ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 25 November 2016
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01859
Implicit Detection of Poetic Harmony
by the Naïve Brain
Awel Vaughan-Evans 1 , Robat Trefor 2 , Llion Jones 3 , Peredur Lynch 2 , Manon W. Jones 1
and Guillaume Thierry 1*
1
School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, UK, 2 School of Welsh, Bangor University, Bangor, UK, 3 Canolfan
Bedwyr, Bangor University, Bangor, UK
Edited by:
F-Xavier Alario,
Centre National de la Recherche
Scientifique, France
Reviewed by:
Arthur M. Jacobs,
Free University of Berlin, Germany
Franck Vidal,
Aix-Marseille University, France
*Correspondence:
Guillaume Thierry
[email protected]
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Language Sciences,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 21 May 2016
Accepted: 09 November 2016
Published: 25 November 2016
Citation:
Vaughan-Evans A, Trefor R, Jones L,
Lynch P, Jones MW and Thierry G
(2016) Implicit Detection of Poetic
Harmony by the Naïve Brain.
Front. Psychol. 7:1859.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01859
The power of poetry is universally acknowledged, but it is debatable whether its
appreciation is reserved for experts. Here, we show that readers with no particular
knowledge of a traditional form of Welsh poetry unconsciously distinguish phrases
conforming to its complex poetic construction rules from those that violate them.
We studied the brain response of native speakers of Welsh as they read meaningful
sentences ending in a word that either complied with strict poetic construction rules,
violated rules of consonantal repetition, violated stress pattern, or violated both these
constraints. Upon reading the last word of each sentence, participants indicated
sentence acceptability. As expected, our inexperienced participants did not explicitly
distinguish between sentences that conformed to the poetic rules from those that
violated them. However, in the case of orthodox sentences, the critical word elicited
a distinctive brain response characteristic of target detection –the P3b– as compared to
the other conditions, showing that speakers of Welsh with no expertise of this particular
form of poetry implicitly detect poetic harmony. These results show for the first time that
before we even consider literal meaning, the musical properties of poetry speak to the
human mind in ways that escape consciousness.
Keywords: language, neuroaesthetics, poetry, event-related potentials, P3b
INTRODUCTION
T. S. Eliot famously argued that “genuine poetry can communicate before it is understood”
(Scofield, 1988; p. 2). Was this an attempt to provoke controversy or can some aspects of poetry
indeed be processed implicitly and independently of meaning? Poetry is a literary expression
of feelings, thoughts, and ideas, traditionally accentuated by metric constraints, rhyme, and
alliteration. Recent scientific research looking into the effects of poetry has highlighted emotional
responses to rhyme (Obermeier et al., 2013) and better memory recall as a result of alliteration
(Hanauer, 2001; Lea et al., 2008). Rhyme violations, in particular, have been shown to increase
pupillary responses (Scheepers et al., 2013) and modulate the amplitude of the N400, a brain
potential index of semantic processing (Hoorn, 1996). Whilst there is little doubt that some poetic
forms, often centuries old, impact human cognition (see Jacobs, 2015, for a recent review), we
have yet to discover the extent to which such sensitivity may rely on automatic and implicit neural
processing.
Here, we investigated event-related brain potentials (ERPs) elicited by the final word of
sentences written in Cynghanedd (‘harmony’ in Welsh), an ancient poetic form that requires
precise consonantal repetition (and/or internal rhyme) in conjunction with distinct stress patterns
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
1
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
they had too few uncontaminated epochs per condition; and
a further four participants were removed as a result of overall
excessive noise in the data. All participants possessed normal or
corrected-to-normal vision. Ethical approval was granted by the
School of Psychology, Bangor University ethics committee, and
participants gave written consent before the experiment session
started.
(Greene, 2012). In certain sub-types of Cynghanedd, consonants
are repeated across the first and second parts of the line, and are
always in the same order: A daeth i ben | deithio byd (‘And it
came to an end | traveling the world,’ as cited in Llwyd, 2010,
critical consonants in bold). A line such as ∗ A daeth i ben | deithio
cwm (‘And it came to an end | traveling the valley’) features a
‘c’ rather than a ‘b,’ which constitutes a consonantal repetition
violation. Traditional Cynghanedd rules also dictate a precise
stress pattern: Ein lluniaeth | a’n llawenydd (‘Our sustenance and
joy,’ Llwyd, 2010, stress vowels underlined and critical consonants
in bold). In contrast, the line ∗ Ein lluniaeth | a’n llu newydd (‘Our
sustenance and new host’) violates traditional rules because ‘n’ in
part one comes after the stress, but ‘n’ in part two precedes the
final stress. Cynghanedd sentences thus consist of foregrounding
features at the sublexical (phonological salience) and lexical
(stress pattern) levels (Jacobs, 2015). Each of these features is
known to independently influence aesthetic appreciation (e.g.,
Aryani et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2016), but their interactive
effect is unclear. In the present investigation, test sentences were
constructed which either adhered to the rules of Cynghanedd, or
violated its rules in terms of consonantal repetition, stress pattern,
or both consonantal repetition and stress pattern (Table 1). Each
condition was pseudo-randomly presented in equal proportion,
resulting in an oddball paradigm with Cynghanedd-orthodox
sentences occurring only 25% of the time.
The P3b is an ERP component commonly observed during
oddball paradigms thought to reflect a context-updating process
whereby a comparison is made between the currently processed
stimulus, and the previous representation held in working
memory (see Polich, 2007, for a review). We anticipated that
participants would show greater P3b amplitudes when singling
out the infrequent target combination of consonantal repetition
and stress pattern conforming to Cynghanedd from the other
three non-Cynghanedd conditions. We were keen to know,
however, whether such potential detection of the Cynghaneddorthodox targets would be accompanied by signs of conscious
evaluation as indexed by behavioral data and at debriefing.
Stimuli and Procedure
Experimental sentences belonged to 36 sets each consisting of
four sentences, resulting in a total of 144 sentences. Twentyfive percent of the experimental sentences followed the rules of
Cynghanedd whilst the remaining 75% violated the Cynghanedd
rules in terms of consonantal repetition (25%), stress pattern
(25%), or both consonantal repetition and stress pattern (25%; see
Table 1). The experiment thus conformed to a classical oddball
paradigm with Cynghanedd as the target condition. Where
possible, sentence final words were rotated across conditions.
However, due to the strict rules of Cynghanedd, it was not
possible to fully rotate all items between conditions. Word
frequency (from the Cronfa Electroneg o Gymraeg; Ellis et al.,
2001) and length did not differ significantly between conditions
[F(3,140) = 1.86, p = 0.14; F(3,140) = 0.76, p = 0.52].
Participants viewed all 144 sentences in three sections,
segmented such that they adhered to the natural rhythm of
the Cynghanedd line, with the final, critical word presented in
isolation. On each trial, the first two segments were presented
for 500 ms each, with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 300 ms.
A varying ISI (ranging between 400 and 700 ms) was used
between the second segment and the sentence final word,
which remained on the screen for a maximum of 2000 ms,
or until a response was made, whichever was the shortest
(Figure 1). Presentation order was pseudorandomized, such that
sentences from the same sentence set never appeared in the
same experimental block. Upon presentation of the final word,
participants were asked to indicate as quickly and as accurately as
possible, whether or not the sentence sounded ‘good’ by pressing
designated buttons on a serial response box. Upon completion
of the experimental task, participants were presented with a list
of the 36 sentence sets and were asked to rank the sentences in
each set in a decreasing order of preference (1 = most preferred;
4 = least preferred).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
Twenty-five fluent native speakers of Welsh (9 males; 16 females),
with no prior knowledge of the rules of Cynghanedd, were
included in the analysis. Of the initial participant pool, one
participant was excluded due to prior knowledge of Cynghanedd
and its underlying rules; two participants were excluded as
ERP Recording
Electrophysiological data was recorded from 32 Ag/AgCl
electrodes set according to the extended 10–20 convention
TABLE 1 | Experimental conditions.
Sentence
Rule adherence
Condition label
Y geiriau brwd ger y bryn
Consonantal repetition+Stress pattern+
Cynghanedd
Y geiriau brwd ger y bont
Consonantal repetition−Stress pattern+
Consonantal violation
Y geiriau brwd ger y border
Consonantal repetition+Stress pattern−
Stress violation
Y geiriau brwd ger y clawdd
Consonantal repetition−Stress pattern−
Double violation
English translation: The fervent words near the hill/trees/border/bank.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
2
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
FIGURE 1 | Structure of an experiment trial and response required from participants.
and maximal sensitivity was verified by inspecting the global field
power produced across the scalp in the Cynghanedd condition.
P3b mean amplitudes were analyzed by means of a one-way
repeated measures ANOVA with ‘Sentence Type’ (Cynghanedd,
Consonantal violation, Stress violation, Double violation) as
an independent variable. Post hoc tests were conducted using
Bonferroni corrections.
at a rate of 1 kHz in reference to the left mastoid. The
electroencephalogram (EEG) activity was filtered online with
a band-pass filter between 0.1 and 200 Hz and again offline
with a band-pass zero-phase shift filter set between 0.1 and
20 Hz. Eye blink artifacts were modeled and mathematically
corrected (Gratton et al., 1983) and remaining artifacts were
removed manually. Epochs ranging from −100 to 1,000 ms after
the onset of the target word were extracted from continuous
EEG recordings. Epochs with activity exceeding ±75 µV at
any electrode site were automatically discarded. There was a
minimum of 30 epochs per condition for every participant.
Baseline correction was performed in reference to pre-stimulus
activity, and individual averages were digitally re-referenced to
the algebraic mean of the left and right mastoids.
RESULTS
Behavioral Results
Online Categorization Task
We found a significant main effect of Sentence Type;
F(3,72) = 8.63, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.26 (Figure 2A.). Pairwise
comparisons revealed that Cynghanedd sentences were
more likely to be categorized as ‘good’ (M = 65%; 95% CI
[60, 70]) compared with Consonantal violation sentences
(M = 58%; 95% CI [51, 65]; p = 0.005) and Stress violation
sentences (M = 55%; 95% CI [50, 60], p < 0.001), but not
Double violation sentences (M = 63%; 95% CI [58, 69],
p = 0.38). Furthermore, Double violation sentences were
more likely to be categorized as ‘good’ than Consonantal
violation sentences (p = 0.04) and Stress violation sentences
(p = 0.001). Comparisons of categorization score against
chance revealed that responses significantly differed from
chance for Cynghanedd, the Double violation condition,
and the Consonantal violation condition [t(24) = 2.325,
p = 0.029], but not the Stress violation condition [t(24) = 1.905,
p = 0.069]. Critically, whereas the greater than chance
performance in the Cynghanedd condition was felicitous
Data Analysis
For the online categorization task, the percentage of ‘good’
responses was analyzed by means of a one-way repeated
measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with ‘Sentence Type’
(Cynghanedd, Consonantal violation, Stress violation, Double
violation) as an independent variable. Reaction times were
analyzed by means of a 2 (Categorization: ‘good,’ ‘not good’) × 4
(Sentence Type: Cynghanedd, Consonantal violation, Stress
violation, Double violation) repeated measures ANOVA.
For the offline ranking task, responses were scored such
that they were given a 1 if they correctly ranked Cynghanedd
sentences as the ‘best’ sentence, and a 0 if they did not. Responses
were then analyzed by means of a one-sample t-test.
For the ERP data, P3b mean amplitude was predictively
extracted between 240 and 340 ms at six electrodes where the P3b
is known to be maximal in amplitude (CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, P4)
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
3
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
Electrophysiological Data
We found a significant main effect of Sentence Type;
F(3,72) = 3.149, p = 0.03, η2p = 0.12; with Cynghanedd
sentences eliciting greater mean amplitudes (M = 5.93, 95% CI
[4.86, 7.01) than Consonantal violation sentences (M = 5.01,
95% CI [3.92, 6.10]; p = 0.01), Stress violation sentences
(M = 4.88, 95% CI [3.58, 6.17]; p = 0.002), and Double
violation sentences (M = 5.00, 95% CI [3.90, 6.09]; p = 0.007),
respectively (Figure 3). Analyses in earlier time windows (P1
and N1) did not show any significant differences as a result
of the experimental conditions. As expected the distribution
of the effect was centroparietal (Figure 4). Furthermore, the
topographic maps show that participants were not sensitive
to the consonantal repetition and stress pattern rules when
presented independently; rather, they were only sensitive to
constructions that complied with both consonantal repetition
and stress pattern rules.
Upon visual inspection, the topography of the P3 appeared to
be right-lateralized, whilst the experimental effect seemed more
left-lateralized. In order to determine whether the interaction was
significant, we conducted an additional ANOVA, with Sentence
Type (Cynghanedd, Consonantal violation, Stress violation,
Double violation) and ‘Laterality’ (Left [CP3;P3], Right [CP4;P4])
as independent variables. We found a significant effect of
Laterality; F(1,24) = 27.66, p < 0.001, η2p = 00.54, with greater
P3b mean amplitudes elicited on the Right (M = 6.05, 95% CI
[5.01, 7.09]) than on the Left (M = 4.37, 95%CI [3.39, 5.36]). The
Sentence Type ∗ Laterality interaction did not reach significance
[F(1,24) = 1.05, p = 0.377, η2p = 0.04], however, indicating that
the experimental effect was not modulated by electrode site.
FIGURE 2 | Online categorization results. (A) Sentence categorization
performance. (B) Reaction times.
DISCUSSION
(these were the Cynghanedd-orthodox sentences), it was
infelicitous in the Double violation and the Consonantal
violation conditions.
For the reaction time data, a main effect of Categorization
was found [F(1,24) = 33.58, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.58; Figure 2B]:
Sentences that were perceived as ‘good’ were responded to faster
(M = 653 ms, 95% CI [568, 738]) than sentences perceived as ‘not
good’ (M = 774 ms, 95% CI [678, 869]). There was also a main
effect of Sentence Type [F(3,72) = 3.24, p = 0.03, η2p = 0.12], but
none of the corrected pairwise comparisons reached significance.
Here, we investigated whether naïve readers of a traditional form
of Welsh poetry are able to unconsciously distinguish phrases
conforming to its poetic construction rules from those that violate
them. In line with our predictions, words correctly completing
a sentence in Cynghanedd elicited significantly greater P3b
mean amplitudes than words completing other sentence types,
indicating a shift of attention associated with target recognition
(Polich, 2007).
The P3b modulation observed here had a typical
centroparietal distribution and a time-range comparable to
that observed in simple target detection tasks, consistent with
the classic P3b effect (Knight, 1996). Thus, participants’ brains
treated correct completion words as targets and implicitly
categorized Cynghanedd-orthodox sentences as sounding ‘good’
compared to sentences violating its construction rules. Strikingly,
however, and in contrast with ERP results, participants showed
no overt knowledge or conscious awareness of Cynghanedd
rules in the online categorization task since (a) they failed
to discriminate between Cynghanedd and Double Violation
sentences, and (b) their performance was either at chance
level (Stress violation condition) or infelicitous with regard to
Cynghanedd rules in the other violation conditions. There was
some differentiation between sentence types, with participants
Offline Sentence Ranking Task
A one sample t-test revealed that participants did not rank
Cynghanedd sentences as the best option significantly better
than chance [M accuracy = 28%; t(24) = 1.87, p = 0.07]. Since
this result was approaching significance, we further tested
whether participants showed any inclination to rank Cynghanedd
sentences in the top two choices by coding the response as 1
if Cynghanedd sentences were ranked 1st or 2nd, or as 0 if
Cynghanedd sentences were ranked 3rd or 4th. In this case, a one
sample t-test revealed that participants did perform significantly
greater than chance on this task [M accuracy = 62%; t(24) = 6.93,
p < 0.001].
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
4
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
FIGURE 3 | Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP) results. P3b mean amplitudes elicited by all four sentence types were computed and compared between 240
and 340 ms after the onset of the final word (gray box).
violation sentences should be judged as ‘good’ more than the
other two sentence types, but this was not the case in our
data. Whilst participants considered Cynghanedd sentences as
sounding better than those from single violation conditions, they
did not consider Cynghanedd sentences as sounding better than
Double violation sentences. This finding could be interpreted in
one of two ways; (1) the consonantal repetition manipulation
was too subtle to influence participants’ explicit judgments,
or (2) the ‘sound good’ decision task implemented in this
study did not depend on the affective qualities of the repeated
phonemes. In addition, the ERP results suggest that appreciation
of Cynghanedd depends on a combination of subtle consonantal
repetition and stress pattern, rather than consonantal repetition
alone.
The P3b effect observed here may be considered counterintuitive, since P3b amplitude is classically reduced with repeated
occurrences of stimuli. Here, the presence of consonantal
repetition patterns in the Cynghanedd condition may have been
expected to reduce the amplitude of the P3b rather than increase
it. Thus, the enhanced P3b response to Cynghanedd appears
to indicate a kind of attentional orienting response, specifically
when both the stress pattern and consonantal repetition rules
are observed, thus making this particular sentence a target.
rating Cynghanedd sentences as sounding better than those
from single violation conditions. It is possible that this difference
occurred due to participants perceiving the rule violations in
these conditions, however, this interpretation cannot account for
the fact that participants did not consider Cynghanedd sentences
as sounding better than Double violation sentences. Participants
did, however, demonstrate a preference toward Cynghanedd
sentences during the offline judgment task. Given that the
ranking task was of a very different nature to the online task
(involving direct comparison between the different alternatives
of each sentence) and that it was not time constrained, it is highly
likely that participants changed cognitive strategy in this task,
and focused on elements of the stimuli that were not attended to
during the online categorization task.
Interestingly, the results of the online decision task are
somewhat incongruent with recent research emphasizing the
influence of foregrounded features on aesthetic appreciation
(Aryani et al., 2013). For example, Aryani et al. (2013)
demonstrated, via use of a text analysis tool, that the salience
of particular sublexical features (e.g., phonological repetition)
correlates with the semantic and aesthetic properties of poetic
phrases. Given that a ‘sound good’ judgment could be influenced
by such foregrounding properties, Cynghanedd and Stress
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
5
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
FIGURE 4 | Topographic maps of ERP difference waves in the P3b analysis window (240–340 ms after the onset of the final word). Cynghanedd
topographies depict differences between Double violation and Cynghanedd conditions. Correct stress patterns topographies depict differences between Double
violation and Consonantal violation conditions. Correct consonantal repetition topographies depict differences between Double violation and Stress violation
conditions.
This is congruent with recent electrophysiological evidence
showing that lyrical stanzas that contain consistent meter and
rhyme facilitate processing compared with those that contain
only one, or neither of these patterns (Obermeier et al.,
2016). Another recent study has shown that electrophysiological
responses to poetry can be modulated by prosodic elements
(e.g., rhyme) alone (Chen et al., 2016). Our findings are
somewhat incongruent with this conclusion, since stress
pattern alone failed to generate a main effect on P3b mean
amplitudes.
Recent eye-tracking studies have also shown that literary
stylistic features in sentences increase attentional engagement
(see Jacobs, 2015, for a review). Our data crucially show that
this attentional orienting effect occurs as early as 240 ms,
after stimulus onset and is therefore likely to reflect implicit
processing. Recall that participants were unable to overtly identify
the Cynghanedd forms, and we found no correlation between
reaction times and P3b mean amplitudes, contra previous
findings (Conroy and Polich, 2007; Ramchurn et al., 2014; but see
McCarthy and Donchin, 1981). Thus, whereas previous studies
have shown that the explicit, aesthetic appreciation of poetry can
be linked to implicit responses (e.g., Jacobs, 2015; Obermeier
et al., 2016), the current findings provide the first tangible
evidence that this link is permeable: our participants were able
to implicitly detect correct poetic forms, even though they could
not explicitly differentiate between conditions (cf. Renault et al.,
1989).
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
Furthermore, despite the relatively complex nature of the
processes underlying the decision task, the observed P3b had
a latency akin to that typical of simple shape-matching tasks
(Kok, 2001), occurring much earlier than typical responses to
linguistic stimuli (Kutas and Hillyard, 1980). This suggests that
spontaneous recognition of poetic harmony is a fast, sublexical
process, and is not strategic nor cognitively effortful. Finally, our
findings show that the brain responds to combinations of poetic –
or foregrounding – features at the sublexical (phonological
salience) and the lexical (stress pattern) levels (cf. Jacobs, 2015
4 × 4 model of neurocognitive poetics). That is, our data suggest
that the interactive effects of poetic features are more potent than
that of features presented in isolation.
Taken together, our results demonstrate the ability of the
human brain to process poetic forms spontaneously, quickly, and
implicitly, in the absence of any formal knowledge or instruction
regarding underlying construction rules. This study shows for
the first time that before we even consider literal meaning, the
musical properties of poetry instinctively speak to the human
mind in ways that escape consciousness.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
GT, RT, LJ, and PL designed research; AV-E performed research;
AV-E and GT analyzed data; AV-E, MJ, and GT wrote the paper;
RT, LJ and PL contributed to the paper.
6
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859
Vaughan-Evans et al.
Spontaneous Recognition of Poetry
FUNDING
PL]; The Gwyneth and D Tecwyn Memorial Endowment
[School of Welsh, Bangor University to RT, LJ, and PL]; the
Economic and Social Research Council UK [RES-E0245561 to GT]; and the European Research Council [ERC- 209704
to GT].
The authors thank Aina Casaponsa and Ceri Ellis for
assistance with data collection. This work was supported by
the Coleg Cymraeg Cenedlaethol [AV-E, MJ, RT, LJ, and
REFERENCES
Lea, R. B., Rapp, D. N., Elfenbein, A., Mitchel, A. D., and Romine, R. S. (2008).
Sweet silent thought: alliteration and resonance in poetry comprehension.
Psychol. Sci. 19, 709–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02146.x
Llwyd, A. (2010). Crefft y Gynghanedd. Gwynedd. Wales: Cyhoeddiadau Barddas.
McCarthy, G., and Donchin, E. (1981). A metric for thought: a comparison of P300
latency and reaction time. Science 211, 77–80. doi: 10.1126/science.7444452
Obermeier, C., Kotz, S. A., Jessen, S., Raettig, T., von Koppenfels, M., and
Menninghaus, W. (2016). Aesthetic appreciation of poetry correlates with ease
of processing in event-related potentials. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16,
362–373. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0396-x
Obermeier, C., Menninghaus, W., von Koppenfels, M., Raettig, T., SchmidtKassow, M., Otterbein, S., et al. (2013). Aesthetic and emotional effects of meter
and rhyme in poetry. Front. Psychol. 4:10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00010
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019
Ramchurn, A., de Fockert, J. W., Mason, L., Darling, S., and Bunce, D. (2014).
Intraindividual reaction time variability affects P300 amplitude rather than
latency. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:557. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00557
Renault, B., Signoret, J. L., Debruille, B., Breton, F., and Bolgert, F. (1989). Brain
potentials reveal covert facial recognition in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia
27, 905–912. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90066-3
Scheepers, C., Mohr, S., Fischer, M. H., and Roberts, A. M. (2013). Listening to
limericks: a pupillometry investigation of perceivers’ expectancy. PLoS ONE
8:e74986. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074986
Scofield, M. (1988). T.S. Eliot, the Poems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Aryani, A., Jacobs, A. M., and Conrad, M. (2013). Extracting salient sublexical
units from written texts: “Emophon,” a corpus-based approach to phonological
iconicity. Front. Psychol. 4:654. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00654
Chen, Q., Zhang, J., Xu, X., Scheepers, C., Yang, Y., and Tanenhaus, M. K. (2016).
Prosidic expectations in silent reading: ERP evidence from rhyme scheme and
semantic congruence in classic Chinese poems. Cognition 154, 11–21. doi: 10.
1016/j.cognition.2016.05.007
Conroy, M. A., and Polich, J. (2007). Normative variation of P3a and P3b from
a large sample: gender, topography, and response time. J. Psychophysiol. 21,
22–32. doi: 1.01027/0269-8803.21.1.22
Ellis, N. C., O’Dochartaigh, C., Hicks, W., Morgan, M., and Laporte, N. (2001).
Cronfa Electroneg o Gymraeg (CEG): a 1 Million Word Lexical Database and
Frequency Count for Welsh. Available at: www.bangor.ac.uk/canolfanbedwyr/
ceg.php.en
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line
removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484.
Greene, R. (2012). The Princeton Encyclopaedia of Poetry and Poetics. Princeton,
NY: Princeton University Press.
Hanauer, D. I. (2001). The task of poetry reading and second language learning.
Appl. Linguist. 22, 295–323. doi: 10.1093/applin/22.3.295
Hoorn, J. (1996). “Psychophysiology and literary processing: ERPs to semantic and
phonological deviations in reading small verses,” in Empirical Approaches to
Literature and Aesthetics eds R. J. Kreuz and M.S. MacNealy (Norwood, NJ:
Ablex Publishing Corporation), 339–358.
Jacobs, A. M. (2015). Neurocognitive poetics: methods and models for investigating
the neuronal and cognitive-affective bases of literature reception. Front. Hum.
Neurosci. 9, 1–22. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00186
Knight, R. T. (1996). Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty
detection. Nature 383, 256–259. doi: 10.1038/383256a0
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of the P3 amplitude as a measure of
processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38, 557–577. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201
990559
Kutas, M., and Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading between the lines: event-related
brain potentials during natural sentence processing. Brain Lang. 11, 354–373.
doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(80)90133-9
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2016 Vaughan-Evans, Trefor, Jones, Lynch, Jones and Thierry. This
is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums
is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the
original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply
with these terms.
7
November 2016 | Volume 7 | Article 1859