The interaction between insight and legal capacity in dementia
Alfredo Calcedo-Barba, Paola Castelli Candia, Javier Conejo Galindo and
Fernando Garcı́a Solano
Purpose of review
In this article we will review the relevant literature published in
the last year on insight and on assessment of competency of
patients with dementia. We have analysed the implications that
recent research on insight may have for capacity assessment.
Recent findings
Studies that have recently been published deal with financial
assessment and financial abilities of dementia patients, and on
the potential impairment of driving, and particularly on the
awareness that patients have of their loss of abilities.
Summary
Recent research on insight may have important implications for
capacity assessment in dementia.
Keywords
mental competency, insight, dementia
Curr Opin Psychiatry 17:397–400.
#
2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Department of Psychiatry, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón,
Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Correspondence to Alfredo Calcedo-Barba, Hospital General Gregorio Marañón, Ibiza
41, 28009 Madrid, Spain
Tel: +34 630 992720; email:
[email protected]
Current Opinion in Psychiatry 2004, 17:397–400
Abbreviation
MCI
mild cognitive impairment
2004 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
0951-7367
#
Introduction
It is our intention in this article to review the research
literature that has appeared in the last year regarding two
closely related and interesting concepts – insight and
competency to make decisions – in a particular group of
diseases, namely dementia. Insight as a concept drew
great attention from researchers in the 1990s, particularly
in the field of schizophrenia. Amador and David [1]
provide an excellent review of this topic. Also, a good
number of studies have been published recently on
insight and different neuropsychiatric disorders including dementia [2].
Similarly, the assessment of legal capacity has been
another area of research with significant activity in recent
years [3 . .]. Capacity assessment is a context-sensible
issue and because of that it has fragmented into several
sub-areas of research such as general capacity, parental
capacity, capacity to stand trial, and so forth. As this
review is limited to patients with dementia, when we use
the term capacity it will be synonymous with general
legal capacity, or the need for guardianship and
conservatorship.
The definition of capacity in dementia patients is not an
easy task. It is important to establish very clearly the
constructs that are used in order to arrive at a
determination of capacity. First of all we must not forget
that the assessment of capacity is a functional assessment
in a specific context. This is an easy task in a field study,
in which the clinician only has to verify the level of
performance of the patient in order to make a judgement
and the corresponding dispositions. But the level of
functioning is not the only criterion in capacity assessment, and the concepts of understanding and appreciation, which are very close to insight, have been widely
used [4]. So the important question is how relevant is
insight in the assessment of legal capacity?
First we must clarify what we understand as insight. This
is not an easy task given that there is a long tradition in
philosophy that teaches us about the difference between
self-knowledge and knowledge of what is outside the
individual [5]. In psychiatry, the term insight has
important connotations within the field of Freudian
theories and, in fact, a good number of psychotherapeutic
techniques based on this model are called insightoriented therapies. Using a neurological approach the
term insight has been considered synonymous with
DOI: 10.1097/01.yco.0000139977.90931.d2
397
398 Forensic psychiatry
anosognosia and anosodiaphoria [6] in stroke patients.
Cognitive psychology has created another model of what
is insight and defines six basic elements [7]: (1)
Inferential processes are intact but perceptual input is
disturbed. (2) There is a breakdown in inferential
processes. (3) There is a breakdown in the process of
self-monitoring. (4) There is a breakdown in the process
involved in error checking. (5) The linkage between
thought and affect is impaired. (6) There are deficits in
the maintenance of representations in memory, the
sequential organization behaviour, and the sustaining of
efforts to establish and achieve a goal or plan.
Another problem is the value that we may give to selfdeception and disease. Some authors [7] consider that
gross distortions of insight are the rigidifying and
overutilization of normal psychological processes which
may be considered pathological, whereas other researchers have accepted that a certain degree of denial cannot
be considered pathological in Alzheimer’s Disease [8]. It
is interesting to note that, for example, according to
Spanish statutory law [9] no patient can be forced to
know his or her diagnosis and patients have the right to
refuse to be given information about their medical
condition. It is clear that the intention of the legislator is
to allow those self-deception mechanisms to work if the
patient so demands. This contradicts what some studies
have demonstrated regarding disclosure of diagnosis.
Pinner and Bouman [10 . .], for example, found that
patients wanted to be informed and only 6% developed
depressive illness 1 year after the disclosure.
Insight and dementia
It is well known that help-seeking behaviour in
dementia varies significantly across different cultures
[11,12]. In the majority of cases dementia is a
degenerative disease for which available treatments can
only, at most, slow the progression of the disease.
However, present practice informs that the limit
between primary prevention and treatment of a clinically
evident disease is not so clear in dementia patients and
the general elderly population. Therefore, we can come
across patients who may be taking anti-dementia drugs
but who are not aware or do not acknowledge that they
suffer from dementia.
There is no conclusive evidence regarding an association
between lack of insight in dementia and other clinical
correlates such as demographic variables, behavioural
symptoms and severity of dementia [2]. Studies published in the last year have not clarified the situation.
Rymer et al. [13] studied the impact of impaired
awareness of memory problems in a sample of 41
patients with Alzheimer’s disease and found that lack
of insight clearly correlated with greater burden in
caregiving relatives. Insight was measured with the
Discrepancy Questionnaire [14], an instrument that
verifies concordance between patient and caregivers’
answers of a memory problems questionnaire.
In a sample of 15 Alzheimer’s disease patients and 15
healthy elderly controls Wild and Cottrell [15 . .] studied
the relationship between driving abilities, the patient’s
own view of their driving performance, and the
caregivers’ opinion of driving abilities. It was found that
Alzheimer’s disease patients considered themselves
better drivers than they really were in nine out of 10
driving tasks. Controls agreed with the objective
evaluator in nine out of 10 tasks. Lack of insight of
poor driving abilities correlated significantly with poor
insight of cognitive performance.
The study by Duke et al. [16 . .] elegantly proved how
patients with Alzherimer’s disease are able to estimate
adequately the deterioration in functioning, but cannot
acknowledge their own limitations. This is related with
the theoretical consideration we made before: that the
abilities needed for self-knowledge may be different.
Alternatively, denial may be another function that helps
to maintain the self-esteem of the demented patient.
Howorth et al. [17 . .] found in a group of demented
patients that poor insight was more related to short-term
frustration or distress, continuous discontent, lack of
concern, normalization of problems, worry and anxiety,
defensiveness, explicit denial and priority given to other
problems.
Insight has been studied not only in Alzheimer’s disease
patients, but also among patients with mild cognitive
impairment (MCI). Vogel et al. [18 .] found that MCI
patients had the same level of insight of their memory
limitations as Alzheimer’s disease patients. Therefore,
they suggest that subjective memory complaints should
not be included in the diagnostic criteria because that
would increase the false positive rate.
Capacity assessment
Griffith et al. [19 . .] studied financial abilities among the
elderly. They divided their sample into three groups:
elderly controls, amnestic MCI patients, and Alzheimer’s
disease patients. The financial capacity instrument [20]
was used as the outcome variable. These authors found a
robust negative correlation between the severity of the
dementia and the loss of financial abilities. Also patients
with MCI presented a significant loss of financial
abilities showing that functional deterioration is more
prominent than cognitive deficits.
The same group from the University of Alabama
compared financial abilities according to financial capacity instrument with the reported abilities from Alzhei-
Insight and legal capacity in dementia Calcedo-Barba et al. 399
mer’s disease patients and caregivers [21 . .]. They found
that Alzheimer’s disease patients tend to overestimate
their financial abilities in comparison with the reports of
their family caregivers. Also when the reports from
patients and caregivers were studied twice within a 1month interval a significant lack of stability was found in
both groups, which means that patients and caregivers
gave different estimates at different times.
Other questionnaires have been developed and published recently regarding financial capacity. Cramer et al.
[22 .] validated another financial capacity instrument that
showed high correlation with standard dementia questionnaires.
From research to clinical and forensic
practice
Clinical and forensic psychiatry are both applied
disciplines. Regarding capacity assessment the problem
we have to face is what criteria we must use to determine
capacity. From what we have reviewed so far we have
demonstrated two basic approaches: functional and
insight-oriented. The functional approach in dementia
patients has been classified in several domains [3 . .]:
finances, health, independent living and transportation.
The domain of finances can be subdivided in three basic
cognitive skills: (1) declarative knowledge that includes
storing of facts, concepts and events (e.g. investments,
bank statements, etc); (2) procedural knowledge that
includes pragmatic skills such as routines and action
sequences that are basically performed and are less
accessible to conscious recollection (e.g. counting
money, arithmetic verification of financial operations);
and (3) financial judgement as the capacity of rational,
practical, considered and astute decisions in novel,
ambiguous or complex social situations (e.g. sensitivity
to fraud, invulnerability to coercion).
The domain of health relates to medical decision-making
and the different abilities related to that task (making a
choice, understanding, appreciating and reasoning)
[3 . .,4]. This domain also includes other tasks such as
following a diet, taking medication properly and attending visits to doctors or nurses.
The domain of independent living involves the capacity
of patients’ abilities at managing themselves and their
place of residence [3 . .]. Specific tasks of this domain
include household cleaning and maintenance, laundry,
meal shopping and preparation, communication, management of different services for the elderly and personal
hygiene.
Finally, in the transportation domain are included tasks
that deal with driving abilities and the use of public
transportation (e.g. ordering a taxi and use of buses or
subway) [3 . .].
Specific questionnaires have been developed for each of
these domains that quantify specific abilities. These
questionnaires are widely available. It may be that other
domains not included here may also need to be
evaluated (e.g. testamentary capacity) for which we do
not yet have good instruments. It would seem from this
review that capacity assessment in dementia patients is
fairly straightforward. Unfortunately, however, this is not
the case and we believe that one of the major problems
is related to insight.
Lack of insight cannot be considered something similar
to anosognosia or inattention, although in some cases
they may overlap. Dementia patients with poor insight
do not have the same problem in their recognition
abilities as the patients that suffer from visual agnosia.
Likewise, patients with a severe stroke who do not
recognize one side of their own body cannot be
compared with patients with mild dementia who
minimize memory problems. Lack of insight is more
closely related to the attitudes of the patient to the
processes of dementia, and this attitude is closely related
to the cultural environment in which the individual is
embedded.
The problem of lack of insight in dementia patients
cannot be equated to schizophrenia patients who suffer
from delusions (unless the demented patient also
presents delusion as part of the clinical manifestation
of the dementia). The delusional patient has, by
definition, lack of insight, which is something different
from a patient who has activated a defence mechanism of
denial. It should not be forgotten that this denial appears
in the majority of serious chronic diseases. When
qualitative research methodology is applied a much
richer picture of the patient’s reactions to the dementia
process is seen [17 . .,23]. Furthermore, how the illness is
experienced by the individual and the mixing of
personal values and facts have, in the end, a strong
influence on how individuals interpret the signs and
symptoms they suffer [24].
Therefore, functional assessment of capacity is something necessary but not sufficient in a capacity evaluation. On top of this information must be included the
attitudes of patients to their own functional limitations,
and it is at this stage when a careful clinical assessment
must start. We may evaluate, for instance, a patient that
has lost the ability to keep track of her bank accounts
and investments, and her capacity is questioned. For a
good forensic examination we must find out the attitude
of the patient to this situation, and also the strategies she
has developed to deal with her limitations. Psycho-
400 Forensic psychiatry
pathology that can distort this analysis and decisionmaking process must also be evaluated.
Conclusion
Insight is an important concept and clinical phenomenon
that has great utility in forensic practice. However, some
caution must be taken when using insight in relation to
competence assessments. Nowadays we use a functional
approach in which insight could be a very important
contribution. Therefore, measuring the patient’s abilities
would be something necessary but not sufficient in a
competence assessment. Knowing a patient’s attitudes to
her deficits is a key element in an evaluation of
competence. Very interesting research has been published recently to ascertain how aware dementia patients
are of their own deficits. On the other hand, insight
should not be taken in clinical and forensic practice as a
present-or-absent sign. In many cases the clinician must
evaluate carefully how clinically significant is the lack of
awareness the patient may have. It is a delicate
judgement to decide if unawareness of a specific
cognitive or functional deficit is something intrinsically
pathological or is just a healthy way to cope with the
mental and physical ailments of old age.
References and recommended reading
Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review, have
been highlighted as:
.
of special interest
..
of outstanding interest
1
Amador X, David A. Insight and psychosis. New York: Oxford University
Press; 1998.
2
Flashman LA. Disorders of awareness in neuropsychiatric syndromes: an
update. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2002; 4:346–353.
Grisso T. Evaluating competencies. Forensic assessments and instruments.
New York: Kluwer Academic; 2003.
This is an excellent review of all the relevant clinical and research instruments
related with competency assessment. The author proposes a model to evaluate
the instruments and a critique is included of all the instruments.
3
..
10 Pinner G, Bouman WP. Attitudes of patients with mild dementia and their
carers towards disclosure of the diagnosis. Int Psychogeriatr 2003; 15:279–
288.
In this interesting study the vast majority of patients with dementia said that they
preferred to be informed about the diagnosis. Also, the prevalence of depression 1
year after the disclosure of the diagnosis was very small.
..
11 Yamashiro G, Matsuoka JK, Help-seeking among Asian and Pacific
Americans: a multiperspective analysis. Soc Work 1997; 42:176–186.
12 Braun KL, Browne CV. Perceptions of dementia, caregiving, and help
seeking among Asian and Pacific Islander Americans. Health Soc Work
1998; 23:262–274.
13 Rymer S, Salloway S, Norton L, et al. Impaired awareness, behavior
disturbance, and caregiver burden in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc
Disord 2002; 16:248–253.
14 Green J, Goldstein F, Sirochman B, Green R. Variable awareness of deficits
in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1993;
6:159–165.
15 Wild K, Cotrell V. Identifying driving impairment in Alzheimer disease: a
comparison of self and observer reports versus driving evaluation. Alzheimer
Dis Assoc Disord 2003; 17:27–34.
This was an original study in which objective measures of driving performance
were compared with patients’ and caregivers’ opinions. This study indicated a
tightening of driving restrictions for Alzheimer’s disease patients.
..
16 Duke LM, Seltzer B, Seltzer JE, Vasterling JJ. Cognitive components of deficit
awareness in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:359–369.
An original study that proves how denial defense mechanisms work. Patients with
Alzheimer’s disease estimate accurately the impairment of functioning of another
patient seen on videotape, but were unable to estimate accurately their own
deficits.
..
17 Howorth P, Saper J. The dimensions of insight in people with dementia.
Aging Ment Health 2003; 7:113–122.
This was another interesting study that supported the idea that poor insight in
dementia patients is more closely related to emotional problems than cognitive
deterioration.
..
18 Vogel A, Stokholm J, Gade A, et al. Awareness of deficits in mild cognitive
impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: do MCI patients have impaired insight?
Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004; 17:181–187.
These authors prove that MCI patients also have poor insight, at the same level as
Alzheimer’s disease patients.
.
19 Griffith HR, Belue K, Sicola A, et al. Impaired financial abilities in mild
cognitive impairment: a direct assessment approach. Neurology 2003;
60:449–457.
This outstanding study with an elegant design demonstrated how MCI patients
have a deterioration in their financial abilities greater than expected with their
cognitive level.
..
20 Marson DC, Sawrie SM, Snyder S, et al. Assessing financial capacity in
patients with Alzheimer disease: A conceptual model and prototype
instrument. Arch Neurol 2000; 57:877–884.
21 Wadley VG, Harrell LE, Marson DC. Self- and informant report of financial
abilities in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: reliable and valid? J Am Geriatr
Soc 2003; 51:1621–1626.
This very interesting study proved that estimation of the financial abilities of
dementia patients does not reflect the real level of functioning. Also, it was found
that caregivers may under or overestimate the abilities of the patient.
..
4
Appelbaum PS, Grisso T. Assessing patient’s capacities to consent to
treatment. N Engl J Med 1988; 319:1635–1638.
5
Gertler B. Self-knowledge. In: Zalta EN, editor. The Stanford Encyclopedia of
Philosophy (Spring 2003 Edition); http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/
spr2003/entries/self-knowledge. [Accessed 13 April 2004]
6
Babinski J. Contribution to the study of mental problems in organic brain
hemiplegia [in French]. Revue Neurologique 1914; 27:845–848.
7
Sackeim HA. Introduction. The meaning of insight. In: Amador X, David A,
editors. Insight and psychosis. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. pp.
3–12.
22 Cramer K, Tuokko HA, Mateer CA, Hultsch DF. Measuring awareness of
financial skills: reliability and validity of a new measure. Aging Ment Health
2004; 8:161–171.
This was an interesting validation study of a financial capacity instrument,
measuring awareness of financial skills, which is very promising but needs further
research.
8
Sevush S, Leve N. Denial of memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J
Psychiatry 1993; 150:748–751.
23 Clare L. Managing threats to self: awareness in early stage Alzheimer’s
disease. Soc Sci Med 2003; 57:1017–1029.
9
Jefatura del Estado. Ley Básica Reguladora de la Autonomı́a del Paciente y
de Derechos y Obligaciones en Materia de Información y Documentación
Clı́nica. BOE; September 2002. pp. 40126–40132.
24 Fulford KWM. Completing Kraepelin’s psychopathology. Insight, delusion,
and the phenomenology of illness. In: Amador X, David A, editors. Insight and
psychosis. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. pp. 47–65.
.