Can Threatened Languages Be Saved PDF
Can Threatened Languages Be Saved PDF
Can Threatened Languages Be Saved PDF
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
D
i
s
t
r
i
c
t
s
f
r
o
m
t
h
e
1
9
8
0
a
n
d
1
9
9
0
C
e
n
s
u
s
e
s
o
f
P
o
p
u
l
a
t
i
o
n
a
n
d
H
o
u
s
i
n
g
.
D
e
p
a
r
t
m
e
n
t
o
f
C
i
t
y
P
l
a
n
n
i
n
g
,
A
u
g
u
s
t
1
9
9
2
60 The Americas
Bronx, all three neighbourhoods of traditional original Puerto Rican settle-
ment (Institute for Puerto Rican Policy, 1996).
But it is important to note again that income and language shift does not
always correlate positively for Puerto Ricans and other Latinos. For
example, in Mott Haven, the south Bronx neighbourhood with the highest
poverty index inthe city andwiththe lowest Latino per capita income, 27%
of Latinos are monolingual Spanishspeakers. Yet, inthe more middle-class
Jackson Heights and Elmhurst, 36% of Latinos are monolingual Spanish
speakers.
This again is uncharacteristic of other US ethnolinguistic groups,
although these figures mask differences between ways of language groups
and ways of national groups. That is, although all Latinos have been
racialised by the majority, the experience of other immigrant minorities is
clearlydifferent fromthat of PuertoRicans as caste or colonised groups.
Jackson Heights and Elmhurst have three times the number of other
Latinos than Puerto Ricans. These other Latinos, although more recent and
more Spanish monolingual, many times come with skills that help them
achieve middle class status. Theyalsohave not sufferedfor longthe inequi-
ties of living in a country that considers them a colonised inferior people
and that subjects them to a marginal public education.
Puerto Ricans with middle-class status tend to move to more middle-
class communities, sometimes white English-speaking communities, but
mostly Latino middle class communities. The two Latino neighbour-
hoods in the Bronx with the highest per capita income among Latinos,
West Farms andSoundview-Castle Hill ($9,739 and$10,121 respectively),
also have the lowest percentage of Spanish monolingual speakers (19%
and 16% of the Latino population respectively, with 59% of the popula-
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 61
Table 3.3 198090 change of Puerto Ricans and other Latinos in New York
City and the five boroughs
% Change
Latinos
% Change
Puerto Ricans
% Change
Other Latinos
New York City 26.8 5.2 60.2
Bronx 32.4 9.7 126.6
Brooklyn 17.6 0.4 60.1
Manhattan 15.3 16.8 37.1
Queens 44.6 24.1 53.7
Staten Island 56.2 54.2 59.3
Source: Demographic Profiles. A Portrait of New York Citys Community Districts from the
1980 and 1990 Censuses of Population and Housing. Department of City Planning, August
1992.
tion in both neighbourhoods being Latino). These middle-class neigh-
bourhoods, mostly Puerto Rican, pull toward English. But as in Jackson
Heights and Elmhurst, there are also Puerto Ricans who choose to join
other Latino middle-class immigrants. Those are constantly pulled
toward Spanish.
The Puerto Rican community in New York thus finds itself in three
different patterns of language use and proficiency and neighbourhood
income:
(1) Those who live in middle-class Latino neighbourhoods with high
community bilingual proficiency (that is, only one of approximately
six speakers are monolingual speakers of Spanish).
In West Farms and Soundview-Castle Hill
(2) Those who live in poor Latino neighbourhoods with mid- community
bilingual proficiency (that is, only one of approximately four speakers
are monolingual speakers of Spanish).
Neighbourhoods are listed here beginning with those with the lowest
per capita income: In Mott Haven, Hunts Point, Morris Heights-Mt.
Hope, Belmont, Crotona Park East-East Tremont, University Heights-
Fordham, Williamsburgh, Highbridge-West Concourse, Lower East Side,
East Harlem.
(3) Those who live in middle-class Latino neighbourhoods with low
community bilingual proficiency (that is, approximately one of two
speakers are Spanish monolinguals).
Jackson Heights, Elmhurst
For Puerto Rican New Yorkers, both English language shift and Spanish
language maintenance are associated with middle-class status.
Although as with other US ethnolinguistic groups, middle class status
often brings Puerto Ricans into more contact with English, it can also bring
them into more contact with Spanish. And although poverty is associated
with many of the neighbourhoods in which Puerto Ricans live, Puerto
Rican poverty, as we saw earlier, is also lived out in English.
Puerto Ricans with Spanish in the Heart and Dominicans with
Spanish in the Mouth
A comparison of Puerto Ricans with Dominicans, the fastest-growing
Latino New York group, allows us to analyse further the role of Puerto
62 The Americas
Ricans as language agents in the NewYork scene. From1980 to 1990 while
the Puerto Rican community grew only 5.2%, the Dominican community
grew by 165%. Yet, Dominicans officially account for only 19% of the city
Latinos (332,713), as compared to the half of Puerto Rican ethnicity
(896,763). So despite the fact that 96% of Dominicans claimed to speak
Spanish at home in 1990, as compared to 87% of Puerto Ricans, there are
many more Spanish-speaking Puerto Ricans in New York (780,129), than
Spanish-speaking Dominicans (319,392). However, this fact, derived from
the 1990 census figures, masks ability in Spanish or degree of use, directing
us to recognise that the presence of Spanish in New York homes is still in
the hearts of Puerto Ricans, although perhaps Spanish may be used there
seldomly or haltingly.
Although census figures do not reveal information about Spanish-
speaking ability, English-speaking ability is accounted for. We know that
71% of Puerto Ricans in 1990 spoke English very well (636,657), whereas
only 38% of Dominicans claimed to do so (126,426). The numbers of those
Spanish-speakers from both groups who do not speak English well then
comes surprisingly close, with 260,106 Puerto Ricans and 206,287 Domini-
cans claiming not to speak English well. Yet, Spanish speaking Puerto
Ricans who need to speak Spanish because of limited English proficiency
are certainlynot the majorityof NewYorkLatinos, especiallywhenwe take
into account the entire Latino universe in New York. Although Puerto
Rican New Yorkers continue to identify with the Spanish language and
continue to hold on to aspects of it in a linguistic vaivn, other NewYorkers
increasingly see themas English speaking, with Dominicans having taken
their place as the Spanish speakers of New York.
That Spanish is in the hearts, although not always in the mouths of
PuertoRican, alsohas todowiththe outstandinggrowthof the native-born
Puerto Rican population, a factor which also greatly contributes to the pull
towards English language shift. In 1970 60%of Puerto Ricans in the United
States had been born in Puerto Rico. That figure decreased to 42% in the
1990 census.
Spanish for New York Puerto Ricans, like the Puerto Rican flag, has
takenonAmericanways. Commentingonthe wayinwhichhe displays the
Puerto Rican flag today, one of the second generation males we inter-
viewed in Bushwick told us:
I put my Puerto Ricanflag onthe rear viewmirror. I never put it out the
window, otherwise people say: Mira ese jbaro con la bandera por la
ventana.
[Look at that hillbilly with the flag out the window.]
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 63
Like the flag on the rear view mirror, the Spanish language is displayed
only within American norms by second generation Puerto Ricans, with
restraint and moderation, and communicating moving forward, although
looking backwards. That is, like the discourse of this second generation
speaker, Spanish is used within the context of English, reflecting, like the
rear viewmirror, anislandrealitythat is always inthe backgroundandthat
is contained in the New York foreground.
Puerto Rican Identity and Language. Everybody I Know is
like Spanglish
The increased immigration fromother Latin American countries clearly
has diffused the original link that had existed between Puerto Rican iden-
tity and the Spanish language. One of the second generation men we inter-
viewed complained about the lack of presence of Puerto Rican culture and
speechinNewYork, as other Latinos tookover the media andthe schools.
Were not exposed to Puerto Rican culture and of Puerto Rican
language as we used to, even on TV. There are more transHispanic
things on TV.
But in reality, the language-identity link between Puertoricanness and
Spanish has been absent fromthe NewYork Puerto Rican community for a
long time, as supported in studies by Attinasi (1979) and Zentella (1990c).
Inher recent book(1997a), Zentella explains howPuertoRicanidentitywas
redefined in el bloque without a Spanish requirement in order to accommo-
date monolingual English youngsters. A second generation Puerto Rican
we interviewed, working as a special education teacher in a school with a
majority of Spanish speakers, had this to say when asked What makes a
Puerto Rican in New York?
The music, the food, the way we talk, sometimes Mira Papi, like
talking con cario, sweet tone of voice. Traditions, msica tpica, you
know, lelolai, salsa, pernil, pastel, arroz con gandules.
It is clear that the Spanishlanguage is not listedas one of the components of
Puerto Rican identity; although it is used intuitively, it is embedded in
cultural expressions such as music, relationships, food, and ways of life. In
the school in which this young man works, the majority of students are still
of Puerto Rican descent, but Spanish is to them a second language. The
Spanish speakers are now Hondurans and Mexicans. It is this young man
whose words tell us a lot about howboth Spanish and English and the mix
between them is what defines a Puerto Rican New Yorker: Everybody I
64 The Americas
know is like Spanglish. Un poquito espaol, un poquito ingls. [A little bit
Spanish, a little bit English.] What makes this quote interesting is that this
young man does not refer to the linguistic vaivn as something spoken, but
as something lived, a Puerto Rican identity itself.
Askedif she thought that it was important toteachSpanishtoher grand-
daughter who accompanied her, a 74-year-old woman who had lived in
Bushwick since 1950 told us:
A los muchachos les enseamos nuestra cultura, nuestra bandera y
Dios. Los llevamos a la Parada para que se sientan puertorriqueos. Y
les hablamos de Dios porque Dios no tiene raza.
[To the children we teach our culture, our flag and God. We take them
to the Parade so that they feel Puerto Rican. And we talk to themabout
God because God doesnt have any race.]
This grandmother cannot consciously distinguish between the Spanish
that she speaks, her flag, and God. But although she consciously takes her
granddaughter to the Pentecostal church where she learns about God, and
takes her to the Puerto Rican parade, she cannot externalise the language
she speaks as anobject that couldbe taught, she merelyspeaks it, it is part of
her, her culture, and her beliefs, and that is the way she transmits it to her
granddaughter, who, of course, speaks English to her.
This idea that Spanish is part of the Puerto Rican being, although its not
always inthe mouth, is clearlyexpressedbythe youngBushwickmale who
says:
Spanishis always part of us. Its innate. Whenyoure inNewYork, you
dont even think Spanish. When you get off the plane, you dont speak
English.
Some proficiency in Spanish is necessary for New York Puerto Ricans to
continue the link to their island identity. One of the young males we inter-
viewed put it this way:
In Puerto Rico everybody is Puertorican. Theres more warmth. El
calor. [The heat.] I can communicate better with my own. There I speak
Spanish to older family members, but I speak English to the younger
ones because most have learned it some in school.
The above quote communicates the changing language situation in Puerto
Rico, for although Puerto Rico is fiercely Spanish speaking, more andmore
of the younger generationare learning Englishat school. So althoughinthe
past, the emotional andreal linktothe islandhadtobe done inSpanish, it is
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 65
becoming possible to a certain extent to speak English to island Puerto
Ricans and to continue being Spanglish even in the island.
This change in language situation in the island has major connotations
for New York Puerto Ricans, for those who still come have much more
familiarity with the English language than those who came before, and
certainly more than other Latin American immigrant groups.
Spanish literacy and New York Puerto Ricans
The colonial status of Puerto Rico has greatly affected Puerto Ricans
ability to maintain Spanish or reverse their language shift. The past Puerto
RicanmigrationtoNewYorkhadlowliteracyrates inSpanish, a product of
a colonised school system with a misguided policy of English language
education for people who spoke Spanish only. This aging population
remains trappedinpoverty, unable to benefit frommany adult Englishas a
Second Language programmes or to connect with the Spanish language
print that is now evident in billboards, and that appears as translations to
the many governmental forms in an increasingly bureaucratic world.
Again, the degree of literacy in Spanish of the New York Puerto Rican
population is also responsible for its differing linguistic vaivn. Spanish, in
those homes, has always been the language of the heart and the home,
whereas English (in the long-gone Puerto Rico they remember) was
supposed to be the language of school and certainly of the literacy of the
powerful United States (Walsh, 1991).
Although maintenance bilingual education in New York was the
struggle of second generation Puerto Rican young adults in the early 70s,
two factors weakened the effort:
(1) La lucha contina [The struggle continues] most often referred to
sociopolitical rights, rather than to language rights.
(2) They themselves many times lacked Spanish literacy, a product of
going to school in English only and having lived in homes where
Spanish was spoken, but neither read nor written.
Today, the use of Spanishprint inPuertoRicanhomes is scarce. The reader-
ship of El Diario La Prensa has shifted from being mostly Puerto Rican to
mostly Dominican. Zentella (1997a: 214) confirms the predominance of
English print in the homes of the former residents of el bloque.
Spanish in Schools for Puerto Ricans: From Schools for Shift
and Maintenance to Schools to Reverse Language Shift
Nowhere is the shift of Puerto Ricans as Spanish speakers to Puerto
66 The Americas
Ricans as English speakers with vaivn more evident than in the NewYork
City public schools. In 199293, 355,889 Latino students were enrolled in
NewYork City public schools, accounting for 36%of the total public school
enrolment. Of these, 101,383 (28%) were identified as English language
learners (Latino Commission on Educational Reform 1994).
As with the census, no quantitative data is available for the number who
are Spanish speakers. But even more disturbing is the fact that the New
York City Board of Education does not keep separate figures on Puerto
Ricans, makingit difficult tosubstantiate withquantitative data the change
that has taken place.
Although bilingual education came into existence for Puerto Ricans as a
result of the Aspira Consent Decree, few Puerto Rican children remain
today in the transitional bilingual education classrooms, agents of
language shift, where Spanish monolingual students acquire English.
Transitional bilingual education classrooms today hold mostly Dominican
and Mexican students.
In the 1970s maintenance bilingual-bicultural education programmes
where Puerto Rican children were taught in both English and Spanish and
where Puerto Rican culture and history were also taught were developed
throughout the city. These programmes, staffed mostly by Puerto Rican
teachers, and supported by the post-Civil Rights climate of the nation at
large, had biculturismand bilingualismas goals. District 4 in East Harlem,
for example, supported and developed a strong maintenance bilingual-
bicultural education model. But as the political climate changed, as the
Puerto Rican population became more dispersed, and more English
speaking, and as other Latino groups, immigrants with a different
sociohistorical connectiontothe UnitedStates, startedtocome intothe city,
maintenance bilingual education programmes started to take a turn.
Today, maintenance bilingual-bicultural education programmes have
been mostly substituted by Dual Language Programmes where students
who are fluent in English are taught with Spanish speakers who are not.
The classic dual language model calls for half the students to be English-
speakingandhalf not, withits primarygoal beingthe full Englishlanguage
acquisition of those who have limited English proficiency, as well as their
cultural assimilation. A secondary goal is to develop the second language
of the English speakers. But a closer look inside those classrooms reveals a
lot about the language shift that has taken place among Puerto Ricans in
New York. Increasingly, the students who make up the English-speaking
and non-Spanish-speaking population of the Dual Language Programmes
are second and third generation Puerto Ricans, nowlacking full fluency in
Spanish. So, although on paper dual language programmes are integrated,
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 67
theymostlyremainethnicallysegregated, withLatinostudents of differing
language ability making up the student body. For the most part, Puerto
Ricans make up the English-ability half, and Dominicans, Mexicans, and
Central and South Americans make up the other Spanish-ability half. Dual
language programmes for New York Puerto Ricans are clearly agents of
reversing language shift.
The shift in the goals of bilingual education in NewYork City, however,
are indicative not only of a changing political climate, but also of the
changing population. Today, for the first time, there is a lack of cultural
congruence betweenthe bilingual teachers, nowmostlyPuerto Ricans, and
the students. In the 1970s and early 1980s bilingual teachers were recruited
fromPuerto Rico andfromthe barrios, where programmes to educate para-
professionals from the community cropped up.
But as the student-body in those programmes became less and less
Puerto Rican, the viability of developing biculturism became more diffi-
cult, onthe one handbecause of the cultural diversityof the Latinostudents
themselves, on the other because although these teachers knewa lot about
Puerto Rican culture and history, they seldom were knowledgeable of the
LatinAmericanworldat large, havingbeenmostlyproducts of a US educa-
tion. The parents have also changed, with immigrant Spanish-speaking
parents whoincreasinglytravel backandforthandwhohave not yet devel-
oped a sense of being an ethnolinguistic US group, showing less commit-
ment to bilingual education than Puerto Ricans. In the 1990s, the goals of
bilingual educationprogrammes inNewYorkhave increasinglynarrowed
to include only English language acquisition, with English as a Second
Language programmes increasingly taking over Bilingual Education
programmes.
Only dual language programmes, possible mechanisms of reversing
language shift for PuertoRicans, give a glimmer of hope. But inthe difficult
sociopolitical climate that ushers in the new millennium, even dual
language programmes are being questioned, and the enthusiasm of the
Puerto Rican community for them is at best mild.
New York Puerto Rican Institutional Changes
The Puerto Rican institutions that were created as a result of the Civil
Rights struggles of the 1960s and 1970s have been under political, social
and economic pressures that have resulted in their change or disappear-
ance.
Academic departments of Puerto Rican studies have been closed or are
experiencingserious cutbacks. For example, inthe early1990s, CityCollege
68 The Americas
of NewYork changed the name of the Puerto Rican Studies Department to
that of Latin American and Caribbean Studies. But this did not save it from
losing departmental status and becoming just an academic programme.
Budgetary constraints have also been experienced in the Puerto Rican
Legal Defense and Education Fund and the Institute for Puerto Rican
Policy. This has ledthese twoorganisations tomerge inorder tosurvive. As
we said earlier, few maintenance bilingual education programmes,
responding to community life, exist. A noted exception is the educational
programmes in El Puente, a community-based group in Williamsburg,
Brooklyn. But even its alternative High School for Social Justice has shifted
from having a mostly Puerto Rican population to one that is mostly
Dominican.
The move to newstandards in education can be particularly destructive
of Spanish language efforts supported by school and society. As of this
year, all high school graduates will have to pass an English Language
Regents Exam, effectively eliminating the possibility that anyone without
near-native English skills will be able to have a high school diploma. This,
coupledwiththe rippling andcontinuing effects of Proposition227 inCali-
fornia, has seriously eliminated the possibility of developmental mainte-
nance bilingual education programmes, where Latino students could
develop their Spanish language skills.
Nowhere has this shift been more evident than in the attacks experi-
encedbythe bilingual communitycollege of CityUniversityof NewYorks
Hostos Community College. Increasingly, the population there has also
shifted from a Puerto Rican one to a mostly Dominican one. But increas-
ingly, academic courses are taught inEnglishonly, closingthe possibilityof
an Associates degree to those who do not have full English proficiency.
As Spanish recedes from academic settings organised and run by the
majority, it has taken refuge in the Pentecostal churches that have
continued to crop up in Latino communities. Urciuoli points out:
The Spanish-speaking Pentecostal churches stand out as a strikingly
grass-roots phenomenon, a place in which Spanish is performed with
authority, ceding no ground to English or Americans. (Urciuoli, 1997:
95)
The Pentecostal church in which we conducted our Bushwick interviews
certainly gave us a glimmer of that reality, with Spanish being the unifying
factor among the old and the young, the first and second/third genera-
tions. And although English was heard often in informal association,
Spanishwas the language not only of the heart andinterior prayer, but also
of proclamations of faith, of the reading of the Bible, and of religious
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 69
instruction. In fact, the children, most in monolingual English classrooms,
had learned to read and write Spanish during Bible study.
Conclusion: Shifting the Focus of Sociology of Language.
From Language Shift/Language Maintenance to Reversing
Language Shift
In 1991 Fishmans Reversing Language Shift turned the focus of language
shift/language maintenance studies from mere descriptions of socio-
linguistic situations to steps that can be taken by ethnolinguistic communi-
ties to do something about their weak sociolinguistic status. RLS, Fishman
states, is an attempt . . . to adopt policies andto engage in efforts calculated
to reverse the cumulative processes of attrition that would otherwise lead
to the contextually weak language-in-culture becoming even weaker (p.
81). Then, speaking of the New York Puerto Rican community, Fishman
had warned that despite the large number of recently arrived monolingual
Spanish speakers, one was left with the impression of a major language
shift tidal wave underway under the surface (p. 192).
Referring to Fishmans Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale,
Zentella 1997a placed the New York Puerto Rican community of the early
1980s between Stages 6 (Intergenerational informal oralcy and its demo-
graphic concentration and institutional reinforcement) and Stage 5
(Spanish literacy in home, community, and schools, especially in own
instructional arrangements). But the previous analysis leads us to believe
that in the last decade, the New York Puerto Rican community not only
may have abandoned efforts of Spanish language maintenance but may
have slipped frombeing between Stages 6 and 5 to being between Stages 7
and6, having given up on almost all efforts of Spanish literacy andmoving
toward Stage 7 where Spanish is used for cultural interaction with the
community based older generation.
It is clear that the New York Puerto Rican community has experienced
great physical and demographic dislocation, severe social dislocation, and
harsh cultural dislocation, all identified by Fishman (1991: 5765) as
leading to language shift. The NewYork Puerto Rican community has lost
its demographic concentration, weakening its language network. It has
had no increase in the use of Spanish in any domain, not in the family, nor
work, nor education, nor religion, nor entertainment and mass media, nor
in any political party or governmental function. There has been little effort
to foster intergenerational Spanish language maintenance, although there
has been limited interest in dual language programmes where New York
70 The Americas
Puerto Rican children have the potential to acquire at least oralcy in
Spanish.
Yet, it is unlikely that the New York Puerto Rican community will
completely face language death or slip toward Stage 8 where Spanish
would be spoken only by socially isolated old folks. But the mechanismby
whichthis is sohas little todowithorganisedefforts of a US ethnolinguistic
group moving towards their ancestral language, and more to do with a
colonised situation that keeps a linguistic vaivn going. This linguistic
vaivn not only enables speakers to connect with their two sociopolitical
realities, but is spurred by the discrimination and inferior education to
which colonised minorities are subjected. New York Puerto Ricans
continue to use Spanish signs when speaking among themselves and other
Spanish speakers, even while speaking English. Urciuoli, defending the
idea that the language of racialised bilinguals does not equal culture
expressed a similar idea:
When bilingual siblings gossip or tease in English and Spanish, the
pragmatics of English is much more like the pragmatics of Spanish
than it is like the pragmatics of English spoken with an Anglo doctor.
(Urciuoli, 1997: 6).
It is clear that the Englishof NewYork Puerto Ricans has beeninfusedwith
the colonised meanings of Puerto Rican Spanish and the meanings of the
Spanish of other New York Latinos. The New York Puerto Rican commu-
nityis oftencomfortable withits linguistic vaivn, showingbilingual ability
over other monolingual Spanish speakers, but also over Anglo monolin-
guals. Thus, its comfort makes prospects of reversing language shift unin-
teresting, since theres little attachment to Spanish as a symbol of identity
or as an instrument of greater social benefit. Yet, when the New York
Puerto Rican community interacts in public forums, especially in public
schools and in written form, English monolinguals and Spanish monolin-
guals evaluate the vaivn negatively, promoting linguistic insecurity. This
clash moves the New York Puerto Rican community along the language
shift axis, while the stigmatisation of the majority prevents it from
completely undergoing shift. It is this distinct sociolinguistic pattern,
reserved for colonised groups, that distinguishes the models of language
shift/maintenance/language shift reversal for New York Puerto Ricans,
who, like the steps of the cha-cha-ch, continue tomarktheir ownlinguistic
space, both mainland and island, both Latino and Anglo, and reflecting, in
language use, the inventedandunique status of the political commonwealth.
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 71
Note
* The term New York Puerto Ricans is used throughout this paper to avoid the
negative connotations that have beenassociatedwiththe termNuyoricans. The
authors wish especially to thank Juan Rodriguez from Long Island University
who introducedus to the Bushwick Puerto Rican community. Were also grate-
ful to Ana Celia Zentella for her reading of a previous version of this paper.
Special thanks go to Nlida Prez and Jorge Matos of the Library of the Center
for Puerto Rican Studies for their helpful suggestions.
References
AndreuIglesias, C. (ed.) (1977) Memorias de Bernardo Vega. Ro Piedras, Puerto Rico:
Ediciones Huracn.
Attinasi, J. (1979) Language attitudes in a NewYork Puerto Rican community. In R.
Padilla (ed.) Bilingual Education and Public Policy in the United States (pp. 40861).
Ypsilanti, MI: Eastern Michigan University.
Blauner, R. (1972) Racial Oppression in America. New York: Harper & Row.
El Diario (1993) 80aos de historia: 19131993. Laluchapor la educacin, pp. 920.
El Diario (1974) Editorial. Victoria para la educacin bilinge, 4 de septiembre de
1974, p. 17.
Fernandez, R. (1994) Prisoners of Colonialism: The Struggle for Justice in Puerto Rico.
Monroe, ME: Common Courage Press.
Figueroa, L. (1977) History of Puerto Rico. New York: L.A. Publishing Company.
Fishman, J.A. (1970) Sociolinguistics: A Brief Introduction. Rowley, MA: Newbury
House.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Fishman, J.A., Cooper, R.L. and Ma, R. (1971) Bilingualism in the Barrio.
Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Garca, O. (1997) World languages and their role in a US city. In O. Garca and J.A.
Fishman (eds) The Multilingual Apple. Languages in New York City. Berlin/New
York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Gumperz, J.J. (1982) Discourse Strategies. Cambridgeshire: Cambridge University
Press.
Hernndez-Chvez, E. (1994) Language policyinthe UnitedStates: Ahistoryof cul-
tural genocide. In T. Skutnabb-Kangas and R. Phillipson (eds) Linguistic Human
Rights: Overcoming Linguistic Discrimination (pp. 141158). Berlin/New York:
Mouton de Gruyen.
Institute for Puerto Rican Policy (1996) New York City Latino Neighbourhoods Data
Book. New York City: Institute for Puerto Rican Policy.
Karnow, S. (1989) In our Image: Americas Empire in the Philippines. New York:
Ballantine.
Language Policy Task Force (1992) English and Colonialism in Puerto Rico. In J.
Crawford (ed.) Language Loyalties (pp. 6371). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Language Policy Task Force (1988) Speech and Ways of Speaking in a Bilingual Puerto
Rican Community. New York: Centro de Estudios Puertorriqueos.
Latino Commission on Educational Reform(1994) Making the Vision a Reality: A La-
tino Action Agenda for Educational Reform. New York City Board of Education.
Lewis, G.K. (1974) Notes onthe Puerto RicanRevolution. NewYork: MonthlyReview.
72 The Americas
Maldonado-Denis, M. (1972) Puerto Rico: A Socio-historic Interpretation. New York:
Vintage Books.
Ogbu, J.U. (1988) Cultural diversity and human development. In D.T. Slaughter
(ed.) Black Childrenand Poverty: ADevelopmental Perspective (pp. 1128). SanFran-
cisco: Jossey-Bass.
Pedraza, P. (1985) Language maintenance among New York Puerto Ricans. In L.
Elas-Olivares, E. Leone, R. Cisneros and J. Gutirrez (eds) Spanish Language Use
and Public Life in the United States (pp. 5972).
Pedraza, P., Attinasi, J. and Hoffman, G. (1980) Rethinking Diglossia (Language Pol-
icy Task Force Working Paper No. 9). New York: Centro de Estudios
Puertorriqueos.
Poplack, S. (1980) Sometimes Ill start a sentence in Spanish y termino en espaol:
Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 18, 581616.
Poplack, S. (1981) Quantitative analysis of a functional and formal constraint on
code-swithcing (Centro de Estudios Puertorriqueos Working Paper No. 2).
New York: Centro de Estudios Puertorriqueos.
Poplack, S. (1988) Language status andlanguage accommodation along a linguistic
border. In P. Lowenberg (ed.) Language Spread and Language Policy: Issues, Impli-
cations and Case Studies (pp. 90118). Washington, DC: Georgetown University
Press.
Snchez-Korrol, V. 1983 [1994]. FromColonia to Community (updatededn). CA: Uni-
versity of California Press.
Torres, L. (1997) Puerto Rican Discourse. ASociolinguistic Study of a NewYork Suburb.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Trask, H.K. (1993) From a Native Daughter: Colonialism and Sovereignty in Hawaii.
Monroe, ME: Common Courage Press.
Tras Monge, J. (1997). Puerto Rico: The Trials of the Oldest Colony in the World. New
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Urciuoli, B. (1997). Exposing Prejudice. Puerto Rican Experiences of Language, Race, and
Class. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Walsh, C.E. (1991) Pedagogy and the Struggle for Voice: Issues of Language, Power, and
Schooling for Puerto Ricans. New York: Bergin & Garvey.
Zentella, A.C. (1982) Code switching and interactions among Puerto Rican chil-
dren. In J. Amastae and L. Elas-Olivares (eds). Spanish in the United States:
Sociolinguistic Aspects (pp. 386412). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Zentella, A.C. (1990) Lexical leveling in four New York City Spanish dialects: Lin-
guistic and social factors. Hispania 73, 10942015.
Zentella, A.C. (1997a) Growing Up Bilingual. Puerto Rican Children in NewYork. New
York: Blackwell.
Zentella, A.C. (1997b) Spanish in NewYork. In O. Garca andJ.A.Fishman (eds) The
Multilingual Apple. Languages inNewYork (pp. 167201). Berlin/NewYork: Mou-
ton de Gruyter.
The Spanish of New York Puerto Ricans 73
Chapter 4
A Decade in the Life of a Two-in-One
Language
Yiddish in New York City (Secular and Ultra-
Orthodox)
J.A. FISHMAN
I remember the first time I realised that Jews as a whole were an item of
considerable curiosity for many Christian Americans. I was then a
freshman at the University of Pennsylvania, in Philadelphia, the city in
which I was born and had grown up in predominantly Jewish neighbour-
hoods. The classmate in the seat next to me volunteered the information
that I was the first Jew he had ever actually met, face to face. He was
surprised to find that I appeared to be just like anyone else! The reason I
mention this here is because many people, even sociolinguists among
them, Jews and non-Jews, have equally uninformed and totally unrealistic
images about Yiddish, images associated with its distant past: in the
pogrom-ridden Old Country, or in turn-of-the-century immigrant
pushcart America, or during the Holocaust, or in scenes of mourning
mountains of deadbodies. Howstrange, it has seemedto them, to findthat
inmany ways it is a language like any other, that it is still around, obeying
the same laws of language-in-society life-and-death that apply to any and
all languages, andthat reversing language shift (RLS) efforts are underway
in conjunction with it. However, just as it has been said about Jews that
they are just like all other people, only a little more so, so Yiddish, without
a country or a government to fend for it, is like all other threatened
languages, only a little more so. As the chasmwithinits ranks, betweenits
secular Yiddishist and ultra-Orthodox advocates, i.e. between its most
visible defenders and its most constant speakers, both deepens and
widens, its situation provides us with many unusually striking opportuni-
ties to ponder RLS concerns, processes and outcomes as a whole.
74
The Americas
Yiddish in New York City
In the Year 2050: Yiddish still Dying
In mid-20th century, the Jerusalem Post tried humorously to anticipate
headlines of the year 2000 andone of those that it felt reasonably sure about
was Yiddish still dying! However, while threatened languages generally
are reacted to by a peculiarly affective mixture of sadness, tenderness and
mirth, Yiddish is the proverbial and archetypical dying language. For
some reason it is also widely considered to be funny (naturally so, since it
has [had, for those who always refer to it in the past tense (such as
Harshav 1990]), a terrific Yiddish sense of humour. Hillary, Bubele, the
NewYork Post intoned on 6 August 1999, when Hillary Clintons senatorial
campaignrevealedthat she hadhada Jewish(andYiddish-speaking!) step-
grandfather. It is exceedingly rare for an article about Yiddish in the Amer-
ican press not to be played for laughs via a sprinkling of Yinglishisms
(yenta, shiksa, oy-vey, kvelling, gevald, meshuggah, shabbos, tuchis,
tsuris, mohel, were all included in this one brief entre nous put-down
column), calculated to amuse or to titillate trendy English-speaking New
Yorkers who generally consider Yiddish to be a fully owned subsidiary of
English anyway). Yiddish is funny, but, of course it is also dying, already
dead, or at the very least archaic, while at the very same time it is tender,
expressive and folksy. Many of these contradictory views have been
advanced by Jews themselves and they should know, because their own
great-grandmother was Yiddish-speaking. How to break out of this
swirling mass of misinformation, wishful thinking and lovehate that has
hung on for well over a century? With some resignation about my chances
of making facts triumph over fiction, I set out to try to do so once again (for
my previous attempts, see Fishman 1965, 1966, 1981, 1985, 1989).
The Current Overall Scene of Yiddish in New York City
What Paris is to FrenchandFrenchliterature andculture, NewYork and
Tel-Aviv/Jerusalem are to Yiddish, whether in secular or in ultra-
Orthodox contexts. For secular Yiddishists in the USA, New York City
(and, to some extent even its total greater metropolitan area) is the
languages undisputed capital-cum-Mecca. Yiddish was already being
spokeninNewYork City evenbefore the AmericanRevolution(the patriot
HymanSolomonwrote manyYiddishletters tofamilyandbusiness associ-
ates back home (in Germany) and Yiddish letters from New York
describing the first fourth of July and the adoption of the Constitution are
well known to historians adept at using archival sources). But Yiddish
became a truly major language in NewYork only when the mass immigra-
tion of Eastern European Jews began in the early 1880s. The city that had
Yiddish in New York City 75
already become a prototypic primary settlement area for various groups
of immigrants and their children well before the beginning of the 20th
century, and that has retained much of that very same flavour for the
grandchildren and great-grandchildren of immigrants to this very day, is
even more than that in the case of Yiddish. For Yiddish, New York is not
only the great American metropolis, not only the great American immi-
grant city, but the American city on which Yiddish has left a permanent
mark, not only in diet and in interactional style (referred to as argumenta-
tion and rudeness as a means of simple communication and human socia-
bility), but in accent, prosodics and lexicon as well. NewYork may well be
the onlycityinwhichmiddle-agedBlackandHispanic residents who grew
up in formerly Jewish neighbourhoods, now bemoan the fact that their
own children dont even knowthe difference between a shmendrik
1
and a
shabes-goy,
2
even as a mass of more common Yiddishisms (both lexical
and grammatical) are fully in the popular domain and require no transla-
tion at all to native NewYorkers of whatever background, even if they are
often translated for those who may be from out of town.
It is here, in New York, that Yiddish events, activities and artifacts that
have disappeared (or never even had established themselves at all) almost
everywhere else in the USA, can still be readily encountered rather than
only sought out by the well informed. Neighbourhoods where Yiddish is
the normal vernacular of young andoldstill exist; all-day-schools in which
Yiddish is the normal language of instruction for Judaic subjects still func-
tion and even regularly increase in number; Yiddish religious lectures/
courses andYiddishliteraryevents still regularlyoccur; Yiddishperiodical
publications are still issued and displayed on newsstands; an active youth
group (Yugntruf) functions for those whose Jewish life-style is completely
or, at least substantially, secular; Yiddish theatrical and choral perfor-
mances are regularlylistedeveninthe Englishdailies for the general public
(not to mention the Anglo-Jewish weeklies); first-rate public library and
university library collections are open to the public and to scholars alike;
hundreds of old and new Yiddish books, musical records and tapes are
available for purchase at several convenient locations; Yiddish courses for
adults both for college credit and for adult education are available at
schools, colleges, synagogues andcommunitycentres throughout the year;
a resident Yiddish theatre still performs and provides special English-
buffeted matinee performances for children; a handful of secular Yiddish
supplementary schools for children still function, as do both a secular and
an ultra-Orthodox publishing house; the headquarters of the Workmans
Circle (the oldest and largest fraternal Yiddish secular organisation), the
YIVO Institute for Jewish Research, the activist League for Yiddish and
76 The Americas
several other Yiddish-involved and Yiddish-promoting organisations
quietly toil year-round. All in all, the above constitute an amazingly active
scene for a purportedly dead language.
Although several of the above efforts (but by no means all of them) are
functioning at a far lower level of intensity than was the case a decade or
two ago, the very presence and persistence of those that are fading is note-
worthy, particularly if one realises that sometimes no more than a few
hundred individuals constitute their local activist core and a fewthousand
their total periphery. When the strikingly modernist Yiddish poet Moyshe
Leyb Halpern (18861932) proclaimed NewYork to be our garden (1919),
he had no idea that more than three-quarters of a century after he boldly
staked out this claim there would still be stalwarts, such as these activists,
working tirelessly in what remains of that garden. Any twilight as
prolonged as this one not to mention that it is still high noon on much of
the ultra-Orthodox side of the Yiddishstreet mayjustifiablyclaimto have
achieved stabilisation of one kind or another, if only to confound the
detractors andthe doubters that have longtriedtohurryYiddishalonginto
the night.
The Secular Side of the Yiddish Street
To fully realise the above, one must also comprehend that the course of
NewYorks secular Yiddishefforts is not only downhill either, eventhough
it has more often than not been moving in that direction for the past half
century. During the course of the decade under review, the Yiddish
Forward(Forverts), that celebratedits 100thanniversary in1996, acquireda
young (under 50 is young in the world of secular Yiddish involvement)
Soviet trainedandthoroughlyprofessional editor whohas transformedthe
paper into a lively and colourful weekly, has attracted a score of young
writers and co-workers and now also publishes an Israeli edition as well.
The YIVOs Uriel Weinreich Summer Program (initiated in 1968 and co-
sponsored since then with Columbia University, where the noted linguist
Uriel Weinreich himself had held the USAs first endowed chair in
Yiddish)
3
the premier Yiddish summer programme in the world (several
other Yiddish summer programmes have since opened, e.g. in Oxford,
London, Paris, Vilnius (Vilne), Kiev and Jerusalem/Tel-Aviv) nowregu-
larly provides intensive courses at all levels to 50 or more students, most of
themenrolled in masters and doctoral programmes at universities around
the country and, indeed, around the globe. The last regularly performing
Yiddish theatre, the Folksbine, founded c. 1915, has been reinvigorated by
the appointment of two very young (forty-something) directors, both
Yiddish in New York City 77
American born, native Yiddish speaking, andsuccessful stage andmusical
stage personalities in English as well. A distinguished (but moribund)
choral group has recently appointed an American born director of very
similar youth, linguistic background and professional experience to be at
its helm. An internecine orthographic war that erupted during the prior
decade quietly ended during the 1990s with a decisive defeat of the Young
Turks. The latter had actually advocated a return to an earlier convention
(after having previously erratically advocated an ultra-radical ortho-
graphic system that attracted even less support), but failed to wean away
any but a few members of that same groups associated writers from the
YIVOs Unified Yiddish Spelling (adopted in 1937).
4
Among the most
adamant defenders of the UnifiedYiddish Spelling were a cadre of young
university professors of Yiddish in New York, throughout the USA and
worldwide. Yugntruf has established its own computer bulletin board/
chat groupinYiddish, to jointhree others that are Yiddish-focusedbut that
are not, generally, conducted in Yiddish.
5
Perhaps even more unusual in
the annals of threatened immigrant languages, Yugntruf also sponsors a
writing circle, most of whose members are American born and in their
40s or younger, and a Sunday youth-group (Pripitshik) for elementary
school-aged children that have at least one Yiddish-speaking parent.
The number of Yiddish vinklekh (once or twice a month clubs, largely
EnglishspeakingandYiddishappreciating, for adults andsenior citizens)
continues to expand throughout the country (some 400 such clubs existing
at this time), with the Greater New York Metropolitan area represented
(although quite underwhelmingly so) among its constituents. A nation-
wide federation of all such vinklekh has recently come into being for the
purpose of sharing information, programmes and the convening of an
annual conference. The Greater NewYork Metropolitan Area also boasts a
small number of active zamlers (collectors) for the National Yiddish Book
Center in Massachusetts, making sure that the immigrant generations
Yiddish books are recycled (at quite elegant prices) to individual special-
ists, libraries and university collections worldwide. The million-plus
copies that the Center has amassednowcontainby far the lions share of all
Yiddish books ever published and efforts to digitalise these texts (so as to
preserve them permanently from the ravages of time) are now underway.
As of this date some 100 Yiddish books are published annually in New
York, mostly of ultra-Orthodox content, out of roughly 220230 published
annually worldwide (mostly in Israel where publishing costs are far lower
but the majority of titles are still ultra-Orthodox in nature). Nevertheless,
unfazed by the competition, secular Yiddish NewYork is looking forward
to the technical compatibility of the internet and of the Web with Yiddish
78 The Americas
fonts, on-line YiddishEnglish and EnglishYiddish dictionaries (in the
proper spelling, of course) and other such marvels of the 21st century.
All in all, although the secularist scene certainly has no reason to be self-
satisfied or smug about its prospects, it can be pardoned for smiling more
oftenthanit usedto. What a difference a decade canmake, onthe one hand,
and on the other, the more things change the more they stay the same:
neither the secular Yiddishist
6
practical prospects nor the ideological under-
pinnings to the still substantial and varied efforts of this wing of the
Yiddish world are such as to currently engage either the attention or the
adherence of the vast majority of American Jews, whether old or young,
native or foreign born.
The Flavour of Secular Yiddish Efforts
What are some concrete examples of the enterprises, acts of devotion
and sources of knowledge and inspiration that constitute the sinews of
community-at-a-distance for secular Yiddishists in New York today?
During the spring and summer of 1999, when this chapter was being
written, my log of events showed the following items (among others):
February 1999: Under the large heading Summers Yiddish Renais-
sance, the weekly English Forward (a fully independent spin-off of the
Yiddish Forverts) started of an entire page devoted to events all over the
country, three-quarters of which were in Metropolitan New York. Of the
more than two-dozen events enumerated and described, one is to be
conducted entirely in Yiddish (the YIVO/Columbia summer session), one
partiallyinYiddish(the Workmans Circle annual Mame-Loshen (mother
tongue) weekend of seminars [in English], workshops [mostly in English]
and entertainment [mostly in Yiddish]. A childrens programme will be
conducted in English and Yiddish, and the event includes outdoor activi-
ties andkosher meals. All remaining listings refer to events many explic-
itly related to Yiddish conducted entirely in English.
February 1999: The weekly quarter-page advertisement of the Work-
mans Circle (founded in 1900, its first afternoon supplementary schools
were opened in 1919, over socialist opposition that these schools would
estrange Jewish children from the international proletariat) regularly
placed in the (English) Forward is headed Where We Stand: Jewish
Children, Their Families and the Jewish People. Yiddish is mentioned in
connection with the fraternal orders I.L. Peretz supplementary schools
focus on familiarity with Jewish history, Jewish holidays, Jewish rites of
passage, andJewishculture, particularlyYiddishliterature, music andart.
Though the fundamental importance of Jewish education to the well-
Yiddish in New York City 79
being of Jews and the Jewish people is universally recognised, no claim is
made that the 12 brief sessions per week of the fraternal orders mere
handful of remaining schools (out of what were well over a hundred in the
early 1930s [Gelibter, 1935]) actually go beyond Yiddish appreciation of
one kind or another. Familiarity with does not mean speaking, reading,
writing or understanding the language by students for whom it is almost
never the mother tongue.
7
March 1999: The Friends of the Secular Yiddish Schools in America
Archival Collection appeal for support for their efforts to reconstruct the
entire history of secular Yiddish schooling in the USA. The Collection,
housed at Stanford University Libraries (Special Collections Division)
includes all pedagogic materials and publications, memorabilia, minutes
of School Board Meetings, note books, report cards and photographs
pertaining to the four major networks of secular Yiddish schools (Zionist,
Socialist, Communist and Non-political, each sponsoring its own schools,
from pre-kindergartens to post-high school courses and teachers semi-
naries), from before World War I to recent days, scattered throughout the
length and breadth of the USA and numbering at their apex in the many
hundreds. The work of the Friends is conscientiously bilingual (in
minutes, correspondence, newsletter and website), in Yiddish and in
English, while the Archival holdings themselves are, by and large, in
Yiddish.
May 1999: A cause-sensationelle was the desertion of Janet Hadda, a
well known and highly regarded Yiddish professor (at UCLA), writer,
translator and literary critic. She published her doubts about the future of
Yiddish in America (and her psychoanalytic interpretation of the unwill-
ingness of admirers of the language to accept the death thereof) in JQ
(Jewish Quarterly, one of the very best Anglo-Jewish literary and cultural
journals today). Her article was then carried in toto in Mendele, the main
Yiddish-interest e-mail bulletinboard, together withthe moderators rejec-
tion of Haddas theses, and then Haddas response to that rejection. There-
after, for many days, Mendele was practically monopolised by reader
critiques of (and far more rarely, agreements with) Haddas point of view,
methodology and data. What made the situation even more poignant was
the fact that similar noteworthy desertions had occurred in previous years
(most visibly, that of Professor Ruth Wisse, at Harvard). Several of New
Yorks most visible Mendelyaner. participated in the e-mail storm.
June 1999: The annual Manger Prize for Yiddish Literature (Israels top
prize for literary or scholarly creativity in the world of Yiddish) was
announced. Of the three prize recipients, two are professors (one, Amer-
80 The Americas
ican-born anda Yiddish activist as well as researcher) most of whose major
work is either in English or in Hebrew. Their biographies and parts of their
bibliographies were reported in the Yiddish and Anglo-Jewish press, as
well as in Mendele.
July 1999. The weekly homily on the Torah portion (parshe fun der vokh),
prepared in Yiddish over the past several years and distributed via e-mail
by a member of Yugntruf, announced that it was time for the summer
break. Its recipients (approximately 100 in all) are varied, but it tends to
appeal primarily to secular Yiddishists with a more traditional (neo-reli-
gious) inclination and meagre exposure to the traditional sources. Sign of
the times? Since the summer break this weekly e-mail newsletter initially
appearedonlyintermittentlyandcurrentlyseems tohave lapsedentirely.
August 1999: A Yiddish terminology for computer topics, initially
prepareda fewyears ago, is nowbeing updated. The e-mail bulletin-board
for Yiddish computer-matters constantly reports (almost exclusively in
English) onnewcommerciallyissuedprogrammes that canhandle Yiddish
and do so with all the letters and diacritics required by the YIVOs Unified
YiddishOrthography. The chat-groupis committedtofollowingthe stand-
ardised Unicode format so that Yiddish encrypted-diskettes or attach-
ments prepared in accord with one programme will be readable by others.
As compared to just a few years ago, there are now many Yiddish
programmes that follow such a standard format.
30 August6 September 1999: A biennial Toronto event, Ashkenaz,
attracts many NewYork area visitors. Its website describes it as follows: A
festival of New Yiddish Culture, . . . both richly traditional and thrillingly
innovative. The largest festival of its kindinthe world. . . eight extravagant
days and nights of soul-searching, performance, provocative exhibition,
celebrity, public spectacle . . . features klezmer ensembles, jazz musicians,
poets, singers, painters, sculptors, actors, dancers, storytellers. Around the
world, Yiddish culture is being reborn as artists andart-seekers of all kinds
turn toward their roots. Conspicuous by their absence: opportunities to
learn, improve and actually to use Yiddish, although many many partici-
pants could sing along when Yiddish songs were being presented.
September 1999: Yiddishvokh, a rare completely in Yiddish week for the
entire family (but with the accent on Yiddish-speaking singles and young
folks, particularly those with Yiddish-speaking children) attracts well over
100 participants to its 24th annual happening. You dont need to be able
to reador write Yiddish. Youdont evenneedto be a fluent Yiddishspeaker.
This is an all-too-rare real life Yiddish immersion situation, where you can
eat, drink, laugh, joke, twist and shout, all in Yiddish! The happening is
Yiddish in New York City 81
sympathetically reviewed in the New York Times (without any attempt at
the usual low-brow humour) as being particularly unusual, because its
participants are modern in every way, rather than ultra-Orthodox or even
necessarily religious. In the last few years, some traditional religious cere-
monies have been added to Yiddishvokhs roster of events (e.g. a Friday
night welcoming of the sabbath and Saturday morning egalitarian [i.e.
men and women sitting together and participating equally] services [in
Hebrew] with a Yiddish sermon on both occasions. Sign of the times?
September 1999: The YIVOannounces the formation of a newSeminar in
Yiddish that will meet every other Friday from noon to 2.00 pm. Such a
seminar hadbeenlackingfor manyyears duringwhichpracticallyall of the
YIVOs efforts were in English only. The speakers at the Seminar will be
scholars, doctoral candidates andpersonalities inthe fieldof Yiddishstud-
ies. It is expected that graduate students at universities throughout the
Greater New York Metropolitan Area, working on topics that require a
knowledge of Yiddish for access to primary source materials and refer-
ences, will attend. The YIVOS libraryandarchives will be openonseminar
days exclusivelyfor those participatinginthe newseminar. The YIVOitself
is nowpart of the Center for Jewish History and no tell-tale Yiddish signs
or sounds are inevidence inits newjoint-quarters. Sanitised, de-ethnicised
ethnic organisations (Fishman, 1966) are still very much a reality on the
American scene.
FebruarySeptember 1999: During this same period there were six all-
Yiddish centre-city evenings, on literary and other current topics, spon-
sored and co-sponsored by Main line Yiddish secular cultural organisa-
tions and oriented primarily to the 70+ generation (almost all of whomare
of Yiddish mother-tongue). These events typically attract audiences of
about 150 people. The largest of these is the Workmans Circle annual Pass-
over meal and pageant (previously known as the third seyder and now
designatedas a cultural seyder, to differentiate it fromthe traditional reli-
gious original), much of which consists of songs and poems in Yiddish
interspersedwith English connective remarks. Asomewhat larger number
of local evenings, convened in various boroughs outside of Manhattan,
were also organised during this same period for the same age-group and
generallyattractedaudiences that were frommuchtosomewhat smaller.
8
The Dimensions of Secular Yiddishist Identity and Behaviour
Secular Yiddishism was never a fully formed or definitively defined
ideology. In pre-World War II Eastern Europe it fostered a non-religious
(even an anti-religious) Jewish peoplehood identity (just as did secular
82 The Americas
and generally Hebrew-oriented Zionism), but one which was Yiddish
speaking. In addition, it often advocated non-immigration (whether to
Palestine or to the Americas or other centres of resettlement outside of
EasternEurope) andgenerallyadheredtoone or another left-wingpolitical
preference. Although Yiddishismwas generally secular in orientation, not
unlike most other modern co-territorial Eastern European nationalist
movements, it remained in close daily interactional proximity with the
mass of religious and traditional Jews there, all of themliving in settings in
whichlanguage, ethnicity andreligionwere the normal triangualations for
most Catholic, Uniate andEasternOrthodoxpeoples as well. Yiddishismin
pre-WorldWar II Eastern Europe didnot advocate political independence,
as had most of the regions other minority nationalist movements since the
very turn of the century. It frequently viewed some mixture of socialism
andcultural autonomy as the desiredbulwarks that wouldprotect modern
Jews against assimilation or relinguification within the local politys
majority population, even after the bonds of religion had ceased to serve
them.
If secular Yiddishism was never fully defined in pre-World War II
Eastern Europe, it was even less so in the USA, where no triangulation of
ethnicity-religion-language existed and where Jews were viewed as reli-
giously defined and increasingly became self-defined mainly (or even
only) along such lines. Judging by the statements and behaviours of the
majority of secular Yiddishist spokespersons and organisational members
and sympathisers in the New York City area today, the primary dimen-
sions of current Yiddishism in action are implemented as follows:
(1) Occasional spoken and even more rarely written or read use of the
language. Even passive understanding is sometimes also slight,
particularly so insofar as traditional Yiddish or modern learned
usage are concerned. Residential propinquity with ultra-Orthodox
speakers is almost non-existent. Among young secularists, Yiddish is
most often acquired as a second language rather than as a mother
tongue.
(2) Yiddish is most frequently a vehicle of spectator enjoyment and print
and non-print media entertainment (via klezmer troupes [ensembles,
bands, orchestras], choruses, theatre groups, festivals, musical tapes,
mulitmedia extravaganzas, comedy sessions, and translated or even
transliterated literary texts), rather than a vehicle of everyday face-to-
face interaction.
(3) Evenhome-basedYiddishinvolvement is veryoftenvia transliterated
print and modern virtual technologies (e-mail, web pages, audio- and
Yiddish in New York City 83
video-cassettes), rather than via actual face-to-face interaction.
Yiddish via secondary community (organisational and media
programmes) institutions vastly predominates over Yiddish via
primary community contacts in family and neighbourhood
interactional settings.
(4) For the most part, non-religious Jewish identity and liberal left of
centre political preferences. Lack of knowledge andlack of contact vis-
-vis religious speakers and their culture or communities is the rule.
Lack of own residential concentration is sometimes portrayed as
futuristic, but it essentially rules out forming a speech community in
the usual sociolinguistic sense.
Secular Yiddishism is often at odds with at least not linked into
such major modern Jewish-American developments as Zionist, reli-
gionist (synagogue membership) or traditionalist (holiday and dietary
observances) efforts and involvements. As a result, and because of the
concomitant disappearance of most mainstream Yiddish fluency and
daily use (outside of ultra-Orthodox circles, to which we will turn,
below), Yiddishismhas become largely peripheral and even exotic vis--
vis the mainstreamof NewYork City Jewish life. It is typified by several
unusual and less than optimal developments relative to RLS efforts
more generally: (a) a penchant for Yiddish entertainment and spectator
sports; (b) an organisational venue and organisational longevity skills
(notwithstanding many admittedly serious losses in this connection
over the years), rather thanvernacular spontaneity. However, the future
independent existence of such a former bastion of the secular Yiddish
scene as the YIVO appears to many to be endangered by its current
incorporation within a newly established, multi-organisational frame-
work favoured by its newfound major contributors (none of whom are
Yiddish speaking or Yiddish committed); and (c) a theoretical prefer-
ence for Yiddish Literary (high) Culture (theoretical because precious
little Yiddish literature in Yiddish is nowgenerally read in these circles,
even though an admiration for the literary culture continues to be
avowed in the abstract) rather than the daily realm that is the expected
functional realm of vernaculars. These three emphases are its major
underlying claims to recognition today. Few would maintain that these
claims come anywhere near to making up for the lack of daily language
use, or of demographic centres of informal speech-network concentra-
tion, or of explicit ideological self-definition, or of practical RLS
prioritisation within its own shrinking orbit.
84 The Americas
Secular Yiddishism from an RLS Perspective
In terms of the GIDS stages, most secular Yiddishist RLS efforts are
clearly at either Stage 7 (for seniors well beyond their child-bearing age) or
at Stages 3 and 2 (local and supra-local media, performance and entertain-
ment events), often utilising the most modern technologies for these
purposes. Such an allocation of functions also clearly leaves a yawning gap
in connection with daily vernacular implementation in home-family-
neighbourhood-community settings or in the intensive and prolonged
school contexts closely linked to the former. The inevitable byproduct of
the foregoing, particularly given the Yiddishist disarray insofar as prior
ideological clarification is concerned, is that every successive generation of
secularist Yiddishists must acquire Yiddish as a second language, ulti-
mately acquiring it imperfectly from a prior generation that had also
acquired it imperfectly. Clearly, the prominent spectator sport status of
Yiddishinthese circles is a direct consequence of the lackof facility vis--vis
the normal speech-community life patterns of vernaculars everywhere.
Rather than belabouring the obviously unenviable position of Yiddish
under secular auspices, it is actually rather remarkable to note what the
remaining devotion to and fondness for the language have nevertheless
accomplished. It is also remarkable that three generations after the Holo-
caust had annihilated the major reserve of its mother-tongue speakers, on
the one hand, and after the major premises of the initial secularist ideolog-
ical manifesto (most usually cultural autonomy, secular peoplehood and
socialism) have become largely inoperative in todays American environ-
ment, on the other hand, secular Yiddishismcontinues to exist at all, given
its heavy interaction with the general, non-Jewish environment of New
York. Clearly, the dangers that secular Yiddishism faces are of two kinds:
those that derive from its external close relationships with the modern
secular world at large, and those that derive fromits own internal ideolog-
ical dynamics. Both of these dangers are to some extent modifiable and, in
the present case, they are both also highly interrelated.
Is there a Future for Secular Yiddishism?
The answer to this question depends on whether stateless and residen-
tially unconcentrated languages can maintain a completely secular exis-
tence (culture and identity) of their own and, through that separateness, a
language of their own in which their culture and identity can be best
expressed? Our answer to the immediately above question must influence
our answer to the question put by the section heading. If France is nervous
about the ability of its culture and identity to withstand the onslaught of
Yiddish in New York City 85
modern, English dominated, globalisation propelled secular modernism
and consumer pop-culture, and if smaller state-forming ethnolinguistic
groups the world over are complaining bitterly about the inroads of
English dominated globalisation, secular modernism and consumer pop-
culture into their local contexts, then what hope can there be for minority
languages in contexts like those of secular Yiddishism in New York that
veritably live within the lions den per se? The power of RLS analysis is that
it both points to what not to do (essentially more of the same buckshot
efforts that are labelled cultural work [kultur-arbet] in Yiddish secular
circles) and what would make most sense at the particular juncture at
which it now finds itself. Of course, practical sense and ideological inertia
may cancel each other out at any particular time and place.
In the very midst of a generational whirlwind of return to English
speakingbut traditionveneratingOrthodoxy(andevenultra-Orthodoxy),
a return which has effectively robbed secular Yiddishism of any but the
most minimal role in the consciousness of the last two younger genera-
tions, the time may have come to admit that progressive and secular
Yiddish culture per se is also a great handicap for RLS and, therefore, a
decided minus for the future of secular Yiddishism. Partaking of, partici-
pating in, and contributing to every aspect of English dominated secular
modernism and consumer pop-culture as it does, secular Yiddishisms
high-tech, spectator-sport andminimalist nowandthen Yiddish(or, even
just about Yiddish or Yiddish appreciation, rather than intimate daily
homefamily use of Yiddish per se) lacks the separation and the insistence
on difference that are needed in order to maintain its own beloved
language as an actively functioning and intergenerationally transmissible
vehicle within its very own ranks. That is not to say that its end is near.
Asymptotic curves constantlyapproximate but never quite hit the baseline;
nevertheless the area remaining under the curve may not be enough to
enable a first language (mother tongue) generation to once more come into
being.
With modern Yiddish literature and literary creativity as its optimal
functional goal andicon, secular Yiddishismcouldbenefit immenselyfrom
implementing the Maori-derived kehanga-reo model as long as a few
natively Yiddish-speaking grandmothers and grandfathers (mostly
foreign born) are still around. Judging by the Maori successes at a juncture
when Maori was in worse straits than secularist Yiddishism is today, it
would take but a few years before such childcare language nests for
toddlers and even pre-toddlers would produce a crop of Yiddish-speaking
nursery school and pre-kindergartners, just at the time that their parents,
who would simultaneously be studying Yiddish for home-family-neigh-
86 The Americas
bourhood-community life (during evenings and weekends) would have
learnedenoughYiddishas a SecondLanguage tomore fullyimplement the
language themselves at home. Such a two-front approach, involving
grandparents, parents still of child-bearing age and newly-borns, would
link Stages 7 and 6 and point the way to Stage 5 as the next step to take
(assuming4 was out of the question, due toresource limitations andcontin-
uingideological oppositiontoweakeningthe public schools). The numbers
involvedinsucha stage-linkingventure wouldinitiallybe small, but eventhe
additionof adozenYiddishmother tonguetots livinginathree-generational
context would be a revolutionary accomplishment for secular Yiddishism.
At the ideological level, more of a plus (additive) rather than an
instead of (subtractive) approach is clearly needed. Secular Yiddishism
needs to be recast fromits original nationalism, anti-clericalism, socialism
plus literature model to a model that stresses Jewish tradition-friendly
Yiddish secularism or Judaism plus as an add-on modification to any
other model of traditional Jewishlife. Most religious Jews do not aspire to a
Jewish secularism of their own. Yiddishists might proudly claimthat as a
goal (via Yiddish theatre, choruses, media, scouting and camping, etc.) in
addition to the usual range and variety of traditional observances that
define traditional American Jewish communities. Cast in this light, Yid-
dish secularismplus Jewish traditions might be viewed as an enrichment,
whereas secular Yiddishism is nowoften seen as an impoverishment of a
thousand-year-old Great Tradition. Modern Israeli Hebrew, of course,
could also possibly fill this same role (as the language of a separate Jewish
secular life in the USA) for some, but Hebrew is largely identified in the
USA with Israeli anti-clerical yordim (those who have left the State of
Israel) or with Israel per se. Fifty years of experience has proven that most
traditional American Jews do not identify with Hebrewas a vernacular for
themselves however much they admire and respect it whereas Yiddish
was at least until recently the vernacular of their very own parents and/or
grandparents as muchas theyconsider it lackinginchic, power or moder-
nity and, therefore, not for them. Once again we find the pattern which
admires but does not implement what is viewed as the high language
from the Holy Land, and simultaneously rejects the possibility of re-
adopting the personal-memories-suffused folk-language of ones one
posterity, even though some still have a passive command of it.
The folk language (still referred to as a dialect or zhargon by some in
the younger generation) might have more personal relevance, particularly
if viewed as an extra plus in connection with the secular lives that most of
them also lead. The availability of Hebrew is least attractive to those for
whom RLS vis--vis Yiddish is on the agenda. A shift from secular
Yiddish in New York City 87
YiddishismtoYiddishsecularismplus might provide entre intothe larger
world of moderately traditional young folks that now stand aside from
Yiddish because for them it is too frum (too religious, i.e. ultra-Orthodox
in their personal experience), whereas once upon a time, for their grand-
parents, it was often associated with being too secular, left-wing or even
atheistic.
Aconscientious shift from(a) secular Yiddishismto Yiddishsecularism
plus, and (b) from Yiddish appreciation to active Yiddish use via
emphasising the first language acquisition locale of home-family-neigh-
bourhood-community functioning, is obviously not for everyone. It may
not appeal to many secular Yiddishists at first. But it is a beginning to the
search for an answer to the dilemmas of being neither fully alive nor fully
dead. These are the dilemmas that typify Yiddish in secularist circles in
NewYork as the 21st century opens. There are more young people who are
professionally involved with Yiddish today than there were a generation
ago (professors andteachers, translators andmusicians, groupleaders and
entertainers, researchers and students), and there is a much more frequent
acknowledgment of regret among many others that ones parents did not
pass the language along to them (or that they were not interested as chil-
dren in acquiring it). But it is not at all clear what can be done about these
regrets, who should do it (e.g. who should try to drawthose hundred or so
college student studying Yiddish in NewYork at any one time into secular
Yiddish society?), and, above all, how can a young secularist couple that
decides to start a family and raise their children in Yiddish connect with a
corresponding speech community out there? Unanswered basic ques-
tions such as these much more than any lack of writers, readers or even
speakers reveal the great uncertainty and even the fundamental dangers
that are ahead for secular Yiddishism in the city that is still its unofficial
New World capital.
The Current Ultra-Orthodox Scene of Yiddish in New York
The ultra-Orthodox Yiddish scene in New York is almost the diametric
opposite of the secularist scene that we have just reviewed. However, in
additiontoeachscene havingstrengths exactlywhere the other has weak-
nesses, they also interact with each other hardly at all and are, in fact, two
soliloquies; eachof themspeaks but hears not the other. WhenJanet Hadda
and other heretofore Yiddish speaking and Yiddish involved secular intel-
lectuals decide to leave the fold, they often reveal that they know that an
ultra-Orthodox Yiddish world exists and flourishes. On the other hand,
they do not analyse its dynamics nor seek to understand the lessons to be
88 The Americas
learned from that world. All that secular Yiddishists see in the ultra-
Orthodox is neanderthal-like religious fanaticismthat does not create (nor
even knowabout, much less read) world-class Yiddish literature. Nor does
it even aspire to modern concepts of social justice and egalitarian gender
roles. These fanatics and their children may speak Yiddish at home, at
work, at the house of worship and in school, but their Yiddish is replete
with anglicisms and Germanisms and follows medieval orthographic
conventions. Thus, secular Yiddishists curiouslyreject that whichlives and
is growing while they cleave to that which is admittedly wilting before
their very eyes and is patently beyond their ability to revernacularise.
Again we see a pattern which had typified all of the 20th century (first
mentioned by Nathan Birnbaum at the very beginning of the 20th century
[1906]) among huge blocks of Westernised Jews: not accepting the
language and culture which lives and breaths, on the one hand, and not
implementing the preferred but totally ethereal language and culture,
which has little chance of being put into active use in their own midst.
German, Polish and Russian Zionists, like the mainstream of American
Jewry and even the secular Yiddishists in New York today were/are all
trapped in a sociolinguistic cycle of rejecting that language which exists
within reach and not implementing that language which they purport(ed)
to prefer. The ultra-Orthodox sector alone seems to have implemented
complementary (and, therefore, non-conflictual) functions for all of the
languages to which they are exposed.
Although participants in the Mendele e-mail bulletin board discussion
of Janet Haddas resignation from the secular Yiddishist struggle almost
unanimously rejected her point of view, they differed with respect to her
dismissal of the ultra-Orthodox users of the language. Most agreed with
her, however, that the ultra-Orthodox disinterest in the best of modern
Yiddish literature and in modern secular progressive movements made
themunworthy partners on behalf of the Yiddish cause. However, only a
minority of mendeliyaner took sharp exception to the chorus of anti-
Haredi views that nevertheless unite Wisse, Hadda and most of their
sharpest critics.
I can only express my total dismay that a self-professed academic can
call a language spoken by at least 200,000 Americans (1990 US Census
figures, not emotional dismay, prejudice, politics) dead. Facing the
fact that Yiddish is spoken by an ever-growing, extremely traditional
Jewish population (haredim) is sound scholarship. Systematically
dismissing this group simply because one does not like haredimis not
sound scholarship and borders on a blatant misrepresentation of
Yiddish in New York City 89
reality, a crime for academics who are committedto educating others.
For those academics who believe that haredim are not interested in
Yiddishliterature, I suggest they start reading Haredi Yiddishpublica-
tions where they will find pious poetry and various forms of Yiddish
narrative. (xi/19/98, Bruce Mitchell)
Another defender of the ultra-Orthodox is also concerned about their
literary reputation (and in the process of so doing, confirms the secular
style of Yiddish as a sometime thing):
Regardless of your personal opinion of these people . . . they are
linguistically living what most of us only discuss on the internet or do
as a hobby or nostalgic whim. If Yiddish is to survive into the 21st
century, it is people like the readers of this paper [Der yid, the major
ultra-Orthodox weekly in New York, see below] and many other
papers like it that quietly publish on a regular basis, who will keep it
alive . . . Yiddish . . . is their mediumof communication, not a cause or a
subject for study. However, by virtue of the fact that it has servedfor so
long as the language of the Jewish people and as a medium for Torah
[study of holy texts] they [also] consider it a holy language (according
toone well knowntorahpersonality. . . ). (xi/10/98, Hershl Goodman)
Suffice it to say, that the secular Yiddishist and the ultra-Orthodox Yiddish
worlds are both intellectually and physically distant from each other, not
knowledgeable about each other and in no way helpful to one other. To
make matters worse, the latter has overtaken the former as the major force
on behalf of the continuity of the language and that rankles the one and
delights the other, thereby further distancing them from one another, not
only intellectually, but emotionally as well.
Some Ultra-Orthodox Facts of Life
The best estimates (at which several investigators have independently
arrived) put the number of ultra-Orthodox speakers of Yiddish in the USA
at about 300,000. Almost all of the foregoing live in the Greater New York
Metropolitan Area or in the counties immediately adjacent to it. As such,
they are at least a hundred fold more numerous than are the secular
Yiddishists, on the one hand, and they are daily speakers most of them
young and at or before child-bearing age, on the other hand. Their rate of
demographic growth is extremely high and, as the total Jewish population
in the USA (and in the Greater New York Metropolitan Area as well)
steadily grows older and decreases in numbers, due both to age and to
intermarriage, the ultra-Orthodox will become proportionately ever more
90 The Americas
prominent among its numbers. Vis--vis all other Yiddish speakers in the
USA and in the GNYMA (including the unorganised and non-ideological
middle range speakers, situated between the Ultra-Orthodox and the
secular Yiddishists), the former may already (or soon will) constitute the
majority. They maintain over 1200 synagogues and smaller houses of
worship and conduct a constantly growing number of Yiddish-using all-
day-schools (108 in the early 1980s and approximately 150 today, i.e.
roughly a third of the total number of Jewish all-day-schools in the USA
today). They live in compact neighbourhoods where the bulk of their total
vernacular activity is in Yiddish. The magnitude of their book and period-
ical production is hard to pin-point, since it does not reach or concern the
secularist record keepers:
WhenI inquiredat the National YiddishBookCenter andYIVOnot too
long ago, I was told that they didnt know of anyone attempting to
comprehensively collect and preserve that type of Yiddish material.
That type . . . consists of many original books [as well as a large
number of republished books that first appeared in ultra-Orthodox
strongholds in Eastern Europe-JAF], poetry, journals and a myriad of
audio tapes . . . , almost all of it being put out by small, independent
publishers in Brooklyn and other centers . . . far surpass[ing] in output
and consumption anything produced by the secular camp. (xi/10/98
Hershl Goodman; see also the comments by Aaron Nadler on the same
date re new scholarly genres in Ultra-Orthodox literary production).
Interestingly enough, it is only at the YIVO, the erstwhile intellectual head-
quarters of secular Yiddishism, that young researchers from the two
worlds of Yiddish see each other andspendmany hours of many days vir-
tually together. In the YIVOs new and quite lavish reading room, these
two worlds sit separately, reading quite different books and periodicals,
and interacting with one another little if at all. The members of one world
converse quietly, in English, and are fully familiar with the etiquette of the
library and archives culture. The others, in ankle-length kaftans and with
earlocks swaying, converse less quietly, in Yiddish, and are intermittently
scolded by the librarians (in English) for their infractions of that culture.
(Items shouldbe returnedto the shelves inexactly the same locations from
which they were removed! Please return the material you have borrowed
before requesting anything further!) Daily scenes of this kind tell us more
about the secular and the ultra-Orthodox worlds of Yiddish in New York
than either group of researchers may realise. Once again, Eastern Euro-
peans are being civilised by Westernised Jews. Once again, Yiddish is
associated with a less polished but a more intergenerationally continuous
Yiddish in New York City 91
tradition of language-in-culture and a tradition that is becoming increas-
ingly literate in the process of contact.
Ultra-Orthodox Yiddish in Print: Insight into the Languages
Communal Role
If substantiated (and I fully expect that it will be), the above-mentioned
claims of GoodmanandNadler wouldconstitute a particularlycruel irony,
since the literaryuses of Yiddishare sostronglyentrenchedinthe secularist
claim to fame and of such minor significance in the total ultra-Orthodox
life-style. The literary uses of a language must inevitably reflect or comple-
ment the roles assigned to it in the total linguistic space, and even in the
total cultural space, of its speech community. Among the ultra-Orthodox,
Yiddish in print is still very often an aide-memoire, an auxiliary translation
and commentary accompanying classical or rabbinic texts that simulta-
neously appear in Hebrew/Judeo-Aramaic. Accordingly, a special pre-
holiday sale of a new edition of the Book of Psalms is advertised as the
Book of Psalms that translates for you as you recite them, the translation
being under every word, in a very clear Yiddish that is interspersed with
and connects the verses themselves and provides you with a clear under-
standing of each and every prayer (Translated from Dos yidishe vort, ix/
1999, p. 26).
The major ultra-Orthodox Yiddish weekly, Der Yid (established in
1950) constantly reveals another facet of Yiddish in the community,
namely, its role as the language of popular adult education. Advice for a
Good Life (in two volumes) is advertised as written in easy and very read-
able Yiddish, beautifully laid out and a pleasure to look at, divided into
topics and paragraphs, and brought to you with the approval of the most
famous and dependable rabbis. Useful in every Jewish home! With the
help of God, all will benefit fromit! (Translated fromDer Yid. 15 October
1999, p. 49.)
Finally, also relevant to the often overlooked role of Yiddish in print
among the ultra-Orthodox, it should be mentioned that every issue of Der
Yid contains a 23 page section entitled For the entire family. This section
includes Judaism-centred games, riddles, jokes, puzzles (including cross-
word puzzles) as well as a separate sub-section For younger children.
Each issue closes with a reminder that the names of children who send in
correct answers will be published and will be entered in a give-away of
worthwhile books. (The Forverts toointroduced(1999) a veryattractive but
generally shorter and quite different monthly section of this kind.) There is
alsoa publishinghouse entirelydevotedtoYiddish(includingYiddishand
92 The Americas
Hebrew) textbooks for ultra-Orthodox schools. Hundreds of such texts
almost entirely devoted to the Judaica curriculum have now been
published, given that all of the schools cover grades k through 8 and, in
addition, there are also a smaller number of Yiddish-mediumhigh schools
for Grades 9 through 12. In addition, individual educators are constantly
preparing andself-publishing story books for children (andfor adults too),
as well as story-tapes, song-tapes and Judaica review-tapes. Clearly, in
parts of the ultra-Orthodox world there is a vibrant child market for suit-
able Yiddishmaterial, something the secular Yiddishworldhas not hadfor
more than50 years. Finally, the circulations andnumbers of pages per issue
of the two major weeklies (the secular one and the ultra-Orthodox one) are
also similarly discrepant. Der Yid has a certified circulation of over 50,000
and its issues are normally 72 tabloid-sized pages long, always accompa-
nied by a supplement of another 4044 pages. In comparison, the Forverts
usual 24 pages tabloid sized [but two-coloured] pages are subscribed to by
under 10,000.
9
The Nature of Ultra-Orthodox Yiddish
Perhaps a few words should be said here about the quality of Ultra-
Orthodox Yiddish. This quality (often negatively commented upon in the
secular Yiddishpress) varies fromverypoor toverygood, the former being
noted for its high rate of quite unnecessary English borrowings (lexically
and even grammatically) and the latter, by its infusion of learned
Hebraisms. As is the case throughout the world, different varieties of the
same language have come totypifyspeechnetworks that interact little with
one another. This was true of Christian and non-Christian Latin in ancient
Rome. But it was also true as late as mid-20th century with respect to the
Arabic of Baghdadi Jews, Moslems and Christians (Blanc 1964), on the one
hand, as well as of Bengali (Bengladeshi) among Moslems and Hindus in
what was then East Pakistan (Dil, 1991), on the other hand. It would be a
comparatively easy task to demonstrate that it is also true of secular and
ultra-Orthodox Yiddish in New York. In matters orthographic the differ-
ence is even bolder, the YIVOs unified Yiddish spelling having only the
slimmest of slim footholds in the ultra-Orthodox world as a whole and
particularly so, in New York.
Nevertheless, the distances between all of the ultra-Orthodox and secu-
larist varieties (each group having more than one) are rather minor andthe
two communities can fully understand one another rather easily, whether
in face-to-face or in written functions. Probably the greatest difference lies
in the fact that the secularists have adopted (and probably even overstress,
Yiddish in New York City 93
to compensate for their rather meagre daily use of the language) the
modern Eastern European normifying tradition and recognise (or give lip
service to) both norm-setting individuals and norm-setting institutions for
this very purpose. Both normifying individuals and institutions for
Yiddish are still lacking in the ultra-Orthodox world, probably because
their most vital and prestigious publication and writing is in Hebrew/
Judeo-Aramaic. For the language of some half million or more ultra-
Orthodox daily speakers of Yiddish (combing their numbers in the USA,
Israel, Montreal and Antwerp) to be declared deficient by a fewthousand
secular devotees, most of whom rarely utilise the language that they love
so muchandthat frequently cannot do so correctly themselves, is not only
another instance of the Birnbaumian paradox referred to several times
above, but a total misplacement of secular Yiddishist emphases.
Yiddish Advocacy in Ultra-Orthodox New York
Two types of Yiddish advocacy are present in ultra-Orthodox New
York. The first is oriented toward the more interactive and out-reach
involved segments of the Khasidic community itself. These segments have
slowly lost Yiddish as their intergenerational vernacular and in this way
they have been influenced by virtue of their more frequent contacts and
interactions by the very Jews whomthey themselves have sought to lead
toward ultra-Orthodoxy. Foremost among the interactionist ultra-
Orthodox are the Lubavitcher, whose late (and perhaps last) Rabbi invari-
ably preached, lectured, wrote and broadcast worldwide in impeccable
and very learned Yiddish, even to audiences to whom his every word had
to be translated. Since the Rebbes demise (in 1994), no substitute speaker
of Yiddish of the very highest authority has made an appearance in
Lubavitsher circles. This mayserve tofurther erode Yiddishthere, notwith-
standing the semi-official role that the weekly Algemeyner Zhurnal plays in
this community. Some notable resistance to any such erosion has also
appeared. Thus, in the February 1999 issue of their publication for women,
Nshei Chabad Newsletter, Rabbi Reuven Wittkes writes (under the heading
The Case for Total Immersion:
I dont think there is any doubt inour minds that Yiddishis andshould
be an important part of our childrens lives and their education. It is
also apparent from the various Sichos [Talks] and Igros [Letters] of the
Rebbe that the children should indeed be learning Yiddish. It is not the
aimof this article to debate this issue, as it has been discussed at length
many times in this and other fine publications. Within the context of
this article, the need for Yiddish is accepted as a given. . . . But that
94 The Americas
doesnt mean it comes easy. [However,] . . . I have found, after nearly
two decades of teaching young children, that when Yiddish is the only
language theyhear inclass, it quicklybecomes familiar enoughtothem
and [thereafter] learning in Yiddish is as simple for themas learning in
English . . . [So] lets give them . . . the language that united the Jews
through the generations, the language of the Rebbeyim [khasidic
rabbis], the language of Moshiach [the Messiah]. (p. 59)
The above article was followed-up a month later by congratulatory letters
from parents and students, including a call for more attention to Yiddish
grammar and syntax, not only for the sake of the pupils but for that of the
teachers themselves. Inaddition, one letter calledfor the constant (inschool
andout of school) use of Yiddish among the pupils themselves, rather than
merely its use between them and their teachers. It was claimed that only
such constant use would be fully in accord with the late Rebbes wishes.
Such intra-Khasidic advocacy (as well as similar advocacy of Yiddish
within ultra-Orthodox but non-Khasidic circles) is probably both a good
sign and a bad sign. On the one hand, the struggle for Yiddishising the
younger ultra-Orthodox generation is still ongoing and new and better
efforts in that connection are underway. On the other hand, this implies
that Yiddish has already generally been lost among many of the younger
members of the most interactive ultra-Orthodox groups, where the
parental and, particularly, the grandparental generations are still thor-
oughly Yiddish speaking.
In addition to the foregoing, Yiddish advocacy is also underway within
ultra-Orthodox circles that are still fully Yiddish speaking inter-
generationally but whose insistence or requirements in that connection
sometimes seem to be wavering. Thus, a letter to the editor of Der Yid (13
August 1999, p. 6) complains about the increase in English advertising
carried by the weekly:
Now I ask you: Is that proper? What would our late rebbe, of blessed
memory, sayabout suchgoings oninthe verypaper that he established
withsomuchof his ownsweat andeffort, . . . sothat his followers could
find a quiet corner in which to slake their thirst for knowledge
without looking at journals and newspapers that are chock full of
material that can harmones own soul andthe souls of ones posterity?
This matter of Yiddish pertains to one of the three main separators
between Jews and gentiles: names, clothing and language. (translated
from Yiddish: JAF)
Yiddish in New York City 95
In a prior issue (10 April 1992), the wife of a rebbe warned and motivated
the weeklys female readership as follow:
All students carry a great responsibility to take an uncompromising
position not to drown in the American sea. Let us turn back and recon-
nect with the lively and holy generations before us. Through Yiddish,
the mother tongue, each Jew will feel the spark of his or her mothers
heart and the fire of sanctity will light up and the presence of God will
be felt in all of our homes. (p. 42, translated from Yiddish: JAF)
Summary: The Two Worlds of Yiddish in New York
The world including most of the Jewish world has long regarded
Yiddish as dead or dying, because it could not imagine (and didnt really
desire or aspire to) the continuity of a separate Yiddish culture and iden-
tity into and throughout the modern period. Many American Jews not
only adopted this dubious stance vis--vis themselves, and joined the
Yiddish is dead or dying chorus, but became its first and chief
heralders. Furthermore, we have noted that within the very two-in-one
worlds of Yiddish itself the doubt as to the survival (nay, the very exis-
tence) of the other wing has become paramount. Neither wing has any
sympathy nor sense of fraternity with the other. Each freely and fully
foregoes the assests that the other could bring to a combined RLS
struggle. Perhaps this is how other weakened groups fractionate. Each
sees in the other only the most complete denial and rejection of its own
existence and raison detre.
The two sundered worlds of Yiddish in New York can also be found,
equally sundered, in Israel, in Antwerp, in Melbourne, in Montreal, i.e.
wherever larger ultra-Orthodox and secular Yiddish speakers live side by
side as non-interacting soliloquies. The secularists are concerned with
Yiddish theatre, Yiddish song concerts, Yiddish literary awards and
Yiddish lectures on intellectual topics and on current affairs. The ultra-
Orthodox are concerned with Yiddish both as an auxiliary vehicle of co-
sanctity as well as as the intra-communal vehicle of all vernacular pursuits,
among adults and children alike. The old-world-like ultra-Orthodox, with
their beards, side curls and black kaftans for men and with their kerchiefs,
wigs and long black stockings for women, are all American born by now,
but they are carefully more in America than of it. Their main concern is
upholding the very compartmentalisation of Jewish vs non-Jewish tradi-
tions, customs and lifestyles into which the secularists seek to integrate in
all but language.
96 The Americas
Ultra-Orthodox Yiddish is firmly anchored in the home-family-neigh-
bourhood and community nexus (Stage 6), with strong links to stages 5
and/or 4a (i.e. their very own schools that are fully under community
auspices and control) and even reaching out to many businesses fully
staffed by its own members (Stage 3). Secularist Yiddish has no such
concentration at the real home-family-neighbourhood-community level. It
uses the Internet for virtual community purposes andmeets face-to-face for
large group events, both of which are by and large unrelated to
intergenerational mother-tongue transition. The ultra-Orthodox are
growing by leaps andbounds, but are confronting andresisting some attri-
tion at the edges, where its outreach activity has brought it into intimate
interaction with the less observant or completely non-observant American
Jewish community as a whole, or where its economic pursuits have led it
into interdependence with the mainstreamof American life. On the whole
however, the Ultra-Orthodox are maintaining (and even improving) their
intergenerational mother-tongue continuity by expanding their institu-
tional community feedback opportunities via schools, summer camps,
family vacation settlements, neighbourhood study groups for adults and
their own market for books, periodicals, tapes, newspapers and even local
theatre performances.
In contrast with their status 10 years ago, the ultra-Orthodox would
seemto be stronger nowthen they were then, although by no means totally
problem-free, even in their own eyes, particularly with respect to the
Stage 6 level and the subsequent institutional stages that must be carefully
coordinated with and related to it. It is particularly in the work-sphere
(Stage 3) that the ultra-Orthodox need to acquire additional economic
opportunities via Yiddish for the younger generations down the pike. The
secularists are weaker than they were a decade ago. They have lost a
number of their most central and beloved institutions at Stages 2 and 3 and
are about to lose the last octogenarians who are a direct link to the vibrant,
natively Yiddish-speaking Eastern European culture that produced them
originally. As their numbers decrease, they increasingly become a groupof
American born Yiddish as a second language hobbyists for whom
intergenerational mother tongue continuity is, generally speaking, neither
intended nor possible. Its future in these networks is the same as the future
of intergenerational hobby networks with respect to their special interests
in a variety of pastimes or artistic and intellectual pursuits. From time to
time, Secular Yiddishism may yet create some new great works of culture
or scholarship, but, generally speaking, it revisits the past and, above all, it
is not a real speechcommunity, i.e. one that canfoster the intergenerational
childhoodlanguage acquisitionandenculturationof its ownoffspring. The
Yiddish in New York City 97
children and grandchildren of stamp collectors and history buffs do not
forma community of interest, muchless a speechcommunity. They are not
expected to become (nor do they by and large become) stamp collectors or
history buffs themselves.
Conclusions
There is a centurys worth of upheaval, heartache and renewal in the
story of Yiddish in New York. A century ago it seemed that progress
would clearly triumph and that the secularist dream of a thoroughly
modernised and socially progressive Jewish people would fully displace
the strictures and orthodoxies of the past. Both the Holocaust and the
collapse of the left, on the one hand, and the incorporative triumph of the
culturally and economically more open and advanced capitalist democra-
cies, on the other hand, have resulted in the collapse of the progressive
dream and a turn toward a more traditional and family oriented Judaism
for many younger Jews. Some have evenembracedultra-Orthodoxy again,
based on the implicit faith of their grandparents and the scrupulous imple-
mentation of Gods commandments. This same turn (or turn-over) has
characterised large populations the world over. The certainties of funda-
mentalismhave provedto be muchmore comfortingthanthe uncertainties
of modernism. Fundamentalism erects barriers against the confusing and
morally conflicted world of compromise, relativism, self-gratification and
innovation. Accordingly, its barriers may also provide a much clearer
boundary maintenance pattern for RLS than modernisation ever does or
could do, particularly in the face of American controlled globalisation,
consumerism and mass pop-culture.
10
The triumph of the ultra-Orthodox
ugly duckling is a Cinderella story seemingly made to order for RLS
efforts on behalf of threatened languages.
Threatened languages can survive their contact with the modern world
only to the extent that they can maintain strong communities and viable
cultural boundaries within the lions cave itself. If they oppose religious
fundamentalism, as they mostly do, then it is up to them to devise equally
sheltering boundaries of a secular nature. Where political boundaries are
impossible or no longer serve as bulwarks against modern Big Brothers
language-in-culture, then threatened languages must answer the dilemma
of what other bulwarks they can propose, erect and successfully defend.
Language itself is not enough of a bulwark to defend threatened cultures
andidentities. Certainlywhenthe underlyingcultures are practicallyiden-
tical, the Big Brothers language must displace the smaller one. Language
cannot be the ultimate defence for threatened cultures and identities,
98 The Americas
because threatened languages themselves need defences: the distinctive
values, cultural practices and beliefs (including uncompromising identity-
beliefs) that make a distinctive language necessary and even possible.
Yiddish is just like all other threatened languages in this connection
particularly like all other immigrant languages without any political
boundaries at all (or anywhere) to fall back on indeed, just like all the
others but even more so.
Notes
1. Lout, oaf, nincompoop.
2. Agentile employed to performvarious tasks, such as lighting the stove or turn-
ing the electricity on/off, on Saturdays, when such activities are culturally
defined as work and, therefore, prohibited by Orthodox strictures.
3. Earlier college level Yiddish courses hadbeen given by A.A. Roback (Harvard),
Nathan Suskind (CCNY and Yeshiva College), Judah Yaffe (at a World War II
precursor of the New School for Social Research) and Max Weinreich (CCNY).
Uriel Weinreichs programme at Columbia Universitys Department of Lin-
guistics was the first (and remained the only one in the USA) to encompass a
graduate course of Yiddishspecialisationculminatinginthe awardof the PhD.
4. It is this orthographic conventionthat is nowrecognisedbyalmost all of the ma-
jor secular literary journals and academic teaching centres throughout the
world. This is a rare instance of largely successful, even if long delayed, corpus
planning for a minority language that is totally without any official (state-re-
lated) enforcement apparatus.
5. At this writing the advent of Yiddish character e-mail and website capability
that is compliant with all Unicode requirements is being eagerly awaited and
frequentlydiscussedbyane-mail chat groupthat focuses onthe technical prob-
lems of Yiddish and cyberspace activity.
6. The termYiddishist has semantically metamorphosedduring the past quarter
century. From its original meaning of denoting those for whom Yiddish con-
cerns constituted the very core of their Jewish identity, it has now come to
denote anyone who is not ultra-Orthodox who is judged to have either full flu-
ency or even just an active interest in Yiddish. The increasing rarity of the
language outside of ultra-Orthodox circles doubtlessly underlies the semantic
shift andthe semantic blurring that has occurred. I will continue to use the term
here as close to its original meaning as I can.
7. The newintroductorytextbook, issuedfor these schools in1999, puts this some-
what more poetically: The goal for this curriculum is to make available to all,
the beauty, the melody, the rhythmof Yiddish and the educational support for
Yiddish holiday lore and pageantry Borodulin, 1999: 1).
8. A few additional 1999 New York events that were sponsored by the secularist
Yiddishsector canbe foundinthe Congress for JewishCultures bilingual In the
World of Yiddish/In der velt fun yidish, planned as an annual publication. On the
other hand, myownrecords itemise manymore NewYorkevents thandoes the
Congress publication, primarily because the latter attempts to give a positive
worldwide picture, whereas my focus is entirely on the Greater NewYork City
Metropolitan Area and does not attempt to screen out problematic material.
Yiddish in New York City 99
9. Another fervently Orthodox weekly, Der Algemeyner Zhurnal, is a full-sized
(rather than tabloid sized) paper. It regularly has a 46 page English supple-
ment and lists a registered circulation of c. 60,000. In addition, there are three
other Ultra-Orthodox weeklies today, all of them entirely in Yiddish: Der Blat,
Di Tsaytung unDi Vokh. Their circulations are uncertifiedbut minimal estimates
would put the total weekly ultra-Orthodox circulation at between 150,000 and
200,000.
10. By the time advanced English language skills may become important to many
members of the male ultra-Orthodoxcommunity, Englishcanbe nomore thana
third language for them (after Yiddish and classical/rabbinic Hebrew). A Yid-
dish display ad in the Algemeyner Zhurnal of vi/25/99 [p. 3] reads: A Good
Opportunity for Observant Young Men: Learn How to Read, Write and Speak
English. This will help you earn a living, and also in daily life, e.g., in filling out
important applications. You will also be able to become familiar with computer
science, which has become an important key to the business world. Dont miss
this rare chance! It is fromjust such ultra-Orthodox circles that the heavily Yid-
dish impacted variety of English often called Yeshivish initially stems.
Although some Yiddish borrowings in English date back much earlier,
Yeshivish has by now attained considerable currency among both Jews and
non-Jews far removed from contact with the Ultra-Orthodox (Gold, 1981,
Fishman, 1985). The precise reachof Yeshivishandother Yiddishborrowings in
English is now being studied (Benor, MS).
References
Benor, S. (MS) What is Yeshivish, Who Knows it and How Much?
Birnbaum, N. (1906) Die todte Sprache; die lebende Nichtsprache. Neue Zeitung.
1(2) 14 September.
Blanc, H. (1964) Communal Dialects in Baghdad. Cambridge: Harvard University
Press.
Borodulin, N. (1999) Yidish oyf a gants yor/Yiddish Year Round. New York: Work-
mans Circle.
Dil, A. (1991) Two Traditions of the Bengali Language. Cambridge: Islamic Academy.
Gelibter, F. (ed.) (1935) Di arbeter-ring shuln: ufkum un antviklung. In his Shul
almanakh: di yidishe moderne shul af der velt. Philadelphia: Central Committee of
the Workmans Circle Schools.
Gold, D. (1981) Jewish English. In C.A. Ferguson and S.B. Heath (eds) Language in
the USA. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Halpern, M-L. (1919) Undzer gortn. In nyu-york. New York: Vinkl .
Harshav, B. (1990) The Meaningof Yiddish. Berkeley: Universityof California Press.
Fishman, J.A. (1965) YiddishinAmerica. International Journal of American Linguistics
31(2) (entire issue). (Also published separately by Mouton, The Hague).
Fishman, J.A. (1966) Language Loyalty in the United States. The Hague: Mouton.
Fishman, J.A. (1981) Never Say Die! AThousand Years of Yiddish in Jewish Life and Let-
ters. The Hague: Mouton.
Fishman, J.A. (1985) The Rise and Fall of the Ethnic Revival. Berlin: Mouton.
Fishman, J.A. (1985) Readings in the Sociology of Jewish Languages. Leiden: E.J. Brill.
Fishman, J.A. (1989) Yiddish: Returning to Life. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
100 The Americas
Chapter 5
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec
1
R.Y. BOURHIS
2
Inhis nowclassic framework, Joshua FishmanportrayedFrenchQuebec as
one of the three success stories of Reversing Language Shift (Fishman,
1991). French Quebec has emerged as a symbolic case for the Xish
language minorities of the world, for it shows that sustained language
planning can reverse language shift even relative to the most powerful
language of this millennium: English. The main goal of this chapter is to
provide an analytic update of the Quebec sociolinguistic situation
following the adoption of language laws designed to increase the status of
French relative to English in the Province. Combining both the group
vitality analysis and the reversing language shift framework, the first part
of the chapter will analyse the circumstances which led successive Quebec
governments to adopt language laws in favour of French during the last
three decades. The second part of the chapter will provide an update of the
FrenchEnglish demolinguistic situation following 20 years of RLS efforts
for the French language in Quebec. As inhabitants of a province of Canada
or as members of a newly proclaimed independent state, the chapter will
conclude with thoughts on how RLS policies have changed the vitality
position of Francophones and Anglophones in the only French majority
jurisdiction in North America.
Group Vitality and Language Shift and Quebec
In this section we propose that the ethnolinguistic vitality framework
(Bourhis, 1979) can be seen as a necessary tool of analysis complementing
the reversing language shift model proposed by Fishman (1991). We will
seek to illustrate this point by providing an analysis of the vitality position
of the Francophone and Anglophone communities of Quebec in the years
leading up to the adoption of the Charter of the French language in 1977
(Bourhis, 1984a).
Given the rise of ethnic revival movements across the world in the latter
part of the 20th century (Fishman, 1999), the group vitality framework was
developedto analyse more systematically the relative sociostructural posi-
101
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec
The Americas
tion of language groups in contact. As pointed out by Harwood et al.,
(1994):
It became clear that processes such as language shift, language atti-
tudes, interethnic communication and ethnic conflict could no longer
be studiedina sociostructural vacuum. . . As pointedout by Johnsonet
al., 1983, the concept of vitality emergedout of the critical needfor situ-
atingthe sociolinguistic andsocial psychological processes underlying
interethnic behaviour within their proper sociostructural contexts. (p.
172)
As it happens, the group vitality framework was originally conceivedwith
the Quebec context in mind at a time when sociolinguistic research was
being conducted to construct different components of Quebecs Charter of
the French language (Giles et al., 1977). Thus the groupvitality concept was
first proposed as an analytic tool designed to better assess the demo-
graphic, institutional support and prestige position of Quebecs French
language majority relative to the English speaking elite of the day . The
vitality of an ethnolinguistic group was defined as that which makes a
grouplikelyto behave as a distinctive andcollective entitywithinthe inter-
group setting (Giles et al. 1977: 308). The more vitality an ethnolinguistic
group was assessed to have, the more likely that it was expected to survive
collectively as a distinctive linguistic community within its bilingual or
multilingual context. Conversely, ethnolinguistic groups that were
assessedtohave little vitalitywouldbe expectedtoeventuallydisappear as
distinctive linguistic communities within the intergroupsetting. Figure 5.1
shows the three broad dimensions of structural variables most likely to
influence the vitality of ethnolinguistic groups: these are namely, demo-
graphic, institutional support and status factors. The elements comprising
these factors will be briefly presented as they pertain to the sociostructural
position of the Francophone and Anglophone communities during the
decade leading up to the adoption of Bill 101.
Demographic variables are those related to the number of members
comprising the ethnolinguistic group and their distribution throughout a
particular urban, regional or national territory. Distribution factors refer to
the numeric concentration of group members in various parts of the terri-
tory, their proportion relative to outgroup members, and whether or not
the group still occupies its traditional or national territory. Number
factors refer to the communitys absolute group numbers, their birth rate,
exogamy/endogamy, and their patterns of immigration and emigration.
Without using the vitality framework as such, Canadiansocial scientists
102 The Americas
102
frommany disciplines were able to drawan accurate portrait of the demo-
graphic vitality of Francophones and Anglophones during the 1970s. Most
of this research was conducted for two important government enquiries:
one commissioned by the Canadian Federal Government monitoring
FrenchEnglish issues across Canada (Royal Commission on Bilingualism
andBiculturalism, B&Breport; Canada, 1969) andthe Quebec Government
Gendron Commission which focused on French sociolinguistic trends in
the Province (Quebec, 1972).
The demolinguistic analyses provided in the B&B report (Canada, 1969)
confirmed perceptions in Quebec that Francophones outside the Province
were undergoing a severe and sustained rate of assimilation which endan-
gered the long-term survival of Francophone minorities across most of
Anglo-Canada (Castonguay, 1998; de Vries, 1994). Factors suchas the isola-
tion of small Francophone communities across the country, declining
birthrate, FrenchEnglish exogamy, lack of French language primary and
secondary schooling, the legacy of anti-French laws and the dearth of
provincial andfederal government services offeredin French, combinedto
erode the number of French mother-tongue speakers outside of Quebec
(Bourhis, 1994a). For many Qubcois francophones, the adoption of the
Canadian Official Languages Act in 1969 providing federal services in
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 103
Figure 5.1 A taxonomy of the structural variables affecting
ethnolinguistic vitality (adapted from Bourhis et al., 1981)
104 The Americas
Figure 5.2 See Note 3 for English caption
French or English where numbers warrant (Fortier, 1994) was seen as too
little too late. Taking stock of these trends, Qubcois nationalists adopted
the slogan hors du Qubec point de salut (beyond Quebec no survival) and
revivedolder nationalist representations (Figure 5.2) of the shrinking terri-
torial base of French speakers not just within Canada but also within North
America. As a linguistic minority within Canada (28% in 1961) and North
America as a whole (less than 2%), Quebec was portrayed by committed
Xmen as the last territorial enclave in which a normal fully developed
French society could survive within the New World.
On the demographic front during this period, more than 80% of the
Quebec population spoke French as a mother-tongue, of whommore than
three-quarters were unilingual French and used only French as their home
language. With strong intergenerational transmission of French as the
mother-tongue, Quebec language planners could intervene in favour of
French with a full deck of cards on the demolinguistic front. In terms of
distribution, Francophones constituted the overwhelming majority in all
the regions of the Province except in the West Island of Montreal where
Anglophones were in the majority and predominantly unilingual English
(70%). Francophones and Anglophones were more equally distributed in
central areas of Montreal as well as in western parts of the Province
adjoining the Ontario border and in the eastern townships. Though consti-
tuting the majority of the population in areas such as the West Island of
Montreal, the English mother-tongue population (Anglophones) made up
less than 15% of the overall provincial population. However, the inter-
generational transmission of English was assured within the Anglophone
minority, also bolstered by the language shift of many immigrants and
Allophones who adopted English as their home language. The term
Allophone is used to describe individuals who have neither French nor
Englishas a mother-tongue regardless of whether suchindividuals are first
or second generation immigrants. By the early 1970s, Allophones and
immigrants made up 8% of the Quebec population, most of whom had
settled in the Montreal region (88%) and tended to adopt English as the
language of integration with the Anglophone host community.
At the same time, Gendron Commission studies documented trends
which were not so favourable for French language maintenance in the
Province (Quebec, 1972). Demographers confirmed the decline in the
Quebec birthrate fromone of the highest to one of the lowest inthe Western
world (Lachapelle & Henripin, 1980). This decline to well below the
replacement value of 2.1 offsprings per woman meant that Francophones
could no longer rely on the so-called revanche des berceaux (revenge of
the cradle) to maintain their demographic strength within Quebec
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 105
(Caldwell & Fournier, 1987). Without a sustained Francophone birthrate,
reliance on immigration and the integration of immigrants to the
Francophone linguistic community rather than to the Anglophone one
became strategically important as a means of stemming the long-term
decline of the Francophone population in the Province (Termote &
Gauvreau, 1988). For the same strategic reasons, the Anglophone minority,
whose fertility was every bit as lowas that of the French majority, was also
dependent on the immigrant population to sustain the demographic
vitality of the English minority in the Province.
With the freedom to choose between the French or English educational
system (from pre-school to university), the majority of immigrants sent
their children to the English system as a way of maximising the economic
and linguistic mobility of their progeny within North America. Further-
more, the Catholic school systemof the day (bothFrenchandEnglishat the
primaryandsecondarylevel) didnot accept immigrant backgroundpupils
whose religion was other than Catholic. However, the Protestant school
system (mostly English) accepted pupils from all denominational back-
grounds (dAnglejan, 1984). Thus, over the years, a majority of first and
second generation immigrant pupils integrated within the English (Cath-
olic and Protestant) rather than the French linguistic milieu of the Province
(McAndrew, 1996). For many Francophones, freedom of language choice
in schools meant that the projected long-term demographic growth in
Quebec favoured the Anglophone rather than the Francophone linguistic
community. Furthermore, Francophone Xmen pointed out that without
control over immigration, the Quebec Government could not readily
portray Quebec as a French majority Province, making it difficult to attract
immigrants fromFrench-speaking regions of the world. Of more symbolic
than demolinguistic significance, studies showed that while a small
number of French mother-tongue speakers shifted to English as the
language of the home, an even smaller number among the English mother-
tongue minority adopted French as their home language.
Taking these factors into consideration, Quebec analysts provided opti-
mistic, neutral and pessimistic scenarios regarding the future number of
Frenchspeakers inthe Province. Ideologically, while Francophone federal-
ists favouredthe optimistic projections, Quebec nationalists focusedonthe
sombre scenarios thus fuelling existing insecurities about the survival of
the Frenchfact inQuebec. Demolinguistic trends where the object of close
scrutinyinthe mass media alongwithdebates about power sharingof state
jurisdictions between the Canadian Federal Government and the Quebec
Government. Thus, issues concerning the demographic vitality of the
Francophone and Anglophone communities in the Province were used
106 The Americas
selectively by rival political parties proposing contrasting options for the
future of the Province: independent Quebec vs Canadian federalism.
Within democracies, demolinguistic vitality represent strength in
numbers andcanbe usedas a legitimisingtool toempower majorities (and
sometimes minorities) with the institutional completeness (Breton, 1971)
theyneedtoshape their owncollective destinywithinthe intergroupstruc-
ture. Within the vitality framework, Institutional Control factors refer to the
extent to which an ethnolinguistic group has gained formal and informal
representations in the various institutions of a community, region or state
(Figure 5.1). Informal support refers to the degree to which a linguistic
group has organised itself as a pressure group to represent and safeguard
its own ethnolinguistic interests in various state and private activities
including: private primary and secondary education, mass media, busi-
ness, finance, sports, culture and religion. Formal support refers to the
degree to which members of an ethnolinguistic group have gained posi-
tions of control at decision-making levels in business, industry, mass
media, cultural production, sports, religious institutions and within the
government apparatus in education, the police, the judiciary and the civil
administration. Ethnolinguistic groups who enjoy strong institutional
control within the state and private institutions are in a better position to
safeguard and enhance their vitality as a distinctive collective entity than
ethnolinguistic groups that lack institutional control in these domains of
activity.
Also contributing to the institutional control of language groups is the
presence andqualityof leaders whocanheadthe informal andformal insti-
tutions representing their ethnolinguistic group. Gains on the institutional
control front can depend on the emergence of Xmen activists and charis-
matic leaders who succeed in mobilising ethnolinguistic groups to fight in
favour of their own language and cultural survival within multilingual
settings. The absence of quality leadership can undermine gains achieved
by previous generations of group members on the institutional control
front and can mortgage future gains needed for the survival of the next
generation of ethnolinguistic group members.
By the early 1970s both Quebec and Canadian nationalists had excep-
tional political leadership: Rne Levesque for the Qubcois sovereignists
andPierre-Elliot Trudeaufor the Canadianfederalists. Charismatic intheir
respective political constituencies, the epic rivalries of these two politicians
came to symbolise the growing clash between Canadas two solitudes.
Early in their political careers the two leaders, along with Canadian and
Qubcois citizens, were confronted with the inescapable results of the
Royal Commission on Bilingualismand Biculturalism(Canada, 1969). The
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 107
study not only showed that Francophones were under-represented in the
Canadian Federal Administration with English as the dominant language
of work, but that with equal qualifications and experience Francophones
hadsystematically lower wages than Anglophones across Canada. Even in
Quebec, this wage gap held for bilingual francophones relative to
unilingual anglophones in the workforce. EnglishCanadian control of the
economyandfinancial institutions was documentedandshownto account
for the generally low position of Francophone employees within busi-
nesses and corporations across the Province. Results of the Gendron
Commission (Quebec, 1972) confirmed these trends and showed that
English, not French, was the language of business and upward mobility in
Quebec. With the growing integration of a modernising Quebec within the
Canadian and USA economy, the Gendron Commission concluded that
without government intervention, the diglossic position of French as a low
status language wouldworsenrelative tothe prestige positionof Englishin
the work world.
However, the position of the French majority was favourable on most of
the other institutional support factors within the vitality framework.
Francophones controlledmost of the provincial andmunicipal civil admin-
istrations, managed their own French health care and educational system
(from pre-school to university), owned and managed their own French
mass-media (radio, TV, newspapers, book publishing), controlled the
majority French Catholic religious system, created and consumed French
language cultural products (music, literature, theatre) and participated in
the provincial and federal political systems. Though lacking control of the
major business, financial and banking institutions of the Province, Franco-
phones took pride in their own network of French language financial co-
operatives supporting local entrepreneurship and business.
By virtue of their historically dominant elite position, the Anglophone
minority also enjoyed considerable institutional control in the Province. In
addition to heading major business and financial institutions, Anglo-
phones controlled their own English medium health care (hospitals,
clinics) and educational system (from pre-school to university), led their
own Catholic and Protestant English language religious institutions,
owned and managed their own English language mass-media (radio, TV
stations and newspapers), were involved in Quebec, Canadian and Anglo-
American cultural and sport activities, and participated in municipal,
provincial and federal politics.
Ethnolinguistic groups that have gained a measure of ascendancy on
institutional support factors are also likely to enjoy social status relative to
less dominant groups within the social structure. The status variables are
108 The Americas
those related to a speech communitys social prestige, its sociohistorical
status andthe prestige of its language andculture bothwithinits ownterri-
tory and internationally (Figure 5.1). The more status a linguistic commu-
nity is ascribed to have, the more vitality it could be said to possess as a
collectivity. Though not as readily quantifiable as demographic and insti-
tutional factors, social psychological evidence shows that high status
group position can contribute to a more positive social identity which in
turn affects the will to mobilise in favour of enhancing the vitality of the
ingroup language (Sachdev & Bourhis, 1991). Even with effective leader-
ship, being a member of a disparaged low status language group can take
its toll on the collective will to mobilise for the survival of the ingroup
language and culture. The reality of a high or low status position is more
vividto the degree that status differentials between ethnolinguistic groups
are perpetuated through language stereotyping (Genesee &Bourhis, 1988,
Ryan & Giles, 1982), internalised through diglossic language norms and
enshrined through language laws establishing the relative status of rival
language groups (Wardhaugh, 1987).
The enduring international interest in the Quebec case stems from the
fact that at stake in this region is the ascendancy of two historically and
culturally important languages in the Western world. Though a minority
language in North America, French benefits from more vitality on the
status front thanif the Quebec case involveda minority language of lesser
historical or cultural influence in the West. Within Quebec, the diglossic
situation in favour of English relative to French was felt mostly in the work
world of bilingual contact zones in Montreal, western regions along the
Ontario border, and in the eastern townships along the USAborder. Arich
tradition of research on the social psychology of language attitudes and
bilingual communicationconvincingly documentedthe diglossic situation
favouring English rather than French as the language of social prestige in
these contact zones (Bourhis, 1994b; Bourhis & Lepicq, 1993; Genesee &
Holobow, 1989; Hamers &Hummel, 1994; Lambert et al. 1960). To this day,
studies showthat Anglophone students testedintheir Englishhighschools
within French majority regions such as Quebec city are likely to use as
much English in their everyday lives as Anglophones tested in the West
Island of Montreal where they are in the majority (Landry et al., 1997). The
same study with French high school students showed that Francophone
students tested in the English majority West Island of Montreal were less
likely to use French in their everyday life than Francophones tested in
majority French settings such as Quebec city. Results point to the contin-
uing appeal and prestige of the English language for Francophones and to
the capacity of Quebec Anglophones to behave as majority group speakers
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 109
regardless of their demographic density across the province (Landry et al.
1997).
Giles et al. (1977) proposed that demographic, institutional control and
status factors combine to affect in one direction or the other the overall
strength of ethnolinguistic groups. An ethnolinguistic group may be weak
on demographic variables such as the case of Quebec Anglophones, but
enjoy strong institutional control in the mass media and considerable
status as speakers of English, the most powerful language in North
America. Inthe Quebec context of the 1970s, Quebec Anglophones couldbe
assessedas enjoying medium-high overall vitality relative to lower vitality
groups such as Allophone immigrant communities and American Indian
(First Nation) language minorities dispersedacross the Province (Drapeau,
1998; Hamers & Hummel, 1998; Maurais, 1997). While enjoying strong
vitality onthe demographic front withinQuebec, Francophones felt threat-
ened as a language minority at the continental level (Figure 5.2) and had
become quite sensitive to the lack of status accorded to French as a
language of workandadvancement inthe business andfinancial world. As
the higher vitality group within Quebec, a majority of Francophones
elected the pro-independence Parti Qubcois in 1976 whose first legisla-
tive act was to adopt the Charter of the French Language (Bill 101).
The need for considering the vitality position of language minorities
within complex multilingual settings was also acknowledged by Joshua
Fishman (1991) within his reversing language shift framework:
In some respects, the GIDS (Graded Intergenerational Disruption
Scale) may be thought of as a sociocultural reverse analog to the
sociopsychological language vitality measures that several investiga-
tors have recently proposed; the higher the GIDS rating the lower the
intergenerational continuity and maintenance prospects of a language
networkor community. The major difference betweenthe two, inaddi-
tion to their different substantive foci, is that the GIDS is at least a
quasi-implicational scale, i.e. higher (more disrupted) scores imply all
or of the lesser degrees of disruption as well. (p. 87)
The GIDS framework focuses on two central markers of language shift;
intergenerational transmissionof the Xishmother-tongue andthe presence
of a diglossic norm governing the use of the Xish vs Yish language in high
vs low language functions of society. These two crucial indicators of
language shift were not given central billing in the original vitality frame-
work simply because the model was a more general one not specifically
designed to guide when and where language planning efforts should be
launched by Xmen. Diglossia was implied in the vitality framework
110 The Americas
dealing with both institutional support and the status factor of the model.
The degree of control achieved by Xmen in the informal and formal func-
tions of the state are central to the vitality framework (Figure 5.1). In turn,
the position achieved by Xmen in the social structure has a cumulative
impact onthe role ascribedto the Xlanguage for highvs lowfunctions uses
within the society in question, the language status factor included in the
vitality model (Figure 5.1).
In the vitality framework, intergenerational transmission was implied
within the demographic factors as part of the absolute number of speakers
still using the Xish language. Contributing to the rise and fall of Xish
language speakers within the vitality framework were the other important
factors includingbirthrate, mixedmarriages, immigrationandemigration.
It is clear that intergenerational transmission should be included as a
distinct factor contributing to the demographic strength of Xish and Yish
ethnolinguistic groups within the vitality framework.
Just as the objective vitality framework can be improved by the RLS
model, the converse is also true. The RLS model can also be enriched by
including in its analysis the full range of factors used to assess the relative
vitality of language groups. It is clear from our Quebec example that the
vitality framework can provide a more systematic tool of analysis for
guiding RLS efforts than what remains implied in the RLS framework
proposed by Fishman (1991). For instance, it is not evident that a strictly
RLS analysis of the position of French in Quebec in the 1970s would have
ledto the conclusionthat language planning infavour of Frenchwas infact
desirable or necessary. As we have seen, intergenerational transmission of
the French language within the Francophone majority was virtually intact
at this epoch. However, it is the Xmen analysis of the weak position of
French on other vitality factors such as institutional control (the language
of work) and demography (declining fertility, immigrant Anglicisation)
which provided the ideological mobilising tool (threat to French survival)
that convinced the Francophone majority that RLS was vital for the
survival of their language. Thus it may be more useful to consider the
vitality framework as a necessary complementary component of Fishmans
(1991) graded intergenerational disruption scale rather than just the
reverse analogue of the GIDS scale.
The ethnolinguistic vitality approach also offers another dimension
likely to contribute to the RLS framework. Bourhis et al., (1981) raised the
issue of whether ethnolinguistic group members perceived subjectively
their sociostructural positioninmuchthe same wayas suggestedbyobjec-
tive accounts provided by expert analysts and language planners. The
subjective vitality questionnaire (SVQ) was proposed as a way of
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 111
measuring group members assessments of Xish and Yish vitality on each
itemconstituting the demographic, institutional control and status dimen-
sions of the objective vitalityframework(Bourhis et al., 1981). Anumber of
studies conducted with the SVQ showed that in many circumstances
ethnolinguistic group members did perceive their vitality position along
the same lines suggested by objective assessments of the situation
(Bourhis &Sachdev, 1984; Allard &Landry, 1994). In other circumstances,
studies have shown that perceivers can be systematically biased in their
assessment of ingroup and outgroup vitality. Harwood et al. (1994) have
shown that these perceptual distortions can favour ingroup or outgroup
vitality depending on the social context but that such biases do not emerge
on more obvious differentials between Xish and Yish vitality items. These
perceptual distortions manifest themselves onitems inwhichthe degree of
difference is objectively marginal. Sachdev andBourhis (1993) have shown
how both cognitive and motivational factors can help account for the
accentuationor attenuationof ingroupandoutgroupvitalitydifferentials.
Furthermore, AllardandLandry(1986) proposedthat subjective vitality
perceptions couldbe more predictive of language behaviour by taking into
considerationnot onlygeneral beliefs about groupvitality(SVQ, Bourhis et
al. 1981) but also goal beliefs about respondents own desire to behave in
certain ways regarding key aspects of their owngroupvitality (goal beliefs:
BEVQ). Thus Xmenmay acknowledge that their owngroupvitality is weak
and declining (SVQ) but have goal beliefs (BEVQ) reflecting an absence of
motivation to reverse the situation through individual or collective action:
some Xmen simply do not care about the decline of their owngroup
ethnolinguistic vitality. Allard and Landry (1994) showed that a combina-
tion of both general beliefs (SVQ) and goal beliefs (BEVQ) could better
predict key aspects of language behaviours, including intergenerational
transmission of the Xish language, than only objective assessments of Xish
and Yish vitality.
Do Xmen perceive the graded intergenerational disruption scale (GIDS)
in the same way as expert language planners do? It is proposed that a
subjective GIDS scale couldbe constructedto assess howmilitant andrank
and file members of the Xish group perceive the severity of the inter-
generational dislocation affecting their owngroup. Group members
subjective assessments of both their group vitality (SVQ & BEVQ) and the
GIDS may be as important in predicting language behaviour and the
success of RLS as the groups objectively assessed vitality and GIDS.
Subjective assessments of both the vitality and GIDS could be monitored
with both lay and expert subsamples of the Xish population before RLS
interventions and then again following sustained implementation of the
112 The Americas
RLS programme. Combined with demolinguistic census data and socio-
linguistic surveys, subjective perceptions of vitality and GIDS could help
fine-tune future phases of the RLS intervention. Thus a combination of
objective and subjective vitality and GIDS information is proposed as a
more sensitive method of planning and implementing successful RLS
interventions in the context of Fishmans (1991) GIDS.
Assessing 20 Years of Language Planning in Quebec
The social changes brought about by the Quiet Revolution of the 1960s
made francophones especially aware of the diglossic threat posed to
French through its co-existence with English as the prestige language of
business and upward mobility (Rocher, 1992). As vividly argued in Ngres
blancs damrique (White Niggers of America; Vallire, 1969), it was urgent
for the working-class Francophone majority to challenge the Anglo-domi-
nation of the Quebec economy and the indolence of the French Canadian
petite bourgeoisie. Relative to its fading rural and religious past, the
modernisation of Quebec society also meant that the French language had
perhaps become the last symbol of Qubcois identity in an increasingly
materialistic and consumer-oriented society (Sachdev & Bourhis, 1990).
With the French language emerging as the last bastion of Qubcois
distinctiveness it was perhaps inevitable that successive Quebec govern-
ments should find it necessary to legislate on language matters (Bourhis &
Lepicq, 1993).
In the Quebec of the 1970s, Francophones were generally well endowed
onthe demographic andinstitutional control fronts while Frenchlanguage
transmission was relatively free of intergenerational dislocation (Fishman,
1991). Nevertheless, Qubcois intellectuals and activists (Xmen) were
successful in focusing the attention of Francophones on the more problem-
atic aspects of their fate as a French minority in North America. Factors
identified as undermining the future of French in Quebec were: (1) the
decline of Francophones in Anglo-Canada; (2) the drop in the birthrate of
the Francophone population; (3) immigrant choice of the English rather
than French school systemfor their children; and (4) Anglo-domination of
the Quebec economy (dAnglejan 1984; Laporte 1984). Between 1969 and
1996 successive Quebec governments promulgated a number of language
laws designed to address each of the above factors undermining the long-
termprospects of the French language in the Province (Bill 63, 1969; Bill 22,
1974; Bill 101, 1976; Bill 178, 1988; Bill 86, 1993; Bill 40, 1996). To this day, the
Charter of the French Language (Bill 101) remains the most important of
these laws (see Bourhis, 1984a for the text of Bill 101). For the architect of Bill
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 113
101, the late Camille Laurin, the goal of the law was not only to address
current challenges to the maintenance of the French language but also to:
redress past injustices and humiliations suffered by my people in my
village and everywhere in Quebec (Camille Laurins last public speech, 11
December 1998, authors translation).
Bill 101 guaranteed the rights of every Quebecer to receive communica-
tions in French when dealing with the civil administration, semi-public
agencies and business firms, including the right by all customers to be
informed and served in French. The law also ensured the right of all
workers to work in French and not to be dismissed or demoted for the sole
reason that they were monolingual in French. As regards the language of
work, Bill 101 essentially applied measures already in place since Bill 22.
Business firms with more than 50 employees were required to apply for
andobtaina Francisationcertificate whichattestedthat they hadthe neces-
sary infrastructure to use French as the language of work within their
organisation (Daoust, 1984; Bouchard, 1991). From 1996 onwards, the
Francisation certificate was necessary for business firms wishing to tender
their services or goods to the provincial government.
Bill 101 also guaranteed English schooling to all present and future
Quebec Anglophone pupils. All immigrant children already in English
schools by the time the Bill was passed along with all their current and
future siblings were alsoguaranteedaccess toEnglishschooling. However,
the law made it clear that all subsequent immigrants to Quebec were
obliged to send their children to French primary and secondary public
schools. Nevertheless, the lawdid not affect freedomof language choice at
the primary and secondary school levels for parents wishing to enrol their
children in full fee-paying private schools. Furthermore, freedom of
language choice was guaranteed to all post-secondary students who could
choose to attend either French or English medium colleges (CEGEPS) and
universities in Quebec. Finally Bill 101 contained a controversial clause
which banned languages other than French fromthe linguistic landscape
including road signs, government signs and commercial store signs.
Though Bill 101 contained some measures related to corpus language
planning, the major aim of the law was designed to improve the status of
French relative to English in Quebec society (Bourhis & Lepicq, 1993).
Cause andeffect relationships are difficult toestablishwhenevaluatingthe
impact of language policies onlanguage behaviour andlanguage shift. The
Quebec case is no exception and the above caveat must be taken into
consideration when assessing the evidence presented in the remaining
parts of this chapter.
114 The Americas
Bill 101 and demolinguistic trends
The immediate reactions of many Francophones to Bill 101 were quite
positive, since the lawwas seen as being effective in securing the linguistic
future of the French majority in the Province (Bourhis, 1984b; Levine, 1990;
Maurais 1987). On the demographic front, the proportion of French
mother-tongue speakers increased slightly from 80.7% of the Quebec
populationin1971 (4,866,410) to81.5%(5,741,438) in1996. Frenchlanguage
use at home increasedfrom80.8%(4,870,100) in1971 to 82,8%(5,830,082) in
1996. Taken together these trends in mother-tongue and home language
use suggest an increasing intergenerational transmission of French from
1971 to 1996. Thus the 1996 census results suggest a language shift in favour
of French as evidenced by the higher score in French home language use
relative to the number of French mother-tongue speakers in the census.
Though this shift is partially accounted for by changes in recent census
methodology, the change is largely attributed to Allophones rather than
Anglophones who have adopted French as the language of the home
(Castonguay, 1998). This favourable trend for French is perhaps neutral-
ised by the continuing weak fertility of the Quebec Francophone popula-
tion: while the fertility rate in 1961 was 4.2 children per woman, this rate
had dropped to only 1.6 by 1996. On this point Castonguay (1998)
concludes: Over twenty years of insufficient fertility spells a coming
decline in absolute numbers for the francophone population in Quebec a
fortiori inCanada as awhole beginningearlyinthe next century. (p. 44).
The growing integration of Quebec Francophones within the North
American economic and cultural mainstream is implied by the increase of
FrenchEnglish bilingualism among Francophones. Whereas only 26% of
French mother-tongue speakers reported being FrenchEnglish bilinguals
in 1971, this proportion had increased to 34% in 1996. This rise in the
number of Francophone bilinguals is especially evident in Montreal where
FrenchEnglish bilingualism remains advantageous for economic and
cultural exchanges with local, Canadian and international partners.
Anglophone reactions to Bill 101 were largely negative because the law
was seen as an attack on the traditional elite status of the English minority
in the Province (Legault, 1992; Scowen, 1991; Stevenson, 1999). Bill 101
forced many Anglophones to see themselves as a minority group rather
thanas individual members of a dominant elite (Caldwell 1984, 1994, 1998).
Following the election of the pro-sovereignty Parti Qubcois in 1976,
many Anglophones dissatisfiedwithQuebecs language andfiscal policies
emigrated to Ontario and other provinces of Canada. As it turns out,
emigration from the province and low fertility rate were key factors
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 115
contributing to the erosion of the demographic vitality of Quebec
Anglophone (Castonguay, 1998, 1999). Census results showed a decline of
12% in Quebecs English mother-tongue population between 1971 and
1981 (Caldwell, 1984). This net loss of 158,000 English mother-tongue
speakers occurred among the more economically mobile elements of the
Anglophone community, a goodnumber of whomwere unilingual English
of British descent, in their twenties and more likely to be university degree
holders. By the 1990s these out-migration trends took their toll on the
proportion of Anglophones in Quebec: while English mother-tongue
speakers made up 13%of the population in 1971 (789,000), this proportion
dropped to only 8.8% by 1996 (622,000), a net drop of 167,000 English
mother-tongue speakers in the Province. English language use at home
droppedfrom14.7%in1971 (887,875) to 10.8%in1996 (762,457). The differ-
ence in favour of English between home language use (10.8%) and English
mother-tongue speakers (8.8%) testifies tothe remarkable drawingpower
of English in Quebec even after 20 years of Bill 101 implementation
(Castonguay, 1997, 1999). Analyses of census results from1971 to1996 have
shown that Allophones have been the source of this language shift in
favour of English not only in Quebec but also across Canada (Castonguay,
1999).
Despite an optimal rate of intergenerational transmission, it is clear that
the Quebec Anglophone minority is experiencing a sharp decline on more
fundamental indicators of demographic vitality such as absolute and rela-
tive group numbers, out-migration and fertility rates. With a declining
fertilityrate from3.3 childrenper womanin1961 toonly1.6 in1996 andfew
prospects for a substantial immigration from Anglo-Canada, Quebec
Anglophones have recognised their growing dependence on the linguistic
integration strategies of Allophones and international immigrants who
settle in the Province.
In Canada the term Heritage languages is used to denote languages
other than the countrys two official languages, English and French. In
Quebec, the gradual equalisation of the drawing power of French and
English as a result of Bill 101 has made the option of maintaining the Heri-
tage language and culture more attractive for Allophone and immigrant
communities in the Province (Bourhis, 1994b). The 1996 census results
showedthat Allophones made up9.3%of the total Quebec population. Out
of these 681,790 Allophones, the most important mother-tongue groups are
the following: Italian (20%), Spanish (10%), Arabic (8.8%), Greek (6.5%),
Chinese (6.2%) and Portuguese (5%). Analyses of the 1986 census data had
shown that Heritage language maintenance was much stronger among
Quebec Allophones than it is among Allophones who settled in Anglo-
116 The Americas
Canada (Pendakur, 1990). For instance, whereas 68% of first generation
immigrants who settled in Quebec reported they still used their Heritage
tongue as their only language of communication at home (and 63% in
1991), only 48% of such groups outside Quebec still reported using their
Heritage tongue as their home language. This difference in Heritage
language maintenance was even more pronounced in the case of second
generation Allophone groups in Quebec (63%) relative to their counterpart
elsewhere in Canada (34%). Evidence from the 1996 census suggests that
Allophones inQuebec remainmore likely to retaintheir Heritage language
use at home than Allophones in the rest of Canada.
Following power sharing deals reachedwiththe CanadianImmigration
Department from1978 to 1991, the Quebec Government gained a measure
of control in the selection of immigrants based on the specific employment
and linguistic needs of the Province. In the last two decades the Quebec
Ministry of Civic Affairs and Immigration has had some success in
attracting immigrants with a knowledge of French, others from former
Frenchcolonies andimmigrants whose knowledge of a Romance language
(e.g. Spanish) facilitated the learning of French. Indeed, studies have
shown that immigrants with some knowledge of French are more likely to
integrate within the Francophone host society while immigrants with a
knowledge of English are more likely to integrate within the Anglophone
host minority (Veltman, 1998). Clearly, the Quebec Government is
committedto using the institutional control at its disposal for selecting and
facilitating the integration of Allophones and immigrants to the Franco-
phone host majority rather than to the Anglophone minority.
Growing linguistic tensions between the Francophone andAnglophone
host communities put added pressure on Allophone minorities to openly
take sides in the Quebec linguistic debate (Bourhis, 1994b). One response
for Allophones was to learn both French and English, a bilingualismtrend
which increased from 33% in 1971 to as much as 48% in 1996. Combining
Allophones who know only French or both French and English, census
results showthat the proportion of Allophones who declared a knowledge
of French increased from 47% in 1971 to 70% in 1996. Conversely, the
proportion of Allophones declaring knowledge of English dropped from
70% in 1971 to 66% in 1996. Finally, Bill 101, along with immigration from
Romance language countries, had an impact on the language shift patterns
of recently arrived Allophone immigrants. Of the Allophones who settled
in the Province before 1976, only 34%had adopted French as the language
of the home, while for Allophones who arrivedafter Bill 101 (between 1976
and 1991), as many as 67% used French as one of their language at home
(Quebec, 1996a).
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 117
Bill 101 and Montreal
Up to the beginning of the 20th century, Montreal was the financial
capital of Canada and was a city whose population was predominantly
English mother-tongue. The urbanisation and proletarisation of Quebec
Francophones changed the demographic make-up of the city and by the
1950s Montreal had become a bilingual city with a predominantly French-
speakingpopulation. Today, the Montreal metropolitanregionis the major
urban centre of Quebec with a population of 3.3 million relative to the total
Quebec population of 7 million. The proportion of French mother-tongue
speakers in the Montreal region increased from65%in 1951 to 70%in 1986
while it dropped slightly to 67% in 1996. Conversely, the proportion of
Englishmother-tongue speakers inthe Montreal regiondroppedfrom26%
in 1951 to 17% in 1986 and to only 13% in 1996. Since the 1950s, it is the
arrival of Allophone immigrants which has had the greatest impact in
changing the demolinguistic profile of the city. The proportion of
Allophones increased from8.6%in 1951 to 13%in 1986 and to 20%in 1996.
While increasinglymixedtoday, it remains true that the West Islandpart of
Montreal is predominantly Anglophone while Francophones form the
majority in eastern regions of the island with considerable mixing of all
ethnolinguistic groups occurring in the central regions of the Metropolis.
Accounting for almost half the population of Quebec, multilingual
Montreal remains quite distinct from other regions of the Province. Other
urban centres are linguistically quite homogeneous with the French
mother-tongue concentration ranging from more than 90% of the popula-
tion (Qubec city, Trois-Rivires, Sherbrooke) to 80% (Hull city region).
The less urbanised regions of the Province are also predominantly
Francophone, with92%of the populationdeclaringFrenchas their mother-
tongue.
When considering the dispersion of immigrants and Allophones settled
in Quebec, one notes that the vast majority (88%) inhabit the Montreal
region, thus constituting 16% of the total Metropolitan population.
Allophone and immigrant communities concentrate in Montreal mainly
because the city offers better economic opportunities and allows such
groups to maintain the ingroup networks which provide the cultural and
economic support needed to adjust more readily within the host society.
The concentration of ethnolinguistic communities within the metropole
has meant that it is the Montreal rather than the Quebec population as a
whole that has had to adjust to the realities of multilingualism and ethnic
pluralism in the Province.
Given the key role of Montreal in the Quebec economy, Francophone
118 The Americas
Xmen consider that it is in this city that the fate of the French language will
be determined in North America. It is the necessity of using French to
communicate with the Francophone unilingual majority which has legiti-
mised the quest for increasing the institutional status of French relative to
English in Quebec. It is argued that if the majority of Montreal Franco-
phones and Allophones become bilingual, then the rationale for using
Frenchwill give wayto the advantage of usingEnglishas the sole language
of business and upward mobility within the city. Among French mother-
tongue speakers in Montreal, the proportion of bilinguals increased from
38% in 1971 to 44% in 1986 and 47% in 1996. Likewise, English mother-
tongue speakers who declared they were bilingual increased from 35% in
1971 to 54% in 1986 and to 62% in 1996. The proportion of Montreal
Allophones whodeclaredknowledge of bothFrenchandEnglishincreased
from 35% in 1971 to 50% in 1986 while decreasing slightly to 49% in 1996.
Though militant Francophone Xmen view individual bilingualism as a
potential threat to the status of French as the majority language of
Montreal, others see the rise of bilingualismas an asset contributing to the
cultural and economic dynamism of the city. Young adult bilinguals of
Francophone, Anglophone and Allophone background share the common
experience of havingbeeneducatedat a time whenBill 101 was more or less
accepted as a fait accompli. With college or university degrees, these young
bilinguals often mix at work and play and develop intercultural communi-
cation strategies which include more balanced FrenchEnglish language
switching as part of their normal communicative repertoire (mutual
language convergence, Amiot & Bourhis, 1999; Moise & Bourhis, 1994).
While remaining attached to their respective ethnocultural community,
these Montreal linguistic chameleons are developing bicultural/multi-
cultural identities which are called upon to suit the immediate needs of the
cross-cultural encounter they happen to be in. Though not likely to gener-
alise to the whole population of Montreal bilinguals, it remains true that
younger subgroups of bilinguals are re-defining FrenchEnglish
Allophone relations in a post-modern, less confrontational style than the
older generation of Montrealers who actually fought the battles over
language laws such as Bill 101.
In a 1997 telephone survey, the Conseil de la Langue franaise (CLF)
sought to assess the impact of Bill 101 on the use of French and English as
the language of public life in settings such as shopping malls, local shops,
banks, hospitals, and with civil servants at the provincial and local school
levels (Bland, 1999). A large representative sample of Francophones,
Anglophones and Allophones were surveyed across the Province
including the greater Montreal region. Combining the language use situa-
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 119
tions into a single public language measure, results showed a sustained
use of FrenchamongbothFrancophones andAllophones but less soamong
Anglophones (cf. Moise & Bourhis, 1994). In greater Montreal, results
showed that 97% of Francophones reported using exclusively or mostly
French in these public settings, while only 3% reported using mostly
English. The majority of Allophones (54%) reported using exclusively or
mostly French in public settings, while 39%reported using mostly English
and only 8% reporting using mainly their Heritage language. Taken
together, these results suggest that French has achieved the status of the
normal public language for Montreal Francophones while for Allo-
phones, French is gaining ground relative to English. In contrast, only 23%
of Anglophones reported using mostly French in these public settings
while 77% reported using exclusively or mostly English. Clearly, Quebec
Anglophones, regardless of their bilingual skills, can still afford to use
mostly English as their public language in greater Montreal, a trend also
obtained for Anglophones in regions of the Province other than the
Montreal area.
Bill 101 and education
Unlike most other language minorities in North America, Quebec
Anglophones benefit fromthe provision of a state fundedEnglish medium
educational system starting from pre-kindergarten schooling up to colle-
giate and university level including graduate and post-doctoral studies
(there are three publicly funded English universities in Quebec). The
majority of Anglophones enrol their children in the English primary and
secondary school board system. However, within the English system,
Anglophone parents have been keen to enrol their children in French
immersion programmes: the proportion of Anglophone pupils in French
immersionclasses increasedfrom24%in1981 to32%in1998. Furthermore,
a growing number of English mother-tongue pupils (Anglophones and
immigrants) attend the French school system: from 10% in 1972 to 17% in
1995 (Quebec, 1996).
In the immediate aftermath of Bill 101, Anglophones were most
concerned about the erosion of their demographic base resulting from the
fact that most newimmigrants toQuebec wouldhave tosendtheir children
to French rather than English primary and secondary schools (Mallea,
1984). Bill 101 has had its intended impact on enrolments within the
English primary and secondary school system in the Province. Allophone
enrolment in the English school system dropped from 85% in 1972 to only
21% in 1995, while their enrolment in the French primary and secondary
school system increased from only 15% in 1972 to 79% in 1995. Thus,
120 The Americas
Anglophones can no longer count on immigrants to maintain the demo-
graphic base of their school system in the Province which dropped from
256,251 pupils in1976 toonly111,466 in1995. Studies suggest that this drop
in the number of pupils attending the English school systemis largely due
to the application of Bill 101 (immigrants must enrol in French schools)
while the lowfertilityof Anglophones alsoaccount for the decline (Quebec,
1996a). The out-migration of many unilingual Anglophones along with
enrolment inFrenchschools andthe enduringpopularityof Frenchimmer-
sion classes contributed to the increase of FrenchEnglish bilingualism
amongQuebec Anglophones from37%in1971 toas muchas 62%in1996.
As in the past, the 1996 census shows that the proportion of Quebec
Anglophones with a university degree is greater (21%) than for Quebec
Francophones (14%) and for the Canadian population as a whole (16%). Of
those enrolled in post-secondary education, more than 92% of Quebec
Anglophones chose English medium colleges and Universities, a trend
which remained stable in the 1980s and 1990s. Anglophone enrolment in
French at the collegiate level has increased marginally from 5% in 1980 to
6.6%in1990, while enrolment inFrenchuniversities has remainedstable at
around 7% up to the 1990s. A brain drain of English-speaking university
graduates has also occurred since the adoption of Bill 101. From 1976 to
1986 the net out-migration of English-speaking university degree holders
has been as high as 40% (26,550 graduates). Studies suggest that factors
accounting for this exodus were: insufficient French language skills, polit-
ical uncertainty in the Province and better job prospects in Anglo-Canada.
However, a small proportion of those who left during this period were in
fact Anglo-Canadians returning to their Province of origin after
completing their university studies in Quebec. The out-migration to
EnglishCanada was less sustainedinthe decade from1986 to1996 (28%net
loss, 21,688 graduates) and was more likely due to poor job prospects in a
declining Quebec economy. Overall, during this 20- year period, English-
speaking university degree holders were 12 times as likely to leave the
Province as Francophone degree holders. The exodus of young university
trained Anglophones is not only having a negative impact on the develop-
ment of Quebec society as a whole but also undermines the capacity of the
Anglophone minority to maintain its vitality in the Province.
Bill 101 is having much of its intended effect of not only increasing the
proportion of Allophone groups enrolled in the French primary and
secondaryschool systembut has alsohadanimpact onthe use of Frenchby
Allophone pupils on school grounds and at home. A survey of Allophone
pupils enrolled in Montreal French high schools showed that on school
grounds (outside the classroom) Allophones reported using French 57%of
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 121
the time, followed by English at 25% and Heritage languages at 18%
(Giroux, 1992). In the family setting with their siblings the Allophone
pupils reported using as much French (40%) as their own Heritage
language (40%) while English use was at 20%. Another revealing measure
of Allophone linguistic preferences can be obtained by considering enrol-
ment in collegiate schooling and university where freedom of language
choice remains in effect for all citisens of the Province. Allophone enrol-
ment in French colleges increased from 18% in 1980 to as much as 40% in
1989 and to 46% in 1994. The 1989 collegiate entry figures are important
because this age cohort represents the first group of Allophone students
whose Grade 1 entry into the primary school system coincided with the
first year of application of Bill 101 in 1978. The post-1989 figures reflect the
fact that many Allophones schooledinthe Frenchsecondary school system
did decide to remain in the French systemat the collegiate level. Indeed of
those Allophones who were schooled in French secondary schools, as
many as 64% enrolled in a French college in 1994. However, the 36% of
Allophones schooled in the French system who switched to English
colleges did so despite the academic risks involved in pursuing collegiate
studies in a language other than the one used for studies at the secondary
school level. Of the Allophones schooled in the English secondary school
system very few are known to switch to French for their collegiate studies
(less than 1%). Finally, the proportion of Allophone undergraduates who
choose to attend French language universities increased from 42% in 1986
to 47% in 1994, while the majority still chose to attend English language
universities.
Basically, many Allophones and immigrants settled in Quebec have
optedfor trilingualismas a cultural integrationstrategy for dealing withthe
presence of two rival host communities in their country of adoption.
Allophones and their children maintain knowledge of their Heritage
language because it is the mother-tongue of their parents, learn French
because of the educational obligations of Bill 101 within a French majority
Province, and adopt English because it remains the lingua franca of
economic and social mobility in North America (Bourhis, 1994b). Bill 101
measures were designed to ensure that Allophones and immigrants have
the opportunity to learn French through the school system. The law does
not oblige Allophones to assimilate linguistically to the Francophone
majority. However, many Francophone Xmen counted on Bill 101 to create
a carry-over effect promoting language shift to French and identification
to the French rather than the Allophone or Anglophone cultural milieu.
Suchexpectations, whennot fullymet, have made militant Xmenimpatient
if not intolerant towards Allophones who identify mainly with their
122 The Americas
owngroup, maintain Heritage language use at home or prefer integration
within the Anglophone rather than the Francophone host community.
Others argue that Allophones and immigrants do not need to identify only
as Francophones or only as Anglophones when multiple group identifica-
tion including the Heritage one remains a viable option especially in multi-
cultural Montreal.
Given that Bill 101 obliged Francophone parents to enrol their children
in the French public school system only, percentage enrolment of French
mother-tongue students in French primary and secondary schools have
remainedstable in the last 20 years: 98%in 1972, 99%in 1995. However, the
demographic decline of the Francophone population has meant that enrol-
ment in the French school system dropped 25% in absolute terms during
the period from 1971 (1,378,788 pupils) to 1995 (1,036,202 pupils). At the
collegiate and university level, Quebec students have the choice to attend
French or English language institutions. At both the college and university
level, government data of the last 15 years shows that a steady 95% of
Francophones choose to pursue post-secondary studies in French institu-
tions, with just 5%choosing to attend English language institutions. Taken
together, these patterns showthat Bill 101 measures torestrict the language
choice of Francophones at the primary and secondary school level are
having their desired effect in maintaining Francophones within the French
educational system at the collegiate and university level. These self-
imposed restrictions on individual freedom of choice also help consoli-
date the collective status of French as a language of higher education in the
scientific, economic and cultural spheres, thus reducing the diglossic pres-
sures on French relative to English in the Province.
Bill 101 and the language of work
The use of the French language and the proportion of Francophones
within the Provincial Civil Administration has long been quite substantial
to the point that the Provincial Government has had to announce hiring
programmes to represent more fairly the proportion of Anglophones and
Allophones (18%) in the general population. However, to this day, the
Provincial Government has been slow in recruiting Anglophones and
Allophones within its Civil Administration (3.5%), a failure noted in the
1998 annual Report of the Quebec Human Rights Commission (CDP, 1998)
Bill 101 was designed to improve the use of French as the language of
work in privately owned industries, businesses, and financial institutions
across Quebec. The most important measure to improve the use of French
in the private work world was the mandatory Francisation certificates
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 123
which could only be delivered to businesses following linguistic analyses
provingthat Frenchuse was generalisedwithinorganisations of more than
50 employees. Most business firms did comply with the Francisation
requirements and the proportion of Quebec businesses who received their
French certification increased from 7.7% in 1980 to 71.6% in 1999 (Daoust
1984; Maurais, 1987; OLF, 1998). A study conducted by the Office de la
langue franaise (OLF) showed that French was more likely to be used as
the language of work (written and spoken) in business firms that obtained
their Francisation certificate (more than 80%French use) than in those that
did not (65% or less; Bouchard, 1998).
During the first decade of application of the Francisation law, a number
of large Anglophone businesses covertly or overtly moved their base of
operation fromQuebec to other regions of Canada (Miller, 1984). Analysis
of the Francisation certificate records compiled by the OLF showed that
businesses whose ownership was Anglophone were the slowest in
obtaining their Francisation certificates relative to businesses owned by
Francophones and Allophones interests (Bouchard, 1991). The plateau
reached in the early 1990s in the percentage of firms which obtained their
Francisation certificate is explained by industry and organisational factors
beyond the reach of the OLF. Companies whose Francisation remain prob-
lematic have the following profile: multinationals with head offices else-
where in the world and whose organisational culture and software
technology is mainly English, local firms using hightechnology andwhose
market is mainly international rather than local (e.g. aeronautic, software
and bio-technology; Bouchard, 1998). Given the high value added profile
of these mobile job creating industries, the OLF has beenpatient inits drive
to implement Francisation programmes in these firms.
The election of pro-independence governments, two referendums on
Quebec separation, fiscal policies andthe Francisationof the Quebec work-
place contributed to the departure of many Anglo-Canadian business
firms. The resultingout-migrationof Anglophone employees andadminis-
trators hadanimpact onthe positionof Francophones andAnglophones in
the work world. For instance in the Montreal region the proportion of
Francophone administrators and professionals within the workforce
increased from 55% in 1971 to 68% in 1991, while the proportion of
Anglophones dropped from 34% in 1971 to just 18% in 1991. Also, the
proportion of Francophones holding senior administrative positions
increased from 41% in 1971 to 67% in 1991, while the proportion of
Anglophones holding such positions dropped from 47% in 1971 to 20% in
1991. However, the linguistic networks of Quebecs two solitudes remain a
reality within the Quebec corporate world. Studies showed that Franco-
124 The Americas
phone CEOs remained concentrated in French-owned large industries
(91%in 1976; 87%in 1993) compared to their presence in industries owned
by Anglophones (9% in 1976, 15% in 1993; Quebec, 1996a).
The modernisation of Quebec society and the cumulative effect of Bill
101 can also be credited for improving the income position of Franco-
phones relative to Anglophones inthe Province. Controlling for education,
experience and age, studies showed that in 1970 Anglophone unilinguals
or bilinguals earned8%more inannual salarythanbilingual Francophones
and 16% more than unilingual Francophones. By 1990 the income gap
between Francophones and Anglophones was considerably reduced or
reversed in some cases. Carefully controlled studies showed that Anglo-
phone unilinguals and bilinguals earned only 3% more than unilingual
Francophone by 1990, while Francophone bilinguals earned 4% more than
Anglophone unilinguals or bilinguals (Quebec, 1996a). In1970, studies had
shown that the income premium for knowing English in Montreal was
16%. By 1980 this income premium decreased to 6% and was further
eroded to only 3% in 1990.
Taking stock of these improvements in favour of Francophones in the
work world, the Quebec Government task force evaluating the position of
the French language since Bill 101 arrived at the following conclusion:
The goals of francisation put forward by the Gendron commission
have been reachedto a substantial degree, gains are especially obvious
in the network of businesses owned by Quebec Francophones. These
fundamental changes have benefited Francophones whose income
profiles have been largely redressed. The historical link between
Francophone worker and low income is a thing of the past. (Quebec,
1996a: 68; authors translation.)
Research conducted by the CLF also showed that Bill 101 did improve the
status and use of French as the language of work in Quebec business firms
(Bland, 1991). In the Montreal region, self-report studies showed that the
proportion of Francophone workers who declared working mostly in
French(90%or more of the time) increasedfrom52%in1971 to63%in1989,
while among Allophones the increase was more modest, from17%in 1971
to24%in1989 (Quebec, 1996a). Anglophone workers were least affectedby
language of work provisions since the use of mostly French ranged from
only 2%in 1971 to 8%in 1989. However a drop in the use of mainly English
at work(51%or more Englishuse at work) was evident for the three groups
of workers followingBill 101: for Francophones the dropinEnglishuse was
from 12% in 1971 to only 6% in 1989, while for Allophones the drop was
from 58% in 1971 to 37% in 1989. However, Anglophone workers still
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 125
worked mostly in English with a drop from 86% in 1971 to 55% in 1989.
Thus the Francisation features of Bill 101 had the intended effect of
increasing the use of French at work especially for Francophone
employees. After 20 years of Bill 101 application, the Quebec Government
task force on the position of French concluded the following:
French mother-tongue workers have gained their rightful position in
the Quebec labour market. Gone are the days when French was
predominant at the bottom of the labour market, bilingualism was
necessary in the middle and English dominated at the top of the
market. (Quebec, 1996a: 71; authors translation.)
Given the Francisation goals of Bill 101, Francophone Xmen nevertheless
bemoanthe slowprogress of Frenchas the language of work for Allophone
workers and remain sceptical of the gains in the use of French reported by
Anglophone employees. Militant Xmen also lobby to expand the require-
ment of Francisation certificates to businesses of less than 50 employees. In
effect, Bill 101 measures to improve the status of Frenchat work have made
it possible for Francophone employees to remain unilingual without being
overly penalised on the income and job promotion front. However, with
the growing integration of the Quebec economy within the continental
market following the implementation of the North American Free Trade
Agreement (NAFTA, 1991), it remains the case that Francophone business
leaders, professionals and skilled technicians are increasingly bilingual,
using English as the lingua franca of international trade, science, tech-
nology and communication (e-mail, electronic media).
Bill 101 and the Quebec linguistic landscape
Slaves who become cruel masters themselves do great damage to the
cause of abolition; previously disadvantaged RLSers who become
cultural imperialists themselves, within their newly dominated net-
work, do great damage to the RLS cause, not only locally or regionally
but internationally as well. Francophone Quebecs restriction of the
public use of English on store and street signs, now that French has
unmistakenly come to the fore there, has harmed the moral and polit-
ical positionof francophone minorities outside of Quebec as well as the
position of a variety of RLS efforts far outside the borders of Canada.
(Fishman, 1991: 84)
Though most crucial for reversing language shift, the Bill 101 measures to
enshrine French as the language of schooling for immigrants and to
normalise French as the language of work were unlikely to produce imme-
126 The Americas
diate improvements inthe status anduse of FrenchinQuebec. However, as
the first legislative act of the Parti Qubcois Government, Bill 101 hadtobe
seen as having an immediate positive impact on the status of French, espe-
cially in Montreal. The architect of Bill 101, Dr Camille Laurin, understood
the need for the Charter to: (1) make Francophones feel more secure about
the place of FrenchinQuebec; (2) senda clear message toAnglophones and
Allophones that the status of Frenchrelative toEnglishwas changinginthe
Province. The Francisation of the Quebec linguistic landscape was chosen
as the highly visible domain in which the newpower relationship between
French and English would be symbolised in an immediately tangible way
(Landry & Bourhis, 1997). In addition to changing road signs and govern-
ment signs to French only and replacing many English place names by
French ones, Bill 101 stipulated that commercial store signs had to be in
French only, except those dealing with religious, humanitarian or cultural
products which could remain in any language. Enforced by the Commis-
sion de la protection de la langue francaise (CPLF), these linguistic landscape
requirements had the advantage of producing highly visible changes in
favour of French within a year of the adoption of Bill 101, thus comforting
Francophones that Bill 101 was achieving its immediate goals set by the
Parti Qubcois Government.
Though some Francophones felt uneasy about effectively banning
English from commercial signs, Xmen cited the Quebec Prime Minister,
Ren Lvesque, who remarkedthat whenimmigrants sawa bilingual store
signinMontreal, the message theyunderstoodwas that thoughFrenchand
Englishwere present inthe Province, knowledge of Englishwas quite enough
to get by in Quebec. While Bill 101 was essentially aimed at enshrining
French as one of the most valueddimensions of Qubcois French identity,
the demotion of English implied in the lawalso served to raise language as
a more highly prized symbol of Quebec Anglophone identity (Sachdev &
Bourhis, 1990). Thus the presence of French vs English within the Quebec
linguistic landscape came tohave muchsymbolic significance for members
of each linguistic community (Landry & Bourhis, 1997).
Of the Quebec Anglophones who stayed after Bill 101, many mobilised
as group members to defend their status as the English minority of the
Province (Levine, 1990; Scowen, 1991). By 1982 this mobilisation culmi-
nated in the creation of an Anglophone pressure group, known as Alliance
Quebec, whose aimwas to safeguardandpromote the linguistic interests of
English Quebec (Caldwell, 1998, Legault, 1992; Stevenson, 1999). Subsi-
dised by the Canadian Federal Government ($1.2 million/year) pro-
English lobby groups such as Alliance Quebec mobilised against key
features of Bill 101 suchas Frenchonly commercial signs andthe education
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 127
provisions forcing international immigrants to send their children to
French schools. The intensity of Anglophone sentiments against the
French-only commercial sign law is conveyed in the caricature published
in the major English language newspaper of the Province (Figure 5.3).
By 1988, following Anglophone challenges of Bill 101, the Canadian
Supreme Court ruledthat French-only commercial signs contravenedboth
the Quebec andCanadianCharter of Rights andFreedoms. While Quebecs
right to require the predominant display of French as one of the languages
on commercial signs was justified, requiring the exclusive use of French
was not. The Canadian Court declared that individual rights to freedomof
expression included the right to display commercial signs in the language
of ones choice. In response to the Canadian Supreme Court ruling and in
the midst of a heated linguistic debate, the Quebec Liberal Government
adopted Bill 178, which stipulated that bilingual signs be allowed inside
commercial stores with less than 50 employees but that only French would
be allowed on outside commercial signs. Bill 178 was passed by invoking a
Canadian Constitution override clause exempting provinces from indi-
vidual rights enshrined within the Canadian Charter of Rights and Free-
doms for renewable period of five years. The Canadian Charter of Rights
was adopted by the nine Anglophone majority Provinces in 1982 but was
never ratified by Quebec though the Province remains subject to its provi-
sions. The Quebec Liberal Government had come to power in 1985 with a
programme that favoured allowing languages other than French on
commercial signs. Anglophone frustration with Bill 178 led to the forma-
tion of the Equality Party calling for the repeal of Bill 178 and a return to
full bilingualism in Quebec.
The Canadian Constitution override clause stipulates that provincial
laws that infringe individual rights either become nullified after five years
or must be renewed or changed by the Provincial Government in question.
Withfive years elapsedthe Quebec Liberal Government decidedto replace
Bill 178 by a new commercial sign law that respected individual rights
enshrined in the Canadian charter. Bill 86 allowed languages other than
French on commercial signs as long as French was twice as predominant
visually than the combined presence of other languages. Polls used to plan
the lawshowedthat over 64%of Francophones across the Province favoured
bilingual commercial signs as long as Frenchwas twice as predominant, an
option also endorsed by 60%of rank and file Parti Qubcois sympathisers
(Gauthier & Bourhis, 1993). Another 14% of Parti Qubcois sympathisers
preferred total freedom of choice for the language of signs, while 28%
favoured a return to the original Bill 101 measure of allowing only French
unilingualism on commercial signs. A majority of Anglophones and
128 The Americas
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 129
Figure 5.3
Allophones (54%) also endorsed the French predominance option though
40%still preferred the freedomof choice option allowing store owners to
decide which languages to use on their store signs. Taken together, the
French predominance option rallied the majority of support across the
three linguistic communities and given the degree of endorsement
obtained even among Parti Qubcois sympathisers, the Liberal Govern-
ment quickly passed the new sign law in 1993. Support for the Bill 86 sign
law increased by 1996, with Quebec-wide polls showing that 87% of
Quebecers wantedto maintain the predominance of French oncommercial
signs. However, as inprevious polls, results showedthat 80%of Quebecers
felt that despite the protection offered by laws such as Bill 101 and Bill 86,
French was perceived to be very or somewhat threatened in Quebec.
To conform to Bill 86, stores could display only in French, or display
twice as many messages in French than in other languages or display signs
with French characters twice as predominant as those of all other
languages combined. Importantly, a store was deemed as not abiding by
the law if one message in a store was not translated in French even if all
other messages were unilingual French. At conventions, trade fairs and
international exhibitions and in stores selling cultural products in
languages other than French such as bookshops, cinemas and video clubs,
signs could be displayed in other languages without the presence of
French. Non-commercial messages could also be displayed in other
languages when such messages dealt with humanitarian, religious, polit-
ical, ideological or strictly personal messages. Equal FrenchEnglish bilin-
gualismis authorised for public signs about health or public safety and for
international events and in museums, cultural or scientific exhibitions and
tourist information. However, advertising must be in French only on bill-
boards, large-scale signs andadvertisingonpublic transportationvehicles.
After only two years of application, the OLF and CLF conducted three
major studies on the Francisation of Montreal Island commercial signs
(Quebec, 1997). The language of signs was studied by independent
observers in 2008 Montreal stores in 1995, 2040 stores in 1996 and in 2016
stores in 1997. The proportion of these Montreal stores containing at least
one message in French increasedfrom94%in 1995 to 97%in 1997 while the
proportion of stores containing at least one message in English increased
from45%in 1995 to 50%in 1997. Across the island the proportion of stores
displaying commercial signs only in English declined from2.5%in 1995 to
less than 1% in 1997, while in the West Island of Montreal where
Anglophones are in the majority, this proportion declined from 3.8% in
1995 to 1.5% in 1997. The proportion of stores across Montreal displaying
commercial signs only in French ranged from46%to 49%during the study
130 The Americas
period. Thus after only four years of application, Bill 86 consolidated the
Francisation of the Montreal linguistic landscape while respecting the
rights of linguistic minorities toalsouse their ownlanguage oncommercial
signs.
However, after 15 years of French unilingualism on commercial signs
resultingfromBill 101 andBill 178, small andlarge stores inthe West Island
of Montreal were slow to introduce English bilingualism on their store
signs as permitted within Bill 86. By 1996 militant Xmen within the
Anglophone community began a campaign to boycott department stores
which did not include the proper share of English on their store signs. The
boycott campaignwas successful andmany department stores onthe West
Island changed from unilingual French signs to FrenchEnglish bilingual
ones. However, the 1998 Alliance Quebec campaign to change French
unilingual signs in downtown Montreal through another boycott met the
opposition of French trade unions who threatened a French majority
boycott of downtown department stores if they shifted from unilingual
French signs to bilingual ones. Caught in the cross-fire, downtown depart-
ment store owners revealedthat a 1993 secret deal withthe Liberal Govern-
ment had established a linguistic equilibrium in which commercial signs
would remain unilingual French for the sake of maintaining the linguistic
landscape mainly French in downtown Montreal. Discussions with the
Prime Minister in 1998 made it clear that the Parti Qubcois Government
also wanted to maintain the linguistic equilibrium even if this meant that
downtown store owners would forgo their right to post bilingual sign as
allowed in Bill 86. The ensuing status quo on French unilingual signs in
downtown Montreal divided the Alliance Quebec leadership. The older
guard of the Alliance formed a rival association focused on improving the
position of English through continuing dialogue with the Quebec Govern-
ment. Other Anglophone Xmen stayed within the more militant Alliance
Quebec vowing to pursue their struggle for bilingual commercial signs,
freedom of choice for the language of education of immigrant pupils, and
preparing the pro-partition strategy for the next referendum on Quebec
independence.
The application of the commercial sign law by the OLF and its enforce-
ment by the CPLF gave rise to other highly publicised controversies which
cumulativelydeepenedthe linguistic insecurityfelt bylanguage minorities
in the Province. For example in 1996, on the eve of Passover, the OLF noti-
fiedMontreal grocery stores that they didnot comply with the lawbecause
kosher products on their shelves were labelled in English, Hebrew or
Yiddish only. As a result, a number of food chains removed kosher prod-
ucts from their shelves during Passover. Despite representations by the
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 131
Montreal Jewish community, the OLF had refused to exempt imported
NewYork kosher products fromthe French labelling rule, though the law
did provide exceptions for specialised items with no equivalent replace-
ment product labelled in French within the Quebec market. In 1997 a
long-established Jewish business selling tombstones was singled out by
the CPLF because the old commercial sign was written with Hebrew
lettering more predominant than French. Both these cases were eventu-
ally settled through special agreement with the Ministry in charge of Bill
101. In 1998, CPLF civil servants requested that most of the commercial
signs in Montreals China Town be replaced because Chinese lettering
was more predominant than French ones. The Chinese Chamber of
Commerce vehemently opposedthese requirements claiming that French
was less threatened by the presence of Chinese on commercial signs than
by the influence of English in Quebec and North America as a whole. At
the time of writing this case was still under review by the Conseil de la
langue franaise.
Overall, militant Xmen agreed with CPLF vigilance in applying the
French sign law, while other Francophones saw these interventions as too
harshgiventhe progress already achievedfor Frenchinthe linguistic land-
scape. However, the above language of signs controversies received much
negative publicityinthe Anglophone press not onlyinQuebec andCanada
but also across the USA and Europe. In 1999, the Quebec government
commissioned a Montreal polling firm to conduct focus group studies on
Quebec language laws with influential Americans in New York, Boston,
Atlanta andChicago. The focus group study undertaken with respondents
in the business, academic, tourist and journalismworld showed that these
well-informedAmericans hada negative impressionof Quebecs linguistic
laws. When asked to think about Quebec in general the respondents
mentionedthe Frenchlanguage, the separatist movement andthe determi-
nation of its population to conserve its language and culture. Themes
emerging from the focus group discussions included negative and often
inaccurate perceptions of the linguistic situation in the Province including
the following statements.
Public schools are all inFrench, andEnglish-speaking students only have
access to private or bilingual institutions. English is not permitted on
signs andbreakingsignlaws merits a prisonsentence. Quebecs language
laws are discriminatory and restrictive. Half the respondents said they
would rather work in Toronto than Montreal and business people
would prefer to set up in Toronto to avoid having French imposed on
132 The Americas
them. Those who had visited Quebec expressed an irritation with
French-only signs and bilingual people who refused to speak English.
When asked to comment on the research, the President of the American
Council for Quebec Studies stated that the study provided a fairly accurate
account of what even educated Americans think of Quebec (Montreal
Gazette, 8 May 1999). He noted that extreme cases of language issues are
what usually made it into American newspapers, like the instance
involving Hebrew lettering on signs and packaging. The President of the
Council for Quebec studies concludedthat: the more extreme cases are the
ones that make it into public consciousness, and those are the ones that are
retained.
Whatever counter-publicity the Parti Qubcois Government may
intendto purchase inthe mass media is likely to be quite expensive andnot
as influential inchanging negative perceptions of Quebec language laws as
stories filed by independent or syndicated journalists. Ministers and civil
servants involvedin the controversial applications of the sign lawhave not
only undermined perceptions of the legitimacy of the lawdealing with the
linguistic landscape but also tarnished the reputation of other Bill 101
measures designed to maintain French in Quebec. Furthermore, it is the
case that controversial applications of Quebec sign laws have been turned
against the legitimate goals of language minorities seeking to reverse
language shift in other parts of Canada and in the world: the most imme-
diate case in point being the English only movement in the USA which
regularly uses controversial features of Quebec language laws reported in
the Anglo-American media to justify its campaigns against minority
language maintenance in the US (Bourhis & Marshall, 1999).
Conclusion: RLS in an Intergroup Perspective
Remarkable gains for the French language have been achieved in
Quebec through language laws such as the Charter of the French Language.
As such, even with the advantage of a low intergenerational dislocation
score to start with, Quebec remains a success story of language planning
for reversing language shift (RLS, Fishman, 1991). However, many
Francophones still feel threatened demographically as a minority in North
America while Anglophones feel threatened as a declining minority in
Quebec.
For many Francophone analysts, Bill 101 was salutary, as it did defuse
a potentially explosive clash between a Francophone majority at the
bottom of the labour market and a dominant Anglophone elite at the top
(Rocher, 1992). The 1970 October crisis, which brought the Canadian
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 133
army on Quebec soil to neutralise the activities of the guerilla Front de
Libration du Qubec (FLQ), demonstrated the divisiveness of the class
and ethnolinguistic cleavage existing within Quebec society at that time.
Language policies such as Bill 101 could be seen as one of the democrati-
cally adopted measures which contributed to the institutionalised
management of a class and ethnic conflict threatening to destabilise
Quebec society (Esman, 1987). Thus RLS measures within language laws
can be necessary tools allowing multilingual states to modernise more
harmoniously.
For more militant Quebec nationalists, Bill 101 was perhaps too
successful in reversing language shift, thus reducing the feeling of
linguistic threat fuelling the drive for Quebec independence. Francophone
Xmen also point out the numerous features of the original Bill 101 which
were annulledor neutralisedbyCanadianSupreme Court rulings basedon
the 1982 Constitution and the Charter of Rights and Freedoms (Bourhis,
1994a). Xmen also point out weaknesses in the application of Bill 101 in the
work worldandeducation (Veltman, 1998) while bemoaning the enduring
drawing power of English in the electronic and mass-media, not just in
Montreal but across the Province (Barbeau, 1998). For many Francophone
Xmen, total independence is offered as the ultimate solution which will
allow a future Quebec state to adopt the full range of language laws
deemed necessary to enshrine the status of French, free from Canadian
Government interference with its constitutional protection of individual
rights, the Official Languages Act (Fortier, 1994) and the 1988 Multicultur-
alism law (Fleras & Elliot, 1992).
Combinedwithanalreadydecliningbirthrate, language policies suchas
Bill 101 had a dramatic impact on the group vitality of Quebec Anglo-
phones. To survive as a minority, Anglophones had to transform them-
selves by becoming a bilingual and multicultural community sharing
English as its lingua franca with Allophone and immigrants communities.
However, it is not clear that the strong institutional control presently
enjoyed by Anglophones will be sufficient to compensate for its eroding
demographic base. Quebec Anglophones remain more mobile economi-
cally than Francophones and continue to leave the Province when seeking
better employment within the Anglo-Canadian mainstream. Recent
survey studies also suggest that close to half a million Anglophones would
leave Quebec following a majority YES vote in the next referendum on
Quebec independence.
In contrast, Quebec Allophones are less likely to leave the Province than
Anglophones under such circumstances. With the drawing power of the
two rival host languages neutralising each other, it is in Quebec that one
134 The Americas
finds Allophones with the highest intergenerational transmission rate of
Heritage languages in Canada. It is especially in Montreal that Allophone
and immigrant background individuals can most readily construct
multiple group identities incorporating their own Heritage culture with
twoimportant cultures inthe Westernworld, the Englishandthe French.
Neither Francophones nor Anglophones can count on increased birth-
rate to improve their respective demographic position in the Province.
Likewise, given the difficult economic situation in Quebec, neither
Francophones nor Anglophones can count on attracting immigration from
other Canadian provinces to improve the strength of their respective
linguistic communities. By default, Allophones and international immi-
grants remain the most promising solution for improving the linguistic
position of the two host communities. Both Francophones and Anglo-
phones depend on the linguistic integration of immigrants to boost their
respective linguistic position within the Province. However, Quebecs
average share of international immigrants to Canada has dropped to only
16% of the total immigrant intake in the last decade, despite the fact that
Quebec accounts for 23% of the Canadian population. Without a substan-
tial increase in immigration or fertility rate, demographers expect the
Quebec population to begin its decline within the next two decades
(Castonguay, 1998, 1999).
Traditionally, Qubcois identity has been synonymous with French
mother-tongue background, French ancestry and identification with
French Canadian culture. However, especially in Montreal, the Fran-
cisation of immigrant background pupils through Bill 101 educational
requirements implies that knowing French may be a sufficient condition
for becoming a true Qubcois (Breton, 1988). Tensions arising fromthis
redefinition of who should be and who can be a true Qubcois have been
at the core of a growing debate oncitizenship inQuebec society (Gagnon
et al., 1996; Labelle, 1990; Taylor, 1992). At one pole of the ideological
continuum (Bourhis et al., 1997), the pluralist and civic orientations
uphold that Allophones and immigrant background individuals can be
considered Qubcois as long as they learn and use French as the
language of public life (e.g. at work, in shops, in hospitals) and partici-
pate intheir civic duties andresponsibilities (e.g. pay taxes, obey civil and
criminal laws). Under this definition, Allophone and immigrant back-
ground citizens have no obligation to identify or participate in Qubcois
mainstream cultural and political aspirations nor adopt French as the
language of the home, as these are matters of private individual choice
beyond the reach of the state as stipulated in the Quebec Charter of
Human Rights. On the language front, this civic position is the one
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 135
adopted by the Quebec Government in its official position on French as
the language of public life (Quebec, 1996b). Moreover, for those
espousing the pluralists position, Heritage language maintenance by
Allophones is considered a linguistic capital worth supporting finan-
cially by the state and likely to enrich the cultural and economic dyna-
mism of Quebec society (Fleras & Elliot, 1992).
Towards the other end of the continuum are assimilationists who, in
the Francophone camp, expect Allophones and immigrants to not only
learn French and attend to their civic duties as Quebec citizens but also
expect suchindividuals toespouse as their own the cultural andpolitical
aspirations of the Qubcois mainstream(e.g. adoption of French as the
language of the home, defence of the French language andculture against
English, voting in favour of Quebec sovereignty). In turn, Anglophone
assimilationists expect Allophones also to learn English, identify first as
Canadian federalists and rally with older stock Anglophones in defence
of the English fact in Quebec. Finally, at the extreme end of this
continuum, Francophone exclusionists are those who wouldfindit diffi-
cult to accept as a true Qubcois, immigrant background individuals
who do not have French as a mother-tongue, have no Qubcois French
ancestry and lack identification with genuine Qubcois cultural and
political aspirations. Among Anglophones, exclusionists are most
readily found among right-wing Reform party supporters in Ontario
and western Canada whose views tends to be anti-bilingualism, anti-
immigration and anti-multiculturalism. In Quebec, the Reform Party
commands no support as Anglophones can ill afford to exclude
Allophone and immigrant background individuals likely to join their
ranks for the cause of the English in the Province. However, as endorsed
in the ReformParty platform, there is strong Anglophone support for the
partition of Quebec in the event of a majority YES vote for Quebec separa-
tion from Canada in the next referendum. Given that most Anglophones
are against Quebec separation, a 1996 poll showed that over 80% of
Quebec non-Francophones favour the partition of Quebec allowing
English majority regions of the Province to remain within Canada
following a majority YES vote for separation. In contrast, three-quarters
of Francophones oppose the partition of Quebec following a YES vote for
independence. In retrospect, RLS measures such as Bill 101 can be cred-
ited for having brought to the fore these competing definitions of
Qubcois and Canadian identity in an uncertain federation that may
not survive the next century but may give birth to a number of newstates
in North America. Whatever the outcome of the Quebec independence
debate, it remains true that laws such as the Charter of the French
136 The Americas
language will continue to be vital for the long-term survival of the only
French majority society in North America.
Notes
1. Comments and suggestions concerning this chapter would be much appreci-
ated and should be addressed to: Richard Bourhis, Dpartement de
psychologie, Universit du Qubec Montral, CP 8888, Succ. Centre-Ville,
Montral, Qubec, Canada, H3C 3P8. Tel: (514) 987-3000 ext. 4852; Fax: (514)
987-7953; e-mail; [email protected]
2. The author wishes to thank lOffice de la langue franaise and the Conseil de la
langue franaise for their generous helpinprovidingdocuments andresearchre-
ports needed to write this chapter. The author would also like to thank the
following individuals for their very helpful comments and suggestions on ear-
lier versions of this chapter: Catherine Amiot, Genevive Barrette, Pierre
Bouchard, Charles Castonguay, Martine Cournoyer, Guy Dumas, Andr
Gagnon, Denyse Lemay, Dominique Lepicq, Marie Mc Andrew and Morton
Weinfeld.
3. English caption for Figure 5.2: This is the front page of a right-wing nationalist
weekly magazine entitled La Nation published in 1937. The large headline an-
nounces the creation of a new Quebec autonomist party. The authors
translation of the caption at the top of the picture is: The great threat. The Eng-
lishtranslationof the captionat the bottomof the picture is: What is the use of a
Congress on the French Language if John Bull (the British) seeks to drown us in
a sea of immigration. Francophones canbe seenswimmingtoreachthe precari-
ous raft on which they huddle for survival. In the 1930s the majority of
immigrants settling in Quebec came from the British Isles.
References
Allard, R. and Landry, R. (1986) Subjective ethnolinguistic vitality viewed as a be-
lief system. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 7, 112.
Allard, R. andLandry, R. (1994) Subjective ethnolinguistic vitality: Acomparisonof
two measures. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 108, 11744.
Amiot, C. andBourhis, R.Y. (1999) Ethnicity andFrench-English communication in
Montreal. Poster presented at the 60th Convention of the Canadian Psychological
Association, Halifax, Nova Scotia.
dAnglejan, A. (1984) Language planninginQuebec: Anhistorical overviewandfu-
ture trends. In R. Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp.
2952) Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Barbeau, P. (1998) French in Quebec. In J. Edwards (ed.) Languages in Canada. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Bland, P. (1991) Lusage du franais au travail: situation et tendances. Qubec: Conseil
de la Langue francaise. Montral: Office de la langue franaise.
Bland, P. (1999) Le franais, langue dusage public au Qubec en 1997. Qubec: Conseil
de la langue franaise.
Bouchard, P. (1991) Les enjeux de la francisation des entreprises au Qubec 19771984.
Montral: Office de la langue franaise.
Bouchard, P. (1998) La francisation des entreprises au Qubec: De la difficult rela-
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 137
tive dhier la complexit de demain. Colloque du Comit scientifique du rseau
Sociolinguistique et dynamique des langues de lAgence universitaire de la
Francophonie. Rabat: Maroc.
Bourhis, R.Y. (1979) Language and ethnic interaction: A social psychological ap-
proach. In H. Giles and B. Saint-Jacques (eds) Language and Ethnic Relations.
Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Bourhis, R. (1982) Language policies and language attitudes: Le monde de la
Francophonie. In E.B. Ryan and H. Giles (eds) Attitudes Towards Language Varia-
tion (pp. 3462) London: Edward Arnold.
Bourhis, R. (1984a) Language policies in multilingual settings. In R. Bourhis (ed)
Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 128). Clevedon: Multilingual Mat-
ters.
Bourhis, R.Y. (1984b) Cross-cultural communication in Montreal: Two field
studies since Bill 101. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46,
3347.
Bourhis, R.Y. (1994a) Introduction and overviewof language events in Canada. In-
ternational Journal of the Sociology of Language 105106, 536.
Bourhis, R.Y. (1994b) Ethnic and language attitudes in Quebec. In J. Berry and J.
Laponce (eds) Ethnicity and Culture in Canada: The Research Landscape (pp.
322360). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
Bourhis, R.Y., Giles, H. andRosenthal, D. (1981) Notes onthe constructionof a Sub-
jective vitality questionnaire for ethnolinguistic groups. Journal of Multilingual
and Multicultural Development 2, 14555.
Bourhis, R.Y. and Lepicq, D. (1993) Qubcois French and language issues in Que-
bec. In R. Posner andJ.N. Green (eds) Trends in Romance Linguistics and Philology,
Volume 5: Bilingualism and Linguistic Conflict in Romance (pp. 345381). The
Hague and Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bourhis, R.Y. and Marshall, D. (1999) The United States and Canada. In J. Fishman
(ed.) Handbook of Language and Ethnic Identity (pp. 24464). Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Bourhis, R.Y., Moise, C., Perreault, S. and Sencal, S. (1997) Towards an interactive
acculturation model: A social psychological approach. International Journal of
Psychology 32, 36986.
Bourhis, R.Y. and Sachdev, I. (1984) Vitality perceptions and language attitudes.
Journal of Language and Social Psychology 3, 97126.
Breton, R. (1971) Institutional completeness of ethnic communities andpersonal re-
lations of immigrants. InB.R. Blishen, F.E. Jones, K.D. Naegels andJ. Porter (eds)
Canadian Society: Sociological Perspectives. Toronto: Macmillan.
Breton, R. (1988) Fromethnic tocivic nationalism: EnglishCanada andQuebec. Eth-
nic and Racial Studies 11, 85102.
Caldwell, G. (1984) Anglo-Quebec: Demographic realities and options for the fu-
ture. In R.Y. Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 20521).
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Caldwell, G. (1994) English Quebec: Demographic and cultural reproduction. In
R.Y. Bourhis (ed) French-English language issues in Canada. International Jour-
nal of the Sociology of Language 105106, 15379.
Caldwell, G. (1998) English Quebec. In J. Edwards (ed.) Language in Canada (pp.
177201). New York: Cambridge University Press.
138 The Americas
Caldwell, G. and Fournier, D. (1987) The Quebec question: Amatter of population.
Canadian Journal of Sociology 12, 1641.
Canada, (1969) Royal Commission on Bilingualismand Biculturalism, Book III: The Work
World. Ottawa: The Queens Printer.
Castonguay, C. (1997) volution de lassimilation linguistique au Qubec et au
Canada entre 1971 et 1991. Recherches sociographiques 38, 46990.
Castonguay, C. (1998) The fading Canadian duality. In J. Edwards (ed.) Language in
Canada (pp. 3660). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Castonguay, C. (1999) Gettingthe facts straight onFrench: Reflections followingthe
1996 Census. Inroads: A Journal of Opinion 8, 5776.
CDP (1998) Les programmes daccs lgalit au Qubec: Bilan et perspectives. Qubec:
Commission des droits de la personne et des droits de la jeunesse.
Daoust, D. (1984) Francization and terminology change in Quebec business firms.
In R.Y. Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 81113).
Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
Drapeau, L. (1998) Aboriginal languages: Current status. In J. Edwards (ed.). Lan-
guage in Canada (pp. 14459). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Esman, M.J. (1987) Ethnic politics and economic power. Comparative Politics 19,
395418.
Fishman, J. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Fishman, J. (1999) (ed.) Handbook of Language and Ethnic Identity. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Fleras, A. andElliot, J.L. (1992) The Challenge of Diversity: Multiculturalismin Canada.
Scarborough, Ontario: Nelson Canada.
Fortier, I. (1994) Official language policies in Canada: A quiet revolution. Interna-
tional Journal of the Sociology of Language 105106, 6997.
Gagnon, F., McAndrew, M. andPag, M. (1996) Introduction. Pluralisme, citoyennet
et ducation (pp. 1321). Paris, Montral: LHarmattan.
Gauthier, C. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1993) Les qubcois et la question linguistique et plus
particulirement la langue daffichage commercial: Sondage dopinion auprs des
qubcois. Montral: Crop inc. et Dpartement de psychologie, UQAM.
Genesee, F. andBourhis, R.Y. (1988) Evaluative reactions to language choice strat-
egies: The role of sociostructural factors. Language and Communication 8,
22950.
Genesee, F. and Holobow, N. (1989) Change and stability in intergroup percep-
tions. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 8, 1738.
Giles, H., Bourhis, R.Y. andTaylor, D. (1977) Towards a theoryof language inethnic
group relations. In H. Giles (ed.) Language, Ethnicity and Intergroup Relations.
London: Academic Press.
Giroux, L. (1992) Les adolescents montralais et la tlvision de langue francaise.
Montral: Universit de Montral.
Hamers, J. and Hummel, K. (1994) The francophones of Quebec: Language policies
and language use. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 105106,
12752.
Hamers, J. and Hummel, K. (1998) Language in Quebec: Aboriginal and Heritage
varieties. In J. Edwards (ed.) Language in Canada (pp. 38499). New York: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Harwood, J., Giles, H. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1994) The genesis of vitality theory: His-
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 139
torical patterns anddiscoursal dimensions. International Journal of the Sociology of
Language 108, 167206.
Johnson, P., Giles, H. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1983) The viability of ethnolinguistic vital-
ity: A reply. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 4, 25569.
Labelle, M. (1990) Immigration, culture et question nationale. Cahiers de Recherche
sociologique 4, 14351.
Lachapelle, A. and Henripin, J. (1980) La Situation Dmolinguistique au Canada.
Montral: Institut de Recherches Politiques.
Lambert, W.E., Hodgson, J., Gardner, R. and Fillenbaum, S. (1960) Evaluational
reactions to spoken languages. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60,
4451.
Landry, R., Allard, R. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1997) Profil sociolanguagiers de jeunes
Francophones et Anglophones du Qubec en fonction de la vitalit des
communauts linguistiques. In G. Budach and J. Erfurt (eds) Identit franco-
canadienne et socit civile qubcoise (pp. 12350). Leipig: Leipziger
Universitatsverlag.
Landry, R. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1997) Linguistic landscape and ethnolinguistic vital-
ity: An empirical study. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 16, 2349.
Laporte, P. (1984) Status of language planning in Quebec: An evaluation. In R.Y.
Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 5380). Clevedon:
Multilingual Matters.
Legault, J. (1992) Linvention dune minorit: Les anglo-Qubcois. Montral: Boral.
Levine, M. (1990) The Reconquest of Montreal: Language Policy and Social Change in a
Bilingual City. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Mallea, J. (1984) Minority language education in Quebec and Anglophone Canada.
In R.Y. Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 22260).
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Maurais, J. (1987) Lexprience qubcoise damnagement linguistique. In J.
Maurais (ed.) Politique et amnagement linguistique (pp. 359416). Qubec and
Paris: Conseil de la Langue francaise, Le Robert.
Maurais, J. (1997) (ed.) The Indigenous Languages of Qubec. Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters.
McAndrew, M. (1996) Ethnicity inQuebec schools. Pluralismand Education: Proceed-
ings of the Conference onPluralismand Cultural Enrichment inEducation(pp. 6584).
Dublin, Ireland: University of Ulster.
Miller, R. (1984) The response of business firms to the Francization Process. In R.Y.
Bourhis (ed.) Conflict and Language Planning in Quebec (pp. 11429). Clevedon:
Multilingual Matters.
Moise, L.C. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1994) Langage et ethnicit: Communication
interculturelle a Montral, 19771991. Canadian Ethnic Studies 26, 86107.
OLF (1998) Office de la langue franaise: Rapport annuel 19971998. Montral: Les pub-
lications du Qubec.
Pendakur, R. (1990) Speaking in Tongues: Heritage Language Maintenance and Transfer
in Canada. Ottawa: Department of Multiculturalism and Citizenship.
Quebec (1972) (Gendron Commission). Report of the Commission of Enquiry on the Po-
sition of the French Language and on Language Rights in Quebec. Qubec: Editeur
Officiel du Qubec.
Quebec (1996a) Le franais langue commune. Rapport du comit interministriel sur la
140 The Americas
situationde la langue franaise. Qubec: Directiondes communications, ministere
de la Culture et des communications (319 pp).
Quebec (1996b) Le franais langue commune: Proposition de politique linguistique. Qu-
bec: Direction des communications, ministre de la Culture et des
Communications (77 pp).
Quebec (1997) Lvolution de la situation de laffichage Montral de 1995 1997. Qu-
bec; Conseil de la langue francaise. (http//www.clf.gouv.qc.ca).
Rocher, G. (1992) Autour de la langue: crises et dbats, espoir et tremblement. In G.
Daigle and G. Rocher (eds) Le Qubec en Jeu: Comprendre les grands dfits (pp.
42350). Montral: Les Presses de lUniversit de Montal.
Ryan, E.B. and Giles, H. (eds) (1982) Attitudes Towards Language Variation. London:
Edward Arnold.
Sachdev, I. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1990) Language and social identification. In D.
Abrams and M. Hogg (eds) Social Identity Theory: Constructive and Critical Ad-
vances. New York: Harvester/Wheatsheaf.
Sachdev, I. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1991) Power and status differentials in minority and
majority group relations. European Journal of Social Psychology 21, 124.
Sachdev, I. andBourhis, R.Y. (1993) Ethnolinguistic vitality: Some motivational and
cognitive considerations. In M. Hogg and D. Abrams (eds) Group Motivation: A
Social Psychological Perspective (pp. 3352). New York: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.
Scowen, R. (1991) A Different Vision: The English in Quebec in the 1990s. Don Mills,
Ontario: Maxwell Macmillan Canada.
Stevenson, G. (1999) Community Besieged: The Anglophone Minority and the Politics of
Quebec. Montreal: McGill-Queens University Press.
Taylor, C. (1992) The politics of recognition. In A. Gutmann (ed.) Multiculturalism
and the Politics of Recognition (pp. 2573). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Termote, P. and Gauvreau, D. (1988) La Situation Dmolinguistique au Qubec. Qu-
bec: Conseil de la Langue Franaise.
Vallire, P. (1969) Ngres blancs damrique. Paris: Maspro.
Veltman, C. (1998) Quebec, Canada and the United States: Social reality and lan-
guage rights. In T. Ricento and B. Burnaby (eds) Language and Politics in the
United States and Canada: Myths and Realities (pp. 30115). Mahwah, NJ: Law-
rence Erlbaum Publishers.
de Vries, J. (1994) Canadas official language communities: An overviewof the cur-
rent demolinguistic situation. International Journal of the Sociology of Language
105106, 3768.
Wardhaugh, R. (1987) Languages in Competition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Reversing Language Shift in Quebec 141
Chapter 6
Otom Language Shift and Some
Recent Efforts to Reverse It
Y. LASTRA
The purpose of this chapter is to report on the language shift of Otom,
which started a long time ago, and some recent local efforts to reverse the
trend. Otom is spoken in central Mexico. It belongs to the Oto-Pamean
familywhichincludes three branches, Pame-Chichimeco, Otom-Mazahua
andMatlatzinca-Ocuilteco. The family belongs to the Oto-Manguean stock
which consists of 10 families. Among its better-known languages are
Zapotec and Mixtec. Most of the languages, except the Oto-Pamean ones,
are located in the state of Oaxaca. Mangue, now extinct, was spoken in
Nicaragua. The stockdoes not extendtothe northas does the Yuto-Aztecan
one; the northernmost Oto-Manguean language is Pame. An interesting
fact about these languages is that Pame and Chichimec are located outside
of Mesoamerica while the rest have belonged to the culture area for a long
time.
Historical Background
The Otom have livedincentral Mexicofor manycenturies. We will refer
to their ancient history, whichmay helpexplainwhat the situationwas like
at the time of the Spanish Conquest. That period, in turn, has had an influ-
ence upon the present-day situation. The Otom were agriculturists and
were probably one of the various ethnic groups present in Teotihuacan,
1
the largest andmost important ancient city of Mexico, which flourishedfor
several centuries fromaround200 to 800 AD. They were certainly present in
Tula, another important city, which goes back to the 8th century of our era
and flourished from 9501150/1200. After the fall of Tula the Otom are
mentioned explicitly in historical documents. The legendary king Xlotl
gave an extensive territory to an Otom leader and the kingdom of
Xaltocan
2
was established in the early 13th century.
When the Mexica (later known as Aztecs) were on their pilgrimage to
foundMexicotheylivedseveral years inChapultepec
3
andthe first tomake
142
The Americas
Otomi Language Shift
war against themwere the people of Xaltocan. This was the beginning of a
long enmity between the two peoples. The Otom kingdom continued to
flourish until the end of the 14th century when it fell to the Mexica-domi-
nated alliance (Torquemada, 1969). Its destruction marks the end of the
political importance of the Otom. Part of the population fled toward the
east and south. Subsequently the Mexica-dominated alliance acquired
their land. The result was an increase of the Nahuatl element in Otom
regions. Subsequently the Mexica included Otoms in the movement of
peoples which they carried out in order to consolidate their power in the
territories they kept adding to their empire by means of war. As the Mexica
conquered more and more territory they completely subjected the Otom
who had to pay tribute to them.
There were other territories in central Mexico which the Otom also
inhabited, but they were gradually forced to migrate to the least desirable
lands and the population of Nahuatl speakers increased in the former
Otom territories. By the time of the arrival of the Spanish, however, the
Otom still occupied three residential sections of Mexico City. To the north
of the Mexica capital Otom was also spoken and its speakers predomi-
nated in the region to the north of the Basin of Mexico, which is roughly
equivalent to the Mezquital Valley in the state of Hidalgo. There were
Otom towns inparts of the fertile lands of the Huasteca
4
area as well. Inthe
highlands of Puebla there were also Otom speakers but they shared the
territory with speakers of Nahuatl and Totonac. Some Otom lived in the
area between Tetzcoco (Mexico) and Tulancingo (Hidalgo). Tlaxcala was
Nahuatl-dominated by the 16th century but there were also Otom chiefly
to the east of the Malinche mountains. In Michoacn they reached as far as
the Balsas river to the southeast of the present-day western states of Jalisco
and Colima (Carrasco, 1950; Lastra, 1992a).
The extent of Otom territory denotes their former importance. They
settled before the Mexica who were the last to arrive in Mesoamerica from
the north. Other speakers of Nahuatl had settled in parts of the centre of
Mexico long before the Mexica did and they shared some territories with
the Otom. Even today it is possible to find contiguous towns where one of
the languages is spokeninone townandthe other inthe townnext toit. The
people celebrate the same fiestas and trade with one another, but there is
little intermarriage and the ethnic groups maintain their separate identi-
ties.
In former times the Otom seemto have transmittedmany cultural traits
to the Mexica, but the latter have never acknowledged this. It is well-
known that they had codices burnt so that people would not knowsome of
the facts of their own history. The indebtedness to the Otom must have
Otomi Language Shift 143
gone up in smoke! Fromthen on history could be re-written and the Otom
were depicted as lowly, despicable and stupid. This view was subse-
quently passed on to the Spanish conquerors, who swallowed the Mexica
story without much reflection. Fray Bernardino de Sahagn, who enjoys
the reputation of being the first ethnographer, was a Franciscan Friar who
took it upon himself to find out everything he could about the religion and
the culture that he and his fellow-countrymen were intent upon destroy-
ing. He gatheredinformation fromelders who belongedto the Mexica elite
and consequently the Otom were portrayed in the most disparaging
terms.
Ancient Mexicanhistorythus has a bearingonthe present-daylanguage
situation because one of the reasons why some Otom are shifting their
language is due to the reputation fabricated by the Mexica after the fall of
Xaltocan in the 14th century, a reputation believed and repeated by the
Spaniards and later on by Spanish-speaking Mexicans.
Nahuatl seems to have been a lingua franca even before the establish-
ment of the Mexica (Aztec) empire. The names of people and places, for
instance, have usually come down to us in that language. The majority of
the names of the municipalities and towns that we mention below in
connection with the statistics of language shift have a Nahuatl origin. After
the Mexica rose topower their language became evenmore prestigious and
bilingualism (local language/Nahuatl) seems to have been common at
least among the elites. The Otom who fled to other lands when the Mexica
destroyed their towns often but not always lost their language and their
identity.
Tlaxcala was an independent state, which the Mexica never conquered
and there were many Otom there, chiefly as defenders of the frontiers.
Tlaxcaltecans and Tlaxcala Otoms joined Corts against the Mexica not
realisingthat after a while their contributionwouldbe forgotten. But due to
the Conquest the Otom were able to expand and be conquerors them-
selves. They foundedthe town of Quertaro andother towns in that region
and in what was to become the state of Guanajuato. These migrants had
already beenconvertedto Catholicismandseemto have acceptedthe Faith
willingly. Nevertheless many ancient customs still survive both among
them and among the Otom who never moved from their former settle-
ments. Galinier (1979, 1990) the best modern ethnographer of the group,
describes many customs and beliefs of the Puebla highlands and is able to
reconstruct their world-view which maintains their ancient philosophy of
life together with some apparently Christian ways.
With the Spanish Conquest the encomienda was instituted. It was a
system which supposedly helped the Indians but which permitted the
144 The Americas
person in charge of a group to utilise their labour. Some towns were called
Indian Republics and there the Indians could govern themselves, and in
many cases aspects of pre-Hispanic organisation were preserved.
During the colonial period, aside from the regions where there were
Otom speakers interspersedwithNahuatl andSpanishspeakers, the main
areas where the language was spoken were the newly settled ones in
Guanajuato and Quertaro, the Mezquital valley, the highlands area of
Puebla, Veracruz, and Hidalgo, the Toluca valley and two enclaves, one in
Michoacn and the other in Tlaxcala. Most of these people were farmers.
The comments that follow refer mostly to the Mezquital valley. The
lands there were so dry that the people also hired themselves as day
labourers and they made use of agave to produce syrups and a traditional
fermented drink called pulque. At first the colonial government forbade
pulque, but later onit taxedit. Richlandowners quicklytookover this busi-
ness and the Otom were forced to use the agave drink only for their own
use. Agave fibres and those of other plants were used to make cords,
baskets and other products, which they sold through intermediaries who
profited the most. They also cut wood, made charcoal and lime.
The Spaniards developedthe silver mines inthe neighbourhood, but not
many Otom became miners unless the Spaniards forced them to. The
Spanish speakers owned the best lands and they used them for cattle
raising.
During the War of Independence the Otoms were almost always on the
side of independence because they resented the Spaniards who had taken
their lands. With independence the Mezquital was divided and sold to
native descendants of the Spaniards. The Indians continued to be hired
hands.
During the Revolution against the dictator Porfirio Daz the Otoms of
the Mezquital didnot fight for land. Theystill have a marginal positionand
they attribute their situation to illiteracy and to the lack of help from the
government but not to their being exploited by non-Otoms.
Otom participation in politics has been non-existent until recent years.
In the Mezquital a government agency set up in the 1940s and 1950s was
supposedto educate andhelpthe Indians economically, but it didnt really
succeed. The people still farma little, work as day labourers and make use
of the products which they gather. It is an economy of subsistence without
profits or capital. All this within a capitalistic system where labour and
land are irrationally exploited by those in control of the economy. Agricul-
ture may be profitable where there is irrigation, but Indian lands mostly do
not have irrigation (Mendizabal, 1947; Nolasco, 1966).
There have been confrontations between family and school and the
Otomi Language Shift 145
influence of the family has diminished and bilingualism has increased.
Religion has diminished in its importance particularly where there is irri-
gation. Traditional celebrations give wayto dances, sports andcommercia-
lisation. In many places Evangelism has been introduced. The converts
read the Bible in Spanish and comment on it in Otom (Franco and Manuel,
1992).
Since independence the government in general adopted an attitude of
looking down upon all living Indians while admiring pre-Hispanic monu-
ments and art particularly Aztec and Maya ones. Thus the lack of prestige
of the Otom continued. This attitude was even adopted by archaeologists
who did not begin to try to look for evidence of ancient Otom way of life
until very recently. It is definitely the attitude of school teachers even of the
so-called bilingual ones. Mexico is supposedly a pluricultural state. These
are high-sounding words, but there is very little evidence of its existence.
The mainstream dominates and permeates every aspect of life even in
remote towns where television and radio come with electricity, Western
medicine with government clinics, Spanish with literacy. Other factors
contributing to language shift are migration to cities, work in cities away
from Otom centres, temporary migration to the United States, commerce,
Spanish-speaking migrants who come to live in indigenous communities,
and so on. Of course, reversing language shift does not mean the denial of
modernity, but Otom is associated with backwardness not only in the
minds of government officials but also inthe minds of some of the speakers
themselves.
However, a lot of non-material culture is still preserved. Otom songs
are heardwhen people go upthe Hueyamelucan hill (in the state of Mexico
about 80 kilometres from Mexico City) on Saturday nights to pray; I
collecteda prayer to the Earth Mother in Guanajuato where the language is
practically extinct; tales are still known by elders and enjoyed by young
people who dont have television and still understand the language;
Galinier has been able to reconstruct Otom philosophy, but I doubt that
ordinary Otom know their history, not even the summary I have given
above. At school Mexicanhistoryincludes a fewwords about the Olmecs, a
few more about Teotihuacan, some Aztec history, and students are told
that the Maya knew the concept of zero, had a writing system and built
beautiful buildings, but not much else. Otoms are weak and despised but
persistent. If they were told about their history and if non-Otom, who do
not understand their culture or their needs were to stop managing their
education perhaps the language and the culture could survive. We will
come back to the issue of education later on.
146 The Americas
Language Shift
In this section the appalling statistics of language shift are reviewed.
According to the last census (1990) there are 280,238 speakers of Otom.
This includes monolinguals and bilinguals. Their number is lower than it
was in 1980 when there were 306,190 speakers. The territory where the
language is spokenhas shrunkandthe percentage of Otom speakers to the
populationof eachmunicipalityis smaller thanit was before (Lastra, 1994).
Let us consider only the censuses taken since independence. The first
was taken in 1895. The territory of each state was divided into districts
which later were subdivided into municipalities (roughly equivalent to
counties). The language habitually used by the people was given for each
district together with the total population. The next census was taken in
1900 when the division into municipalities had been made, but luckily the
names of the former districts are also given which makes some compari-
sons possible.
Fromsuch comparisons we note that according to the 1895 census there
were speakers of Otom ineight states including SanLuis Potos whichhad
six districts with a small Otom population. The district with the largest
population was Tamazunchale, situated near the border of Quertaro; it
only had 204 speakers of Otom. By the time of the 1900 census we find no
speakers left in this district. The district of Tancanhuitz, which had 175
speakers according to the former census shows an increase in the absolute
number of speakers (388), but the figure amounts to less than 1%.
The 1910 census has no information on languages. The next census was
taken in 1921 and it lists only total numbers of Otom speakers for states.
The 1930 census only registers monolinguals. Comparison of the figures in
these two censuses shows that the number of monolinguals is smaller in
1930 than the total number of speakers was previously, but we are left with
many questions since the figures are not really comparable. The 1940 and
the 1950 censuses asked if people spoke an Indian language, but without
specifying which one.
The following census, that is the one for 1960, lists the information by
municipality giving the number of bilinguals andmonolinguals older than
five years andthe name of the language. The format of the censuses has not
changed significantly since then, which simplifies comparison. On the
other hand, the comparison with the 1900 figures is much harder. In Table
6.1 I convertedthe census figures into percentages inorder to showthat the
proportion of speakers of Otom has progressively diminished even
though in some cases absolute numbers have increased. One has to keep in
mind that the 1900 census does not specify age, while the 19601990
Otomi Language Shift 147
148 The Americas
Table 6.1 Comparison of percentages of Otom population by municipal-
ity: based on census figures for 19001990
1900 1960 1970 1980 1990
Guanajuato
1. Allende 7% 2% 1%
2. Apaseo el Alto 3%
3. Comonfort 29% 1%
4. Cortazar 1%
5. Dolores 2% 1%
6. Villagrn 1%
Quertaro
1. Amealco 28% 36% 25% 32% 28%
2. Cadereyta 36% 9% 6% 5% 2%
3. Peamiller 6%
4. Quertaro 4%
5. San Juan del Ro 3%
6. Tequisquiapan 5%
7. Tolimn 67% 33% 24% 35% 28%
Hidalgo
1. Actopan 28% 66% 13% 11% 6%
2. Alfajayucan 66% 53% 31% 33% 25%
3. Arenal 14% ? 3% 3% 1%
4. Atotonilco el Grande 7% 2%
5. Cardonal 54% 78% 47% 71% 66%
6. Chilcuautla 62% 50% 70% 67% 53%
7. Franciso I. Madero 11% 9% 6% 3%
8. Huautla 24%
9. Huehuetla 38% 70% 45% 44% 55%
10. Huichapan 11% 17% 3% 2%
11. Itzmiquilpan 82% 96% 56% 63% 54%
12. Metztitln 14% 12% 13% 12%
13. Mixquiahuala 38% 5% 4% 2%
14. Nicols Flores 96% 70% 64% 59%
15. San Bartolo
Tutotepec
52% 68% 47% 45% 45%
16. San Salvador 78% 39% 44% 36% 29%
17. Santiago 82% 93% 56% 70% 58%
18. Tasquillo 63% 60% 51% 55% 47%
Otomi Language Shift 149
1900 1960 1970 1980 1990
19. Tecozautla 44% 19% 12% 13% 7%
20. Tenango de Doria 41% 36% 23% 35% 32%
21. Tepeji del Ro 10% 5% 6% 6%
22. Tepetitln 9% 4% 1%
23. Tezontepec 2% 5%
24. Tlaxcoapan 12%
25. Tula 11%
26. Tulancingo 8% 35% 3% 3%
27. Zimapn 41% 50% 25% 24% 14%
Puebla
1. Chila Honey 5% 11% 11% 9%
2. Metlatoyuca 15% 1% 1% 1%
3. Jalpan 22% 4% 3% 3%
4. Pantepec 33% 12% 13% 13%
5. Tlaxco 8% 8% 5%
6. Venustiano Carranza 1% 2% 2%
7. Huauchinango 20%
8. Pahuatln 17% 19%
9. Xicotepec 4%
10. Tlahuilotepec 6%
11. Naupan 4%
Veracruz
1. Huayacocotla 22% 5%
2. Ixhuatln 31% 69% 10% 15% 14%
3. Tlachichilco 38% 31% 9% 16% 17%
4. Zacualpan 50% 9% 2% 3% 1%
5. Zontecomatln 40% 45% 12% 14% 11%
6. Espinal 1%
7. Temapache 11%
8. Texcatepec 58% 38% 75% 72%
9. Tihuatln 6%
Michoacn
1. Zitcuaro 9% 4% 1%
Mxico
Table 6.1 (cont.) Comparison of percentages of Otom population by mu-
nicipality: based on census figures for 19001990
censuses consider the populationwhichis five years or older. The table lists
municipalities with a number which is keyed to Figure 6.1.
5
In the table we
list only those municipalities which have 1% Otom population or above.
The figure shows other municipalities not includedin the table which have
a very low number of speakers.
Examining the data presented in Table 6.1 one can see that the percent-
150 The Americas
1900 1960 1970 1980 1990
1. Acambay 22% 35% 29% 30% 23%
2. Aculco 3% 22% 14% 17% 9%
3. Amanalco 23% 18% 10%
4. Capulhuac 6%
5. Chapa de Mota 18% 54% 37% 29% 23%
6. Huixquilucan 26% 15% 4% 2% 1%
7. Jilotepec 3% 1%
8. Jilotzingo 1%
9. Jiquipilco 11% 27% 29% 27% 16%
10. Lerma 39% 16% 8% 5%
11. San Bartolo Morelos 50% 66% 47% 42% 31%
12. Naucalpan 6% 5% 1%
13. Nicols Romero 11% 2%
14. Ocoyoacac 42% 2% 6% 2% 1%
15. Otzolotepec 46% 28% 25% 25% 13%
16. Polotitln 3%
17. San Jos Malacatepec 11%
18. Temascalcingo 7% 1% 5% 4% 2%
19. Temoaya 71% 66% 58% 55% 46%
20. Tianguistenco 8% 4% 2% 2%
21. Timilpan 23% 17% 15% 10% 7%
22. Tlalnepantla 1%
23. Toluca 2% 9% 7% 4% 4%
24. Villa del Carbn 6% 7% 4% 1%
25. Xonacatln 29% 14% 8% 7% 5%
26. Zinacantepec 33% 3% 1% 1%
Tlaxcala
1. Ixtenco 54% 13% 24% 18% 14%
Table 6.1 (cont.) Comparison of percentages of Otom population by mu-
nicipality: based on census figures for 19001990
Otomi Language Shift 151
F
i
g
u
r
e
6
.
1
P
r
e
s
e
n
t
-
d
a
y
d
i
s
t
r
i
b
u
t
i
o
n
o
f
O
t
o
m
s
p
e
a
k
e
r
s
(
1
9
9
0
)
ages of Otom speakers diminish as the years go by, so much so that some
municipalities which had speakers at the beginning of the century no
longer have them. The present-day distribution of the Otom population
can be seen in Figure 6.1.
6
The figure includes municipalities which have
between 20 and 99 speakers; 100999; 10004999; over 5000. It is easy to see
that communities with a very low number of speakers are located in the
municipalities that only have between20 and99 speakers. Suchmunicipal-
ities appear inthe figure because their number is verylarge andtheyare the
ones where the language will probablydisappear withina generationor so.
In Guanajuato there were six municipalities in 1900 which had between
1%and29%, nowonlyone remains andit has 1%. All of the speakers are old
people, which means that after their generation passes away the language
will be gone in that state.
In Quertaro at the beginning of the century there were seven munici-
palities with Otom population (between 67% and 3%), and now there are
three, two with 28% and the other with 2%.
Hidalgo had 27 municipalities where the language was spoken in 1900;
now there are 21, 3 of them with only 1%. In 1900 13 of the municipalities
hadover 30%speakers of the language, nowonly9of themhave that many.
Puebla and Veracruz have suffered many changes in the nomenclature
because of the creation of new municipalities so that the comparison is
difficult to make.
InZitcuaro, Michoacnthe Otom populationwas 9%andnowit is less
than 1% and the speakers are all old people.
In the state of Mexico at the beginning of the century 26 municipalities
had over 1% Otom population; now there are 18 and 4 of them have only
1%. The highest percentage was 71% and now it is 46%.
In Ixtenco, Tlaxcala, Otom is no longer spoken by young people, only
14%of the population speaks it and they are all over 60 while in 1900 more
than half of the population spoke the language.
As far as knowing whether the number of speakers has dropped
because the number of speakers who can speak has decreased or because
the number who want to speak has decreased (Fishman: 1991:49) is
concerned, unfortunately the latter is probably what has happened owing
to centuries of hearing that they are inferior, because of their bad experi-
ences in school anddue to the necessity of finding jobs outside the commu-
nity for which Spanish is necessary and the mistaken view that
bilingualism is difficult and it is better to shift completely to Spanish.
Schooling in Spanish is probably the main cause of language shift. In
colonial times, friars generally used Indian languages to evangelise and
educate Indians, but after independence public education in Spanish was
152 The Americas
instituted throughout the country. Language policy has changed from
administration to administration, but the main language of instruction has
remained Spanish. At present, education is being decentralised, meaning,
theoretically, that states will have more control over education than the
federal government: but even so the Ministry of Education still has ample
control. In indigenous communities parents can register their children in
regular schools or in so-called bilingual schools. The latter are managed by
an agency of the ministry called Direccin General de Educacin Indgena
(DGEI). It has fewresources, politics plays a large part in its appointments,
and consequently it has not been very successful in maintaining indige-
nous languages. Poor administration results, for instance, in cases where
bilingual teachers in language A are assigned to community Z.
Language shift has also occurred because of human intervention. For
instance, in San Miguel Allende, Guanajuato, a dam was built in the 1970s
and people of many small villages were relocated. This contributed to the
abandonment of the language. In an area of the valley of Toluca, some 40
years ago, water was pumped from a marshy area which was part of the
source of the river Lerma in order to add to the supply of Mexico City. A
whole way of life thus came to an end: no more gathering of many edible
weeds, no more fishing, or collecting a type of local shrimp. As the story
goes, a mermaid (an ancient water goddess) who had protected her chil-
dren from time immemorial left and the water dried up. Now the people
farmthere but theyare nostalgic about their lost resources anddonot speak
Otom among themselves because even though some adults still speak the
language, most of their children do not.
Another cause of language shift is the literal invasionof their territoryby
speakers of Spanish. Members of the Otom minorityare sociallydisadvan-
taged, that is less economically and educationally fortunate than the
Spanish-speaking population surrounding them. In many cases they have
been peripheralised even while remaining in their traditional areas of resi-
dence (Fishman, 1991: 59).
Others who emigrate to Toluca, Pachuca, Puebla, Tulancingo, or Mexico
City are worse-off. If they are lucky they can find housing near other
Otoms, but they have to work in a completely Spanish-speaking environ-
ment and their children will not even learn the parental language. Still
others migrate or seek temporary jobs in the United States where their
lingua franca will be Spanish.
They are poor andless educatedthan the average Spanish monolingual.
Their language is the symbol of backwardness, but abandoning the
language is even worse because they lose their identity and their culture.
Onthe other hand, there are many cases of people who succeedinclimbing
Otomi Language Shift 153
the social ladder, such as the relative of an old Otom fromIxtenco who is a
successful lawyer in Puebla, andthe son of another man who is a teacher in
Toluca. These people have no regrets, but they could have been just as
successful if in addition to acquiring Spanish they had kept their former
language.
Economic mobility requiring the official language can and should be
available to those Otoms who wish to acquire Spanish but retain their
language andculture (cf. Fishman, 1991: 63). Some people will prefer tojoin
the mainstream, but those who want to preserve their language and be
bilingual should have that option. Attitudes of teachers and other main-
stream people have to be changed. The trouble is the Otom in general are
so poor that they can devote very little time to RLS efforts. Also, they are
used to having schools, libraries, sports and such be managed by the
government and therefore in Spanish and not by the indigenous people.
Mexico is, constitutionally, a federal republic, but in practice the central
government in Mexico City has much more power than the state govern-
ments. Currently some changes are being made and supposedly state
governments are gaining more control. In turn, municipalities ought to
gain more control over local matters and this apparently has become
possible in many cases. But if people want to be involved in local govern-
ment, they have to participate, and again, many of the speakers of indige-
nous languages do not because of their poverty and lack of education, and
also because it has been customary for the local mestizos (usually monolin-
gual inthe official language) not totake themintoaccount for anydecisions
that have to be made.
Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale
As Fishman (1991) argues, intergenerational mother-tongue transmis-
sion is essential if a language is to be maintained. In order to help people
interestedinreversinglanguage shift he sets upa typologycalledthe GIDS
scale. In this chapter this scale serves as a useful point for comparison. In
what follows the Otom areas going roughly from north to south will be
given a GIDS scale.
In Guanajuato the language is practically extinct. No children learn it.
Only a fewscattered old people knowit (in some fewcases, very well). No
one has made a detailed study of the situation, but I am basing these
comments on my own experience. San Miguel Allende is the only munici-
pality where 1%of the population speaks the language. The speakers may
be a handful in each village so that most of the villages would be classified
as Stage 8 (vestigial use; language needs documentation) onthe GIDSscale.
154 The Americas
Perhaps Don Francisco is in Stage 7. (Most users of the language are
ethnolinguistically active, but they are beyond child-bearing age.) There
are noRLS efforts apparent inthe municipality. The dialect has hardlybeen
described except for a few remarks in Lastra (199495). There are two old
people in the municipality of Villagrn, a few more in Comonfort, and
there may be some in Cortazar.
In Quertaro there are two municipalities where the language is quite
healthy, Amealco and Tolimn. I would classify them as GIDS 4b (schools
supported by the government) because there are bilingual schools func-
tioning there. The Amealco dialect, more precisely that of Santiago
Metzquititlan, has been described and there is a dictionary (Hekking and
Andrs de Jess, 1984, 1989). Furthermore Hekking (1995) has also
described the linguistic situation very ably. The Tolimn dialect is being
worked on by Hekking as well. Moreover, there is an incipient association
of enthusiastic bilingual teachers whose RLSefforts are tobe commended.
In the Puebla highlands, I only knowof the situation in the famous town
of San Pablito in the municipality of Pahuatln. Nahuatl is also spoken in
this municipality. San Pablito is famous because the Otoms there make
bark paper by handandsell it quite profitably to Nahuatl artists inthe state
of Guerrero and to artists the world over. In addition the women work in
embroidery andmake artifacts with small beads. Otom in the town would
definitely be classified Stage 4a, but the bilingual school is only one or two
years old. The whole town is devoted to paper-making and the language
usedis Otom. The womendress intraditional garbandare monolingual in
Otom or bilingual but Otom-dominant. Children play using the local
language. Otom is used in loud speakers (which are about the only media
used) so the town could possibly be classified as Stage 2 (language used in
government services and mass media). The language and the culture are
vigorous and not stagnant. In a brief visit I became aware of change agents
on behalf of persistence; it is the only place where I have foundexamples of
leaders, aware of the necessity of change and persistence, who are not
teachers. One of themis makingefforts for archaeologists toexcavate a cave
where there are petroglyphs, which first served as models for some of the
designs used in embroidery. The development of a tourist centre would
help the economy. The other change agent is a young man who travels
constantly to Europe and the United States giving demonstrations of bark-
paper products andembroidery. He is well educatedandproudof his heri-
tage.
The town has attracted the attention of anthropologists, but not of
linguists. The dialect is a conservative one inphonologyandgrammar; that
is, the phonology is close to what an older formof the language must have
Otomi Language Shift 155
been like and the grammar preserves categories which have disappeared:
for instance, in the Mezquital Valley; very few Spanish loans are heard.
There is valuable Otom terminology for paper-making and for religious
rites, so it should be thoroughly described.
Texcatepec, Veracruz, also in the highlands, is the municipality with the
highest percentage of Otom speakers in the country. It is governed by
Otoms, which is a rare phenomenon, and the government hasnt even
bothered to establish bilingual schools, so for the time being it is safe and
can easily be classified in GIDS 2. There is a radio station in Huayacocotla
which uses Otom at least part of the time. The dialect has not been
described; it seems to be fairly conservative. I have no information about
the other municipalities in the highlands of Veracruz.
Hidalgo is the state with the largest number of Otom speakers. It has
two areas: the Mezquital valley and the highlands. The Mezquital valley is
probably also GIDS Stage 4b and to a certain extent GIDS Stage 3 (used in
the lower workspheres) andevenStage2 (radio) at least insome municipal-
ities. In this area the dialect has been the subject of books and articles by
linguists andamateurs as well. There is a small dictionary(Wallis &Lanier,
1956), another written by native speakers, and one very complete one
written in collaboration by native speakers and linguists in preparation
under Doris Bartholomews supervision; it circulates in photocopies, but
will soonbe printedbythe Universityof Hidalgo. InItzmiquilpanthere are
people interested in language maintenance. Their activities will be
mentioned below.
In the area of the highlands of this same state, the GIDS scale wouldbe
6 or 4b depending on the municipality. I am only familiar with the town
of Santa Ana Hueytlalpan on the way toward the highlands in the
municipality of Tulancingo. People who are about 25 or older speak the
language well and children understand it but as far as I know the
language is not taught in schools at all. It could be classified as Stage 6
but in danger of being 7. The dialect is a conservative one which has not
been described.
The state of Mexico has places which are Stage 7 and are in danger of
being 8 such as Huixquilucan, Acazulco in Ocoyoacac, and Tilapa in
Tianguistenco. Others are definitely Stage 6 and some have bilingual
schools so they can be considered 4b. There is an association of teachers in
charge of making uniform the writing systems not only of Otom, but also
of other languages spokeninthe State, andof preparingteachingmaterials,
so we can say that there are some organised RLS efforts.
In addition, Guillermo Linarte Martnez (1998), of Acazulco in
Ocoyoacac, has made some individual efforts to preserve the customs at
156 The Americas
religious holidays. These include traditional dances which in that region
are called danza de arrieros. Mr Linarte participated in a contest and won a
prize which enabled him to publish a book about the dances which
includes a prayer to the patron saint (San Jernimo) in Otom and the
speeches which were customarily spoken in Otom when the dance organ-
isers invitedpeople to participate going fromhouse to house. Linarte anda
group of friends are interested in RLS as well as in preserving the local reli-
gious traditions.
The phonology and verb morphology of Temoaya Otom have been
studied by Andrews (1949, 1993), and the dialect of San Andrs Cuex-
contitlan, in the municipality of Toluca, is described in Lastra (1992b).
Zitcuaro is the name of the municipality where the SanFelipe los Alzati
is located. Here some oldpeople speak the language well but have not been
able to transmit it to their children; there are two other localities where the
situation is reportedly the same, that is, the GIDS classification would be 8.
There are no RLS efforts going on.
Ixtenco is the only municipality in Tlaxcala where the language is still
spoken. It wouldbe 7 inthe GIDS scale; people aged60 or over speakit. The
dialect has been studied in Lastra (1997). There is one teacher interested in
maintaining the language and one native speaker who teaches children: he
has had no training in language teaching, but at least the language is not
looked-down upon by all of the townspeople.
Intergenerational Transmission Status, 198898
Very little can be said in respect to the intergenerational transmission
status of Otom because of the lack of studies. Furthermore, the evidence
is contradictory: we have seen the decline in the number of speakers, but
at the same time the persistence is admirable. It is due to the willingness of
the people to maintain their culture in spite of the generally hostile atti-
tude (or at least benevolent but never admiring attitude) surrounding
them.
On the other hand, there is a countercurrent among some of the authori-
ties toencourage indigenous languages tolive. I amreferringtothe amend-
ment to the Constitution (article 4) which reads: The Mexican nation has a
pluricultural composition based originally on its indigenous peoples and
the law will protect and promote the development of its languages,
cultures, usages, customs and resources, and specific forms of social
organisation and will guarantee that its constituents will have effective
access to the jurisdiction of the state (my translation).
There have always been a lot of unimplemented plans and wishful
Otomi Language Shift 157
thinking on the part of the government and very little action as far as the
education of minorities goes. But writing this amendment into the Consti-
tution gives people who may be interested in reversing language shift a
possible legal foothold. It is yet tooearlytotell whether this declarationwill
make any difference or not, but quite informally I think it has. People are
not so ashamed of their languages as they used to be; there are more bilin-
gual schools; books are distributed in the schools instead of remaining in
government warehouses, as was the case with primers formerly. A few
bilingual teachers (very few) have received proper training. There is a
Masters degree being offered to speakers of Indian languages at CIESAS
(Centrode Investigaciones yEstudios Superiores enAntropologa Social in
Mexico City). One hears of students of Anthropology and Linguistics who
are native speakers of Indian Languages. For a while there was very little
interest in linguistic fieldwork, I think that interest has been renewed.
There are occasionally small newspapers and also occasionally articles
about Indian cultures in national newspapers.
In 1995 the First Colloquium on Otopames took place in Quertaro.
This was a national event organised by UNAM(Universidadad Nacional
Autnoma de Mxico), INAH (Instituto Nacional de Antropologa e
Historia) and several state universities and centres. The following year a
state colloquium took place at the University of the State of Mexico in
Toluca. In 1997 another national colloquium took place in Mexico City
and this year the third state colloquium was held at the University of
Hidalgo in Pachuca. The third national one is to be held in Toluca. The
result of the two first colloquia has been a newperiodical entitledEstudios
de cultura otopame published by the Instituto de Investigaciones
Antropolgicas of the UNAM (Lastra & Quezada, 1998). These colloquia
are witness to the interest in Otopamean studies. Indians have attended
all except the Mexico City one. To my mind these colloquia are important
in building up Indian morale and showing them that not all mainstream
people despise them.
Also the Chiapas phenomenonhas occurred. There have beenrebellions
before, but none have had the impact of this one with television and
internet. Indians now have access to television and the fact that some
Tzeltales have supported the rebellion and hold the army in check must be
a source of pride and glee for any Indian.
The facts mentioned have to do indirectly with intergenerational trans-
mission because a positive attitude toward the language is necessary for
anyparent totransmit it tohis or her child. Therefore all of the above factors
should have contributed to encourage people to use it with their children
and thus transmit it to them.
158 The Americas
Successes and Failures During the Last Decade
Due to the lackof studies it wouldbe impossible to reviewsuccesses and
failures inthe last decade for the whole Otom-speakingarea. It canbe done
for the Mezquital area which attracts students more than other regions and
for which we can survey what the situation was like approximately 10
years ago.
Claro and Botho (1982) regret the fact that schools teach Spanish and
contribute to people giving up their language. They complain about the
low status of Otom and of Indians in general. In my opinion the negative
attitude that the Otoms themselves had of their language is as much to
blame as the negative attitude of Spanish speakers.
Coronado et al., (1982) also refer to a period when Spanish was taught as
a foreignlanguage inthe Mezquital. Theyexamine the effect of the policyin
two Mezquital communities, Remedios (which in 1982 was 74%bilingual)
and Ocotz (which was 91% bilingual). Remedios had had a complete
primary school for 25 years and Ocotz only had a school with the first
three elementary grades for seven years. Remedios has 1500 inhabitants; it
is a Catholic village with private property. Ocotz has 400 inhabitants, it is
Pentecostal and the land is held in common in an ejido.
The authors believe that a greater or lesser resistance to language loss is
associated with a greater or lesser social cohesion in which language plays
a primary role. To the degree that the group is organised differently from
the mainstreamthe language becomes not only a means of communication
but a central part of the organisation itself.
Spanish has been learned only minimally. The speakers do not use
Otom in certain contexts but Spanish has not been developed to the extent
that it can be used effectively in those contexts. Without Otom there is a
lack of identity; people migrate and they are not accepted socially in their
new abodes because they are poor and ignorant and speak poor Spanish.
InOcotz kinshipis useful for gettinga job. InRemedios most people are
hired hands and there is not enough land, less land per family in compar-
ison to Ocotz. In both places children are addressed in Spanish, but in
Ocotz they learn Otom when they play but in Remedios they dont. In
Ocotz migrants who return still speak Otom; in Remedios they are
ashamed to. In Remedios people have a hard time understanding and
speaking Spanishwhile at the same time the use of Otom is being reduced.
The control of Spanish is better in Ocotz than in Remedios andat the same
time the vitality of Otom is greater in Ocotz than in Remedios.
While the teaching of Spanish in school has not fulfilled its goal, it has
contributed to the loss of Otom. Schooling has been instrumental in
Otomi Language Shift 159
producing marginal individuals. This short article by Coronado, Ramos
and Tellez is very moving. To my mind it shows that formal Western-style
education is not a solution to end marginality and that uniformation is
never an optimum human solution. It necessarily involves subjugation of
the weak by the strong; of the few by the many (Fishman, 1991: 31).
Coronado et al., (1984) further examine the situation in Ocotz
emphasising the fact that the children learn Spanish in an abnormal way
because they learn it from their Otom-dominant parents who do not
transmit their native language to them. The Spanish learned in school is a
series of memorised songs and riddles. They also memorise church songs
and radio commercials. They hardly ever listen to Radio Mezquital, a
government-sponsored station which combines Otom with the most
boring aspects of school learning. There is no code-switching which is
evidence of the poor control of the languages which the children have and
of the poor Spanish spoken by the mothers.
Language Policy in Independent Mexico
Language policy in Mexico can be summarised as a tendency to unify
the country linguistically and make native languages disappear. The
policy is basedonthe relations establishedby the indigenous groups with
Spanish-speaking sectors which in turn are based on economic relations
and social discrimination transmitted by the media, religion and
primarily by the educational system. Since 1964 there is supposedly
bilingual and bicultural education. It has amounted to making the
communicative barrier between teachers and students less abrupt, but it
isnt a real systemof bilingual education and it certainly is not bicultural:
the teaching materials are inadequate, the teachers are not qualified and
above all their attitude is negative. What they do is use the native
language to teach Spanish.
Coronado et al., (1981) examine the language and educational situation
in various Mezquital communities. The National Indigenist Institute (INI)
sponsored pre-schools for children in 60 communities. The teachers were
middle-school graduates with hardly any training. In the regular schools
Spanishis the mediumof instructionandthe childrenhave hadno training
in Spanish at all. The groups are very large, teachers do not remain in the
same townfor long, andthe indexof school desertionis veryhigh. The texts
used in the bilingual schools vary in quality, a couple of them being
adequate but most are not graded properly and in the worst one we read:
The Indian while learning to read his language simultaneously learns to
read the national language. The difference is that he understands what he
160 The Americas
reads in his own language while he doesnt yet understand the other one
(quoted in Coronado et al., 1981: 127), and my translation).
The above extracts give an idea of the backwardness of the pedagogy
employed and clearly emphazise the low opinion that the mainstream
members have of the minority, the poverty of the people, and their
marginalisation.
Inthe last decade the situationhas not improvedverymuchas far as atti-
tudes are concerned. There are manymore schools whichhave presumably
contributed to the increase of bilingualism. However, an important devel-
opment has been the establishment of the Academia de la Lengua ah.
The word ah is the Otom word for the language and the ethnic group
inthe dialect of the Mezquital. The academywas foundedbygraduates of a
special BAprogramme in Ethnolinguistics sponsored by CIESAS (Centro
de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropologa Social in Mexico
City), which functioned in Ptzcuaro, Michoacn in the 1970s. It had polit-
ical overtones and was not as academic as one would have wished, but still
it gave some Indian teachers an opportunity to acquire higher education
including some knowledge of linguistics, social anthropology, and
language teaching.
The purpose of the Academy, founded in 1986 with 16 members, is to
reverse language shift. At first it was a voluntary group of bilingual
teachers, but as of 1989 they are officially recognised in the sense that their
salaries as teachers are paidfor, but insteadof eachmember being assigned
to a school as a regular teacher they can devote themselves to the activities
whichtheyconsider will foster RLS. Theyhave beengiventhe use of a large
room in a government building outside Itzmiquilpan which they use for
their work.
According to the Academy members, the main problem they face is the
negative attitude of the so-called bilingual teachers, many of whomdo not
knowhowto write inOtom. The Academy sponsoreda regional forumfor
the regularisation of Otom orthography; it organises courses in reading
and writing for adults; it organised a workshop to write stories with the
parents of school childrenin199495; it sponsors songcontests, spreads the
knowledge of indigenous rights, and sells cassettes of Otom stories.
Currently, for instance, they are preparing two cassettes, one of ah
stories and another one of ah songs.
The Academy members have been in charge of advising as to language
matters in 10 pilot schools in the Mezquital since 1991. They developed a
plan for the preparation of teaching materials which the DGEI (Direccin
General de EducacinIndgena), the government agency incharge of bilin-
gual schools nationwide, did not accept in 1992, but which was modified
Otomi Language Shift 161
andacceptedin1993. As a result, language textbooks were preparedfor the
first four years of primary school. These were printed, but the DGEI head
was changed and, as a result, the books could not be used initially but now
they do have books which are distributed to all the bilingual schools of the
Mezquital. There are 1800 bilingual teachers: not all of themuse the books,
but some 40% do. Of course not all the schools are bilingual; most are
regular schools where Spanish is the mediumof instruction for every child
regardless of his language background.
Among other developments, there is one close to their hearts: this is the
general acceptance of the word ah in the area. For instance, the vans
used for public transportation have signs with this word in them. A few
stores have signs written in the language. Radio Mezquital offers more
programmes in Otom. There is another radio station situated in Cardonal
sponsored by INI. The academy members asked the radio speakers not to
call Otoms ahucitos, with the Spanish diminutive, which they
consider demeaning, but to use the Otom diminutive ci- which is also a
reverential. The Academy has lately received the help of some Catholic
German missionaries who live in El Cardonal. They have prepared songs,
stories, prayers and a translation of the mass into ah. As a whole, the
group seems very optimistic, and intends to go on with its work in spite of
all the odds.
All members of this enthusiastic group are hard working and influen-
tial. Tomymindtheyhave tobe made aware of the fact that school-learning
is not all there is to RLS. Also, they should not fall into the trap of
folklorisingthe language bymakingtoomuchof those songs that caneasily
be memorised. Language is for communication about any subject. The
Academy members think that the bilingual situation is stable and that chil-
drenare learningOtom inthe normal way; but I thinktheyshouldcarryon
more activities toturnthe more or less passive attitude of the people intoan
active one of love and admiration for their own language and identity.
They have to be reminded of the fact that language is a symbol funda-
mental to the preservation of culture. Culture and language should not
remainstatic, theyhave toevolve andbe adaptedsotheycansurvive inthis
modern world. At the same time, the Academy members have to realise
that their dialect is not the standardlanguage. There is no standardandthe
other dialects deserve the same respect as their own.
Conclusions
The mainthesis of Fishman(1991) is that normal intergenerational trans-
missionof language is the sine qua nonof language maintenance. Withthis I
162 The Americas
agree wholeheartedly. Whenever parents decide to speak to their children
in the official language (often to help them in school) they doom the
language intheir community andat the same time they doomtheir culture.
Usually they dont realise what is happening and when they slip into the
native language they wonder why the children dont respond or are
unwilling to learn their language!
Otom is vigorous inthe communities where the language is transmitted
normally, regardless of the presence or absence of bilingual schools. As a whole,
schools have been responsible for language shift, but the primary cause is
the negative attitude toward the Otom language held by the dominant
classes and by many of the Otom speakers themselves. If we could
contribute to change these attitudes, the speakers themselves would
continue to speak to their children in the language in Otom in those
communities which are now at Stage 6. More strenuous efforts would be
necessary in areas nowat Stage 7 and 8, but the possibility is there and it is
to be hoped that enlightened speakers will lead the way for the preserva-
tion of this ancient culture.
Notes
1. In Spanish, stress falls on the last syllable of the word, e.g. beber, unless the
wordends ina vowel, nor s inwhichcase the stress falls onthe next-to-last syl-
lable as in comes, cantaba. All exceptions to this rule have a written stress
mark on the vowel to be stressed: frtil, mencin, espritu. In Mexican Spanish
there are many loan words from Nahuatl, the language of the Mexicas (Az-
tecs). Many of these loans are place names; they are stressed following
Spanish rules.
2. The present-day location of the town of Xaltocan is in the State of Mexico.
3. A hill which is now a famous park within Mexico City.
4. The lowlands located in the present-day states of San Luis Potos, Hidalgo,
Veracruz and Tamaulipas.
5. Mexico City appears in Figure 6.1 as D.F. (Distrito Federal). The state of Mexico
appears as MEXICO.
6. Figure 6.1 shows municipalities with Otom population above 20. The table
only lists those which had more than 1%Otom population at some point. Cen-
sus figures, of course, do not discriminate between those municipalities which
have been traditionally Otom and those with recent Otom migration. Some of
the latter may appear in Figure 6.1 but not in Table 6.1.
References
Andrews, H. (1949) Phonemes andmorphophonemes of TemoayanOtom. Interna-
tional Journal of American Linguistics 15, 21322.
Andrews, H. (1993) The Function of Verb Prefixes in South-western Otomi. Dallas:
Summer Institute of Linguistics and University of Texas, Arlington.
Carrasco Pizana, P. (1950) Los otomes. Cultura e historia prehispnica de los pueb-
Otomi Language Shift 163
los mesoamericanos de habla otomiana. Facsimile edition, Mxico: Bibliotca
enciclopdica del Estado de Mxico, 1979.
Claro, M.G. and Botho, A.M. (1982) Qu Somos Los Maestros Bilinges en el Valle del
Mezquital? Mxico: Secretara de Educacin Pblica and Instituto Nacional
Indigenista.
Coronado, G., Ramos, M.T. and Tellez, F.J. (1982) Castellanizacin formal: Un
mtodo para el desaprendizaje. Cuicuilco. Revista de la Escuela Nacional de
Antropologa e Historia 9, l929.
Coronado, G., Ramos, M.T. and Tellez, F.J. (1984) Continuidad y Cambio en una
Comunidad Bilinge. Mxico: Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en
Antropologa Social .
Coronado, G., Franco, V.M. andMuoz, H. (1981) Bilingismo y Educacin en el Valle
del Mezquital. Mxico: Cuadernos de la Casa Chata.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Theoretical and Empirical Foundations of
Assistance to Threatened Languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Franco, P. and Manuel, V. (1992) Grupo domstico y reproduccin social. Parentesco,
economa e ideologa en una comunidad otom del Valle del Mezquital. Mxico:
Centro de Investigaciones y Estudios Superiores en Antropologa Social.
Galinier, J. (1979) Nyh Les indiens otomis. Hirarchie social et tradition dans le Sud de
la Huasteca. Mexico: Mission Archeologique et Etnologique Franaise en
Mexique. Spanish translation: Pueblos de la Sierra Madre. Etnografa de la
comunidad otom. Mxico: Centre dEtudes Mexicaines et Centramericaines and
Instituto Nacional Indigenista, 1987.
Galinier, J. (1990) La mitad del mundo. Cuerpo y cosmos en los rituales otomes. Mxico:
Instituto de Investigaciones Antropolgicas, Universidad Nacional Autnoma
de Mxico.
Hekking, E. (1995) El otom de Santiago Mexquititln: desplazamiento lingstico,
prstamos y cambios gramaticales. Amsterdam: IFOTT(Institute for Functional Re-
search into Language and Language Use.
Hekking, E. andAndrs de Jess, S. (1984) Gramtica otom. Quertaro: Universidad
Autnoma de Quertaro.
Hekking, E. and Andrs de Jess, S. (1989) Diccionario espaol-otom de Santiago
Mexquititln. Quertaro: Universidad Autnoma de Quertaro.
Lastra, Y. (1992a) Estudios antiguos y modernos sobre el otom. In R. Barriga
Villanueva and J. Garca Fajardo (eds) Reflexiones Lingsticas y Literarias Vol 1.
Lingstica. Mxico: El Colegio de Mxico. Enlarged version, Anales de
Antropologa 29, 45389.
Lastra, Y. (1992b) El otom de Toluca. Mxico: Instituto de Investigaciones
Antropolgicas, Universidad Nacional Autnoma de Mxico.
Lastra, Y. (1994) Es el otom una lengua amenazada? 48th International Congress
of Americanists, Stockholm and Upsala, 49 July 1994.
Lastra, Y. (199495) La lengua otom en Guanajuato y Quertaro. Cuadernos del Sur,
Letras No 26, 5966, Baha Blanca, Argentina.
Lastra, Y. (1997) El otom de Ixtenco. Mxico: Instituto de Investigaciones
Antropolgicas, Universidad Nacional Autnoma de Mxico.
Lastra, Y. and Quezada, N. (eds) (1998) Estudios de Cultura Otopame I.
Linarte Martnez, G. (1998) La Danza de Los Arrieros. Toluca, Mxico: Norte/Sur.
164 The Americas
Mendizabal, M.O. (1947) Evolucin econmica y social del Valle del Mezquital.
Obras Completas VI, 7150 Mxico: Talleres Grficos de la Nacin.
Nolasco, A.M. (1966) Los otomes del Mezquital. Epoca post-revolucionaria. In
Homenaje a Roberto J. Weitlaner, 637658 Mxico: Instituto Nacional de
Antropologa e Historia.
Torquemada, fray Juan de (1969) Monarqua Indiana. Mxico: Porra [1615 1st print-
ing].
Wallis, E. and Lanier, N. (l956) Diccionario castellano-otom, otom-castellano.
Itzmiquilpan, Mxico: Ediciones del Patrimonio Indgena del Valle del
Mezquital y del Instituto Lingstico de Verano.
Otomi Language Shift 165
Chapter 7
Reversing Quechua Language Shift
in South America
N.H. HORNBERGER and K.A. KING
This chapter is dedicated in loving memory of Faustino Espinoza Navarro, b.
15 December 1905, d. 15 January 2000, Cusco, Peru, lifelong champion of the
Quechua language.
Introduction
Quechua is a threatened language (Dorian, 1998: 4). Yet with 8 to 12
million speakers (Grinevald, 1998: 128), most of them concentrated in the
Andean highlands of Peru, Bolivia and Ecuador, Quechua is also the most
widely spoken indigenous language in the Americas. When the status of
Quechua is compared with that of the hundreds of languages in the world
whichhave onlya fewdozenelderlynative speakers andnoactive commu-
nity of language learners or users, Quechua may appear to be a safe, even
robust language. After all, one might well ask, how can a language with
millions of speakers possibly be endangered?
Quechua as threatened language
The relatively large number of Quechua speakers obscures the unfa-
vourable social and political circumstances under which Quechua has
endured for many of the last 500 years. While in the initial stages of the
Spanishconquest Quechua was usedbySpaniards andindigenous persons
as a lingua franca, this period was short-lived (Cerrn-Palomino, 1989,
1997: 55; Botero, 1991: 115; Haboud, 1996: 57). A decree from the Spanish
crown in the second half of the 18th century mandating compulsory
Castilianisation of native Americans marked the end of toleration of indig-
enous languages (von Gleich, 1992: 49; Cerrn-Palomino, 1997: 57). As the
Spaniards solidified their control of the region in subsequent decades, the
Spanish language spread and became revered as the language of power
and prestige, while the Indian population and their language(s) were
further denigrated (Haboud, 1996: 25). Although formal measures to
166
The Americas
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America
instruct Spanish were generally ineffective, the indigenous population
learnedthe dominant language informally for social survival andacquired
it as part of the biological and cultural process of mestisation (von Gleich,
1992: 4950; see also Heath, 1982).
As the nations of Ecuador, Peru and Bolivia gained their independence
fromSpaininthe 1820s and1830s, Spanishcontinuedto reignas the official
language; Quechua and other indigenous languages were at best ignored,
and at worst, actively oppressed. Indeed, throughout much of the regions
post-colonial history, governmental policy concerning the indigenous
sectors has remained relatively unchanged: shift away from indigenous
languages and cultures has long been either an implicit or explicit goal
(Mannheim, 1984: 303). This clearly assimilationist language policy has
resulted in the current linguistic shame and self-negation among many
incipient bilinguals (von Gleich, 1992: 51). As Coronel-Molina notes, it
took centuries to devalue the language; it may take centuries again to
totally revalorise it (1999: 168).
Recent decades in the major Andean countries have been marked by the
rise of indigenous political organisations andthe implementationof political
reforms targeting the indigenous sectors.
1
Yet while there has been substan-
tial progress on some levels, the Quechua language and Quechua speakers
generallyremainpowerless andmarginalisedwithintheir national contexts.
Quechua continues to be strongly linked with the rural, uneducated and
poor, while Spanish remains the primary language of national and interna-
tional communication, literacy and education, and professional and
academic success. What Mannheim has noted for Southern Peruvian
Quechua has long been characteristic of all Quechua varieties, namely that
formal political participation and economic mobility both depend not on
overcoming the stigma attached to the language, but on abandoning alto-
gether the language andthe cultural universe that accompanies it in favor of
an alien language and culture (1984: 292).
Given that Quechua is clearly the loser in the language and culture
contact resulting from the Spanish conquest (von Gleich, 1994: 78), its
current precarious state is hardly surprising. While the position of Quechua
varies greatlyfromone communityandregiontothe next, there are substan-
tial sociolinguistic data which indicate that Quechua is indeed a declining
andthreatenedlanguage. InPeru, for example, figures fromthe official census
reveal that Quechua monolingualism is steadily giving way to temporary,
subtractive bilingualism in one generation, followed by Spanish mono-
lingualisminthe next. As the percent of Quechuamonolinguals has declined
(from 31% in 1940, to 17% in 1961, to 11% in 1982), the percent of bilinguals
has heldalmost constant, andthe percent of Spanishmonolinguals has risen
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 167
(from 50% in 1940, to 65% in 1961, to 72% in 1982) (von Gleich, 1992: 59).
Data fromBolivia suggest a similar pattern. While 36.5%of the population
reported using Quechua most frequently in 1950, only 25.7% did in 1976.
During that same period, the number of Bolivians who reportedly used
primarilySpanishdoubled(from26%in1950 to54.1%in1976) (vonGleich,
1994: 90). Recent sociolinguistic surveys in the Ecuadorian highlands also
provide evidence that language shift is well underway. Haboud, for
example, finds that the indigenous household, traditionally an exclusively
Quechua domain, has largely become a bilingual space, where, at the
expense of Quechua, Spanish is now regularly employed (1996: 175).
Correspondingly, ethnographic investigations provide insight into the
patterns of community language use leading to language shift in the
Andes. Although conducted in different areas, comparison of Horn-
bergers (1988) study of language use in the mid-1980s with Kings investi-
gation (1997; to appear) of the mid-1990s reveals the processes at work in
the slow, but steady decline of Quechua. Hornbergers data, collected in
highland communities in Puno, Peru, revealed that although use of Spanish
was increasing, there were clearly defined domains of use for both
Quechua and Spanish. Hornberger reports that Quechua was used consis-
tently in discourse between and among community members in home and
traditional community settings, while Spanish was reserved for communi-
cation with non-members and in formal, non-traditional settings (e.g.
during school hours). Kings data, in contrast, reveal that even the more
remote and isolated of the two communities she studied in highland
Saraguro, Ecuador could no longer be considered diglossic. While
Quechua is often selected for communication between and among elders,
for kiddingandmakingjokes, andat certaintraditional communityevents,
Spanish has made in-roads into seemingly every speech situation, and
presently, only what might be the traces of former domains are left (King,
1999: 25). There is no longer a safe space, for instance, in the home, in the
community, or among family, for Quechua to be used exclusively and
therefore ensured transmission to younger generations. The limited bilin-
gualism which exists is extremely unstable and likely a transitional phase
leading to Spanish monolingualism. Thus, in the communities studied by
Hornberger and King, and in many other Andean regions as well, shift
away fromQuechua takes place domainby domain, as Spanishencroaches
into every arena of use.
2
The diversity of Quechua language contexts
If one aspect of Quechuas unusualness lies in its large number of
speakers, yet precarious status, a second lies in its remarkable diversity.
168 The Americas
This diversity runs along at least three related lines: linguistic diversity (in
terms of the multiple varieties which exist), geographic diversity (in terms
of the range of contexts of use of the language), and a particular type of
sociolinguistic diversity (in terms of varied levels of contact with other
languages).
Linguistically, Quechua has been characterised as a group of varieties
within a language family, but lacking the superstructure of parents and
grandparents (von Gleich, 1994: 81). While many of the original languages
of the Andes have disappeared (Grinevald, 1998: 129), Kaufman (1994)
argues that Quechua consists of 17 emergent languages, that is, 17
Quechua varieties, each with three to five centuries of diversification (in
Grinevald, 1998: 129). Given the difficult andcontroversial nature of classi-
fying language varieties, there is little agreement concerning the total
number of varieties. However, basedonphonological andlexical data, two
main groups of Quechua have traditionally been recognised: Quechua I
(Torero, 1974), also known as Quechua B (Parker, 1973), which is spoken in
central Peru; and Quechua II (a.k.a. Quechua A), which consists of many
other varieties found to the south and north of Quechua I, in Ecuador,
Bolivia, Chile, and Argentina, and southern and northern Peru.
Linguistic classification of Quechua is complicated by the fact that it is
spoken in a half dozen Andean nations, each of which has treated the
language somewhat differently. In Peru, for example, dictionaries and
grammar books were published in the 1970s for six distinct Peruvian
dialects: Ancash-Huailas, Ayacucho-Chanca, Cajamarca-Caaris, Cuzco-
Collao, Junn-Huanca, and San Martn (de Vries, 1988: 1089; Hornberger,
1988: 301; Cerrn-Palomino, 1997: 61). The Bolivian government took
quite a different approach: in 1980, the government approved a Unified
Alphabet, which serves as the standard system for both Quechua and
Aymara. In Ecuador, in contrast, an Ecuadorian National Unified Alpha-
bet was established for all Quichua varieties in the country as part of the
National Quichua Literacy Conference in 1980.
3
Known as Quichua
Unificado, this formdivergedlinguistically fromwhat was commonly used
in Bolivia and Peru. As Montaluisa describes, the decisions surrounding
the establishment of EcuadorianQuichua Unificado were made largelyinde-
pendent of either scientific or previously established international criteria
(1980; in de Vries, 1988: 61).
4
Non-governmental organisations, such as the Summer Institute for
Linguistics (SIL), have further emphasised the diversity of Quechua vari-
eties. SILs catalogue of world languages, Ethnologue, lists 47 different
Quechua languages belonging to the Quechuanlanguage family. Some of
these, such as Quechua-Yauyos of the Department of Lima, are further
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 169
dissected and described as cover terms for many different one-village
varieties (Grimes, 1996).
Geographically, the situation of Quechua speakers is equally complex.
Quechua reside in Peru, Bolivia and Ecuador, with smaller numbers in
Argentina, Colombia, and Brazil.
5
While the rural Andean highlands have
been the traditional stronghold of the language, there are substantial
numbers of Quechua speakers, and indeed, whole communities, who
reside in a variety of different non-highland, non-rural settings.
Massive migration over the last 50 years has transformed all Andean
countries, to a greater or lesser extent, from largely rural nations to
primarily urban ones. Official census figures for Peru, for example, reveal
howpronouncedthis shift has been: in 1940, 35%of the population resided
in urban areas, 65%in rural sectors; by 1982 these numbers were reversed,
with 65% of the country counting as urban and 35% as rural (von Gleich,
1992: 59). Quechua speakers have been a major part of this trend, leaving
their communities for positions as domestic, construction, or factory
workers in the cities. Furthermore, in Peru, the violence and fear which
accompanied the Sendero Luminoso Shining Path revolutionary guerrilla
movement intensified this trend in some regions, as many Quechua high-
landers fled their communities for shanty towns surrounding Lima and
other major cities in the 1980s.
In addition, there are also Quechua communities which have long
existed in the lowland Amazon regions. The Quichua del Oriente, for
example, number approximately 60,000 and have subsisted in the Ecua-
dorian provinces of Napo and Pastaza much like (and sometimes in close
contact with) other Amazonian groups such as the Shuar and the Huarani
(Bentez &Garcs, 1992: 17680). Other Quechua, such as the Saraguros of
the southernEcuadorianhighlands, have slowlymigratedeastward, either
cyclically or permanently, into the Amazonian lowlands to raise cattle.
Lastly, a much smaller number of Quechua speakers travel and reside
abroad in US and European cities as students, musicians, and business
persons.
The geographic diversity of Quechua speakers and communities results
in great variation in the amount, duration, and intensity of contact with
speakers of indigenous languages such as Aymara and Huarani, as well as
speakers of European languages such as Spanish and English. The number
of language contact scenarios is nearly as great as the number of Quechua
speaking communities. There are, for example, Quechua who live in major
urbancentres, interactingalmost exclusivelyinSpanish; Quechua wholive
in large pueblos jovenes shanty towns surrounding major cities, whose
traditional patterns of language use disappeared with their move and the
170 The Americas
loss of physical boundary domains, and who increasingly use Spanish;
Quechua who live in close contact with speakers of Aymara or other indig-
enous languages; and there are still a limited number of Quechua who
reside in remote, monolingual Quechua communities, with limitedcontact
with non-Quechua. Furthermore, as Cerrn-Palomino (1997: 52, 62) has
pointed out, Quechua continues to spread in some areas (the Ecuadorian
Amazon or northern Potos in Bolivia, for example), even while it is on the
verge of extinction in others (for example, the Peruvian Quechua varieties
of Chachapoyas, Cajatambo, Huaral, Yauyos, and Valle del Mantaro). The
degree of change in different regions is so varied and the range of
sociolinguistic situations is so great that at the turn of the 20th century it is
difficult to describe a typical Quechua community. Quechua is character-
ised, then, not onlybyits precarious linguistic status, but bythe remarkable
diversity of its communities.
Quechua in Relation to the Graded Intergenerational
Disruption Scale
The Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale (GIDS) (Fishman, 1990,
1991) is designed to provide a means of assessing the status of a language,
the prospects of intergenerational transmission of the language, and, by
implication, the level of success of efforts to maintain and revitalise the
threatened language. In Fishmans words, the scale is appropriate for
determining the intergenerational continuity and maintenance prospects
of a language network or community (1991: 87; emphasis ours).
While speech networks and speech communities have been defined
somewhat differently by various scholars (e.g. Hymes, 1972; Labov, 1972;
Milroy, 1987), most definitions demand that members of a given speech
community share norms for interacting in the language, attitudes con-
cerning language use, or the use of a specific variety, typically developed
by engaging in regular interaction. Given the linguistic, geographic, and
sociolinguistic diversity of Quechua communities, by most definitions,
Quechua speakers donot constitute one language networkor one language
community.
6
Hence, it is impossible to outline uniformly the situation of
the language; to do so would entail making over-simplified and at least
partially inaccurate generalisations.
Similarly, Quechua language revitalisation and maintenance efforts are
underway in many regions, and in some cases, have been for multiple
decades. These efforts to reverse Quechua language shift have been far
from uniform, varying in goals, means, intensity, duration, and results.
Because of this dramatic diversity, it is not feasible to detail and analyse all
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 171
of the efforts and their impact as it might be for a language with a smaller
number of speakers and more unified functional distribution. It is thus
impossible completelyandaccuratelyto describe the situationof Quechua,
a language spoken by at least eight million people in thousands of commu-
nities, in terms of the GIDS or any other framework.
It is, however, possible to provide sketches of some of the diverse
language revitalisation activities taking place on behalf of Quechua in
different communities andanalyse theminterms of Fishmans framework.
In what follows, we discuss the range of uses of Quechua and types of RLS
attempts in relation to the GIDS scale, drawing from multiple regions and
contexts and illustrating the diversity of efforts being undertaken at a
variety of levels on the behalf of Quechua. We conclude with some
comments about both the status of Quechua language revitalisation efforts
and the validity of the GIDS framework.
Revitalisation attempts illustrative of each GIDS stage
Stage 8: Reconstructing Quechua
The major goal at this stage of revitalisation efforts is to reassemble the
language itself and to build up a core of those who have at least some
knowledge of it (Fishman, 1991: 90). Fishman suggests that the process of
reconstruction may begin with piecing together folksongs, proverbs, folk-
tales and formulaic expressions collected from the few remaining users of
the language and subsequently, on the basis of these, assembling partial
phonologies, grammars, and lexicons (1991: 88). Re-establishing commu-
nitynorms not onlyof grammar andphonology, but alsoof intonationand
prosody . . . and semantic typologies is highly desirable (1991: 397).
Giventhe diversityof Quechua communities describedabove, Quechua
activists
7
who recognise and seek to address the threat to the language in
their own community or region generally have had the option of seeking
out more isolated, rural, inaccessible locations where Quechua is still
spoken. This has been the recourse and resource which, for example,
Faustino Espinoza Navarro (b. 1905d. 2000) cultivated throughout a life-
time dedicatedto promoting andrevitalising Quechua. Beginning fromhis
years as a functionarywithvarious governmental ministries inthe first half
of the last century and continuing throughout his life, Espinoza relished
travelling and working in remote highland communities throughout Peru,
where, he said, speakingwell withthose whodont knowSpanish, I acquired
a better, clearer form of the language of the Inca (Hornberger, 1994: 245).
As a result of his efforts spanning nearly a century, he was widely recog-
nisedinCuscoas the most eloquent speaker of Quechua whenhe was alive,
172 The Americas
not only for his enormous working vocabulary, but also for the elegance of
his sentence structure, intonation and prosody, which found expression in
invited public speeches, radio broadcasts, and video appearances.
Visiting the campo countryside and seeking out monolingual Quechua
speakers is also a principal means of cultivating and revitalising Quechua
linguistic and cultural knowledge for Rufino Chuquimamani of Puno,
Peru. Like Espinoza, Chuquimamani seeks to document Quechua lexicon,
and in particular is interested in semantic domains central to rural life
(Chuquimamani, 1988; Hornberger, 1994: 249). In addition, in his zeal to
investigate and document Quechua knowledge, he asks for stories wher-
ever he goes; some of these have been published in volumes produced by
the Experimental Bilingual Education Project of Puno with the express
purpose of cultivating a Quechua-literate adult population (Chuquima-
mani, 1983, 1984).
The efforts of these two Peruvian Quechua activists devoting their own
unpaid time to reconstructing Quechua lexicon and oral literature are
consistent with those of others in this century such as Jorge Lira
(Hornberger, 1992: 443), and indeed of efforts extending back to the 16th
century arrival of the Spanish and the first publication of dictionaries and
oral traditions (e.g. Gonzalez Holgun, Domingo de Santo Toms, Guaman
Poma, Garcilaso de la Vega, The Huarochiri Manuscript; see Adelaar, 1991,
for references). Given Quechuas centuries-long history as an oppressed
language (Alb, 1977: 5), surviving under threat, the reconstruction and
documentation of Quechua language and literature is an ongoing and
never-ending uphill battle. And given that there is extremely limited state
support for the work of Quechua language activists, and that the vast
majority of their efforts have been self-initiated and self-supported, they
seem to exemplify well Fishmans admonition that the road to RLS is a
long and difficult one . . . paved with self-sacrifice (1991: 98).
Stage 7: Cultural interaction in Quechua
At Stage 7 of the GIDS, most users of Xish [Quechua] are a socially inte-
grated and ethnolinguistically active population, but they are beyond
child-bearing age (Fishman, 1991: 89). The major goal at Stage 7 of the
GIDS is to develop a younger cohort of Quechua-as-a-second-language
learners who will eventually become regular Quechua users. Since the ulti-
mate aimof language reversal is to re-establish intergenerational transmis-
sion, it is important that these new Quechua learners and users be young
enough to bear children to whom they can eventually transmit the
language as a mother tongue. The development of this younger cohort of
language learners and users, according to Fishman, happens through the
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 173
establishment of a variety of youth groups, young peoples associations,
young parent groups and, finally, residential communities or neighbor-
hoods all of which utilise (or lead to the utilisation of) Xish [Quechua]
(1991: 91).
In some Quechua communities, these sorts of youth groups are well
established and very active. For example, among the Saraguros, a group of
about 20,000 who reside principally in the southern Ecuadorian Andes,
youth organisations, primarily focused on music and dance, have been
popular for at least four decades. Despite the fact that the Saraguros are in
the late stages of shift away from their once native Quechua and are now
primarily Spanish dominant, the Saraguros continue to maintain a long
tradition of forming youth-oriented cultural groups.
One of the first of these organisations, Grupo Intian Sunroad Group,
was formed in the late 1950s with the support of two non-Saraguros
(Volinsky, 1996: 11). Grupo Intian performed traditional Saraguro music,
dance, oration and poetry, and enjoyed popular success both within
Saraguro and in national venues. In addition to reviving traditional
Saraguro performance arts, Grupo Intian was effective in spreading an
indigenous Saraguro perspective by discussing the social injustice and
exploitation suffered by the Saraguros during the performances of
orations (Belote &Belote, 1981: 469). Inthe 1970s Grupo Intianevolvedinto
the Grupo Artstico Cultural Saraguro Artistic Culture Group Saraguro and
was legally recognised as an indigenous organisation (Volinsky, 1996: 12).
Since that time, the group has continued to promote Saraguro culture by
performing at various youth events in the region.
The grouphas alsobeeninfluential inthe establishment of other cultural
groups such as Grupo Rumiahui Stone-eye Group
8
a youth organisation
which aims to rescue, promote, and revitalise what they considered to be
the cultural patrimony of the Saraguros (Volinsky, 1996: 13). There are
currently numerous other youth-oriented cultural organisations which
performtraditional Saraguro music anddance. While the membershipand
participation in the organisations are somewhat fluid, many young
Saraguros have participated over the years and performances continue to
be well attended by young and old alike.
Language plays an important symbolic role in many of the cultural
performances. While in recent decades the audience, as well as the
performers, have tended to be Spanish dominant if not Spanish monolin-
gual, most groups make an effort to use Quechua in their lyrics, in their
poetry, or in the formal announcements to the audience. However, given
the sporadic use of the language andlackof regularityof the performances,
174 The Americas
it is doubtful that much real language learning takes place. As Fishman
notes (1991: 398), these sorts of special events are precisely that; and as
such they are not likely to impact everyday language use. Yet such
symbolic use of the language at least provides some exposure to Quechua,
possibly raising ethnolinguistic consciousness among young Saraguros
and perhaps indirectly contributing to the formation of future Quechua
learners.
While the Saraguro cultural groups provide a clear example of Stage 7
efforts, two points concerning these activities should be made clear. First,
these groups have not developed into the young parent groups and,
finally, residential communities or neighborhoods which use Quechua
that Fishmans model suggests (1991: 91). Indeed, cultural groups in
Saraguro have been active for four decades and have yet to take these next
important steps. The second point directly concerns Fishmans emphasis
on the role of the elder generation in organising and actively participating
in these activities. In Saraguro, the cultural activities are typically organ-
ised by the younger generations, not the elders. Cultural interaction is not
taking place exclusively among the old-timers (Fishman, 1991: 398).
Indeed, the most active and vocal promoters and users of the language are
members of the younger generation who are in their teens or twenties. It is
not the case that they are young guests, who are simply passive observers
at this stage, as Fishman describes (1991: 398), but rather, the young are the
primary performers, organisers, and participants in these cultural events.
The Saraguro case, then, suggests that the role of the younger and often
highly politicised generation may be insufficiently stressed in the frame-
work.
Stage 6: Intergenerational and demographically concentrated
home-family-neighbourhood transmission
Stage 6 of the GIDS, according to Fishman (1991: 935), is not only the
most difficult stage, but also the most crucial. At this stage, the threatened
language becomes the normal language of informal, spoken interaction
between and within all three generations of the family, as well as the lan-
guage of interfamily interaction, of interaction with playmates, neighbors,
friends and acquaintances (1991: 923). It cannot be over-stressed how
difficult the attainment of this stage is once it is lost.
Returning once again to the communities of Saraguro in southern
Ecuador, despite the intensive efforts at schools, among various commu-
nity and political organisations, and, as discussed above, by cultural
groups, regular, everyday use of the languages in homes and neighbour-
hoods among young adults and children remains exceedingly rare.
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 175
Saraguro parents, many of whom are Spanish dominant with limited
Quechua skills, speak of their desire to transmit the language to their chil-
dren, but findit very difficult to use the language consistently at home. The
reasons behind this phenomenon are complex. Saraguro parents explain
the dearth of Quechua use in the home as a result of their lack of Quechua
skills; their lack of confidence in using the language; their childrens resis-
tance to Quechua; and the force of habit. Some Saraguro parents who are
strongly committed to Quechua revitalisation do use Quechua occasion-
ally with their children informally, as well as more formally in order to
teach them new words (King, 1999). Yet even these unusually dedicated
parents report being unable to use the language routinely.
Similarly, Haboud (1996), in her large-scale sociolinguistic survey of the
Ecuadorian Andes finds that even some of the most committed bilingual
education professionals experience difficulty in regularly using the
language at home. She quotes one suchindividual whoobserves: [Mychil-
dren] . . . talktome inQuichua andwant me totalktothembut I amnot able
to, it is the habit, you know, even if I want to [ . . . ]. It is contradictory
because, you know, I have taught Quichua and various things in Quichua
for a long time. I amas one wouldsay, anexpert inthe language (Quichua),
but I cannot teach it to my own children (Haboud, 1996: 178).
Both Habouds data and the Saraguro case make clear howdifficult and
unlikely attainment of intergenerational transmission of Quechua is, and
give us cause to question the position of Stage 6 on the framework. In the
GIDS, Stage 6 is framed as following the reconstruction of the language
(Stage 8) and its use in cultural activities (Stage 7). The Quechua case
suggests that attainment of these two prior stages may be necessary, but
not sufficient to support intergenerational transmission of Quechua. In
other words, attainment of Stage 6 may require, at least in some cases, the
concomitant support of activities at higher GIDS stages.
Stage 5: Schools for literacy acquisition, for the old and for the
young and not in lieu of compulsory education
Stage 5 deals with educational efforts that are entirely [Quechua],
primarily literacy-focused, socialisation-related and entirely under intra-
communal control (Fishman, 1991: 98). The goal is the attainment of a
guided literacy; the agencies of this literacy acquisition, for example, may
be the home, a local religious unit, or a local literacy programme; and these
efforts characteristically do not receive government funding nor satisfy
compulsory education requirements (1991: 97).
Returning again to Cusco, Peru for illustration, we will briefly consider
three institutions and the degree to which they meet these criteria: the
176 The Americas
Iglesia Evangelica Peruana Evangelical Church of Peru (IEP); the Academia
[Peruana] de la Lengua Quechua [Peruvian] Academy of the Quechua
Language (ALQ); and a non-governmental organisation, Centro Andino de
Educacin y Promocin Andean Center for Education and Promotion
(CADEP). All three institutions promote Quechua literacy, and do so
without government funding and outside the compulsory education
system; however, a focus on literacy, a fostering of socialisation into
Quechua ways, and control by Quechua speakers are not consistently
present in every case.
Maria de Centeno, already literate in Spanish, became literate in
Quechua through her association with the Evangelical Church of Peru. She
first began to attend the IEP at age 13 and was baptised at age 18 (in 1943).
Missionaries asked her to use her knowledge of spoken Quechua to serve
as interpreter at church conventions and to teach pastors wives at semi-
annual two-week Bible Institutes. Then one day, she was asked to give the
closing address at the Bible Institute. As she tells it, she set out to prepare
her message with the intention of writing it in Spanish andthen translating
into Quechua as she spoke. However, at one point, asking Gods help . . .
she began to write directly in Quechua, to the best of her knowledge. . . .
From that time on, .. she knew how to prepare her messages directly in
Quechua. She bought the Quechua NewTestament and began to drawher
messages directly fromthe Quechua text . . . [S]he learned to read Quechua
and now can read, not only the Bible, but also the Quechua songbook . . .
[S]he remains dedicatedto teaching in Quechua; she teaches a lesson every
other Monday afternoon for the Womens League and a womens Bible
class every Sunday morning (Hornberger, 1994: 247). Marias case, repre-
sentative of thousands like her in the Andes, illustrates the role of the IEP
(and other evangelical Christian churches) in fostering Quechua literacy
acquisition, with a primary goal of socialisation to Christianity (albeit a
Quechua expression of Christianity) rather than to Quechua culture per se,
and with a pattern of control involving missionaries systematic devolu-
tion of leadership positions to local Quechua (and Spanish) -speaking
Christians.
Incontrast, the Academy of the Quechua Language has perhaps less of a
role in directly promoting Quechua literacy acquisition than the IEP, but
has a more primary orientation to fostering Quechua language and culture
and a more immediate control by local Quechua speakers (albeit urban,
bilingual mestizo intellectuals rather than rural Indian monolingual
Quechua speakers, cf. Nio-Murcia, 1997: 134, 147). The ALQ, founded in
1953, regularlyoffers Quechua instruction, publishes books andjournals in
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 177
Quechua, sponsors an annual competition of Quechua literary works
(drama, poetry, short stories, novels), and broadcasts programmes
promoting Quechua onlocal radio networks (Hornberger, 1994: 245; Nio-
Murcia, 1997: 148), all without government support andoutside the formal
educational system.
More recent institutional efforts to promote Quechua literacy are those
of CADEP and other non-governmental organisations implementing
mother tongue adult literacy programmes in rural communities of the
department of Cusco beginning in the 1990s. The CADEP literacy projects,
for example, explicitly seek to foster literacy acquisition in Quechua (as a
first literacy), using culturally relevant materials for rural Quechua
speakers and in particular for women, and working with existing local
leadershiporganisations, suchas the Federationof Peasant Womenof Anta
(FEMCA) (Chirinos, 1996: 25165).
Although each of the above efforts falls somewhat short of a total
commitment to Quechua literacy acquisition, Quechua socialisation, and
Quechua community control, they do nevertheless contribute to facili-
tating and fostering intragroup communication and goal attainment for
Quechua speakers. Furthermore, despite Fishmans admonition to the
contrary (1991: 96), these efforts afford group members some degree of
social mobilityinthe wider society, at least for those inleadershippositions
in the institutions: the CADEP programme contributes to the empower-
ment of FEMCAs leadership, the ALQandits activities affordits members
access to symbolic cultural capital (Nio-Murcia, 1997: 147), and the IEPs
evangelisation efforts continuously recruit new leaders from among its
members. It is still true, though, that in this, the last stage in the first and
most difficult phase of RLS (Fishman, 1991: 98), it is primarily internal
rather than external reward that must sustain the efforts of Quechua
speakers for the revitalisation of their language.
Stage 4: Schools in lieu of compulsory education
Stage 4 of the GIDS focuses on use of Quechua in lower education
systems which meet the requirements of state compulsory education laws.
There are two forms which schooling may take at this stage of the GIDS:
type a, whichis substantiallyunder Quechua curricular andstaffingcontrol;
and type b, which offers some instruction via Quechua, but is substantially
under state control. Type a schooling entails community organisation and,
often, community capital in order to develop a cultural space to educate
the communitys children in the manner which the community, not the
state, deems appropriate (Fishman, 1991: 100). Type b involves greater
compromise on the part of the state primarily since this education will be
178 The Americas
conductedandpaidfor out of general taxfunds (Fishman, 1991: 100), even
while some instruction is carried out in Quechua.
The Saraguros once again provide a useful example to illustrate how
these two steps have developed as part of community based efforts to
maintain and revitalise Quechua, but also in conjunction with national
policychanges andgovernment compromises. Type a schools, those which
are organised and run by community members, but recognised as meeting
state educational requirements, have existed in Saraguro
9
for 15 years.
These schools, known as escuelas activas active schools were formed in
three Saraguro communities in the mid-1980s by a small group of indige-
nous teachers in response to what they sawto be the generally inadequate
and often detrimental teaching methodologies of traditional state schools.
The teachers felt the methodology of the traditional schools was lacking in
two respects: the schools were unconcerned with the individual, intellec-
tual, and emotional needs of the indigenous students, and they failed to
support or, in the opinion of some, actually undermined, indigenous
Quechua culture and language.
With financial and technical assistance from an international organisa-
tion, but also through use of their own personal funds, this group of local
indigenous teachers established escuelas activas in three Saraguro commu-
nities. Similar in some ways to Montessori schools, the active schools
pedagogical philosophy and practice are based on the theory that children
learn best through hands-on self-directed manipulation of pedagogical
materials. Instruction is individualised and for the most part, teachers
attempt to serve primarily as guides, respecting the childs level of intellec-
tual and emotional development. Emphasis is also given to supporting
indigenous culture. One of the principal goals of the school is to recapture
and revitalise the traditional ways of the community, including dress,
language, food, and lifestyle. Although currently the teachers salaries at
the active schools are paid for by the state, much of the cost and all of the
initiative for the development of these schools came from the Saraguro
communities. The state has tolerated their existence, but has not fully
supported them.
In 1989, policy shifts at the national level brought major changes to
education in Saraguro, not just for the active schools in Saraguro, but for all
schools serving indigenous populations. Type b schools, those which
include Quechua language and culture, yet are financed by and incorpo-
rated into the state system, arose in Saraguro as part of national-level gains
hard won by indigenous political groups. In November of 1989, after a
decade of pressure, the DireccinNacional de EducacinIntercultural Indgena
Bilinge National Directorate of Bilingual Intercultural Indigenous Educa-
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 179
tion (DINEIIB) was established by the federal government. An arm of
(what was then) the Ministry of Education and Culture, DINEIIB was
created to administer schools in indigenous areas.
10
At the local level this
meant that in all areas where more than half of the school-age population
identifies as indigenous, schooling is directed by the regional indigenous
education directorate which reports to DINEIIB. In Saraguro this resulted
in dramatic changes. One of the two local indigenous political organisa-
tions became responsible for administering all indigenous education in the
region, includingmakinghiringandpromotiondecisions, conductingstaff
development sessions, and setting some aspects of the schools curricula.
While controlled by indigenous persons at the local level, the schools are
part of the national education system, ultimately reporting not just to
DINEIIB, but to the national education ministry. Thus, the creation of
DINEIIB resulted in the emergence of schools which approximated type b;
although they are administered locally by Quechua, the schools are ulti-
mately controlled and financed by the state.
For both the type a (active) schools and the type b (DINEIIB) schools in
Saraguro, issues of language and culture are at the forefront of planning
and politics. Teachers and administrators at both types of schools are
concerned about instruction of Quechua-as-a-second language in partic-
ular and the revitalisation of Quechua in general. Some schools of both
types attempt to teach anduse Quechua with varying degrees of regularity
and success. It is important to note that no schools in Saraguro, however,
have established immersion programmes where Quechua is used as the
language of instruction for all, or even part of the day.
The Saraguro case brings two important points to the surface. First,
indigenous control of schools does not necessarily ensure that the indige-
nous language will be emphasised, or even included. There are schools in
Saraguro, for example, which although run by indigenous Saraguros, do
not regularly teach Quechua. The second point is that the control and
financing of schooling may be too complex to be neatly dichotomised into
only two types. As the Saraguro case illustrates, there are complex and fine
gradations of government support, control, and financing of indigenous
education. And looking beyond Saraguro, there are numerous indigenous
language education programmes which cannot be clearly designated as
type a or as type b. One such well known example is the multinational,
Bilingual Intercultural Education Project (known by its Spanish acronym
PEBI) which was largely funded and designed by a German organisation,
but which also cooperated and collaborated with the education ministries
of several Andean governments, and in some instances with indigenous
organisations.
11
PEBI schools were thus neither under the direct control of
180 The Americas
Quechua communities, nor the national governments. Both the local
Saraguro case and the multinational PEBI example suggest that the two
GIDS sub-stages may be insufficient to capture the diverse configurations
of community and government involvement in education.
Stage 3: The local/regional (i.e. non-neighbourhood) work sphere,
both among Quechua and non-Quechua
Stage 3 on the GIDS emphasises the use of Quechua in work spheres
which are outside of the traditional Quechua communities and involves
interaction between indigenous and non-indigenous persons in the threat-
ened language. For the most part, this stage has not yet been reached for
Quechua. Regular use of Quechua outside of traditional indigenous
contexts is unusual; use of Quechua by non-indigenous persons is exceed-
ingly rare. Despite these facts and the continued stigmatisation of indige-
nous languages in much of South America which lies behind them,
Quechua has gained some ground in recent years.
Quechua has become popular among businesses which cater to foreign
tourists and certain upper-class sectors of the national population. A stroll
down the streets of the commercial sectors of Quito or Cusco, for example,
reveals a large number of tourist agencies, souvenir shops andart galleries with
Quechua names. In large open-air markets, such as those of Otavalo, Ecuador
and Chinchero and Pisaq, Peru, where indigenous retailers sell sweaters,
weavings, blankets, bags and other goods to national and international tour-
ists alike, Quechua is used both as a promotional tool and for conversation
amongvendors. Inthriving, urbancommercial centres suchas Juliaca, Peruand
Cochabamba, Bolivia, where large numbers of rural Quechua speakers migrate
both cyclically and permanently, transactions in both street markets and
commercial establishments commonly occur in Quechua. These transactions
in Quechua embrace all types of commerce: including food, clothing, elec-
tronics, hardware, appliances, automotive, among others.
The use of Quechua in these work spheres is one indicator of its status
and cach in certain circles. However, it is important to note that Quechua
remains outside of the traditional channels of authority and power.
Quechua is still not used regularly, for example, for formal interaction in
banks, police stations or, as noted below, in government offices. Further-
more, the crucial point should not be overlooked that use of Quechua
among non-indigenous persons is extremely uncommon. And despite the
fashionability of Quechua in some contexts, the burden of bilingualism
continues to fall uniformly on indigenous shoulders.
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 181
Stage 2: Local/regional mass media and governmental services
At the level of local government and mass media (Stage 2), the use of
Quechua is even less noticeable than in the education and work domains
(Stages 4 and 3). Fishman notes that this stage, and Stage 1 which follows,
are reached by relatively few RLS movements, since they involve taking
on the most powerful and most central institutions and processes of the
polity (1991: 106). Nevertheless, there are some instances of Quechua use
in these domains, which can be seen as exceptions proving the rule.
In Peru, for example, Quechua is in use at local health posts in rural
communities. These posts, set up by the Ministry of Health, are often
staffed by local community members trained as health promoters who use
Quechua in interaction with their fellow community members. There is
also radio programming in Quechua by regional radio stations (e.g. Radio
Tawantinsuyu in Cusco or Radio Kollasuyu in Puno), directed mainly to
rural communitymembers andbroadcast at hours of the daysuitedto their
agricultural work cycle (such as the early morning hours when most urban
dwellers are still asleep) (see also Hornberger, 1988: 1701). Both these
cases exemplifythe marginalityof Quechua inthese domains, as well as the
lack of control of Quechua community members over these uses of
Quechua.
Grade 1: Higher education, occupational, governmental, and
mass media efforts
What use there is of Quechua in local level government or mass media
emanates primarilyfromnational or international level initiatives andthus
falls under Stage 1. The paradox here is that, while Fishman emphasises
that Stage 1 essentially represents the arrival of the pursuit of cultural
autonomy (1991: 107), for Quechuas this is not the case. Even when
Quechua is put to use for national and international educational, occupa-
tional, governmental, and mass media purposes, it is very often still out of
Quechua speakers control.
For example, in the higher education domain, Quechua is widely avail-
able as an academic subject in universities and institutes worldwide,
including more than a dozen sites in the United States, and several in
Britain, Germany, Japan, and elsewhere.
12
It is also offered in universities
andinstitutes inEcuador, Bolivia, andPeru.
13
Indeedthe Chair of Quechua
studies at the University of San Marcos, Lima, dates back more than four
hundredyears (Grinevald, 1998: 147). Yet none of these efforts is primarily
initiated and controlled by Quechua speakers, but rather by interested
linguists or anthropologists, who often employ native Quechua speakers
182 The Americas
as instructors. Furthermore, the availability of these courses to indigenous
Quechuas is highly limited, for socioeconomic reasons.
14
Similarly, there is by now, dating from the 1960s, a considerable accu-
mulation of top-down national and international initiatives to introduce
Quechua as subject and medium of instruction in primary education in
Peru, Ecuador, and Bolivia; yet again few of these are in the hands of
Quechua speakers.
15
One clear exception to this pattern is the creation of
the DINEIIBinEcuador (mentionedabove), whichhas empoweredindige-
nous Ecuadorians to formulate and implement school policy, curriculum,
and staffing in the indigenous sectors of the population.
16
One such top-down initiative which was originally intendedto raise the
status of Quechua not only in the schools, but also in government, occupa-
tional, and media spheres as well, was the 1975 Officialisation of Quechua
in Peru which made Quechua an official language of Peru, co-equal with
Spanish, and specified that it would be taught at all levels of education
beginningin1976 andusedinall court actions involvingQuechua speakers
beginning in 1977; these innovations were to be implemented by various
ministries of the government. The decree was immediately followedby the
launching of a national daily Quechua newspaper by one of Perus major
dailies (Cronicawan, published by La Crnica), an effort which was unfortu-
nately short-lived due primarily to a lack of readership. The reaction of
Perus Spanish-speaking majority against Quechua Officialisation is by
now well known. In fact, it was in part because of the Spanish-speaking
majoritys resistance that the implementation of this language policy fell
far short of the stated goals. Indeed, as Coronel-Molina has pointed out, if
real progress is to be made in reversing language shift from Quechua to
Spanish, it is not primarily Quechua speakers who should be the target of
revalorisationefforts, but rather the hegemonic Spanish-speaking majority
(1999: 177).
Coronel-Molina also points to the shameful irony in the fact that, while
denigrated in its home countries, Quechua is valued internationally, as
evidenced for example in its spread on the electronic media, with
numerous webpages on the internet, as well as a host of Quechua soft-
ware programs (1999: 171). He goes on to describe a newradio program-
ming effort, . . . uqanchik, . . . distributed via e-mail and the internet to
radio stations throughout the Andean region, as well as maintaining a
web site on the World Wide Web (1999: 171172). In light of develop-
ments such as these, he notes the paradox that it is more likely that
Quechua speakers, given the proper technology, could communicate
withforeigners fromaroundthe globe thanwiththe majority of their own
countrymen (1999: 171).
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 183
The international prestige of Quechua is indeed a double-edged sword
for Quechua revitalisation efforts, especially vis--vis the braindrain that
Fishman alludes to as a possible threat once Stage 2 is reached (1991: 106).
Quechua activists in leadership positions can on the one hand receive
invaluable moral, intellectual, and financial support from beyond their
national boundaries to advance the cause of Quechua revitalisation at
home; but on the other, they may also find themselves crossing those
national boundaries to seek more favourable venues for their efforts,
thereby leaving their own national compatriots to fend for themselves. To
the extent that reversing Quechua language shift is truly an international
endeavour, this is all to the good, but to the extent that it also requires
community-level fostering of intergenerational interaction in Quechua, an
internationallymobile leadershipcannot provide the necessaryday-to-day
modelling and practice of Quechua discourse.
Indeed, an internationally mobile Quechua activist leadership becomes
peculiarly susceptible to the question Fishman poses, of whether having
reached this stage, the promoters of Xish [Quechua] are now merely
moderns, like all other moderns, but in Xish? (1991: 108). While this ques-
tion has not really arisen in any great measure for the vast majority of
Quechua speakers, it is very much present for those who have pursued
international avenues for reversing Quechua language shift; ironically so,
since they pursuedthose avenues in most cases because they were the only
possible routes to promoting Quechua.
Conclusion
As is clear fromthe above discussion, the process of reversing Quechua
language shift in the Andes is highly complicated and difficult to summa-
rise neatly. While the GIDS framework is helpful for organising and
analysing various efforts taking place at different levels and in different
Quechua communities, the Quechua case also serves to highlight some
unanswered questions implicit in the framework. In the paragraphs that
follow, we briefly touch on some of these unresolved issues, organised
around the questions: who? what? when? and where?
Who?
Who are the activists that carry out RLS? The GIDS framework tends to
assume that pro-RLSers are in all cases members of the language commu-
nityandfurthermore that RLSoftenbegins withthe older generation(1991:
391, 98). We have seen in the Quechua case, however, that non-Quechua
entities such as international linguists, non-governmental organisations,
184 The Americas
and missionary churches may also be effectively involved in, and indeed
initiators of, RLS efforts. Yet, as Grinevald points out, throughout South
America, the lack of locally based, indigenous language projects is prob-
lematic, and the need for truly collaborative linguistic efforts is real (1998:
14759). Furthermore, we have also noted the important role that the
younger, oftenhighlypoliticised, generationmayplayinRLSinpromoting
cultural activities which highlight the use of Quechua, as well as in their
important potential choice to raise their children in Quechua-speaking
households.
A more profound issue, perhaps, is the question of who the Quechua
activists are or become as they carry out RLS efforts. To what degree is it
possible for Quechua people, who have for centuries made up the rural,
poor, and marginalised sectors of their societies, to be engaged in higher
level RLS activities such as those represented in Stages 41 and still be
authentically Quechua? Luykx has written about indigenous teachers in
Bolivia, part of whose socialisation [as teachers] involve[s] coming to
grips with the fact that the achievement of professional status [will] distance
them from their ethnic and class origins, while simultaneously requiring
themto live and work among those fromwhomthey [have] differentiated
themselves (1996: 246). The Quechua activists plight seems similar. As
Fishman suggests, the question of authenticity, and the tensions between
traditionalists andmodernists responses toit, is perhaps anunending, but
not necessarily unproductive, unresolved issue in RLS (1991: 109).
What?
What are the stages of RLS? Although the GIDS is intended as a quasi-
implicational scale (Fishman, 1991: 87), in some cases the discreteness and
sequencing of the stages in actual RLS efforts is not clear-cut. In the
Quechua case, for example, we saw that schooling administration issues
within Stage 4 are too complex to be neatly divided up into a and b, and
could perhaps be more usefully conceptualised as a continuum. Similarly,
Stage 8 and Stage 1 appear to overlap somewhat in view of the fact that
adult acquisition of Quechua (Stage 8) occurs very often in institutions of
higher education or teacher professional development (Stage 1). Fishman
has noted that the GIDS framework represents a heuristic theoretical
stance, rather than a fully proven verity (1991: 396). The Quechua case
suggests that the stages are perhaps best seen as exactly that a useful
heuristic, rather than a step-by-step prescription.
When?
When are the stages of RLS to be carried out? This question also
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 185
addresses the discreteness and sequencing of the stages, and in particular
the relationship of Stage 6 to all the rest. It seems to us that, from both a
theoretical and practical standpoint, Stage 6, the attainment of
intergenerational informal oralcy, stands outside the other stages, as a
central focus before, during, and after all other RLS efforts.
Stage 6, according to Fishman, is the heart of the entire inter-
generational transmission pursuit and the sine qua non of the initial stages
thereof (1991: 398). The centrality of Stage 6 to the language revitalisation
endeavour is partly based on the premise that functional differentiation
between languages leads to stable bilingualism within a community.
Fishman notes that an RLS focus on Stages 8 through 5 presumes a stable
bilingual model of . . . society in which . . . diglossia is attained and main-
tained (1991: 400). Luykx (1998) has recently argued, with reference to the
Andean case, that it is precisely diglossias functional differentiation that
allows for the maintenance of bilingualism. Ina relatedcase of a threatened
language, native Montagnais in Quebec, Drapeau raises the question
whether code-switching, which may be construed as a manifestation of
balanced bilingual competence on the part of middle-aged adults, when it
occurs incaretaker speechinsteadturns intoerosionof lexical skills among
the younger generation (1995: 163). All of this suggests that use of Quechua
in the home is the most important effort Quechua activists can undertake.
And yet, we have seen how hard it is for many of them to carry it out.
The ordering of Stage 6 in the scale implies that it must take precedence
over Stages 5 through1; repeatedly Fishmantells us that Stages 4 through1
must be linked to Stage 6 (1991: 4024). Yet, we have seen that, for better or
for worse, reversing Quechua language shift in the Andes has not neatly
climbed up the RLS stages of the framework. Rather, in at least some
Andean communities, Quechua has been promoted, used and (to some
extent) acquired in public, often culturally symbolic and political domains
first and foremost, with daily face-to-face household interaction in
Quechua remaining an elusive goal. Given this practical reality, must we
assume that all the other Quechua RLS efforts are in vain? Rather, it seems
important to locate this all-important dimension of RLS outside the stages,
as either an overarching or underpinning essential to all of them, to be
pursued at all times.
In a similar vein, Fettes (1997) has suggested that effective language
renewal practices are best conceived of as a triple braid interwoven of
three discursive strands: (1) critical literacy, or what Fishman would
likely term initial ideological clarification; (2) local knowledges, Fish-
mans institutional domain; and (3) living relationships, Fishmans Stage
6. The concept of the braidis meant toremindus that one approachis never
186 The Americas
enough. Only when woven together can the strands endure (1997: 315).
Furthermore, concerning Stage 6 in particular, Fettes suggests, as we do
above, that it is best conceptualised as a long-term and perhaps over-
arching goal, noting that any meaningful long-term relationship
conducted in the language helps to establish an intergenerational network
of relationships, which clearly at some stage should involve children but
which may not reattain the stage of stable transmission as a first language
for years, possibly even generations (1997: 311).
Where?
Where are RLS efforts carried out? We return here to the tremendous
linguistic, sociolinguistic, and geographical diversity that characterises
Quechua, with its several million speakers spread across several national
boundaries. In this, the case of Quechua shares similarities with that of the
Mayan languages in Guatemala, Mexico, and Belize (England, 1998;
Grinevald, 1998: 140) and the Eskimo languages in Alaska (US), Canada,
Greenland, and Russia (Iutzi-Mitchell & Graburn, 1993). A recent status
and prospectus report on Eskimo languages included recommendations
which seem relevant for the Quechua case as well, and which suggest
considerations for RLS in general.
Top among these is the continuing need for focused understanding of
the status and processes of Quechua language shift and revitalisation in
specific communities and regions. With regard to the Eskimo languages,
Krauss calls for focused research . . . on the social, cultural, and linguistic
processes of the ever-shifting boundaries between the simply endangered
Eskimo language regions and those where the languages have become
moribund and Gumperz urges the use of ethnography of communication
in order to discover the discursive uses to which Eskimo people put the
colonial [and indigenous] languages when speaking with each other
(Krauss and Gumperz as reported in Iutzi-Mitchell & Graburn, 1993: 130).
Above, we used our own ethnographies of communication to corroborate
large-scale language surveys, as well as to illuminate some of the processes
at work in language shift (Hornberger, 1988; King, to appear). This kind of
detaileddescriptionof howlanguages are currently being usedandvalued
within specific communities can provide a sound basis for planning to
reverse shift.
However, a focus on social, cultural, and linguistic processes in these
communities is insufficient economic processes are crucial as well.
Indeed, Grenoble andWhaleysuggest that economics . . . maybe the single
strongest force influencing the fate of endangered languages and they go
onto note the role of economics inthe pressure to assimilate to the econom-
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 187
ically dominant culture and the availability of published materials,
schools, teachers, radio and television broadcasting (1998: 523). An
example fromaboriginal Australia, reportedby Haviland(in Tomei, 1995),
graphically demonstrates the impact of politico-economic forces onendan-
geredlanguages. The Guugu Yimidhirr-speaking community of Hopevale
in Queensland was founded by missionaries in 1886 with the purpose of
educating and protecting remnants of Aboriginal tribes from the area.
Major language shift did not take place until World War II, when the entire
populationof the missionwas, without warning, suddenlyevacuatedtoan
inland Aboriginal settlement; they were eventually permitted to return in
the early 1950s, but to a site about 25 kilometres inland from the original
location. By then, the balance between English and Guugu Yimidhirr had
been disrupted; andby late 1983, the Aboriginal language was in danger of
disappearing. However, this changedwiththe passingof a newAboriginal
Land Act which made provisions for Aboriginal claims to certain lands in
Queensland, withthe result that youngpeople whohadgivenupspeaking
Guugu Yimidhirr ha[ve] begun to speak it again, seeing the language as a
necessary part of making a land claim on traditional lands (Tomei, 1995:
1812). In this case, the question of where? is quite literal: specifically,
where are the people and their language in relation to their lands? In light
of Quechuas centuries of marginalisation to the most remote Andean
highlands, their increasingmigrationfromrural communities tourbanand
coastal areas in search of employment, and a language ideology which ties
the language very closely to the rural community (Hornberger, 1988: 100),
the question of land is a highly significant one for the future of Quechua as
well.
17
Beyond the need for focused research about the specifics of specific
communities, another key to minority language survival, Krauss said, is
the creation of newnotions of nationhood, cross-cutting the nation state. If
the Inuit of Greenland, Canada, andnorthernAlaska cancome to see them-
selves as one people, . . . then perhaps through common effort and shared
resources theywill be able toachieve their goal of survival, as a people with
their own language and culture, through the twenty-first century (Iutzi-
Mitchell & Graburn, 1993: 131). The same could be said for Quechua. Not
only are Quechua speakers present across several nation-states, but we
have also seen evidence of a worldwide extension of Quechua through
electronic media and courses of study at institutions of higher education.
Efforts toreverse Quechua language shift will have totake intoaccount, not
just the local reality of specific Quechua communities, but also the multina-
tional character of Quechua use andthe global realityof Quechuas place in
the world.
188 The Americas
Along these lines, Grenoble and Whaley, building on the work of
Edwards (1992), consider the merits of a typology of endangered
languages which distinguishes between micro-level (community-
internal) variables and macro-level (community-external) variables and
whichfurther organises macro-level variables at a number of levels: Local,
Regional, National, and Extra-National (1998: 52). Similarly, in a recent
consideration of bilingual education efforts in Peru over the past half
century, Freeland concludes that we need to reconsider the roles of institu-
tions at each level global, national, and local. Noting that international
multilateral organisations are too large and too influenced by prevailing
economic ideology to be sensitive to local realities, nation-states are too
weak, and NGOs are too small and too localised to be more than initiators
and pump-primers, she argues for targeting efforts to the development of
institutions occupying an intermediate level between international and
domestic, global and national (1996: 1845). A recent initiative along these
lines is the Programa de Formacin en Educacin Intercultural Bilinge para los
Paises Andinos Andean Program in Bilingual Intercultural Education
(PROEIB-Andes), a regional MA programme sponsored by a five-nation
consortium with coordination between Ministries of Education, universi-
ties, andindigenous organisations (Bolivia, Chile, Colombia, Ecuador, and
Peru), established at the University of San Simn in Cochabamba, Bolivia,
in 1997. Efforts such as these for Quechua and other South American
languages (Grinevald, 1998: 14950), as indeedfor Eskimo, Mayan, or other
endangered languages, hold much promise for reversing language shift.
Not only do efforts such as PROEIB-Andes cut across nation-state
boundaries, taking into account ethnolinguistically based concepts of
nationhood, but they also, directly or indirectly, continue the tradition of
working to reverse Quechua language shift at multiple stages simulta-
neously. For example, the MA programme not only involves use of
Quechua in higher education (Stage 1) and promotes its use in the work
sphere (Stage 3) and in public schools (Stage 4), but it also continues the
process of reconstructionof the language andadult acquisitionof Quechua
(Stage 8), as well as facilitating cultural interaction in the language (Stage
7). Given the tremendous linguistic, sociolinguistic, and geographic diver-
sity of Quechua outlinedabove, coupledwiththe languages unfavourable
odds for long-term survival, efforts like this, which target many of
Fishmans stages concurrently, seem to hold the most hope for the uncer-
tain future of the language.
18
Notes
1. For example, Quechua enjoyed legal recognition as an official language of Peru
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 189
under Velasco Alvarados presidency in the 1970s; Ecuador saw the rise of a
powerful national political organization in the early 1980s, CONAIE, which
continues torepresent the indigenous populationof that country; andthe Boliv-
ian government passed sweeping educational reforms in the 1990s, many of
which were directedat improving educational services for the indigenous pop-
ulation. See Hornberger (2000) for further discussion of these developments.
2. Coronel-Molina (1997) presents an overviewof the threatened status of Quech-
ua in Peruvian society, reviewing the functional domains that Quechua
currently does or does not serve, in terms of Stewarts (1968) 10 language func-
tions.
3. Quechua is the term used to refer to the varieties spoken in Peru, Bolivia, and
parts of northern Chile; it is also the cover termfor all varieties of the language.
Quichua is used exclusively for varieties in Ecuador. The difference in terms
has to do with the differing phonological evolution of the language in Ecua-
dor, as compared to the other countries. In the former case, the uvular stop /q/
has beenlost, andwithit the loweringof the /i/vowel to/e/inproximitytothe
/q/.
4. Ecuadorian Quichua planners, for example, rejected the use of the Bolivian and
Peruvian k and favoured instead c and q, and likewise they rejected the w
in favour of hu.
5. The distribution of Quechua speakers is estimated as follows: Peru (4,402,023),
Ecuador (2,233,000), Bolivia (1,594,000), Argentina (120,000), Colombia (4,402),
and Brazil (700) (Cerrn-Palomino, 1987: 76).
6. Saville-Troike (1989: 18), however, would probably consider Quechua speak-
ers to be a speechcommunity since they comprise a racial andethnic group, one
of the possible definitional criteria she cites.
7. We use the term activists to refer to those actively engaged in promoting the
Quechua language and its revitalisation; our term is equivalent to Fishmans
pro-RLS advocates or pro-RLSers.
8. Rumiahui was a loyal Inca general during the initial phase of the Spanish con-
quest andis alsothe name of a character inthe colonial Quechua drama, Ollanta,
revived in this century and currently performed annually in the town of
Ollantaytambo in Cuscos Sacred Valley.
9. Saraguro refers to a particular group of Quechua people, but also to the parish
inwhichtheylive. For more informationonthe Saraguros, see Belote andBelote
(1994) or URL [www.saraguro.org].
10. The name has since changed to the Direccin Nacional de Educacin Intercultural
Bilinge National Directorate of Bilingual Intercultural Education (DINEIB).
11. See Hornberger (1988) for more information on PEBI (PEEB) in Peru.
12. In the United States, this includes past and present offerings at Cornell Univer-
sity, Stanford University, University of California at Los Angeles, University of
Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, University of New Mexico, University of Pitts-
burgh, University of Texas at Austin, University of Wisconsin at Madison.
European universities include Liverpool, St Andrews, London, Bonn, and
Hamburg.
13. These include, for example, the Instituto de Idiomas Maryknoll and the
Universidad de San Simn in Cochabamba, Bolivia; the Instituto de Pastoral
Andina in Cusco, Peru; and the Universidad de Cuenca, Ecuador.
14. Compare the case of Hawaiian: Arecent bill introducedinthe Hawaiianlegisla-
190 The Americas
ture would waive tuition for any Hawaiian enrolled in a Hawaiian language
course (Kapono, 1994: 131). For Quechua speakers, such an initiative would
only begin to address issues of access (Grenoble & Whaley, 1998: 53).
15. See Hornberger (2000) for an overview of recent initiatives in Peru, Ecuador,
and Bolivia, and for additional references on earlier initiatives.
16. See Hornberger (1996) for more onbothtop-downandbottom-upefforts topro-
mote Quechua literacy in the Andes.
17. Teresa McCartyhas alsorecentlyraisedthese issues for the case of Navajointhe
US (McCarty, in preparation).
18. We would like to thank Kenneth Hyltenstamand other seminar participants at
the Centre for Research on Bilingualism at Stockholm Univerisity for their
thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this paper. This paper was also
presentedat the Conference onNationalism, IdentityandMinorityRights, Uni-
versity of Bristol, UK, September 1999.
References
Adelaar, W.F H. (1991) The endangered languages problem: South America. In
R.H. RobinandE.M. Uhlenbeck(eds) Endangered Languages (pp. 4591). London:
Berg Publishers.
Alb, X. (1977) El Futuro de Los Idiomas Oprimidos en Los Andes. Lima: Centro de
Investigacin de Lingstica Aplicada.
Belote, L. and Belote, J. (1981) Development in spite of itself: The Saraguro case. In
N.E. Whitten, Jr (ed.) Cultural Transformations and Ethnicity in Modern Ecuador
(pp. 45076). Urbana-Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Belote, L. and Belote, J. (eds) (1994) Los Saraguros. Fiesta y Ritualidad. Quito: Abya-
Yala; Universidad Politcnica Salesiana.
Bentez, L. and Garcs, A. (1992) Culturas Ecuatorianas Ayer y Hoy. Sixth edition,
Quito: Abya-Yala.
Botero, L.F. (1991) La iglesia y el Indio en la colonia. In J. Botasso (ed.) Poltica
Indigenista de la Iglesia en la Colonia (pp. 55144). Quito, Ecuador: Abya-Yala,
MLAL.
Cerrn-Palomino, R. (1987) Lingistica Quechua. Cusco: Centro de Estudios Rurales
Andinos Bartolom de Las Casas.
Cerrn-Palomino, R. (1989) Language policy in Peru: a historical overview. Interna-
tional Journal of the Sociology of Language 77, 1133.
Cerrn-Palomino, R. (1997) Pasado y presente del quechua. Yachay Wasi 4, 4964.
Chirinos, A. (1996) An experience of indigenous literacy in Peru. In N.H.
Hornberger (ed.) Indigenous Literacies in the Americas: Language Planning from the
Bottom up (pp. 25165). Berlin: Mouton.
Chuquimamani Valer, R. (1983) Unay Pachas: Qheshwa Simipi Qollasuyu
Aranwaykuna (Vol. 1). Lima, Peru: Proyecto Experimental de Educacin
Bilinge/Puno.
Chuquimamani Valer, R. (1984) Unay Pachas: Qhishwa Simipi Qullasuyu
Hawariykuna (Vol. 2). Cusco, Peru: Centro de Estudios Rurales Andinos
Bartolom de las Casas.
Chuquimamani Valer, R. (1988) Una muestra del conflicto lingstico nacional enel
habla de los sollocoteos: El caso del parto. In L.E. Lpez (ed.) Pesquisas en
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 191
Lingstica Andina, (pp. 16380). Lima-Puno, Peru: CONCYTEC/UNA-Puno/
GTZ.
Coronel-Molina, S. (1997). Language policy: Status planning for the Quechua lan-
guage in Peru. Working Papers in Educational Linguistics 13(1), 3148.
Coronel-Molina, S. (1999) Functional domains of the Quechua language in Peru: Is-
sues of status planning. International Journal of Bilingual Education and
Bilingualism 2(3), 166180.
de Vries, L. (1988) Poltica Lingstica en Ecuador, Peru y Bolivia. Quito, Ecuador:
Proyecto EBI-CEDIME.
Dorian, N. (1998) Western language ideologies and small-language prospects. In L.
Grenoble and L. Whaley (eds) Endangered Languages: Current Issues and Future
Prospects (pp. 321). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Drapeau, L. (1995) Code switching incaretaker speechandbilingual competence in
a native village of northern Quebec. International Journal of the Sociology of Lan-
guage 113, 15764.
Edwards, J. (1992) Sociopolitical aspects of language maintenance and loss: To-
wards a typology of minority language situations. In W. Fase, K. Jaspaert and S.
Kroon (eds) Maintenance and Loss of Minority Languages (pp. 3754). Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
England, N.C. (1998) Mayan efforts toward language preservation. In L. Grenoble
and L. Whaley (eds) Endangered Languages: Current Issues and Future Prospects
(pp. 99116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fettes, M. (1997) Stabilizing what? An ecological approach to language renewal. In
Jon Reyhner (ed.) Teaching Indigenous Languages (pp. 30118). Flagstaff: North-
ern Arizona University and Center for Excellence in Education.
Fishman, J.A. (1990) What is reversinglanguage shift (RLS) andhowcanit succeed?
Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 11(1 & 2), 535.
Fishman. J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Freeland, J. (1996) The global, the national and the local: Forces in the development
of education for indigenous peoples the case of Peru. Compare 26(2), 16795.
Grenoble, L. and Whaley. L. (1998) Toward a typology of language endangerment.
In L. Grenoble and L. Whaley (eds) Endangered Languages: Current Issues and Fu-
ture Prospects (pp. 2254). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Grimes B.F. (ed.) (1996) Ethnologue. Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics, Inc.
[WWW Document] URL http: // www.sil.org/ ethnologue/ ethnologue.html.
Grinevald, C. (1998) Language endangerment in South America: A programmatic
approach. In L. Grenoble and L. Whaley (eds) Endangered Languages: Current Is-
sues andFuture Prospects (pp. 12459). Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.
Haboud, M.I. (1996) Quichua and Spanish in the Ecuadorian highlands: The effects
of long-term contact. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon,
OR.
Heath, S.B. (1982) Castilian colonization and indigenous languages: The cases of
QuechuaandAymara. InR. Cooper (ed.) Language Spread: Studies inDiffusionand
Social Change (pp. 118147). Bloomington: Indiana University Press/C.A.L.,
Washington, DC.
Hornberger, N.H. (1988) Bilingual Education and Language Maintenance: A Southern
Peruvian Quechua Case. Berlin: Mouton.
Hornberger, N.H. (1992) Verse analysis of The Condor and the Shepherdess. In B.
192 The Americas
Swann (ed.) On the Translation of Native American Literatures (pp. 44169). Wash-
ington, DC : Smithsonian Institution Press.
Hornberger, N.H. (1994) Continua of biliteracy: Quechua literacy and empower-
ment in Peru. In L. Verhoeven (ed.) Functional Literacy: Theoretical Issues and
Educational Implications (pp. 23756). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Hornberger, N.H. (ed.) (1996) Indigenous Literacies in the Americas: Language
Planning from the Bottom up. Berlin: Mouton.
Hornberger, N.H. (2000) Bilingual educationpolicyandpractice inthe Andes: Ideo-
logical paradox and intercultural possibility. Anthropology and Education
Quarterly 31(2), 173201.
Hymes, D. (1972) Models of the interaction of language and social life. In J.
Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds) Directions in Sociolinguistics: Ethnography of Com-
munication (pp. 3571). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Iutzi-Mitchell, R. and Graburn, N.H.H. (1993) Language and educational policy in
the North: Status andprospectus report on the Eskimo-Aleut languages froman
International Symposium. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 99,
12332.
Kapono, E. (1994) Hawaiian language revitalization and immersion education. In-
ternational Journal of the Sociology of Language 112, 12135.
Kaufman, T. (1994) The native languages of Latin America. In C. Moseley and R.E.
Asher (eds) Atlas of the Worlds Languages (pp. 3176). London and New York:
Routledge.
King, K.A. (1997). Language revitalization in the Andes: Quichua instruction, use,
and identity in Saraguro, Ecuador. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.
King, K.A. (1999) Language revitalisation processes and prospects: Quichua in the
Ecuadorian Andes. Language and Education 13(1), 1737.
King, K.A. (to appear) Language Revitalisation Processes and Prospects: Quichua in the
Andes. Clevedon, England: Multilingual Matters.
Labov, W. (1972) On the mechanism of linguistic change. In J. Gumperz and D.
Hymes (eds.) Directions in Sociolinguistics: Ethnography of Communication (pp.
51238). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Luykx, A. (1996) From indios to profesionales: Stereotypes and student resistance
in Bolivian teacher training. In B. Levinson, D. Foley and D. Holland (eds.) The
Cultural Production of the Educated Person: Critical Ethnographies of Schooling and
Local Practice (pp. 23972). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
Luykx, A. (1998) La diferencia funcional de cdigos y el futuro de las lenguas
minoritarias. In L.E. Lpez and I. Jung (eds) Sobre la Huellas de la Voz:
Sociolingustica de la Oralidad y la Escritura en su Relacin con la Educacin (pp.
192212). Madrid: Ediciones Morata.
Mannheim, B. (1984) Una nacin acorralada: Southern Peruvian Quechua lan-
guage planning and politics in a historical perspective. Language in Society 13,
291309.
McCarty, T.L. (in preparation) A Place to be Navajo. Unpublished manuscript.
Milroy, L. (1987) Language and Social Networks. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Montaluisa, L.O. (1980) Historia de la escritura del Quichua. Revista de la
Universidad Catlica del Ecuador 28, 12145.
Reversing Quechua Language Shift in South America 193
Nio-Murcia, Mercedes (1997) Linguistic purism in Cuzco, Peru: A historical per-
spective. Language Problems and Language Planning 21(2), 13461.
Parker, G.J. (1973) Clasificacin gentica de los dialectos quechuas. Revista del
Museo Nacional 32, 24152.
Saville-Troike, M. (1989) The Ethnography of Communication: An Introduction. New
York, NY: Basil Blackwell.
Stewart, W. (1968) A sociolinguistic typology for describing national
multilingualism. In J. Fishman (ed.) Readings in the Sociology of Language (pp.
53145). The Hague: Mouton.
Tomei, J. (1995) The practice of preservation: Views from linguists working with
language renewal. Areport ona sessionof the 1993 Meetingof the AmericanAn-
thropological Association. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 115,
17382.
Torero, A. (1974) El Quichua y la Historia Social Andina. Lima, Peru: Universidad
Ricardo Palma.
Volinsky, N. (1996) Standing up: Violin performance technique and Saraguro eth-
nic resurgence. In-progress dissertation draft. University of Illinois, IL.
von Gleich, U. (1992) Changes in the status and function of Quechua. In U. Ammon
andM. Hellinger (eds) Status Changes of Languages (pp. 4364). NewYork: Walter
de Gruyter.
von Gleich, U. (1994) Language spread policy: The case of Quechua in the Andean
republics of Bolivia, Ecuador, and Peru. International Journal of the Sociology of
Language 107, 77113.
194 The Americas
Chapter 8
Irish Language Production and
Reproduction 19811996
P. RIAGIN
Introduction
In Reversing Language Shift, Fishman (1991: 1) attempts to diagnose the
difficulties with reverse language shift policies, and to prescribe
ameliorative and restorative efforts in a sociolinguistically informed way.
The eight-stage model he developed to structure policy options suggests
some kind of evolutionary rationale or temporal sequence and there is
more than one suggestion in the text that Fishman intends his readers to
understand his model in this way. For example, he speaks of the need to
attain certain stages and to transcend others. Viewed this way, there are
some difficulties applying the model to the Irish situation, as Fishman
himself acknowledged (p. 122).
While the Irish case can be seen to have some parallels with other
minority language situations in Europe Wales, Catalonia, the Basque
Country, Friesland, etc., Ireland also differs from most, if not all, of these
examples. Unlike other minority language situations, in Ireland the state
tried to deal with its minority language problemby seeking to re-establish
it as a national language. No other minority language problem in Europe
was tackled in this way, although the cases of the regional languages in
Spain have in recent decades shown some similarities. While that partic-
ular element of the language policy has not succeeded, it gave the overall
policy adopted by the newly independent Irish government in 1922 a
unique shape and character.
Inthe 1926 Census of Population, some 18%of the populationof the state
were returnedas Irish-speakers. The Gaeltacht Commission (1926) estimated
that nearly half of these were concentrated in western areas and, within
that region, roughly half again lived in core Irish-speaking districts (The
Gaeltacht), i.e. about 5% of the total population in the state. (Recent
research(Riagin, 1997, Part II) has shownthat eventhat latter figure was
an overestimate 3% might be a more accurate estimate .)
195
Europe
Irish Language 19811996
Despite the marked regional bias in the distribution of Irish-speakers at
the time towards western areas, the Irish state did not legislate for a bilin-
gual policy organisedon territorial lines. It didnot designate two language
regions, one Irish-speaking and the other English-speaking, within which
each language would be defined as the norm. While an Irish-speaking
region was defined (The Gaeltacht) and special measures were formu-
lated to deal with it, Irish language policy applied to the state as a whole
and not just a region of it. Of course, outside the Irish-speaking areas, Irish
speakers formed negligible proportions of an almost entirely English-
speaking population. In this region, the bilingual policy was not, therefore,
one designed to meet the needs of an already existing bilingual commu-
nity, but rather it sought to create one. The policy was, in that qualified
sense, one of language revival.
Thus fromthe outset, it seems fair tosaythat Irishlanguage policyincor-
porated, to varying degrees, practically all of the stages in Fishmans
model. There was no gradual progression from one stage to the next. Irish
language policy over the 20th century includes, for all or part of the period,
policies that were directed at economic development in the Irish-speaking
areas, language teaching in the schools, recruitment procedures in the
public sector, language standardisation, radio and television services,
together with a myriad of other less important policies to promote and
extend the use of Irish. Some of these policies are or were universal type
policies, that is, designed to affect all members of the public, while others
selectively target particular geographic or social groups. To reverse the
process of language shift, the state had to try to create pressures of suffi-
cient persuasiveness in advance of society itself generating such pressures.
Drawing on the theoretical framework developed by Bourdieu (1991), it
may be said that the state used its authority to change the structure of the
language market prevailing in Ireland in order to enhance the symbolic,
cultural and economic value attaching to competence to speak Irish. This
affected both the national language market and the internal language
market of the Gaeltacht areas and had real economic and political effects.
However, while he frequently presents his stages as actual way-
stations on the road to successful reverse language shift, at other times
Fishman appears to be largely, and at times very perceptively, assessing
the relative importance of a number of common language planning objec-
tives. For example, when discussing Stage 6, he emphasises that unlike
other stages, when it (Stage 6) is transcended it is not merely left behind;
on the contrary: all subsequent stages must be diligently tied back to and
connected with Stage 6 if they are to contribute to the living reality of RLS.
It is easy to see what Fishman is emphasising here. At Stage 6 a self-
196 Europe
priming intergenerational transmissability system is established because
the minority language is (or has become) dominant in at least some neigh-
bourhood and family clusters. This correctly asserts the primacy of
language reproduction over language production, and with this proposi-
tion most Irish speakers would, I think, agree. They would also agree with
Fishman that, when measured against this criterion, Irish language policy
has only had limited success.
However, while the effort to re-establishIrishas a national language has
not been successful, neither can the impact of Irish language policy be
described as negligible. Not only did it slow down a long-established
process of language shift, but it clearly altered the spatial and social struc-
ture of bilingualism in Ireland. After 70 years, the linguistic division of
labour has changed to the extent that, while the original communities of
Irish-speakers have declined in both absolute and relative terms, there has
been an increase in the proportions of Irish-speakers in some, mainly
middle-class, groups elsewhere.
The policies which have effected those changes have largely been poli-
cies which Fishman sees as characteristic of Stages 4 to 1. However, in his
discussion of the Irish case in Reversing Language Shift, Fishman chose to
place his emphasis onsome, largelyvoluntary, programmes aimedat Stage
6 objectives. To present a more rounded picture, in later sections of this
paper I wouldlike toexamine more fullythe impact of formal state policies,
bearing in mind, of course, Fishmans strictures that the impact of these
must always be related to Stage 6 objectives. As an introduction to this
discussion, I will first review the data on language reproduction and
production which has become available since Fishman undertook his
assessment of the Irish experience. At the very outset, however, it is neces-
sary to digress briefly in order to say something about the data available in
Ireland on the language situation.
Data
Irelands census language question provides one of the oldest contin-
uous series of regularly collected, standard public statistics on any
language in the world. Apart from two 15 year intervals, it has been
included every ten years in the national census of population since 1851.
However, until 1996 the main part of the question, as put to census respon-
dents, simply asks if they can speak Irish only or Irish and English. While,
as a measure of language proficiency, the language questioninthe census is
neither very precise nor informative, recent research has shown that the
marginof error incensus data does not invalidate its use for the purposes of
Irish Language 19811996 197
analysis ( Riagin, 1997). However, it should also be noted that the
language ability question askedin the Census of Population includes a less
well known option Read but cannot speak Irish which has only been
reported in published returns in recent censuses. This is presumably
because it is only in the past 20 years or so that the Central Statistics Office
have found a significant number of respondents choosing this option. In
the 1986 Census of Populationsome 330,272 persons (10%of the total popu-
lation) reported that, while they could read Irish, they could not speak it!
This rather unusual linguistic situation, which reflects the emphasis on
literacy skills in school programmes, is confirmed by the evidence of
survey research ( Riagin & Gliasin, 1994).
In the 1996 Census of Population, the language question was changedto
a significant degree. The ability question was shortened to a simple Can
the person speak Irish? (Yes/No). But if the answer was yes, the census
then asked Does the person speak Irish daily, weekly, less often or never.
The results, which were published only when this paper was in the final
stages of writing, were somewhat surprising and perplexing. While the
census confirmed earlier survey findings that a large proportion of those
claiming anability to speak Irishdo not, infact, use it ona regular basis, the
total percentage of such persons in the state rose from 32.5% to 41.1%
between 1991 and 1996 the highest intercensal increase since 1926. It will
require some detailed analysis, and probably further survey work, to
explain this unexpected finding, but my own hypothesis is that respon-
dents who had previously chosen the read but cannot speak Irish option
as a way of recording weak speaking skills, in the 1996 census chose to
return themselves as Irish-speakers rather than declare themselves as
knowing no Irish at all.
Apart fromlanguage data inthe Census of Populationanda small range
of other relevant statistics produced by certain government departments
(e.g. school examination results), practically all of the data available to the
researcher who wishes to examine the foregoing research issues has been
collected by means of specially designed surveys. The main national
surveys were conducted in 1973, 1983 and 1993. A large proportion of the
questions originally formulated for the 1973 survey were repeated in the
1983 and 1993 surveys and thus provide a bank of directly comparable
data. These questions may be groupedinto five large categories: (1) Ability
or proficiency in Irish; (2) Acquisition of Irish; (3) Use of Irish; (4) Attitudes
to Irish, and (5) Social and demographic characteristics of the respondents.
(Full details of the questionnaires may be found in CILAR, 1975 and
Riagin, 1992.)
198 Europe
The Irish Language 19811996: An Overview
While the proportion of persons claiming the ability to speak Irish has
increased consistently throughout the 20th century, the regional distribu-
tion has changed radically. As can be seen from the percentages in Table
8.1, there has been a gradual convergence in the pattern of regional varia-
tions.
In the mid-19th century, the regions in the south and west contained the
most extensive Irish-speaking districts but these were, even then, con-
tracting. Since 1926, there has been a gradual, but continual, revival in the
ratios of Irish-speakers, particularly among residents of the eastern region.
As there has also beena shift inthe regional distributionof populationover
the period towards the east (and to towns generally), the combined result
of these two trends has been to move the spatial concentration of Irish-
speakers eastwards as well.
About 50% of Irish-speakers now reside in Leinster (including Dublin),
compared with about 5%in 1851. Therefore, the overall trend has been for
the proportionof Irish-speakers inall regions to move towards the national
average.
Table 8.2, whichpresents the percentages of Irish-speakers (as definedin
census data) in each age-group, allows the changes in patterns of language
reproduction since 1926 to be broadly identified. The age-specific data
shows that the national increase in the proportion of Irish-speakers was
primarily caused by a continual improvement since the 1920s in the
proportion of young adult cohorts claiming the ability to speak Irish.
The pattern for 1926 is bi-modal, peaking around the 1020 year-old
group and again among the over 65s. It is clear that, even then, two
processes of language change were operating. The relatively higher
Irish Language 19811996 199
Table 8.1 Percentage of persons claimingthe abilitytospeakIrishbyregion
18511991 (selected years)
Year Leinster
(East) (%)
Munster
(South) (%)
Connacht
(West) (%)
Ulster (North-
west) (%)
State
(%)
1851 3.5 43.9 50.8 17.0 29.1
1901 2.3 25.7 38.0 20.7 19.2
1946 15.1 22.0 33.2 26.0 21.2
1981 28.1 34.6 38.8 30.8 31.6
1991 30.5 36.3 40.1 32.0 32.5
1996 37.4 45.4 48.2 39.3 41.1
Source: Census of Population
percentages among the older age-groups reflects the typical pattern found
in communities where a language is in decline, while the higher percent-
ages within younger age-groups reflects in initial impact of revival strate-
gies. By 1991, the pattern revealed by census data had become most
unusual, if not unique, as compared to other minority language situations.
The largest proportionof Irish-speakers is to be foundinthe 1020 year-old
age-groups, after which it consistently becomes smaller. As regards the
absolute percentages of Irish-speakers in each age-group, it is clear from
earlier remarks that I wouldtreat the increases recordedinthe 1996 Census
with caution due to changes in the census question. If my hypothesis is
correct, then these increases are largely due to persons with weak speaking
skills mostly of school-going age nowbeing returned as Irish-speakers.
This hypothesis receives some indirect confirmation from the survey
results, which, however, measure adult abilities only (i.e. over 18 years).
The three national language surveys (197393) asked a number of ques-
tions about the respondents ownabilityinIrishat different periods of their
lives, as well as questions about the Irish-speaking abilities of various
members of their families. The speaking ability question, which was
devised by CILAR (1975: 116), asked respondents to rate their ability to
cope with normal conversations in Irish on a six-point scale and the same
question was used in all three surveys. Rather than use categories such as
poor, fair, good, etc. the question was phrased to offer the respondent
somewhat more guidance. Respondents were asked if they knew No
200 Europe
Table 8.2 Percentage of persons with ability to speak Irish by age-group
(selected years 192696)
Age group 1926 (%) 1946 (%) 1981 (%) 1996 (%)
34 4.6 4.1 4.9 10.0
59 19.8 21.6 27.8 48.2
1014 39.2 47.5 50.8 68.0
1519 27.6 43.4 51.0 68.0
2024 15.8 32.2 40.0 51.7
2534 13.2 20.8 32.8 37.3
3544 11.9 11.2 30.0 39.8
4554 13.7 10.0 28.3 37.1
5564 16.9 9.5 22.9 32.9
65+ 25.1 11.9 13.0 27.7
Total (over 3 years) 18.3 21.2 31.6 43.5
Source: Census of Population
Irish, the Odd Word, a Few Simple Sentences, enough for Parts of
Conversations, for Most Conversations or, finally, if they regardedthem-
selves as Native Speakers. The replies (Table 8.3) suggest a far more vari-
able picture than that revealed by the census reports.
In each survey, about half of the sample said that they had little or no
Irish, about 40%felt that theycouldmanage a fewsimple sentences or parts
of conversations, and just over 10%said that they could handle most or all
conversational situations. It can be seen from the table that there has been
little or no change in the 20 years since 1973. Even combining the top or
bottomthree points of the scale in the three surveys produces a statistically
insignificant variation of only three percentage points.
The survey evidence would suggest that only about 5% of the national
population use Irish as their first or main language. Afurther 10%use Irish
regularly but less intensively in conversation or reading. In the state gener-
ally, these levels would appear to have remained stable over recent
decades. As opposedtothese relativelylowratios of spokenor active use of
Irish, the ratios of passive use, primarilylistening/watchingIrishlanguage
radio and television programmes, are considerably higher. About 12% of
the population watch some Irish language programme weekly (this figure
would appear to have increased to about 20%since the newIrish language
television station began broadcasting in 1996). The new 1996 Census of
Population data on language use are in accord with these findings. On
average, about 10% of the population said that they used Irish on a daily
basis, but this percentage ranges from 2736% for school-going groups, to
about 3% for age-groups over 20 years.
The age-specific language data in the census shows that the national
increase in the proportion of Irish-speakers was primarily caused by a
continual improvement since the 1920s inthe proportionof youngadult
cohorts able to speak Irish. Although over time the proportion of Irish-
Irish Language 19811996 201
Table 8.3 Ability to speak Irish
Ability level 1973 (%) 1983 (%) 1993 (%)
1. No Irish 21 16 18
2. The Odd Word 27 32 32
3. A Few Simple Sentences 22 19 17
4. Parts of Conversations 17 20 22
5. Most Conversations 10 10 9
6. Native speaker ability 3 3 2
Total 100 100 100
speakers in older adult cohorts also improved and continues to improve,
the improvement is muchsmaller thanthe ratio of Irish-speakers inschool-
age cohorts wouldsuggest. The constant slippage fromthe relativelyhigh
levels of ability attained at school as the cohort moved into adulthood
clearly reflects the low incidence of bilingualism in society.
However, it should also be noted that in the small Gaeltacht districts the
use of Irish as measuredby language surveys is still very much higher than
the national average. The 1996 Census would suggest that 43% of persons
resident in this area speak Irish on a daily basis compared to 10%nation-
ally. However, this relatively high ratio may be declining. In 1973, which is
the last year inwhichall areas were surveyed, frequent andextensive home
use of Irish was reported by about 60%of respondents (as compared to 5%
nationally in that particular survey) and differences in work and social
contexts are of the same order. For obvious reasons, the 1973 survey and
1996 census figures cannot be directly compared, although there is other
evidence to suggest that language shift continues to occur in these areas (
Riagin, 1997)
Except in Gaeltacht areas, bilingualismin Irelandis basedrather loosely
on a thin distribution of family and social networks which have a degree of
underpinning froma variety of state policies in education, work-place and
media institutions. But these networks are dispersed and weakly estab-
lished and are very vulnerable to the loss of members over time as they
are not sufficiently large or vibrant enough to easily attract and retain
replacements.
Overall, if stability tending towards slight improvement describes the
trend with Irish speaking and reading abilities, then stability tending
towards slight decline might be a fair way of summarising the evidence on
levels of use of Irishover the period19811996. The rather complex mixture
of success and failure, of growth and decline to be found in the present
bilingual pattern indicates that while falling far short of the original policy
objective, some modest element of revival and maintenance is still being
accomplished.
Bilingual Reproduction
Not surprisingly, the ability of Irish-speaking networks to reproduce
themselves is limited by their distribution, number and size in an English-
speaking environment. Overall, both within and outside the Gaeltacht,
bilingual reproduction is extremely problematical.
Even on census evidence, it is clear that Irish is not being maintained in
its traditional core-area the Gaeltacht ( Riagin, 1992, 1997). The weak-
202 Europe
ening rate of bilingual reproduction in these areas has not, however, been a
continuous process. Up until about 1960 the degree of continuity with
pre1926 patterns was considerable, although concealed by the rather
ambitious and optimistic way in which the Gaeltacht had been defined in
1926. However Riagin has shown that in the 1970s and 1980s the Irish-
speakingcore, whichhadremainedstable over the period1926 to1956, had
itself become unstable and was showing signs of substantial and rapid
language shift. Not only is the number of marriages among fluent Irish-
speakers in decline, but there is also evidence to suggest that some parents
withhighability inIrishhave become less efficient inreproducing children
with equally high levels of Irish in recent decades.
Outside of the Gaeltacht, both survey and census data would indicate
that only about 4% of households are Irish-speaking (The 1996 Census
suggests that only 5% of 34 year-olds i.e. the pre-school group are
reported to speak Irish on a daily basis). If this is accepted as a reasonably
accurate measure of the extent of active home bilingualism in the commu-
nity, the task of maintaining that lowlevel requires the production in each
generationof a substantiallyhigher proportionof people withthe potential
toparticipate activelyinIrish-speakingnetworks. This is because ina situa-
tion where Irish-speakers are distributed widely in the English-speaking
areas a highdegree of loss across the life-cycle is to be expected(APC, 1986:
ix). While bilingualism, so measured, appears to be relatively stable, only
one-quarter of those who grew up in Irish language homes use Irish with
the same intensity in their current homes.
Apart fromthe bilingual output of the schools and the small minority of
Irish-speaking homes, it also appears that some element of home bilin-
gualism derives from a group who did not have an Irish language home
background, and did not appear to have had any strong association with
Irish during their school years. Howand why this group began to use Irish
in their adult years is not clear fromthe research, but in the past it appears
that Irish began to be used on marriage or on the establishment of their
own households (CILAR, 1975: 212). This group includes many of the
small but growing minority of parents who have chosen Irish medium
education for their children, and there is evidence to suggest that these
schools help to establish some degree of home bilingualism and to intro-
duce parents to Irish-speaking networks.
Despite their small size, therefore, there is evidence that Irish-speaking
networks possess some capacity to recruit new members. This must be set
against their apparent inability to secure a permanent character, to ensure
efficient reproductionof bilinguals andto absorb all of the bilingual output
of homes and schools.
Irish Language 19811996 203
Bilingual Production
The maintenance of more or less stable rates of bilingualismover recent
decades is therefore due more to the capacity of the schools to produce
competent bilinguals rather than the capacity of the bilingual community
to reproduce itself. Until recently, the educational systemwould appear to
have been able to fulfil this task. However, more recently the pattern
appears to have been changing.
Most Irish children learn Irish in both primary and post-primary school
as a subject, but despite some 13 years experience inthe case of the average
child, these programmes donot generallyproduce highlycompetent active
users of Irish. When they do, they are usually among those who stay in the
systemthe longest andtake the academically most demanding syllabus, or
else among the small minority who attend all-Irish schools .
Paradoxically, in a period when Irish language policy in the schools
generally is experiencing considerable difficulties, the number of Irish
immersionprimaryschools inEnglishspeakingareas continues togrow. In
1981, there were 28 such schools. In 1991, this figure hadrisen to 66 andit is
now over 100. As a consequence, the proportion of children receiving this
type of education has increased from 5% to 8%.
The position in mainstream schools is not so healthy. In these schools
Irish is taught as a subject only. In 1993, nearly three-quarters of current
users of Irish had post-primary schooling and nearly half had taken the
higher level course in Irish. However, since 1980 only 1015% of a cohort
opt for the higher level courses in Irish in post-primary schools and even
after 13 years study of the subject the speaking ability of the majority of the
cohort is only moderate or, in the case of a growing minority, negligible (
Riagin, 1997).
Notwithstanding the trends in examination performance, there has
been a clear rise since the 1960s in the proportion of the total cohort
which now receives some certification in Irish at Leaving Certificate
level. It has grown from 17% in 1963 to 59% in 1991. The pattern which
has developed in the school system reveals the operation of two
processes, the more significant being the increase in post-primary
participation rates since 1966. This was able to compensate for the
decline in the proportions achieving higher grades in public examina-
tions and for the combined increase in the proportions failing or not
taking Irish. The education system, therefore, is producing larger
numbers of graduates with high qualifications in Irish, but it is realising
the potential of a smaller proportion of candidates than in former years.
This appears particularly true of male students, whose representation
204 Europe
among higher level Irish candidates has declined in recent years while
that of female students has increased.
In tandem with the growth of second level education, the third-level
sector has expandedenormouslysince 1996. Numbers inthird-level educa-
tion have increasedfrom21,000 in 1965 to about 75,000 in 1991/92. Student
intake in 1991/92 was close to 26,000, representing almost 40% of the age-
group. About half of the intake proceedtodegree-level programmes (DOE,
1992: 183). Thus the continuing requirement to have a pass in Irish, at
Leaving Certificate level, for entry to the constituent colleges of the
National University of Ireland and the Teacher Training Colleges main-
tains some support for the language. In addition to the entrance require-
ment, note must be taken of the extremely competitive nature of the entry
process to the third-level system. Applications greatly outnumber the
available places. Places are allocated on the basis of the grades awarded in
the Leaving Certificate examinations, with more points being allocated for
Higher level courses than Lower. As Irish must be included in the set of six
subjects upon which the points score in calculated for entry to the national
University and Teacher Colleges, it is obviously in the interests of those
students seeking places to score highly on the Irish paper.
Just half of those with a higher grade in Irish went on to third-level
education, comparedtoone-quarter of those witha Pass grade. Ananalysis
conducted by the Advisory Planning Committee (APC, 1986) of the educa-
tional attainments of applicants for third-level places would suggest that
the position has become more complex with growth in the range of third
level institutions. These now include a number of new universities, Tech-
nical Colleges andInstitutes of Technology. None of these newinstitutions
insist on an Irish qualification on entry and neither does the oldest univer-
sity in Dublin, Trinity College. Thus, while a high proportion of those
selecting the Higher level paper in the Leaving Certificate examination
transfer to third-level (usually university) education, a significant and
growing proportion of pupils who have selected the Pass level course or
who do now select Irish at all appear to have the academic ability to attain
third-level places (APC, 1986: 50).
The relationship between third-level education and achievement levels
in Irish, which has worked to the advantage of Irish in the post-primary
system has, however, certain sociodemographic implications (APC, 1986:
31; see also Riagin, 1997). Although only a minority of post-primary
graduates proceed to third-level education, this minority is not randomly
determined. Access to education, particularly third-level education, is in
large part determined by social class (Breen et al., 1990: 131)
Whereas over one-thirdof respondents in the toptwo classes proceeded
Irish Language 19811996 205
to third-level education, this was true for only about one-twentiethof those
from the two lowest classes. The consequences of these inequalities for
social policy are, of course, the subject of much political debate but, as the
foregoinganalysis wouldsuggest, theyare also reflectedinthe class profile
of those who study Irish at the most advanced level in the post-primary
sector
The degree of social polarisation is pronounced. Although respondents
fromfamilies inthe topthree classes constitute just 38%of the total sample,
they account for nearly three-quarters (71%) of the Higher level Irish grad-
uates. However, within the upper classes some changes have been occur-
ring in recent years. While the achievement levels of those fromfamilies in
Classes II to IVappear to be improving, that of Class I is weakening. This is
not due toanylesseninginthe propensityof families inthis class toeducate
their chilren to the third-level. The contrary, in fact, is the case. Among the
younger age-group in this class, nearly half (45%) had received third-level
education, compared to 27% of the older group. It would thus appear, as
APC (1986: 44) has suggested, that for this group in particular, the educa-
tional decisions taken by students and families involve the adoption of a
strategy which increasingly does not include Higher level Irish or any Irish
at all. As this group includes the elite elements in Irish society, this
tendency clearly has implications for the long-term societal support for
Irish.
Furthermore, now that the major expansion in post-primary education
participation has run its course, the continued reliance on current
schooling procedures as a means of generating competence in Irish places
the language in a very vulnerable position.
Explaining Bilingual Patterns and Trends
Language policy is formulated, implemented and accomplishes its
results within a complex interrelated set of economic, social and political
processes whichinclude, inter alia, the operationof other state policies. The
modernisation of Irish society, a process which developed slowly prior to
1960, exerted a powerful influence in subsequent years. With the rapid
growth of economic prosperity since 1960 has come a widening of
economic, political, demographic and cultural contacts and influences as
Ireland became more comprehensively incorporated within the frame-
work of international capitalism. These external influences, with their
various and differentiated impacts on Irish society have created severe
problems for policies designed to maintain Irish as a minority language.
Irish language policy, particularly in the period 192260, succeeded to
206 Europe
the extent it directly or indirectly changed the rules of the social mobility
process. It is the relationship between educational qualifications and the
labour market and not pedagogic factors per se which is crucial to an
understanding the effectiveness of Irish language policies in the schools.
The Irish economy before 1960 was dominated by family businesses
mostly in farming and the self-employed. Job opportunities for those
with, in Bourdieus terms, cultural rather than economic capital, were very
limited (Bourdieu, 1991). Therefore, participation rates in post-primary
education were low. Yet research studies have consistently shown that the
education systems capacity to produce competent bilinguals was closely
related to the number of years an individual spent in school and, of course,
the type of language programme followed. Of these two variables, the
durationof educationhas provedtobe the more relevant for the majorityof
pupils. Notwithstanding the influence of other factors, it remains the case
that most of those withhighlevels of ability inIrishhadremainedinschool
at until the end of post-primary education.
The selective impact of language policy in the schools was reinforced by
policies regulating entry to the professions (via third-level education) or
the public service. As a consequence the percentages of Irish-speakers in
bothsets of occupations increasedafter 1922. But Irishlanguage policy was
relatively ineffectual in the case of those labour market sectors where entry
depended on inheritance rather than educational qualifications. Thus the
very limited degree of social mobility in Ireland in these early decades
placed some severe limitations on the social impact of Irish language
policy.
However, Irish society has changed considerably since 1960. The new
economic policy, which was elaborated in a series of economic develop-
ment programmes between 1960 and the early 1970s, placed emphasis on
export markets and foreign industrial investment. Education was
primarily seen as a form of investment within context of economic
growth. In short, the modern industry targeted by the economic
programmes were perceived to require a well-educated labour force. A
range of new policies led to huge increases in second-level participation
rates, and simultaneously the number and range of third-level institutions
was widened. Meanwhile, as a more direct consequence of the economic
programmes, the range of employment opportunities outside the profes-
sions and the public sector expanded. Thus the relationship between
education and the labour market was strengthened, but the state was
unable to adapt the scope of its language policies to regulate these new
developments. In fact, it effectively gave up the struggle. In 1973, Irish
ceased to be a compulsory subject for examinations or a requirement for
Irish Language 19811996 207
entry to the public service. These societal trends severely reduced the insti-
tutional support for Irish and the reproduction of high levels of ability in
certain key social groups.
However, some elements of the original policy continue to operate. Irish
must still be included on the curriculum of schools in receipt of state
support and it remains necessary for entry to the four colleges of the
National University of Ireland. As post-primary education became more
widely available, children from the upper classes increasingly sought
third-level education. Thus, the Irish language requirement for entry to the
universities maintained the class element in language reproduction, but
withrisingparticipationrates inbothsecondandthird-level education, the
class base has widened.
It is important to note that, while the Gaeltacht is located in peripheral
rural areas, it too was directly affected by these changes in occupational
structures. As late as 1961 nearly three-quarters of males in these areas
were working in agriculture, but by 1981 agriculture was employing less
than half of the workforce, while other sectors had grown proportionately
and in absolute terms.
Social Reproduction and Irish-speaking Networks
The significance of socioeconomic trends for language reproduction
arises fromtheir impact of Irish-speaking networks and communities. The
available research evidence indicates that the small farm economy of
Gaeltacht areas supported a pattern of social networks which were very
localised and restricted in spatial scope in the first half of this century. The
relative stability of these network boundaries was an important factor in
sustaining Irish-speaking communities. However, state policies were not
able to maintain population levels. By the early 1980s, the population had
beenhalvedandmajor structural changes hadoccurredwithinthe commu-
nity. As economic development began to percolate into rural areas in the
post1960 period, the minimum threshold population levels were no
longer available in many rural communities to support traditional activi-
ties (primary school, parish, etc.), even less so new functions, e.g. post-
primary education. The growth in non-agricultural employment had
resulted in increases in commuting to and from towns. This, combined
with the growth in post-primary education (also centred in the towns),
changes in shopping and recreation patterns, and shifts in migration
patterns all signified a major transformation of social network patterns
which occasioned significant changes in patterns of bilingualism.
These developments in the structure of the community which served to
208 Europe
intensifythe frequencyof interactions betweenIrish-speakers andEnglish-
speakers, or to increase the probability of young people working outside
the community, servedto diminish the possibility of maintaining Irish. For
example, network changes resulting from both in-migration and return
migrationhave beenincreasinginimportance inrecent decades. There was
also a growing involvement insocial andoccupational networks outside of
the Irish-speaking area, As a consequence, the overall proportion of
married couples with joint native-speaker ability in Irish has been
declining. These homes are substantially more efficient in reproducing
similarly competent Irish-speakers than are homes in which only one or no
parent is a native-speaker.
Outside of Gaeltacht areas, the relationship between social class and
Irish has been a significant factor in the formation of Irish-speaking
networks in these English language regions. Because of the relationship
between social class and the housing market, the spatial distribution of
bilinguals varies withthe class character of residential areas and, therefore,
spatial variations in the distribution of Irish-speakers are apparent in large
urban areas. One of the few studies of Irish-speaking networks in urban
areas found a strong relationship between the distribution of Irish
language schools and the sociospatial concentrations of Irish-speakers (
Riagin, 1997). Thus it seems plausible to argue that the social concentra-
tion of high ability Irish speakers within the middle-classes and, in partic-
ular, within the public sector provides the potential, at least, for the
formation and maintenance of Irish-speaking networks. For network
forminginstitutions, like schools, tobecome viable operations, theyrequire
sufficientlylarge numbers of supportive parents withina reasonable catch-
ment area. But once the school is established in an area, all the indications
suggest that it hadanimportance far beyondits basic aimof educatingchil-
dren through Irish. The capacity of the school-based networks to attract
novice or reluctant bilinguals is evidence that Irish-speaking networks
are capable, in these circumstances at least, of recruiting new members.
This must be set against their acknowledged inability to secure a perma-
nent character that could ensure the reproduction of Irish speakers and
absorb the bilingual output of homes and schools (APC, 1988: 31).
Current Policy Dilemmas and Future Directions
At the political level it is not surprising, therefore, to find state agencies
in the 1980s and1990s speaking more of survival rather than revival. While
Bord na Gaeilge (Irish Language Board) continues to present bilingualism
as a model of language choice for Irish society as whole, the practical focus
Irish Language 19811996 209
of its programme is directed at the provision of certain basic state services
in and through Irish, i.e. Irish language schools, television and radio
stations, sections of public offices, etc. One can detect in this approach the
beginnings of a more selective strategy that of institutional parallelism.
For example, the state broadcasting organisations have always experi-
enced considerable difficulties in trying to find a balance between their
budgetary responsibilities and the programming requirements of a scat-
teredlinguistic minority, whose bilingual competences, social background
and interests are also very variable. These operational problems are
compounded by others which relate to the ideological character and stra-
tegic shape of the states language policy. The broadcasting system is
expectedat one andthe same time to reinforce boththe revival andmainte-
nance elements of the strategy, but these objectives envisage somewhat
different target audiences with very different requirements. These issues
form a set of related co-ordinates around which Irish language broad-
casting has been organised.
The principal re-alignment of broadcasting policy over the past two
decades has been a move towards the maintenance pole of the overall
stategy and a consequent weakening emphasis on the revival dimension.
This is most clearly seen in the fact that Raidio na Gaeltachta originally a
local stationservingIrish-speakingareas onthe westerncoast nowcarries
the overwhelming bulk of national radio programming inIrish. It is impor-
tant to recognise, however, that this shift also eases operational and ideo-
logical problems. The programmers are offered a more clearly defined
audience (evenif it constitutes onlypart of the bilingual population), andas
the focus is directed towards the day-to-day problems of the Gaeltacht
community the programme makers can justifiably dodge the more tricky
ideological issues.
Televisionprogramming inIrishhas beenmoving inthe same direction.
At present, such programming has already been moved to the less popular
of the two national channels. Although an Irish language television
station was established in 1996, the long-term implications of this for
Irish language programming on other channels is unclear. If television
programming for the Irish language in any significant way follows the
patternof radio programming, thenit canbe concludedthat the earlier role
of helping to create bilingual communities will have been abandoned. The
underlying principle will become one of servicing the existing bilingual
population.
One can also detect some signs of a similar development within the
education system. The long-termdrift fromthe objective of Irish language
mediumeducation for all seemedto have recededto the last line of defence
210 Europe
in1973, whenIrishceasedtobe a compulsorysubject instate examinations,
but was retained as a required subject on the curriculum of schools in
receipt of state funds. But the patternof recent examinationresults inIrish
which show a growing percentage of pupils failing or not taking the
paper together with a number of recent policy decisions, suggest that this
line is itself showing signs of erosion. While the government is careful to
support the expanding all-Irish school movement, it has also relaxed
further the requirements for pupils to study Irish and the requirements for
teachers to have a professional competence in Irish. The proposals to
restructure the National University of Ireland raise questions about the
continuedstatus of Irish as a requiredmatriculation subject. There is nowa
clear possiblity that Irish as a school subject will revert to its pre-independ-
ence status as a voluntary subject.
There are dangers in this development. Tovey (1988: 67) points out that
the more policy singles out Irish-speakers as the target for language poli-
cies on the grounds of their rights as a minority group, the less plausible it
becomes tosustainexistingpolicies torevive Irish. Nor is it easy, inpolitical
terms, to move froma universal policy, which has been in operation for 70
years, to one which is more selective without severely damaging public
confidence in the overall policy objective.
There is another consideration. The stability of current Irish usage is
dependent on the stability of the social networks of users, that is, on the
series of interlinkedsocial relationships that maygrowout of contacts inan
institutional setting, but whose survival depends on the achievement of
some degree of friendship, intimacy and interpersonal knowledge among
participants. It seems unlikely that these are strong enough at present to
guarantee the reproduction of spoken Irish, or its expansion, into the next
generation. A policy built around the provision of state services to Irish-
speakers may find that they do not exist in large enough numbers nor are
they sufficiently concentrated to meet the operational thresholds required
to make these services viable.
In all of these respects, one feels that Irish language policy is now at a
critical stage. The way forward is no longer clear. To further confuse the
issue, Irish language policy has emerged as a key element in the Northern
Ireland peace process. Under the terms of the peace agreement concluded
in April 1998, a newall-Ireland language policy unit is to be established to
oversee language policy in both the Republic and Northern Ireland. This
will bring together, as part of one policy operation, two very diferent Irish
language situations. The Irish language is very much part of the nationalist
programme in Northern Ireland, while the Republic has already moved
into a post-nationalist phase. On the other hand, despite all the foregoing
Irish Language 19811996 211
analysis of its weaknesses, the Irish language is considerably stronger in
the Republic thanit is inNorthernIreland. It remains tobe seenwhether the
political impulse fromthe Northwill give language policya lift, or whether
the practical situation in the North will operate to further dilute the policy
in the Republic. For example, the Irish language is an optional subject in
Northern schools, while it is still a required subject in all schools in the
Republic. It remains to be seen how this will work out.
Conclusions
As opposed to Fishmans evolutionary universalistic approach I would
wish to argue that before we can place policy interventions on a sound
theoretical footing, we needtomuchmore systematicallyaccount for varia-
tions within and between in language shift situations and seriously and
explicitly to locate these situations in time and space (Giddens, 1984). This
inevitably means linking local interaction sytems, within which language
behaviour occurs (andwhichare the mainfocus of Fishmans overview), to
the wider economic and social systems which impinge upon them (Gal,
1988, 1989; Bourdieu, 1991). There is nothing in Fishmans model to indi-
cate how variables of an economic, social or spatial nature are to be incor-
porated for either analytical or prescriptive purposes. Yet clearly these
issues have had an important bearing on the way language shift evolved
and formed part of the Irish governments response to these issues. The
complex sequence of maintence, shift, revival, etc. apparent in Irelandover
this century is intimately related to changes in the economic base of the
community within the wider society. Unless we can understand how
different social groups and categories vary in their response to broadly
similar external circumstances, use existing sets of relationships and
resources and reinterpret traditional norms and values (Long, 1977), we
cannot begin to evaluate the potential effectiveness of different kinds of
policy interventations.
There is a further point to be made. As Glyn Lewis (1983) pointed out
many years ago, it is necessary to avoid the tendency, only too apparent in
manydiscussions of language planningandpolicy, toregardthe process as
autonomous. The social uses of language owe their specifically social value
to the fact that they are organised in systems of social interaction which
reproduce the systemof social differences (Bourdieu, 1991). The state plays
a very dominant role in shaping the socioeconomic development and thus
it is necessary to examine policies which relate to economic, and social
(particularly education) issues, and whose intent was not at all language
oriented, but which greatly affect the operation of language policy. It is
212 Europe
probable that, in total, their consequences for language maintenance objec-
tives are extensive andof more importance thanlanguage policies per se. As
Pierre Bourdieu observes, those who seek to defend a threatened
language, . . . are obliged to wage a total struggle. One cannot save the
value of a competence unless one saves the market, in other words, the
whole set of political andsocial conditions of productionof the producers/
consumers. (Bourdieu, 1991: 57)
Note
1. This chapter revises and updates sections of my book Language Policy and Social
Reproduction: Ireland 18931993. (OxfordStudies in Language Contact). Oxford,
Clarendon Press.
References
Advisory Planning Committee (1986) Irish and the Education System: An Analysis of
Examination Results. Dublin: Bord na Gaeilge.
Advisory Planning Committee (1988) The Irish Language in a Changing Society:
Shaping the Future. Dublin, Bord na Gaeilge.
Bourdieu, P. (1991) Language and Symbolic Power (edited and introduced by J.B.
Thompson). Cambridge: Polity Press.
Breen, R., Hannan, D.F., Rottman, D.B. and Whelan, C.T. (1990) Understanding Con-
temporary Ireland: State, Class and Development in the Republic of Ireland. Dublin:
Gill & Macmillan.
Committee on Irish Language Attitudes Research (CILAR) (1975) Report. Dublin:
The Stationery Office.
Department of Education. Education in a Changing World (Green Paper) (1992) Dub-
lin: Government Publications Office.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift: Theoretical and Empirical Foundations of
Assistance to Threatened Languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
(Gaeltacht Commission) Commission of Inquiry into the Preservation of the
Gaeltacht, (1926) Final Report. Stationery Office: Dublin.
Gal, S. (1988) The political economy of code choice. In M. Heller (ed.)
Codeswitching: Anthropological and Sociolinguistic Perspectives. Berlin, Mouton
de Gruyter.
Gal, S. (1989) Language and political economy. Annual Review of Anthropology 18,
34567.
Giddens, A. (1984) The Constitution of Society. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Lewis, G. (1983) Implementation of language planning in the Soviet Union. In J.
Cobarrubias and J.A. Fishman (eds) Progress in Language Planning: International
Perspectives. Berlin, Mouton.
Long N. (1977) An Introduction to the Sociology of Rural Development. London:
Tavistock.
Riagin, P. (1992) Language Maintenance and Language Shift as Strategies of Social Re-
production: Irish in the Corca Dhuibhne Gaeltacht 192686. Institiid Tean-
geolaochta ireann.
Irish Language 19811996 213
Riagin, P. (1997) Language Policy and Social Reproduction: Ireland 18931993. (Ox-
ford Studies in Language Contact). Oxford, Clarendon Press.
Riagin, P. & O Gliasin, M. (1979) All-Irish Primary Schools in the Dublin Area: A
Sociological and Spatial Analysis of the Impact of all-Irish Schools on Home and Social
Use of Irish. Dublin: Linguistics Institute of Ireland.
Riagin, P. and O Gliasin, M. (1994) National Survey on Languages 1993: Prelimi-
nary Report. Research Report 18. Dublin: Institiuid Teangeolaiochta ireann.
Tovey H. (1988) The State and the Irish language: the role of Bord na Gaeilge. Inter-
national Journal of the Sociology of Language 70, 5368.
214 Europe
Chapter 9
A Frisian Update of Reversing
Language Shift
D. GORTER
Introduction
The general meeting of the Provincial Council of Friesland on 24 April
1985 was an important milestone for language policy for the Frisian
language. On that day the politicians adopted the conclusions and recom-
mendations of the language planning report Fan Geunst nei Rjocht (From
favour to right), a title significant of the message they wanted to get across.
The Frisian provincial representatives were well aware that their accep-
tance of this report was going against the views of the central state govern-
ment. The vote symbolised the wide divergence on the development of a
language policy for Frisian as a minority language (Van Dijk, 1987). As a
solution to the ensuing deadlock in good Dutch tradition a compromise
was worked out between both layers of government. It took shape in the
form of a covenant. However, that document was only going to be signed
after four years of longandtedious negotiations. Then, ontopof all that, the
highest court in the Netherlands rejected the heart of the covenant within a
year, when it found the provisions for translations of official documents
not applicable. Although there were no provisions on official language use
inthe systemof laws, the court ruledthat Dutchhadto be takenfor granted
as the only language of all administrative affairs, until a law specifically
would declare otherwise. In retrospect, the debate in April 1985 demar-
cates the endpoint of a process, which began in the mid-1960s, of increased
political priority for language matters. In the 1990s issues surrounding the
promotion of the Frisian language have gradually become less of a priority
and are more and more incorporated into the daily business of doing
politics.
At the threshold of the newmillennium, on the basis of survey research
(Gorter & Jonkman, 1995b), one may claim that in absolute numbers the
Frisian language has more speakers than ever before.
1
Even though such a
statement has limited scientific value, it scores well in the media and it also
215
Europe
Frisian Language Update
gives new heart to RLS activists. The claim about the absolute number of
speakers also partially answers the main question of this article. On the
surface things have not really changed in the position of the Frisian
language over the last 25 years. The basic percentages of people who have
the ability to understand (94%), speak (74%), read (67%) or write (17%)
Frisian have remained more or less the same. Those four percentages have
onlychangeda little betweenthe first general sociolinguistic surveyof 1967
(Pietersen, 1969) and the most recent one of 1994. Gorter and Jonkman
(1995b: 55) concluded that the preliminary results of their survey, in terms
of language ability, usage in intimate and more public settings and
language attitudes, point to a stable situation for the Frisian language.
Inthis article I will lookbackover a periodof roughlya decade anda half
of Reversing Language Shift efforts on behalf of Frisian, in order to update
the chapter on Frisian by Fishman (1991).
2
I will first characterise this
period of well over a decade by three major developments: (1) internation-
alisation in a European context, (2) legitimisation inside a framework of
laws andregulations, and(3) stabilisation of the language situation. Subse-
quently, I will discuss step by step each of the eight stages of the GIDS
model as Fishman presented them in his RLS book. I will apply the model
again to Frisian using the most recent data.
Internationalisation
Europe has become part and parcel of the everyday reality of speakers
of Frisian. This is true in the general sense as it would be for many inhabit-
ants of the member states of the European Union (think only of the intro-
duction of the common currency the Euro). It is also true in the sense that
the Frisian language has become officially recognised as a European,
autochthonous minority language in the Charter for Regional or Minority
Languages of the Council of Europe. The international context is conspicu-
ously missing from Fishmans model of the GIDS, even from the highest
Stage 1, whichis limitedtonationwide levels. However, inmyopinionthe
developments of European unification, in particular those aimed at the
promotion of minority languages, do hold relevance for the way in which
RLS efforts take place, also for the Frisian case. I can mention a fewof those
developments.
Numerous publications, symposia, study days, letters to the editor and
discussion programmes have been filled with the consequences for the
Dutch language of a Europe without borders. Leading up to the magic
year 1992, a lasting public debate arose in the Netherlands surrounding
issues of national identity and language. Many opinion leaders have
216 Europe
painteda bleakfuture for Dutch. Accordingtosome, Dutchitself was likely
to become some sort of threatened minority language. Or, as was some-
times said, the future position of Dutch in Europe can be compared to the
position of Frisian as a minority language in the Netherlands. It has to be
admitted that in line with the general lack of pride about their national
language, quite a few people in the Netherlands would not mind at all if
Dutch were cast in such a subordinate role, or even completely disappear
and be replaced by English as a common language. Others rightfully point
out that the chances of Dutch disappearing in the short run are quite small,
as the language has some 20 million speakers anda strong infrastructure in
two national states, the Netherlands and Belgium. This debate on national
language andidentity startedin the late 1980s andhas continuedfor over a
decade now. As recentlyas 1998, the EuropeanCultural Foundation(based
in Amsterdam)
3
launched a new programme under the title Which
languages for Europe?. This programme is mainly concerned with the
problems of the use of a multitude of languages in the institutions of the
European Union (with 11 official languages). The programme also deals
with ways to maintain the pluralinguality of Europe as a whole and it
devotes some attention to minority languages such as Frisian. Another
recent example is the initiative of the Dutch Language Union (Nederlandse
Taalunie), whichis the joint BelgianDutchgovernmental bodyresponsible
for the development of the Dutch Standard language. Early in 1999 this
organisation initiated a project to discuss the Institutional Status and Use
of National Languages in Europe. The project emphasises the contribu-
tions that smaller national languages such as Dutch can make to a Euro-
pean language policy. It is feared that these languages may become
crushed between the smaller minority languages and the big three
(English, French and German).
Another development, which has already been mentioned, is the Euro-
pean Charter for Minority or Regional Languages of the Council of Europe.
After about 10 years of preparatoryworkthe Charter was openedfor signa-
ture by the member states in 1992. The Netherlands were among the first
signatories. After ratification by five member states the Charter became
effective in 1998. The Charter has been fully ratified for Frisian.
An important outcome of the process of internationalisation is the enor-
mous increase in contacts among speakers of minority languages across
Europe. Since the beginning of the 1980s the European Union has taken a
lively interest in the question of minority languages. One significant
measure was the introduction of a special budget line in 1982. This budget
line enables support of projects to preserve and promote minority
languages. For instance, the first international conference organised from
Frisian Language Update 217
this budget line took place in Ljouwert (Friesland) in 1985. Later on many
projects were carried out in Friesland with European Union support,
among them the Mercator-Education project.
4
Although we may observe a general increase in awareness of the impor-
tance of language issues in the Netherlands, this has not really changedthe
basic attitude of the majority of politicians and civil servants towards
Frisian. Ignorance seems to dominate and, even when there may be some
goodwill, this is not easily translated into positive measures for Frisian.
Legal Framework
Alexander Schmidt (1997: 30) of the Max Planck institute of Interna-
tional Lawin Heidelberg recently concluded: the Frisian language under-
went an important increase in prestige. In his article he describes what has
happened in Friesland in terms of building a legal framework over the last
decade. This legal framework can be summarised in four parts: (1) the
Covenants between the State and the Province in 1989 and 1993; (2) the
General Act on Administrative law in 1995; (3) the Act on Frisian in the
Courts in 1997; and finally (4) the European Charter for Regional or
Minority Languages in 1998. Each of these four parts of the legal frame-
work will be briefly amplified below.
(1) In 1989 a Covenant on Frisian language policy was entered into
between the State and the Province (as referred to in my introduction). The
covenant was renewed in 1993 and negotiations began in 1998 for a new
covenant with a long-termperspective of at least 10 years. These covenants
include provisions for education, media, culture and scientific research,
but also for public administrationandthe use of Frisianinthe courts. Every
section contains specific agreements and provisions for a budget for
several parts of the infra-structure of the Frisian language. Thus, for
instance, arrangements are outlined for the only professional theatre
company Tryater, the broadcasting organisation Omrop Frysln and the
centre for scientific research the Fryske Akademy. The advantage of a
covenant is clear. It provides anexceptionandprotectiontostatewide rules
which otherwise would not easily apply to Frisian organisations. It also
provides some safeguard against cutbacks. On the negative side one has to
mentionthat the covenant has a limiteddurationandhas tobe renegotiated
again and again. Moreover, it is only a weak form of legal protection and
provided no guarantee. This was almost immediately made clear by the
rejection by the highest court of the arrangements in the first covenant
about the use of Frisian in public administration, which made the new
legislation of 1995 necessary.
218 Europe
(2) Until recently the official language of the Netherlands was not estab-
lished by law. Therefore, a motion was introduced in parliament to change
the constitution, whichis alsoa signof the increasedlanguage awareness in
the Netherlands. However, in the debate over the protection of Dutch,
nothing even remotely near a two-thirds majority was to be found among
the parliamentarians. The majority did rightfully think that formulating a
phrase such as Dutch is the language of the Netherlands wouldnot in any
way protect the language in a unified Europe. Thus the constitution was
not going to be changed. Acompromise was found in changing the Act on
Administrative Law. In 1995 regulations were accepted that Dutch is
declared to be the official language of public administration. Frisian has
pickedsome fruits of this debate, as it has beenincludedwithsome specific
provisions in the same law. The Act has made it possible to use Frisian in
most administrative affairs, both in written and in spoken form.
Still, suchlegal arrangements are onlya first step, as canbe illustratedby
an example fromthe practice of language policy. As part of their language
policy, the provincial government and many municipalities in Friesland
ostensibly accept the rule of following language choice. This means that
local government does not want to take the first step when using Frisian,
and, in principle, only gives an answer in Frisian when addressed in that
language. However, some research among civil servants showed what
happens inreality. Of the fewletters writtenby citizens inFrisian(less than
5% of all letters), these letters were not answered in Frisian in about two-
thirds of cases (Gorter & Jonkman, 1994). There are several reasons why
they were not answered in Frisian. I can give a few examples. The civil
servant who had to write the answering letter could not write Frisian and
he did not want to go to the translation service. Perhaps he just took an old
example fromhis files of a similar case, or he was insecure about the Frisian
legal terminology. He may also have felt that a Frisian letter would be
disapproved of by his colleagues, etc. (Gorter, 1993). In this way, many
practical reasons make civil servants almost always choose Dutch. Thus
far, little has been done to change this phenomenon of silent obstruction,
neither by the provincial administration nor by the municipalities.
(3) Similar problems existed with the official use of Frisian in the courts.
Frisian was allowed to a limited extent by a law dating from 1956.
According to that law, if one insisted, one could use Frisian before a court,
at least when the presiding judge allowed one to do so. Therefore, only in a
few cases would a person actually use the Frisian language. In 1997 a new
law became effective which contains better provisions. Today, one does
indeed have the full right to use the Frisian language in court. Whether
many persons will nowactually use it remains uncertain, seeing the hardi-
Frisian Language Update 219
ness of traditions. Practice will tell how this law works and what jurispru-
dence will need to be added in order to implement it.
(4) The European Charter for Regional or Minority Languages is perceived
as quite important by the Dutch State. After signing it in 1992, the Nether-
lands were among the first to ratify the Charter in 1995.
5
One reason why
the Dutch government wanted to be among the first signatories was the
opportunity to play a leading role inEuropeanmoral affairs, a role whichit
likes to play anyway. Thus, parliamentarians mentioned in their state-
ments the importance of supporting the Charter in the light of develop-
ments of new democracies in Central Europe. Another reason was that by
codifying the existing situation for Frisian it was possible to realise some
minimal Frisian aspirations.
The Charter consists of three parts. Part I contains general consider-
ations; in Part II the languages are entered for which the Charter will be
applied; and Part III consists of a long list of more than 90 articles with
specific provisions to preserve and promote minority or regional
languages. When a state decides to sign Part III for a specified minority
language, it has to choose a minimum of 35 articles which it will apply. In
the case of the Netherlands five languages were brought under Part II
(along with Frisian, the languages are Low-Saxon, Limburgish, Romani
and Yiddish)
6
but Part III has only been applied to Frisian. The approach
chosenby the Netherlands is one of codification. The Charter was ratifiedby
the Netherlands in such a way that specific provisions were chosen which
were already being implemented.
Thus, the Charter affirms existinglanguage policydrawnfromthe cove-
nants and existing laws. Still it has a positive symbolic effect because the
Dutch member state is binding itself in this way to international law. It also
becomes more difficult to reverse existing measures. For Frisian RLS activ-
ists the hope of the Charter is that in the future it will be possible to sign
additional provisions.
Stable Language Relationships
The common expectation in Friesland seems to be that the Frisian
language is decreasing in use. There is a general sense of a threatened
language underlying the development of language policy and all kinds of
other RLS efforts. In 1994 the Fryske Akademy carried out a newlanguage
survey among the population, in which many questions were repeated
from earlier surveys in 1967 and 1980. The first results were eagerly
awaited, but they were also quite surprising to many people. The survey
did not find that Frisian was diminishing in use. Stability over the last
220 Europe
decade proved to be a characteristic of the language relationships in many
ways. This can be illustrated by a central variable such as home language.
The proportions for language usually spoken at home are shown in the
Table 9.1.
The outcomes of the table do make clear that the use of Frisian as a home
language declined sharply between 1967 and 1980, from 71% to 56%.
7
Dutch as a home language in Friesland increased substantially between
1967 and 1980 to about one-third, but afterwards remained at about the
same level. Migration is an important factor is this regard (see below).
Compared to home language, on average, the figures for proficiency in
Frisian have changedless over time. In 1980 and1994, three-quarters of the
Frisian population could speak Frisian, whereas in the period 196780 a
decrease of about 10% in speaking Frisian occurred. For understanding
and reading Frisian the decline was small, within margins of error of the
samples. Ability to write remained stable for the period 196780, while
from 198094 an increase of over 5% was observed.
The distributionof language use of Frisianover different social domains
shows an uneven pattern. A majority of the population habitually uses
Frisian in the domains of the family, work and the village. Frisian holds a
relative strong position there. In the more formal domains of education,
media, public administration and law, the use of Frisian has made some
inroads during the last decades, but overall it is still fairly limited.
Figure 9.1 contains a summary of 12 situations in public life for which
the respondents have stated the language they ordinarily use. The situa-
tions can be distinguished according to the degree of formality and the
familiarity with the interlocutor. A cross-tabulation has been made with
language background: those respondents whohave learnedFrisianas their
first language (L1) are distinguished from those who indicated that they
could speak Frisian, but it was not their mother tongue (L2) (those who
cannot speak Frisian are excluded).
At the bottomof the figure we findthat 85%of FrisianL1-speakers habit-
ually speak Frisian in the shop where they do their daily shopping,
whereas only 42% of L2-speakers use Frisian. So language background
Frisian Language Update 221
Table 9.1 Home language 1967, 1980 and 1994
1967 (%) 1980 (%) 1994 (%)
Frisian 71 56 55
Dutch 13 33 34
Dialect 16 11 10
Total 100 100 99
seems to determine much more which language will be usedin public than
language ability by itself. In climbing this mountain graph the gap
between L1 and L2 speakers narrows in terms of the percentage that does
use Frisian in the selected situations. For instance, when we look at
language used with a medical specialist all seem to agree that this is a
formal situation with status differences between both interlocutors and
thus less fit for Frisian.
Still, inthe situationof language spokenwithDutchneighbours second-
language learners barely use Frisian with their Dutch-speaking neigh-
bours, where first language speakers do so in about one-fifth of the cases
(19%). At the top of the graph there is very small difference between first-
language speakers and second-language speakers of Frisian. Speaking to a
Dutchtourist is obviously something for whichFrisiancannot be used. Of
course, these results have informedus only about a limitednumber of situ-
ations of language choice. It is quite obvious that the language of the inter-
locutor is an important factor. Language choice is person bound.
On the basis of systematic participant observation a set of four rules for
interactional language behaviour has been formulated which is a sort of
linguistic etiquette (Gorter, 1993: 16781). The first rule is: [1] Dutchis the
222 Europe
Frisian speakers (L2) native Frisian speakers
42
41
32
31
23
21
22
18
13
5
4
1
85
83
80
76
71
68
60
56
41
22
19
4
shop assistants
nearest neighbours
at postoffice
teacher
policeman
stranger at home-door
mayor
physician
stranger in town
medical specialist
Dutch neighbours
Dutch tourist
20 40 60 80 100 120 140
Figure 9.1 Use of Frisian in 12 situations: as first language (L1) and as
second language (L2)
common language for everyone. This rule does not take bilingualisminto
consideration. Because everyone can speak Dutch, everyone could theo-
retically act in accordance with this rule. There is a second, contrasting
rule, which is [2] Everyone may speak his/her own language. Because
almost all inhabitants can understand Frisian, in principle all Frisian-
speakers couldact inaccordance withsucha normandspeakFrisianall the
time without making communication impossible. However, the rule of the
right to speak their own language is only applied cautiously. Thus, addi-
tionally, a third rule can be formulated: [3] Frisians must speak Dutch to
Dutch-speakers andonlyFrisiantoFrisian-speakers. This rule seems quite
simple and straightforward but, as could be shown, is not without prob-
lems when applied. For instance, second-language learners are a compli-
cating factor. In case of a conversation with three or more participants still
another rule is put forward: [4] Aspeaker will have to accommodate to the
language of conversation in the company of others. There is a preference
for one language in one conversation (Feitsma, 1984).
These four rules do not predict the language choice precisely inall cases.
Interaction generally is a process of negotiation where not everything is
arranged beforehand. The rules are part of the normative ideas about
appropriate language use: what language is marked in certain cases and
what is not. Frisian is usually the noticeable, the marked language and
Dutch is the expected, the unmarked language. This is an indication of an
underlying power process. Dutch sets the tone in the community, a fact
which is very much taken for granted. The persistent alternation of Frisian
and Dutch in one conversation usually costs an extra effort. Aperson who
continues to speak Frisian to a non-Frisian person runs the risk of being
labelled a Frisian activist or, in other situations, as someone who cant
even properly speak Dutch.
The language situation is faced with a paradox: there is stability for the
minority language Frisian, but at the same time an increase in the presence
of the dominant language Dutch. One of the reasons is that bilingual
speakers have learned to speak and use Dutch better, but at the same time
have not unlearned their Frisian.
GIDS Stages for Frisian
In my description of the eight stages of the GIDS (Graded Inter-
generational Disruption Scale) I will follow Fishmans description of
FrisianRLS efforts andupdate or criticise it where necessary. Inhis chapter
Fishman (1991: 149) states that the province of Friesland as a moderately
prosperous region has as a result . . . experienced relatively more in-
Frisian Language Update 223
migrationthanout-migrationfor manyyears. This seems tobe toogeneral,
because this was onlytrue duringthe 1970s. Duringthe 1980s and1990s the
migration pattern changed again to what it had been for over a century:
more people leaving than settling.
From 1960 till today the number of persons leaving the province every
year has remained fairly constant, averaging 25,000. However, the
number of newcomers has fluctuated from just over 20,000 in 1960, going
up to a high point of almost 35,000 in 1974, decreasing to 22,000 in 1984 and
settling at almost 27,000 in 1997. The outcome has been a surplus of immi-
grants between 1971 and 1982 and a negative departure balance in most
other years. During the last two years (1997 and 1998) there was a slight
positive balance again, mainly due to higher figures for immigrants from
abroad (refugees). The population has increased from 508,000 in 1980,
580,000 in 1990, to 621,000 in 1999. Growth has come predominantly froma
surplus of births.
There is also internal migrationinthe province, to andfromthe country-
side. Living in towns has become more important. The effect of this reloca-
tion of the population on the distribution of the language has been
substantial. Bothprocesses of migrationhave made geographic differences
related to language less distinct.
Stages 8 and 7: Reassembling Xish, learning it as a second
language during adulthood and enriching the ethnocultural and
ethnolinguistic experiences of those who are already Xish-
speaking
As Fishmansays, FrisianRLSefforts are more focusedonstage 7 than8.
The Frisian language is still well and naturally spoken by . . . speech
networks engaged in many activities. Stage 8 is not really at stake in the
Frisian case, notwithstanding discussions on the quality and development
of the standard language, where some seem to oppose the re-introduction
of older speech forms.
The expressions of culture mentioned by Fishman (theatre, Frisian
festival, film, music andliteraryevenings) do, ina sense, flourishmore than
ever. For instance, open-air theatre performances in Frisian are enor-
mously popular: the most famous annual theatre in Jorwert has a series of
10 evenings, for which tickets are sold out through special phone-lines
within hours. In literature there are still no real literary big young talents
or discoveries, but there is anongoingdiscussionbetweenthe older grey-
hair establishment (over 50) and a younger generation (under 40), which
seems to gain momentum and may become quite interesting. Because
open-air theatre and also music are so popular, today it would be harder to
224 Europe
conclude that most of stage 7 reaches only those already committed to
Frisian as Fishman did. One of the breakthroughs for Frisian has been its
remarkable acceptance in pop music over the Netherlands as a whole: two
top-10 hits over the last two years. Also the language has been used as a
commercial gimmick inDutchTVandradio, stressing humour for selling a
traditional Frisian alcoholic beverage. Part of the reason for this success
certainly lies in the Eleven cities tours (Alvestdetocht) in 1985, 1986, and
1997. This ice-skating tour of some 200 kilometres can only be organised in
very severe winters. These tours were mass media events of mega size and
as a result the province of Friesland, but also the Frisian language and
culture, became a bit more popular in the rest of the Netherlands.
Stage 6: The intergenerational, demographically concentrated
family-home-neighbourhood-community sphere: the basis of
transmission
The slowdecline of percentages of mother-tongue speakers in the tradi-
tional rural areas has continued and there has been a slight increase in the
towns. Today, on average some 70% in the countryside and some 40% in
the towns are Frisian L1 speakers. This means that the speakers are
geographically more evenly distributed (during the 1950s these percent-
ages were over 90 and below 30, respectively). Migration from rural areas
to the towns is, of course, an important factor. It also means that almost all
neighbourhoods andmost families have become linguisticallymixed. Here
lies a potentially dangerous situation for Frisian, because research tells us
also that in mixed marriages by far the majority will opt for Dutch as the
language for the next generation. Against this goes the finding that quite a
few persons learn to speak Frisian as second language (20% of the total
population) and will use it to some degree, even if much less than first-
language speakers. Another surprising finding was that Frisian-speaking
parents, on average, have slightly more children than non-Frisian speak-
ers, this birthsurplus has compensatedsomewhat for the loss due to mixed
families (Gorter and Jonkman, 1995a).
Also of some apparent importance is a process of mental urbanisation
of the rural areas (Van der Vaart, 1999; Mak,1997) which leads to further
Dutchification of everyday life. As Fishman already rightfully observed,
the few cultural manifestations of Frisians cannot repair damage to foun-
dations. Similarly the religious domain cannot do much repair. In partic-
ular not because there have been fewif any changes in the use of Frisian in
the church (which is generally low), although an organisation of ministers
is rather active.
Frisian Language Update 225
Stage 5: Schools for children (that do not meet the requirements of
compulsory education) and courses in language and literacy
acquisition for adults
In his original chapter Fishman (p. 166) mentions the Afk organisation
that provides adult courses in the Frisian language. Ten years ago Afk
was perhaps a somewhat old-fashioned operation, but it nowhas renewed
its courses and has diversified its products, also catering for special target
groups such as legal professionals, workers in health care and an intensive
course for foreigners. Afk is also working on a general course on CD-
ROM, but, other thanadvertisingits products, is not yet active withcourses
through the Internet, although this can be expected in the years to come.
As Fishman (p. 166) states every RLS movement will have its great
dictionary project. When he wrote his chapter the Larger Frisian Dictio-
nary had published five volumes. Ten years later there are 15 and the
Fryske Akademy has almost finished this monument to the language. In
total either 22 or 23 volumes will appear. Other more applied dictionary
projects are also nearing completion, such as the FrisianFrisian desk
dictionary and the FrisianEnglish dictionary. In 1985 work began on the
establishment of a Language Data Bank for Frisian, which now contains
over 15 million tokens fromwritten texts. Areverse dictionary and a spell-
checker were the fewdirectly usable products of the Language Data Bank.
In terms of computational linguistics results are below original expecta-
tions. Of more recent date, a new project has begun in the field of spoken
language. Following the model and procedures for the Corpus of Spoken
Dutch, a major project in the Netherlands launched in 1998, the Fryske
Akademy is attempting to set up a parallel, although smaller project for a
Corpus of Spoken Frisian. This will result in a cross-section of the Frisian
language as it is in use by the end of the 20th century. Probably it will also
have some new applications in speech technology. The relevance of this
work lies inkeeping a language suchas Frisianat least ina minimal way up
todate withsomanynewtechnological developments relatedtolanguage.
Stage 4a: Schools in lieu of compulsory education and fully under
RLS control
In his chapter Fishman (p. 16971) outlines a brief historical account of
developments in education, which I will not have to repeat here. There
have not been serious new developments in the teaching of Frisian in
primary schools during the last few years (Zondag, 1993). The great
majority of primary schools comply with the requirement that they teach
the subject of Frisian. A survey by the Inspectorate in 1988 showed that
226 Europe
about 3045minutes a weekwere spent onteachingFrisianas a subject. The
same survey told us that one-fifth of the primary schools used Frisian as a
mediumof instruction with other subjects. In 1993 the central government
published official key objectives for Frisian, which describe the knowl-
edge and skills pupils should have at the end of primary school. It was
striking that the objectives for Frisian and Dutch were exactly the same.
Although the data were collected before the key objectives were
published, studies by De Jong and Riemersma (1994) and Ytsma (1995),
made it very clear that school practice was not reaching those ideal objec-
tives for Frisian at all. The studies, however, reaffirmedthe conclusion that
teaching Frisianas a subject or using Frisianas a mediumof instructionhas
no detrimental effect upon the results obtained in Dutch. Children in
Friesland, on average, have the same level of achievement in Dutch at the
end of primary school as do other children in the rest of the Netherlands.
Their achievements in Frisian tend to lag behind their achievements in
Dutch.
Aseeming breakthrough came in 1993 when Frisian became a compul-
sory subject in the first, basic stage of secondary education. But the obliga-
tionturnedout tobe light as a feather. Muchdissatisfactionremains among
RLS activists and responsible policy-makers about such a minimal imple-
mentation.
Type 4b schools: The object of RLS affection as a distinctly part-
time guest in the public school arena
There were no alternative schools of their own of whatever type. In 1989
an initiative was taken by a group of parents to establish all-Frisian
language playgroups (pjutteboartersplakken), aiming both at Frisian-
speaking and Dutch-speaking children. The first playgroups were estab-
lished in larger towns because parents were dissatisfied with the fact that
existing pre-school provisions took no account of the Frisian language
background of their children. In 1999 the Frisian playgroups organisation
was responsible for 11 groups; not a very spectacular number considering
that over 200 other playgroups exist in Friesland. As Fishman (p. 173)
concluded The many weaknesses of Frisian that we have encountered at
levels 6, 5 and 4 constitute a very serious handicap [to] future possibilities
in the RLS arena, particularly insofar as the improvement of the inter-
generational transmissibility of the language is concerned.
A new development in 1998 was an experiment with trilingual educa-
tion. That is, some primary schools have started to develop a curriculum
where Frisian and Dutch are used on a 5050 basis during the first 56
grades andEnglish is introducedas subject andmediumat the upper stage
Frisian Language Update 227
as a thirdlanguage for about 20%of teachingtime. This experiment is being
scrutinised by intensive research (Ytsma, 1999), but it is premature to
predict any of the outcomes.
Stage 3: The world of work, both within the ethnolinguistic
community (among other Xmen), as well as outside it (among
Ymen)
Frisian plays a small role in trade and industry (Fishman, p. 173). As a
spoken language its use is quite common on the workfloor, but may be
quite exceptional at the management level. In all kinds of client-service
situations, with unknown interlocutors, there is an expectation that Frisian
will be understood. The figure of 94% of the population being able to
understand the language, makes clear why this expectation is almost
always fulfilled. Every now and then there may be a small riot over this
issue. For instance, in a public meeting when someone asks that Dutch be
used. Whereas 30 years agothe chairmanof sucha meetingwouldimmedi-
ately yield to a request for Dutch, today such concession runs the risk of
protests by Frisian speakers and usually some negotation takes place.
Spoken Frisian may be generally accepted, written Frisian still stands out
as something special. Thus, in some advertisements that fact is used to
attract attention. All in all Frisian is spoken in the work environment,
outside the neighbourhood, without anyproblems, but the domainof work
is not an area of any RLS efforts, except for civil servants and teachers.
Stage 2: Xish in regional mass media and government services
Quite a bit of energy in Frisian has been devoted to developments in the
domains of media (Radio/TV) and the government (official domain). For
this reason I will discuss it more elaborately. Radio Frysln had 20 hours
per week in 1988 and almost no TV (30 hours per year). Ten years later the
media position has improved substantially. Today there are daily TV
broadcasts in Frisian (one hour original production early in the evening of
every day, with reruns). These broadcasts are quite popular: the viewer-
density is the highest of all channels in Friesland at that hour. This is
regardeda big success by most RLS activists. Opinions differ on howmuch
it serves as an example and influences the use of Frisian by its speakers.
Moreover, in both regional newspapers the same weak position of 10 or 20
years ago still continues, where Frisian is used very marginally in less than
5% of all texts. Frisian is just a prominent topic for regional journalists,
especially for human interest (e.g. a student from Vienna writing a
doctoral thesis on Frisian) or a riot (e.g. civil servants protesting against
the language policy for Frisian). In 1999 a new monthly magazine will be
228 Europe
launched. There are some prudent developments inmultimedia (e.g. a CD-
ROM with the history of Friesland). On the Internet there is a Frisian
ribbon campaign promoting the use of Frisian for web pages, there is even
an all-Frisian search engine (Frysyk), but not yet any on-line course in
Frisian or even an on-line dictionary.
As Fishman (p. 177) rightly observed a great deal of attention has been
given to the legalistic niceties of language legislation and policy state-
ments, and the overcoming of Dutch resistance in these areas is greatly
stressed as a matter of principle. The legal framework which has been
described above is the outcome of this attention and may be seen as a
victory over Dutch resistance. However, Fishman also already observed
that the implementation of newopportunities leaves much to be desired.
Policy plans have been retarded and plans have not been implemented (a
lot of attention was given to the highly symbolic, but really non-issue of
Frisian place-names). Language policy has lost its prominence on the polit-
ical agenda. The Charter for European minority or regional languages was
important to keep policy development going. That Charter is mainly of
symbolic importance toFrisian, but at the same time the Dutchgovernment
has recognised Low Saxon (a collection of dialects), among others, as a
regional language. Suchrecognitionhas stimulatednewinitiatives inother
regions of the Netherlands, but at the same time has helped to keep Frisian
ambitions down.
The Hague (the central state government) has acceded to demands
fromthe provincial government very slowly and especially when it would
cost little to nothing. Fishman used the term blockage , which might be
less applicable these days. A turn of policy in The Hague took place in the
period198993 whenthere was anUnder-Minister for the Interior whowas
of Frisian descent and who was proud of it. Moreover, she was made
explicitly responsible for coordinating Frisian affairs at the central state
level. The Council of Frisian movement, an organisation of volunteers, has
attracted some young persons, but has not yet been able to obtain a clear
new profile. Activism for Frisian RLS efforts still has quite a low profile.
Stage 1: Government, employment and education at the highest
levels
Fishman (p. 178) observed that the Frisian scene was far less promising
andfar less coordinatedandvigorous thanthe Basque counterpart, consid-
ering the highest levels. Not much has changed and there still is no well-
considered set of urgent priorities defined by the Frisians themselves.
Language policy plans have to a large degree indeed been public
Frisian Language Update 229
posturing and the adoption of well-meaning and good-sounding resolu-
tions; implementation has turned out to be difficult.
There is still no leading intellectual centre in Friesland because the
plans for a university have failed. Even where the differences between
colleges for higher vocational training and universities are becoming
smaller, the three institutions for higher education in the capital of
Friesland have not provided anything like intellectual leadership to the
community. They are involved in a battle to attract students fromall over
the Netherlands (or from abroad), thus Frisian is usually more seen as a
nuisance than as an asset.
Cutbacks in Dutch higher education have resulted in the disappearance
of two of the five chairs in Frisian linguistics outside Friesland. The full
chair at the Universityof Amsterdamcouldbe savedthroughclose collabo-
ration with the Fryske Akademy and fulfils more or less the function of a
Frisian embassy in the Dutch capital.
8
During the 1990s four doctoral
dissertations were published in Frisian and another eight dealt with a
specific Frisian topic from linguistics or literature studies. The Fryske
Akademy underwent a small reduction of staff, notwithstanding a very
positive peer review by a committee of international experts in 1995.
General Conclusion
As far as the effect of the RLSbookbyFishmanis concerned, I findit hard
to judge its impact. The chapter on Frisian and Basque was translated into
Frisian and separately published, but it did not get any further attention in
the public debate. The provincial advisory body on Frisian (Berie foar it
Frysk) has looked into it seriously. I have come across it several times in the
scientific literature, but it has not become a handbook for RLS activists.
Fishman (p. 180) concluded that things may not be entirely bleak for
Frisian. The basic problem seems to be in activating the goodwill. This is
most certainlythe case andI doubt whether muchhas changedover the last
decade. The struggle is far fromover. Indeed, it may go onforever, ineffec-
tive though it may generally be insofar as intergenerational trans-
missibility of Frisian as mother tongue is concerned (p. 181).
My conclusion is that things have, on the whole, not gotten worse for
Frisian. Of course, there are also some real threats today, especially in
mixed families and neighbourhoods as well as for the continued erosion of
the language itself. The quality of the language is deteriorating and Frisian
may at a certain point run the risk of dissolving into Dutch. In Friesland
both languages are used and they are sometimes in conflict. The norms for
language use can also be seen in terms of a power process. The Frisian
speakers are still a quantitative majority in their own area, but the Dutch
230 Europe
speakers are the group with most power. There is an unequal chance for
Frisian speakers to realise their language preference vis--vis the Dutch
speakers. So, the rule that everyone may speak his/her own language is
really restricted in practice. Dutch is the common language of everyone
because all Frisian-speakers are bilingual and most Dutch-speakers are
only passively bilingual. Youcan always say as a kindof powerplay, let us
all speak Dutch and only fewdiehards will not comply. Frisian is allowed,
and its use must be possible, but other mechanisms are doing the work of
constraining its use.
The well-known concept of diglossia may still be of some use to describe
the relationship between Frisian and Dutch. Application of the concept is a
matter of some debate among scholars of the Frisian language situation
(Gorter, 1993: 247). It is clear that the older static division of functions
between the two languages has given way to new patterns. There used to
be some sort of stable diglossia relationship, where Dutch was the higher
and more prestigious language, and Frisian was the language of the home
and of the family. Today Dutch enters into and cannot be kept out of the
intimate spheres of the home, friends, family and neighbourhood. At the
same time Frisian seeks to conquer some of the higher domains of educa-
tion, media and public administration. Frisian has made inroads in areas
where it was not used 50 years ago. This implies that it is far less clear what
language to use, at what moment in time, or who is going or not going to
use Frisian. Dutch is the language that is taken for granted, Frisian is the
marked language most, but not all, of the time. Conscious speakers of
Frisian or RLS activists may find themselves frequently in a situation of
competing bilingualism. However, as a written language Frisian has
remainedquite marginal, thus there is still a diglossic distributionbetween
spoken and written language functions. A number of structural power
processes work against the use of Frisian. This is not only at the level of
society as a whole, but also in personal interaction between people. It
implies at the same time that, in these conflicts between the two language
groups, some positive tensions are also created that lead to more dyna-
mism in society, a dynamism which is lacking in monolingual societies.
Notes
1. The province of Friesland has roughly 340,000 mother-tongue speakers of
Frisian or 460,000 inhabitants who claim to have at least reasonable profi-
ciency. Respectively 55%and74%of 621,000 inhabitants (1 January 1999). Take
note, these percentages are based on survey-research among the population of
12years andolder from1994. The sample size was 1368successful interviews.
2. Fishmans chapter in the original RLS book on Frisian (and Basque) was based
on visits to Friesland in 1982, 1983 and 1989 (Fishman, 1991: 183). Mine is based
Frisian Language Update 231
on (almost) continuous presence as a professional sociologist of language,
workingfor the Fryske Akademy(andsince 1994 part-time for the Universityof
Amsterdam). Fishmans chapter was translated into Frisian and published in
the scientific journal of the Fryske Akademy, It Beaken (jrg 53, 1991, nr 3/4, pp.
12049). Comments were added by Jansma, Jelsma, and Van Rijn and by Van
der Plank (pp. 15060). Even though It Beaken has a circulation of 3000, to my
knowledge, it did not attract any further response.
3. In cooperation with the European Parliament and the Ministry for Culture of
Luxembourg; based on a need for improved communication.
4. The Mercator Education project is concerned with information, documentation
and research in the field of education involving autochthonous minority lan-
guages inthe EuropeanUnion. It is part of a networkincludingMercator Media
andMercator Legislation. For further information: www.fa.knaw.nl\mercator.
5. There are eight member states of the Council of Europe, thus far, that have rati-
fied: Finland, Norway, Hungary, the Netherlands, Croatia, Liechtenstein,
Switzerland and Germany.
6. Fromthis list it becomes clear that certain variants that were earlier regardedas
dialects have now been officially declared (regional) languages.
7. One point of caution is in place: the 71% comes from the sample by Pietersen
where Frisian may have been somewhat oversampled. Due to randomdrawhe
did not include a number of municipalities where less Frisian has traditionally
beenspoken, whereas in1980 and1994 all municipalities were representedpro-
portionally.
8. Although right next door to the university building in the centre of Amsterdam
there is an arts gallery cum antiques and books shop that is literally called the
Embassy of Friesland (Ambassade van Friesland).
References
Dijk, K.J. van (1987) Language policy in Frysln: Diverging concepts. International
Journal of the Sociology of Language 64, 3745.
Feitsma, A. (1984) Interlingual communication Dutch-Frisian, a model for Scot-
land? In D. Strauss and H.W. Drescher (eds) Scottish Language and Literature,
Medieval and Renaissance (pp. 5562). Frankfurt.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift (inparticular the chapter: The Cases of
Basque and Frisian). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gorter, D. (1993) Taal fan klerken en klanten. Ljouwert/Leeuwarden: Fryske
Akademy.
Gorter, D. et al. (1984) Taal yn Frysln. Ljouwert: Fryske Akademy.
Gorter, D. and Jonkman, R.J. (1994) Taal op it wurk fan provinsjale amtenaren.
Ljouwert: Fryske Akademy.
Gorter, D. and Jonkman, R.J. (1995a) Enkele veranderingsprocessen binnen de
taalverhoudingen in Friesland. In E. Huls and J. Klatter-Folmer (eds) Artikelen
van de Tweede Sociolingustische Conferentie (pp. 21728) Tilburg.
Gorter, D. and Jonkman, R.J. (1995b) Taal yn Frysln: op e nij besjoen. Ljouwert/
Leeuwarden: Fryske Akademy.
Jansma, L,G., Jelsma, G.H. and van Rijn, J.G. (1991) It fuortbestean fan de Fryske
taalminderheid. It Beaken, jrg 53, nr 3/4, 15055.
232 Europe
Jong, S. de, and Riemersma, A.M.J. (1994) Taalpeiling yn Frysln. Ljouwert/
Leeuwarden: Fryske Akademy.
Mak, G. (1997) Hoe God verdween uit Jorwerd. Amsterdam: Atlas.
Pietersen, L. (1969) De Friezen en hun taal. Drachten: Laverman.
Plank, P. van der (1991) De ferhlding tusken taalpolityk, aktivisme en boppelaach
yn Frysln. It Beaken, jrg 53, nr 3/4, 15660.
Schmidt, A. (1997) Die friesische Sprache im Verwaltungsverfahren und vor
Gericht-Neuregelungen in den Niederlnden zur Frderung einer Minder-
heitensprache. Europa Ethnica, 12, 54 jrg, 309.
Vaart, J.H.P. van der (1999) Boerderijen en platteland in verandering. Ljouwert/
Leeuwarden: Fryske Akademy.
Ytsma, J. (1995) Frisian as First and Second Language. Ljouwert/Leeuwarden: Fryske
Akademy.
Ytsma, J. (1999) Tweetaligheid bij kleuters in Friesland. In E. Huls and B. Weltens
(eds) Artikelen van de derde sociolingustische conferentie (pp. 497508). Delft:
Eburon.
Zondag, K. (ed.) (1993) Bilingual Education in Friesland (Facts and Prospects).
Ljouwert/Leeuwarden: GCO-Frysln.
Frisian Language Update 233
Chapter 10
Reversing Language Shift: The Case
of Basque
M.-J. AZURMENDI, E. BACHOC and F. ZABALETA
Fishmans 1991 publication, Reversing Language Shift (RLS), marked an
important milestone in the sociology of languages and linguistic groups. It
was also important for Euskara (the Basque language) and for the
Euskaldun (Basque speaking) group, included in his RLS model. This work
has several aims. One is to describe the situation of Euskara in 1999, to
compare it withFishmans 1991 descriptionandtoestablishits evolutionin
the 1990s. Another goal is to diagnose the situation of Euskara according to
the RLS model.
This discussion predominantly takes on the sociological theoretical
perspective of the graded intergenerational disruption scale in eight
stages (GIDS), Fishmans RLS model (1991). It will attempt to show some
empirical informationreflectingthe present situationof Euskara todayand
its development in recent years. There are several descriptive studies to
refer to, most of them fragmentary and dispersed. However, rare are the
ones that apply a theoretical model and a general perspective for the
different situations in the Basque Country. In this sense, Fishmans 1991
paper on The Case of Basque was something of a first with respect to
Euskara. The main sources will be the results of the following two empir-
ical studies: Encuesta Sociolingstica de Euskal Herria: La continuidad del
Euskara I (ASociolinguistic Survey of the Basque Country: The Continuity
of Euskara I) (Basque Government, 1991) (ES-1991) and the Encuesta
Sociolingstica de Euskal Herria: La continuidad del Euskara II (A
Sociolinguistic Survey of the Basque Country: The Continuity of Euskara
II) (Basque Government, 1996) (ES-1996). Both surveys have the same
empirical design and cover all three Basque political territories: the Basque
Autonomous Community(BAC), the Navarrese Autonomous Community
(NAC) and Iparralde (Basque territories within France); this will simplify
comparison between the data offered. Only bibliographical references
234
Europe
The Case of Basque
directlyrelatedtodata andempirical results havingtodowithEuskara will
be cited.
The Social Situation of Euskara in the 1990s
Territorialisation of the Basque Country
It may be useful here to remindreaders of the Basque Countrys political
and territorial differences, being divided between Spain and France. In
Spain the southern Basque Country is further broken down into two
Autonomous Communities: the BAC comprises the territories of Araba,
Bizkaia and Gipuzkoa, and the NAC, the territory of Nafarroa (Navarre).
Eachof these political divisions has its owngovernment andparliament (as
of 1979 inthe BACand1980 inthe NAC) andtherefore eachapplies its own
linguistic policies. The French northern Basque Country, or Iparralde, is
made up of the Basque territories of Benafarroa, Lapurdi and Zuberoa,
where they are not recognisedofficially as a group, nor are they any kindof
politicaladministrative unit within France, and, therefore, cannot apply
linguistic policies of their own. (In Fishman, 1991, a map of the Basque
Countrycanbe seeninfigure 6.1, p. 151, andinfigure 6.2, p. 153.) This polit-
ical-territorial division is directly reflected in the situation of Euskara.
Inthe BAC, the Basic Lawfor the Normalisationof the Basque Language
(1982) set down a single linguistic plan for its three territories, designing a
single Euskara recovery and normalisation plan. It is based on the shared
official status of the two languages: Euskara and Spanish. This joint official
status opens the doors to equal formal treatment for both languages. The
official Autonomous Community status foments the recovery and
normalisation of Euskara throughout the BAC.
In the NAC, the Law of the Basque Language (1986), establishes three
linguistic areas, and creates a different linguistic policy for each. In the
Euskara-speaking area, lightly populated and rural, approximately 75%of
the inhabitants are Euskaldunes (Euskara speakers). Linguistic policy calls
for official status for bothEuskara andSpanishinthis area andits approach
is somewhere between maintenance and recovery of Euskara. The non-
Euskara-speaking area is more populated, semi-rural and somewhat
industrial. Here, the presence of Euskara is not formally recognised and
therefore there is no linguistic policy. The official position with respect to
Euskara here fluctuates between tolerance and prohibition. Finally, the
mixed area around the capital of Iruea/Pamplona is the most important
quantitatively, as it accounts for more than half of Navarres total popula-
tion. Qualitatively it is the most active and innovative, dedicated to
industry, trade and services. Approximately 15% of the population are
The Case of Basque 235
Euskaldunes and there is a linguistic policy which supports Euskara to a
certain degree.
In addition to the official linguistic policies in the BAC and the NAC,
there is also the Spanish central governments policy. Here, only Spanish is
recognised as the official language of the country, although it allows each
Autonomous Community having its own language to establish shared
official status and to develop its own internal linguistic policy.
In France there is no official recognition of any language other than
French (specifically laid down in the 1994-revision of the French Constitu-
tion), nor of any cultural-linguistic groupother than the French. Hence, the
official positioninIparralde withrespect to Euskara is either prohibitionor
tolerance. This means that virtually all of the activity related to the promo-
tionandrecoveryof Euskara stems fromvoluntaryinitiative, as canbe seen
in primary education, roadsigns, songs andbooks, folk expressions, etc. In
1994 the semi-official Institut Culturel Basque/Euskal Kultur Erakundea
(the Basque Cultural Institute) was createdandbecame the most important
organisation for the promotion of Euskara.
Table 10.1 shows the percentage of Euskaldunes (Basque speakers) in the
different Basque territories in 1991 (ES-1991) and 1996 (ES-1996), with an
end to establishing Euskara loss or recovery trends.
Table 10.1 implies the degree of loss sufferedbyEuskara, and, as a conse-
quence, its status as minority language within the Basque Country itself.
Various factors confirm this loss: there are virtually no Basque-speaking
monolinguals, the majority of the population is Erdaldun monolingual
(Spanish-speaking or French-speaking only) and furthermore, the Euskal-
dunes are almost all bilingual. But what is most important now is to point
out the trends between 1991 and 1996. Similar to what was seen in the
1980s, these trends show an increase in Euskaldunes, particularly in the
BACbut also in the NAC. However, there has been a decrease in Iparralde.
The differences between the three Basque political territories are clear. The
greatest rate of recovery of Euskara in the BAC is interpreted, at least in
part, as a consequence of a linguistic policy at aiming recovery and
normalisation of Euskara. In the NAC, recovery of Euskara also took place,
inspite of the different linguistic policies there. However, inIparralde there
was a rise in the Euskara-loss rate due, in part, to the fact that voluntary
initiative efforts promoting Euskara are not enough. They do not make up
for the lack of an official linguistic policy to foment Euskara, and do not
suffice even to maintain the language.
These different results tend to be interpreted as reflections of the
different degrees of the Basque linguistic groups political power and self-
government in the three Basque territories. That is, if we go back to the
236 Europe
T
a
b
l
e
1
0
.
1
L
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
c
o
m
p
e
t
e
n
c
e
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
/
o
r
E
r
d
a
r
a
,
b
y
p
o
l
i
t
i
c
a
l
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
,
i
n
1
9
9
1
a
n
d
1
9
9
6
(
a
b
s
o
l
u
t
e
a
n
d
%
)
1
9
9
1
1
9
9
6
T
o
t
a
l
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
T
o
t
a
l
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
M
o
n
o
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
2
3
,
5
0
0
1
.
0
1
7
,
9
0
0
1
.
0
2
,
7
0
0
0
.
7
2
,
9
0
0
1
.
4
1
2
,
4
0
0
0
.
5
9
,
8
0
0
0
.
6
1
,
1
0
0
0
.
6
1
,
5
0
0
0
.
7
A
c
t
i
v
e
B
i
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
5
0
5
,
2
0
0
2
1
.
4
4
0
1
,
5
0
0
2
3
.
1
3
7
,
5
0
0
8
.
9
6
6
,
2
0
0
3
2
.
7
5
3
4
,
1
0
0
2
2
.
0
4
3
8
,
4
0
0
2
4
.
7
4
1
,
0
0
0
9
.
4
5
4
,
7
0
0
2
5
.
7
P
a
s
s
i
v
e
B
i
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
1
8
2
,
7
0
0
8
.
4
1
4
8
,
7
0
0
8
.
5
1
9
,
3
0
0
4
.
6
1
4
,
7
0
0
7
.
0
3
5
2
,
9
0
0
1
4
.
5
2
9
0
,
2
0
0
1
6
.
3
4
2
,
8
0
0
9
.
8
1
9
,
8
0
0
9
.
3
E
r
d
a
r
a
M
o
n
o
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
1
,
6
5
9
,
8
0
0
6
9
.
9
1
,
1
7
3
,
5
0
0
6
7
.
4
3
6
1
,
2
0
0
8
5
.
9
1
2
5
,
1
0
0
5
8
.
8
1
,
5
2
8
,
7
0
0
6
3
.
0
1
,
0
4
0
,
0
0
0
5
8
.
5
3
5
2
,
3
0
0
8
0
.
6
1
3
6
,
4
0
0
6
4
.
2
2
,
3
7
1
,
2
0
0
1
0
0
1
,
7
4
1
,
6
0
0
1
0
0
4
2
0
,
7
0
0
1
0
0
2
0
8
,
9
0
0
1
0
0
2
,
4
2
8
,
1
0
0
1
0
0
1
,
7
7
8
,
5
0
0
1
0
0
4
3
7
,
2
0
0
1
0
0
2
1
2
,
4
0
0
1
0
0
(
S
o
u
r
c
e
:
S
o
c
i
o
l
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
S
u
r
v
e
y
,
1
9
9
1
a
n
d
1
9
9
6
;
E
S
-
1
9
9
1
,
E
S
-
1
9
9
6
.
)
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
;
E
r
d
a
r
a
=
S
p
a
n
i
s
h
(
i
n
B
A
C
a
n
d
N
A
C
)
o
r
F
r
e
n
c
h
(
i
n
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
)
;
B
A
C
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
N
A
C
=
N
a
v
a
r
r
e
s
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
(
F
r
a
n
c
e
)
The Case of Basque 237
theory of relative power or relative ethnolinguistic vitality to explain this
situation, we would have to underline the importance of institutional
backing and control (Azurmendi, 1998), in stimulating Euskara loss/
recovery. This interpretation relates to and/or reinforces other political
demands pertaining to power and greater self-rule. Some of these
demands include changing the Spanish and French Constitutions, recog-
nising Basque decision-making powers, unification of the Basque territo-
ries into a single political and administrative entity, and even co-
dependence or political independence within the framework of the Euro-
pean Union, etc.
The cultural-linguistic heterogeneity of the Basque Country
The Basque Country reveals a great deal of internal heterogeneity in
terms of history, demographics, socioeconomics, language, culture and
ideology. This heterogeneity is important in order to explain the
processes of the loss and recovery of Euskara in each of the eight stages
suggested in the GIDS model. This chapter will showsome of the aspects
of this culturallinguistic heterogeneity derivedmainly fromits situation
of contact between two ethnic groups: on the one hand we have the
autochthonous indigenous group of the Basque Country, those who
make it possible to continue talking about the case of Basque and, on the
other, the Spanish-origin group, in the BAC and the NAC, or of French
origin in Iparralde. In the BAC, the most densely populatedBasque polit-
ical territory and the one taking in the highest proportion of immigrants,
approximately half of todays population is of first- and second-genera-
tion Spanish origin (Ruiz de Olabuenaga & Blanco, 1994). The level of
culturallinguistic integration of these people in the Basque Country, for
a variety of reasons, has been very poor. Repercussion in the present-day
situation of Euskara has been substantial, as can be seen in Table 10.2.
The data in Table 10.2 shows a disturbing situation from the point of
viewof the intergenerational transmission of Euskara. This transmission
seems to be moderately successful in the case of natives (born in the
Basque Country) whose parents are also natives, but not otherwise.
All in all, ES-1991 results showthe prevailing sociodemographic char-
acteristics of the four linguistic types taken into consideration, both as a
whole and by political territories. These characteristics are as follows:
The monolingual Euskaldunes (Euskara speakers) and the active
bilinguals (the most significant linguistic types in terms of
Euskara transmission) are over 35 (64%), almost always natives
(97%), speak Euskara as their first language (L1) (86%), live
primarily in the BAC(79%) and in the territory of Gipuzkoa (46%),
238 Europe
T
a
b
l
e
1
0
.
2
L
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
c
o
m
p
e
t
e
n
c
e
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
/
o
r
E
r
d
a
r
a
,
a
c
c
o
r
d
i
n
g
t
o
e
t
h
n
o
g
r
a
p
h
i
c
o
r
i
g
i
n
a
n
d
b
y
p
o
l
i
t
i
c
a
l
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
,
i
n
1
9
9
1
(
%
)
N
a
t
i
v
e
s
(
B
o
r
n
i
n
t
h
e
B
a
s
q
u
e
C
o
u
n
t
r
y
)
S
p
a
n
i
s
h
i
m
m
i
g
r
a
n
t
s
(
F
i
r
s
t
g
e
n
e
r
a
t
i
o
n
)
T
o
t
a
l
N
a
t
i
v
e
p
a
r
e
n
t
s
N
a
t
i
v
e
f
a
t
h
e
r
/
m
o
t
h
e
r
I
m
m
i
g
r
a
n
t
p
a
r
e
n
t
s
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
M
o
n
o
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
T
o
t
a
l
1
2
0
0
0
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
1 1 1
2 1 2
0 0 0
0 0 0
0 0 0
A
c
t
i
v
e
B
i
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
T
o
t
a
l
2
1
3
8
1
0
4
1
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
2
3 9
3
3
4
6
1
6
5
0
1
2 2
1
2
4 1 3
1 0 3
P
a
s
s
i
v
e
B
i
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
T
o
t
a
l
8
9
1
2
1
2
2
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
9 5 7
1
0 3
1
0
1
4 2
1
2
1
5 4 3
2 4 1
E
r
d
a
r
a
M
o
n
o
l
i
n
g
u
a
l
s
T
o
t
a
l
7
0
5
1
7
8
8
3
9
7
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
6
7
8
6
5
9
4
2
8
0
3
8
7
4
9
6
7
6
8
1
9
5
9
4
9
7
9
6
9
6
T
o
t
a
l
/
p
o
l
i
t
i
c
a
l
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
y
:
1
0
0
1
0
0
1
0
0
1
0
0
1
0
0
(
S
o
u
r
c
e
:
S
o
c
i
o
l
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
S
u
r
v
e
y
,
1
9
9
1
;
E
S
-
1
9
9
1
)
.
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
;
E
r
d
a
r
a
=
S
p
a
n
i
s
h
(
i
n
B
A
C
a
n
d
N
A
C
)
o
r
F
r
e
n
c
h
(
i
n
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
)
;
B
A
C
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
N
A
C
=
N
a
v
a
r
r
e
s
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
(
F
r
a
n
c
e
)
;
T
o
t
a
l
/
p
o
l
i
t
i
c
a
l
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
=
t
o
t
a
l
i
n
e
a
c
h
p
o
l
i
t
i
c
a
l
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
y
:
B
A
C
,
C
A
N
o
r
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
The Case of Basque 239
live in relatively high Euskara-speaking sociolinguistic areas (60%
in areas with a proportion of Euskara speakers of more than 45%),
have a basic or high school education (57%), mainly work for some-
body else or are retired (55%) and have a medium to lower-medium
socioprofessional level (61%).
The passive bilinguals are young people between 16 and 24 years of
age (46%), native (63%), speak Spanish as L1 (76%), live mainly in the
BAC (82%) and in the territory of Bizkaia (40%), live in low Euskara-
speaking sociolinguistic areas (54%live in areas with a proportion of
Euskara speakers lower than 20%), have a basic or high school educa-
tion (64%), work for somebody else or are students (68%), and have a
medium-low to medium socioprofessional level (63%).
The monolingual Erdaldunes (non-Basque speakers: Spanish- or
French-speaking) are over 35 (64%), are both immigrants (40%) and
natives (39%), speak Spanish as their L1 (95%), also live in the BAC
(71%) and in the territory of Bizkaia (44%), live in relatively low
Euskara-speaking sociolinguistic areas (79% in areas with a propor-
tion of Euskara speakers lower than 20%), have basic education
(46%), work for somebody else or are housewives (60%), and have a
medium-low or low socioprofessional level (58%).
The results of ES-1996 would seem to indicate a relatively similar situa-
tion, although the overall tone is somewhat more favourable towards
Euskara.
One of the limitations of both ES-1991 and ES-1996 lies in the fact that
they only take into account the over16s. Therefore, they do not reflect the
most important outcome of the Euskara recovery process, the result of the
educational system efforts in the BAC and the NAC, as demonstrated by
the Censuses of 1981 and 1991.
The intergenerational transmission of Euskara as an L1
As proposed in the GIDS model, a language can be considered to be
undergoing a clear process of recovery when it succeeds in being infor-
mally transmitted as an L1; that is, through the childs earliest and most
immediate home-family-neighbourhood socialisation process. For this
reason in ES-1991 and ES-1996, which have tried to apply the Fishman
model, territoriality has been stressed for the purpose of studying the
impact of the linguistic policies applied in the different Basque political
territories. The mother tongue or first language (L1) has also been taken
into account as well as the sociodemographic characteristics of people
who have started speaking Euskara, and those who have stopped.
240 Europe
One of the most positive changes seenover the last twodecades has been
the reversal in the process of the loss of Euskara transmission as an L1
within the family. This has particularly been the case in the BAC, and has
also occurred in the NAC, although not in Iparralde. According to ES-
1991 this process reversal was seen in the 16 and 24-year-old group in the
BAC, meaning that it precedes the effect of the linguistic policy carried
out since the 1980s. In the NACthe reversal was seen in 315-year-olds as
a result, or at least partlyso, of the linguistic policydevelopedthere (Table
10.3).
In terms of the transmission of Euskara as an L1, Table 10.3 shows huge
differences between the BACand the NAC, on one hand, and Iparralde on
the other. In the BAC and the NAC, with two Euskara-speaking parents
Euskara is generally transmitted to children as an L1, this being particu-
larly pronounced in the 1980s. In addition, Euskara is usually the only L1
transmitted. This situation is attributed to the impact of the linguistic
policies in both Basque political territories, owing mainly to the increase
in power and control of Basque institutions (Azurmendi, 1998) and to the
priority of the educational systems in transmitting Euskara (Zabaleta,
1996). In Iparralde, the loss of this transmission is continual, as a conse-
quence, at least in part, of the lack of a Euskara-fomenting linguistic
policy.
ES-1996 results showthe prevalent characteristics of the three linguistic
groups depending on their L1:
People whose L1 is Euskara are generally Basque Country natives,
who have two Euskara-speaking parents; their family and social
environment is largelyBasque-speaking; theyare highlyinterestedin
Euskara and are in favour of its promotion. To a lesser degree, they
live in towns of less than 25,000 inhabitants and have an average age
slightly higher than the population as a whole
People who have two L1s, Euskara and Spanish, are Basque Country
natives, only half of whomhave two Euskara-speaking parents; their
family and social environment is only partially Basque-speaking;
they are interested in Euskara and are in favour of its promotion.
Their average age is slightly lower than the population as a whole
and, due tobeingyounger, have a muchhigher educational level than
the average population.
People whose L1 is Spanish(78%of the populationof the total Basque
Country) are the sons and daughters of non-Euskara-speaking
parents, one-third of whomare immigrants and nearly another third
the children of immigrants; their family and social environment is
The Case of Basque 241
T
a
b
l
e
1
0
.
3
T
r
a
n
s
m
i
s
s
i
o
n
o
f
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
/
o
r
E
r
d
a
r
a
,
a
s
f
i
r
s
t
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
(
L
1
)
,
i
n
1
9
9
6
(
%
)
(
R
e
a
d
v
e
r
t
i
c
a
l
l
y
)
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
L
1
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
E
r
d
a
r
a
L
1
E
r
d
a
r
a
L
1
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
A
g
e
:
2
5
3
4
1
6
2
4
3
1
5
1
6
1
7
2
2
6 6 8
1
6
1
1 6
5 5 8
2 1 4
7
1
0
1
4
7
9
7
8
7
0
9
2
9
3
8
8
7
7
7
9
8
0
C
o
m
p
e
t
.
E
u
s
k
.
P
a
r
e
n
t
s
:
B
o
t
h
E
u
s
k
.
-
s
p
e
a
k
i
n
g
O
n
e
E
u
s
k
.
-
s
p
e
a
k
i
n
g
8
3
1
9
6
5
1
5
7
2
1
1
8
1
8
7
1
4
1
1
1
0
9
6
3
2
8
7
2
1
7
7
9
C
o
m
p
e
t
.
E
u
s
k
.
P
a
r
e
n
t
s
w
i
t
h
c
h
i
l
d
r
e
n
3
1
5
:
B
o
t
h
E
u
s
k
.
-
s
p
e
a
k
i
n
g
O
n
e
E
u
s
k
.
-
s
p
e
a
k
i
n
g
9
4
5
0
9
3
4
4
3
5 5
6
3
8
5
3
7
4
1
3
9
0
1
2
2
2
0
2
4
5
6
(
S
o
u
r
c
e
:
S
o
c
i
o
l
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
S
u
r
v
e
y
,
1
9
9
6
;
E
S
-
1
9
9
6
)
.
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
;
E
r
d
a
r
a
=
S
p
a
n
i
s
h
(
i
n
B
A
C
a
n
d
N
A
C
)
o
r
F
r
e
n
c
h
(
i
n
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
)
;
L
1
=
f
i
r
s
t
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
;
C
o
m
p
e
t
.
E
u
s
k
.
=
C
o
m
p
e
t
e
n
t
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
;
E
u
s
k
.
-
s
p
.
=
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
-
s
p
e
a
k
i
n
g
;
B
A
C
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
N
A
C
=
N
a
v
a
r
r
e
s
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
(
F
r
a
n
c
e
)
242 Europe
largely Spanish-speaking; most of them live in towns of over 25,000
inhabitants, anda quarter are against promotingthe use of Euskara.
The intergenerational transmission of Euskara as an L2
The greatest efforts made in favour of Euskara have always been chan-
nelled through the educational system. One of the most interesting effects
was the creationof ikastolas (schools inEuskara for pre-school, primaryand
secondary education) in the 1960s. These schools became the most impor-
tant transmitters of Euskara as an L2. The other public school programmes
which were developed during the early 1980s in the BAC and the NAC
strengthened the tendency, leading to the possibility of different linguistic
models of teaching in these phases:
Model D: material taught in Euskara, with Spanish as an additional
subject, following the experience of the ikastolas.
Model B: bilingual.
Model A: taught in Spanish, with Euskara as a subject.
Model X, whichuses Spanishas the sole teaching language, was typical of
the pre-autonomy political era. In Iparralde, along with the voluntarily
instituted, Model D-type ikastolas, there have been signs of doors being
opened in the 1990s to the possibility of introducing Models A and B into
the public schools.
From the point of view of the transmission of Euskara, the most signifi-
cant fact is the tendency towards pro-Euskara linguistic models, mainly in
the BACat the pre-school and primary school levels. In the 199091 school
year each of the three linguistic models accounted for a third of the chil-
dren. In the 199899 school year 56% of students studied under Model D,
31% under Model B and only 13% under Model A. In the same territory,
data for the 199899 school year indicates 37% of secondary students, 70%
of primary school students and87%of pre-school students studying under
Models Dand B (EUSTAT, 1998). The same trend, although weaker, can be
seen in the NACand even in Iparralde. Table 10.4 (Etxeberria, 1999) shows
the present situation of the linguistic models.
One of the changes in the 1990s has been the incorporation of many of
the ikastolas into the public school system. This change has had positive
aspects, such as an increase of Euskara and Basque culture in the public
schools. However, it has also had negative aspects such as the presence of
pro-Euskaldun intragroup conflict between some of the more radical social
groups and the more moderate government of the BAC. This conflict
remains partially unresolved today.
The Case of Basque 243
At the university level there has also been an increase in courses being
taught in Euskara, especially at public universities. In the BAC, there has
been a continual 2%annual increase. In the 199899 school year 33%of the
nearly 65,000 students at the University of the Basque Country (UPV/
EHU) studies totally or partially in Euskara. The II Plan for Normalisation
of the Use of Euskara in the UPV/EHU (1999), which will be set in motion
during the 19992000 school year, will increase the use of Euskara at all
levels: teaching, lecturers, university materials, and so on. This tendency is
still fairly weak at the Public University of Navarre in the NAC. The rise in
the presence of Euskara at this level has served to increase its status and
prestige and also to impel the modernisation of the corpus of Euskara and
its adaptation to the world of science and technology.
At the university level, we must also mention the Udal Euskal
Unibertsitatea (UEU, or Basque Summer University), which got underway
in the mid-1960s thanks to voluntary initiative, and which was the first
university experience totally in Euskara.
Basquisation and Euskara literacy in adults (aged 16 upwards)
This has been the most important goal after pre-university educational
Basquisation, and has required the most amount of effort in terms of the
linguistic policies applied in the BACand the NAC. Parallel to the ikastolas
244 Europe
Table 10.4 Schooling in the different linguistic models, in preschool, in the
199798 school year (%)
BAC NAC Iparralde
Model D (in Euskara + Erdara subject) 55 25 5
Model B (Bilingual) 28 10
Model A (in Erdara + Euskara subject) 16 25 7
Model X (in Erdara) 1 50 78
100 100 100
(Source: Etxeberria, 1999).
Euskara = Basque language; Erdara = Spanish (in BAC and NAC) or French (in Iparralde);
BAC = Basque Autonomous Community (Spain); NAC = Navarrese Autonomous Commu-
nity (Spain); Iparralde = Basque territories (France); Model D = Linguistic model in which
schooling is done in Euskara with Erdara as a subject: preschool, primary and secondary
stages; Model B=Linguistic model with schooling in Euskara andErdara: preschool, primary
andsecondarystages; Model A=Linguistic model withschoolinginErdara, withEuskara as a
subject: preschool, primary andsecondary stages; Model X=Linguistic model with schooling
in Erdara: preschool, primary and secondary stages
andthe UEU, Euskaltegis (centres specifically createdto foster Basquisation
and teach Euskara literacy to adults) first arose in the mid-1960s. They are
centres for transmitting Euskara as a second language (L2) to adults. There
is a growing number in the BAC, whereas in other Basque territories there
are relatively few. In the BAC, the number of students in these centres has
grown from 41,000 in the 199192 school year to almost 45,000 in 199596.
(HABE, 1998; Arratibel, in press).
The prevailing sociodemographic characteristics of Euskaltegi students
for the 199596 school year show that they are between 21 and 30 years of
age (44.2%); are mostly women (63.9%); live in Gipuzkoa (the most
Euskara-speaking territory) (55.7%); are university students (31.3%) or
unemployed workers (25.2%); have a high school (36.7%) or university
education (undergraduates, 22.5%); and are students of private offi-
cially approvedEuskaltegis (whichare therefore officially controlledand
subsidised) (54%) (Arratibel, in press). Furthermore, most students
starting the courses follow through to the end (83%), signing up each year
until they reach the level of competence required for that level (Arratibel,
1999).
The Use of Euskara and its transmission
In the above sections we have described the situation as to the knowl-
edge of Euskara. This is important, since without it, using the language
would be impossible. But what is really important is to describe use per se,
that is, the extent to whicha personis capable of living inEuskara. This was
obvious right from the start for the planners of Euskara recovery, both via
official institutions with their linguistic policies, and via voluntary initia-
tives, especially in the BAC. The BAC Basic Law for the Normalisation of
the Use of Euskara (1982) explicitly refers to the normalisation of the use
of Euskara. This is why in this analysis we must highlight use rather than
knowledge alone (Table 10.5).
The results of Table 10.5 (ES-1996) show that Euskara speakers use
Euskara to a larger extent in some environments than in others. It is impor-
tant to remember that language-use norms in the Basque Country tend to
linguistically accommodate non-Euskara speakers, meaning that if there is
only one person who does not speak Euskara, the rest will switch to
Spanish or French. As a result of this social norm, the use of Euskara in the
different environments corresponds to the level of knowledge in the
different environments: Euskara is better known where it is more often
used. In addition, Euskara is proportionally used to a greater extent with
children: parents with their children and teachers with their students,
The Case of Basque 245
T
a
b
l
e
1
0
.
5
U
s
e
o
f
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
E
r
d
a
r
a
i
n
d
i
f
f
e
r
e
n
t
s
o
c
i
a
l
e
n
v
i
r
o
n
m
e
n
t
s
(
o
n
l
y
f
o
r
s
p
e
a
k
e
r
s
o
f
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
)
,
b
y
r
e
g
i
o
n
s
,
i
n
1
9
9
6
(
%
)
(
r
e
a
d
h
o
r
i
z
o
n
t
a
l
l
y
)
B
A
C
N
A
C
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
I
n
b
o
t
h
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
E
r
d
a
r
a
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
r
d
a
r
a
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
I
n
b
o
t
h
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
E
r
d
a
r
a
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
r
d
a
r
a
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
I
n
b
o
t
h
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
a
n
d
E
r
d
a
r
a
P
r
i
n
c
i
p
a
l
l
y
i
n
E
r
d
a
r
a
F
A
M
I
L
Y
W
I
T
H
:
M
o
t
h
e
r
F
a
t
h
e
r
H
u
s
b
a
n
d
/
w
i
f
e
C
h
i
l
d
r
e
n
A
T
H
O
M
E
5
6
5
3
5
1
7
3
4
8
7 6
1
1
1
2
1
8
3
7
4
2
3
9
1
5
3
4
5
2
4
7
4
8
6
5
4
6
6 6
1
0
1
1
1
6
4
2
4
7
4
2
2
4
3
8
5
3
5
6
4
5
3
7
3
5
2
9
2
2
1
2
1
6
2
2
1
8
2
2
4
3
4
8
4
4
C
O
M
M
U
N
I
T
Y
W
I
T
H
:
F
r
i
e
n
d
s
M
e
r
c
h
a
n
t
s
A
t
w
o
r
k
M
a
r
k
e
t
P
r
i
e
s
t
4
9
4
8
4
5
7
8
7
4
2
0
1
7
1
8 7
1
0
3
0
3
6
3
7 7
1
0
5
1
3
9
5
1
6
8
6
7
1
6
1
0
1
1 6 4
3
3
5
1
3
8
2
6
2
9
4
4
2
1
3
2
7
3
6
4
1
9
1
5
1
5
1
4
1
5
3
7
6
4
5
4
1
4
2
1
S
O
C
I
E
T
Y
I
N
:
B
a
n
k
s
T
o
w
n
H
a
l
l
C
h
i
l
d
s
t
e
a
c
h
e
r
H
e
a
l
t
h
S
e
r
v
i
c
e
s
5
6
5
9
8
5
3
3
1
4
1
5 7
1
5
3
0
2
5 8
5
2
4
3
4
9
7
6
3
4
8 7 6
1
0
5
0
4
3
1
8
5
6
1
9
3
1
2
1
9
1
3
1
4
1
1
1
2
6
7
5
4
6
8
7
9
(
S
o
u
r
c
e
:
S
o
c
i
o
l
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
S
u
r
v
e
y
,
1
9
9
6
;
E
S
-
1
9
9
6
)
E
u
s
k
a
r
a
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
;
E
r
d
a
r
a
=
S
p
a
n
i
s
h
(
i
n
B
A
C
a
n
d
N
A
C
)
o
r
F
r
e
n
c
h
(
i
n
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
)
;
B
A
C
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
N
A
C
=
N
a
v
a
r
r
e
s
e
A
u
t
o
n
o
m
o
u
s
C
o
m
m
u
n
i
t
y
(
S
p
a
i
n
)
;
I
p
a
r
r
a
l
d
e
=
B
a
s
q
u
e
t
e
r
r
i
t
o
r
i
e
s
(
F
r
a
n
c
e
)
246 Europe
reflecting an awareness of the importance of the intergenerational trans-
mission of Euskara.
Other results of ES-1996 underline the importance of the factors
explaining the use of Euskara:
Sociostructural and sociodemographic factors are the most signifi-
cant, the most noteworthy of which are the density of Euskara
speakers, in both sociolinguistic areas and spheres, and each individ-
uals social network.
Psycholinguistic factors are secondinimportance. The relative higher
or lower linguistic competence in Euskara as compared to Spanish or
French also determines the amount of Euskara used.
Psychosocial factors (i.e. attitude or identity) are in third place.
These results appear to be consistent with others coming from different
psychosociolinguistic research projects (SIADECO, 1991; Arratibel,
1999; Garca, 2000). These results are important for Euskara recovery
planners.
The Psychosocial Situation of Euskara
It is useful to understand the social situation of Euskara, also from a
psychosocial perspective. This is a more complex task owing to the amount
and heterogeneity of the interpretations that exist today concerning
Euskara. But this psychosocial perspective seems to be important in the
case of Basque. Among other reasons is the fact that linguistic policies and
planning attach more importance to the person than to the group, and are
based on personal choice and will (Azurmendi, 1997a).
ES-1991 andES-1996 lookedat aspects such as interest, attitude, motiva-
tion and identity with respect to Euskara. Some of the results concerning
attitudes are shown in Table 10.6.
These results showthat attitudes are predominantly pro-Euskara in the
BACand Iparralde, while unfavourable in the NAC. However, favourable
attitudes are on the rise in the NACand in Iparralde, while the tendency is
towards indifference (neither favourable nor unfavourable) in the BAC. In
terms of the transmission of Euskara, it shouldbe pointedout that the most
favourable attitudes revolve around the idea that it is essential for all chil-
dren to learn Euskara.
The prevailing sociodemographic characteristics of the three attitudinal
types obtained in ES-1996 are as follows:
Nearly half of the people with pro-Euskara attitudes are Euskara-
The Case of Basque 247
speaking; they prefer having their children go to school under B
(bilingual) or D (Euskara) linguistic models; a third of them have
studied Euskara outside of school; most are Basque Country natives;
and half have a predominantly Euskaldun family and social environ-
ment.
People with unfavourable attitudes are mostly Erdaldunes (non-
Euskara-speaking), with a predominantly Erdaldun family and social
environment; they mainly live in urban areas; their interest in
Euskara is scarce or non-existent; they generally intend to send their
children to B or D linguistic teaching models; and more than half are
immigrants or the children of immigrants.
The sociolinguistic characteristics of people with neither favourable
nor unfavourable attitudes are fairly similar to the unfavourable-atti-
tude group, with the difference that their interest in Euskara is
greater.
Reversing Basque Shift, in Eight Stages
The following section will attempt to interpret the data presented in the
previous section, as well as other Euskara-related data, according to the
GIDS model in eight stages and to RLS (Fishman, 1991). Several questions
are emphasised in this model, including the importance of linguistic plan-
ning and of clarification of pre-existing ideology, the gradual nature of
linguistic loss and recovery processes, the centrality of intergenerational
248 Europe
Table 10.6 Attitudes promoting Euskara (Basque language), by political
territories, in 1991 and 1996 (%)
1991 1996
BAC NAC Iparralde Total BAC NAC Iparralde Total
(Very)
favorable
52 23 45 47 46 38 49 45
Neither favourable
nor disfavourable
30 24 41 30 38 30 39 37
(Very)
unfavorable
19 54 14 24 16 32 13 18
100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
(Source: Sociolinguistic Survey, 1991 and 1996; ES-1991, ES-1996).
BAC = Basque Autonomous Community (Spain); NAC = Navarrese Autonomous Commu-
nity (Spain); Iparralde = Basque territories (France)
transmissionandthe importance of voluntaryinitiatives alongwithofficial
initiatives (government control and backing).
The ideology accompanying Euskara recovery
Explicit ideologyhas always accompaniedthe Euskara recoveryprocess
and becomes even more crucial as the process moves forward, as has been
seen in the 1990s. With regard to the ideology coming from inside the
Basque Country, different aspects canbe seeninterms of previous ideolog-
ical clarification and later ideological evolution. Two ideological macro-
types can also be seen: favourable, or pro-Euskaldun and unfavourable, or
anti-Euskaldun. Three sources can also be distinguished: voluntary initia-
tive, official initiative (parliament, government andjudicial) andsemi-offi-
cial initiative (political parties, labour unions, the Catholic Church, and so
on). The two predominant issues are the final goals to be attained and the
procedures to be used. The degree of homogeneity, versatility and
dynamics is substantially greater in anti-Euskaldun than in the pro-
Euskaldun ideology (Azurmendi, 1997b). Here, because of its repercus-
sions on the ideology produced inside the Basque Country, we should also
mention the generally anti-Euskaldun ideology coming from voluntary,
official and semi-official initiatives originating outside the Basque
Country. This leads us toconsider ideologyas bothintra- andintergroup.
Previous ideological clarification, on the official level, was set down in
the Spanish Constitution of 1978 and in the Autonomy Statutes of the BAC
(1979) and the NAC (1980), although nothing similar exists in Iparralde.
This clarification established Spanish as the only official language for the
whole of Spain, and therefore the need for everyone to know it and use it,
while the other languages within Spain could also be maintained and
protected. It also states that the Autonomous Communities having their
own languages (such as Euskara in the BACand the NAC) may recognise
themas official languages, drawuplegislationandestablishlinguistic poli-
cies for their protection and promotion. Another very different matter is
the ideology accompanying the Euskara recovery process through the
1980s and 1990s.
The ideology seen in the last 20 years indicates a very heterogeneous
situation, but couldbe dividedintothree more polarisedgroups, especially
in the BAC (Azurmendi, 1997b):
The most radically pro-Euskaldun ideology wants to see society-
wide Euskara monolingualism throughout the Basque Country, and
generalised individual multilingualism with regard to other
languages (Spanish and/or French, English and others), plus the
The Case of Basque 249
transmission of Euskara as L1. As far as the procedures to follow, it
believes that using Euskara must be a necessity and an obligation,
applied mainly to the younger population. This ideology stems from
certain voluntary and semi-official initiatives frominside the Basque
Country.
The most radically anti-Euskaldun ideology wants to see society-
wide Spanish monolingualismin the BACand the NAC, or French in
Iparralde, although it does make a few concessions to bilingualism.
At the same time, it favours generalised individual multilingualism
with regard to other languages (English, French/Spanish, etc., or
Euskara). For this reason, the transmission of Spanish/French as L1
must be ensured by maintaining the compulsory nature of knowing
and using Spanish/French. This ideology comes from some volun-
tary and semi-official initiatives, from inside the Basque Country,
and also from all types of initiatives from outside the Basque
Country.
The symmetry ideology wants to see a balance between pro-
Euskaldun and pro-Spanish/French. The idea is to overcome the
ongoing conflict between languages and linguistic groups. The goals
are symmetrical societal bilingualism in Euskara and Spanish/
French, and generalised individual multilingualism (with regard to
these two languages, English and others). It strives for both Euskara
and Spanish/French to be transmitted as L1. The procedures to be
followed involve ensuring the necessity and obligation of both
languages, mainly by BACautonomous official initiative (parliamen-
tary and governmental), reflected in linguistic policy. This view also
comes from some voluntary and semi-official initiatives from within
the Basque Country.
Stages 8 and 7 on the GIDS (Graded Intergenerational Disruption
Scale)
Stages 8 to 5 on the GIDS are the minimum needed to guarantee the
natural intergenerational transmission of a language and ensure the RLS
process. They are also the stages of diglossia consolidation, guaranteeing
the use of Euskara at least in the spheres most closely related to people: the
home-family-neighbourhood-community.
Stage 8 proposes reconstructing Xish (Euskara) and adult acquisition of
XSL (Euskara as L2). And Stage 7 proposes the cultural interaction in Xish
primarily involving the community based older generation. The most
significant difference betweenthese two stages lies inthe intergenerational
250 Europe
transmission of the language. Goals are of two kinds. From the point of
view of corpus planning of Euskara: reunification, standardisation and
modernisationof Euskara, andat the same time recoveryandrevitalisation
of cultural tradition in Euskara. And fromthe point of viewof status plan-
ning: reunification, recovery and revitalisation of the use and users of
Euskara. It can be said that these two stages are being met successfully,
particularly in the BAC. The situation pointed out by Fishman in 1991 (pp.
15861) has improved in the 1990s. Beloware some examples of this, refer-
ring particularly to the BAC.
Interms of the corpus of Euskara, inthe 1990s, aspects suchas recovery
and authentification were emphasised through an historic dictionary.
Modernisation was also underlined via specific dictionaries for different
areas (media, administration, commerce, finance, etc.), and by means of
specific dictionaries for the different fields of knowledge (science andtech-
nology). In addition to the semi-official institutions already in existence
(Euskaltzaindia, UZEI, ELHUYAR, etc.), newones have been created, such
as the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU) Institute of Euskara,
in 1998. Concerns for the quality and use of Euskara have mounted,
bringing about the need for new research.
Withregardtothe status of Euskara, initiatives alreadyinexistence have
been stepped up. Publications in Euskara, although still predominantly
literary or educational, amount to over 1000 books per year (according to
the Annual Basque Book, Record and Video Fair of Durango, in the BAC).
Literature is attracting more readers and therefore has become less
dependent on government subsidies. In fact, Basque literature is gaining
international prestige and acclaim, with works often being translated into
other languages. New, mostly monthly, bulletins, are being put out either
in Euskara or bilingually, such as the Council on Linguistic Policys
Berripapera, the UPV/EHUs Campus, Eusko Ikaskuntzas Asmoz ta Jakitez,
the Donostia-San Sebastin Town Councils Arkupe, etc. In terms of daily
newspapers, the Egunkaria is written totally in Euskara and Euskara is
being increasingly used in the remaining papers, particularly in the new
paper, Gara. Radio and TVaudiences in Euskara have also risen. All of this
has added to the status of Euskara, both inside and outside the Basque
Country.
In sum, it can be said that these efforts are being maintained and
augmentedinthe BAC, have beeninitiatedinthe NAC, andare still intheir
initial stages in Iparralde. All of these initiatives encourage the use of
Euskara in community environments, and therefore act as a bridge
between these stages and Stage 6 on the GIDS.
The Case of Basque 251
Stage 6 on the GIDS
This is the stage concerned with intergenerational oralcy and demo-
graphic concentration in the home-family-neighbourhood-community:
the basis of mother-tongue transmission. The home-family-neighbour-
hood-community environment can be viewed apart from the school envi-
ronment. All of these spheres have undergone a certain amount of change
in the 1990s, particularly in the BAC. One of these changes concerns the
mounting interdependence between all of these spheres of daily life and
linguistic behaviour.
In the school system, two new experiences should be highlighted. The
first, not compulsory but commonplace, is that of children beginning pre-
school at the age of two in Euskara. The second is the teaching of English at
the age of four, still within the pre-school stage. Both of these are societal
initiatives which originates in 1995 as experimental projects in the ikastolas.
Their maingoals were tofacilitate the transmissionof Euskara, almost as an
L1, and to acquire English as an L2, in an attempt to reach maximumlevels
of linguistic and communicative competence. The idea was to ensure the
intergenerational transmission of Euskara as an L1, in addition to attaining
generalisedindividual multilingualism. After a positive evaluationof both
experiences, these projects will be set into motion on a broader scale as of
the 19992000 school year, in the rest of the ikastolas, as well as in the public
school system; hence they will reach virtually all children.
These innovations inthe area of educationwill boost the current trendof
interdependence between education and the home-neighbourhood-
community sphere, with respect to the intergenerational transmission of
Euskara. Families consist less andless of captive audiences, andshare more
and more with other areas in the initial socialisation of children. There are
increasingly more curricular and extra-curricular activities, especially in
pre-school (2-to-5-year-olds) andprimary school (6-to-12-year-olds). All of
this encourages multiple parent-children, parent-teacher, parent-parent
and childrenchildren relationships. The general population is aware of
the importance of early, generalised Basquisation. Therefore, we can
predict success in these joint initiatives, at least in the BAC. All of these
process are not yet available in the NAC and are unheard of in Iparralde.
As far as teenagers and children are concerned, awareness in fostering
the use of Euskara has also grown. One example of this is the increase in
activities in Euskara organised for young people and adults by the govern-
ment-sponsored casas de cultura, or cultural centres. The 1990s have seen
these cultural centres being opened in virtually every city neighbourhood,
and in small municipalities, for the purpose of organising cultural, sports
252 Europe
and leisure activities. But these initiatives have been both a blessing and a
curse to daily life in Euskara and to intergenerational transmission. In fact,
so far they have been more effective in ensuring the generalised transmis-
sion of Spanish than of Euskara.
There are other aspects introduced by Fishman (1991) in Stage 6, related
to daily life and the immediate environment, such as linguistic landscape:
place-names and anthroponymy, street and road signs, shop and building
names, advertising, and so on. These aspects of the recovery process of
Euskara have not yet seen much progress.
To summarise, this stage is clearly being focused upon in the BAC,
although not to the same extent in all spheres. In the NAC, its implementa-
tion is still weak. In Iparralde, sadly enough, Euskara continues to decline,
except interms of linguistic landscape, where Euskara has made its biggest
strides.
Stage 5 on the GIDS
This is the stage of school for literacy acquisition, for the old and for the
young, and not in lieu of compulsory education by focusing on intragroup
resources and processes with respect to its acquisition, its content and its
control. Therefore, these goals are, to a certain extent, the same as the goals
in previous stages. In terms of these goals, too, the situation of Euskara has
improved in the 1990s.
One of the problems detectedinthe 1990s researchdeals withthe level of
competence in Euskara. The increase in Basque-speakers is one thing, but
the increase in competent Basque-speakers is another altogether (Tables
10.1 and 10.2). This is important because it is directly related to the use of
Euskara. Recalling information offered previously, the results of ES-1991
and ES-1996 show that Euskara is used when competence in Euskara is
equal to or greater than that of competence in Erdara (Spanish or French).
But ES-1996 alsoshows that out of 21%of the active bilinguals inthe Basque
Country (Table 10.1), 32% are balanced bilinguals and 30% are bilinguals
with predominance in Euskara, while 38% are bilinguals with predomi-
nance in Erdara.
Etxeberria (1999), after analysing the 39 studies done on pre-university
educationinthe BAC, comes tosome alarmingconclusions. Onlylinguistic
Model Dguarantees a good enough level of competence in primary educa-
tiontobe able togoonstudyingunder Model Dinsecondaryschool. Model
B ensures quite a good level of competence in primary education, but not
good enough to go on to Model Din secondary school. Following Model A
in primary school, students barely reach a rudimentary colloquial level of
Euskara and are generally incapable of using Euskara as a work tool in the
The Case of Basque 253
classroom (Etxeberria, 1999: 218). However, none of these linguistic
models influence the acquisition of Spanish, whose high level of compe-
tency is always guaranteed. For this reason, after 15 years of experience in
linguistic educational models, there has been a need to revise and change
educational planning. The innovationof beginning preschool inEuskara at
the age of two and teaching English at four can be seen as one way of
responding to this need.
In the process of adult Basquisation a similar problem was detected.
Adults acquire a higher level of linguistic competence in written skills
(reading and writing) than in oral skills (understanding and speaking)
(Arratibel, 1999), typical in the formal acquisition of foreign L2 languages.
This means that learners are not taking full advantage of the close prox-
imity of Euskara, and hence, are not using informal language acquisition
processes.
In short, the achievements in Stage 5 are not very favourable as far as
natural intergenerational Euskara transmission is concerned. In addition,
Stage 5 on the GIDS is being carried out currently only in the BAC, is just
getting underway in the NAC and barely exists in Iparralde.
Stage 4 on the GIDS
Stage 4 is the beginning of the secondphase of the RLS process. The goal
in this stage is also related to the educational system. Fishman points out
two types of schooling: Type 4a consists of schooling in lieu of compulsory
education and substantially under Xish (Euskara) curricular and staffing
control. Whereas Type 4b, offers public schools for Xish children,
providing some instruction via Xish, but substantially under Yish
(Spanish) curricular and staffing control. In Type 4a the task lies in making
sure that the efforts andachievements of Basque schools, the ikastolas inthis
case, spread into extracurricular activities, so that intergenerational trans-
mission can take place. In Type 4b, the difficulty lies in the fact that the
Euskaldunlinguistic groupneeds the approval of the other linguistic group,
which continues to be the ultimate decision-maker in all important educa-
tional aspects. These goals partially overlap with the goals of previous
stages on the GIDS.
The situation described by Fishman in the 1980s with regard to Euskara
(1991: 16773) has improved in the 1990s. The goals for Type 4a are being
implemented as are the 4b goals, although the latter are always controver-
sial. For this reason, the difficulties describedstill exist. This is the situation
inthe BAC. Inthe other Basque territories, the Euskara recovery process, in
terms of these goals, is still weak and uncertain.
254 Europe
Stage 3 on the GIDS
Stage 3 pursues the use of Euskara in the lower work sphere (outside of
the Basque neighbourhood-community), involving interaction between
the Basque group and the Spanish group. Fishman distinguishes two
different situations. First, there are the companies controlled by the
Euskaldun group, but revolving around the needs of the Erdaldun group, in
which internal communication can be carried out in Euskara. Then there
are the companies controlled by the Erdaldun group, serving the needs of
the Euskaldun group; the Euskaldun group can always fall back on the
customer is always right principle to get the conversation to switch to
Euskara. Fishmans differentiation is important in terms of analysis, but in
the case of Basque is difficult to use empirically due to the predominance of
more varied situations.
The situation of Euskara described by Fishman (1991: 173) has
improved in the 1990s. The first experiences planned for Basquisation
and/or to boost the use of Euskara as a work-sphere language in private
industry, began in the 1990s, once again in the BAC. The Qubec model of
self-managed linguistic planning was followed. The first experiences are
being carried out in high-density Basque-speaking sociolinguistic areas
so as to facilitate success. The companies are relatively large and impor-
tant so as to be able to set an example for other companies. Some of the
companies selected belong to the Mondragn Grupo, a truly substantial
corporate group in the BAC.
The goals in this stage involve difficulties of all types: inertia, pragmatic
issues, social rules regarding the choice of which language to use, and so
on. For example, from the ideological point of view, the consideration of
Euskara as a working language is still a matter of debate. The Euskaldun
linguistic group demands this, while the Erdaldun linguistic group either
rejects or simply tolerates it, with the excuse that it couldfoster discrimina-
tion and intergroup conflict.
This stage is still in the initial phase. The General Plan for the
Revitalisationof Euskara (EuskoJaurlaritza/GobiernoVasco, 1998), for the
BAC, and the General Plan for the Normalisation of Euskara (Euskararen
Gizarte Erakundeen Kontseilua, 1999), for the Basque Country, include
plans for incorporating Basque into the workplace.
Stage 2 on the GIDS
Stage 2 involves the implementation of Euskara in lower governmental
services and mass media but not in the higher spheres of either. This is an
interesting stage, because it means taking part in the world of politics, of
The Case of Basque 255
institutional support and control, and participating in making decisions
about and having authority over the distribution of available public
resources. But it is also a difficult stage because it is basically a philosoph-
ical/ideological position and, as such, it requires constant philosophical/
ideological restatement and updating if it is to remain effective.
The situation describedby Fishman (1991: 17475) has also improved, at
least in the BAC, with a significant presence of pro-Euskaldun political
parties. However, this is not the case in the NAC, where there is a majority
of anti-Euskaldun political parties. This process has not even gotten
underway in Iparralde. However, the advances attained have also given
rise to an increase in polarised ideologies. In the pro-Euskaldun group, the
conflict is between the more radical voluntarily initiated positions and the
more moderate, coming from both voluntary and official initiatives. The
recent 1999 agreement signed jointly by the BACs Council on Linguistic
Policy and the voluntarily-initiated Euskararen Gizarte Eradundeen
Kontseilua, can be seen as a giant step toward internal social consensus.
The anti-Euskaldun groupwants to remainthe dominant groupandassimi-
late the Euskaldun group; therefore any advance in the recovery process of
Euskara is viewed as contrary to their aims
Stage 1 on the GIDS
Stage 1 seeks some use of Euskara in higher level educational, occupa-
tional, governmental and media efforts, but without the additional safety
provided by political independence. This is the final stage of the GIDS
model, andrepresents the highest level of cultural autonomy (but not inde-
pendence), for Euskara and the Euskaldun group. Once more we would
have to say that there has been progress, especially in the BAC. But this
does not necessarilymeanthe endof RLS problems, neither fromthe stand-
point of the pro-Euskaldun group, which may ultimately aspire to be the
only official language and culture in the Basque Country, nor from the
point of view of the anti-Euskaldun group, which may not want to relin-
quish Spanish linguistic and cultural dominance over Euskara.
Conclusion
Summing up, the following conclusions can be reached from this
presentation:
(1) The process of recovery of Euskara has moved forward in the 1990s,
although it has not been equal throughout all of the Basque political
territories. Although some new initiatives have been started, most
advances were based on initiatives already developed in the previous
256 Europe
decade. The most important results have more to do with the acquisi-
tionof Euskara thanwithits use, mainlydue toeducation(pre-univer-
sityanduniversity), andalsothroughthe Basquisationof adults. From
the point of viewof the GIDS model, there has been more quantitative
than qualitative progress.
(2) The GIDS model proposed by Fishman (1991), as a gradual process in
two phases and eight stages, has only been applied in part in the case
of Basque. The process followedfor the recovery andnormalisation of
Euskara has not been entirely linear, but rather spiral, and with some
overlapping. That is, goals corresponding to different stages on the
GIDS model have been pursued simultaneously. The GIDS model has
been followed in terms of establishing priority goals for the recovery
and normalisation of the use of Euskara in the home-family-neigh-
bourhood-community spheres, and the intergenerational transmis-
sion of Euskara as an L1. These goals have been present throughout
the entire Euskara recovery process. There are different reasons for
this, some of whichhave already pointedout inthis paper. But there is
still not enough research on the matter.
(3) The GIDS model places emphasis on intragroup process: Xish-to-
Xmen. But in the case of Basque, the reality is more complex, both in
the make-up of the groups and in the processes, which are both
intragroup and intergroup. With regard to Euskara (Xish), in the
Basque Country there is the Euskaldun (Xmen) group and the
Erdaldun (Ymen) group. But there is also the pro-Euskaldun group
(Xmen+Ymen-via-Xish) and the anti-Euskaldun group (Xmen+Ymen-
via-Yish). Both types of groups are interwoven and partially overlap-
ping. In addition, from outside the Basque Country there is also an
anti-Euskaldun group (Ymen-via-Yish). This is why the relationships
to be considered are so complex. With regard to the recovery of
Euskara, the most pertinent groups are the pro-Euskaldun and the
anti-Euskaldun groups, and not simply the Euskaldun and the
Erdaldun (non-Euskaldun) groups.
(4) The GIDS is predominantly sociological, although concessions are
made to the psychosocial perspective, especially through the impor-
tance placed on identity. In this sense, the experience of the case of
Basque fits well in this model. In spite of this, it seems that the
psychosocial perspective plays a more important role in the case of
Basque, as canbe seeninsome of the researchcarriedout (Azurmendi,
1997a; Azurmendi &Bourhis, 1998; Azurmendi et al., 1998). Linguistic
policies today plan the recovery of Euskara as a right and not an obli-
gation, anindividual right basedonthe principal of the individual and
The Case of Basque 257
not the territory. This may be why the general population thinks that
the will to be Basque is the most important factor in considering
oneself really Basque (more so than being Basque-speaking)
(Azurmendi et al., 1993). This explains the importance of this way of
thinking during the process of recovery of Euskara and why it has
increased as recovery advances.
(5) The enormous differences existing today between the Basque territo-
ries (ES-1991, 1996; Zabaleta, 1996; Bachoc, 1998) allow us to make an
internal, diachronic andsynchronous comparison, whichcanbe inter-
preted from the GIDS point of view. One of the interpretations refers
to the predominance of intergroup relationships and their impact on
intragroup relationships, both in the pro-Euskaldun and the anti-
Euskaldun groups. Another interpretation is the importance of polit-
ical power in developing successful linguistic planning and policy.
Yet another interpretation speaks to the importance of political and
territorial unity in order to draw up a single linguistic policy.
(6) To finish up, the most important conclusion should refer to a general
assessment of the process followedinthe Basque Countryinthe 1990s,
according to the GIDS model. From the standpoint of GIDS predic-
tions, we would have to say that changes have not been very signifi-
cant, in the sense that they have not been effective enough to ensure
the goals in Stage 6. We are looking more at quantitative than qualita-
tive changes. Therefore, the situation of Euskara in the GIDS model
proposed by Fishman in 1991 (p. 405) has changed very little by 1999
as far as the BAC is concerned. The situation in the other territories is
even weaker or still. But the perspective of the GIDS model is effective
in understanding the processes related to Euskara and its evolution.
References
Arratibel, N. (1999) Helduen Euskalduntzean Eragiten Duten Prozesu Psikosozialak:
Motibazioaren Errola. Donostia/San Sebastin: UPV/EHU, Tesis doctoral.
Azurmendi, M.-J. (1997a) Psicologa social y planificacin lingstica. In Actes del
Congrs Europeu sobre Planificaci Lingstica, Barcelona, 1995. Barcelona:
Generalitat de Catalunya.
Azurmendi, M.-J. (1997b) Lengua, identidad y nacionalismo. Curso de Verano de la
Universidad Complutense, 1997: Lenguas y Nacionalismos en Espaa y en Europa. El
Escorial: Universidad Complutense.
Azurmendi, M.-J. (1998) Importancia del soporte institucional para la
normalizacin del uso del Euskara. In Actes de la Cinquena Trobada de
Sociolingistes Catalans, Barcelona, 1997. Barcelona: Generalitat de Catalunya.
Azurmendi, M.-J. and Bourhis, R.Y. (1998) Presentacin del Proyecto de investig-
acin ICYLCABE1996: Identidades culturales y lingsticas en las
258 Europe
Comunidades Autnomas bilinges (CAB) de Espaa. Revista de Psicologa Social
13, 3, 54558.
Azurmendi, M.-J., Bourhis, R.Y., Ros, M. and Garcia, I. (1998) Identidad etno-
lingstica y construccin de ciudadana en las Comunidades Autnomas
bilinges (CAB) de Espaa. Revista de Psicologa Social 13(3), 55989.
Azurmendi, M.-J., Espi, M.J., Garcia, I., Gonzalez, J.L. andMuoz, I. (1993) Lengua e
identidad social en el Pas Vasco. In B. Gonzlez and A. Guil (comps.) Psicologa
Cultural (IV Congreso Nacional de Psicologa Social). Sevilla: Eudema.
Bachoc, E. (1998) LAvenir de la Langue Basque (Euskara) en Pays Basque de France.
Bayonne/Baiona: Institut Culturel Basque/Euskal Kultur Erakundea.
Etxeberria, F. (1999) Bilingismo y Educacin en el Pas Vasco.
Euskararen Gizarte Erakundeen Kontseilua (1999) Euskara Normalizatzeko Plan
Nagusia. Donostia/San Sebastin: Euskararen Kontseilua.
Eusko Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco (1998) Euskara Biziberritzeko Plan Nagusia.
Vitoria/Gasteiz: Eusko Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco.
Eusko Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco and Nafarroako Gobernua/Gobierno de
Navarra (1991) Encuesta Sociolingstica. La Continuidad del Euskara I. (ES-1991).
Vitoria/Gasteiz: Eusko Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco.
Eusko Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco, Gobierno de Navarra/Nafarroako Gobernua
and Institut Culturel Basque/Euskal Kultur Erakundea (1996). Encuesta
Sociolingstica. La Continuidad del Euskara II. (ES-1996). Vitoria/Gasteiz: Eusko
Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco.
EUSTAT (1998) Anuario Estadstico Vasco. Vitoria/Gasteiz: EUSTAT.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Theoretical and Empirical Foundations of
Assistance to Threatened Languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Garcia, I. (2000) Euskararen Erabileran Eragiten Duten Brozesin Psikosozialak: Identitate
Etnolinguistikoaren Gorratzia. Donostia/San Sebastin: UPV/EHU, Tesis doctoral.
HABE (1998) Statistical data. Donostia/San Sebastin: HABE.
Ley Bsica de Normalizacin del Uso del Euskara, 1982. Vitoria/Gasteiz: Eusko
Jaurlaritza/Gobierno Vasco.
Ley Foral del Vascuence, 1986. Pamplona/Iruea: Gobierno de Navarra.
Ruiz de Olabuenaga, J.I. and Blanco, M.C. (1994) La Inmigracin Vasca. Anlisis
Trigeneracional de 150 Aos de Inmigracin. Bilbao: Universidad de Deusto.
SIADECO (1991) Euskararekiko Eritzi eta Jarrerak. (Report).
Universidad del Pas Vasco/Euskal Herriko Unibertsitatea (UPV/EHU) (1999). II
Plan de Normalizacin del Uso del Euskara en la UPV/EHU. Leioa: UPV/EHU.
Zabaleta, F. (1995) El factor educativo en el conocimiento y uso del euskara en
Navarra. In Actes del Simposi de Demolingstica. Barcelona: Generalitat de
Catalunya.
Zabaleta, F. (1996) Nafarroako Mapa Soziolinguistikoa, 1991. Biztanleria Euskaldunaren
Dinamika eta Bilakaera/Mapa Sociolingstico de Navarra, 1991. Dinmica y Evolucin
de la Poblacin Vascfona. Pamplona/Iruea: Gobierno de Navarra.
The Case of Basque 259
Chapter 11
Catalan a Decade Later
M. STRUBELL
1
Introduction
When Professor Fishman discussed the case of Catalan (in Catalonia, as
the state of the language in other areas is far less healthy) in his book
Reversing Language Shift, he classified it at Stage 1
2
on his Graded
Intergenerational Disruption Scale (GIDS). While regretting that an overall
viewof all Catalan-speaking lands cannot be given here, the author of this
chapter will revisit the same area, 10 years on, to judge whether Catalan
remains at Stage 1. There is also a second request by Fishman: to use,
modify, critique or abandonthe RLStheoryproposedinmy1991 book.
Research on Language Shift and its Reversal
General
Let us look first at the second part of Professor Fishmans request: a
critique of the RLS theory.
A number of Fishmans statements ring truer today, at least in the
Catalan context, than they did when RLS was published in 1991. The issue
of a sufficientlyrefinedtaxonomyof language status planningtoexplicitly
provide for the consideration of RLS activity (Fishman, 1991: 381) is one.
A second issue is the apparently endemic neglect of RLS as a research
topic in the social sciences. There are of course notable exceptions. Yet it is
extraordinary that such an easily observed and recorded social phenom-
enon language choice among individuals has not yet become the bench-
mark for theoretical innovation in these fields. Fishman offers a plausible
reason for this:
RLS is an activity of minorities, frequently powerless, unpopular with
outsiders andquerulous among themselves [ . . . It strikes many] other-
wise intelligent social scientists as unnatural, i.e. as counter to some
supposedly natural drift of historical events or the obvious direction
of social change. (Fishman, 1991: 382)
260
Europe
Catalan a Decade Later
This lackof interest is especiallysurprisinggiventhat RLS-efforts are char-
acteristically socially patterned and organised activities of the type that
sociologists refer to as social movements. (Fishman, 1991: 382).
Fishman (1991: 383) also underlines the commitment
3
involved in RLS
efforts, which the mainstreammay viewwith fear, suspicion and rejection,
as bordering on the irrational and the mystic. We shall see below how
Catalan language planning is sometimes portrayed in the rest of the Spain
as being a fanatical mania.
What Fishman refers to as ethnicity is usually, in Europe, termed
nationalism, the will to exist on the part of stateless nations. Since nation-
alism has fuelled a wide range of claims, is more a sentiment than an
ideology and is not based on common premises (e.g. Conversi 1997), it is
perhaps not surprising that social scientists seembewildered and regard it
as an anomaly, rather than as one of the prime motivating forces of
mankind in the present age (Fishman, 1991: 384). RLS theory as presented
raises some questions which deserve further attention.
The causes of intergenerational transmission in minority language
situations
Among monolinguals this issue is not relevant, but it is, and very much
so, among bilingual people, especially young couples starting to raise a
family. There can surely be two main factors behind the choice to be made:
language group loyalty and the perception of language usefulness. RLS
theoryhas hithertoconcentrated as might be expectedif built onthe foun-
dations of cases of extreme minoritisation on integrative, or identity,
factors. However, as we move along the scale towards the least disrupted
values, the pragmatic worth of the language increases. Fishman makes
some direct allowance for this:
Clearly, there is and must be an economic basis to Xish life and to the
advancement of RLS. [ . . . ] InStage 3 [ . . . ] this alreadyimportant lower
work sphere is attended to additionally along two different lines [ . . . ].
On the one hand, Xish-controlled and staffed enterprises and services
seek to meet the needs of the Yish market; on the other hand, Yish-
controlled enterprises and services seek to meet the needs of the Xish
market. (Fishman, 1991: 1034)
Let us look at several other economic factors relevant in intergenerational
language choice.
(1) The perceivedusefulness of the language for social advancement is crucial
(Colomer, 1996). Parents want the best for their children. They do not want
themto be held back, or subject to ridicule or abuse, the way they may feel
Catalan a Decade Later 261
they themselves have been. The significance of this factor in language
reproduction was underlined in the Euromosaic report (Nelde et al., 1996).
In dealing with subordinated languages, it would seem reasonable to
suggest that at high levels of disruption, the affective, identity appeal for
intergenerational language transmission is more powerful, and that lower
down the scale, pragmatic motives take over or at least are added to the
decision-making process.
It is thus essential to study the role each language plays in the labour
market which most school-leavers will be entering. It may be local, if there
are newjobs available; it may be outside the linguistic area, in which case it
will be less likely that the family will speak the subordinate language
(which may well be viewed as less useful) to their children.
(2) The Euromosaic report also drewattention to the degree of linguistic
homogeneity or heterogeneity in families. Economic factors are absolutely
crucial in population movements, and provided the social integration of
new arrivals is facilitated, such immigration usually leads to some degree
of intermarriage. Whether the offspring of such marriages are bilingual in
both parents languages, or have only one of the two as their home
language, will certainly have a large impact on the future of the subordi-
nate language whenever such families are numerous.
Intergenerational transmission of the language is of course not the only
short-termmechanismfor a language community to survive. Other agents
canhelpto produce the language, principally the school, the media andthe
community or neighbourhood. RLS efforts are also directed, in the more
powerful and geographically concentrated minorities, towards the
linguistic recruitment of in-coming families andtheir offspring. This is crit-
ical where the local socioeconomic climate is attracting outsiders. Nelde et
al., (1996) dwell at some length on the various agents.
The influence of the mass media
The Catalan experience shows the great importance, on several scores,
of the mass media. The existence of printed or, especially, audio-visual
media in a language firstly helps to create a sense of community among
those who consume such media. Secondly, it provides a formal model of
language which may well help to improve levels of competence in the
community. Thirdly, it helps to generate discourse within the community.
Finally, it offers considerable job opportunities to speakers of the language
(as has also happened in Ireland, Scotland and Wales, following the
setting-up of television channels in the respective languages).
However, the mainstreammedia may also be players in the process. We
shall see below how Madrid-based media had a clearly detrimental effect
262 Europe
on public opinion and perceptions of the language situation in Catalonia
and the policies being pursued, in the period 199397. In this context,
Fishmans statement about achieving ethnocultural separation perhaps
underestimates the penetration that Yish media can achieve:
Those RLS movements that safely secure stage 5 andbelowcan remain
intergenerationally secure, provided they can maintain sufficient
ethnocultural separationfromYishencroachment ontheir ownfamily-
home-neighbourhood-community intragroup institutional bases.
(Fishman 1991: 105)
The political climate
The viewof the dominant (or majority) language group with regard to
RLS efforts is greatly important, for given its access to power (and particu-
larly to public opinion) it can place serious obstacles in the way of such a
process. Fortunately, in Europe there is an ongoing and unparalleled
process of unification which will tend to lessen such obstacles. The Euro-
pean Union, founded in the 1950s with six members, now has 15, and six
central and eastern European countries may soon join. Though the effects
on a language such as Catalan may be marginal, there is an interesting and
promising discourse developing. In order for citizens to embrace the idea
of Europe, andaccept historical foes as friends, cultural diversity andtoler-
ance have to be promoted, as the only way to move to greater European
integration and unity. In such a climate, the tolerance of multilingualism
that was for so long denied Catalan, Basque and Galician in Spain can
surely have a beneficial effort on RLS-efforts.
Demographic considerations
Declining numbers of speakers may not result simply from language
shift inside families, i.e. from intergenerational transmission ceasing. Low
birth rates, or high infant mortality, may also have significant effects.
In Catalonia there has been concern for decades about the lowbirth rate
(the small average number of childrenthat eachwomanhas). Over the past
20 years there has been a dramatic decline in the birth rate and general
fertility of the population in both Spain and Catalonia. It is estimated that
women are now having an average of under 1.2 children, well below the
replacement level, usually put at 2.1.
4
Annual births in Catalonia have
halved, and many schools have closed down. The typical dumb-bell-
shapeddistributionof the populationis developinga taperingbase. In1996
the numbers of births and deaths were almost identical (54,602 and 53,433
respectively).
Thoughthere is no specific researchonthe issue, several studies suggest
Catalan a Decade Later 263
tangentially that the average number of children born to native Catalan-
speakers continues to be lower than among Spanish-speaking incomers.
This would explain why the percentage of native Spanish-speakers in
primary schools is higher than it is in the general population. This trend
obviously has serious implications indeed for the future of the Catalan
language community, givenits weakpowers of linguistic linguistic recruit-
ment, on the one hand, and assimilation, on the other.
Though levels of immigration fromSpanish-speaking Spain have, since
1975, been well belowthose of the 1950s and, especially, the 1960s, there is
still a steady annual flux of about 20,000 moving each way. Most of those
leaving Catalonia are apparently non-Catalan-born, which reduces the
negative impact of such movements on the overall linguistic situation in
Catalonia.
There is also a smaller, but growing, proportion of migrants from the
Third World, mostly from west Africa and Morocco. Exact figures are
impossible, for many are illegal migrants and are not on the census (Hall,
1997). But they still amount to less than 2% of the whole population. The
question remains of whether their linguistic integration will be just to
Spanish, to both Catalan and Spanish, or just to Catalan.
RLS and language planning in Catalonia
Let us nowmove to the other part of our task: to reviewthe past decade.
Anumber of crucial statements made by Fishmanare clearly verifiedinthe
Catalan context. (1) the important role of language, (2) bilingualism and
language shift, (3) the crucial role of family transmission, (4) the indirect
relationshipbetween language policy andthe family, (5) concern about the
language commitment of young Catalans, (6) the passive acceptance of the
language situation, (7) the needfor steps to increase social language use, (8)
the need for civil society to be active, (9) policy as official commitment and
example, (10) the need to relegitimise the language policy.
The important role of language
Fishman describes the salience of language and language issues among
supporters of RLS: Ethnolinguistic persistence involves a basic continuity
inthe meaningof symbols. For RLSers a givenlanguage is the first andfore-
most of these symbols, as is their interpretation of that language as being
truly fundamental to identity and continuity (Fishman, 1991: 387; see also
Conversi, 1997).
Mar (1996: 18) also underlines the central role that language andculture
enjoy, as the result of the historical definition of a collective identity, as the
repertoire of available options tofreelydefine eachindividuals identityand
264 Europe
the position of each person before the world, and as the symbolic vehicle of
socialisation and of the integration of ones own personality.
Bilingualism and language shift
In Fishmans chapter on Three success stories (more or less): Modern
Hebrew, French in Quebec and Catalan in Spain,
5
he refers in detail to
three language-and-culture settings which [ . . . ] have traversed the most
sensitive and dangerous sections of the difficult path from essentially
problematic to essentially non-problematic within the lifetimes of indi-
viduals still alive today (Fishman, 1991: 287). In reference to Catalan he
describes the governments policy as a triple balancing act: attaining . . .
the symbolic promotion and the functional institutionalisation of
Catalan in [ . . . ] the most influential and powerful areas of modern life
[; 2. overcoming] the legacy of mother-tongue illiteracy and inferiority
that many native, middle-class Catalans have inherited from the
Franco years [ . . . and 3.] activating the passive Catalan that Spanish
speakers quickly acquire. (Fishman, 1991: 299)
Catalan sociolinguistics has long been wary about the possibilities, in prac-
tice, of personal bilingualism in a given society being a stable and long-
lasting social phenomenon. Valencians such as Llus-Vicent Aracil and
Rafael-Llus Ninyoles have witnessed a three-generation shift in the larger
cities of Valencia, from Catalan monolingualism in the first generation,
through a bilingual second generation, to a third generation practically
monolingual in the state language. While the author shares this profound
doubt, it may be that given certain circumstances, such as those that apply
to the survival and recovery of Hebrew(in which, outside Israel itself Jews
have over the centuries retained very specific, if limited, functions for the
language, in the fields of religious life, literature and popular culture),
bilingual situations can be stable. However, few would claim that such
circumstances prevail inthe Catalancontext. Prats, for instance, claims that
history shows us that language conflicts are resolved either through
language shift or through language normalisation. They always pass, he
claims, througha phase of bilingualismwhichleads onto shift almost inex-
orably. To defy history, leading people to believe that we shall be the first
to maintain the balanced coexistence of two languages in a given territory,
is an error which will be paid for dearly (1990: 23).
Such a stance would require that the former subordinate language
displace the formerly dominant language, rather than aiming at an impos-
sible balance; so what Fishman termed success stories in RLS can only be
successful in the long run if their aim is a monolingual society such as is
Catalan a Decade Later 265
being achieved in Israel. However, the prospects of such a society in
western Europe are bleak even in large countries such as Italy or Germany,
as the needfor international languages suchas Englishor Frenchis increas-
ingly clear. So it is hard to envisage a language policy that could indeed
displace Spanish and leave Catalan in a totally consolidated and safe posi-
tion, given the deeply rooted habits still prevalent in Catalonia, as well as
the highproportionof the populationwhose familyorigins, unlike the situ-
ation say 50 years ago, are monolithically Spanish-speaking.
There are certainly signs that language choice norms are more flexible
than they used to be (Vallverd, 1998: 614). Yet the changes that had been
hoped for in such norms for interethnolinguistic contacts have been slight,
and the dominant position of Spanish in Barcelona seems unchallenged.
According to Boix (1993: 211), despite the introduction of new language
choice norms which are more favourable towards Catalan, the demo-
graphic context and the more general cultural patterns support the
predominance of Spanish in interpersonal relations. This is further
increased by the fact that most Catalans nowadays speak Spanish with
little or no accent, thus even leading to conversations between Catalan-
speaking strangers often taking place in Spanish (Flaquer, 1996: 1778).
How true, in the context of Catalan, is Fishmans statement (1991: 108)
that even in the heartland and even with cultural autonomy the pressures
from Yish may still be omnipresent!
Some argue that Catalan has put up with similar situations before and
can come through relatively unscathed. Others disagree. Prats et al., for
instance (1990: 602), believe that the danger threatening the Catalan
language today [ . . . ] has no equivalent in earlier situations, as it nowrests
upon an unprecedented situation: in the complete, definitive extension of
social bilingualism in the Catalan-speaking community. In their view,
social bilingualism is by definition unstable, and that the imbalance
between the two languages in contact gives rise to a set of attitudes which
are disastrous for Catalan. In all segments of social life Spanish becomes de
facto the only language of communication which is truly indispensable,
necessary, they claim. There are never any domains where Catalan is the
only language used, exposed as it is to the bilingualisation of its users.
The crucial role of family language transmission
The absolutely crucial role of intergenerational transmission is under-
lined by several Catalan researchers. Boix (1993: 34) for instance states that
the plight of a minoritised language becomes virtually irreversible when it
ceases to be transmitted intergenerationally, when the cobweb which for
266 Europe
generations has linked the use of a language to various fields of everyday
activity of a community is broken.
Another, Valencian, sociolinguist is equally clear in his opinion. The
end-causes of the extinction of a language are to be found in the interrup-
tion of its intergenerational transmission, a phenomenon of which we have
good illustrations in languages such as Occitan or even in Catalan itself
(Montoya-Abat, 1996: 215). His mention of Catalan refers to some cities in
the southernmost part of the Catalan-speaking lands, such as Alacant
(Alicante) or the totally hispanified Oriola (Orihuela).
Of Stage 6 (the attainment of intergenerational informal oralcy and its
demographic concentration and institutional reinforcement; Fishman,
1991: 92), Fishman says (p. 399) that if this stage is not satisfied, all else can
amount to little more than biding time,
6
at best generation by generation,
without a natural, self-priming social mechanismhaving been engendered
thereby. On re-reading this passage several thoughts spring to mind.
Surely it is not just RLS proponents that aim to achieve a change in the
linguistic child-rearing practices of citizens. A process of language shift as
dramatically successful as that undertaken by the French republicans
broke the back of the so-called regional languages when the first whole
generation of adult speakers of Occitan, or Dutch or Breton or Catalan
decided to speak to their children only in French.
It is true that the OccitanCalandretas, the CatalanBressola schools andthe
Arrels educational movement do a great deal to keep their languages alive:
but it is very uphill work if most of their pupils speak only French at home.
The Welsh and the Basques (in Spain) have made great strides, achieving
quite high proportions of bilingual schooling for children, and the number
of people fluent in these languages has recently stabilised thanks to the
growing proficiency of the younger generation. Nevertheless, the starting
point is very low: perhaps only 1215% of young children have Welsh or
Basque as their main home language.
An overarching model of sociolinguistic change is needed which can
cope withRLSina lesser-usedor minority language suchas Irishor Welsh
or, more recently, Latvian or Estonian, and the spread of a national or
imperial language such as Latin, French, Russian or English, whilst incor-
poratingFishmans reference to a natural, self-primingsocial mechanism.
The model wouldperceive of societal language status changes as processes
which can become self-maintaining, perhaps through feedback mecha-
nisms. It would then look at the obstacles that prevent such a natural
process taking place. It wouldalso leadto simple formulations of language
Catalan a Decade Later 267
policy, as deliberate procedures to remove obstacles to natural changes
occurring. We shall returntosucha model towards the endof the chapter.
The indirect relationship between language policy and the family
The needfor a sturdylanguage policyoverflows intothe familydomain,
though only as an indirect result thereof, according to Torres (1994: 47),
who argues that an intensification of the policy of linguistic normalisation
is needed, among other important motives, to help maintain in as crucial a
domain as the family a sufficiently high presence of Catalan to ensure its
future.
Bastardas explains the relationship between language policy and
expected changes in individual behaviour. There seems to be a dynamic
relationship between the spread of the use of the standard autochthonous
variety in communications which Corbeil (1980) calls institutionalised,
7
and
the later effects of language shift or normalisationwhichcanbe observedin
individualised communications.
8
In Bastardas view, if and when the
normalisation process is perceived as successful and irreversible, the
private behaviour of individuals individualised communications will
tend to adapt accordingly: though probably only very gradually
(Bastardas, 1994: 35; see also Fishman, 1991: 404). Given the complexity of
the many other intervening socio-environmental factors, and the impor-
tance of this issue, Bastardas (personal communication, February 1999)
calls for urgent research.
It is clear that the nature of language planning changes substantially as
we move upthe GIDSfromStage 7 toStage 1. Planningat Stage 7 is anunof-
ficial, social, group activity. A good example is the extraordinary case of
Cornish, which has literally been resuscitated in this century, several
generations after the last speaker of the language died. Planning at Stage 1,
on the other hand, is official, often legally legitimated, and affects institu-
tional bodies and services, as Fishman (1991: 381) reminds us: Language
status planning, of which RLS is a sub-category, is overly identified with
central government efforts, hardly the most likely or the most sympathetic
auspices for minority-RLS efforts. Now, the way people bring up their
families including the language they choose is not for the authorities to
decide. Policies may influence language attitudes through a discourse that
can be perceived through the language behaviour of politicians, govern-
ment bodies and civil servants (particularly teachers); and through legisla-
tion. But such measures cannot legitimately intervene in peoples domestic
lives and decisions: any influence is much more subtle, and therefore long
term, in nature. In Catalonia, older people still remember the savage
repression of the language following the Spanish Civil War of 193639
268 Europe
(Benet, 1973; Ferrer i Girons, 1985): yet the authorities could not force
people to use Spanish at home. Such a change took place mainly in social
and ideological circles either close to the regime or keen to take advantage
of the situation for their own economic advancement.
Concern about the language commitment of young Catalans
Fishman practises what he preaches in reminding readers (1991: 386)
that those committed to RLS are subject to bouts of depression or pessi-
mism in the face of negative facts: he describes the diminution in the
numbers or proportions of speakers as constant, which may be so in
immigrant communities but is not always the case (namely the Catalans,
the Welsh).
Some books on the language situation in Catalonia, published since
Reversing Language Shift appeared, have quite dramatic titles andreflect the
concern about the future of the language: references are made to a contro-
versial future for Catalan (Vallverd, 1990); to an interrupted language
(Montoya-Abat, 1996); to choosing not being betraying (Boix, 1993); to a
language war (Voltas, 1996); to Catalan being a public or a private
language (Flaquer, 1996); and to an improbable normality (Branchadell,
1998).
Fishman (1991: 108) argues against head-in-the-sand reactions: It is
unwise to disregard the pessimism of the pessimists, the alarms of those
who constantly view with alarm the state of health of Xish language-in-
culture. Yish, the constant competitor, is always part of the local heart-
land scene too, not to mention that its undisputed domination in all other
parts of the polity [ . . . ] also remains inviolate. His message is grim:
Eternal watchfulness is the price of RLS and that price must be paid at
stage 1 too.
In Catalonia there is particular concern about the language attitudes of
young people. Larreula (1998: 20) has recently written that the offspring of
those who arrived in the 1950s and the 1960s fromSpanish-speaking Spain
fully identify themselves as Catalans, yet perceive that most Catalans
communicate naturally in Spanish. As a result, he continues, they may
viewthe language of the institutions and the school (Catalan) as artificially
fosteredbythose inpower. Sucha policymight be construedas favouringa
minority simply because Catalan is its family language. The fact, Larreula
concludes, that Catalan is not the clearest sign of identity among most
young people who fully identify themselves as Catalans, fills many people
with anxiety, frustration and pain.
Facedwith massive immigration andlowbirth rates, the only long-term
hope for Catalan is to recruit new speakers among the immigrant groups.
Catalan a Decade Later 269
There is growing evidence that this recruitment is at least partly taking
place. Comes et al., (1995), for instance, lookedat 11 to 14-year-oldSpanish-
speaking pupils throughout Catalonia. It may be premature to look at the
language habits of youngsters who have not yet left school; nevertheless,
the attitudes found were very positive, and language habits are beginning
to change: 50% claim to speak Catalan a lot or quite a lot, and only 7%
said not at all (table 42: 118): 58%claimed they could hold a conversation
completely in Catalan (table 41: 117). To speak with their Catalan friends,
14%said they use Catalan, while 47%said both Catalan and Spanish (table
24: 98).
Flaquer (1996: 612) argues forcefully that the recent process of
economic and political modernisation has given immigrants a new status
and dignity as citizens rather than subjects, and that this, coupled with
greater mobility and the impact of television, has radically reduced the
level of linguistic integration among Spanish-speaking incomers to
Catalonia.
The passive acceptance of the language situation
Boix (1993) and Bastardas (1994: 39) believe that appropriate language
choice norms are crucial in any process of language status change. During
periods of crisis and of rapid changes in which language market tensions
are strong, Boixargues, some people will probablyrealise that the language
use norms that regulate their code changes can be changed, and wish to do
so. Beside these sectors with language loyalty, however, other members of
the subordinated community continue to perceive the language use norms
in a non-conflictual manner and have internalised themso much that they
consider them natural and immutable.
Boix is concerned that the lack of clear language-use guidelines favour-
able to the recovery of Catalan, among youngsters in Barcelona, is an
ominous sign for its future. They accept quite happily being spoken to in
Catalan or in Spanish. He detects no perceived need for any voluntary
(personal or group) attempt to modify the norms. Boix thinks that this
contemporising approach is, as a Quebec policy-maker said of youngsters
in his own country, a sign of a poor knowledge of the linguistic situation
[ . . . ] and of a false feeling of security (Plourde, 1985: 303). Despite all the
advances inthe disseminationof CatalaninBarcelona andthe introduction
of new language use norms which are favourable to Catalan, Boix (1993:
211) concludes, the demographic context and the more general cultural
traits favour the dominance of Spanish in interpersonal relations.
This conformity is also underlined by Flaquer (1996: 353), who realises
that it is the citizens, in the last analysis, who will decide the future of the
270 Europe
language andare the true architects of any change inthe linguistic habits of
the population. He is surprisedat the passivityandlackof interest that both
political leaders and the Catalan-speaking community as a whole display
towards these issues. Flaquer calls for a public discussion which will allow
Catalans to calibrate and investigate which options they have.
The need for steps to increase social language use
Aym stresses the need to act outside institutional areas, to counteract
the dropin the social, interpersonal use of Catalan which has resultedfrom
the incorporation into Catalan society of the offspring of the Spanish-
speaking migrants of 195075. Policies on providing for the needs of the
Catalan-speakers in the field of cultural industries, the media and in deal-
ings with the public authorities are not enough. There is a danger of:
tipping the concept of normalisation too far in the direction of satis-
fying Catalan-speakers demands for consumption (of andin) Catalan,
withthe aidof protective linguistic rights, andof not producing, onthe
other hand, sufficient generating activity to achieve language change
on the part of the Spanish-speakers. (Aym 1993: 889)
The need for civil society to be active
Some, in describing RLS as a political task, seem to suggest that the rest
of society need not be committed to participating. Prats et al. (1990: 75)
analyse two main threats to the survival of Catalan a direct one, the
passage to Spanish and an indirect one, following the path of progressive
linguistic empoverishment and demand a political project capable of
creatingthe conditions whichwill ensure language normalisationthrough-
out the territory. In the opinion of Prats et al. (1990), some sociolinguists do
not fully appreciate that preventing the retreat of Catalan, so often blurred
by the (limited) access to some high-prestige domains, is a political task.
A leading Catalan sociolinguist, Vallverd, vehemently criticises those
whocall for still more dynamic policies while seeminglynot acceptingtheir
own share of responsibility in the process. He finds it shameful that among
those who call for a radicallynewlanguage policywithanalmost exclusive
use of Catalan, some have given up practising passive bilingualism:
under the pretext that they do not want to be martyrs or heroes, that
they only want to live in peace, they discharge their own responsi-
bility upon the institutions Generalitat, local councils, etc. as if
everything depended on political decisions from up top (Vallverd,
1990: 84)
There has indeed been a constant insistence that a language policy should
Catalan a Decade Later 271
not have to rely on volunteers or committed martyrs. I contend that this
belief seriouslyweakens the Catalanprocess, yet inother Catalan-speaking
lands (the Balearic Islands and Valencia, in particular) social movements
supporting the language are far more dynamic and active.
Flaquer has pointed out (1996: 3556) that there is a risk of a divorce
developing between official circles (which invest in support of the Catalan
language) and the man in the street, unless the necessary effort is actively
supportedbycivil society, includingthe citizens themselves. The success of
the language policy will, inhis opinion, occur by means of the steady incor-
porationtothe use of Catalanbythe large number of residents bornoutside
Catalonia, or Spanish-speakers who hitherto have felt no need to do so. In
his view, the former social pressure for the cultural integration of incomers
has been displacedby institutional pressure, which, he claims, is much less
effective thancommunity pressure andmay be perceivedas animposition.
Flaquer stresses that it is up to the citizens of Catalonia to decide the future
of the language; unless they actively defend Catalan in daily life and stop
relying on institutional campaigns, it could well be threatened. (Flaquer,
1996: 357)
Language policy as official commitment and example
Several authors, and particularly politicians, have stressed the exem-
plary role of the language policy and practice of the Catalan government
(Generalitat de Catalunya). The present director general, Llus Jou, has
written that the 1998 Linguistic Policy Act is a message of the political will
of the Generalitat to continue impelling the process of recovery and pres-
ence of Catalaninall fields andall segments of social life anda commitment
on the part of the Catalan institutions to the language of the country (Jou,
1998: 22).
Many have stressedthat legislation alone cannot save a language. Apart
from the participation of what is often called civil society, the need for
intelligent and ingenious measures by language planning policy imple-
menters, and the need for institutions including researchers to work
together, have also been pointed to. According to Bastardas (1994: 38) the
study of and research on the elements of persistence and strategies of
change, real controlled and evaluated experimentation, and the wholly
scientific attitude of questioning the very representations of the planners
and militants, are aspects of great importance in the success or failure of
sociolinguistic planning.
The need to relegitimise the language policy
The theoretical bases for the present language policy in Catalonia need
to be renovated and publicly disseminated for several reasons. Firstly,
272 Europe
oppositiontosucha policy, whichwas fairlydisorganisedin1981 whenthe
first attempt to pre-empt the re-catalanisation policy was made (a
Manifiesto published in a Madrid newspaper just a few days after the
attempted coup of 23 February), is much better articulated now, and was
particularly virulent in nature between 1993 and 1997, to try first to bring
down the Socialist government in Madrid, and later, to block the Catalan
language policyBill (enactedinJanuary1998). The political context of these
attacks has been described elsewhere (Strubell, 1998a, 1999). Voltas has
traced the history and development of the movement. Not since 1981, he
wrote (Voltas, 1996: 11), had the people, interests and associations who
organised the 1994 revolt against the Generalitats language policy been at
all successful. This time, Catalan civil society and the nationalist world in
general were unable to articulate an organised response: the 1981
Manifiesto was neutralised, among other things, by the formation of a
dynamic NGO committed to promoting the Catalan language, la Crida a la
Solidaritat,
9
while the 1994 one arrivedafter the dissolutionof this organisa-
tion of linguistic self-defence.
On 12 September 1993, the main right-wing Madrid daily newspaper
plastered the following headline across its front page, under a photograph
of the king of Spain in full military regalia: Igual que Franco, pero al revs:
Persecucin del castellano en Catalua.
10
Voltas described his reaction
thus:
The front page of ABC had all the makings of something else: the start
of a crusade whichwas tobe takenupas if it were their ownbymanyof
the mass media throughout Spainandwhichwas to focus the limelight
of political life inthis countryfor the followingtwoyears. (Voltas, 1996:
28)
The text in the papers leading article was a head-on attack on the
Generalitats language policy, describing it as the same, in reverse, as what
had been done under the dictatorship, including a campaign of persecu-
tion against Spanish, in an attempt to eradicate it.
The Generalitat decided to publicly ignore such attacks. However, while
this helped to prevent a mud-slinging contest, the perceived legitimacy of
Catalonias language policy suffered, since replies by intellectuals (e.g.
Branchadell, 1997) had little effect on public opinion. There is a clear need
to relegitimise the aims of the policy, for the main actor and agent is the
Catalan-speaking community, which has retreated into a defensive role
instead of still actively supporting the process.
There is another reason for renovating the RLS discourse in Catalonia.
Catalan a Decade Later 273
Flaquer (1996: 355) has pointedout that as time goes by, the continual refer-
ence to political repression in the past will have less and less explanatory
appeal, so a new discourse is needed to relegitimise a policy which
promotes the Catalanlanguage. The author feels that inthe present context
there is certainly a place for a policy to neutralise the (linguistic) effects of
globalisation not just for Catalan, but for Danish, Dutchor Greek as well
but Flaquers point is nevertheless important.
Some have tried to criticise Catalonias language policy on neo-liberal
grounds. However, appeals to liberalism have been significantly one-
sided, and have ignored the much more numerous and more stringent
norms imposing the use of Spanish. A legal specialist claims that whoever
favours non-intervention in relation to threatened languages either
considers that the linguistic heritage is not a common value needing active
protection, or else defends that conflicts be resolvedaccording to the lawof
the strongest, provided the language in danger of extinction is not their
own (Sol, 1998: 11). He discounts appeals to linguistic liberalism in
debates on norms, such as the recent Linguistic Policy Act, which are
attempts to protect languages from the overwhelming and predatory
might of other stronger ones. As has been repeated pointed out, Sol
recalls, those who call out against quotas and sanctions in the Catalan
Linguistic Policy Act have said nothing against the two hundred
Spanishpre-constitutional andpost-constitutional norms whichimpose
Spanish. Arecent one is a Royal Decree
11
which makes Spanish compul-
sory on part of the information relating to cosmetic products. It makes
things worse by allowing the use of French or English instead of
Spanish . . . but not Catalan.
Catalan in Catalonia: A Brief Overview (198898)
In this part of the chapter we shall reconsider first of all Fishmans
classification of Catalan in Spain at Stage 1, and then look at the specific
development of Catalan in Catalonia, in the 10-year span that the book
covers.
Fishman (1991, fig 14.2: 405) stated his opinion that the following stages
on the Graded Intergenerational Dislocation Scale (GIDS) were receiving
most attention in RLS-efforts:
Stage 1: Some use of Xish in higher level educational, occupational,
governmental and media efforts (but without the additional safety
provided by political independence)
Stage 1 essentially represents the arrival of the pursuit of cultural
autonomy for those who have pursued the vision of Xmen-via-Xish.
274 Europe
[The languages] spokesmen and representatives would become
responsible for planning, conducting and evaluating such activities . . .
Even if this stage is attained, there will inevitably be some pro-RLSers
who are still dissatisfied, perhaps because Xish is not the sole and offi-
cial and ethno-national language of the region, [ . . . ] or perhaps
because even in the heartland and even with cultural autonomy the
pressures from Dish may still be omnipresent. (Fishman 1991: 1078)
Whereas Stage 1 can make a definite contribution to RLS, this contribution
must be successfully translated into intergenerationally transmissible
Stage 6 processes and interactions before it will have more than bureau-
cracy-building and elite-building effects. (Fishman, 1991: 404)
He placed Catalan at this stage.
Stage 2: Xishinlower governmental services andmass media but not in
the higher spheres of either. Plenty of work is still being done at this
level as well.
Stage 3: use of Xishinthe lower worksphere (outside of the Xishneigh-
bourhood/community) involving interaction between Xmen and
Ymen. Such interactions may be increasing: but the use of Catalan is
such contexts is perhaps no longer recovering.
There seems to be no reason for placing Catalan in Catalonia anywhere
other than at Stage 1. However, in the Balearic Islands and Valencia, where
well over a third of all Catalan-speakers live, the intergenerational trans-
mission of Catalan foreseen at Stage 6 and above is certainly not guaran-
teed at present in urban areas.
We shall nowlook at language competence, language quality and a few
aspects of language use in Catalonia. Much of the following data comes
from a recent overview (Roman and Strubell, 1998).
Competence in the language
The census data for 1986 and 1996 in Catalonia show how many people
report they can understand, speak, read and/or write the language. In all
four aspects the increase has been great. Comprehension is approaching
100%. In terms of the numbers of the other three competences there has
beena remarkable increase over this period, withrises inthe percentages of
between 11 and 14 points. Literacy has grown especially rapidly (Table
11.1).
In a country with just over six million inhabitants, such improvements
are spectacular and are probably unrivalled anywhere in the world. They
Catalan a Decade Later 275
stand as a fitting tribute to the work being done in schools, since the
changes have occurred above all among young people (Table 11.2).
The rise in oral fluency between 1986 and 1996 is clearly visible: it is
concentrated in the 214 year age group (a 21.4 point rise) and continues in
the 1529 year age group (a 17.8 point increase) and in the 3044 year age
group (a 14.3 point increase). The overall impression, which is even clearer
when looking at literacy-related competence, is that of a tidal wave
sweeping up fromthe school-age groups, peaking at 1519, and then grad-
ually declining with age. After each successive five-year period the wave
peaks at a higher level, andits effect spreads across into older groups as the
cohorts age.
The learning process is also taking place, naturally enough, among
recent arrivals to Catalonia. The improvement in the period between 1986
and1991 was clear: even among the (extraordinarily large number of) non-
Catalan-born people who had arrived in the 1960s there was a noticeable
276 Europe
Table 11.1 Proportion of population of Catalonia recorded as being able to
understand, speak, read and write Catalan in 1986 and 1996 (official
statistics)
1986 1996 Increase
Can understand
Catalan
5,287,200 90.3% 5,683,237 95.0% 396,037 4.7
Can speak
Catalan
3,747,813 64.0% 4,506,512 75.3% 758,699 11.3
Can read
Catalan
3,542,012 60.5% 4,330,251 72.4% 788,239 11.9
Can write
Catalan
1,844,493 31.5% 2,743,326 45.8% 898,833 14.4
Total (aged 2 or
more)
5,856,435 5,984,334 127,899
Table 11.2 Proportion of population of Catalonia recorded as being able to
speak Catalan, by age-group (official 1986, 1991 and 1996 statistics)
Age group (years) 1986 (%) 1991 (%) 1996 (%)
From 2 to 14 62.2 77.9 83.6
From 15 to 29 72.6 79.1 90.4
From 30 to 44 60.6 64.1 74.9
From 45 to 64 59.5 57.7 63.0
More than 64 64.2 62.6 61.9
overall increase in the proportion of those claiming oral fluency during the
five-year period. There was no significant increase among those who
arrived before 1960 (most of whomwere at least in their fifties in 1991); but
the increase was very marked among those who arrived in the 1970s. The
base-line is also improving: there is a rise in the percentage of the latest
cohort of arrivals that claim oral fluency within the space of 05 years
(Table 11.3).
Little reference has been made to the other highly populated Catalan-
speaking areas in Spain. Neither the Valencian nor the Balearic Islands
governments included a language item in their 1996 municipal censuses.
However, it seems probable that in neither qualitative nor quantitative
terms have competence in, or use of, the language, increased as much in
these two areas; and recent events can hardly prove positive for the
language in Valencia (Strubell, 1998c).
The quality of the language
A brief reference to the issue of the degradation of the language has
already been made (Prats et al. 1990: 75). There is a danger of the specific
features of Catalan, in phonetics, in grammar and syntax, and in lexicon,
gradually being lost, and of Catalan becoming, in effect, a mere dialect of
another language (Spanish).
Has the quality of the language used in Catalonia improved between
1986 and 1996? Even giving full credence to the census data themselves, a
growing proportion of the population speak Catalan as a second, not first,
language, or canevenbe classedas ambilinguals (Vallverd, 1998: 30). Since
Catalan a Decade Later 277
Table 11.3 Percentage claiming oral fluency in Catalan (official statistics)
Period of arrival 1986 census 1991 census
1920 and earlier 77.5 77.7
19211930 69.6 69.5
19311940 58.5 55.8
19411950 44.3 42.6
19511960 33.0 33.7
19611965 23.7 27.0
19661970 24.2 28.8
19711975 25.1 34.6
19761980 30.9 39.3
19811985 25.6 41.1
19861991 30.2
nearly all learners are native Spanish-speakers (unlike countries like the
UnitedStates or Canada, where the linguistic backgroundof immigrants is
varied), a decline in the quality of Catalan is unavoidable in lexicon,
phonetics, grammar on account of interference from Spanish. (This has
been going on much longer in Valencia, south of Catalonia, where
schooling in Catalan is more recent and less widespread, and where the
colloquial language has many, many hundreds of loan-words from
Spanish.)
As regards the level of literacy, things are somewhat better. Never
before have so many people been able to read and write Catalan. Whether
they actually do so or not should more appropriately be answered in the
next section. Suffice it to say that one of the two leading Spanish-language
newspapers in Catalonia, El Peridico de Catalunya, decided to start issuing
a Catalan-language edition in parallel in October 1997, and are very satis-
fied that this edition is accounting for 4042% of their sales. Their market
research revealed a growing number of potential readers, to whom they
have successfully offered their product.
Incidentally, as far as proficiency in Spanish is concerned, research (e.g.
Consell 1998) shows that average levels, though not as high as in some
monolingual regions, are higher than in several; there is therefore no cause
for concern on this score.
The use of the language
Roman and Strubell (1998: 80910) quote research results which
suggest increases in the overall intergenerational use of Catalan. These
changes are primarily due to first-generation Catalans: their average self-
estimated use of Catalan, on a 010 scale, was 1.31 with parents, and 4.08
with offspring.
In the metropolitan area of Barcelona, where over half the population
(and a much higher proportion of non-Catalan-born residents) live, only
18% of couples had different first languages. According to a 1986 survey,
98% of the offspring of Catalan-speaking parents were returned as being
themselves Catalan-speaking; the figure was 97% among the offspring of
Spanish-speaking parents (Subirats, 1990: 28). Vila (1993) considers that
linguistically mixed couples have a crucial role in deciding the future of
Catalan in the region. The 1986 and 1990 surveys in the above-mentioned
area suggest that unlike the situation in the 1970s, such linguistically heter-
ogeneous couples now grant Catalan a more substantial place than
Spanish, as shown by the first language of the offspring, and that for the
first time, a large number of young people are being brought up as natural
bilinguals (Table 11.4).
278 Europe
No trend should be sought fromthese diachronic figures: the questions
regarding subjects main language were slightly different, and the two
areas were not identical. However, as statedabove, the persistence of nega-
tive interethnolinguistic language choice norms (e.g. Bastardas, 1994), and
the sheer demographic weight of non-Catalan-speakers living in and
around Barcelona, make the future of the language, even in its heartland,
much less secure than many believe.
Closing Section: A Model of Language Status Change
Of stage 6,
13
Fishman says that if this stage is not satisfied, all else can
amount to little more than biding time,
14
at best generation by generation,
without a natural, self-priming social mechanismhaving been engendered
thereby (Fishman, 1991: 399). In fact, however, surely it is not just RLS
proponents that aim to achieve a change in the linguistic child-rearing
practices of citizens. The dramatically successful language shift achieved
by the French republicans broke the back of the so-called regional
languages when the first whole generation of adult speakers of Occitan,
Dutch, Breton or Catalan decided to speak to their children only in French.
There is perhaps more in common than meets the eye between RLS and
these traditional policies regarding national languages.
I amat present developing a circular model to describe social processes
of language status change, both LS and RLS. There is a great deal of
feedbacking in any process of social change, including RLS. The identifica-
tion of obstacles to such processes, and designing ways of overcoming (or
sidetracking) them, is probably the main task of language planners. The
main idea is that learning a language, using it, and having positive percep-
tions and motivation to further increase its study and use, are linked
together to form a natural, self-priming social mechanism, but that the
passage fromone to the next may be blockedby external or internal factors.
The motivation, andthe learningof the language, are relatedtothe function
or domain: it is not the same toregardthe individual as a consumer of prod-
ucts, as a member of a labour market, or as a member of a local community.
Catalan a Decade Later 279
Table 11.4 Percentage of young people brought up as natural bilinguals
Catalan (%) Both (%) Spanish(%)
1986 Metropolitan area 45 22 33
1990 Metropolitan region 36 45 20
Source: Vila (1993:13)
Each of these makes special demands upon language use, which may be
passive or active, or both. An early draft of the model, which the author
terms the Catherine wheel to convey the objective of a self-perpetuating
change, has been published recently (Strubell, 1998b). (See Figure 11.1.)
The Catherine wheel model views the individual as the centre of any
process of democratic social change. In the illustration, the individual is
taken as a consumer. Any of the six steps may be subject to blockage, and it
is the task of language policymakers to overcome the causes of blockage
withspecific measures where they are required. However, more oftenthan
not the measures are neither directly linguistic in nature, nor can they be
adopted by language planners on their own.
In the Catalan case I feel that there are growing obstacles in the prag-
matic fields of perception and motivation. These are crucial areas; despite
considerable interest displayedin our experience among Quebec language
planners, for instance, Catalonias language planners have tended to
neglect them in recent years, relying perhaps more than is advisable on
legislation.
Thus, a public discourse to relegitimise the language policy, and to
280 Europe
Figure 11.1 Catherine wheel (adapted from Strubell, 1998b: 28)
convince the populace of the importance of working together in
everyday life to fully restore the language, would I am sure pay hand-
some dividends and allow us to look forward to the next 10 years with
high hopes.
Notes
1. The author would like to express his gratitude for the many remarks and sug-
gestions made to an earlier draft by A. Bastardas, D. Conversi, L. Flaquer, B.
Montoya, A. Rossich, F. Vallverd and Professor J.A. Fishman.
2. Some use of Xish in higher level educational, occupational, governmental and
media efforts (but without the additional safety net provided by political inde-
pendence (Fishman, 1991: 107).
3. A very palpable degree of affect, a sentimental (rather than merely an instru-
mental) bonding, a stress on real or putative ethno-kinship, an aspiration
toward consciousness and identity (re)formation, a heightened degree of altru-
istic self-sacrifice and a disregard for least effort advantages.
4. The causes of this decline are beyondthe scope of this paper to consider, though
they probably include working-womanhood, an endangered welfare state and
job uncertainty.
5. Published in a Catalan translation in 1993 (Fishman, 1993).
6. The italics are Fishmans.
7. That is, those which are usual in public spheres, generally corresponding to the
organisations into which a modern society is structured.
8. That is, those produced by people as private individuals.
9. Literally, Call to Solidarity [in Defence of the Catalan Language, Cultura and Na-
tion].
10. Just like Franco, but the other wayround: Persecutionof SpanishinCatalonia.
11. No. 1599/1998, of 17 October.
12. The attainment of intergenerational informal oralcy and its demographic con-
centration and institutional reinforcement (Fishman, 1991: 92).
13. The italics are Fishmans.
References
Aym, J.M. (1993) Estendre ls interpersonal del catal (per, i) conservar-ne la
qualitat. Revista de llengua i dret, nm. 20, 8794.
Bastardas, A. (1994) Persistncia i canvi en el comportament lingstic: la
planificaci lingstica. Treballs de Sociolingstica Catalana, nm. 12, 319.
Benet, J. (1973) Catalunya sota el RgimFranquista. Paris: Edicions Catalanes de Par s.
Boix i Fuster, E. (1993) Triar no s trair. Identitat i llengua en els joves de Barcelona.
Edicions 62, Barcelona.
Branchadell, A. (1997) Liberalisme i poltica lingstica. Barcelona: Empries.
Branchadell, A. (1998) La normalitat improbable. Barcelona: Empries.
Colomer, J.M. (1996) La utilitat del bilingisme. Barcelona: Edicions 62.
Comes, G., Jimnez, B. and Alcaraz, V. (1995) La llengua catalana i els castellano-
parlants de 10 a 15 anys. Barcelona: Columna Edicions.
Consell Superior dAvaluaci del Sistema Educatiu (1998) Resultats acadmics de
lalumnat de 14 i 16 anys a Catalunya. Informes davaluaci, 2. Consell Superior
Catalan a Decade Later 281
dAvaluacidel Sistema Educatiu, Departament dEnsenyament, Generalitat de
Catalunya, Barcelona.
Conversi, D. (1997) The Basques, the Catalans and Spain. Alternative Routes to National-
ist Mobilisation.London: Hurst & Co.
Corbeil, J.C. (1990) Lamnagement linguistique du Qubec. Montreal: Gurin.
Ferrer i Girons, F. (1985) La persecuci poltica de la llengua catalana. Barcelona:
Edicions 62.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Fishman, J.A. (1993) Tres casos amb (ms o menys) xit: lhebreumodern, el francs
al Quebec i el catal a Espanya. Treballs de Sociolingstica Catalana 11, 1948.
Flaquer V.L. (1996) El catal, llengua pblica o privada? Barcelona: Empries.
Hall, J. (1997) Nascuts i vinguts a Catalunya: la poblaci estrangera. In M. Reixach
(ed.) El coneixement del catal. Anlisi de les dades del cens lingstic de 1991 de
Catalunya, les Illes Balears i del Pas Valenci (pp. 10140). Barcelona: Departament
de Cultura de la Generalitat de Catalunya.
Jou, L. (1998) Els principis de llengua prpia i llenges oficials enlarticulat de la llei
1/1998, de 7 de gener, de poltica lingstica. Revista de llengua i dret, nm. 29,
722.
Larreula V. (1998) Enric. Identitat. Canvi de llengua i crisi didentitat a Catalunya,
Escola Catalana, Any XXXIII, Nm. 353, 1820.
Mar, I. (1996) Plurilingisme europeu i llengua catalana. Biblioteca Lingstica
Catalana, Universitat de Valncia, Valncia.
Montoya-Abat, B. (1996) Alacant. La llengua interrompuda. Valncia: Denes editorial.
Nelde, P., Strubell, M. and Williams, G. (1996) Euromosaic: Production and reproduc-
tion of minority language communities in the European Union. Luxembourg:
Commission of the European Communities.
Plourde, M. (1985) Lattitude des jeunes face la langue. In La langue franaise au
Qubec. Confrences et al.locutions (pp. 3013). Quebec: diteur officiel duQubec.
Prats, M. (1990) Reflexi ignasiana sobre la normalitzaci lingstica. InM. Prats, A.
Rafanell and A. Rossich (eds) El futur de la llengua catalana (pp. 928). Barcelona:
Empries.
Prats, M., Rafanell, A. and Rossich, A. (1990) En lesperana, contra lesperana. In
M. Prats, A. Rafanell and A. Rossich (eds) El futur de la llengua catalana (pp.
3983). Barcelona: Empries.
Roman, J.-M. and Strubell, M. (1998) Ls de la llengua a Catalunya. In S. Giner
(coord.) La Societat Catalana (pp. 80520). Barcelona: Institut dEstadstica de
Catalunya, Generalitat de Catalunya.
Sol, J.-R. (1998) La legislaci sobre llenges en el mn, Escola Catalana, Any XXXIII,
Nm. 351, 911.
Strubell i Trueta, M. (1998a) Language and diversity: The case of Catalonia. Paper
readat the OSCESeminar onLaunching the Oslo Recommendations onthe Linguistic
Rights of National Minorities, Vienna, February.
Strubell, M. (1998b) Can sociolinguistic change be planned? In Proceedings of the 1st
European Conference Private Foreign Language Education in Europe. Its contribution
to the Multilingual and Multicultural Aspect of the EuropeanUnion. Thessaloniki, No-
vember 1997 (pp. 2331). Palso, Thessaloniki.
Strubell i Trueta, M. (1999) Language, democracyanddevolutioninCatalonia. Cur-
rent Issues in Language and Society 5(3), 146180.
282 Europe
Subirats, M. (ed.) (1990) Enquesta metropolitana 1986. Condicions de vida i hbits de la
poblaci de lrea metropolitana de Barcelona. Vol. 20: Transmissi i coneixements de la
llengua catalana a lrea metropolitana de Barcelona. rea metropolitana de Barce-
lona. Bellaterra: Institut dEstudis Metropolitans.
Torres, J. (1994) Evoluci de ls del catal com a llengua familiar a Catalunya,
Treballs de Sociolingstica Catalana, nm. 12, 417.
Vallverd, F. (1990) Ls del catal: Un futur controvertit. Barcelona: Edicions 62.
Vallverd, F. (1998) Velles i noves qestions sociolingstiques. Barcelona: Edicions 62.
Vila, F.X. (1993) Transmissi dels idiomes en les parelles lingsticament mixtes.
Pubblicacions de lInstitut de Sociolingstica Catalana. Documents de Treball nm. 1.
Barcelona: Departament de Cultura de la Generalitat de Catalunya.
Voltas, E. (1996) La guerra de la llengua. Barcelona: Empries.
Catalan a Decade Later 283
Chapter 12
Saving Threatened Languages in
Africa: A Case Study of Oko
E. ADEGBIJA
Introduction
The threat of deathhangs over manylanguages inAfrica, Asia andLatin
America, whichhave anunenviable concentration of status-poor, officially
inconsequential andfunctionally emaciatedlanguages. All over the world,
such languages are differently christened: minority, small-group, lesser-
used, small population, etc. Sub-Saharan Africa alone has far more than
1700 languages and very little institutional attention is given to most of
them (cf. Adegbija, 1994: 1, 1997). Over 90% of African languages, there-
fore, exist as if they dont really exist; they live without being really alive.
Living functional blood is being sucked out of them because they are
consigned to low level functions in the national scheme of things.
This chapter attempts to relate and document the current inter-
generational transmission status in uses and in attitudes of languages in
Africa, using Oko, a Kwa language in Kogi State, Nigeria, as a specific case
study. The chapter demonstrates that while Oko seems to be doing quite
well in its own small village hinterland, this is not the case in larger cities
and in other regions to which Oko-speaking immigrants have moved.
Additionally, it will examine the 1991 intergenerational transmission
theory proposed in Fishman (1991), with respect, particularly, to its
adequacy or otherwise for the Africancontext. As a prelude to this primary
task, it is desirable first to investigate some of the principal causes of
language shift in the African context. Put differently, why are several
African languages under threat? Why are they likely to continue to be
endangered? And what are some of the strategies for reversing language
shift in the African context particularly?
For a proper understanding of why many African languages are
constantly under threat a proper context and background of their present
plight is a desideratum and our attention will now be focused on this.
284
Africa and Asia
A Case Study of Oko
Primary Sources of Threat to African Languages
The presence and prestige of ex-colonial languages
The very presence of European languages and the disproportional pres-
tige associated with them overtly and covertly by virtue of the dynamic
roles that they have played in national life since colonial times is a major
threat to African languages, which, functionally, become insignificant by
comparison. Ex-colonial languages began their lives in Africa as the
masters languages, andinmost parts of Africa have continuedtoenjoythis
enviable attitudinal posture. If anything, subsequent events all over the
African sociolinguistic scenario have only served to entrench their being
functionally placed on the pedestal, being attitudinally extolled, and being
seen as inevitable both at the individual and societal level. The educational
domainwas, andstill is, a major arena for the language shiftingpull. Part of
the evidence for this claim is that many children who have been to school
have completely shifted away from their mother-tongues to the English
language. In Nigeria, Ghana, Uganda, Kenya and many other African
countries colonised by the British, for instance, to be educated is virtually
synonymous with knowing and being able to use English. To be educated,
in this sense, also provides a green card for some degree of membership in
the power-brokering elite club. Membership in this club connotes being up
there whereas non-membership is tantamount almost invariably to being
down there. Given the fact that English has assumed many official roles in
education, administration, the mass media, the judiciary and politics, its
perceptual salience naturallyimpels andconstrains a shift towards it wher-
ever possible. The same can be said to be true of European languages such
as French and Portuguese in other African countries. To knowand be able
to use themmeans power and well-being: not to, means impotence, voice-
lessness and, often, social and economic ill-health. The official dominance
of ex-colonial languages is therefore a potent language shifting trigger
constantly pulled by the desire of every individual to rise on the vertical
and horizontal social and economic ladder.
The inferiority syndrome associated with Afrian languages
The official neglect of indigenous languages is another main source of
threat to the existence of African languages that triggers language shift in
African countries. Indigenous languages are officially considered un-
worthy of being used in official contexts because of their low develop-
mental status. Their perceived unworthiness increases year after year as
frontiers of knowledge continue to expand. This has been particularly so
withFrenchandPortuguese colonies, thoughsomewhat less true of British
A Case Study of Oko 285
and Belgian. The Portuguese and French deliberately trampled upon and
indeed wanted African languages dead, as they were considered obstacles
to their civilising mission. Indigenous languages were conferred with an
inferior status which, sadly, and due to the functions that indigenous
languages are palpably seen as performing or not performing, have
become accepted by many individual citizens of African countries. This
inferiority syndrome or complex associated with indigenous languages,
sadly, has stuck to the minds of many Africans, especially as far as indige-
nous languages are concerned in official domains, and so their impotence
in such domains virtually appears to have become institutionised and
canonised.
Officially and functionally recognised big or indegenous major
languages make smaller languages seem insignificant
Big-population, officially and functionally recognised languages in
Africa constitute a threat to small-population languages because their
speakers are the power brokers anddecision makers. They thus make deci-
sions that favour big languages and threaten the very existence of small
ones. Smaller languages are thereby made virtually irrelevant and func-
tionally impotent at the societal level andeven sometimes at the individual
level. There is consequently a pull towards the major languages. The more
powerful and functionally dominant the language, the more pressing its
attraction and pull and the greater the tendency to shift towards it, given
the fact that the pressure for social vertical mobility is virtually irresistible.
This seems quite obvious when we examine the stature of European
languages and of major African indigenous languages which have
attracted many speakers. In the northern part of Nigeria, many speakers of
other languages have been attracted to the Hausa language because of its
functional dominance and power. Virtually every African country has
regionally located major languages which pull and attract speakers of
smaller languages to themselves because of their perceptual salience, func-
tional dynamism and distinct ecological standing and weight in the
national scheme of things. For instance, Mali has Bambara and Fulfulde;
Gambia has Manding, Wolof andFulfulde; Ethiopia has Amharic, Tigrinya
and Galla; Congo Kinshasha (formerly Zaire) has Lingala, Luba, and
Kongo; Malawi has Nyanja andLomwe andYao; Uganda has Runyankore,
Luganda, Luo, Lugbara; Togo has Ewe and Kabiye; Benin has Fon-Ewe;
Yoruba and Bariba; Kenya has Kikuyu, Swahili, Luhya, Luo and Kamba;
Ghana has Dagbani, Ewe, Akan, Adangme, Nzema, Ga andDagaari, while
Nigeria has Hausa, Yoruba and Igbo. These major languages are like big
fish that deliberately go out to swallow up the smaller languages (cf.
286 Africa and Asia
Batibo, 1992 with reference to Kenya). Their functional dominance in the
national scheme of things dictates, willy nilly, that anyone who desires any
meaningful participation in national life must learn to use at least one of
them. Unhealthy rivalry between speakers of major languages also some-
times encourages a shift towards European languages, rather than vote for
(or subject oneself to the language of) an intimidating and supposedly
oppressive ethnolinguistic group. This does not mean, however, that any
kind of bilingual acquisition will necessarily result in language shift.
In such an atmosphere, smallness in number of speakers of a language
results in official neglect anddenial of developmental attention. Moreover,
it is often argued in many African countries that hunger, poverty, and war
constitute more pressing priorities than the development of small
languages, which, the argument normally goes, is contrary to the prag-
matic and administrative need for maintaining unity in a multilingual
polity. Moreover, undue attention to small languages, it is reasoned rather
innocently, could cut themoff frommainstreamlife. In most African coun-
tries, such logic has resulted in more and more attention being paid to
bigger languages, and less and less attention to lesser-used languages: the
rich thus become richer and the poor poorer. Many speakers of smaller
languages thus shift to Europeanlanguages or major languages simplyas a
survival ploy because they perceive that their own languages are threat-
ened by extinction.
Low emotional, intellectual and functional investment in languages
Low emotional, intellectual and functional investment in ones own
language is frequentlya dominant cause for shiftingtoanother language. A
quick examination of the bottom-line language attitude determinants in
sub-Saharan African countries will easily demonstrate why this has to be
so. Among such attitudinal determinants identified in Adegbija (1994:
6873) are: socio-historical forces relating to the African colonial heritage
and the consequent association of colonial languages with power; the
linguistic andsocial politics of complex andgreat multilingualism; the irre-
sistible quest andpressures for social mobilityinthe individual, oftenasso-
ciated with big languages and European languages; the functional
dynamics inherent inlanguages, demonstratedbythe fact that the prestige,
respect and the value judgements made concerning a language tend to
grow when a language is assigned significant functions and given official
recognition; individual and societal pressures to survive, which often
impinge on, and dictate attitudes towards particular languages. All such
attitudinal factors, heavily, and, on the aggregate, overweighted in favour
of European languages and big languages, tend to reduce the emotional,
A Case Study of Oko 287
intellectual, functional and loyalty stake of speakers of small languages in
their own languages, thereby diverting their attention, love, involvement
and loyalty in the direction of both European languages and major
languages, which are associated with achieving and being up there. Posi-
tive attitudinal stake in a language is a dominant factor in its maintenance
both at the individual and societal level. Conversely, attitudinal doldrums
withrespect toa particular language constitute the principal precipitator of
language shift.
The Sociolinguistic Atmosphere that puts Oko under Threat
The village of Oko, known to non-natives as Ogori, is inhabited by a
small ethno-linguistic group in Nigeria. Formerly in Kwara State, the
village has belongedtoKogi State since the state creationof 27 August 1991.
Along with Magongo, another small village about three kilometres away,
the people speak a very unique minority language, known to the native
speakers as Oko, to outsiders as Ogori, but referred to as Osanyin in
Magongo. The uniqueness of the Oko language lies in the fact that it shares
very little with the languages of the immediately surrounding neighbours
and also because only Ogori and Magongo (with perhaps less than 50,000
speakers, in all of Nigeria, and, most likely all over the world) speak this
language; in the Nigerian context, this very fact is a potential source of
threat.
An interesting fact about the Ogori-Magongo ethnic group is that
although they are surrounded by different and relatively powerful
language groups, they have, within their small village setting, stubbornly
and doggedly stuck to their culture and language because of their pride in
their language and have resisted every attempt, covert or overt, to assimi-
late them. Politically and for administrative convenience, most Nigerian
governments have found it convenient to group Ogori-Magongo with the
Okene Local Government area until 1996, whenthey were awardeda Local
Government Area with the name Apafa Magongo. This has increased
contact with the Ebira, a much larger ethnic and language group that has
often attempted to solidarise with the Ogori people, identifying them as
part of Ebira. This has resulted in intermarriages. Moreover, many Oko
people have learned to use the Ebira language in the attempt to interact
with their Ebira neighbours. On the other hand, very fewEbira people ever
learn how to use the Oko language because they see no need for it. This
study is based on participant observation among the Ogori people as well
as on informal interviews and interactions with native speakers of the Oko
language.
288 Africa and Asia
Although no Yoruba community exists in the immediate neighbour-
hood, many Oko people, intriguingly, and to the surprise of most neigh-
bours, speak or understand Yoruba. It is sometimes considered a thing of
pride for anOkopersontobe able tospeakYoruba because it is one of Nige-
rias three major functionally and officially recognised languages. Many
non-Ogorians, have, in fact constantly wondered howOko people manage
to be able to speak Yoruba. Yoruba forenames and surnames are also very
common among Oko indigenes. Also, Yoruba is spoken along with Oko in
the church. Pastors posted to Ogori by the Anglican Mission have been
Yoruba-speaking. Thus, the reading of the Bible often occurs in Yoruba
even though simultaneous translation into Oko while reading is occasion-
ally done as well. The practice of reading the Bible in Yoruba constitutes a
threat to Oko in the religious domain. Most of the local people who are able
to read and need a Bible invariably buy a Yoruba Bible because there is
none in Oko as yet. Moreover, the utility of a Yoruba Bible in the church
context makes possession of a Yoruba Bible desirable. The church is thus
the chief source of the transmission of the Yoruba language among the
Ogori people. In addition, however, the youths and the educated elite in
general have oftenhadone cause or another to interact withthe Yorubas by
virtue of Ogori being a part of the former Kwara State, which had a very
large concentration of speakers of Yoruba. Songs in churches are rendered
in both Yoruba and Oko. Sermons are also frequently delivered in Yoruba,
simply because many of the vicars cannot speak the Oko language. The use
of Yoruba is beginning to infiltrate many Oko homes, though it is still
uncommon in others. Children reared outside the community are particu-
larly disadvantaged as far as speaking Oko is concerned because many of
themreturn to Oko, not being able to communicate with their kith and kin
who are not competent in the Yoruba language. Such children also have
difficulty in interacting with other children in the Oko community. In the
homes of such children, parents often have to speak either Yoruba or
English even when they are within the Oko community. When these chil-
dren also grow up and have children of their own, they invariably speak
Yoruba or English, learned at school, to their own children even in their
homes. The major threat to the Oko language, therefore, is among the
younger generation who, invariably, have to emigrate to urban centres
because jobs are not available within the Oko village setting. In such urban
settings, there is no environmental enrichment for the use of the Oko
language amongchildren. Functionally, it is alsoseentobe irrelevant inthe
daily chores. Friends and acquaintances cannot speak it, it is not used in
schools, it is not neededininteractionwithpeers, andeveninmany homes,
many parents would rather speak English, in order to give their children a
A Case Study of Oko 289
headstart inthe educational domain, thanspeakOko, whichis perceivedto
be of no major utilitarian value.
Internally and for administrative convenience, the Ogori district is
dividedintosevensub-districts: Eni, Okesi, Udoba, Oshobane, Okigbo, Udo,
Ileteju. Ebira is spoken by many people in the Oshobane sub-district of
Ogori. It was principallly initially used in trade interactions with the Ebira
people. There have been some inter-marriages between Ebira and Oko
people and most people involved in such intermarriages have settled in
Oshobanedistrict, thus resultinginaheavypresenceof theEbiralanguagein
that sub-district of Ogori, and the transmission of Ebira there. This consti-
tutes a big threat to the Oko language in such districts. However, the Ebira
threat is not greater thanthe threat fromEnglishbecause Ebira does not have
the national status that Englishhas. It is alsonot quite relevant inthe educa-
tional process.The utilitarianvalue of Ebira intrade links, ininterpersonal
interactions with Ebira people, in communicating within some homes
has resulted in the language becoming more dominant among many Oko-
speaking people even within the Oko community. Frequent interactions
with the Ebiras has also been common over the years, especially because
Ogori belonged in the past to the Okene local government area.
Another language that is perceptually salient, and thus constitutes a big
threat to the Oko language is English. In official contexts, it is non-pareil.
Many youths take pride inbeing able to speak English. Infact, they use it to
exclude their parents from several conversations. Ability to speak English
competentlyis a reflectionof peer grouppride inOgori. The highregardfor
English is heightened by its international stature, the functional load it
carries withinNigeriansociety, andthe fact that it is requiredininterethnic
interaction. English is also required for vertical upward social mobility. To
achieve and be considered an achiever in the Oko community, one must be
able to speak English. In most official domains, most Oko people, espe-
cially youths, are virtually incapable of functioning in the Oko language.
The English language thus constitutes a major threat to the development of
the Oko language.
Every Sunday morning, there is a worship service at the Anglican
Church conducted in English. It is attended principally by youths and
foreigners in the Ogori community. Ogori students also frequently speak
English, both to display their achievement of the educated status and to
exclude from interactions some of their fellow Ogorians. Except for
isolated instances, English is not commonly heard in homes. Those of the
secondary school generation may, however, occasionally speak it among
themselves in interpersonal interactions just to boost their ego. The
working educatedelite in the Oko village setting seemto insist often on the
290 Africa and Asia
use of the Oko language, in which they take great pride. However, their
counterparts that have emigrated to urban centres in search of jobs are
unable to do so because their children are beginning to lose competence in
the Oko language.
English, as anofficial language inNigeria, carries withit a lot of prestige;
Yoruba, as a language of a major ethnic group, is also very conspicuous in
Nigeria; Ebira is the language of a former governing ethnic group that is
also numerically superior to Oko. These are formidable ethnolinguistic
forces that have remained a threat to the Oko competence of people of the
younger generation. While Oko youths living within the village often
appear to be able to weather such competition, their counterparts in urban
centres have not been so lucky as they. The situation is so bad that some of
them completely lose their competence in the Oko language. When such
youths get married to non-Oko speaking spouses, the Oko language
completely dies off in their own generation. Sometimes, Oko speakers are
mistaken for people belonging to the Yoruba ethnic groups because they
speak more of Yoruba than the Oko language.
Oko and the GIDS Scale
Inthis section, weshall attempt todocument thecurrent intergenerational
transmission status in overt uses and attitudes of Oko, using the GIDS scale
(Fishman, 1991) insofar as is possible, as a point of departure.
Inhis attempt to formulate a theoryfor RLS, Fishman(1991: 817) makes
the very important point that RLS involves a prior value consensus among
those who advocate, formulate, implement and evaluate it. Without such
prior consensus, RLSpolicy may become a bone of contentionevenamong its own
advocates (Fishman, 1991: 82). He thenproposes a GradedIntergenerational
Disruption Scale) typology of threatened statuses, on analogy of the
Richter Scale measures of the intensity of earthquakes. High numbers are
intended to indicate stronger tremors or greater disruption of the estab-
lished or normal geological strata and are, accordingly of greater threat to
those living in the vicinity of the quake. Similarly, in the sociolinguistic
disruption scale higher numbers are intended to imply greater disruption
andtherefore, more fundamental threat tothe prospects for the language to
be handed on intergenerationally (Fishman, 1991: 87). The GIDS,
according to Fishman, is a quasi-implicational scale in that higher (more
disrupted cores imply all or nearly all of the lesser degrees of disruption as
well (p. 87). We shall now attempt to evaluate the current inter-
generational transmission status of Oko in particular and African lan-
guages in general on the basis of this scale.
A Case Study of Oko 291
Stage 8 on the GIDS (Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale)
Most vestigial users of Xish are socially isolated old folks and Xish needs to
be reassembled fromtheir mouths and memories and taught to
demographically unconcentrated adults
Ohiri-Aniche (1997: 75), observes that many Nigerian languages are
dying because of the trendto bring upchildren as monolingual speakers of
English. This is
exerting a heavy affective price in that such children now hold our
languages in disdain andfeel ashamedto be associatedwith them. The
situation is now such that in many urban schools even those children
who understand local languages will pretend not to. Otherwise, they
speak such languages at the peril of being mocked and jeered at by
their peers . . . We needtopayattentiontothe damage beingdone tothe
psyche of Nigerian children who now are neither wholly European
nor Nigerian while also giving thought to the social malaise that this
confusion of identities is engendering.
She continues by observing that the great danger inherent in this phenom-
enon is that
when this generation of children become parents, they will not be able
to pass on these languages to their own children. The result is that in a
few generations, as the old people who know these languages die off,
the languages also decline and eventually become extinct. This is the
fate that has befallen many formerly flourishing languages of the
world, the best examples being the native Indian languages of North
and South America. (p. 75).
Some of the Nigerian languages cited as suffering such a fate, formerly
spoken by some northern communities in Nigeria, include: Ashaganna, Feli
of Baissa, Shirama, Ajanci, and Basa-Gumna. Others mentioned are Bassa
Kontagora, with only 10 speakers left in 1987. An American linguist, Ron
Shaefer, was also reported to have raised an alarm over Emai, spoken by a
small communityinEdoState, intheThis Daynewspaper of 31March1997:
Emai language and the 30 different languages spoken in the area,
would probably be dead by the year 2050 as none can serve as a lingua
franca and are therefore being supplanted by English. (cf. Ohiri-
Aniche, 1997: 75).
Ohiri-Aniche further reports a 1990 study in which she discovered that
among the Primary and Secondary School children studied in five states in
292 Africa and Asia
Nigeria, a growing number are becoming non-speakers of local languages.
The 1990 study revealed that among the Junior Secondary School students
in five Nigerian states, those shown in Table 12.1 did not speak the ances-
tral language of their parents.
It is not surprising that minority languages have the largest percentages
of students not speaking their local languages. Rivers State, for instance,
has about 33 languages and most of them are minority languages. Ohiri-
Aniche cites Afisimania who, in a 1991 doctoral dissertation, discovered
that whereas 5% of educated adults interviewed in the state could not
speak their local language, the figure had jumped to between 31.3% and
40.8%among the children of these adults and that this is a very significant
difference in only one generation (Ohiri-Aniche 1997: 74). Given the
dynamic role of European languages in education and their unique poten-
tial for pullingspeakers of minoritylanguages inparticular, it is most likely
that the situationinAfricaas awhole will not be muchdifferent fromthis.
With respect to Stage 8 of GIDS, Oko, within the village setting, is not
anywhere near this stage as a society or community of language speakers.
However, this stage of GIDS might be considered somewhat applicable to
some families of Oko speakers, especially those who have emigrated to
urban centres. Among some of these people, neither the parents nor the
childrencanspeakthe Okolanguage anylonger. Generally, manyspeakers
of African languages living in urban centres have completely abandoned
their native languages andperhaps have lost contact withtheir kithandkin
back home and shifted to the European language or a major indigenous
language. For such categories of individuals, no trace of the language will
probably be found within a few years.
Stage 7 on the GIDS
Most users of Xish are socially integrated and ethnolinguistically active
population but they are beyond child-bearing age
This stage is not applicable to Oko in the village setting andas a commu-
nity in totality. It is, of course, quite applicable to individual families that
have migrated to urban centres and visit Oko only very occasionally.
A Case Study of Oko 293
Table 12.1 Percentage of JSS students not speaking their local language
Lagos
State
(Yoruba)
Imo
State
(Igbo)
Kano
State
(Hausa)
Gongola State
(Minority
Languages)
Rivers State
(Minority
Languages)
Average
(Nigerian
Languages)
2% 8% 1% 8% 14% 7%
Languages like Emai, referred to earlier, may be at this stage of the GIDS.
The same may be true of Bassa-Kontagora, reported to have only 10
speakers in 1987.
Undoubtedly, many African languages are at Stages 8 and 7 of the GIDS
and their survival is seriously threatened unless urgent RLS measures are
taken.
Stage 6 on the GIDS
The attainment of intergenerational informal oralcy and its demographic
concentration and institutional reinforcement
Oko, just like many of Africas languages, may be placed on this stage of
the GIDSwithinthe Okovillage setting. The family, withinOgori itself, and
even among native speakers of Oko living in urban centres, may be said to
be the locus and nerve centre of intergenerational transmission of the Oko
language. However, the familyas a transmissionagencyof Okois seriously
under threat, especially among Oko people in urban centres because
people of the younger generation find no valid reason for continuing to
speak the language in urban centres. Often, they have no one they can
speak it to. Their parents do not speak it to them within the home setting
because they want them to do well at school. Their peers do not knowOko and
so interaction with them is often in English or in the major Nigerian
language spoken in the particular urban centre. Several strategies may be
employed to assist these non-retentive sub-groups in the promotion of the
Oko language or for ensuring the transmission of the Oko language. Else-
where, we have referred to these RLS devices as survival strategies (cf.
Adegbija, 1993a). Such RLS strategies are indeed of great importance
because there are over 400 languages in Nigeria. The Nigerian government
has not even given the officially designated three celebrity languages,
Hausa, Yoruba andIgbo, the kindof developmental attentiontheydeserve.
The hope of languages on the lower rung of the minority ladder, because of
numerical inferiority, is therefore a very meagre one. Internal efforts of the
Oko people are needed to blowtheir own trumpet culturally and linguisti-
cally and such efforts are therefore of immense sociolinguistic and RLS
interest. Internally motivated RLS activists are usually immune to the
apathy that commonly befalls projects that have a governmental tagging.
Members of the Okocommunityinurbancentres needtotake their cultural
and linguistic destinies into their own hands. Such RLS efforts, anchored
withina particular Oko-speakingcommunityinanurbancentre andwitha
strong family base, have been grouped by Adegbija (1993a) into three
types: cultural, linguistic or language related and political, with the first
294 Africa and Asia
two being more critical in terms of their being totally internally generated
and controlled.
How Non-Retentive Oko Sub-groups can be Assisted in
Terms of RLS
Cultural RLS strategies in non-retentive Oko communities
The annual Ovia Osese Festival is perhaps one of the most potent and
perceptually salient device for the promotion, maintenance and transmis-
sion of the Oko language. With its base in individual families, it has, over
the years, metamorphosed from a quietly celebrated local festival into a
gargantuan cultural and linguistic carnival of state and national repute. It
has often attracted the attention of the Nigerian Television Authority both
at the state and local levels. Through the festival, the Oko people not only
advertise, maintain and preserve their language and culture, but also cele-
brate and preserve their local prominence and their very existence as a
cultural and linguistic minority. Its focus is on the initiation of girls into
womanhood but it touches the very chords or okoness. The occasion
brings to Oko many important dignitaries and there is emphasis on the
oke song, with its sonorous eeeeeeeee refrain. The occasion is markedby
dancing, drumming and the singing of numerous songs in the Oko
language. Duringthe festival, the Oko people nurture andcelebrate a sense
of community and belongingness to a unique ethnolinguistic entity.
Several family and community preparations precede the Ovia festival.
These include the rendition of Eregba songs, the preparations of the
maidens attaining womanhood through informal and formal as well as
verbal and non-verbal interactions with parents on the values of woman-
hood; Ohenhenne, that is, the singingor recitingof the praise names of the
clan to which a particular family that has a child celebrating the woman-
hoodfestival belongs, etc. The Eregba songs directly involve the meninthe
Ovia festival, since the Eregba artists, and a majority of its audience, are
typically men. However, women also join Eregba song groups as new
songs are created in the Oko language that are relevant to the history,
culture, social life andthe very essence and raison dtre of the Oko commu-
nity. Fromthis perspective, the Ovia festival constitutes a potent language
maintenance and RLS device.
Unfortunately, however, many Oko sub-groups that have emigrated to
urban areas have completely lost touch with the Ovia Osese festival. These
sub-groups cease to practise the rituals and observances described above.
For some, affinity with Christianity has made themto look down on many
Oko cultural practices, which are considered unholy, immoral or ungodly.
A Case Study of Oko 295
Children in such family sub-groups growup not knowing anything about
the Ovia festival, and not being able to participate in the numerous rituals
and cultural observances that perpetuate, promote and entrench the Oko
language. Whenthe childrenof suchparents returnto the Oko community,
they are unable to communicate with their kith and kin. Although most
festivals are of limited duration and constitute a departure from ordinary
daily life, they are so effective in RLS because they bring the entire commu-
nitytogether as one andcreate pleasant, long-lastingimpressions about the
togetherness and the very essence of the Oko community as an
ethnolinguistic group.
Most clans in sub-districts within Ogori have their Ohenhenne or praise
song, which is usually poetic in nature, as illustrated by the following
praise song of the Okpowu clan, with family members clustered in Okesi,
Ileteju, Udoba and Eni districts:
Oko English Gloss
Okpowu ro egben Children of Okpowu
Alewere were (no gloss: connotation of
brightness, radiance and glory
Ecincin era The fire of chaff
Ba ka ewu, ede jin yeeen When attempting to quench it, it
opens up and burns even much
brighter
The above ohenhenne is used by the Okpowu clan and in individual
families belonging to the clan for self-adulation and self-identification on
festive occasions, at moments of crisis, and at moments of great achieve-
ments. In doing this, the Oko language is glorified, the culture it represents
is exalted and the togetherness and identity as members of the micro-
group, Okpowu clan, within the macro-culture, Oko, are emphasised,
magnifiedandaccentuated. This practice is passedon fromone generation
to another in individual families. Each clan has its praise song, frequently
remembered in individual families and constantly recited in frequent
festive moods and occasions.
RLS festivities in Oko are by no means limited to the Ovia festival
which, one must admit, is non-pareil. Ogben esen (a ceremony that
heralds the birth of a new baby, especially the very first) is another occa-
sion in which every family can share a part at one time or the other. It also
is a carnival experienced by verbal creativity and cultural awareness.
296 Africa and Asia
After the first child is born by a woman, married to a member of the Oko
community, women sing songs and dance the distinctive rhythm of the
Ogbenesen dance fromhouse to house greeting each other as they join the
smallest finger of the left hand and shout Odiororo, an exclamation of
excitement insharing the joys of a fellowmember of the Oko community in
contributing to the perpetuation of the community and a sign of gratitude
to Osibina (God), the giver of all good gifts.
Eregba, Ogun, Erimi and other related cultural songs are used to honour
the departure (death) of aged members of the community. They are also
common when traditional titles such as Osiako; Uboro, Otaru, etc. are
to be conferredon Oko indigenes who have made significant contributions
to the growth and development of the community. In such festivals, prov-
erbs, which normally punctuate day-to-day interpersonal interactions, are
freely quoted and used and many children growing up in the community
are able to learn their use in appropriate contexts.
In the Oko village setting, the frequent telling of Ogarega or folktales
during moonlit nights is another RLS intergenerational transmission
device which the non-retentive sub-groups in urban centres could prac-
tise as a strategy for promoting the Oko language. Games like alele
puepue (a blindfolding game); eba, a traditional type of chess game,
and Eto fomaya (where are you taking me to); gbio or riddles, which
also serve as powerful means for transmitting Oko from parents to chil-
dren, wouldbe of interest to the children of parents in these non-retentive
sub-groups andit wouldencourage andenable them, inurbansettings, to
observe more Oko cultural traits within their own homes and communi-
ties. The Ogori Descendants Union (ODU), which has branches in most
urban centres in Nigeria where native speakers of Oko are found, is
another language and cultral transmission organ. In fact, its primary
purpose is to promote the identity and culture of the Ogori people. Thus,
as an unwritten statute, its meetings, both in Ogori and in all the branches
spread throughout Nigeria, are held through the medium of the Oko
language. Unfortunately, however, inmany urbansettings, only very few
natives of Oko attendthese meetings that couldbe very useful as far as the
promotion and transmission of the Oko language is concerned. In fact,
young people rarely frequent such meetings, which are held largely
among people of the older generation. Such customs and traditions are
not passed on to the children of parents who have emigrated to urban
centres. The rate of emigration will likely continue to increase because of
better amenities, better jobs, and a more attractive modern life-style for
Oko youths in urban centres.
A Case Study of Oko 297
Linguistic or language-based RLS strategies for non-retentive sub-
groups
Adegbija (1993b: 9) makes the following pertinent observation:
Globally, language is the dynamite of thought. If you kill or render
impotent a peoples language, youcanas well kill the people, but if you
effectively plan or develop a peoples language, you are laying trea-
sures that can affect eternity and providing the people with tools of
self-actualization, self-identity and self-promotion.
Commenting specifically on the Nigerian context, Agheyisi (1984: 234)
makes the followingunfortunatelyvalidobservation: Language planning,
as a systematic programme of development has remained largely periph-
eral to the mainstream of national planning. Language planning can help
in saving threatened languages because of its potential for focusing atten-
tion on threatened languages and ensuring their continued use. We have
observed earlier on that if lack of attention applies to the so-called major
Nigerian languages, then it applies even more to the acutely minor
minority languages such as Oko, andany language group that will survive
or succeed in reversing language shift may do so by the skin of its teeth. In
essence, those who speak that language have to show an active and
dynamic interest in planning for the existence and survival of their own
language. This kind of take-your-own destiny in your own hands attitude
has again been displayed by the Oko community in its own small-village
hinterland with respect to corpus planning and language promotion (cf.
Rubagumya, 1991). Encouragement of linguistic studies on different
aspects of Oko constitutes a specfic aspect of corpus planning. Chumbow
(1982) did an autosegmental analysis of vowel harmony in Oko, and was
very much encouraged by native speakers of the language who served as
informants. He, in turn, encouraged several undergraduate students to do
their undergraduate projects on the Oko language. Adegbija (1989) did a
study on aspects of the pragmatics of Oko. Adegbija (1993b) also focused
on The graphicization of a small-group language: A case study of Oko.
Earlier on in the yearning to have a writing system for the language, an
orthography of the language was devised by some non-linguist indigenes,
examined with keen interest by the ODUand sent to linguists for scrutiny.
The revision done on it has subsequently resultedin the preparation of try-
out texts in the language, some original creations, others a translation of
general and familiar texts such as Osibina Adura (Our Lords Prayer)
which is frequently recited in the homes of many Ogori Christians. As
Adegbija (1993b: 153) remarks:
298 Africa and Asia
The graphicization of a language brings that language into fellowship
with the worlds community of languages that can do things, that can
achieve, and that can tread in realms where the spoken language is
incompetent to tread. It constitutes a basic prerequisite for the use of a
language in education, in print, and in other realms of language func-
tionality.
For a long time, Asekee, a magazine in the Oko language, was published by
the Ogori Students Association. The word Asekee connotes listen, there
is an important message. It is normally used when the town crier beats his
gong round the town to announce an important message, often from the
Ologori of Ogori, the village head, or one of the titled chiefs.
Also of interest in terms of its potential for RLS is the current trendin the
patterns of naming among some Ogori people. Before the 1970s it was
common for children to be given Yoruba first names such as Kayode,
Oluwole, Ayo, Taiyewo, Kehinde, Ayodeji, Olorunfemi, etc. Infact, Yoruba
surnames such as Abisoye, Akerejola, Atere, Aje, Adegbija, Asiribo,
Abanikanda, etc., which are normally difficult to change because of the
implications of changingsurnames, are still verycommonandwere almost
the normtoo. It is not surprising, therefore, to see people of the older gener-
ation answering to Yoruba forenames and surnames. This onomastic
process has resultedinprobing into andconsiderably enriching the lexicon
of the Oko language recently. Although a few children, even in the past,
were given distinct Oko names such as Osigbodi, Osebekwin, Osekafore,
Oreka, etc., the trend of Oko-naming has become much more common and
has been inherited by members of the newgeneration of Ogorians. Hence,
vivid and striking new names in the language have been created. We now
find many more individuals carrying very original distinctive and typi-
cally Okoish names such as Titiosibina, Tosibinaronmuro, Amadosibina,
Osibinadinfenyan, Mosiefilemu, Igule, etc. Our investigations revealed
that the trend in the giving of Oko names began in the 1960s and appeared
to have increased in scope as a deliberate ethnic identity and RLS or
language promotion strategy. Then, many of those rebelling against for-
eign names had no children. Consequently, they demonstrated their reac-
tion against non-Oko names given to them by their parents by giving
themselves newly created or existing Oko names, different from the
Yoruba or biblical names given to themby their parents. Some of these are
illustrated by the instances shown in Table 12.2.
Today, most members of the Oko community living in Ogori tend to
follow this pattern of naming. Unfortunately, however, this cannot be
totally said to be equally true of those living in urban centres, many of
A Case Study of Oko 299
whom prefer Yoruba or biblical names. While the Yoruba names conform
with the societies they live in, the biblical or Hebrewnames are in line with
the religion they practice. As in many other Nigerian and African commu-
nities, every name has a meaning relevant to the personal, interactions
history, culture and life of the people. As the Oko-speaking community
withinthe village is concerned, therefore, there is everything ina name and
this explains the desire to ensure that children have Okoish names that
have a strong and palpable bearing on the maintenance, promotion and
perpetuation of the Oko language and culture. Every child with an Oko
name will, at the very least, be always reminded of his linguistic back-
ground and identity whenever the name is written or called. The giving of
indigenous Okoish names bearing sociocultural import is therefore an
indirect means for ensuring that the children remember their origin and
that the language remains prominent, for we carry our names with all their
connotations with us wherever we go. In essence, far from being a mere
identity tag, naming for the Oko people, now, constitutes a means of audio
and mobile culture and language promotion, and hence, subtly, an RLS
effort. The Oko speaking sub-groups in urban centres would also need to
apply such linguistic strategies for reversing language shift. Unfortu-
nately, hard economic conditions have increased the barriers between
those in urban settings and those in the Ogori village. Consequently,
contact with the Oko language is being reduced both among the older and
younger generationof Okospeakingsub-groups inurbancentres. Whereas
in the past, they visited home during festivals, and quite frequently, it has
now become commonplace, for economic reasons, in particular, for many
families not to have any contact with their kith and kin in the villages for
several years on end. Space is also creating language distance and
intergenerational transmission of the Oko language is the worse for it. In
view of the increasing rate of emigration in search of jobs and a more
prestigeous life-style, this distance is likely to continue to increase.
300 Africa and Asia
Table 12.2 Newly created names
Old Name Self-given Okoish names
Phillip (biblical source) Mekamagba (Its mine that I see)
Ebenezer (biblical source) Osebekwin (to God belongs thanks or kneeling)
Joshua (biblical source) Imunefan (I ran and escaped)
Olorunfemi (Yoruba source) Efurosibina (connotes outside God Im helpless)
Victoria (English source) Mosiforeba (my God has conquered them)
Politically-oriented RLS strategies for non-retentive sub-groups
Behind every successful RLS programme there is often a subtle and
successful political manoeuvring at the individual, local, regional or
national level or, sometimes, at all the levels combined. This is because
language planning and the functional allocation to languages in multilin-
gual contexts always have sharp political cutting edges. In the Ogori
community, the politics behind the promotion of the Ogori language is
played both within the community itself, to convert the uninitiated, and
outside it. It also occurs at the individual, family, local, regional and
national levels. For instance, at family levels within the Ogori village itself,
childrenare frequently appealedto about the needto speak their language,
to know it, to be proud of it and to get used to it. Fathers and mothers
frequently engage in this kind of family politics as an RLS device. Through
socialisationandinformal educationat home, this strategycanbe extended
to Ogori indigenes in urban centres whose children stand a greater risk of
losing their competence in the Oko language because they have no daily
access toit, andcannot findanyutilitarianvalue inusingit inurbancentres.
Children could be taught and encouraged from their earlier years to, for
instance, pray inthe language, interact witheachother inthe language ona
daily basis, and to always insist on maintaining an Okoish identity wher-
ever they go. Parents should encourage their children to interact with each
other in the Oko language, especially in the home setting. Individual
members of the Oko community, on their part, should use every opportu-
nity to announce to interlocutors that they belong to the Oko community,
and that their language is quite different and distinct from Yoruba, or
Ebira, the larger ethnolinguistic community around themwith which they
are commonly linked.
At the regional and national levels, the politics behind the language
should be played with greater dexterity and efficiency, principally by
members of the ODU, which has branches all over Nigeria. Its members
should meet more regularly and national meetings should be held more
faithfully. Wranglings over chieftaincy affairs by rival clans have recently
crippledthe power of the ODU, whichhas beenthe major political organof
the Oko people. Such feuds among different clans laying claim to chief-
taincy have distracted attention from the growth and development of the
Oko language. Rival clans have taken each other to court and the entire
village has been divided accordingly. This has made it difficult for
members of the community to act as one force and has been a great draw-
back for interest in, and the development of the Oko language.
Political wranglings among rival clans have also reduced the publicity
A Case Study of Oko 301
that the Oko language normally enjoys during the Ovia and other festivals
within the community. This has been a great setback for Ogori identity and
the promotion of the Oko language. The reversal of ethnicity annihilation
andof language shift, previouslycreatedbypolitical togetherness, has thus
been seriously jeopardised, compromised and undermined.
Stage 5 on the GIDS
Xish literacy in home, school and community, but without taking extra-
communal reinforcement of such literacy
Not many African languages have reached this level on the GIDS scale.
Our language, Oko, certainly has not. Very few are literate in Oko, as its
orthography is not yet very well publicised and so has not been learned by
many of the indigenes. Moreover, the internal political wranglings over
chieftaincy, referred to above, have constituted a serious setback for the
marketing of the orthography and RLS efforts in general.
Nigerian languages such as Yoruba, Hausa, Igbo, may be said to have
reached this level to a very limited extent. Limited because literacy in the
languages among native speakers is at a very low rate, especially when
compared with literacy in the English language. Even many speakers of the
Yoruba language cannot read it. The same is true of virtually every other
major African language. One principal reason why many African languages
have not attainedthis level is that literacy in themis not requiredas a key to
social mobility and to competitiveness in the modern work sphere
(Fishman, 1991: 96). In most African countries, except perhaps in Tanzania,
to some degree, social mobility is possible only with literacy in the language
of the former colonial master. Most African languages, for instance, have no
newspapers in them. Of all languages in Nigeria, only Hausa, Yoruba and
Igbo have newspapers published in them, and these are not published
regularly or daily in all three languages. When literacy in an African
language is conducted at the local level, participants often cannot see its
functional value or utility and so tend eventually to get discouraged and
give up the attempt at getting literate in the indigenous language. Many
prefer literacy in the European languages, which make one to be counted
among those who know book (i.e. the educated or the knowledgeable).
Stage 4 on the GIDS
Xish in lower education (types a and b) that meets the requirements of
compulsory education laws
Although the Nigerian National Policy on Education stipulates that the
mother-tongues be used as media of public education at the lower levels of
302 Africa and Asia
education, very few schools follow this stipulation and no governmental
sanctions have been applied on the majority of schools flouting it.
Governmental inertia with regard to applying sanctions could be
because of awareness that a majority of languages have not been prepared
for use in education at any level because no orthographies have been
devised for them. Unofficially, and in reality, however, many teachers
often discover that their pupils at the lower levels of education can neither
understand them nor follow explanations in English and so have to carry
out explanations inthe indigenous languages. This observationmost likely
cuts across many African countries. In essence, most African languages
have not even gained co-control of the lower . . . educational and lower
work domains officially. In fact, in many schools all over Africa, children
are frequently penalised for speaking their mother-tongues, and not the
language of the former colonial master, in the school environment. Conse-
quently, children grow up cultivating the attitude of subtle distaste or
outright hatred for their mother-tongues in the educational domain. Even
if there is no hatred, the clear message being sent across, which affects the
attitude built round the indigenous languages throughout life, and is very
evident in the attitude of most Africans, is about the irrelevance, the lack of
consequence, and the impotence of the indigenous languages for educa-
tional purposes. Given the fact that over 80% of African languages as yet
have no orthographies, the prospect of their use at the lower rungs of the
educational ladder remains very dim indeed in spite of the educational
policies in many countries stipulating that they be so used.
Stage 3 on the GIDS
Use of Xish in the lower work sphere (outside of the Xish neighbourhood
community) involving interaction between Xmen and Ymen
Again, Oko is not used in the lower work sphere. A majority of African
languages similarly, are yet to arrive at this level. Whereas it is true that
speakers of the same African language will most likely select to interact
with each other in their mother-tongues even when they are in other
communities in the lower work sphere, there would be no need, or rather
no opportunity to use Xish outside of the neighbourhood community in
interactions between Xmen and Ymen, simply because most Ymen prob-
ably do not understand Xish.
In most informal interactions even outside the neighbourhood, Xmen
would most likely interact with each other in Xish. Thus, Oko people
within the Ogori village would probably interact using the Oko language,
both at the lower and higher work sphere in informal contexts and situa-
A Case Study of Oko 303
tions. This example needs to be copied by their counterparts in urban
centres. In formal contexts, however, Oko is completely endangered
because it is not used at all. Instead, English is most frequently used in
formal interactions. The picture, in varying degrees in different countries,
is virtually the same: namely, that indigenous languages are used in
informal interactions even at the work sphere, and the language of the ex-
colonial master is used in formal interactions and for official purposes
whenever interactions with speakers of other languages are involved.
Stage 2 on the GIDS
Xish in lower governmental services and mass media but not higher
spheres of either
Of all Nigerian languages, perhaps only Hausa, Yoruba and Igbo could
be said to partially be at this stage on GIDS in that they have some newspa-
pers being publishedin them. But they are not officially usedeven in lower
governmental service even though, in reality, they function most in
informal situations, just as is the case with most other indigenous
languages. The principal difference between these three major languages
and other Nigerian languages is that some formof written communication
does occur in them at the lower governmental services, even though these
could be rather sporadic and uncoordinated. They are used in political
campaigns, advertisements, commercial jingles, etc. perhaps more fre-
quently than other indigenous languages.
Stage 1 on the GIDS
Some use of Xish in higher level educational, occupational, governmental
and media efforts (but without the additional safety provided by political
independence)
Only very few major African languages have attained this stage on the
GIDS scale. InNigeria, Hausa, Yoruba andIgbo certainly are partially used
in higher education, but mainly as subjects in the curriculumfor those who
wish to study them. For such people as well, they would often be used as
medium in courses relating to the specific languages. Kiswahili in
Tanzania, perhaps also in Kenya and Uganda, may also be said to have to
some degree attained this level since, in Tanzania, for instance, it is often
used as a medium of instruction in some aspects of higher education.
Somali in Somalia is also used in some aspects of higher educational, occu-
pational, governmental andmass media activities. Inmost previous French
andPortuguese colonies, e.g. Togo, Benin, Angola, etc. the journeytowards
this stage of GIDS is still a very tortuous and bleak one. In general, perhaps
304 Africa and Asia
over 95%of Africanlanguages have not yet reachedthis stage andare most
unlikely to, given the present language planning postures and attitudes
towards small languages within the continent.
Desirable RLS Strategies in Africa
Before concluding this paper, it would be in order to examine some
specific RLS strategies that are particularly suitable and desirable for the
African context. Adegbija (1997) proposes some concrete strategies for
combatting RLS problems bedevilling the African continent in particular,
which could also be relevant for, and have deep implications for other
contexts in which RLS is either a major preoccupation, or is considered
desirable. The strategies are intended for promoting and ensuring the
survival of small languages and for combatting some of the problems
responsible for their neglect. These strategies will now be briefly consid-
ered:
A strong basic commitment and developmental RLS philosophy
Such a philosophy: that all languages, no matter the number of their
speakers, qualify for and should be given a chance to survive and grow to
their optimum potential without being stifled by government policy
actions, is a desideratumfor RLS. Where there is first the will, a way can be
found and nagging obstacles can be overcome and boldly confronted.
Policies shouldbe formulatedthat will require speakers of particular indig-
enous languages, at least, to be literate in their mother-tongues. Teachers
would need to be trained to make this possible. In essence, just as vital
economic resources, all languages should be treasured. There should be a
vision to rescue all languages from extinction, death or the endangered
languages list.
The establishment of national and local language-development
coordinating bodies, committees or agencies
In every context, an RLS committee or body is required to assume
responsibility for RLS. When a responsibility belongs to nobody, nobody
will feel committedtoit. Of course coordinatingbodies are desirable for the
formulation of policies rather than to stifle local and internally generated
development efforts. Anational agency is also required to orchestrate and
encourage the reversal of language shift.
A Case Study of Oko 305
Deep involvement of the language communities concerned in
development efforts
An Oko proverb is germane to this: When you do not attempt to carry
your own load, you cannot expect assistance from anyone. Another says:
Only a child that knows how to wash his hands can eat with elders. This
proverbial hand washing is required by all communities desiring the
reversal of language shift. Small language groups need to take their own
destinies in their own hands before expecting to cross the periphery,
oblivion anddamnation hurdles andenter the crookedandnarrowroadto
salvation (Adegbija, 1997: 20). Communities concerned therefore need to
be involved emotionally, intellectually and mentally in RLS efforts. In fact,
to be maximally successful and effective, RLS efforts must be internally
motivated and perpetuated. When the community has a stake in it, it will
feel constrained to continue carrying its own load even when no assistance
is forthcoming from other quarters.
Tapping all available resources and reducing language-
development costs
If RLS is properly planned at the local and national levels, resources,
both human and material, can be judiciously harnessed to meet RLS goals,
especially given the fact that the excuse of scarcity of resources is often the
alibi for lack of RLS initiatives at governmental quarters (cf. Adegbija, 1997
for details of available resources in most African countries that can assist
RLS efforts).
Official institutionalisation of multilingualism
Opportunities for the use of a language in the public sector tend to
promote its growth and boost its prestige. At the moment, Oko does
not have such opportunities. Besides securing the use of endangered
languages in the intimate spheres of family and community, which
Fishman (1988: 1213) likens to the stopping of the haemorrhaging of the
vital arteries, there is the need to officially institutionalise their use, if they
are to gain prestige or be lifted up fromthe lower rungs of the social ladder
in the minds and psyche of many Africans. As a first step, bold attempts
must be made to graphicise all African languages, for this is a deliberate
language reversal ploy. Time should also be created for their use on radio
and television. Use in the public sector will promote a language from the
kith and kin domain to the public limelight and thus elevate its esteemand
increase its functional stamina and utility calibre.
306 Africa and Asia
Conclusion
Every language is entitled to a buoyant life. A language that is deliber-
atelyusedinthe home andpublic sector, whichits speakers are proudto be
associated with, which has a vibrant culture that is consciously promoted
and orchestrated into prominence, and which the younger generation is
eager to use and be associated with, can never die. Conversely, a language
that is restricted in use both in the private and public sectors, family, local,
regional and national settings, which its speakers are ashamed of, which
has no vibrant culture to boast of or exhibit, andwhichthe younger genera-
tion would rather forget, is already dead, even if apparently living.
Given the GIDS, virtually every African language will, unfortunately,
belong to the endangered language group, at one degree or another. In this
paper, we have shown that Oko seems to be doing well in its own small
village setting. This is, however, not the case in larger cities to which many
Oko indigenes frequently emigrate, and would still need to continue
emigrating, insearchof jobs, a better life-style, better pay, etc. Suchemigra-
tion will most likely intensify because the prospects of enough better-
paying jobs in the village have remained dim for a very long time. More-
over, the younger generation is more ambitious about rising high and the
available jobs within the village setting are not commensurate with the
height of their ambitions. Immigrants, we have shown, find no motivation
or encouragement to practise the customs and rituals of their kith and kin,
the major avenue for promotingthe Okolanguage, amongtheir children, in
urban centres. This thus creates an intergenerational gap in the transmis-
sionof the Okolanguage andconstitutes agreat threat tothe growth, devel-
opment andvery existence of the Oko language. Consequently, children of
suchparents returnhome unable to communicate withtheir kithandkinin
the Oko language. The Oko language is thus already dead among many
individual city-dwelling indigenes, due to an extremely low inter-
generational transmission status and is seriously under threat in most
others. For the language toremainalive, its speakers bothwithinthe village
andinurbancentres must have a higher affective stake anddeepemotional
inventory in its survival, promotion and plight. As long as speakers of a
language have a deep stake in its survival and a high emotional involve-
ment and commitment to its existence, all the language shift agents and
triggers in this world will not be able to kill their resolve.
Acknowledgements
I am grateful to Professor Joshua Fishman for detailed comments on
earlier drafts of this chapter.
A Case Study of Oko 307
References
Adegbija, E. (1993a) The graphicization of a small language: The case of Oko. Inter-
national Journal of the Sociology of Language 102, 15373.
Adegbija, E. (1993b) Survival strategies for minority languages: the case of Oko. ITL
Review of Applied Linguistics 1034, 1937.
Adegbija, E. (1994) Language Attitudes in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Sociolinguistic Over-
view. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Adegbija, E. (1989) Acomparative study of politeness phenomena inNigerianEng-
lish, Yoruba and Ogori. Multilingua: Journal of Cross-cultural and Interlanguage
Communication 8(1), 5780.
Adegbija, E. (1997) The identity, survival, and promotion of minority languages in
Nigeria. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 125, 527.
Agheyisi, R.N. (1984) Minor languages inthe Nigeriancontext: Prospects andprob-
lems. Word 35(3), 23553.
Batibo, H.M. (1992) The conflict between elitist, national and ethnic loyalties in lan-
guage attitude anduse: The case of Tanzania. Lecture deliveredat the University
of Duisburg, March 1992.
Chumbow, B.S. (1982) Ogori vowel harmony: An autosegmental perspective. Lin-
guistic Analysis 10(1), 6193.
Fishman, J. (1988) Language spread and language policy for endangered lan-
guages. In P.H. Lowenberg (ed.) Language Spread and Language Policy: Issues,
Implications and Case Studies (pp. 115). Washington, DC: Georgetown Univer-
sity Press.
Fishman, J. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Ohiri-Aniche, C. (1997) Nigerianlanguages die. Quarterly Reviewof Politics, Econom-
ics and Society 1(2): 739.
Rubagumya, C.M. (1991) Language promotion for educational purposes: The ex-
ample of Tanzania. International Review of Education 37(1), 6785.
308 Africa and Asia
Chapter 13
Andamanese: Biological Challenge
for Language Reversal
E. ANNAMALAI and V.GNANASUNDARAM
Geography
Acluster of islands running fromnorth to south in the Bay of Bengal on
the south-eastern side of the Indian sub-continent is the Andaman Islands.
They lie between 10 13 and 10 30 N. latitudes and 90 15 and 93 10 E.
longitudes. They are spread over an area of 6430 square kms and are sepa-
ratedfromthe Nicobar Islands in the south by the Ten Degree channel. The
northernmost island of the Andaman Islands is at a distance of 9016 kms
south-east of the confluence of the Hugli River inWest Bengal, India, andat
a distance of 193.12 kms south of Ca Negrais in Myanmar (Burma). The
capital town Port Blair is at a distance of 1255.25 kms from Calcutta and
1190.88 kms from Chennai (Madras). The Andaman Islands are divided
into Great Andaman Islands and Little Andaman Islands. The Little
Andaman Islands are separated from Port Blair by 100 kms of sea.
In the Little Andaman Islands live the Onges. In Great Andaman
Islands, Jarawas live in the South Andaman and Rutland Islands, Sentinels
in the North Andaman Island and the Andamanese in the Strait Island.
Anthropology
Anthromorphically, the native population of the Andaman Islands is
Negrito (of South East Asian region according to Pandit 1976) and that of
Nicobar Islands is Mangoloid. The Andamanese belong to the former.
Their population is estimated to have been between 5000 and 8000 in 1858
whenthe BritishGovernment of India foundeda penal colony inPort Blair.
They inhabited most of the Great Andaman Islands at that time.
The Andamanese comprised 10 different ethnic groups each with a
separate speech form and a name (Brown 1914: 12). They were: (1) Aka
Cari; (2) Aka Kora; (3) Aka Jeru; (4) Aka Bo; (5) Aka Kede; (6) Aka Kol; (7)
Oko Juwoi; (8) Aka Pucikwar; (9) Aka Bea and (10) Aka Bale. These 10
309
Africa and Asia
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge
groups may be classified into two broad divisions on the basis of cultural
differences. They are the Northern group consisting of Cari, Kora, Bo and
Jeru; andthe Southerngroupconsisting of Kede, Kol, Juwoi, Pucikwar, Bea
andBale. All were, however, lumpedtogether bythe Britishadministrators
under the name of Great Andamanese or Andamanese, for short. Brown
(1948: 245) observes about the language of the Andamanese that
although the natives themselves . . . recognise and give names to ten
distinct languages, all of them are closely related. There is, on the
whole, not a great deal of difference between two neighbouring
languages. A man of Aka-Jeru tribe could understand without any
great difficulty a man speaking Aka-Bo. On the other hand, many of
the languages included two or even more distinct dialects.
This is to be expected as there was no or very little interaction between
them. Brown (1914) observes, with reference to Bea and Jeru, that until the
islands were occupiedbyEuropeans by1858, these twotribes didnot know
of each others existence. This is supported by Mann (1932: xxi), who says
that in many cases no knowledge was possessed regarding tribes distant
only fifteen or twenty miles.
Demography
The Andamanese population started to decline from the middle of the
19th century when it became a penal colony. The native people were killed
by the British soldiers during their punitive expeditions into the forests to
claim land for settlement. When peace was established leading to interac-
tion between the Andamanese and the non-tribal settlers, the former were
introduced to tobacco, liquor and opium and exposed to new diseases,
which became epidemic pneumonia in 1868, syphillis in 1876, measles in
1877 andinfluenza in 1892 killing a large number of the people (Awaradi,
1990: 227). In the earlier battles with the colonisers, most of the young men
of reproductive age had died. Among the surviving Andamanese, the
changed life style and food habits contributed to the deterioration of their
health. For example, the clothes, whichtheyoftenwore wet duringhunting
expeditions in the rain in the forest or in the sea, caused bronchial and
pulmonary infections. When the Japanese occupied the Andaman islands
from March 1942 to October 1942 during the second world war, they
suspected the Andamanese to be spying for the British and massacred a
good number of them.
Table 13.1 illustrates the decline of the population of the Andamanese in
the first six decades of this century.
310 Africa and Asia
The decline of population in different ethnic groups of Andamanese is
given in Table 13.2.
When the 1961Census reported only 19 speakers of Andamanese scat-
tered in many islands, the Government of India, pursuing a policy of pres-
ervation of tribes, collected themand placed themin 1970 in a small island
called Strait Island in South Andaman. They were provided free shelter,
clothing, food and medicine. These 19 people belonged to five ethnic
groups namely, Jeru, Kora, Bo, Cari and Balwa, but were mixed with
Karens of Burma and Oroans of Bihar, India (locally called Ranchis), who
were brought by the British to the Andaman Islands as labourers. The
policy of preservation has helped to increase somewhat the population of
the Andamanese.
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 311
Table 13.1 Population decline in total
Year 1901 1911 1921 1931 1951
Population 625 453 209 90 23
Year 1961 1971 1975 1988 1998
Population 19 24 23 28 35
Figures from1901 to 1971 are fromthe Census of India. The figures for 1975, 1988 and1998 are
from field work.
Table 13.2 Decline in the population of ethnic groups
1901 1911 1921 1975
Adult Child Total Adult Child Total Adult Child Total Total
Aka-Cari 31 8 39 26 6 32 15 2 17 4
Aka-Kora 63 33 96 57 18 78 36 12 48 1
Aka-Bo 31 17 48 40 22 62 12 5 17 6
Aka-Jeru 178 40 218 143 37 180 84 17 101 11
Aka-Kede 54 5 59 28 4 32 16 0 16 0
Aka-Kol 8 3 11 2 0 2 0 0 0 0
Oko-Juwoi 40 8 48 9 0 9 5 0 5 0
Aka-Pucikwar
(Bojigyah)
45 5 50 33 3 36 8 1 9 0
Aka-Bale 15 4 19 14 1 15 4 0 4 0
Aka-Bea 30 7 37 9 1 10 0 1 1 1
Total 625 453 209 23
Figures for the period 1901 to 1911 are from the census of India and are taken from
Chakraborty (1990). Figures after 1921 are not available for different ethnic communities, and
figures for 1975 are from field work.
The age and sex-wise distribution of Andamanese population in 1998
taken from the field work is given in Table 13.3.
Language
The language is notedinthe Census as anunclassifiedlanguage spoken
bya fewremainingindividuals of the tribe of the same name (Nigam, 1972:
8) echoing Brown (1948: 495), who stated that the Andamanese languages
constitute a separate family having no apparent affinity with any other
family of languages; and Colonel Temple, who observed that the
Andaman languages are one group, they are like (i.e. connected with) no
other group, they have no affinities by which we might infer their connec-
tion with any other known group (quoted in Mann, 1932: 5). Other
members of this yet unidentified language family are the Onges, Jarawas
and Sentinels in the Andaman Islands (Manoharan, 1989: 8). Brown (1948)
divides the Andmanese family of languages into two: (1) Northern group
and (2) Southern group. Mann (1932: xxvii) divides them into three: South
Andaman, Middle AndamanandNorthAndaman. Manoharan(1989: 173)
groups them as shown in Figure 13.1.
All the speech forms, whether languages or dialects, that were geneti-
cally related and were spoken in the Great Andaman Islands are now lost
except the one now called Andamanese.
The present native speakers of Andamanese claimthat their language is
of the Jeru group, the largest group. But, according to Manoharan (1989),
the present language is a mixure of different extinct varieties with a high
percentage of Jeru forms.
312 Africa and Asia
Table 13.3 Current age and sex-wise distribution
Age in years Male Female
04 2 1
59 2 1
1014 2 3
1524 3 4
2544 6 5
4554 1 0
5564 1 2
65 and above 1 1
Total 35
Social Organisation
The Andamanese were hunters in the forest and in the sea and food
gatherers. Each ethnic group had a specific territory for hunting and gath-
ering. Tropical forests suppliedthemwithvarious types of fruits, roots and
wildpigs. The sea was the source for tortoise, dugonganddifferent types of
fishincluding shell fish. They also collectedthe eggs of birds. They are now
given free rice, wheat, cereals, etc. by the government. They have also been
trained to grow in small plots sweet potato, tobacco, green pepper, pine-
apple, banana and papaya.
Marriage was originallywithinthe ethnic group(Brown, 1948: 723) and
was prohibited between siblings, step-brothers, step-sisters and between
consanguineal relatives of the same generation, who were regarded as
brothers and sisters (Chakraborty, 1990: 18). The small size of the popula-
tion does not allowthemnowto followthese normative rules. Awoman is
permittedto share her husbandwith another woman for reproduction and
similarly a man is permitted to share his wife. The marriage rule is relaxed
with regard to age also but the family remains the unit of social organisa-
tion. There are now seven families in the Strait Island. There is a chief
selected by age by the community. Government reaches the people for
developmental activities through him. This is the only hierarchical struc-
ture of the community.
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 313
Andamanese Languages
Little Andaman Great Andaman
Onge Jarawa Sentinelese South Non-South
Andaman Andaman
Bea Bale Middle North
Andaman Andaman
Puchi- Juwoi- Kede Bo-Cari Jeru-Kora
Kwar Kol
Juwoi Kol Bo Cari Jeru Kora
Figure 13.1 Andamanese languages
Development
The government has constructed houses with tin roofs, brick walls and
concrete floors for each family of the Andamanese in the Strait Island.
Coconut trees have beenplantedas a cashcropandcanoes have beengiven
for fishing but they hardly use both. Besides the food, stitched clothes are
given to them by the government. This kind of development from above
has made themlose their traditional skills. It has robbedthemof their pride
and purpose of life. It manifests itself at the individual level in different
forms such as occasional fits of depression, bouts of drinking or the
sporadic spree of visiting cinema theatres or video parlours at a stretch
watching all shows available and at the end experiencing the void
(Awaradi, 1990: 241)
Grammar
The details of the grammar of the Andamanese language may be seen in
Manoharan (1989). The grammar shows that the language does not exhibit
features of attrition. There is no significant difference in the complexity of
the grammar among the users of the language of all ages. There is however
a difference inthe active commandof the vocabularyandcode mixingwith
Hindi between older and younger generations.
Andamanese has a phonemic inventory of 7 vowels and 21 consonants;
vowel length is phonemic. It has an elaborate pronominal system. The
three person pronouns also distinguish number and respect and the first
person plural pronouns distinguish inclusion and exclusion of the
addressee. There are three deictic pronouns. There are nine cases. The
verbs belong to 17 classes on the basis of the lexically determined allo-
morphs of the imperative suffix. Transitivity of the verb is expressed by
derivational morphology. The finite verbs are not marked for gender and
number. There is a three way tense system (past, present and future) and
two-way aspectual system (durative and perfective). The word order is
SOV. The modifier (like the adjective) precedes the head (like the noun).
Inthe lexicon, native vocabularyhas beenreplacedbyHindi words even
in the semantic domains of kinship, colour, numerals, etc. There is heavy
borrowing fromHindi for culturally newobjects and concepts such as sari,
pants, cooked vegetable, frying pan, medicine, hospital, electricity, etc., as
well as for numbers beyond three.
There is heavy code switching between Hindi and Andamanese and
code mixing of Hindi in Andamanese particularly among the younger
people. The borrowing and mixing at the lexical and structural levels do
not necessarily indicate a shift towards language loss. They may indicate a
314 Africa and Asia
patternof language use of bilinguals andlanguage change ina contact situ-
ation as long as they can be shown to be instances of convergence, a
phenomenon well attested among the languages of the Indian sub-conti-
nent (Pandit, 1972, 1977). There is an increase in borrowing and mixing
along generational lines and the younger generations borrow and mix
more. If this trend continues to cover the entire lexicon and grammar and
there is nosociolinguistic identificationof it as the current Andamanese but
as a variety of Hindi, then it will be possible to speak of language loss. At
present, though there is resentment among the older generation about the
excessive mixing of Hindi, it is accepted as a feature of Andamanese of the
younger generation. The sociolingusitic identity of ethnicity through
language, though weak among the younger generation, seems to gain
ground as this generation grows older and strengthens its ties with the
community.
Bilingualism
Constant contact with mainland people settled in the islands and with
government officials in charge of tribal development has made Andaman-
ese bilingual in Hindi. This bilingualism was necessary for their survival
with changed occupations, cultural habits and social needs that are
dependent on the mainlanders. Their basic needs of food and clothing are
to be obtainedfromthe market or government, which are controlledby the
mainlanders. Their everydayfooditems include rice, wheat, oil andmarket
vegetables and meat besides fish and other sea products, which they occa-
sionally hunt on their own. They use cooking vessels like tava, frying pan,
kada:i a kindof frying pan, pati:la: a kindof cooking vessel, etc. Men wear
shirts, pants and lungi and women wear sari, blouse, salwar, khameez (both
are girls wear in the town) and they are purchased ready-made in the
market. Linguisticallytheyuse Hindi words for cookingvessels mentioned
above and other words pertaining to daily life like capa:ti a wheat bread,
sabji vegetable curry, bhuna:na: frying, se:ka:na: roasting, sa:ri sari,
kami:c khameez and English words (through Hindi) sart shirt, pE:nt
pants etc.
All Andamanese (30 of themexceptingthe five childrenbelowthe age of
three) know two languages, namely Andamanese and Hindi. Bengali and
Burmese are known besides these two languages to four and five persons
respectively. All Bengali knowing persons except one are male in the age
group of 40 and above. The earlier generation was in contact with Burmese
and there was also intermarriage with the Burmese. This contact declined
after the resettlement of Andamanese in Strait Island. The adult women
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 315
under 40 studied in Hindi mediumschools in Port Blair, where there were
Bengali-speakingstudents. Marriage tookplace betweenAndamanese and
mainlanders. The boy or the girl may be Andamanese (as in the case of a
Muslim boy marrying an Andamanese girl and as in two cases of
Andamanese marrying girls from Ranchi of Bihar). Such marriages are
accepted by the community, as a spouse is not available for all eligible
Andamanese boys and girls locally.
Whether this total bilingualism leads to language shift and loss of
mother tongue is an important question for discussion. Andamanese of 40
plus (8 people of the elder group) have a good command of Andamanese.
They could give effortlessly the equivalent Andamanese words for the
Hindi borrowings that have replacedthem. They couldalso produce freely
sentences and texts in Andamanese. Men belonging to this age group also
speak Hindi confidently. Women of this age group are hesitant to speak in
Hindi, but their passive comprehension of Hindi is good. When questions
are askedin Hindi they prefer to answering in Andamanese. Visits of these
womento the mainislandare rare andwhenthey go they are accompanied
by men. This could be a reason for their less active command of Hindi.
People of 1640 years (10 people) speak Andamanese fluently, but it is
heavily mixedwith Hindi. This groupin both genders has the highest level
of command of Hindi among the Andamanese. Children 315 years (12
people) were tested for the basic vocabulary items: 100 word list of
Gudschinsky(1956) withrespect toAndamanese andHindi. The elicitation
technique of namingandfillinginthe blankwas usedtotest the vocabulary
items in Andamanese and Hindi in their active command. Without giving
any of the test lexical items directly either in Hindi or in Andamanese, cues
were given (in Hindi as the investigators did not know Andamanese) in
sucha way that the childrespondedto the cue andgave the knownwordin
Andamanese and in Hindi. The words not known to the child in either of
the languages were left blank and they did not count. On the basis of the
responded words, the percentage of words known to the child in
Andamanese and in Hindi was calculated and it is given age-wise in Table
13.4.
Table 13.4 shows that the child starts with relatively a larger vocabulary
in Hindi but there is a progression in the knowledge of words both in
Andamanese and Hindi as the child grows up. The Andamanese vocabu-
lary is acquired through the childs participation in the presently rare
collective activities like fishing and turtle hunting and in cultural activities
like the puberty ceremony. The Hindi vocabulary is acquired during visits
to Port Blair with their parents, visits to hospitals for treatment and from
watching Hindi movies. Hindi is learned in the school also. Childrens
316 Africa and Asia
Hindi vocabulary is larger than the Andamanese vocabulary at any given
time but the latter is neither stagnant nor regressive. Nevertheless, Hindi
may be said to be dominant in their bilingualism in terms of the size of its
vocabulary and variety of situations of use.
Hindi is also the language of literacy. Though Andamanese is written in
Devanagari, the script used for Hindi, in the school primers prepared in
1994, it is not usedfor readingor writinginanycommunicative context. No
other written material besides the primers is available in Andamanese.
Hindi, being the language of literacy and of administration and market, is
the language of prestige also.
The older group of Andamanese use only Andamanese among them-
selves. They prefer to use it with the younger groups, but switch to Hindi
occasionally when, for instance, the children do not follow their instruc-
tions in Andamanese. These instructions may pertain even to performance
of cultural rituals. The instructions are generallygivenbymale elders andit
may be saidwomen always use Andamanese withthe younger people.The
middle groupuse mostlyAndamanese withelders andamongthemselves.
Withchildren, they use bothAndamanese andHindi andcode switching is
common. Their Andamanese is mixedwithHindi more whentheyspeakto
the younger group.
The younger groupuse Hindi andAndamanese among themselves. The
frequency of use of the two languages is in reverse order compared to the
middle group. They try to speak only in Andamanese with the older group
and speak both in Andamanese and Hindi with the middle group. They
are, however, more confident in speaking Hindi. Code switching is more
common when strangers are present. When secrecy is required in the pres-
ence of strangers, the younger group use Andamanese exclusively among
themselves.
All Andamanese use Hindi in meetings with the administrative and
hospital staff andwithstrangers inthe bazaar. The inter-generational nega-
tive correlation with age in the use of Hindi, described above, has led some
researchers to conclude that the Andamanese have learnt Hindi to the
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 317
Table 13.4
Age
group
Percentage of vocabulary
given in Andamanese
Percentage of vocabulary
given in Hindi
35 37 50
610 63 85
1115 82 90
extent that it is replacing their tribal language, especially among the
younger generation. (Awaradi, 1990: 230).
Language Attitude
The language attitude survey conducted during 199697 in Strait Island
with regard to Andamanese and Hindi gives the following results. The
older people have positive attitudes towards Andamanese. They believe in
the preservation of their language and culture for maintaining Andaman-
ese identity. While they are satisfied with the use of Andamanese by the
middle group, they are not happy with the Andamanese of the younger
group that is mixed with Hindi. They want the younger group to improve
their Andamanese. The older group, along with the middle group,
emphasise the needfor teaching Andamanese in the school andfor writing
books in it to impart their culture and heritage to the younger group. They
also believe that the use of Andamanese in the school will improve the
status of their language.
Regarding the attitude towards Hindi, men of the older group believe
that learning Hindi is inevitable to overcome marginal existence. They,
however, strongly feel that Hindi must not be used exclusively in their
homes, incommunity gatherings andincultural practices. This groupfeels
that the dominance of Hindi will destroy the Andamanese language and
culture. The women of this group are shy of speaking in Hindi, as
mentioned above; they even refuse to acknowledge their knowledge of it.
The older group accuses the middle group of discouraging the younger
group from learning Andamanese by speaking frequently in Hindi at
home.
The attitude of the middle group towards Andamanese is ambivalent. It
is not as strongly positive as the older group, thoughthey feel that there is a
needtopreserve Andamanese, totransmit it totheir childrenandtoteachit
in school. They also support the view of the older group that for their
survival and identity as Andamanese, preservation of their language is
essential. They are more tolerant of the use of Hindi than the older group
even in the home domain. For them, Hindi is the language of wider
communication and of prestige and power, that gives them opportunities
and contacts.
The younger groupis extremely shy of speaking Andamanese, partic-
ularly in the presence of strangers (except when they want to be secre-
tive). They tend not to acknowledge their knowledge of it. They prefer a
mixed code of Hindi and Andamanese when they speak among them-
selves and when they speak to their elders. They are critical of the Hindi
318 Africa and Asia
of the older group believing that it is not pure. They tend to mix less of
Andamanese in their Hindi in contrast to mixing more of Hindi in their
Andamanese. As they grow older, however, there is an attitudinal
change and they increasingly feel the Andamanese is important to give
them an identity.
The above description of use and attitude among three age groups
regarding Andamanese and Hindi shows that the entire community is
positive about bilingualism and there is no loss of the mother tongue yet.
The gradual decline in the use of and in the empathy for Andamanese
down the age groups does not necessarily point towards ultimate loss of
the mother tongue. This is because both the utility value of Hindi and the
identity value of Andamanese are at work and there is increasing realisa-
tion of the need for identity and preservation cyclically, as persons grow
older. The greatest threat to the language is the biological extinction of the
community. The community tries to check or delay it by relaxing its repro-
ductive rules, by allowing marriage outside the community and by raising
the off-springs in relative isolation. The children born of such an inter-
ethnic marriage are kept and raised as Andamanese, even if the outsider
deserts the community and leaves the island. The mother tongue is
preserved, though may be similarly mixed with the dominant language,
for the identity of the community. The conscious efforts, though meagre, to
preserve the language are described below.
Reversing Shift
The Andamanese language is not an instance of language loss to be
revived. It is used, as described above, by the entire population, though
that population is now quite small. The entire community is bilingual
with a functional distribution of the two languages, Andamanese and
Hindi, though the distribution is less categorical and more diffused
along the generational line. The present situation is closer to Stage 6 of
Fishman (1991) but not identical to it in which Xish is the normal
language of informal, spoken interaction between and within all three
generations of the family, withYishbeing reversedfor matters of greater
formality and technicality than those that are the common fare of daily
family life.
Unlike the above stage, the boundary betweenformal andinformal situ-
ations of language use is diffused. Hindi is used alternatively with
Andamanese by the younger generation in informal domains like the
family. The middle generation is compelled to use Hindi with children
when their competence in Andamanese is wanting in domestic and ritual
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 319
matters. Though there is a concern among the elder generation about this
situation, there is no community action to change it other than lending
passive support to the efforts of the government school to teach Andaman-
ese to the children.
There has beena primary school inStrait Islandfrom1970, but not much
learning takes place there. Subjects were taught earlier through the
medium of English; the two languages taught were English and Hindi;
there was no Andamanese. In 1996 the medium was changed to Hindi. At
this time, Andamanese was introduced as a language and as a vehicle to
transfer the students to the Hindi medium. The Central Institute of Indian
Languages, in collaboration with the Tribal Welfare Department and the
Education Department of Andaman Islands, prepared bilingual educa-
tional materials and trained teachers (who are not Andamanese). It will
take a few years to evaluate their effectiveness with regard to the acquisi-
tion of competence, including literacy in Andamanese, by the children and
developing a positive attitude towards their language.
The most specific feature of Andamanese is the small size of its popula-
tion. Given its size, it cannot hope to rise to Stage 4 of Fishmans GIDscale,
where the use of Andamanese in work place outside the community, in
governmental services and in the media at the local and lower levels must
come into play. Stage 4a, where Andamanese is used in lower education as
part of compulsory primary education, is now in emergence at the initia-
tive of the government with the acceptance of the community, as
mentioned above. But the bilingual education model used is not geared to
mother tongue maintenance, though it is not averse to it. The support for
maintenance must come from outside the school. But there is no opportu-
nity for using the literacy skills in Andamanese even for personal purposes
like letter writing, as this small community functions on face-to-face oral
communication. For reading, there is no material other than the school
primers. There is no religious factor in literacy learning for reading sacred
materials. Recreational reading is nil in this community as it is deeply
depressed about its endangered state. These factors prohibit Andamanese
literacyat home andinthe communityfrombeingpractisedbythe commu-
nity as proposed for Stage 5.
This raises the question of whether intergenerational orality and the
demographic concentration of the community in one place, which are
features of Stage 6 and possessed by the Andamanese, are sufficient to
arrest language loss. The institutional reinforcement of language use,
which is another feature of this stage, is limited to rituals, which are trun-
cated, and to hunting expeditions, which are becoming rare. The demo-
graphic concentration is a forced one as a result of the governments policy
320 Africa and Asia
of isolation of the community geographically. The isolation is also from
market related economic activities, as the communitys basic needs are
provided free by the government. Nevertheless, the lure of attractions of
the town (Port Blair) is strong, particularly among the younger generation,
primarily driven by their depression to seek an outlet in self-indulgence.
The boat trip to the town (Port Blair) is dependent on the service run by the
government once a week, but the tendencytostaylonger, andina fewcases
permanentlyinthe town, is increasing. Intergenerational oral transmission
of language, as mentioned earlier, is getting weakened in terms of prefer-
ence and competence among the younger generation.
There is no marked change in the Stage 6 in the last 10 years and so
sliding into Stage 7 does not appear to be imminent. The major reason for
this is the consciousness of the threat to their survival as a distinct
community defined linguistically and culturally. The community
strongly believes that its cultural distinctiveness and ethnic survival are
not possible without keeping their language. But this consciousness may
not help to overcome the biological threat to the communitys survival and
hence its language.
1
Note
1. The field work was conducted by the second author with M.R Ranganatha and
K.S Rajyashree of C.I.I.L. and we are grateful to them.
References
Awaradi, S.A. (1990) Computerised Master Plan (19912021) for the Welfare of the
Primitive Tribes of Andaman&Nicobar Islands. Port Blair: Andaman&Nicobar
Administration.
Basu(1952) Alinguistic introductiontothe Andamanese. Bulletinof the Anthropolog-
ical Survey of India 1(2), 5570.
Brown, R.A.R. (1914) Notes on the languages of the Andaman Islands. Anthropos 9
(3652).
Brown, R.A.R. (1948) The Andaman Islanders. Chicago: Free Press.
Chakraborty, D.K. (1990) The Great Andamanese: The ASI Andamanese and Nicobar
Tribal Series. Calcutta: Seagull Books.
Fishman, J. (1991) Reversing Language Shift: Theoretic and Empirical Foundations of
Assistance to Threatened Languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gudschinsky, S.C. (1956) The ABCs of Lexico-statistics (Glottochronology). Word
12, 175210.
Mann, E.H. (1932) On the Aboriginal Inhabitants of the Andaman Islands. London:
Royal Anthropological Institute (reprint).
Manoharan, S. (1989) A Descriptive and Comparative Study of the Andaman Language.
Calcutta: Anthropological Survey of India.
Nigam, R.C. (1972) India: Language Handbook on Mother Tongues in Census. New
Delhi: Ministry of Home Affairs.
Andamanese: A Biological Challenge 321
Pandit, P.B. (1972) India as a Sociolinguistic Area. Pune: University of Poona.
Pandit, P.B. (1977) Language ina Plural Society. Delhi: The Devraj Chanana Memorial
Committee.
Pandit, T.N. (1976) The original inhabitants of the Andaman & Nicobar islands.
Yojana (Journal of the Planning Commission of India) 20(13), (8196).
322 Africa and Asia
Chapter 14
Akor Itak Our Language, Your
Language: Ainu in Japan
J.C. MAHER
Introduction
Efforts to reverse language shift, to preserve a mother tongue in
decline, commonly make upa patchwork of the quotidienandthe elegaic.
The design of the effort can reveal many things: nostalgia, ethno-ideology
and stereotype, language planning formulation and what-can-we-do
strategies for repair. It may reveal a challenge to official propaganda-
myths of the vanishing people and dying language. It may reveal
Gemeinschaft aspirations, heightened contrastivity and adversarialness.
Involving so many different people and change-agents a patchwork of
shift differentials is likely inevitable. Involving so many different ideas
and aspirations, a range of shift patterns can be expected. People have, as
the saying goes, different agenda. Ainu has powerful symbolic resonance
since it calls forth the socio-political landscape of the past, the good old
days and bad old days, colonialism, forced-removal fromland, schooling
in Japanese and prohibition of the Ainu language. Also, in the matter of
language revival, what constitutes transmission to one person is not
transmission to another. The issue is certainly ambiguous among Ainu
speakers. If the call to arms to defend a language in danger displays in
extremis epic heroism and occasionally ethnocentrism at one end of the
revival spectrum, at the other pole it fosters genuine solidarity among
people. Manifestos celebrate this:
Even though I, as one Ainu, make tireless efforts for the language I
cannot alone hold back the tide which washes over my language and
hollows it out. But nowthere are many pebbles against the destructive
waters and the number is growing. We have now started to build a
breakwater, a sea wall. We are an ever-increasing rock. (Kayano, 1987,
Ainu Bunka Kiso Chishiki [The Ainu Culture: the Basic Knowledge] pp. 14,
22, 25)
323
Africa and Asia
Ainu in Japan
In spite of the usual sociolinguistic disarray about who is doing what and
what needs to be done, in the case of Ainu revitalisation it is obvious, to
even the casual observer, that the Ainu language is on the move and has
made substantial gains both in grass-roots consciousness and at national
policy level.
Optimism and Universalism
To pursue the water metaphor, the effort to crack open the lock-gates to
provide an albeit narrow intergenerational channel of Ainu tradition
comprises also the idea of the oppressiveness of mainstream Japanese
tradition whose sea has been turbid, even devastating. Ainu language
revival nevertheless evokes optimism, futurism, the sense of familial
gathering, sometimes euphoria in the call to defence of the mother-
tongue:
We are getting better and better. Through this gathering around our
language we can begin to make our first steps to the learning of Ainu.
And with these steps we will bend all our effort on the road to the
development of the Ainulanguage andthe learning of it. (Introduction
to Ainu Language Classes. The 4th Ainu Cultural Festival Hokkaido
Utari-Kyokai [Hokkaido Ainu Association]. Hokkaido, 1992: 15.)
The idea of minoritylanguage maintenance holds that mainstreamidentity
is partial andtendentious andmust irrevocablyleadthe individual toculti-
vate a more humane and rich social affiliation. The idea is, ineluctably, a
search for sidestream roots which in turn comprises a wider existential
desire for historical relevance. In this model, there is enclosed a univer-
salism applicable to all men. The mother tongue bears a nature spirit, the
protector of all. It is linked to earth-values. By contrast, mandatory main-
streamidentity engenders social confusion and throws the individual into
a landscape of solitude and alienation. The mother tongue is a room of
ones own:
When an Ainu does it you might say it is only to be expected. But each
time a Japanese andAinulearnto speak Ainutogether they participate
in the joys of Ainu. They preserve not only the culture of Ainu but also
the culture of a whole land, a whole earth, one village. (Kayano, 1987,
Yasashii Ainu-go [Easy Ainu (1)] Shiratori Nibutani Ainu-go Kyoshitsu
[Shiratori Nibutani Ainu language class])
The cultural renaissance of Ainu has been accompanied by a skirting of
politico-historical frontiers in which the primordial affiliations to the
324 Africa and Asia
indigenous andthe static-ethnic jostle for space witha re-forgingof a polit-
ical community. This is a community in which ethnic mobilisation is a stra-
tegic choice rather than an ontological given. If we were to point to a
historical watershed wherein all of these issues coalesced it would begin in
the mid-1980s.
Six years after Prime Minister Yasuhiro Nakasones public comments
(1986) that Japan has no racial minorities a de rigeur official statement
invoking Japanese homogeneity Giichi Nomura stepped up to speak
before the General Assembly of the United Nations in New York. It coin-
cided with the inauguration of the United Nations International Year of
the Indigenous Peoples. Nomura spoke of the ravaging, the
marginalisation and discrimination suffered by the Ainu in Japan and
concluded with words of thanks in Ainu. This version of history runs
counter both to successive Japanese governments master narrative of
national homogeneity but also to a popular narrative of brave conquest of
difficult virgin lands in the freezing north. Quite simply, the Ainu version
flouts common sense in Japan. Siddle (1995, 1996) has traced brilliantly the
construct indigenous people in the global postwar movement for decolo-
nisation, emphasising the making and remaking of Ainu identity by both
the dominant Japanese and the Ainu themselves ways in which
competing versions of Ainu identity have been articulated and rearticu-
lated during different historical periods and how these identities racial
and ethnic have served to shape relations between Ainu and Japanese
(1996: 2). The historical racialisation of the Ainu spawned popular images
of Ainu minzoku (race people, Volk, ethnic group) as being an inferior
racial group. Until recently, advertisements have invited tourists to visit
real Ainu villages where they can see the ancient customs of the famed
hairy Ainu (Siddle, 1995: 90; Narita & Hanazaki, 1975: 312). Meanwhile,
the Ainulanguage is popularly misconstruedtypologically (Is it Japanese
Is it a dialect?). At the same time, it retains a comfortable identityas a dead
language. Mervio (1995: 51) succinctly elaborates the cultural representa-
tion of Japanese (Ainu and other minorities) in the political economy, thus:
Not only in Japan is language an ideological issue and at the core of
many discourses on race, cultural proximity and nationalism . . . one can
find a strong tendency to place attention to language as a distinguishing
feature.
In Japan, in contrast to the fin de siecle propaganda of internationalisa-
tion inductedby politicians of the ruling class of 1980, the search is on for a
new paradigm of cultural pluralism involving the recognition of other
languages. In this new matrix, Ainu holds a central place. (I have
Ainu in Japan 325
elaborated the paradigm of multilingualism in Japan elsewhere outlining
the dynamics of manycommunitylanguages andmajor dialects, the role of
multilingualism in trade, education and the professions and pointing to
creole aspects of the historical origins of Japanese itself. See Maher, 1985,
1989, 1991a, 1991b, 1993, 1994, 1996, 1997, 1999; Maher & Honna, 1994;
Maher & Macdonald, 1995; Maher & Yashiro, 1991). The search for a new
paradigm has been powerfully assisted by the ever-widening crack
between national and local views of what constitutes community, society.
An oft-cited example (Tsunemoto, 1999) is the ruling of the Sapporo
District Court of Hokkaido in 1997 which declared: (1) that the controver-
sial flooding of Ainu lands for dam construction was illegal, that the Ainu
are indeed indigenous people by any recognised definitions of the term
(ILO, Cobo Report), (2) that Article 27 of the UN Covenant on Civil and
Political Rights whichprotects the cultural andlinguistic rights of minority
peoples is legally binding domestically. Finally, the Hokkaido court
invoked Article 13 of the Japanese Constitution which makes the govern-
ment responsible for protecting a minority members cultural rights and
stated that governments culpability and subsequent responsibilty for
Ainus forcible minoritisation is unequivocal. This decision was widely
viewed as surprising, epoch-making (Tsunemoto, 1999: 368) and gave an
additional surge to an increasing optiminism and confidence about Ainu
culture and the framework whose framework is, as Sasamura opines,
urespa mosir a land where all things growhelping each other (Sasamura,
1999: 170).
Charting Language Shift and Revitalisation
Ainuis one of the languages of Japan. The Japanese archipelago, located
off mainland Asia with which it has had linguistic contact throughout
history, consists of approximately 3000 islands over which a variety of
languages and dialects are spoken by a population of 121,000,000. The
general population lives mostly in the densely populated coastal areas
along four main islands of Honshu, Kyushu, Hokkaido and Shikoku.
Typologically regarded as a language isolate, the Ainu language is of
linguistic, historical and social significance in the history of Japan. Ainu is
now spoken predominantly in Hokkaido, an island bordered in the north
bySakhalinandSiberia. The Nibutani valleyinthe Sarudistrict of southern
Hokkaido is considered to have the highest concentration of speakers and
learners of Ainu in Japan.
The traditional Ainu-speaking areas have undergone massive language
shift over the last 200 hundred years. An actual figure for the number of
326 Africa and Asia
native speakers is not known (see below). There is much evidence of
renewedinterest inthe studyof Ainuat university, communitycentres and
private institutions andthe matter of AinuRSL(Reversing Language Shift)
represents a huge challenge witha typical snakes-and-ladders scenario, i.e.
the problem of
too much to be done urgently;
much goodwill;
much uncertainty;
lack of coordination on how to achieve renewal;
potentially rich resources with which to accomplish the task.
Ainu language rights are not a matter of a linguistic minority requesting
government cooperation to recognise linguistic difference and offer
support. The issues involve an uncomfortable interaction of centre and
periphery. The imposing fact that Ainu is conceived primordially
(Fishmans term) as an indigenous language of the far north a local
phenomenon like a dialect rather than as a language of Japan is an issue
of phenomenology which impacts upon language perceptions. I
mentioned above that the territory of Hokkaido was for long the site of
pioneer struggle and settlement, of snowand cold, the felling of trees and
the clearing of forests. Hokkaido was for many generations controlled by
the independent Matsumae elite, a clan whose interest was as much with
the rich Russian north as with Edo-Tokyo. Hokkaido was long regarded
with suspicion. The territory is geographically and politically distinctive
as a region being administered, even now, in a somewhat different
manner from that of other prefectures. The peripheral warlords did not
perceive themselves as unequivocallyJapanese but rather as independent
entities. As the Daimyo Matsumae Kimihiro declared to the Jesuit
missionary de Angelis in 1618 Matsumae is not Japan (quoted in Siddle,
1995: 34). In the modern nation-state of Japan, forged through nationalist
wars and burnished with carefully crafted ethno-cultural narratives and
history, the Daimyos matter-of-fact statement is heard nowas a thunder-
clap. At the centre of the suspicion about Hokkaido (formerly Ezo) lay
the Ainu language: curiosity, nuisance, potential Trojan horse of foreign
influence, important trade language. (Paradoxically, Ainu interpreters,
Ainu-Russian-Japanese, likelyprovidedthe first gatewayto international
trade between Japan and the rest of the world. Mixed factors thus led to
the formulation of language laws which accelerated language shift.) I
suggest that shift factors can be traced from the beginning of the 19th
century running continuously through the 1900s. However, with the
Ainu in Japan 327
complete cessation of anti-Ainu laws in the late 20th century (the drafting
of pro-Ainu legislation and official structures in Hokkaido) events have
also occurred that point unequivocally to revitalisation. The flow of
events and laws which, I suggest, forged language shift, as well as the
slowing down of shift and exertions towards revitalisation are shown in
Chart 1.
Chart 1 A chronology of Ainu language law and revitalisation
1799 Ainu not subject to Japanese lawsince the edict of Hideyoshi (1593)
andIeyasu(1604) establishprinciple of Ezo shidai (Ainusort out their
own affairs). In practice, various regulations are set up by local
contractors and Matsumae officials forbidding use of Japanese
language and customs.
17991821 The Bakufu (Tokugawa Shogunate regime) take over adminis-
tration of Ezo-chi (Hokkaido) and reverse previous policy,
encourage Japanisation (occasionally by force), particularly in stra-
tegic areas like Etorofu. Matsumae-han (clan) regain position in
1821. Revert to previous policy of non-assimilation. Discourage the
learning of Japanese.
1855 Bakufu again revise the former policy of cultural non-assimilation
of the Ainu. Educational measures instituted to enable and
encourage the Ainu to learn Japanese.
1869 Renaming of Ezo-chi (Ezo-chi Kaisho). On the formal establishment
of the Colonial Mission to Hokkaido by the Japanese government,
former Japanese names of Ezo-chi (Ainu Ainu Moshir) to Hokkaido
and Kita Ezo-chi (Ainu Rebun Moshir) to Karafuto. Other regula-
tions standardise Japanese characters for reading Ainu place names
(e.g. Karafuto place names in Dec.1873 Karafuto Chimei moji Seitei)
(There seem to be no official names for these laws in the early
Kaitakushi (pioneer) years; they are instead in the nature of procla-
mations.)
1871 Ainu rituals (e.g. funeral customs, songs and dances) prohibited
(Kyudojin Tamamono Hei kin Moku). Repeated in 1876 Kyudojin
Kyoka Yutatsu, Kyudojin Mimiwa Irezumi Kinshi.) This emasculates
the cultural contexts for language encouraged. In this year Ainu are
designated as heimin (common people).
1872 Land Regulation Ordinance (1872) Jisho Kisoku. Article 7 takes all
land formerly used by Ainu, i.e. Ainu land use not recognised as
ownership terra nullius. Not directly concerned with language
328 Africa and Asia
issues but in Ainu history a watershed in that the link between a
culture and a territory in which it may thrive is finally severed.
1875 Karafuto-Kuriles Exchange Treaty (Karafuto Chishima Kokan Joyaku).
Indigenes given three years to choose nationality, but Japanese
quickly relocate 841 Ainu people to Tsuishikari in Hokkaido. Set up
school with Japanese instruction only. Lifestyle totally different
from that in Karafuto.
1877 Article 15 of the HokkaidoLandCertificates PublishingRegulations
(Hokkaido Jiken Hakko Jorei) places all remaining mountains, forests
andwilderness under the control of the government. Some Ainuare
given tiny plots of land designated as third class government
controlled property under Article 16. By inhibiting access to tradi-
tional areas these laws inhibit social communication between Ainu
speech communities.
1878 Standardisationof the categoryname for Ainuinthe familyregister.
(Kyudojin Meisho Ittei). The terms Dojin, Komin, Kyu Ezojin replaced
by Kyu Dojin (former aborigine). The notionof a former people and
a horobiyuku gengo (a language on the road to extinction) is first
introduced.
1883 Nemuro-ken Dojin Kyusaiho. Relief Law grants Ainu small allot-
ments, tools, etc. Start of concentration and relocation of Ainu into
hogochi reservations set upunder 1877 Class Three landregulation.
Traditional communities broken up. Relocations have deleterious
effect upon the maintenance of customs and speech forms.
1884 AinufromShumshuinN. Kuriles relocatedto Shikotan. Aggressive
language andreligious (Buddhist) policy; these Ainuspeak Russian
and are Orthodox Christians. Russians have been in Shumshu since
1730s.
1885 Sapporo-ken Dojin Kyusaiho. (Same as for 1883 Relief laws).
1898 The Hokkaido Kyu Dojin Hogo Ho (Hokkaido Former Aborigines
Protection Act) proposed to the 13th Imperial Congress by
Hokkaido Prefectural Government for the third time (Private
Members Bills failed in 1893 and 1895). Passed in the House of
Representatives. This far-reaching law includes educational provi-
sion for all Ainu children. The use of the Ainu language is not
permittted in schools (this is not in law but was policy in practice).
Japanese language education for all Ainu.
1899 The Hokkaido Kyu Dojin Hogo Ho (Hokkaido Former Aborigines
Protection Act) promulgated and put into effect.
1901 The first Statute on Former Aborigine Childrens Education.
Ainu in Japan 329
(Kyudojin Jido Kyoiku Kitei). Ainu and Japanese children to be
educatedseparately. Ainueducationis inferior, muchtime spent on
Japanese language learning. The Ainu education policy has a huge
impact on Ainu language, by 1920s only very elderly Ainu cannot
speak Japanese. First Regulations abolished 1908.
1916 Second Regulations for the Education of Former Native Children
(Kyudojin Jido Kyoiku Kitei). Reintroduces shorter and inferior Ainu
curriculum. Abolished 1922.
19191968 Revisions 15 of the 1899 Act (Hokkaido Kyu dojin Hogo Ho). 1919
revision increases health care provisions, subsequent revisions
graduallywhittle awayat welfare andeducationprovisions (Native
Schools scrapped in 1937) until only Articles concerning allotments
and communal property are left.
1925 Speech by Ainu activist and poet Iboshi Hokuto to the linguist
Kindaichis Tokyo Ainu Gakkai (Tokyo Ainu Study Group) estab-
lishes context for future reclamation of Ainu culture and language.
Attacks on dying race stereotype, call for equality and justice,
linkage with ethnic extinction in global context (e.g. Australian
aborigine).
1973 First issue of Ainu newspaper Anutari Ainu containing references to
the need for Ainu language revival.
1980 Declaration by the Japanese government that no linguistic minori-
ties are present in Japan. 1980 Report on Human Rights in accor-
dance with the International Covenant on Civil and Political
Rights. Referring to Article 27 which recognises the existence of
linguistic minorities and recognises the right to enjoy their own
culture . . . or to use their own language. The government states:
Minorities of this kind mentioned in the covenant do not exist in
Japan.
1983 The first Ainu language class established by folklorist Shigeru
Kayano in Nibutani, Hokkaido.
1984 Hokkaido Utari Kyokai (Hokkaido Utari Association) proposes the
Ainu Minzoku ni kan suru Horitsu NewLawfor the Ainu to replace
the 1899 Act. This proposal comprises recommendations on the
revival and maintenance of the Ainu language and makes reference
to language rights (Article 3). Also explicit policy recommenda-
tions for revitalisation of Ainu Language.
1986 Prime Minister Nakasones speech proclaiming Japan as a racially
homogeneous nation blessed with an absence of minorities and
consequently possessing a high level of intellectual competence
330 Africa and Asia
unlike the United States with its black and other problematic racial
minorities. These minorities have debilitated the intellectual level
of the nation. Ainu and other minorities in Japan, angered by this
statement, reaffirm the need to assert their cultural, including
linguistic, presence. Widely viewed by the Ainu as a watershed in
their postwar history. The planning of language revitalisation
measures begin: includes the setting up of Ainu kyoshitsu (private
language schools or other facilities).
1987 The government reports on its 2nd Human Rights Report to the
United Nations. In this document, the government officially recog-
nises the existence of the Ainu people (as individuals, not as an
ethnic minority). Denies the existence of any discrimination
(regarding language, etc.).
1987 The first Ainu language class (Ainugo Kyoshitsu) to be financially
supported by the Hokkaido Prefectural Government and the
Government of Japan. Nibutani, Hokkaido.
1987 The first Ainu for Radio Language Course established. Sapporo
Radio, Hokkaido
1988 The Utari Mondai Konwakai proposes the necessity of enacting the
Ainu Shinpo (Ainu New Law) consultative committee. The
Hokkaido Prefectural Governor demands that the government
enact the AinuNewLaw. The NewLawproposes specific measures
to maintain and promote Ainu language education.
1989 The Dai Ikkai Ainugo Benron Taikai (1st Ainu Language Speech
Contest) held in Tokyo, Japan.
1991 Government of Japan admit that Ainu exist as a minority as defined
under Article 27 of UNInternational Covenant onCivil andPolitical
Rights.
1993 International Year of the Indigenous Peoples. New momentum in
the Ainu movement for language rights.
1994 Shigeru Kayano the first Ainu to become a member of the Diet
(House of Counsellors). A leading figure in the Ainu language
revival movement and a fluent speaker of Ainu delivers inaugura-
tion speech first in Ainu then Japanese.
1994 Akor Itak (Our Language) the first standard textbook on the Ainu
language published in Hokkaido.
1994 The Hokkaido Ainu Culture Research Centre established in
Hokkaido by the Hokkaido Prefectural Government.
1997 Enactment of the Ainu Culture Promotion Lawand abolition of the
Ainu in Japan 331
1898 law. Formation of the Foundation for Research and Promotion
of Ainu Culture.
The Graded Intergenerational Disruption Model
Inthe effort to reverse language shift, Ainuis onthe weakside. Hitherto,
there has been sociolinguistic disarray. The work of linguists has concen-
trated on informant-based analysis of lexico-grammar and phonology.The
underpinning of much of this work has been an unquestioning acceptance
that Ainu is inevitably doomed. Linguistic and anthropological work has
thus been haloed with tragedy and heroism. This as well as the philosophy
and situation underpinning fieldwork in Ainu has undoubtedly changed
in a more engaging and positive manner as Nakagawa (1998) describes.
The current future and prospect for Ainu lies in functional differentation
(e.g. ritual, greeting, archival) and vigorous ethnosymbolic usage as found
in code-switching. DeChicchis, in his wide-ranging review of the current
state of the Ainu language, is upbeat in a rather specific way: With thou-
sands of Ainu singing and praying and greeting each other, and teaching
their children these Ainu songs and prayers and greetings, it is also a safe
bet that the Ainulanguage is not about todie (1995: 11819). There is surely
an overall and ever-present and increasingly vigorous diglossia whereby
transmissibility has been demonstratedin certain areas. Ainuhas achieved
some stability in the wake of pervasive language shift.
Stage 8 Most vestigial users are socially isolated and aged
individuals. The reassembly of Ainu must take place from their
mouths and memories and taught to demographically
unconcentrated adults
The reassembly of the Ainu language model is the most salient stage in
the Ainu languages path through contemporary society. The standard
variety is now gathered around the Saru dialect of Nibutani (see below)
and is in good repair (see Tamura 1956, 1960, 1970, 1974, 1981).
Fieldwork on the Ainu language continues with some vigour and the
reconstruction of an effective Ainu standard (sic Standard Ainu) has
focusedon grammar. Alandmark was reachedin 1994 when Akor Itak Our
Language the first general textbook of the Ainu language was issued in
Hokkaido. Arguably, the first modern attempt to show everyday Ainu
speech and organise it in some instructional format was Iwao Yoshidas
Ainu-go kaiwa-hen (Conversation Guide in Ainu) (191516). Since then, the
notion that Ainu cannot function as a language for real-world communica-
tion has been the prevalent view among students and scholars of Ainu
332 Africa and Asia
until recently. The emphasis has shifted from focus on decline to practical
measures for reconstruction.
Adoptinga methodusedwidelyinmodernlanguage teaching/learning
in Japan, an Ainugo Benron Taikai (Ainu Language Speech Contest) the
first such competition was held in 1989. It received extensive media
coverage television and newspaper and was attended by several
hundred people including Ainu, Japanese and non-Japanese. Participants
came fromall parts of the Tokyo (Kanto) area and Hokkaido and their ages
ranged from pre-school children of three years old to university students
and workers. Speech contests continue to be held annually.
Yoneda (Honda) noted the need for linkage between Ainu language
teaching and other foreign language instruction such as Japanese, Korean
and English as foreign languages (Kokuritsu Kokugo Kenkyujo discussion,
1995). The absence of the sharing of expertise in materials production and
methodology andthe needfor an establishedassociation of Ainu language
teachers must be addressed in the future. Meanwhile, a series of video-
cassettes was produced by an Ainu-based group (Kamuyturano Associa-
tion, 1988) to provide instruction in conversational, situation-based Ainu.
The materials deal with the customs and language of traditional everyday
life of the Ainu including the construction of houses, cooking, greetings,
leave-taking, andother formulaic expressions. Language material is organ-
ised not according to a graded structural syllabus but by means of situa-
tional contexts in which newvocabulary is presented. The vocabulary and
paradigmatic forms which emerge from these situations are discussed in
the accompanying text. On the screen appear both katakana (Japanese
phonemic syllabary) and romaji (romanised) transcriptions with glosses in
Japanese.
The textbooks used for teaching Ainu conversation and grammar
employ katakana (unlike the older grammars such as Batchelors which
used romanised Ainu). Most written materials are remarkably similar in
format, based on a phrase-book type approach and requiring memorisa-
tionrather thancommunicative manipulation. Wordlists or labelleddraw-
ings of traditional Ainu implements for hunting, fishing, and cooking, as
well as place-names are standard textbook presentation. The lack of
activity based language learning could be attributed as much to the need
for modernisation of the language itself as the formalism of the grammar-
translation methods which is still in vogue in modern language teaching in
Japan (see Honda, 1997).
There are now progressive approaches to Ainu language teaching
instruction, for example through manga (cartoon) as in Yokoyama and
Ainu in Japan 333
Chiris little books Ainu Itak (1987) (Ainu Language Manual) and Ainu
Ukoyso-Itak (1988) (Ainu Conversation Manual). Here, standard Greet-
ings situations situations typical to Japan and Japanese are employed, as
when Ainu speakers are shown bowing. (Traditional Ainu greeting
involves rubbing the palms of the hands together from side to side away
from the body.) Dialogues in social context (travel, supermarket, televi-
sion) are now routine in many instructional materials and illustrations
typically include representations of Ainu cultural production such as the
highly regarded Ainu kimono (e.g. embroidery, appliqu, handweaving;
see cover designs by artist Sanae Ogawa for Maher & Macdonald, 1995).
Nakagawa has constructedanimportant pathwaynot onlyinAinulinguis-
tics generally (data collection, analysis, pedagogy) but also in the orienta-
tion of Ainu language teaching towards the Chitose dialect (see his Ainu-
Japanese Dictionary: Chitose Dialect, 1995 and Express Ainu, 1997 a self-study
textbook for beginners). He notes: Although today the Chitose region is
considered to be the centre of the Ainu language-revitalisation movement,
serious study of the regions dialect began only in the late 1980s, prior to
that time, few Ainu-language researchers knew that the area had so many
Ainu speakers (1999: 374).
Ainu language study has been propelled also by interest in the kamui-
yukar andoina: the language of music andceremony. The worldwide ethno-
music/world-music boom, which employed the vernacular languages of
Africa, South-East Asia and Europe, arrived in Japan in the 1980s and has
had no small impact on Ainu music and dance. Along with the distinctive
Okinawan music of the Japanese south, Ainu yukar has now emerged in
modern format. An Ainu jazz musician Moshiri (mosir being the Ainu
peoples designation for the territory Hokkaido) has adapted Ainu songs
such as Kamuy Chikap (Gods Bird) for jazz. Likewise, some cultural events,
the language of Ainu prayers and ceremonies are experiencing a renais-
sance. In the chanting of iyaihumke (lullabies) and two main types of song
(kamui-yukar totemic gods; oina ancestral heroes) ritual language inAinu
is performed during the following ceremonies. These ceremonies, many of
which, like the Fox God Ceremony from1986, are being performed for the
first time in several decades. This is summarised in Chart 2 (various
sources).
Chart 2 Ritual language maintenance and revival
Kamui-nomi (prayer of offering to the spirits)
Bekambe (Water Caltropo Festival)
Upopo and Tapkar (dance of the Menoko [Ainu women])
334 Africa and Asia
Rimse Dance Songs
Shakushain Warrior Memorial Ceremony
Icharupa (Memorial Ceremony for Ancestors)
Icharupa (Memorial for Ainu skeletons seized from Ainu burial places by
universities)
Kunashiri-Menashi Ceremony (Commemorating Japanese invasion of
Hokkaido 200 years before)
Chiron-up-Kamui-Iyomante (Fox God Ceremony)
Kimun Kamui Iyomante (God of the Mountain Bear Ceremony)
Ashiri-Cheppu-Nomi (First Fish Ceremony)
Chipsanke (Boat Launching Ceremony)
The Imeru movement led by Ainu artist and activist Mieko Chikap has
beeninvolvedinthe resurgence of these traditional customs andtraditions;
the word imeru (thunderbolt) is taken from an Ainu epic and symbolises
activism and awareness.
Stage 7 Most users of Ainu are socially integrated individuals and
an ethnolinguistically active population
The precise number of native speakers of Ainu is inconclusive, and the
proposition that the Ainu language is virtually extinct is questionable.
The number is certainly small; according to Fujimura (1989: 37) possibly
around 30, almost all in their eighties. (Speakers of Japanese in Japan
number around 120 million). These are apparently drastic circumstances
for the Ainu population, conservatively estimated by the Hokkaido Regis-
tration Office to be close to 25,000. The Hokkaido Ainu Association has
been quoted as estimating the actual number to be 5060,000 (Oda, 1984:
287). However, given the very long and formidable history of the Ainu
people throughout Japan, not just in Hokkaido but in the Tohoku region
also, the history of enforced assimilation and fear of admitting ethnic iden-
tity, a more likely estimation would involve much higher numbers of
people throughout the entire archipelago who feel able to acknowledge
their Ainu heritage. A more accurate figure, one might estimate, is five
times the above. We just do not know.
Stage 7 must be treatedcarefully in the case of Ainu. Perennial problems
arise at the level of definition of native speaker, crucially in ascertaining
numbers and even more crucially in what constitutes ethno-linguistically
active. We revisit the numbers problem. The number of native speakers
(broaddefinition) might safely be estimatedto be less than100 whilst some
people would maintain that it is less than five. Ainu people avoid the
numbers game for good reason. It is not merely a matter of statistical
Ainu in Japan 335
interest. Talk of numbers is redolent of the anthropologists myth of the
dying race, the vanishing people (Maher, 1994; Sawai, 1998). Okuda (1998:
148) correctly warns: It is very difficult to estimate the number of speakers
of the Ainu language. There is no official census and the number of
speakers who are known by researchers or journalists is very small. But
there are alsomanypeople whodonot reveal their competence tospeakthe
Ainulanguage . . . In1988, it was saidthat the last three speakers of a certain
dialect had died. But after their death, one or more other fluent speakers
became known. The number also varies according to how competence is
measured. The most active user/learners are not native speakers but are
rather second language speakers most of whom are adults.
Concerning the matter of categories of Ainu users and what constitutes
an active user, a useful schemata is elaborated by DeChicchis (1995: 110).
He distinguishes the main types of Ainu speakers: (1) archival Ainu
speakers, members of a robust multigenerational Ainu speech commu-
nity . . . Few of these people remain alive, however, the audio and video
recordings of their speech are culturally esteemed as representing a gener-
ally pure form of the Ainu language, and this has secured their influential
role as models to be emulated by living speakers; (2) old Ainu-Japanese
bilinguals who, nowelders, have a variedrange of fluency andwho as chil-
dren and young adults spoke Ainu on a routine basis in a natural commu-
nity setting ; (3) token Ainu speakers who, as ethnic Ainu, consider
themselves unable to speak Ainu but recall older relatives speaking Ainu
and who possess a command of words and formulaic phrases for certain
contexts; and finally, (4) second language learners (both ethnic Ainu and
non-Ainu) who possess no personal memories of anyAinuspeechcommu-
nity but who are studying and speaking Ainu as a second language. Atti-
tudes and self-perception often distinguish categories (3) and (4). As
DeChicchis notes: These younger second-language learners see them-
selves as learning a new language. Even those who are ethnically Ainu do
not view themselves as having lost a language. These younger second-
language learners recognise their marginal abilities in Ainu, but they view
these abilities as being a partial competency rather than as being a partial
incompetence, and in this respects they differ from the token speakers. At
Ainu cultural events, these younger second-language learners seem more
willing to speak Ainu than do the generally older token Ainu speakers
(DeChicchis, 1995: 111).
There is a perceptual if not objective gapbetweenthe things some linguists
have observed or passed on as a socio-linguistic canon and how the Ainu
people view themselves (Toyoka, 1983). Pons (1976) work, for example, is
336 Africa and Asia
entitledAinu-go wa Ikite iru[The AinuLanguage is Alive] (see alsoPons asser-
tions of Ainulanguage vitality, 1986).There is, inthe first place, anintense self-
awareness of alivinghistoryandheritage(Maher, 1991b) andat thesametime
doubts that Ainu can be revived in toto and therefore Is it worth bothering at
all? (R. Toyoka personal communication).
The ethno-symbolic vitality of Ainu plays a role in revitalisation. Here
the objective realityof a symbolic role inpersonal life uncouples traditional
criteria on the existence or non-existence of a language. Replying to my
question: Do you speak Ainu? an Ainu woman replied after a long pause:
Thats a difficult question. I dont speak it and I cant understand it but I
knowit and I can sing in it. Its always kind of here, a voice inside. And its
never left me (personal communication Kyoko Kitahara). Aholistic view
of language existence (presence?) obliges sociolinguists to re-examine
both traditional criteria of language vitality.
Stage 6 The attainment of intergenerational informal oralcy and its
demographic concentration and institutional reinforcement
The stage of reconstituting Ainu is a difficult one and depends upon
what elements are considered important in the preservation of Ainu
culture. Asurvey of adult Ainu people living in Hokkaido (membership of
the Ainu association Hokkaido Ainu Utari Kyokai is roughly 24,000)
submitted to the Hokkaido government in 1994 gathered opinion on atti-
tudes tothe Ainulanguage. The Report onExistingConditions of the Ainu
People in Hokkaido indicated that more hope and aspiration for the Ainu
language exists than previously thought.
Q: What do you know about Ainu culture?
A1: The Ainu language (37.3%)
A2: Wood carving (33.6%)
Q :How much Ainu language do you speak?
A1: I am able to speak it (0.8%)
A2: I can a little (5.4%)
A3: I cannot speak Ainu but have some knowledge of the language
(37.1%)
Q: Would you yourself like to speak Ainu?
A1: Yes, willingly (6.5.%)
A2: Yes, if the opportunity presented itself (56.9%)
The issue of what constitutes language knowledge is pointed out by
leading folklorist and retired politician Shigeru Kayano (1993: 365) who
has noted, for instance: When I go to a village for recording and tell a
Ainu in Japan 337
wepeker [traditional folktale] to people of my age they listen intently. Many
say afterwards that although they cannot repeat it they can more or less
understand the story. In short, they can comprehend Ainu although they
cannot speak it. Those who have this comprehension can soon come to be
able to speak the language. Sawai sums upthe present situationas follows:
Despite the fact that all Ainu speakers today speak Japanese as a result of
language shift, they enjoy speaking and listening to the Ainu language on
manyoccasions, at ceremonies, prayers, songs or oral traditions; theyenjoy
the Ainu language irrespective of fluency . . . (Sawai, 1998: 186).
Code-switching is normal in Ainu families. Sometimes this is what
might be considered low-level, like lexical borrowing, and at other times it
takes the formof vigorous code-mixing. Evendialectal differences between
individuals from families can be perceived R- san and I chat and as we
talk and use Ainu words sometimes we realise that what I said was a word
in my dialect and what R-san said was in her dialect (Harumi Sawai
personal communication, 1996).
In 1997, after years of deliberation and controversy, the Japanese
government enactedthe Ainu Shinpo (AinuNewLaw) formally Ainu Bunka
no Shinko narabi ni Ainu no Dento tou ni kansuru Chishiki no Fukyu oyobi
Keihatsu ni kansuru Horitsu (Act for the Promotion of Ainu Culture, the
Dissemination of Knowledge of Ainu Tradition, and Educational
Campaign). The newAct (hereafter APCL) contains the following flagship
statement:
This Act aims to bring to reality a society in which the ethnic pride of
the Ainu people is respected, enhancing the development of diverse
cultures in our nation by the implementation of measures for the
promotion of Ainu culture (hereafter Ainu Traditions), the dissemi-
nation of knowledge about Ainu Traditions, and education of the
nation (My translation)
The provisions of the law did not generally meet the demands of the Ainu
people. Certainly, the newlawdoes treat language as a defining character-
istic of Ainu culture: The Ainu Culture in this Act means the Ainu
language and cultural properties such as music, dance, crafts, and other
cultural properties which have been inherited by the Ainu people, and
other cultural properties developedfromthese (Article 2, ACPL). The new
law, however, provides for cultural revival without addressing any of the
social and political conditions which led to and which continue to provoke
the debilitation of the Ainu language. Needless to say, the law received
338 Africa and Asia
Ainu in Japan 339
Table14.1Development of newmeasures for thepromotionof Ainuculture
Measures Implementation
Comprehensive and
practical research
Enhance the role of Hokkaido as research centre
Establish research organisation and system including but
going beyond ethnology, sociology, history, linguistics
Establish Ainu Research Promotion Centre
The training of young researchers
Introduction of education about Ainu at Institutes of
Higher Education
Promotion of the
Ainu language
Establishment of regular Ainu language classes for sys-
tematic study
Establishment of teacher training programmes
Development of teaching materials
Support for existing programmes at institutes of higher
education
Support for existing language classes at local level
Ainu language-programmes (courses) on TVand radio
Ainu language speech contest
Reproduction of
traditional living
space promotion of
Ainu culture
Establish suitable places and facilities (e.g. parks, exhibi-
tions) for the display of Ainu culture
Collection, translation, soring of Yukar and oral literature
Training storytellers
Preparation of manual for reproduction of Ainu lifestyle
and culture
Dispatching advisers on Ainu cultural activity
Exhibitions of Ainu craftwork
Promotion of the traditional performing arts
Video displays of oral Ainu literature
International exchange related to Ainu culture
Presentation of the Ainu Cultural Award
Ainu Cultural Festival
Dissemination of
understanding and
knowledge about
Ainu tradition
Publication of leaflets about the Ainu and textbooks for
elementary junior high school
Maintenance of home page
Ainu Culture Centre activities
Seminars and lectures to disseminate knowledge
Adapted fromthe Report of the Experts Meeting on Ainu Affairs (1996) to Chief Cabinet Sec-
retary. Www.frpac.or.jp/english/Report.Report.html. Jan. 2000
much criticism (see Nakagawa, 1999). Details that relate to language
revival (measures and forms of implementation) are shown in Table 14.1.
Stage 5 Ainu literacy at home, school, community, but without
taking on extra-communal reinforcement of such literacy
Renewed study of Ainu oral literature was given historical impetus by
Kyosuke Kindaichis eight-volume compilation of the Yukar, the Yukarshu
(Kindaichi & Kannari, 195968) in collaboration with the Ainu reciter
Matsu Kannari. Several books and pamphlets have been published
explaining aspects of traditional life and language in Ainu communities
(see Maher, 1991). Many of these are based on the Ainu oral tradition
whereby the telling of stories was passed on fromgeneration to generation
expressed in Ainu as Ek ipekoro ae pashikuma or Come and eat and I will tell
you the story. Shigeru Kayano has been a major figure in the updating of
this literature andindeedinthe Ainurevival as a whole (1983, 1984a, 1984b,
1985a, 1985b, 1985c,1986a, 1986b,1987, 1988, 1989a). Books written for the
general audience include AinuNenoanAinu[AnAinuTells his ownStories]
(1989b) which is a description of Kayanos childhood in the Ainu commu-
nity. Pamphlets on individual topics have been compiled by Kayano and
printed by the Nibutani Bunka Shiryokan (Nibutani Cultural Information
Centre), e.g. place-name descriptions, in particular the Shishirimuka zoroi
no chimei [Place-names along the Shishiri river] (1985). Typical of the
recent range of popular materials is The Ainu Bunka no Kiso [Fundamental
Ainu Culture] (Ainu Minzoku Hakubutsukan, 1987) which sets out brief
descriptions of the Ainu language, history of the people, hunting, food,
houses, religious customs, songs and play, and so on. Other personal biog-
raphies include that compiled by Hokkaido Universitys 1983 edition of
Kura Sunasawas Ku sukup oruspe (My Life). In this oral history, a speaker
of the Ishikari Dialect of Ainu, (a dialect stretching roughly from
Asahikawa to Sapporo), describes her life (she was born in 1897) and expe-
riences with the Ainu language.
Stage 4 Ainu in lower education and community institutions
From the 1980s onwards, vigorous efforts have been made to increase
the cultural vitality of Ainu in the form of the revival of traditional rituals,
the development of teaching materials, language classes in community
centres and some universities, and a body of Ainu-sponsored political
proposals whichtouches uponlanguage maintenance. The UnitedNations
declaration on language rights in the Year of the Indigenous Peoples (1993)
was a landmark in the history of language maintenance among the periph-
340 Africa and Asia
eral language communities in Japan. Supported by many other language
minorities, the Ainu achieved significant progress in their struggle for
language protection. Progress is a slippery concept. Farina Becsky (1999),
in a comparison of educational regimes for minority languages in Vene-
zuela (Pemon) and Japan (Ainu) cautions that an educational system
(always the toy of political interests) must make language diversity more
understandable to all participants and a humanistic goal rather than a
token of progress if the aim of common society is to go beyond the mere
highlighting of difference.
Ainu is not taught in any primary or secondary (public or private)
schools of the Japanese educational system. It is, however, taught in
language centres throughout Hokkaido, in some universities in Hokkaido
and in Chiba National University on the outskirts of Tokyo.
In 1987, a commercial radio station in Hokkaido began broadcasting an
Ainu-for-Radio language course (STV Radio 1988). The programmes are
not broadcast nationwide but inHokkaidoonlyandtheyare airedbetween
6.05 and 6.20 on Sunday morning. Ainu for early risers. This is a significant
advance for maintaining the presence of the Ainu language in Hokkaido.
The audience rating is relatively high for that day and time: 0.10.2%.
(Hatakeyama, 1990: 47). The programme aims to create public interest in
both the language and the traditional Ainu outlook on nature and life.
Sapporo Broadcasting Corporation received more than 1000 requests for
textbooks in 1988 and of these 40 were from the Tokyo region outside the
transmission area. In 1998, broadcasts were expanded to Sunday and
Saturday morning (15 minutes) andon the STVinternet home page. All the
lecturers are Ainu and they (and their dialects) rotate in three-monthly
cycles. The system is particularly useful in training and cultivating poten-
tial lecturers in Ainu.
At no other time in the history of Ainu studies in Japan have there been
more courses on the Ainu language at university level. In the Tokyo area, a
syllabus was formulated at Waseda University in 1975 and the language
continues to be offered at the undergraduate level. Chiba National Univer-
sityis a centre for Ainulanguage studies, offeringAinulanguage instruction
as well as linguistic analysis (Nakagawa, 1991). At International Christian
University, Tokyo, there is thesis and research supervision on
multilingualismincludingAinusociolinguistics. Ahandful of universities in
Hokkaido already offer Ainu courses and instruction. Language learning
classes havebeenheldintheNibutani Valleyareafor manyyears but this has
been joined by a number of other Ainugo kyoshitsu (Ainu language classes)
now in operation in community centres such as (in order of establishment)
Nibutani (1987), Asahikawa (1987), Urakawa (1989), Kushiro (1989), Akan
Ainu in Japan 341
(1991), Chitose (1991), Shizunai (1992), Mukawa (1992), Obihiro (1993),
Shiranuka (1994), Noboribetsu (1996), Tomakomai (1997) and occasionally,
language institutes in Tokyo (e.g. Shorin). The Ainu Taimuzu (Ainu Times)
was launched in 1987 as a means of promoting literacy in Ainu. The text is
entirely Ainu both romanised (romaji) and katakana.
In the RSLmodel appliedto Ainuthere is virtually no place in which the
remainingstages are relevant. Inthis I refer toStage 3 (the use of Ainuinthe
lower work sphere (outside of the Ainu neighbourhood community)
involving interaction between Ainu and non-Ainu; Stage 2 (no use of Ainu
in lower governmental services and mass media but not in the higher
services of either) Stage 1 (no use of Ainu in higher level educational, occu-
pational). However, there is governmental effort (see chronology above) to
promote the study of Ainu (without the safety provided by political inde-
pendence). I refer here tothe (1997) AinuCulture PromotionLaw. It should
be noted that when Kayano (above) was elected socialist member of the
Diet in 1985 he gave his inaugural speech in Ainu a massive symbolic
gesture. Clearly, this indicates that any RSL model must assume that a
language may move across stages simultaneously or stage-jump, in the
sense that the Ainu language revitalisation is now receiving government
support legislatively andinstitutionally without any conceptionthat it will
or can be used in public education or the media.
Conclusion
Stage-jumping the Ainu language reversal continuumis most visible in
the political rationale advanced for the Ainu Culture Promotion Law
enacted in the late 1990s.
IntheRLSmodel, theearlystages of re-assembly of alanguageis pressed
by the need for reformulation of theoretical constructs surrounding the
language. For aneffective retooling of the Ainulanguage nothing less thana
remaking of history is needed. Let me examine this further.
Hitherto, many historians have sought to select, emphasise and exclude
in order to further elite interests, sometimes not even conscious of the fact
that they further the interests of the state because they are so much part of
the power system from within which they write (Macdonald, 1995: 301).
For the tamingof history, Macdonaldnotes that incontemporaryJapanese
mythology, the past is revered, the nationglorified, andcontradictions and
conflicts, including discrimination, are concealed. Vast social and
economic differences are obliterated. Certain practices are selected to
symbolise traditional Japanese culture (301). This idealised culture finds
342 Africa and Asia
its highest expression in Kyoto an elegant city which played court to
Imperial culture rather than Sapporo the new and cold far north).
We sometimes hear that Japan has lost its culture in keeping with the
peripheralisation of other cultural experience. There is no space to elabo-
rate on this theme here; suffice to note first that advocacy of cultural and
linguistic pluralism in Japan is not a postmodern phenomenon but advo-
cacy has a heritage; secondly, pluralismis usually conceptualised in Japan
in the form of a centre-periphery dichotomy. If language shift is to be
reversed, Ainu can draw upon traditional as well as new formulations. I
will cite one such historical formulation.
The traditional movement to preserve and encourage mingei peoples
art began in the late 19th century. In this movement, the machine became
the metaphor for the capitalist-industrial mainstream with its speed,
power, complete uniformityandprecision contrastingwiththe little hand-
iwork of the regions, the craft of Ainu and Okinawan which alone could
produce creativity, adaptability, freedom, heterogeneity (Yanagi, 1982:
108, quoted in Steele, 1995). Fully aware of the tension between national
and local allegiances, support for the ideals of the mingei movement coin-
cided with founding the so-called Shirakaba-ha (White Birch Society) led by
Yanagi Soetsu a complex and paradoxical figure of imaginative but also
authoritarian and romantic-nationalist tendency. These ideals embraced
cultural pluralismincorporatinglanguage maintenance andfreedomat the
same time. In1939, the pushtoeliminate local dialects andthe Okinawanin
particular to be replaced by hyojungo (standard language) was initiated
by the Japanese government. There was opposition to this attack on local
languages and a damning of the policy of centralisation. Fear of the
periphery and (regional/cultural) fragmentation whereby the centre
cannot hold is not confined to Japan but has long been foregrounded in
language politics. As Yanagi wrote in a document sent to the Office of
Education on the Problemof the Okinawan Language: StandardLanguage
andOkinawan Language are both languages of Japan. One is a language of
the centre: the other is a language of the periphery. These two languages
have a close connection with each other. It is our understanding that both
should be respected as Japanese national languages (Yanagi, 1981,
Zenshu, 15: 1489, in Steele, 1995). He continued:
Languages of the periphery are important elements in the national
language . . . Anyweakness of the languages of the peripheryis anindi-
cation of a weakness in the cultures of the periphery. Local areas that
are vigorous normally have a vigorous lifestyle, and possess vigorous
literature and music. If Japan were to be a country without languages
Ainu in Japan 343
of the periphery, this wouldsurelytransformJapanintoa countrywith
little that would be unique . . . Just the existence of the Japanese is
important withregardtoworldculture; there are strongreasons for the
existence of the languages of the periphery as they relate to Japanese
culture (Yanagi, 149).
Whilst Yanagi at no point questioned the rightness of the colonial status of
either Koreans or Ainu he defended their cultural contribution and
language rights. He held an exhibition of Ainu crafts in Tokyo in 1941 to
demonstrate that the Ainudidnot leada cultureless existence andto chal-
lenge the prevalent view that they should be left to wither away.
No less than in the 1880s when Ainu began performing the yukar and
dances for mainland tourists who visited Hokkaido, Japans wild north,
there arises in the 21st century the question of the future role of Ainu tradi-
tion in mediating cultural identity. The issue spirals into larger issues in the
anthropology of tourism, the tradition of invented tradition. After all, the
tourist trademark of the Ainu wood carving of the bear was an idea
importedfromSwitzerlandinthe 1920s andthat the exotic Marimo festival
at Lake Akan was first held only in 1950. This is no a criticism but rather
witness to the ever-present globalisation of culture and the fabric of
economic relations in micro-systems of society. The implications for
language revival are complicated but the overall effect is to arouse interest
amongAinuandJapaneseinthematter of cultural revival andmaintenance.
At some stage of RLS there arises the need for political struggle. This is
the case with Ainu. The campaign for legal recognition of the Ainu
language as one of the authentic languages of Japanis verysignificant. This
is the site of media attention and the concern of politicians both at the local
and national level.
The Ainu people are very conscious of the connection between the
struggle for recognition of human rights and maintenance of their
language. This is typified in the wording of a petition by Tokyos Ainu
community, the Kanto Ainu Association: The government for the last 120
years has not recognisedthat Ainupeoplehavetheright tospeaktheir native
language in daily life. Ainu children wanting to learn the language must
endureprejudiceandbullying. It is ridiculous that wehavetohavelessons to
learn our native language (Kitahara, Kyoko, July 1990, Asahi Shinbun).
The official response by the government to these proposals has been
slow but nevertheless forthcoming. There is now emerging the establish-
ment of clear goals. For the Ainulanguage this is a positive steptowards the
integration of aims for ethnolinguistic revitalisation.
In the intergenerational transmission of Ainu the perennial issue of
344 Africa and Asia
ownership of the language is crucial. Hondas observation is insightful in
this regard. She has noted, firstly, the difficulty encountered by younger
learners of Ainu whose normal learner errors (particularly pronunciation)
are seized upon by older native-speakers. Nagging discourages younger
learners who then categorise Ainu as the property of the old rather than the
heritage of a coming, younger generation (Honda personal communication,
1994). There is also the fact that more and more Japanese and not just ethnic
Ainu take up Ainu language study. There is a crossing of ethnic allegiances
in which language and culture are floating variables in shifting ethno-
linguistic boundaries. The role of Japanese participants in an Ainu language
programme receives attention in Anderson and Iwasaki-Goodmans useful
study (2000) of an Ainu language programme in the Nibutani community.
The sociolinguistic expression language transmission is conceptually
polyvalent. Among Ainu speakers attitudes towards transmission are
indeed complex and varied and rich. An effective RSL will take this into
account. An example from Okuda (1998), is worth noting here. This Ainu
fieldworker worked with two old ladies who undoubtedly wished for the
transmission of their mother-tongue, but what puzzled the linguist was
that ideas of transmissiondifferedconsiderably. One of his informants was
eager to recite the oral literature and keen to translate Japanese into Ainu:
It appears that she wished for the revival of her language in daily life or its
bilingual use with the Japanese language (Okuda, 1998: 144). The second
informant was principally concerned with Ainu ritual speech and not in
daily conversation. She resisted translating modern Japanese into Ainu
and refused to translate separate lexical items into Ainu preferring instead
to recite a complete piece of literature in which the word occurred. This
Ainu speaker permitted the use of Japanese for Ainu rituals. Okuda noted
that among many speakers he could not perceive any apparent wish for
the transmission of their language to the next generation.
What is somewhat unclear in the ascending RLS model is where the
political andthe historical fit inandwhere theycanfit incomfortably. Polit-
ical reformism and the rewriting of history are crucial factors in Ainu
revitalisation.
Postcript
The last wordinthis paper belongs tothe Ainulanguage itself. The tongue-
twister Who is Greater told by Sadamo Hiraga (in Kayano, 1986) contains a
useful summaryof theparadoxfacedbyanylanguagethat is undergoingshift
and revitalisation: a paradoxical, optimistic affirmation of the future.
Ainu in Japan 345
Who is Greater?
A little wolf falls on the ice
For the ice is greater
The ice is greater but is thawed by the sun
For the sun is greater
The sun is greater but above are the clouds
For the clouds are greater
The clouds are greater but leak the rain
For the rain is greater
The rain is greater but falls on the ground
For the ground is greater
The ground is greater but is pierced by the trees
For the trees are greater
The trees are greater but are cut by the Ainu
For the Ainu are greater
The Ainu are greater but die in the end
Cry fussa, fussa as the Ainu die
And the Ainu will return again.
[Note: fussa: a shout or call when felling trees]
References
Ainu Minzoku Hakubutsukan (1988) Ainu Bunka no Kiso Chishiki. Hokkaido: Ainu
Minzoku Hakubutsukan.
Anderson, F. and Iwasaki-Goodman, M. (2000 forthcoming) Language and culture
revitalization in a Hokkaido community. In Noguchi and Fotos (eds) Studies in
Japanese Bilingualism, Chapter 4. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Asahi Shinbun (newspaper) (1990) Ainugo o Ima ni Ikasu. 5 May, 29.
Batchelor, J. (1938) An Ainu-English-Japanese Dictionary. 4th edn.
Davis, G. (1987) The Ainu: Japans indigenous Indians. Tokyo Journal October: 79
and 1819.
DeChicchis, J. (1995) The current state of the Ainu language. Journal of Multilingual
and Multicultural Development. Also in Multilingual Japan (pp.10324). Clevedon:
Multilingual Matters.
Farina Becsky, V. (1999) The Relation Between Endangered Languages Revival and Edu-
cation for Mainstream Society with Refence to the Ainu (Japan) and the Pemon
(Venezuela) Indigenous Groups. MAThesis, Kyoto University Graduate School of
Education, Kyoto.
Hatakeyama, S. (1990) Attitudes towards the Ainu and its language. Unpublished
MA thesis. International Christian University, Tokyo.
Honda, Y. (1992) Ainugo Kyoshitsu Piratori [Piratori Ainu Language Class]. In
Kubota, Kuritsubo, Noyama, Hino and Fujii (eds) Kosho Bungaku 2: Ainu
Bungaku [Oral Literature 2: Ainu Literature]. Tokyo: Iwanami Shoten.
Imura, K. (1985) Ainugowaikinokoruka. Gengo 14(2), 3641. KamuytoranoKyokai.
346 Africa and Asia
Imura, K. (1987) Ainu go Kaiwa: shokkyuken. Hokkaido: Kamuy torano Kyokai.
Kayano, S. (1983) Okikurumi no boken. Hokkaido: Kominshoten.
Kayano, S. (1984a) Pasui wa ikimono. Hokkaido: Hokuto Shuppan.
Kayano, S. (1984b) Kitsune no Charanke. Tokyo: Komine Shoten.
Kayano, S. (1985a) Shishirimuka Zoroi no Chimei. Hokkaido: Nibutani Ainu Bunka
Shiryokan.
Kayano, S. (1985b) The Romance of the Bear God: Ainu Folktales. Tokyo: Taishukan.
Kayano, S. (1985c) Hitotsubu no Sachipoporo. Tokyo: Heibon.
Kayano, S. (1986a). Ainu no Mingu. Tokyo:Suzusawa Shoten.
Kayano, S. (1986b) Kibori no Ookami. Tokyo: Komine Shoten.
Kayano, S. (1987). Ainugo Kaiwa. Shokyuhen (Videotape and textbook).
Kayano, S. (1988) Ainu no Hi. Tokyo: Asahi Shinbunsha.
Kayano, S. (1989a) Yasashii Ainugo (I). Hokkaido: Nibutani Ainugo Kyoshitsu.
Kayano, S. (1989b) Takusan no fushigi: Ainu ne no an Ainu (An Ainu tells his own sto-
ries). Tokyo: Fukuinkan Shoten.
Kayano, S. (1993) Ainu ethnic and linguistic revival. In Loos and Osanai (eds)
Nibutani AInugo Kyoshitsu Kohoshi (Sokango 20) [Magazine of Kayano
Shigerus Dictionary of the Ainu language] (pp. 36067). Tokyo: Sanseido.
Kindaichi, K. and Kannari, M. (195968) Yukara-shu 18. [Collection of Yukar]. To-
kyo: Sanseido.
Macdonald, G. (1995) The politics of diversity in the nation-state. In J. Maher andG.
Macdonald (eds) Diversity in Japanese Culture and Language (pp. 291315). Lon-
don and Sydney: Kegan Paul International.
Maher, J. (1985) The role of English as an international language of medicine. Ap-
plied Linguistics 5, 1836.
Maher, J. (1989) Doctors who write: Language preference in medical communica-
tion in Japan. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 52, 23042.
Maher, J. (1991a) Hashigaki [Preface]. In Maher and Yashiro (eds) Nihon no
Biringarizumu (pp. ivvii). Tokyo, Kenkyusha.
Maher, J. (1991b) Ainugo no Fukkatsu (The Ainu Language Revival). In Maher and
Yashiro (eds) Nihon no Biringarizumu (pp. 14969). (Tokyo: Kenkyusha).
Maher, J. (1993) The language situation of Japan. The Encyclopedia of Language &Lin-
guistics 4, 4523.
Maher, J. (1994) Shigo to iuShinwa: Ainugo no Renaissance (The Mythof Language
Death: The Ainu Language Renaissance). In J. Maher and N. Honna (eds)
Atarashii Nihonkan, Sekaikan ni mukatte: Nihon ni okeru Gengo to Bunka no Tayosei
(pp. 116131). Tokyo: Kokusaishoin.
Maher, J. (1996) North Kyushu Creole: Alanguage-contact model for the origins of
Japanese. In D. Denoon, M. Hudson, G. McCormack and T. Morris-Suzuki (eds)
Multicultural Japan: Palaeolithic to Postmodern (pp. 3145). Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Maher, J. (1997) Community languages in Japan. In Kokuritsu Kokugo Kenkyujo
(ed.) Proceedings of the Conference on Language Treatment. Tokyo: Bonjinsha.
Maher, J. and Honna, N. (1994a) Atarashii Nihonkan, Sekaikan ni mukatte: Nihon ni
okeru Gengo to Bunka no Tayosei. [Towards a NewOrder: Linguistic and Cultural
Diversity in Japan]. Tokyo: Kokusai Shoin.
Maher, J. andHonna, N. (1994b) Hajime ni. InJ. Maher andN. Honna (eds) Atarashii
Ainu in Japan 347
Nihonkan, Sekaikan ni mukatte: Nihon ni okeru Gengo to Bunka no Tayosei (pp. 715).
Tokyo, Kokusaishoin.
Maher, J. and Macdonald, G. (1995) Diversity in Japanese Culture and Language. Lon-
don and Sydney: Kegan Paul International.
Maher, J. and Yashiro, K. (1991) Nihon no Bairingarizumu. [Bilingualism in Japan].
Tokyo: Kenkyuusha.
Maher, J. andYashiro, K. (1995a) Multilingual Japan. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Maher, J. and Yashiro, K. (1995b) Introduction. In J. Maher and K. Yashiro (eds)
Multilingual Japan (pp. 112). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Mervio, M. (1995) Cultural representation of the Japanese in international relations
and politics. Acta Universitatis Tamperensis, Ser. A, Vol. 448.
Nakagawa, H. (1995) Ainugo Chitose Hogen Jiten (AinuJapanese Dictionary.
Chitose Dialect). Tokyo: Sofukan.
Nakagawa. H. (1997) Ekusupuresu Ainugo (Express Ainu). Tokyo: Heibonsha.
Nakagawa, H. (1999) Present andfuture. InW.W. FitzhughandC.O. Dubreuil (eds)
Ainu: Spirit of a Northern People (pp. 3713). National Museum of Natural His-
tory, Smithsonian Institution.
Narita, T. and Hanazaki, K. (1975) Kindaika no naka no Ainu Sabetsu Kozo (The Struc-
ture of Ainu Discrimination within Modernization). Tokyo: Akaishi Shobo.
Oda, K. (1984) Ikiru Ainu no sonzai to mirai. Sekai 450, 2837.
Okuda, O. (1998) Onthe objectives of linguistic researchonthe Ainu. InK. Matsumura
(ed.) Studies in Endangered Languages (pp. 143148). Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo.
Peng, F.C.C. andGeiser, P. (1977) The Ainu: The Past inthe Present. Hiroshima: Bunka
Hyoron.
Pon, F. (1976) (revised1986) Ainugo wa ikite iru[Ainuis alive]. Tokyo: Shinsensha.
Sasamura, J. (1999) Beyond the Ainu Shinpo: An Ainu viewIn W.W. Fitzhugh and
C.O. Dubreuil (eds) Ainu: Spirit of a Northern People (pp. 36970). National Mu-
seum of Natural History, Smithsonian Institution.
Sawai, H. (1998) The present situation of the Ainu language. In Matsumura Kazuto
(ed.) Studies in Endangered Languages. Ichel Linguistic Studies, Vol. 1. Tokyo:
Hituzi Syobo.
STV Radio (1988) Ainugo Koza: Irankaratte. Text. Hokkaido: Sapporo Radio.
Siddle, R. (1996) Race, Resistance and the Ainu. London: Routledge.
Siddle, R. (1995) The Ainu: Construction of an image. In Maher and Macdonald
(eds), (pp. 7394).
Steele, W. (1995) Nationalimand cultural pluralismin modern Japan: Soetsu Yanagi
and the Mingei Movement. In J. Maher and G. Macdonald (eds) Diversity in Japa-
nese Culture andLanguage (pp. 2748). LondonandSydney. KeganPaul International.
Tamura, S. (1956) Ainugo no doshi no kozo [The structure of Ainu verbs]. Gengo
Kenkyo 39, 2138.
Tamura, S. (1960) Studies ontheAinulanguage. Current Trends inLinguistics 2, 60832.
Tamura, S. (1970) Personal affixes in the Saru dialect of Ainu. In R. Jakobson and S.
Kawamoto (eds) Studies in General and Oriental Linguistics, Presented to Shiro
Hattori on the Occasion of his Sixtieth Birthday. Tokyo: TEC.
Tamura, S. (S. Fukuda) (1974) Ainugo ni tsuite. Gengo 3(1), 2732.
Tamura, S. (1981) Ainu-gonoonsei shirio, 3. WasedaDaigakuGogakuKyoikuKenkyujo.
Tamura, S. (1985) Ikita Ainugo o manabu tame ni: daigaku nogogaku kyoiku no
naka de. Gengo 14(2), 425.
348 Africa and Asia
Tanimoto, K. (1999) To Live is To Sing. In W.W. Fitzhugh and C.O. Dubreuil (eds)
Ainu: Spirit of a NorthernPeople (pp. 2826). National Nuseumof Natural History,
Smithsonian Institution.
Toyoka, S. (1983) Ainu minzoku kara no Ainuron. Chuo Koron 98(2), 30413.
Tsunemoto, T. (1999) The Ainu Shinpo. In W.W. Fitzhugh and C.O. Dubreuil (eds)
Ainu: Spirit of a NorthernPeople (pp. 3668). National Nuseumof Natural History,
Smithsonian Institution.
Yoshida, I. (191516) Ainu-go kaiwa hen. Nihon Jinruigaku, Vol. 30, Nos. 2, 5, 9, 10:
5863,1902, 2192,38792; Vol. 31, No. 6: 13555.
Yokoyama, T. andChiri, M. (1987) AinuItak (AinugoIlastoJiten)Tokyo: Kagyusha.
Yokoyama, T. and Chiri, M. (1988) Ainu Ukoyso-Itak (Ainugo Kaiwa Ilasto Jiten). To-
kyo: Kagyusha. Yoneda (Honda) Y. (1995) Comment on Mahers Community
Languages in Japan. Language Management for Multicultural Communities: Individ-
uals andCommunities Livingthe Differences. KokuritstuKokugoKenkyujo, Tokyo.
Ainu in Japan 349
Chapter 15
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts
B. SPOLSKY and E. SHOHAMY
The Revitalisation of Hebrew and the GIDS Model
In Fishman (1991), there is an admirable assessment of the revitalisation
of Hebrewwithin terms of the GIDS (Graded Intergenerational Disruption
Scale) model presentedinthat book. The antecedent toFishmans bookwas
his long career studying language shift. While many in the field of linguis-
tics appearednot to notice what was happening to the objects of their study
until Krauss (1991a,b) wrote about endangered languages, Fishman, from
his earliest publications (Fishman, 1951), had been dealing with language
loyalty and its reverse, language loss. He had spent his career working
closely and sympathetically with members of groups which were
concerned to stem language loss. From this rich and caring experience, he
developed a sensitive and compassionate appreciation of the complexities
and difficulties of the struggle to keep alive a language threatened by a
larger and more powerful one. In Fishman (1991), he aims to translate this
experience and his observations into a theory of what he calls RLS or
reversinglanguage shift. This theorypostulates sevensteps or levels which
define a state of language loss or determine the course of action that might
best remedy it. Noble as the goal is, and valuable as the advice will hope-
fullyturnout to be for groups resistinglanguage loss, there are a number of
problems left unsolved by the theory.
When the RLS and GIDS models are applied to Hebrew, some of the
incertitude become clear. The facts about Hebrew presented in Fishmans
book offer anexcellent account of the process of Hebrewlanguage revival
or revernacularisation or revitalisation. The three terms suggest the
complexity of the process and select different views of it. Revival, perhaps
the most popular, misses the frequently made point that Hebrew was in
active if limited use during the hundreds of years that it was no longer a
spoken language. Fishmans use of the termrevernacularisation stresses the
change from Hebrew as a classical literary sacred language to one with
dailyvernacular use (the termvernacular is definedinthis wayinStewart
(1968) and in Fishman (1970)). My preference for the term revitalisation
350
Africa and Asia
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts
(Spolsky, 1991, 1996a) highlights rather the critical importance of restoring
vitality or normal intergenerational transmission of the language as a
mother tongue.
The question that we will ask in this paper is whether what has
happened to Hebrewin the past century the revival, revernacularisation
andrevitalisation is best analysedas a case of reversing language shift, or
whether it is sufficiently unique in both task, method and outcome to chal-
lenge the RLS model. Consider first the literal meaning of the term. The
process of shifting from the use of Hebrew as a spoken vernacular may
have startedas early as the 6thcentury BCE, andcertainly was well along for
many living in ancient Israel by the beginning of the Common Era
(Chomsky, 1957; Rabin 1973). How long Hebrew continued to be spoken
among Jews is a matter of some debate but it is nowgenerally believedthat
there were still monolingual speakers of Hebrewinvillages of Judaea at the
time of the Bar Kochba Revolt, in the second century of the Common Era,
and native speakers even later.
It is during the latter part of this period of language loss that we should
look for efforts to reverse Hebrew language shift. We find evidence in a
number of statements in the Talmud calling for the use of Hebrew instead
of Aramaic. When the battle to save the language is close to the end, a new
strategy was proposed. Once it was clear that normal intergenerational
transmission could no longer be counted on, a formal system of language
preservation was mandated. When a boy reached the age of five, every
father was required to start teaching him Hebrew. Fathers might delegate
this responsibility to a school teacher. Every Jewish community was
required to establish a school where this teaching could be done. Here we
had the beginning of a policy of language preservation, of reversing
language shift, through establishing institutions for formal educational
transmission .
For a thousand and more years, this policy succeeded in keeping
Hebrew alive as an active literary and sacred language, albeit in a special
relationship with a functionally differentiated use of written Aramaic. The
two closely related languages, Hebrew and Aramaic, have remained
entwined ever since their first fateful meeting. Starting out as an imperial
language of government and business (and it remains until this day the
language in which a Jewish marriage contract (ketuba) and divorce papers
(get) are written), Aramaic developed after the return from Babylon into a
widespread vernacular, and thus into the first of a series of Jewish
languages. Its use in the compilation of the Babylonian Talmud after the
destruction of the Second Temple gave it a fixed place as a partner with
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 351
Hebrew in the leshon hakodesh that is still the language of Jewish sanctity.
For most Jews, Hebrew-and-Aramaic remainedthe language of prayer, the
language in which the sacred texts were written, the language most appro-
priate for writing. While there were Jewishcommunities witha lowlevel of
literacy, there were many others over the following centuries in which
there were glorious examples of literary production in the language. One
thinks of the religious poetry of the payetanim in the 7th century, or the
magnificent Hebrewliterature of the Arabic-Spanish period starting in the
10th century. This was again true of the Haskalah, the Enlightenment, at the
end of the 18th century, a period marked also by the flourishing of Hebrew
letters. This literary flame was still alight at the end of the 19th century,
whenit was pre-emptedbya newconcernfor revivingHebrewas a spoken
language, in line with European nationalist movements of the time.
Treating the revitalisation of Hebrew at the end of the 19th century as a
simple case of reversing language shift seems to call for one of two quite
unlikely interpretations. First, we might consider it a newstep, a thousand
years later, to preserve the spoken language. But this is somewhat far-
fetched for a thousand years, Jews had been using a series of Jewish
languages and co-territorial gentile languages for most vernacular pur-
poses. Alternatively, we might consider it as an endeavour to reverse the
loss of Hebrew literacy that was a consequence of the process of dimin-
ishing religious observance and education that occurred during the 19th
century. This case too is hard to make, for it was an essential feature of
Hebrew revival, as Fishman recognises, that it was to be a new revolu-
tionary break with the past and so with the religiously motivated teaching
of the sacred tongue to young boys. Both the territorial nationalismassoci-
ated with Hebrewand the cultural nationalismthat developed at the same
time associated with the revival of Yiddish and its standardisation were
secular movements, rejecting the religious beliefs that underlay the
previous efforts to maintain Hebrew. The leaders of the Hebrew revival
movement certainly did not wish to return to the old religiously controlled
educational system that had preserved literacy for so long.
The drive for Hebrew revival in both Europe and Palestine, like the
contemporaneous European-focused drive for changing the status of the
Yiddish that was the communal vernacular of east European Jews, came
not fromwithinthe religious community but fromsecularisedandsecular-
ising ideologists, supporters of a newJewish nationalismthat in the case of
Hebrew was largely territorialist and in the case of Yiddish, primarily
cultural. In fact, the early opponents of the revernacularisation of Hebrew
were those within the religious establishment who were most concerned
352 Africa and Asia
with the continuation of the use of Hebrew for its restricted sacred, reli-
gious and literacy functions.
Except in a rhetorical sense, there was no aim to revive the variety or
pattern of Hebrew that was being lost, but rather an ideological use of its
associations with a Great Tradition to justify its inclusion among a set of
other newslogans andsymbols for nationhood. The rhetoric andimages of
the ancient language were recruited to the demands of the new Zionist
movement. Look at the call for building the new city of Tel Aviv:
We must urgently acquire a considerable chunk of land, on which we
shall build our houses. Its place must be near Jaffa, and it will formthe
first Hebrew city, its inhabitants will be Hebrews a hundred percent;
Hebrewwill be spoken in this city, purity and cleanliness will be kept,
and we shall not go in the ways of the goyim. (Cited and translated in
Harshav (1993: 143) from the 1906 Prospectus for Tel Aviv.)
Hebrews, not Jews, speakingHebrewonly, pure andclean. The founders of
Tel Aviv rejected at once the ways of the goyim (gentiles) and their
languages and of the Jews (yidn) and their language Yiddish.
Returning to the terms of the GIDS model, there were at the time of this
call for revival no socially isolated old folks who were the vestigial users
of Hebrew, but rather a well-developedclassical sacredlanguage, the focus
of universal (more or less) male (mainly) elementaryeducation, withdevel-
opedhome, school andcommunityliteracy, andwithuse of that literacyon
the highest intra-community functions. The task undertaken by the
Hebraisers then was quite unlike normal RLS activity (including the activi-
ties tried a little later for Yiddish). Some of the difficulties of attempting to
fit both in the same model are illustrated in the effort to compare Hebrew
language revitalisation with contemporary Maori RLS activities (Spolsky,
1996a). As a heuristic for checking structural similarities, the approach has
some value, but the results are to show dissimilarity rather than shared
features.
As the account in Fishman (1991) concedes, the process of revitalisation
of Hebrew did not follow the model implied by GIDS, but it did include
some of the specific steps included in that model. Fishman (1991: 291
accepts this uniqueness: the unprecedented goal of revernacularising a
language of sanctity and/or literacy. It was a task to be accomplished, he
also notes, with active opposition of the preservers of sacred literacy the
very people who would be expected to be involved in normal RLS.
Translating the complex multi-directional process that occurred into a
simple model, Fishman acknowledges, is not easy, but he does summarise
the Hebrewrevernacularisation in these terms: The revernacularisation of
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 353
Hebrew started at stage 7 and then went onto stages 6, 5 (particularly for
adults) and 4 in a fairly rapid but far from inevitable succession. Now
Stage 7 is defined as socially integrated and ethnolinguistically active
speakers beyond child-bearing age. It is hard to see how this fits the
Hebrew case, for the theorists like Ben-Yehuda and the practitioners like
the Zionist pioneers were all social misfits, and of child-bearing age. They
took little interest in the older generation, but, it is true, did hope to recruit
others of their ownage grouptoaccept the ideologyof the newlanguage.
Stage 6 is more apt: the development of demographic concentrations
where intergenerational informal oracy can be established. These concen-
trations the older agricultural settlements like Petah Tikva, the revolu-
tionary kibbutzim, and the spanking new city of Tel Aviv played a key
role in this crucial stage of revitalisation. There appear to have been two
reinforcing stages. The first was the promulgation and acceptance of an
ideological principle of using Hebrew in public, most effective in devel-
oping intergenerational transmissioninthe kibbutzimwhere most areas of
life includingthe childrens house were public. The secondwas the accep-
tance of the need for intergenerational transmission and the willingness to
allow or encourage children to speak the language.
Stage 5 (literacy at home, school and community) had in fact in the
Hebrew case preceded Stages 7 and 6 (and by more than a 1000 years!), so
that the next significant stepwas 4, the establishment of lower education in
Hebrew. There were at this stage (under Turkish rule in Palestine) no
compulsory education laws, so that again the GIDS model needs to be
modified to fit the case. What happened was a process lasting about 20
years, as more and more Jewish schools started to teach in Hebrew. (Some
orthodoxschools, especiallyHassidic ones, still continue toresist this step.)
Fishman does not analyse the remaining stages in GIDS terms, except to
remark that Stage 3 was not a necessary stepfor Hebrew. This stepof use in
the lower work domain has however played its role, as Hebrew has
spreadamongArabic speakers forcedtolearnit inthe workplace. Nor does
he discuss the critical importance of the political work in England that
assured Hebrew of its official recognition by the British government from
the verybeginningof their rule over Palestine, movingit effectivelyintothe
lower governmental services and mass media (Stage 2) and into the
higher level educational, governmental and media efforts even before
independence.
We findit difficult, therefore, to fit the Hebrewcase comfortably into the
GIDS model, but clearly have benefited fromit in identifying key factors to
consider. The fact that the model did not prevent Fishman (1991) from
giving such an accurate picture of Hebrewrevival also suggests that he too
354 Africa and Asia
sees the needfor muchgreater flexibility. But if this unprecedented case of
success is not accounted for by the model, might one not start to wonder
about whether it is too tight a straitjacket? Does the term stage perhaps
unduly constrain the model, by its implication of an ordered progression
up (towards autonomy) as well as down?
The Costs of Hebrew RLS Success
To explore our concerns with the model further, we turn to the second
part of our task, that of considering developments in the case of Hebrew
since Fishman (1991) was written. When we try to do this, we are brought
up by the realisation, adumbrated in the opening of this paper, that, in RLS
studies, point of viewis all important. For the last fewyears, we have been
studying what has happened to the other languages in Israel, and how
they have fared under the impact of the strong Hebrew-focused ideology
that appears to have been so important in establishing Hebrewvitality and
vernacular use (Shohamy, 1995; Spolsky & Shohamy, 1999; Spolsky &
Shohomy, in preparation). In this enterprise, we have become vividly
aware of the many other languages that have succeeded or failed or not
even tried to resist Hebrew. The fate of these languages is in fact the most
obvious cost of the success of Hebrew RLS activities.
Here, again, it is not too clear that the GIDS model is going to be the most
useful approach. Presumably, it has nothing much to say about the
majority of languages brought to Israel by immigrants over the past
century, who seem to have accepted without a murmur the established
Hebrew-only (or, more recently, Hebrew-only-plus-English) ideology.
Most have gone quietly through the normal language shift, arriving peace-
fully at a resigned Stage 8, with vestigial individual use. Even a major
world language like German, major competitor for functional control of
higher education 85 years ago, accepted its defeat and was not generally
passedonfor more thanhalf a generation; andanother, French, depends on
efforts fromthe metropolitan authorities and its ministry of francophonie to
keep a grip of its impuissant status in the Israeli educational system. The
vast majority of the Jewish languages, with or without a history of literacy,
have generally failed to maintain status or use. But before we set out to
analyse these cases andthenreconsider the progress of Hebrewrevival, we
might ask whether there might not be a better framework for analysis than
GIDS.
As we do this, it will become clear that we are not so much disagreeing
with Fishman (1991) (we could probably write the rest of this paper simply
using sentences from his book) as trying to work through our discomfort
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 355
with some of his formulations. We use our own model (Spolsky &
Shohamy, 1999), to suggest that RLS is a kind of language policy, which we
define as an effort by someone claiming authority to alter the language
practice or ideology of someone else.
We find the distinction between practice, ideology and policy to be
useful. The language practice of a community what Hymes (1974) called
its ethnography of communication comprises its language uses and
implied rules for language use. Colouring but not necessarily changing
language practice, a speech community also has a language ideology, or a
set of beliefs about the reasons and values for those uses and rules for use.
(This distinction comes fromFishmans reanalysis of Stewarts typology of
languages, where Fishman showedthe greater value of considering auton-
omy, historicity, etc. as beliefs or attitudes.) Alanguage policy, onthe other
hand, is a formal attempt by someone who has or who claims authority to
change the language use (or ideology) of members of a speech community.
The critical element in the definition is that the sentence is active, with a
definable agent. It excludes therefore hegemonies or unidentified conspir-
ators, and requires that one says specifically who is doing the policy
making. It also excludes (andthis is more awkward), cases where a speaker
makes a policy for himself only. Thus we distinguish between a family that
has a practice of speaking Xish when grandfather is present from a family
where the father has proclaimed a policy of speaking Xish at meals.
When applied to RLS, the definition we propose gives us a chance of
defining the agent as small or weak or powerless or (at the very least) unof-
ficial members of a small or weakor powerless or (at the veryleast) margin-
alised speech community. Putting things this way helps us understand the
uneasiness we feel when the unofficial becomes official, when the power-
less languages supporters become powerful, and when members of less
powerful speech communities are forced to defend themselves against the
newly empowered David, king and no longer shepherd boy. It helps us
understand the reason that, while we sympathise with the start of the RLS
activities inQubec onbehalf of French, we donot sympathise withthe RLS
activities of France itself against its regional languages, Breton and Basque
and Occitan (Ager, 1999), or even the Qubec policy when it works against
Hebrew signs in a kosher food store. Or why we do not consider Unzs
activities in California to preserve English as deserving of the same
regardas Estonianefforts to re-establishtheir language. It helps us under-
stand too why we see Hebrew as a success story, up to the point that we
realise its victory was not just over competing world languages like
German and French, but also over small powerless ethnic languages like
Yiddish andJudezmo andHulaul (Judeo-Aramaic) andJudeo-Yemenite
356 Africa and Asia
and Juhuri (Judeo-Tat), to mention just a few, all with their own histories
and cultures.
The problem is that RLS is not an isolated event affecting a single
language but rather is interwoven with all the other languages in the
speechcommunity. Successful RLSfor one language maybe deathor exter-
mination for other languages. In the case of Hebrew, its success came at the
cost of the indigenous language (the Arabic introduced into the region a
thousand years earlier, which itself had displaced the Aramaic which had
replaced Hebrew), and of the 40 or more languages brought in by Jewish
immigrants. Its price was also the nurturing of a monolingual linguistic
ideology that replaced the earlier general acceptance of the value of
plurilingualism. Thus, if we were to write a book on RLS activities in
Israel today (which is what we have done in our book called The
Languages of Israel), it would (and does) start of by recounting the RLS
success of the revitalisationandrevernacularisationof Hebrew, andthen
turns to the fate and RLS activities of Arabic and Yiddish and Ladino and
Russian andAmharic andCircassian speakers. Keeping of course one eye
open to the RLS efforts of Hebrew supporters who feel threatened by
English.
Asecondpoint needs tobe stressed(againit is alreadymade inFishman,
1991), and that is that the goal of RLS activities is seldom if ever purely a
linguistic change. Most language policy is directed to non-linguistic goals,
whether political unification or domination or independence, or cultural
maintenance or reshaping, or economic efficiency or success, or educa-
tional enrichment or achievement. Even such a driven language policy
maker as Eliezer Ben Yehuda started off, as Mandel (1993) has shown, as a
nationalist before he sawthe critical value of language as an instrument of
achieving nationalism. The concentration on the language aspect in RLS is
understandable from the point of view of the sociolinguist, but from the
point of view of the language user, language is just one aspect of complex
social and cultural and economic choices. As a Navajo student once
remarked, she could make sure that her child grew up speaking Navajo,
but only if she was prepared to live in a hogan and carry water a mile or
more every day.
The Revitalisation of Hebrew in a Model of Language
Practice, Ideology and Policy
Looking first at Hebrew RLS activities from the point of view of our
model of practice, ideology and policy, then, the picture that emerges is
somethinglike this. Inthe periodbetween1890 and1913, a number of Jews,
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 357
mainly in eastern Europe and Turkish administered Palestine, but also in
the United States, began to use Hebrewas a spoken language for daily use
as well as for various written functions. These were in general not the same
Jews who continued to use it for sacred and traditional Jewish educational
functions. Starting with use in a few schools in agricultural settlements in
Palestine and spreading to the newly established town of Tel Aviv and to
some schools elsewhere, the practice of using Hebrew as a vernacular
became common among children and a growing norm among committed
adults in public. The practice was supported and spread by being part of
the ideology of the newZionist nationalist movement. It was promulgated
as policy by some of the leaders of that movement, and won acceptance in
some critical groups such as the Zionist Labour Party in Palestine. The
supporters of the new policy were successful in 191113 in Palestine in a
public struggle for a language policy for a planned tertiary technological
institution.
The struggle inthe early years was directedagainst two other languages
in particular. The first enemy was Yiddish, proclaimed in Tshernovits as
a Jewish national language by supporters of a non-territorial cultural
nationalist movement (Fishman, 1980), and seen by supporters of Hebrew
as a threat for ideological reasons as well as because of the fact that it was
the normal vernacular of east European Jews, and of course of the very
people who were nowmoving to Hebrew. The secondenemy in this early
period was German, a generally accepted world language of culture and
education, and the language naturally favoured by the major educational
system, the Hilfsverein that was the founder of a number of schools at
various levels inpre-war Palestine (Wahl, 1996). The battle against German
was fought first in Palestine during the so-called Language War, and then
again, in England, in the latter part of the First World War, where the
German plan to replace Hebrew was decried in an active campaign
conducted by supporters of Zionism and Hebrew (Cohen, 1918) in 1918.
Bolstered by the results of a language census conducted by the Zionist
organisation that appeared to show that most Jews in Palestine spoke
Hebrew (Bachi, 1956), the language was accepted by the British for inclu-
sion in the official languages for the Military government and the subse-
quent Mandate.
Hebrew thus started in the 1920s ideologically supported within the
Jewish community and officially recognised by the Mandatory govern-
ment as the language for that community and its schools. The enemies
now changed. The active campaign for the diffusion of the language
remained a central feature of Zionist and Israeli ideology. AYouth Legion
formed in 1923 for the Protection of the Language continued its activities
358 Africa and Asia
until 1936. During this critical period, strong campaigns were conducted
against the two principal enemies, as they were identified by Ussishkin in
his important speech at the 1923 third national conference of the Legion,
English and Yiddish. He attacked those who used English in order to
assert their elite status and closeness to the British rulers. His bitterest
complaints however were against Yiddish, and one of the major victories
of the Legion was to block the establishment of a chair of Yiddish at the
Hebrew University.
Bythe late 1930s, however, Hebrewappears tohave beensowell enough
established and institutionalised that there was no longer danger of loss.
That is not to say that there have been no continuing pro-Hebrew or anti-
Yiddish policies or arguments just as French andEnglish continue to take
steps to protect themselves against suspected foes, so the supporters of
Hebrew occasionally make efforts to strengthen the prospects for their
language. The establishment of the State in 1948 provided an opportunity
to reaffirm the place of Hebrew, but this was handled quite modestly, by
dropping the Mandatory status of English as official and leaving Hebrew
and Arabic. There is no constitutional requirement for Hebrew, and
perhaps also no formal legal affirmation of its centrality. There were
however numbers of localised decisions maintenance of Arabic as
language of instructioninArabschools, establishment of the ulpanto teach
Hebrew to professional immigrants, acceptance of use of other languages
in limited areas that worked together with the established ideology and
that ledinpractice tothe maintenance of Hebrewandits replacement of the
multilingualism of earlier generations and of immigrants.
Resistance to Hebrew RLS by Other Languages
Changing sides, as it were, if we look at the 40 or so other languages that
were themselves threatened by the shift to Hebrew, we find minimal
evidence of RLS activities. There were some people, but very few, who
followed the example of one head of family from Morocco and required
Hebrew to be used and spoken before the family came to Israel, while he
encouragedthe use of Jewish Moroccan Arabic in the home after they were
settledhere. More general was the normal process of immigrant shift to the
new language, with the second generation being passively bilingual and
the third unable to speak the immigrant language. The ideological effects
were to breeda locally nationalistic disparagement (Dorian, 1998) of all but
the approvedstandardlanguage, anda rejectionof the seriousness or value
of home or ethnic or community or traditional Jewish languages. Even
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 359
without formal policy, the monolingual Hebrew ideology came to domi-
nate and have its inevitable effects on practice.
The main exceptions occurred in marginal or marginalised groups. As
already mentioned, the 1948 decisions maintained Arabic as language of
instruction in schools in the Arab sector. At the same time, Arab pupils
were expected to learn Hebrew. The result over half a century has been a
serious incursionof Hebrewinto the Israeli PalestinianArabic community:
Arabic remains the language of the home and village, but Hebrew is the
language of tertiary education and of activities outside the community.
Only lately have there been modest RLS efforts complaints about the
loss of Arabic by children attending mixed kindergartens and schools in
towns like Jaffa and Haifa, legal cases seeking to have Arabic used on
public signs, moderate pressure to have it used in the Knesset, and even,
recently objection to use of Hebrew in a meeting of English teachers from
Arabic schools.
In another marginalised group, among the haredi (ultra-orthodox or
fundamentalist) opponents of political Zionism and Hebraisation, RLS
efforts have continuedinfavour of Yiddish, not of course for its secular and
cultural values but for its maintenance of an enclave within which
endorsed traditions can be continued. Here too, the evidence is of serious
erosion most haredi children, including Hassidim, appear now to be
Hebrew-speaking or at best bilingual when their heder (elementary school)
teachers start a long process of bilingual education for boys in Hebrewand
Yiddish (Isaacs, 1998, 1999).
Of the immigrant groups, only the English-speakers, the recent Russian-
speakers and the Amharic-speakers among the Ethiopians, have been
successful in having the schools give a serious place to the teaching of their
language. In spite of the decision dropping English from the list of official
languages, it has continued to grow in status and use, bolstered by
economic prestige and by the status of English-speaking immigrants
(Spolsky, 1996b).
The success of Hebrew revitalisation (a success achieved half a century
ago) has left plentyof roomfor RLSactivities for the other 40 languages. For
those who transfer their view of Hebrew as a language struggling for
revival to Hebrewas a language dominating others, there have been some
causes for moderate optimism in recent developments. There is for
instance the continued pressure for teaching of Arabic in Jewish schools,
recognisedin the adding of one more compulsory year to the school curric-
ulumand in renewed interest in teaching spoken Arabic in the elementary
schools mainlyinthe Tel Avivregion. There is the adoptionbythe Ministry
of Educationin199596 of a policyfor language educationinIsraeli schools
360 Africa and Asia
that recognises multilingualism as a goal, and that accepts the value of
maintaining immigrant languages (Ministry of Education, 1995, 1996).
There is the subsequent approval of new curricula for Russian and
Amharic.
There are also signs of affection for the now virtually extinct traditional
Jewish languages, illustrated by the passing of a law establishing national
authorities for Yiddish (not yet set up) and for Ladino (already in place).
We do not have space or time to trace here these activities or to assess their
prospects (see Spolsky & Shohamy, 1999). We mention them to stress the
point that a study of current RLS activities inIsrael wouldneedto focus not
just on efforts to defend Hebrew against real or imagined threats from
English and other languages, but on attempts to defend other languages
from now dominant Hebrew.
Nor do we have space to do more than allude to the continuing RLS
activities for Hebrew outside of Israel. The Hebrew revival movement
startedineasternEurope inthe 19thcentury, andsome of its energymoved
toother Jewishcommunities, especiallyNorthAmerica inthe earlyyears of
the 20th century and in parts of Latin America and elsewhere later in the
century. There were bursts of activity in the US in the 1920s and again
withthe growthof the Jewishday school movement inthe 1970s and1980s.
Bolstered by a number of Hebrew-speaking children of Israelis working or
living in the Diaspora, there were hopes of serious RLS, but more recently
the tone has become pessimistic, andmost people involvedbelieve that the
chances for Hebrew revival in the Diaspora are now slight.
Colouring our arguments and analysis has been a reluctant (and hope-
fully erroneous) presentation of RLS as conflict, confrontation, forced
binary choice between incompatible monolingualisms. One looks in vain
for the kind of pluralism expressed in the Tshernovits resolution in
favour of Yiddish as a (and not the) Jewish national language (Fishman,
1980). When we move outside the policy and ideological spheres into
language practice, we do find in the perseverance of complex socio-
linguistic repertoires in Israel some signs of a more pluralistic solution.
This shows up in the complex code-switching, within as well as between
languages, that marks actual language practice. Ideological acceptance of
pluralismwould permit a policy that treats RLS not as a struggle for exis-
tence but as the fine-tuning of complementary functional andsocial use of
languages.
References
Ager, D. (1999) Identity, Insecurity and Image: France and Language. Clevedon: Multi-
lingual Matters.
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 361
Bachi, R. (1956) A statistical analysis of the revival of Hebrew in Israel. Scripta
Hierosolymitana 3, 179247.
Chomsky, W. (1957) Hebrew: The Eternal Language. Philadelphia: The Jewish Publi-
cation Society of America.
Cohen, I. (1918) The German Attack on the HebrewSchools in Palestine. London: Jewish
Chronicle and Jewish World.
Dorian, N. (1998) Western language ideologies and small-language prospects. In
L.A. Grenoble andL.J. Whaley(eds) Endangered Languages: Current Issues and Fu-
ture Prospects (pp. 321). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fishman, J.A (1951) Zsveyshprakhikeyt in a yidisher shul. Bleter far yidisher
dertsiung 4, 324.
Fishman, J.A. (1970) Sociolinguistics: ABrief Introduction. Rowley: NewburyHouse.
Fishman, J.A. (1980) Attracting a following to high-culture functions for a language
of everyday life: The role of the Tshernovits Conference in the Rise of Yiddish.
International Journal of the Sociology of Language 24, 4373.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift: Theoretical and Empirical Foundations of
Assistance to Threatened Languages. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Harshav, B. (1993) Language in Time of Revolution. Berkeley: University of California
Press.
Hymes, D. (1974) Foundations inSociolinguistics: AnEthnographic Approach. Philadel-
phia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Isaacs, M. (1998) Yiddish in the orthodox communities of Jerusalem. In Dov-Ber
Kerler (ed.) Politics of Yiddish: Studies in Language, Literature and Society (pp.
8596). Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira Press.
Isaacs, M. (1999) Contentious partners: Yiddish and Hebrewin Haredi Israel. Inter-
national Journal of the Sociology of Language 138, 101121.
Krauss, M. (1991a) Endangered languages. Paper read at Linguistic Society of
America Annual meeting, January 1991.
Krauss, M. (1991b) The worlds languages in crisis. Language 68 (1), 410.
Mandel, G. (1993) Why did Ben-Yehuda suggest the revival of spoken Hebrew? In
L. Glinert (ed.) Hebrewin Ashkenaz (pp. 193207). NewYork and Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Ministryof Education, Culture andSport (1995) Policyfor language educationinIs-
rael (in Hebrew). Jerusalem: Office of the Director-General.
Ministryof Education, Culture andSport (1996) Policyfor language educationinIs-
rael (in Hebrew). Jerusalem: Office of the Director-General.
Rabin, H. (1973) AShort Historyof the HebrewLanguage. Jerusalem: JewishAgency.
Shohamy, E. (1995) Problems in Israeli language policy: Language and ideology. In
DavidChen (ed.) Education in the Twenty-first Century (pp. 24956). Ramat-Aviv:
Ramot Publishing, Tel Aviv University.
Spolsky, B. (1991) Hebrew language revitalization within a general theory of sec-
ond language learning. In R.L. Cooper and B. Spolsky (eds) The Influence of
Language on Culture and Thought: Essays in Honor of Joshua A. Fishmans Sixty-fifth
birthday (pp. 13755). Berlin: Mouton.
Spolsky, B. (1996a) Conditions for language revitalization: A comparison of the
cases of Hebrew and Maori. In S. Wright (ed.) Language and the State: Revital-
ization and Revival in Israel and Eire (pp. 550). Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters.
362 Africa and Asia
Spolsky, B. (1996b) English in Israel after Independence. In J.A. Fishman, A. Rubal-
Lopez and A.W. Conrad (eds) Post-Imperial English. Berlin: Mouton.
Spolsky, B. and Shohamy E. (1999a) Language in Israeli society and education. In-
ternational Journal of the Sociology of Language 137, 93114.
Spolsky, B. and E. Shohamy (1999b) The Languages of Israel: Policy, Ideology and Prac-
tice. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Stewart, W. (1968) A sociolinguistic typology for describing national
multilingualism. In J.A. Fishman (ed.) Readings in the Sociology of Language (pp.
53145). The Hague: Mouton.
Wahl, R. (1996) German language policy in 19th century Palestine. MAthesis, Eng-
lish, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat-Gan.
Hebrew After a Century of RLS Efforts 363
Chapter 16
Can the Shift from Immigrant
Languages be Reversed in
Australia?
1
M. CLYNE
In this chapter, I will use Joshua Fishmans model to consider how major
changes in Australian society, government policies across the board, and
the Australian and global economic situation are affecting language use
and the prospects of the reversal of language shift of shift. I will first
compare statistics fromthe 1986 and1996 Australian censuses on language
use and shift and other data on resources for the use of community (i.e.
immigrant) languages. This will enable us to assess the extent to which the
past decade has confirmed Fishmans (1991: 277) overall comment: Aus-
tralianpolicies andprocesses constitute a positive but ineffective approach
to RLS on behalf of recent immigrant languages . . .
Although the general data will cover all community languages, specific
points will be illustrated with information on particular languages. It
should be stated that Australian census statistics on language use are
limited to the home. In fact, many people speak a language other than
English not in their own home but in those of their parents and other
elderly relatives or in community groups. In general, the census statistics
do not indicate evidence of large-scale reversal of language shift.
It is evident fromTable 16.1 that all the groups for whomlanguage shift
data is available for both1986 and1996 have increasedtheir shift to English
in the first generation over the decade.
The number of groups for which comparable data at both the beginning
andendof the decade are available, is smaller inthe secondgenerationthan
in the first. All groups on whomdata was available in 1986 have increased
their language shift, with the exception of Dutch-Australians whose shift
was already extremely high in 1986.
364
The Pacific
Immigrant Languages in Australia
Immigrant Languages in Australia 365
Table 16.1 Language shift in the first generation: 1986 and 1996
Birthplace Percent shift
1986
Percent shift
1996
Austria 39.5 48.3
Chile NA
a
9.8
France 27.5 37.2
Germany 40.8 48.2
Greece 4.4 6.4
Hong Kong NA 9.0
Hungary 24.4 31.8
Italy 10.5 14.7
Japan NA 15.4
Korea, Republic of NA 11.6
Lebanon 5.2 5.5
Macedonia, Former Yugoslav Republic of NA 3.0
Malta 26.0 36.5
Netherlands 48.4 61.9
Other Sth America NA 17.2
Poland 16.0 19.6
PRC NA 4.6
Spain 13.1 22.4
Taiwan NA 3.4
Turkey 4.2 5.8
a
Not available, but Spanish-speaking South America: 10.1%
Source: Clyne and Kipp (1997:459)
Table 16.2 Language shift in the second generation (aggregated), 1986 and
1996
Parents birthplace 1986 1996
Germany 73.1 89.7
Greece 8.7 28
Hong Kong NA 35.7
Italy 29.3 57.9
Malta 58.8 82.1
Netherlands 85.4 95
Poland NA 75.7
PRC NA 37.4
Source: Clyne and Kipp (1997: 464)
Stages 7 and 8
In terms of the stages on Fishmans Gradual Disruption Scale, Stage 8 is
not relevant to community languages as Fishman himself indicates (1991:
255) because they are not in a complete state of disrepair and, in any case,
their heartlandlies outside Australia. Stage 7, where the use of the commu-
nity language is confined to the elderly and not transmitted to younger
people, already applied to some language communities at the time of
Fishmans original study and has progressively reached other languages.
For instance, the proportion of the 55 plus age group among Greek home
users has almost doubled in the decade from13.86%to 26%; among Dutch
speakers it has risen from 38.66% to 53.8%; among German speakers from
33.6% to 47.8%, and among Latvian speakers from 63% to 66.7%. While
there have been proportionate increases among Vietnamese (3.95% to 8%)
and Arabic speakers (7.89%to 12.7%), the older language users still do not
constitute a significant part of the population of these communities. A
comparison between Tables 16.1 and 16.2 will indicate a high inter-genera-
tional language shift (e.g. German 48.2% to 89.7%; Greek 6.4% to 28%;
Italian 14.7% to 57.9%). One of the reasons for the high inter-generational
language shift is exogamy, which, while high for groups that are culturally
similar to the dominant group or who share a religious denomination with
them, increases for all groups with length of time in Australia. However,
marriage between second and first generation Australians of similar
ethnolinguistic background has revitalised community language use.
Unfortunately, Australian census data does not record information
which might enable us to identify the third generation, thus we cannot
ascertain third generation language maintenance or shift.
Stage6: Intergenerational CommunityNeighbourhoodCommunity
Links onaDailyBasis
This is projected by Fishman (1991: 92) to be crucial to reversing
language shift. It involves leav(ing) behind an already ongoing
sociolinguistic modus vivendi and creat(ing) another that is demographi-
cally concentrated and intergenerationally continuous. Fishman indicates
that this has not occurred for immigrant languages in Australia, and we do
not have much evidence that the situation has changed drastically since.
On the contrary, many of the language communities have dispersed from
their concentration areas in the past 10 years. This applies, for instance, to
the Greek community which, in their stronghold, Melbourne, has moved
from being one of the most concentrated communities to the second most
dispersed one.
366 The Pacific
Arecent study of Arabic, Chinese andSpanishlanguage maintenance in
Melbourne (Clyne & Kipp, 1999) indicates a sharp dividing line between
over and under 35-year-olds and between the first and second generations
in all the communities in language use and maintenance patterns. The
lowest intergenerational shift is recorded in the community where the
community-family-neighbourhood links are the strongest, the Arabic
speakers.
In some of the older language communities, e.g. German (see Kipp et al.,
1995: 13840), grandparents rather than parents are playing an increasing
role inintergenerational language maintenance. There are three reasons for
this: (1) a desire on the part of the parents to make their children bilingual,
even if they themselves speak English at home; (2) the high degree of
exogamy; and (3) upward mobility and socioeconomic conditions making
it likely that both parents are in paid employment which often means that
the grandparents or grandmother is/are the main caregiver(s) during the
week, at last up to the time the children enter primary school. Many grand-
parents cherish this role, not only because of the grandchildren but also
because it gives them the chance to pass on their language under societal
conditions attitudinally far more favourable than when their own children
were growing up. Even where the grandparents do not play this role, there
is an increasing number of young people, notably middle class second
generation Australians, who are using the one parent, one language prin-
ciple (e.g. Saunders, 1982; Dpke 1993) to raise their children bilingually.
However, I can only provide small-scale information in support of this
statement. Of several hundred such parents participating in seminars
conducted by the Language and Society Centre at Monash University over
the past eight years, at least half are raising their children in German/
English, and the other community languages represented have varied but
include French, Italian, and to a lesser extent Greek, Polish, Spanish, Thai,
Vietnamese and others. The motives for raising children bilingually are
diverse. They include instrumental reasons such as the need to communi-
cate with relatives in the familys country of origin, questions of identity
(heritage), and particularly the perceived economic, cultural and cognitive
advantages of bilingualism. Languages other than English are nowconsid-
ered a useful commodity in educational and business circles in Australia.
In many cases, simply the intrinsic value and enjoyment of being bilingual
is considered something to pass on to the next generation and this is often
done without community or extended family. This is an exception to the
situation that Fishman (1991: 261) correctly detected, namely that his
prerequisite of ethnoculturally intact communities that are self-regulating
in cultural respects is not being fulfilled in immigrant communities in
Immigrant Languages in Australia 367
Australia because conceptual, residential and occupational modernity
submerges thementirelyinprocesses that theycanneither control nor miti-
gate. The situation unfavourable to RLS can be attributed to use ethnic
separatismconflicting with the interactionist ideology of multiculturalism
in Australia to which both the dominant and most immigrant groups
subscribe (Clyne & Kipp, in press: 331).
Demographically, there are some communities that are stronglyconcen-
trated in particular districts of a large city, such as Macedonian, Maltese,
and Vietnamese in Melbourne and Macedonian, Vietnamese and Indone-
sian in Sydney. The areas of strong concentration are also those in which
religious and secular community centres and ethnic shops are clustered. In
the case of Macedonian and Vietnamese,. concentration has led to high
community language use, but this cannot be said for Maltese, which has
consistently registered a high language shift. This needs to be seen in the
light of MalteseEnglishdiglossia inpre-migrationMalta andthe influence
of other languages and cultures over time in Malta. Maltese is a Semitic
language, closely akin to Arabic. However, its vocabulary has been
strongly influenced by English and Italian, and the language is written in
Latin script. The Roman Catholic Church, to which almost all the popula-
tion belongs, proximity to Italy, and a recent history of British colonialism,
have greatly affected Maltese culture. In the late 1940s and early 1950s,
when most Maltese immigrants to Australia left their homeland, English
was the H language in Malta in a diglossic relationship with Maltese (in
Fishmans (1967) sense of diglossia).
Another language for which community-language-neighbourhood
links are available is Yiddish. Some 81%of the 2842 home users of Yiddish
live in Melbourne, especially in two adjacent suburbs in which are located
numerous synagogues and Jewish community centres, the majority of
Melbournes seven Jewish all-day schools, shops, and a Yiddish cultural
centre and library. Generally, children and grandchildren live in close
proximity to the (grand)parents. However, the vast majority of the Yiddish
speakers are over 65. It is only in the most ultra-Orthodox of the Jewish
communities, a Chassidic one, that Yiddish is surviving in the younger
generation. This is probably related to the sharp boundary which the
community has drawn around itself and the emphasis on Yiddish in the
communitys school. This is in turn necessitated by the reservation of
Hebrew for the religious domain and the use of Yiddish as a medium of
instruction for some secular subjects. In the less Orthodox and non-
Orthodox communities/families of Yiddish background in the area, the
strong concentration has little or no effect on the language maintenance. If
anything, it has generated a Yiddish or Jewish ethnolect of Australian
368 The Pacific
English, which has taken over the symbolic function from Yiddish in in-
group interaction. The ethnolect has lexical items and grammatical struc-
tures transferred from Yiddish (cf. Clyne et al., in press). There is a
primary day school run by a secular Yiddishist groupoperating frompre-
school to Grade 6. This school has instruction in culture subjects (e.g.
History, Literature, Music) through the mediumof Yiddish. The children
are generally fromfamilies in which either English or Russian is the main
home language but many of the pupils have some access to the language
through grandparents. Nine of 18 in Grade 6 in 1998 and 4 out of 20 in
Grade 5 indicated to the school that they were speaking Yiddish to family
members as a result of being in the Yiddish programme while 6 out of 18
and 4 out of 20 in Grade 5 have introduced some Yiddish (through words
and songs) into non-Yiddish-speaking families (pers. comm., Renee
Zufi). Due to the small numbers involved, the evidence for RLS of Yiddish
is somewhat tentative.
Stage 5: Formal Linguistic Socialisation Through Agencies or
Institutions under Xish Control that do not Need to Satisfy Yish
Standards re Compulsory Education
As Fishman (1991: 262) points out, immigrant languages are served by
impressively large numbers of part-time community-supported ethnic
group schools. There were, in 1997, nearly 90,000 pupils attending ethnic
supplementary schools in a total of 73 languages, including 40,000 in New
South Wales and 30,000 in Victoria. Chinese (22,026), Arabic (12,219), and
Greek (12,139) are the languages withthe largest numbers of students. This
constitutes a substantial change from1986, when Greek schools taught the
majority of ethnic school students. Greek schools are usually conducted
several afternoons a week after day school. Most of the other programmes
are onSaturday or Sunday mornings andmost of the part-time community
language schools teach language and culture. Many, particularly Greek
ones, also give religious instruction, and some impart history of the
country of origin.
The schools are utilised in many communities, especially the longer
established ones with high language shift rates, e.g. German, for RLS, e.g.
German, often by parents who are themselves second generation Austra-
lians with rather limited German (Kipp et al., 1995: 13940). However, the
expectations of parents froma programme of two or three hours per week
are enormous considering that there is often virtually no backup at home.
This applies particularly to exogamous families. There is a little more
success in making oral bilinguals literate in the community language.
Immigrant Languages in Australia 369
Although part-time ethnic schools are the responsibility of the commu-
nity group running them, they are subsidised by the Australian govern-
ment. At the primary level, they are fully in control of their syllabus. Some
of these part-time schools, however, also prepare candidates for the exami-
nation at the end of secondary school which also serves as university
entrance. In Victoria, this is less frequent than in other states because the
Victorian School of Languages, a part of the Victorian Education Depart-
ment operating on Saturdays fromcentres in 10 state schools (see Stage 4b)
has taken over this task for most languages. (The exceptions are Latvian,
where the ethnic school forms part of the Victorian School of Languages,
and Chinese, where some ethnic schools prepare candidates for the Victo-
rianCertificate of Languages, the matriculationexamination, inChinese).
There is no provision, in the RLS model, for the supportive role of public
libraries with holdings (books, newspapers, magazines, videos) in the
communitylanguages of the district. Libraries couldnot be seeninterms of
increased power sharing. Nevertheless, they provide major resources for
literacy development in community languages as well as inout in formal
registers of the languages. Such holdings are utilised by community
groups, families, andindividuals engagedinRLSor inthe literacydevelop-
ment of children who have oral skills in the community language.
The comparison between 1986 and 1997 for Victoria shows only a
41.4% increase in the number of books in community languages with
substantial increases inChinese, Arabic, Greek, andRussian, holdings for
the first time inKurdish, Somali, andUkrainian, andthe small holdings in
languages such as Armenian, Latvian, and the Scandinavian languages
beingwrittenoff. The latter means that the language maintenance of some
low demand languages is no longer being supported by libraries. (See
Table 16.3)
Stage 4
Stages 4 to 1 are, according to Fishman (1991: 401) that RLS is on the
strong side, transcending diglossia insearchof increasedpower-sharing.
However, as we shall see under 1 and 2, it is unlikely that the media are
having an RLS effect in Australia.
Stage 4a: X-ish Sponsored and Controlled Schools that are
Attended in Lieu of Meeting Compulsory Education
Requirements
There is a tradition in Australia of non-government primary and
secondary schools, which are attended by about a third of Australian
370 The Pacific
Immigrant Languages in Australia 371
Table 16.3 Book holdings in community languages in Victorian public
libraries
Language 1986 Total 1997 Total
Albanian 438 109
Arabic 8,344 15,254
Armenian 78
Bulgarian 4
Chinese 5,131 47,063
Croatian
a
5,825
Czech 912 194
Danish 61
Dutch 6,381 3,139
Finnish 53
French 9,011 7,472
German 13,074 9,142
Greek 39,821 54,512
Hebrew 74 460
Hindi 325 1,255
Hungarian 4,134 979
Indonesian 115 705
Italian 57,152 54,752
Japanese 306 379
Khmer 106 529
Korean 91 1
Kurdish 133
Laotian 6
Latvian 260
Lithuanian 645 718
Macedonian 3,188 2,223
Maltese 3,369 2,174
Norwegian 4
Polish 6,748 9,573
Portuguese 1,800 95
Romanian 147 239
Russian 3,650 7,279
Serbian 1,885
Serbo-Croatian/Croatian/Serbian/Yugoslav
b
9,602
Singhalese 3
school pupils. Of these, about 80% are at Roman Catholic schools, either
parish primary schools or secondary regional schools or secondary schools
varying in their socioeconomic status, previously conducted by religious
orders. As Fishman (1991: 264) points out, Catholic schools are generally
multiethnic. The exceptions are three Maronite (Lebanese Eastern rite
Catholic) primary schools, two in Sydney and one in Melbourne which
teach for a number of hours of the day in Arabic. Not all the children speak
Arabic at home. Almost all the Roman Catholic schools teach Italian as a
secondlanguage, whichis no longer identifiedwitha local communityand
some of them also offer other languages, usually French or an Asian
language. A small number of Catholic schools have maintenance
programmes in languages of special importance in the local community,
such as Arabic, Chinese, French, Maltese, Tagalog or Vietnamese.
Most of the other non-government schools are elitist, being modelledon
the British public schools, charge high fees, and are affiliated with either
the Anglican or Uniting Church.
2
Lutheran schools teach German but as a
heritage rather than a community language. The 16 Jewish day schools,
seven of which are in Melbourne, all teach Hebrew, although this varies
from Classical Hebrew to Ivrit (Modern Israel Hebrew) depending on the
ideology of the school (Klarberg, 1983). Hebrewis sometimes the medium
of instruction in the Jewish Studies or Religious Studies programme. Two
372 The Pacific
Language 1986 Total 1997 Total
Slovak 110
Slovenian 1,493 248
Somali 8
Spanish 15,283 19,201
Swedish 14
Tagalog 10 467
Thai 242 13
Turkish 1,222 12,512
Tuvalli/Kiribati 13
Ukrainian 264
Vietnamese 3,601 41,286
Yiddish 370 85
Total 212,274 300,179
a
See 1986 Serbo-Croation/Croation, etc. figures
b
See Croatian and Serbian
Table 16.3 (cont.)
of the Jewish schools in Melbourne teach part of the secular curriculum in
Hebrew, one at primary, the other at secondary level. The latter excludes
from this elective programme the small minority of native speakers of
Israeli Hebrew. In addition there are the Chassidic and Yiddishist schools
using Yiddish as a medium of instruction (see Stage 6, above).
During the past decade, there has been an increase in the number of
Greek Orthodox and Greek Community, Coptic Orthodox, Islamic, and
other ethnic or non-mainstream religious day schools. Typically, the
Greek Orthodox and Greek Community private schools teach Modern
Greek for more hours per week than most state schools would but few of
them have any component of bilingual education. The children in most of
the schools are nearly all of Greek background but not all of those speak
Greekat home. Inone school inMelbourne, however, whichdescribes itself
as multicultural, non-denominational and with a lowfees structure, about
64% of the children are non-Greek speaking. There are separate Greek
classes for home background and non-background students. The school
has a sizeable Greek library, and the general environment gives plenty of
opportunity for the use of the Greek language (see Clyne et al., 1995). Eccle-
siastical Greek is introduced through the teaching of the Liturgy in Greek
Orthodox Religious Instruction in both ethnic day schools and state
schools. Passages fromthe Koran are committedto memory as part of Reli-
gious Instruction in Islamic day schools, in part-time ethnic schools (see
Stage 5), andinthose state schools where communities have providedReli-
gious Education teachers.
About 90% of the pupils in the two Coptic schools in Melbourne are of
Egyptian background and the others are fromother Arabic-speaking fami-
lies. Arabic is taught to all pupils but it is not a medium of instruction in
other subjects although some written work can be submitted in Arabic in
some other subjects. As parents were initially apprehensive about giving
Arabic too central a role in the school, most teachers of secular subjects
appointed are not Arabic speakers, and this has weakened the position of
Arabic. The 22 Islamic schools in Australia are multicultural insofar as the
children come froma range of ethnolinguistic backgrounds and for many,
such as those of Pakistani, Malay and Bosnian backgrounds, Arabic is for
them a sacred but not a spoken language. Arabic is taught as a second
language in Islamic schools but not used as a medium of instruction.
Insofar as the language taught is the familys (former) community
language, the above schools have been successful in some cases, in
reversinglanguage shift intwoways bothbygivingthe childrena compe-
tence in the language sufficient to use with elderly relatives where the
language is no longer spoken in their own homes, andby raising interest in
Immigrant Languages in Australia 373
the language, which has encouraged them to use it where possible and
utilise stays in the country of origin to improve their competence. As there
has been no detailed study of this, the above statements remain anecdotal.
Certainly the numbers of second generation speakers of languages such as
Greek marrying first generation speakers either by bringing them home
from study periods in the country of origin or after meeting them here
encouraged by their own competence in and enthusiasm for the language
has provided opportunities for home language transmission to the third
generation.
Stage 4b: Xish Programmes in Yish Schools
In the 1970s and 1980s, the number of languages taught in Australian
schools and assessed at the end-of-school examinations increased
dramatically as a result of multiculturalismpolicy. Also, languages other
than English (LOTEs) are now widely taught at primary level in many
states. Constitutionally, education is a responsibility of the states, so
prioritisation of languages and provision for their delivery varies vastly
between states. Also, the quality of programmes differs very much
between schools. At one end there are partial immersion-type
programmes, at the other end satellite television language programmes
supervised by teachers with a very limited knowledge of the language.
States have usually prioritised eight languages, which include some of
the major community languages, such as Italian, Greek, and sometimes
Vietnamese and/or Arabic, and several of the languages prioritised for
other reasons, such as Mandarin and German, are also among the more
widely used community languages. Some states also emphasise other
languages of community significance. Of the 43 languages assessed for
examination purposes, not all are taught in mainstreamday schools and of
course noone school offers more thana few. The EducationDepartments in
Victoria, NewSouth Wales and South Australia have a section which runs
language programmes inschools onSaturdays for those pupils whocannot
take themintheir regular school. Soat one centre, courses maybe offeredin
French, Japanese, Vietnamese, Spanish, Ukrainian, Hungarian, Macedo-
nian, and Arabic, while at another, say, German, Italian, Russian,
Mandarin, Farsi, Khmer, Croatian, Greek, Latvian, and Turkish. There are
20 such centres in Victoria.
It is difficult to generalise on whether Xish or Yish school institutions or
part-time ethnic schools are likely to stimulate RLS the most. However,
students from both mainstream day schools and the Education Depart-
ments Saturday schools have produced far better results at the 1997 Year
374 The Pacific
12 Arabic examination than their counterparts in Dish schools while 21.3%
of the top students in Chinese were prepared by part-time ethnic schools
and the rest by Yish schools (Clyne & Kipp, in press).
Some language subjects are taken mainly by people with a home back-
ground in the language, which in itself varies a lot but some (e.g. Spanish,
Mandarin, Indonesian) are taken by pupils with different levels of home
background as well as beginners. In some primary school immersion
programmes this stimulates language development in both groups and a
stronger motivation to speak the language among the home background
students (Imberger, 1986). However, now that LOTEs are perceived as a
commodity, they are accompanied by false assumptions about who the
programmes are intendedfor andabout students witha home background
taking something away fromthose without a background. Standardisation
procedures limit the number of very high scores and make them more
uniform across subjects. So for instance in Asian languages perceived to
be more difficult languages there is a fear that the standard has risen out of
reach of students from other ethnic backgrounds. Four state examination
boards have devised LOTE forms to interrogate students on their home
and other use of the language to classify them according to advantage in
order to moderate the grades. In some languages there are now first
language examinations with constraints on admission to second language
examinations. While there are good reasons for examining recent arrivals
with substantial experience of schooling in a country where the language is
spoken in a different way from other students, this does not apply to, say,
second or third generation Australians with a limited background in the
language. For them, what they have achieved in the language due to deter-
mination deserves credit, and special treatment and the suspicion of an
unfair advantage is a serious demotivation for maintenance of reversing
language shift. It seems as if they are not even permitted to have a share in
their own community language when it has a coveted market value. This is
particularly so because the special needs of later generation limited
bilinguals (and, for instance, of speakers of other fang yan (regional vari-
eties) suchas Cantonese or Hakka learningMandarin) are hardlyaddressed.
Fishmans scepticism that language programmes in Yish schools can
contribute to long-term language maintenance or to RLS is justified, espe-
cially in an atmosphere of greed and self-interest as we have at present.
Stage 3: Xish Work Spheres
I cannot add anything to Fishmans perceptive account, indicating that
any use of immigrant languages in the work sphere is unlikely to promote
Immigrant Languages in Australia 375
intergenerational transmission. The only thing that has happened since
1991 has been the large-scale immigration of East, South and South-East
Asians who oftenrunfamily businesses, andthe development of the policy
of Productive Diversity. The former has only had a temporary effect
because of the large proportion of children of Asian immigrants entering
university and the professions. However, there are certainly young people
who work in businesses in areas of, say, Vietnamese concentration. The
policy of Productive Diversity owed its origins to former Prime Minister
Paul Keating (1992) and was soft pedalled by the subsequent Conservative
federal government but further developed in Victoria under the conserva-
tive state government there under the title of The Multicultural Advantage.
The aim is to utilise the cultural and linguistic resources of the nation or
state to further the economic progress but at the same time celebrate
cultural diversity. This gives motivation and input and output opportuni-
ties to bilinguals, especially younger ones.
Stage 2: Media and Government Services
Fishman differentiates between local and national media. In Australia,
the community language press generally has a nationwide circulation
regardless of where it is published. There is a government-run network of
community language radio stations and a television station that transmits
to the whole of Australia community language programmes with English
subtitles, English programmes of a multicultural nature, and regular (in
some cases, daily) satellite news broadcasts in 15 languages. The network
has two stations in each of Sydney and Melbourne, one local and one
national, and one (taking national broadcasts) in each of the other capital
cities and regional areas. Other radio and TV stations are local. These
include three Arabic-language, two Greek-language, one Turkish-
language, one Italian-language and one ItalianEnglish bilingual commer-
cial stations in Melbourne, two Arabic, two Greek, two Spanish and one
Italian in Sydney, one Macedonian in nearby Wollongong and one each in
Greek and Italian in Adelaide.3 Whether a programme is local or national,
has very little significance interms of LMandRLS. What is more important
is whether the broadcasts are produced by professionals or by community
members. The government-run network and the commercial stations have
professionals. The remaining multilingual stations (including one televi-
sion station each in Melbourne, Sydney, Adelaide) are community stations
where the people make the programmes. This contributes to LMin that it
encourages people to prepare material of a high standard in the commu-
nity language. However, many listeners complain about non-standard
376 The Pacific
regional varieties, regional accents or mixed language, and the broadcasts
are not taken seriously as models. In Melbourne, in addition to the
language-specific stations, government community language radio broad-
casts outnumber those on community stations by about 2:1; in Adelaide
and Brisbane they are about equal; and in Perth, the government run
service offers about 25% more hours than the community station. The
community-run programmes are less numerous in other centres. Most
languages are represented on both types of stations but sometimes newer
languages (Oromo, Somali), those of smaller communities not desiring
government support (Romany) or languages or varieties equated with
those of other communities (Flemish, Malay) are transmitted only on
community stations. All inall, there are now75 radio stations broadcasting
in a total of 97 community languages for 1393 hours per week. The
following comparison between 1986 and 1998 covers the 11 stations with
the most community language broadcasting. (See Table 16.4)
It will be seen that the number of hours of community language broad-
casts on these stations has doubled since 1986, that the allocation for nearly
all languages has increased, and that many new language have been
added.
Young people continue to show little interest in the radio programmes
(see Clyne &Kipp, 1999, for the situation in Arabic, Chinese, and Spanish).
There are a small number of exceptions where secondgenerationbilinguals
run targeted segments on some of the language programmes.
Public access multilingual television programmes run for 8 hours per
week in 28 languages in three capital cities: The national television details
are given under Stage 1 below.
As Fishman (1991: 271), points out, the second generation disinterest in
the ethnic press and community language radio makes themineffective in
RLS. However, I wouldlike to stress the role of radio andespecially TVas a
resource for and an indirect contribution to RLS. The presence of commu-
nity languages in the public domain gives a legitimacy to inter-genera-
tional transmission and parentchild interaction in a community language
in the presence of non-speakers of the language. Such consistent commu-
nity language use without apology or criticism, representing one of the
major changes in Australia in the past two decades, is essential for the
success of RLS. It is one of the issues that many families raising children
bilingually are concerned about. Moreover, the electronic media do
provide a resource towards reactivating the community language andacti-
vating passive skills, though, of course, they cannot instigate or even
provide a context for such RLS.
Immigrant Languages in Australia 377
378 The Pacific
Table 16.4 The 11 ethnic language community radio stations with the most
broadcasts in community lanauges 1986 and 1998 [Transmission of LOTE
per week (hrs)] 1:10 = 1 hr. 10 mins
Languages Number of hours
for 1986
Number of hours
for 1998
Afghani 2:00
Albanian 1:00 6:45
Arabic 17:45 105:45
Armenian 5:00 10:00
Assyrian 2:30 7:00
Azerbaj 1:00
Baltic (Estonian/Latvian/Lithuanian) 11:30
Belarusan 1:30
Bengali 0:15 4:20
Bosnian
a
10:00
Bulgarian 2:45 3:00
Burmese 6:00
Cantonese
b
28:45
Catalan 0:45 1:00
Celtic
c
1:00
Chinese 8:30
Cook Islander Maori 3:45
Croatian 16:20 34:30
d
Czech 5:15 5:45
Cypriot 1:30
Danish
e
4:30
Dari 0:10 3:21
Dutch (incl. Flemish) 21:00 23:00
Esperanto 1:00
Estonian
f
4:00
Ethiopian 1:30
Fijian 8:00
Finnish
g
7:15
French 14:00 20:30
Gaelic (Irish) 9:30
Geetanjal
i
0:30
Immigrant Languages in Australia 379
Languages Number of hours
for 1986
Number of hours
for 1998
German (incl. Swiss and Austrian) 31:00 37:15
Greek 68:00 104:15
h
Gujarati 0:15
1:00
i
Hebrew
j
10:30
Hindi 0:30 19:20
k
Hindustani 1:30
Hmong 2:00
Hungarian 12:00 18:15
Indian 5:15
Indonesian 2:25 16:00
Islanders 1:00
Italian 51:30 76:45
Japanese 0:30 5:00
Jewish (Hebrew/Yiddish) 8:00
Kannada 0:15 1:00
Khmer 3:00 11:00
Korean 2:00 14:00
Kurdish 0:38 5:30
Laotian 2:00 6:00
Latvian
l
7:00
Lithuanian
m
5:30
Macedonian 13:40 48:30
Malay 3:30
Maltese 20:30 25:00
Mandarin
n
25:00
Maori 1:00 7:45
Marathi
o
2:00
Mauritian
p
2:00
Norwegian
q
2:30
Oromo 1:00
Pakistani 0:45
Persian-Farsi 6:45
Polish 20:45 39:30
Table 16.4 (cont.)
380 The Pacific
Languages Number of hours
for 1986
Number of hours
for 1998
Portuguese 9:15 22:00
Punjabi 1:00 8:20
Pushtu
r
0:09
Romanian 2:00 9:00
Romany 0:30
Russian 10:45 20:00
Samoan 0:45 11:15
Scandinavian (Danish/Swedish/Icelan-
dic/Finnish)
14:15
Scottish 2:45 3:00
Serbian 9:50 26:00
Serbo-Croatian 2:00 5:00
s
Sinhalese 1:15 12:15
t
Slovak 3:30 4:45
Slovenian 6:06 8:30
Somali 2:15
Spanish 33:00 50:30
Sri Lankan 2:00
Swedish 3:30
u
Tagalog 3:45 9:15
Tamil 2:45 8:45
v
Telegu
w
1:30
Thai 0:30 7:45
Tibetan 1:00
Timorese 1:00 2:00
Tongan 2:15 9:15
Turkish 18:30 30:15
x
Ukrainian 12:45 12:15
Urdu
y
1:00 8:45
z
Vietnamese 7:30 46:30
Vlach 1:00
Welsh 0:15 3:00
Yiddish
a1
7:00
Table 16.4 (cont.)
Stage 1: National Media and Central and Regional
Government Services
Public notices are nowissuedina range of community languages. These
include numerous announcements on elections and voting procedures,
health, safety and social security matters (including child care and old age
pensions), legal questions and rights, traffic code, the education system,
and library facilities. The choice of languages depends largely on the
specific needs of the language community, but most multilingual notices of
a general nature are now in 20 languages. In local announcements it tends
to depend on the ethnolinguistic composition of the population. The
Department of Social Security has targeted Arabic, Bosnian, Cambodian,
Chinese, Croatian, Farsi, Greek, Hungarian, Italian, Korean, Maltese,
Macedonian, Filipino/Tagalog, Polish, Portuguese, Russian, Serbian,
Spanish, Turkish, and Vietnamese. A telephone interpreter service oper-
ates in about 90 languages. However, this is to satisfy the needs of some
first generation groups and not an incentive for reversing language shift
although it probably does afford legitimacy to community languages.
Although Australia does not have a de jure official language, most of the
business of government and administration takes place in English.
Government departments do employ bilinguals to offer services to people
of non-English-speaking backgrounds. Such employees receive a special
language loading. This provides an incentive to maintain and develop
bilingual skills.
Immigrant Languages in Australia 381
Languages Number of hours
for 1986
Number of hours
for 1998
Yugoslav 7:51
Zaza 0:30
Totals 506:15 1154:15
a
See 1986 Serbo-Croation figures for comparison;
b
See 1986 Chinese figures for compari-
son;
c
See 1986 Scottish figures;
d
See also 1986 Yugoslav figures;
e
See 1986 Scandinavian
figures;
f
See 1986 Baltic figures for comparison;
g
See 1986 Scandinavian figures;
h
See also
1986 Cypriot figures;
i
See also1986 Indian figures;
j
See 1986 Jewish figures;
k
See also1986
Indian figures;
l
See 1986 Baltic figures for comparison;
m
See 1986 Baltic figures;
n
See 1986
Chinese figures;
o
See 1986 Indian figures;
p
See 1986 French figures;
q
See 1986 Scandina-
vian figures;
r
See also1986 Afghani figures;
s
See also1986 Yugoslav figures;
t
See also1986
Sri Lankan figures;
u
See 1986 Scandinavian figures for comparison;
v
See also1986 Sri Lan-
kan figures;
w
See Indian figures;
x
See also 1986 Cypriot figures;
y
See also 1986 Pakistani
figures;
z
See also 1986 Pakistani figures;
a1
See 1986 Jewish figures.
Table 16.4 (cont.)
382 The Pacific
Table 16.5 Community language newspapers/periodicals
Language 1986 1996
Arabic 7 8
Armenian 1
Bulgarian 1
Chinese 2 24
Czech 1 1
Dutch 3 2
Fijian-Indian 1
Estonian 1 1
Finnish 2 1
French 1 1
German 3 2
Greek 15 10
Hungarian 4 1
Indian 6
Indonesian 1
Iranian (Farsi) 2
Italian 10 2
Japanese 4
Korean 3 7
Latvian 1 1
Lao 1
Macedonian 2 4
Maltese 2 1
Polish 5 4
Portuguese 2 2
Russian 2 4
Scandinavian 1
Slovenian 1 1
Spanish 6 4
Thai 1
Turkish 14 7
Ukrainian 5 1
Urdu 1 (See Indian)
Vietnamese 9 13
Serbian 3 2
Croatian 5 3
Yugoslav 2
Total 102 117
Since 1986, the number of community language newspapers has
increased marginally from 102 to 117. There are now more newspapers in
new languages and some of the old ones have folded. Most of them are
weeklies; there are also monthly, fortnightly and twice-weekly newspa-
pers. For the first time there are now daily newspapers in an immigrant
language five in Chinese. (See Table 16.5)
Community language newspapers present material in varying propor-
tions, according to the period of settlement in Australia, on homeland and
international events and Australian news, especially from the multicul-
tural scene and the relevant ethnic communities. This helps maintain
vocabulary and structures, and sometimes introduces readers to neolo-
gisms fromthe heartlandof the language. Newspapers sometimes provide
readers with a model for expressing in the community language the
exigencies of life in Australia. Fishmans (1991) contention that such news-
papers have little appeal to the second generation because of their content.
is confirmed in a recent study of Arabic, Chinese and Spanish language use
and maintenance patterns, where not only the second generation but gener-
ally the under-35s rarely read ethnic newspapers (Clyne & Kipp, 1999).
The number of hours of programmes in community languages on the
government station, SBS, has more than doubled since 1986. The distribu-
tionof the languages has altereda little inthat periodas Table 16.5 will indi-
cate, withnewlanguages addedandsome allocatedmore time. Substantial
gains have beenrecordednot only for languages of newer migrants suchas
Cantonese, Mandarin, Russian, and Indonesian but also, for example,
French, Italian, Greek, Spanish and German, largely due to regular news
broadcasts inthose languages. It will be notedthat the annual transmission
time for a number of languages is minimal, probablyone film. Unlike radio,
television does not allocate a specific slot to a particular language
according to demographic statistics. Availability of suitable films is also a
criterion. Unfortunately, regular childrens TV series in community
languages are no longer shown, as language-specific telecasts other than
the news have to be sub-titled to ensure a wide group of viewers and it is
considered that children are unable to process sub-titles sufficiently well.
Young speakers of Arabic, Chinese (Cantonese or Mandarin) and Spanish
are more likely to watch television and videos in their community
languages (e.g. movies, game shows, series) than listen to radio in them
(Clyne &Kipp, 1999). However, there is also discontent with the content of
television programmes. (See Table 16.6)
The Internet could be a means of assisting RLS in that it could make age
and interest appropriate input available, and force theminto output in the
community language in interaction with people in touch with a peer group
Immigrant Languages in Australia 383
384 The Pacific
Table 16.6 Number of hours on national (SBS) television, 1986 and 1996
Language 1986 1996
Arabic 33.95 131.07
Armenian 3.94
Assamese 3.39
Bengali 1.47 7.52
Cantonese 19.48 133.26
Catalan 2.54
Czech 43.72 23.00
Danish 8.97 29.09
Estonian 2.37
Euskal 1.18
Farsi 10.56
French 136.81 561.28
Gaelic 1.07
German 197.09 325.49
Greek 123.53 264.15
Hebrew 6.07 17.59
Hindi 14.07 26.35
Hungarian 32.54 35.41
Icelandic 2.54 3.47
Italian 255.63 436.37
Japanese 77.16 155.12
Khmer 4.45
Korean 4.77 14.30
Kurdish 2.48
Letzebuergesch 1.36
Lithuanian 1.53
Macedonian 9.22
Malayalam 2.04
Maltese 1.59
Mandarin 15.29 202.28
Mongolian 1.20
More 1.25
Norwegian 2.83 14.30
Polish 42.43 90.02
Portuguese 36.08 44.10
Romanian 9.43
in the country of origin and in other immigrant communities, as well as
promoting literacy skills. However, most young people use the Internet
exclusively in English. (There are some exceptions in TaiwanMandarin
community, a very recent group, and among Spanish speakers.)
General Context and Outlook
During the decade or so under review, substantial changes have
occurred in the expectations of the role of government. The user principle
now presides in all areas. The partly social agenda of the 1970s and 1980s
was replaced in the subsequent years by an anti-social one determined by
short-term economic objectives. Governments have privatised many of
their assets, rangingfromhospitals to prisons, frompublic transport to gas,
water supply to electricity. They have shed many of the responsibilities
that go with these assets. One year after the first year of our statistical
decade, Australias National Policy on Languages (Lo Bianco, 1987) was
released. Its guiding principles included the maintenance and develop-
ment of community languages and the provision of services in community
languages. It provided rationale for language maintenance on the basis of
social equity, cultural enrichment, and economic strategies.
Immigrant Languages in Australia 385
Language 1986 1996
Romany 1.50
Russian 36.71 228.03
Serbian 10.97 5.05
Serbo-Croatian 33.54
Slovenian 10.16 3.36
Spanish 105.68 232.17
Swedish 25.25 26.38
Tagalog 6.42
Thai 3.51
Tok Pisin
1.51
Turkish 20.56 28.38
Ukrainian 1.50 20.08
Vietnamese 22.43
Welsh 4.32 6.23
Yiddish 2.05
Total 1415.10 3181.19
Table 16.6 (cont.)
Under community pressure, the Australian Language and Literacy Policy,
the brainchild of the then Minister for Employment, Education and
Training, John Dawkins, retainedthe guiding principles rhetorically. But it
marked the shift to an economic agenda, stressing English literacy and
competence in languages of economic importance to Australia (especially
those of the Asian region). Fourteen languages were declared priority
languages, including some that are predominantly justified on the basis of
being prominent community languages (e.g. Greek, Vietnamese) as well as
some that are, among things, important community languages (e.g.
Mandarin, Italian, Spanish, Arabic) This had the effect of relegating
languages-in-education policy to the states, which have the constitutional
responsibility for (primary and secondary) schools. It meant that vastly
different policies were developed and different priority languages were
chosen in the individual states.
A subsequent policy, Asian languages and Australias Economic Future
(Rudd, 1994) reinforced more explicitly the emphasis on Japanese, Indone-
sian, Mandarin, and Korean for economic motives. Nevertheless, the state
Education Departments Saturday schools continuedto teach a wide range
of languages, andthe number of languages examinedat the final secondary
school-cum-university entrance examinationwas increasedto 43, covering
a large proportion of the community languages used in Australia. This
includes a considerable number with very low enrolments. These have
remained on the list despite occasional threats due to the cost of adminis-
trating examinations for such a small clientele.
On the other hand, savage cuts to the operating grants of universities
and especially Arts faculties has led to the closure of many language
programmes and to rationalisation in the case of others (offering most
languages inonly one university ina cityandrequiring the students to take
themat another institution). This is frustratedby travel time andtime table
clashes. Sometimes arrangements are made by which staff from one
university offer a language course on another campus. However,
numerous language programmes that have been closed in recent years
were unique in that state (e.g. Hindi in Victoria) or nationwide (e.g. Dutch
and Khmer). This not only takes away the opportunity to study the
language for language maintenance purposes; it also wipes out the chances
of an ongoing supply of secondary and primary teachers of the language
(or of other professionals with a high competence and credentials in the
language trainedinAustralia). All this, together withthe situationoutlined
under Stage 4b, underlines the need for ongoing involvement of Xians in
the implementation of policies affecting Yian languages.
Financial cuts and the need to prioritise have reintroduced the distinc-
386 The Pacific
tion between maintaining an important and an unimportant language
which had been soft-pedalled with the advent of the policy of Multicultur-
alism in the 1970s. Such a differentiation is not (as yet) influencing
language maintenance/shift trends. With high shifts from German and
French and quite substantial ones from Italian and Spanish Spanish and
low shifts from Macedonian, Turkish, and probably Vietnamese, they
suggest the reverse trend (see Tables 16.1 and 16.2).
Of the categories of immigrants entering Australia, business migration
has gained most in significance over the past decade. Many of the families
coming under this scheme are from Hong Kong and Taiwan. On the one
hand, many of them (especially those from Hong Kong) already have a
high level of English proficiency, on the other hand they tend to maintain
close contacts with the countries of origin through business and families; it
is likely that they will not need to reverse shift in the future.
A success story in RLS motivated by factors other than the above but
utilising the family-community-neighbourhood links (Stage 6) is that of
Macedonian. In 1994, in deliberations on the recognition of the newly inde-
pendent Republic of Macedonia, the Australian government bowed to
pressure from Greek diplomatic representatives and sections of the Greek
community to refer to the new state as the Former Yugoslav Republic of
Macedonia. In addition, the Macedonian community should now be offi-
cially called the Macedonian-Slav community to distinguish it from
members of the Greek community from the Northern Greek region of
Macedonia. The state government of Victoria, which has the strongest
concentration of both Greek and Macedonian speakers, declared that the
Macedonian language should in future be known as Macedonian
(Slavonic). Both the official change of the name of a community language
by a government in Australia and the affixation of the name of a language
family to the name of a language are unprecedented. Froma practical point
of view, the same final school examination is termed Macedonian
(Slavonic) in Victoria and Macedonian elsewhere in Australia. From a
linguistic point of view, there is no other Macedonian language from
which this one needs to be distinguished. An appeal by the Macedonian
community to the Human Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission
(1997) was unsuccessful but was overturned by the Supreme Court
following an appeal by the Macedonian community in 1998. The latter
verdict was affirmed in 2000, following a further appeal by the Victorian
Government. However, the communitys outrage has translated into very
strong determination to maintain and transmit the language. Macedonian
is the only European language which has experienced an increase in the
number of home users in Australian over the period 1991 to 1996 (10.7%
Immigrant Languages in Australia 387
following a rise of 41.3% in the five years before the numbers having
increased from 45,610 in 1986 to 71,347 in 1996). This could not be due to
newmigration but shouldbe attributedto a combination of underclaiming
before, second generation maintenance, RLS, and perhaps even some
overclaiming. While the activists and teachers tend to come from the
Aegean region, and of earlier vintage, the highest maintenance rates are
among families from former Yugoslavia. It would be interesting to see if
similar RLS is prompted for other languages in immigrant settings due to
grievances at home and abroad.
In the 1980s, a number of young couples of Latvian parentage who
normally communicated in English at home shifted back to Latvian when
their first child was born to ensure intergenerational transmission.
However, the strong impetus to maintain the language declined when the
political motivation subsided with the collapse of the Soviet Union and the
independence of Latvia.
While the vast majority of Australians are opposed to racism4, some
public opposition to non-discriminatory immigration policy in 1984, some
hostility to Arabs and Muslims during the Gulf War (1990), and the emer-
gence of a far right party (199798) seemto have affected the motivation to
language maintenance of different groups in different ways. Chinese-
Australians have been more reluctant to transmit their community
language while in other groups, such as Arabic speakers, perceived racism
has led to stronger language, maintenance efforts and successful represen-
tations to government and the teaching profession arguing for the impor-
tance of their language andculture for all Australians (Clyne &Kipp, 1999).
This cultural variation could support intercultural differences in the
coreness of language in the value system (Smolicz, 1981) and the
ambivalencce of host community attitudes as a factor in language mainte-
nance and shift (Kloss, 1966).
Closing Remarks
From the above it will be evident that reversing language shift is not
something that has occurred widely or successfully in Australia, although
opportunities for language maintenance and delaying language shift are
quite plentiful. Language shift can be reversed in Australia but it has not
been reversed very much. Many of the measures discussed in Fishmans
writings are not favoured by either ethnic communities and families or by
wider Australian society because they detract from the interactionist
aspects of multiculturalism and from desired socioeconomic mobility, at
388 The Pacific
least by the second generation. The crucial Stage 6 is therefore difficult to
achieve.
The Australiansituationdemonstrates clearlythe usefulness andappro-
priateness of Fishmans model for the description, explanation and predic-
tionof RLS. As all sociolinguistic situations vary, it is necessarytoadapt the
model to local conditions. In this case, a small number of adjustments of a
general or particular nature have been canvassed:
(1) Deliberate efforts for intrinsic, instrumental or integrative motives to
raise childrenbilinguallyeither inthe context of anextendedfamilyor
community or simply through targeted communication with one
parent (thus even without Stage 6).
(2) National and local media may not always be a useful basis for a
distinction but there may be other differentiations in the nature of the
media.
(3) Media, both print and electronic, may not be as far forward in the
GiDS schema as suggested for intergenerational transmission of
minoritylanguages as theymaynot be usedor desiredbylater genera-
tions for content reasons.
(4) Since Fishman(1991), newtechnologies have startedplaying a greater
role in the lives of ordinary people, and they could facilitate or inhibit
RLS. The place of new technologies such as the Internet are perhaps
worth considering as they potentially link later generation users of a
language with the heartland of the language community and with
communities in other minority situations maintaining the language.
They can also add motivation, input and output opportunities for
literacy in the community language. This relies on the heartland
ensuringthat the language has andmaintains a place onthe Internet.
It is also suggestedthat the political context andpositionof the language
in Australia and beyond could provide incentives for the various stages in
RLS.
Notes
1. My thanks are due to Kylie Martin for help with the collection and analysis of
data. I also thank the ethnic schools associations and media authorities in each
state and territory, Irene Donohoue Clyne, Edina Eisikovits, Renee Zufi, Lee
Hubber and various community language radio stations, and the Working
Group on Multicultural Library Services (Victoria) for valuable information,
and Joshua Fishman and Joseph Lo Bianco for helpful comments.
2. The Uniting Church is the product of the amalgamation of the Methodist, Con-
gregational and most of the Presbyterian Churches.
3. In addition, there are some pay stations two Chinese and one each in Arabic,
Immigrant Languages in Australia 389
Croatian, Farsi, Greek, Korean, Portuguese, Turkish, and Indian languages in
Sydney, one each in Chinese, Greek, Macedonian, and Portuguese in Mel-
bourne, and one in Greek in Adelaide.
4. This was clear during the 1998 election and in the results.
References
Clyne, M.E., Eisikovits, E. and Tollfree, L. (in press) Ethnic varieties of Australian
English. In P. Collins and D. Blair (eds) English in Australia Amsterdam:
Benjamins.
Clyne, M., Jenkins, C., Chen, I. and Wallner, T. (1995) Developing Second Language
From Primary School. Canberra: National Languages and Literacy Institute of
Australia.
Clyne, M. andKipp, S. (1997) Trends andchanges inhome language use andshift in
Australia, 19861996. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 18,
45173.
Clyne, M. and Kipp, S. (1999) Pluricentric Languages in an Immigrant Context. Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Dawkins, J. (1991) Australias Language: The Australian Language and Literacy Policy.
Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.
Dpke, S. (1993) One Parent One Language: An Interactional Approach. Amsterdam:
Benjamin.
Fishman, J.A. (1967) Bilingualismwithor without diglossia: Diglossia withor with-
out bilingualism. Journal of Social Issues 23(2), 2938.
Fishman, J.A. (1991) Reversing Language Shift. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Imberger, B. (1986) Children fromGerman-speaking families. In M. Clyne (ed.) An
Early Start, (pp. 11227). Melbourne: River Seine.
Kipp, S., Clyne, M. and Pauwels, A. (1995) Immigration and Australias Language Re-
sources. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.
Klarberg, M. (1983) The Effect of Ideology on Language Teaching. PhD thesis. Monash
University.
Kloss, H. (1966) German American language maintenance efforts. In J.A. Fishman
(ed.) Language Loyalty in the United States (pp. 20652). The Hague: Mouton.
Lo Bianco, J. (1987) National Policy on Languages. Canberra: Australian Government
Publishing Service.
Ozolins, U. (1993) The Politics of Language in Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
SBS (1997) Annual Report 1996/97. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing
Service.
Saunders, G. (1988) Bilingual Children from Birth to Teens. Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters.
Smolicz, J.J. (1981) Core values and cultural identity. Ethnic and Racial Studies 4,
7590.
390 The Pacific
Chapter 17
Is the Extinction of Australias
Indigenous Languages Inevitable?
1
J. LO BIANCO and M. RHYDWEN
Introduction
InReversing Language Shift (1991) Joshua Fishmanconsideredimmigrant
origin minority languages and Australian indigenous languages together.
Fishman was well aware of the improbability of the co-location of such
different categories of languages, stating that these: represent two very
different sociolinguistic constellations and, also, two very different sets of
experiences and expectations vis--vis the Australian anglo and anglified
mainstream (p. 252).
We are grateful that in Can Threatened Languages be Saved? Professor
Fishman has decided to allocate separate chapters to these two categories
of languages. This chapter of RLS Revisiteddoes not contrast the status of a
single language in 1988 with its status in 1998. The demography of indige-
nous languages in Australia is such that precise quantification is impos-
sible. However, several instances of RLS effort (and instances of language
revival) are reported. Many of these were obtained by direct contact with
field workers and communities, and others from literature and research
produced since RLS 1991. We also evidence the manner in which policy-
making canmarginalise minority languages whenpower structures do not
represent the interests of their speakers.
The treatment of aggregated Australian RLS efforts contrasts with the
single-language focus of other case studies: Irish, Basque, Frisian, Navajo,
Spanish, Yiddish (secular and ultra-orthodox), Maori, Hebrew, Quebec
French and Catalan. The specificity of these instances makes discussion of
case studies straightforward and generalisations may be made depending
on the representativeness and typicality of the cases.
However, for Australian indigenous languages we must either assume
that all the settings (there are many hundreds) are among themselves
consistent to allowextrapolations to be made or we needto discuss specific
391
The Pacific
Australias Indigenous Languages
examples with a view to discerning what is particular about them in the
context of the Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale (GIDS).
The GIDS and Australian Indigenous Languages
The GIDS is a heuristic device for ascertaining the extent of dislocation
of intergenerational transmission of a language. The higher the stage at
which the language is located(8 being higher than 1) the greater the degree
of disruption of the normal transmission pattern and characteristics.
Stage 8, however, involves disruption not to the pattern of transmission of a
language code, but of the code itself, i.e. of its integrity as a code for trans-
mission.
The sociocultural setting and context of Australian languages is unique.
The speaker populations reside only in Australia and for many of the
languages the local relationshipto landis a critical context for the use of the
language. The transfer of the communicative practices of speakers to other
forms of speechresults inthe deathandextinctionof these languages. This,
in turn, means the disappearance of the sociocultural universe uniquely
given life by the languages. It is our contention in this chapter that GIDS (a
scale to calibrate an index of disruption) may require modification to
accommodate such characteristics.
This is not just as a result of the sociolinguistically marked differences
between indigenous and immigrant languages and their relationship to
English. It has also to do with the complex language ecology of communi-
cation among indigenous communities, traditionally multilingual and
multidialectal. The social structures of indigenous speech communities are
dramatically unlike those of the wider society, nor are they similar to those
of other minorityspeechcommunities that have enteredwesternisedmain-
stream society.
There are three principal reasons for considering modifications to GIDS.
Firstly, those minority languages that function in societal structures that
resemble the state hierarchies of late modernity(withelaborate administra-
tion and authority processes, personalised notions of identity and individ-
ualism, and capitalist techno-economy rewards and patterns) could
conceivably incorporate governmental functions, mass media and higher
education and other macro-functions anticipated in the GIDS. For many
Australian indigenous languages, however, Stage 1 and Stage 2 compre-
hensiveness is neither possible nor ever traditionally encountered. Stage 6
would have represented the maximal level and may still be the maximally
achievable objective.
Secondly, the functions for which intergenerational transmission of an
392 The Pacific
Australian indigenous language are critical are transmissive activities
across age cohorts, significant culturally marked relationships, and the
whole ensemble of traditional life (belief systems, practices, customaryand
lawactivity). Acomplete activity is conceivable with a Stage 6 character to
the language but no stage beyond this seems to be indispensably impor-
tant.
This last point, of course, raises a recurring tension about
intergenerational language maintenance. Domainseparationanddiglossic
patterns of communication have been identified as essential for some
intergenerational language retentioncontexts, but this maynot be possible,
or needed, in all cases. Fishman (1991: 401) points out that at GIDS Stages 4
to 1 RLS efforts are strong and transcending diglossia in search of
increased power sharing. This aspiration seems beyond any Australian
indigenous language, except perhaps for traditional ceremonial life where
the power relations are dramatically reversed between indigenous
languages and English.
It is here though that the changing the climate approach of much of
Australian RLS effort is centrally important. Without a dislodging of
English from what McConvell (1992: 215) calls the principle of conveying
of social meaning (rather than domain) the power imbalance between
these languages and English is too great. This is especially acute in relation
to GIDS 4b (Xish programmes in Yish schools) where considerable RLS
effort is directed in Australia, in the belief that the wider identification of
Aborigines with their ancestral life (and languages) needs to be restored
prior to any possibility of greater Xish use.
Thirdly, indigenous communities communication repertoires involve
Australianlanguages other thantheir own, other dialects of their language,
and evolving linguae francae, as well as English (including indigenised
Englishes). In such a communication complex the shifting that occurs can
be among a dynamic range of alternatives, in several directions, and
continually.
A central premise of the GIDS thesis is that intergenerational language
maintenance depends on community-family-neighbourhood control of
institutional resources and processes that permit continuous and socially
grounded natural or at least convenient domains of use of the marked
language. Boundary maintenance is critically important. The outside
language cannot be permittedto invade the boundaries of the inside since
it will erode its distinctive domain and undermine its transmission pros-
pects.
Stable situations of bilingualismare thus predicated on domain separa-
tion and diglossic patterns of communication, except perhaps for
Australias Indigenous Languages 393
languages which can marshal sufficient territorial and institutional
resources (GIDS 1 languages) to power share with languages of wider
communication such as English.
Types of Language Loss
Two main types of language loss have taken place among indigenous
languages in Australia, both of which result in extinction: an abrupt
dislocative and extreme form and a slower, generational, attrition. The
former often results in the total disruption of all transmission (and of any
later re-learning prospects) of the language, while the second can, at best,
retain within the living memory of speakers sufficient language resources
on which to base a revival or renewal activity. The former may be called
language loss by rupture and the latter language loss by attrition.
The initial contact periodbetweenwhite settlers andindigenous Austra-
lians produced many examples of the loss of language by rupture, via the
extermination of all speakers, their total dispersal so that no transmission
possibilities remained, or other forms of extreme denigration and damage
to the speech community. The period since has involved a slow, seemingly
inexorable attrition of all the domains of life traditionally functioning in
Australianlanguages andtheir colonisationbyother forms of speech, espe-
cially various forms of English.
Schmidt (1990/1993: 1235; see also Dixon, 1989: 123) identified five
stages of language loss by attrition. She points out that these are . . .
abstract points ona sliding scale, withthe healthy language at one pole and
total adoption of the replacing language at the other noting that it is not
necessary for all languages to pass through all stages.
Stage 1 languages have strongvitality, beingactivelytransmittedtochil-
dren and being the primary language of the community. The language is
known fluently by most speakers who rely on it as their principal means of
communication. A wide range of speech styles is in regular use and the
language tends not to have wide internal morpho-syntactical or lexical
variations.
Stage 2 languages involve contractions in speaker numbers and
increasing use of an alternative language. However, the language is still
transmitted intergenerationally, (and in a relatively complete form)
although not all of its new learners acquire a fully functional range of
competency in the speech styles of the language. There is evidence of the
influence of the replacing language within the linguistic features of the
threatened language.
Stage 3 languages are no longer transmitted to the younger generation.
394 The Pacific
Such languages remain fully functional only for some older users. Radical
simplifications are occurring from the replacing language which younger
members of the community are already using as their main means of
communication. Intergenerational communication makes use of pervasive
code-switching. The repertoire of speech styles activated in the threatened
language for all speakers is much restricted.
Stage 4 languages have no fluent speakers left. Their use is only in piece-
meal fashion (as markers and acts of identity) interspersed in speech that
is principally conducted in the replacing language.
Stage 5 has seen the loss not only of fluent, but also of semi-speakers of
the threatened language. The replacing language is used as the first
language of all the communitys members. Very occasional markers of dif-
ference remain in the speech of the community. The language is near-
death and can be considered extinct when its last fluent speaker dies.
Schmidts (and Dixons) Stage 1 and Stage 2 (healthy) levels are built on
rather different criteria from the GIDS levels 1 and 2, where societal
macrofunctions are critically important criteria.
Schmidts rough calculation is that there are 20 Australian indigenous
languages at Stages 1 and 2, 5060 at Stage 3, between 170 and 180 at Stages
4 and5. More thantwothirds of Aboriginal languages are alreadyextinct or
are nearing extinction (i.e. at Stages 4 and 5). About 90% of Aboriginal
Australians no longer speak their Aboriginal language.
Only Stage 1 and 2 languages can be considered to have vitality and at
present rates of attrition (at least one distinct language becomes extinct
each year) the number of languages at Stages 1 and 2 will halve within the
lifetime of the next generation.
The Status and Prospects for Australian Indigenous
Languages
The grim conclusion in Fishman (1991) about the negligible long-term
survival prospects of Australias indigenous languages has been continu-
allyreiteratedfor some time. Since 1991, however, several important public
inquiries have been undertaken into the status of and issues surrounding
Australias indigenous language heritage (Schmidt, 1990/1993; House of
Representatives, 1992; NLLIA, 1994; McKay, 1996).
Regrettably, none of these reports has yielded any coherent or
committed policy response (for utterly different specific reasons, though
for an overarchingly common factor): the relative political powerlessness
of the speakers of endangeredlanguages andthe overwhelmingpower of a
systemof policy making that is oriented towards granting greater priority
Australias Indigenous Languages 395
to English, and perhaps inadvertently but nevertheless powerfully, to
marginalising language maintenance.
(Indemonstrationof the powerful effect of the abilityof policymakers to
constitute the primary focus of educational language policy towards domi-
nant linguistic norms we discuss the discursive practice of making policy
(if not quite on the run at least policy in the making) with an extract of a
recent and highly relevant Parliamentary debate (see Lo Bianco, 1999a).
The asymmetrical power to name the problems experiencedby indige-
nous people means that in Parliament questions about support for indige-
nous language and cultural education can be deflected into a discussion
about the underperformance of Aboriginal children on English literacy
tests.
2
Many language workers and indigenous language advocates maintain
strong activity and commitment toward RLS but not usually with the
expectation of full restoration. In this regard Schmidt (1990/1993: 106)
states: If successful language revival is takento meanthe full restorationof
the language to a state of strong vitality (by re-establishing the broken
language transmissionlink; regaining full conversational language knowl-
edge and fluency; and active use of the language by all generations in wide
range of social contexts), the chances of success for threatened Aboriginal
languages are, in all probability, fairly remote. To my knowledge, there are
noinstances inAboriginal Australia of language revival efforts achieving
such goals. Schmidt does however support language revival efforts with
more modest (realistic?) goals.
Indigenous language speakers and language workers sometimes refer
tothe workof regional language centres as resemblinglanguage hospitals
where a range of emergency procedures (corpus planning and status
planning) are undertaken to restore languages to some semblance of
health.
Working with Australian Aboriginal languages the signs apparently
indicate that most of them are beyond saving. Yet despite the chances of
survival being negligible, there is a vast community based effort to keep
languages alive. Subsequent sections discuss the intervention of language
workers in a variety of maintenance and revival initiatives.
Categorising Aboriginal Languages
On its release Reversing Language Shift was hailed in Australia as the the
nearest thing that I know of to a users manual for language maintenance
intervention. (McConvell, 1992: 210). Aboriginal language workers have
expressed misgivings about the applicability of all levels of the GIDS
396 The Pacific
framework for Aboriginal language maintenance. Some of these misgiv-
ings undoubtedly arose because it is depressing to read that most of the
languages display symptoms that indicate they are not going to survive
and that the kinds of strategies being used are unlikely to be successful.
However, the reactions toRLShave not all beencouchedinthese emotional
terms. Rather, the message is that because of the particular nature of
Aboriginal languages (and their sociocultural contexts) some of the theo-
ries on which RLS is based may need modification (McConvell, 1992;
McKay, 1996). It may be that the apparent hopelessness of the situation of
Aboriginal languages has contributed to the generation of critiques and
alternative theories that allow at least a little optimism about the future.
A first reading of RLS as a users manual might lead one to think that
what is necessary is to work systematically through the stages from wher-
ever ones particular Xish is on the GIDS scale, up to Stage 1. There is no
doubt that the majority of Aboriginal languages are at Stage 8 on the GIDS.
Although estimates about the number of extant languages and dialects
vary considerably,
3
there is little doubt that most have fewyoung speakers
(only 20 according to Schmidt, 1990/1993). In viewof the bleak outlook for
most Aboriginal languages, the enthusiastic language maintenance worker
might look at the languages which were high on GIDS (but not necessarily
on Schmidts Stages 1 and 2 where different criteria are applied) and then
try to discover how this might be achieved in other contexts. Yet it soon
becomes apparent that there are problems with categorising any Aborig-
inal languages.
Some languages are being transmitted intergenerationally and used
extensively within particular speech communities, but whether they would
be defined as GIDS 1 is debatable because of the incommensurability of
emic and etic (insider and outsider) formulations of what constitutes the
highest educational, occupational, governmental and media activities
(Fishman, 1991: 107).
For Aboriginal people prior toinitial contact withwhites (andfor people
in some areas first contact is still within living memory) the highest educa-
tional, occupational, governmental and media activities always did take
place in their own language/s (though what these terms actually mean in
such contexts is difficult to decide). This language may now be used for
those same activities; but what is widely judged, certainly by the wider
community, to constitute, say, the highest educational activity is no
longer knowledge of traditional lawwhich is embodied in the oral stories,
rituals and songs, but nationally accredited qualifications such as univer-
sity degrees.
Thus, from one perspective, the highest activities, such as important
Australias Indigenous Languages 397
ceremonies and discussion of community politics, take place in the local
languages in some Aboriginal communities. Fromanother perspective it is
apparent that those people who exert institutionally sanctioned power in
the community (teachers, medical staff, administrators and community
leaders) must use Englishfor some, if not all, their occupational activities.
It could be argued that the fewcommunities in which intergenerational
transmission is still strong (and many community activities take place in
local languages) the languages are at 1 onthe GIDS, at least for the purposes
of serving as a model of what is possible in the Australian context.
However, given the pervasive spread of incorporation of the entire national
population of most countries into the web of economic and governmental
information collection and technological invasion of previously secure
social spaces, even the strongest languages remain extremely vulnerable.
In Europe, a language such as Breton with 500,000 speakers, is regarded
as a minority language. Even Manx, spoken in the Isle of Man off the west
coast of northern England, long regarded as extinct, has 200300 speakers
who learned it as adults. Such a result would be regarded as a major
triumph in Australia and evidence of great vitality. Of course, stating the
bald facts (that most Aboriginal languages have fewer that 500 speakers)
disguises the fact that many languages have never had more than a
comparatively small number of speakers.
Yet it would be dangerous to be lulled into thinking that because
languages with small populations have survived in the past, they will
continue to do so, particularly in the face of accelerating technological
change. Each situation, where Aboriginal language maintenance is an
issue, is unique and any attempt to generalise or typify is likely to run into
problems.
A few situations will nevertheless be described below, because they
exemplify some of the variety of issues that arise in working with Aborig-
inal languages and which impact on RLS prospects. Some of the complexi-
ties that arise in situations where the language is regardedas strong will be
discussed first.
What is Shifting?
Among the languages always included on any list of strong Aboriginal
languages is Yolngu Matha, a group of dialects spoken by around 6000
people in North East Arnhem Land. The Yolngu people are renowned for
their independence and pride. Their land was not impinged upon by
whites until the founding of a mission station at Yirrkala in 1934 followed
by mineral exploration in the 1960s and they have been able to retain their
398 The Pacific
languages. Indeed, as McConvell reports, the area is unusual inthat, unlike
most speakers of Aboriginal languages, the Yolngu people of NE Arnhem
Landdonot feel compelledtospeakEnglishwheninthe presence of whites
(McConvell 1991: 50). However, even in those areas where there is no
apparent or immediate threat to the survival of Aboriginal community
languages, the nature of those languages and the extent to which shift is
occurring within them, are issues of considerable concern.
The children in Yirrkala andthe surrounding homelands who would, in
the past, have spoken one of the Yolngu Matha clan languages, are now
speaking Dhuwaya, a koine. This is a simplified version of one of the
closely related local dialects, Gumatj, which was used initially for Bible
translation and later in the school Bilingual Program (Amery, 1985).
Language shift within Aboriginal languages is occurring elsewhere in
Arnhem Land. Increasingly linguae francae are replacing clan dialects. At
Galiwinku and Milingimbi Djambarrpuyngu is generally spoken by both
adults andchildren. This phenomenon is not confinedto ArnhemLand. At
Wadeye (formerly Port Keats) Murrinh-Patha has become the lingua
franca.
This raises interesting questions. Although some of these shifts are
partially attributable to white intervention, in the sense that the language
used as a lingua franca is often that used by early Bible translators (and in
school bilingual programmes) it is also the case that the languages were
selectedfor those purposes because theywere alreadydominant languages
in the community. Certainly the changing patterns of settlement, with
Aboriginal people living together in permanent housing in settlements
much larger than in the past, has led to changes in language use that reflect
the different situation. Traditionally in Arnhem Land, and many other
areas, clans are exogamous so that people would have married someone
whospoke a different dialect andeveryone wouldbe bilingual, if not multi-
lingual.
According to Gale, the immediate threat to clan languages in NE
Arnhem Land is not English (1993: 30). As in several other communities
where the use of ancestral Aboriginal languages is strong and the
languages are being transmitted intergenerationally, other local languages
are being lost in the process of language shift to a lingua franca.
That an Aboriginal language is healthy and being transmitted
intergenerationally is a cause for celebrationinthe face of the dominance of
English. Yet if the vitality of the language is at the expense of other local
languages, the celebration is also a wake. Moreover the importance of clan
dialects to Aboriginal systems of land tenure should be recognised.
Describingthe Yolngulandtenure system, Williams asserts that Words,
Australias Indigenous Languages 399
especially names, comprise a category of a land-owning groups most
important non-corporeal propertythe importance of names lies in their
relation to land, the groups most important real property (1986: 42).
Knowledge of the clan language is essential to maintaining evidence of
land tenure, and the complex relations between social groups and a shift
fromclan languages to a single Aboriginal language impacts upon cultural
maintenance just as a shift to English would.
At Roeborne, in Western Australia, Yindjibarndi is taught in the local
primary school. The Western Australian education department is
committed to offering a language other than English in every primary
school by the year 2000. Yindjibarndi is not the language of the locality of
Roeborne, which is Ngarlama country; it has become the lingua franca
following the removal of people froma wider area to what was previously
namedthe Roeborne Native Reserve. While the teaching of Yindjibarndi in
the school is welcomed by the local Aboriginal community such a move
reinforces the status of a language that is not in fact local.
It shouldbe notedthat the disruption of traditional patterns of language
use can have profound legal and cultural implications. Land claims, in
which Aboriginal people make legal bids to claim title to their traditional
lands, are based upon Aboriginal people being able to show an ongoing
and continuous relationship to the land being claimed. Linguistic knowl-
edge can be evidence of such a relationship. If local people are no longer
able to speak the local language because it has been superseded by a
different language (albeit a traditional Aboriginal language that may be
closely related) this can have far-reaching legal implications.
In some strong languages, such as Warlpiri (with 3000 speakers) and
Tiwi (1500 speakers) there is evidence that the varieties spoken by younger
people are influenced by English. The Warlpiri spoken by children at
Lajumanu is reported to be changing and has far more English borrowings
than their parents variety (Cataldi, 1990); and at Bathurst and Melville
Islands, the Modern Tiwi spoken by younger people shows the influence
of English (Lee, 1983). Thus, although the languages are surviving, there is
concernthat the varieties spokenby the younger generationmay signal the
start of their gradual demise. Other Aboriginal languages are being
replaced not by English but by one of the two widely spoken creoles.
Both Kriol (spoken across much of north Australia) and Torres Straits
Creole (spoken in the Torres Straits Islands andparts of north Queensland)
are English-based.
4
Kriol is used formally in a bilingual school programme
in Barunga and informally in many other schools. Both creoles are Aborig-
inal and/or Islander languages and they retain many characteristics
typical of such languages. For example, the pronominal system of Kriol,
400 The Pacific
with its dual and plural inclusive and exclusive forms, is like that of the
local ancestral languages rather than like English.
The use of Kriol as a lingua franca is oftenseenas the cause of the decline
in ancestral language use, yet it is sometimes argued that in such cases,
Aboriginal languages are being replaced by new Aboriginal languages.
These languages, with around 20,000 speakers each, are evidently very
healthy, yet the fact that they are replacing the use of ancestral languages in
many communities means that language maintenance there is often
complex and contentious.
It has been demonstrated that even in the situations where Aboriginal
languages are relatively strong (and which might be regarded as GIDS 1
languages within the communities where they are used) there is no room
for celebration or complacency. Languages are often strong only because
others are weak, because they have become linguae francae.
In situations where this is not the case, there is often evidence of other
kinds of change. Thus, it is clear that language shift is occurring every-
where, andis not restrictedto those situations where Aboriginal languages
are apparently heading towards extinction; nor is it unidirectional towards
English (though undoubtedly this is the most common type of language
loss by attrition).
Languages on the Edge
However, the majority of languages are not strong but weak. Very many
Aboriginal and Islander languages are extinct or have only a few elderly
speakers and most are at GIDS 8, requiring extensive intervention by both
community members and professionals. Given the endangered state of
Aboriginal languages the term revival is sometimes used to refer to virtu-
ally all language work. However, revival is more accurately applied when
speaking about languages that are not in daily use by several generations.
Revival includes reclamation (when there are no speakers of a language but
only historical records), renewal (where there are a few older people who
have some, usually incomplete, knowledge of the language) and
revitalisation (when there are some older speakers but younger people do
not use the language).
The terms language maintenance and language revival clearly refer to
different phases of a cognate activity (revival in its various forms must
precede maintenance for endangered languages). This is mainly due to the
massive disruption to indigenous people caused by European colonisation,
a disruption that has amounted to genocide in some instances.
5
Much of the work by linguists at the language centres around Australia
Australias Indigenous Languages 401
focuses ondocumenting andrecording the fewremaining speakers of such
languages. However, despite a vast communitybasedeffort for revival and
documentation, and a strong maintenance effort by communities and
language professionals, the data and facts are elusive.
As a dramatic illustration of this an employee at the Yamaji Language
Centre inGeraldtoninWesternAustralia reportedtoRhydwenin1998 that
she had recently found three speakers of Ngarlawankga, a language previ-
ously considered to be extinct! Ngarlawankga was spoken by people in a
neighbouring region but the workers at the language centre in that region
thought the last speakers had died. It transpired that three speakers of
Ngarlawankga had relocated to an area near the boundaries between the
two regions and had somehow been overlooked in previous language
survey work. The same worker had also found two speakers of Malgana
(another language previously reported to be extinct) and was able to docu-
ment some of the language before the speakers passed away.
It is clear (from the frequency with which one hears such stories from
those who work with languages at grassroots level) that this is not an
unusual occurrence. For this reason it is not really possible to document
withcertainty the socio-demographics of the indigenous speechcommuni-
ties (though it is undoubtedly the case that the overwhelming trend is
towards attrition).
The last decade of language policy has produced a network of funded
Regional Language Centres which conduct a wide range of community
based activities for documentation, surveying and describing languages,
as well as for RLS activity of many kinds. Nevertheless work with such
languages is still hamperedbythe financial constraints onlanguage centres
in remote areas where off-road vehicles are essential and fuel bills astro-
nomical.
Case Studies of Revival Initiatives and Other RLS Activity
There are some remarkable instances of language initiatives, some of
which are provisionally successful instances of the kinds of modest revival
that are feasible in the Australian context. The following four case studies
are taken from McKay (1996).
Borroloola and Barkly Tableland, Northern Territory
The white impact on the various Aboriginal groups who live in the
Borroloola region has affected each group differently over time depending
on the whites economic interests in the areas where each group lived (i.e.
402 The Pacific
first cattle industry, then fishing and more recently mining and tourist
industries).
Historically, the Yanyuwa and Garrawa people suffered less than any
other group in the region fromthe white impact because white settlers had
a lesser interest intheir geographical areas. Interestingly, the Yanyuwa and
Garrawa languages are the only two Aboriginal languages still spokenby a
reasonable number of people in the region.
Currently, around 500 Aboriginal people live in the Borroloola region.
Kriol or Aboriginal forms of English are the two main languages spoken in
this community. The two main Aboriginal languages still spoken are:
Garrwa and Yanyuwa. There remain a few speakers of two other Aborig-
inal languages: Marra and Kudanyji. About 14%of the Borroloola Aborig-
inal population are still full speakers of Garrwa, and 11% are full speakers
of Yanyuwa. All fluent speakers inthese two languages are middle-agedto
older people. In each language group, the equivalent of twice the number
of fluent speakers have partial competence in the language of the commu-
nity. Out of the total Aboriginal population, 31% of the people speak no
traditional Aboriginal language.
In 1916 the Marlandarri camp was set up. It included a strong Yanyuwa
community. The camp served to enforce segregation of Aborigines from
whites but it also provided Aborigines more control over their lives.
In 1963, the linguist Jean Kirton noted that parents spoke to their chil-
dren in Kriol. There are two different possible explanations for this: (1)
parents used Kriol to help their children learn English; or (2) parents used
Kriol in response to their childrens use of Kriol. They would do so out of
traditional politeness that promotes understanding between speakers as
the primary goal of verbal interaction over the protection of ones
language. However, children heard Yanyuwa being spoken around them
all the time and consequently they were still able to speak it themselves
later on in their lives. The youngest people who can still speak Yanyuwa
today spent their childhood at Marlandarri camp. Over time, the linguistic
homogeneity of the Yanyuwa people was broken down by different
circumstances and events. The most significant of these being work
patterns and intermarriages. Kriol was the lingua franca used by the
different linguistic groups working in the stations.
In the past, flu epidemics which caused the death of older speakers or
floods which forced people to move (and more recently the introduction of
television and videos) are other events which impacted negatively on the
Yanyuwa peoples ability to retain their language. Still further factors
contributing to language loss are governmental disregard for residence
patterns in decisions affecting the rebuilding of Aboriginal dwellings.
Australias Indigenous Languages 403
Those decisions contribute to the breakdownof kinrelationships so impor-
tant for language maintenance. Finally, the increase of non-Aboriginal resi-
dents in the region make traditional languages and cultures harder to
retain.
In the face of linguistic adversity, the response of Aboriginal people has
been to switch to English (non-standard English) hoping to give their chil-
dren a better chance in the non-traditional world. It is only recently that
newgenerations are starting to see that it is possible for two languages and
cultures, despite their differences in status, to coexist just as traditional
languages have coexisted in the past.
Historically the first school established in the Borroloola region
promoted the teaching of English (at the request of the Aboriginal commu-
nity). Aboriginal languages were usedby children in andoutside the class-
room. During the early 1970s indigenous languages were prohibited and
denigrated in the school system. In 1994, a division of opinion was docu-
mented between the different groups involved in the school community as
to whether and/or how indigenous languages should be taught. Aborig-
inal people themselves (like the small groupof white parents andteachers)
are divided on this issue.
Also in the past, the work of linguists such as Jean Kirton in Christian
missions has helped the maintenance of Aboriginal languages. Notably
these linguists translated songs of the Bible in the local Aboriginal
languages. Those songs are today often the only form of traditional
language Aboriginal children (and white children too) hear and use at
school and also in churches. The Borroloola Project is an example of a
language revival initiative.
The project started in 1987 as one of the activities of the Papulu Apparr-
kari Language Centre in Tennant Creek. The aimof the project is to record
material from elderly speakers of Garrwa and Yanyuwa (in writing or on
video) to ensure that younger generations have access to local Aboriginal
knowledge. The material collected ranges from lists of body parts, trees,
plants, etc. as well as taped stories, oral history and ceremonial singing,
videos of bush medicine and collection of dyes and pandanus for basket
weaving. There are positive outcomes to the project despite the lack of
appropriate funding which prevents the materials collected from being
turned into usable teaching materials:
Through the project, languages and cultures that are in decline are
being documented.
The children who take part in the bush trips to collect data interact
with the old people in the indigenous languages.
404 The Pacific
Finally the project helps the language project teamto gainstatus as an
official source of authority from which help can be sought regarding
language matters in the community.
As an example of language renewal and revitalisation we can consider
the Aboriginal Language and Culture Programmes in Barkly Region
Schools (includingBorroloola). Astudycarriedout in199091 inthe Barkly
and Sandover regions of the Northern Territory showed that a majority of
Aboriginal people spoke Aboriginal English or a variety of Kriol as their
first language. They expressed the need for better English and literacy
programmes in schools. Most Aboriginal people also wanted the local
Aboriginal languages and cultures to become part of the school curric-
ulum.
The region does not have an official bilingual programme. However,
local (and often short-lived) Aboriginal studies programmes are run in
schools under, inmost cases, the initiative of Aboriginal student teachers or
graduate teachers who have gained the confidence to act within the
Western education system. One of the main drawbacks of these
programmes is that they often do not provide actual language teaching.
In the Barkly area, Aboriginal studies programmes either fall within the
Northern Territory Department of Educations second languages policy or
its Social and Cultural Education (SACE) Guidelines. The second
languages policy is only a recommendation to schools and this category
also does not appropriately cover the specific language needs of Aborig-
ines. The SACE Guidelines, on the other hand, do not cover the sort of
programmes Aborigines wish to implement. Overall, the lack of strong
policy support means that Aboriginal language and culture programmes
have not attained a secure place in the school system.
Apart from policy issues, one of the main problems in the successful
implementation of Aboriginal Studies programmes is the lack of appro-
priate teacher training. Firstly the school system needs to recognise and
support Aboriginal people whoare inthe positiontoprovide language and
culture education (elders and younger people alike). This support must
include training in indigenous language literacy and language matters at
all levels, according to need and particularly on-site in remote communi-
ties.
The study referred to suggests that measures can be taken which have
provedto encourage andmotivate Aboriginal people to establishlanguage
andculture programmes inlocal communities. These measures include the
provision of opportunities involving Aboriginal people in language work
of various types and the provision of language education for teachers in
Australias Indigenous Languages 405
order to prepare them to find local solutions related to language mainte-
nance which includes learning howto work with the appropriate commu-
nity elders.
Gumbaynggir people and language near Kempsey, New South
Wales
Generally speaking the Gumbaynggir people, the main Aboriginal
community from the mid-north coast of New South Wales, were less
affected by white contact than other Aboriginal groups in other regions.
However, in the 1860s when white settlers started to introduce European
agriculture in the area, the Gumbaynggir people started to suffer severe
discrimination and social isolation. Most Gumbaynggir people nowlive in
the main small towns in the area.
The Gumbaynggirr people started to lose their traditional language
during the Protection era (1880s) when Aboriginal children where
removedfromtheir families andinstitutionalisedinorder tobe assimilated
into white society. Adults were also moved from their original areas to
workinstations inother regions where theymixedwithAboriginal groups
who spoke different traditional languages. Intra-group marriages became
more common and contributed to the decline in the use of traditional
languages. In the 1950s only pockets of older speakers still spoke
Gumbayngirr.
In 1994, only seven speakers of the language were identified in the
region. One of those speakers mentioned that it is only in 1986 that she
startedtore-use her traditional language duringmeetings organisedbythe
newly established Gumbaynggir Language and Culture Group.
Gumbaynggirr people now mainly speak various forms of English.
However, the traditional social and cultural system is still alive and
apparent in, for example, the continuing importance given to kinship rela-
tionships and Aboriginal speech forms of English.
The use of Aboriginal Englishes, however, has not helped the
Gumbaynggir people to be more accepted by the dominant society. It is
only in recent years that Aborigines in the region are trying to reconnect
with their linguistic and cultural heritage mainly through the work of a
Language Centre.
Language revival initiatives work through the Muurrbay Aboriginal
Language and Culture Cooperative in Kempsey. As in the Borroloola case
it is mainly under the initiatives of elders (in this case encouraged by an
outsider, Brother Steve Morelli) that the Gumbaynggir Language and
Culture group was established and later became the Muurrbay Aboriginal
and Culture Cooperative. The Cooperative is now funded as a Regional
406 The Pacific
Language Centre. The Centre has undertaken initiatives in language
revitalisation and revival including: writing Gumbaynggir in a simple
way, collecting and recording excerpts of the language, memories and
songs from living speakers. This groundwork for language renewal was
then used to produce language teaching materials, writing of dreaming
stories and plays and writing of a Gumbaynggir dictionary.
One of the Centres concerns is to avoid superficiality in the materials it
produces and the teaching it delivers. Language materials are based on
research from various sources including the Centres own data by also
tapes and documents held by the Australian Institute of Aboriginal and
Torres Strait Islander Studies and the University of New England.
In 1994, the Language and Culture Centre conducted a traineeship in
language and culture maintenance as well as other language and culture
programmes in primary and secondary schools. Among the outcomes of
the centres activities towards language renewal and language
revitalisation are that the Language Centre believes that there are now
more Aboriginal people who have basic knowledge of Gumbaynggir than
five years before it started its language activities. A far greater number of
members in the community (children and adults) have at least had some
contact with their traditional language and culture. It was noted that most
Gumbaynggir people want to learn Gumbaynggir names, phrases, place
names, songs and stories.
The Centres main successes have been to revive the language skills of
the surviving speakers and to pass them on to younger people who now
have the responsibility to keep the language and culture alive. It has also
helped the Aboriginal community in the region to reconnect with its heri-
tage.
Jaru at Yarunam (Ringers Soak), East Kimberley, Western Australia
One characteristic of the Jaru peoples history with white contact is that
as one of the latest groups to be dispossessedof their landthey were able to
use changes in political and social circumstances to successfully regain
access to their land in 1983.
In the stations where Aborigines worked Jaru language was tolerated,
unlike in other stations in other regions. This contributed to the mainte-
nance of the local traditional language. The whole Jaru community was
keen to enter the Catholic value systemas long as it was not at the expense
of their traditional culture. Most Aboriginal people in the Yarunam
community speak Jaru, including all children and Aboriginal adults from
non-Jaru descent such as the Warlpiri. Other languages (of limited use) in
the community are Warlpiri, Kriol, Ngardi and Gurindji.
Australias Indigenous Languages 407
The local school has run a bilingual English-Jaru programme since its
establishment. Since 1990, a teacher linguist visits regularly to assist in the
ongoing running and development of the programme. Local women are
heavilyinvolvedinthe programme. In1990, some of these womenenrolled
in a language worker training course. The two nuns involved in the
running of the school provide evening classes for adults (religious instruc-
tion, literacy classes, etc.).
Some of the problems regarding the teaching of Jaruat the school are that
the programme seems to employ well-used materials, thereby preventing
older children from expanding their language skills. Language games
seem to dominate the approach to teaching the Jaru language leaving
doubts as towhether anyeffective language learningis reallytakingplace.
In 1994 some initiatives were taken to start teaching English as a second
language inthe school. Some of the issues whichhave preventedsuccessful
learning of the Jaru language have been when non-Aboriginal linguists or
teachers become the experts onspelling andgrammar inthe local language
leavingthe Aboriginal populationof the school feelingdisempowered. The
sometimes heavy-handed criticism of the older Aboriginal speakers
towards the younger generations language also accounts for some of the
young Jaru people turning to English or a Creole to avoid negative feed-
back.
The local Catholic Churchuse of the Jarulanguage andculture inliturgy
plays a role in language maintenance. Both a local priest and a linguist
developed Jaru writing which was introduced at the school in the mid-
1980s. An extensive grammar of Jaru was published in 1981. In 1992 the
Kimberley Language Resource Centre also produced a draft Jaru Dictio-
nary.
There are several factors that contribute to language maintenance pros-
pects in the Yarunam community. Jaru is the dominant language of use in
daily life. The history of the region has allowed the language to live on
(through tolerance of Jaru in the stations). There has been a close and non-
oppressive relationship between the Jaru community of the local Catholic
Church. The Jaru programme in the school has fostered the learning of the
language and its status. The womens involvement in the school has
provided a bridge between the school and the community. The support
provided by the teacher-linguist has also been important. There is a positive
sense that Jaru people are in control of their community and that their rela-
tiveisolationfromthetownlifestylehas beenapositivecontributoryfactor.
However, there are several factors that threaten Jaru language mainte-
nance. The gradual increase of the use of Aboriginal English is an indicator
of future problems. This is compounded by the number of intermarriages
408 The Pacific
and the increase in interaction with non-Jaru people, mainly through
council meetings, school and church. There is also concern that the
language programme at the school is not developingas a proper language
programme.
Saibai Island and Torres Strait, Far North Queensland
Traditionally two distinct Aboriginal languages were spoken in the
Islands of Torres Strait along with many dialects; most people, however,
were monolingual. Torres Strait Islanders had much contact with the
inhabitants of the Papuan Coast.
In the 1840s, indigenous languages entered into competition with other
languages (EuropeanandMelanesian) withthe discovery andexploitation
of commercial quantities of Beche-de-mer, pearl shell and trochus. Pacific
Pidgin English soon became the lingua franca among marine workers. The
arrival of the London Missionary Society further activated the progressive
loss of the traditional languages in the region. In the late 19th century as
Pacific Pidgin English started to be used by children as their first language
it soon became a Creole.
The widespread use of Torres Strait Creole came about out of necessity
for a lingua franca among the diverse ethnic groups brought together by
the development of the marine industries, but alsobecause the local indige-
nous population perceived this Creole as being English itself. This erro-
neous belief started to be eroded only when Torres Strait Islanders meet
with native English-speaking soldiers in the Australian Army during the
Second World War.
The association of English and Creoles in local administration churches
andschools contributedto the prestige of Creoles. It is also through the use
of Creoles that the different groups of Torres Strait Islanders were able to
develop a pan-Islander consciousness in between the two world wars.
After the Second World War (and by 1980) half of the population of
Islanders hadmigratedto the Australianmainland. This groupof islanders
realised then that the Creole they spoke did not give them equal status to
whites in Australian society. There are, however, different attitudes
towards the use of Creole among older and younger generations of Torres
Strait Islanders.
The Native Title decisioninthe HighCourt of Australia (1993) affects the
Murray Islands and has prompted newinterest in the revival of traditional
languages. (Under the terms of the Native Title decisionone demonstration
of continuingassociationwithclaimedlandas a criterionfor a gainingland
ownership is language retention).
Overall, despite the various language maintenance projects inthe Torres
Australias Indigenous Languages 409
Strait and some isolated instances of individual initiatives, the indigenous
language movement in the region tends to be relatively inactive.
Compared with Torres Strait, Sabai was less affected by white impact
because the islandhadless to offer economically. The biggest white impact
was felt mainlyafter the arrival of missionaries in1871. The white impact as
well as new diseases and abnormal floods after the Second World War
contributed to the dispersal and dislocation of the Sabai population. In the
1990s only 300 Sabai people still lived on the island. The Anglican Church
has had a significant role over the past 100 in years in providing strength
and unity among the local population.
The main language used by the majority of the population on the island
is Kalaw Kawaw Ya. Most people use the Torres Strait Creole as a second
language. Children can speak some Creole before they start school. Both
English and Creole are widely reinforced among children when they start
secondary school on Thursday Island.
In Sabai Island State School the official language of instruction is
English. There is no formal Kalaw Kawaw Ya language programme in the
school but the language is usedwidelyinthe lower grade classrooms. Sabai
people are divided between their interest in maintaining their language
(although this interest is not always strong among children) and their
recognitionof Englishas a language of necessityto have access to the wider
white Australiansociety. There is a perceptioninthe Sabai community that
language matters should be left to elders and people with linguistic
training. This does not always permit language revival initiatives.
The language used in the local church is usually KalawKawawYa with
some uses of English and Creole. Written KalawKawawYa has existed for
a long time. There is a tendency nowto rely on the spreadof KalawKawaw
Ya literacy for language maintenance to the potential detriment of the use
of spoken language.
Interest in language issues is relatively high among the Sabai commu-
nity. However, many Sabai people who have received linguistic training
are often not employedin positions where they make use of this training or
they no longer live on the island. Two major initiatives have contributed
(andstill contribute) to the maintenance of KalawKawawYa: the introduc-
tion of a KalawKawawYa Language Programme in Thursday Island State
High School and the use of Kalaw Kawaw Ya in Indigenous Language
Broadcasting.
Language maintenance onSabai Islandhas not beenattributedto partic-
ular programmes or activities but to a complex of attitudes and circum-
stances which combine to facilitate the transmission of the traditional
language across generations. The relative homogeneity of the local
410 The Pacific
community together with the unifying influence of the Church (combined
with the generally experienced feeling among Sabai people that their iden-
tityis linkedwiththe use of their language) are the mainsupport factors for
language maintenance. However, the factors that currently threaten
language maintenance are the political and social status of English and of
English speakers as well as the dual roles of Torres Strait Creoles (Creole as
violating identity or as a marker of identity). The breakdown of
intergenerational linkages (children spend less time with elders in the
bush) is another disruptive factor. Insistence on the use of English as the
language of instructionineducationis also problematic since it reduces the
style range that children have access to.
Apart fromthese cases inMcKay(1996) direct contact withfieldworkers
indicates other interesting language maintenance initiatives across the
continent. Kaurna, a language spoken in the region of South Australia
where Adelaide is situated, lost its last fluent speaker in 1929. Kaurna had
beenwell documentedinthe middle of the 19thcenturybut writtenrecords
did not provide a good indication of the sound system. This was recon-
structed by comparing the written records with recordings of a neigh-
bouring andrelatedlanguage, made earlier this century. Recently, Nungas
(Aboriginal people of the Adelaide area) have renewed their interest in
tracing their Kaurna ancestry.
In 1986, Kaurna Plains School (a Nunga school) started a Kaurna
language programme and in 1994 a senior secondary programme started
and Kaurna has since been recognised as an accredited secondary school
subject (Nathan, 1996). Kaurna is certainly worthy of inclusion as a revival
initiative and immensely commendable in its scope and ambition.
However, it is still an acquired language and its intergenerational trans-
mission status and chances must rank as problematical.
The case of Noongar is also encouraging. As previously mentioned, in
Western Australia all primary schools will have a Languages other than
English programme by the year 2000. In Langford Primary School in
suburban Perth, all the children learn Noongar as their second language.
Almost half the childrenat the school are Aboriginal. About 3%are froma
variety of other non-English-speaking (immigrant) backgrounds.
Noongar is the language that incorporates the dialects of the Noongar
people, the Aborigines of the south-west of the state, including Perth.
When the first edition of Wilfred Douglas book documenting Noongar
was written in 1968, many of the remaining speakers were very elderly.
Douglas describes the difficulty of eliciting evidence that people had any
knowledge of the language at all. Now, however, there has been a resur-
gence of interest and pride. The people who teach Noongar at the school
Australias Indigenous Languages 411
have themselves had to learn the language, reclaim it for themselves, and
now are teaching it to both Aboriginal and non-Aboriginal children, none
of whomare familiar withit. By having the language taught as a core main-
stream subject, it is hoped that the stigmatisation of Aboriginal languages
will be reversed.
Such programmes acknowledge the fact that generations of banning the
use of Aboriginal languages by those children for whom it is a first
language is why Aboriginal people have grown up ashamed to speak or
transmit their languages. This is a problem in most areas, even away from
major urban centres, where there has been extensive contact with whites.
The Noongar example is also positive but its capacity to become the first
language of a self-regulating cultural community, and the language trans-
mitted to a newgeneration, however small numerically, of Xmen is highly
problematical. Such revival programmes have value as cultural mainte-
nance and awareness even if they do not succeed, or cannot succeed, in
totally restoring a language.
The stigma attached to the speaking of Aboriginal languages is wide-
spread. McConvell has documented how children at Turkey Creek, in the
north of Western Australia (whose ancestral language is Kija) laughed
uproariously at a film of children in another community speaking Pitjant-
jatjara (1986: 116). The Kija childrens mother tongue is Kriol, although
their parents are bilingual in Kija and Kriol.
Thus, in a few situations where a language is a GIDS level 8, it is taught
within a mainstream school programme in Aboriginal schools or schools
with a high Aboriginal population. In Perth, the introduction of Noongar
into schools has occurred as there has been development of Aboriginal
Studies programmes in schools and universities.
Whilst ultimately languages cannot be maintained without inter-
generational transmission, the climate in which such transmission can
occur has to be created. Reversing Language Shift cannot meanfossilising
language. According to Mhlhausler (1992) it is language ecologies that
ought to be cultivated rather than languages in isolation (this notion is
suggestedinFishmans call for boundarypreservation) andsocial space for
minority speech communities. All living languages are constantly shifting.
Any attempt to stop that process is bound to be frustrated but there are
tools that can be used to try to control the direction, speed and conse-
quences of language shift.
Australianinitiatives for language revival of indigenous languages have
been fired by optimism and struggle rather than any widespread or
systematic adoption of a planned revival approach. In any case the docu-
mentary basis for this is largely unavailable. It has been demonstrated that
412 The Pacific
apparently strong languages are extremely vulnerable. It has also been
shown that, despite this, unexpected examples of revival in places where
languages have been believed to be long dead can sometimes occur.
Chronology of Policy Moves for Indigenous Languages
The public policy interest in indigenous languages commenced seri-
ously in 1973
6
with Commonwealth (Federal) government intervention in
Northern Territory schools (then under Commonwealth jurisdiction) to
commence bilingual education.
7
However, in policy terms it was not until
1987 and the adoption of a comprehensive approach to national language
planning that explicit attention was paid to language maintenance and
intergenerational retention of indigenous languages.
For the first time since the European arrival about 200 years before,
the Commonwealth Government formally recognised indigenous
Aboriginal languages through a National Policy on Languages in
1987. The National Policy on Languages is noteworthy as the first
formal Commonwealth Government policy recognising the viability
and worth of Aboriginal languages and their endangered state .
Although considerably less than the $6 million originally advocated
in the Lo Bianco report, the NALP (National Aboriginal Languages
Program) is significant, for this was the first time that the Common-
wealth Government hadallocateda sizeable portion of money specif-
ically for the maintenance of Australian Aboriginal languages, as a
means of implementing an official language policy statement.
(Schmidt, 1993: 434)
Fishman (1991: 2778) contrasts the critical need for efforts at RLS to be
sequentiallyplannedandtobe RLSinformed against the superficial insti-
tutional flourishes that government supports, and earlier remarks that
many of the steps taken are either largely unrelated, non-productive or
even counter-productive as far as intergenerational RLS-pay off is con-
cerned. He regards boundary setting and boundary preservation as
crucially important (domain separation requires this) and notes that such
needs contrast with the ethos of shared participationism that dominates
bothdemocratic andauthoritarianregimes. He advocates small scale self-
regulation and self-help at the local level.
These critiques of policyare of fundamental importance for anyappreci-
ation of what ought to be done. But what is policy? And whose policies
count? And, as Fishman asks, can policy really make a difference? In
considering these questions we will need to address what kinds of policy
Australias Indigenous Languages 413
exist and notions of the linguistic culture within which these policies are
enacted.
Ball (1993) makes a distinction between policy as text (referring to the
legislative-administrative form that policies take) and policy as discourse
(referring to the ongoing talk andwriting that precedes, accompanies and
follows the text).
Schiffman (1996) introduces the idea of linguistic culture (a notion that
folds all language policies intoanoverarchingenvelope of cultural framing
attitudes, ideologies and histories). According to Schiffman linguistic
culture impacts seriously on the prospects of achieving the stated goals of
language policy. We cannot understand overt language policy and its
chances of success without reference to the covert realm of underlying
linguistic culture, the belief system, collection of ideas anddecisions and
attitudes about language (Schiffman, 1997: 59).
Goals of RLS in Australian indigenous languages must be understood
withinframes of the complex relationbetweenpolicyandcontext, between
ideological and societal framing and the actual realisation of RLS, what
Fishman refers to as RLS payoff.
There have been constant shifts in public policy for languages in
Australia. The main official positions of government policy, as well as
influential documents or other developments, can be dated from the 1973
introductionof bilingual educationinareas of Commonwealthjurisdiction
(essentially the Northern Territory, which subsequently attained self-
rule).
The formation in 1981 of the Aboriginal Languages Association (by
community activists, academics, teachers and others) to lobby, support,
research and coordinate national effort on behalf of Aboriginal languages
coincidedwithvigorous lobbying by ethnic community organisations for a
national language policy which would have language maintenance as one
of its key goals. A national coalition involving both ethnic and Aboriginal
activists came into operation in that year (Ozolins, 1993).
Some 10 significant policy-influencing documents succeededeachother
until a policy breakthrough under the National Policy on Languages,
adopted by Federal Cabinet on 4 June 1987, the first official explicit
language policy. This incorporated principles of Aboriginal self-determi-
nation and language maintenance rights. Under the NPL the first
programme for Aboriginal language support, the National Aboriginal
Languages Programme, was established.
Also at this time the AustralianAdvisoryCommittee of Languages and
Multicultural Education was established to oversee the implementation
of the NPL; with a Standing Committee comprised mainly of (and
414 The Pacific
chaired by) indigenous people, to advise on Aboriginal languages and to
disburse NPL funding to community language maintenance projects.
NALP was subsequently incorporated into the Aboriginal Education
Strategic Initiatives Programme (AESIP) under the Education (Supple-
mentaryAssistance) Act 1989 as part of AEPandthenlater subsumedbya
new national language policy: Australias Language: The Australian
Language and Literacy Policy. Finally, the National Languages andLiteracy
Strategy (NALLS), nominated by the Minister for Aboriginal and Torres
Strait Islander Affairs in the Joint Policy Statement for the AEP National
Priorities for 199395, took over responsibility for indigenous languages
funding.
This pattern of progressive assertion in policy of language maintenance
rights has, however, been interrupted with the expansion of public
concern, fanned by political intervention, over English literacy achieve-
ment standards. In 1997 this concern took the form of several key docu-
ments:
MappingLiteracyAchievement, Masters, G. andFoster, M., DEETYA
Literacy Standards in Australia, Minister for Employment, Educa-
tion, Training and Youth Affairs
Aspects of Literacy, AssessedSkill Levels, 1996, Australian Bureau of
Statistics.
The cumulating effect of these early developments had been a widespread
acceptance of the benefits and desirability of two way education: Aborig-
inal and wider society, English and ancestral language (where applicable)
within a culturally appropriate pedagogy. However, these effects have
recentlycome starklyupagainst a countervailingforce withthe concernfor
English literacy attainments generally (andspecifically for Aboriginal chil-
dren) making vulnerable the case for bilingual education and language
maintenance initiatives.
Policy and Power: Defining Problems and Shaping Prospects
The latest (and bleakest) chapter in public policy concerns the 199899
abandonment of public fundingsupport for indigenous language bilingual
education. The Northern Territorys education approach is exemplified in
its 1995 report on its bilingual education (Northern Territory Department
of Education 1997a and 1997b). In 1995 there were 21 accredited bilingual
programmes (using 34 languages and dialects) distributed across 16
Department of Education schools, 3 Catholic and 1 independent school.
The stated main aims of these programmes are:
Australias Indigenous Languages 415
to support and promote Aboriginal languages and culture;
to foster proficiency in school work through the use of the Aboriginal
languages;
to develop competency in Literacy in English and Aboriginal
languages;
to develop competency in Mathematics (Northern Territory Depart-
ment of Education, 1997a: v).
It is interesting that these programmes are seen to respond to key policy
developments referred to above such as the National Aboriginal and
Torres Strait Islander Education Policy, the goals of the National Aborig-
inal Languages and Literacy Strategy and recommendations of the Royal
Commission into Aboriginal Deaths in Custody.
Appendix One is an extract from the Official Hansard record of the
stormy debate that took place in the House of Representatives on 10
December 1998. The extract serves to highlight a critically important aspect
of public policy (policy here understood as discourse). The fracas is about
the decision of the NT government to remove funding from Aboriginal
bilingual education programmes, originally established by the Common-
wealth government when education in the NT was jurisdictionally
Commonwealth responsibility (Lo Bianco, 1999a).
The Commonwealth government has been accused of backing the NT
decision. Both governments are pushing English and are accused of using
the low(English) literacy achievement standards of Aboriginal children as
a ruse for getting rid of bilingual education. The extract points out how
language policy (i.e. language policy as discourse) is made in debate by who
has the power to define what the problemis and points up that in the RLS
versus English dichotomy how without any capacity for self-regulation
minority community languages maintenance is vulnerable.
Several scholars of public policyprocesses have notedthe crucial impor-
tance of what is sometimes referred to as problem definition (Edelman,
1988; Majone, 1989; Drey, 1984). In this process there is a contest to name
(and constitute by such naming) a field or area as the problem to which
public resources will be devoted.
One of the deficiencies of formal language planning theory is precisely
its inability(see LoBianco, 1999b) todeal withpolicyas discourse, andwith
the discourse as shaping the policy (Yeatman, 1990).
In the extract we note a contest (made sharp and angry by the time
constraint and the overtly politicised character of the debate) about what
precisely is lost and gained by the Northern Territorys decision in relation
416 The Pacific
to bilingual education. For the Minister the issue and problem is the
overrepresentation of Aboriginal children among those performing least
well in English literacy assessments. For the opposition questioners the
issue with bilingual education is bilingualism and issues of cultural main-
tenance. Which construction of the problem prevails (and which is
silenced) makes a significant difference to how complex multifaceted
issues become the object of policy makers attention.
Constructing literacy as the main problem of Aborigines (and of literacy
as being effectively synonymous with English) is the culmination of a
process commenced in the 1992 Australian Language and Literacy Policy
which aimed to reverse the multiculturalist orientation of the National
Policy on Languages (Moore, 1996).
The dominant rationalisationfor language policyat the Commonwealth
has been progressively changed fromone concerned with efforts to bolster
multilingualism in community contexts and in the public sphere, to one
where economic and trade (and general efficiency criteria) dominate.
Such rationalisations while they avoid the overt language and nomencla-
ture of assimilation may often lead to the same effect.
Literacy in English is named and constituted as the principal focus of
language policy. As far as languages other than English are concerned
the facilitation of external trading relations with newly powerful
northern trade partners occupy the most persuasive parts of the policy
discourse.
Community-oriented language maintenance is relegated to a subordi-
nate standing with neither vigour nor resources to sustain those fewtoken
acclamations which remain in the policy documentation. The policy texts
often remain very positive. The policy discourse, how the powerful have
talked about why the state should concern itself with language issues, has
side-lined and made vulnerable multicultural oriented language policy.
At the same time the wider community focusedapproachof the National
Policy on Languages has been narrowed to an education focus. Despite the
commendable efforts of education based interventions, the narroweduca-
tion framework within which language maintenance, retrieval and revival
activities have been grounded is doomed to failure.
Linguistic culture has been captured by an ideology of economism, in
which rationales of trade, efficiency, consumerism and globalised econo-
mic pressures dominate. Intergenerational transfer and maintenance of
minority languages (as the GIDS stipulates) depends on coherence of
community-family-neighbourhood institutions and their authenticity.
These in turn depend on some capacity of speaker populations to autono-
Australias Indigenous Languages 417
mously create, govern and reform their communal institutional environ-
ments and settings.
But all this runs up against the long reach of consumer economy and the
kind of society consumption economics generates. All community con-
trolled social space is eroded by the long reach of administration, economy
and wider societal values. The net of public-bureaucratic monitoring (with
the establishment of standards for measurement of normalised achieve-
ment in education, certification and accreditation of virtually all fields of
endeavour) is incorporating more and more of previously private life
within the surveillance systems of government, public authorities and
corporations.
Whereas literacy issues were once a primary concern of developing
nations (based on its presumed universal achievement in developed
nations) for reasons of national consolidation and the creation of national
cultures in often post-colonial settings the knowledge economy and cut-
throat globalisation has stimulated developed nations to embrace policies
for literacy enhancement.
These trends were anticipated in the 1991 Australian Language and
Literacy Policy (prefiguring a monolingual orientation it was ominously
titled: The Language of Australia). 1997 is probably the culmination of that
trend. It can be called the year of literacy. Virtually all other considerations
of language policy are sublimated to literacy, which is becoming the
Australianvariant of English-only. Moore (1996) shows some of the origins
of this inthe adoptionbybothmajor political forces inAustralianpublic life
of the economistic logic previously associated with the new right.
International global competitiveness has also made a belief in the
achievement of improvements in average education levels (symbolised by
literacy in national languages) a commitment of all governments (OECD,
1996, 1998) within a paradigmof human capital theorisations. Investing in
humans (as capital assets) is conceived as one of the main roles of the state.
All else is left to the competitive market place. Normalising measurements
are given encouragement by the need for comparison which reduces
cultural differences and contextual settings. In such a climate the policy
discourse is so utterly dominated by what in Australia has come to be
called economic rationalism (user pays, small government, privatisation
of public resources, etc.) that public policy around sustaining community
efforts to reverse language shift have no place.
If this were toresult ina shrinkingof the state awayfromits involvement
in community and family life, Aboriginal Australia may be permitted the
space for self-determination and self-regulation sufficient to allow ances-
418 The Pacific
tral languages legitimate control of GIDS 1-like activity and resources.
Regrettably, this does not appear to be the case.
Observations about GIDS and RLS for Australian Indigenous
Languages
To summarise then, it is clear that the principles of the GIDS and RLS
(1991) describe the parlous state of Australias indigenous language ecolo-
gies and document their meagre prospects for longer-term survival.
Despite occasional instances of revival efforts that attain short-term, unex-
pectedandspiritually uplifting gains for communities of speakers of tradi-
tional languages the pattern of attrition and extinction appears inexorable.
Indeed it has probably accelerated in the decade since RLS was published.
We have also arguedthat the state, motivatedby economistic logic of social
incorporation, and even benevolently intended discourses of equality
continues to find ways to invade the social spaces and social meanings
whose conservation is critical to the retention and intergenerational trans-
mission of indigenous languages. Finally we have argued that language
policy is discursive as muchas it is textual. Inits manifestationas discourse
asymmetrical power relations mean that the interests of monolingual
ideologies and practices of nation making are destructive of diversity and
multilingualism.
Specifically about the Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale we
argue that:
(1) It is unclear how the levels one and two of the GIDS apply to indige-
nous situations or to languages whose terminological range and
discourse patterns (andpresent power structures inthe wider society)
make it unlikely that these languages would ever fulfil all of the func-
tions envisaged (e.g. higher education). Conversely, the principles of
diglossia anddomain separation make it unlikely that such functional
extension would be needed for intergenerational language mainte-
nance if the social meanings that the indigenous languages uniquely
carry were able to be assured even within social spaces and practices
unprotected from the invasive pattern of English.
(2) There is a major needtotheorise policyinways that permits a nuanced
consideration of the discursive power of monolingualising institu-
tions andforces. Policy can only leadto real change for RLS if control
of resources and means for decision making and the institutional
domains where language socialisation occurs is in the hands of those
affected. The effects of competitive globalisationare powerfully coun-
teracting any such capacity.
Australias Indigenous Languages 419
(3) Although the National Aboriginal Languages Programme was com-
munity based and did not just operate within the formal (Yish) educa-
tion sphere later appropriations of language maintenance policy,
although they were better funded, did overly emphasise the Yish
education domain.
(4) These developments and the general orientation of formal policies
beginning with the 1991 Australian Language and Literacy Policy
have commenced a policy conversation in which the relative claims of
first language maintenance have been ranked against the demands of
a national standardising claim for English literacy. This has had the
deleterious effect of judging bilingual education in terms of Yish
norms and specifically elevating English literacy to superordinate
importance. The planned removal of funding for bilingual education,
and the policy makers reconstrual of the problem of indigenous
bilingual education as a matter of English literacy to approximate
national rigidly normalised benchmarks are direct consequences of
this strategic naming of language issues.
(5) The notion of domain only applies in a most general sense, and
McConvells suggested replacement of the principle of social mean-
ing for macro-domain and diglossia is promising in that indigenous
languages communicative practices may better be able to be accom-
modated within GIDS.
Notes
1. The assistance of Chantal Crozet is gratefully acknowledged.
2. Official Hansard Commonwealth of Australia Parliamentary Debates, House
of Representatives Thursday 10 December 1998. Topic: Northern Territory, Bi-
lingual Education, pp. 18721875. See Appendix One.
3. According to a draft report by FATSIL (p.c. Lester Coyne, president FATSIL)
there are 328 indigenous languages anddialects whichare either incurrent use
or are being researched for the purpose of revival. Ethnologue lists 151, of
which67 are describedas nearlyextinct or extinct and33 that have fewer than
50 speakers. Dixon (1980: 1) reports 200 distinct languages; while the House of
Representatives (1992: 1) comments: Before European settlement of Australia
there were approximately 250 Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander languages.
These were quite distinct languages which between them included about 600
dialects.
4. For descriptions of Australiancreoles see Sandefur (1979) andShnukal (1991).
5. See, for example, Bringing them Home, Report of the National Inquiry into the
Separation of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Children from their Fam-
ilies, Sydney: Human Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission (1997),
(AGPS).
6. Watts, B., McGrath, W. and Tandy, J. (1973) Bilingual Education in Schools in Ab-
420 The Pacific
original Communities in the Northern Territory. Canberra: Department of
Education.
7. These programmes have recentlycome intothe public spotlight withthe release
of an NT government report announcing the progressive removal of funding
for bilingual educationinpublic schools. Interestinglyinthis context the former
Minister for Educationresponsible for establishingthese programmes wrote on
15 December 1998 in a letter published in The Australian Newspaper (p. 12) (Kim
Beazley Snr, Minister for Education 197275) that while (preserving) an aspect
of Aboriginal culture might have been an effect of such programs it was cer-
tainly not the intention. The main aim was enhancing the learning of English
literacy. This claim has been much contested in the subsequent debate.
References
Ball, S.J. (1993) What is policy? Texts, trajectories and toolboxes. Discourse 13(2),
1017.
Cataldi, L. (1990) Language maintenance and bilingual education at Lajamanu
School. In C.Walton and W. Eggington (eds) Language: Maintenance, Power and
Education in Australian Aboriginal Contexts. Darwin: Northern Territory Univer-
sity.
Dixon, R.M.W. (1980) The Languages of Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Dixon, R.M.W. (1989) The original languages of Australia, 3, 2634.
Douglas, W.H. (1976) The Aboriginal Languages of the South-West of Australia. Can-
berra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.
Drey, D. (1984) ProblemDefinition in the Policy Analysis. Lawrence: The University of
Kansas Press.
Edelman, M.J. (1988) Constructing the Political Spectacle. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
House of Representatives Standing Committee of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Is-
lander Affairs (1992) Language and Culture A Matter of Surivival: Report of the
Inquiry into Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Language Maintenance. Canberra:
Australian Government Publishing Service.
HREOC(1997) Bringing themHome, Report of the National Inquiry into the Separation of
Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Children from their Families. Sydney: Human
Rights and Equal Opportunity Commission, Australian Government Pub-
lishing Service.
Gale, M.A. (1993) Code-Switching, Manymak or Yaka Manymak? ADiscussion Paper on
Language Use, Language Change and Language Loss. Occasional Papers in Applied
Linguistics. Darwin: Center for Studies of Language in Education, NTU.
Lee, J. (1983) Tiwi Today. PhD thesis. Australian National University.
Lo Bianco, J. (1987) National Policy on Languages. Canberra: Australian Government
Publishing Service.
Lo Bianco, J. (1999a) Policy words: Talking bilingual education and ESL into Eng-
lish literacy. Prospect 14(20) 4052.
Lo Bianco, J. (1999b) The language of policy, What sort of policy making is the
officialization of English in the United States? In T. Huebner and K. Davis (eds)
Socio-political Perspectives on Language Policy & Planning in the USA (pp. 3962).
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Australias Indigenous Languages 421
Majone, G. (1989) Evidence, Argument and Persuasion in the Policy Process. New Ha-
ven: Yale University Press.
McConvell, P. (1992) Reviewof Fishman1991. Australian Journal of Linguistics 12 (1),
20920.
McConvell, P. (1991) Understanding language shift: A step towards language
maintenance. In S. Romaine (ed.) Language in Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
McConvell, P. (1986) Aboriginal language programmes andlanguage maintenance
in the Kimberley. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics, 10822.
McKay, G. (1996) The Land Still Speaks: Review of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander
Language Maintenance and Development Needs and Activities. Commissioned Re-
port No. 44. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.
Moore, H. (1996) Language policies as virtual reality: Two Australian examples.
TESOL Quarterly 30(3), 47399.
Mhlhausler, P. (1992) Preservinglanguage or language ecologies? Atop-downap-
proach to language survival. Oceanic Linguistics 31(2), 163180.
Nathan, D. (ed.) (1996) Australias Indigenous Languages. Adelaide: Senior Secondary
Assessment Board of South Australia.
NLLIA(1994) Backing Australian Languages, Review of the Aboriginal and Torres Strait
Islander Languages Initiative Program(Confidential Report). Canberra: Aboriginal
and Torres Strait Islander Commission.
Northern Territory Department of Education (1997a) 1995 Annual Reports from Spe-
cialist Staff in Bilingual Programs in Northern Territory Schools. Darwin:
Government Printery Office of the Northern Territory.
Northern Territory Department of Education (1997b) Aboriginal Education News, Is-
sue 8, Aboriginal Languages in Schools. Aboriginal Education Policy Support Unit
on Northern Territory Indigenous Education Issues. Darwin: Northern Terri-
tory of Education.
OECD(1996) Measuring What People Know, Human Capital Accounting for the Knowl-
edge Economy. Paris: Organization for Economic Co-Operation and
Development.
OECD(1998) Human Capital Investment, An International Comparison. Paris: Organi-
zation for Economic Co-Operation and Development.
Ozolins, U. (1993) The Politics of Language in Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Sandefur, J. (1979) AnAustralianCreole inthe NorthernTerritory: ADescriptionof Ngu-
kurr-Bamyili Dialects (Part 1). SIL-AAB Series B, Volume 3. Darwin: Summer
Institute of Linguistics.
Shnukal, A. (1991) Broken: An Introduction to the Creole Language of Torres Strait. Can-
berra: ANU. (Pacific Linguistics C107).
Schmidt, A. (1990/1993) The Loss of Australias Aboriginal Language Heritage. Can-
berra: Aboriginal Studies Press.
Schiffman, H.F. (1996) Linguistic Culture and Language Planning. Routledge: London
and New York.
Williams, N. (1986) The Yolnguand their Land: ASystemof Land Tenure and the Fight for
its Recognition. Canberra: Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.
Yeatman, A. (1990) Bureaucrats, Technocrats, Femocrats: Essays on the Contemporary
Australian State. Sydney: Allen & Unwin.
422 The Pacific
Chapter 18
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand
19891999
R. BENTON and N. BENTON
When I say there is not, this does not necessarily mean a negation;
when I say there is, this also does not signify an affirmation. Turn East-
wardandlookat the WesternLand; face the southandthe NorthStar is
pointed out there! (Yung-chia, Song of Enlightenment, quoted in
Suzuki, 1959, 65)
The 1996 Census of PopulationandDwellings inNewZealandincluded
at least one amazing revelation: almost 22% of the population of Maori
ancestry could have a conversation about a lot of everyday things in
Maori, and about half of these were under the age of 25 (Statistics NZ,
1997b: 7980). Only a year before, a survey undertaken by Statistics New
Zealand(whoalsoconductedthe census) for the Maori Language Commis-
sion and the Ministry of Maori Development, indicated that less than 14%
of the Maori population over 16 years of age had medium, high, or very
high fluency in Maori, and that the median age of these speakers was
closer to 50 than to 25 (Te Puni Kokiri, 1998: 345). These latter figures
corresponded quite closely to what might have been expected based on
projections froma more extensive sociolinguistic surveyof Maori language
use undertaken in the 1970s (Benton, 1997).
On the surface, the census figures indicated a spectacular reversal of
language shift in the space of a year, and one for which formal institutions
couldpresumablytake muchof the credit, since the official summaryof the
1995 survey results noted that the marae [community gathering place for
ceremonies and important meetings], school and church were the places
where Maori was most commonly heard or spoken, and that although
educational institutions have become very strong Maori language
zones, for many children at kohanga [Maori-language pre-schools] and
kura [Maori-medium primary schools] these are the only places they ever
hear or speak Maori. (Te Taura Whiri, 1995). They are thus at once an affir-
mationof the durabilityof the quasi-miraculous mystique of the upturnin
423
The Pacific
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand
Maori language knowledge and use noted six or seven years before, in
Reversing Language Shift (RLS), while callingintoquestionthe central thesis
of that work, that such transformations can only be sustained when they
are anchored solidly in home and neighbourhood intergenerational
language transmission.
The account which will be presented here must lie somewhere within
the mystic triangle which has the 1995 survey, the 1996 census, and the
potentiallyobservable but still well-hiddenobjective realityof what people
really knowand do, as its defining points. It is based on personal observa-
tions, discussions with parents, kaumatua [respected elders], teachers,
children, scholars and people-in-the-street, along with the perusal of liter-
ally hundreds of reports and documents (one of the authors has, for
example, looked at every report on a Maori-medium educational institu-
tion produced by the Education Review Office from 1997 to the middle of
1999). But in the end, although we hope that it is located closer to the point
where the triangle touches the situation as it was (more or less!) at the
beginning of 1999, this exposition remains the personal viewof two people
who have beeninvolvedinsupporting andresearching the resurgence and
fortunes of the Maori language in New Zealand over the last four decades
of the 20th century.
Then and Now
The overviewof the situation of the Maori language in Chapter 8 of RLS
is a good starting point for a discussion of what has happened in the inter-
vening decade, which in turn can form the basis for reflecting on the
validity of the Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale (GIDS) as a
representation of the stages through which the process of reversing
language shift must pass on the way to complete and comprehensive
revitalisation.
We will first look at what the situation was then, and how it appears
now, in relation to each of the stages on the GIDS.
Stage 8
Stage 8 is that at which most vestigial users of Xish are socially isolated
old folks and Xish needs to be reassembled from their mouths and memo-
ries (RLS: 88). Although it would be an exaggeration to assert without
qualification that Maori as a living language had been disrupted to this
extent by 1989, in many parts of New Zealand, including many communi-
ties where most people regarded themselves as Maori, this was in fact the
case, and quite probably still is. For example, in 222 of the 275 North Island
424 The Pacific
geographical areas listed in Appendix 1 of the overview of the 197379
survey of Maori language use (Benton, 1991), fluency in Maori was limited
mainly to adults over the age of 45, and in 29 of these areas, understanding
of the language was similarly restricted, although there were only four
localities where the language had virtually disappeared. It is likely that a
surveyof the SouthIslandwouldhave yieldedanevenbleaker picture, and
almost certainly the South Island dialect of Maori was without native
speakers at that time.
The kohanga reo (Maori-medium pre-school) movement was designed
to help remedy this situation, by reassembling the language from the
mouths and memories of the grandparental generation, for transmission
to the very young while enabling the parental generation to learn along-
side their children if they had the time and inclination. Since the number of
pre-school childrenwho couldspeak Maori fluently in1979 was very small
indeed, almost certainly less than a hundred, this situation has changed
dramatically, even if the census figures of 10,263 Maori children under 5
capable of holding a conversation in the language (compared with 45,202
who could speak only English) are wildly exaggerated. Furthermore,
kohanga supervisors, parents, and teachers in Maori-mediumclasses have
put considerable effort into discovering ways of speaking Maori naturally
in everyday situations. Even taking Maori fromthe status of a language to
be taught to a language to be used in school, let alone at home, has required
these language activists to discover and reconstruct the ways in which the
language-in-culture might be transmitted naturally to a new generation
of speakers.
At the end of the 1980s, Maori was, at the macro-level, still teetering on
the brink of Stage 8; the pretence that it was far more secure, while justifi-
able as a fictionof the kindone of NewZealands most eminent jurists char-
acterisedapprovinglyas a device not toestablishthe truth, but tosubvert it
inthe interests of justice (Salmond1893: 85), was anexpressionof faithand
hope, rather than a dispassionate reading of the facts. By 1999, it is a few
centimetres further fromthe edge of the abyss, with an impressive array of
ropes and safety gear to help ensure that this distance is increased. At the
micro-level, there are undoubtedly many communities where the Stage 8
scenario is still a lived reality, with rescue and reconstruction yet to be
accomplished.
Stage 7
At Stage 7, most active users of the language are socially integrated and
ethnolinguistically active adults whose own children are grown up. This
was the general situation in many of the 222 worst-affected areas surveyed
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 425
in the 1970s. It was the widespread presence of older Maori-speakers in
rural communities and urban neighbourhoods which made possible the
spectacular growth of both the kohanga reo, andthe Ataarangi movement
1
(oriented towards the Maori language needs of whole families, and
working through homes rather than educational institutions), which pre-
dated the kohanga and continues to flourish. These two movements often
act in partnership at the local level, but the Ataarangi receives no govern-
ment funding and is therefore not subject to government control. The
revitalisation of Maori is still heavily dependent on the ethnolinguistically
active members of the grandparental generation to sustain it. However,
their ranks must be filledincreasingly by second-language learners, that is,
by members of the generation raised as English-speakers, for many of
whomMaori was a language occasionally heard in childhood, but seldom
understood. Their ownparents (or, for quite a number, their grandparents)
cooperate in this enterprise, helping to enhance and extend the Maori
language competence of a new parental generation.
Leaving the census figures to one side, casual observation would lead
one to conclude that, especially in localities served by strong Maori-
mediumeducational institutions, and among Maori teachers, tertiary level
students, and professionals, there are considerably more younger adults
who are actively using Maori in everyday situations, and especially in
bringing up their own children. But whether these people number
hundreds or thousands is very difficult to say. The official report on the
1995 National Maori Language Survey found that only 10% of Maori-
speakers under 45 spoke Maori at home to some extent every day,
compared with 56% who said that they never spoke Maori at home. An
impressive 11%of all Maori speakers, that is about 6.5%of Maori over the
age of 16 (whichwouldhave amountedto about 20,000 people if the survey
results were extrapolated to the entire Maori population) said that they
spoke Maori at home all the time (TPK [1998]: 33). Most of these were
highly fluent, andtherefore older, speakers, but the potential of this trend
for moving the disruption of the language situation away fromlevels 7 and
8 would, if confirmed and sustained, be considerable.
Stage 6
This is the critical stage in the RLS scheme of things, the stage which
must be consolidated and secured if the later stages are to be achieved on a
sustainable basis. It has three components: (1) the establishment of
informal communicative links between generations through the revital-
ised language; (2) the demographic concentration of this activity that is,
anchoring it within a community or neighbourhood; and (3) institutional
426 The Pacific
reinforcement of this natural use of the language. The 6.5% of the popula-
tion surveyed in 1995 who spoke Maori most of the time could be the
vanguard for the intergenerational transmission of the language. Of
course, one might speak Maori consistently without securing the coopera-
tion of the rest of the household. In the 1970s survey, 10.5% of fluent
speakers used Maori as the main language for talking with their children,
which mirrors the 1995 results. However, in only about 8% of homes was
Maori the main language used when the family was gathered for a meal.
(Benton, 1991: 10, 28) We do not know what the situation is in 1999, but it
would probably be safe to say that while the number of Maori-speaking
households may not have increased, there would be many more children
spoken to and speaking Maori regularly at home than there were in 1989.
However, it is clear that even on the most liberal reading of the census
figures, whichindicate that while 17%of Maori childrenunder the age of 15
can speak Maori, almost 70%speak only English (Statistics NZ, 1997b: 79),
that this is not yet the norm, and therefore, for the overwhelming majority
of Maori people, eventhe first component of Stage 6 has yet tobe secured.
The second aspect, the demographic concentration of Maori-speakers,
so that they could form a geographic as well as an affective community, is
still far frombeing realised, andmay be very difficult to achieve eveninthe
long term, unless language-in-culture can be meaningfully and accept-
ably separated from language-in-biologically-mediated-ethnicity. The
reasonfor this is that inverymanyimportant respects, while Maori descent
may not always be a sufficient condition for claiming Maori ethnicity in
Maori eyes, it is still inmanyimportant respects a necessaryone. The condi-
tionis genealogically rather thanracially based, but it nevertheless acts as a
very formidable barrier to the extension of group membership. It is one
whichfor some tribal groupings at least is appliedwithequal force toMaori
and non-Maori alike; for example, legally adopted children who do not
have a biological parent of appropriate ancestry cannot be registered as
beneficiaries of the Tainui Confederations Settlement with the Crown,
even if they are of (non-Tainui) Maori ancestry and were adopted at birth
by qualified foster-parents (personal communication from Tainui Benefi-
ciary Roll Officer, 1998).
Since the 1980s the Maori population has been overwhelmingly located
in urban areas, with more than 60%in the larger cities, and only 1314%in
small rural localities (Statistics NZ, 1997b: 24). Although in some of the
minor urban areas, and indeed in some suburbs of the cities, Maori may be
either ina majority, or at least livingclose enoughtoeachother toconstitute
a community which could influence the linguistic norms of the area, there
is no reason at the moment to imagine that such a radical move away from
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 427
English would happen on a significant scale. Even where there are institu-
tions to support such a development, there may be a notable lack of
consensus on the extent to which Maori should be used. For example, in a
section of a small town just south of Auckland, which includes an impor-
tant traditional Maori settlement and tribal centre, there are two state-
funded primary schools, located very close to each other. Each is attended
by over 300 children, mostly Maori, fromsimilar socio-economic andtribal
backgrounds. One has taught entirely through the medium of Maori for
more than 10 years, and the other through the medium of English with a
little Maori language taught on the side (although in May 1999 plans were
announced for possibly one bilingual class in 1999 or 2000). Both are
regarded as excellent schools with very well-qualified and highly
committedstaff (manyteachers at the English-mediumschool have qualifi-
cations for Maori immersion teaching). This situation underlines the fact
that whenMaori parents are givena choice, aneducationthroughMaori, in
order to enhance and reinforce the opportunity for their children to use the
language inother settings, is just one factor amongothers whichinfluence a
decision.
Withinsuchcommunities, some families will actively encourage the use
of Maori at home and in the neighbourhood, but others will remain basi-
cally English-speaking. There will be clusters of Maori-speaking house-
holds forming mini-neighbourhoods, but their members will also be
socialising regularly in English with other people living very close by. The
use of Maori may spread gradually out from these clusters, but there are
powerful counter-forces not just outside the gates, but within the home.
Meanwhile, the communities which do form (apart from a few isolated
rural areas where the language survived as the lingua franca through the
1980s), are likely to be networks of non-contiguous households whose
members have strong ideological commitments to revitalising the
language and who may indeed adopt compensatory measures like those
outlinedinRLS(p. 94) andusedalreadybythe Friulians andothers. InNew
Zealand, the Maori Language Commission has taken this point to heart,
and has started publishing a special bilingual newsletter to encourage and
assist families in this respect (Taura Whiri, 1998).
The third element which must be present for Stage 6 to be completed is
institutional reinforcement. Joshua Fishman is right in emphasising the
family, despite the erosion of its effectiveness in Euro-American society, as
a potential bulwarkof resistance tothe forces of homogenisation. RLSis not
politically neutral; it can be, and undoubtedly often is, when part of a
community or ethnically-basedmovement, an antidote to the effectiveness
of the propaganda of conformity (Ellul, 1973: 74). As Jacques Ellul points
428 The Pacific
out, it is primary groups such as the family which protect the individual
sociologically from becoming a mere cipher within a highly manipulable
mass society:
Inindividualist theory the individual has eminent value, manhimself is
the master of his life; in individual reality, each human being is subject
toinnumerable forces andinfluences, andis not at all master of his own
life. As long as solidly constituted groups exist, those who are inte-
grated into them are subject to them. But at the same time they are
protected by them against such external influences as propaganda.
(Ellul, 1973: 91)
Fishman sees the home-family-neighbourhood-community institutions
andprocesses as the heart andsoul of stage 6 (RLS: 95). He states categori-
cally that one cannot jump across or dispense with this stage (emphasis in
original), and adds that without an intimate and sheltered harbor at stage
6, an RLS movement tends towards peripheralisation and faces the
danger of prematurely tilting at dragons (the schools, the media, the
economy). Yet NewZealandschools, despite the ultimately external regu-
lation to which they are subjected, are in fact often key community and
neighbourhood institutions, subject to a fair degree of community influ-
ence and control.
Furthermore, from our observations, so many people, and especially
children, spend a significant portion of their day watching television
within their own homes, that this institution cannot be written out of the
Stage 6 equation. Although (and because) family members have control of
this institution only to the extent that they can refuse to watch its effusions,
the television set undoubtedly constitutes a powerful force for the sociali-
sation of all family members, and particularly the young. It occupies a
central place inthe lives of many families, occupying oftena number of key
locations within the home a central place within the family room, and
often individual bedrooms as well. It is still almost entirely English-
medium, and constitutes a powerful counter-force within the home to the
efficacy of Maori language revitalisation efforts. This medium unremit-
tingly whispers, shouts, and displays its message get with it; only
English really counts. Although its comparatively poor relations, radio
andthe print media, may have less immediate influence, there is a case also
for notingtheir effects at Stage 6, rather thanat Stage 2 (where we will rejoin
them).
Another neighbourhoodinstitutionis the local shop. Perhaps inthe long
runwe will see the emergence of small shopkeepers as a subset of the Maori
petty bourgeoisie. At present this role is undertaken mostly by immigrants
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 429
from the Indian subcontinent and Fiji, who are more likely to speak
Bengali, Gujarati, Hindi or Urdu than Maori. Like their predecessors of
various ethnicities in many rural areas in the days when everyone spoke
Maori, they would undoubtedly learn that language if knowing it would
bring themmore andbetter business. For the moment, there are fewneigh-
bourhood institutions and processes, apart from the kohanga reo and, in
some cases, schools, which most Maori-speaking families can rely on to
support their efforts effectively and consistently.
The Maori-medium elementary schools belong to Stage 4 of the GIDS
scale, andwill be discussedinrelationtothis. However, inthe discussionof
the New Zealand situation in RLS, the kohanga reo were seen as a
supporting institution in the context of Stage 6. Throughout the 1990s, just
under half the Maori children enrolled in pre-school facilities attended
kohanga reo, although the proportion had dropped from 49.2% (14,027
children) in 1993 to 43.5%(13,353 children) in 1997. However, Maori atten-
dance at pre-school institutions of any kind is much lower than for the
general population (around 45%, compared with nearly 70% for non-
Maori), so less than a quarter of the total number of pre-school children of
Maori ancestry will have experienced Maori-medium care of this kind
during the 1990s. Although kohanga reo are in theory entirely Maori-
medium, in practice the amount of Maori useddepends on the capabilities,
skill, and support received by the kaiako (teachers). Increasingly, these are
second-language learners of Maori, whose fluency in the language ranges
fromexcellent to minimal. It is possible therefore that some Maori children
catered for at other pre-school institutions (increasing, although still small,
numbers of which are also bilingual or Maori-medium) will have received
better exposure to everyday Maori than those at some kohanga. According
to the Ministry of Education (1998), by 1997 83.5% of Maori children and
61.5%of non-Maori received at least some Maori-language tuition at a pre-
school centre.
Most if not all of the caveats concerning the likely effectiveness of
kohanga reo expressed in Chapter 8 of RLS (see pp. 2389) remain valid in
1999. The potential of these institutions is still powerful, and many operate
effectively, achieving their goals of transmitting the language-in-culture
from one generation to another in a way that approximates the ideal.
Others (as might be expectedwithin a federation of 700800 disparate local
groups) fall far short of expectations. The centres no longer depend on the
Department of Maori Affairs (which was replaced by a much smaller
Ministry of Maori Development in the restructuring of government agen-
cies launchedat the endof the 1980s). They are nowgovernedby a separate
Trust, which receives substantial government funding through the
430 The Pacific
Ministryof Education, andeachkohanga operates under a charter fromthe
Minister of Education. While nominally free, community-basedinitiatives,
the kohanga are infact boundbya multitude of regulations enforcedbythe
Trust andvarious government agencies, andvery closely monitoredby the
Education Review Office.
In some rural areas, as noted in RLS (p. 239), kohanga (and other educa-
tional agencies) have become markedly antiquarian, emphasising or even
reassembling a local or regional dialect (in a way reminiscent of Stage 8),
and seeking out distinctively local traditions and ways of doing things.
This does complicate the taskof encouragingliteracyinMaori, but not to as
great a degree as might be imagined. Most teachers and parents are now
quite happy to have and used printed materials written in other dialects,
provided they can also get materials in their own as well. Many teachers,
even those using a de facto standard in urban, multi-dialectal settings, and
whether at kohanga or elementary school, make a lot of their own reading
materials anyway, pasting Maori text over English, or writing and illus-
trating their own books. Furthermore, the differences between the major
North Island dialects are very slight when compared with, say, English
dialects in England. It is the political corollary of the strong resurgence of
tribal chauvinism outside the metropolitan areas which is more serious
than the linguistic, as the State is the main beneficiary of such divisions
within Maoridom.
The flow-on effects from the kohanga to the families of the children
enrolled are varied. However, although it would be easy to find cases to
illustrate the lack of influence on language policy andpractice (in regardto
a shift from English) in the home, let alone the neighbourhood, the best
kohanga have been able to involve families in language learning and play
their intended role in revitalising both the language and the culture (cf.
Royal Tangaere, 1997a; Douglas, 1991). An excellent description and anal-
ysis of one familys experiences in this regard has been completed recently
by Margie Hohepa (1998), and another useful study has been published by
the NZCouncil for Educational Research(Royal Tangaere, 1997b). It is very
unlikely that the degree of parental involvement and relinguification
suggested by Fishman (RLS: 239) as a prerequisite for entry to the kohanga
reo could be enforced without decimating the institutions. In the past, the
Maori Language Commissioner had advocated this kind of drastic action,
preferring 80 properly organised kohanga to 800 disorganised ones. But
there are now too many vested interests at stake and furthermore the
danger of throwing the babies out with the bath water is too great to
contemplate such draconian measures now. Relinguification remains a
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 431
goal, but one which may be only a little less difficult to achieve within the
kohanga than outside it.
We will return to the significance of Stage 6 later in the chapter, in rela-
tion to Stage 2, and when we consider official policy pronouncements and
how some of these have been implemented, along with the role of the
language in symbolising and maintaining inter-group boundaries.
Stage 5
The fifth stage in the RLS exposition of the GIDS is characterised by the
extension of the oral communication which is the main focus of level 6, to
literacy activities within the home, school and community. The social and
political constraints mentionedinconnectionwiththis stage inRLS are real
and well-attested in many societies. The most important factors for the
New Zealand situation relate to Fishmans caution that this stage deals
with only part of the total education domain, a part that is entirely Xish,
primarily literacy focused, socialisation-related and entirely under intra-
communal control (RLS: 98). This stage constitutes, in the RLS scheme of
things, the roof to the first and most difficult phase of RLS. If and when
achieved, it will give speakers of the revitalised language independence
from the print media of the dominant one where informational, attitu-
dinal, ideological/philosophical and recreational communications are
concerned, enhancing the psychological bonds and cultural integration of
the revitalising community. Very clearly then, the literacy we are dealing
with here is not simply initial literacy development for children, but a
Freireancritical literacyfor the empowerment of the entire community. It is
in this latter aspect of Stage 5, vernacular literacy for adult empowerment,
that New Zealand may be weighed in the balance and found wanting.
The achievement of Stage 5 inrelationtoMaori language revitalisationis
complicatedbythe fact that most Maori adults are able toreadandwrite (in
English), althoughthe level of adult literacyingeneral causes some concern;
the governments LiteracyTaskForce (1999: 1011) notedclaims the 20 per
cent of children are failing in their literacy learning in New Zealand
schools, and that levels of performance among adults are lower among
Maori than for the general population. While some Maori nationalists and
RLS advocates have rejected literacy in Maori as an unwanted intrusion of
pakeha technology into the sacred core of Maori culture, in general Maori-
medium educators have vigorously promoted the acquisition of literacy
through Maori. The pioneers among them established their elementary
schools quite independently of the government, and some continue to
work through informal home-schooling networks in preference to
government-financedinstitutions. Furthermore, as is notedinRLS, histori-
432 The Pacific
cally literacy was acquired by Maori very soon after the first contact with
print, and for more than half a century there was a vigorous Maori-
language press witha wide readership, coveringall the topical issues of the
day, local and international.
However the availability of wide-ranging and topical printed matter in
Maori has beenminimal since the 1920s, for a variety of reasons, not least of
which has been compulsory education largely or entirely through the
mediumof English, with the accompanying propaganda relegating Maori
language to the status of an archaic survival of little consequence in
modern life (cf. Benton 1981). For adults, the problem is now fundamen-
tally not a lack of ability to read in Maori, but that there is an inadequate
quantity and variety of material to read. Throughout the 1990s a very
strong emphasis has been placed on the provision of reading materials for
younger children, and substantial progress have been made. However,
adults still have little tochoose from. Asuccessionof Maori-orientedmaga-
zines which have been produced since the 1960s, published alternatively
bythe former Department of Maori Affairs (Te Ao Hou, Te Kaea, TuTangata),
NZMaori Council (Te Kaunihera Maori), andprivate interests (most notably
Mana), have all been predominantly English-medium, with Maori-
language content declining rather than increasing in volume over the
years. The Maori local newspapers, which are far more modest in their
appearance, also cater most comprehensively for their primarily English-
speaking readers.
In 1998, Mana published an issue highlighting the current state of Maori
language revitalisation the set of articles was titledAdyinglanguage or
are the kids the cure?. The Maori language content was confinedtothe odd
sentence and heading, mostly in advertisements. The magazine is a very
high quality, full-colour production, and therefore expensive to print and
unable to devote costlyspace to material that wouldbe inaccessible to most
readers. It highlights the dilemma of the Maori press. Although there is
limited government financial support for Maori radio, print media have to
take their chance in the open market. To remain financially viable, they
have to cater to a largely English-speaking readership, even if most of their
readers are Maori. This applies also to creative writing and non-fiction,
except when it is targeted towards the school market and likely to succeed
there. Thus, except when writing for school children, Maori authors,
including Maori-speaking authors, are likely to write in English. There is a
much larger, often international, readership for these writers, along with
the possibility of gaining major recognition and prestigious awards (Keri
Hulmes The Bone People, which gained the Booker Prize, may never have
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 433
been noticed if it had been written in Maori). The very small domestic
Maori-language market (the English-language market is small enough!)
could not sustain an unsubsidised publishing programme. Authors also
have to compete with television, and possibly too the negative side-effects
of the notion that, in any case, Maori is not a language for reading. What is
available is not designed to stimulate the growth of a mass market for
Maori-language print mostly serious biographies, historical and tradi-
tional accounts, and pamphlets conscientiously produced by government
agencies warning of the dangers of smoking, or advising one howto enrol
as an elector. There is also a growing stack of Education Review Office
reports, curriculum statements and other official documents, frequently
writtenina language loadedwithtechnical neologisms whichrender them
unintelligible to many otherwise highly literate native speakers.
Stage 4
Stage 4 sees the incorporation of the revitalising language into formal
education at the elementary level, either in a partial way under outside
control (type 4b schools), or as the major or sole medium in schools which
also are controlled by the language community rather than by outside, Y-
ish, authorities (type 4a). Although this dichotomy is convenient, it is hard
to apply in New Zealand, where all schools, official disclaimers notwith-
standing, are very firmly under state control, and where several genera-
tions of primary socialisation through the formof the English language (the
semantic content hiding behind the formmay often have been Maori) have
made it possible for Maori to transforman institution, retaining English as
its workinglanguage while operatingwithinpalpablyMaori frameworkin
almost every other respect. Many people do this at home every day, and
they can do it equally effectively and naturally at school (given the official
constraints that affect everyone).
There has been a very substantial growth in type 4a schools in the 1990s.
According to the Ministry of Educations Statistical Profile for 1998, there
were 27,181 Maori children and 4,886 non-Maori receiving Maori-medium
education in 1997. The comparable figures for 1992 were 16,501 and 1375
respectively. These programmes were spreadover a total of 472 schools, up
from318 in 1992. By 1997 there were 54 Kura Kaupapa Maori (state schools
teaching almost solely through Maori and run according to Maori cultural
norms), 11 other immersion schools, 86 bilingual schools, 115 immersion
classes, and 220 bilingual classes. The points of growth had been in the
bilingual schools and kura kaupapa Maori which had numbered 20 and
8 (mostly private) schools respectively in 1989. However, while the defini-
tion of immersion education was fairly strict (at least 20 hours a week, i.e.
434 The Pacific
80% of the schools teaching hours), bilingual education included every-
thingbetween3 and19 hours instructionthroughMaori. By1997, 9654 chil-
dren were receiving immersion education (as defined above), just over
twice as many as in 1992, and 10,405 were receiving between 12 and 30%,
more than treble the 1992 figure. The middle group (just over 12,000 chil-
dren) receiving 3080%of their education through Maori had grown more
slowly (from just under 10,000 five years before). Overall, this meant that
about 19%of Maori childrenwere receivinga part (sometimes quite a small
part) of their education through Maori by 1997, as were just under 1% of
non-Maori. In addition, over 55,000 children (25,528 Maori and29,871 non-
Maori) were studying Maori language as a subject in elementary school for
a minimumof three hours a week. This meant that just over a thirdof Maori
childrenwere spendingthree hours or more a weeklearninginor about the
Maori language by 1997. It should be noted that there were more children
engaged in learning through Maori in 1996, and the 1998 figures had not
been released when this chapter was written, so although there is a strong
overall upward movement between 1992 and 1997, a plateau could also
have been reached. The Ministry of Educations report (1998) makes this
comment:
There was a 2% decrease in the numbers of schools offering Maori
medium education, and a 4% decrease in the number of student
enrolments between 1996 and 1997. However, there was an 11%
increase in the numbers of students enrolled in schools with a total or
near total immersion in Maori over the same period. The more favor-
able funding policy for high levels of immersion was likely to have
been a causal factor here.
Mention has been made of Kura Kaupapa Maori, the Maori agenda
schools referred to also in RLS. The founders of the original Kura Kaupapa
Maori formed themselves into a very powerful lobby group in 1989, the
year when a major restructuring of the education system took place, and
secured statutory recognition for their then private or unofficial schools as
a special category of state school. The government shortly afterwards
agreed to allow five new Kura to be established each year, either as new
schools, or through the re-designation of existing schools. One of the tenets
of the movement was adherence to a philosophical statement known as Te
Aho Matua (literally the first thread, i.e. the statement of fundamental
principles underlying Maori thought and action). By the mid-1990s, the
Ministry of Education had agreed to consult the Runanga (council) to
which the majority of the Kura belonged before approving new ones, and
also included acceptance of Te Aho Matua as a pre-condition for being
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 435
given state recognition. This move was disputed by some schools and
communities, and indeed one of the first and largest of the Kura refused to
formally subscribe to the statement and left the Rnanga. In 1998, at the
request of the Runanga, a Bill to amendthe EducationAct was presentedto
the House of Representatives seeking among other things to amend the
principal Act by inserting a requirement that to be designated a Kura
Kaupapa Maori a school must be one that operates in accordance with Te
Aho Matua (as defined in section 155A), and specifying that:
Te Aho Matua is a statement that sets out an approach to teaching and
learning that applies to Kura Kaupapa Maori (s. 155A (1))
Te Kaitiaki o Te Aho Matua [the Guardian of Te Aho Matua] is the
body identified by the Minister of Maori Affairs as being the most suit-
able to be responsible for determining the content of Te Aho Matua,
and for ensuring that it is not changed to the detriment of Maori.
(s.155B (1))
The Bill received enthusiastic endorsement from many politicians,
convinced that this regulation was what Maori wanted, but at the Select
Committee stage they learned that it was not what all Maori wanted. It
remains to be seen whether what were originally independent schools will
come under the ideological control of an organising group chosen by the
state to represent and enforce a legislatively defined Maori world view.
Disputes about Te Aho Matua aside, type 4a education is much better
established than it was in 1989, although very far from universal and with
as yet unknown effects on supporting or being a catalyst for the consolida-
tion of Stage 6. Again, as with so much in Maori education and language
policy in New Zealand, form is one thing, content may be something else.
Many Kura Kaupapa Maori are small (under 50, sometimes less than 20
students), ill-equipped, lacking stable staffing, unable to recruit trained
teachers, adversely affected by internal disputes, and sometimes without
teachers who have sufficient knowledge of Maori to teach effectively
through the language. Even when the staff are competent and the curric-
ulum is well-handled, there is always the possibility that the strain of
having to receive all their schooling through a second language which is
not well-supported outside the school will eventually produce a negative
reactionamongchildren. Parents sometimes report that their childrenhave
become fed up with Maori after a few years of immersion education and
have had to be transferred to an English-medium school; teachers at one
Kura Kaupapa Maori reported to us that they had noticed Maori was used
much less in the playground once the school changed froma bilingual to a
total immersion programme. They put this down to the fact that the chil-
436 The Pacific
dren had to have a release once they got out of the classroom because not
being allowed to use English was so stressful and difficult for many of
them.
There has been a rapid expansion in the number of type 4b schools and
programmes, some of which are substantial enough to constitute genuine
dual medium approaches, mid-way between types 4a and 4b, but others
would be token programmes linguistically, although some of the latter
would be palpably Maori in most other respects. In RLS, it was noted that
all too often this taha Maori approach lacks real life, real results, real soci-
etal input when measured from the point of view of the urgent RLS needs
of a severely weakened language and culture (p. 243). Grin and
Vaillancourt in their report to the NZ Treasury on language revitalisation
policy suggested that the apparent waste of resources on type 4b schooling
might be addressed by:
in the short run, giving preference for enrolment in Maori-medium
education to Maori and increasing their supply by shifting teachers to
these schools. This may require temporarily reducing the teaching of
Maori to Pakeha to zero. (1998: 215, emphasis in original)
However, teachers cannot be movedaroundlike pawns ona chess boardin
a democratic country, and even if they could, simply having a Maori
teacher, even a Maori-speaking teacher in front of a class is not going to
guarantee either excellence in Maori-medium education, nor acceptance
by the community (the Boards of Trustees of some community schools
would still prefer an unqualified teacher from their own tribal group to a
better-qualified outsider). Furthermore, despite their short-term useless-
ness in terms of ensuring a language shift of the kind necessary for Stage 6
success, many of these programmes have had a real and lasting effect on
getting Maori language accepted as something normal, interesting, and
even learning well when the opportunity arises. Of course, they may also
lull people into a sense of complacency, and are very useful to the Ministry
of Educationas aneasywayof boostingthe Maori language learningstatis-
tics. The real impact in support of RLS has undoubtedly been made by the
best Kura Kaupapa Maori (official and otherwise) and dual medium
programmes. But in relation to immersion programmes generally, it is
asking too much of many parents to choose small, ill-equipped schools
staffedbyuntrainedand/or a successionof short-termteachers, evenif it is
type 4b, when a type 4a or even a well-run English-medium school is just
around the corner. That many parents still do choose an immersion
programme insuchcircumstances is a tribute totheir stubbornresolve, and
an indication of the desperate state of the revitalisation process.
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 437
Joshua Fishman commented that as long as Maori parents themselves
are neither Maori-speaking nor RLS-conscious there is no certainty that
they will prefer type 4a schools to the type 4b schools whichare muchmore
plentiful and much less Maori-focused (RLS: 241). In fact, Maori parents
when asked to choose in the abstract often prefer Maori-focused dual-
mediumschools. Verymanyparents are RLSconscious, but, withaneye on
their childrens future, and knowing what has happened to them as a
result of an inadequate education, would still opt for English-medium
education if monolingual schooling in Maori were the only alternative.
Furthermore, there is at present no guarantee that even type 4a schools can
escape sufficiently from the Y-controlled regulatory framework to
transmit the language-in-culture to the satisfaction of those who have
been raised in more traditional backgrounds. One member of the Maori
Education Commission notes that Most people learning the Maori
language in the schools speak the same type of language, no matter
whether they are non-Maori or Maori. The ethos of the culture is being
lost (Pere, 1999: 9).
Maori focus within an English linguistic milieu, in the formation of
Xmen through Y-ish, is the life experience of many Maori. In the 1995
Maori language survey, only 13% overall agreed with the statement that
you have to speak Maori to be a real Maori (with practically none in the
under25 age group, and little difference between speakers and non-
speakers), while 82% disagreed (TPK, 1998: 579). Something more even
than the language-in-culture will be needed to convince the mass of
people that the effort to become fluent in Maori is worth making by
sending their children to Maori-mediumschools, many are hoping that for
the next generation making such a decision will be easier.
Stage 3
For Maori, the discussion of Stages 3, 2 and 1 in RLS was combined,
because they generally constitute no more than gleams in the eyes of a few
stubborn idealists rather than any substantial reality or genuine RLS
opportunity (p. 243). We will separate them in this account because the
gleams are stronger and the RLS opportunities vary in potentiality from
one stage to another.
Stage 3 covers the lower work sphere involving inter-communal trans-
actions, andis still almost entirely anEnglishdomain. This is almost as true
of the intra-Maori work sphere (which is part of Stage 6 andwill be alluded
to againbelow) as it is of the mixedone (inwhichmost employedMaori are
located). The 1995 surveyindicatedthat while the majority(59%) of the 44%
of Maori-speakers who were in paid employment spoke Maori occasion-
438 The Pacific
ally in that domain, only 11% spoke Maori most or al1 of the time, with
another 11%speakingMaori about half the time (TPK, 1998: 51). Opportu-
nities to use Maori at workthus appear to be verysimilar overall nowto the
situationinthe 1970s, when57%of fluent speakers reportedusing Maori at
least occasionally to fellow-workers, and 62% when talking with clients.
This holding the line probably represents a net gain since the publication
of RLS, but the toehold remains precarious.
It remains the case that the Maori Trust Boards and land incorporations
have done little or nothing to create Maori-speaking employment opportu-
nities for the Maori masses. The mass employment opportunities which
have been created have been mainly in areas such as hotels and other
tourism-related businesses, and, on a smaller scale, in fisheries, forestry
and farming. Since a majority of the beneficiaries of these trusts do not
speak Maori, it would be difficult for them to discriminate in favour of
Maori-speakers where this is not clearly justified by the work involved. In
any case, the investment policy of the wealthier trusts generally follows
that enunciated by the Waikato Raupatu Lands Trust:
The subsidiary companies were set up to separate the governance
functions from commercial activities. The subsidiaries role is to make
money.
Funds made available by the corporates to date are coming back to
tribal members through marae, education, sports and cultural grants.
(TMTB, 1998: 8, emphasis added)
The grants do include support for kohanga reo, but RLS is not a toppriority
for this or anyother Maori corporate giant. Interventions at the compulsory
education level are not generally supported financially by these tribal
agencies, however, because it is felt very strongly that this is a state respon-
sibility, which under the Treaty of Waitangi includes nurturing the Maori
language, and tribal resources should not be used to subsidise state func-
tions. If it could be shown that RLS would increase the tribes monetary
wealth, the attitude would be different. So far, the evidence for this is
lacking.
We have not heard of anyone being sacked in recent years for speaking
Maori at work, and there are locations (e.g. for service workers in some
universities, or road gangs) where speaking Maori at work would be
normal, or evenencouraged, so this aspect of safety at work is not particu-
larly problematic. However, as only a minority even of Maori workers and
customers are capable of sustaining a conversation in Maori, the balance is
still tilted towards English in the lower work sphere as much in 1999 as it
was in 1989.
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 439
Stage 2
Stage 2 embraces language inthe lower governmental services andmass
media local, community based, influenced and influencing but
excludes the higher spheres of either. Maori was supported better at this
stage than at Stage 3 in 1989, and further advances appear to have been
made in the decade following. By the end of 1998, for example, the Cabinet
hadapprovedinprinciple a set of guidelines toassist public service depart-
ments to implement the governments Maori language policies, plans and
practices. These included an admonition to departments to ensure that all
staff had a basic knowledge of Maori. An example of what might be
regarded as basic would, however, allay the anxiety of anyone worried
about the possibility of a massive shift to Maori as the lingua franca of the
public service: all staff will be able to pronounce Maori place names and
other words correctly, will knowthe sound values for the alphabet used in
Maori and will know and be able to use as appropriate simple greetings.
There is a lot of attention directed towards form (bilingual signage and
stationery and so on, but there are also provisions for creating special posi-
tions which require communicative abilities in Maori, and for additional
remuneration for appointees with this skill. Furthermore, many govern-
ment offices have made Maori language tuition above the basic level
available to their employees, and, possibly better focused towards RLS at
Stage 6, Maori is taught and often enthusiastically learned by inmates in
many prisons.
In fact, in the public sector, job opportunities for Maori-speakers at this
level are good, and have increased rapidly throughout the decade. Ability
to speak Maori to a high level is often listed as a desirable quality for
government employees, even when it is not essential. However, there is no
guarantee that holders of such positions will have the opportunity to use
the language regularly for ordinary communicative purposes. Exceptions
would be some social workers, and Education ReviewOffice officials who
are responsible for auditingkohanga reoandimmersionprogrammes not
only do they have to deal with Maori-speaking school staff members, but
their official reports are written in Maori, with only a brief summary
appended in English. However, the continued closure, sale, downsizing or
corporatisation of state agencies means that the scope of directives of this
kind is increasingly limited in range, and the overall impact on RLS has yet
to be ascertained.
Inbroadcasting, the potential to make a positive impact onRLS has been
greater, both in relation to the broadcasters themselves (who, if broad-
casting through Maori, have enhanced opportunities both for interaction
440 The Pacific
with other speakers and for developing their personal communicative
competence and confidence), and to their audience of mature and devel-
oping speakers. The area of consolidation has been radio broadcasting,
where government funding has enabled 22 local (mostly tribally based)
radio stations to eke out a fairly miserable existence. The amount of state
funding is dependent on the number of hours devoted to Maori-language
broadcasting per week, but it is insufficient to meet the costs involved
most stations rely on volunteer staff and fund-raising in order to survive.
This latter feature does make them more like neighbourhood associations
of the Stage 6 type. They do support RLS in a tangible manner, by commu-
nicating with their grassroots listeners in local talk shows, broadcasting
world and local news in a manner reminiscent of the Maori newspapers of
the turn of the previous century. They also supply a Maori-speaking work
environment straddling Stages 3 and2. Furthermore, they contribute to the
natural development of a communicative standard Maori, by providing
opportunities through networking for speakers of various dialects to
communicate regularly and meaningfully with each other.
Apart from a disastrous experiment with a channel in Auckland in
199697, which produced some very good programmes for a very small
fraction of the possible audience (it was allocated a frequency outside the
range of most television sets), Maori is seldom heard on local, let alone
national, television. We have argued here and elsewhere (see above, also
Benton, 1991) that Maori-language television is essential to the survival of
the language while television retains such a significant place in family life. If
people would turn off their television sets, the channel would not be
needed. Be that as it may, television in New Zealand promotes language
shift away fromMaori, rather than helping to reverse it. In the print media
at the local level, there is also little to promote RLS. Some local papers have
a paragraph or two of editorial in Maori, and a few locally-based Maori
papers have more, but nothing remotely comparable to what is available
through English for the same potential readership.
On balance, one could say that the changes at Stage 2 appearing to
support RLS have been largely cosmetic, although local Maori radio
stations have almost certainly fed back positively into what RLS develop-
ments there may have been at Stage 6.
Stage 1
Stage 1 inRLS, the prelude tosuccess if built onsolidfoundations at each
of the lower levels, is characterised by some use of the language in the
higher educational level, and higher government, occupational and media
efforts. Maori has made some inroads at this level. For example, at the
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 441
University of Waikato it is possible to obtain a Bachelors degree entirely
through the medium of Maori (several dozen students have graduated
through this programme over the last fewyears), and to write a Masters or
Doctoral thesis inMaori (again, these options have beenandare beingexer-
cised). Furthermore, students in any course are permitted to write essays
and examination answers in Maori (which the university translates into
English if a suitably qualified Maori-speaking examiner is not available).
Many subjects in the Maori Department are available only through the
mediumof Maori. However, in an effort to attract more students to bolster
a falling roll, this department has recently decided to offer English-
medium versions of some of these courses.
In the higher levels of government, Maori has ceremonial functions, but
this does not keep large numbers of Maori-speakers employed or speaking
Maori. At least one influential Maori organisation (the Waikato Raupatu
Lands Trust) plans to require all its employees to be fluent Maori-speakers,
andhas institutedcompulsory Maori-language classes to achieve this goal.
However, such a policy, like that of government agencies employing
Maori-speakers, may not necessarily be followedby RLS at home. National
television has miniscule amounts of Maori-medium transmission on only
one channel. National public radio has been broadcasting news and
commentaries in Maori since World War II, in recent years several times a
day at peak listening periods. However, in May 1999 the government
fundingagencydecidedthat these broadcasts were not cost-effective, and
that they should cease from 1 July. The Chief Executive of the funding
agency, commenting on the decision, commented that There are probably
better ways by which RNZcan contribute to the revitalisation of the Maori
language, such as programmes in English that create positive and accurate
beliefs about the Maori language, andprofiling Maori language events and
music (The Dominion 12 May 1999, p. 9). The normalisation function
which they fulfilled seems to have been discounted completely.
All in all, with the partial exceptions notedabove, the situation in 1999 is
still marked mostly by symbolic flourishes (RLS: 245) with one of the
most effective of these, the dailyMaori language current affairs bulletins on
national radio, about to disappear.
Stage 6 Revisited
The importance of this Stage 6 is well recognised by people working for
the revitalisation of the Maori language in New Zealand, and the ideas
expressed by Joshua Fishman in RLS, and especially on the importance of
home and neighbourhood language use, have been studied and quoted
442 The Pacific
extensively by scholars, students, activists, politicians and government
officials. Studying and quoting is one thing, however, and putting ideas
into practice quite another. When asked in November 1998 by an opposi-
tion member:
given that the home and community are arguably the essential
domains for Maori language revitalisation and maintenance, what are
his current specific educational plans to address the development of
Maori language within the home and community? (Hansard, 1998:
3282)
the Minister of Maori Affairs initially replied that the home and commu-
nity were only one vital element in revitalisation, the other being
teaching of Maori language in all our Primary Schools. Pushed further on
this issue with additional questions in February 1999, the Minister finally
announced that:
Throughthe Maori Language strategy, andits Maori Language Educa-
tion Plan, Government will be seeking to encourage Maori adults to
learn Maori andto use it with their children of all ages. In addition, it is
anticipatedthat Maori will developtheir ownMaori language learning
and Maori language use schemes. (Hansard, 1999: 51)
Although the Maori Language Education Plan has yet to be revealed, the
government does have defined policy objectives (Te Puni Kokiri, 1998: 65),
and a strategy which only obliquely or inferentially touches on supporting
the use of the language in the home andneighbourhood. The official Maori
language objectives, paraphrased, are:
(1) to increase opportunities to learn Maori;
(2) to improve proficiency of people in speaking, reading, writing, and
listening to Maori;
(3) increasing the number of situations where Maori can be used;
(4) increase the rate at which the language is modernised;
(5) to raise the prestige and appreciation of Maori language and
MaoriEnglish bilingualism among all New Zealanders.
The strategy through which they are to be implemented also has five
points:
(1) the development and implementation of a Maori language plan;
(2) the fostering of Maori language radio and television;
(3) the supplyof Maori language guidelines togovernment departments;
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 443
(4) the development and implementation of Maori language corpus
activities;
(5) coordinatedmonitoringandevaluationof Maori language activities.
This strategy once more seems to concentrate on the frills, which look good
and are easily counted, than on the substance which can only be achieved
through hard work with and by communities. San de Frine, in his
perceptive analysis of the Irish situation, stresses that the channels of
communication which sustain the life of a language and culture within a
community need to be embodied in permanent institutions and customs
whose role is thus primarily functional (1965: 87, emphasis added). In this
context, the remarks in RLS about the lack of neighbourhood economic
bases for Maori (p. 237) are very important. Some attention does seemto be
giventothe extra-linguistic goals of this aspect of communitydevelopment
by at least one of the very large tribal confederations (Ngai Tahu), which
has also sponsored concentrated language-immersion courses for adults.
Evenmore workalongthese lines has beendone bythe muchsmaller Ngti
Raukawa tribe at Otaki, near Wellington, which has also set up its own
higher educational institution, and supported grass-roots radio stations
andother ventures whichhave hadlanguage andcultural objectives linked
to the economic ones. The general trend, however, has been to join the
global economy and put financial dividends and the development of
managerial and entrepreneurial skills first (while assuming that necessary
or desirable linguistic and cultural investments can be supported through
these later).
The Basques are Euskaldunak speakers of the Basque language
(Saizarbitoria, 1985: 18); Maori on the other hand are Tangata whenua,
people of the land; the ideology of language has come to the fore after the
loss of both, but it is perhaps the land which is more important in defining
identity. Thus, to succeed, a strategy aimed at restoring language in
culture may also have to include language with the land. This taking
account of fundamentals, which does not need to degenerate into funda-
mentalism or atavism, becomes even more important with the govern-
ments plan to emphasise corpus planning to modernise the language.
Sucha policycouldwell leadtoMaoris becominga calque of English, if the
puristic policy followed since its inception by the Maori Language
Commission is continued. This already has had a potentially disruptive
effect on Stage 6 efforts by unnecessarily widening the gap between the
official language taught through the schools and the language familiar to
older native speakers. The general approach taken by Maori since the first
contact with other languages is to appropriate the foreign word (with suit-
444 The Pacific
able phonological modifications) along with the new object or concept
(thus miere, from French miel, honey, and so on). This has enabled the
semantic integrity of indigenous Maori vocabulary to be maintained.
However, the Commission has brought a Trojan horse into the semantic
citadel, and in the name of protecting the purity of the language in effect
hastened its colonisation. This point is well illustrated in the concluding
sentences of one of the contributions to the Mana feature on the state of
Maori:
I still tend to use words like Hanuere for January rather than Kohitatea
when I write letters, because its more familiar to me
Im not denying the place of these new words, but its a form of Maori
that Im not comfortable with. I suppose Ill get used to it and start
saying them.
When we lose our old people who are native speakers, this form of
Maori language will eventually be usedmore widely. This is the Maori
language of the days ahead. (Kingi 1998: 36)
His discomfort is well-founded. Kohitatea is the eighth month in the Maori
lunar calendar (which begins the yearly cycle with the riding of the
Pleiades around the end of June), and thus (traditionally) partly coincides
with January. Until the Maori Language Commission decided to replace
the borrowed words used to denote the Greco-Roman solar months for
the previous 150 years, there was a clear conceptual difference embedded
in the different lexical representations for these notions. Anewgeneration
of Maori will have to learn when Kohitatea is not Kohitatea, something
their elders did not need to worry about at all.
Assessing GID
The developments in New Zealand relating to the various levels of the
GIDS for Maori illustrate, we believe, both the general usefulness of the
scale as a framework for assessing the degree of disruption, along with the
multiple interconnectedness of the stages through both overlapping and
feedback loops.
The analogy with the Richter Scale (RLS: 87) is interesting and could be
developed further. The most violent earthquake at Stage 8 could have an
amplitude 400,000,000 times greater than the smallest recordable tremor;
the logarithmic progressionof the stages, not at all inappropriately applied
to the disruption that affects language transmission in cases like those
discussed in RLS, is something which may easily be overlooked by readers
with an arithmetical frame of mind. Thus the disruption which has
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 445
occurredwhenStage 8 has beenreachedis almost infinitelyworse thanthat
which obtains generally when Stages 1 and 2 are all that need attention.
However, just as the damage done by earthquakes varies according to
distance from the epicentre and the nature of the terrain, the GIDS to be
really useful must be able to take into account measurements of damage
across the countryside, so to speak, rather than be linked too closely to
either the least or worst-affected regions.
In the computation of the Total GIDS Scores in RLS (405, table 14.4) a
weighting has been allocated according to the stage at which intervention
is necessary and proceeding, with Stage 6 and stages higher than those
receiving attention seemingly ignored, giving, in the Maori case, a much
gentler estimate of the extent of the damage to be repaired. There will, of
course, be a large element of arbitrariness in any measurement of this kind,
with much depending on the viewpoint, perceptiveness, and priorities of
the observer. It is axiomatic in RLS, and indeed well supported by empir-
ical evidence, that Stage 6 is the critical one; that the process of RLS (or
enabling language regeneration, of which we will say a little more below)
cannot proceed with the hope of ultimate success until this stage has been
achieved. Indeed, inthe economic analysis completedfor the NewZealand
government by Grin and Vaillancourt, seven status of success conditions
were identified. Of these, Avant-garde (political agitation and conscious-
ness), and Technical effectiveness were, in their judgement, well met; Redis-
tribution (availability of government funding) and Normalcy (public accep-
tance) partly met; Shadowprice (the comparative cost of using Maori rather
than English) and Strict preference (the degree of commitment to use the
language whenever possible) mostly not met; and Individual preference
hardlyevenattempted. This analysis bothsupports the contentionthat Stage
6 factors are the most critical, and confirms the negative rating for Maori
language revitalisation at this stage in RLS. On almost any reading of the
scale, Maori would still be in the zone of maximum disruption.
2
The Vitality of Hope
Anyreadingof the situationinNewZealand, at the beginningor the end
of the decade that sawthe publication of RLS, is gloomy, but the fact that a
fewsurvivors seemtohave beenpulledfromthe rubble shouldat least give
cause for hope. The overall assessment of one of New Zealands pioneer
Maori-speaking broadcasters, writing in the issue of Mana devoted to the
language situation, confirms our reading of the GIDS. He offers scant
comfort for the complacent, or for those who think that the icing cansubsti-
tute for the cake:
446 The Pacific
And now [the Minister of Broadcasting has] come up with his
cunning plan. A government-appointed trust to run a television UHF
channel that will broadcast a few hours a day of Maori language
programs.
.
Maori ought to think twice on this one, though. If they go along with
[the Ministers] plan, and it turns out not to be so cunning after all,
theyre putting more nails in the coffin of the Maori language.
As our cover story indicates, its on its death bed. (Fox, 1998)
One Maori scholar, writing about the struggles of a group of Maori parents
to meet the economists strict preference and individual preference
conditions, while striving to build a Stage 6 environment for their fami-
lies, points out that all living languages, whether endangered or not, are
constantly re-created by those who speak them. She suggests that rather
than working to reverse language shift, with the implication that we are
travelling back to something old and by-passed, or even of revitalisation,
with its implication of emulating the work of Dr Frankenstein (our meta-
phor, not hers!), we talk instead of facilitating language regeneration
(Hohepa, 1998: 46). Such a renewal is constant and forward-looking. It is
deeply rooted in the past through cultural and historical bonds which go
far beyond mere sentimental attachment, providing both a secure anchor-
age in the present, and the sustenance and impetus for growth and devel-
opment inthe future. Maori-speakingfamilies are thus heirs of the past, but
not its prisoners.
The road towards RLS/revitalisation/regeneration for Maori winds its
way through poorly signposted and almost certainly dangerous territory.
The reasons why some may try to continue to travel this road are well
rehearsed in RLS and need not be repeated here. But these reasons are ill-
understood, and may not convince the average citizen. The views
expressed in a letter to the editor of the Waikato Times (in response of an
attempt by a local High School principal to compel all students claiming
Maori ethnicity to study Maori language) are probably representative of a
significant portion of the New Zealand mainstream. It was published
under the heading Wheres the link?
Responsibility of education is to prepare children for what they will
experience ina real modernworld. For the life of me, I cannot see where
a Maori language fits into that arena. However I have no problemwith
childrenonce theyhave grownup, or anyone choosingtolearna Maori
means of verbal communication. However the user and not the
taxpayer should pay.
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 447
There is nothing about Maori culture which cant be explained
adequately in any other language. To suggest, as Mr Elliott of Fraser
High School does, that Maori language is the key to Maori culture, is
arrogant nonsense and a typical example of the brain washing tactics,
some would call subversion, being used to dupe the taxpayers of this
country and to milk us to the tune of billions of dollars each year to
implement and support an abhorrent apartheid regime.
Feelings, thoughts, actions, events and material things, all relate to
culture. Language (a completely separate issue) does nothing else but
communicates the different perceptions of culture. Language certainly
doesnt create or even influence culture. I challenge Mr Elliott or
anyone to prove and give examples of a clear link between any culture
and any language. (Gordge, 1998)
Givenwhat we know, andthe comparativelysmall gains for Maori, despite
the hard work and sacrifice of many people in the closing decades of the
20th century, is it reasonable to continue to strive towards the regeneration
of the language? George Lakoff has given us a definition of reason:
Reason is the mechanical manipulation of abstract symbols which are
meaningless inthemselves, but canbe givenmeaning by virtue of their
capacity to refer to things either inthe actual worldor inpossible states
of the world. (1987: 7)
And Miguel de Unamuno long before provided a response:
For living is one thing, andknowing is another; andperhaps there is
such an opposition between the two that we may say that everything
vital is anti-rational, not merely irrational, andthat everything rational
is anti-vital. And this is the basis of the tragic sense of life. (Unamuno,
1954: 34)
It is also the hope for the future, the difference between being human and
being a machine. RLS has to transcend mere rationality, lest rationality
itself be transformed into an oppressive instrument for ensuring predict-
ability and conformity.
Notes
1. The Ataarangi movement grew out of an initiative taken by a Maori writer,
Katerina Mataira, andthe late Ngoi Pewhairangi, supportedby the NZCouncil
for Adult Education, totrainvolunteers inanapproachtolanguage teachingin-
fluencedby the work of Caleb Gaetano. The volunteers workedwithfamilies to
bring the Maori language competence of the group up to a level from which it
448 The Pacific
could be used communicatively within the family and further developed inde-
pendently. The movement was well-established by the end of the 1970s, and
continues to support language revitalization initiatives through volunteer
work and through courses organized in partnership with some educational in-
stitutions. Apart froma small seeding grant in the early stages of its operation,
Te Ataarangi has received no funding from NZ government agencies and has
remained a purely grassroots organization.
2. The overall placement of a language on the GID Scale could well be calculated
by assessing on a percentage basis at each level the degree to which the status
applied to the (potential) linguistic community as a whole, with a weighting
then applied to calculate the significance of this disruption. The weighting
could follow that implied in the RLS computations (i.e. level 8 counts as 8
points, level 1 as 1), and then the overall disruption summarized by the match
betweenthe assessedscore andthe range of scores obtainedbyaddingthe max-
ima for each level. So a total score of 31 on the revised1989 scale (which is about
where Maori would have been) becomes equivalent overall to Stage 8, in other
words, veryserious disruptionindeed, despite a fewticks insome of the higher-
level boxes. Such an exercise might do much to jolt out of their complacency
those who think they are working for RLS, but assume a top-down approach is
sufficient. To underline this still further, the weightings could be determined
through the accumulation of Fibonacci numbers, which would give the com-
plete achievement of Stages 8, 7 and6 a possible 78%influence onthe total score,
as against 58% on the current RLS scale. Under such a system, Maori, alas,
wouldstill be locatedat Stage 8 in1999 (as against possiblyscrapingintoStage 7
on the alternative suggested), although a movement towards a higher level of
security between 1989 and 1999 would be discernible.
References
Benton, R.A. (1981) The Flight of the Amokura: Oceanic Languages and Formal Education
in the South Pacific. Wellington: NZCER.
Benton, R.A. (1991) Notes on the case for Maori language television. New Language
Planning Newsletter 5(4), 14.
Benton, R.A. (1997) The Maori Language: Dying or Reviving? Wellington: New Zea-
land Council for Educational Research.
De Frine, S. (1965) The Great Silence. Dublin: Foilseachin Nisinta Teoranta.
Douglas, E.TeK. (1991) Maori language nests (Kohanga Reo) Their impact onNew
Zealand communities. Journal of Indigenous Studies 3(1), 1331.
Ellul, J. (1973) Propaganda: The Formation of Mens Attitudes. New York: Vintage
Books.
Fox, D.T. (1998) A cunning plan. Mana 22(4).
Grin, F. and Vaillancourt, F. (1998) Language revitalisation policy: An analytical
survey theoretical framework, policy experience and application to Te Reo
Maori. (Report to The Treasury, Wellington, New Zealand/Aotearoa; released
under the Official Education Act).
Gordge, M. (1998) Wheres the link? (Letter to the Editor). Waikato Times, 24 Febru-
ary 1998, p. 6.
Hansard(1998) Parliamentary Debates (Hansard) House of Representatives First Session,
RLS in Aotearoa/New Zealand 449
Fortyfifth Parliament, 1998, Hansard Supplement 10. Wellington: New Zealand
Government.
Hansard(1999) Parliamentary Debates (Hansard) House of Representatives First Session,
Fortyfifth Parliament, 1999. Hansard Supplement 11. Wellington: New Zealand
Government.
Hohepa, M.K. (1998) Hei Tautoko i te Reo: Maori language regeneration and
whanau bookreading practices. Unpublished PhD. Thesis (Education), Univer-
sity of Auckland.
House of Representatives (1998) Education (Te Aho Matua) Amendment Bill.
Wellington: New Zealand Government.
Kingi, M. (1998) Be strong and let others hear. Mana 22(36).
Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the
Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Literacy Task Force (1999) Report of the Literacy Task Force. Wellington: Ministry of
Education.
Ministry of Education (1998) Nga Haeata Matauranga: Annual Report 1997/8 and Stra-
tegic Direction for Maori Education 1998/99. Wellington: Ministry of Education.
Pere, R.T.R. (1999) Te reo rangatira me ona tikanga. Maori Education Commission
[Newsletter] Issue 2, May 1999, 710.
Royal Tangaere, Arapera (1997a) Te kohanga reo: More thanjust a language nest, in
Maori Keynote Addresses to the NZARE Annual Conference, 1996. Palmerston
North: NZ Association for Research in Education.
Royal Tangaere, Arapera (1997b) Te Puawaitanga o te reo Maori: Ka hua te h o te
ptiki I roto i te whanau: Ko tenei te tahuhu o te Kohanga Reo. Wellington: New
Zealand Council for Educational Research.
Saizarbitoria, R. (1985) Authors Foreword to 100 Meter. [Bilbao]: Basque American
Foundation Spain.
Salmond, J. (1893) The First Principles of Jurisprudence. London: Steven & Haynes.
Statistics New Zealand (1997a) Census 96: National Summary. Wellington: Statistics
New Zealand.
Statistics New Zealand (1997b) Census 96: Maori. Wellington: Statistics New Zea-
land.
Suzuki, D. (1959) An Introduction to Zen Buddhism. London: Arrow Books.
Tainui Maaori Trust BoardandWaikato RaupatuLands Trust (1998) He Riipoata-A-
Tau 1998. [Hopuhopu: TMTB & WRLT].
Te Puni Kokiri/Ministry of Maori Development (TPK) [1998]. The National Maori
Language Survey/Te Mahi Rangahau Reo Maori. (Wellington: Te Puni Kokiri/Min-
istry of Maori Development).
Te Taura Whiri i te Reo/Maori Language Commission (1995) Ae Ranei, he Taonga
Tuku Iho? National Maori Language Survey 1995, Provisional Findings.
Wellington: Te Taura Whiri i te Reo & Te Puni Kokiri.
Te Taura Whiri i te Reo/Maori Language Commission (1998) Ko te Whnau:
Wellington: Maori Language Commission.
Unamuno, M. de (1954) The Tragic Sense of Life. New York: Dover Publications.
450 The Pacific
Chapter 19
From Theory to Practice (and Vice
Versa): Review, Reconsideration and
Reiteration
J.A. FISHMAN
Having read around the world, visiting both old and new cases of RLS
efforts (a dozen and a half in all), the time has come both to attempt a
summary of what has been learned and to consider howwell RLS appears
to be formulated and implemented a decade after its launching (Fishman,
1991). All of my comments below take as their points of departure the
views and opinions reported by my colleagues in this volume, to whom I
ammost grateful for their cooperation, no matter howmuch I may agree or
disagree with one or another of them, whether theoretically or empirically.
Although I will use and explain much of the theoretical schemata initially
employed in 1991, it is nevertheless the case that as a result of working on
this volume there have been some changes made, both in detail and in
general approach.
1
Ideological Clarification
My initial stance that it was not terribly crucial for the practice of RLS to
spell out its ideological foundations (the why? of RLS), since those
involved in RLS obviously knew why they were doing so, seems to me, in
retrospect, to have assumed greater unanimity and clarity of purpose than
RLSers themselves frequently evince. It still amazes me that individuals
who have spent their entire adult lives (and often good chunks of their
adolescent years as well) engaged in RLS efforts are, nevertheless, often
uncomfortable and somewhat tongue-tied about explaining why they are
involved in this way, whether to themselves or to the doubting Thomases
that surround them, among Xians and among Yians alike. It is as if there
were some contra-intuitive, contra-normative or irrational aspects of their
own beliefs, values and behaviours that are so generally questioned by
others that RLSers themselves often cant really totally free themselves of
451
Conclusions
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration
some nagging doubts about their preoccupationwiththeir ownthreatened
language.
Five Ideological Challenges to RLS
Monetary values, ethnocultural values and cultural democracy
RLS is concerned with the recovery, recreation and retention of a
complete way of life, including non-linguistic as well as linguistic features.
Some of the features of both kinds are solidly documented in memories,
texts and realia of the near and distant past. Others are innovative exten-
sions and inventions required in order to cope with the differences
between now and then, between an interrupted past and the partly
unprecedented present. All cultures and the social identities that they
foster even those of well-established and seemingly unthreatened Yian
societies are partially continuations andpartially innovations relative to
their own pasts. When both continuations and innovations are under
local self-regulation they fuse together into a seemingly seamless
authentic whole. RLS is the linguistic part of the pursuit of ethnocultural
self-regulation which democracies and international bodies are increas-
ingly recognising as a basic right for indigenous (and often also for immi-
grant) populations. Entry into the European Union and several other
EuropeanandNorthAtlantic organisations for cooperationandsecurity
has repeatedly been postponed for post-Soviet eastern European coun-
tries because these countries still have not worked out amicable,
accepting and supportive policies with respect to their own indigenous
minorities. The absence of such mutually agreed upon policies in
Slovakia, Roumania and Belorus, for example, has been taken as contra-
veningthe standards that members of these organisations must attainand
abide by. RLSers must realise that such organisations and their declara-
tions, treaties andconventions are pursuing goals that are consonant with
the building blocks of modern democratic societies. Rather than being in
any way questionable or retrogressive, these are goals that should be
pursued with heads held high.
But are these goals realistic with respect to maximising success vis--vis
the material dimensions of individual and societal success? The European
Union seems to think so, pointing to ultimate higher productivity (overall
and in minority regions) of countries with more progressive minority poli-
cies. But the opponents of RLS efforts never tire of pointing out that most
major reward systems are linked to Yish use and, indeed, Yish mastery.
They claimthat it is more rewarding to link local populations linguistically
with the widest economic and social systemto which they can gain access.
452 Conclusions
They stress that those languages are most useful that yield the greatest
social advancement and that Xish is hardly in the running in those terms.
They claimthat language policies succeedwhenandonly whenthey corre-
spondto labour-market considerations. In short, Yians andXians- via-Yish
frequently persuade those advocating and using Xish that such efforts are
pernicious vis--vis the economic success of their own co-ethnics. Such
claims are often devastating for Xish parents who need to establish a Xish
home-language policy in which to raise their own children. Parents are
seldomfamiliar with the objective economic findings on which the policies
and declarations of the most modern inter-polity organisations are based
andthey needto be reassuredthat they will not be handicapping their own
children by socialising them and educating them in Xish (see, e.g. Grin,
1997, 1999).
It is in such grass-roots discussions particularly when the immediate
economic potential of Xish is low that the true complexity of Xish values
andidentity needto be stressed. Xians are invariably bilingual (muchmore
so than Yians) and, therefore, in no way cut off fromthe economic rewards
that are presumably inherent in Yish. Yish will long (and perhaps even
always) be part of the educationof Xishchildren. If only knowledge of Yish
stood between Xish workers and Yish-controlled rewards, the economic
well-being of the former would be much better off than it usually is.
Furthermore, the economic reward dimension is not the only one that
defines Xishindividual andsocial identity(nor, for that matter, that defines
Yish individual and social identity either). Societally weaker languages
always need more than mere economic rationales. It is not labour-market
access but economic power which is disproportionately in Yish hands and
that is a problem that will rarely be overcome on linguistic grounds alone.
As a result, even Xian bilingualismusually does not lead to any redistribu-
tion of economic power and, that being the case, the maintenance of Xish
identity and cultural intactness becomes all the more important for
community problem solving, health, education and cultural creativity.
Vulgar materialism (whether Marxist or capitalist) does not begin to do
justice to the nuanced and complexly interrelated human values, behav-
iours andidentities that are essentiallynon-materialistic or evenanti-mate-
rialistic in nature (among them being family loyalty, friendship ties,
spirituality, affection and intimacy, aesthetics and the corpus of ethics that
eachculture expresses andcontinuallydevelops). Isnt it the markof higher
cultures tohave other thanmaterial values, the latter beingmerelythe most
elementary expression of individual and group needs? Do not Yians too
pride themselves precisely on the non-material attainments of Yish
ethnocultural life?
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 453
The normality of minority language death
Another frequent ideological trap faced by RLSers is the claim that
language shift is only a normal consequence of minoritymajority relations
andthat previous generations of Xians not onlyacceptedsuchshift without
a murmur, but even did so gladly as the price of admission. Ostensibly,
then, current RLSers are just trouble-makers, disturbers of civility, by
arousing the natives, in addition to standing in the way of the latters
material progress. On the other hand, however, Yians would probably
agree that (insofar as they themselves are concerned) dying without a
murmur is hardly a great virtue andthat those indanger of deathought, at
least, to be read their Miranda rights, i.e. informed of the probable conse-
quences of their actions and allowed to request the aid of a lawyer or advo-
cate. Wouldnt at least some of those Xians, who presumably accepted
language shift without a murmur, have been more reluctant to do so if they
had been made aware of the fact that there were other viable options (X + Y
bilingualism, for example), on the one hand, and that the promised
rewards for language shift were by no means guaranteed? Shouldnt Xians
be suspicious of a point of view that assumes that it is natural for their
culture and identity to die, whereas it is not at all natural for the same to
happen to Yians? Isnt natural merely used to disguise a power play and
to discourage resistance to it? And, finally, arent those fewwho did mur-
mur (protest and attempt RLS), who denied that it was more noble and
civilised for their culture to die than to live, being overlooked or even
besmirchedfor resisting? Isnt historybeingrewrittenex post facto to favour
the winners ab initio?
Yish is peaceful; RLS is inherently conflictual
A third Yian mainstream ploy is to bemoan the purported conflictual
nature of RLS. It is not the majority Yians who are conflictual (eventhough
they punitively control and implement by far the major reins of power and
of the purse) but the unequal efforts of Xians that are responsible for all
trouble-making. Even the much vaunted no language policy of many
democracies is, in reality, an anti-minority-languages policy, because it
delegitimises such languages by studiously ignoring them and, thereby,
not allowing them to be placed on the agenda of supportable general
values. Certainly most RLS efforts (those referred to throughout this
volume as constitutingStages 85) are quite the opposite of confrontational
in any real sense; they are inherently based upon efforts to win over public
opinion by seeking support, approval and recognition, both in the Yian
camp and in the Xian camp as well. Many would argue that even Stage 4 is
454 Conclusions
non-confrontational since compulsory education, of whatever kind, must
still meet all of the requirements of the Yian authorities. Stages 31 are
equally non-confrontational if pursuedvia the normal democratic political
processes of compromise, bargaining andcoalitionbuilding. Isnt the accu-
sation against RLSers of fostering conflict and confrontation a case of the
pot calling the kettle black? In almost all cases the power at the disposition
of RLSers is puny indeed in comparison with the power directed against
themthat they must overcome. Isnt the confounding of political andphys-
ical struggle a Yian demagogic rather than a democratic tactic? Isnt even
the expression political struggle merely a metaphor which includes the
entire political process, includingcompromise, coalitionbuilding, bargain-
ing, the right to petition and organise, etc., etc.? And isnt a political
struggle the best guarantee that no physical struggle will obtain? Is it really
wise to leave RLSers with only a damned if you do, damned if you dont
Hobsons choice?
Your nationalism is worse than mine!
The relationship between RLS-efforts and other types of organised
minority ethnonational efforts is a fourth phenomenon that troubles some
Yianideological critics. It is undoubtedlytrue that sucha relationshipoften
obtains. Just as nationalismis ethnicity rendered conscious and mobilised,
so RLS is language maintenance rendered conscious and mobilised. RLS
depends on eliciting andactivating the ethnocultural sympathies, conscious
identities and overt loyalties of a threatened languages traditionally
associated ethnocultural population. Local/regional ethnocultural
movements exist peacefully within most (if not actually all) democratic
polities of the world. If not permitted to function peacefully, they have no
optionbut to functionclandestinely, but functionthey must, because they
are an expression of an important part of the identity and of the problem
solving potential of their supporters. Do they constitute a potential for
trouble for Yians and their authorities? Only if oppressed. In an increas-
ingly global society such oppression makes even less sense than hereto-
fore, since even the already recognised political boundaries are
increasingly meaningless for the purposes of directing or conducting the
major econotechnical, commercial and communicational processes that
are at the very heart of globalisation. However, the coming of
globalisationincertainaspects of humanfunctioning makes localisation
even more important in modern part-identity, equally so for state-nation,
nation-state and sub-state populations (RLS being of particular relevance
for the latter).
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 455
One language per country is enough (particularly if it is my
language in my country
The disingenuous nature of the Yian attack on RLS-efforts as an expres-
sionof Xishethnicity is evident ina fifthYianideological positionthat one
language per country is enough (Myhill, 1999; Edwards, 1994). Cynically
claiming that Yians have as much right to a place of their own as do Xians,
they champion territoriality as the basis of Yian language policy, returning
the moral level of the discussion to the level of 19th century imperialisms.
There are only c. 200 polities in the world today and some 4000 to 6000
languages. Carrying the above outlandish view to its logical conclusion
would be tantamount to consigning thousands of languages to extinction
or suicide, on the one hand, or to that very same endless territorial subdivi-
sion of the world of which Yians frequently and falsely accuse minority
ethnocultural activists. There is noneedfor sucheither/or scenarios, just as
there is no needto choose between the territorial andthe ethno-identity (or
personality) principle of human aggregation. These are both false Carte-
sian dichotomies imposed upon phenomena that are in reality hierarchical
co-occurrences. Just as there is no mindbody dichotomy, so minorities are
well accustomed to both an ethnic and a civil (territorial) identity and it is
high time that Yians accepted the true complexity of their own identi-
ficational potential, particularly as the world of globalisation engulfs them
just as surely as it will engulf the Xians. To expect Xians to give up Xish
merely because they are living in Yland, is tantamount to expecting Yians
to give up Yish just because they are living in a world increasingly domi-
nated by Anglophone technological-economic-military power.
It is ludicrous for Yians to appeal for sympathy with their agony on
being faced by ethnolinguistic minorities in their midst. Are Yians, there-
fore, unable to socialise their own children into their traditional ethno-
linguistic cultures? Are they unable, therefore, to manage their own
ethnocultural affairs, to support their own educational, governmental and
mass-media institutions as well as foster their own economic viability? To
equate the dangers to Yish exclusive territorial andinstitutional domination
to the dangers to Xish cultural existence is to evince a moral blindspot of
disconcerting proportions. It is but one more example of the truism that
ethnolinguistic democracyis denieddownwardinthe power hierarchybut
appealed for (and struggled for) upward in the power hierarchy. It is yet
another example of Yians wanting to have it both ways, i.e. to dominate
those hierarchicallybelowthem, without beingdominatedbythose hierar-
chically above them, in a world in which they themselves have much more
power than do Xians.
456 Conclusions
The above five ideological criticisms of RLS activities require constant
orientational efforts, both within Xian circles as well as in explaining Xian
aspirations for ethnocultural self-perpetuation to Yians. RLSers need feel
noembarrassment inopposingthe above charges because theyare not only
baseless and self-serving, but they are also fed by impoverished and
impoverishing views of the true richness of the human personality, of
human identificational processes and of the growing search for mean-
ingfulness inpost-modernglobal society. The struggle for RLS is a struggle
for a more humane humanity all over the world and, yes, it is ultimately a
struggle onbehalf of Yians too! Intodays world, withits growingmulticul-
tural emphases, with its march toward civil nationalism in state-nations
and in nation-states alike, with its growing terralingual environmen-
talism (in which the protection of ethnolinguistic diversity is viewed as
literally part and parcel of environmental protection of all other natural
resources), there should be less and less need for RLSers to explain them-
selves, let alone defend themselves, in their efforts which can ultimately
benefit us all.
Ideological Supports for RLS
We are the only ones on whom Xish can count for support
Hungary and Finland are two European polities with a joint myth: that
they are orphans in Europe and that the very future of their language,
culture andpeople, therefore, rests entirelyontheir ownshoulders. No one
else will come to their assistance because they have no family relationship
withany others. Quebec too has made muchof this line of support. It is the
only possible champion of French on the entire American continent
(setting aside French Guiana, the francophone Caribbean islands and St
Pierre and Miquelon as too insignificant to count in the struggle with such
giants as English and Spanish). But this moving argument, a justification
for special efforts because of special responsibilities, is one that many can
utilise. Not only is Spain the only possible defender of Spanish in Europe,
but so is Galicia the only possible defender of Gallego. Not only is Israel the
only possible political entity inthe entire worldthat might defendHebrew,
but so is Malta with respect to Maltese. Indeed, this argument lays bare the
constantly surprising fact that even apparently mighty nations and
languages often are self-declared weaklings in need of urgent self-protec-
tive measures. French in France is deathly afraid of the worldwide (and
particularly the European and most particularly the entertainment-media
and econotechnical) power of English and does not blush to claimthe right
of self-protectionat every turn. But this is obviously anargument of special
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 457
relationship, affection and responsibility which applies many-fold more
or at least equally to most really threatened languages the world over.
Coupled with the admonition that modern polities cannot really be
monolingual anyway, and that in a multicultural stance they can better
attract andholdthe loyalties of all of their citizens of varyingethnicities, the
special responsibility of Xians for Xish seems eminently reasonable and
even more urgent. If the defenders of English in the USAfeel threatenedby
Spanish, what should the defenders of Navaho feel vis--vis English?
Sweden is a perfect example of the balance between Yish recognition of
Xishminorities andthe self-defence that Yishadvocates are alsoentitledto.
Having recognised five minority languages as co-official languages of
Sweden (among them Finnish, a regional variety of Finnish, Eskimo,
Romani and Yiddish) by virtue of their having been used there continu-
ously for several centuries, Sweden has also adopted special measures for
the overarching goal of cultivation and support of Swedish (Jernudd,
1999). Indeed, no one (neither Xians nor Yians) feels safe when not in
control of their own cultural boundaries and cultural functions. This is a
feeling that Xians andYians can both recognise andon behalf of which that
they can hopefully work together via the democratic processes of compro-
mise andmutual accommodation. The EuropeanUnions Bureauof Lesser
Used Languages and various of its treaties, charters and conventions have
established the significance of such languages for the unity and the prog-
ress of Europe, in addition to the numerous state languages (potentially 30
to 50 in number) and the 11 official languages of the Union itself. Certainly
this is an ideological precedent that RLSers can and should broadcast, both
within Europe and elsewhere as well.
Community and virtual community are not the same thing at all
as far as intergenerational mother tongue transmission are
concerned
The alacrity with which some RLSers have turned to bringing out their
own language-encodedversions of Windows 2000, lends a special urgency
to the realisation that intergenerational mother tongue transmission
depends on the existence of real community rather than its virtual pale
shadow. Indeed, although cyber-space can be put to use for RLS purposes,
neither computer programmes, e-mail, search engines, the web as a whole,
chat boxes nor anything directly related to any or all of themcan substitute
for face-to-face interaction with real family imbedded in real community. Ulti-
mately, nothing is as crucial for basic RLS success as intergenerational
mother-tongue transmission. Gemeinschaft (the intimate communitywhose
members are related to one another via bonds of kinship, affection and
458 Conclusions
communality of interest and purpose) is the real secret weapon of RLS. This
may seem like a puny and archaic weapon in competition with
globalisation, which threatens not only minority languages but even much
of worldwide Yish capacity to maintain resources, decision making and
even many of the institutions of socialisation (such as the school) in the
hands of local languages and cultures. Globalisation itself is built around
controls andconnections that make it possible for the major players to shift
jobs, cultures, environmental problems and labor standards back and
forth. The basic problemof RLS is the problemof howXishcanpossiblyeke
out a safe-harbour for itself inthe face of suchunprecedentedcompetition.
The answer is that RLS need not be in opposition to globalisation per se; it
can function as an addition to and as a welcome alternative to complete
globalisation and can be orthogonal to it. Modern humanity desperately
needs torestrict or compartmentalise-off certainof its most humanprocesses
fromcontamination by globalisation. The family is not a business and is not
operated efficiently so as to show a bottom line profit. Neither is friend-
ship, childhoodsocialisationandenculturation, neighbourliness or commu-
nity mindedness and community participation in religious, educational,
entertainment, hobbying activities or acts of comforting and assisting the
sick, poor andbereaved. These goonwherever communities insist uponand
contribute to retaining a realm of existence which is not governed by office
andbusiness affairs. They are powerful shields against alienationandtheir
power resides exactly in the fact that they can be kept unrelated to and
uncompetitive withthe massive Gesselschaft withwhichtheyco-exist. Their
weakness is their strength, their unimportance from a purely dollars and
cents perspective is their importance for our sanityandprivacyinthe midst
of myriad urban, modern, global intrusions into our lives.
Although most RLS movements are urban in location and in the
economic processes in which their supporters are engaged, they are rural
andreligious in metaphor andin myth and, therefore, they strike a bargain
with urban hyperconnectedness, compartmentalising it off from the
realms of intimacy and ethnocultural identity. RLS movements seek link-
ages to histories, cultures and identities that predate globalisation and that
smack of eternal verities that are substantially outside the orbit of
globalisation. The hunger for ethnocultural roots and for more traditional
religious affiliation, unrelated as the two may be, are notable precisely in
the populations most impacted by the pace of globalisation. RLS promises
greater self-regulation of ones home, family, neighbourhood and commu-
nity, on the one hand, and of ones own history and culture, on the other
hand. It is a promise that appeals to all those who realise that notwith-
standingall of the much-toutedbenefits of globalisation, the worldis all too
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 459
much with us and in us. Many also long for a corner in which they can be
their intergenerational selves and be with others of that same longing too,
and that is the RLS corner in which their own traditionally interpreted
language, customs, beliefs, holidays, stories, foods and sanctities can
continue toprevail. ManyYians longfor this tooandthat longingshouldbe
tapped in soliciting Yian sympathy, understanding and even assistance in
conjunction with the far greater Xian plight.
On Remaking, Rethinking and Generalising from Local
History
Like the post-60s ethnic revival as a whole, RLS was not supposed to
happen. However, not onlyhas it happened, but some of the veryprocesses
of globalisation and post-modernism that were supposed to be most dele-
terious topurportedlyparochial identities have actuallycontributedmost
to their re-emergence as part-identities. The increasing ubiquity of the
civil state, of civil nationalismand, therefore, of a shared supra-ethnic civil
nationalismas part of the identity constellation of all citizens, has resulted
in more rather than less recognition of multiculturalismat the institutional
level and a more widespread implementation of local ethnicity as a coun-
terbalance to civil nationalism at the level of organised part-identity. The
result is a growingrecognitionof the needtorethinkthe historyof the Yish-
controlled state in terms of one country, many cultures.
The Latvian government recently revised its originally anti-Russian Lat-
vian Only language policy (Ozolins, 1999) not so much to curry favour with
Russia as to curry favour with the Council of Europe, the European Union,
the Organisation for Security and Cooperation in Europe and Nato, all of
which require conformity with international norms vis--vis minorities in
their member states (as well as in states that seek to be considered for
membership). The Israeli premier Ehud Barak, in his 1999 post-election
victory speech, extended a warm and firm hand to the secular, religious,
ultra-Orthodox settlers, Sephardim and Ashkenazim, Ethiopian and Rus-
sian immigrants, Arabs, Druze, Circassians and the Bedouins [because] all
are part of the Israeli people. What a revision of history, when an
ethnonational state (a state that originally vieweditself as createdby andfor
one people) comes to view itself as a civil state which belongs to all its citi-
zens, of whatever ethnicity, all of them entitled to retain their respective
ethnicities as part of their total identities! Indeed, Yians too are merely
another local minority when viewed in global perspective, particularly if
they seek to be recognised by the international community of nations. To
attainsuchrecognitionandacceptancefromabove, theyneedtoimplement
460 Conclusions
justice for those whomthey rule. Thus does globalisation itself foster local-
isation in its wake. The removal of trade-walls makes bilingualisman asset
rather than a debit and a more rather than a less valuable individual and
collective attribute. How can all of this be explained, and how does RLS fit
into any explanation that is parsimoniously satisfying and predictively
useful?
Of course, both vulgar Marxism and vulgar historicism immediately
come to the fore. If even Cicero realised that local identities tend to be rein-
terpreted in the direction of the greatest possible rewards (Roman roads,
Roman commerce, Roman peace), then it should come as no surprise that
some latter-day thinkers have also concluded that economics may be the
strongest force influencing the fate of endangered communities. But it is
too late for a fruitful replay of the 19th century Marx/Weber debate, in
which the latter held that culture helped fashion economic reality every bit
as much as the latter contributed to the former. The exact borders sepa-
rating the two (culture and economics) are not that clear at any rate, given
that there are no cultures without their own economic processes and that
there are no economic processes that are not culturally imbedded. One
thing we can be sure of, however, is that RLS becomes necessary because of
extra-economic considerations as well and that an effective programme of
RLS involves a plan as to how to focus resources rather than merely the
extent of the programmes economic wherewithal per se. Vulgar Marxism
and economic determinism merely leave RLSers empty and hopeless, for,
under most foreseeable circumstances they will possess markedly fewer
resources than will their opponents. The basic RLS question is, therefore,
howbest to use the scarce resources at the command of RLSers, rather than
to call a moratorium on RLS until such time as more nearly competitive
resources are at hand.
Another danger is toarrive at conclusions withrespect toRLSdesiderata
by overemphasising a particular (but limited) pool of historical experience.
Most RLS theories and efforts are byproducts of European indigenous
minority problems. Certainly, however, such problems cannot be the only
ones from which a general (and all-inclusive) RLS theory should be
derived. On the other hand, only the local circumstances of any particular
case canfullyaccount for that case (but for noother case). Bothof these risks
are well illustratedinthe papers that constitute the bulkof this volume. The
indigenous minority cases do not, however, speak to the immigrant cases
and the latter rarely speak to the former. The fortunes of immigrant
languages are substantially influenced by home country events, most
prominent among themthe relationships between the home country and
the host country. The ease (cost and frequency) with which immigrants
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 461
can revisit the home country, economic cycles in both places and the extent
to which they are in tandemor unrelated, the degree or similarity between
old and new immigrant cohorts (enabling or disabling them insofar as
united ethnocultural efforts are concerned), the presence or absence of co-
linguals from different countries of origin, are other important consider-
ations that are particularly relevant to immigrant languages but far less so,
if at all, for indigenous ethnolinguistic groups.
Obviously, all of the above considerations, pertinent though they may
be for the future of Spanish in the USA (currently a touchy topic for many
adherents and opponents of RLS among American linguistic minorities),
are not relevant unless reconceptualisedinmuchmore general terms for
indigenous (aboriginal) minorities in the USA or elsewhere of the very
kinds that Terralingua (Note 2, Preface) is most concerned about. Obvi-
ously, the outcome of RLS efforts inanyparticular case are overdetermined
by that cases own particular historical dynamics; but to convert that
history into a general theory (or into a substitute for general theory) is to
engage in vulgar historicism. Certainly, the specifics of any case must be
taken into consideration in all local action, but general theory must rise
above such specifics, just as the specifics themselves, important though
they may be, must be seen as merely exceptional addenda and refinements
for the general theory. Examples of suchspecifics are the mobilisingimpact
of language insult sufferedbyXishinYland, the adrenalin-like impact of
the annual speaking-singing-listening convention of Xish-speakers in
Zland, andthe certainty that a single Yianliving for many months withone
of the last, agingspeakers of Xishwill thengive the language a newlease on
societal life amongXians-via-Yish; these are other examples of goodthings
to do that can only naively be recommended as sufficient for general
theoryor for general applicationelsewhere. Local descriptionis a verydeli-
cate and indispensable intellectual tool, but it is not the same as, nor does it
necessarily even contribute to general theory or general practice, any more
than clinical practice necessarily leads to a generalised understanding of
health and disease (although it is essential for arriving at such).
Desiderata for a Value-based General Theory of RLS
I have sometimes been asked why RLS is so little related to the general
theory of social change. My first answer has been that RLS recognises
democratic responsibility for cultural self-determination by minorities,
whereas social change theory has ignored any moral responsibility toward
those who suffer as a result of social change. My second answer has been
that social change theory views modernisation as an inevitable universal
462 Conclusions
and ubiquitous process that pertains to all of culture, whereas RLS
attempts to differentiate between faster and slower moving sectors of
change and to foster greater self-regulation of the latter for the purposes of
language-and-identity retention. Both of these differences are important
for a general theory of RLS. RLS is appliedto individual cases of threatened
languages on the basis of a general theory derived froman examination of
many diversifiedindividual cases (immigration, conquest, exile, genocide,
commercial domination, spread of innovation and superior knowledge,
etc.). Individual cases, like clinical medicine, oftenrequire adjustments and
modifications of the general theory so that it can apply to all cases rather
than just to one or more specific individual ones.
A Value-based General Theory
Although RLS is firmly grounded in democratic processes, it neverthe-
less recognises that simple majority rule does not in and of itself satisfy
important requirements for the existence of a just and good society. It
utilises and espouses advocacy and self-reliance on behalf of neglected,
overlookedor evendemeanedminorities as its two chief means of bringing
about needed changes to which the majority is generally oblivious or
outright unsympathetic. Rank and file minority members too, taking their
cue from the majority, may also be silent with respect to their cultural
misfortunes. RLS-efforts seek to make both Xians and Yians more
conscious and more knowledgeable about all that is lost, individually and
collectively, when a cultures traditionally associated language is lost. On
the one hand, it criticises the implicit Yish view that Xish is of little value.
On the other hand, it offers leadership and advice vis--vis fostering Xish.
The RLS movement realises that ideological clarificationwithrespect tothe
importance of RLS and cultural self-management is also not enough and
that cultural democracy must be actively promoted and implemented as
well. Justice, cooperation, compassion, self-reliance and cultural democ-
racy are espoused as interrelated avenues toward cultural empowerment
of the powerless who may have come to take Yish control as natural and
inevitable. Many of the above-mentioned features of RLS are similar to
other modern movements on behalf of greater distributive justice, e.g.
unionism, feminismandcooperativism. Like them, RLS alsoaspires toacti-
vate educators, the helping professions and researchers, not to mention
ordinary members of the Xish community, on behalf of transforming those
segments andinstitutions of their society whose contributions are essential
if greater justice for all is finally to be attained and if biases, hopelessness
and ignorance in connection with Xish are to be overcome.
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 463
A General Theory with Applied Implications
RLS theory seeks to be directive or implicational vis--vis social action,
rather than merely descriptive or analytic of the sociocultural scene. It
steers clear of shibboleths, such as leaving it to the government, or to the
schools, or to symbolic and formulaic insertions of Xish on web-sites, or e-
mail in Xish as substitutes for home, family, neighbourhood, community
based face-to-face language use per se. Prescribing more institutions, posi-
tive attitudes or prestige and active speakers for a threatened language is
nobetter thangivinga patient a peptalk, urginghim/her toget a goodgrip
on yourself. Stressing the importance of the social meaning of Xish even
in unprotected environments leaves activists totally at sea as to where in
real life to start, given different degrees of erosion in different community
settings and unprotected environments. Certainly, situational switching
into Xish for the attainment of traditional social meaning cannot long
continue after massive language shift has galloped along for any appre-
ciable number of years. Similarly, festivities of social meaning are notori-
ously weak as effective social forces for engendering normal, daily
language use.
Attitudinal advice is easy to give but hard to implement. Furthermore,
starting as far back as the 1940s, Einar Haugen noted that attitudes toward
threatened languages seem to improve quite noticeably toward the very
end, when their imminent demise is already quite evident. The Irish case
also is a good example of how attitudes and use can be quite unrelated to
one another. andhowa plethora of governmentallyfundedinstitutions can
yield very little active language use either in the short run or in the long.
Small and retentive languages in many parts of the globe are seemingly
without formal institutions, large numbers of speakers or noticeably posi-
tive attitudes or prestige. On the other hand, Latin seems to have had all of
the above highly touted desiderata and nevertheless lost its vernacular
speakers, remaining only as a classical tongue for very limited functions
among a very limited clientele. Additionally, if such ad hoc considerations
are to have any utility as guides to action, we must know how they rank
relative to each other. We must also know why they and only they have
been selected for use (rather than, e.g. demographic concentration as
distinct from simply the total number of speakers, or the proximity of a
stronger economy associated with a rival language). Lastly, it would be
very desirable to know what proportion of what criterion of language use
they account for, either singly or en toto, and the degree of usefulness that
RLSers can derive from them. Unfortunately, such data is unavailable for
any of the proposed approaches, including those that I have proposed.
464 Conclusions
The stagewise nature of 1991 RLS-theory has been accused of having
implications of evolutionary inevitability. Actually, these stages are
intended only for purposes of diagnostic and programmatic location.
(where to start and what to aimat when). The movement fromone stage
to another is totally a result of self-directed activity, rather than of any
natural process of a built-in developmental nature. The stages (which will
be commented upon below) are nothing but a logical set of priorities or
targets to guide RLS-efforts toward a desired goal. It places the responsi-
bility for most RLS action on those who advocate an Xian-via-Xish iden-
tity. It requires an enterprising and committed Xian community for its
stability and does not take any comfort in the possible assistance of Yians-
via-Xish (Germans who have learned to speak Yiddish (as a means of
penance for the Holocaust) or mainstream New Zealanders who have
learned to speak Maori as an expression of sympathy for the Maori plight),
who have a different community base and for whom pro-Xish efforts are
normally situational, temporary, idiosyncratic and even reversible. RLS
cannot be based on acts of charity by outsiders. Accordingly, RLS theory
realises that multiculturalism, though welcome as an atmosphere effect, is
not enough. It is overly diffuse in its ideological and practical focus and
usually ignorant as to the consequences of the far greater compartmentali-
sation, minimalisation and subsequent evanescence of Xish than of Yish.
None of the foregoing substitutes for genuine RLS-efforts by Xians, just as
none lead anywhere near the establishment or the reinforcement of an
intergenerational speech-community of Xians-via-Xish. Only such a
community will successfully integrate Xish into a continuing and evolving
Xian way of life. Without an actual ethnolinguistic community home, the
greater prestige of a thousand computer specialists constituting a virtual
interactive community, or a dozen Nobel prize laureates posting their
works on the internet, will not augur nearly as well for the future of Xish as
a thousand intergenerationally related ordinary rank and file daily
speakers living in proximity to one-another. RLS theory can explain why
this is so, while others cannot do so, because they are not anchored in a
theory that relates intergenerational Xish transmission to a theory of
normal speech community functioning.
Stages of RLS: Priorities and the Linkages Between Them
RLStheory, beingmore thanmerelya descriptive tool, attempts tolocate
the functional disruption of X in social space and has suggested a widely
(which is not yet to say universally) useful series of stages for doing so.
Such location helps establish both focus and priorities for RLS efforts, rather
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 465
466 Conclusions
STAGES OF REVERSING LANGUAGE SHIFT:
SEVERITY OF INTERGENERATIONAL DISLOCATION
(read from the bottom up)
1. Education, work sphere, mass media and governmental operations at
higher and nationwide levels.
2. Local/regional mass media and governmental services.
3. The local/regional (i.e. non-neighbourhood) work sphere, both
among Xmen and among Ymen.
4b. Public schools for Xish children, offering some instruction via Xish,
but substantially under Yish curricular and staffing control.
4a. Schools in lieu of compulsory education and substantially under Xish
curricular and staffing control.
II. RLS to transcend diglossia, subsequent to its attainment
5. Schools for literacy acquisition, for the old and for the young, and not
in lieu of compulsory education.
6. The intergenerational and demographically concentrated home-
family-neighbourhood-community: the basis of mother-tongue trans-
mission.
7. Cultural interactioninXishprimarilyinvolvingthe community-based
older generation.
8. Reconstructing Xish and adult acquisition of XSL.
I. RLS to attain diglossia (assuming prior ideological clarification)
Figure 19.1 GIDS scale
than merely presenting a redundant restatement of Xishs functional, insti-
tutional or attitudinal deficiencies. The fulcrum of the GIDS scale is
commonly Stage 6 (the home-family, neighbourhood, community, i.e. the
crucial nexus of intergenerational mother tongue transmission), but other
stages may be considered the fulcrum for particular languages vis--vis
particular functions that are being targeted in particular cases. Stages
below6 (i.e. withnumbers larger than6) are indicative of either the absence
of native speakers (Stage 8) or the absence of speakers of child-bearing age
or younger (Stage 7). These stages certainly represent serious circum-
stances for any RLS movement aiming at a return to vernacular functions.
The further away from the selected fulcrum stage, the less direct and
certain is the possibility of linkage to that fulcrum. This then is the second
purpose of the GIDS scale: to cause the viewer to consider the linkage factor and
its potential for strengthening the selected fulcrum.
The functions of threatened languages in particular tend toward self-
compartmentalisation. They do not possess the power (in terms of undis-
puted association with dynamic functions) to spill over substantially and
predictably into other functions, particularly not into functions at any
distance from those in which they are acquired. Thus, RLS is not, as some
have claimed, a step-by-step from the bottom upward effort or theory,
because linkages must be sought and instituted fromabove as well as from
below, so that additional andattractive functions canpropel RLS andmoti-
vate those attracted to it. Nor must RLS be worked on in a lock-step stage-
by-stage progression. As long as the crux-stage is targeted consensually
and with a clear understanding of what false priorities will cost, and as
long as the linkages from that stage to those below and above it are
constantly reinforced and reassessed, there is no harm in skipping over a
stage that does not correspond to a desired functional sphere for Xish.
Multistage efforts are definitely not contra-indicated if: (1) the priority
stage is kept first and foremost, and (2) the linkages to it are really opera-
tional rather than merely assumed as self-evident. Not all stages are
germane in all cases, because not all cases are equally or similarly in
disarray. Stages 8 and 7 may well be immaterial for RLS efforts aimed at
Stages 2 and 1. An RLS effort that has revernacularisation as its crux goal
and that then mostly emphasises the secret language function of Xish
when travelling abroad, or Xish greetings by the president at international
conferences taking place in Xland, or of Xish exhibits for touristic and
commercial visitors, is clearly disregarding the question of linkages
between its goals and its efforts. Obviously, the crux-stage for religious or
learnedClassicals must be different thanthat for lingering vernaculars that
still possess some immediate everyday functional possibilities and safe
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 467
harbours. While it is alsotrue that manycases consist of separatedor dispa-
rate stages (e.g. 6 + 2), the GIDS scale asks which of these is the crux-stage
and how substantial and reliable is the linkage between the auxiliary
stage(s) and the crux stage?.
Some of my colleagues have grantedthat the GIDS scale has a heuristic
value. but caution that it is not to be taken literally or at face value. If by
heuristic they mean roughly orientational, I beg to differ. If by heuristic
they mean an aid to discovery and to trial and error solutions I will not
protest. It is not the last word that will ever be said in the difficult area of
RLS. Certainly, no culturally neutral set of universal functions either exists
or can exist. But it is not a minor matter to have ones priorities right and to
be sure that they are supported by functions outside of themselves and,
particularly, on either side of themselves. I do not remember these points
havingbeenmade byothers before GIDS-scale-basedRLS theoryappeared
on the scene.
Comments on Particular Stages
The pre-intergenerational transmission stages
Stages 8 and 7 are neither unimportant nor hopeless insofar as Stage 6 is
concerned. RLS movements have a great deal to gain from data collection
from elderly and isolated native speakers (or once speakers, now semi-
speakers) that are highlighted by Stage 8. The activists, linguists and
teachers associated with the Xish movement can find, via tapping Stages 8
and 7) folklore (songs, proverbs, riddles, stories, jokes, prayers), vocabu-
lary and grammatical forms that can subsequently be utilised in young-
adult second language instruction groups, in infant-care and child-care
groups, in Xish instruction to parenting and grandparenting groups, etc.
Authentic linguistic paradigms, lexical items and texts enable RLS-efforts
to attain the linkage with Xian history and tradition which Xish is presum-
able better able to express and convey than can Yish. Furthermore, such
materials are more likely to be derived from a pre-Yian-hegemony stage
and, therefore, foster autonomy motivated distancing (Ausbau) in those
cases where Xish has been considerably altered over time due to growing
Yian social and linguistic hegemony (whether or not it is a closely related
language to begin with). Even RLS efforts that are well-focused at Stage 6
should seek to benefit from feedback linkages from Stage 8 and 7 efforts.
But there are yet other ways in which Stage 7 can be even more produc-
tive insofar as Stage 6 is concerned. Clubs andother neighbourhoodgroups
consisting of Seniors can be linked to Stage 6 as visitors, guides or
demonstrators for story-telling, traditional arts and crafts instruction (and
468 Conclusions
the attainment of personal competence for community events when such
arts and crafts are particularly appropriate), camping and hiking guides,
singing and cooking partners and, above all, as baby sitters and infant and
child-care resources. An ongoing relationship between a group of children
and a group of still active seniors can be extremely rewarding for all
concerned, although it requires supervision and may not be entirely
without monetary costs or associated legalities. The benefits are definitely
two way since the seniors (who may be anywhere from mid-40s and up
inage) mayalsoneedlanguage maintenance reinforcement andtheir expo-
sure to supervised interaction with school and nursery youngsters may
foster such language maintenance among the seniors themselves. RLS-
efforts needto include anactive LMoutreachcomponent since a revolving
door inflowof young speakers and an outflowof old speakers is not in the
best interests of RLS by any means, Seniors represent a clientele for all
neighbourhood public use of Xish: lectures, choruses, dramatic circles,
writing contests, speaking contests, folkloristic events (including folk-
dances and folk concerts etc., etc.). Seniors and their events must be trea-
sured and nurtured, rather than considered expendable merely because
seniors are well beyond childbearing age. Particularly where their own
children are the generation of returnees to Xish, the parents of these
returnees are also the grandparents of a hoped for new growth for Xish.
Given the mobility of modern generations, even adopted parents and
adopted grandparents are extremely valuable kin-relationships for any
RLS movement.
The Intergenerational Transmission Stages
Home, family, neighbourhood, community
Several colleagues who treat essentially non-urban and not yet fully
modernised speech communities have commented that Stage 6 essentially
encompasses their entire sociocultural space. I must grant that this points-
up a deficiency in the original GIDS model. There must be intra-stage
differentiations, so that not all such groups are necessarily lumped
together and so that any language maintenance and RLS variation that
exists among them can be more fully highlighted. As a sociologist
primarily oriented towards the study of urban and national societies, I was
insufficiently aware of intra-Stage 6 sociofunctional differentiation in
connection with inter-generational mother tongue transmission. The
mores the pity, then, that my anthropological colleagues who are more
attuned to such intra-Stage 6 variation have not suggested some lines of
analysis that could be followed. Is the nuclear family such a sub-dimen-
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 469
sion?, or the early childhood play-group?; or grandparents and child/
infant care arrangements?; or ceremonial occasions? etc., etc. It would
certainly be highly desirable to study several cases where GIDS Stage 6
exhausts more than half of any local cultures total speech-interaction
space, in order to refine our current general theory of RLS in accord with
frequently occurring situational interactions. A 6a, 6b, 6c . . . 6n typology
could then be worked out and examined for international compatibility.
When Stage 6 is all there is, it becomes even more crucial to protect it in
order that intergenerational mother tongue transmission not be disrupted
or dissipated.
Colleagues have also presented (wittingly and unwittingly) at least two
modern dislocations that undercut the effectiveness of Stage 6 when
vernacular functions are considered the prime target of RLS. The first of
these is concerned with major modernity related disruptions of family
functioning (e.g. via divorce, intermarriage, frequent family dislocation)
during early childhood. The second is the assumption that any long-term
meaningful relationship in Xish is sufficient to establish an inter-
generational mother tongue transition. Neither of the circumstances
mentioned above need be fatal for RLS, but neither of them is in any way
obviously conducive to RLS and, therefore, both can be considered extra-
neous if not contra-indicated. Certainly post-Stage 6 interactions may be
linked to Stage 6 in a mutually reinforcing way, but they cannot be substi-
tuted for that stage due to their lesser affective, more time and place
boundedness and more encapsulated or narrowly compartmentalised
functional natures.
Perhaps it is in connection with Stage 4, the school that meets compul-
sory education requirements, that the GIDS scale and its approach to RSL
has received the most criticism among readers worldwide (although far
less so from the colleagues participating in this volume). Teach it in the
schools has been such a popular nostrum for whatever ails Xish (or Xian
culture as a whole, and, truth be told, for far too many things that ail Yish
and Yian culture as well among their own adherents which may be the
original source from which this error spills over to Xish RLSers), that any
questioning of the efficacy of the school for RLS purposes has smacked as
rank heresy to many. But it is exactly here that the difference between
language learning andother school subjects, on the one hand, andbetween
language learning and RLS, on the other hand, appear most clearly.
Languages are not subjects per se andtheycannot be successfullyacquired
unless they are usedfor the purposes of active communication.
2
The lack of
appreciation for this truth underlies the failure of solely school-based
language study worldwide. Language study for RLS purposes will fail
470 Conclusions
even more drastically and painfully, unless Xish has a society in which it
can function, before school begins, outside of school during the years of
schooling and afterwards, when formal schooling is over and done with.
3
Untold hours and dollars have been spent for naught because of disregard
or mere lip-service for this fundamental truth.
4
RLS is essentially a func-
tional reversal vis--vis Xiansocietymore generally, muchmore sothanit is
merely the pro-forma introduction of a subject in schools that meet
compulsory education requirements.
Evenschool-basedinstructioninreligious or secular classicals cannot be
retained unless there is an after-school life for these venerable languages,
either in churches or in vocations that require them. If these functions are
neighbourhood based, they are part of Stage 6. If they are part of out-of-
neighborhood reality, they are part of Stages 3 to 1. In all cases (with the
possible exception of academic linguists), languages are not their own
rewards. If they remain functionally locked away in the schools, they may
be learned (youngsters have an amazing ability and capacity to learn use-
less matters whichtheynever needagain, once their schoolingis over), but
they will not accomplish the wonderful goals of communication with
neighbours far and near, improvement of commerce and diplomacy,
attainment of social mobility, and familiarity with the brightest stars of
world literature and much, much less yet will they accomplish RLS
unless they are linked to real life stages before, during and after school.
The RLS school (whether of Stage 4a or 5, or even at the university level)
must keep reality firmly in mind. Schools tend to be compartmentalising
institutions. They have their own authority structure, personnel, reward
system, budget and evaluation procedures. It is very easy for them to
become sheltered workshops, a world apart and unto themselves. When
that happens, they effectively cease to contribute to the RLS process. This
realisation does not constitute a downgrading of the schools functions in
conjunction with RLS. It merely stresses the fact that, with respect to
languages, linkages are crucial for schools, regardless of whether another
Stage (like Stage 6) is the crux-Stage or the school itself is the crux-Stage.
Literacy in Xish is frequently an important RLS goal. Yish advocates
often play down the legitimacy or importance of Xish literacy, as part and
parcel of denying Xish any serious intellectual claims as a whole. If schools
are to help attain literacy in Xish there must be linkages between the school
and an out-of-school literature, press, commercial institutions, organisa-
tional and governmental record keeping or other literacy-based occupa-
tions. On the whole, however, literacy in Xish will usually remain a weak
competitor withliteracyinYish(most frequentlyacquiredinthe verysame
schools). It is Yish literacy that makes the greater contribution to social
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 471
mobility and the Central Education Authorities that ultimately control
both Stage 4a and 4b schools, constantly make sure that the Yish education
domain is particularly stressed, evaluated and rewarded. Thus, the
schools role in Xish literacy training is inevitably a two-edged sword. It
often reveals a somewhat naive and inevitably self-serving view when
over-eagerly pushed by educators. Furthermore, oral Xish education can
more easily be out-of-school linked and may explain why some Third
World groups (e.g. many Hopi, ultra-Orthodox Yiddish speakers and
Quechua) have never fully opted for school-based literacy to begin with
andbasicallytrust inStage 5 education, because it is almost completelyfree
from the control of Yish authorities and can, therefore, more often be
viewed as a form of indigenous enculturation and socialisation. This is
something no Stage 4 school can do for them with so few compromises.
Students frequently bolster the numbers of speakers, readers and
writers of Xish, but their RLS ability (like anyone elses ability) is far from
being the same as performance and it is performance in social functions
which is the litmus-test of RLS attainment. As one of my colleagues in this
volume has rightly observed, the capacity of schools to produce competent
bilinguals is far greater than the capacity of the bilingual community to
reproduce itself! The size of the hiatus between these two criteria is
inversely proportional to the success of RLS and of the schools contribu-
tion to that success. The entire difference among the three types of schools
includedin the GIDS scale (recognition of three types of schooling admit-
tedlythe trichotimisationof a continuum is hardlya signof downgrading
the phenomenon under consideration!) is trivial if these differences are not
related to functional linkages. Nevertheless, schools may serve RLS in
additional ways, beyond the pursuit of literacy and of oral competence
alone. Schools can serve to ideologise the young on behalf of RLS.
Secondary and Tertiary School students and young adults as a whole are
the ones that most need to be moved (and to be moved most quickly) from
school to Xish life out of school, either by moving fromStage 5 to 6 or from
Stage 4 to 6. It is relatively easy to found (and even to fund) a school; it is
much harder to graduate a cohort of fluent and active Xish speakers; and it
is hardest of all for this cohort to continue being fluent and active speakers
of Xish until they find an equally committed life-partner and start nuclear
families of their own. It is at this step that intergenerational transmission is
achievedandthat the requiredsocial linkages tothe Xishschool attaintheir
RLS goal.
RLS-related schools are not at all the same as ordinary FLES or other
second language education. RLS related schools must be cognizant of the
special methods and materials that are required for a language that has
472 Conclusions
only a limited out-of-school reality. These schools cannot afford to teach
imaginary Xish conversations at airports, hotel check-ins, luxury-car sales-
offices and other social mobility related and Yish dominated interactions.
The programme of study that is required for RLS is an identity formation
one, an Xian-via-Xish ideologising one, a community building one and a
community membership one. The teachers, programmes and materials for
such programmes are likely to be non-existent or in extremely short
supply. Teachers mayneedtoprepare their ownmaterials for years ledby
exemplary teacher trainers and curriculum/materials specialists before
there is a sufficient market for recognised publishers to enter into the prep-
aration andsale of such items. However, the proximity of schools to homes
andparents, the crucial links betweenschools andcommunities, onthe one
hand, andbetweenschools andideology onthe other hand, constitute both
the rewards and the challenges that RLS teachers can expect for their
labours. If theyworkas hardonlinkages to out of school andafter school
as they work on their in school lesson plans, then they will really be an
asset to RLS rather than a detour or a cul-de-sac.
The high power stages
The further above Stage 6 (andparticularlyabove Stage 4) the greater the
contribution that GIDS stages make to fostering Xian social power and
individual social mobility. As might be expected, these stages are particu-
larly problematic for Xish, because of the Yish social control and superi-
ority in resources that envelopthem. An example of this can often be found
even in the local work-sphere which is often ethnically mixed as a result of
the demographic marginalisation of Xians even in their own traditional
region. Xish media are often very pale imitations of Yish media, both in
quality and in quantity and the minor Xish media that do exist have never
been evaluated fromthe point of viewof their linkage realisation, particu-
larly vis--vis Stage 6. The media can interfere with intergenerational Xish
mother-tongue transmission more easily and more frequently than they
can reinforce it, if only because there are ever so much more Yish media
than Xish media. Higher education is another instance of the potential
danger of higher stages for RLS. Not only is such education oriented
toward life and success in the Yish world, but (unless radicalised for RLS
purposes) it rarely links back to Xish mother tongue transmission and to
the Stage 6 nexus in which such transmission transpires. Every Xish dollar
devoted to Stage 1 consumes dollars that could have been devoted to Xish
neighbourhood institutions and activities of all kinds. The media are less
under Xian community control than are local nurseries, kindergartens,
neighbourhood health services, scouting groups, sports clubs, choruses,
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 473
dramatic groups, job training services, libraries, homework assistance
groups, test-review groups, hobby clubs, charitable outreach organisa-
tions, summer camps, etc., etc.
5
The latter serve as the sinews of growingup
Xian in an Xish-speaking community and can more evenly compete with
their Yish counterparts, andat lesser expense when doing so, than can Xish
media with Yian media (and international media under Yian auspicies) en
toto. Neighbourhood and community events and activities are real neigh-
borhoodlife andthey feedback to ones real family immediately. Media, at
best, only creates a virtual community and, taking a leaf from Australian
experience along these lines, much more of a Yish than of an Xish one at
that.
6
Black English media programmes have not made Black English
speakers out of White English listeners, nor will they ever do so based on
the few hours per week involved and the differential statuses of the two
dialects. Indeed, if the truth be told, the comical and counterculture
contexts in which Black English programmes are commonly situated may
well turn off more Black English speakers than they attract White
English speakers!
All or Nothing at all!
Rocking chair languages vs reaching too high and losing it all
There are three possible alternative strategies for RSL efforts. One is
shoot for the moon! Another is anything is better thannothing. The third
is the right step at the right time. Tragically, the first approach mentioned
above has attracted all too much attention, most of it of the lip-service
variety. In reality, never being satisfied is much more of an RLS disease
than being satisfied with minimal and purely symbolic accommodations
by the Yishauthorities. Inthe face of the truththat Yishis still the only truly
indispensable language, one that every Xian must learn for certain vital
functions, those who opt and counsel to shoot for the moon actually
advise that those who seek to defend a threatened language are obliged (or
so some argue) to wage a total multi-front struggle. One cannot save the
value of a competence unless one saves . . . the whole set of political and
social conditions of production of the producers/consumers (Bourdieu,
1991: 57). But surely this is a prophecy of doom disguised by whistling in
the dark. Which Yish communities or even polities today can be said to
control the whole set of political and social conditions of production?
Similarly one-sided and misguided vis--vis the realities of RLS is the
very similar view that real change can be attained only if full control of
resources and means for decision making and the institutional domains
where language socialisation occurs is in the hands of Xians. This merely
474 Conclusions
restates the problem of Xish in more academic terms and still leaves the
threatened language-in-society without a plan of action. Distributive
justice may strike some as highly desirable, but the barriers inthe way of its
direct attainment are so formidable that postponing RLS until they are
overcome essentially means postponing meaningful RLS until it may be
too late for its more than ornamental attainment. Views such as the above
are also impoverishing, socially and morally, because they imply that RLS
can succeed only to the extent that it replaces Yish monism by Xish
monism. This spells the death knell for pluralismandpeaceful coexistence.
Inmost cases, the best that RLS canhope for, as a starting point for its never
ending road into the future, is to disengage from dependence upon and
supervision by the Yish authorities. The most meaningful way in which
this can be done from the very outset is to stress the role of Xish in family,
intimacy, and ethnocultural authenticity in local identity. Rendering kith
and kin into important allies (even on a cross-border basis) is more impor-
tant initially (and for a long time thereafter) than conquering outer space.
Pointing out the perfidy that may hide behind the use of Xish by Yish
authorities (a use that canbe haltedat will andthat does not feedbacktothe
intergenerational transmission nexus) is more immediately andeffectively
instrumental than being co-opted by such gambits.
While it is undoubtedlytrue that a [persons] reachmust exceedhis/her
grasp, the theory of linkages between higher order and lower order stages
is a more instrumental truth. It helps differentiate between RLS for the
current generations (by focusing on 7, 6 and5) andRLS for the next genera-
tion (by focusing on 4, 3 and 2). Neighbourhood and community focus
already provides a series of targets for RLS that go beyond the home and
family into pre-school, extra-curricular and a host of parent alone, child
alone and parentchild interactions. Home-family-neighbourhood-
community undoubtedly benefit from higher level linkages (especially if
such linkages have been built-in ab initio), but the outreach vs the compart-
mentalisation impact of these linkages is under RLS guidance rather than
under Yish control. Thats a big difference. Does this make Xish into a
rocking chair language? How many languages of atomic physics are there
really today anyway (even in the Yish sphere)? Is anything short of every-
thing a capitulation to obsolescence? Hardly! The vast majority of
languages in the age of globalisation will, at most, reach as high as being
languages of popular science for the majority of their speakers, of up to
secondary education and lower tertiary education for a much smaller
majority and of local humanistic specialisation only for a small minority.
Dutch is not insulted thereby, nor Danish, exposed as they already are to
English as Basque and Catalan are to Spanish (and English too). Reason-
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 475
ableness in all things is a far better maxim for the pursuit of RLS in a
focused linkage pattern than is the skys the limit.
Focus and priorities are not equivalent, by any means, to obsolescence,
folklorisation, self-limitation to rocking chair status or oblivion in the
rocket age. Focus and priorities merely build the recognition that, whether
with or without political boundaries, most languages will not have signifi-
cant participatory functions in global power attainment nor in direct
competition for functional superiority with those few languages that do
have such power attainment. Weakened and threatened languages must
certainly pursue their security and their future elsewhere. They must not
exhaust themselves anddiminishthemselves inanimpossible as well as an
impoverished view of what they signify in the lives of their own speech
communities. Their members will forever be bi- or trilingual, with each of
their languages even the most powerful among them enacting only
special and complementary functions. If they play their cards intelligently,
there will always be functional space for smaller languages and RLS-advo-
cates must be in the forefront of those who knowwhat to aimat andhowto
reinforce their primary goals through subsidiary ones.
When is too high, too high? Certainly when it neglects the basic crucial
stages for intergenerational transmission. Certainly when it neglects the
linkages betweenhigher stages andthe crux stages that are requiredif Xish
is to break out of a pattern that is both downward and dependent on Yish
approval, sufferance or support. Any RLS theory must realise that many
languages (not all, as some claim) function across several stages simulta-
neously. Some even stage-jump, and develop a pattern that ignores a
particular stage (e.g. Stage 5 or 4a). Stages are linear and human lives and
societal functioning are not. Nevertheless, not all language-communities
have the same problems, resources, goals or aspirations and, therefore,
they cannot all have the same tactical strategies. Xish must cut out of Yish
control and do so without entering into competition with Yish on the very
turf that Yish controls. There are haves and have nots on the language
scene as there are on all other scenes. But there are functions that Xish has
andcan defendthat Yish does not have. Andthese are the logical functions
around which to build the RLS fortresses and to which all efforts must link
back. They must also be the points of departure fromwhich to attain func-
tional expansion, doing so from a few positions of relative strength rather
than from many positions of relative weakness.
Of course, it is also possible for RLS to aimtoo low(to see the telephone
and the personal computer as the same kind of intrusive danger as televi-
sion and cinema). But, as this volume amply illustrates, they dont hardly
make that kindnomore. Certainly, myownviews donot counsel a retiring
476 Conclusions
and isolating self-enfeeblement. The highest power stage that one can
safely and effectively productively regulate and make use of is the crux
beyond which functional attempts must be made and back to which link-
ages must be secured. To do otherwise is simply to demonstrate that Xishis
second best, rather than putting its best foot forward. The third mistaken
RLS pathis the anythingis better thannothing one. Fortunately, it has few
principleddefenders (although it does sometimes have exploitative practi-
tioners) andis little more thanaspirinfor terminally diseasedpatients. RLS
must be a plan of action and a plan must certainly have priorities rather
than lack them.
Gains and losses during the final decade of the 20th century
A decade is usually very little time in which to either experience much
change in RLS circumstances or to note much payoff fromdifferential RLS
efforts. However, the decade of the 90s has revealed a number of notable
changes in this field. First of all, the field as such came into being, as an
identifiable area of international interest, both among individual acad-
emics, organisational entities and, more and more often, among Yish
authorities as well. Many of those who had laboured alone in this field for
what must have seemed an eternity, have finally realised that they are not
alone at all, that others share their concerns, realise the justice of their
complaints and the significance of their goals, not only locally but at an
international and pan-human level. This constitutes a tremendous change
in general atmosphere and we must pause not only to acknowledge it, but
to ponder why it occurred when it did and whether it will really make a
difference in RLS (or merely amount to window-dressing disguising and
hiding old wounds). Just as the ethnic revival of the 60s and 70s led to the
unprecedented growth in multiculturalism advocates in the mainstream
per se during the 80s and 90s, so the latter turn of events, in turn, has
prompted calls for affirmative action and positive intervention on the
language scene too. But will the sympathies of European Union affiliates,
the advocacy of environmentalists sensitive to the linkage between terra-
diversity and lingua-diversity, and the pious resolutions of well-meaning
international bodies really make a difference either in the rates of
intergenerational mother-tongue transmission or in the attainment of any
other specific RLS goals (or will they merely become undertakings without
any differences in outcome)?
Advanced technology and globalisation have redefined much of life. In
the process of sodoingtheyhave impactedandchangedsuchcore relation-
ships and experiences as family, neighbourhood and community. Yet each
child is still born into a specific nuclear family and that family is situated in
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 477
a particular community, however alteredbothfamilyandcommunityhave
become over the past century. Xian families nowneedRLS education (how
to be parents and even how to be grandparents in Xish. How to do
RLS inyour home is a course that one canandshouldbe enrolledinrepeat-
edlyineveryRLSsettingandthat shouldbe sponsoredbyeveryRLSmove-
ment and team-taught by the most experienced, inventive and dedicated
advocates that are available. Among the things that need to be taught are
the joys and responsibilities of immediate community. The benefits of RLS
in counteracting anomie should not be minimised. Gemeinschaft can be
alive and well in the very midst of galloping Gesellschaft for those who
realise that what it has to offer (above and beyond the ideological claims to
authenticity and fidelity, neither of which should be dismissed in the
slightest) is a balmto the spirit and a newand special kinship with those to
whom one belongs in a very special way and with very special links that
only Xish can activate and maintain. An RLSers life is not an easy one, but
the struggle for Xish is a struggle for meaning in life and, therefore, that
struggle is, in great part, also its own reward, a reward beyond rubies.
So, canthreatenedlanguages be saved? It is reallytooearlytoanswer the
above basic questiononempirical grounds, but it maybe indicative of what
the future portends that none of our cases has reported any substantial
progress attributable by local observers to general atmospheric circum-
stances such as international treaties and conventions. Although a positive
atmosphere for RLSmaybe preferable toa negative one (actually, eventhat
is by no means certain, since the motivational intensificationattributable to
moderate opposition must not be entirely written off), the direct and
evaluatable linkage between such general positiveness and specific RLS
goals may be lacking or insufficient. Certain litigious climates (e.g., the
Frisian and the Maori scenes) should be watched with particular care in
order to fathom the likelihood and the circumstances required for legal
measures, legislation, treaties, agreements, court orders and monetary
compensatory awards to be able to make a practical difference in connec-
tion with specific RLS priorities and linkages. Should such actions prove to
be effective in specific cases, they must still be differentiated from the
contributions of more general conventions and agreements arrived at by
international agencies and organisations that lack concrete power to
enforce their resolutions. Although conclusive evidence is lacking, there is
no reason to be overly optimistic in either case, because a lack of priorities
and linkages seems to characterise the entire legalistic approach.
None of the dozen individual cases that were studiedin the late 80s, and
early 90s andthat have beenre-examinedinthis volume at the very close of
one century and the beginning of the next, have experienced dramatic
478 Conclusions
successes during the decade separating one evaluation from the other.
Althoughwidespreadattitudinal positiveness towardXishandsubstantial
self-reported language competence in Xish are given in many cases, the
discrepancies between attitudes and performance are as noteworthy in
conjunction with RLS as they are in conjunction with all areas of moral
behavior where the spirit is willing but the flesh is weak. On the other
hand, there are a few telling cases in which a palpable sense of exhaustion
seems to hang heavily inthe air. Somewhat more thana dozenyears ago, at
an international sociolinguistic conference held in the Basque Country, the
local university students in the audience asked a representative of the
Catalan Secretariat of Language Policy how Basque could best get to the
initial take off stage whichCatalanhadpresumablyalreadyexperienceda
decade earlier. Today, the question might well be reversed, with Catalans
asking the Basques where they get the energy to keep at it, in view of the
post-nationalism that globalisation is fostering so widely. Catalan is still a
success case by any reasonable criterion, but can RLS succeedin advancing
and stabilising itself vis--vis Yish if only what is reasonable is pursued?
The fatigue that is fostered by not being able to hold on (on a long-term
basis) toreasonable success maybe everybit as wearyingas the fatigue that
ultimately overcomes those who have never achieved any success at all.
The comfort that minority languages could take a decade ago from
Australianlanguage policy, incomparisontothe policies regardingaborig-
inal languages there or immigrant languages in the USA, has largely
vanished in the interim. Instead of Australia being the forerunner of a sea
change inthe valuationof aboriginal or immigrant languages onthe part of
English-dominant host-countries, the direction of influence has run in the
opposite direction. The traditional American negativeness toward such
languages (whether with or without an English Only stampede) has
crossed the Pacific and invaded Australia. Immigrant languages, in partic-
ular, usually remain in the most unenviable position, but note the special
circumstances, external in one case and internal in the other, that have
made both Puerto Rican Spanish and ultra-Orthodox Yiddish into excep-
tions to that general rule.
Quebec has thus far twice not opted for independence (whether with or
without association with Canada), and, were it to do so in the coming
decade (it would only require a miniscule change in NewCanadian votes
for this to happen), the impact onFrenchRLS inits neighbouring provinces
would almost certainly not be an immediately positive one. As for the
remainingsuccess case, HebrewinIsrael does showsome movement (even
if only in a cleverly ineffective way) toward regrets about the Zionist deci-
mation of the other major Jewish languages there (particularly Yiddish,
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 479
Ladino/Judezmo, Judeo-Arabic and Judeo-Persian). Such regrets have
multiplied sufficiently during the past decade (see, e.g. Shohamy, 1999) to
leadat least toa tokenistic establishment of entertainment orientedgovern-
mentally funded authorities for the first two language mentioned above.
If there was ever any doubt that RLS should not be held captive by Yish
governments, Israel seems destined to become the perfect proof of that
point. On the other hand, political independence seems to have become an
uncertain RLS attraction or proven value, and not in Quebec alone (where
the overwhelmingly francophone population may soon have needed three
chances in which to think it over, if, indeed, it is only three). Puerto Rico
seems to be inching toward a plebiscite on statehood vs either independ-
ence or continuedCommonwealthstatus duringthe next decade (Ramirez,
2000). There seems to be even less interest than usual in independence
there. Instead, there is a growing realisation that Spanish can be as much
endangered in Puerto Rico with independence as without it, given that
SpanishEnglish bilingualismis inevitable, whether on the mainlandor on
the island, due to continuing individual and societal dependence on the
USA in both locations.
We are left then with an ambiguous scenario: the general climate of
opinion vis--vis threatenedlanguages has improvedin an amorphous and
largely still ineffectual sense, whereas actual RLS prospects and attain-
ments have improved very little, if at all, during the past decade, and seem
even to have deteriorated somewhat in some surprising cases. Is this a
harbinger of the growing and long-heralded triumph of globalisation over
ethnonationalism? Probably no such complete triumph is in the offing,
because, as we have noted before, globalisation itself also prompts (and
even requires) a greater recognition of local co-identity and authenticity.
We are, therefore, left with a human struggle which defies the complete
victory of either the old or the new. The complexity of human motives and
identities is rarely better illustrated than via the RLS scene, where neither
total triumph nor total resignation, neither total reason nor total irratio-
nality are in the offing and where particularism and globalisation cohabit
in a sometime antagonistic as well as in a sometime cooperative marriage.
Human societies will just have to make room for both and, indeed, will
have to do so increasingly, as migration and globalisation (the free move-
ment of populations and goods) both continue to advance during the next
century. Because RLS may well become somewhat more rational and prac-
tical in focus-selection and in linkage-attainment efforts, many more
threatened languages may, as a result, be better off with it than without it.
Yet these languages may not cease to be threatened as a result. Yish
languages too may come to realise that RLS tactics also have much to offer
480 Conclusions
them(as, e.g., inthe Swedishcase), rather thanbeing totally devices of Xish
languages alone.
So can threatened languages be saved? This question now has an
informed though uncertain answer: Yes, more of them can be saved than
has been the case in the past, but only by following careful strategies that
focus on priorities and on strong linkages to them, and only if the true
complexityof local humanidentity, linguistic competence andglobal inter-
dependence are fully recognised. More languages are threatened than we
think, and they are not necessarily only the smaller, more disadvantaged
ones either. The languages of the world will either all help one another
survive or they will succumb separately to the global dangers that must
assuredly await us all (English included) in the century ahead.
Notes
1. The notational convention of designating the threatened language as X or Xish
and its speakers as Xians (not as Xmen, as before, since it has been pointed out
that the latter designation is not always regarded as gender neutral [even
though German may be so regarded]), while its threatening and stronger co-
territorial competitor is referredtoas Yor Yish, will be continuedhere. This con-
ventiontoo has beencriticised, purportedly for reducing languages andhuman
population to mere lifeless and morally equivalent symbols. Nevertheless,
since the entire message and intent of RLS is to champion and foster the unique
role of languages in their own traditionally related populations and functions,
the handiness of the shorthand designations, coupled with the transparency of
the contexts in which they are employed, seems to me to outweigh the objec-
tions to them.
2. It is here, at the level of communication with real speakers, that school lan-
guage canactuallyweakenRLSbyfosteringa varietywhichdiffers fromthat of
the last Mohican speakers. School language differs fromhome andstreet lan-
guage everywhere in the world, and it is a problem everywhere for children
fromcommunities in which school-discrepant speech dominates andliteracy is
slight. But for threatenedlanguages there is a danger that the school varietywill
further weakenthe vernacular andeveneventuallyreplace it, undercuttingany
claimfor authenticity that Xish may have sought to stake out, and fostering in-
stead a variety which is both overly puristic and stilted lexically, on the one
hand, and rife with the usual learners errors and foreign markedness, on the
other.
3. The entire attempt to ideologically clarify the Xian-via-Xish authenticity Welt-
anschauung anattempt that must precede rather thancome as anafterthought
to the establishment of schools for RLS purposes is rendered null and void
when Xish becomes merely a subject (much like algebra) with no genuine soci-
etal roles or statuses toenact. The de-ethnicisedethnic school is a phenomenon
which results from the routinisation of Xish and its detachment from parental
or communal reality.
4. Indeed, the school dedicatedtoXishmayhave tobecome anactive builder of af-
ter-school andout-of-school Xian society, in order that the school itself can then
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 481
make sense and make a difference in the lives of the generations that are associ-
ated with it. Such a school may well have to have a longer school day, a longer
school week and a longer school year in order to involve parents, teachers and
students in the joint and constant task of creating and fostering the community
throughwhichintergenerational mother tongue transmissiontakes place andis
nourished. Obviously, the child-centred, permissive and progressive school,
whatever its virtues, is not in tune with RLS needs and purposes.
5. The mass-media fetish of some minority language activists appears in its true
unrealistic light when one but pauses to consider how few books and records
get produced per annum(notwithstanding their much lower cost) in most Xish
settings. If small-scale, thin market literacies require a topical or genre niche in
order to holdtheir ownina Yishworld, this is all the more so inconnectionwith
self-produced radio or TV, where Yish programmes are super-abundant. I do
make an obvious exception for self-regulated communication technologies such as
computers, e-mail and web sites which enable Xian youngsters, parents, teach-
ers and activists of all kinds to network, stay in touch with one another,
exchange pro-Xish materials and programmes, engage in desk-top publishing
and arrange regular actual community functions. Indeed, these self-regulated
communication technologies are content neutral. They are facilitators of any
stage and can, therefore, constitute part of regular Stage 6 or Stage 4 function-
ing, just as does the telephone and the fax machine. They do not, however,
constitute substitutes for the natural intergenerational mother tongue transmis-
sion process and, therefore, must be linked to it by RLSers themselves.
6. There might be some promise in adding a language key to TVprogrammes in
largely Xianregions, muchlike the violence, sex, vulgarity keys that are already
commonly available to parents who want to block certain types of programmes
fromtheir childrens viewing. If a Yishkey is either practicallyor ethicallycon-
tra-indicated, then it may be preferable to set certain days or hours for TV
viewing by younger children. Sooner or later, however, they must be helped to
face the pervasiveness of Yish and its functional role in their individual and so-
cietal role repertoire. This is a far more constructive educational goal for home
andschool, than to patronise tokenistic Xish programmes, producedby andfor
Yishauthorities, that merelyuse a number of Xishlexical items inways that pre-
pare for Yishlife, rather thanfostering RLS at the home, family, neighbourhood
and community levels.
References
Bourdieu, P. (1991 Language and Symbolic Power. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
sity Press (translation of Ce que parler veut dire. 1982. Paris: Fayard).
Edwards, J. (1994) Ethnolinguistic pluralismand its discontents: ACanadian study
and some general observations. International Journal of the Sociology of Language
110, 582. (Also note a dozen comments by other specialists, immediately after
the focus article).
Fishman, J.A. (1997) In Praise of the Beloved Language: A Comparative View of Positive
Ethnolinguistic Consciousness. Berlin: Mouton.
Fishman, J.A. (ed.) (1999) Handbook of Language and Ethnic Identity. New York: Ox-
ford University Press.
Grin, F. (1977) Amanegement linguistique; de bon usage des concepts doffre et de
482 Conclusions
demande. In Norman Labie (ed.) Etudes recentes en linguistique de contact. Bonn:
Bimmler, 11734.
Grin, F. (1999) Market forces, language spread and linguistic diversity. In Kontra,
Miklos, Robert Phillipson, Tove Skutnabb-Kangas and Tibor Varady (eds) Lan-
guage: A Right and a Resource: Approaches to Linguistic Human Rights (pp. 16986).
Budapest: Central European University Press.
Jernudd, B. (1998) Proposal for an action plan to benefit the Swedish language
(translation of the summary of the original document published in the Swedish
Language Committees Newsletter Sprakvard, 2, 1998). New Language Planning
Newsletter 13(1), 45.
Haugen, E.I. (1953) The NorwegianLanguage inAmerica. 2 vols. Philadelphia: Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania Press. (Reprinted in one volume. 1969. Bloomington:
Indiana University Press.)
Myhill, J. (1999) Identity, territoriality and minority language survival. Journal of
Multilingual and Multicultural Development 20(1), 3450.
Ozalins, U. (1999) Between Russian and European hegemony: Current language
policy in the Baltic Sates. Current Issues in Language and Society 6(1),647.
Prilleltensky, I. (1994) The Morals and Politics of Psychology. Albany: State University
of New York Press.
Ramirez, C. (ed.) (2000) Spanish and English in Puerto Rico: Past, present and fu-
ture. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 145 (entire issue).
Shohamy, E. (1999) Language and identity of Jews in Israel and in the diaspora. In
D. Zisenwine and D. Shers (eds) Present and Future: Jewish Culture, Identity and
Language (pp. 7999). Tel Aviv: Tel Aviv University (School of Education).
Review, Reconsideration and Reiteration 483
Index
Note: Page references in italics indicate tables and figures.
Aboriginal Education Strategic Initiatives
Programme (AESIP) 415
Aboriginal Land Acts (Australia) 188
Aboriginal Languages Association 414
Academia de la Lengua ah, Mexico
161-2
Academia [Peruana] de la Lengua Quechua
176-8, 179
acquisition
adult 14-16, 29, 85-6, 97, 176-8, 185, 189,
226, 244-5, 250, 254, 256, 444
child 14-15, 35, 38, 86-7, 94-5, 97, 253
activism
and Ainu 335
and Australian immigrant languages
387-8
and Australian indigenous languages 396,
414
and Frisian 227, 228, 229, 230-1
and Maori 425
and Navajo 24, 39-41
and Oko 294-5
and Quebec French 107-8, 113, 122-3,
131-2, 134
and Quechua 172-3, 183-5
and RLS 16, 468, 478
Adangme language (Ghana) 286
Adegbija, E. 287, 294, 298-9, 305-6
adults see acquisition; education
Advisory Planning Committee (APC;
Ireland) 205-6
advocacy, of Yiddish 94-6
Afisimania, 293
Africa
and language attitudes 287-8
and language shift 284, 285-7
and reversal strategies 305-6
and threatened languages 292-3, 294
see also Oko
African Americans, and Puerto Ricans 45,
49, 51-2, 58-9
Afk organisation 226
Agheyisi, R.N. 298
Ainu
and Chitose dialect 334
and GIDS 331-42
grammar 332, 333
and Japanese 323, 328-9, 333, 337
and language shift 326-8
and language shift reversal 323-48
mother-tongue speakers 335-6
and New (Culture Promotion) Law 331,
338, 342
second-language speakers 336
Standard Ainu 332
see also children; culture; identity;
Japanese; transmission
Ainu Language Speech Contest 331, 332-3
Ajanci language (Nigeria) 292
Akan language (Ghana) 286
Akor Itak (Our Language) textbook 331, 332
Alaska, and Eskimo languages 187
Allard, R. & Landry, R. 112
Alliance Quebec 127, 131
Allophones (Quebec)
and home language 105, 115, 136
and language of education 121-3
and language law 116-17, 118-20, 127-8,
134-6
and language of work 124-6
Altsiisi, F. 23
Alvarado, Velasco 189 n.1
American Indian languages 24
see also Navajo
Amharic
in Ethiopia 286
in Israel 357, 360-1
Andaman Islands
anthropology 309-10
and development 314
geography 309
and inter-ethnic marriage 316, 319
and social organisation 313
Andamanese 309-22
484
Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
Index
and demography 310-12, 311, 312, 319,
320-1
and GIDS 319-20
grammar 314-15
and Hindi 314-20
and language attitudes 318-19
and RLS 319-21
see also bilingualism; children;
code-mixing; community; family
Anderson, F. & Iwasaki-Goodman, M. 345
Andreu Iglesias, C. 52
Andrews, H. 157
Angelis, Fr de 327
Angola, and indigenous languages 304
Aotearoa 423-50
Arabic
in Australia 366-7, 369-70, 372-5, 376, 381,
383, 386, 388, 389-90 n.3
and Hebrew 354, 357, 359-60
in Quebec 116
and RLS 360
as sacred language 373
Aracil, Llus-Vicent 265
Aramaic, and Hebrew 351-2, 357
Argentina, and Quechua 169, 170
Armenian, in Australia 370
Arrels movement (Catalonia) 267
Ashaganna language (Nigeria) 292
assimilation
and economic pressures 187-8
and education 67, 417
forced 48, 406
and language shift 45, 46-7, 103, 136, 167,
256
Ataarangi movement (New Zealand) 426,
449 n.1
Attinasi, J. 64
attitude, language 464, 479
and African minority languages 287-8, 303
and Ainu 337
and Andamanese 318-19, 320
and Catalan 268, 269-70
and colonial languages 285
and Euskara 247-8, 248
and Hindi 318
and Irish 464
and Oko 284, 307
and Otom 158, 159, 161-2, 163
attrition, and language loss 394-5, 401-2, 419
Australia
and immigrant languages 364-90, 479
and GIDS 366-85, 388-9
and language policy 364, 385-7
language shift 364-8, 365
Productive Diversity policy 376
see also bilingualism; children;
community; education; English; family;
media; transmission
and indigenous languages 188, 391-422
categorisation 396-8
and GIDS 392-4, 396-7, 412, 419-20
and language loss 392, 394-5, 402-4, 419
and language policy 395, 413-19
and language shift 398-401
and Native Title decision 409
and RLS 402-13, 419-20, 479
and sociolinguistic context 392-4, 397
status and prospects 395-6
and threatened languages 401-2
see also bilingualism; children;
community; culture; education; family;
status
Australian Advisory Committee of
Languages and Multicultural Education
414-15
Awaradi, S.A. 314, 318
awareness, language 219
Aym, J.M. 271
Aymara language (Andes) 169, 170-1
Azurmendi, M.-J. 236
Balearic Islands, and Catalan 275, 277
Ball, S.J. 414
Bambara language (Mali) 286
Barak, Ehud 460
Bariba language (Benin) 286
Bartholomew, Doris 156
Basa-Gumna language (Nigeria) 292
Basic Law for the Normalisation of the Use
of Euskara 245
Basque see Euskara
Basque Autonomous Community (BAC)
Council on Linguistic Policy 256
and language attitudes 247-8, 248, 249
and language policy 235-6, 249, 255
and language recovery 250-3
and language transmission 240-1, 242,
243-4, 254-6, 258
and language use 245, 246
and linguistic competence 236, 237,
238-40, 239, 253
territory 234
Basque Country
cultural-linguistic heterogeneity 238-40,
257
Euskara speakers 236, 237, 240-1
and ideology of language recovery
248-50, 255
Index 485
territorialisation 234, 235-8, 356
Bassa Kontagora language (Nigeria) 292, 294
Bastardas, A. 268, 270, 272
Batchelor, J. 333
Becsky, Farina 340
Belgium, and Dutch 217
Belize, and Mayan 187
Belorus, and minority languages 452
Ben-Yehuda, Eliezer 354, 357
Bengali
and Andamanese 315-16
in New Zealand 430
Benin, and indigenous languages 286, 304
biculturism
in Mexico 160
in New York 67-8
Bilingual Intercultural Education Project
(PEBI; Andean nations) 180-1
bilingualism
advantages 40, 453, 461
and Ainu 336
and Andamanese 315-18
and Australian immigrant languages 367,
373, 375-6, 381, 389
and Australian indigenous languages 393,
399-400, 405, 408, 412, 413-14
in Catalonia 265-6, 271, 279
and Euskara 236, 238-40, 250, 253, 267
and Frisian 223, 231
and Hebrew 359, 360
and Irish 196, 197, 202-10
and Maori 443
in Mexico 144, 146, 152-4, 155-6, 158, 160-3
Navajo 30, 34-5, 40
Puerto Rican 44, 45, 50-1, 53, 56, 61-2, 66-8,
69, 71, 480
in Quebec 109, 115, 117, 118-21, 125-6,
128-31, 134
and Quechua 167-8, 176, 181, 186
subtractive 167-8
transitional 9, 67, 168, 265
in Wales 267
in workplace 125, 126
see also education, bilingual
Bill 86 (Quebec) 128-31
Bill 101 (Quebec) see Charter of the French
Language
Bill 178 (Quebec) 128, 131
biology, and language shift reversal 309-22
Birnbaum, Nathan 89, 94
Boix i Fuster, E. 266-7, 269, 270
Bolivia
and Quechua 167-8, 169, 170-1, 181-3, 185,
189, 189-90 n.1
and Spanish 167-8
Bord na Gaeilge (Irish Language Board)
209-10
Borodulin, N. 99 n.7
borrowings
in Ainu 338
in Andamanese 314-16
in Australian indigenous languages 400
in Maori 444-5
in Yiddish 93, 100 n.10
see also loan words
Bosnian, in Australia 381
boundaries, language 98-9
Bourdieu, P. 196, 207, 213, 474
Bourhis, R. et al. 111-12
Branchadell, A. 269, 273
Brazil, and Quechua 170
Bressola schools (Catalonia) 267
Breton, and intergenerational transmission
267, 279, 356, 398
Breton, R. 107
Brown, R.A.R. 310, 312
Burmese, and Andamanese 315
Bushwick, Brooklyn, Puerto Rican
community 56-9, 63, 65
Cambodian, in Australia 381
Canada
and Eskimo languages 187
Francophones 102-3
and Puerto Rican community 50
see also Quebec
Canadian Official Languages Act (1969)
102-3, 134
Cantonese, in Australia 383
Castonguay, C. 115
Catalan 260-83
and GIDS 260, 268, 274-5
and language shift reversal 264-74, 479
and language use 268, 270, 271, 278-9
and linguistic competence 262, 275-7
and quality of language 277-8
see also community; family; Spanish;
status; transmission
Centeno, Maria de 177
Center for Puerto Rican Studies, Language
Task Force 50
Centro Andino de Educacon y Promocon
(CADEP) 177, 178
Cerrn-Palomino, R. 171
Chakraborty, D.K. 313
Charter of the French Language (Bill 101) 101,
102, 110, 113-14, 133-6
and demolinguistic trends 115-17, 123
486 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
and education 120-3, 134
impact of 126-33
and language of work 123-6, 134
and Montreal 118-20, 123, 126, 130-1
Charter for Regional or Minority Languages
(Council of Europe) 216, 217, 218, 220,
229
Chikap, Mieko 335
children 453, 468-9
and African languages 292-3
and Ainu 332, 344
and Andamanese 316-17, 319-20
and Australian immigrant languages 367,
370-4, 377, 383, 389
and Australian indigenous languages 394,
399-400, 403-4, 406-7, 410-12
and Euskara 240-2, 245, 247, 252, 267
and Frisian 226-8
and Hebrew 354, 358, 360
and Irish 203, 204-6
and Maori 86, 423, 425, 426, 428-30, 434-8
and Navajo 26-7, 29, 31-5, 36
and Oko 289, 292, 296-7, 301, 307
and Quechua 173, 175-6, 178-80, 185-7
and Spanish 51
and Yiddish 79, 86-7, 92-3, 94-5, 369
Chile, and Quechua 169, 189
Chinese
in Australia 367, 369, 370, 372, 375, 381,
383, 388, 389 n.3
in Quebec 116, 132
Chitose dialect, and Ainu 334
choice, language
in Basque Country 247, 255
in Catalonia 266, 270, 278
in Friesland 219, 222-3, 231
in Ireland 209-10
in Navajo Nation 30
in Quebec 114, 122-3, 128-31, 135
and RSL 260, 261-2
Chumbow, B.S. 298
Chuquimamani, Rufino 173
CILAR see Committee on Irish Language
Attitudes Research
Circassian, and Hebrew 357
citizenship, and language 135-6
civil society, and RLS 271-2, 273
Claro, M.G. & Botho, A.M. 159
class, social
and Irish 205-8, 209
and language shift 59-62, 238-40
Clyne, M. & Kipp, S. 368
code-mixing
in Ainu 338
in Andamanese 314-16, 318-19
code-switching
and Ainu 332, 338
and Andamanese 314, 317
and Australian indigenous languages 395
and Euskara 245, 255
and Hebrew 361
and identity/Puerto Rican 50, 57
metaphorical 46
in Mexico 160
in Quebec 119, 186
cognition, and language 4-5
Colombia, and Quechua 170, 189
colonialism
and African languages 285, 287, 302-4
and Andaman Islands 310
and Puerto Rico 44-6, 47-50, 51-2, 62, 66, 71
Comes, G. et al. 269-70
Commission de la protection de la langue
franaise 127, 130-2
Committee on Irish Language Attitudes
Research (CILAR) 198, 200-1, 203
community
and Ainu 335-6, 338-42, 466
and Andamanese 318-21
and Australian immigrant languages
366-70
and Australian indigenous languages 393,
397-8, 417-18, 420
and Catalan 268, 271-2
and Euskara 246, 250, 251-3, 257
and Frisian 221-2, 222
and Hebrew 353, 354, 356-7
and language ideology 356, 465
and Maori 424-32, 442-3
and Navajo 24, 30-1, 37-40
and Oko 293-4, 295-7, 306
and Puerto Rican Spanish 54
and Quechua 168, 171, 173-5, 178-9, 184
and RLS 39, 40, 172, 468-73, 475, 476-7
and virtual community 97, 458-60, 464-5,
474
and Yiddish 83-4, 85, 88, 96-7, 98
compartmentalisation, functional 11-12, 14,
50, 89, 168, 221, 231, 319, 332, 466
competence, linguistic
in Ainu 335-6
in Andamanese 320
in Australian immigrant languages 373-4
in Australian indigenous languages 394-5
in Basque 236, 237, 238-40, 239, 247, 252-3
in Catalan 275-7, 276, 278-9
in Oko 292-3, 293
and second language learning 252, 373
Index 487
competition, language 2
conflicts, language 265, 453-4
Congo Kinshasa (Zaire), and indigenous
languages 286
Conseil de la Langue franaise (CLF) 119-20
contact, language 1-2, 102, 170-1, 206, 217,
266, 314-15, 444-5
control, institutional 241, 255-6, 267, 306,
452, 474-6
and Ainu 342
and Andamanese 320-1
and Australian immigrant languages
369-70
and Australian indigenous languages 393
and Euskara 236-8
and Maori 428-9
in Quebec 107-8, 110-12, 113, 116, 117, 134
convergence, language 119, 315
Corbeil, J.C. 268
Cornish, and RLS 268
Coronado, G. et al. 159-61, 183, 190 n.2
Coronel-Molina, S. 167
corpus planning
and Australian indigenous languages 396
and Euskara 244, 250-1
and Maori 444
and Oko 298
and Quebec French 114
and Yiddish 99 n.4
Corpus of Spoken Frisian 226
Council of Europe, Charter for Regional or
Minority Languages 216, 217, 218, 220,
229
Council of Frisian movement 229
creoles, and Australian indigenous
languages 400-1, 408
Croatian, in Australia 374, 381, 389-90 n.3
culture
Ainu 325-6, 328, 330-1, 333-8, 339, 343-4
Andamanese 316-18, 321
Australian indigenous 400, 404-5, 406-8,
412
Basque 243, 250, 252
Catalan 271
in education 34-5
Frisian 224-5
and globalisation 6-8
and language 2-5, 17, 21, 31, 38, 98, 452-3,
459, 475
linguistic 414, 417
Maori 432, 444, 447-8
Oko 294-7, 307
Otom 146, 153-4, 155, 156-7, 162-3
Puerto Rican 47-8, 64, 67
Quechua 167, 174-5, 177, 179-80, 185
Yiddish 81, 82, 84, 86, 96, 98
see also identity
Dagaari language (Ghana) 286
Dagbani language (Ghana) 286
Darwinism, cultural 24
Dawkins, John 386
DeChicchis, J. 332, 336
democracy, and RLS 7, 18, 451, 456, 462, 463
demography
and Andamanese 310-12, 311, 312, 319,
320-1
and Australian immigrant languages 368
and Australian indigenous languages 391,
402
and Basque Country 238, 240, 245, 247
and Catalonia 263-4, 266, 269-70
and Frisian 224
and Irish 195-6, 205-6, 208-9
and language law 115-16, 118, 120-1, 123,
134-5
and Maori 426-7
and Navajo 25-8
and Otom speakers 147-52, 148-50, 151
and Quebec French 102-7, 113, 115-16,
118, 120-1, 123, 134-5
and Quechua 168
Der Algemeyner Zhurnal (Yiddish weekly)
94, 99 n.9, 100 n.10
Der Yid (Yiddish newspaper) 90, 92-3, 95-6
Dhuwaya (Aboriginal language) 399
Daz, Porfirio 145
dictionaries
of Australian indigenous languages 407,
408
of Euskara 251
of Frisian 226, 229
of Navajo 29
of Otom 155, 156
of Quechua 169, 173
of Yiddish 79
diglossia
and Ainu 332
and Australian indigenous languages 393,
419, 420
and Euskara 250
and Frisian 231
in Malta 368
and Puerto Rican Spanish 50
and Quebec French 108-11, 113, 123
and Quechua 186
and RLS 466
Din see Navajo
488 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
Din Language Teachers Association 37
DINEIIB see Direccon Nacional de Educacon
Intercultural Indgena
Direccon General de Educacon Indgena
(Mexico) 153, 161-2
Direccon Nacional de Educacon Intercultural
Indgena (DINEIIB; Ecuador) 179-80, 183
Djambarrpuyngu (Aboriginal language) 399
Dominicans, in New York 55, 59, 62-4, 66,
67, 69
Douglas, Wilfred 411
Drapeau, L. 186
Dutch
in Australia 364, 366, 386
and French 267, 279
and Frisian 222-3, 225, 227-31
as home language 221
and internationalisation 216-17, 475
as language of administration 215, 219
as official language 219
Dutch Language Union (Nederlandse
Taalunie) 217
East Harlem, Puerto Rican community 50-1,
53, 56, 59, 67
Eastern Europe, and Yiddish 82-3, 91, 93, 97
Ebira language (Nigeria) 288, 290-1, 301
economics
and language choice viii, 261-2, 444, 452-3,
461
and reversing language shift 187-8, 196,
206-7, 212, 300, 385, 417-18, 452-3
Ecuador
and Quechua 168, 169, 170-1, 174-6, 181-3,
189, 189 n.1
and Spanish 167, 175-6
education
adult 92, 432-3, 444, 478
and Ainu 329-30, 331, 333-4, 340-2
and Andamanese 318, 320
and Australian immigrant languages
369-75, 386
and Australian indigenous languages 396,
397, 400, 404-10, 413-15, 420
and Catalan 275
and Euskara 241, 243-4, 252, 253-4, 256
and Frisian 221, 226-8
and Hebrew 351, 353, 354, 358, 360
and Irish 196, 202-3, 204-12
and language functions 14-16, 453
and language law 120-3, 127-8, 134
and language shift 285
and Maori 423, 428-33, 434-8, 441-2, 443,
444, 447-8
and Navajo 27-8, 29, 32, 33-5, 37-9
and Oko 302-3, 304-5
and Otom 146, 152-4, 155-6, 158, 159-63
and Quebec French 103, 106, 108, 113, 114,
120-3, 126, 131-2
and Quechua 176, 178-81, 182-3, 185, 189
and RLS 470-4
and Spanish
in Ecuador, Peru and Bolivia 168
in United States 48, 53, 66-7, 69
trilingual 227-8
and Yiddish 76, 79-80, 91, 92-3, 94, 97, 369
see also immersion programmes;
materials, teaching; teachers;
universities
education, bilingual
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages 373
and Australian indigenous languages
413-17, 420
and Euskara 243, 267
and Maori 434-5
and Navajo 34-5
and Otom 153-4, 155-6, 158, 160-3
and Quechua 176, 189
in United States 48, 53, 66-8, 69
and Welsh 267
Edwards, J. 188-9
Ellul, Jacques 428-9
Emai language (Nigeria) 292, 294
employment, and language law 114, 123-6,
134
Encuesta Sociolingstica de Euskal Herria I
234, 238-41, 247, 253
Encuesta Sociolingstica de Euskal Herria II
234, 240, 241, 247, 253
English
in Africa 285, 289-91, 292, 294, 302-4
and Ainu 333
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages 188,
367, 369, 381, 385-8
and Australian indigenous languages
393-4, 396, 398-400, 403-6, 408-11, 415-18,
420
in Basque Country 252, 253
and Dutch 217, 227-8
as international Language 266, 290
in Ireland 196, 202
in Israel 355, 357, 359, 360-1
as language of education 120-3, 131-2
and language law 115-16, 120-3, 127, 134
as language of work 124-6
and linguistic landscape 127-31
Index 489
and Maori 426-9, 431-8, 439, 442
in Montreal 118-20
and Navajo 24, 27, 28, 30-7, 458
and Oko 289-91
and Puerto Ricans in New York 44, 48,
50-3, 55-6, 59, 61-7, 69
and Quebec French 101, 105-6, 108-10,
113-14, 120-1, 457
Erdara language, use 246
Eskimo languages 187, 189
in Sweden 458
Espinoza Navarro, Faustino 172-3
Estonian, and RLS 267, 356
Ethiopia, and indigenous languages 286
ethnicity, and language 5
ethnocognition 4
ethnoculture see culture
ethnonationalism 17-18, 460, 480
Etxeberria, F. 253
Euromosiac Report (Nelde et al.) 262
European Cultural Foundation 217
European Union, and minority languages
12, 20, 217-18, 238, 263, 451, 458, 460,
476
Euskaltgeis (Basque teaching centres) 244-5
Euskara (Basque)
General Plan for Revitalisation 255
and GIDS 234, 248-56, 257-8
language shift reversal 229, 248-59, 479
language use 245-7, 246, 253
as mother tongue 238-40, 239, 240-1, 242,
245, 251-3, 257
as official language 235, 236, 249
psychosocial situation 247-57
as second language 243-4, 254
social situation 235-47
Euskararen Gizarte Eradundeen Kontseilua
256
Evangelical Church of Peru 176-7, 178
Ewe language (Ghana and Togo) 286
family
and Ainu 338
and Andamanese 313, 318, 319-20
and Australian immigrant languages
366-9, 373, 377, 389
and Australian indigenous languages 393,
417
and Catalan 268-9, 278
and Dutch 221, 231
and Euskara 240-1, 246, 251-3, 257
and Frisian 221, 221, 231
and Irish 203
and language choice 261-2
and Maori 424, 426, 427-31, 442-3, 447, 449
n.1
and Navajo 24, 31, 37-40
and Oko 293-4, 295-7, 301, 306, 307
and Otom 145-6
and Puerto Rican Spanish 54
and Quebec French 115, 122
and Quechua 168, 175-6, 186
and RLS 453, 458-60, 464, 466, 469-70, 475,
476-7
and Yiddish 83-4, 85, 86-8, 96-7
Farsi, in Australia 374, 381, 389-90 n.3
federalism, Canadian 106-7, 136
Feli language (Nigeria) 292
Fernadez, R. 47
festivals, and language transmission 295-7,
300, 302, 464
Fettes, M. 186-7
Finnish, in Sweden 458
Fishman, J. A. et al. 50
Fishman, Joshua
Reversing Language Shift 70, 126
and Australian immigrant languages
364-7
and Australian indigenous languages
391, 393, 395, 413-14
and Catalan 260, 264-7, 269, 274-5
and Euskara 234, 235, 250-1, 254-5, 256,
258
and Frisian 216, 223-7, 229, 230
and Hebrew 350, 352-6
and Irish 195-7, 212
and Maori 437-8, 442
and Navajo 24, 29-30, 40
and Otom 152-3, 154, 160, 162
and Quechua 172-6, 182-6
and RLS 1-22, 47, 101, 110, 260-1, 291, 306
see also Graded Intergenerational
Disruption Scale (GIDS)
Flaquer, V.L. 269-71, 272, 273-4
Flemish, in Australia 377
folklore, and language transmission 146,
172, 297, 337, 468
Folksbine (Yiddish theatre) 77-8
Fon-Ewe language (Benin) 286
Fortier, I. 102
Forward (Forverts) 77, 79, 92-3
Fox, D.T. 447
France
Basque Country 234, 235-8
and regional languages 267, 279, 356
Freeland, J. 189
Frine, San de 444
Freire, Paulo & Horton, Myles 41
490 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
French
and African languages 285-6, 304
in Australia 372, 374, 383, 387
and Catalan 279
and English 85, 457
and Euskara 236, 240, 245, 249-50
as international language 266, 457
in Israel 355, 356, 359
French, Quebec 101-41, 356, 457, 479
and group vitality analysis 101-13
as language of education 120-3, 126, 127-8,
132
and language planning 113-33
as language of work 123-6
and linguistic landscape 127-33
see also activism; bilingualism;
education; English; nationalism;
Quebec; transmission
Friesland
Covenant on language policy 215, 218
and language planning 215, 218-20
Mercator-Education project 218
Frisian
and effects of internationalisation 216-18
and GIDS 223-30
and language of administration 215, 219
and language of law 215, 219-20
and language law and policy 215, 218-20,
229-30
language shift reversal 215-33, 478
mother-tongue speakers 215-16, 220-2,
221, 225, 230, 231 n.1
as second language 221-2, 222, 225
and stable language relationships 220-3
as threatened language 220-1
see also activism; bilingualism; children;
community; Dutch; education; family
Front de Libration du Qubec 133
Fryske Akademy 218, 220, 226, 230, 231-2 n.2
Fujimura, 335
Fulfulde langage (Mali and Gambia) 286
function, language 2, 7-8, 21, 84-5, 171-2,
186
and compartmentalisation 11-12, 14, 50,
89, 168, 221, 231, 319, 332, 466
and education 14-16
and power 9-13, 17, 51, 110-11, 285-6, 287,
289-90, 466
prioritisation 8, 13-17, 84
Ga language (Ghana) 286
Gaeltacht Commission 195
Gaeltacht (Ireland) 195-6, 202-3, 208-9, 210
Gaetano, Caleb 449 n.1
Gale, M.A. 399
Galinier, J. 144, 146
Galla (Ethiopia) 286
Gallego (Spain) 457
Gambia, indigenous languages 286
Garrwa (Aboriginal language) 403-4
Gendron Commission, Quebec 102, 105-6,
108, 125
General Plan for the Normalisation of
Euskara 244, 255
General Plan for the Revitalisation of
Euskara 255
German
in Australia 366-7, 369, 374, 383, 387
in Israel 355, 356, 358
Ghana, and indigenous languages 286
GIDS see Graded Intergenerational
Disruption Scale (GIDS)
Giles, H. et al. 102, 110
globalisation
impact viii, 6-9, 17, 46, 85-6, 274, 344,
418-19, 456-7, 475, 476
and localisation 6, 455, 459, 460-1, 480
Goodman, Hershl 91, 92
Gordge, M. 448
Gorter, D. & Jonkman, R.J. 215-16, 219
government
and Ainu 341-2
and Andamanese 320
and Catalan 268
and Euskara 236, 256
and Frisian 228-9
and Hebrew 354
and Irish 196
and Maori 442
and Otom 154
and Quechua 183
government, local
and Ainu 341
and Catalan 275
and Euskara 235, 255-6
and Frisian 219
and Hebrew 354
and Maori 440
and Navajo 30, 36
and Oko 304
and Otom 155, 156
and Quechua 181-2
Graded Intergenerational Disruption Scale
(GIDS) 468-74
and Ainu 331-42
and Andamanese 319-20
and Australian immigrant languages
366-85, 388-9
Index 491
and Australian indigenous languages
392-4, 396-7, 412, 419-20
and Catalan 260, 268, 274-5
and Euskara (Basque) 234, 248-56, 257-8
and Frisian 223-30
and group vitality framework 110-11
and Hebrew 350-5
and international context 216
and Irish 195-6
and Maori 424-46
and Navajo 24, 29-40
and Oko 291-5, 307
and Otom 154-7, 163
priorities and linkages 465-8, 476, 481
and Puerto Rican Spanish 51, 70
and Quebec French 110-11, 112-13
and Quechua 171-85
and Yiddish 84-5, 96-7
grammar
Ainu 332, 333
Andamanese 314-15
Catalan 277
Jaru 408
Otom 155-6
Greek
in Australia 366-7, 369-70, 373-4, 376, 381,
383, 386, 389-90 n.3
in Quebec 116
Greenland, and Eskimo languages 187
Grenoble, L. & Whaley, L. 187, 188-9
Grimes, B.F. 170
Grin, F. & Vaillancourt, F. 437, 446
Grinevald, C. 182, 184
group vitality analysis
and demography -5, 102-7, 110-12, 113,
120-1, 133
and Euskara 236-8
and Graded Intergenerational Disruption
Scale 110-13
and institutional control 107-8, 110-12,
113, 117, 119, 241, 255-6
and Quebec French 101-13, 103
and status 108-10, 111-12, 119
and subjective vitality questionnaire
111-12
Grupo Artstico Cultural Saraguro 174
Grupo Intian 174
Grupo Rumiahui 174
Guatemala, and Mayan 187
Gudschinsky, S.C. 316
Gujarati, in New Zealand 430
Gumatj dialect 399
Gumbayngirr (Aboriginal language) 406-7
Gumperz, 187
Gurindji (Aboriginal language) 407
Guugu Yimidhirr (Aboriginal language)
188
Haboud, M.I. 166, 168, 176
Hadda, Janet 80, 88-9
Halpern, Moyshe Leyb 77
haredim
and Hebrew 360
and Yiddish 89-90
Harshav, B. 75, 353
Harwood, J. et al. 102, 112
Haugen, Einar 464
Hausa language (Nigeria) 286, 294, 302, 304
Haviland, 188
Hawaiian language, and university courses
190-1 n.14
hebraisms, in Yiddish 93
Hebrew 350-63, 457
and Arabic 354, 357, 359-60
and Aramaic 351-2, 357
in Australia 372-3
and GIDS 350-5
and other languages 355-61, 479-80
and practice, ideology and policy 355-9
in Quebec 132, 356
revitalisation 265-6, 350-5, 357, 361
as sacred language 351-3, 358, 368
and Yiddish 87, 93, 94, 356-7, 358-9
see also children; community
Hekking, E. 155
Herder, J. G. 8
Heritage languages, in Quebec 116-17,
120, 122-3, 134, 136
Hindi
and Andamanese 314-20
in Australia 386
in New Zealand 430
Hirago, Sadamo 345-6
Hohepa, M.K. 431
Hokkaido, and Ainu 326-32, 337, 341, 344
Hokkaido Ainu Association 324, 335, 337
Hokoto, Iboshi 330
Holland see Dutch; Friesland; Frisian
Holm, W. & Holm, A. 34
Holm, Wayne 31
Honda, Yoneda 333, 344
Hopi language 472
Hornberger, N.H. 168, 172, 177, 189-90 n.1,
191 nn.15,16
Huarani language (Andes) 170
Hulaul (Judeo-Aramaic), and Hebrew 356
Hulme, Keri 433
Hungarian, in Australia 374, 381
492 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
Hymes, D. 356
identity 4, 47-50, 54
and Ainu 324, 325, 344
and Andamanese 315, 318-19
and Australian immigrant languages 367
and Australian indigenous languages 411
and Catalan 262, 264-5, 269
and code-switching 50, 52
and Euskara 257
and Frisian 216-17
and globalisation 6-9, 85, 459-60
immigrant 20, 46, 64
and language 3-5, 17, 18, 20-1, 38, 452-3,
455-6, 473, 475
multicultural 119, 123, 134-5
national 216-17
and Oko 295-7, 298, 299-300, 301-2
and Otom 153, 159, 162
and Puerto Rican Spanish 46-50, 64-6
and Quebec French 113, 127, 136
and religion 82-4
and Yiddish 82-4, 85, 96
see also culture
ideology
and challenges to RLS 452-7
and language policy 355-9
and language recovery 248-50, 255-6, 361,
414, 451-2
and support for RLS 457-60
Igbo language (Nigeria) 286, 294, 302, 304
Iglesia Evangelica Peruana 176-7, 178
ikastolas (Basque schools) 243, 244, 252, 254
immersion programmes
in Australian immigrant languages 374-5
in Irish 204
in Maori 37, 428, 434-7, 440, 444
in Navajo 35, 37
in Quebec French 120-1
in Quechua 180
in Yiddish 81-2, 94-5
immigrants
to Australia 364-90, 479
to Canada 105-6, 111, 113, 114, 116-17, 118,
120-1, 122-3, 126-8, 131, 134-5
to Catalonia 262, 264, 269-70, 271, 276-7
to Israel 355, 357, 359-60
and language shift 105-6, 113, 114, 461-2
Oko-speaking 284
to United States 45, 53-5, 64, 75-6
Yiddish-speaking 75-6, 82-3
see also migration
Indonesian, in Australia 368, 375, 383, 386
Institut Culturel Basque/Euskal Kuktur
Erakundea 236
interaction, cultural
and Ainu 338-42
and Andamanese 319-20
and Australian immigrant languages
366-9
and Catalan 271
and Euskara 247, 250
and Frisian 221-3, 224-5
and Hebrew 354
and Irish 208-9, 211, 212, 444
and Maori 438-9, 444
and Navajo 25, 30
and Oko 293-4, 301, 303-4
and Otom 155, 156
and Quechua 173-5, 184
and RLS 469-74
and virtual community 458-60, 465
and Yiddish 83-4
interference, Spanish-Catalan 277-8
intermarriage 28
and Andamanese 315-16
and Australian immigrant languages
366-7, 369
and Australian indigenous languages 399,
408-9
and Catalan 262
and Frisian 225
and Oko 288, 290
and Quebec French 103, 111
internationalism, and RSL 19-20
internet
and Ainu 341
and Australian immigrant languages
383-5, 389
and Frisian 226, 229
and Quechua 183
as virtual community 458-60, 464
and Yiddish 78-9, 80-1, 89-90, 97
Inuit peoples, and nationhood 188
Iparralde
and Euskara 234
and language attitudes 247, 248, 250
and language policy 235-6, 249
and language recovery 251-3
and language transmission 241, 242, 243,
254
and language use 246
and linguistic competence 236, 237, 238,
239
and political parties 256
Ireland
and census data 197-202
and Gaeltacht 195-6, 202-3, 208-9, 210
Index 493
and language shift 197, 202-3
Irish
age distribution 199-200, 200, 201-2
and GIDS 195-6
in Northern Ireland 211-12
production and reproduction 195-214, 464
regional distribution 199, 199
and RLS 195-214, 267
see also bilingualism; children; class,
social; demography; education
Ishikari dialect, and Ainu 340
Israel
and Hebrew 265-6, 350-63, 457
and Language War 358
and other languages 355, 479-80
Italian
in Australia 366, 372, 374, 376, 381, 383,
386-7
in Quebec 116
Iutzi-Mitchell, R. & Graburn, N.H.H. 188
Japan
and language laws 327-31, 344
and linguistic minorities 325, 330, 340, 343
see also Ainu
Japanese
and Ainu 323, 328-9, 333, 337
in Australia 374, 386
Jarawa people, Andaman Islands 309, 312
Jaru (Aboriginal language) 407-8
Jews
in Australia 372-3
Lubavitcher 94-5
in Montreal 131-2
in New York City 75-99
secular 75-88, 89-91, 93-4, 96-8
ultra-Orthodox 78, 88-97, 98, 360, 368, 472,
479
see also Hebrew; Israel; Yiddish
Johnson, P. et al. 102
Jong, S. de & Riemersma, A.M.J. 227
Jou, Llus 272
Judeo-Yemenite, and Hebrew 356
Judezmo, and Hebrew 356, 480
Juhuri (Judeo-Tata), and Hebrew 356
Kabiye language (Togo) 286
Kalaw Kawaw Ya (Aboriginal language) 410
Kamba language (Kenya) 286
Kankannari, Matsu 338
Kaufman, T. 169
Kaurna (Aboriginal language) 411
Kayano, Shigeru 323, 324, 330-1, 337, 340, 342
Keating, Paul 376
Kenya, and indigenous languages 286, 304
Khmer, in Australian 374, 386
Kija (Aboriginal language) 412
Kikuyu language (Kenya) 286
Kimihiro, Daimyo Matsumae 327
Kindaichi, Kyosuke 338
King, K.A. 168
Kingi, M. 445
kinship, and language 5, 9, 475
Kirton, Jean 403-4
khanga-reo movement (New Zealand) 86,
423, 425-6, 430-1, 439, 440
Kongo language (Congo Kinshasa) 286
Korean
and Ainu 333
in Australia 381, 386, 389-90 n.3
Krauss, M. 187, 188, 350
Kriol (Australia) 400-1, 403, 405, 407, 412
Kudanyji (Aboriginal language) 403
Kura Kaupapa Maori 423, 434, 435-7
Kurdish, in Australia 370
Kwa languages 284
Ladino, and Hebrew 357, 361, 480
Lakoff, George 448
land ownership, and language 399-400, 409,
444
Landry, R. & Allard, R. 112
landscape, linguistic
in Basque Country 252-3
in Israel 360
in Navajo Nation 36
in Quebec 114, 127-31
language
and cognition 4-5
and culture 2-5, 17, 21, 40, 48-50, 64, 98,
162-3
functional diversification 2, 7-8, 9-13, 21,
84
Heritage 116-17, 120, 122-3, 134, 136
international 266, 290
official 103, 134, 163, 167, 183, 189 n.1, 219,
235-6, 249, 285-7
role 264-5
sacred 351-3, 358, 368, 373, 466, 471
stateless see Yiddish
Language Data Bank for Frisian 226
language shift
in Africa 284, 285-8
and Ainu 326-7
and Australian immigrant languages
364-8
and Australian indigenous languages 188,
394-5, 398-401
494 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
and Catalonia 265
and globalisation viii, 6
and group vitality analysis 102
and immigration 105
and language attitudes 287-8
as natural 454
and Otom 147-54
Puerto Rican 45-6, 50-2, 59-61, 63, 66-7
and Quechua 167-8, 187
reversal see Reversing Language Shift
(RLS)
Language Task Force (Center for Puerto
Rican Studies) 50
languages, threatened
in Africa 284, 285-8
difficulties of saving viii-ix, 1-22, 478-81
as second language 14-16
typology of 188-9
see also Basque; Catalan; Frisian; Irish;
Navajo; Oko; Otom; Quechua; Spanish;
Yiddish
Larreula, V. 269
Lastra, Y. 155, 157
Latinos, in New York 53-4, 55, 55, 58-64, 60,
61, 64
Latvia, and Russian 460
Latvian
in Australia 366, 370, 374, 388
and RLS 267
Laurin, Camille 114, 127
laws, language 133-6
and Ainu 327-31, 338, 344
and Catalan 272
and demolinguistic trends 115-17, 134
and education 120-3
and Euskara 235
and Frisian 215, 218-20, 229
and language status 101, 109, 113-33
leadership
and Frisian 230
and Otom 155
and Quebec French 107-8, 109, 113
and Quechua 178, 183-5
levelling, linguistic 55
Levesque, Rne 107, 127
Lewis, Glyn 212
lexicon
Ainu 333, 338
Andamanese 314-15, 316-17
Catalan 277
Maori 444-5
Oko 299
Quechua 172-3
liberalism, linguistic 274
libraries, RSL role 76, 91, 154, 370, 371-2,
373
Limburgish, in Netherlands 220
Linarte Martnez, Guillermo 156-7
Lingala language (Congo Kinshasa) 286
lingua franca
Aboriginal languages as 393, 399-401, 403,
409
English as 122
Nahuatl as 144
Quechua as 166
Linguistic Policy Act (Catalonia; 1998) 272,
274
linguistics, Frisian 230
Lira, Jorge 173
literacy
in Ainu 338-40, 341
in Andamanese 317, 320
in Australian immigrant languages
369-70, 408
in Australian indigenous languages 405,
410
in Catalan 275-6, 276, 278
critical 186
in English 396, 415-18, 420
in Euskara (Basque) 244-5, 251, 253-4
in Frisian 221
in Hebrew 352, 353, 354
in Hindi 317
in Irish 198
in Maori 431, 432-4
in Navajo 34-5, 38
in Oko 302
in Quechua 173, 176-8
and RLS 471-2
in Spanish 48, 66, 70
in Yiddish 91, 92
literature
Ainu 338
Frisian 224
Hebrew 352
Yiddish 80-1, 84, 86, 88-90, 96
loan words
in Catalan 278
in Mexican Spanish 163 n.1
in Otom 156
see also borrowings
localisation, and globalisation 6, 455, 459,
460-1, 480
Lomwe language (Malawi) 286
Long, N. 212
Low Saxon, in Netherlands 220, 229
loyalty, language 261, 270, 288, 350, 455
Luba language (Congo Kinshasa) 286
Index 495
Lubavitcher Jews 94-5
Luganda language (Uganda) 286
Lugbara language (Uganda) 286
Luhya language (Kenya) 286
Luo language (Uganda and Kenya) 286
Luykx, A. 185, 186
McCarty, Teresa 191 n.17
McConvell, P. 393, 396, 399, 412, 420
Macdonald, 342
Macedonian, in Australia 368, 374, 376, 381,
387-8, 389-90 n.3
McKay, G. 402-13
McLaren, Peter 23-4
Magongo village, and Oko/Osanyin 288
Maher, J. 326
maintenance, language
and education 66-7, 69, 156, 157, 320, 372,
375, 386, 417, 420
and identity 324
intergenerational 11, 46, 70, 110, 154,
162-3, 171, 385, 393, 410-11, 413
and language law 113-14, 136, 166-7, 235
and libraries 370, 371-2, 373
and migration 20, 66, 366-9
and religion 98, 408
and state role 212-13, 340
Malawi, and indigenous languages 286
Malay, in Australia 377
Malgana (Aboriginal language) 402
Mali, and indigenous languages 286
Maltese 457
in Australia 372, 381, 3367
Mandarin, in Australia 374-5, 383, 385, 386
Mandel, G. 357
Manding language (Gambia) 286
Manger Prize for Yiddish Literature 80-1
Mangue language (Nicaragua) 143
Mann, E.H. 310, 312
Mannheim, B. 167
Manoharan, S. 312, 314
Manx language 398
Maori
and GIDS 424-46
immersion programmes 37, 428, 434-7,
440, 444
khanga-reo model 86, 423, 425, 430-1,
439, 440
language shift reversal 353, 423-50, 478
as second language 430
see also children; community; English;
family; transmission
Maori Language Commission 423, 428, 431,
444-5
Mar, I. 264-5
market, language
in Australia 375
in Catalonia 270
in Ireland 196, 212-13
Marra (Aboriginal language) 403
marriage, mixed see intermarriage
Mateira, Katerina 449 n.1
materials, teaching 15, 92-3, 156, 161-2, 320,
407-8, 431, 433, 472-3
Matlatzinca-Ocuilteco languages (Mexico)
142
Mayan languages 187, 189
media
and Ainu 330, 331, 332, 341
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages 370,
376-7, 378-81, 381-5, 382, 384-5, 389
and Australian indigenous languages 410
and Catalan 262-3, 271, 275
and Euskara 251, 256
and Frisian 218, 225, 228-9
and Hebrew 354
and Irish 201-2, 210
and Maori 429, 431-2, 440-1, 442, 444, 447
and Navajo 36, 40
and Nigerian languages 304
and Otom 155-6, 160, 162
and Puerto Rican Spanish 44-5, 54, 64
and Quebec French 108, 110, 134
and Quechua 178, 181-2, 183
and RLS 14, 473-4, 482 n.5
and Yiddish 76, 81, 83, 85, 92-3, 99-100 n.9
Mercator-Education project (Friesland) 218
Mervio, M. 325
Mexica (Aztecs) 142-4
Mexico
and Chiapas rebellion 158
language policy 160-2
and Mayan 187
as pluricultural state 146
Spanish conquest 144-5
War of Independence 145
see also Nahuatl; Otom
migration
and Australian indigenous languages 188
and Frisian 221, 223-4, 225
and Irish 208-9
and language maintenance 20, 307
and language shift 17
and Oko 289, 291, 293, 297, 300
and Otom 146, 153, 159
and Quechua 181
see also immigrants
496 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
Miles, Nelson 47
Mixtec (Mexico) 142
modernisation
and Ainu 333
and Catalan 270
and Euskara 250-1
and Maori 443, 444
and RSL 6-7
Mondragn Grupo 255
Montaignais, in Quebec 186
Montaluisa, L.O. 169
Montoya-Abat, B. 267, 269
Montreal
and language law 118-20, 123, 124, 127,
130-2
and multiculturalism 119, 123, 134-5
Moore, H. 418
Morelli, Br Steve 406
Morgan, William 29
morphology
Andamanese 314
Otom 157
mother tongue
and education in threatened language 14,
16, 302-3
transmission 9, 14, 16, 105, 154, 238-40,
239, 465-74
Mulhausler, 412
multiculturalism 17-18, 457-8, 460, 465
in Australia 368, 373, 374, 376, 383, 387-8,
417
in Japan 325-6, 342-3
in Mexico 146
in Montreal 119, 123, 134-5
multilingualism 17-18
in African states 287, 301
in Australia 376-7, 381, 417, 419
in Basque Country 249-50, 252
in Catalonia 263
in Israel 357, 359, 360-1
in Japan 325-6, 342
in Quebec 118
Murrinh-Patha (Aboriginal language) 399
Nadler, Aaron 91, 92
Nahuatl language (Mexico) 143, 144-5, 155,
163 n.1
Nakagawa, H. 332, 334
Nakasone, Yasuhiro 325, 330
naming
in Aboriginal languages 400
in Japanese 328
in Oko 299-300
National Aboriginal Languages Programme
414, 420
National Indigenist Institute (INA), Mexico
160
National Languages and Literacy Strategy
(Australia) 415-16
National University of Ireland 205, 208, 211
National Yiddish Book Center 78, 91
nationalism
Catalan 273
civil 460
and Hebrew 352-3, 357, 358
Northern Irish 211
Quebec 105, 106, 107-8, 133-4
and RLS viii-ix, 17-21, 261
and Yiddish 352
nationhood
and Inuit 188
and Quechua 188-9
and Yiddish 85, 98-9
Native American Church 27, 30, 33
Native Americans
and Puerto Ricans 45, 48, 49
see also Navajo language; Navajo Nation
Navajo language
and cultural interaction 30
and GIDS 29-36, 37-40
and language demographics 28
present status 25-9
and RLS 23-43, 357
see also bilingualism; community;
education; English; family; government,
local; transmission, intergenerational
Navajo Nation
education 27-8, 32
income and employment 26-7
land base 25-6
religious beliefs 27, 30, 33
subsistence 26-7
Navarrese Autonomous Community
(NAC) 234
and language attitudes 247-8, 248, 249
and language policy 235-6, 241, 249
and language recovery 250-3
and language transmission 240-1, 242,
243-4, 251, 254
and language use 246
and linguistic competence 236, 237, 238,
239
and political parties 255-6
Nelde, P. et al. 262
Netherlands see Dutch; Friesland; Frisian
networks, social
and Euskara 247
and Frisian 224-5
Index 497
and Irish 202, 208-9, 211, 212
New York
Dominican community 62-4, 66, 67, 69
Latino communities 53-4, 55, 55, 58, 59-64,
60, 61
Puerto Rican community 52-64, 60, 61
and Puerto Rican Spanish 44-73
and salsa 54-5
and Yiddish 74-100
New Zealand 423-50
Ngardi (Aboriginal language) 407
Ngarlawankga (Aboriginal language) 402
Nigam, R.C. 312
Nigeria
and Ebira language 290-1, 301
and Hausa language 286, 294, 302, 304
and Igbo language 286, 294, 302, 304
and Oko language 284-308
and threatened languages 292-3
and Yoruba language 286, 289, 291, 294,
299-300, 301-2, 304
Ninyoles, Rafael-Llus 265
Nomura, Giichi 325
Noongar (Aboriginal language) 411-12
North American Free Trade Agreement
(NAFTA) 126
Northern Ireland, and Irish 211-12
Nyanja language (Malawi) 286
Nzema language (Ghana) 286
Riagin, P. 195, 203
Occitan, and intergenerational transmission
267, 279, 356
Office de la langue franaise (OLF) 124,
130-2
Official Languages Act (1969) 103, 134
Ogben esen ceremony (Oko) 296-7
Ogbu, J.U. 45
Ogori see Oko
Ogori Descendants Union (ODU; Nigeria)
297, 298, 301
Ohiri-Aniche, C. 292-3
Oko
and corpus planning 298
and GIDS 291-5, 307
language shift reversal 284-308
mother-tongue speakers 288
sociolinguistic context 288-91
strategies for non-retentive sub-groups
295-305
as written language 298-9
and Yoruba 289, 291, 299-300, 301
see also children; community; education;
family; identity; transmission
Oko (Ogori) village 288, 294
Okuda, 335, 345
Onges people, Andaman Islands 309, 312
Oromo, in Australia 377
orthography
Navajo 29
Oko 298, 302
Otom 161
Yiddish 78, 81, 89, 93, 99 n.4
Osanyin see Oko
Oto-Pamean language family (Mexico) 142,
158
Otom
and cultural interaction 155, 156
and GIDS 154-7, 163
historical background 142-6
and intergenerational transmission 154,
157-8, 162-3
and language shift 147-54, 163
language shift reversal 142-65
orthography 161
reconstruction stage 154, 156
see also culture; education; family;
religion; status
Otom-Mazahua languages (Mexico) 142
Ovia Osese Festival 295-6, 302
Pacific Pidgin English 409
Pame-Chichimeco langauges (Mexico) 142
Pandit, T.N. 309
parallelism, institutional, in Ireland 210
Parsons-Yazzie, Evangeline 31
Parti Qubcois 110, 115, 127-33
Pedraza, P. 50
Pedraza, P. et al. 50
Pemon language (Venezuela) 340
pentecostal church, and Puerto Rican
community 69-70
Pere, R.T.R. 438
Peru
official languages 183
and Quechua 167-8, 169, 170-1, 172-3,
176-8, 181-3, 189, 189 n.1
and Spanish 167-8, 183
Pewhairangi, Ngoi 449 n.1
phonetics, Catalan 277
phonology
Otom 155-6, 157
Quechua 169, 190 n.3
Pietersen, L. 232 n.7
Pitjantjatjara (Aboriginal language) 412
planning, language
in Australia 413, 416
in Basque Country 235, 245-7, 248, 250-1,
498 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
255, 257-8
in Catalonia 260, 261, 264-74, 279-80
in Friesland 215
and GIDS 196, 268
and Maori 443-4
in Nigeria 298
in Quebec 101, 105, 110-11, 113-33, 255,
280
and Yiddish 99 n.4
Platero, Paul 31
playgroups, and Frisian 227
Plourde, M. 270
policy, language
and Australian immigrant languages 364,
385-7
and Australian indigenous languages 395,
402, 405, 413-20
in Basque Country 235-6, 240-1, 244-7,
249-50, 257-8
in Catalonia 265-6, 267, 268-9, 271-4, 280
in Friesland 215, 218-20, 229-30
in Ireland 196-7, 202, 204, 206-8, 209-13
in Israel 355-9, 360-1
in Mexico 153, 160-2
in New Zealand 431-2, 440, 443-4
no policy policies 454
RLS as 355-6
in South America 167, 183
see also laws, language; planning,
language
Polish, in Australia 367, 381
politics
and Catalan 263
and Euskara recovery 255-6
and Maori 428-9
and Oko 301-2
Pon, 336
Poplack, S. 50
Portuguese
and African languages 285-6, 304
in Australia 381, 389-90 n.3
in Quebec 116
poverty, Puerto Rican 56-62, 66
power
and language 24, 127, 166, 181, 223, 230-1,
236-8, 258, 284, 356, 393-6
and language function 10-12, 17, 51,
110-11, 285-6, 287, 289-90, 466
Prats, M. 265
Prats, M. et al. 266, 271
pre-school
and Arabic 360
and Euskara 252, 253
and Frisian 227
and Maori 86, 423, 425-6, 430-1
and Navajo 31
in Quebec 120
and RLS 16, 473-4
and Yiddish 86
production, bilingual 204-6
Programa de Formacon en Educacon
Intercultural Bilinge para los Paises
Andinos (PROEIB Andes) 189
publishing
Ainu 338-40
in Australian immigrant languages 376
Euskara 251
Maori 433-4
Oko 299
Quebec French 108
Quechua 177
Yiddish 76, 91, 92-3, 97
Puerto Rico
and colonialism 45-6, 47-50, 51-2, 61, 66
and Spanish 48-9, 65, 480
Quebec
and Civil Administration 123
and group vitality analysis 102-13
and language planning 113-33
and Montaignais 186
and multilingualism 118-19
and reversal of language shift 101-41, 356,
457
and separatism 136, 479-80
Quechua
and cultural interaction 173-5, 189
geographic diversity 169, 170, 187, 188-9
and GIDS 171-85
language shift reversal 171-2, 183-9, 472
as official language of Peru 183
and reconstruction efforts 172-3, 189
sociolinguistic diversity 169, 170-1, 172,
187, 189
as threatened language 166-8, 187
varieties 167, 168-71, 187, 189
see also bilingualism; children;
community; education; family; literacy;
status; transmission
Quechua-Yauyos 169-70, 171
Quichua Unificado 169
racialisation, and language 52, 61, 71
Radio Frysln 228
Raidio na Gaeltachta 210
reconstruction/recovery stage 468-9
and Ainu 330-1, 332-7, 342
and Australian indigenous languages 396,
Index 499
401-2, 406-7, 411-13, 419
and Catalan 260, 268, 274
and Euskara 235-6, 240, 248-51, 253-4,
256-7
and Frisian 224
and Maori 424-6, 446, 447-8
and Navajo 29
and Oko 292-3
and Otom 154, 156
and Quechua 172-3, 189
Reform party, Quebec 136
religion
and Ainu 334-5
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages
372-3
and Australian indigenous languages 408,
410-11
and Frisian 225
and Navajo language 27, 30, 33
and Oko 289, 290, 298
and Otom 146, 157, 160, 162
and Puerto Rican Spanish 69
and Quebec French 108
and Quechua 177
and Yiddish 82-4, 88-90
Report of the Quebec Human Rights
Commission 123
reproduction
bilingual 202-3, 208-9
and language choice 261-2
Reversing Language Shift (RLS)
Australian immigrant languages 364-90
Australian indigenous languages 391-422
biological challenge to 309-22
and civil nationalism 17-21
ideological challenges to 452-7
ideological foundation 451-2
ideological support for 457-60
increased interest in vii-viii, 70
individual and community activities 37,
38, 39-40, 39
principles and states 13-17
priorities and linkages 13-17, 465-8, 476,
481
review 451-81
and survival strategies 294-306, 474-7
theoretical basis viii, 16, 260-1
value-based general theory 462-5
see also Ainu; Andamanese; Catalan;
Euskara; French; Frisian; Hebrew;
Maori; Navajo; Oko; Otom; Quechua;
Spanish; Yiddish
Rhydwen, M. 402
ritual, Ainu 334-5
RLS see Reversing Language Shift (RLS)
Roback, A.A. 99 n.3
Roman, J.-M. & Strubell, M. 278
Romani (Romany)
in Australia 377
in Netherlands 220
in Sweden 458
Roumania, and minority languages 452
Royal Commission on Bilingualism and
Biculturalism, Canada 102, 107-8
Runyankore language (Uganda) 286
rupture, and language loss 394
Russia, and Eskimo languages 187
Russian
and Ainu 329
in Australia 369-70, 374, 381, 383
and Hebrew 357, 360-1
in Latvia 460
Sachdev, I. & Bourhis, R.Y. 113
Sahagn, Fray Bernardino de 144
Salmond, J. 425
Snchez-Korrol, V. 52
Saraguros
and education 179-80
and language transmission 174-6
Sasamura, J. 326
Saville-Troika, M. 190 n.6
Sawai, H. 337, 338
Schiffman, H.F. 414
Schmidt, A. 394-5, 396-7, 413
Schmidt, Alexander 218
Sentinel people, Andaman Islands 309, 312
Serbian, in Australia 381
Shaefer, Ron 292
Shirama language (Nigeria) 292
Siddle, R. 325, 327
Slovakia, and minority languages 452
social change theory 462-3
Sol, J.-R. 274
Solomon, Hyman 75
Somali
in Australia 370, 377
in Somalia 304
song in language transmission 295-7, 334,
369, 397, 404, 407
and Otom 146, 162
and Quechua 172
South America see Quechua
Spain
and Basque 234, 235-8, 249, 475
and Catalan 261, 475
conquest of Mexico 143-5
500 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
conquest of South America 166
and regional languages 195
Spanish
in Australia 367, 374-5, 381, 383, 385, 386-7
and Catalan 264, 266, 269-71, 273-4, 277-8
dialects 51, 55
and Euskara 235-6, 240-1, 245, 249-50,
252-3
and GIDS 51
and identity 64-6
and literacy 66, 70
loan words in Otom 156
in Mexico 145-6, 152-4, 159-60, 162-3, 163
n.1
and New York Dominicans 63, 66
and New York Puerto Ricans 44-73, 479
as official language 249
in Puerto Rico 48-9, 65, 480
in Quebec 116-17
and Quechua 166-8, 170-1, 174-6, 183
and RLS 67-8
speech community see community
spelling see orthography
Spolsky, Bernard 31
standardisation, language
and Irish 196
and Yiddish 352
status, language
of African languages 285-6
of Andamanese 317, 318
of Australian indigenous languages 395-6,
400, 404, 411
of Catalan 260, 267, 270, 279-80
of English 360, 411
of Euskara 235-6, 244, 250-1
of Frisian 218
of Hindi 317
of Irish 211
and language laws 101, 109, 113-33, 268
of Maori 433, 443
of Otom 153, 158, 159, 163
of Quebec French 101, 108-10, 111-12, 134
of Quechua 166-7, 181, 183, 187
of Spanish 166
see also power
Stewart, W. 190 n.2, 350, 356
stigmatisation, and language use 52, 71,
181, 412
Strubell i Trueta, M. 279
Summer Institute for Linguistics (SIL)
169-70
Sunasawa, Kura 340
Suskind, Nathan 99 n.3
Swahili (Kenya) 286
Sweden, minority languages 458, 481
syntax, Catalan 277
Tagalog, in Australia 372, 381
Tanzania, and Kiswahili 304
Te Aho Matua (New Zealand) 435-6
Te Taura Whiri i te Reo (Maori Language
Commission) 423, 428
teacher training 158, 161, 437, 473
teachers
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages 386,
388
and Australian indigenous languages
405-6, 408
and Maori 428, 430-1, 436-7
and Navajo 35, 37
and Otom 146, 153, 155, 157, 158, 161
and Puerto Rican Spanish 67-8
and Quechua 179
role in RLS 15-16, 473
teenagers see youth
Temple, Col. 312
Thai, in Australia 367
theatre
Frisian 218, 224
Yiddish 76, 77-8, 83, 96, 97
Tigrinya (Ethiopia) 286
Tiwi (Aboriginal language) 400
Togo, and indigenous languages 286, 304
Tomei, J. 188
Torres, J. 268
Torres, Lourdes 52
Torres Strait Creole 400, 409-10
Totonac language (Mexico) 143
tourism
and Ainu 325, 342
and Otom 155
and Quechua 181
Tovey, H. 211
transmission, intergenerational 9, 14, 16,
134, 465-74
of Ainu 332, 344-5
of Andamanese 320-1
of Australian immigrant languages
366-85, 388-9
of Australian indigenous languages 392-3,
394, 397-9, 410-12
of Catalan 261-4, 266-7, 275
of English 105, 116
of Euskara (Basque) 238-40, 239, 240-4,
242, 245, 248, 250, 251-4, 257
of Frisian 225, 230
of Hebrew 351, 354
Index 501
of Irish 196-7
and Maori 424, 426-46
of Navajo 24, 29, 30-3, 37-40
of Oko 284, 291-5, 300, 307
of Otom 154, 157-8, 162-3
of Quebec French 105, 110-12, 113, 115
of Quechua 168, 171, 173, 175-6, 185-7
and virtual community 458-60
of Yiddish 86-7, 91, 94-5, 97
Tras Monge, J. 49
trilingualism
in Friesland 227-8
in Quebec 122
Trudeau, Pierre-Elliot 107
Tsunemoto, T. 326
Turkish, in Australia 374, 376, 381, 387,
389-90 n.3
Udal Euskal Unibertsitatea (Basque
Summer University) 244
Uganda, and indigenous languages 286, 304
Ukranian, in Australia 370, 374
UN Covenant on Civil and Political Rights
326, 330, 331, 340
Unamuno, Miguel de 448
United States
and Hebrew 361
and Puerto Rico 45-6, 47-50
and Spanish 44-73, 458, 462
see also New York
universities
and Ainu 327, 341
and Australian immigrant languages 386
and Australian indigenous languages 412
and Euskara 243-4, 251
and Frisian 230
and Hawaiian 190-1 n.14
and Hebrew 359
and Irish 205
and Maori 442
in Quebec 120, 121-3
and Quechua 182, 189
and Yiddish courses 77, 82, 99 n.3, 359
Urciuoli, B. 52, 58, 69, 71
Urdu, in New Zealand 430
Ussishkin, 358-9
vaivn, linguistic 46-7, 51-2, 55-6, 63, 64-5,
66, 71
Valencia, and Catalan 275, 277-8
Vallire, P. 113
Vallverd, F. 269, 271
varieties, language
and Australian indigenous languages 400
and Quechua 167, 168-71, 187
Venezuela, and Pemon language 340
Victorian School of Languages (Australia)
370, 374
Vietnamese, in Australia 366, 367-8, 372,
374, 381, 386-7
Vila, 278
vocabulary see lexicon
Volinsky, N. 174
Voltas, E. 269, 273
Von Gleich, U. 167, 169
Waitangi treaty 439
Warlpiri (Aboriginal language) 400, 407
Weinreich, Max 99 n.3
Weinreich, Uriel 77, 99 n.3
Welsh, and RLS 267
Williams, N. 399-400
Wisse, Ruth 80, 89
Wittkes, Reuven 94-5
Wolof language (Gambia) 286
Workmans Circle, New York 76, 79, 82
workplace
and Ainu 341
and Andamanese 320
and Australian immigrant languages
375-6
and Catalan 275
and Euskara 254-5
and Frisian 221, 228
and Hebrew 354
and Irish 202, 207-8
and language law 114, 123-6, 134
and Maori 438-9
and Navajo 36
and Oko 303-4
and Otom 155, 156
and Quebec French 108
and Quechua 181
and RSL 261-2, 473
and Yiddish 96-7
Yaffe, Judah 99 n.3
Yanagi, 343
Yanyuwa (Aboriginal language) 403-4
Yao language, in Malawi 286
Yeshivish 100 n.10
Yiddish
in Australia 368-9, 373
and GIDS 84-5, 96-7
and Hebrew 87, 93, 94, 356-7, 358-9, 361
and nationalism 352
in Netherlands 220
in New York City 74-100
502 Can Threatened Languages be Saved?
Index 503
in Quebec 131-2
secular speakers 75-88, 89-91, 93-4, 96-7,
369
as stateless language 85, 98-9
in Sweden 458
threat to 75
ultra-Orthodox speakers 78, 88-97, 98, 360,
368, 472, 479
see also children; community; culture;
education; family; literature;
transmission
Yiddishists 79, 99 n.6
Yiddishvokh 81-2
Yindjibarndi (Aboriginal language) 400
YIVO 91
Institute for Jewish Research 76
Seminar in Yiddish 82
and Unified Yiddish Spelling 78, 81, 93
Uriel Weinreich Summer Program 77, 79
Yokoyama, T. & Chiri, M. 333
Yolngu Matha (Aboriginal language) 398-9
Yoruba language
in Benin 286
in Nigeria 286, 289, 291, 294, 299-300,
301-2, 304
Yoshida, Iwao 332
Young, Robert 29
youths
and Andamanese 316
and Australian immigrant languages 377
and Catalan 269-70, 275-6
and Euskara 252
and Navajo 27, 29, 31-3, 36, 38
and Oko 289, 290-1, 297, 307
in Quebec 270
and Quechua 173-5
and RLS 15
and Yiddish 76, 78, 81
see also children
Ytsma, J. 227
Yungtruf (Yiddish youth group) 76, 78, 81
Zapotec (Mexico) 142
Zentella, A.C. 50-1, 54, 55, 56, 64, 66, 70