15th ICPhS Barcelona
Prosodic typology and compound – phrasal contrasts
John Ingram† and Thu Nguyen†
† University of Queensland, Australia
E-mail:
[email protected],
[email protected]
ABSTRACT
stress, and phrase boundary marking: i. the locus and
duration of any junctural pauses, ii. Pre-pausal lengthening,
and iii. mid-vowel f0 measurements.
This study investigates pitch (f0) and timing cues that
distinguish compound words and phrases in left and right
branching structures in three typologically different
languages, English: a stress-accent language, stress timed,
Japanese: a pitch-accent language, mora timed and
Vietnamese: a tone language, syllable timed.
1.
2.
ENGLISH
The English data set comprised 7 triplets involving
contrasts between left and right- branching structures in
Phrasal compounds and left-branching embedded noun
phrases:
INTRODUCTION
In English, compounds are distinguished from phrasal
constructions, by a contrasting pattern of pitch prominence
and by duration characteristics that appear to have their
origin in the fact that compounds are required to conform to
word prosody. i.e.: licensing of a single accent-bearing
syllable, and being subject to temporal compression effects
associated with affixation in polysyllabic word forms
(Lehiste, 1972). The phonological features which
distinguish compounds from phrases are closely related to
prosodic cues that (optionally) permit speakers to resolve
‘attachment ambiguities’, which are traditionally termed
‘juncture’.
1a. A black-bird cage
[[adj - n]CP n]NP
1b. A black bird-cage
[adj [n - n]CP]NP
1c. A black bird cage
[[adj n]NP n]NP
Each triplet was elicited three times in separate readings of
the stimulus set by two readers, resulting in 126 (7 x 3 x 3 x
2) items for acoustic analysis. , Both readers were native
speakers of Australian English, a (the author), and a
Linguist with professional voice training and acting
experience.
2.1 ENGLISH RESULTS
Juncture is variously realized phonetically in different
languages by boundary accent marking, pause breaks,
pre-boundary lengthening, and fundamental frequency
re-setting. We assume that junctural disambiguation of
structural ambiguity is universal in phrase level phonology,
though phonetically implemented in various ways
depending on a language’s prosodic and syntactic type (e.g.:
left versus right branching). We postulate that not all
languages possess independent prosodic means of
distinguishing phrases from compounds (words). In this
paper we undertake an acoustic analysis of prosodic
features that distinguish compounds and phrases, and
which resolve structural ambiguities in noun phrases in
three typologically different languages: English (a
stress-accent language, with stress timing), Japanese (a
pitch-accent language, with moraic timing) and Vietnamese
(a tonal language with syllable timing).
As expected, word or syllable duration and peak vowel
fundamental frequency, normalized for individual speech
rate and intrinsic f0 were effective in classifying the
utterances into the three target stress groups. The duration
differences are shown in Fig.1, where the timing patterns of
the left and right branching phrasal compounds are
contrasted with the left branching (pre-modified) noun
phrase.
A common methodology was used in each of the three
experiments reported here. Six native speakers (2 for each
language) read pairs of phrases spoken in ‘the careful but
natural style of a newsreader which contrasted left versus
right branching readings of a pair of noun phrases, or
phrasal versus compound construction. The readers all had
considerable experience in recording items for language
teaching or phonetic experiments. Multiple tokens (3) of
the contrasts were recorded, digitized, phonetically
annotated, and analyzed for the temporal and voice pitch (f0)
parameters known to be effective acoustic cues to linguistic
Figure 1. Word duration in English phrasal compounds
Duration was a more effective cue than f0 for identifying
stress patterns. A linear discriminant function analysis
successfully classified 82.5% of tokens into one of the three
target categories on the basis of rate-normalized first and
second word duration measurements (w1 and w2 in Fig.1
above.) The successful classification rate rose to 91.2%
when peak vowel f0 measurements for w1 and w2 were
included in the discriminant function.
479
ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB
15th ICPhS Barcelona
compound (as in 3a). Comparison of the acoustic cues
associated with left and right-branching readings in the two
types of minimal prosodic pairs (2a-b vs. 3a-b) should
inform us as to whether Vietnamese registers an
independent distinction between compound and phrasal
constructions.
Temporal compression associated with the compound
constituents is clearly evident from Fig.1. Notably, the
accent-bearing constituent of the compound word
undergoes more temporal compression than the unaccented
constituent, when compared with their respective
counterparts in the full noun phrase. This word shortening
effect, first reported by Lehiste (1972) has been observed to
operate in attenuated form in discourse reading (Farnetani
and Cosi, 1988). However, the precise mechanism of this
temporal compensation in compound words remains
unclear and merits further study.
A series of two-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) was
undertaken, with Phrase type (2 levels: [NP-NP] vs.
[NP-CP]) and Direction of branching (2 levels: Left vs.
Right) as independent variables. The dependent variables
used in these analyses were, by turn: juncture duration,
syllable duration, and f0. For syllable duration and f0,
separate ANOVAS were undertaken at each position in the
phrase, with positions 1 and 2 critical for pre-pausal
lengthening effects in right and left branching respectively.
The role of juncture in prosodic disambiguation was
marginal in the English data. Were it operating, pre-pausal
lengthening would be expected on W1 in right-branching
constructions or on W2 in left-branching constructions. No
evidence of this was found. However, what we interpret as
potential junctural breaks (silent periods > 50 m.sec)
occurred between W1 and W2 on a significant minority
(15/42 = 36%) of right-branching compound phrases (1b.
above). Although English strongly favors right- branching
at clause level syntax, it is left branching in its phrase
structure.
3.
3.1 Results - Vietnamese data
Juncture: The direction of branching was clearly marked
by a junctural pause at the anticipated location (after
syllable 1 for right-branching structures and syllable 2 for
left branching structures) in all but one or two items. The
break was significantly longer (p < 0.0001) in
left-branching than in right branching structures (fig. 2.).
Vietnamese is right branching in its NP structures. Left
branching phrase structure may be regarded as the marked
case.
VIETNAMESE
Vietnamese is a highly analytical language, with a
one-for-one correspondence between syllables and
morphemes and a virtual absence of inflectional
morphology. Compounding is the principle word-building
morphological process of which there are several subtypes.
In noun phrases, head nouns are right-modified by
adjectives or stative verbs. Thus, noun phrases in
Vietnamese are basically right-branching. Vietnamese is a
tone language, with 5 or 6 tones depending on regional
dialect. It has no system of lexical or word stress.
Pre-boundary lengthening: Two way ANOVAS, with
Phrase type (NP-NP vs NP-CP) and Direction of
embedding (Right - Left) as independent variables and
syllable duration as the dependent variable were conducted
at syllable positions one, two and three in the contrasting
pairs of phrases. For all three ANOVAS, there was no effect
of Phrase type, or any significant interactions between
Phrase type and Direction of embedding. However, there
was a highly significant main effect of Direction of
embedding upon the duration of the first and second
syllables, as shown in Fig. 2. Lengthening occurs on the
syllable immediately preceding the juncture break. Syllable
3 was subject to phrase final lengthening in all conditions.
Three word noun phrases (illustrated in 2a-b below) may be
constructed in Vietnamese, which have distinct right or
left-branching readings, which may be disambiguated with
prosody though they are usually not, with speakers relying
on context.
2a. Bao [goái vaøng]
[Noun [Noun Adj.]NP]NP
2b. [Bao goái] vaøng
[[Noun Noun]NP Adj.]NP
case pillow yellow
While 2b above may appear to contain a [noun - noun]
compound, such forms are regarded as NPs by Vietnamese
grammarians. They are semantically compositional and
therefore phrase like, quite distinct from the
non-compositional compound form in 3a (meaning: rose):
3a. Loï [hoa hoàng]
[Noun [Noun Adj.]CP]NP
3b. [Loï hoa] hoàng
[[Noun Noun]NP Adj.]NP
Figure 2. Vietnamese: pre-boundary lengthening and
direction of branching.
vase flower pink
Thus, Vietnamese grammar permitted us to construct pairs
of left and right-branching noun phrases containing an
embedded NP (as in 2a-b), and to contrast the prosodic
behavior of such pairs with phrasal constructions
containing the same left versus right branching ambiguity,
but where one member of the pair is clearly a phrasal
ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB
Fundamental frequency (f0): Three 2-way ANOVAS were
conducted, with Phrase type and Direction of embedding as
the treatment conditions and f0 of the first, second and third
syllable as the dependent variable in successive analyses. A
single significant main effect (p < .003) for phrase type was
observed for the first syllable position. No significant
480
15th ICPhS Barcelona
interactions were found. The most likely cause of this effect
was nothing to do with prosodic cues to constituent
structure but simply the fact that the first syllable in the
NP-NP set has a higher proportion of rising or +high tones
than the NP-CP set.
3.2 Conclusions: Vietnamese data
Left and right-branching readings of Vietnamese 3 word
phrases seem to be phonetically realized by the same
prosodic device of juncture assignment in the absence of
contextual cues and under elicitation conditions that
highlight the semantic contrast. The lack of any significant
interaction effects between Phrase type and Direction of
branching indicates that Vietnamese has no prosodic device
for marking the word-like status of compounds or for
differentiating them from phrasal constructions.
Figure 3. Downstep within a phrase in Japanese
4. JAPANESE
In contrast to Vietnamese, Japanese permits a more
clear-cut comparison of prosodic structures at the word and
phrase levels. It is possible to construct minimal prosodic
pairs at both word and phrase levels:
Japanese
English
4a. [nurí gasa] ire
[lacquered umbrella]case
4b. nuri [kasá ire]
lacquered [umbrella case]
5a. óokina [nooen-no óonaa] a big [farm owner]
5b. [óokina nooen-no] óonaa a [big farm] owner
Figure 4. F0 resetting across a phrase boundary
Examples (4a-b) above are compound expressions and
(5a-b) phrasal constructions in Japanese. The application of
the voicing assimilation rule rendaku, which only applies
word-internally, clearly signals the compound status of
(4a-b) and the inflectional suffix (genitive case) on nooen
‘farm' signals the phrasal status of (5a-b). The left and right
branching readings of (4a-b) are disambiguated by pitch
accent in careful reading (Tokyo dialect). The left and right
branching readings of the phrasal constructions (5a-b) are
also prosodically disambiguated in careful speech
(Kubuzzono, 1992), but by different cues than in the case of
the compounds.
Results Japanese data
Juncture breaks: Very few juncture breaks were observed
in compound expressions and in phrases juncture breaks
were only consistently found in right-branching structures
(Table 1).
Compounds
Phrases
Left branching
.04
.23
Right branching
.14
.95
Table 1. Proportion of juncture breaks at constituent
boundaries in Japanese.
It was expected that fundamental frequency changes would
play a more prominent role in phrase boundary marking in
Japanese than in Vietnamese, through the downstep of
accentual prominence within the phrase, and fundamental
frequency re-setting which occurs between phrases at
phrase boundaries. Downstep is a mechanism whereby the
pitch register for marking accentual prominences is
lowered with each successive occurrence of a pitch accent
within a phrase. At the end of the phrase, the pitch register
is re-set for the next phrase, producing a ‘sawtooth’ pattern
of intonation contours. The re-setting of pitch register at
phrase boundaries may involve an extra increment of f0
raising, which Kubozono (1989) termed a ‘metrical boost’.
Downstep within a phrase and F0 resetting across a phrase
boundary (with some ‘metrical boost’) is illustrated in
figures 3 and 4 below.
Pre-boundary Lengthening: Three two-way ANOVAS
were conducted with Phrase type and Direction of
branching as the independent variables and the duration of
the first, second and final words in the compound/phrase as
successive dependent variables. There was a significant
main effect for Direction of branching for the first word
only. There were no significant interaction effects. The
main effect for Phrase type simply reflected the differing
number of syllables that made up the words in the
compound and phrasal expressions. It is apparent from
Figure 5 that the greater length of the first word can be
attributed to a pre-pausal lengthening effect in a
right-branching structure. This lengthening effect on the
first syllable of a right branching structure was found in
both compound and phrasal expressions, in spite of the fact
that few junctural breaks were observed in compound
expressions.
481
ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB
15th ICPhS Barcelona
is consistent with the occurrence of a phrase boundary
between S2 and S3. The peak at S4 in the case of the right
branching items is consistent with phrase boundary f0
re-setting. (One might expect to see a peak at S3 were it not
for the fact that Japanese has a rule of ‘initial lowering’ that
lowers f0 on the first syllable of a phrase.) The minor pitch
peak at S4 for the left branching items is attributable to a
down-stepped accent.
Figure 5. Word duration and direction of branching in
Japanese
To further investigate the mechanism of the word
lengthening effect, we examined patterns of syllable
duration, in particular, lengthening on the final and the
penultimate syllables of a word preceding a potential
juncture. (i.e.: the first word in right-branching phrase,
compared with the first word in the left-branching phrase).
The lengthening effect was found to be confined to the final
syllable of the word before the potential juncture break
(Figure 6).
Figure 7. F0 resetting and direction of branching.
5.
Figure 6. Syllable lengthening before a potential juncture
break in Japanese.
Fundamental frequency (f0): To assess effects of downstep
within phrase boundaries and pitch register re-setting
across phrase boundaries, mid vowel fundamental
frequency measurements were taken on the two syllables
that preceded and followed the point of left branching and
the point of right branching (i.e.: f0 measures on the
penultimate and final syllables and the first two syllables
following an internal phrase boundary). Two separate series
of 2-way ANOVAS were conducted, one on the compound
expressions and another on the phrasal constructions. The
two factors or independent variables were Items (7 levels)
and the Direction of branching (2 levels) and the dependent
variable was the f0 of syllables at the relevant position in the
phrase.
REFERENCES
[1] Farentani, E and Cosi, P. (1988) English compound
versus non-compound noun phrases in discourse: An
acoustic and perceptual study. Language and speech,
31, 157-180.
[2] Kubuzono H. (1992) Modeling syntactic effects on
downstep in Japanese. In G. J. Docherty & D. R. Ladd
(eds.), Papers in Laboratory Phonology II: Gesture,
Segment, Prosody. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press, 368-387.
As expected, there were no systematic differences
attributable to the Direction of branching in the compound
expressions. However, there was a highly significant main
effect of Direction of branching in the f0 values at all but
one position in the phrasal constructions. The results (Fig. 7)
show the effects of f0 resetting across the internal phrase
boundary in the right branching condition and a
corresponding absence of f0 resetting in the left branching
condition. Both graphs have the same form but the f0
changes with syllable position are more clearly marked for
the right- branching items and the pattern of pitch changes
ISBN 1-876346-48-5 © 2003 UAB
CONCLUSIONS
In speaking conditions that encourage maximization of
perceptual (phonological) contrasts, Vietnamese, Japanese,
and English speakers have access to a universal juncture
marking strategy for phrase structure disambiguation. In all
three languages, juncture is preferentially used to signal
departures from the unmarked direction of phrase
branching. Pre-pausal lengthening appears to be the most
reliable acoustic expression of juncture. Compounds are
distinguished from phrases in Japanese and English by
de-accenting. Vietnamese appears to lack any distinctive
word prosody for compound formation.
[3] Lehiste I. (1972) The timing of utterances and
linguistic boundaries Journal of the Acoustical Society
of America 51(6-2) 2018-2024.
482