325
Journal of Physiology (1993), 463, pp. 325-348
With 5 figures
Printed in Great Britain
TEMPORAL AND SPATIAL SUMMATION IN THE HUMAN ROD
VISUAL SYSTEM
BY LINDSAY T. SHARPE, ANDREW STOCKMAN*, CLEMENS C. FACH
AND UWE MARKSTAHLER
From the Neurologische Universitdtsklinik, HansastraJ3e 9, D-7800 Freiburg im
Breisgau, FRG and the *Department of Psychology, University of California at
San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109, USA
(Received 13 December 1991)
SUMMARY
1. Absolute and increment thresholds were measured in a retinal region 12 deg
temporal from the fovea with 520 nm targets of varying size and duration.
Measurements were made under rod-isolation conditions in two normal observers
and in a typical, complete achromat observer who has no cone-mediated vision. The
purpose of these experiments was to determine how the temporal and spatial
summation of rod-mediated vision changes with light adaptation.
2. The absolute threshold and the rise in increment threshold with background
intensity depend upon target size and duration, but the psychophysically estimated
dark light of the eye (the hypothetical light assumed to be equivalent to
photoreceptor noise) does not.
3. The rise in increment threshold for tiny (10 min of arc), brief (10 ms) targets
approaches the de Vries-Rose square-root law, varying according to the quantal
fluctuations of the background light. The slope of the rod increment threshold versus
background intensity (TVI) curves in logarithmic co-ordinates is about 0-56+0-04
(when cones are not influencing rod field adaptation). For large (6 deg) and long
(200 ms) targets, a maximum slope of about 0 77 + 0 03 is attained.
4. The steeper slopes of the rod-detected TVI curves for large, long targets implies
some reduction in temporal or spatial summation. In fact, the change in summation
area is much more critical: under conditions where only the rod system is active the
TVI curve slope is independent of target duration, suggesting that temporal
summation is practically independent of background intensity.
5. The rise in threshold also depends on the wavelength of the background field in
the normal observer but not in the achromat, confirming reports that the field
adaptation of the rods is not independent of the quantal absorptions in the cones.
The cone influence is most conspicuous on long-wavelength backgrounds and is found
for all target sizes and durations, but is greater for large and long targets than for the
other conditions.
MS 9972
326
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
INTRODUCTION
These experiments concern the changes in temporal and spatial summation that
occur as the isolated human rod visual system light adapts. To estimate the extent
of such changes, we compare rod increment threshold versus background intensity
(TVI) functions obtained with rod targets of different duration and size. It is well
known that increasing target size or duration steepens the slopes of TVI functions
(e.g. Stiles & Crawford, 1934; Graham & Kemp, 1938; Bouman, 1950; Barlow, 1957,
1958), and this has often been taken as evidence that spatial and temporal
summation decreases as the visual system light adapts (but see Chen, MacLeod &
Stockman, 1987, and the Discussion, below).
Previous studies of changes in temporal and spatial summation have employed
procedures in which threshold is determined either primarily by cones (e.g. Graham
& Kemp, 1938) or by rods at low adapting levels and by cones at higher levels (e.g.
Stiles & Crawford, 1934; Bouman, 1950; Barlow, 1958), with - in most cases - no
explicit attempt being made to distinguish rod from cone thresholds. As a result, the
changes in summation inferred from these studies often confound changes in
summation that occur under scotopic conditions with those that occur under
photopic conditions.
It is important to look at the changes that occur under pure scotopic conditions
because of the differences that underlie the human rod and cone visual systems. Not
only do primate rod and cone photoreceptors vary in their dynamic responses to light
(e.g. Baylor, Nunn & Schnapf, 1984; Schnapf, Nunn, Meister & Baylor, 1990), but
also rod signals have access to a distinct pathway to the retinal ganglion cells, which
is unavailable to cone signals and which is the primary visual pathway at low
scotopic luminances (see Daw, Jensen & Brunken, 1990 for a recent review). Thus,
photoreceptor isolation is likely to reveal differences in summation and adaptation
that are postreceptoral as well as receptoral in origin.
In this study, we were careful to distinguish rod from cone thresholds. Only data
measured well below the cone plateau thresholds following a rod bleach or data
measured in a typical, complete achromat who displays no signs of functioning cone
vision are used to estimate changes in temporal and spatial summation.
Additional precautions are necessary, if we are to use TVI functions to estimate
changes in summation in the isolated rod system since it is well known that cones can
raise rod threshold (e.g. Makous & Boothe, 1974). For example, under conditions
similar to those used in the classic 'rod isolation' study of Aguilar & Stiles (1954), we
have shown that a long-wavelength field steepens the logarithmic slope of the rodmediated TVI curve for a 6 deg, 200 ms flash by nearly 20 % compared to slopes
measured on rod-equated shorter wavelength fields, even though target detection is
mediated by rods (Sharpe, Fach, Nordby & Stockman, 1989 a; Sharpe, Fach &
Stockman, 1992).
To control for this cone intrusion, we obtain rod threshold data on short and
middle wavelength backgrounds (, = 450, 520 and 560 nm), where rod sensitivity
below the cone plateau is controlled almost exclusively by rods (Sharpe et al. 1992),
and compare them with curves obtained on long-wavelength backgrounds (u=
640 nm), where rod sensitivity is strongly influenced by the cones. Additionally, we
327
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
exploit the rod threshold data of an achromat, which is free of cone influence, as a
reference.
Applying such procedures yields a surprising result that deviates from conventional wisdom: increasing target duration has little effect on the slope of rodmediated TVI curves below 1 scotopic troland (td). This suggests that there is little
change in temporal summation with background intensity (see also Hallett, 1971).
Increasing the target diameter, on the other hand, causes a substantial increase in
the slopes of rod-mediated TVI curves.
METHODS
Subjects
Three male subjects were tested in these experiments, K. N. and authors C. F. and U. M. They
were all informed about the nature of the experiment and that no risk to their health was involved.
C. F. and U. M. are normal trichromats with normal (uncorrected) visual acuity. K. N. is a typical,
complete achromat, who displays all the classic symptoms of typical, complete achromatopsia,
without any evidence for cone function (for details, see Sharpe & Nordby, 1990). During the
experiments, he wore a + 9 0 dioptre convex lens, which magnified the retinal image 1P22 times.
Thus, all of the visual angles in K. N.'s external field of view have to be corrected by this factor to
bring them into agreement with visual angles in the uncorrected emmetropic eye.
Stimuli
Our experimental conditions were essentially the same as those used in the previous paper
(Sharpe et al. 1992). The target and adapting field parameters were chosen to favour the rods
relative to the cones - an unnecessary precaution in the achromat. The target had a wavelength of
520 nm. It was presented 12 deg extrafoveally (in the nasal field of view) in the centre of an 18 deg
diameter, adapting field; and it entered the edge of the dilated pupil (3 mm off-centre) to take
advantage of the Stiles-Crawford effect, which is much larger for the cones than for the rods (e.g.
Stiles, 1939). The entry point of the adapting field was central. The duration and the size of the
target was varied; as was the wavelength of the adapting field. It was either 450, 520, 560 or
640 nm.
Apparatus
The stimuli were seen in a four-channel Maxwellian view, optical system, interfaced with a
computer (see Sharpe et al. 1992). In these experiments, only two channels were required: one to
provide the target (incremental) stimulus; the other the adapting field.
The duration of the test flashes was controlled by a computer-operated electromagnetic shutter
cutting a filament image in the target channel. The shutter had rise and fall times of less than
041 ms.
At the beginning and end of each experimental session, the quantal flux densities of the light
beams were measured at the Maxwellian image with a silicon Pin-10 diode coupled to an
operational amplifier (United Detector Technology, Model 80X Optometer, Orlando, FL, USA).
Procedure
About 30 min before beginning each experimental session, the subject had his left pupil dilated
with 0-5 % tropiciamide (Mydriaticum Rocheg). He then positioned himself in the optical system
by biting into a silicone-base, dental-wax impression of his teeth mounted in a machine tool-rest.
Following 40 min of dark adaptation, he fixated a small illuminated cross placed so that the test
flash and the adapting field were centred 12 deg on the temporal retina. The observer's absolute
threshold was then measured several times by a computer-controlled, single staircase procedure.
Threshold was defined as the mean wedge setting of twelve staircase reversals. At each new level
the observer was pre-adapted for at least 3 min, before his threshold was determined. The
328
L. T. SHARPE AXD OTHERS
procedure continued until a full series of increment thresholds were obtained for a wide range of
adapting field radiances. Such series were collected for all combinations of target durations, size
and backgrounid wavelength.
In order to assess the extent of rod isolation for each background intensity (and wavelength),
thresholds were also measured during the plateau that terminates the cone phase of recovery from
a wNhite (3100 K) bleaching light of 7 7 log10 photopic troland seconds (td s).
TABLE 1. Rod incremental threshold responses for a 520 nm target of varying size and duration:
absolute thresholds and dark noise values for a typical, complete achromat K. N. and two normal
trichroinat observers U.M. and C.F. estimated using eqn (1) (see text)
K.N.
C.F.
Field
U.M.
(achromat)
wavelength, ,t
(normal)
(normal)
(nm)
(loglo scotopic td)
6 deg diameter, 200 ms duration
- 3-65 +0 03
-4 09 +0 03
Absolute threshold (mean ± S.D.)
-3-39 +0-06
-2-87
Dark noise
450
- 3-77
-3.79
-2-57
- 3-57
-3-85
520
-2-71
-3-65
560
-350
-2-99
640
-3-60
-3-58
-3 64+0-09
-2-79+0-18
Alean+ S.D.
-3 69+0-16
6 deg diameter, 10 ms duration
Absolute threshold (mean +S.D.)
-2-48+0 09
-2-98+0-10
-2-80+0-10
Dark noise
-2-77
450
-3 77
-3-66
520
-2-72
-3-15
-3-60
-2-92
-3 61
560
-3-51
640
-2-99
-3-66
-300
-3 47 +0 34
-2-85+0-13
AMean + S.D.
-3 52 +0-25
10 min of arc diameter, 200 ms duration
- 1-28+005
Absolute threshold (mean + s.]D.)
-1-51 +0-08
-1-15+0-07
- 3-39
Dark nioise
450
-3.95
-2-49
520
-2-91
-384
-3-66
-2-62
560
-302
-3-37
-2-72
640
-3.74
-3-34
Alean + S.D.
-3 48 +0 37
-3 58+0-29
-2-67 +0-18
10 min of arc diameter, 10 ms duration
Absolute threshold (mean + S.D.)
-044 + 0-13
-0-29 + 009
-0 39 + 002
Dark nioise
450
-3-82
-3 39
-2-95
520
- 3-73
-3-46
-2-84
- 3 42
560
-3-51
-2-72
- 3-53
640
-3 61
-3-88
Alean + S.D.
-3-67 +0-21
-3-56 +0-22
-3-01 +0-36
-3-58
-3-58
Average dark noise (mean)
-2-83
Data treatment
Slope. To determine the slopes of the rod detected portions of the increment threshold curves,
the thresholds were fitted bv means of a computerized curve-fitting program (Sigmaplot, Jandel
Scientific. Corte-Madera. CA, USA) using the logarithmic form of the following equation:
AI=
(1)
k=(I, +l1)
In eqn (1). AI is the increment threshold intensity and I, is the background field intensity of
wavelength ,I. I is a dark light' constant added to account for the levelling off of the increment
threshold curve at low background intensities; it determines the point at which the slope of the
.
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUTAXA. ROD V,ISION
329
increment threshold curve rises above the asymptotic absolute threshold value. Changing the value
of Io has the effect of shifting the curve laterally along the log10 I,, axis. The exponent n is the
asymptotic slope and k is a vertical positioning constant (kId corresponds to the absolute threshold
tabulated in Table 1, below).
For each individual observer, the fitting of eqn (1) to the increment threshold data was
completed in two stages. In the first stage, the three parameters, Io, k and n, were allowed to vary
freely and individual fits were determined for each of the TVI curves measured against the four
(450, 520, 560 and 640 nm) field wavelengths m. Since the values for k for each observer did not
vary with ,t (i.e. absolute threshold is independent of adapting field wavelength) and those for lo
did not vary with ,t or with target condition (see Table 1, below), average values were applied in
the second fitting stage. In the final fits to the sixteen TVI curves only n was allowed to vary.
In both fitting procedures, eqn (1) was fitted to the threshold data from absolute threshold to
a background on which threshold still lies clearly below the cone plateau thresholds. For each
observer, the fits at 450, 520 and 560 nm (and the secondary fit at 640 nm, see below) were made
over the same range of background intensities. This range was increased at 640 nm, since rod
isolation extends to higher scotopic intensities at that wavelength. In general, the upper limit was
an increment threshold lying at least 0 5 loglo unit below cone threshold. The range of backgrouind
intensities over which each fit was made is indicated bv the horizontal extent of the continuous
curves shown in Figs 1-3.
Temporal and spatial integration. To determine the extent of the change in spatial or temporal
integration, we measured thresholds with targets of two diameters (6 deg and 10 min of arc) and
two durations (10 and 200 ms).
Traditionally, the range of sizes over which threshold is inversely proportional to the target size
(i.e. when Ricco's law holds; Ricco, 1887) is taken to be the range over which there is complete
spatial summation, and the range of durations over which the threshold is inversely proportional
to the target duration (i.e. when Bloch's law holds; Bloch, 1885) is taken to be the range over which
there is complete temporal summation (but see Discussion). The 6 deg and 200 ms targets that we
used were considered large and long enough to exceed the upper limits of complete spatial
(estimated to be between 0 5 and 2 deg; see, for example, Barlow, 1958) and temporal (estimated
to be about 100 ms; see Fig. 5, below) summation of rod-mediated vision, even in the dark. The
10 min of arc and 10 ms targets were considered small and brief enough to be within the limits of
complete spatial and temporal summation of rod-mediated vision, even at the highest background
intensities. We note that the 200 ms target may fall within the region where temporal summation
is only partial, at least for small fields (Barlow. 1958: and Fig. 5, below).
We first averaged the 450, 520 and 560 nm TVI data for each normal observer within 0 5 log1o
unit bins. (The 640 nm TVI data were separately analysed to determine how the concomitant
desensitization of the cones affects the estimates of rod temporal and spatial integration.) Data
points were only included between the adapting field intensities corresponding to Io (see Table 1)
and the level at which the cones mediate threshold. (In the achromat K. N., the full extent of the
curves could be used, so two separate analyses were made: one included data up to an adapting
intensity at which cone intrusion occurs in the normal observer, the other included data up to rod
saturation. His data were averaged over all four background wavelengths.) The data for the
appropriate target conditions were then subtracted from each other and the remainders were fitted
by simple linear regression equations to yield the following estimates:
'Total' or the combined temporal and spatial change:
(10 min of arc, 10 ms thresholds) - (6 deg, 200 ms thresholds);
Slong or the spatial change for long duration targets:
(10 min of arc, 200 ms thresholds) -(6 deg, 200 ms thresholds);
sbrief or the spatial change for brief duration targets:
(10 min of arc, 10 ms thresholds) -(6 deg, 10 ms thresholds);
or
the
for
diameter targets:
temporal
change
large
Tiarge
(6 deg, 10 ms thresholds) - (6 deg, 200 ms thresholds);
or
the
for
small
diameter
temporal
change
targets):
small
(10 min of arc, 10 ms thresholds) -(10 min of arc, 200 ms thresholds).
@4~ ~
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
330
A
22
20
18
16
10
O
8
-
4
v
20
-6 -4
-6
-4
0
-00
cn
0)
3
-2
-2
00
2
p=640nm
14
063
<~~~~~~
12
8
=
y
r0 58 00
560 nm
6
4
8-
2
'01,
17tV
055
b~~~~~
p=56nm
10
*
0
*_S
p =450nm
0.51
-2
0.74
p =450nm
-4
1
*
16
0.73
p = 520 nm
E
co
18
t0.78
/I= 560 nm
6
2
20
0.94
p = 640 nm
12
10 min of arc,200 ms
22
*~~
14
la
C
6 deg,200 ms
44
-6
-4
-00 -6 -4
6
ID
6 deg, 10 ms
E 22
22
c 20
20
10 min of arc, 10
* * *
14
p=640nm
12
0e-
=
520 nm
=640
12
v
0.64
nm
V
V
V
V
TVT
.
560 nm
0.51
u=520nm
0.52
=
_yV7
10
0.68
5
-
14
tt_,~v
VVv
l7--
p
2
v
p = 560 nm
p=
16
08
V
64-
.*o
V
10
8-
*O*
18
1016
6
ms
CD
° 18
4
2
0
-2
8
o
6
0~~~0
9-
4
0.70
n
08
2
00
U
=
450nm
0
053
0
=450 nm
-2
.071
-2
-4-
-6
-o
-6
-4
-2
0
2
4
6
-4
_o
-6
-4
log background intensity (scotopic td)
Fig. 1. For legend see facing page.
-2
0
2
4
6
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
331
RESULTS
Absolute threshold
Table 1 lists, for the four target conditions, each observer's absolute threshold,
estimated according to eqn (1). Expressed in terms of scotopic trolands, the absolute
threshold increases as the target is made small and brief (the values given in Table
1 for each target condition are averaged over the four adapting field wavelengths).
Expressed in terms of the total amount of light incident at the cornea, however, it is
actually lowest for the tiny, brief target.
Absolute threshold for the tiny (10 min of arc), brief (10 ms) target requires
approximately 41, 36 and 52 (507 nm) quanta incident at the cornea, respectively,
for observers U. M., C. F. and K. N. These values are slightly lower than the 'classic'
values reported by Hecht, Schlaer & Pirenne (1942). They found that for a tiny
(10 min of arc), brief (1 ms) target between 54 and 148 incident quanta were required
for threshold.
The comparable absolute threshold in quanta for the other targets are: 580 (U. M.),
440 (C.F.) and 1055 (K.N.) for the 6 deg, 200 ms target; 136 (U.M.), 205 (C.F.) and
429 (K.N.) for the 6 deg, 10 ms target; and 141 (U.M.), 62 (C.F.) and 105 (K.N.) for
the 10 min of arc, 200 ms target. These larger values reflect the fact that the longer,
larger targets exceed the upper limits of complete spatial and/or temporal
summation.
Dark light
The absolute threshold is believed to be limited by internal noise within the visual
system or the 'dark light' (Barlow, 1956, 1957). Estimates of each observer's dark
light, Io in eqn (1), are listed in Table 1 for each target and adapting field condition
(Io roughly accords with the background intensity corresponding to the intersection
of the line of constant slope with the absolute threshold ordinate in Figs 1-3).
The estimates of Io are remarkably consistent across conditions for any one
observer. For achromat K. N., the estimated dark light is - 2-83 + 0-24 log1o scotopic
td or 676 quanta (507 nm) s-5 deg-2 at the cornea, which corresponds to a
Fig. 1. The effect of background wavelength (iu) on the form of the rod increment
threshold versus intensity curve for normal observer C.F. The threshold curves were
measured for four target conditions: 6 deg, 200 ms (A); 6 deg, 10 ms (B); 10 min of arc,
200 ms (C); 10 min of arc, 10 ms (D). In all conditions, the target had a wavelength of
520 nm and was presented 12 deg extrafoveally in the centre of an 18 deg diameter
adapting field. It entered Lhe edge of the dilated pupil (3 mm off-centre) to take advantage
of the Stiles-Crawford effect. Four background wavelengths were used for each target
conditions: 450 (0), 520 (Li), 560 (V) or 640 (O) nm. The corresponding filled symbols
represent the thresholds measured for the same stimulus conditions during the plateau
that terminates the cone phase of recovery from a white (3100 K) bleaching light of
7-7 log1o photopic td s. The curves are correctly placed with respect to the axis of the
abscissae, but the axis of the ordinates is correct only for the lowest curve (,U = 450 nm)
in each of the four panels; the other curves are displaced upward in intervals of 5 0 log1o
units. Each data point is a mean based on at least three sets of measurements made on
different days. The continuous lines drawn through each set of increment threshold data,
below the point at which the cone plateau thresholds intersect the steady-state
thresholds, are the best-fitting logarithmic forms of eqn (1). The slope (n) in logarithmic
co-ordinates is shown to the right of each curve (see Table 2).
332
22
20
18
14
G-
10
6
4
-
2
-
0
-6
0)
a) 22
E
20
-2
u = 640 nm
14
12
6
4
0
,u= 560
2
17--
520
2
0.72
nm
,u=450nm
0-
-4
_
Oo
-6
-4
~
v
u*bJ
000
.0
uu=5202nm
E
C;-
0.54 0 o
@ 0 0
.53
~~0-5
-4
4
_ 00 -6 -4
65
-2
0
2
4
6
4
6
10 min of arc, 10 ms
D
22
20
0
18
=
16
6406nm
s
'V1
14
VVVVv
v
12
=
560nm
3
10
00
8
6
,u =
4
0
2
0
074
-2
vv
..UE
-2
*888880088
0
v
u= 450 nm
0
SR-M
MEEM0o
6
p=
0.73
,u = 560 nm
vv
-
2
''vvv,ww~~Vq
nm
[
.0
0.77
O.-
10
-2
8
+Oti
16
4
10
0.65
0--~ ~
12
0
18
8
,v
6 deg, 10 ms
B
0
-4
,u= 640 nm
14
0
-0
.C
16
0.76
*@ @S@@g@v
p=450nm
0.79
*
~0
0
18
0.74
= 520 nm
2
0
20
vv'--v vvvvlvv
=560nm
10 min of arc, 200 ms
22
vv
v
8
0
0.88
,u=640 nm
12
0
C
*~~~~
16
0
E}~ 8
6 deg, 200 ms
A
0
,~ ~ ~v
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
,
= 450 nm
0-59
0.59
-2
-4
-00
-6 -4 -2
4
0 2
log background intensity (scotopic td)
Fig. 2. For legend see facing page.
6
0
2
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
333
photoisomerization rate of approximately once every 92 s per rod. For normal
observer C. F., it is - 3-58 + 0-20 log1o scotopic td or 120 quanta (507 nm) s-' deg,
which corresponds to a photoisomerization once every 521 s per rod. For normal
observer U. M., it is - 3-58 + 0-27 log1o scotopic td or 120 quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-',
which corresponds to a photoisomerization once every 521 s per rod. The fact that
the dark light of the normal observers is smaller than that of the achromat K. N. is
consistent with their lower absolute thresholds.
Slope
Figures 1-3 present, for four background wavelengths, the increment threshold
curves measured for the two normal observers, C. F. (Fig. 1) and U.M. (Fig. 2), and
for the achromat observer K. N. (Fig. 3). Results are given for each of four target
conditions: 6 deg, 200 ms (panel A); 6 deg, 10 ms (panel B); 10 min of arc, 200 ms
(panel C) and 10 min of arc, 10 ms (panel D).
6 deg, 200 ms target
For the largest (6 deg), longest (200 ms) target (panel A), the results agree with
those given in the preceding article (Sharpe et al. 1992, Figs 1-3). In the two normal
observers, U. M. and C. F., there is a wavelength dependence in slope of the increment
threshold curve with adapting field wavelength: the 640 nm background causes the
rod threshold to rise more steeply than do the other backgrounds. The change can be
assessed by comparing the final logarithmic slopes of the TVI functions; these are
shown to the right of each set of increment threshold data. These slopes refer to the
curves (continuous lines) fitted to the rod-detected increment thresholds, according
to eqn (1). The final slopes are listed along with the other curve-fitting parameters
in Table 2. In the achromat K. N., who has no cone vision, there is no dependence
upon background wavelength. His slope is constant across wavelength, averaging
0-78 + 0 04 against all backgrounds. For the normal observers it averages 0-75 + 0-03
(C. F.) and 0-76 + 0-02 (U. M.) against the 450, 520 and 560 nm backgrounds and is
0 94 (C. F.) and 0-88 (U. M.) against the 640 nm background.
It can be seen in Figs 1 and 2 that in the normal observers the rods determine
threshold up to higher scotopic background intensities on the 640 nm background
than on the shorter wavelength backgrounds. This allows us to fit eqn (1) over
a more extended range of background intensities at 640 nm. To ensure that the
increased slopes found at 640 nm are not entirely due to the inclusion of these higher
intensity threshold data (for it could be that the slope simply increases with
intensity), we also fitted eqn (1) over the same range of scotopic background
intensities at 640 nm as we used for the fits at shorter wavelengths. The resulting
slopes are tabulated in an additional row of Table 2 (this has also been done for the
other target conditions). The slopes are still markedly higher at 640 nm than at
shorter wavelengths even for the reduced fit.
Fig. 2. The effect of background wavelength (,u) on the form of the rod increment
threshold versus intensity curve for normal observer U. M. Same conditions as in Fig. 1.
The parameters of the continuous lines drawn through each set of increment threshold
data are given in Table 2.
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
334
A
22
I
22
20
20
18
18
16
0
14 -
0
16
p=640nm
-
v
S
-
,-V
v-
6
0
4
p
=
0
8
~~~~~~0
0
6
2
4-)
0
450 nm
,u=0-~~~~~
~~ ,/
-2
2
4
0
0
0
0
p = 450 nm
G0G57
0
-2
~~0.83
-4
-4
-oo -6
'a
c5C,
B
-4
0
-2
2
4
6
-6 -4
-00
6deg,10ms
D
0)
E0
0 58
2
-0
Q
0
-S
u = 560 nm
4
520 nm
'a
._
v
V
10
0.75
p=S6Onm
v
0.63
12
v
10
8
u = 640 nm
14
0.79
12
40
10 min of arc, 200 ms
C
6 deg, 200 ms
22
22
20
20
18
18
16
16
-2
0
2
4
6
44
6
6
10 min of arc, 10 ms
c
0.
0
14
12
p = 640 nm
640 nm
u =0-~~~~~
0-61
14
-079
01-
v
v
12
v
10
u
8
10
=560 nm
0-7
0
0
6
0
8
6 -
4
p=520nm
07
4
2
0-
0-74
2
0
=
u
450
nmo
G--~
~
-2
~
O
u=5202nm
.0.52
C-
0
0
0
0-59
0-
-2
-4
-4
1
-oo -6
-4
-2
-4
--2
-6 -4
--o 1-6
4
2
6
0
log background intensity (scotopic td)
Fig. 3. For legend
see
facing
page.
0
0
2
2
'
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
335
6 deg, 10 ms target
When the target duration is decreased to 10 ms (Figs 1-3, panel B), there is still a
dependence of slope on background wavelength for normal observers, but it is weaker
than for the 6 deg, 200 ms target. In the achromat, as before, the slope remains
constant across wavelength. The slopes in both the normal and achromat observers
TABLE 2. Rod incremental threshold responses for a 520 nm target of varying size and duration:
TVI slopes for a typical, complete achromat K. N. and two normal trichromat observers U. M. and
C. F. estimated using eqn. (1) (see text) and the mean absolute thresholds and dark noise values
listed in Table 1
Field
wavelength,
U. M.
C. F.
K. N.
u (nm)
(normal) (normal) (achromat)
6 deg diameter, 200 ms duration
450
0-79
0-74
0-83
520
0-76
0-73
0-76
560
0-74
0-78
0-75
640
0-88
0-94
0-79
640*
0-86
0-93
6 deg diameter, 10 ms duration
450
0-74
0-71
0-75
520
0-72
0-70
0-74
560
0-73
0-68
0-76
640
0-77
0-78
0-79
640*
0-74
0-78
10 min of arc diameter, 200 ms
duration
450
0-53
0.51
0-57
520
0-54
0-58
0-60
560
0-53
055
0-58
640
0-65
0-63
0-63
640*
0-62
0-61
arc
10 min of
diameter, 10 ms
duration
450
0-59
0-53
0.59
520
0.59
0-52
0-52
560
0-63
0-51
0-56
640
0-65
0-64
0-61
640*
0-64
0-62
* This row tabulates the 640 nm slopes estimated over the same range of background intensities
for each subject as the 450, 520 and 560 nm slopes.
Fig. 3. The effect of background wavelength (,u) on the form of the rod increment
threshold versus intensity curve for achromat observer K. N. Same conditions as in Fig.
1, except that no cone plateau thresholds could be measured for this observer. The
parameters of the continuous lines drawn through each set of increment threshold data
are given in Table 2.
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
336
A
4.0 -
C
3.5k
Total
3.5
Total
-
3.01
-0*19
3.0 F
*
0
0
2.5 F
aF
U
Sbrief
0
0
°-.-0-20
*
2.0 F Sbrief
-0-20
*
Siong
2.5 F
**
2.0k
o
-p
Siong
-0-17
0
-0.16
-0.19
.
1*5
1.5k
v7
1.-0
'-
-0.04
vW
Vsmal
0.5
CD
1.0k
IV
v
v
w
v
V
Tsam81
0
V
V
-3
1
0
0
Y
.
.
v
'7
-0.01
-2
0
-1
1
D
Total*
3.5 F
-0.15
3.0 F
*
Sbrief
0
3.0k
*
2.5 F
-04"14
o.°
S
"'°
2.0 F
d 0*.
2.5
-
--o.. o
-0-20
0
.
o
-o
Sbrief
-o
Siong
V
v
y? V
vv
-Q
og -
1.01
-0.06
x,
7Tiarge
v
0.00
.
Tsmall'
~~v v
.
v
Tiarge
v
-0-16
---V
V
1.5k
0.05
1.0F Tsmall
~~-0-23
*<
2.0 F
1.5
-
0.5k
V
--
-3
-4
B
0)
-
0.5
-1
-2
V
Tiarge
0.01
-4
Fr
0
-0.04
Tsmall
Tiarge
-0 04
vv
0.5
0
-1
1
-3
-2
0
1
2
log background intensity (scotopic td)
Fig. 4. Estimates of the change in the spatial and temporal summation of the rod visual
system of the two normal observers C. F. (A) and U. M. (B) and the achromat K. N. (C) as
a function of background intensity, from the intensity corresponding to the observer's
dark light up to the intensity at which the cones start to influence threshold in normal
observers (approximately 0 5 log1o scotopic td). Also shown are the estimates of achromat
-4
-3
-2
-1
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
337
tend to be slightly less than those for the 200 ms test flash (see Table 2). In the
normal observers, the average slope measured on all backgrounds other than 640 nm
is 0 70 + 0-02 (C. F.) and 0 73 + 0-01 (U. M.) (vs. 0-75 and 0-76, respectively, for the
200 ms test flash condition); on the 640 nm background it is 0-78 (C. F.) and 077
(U. M.) (vs. 0-94 and 0-88, respectively). In the achromat, the slope on all backgrounds
including 640 nm is 0-76 + 0-02 (vs. 0 78). The small reduction in slope accords with
the idea that increasing the background intensity reduces the time over which the
rod visual system integrates light (Barlow, 1958). However, the effect (except for the
640 nm field in normals, for which the reduction is larger), if any, is very small.
10 min of arc, 200 ms target
Panel C of Figs 1-3 presents the field adaptation curves obtained when the target
duration is kept long (200 ms), but the target diameter is reduced to 10 min of arc.
This provides a measure of the influence of changes in spatial reorganization upon the
rod field adaptation curve, which will influence the slope of the curve for a large flash
but not for a small one if the size of the small target is smaller than the minimum
summation area of the rod visual system. Reducing the target size clearly results in
increment threshold curves of shallower slopes. For the achromat K. N., the slope has
an average value of 0-60 + 0 03 across wavelength (vs. 0-78 for the large, long target,
see Table 2). For the normal observers, the slope on the 450, 520 and 560 nm
backgrounds average 0-55 + 0 04 (C. F.) and 0 53 + 0-01 (U. M.) (vs. 0-75 and 0 76), but
are 0-63 (C. F.) and 0-65 (U.M.) (vs. 0-93 and 0 86) on the 640 nm background.
10 min of arc, 10 ms target
Panel D of Figs 1-3 presents the field adaptation curves obtained when the target
duration is shortened to 10 ms and the target diameter reduced to 10 min of arc.
Jointly reducing target duration and size does not markedly further decrease the
slope of the curves compared to reducing the size alone (see Table 2). For the
achromat K. N., the slope has an average value of 0 57 + 0 04 across wavelength. For
the normal observers, the slopes on the 450, 520 and 560 nm backgrounds average
0-52+0-01 (C. F.) and 0-60+0-02 (U.M.). On the 640 nm background, the slopes are
0-62 (C.F.) and 0-64 (U.M.).
K. N. determined over an extended adapting field range (D), including levels up to
those corresponding to the onset of rod saturation. 'Total' (combined temporal and
spatial change) is the log ratio of the observer's threshold quantity of light for a 10 ms,
10 min of arc diameter target to his threshold intensity for a 200 ms, 6 deg diameter
target. T'ma11 (temporal summation for small flashes) is the log ratio of the observer's
threshold quantity of light for the 10 ms, 10 min of arc diameter target to his threshold
intensity for the 200 ms, 10 min of arc diameter target. Tiarge (temporal summation for
large flashes) is the log ratio of the observer's threshold quantity of light for the 6 deg
diameter, 10 ms target to his threshold intensity for the 6 deg diameter, 200 ms target.
Sbrief (spatial summation for brief flashes) is the log ratio of the observer's threshold
quantity of light for the 10 ms, 10 min of arc diameter target to his threshold intensity
for the 10 ms, 6 deg diameter target. SI.ng (spatial summation for long flashes) is the log
ratio of the observer's threshold quantity of light for the 200 ms, 10 min of arc diameter
target to his threshold intensity for the 200 ms, 6 deg diameter target. The threshold
ratios for normal observers C. F. and U. M. were averaged over the 450, 520 and 560 nm
background data; those for achromat K. N. over the 640 nm data as well.
338
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
Additional targets
The slope values listed in Table 2 are roughly confirmed by additional values
measured for normal observer U. M. when the target diameter was held constant at
03 deg, but the target duration was decreased from 100 to 20 ms. Slopes were
determined for two adapting field wavelengths, 450 and 640 nm (The slopes were
determined from graphic plots and not according to eqn (1)). On the 450 nm
background, where rods are predominantly desensitized (or activated), all slopes
roughly approximate 0.50, regardless of target duration: 0-53 (100 ms), 0-45 (70 ms),
0-48 (50 ms) and 0 50 (20 ms). On the 640 nm background, however, where cones
contribute to desensitization by the background as well, the slopes are significantly
higher than 0.5 for all target durations: 0-68 (100 ms), 0-65 (70 ms), 0-62 (50 ms),
0-60 (20 ms).
Estimates of spatial and temporal summation
Figure 4 provides an estimate for each observer of the change in the spatial and
temporal summation of the rod visual system as a function of background intensity.
First, it plots the effect of the combined change in temporal and spatial summation
(labelled 'Total'); that is, the log ratio of each observer's threshold quantity of light
for the 10 ms, 10 min of arc (0-022 deg2) target to his threshold intensity for the
200 ms, 6 deg (28-27 deg2) target (from Figs 1-3). For achromat K. N., the comparison
has been made over the same adapting field range as for the normal observers (i.e.
from the adapting field intensity corresponding to the dark noise up to the
intensity level at which the cones start to influence threshold in the normal
observers, approximately 05 logl0 scotopic td). It has also been made over an
extended adapting field range (i.e. up to the adapting field intensity corresponding
to the onset of rod saturation, approximately 2-0 logl0 scotopic td) to determine if
further temporal and spatial reorganization occurs in the rod visual system at
intensity levels where the rod responses are normally masked by those of the cones.
The comparison assumes that changes in temporal and spatial summation do not
affect the 10 ms, 10 min of arc threshold because the target area and duration are
always within the ranges where complete temporal and spatial summation occur, but
that the changes affect the 200 ms, 6 deg threshold because the target area and
duration exceed these ranges as the adapting level is increased. It can be seen that
the amount of total summation change is similar for all three observers, decreasing
as the powers 0-20 (K.N.), 0-19 (C.F.) 0-15 (U.M.), as the background intensity is
raised from about -3.5 (normal observers) or - 3-0 (achromat K. N.) to 05 logl0
scotopic td. An additional small change may be occurring at higher intensities because
the value measured for K. N. over the extended range (from about - 3 0 to 2-0 log10
scotopic td) is slightly larger (0-23). The reduction in summation causes an overall
threshold elevation by factors of 5 0 (K. N.), 14-1 (K. N., extended range), 5-8 (C. F.)
and 4-0 (U.M.).
These values for the exponent change are similar to that (0 25) reported by Barlow
(1958), when he compared thresholds measured in normal observers for a 7-5 ms,
0-0077 deg2 target and a 935 ms, 19 deg2 target.
Second, the figure plots the change in spatial summation for each observer for brief
flashes (labelled Sbrief); that is, the log ratio of the observer's threshold quantity of
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
339
light for the 10 ms, 10 min of arc target to his threshold intensity for the 10 ms, 6 deg
target. As the background intensity is raised, the amount of spatial summation for
all three observers decreases by powers of 0 19 (K. N.), 0-19 (K. N., extended range),
0 16 (C. F.) and 0 14 (U. M.). Over the same intensity range, threshold correspondingly
rises by factors of 4-6 (K.N.), 8-9 (K.N., extended range), 4-4 (C.F.) and 3-6 (U.M.)
in area.
Third, Fig. 4 plots for each observer the change in spatial summation for long
flashes (labelled Slong); that is, the log ratio of each observer's threshold quantity of
light for the 200 ms, 10 min of arc target to his threshold intensity for the 200 ms,
6 deg target. As the background intensity is raised, the amount of summation for all
three observers decreases by powers of 0-17 (K. N.), 0 16 (K. N., extended range), 0-20
(C.F.) and 0-20 (U.M.), corresponding to threshold factor rises of 3.9 (K.N.), 6-3
(K.N., extended range), 6-3 (C.F.) and 6-3 (U.M.). There is little difference between
the Sbrief and Slong estimates, suggesting that the change in spatial summation is
roughly independent of target duration.
Fourth, Fig. 4 plots the change in temporal summation for each observer measured
with small flashes (labelled as TrAmau); that is, the log ratio of each observer's
threshold quantity of light for the 10 ms, 10 min of arc target to his threshold
intensity for the 200 ms, 10 min of arc target. As the background intensity is raised,
the amount of temporal summation for all three observers changes by powers of
-0 04 (K.N.), -0-06 (K.N., extended range), 0-01 (C.F.) and 0 05 (U.M.),
corresponding to threshold factor changes of 1-4 (K. N.), 2-0 (K. N. extended range),
11 (C.F.) and 1-6 (U.M.).
Finally, the figure plots the change in temporal summation for each observer
measured with large diameter targets (labelled as Tiarge); that is, the log ratio of each
observer's threshold quantity of light for the 10 ms, 6 deg target to his threshold
intensity for the 200 ms, 6 deg target. As the background intensity is raised, the
amount of temporal summation for all three observers changes by powers of -0-01
(K. N.), -0-04 (K. N. extended range), -0 04 (C. F.) and 0 00 (U. M.), corresponding
to threshold factor changes of 1 1 (K.N.), 1-6 (K.N., extended range) 1-4 (C.F.) and
10 (U. M.). That there is virtually no difference between the Tlrmall and Tiarge
estimates suggests that the change in temporal summation - what little there is - is
independent of target size.
DISCUSSION
In general accord with Barlow (1957), we find that thresholds determined with
a tiny, brief target rise less quickly with adaptation than those determined with a
large, long target. The former, measured in the achromat or in the normal observers
for ,u < 560 nm (see Table 2), rise with an average slope of 0-57 (± 0-04), and the latter
with a slope of 0 77 ( ± 0 03). This difference in slope is conventionally taken to reflect
a reduction of the visual system's temporal and spatial summation capability with
light adaptation. The tiny, brief target is assumed to be unaffected by the intensitydependent changes in summation, since it is always within the limits of complete
summation. But, because the larger and longer targets exceed those limits, they show
an accelerated loss of sensitivity with light adaptation.
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
340
At first glance, then, our data seem consistent with the notion that the visual
system sacrifices sensitivity at higher levels for improvements in temporal and
spatial resolution. However, in the achromat and in the normal observer for ,t <
560 nm, the average TVI slope is only 0-56 (± 0-04) for the 10 min of arc, 200 ms
7.1 min of arc
diameter target
2
5.9 deg diameter target
1
0
01
0
ep
0
0
-2
Background
illuminance
0 -0.70
V -2-01
-3 _
O
-00
4
-2
-1
0
-2
log duration (s)
-1
0
Fig. 5. Scotopic temporal summation estimates from Barlow (1958; his Fig. 2). The two
panels show log increment threshold intensity plotted against log duration for small
(7-1 min of arc, left panel) and large (5 9 deg, right panel) diameter rod-detected targets
at three adapting field illuminances, - oc, - 2-01 and - 070 scotopic td. The three sets
of data in each panel have been shifted vertically, so that the thresholds superimpose (for
details, see text).
flash. This is nearly identical to the average slope for the briefer, 10 min of arc, 10 ms
flash (0-57 + 004). Similarly, the average TVI slope for the 6 deg, 200 ms flash
(077 + 0-03) is very similar to the average slope for the briefer 6 deg, 10 ms flash
(0 73 + 0-04). Clearly, these TVI slopes show little dependence on target duration,
suggesting that within the range of our measurements there is only a small change
of temporal integration with light adaptation. Thus, below 0 5 log1o scotopic td, the
rod visual system does not sacrifice sensitivity for improvements in temporal
resolution.
This is made clear in Fig. 4, where we plot the differences between the thresholds
obtained with the four targets. From the regression lines fitted to the data in Fig. 4,
we conclude that there is a 4-5-8 change in threshold due to both spatial and
temporal factors, a 3-6-4-6 change due to spatial factors alone and a 1-0-1-5 change
due to temporal factors alone. Thus, the decline in sensitivity due to changes in
target size is about 3 times greater than that due to changes in its duration. The
decline in rod temporal summation is surprisingly small and is not significantly
341
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
enhanced by extending the range of adapting luminance (K. N.'s data in Fig. 4
suggest that there is only a small additional change in temporal integration for rodmediated vision at luminances between 0 0 and 2-0 log10 scotopic td).
The change in temporal 8ummation with light adaptation
When detection is mediated by rods, our data, therefore, give little support for the
traditional assumption that there is a large change in temporal integration with
adaptation. It is curious that no-one has apparently challenged this assumption
before, especially since an examination of the previously published data reveals little
evidence for a change in temporal integration for rod-mediated vision below 0 5 logl0
scotopic td (however, see Hallett, 1971; his Fig. 7).
Take, for instance, the frequently cited temporal summation measurements of
Barlow (1958). Barlow used homochromatic target and adapting conditions
(497 nm), so that rod-isolated target detection and field adaptation can be safely
assumed for his three lowest adapting levels, -oo, 3-65 and 4-96 log10 quanta
(507 nm) s-1 deg-2 (corresponding to -oo, -0 70 and -2-01 log10 scotopic td).
Barlow's original TVI thresholds measured with 7-1 min of arc (0-011 deg2) and
5.9 deg (27-6 deg2) targets, replotted from his Fig. 2, are shown in Fig. 5, shifted
vertically to minimize the squared deviations between them. When so shifted, they
superimpose almost exactly. This indicates that changes in temporal summation
cannot be significantly influencing the thresholds; for otherwise clear differences in
shape would be found. Bouman (1950) made similar measurements to those of Barlow
shown here; and an examination of them yields a similar conclusion. If Barlow's
(1958) data shown in Fig. 5 are shifted to minimize the squared-deviations only for
durations of less than 100 ms (the leftmost points in each panel, where Bloch's Law
holds), there are minor differences at longer durations that might indicate a small
reduction in temporal integration, but only for durations greater than 500 ms.
Another example is Lennie (1979), who measured TVI curves in one subject for
0-2 deg, 15 ms; 0-2 deg, 1 s; 7-5 deg, 15 ms; and 7-5 deg, 1 s targets. He, like us, found
that the TVI slope was independent of target duration: it was 0 5 for the 0-2 deg
target, and 0-83 for the 7.5 deg target. Curiously, Lennie does not comment on the
fact that his results suggest that there is no change in temporal integration for rodmediated vision with light adaptation. Instead, he speculates that the change in
spatial integration is caused by a decrease in the latency of the receptive field
surround. But if that were the case, the rod-mediated TVI slopes would not be
independent of target duration.
Previous measurements of TVI functions in the same achromat observer as used
in this study also imply very small changes in temporal summation with light
adaptation. Stabell, Nordby & Stabell (1987) measured K. N.'s TVI curves with a
520 nm, 1 x 2 deg diameter target of varying duration. An examination of their
Fig. 3 reveals no change in slope for targets measured with 8 ms, 125 ms and 1 s
flashes.
One study that does show evidence for a change in temporal integration for rodmediated vision is the elegant two-target work of van den Brink & Bouman (1954).
They find a change in temporal integration in the scotopic range (see their Fig. 2).
But the effect is small: over the whole scotopic and photopic range the change in
12
PHY 463
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
342
temporal summation decreases threshold by a factor of only 1-8 (van den Brink &
Bouman, 1954; p. 620).
As mentioned above, the data shown in Fig. 4 suggest that temporal integration
for rod-mediated vision may start to decline at mesopic luminances above 0.0 logl0
scotopic td. This change in temporal summation may be related to the transition
TABLE 3. The slopes of incremental threshold versus intensity curves measured
for achromat K. N.
Slope
Target duration
Target area
Target diameter
(deg)
600
6-00t
6-00t
1 85§
1 x2*
1 x2*
1 85§
1 00:
1 x2*
017§
0 17t
0.20*
017§
0 171
(deg2)
(ms)
(log-log co-ordinates)
28&30
2830
2830
2 69
2 00
200
2 69
0 79
200
002
002
0 03
0-02
002
* Stabell et al. (1987).
200
200
10
200
1000
125
50
100
8
200
200
125
50
10
0 78+0 04
0-77 +002
076+0002
0 73
0 70
067
0-68
0 67
0-65
063
058+0-02
0 62
059
057+004
t Sharpe, Fach, Nordby & Stockman (1989a).
t Present paper.
§ Sharpe, Whittle & Nordby (1993).
from the slow rod pathway, no, to the faster pathway, no', which can be clearly
identified in both psychophysical and electrophysiological flicker data (e.g.
Stockman, Sharpe, Zrenner & Nordby, 1991). The temporal frequency response and
the phase characteristics of the slow rod pathway, which predominates below
0.0 logl0 scotopic td, suggest that the time constant of the slow rod pathway changes
little with light adaptation. In contrast, the time constant of the faster pathway,
which predominates at mesopic levels, may shorten with light adaptation (Sharpe,
Stockman & MacLeod, 1989b). The transition between detection by slow and fast rod
pathways seems to depend surprisingly little on temporal frequency (Sharpe et al.
1989b; see their Fig. 9), and so will be more or less independent of target duration.
A dramatic change in the time constant of the photopic cone system with light
adaptation can be easily demonstrated (e.g. Kelly, 1961). Thus, our results point to
a major difference in the sensitivity regulation of the scotopic and photopic visual
systems.
Changes in spatial integration with light adaptation
Our finding of a change in spatial summation as the human rod visual system light
adapts accords with the findings of many other workers (e.g. Bouman, 1950; van den
Brink & Bouman, 1954; Barlow, 1958; Lennie, 1979). Parallel changes have been
found for extra-foveal thresholds measured with 2-7 min of arc and 7 deg diameter
343
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
fields during the rod-mediated portion of the dark-adaptation curve; that is, the rate
of dark adaptation was found to increase with the size of the target (Arden & Weale,
1954).
In the achromat, other TVI measurements also suggest a change in spatial
integration with light adaptation. Blakemore & Rushton (1965) measured increment
threshold in a rod-monochromat with 1 s duration, achromatic flashes of large and
small target diameter. They found that the curve measured with a small (5 min of
arc) diameter flash rose with a slope of 0-59, whereas that measured with a large
(6 deg) diameter flash rose with a slope of 0-87. Stabell et al. (1987) measured TVI
curves in the same achromat as used in this study with a 520 nm, 125 ms flash of
varying diameter. An examination of their fig. 2 reveals a change in slope between
the thresholds measured with a small (0-2 deg) and large (1 x 2 deg) diameter target.
Table 3 lists how the slope of achromat K. N.'s increment threshold curves depends
upon target size and duration. It summarizes the data obtained here and in earlier
publications. Clearly, the effect of reducing the size of the target has a greater effect
on the slope than reducing the duration of the target.
Other explanations of the steepening of rod-mediated TVI curves with target size
A local adaptation-dependent non-linearity
Chen et al. (1987) have argued that the change in the slopes of TVI curves with
target size can be explained by strictly local processes. As an illustration of their
model, consider the targets used in the experiments reported here: the small, 10 min
of arc target and the large, 6 deg target differ not only in size, but also in the
luminance required to detect them. Thus, locally, the 10 min of arc target produces
a much larger response at threshold than the 6 deg target. If light adaptation
increases the steepness of the function relating stimulus intensity to local response
(the local input-output function), the threshold for the larger target will rise more
steeply with adaptation than the threshold for the smaller one, without any need to
invoke changes in spatial integration (Chen et al. 1987; see their Fig. 1). The
experiments of Chen et al., which were directed mainly at cone vision, showed that
there was little change in spatial integration with light adaptation. Sharpe et al.
(1993) have recently repeated the experiments of Chen et al. in the achromat
observer, K. N. Their results suggest that there is a change in spatial integration in
the rod visual system during light adaptation, over and above that due to local
changes, but that it is small.
Spatial filters
Another explanation of the change in the slopes of TVI curves with target size is
one based on the model of the early visual system as several channels sensitive to
different spatial frequencies (e.g. Campbell & Robson, 1968), each with independent
gain controls (e.g. Enroth-Cugell & Shapley, 1973). The TVI curves for the large,
6 deg target could be steeper than the TVI curves for the small, 10 min of arc target,
if the high spatial frequency channels detecting the 10 min of arc target suffer less
desensitization in bright light than do the low spatial frequency channels detecting
the 6 deg target (Enroth-Cugell & Shapley, 1973; Hess, 1990). This argument is
complicated by the fact that our sharply focused, 6 deg target is spatially broadband,
12-2
344
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
and so might be detected by channels tuned to low spatial frequencies at low
intensities and high spatial frequencies at high intensities. It remains true, however,
that the multichannel model is essentially inconsistent with the results of Chen et al.
(1987), and is difficult to reconcile with the simple reciprocity of stimulus intensity
and target size found for small target sizes (though, see Hess, 1990, Fig. 1.8).
The cone influence on long-wavelength fields
So far, we have discussed the isolated rod system. In the normal observer,
however, it has been shown by several workers that the cones can raise rod threshold
(see above). A secondary aim of this work was to determine how the cone influence
on rod threshold depends on target size and duration.
This issue has also been considered by Makous & Peeples (1979), who measured
the cone elevation of the rod detection threshold for 20 ms duration targets of
various diameters ranging from 2 min of arc to 1 deg. They found no systematic
change in the size of the rod-cone interaction with target size. However, they also
measured the cone elevation for rod detected, 200 ms targets of 1 deg diameter or
1 deg square, and found that the rod-cone interaction was slightly larger for these
larger and longer targets than for any of the 20 ms targets (0i38 vs. 0-29 log10 unit).
Like Makous & Peeples (1979), we find that the increase in rod increment threshold
on long-wavelength backgrounds is greater than on rod-equated short- and middlewavelength backgrounds - irrespective of the target size and/or duration. The
difference in slope between the TVI curves for ,u < 560, and those for ,u = 640 nm
ranges from 0-04 to 0-18. There seems to be no particular pattern to these results,
except that, consistent with the findings of Makous & Peeples (1979), - the increase
in slope for the TVI curves obtained with the 6 deg, 200 ms target is greater than
that for the TVI curves obtained with the other targets. This suggests that there
is a spatio-temporal interaction when cones influence rod threshold. Such interactions
have been discussed by Barlow (1958), who attributes them to a sluggish lateral
inhibition in the retina, which must be greater for long than for brief targets.
Perhaps, then, cone adaptation raises rod threshold by introducing a sluggish lateral
inhibition.
The limits of 'complete' spatial and temporal summation
As the target is made larger and longer, more quanta are required to detect it at
absolute threshold (see Table 1 and Results above). This has been observed by many
workers (see Hecht et al. 1942), and can be explained by the inefficiency of targets
whose size or duration extends beyond the spatial and temporal limits within which
there is a reciprocity between stimulus area and intensity (Ricco, 1887) and between
stimulus duration and intensity (Bloch, 1885). At absolute threshold in the
peripheral retina, the spatial limit of complete summation is typically assumed to be
between 30 min of arc and 2 deg in diameter, and the temporal limit of complete
summation is assumed to be about 100 ms. But estimates of complete spatial and
temporal summation vary widely and depend upon stimulus configuration and
retinal eccentricity (for a review, see Sharpe, 1990).
We can estimate the upper limits of spatial and temporal summation implied by
our TVI data by assuming an abrupt transition from complete summation to no
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
345
summation. In the dark-adapted eye, the upper limits estimated from our data are
58, 117 and 98 ms for U.M., C.F. and K.N., respectively, for temporal summation,
and 3 4, 2-6 and 2-1 deg for spatial summation.
These estimates ignore the fact that beyond the limits of complete spatial and
temporal summation there is a region of partial summation (AI = kl/An and
AI = k2/T , 0 < n < 1; where AI represents detection threshold, A the target area,
T the target duration, and k, and k2 are constants; Ricco's and Bloch's laws
correspond to n = 1) (e.g. Barlow 1958).
Cohn (1990) has recently reiterated that if quantum fluctuations limit incremental
detection, complete summation does not correspond to Ricco's law or Bloch's laws,
but instead to the range of sizes or durations for which AI = k1/AO'5 or AI = k2/T0 5
(see also Piper, 1903; van der Velden, 1944; Barlow, 1964). According to this model,
Ricco's and Bloch's law correspond to sizes and durations where the stimuli are
detected less efficiently due to the counting of too many quanta from the background
(Barlow, 1964). This argument, however, is valid only if detection is actually limited
by quantum fluctuations. As Cohn points out (Cohn, 1990, p. 380), the fact that the
three square-root laws of temporal summation, spatial summation, and background
intensity (the de Vries-Rose law) do not co-exist in Barlow's (1958) data (see Cohn,
1990, his Fig. 33.2) suggests that detection is not limited by quantum fluctuations.
Dark light, Io
The so called 'dark light' has been attributed to the spontaneous thermal
activations of rhodopsin molecules, which are assumed to be indistinguishable from
photo-elicited events (e.g. Barlow, 1956, 1957, 1958; Baylor et al. 1984; Aho, Donner,
Hyden, Reuter & Orlov, 1987). Baylor et al. (1984) have determined that the thermal
isomerization rate in individual monkey rods is about once every 160 s, which
corresponds to 390 quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-2 being presented to the cornea (for a
discussion of this point, see Sharpe, 1990; and for a discussion of dark light in cat
retinal ganglion cells, see Shapley & Enroth-Cugell, 1984). In support of this
explanation, a firm link has been established between the rate of thermal
isomerizations and the performance limit of visually guided behaviour in the darkadapted frog and toad (Aho et al. 1987) and, in the human, it has been observed that
the absolute threshold rises with body temperature, roughly according to the
relationship between thermal isomerization rate and temperature (Fach & Sharpe,
1990).
Estimates of dark light are roughly constant within an observer, but, as is already
well known, vary considerably between observers. Barlow (1957) found estimates in
the literature differing by a factor of 80, from 200 to 16000 quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-2
at the cornea (see his Table 1). Because some of the larger estimates were imprecise
he concluded that the true variance is closer to a factor of 16 and that a reasonable
average value is about 1000 quanta (507 nm) s-' deg-2 (-2-66 logl0 scotopic td). This
corresponds to about one photoisomerization every 62 s, according to the conversion
factors given in Sharpe (1990, pp. 59-60). Other studies give values of 200-1300
quanta (506 nm) s-1 deg-2 or about one photoisomerization every 48-313 s (Aguilar
& Stiles, 1954), 400 quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-' of about one photoisomerization every
156 s (Hallett, 1969), and 50-200 quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-2 or one photoisomer-
346
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
ization every 312-1250 s (Sakitt, 1972). The range in our observers is from 676
(K.N.) to 120 (U.M.) quanta (507 nm) s-1 deg-2 at the cornea; corresponding to a
photoisomerization rate of from once every 92 s to once every 521 s per rod.
The analysis of our TVI data suggests that target size and duration do not
significantly affect estimates of the intrinsic dark light of the eye (see Table 1). But,
since there is little or no change in temporal integration with light adaptation, we
should expect Io to be independent of target duration - as indeed it is. The finding
that Io is independent of target size is more unexpected. There are several reasons
why this might be so. One possibility is that there may be no change in spatial
summation at light intensities equivalent to the dark light levels. Another is that the
source of the dark light noise may originate at a stage beyond the early stages of
spatial integration.
Physiological evidence
Our psychophysical data show that the slopes of rod-mediated TVI curves depend
more on changes in target area than on changes in target duration. Comparable TVI
curves have been derived from cat retinal ganglion cell recordings by plotting the
amplitude of the cell response divided by the stimulus strength (i.e. the retinal
ganglion cell gain) versus the background level.
In the scotopic range, the relation of the slope to target parameters follows the
same pattern as that observed psychophysically. Thus, Barlow & Levick (1976)
report that, for large-area (usually 4-6 deg), long-duration (0-32-1-26 s) targets,
slopes of single units average 0-82 and approach 1-0 in the steepest cases, whereas for
small (10 min of arc), long-duration (1 s) targets the slopes average 0-58 and for small
(less than the receptive field centre size), brief (10 ms) targets, the slopes average
about 0-53. Likewise, Lennie (1979) reports that the slopes for large stimuli covering
the whole receptor field are steeper (1-2-1-4) than those for small stimuli falling
within the centre of the receptive field (0 65).
There are several noteworthy aspects of these findings. First, the slopes found by
Barlow & Levick in the cat conform surprisingly well to those given in Table 2 for,
for example, achromat K. N.: 0-77 + 0-03 for the large, long test flash; 0-56 + 0 04 for
the small, long one; and 0-57 + 0-04 for the small, brief one. The slopes found by
Lennie in the cat, however, are steeper than those measured psychophysically in
K. N. The significance of this difference is complicated by the fact that the method
of determining threshold for a ganglion cell influences the shape of the increment
threshold curve. For instance, the slope of the increment threshold curve can be
reduced by optimizing the analysis period (see Lennie, 1979).
Second, the results of both Barlow & Levick (1976) and Lennie (1979) indicate that
changes in target duration have less effect on the ganglion cell thresholds than
changes in target area (though curiously neither reported this). This accords with the
psychophysical thresholds presented above and suggests that changes in spatial
summation of cells are more important than changes in temporal summation.
Changes in spatial summation with light adaptation have been directly measured in
the excitatory receptive field centre of cat retinal ganglion cells and have been shown
to be between 32 and 50 % over a 4-5 to 5 0 log10 unit range (Enroth-Cugell & Robson,
1966; Derrington & Lennie, 1982).
CHANGES IN SUMMATION IN HUMAN ROD VISION
347
This research was supported by the Alexander von Humboldt-Stiftung, Bonn-Bad Godesberg,
by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (SFB 325, B4 & B13 and Heisenberg-Programme),
Bonn-Bad Godesberg, and by the National Science Foundation (BNS 88-12401). The visit of
K. N. was made possible by funds received from the Research Division of the Norwegian
Telecommunication Administration. We thank Drs D. I. A. MacLeod and P. Whittle for advice
and comments.
REFERENCES
AGUILAR, M. & STILES, W. S. (1954). Saturation of the rod mechanism at high levels of stimulation.
Optica Acta 1, 59-65.
AHo, A.-C., DONNER, K., HYDEN, C., REUTER, T. & ORLOV, 0. Y. (1987). Retinal noise, the
performance of retinal ganglion cells and visual sensitivity in the dark-adapted frog. Journal of
the Optical Society of America A4, 2321-2329.
ARDEN, G. B. & WEALE, R. A. (1954). Nervous mechanism and dark adaptation. Journal of
Physiology 125, 417-426.
BARLOW, H. B. (1956). Retinal noise and absolute threshold. Journal of the Optical Society of
America 46, 634-639.
BARLOW, H. B. (1957). Increment thresholds at low intensities considered as signal/noise
discriminations. Journal of Physiology 136, 469-488.
BARLOW, H. B. (1958). Temporal and spatial summation in human vision at different background
intensities. Journal of Physiology 141, 337-350.
BARLOW, H. B. (1964). The physical limits of visual discrimination. In Photophysiology, vol. II, ed.
GIESE, A. C., pp. 163-202. Academic Press, New York.
BARLOW, H. B. & LEVICK, W. R. (1976). Threshold settings by the surround of cat retinal ganglion
cells. Journal of Physiology 259, 737-757.
BAYLOR, D. A., NUNN, B. J. & SCHNAPF, J. L. (1984). The photocurrent, noise and spectral
sensitivity of rods of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Journal of Physiology 357, 575-607.
BLAKEMORE, C. B. & RUSHTON, W. A. H. (1965). Dark adaptation and increment threshold in a rod
monochromat. Journal of Physiology 181, 612-628.
BLOCH, A. M. (1885). Experiences sur la vision. Societe Biologie Memoir (Paris) 37, 493-495.
BOUMAN, M. A. (1950). Peripheral contrast thresholds of the human eye. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 40, 825-832.
CAMPBELL, F. W. & ROBSON, J. G. (1968). Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of
gratings. Journal of Physiology 197, 551-566.
CHEN, B., MACLEOD, D. I. A. & STOCKMAN, A. (1987). Improvement in human vision under bright
light: grain or gain? Journal of Physiology 394, 41-66.
COHN, T. E. (1990). Spatial and temporal summation in human vision. In Vision: Coding and
Efficiency, ed. BLAKEMORE, C., chap. 33, pp. 376-385. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge.
DAW, N. W., JENSEN, R. J. & BRIJNKEN, W. J. (1990). Rod pathways in mammalian retinae.
Trends in Neurosciences 13, 110-115.
DERRINGTON, A. M. & LENNIE, P. (1982). The influence of temporal frequency and adaptation level
on receptive field organization of retinal ganglion cells in cat. Journal of Physiology 333, 343-366.
ENROTH-CUGELL, C. & ROBSON, J. G. (1966). The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells of the
cat. Journal of Physiology 187, 517-522.
ENROTH-CUGELL, C. & SHAPLEY, R. M. (1973). Flux, not retinal illumination, is what cat retinal
ganglion cells really care about. Journal of Physiology 233, 311-326.
FACH, C. C. & SHARPE, L. T. (1990). Absolute threshold and body temperature. Investigative
Ophthalmology and Visual Science 31, suppl. 413.
GRAHAM, C. H. & KEMP, E. H. (1938). Brightness discrimination as a function of the duration of
the increment in intensity. Journal of General Physiology 21, 635-650.
HALLETT, P. E. (1969). Quantum efficiency and false positive rate. Journal of Physiology 202,
421-436.
HALLETT, P. E. (1971). Rapid changes and hysteresis in spatial integration for human rod vision.
Journal of Physiology 215, 433-447.
HECHT, S., SHLAER, S. & PIRENNE, M. H. (1942). Energy, quanta and vision. Journal of General
Physiology 25, 819-840.
348
L. T. SHARPE AND OTHERS
HESS, R. F. (1990). Rod-mediated vision: role of post-receptoral filters. In Night Vision: Basic,
Clinical and Applied Aspects, ed. HESS, R. F., SHARPE, L. T. & NORDBY, K., chap. 1, pp. 3-48.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
KELLY, D. H. (1961). Visual responses to time-dependent stimuli. I. Amplitude sensitivity
measurements. Journal of the Optical Society of America 51, 422-429.
LENNIE, P. (1979). Scotopic increment thresholds in retinal ganglion cells. Vision Research 19,
425-430.
MAKOUS, W. L. & BOOTHE, R. (1974). Cones block signals from rods. Vision Research 14, 285-294.
MAKOUS, W. L. & PEEPLES, D. R. (1979). Rod-cone interaction: reconciliation with Flamant and
Stiles. Vision Research 19, 695-698.
PIPER, H. (1903). Uber die Abhiingigkeit des Reizwertes leuchtender Objekte von ihre Flachenbezw. Winkelgrasse. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 32, 98-112.
Ricco, A. (1877). Relazione fra il minimo angolo visual e l'intensita luminosa. Memorie della Regia
Accademia di Scienze, lettere ed arti in Modena 17, 47-160.
SAKITT, B. (1972). Counting every quantum. Journal of Physiology 233, 131-150.
SCHNAPF, J. L., NUNN, B. J., MEISTER, M. & BAYLOR, D. A. (1990). Visual transduction in cones
of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. Journal of Physiology 427, 681-713.
SHAPLEY, R. M. & ENROTH-CUGELL, C. (1984). Visual adaptation and retinal gain control. In
Progress in Retinal Research, vol. 3, ed. OSBORNE, N. 0. & CHANDLER, G. J., chap. 9, pp. 263-346.
Pergamon Press, Oxford.
SHARPE L. T. (1990). The light-adaptation of the human rod visual system. In Night Vision: Basic,
Clinical and Applied Aspects, ed. HESS, R. F., SHARPE, L. T. & NORDBY, K., pp. 49-124.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
SHARPE, L. T., FACH, C. C., NORDBY, K. & STOCKMAN, A. (1989 a). The incremental threshold of the
rod visual system and Weber's law. Science 244, 354-356.
SHARPE, L. T., FACH, C. C. & STOCKMAN, A. (1992). The field adaptation of the human rod visual
system. Journal of Physiology 445, 319-343.
SHARPE, L. T. & NORDBY, K. (1990). The photoreceptors in the achromat. In Night Vision: Basic,
Clinical and Applied Aspects, ed. HESS, R. F., SHARPE, L. T. & NORDBY, K., pp. 335-389.
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
SHARPE, L. T., STOCKMAN, A. & MACLEOD, D. I. A. (1989b). Rod flicker perception: scotopic
duality, phase lags and destructive interference. Vision Research 29, 1539-1559.
SHARPE, L. T., WHITTLE, P. & NORDBY, K. (1993). Spatial integration and sensitivity changes in
the human rod visual system. Journal of Physiology 461, 235-246.
STABELL, B., NORDBY, K. & STABELL, U. (1987). Light-adaptation of the human rod system.
Clinical Vision Sciences 2, 83-91.
STILES, W. S. (1939). The directional sensitivity of the retina and the spectral sensitivities of the
rods and cones. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 127, 64-105.
STILES, W. S. & CRAWFORD, B. H. (1934). The liminal brightness increment for white light for
different conditions of the foveal and parafoveal retina. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 116,
55-102.
STOCKMAN, A., SHARPE, L. T., ZRENNER, E. & NORDBY, K. (1991). Slow and fast pathways in the
human rod visual system: electrophysiology and psychophysics. Journal of the Optical Society of
America A 8, 1657-1665.
VAN DEN BRINK, G. & BOUMAN, M. A. (1954). Variation of integrative actions in the retinal system:
an adaptational phenomenon. Journal of the Optical Society of America 44, 616-620.
VAN DER VELDEN, H. (1944). Over het antaal lichtquata dat nodig es for en licht prikel bij dat
menselyk oog. Physica 11, 179-189.