intelligence
(Treffert 1989, Myers 1903).
4. Is integer arithmetic fundamental to mental processing?
If, as we believe, all savant skills have a common origin, then
the skill for integer arithmetic, (like that for drawing, perfect
67

pitch, and recall for meaningless detail), arises from an ability to access some mental process which is common to us all,
but which is not readily accessible by normal individuals.
From this reasoning, we believe that everyone has the underlying facility for performing lightning fast integer arithmetic. This facility can not normally be tapped for the purpose
of arithmetic nor do we have any idea of its primary function.
Rather, we must learn arithmetic the way we learn to draw
naturalistic scenes, by implementing tricks and algorithms
(Gombrich 1960; Snyder and Barlow 1988; Snyder and Thomas 1997). Learning arithmetic is hard work for normal individuals (Dehaene 1997), whereas it seems effortless for
mathematical savants. Why this should be is deeply mysterious.
As with drawing, tricks and algorithms can be learned for
doing rapid arithmetic, but some savant lightning calculators
vastly out-perform those who adopt these methods, both in
speed (Hermelin and O'Connor 1990) and complexity (Sacks
1985; Waterhouse 1988). For example, in a pioneering empirical study, a mathematics graduate trained in the appropriate algorithms took 11.46 seconds to generate all the primes
between integers 301 and 393 whereas a non-verbal autistic
young man who had not previously confronted such a task
took only 1.16 seconds (Hermelin and O'Connor 1990). Not
only was the savant ten times faster, but he also made far
fewer errors. Importantly, no practically realisable algorithm
has yet been invented for rapidly identifying primes in excess of 8 figures as apparently performed by the autistic savant twins (Sacks 1985).
It would be interesting to compare the active (functional) brain
images of autistic savant calculators with those of
68

individuals who calculate via learned algorithms. We might


anticipate significant differences between the two, possibly
analogous to those between native and second language performance as recently observed by Hirschs groups (Kim,
Relkin, Lee and Hirsch 1997). Our native language is acquired unconsciously, whereas second language acquisition
is hard work. Accordingly, the arithmetic ability of autistic
savants could be functionally like that of a native language
whereas it is expected to be more like a second language in
most of us.

and equipartitions them analogous to the mathematical procedure of factorising. This could also explain why primes to
mathematical savants are the odd man out in groups of numbers and are reacted to as if they are very peculiar indeed
(Hermelin and O'Connor 1990). Others have suggested the
possibility of savants using modular arithmetic (Sacks 1985;
Steward 1975).

5. What is required for arithmetic calculations?


Apart from learning the nomenclature or the symbolic representation of numbers, integer arithmetic is simply the ability
to separate groups into an equal number of elements - that is
to equipartition. For example, 12 elements can be represented
as two equal groups of 6 elements or 4 equal groups of 3
elements. Equipartitioning may also be pertinent to another
common skill of autistic savants - calendar calculating - where
the day of the week is given upon being presented with any
date, say 1000 years in the past or future (Sacks 1985;
Hermelin and O'Connor 1986; Treffert 1989; Young, R.L. and
Nettlebeck, T. 1994). We surmise from this that
equipartitioning is fundamental to some yet unknown aspect of mental processing. It is intriguing to contemplate
which aspect, analytical or perceptual.
The actual method of calculation, while intriguing, is not central to our thesis. Rather, our hypothesis rests on the very
existence of an ability to do lightning calculations without
training. Perhaps mathematical savants tap a mental process
which spatially represents groups and patterns (Welling 1994)
69

Fig 3 Generating primes: to determine whether it is has any factors other than 1 or itself. We need only consider prime factors less
than the square root of the number. There are well known tricks for
rapidly determining whether a number is divisible by 2, 3, 5 or 11,
but in general it is necessary to divide by each prime. However if
we are testing a sequence of consecutive numbers it is not necessary to test every number separately by dividing by each prime.
Once you have determined that a number is divisible by 7 then you
know every 7'th number thereafter is divisible by 7 . This gives us
an alternative way to find primes, one known in antiquity, that does
not explicitly involve division. Above we have adapted this ancient method to find the primes in the range 101-120 (row 1). First
we eliminate the even numbers and numbers ending in 5 (row 2).
Dividing 101 by 3 leaves a remainder of 2, revealing 102 as the
first multiple of 3. We then eliminate every third number starting
with 102 (row 3). Finally, dividing 101 by 7 leaves a remainder of
3, revealing 105 as the first multiple of 7. We then eliminate every
seventh number starting with 105 (row 4). Since we need only
consider prime factors less than the square root of 121 equals 11,
the remaining numbers are all prime.
70

Memory and algorithms (learned or induced) are known to


play a crucial role in the techniques employed by lightning
calculators from the normal population (Smith 1983; Dehaene
1997). But the beautiful work of Anderson, O'Connor and
Hermelin (in press) has ruled out the role of memory for savant calculators. They also found that the performance profile of the savant calculator closely matched that of the control who was using the Eratosthenes algorithm for identifying primes as suggested earlier by Hermelin and O'Connor
(1990). But it remains possible that other strategies for finding primes (see Fig. 3), not all of which need be arithmetic,
could also have similar performance profiles. Whatever the
case, this suggests that savant calculators have privileged
access to some form of algorithmic mental processing.
6. Music and number
Some autistic savants have the ability to keep time for extended periods with accuracy to the second (Treffert 1989).
Apparently, our internal clocks are more precise than might
have been imagined. For example, when one autistic child
was awakened he said, "It's 2.14 AM", then he went back to
sleep (Rimland and Fein 1988, p 485). This ability to
equipartition time could also contribute to the impressive
musical skills of many autistic savants (Treffert 1989, Hill
1978; Miller 1989) and, when coupled with equipartitioning
of space, could suggest a mechanism which interrelates music and mathematics.

senses of normal individuals are mixed and accessible for


comparison in a way that individuals with synaesthesia (Luria
1987; Cytovic and Wood 1982) might suggest?
7. Discussion
As Howe (1989, p. 83) so aptly puts it, We experience only
the whole: it takes the evidence provided by unusual people in whom mental integration is incomplete owing to retardation, brain damage, or some other kind of mental disturbance to make us appreciate how smooth functioning of a
persons total mental system depends on the parts, or subsystems, that underlie it. This philosophy underpins our
present investigation. In particular, we believe that savants
offer a window into lower level information used to construct our percepts and our judgements. From this we have
argued that some mental processing exists in us all, for purposes yet unknown, which is recruited by autistic lightning
calculators to perform precise integer arithmetic calculations,
such as multiplication, division, factoring and identifying
primes.

There no doubt are other surprises that can be revealed by


those abnormal minds which are somehow aware of interim
mental processes and information not normally available
through introspection. For example, is it possible that the

This highly quantitative numerical ability of autistic lightning calculators is in sharp contrast with the well known qualitative sense of numerosity displayed by human babies and
even animals (Gallistel and Gelman 1992; Gallistel 1990;
Wynn 1992; Wynn 1995; Dehaene 1997). For example, babies and animals can estimate the number of objects in a collection with an error that is proportional to the number itself.
This turns out to be accurate for perceiving and estimating 1,
2 or 3 objects but is grossly inaccurate for judging large numbers. As Dehaene 1997 (p 119) concludes in his authoritative
overview, "An innate sense of approximate numerical quan

71

72

tities may well be imbedded in our genes; but when faced


with exact symbolic calculation we lack the resources".
Our paper is concerned with mathematical savants. Now,
savants are far more prevalent in the autistic population than
in any other group and also it is among this group where multiple savant skills most frequently occur (Rimland and Fein
1988; Triffert 1989). But, only a small fraction of the total
autistic population are savants. And, this fraction tends to be
predominately composed of those with early infantile autism
(Treffert 1989), a condition first described by Kanner (1943).
Our theoretical perspective is derived from savants in this
category, although it could apply to savants in general. We
believe that savants with early infantile autism have privileged access to lower levels of information and that they are
impoverished in concept formation, compared with the general autistic population (Charman and Baron-Cohen 1993).
With maturity certain concepts can be acquired, but often at
the loss or reduction of their savant skills (Selfe 1977; Smith
1983).
It is worth mentioning, as have mathematicians of acclaim
(Hadamard 1949), that savant lightning calculations are idiosyncratic and not representative of what would normally be
considered a mathematical talent. Mathematicians are primarily concerned with the conceptual, whereas autistic savants have extreme difficulty with learning even the simplest
mathematical concepts. In this regard it is interesting that
Baron-Cohen and his colleagues (1997) have recently found
that fathers and grandfathers of children with autism were
more than twice as often in the field of engineering than were
fathers and grandfathers of normal children. Similarly, they
found that autism occurred significantly more often in
73

families of students in the field of physics, engineering and


mathematics (Baron-Cohen et al. 1998). Although these studies address the general autistic population and are not restricted
to savants, they are nonetheless fascinating and deserve further investigation.
Prevalent explanations for savant skills: Mathematical savants have fascinated their investigators through the centuries (Smith 1985, Treffert 1989) so it should be of no surprise
that various theories have been advanced to explain the phenomenon. These are discussed in depth elsewhere (Treffert
1989; Nettelbeck and Young in press). We critique the conceptual thrust of the most prominent views so that they can
be contrasted with the perspective leading to our claim that
integer arithmetic is fundamental to mental processing. Essentially there are two popular explanations for savant skills:
one holds that obsessive focussed learning promotes savant
skills just as it does for any expertise: the other postulates
that genius and savants alike have highly developed domain
specific neural structures (innate talent). In contrast, our view
is that the mechanism for savant skills resides equally in us
all but that (without some abnormality like autism) it can not
normally be accessed for the skill in question.
Mathematical savants arise from obsessive learning: Many
authors believe that the extraordinary feats of lightning calculators are a consequence of their passion and preoccupation for learning mathematics in much the same way it is for
truly innovative mathematicians, see for example, e.g. Smith
(1983) and Rimland and Fein (1988). Dehaene (1997, p. 164)
especially presents a compelling discussion, concluding that,
A talent for calculation thus seems to arise more from precocious training, often accompanied by an exceptional or even
74

pathological capacity to concentrate on the narrow domain


of numbers, than from an innate gift. Howe (1989 p. 150)
agrees, The circumstances that give rise to a retarded savants achievements are not entirely different from those in
which a person of normal or above average intelligence
chooses to specialize in a particular area of interest.
Savants have better brains for arithmetic: Another view holds
that both genius and savant alike are endowed with exceptional domain specific neural structures (innate talent) which
promote their specialized skills. This view also has a number
of distinguished advocates. For example, O'Connor (1989,
p. 19) says But just as there are specialized centres mediating speech, so there may be centres for calculation, graphic
skills or music. One can suffer deficits in these abilities so
why not also have specific gifts! Ramachandran (1998, p.
197) enriches the story further, speculating about savants, that
some specialized brain regions may have become enlarged at
the expense of others, e.g. the angular gyrus for mathematical talent. Howe (1989, p. 153), also suggests that the savant
artist Nadia and the man with the seemingly perfect memory,
Shereskeskii, might fall into this category.

it would appear highly coincidental that such a peculiar subset of mathematics should be so compelling to a significant
fraction of autistic savants across all cultures, and also that
many of these same savants simultaneously have several savant skills (Rimland and Fein 1988) each of which are similarly peculiar and restricted. And why is there little or no
invention or creative component in the skill? All of this mitigates against either obsessive learning or better brains being
a plausible explanation for mathematical savants, as does the
fact that savant skills can even arise after an accident or illness in otherwise normal individuals (Treffert 1989).

While the obsessive learning and the better brains theory for
savant arithmetic may not be mutually exclusive (Hermelin
and O'Connor 1990) and while they have their compelling
aspects, we find them improbable for the following reasons:
those who have protracted experience with savants frequently
report that the core ability behind the skill emerges spontaneously and does not improve qualitatively with time even
though it might become better articulated (O'Connor 1989;
Selfe 1977; Treffert 1989). This argues against obsessive
learning. Furthermore, from the perspective of either theory,

The mechanisms for savant mathematics reside equally in us


all but can not normally be accessed: Now in contrast to the
popular views discussed above, the unique aspect of our perspective is that the mechanism and information drawn on for
savant mathematics resides equally in us all but it can not
be recruited by us for mathematics. In other words, we believe that mathematical savants, like all autistic savants, arise
from their privileged access to lower levels of raw information. Their skills are essentially a form of mimicry, and thus
naturally lead to drawing, perfect pitch, time telling, astonishing recall, hyperlexia, echolalia, etc. Hence, the very same
peculiar savant skills appear across different cultures. However, unlike drawing and perfect pitch, we do not know what
lower levels are recruited for savant mathematical skills. But,
we hypothesize that savant mathematics is propelled by some
fundamental mechanism which equipartitions - possibly in
both space and time. Why is it that savants have privileged
access to lower levels of information? Perhaps it is promoted
by a loss of those centres that control executive or integrative
mechanisms as elaborated on by Treffert (1989) and also by
Baron-Cohen (1995) in relation to the fact that individuals

75

76

with autism lack a theory of mind. This in turn could leave


savants less concept driven (Snyder and Thomas, 1997;
Snyder 1998), or as Frith (1989) argues, lacking central coherence (Pring, Hermelin and Heavey 1995; Heaton, Hermelin
and Pring 1998).
An intriguing question remains. Although we do not normally have access to the lower levels of information as do
savants, is there nonetheless some artificial means to promote this access, say via induced altered states of consciousness? Possibly pertinent to this suggestion is the fact mentioned above that savant skills have been known to follow a
severe physical illness, an operation, or a near drowning
(Treffert 1989). This reinforces our belief that savant skills
are innate in us all but are normally suppressed.
So in conclusion, we believe that the mental apparatus to perform lightning fast integer arithmetic calculations such as
multiplication and division resides in us all, even though it is
not normally accessible. The brain appears to perform something tantamount to arithmetic calculations (or analogously
equipartitioning) for some unknown aspect of mental processing. The challenge now is to unravel which aspect.
We appreciate the critical insights of Mike Anderson, Kirsty
Galloway McLean, Ted Nettlebeck, Mandy Thomas and Robyn
Young.
References
Anderson, M., O'Connor, N. and Hermelin, B. (in press) A
specific calculating ability. Intelligence.
Asperger, H. 1944 Die "Autistischen Psychopathen" in
Kindersalter. Archiv fr Psychiatrie and Nervenkrankherten
77

117, 76-136. English translation in: V. Frith, 1991 Autism and


Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Baron-Cohen, S. 1995 Mindblindness. Cambridge, M.A.: MIT
Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., Short, L., Mead.
G., Smith, A. and Scahill, V. 1998 Autism occurs more often
in families of physicists, engineers and mathematicians.
Autism, 2, 296-301.
Baron-Cohen, S. Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P. and
Goodyer, I. 1997 Is there a link between engineering and
autism? Autism, 1, 101-109.
Barnes, R.C. and Earnshaw, S.M. 1995 Problems with the savant
syndrome: A brief case study. British Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 23, 124-126.
Bartlett, F.C. 1932 Remembering: A study in experimental and
social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Charman, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. 1993 Drawing development in
autism: The intellectual to visual realism shift. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 11, 171-185.
Cytovic, R.E. and Wood, F.B. 1982 Synaesthesia II: Psycho
physical relationships in the synaesthesia of geometrically
shaped taste and colored hearing. Brain and Cognition, 1, 2335.
Dehaene, S. 1997 The Number Sense. New York: Oxford.
Frith, V. 1989 Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Gallistel, C.R. 1990 The Organization of Learning, Chapter
10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gallistel, C.R. and Gelman, R. 1992 Preverbal and verbal
counting and computation. Cognition, 44,43-74.
Gombrich, E.H. 1960 Art and Illusion. Oxford: Phaidon
Press.
Hadamard, J. 1949 The Psychology of Invention in the
Mathematical Field, p 58. New York: Dover.
Heaton, P., Hermelin, B. and Pring, L. 1998 Autism and pitch
processing: a precursor for savant musical ability? Music
78

Perception, Spring 15 (2) 291-305.


Hemholtz, H. 1910 Handbuck der physiologischen optic, vol 111
Hamburg: Leopald Voss.
Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N. 1990 Factors and primes: a
specific numerical ability. Psychological Medicine 20, 163189.
Hermelin, B. and O'Connor, N. 1986 Idiot savants calendrical calculators: Rules and regularities. Psychological Medicine 16, 885893.
Hill, A.L. 1978 Mentally retarded individuals with special skills. In
International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 277298 vol. 9. (ed. N.R. Eller). New York: Academic Pan.
Howe, M.J.A. 1989 Fragments of Genius. London and New York:
Routledge.
Kanner, L. 1943 Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous child 2, 217-250.
Kim, K.H.S., Relkin, N.R., Lee, Kyoung-Min and Hirsch, J. 1997
Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171-174.
Luria, A.R. 1987 The mind of a mnemonist. Cambridge, Mass:
Harvard University Press.
Miller, L. 1989 Musical savants: Exceptional skills in the mentally
retarded. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Myers, F.W.H. 1903 Human Personality and its Survival of Bodily
Death. New York: Longmans and Green.
Nettlebeck, T. and Young, R. In press Savant syndrome. In International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, vol. 22. (ed.
Larraine Glidden). New York: Academic Press. (In press).
O'Connor, N.O. 1989 The performance of the "idiot-savant": implicit and explicit. British Journal of Disorders of communication 24, 1-20.
Penfield W. and Roberts, L. 1966 Speech and Brain Mechanisms.
New York: Atheneum.
Pring, L., Hermelin, B. and Heavey, L. 1995 Savants, segments, art
and autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 36,
1065-1076.

Profita, J. and Biddes, I.G. 1988 Perfect Pitch. American Journal


of Medical Genetics 29, 763-771.
Ramachandran, V.S. 1998 Phantoms in the brain. New York:
William Morrow.
Rimland, B. and Fein, D. 1988 Special Talents of Autistic Savants
in the Exceptional Brain 472-492 (ed. Obler, L.K. and Fein, D.)
New York: The Guilford Press.
Sacks, O. 1985 The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat.
London: Duckworth.
Selfe, L. 1977 Nadia: A Case of Extraordinary Drawing Ability in Children. London: Academic Press.
Smith, S.B. 1983 The Great Mental Calculators, the Psychology, Methods and Lines of Calculating Prodigies, Past and
Present. New York: Columbia University Press.
Snyder, A.W. and Barlow, H.B. 1988 Human vision: Revealing the
artists touch. Nature 331, 117-118.
Snyder, A.W. and Thomas, M. 1997 Autistic artists give clues to
cognition. Perception 26, 93-96.
Snyder, A.W. 1998 Breaking Mindsets. Mind and Language 13, 110.
Stewart, I. 1975 Concepts of Modern Mathematics. New York:
Harmondsworth.
Sullivan, R.C. 1992 Rain Man and Joseph. In High-functioning
Individuals with Autism. 243-251 (ed. E. Schopler and G.B.
Mesibov). New York: Plenum Press.
Takeuchi, A.H. and Hulse, S.H. 1992 Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin 113(2), 345-361.
Treffert, D.A. 1989 Extraordinary People. New York: Harper and
Row.
Waterhouse, L. 1988 Extraordinary visual memory and pattern perception in an autistic boy. In The Exceptional
Brain, Neuropsychology of Talent and Special Abilities
348-356 (ed. L.K. Obler and D. Fein ). New York: The
Guilford Press.
Welling, H. 1994 Prime number identificators in idiot savants:
can they calculate them? Journal of Autism and Developmen-

79

80

tal Disorder 24, 199-207.


Wiltshire, S. 1987 Drawings. London: Dent and Sons.
Wynn, K. 1992 Addition and subtraction by human infants.
Nature, 358, 749-750.
Wynn, K. 1995 Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 35-60.
Young, R.L. and Nettlebeck, T. 1994 The "intelligence" of
calendrical calculators. American Journal of Medical Retardation, 99(2), 186-200.

The following text is drawn from the cover story of the popular
New Scientist magazine (www.newscientist.com) on research from
the Centre for the Mind. Im a Genius suggests that just switching off part of the brain may reveal superhuman skills. The article
declares this research startling.

81

82

Tune in, turn off


New Scientist, 9 October 1999
You too could have seemingly superhuman mental skills.
All you have to do is switch off part of your brain. Sounds
bizarre? Rita Carter investigates.
James can tell you the precise time to the second without
looking at a clock. Jennifer can measure anything to within a
fraction of an inch just by glancing at it. And Christopher
can speak 24 languages including a couple of his own devising. Amazing? Definitely. But unusual? Not necessarily. According to a controversial new theory you too can do
these things. Or at least you could if only you could just
stop being so clever for a moment.
Christopher, James and Jennifer are autistic savants people
who score low on IQ tests and have severe difficulties in communicating and interacting with others but who nevertheless
have seemingly superhuman competence in a specific area
like music, art or maths. About one in ten autistic people
have notable talents, but truly prodigious savants like Stephen
Wiltshire, who can draw spectacularly detailed and accurate
representations of buildings, or the lightning card-counting
calculator played by Dustin Hoffman in the film Rain Man
are very rare. There have probably only been about 100 people described as savants since the phenomenon was first identified a century ago and only about 25 are alive at the moment.
Such is our fascination with these people that nearly all of
them are publicly known and celebrated, and many of their
skills have been studied exhaustively. Yet there is still no
83

generally accepted understanding of how savants do whatever it is that they do. Theories range from enlargements of
certain specialised brain regions to the simple practice makes
perfect but none of them alone satisfactorily explains all
the weird anomalies.
The latest contribution to the puzzle is startling because it
proposes that savant skills far from being unique are possessed by everyone, and might even be unleashed with quite
simple, existing technology.
The idea comes from psychologists Allan Snyder and D. John
Mitchell from the Centre for the Mind at The Australian National University in Canberra. Essentially they think that savant skills are the manifestation of brain processes that happen within us all, all the time, but are usually speedily
swamped by more sophisticated conceptual cognition. While
this high-level stuff fills our consciousness, the savant-style
information-crunching that the researchers suggest precedes
it is relegated to the unconscious back rooms of the brain.
It's not that savants are cleverer than the rest of us, says
Snyder, its just that most of us go one step further in our
brain processing from detailed facts to meaningful concepts
and once we've done that we cant go back.
Snyder and Mitchell formulated their theory from analysis of
many existing studies of savants mainly mathematically
gifted ones. Among the findings they rely on are brainimaging experiments, which reveal the extent of unconscious
processing that goes on before we ever become aware of perceptions, thoughts and feelings.

84

A visual image falling on the retina, for example, takes about


a quarter of a second to pop up in a persons mind as a conscious perception. Before that moment, each element of the
image including its colour, shape, movement and location
is identified separately by various specialised regions in the
brain. These components are then assembled into a pattern
which is shunted onwards to regions that attach meaning to
it. Normally we have no idea that all this is happening we
only become conscious of it after the detailed processing is
complete and we have a fully constructed perception.
What matters for survival is that we have a concept we can
work on its a face and its friendly, say not a mass of
detail about how we arrived at that conclusion, says Snyder.
So in normal people the brain takes in every tiny detail, processes it, then edits out most of the information leaving a single useful idea which becomes conscious. Taking these ideas
a step further, he asserts: In savants the suppression doesnt
happen so they see the picture in fantastically detailed components, like individual pixels in a photograph.
Using the same reasoning, Snyder believes that if, for example, you were asked to calculate the day of the week on which
any particular date falls (an obsession peculiar to savants) or
to discern the precise pitch, length and sequence of notes in a
musical score, you would do it, more or less instantly, in your
unconscious mind. But because knowing what day of the
week 1 September 2056 would be is of no practical use, he
thinks the information would be edited out before it passed
into consciousness. Equally, because notes in isolation usually carry little meaning you would tend to hear the music as
a melody rather than as separate sounds.

85

If Snyder and Mitchell are correct in supposing that savant


cognition is happening in us all, is it possible that we could
learn to shift our consciousness back a gear and become aware
of it? Niels Birbaumer of the Institute of Behavioural Neurobiology at the University of Tbingen, in Germany, an enthusiastic supporter of Snyder and Mitchell's theory, believes
we could. Birbaumer recently led a team that fitted paralysed patients with scalp electrodes that picked up signals
from the brain and translated them into movement of a computer cursor. The patients first had to learn to control brain
activity that was normally unconscious (New Scientist, 16
January, p 4). Birbaumer thinks it would be possible to access pre-conscious savant cognitive processes in much the
same way and that some people have already learnt to do
so, without even realising what they were doing.
Accessing the subconscious
He cites, for example, a non-autistic student whose calculating skills rival those of the best mathematical savants. Electrical monitoring of the students brain waves while he was
doing a calculation showed that his brain was more active
than usual at the start but less active just before he answered
(Psychophysiology, vol 33, p 522). Later cognition involves
more cortical activity and is associated with conceptual thinking, says Birbaumer. This student seems to be able to prevent this activity from occurring when he is calculating leaving him free to access the earlier low-level processes.
Other researchers in the field though expressing polite interest in Snyder and Mitchell's theory remain sceptical that
we all have latent savant skills. The most commonly favoured
explanation for savant talents is that they are islands of highly
86

developed ability, probably linked to physically enlarged specialist brain regions. In most people the development of such
skills is held back because the brains resources are focused
from an early age on conceptual thinking and what is known
as global processing pulling together various thoughts and
perceptions and extracting meaning from the overall picture
rather than concentrating on the concrete details of each perception.
Autistic people seem to be unable to process things in this
way. The result is a detailed but incoherent cognitive style
described by autism experts Uta Frith from the Institute of
Cognitive Neuroscience at University College London, and
Francesca Happ, senior scientist at the Institute of Psychiatry, also in London, as weak central coherence. Their idea
is different from Snyder and Mitchell's because they assume
that savant processing never happens in non-autistic people
consciously or unconsciously. They believe the drive towards
central coherence is so strong that it sweeps perceptions and
thoughts into meaningful concepts before every tiny detail of
them is registered, so we wouldnt be able to access this information.
Happ explains: If you were able to look inside the brain of
an autistic savant I think you would find that their talent arises
from very specific and circumscribed brain areas which are
neurologically isolated from the areas which bind things together to make concepts. This allows the areas dedicated to
savant abilities to develop without interference from parts of
the brain which deal with concepts. As a result they may turn
into large specialised brain areas like those that normal people have for speech.

87

The idea that unusually enlarged brain regions may create


exceptional artistic, mathematical or musical skills in the people who possess them took an interesting turn recently. An
anatomical study of Einsteins carefully preserved brain
showed the area associated with maths was bigger than normal and not dissected by the usual groove. Grooves often
mark the boundaries of functional brain areas, so its fascinating to toy with the notion that the mathematical module
in his brain had annexed neurons from an area next door that
would normally do something else.
The trouble with the big brain hypothesis is that anyones
brain will enlarge or get denser in an area that is constantly
active, so it is hard to know if an enlarged module is the cause
or result of a particular skill.
Vilayanur Ramachandran, Director of the Center for Brain
and Cognition at the University of California, San Diego, has
charted neuronal hijacking in cases of phantom limbs when
amputees continue to feel their lost body parts because the
brain regions that once gathered sensory signals from the limb
are drawn into the regions monitoring neighbouring body
parts. He thinks something similar might explain the astounding quality of savant cognition. Maybe when the brain, or a
bit of it, reaches a critical mass new and unforeseen properties emerge, he speculates. So a doubling of neurons
wouldnt produce a doubling of talent but a hundred-fold increase.
A simpler explanation comes from Michael Howe, a psychologist at Exeter University who has studied both autistic and
non-autistic people with exceptional skills and believes that
constant practice is generally enough to account for both types
88

of talent. Savants seem to just see things effortlessly, he


says, but I think if a non-autistic chess player who has been
immersed in the game for thirty or forty years looks at a game
in progress they just see the position and the best moves in
a similar way. He adds: The main difference between experts and savants is that savants do things which most of us
couldnt be bothered to get good at.
Not just practice
Howe admits, though, that mere practice cannot account for
the abilities shown by very young savants, simply because
they have not had time to hone their skills. One celebrated
artistic savant, named Nadia, drew stunningly animated pictures of prancing horses in perfect proportion and perspective from the age of three. She did not seem to learn the skill.
Unlike normal children, who go through very specific stages
as they develop drawing ability, such as putting huge heads
on people and showing limbs as sticks, Nadia was drawing
brilliantly from the moment she could grasp a pencil. And
there are children who can do the amazing day of the week
calculations, who have not yet learnt to divide and have developed the skill without adult help.
It may be that all very young children perceive the world in a
savant-like way. One incredible skill shown by children is
language acquisition. Eight-month-old babies seem to carry
out fantastic calculations in order to work out where word
boundaries fall in a stream of speech (New Scientist, 21 August, p 36). They do not consciously work it out. They simply learn to know when a word begins and ends, just as a
mathematical savant may say they just know the square root

89

of a six-figure number. Adults, by contrast, have to labour


over learning these patterns in a new language; simply immersing themselves in it is usually not enough.
Similarly some researchers believe that perfect pitch a skill
common in musical savants is easily acquired by children
but rarely develops in adulthood. And eidetic memory the
automatic perception, storage and retrieval of visual images
in photographic detail is far more common in children than
in adults.
Savant-like skills may be lost or hidden, according to Snyder
and Mitchell's theory in non-autistic people as they grow
up because of a shift in the way we process information.
Imaging studies show that brain activity in newborn babies is
limited to regions we are unconscious of in adults but which
register incoming sensory information and respond to it by
generating urges, emotions and automatic behaviour. The
cerebral cortex the area associated with conscious thought
and perception becomes active within a few months, however, and as the child grows up an increasing proportion of
information processing is done cortically. This shift accelerates in non-autistic children around the age of eighteen
months, when they start to babble, and language acquisition
may help to kick-start activity in the frontal cortex where
conceptual processing is mainly carried out.
In autistic children this shift appears to be slowed or incomplete and so their savant-like processing style may be preserved. Autistic savants who do seem to make the change,
albeit belatedly, may thus lose their abilities. Nadia, for example, lost much of her prodigious talent when she finally

90

mastered language around the age of 12.

about six times more common in males that in females.

Language development also seems to bring about the dominance of one hemisphere of the brain. In right-handers this is
nearly always the left hemisphere, where the main language
regions develop, but in left-handers language may occupy
the right brain. Many researchers argue that savant skills
tend to be those which are associated more with the right
hemisphere: music, identifying mathematical patterns and art,
for example, rather than skills that are predominantly associated with the left-hemisphere. Even the rare savants who
have amazing word power, like Christopher, tend to be less
interested in reading or the meaning of words, and more interested in skills like translation. Because of this, many have
suggested that savant skills are produced by a dominant right
hemisphere which has flourished in the absence of effective
communication with or inhibition by the left.

His theory seems to be supported by a number of extraordinary cases in which normal people have suddenly developed
savant-like abilities after left-sided brain injuries. One 9-yearold boy, for example, was transformed from an ordinary
school-kid to a genius mechanic after part of his left hemisphere was destroyed by a bullet.

Held back
Autistic people often show both structural and functional
dysfunction in the left hemisphere, says Wisconsin psychiatrist Darold Treffert, author of a book called Extraordinary
People: Understanding Savant Syndrome, back in 1989.
Most cases are probably due to some prenatal interference
with brain development which prevents normal development
of the cortex and left hemisphere, he says. Testosterone,
for example, is known to inhibit left-hemisphere development and in male fetuses temporary slowing of the left hemisphere may be a normal developmental stage. In autism that
slowing may be protracted beyond normal, resulting in an
overdeveloped right hemisphere and stunted growth on the
left. This could explain why autism, and savant skills, are
91

And Bruce Miller and co-workers at the University of California Los Angeles School of Medicine recently reported five
patients who developed amazing drawing skills after dementia destroyed part of the left side of their brains (Neurology,
vol 51, p 978). One of our patients had spent his life changing car stereos and had never shown any interest at all in art,
says Miller. Then he developed dementia which destroyed
neurons in the left frontotemporal cortex an area which gives
meaning to things and suddenly he started to produce sensational images recalled from early childhood. It was as
though the destruction of those brain cells took the brakes off
some innate ability that had been suppressed all his life, and
opened access to an amazing personal memory store he never
knew he had.
As yet it isnt clear whose interpretation of these cases is correct, if indeed anyones is, but Snyder thinks there might be a
way to test it. He is planning an experiment in which, he
hopes, the unconscious savant will be unleashed at the flick
of a switch. Magnetic pulses can interfere with normal brain
activity. If you time and position the surge just right, it can
temporarily turn off activity in a particular region. Snyders
plan is to switch off the conceptualising area. If his theory
is correct, and if he can find the area, this should cause the
92

normally pre-conscious savant skills to burst into consciousness.


I'm thinking of trying it on myself first, says Snyder. If I
start to get crystal clear pictures of my childhood or a sudden
knowledge of prime numbers Ill really know Im onto something.

Rita Carter is author of Mapping the Mind, published in


paperback by Seven Dials, price 1499.
James and Jennifer are not the real names of the people described.
Further reading:
Extraordinary Minds by Howard Gardner, (Phoenix, 1998).
Is integer arithmetic fundamental to mental processing?
The minds secret arithmetic by Allan W. Snyder & D.
John Mitchell, Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol 266,
p 587 (1999).

The following text is a popularised version of the article, published


in the Proceedings of the Royal Society, London. The Genius
Within explores the secret powers of the mind. The article, by
Professor Allan Snyder, appeared in The Australian, November 12,
1999, as part of his occasional Mindspace column.
93

94

The Genius Within


Professor Allan Snyder
Art, music and mathematics are often presumed to be the
supreme expressions of creative achievement. But, skills in
these areas do not come easily. They require arduous training
and practice.
So, imagine the shock reaction to our latest research, reported
in the Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, which
claims that the genius behind these skills is innate to us all!
And, paradoxically, this genius is supposedly released, not
by hard work, but simply by switching off part of the brain.
Anyone can then have virtuosity in music, art and mathematics
without training.
We made these provocative claims based on intriguing studies
of savants. These are rare individuals who, although severely
brain damaged, display extraordinary skill often in areas
traditionally believed to be the preserve of gifted intellects.

Joseph, the inspiration for the film Rain Man about an autistic
savant, could spontaneously answer What numbers give
1234567890?, stating nine times 137174210. Oliver
Sacks, acclaimed for his work on unusual minds, observed
autistic twins who could exchange prime numbers in excess
of eight figures.
Many savants simultaneously possess all three of these
extraordinary skills in art, music and mathematics. Each
seems to come from nowhere and without training. How do
they do it?
By simple mimicry, according to our latest research. Savants
can somehow peer into the inner workings of the brain. They
merely copy what they see. If we had access to our
unconscious brain processing, we too would have the
extraordinary skills of savants.

For example, by age three, Nadia drew horses with astonishing


life-like perspective. She did so spontaneously, without any
training and always from memory. Nadia was severely
mentally retarded due to autism. She had no language ability
and could not even recognise her mother from her nurse.

It is clear that considerable processing goes on in our brain


before we are ever made aware of something. Through a
number of stages, the brain processes raw facts into
meaningful products concepts, ideas, objects we
understand. But, we are only aware of the last stage in this
assembly line the fully assembled product on which we
act.

Tom, from the age of four, could play Mozart piano sonatas
flawlessly upon one hearing. He could also repeat, word for
word, extended conversations, in any language, even
impersonating the speakers. But, he was mentally retarded
and lacked the ability to communicate.

It is this last stage which blocks us from using or even seeing


the raw facts before they are neatly packaged into this
product. For example, we are not conscious of the reasons
for recognising a familiar object or of the way we construct
sentences while conversing.

95

96

Now, suppose the part of the brain that executes this last stage
of processing is switched off. The barrier is then lifted,
allowing us access to the full riches of unconscious brain
processing. Savants are believed to have this privileged
access. Consequently, autistic savants are literal. They see
detail at the expense of recognising the big picture. Ordinary
people are holistic and conceptual. They see the big picture
but at the expense of recalling detail.
This explanation resonates well with the autistic artist Nadia.
She lacked the last stage of brain processing required to
assemble object attributes, such as shape, colour, texture, into
meaningful names and labels. This enabled her to draw like
the master Leonardo da Vinci, but at the cost of language and
social skills. With maturity, Nadia started to communicate
verbally, but her drawings then became conventional. She
lost her gift.
An intriguing question arises. Although we do not normally
have conscious access to the early stages of brain processing
as do savants, is there nonetheless some artificial means to
promote this access? Can we, on command, switch off the
last stage of brain processing to exhibit savant skills?
Compelling medical histories suggest we can.

October, 1982. He would hear every chord, every riff, every


inflection in a rappers voice sometimes two songs at once.
Another man became musically gifted after suffering from
spinal meningitis. And, a nine-year-old boy became a
mechanical genius after being shot on the left side of his head.
Recently Dr Bruce Miller, a neurologist from the University
of California in San Francisco, discovered that a rare form of
dementia triggered amazing artistic prowess in patients who
previously had no interest or ability in art. But, as with Nadia,
this came at the loss of language and social skills.
So, savant skills in art, music and mathematics can be turned
on in perfectly ordinary people who previously had no training
or interest in such things. Scientists are now trying to devise
methods to do this on command, but in a way that is reversible.
Child prodigies also have skills that are primarily in art, music
and mathematics. And, like autistic savants, they often lose
this gift as they age. Could it be that child prodigies, like
savants, have partial access to the inner working of the mind?
The jury is out on this question.

After being hit in the head by a baseball at ten years old,


Orlando Serrell displayed spectacular savant-like abilities to
perform mathematical calendar calculations and to remember
complex music. For example, he would correctly say it was
a rainy Saturday when asked for the day and weather for 23

Even more curious is the often mentioned relationship


between madness and genius. Perhaps certain forms of mental
illness, like bipolar disorder, intermittently switch off that part
of the brain which presents the meaningful picture, allowing
access to the unconscious early stages of processing. This
would lead to alternating views of the world. One is coherent
and consistent with past experience. The other is chaotic but
with the potential for imaginative connections and unorthodox

97

98

interpretations. Taken together, these two views would convey


awesome creative potential.
Professor Allan Snyder is Director of the Centre for the Mind,
a joint venture of the Australian National University and the
University of Sydney, and was awarded the International
Australia Prize in 1997.

The following text is from Professor Allan Snyders occasional Mindspace


column in The Australian. Game, mindset and match was published in
the 4-5 December, 1999, Weekend Australian. The article explores the
theme that we can only look at this world through our prior experiences.
99

100

Game, Mindset and Match


Professor Allan Snyder
Allan Snyder, in the second of an occasional series, says we
are all innately prejudiced.
I want to confront a disturbing reality. One that no-one ever
wishes to admit. We are all by nature prejudiced. We can
only see this world through our mindsets our preconceptions
derived from past experiences and our prior knowledge. Put
simply, it is extremely difficult to experience the world anew.
And, in my opinion, there is no escaping this condition. Our
brains have craftily evolved this particular strategy for good
reason. Mindsets are a biological necessity if we are to
manoeuvre rapidly in important and familiar situations.
How can we ever become more receptive to novelty when
our minds are subject to these powerful constraints? I say
ride with the tide. Take on all the more mindsets. Because
the more mindsets we imbue, the more different snapshots
we have of this world. The lesson is clear. After mastering
one situation, move on to master another.
Have you ever wondered why you can see meaningful pictures
in what are obviously randomly formed cloud formations?
And, why two people who look at the very same cloud often
see completely different objects? The portrait painter sees a
face of dignity while the ultrasound sonographer sees a
diseased gall bladder. Clearly, our interpretation of the world
depends on our frame of reference.
101

This somewhat apocryphal observation about cloud gazing


lays bare a master plan of the mind. There is no definitive
interpretation of the raw sensory information that bombards
our senses. It would take an eternity for our brains to work
through all the possibilities. But, we have no interest in all
possibilities, only the most likely ones. So, we have evolved
a rather cunning strategy for rapid decision making. We make
assumptions about what is most likely. Our brains do this by
constructing mindsets or mental images about what is familiar
and important. These mindsets then act as templates through
which we view this world.
Mindsets facilitate rapid decision-making. They are the
building blocks for acquiring expertise. Skilled medical
diagnosis is a classic case. A doctor automatically matches a
constellation of symptoms to the most likely disease out of a
finite number of known diseases.
The act of recognition is another example where mindsets
facilitate expertise. We often have difficulty recalling faces.
But it is effortless to recognise them, even when they are
radically altered. When someone familiar to you shaves his
heavy beard, you are often uncertain of what is different about
him even though he has dramatically changed his appearance.
In other words we are conscious of names and labels of things
and not all the detailed reasons assembled for the label in the
first instance. Mindsets allow for these short cuts in decision
making.
But as a consequence of mindsets, we are vulnerable to
102

prejudice in the form of illusions and assumptions. Put simply,


there is a cost for adopting any strategy that accelerates our
decision making process. Nothing can be seen within a neutral
frame of reference.
Anyone who doubts this fact is reminded of the myriad of
illusions, jokes and puzzles which capitalise on our prejudice
from prior knowledge. For example, consider these two
sentences:
The president is waiting for the boys father before awarding
the boy with a gold medal. Coincidentally, the boy is the
presidents son.
Now, this can only appear puzzling if we have a mindset that
all presidents must be male. Few people are confused if the
president is replaced by a nurse. Yet, everyone knows that
there are female presidents and male nurses. But even so,
our minds still jump to the most familiar possibility, instead
of logically sifting through alternatives.
Many visual illusions operate in precisely the same way. Take
the illustration on this page as an example. You see a square,
but in reality no square exists. Why? Our mind has assumed
that the most likely interpretation of this image is a square in
front of four circular objects. So, we mentally extrapolate a
fictitious square boundary to match our expectation.

as concave surfaces appearing convex.


Powerful images distort our view about what is most likely.
For example, airline crashes feature prominently in the news.
They are great stuff for crystallising our mindsets about the
horrors of flying. So, fear of flying prevails despite solid
statistics that airlines are far safer than cars. Statistics are
even ridiculed, as an old joke reveals:
If you are afraid of being on an aeroplane with a bomb, then
you should always carry a bomb on the plane with you. After
all, what is the probability of there being two bombs on the
same aeroplane?
These examples should convince everyone that we experience
this world through our mindsets, assumptions which are
derived from our past experiences. This is the state of ordinary
minds. Only a pathological mind sees the world unfiltered
through prior knowledge. But this comes at the enormous
price of being unable to cope with decision making. I have
discussed such individuals in this column previously (12
November, 1999, page 15). For example, people with infantile
autism observe the world without interpretation or
expectation. Everything for them must be evaluated anew.
Basically, they lack mindsets. So they compensate for this
by repetitive and stereotyped behaviour.

This is but one of many assumptions we unconsciously use


when interpreting our visual world. For example, all of us
unconsciously assume that light comes from above. Bizarre
illusions occur when objects are illuminated from below such

Clearly, mindsets are a masterful strategy which allows us to


operate automatically in familiar situations. But, they come
at the unavoidable cost of prejudice. We only see a filtered
version of the world. What then is my suggestion for seeing
more and thus becoming more open-minded and creative?

103

104

Obviously, it is to take on more mindsets. Because the more


mindsets we imbue, the more different views we have of the
world. So, after mastering one situation, it is best to go on
and master another. The rabbit-duck drawing on this page
serves as an illustrative example.
If we only know about ducks, then we only see a duck when
looking at this drawing. And, if we only know about rabbits,
then we only see a rabbit. But, familiarity with both rabbits
and ducks gives us the luxury of having two interpretations.
My suggestion that creativity is facilitated by having an enormous repertoire of mindsets complements the opinion of the
brilliant historian of ideas, Jacob Bronowski. We become
creative by finding a likeness between things which were not
alike before. The creative mind is a mind that looks for unexpected likenesses. In other words, creativity flourishes
by juggling our mindsets. The more we can juggle, the better. So our mindsets make us prejudiced, but in return they
bestow immense creative potential.

105

You might also like