American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Exploring Antecedents
Of Unethical Business Decisions
Genevieve O'Connor, Fordham University, USA
Mohammad G. Nejad, Fordham University, USA
Hooman Estelami, Fordham University, USA
ABSTRACT
Adhering to recent arguments that unethical decision making is driven by the underlying psychological processes, the
purpose of this study is to investigate the role of individual psychographics (i.e., cognitive style, decision confidence,
emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk) in ethical decision making within a business context. Using a novel
approach, the researchers capture ethical decision making using a computer-simulated market context and couple the
results with survey data. The results of the regression analysis reveal that an individual’s cognitive style, decision
confidence, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk play a significant role in ethical decision making.
Participants with an analytical cognitive style were less likely to engage in unethical means to accomplish business
objectives compared to those with intuitive or adaptive cognitive styles. Similarly, greater confidence in decision
making, higher emotional intelligence, and a greater preference for risk were also found to increase the chances that
an individual avoids engaging in unethical decision making. Finally, the effect of cognitive style was found to be
stronger than the other factors examined in this research. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Keywords: Ethical Behavior; Ethical Decision Making; Cognitive Style; Emotion; Risk; Decision Confidence
INTRODUCTION
A
s the corporate and social world continue to recover from the effects of financial misconduct and the
widespread fraud that ignited the world financial crisis nearly a decade ago, greater attention is being
given to ethical business decision making. Across the globe, codes of conduct and corporate policies
are being implemented to protect consumers from unethical business practices. Examples are the US pharmaceutical
industry's code on interactions with healthcare professionals, and the British Psychological Society's code of ethics.
Nevertheless, unethical decision making in business remains a harsh reality that must be addressed, as the outcomes
of unethical decisions can severely affect the quality of life for the general consumer population as well as
organizations that are staffed by managers with questionable business practices (Woiceshyn, 2011).
Research on ethical decision making continues to proliferate across disciplines. Within business, most of the research
has focuses on antecedents of ethical decision making by considering how individuals will respond when faced with
a moral or ethical issue or dilemma created by social, cultural, economic or organizational factors (Billiot, Daniel,
Glandon & Glandon, 2012; Jones, 1991). Recognizing that there is no “one size fits all” approach to identifying drivers
of ethical decision making, much work has been done to ascertain how individual level differences relate to ethical
and unethical choices (e.g., Detert, Trevino & Sweitzer, 2008; Reiss & Mitra, 1998. Specifically, extant research has
primarily focused on how consumers respond to ethical dilemmas through the lens of demographics such as gender
and age (Ruegger & King, 1992), psychographics such as individual values (Marques & Azevedo-Pereira 2009), and
emotional intelligence (Angelidis & Ibrahim, 2011; Deshpande, Joseph & Berry, 2012). Adhering to arguments that
unethical decision making is driven by underlying psychological processes (Messick & Bazerman, 1996; Tenbrunsel
& Messick, 2004; Detert, Trevino & Sweitzer, 2008), and the functioning of the human brain during decision making
(Woiceshyn, 2011), this study explores the role of psychographic factors, namely cognitive style, decision confidence,
emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk, in ethical decision making. Specifically, we study the relationship
between psychographics and ethical decision making in a real-world business scenario. To do so, we study the effect
of the aforementioned variables on the degree to which individuals are willing to engage in unethical choices as a
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
27
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
mean to make their business more profitable in a simulation exercise. Our overarching research question is how ethical
decision making in business relates to cognitive style, emotional intelligence, attitude toward risk, and one’s level of
decision confidence.
This paper advances theory and practice by addressing the increasing interest in business ethics. In addition, this study
employs a novel methodological approach in examining ethical decision making in a business context by capturing
individual’s real-time decisions using a computer-simulated business environment. Extant research has primarily
adopted survey and experimental approaches in exploring ethical decision making. This study enriches these
approaches by coupling survey data from each decision maker with his/her actual decisions in a dynamic decision
environment. The resulting findings contribute to our understanding of ethical decision making, which in turn provides
managers greater insight into how certain individual characteristics (i.e., cognitive style, confidence in decision
making, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk) can be cultivated to improve ethical decisions in business.
The remainder of the paper proceeds as follows: first we construct a conceptual framework to better understand the
antecedents of ethical decision making by paying particular attention to the role of psychographics. Next, we
empirically test our model and examine the relationships of interest using regression analysis. We then discuss the
implicaitions and outline possible interventions that can target specifc antecedents to help reduce unethical decision
making in business. Finally, we conclude with directions for future research.
BACKGROUND AND THEORY
Ethical decision making is typically viewed within the framework of being both morally and legally acceptable to a
larger community (Jones, 1991). In this broad, and at times, ambiguous framework, business ethics has been
considered by some as being analogous to “nailing jello to a wall” (Lewis, 1985, p 377). Despite this, most utilized
definitions reflect the premise that business ethics are regulations and normal prescriptions of managerial decisions
and actions (Steiner, Steiner & Steiner, 1980) for the application of “one’s understanding of what is morally right and
truthful at time of ethical dilemma” (Lewis, 1985, p.383) where honesty, fairness, and integrity are central to decision
making (Fleming, 1985). This study builds on previous research by exploring how an individual’s unique sociopsychological makeup influences ethical decisions, as outlined below.
Ethical Decision Making and Psychological Processes
The criteria that individuals utilize to make ethical decisions varies from the “relative objectivity of obeying the law
to the comparative subjectivity of the dictates of the decision-makers’ values or conscience” (Fleming, 1985, p 137).
From a subjective standpoint, what one constitutes as ethical or unethical behavior can objectively be evaluated by
the individual’s psychographic makeup. Since ethical issues are comprised of both rational and emotional components,
including both cognitive and affective skills, both components must be included when analyzing ethical issues (Holian,
2006). Therefore, in this study we will examine the relationships between ethical decision making and psychographics,
including the way an individual processes information (i.e., cognitive style), an individual’s assurance in decision
making (i.e., decision confidence), an individual’s awareness of his/her own and other’s emotions (i.e., emotional
intelligence), and an individual’s attitude toward risk (i.e., risk preference). The effects of these factors will be
discussed below.
Cognitive Style and Ethical Decision Making
In the decision-making literature, conflicting theories exist as to how individuals approach ethical dilemmas. The
traditional approach assumes that individuals adopt a subjective moral compass to direct their response to ethical
issues and employ deliberate extensive moral reasoning to weigh the evidence before coming to a decision
(Sonenshein, 2007). In contrast to this rationalist approach, emerging research focuses on the intuitive nature of
decision making. According to the sense making-intuition model (SIM), individuals use intuitive judgements rather
than deliberate extensive moral reasoning when responding to ethical issues (Sonenshein, 2007).
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
28
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Cognitive style, which reflects an individual’s approach to organizing, integrating, evaluating and processing
information for decision making (Allinson & Hayes, 1996; McIntyre & Capen, 1993; Riding & Sadler-Smith, 1997),
captures both the analytic and intuitive aspects of decision making processes. One of the most extensively utilized
operationalizations of cognitive style is the Cognitive Style Index (CSI), developed by Allinson and Hayes (1996).
Cognitive style recognizes that some individuals are more prone to make decisions methodically whereas others may
prefer to base their decisions on feelings and intuition, rather than contemplating their actions. Furthermore, many
individuals may be somewhere on the spectrum between these two extremes. CSI therefore quantifies individuals'
cognitive styles along a bipolar continuum; from highly intuitive at one pole, to highly analytical at the other pole,
with a combination (i.e., adaptive style) of the two cognitive styles in-between. To group individuals into more refined
cognitive style categories, five groupings of cognitive style based on the CSI score exist. On the cognitive style
spectrum, between adaptive and intuitive cognitive styles is the quasi-intuitive cognitive style. Similarly, between
adaptive and analytical cognitive styles is the quasi-analytical cognitive style (Allison & Hayes, 1996). Thus, decision
information is structurally managed by how individuals manipulate the information (Streufert & Streufert, 1978),
whereby individuals who are more analytical tend to apply a systematic approach and those who are more intuitive
tend to apply a holistic approach to decision making (Sagiv, Amit, Ein‐Gar, & Arieli, 2014).
Drawing on the above research, it can be argued that individuals approach ethical decisions from either a rationalist
or intuitive approach. As such, an individual’s own cognitive style can dictate his/her approach to problem solving,
including decision problems of an ethical nature (McIntyre & Capen 1993). Furthermore, such forces may influence
the nature of decisions made in the specific context of business decisions (Fleming, 1985). The emerging research
question, with respect to the role of cognitive style on business ethics, is outlined below:
RQ1: What is the effect of cognitive style on an individual’s propensity to engage in unethical business decisions?
Decision Confidence and Ethical Decision Making
Decisions that carry a fair amount of ambiguity demand a certain degree of confidence from the decision maker. Selfconfidence in decision making is defined as the assurance in one’s ability to meet the objectives that generate positive
outcomes for oneself and others (Bearden, Hardesty, & Rose, 2001). Business executives need to have confidence in
their ability to make difficult decisions, especially decisions where there are no formal guidelines as to what is ethically
“right” or “wrong”. When individuals are confident in their decisions, their decision confidence empowers them with
the ability to take control of circumstances rather than being dragged along with them (Kanter, 2006).
Early research on decision making shows that individuals who have low self-esteem are more affected by
environmental circumstances and more inclined to be inconsistent in decision making than those who are high in selfesteem (Mossman & Ziller, 1968). In the same vein, overconfidence has been shown to impact the quality of decision
making (Alba & Hutchinson 2000). Based on these findings the following research question is posed:
RQ2: What is the effect of decision confidence on an individual’s propensity to engage in unethical business
decisions?
Emotional Intelligence and Ethical Decision Making
Emotional intelligence (EI) is the ability to accurately perceive, understand, and manage one’s own emotions as well
as the emotions of others (Mayer, 2004). It is a critical factor in individual performance which has a profound effect
on decision making, even beyond the effects of IQ (Goelman, 1998). As a critical factor in decision making, EI has
been found to be a significant predictor of one’s own ethical behavior as well as the perception of others' (e.g., peers')
ethical behavior (Mesmer-Magnus, Viswesvaran, Joseph & Deshpande, 2008).
The ability to effectively understand and regulate one’s own emotions for focused actions (Law, Wong & Song, 2004),
as well as the ability to promote emotional knowledge, perception, and regulation (Mayer & Salovey, 1997) has been
associated with high levels of EI. In turn higher EI has been associated with greater performance across various
disciplines, where most work has found it to be a significant factor in individual behaviors. For example, within the
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
29
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
healthcare setting, employees who are better at reading others’ emotions have been found to be more successful at
work and social settings (Freshman & Rubino, 2002). Similarly, the level of EI of nurses has been found to have a
significant impact on their behavior (Deshpande & Joseph, 2009). Within academia, EI has also been shown to be
positively related to academic performance (Parker et al. 2004, Petrides, Frederickson & Furnaham, 2004).
EI is an important aspect of ethical decision making, as it offers a foundation to approach complex issues. Though still
a nascent area of research, increased focus has been placed on uncovering drivers of ethical behavior, particularly
focusing on the role of individual’s emotional skills and ethical decision making. For example, Angelidis and Ibrahim
(2011) studied ethical judgement among managers and found that individuals who had personal qualities that helped
them identify and assess their own and others' emotions (i.e., higher levels of EI) were found to be more likely to
make favorable decisions for all involved. Similarly, Carmeli (2003) found that a higher degree of EI among senior
managers is positively related to work performance. In a consumption setting, Chowdhury (2017; p. 527) also asserts
that that an individual’s “ability to experience, perceive, and regulate emotions should influence consumers’ ethical
decision making”. Nevertheless, little research has empirically investigated the role that emotional intelligence plays
in ethical decision making in business decision contexts. To address this, the following research question is posed:
RQ3: What is the effect of emotional intelligence on an individual’s propensity to engage in unethical business
decisions?
Attitude toward Risk and Ethical Decision Making
Individuals differ in the way they make decisions that involve risk or uncertainty. Individuals' attitudes toward risk
reflects their evaluations of uncertainties regarding potential outcomes of decisions, and can therefore affect how they
interact with their decision environment (Rohmann, 2002). An individual’s risk attitude describes the shape of his/her
utility function, which is derived from a series of risky choices for the outcomes in question (Weber, 2010. An
individuals’ attitude toward risk can generally be portrayed as falling along a bipolar continuum from risk averse at
one polar end to risk seeking at the other polar end.
Singh (1986) suggests that there is a direct relationship between one’s attitude toward risk and decision making
behavior. Although an individual’s attitude toward risk is generally considered to be a personality trait, it has also
been shown to be both domain-specific and task-specific. For example, MacCrimmon and Wehrung (1986, 1990)
found that managers’ attitude toward risk varies as to whether the decision involves personal versus corporate funds,
or when assessing financial versus recreational risks. To gain further insight into the role of risk attitude in ethical
decision making within business decision context, the following research question is posed:
RQ4: What is the effect of attitude toward risk on an individual’s propensity to engage in unethical business decisions?
METHODS
This study seeks to explore the effects of four individual psychological factors - cognitive style, decision confidence,
emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk on an individuals’ ethical decision making behavior. Exploring this
research question in a systematic way requires an actual business decision setting in which individuals engage in
making decisions. Since capturing this type of data and examining the effects of the aforementioned four factors in
the real world is challenging, if not unfeasible, a two-step research design was designed. In the first step, participants
completed a detailed questionnaire comprising pre-established multi-item scales that measured each participant's
individual characteristics including cognitive style, decision confidence, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward
risk. In the second step, the researchers used a computer-simulated market to examine participants’ decision making
behavior. Each participant assumed the role of a manager of an independently-owned and operated hotel in a town
where three other competitor hotels operated. Participants were advised that the objective of the simulation was to
outperform other participants on the cumulative profit over the course of eight quarters. After each quarter, participants
were provided with quarterly reports regarding the performance of their company, market research, competitive
activity, and industry information. Participants made their decisions on an individual basis and entered their decisions
regarding price, advertising, and staffing of their hotel, in the simulation program. Each ethical decision was framed
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
30
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
as a ‘tactical decision’ that may improve the hotel's profits. Participants needed to decide whether they would engage
or refrain from the activity. Throughout the course of the eight simulation periods, eight different questions were
presented to participants and each question was presented only once. These decisions are listed in the Appendix. The
extent to which an individual proceeds with these decisions reflects the individual's propensity to engage in unethical
business decision making.
The data collected from the questionnaire and the decisions made during the simulation exercise were then coupled
and matched with each other, hence creating a rich dataset. This approach overcomes the issue of common-method
bias in research as the independent and dependent variables in the model are collected through different means. This
dataset included both the participants' scores on each of the attributes of interest in this study (cognitive style, decision
confidence, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk) as well as the participants’ decisions in the simulated
business environment, which is the degree to which they engaged in unethical behavior. The use of a computersimulated environment to resemble a real-world environment for exploring managerial decisions has been used in
related research areas. For example, Estelami and Nejad (2017) employed this methodology to examine the effects of
a manager’s psychographic attributes on his/her reactions to competitive price reductions by competitors.
Montgomery, Moore & Urbany (2005) studied managerial reactions in a competitive market using a similar approach.
DeBruyne, Frambach & Moenaert (2010) employed a simulation approach in exploring the effects of resource
availability and the market environment on managerial reactions to new product introductions by competitors.
The growing research on exploring decisions in computer-simulated environments coupled with survey data creates
new opportunities to examine important research questions, such as the ones that are posed in this study. Collecting
such detailed information is very challenging and typically unattainable using mainstream research methodologies
that are often used by previous research (DeBruyne et al. 2010; North & Macal, 2007). Ethical decisions are a sensitive
topic, hence, individuals are often reluctant to acknowledge whether they would engage in unethical behavior. The
methodology used in this study captures individual decision making as these decisions are made, rather than asking
individuals about their previous experiences or what they would hypothetically do in the future. All participants
completed the simulation exercise under the same competitive environment, to ensure that they were evaluated in a
consistent way with regards to the effects of individual attributes on ethical decision making.
Measurement Scales
Decision confidence, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk were measured using multi-item scales, all of
which were adopted from previous studies. Decision confidence was measured using a 4-item scale used in existing
decision research (Chernev, 2009; Patalano & LeClair, 2011). Emotional intelligence was measured using 4 items
adopted from Schutte et al. (1998). Attitude toward risk was measured using a 4-item scale that was used in (Barbosa,
Gerhardt & Kickul, 2007; Chernev, 2009). A factor analysis of these items using an orthogonal transformation resulted
in three distinct factors. Each scale item loaded on the corresponding measurement scale. The scales produced high
coefficient alpha values reflecting high degrees of reliability, as listed in Table 1.
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
31
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Table 1. Multi-item Measurement Scales and Factor Loadings
Decision
Emotional
Confidence
Intelligence
Multi-item Scales and Individual Scale Items*
(Cronbach's
(Cronbach's
α = .76)
α = .72)
Decision Confidence Questions:
I am confident in business decisions that are delegated to me
.784
The quality of my decisions as a manager are likely to be above the
average for my peers
.620
I am satisfied with how I make business decisions
.734
When making important decisions I can get stressed out with the
amount of information I have to process.
When making important decisions, I feel that I spend too much time
considering all options.
Picking the best option in business scenarios would be a difficult task
for me**
Emotional Intelligence:
.585
.647
.722
I am aware of my emotions as I experience them
.488
By looking at their facial expressions, I am good at recognizing the
emotions people are experiencing
.848
I am aware of the nonverbal messages other people send
.810
I know what other people are feeling just by looking at them
.742
Risk Preference:
Having a stable income over a long period of time is more important to
me than making a lot of money in a short period of time but not being
sure if it can be sustained over the long term.
I tend to take greater risks than most of my friends and family
members.
Risk
Preference
(Cronbach's
α = .72)
.782
.684
Long-term financial stability is very important to me.
.617
I am generally a risk-averse person.
.776
* All questions are asked on a 1 to 7 scales with 1 being “strongly disagree” and 7 being “strongly agree”.
** Reverse-scaled item
*** Factor loadings produced with factor analysis using a Varimax rotation. Factor loadings below .2 are not displayed, indicating that there is no
cross-loading across different factors.
Cognitive style was measured using the Cognitive Style Index (CSI) of Allinson and Hayes (1996). The scale consists
of 38 discrete choice questions and employs a scoring algorithm to classify individuals on a continuum of cognitive
style (Allinson & Hayes 1996; 2012). Individuals are then categorized along this continuum as having an intuitive,
quasi-intuitive, adaptive, quasi-analytical, or analytical cognitive style, based on their CSI score. The cognitive style
index has exhibited a high degree of discriminant validity in previous studies (Hayes, Allinson, Hudson, & Keasey,
2003) and has been employed in various research areas, including management decision making (Boyatzis, Stubbs, &
Taylor, 2002; Estelami & Nejad, 2017; Hayes & Allinson, 1996), information processing (Banning, 2003), human
learning (Armstrong, 2000), and business education (Backhaus & Liff, 2007).
Data
The participants for this study consisted of 154 business students. All participants were in their senior-year at a private
undergraduate college in a major urban area in the northeastern United States. The gender composition of the sample
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
32
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
was 33.1% female and 66.9% male, the average self-reported GPA was 3.48 (out of 4.0), and the average age was
21.2 years old. The majority of the participants had completed two or more industry internships at the time of the
study (94.2% had completed one internship and 82.5% had completed two or more internships). This is a uniquely
qualified sample, as the participants are formally trained to manage businesses and engage in business decision
making. Furthermore, use of this sample is consistent with prior studies on business decision making (Van Bruggen,
Smidts, & Wierenga,1996, 1998; Debruyne et al. 2010; Estelami & Nejad, 2017; Glazer, Steckel & Winer 1992;
Montgomery et al., 2005), whereby trained participants' actions are measured in pre-specified business decision
contexts.
In the first step, participants completed an online questionnaire that consisted of the measurement instruments for the
aforementioned individual attributes as well as questions regarding their demographics. In a separate session, the
participants completed the simulation as part of their class assignment for which they received credit. This increased
the participants’ involvement with the simulation. To ensure that participants had an adequate knowledge about the
business that they were being asked to manage, they completed a brief quiz prior to the simulation session. They were
advised that the main objective of the study is to maximize the cumulative profits when competing across 8 quarters
in a competitive environment. The simulation program captured detailed data regarding participants’ business
decisions, such as price, advertising, and staffing, as well as decisions to engage in unethical behaviors outlined in the
Appendix. Between 30 and 35 participants attended each simulation session in a computer lab. The data collected
from the business simulation was coupled with the data collected through the questionnaire, to match each participant's
decision history with his/her measures of cognitive style, emotional intelligence, attitude toward risk, and decision
confidence. The cumulative number of unethical decisions (i.e., total number of times that the individual engaged in
an unethical decision) was calculated and used as the dependent variable for this study. This measure would range
from a low of 0 (in which case no unethical decisions were made) to a high of 8 (in which case the participant engaged
in unethical decisions during all 8 simulated decision periods). The mean, median, and standard deviation for the
dependent variable were 3.47, 4.00, and 2.00, respectively.
ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
In order to address the four research questions posed, regression analysis was used. In this regression analysis, the
dependent variable was the propensity of the participant to engage in unethical decisions, as had been outlined above.
The independent variables consisted of the four measures of focus, namely cognitive style, decision confidence,
emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk. The regression analysis was found to be statistically significant (F5,
148 = 4.99, p < .001) and the results are shown in Table 2. As can be seen from the regression analysis results, the
variables that are the focus of the research in this study show statistically significant effects on study participants'
propensity to engage in unethical business decisions.
Table 2. Regression Analysis Results for Propensity to Engage in Unethical Business Decisions
Standardized
Coefficient
t-value
Coefficient
Adaptive Cognitive Style (dummy variable)
-.559
-112
-1.208
Intercept
9.737
5.841
Decision Confidence
-.393**
-.174
-2.100
Emotional Intelligence
-.382**
-.157
-2.032
Attitude Toward Risk
-.324*
-.179
-1.913
Analytical Cognitive Style (dummy variable)
-1.133**
-.284
-2.572
p-value
.229
.0001
.037
.044
.058
.011
* p<0.1; ** p <.05; *** p <.01; R2 = .141; F (5,148) =4.99 (p<. 001)
To examine the role of cognitive style, participants were categorized into three groups with regards to their cognitive
style index. Two dummy variables were used to identify the groups. If an individual possessed an analytical or quasianalytical cognitive style, the dummy variable Analytical Cognitive Style took on a value of 1, otherwise it was set to
0. Similarly, if an individual possessed an adaptive cognitive style, the dummy variable Adaptive Cognitive Style took
on a value of 1, otherwise it was set to 0. Through this dummy-variable coding, the intercept term in the regression
analysis captures the average value of the dependent variable for the remaining cognitive style categories, namely
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
33
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
intuitive and quasi-intuitive, forming the baseline for the analysis (Lehmann, Gupta, & Steckel, 1988). This allows
for a clearer evaluation and presentation of the effect of cognitive style on individuals’ ethical decision making. The
coefficient for the Analytical Cognitive Style dummy variable is negative and statistically significant (p = 0.011). The
negative sign of this coefficient indicates that individuals who have analytical or quasi-analytical decision styles are
less likely to engage in unethical decision making. Their greater inclination to approach business decisions from an
analytical perspective may increase their ability to manage complex business situations and reduce their need to use
ethically questionable practices in managing a business. This issue will be further explored in the Discussion section
of the paper.
A negative relationship exists between decision confidence and the cumulative number of unethical decisions
(p=.037). This indicates that individuals with higher levels of decision confidence are less likely to make unethical
decisions. This result may be due to the fact that individuals with high levels of decision confidence believe in their
own decision ability such that, to achieve good business outcomes, they do not believe they would need to resort to
unethical decisions.
The regression analysis results also indicate that emotional intelligence is negatively related to unethical decision
making (p=.044). Participants with higher levels of emotional intelligence make fewer unethical decisions. This may
be due to the fact that individuals with greater levels of emotional intelligence are more stable in their thinking and
less likely to be drawn into contemplating decision possibilities that may cross ethical boundaries. In addition, attitude
toward risk was shown to have a negative relationship with participants' propensity to engage in unethical decisions
(p=0.058), where individuals who are prone to take more risks are less likely to make unethical business decisions.
This finding is likely due to the ability to identify the risk itself, which creates a moral obligation to act. Thus, those
who are more prone to taking risks, may be more likely to recognize that the decision is unethical in nature.
A comparison of the standardized coefficients generated by the regression analysis results in additional interesting
observations, with respect to the relative impact of the various variables on participants' propensity to make unethical
business decisions. Based on the standardized coefficients, among the four predictors, it appears that cognitive style
has the greatest impact on participants' ethical decision making. Among the predictors in the regression analysis, the
effect of the dummy variable Analytical Cognitive Style is the greatest, as the absolute value of the standardized
coefficient for this predictor (0.284) is significantly greater compared to the absolute value of the standardized
coefficient for the other variables (0.157 for emotional intelligence; 0.174 for decision confidence, 0.179 for attitude
toward risk). In other words, while one may assume that traits such as decision confidence, emotional intelligence, or
attitude toward risk are the major factors that may influence unethical decision making, the effects of cognitive style
seem to dominate.
Discussion and Implications
This study has explored the role of an individual’s psychological traits on ethical business decision making. With
respect to the research questions posed, it is evident that variations in propensity to engage in unethical decisions exist,
as a function of cognitive style (research question 1). Furthermore, decision confidence influences unethical decision
propensity (research question 2) and has a negative effect, whereby individuals with greater levels of decision
confidence are less likely to engage in unethical decisions. In addition, the relationship between emotional intelligence
and unethical business decision making is found to be negative (research question 3). Finally, an individual's attitude
toward risk has a negative relationship with his/her propensity to engage in unethical business decisions (research
question 4).
The results of this study also demonstrate that of the four variables of interest, cognitive style had the greatest impact
on individual's propensity to engage in unethical business decisions. Across research domains, cognitive style has
received a great deal of attention. Nevertheless, little research has studied its effect on ethical decision making within
a business context. The finding that individuals who possess analytically oriented cognitive styles are less likely to
make unethical business decisions may be attributed to greater tendency to conduct quantitative analysis and adopt
analytical approaches to increase firm profits. In contrast, non-analytical decision makers may be less likely to
approach managerial decisions analytically and scrutinize the deep impact that unethical decisions may have on the
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
34
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
business overall. To help individuals with cognitive styles that make them more inclined to make unethical decisions,
corporate education programs and workforce training may need to incorporate a focused proactive approach to ethics
training, specifically for this target population, and provide clear communication of related company codes of ethics
and policies.
Though the efficacy of ethical codes has been disputed (Kaptein & Schwartz, 2008), internal communication of
established codes of ethics may be especially valuable for individuals who generally rely on instinct (i.e., possess
intuitive or quasi-intuitive cognitive styles) rather than analysis in decision making. Communication of the code of
ethics to this target population can offer a distinct set of prescriptions developed by the firm to provide the employees
with an ethical compass. As such, organizations need to be transparent as to what does and what does not constitute
unethical behavior. For example, an ethical scorecard (as proposed by Spiller, 2000) which focuses on keeping an
account of an organization’s practices, can be readily employed by management. Organizations can implement such
a scorecard as a diagnostic tool to identify areas of concern while also fortifying areas of strength.
Furthermore, to utilize the findings of this study, and similar studies on ethical decision making in business contexts,
a proactive rather than reactive approach by organizations may be called for. As such, management training programs,
as well as incentive systems and recruiting practices, may need to focus on hiring, developing, and cultivating
individuals with high ethical decision-making propensities. The results of this study demonstrate that certain
psychological factors can contribute to such propensities, and as such their effects on organizational practices and
management training programs should be carefully considered, as individual psychographics (i.e., cognitive style,
confidence in decision making, emotional intelligence, and attitude toward risk) significantly influence ethical
decision making within a business context.
The results of this study show that those who have high levels of emotional intelligence make fewer unethical business
decisions. This finding suggests that individuals with greater emotional intelligence may be more understanding of
the adverse effects of such unethical decisions and realize the long-term impact of making such decisions and hence,
are less likely to engage in these decisions. As the ability to experience, interpret, and control emotions has been
shown to be important for ethical decision making among consumers (Gaudine & Thorne 2001), corporate training
programs may need to focus on enabling employees to appraise and recognize emotions in themselves as well as
others. For example, personal training and coaching sessions can be implemented to improve employees' emotional
intelligence levels. Further, we concur with Kosnick, Tingle, & Blanton (2013) and support the use of
experiential learning projects where individuals benefit through hands on learning to develop their moral
character and in turn improve their ethical conduct.
The findings of this study also show that individuals who are more confident in their decision making engage in fewer
unethical decisions compared to others. This observation may be due to the fact that these individuals are more
confident in their ability to make decisions and thereby their ability to successfully manage a business. Thus, they
may see less need to deploy unethical means for achieving business success. This is an important finding, since in
organizations where managers function under ambiguity and uncertainty, the resulting negative effects in decision
confidence may encourage unethical decision making. Proper planning, communications, and equitable performance
reviews may help managers achieve greater levels of decision confidence and reduce the potentially negative effect
that low decision confidence may have on ethical behavior.
Finally, the results show that individuals who are more prone to take risks engage in fewer unethical decisions than
those who are more risk averse. This finding highlights the need to better educate individuals on what is “right” or
what is “wrong”. Here we see that those who are more risk averse may not recognize the moral dilemma associated
with these decisions and unlike their counterparts (i.e., risk takers) they fail to recognize that the decisions are unethical
in nature.
LIMITATIONS
The definition of what is, and what is not considered ethical in business, continues to evolve and varies by industry
and culture. For example, in the pharmaceutical industry, taking a client out to dinner is widely recognized as an
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
35
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
ethical violation, whereas in the legal practice, client-practitioner dinners are the norm. Here, insights into individual
decision making could be further extended, offering insights into whether companies should implement policies that
explicitly state what is “right” or “wrong” on a wide array of ethical issues, to avoid individual misinterpretation of
what is and what is not ethical. Similarly, what is considered to be ethical business in one country, may be highly
frowned upon in another. As research shows that organizational culture is one of the key influences in driving ethical
or unethical behavior (Trevino & Nelson, 2004), future studies may need to consider the role of psychographics within
other contexts, including other business setting, cross-culturally, and globally.
Clearly, unethical behavior is not restricted to one group, as any group may be more or less inclined to make unethical
decisions. Although demographics have long been studied as correlates of ethical decision making, mixed results have
been found in the literature. For example, regarding the effects of age and gender on ethical decision making, some
studies show that age and gender fail to have a significant effect on ethical decision making (e.g., Callan, 1992)
whereas other studies have shown significant effects. Therefore, future research can consider the role of demographics
in ethical business decision making.
Lastly, future research can pay more attention to how ethical decision making is related to the decision maker’s unique
circumstances. For example, to what degree will individuals justify and rationalize their unethical behavior if they
believe that they themselves have been the target of unfair actions? Schweitzer and Gibson’s (2008) study on the effect
of perceived fairness (i.e., violation of community standards of fairness) on ethical behavior found that individuals
who were offered an explanation that violates community standards of fairness (e.g., merchant admitting to price
gouging) are more likely to engage in unethical behavior than individuals who were offered an explanation that does
not violate community standards of fairness (e.g., merchant made general price increase). As such, the effects of such
experiences and self-perceptions on the propensity of managers to make unethical business decisions requires further
research.
CONCLUSION
Oftentimes in business, making the "right" decision is not a clear-cut matter. There is usually a great deal of ambiguity
and complexity involved, as to what is ethically “right” or “wrong”. Recognizing that ethical decisions are highly
complex, and the right choice varies by both domain and circumstances, it is hoped that this research has provided a
better understanding as to how to identify psychographic traits that play a role in an individuals’ propensity to behave
ethically in a business setting. Managers, corporate trainers, and business educators, can consider these traits to
identify areas of need, and offer tools that are uniquely tailored to individuals’ ethical training needs, to ultimately
afford the firm a competitive edge, without compromising ethical principles or violating organizational policies and
norms.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Genevieve O'Connor is an assistant professor of marketing at the Gabelli School of Business, Fordham University.
She earned her PhD from Rutgers University. Her publications have appeared in the Journal of Product and Brand
Management, Journal of Financial Services Marketing, and International Journal of Bank Marketing. E-mail:
[email protected]
Mohammad Nejad is an associate professor of marketing at the Gabelli School of Business, Fordham University. He
received his PhD in marketing from the University of Memphis. Dr. Nejad's research relates to innovation diffusion,
marketing new products and services, social contagion and social networks, and agent-based modeling and simulation.
His research has appeared in the Journal of Retailing, International Journal of Research in Marketing, and European
Journal of Operational Research, among others. E-mail:
[email protected]
Hooman Estelami (contact author) is a full professor in marketing at the Gabelli School of Business. He received
his PhD in marketing from Columbia University. He is the editor of the International Journal of Bank Marketing and
former Associate Editor of the Journal of Product and Brand Management. His work has been published in the Journal
of Financial Services Marketing, Journal of Retailing, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, International
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
36
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Journal of Research in Marketing, Journal of Business Research, Journal of Service Research and Journal of Services
Marketing, American Journal of Business Education, Journal of Consumer Affairs, and elsewhere. He is the author of
the textbook Marketing Financial Services. His other books are Marketing Turnarounds: A Guide to Surviving
Downturns and Rediscovering Growth and Frontiers of Distance Learning in Business; Simplified Business Research
Methods; Marketing Strategy; and Frontiers of Distance Learning in Business Education. E-mail:
[email protected]
REFERENCES
Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (2000). Knowledge calibration: What consumers know and what they think they know. Journal
of Consumer Research, 27(2), 123-156.
Allinson, C. W., & Hayes, J. (1996). The cognitive style index: A measure of intuition‐analysis for organizational research.
Journal of Management Studies, 33(1), 119-135.
Allinson, C.W. & Hayes, J. (1996). The Cognitive Style Index: A Measure of Intuition-Analysis for Organizational Research,
Journal of Management Studies, 33(1), 119-135.
Allinson, C.W. &Hayes, J. (2012). The Cognitive Style Index: Technical Manual and User Guide. New York: Pearson.
Angelidis, J., & Ibrahim, N. A. (2011). The impact of emotional intelligence on the ethical judgment of managers. Journal of
Business Ethics, 99(1), 111-119.
Billiot, M. J., Daniel, D., Glandon, S., & Glandon, T. (2012). Educational Context: Preparing Accounting Students to Identify
Ethical Dilemmas. American Journal of Business Education, 5(3), 277-286.
Armstrong, S.J. (2000). The Influence of Individual Cognitive Style on Performance in Management Education. Educational
Psychology, 20, 323-339.
Backhaus, K. & Liff, J.P. (2007). Cognitive Styles and Approaches to Studying in Management Education. Journal of
Management Education, 31(4), 445-466.
Banning, K.C. (2003). The Effect of the Case Method on Tolerance for Ambiguity. Journal of Management Education, 27, 556567.
Barbosa, S.D., M.W. Gerhardt & Kickul, J.R. (2007). The Role of Cognitive Style and Risk Preference on Entrepreneurial SelfEfficacy and Entrepreneurial Intentions. Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 13(4), 86-104.
Bearden, W. O., Hardesty, D. M., & Rose, R. L. (2001). Consumer self-confidence: Refinements in conceptualization and
measurement. Journal of Consumer Research, 28(1), 121-134.
Boyatzis, R.E., E.C. Stubbs & Taylor, S.N. (2002). Learning Cognitive and Emotional Intelligence Competencies through
Graduate Management Education. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 1, 150-162.
Callan, V. J. (1992). Predicting ethical values and training needs in ethics. Journal of Business ethics, 11(10), 761-769.
Carmeli, A. (2003). The relationship between emotional intelligence and work attitudes, behavior and outcomes: An examination
among senior managers. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 18(8),788-813.
Chernev, A. (2009). Choosing Versus Rejecting: The Implication of Goal-Task Compatibility on Decision Confidence. Social
Cognition, 27(2), 249-260.
Chowdhury, R.M. (2017). Emotional intelligence and consumer ethics: The mediating role of personal moral philosophies.
Journal of Business Ethics, 142(3) 527-548.
Deshpande, S. P., & Joseph, J. (2009). Impact of emotional intelligence, ethical climate, and behavior of peers on ethical
behavior of nurses. Journal of Business Ethics,85(3), 403.
Deshpande, S. P., Joseph, J., & Berry, K. (2012). Ethical Misconduct Of Business Students: Some New Evidence. American
Journal of Business Education (AJBE), 5(6), 719-726. https://doi.org/10.19030/ajbe.v5i6.7394
Detert, J. R., Treviño, L. K., & Sweitzer, V. L. (2008). Moral disengagement in ethical decision making: a study of antecedents
and outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 374.
Estelami, H., & Nejad, M. G. (2017). The impact of cognitive style, entrepreneurial attitudes and gender on competitive price
responses. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(7), 759-770.
Fleming, J. E. (1985). A suggested approach to linking decision styles with business ethics. Journal of Business Ethics, 4(2), 137144.
Freshman, B., & Rubino, L. (2002). Emotional intelligence: a core competency for health care administrators. The health care
manager, 20(4), 1-9.
Debruyne, M., Frambach, R.T. & Moenaert, R. (2010).Using the Weapons You Have: The Role of Resources and Competitor
Orientation as Enablers and Inhibitors of Competitive Reaction to New Products. Journal of Product Innovation
Management, 27 (2), 161-78.
Deshpande, S. P., Joseph, J., & Berry, K. (2012). Ethical Misconduct of Business Students: Some New Evidence. American
Journal of Business Education, 5(6), 719-726.
Gaudine, A., & Thorne, L. (2001). Emotion and ethical decision-making in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 31(2), 175187.
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
37
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Glazer, R., Steckel, J. H. & Winer, R. S, (1992). Locally rational decision making: The distracting effect of information on
managerial performance. Management Science, 38(2), 212-22
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence: Why it matters more than IQ. London, UK: Bloomsbury.
Hayes, J. & Allinson, C.W. (1996). The Implications of Learning Styles for Training and Development: A Discussion of the
Matching Hypothesis. British Journal of Management, 6, 63-73.
Hayes, J., Allinson, C. W., Hudson, R. S., & Keasey, K. (2003). Further reflections on the nature of intuition‐analysis and the
construct validity of the Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76(2), 269278
Hayes, J., C.W, Hudson, A. R. & Keasey, K. (2003). Further Reflection on the Nature of Intuition-Analysis and the Construct
Validity of the Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 269-278.
Holian, R. (2006). Management decision making, ethical issues and “emotional” intelligence. Management Decision, 44(8),
1122-1138.
Jones, T. M. (1991). Ethical decision making by individuals in organizations: An issue-contingent model. Academy of
Management Review, 16(2), 366-395.
Kanter, R. M. (2006). Confidence: How winning streaks and losing streaks begin and end. Crown Business.
Kaptein, M., & Schwartz, M. S. (2008). The effectiveness of business codes: A critical examination of existing studies and the
development of an integrated research model. Journal of Business Ethics, 77(2), 111-127.
Kosnik, R. D., Tingle, J. K., & Blanton III, E. L. (2013). Transformational learning in business education: The pivotal role of
experiential learning projects. American Journal of Business Education, 6(6), 613.
Law, K. S., Wong, C. S. & Song, L. J. (2004). The Construct and Criterion Validity of Emotional Intelligence and Its Potential
Utility for Management Studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(3), 483–496.
Lehmann, D.R., Gupta, S. & Steckel, J.H. (1998). Marketing Research. New York: Addison-Wesley.
Lewis, P.V. (1985). Defining ‘business ethics’: Like nailing jello to a wall. Journal of Business Ethics, 4(5), 377-383.
MacCrimmon, K.R. &Wehrung, D.A. (1986) Taking Risks: The Management of Uncertainty. Free Press: New York.
MacCrimmon, K.R., Wehrung, D.A. (1990) Characteristics of risk taking executives. Management Science, 36, 422–435.
Marques, P. A., & Azevedo-Pereira, J. (2009). Ethical ideology and ethical judgments in the Portuguese accounting profession.
Journal of Business Ethics, 86(2), 227-242.
Mayer, J. (2004). Be realistic. Harvard Business Review, 82 (1), 28
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence. Emotional development and emotional intelligence.
Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Implications for Educators, 3-31.
McIntyre, R. P., & Capen, M. M. (1993). A cognitive style perspective on ethical questions. Journal of Business Ethics, 12(8),
629-634.
Mesmer-Magnus, J., Viswesvaran, C., Joseph, J., & Deshpande, S. P. (2008). Chapter 10 The role of emotional intelligence in
integrity and ethics perceptions. Emotions, Ethics and Decision-Making, 225-239.
Messick, D. M., & Bazerman, M. H. (1996). Ethical leadership and the psychology of decision making. Sloan Management
Review, 37, 9–22.
Montgomery, D.B., Moore, M.C. &. Urbany, J.E. (2005). Reasoning about competitive reactions: Evidence from executives,
Marketing Science, 24 (1), 138-49.
Mossman, B.M. & Ziller, R.C. (1968) Self-Esteem and consistency of social behavior, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73 (4),
363–36.
North, M. J., & Macal, C. M. (2007). Managing Business Complexity. New York, NY: Oxford University press.
Parker, J. D., Creque Sr, R. E., Barnhart, D. L., Harris, J. I., Majeski, S. A., Wood, L. M., ... & Hogan, M. J. (2004). Academic
achievement in high school: does emotional intelligence matter? Personality and individual differences, 37(7), 13211330.
Patalano, A. & LeClair, Z. (2011). The influence of group decision making on indecisiveness-related decisional confidence.
Judgment and Decision Making, 6(1), 163-175.
Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Furnham, A. (2004). The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance and
deviant behavior at school. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(2), 277-293.
Reiss, M. C., & Mitra, K. (1998). The effects of individual difference factors on the acceptability of ethical and unethical
workplace behaviors. Journal of Business Ethics, 17(14), 1581-1593.
Riding, R. J., & Sadler‐Smith, E. (1997). Cognitive style and learning strategies: Some implications for training design.
International Journal of training and Development, 1(3), 199-208.
Rohrmann, B. (2002). Risk attitude scales: Concepts and questionnaires. Melbourne: University of Melbourne, 12.
Ruegger, D. & King, E. W. (1992). A study of the effect of age and gender upon student business ethics. Journal of Business
Ethics, 11(3), 179-186.
Sagiv, L., Amit, A., Ein‐Gar, D., & Arieli, S. (2014). Not all great minds think alike: Systematic and intuitive cognitive styles.
Journal of Personality, 82(5), 402-417.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., & Dornheim, L. (1998). Development and
validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(2), 167–177.
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
38
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
Schweitzer, M. E., & Gibson, D. E. (2008). Fairness, feelings, and ethical decision-making: Consequences of violating
community standards of fairness. Journal of Business Ethics, 77(3), 287-301.
Singh, J. V. (1986). Performance, slack, and risk taking in organizational decision making. Academy of Management Journal,
29(3), 562-585.
Sonenshein, S. (2007). The role of construction, intuition, and justification in responding to ethical issues at work: The sense
making-intuition model. Academy of Management Review, 32(4), pp.1022-1040.
Spiller, R. (2000). Ethical business and investment: A model for business and society. Journal of Business Ethics, 27(1-2), 149160.
Steiner, G.A., Steiner, J.F. & Steiner, J.F. (1980). Business, government, and society: A managerial perspective. New York:
Random House Business Division.
Streufert S. & Streufert S. (1978), Behavior in the Complex Environment, Winston-Wiley, Washington DC.
Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Messick, D. M. (2004). Ethical fading: The role of self-deception in unethical behavior. Social Justice
Research, 17, 223–236.
Trevino, L., & Nelson, K. (2004). Managing business ethics: Straight talk about how to do it right. New York: Wiley.
Van Bruggen, G. H., Smidts, A., & Wierenga, B. (1996). The impact of the quality of a marketing decision support system: An
experimental study. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 13(4), 331-343.
Van Bruggen, G. H., Smidts, A., & Wierenga, B. (1998). Improving decision making by means of a marketing decision system.
Management Science, 44(5), 645−658.
Weber, E.U., Blais, A. & Betz, N.E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors.
Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,15(4)263.
Weber, E. U. (2010). Risk attitude and preference. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 79-88.
Woiceshyn, J. (2011). A model for ethical decision making in business: Reasoning, intuition, and rational moral principles.
Journal of Business Ethics, 104(3), 311-323.
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
39
The Clute Institute
American Journal of Business Education –Second Quarter 2020
Volume 13, Number 2
APPENDIX
Decisions Used in the Business Simulation
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
You are considering giving the really shabby rooms to those customers that you deem will not be
loyal/repeat guests in the future
You are considering cutting costs by instructing the cleaning staff to not wash towels that they think are
clean enough
Your communications manager has discussed with you the possibility of using positive social media
reviews of you to launch a social media campaign, mostly relying on 'anonymous' reviewers to boost
your online ratings
You are considering the sale of web site usage information of your guests to a third-party marketing
company
You are considering using legal loopholes to avoid having to hire a lifeguard (required by law in your
state) for the swimming pool
You are considering an ad campaign that promises rooms with a great view (but most likely will not be
able to provide such rooms when the guests eventually stay at your hotel
There have been several cases of bed bugs reported by your guests in the past three months. You have
to decide whether you will reveal this to your state hotel industry regulator and to new guests coming to
stay at your hotel.
The management at Hotel C have approached the management at Hotel B as well as you, to coordinate
your marketing efforts with hopes of reducing the market share of Hotel D
During each simulation period, participants were given the option of engaging in an activity that would increase the
hotel's profits. Each question would appear once in one of the quarters. The decisions are listed above in the order
they appeared in the simulation.
Copyright by author(s); CC-BY
40
The Clute Institute