Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jretconser
Competing for attention with in-store promotions
Megan Phillips a, Andrew G. Parsons a,n, Helene J. Wilkinson a, Paul W. Ballantine b
a
b
Department of Marketing, Advertising, Retailing, and Sales, Auckland University of Technology, Private Bag 92006, Auckland 1142, New Zealand
Department of Management, Marketing, and Entrepreneurship, University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch 8140, New Zealand
art ic l e i nf o
a b s t r a c t
Article history:
Received 9 August 2014
Received in revised form
24 May 2015
Accepted 24 May 2015
Supermarkets typically have an in-store demonstration located near the promotional end-of-aisle (or
end-cap) area due to space requirements. Using a field experiment, we examine whether the occurrence
of these in-store promotions competing for attention and engagement can disrupt each other, using
binary logistic regression to analyse shopper behaviour. Results show the best way to attract attention to
the end-of-aisle is not to have an in-store demonstration near it, or if required, a complementary product
to the end-of-aisle should be used. Inferences based upon shopper characteristics are also given, providing important nuances in the attention to, and engagement with, in-store promotions.
& 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Keywords:
In-store promotions
End-of-aisle
Shopper behaviour
Attention
1. Introduction
A supermarket shopper will encounter a range of in-store
promotional activities during a typical shopping trip, including
store coupons, manufacturer coupons, product demonstrations,
and end-of-aisle (or end-cap) displays. Each of these is designed to
make an item stand out from the competition, attract the shopper's attention, and engage them with that item. However, with a
plethora of activities competing for attention, does the “surprise”
element, which has been shown (Itti and Baldi, 2009) to positively
moderate the planned behaviour of a shopper, become obfuscated
or even lost, and thus reduce the effectiveness of in-store promotions? This study examines the behaviour of shoppers confronted with in-store promotions competing for their attention
and action. Specifically, this study will explore whether the effectiveness of an end-of-aisle display is diluted if there is a product
demonstration occurring near the end-of-aisle. The implications of
this have significant value for suppliers, who typically pay for both
these locations. A supplier will be able to make more effective
promotion choices if they know their activity is enhanced or reduced by the accompanying promotional activity.
In-store promotions are increasingly favoured over external
advertising because a connection can be made between the ability
of in-store promotions to actively engage shoppers and increase
sales (Abratt and Goodey, 1990; Bava et al., 2009; Wilkinson et al.,
n
Correspondence to: Retailing Faculty of Business and Law, Auckland University
of Technology, Level 4, WY Building, 120 Mayoral Drive, Auckland 1010, Private Bag
92006, Auckland 1142, New Zealand.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (A.G. Parsons).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2015.05.009
0969-6989/& 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
1982). Intensification of competition between brands has led to
more vigorous promotions and significant expenditure (Gilbert
and Jackaria, 2002). Research to-date has examined the value of
in-store promotions and the responses of shoppers. These responses have included generating attention (Bava et al., 2009),
stimulating purchase (Wilkinson et al., 1982), enhancing impulse
buying likelihood (Abratt and Goodey, 1990), and increasing traffic
(Spiekermann et al., 2011). Nordfalt and Lange (2013) in their
comprehensive study took the novel approach of considering the
timing (day-of-week) of in-store promotions. In the context of our
study, Nordfalt and Lange (2013) also looked at whether a demonstration was with or without a display. What has not been
examined, however, is whether the occurrence of in-store promotions competing for attention and engagement can disrupt each
other, even when they are not in the same product category.
2. Theoretical background
2.1. Literature
There are three typical in-store promotions for a supermarket.
The first – in-store demonstrations – can be the presentation of a
product (e.g. cooking demonstration), trial of a product (e.g. free
tasting), or a combination of both. While there is little research on
in-store demonstrations, and in particular trials (Heilman et al.,
2001, 2011), the expectation is that they would be effective in
gaining attention due to their physical presence and difference
from surrounding aisles, and that they would be effective in
creating engagement because the shopper is spoken to and has the
142
M. Phillips et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
option to trial the product (see Nordfalt and Lange (2013), for an
excellent discussion). Furthermore, demonstrations should have a
direct impact on sales given sampling is in the store where the
product is available for immediate purchase, and is often available
for selection at the demonstration point.
The second in-store promotion is the end-of-aisle display.
These are considered highly visible by manufacturers and supermarkets, and are changed frequently to maintain interest (Suher
and Sorenson, 2010). The end-of-aisle is also the fixed in-store
promotion space, and has traditionally been regarded as the prime
real-estate of a supermarket. The normal expectation from shoppers is that there is some discount associated with the displayed
item, though they are used to display new merchandise as well,
which may or may not be discounted (Chevalier, 1975). End-ofaisle displays expose shoppers to items in a space free from direct
aisle-based competition (Dulsrud and Jacobsen, 2009; Schindler
et al., 1987). While the display itself is only passive engagement,
the call-to-action is the associated discount price, which is announced with prominent signage, or the “newness” of the item,
similarly announced.
The third in-store promotion is the one that has received a
greater level of research interest – coupons (Barat and Ye, 2012).
Coupons are redeemable vouchers used to receive a specified reduction in price. They may be a straight discount (e.g. 10c off
regular price), or they may be some value deal (e.g. two for the
price of one). They are used by both manufacturers and retailers,
and have been found to be more effective at increasing sales than
advertised price discounts. They are known to have an impact on
sales because they encourage shoppers to purchase more than
they normally would of the promoted item, purchase earlier than
their normal cycle of purchase, and they encourage switching
behaviour from competing brands due to trial risk reduction (e.g.
Aggarwal and Vaidyanathan, 2002; Heilman et al., 2002; Kahn and
Schmittlein, 1992).
In practise, a supermarket is likely to have an in-store demonstration located near or at the end of an aisle. This is simply
because in a typical supermarket layout, this is where the most
clear space is available to set-up a temporary stand. The location
often has no relationship to the item′s normal product category
location in an aisle. Therefore, an end-of-aisle display is competing
for attention with the demonstration. In-store coupons, on the
other hand, are typically located with the item in the normal aisle
space. Sometimes they are associated with the end-of-aisle display
as well, but we treat this as the same case as the end-of-aisle
display. Thus, competition for attention is most likely to arise
between product demonstrations and end-of-aisle displays.
This potential conflict needs to be addressed because of the
pragmatic implications for both manufacturers and retailers.
Manufacturers often seek access to end-of-aisle space to gain an
awareness advantage over competitors, and can be charged a
premium for displays in these locations (Rossiter and Percy, 1987).
Similarly, retailers looking to shift stock use this space to gain
attention for a product that otherwise may be relatively unnoticed.
At the same time, manufacturers also use demonstrations to create
awareness. They are often used to introduce new products in an
attempt to secure a viable level of sales to warrant allocation by
the retailer of limited shelf space. Manufacturers pay retailers for
the demonstration space, and of course also incur the demonstration costs (e.g. giving away trial items; the demonstrator wages), while retailers incur an opportunity cost by allowing selling
space in their store to be used by the manufacturer. Any reduction
in attention or engagement due to competing attractions harms
the potential value of the in-store promotion.
2.2. Hypothesis development
2.2.1. Product demonstrations and end-of-aisle displays
In-store product demonstrations are largely under-researched
(Heilman et al., 2011; Nordfalt and Lange, 2013), with only a
handful of studies focused specifically on sampling in the retail
environment (e.g. Heilman et al., 2011; Lammers, 1991; Steinberg
and Yalch, 1978), or considering sampling as part of the wider
study of in-store promotions (e.g. Gedenk and Neslin, 1999; Shi
et al., 2005). Trade and academic research shows that in-store
product demonstrations have an impact on sales – including those
of other products – and trial (Lammers, 1991; Laposky, 2007;
Lawson et al., 1990; Major, 2002; Moses, 2005; Troy, 2005; Zwiebach, 2005), and it is believed that a key reason for this is that the
demonstrator grabs attention, and the product is there for the
shopper to purchase (Heiman et al., 2001). Surprise is also likely to
have an impact, given that the in-store product demonstration is
where the shopper normally expects nothing to be.
Similarly, research on end-of-aisle displays has tended to examine them in the context of wider in-store promotions (e.g.
Curhan, 1974; Fader and Lodish, 1990; Haans and Gijsbrechts,
2011; Lemon and Nowlis, 2002; Wilkinson et al., 1982). Underhill
(1999) suggests that, like product demonstrations, they act as a
prompt for immediate purchase. They are seen as very effective
attention generators (Dulsrud and Jacobsen, 2009; Schindler et al.,
1987), even when goods are not discounted (Chevalier, 1975).
Bezawada et al. (2009) examined the placement in aisles and endof-aisle displays in the context of cross-category placement (i.e.
complementary goods), using aggregate sales data to measure
placement effectiveness.
Together, in-store product demonstrations and end-of-aisle
displays appear to have similar characteristics – they locate in similar areas, they focus on gaining attention from their physical
presence rather than price, and they promote immediacy of purchase by having the product available for the shopper to select
easily. This suggests that competition for attention is likely to
occur when both are present in neighbouring spaces. As we are
unable to remove the end-of-aisle (by definition) we therefore
couch our hypothesis in terms of the end-of-aisle being the basepoint.
H1. The presence of an in-store demonstration near an end-ofaisle will affect shoppers' attention paid to the end-of-aisle.
‘Near’ is defined as having to pass between the in-store demonstration and the end-of-aisle to navigate past the end-of-aisle.
Our expectation from the literature is that an in-store demonstration in proximity to the end-of-aisle will grab attention away
through surprise and immediacy.
2.2.2. Surprise and shopper profile
Itti and Baldi (2009) state that the strongest attractors of attention are stimuli that pop-out from their neighbours in space or
time, and that surprise is the best known attractor of human attention. The same authors (Baldi and Itti, 2010) note that novel or
salient events attract attention. In-store promotions fall into this
description, and the study by Heilman et al. (2002) considers the
impact of unexpected in-store coupons as a pleasant surprise,
showing that shoppers encountering this surprise are more likely
to make unplanned purchases. This notion of planning (i.e. the
Theory of Planned Behaviour, Ajzen, 1991) is important in our
context because the disruption of the planned behaviour – which
is prevalent in shopping (Iyer, 1989), because of the multiple
buying goals characterising supermarket shopping (Park et al.,
1989) – is likely to be modified by whether the overall shopping
trip goal is very precise and concrete (e.g. to take advantage of a
specific promotion) through to relatively abstract (e.g. to fill up on
weekly needs; Bell et al., 2011). Lee and Ariely (2006) suggest that
M. Phillips et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
the success of marketing actions, such as promotions, depends on
the goals shoppers have when they are exposed to such promotions. In another context we see this occur with the use/prominence of shelf facings when it comes to brand attention (Chandon
et al., 2009). We expect these goals to manifest themselves
through the shopper profile.
The profile of the shopper will impact upon attention, and the
ability to engage. Shopper orientation – hedonic or task oriented –
is known to impact attention paid to environmental cues, such as
in-store displays/demonstrations (Breugelmans and Campo, 2011),
and we also know that the gender of the shopper is a moderator of
shopper orientation (Mortimer, 2012; Mortimer and Clarke, 2011;
Tifferet and Herstein, 2012). The type of shopping trip also has an
effect (Mazumdar and Papatla, 1995). “Fill-in” and “major-trip”
shoppers have different responses to displays than “intermediatetrip” shoppers. The shopping party (e.g. size, relationships) can
interfere with the shopping environment and influence engagement (Borges et al., 2010), and even the shopping device – anything from a cart to carrying items in one's hands – can influence
attention and the ability to engage (Cochoy, 2008).
Given the preceding discussion, the focal research objective of
this study, addressed in H1, is to examine whether having an instore product demonstration situated near an end-of-aisle affects
shoppers' attention to the end-of-aisle display. Addressing this
objective will help to understand where the most effective place to
locate a product demonstration is, and will help to determine
whether two promotional activities next to each other are beneficial or not. Moreover, this objective will help address whether
the attention paid to the in-store promotion (the end-of-aisle and/
or product demonstration) will be affected by whether the in-store
promotions are competing with each other, and whether that
competition involves the same or different product categories. To
this end we add a second hypothesis to the study:
H2. The presence of an in-store demonstration near an end-ofaisle with the same (different) products will affect shoppers' attention paid to the end-of-aisle.
We also explore how the characteristics of the shopper, including gender, the shopping device being used, and the direction
and movement of their travel, impact on the attention given to
these two in-store promotion types. In essence we examine
whether differences in any of the shopper profile characteristics
affect attention, establishing the following pair of generalised
hypotheses:
H3. Differences in gender/shopping device/direction of movement/travel will affect shoppers' attention paid to the end-of-aisle.
H4.Differences in gender/shopping device/direction of movement/travel will affect shoppers' attention paid to the in-store
demonstration.
3. Method
3.1. Overview
A field experiment following an after-only with control group
approach was undertaken in a large supermarket situated in a
suburban area of a mid-sized city. The supermarket cooperated by
sourcing manufacturer product demonstrators and coordinating
them with product change-outs in the end-of-aisle displays, allowing combinations of demonstrations and displays to be deployed. Juice and cookies were selected as the two product lines
for the promotional activities. Each was easy to display at the endof-aisle, and the juice was simple to demonstrate (offer to taste).
They were not in the same (or even similar) categories, but were
likely to appeal to a similar market. Nordfalt and Lange (2013)
cleverly noted that there might be a relationship between day-of-
143
Table 1
Daily demonstration and display combinations; indicator variables.
Group
Product demonstration
End-of-aisle display
A (Day 1)
B (Day 2)
C (Day 3)
Juice
Juice
n/a
Juice
Cookies
Juice and cookies
the-week and product, when it came to promotion effectiveness
in-store. The two products selected do not appear to have any
discernible cyclical pattern, so this was not considered an issue.
Video-recorded observations were made of shopper behaviour
over a three-day period. The three daily combinations that were
observed are shown in Table 1.
The two product demonstration days (A and B) were located
adjacent to the end-of-aisle, allowing approximately seven feet of
space for shopper movement, which was the same space as in the
aisles. The end-of-aisle only day (C) used the two adjacent end-ofaisles that the product demonstration (A and B) had been located
next to.
Collection of data was for the duration of each product demonstration, and for one hour on the non-demonstration (end-ofaisle only) day, with 1807 observations being collected overall.
Also observed was the gender of the shopper, the shopping device
being used, and the direction of movement/travel (i.e. from which
direction did the shopper approach the location and where they
then went). The observations were made via security cameras so
that the shoppers were unaware that their behaviour was being
observed, which should result in natural behaviour, and reliable
observation. A detailed map of the observation area was used for
the manual recording and coding of shopper movement patterns
in order to evaluate their activity and behaviour.
In addition to the variables discussed in Table 1, there were a
number of sample variables that were used. These are expanded
upon in the next sub-section, and are shown in Table 2. All the
variables utilised were categorical and the coding scheme was
indicator-variable coding (i.e. a 1 for present; 0 for not present).
Reference groups for each of the independent variables were
Table 2
Summary of sample data.
Group
A (Day 1)
B (Day 2)
C (Day 3)
Total sample
Gender
Femalen
Male
178 (65.9)
92 (34.1)
194 (71.9)
76 (28.1)
176 (65.2)
94 (34.8)
548 (67.7)
262 (32.3)
Shopping device
Nothing
Trolley
Basketn
93 (34.4)
109 (40.4)
68 (25.2)
97 (35.9)
90 (33.3)
83 (30.7)
88 (32.6)
91 (33.7)
91 (33.7)
278 (34.3)
290 (35.8)
242 (29.9)
Movement
Anticlockwisen
Clockwise
Both
218 (80.7)
208 (77.0)
208 (77.0)
634 (78.3)
44 (16.3)
8 (3.0)
54 (20.0)
8 (3.0)
49 (18.1)
13 (4.8)
147 (18.1)
29 (3.6)
Travel
Perimetern
Aisle
Both
97 (35.9)
20 (7.4)
153 (56.7)
135 (50.0)
11 (4.1)
124 (45.9)
75 (27.8)
30 (11.1)
165 (61.1)
307 (37.9)
61 (7.5)
442 (54.6)
Paid attention to
End-of-aisle
Demonstration
78 (28.9)
88 (32.6)
85 (31.5)
116 (43.0)
176 (65.2)
n/a
339 (41.6)
204 (37.8)
Frequencies and percentages (in parentheses).
n
Reference category.
144
M. Phillips et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
determined and are shown in Table 2 with a * indicating selection,
so that when the variables were recoded, the coefficients for the
new variables represented the effect of each category compared to
the reference category (i.e. similar to the use of dummy variables
in linear regression). As both the dependent and independent
variables were categorical, binary logistic regression was used,
with all variables being entered into the analysis simultaneously.
Each of the reference groups was chosen based upon characteristics found within the sample, and these are discussed next.
3.2. Sample
Of the 1807 shopper observations, 1186 (65.6%) were female,
494 (27.3%) were male, and 127 (7.0%) were couples. The proportions found in the sample are similar to recent studies, and as
female shoppers are the largest group, they were chosen as the
reference category for the variable gender. The majority of observations were also of individual shoppers (n ¼1463), with fifteen
other shopping party configurations being observed. In order to
more clearly focus on individual, as opposed to group level behaviour, only those observations that included a single shopper
were retained for later analysis.
Of the 1807 observations, there were six types of shopping
devices used, with shopping carts (n ¼ 633; 35.0%), nothing
(n ¼615; 34.0%), and baskets (n ¼522; 28.9%) being the most
common. In this case, baskets were chosen as the reference category. Although they were only the third largest group, speaking
with the management of the supermarket revealed that a basketcarrying customer was their primary target.
Direction of movement and direction of travel around the supermarket was considered. In terms of movement, and of the 1807
observations, 1457 (80.6%) moved anti-clockwise, 269 (14.9%)
moved clockwise, and 81 (4.5%) moved both ways. Anti-clockwise
was the natural movement pattern given the store layout (and was
the largest group), so was chosen as the reference category. Finally,
shoppers had three ways they could travel about the store –
around the perimeter, through the aisles, or in both the perimeter
and aisles. Of the 1807 original observations, 990 (54.8%) travelled
both, 662 (36.6%) travelled the perimeter only, and 155 (8.6%)
travelled only the aisles. Perimeter travel was selected as the reference category, as basket shoppers (the target group for the
supermarket) were targeted with perimeter products.
Due to the differing durations of the two product demonstration days, there was a discrepancy in the number of observations
recorded. Specifically, 1102 observations were recorded on Day
One, while 347 were recorded on Day Two (358 were recorded on
the day when there was no product demonstration). To ensure an
equal number of observations were analysed for each of the three
days, in addition to the omission of those observations which did
not include a single shopper only, it was also decided to only include the three main types of shopping device in the analysis
(shopping carts, nothing, or baskets). Given these criteria, and the
desire for an equal sample size for each of the three days, observations were randomly deleted to achieve this goal, resulting in
an analysis sample of 810 observations, or 270 observations for
each of the three days. A summary of the sample used for the
statistical analysis presented in the next section (including frequencies and percentages) is provided in Table 2.
4. Results
Attention was operationalized as the visual connection the
observed shopper made with the in-store demonstration and/or
the end-of-aisle. If the shopper slowed down or stopped, and
looked at the in-store demonstration, or went further and engaged
with the demonstrator and/or examined the product then they
were considered to have visually connected with the in-store demonstration and thus gave it attention. Similarly the shopper who
slowed down or stopped at the end-of-aisle display, and looked at
it, or went further and examined the product, was considered to
have given their attention to the display. We can see from the last
two rows of Table 2 that there is a clear difference in attention
when there is no competing promotion. A simple Chi-square test
reveals that there is a significant difference (χ2 ¼90.98, p o.01)
and further investigation of the direction shows that attention
towards the end-of-aisle improves without competing promotions. Therefore we find that H1 is supported, and that the presence of an in-store demonstration near the end-of-aisle does affect shoppers’ attention paid to the end-of-the-aisle.
Binary logistic regression analysis was used to examine the
impact of the four independent variables (gender, shopping device, movement, and travel) on the two dependent variables of
interest (attention to the in-store demonstration and attention to
the end-of-aisle display). This section presents the results for both
main and two-way interactions. The significance of each model
was indicated by the chi-square statistic, while the Wald statistic
was used to evaluate the statistical significance of each independent variable. Finally, the percentage of the number of cases
correctly predicted by each model was also determined.
4.1. Main effects
Both models (attention to the in-store demonstration and attention to the end-of-aisle display) were significant when the instore demonstration and end-of-aisle display were complementary to each other. Both movement and travel were significant in terms of their impact on attention being paid to the instore demonstration, while only movement was found to be significant in terms of explaining attention being paid to the end-ofaisle display (see Table 3). On the day when the in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display were not complementary to each
other, neither model was significant (see Table 4). Finally, on the
day when there was no in-store demonstration (see Table 5), it
was found that shopping device and movement were significant in
terms of their ability to help explain the attention being paid to an
end-of-aisle display.
The results indicate that gender was not a significant predictor
in any of the models. Movement was found to be a significant
predictor in all three models which were statistically significant,
while the impact of the shopping device used and travel pattern
were inconsistent.
4.2. Two-way interactions
When the in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display
were complementary to each other, it was found that only the
model for attention being paid to the end-of-aisle was significant
Table 3
Complementary in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display.
Attention to in-store
demonstration
Attention to end-of-aisle
display
Wald
Sig
Wald
Sig
Gender
.10
Shopping device
2.39
Movement
13.17
Travel
7.83
Model chi-square 31.39
Percentage
68.50
.76
.30
.00
.02
.00
.21
5.22
14.54
4.58
37.49
73.30
.65
.07
.00
.10
.00
M. Phillips et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
Table 4
Competing in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display.
145
Table 8
End-of-aisle display only.
Attention to in-store
demonstration
Attention to end-of-aisle
display
Wald
Sig
Wald
Sig
Gender
.30
Shopping device
2.20
Movement
.43
Travel
3.70
Model chi-square
6.42
Percentage
60.40
.59
.33
.81
.16
.49
.25
5.14
5.34
1.41
11.41
69.60
.62
.08
.07
.49
.12
Attention to end-of-aisle display
Gender Device
Gender Movement
Gender Travel
Device Movement
Device Travel
Movement Travel
Model chi-square
Percentage
Wald
Sig
2.09
9.21
1.30
19.87
3.07
1.05
57.96
73.30
.35
.01
.52
.00
.55
.90
.00
Table 5
End-of-aisle display only.
Looking at our remaining hypotheses, we find that in general,
H2 is supported, and that if the product in the in-store demonstration is different (competing for attention) this distracts from
the end-of-aisle. Considering our third and fourth hypotheses, we
find that in some cases shopper profile characteristics do have an
effect on attention paid to the in-store demonstration and/or the
end-of-aisle promotion. We discuss these and the broader findings
in the next section.
Attention to end-of-aisle display
Gender
Shopping device
Movement
Travel
Model chi-square
Percentage
Wald
Sig
1.29
19.10
16.54
.05
48.62
69.60
.26
.00
.00
.97
.00
5. Discussion and conclusions
Table 6
Complementary in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display.
Attention to in-store
demonstration
Attention to end-ofaisle display
Wald
Sig
Wald
Sig
Gender Device
.36
Gender Movement
1.50
Gender Travel
2.38
Device Movement
3.14
Device Travel
3.51
Movement Travel
.30
Model chi-square
27.03
Percentage
69.30
.84
.47
.31
.54
.48
.99
.08
1.58
1.18
.37
12.17
12.31
5.68
42.54
72.60
.45
.55
.83
.02
.02
.23
.00
Table 7
Competing in-store demonstration and end-of-aisle display.
Attention to in-store
demonstration
Attention to end-ofaisle display
Wald
Sig
Wald
Sig
Gender Device
.67
Gender Movement
.73
Gender Travel
1.07
Device Movement
6.05
Device Travel
3.38
Movement Travel
4.68
Model chi-square
23.23
Percentage
64.10
.72
.69
.59
.20
.50
.32
.18
2.03
1.33
2.64
2.98
1.58
.53
18.28
70.40
.36
.52
.27
.56
.81
.97
.44
(see Table 6), with two significant two-way interactions being
found between shopping device and movement, and shopping
device and travel. When the in-store demonstration and end-ofaisle display were not complementary to each other, neither
model was significant (see Table 7); consistent with when only the
main effects were examined. Finally, on the day when there was
no in-store demonstration (see Table 8), two significant interactions were found in their ability to predict attention being paid to
an end-of-aisle display: gender and movement, and shopping
device and movement.
The purpose of this study was to establish whether having an
in-store product demonstration situated near an end-of-aisle affects shoppers' attention to the end-of-aisle display. The results
shown in Table 2 clearly show (in the final two rows “end-of-aisle”
and “demonstration”) that the best way to attract attention to the
end-of-aisle is not to have an in-store demonstration anywhere
near it. This has clear implications for suppliers being charged for
these spaces yet clearly having concerns about erosion of investment and ROI. At present suppliers are investing in these spaces
with reported returns on their promotions, yet until now there has
been no research into the interaction between end-of-aisle and
demonstration. Our results suggest that supermarkets may have
incomplete information when reporting success of previous instore promotions.
However, assuming the store wants to deploy in-store demonstrations, and given the end-of-aisle space is the easiest place
for one, it is worth discussing some nuances from the results. First,
it is also clear that both in-store promotions have attention paid to
them when they are not competing with each other, but do not
have attention paid when they are competing. Thus, it would seem
that if a store is going to have a product demonstration, the
management should ensure they have a complementary end-ofaisle display. Management can add significant value and benefit to
their stakeholders by strategically planning these types of in-store
promotions. A supermarket which is aware of and actively engaged in such planning activities will create a mutually beneficial
relationship beyond the single in-store promotion.
Second, it is clear that when a store does have both (and they
are complementary), movement that is against the natural
movement pattern (clockwise in this case) is an important factor.
This makes sense if we think about the shopper going with the
store layout flow, easily moving around the end-of-aisles and
looking ahead to what is expected next; not noticing as much the
in-store promotions, whereas the shopper moving against the flow
is navigating around points not designed with them in mind. This
resonates with the Itti and Baldi (2009) and Baldi and Itti (2010)
suggestions around standing out and surprise. We see that attention to both the end-of-aisle and the in-store demonstration
promotions are affected by movement against the flow, and in-
146
M. Phillips et al. / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 26 (2015) 141–146
store demonstrations are also noticed when the travel is through
the aisle rather than around the perimeter. When there is no instore demonstration – so end-of-aisle promotion only – pushing a
shopping cart increases attention paid to the end-of-aisle promotion. Furthermore, when looking at the interactions, we see
that pushing a shopping cart up and down the aisles, and pushing
a shopping cart against the natural flow, increases attention paid
to the end-of-aisle when the two in-store promotions are complementary. Again, this notion of the shopping device being important (Cochoy, 2008) makes sense when we think of the shopper
with a shopping cart needing to negotiate the obstructions more
carefully, but also perhaps having more time for the overall trip so
more willing to explore promotions. Thus the supermarket and
their suppliers need to consider the profile of the shopper in terms
of how they move about the store and the shopping device used,
when employing in-store promotions.
We have shown that attention to in-store promotions is best
when “competing” promotions are actually for the same product–
so complementary, and not actually competing. Or, they can work
when the promotion is restricted to the end-of-aisle only. This is
important for the marketer working with the retailer, as spend on
marketing efforts that are disrupted by promotions competing for
attention is largely ineffective. The marketer is better to ensure
agreement that the end-of-aisle is maintained as a clear way for
promotional purposes, or to invest in complementary in-store
demonstration promotional activity when their product is on the
end-of-aisle display.
Our results clearly show that the effectiveness of the end-ofaisle display is diluted if there is a product demonstration occurring nearby. This has meaningful implications, as previously discussed, for suppliers, shoppers, and supermarkets. To maximise
the return on these in-store promotional activities, suppliers and
supermarkets must work together.
References
Abratt, R., Goodey, S., 1990. Unplanned buying and in-store stimuli in supermarkets. Manag. Decis. Econ. 11, 111–121.
Aggarwal, P., Vaidyanathan, R., 2002. Use it or lose it: purchase acceleration effects
of time-limited promotions. J. Consum. Behav. 2, 393–403.
Ajzen, I., 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Org. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50,
179–211.
Baldi, P., Itti, L., 2010. Of bits and wows: a Bayesian theory of surprise with applications to attention. Neural Netw. 23, 649–666.
Barat, S., Ye, L., 2012. Effects of coupons on consumer purchase behavior: a metaanalysis. J. Mark. Dev. Compet. 6, 131–144.
Bava, C., Jaeger, S., Dawson, J., 2009. In-store influences on consumers' grocery
purchasing decisions: a qualitative investigation. J. Cust. Behav. 8, 221–236.
Bell, D., Corsten, D., Knox, G., 2011. From point of purchase to path to purchase: how
preshopping factors drive unplanned buying. J. Mark. 75, 31–45.
Bezawada, R., Balachander, S., Kannan, P., Shankar, V., 2009. Cross-category effects
of aisle and display placements: a spatial modeling approach and insights. J.
Mark. 73, 99–117.
Borges, A., Chebat, J., Babin, B., 2010. Does a companion always enhance the
shopping experience? J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 17, 294–299.
Breugelmans, E., Campo, K., 2011. Effectiveness of in-store displays in a virtual store
environment. J. Retail. 87, 75–89.
Chandon, P., Hutchinson, J., Bradlow, E., Young, S., 2009. Does in-store marketing
work? effects of the number and position of shelf facings on brand attention
and evaluation at the point of purchase. J. Mark. 73, 1–17.
Chevalier, M., 1975. Increase in sales due to in-store display. Jo. Mark. Res. 12,
426–431.
Cochoy, F., 2008. Calculation, qualculation, calqulation: shopping cart arithmetic,
equipped cognition and the clustered consumer. Mark. Theory 8, 15–44.
Curhan, R., 1974. The effects of merchandising and temporary promotional activities on the sales of fresh fruit and vegetables in supermarkets. J. Mark. Res. 11,
286–294.
Dulsrud, A., Jacobsen, E., 2009. In-store marketing as a mode of discipline. J. Consum. Policy 32, 203–218.
Fader, P., Lodish, L., 1990. A cross-category analysis of category structure and promotional activity for grocery products. J. Mark. 54, 52–65.
Gedenk, K., Neslin, S., 1999. The role of retail promotion in determining future
brand loyalty: its effect on purchase event feedback. J. Retail. 75, 433–459.
Gilbert, D., Jackaria, N., 2002. The efficacy of sales promotions in UK supermarkets:
a consumer view. Int. J. Retail Distrib. Manag. 30, 315–322.
Haans, H., Gijsbrechts, E., 2011. . “One-deal-fits-all?”: on category sales promotion
effectiveness in smaller versus larger supermarkets.. J. Retail. 87, 427–443.
Heilman, C., Lakishyk, K., Radas, S., 2011. An empirical investigation of in-store
sampling promotions. Br. Food J. 113, 1252–1266.
Heilman, C., Nakamoto, K., Rao, A., 2002. Pleasant surprises: consumer response to
unexpected in-store coupons. J. Mark. Res. 39, 242–252.
Heiman, A., McWilliams, B., Shen, Z., Zilberman, D., 2001. Learning and forgetting:
modeling optimal product sampling over time. Manag. Sci. 47, 532–546.
Itti, L., Baldi, P., 2009. Bayesian surprise attracts human attention. Vis. Res. 49,
1295–1306.
Iyer, E., 1989. Unplanned purchasing: knowledge of shopping environment and
time pressure. J. Retail. 65, 40–57.
Kahn, B., Schmittlein, D., 1992. The relationship between purchases made on promotion and shopping trip behavior. J. Retail. 68, 294–315.
Lammers, B., 1991. The effect of free samples on immediate consumer purchase. J.
Consum. Market. 8, 31–37.
Laposky, J., 2007. In-store demos drive audio sales. Twice 22, 14.
Lawson, M., McGuinness, D., Esslemont, D., 1990. The effect of in-store sampling on
the sale of food products. Mark. Bull. 1, 1–6.
Lee, L., Ariely, D., 2006. Shopping goals, goal concreteness, and conditional promotions. J. Consum. Res. 33, 60–70.
Lemon, K., Nowlis, S., 2002. Developing synergies between promotions and brands
in different price – quality tiers. J. Mark. Res. 39, 171–185.
Major, M., 2002. Show and tell – and sell. Progress. Grocer 81, 68–72.
Mazumdar, T., Papatla, P., 1995. Effects of shopping basket size on price and promotion responses. Pricing Strategy Pract. 3, 16–27.
Mortimer, G., 2012. Toward a shopping typology of primary male grocery shoppers.
Int. J. Retail Distri. Manag. 40, 790–810.
Mortimer, G., Clarke, P., 2011. Supermarket consumers and gender differences relating to their perceived importance levels of store characteristics. J. Retail.
Consum. Serv. 18, 575–585.
Moses, L., 2005. Food city expands sampling program. Supermark. News 53, 43.
Nordfalt, J., Lange, F., 2013. In-store demonstrations as a promotion tool. J. Retail.
Consum. Serv. 20, 20–25.
Park, C., Iyer, E., Smith, D., 1989. The effects of situational factors on in-store grocery
shopping behavior: the role of store environment and time available for
shopping. J. Consum. Res. 15, 422–433.
Rossiter, J., Percy, L., 1987. Advertising and Promotion Management. McGraw-Hill,
Singapore.
Schindler, R., Berbaum, M., Weinzimer, D., 1987. How an attention-getting device
can affect quick choice among similar alternatives. Adv. Consum. Res. 14,
505–509.
Shi, Y., Cheung, K., Prendergast, G., 2005. Behavioural response to sales promotion
tools: a Hong Kong study. Int. J. Advert. 24, 467–486.
Spiekermann, S., Rothensee, M., Klafft, M., 2011. Street marketing: how proximity
and context drive coupon redemption. J. Consum. Mark. 28, 280–289.
Steinberg, S., Yalch, R., 1978. When eating begets buying: the effects of food samples on obese and nonobese shoppers. J. Consum. Res. 4, 243–246.
Suher, J., Sorensen, H., 2010. The power of atlas: why in-store shopping behavior
matters. J. Adver. Res. 50, 21–29.
Tifferet, S., Herstein, R., 2012. Gender differences in brand commitment, impulse
buying and hedonic consumption. J. Prod. Brand Manag. 21, 176–182.
Troy, M., 2005. The lure of in-store freebies. DSN Retail. Today 44, 54.
Underhill, P., 1999. Why We Buy. Simon and Schuster, New York.
Wilkinson, J., Mason, J., Paksoy, C., 1982. Assessing the impact of short-term supermarket strategy variables. J. Mark. Res. 19, 72–86.
Zwiebach, E., 2005. Marsh increase in-store sampling. Supermark. News 53, 24.