UC Berkeley
UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report
Title
Morphological Tonal Assignments in Conflict: Who Wins?
Permalink
https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7zk198bk
Journal
UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report, 9(9)
ISSN
2768-5047
Author
Hyman, Larry M
Publication Date
2013
DOI
10.5070/P77zk198bk
eScholarship.org
Powered by the California Digital Library
University of California
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
Morphological Tonal Assignments in Conflict: Who Wins?
Larry M. Hyman
University of California, Berkeley
Presented at the Workshop on “Tons et Paradigmes Flexionnels: Modélisation et Parcimonie/ Disentangling the Inflectional Role of Tone”, Maison de la Recherche, Université de Paris 3, 17-18 June 2013
ABSTRACT
In this paper I am concerned with the following three issues: (i) What is the inventory of
morphological “contributors” to verb tone paradigms? (ii) What happens if the different
contributors conflict? (iii) What does this say about how (tonal) morphology works in general?
Drawing mostly on African examples I show that although tonal morphology can do everything that
non-tonal morphology can do, it can also do much more. The paper ends with cases where tonal
morphology applies at the phrase level, blurring the distinction between phonology, morphology,
and syntax.
1.
Introduction
The goal of this paper is to address the following questions: (i) What is the inventory of
morphological “contributors” to verb tone paradigms? (ii) What happens if the different
contributors conflict? (iii) What does this say about how (tonal) morphology works in general?
In §2 I present examples showing that tonal morphology can do anything that non-tonal
morphology can do. This is followed by an examination of Haya verb stem tonology in §3. In §4
I then present cases that show that the reverse is not true: tonal morphology can do things that
non-tonal morphology cannot do. In these cases which involve tonal action across words, the
result is that tonal morphology often obscures the compartmentalization of phonology,
morphology and syntax. The question of why tone should have such unique properties is
addressed in the conclusion in §5.
2.
Tonal morphology can do whatever non-tonal morphology can do
As discussed in Hyman (2011: 203), it is sometimes claimed that tone cannot mark certain
things. For example, a proposed universal made in the presidential address at the Linguistic
Society of America a few years ago was that “No language uses tone to mark case”. That this is
not true is observed in the following examples from Maasai [Nilotic; Kenya, Tanzania] (Tucker
& Ole Mpaayei 1955: 177-184; cf. Bennett 1974; Plank 1995: 59-62; Payne 2008):
(1)
class I:
class II:
class III:
class IV:
nominative
èlU$kU$nyá
èncU$màtá
èndérònì
ènkólòpà
òlmérégèsh
òlósówùàn
òmótònyî
òsínkìrrî
accusative
èlU@kU@nyá
èncU@mátá
èndèrónì
ènkòlópà
òlmérègèsh
òlósòwùàn
òmótònyî
òsínkìrrî
‘head’
‘horse’
‘rat’
‘centipede’
‘ram’
‘buffalo’
‘bird’
‘fish’
327
nom. vs. acc. tone patterns
Ln-H vs. L-Hn
H on σ2 vs. σ3
H on σ2 & σ3 vs. on σ2 only
identical tones—no change
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
As seen in the examples and summarized to the right, the first three declension classes show only
a tonal difference between their nominative vs. accusative forms. Many other languages could be
cited to show that it is not only case that can be exclusively marked by tone. As Hyman & Leben
(2000: 588) put it, “tonal morphology... exhibits essentially the same range of morphological
properties as in all of segmental morphology”. As common linguistic sense tells us, if tone can
be a morpheme, it can do everything that a morpheme can do.
Concerning verb tone paradigms, the focus of this paper, we therefore expect that anything
that can be marked by a segmental affix or process can also be marked by tone. This includes
inflectional marking of subject, object, transitivity, tense, aspect, mood, negation, clause type etc.
as well as derivational marking of causative, applicative, reciprocal, passive and other verb
forms, as well as processes that derive one word class from another. As an example, verbs are
detransitivized in Kalabari [Ijoid; Nigeria] by assigning a /LH/ melody (Harry & Hyman 2012):
(2)
a.
b.
transitive
kán
kç$n
ányá
Îìmà
sá↓kí
c.
kíkímà
pákI›rI@
gbóló↓má
H
‘tear, demolish’
L
‘judge’
H-H
‘spread’
L-L
‘change’
↓
H- H
‘begin’
H-H-L
‘hide, cover’
H-L-H
‘answer’
H-H-↓H
‘join, mix up’
intransitive
kàán
LH
kç$ç@n
LH
ànyá
L-H
Îìmá
L-H
sàkí
L-H
kìkìmá
L-L-H
pàkI›rI@
L-L-H
gbòlòmá
L-L-H
‘tear, be, demolished’
‘be judged’
‘be spread’
‘change’
‘begin’
‘be hidden, covered’
‘be answered’
‘be joined, mixed up’
The “tone is like everything else” idea would lead us to expect the same degree of uniformity of
tonal exponence as in a segmental paradigm. The same tone or tonal melody should be as
consistent a spell-out of a given morphosyntactic feature. However, this may not always appear
to be the case. Tone has a greater independence (“autosegmentality”) and ability to wander (see
§4). Any study of a tone system with a reasonably complex tonal morphology must find a way to
describe the alternations that take place within the verb system. Verb tone paradigms are often
presented in prose, as a table, or more rarely of individual tone assignment rules. I reproduce an
example of one such table in (3) vs. a set of rules in (4).
(3)
Tone on verb stems in Mambay [Adamawa; Cameroon] (Anonby 2011: 374)
328
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
(4)
Final stem tone assignment rules in Haya [Bantu; Tanzania] (Hyman & Byarushengo 1984: 76)
Such differences raise the issue of how tones should be assigned within a verb paradigm: (i) by
global patterning of partially or fully arbitrary “tonal verb classes”, as proposed for Mambay; (ii)
by reference to the morphosyntactic features of individual cells, as proposed for Haya; (iii) by
some other way? To a large extent it may depend on the situation in the individual language. (I
return to Haya in §3.)
What is particularly striking in such systems is that the tone assignments can conflict in a
number of ways: (i) by domain (e.g. root vs. stem vs. word); (ii) by function (e.g. lexical vs.
derivational vs. inflections); (iii) by morphosyntactic feature (e.g. tense vs. aspect vs. negation).
Such potentials hold whether the base is mono- or polysyllabic, contrasting only two tone heights
or several. In the latter case consider for example the eight tone patterns on monosyllables in Iau
[Indonesian; Papuan], which are lexical on nouns vs. morphological on verbs (↑H = superhigh).
(5)
Tone
H
M
H ↑H
LM
HL
HM
ML
HLM
Nouns
bé
‘father-in-law’
be#
‘fire’
bé↑´ ‘snake’
bè # ‘path’
bê
‘thorn’
bé # ‘flower’
be#` ‘small eel’
bê # ‘tree fern’
Verbs
bá
‘came’
ba#
‘has come’
bá↑´ ‘might come’
bà # ‘came to get’
bâ
‘came to end point’
bá # ‘still not at endpoint’
ba#` ‘come (process)’
bâ # ‘sticking, attached to’
(Bateman 1990: 35-36)
totality of action punctual
resultative durative
totality of action incompletive
resultative punctual
telic punctual
telic incompletive
totality of action durative
telic durative
Although the inflectional categories on Iau verbs in (5) lend themselves to a featural,
paradigmatic display, the portmanteau tone patterns do not appear to be segmentable. From the
summary table in (6) the only generalizations that can be extracted are that telic and
incompletive both begin H and resultative ends mid:
329
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
(6)
telic
Punctual
HL
Durative
HLM
Incompletive HM
totality of action
H
ML
H ↑H
resultative
LM
M
In other cases segmenting the tones by morpheme is straightforward, as in Modo [Central
Sudanic; Sudan] (Nougayrol 2006):
(7)
/ata, H/ ‘be bitter’
átá ‘you are bitter’
H-H
àtá ‘it is bitter’
L-H
1sg, 2sg, 2pl /H-/
3sg. 1pl, 3pl
/L-/
/uba, L/ ‘sing’
úbà ‘you sing’
H-L
ùbà ‘s/he sings’
L-L
As seen, verb roots can be H or L which, when conjugated, can acquire a H- vs. L- prefix. While
the output consists neatly of the four logical combinations in succession of two tones x two tones
(inflection + root) in succession, a consistent exponent can be “subtonal”, consisting of less than
a full tone. For example, the four combinations of person + tense features also produce a fourway distinction in Gban [Mande; Ivory Coast] (Zheltov 2005: 24):
(8)
present
sg. pl.
1st pers.
I‚2
u2
2nd pers. EE2 aa2
3rd pers. E1 ç1
[-raised]
past
sg. pl.
I‚4
u4
EE4 aa4
E3
ç3
[+raised]
(1 = lowest tone, 4 = highest tone)
[+upper]
[-upper]
As seen, tone differs consistently between 1st/2nd vs. 3rd person, the latter being one step lower.
In addition, all tones are two steps higher in the past than they are in the present. In (8) I have
arbitrarily represented the person features as [±upper] and the tense features as [±raised],
although they could have been reversed.
In addition to the above concatenativity, tone (and other prosodic features) show the
familiar sensitivity to internal morphological structure. Thus consider the “tonal layers” [strata]
which Andersen (1992-4: 61) reports for Dinka [Western Nilotic; Sudan], which is monosyllabic,
but polymorphemic:
(9)
wé-ec ‘kick it hither!’ [kick.CENTRIPETAL.2sg]
inflectional layer
derivational layer
root layer
(2sg)
(CP)
(‘kick’)
voice
—
[+breathy]
—
length
—
+1
1
tone
H
L
HL
In derivational terms, the root /wêc/ ‘kick’ acquires breathiness, vowel length, and L tone to
become intermediate wè-ec when undergoing the centripetal (‘hither’) derivation. The L is then
replaced by H to realize the 2nd person singular subject of the imperative. As Andersen puts it,
“The morphological layers are simultaneous but ‘vertically’ ordered, with the root as the
‘deepest’ layer, optionally followed by the derivational layer, followed by an inflectional layer.”
330
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
This results in the above “cyclic” effects. (For more on cyclicity in tonal phonology and
morphology, see Pulleyblank 1985, 1986.)
In a quite different kind of system, polysyllabic Chichewa [Bantu; Malawi], verb stems can
be toneless or can have a single H on either their final or penultimate syllable (Kanerva 1989,
Mtenje 1987, among others). With some dialect differences, H tones are assigned as follows
(Hyman & Mtenje 1999: 98-99):
(10) a.
Final H tone is assigned by
(i) verb roots with a lexical /H/
(ii) certain derivational suffixes such as /-its-/ ‘intensive’, /-ik-/ ‘stative’, /-uk-/ ‘reversive
intr.’ and (in Nkhotakota dialect) passive /-idw-/
(iii) the subjunctive final vowel /-é/ (= the only case of a TAM conditioning final H
(iv) non-reflexive object prefixes (in Nkhotakota dialect).
b.
Penultimate H tone is assigned by
(i) some affirmative tenses
(ii) most negative tenses
(iii) the reflexive prefix /-dzí-/
(iv) object prefixes in Ntcheu dialect (vs. final H in Nkhotakota)
c.
In the absence of one of the above conditioning factors the stem will be toneless.
As seen, final H is mostly lexical and derivational (with the exceptions of subjunctive -é and
object prefixes in Nkhotakota dialect), while penultimate H is inflectional, marking TAM,
negation, the relative prefix, and object prefixes in Ntcheu dialect.
In cases where the more than one morpheme contributes the same tonal assignment, only
one H is realized. Thus, although the following example from Nkhotakota dialect (Sam
Mchombo, pers. comm.) has four sponsors of final H tone, only one final H actually materializes
(Hyman & Mtenje 1999: 101):
(11) [ ti- [ [ [ [ pez- ] -etsets- ] -edw- ] -e ] ] →
H
H
H
H
ti-pez-etsets-edw-é ...
‘let’s be found a lot’
1pl-find-INTENS-PASS-SBJV
In general, in cases of conflict, penultimate H overrides final H—but with one crucial exception:
subjunctive final -é overrides reflexive/object prefix penultimate, presumably because of scope
considerations. While it may seem that all Hs may be assigned by rule, Hyman & Mtenje point to
evidence that at least some Hs need to be underlyingly linked to their sponsor—but still follow
instructions as to where to go (to the final or penultimate syllable). In (12a), the object prefix /H/
shifts to the penult (Ntecheu dialect). (The remote past tense marker is /-naa-/ to which the H of
the subject prefix /tí-/ spreads. The phrase-penultimate vowel lengthens by general rule.)
(12) a.
/tí-naa-mú-fotokozer-a/ →
|
|
Hi
Hj
tí-naa-fotokozéer-a
|
|
Hi
Hj
‘we explained to him’
b.
/ti-ná-mú-fotokozer-a/ →
| |
H H→Ø
tí -na-mu-fotokozeer-a
|
H
‘we explained to him’
331
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
However, whenever an object prefix is preceded by a H tone morpheme, there is no penultimate
H tone. This is seen in (12b), where the general past prefix is /-ná-/. As shown, the /H/ of the
/-mú-/ is deleted by what is commonly known as Meeussen’s Rule in Bantu: the second of two
Hs in succession is deleted. Since it would be odd to have a rule that said “object prefixes assign
a H to the penult unless they are preceded by a H”, I follow the earlier account with underlying
sequences of Hs as in (12b).
Finally, note that there no rules in Chichewa which shift a pre-existing final H to the
penult: all penultimate assignment rules insert a /H/ at the same time. For this reason one cannot
assume that the penultimate pattern simply applies an instruction to mark off the last syllable as
extrametrical. Hyman & Mtenje’s (1999: 102) proposal is that final H is assigned at the stem
domain, while penultimate H is assigned within a larger “macro-stem” domain. The “later”
macro-stem domain thus overrides the earlier stem domain. The major exception is subjunctive
-é, which is stem-level despite its greater scope.
Other cases in the literature show that the “uppermost” morphological structure wins. Thus,
Inkelas (2011: 75) provides the following word tree structure from Hausa [Chadic; Nigeria]:
(13)
nèn-nè:mó: ‘seek repeatedly!’
nén-né:mó:
i.e. imperative >> ventive >> base
né:mó:
CVC-
nè:má: (LH)
‘seek’
PLURACTIONAL-
-ó: (H)
-VENTIVE
-Ø (LH)
-IMPERATIVE
As seen, there is an override system of imperative >> ventive >> base not unlike the Dinka
example: inflectional tone overrides derivational tone which in turn overrides base tone.
In addition to “layers” (cycles, strata, domains) based on derivational vs. inflectional
morphology, paradigmatic conflicts may require a hierarchical ranking of the tonal spell-outs by
inflectional features (tense, aspect, mood, negation). A case of this arises in Leggbó [Cross
River; Nigeria] (Hyman, Narrog, Paster & Udoh 2002: 407). In the following table, the first
indicated tone goes on the root and the second on a suffix (if present). (MCA = main clause
affirmative; SRA = subject relative affirmative clause; ORA = object relative affirmative clause;
NEG = negative (all clause types); “irrealis” = future/conditional.)
(14) a.
Root tone:
Perf./Prog.
MCA/ORA
/L/
/M/
H-M M-M
SRA
NEG
/L/
/M/
/L/
/M/
L-M M-M H-M M-M
Habitual
Irrealis
L-L
L-L
L-L
L-L
Root tone:
MCA/ORA
/L/
/M/
b.
Perf./Prog.
Habitual
Irrealis
H-M
L-L
M-L
M-L
M-M
M-L
M-L
M-L
SRA
/L/
/M/
L-M
L-L
M-M
M-L
L-L
M-L
H-M
L-L
M-M
M-L
(irrealis assigns L-L / M-L)
NEG
/L/
/M/
H-M
H-M
332
M-M
M-M
(other than irrealis, negatives
assign H-M / M-M)
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
c.
Root tone:
Perf./Prog.
Habitual
MCA/ORA
/L/
/M/
H-M
L-L
M-M L-M
M-L L-L
M-M
M-L
L-L
M-L
Irrealis
d.
Root tone:
Perf./Prog.
Habitual
MCA/ORA
/L/
/M/
H-M
SRA
/L/
/M/
/L/
SRA
/M/
M-M L-M
L-L M-L
M-M
L-L
M-L
Irrealis
NEG
/L/
/M/
H-M M-M
(other than irrealis and negatives,
habituals assign L-L / M-L)
NEG
/L/
/M/
H-M M-M
(unshaded leftover cells require
specific tone assignments)
Although Leggbó noun roots lexically contrast H, M and L tone there is only a binary contrast on
verb roots: M tone roots vs. roots which alternate between H and L. In (14) I have represented
the contrast as one between /L/ and /M/. In looking over such tabular arrays, the strategy is to
start with tone assignments that affect an entire row or column. We see first see that no column
has the same tonal pattern throughout. However, in (14a) there is one row that is consistent: the
irrealis assigns a L tone suffix deriving L-L and M-L patterns independent of clause type or
negation. Once we take this predictable assignment out of the equation we see in (14b) that the
next generalization is that negation assigns H-M to L roots and M-M to M roots. In (14c) we can
now see that the habitual aspect assigns L-L/M-L to cells unclaimed by the irrealis or negation.
This leaves a few leftover cells in (14d) where the lexical L vs. M root tones are realized with a
M suffix. By following this precedure Hyman et al (2002) were able to establish the following
ordered hierarchy, where earlier assignments block later tonal assignments:
(15)
Irrealis >> Negative >> Habitual >> Other
L-L/M-L
H-M/M-M
L-L/M-L
Hyman & Olawsky (2004: 107) follow the same procedure in analyzing verb tones in
Dagbani [Gur; Ghana], shown in the tables in (16). (Incipient = ‘about to’; tones in parentheses
were inadvertenly not elicited, but are extrapolated.)
(16) a.
Present
Rec.Past
Gen.Past
Incipient
Future
b.
Present
Rec.Past
Gen.Past
Incipient
Future
MCA
Perf Imperf
LH
LH
LH
LH
LH
LH
lex
LH
H
H
MCN
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
H
H
RCA
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
(H)
(H)
RCN
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
(LH)
H
H
MCA
Perf Imperf
LH
LH
LH
LH
LH
LH
lex
LH
MCN
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
RCA
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
RCN
Perf Imperf
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
H
333
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
c.
MCA
Perf Imperf
Present
Rec.Past
Gen.Past
Incipient
Future
d.
LH
LH
lex
RCA
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
RCN
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
RCA
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
RCN
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
H
MCA
Perf Imperf
Present
Rec.Past
Gen.Past
Incipient
Future
MCN
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
LH
LH
lex
MCN
Perf Imperf
lex
lex
LH
lex
lex
H
In 16a) we observe that the future assigns the same H tone to all verbs in all contexts. Once the
future is removed, in (16b) we see that the imperfective assigns LH tone to all remaining verbs.
In (16c) we assign LH tone to main clause affirmative perfectives—with the exception of the
incipient . At this point, in (16d), the remaining perfectives (non-MCA and incipient) receive
their lexical H or L tone as a kind of default. (It is interesting to note that the underlying verb
root tones are distinguishable only in these contexts.) We can in fact combine (16b,c) to arrive at
the following ranking:
(17)
Future
H
>> {MCA [-Incip], [Imperfective]}
LH
>>
Lexical (default)
H vs. L
It can be noted that in both Leggbó and Dagbani, future tense ~ irrealis mood are ranked higher
than aspect, as per Bybee’s (1985) Relevance Hierarchy. We should think of such disjunctive
tone patterns as vying for the same “slot” exactly as Anderson (1986) discussed some time ago
for Georgian prefixes. With this in mind we now return to a more complicated case from Haya,
which was briefly displayed in (4).
3.
Haya verb stem tonology
Recall the Mambay and Haya cases in (3) and (4), neither of which recognized hierarchies as in
Leggbó and Dagbani. In Mambay, Anonby (2011) sets up a table of “tone classes” to summarize
the paradigmatic tones across different TAMs, while Hyman & Byarushengo (1984) present a
number of morphological rules assigning tones by specific combinations of inflectional features.
At the time I considered Haya to be relatively unruly, defying generalization. I now return after
three decades to see if we can do better. In what follows I am concerned only with the
assignment of H tones within the verb stem (root + suffix(es)), not with the prefixal domain.
As seen in (18), Haya verb stems may have one of four tone patterns with at most one H
tone, predictable from whether the root is underlying /H/ or toneless and whether there is a
suffixal H or not:
334
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
(18)
a.
b.
c.
d.
root tone + suffix tone
/H/
/H/
/Ø/
/H/
/H/
/Ø/
/Ø/
/Ø/
output H
on final vowel
on second mora
on first mora
none
suffix H is assigned to FV; root H is deleted
suffix H is realized on second mora of stem
root realizes its /H/ on its first mora
no stem H
As summarized to the right, a single H may be realized on the final vowel, the second mora, or
the first mora, depending on the input tones. In the one case where the root is toneless and there
is no suffix H, the verb stem will be toneless. Examples are given in (19), where the tones are
indicated as they appear before the application of postlexical tone rules (Hyman & Byarushengo
1984: 60):
(19)
a.
root + sfx
/H/ /H/
output H
on FV
underlying
(ba-) /kom-il-e/
H
b.
/Ø/
/H/
on µ2
→
output of lexical phonology
(ba-) kom-il-é
‘they tied up’ (Past2)
→
(ba-) jun-íl-e
H
H
(ba-) /jun-il-e/
c.
/H/
/Ø/
on µ1
(ba-aa-) /kom-il-e/
H
d.
/Ø/
/Ø/
Ø
‘they helped’ (Past2)
H
H
→
(b-áa-) kóm-il-e
H
‘they have tied up’ (Perf)
H
(ba-aa-) /jun-il-e/
(b-áa-) jun-il-e
‘they have helped’ (Perf)
H
The main clause affirmative forms in (19a,b) are in the yesterday past tense (Past2) which
requires a suffixal H, while those in (19c,d) are in the perfect (Perf), which does not assign a H.
In fact, such suffix tones are assigned by the morphology in one of three ways: (i) a suffixal H is
assigned to the FV, e.g. the Past2 tense in (19a,b); (ii) no suffixal H is assigned, e.g. the Perfect
tense-aspect in (19c,d); (iii) a suffixal H is assigned to the FV only if the root is toneless. This is
the case in the past habitual (PH) forms in (20).
(20)
a.
root + sfx
/H/ /-Ø/
b.
/Ø/
output H
on µ1
underlying
(ba-a-) /kom-ag-a/
on µ2
(ba-a-) /jun-ag-a/
→
output of lexical phonology
(ba-a-) kóm-ag-a ‘they used to tie up’ (PH)
→
(ba-)
H
H
/-H/
Ø
H
jun-ág-a
‘they used to help’ (PH)
H
In (20a) there is no suffixal H tone, since the root /-kóm-/ ‘tie up’ has H tone. In (20b), however,
there is a suffix H, since /-jun-/ ‘help’ is toneless. This H is realized on the second mora of the
verb stem, as expected. Hyman & Byarushengo refer to this as the “polar H” suffix, since it is
present if the root is Ø, but absent if the root is H. One might propose that the H suffix is
assigned to all verb forms in such tenses, but that it is subsequently deleted after a H root. The
same Meeussen’s Rule applying in (12b) in Chichewa is independently required in Haya to
delete the second of two Hs are on adjacent moras, so one might first shift the suffixal H to the
second mora and then delete it after a H root initial mora. Since the two types of H suffix would
still have to be distinguished (those which would shift even after a H root vs. those which don’t),
I will instead assume that there are two different H assignment rules, one of which is sensitive to
whether the root has a H tone or not.
335
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
With this established we now can consider which combinations of inflectional features and
clause types require which finals. In the following table, H indicates that a suffix H is assigned
when the root is either Ø or /H/, while (H) indicates the polar suffix which is assigned only when
the root is Ø. Those cells which do not take either suffixal H are indicated by Ø:
(21)
MCA
H
Ø
H
Ø
(H)
(H)
H
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
SRA
ORA
MCN
SRN
ORN
Focus
Pres Hab
Ø
H
(H)
(H)
(H)
---+++
Past1
Ø
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
---+++
Past2
Ø
H
H
H
H
---+++
Past3
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
+--+++
Past Hab
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
---+++
Future1
(H)
(H)
(H)
(H)
(H)
---+++
Future2
Ø
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
---+++
Progressive
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Perfect
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Inceptive
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Persistive
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Subjunctive
(H)
(H)
++
Imperative
H
(= subjunctive)
++
(MCA = main clause affirmative; SRA = subject relative affirmative; ORA = object relative affirmative;
MCN = main clause negative; SRN = subject relative negative; ORN = object relative negative.)
As seen, Haya distinguishes three degrees of past tense, and two degrees of future tense. The
inceptive is translated as ‘to have done X before’, while the persistive translates as ‘to still do X’.
(I address the last Focus column below.) As in the Leggbó and Dagbani cases, I have begun by
shading in the rows that have the same tone assignment throughout. As seen, the Future1 assigns
a H suffix tone throughout, despite the fact that the segmental morphology may differ between
main vs. relative and affirmative vs. negative clauses. In the following examples it is observed
that Future1 takes the marker /-la-V-/ in the affirmative, but only the empty mora /-V-/ in the
negative (-V- is realized by lengthening of the preceding vowel, i.e. [a] in these examples):
(22) a.
AFF
(ba-la-a-) kom-a vs.
(ba-la-a-) /jun-a/
b.
NEG
(ti-ba-a-) kom-e
‘they will tie up/help’ (F1)
H
H
vs.
(ti-ba-a-) /jun-e/
‘they will not tie up/help (F1)
H
H
Note that the FVs are also different in the affirmative vs. negative, and yet the polar suffixal H
remains constant. In addition, a number of TAMs fail to have a suffix H, which I have also
shaded. One other important generalization is that although the segmental morphology can vary,
any given tense has the same negative tones, whether in the MCN, SRN, or ORN. I shall
therefore conflate the last three columns as NEG in what follows.
In (23) I reorganize the material in the earlier table by their affirmative tone patterns to
reveal that there are in fact three TAM “tone classes”, with affirmative TAM residues marked by
an asterisk (I will deal with each of these separately below):
336
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
(23)
1a
1b
1c
1d
2a
2b
2c
3a
3b
Past2
Present Habitual
Future2
Past1
Future1
Past Habitual
Past3
Perf, Incep, Persist
Progressive
#H
=
# (H)
=
#Ø
=
MCA
H
H
H
Ø*
(H)
(H)
Ø*
Ø
H*
4
2
5
SRA
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
(H)
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
3
8
ORA
H
H
H
H
(H)
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
4
3
4
NEG
H
(H)
Ø
Ø
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
1
2
8
Focus
---+
---+
---+
±--+
---+
---+
+--+
++++
++++
=9
= 10
= 25
comments:
none of these change their TAM
segmental marking in neg (or affirm)
MCA has different segmental marking
(H) throughout, -la- prefix only in AFF
all have same segmental TAM marking
MCA has different segmental marking
all Ø; Perf, Incep change marking
MCA has different segmental marking
the most common = Ø, hence Hyman &
Byarushengo propose rules assigning Hs
Since there are three possible suffix tone assignments (H, (H), Ø) and three affirmative clause
types (MCA, SRC, ORA), there are in principle 3 x3 = nine possible tone assignments. Instead,
there are three general affirmative patterns, with the asterisked exceptions to be explained below.
(There are nine patterns when we include negatives, not 27.) Class 1 consists of TAMs which
have H-Ø-H tonal suffix assignments in the three affirmative columns. Class 2 consists of TAMs
which have polar (H) tonal suffix assignments throughout. Class 3 consists of those TAMs which
do not receive either type of suffixal H. In other words, we come close to being able to equate
class 1 with -H, class 2 with -(H), and class 3 with -Ø. (The class 1 SRA forms present an
obvious obstacle.) For reference, I have included comments on the segmental morphology in the
last column. Full paradigms of examples are available in Hyman & Byarushengo (1984: 93-101).
I have yet to discuss the focus column, where [+F] refers to TAMs which resist H tone
deletion when non-final in their clause, while those which reduce their H tones are marked [-F].
For example in (24a) that the present habitual affirmative final H suffix is deleted when a word
follows:
(24) a.
b.
Pres Hab Affirmative is [-F] :
All negatives are [+F]
:
/ba-jun-á/
→
/ti-ba-jun-á/ →
ba-jun-a káto
‘they help Kato’
ti-ba-jun-á káto ‘they don’t help Kato
In the corresponding negative in (24b), however, which is built by prefixing ti- to the affirmative,
the same final suffix H does not delete. As indicated, all negatives are [+F], whereas affirmative
TAMs can be [+F] or [-F], as discussed by Hyman & Watters (1984: 259-262), who argue that
the [+F] TAMs are “intrinsically focused”. The reason for going into this is that there is further
generalization: all tensed affirmative [+F] are Ø except the MCA progressive, which receives a
H suffix. Progressive forms are illlustrated in (25).
(25) a.
b.
MCA marked by ni- : /ni-ba-jun-á/
→ ni-ba-jun-á káto
‘they are helping Kato’
MCN Prog with -li : /ti-bá-lí-ku-jun-a/ → ti-bá-li-ku-jun-a káto ‘they aren’t helping Kato’
NEG-they-COP-INF-help-FV
c.
SRA Prog with -li : /á-ba-lí-ku-jun-a/ → a-bá-li-ku-jun-a káto ‘they who are helping Kato’
The reason why the MCA progressive is exceptionally in class 1 is that it is built by adding the
focus marker ni- to the present habitual (cf. ni káto ‘it’s Kato’). While the progressive is
intrinsically [+F] and therefore should have a -Ø final, its H suffixal tone is a carry-over from the
337
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
present habitual. The two other asterisked exceptions can also be accounted for historically (see
below).
What the above analysis suggests is that the tone patterns can be assigned by classes of
TAMs, at least as far as the affirmative forms are concerned. The following questions thus
naturally arise:
First, do the affirmative groupings represent natural classes of TAM features? It does not
appear so. Class 1 includes the present habitual, the two more recent past tenses (Past1, Past2),
and the general future tense (Future2). Class 2 is also incoherent: while past habitual and Past3 go
together as distant pasts (they partially share segmental morphology as well), the near future
(Future1) doesn’t. Finally, however, class 3 has been claimed to be coherent: all are [+F]. (I am
limiting my attention to the indicative TAMs, since the imperative and subjunctive, both [+F],
take H and (H), respectively.)
The second question concerns whether one can predict one column from another. The quick
answer is: not across the board. In class 1 the MCA and ORA are identical, both receiving
suffixal H (vs. SRA Ø). Class 2 affirmatives all take suffixal H, with the MCA Past3 being
exception (see below). We have already seen that all class 3 TAMs are Ø except for the MCA
progressive explained above.
A third question is what the significance is of the near future (F1) always receiving a polar
(H) suffixal tone? (There is undoubtedly an historical explanation involving the source of the
empty -V- mora alluded to above.)
What remains to be explained is why the MCA Past1 and Past3 are exceptional. The Past1
should be H, but is exceptionally Ø. The reason for this is seen in (26).
(26) a.
b.
y-áa-kóm-a
‘he tied up’
[+F]
y-a-kom-a káto ‘he tied up Kato’ [-F]
(= “disjoint” form)
(= “conjoint” form)
As seen, when the verb is phrase-final, the tense marker is -áa-. When it is followed by any word
within the same clause, as in (26b), not only do the H tones of the tense marker and verb root
reduce, but the tense marker is now -a-, i.e. short. This is the only tense that does this. The
prefixal differences in (26a,b) are in fact a relic of an older system which Meeussen (1959)
termed “disjoint” vs. “conjoint” verb forms: -áa- was the focused (or disjoint) marker of this
tense and -a- the unfocused (or conjoint) marker, as they are in the recent past in Kirundi. My
hypothesis therefore is that phrase-final Past1 ends Ø rather than H because the form with -áa- is
in fact [+F]. We of course can’t tell this for certain, because the [±F] distinction has an effect
only when something follows the verb. The second part of the hypothesis is that the non-final
form with -a- is not only [-F], as we know from (26b), but also takes a suffixal H in line with
other class 1 TAMs, which however automatically deletes. In this way Past1 falls into place.
There is also an historical explanation for why Past3 is [+F] only in the MCA. First, as seen
in (27), it is only in the MCA that it is marked with the prefix -ka- and the FV -a, as in (27a).
(27) a.
b.
/bá-ka-jun-a/
‘they helped’
→
/a-ba-a-jun-íl-e/ ‘they who helped’ →
bá-ka-jun-a káto
a-ba-a-jun-il-e káto
‘they helped Kato’ [+F]
‘they who helped Kato’ [-F]
In the SRA in (27b) the markers are -a- and the final -il-e, which also occur in Past3 negative
forms. What’s significant is that the prefixal and suffixal segmental allomorphy corresponds with
[±F]. This is because the -ka- prefix has a rather complex history (see Botne 1999 and Nurse
338
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
2008 for discussion). In Haya, -ka- has an interesting complementary distribution in three
different contexts: In MCA, it marks distant past (Past3), as in (27a). In negative clauses it marks
the perfect and incipient, contrasting with the affirmative as in (28a,b).
(28) a.
b.
MCA perfect :
MCN perfect :
b-áa-jun-il-e
ti-bá-ka-jun-il-e
‘they have helped’
‘they haven’t helped’
[+F]
[+F]
Its third function is to mark past consecutive clauses following either a negative or relative
clause (‘he didn’t come and see me’, ‘the man who came and saw me’). To account for the
exceptional Ø and [+F] of the MCA Past3 form all we need to say is that these were features of
its originally function (I would guess perfect, since the perfect tends to be inherently focused in
the sense of Hyman & Watters 1984).
As seen, although Haya tonal assignments appear to be somewhat chaotic on first approach,
there are some major regularities, which are obscured in a few cases by recent changes in the
TAM system. What then can we conclude from this admittedly limited look at three languages?
If Leggbó, Dagbani, and Haya are representative—and I can add that other Bantu languages are
often like Haya—then I would suggest two generalizations: (i) there is a tendency for negatives
to be tonally identical across clause types (MCN, SRN, ORN); (ii) there is a tendency for a
future or irrealis to be tonally identical across clause types and negation. However, I would not
be surprised to find completely contradictory systems, as TAM systems are relatively unstable
and may change in one, but not other clause types, as we saw in the case of Haya Past3.
4.
Tonal morphology can do more than non-tonal morphology can do
In §1 I suggested that tonal morphology often obscures the compartmentalization of phonology,
morphology and syntax. In this final section I would like to demonstrate this, and at the same
time show that grammatical tone can do things that non-tone cannot.
The first example comes from Kikuria [Bantu; Tanzania, Kenya], which assigns a H tone to
one of the first four moras of the verb stem, depending on the tense (Marlo & Mwita 2009: 2).
As seen in (29), once this H is assigned to the underlined mora, the H spreads to the penult:
(29) a.
b.
c.
d.
µ1
µ2
µ3
µ4
n-to-on-to-on-to-reto-ra-
[ hóótóótér-a
[ hoótóótér-a
[ hootóótér-a
[ hootoótér-a
‘we have reassured’
‘we have been reassuring’
‘we will reassure’
‘we are about to reassure’
Past
Past progressive
Future
Inceptive
The interesting question which arises is: What happens if the verb stem is too short, i.e. doesn’t
have enough moras for the intended H tone assignment, e.g. to the fourth mora? Marlo & Mwita
demonstrate the results as in (30).
(30) a.
b.
c.
d.
µ4
µ4
µ4
µ4
to-rato-rato-rato-ra-
[ karaaNg-á
[ sukur-a&
[ Bun-aº
[ ry-aº
‘we are about to fry’
‘we are about to rub’
‘we are about to break’
‘we are about to eat’
H tone assignment:
[ sukur-a µ@
[ Bun-a µ µ@
[ ry-a µ µ µ@
In (30a) the verb stem has four moras and the H therefore is assigned to the FV. In (30b), where
the stem is one mora short, a rising tone is obtained. When the stem is either two or three moras
short as in (30c,d), there is a level Lº tone, as if the H tone is floating after the verb, keeping the
339
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
L tone from downgliding, as a prepausal L would normally do. What is extremely interesting is
that when the verb is non-final, the mora count continues onto the next word. Marlo & Mwita
show this with the toneless noun object eƒetççkE ‘banana’:
(31) a.
b.
c.
d.
µ4
µ4
µ4
µ4
to-rato-rato-rato-ra-
[ karaaNg-á
[ sukur-a
[ Bun-a
[ ry-a
éƒétç@ç@kE
éƒétç@ç@kE
eƒétç@ç@kE
eƒetç@ç@kE
‘we are about to fry a banana’
‘we are about to rub a banana’
‘we are about to break a banana’
‘we are about to eat a banana’
Again, the H is assigned to the FV in (31), since the verb stem has four moras. The H continues
to spread to the penult of the noun object. In (31b) it is assigned to the first mora of the noun and
then again spreads to the penult. In (31c) the H is assigned to the second mora of the noun, and
then spreads. Finally, in (31d), the H is assigned to the third mora of the noun and spreads just
one mora to the penult. Such an array of tone assignments is already quite remarkable
(particularly to the fourth mora). However, what is really unusual is that this suffixal H is
expected to be stem- or perhaps word-level morphology (although the prefixes are irrelevant), as
in other Bantu languages, but is calculated at the phrase level! There seems to be a violation of a
basic principle, a violation of what we might think of as canonical morphology (Corbett 2007):
Morphs should stay on their own word! Instead, we have something that seems like cophonologies operating at the phrase level. (For co-phonologies, see Inkelas 2011 and references
cited therein.)
A second such violation occurs in the rather restricted tone system of Chimwiini [Bantu;
Somalia], which has the following properties (Kisseberth 2009): (i) Tone is only grammatical.
There are no tonal contrasts on lexical morphemes, e.g. noun stems or verb roots. (ii) Privative H
tone is limited to the last two syllables: final H vs. penultimate H. As an example consider the
paradigm in (32).
(32)
1st pers.
2nd pers.
3rd pers.
final H:
penult H:
singular
n- ji:lé
ji:lé
≠
jí:le
‘I ate’
‘you sg. ate’
‘s/he ate’
plural
chi- chi-ji:lé
ni- ni-ji:lé
wa- wa-jí:le
‘we ate’
‘you pl. ate’
‘they ate’
As seen, first and second person subjects condition final H tone in the past tense, while third
person subjects condition penultimate H tone. As also seen in the above table, the only difference
between second and third person singular is tonal. It is clear that tone has a morphological
function in the above examples.
But the plot thickens when an object is added, as in (33).
(33) a.
b.
jile: n5amá
jile: n5áma
‘you sg. ate meat’
‘s/he ate meat’
jile ma-tu:ndá
jile ma-tú:nda
‘you sg. ate fruit’
‘s/he ate fruit’
We now see that the final vs. penultimate distinction is realized on the noun object. The tonal
morphology is thus phrasal. Kisseberth (2009) also shows that phrasal domains can be nested,
depending on information structure, with each right edge receiving the appropriate final or
penultimate H:
(34) a.
Ø-wa-t5ind5il5il5e w-a:ná ] n5amá ] ka: chi-sú ]
340
'you sg. cut for the children meat with a knife'
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
b.
Ø-wa-t5ind5il5il5e w-á:na ] n5áma ] ka: chí-su ]
's/he cut for the children meat with a knife'
What we have here is a case of tone being able to have long distance effects: If the H had been a
lexical property, say, of the verb root, as it can be in Digo (Kisseberth 1984) or Giryama (Volk
2011), we would treat this as pure phonology. Given however that the tonal distinctions are
exclusively grammatical, conditioned by specific grammatical morphemes or constructions, we
have to address the question of what exactly this is? If tone is a phrasal clitic, it’s quite unlike
English possessive ’s, which serves an appropriate grammatical function at the end of a noun
phrase. Should the above tonal distinctions be identified with:
(35) a.
b.
c.
c.
morphology?
phonology?
syntax?
intonation?
= a property of [1st/2nd pers.] vs. [3rd pers.] subject prefixes
= a property of the phonological phrase—H is semi-demarcative)
= a property of the syntactic configurations which define the P-phrases
= not likely—who ever heard of a 1st/2nd vs. 3rd person intonation?
As seen, both the Kikuria and Chimwiini cases do a good job of obscuring the boundaries
between morphemes, words and phrases, and ultimately phonology, morphology, and syntax.
Both are effective examples of how tone can function as the glue holding a grammar together.
This brings us to the last question: What else can do this other than tone? What can be sponsored
by a morpheme in one word but travel at considerable distance to be realized on another? There
are more such cases, in fact (see Harry & Hyman 2012 for a preliminary survey). Speakers of
languages seem to be better equipped to package and exploit melodies and other syntagmatic
properties of pitch at both the word and phrase level than any other phonological property. It
seems superfluous at this time, but I can’t help repeating: Tone is different! (Hyman 2011)
References
Andersen, Torben. 1992-1994. Morphological stratification in Dinka: on the alternations of voice quality,
vowel length, and tone in the morphology of transitive verbal roots in a monosyllabic language.
Studies in African Linguistics 23.1-63.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1986. Disjunctive ordering in inflecitonal morphology. Natural Language and
Linguistic Theory 4.1-31.
Anonby, Erik J. 2011. A grammar of Mambay: An Adamawa language of Chad and Cameroon. Köln:
Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.
Bateman, Janet. 1990. Iau segmental and tonal phonology. Miscellaneous Studies of Indonesian and
Other Languages in Indonesia 10.29-42.
Bennett, Patrick R. 1974. Tone and the Nilotic case system. Bulletin of the School of African and Oriental
Studies 37.19-28.
Botne, Robert. 1999. Future and distal -ka-’s: Proto-Bantu or nascent form(s)? In Jean-Marie Hombert &
Larry M. Hyman (eds), Bantu historical linguistics: Theoretical and empircal perspectives, 473-515.
Stanford: C.S.L.I.
Broadwell, George Aaron. 2000. Macuiltianguis Zapotec tone paradigms. Ms. SUNY Albany.
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology. A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Corbett, Greville G. 2007. Canonical typology, suppletion, and possible words. Language 83.8-42.
Harry, Otelemate & Larry M. Hyman. 2012. Construction Tonology: The Case of Kalabari. UC Berkeley
presentation February 27-2012. Revised and expanded version submitted. Early draft posted:
Hyman, Larry M. 2011. Tone: Is it different? In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd edition, 197239. Blackwell.
341
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
—. 2012. Issues in the phonology-morphology interfacein African languages. To appear in the
proceedings of the 43rd Annual Conference on African Languages, Tulane University, Marhc 15-17,
2012. Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
— & Ernest Rugwa Byarushengo. 1984. A model of Haya tonology. In G. N. Clements & John
Goldsmith (eds), Autosegmental studies in Bantu tone, 53-103. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.
— & William R. Leben. 2000. Suprasegmental processes. In Handbook on Inflection and Word
Formation, 587-594. Berlin: De Gruyter.
— & Al Mtenje. 1999. Prosodic morphology and tone: the case of Chichewa. In Harry van der Hulst,
René Kager & Wim Zonneveld, eds., The prosody-morphology interface, 90-133. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
— , Heiko Narrog, Mary Paster, & Imelda Udoh. 2002. Leggbó verb inflection: A semantic and
phonological particle analysis. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Berkeley Linguistic Society Meeting,
399-410.
— & Knut Olawsky. 2004. Dagbani verb tonology. In Trends in African Linguistics 4, 97-108. Africa
World Press, Inc.
— & John R. Watters. 1984. Auxiliary focus. Studies in African Linguistics 15.233-273.
Inkelas, Sharon. 2011. The interaction between morphology and phonology. In Handbook of
Phonological Theory, 2nd edition, 68-102. Blackwell.
Kanerva, Jonni. 1989. Focus and phrasing in Chichewa phonology. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford
University.
Kisseberth, Charles W. 1984. Digo tonology. In G. N. Clements & John Goldsmith (eds), Autosegmental
studies in Bantu tone, 105-182. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.
— , 2009. The theory of prosodic phrasing: the Chimwiini evidence. ACAL 40, Urbana-Champaign,
April 9-11, 2009.
Marlo, Michael & Chacha Mwita. 2009. Problems in Kuria H tone assignment. Ms. Indiana University &
Kenyatta University.
Meeussen, A. E. 1959. Essai de grammaire rundi. Tervuren: Musée Royal de l’Afrique Centrale.
Mtenje, Al. 1987. Tone shift principles in the Chichewa verb: a case for a tone lexicon. Lingua 72.169209.
Nougayrol, Pierre. 2006. Tones and verb classes in Bongo. In Al-Amin Abu-Manga, Leoma Gilley,
&Anne Storch (eds), Insights into Nilo-Saharan Language, History and Culture, 335-345. Köln:
Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.
Nurse, Derek. 2008. Tense and aspect in Bantu. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Payne, Doris. 2008. XX http://darkwing.uoregon.edu/~maasai/Maa%20Language/masyntax.htm
Plank, Frans. 1995. (Re-)introducing Suffixaufnahme. In Frans Plank (ed.), Double case: agreement by
Suffixaufnahme, 3-110. OUP.
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1985) A Lexical Treatment of Tone in Tiv. in D.L. Goyvaerts (ed.) African
Linguistics, 421-476. Benjamins.
—. 1986. Tone in lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Tucker, A. N. and J. tompo Ole Mpaayei. 1955. A Maasai Grammar with Vocabulary. London:
Longmans, Green and Company.
Volk, Erez. 2011. Mijikenda tonology. Doctoral dissertation, Tel Aviv University.
Zheltov, Alexander. 2005. Le système des marqueurs de personnes en gban. Mandenkan 41.23-28.
APPENDIX
Since the workshop at which this paper was presented was organized by Jean-Léo Léonard and
Enrique Palancar, specialists of Mexican tone systems, I decided to offer a few words on an
interesting tonal paradigm from Macuiltianguis Zapotec for which Broadwell (2000) presents
evidence for the following (partial) structure:
342
UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2013)
VERB
ASPECT
completive Lhabitual Ø- [M]
potential H-
1sg. H
(→ stressed syllable)
ROOT
(base tones)
In the following table, two possible input systems are considered: Broadwell’s /H, M, L, Ø/ vs.
/H, M, Ø/. The stressed syllable is underlined.
Base Tone
(Broadwell) (without /M/)
Ø-M
Ø-Ø
Ø-M
Ø-Ø
Ø-H
Ø-H
Ø-H
Ø-H
Ø-L
Ø-L
L-M
L-Ø
L-H
L-H
L-H
L-H
LH
LH
L
L
H-M
H-Ø
HL + H
HL + H
(underline =
stress)
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
H
Ø
Completive
LH-M
L-M
L-H
L-M
H-H
L-H
L-H
L-H
H-L
L-L
HL-L
L-M
HL-L
L-H
L-H
L-H
HL
LH
HL
L
H-M
H-M
HL-H
HL- H
Habitual
ØH-M
M-M
M-H
M-M
H-H
M-H
M-H
M-H
H-L
M-L
HL-L
L-M
HL-L
L-H
L-H
L-H
HL
LH
HL
L
H-M
H-M
HL- H
HL- H
Potential
HH-M
H-M
H-M
H-M
H-H
H-H
H-H
H-L
H-L
H-L
HL-L
HL-L
HL-L
HL-L
HL-H
HL-H
LH
LH
HL
L
H-M
H-M
LH- H
LH- H
assignment of grammatical
Hs:
H1:1sg
H1:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H2:1sg
H2:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg H1:1sg
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
The rules that appear to be needed are as follows:
1.
2.
3.
4.
1sg. H is assigned to the stressed syllable. If the latter is lexically H, the rule is blocked.
Aspectual H- or L- is assigned to first syllable, overriding /Ø/. Potential H- forms HL
contour with lexical L
Phonology: HL-Ø → HL-L (H-L if first vowel is short); ex. of HL-H has V: in both
syllables (dùàdíí’)
Shaded = not predicted by my rules:
(i) /Ø-H/ 3sg. potential should be H-H (H → L after H?)
(ii) /LH/ 1sg. potential should be HL if 1sg. is spelled out first
(iii) 3rd pers. /L/ potential should be HL. Avoidance of *HLH is general.
343