CHILD DEVELOPMENT PERSPECTIVES
Foundations of Reasoning About Social Dominance
Anthea Pun, Susan A.J. Birch, and Andrew S. Baron
University of British Columbia
ABSTRACT—Understanding
the factors that shape the
social landscape is essential for living in a group, where
dominant individuals often have greater control over and
access to desired resources such as food and mates.
Recently, researchers have demonstrated that preverbal
infants, similar to their nonhuman primate relatives,
already possess the cognitive schemas necessary to represent social dominance in relationships, using ecologically
relevant cues such as relative physical size and group size.
In this article, we discuss the phylogenetic and ontogenetic origins of infants’ and children’s capacity to represent social dominance in relationships and hierarchies,
and examine how these initial representations are
enriched across early childhood.
KEYWORDS—social
dominance; infancy; cognitive develop-
ment; social status
INTRODUCTION
Navigating the complexities of social relationships is a fundamental task that many animals face throughout life. Although
social animals must cooperate, conflict over valuable resources
such as food, territory, and mates inevitably occurs. Pursuing
such resources through physical competition can be costly as it
Anthea Pun, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia; Susan A.J. Birch, Department of Psychology, University
of British Columbia; Andrew S. Baron, Department of Psychology,
University of British Columbia.
The research reported in this article was funded by a Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council Insight Development
Grant (435-2013-0286) to Andrew Scott Baron.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Anthea Pun, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada;
e-mail:
[email protected].
© 2017 The Authors
Child Development Perspectives © 2017 The Society for Research in Child Development
DOI: 10.1111/cdep.12235
may result in injury or death (1, 2). To help competitors assess
their likelihood of succeeding or failing before engaging in a
direct competition, cognitive and behavioral adaptations have
evolved that facilitate the ability to detect and track cues to
social dominance, thereby supporting the formation and maintenance of social hierarchies (3–5). Similar to how the biological
(6), cognitive (7), and social sciences (8) define dominance, we
treat it as the tendency for an individual or group to prevail and
exert influence over a subordinate individual or group. Dominance confers differential access to desired resources, such as
food, mates, and territory (acquired through superior competitive
skills or relinquished by subordinates), thereby promoting
greater chances of survival and reproductive success. Consequently, aligning oneself with a dominant individual can be beneficial, as individuals of higher status can confer benefits, such
as resources and protection, to subordinates.
In this article, we propose that the cognitive capacities necessary for detecting dominance relations evolved in our nonhuman
primate ancestors and may have been preserved in human lineage, as evidenced by young infants. We begin by outlining the
evolutionary pressures that likely perpetuated the development
of cognitive adaptations that aid in detecting ecologically relevant cues to social dominance. Then, we address whether human
infants can establish representations of dominance using cues to
which nonhuman primates are sensitive. Last, we discuss how
representations of social dominance can influence young children’s capacity for cultural learning.
CUES TO DETECTING DOMINANCE
Physical size is often associated with dominance ranking in conflicts both within and between species, with larger individuals
often benefiting from greater strength and power over smaller
individuals (9). Indeed, natural selection has favored adaptations
that exploit this inference, so that when some species are threatened, they adopt postures that make them appear bigger to
intimidate an opponent (9, 10).
Although, physical size is a reliable cue to dominance in
many social and nonsocial animals, one cue to dominance that
is apparently unique to animals who live in groups is the
Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6
2
Anthea Pun, Susan A.J. Birch, and Andrew S. Baron
number of allies an individual has. The importance of tracking
group size is illustrated by chimpanzees, which use different tactics to advertise their numerical strength while patrolling the
borders of their territory (11, 12). Signals indicating the size of
one’s group can be detected both visually and acoustically (2,
13, 14). For example, to advertise the numerical strength of their
group to others (11, 12) and deter opposing groups from
approaching (15), both male and female chimps engage in noisy
pant-hoot calling. And both chimpanzees and lions are more
likely to engage in conflict if they outnumber intruders, but will
stay silent or retreat if they do not (13, 14). In summary, a group
is more likely to compete if it has more individuals than the
opposing group (14, 16).
DEVELOPMENTAL FOUNDATIONS OF REASONING
ABOUT SOCIAL DOMINANCE
Paralleling findings with nonhuman primates, preverbal human
infants use cues such as relative physical size and numerical
group size to infer social dominance between competitors (2, 7,
14, 17). In one study (7), preverbal infants inferred social dominance relationships by comparing the physical size of two competing agents. Infants were introduced to two agents (one twice
as large as the other), each with the apparent goal of crossing to
the opposite side of a platform. When both agents tried to cross
at the same time, they met on the path. Then, infants were
shown two scenarios: one in which the larger agent prevailed
and one in which the smaller agent prevailed. Although 10- to
13-month-olds expected the larger agent to be dominant, 8- to
9-month-olds failed to demonstrate an expectation about which
agent should prevail. This suggests that only older infants could
use the relative physical size of two competing agents to infer
which one would get the right of way. Because younger infants
did not reliably use physical size as a cue to social dominance,
it was unclear whether they were incapable of representing dominance relationships in general or simply were insensitive to this
particular cue.
Physical size is not always an ecologically reliable cue, especially among species that form cooperative social relationships
with conspecifics (6, 18). For example, in some primate species
(e.g., chimpanzees), competing males may not differ greatly in
size. Consequently, a male striving to achieve a position of high
status may not be able to attain this on his own, but can succeed if he forms alliances with other males (19–21). This
demonstrates that high-status positions are not necessarily
reserved for the largest individuals, but can be achieved by
smaller (or younger) individuals that cultivate social alliances
(6, 18).
In another study (17), we examined whether infants’ understanding of social dominance extends beyond physical size—to
numerical group size—and if so, whether such sensitivity
emerges earlier in development than a sensitivity to physical
size. Similar to the methods used in an earlier study
investigating infants’ ability to use physical size as a cue to
dominance (7), 6- to 12-month-olds were familiarized to an
agent from each group independently achieving the goal of
crossing a platform. Each of the competing agents were the same
size, but differed in the number of members of the group to
which it belonged (one belonged to a group of three members
and the other to a group of two). When both competing agents
attempted to cross the platform simultaneously, they bumped
into one another. Then, infants viewed two outcome trials, one
in which the agent from the larger group prevailed and one
which the agent from the smaller group prevailed. Infants
expected the agent from the larger group to succeed. This
demonstrates that infants use the numerical size of a group as a
cue to social dominance and expect an agent from a numerically
larger group to be dominant. In this study, the other group members did not assist during the event. Thus, the fact that these
infants inferred which agent would be dominant through their
alliance with a larger group suggests that infants may understand that the presence of group members confers an advantage,
even when those members are not involved directly in conflict.
As with nonhuman primates, humans recognize dominance
relationships when agents forfeit desired resources to the dominant agent. For example, in one study (22), 9- and 12-montholds were shown a video of an animated agent collecting small
objects. When another agent entered, the first agent stopped collecting the desired objects, allowing the second agent to gather
the remaining objects. By forfeiting the remaining objects, the
first agent was shown to be subordinate to the second agent. At
test, the two agents competed for a new type of desired object,
and 12-month-olds (but not 9-month-olds) were more surprised
when the subordinate was allowed to take the last object, compared to when the dominant agent was allowed to take the last
object. This demonstrates that 12-month-olds expected the dominant agent to obtain desired resources, even when no physical
conflict occurred.
From the aforementioned studies, it is unclear whether
infants can keep track of an individual’s dominance over
another agent across different contexts because actors in those
studies were observed only once in a scenario where their
dominance status was established. To investigate infants’
understanding of social dominance as a stable relation, we carried out a study in which 12- and 15-month-olds were first
familiarized to a dominant agent monopolizing a bounded area
(i.e., pushing the subordinate away); at test, the infants were
shown a novel scenario in which the previously established
dominant and subordinate agents competed for a desired
object. Infants viewed two outcomes: one in which the previously dominant agent succeeded and one in which the subordinate succeeded. We saw a developmental trend in which older
infants demonstrated an increased capacity to track dominance
relationships across different contexts. More specifically, 12month-olds could represent the dominance relationship
between two agents only when the test scenario was identical
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6
Origins of Social Dominance Reasoning
to the conflict scenario they had witnessed. But, 15-month-olds
could generalize the established dominance relationship to
many scenarios. For example, if one agent had been shown to
dominate a second in one setting, 15-month-olds expected the
dominant agent to obtain the desired objects in all settings
(22). This suggests that by 15 months, infants expect established dominance relations between two agents to be consistent
over time and across contexts.
To further examine infants’ capacity to track dominance relationships among individuals, researchers in another study (23)
tested whether infants could represent the dominance hierarchy
among three agents. Following an earlier study (22), 15-montholds were shown three pairs of agents, each of which wanted to
monopolize a confined space. Infants could recognize the dominance relationship between each pair of agents, but only when it
was presented incrementally and in a linear order. For example,
if infants were shown that A was dominant to B, then B was
dominant to C, and C was dominant to D, they understood that
A was dominant to C. However, infants could not recognize the
dominance relationships if they were established discontinuously. For example, if they were shown that C was dominant to
D, then B was dominant to C, and A was dominant to B, infants
could not recognize that A was dominant to C. This work
demonstrates that in particular circumstances, infants may use a
form of deductive reasoning called transitive inference to derive
a relationship between agents that have not been compared to
one another directly.
When and how humans develop the ability to use transitive
inference to reason about social dominance in relationships is
not fully understood and is a matter of debate. In some studies
(23, 24), transitive inference emerges as early as infancy, while
in others (25, 26), the ability appears to emerge around age 4.
However, this developmental discrepancy in humans’ ability to
use transitive reasoning may be related to the particular domain
tested because sensitivity to the social dominance domain may
emerge prior to domain-general reasoning.
This evidence supports the argument that transitive inference
is central to social dominance cognition in humans, as it allows
humans to represent the complex structures of their social
groups by building on dyadic relationships. This ability is also
essential for maintaining social hierarchies in other species that
live in groups (e.g., nonhuman primates). However, it remains
unclear whether the capacity to organize social hierarchies
through transitive inference operates similarly in nonhuman primates and humans. Therefore, further investigation is needed to
compare and contrast nonhuman primates’ ability to build representations of social hierarchies with those of human infants and
children.
EVALUATING DOMINANT INDIVIDUALS
Not only do young children recognize dominance relationships
among individuals, they also appear to exhibit a positivity bias
3
toward more dominant individuals. For example, when
preschoolers view an individual behaving in a dominant way
(e.g., giving orders to others), they also expect the individual to
have more resources (27). In addition, children are willing to
maintain and perpetuate this inequality themselves, by ensuring
that the dominant individual always has more resources than the
subordinate individual (28).
Why are young children willing to perpetuate inequality in
this context? One reason may stem from our nonhuman primate
ancestors, who affiliate with dominant individuals to increase
access to coveted resources, thereby improving their dominance
rank and fitness for survival. For example, male chimpanzees
with lower status attempt to develop alliances with dominant
individuals by attending to and appeasing the dominant chimpanzees (e.g., through grooming; 29–32).
Similarly, recent work suggests that young children may also
recognize the benefits of affiliating with dominant individuals. In
one study (33), researchers investigated whether 2-year-olds
preferred a dominant agent over a subordinate agent. To accomplish this, they modified the dominance paradigm (7) with one
important change: Both agents were equal in size. After establishing that one agent was dominant over another, researchers
presented both agents to the children, placing them side by side.
The children were asked which agent they liked. Two-year-olds
preferred the dominant agent, suggesting that young children,
like nonhuman primates, may be motivated to affiliate with dominant individuals. Whether this choice reflects a preference for
dominant individuals that could share resources or provide protection, or a preference for individuals who complete their goal
(akin to a success bias; see 34) requires further investigation.
PREFERENTIAL LEARNING FROM DOMINANT
INDIVIDUALS
In addition to greater access to valuable resources, dominant
individuals also directly influence their subordinate counterparts, as evidenced by studies with nonhuman primates in which
decisions made by dominant individuals directly influenced the
behavior of subordinates in different domains (35–37). For
example, baboons willingly (i.e., without force or coercion)
accepted and followed despotic decisions about foraging patches
made by dominant baboons, even when this incurred a shortterm cost for subordinates. In addition, in a recent study (38),
chimpanzees were biased to learn from a more dominant individual. Researchers devised a task in which chimpanzees could
acquire desired food from a box with two openings. Dominant
chimps did not prefer to obtain the food from either opening.
However, subordinates paid more attention to the dominant individual and appeared to imitate her manner of interacting with
the box. Therefore, subordinates na€ıve to the box task were more
likely to obtain the food from the same opening as the dominant
individual they had observed, even though either opening would
have provided food. This demonstration of deference to the
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6
4
Anthea Pun, Susan A.J. Birch, and Andrew S. Baron
dominant, not only in competitive scenarios but in foraging
choices and cultural learning, may reflect an evolved tendency
to follow the leadership of dominant individuals.
Evidence of preferential learning from dominant individuals
may reflect an expectation that dominant individuals are more
optimal sources of information, all else being equal. Indeed,
research with human adults seems to support this view. In one
study (39), human adults rated high in trait dominance were perceived as more competent by their peers, randomly assigned
group members, and even researchers blind to the hypotheses of
the study. This suggests that individuals higher in trait dominance may attain social influence because they are perceived to
be more competent, even when no evidence of competence is
provided.
Like nonhuman primates, 3- and 4-year-olds show a similar
learning bias, favoring the labels provided by a socially
dominant individual over those of a subordinate individual in a
novel word learning task (40). In this study, children were na€ıve
to individuals’ previous knowledge or history of accuracy, and
only knew that one individual was dominant over the other
because he had been given the right of way to cross a platform
(see 7, 17). At a minimum, these results show that children are
more likely to learn new information from more dominant individuals than from less dominant ones. Children’s learning preference could also stem from an evaluation of individuals’
competence. That is, like adults (39), young children may associate dominance with competence (even if no direct evidence of
competence is observed), and that evaluation drives children’s
learning decisions. In addition, we also recognize that dominance and competence may influence children’s preferences
independently. Consequently, researchers should attempt to disentangle these possibilities by examining the specific contexts
in which dominance and competence may influence children’s
learning decisions.
Whether children’s (and chimps’) preference to learn from
dominant individuals includes an evaluative component remains
a question, but the aforementioned results suggest that both nonhuman and human primates are predisposed to learn from dominant or prevailing individuals. This learning bias may reveal
one way in which dominant individuals influence cultural learning, the products of which are passed down to later generations.
CONCLUSIONS AND LOOKING AHEAD: A
PREDISPOSITION TOWARD INEQUALITY?
We argue that the cognitive mechanisms used by nonhuman primates and humans to detect and track social dominance relations may share common roots, as evidenced by a growing body
of evidence in both evolutionary and psychological research. By
studying, and capitalizing on, infants’ early sensitivity to goaldirected action and goal attainment, we have detected infants’
early-emerging recognition of dominance relationships. For
instance, in much of this work (7, 17, 22), infants expect a
dominant agent to accomplish his or her goal at the expense of
another agent. Therefore, like nonhuman primates and adult
humans, infants apparently link success (e.g., in the form of
completing goals and accessing resources differentially) with
dominance.
In addition, we propose that recognizing social dominance
early in development is facilitated by ecologically relevant cues
(e.g., relative physical size, numerical group size, amount of
resources). However, we recognize that research on infants’
understanding of dominance relationships and hierarchies is
limited, and that infants and children may be sensitive to other
cues to dominance (e.g., age, vocal tone and pitch, facial cues,
and body language, including posture and other nonverbal
behaviors). Given the preliminary nature of this work, researchers should investigate the developmental trajectory of reasoning
about social dominance across the lifespan. Such work should
focus on whether young children can recognize more nuanced
cues to dominance (in particular, those unique to a particular
individual), and whether such cues lead to similar inferences
about dominant individuals (e.g., decisions about with whom to
align or from whom to learn).
Given that culture can also affect social evaluations significantly, it is important to consider how socialized values may
moderate perceptions of dominance and status over time. For
example, in a recent study with human adults (41), explicit associations between social status and numerical group size may
have been shaped by the beliefs and social norms reinforced by
culture: Americans were more likely to explicitly associate smaller groups with higher status, rationalizing that smaller groups
are more exclusive and elite, and that they represent the richest
1% in the country. In contrast, implicit measures—designed to
tap unconscious and automatic associations—revealed the
opposite association: Larger groups were associated with higher
status. This finding demonstrates that culturally acquired values
can lead to judgments about dominance that differ from those
based on the mechanisms that humans share with nonhuman
primates.
Finally, human infants’ expectations that one individual is
more likely to be dominant than another may reveal an early,
tacit awareness of inequality in social relationships. As such, we
cannot help but wonder whether this early awareness contributes
to children’s and adults’ tendency to reinforce, provide justification for, and even favor inequality in social relationships (27,
42–45). Some studies have investigated whether an individual’s
position in society affects children’s social preferences regarding
that person. For example, 5- to 7-year-olds prefer advantaged
over disadvantaged groups (44), and as early as age 2, children
prefer dominant over subordinate agents (33). These choices
may reflect an implicit understanding that being affiliated with
advantaged or dominant individuals can be beneficial. As such,
researchers need to determine whether young children’s underlying motives are ultimately self-serving if dominant individuals
are viewed as sources of protection, distributors of resources, or
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6
Origins of Social Dominance Reasoning
valuable sources of information. These questions require further
exploration, and the answers may help explain why prejudice is
so widespread, and why fairness and tolerance can sometimes
be difficult to attain.
REFERENCES
1. Smith, J. M. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal
conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47, 209–221. doi:10.1016/
0022-5193(74)90110-6
2. Wilson, M. L., & Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in
chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 363–392. doi:10.
1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.120046
3. Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. American Association for the Advancement of Science, 211,
1390–1396.
4. Geist, V. (1978). Life strategies, human evolution, environmental
design: Toward a biological theory of health. New York, NY: Springer.
5. Smith, J. M. (1984). Game theory and the evolution of behavior.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 95–101. doi:10.1017/
S0140525X00026327
6. Hand, J. L. (1986). Resolution of social conflicts: Dominance, egalitarianism, spheres of dominance, and game theory. The Quarterly
Review of Biology, 61, 201–220. doi:10.1086/414899
7. Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., & Carey, S.
(2011). Big and mighty: Pre-verbal infants mentally represent social
dominance. Science, 331, 477–480. doi:10.1126/science.1199198
8. Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 19, 97–
132. doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0470
9. Brown, J. H., & Maurer, B. A. (1986). Body size, ecological dominance and Cope’s rule. Nature, 324, 248–250. doi:10.1038/
324248a0
10. Buston, P. (2003). Social hierarchies: Size and growth modification
in clownfish. Nature, 424, 145–146. doi:10.1038/424145a
11. Clark, A. P. (1993). Rank differences in the production of vocalizations by wild chimpanzees as a function of social context. American
Journal of Primatology, 31, 159–179.
12. Ghiglieri, M. P. (1984). The chimpanzees of Kibale forest: A field
study of ecology and social structure. New York, NY: Columbia
University Press.
13. McComb, K., Packer, C., & Pusey, A. (1994). Roaring and
numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions,
Panthera leo. Animal Behavior, 47, 379–387. doi:10.1006/anbe.
1994.1052
14. Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., & Wrangham, R. W. (2001). Does
participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment,
range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour,
61, 1203–1216. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1706
15. Boesch, C., & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The chimpanzees of
the Ta€ı Forest: Behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
16. Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior.
Cambridge, MA: Belkap Press of Harvard University Press.
17. Pun, A., Birch, S., & Baron, A. S. (2016). Infants use relative
numerical group size to infer social dominance. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113,
2376–2381. doi:10.1073/pnas.1514879113
5
18. Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1992). How monkeys see the world:
Inside the mind of another species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
19. Nishida, T., & Hosaka, K. (1996). Coalition strategies among adult
male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In W. C.
McGrew, L. F. Marchant, & T. Nishida (Eds.), Great ape societies
(pp. 114–134). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
20. de Waal, F. B. (1984). Sex differences in the formation of coalitions
among chimpanzees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 239–255.
doi:10.1016/0162-3095(84)90004-9
21. Duffy, K. G., Wrangham, R. W., & Silk, J. B. (2007). Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Current
Biology, 17, R586–R587. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.001
22. Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable social
dominance relations by human infants. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 6862–
6867. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113194109
23. Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2014). Human infants’ learning of social
structures; the case of dominance hierarchy. Psychological Science,
25, 250–255. doi:10.1177/0956797613500509
24. Gazes, R. P., Hampton, R. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2015). Transitive
inference of social dominance by human infants. Developmental
Science, 20, e12367. doi:10.1111/desc.12367
25. Bryant, P. E., & Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive inferences and
memory in young children. Nature, 232, 456–458. doi:10.1038/
232456a0
26. Wright, B. C. (2012). The case for a dual-process theory of transitive
reasoning. Developmental Review, 32, 89–124. doi:10.1016/j.dr.
2012.04.001
27. Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clement, F., Kaufmann, L., Berchtold, A., Reboul, A., & Van Der Henst, J.-B. (2015). How preschoolers use cues of dominance to make sense of their social
environment. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 587–604.
doi:10.1080/15248372.2014.926269
28. Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clement, F., Kaufmann, L., Reboul,
A., & Van der Henst, J. (2016). Children’s allocation of resources in
social dominance situations. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1843–
1857. doi:10.1037/dev0000164
29. Schino, G. (2007). Grooming, coalitions, and reciprocal altruism in
primates. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 85, 235–236. doi:10.
1093/beheco/arl045
30. Seyfarth, R. M. (1977). A model of social grooming among adult
female monkeys. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 65, 671–698.
doi:10.1016/0022-5193(77)90015-7
31. Slater, P. J., Snowdon, C. T., Rosenblatt, J. S., & Milinski, M.
(1997). Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 26). San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
32. Tiddi, B., Aureli, F., & Schino, G. (2012). Grooming up the hierarchy:
The exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new
world primate. PLoS ONE, 7, e36641. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.
0036641
33. Thomas, A., Abramyan, M., Lukowksi, A., Thomsen, L., &
Sarnecka, B. (2016). Preferring the mighty to the meek: Toddlers
prefer superiors to subordinates. In A. Papafragou, D. Grodner, D.
Mirman, & J. C. Trueswell (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Annual
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 241–246). CogSci
2016: The annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society,
Philadelphia, PA, August 10, 2016. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science
Society.
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6
6
Anthea Pun, Susan A.J. Birch, and Andrew S. Baron
34. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige:
Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22,
165–196. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00071-4
35. Chance, M. R. A. (1967). Attention structure as the basis of primate
rank orders. Man, 2, 503–518.
36. Douglas-Hamilton, I., & Douglas-Hamilton, O. (1975). Among the
elephants. New York, NY: Viking Press.
37. La Freniere, P., & Charlesworth, W. R. (1983). Dominance, attention, and affiliation in a preschool group: A nine-month longitudinal
study. Ethology and Sociobiology, 4, 55–67. doi:10.1016/01623095(83)90030-4
38. Kendal, R., Hopper, L., Whiten, A., Brosnan, S., Lambeth, S., Schapiro, S., & Hoppitt, W. (2015). Chimpanzees copy dominant and
knowledgeable individuals: Implications for cultural diversity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 65–72. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbe
hav.2014.09.002
39. Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009). Why do dominant personalities attain influence in face-to-face groups? The competence-signaling effects of trait dominance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 96, 491–503. doi:10.1037/a0014201
40. Pun, A., Lai, J., Birch, S. A. J., & Baron, A. S. (2017). Follow the
leader: Young children’s selective trust in dominant agents. Unpublished manuscript.
41. Cao, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Social inferences from group size.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 70, 204–211. Advance
online publication doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.11.005
42. Baron, A., & Banaji, M. (2009). Evidence of system justification in
young children. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3,
918–926. doi:10.1111/j.1751- 9004.2009.00214.x
43. Jost, J. T., Pelham, B. W., & Carvallo, M. R. (2002). Non-conscious
forms of system justification: Implicit and behavioral preferences for
higher status groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38,
586–602. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00505-X
44. Olson, K. R., Dweck, C. S., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2011).
Children’s responses to group-based inequalities: Perpetuation and
rectification. Social Cognition, 29, 270–287. doi:10.1521/soco.2011.
29.3.270
45. Rudman, L. A., Feinberg, J., & Fairchild, K. (2002). Minority members’ implicit attitudes: Automatic ingroup bias as a function of
group status. Social Cognition, 20, 294–320. doi:10.1521/soco.20.4.
294.19908
Child Development Perspectives, Volume 0, Number 0, 2017, Pages 1–6