Nigerian Pidgin and West African Pidgins:
A sociolinguistic perspective
Christine I. Ofulue1
Abstract
Sociolinguistic factors play a significant role in the
emergence and development of pidgins and creoles, and their role
in the development of West African Pidgin English based (WAPE)
varieties is not an exception. Nigerian Pidgin (NP) along with
Ghanaian Pidgin (GP) and Cameroon Pidgin (CP) form a
continuum of mutually intelligible WAPE varieties spoken as
lingua francas along the West African coast. While previous
studies provide sociolinguistic descriptions of the individual
varieties, there is no comprehensive comparative study of the
WAPE varieties. This study aims to fill the gap by providing a
comparative analysis of similarities and differences in current
domains of use, functions, and attitudes that have shaped their
current status in the context of their socio-historical interrelatedness. The study shows that while the WAPE varieties share
similarities in their demographic and sociolinguistic
contexts, they differ in status. It observes a more rapid
development and expansion in NP. The findings of this study
contribute to a holistic understanding of the role of sociolinguistic
factors in evaluating the status of the mutually intelligible WAPE
varieties to inform its future development.
Introduction
The emergence of pidgins and creoles has been described
as one of the sociolinguistically significant outcomes of European
contact in Africa (Adegbija 2004:14, Echu & Obeng 2004:12).
1
Christine Ofulue teaches at the National Open University of Nigeria.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |1
In West Africa, English pidgins and creoles are found from the
Gambia in the northwest to Cameroon in the southeast (Holm
1988: 406). The term ‘West African Pidgin English’ (WAPE) has
been used in the literature to refer to a variety of related pidgins
‘that range from rudimentary to highly expanded, creole like
varieties’ spoken in the coastal countries where English is an
official language - the Gambia, Sierra-Leone, Liberia, Ghana,
Nigeria, and Cameroon (Holm, 1988: 426, Sebba, 1997:126).
However, this study focuses on the varieties of WAPE that are
spoken in Nigeria, Ghana, and Cameroon because they have the
most substantial population of pidgin speakers among West
African countries (cf. Peter & Wolf 2007). WAPEs are classified
under the Atlantic group of English based pidgins and creoles
according to historical, geographical and linguistic factors (Holm
1988).
There is a general consensus in the literature that WAPEs
emerged from trading contacts between Europeans and
indigenous peoples along the coast and later spread into the
interior of the coastal countries (Holm, 1988). Huber (1999a) is of
the view that many of the similarities shared by WAPE varieties
are largely a result of the influence of Krio through a diffusion
process along the West African coast. Although WAPEs share
common origins, socio-historical, sociolinguistic and structural
similarities, such that they are mutually intelligible to a large
extent, they also exhibit sufficient differences to make them
distinct varieties (cf. Sebba 1997, Peter & Wolf 2007).
The West African varieties being examined in this study
are identified variously in the literature as Nigerian Pidgin
[English], Ghanaian Pidgin [English], and Cameroon
Pidgin[English] (also called Kamtok),and will be referred to as
Nigerian Pidgin (NP), Ghanaian Pidgin (GP), and Cameroonian
Pidgin (CP) respectively. Studies have also shown that the three
Ofulue | 2
varieties share structural features that distinguish them, as
expanded pidgins, from other varieties like the more developed
Krio creole variety of Sierra Leone (e.g. Huber 1999: 6).
Although these varieties share a common sociohistory,
they have evolved into three sociolinguistically distinct varieties
in terms of use, functions and attitudes towards them. Since
several detailed studies have been carried out on the
sociolinguistic aspect of individual varieties, to conduct another
investigation will not only amount to repetition, but will also be
too much for a single paper. The present study will be limited to
presenting an overview which will form the basis for a
comparative analysis.
In this paper therefore, we shall first briefly review the
literature drawing on available detailed sociolinguistic research in
the three varieties. Then, we shall present an overview of the
sociolinguistic contexts of their emergence. The overview forms
the background for sociolinguistic descriptions of language use,
functions and attitudes towards WAPE varieties. The paper also
discusses language development efforts and the implications for
language planning. In the next section, we carry out a
comparative analysis of the similarities and differences, and the
factors that have impacted their development and spread in their
current social contexts. The final section of the paper will examine
the patterns that characterize the status of the WAPE varieties and
the implications for future developments.
2. Previous Studies
Peter & Wolf (2007) provide the first comparative account
of the structural features that distinguish the WAPE varieties from
one another. They note that while WAPE varieties share structural
similarities, they also display some distinctive structural features
‘given the different linguistic situation in each country and their
close interaction, if not sociolinguistic continuity, with the
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |3
national varieties of WAE (see e.g. Simo Bobda and Wolf, 2003),
whose structural differences are in turn the outcome of a number
of factors peculiar to each country’ (p. 4). Peter & Wolf (2007)
observe that a number of structural features set GPi apart from NP
and CP on the one hand, while NP and CP, on the other hand, are
more similar structurally. They also found that most of the
differences are at the level of pronunciation, followed by the
grammatical and lexical levels1. Similarly, it is expected that the
WAPE varieties will have sociolinguistic similarities as well as
some differences. Of interest to the present study, therefore, is
Peter & Wolf’s observation that the structural features that set GP
apart from NP and CP are attributable to the social parameters of
GP’s geographical distribution, low functional load, and low
social prestige.
Of the three varieties of WAPEs, Nigerian Pidgin (NP) is
perhaps the most researched, with studies on the historical,
structural, and sociolinguistic aspects. Faraclas (1996) describes
NP as the most widely spoken variety in Africa and the most
extensively used Pidgin form in the world, with an estimated
population of over 60 million second language speakers (Faraclas
et al 2005) and more than 1 million first language speakers. It is in
the light of its increasing population of first language speakers that
NP is viewed by some as a variety that is creolising, especially in
coastal parts of the country (cf. Mafeni 1971, Marchese &
Schnukal 1982, Elugbe & Omamor 1991, Ofulue 2004. The
findings of the various studies represent major research in NP and
their findings illustrate NP’s sociolinguistic development over a
period of about thirty years.
Mafeni (1971) provides an overview of NP’s historical
background, functions and structure. In his view, the development
and spread of NP is a result of the process of urbanization which
facilitated the growth of many multiethnic towns and cities where
Ofulue | 4
NP is the lingua franca along with a dominant indigenous
language. Based on a sociolinguistic survey conducted in a speech
community in Warri in the Niger Delta region, Marchese &
Schnukal (1983) observe that NP has become a lingua franca and it
is creolising in view of its expressive functions and acquisition as
a first (primary) language of the speech community. In a study of
NP’s social and linguistic history, Barbag-Stoll (1983) examines
NP’s emergence and development as a variety of WAPE. The
study shows that NP is a developing language that fulfils a social
function as a medium of communication and plays an integrative
role within Nigeria’s multiethnic context. Agheyisi (1984)
examines the widespread use of NP and its changing role resulting
in the development of social varieties of the language. She notes
that although NP is spoken by all socioeconomic classes of people,
the social negative attitude towards it is due to its association with
the uneducated and the lower socioeconomic class.
Elugbe & Omamor (1991), in the first book length study
that covers historical, structural and sociolinguistic aspects of NP,
confirm Marchese & Schnukal’s view of NP’s creolising status.
They take the position that NP should be treated as a language in its
own right, distinct from English, in view of its creolised status in
some sections of the country. They note its spread into more
informal and formal domains of literary writing, media, religion
and education. From a broader perspective of speakers’ social
experience, Oloruntoba (1992) in a doctoral study, examines how
NP speakers negotiate its status and social identities for
themselves in the Western Niger Delta region. She observes that
speakers’ usage practices are framed by their social experiences
within the larger context of the region’s social history. She also
notes that the functions of NP have changed over time from
serving purely communicative purposes to indexing social
identities. Similarly, its status, which was previously associated
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |5
with the uneducated and lower class during the colonial era, has
been redefined in the post-colonial era. Deuber’s (2005) is the
most recent sociolinguistic study that investigates the relationship
between NP and English, its lexifier language, among urban,
educated NP speakers in Lagos, a metropolitan city in the
southwest of Nigeria. Her findings also support Elugbe &
Omamor’s view of NP as a language that is separate from English.
In her view, NP functions as an ‘unofficial national language’ and
its vitality resides in its role of indexing ‘a culture’ (2005:208).
The studies reviewed above were conducted in the South,
where there are large concentrations of NP speakers. In their
studies, Igboanusi & LOTHAR (2005) and Mann (2011) include
data from the northern parts of the country. Igboanusi & LOTHAR
(2005) focus on conflict and competition among Nigeria’s major
languages (Hausa, Igbo, and Yoruba), English and NP. They hold
the view that English and NP pose a threat to the major Nigerian
languages due to the spread of NP into areas where the major
languages are predominant. Based on a survey of language
attitudes and loyalties covering southern and northern states, they
report that NP does not rank highly in comparison with the other
languages.In his survey of southern and northern states that
investigates attitudes toward NP, Mann (2011) observes that
attitudes are based on the socioeconomic benefits and perceived
prestige which the use of NP accords speakers. Results from both
studies show that while NP is used in both the south and the north,
attitudes towards the language are more favourable in the south
than in the north.
Empirical sociolinguistic research in Cameroon Pidgin
[English] (CP) is comparatively minimal (cf. Schröder 2003:22).
Detailed studies include Koenig et al’s (1983, cited in Schröder
2003), which provides a sociolinguistic profile of urban centres in
Cameroon; Mbassi-Manga’s (1973, cited in Schröder 2003),
Ofulue | 6
which describes CP in its historical and sociolinguistic context;
and, more recently, Schröder’s (2003) book length study on the
status, functions and prospects of CP. Schröder (2003:82)
confirms the view in previous studies that CP is the most widely
spread language in Cameroon spoken by about 2 million speakers.
In view of a twenty year gap between Schröder’s and the last major
sociolinguistic study before it, she observes that, ‘the situation of
CamP [CP] can be assumed to have changed considerably’
(Schröder 2003:23). He reports that while Mbassi-Manga’s study
reports that CP’s general function is mainly that of communicative
purposes, his findings (2003: 182) indicate that CP has acquired
more functions as an intra-group and inter-group language for
both anglophones and francophones. CP is associated with the
anglophones, even though it is also used by francophones living in
anglophone areas of Cameroon. While it has a strong hold in
domains it is traditionally associated with, Schröder observes that
its acquisition of more functions in other domains is generally
impeded by the spread of English and French.
Amoako’s (1992) and Huber’s (1999) are the most
comprehensive studies on Ghanaian Pidgin (GP), though there are
several other scholarly publications (e.g. Dako 2002). Huber
(1999) observes that very little work has been done on the
sociolinguistic aspect of GP, and a detailed systematic study of the
sociolinguistic aspects is yet to be conducted. In Ghana, two
varieties of GP have been identified according to the demographic
context of use, age, gender, and socioeconomic class of the
speakers. The variety that is associated with urban, informal use
by young educated male speakers is variously referred to in
literature as the student, educated, institutionalised or acrolectal
variety (e.g. Dolphyne 1995, Huber 1999, Dako 2002). The
variety that is associated with informal use by the uneducated
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |7
lower socioeconomic class of speakers is variously referred to as
the uneducated, non-institutionalised or basilectal variety.
However, the student variety is the more researched of the two and
will be referred to in this study as the student variety of GP (cf.
Huber 1999). It is the variety spoken by secondary school and
university students, perhaps informed by its predominant spread
in educational contexts (Huber 1999:3). Amoako’s study (cited in
Huber 1999) examines GP from historical, structural and
sociolinguistic perspectives. He holds the view that GP is
historically a derivative of NP aided by social factors of
migration, which explains NP’s notable influence2. Huber (1999)
investigates the diachronic and synchronic aspects of GP within
its West African context. His view differs from that of Amoako
with regard to GP’s diachronic development and its relationship
with NP. Huber devotes a chapter in his study to GP’s
sociolinguistic aspects.
While these studies provide sociohistorical and
sociolinguistic descriptions of the individual varieties, the present
study will analyse the sociolinguistic similarities and differences
that have shaped their status within their social contexts. The
findings of these individual studies will therefore constitute data
for a comparative analysis of WAPE varieties in this study.
Ofulue |8
3. Socio-historical Contexts of WAPE varieties
Fig.1: Map of West Africa showing the West African coast
The emergence of WAPE varieties has been traced to trade
contacts with Europeans, beginning with the Portuguese, the
Dutch and then the British (and Germans in Cameroon) along the
West African coast from as early as the 15th century. Other
participants who played significant roles in the formation phase
include Krio speakers and Krumen from the 18th century onwards
(Holm, 1988).There is some debate, however, with regard to how,
where, and when the early forms of Pidgin English (restructured
English Pidgin) replaced Pidgin Portuguese, which is believed to
have been in existence as an established lingua franca prior to the
formation of the WAPE varieties. According to Holm (1988), the
use of restructured English increased dramatically in West Africa
as the British came to dominate the slave trade during the 18th
century, while recaptives and others from Sierra Leone introduced
Krio to Ghana, Nigeria and Cameroon during the 19th century.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |9
The influence of English on the three WAPE varieties,
which was facilitated by British presence within a context of
geographical proximity, intermingling and migration of the
various participants occasioned by trading and administrative
exigencies, can be traced to early contacts with the British along
the Gold Coast and Slave Coast around the early 17th century,
followed by Nigeria and Cameroon in the 18th century. The British
took over from the Portuguese as major trading partners along the
coastal shores of Nigeria and had established two trading ports in
Bonny and Calabar on the Bight of Biafra by the end of the 18th
century (cf. Holm 1988).
Holm (1988) and Huber (1999) attribute the similarities
among the three varieties to interregional connectedness that was
occasioned by geographical proximity, British administrative
influence and structure. The relationship was aided by the fact
that the Gold Coast and Lagos had once been governed from
Sierra Leone. This situation changed and they were governed
under the Gold Coast Colony. West Cameroon was governed by
Britain from Nigeria from 1919,thus facilitating
interconnectedness through inter-regional migration. These
administrative arrangements provided a context for extended
contacts and interaction among the various groups. Furthermore,
Huber (1999:129) notes that NP and CP share more core
grammatical features that originated in Krio than either of them
has with GP, and concludes that the relationship predates the
period of any influence NP may have had on GP. In his view, NP
and CP can be regarded as one and the same variety. However,
while the evidence supports a closer relationship between NP and
CP than with GP, there is also sufficient evidence of structural (cf.
Peter & Wolf 2007) and sociolinguistic differences to distinguish
them.
Ofulue | 10
4. Demographic profile and sociolinguistic context of WAPE
varieties
According to Adegbija (1994:15), ‘the language situation
in sub-Saharan Africa is generally characterised by a type of dense
multilingualism composed of indigenous languages, exogenous
languages, and in many cases, Pidgin languages’.This
observation is illustrated by the demographic profile of the three
countries where WAPE varieties are spoken. The contexts are also
characterised by different language policies and language
situations.
Nigeria
Demographic profile: Of the three countries, Nigeria has
the highest number of languages (510) spoken by a population of
over 160 million, over a land area of 923,768 km with a population
density of 153 persons per square kilometre (NBS 2010, Lewis
2009). Population size is a key criterion for the classification of
major versus minority languages. The three ‘major’ languages,
Hausa, Igbo, and Yoruba are spoken as first or second languages
by about half of the population, and they function as regional
lingua franca and Languages of Wider Communication (LWC).
The other languages, which account for the bulk of indigenous
languages, are viewed as ‘minority’ languages and they function
as languages of immediate community. About 58% of the
population comprising second language speakers are literate in
English (NBS 2010). Adegbija (1994: 16) aptly surmises the
linguistically diverse nature of Nigeria’s population when he
notes that ‘although precise statistics are not available, one can
safely
conclude from the evidence available that no
language in Nigeria is spoken by as much as 50% of the entire
population as a first language’.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |11
Language policy: The language policy as outlined in the
country’s policy on education assigns the functions of an official
language to English, which therefore is the language of
government (cf. NPE 2004), education, the media, and business.
While the major languages, Igbo, Yoruba and Hausa, are national
languages, the indigenous languages, as ‘languages of the
immediate community’, are meant to serve as languages of
instruction for the first three years of schooling. The policy is
however not strictly adhered to as English or NP is often used in
heterogeneous contexts where it is predominant (cf. Bamgbose
1991).
Language situation: English was introduced into Nigeria
twice at different times and in different ways, first as spoken by
the British traders in contact with indigenous groups from which
NP emerged, and second as a language of education in the colonial
era (Mufwene 2009). NP, which coexists with English and the
indigenous languages, functions as a lingua franca and is
predominantly spoken in the southern region
Language Situation: English was introduced into Nigeria
twice at different times and in different ways, first as spoken by
the British traders in contact with indigenous groups from which
NP emerged, and second as a language of education in the colonial
era (Mufwene 2009). NP, which coexists with English and the
indigenous languages, functions as a lingua franca and is
predominantly spoken in the southern region and in urban
contexts, although with speakers that cut across Nigeria’s
geographical, multiethnic and socioeconomic class terrain.
Deuber (2005:208) describes NP’s position in relation to the other
languages as a triglossic language situation in which it is in a
‘broad diglossic’ relationship with English, but it is also distinct
Ofulue | 12
from the indigenous languages. NP is used for communication
alongside indigenous languages and regional languages at the
local/regional level as a language of the immediate community,
and as a lingua franca in certain geographical locations. At the
national level, NP has parallel functions with the national
languages, and it is used as a lingua franca among Nigerians at the
international level.
Cameroon
Demographic profile: Cameroon has about 286 languages spoken
by a population of 18 million over a land area of 475,440 km2, and
a population density of 37 persons per square kilometre (Lewis
2009). The most widely used indigenous languages that function
as languages of wider communication for inter-ethnic and
regional communication are Bulu (South), Duala (Littoral),
Ewondo, and Ewondo Populaire (Centre/South), Fulfulde
(North), Mungaka (North-West), and CP (Schröder 2003). Even
though it is viewed as an Anglophone based language, it is spoken
by both Anglophone and Francophone people. Given the number
of indigenous languages, the Cameroon linguistic situation has
been described as a ‘highly complex’ one. However, the situation
is made even more complex by the presence of two official
languages, English and French (Schröder 2003: 42).
Language policy: Cameroon’s language policy of official
bilingualism makes it the only country on the continent with two
exogenous languages as official languages. Schröder (2003: 47)
reports that although Cameroon can hardly be described as a
bilingual country in practice, there is evidence of relative
openness to English/French bilingualism and interest in the other
official language among educated Cameroonians.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |13
Language situation: The first contact between the
indigenous population and Europeans in Cameroon occurred in
the 15th century (1472), first with the Portuguese, and then with the
British (1850 – 1884),followed by the Germans (Schröder 2003).
CP is believed to have grown out of the 18th century Pidgin English
used around Calabar on the Bight of Biafra (Holm 1988).
Igboanusi and LOTHAR (2005) attribute the mutual intelligibility
between NP and CP to this sociohistorical relationship. CP is used
at all levels of informal and religious communication, except at
the international level. CP co-exists with the two official
languages and the indigenous languages in the sociolinguistic
context described above. Schröder (2003:148) confirms that CP
follows the official languages in order of importance and is used
more often than the indigenous languages to serve the role of a
common language, especially in the Anglophone part of the
country.
Ghana
Demographic profile: Ghana has about 80 languages3
spoken by a population of 23 million, over a land area of 238,000
km2 with a population density of 79 persons per square kilometre
(Huber 1995, Lewis 2009). The major languages, namely Akan,
Ewe, Dangme, Ga, Nzema, Dagaare, Gonja, Kasem and
Dagbani, are national languages. Five of the languages, Akan
(43%), Ewe (10%), Ga-Dangme (7%),Dagaare (6%) and Dagbani
(3%) (Huber 1995) are spoken by about 70% of the population.
Akan is a major lingua franca spoken in the southern region and in
some urban contexts. Hausa4 is also a lingua franca spoken in the
northeast and in Hausa speaking communities located in the
southern region.
Ofulue | 14
Language policy: The language policy, which required
that educational instruction should be conducted in an indigenous
language for the first three years of schooling, was replaced in
2002 with a law that stipulates the use of English from the first
year (Owu-Ewie 2006). Prior to the policy reversal however,
English has been in use because of the heterogeneous nature of the
schools, particularly in the urban centres (Huber 1999). English is
Ghana’s official language and it is the language of government,
administration, the media and other formal situations, while the
indigenous languages are predominant in traditional contexts and
in the regions, including Ga in Accra and Akan in southern Ghana.
Language situation: Although not officially recognised,
GP co-exists with English, which is its major lexifier language,
and the indigenous languages. According to Huber (1995),
‘Ghana’s [socio]linguistic situation is best described as diglossic
with English dominating formal situations and Ghanaian
languages dominating the informal, traditional, and in less
heterogenous areas’. GP is used mainly in informal contexts along
with indigenous languages at the local level of communication,
and following English and major indigenous languages at the
national levels of communication.
5. Findings
This section provides a comparative analysis of
sociolinguistic similarities and differences that characterise the
sociohistorical, demographic, and sociolinguistic contexts of
WAPE varieties.
The patterns that characterise the sociohistorical contexts
of the WAPE varieties include: 1) their parallel emergence within
the same period; 2) their contact with similar groups of people;
and 3) the linguistically diverse nature of the contexts of the
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |15
individual contact situations. Krio played a significant role in the
emergence of the WAPE varieties and the factors that facilitated
its influence directly and indirectly through NP include
interregional connectedness and large scale migration. The
individual contact situations differed with regard to the
indigenous languages that were involved as well as the sources of
Krio influence. It is worth noting that NP’s influence on GP and
CP appears to be unidirectional in nature as there is no evidence
of similar transmissions from GP and CP to NP. It is also possible
that NP’s direct access to Krio influences may have contributed
to its advanced development ahead of CP and GP.
Demographically, Nigeria, Ghana and Cameroon are a
reflection of sub-Saharan Africa’s linguistic diversity and dense
multilingualism. Such linguistically complex contexts engender
language contact, which facilitates the development of pidgins
(Adegbija 2004). The demographic patterns, in terms of high
population density vis-a vis number of languages, show that the
three countries have highly complex and linguistically diverse
contexts in common. The pattern suggests that the higher the
population density and number of languages, the greater the
complexity of the linguistic landscape. Consequently, the
propensity for pidgins to be used predominantly in linguistically
heterogeneous areas is a characteristic shared by the WAPE
varieties. Their sociolinguistic contexts confirm this observation
as the WAPE varieties thrive in linguistically heterogeneous
locations like urban centres and regions (e.g. Lagos, Port
Harcourt, Southern region in Nigeria; Accra, Southern region in
Ghana; and Yaounde, Western region in Cameroon).
The sociolinguistic contexts of the WAPE varieties are
defined by the configurations that characterise their language
situations and the type of language policies adopted. All the three
Ofulue |16
WAPE varieties are similar in their use at all levels of
communication except for international communication. NP and
CP share similarities in their roles as lingua franca, which places
them in a competitive stance with the indigenous languages that
have similar functions. The use of NP and CP as lingua franca at
all levels of communication is an indication of the relatively
strong position NP and CP varieties of WAPE occupy in their
individual language situations; a factor that has positive
implications for their future development. The analysis shows
that while NP and CP share similarities with regard to the
prominent positions they hold, GP is characterised by its
restricted and limited use across the levels of communication in
their individual linguistic systems.They also have the dominant
presence of exogenous languages in common. As is the case in
many African countries, the language policies of these countries
favour and promote the exogenous languages. The language
policies of Ghana and Nigeria are similar in that they promote one
exogenous language, English, as official language and major
indigenous languages as national languages, while Cameroon’s
policy promotes two exogenous languages as official languages.
The presence of two exogenous languages in relation to CP is a
factor that has significant implications for the status of CP and
distinguishes it from NP and GP. Table 1 summarises the language
situations of the WAPE varieties using Bamgbose’s (1991)5 threelanguage model to illustrate the position of the varieties in relation
to the other languages in their sociolinguistic contexts.
Table 1: Summary of status of WAPE varieties
LEVEL
Local
NIGERIA
GHANA
CAMEROON
Indigenous languages
Nigerian Pidgin
Indigenous languages
Ghanaian Pidgin
(mostly uneducated
variety)
Indigenous languages
Cameroon Pidgin (in
towns)
Regional Indigenous languages
Regional languages
(Fulfulde, Nupe,
Kanuri, Idoma, Tiv,
Ibibio, Efik, and Ijo
Nigerian Pidgin
English
Indigenous languages
Ghanaian Pidgin
Indigenous languages
Cameroon Pidgin
(language of wider
communication)
Other LWCs:
Duala, Bulu, Ewondo,
Ewondo populaire,
Mungaka, Fulfulde,
English
French
National English
National languages;
Yoruba (lingua franca
in southwest)
Lingua franca in
southwest
Hansa (lingua franca
in north)
Nigerian Pidgin
(lingua franca in south
zone, urban linguistic
heterogenous contexts)
English
English and French
National languages:
Cameroon Pidgin
Akan (lingua franca in
southern region)
Ewe, Dangame, Ga,
Nzema, Dagaare,
Gonja, Kasem,
Dagbani, Hausa (lingua
franca in the northeast,
Hausa speaking
communities in
southern region)
Ghanaian Pidgin
(educated variety in
urban contexts)
International English
English
English and French
Sources: Bamgbose (1991), Deuber (2006), Huber (1999), Schröder
(2003)
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |17
Ofulue |18
Language Use and Functions
WAPE varieties are examined in terms of major domains
of use and functions using Smith’s (1972 cited in Barbag-Stoll
1983) approach which identifies three language functions:
communicative, integrative (and/or instrumental), and
expressive. The communicative function marks transmission of
referential, denotative information between speakers, e.g.,
inter-ethnic communication; the integrative function marks a
speaker’s use of the language as a member of a particular social
group, e.g., in-group communication; and the expressive function
marks the speaker as a valued member of a particular
linguistic group (or speech community). 6
NP has spread into virtually all domains of use even
though its primary domains are informal contexts (e.g. Elugbe &
Omamor 1991, Oloruntoba 1992, Deuber 2005). NP is gradually
finding its way into more formal domains of use. Recent
developments include literacy primers and a Bible translation that
is in progress.7 Worthy of note is its expansion into the nontraditional domains of Information Communication Technology
(ICT) and the new media (e.g. the Internet and social media). For
example, the establishment of the WAZOBIA radio station which
broadcasts only in NP and which is now widely accepted with
stations in four metropolitan locations, Lagos (southwest), Abuja
(the Federal capital), Port Harcourt (south) and Kano (north) (cf.
wazobiafm.com 2012), is a reflection of the extent of NP’s use.
There is also an attendant expansion of NP into the written
medium on social media platforms by a predominantly young
literate audience. Speakers, therefore, not only use NP to perform
communicative, integrative and expressive functions; they are
expanding NP’s functions to meet needs in the written medium
within ICT and the new media.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 |19
CP is still an informal register that is used in a wide range
of domains and features prominently in discussions expressing
humour, intimacy and secrets (Schröder 2003), except in a few
formal domains like the religious domain. Todd (1984:96) attests
to its use as a major liturgical language of the Catholic Church in
the Anglophone part of the country. The translation of the New
Testament, Gud Nyus fo ol pipul (2002) in CP is further evidenceof
CP’s role and importance in the religious domain. CP performs a
communicative role in its use for anglophone-francophone
communication, for mass mobilisation by politicians (cf. Ayafor
2000, cited in Schroder 2003); and an integrative role in its use
among anglophone university students (cf. Atechi 2011). Only the
use of CP to express intimacy and secrets comes close to fulfilling
an expressive role. This is expected, particularly in view of the fact
that there are speakers who acquired CP as a first or primary
language, which suggests that CP is being used to mark their
identity as members of speech communities where CP is
predominantly used as a first language. Indeed, the results of a
survey carried out by Alobwode (1998, cited in Atechi 2011)
reveal that between 10% and 30% of the respondents who live in
areas within the anglophone part of the country and urban cities
(e.g. Mamfe, Kumba, Limbe, Buea, Bamenda, Douala, and
Yaounde) acquired CP as a first language. Simo Bobda and Wolf
(2003) also confirm the expansion of CP into domains that were
the ‘preserve of the official languages, English and French’.
GP’s most prominent domain of use is among the educated
young male population, especially secondary school and
university students in informal urban contexts where it functions
as an in-group language (cf. Dolphyne 1995, Huber 1999, Dako
2002). Huber (1999) observes that the student variety of GP8 is
spoken mainly in secondary and tertiary institutions as an
Ofulue |20
in- group social register, while the other [uneducated] variety
functions as a lingua franca that is spoken in urban multilingual
contexts. The student variety of GP is used by speakers with high
educational attainment, e.g., secondary school and university
students. It gained wider currency in educational institutions in the
20th century with functions of signaling peer group/in-group
identity and solidarity. Huber (1999) also notes that GP is
restricted to a small but growing section of the society and is less
widespread in terms of area and number of speakers than it is in
other Anglophone West African countries. The functions that
these varieties of GP fulfill constitute one of the parameters that
are used to identify and distinguish them. It is assumed that this
variety of GP, which is gender specific, has also spread among the
educated adult urban male population aged up to 45 years in urban
informal contexts (Huber 1999: 151). Dako (2002) estimates that
about 80% - 90% of the educated male population who are below
the age of 50 years use this variety of GP.
From the foregoing, it is clear that GP is not used
extensively across major domains. Consequently, its functions are
limited. The use of the varieties of GP is determined by a number
of variables: 1) domains of use in terms of urban/rural, contexts;
and 2) speakers’ status in terms of education, age, and gender (cf.
Huber 1999: 159). While the variety associated with the less
educated performs a communicative function, the student variety
of GP fulfils an integrative function for its speakers in that it serves
as a social group identity marker.
A comparative evaluation of the status [vitality] of WAPE
varieties is presented based on domains of use and identified
functions. Eight major domains of use are compared across the
three varieties: administration, education, mass media, political
campaigns, religion, literature, and entertainment.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 21
Table 2: Summary of Domains of use and Functions of WAPE Varieties
NP
GP
CP
Formal
X
X
*
Informal
***
X
***
*
X
*
Informal (outside class)
***
***
*
Mass Media ( Print and Electronic)
****
*
*
New Media ( Internet and Social Media)
***
X
X
Political Campaigns
****
X
***
Religion ( Churches)
****
*
***
Commerce / Trade
****
***
***
Literatures (novels, drama , poetry )
***
*
*
****
****
*
*
*
***
National Identity
X
***
X
X
X
***
Lingua Franca ( Communicative)
****
***
****
Social Identity ( integrative)
****
****
***
X
***
***
Domains of Use
Administration/work
(Government, Parliament, National/State Assemblies
Education
Formal (in class)
Entertainment
Music
Humour (Comedy)
Roles and Functions
Ethnic/ Cultural Identity
Group Identity (expressive)
Key: **** high *** medium * low frequency/degree of use
Sources: Elugbe & Omamor (1991), Oloruntoba (1992), Deuber (2005),
Adekunle (1995), Dolphyne (1995), Huber (1999), Dako (2002), Chumbow &
Simo Bobda (1996), Schröder (2003
Ofulue | 22
Table 2 shows that although WAPE varieties are quite
varied across the eight selected domains, there are some patterns
that characterise their use and the functions that they perform
within the different domains of use. These patterns will be
analysed in terms of restriction/ expansion and degree of use. NP
is the most widespread of the three varieties in view of its
comparatively high degree of use in a greater number of formal
and informal contexts (cf. Elugbe & Omamor 1991, Oloruntoba
1992, Deuber 2005, Taiwo & Babalola 2009). NP is followed by
CP which, though also widespread across the domains, is used
mostly in informal domains and, to a limited degree, in some
formal domains (cf. Schröder 2003, Simo Bobda & Wolf 2003,
Mbangwana 2004, Atechi 2011). GP shows the least spread across
the domains and the degree of use is equally low (cf. Dolphyne
1995, Huber 1999, Dako 2002). The above shows that there is a
correlation between the patterns that characterise their spread and
their degree of use.
Table 2 also reveals that NP and CP are more similar
compared with GP which is restricted in terms of spread across
domains of use and degree of use within domains. The use of NP
and CP is more widespread because they are used in more domains
than GP. NP and CP also show a higher degree of use (medium to
high) than GP which, apart from the informal context of the
education domain and the commerce domain, shows a
comparatively low degree of use across the domains. The
restricted and limited use of GP is a result of the factors that
determine the use of its two varieties. The student variety of GP is
therefore a functionally marked variety while the variety
associated with the uneducated is the unmarked variety of GP.
Age is an important factor that characterises the use and
functions of WAPE varieties. Various studies have observed a
more widespread and higher degree of use of WAPE varieties
among the younger generation (e.g. Deuber 2005, Schröder 2003,
Huber 1999). For NP, its spread into non-traditional
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 23
domains like the new media as well as expansion in social identity
functions is attributed to the younger generation. Similarly, Huber
(1999: 159) and Schröder (2003: 113) observe that GP (educated
variety) and CP are used in a wider range of contexts by the
younger generation than by the older generation.
In table 2, WAPE varieties mark three of the five identities.
None of the varieties was observed to mark cultural identity,
which is a function of the mother tongue. The functions comprise
marking different identities and fulfilling certain communication
needs. The role of WAPE varieties as lingua franca in
inter-ethnic communication remains their most prominent
function, even though GP performs that role in more restricted
contexts. GP is the most restricted of the three varieties in use and
functions. All three varieties are also similar in marking social
identity. NP and CP are more similar in comparison with GP in
marking national, group and social identities. The group and
social identities fulfil integrative and expressive functions
respectively. GP, on the other hand, performs communicative
functions in inter-ethnic communication, and in limited contexts,
integrative functions for in-group communication. Factors that
affect their use and functions include age, speaker’s socioeconomic status and the context of use. Huber (1999) and
Schröder (2003) report a difference in the use of GP and CP
respectively by the younger generation who use them in a wider
range of contexts than the older generation; the latter use them
mainly in informal contexts and to communicate with nonliterates.
Attitudes
According to Adegbija (2004: 133), ‘Generally, languages
acquire value, prestige, and esteem commensurate with their
perceived utility in different domains of life’. In sub-Saharan
Africa, attitudes towards languages are shaped by a variety of
Ofulue | 24
factors including socio-historical forces, especially the effects of
colonialism; pressures for upward social mobility as a result of the
imposition of European languages and colonial/post-colonial
language and educational policies; and people’s perceptions about
the kinds of functions particular languages can fulfil. From a broad
perspective that accounts for the sub-Saharan African context,
therefore, Adegbija’s (1994: 255) view of language attitudes
‘accommodates evaluative judgements made about a language,
its variety or its speakers towards efforts at promoting,
maintaining, or planning a language, or even towards learning and
teaching it’.
Pidgins and creoles have a history of being viewed as
socially-marked varieties of language and wrongly perceived as
inferior languages (e.g. Mann 2011). Factors that have contributed
to this view include the circumstances of their emergence, their
association with the lower socioeconomic and non-literate
sections of society, and their co-existence with European lexifier
languages within their post-colonial contexts (e.g. Elugbe
&Omamor 1991, Oloruntoba 1992). However, studies have also
observed changing attitudes towards the use of WAPE varieties
(e.g. Agheyisi 1984, Oloruntoba 1992, Deuber 2005, Huber 1999,
Bobda & Wolf 2003).
Since attitudes are evaluated on the basis of the kind of
functions languages fulfil, we examine particular functions that
have added value or prestige to WAPE varieties, as well as the
acceptability of WAPE varieties as lingua franca, national
languages and medium of instruction. The three WAPE varieties
are characterised by patterns of covert prestige and varying
degrees of stigmatisation which derive from functions that mark
in-group identities and fulfil group solidarity. The findings show
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 25
that the three varieties share a high degree of covert prestige,
particularly among the younger generation who derive a group
identity value from their use (cf. Marchese & Schnukal 1983: 218,
Deuber 2005:51, Schröder 2003:210, Huber 1999:159). However,
the varieties differ with regard to the range of contexts, with NP
having the widest range of contexts in which the high value
attached to its use accords it high covert prestige, and in some
cases, overt prestige. It is followed by CP in a number of contexts
in which the high value attached to its use gives it high covert
prestige in a number of contexts; and GP in which the value
associated with its use accords it high covert prestige in certain
restricted contexts.
The studies examined show that attitudes towards the use
of WAPE varieties for in-group communication are more
favourable than attitudes towards their use if they are to function
as official languages/ lingua franca. Mann’s (2011) survey shows
that 33.5% of respondents in southern states and 17.5% of
respondents in northern states are in favour of NP’s adoption as an
official lingua franca (official language), ‘in spite of its
sociolinguistic vitality as the urban lingua franca in Nigeria’.
However, Mann considers these results favourable in the light of
the fact that NP is ‘a non-formalized, non-graphized, sociallymarked variety of language which is still not recognized in any
official government document as one of the languages in Nigeria’.
For CP, Schröder’s (2003: 204) survey shows that only 4.5% and
6.7% of respondents said they would like to know CP and to
improve their knowledge of CP respectively. Conversely, Atechi’s
(2011: 19) survey among university students shows a more
positive trend, in which 85% of the respondents agree that CP is a
Ofulue | 26
lingua franca in the Anglophone part of the country as well as in
most sections of the Francophone part of the country, and about
49% of respondents agree that CP should be made one of
Cameroon’s official languages. For NP and CP, geographical
location is a factor, as attitudes in northern Nigeria and in the
Francophone part of Cameroon are generally less favourable
towards NP and CP as official languages/lingua franca (cf. Mann
2011, Bobda & Wolf 2003). With regard to GP, negative attitudes
towards it as an official language/lingua franca constitute one of
the effects of comparatively exceptional positive attitudes towards
English (cf. Huber 1999).
Attitudes towards the WAPE varieties as national
languages follow similar patterns of covert prestige. Studies (e.g.,
Elugbe 1995) observe that NP meets the various criteria for
national languages which include neutrality, population,
geographical spread and acceptability. Various studies (e.g.,
Elugbe 1995 and Deuber 2005) also show that NP is the most
ethnically neutral language in Nigeria. However, the result of a
survey conducted by Deuber (2005) in Lagos, an urban context
among educated speakers of NP, shows that the number of
respondents who agree that NP is a corrupt form of English is
equal to the number of respondents who disagreed (22% each),
while 53% of the respondents opted for a middle ground response
(‘Pidgin is similar to English, but it does have its own grammatical
rules’). In comparison, of the languages in Schröder’s (2003:196)
survey of which languages have acquired the status of a national
language, CP ranked the highest with 29.1% . Both NP and CP
fulfil the major criteria for national languages, except in terms of
the acceptability criterion where attitudes are not as favourable
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 27
(cf. Elugbe 1995, Deuber 2005, Schröder 2003). However, the fact
that NP is viewed as an ‘indigenous’ language (Elugbe 1995: 291),
and CP as more African than the official European languages
(Schröder 2003:196), shows that attitudes towards them are
favourable. GP is the least eligible, as it is yet to acquire the status
of a predominant lingua franca as a prerequisite.
Findings of attitudes towards WAPE varieties as a medium
of education are also varied. For NP, location is a factor, as
attitudes towards it as a medium of education are more favourable
in parts of the country where NP is the lingua franca. In their
survey of the Delta region, Schnukal & Marchese (1983) report
that 40% of their respondents (under 15 years) responded
positively; Oloruntoba’s (1992) survey of Benin, Sapele and
Warri reported a positive response from 52% of the respondents.
Mann’s (2011) survey of northern states had positive responses
from only 18.3% of the respondents compared with 28% from the
southern states. Gani-Ikilama (1990: 225) notes that the most
common basis for objection to the use of NP in education is the
notion that it will interfere with the acquisition of English. In
comparison, attitudes towards CP as a medium of education are
less favourable, while they are negative for GP. Atechi’s survey
of attitudes towards CP as a medium of instruction shows that only
31% agree that it should be used in schools. Schröder’s (2003:
245) results are similar to Atechi’s (2011), as a greater majority
(89.6%) do not think CP should be used in schools.
The findings confirm that attitudes towards appropriate
use of codes in a speech community have a high correlation with
their functional distribution and the relative social status of their
speakers (e.g., Saville-Troike 1982:185).While NP and CP’s
predominance in terms of their function as lingua franca
Ofulue | 28
contributes to the more positive attitudes towards them, GP’s nondominant inter-ethnic communication function contributes to the
less positive attitudes towards it (cf. Simo Bobda & Wolf 2003).
The most relatively favourable attitudes are towards NP,
both as an official language/lingua franca and as a national
language.It is followed by CP, while GP shows the least
favourable attitudes. The findings of this study corroborate
Huber’s (1999: 160) observation that there is a positive perception
among Ghanaians that Nigeria has ‘the best and real Pidgin’, and
that it stems from the belief that the development of GP in Ghana
is a result of the influence of NP. They also substantiate his
observations 1) that attitudes towards GP are more negative than
those towards NP in Nigeria, particularly among the educated
elite; and 2) that attitudes towards NP and CP are more similar than
those towards GP. The higher degree of stigmatisation observed in
GP, in comparison with NP and CP, is a reflection that GP is still in
the early stages of development, thus corroborating Huber’s
assessment of GP that (1999:158) ‘the position of pidgin in Ghana
mirrors that of Nigeria in the 1960s’. These observations indicate
that there is a correlation between predominant functions and
attitudes towards the WAPE varieties.
A comparative analysis of attitudes towards WAPE
varieties therefore confirms Peter & Wolf’s (2007:2) observations
that there are ‘different attitudes towards Pidgin English that
prevail in Ghana, Nigeria and Cameroon. Researchers agree that
in Ghana WAPE is held in the lowest esteem, while it has the
widest acceptance in Nigeria; WAPE in Cameroon seems to range
somewhere in the middle’. The expanding functions of NP and
CP are a positive indication of a concomitant shift in attitudes
towards greater acceptability of WAPE varieties.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 29
Language development
WAPE varieties function mainly as spoken languages.
However, the expansion of their functions and the evolvement of
attitudes towards them has made language development
necessary. Ferguson (1968, cited in Deuber 2005: 52)
distinguished three dimensions for evaluating language
development (corpus planning activities):graphisation,
standardisation and modernisation. Indeed, language in written
form is considered a significant factor, and consequently the next
step in the maintenance and development of spoken languages
(cf. Bendor-Samuel 1996), since codified languages attract higher
status and functions. However, none of the WAPE varieties enjoys
official recognition or has accepted standardised
orthographies, even though they all have a history of
literarydevelopment and an increasing literate population who
have facilitated the expansion of their functions into more
domains. For example, NP is characterised by etymological-based
writing practices that follow the English writing system, while
linguists have proposed phonemic based writing systems that
depict NP as a language in its own right, independent from its
English lexifier (e.g., Mafeni 1971, Elugbe & Omamor 1991,
Faraclas 1996, Literacy International 2007, and Naija Language
Academy (NLA) 9 2010).The issues that arise from
non-standardisation include inconsistent and highly variable
writing and spelling systems. Another aspect of language
development, which refers to status planning activities, involves
making decisions to expand, restrict or assign particular functions
to a language. Currently, these decisions evolve from speakers’
usage practices, but are not officially guided in any coordinated
fashion.
Ofulue | 30
The issues highlighted in previous studies confirm
Sebba’s (1997) observations about four major problems that make
it difficult for pidgins and creoles to become standardised
languages, namely their low status; their similarity with their
lexifiers (which causes them to be perceived as inferior); their
variable nature; and the adoption of a model/variety for their
standardisation. In view of negative perceptions by the greater
majority towards the use of WAPE varieties in schools, any
language development effort to promote their use will have to take
into account the various concerns expressed about the perceived
detrimental effects of their use on the acquisition of languages of
upward social mobility. Simo-Bobda &Wolf (2003) note that
many, especially the elite and teachers, will need to be convinced
of the benefits to be accrued from developing the pidgin to
function as an official language or as a medium of education.
However, Elugbe & Omamor (1991) argue for a broad view of
literacy that includes other languages apart from English for
communicative purposes and national development. Since
‘people are best reached in their language or that of the immediate
community’, they recommend the inclusion of NP for
programmes that are designed to promote literacy, such as Adult
Literacy (1991: 137). The current realities in Nigeria, for
example, show that the government and its agencies engage
intranslating information from English into the local languages
and Nigerian Pidgin. Similarly, Gani-Ikilama (1990) and
Schröder (2003) recommend the development of NP and CP
respectively as tools to facilitate the acquisition of the languages
of education, at least at the basic/primary levels. In line with
lessons from the African American Vernacular English (AAVE)
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 31
experience, this study advocates the adoption of a curriculum that
incorporates the differences between WAPE varieties and
Standard English to help children transit from the language of the
home or community to the language of formal education (cf.
Migge et al 2010).While NP, and to a fair extent CP, has enjoyed a
greater degree of development as a result of extensive research,
attitudes towards GP have largely impeded its development.
Hence, GP is the least developed of the three varieties with
regard to corpus and status planning research activities.
6. Conclusion
Drawing on previous studies that provide socio-linguistic
descriptions of three WAPE varieties, the major goal of this study
has been to account for the similarities and differences in domains
of use,functions and attitudes that have shaped their
sociolinguistic status. The study confirms Adegbija’s (1994)
observation that ‘there is a close link between history, language
policies, social interactions and the functions and uses of
languages in the shaping of the socio-linguistic’ status of WAPE
varieties. There is a vertical connection within each variety and a
horizontal relationship across the varieties in the socio-histories,
demographic profiles and language policies of the WAPE
varieties shaping their use, functions and attitudes towards them
and their subsequent development. Despite the similarities in their
socio-histories, demographic and sociolinguistic contexts, the
observed differences in the outcome in their use, functions and
attitudes towards them confirm ‘the unpredictable nature of
socio-historical forces which cause a pull in different directions’
(Adegbija 1994: 47).
Ofulue | 32
Taken together, the findings of this study show that a
combination of sociolinguistic forces and factors has shaped the
current status of the WAPE varieties. For example, the effects of
NP’s earlier and direct exposure to Krio influences for
aconsiderable period of time; its more heterogenous linguistic and
demographic context; a pluralistic language policy; a triglossic
language situation; and the larger population of speakers who use
NP as a first/primary language, have contributed to its being the
most developed of the WAPE varieties. In contrast, under similar
sociohistorical, demographic and sociolinguistic contexts, a
diglossic language situation and a particularly high value
attributed to English have contributed to GP’s status as the least
developed of the WAPE varieties. While CP shares similarities
with NP and GP in terms of sociohistorical, demographic and
sociolinguistic contexts, and with NP in terms of a growing
population of first language speakers, it also differs from NP and
GP with regard to its language situation which is affected by its
bilingual language policy, thus influencing its status as a
developed WAPE variety.
In line with Lanehart’s (2001: 4) observations about
language and African American English compared to WAPE
varieties, this study has provided a holistic view of the
interrelatedness of the various aspects of their sociolinguistic
development and why speakers of WAPE varieties ‘continue to
speak it despite antagonistic pressures socially, economically,
educationally, and otherwise’. While official recognition in their
current sociolinguistic contexts will certainly increase positive
attitudes towards WAPE varieties, the nature of their expansion
indicates that it is the speakers and their communication needs that
determine the direction of development. For example, NP and CP
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 33
have continued to expand into more formal domains and
functions, and attitudes towards them are changing in spite of
stigmatisation, lack of codification and official recognition.
Age or generation are primary variables that affect the use,
functions and attitudes towards WAPE varieties. The study shows
that generally, the younger generation had more positive attitudes
and attributed more prestige to the use of WAPE varieties than the
older generation, especially the educated conservative elite, who
had more negative attitudes. The younger generation are also the
primary agents of expansion into more domains of use. Wolf
(2001: 192) confirms the importance of the youth as a major factor
when he states that linguistic usage of children [and youth in
general] reflects current developments and is the best indicator to
predict future trends. Future research should focus on this section
of the population, as their patterns of language behaviour
constitute a point of reference for future spread and expansion of
the WAPE varieties. Huber (1999: 156) notes for GP that ‘a
comparison of the status of GhaPE [GP] with that of Pidgin in
Nigeria reveals interesting similarities and differences. It may also
point to future sociolinguistic developments of Pidgin in Ghana’.
By extension, a comparison of similarities and differences in the
status of the WAPE varieties also offers interesting indications of
their future sociolinguistic developments. By focusing on the
sociolinguistic aspect of WAPE varieties, this study contributes
to a greater understanding of the nature and sociolinguistic status
of the WAPE varieties that make up the continuum to inform its
future development.
Ofulue | 34
Notes
several languages are acquired at the same time, .e.g., similar functions that a
primary or first language fulfils.
The use of postpositional d?m as a plural marker; the use of na as a copula, as
focus marker, and as intensifier; the use of d?n as retrospective marker; the use
of bin as tense marker are found in NP and CP, but not in GP. In pronoun system:
Ji as the third person singular pronoun is found in CP, but not in GP and NP. una
as the second person plural pronoun is found in NP and CP (as wuna) but not in
GPmi as (unmarked) subject pronoun and mi as possessive pronoun are found
in NP, but not in GP and CP (cf. Peter & Wolf 2007: 17).
7
Ehrihabor’s (2011, 2012) collection of poems was published using Naija
Language Academy’s ethnophonemic based writing system. Mercy Ministries
Bible Translation project is in collaboration with Literacy International.
Literacy International has produced two literacy primers for the learning and
teaching of NP.
8
2
Reviewer’s comment: There was a strong Nigerian influence through the
police and the military – often referred to as Abongo brofo (the English of the
military/police). Today the barracks are still pidgin speaking. There was also a
considerable presence of Nigerian traders – from the riverine states and Yoruba
speaking areas. Accra had a ‘Lagos Town’ where pidgin was used.
3
Unlike NP and CP, GP is not listed in Ethnologue as one of Ghana’s languages
(Lewis 2009).
The reviewer of this paper confirms this distinction and refers to the educated
variety as ‘Ghanaian Student Pidgin’.
9
The Naija Langwej Akademi (NLA) was inaugurated in July 2009 in
collaboration with the French Research Institute (IFRA). The academy was
established to coordinate research in NP for the promotion and standardisation
of NP, produce a reference grammar and a dictionary. The academy is currently
engaged in corpus development.
4
Reviewer’s comment: Hausa is actually more prevalent and spoken by more
people today in the big zongos of the southern cities.
5
The model is based on the fact that, in multilingual situations, speakers learn
another language apart from their first language for instrumental or integrative
purposes. At the local/regional level therefore, several indigenous languages
will be in use with one regional language as a second language. The language
use at different levels of communication is dependent on a number of factors
including the heterogenous nature of the context, the functional load, the
participants, setting, topic etc. In general, NP is used alongside the other
languages at these levels of communication.
6
This function is best understood in the light of Marchese & Schnukal’s
‘functional’ definition of a creole as a ‘language of pidgin origin which must
fulfil an ‘expressive’ function for individuals in a speech community’. This
definition is meant to replace the traditional definition of a creole which
requires identifying a speaker’s first language and as M&S have pointed out, it
is difficult to determine a child’s first language in a multilingual setting where
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 35
Ofulue | 36
References
Adekunle, M. 1995. ‘English in Nigeria: Attitudes, policy and
communicative realities’ In A. Bamgbose,A. Banjo & A.
Thomas (eds.). New Englishes: A West African
Perspective. Ibadan: Mosuro. 35-56.
Agheyisi, R.N. 1984. Linguistic implications of the changing
role of Nigerian Pidgin English. English World-Wide 5:
211-33.
Chumbow, Beban S. & Augustin Simo. 1996. The life cycle of
post-imperial English in Cameroon. In J. A. Fishman,
A.W. Conrad, and A. Rubal-Lopez. (eds.). Post-Imperial
English: Status change in former British and American
colonies. pp. 401-429. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Dako, K. 2002. Student Pidgin (SP): Language of the educated
male elite. Institute of African Studies Research Review
18(2): 53-62.
Adegbija E.E. 1994. Language Attitudes in Africa: A
Sociolinguistic Overview. UK: Multilingual Matters.
Dolphyne, F. 1995. A note on the English language in Ghana.
In A. Bamgbose, A. Banjo & A. Thomas (eds.). New
Englishes: A West African Perspective., pp. 10-26.
Ibadan: Mosuro.
Adegbija, E.E. 2004. Multilingualism: A Nigerian Case Study.
NJ: Africa World Press, Inc.
Deuber, Dagmar. 2005. Nigerian Pidgin in Lagos. London:
Battlebridge.
Atechi, S. 2011. Pidgin English in Cameroon: To teach or not to
teach? International Journal of Language Studies
(IJLS) 5(1): 17-30.
Bamgbose, A. 1991. Language and the Nation: The Language
Question in Sub-Saharan Africa.Edinburgh: Edinburgh
University Press.
Barbag-Stoll, A. 1983. Social and Linguistic History of
Nigerian Pidgin English: As Spoken by the Yoruba with
Special Reference to the English Derived Lexicon.
Tübingen: Stauffenberg.
Bible Society of Cameroon. 2000. Gud Nyus fo ol Pipul. Yaounde:
Bible Society of Cameroon.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 37
Echu, George and Obeng, Samuel. 2004. Africa Meets Europe:
Language Contact in West Africa. New York: Nova
Publishers Inc.
Egbokhare, Francis O. 2003. ‘The story of a language: Nigerian
Pidgin in spatiotemporal, social and linguistic context’.
In P. Lucko, L. Peter and H. Wolf (eds.). Studies in
African Varieties of English.Frankfurt: Lang. Pp.21-40
Elugbe, Ben O. and Omamor, Augusta.P. 1991. Nigerian
Pidgin: Background and Prospects. Nigeria:
Heinemann.
Elugbe, Ben O. 1995. Nigerian pidgin: problems and prospects.
In A. Bamgbose, A. Banjo & A. Thomas (eds.). New
Englishes: A West African Perspective., pp.268-283
Ibadan: Mosuro.
Ofulue | 38
Faraclas, N. 1996. Nigerian Pidgin. USA: Routledge.
Faraclas, N., Lourdes G., Medina, M. and Reyes, W. V. 2005.
Ritualised insults and the African diaspora: Sounding in
African American Vernacular English and Wording in
Nigerian Pidgin. In
Susanne Muhleisen (Ed.).
Politeness and Face in Caribbean Creoles. Philadelphia,
PA, USA: John Benjamin Publishing Co.
Federal Republic of Nigeria. 2004. National Policy on
Education. Fourth t h edition. Lagos: Federal
Government Press.
Holm, J. 1988. Pidgins and Creoles Vol. 1: Theory and
Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Huber, M. (1999). Ghanaian Pidgin English in its West African
Context. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.
Huber, M. 1995. Ghanaian Pidgin English. Retreived from
lingweb.eva.mpg.de/apics/images/0/00/SurveyGhana.pdf
Igbohanusi, H. & Lothar, P. 2005. Languages in Competition.
Berlin: Peter Lang.
Lanehart, S. L. 2001. State of the art in African American
English research: Multidisciplinary perspectives and
directions. In S. L. Lanehart (ed.). Sociocultural and
Historical Contexts
of African American English
Philadelphia: John Benjamin Publishing Co. pp. 1-20.
Lewis, M. Paul (ed.). 2009. Ethnologue: Languages of the
World. Sixteenth edition. Dallas, Tex.: SIL International.
Online version:http://www.ethnologue.com/.
Okonkwo R.C. et al. 2007. Mek wi rid Naijiria Pijin. USA:
Literacy International.
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 39
Oribhabor, E. 2011. Abuja na Kpangba an oda puem-dem.
Ibadan: IFRA.
Oribhabor, E. 2012. If yu hie se a de Prizin: Antoloji of
Puem-dem fo Naija Langwej. Ibadan: IFRA.
Lothar, P. & Wolf, H. 2007. A comparison of the varieties of
West African Pidgin English. World Englishes 26 (1):
3–21.
Mafeni, B.O.W. 1971. Nigerian Pidgin. In J.Spencer (ed.). The
English Language of West Africa. pp. 95-112.
London: Longman.
Mann, C.C. 2011. North and South: Attitudes Toward AngloNigerian Pidgin in urban Nigeria.
Paper presented at
Society for Pidgin and Creole Linguistics (SPCL)
Conference, 2-6 August 2011, University of Ghana,
Legon, Accra, Ghana.
Marchese, L. & Schnukal, A. 1983. Creolization in Nigerian
Pidgin English: A Preliminary Report. English
World-Wide 4: 17-26.
Mbangwana, P.N. 2004. Pidgin English in Cameroon: A
veritable linguistic menu. In G. Echu and S. Obeng
(eds). Africa Meets Europe: Language Contact in West
Africa pp. 26-49. New York: Nova Publishers Inc.
Migge, B., Leglise, I. & Bartens, A. 2010. Creoles in education:
a discussion of pertinent issues. In A. Migge, I. Leglise
and A. Bartens (eds.). Creoles in Education. .
Philadephia: Amsterdam. pp. 1-31
National Bureau of Statistics (NBS). 2010. The National Literacy
Survey. Retrieved from www.nigerianstat.gov.ng
Ofulue | 40
Ofulue, C. I. 2004. Creolisation in Nigerian Pidgin: a
sociocultural perspective. In S. Awonusi & E. A.
Babalola (eds.). The Domestication of English in
Nigeria: A Festscrift in Honour of Professor Abiodun
Adetugbo. Lagos: University of Lagos Press. pp. 265-279
Oloruntoba, Y. 1992. Sociocultural Dimensions of Nigerian
Pidgin Usage (Western Niger-Delta of Nigeria). PhD
Thesis, Indiana University, Ann Arbor: UMI.
Wolf, Hans-Georg. 2001. English in Cameroon. Contributions
to the Sociology of Language 85. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Wolf, H. & Simo Bobda, A. (2003). Pidgin English in
Cameroon in the new millennium, In Peter Lucko, Peter
Lothar & Hans-Georg Wolf, (eds.) Studies in African
Varieties of English Frankfurt: Peter Lang. pp. 101-117.
Owu-Ewie, C. 2006. The language policy of education in
Ghana: A critical look at the English-Only language
policy of education. In Selected Proceedings of the 35th
Annual Conference on African Linguistics, ed. John
Mugane et al., 76-85. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
Proceedings Project. www.lingref.com, document
#1298.
Schröder, A. 2003. Status, Functions, and Prospects of
Pidgin English. Tubingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.
Sebba, M. 1997. Contact Languages: Pidgins and Creoles. New
York: PalgraveSimo.
Taiwo R. & Babalola, T. 2009. Code-switching in contemporary
Nigerian Hip-hop music. Itupale Online Journal of
African Studies 1: 1-26.
Todd, L. 1984. Modern Englishes: Pidgins and Creoles.
London: Blackwell.
Wazobia FM95.1. 2011. Facebook comments Retrieved from
www.wazobiafm.com
Legon Journal of the HUMANITIES
Special Edition , 2012 | 41
Ofulue | 42