DOCUMENT RESUME
ED 438 563
AUTHOR
TITLE
PUB DATE
NOTE
PUB TYPE
EDRS PRICE
DESCRIPTORS
CS 217 023
Rios, Daisy M.
Gender Differences in Spelling Achievement.
2000-05-00
37p.; M.A. Research Project, Kean University. Colored bar
graphs in the appendixes may not reproduce well.
Dissertations/Theses (040)
MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage.
Inner City; Postsecondary Education; *Reading Attitudes;
*Sex Differences; *Spelling; Vocational Education
ABSTRACT
In this study, gender differences in spelling achievement
were investigated for young adults (ages 17 to 21). Performances of males and
females on standardized and written spelling tests were compared. All of the
40 students (20 males and 20 females) are from a low-economic, inner city,
vocational program. The students were also given a questionnaire to determine
their reading attitudes. The females, on the whole, performed better on both
tests, but the difference was not significant. Their attitudes towards
reading were consistent with their spelling scores. Appendices contain 7
colored bar graphs of data. (Contains 16 references and 2 tables of data).
(Author/RS)
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made
from the original document.
GENDER DIFFERENCES
IN SPELLING
ACHIEVEMENT
By
Daisy M. Rios
Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the
Degree of Master of Arts
KEAN UNIVERSITY
May, 2000
U.S. DEPARTMENT
Office of Educational
EDUCATIONAL
VI
Cl
0
s-,
Cs1
OF EDUCATION
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS
BEEN GRANTED BY
Research and Improvement
RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER (EIC)
R
This document has been
reproduced as
received from the
person or organization
originating it.
Minor changes
have been made to
improve reproduction
quality.
CID
Points of view
or opinions stated in this
document do not
necessarily represent
official OERI
position or policy.
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
2
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
1
Abstract
In this study, gender differences in spelling achievement were investigated
for young adults (ages 17
21). Performances of males and females on standardized and
written spelling tests were compared. All of the 40 students (20 males and 20 females),
are from low- economic, inner city, vocational program. The students were also given a
questionnaire, to determine their reading attitudes. The females, on the whole performed
better on both tests, but their difference was NOT significant. Their attitudes towards
reading was consistent with their spelling scores.
ii
.
3
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank Dr. Albert Mazurkiewicz for his helpful
guidance and patience. I also wish to express my gratitude to my husband,
Robert, for his patience and encouragement in my endeavors. Special thanks
to Vocational Foundation, Inc. for aiding me in my research, especially,
Anthony Flores, whose help on the charts and tables was immeasurable.
iii
4
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page number
I.
Abstract
ii
II.
Acknowledgments
iii
III.
List of Tables
v
IV.
Gender Differences in Spelling Achievements
Introduction
1
Hypothesis
4
Procedures
4
Results
6
Conclusions
8
V.
Related Literature
9
VI.
References
20
VII. Appendices
23
Appendix I: Individual Results of the Written Spelling Test
Appendix II: Results of the Individual Words on the Written Spelling
Test
Appendix III: Statistical Results of the Written Spelling Test
Appendix IV: Individual Results of the Standardized Spelling Test
Appendix V: Statistical Results of the Standardized Spelling Test
Appendix VI: Reading as Masculine or Feminine
Appendix VII: Writing as Masculine or Feminine
iv
5
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations and t of the Written
Spelling Test
Table 2
Means Standard Deviations and t of the
Standardized Spelling Test
V
6
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
Research indicates that girls read better than boys as a whole and, because of the
relationship between reading and spelling, one might assume that girls would also
perform better in spelling than boys (Allred, 1990). Are better readers automatically
better spellers? Poor spellers are not less intelligent, less educated or less motivated than
better spellers are (Gentry & Gillet, 1993). Still many people interpret poor spelling as a
sign of poor education (Mannings, 1986).
A basic difference exists between reading and spelling; reading is a decoding
process. Readers are expected to say what they see on the printed page. Spelling on the
other hand, is an encoding process. Spellers are required to write what they hear or think.
Although both reading and spelling are important aspects of literacy, their processes are
different. Although a high correlation exists between the two subjects, one may find good
spellers who are not good readers. In addition, good readers does not guarantee good
spellers (Allred, 1990).
An individual's skill in the mechanics of written expressions are assumed to be
mature by the end of the eighth grade. From that point on, usually little attention is given
to spelling at school except to call attention to the students' errors in written papers. This
is unfortunate because a large number of high school students cannot spell correctly the
words they are suppose to have learned in elementary school. The estimate is that one
student in ten gets through high school without sufficient knowledge of spelling for
everyday purposes in writing (Hildreth, 1955). A common complaint is that high-school
graduates applying for jobs in business offices cannot write, spell or punctuate. In order
to function in society, most individuals are required to write in a personal, school or
1
professional setting. Generally, this requires accurate spelling. Our culture inflicts
embarrassment and, in some settings serious consequences on persons who lack
competence in spelling. A lack of spelling ability can even adversely effect a person's
educational and occupational status (Hayward, at al, 1994).
Spelling is related to oral expression, because the way words are pronounced will
affect one's spelling habits. English orthography is often considered a highly inefficient
system that fails to represent in consistent fashion the relationship between graphic
symbols and phonetic expression (Templeton, 1980). Most people inside and outside of
education believed that English spelling does not do a good job of representing the
pronunciation of words, thus, primary instructional emphasized was usually placed upon
rote memory. Since only 50% of all words in the English language are rule-governed,
neither the learning of spelling nor the teaching of spelling can be based on the formal
drill and repetition of words out of content (Mershon, 1997/98). In recent years a more
comprehensive understanding of the nature of English spelling has led to more promising
instructional thrusts. More educators now understand that, while memory does play an
important role in learning how to spell, it does not play the only role.
Memorization of correct spelling is both possible and necessary. But mastery of
the total lexicon cannot be achieved by rote alone. Learners must examine words that
they have memorized and learn in a functional way how the system works. We now know
that a good speller is not a person who has successfully memorized the most words, but
rather someone who knows ways to figure out the logic of words and can construct them
as needed (Mershon, 1997/98). For a language as complex as English it takes time.
2
(Henderson, 1985). Expert spellers visualize words. They have the ability to store and
retrieve the visual form of the word in their brain. Poor spellers do not demonstrate equal
ability to store and retrieve the visual form of a word. When asked to spell an unfamiliar
word, they spell it like it sounds (Gentry and Gillet, 1993). The problem with this
approach is the difficulty it poses for the one who must spell it. Not only do our twentysix alphabet letters not represent sounds consistently, but also some sounds have no letter
to represent them. Some letters may have no sound of their own, or some sounds can be
signaled by different letters, and some letters may represent different sounds (Henderson,
1985).
Traditionally, comprehensive studies in spelling have utilized proofreading-typestandardized test to measure spelling achievement. In this process, students are asked to
proofread words or selections and determine if words are spelled correct. The advantage
of such a test is that it is standardized, easily administered, easily scored, and it resembles
other sections of achievement tests of which it is usually apart. Also, it can cover four
times as many words in the same administration time as the dictated word list. The
written spelling test is widely used by classroom teachers and is considered to be a valid
method of measuring spelling progress. It is inexpensive and easy to administer, takes
little advanced preparation, and all students can be tested simultaneously. Perhaps the
greatest strength is that the students actually write the words. A few of the limitations of
this method are poor handwriting, clues given by the examiners (need to pronounce the
words effectively), and scoring errors. The question arises as to whether spelling ability
or proofreading skill is being measured. Evidence exists that there are significant
3
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
9
correlation's between the ability of elementary school children to recognize misspelled
words and their ability to actually spell those words (Allred, 1984).
HYPOTHESES
The following study was designed to examine the reading attitudes of an older
student and to determine whether young ladies spell better than young men do. It was
hypothesized that no significant difference in spelling ability between the sexes will exist.
PROCEDURES
The participants in this study were 20 young men and 20 young ladies (ages 17
21). All of the students are from the same socio-economic backgrounds. The students
were enrolled in either Office Training or Computer Technology or Hotel and Hospitality
classes at Vocational Foundation Inc. (VFI), in New York City. Some of the students are
taking the GED (General Education Development) course work.
Albert Vadon, a fellow researcher, and I designed the questionnaire. The
questionnaire was developed to gather information on the student's attitude towards
reading. The written spelling test (WST) contained 20 words from Contemporary's Word
Power. The standardized spelling test (SST) contained 30 multiple-choice questions
obtained from Adult Basic Learning Examination (ABLE). The purpose of the spelling
tests was to determine if gender made a difference in spelling.
4
10
The questionnaire and spelling tests were given in small groups. The first group
was 16 males from the Computer Technologies class. Instead of attending their regular
classes they had volunteered to fill out a questionnaire and take a spelling test. After the
students got settled in their seats I did not state that I was exploring gender differences in
spelling. I informed them that I needed the data for school records, this explanation was
accepted. As I handed out the questionnaire, I instructed them that I would not be able to
help them with any of the questions. I did however go over 1 = strongly disagree and 5 =
strongly agree, gender, age and last grade completed. Names were optional. If they
weren't sure of a response to make, students were told that they were to look again and
make a choice. The questionnaire was collected and a blank spelling sheet was handed
out. The students were asked to write only their ages, last grade completed and their sex,
names were optional. The procedure used for the written spelling test included the
following. The spelling word was said, followed by saying the spelling word in a
complete sentence, and then by saying the spelling word again. This format was followed
throughout. The spelling sheets were then collected. The standardized spelling test was
then administered, with little instruction, except the usual about sex, last grade completed
and age. The second group consisted of 13 females from the Office Technology class. I
followed the same procedure except that I had both spelling tests stapled together. The
third group consisted of 7 females and 4 males from the Hotel and Hospitality class,
which is a co-ed group. The only deviation was that I administered the WST first and had
the students complete the questionnaire, then complete the SST. The survey and testing
was done on three separate days.
5
RESULTS
On the written spelling test (WST), the females averaged 40.25 percent correct,
whereas the males averaged 36.75 percent correct. The females outperformed the males
by a small percentage of 3.5 percent; this was not a significant level, as indicated on
Table 1.
Table 1
Written Spelling Test Results
t
Standard Deviation
Gender
Males
Mean
7.35
5.81
Females
8.05
4.48
- .43
The results of the individual performances of both males and females on the WST are
shown on appendix I. The results of how the males and females performed on each
individual word on the WST are shown on appendix H. The median for the males was
30% and the females 5% higher. The mode for the males was 10% and the females 27%.
See appendix III. The range for the males on WST was 5
females, which was 0
95% as opposed to the
80%.. Although, the range results indicate some of the males had
higher percentage, the females, as a group performed slightly better, but not at a
significant level.
6
12
On the standardized spelling test (SST), as indicated on Table 2,
Table 2
Standardized Spelling Test Results
Gender
Males
Mean
13.10
Standard Deviation
Females
17.30
6.31
7.61
- 1.90
the males averaged 41.5 percent, and the females again showed a slight, but not at a
significant level, by averaging 59.6%, 16 percent more than the males. On the SST the
males had the higher individual scores, but the females averaged higher scores. The range
for the males was 7 90% and the females 17
83%. The mode for the males was 33%
and the females 80%. See appendix V. The individual results of the SST are on appendix
IV.
The questionnaire was design to find out:
1. How much time was spent on reading?
2.
Whether reading was considered as masculine or feminine by the genders.
3. Whether or not they considered themselves as good spellers.
Those who scored highest on the spelling test also claimed to read more than 2
hours a day. Ninety-five percent of the females indicated that reading is a feminine
activity. Forty-two percent of the males considered reading as a feminine activity. See
Appendix VI. Seventy-five percent of the females considered writing as a feminine
activity, whereas the sixty-eight percent of the males considered writing a masculine
activity. See appendix VII. Of the thirty-five percent of the males that considered
7
13
themselves good spellers, sixty proved to be correct. Forty percent of the females
thought themselves to be good spellers and 100% of that forty was correct
CONCLUSIONS
On both the standardized and the written tests, there was no significant difference
between the genders. The females performed slightly better, but not at a significant level.
The males perceived both reading and writing as a masculine activity. The females
perceived reading and writing as feminine activity. Those students who considered
themselves as "good" spellers did indeed receive the highest scores.
IMPLICATIONS
On the WST, none of the words were phonetically correct. For each group I wrote
the correct spelling (after the WST), most of the students were amazed that they had
gotten so many incorrect spelling. I explained that these words were difficult to spell due
to their non-phonetic spelling. On the WST, some of the females rewrote the words to see
which one was visually correct, none of the males used this method. More research
needs to be done in order to find out if the same results appear if phonetically correct
words are used in the WST.
8
14
RELATED RESEARCH
9
15
In a study done by Allred (1990), results of a standardized spelling test and a
written spelling test were compared. Allred had a sample of 3,024 students, 252 boys and
252 girls at each grade level. A random selection of 28 children (14 boys and 14 girls)
from each grade level (grades one through 6). The samples came from high-, medium-,
and low-achieving schools as identified by district administrators. The Comprehensive
Test of Basic Skills (CTBS) was used for the standardized test and the written spelling
test (WST) was constructed using the same words found on the CTBS.
Students in grades one through five achieved significantly higher raw test scores
on a test where they are asked to recognize a word than on a test requiring them to
actually write the word. When educators are interested in determining elementary school
students' actual spelling performance, they should have students write the words. An
exception to this might be students in the sixth grade that may perform as well on written
test as they do on a spelling recognition test. While students in earlier grades did
significantly better on spelling recognition test than on written spelling test, sixth-grade
students performed as well on both tests. It can be concluded that differences between
students' ability to recognize misspelled words and their ability write words correctly
decreased, as students grow older. Allred found a significant correlation between the
ability of students to recognize a misspelled word and their ability to physically write the
word. This finding confirms the assumptions presently held by many educators that
recognition-type standardized spelling test are effective indicators of student's ability to
spell. Therefore, if educators desire information concerning students' spelling ability,
they are justified in using the present recognition-type standardized spelling achievement
10
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
16
tests to obtain that information. Additional research is needed to determine if this
apparent increased ability to spell words correctly in writing was due entirely to increased
maturity.
Allred also considered maturation to be a logical major cause of reading
differences. Girls matured earlier than boys physically, and one could question whether
their verbal abilities, in this case reading, also matured more rapidly and thus accounted
for their (the girls) consistently higher reading performance. This theory and other
potential causes such as cross-cultural expectations, needs to be further researched. In
other countries such as Germany, boys out performed girls in reading. Boys also out
performed girls in reading, in Nigeria and England. There seems to be a need for crosscultural research in the areas of gender differences on reading attitudes and aptitudes.
In a later study done by Allred (1990), gender differences in spelling achievement
were investigated for students in grades 1 through 6. Using the same sample as used in
his 1984 study. Performances of boys and girls on standardized and written spelling test
were compared. The Comprehensive Test of Basic Skills (CTBS) was used for the
standardized test and the written spelling test (WST) was constructed using the same
words found on the CTBS. At all grade levels the girls identified the correct spelling of
substantially more words on the CTBS and substantially more words were spelt correct
on the WST. The results of the analyses of variance revealed a significant difference (all
p values < .001) between boys and girls when averaged across both tests. In each case
girls scored significantly higher. Interactions between gender and tests at each grade level
11
17
were non-significant, meaning that the overall differences also existed separately for
each test. Conclusion of Allred's study was that girls in general spell better than boys do.
In an older study of achievement differences, Clark (1959) used information he
obtained from the California Test Bureau (CTB). The CTB had available the California
Test of Mental Maturity (CTMM) and the California Achievement Test (CAT) scores for
69,354 pupils from 341 schools in 48 states. From this a random sample of 75 boys and
75 girls were drawn at grades 3, 5, and 8, from 75 different school systems and cities
were represented. The CTMM, which measures intelligence through a sampling of
mental processes in five areas:
1. Memory
2.
Spatial relationship
3. Logical reasoning
4. Numerical reasoning
5.
Verbal concepts
The CTMM, used to measure intelligence, found no sex difference in the area of
general intelligence. Any difference in performance was due to differences in mental
ability and not due to the sex of the individual.
The CAT has six sections:
1.
Reading Vocabulary
2. Reading Comprehension
3.
Arithmetic Reasoning
4. Arithmetic Fundamentals
12
5. Mechanics of English
6. Spelling.
No significant differences were found between the performances of boys and
girls at grades three, five and eight for reading vocabulary, reading comprehension, and
arithmetic reasoning. For arithmetic fundamentals, there were no differences between the
performances of boys and girls at grades three and five, but at grade eight the
performances of the girls was slightly better. For mechanic of English, the girls did better
than the boys at grades five and eight. As for spelling, the girls performed better at all
grades. Conclusion appears to be that for reading and arithmetic there is no basic
difference in the performance of boys and girls. However, in the basic skills area of
language (mechanics of English and spelling) the performance of girls is superior to the
boys. The superior achievement of girls on both the mechanics of English and spelling
implies that significant sex differences do exist in the language area. It may be necessary
for additional instructional time and materials in language arts, as to bring the boys up to
par.
Spelling achievement of fourth graders was assessed using four self-corrected test
methods (Harward et al., 1994). Two hundred nine children in two elementary schools in
a predominantly middle class community in Utah County, Utah. A 2x2x2 factorial design
was implemented. Factors included :
(a) type of feedback (wordwise versus listwise),
(b) presentation mode (oral versus visual), and
(c) student's gender.
13
19
Answers to the following questions were sought:
(a) Is there a significant difference in spelling achievement when students selfcorrect after each word has been dictated as opposed to self-correction after an
entire list has been administered?
(b) Is there a significant difference in spelling achievement when students' selfcorrect hearing the word correctly spelled by the teacher as opposed to selfcorrecting after seeing a word presented in visual form?
(c) Is there a significant difference in spelling achievement between boys and girls
using the four variations of self-correction?
The number of words spelled correct on the wordwise (each word) was higher
than the listwise (whole list). The wordwise group gained an average of 2.2 more words.
This difference is statistically significant (p = .05). The number of words spelled
correctly by the visual was 1.0 over the oral. This was statistically non-significant (p =
.40). Female students outscored male students on the test. However, when gain scores
were considered, male students had a non-significantly higher gain of 0.5 words from the
pretest to posttest and 1.8 words form pretest to delay test over female students. This
difference may be attributed in part to the ceiling effect for girls. Girls who scored higher
on the pretest, had little room to make gains. The results for gender were similar across
schools, both feedback groups and both presentation groups, and all ability levels.
Although much research has been done, most of it has been at the elementary level or
through the 8th grade.
14
20
In a study made by Oliver E. Harris (1955), spelling achievement in high school
and college was found to be surprisingly low. However, the range of ability in every class
tested was wide. Average achievement levels found were as follows:
Grade 7
Grade
levels 3
4
Grade 8
Grade
levels 4
5
Grade 9
Grade
levels 5 - 6
Grade 10
Grade
levels 6
Grade 11 12
Grade - level 7
7
Achievement for each year except Grade 12 ranged all the way from below Grade 2 to
high school level. For Grade 12 the range was from Grade 2 to Grade 12.
Stipek's (1984) study of sex differences in children's attributions for success and
failure on math and spelling tests, used 165 students (82 boys and 83 girls) in two fifth
grades and two sixth grades classrooms. All classrooms were racially integrated and
included children form a wide range of socioeconomic levels. The main focus of this
study is to identify sex differences in attributions. Stipek used a questionnaire to measure
student's attributions for success and failure. She also had a teacher-prepared math and
spelling test. This article did not state what type of math and or spelling test was used.
Girls were not more likely than boys to attribute success to luck. On the contrary, luck
was not an important factor for either girls or boys. However, girls were more likely to
attribute failure on the math test to lack of ability and less likely to attribute than were
boys. Girls were also more likely to attribute success to ability than were boys. Girls were
also more likely to claim that they did poorly on the math test because math in general,
15
21
and this particular math test, was difficult. All of these attributional biases suggest that
girls perceive themselves to be relatively inadequate in math. Despite the boys' poorer
performance on the spelling test, they did not evidence the kind of self-derogating
attributional bias on this test as the girls evidenced on the math test. The one significant
sex difference in attributions among in the spelling failure group showed that boys were
more likely than girls to attribute their failure to an unstable cause: bad mood, which
would not threaten their self-esteem or their expectations for future success. Results
indicated that sex differences existed in math but not spelling. Compare to girls, boys
perceived themselves to be more competent and did better on the math test. Boys were
also less likely attribute failure on the math test to lack of ability and more likely to
attribute success to ability than were girls.
May and 011ila (1981) study of reading sex- role attitudes in preschoolers, had a
sample of 136 children, 98 from day-care centers and 38 from kindergarten in two
elementary schools. Sixty were 5 1/2 years old and 76 were 3 '/2 years old. The ratio of
boys to girls was 1
:
1. Each student received a booklet containing five object items and
five action items. Of the five object items, two were considered reading objects. Of the
five action items, two were considered reading items. The objects and actions NOT
associated with books or reading appear to be evaluated by older boys and girls as about
equally appropriate for both sexes. Only younger boys evaluated both reading and nonreading items as more appropriate for boys. Thus, preschool children of both sexes are
more likely to assess reading objects and actions as more appropriate to boys than to girls
and generally assess non-reading items to be about equally appropriate for both sexes. In
16
22
BEST COPY AVAILABLE
general the present study indicates that preschool children evaluate reading as an activity
more appropriate to males than to females. Children of both sexes were more apt to
respond that the reading items and objects pictured would be more appropriate for a boy
rather than a girl. Boys were on the average stronger in this bias than the girls were.
Previous research has indicated that older children perceive reading objects and actions
as more feminine than masculine. Why is reading perceived as masculine behavior by
preschool children but, later as a feminine activity by older school-age children? If these
findings are reliable, further research seems to called for to determine why there is a
change of attitudes as children grow older.
A study done by Emans and Patyk (1967), on a population of 145 females and
179 males, ages 12
15 and 16
19, on reading interests, suggests that individuals are
influenced by two factors: the nature of the topic itself and the motive of the reader. In
trying to distinguish the reasons for reading, the list of questions included:
1. To get away from it all. (recreational)
2. When I need to make a speech. (informative)
3. About people my own age. (identification)
4. Something artistically written. (aesthetic)
Sex was found to be related to information and aesthetic motives at the .001 level and to
the identification motive at the .10 level of significance. Boys ranked seeking information
considerably higher than girls (rank first 41% boys, 24% girls). Thirty-one percent of
the girls ranked information fourth, but only 17 percent of the boys gave information a
fourth place ranking. Boys had no strong or weak identification motive compared with
17
23
girls. Girls were more apt to read for identification than boys. Few members of either
sex gave aesthetics a high rating; 5%of the boys and 12% of the girls ranked it first.
When the reading level factor was examined, it was the "poor reader" who sought
identification for reading. Younger students were most likely to rank recreation higher
than the older student (.10 level). Forty-seven percent of the younger group ranked
recreation first. Nevertheless, 38 percent of the older students still gave recreation the
number one position.
Pajares and Valiante's 1998, study of grade level and gender differences in
writing, used 742 students from a public middle school in the South (376 girls and 366
boys, 243 6th grade, 237 7th grade, and 262 8th grade). The social-economics status of the
school was largely middle class, and primarily White. During the first period students
were asked to complete an attitude questionnaire, and during the second period a 30
minute essay. The focus of the study was to analyze gender differences in confidence.
The students were asked six questions to determine how they compared their writing
ability with that of their peers:
1.
whether they believed they were better writers than the boys in the class
2. whether they believed they were better writers than the girls in the class
3. whether they believed they were better writers than the boys in the school
4. whether they believed they were better writers than the girls in the school
5.
whether they believed they were better writers than all the students in the class
6. whether they believed they were better writers than all the students in the
school.
18
24
Students in the 8th grade were judged more competent writers by their teachers,
and results of the ability comparisons revealed that they considered themselves better
writers than their peers at the school to a greater degree than did the 6th or 7th grade
students. When students were asked whether they were better writers than the boys in
their class or in their school, girls expressed a greater degree of superiority than did the
boys. It is especially noteworthy to contrast the nearly 1-point difference on the 6-point
Likert scale between girls' comparison with the boys in their class (4.3) and boys'
comparisons with the girls in their class (3.4), t = 10.3. p<.0001. Similarly, when asked
whether they were better writers than the boys in their school, girls averaged 3.5; when
asked whether they were better writers than the girls in their school, boys averaged 2.9, t
= 7.3, p,.0001. Clearly, girls believed themselves superior to the boys, and the boys
tented to agree.
19
25
REFERENCES
20
26
Allred, RA. (May/June 1984). Comparison of proofreading-type standardized spelling
tests and written spelling test scores. Journal of Educational Research, 77, pp. 298 303.
Allred, R.A. (March/April 1990). Gender differences in spelling. Journal of Educational
Research, 83, No.4, pp.187-193.
Clark, W.W.( 1959). Are there significant ability and achievement differences? Phi
Delta Kappan, pp.73 76.
Emans, R & Patyk, G. (Feb. 1967). Why do highs school student read? Journal of
Reading, 10, pp.300-304.
Gentry, J.R & Gillet, J.W. (1993). Teaching Kids to Spell. Portsmouth, NH:
Heinemann.pp.49 56.
Harris,Oliver. (1947). An investigation of spelling achievement of secondary school
pupils. Educational Administration and Supervision. 34, pp.208 219.
Harward, S.V., Allred, R.A., & Sudweeks, RR (1994). The effectiveness of fourcorrected spelling test methods. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 15, pp.
245-271.
Henderson, E. H. (1985). Teaching Spelling. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.
pp.189 204.
Hildreth, G. (1955). Teaching Spelling: A Guide to Basic Principles and Practices. pp.
267 285.
Karlson, Bjorn & Gardner, Eric, F. (1986). Psychological Corporation, 2nd Edition.
(SST)
May, R.B. & 011ila, L.O. (1981). Reading sex-roles attitudes in preschoolers. Readlgi
Research Quarterly, 16, No. 4, pp. 583-597.
Manning, M.M. & Manning, G.L. (1986). Improving Spelling in the Middle Grades.
Washington, D.C.: National Education Association.pp.5 14.
Mershom, C. (Fall/Winter 1997-98). Things That Go Bump in the Night: Integrated
Spelling Instruction and Standardized Tests. The Reading Instruction Journal, pp. 26-31.
Parares, F. & Valiante, G. (1999). Grade level and gender differences in the writing
self-beliefs of middle school students. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 24, pp.390
405.
21
27
Templeton, S., (1980). Spelling Phonology, and the Older Student. In E.H. Henderson,
& J.W. Beers, Developmental and Cognitive Aspects of Learning to Spell: A Reflection
of Word Knowledge (pp. 85-96). Newark, Delaware: International Reading Association.
Templeton, S. & Morris, D. (Jan./Feb./Mar.1999). Questions teachers ask about
spelling. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, pp. 102-112
22
28
APPENDICES
23
29
a
1111111111111111
4.1
to,
11
ee
0
t.
Percent Correct
-..
0
gco
co
=
g0
0co
CD
CI:
Tli
7
IV
0
r.)
01
a
c..1
co
al
.o.
o
41
th
Percent Correct
N
0
A,
111526§11
rraTREEZAII
rg"
mow
0
'
,1411
.,
,
0
0
0
::,
,
0
,,
,
,
:
0
.
x
, 4i,
OffininaniffitigliiiT; . A,
4W'
..........iiiiiiiMES I
.
.
91.
,
,
%
.
,,,
,
s ei
-
,,,
.,,
4
TI
, .a.:,i-
,
...
aar
.
Or
-
__
r.'r : R
14
_ _ _...
-.
,
I
,-3,..
..
0
I,
,
.,
.-
co
03
0
C3
2.
71 C
3 Fp-
rt,. ie,
.....
.:..
r motor ,.
mclg:
',6t
a:
I
4
,
....
7'C,1
4,..y4tz
.
Of
TI
.
IISIMME 4WEPENNEEITM 6,
I!
i 111111111011
'
.
Ws,,,:.,,raa
,
;,,,, ...
.
4
Ot
0.
,f
0.
','
...----,......
-.
a
.
.
'
:-..,ISIVIV,,':-:
.
-:.
-
--
-----
' IVEffra'52---- Ei"I'VEM:ri2
m
cn
is
a
rYtca
II
.----
.
40% .
+
-. -- -. - --
,
g
a.
*
1
17-
Cl)
'
1
Vt.
'
"
''.:
.,;. 4t,', -1... 1/
-1
V: - ..:**::;,"
' -''''''"7/4,,,,,
EMEME.PEAMMEEMEE
'3A
T
'
,
.,,
,
EM, '1'
..r
I
--Imam
rrigilEgz-
.
aPtea'!';
-
r , IA '"
itge,W.
CD
..
-
(lust
rameittme.
I
-
,
fammx:,,:.:,.:,-.
.7 .
tWnataMEMMEEZER
.
,
' IOW 4,4
__
it
t..y*?1:005 47.5r.i:,.i.R:
.-
. ..
...
.
____.,
cD
it
.,......,...,,,.....,,...,e...-.......,,.......,...-
--.,..........,...,..........,...............
f7CIN''-
'
,
,
0
..,,
,. ;,136449M, -
,
.?:,
_ ............._
1
Percent Correct
CD
7
cu
cD
0
O
*.,4).:Kii.4211ZeitMdiXr
rt
cD
At
a.
a
N
cD
O.
cD
eD
se.
00
Cg
a,
co
3
co
cm
U)
(7)
cn
Students
CD
11)
21
5
C
C.4.)
Tt
E
N
cn
....
C
0
>l
=
c)
a)
74:
CD
Cn
a)
To
rn
E
2 u_
0o
N
quapms
ERIC/RCS
2805 East Tenth St.
t.
Bloomington, IN 47405
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
OFFICE OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH AND IMPROVEMENT (OERI)
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
CS 217 023
REPRODUCTION RELEASE (Specific Document)
I.
DOCUMENT IDENTIFICATION
'Title: Gender bt-CCe-ce--bnc-e
Cr. 12.1oG
.-Author(s). DC1 1s
Corporate Source (if appropriate)
1Af'4
SP e \
KEAN
ACAltevernenA's
Uhl iv eys'tt.,_
Publication Date Ifl
000
I I . REPRODUCTION RELEASE
In order to disseminate as widely as possible timely and significant materials of interest to the educational community,
documents announced in the monthly abstract journal of the ERIC system, Resources In Education (RIE), are usually made
available to users in microfiche and paper copy (or microfiche only) and sold through the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDRS). Credit is given to the source of each document, and, if reproduction release is granted, one of the following
notices is affixed to the document.
If permission is granted to reproduce the identified document, please CHECK ONE of the options and sign the release
below.
"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS
MATERIAL IN MICROFICHE ONLY
HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS
MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
Microfiche
(4" x 6" film)
and paper copy
CHECK 1\
HERE
wr
Dota,440,R61, es,
AW-efivevi
(81/2" x 11")
aMicrofiche
(4" x 6" film)
reproduction
(PERSONAL NAME OR ORGANIZATION,
only
AS APPROPRIATE I
reproduction
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)."
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)."
Documents will be processed as indicated provided reproduction quality permits. If permission to reproduce is granted, but neither box is checked,
documents will be processed in both microfiche and paper copy.
"I hereby grant to the Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) nonexclusive permission to reproduce this document as
indicated above. Reproduction from the ERIC microfiche by persons other than ERIC employees and its system contractors requires
permission from the copyright holder. Exception is made for non-profit reproduction of microfiche by libraries and other service
agencies to satisfy information needs of educators in response to discrete inquiries."
SIGN
HERE
sh'7
/0 k A t.1
1\
rf
Signature'
Organization-
Printed -Name.
UN I lig.st..S. I 1-4
Address: 5-(o0 C_ telmeryux
ail I S 14.4. f
III.
e_+
Zip Code: al 2-6 .
DA ISLi n't
R-I 0 -C
Position. 5.1-41.44
Tel No of o - lo 1 a-1 SD
Date-
Apr-
.1
'1_0 00
DOCUMENT AVAILABILITY INFORMATION (Non-ERIC Source)
If permission to reproduce is not granted to ERIC, or, if you wish ERIC to cite the availability of the document from
another source, please provide the following information regarding the availability of the document. (ERIC will not announce a document unless it is publicly available, and a dependable source can be specified. Contributors should also be
aware that ERIC selection criteria are significantly more stringent for documents which cannot be made available throUgh
EDRS.)
Publisher/Distributor
Address:
Price Per Copy
IV.
Quantity Price:
REFERRAL TO COPYRIGHTIREPRODUCTION RIGHTS HOLDER
If the right to grant reproduction release is held by someone other than the addressee, please provide the appropriate
name and address:
EFF-53 (Rev. 4/86)