THE ROLE OF CONSTRAINTS IN GREEK COMPOUND FORMATION ∗
ANGELA RALLI & ATHANASIOS KARASIMOS
Abstract
In this paper, we address the issue of constraints in word formation. We claim that the
absence of derivational suffixes within Modern Greek compounds is due to the
operation of the so-called Bare-stem constraint, which applies to output
configurations. Our analysis builds on different types of compounds from Standard
Modern Greek and its dialects. However, we focus mostly on dvandva [V V]
compounds, which are unique in Modern Greek from all Indo-European languages.
We also discuss a limited number of counter-examples, and show that they are only
apparent exceptions to the operation of the constraint. We argue that most of them
result from a reanalysis procedure, or refer to lexicalizations and loan words, which
do not usually obey the rules of the language. Τhe paper also adds to the discussion
about the interaction between derivation and compounding. It is argued that the two
processes intermingle in such a way that compounding cannot be treated separately
from derivation. This conclusion is advocated by the postulation of a morphologically
proper constraint restricting the form of compounds with a derived item as left-hand
constituent, as well as by the unclear order according to which the two processes
occur.
Key words: Constraints, dvandva compounds, derivation, morphology, Greek.
1. Introduction
Constraints constitute an efficient device for restricting grammars and filtering out
ungrammatical structures. They have become popular in phonological theory, and
occupy a prominent position in the constraint-based framework of Optimality
Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993). However, constraints may also apply to the
other levels of grammar, for instance, to morphology, where they may elucidate why
certain word structures are possible while other structures are not. Although there is
∗
Parts of the paper were presented at the 136th International Conference of the Linguistic
Society of Japan (Tokyo, Gakushuin University: June 2008), and at the Meeting Words don’t
come easy (Verona, Università degli Studi di Verona: November 2008). The authors thank the
audiences of both meetings for constructive remarks, as well as Geert Booij for his precious
comments. The paper has already been published in the Japanese Journal Gengo Kenkyu 135
(2009) and also appears in Lingue e Linguaggio (2009).
Patras Working Papers in Linguistics, Vol.1 (2009)
Special Issue: Morphology
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
no extended literature on this topic in morphology, hints about the operation of
constraints can be found in Rainer (2000), where he mentions the occurrence of
morphological restrictions on the input, which he calls ‘rule- or process- specific
constraints’.
In this paper, we deal with the operation of constraints on compounding. In
particular, we propose the existence of a constraint which affects the form of
Modern Greek (hereafter Greek) compounds with a derived item in the left-hand
position. By dealing with data that involve the application of both derivation and
compounding, we also add the discussion about the interaction of these two
processes, an interesting topic in the recent literature (see, among others, Bauer
2005, Booij 2005, ten Hacken 2000, Ralli and Dimela to appear, etc.). We provide
evidence in favor of the thesis that compounding intermingles with derivation in
such a way that it cannot be treated separately from affixation, contrary to Anderson
(1992) who proposes that compounding should be taken care of by syntax.
The paper is structured as follows. We start by presenting the general
background and hypotheses about Greek compounding, its order of application with
respect to derivation, and describe the problem of not having overtly realized
derivational suffixes within compounds even though these suffixes seem to affect
the semantic interpretation of these morphological constructions. Next, we propose
the existence of a constraint (the Bare-stem constraint), which requires the surfacing
of stems in the left-hand position of compound words to be as bare as possible.
Significant evidence for this constraint is provided by dvandva [V V] formations
which are described and analyzed in Section 4. A small number of counter-examples
is thoroughly examined in the subsequent section, where evidence is provided that
they do not constitute real counterevidence to the correctness of the constraint. In
Section 6, we return to the question of the order of application of derivation and
compounding, in conjunction with the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, in
order to show the close interaction of the two processes, a phenomenon which
requires a place of compounding within morphology. The paper ends with a
summary of our conclusions in Section 7.
2. Background and Hypotheses
Within a lexical morphology framework (Kiparsky 1982), Ralli (1988) has claimed
that in Greek derived items appearing in compounds are formed before
compounding takes place, and that the stratum/level of derivation precedes that of
compounding. This proposal seems to be borne out as far as the second member of a
compound word is concerned, which, in several instances, constitutes a derived item
belonging to one of the three major grammatical categories, noun (1a), verb (1b),
and adjective (1c):
2
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
(1)
a. nixokoptis 1
< [nix]-o-[kop-ti-s]
lit. nail cutter [nail]N-CM-[cutV-DERN-INFL(NOM.SG)]N
‘nail clipper’
‘nail’ ‘cutter’
b. krifoxorevο
< [krif]-o-[xor-ev-o]
lit. secretly dance
[secretly]ADV-CM-[danceN-DERV INFL(PRES.1P.SG)]V
‘dance secretly’
‘secretly’ ‘dance’
c. aksiaγapitos
< [aksi]-o-[aγapi-t-os]
‘worth loving’ [worth]A-CM-[loveV-DERA-INFL(NOM.SG)]A
‘worth’ ‘loving’
In the examples listed above, the two constituents are linked together by a
linking vowel –o- (‘compound marker’ for Ralli 2008a). The first constituent is a
morphologically simple stem, 2 while the second constituent is a derived item, which
contains a stem, a derivational suffix (-ti-, -ev-, -t-), and the appropriate inflectional
ending (-s, -o, -os). We assume that in these words, derivation occurs before
compounding, since compounds such as the verbal *nixokovo ‘cut nails’ (< nix(i) 3
‘nail’ + kovo ‘cut’), the nominal *krifoxoros ‘secret dance’ (< krif(os) ‘secret’ +
xoros ‘dance’), and the verbal *aksiaγapo ‘worth to love’ (< aksi(os) ‘worth’ +
aγapo ‘love’) are not generally acceptable for native speakers of Greek.
Corroborating evidence for the claim that the derivation of the second
constituent occurs before compounding is also provided by the position of stress: as
argued by Nespor and Ralli (1996), a derived word at the right-hand side of a
compound blocks the application of a compound-specific stress rule, which places
stress on the antepenultimate syllable of Greek compounds. Consider the following
examples:
1
The glosses should be read as: CM=compound marker, INFL=inflectional suffix,
DER=derivational suffix, NOM=nominative, SG=singular, PRES=present, 1P=first person,
SG=singular. See Ralli (2008a) for details about the compound marker, which does not show
up when the second constituent of the compound begins with a vowel, as in (1c).
2
A bare stem coincides with what is usually called ‘root’. Following Ralli (1988, 2005), we
assume that in Greek morphology there is no structural difference between a stem and a root,
since stems can be morphologically simple (in this sense, they correspond to roots), or
morphologically complex. The latter may contain derivational affixes (derived stems) or more
than one stem (compound stems). This position is also diachronically justified because
Ancient Greek stems were formed out of roots with the adjunction of a thematic vowel.
Today, thematic vowels have lost their original role and are not recognizable as distinct units.
See also Kiparsky (to appear) for the use of stem as the base for the formation of verbal
derivatives and compounds.
3
In this paper, segmental material, which is not relevant for the discussion, e.g. inflection of
the first constituent will be included in parentheses.
3
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
(2)
a. thalasodarménos < thálas(a) 4 dar-mén-os
vs *thalasodármenos
‘sea beaten’
‘sea’ beat-DER-INFL(NOM.SG)
‘beaten’
b. pagóvuno
< pág(os) vun-ó
lit. ice mountain
‘ice’ mountain-INFL(NOM/ACC.SG)
‘ice berg’
‘mountain’
We see that in (2a) the position of stress of the compound as a whole is identical
to the position of stress of the second member, which is a derived word. On the
contrary, the stress of the compound in (2b), which contains two morphologically
simple stems, falls on the antepenultimate syllable, that is on a different position
from that in the two members when taken in isolation.
However, the proposal that derivation precedes compounding is not confirmed
as far as the first constituent is concerned. As noticed by Karasimos (2001) and Ralli
(2007), usually derivational suffixes do not appear in the first constituent of
compounds, which is generally a bare (morphologically simple) stem. In the
examples listed below, the first constituent behaves like a derived item from the
semantic point of view, and its lexical category is not the one that is predicted by its
overt form. However, no derivational suffix is overtly realized:
(3)
Compound noun
Derived const. 1
a. sideroporta
siderN-eniaA
lit. iron made door
iron-DER
‘iron door’
‘iron made’
b. krifotragudo
krifA-aADV
lit. secretly sing
secret-DER
‘sing in secret’
‘secretly’
c. xoropidο
xorN-evVlit. dance – jump
dance-DER
‘jump like dancing, bob’ ‘dance’
Const.2
porta
‘door’
tragudo
‘sing’
pido
‘jump’
For instance, while a compound such as krifotragudo means ‘sing in secret’, a
semantic interpretation which reveals the presence of the adverb krifa ‘secretly’ in
the position of the first constituent, the form of this item is similar to the one of the
adjectival stem ‘secret’ (krif-), since it does not bear the adverbial suffix –a which is
usually added to adjectival stems in order to form adverbs. Similar considerations
apply to the other two examples, (3a) and (3c), as well.
The non-occurrence of derived items as first constituents of compounds could
be used as an argument against a linear ordering in which compounding follows
4
thálasa and págos are the forms of the fully inflected words in the nominative singular. In
this paper, stress is noted only if it is relevant for the argumentation.
4
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
derivation. Since we have already seen evidence in favor of the opposite ordering,
the question is why derivational affixes are absent from compound-internal
constituents.
Note that with respect to derivation, we restrict our attention to suffixation: it is
generally known that the derivational status of several prefixes is not clear, and that,
several prefix-like morphemes behave like the left-hand constituents of compounds.
Suffice it to mention the characteristics of stress subordination and categoryneutrality that are shared by the so-called Class II prefixes in English (e.g. pro- and
en- as in the words proclitics and enclitics), and the left-hand constituents of
compounds (see, among others, Stekauer 2005) 5 .
3. The Bare-Stem Constraint
In our opinion, a plausible answer to the question above should be looked for in the
operation of constraints on word structure. We would like to claim that the absence
of derivational suffixes within compounds is only superficial, and that it is
independent from the order according to which the processes of compounding and
derivation occur. We propose that derivational suffixes within the first constituent of
compounds become invisible because of the operation of a morphological constraint,
which applies to output configurations, and restricts the surface form of compounds
with derived items in the left-hand position. Let us call it the Bare-stem constraint.
We will see below that Greek compounds are generally subject to this constraint,
which modifies their structures by not permitting derivational suffixes to surface
word-internally, and requires the first stem component to be as bare as possible, i.e.
without any suffixal material 6 . Since constraints should not apply at random, but for
a particular reason, we further propose that the Bare-stem constraint ensures a better
cohesion of the internal structure of compounds, i.e. a strong structural bond
between their two basic components:
(4) BARE-STEM CONSTRAINT
The cohesion of a compound is better guaranteed if the first stem is as
bare as possible 7 .
We believe that the existence of this constraint is justified by the general
structure of the vast majority of Greek compounds that have a stem in the position of
the first constituent, i.e. an item with its inflectional ending stripped off, tightly
5
These two characteristics carry over to the corresponding Greek prefixes as well.
According to Booij (p.c.) a constraint according to which the left-hand constituent must be
simplex may also be found in certain compounding patterns of Dutch, which combine an
adjective with a noun or another adjective.
7
In an Optimality-Theory framework, this constraint should be ranked higher than the
faithfulness constraint, in order to make its effect visible in compound formation.
6
5
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
combined with a following stem or a word. As shown by a number of authors
(Drachman and Malikouti-Drachman 1994, Nespor and Ralli 1996, MalikoutiDrachman 1997, Revithiadou 1997, and Ralli 2005, 2007), with few exceptions,
Greek compounds are mainly built on two patterns: [stem stem] (5a) and [stem
word] (5b):
(5)
a. [stem stem]
ambeloxόrafo <
‘vineyard field’
b. [stem word]
domatosaláta <
‘tomato salad’
ambél(i)
‘vineyard’
xoráf(i)
‘field’
domát(a)
‘tomato’
saláta
‘salad’
The criteria according to which compounds are assigned to one of these
categories are the position of stress and the form of the inflectional ending.
Compounds which are subject to a compound-specific stress rule (that places stress
on the antepenultimate syllable), and inflect differently from their second
constituent, when used as an autonomous word, are assumed to have the [stem stem]
structure (see 5a). Those which preserve the stress and the inflectional ending of the
second constituent (in endocentric constructions this constituent has the role of the
head) are assumed to have a [stem word] structure (see 5b). Following the Structure
Preservation Principle, as proposed by Emonds (1985), Nespor and Ralli (1996)
have argued that the structure of a word constituent that appears in the position of
the right-hand head, is preserved in a compound, since it constitutes a fully specified
entity from all points of view: it is an autonomous item on structural grounds, one
phonological word, and it bears the appropriate morphosyntactic features that are
needed for syntactic purposes. Unlike words, stems have no structural autonomy, are
not complete phonological words, and are underspecified with respect to some
morphosyntactic features (e.g. case, number, person, etc.) 8 . Therefore, [stem stem]
compounds may display properties that do not belong to those of their members,
when these members are used as autonomous items. As an illustration, consider the
compound meronixto ‘day (and) night’, which shows a different gender value, a
different stress position, and a different inflectional ending from those of its two
components:
(6)
merόnixto
< mér(a)
lit. day-night.NEU
day.FEM
‘day and night’
‘day’
níxt(a)
night.FEM
‘night’
The fact that a stem, i.e. a non-autonomous constituent, appears at the left-hand
side of Greek compounds makes their internal structure to exhibit a stronger
8
6
Only gender is a fully specified feature of noun stems, as claimed by Ralli (1999, 2002).
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
structural cohesion than the internal structure of compounds which would have a
fully specified word as left constituent. We, thus, suggest that this desire for
structural cohesion justifies the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, which does
not allow for the overt presence of material other than the segments of the bare stem
in the first position of compounds.
However, the degree of internal structural cohesion may vary from one
compound type to another. It does not depend only on the morphological category of
the constituent parts (stem or word), but also on the kind of structural relation that
holds between them. For instance, there are compounds with a weak structural
relation between their members, the so-called loose compounds. The absence of a
strong structural bond between the constituents of loose compounds should not
normally forbid the overt presence of any suffixal material within their structure. If
this is not the case, the constraint finds robust support: it would prove that
requirements for internal structural cohesion hold across compounds, and apply even
to those whose members are not tightly bound.
4. Dvandva [V V] Compounds
Significant evidence for the Bare-Stem Constraint comes from the domain of
dvandva [V V] compounds, which are also called copulative or coordinative
(Bloomfield 1933), or co-compounds (Wälchli 2005). 9 These constructions are an
innovation of the language, since they did not exist in Classical Greek (5th-4th c.
BC). They are unique in Modern Greek within the family of Indo-European
languages, but are frequently used in the East and South East Asian languages as, for
instance, in Japanese (Kageyama to appear), Chinese (Packard 2000), Korean (Sohn
1999), and Vietnamese (Nguyen 1997). Dvandva [V V] compounds have appeared
during the late medieval period (around the 14th c. AD), as shown by Manolessou
and Tsolakidis (2007). They belong to the productive structures of Greek
compounds:
(7)
a. anigoklino
<
‘open – close’
b. anavozvino
<
‘switch on – switch off (the light)’
c. benovgeno
‘go in - go out’
d. trogopino
‘eat – drink’
anig(o)
‘open’
anav(o)
‘switch on’
< ben(o)
‘go in’
< trog(o)
‘eat’
klino
‘close’
zvino
‘switch off’
vgeno
‘go out’
pino
‘drink’
9
The term ‘dvandva’ comes from the Sanskrit tradition, but is adopted by a number of
linguists, including (Bauer 2008)
7
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
Structurally, these compounds combine a stem and a word (they are [stem
word] compounds, see Ralli to appear), and semantically, the two coordinated verbs
express compatible (often synonymous) or opposite meanings. According to the
semantic relationship that holds between the first and the second verb, dvandva [V
V] compounds can be classified into three groups, additive (8a), synonymic (8b) or
antonymic (8c), while most of the times it is difficult to distinguish additive from
synonymic ones. 10 If the two verbs are synonymous the compound denotes the joint
activity over some period (Kiparsky to appear), and one of the verbs is used to
reinforce the meaning of the other. On the other hand, compounds involving
antonymic verbs express an iterative alternation (Nicholas and Joseph 2007, to
appear, Kiparsky to appear), and occur more often than the constructions whose
constituents are of compatible meanings:
(8)
a. zimomagirevo
‘knead – cook’
b. klidomadalono
‘lock – bolt’
c. pigenoerxome
lit. go - come
‘come and go’
< zim(ono)
‘knead’
< klid(ono)
‘lock’
< pigen(o)
‘go’
magirevo
‘cook’
madalono
‘bolt’
erxome (iteration)
‘come’
As opposed to subordinative verbal compounds, for instance, [N V] formations
(e.g. afisokolo ‘stick posters’ < afis(a) ‘poster’ + kolo ‘stick’) and [Adv V] ones
(e.g. kalotroo ‘eat well’ < kal(a) ‘well’ + troo ‘eat’), which are generally rightheaded, in dvandva [V V] compounds it is not clear whether the second constituent
has the role of the head: the two internal members are of the same grammatical
category, they display parallel argument structures, and their meaning is a
conjunction of the meanings of their subparts. Since neither of the components
dominates the other, we could adopt Kageyama’s (to appear) suggestion about
similar Japanese constructions, that they are double-headed. However, the form of
their inflectional paradigm, that is their inflection class (IC), implies that the second
verb has a more prominent role, at least formally. When two verbs of different
inflection classes combine in order to form a dvandva [V V] compound, the
construction adopts the inflection class of V2. 11 As an illustration, consider the
examples vrodoastrafto ‘thunder - lighten’, from Standard Modern Greek, and
vromomirizo ‘stink - smell’, from the Asia-Minor dialect of Krini, in (9). In both
cases, the compound as a whole inflects according to the inflection of V2:
10
For Wälchli (2005: 137-139), additive compounds are the most prototypical.
Matsumoto (1996) has claimed that V2 is the head in Japanese dvandva compounds, since
it shows the inflectional pattern of the compound.
11
8
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
(9)
Compound. IC
a. vrodoastrafto.IC1
‘thunder – lighten’
b. vromomirizo.IC1
‘stink – smell’
V1.IC
vrod(o).IC2
‘thunder’
vrom(o).IC2
‘stink’
V2.IC
astrafto.IC1
‘lighten’
mirizo.IC1
‘smell’
The question, though, is whether headedness can be identified only on the basis
of the criterion of inflection class, since V1 and V2 have an equal status with respect
to the rest of their features.
Since headedness is not clear-cut in dvandva [V V] compounds, neither of the
verbs has a more prominent role over the other, and they express a conjunction of
events, we conclude that these formations display a weaker structural relation
between their components than that shown by compounds whose members are in a
subordinative (or even attributive) relation. 12 Additional proof for this conclusion
comes from the fact that dvandva compounds generally display structural and
semantic transparency, as opposed to subordinative and attributive compounds,
which are easy to lose structural transparency and to develop an unpredictable
meaning. Therefore, they could be considered as a kind of loose compounds.
Returning now to the issue of the Bare-stem constraint, we have seen in (3)
instances of its application to a number of subordinative (3b, c) and attributive (3a)
compounds. Nevertheless, as already stated, the existence of the constraint would be
better motivated if dvandva compounds are also submitted to its operation, since the
superficial absence of word-internal derivational suffixes would show that the need
for structural cohesion in compounds also applies to loose structures.
In fact, there are dvandva [V V] compounds, the first member of which does not
have any overt derivational suffixes. However, it has the meaning of a derived stem.
Consider the examples below, from Standard Modern Greek (SMG) and its dialects,
where this type of compounds really abounds. They are taken from Andriotis (1960)
and the Dialectal Data Base of the Centre of Modern Greek Dialects at the
University of Patras. The origin of each example is listed in parenthesis:
12
See Bisetto and Scalise (2005) for a classification of the compounds according to the
relation that holds between their basic components.
9
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
(10)
Compound
a. alonotherizo
‘thresh – reap’
b. klidabarono
‘lock – bar’
c. kuklustsipázumi
‘wrap up – cover’
d. magirukinónu
‘cook – pour’
e. kseromarenome
‘dry – wither’
Derived Const. 1 Const. 2
therizo (Crete)
< alonN-izVthreshing-DER ‘reap’
‘thresh’
abarono (SMG)
< klidN-onVkey-DER
‘bar’
‘lock’
< kuk(u)lN-ónV- stsipázumi (Lesbos) 13
hood-DER
‘be covered’
‘wrap up’
kinónu (Imbros)
< magirN-évVcook-DER
‘pour’
‘cook’
marenome (Skiros)
< kserA-enVdry-DER
‘wither’
‘dry’
Like in other typical dvandva compounds ([N N] and [A A] ones, see Ralli
2007, 2008b), in these examples, stem constituents like aloniz(o) ‘thresh’, klidon(o)
‘lock’, etc. are juxtaposed to words of the same category, in this particular case to
verbs, and express a compatible or an opposite meaning. It is important to note that
examples such as the ones reported in (10) do not constitute blends, and should be
distinguished from them. The segments that do not surface in these examples are
those of the derivational suffixes, which are normally attached to the first derived
constituent, when taken in isolation (with the appropriate inflectional ending). In
blends, on the other hand, portions of the two constituents may be subtracted, and
this subtraction may also involve segments of the stem, other than those of the
suffixal part. For instance, in Hatzidakis (1905-1907) and Koutita and Fliatouras
(2001), we find blends of coordinative verbs such as malafo ‘massage and touch’ (<
malas(o) ‘massage’ + psilafo ‘touch’), and korojelao ‘mock and laugh’ (<
korojδev(o) ‘mock’ + jela(o) ‘ laugh’). 14 Crucially, the derivational suffix, which is
not overtly realized in the dvandva compounds of (10), is responsible for the
grammatical category (verbal) and the semantics of the first constituent. In fact, it is
always present when the constituent is used as an autonomous word, as shown by
the examples in (11), where for clarity purposes, the word internal constituents are
separated by a hyphen, and their lexical category is marked:
13
The examples from Lesbos and Imbros are given in their dialectal phonological form,
where unstressed /o/ and /e/ become /u/ and /i/ respectively.
14
See Koutita and Fliatouras (2001) for detailed information on Greek blends, mostly with
respect to the dialects.
10
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
(11)
a. alonN-izV-o
threshing-DER-INFL (PRES.IP.SG)
‘I thresh’
b. klidN-onV-o
key-DER-INFL (PRES.1P.SG)
‘I lock’
c. magirN-evN-o
cook-DER-INFL (PRES.1P.SG)
‘I cook’
etc.
It is worth noticing that Andriotis (1960: 55) has tried to explain the nonappearance of the word-internal derivational suffix as a syllable erasure affecting
verbs with more than two syllables, since, according to him, disyllabic verbs are
easier to pronounce than trisyllabic ones. However, this is not always the case.
Andriotis himself notes that the use of trisyllabic verbal constituents in compounds
is not unknown in Greek. As an illustration, see, for instance, the examples
anigοklino ‘open-close’ < anig(ο) ‘open’ + klino ‘close’ and pigenoerxome ‘go come’ < pigen(o) ‘go’ + erxome ‘come’, etc. It is crucial to stress that the part which
is systematically absent from the examples of (10) is not any particular syllable, but
the derivational suffix itself. Therefore, dvandva [V V] compounds are affected by
the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, which applies to their structure in order to
maximize the bound between V1 and V2, and in spite of the fact that these
compounds constitute loose structures.
5. Specific Cases
In this section we examine a small number of compounds with internal derivational
suffixes, which are not affected by the Bare-stem constraint. We provide a detailed
study of these formations, and try to show that they do not provide counter evidence
to the application of the constraint.
5.1. The verbal suffix –en–
There are few counter-examples to the Bare-stem constraint, which do not allow for
any suffixal material within compounds, namely those containing the verbal stems
pigen(o) ‘go’ and ben(o) ‘go in’. These stems keep their -en- segments in formations
like pigenoerxome lit. ‘go - come’ ‘come and go’, pigenoferno ‘go - bring’, and
benovgeno ‘go in (and) out’ (12 b, c, d). As opposed to these formations, other
compounds with –en-, for instance, anevokateveno (12a) do not display an overt –
en-, as predicted by the operation of the constraint:
11
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
(12)
Compound
a. anevokateveno
lit. go up - go down
‘go up and down’
Derived Const. 1
< anev-en‘go up’
b. pigenoerxome
<
lit. go - come
‘come and go’
c. pigenoferno
<
lit. bring forth - bring back
‘bring forth and back’
d. benovgeno
<
lit. go in - go out
‘go in and out’
Const. 2
kateveno
‘go down’
pig-en‘go’
erxome
‘come’
pig-en‘bring forth’
ferno
‘bring back’
b-en‘go in’
vgeno
‘go out’
In order to explain the examples of (12 b, c, d), a solution would be to suppose
that –en- is a suffix in the case of aneven(o) ‘go up’ (12a), and as such, it loses its
overt form in compounding, while it has no suffixal character but is part of the
morphologically simple stem, in the cases of pigen(o) ‘go’ and ben(o) ‘go in’.
However, the question is whether there is any supporting evidence in favor of this
hypothesis.
It is important to note that –en- is not a category-changing derivational suffix
but rather a morpho-syntactic marker, since its main function is to add the [perfective] aspectual value to a verbal stem. Substantial proof for this interpretation
is offered by verbs which show –en– in the [-perfective] forms (e.g. in the present
tense), but have a stem form without –en– in the [+perfective] forms, for instance in
the past tense (aorist):
(13)
Present [-perfective]
a. anev-en-o
‘I go up’
b. pig-en-o
‘I go’
c. b-en-o
‘I go in’
Aorist [+perfective]
anev-ik-a 15
‘I went up’
pig-a
‘I went’
b-ik-a
‘I went in’
This explains why in the [+perfective] context morphologically simple stems
such as anev-, pig-, and b-, do not belong to a different grammatical category, and
do not have a different semantic interpretation from their correspondent stems
aneven-, pigen-, and ben- in the [-perfective] context. However, while a verb like
15
-ik- is one of the overt markers of the morphosyntactic-feature of [+perfective]. See Ralli
(1988) for more details.
12
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
aneveno ‘ go up’ is affected by the Bare-stem constraint, and shows only the bare
stem anev- (the one without the –en- suffix), when used as first constituent of
dvandva [V V] compounds, beno ‘go in’ and pigeno ‘go’ behave differently. In these
verbs, the stem forms ben- and pigen- are not only unaffected by the Bare-stem
constraint, but are also used in both the [+perfective] and [-perfective] contexts, in
spite of the fact that –en- is the [-perfective] marker. Compare the examples of (14a,
b, c, d) with those of (14e, f), where –en- is underlined:
(14) a. To pigenoferni/*pigοferni arketes fores prin apofasisi na mas
to xarisi
lit. it brings.back.and.forth several times before decides to us it give
‘(S)he brings it back and forth several times before (s)he decides
to give it to us’
b. To pigenoefere/*pigoefere arketes fores prin apofasisi na mas
to xarisi
lit. it brought.back.and.forth several times before decided to us
it give
‘(S)he brought it back and forth several times before (s)he decided
to give it to us’
c. Benovgeni /*bikovgeni apo to proi os to vradi
lit. (S)he comes.in.and.out from the morning till the evening
‘(S)he is coming in and out from morning to night’
d. Benovgike/*bikovgike arketes fores apo to proi
lit. (S)he came.in.and.out several times from the morning
‘(S)he came in and out several times from the morning’
vs.
e. Anevokateveni /*anevenokateveni ta skalia arketes fores ti mera
lit. Climbs.up.and.down the steps several times the day.
‘(S)he climbs up and down the steps several times a day’
f. Anevokatike/*anevenokatevike ta skalia arketes fores simera
lit. climbed.up.and.down the steps several times today.
‘(S)he climbed up and down the steps several times today’
With respect to beno ‘go in’, it is important to note that if –en- does not surface
(because of the Bare-stem constraint), the stem is reduced to one consonant b-. We
would like to suggest that in order to preserve its form integrity the particular stem
escapes the application of the constraint, and that the internal structure of the stem
[b-en] has been reanalyzed as a morphologically simple stem. As a result, a
compound like *bovgeno (< b-CM-vgeno) ‘go in (and) out’ is impossible, and benis used in the [+perfective] context as well (14d).
A reanalysis procedure reducing a morphologically complex stem to a simple
one seems to have been applied to the internal structure of the verb pigeno ‘go’ too.
13
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
In this way, we could explain not only why -en- appears inside dvandva [V V]
compounds, but also why the form pigen- is used in the [+perfective] forms of the
aorist, as the sentence of (12b) illustrates, in spite of the fact that –en- has been
described as a [-perfective] marker. Additional proof for this claim is offered by the
free alternation of pigen- with the bare stem form pa- in the paradigm of the present
tense of Modern Greek, where pa- does not contain any overt [-perfective] marker:
(15)
a. pigeno
b. pao
pigenis /
pas
pigeni /
pai
pigenume/
pame
pigenete /
pate
pigenun /
pane
‘I go’
‘you go’
‘(s)he goes’
‘we go’
‘you go’
‘they go’
If -en- in pigen- has lost its role as a [-perfective] marker, and its contribution to
the formation of the verb stem is not morpho-syntactically transparent, it follows
that it cannot be affected by the operation of the Bare-stem constraint.
5.2. The nominal suffixes
The validity of the Bare-stem constraint is also put into doubt by the presence of
certain nominal suffixes, which are found at the end of the first stem constituent of
nominal compounds. Consider the following examples, which display a wordinternal derivational suffix regardless of the operation of the constraint:
(16)
14
< kiniV-siN
move-DER
‘movement’
b. klistofovia
< klisV-tA
‘claustrophobia’
close-DER
‘closed’
c. aeriagοgοs
< aerN-iN‘gas-pipe’
wind-DER
‘gas’
d. agrotospito
< agroN-t(i)N‘farmer’s house’
land-DER
‘farmer’
e. anixtomialos
< anixV-tA‘open-minded’
open-DER
‘open’
f. ikonomikopolitikos < ikonomN-ikA‘economic-political’
economy-DER
‘economic’
a. kinisiotherapia
‘kinesiotherapy’
therapia
‘therapy’
fovia
‘phobia’
agogos
‘pipe’
spit(i)
‘house’
mial(o)
‘mind’
politikos
‘political’
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
The fact that these compounds are nominal, and that their left-hand stem
belongs to the nominal category, is crucial to our argumentation. As is the case for
nouns and adjectives, nominal compounds differ from verbal ones, in that they can
be loan-words or ‘calques’. 16 If compounds like those in (16) belong to a specific
register of words, and if only these compounds display a word-internal derivational
suffix, we could claim that they are not real counter-examples to the operation of the
Bare-stem constraint, which only affects ordinary Greek compounds, both verbal
and nominal, i.e. compounds which do not belong to a particular language register.
Depending on the origin and their structure, the examples in (16) are marked for
certain specific characteristics, which can classify them into three categories: a)
loans, calques and pure translations from other languages, b) compounds which keep
the word-internal derivational suffix in order to avoid a meaning confusion, and c)
compounds which originate from lexicalized phrases.
5.2.1. Words like kinisioθerapia (16a) and klistofovia (16b) are calques, or
translations of terms from other European languages, in this particular case, from the
English kinesiotherapy and the French claustrophobie. 17 As is well-known, the form
of loans and calques may deviate from the usual formations of the target language,
and thus, may not be affected by the Bare-stem constraint. In fact, kinisiotherapia
contains the compound-internal suffix –si, which also appears in the English
kinesiotherapy but without being identified as such in the source language.
Furthermore, at the moment of the adoption of the French term claustrophobie, the
latinate claustro- was translated into the Greek derived adjective klisto- ‘closed’,
which can be transparently analyzed into the verbal stem klis- ‘close’, the adjectival
suffix –t– and the compound marker/linking element –o–.
With respect to (16f), we should point out that the violation of the Bare-stem
constraint is not due to the specific type of –ik–, since there are similar compounds,
i.e. dvandva [A A] ones, whose first component is a derived item in –ik–, and this –
ik– is not overtly realized. Consider the following examples, which display a
juxtaposition of ethnic names, and a flexible order between constituents:
(17)
a. anglogermanik(os)/
English-German
b. italorosik(os)/
Italian-Russian
c. rinolaringik(os)/
rhinolaryngic
germanoanglik(os)
German-English
rosoitalik(os)
Russian-Italian
laringorinik(os)
laryngo-rhinic
16
The existence of a big range of verbal compounds makes Greek distinct from other
European languages, where these formations are either rare or not productive (see Booij 1992,
among others).
17
According to the most recent Greek dictionaries, i.e. Babiniotis (2002) and Idryma
Triantaphyllidi (1998).
15
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
d. kiklokilindrik(os)/
cyclocylindrical
kilindrokiklik(os)
cylindro-cyclic
In these examples –ik– has no overt form when the constituent is at the left-hand
position, but is morphologically present when the same constituent is used as second
member of the compound. Therefore, the reason why there is –ik– in (17f) should be
searched elsewhere. Note that (16f) belongs to a small group of dvandva [A A]
compounds, like ikonomikopolitikos ‘economic-political’, politikokinonikos
‘political-social’, iθikoθriskeftikos ‘etchical-religious’, etc, which have been created
during the 19th century in order to fulfill specific scientific needs (see Babiniotis
2002). Like the examples of the previous category, some of them constitute simple
calques from French (e.g. fisikoximikos < Fr. physicochimique, attested in 1821
according to the Idryma Triantaphyllidi Dictionary), while others have been created
by analogy, more or less at the same period (e.g. politikokinonikos in 1825,
ikonomikopolitikos ‘economic-political’ in 1894). Again, words of this type, which
are constructed for specific purposes do not constitute sufficient evidence to cast
doubt on the validity of the Bare-stem constraint.
5.2.2. In certain formations, the presence of the derivational suffix seems to be
necessary in order to disambiguate the meaning of the compound. For instance, in
the examples aeriaγογοs ‘gas pipe’ (16c) and aγrotospito ‘farmer’s house’ (16d) the
alternative forms without the derivational suffixes –ti(s) and –i(o), would be
aeraγογοs and aγrospito, which are also possible in Greek, but have a different
meaning, ‘air-hole’ and ‘country-house’, respectively. Therefore, semantic
ambiguity can be avoided if the compounds in (16c, d) keep the suffix in their
surface morphological form.
5.2.3. The occurrence of the derivational suffix –t– within compounds like
anixtomialos ‘open-minded’ (15e), is restricted to cases where the first component
slot is filled by the deverbal adjective anixt(o) ‘open’. 18 We would like to propose
that compounds with anixt(o) as their first constituent originate from phrases, in this
particular case, from the phrase anixto mialo ‘open mind’, the structure of which has
undergone lexicalization, and, as is well-known, lexicalized structures may be
different from the ones which are built within morphology.
However, compounds with anixt(o) at the left-hand side are generally
considered to be structurally transparent exocentric formations, and their structure
can be analyzed according to the rules of Modern Greek compounding. Following
Ralli (2007), we further suppose that after lexicalization, items like (16e) have been
submitted to a structural reanalysis as compounds, analogically to other exocentric
compounds of a similar structure, i.e. to compounds containing the combination of
an adjective and a noun (e.g. oligomelis ‘few membered’ < oliγ(o) ‘few’ + mel(os)
‘member’). It is crucial to note though that this reanalysis has affected only the
18
Other similar occurrences with anixt- as first constituent are the examples of anixtoxeris
‘open- handed’, anixtokardοs ‘open-hearted’, and anixtomatis ‘open eyed’.
16
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
functional elements of the construction, i.e. the inflectional ending –o of the
adjectival word anixto ‘open’, which got reinterpreted as the compound marker –o–,
and the inflectional ending –o of the noun mialo ‘mind’, which was replaced by the
adjectival inflectional ending –os, as seen in (16e). Items with a lexeme status, such
as the verbal stem anix- and the noun stem mial-, as well as the derivational
adjectival suffix –t–, did not lose their identity. As a consequence, the derivational
suffix –t– is overtly present within the structure of the compound anixtomialos.
6. Ordering between Derivation and Compounding Revisited
In Section 1, we tackled the issue of the order of application of derivation and
compounding. In the subsequent sections, we showed that the absence of compoundinternal derivational suffixes is only apparent, since the non-surfacing of
derivational material is due to the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, which
renders invisible material other than the segments of the first component’s bare stem
in order to ensure a better structural cohesion between the two components of a
compound. Therefore, it may be misleading to conclude that the absence of
compound-internal derivational suffixes provides arguments in favor of an ordering
of compounding after derivation. Since we have seen examples advocating the
opposite order (see (1)), should we deduce that there is a linear order which requires
derivation to occur first? It is important to point out that there is no positive answer
to this question. On the one hand, there are derived words which feed derivation, as
shown by the examples in (1), but on the other hand, there are compound structures
which are subject to derivation. For instance, consider the adjectival compound
xartopektikos ‘gambling’ and the noun peδerastia ‘pederasty’. These words are built
on the basis of the combination of a compound stem with a derivational affix, as
depicted in (18):
(18)
a. xartopektikos
lit. card-playing
‘gambling’
b. peδerastia
lit. child-loving
‘pederasty’
< xart-o-pekt(i)-ik-os
card-CM-player-DER-INFL(NOM.SG)
‘card’ playing’
< peδ-erast-ia-Ø
child-lover-DER-INFL(NOM.SG)19
‘child’ ‘loving’
In (18ab), there are no actual de-adjectival words *pektikos ‘playing’ and
*erastia ‘loving’, which would imply a linear order in which derivation occurs
before compounding. Moreover, the existence of compounds like xartopektis
‘gambler’ (< xart(ia) ‘cards’ + pektis ‘player’) and peδerastis (< peδ(i) ‘child’ +
19
In this compound, there is a zero inflectional ending. Moreover, there is no compoundinternal marker –o- because the second member begins with a vowel. See Ralli (2008a) for
more details.
17
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
erastis ‘lover’) offers arguments in favor of the opposite order, i.e. compounding
preceding derivation.
It should be noticed that there are also occurrences of verbal compounds, like
alatopiperono ‘put salt and pepper’ (19a), where native speakers cannot take a clear
decision in favor of one particular order:
(19)
a. alatopiperono
< [[[alat-o-piper]-on]-o]
lit. put salt - put pepper
[[[salt-CM-pepper]-DER]-INFL(PRES.1P.SG)]
‘to salt and pepper’
‘to salt’ ‘to pepper’
b. alatopiperono
< [[[alat-iz]-o-[piper-on]]-o]
lit. put salt - put pepper
[[[salt-DER]-CM-[pepper-DER]]-INFL(PRES.1P.SG)]
‘to salt and pepper’
‘to salt’
‘to pepper’
c. alatopipero
< [[alat-o-piper]-o]
lit. salt-pepper
[[salt-CM-pepper]-INFL(NOM/ACC/SG)]
‘salt and pepper’
‘salt’ ‘pepper’
d. alatizo
alat-iz-o
‘to salt’
salt-DER-INFL(PRES.1P.SG)
‘to salt’
e. piperono
piper-on-o
‘to pepper’
pepper-DER-INFL(PRES.1P.SG)
‘to pepper’
In (19), the very frequent dvandva [N N] compound alatopipero ‘salt-pepper’
(19c) provides an indication for a subsequent derivational formation alatopiperono
‘to salt and pepper’ (19a), on the basis of the compound noun stem alatopiper- ‘salt
and pepper’ and the derivational suffix –on- (-o being the inflectional ending).
However, this is only an indication borne out by the dictionaries, which view the
derived verb alatopiperono as a secondary compound formation on the basis of the
primary nominal compound alatopipero. Theoretically, we could suppose that the
structure is built on the combination of two derived verbal stems, the most common
alatiz- ‘to salt’ (19d) and the less common piper-on- ‘to pepper’ (19e), a hypothesis
which would denote exactly the opposite order, according to which derivation takes
place before compounding, as in (19b). Moreover, in accordance with our
argumentation at the previous sections, we should also suppose that the structure is
affected by the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, which renders the overt form
of the derivational verbal suffix –iz– of the verbal stem alatiz- ‘to salt’ invisible
(17d).
To conclude, there is no clear evidence for an extrinsic linear ordering of the
two processes. We have seen that a derived item may be used either as first or as
second member of compounds, but the operation of the Bare-stem constraint hides
the overt form of derivational suffixes within their structure. We have also seen that
18
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
derived items can be created after compounding takes place. Thus, the interaction of
the two processes provides arguments for compounding being a word-formation
process, which should be accounted for in the same way as derivation, i.e. within
morphology. More crucially, the existence of a specifically morphological
constraint, the Bare-stem constraint, which has a specific domain of operation, i.e.
compounding, and affects specific morphological units, i.e. derivational suffixes,
stresses the close interaction of the two processes and also implies a morphological
account of compounding.
7. Conclusions
In this paper we have shown that there are morphological constraints that have an
impact on the form of morphologically complex items. We have proposed the
existence of the so-called Bare-stem constraint, which affects the output form of
compounds with a derived item in the position of the left component. In order to
preserve structural cohesion, this constraint renders invisible the derivational suffix,
and makes the stem component as bare as possible, even though its category and
semantics are those of a derived item. The few problematic examples that exist do
not provide sufficient evidence against the postulation of this constraint. Unless they
keep the derivational suffix for purposes of disambiguation, or to maintain integrity,
it is shown that these occurrences result from reanalysis or originate from foreign
formations and lexicalized phrases.
Finally, our paper comments on the place of compounding within the grammar.
By examining the order of application between derivation and compounding, in
conjunction with the operation of the Bare-stem constraint, we have shown the close
interaction between the two, which argues in favor of an account of compounding in
morphological terms.
Bibliography
Anderson, S. (1992) A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Andriotis, N. (1960) Ta parataktika rimatika sintheta stin Elliniki glossa [Coordinative verbal
compounds in the Greek language]. In Memory of Manolis Triantphyllidis.
Thessaloniki: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, 43-61.
Babiniotis, G. (2002) Leksiko tis Neas Ellinikis glossas [Dictionary of Modern Greek
language], 2nd ed. Athens: Centre of Lexicology.
Bauer, L. (2005) The borderline between derivation and compounding. In: W. Dressler, D.
Kastofsky, O.Pfeiffer and F. Rainer (eds.), Morphology and its Demarcations, 97108. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Bauer, L. (2008) Dvandva. Word structure 1: 1-20.
Bisetto, A. and S. Scalise (2005) The classification of compounds. Lingue e Linguaggio IV:
319-332.
Bloomfield, L. (1933) Language. New York: Holt.
19
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
Booij, G.(1992) Compounding in Dutch. Rivista di Linguistica 4: 37-61.
Booij, G. (2005) Compounding and derivation: Evidence for construction morphology. In: W.
Dressler, D. Kastofsky, O. Pfeiffer and F. Rainer (eds.), Morphology and its
Demarcations, 109-132. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Booij, G., C. Lehmann, and J. Mugdan (eds.) (2000) Morphologie/ Morphology. Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Drachman, G. and A. Malikouti-Drachman (1994) Stress and Greek compounding.
Phonologica 1992: 55-64.
Dressler, W., D. Kastovsky, O. Pfeiffer and F. Rainer (eds.) (2005) Morphology and its
Demarcations. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Emonds, J. (1985) A unified theory of syntactic categories. Dordrecht: Foris.
Hatzidakis, G. (1905–1907) Meseonika ke Nea Ellinika [Medieval and Modern Greek].
Athens.
Kageyama, T. (to appear) Isolate: Japanese. In R. Lieber and P. Stekauer (eds.) The Oxford
handbook of compounding, 512-526. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.
Karasimos, A. (2001) Allomorphy in inflection and compounding of Modern Greek. Ms.
University of Patras.
Koutita-Kaimaki, M. and A. Fliatouras (2001) Blends in Greek dialects: A morphosemantic
analysis. In A. Ralli, B. D. Joseph and M. Janse (eds.), Proceedings of the First
International Conference of Modern Greek Dialects and Linguistic Theory, 117130. Patras: University of Patras.
Kiparsky, P. (1982) Lexical morphology and phonology. In: The Linguistic Society of Korea
(ed.) Linguistics in the morning calm, 3–92. Seoul: Hanshin.
Kiparsky, P. (to appear) Verbal co-compounds and subcompounds in Greek. MIT working
papers in linguistics: MIT workshop on Greek syntax and semantics.
Leksiko tis Koinis Neoellinikis [Dictionary of Modern Greek Koine] (1998) Thessaloniki:
Idryma Manoli Triantaphyllidi.
Lieber, R. and P. Stekauer (eds.) (to appear) The Oxford handbook of compounding. Oxford /
New York: Oxford University Press.
Malikouti-Drachman, A.(1997) Prosodic domains in Greek compounding. In: G. Drachman,
A. Malikouti-Drachman, C. Klidi and J. Fykias (eds.) Proceedings of the 2nd
International Conference of Greek Linguistics, 87-96. Graz: Neugebauer Verlag.
Manolessou, I. and S. Tsolakidis (2007) Modern Greek coordinative compounds: Diachrony
and synchrony. Electronic Proceedings of the 8th International Meeting of Greek
Linguistics. Ioannina: University of Ioannina.
Matsumoto, Y. (1996) Complex predicates in Japanese: A syntactic and semantic study of the
notion ‘word’. Stanford: CSLI.
Nguyễn, Ð.-H. (1997) Vietnamese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Nespor, M. and A. Ralli (1996) Morphology–phonology interface: Phonological domains in
Greek compounds. The Linguistic Review 13: 357–382.
Nicholas, N. and B. D. Joseph (to appear) Verbal dvandvas in Modern Greek. MIT working
papers in linguistics: MIT workshop on Greek syntax and semantics.
20
The Role of Constraints in Greek Compound Formation
Nicholas, N. and B. D. Joseph (2007). Verbal dvandvas in Greek: What, when and why?
Paper presented at the 8th International Meeting of Greek Linguistics. University of
Ioannina.
Packard, J. (2000) The morphology of Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Prince, A. and P. Smolensky (1993) Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative
grammar. Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. [Published by
Blackwell in 2004.]
Rainer, F. (2000) Produktivitätsbeschränkungen. In G. Booij, C. Lehmann, J. Mugdan (eds.)
Morphologie/ Morphology, 877–885. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Ralli, A. (1988) Eléments de la morphologie du grec moderne: La structure du verbe. Ph.D.
Diss. Montréal: Université de Montréal.
Ralli, A.(1992) Compounds in Modern Greek. Rivista di Linguistica 4: 143-174.
Ralli, A. (1999) Inflectional features and the Morphological Module Hypothesis. Working
Papers in Theoretical and Applied Linguistics 6: 111-142. Thessaloniki: English
Dept. of Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.
Ralli, A.(2002) The role of morphology in gender determination. Linguistics 40: 519-551.
Ralli, A. (2005) Morfologia [Morphology]. Athens: Patakis.
Ralli, A. (2007) I sinthesi lekseon : Diaglosiki morfologiki prosengisi [Compounding : A
cross-linguistic morphological approach]. Athens: Patakis.
Ralli, A.(2008a) Compound markers and parametric variation. Language Universals and
Typology and Universals (STUF) 61: 19-38.
Ralli, A. (2008b) Greek dvandva [V V] compounds: A linguistic link between Greece and
East/South-East Asia. Paper presented at the 136th Meeting of the Linguistic Society
of Japan. Tokyo: Gakushuin University.
Ralli, A.(to appear) Hellenic compounds. In Rochelle Lieber and Pavol Stekauer (eds.), The
Oxford handbook of compounding, 243-253. Oxford/New York: Oxford University
Press.
Ralli, A. and E. Dimela (to appear) On the borderline between prefixation and compounding:
The sa-adverbs in the dialectal varieties of Lesbos, Kydonies (Aivali) and
Moschonisia. In M. Janse. B. D. Joseph, P. Pavlou and A. Ralli (eds.) Proceedings
of the 3rd International Conference of Modern Greek Dialects and Linguistic
Theory. Nicosia.
Revithiadou, A. (1997) Prosodic domains in Greek compounding. In: G. Drachman, A.
Malikouti-Drachman, C. Klidi and J. Fykias (eds.) Greek Linguistics 95.
Proceedings of the 2nd International Conference on Greek Linguistics, 107-116.
Graz: Neugebauer Verlag.
Sohn, H.-M. (1999) The Korean language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stekauer, P. (2005) Compounding and affixation: Any difference? In: W. Dressler, D.
Kastovsky, O. Pfeiffer and F. Rainer (eds.), Morphology and its Demarcation, 151–
159. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Ten Hacken, P. (2000) Derivation and compounding. In: G. Booij, C. Lehmann, and J.
Mugdan (eds.) Morphology / Morphologie, 349-359. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
21
Angela Ralli & Athanasios Karasimos
Wälchli, B. (2005) Co-compounds and natural coordination. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
ANGELA RALLI
CENTRE OF MODERN GREEK DIALECTS
DEPARTMENT OF PHILOLOGY
UNIVERSITY OF PATRAS
UNIVERSITY CAMPUS, 26504, RIO
GREECE
[email protected]
22
ATHANASIOS KARASIMOS
CENTRE OF MODERN GREEK DIALECTS
DEPARTMENT OF PHILOLOGY
UNIVERSITY OF PATRAS
UNIVERSITY CAMPUS, 26504, RIO
GREECE
[email protected]