New Evidences on the Disposition Effect of Individual Investors
Cristiana Cerqueira Leal
Lecturer in Finance
Management Department
Minho University - Portugal
[email protected]
Phone: +351-253-60-4561
Fax: + 351-253-28-4729
Manuel J. Rocha Armada
Professor of Finance
Management Department
Minho University - Portugal
[email protected]
Phone: + 351-253-60-4555
Fax: + 351-253-28-4729
João L. C. Duque
Professor of Finance
ISEG – Lisbon - Portugal
[email protected]
Phone: + 351-21-392-5955
Fax: + 351-21-392-2808
This version: March 2006
New Evidences on the Disposition Effect of Individual Investors
Abstract
Financial theory has identified the tendency of investors to hold loosing investments too long
and sell winning ones too soon. This tendency was denominated the disposition effect by
Shefrin and Statman (1985). This research provides evidence of the disposition effect on the
Portuguese stock market, by studying a unique database that consists on trading records of
1496 individual investors. The preference for realising gains to losses was observed every
month of the year and for all individual investors. Even at the end of the fiscal year, the
disposition effect still holds (in spite of the existence of fiscal incentives for the so-called
fiscal effect), as opposed to the evidence found in other markets. We also found that more
sophisticated investors (classified on the basis of frequency of transaction, volume and
portfolio value), are less prone to the disposition effect than less sophisticated ones. We also
identified differences as to the disposition effect in terms of its intensity, when considering
different market trends. In bull market periods, the disposition effect is even more evident
than in bear markets and significant in both markets.
2
New Evidences on the Disposition Effect of Individual Investors
1. Introduction
Financial theory has identified the tendency of investors to hold loosing investments too long
and sell winning ones too soon. This tendency was denominated the disposition effect by
Shefrin and Statman (1985). According to several financial theories, buying and selling
decisions should be taken based on price expectations. Then, comparing historical prices,
namely acquisition prices, with current prices is not a rational criteria for deciding to hold or
to sell. If market efficiency holds, even in its weak form, past prices should not be relevant to
resource allocation decisions.
The main aim of this study is to analyse the disposition effect of individual investors using the
Portuguese stock market and analysing a unique database of 1496 Portuguese individual
investors’ trades, from 1st January 1999 to 31st December 2002 (159 406 trades).
We can state four central questions to which we intend to answer in this study:
- Are Portuguese individual investors prone to the disposition effect?
- Does the fiscal effect reduce or invert the effect?
- Are there significant differences in the disposition effect motivated by market tendency, i.
e., is the disposition effect level different for bull and bear markets?
- Which investors are more prone to the disposition effect, that is, does investors’
sophistication affect the disposition effect?
We found the disposition effect for the entire period of the study, even in the end of fiscal
year, suggesting that fiscal effect seems to have no significant impact in investors’
preferences. We also found that in bull markets the disposition effect is stronger than in bear
markets and that more sophisticated investors are less prone to the disposition effect.
This paper brings new evidence about disposition effect. Firstly, as we know, Portuguese
market is the first studied market where fiscal effect does not reduce disposition effect by the
3
end of fiscal year, despite the incentives to do so. The legislation is not homogeneous for the
investors within the sample and there were changes in fiscal law during the sampling period.
Even tough, the principal explanation is that mental account plays a role that blinds investors
for the real reasons for realising losses: they want to close up the year with good performance
and do not take taxes into consideration.
Then, we relate disposition effect with market mood. Kim and Nofsinger (2002) analyse the
behaviour and performance of individual investors in Japan and found that trading behaviour
varies depending on bull or bear market conditions. Based on this evidence we aim to analyse
if also disposition effect depends on market mood.
The disposition effect found in the Portuguese stock market is stronger than in other studied
markets. This raises the question in which way Portuguese investors are different form other
markets in a way that explains these difference. Several studies that take into consideration
several classes/groups of investors give indications that investors do not exhibit disposition
effect in the same degree. Grinblatt and Keloharju (2000) show that the selling behaviour is
associated with the investor’s sophistication level and with their investment size. The most
sophisticated investors have larger investments and pursue momentum strategies which
results in superior performance. Moreover, Odean (1998) found differences in disposition
effect for frequent and infrequent traders. Based on this evidence, we also intend to analyse if
investors’ sophistication has impact in the disposition effect.
This paper is organised as follows: first we introduce the nature of the disposition effect as
well as some literature on previous studies. Then we present the database that was used in this
study. It follows a presentation of the methodology and a discussion of the empirical results.
We conclude with a summary of the paper.
2. The Disposition Effect and Previous Studies
Disposition effect concerns to patterns of gains and losses realization. The explanations to the
preference for realising winners than losers are generally based on behavioural factors
emerging from Kahneman and Tversky’s (1979) “Prospect Theory” and Thaler’s (1985)
“Mental Accounting Theory”.
4
Prospect theory describes how people evaluate their wealth based in psychological aspects.
Within this framework, the most important to human perception is the difference of stimulus
rather than their absolute magnitude, as alleged by Expected Utility Theory. This means that
perception is a relative processus rather than an absolute one. According to the Prospect
Theory individuals codify their wealth changes in terms of gains and losses using a reference
point. Results above the reference point are seen as gains while results below are seen as
losses. Usually, the reference point is the current status of wealth and a change in the
reference point has an impact on the classification of gains or losses. It can also affect options
formulation and decision making.
According to the Prospect Theory, individuals also exhibit decreasing sensitivity to outcomes.
This means that when gains or losses are distant from the reference point they lose
significance. Once investors usually take the acquisition price as its reference point,
accounting gains and losses anchored on it, the first euro gained will be recognised as being
more valuable than the second, the second one as being more valuable than the third one, and
so on. Identically, the pain associated with the loss of first euro will be recognised as being
stronger than the loss of the second one, the second stronger than the third, and so on. As a
result of this approach, marginal stock price increases of the same magnitude will provide
decreasing marginal recognised value.
On the contrary, when stock prices fall below the reference point, the first loss represents a
greater pain than the subsequent ones. This non-linear decreasing function will tend
asymptotically to a flat region of the recognised value function. However, this recognised
value function changes in shape if prices restart raising after an initial fall. When prices restart
raising a new reference point is established in the investor’s mind. A new and reshaped
recognised value function branch, with a positive but declining marginal recognised value
starts. Therefore, while additional price decreases will still be treated through the original
value function being and considered less penalising because they are placed in a flatter region
of the value function, the same price decrease following a significant raise results into a very
different story. With the change of the reference point to a higher value we would observe a
shift on the recognised value function. The decrease after the raise would now coincide with a
high sensitivity area of the recognised value function Investors would than exhibit an
5
increasing sensitivity to recovery with a stepper zone of value function. This explains why
investors prefer to realize gains and to defer losses.
They prefer to realize a gain because marginal gains are recognised as less valuable that
possible marginal losses of the same amount. On the contrary, when they are in the losses
zone, additional losses will not be recognised so painfully, while a possible recovery has a
greater value. Investors are not very sensitive to additional losses but are very sensitive to
possible price recoveries.
Another fundamental aspect of disposition effect analysis relates to the investors’ nonportfolio approach. Thanks to the mental accounting process individuals segregate wealth in
numerous accounts Investors tend to keep them separated by mental barriers and exhibit
different behaviour for each account. The disposition effect, based on mental accounting,
considers each stock individually treated instead of considering them a part of a portfolio.
When a new stock is bought, a new mental account is opened with its reference point, from
which gains and losses are calculated and disinvestment decisions are made. While stocks are
hold gains or losses are not considered real.
The disposition effect has been found for many markets and data periods. Based on US
investors’ trading records, Schlarbaum, Leweleen and Lease (1978) found evidence for retail
brokerage clients from 1964 to 1970; Odean (1998) found evidence to discount brokerage
clients from 1987 to 1993 and Locke and Mann (1999) document the same behaviour to
professional futures traders in 1995. The disposition effect was also identified for home
buyers and sellers by Case and Shiller (1998) and Genesove and Mayer (2001). This
preference was also found in other markets, namely, by Brown, Chappel, Rosa and Walter
(2002) in Australian Stock Exchange, and Shapira and Venezia (2001) among professional
investors in Israel. Grinblatt and Keloharju (2000) found that Finland’s domestic investors
pursue contrarian behavior and Kim and Nofsinger (2002) found a preference for selling past
winners in Japan, which is consistent with being disposition-prone.
In terms of measurement methods we can identify three approaches based on the data format
used: market data; portfolio data and experimental markets.
6
The methods based in market data (market perspective) compare volume and market prices’
changes (e.g.: Dyl (1977); Lakonishok and Smith (1986); Ferris, Haugen and Makhija (1988)
and Kaustia, 2000). In general, the purpose is to identify whether volume changes are
motivated by winners or losers. If disposition effect holds, it is expected higher volumes for
bulish than for bearish times, which means the existence of a preference of winners to sell.
The methods based in portfolio data (investor perspective) allow a deeper and accurate
analysis. It turns possible to look in detail into each investor portfolio and check whether the
stocks sold are the winning or the loosing ones (e.g.: Schlarbaum, Leweleen and Lease
(1978); Odean (1998) and Brown, Chappel, Rosa and Walter, 2002).
Finally, the methods based on experimental markets attempt to reproduce the stock trading to
assess the preference for holding loosing investments while selling winning ones (see Weber
and Camerer (1998); Chui (2001) and Oehler, Heilmann, Lager and Oberlander, 2002).
3. Data
This study is based on a unique database of 1496 individual investors’ accounts with detailed
data on their registered trades. The data set goes from 1st January 1999 to 31st December 2002
and comprises 159 406 trades. In order to ensure that the accounts represent the entire stock
portfolio for each investor, we only consider investors that trade exclusively in Portuguese
market. Otherwise we would, eventually, be considering partial accounts. According to
CMVM (2003) the national market represents the main destiny of the security investment for
Portuguese investors (94.3%).
The data was provided by a discount brokerage firm with a market share of about 1% of
Portuguese market in the sampling time period. The analysis considers 1496 investor accounts
that trade at least once in the sampling time period. The data is composed by initial value
positions, initial quantity positions, account movements both in value, and in volume, events,
and daily closing stock prices. The files include all movements that occurred in each account
within the time period. We have excluded non security data, namely, on bonds and on
warrants. We excluded accounts of investors that trade derivatives, since these could act on
the underlying asset for hedging or arbitrage purposes that would undermine our analysis. The
7
most important files for the analysis are account movements in value and account movements
in quantity. The database includes 159 406 trades (81 914 executed buys and 77 492 executed
sells). This means an average of 106.6 trades per account for the entire period and an average
of 159.4 trades per trading day for the entire set of accounts.
Based on this information, we have reconstructed each investor account for each day of the
sampling period. We have net all trades on the same day and asset for the same investor and
ignored all sells to which it was not possible to identify the purchase date and its price
(purchases before 1st January 1999, because lack of information). We have also corrected the
data for stock splits, mergers and acquisitions.
4. Methodology
We start by a simple test checking whether Portuguese individual investors exhibit the
disposition effect (as defined by Odean (1998)). If this is true, we expect to find empirical
evidence that the Proportion of Gains Realized is superior to the Proportion of Losses
Realized in the sampling period. In order to accept this hypothesis, we will test the null
hypothesis under which the proportion of investors’ realised gains isn’t greater than realized
losses, that is:
H0: Proportion of Gains Realized ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized.
H1: Proportion of Gains Realized > Proportion of Losses Realized.
Then we analyse if the disposition effect is affected by the fiscal effect. If the fiscal effect
holds in the Portuguese market, in December investors would exhibit a preference for
realizing more losses than gains and this would be rather distinct from the other months of the
year calendar. However, and contrary to the evidence in other markets, if hedonic reasons
hold we would expect opposite evidence. If investors tend to realise gains to close up the year
with a good performance then, the fiscal effect if noticed, would not have enough significance
in order to reduce or invert the disposition effect. Consequently, we will test the hypothesis
that the pattern of gains and losses realization on December is not significantly different from
the other months of the year.
8
H0: Proportion of Losses Realized in December - Proportion of Gains Realized in
December > Proportion of Losses Realized from January to November - Proportion of
Gains Realized from January to November.
H1: Proportion of Losses Realized in December - Proportion of Gains Realized in
December ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized from January to November - Proportion of
Gains Realized from January to November.
These two tests for global disposition effect and for the difference on disposition effect for
data partitions were defined by Odean (1998) and are generally found in the literature that
studies disposition effect based on investors’ portfolio thereafter. On this basis, we will also
analyse the disposition effect related to the market mood and the disposition effect related to
investor’s sophistication.
In the literature Kim and Nofsinger (2002) based on market data analyse the behaviour and
performance of individual investors in Japan and found that trading behaviour varies
depending on bull or bear market conditions. Then, it is relevant to study whether disposition
effect is affected by market mood. Once our analysis is based on investor portfolio data the
behaviour measure will be much more reliable. Then, the third question that we intent to
address relates the disposition effect with market mood. There are more opportunities of
gains realisation during bull periods. Therefore, we should notice this fact in our database, if
disposition effect is dependent upon the market mood. The measure of the disposition effect
we used, considers the realization of gains and losses compared to the opportunities that exists
due to market tendency. Consequently, the methodology is suitable to measure differences in
bull and bear periods.
We will assume bull periods those where daily market capitalization is consistently going up
and bear periods those where the daily market capitalization is consistently going down.
Then, the bull market period is from 1st January 1999 to 3rd March 2000 and the bear market
period is from 4th March 2000 to 31st December 2002.
Then the third hypothesis compares the disposition effect in each period:
H0: Proportion of Losses Realized in bull periods - Proportion of Gains Realized in
bull periods > Proportion of Losses Realized bear periods - Proportion of Gains
Realized on bear periods.
9
H1: Proportion of Losses Realized in bull periods - Proportion of Gains Realized in
bull periods ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized in bear periods - Proportion of Gains
Realized in bear periods
One could expect that in bull periods the disposition effect should be attenuated or even
inverted if investors follow momentum strategies. Nevertheless, it is harder to realise losses
during bull periods as a result of behavioural reasons. When prices tend generally to raise,
losses realisation affects investors’ confidence and in order to hide mistakes investors tend to
keep loser investments hoping their recovery.
Finally, we will also analyse whether individual investors are equally prone to the disposition
effect whatever the level of sophistication they show is. We try to identify the sophistication
by segregating investors into different groups according to the number of trades they execute,
the trading volume that their trades involve and according to the portfolio value (wealth).
Some studies give indications that different groups of investors have different degrees of
disposition effect. Grinblatt and Keloharju (2000) show that the most sophisticated investors
have larger investments and pursue momentum strategies while less sophisticated ones follow
contrarian strategies. This suggests that less sophisticated investors should denote stronger
disposition effect. Dhar and Zu (2002) found that wealthier investors and investors with
trading experience exhibit less disposition effect. Furthermore, Odean (1998) found intensity
differences in disposition effect for frequent and infrequent traders even though booth groups
show preference for sell winning investments. Based on this, we will analyse the impact of
investors’ sophistication in the disposition effect. Finally, Brown, Chappel, Rosa and Walter
(2002) found that more sophisticated investors (considering value of transaction as
sophistication criteria) express lower disposition effect, although even large traders prefer
hold their losing investments and sell their winning ones.
The segregation by the number of trades allows the comparison of investors based on their
trading frequency. We assume that more active investors tend to be more sophisticated.
However, this figure ignores the trading volume per trade, which means that trading one share
would equal the significance of trading one thousand. In order to take theses differences into
account we will also test differences in behaviour based on the number of shares traded,
assuming that more sophisticated traders tend to present higher trading volume per trade.
10
Finally we also use the average portfolio value within the sampling period which may be,
probably, the best criterion to identify investors’ sophistication.
For each criterion, we will test if there are significant differences in the disposition effect by
dividing investors into two groups. This means the following hypothesis:
A) Using the number of trades:
H0: Proportion of Losses Realized by low frequency traders - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low frequency traders ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized by high frequency
traders - Proportion of Gains Realized by high frequency traders.
H1: Proportion of Losses Realized by low frequency traders - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low frequency traders > Proportion of Losses Realized by high frequency
traders - Proportion of Gains Realized by high frequency traders.
B) Using the trading volume per trade:
H0: Proportion of Losses Realized by low volume traders - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low volume traders ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized by high volume
traders - Proportion of Gains Realized by high volume traders.
H1: Proportion of Losses Realized by low volume traders - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low volume traders > Proportion of Losses Realized by high volume
traders - Proportion of Gains Realized by high volume traders.
C) Using the trading volume per trade:
H0: Proportion of Losses Realized by low portfolio value - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low portfolio value ≤ Proportion of Losses Realized by high portfolio
value - Proportion of Gains Realized by high portfolio value.
H1: Proportion of Losses Realized by low portfolio value - Proportion of Gains
Realized by low portfolio value > Proportion of Losses Realized by high portfolio
value - Proportion of Gains Realized by high portfolio value.
As mentioned by Odean (1998), in order to test if the investors have the tendency for selling
winners too soon while holding losers too long, we have to take into consideration the impact
of market trends. In a bullish market, where the majority of share prices raise, , there are more
winning stock. Consequently, we observe more opportunities to sell winners than losers, even
though investors may be indifferent in selling winners or losers. On the contrary, in a bearish
11
market, with a large number o share prices falling, investors have more loser stocks in their
portfolios and, as a result, in the assumption that they are indifferent in selling winners or
losers, it is expected they sell more losers. Again, this may have nothing to do with their
human nature but is just the result of the market downward, creating more opportunities to
sell. Therefore, it is necessary to remove the impact of market tendencies. In order to detect
investors’ behavioural tendencies without the market effect, the disposition effect will be
identified and measured taking into account the selling of winners and losers relative to the
potential opportunities for selling winners and losers that are hold in their accounts.
We start by computing Realised Gains and Realised Losses. Realised Gains (RG) and
Realised Losses (RL) in a security i, in a specific account l, are calculated as the difference
between the selling price and the reference price (the average acquisition price for that
security).
RGi, l = S i, l − Ai, l > 0
RLi, l = S i, l − Ai, l < 0
where S i , l represents the selling price for security i in account l, and Ai , l represents the
average acquisition price of security i in account l. The average security price is a weighted
average considering the number of shares bought in each buying transaction. If an account
registered k buying trades on security i before being sold, with trades composed by n shares
bought at price P, the average security price become:
k
∑ n j Pj
Ai, l =
j =1
k
∑nj
j =1
If only one buying trade occurred, the average security price is the sole buying price
registered in that account for that specific security. Realised Gains and Losses are only
computed when a selling trade occurs for a security in an account.
Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) in an account are only calculated when a
selling trade is registered in that account. That is, we only care about Potential Gains or
Losses when a selling trade occurs. Potential Gains and Potential Losses are calculated as the
difference between the reference price (the average acquisition price) and the closing price of
the day when security i is sold:
12
PGi, l = Ci, l − Ai, l > 0
PLi, l = Ci, l − Ai, l < 0
where Ci , l represents the closing price for security i in account l on the day when security i
was sold.
As we did for Realised Gains and Losses, Potential Gains and Potential Losses are only
computed when a selling trade occurs for a security in an account.
We calculated Realized Gains, Realized Losses, Potential Gains and Potential Losses for each
day there is one or more sells in an account that has at least two securities and that does not
sell the entire portfolio in that day. Otherwise, we can not calculate Potential Gains and
Losses as there are no residual securities that could potentially be sold.
Then, we calculated the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses
Realized (PLR) as follows:
N
∑ RG
i
i =1
PGR =
l, t
M
N
∑ RGi + ∑ PG j
j =1
i =1
N
∑ RLi
i =1
PLR =
l, t
N
M
∑ RLi + ∑ PL j
i =1
j =1
where PGRl ,t represents the Proportion of Gains Realised in account l on day t, RGi stand for
the Realised Gain on security i on day t. As we only calculate Realised Gains and Potential
Gains when sells occur, N represents the number of securities sold in day t, and M represents
the residual number of securities kept intact in account l. The same methodology was used for
PLRl ,t that stands for Proportion of Losses Realised.
As in Odean (1998), we use t-test for testing the statistical significance of the differences in
the proportions PGR and PLR. A significant negative difference means that investors exhibit
a preference to hold losing investments and to sell the winning ones. In other words, if a
significant and negative difference is found, the disposition effect exists.
13
The standard error for the difference in the proportions PGR and PLR is given by:
σ (PRL − PGR ) =
PGR(1 − PGR) PLR( 1 − PLR)
+
N RG + M PG
N RL + M PL
Where N RG , M PG , N RL and M PL stand, respectively, for the number of Realized Gains,
Potential Gains, Realized Losses and Potential Losses.
In order to segment investors, we will divide the database in percentiles using the following
criteria: trading frequency; trading volume and average portfolio value in the sampling period.
The next question is which point should be used as the division point. We divided investors
into two different groups testing differences for the following percentiles: 50%, 75%, 90%
and 95%.
5. Discussion of Empirical Results
5.1. The Disposition Effect and the Fiscal Effect
We find evidences of strong disposition effect for the entire sample with a difference between
the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) of
about 0.21. Table 1 shows the results for PGR and PLR first for the entire year, and second
segregating results from January to November from those observed in December. Those tests
are based on the number of realized gains, number of realized losses, number of potential
gains and number of potential losses. These observations are aggregated for all investors and
throughout time, assuming that these observations are independent across investors and time.
14
Table 1 – Global Disposition Effect
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) from 1-1-1999 to 31-12-2002. The
number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across accounts and
days. T-statistics assume that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
Entire Year
25 891
21 629
55 553
176 975
December
2 334
1 363
5 597
12 829
Jan.-Nov.
23 557
20 266
49 956
164 146
0.109
0.318
-0.209
2.919
0.00178
-117.735
0.096
0.294
-0.198
3.064
0.00568
-34.881
0.110
0.320
-0.211
2.916
0.00187
-112.663
The differences for PGR and PLR are significant for the entire year. However, contrarily to
the empirical evidence from similar studies (such as Odean (1998), Brown, Chappel, Rosa
and Walter, 2002), we found the disposition effect even in the end of fiscal year. The
difference between PGR and PLR in December is 0.2, only slightly lower than the rest of the
year. When testing if the difference between PGR and PLR in December is significantly
different from that obtained from January throughout November (i. e., PLR-PGR in
December ≤ PLR- PGR in January - November), we get a t-statistic of 2.165. Therefore, we
conclude that this difference is not significant for a confidence level of 0.011. Moreover, the
preference to sell winners in December is three times superior to the preference to sell losers.
This is even higher than during the period from January to December. Then, we can conclude
that in the period, investors show disposition effect for the entire year, including December.
The difference found between PGR and PLR is clearly higher than the evidence of other
similar studies (such us Odean, 1998). We believe that this is due to the relatively low level of
sophistication of the individual investors. Even though the Portuguese stock market stoped to
be considered an emergent market, it still exhibits some characteristics of those markets,
namely higher volatility and longer time for crisis recovery. Investors in such markets tend to
reveal low sophisticated behaviour, particularly individual investors. Grinblatt and Keloharju
(2000) state that Finish investors, specially, individual ones behave with low sophistication,
being prone to contrary behaviour and consequently to the disposition effect.
1
Given the possibility of the test being inflated due to the lack of independence of the parameters, we demand a
high confidence level.
15
As opposed to the evidence found in other markets, we observed the disposition effect for the
entire year, even in December (the end of fiscal year), in spite of the existence of fiscal
incentives to engage in tax-motivated selling. It was already expected that fiscal effect had a
low impact in realised gains and losses first because the legislation is not homogeneous for
the investors within the sample, and second because there were changes in fiscal law during
the sampling period. Even if some investors decide to realize capital losses for tax purposes,
there are other investors which are not motivated by fiscal aspect and will prefer realize
capital gains at the end of the year to evidence improved performance. Obviously, these acts
result from mental accounting mechanisms that only consider gains those already realized.
Therefore, the fiscal effect and the disposition effect counterbalance themselves and the
pattern of gains and losses realization is not significantly modified in December, for the
Portuguese market. Another possible explanation is that investors do no take taxes into
consideration at all and consequently the pattern of gains and losses holds in December. As
can be observed in graph 1 there are no change in PGR/PLR in December. Notwithstanding
the taxation of capital gains, it is estimated that a larger amount of individual investors do not
declare capital gains. Even those who declare capital gains for taxation purposes are blinded
by mental accounting and do not consider this variable in gains and losses computation.
Although we found evidence of the disposition effect for the Portuguese individual investors,
it is relevant to investigate if it is constant during the sampling period. Table 2 reports the
PGR and PLR for each year in analysis. We found the same disposition effect for every year,
as we found for the entire period.
Table 2 - Disposition Effect for Each Year
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) for each year over the period from
1-1-1999 to 31-12-2002.The number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are
aggregated across accounts and days. T-statistics assume that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
1999
8 893
5 095
17 809
46 861
2000
11 354
8 334
24 508
70 442
2001
3 231
4 899
7556
35434
2002
2 413
3 301
5680
24238
0.098
0.333
-0.235
3.396
0.00317
-74.230
0.106
0.317
-0.211
2.993
0.00269
-78.378
0.121
0.300
-0.178
2.466
0.00470
-37.881
0.120
0.298
-0.178
2.487
0.00545
-32.722
16
Graph 1 details the evolution of the ratio PGR to PLR. The preference to realize winners is at
least 2.5 times superior to the preference to realize losers for the entire sampling period. The
ratio varies between 2.5 and 3.3. Once again, we observe how fiscal effect has little influence
into the pattern of the realization of gains and losses, showing a growing preference for
realising winners at the year end. February, July and August are the months that evidence the
highest ratio while March, April, September and October have the lowest. In terms of the
difference between PGR and PLR, for every month the values are closer to 0.2, corroborating
that, consistently, investors prefer realizing winners than losers.
Graph 1 – Evolution of PGR/PLR Aggregated by Month
December
November
October
September
August
July
June
May
April
March
February
3,50
3,00
2,50
2,00
1,50
1,00
0,50
0,00
January
PGR/PLR
This graph reports the ratio Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR)/Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) aggregated by month from 1-1-1999 to
31-12-2002. The number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated
across accounts and days.
These tests were developed under the assumption that the number of realized gains, the
number of realized losses, the number of potential gains and the number of potential losses
are independent across investors and time. Although this assumption does not bias the tests, it
can inflate them. As the t-statistics we get are very high this is not problematic to the analysis.
In spite of this, in order remove any doubt about the effect, we will, additionally look to an
alternative test.
Firstly, we started by assuming that independence only exists throughout time. In order to
overcome the problem with investors’ dependence, we calculate PGR, PLR and its difference
per investor. Then, we calculate the average PGR, and the average PLR and PLR-PGR and
we test the average difference. The results for this test are shown in table 3: the PGR is 0,57;
the PLR is 0.21 and the difference 0.36. By comparison to the previous test (table 1), we
17
conclude that in this alternative test both the proportions and the differences found are higher,
and so is the t-statistic. As a result, in this alternative test the null hypothesis was also rejected
with a t-test statistic of 184. The same happened with the December data, that is, the average
PGR is significantly superior to PLR, exhibiting a t-statistical even higher than that in
previous test.
Table 3 – Disposition Effect – Alternative Measure to Control Accounts Dependence
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) from 1-1-1999 to 31-12-2002. The
number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across days. The tstatistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions over time.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR)
σ(PGR)
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-estatistic
Entire Year
29.03
24.19
62.28
197.96
December
6.05
3.35
14.50
31.52
0.205
0.566
-0.360
2.755
0.17379
0.24148
0.00196
-183.985
0.167
0.499
-0.333
2.994
0.18999
0.31106
0.00643
-51.740
Surprisingly, the test to control the lack of investors’ independence reveals a stronger
statistical significance. The higher results for PGR, PLR and for the differences and,
consequently, the higher t-statistic, can be explained by the equal weight assigned to each
investor in this alternative measure. Each investor has the same weight in the calculation of
the disposition effect, independently of the high or low level of transaction in its account.
These results suggest that investors, which trade less frequently, exhibit stronger disposition
effect. When we weight them more heavily, PGR, PLR and its difference becomes higher.
This means that PGR and PLR are dependent of the investors to which they are calculated.
This fact can be considered critical to the alternative test, once that the accounts with high
frequency transaction provide more accurate estimative for the calculation of the proportions.
Nevertheless, this test satisfies its purpose of controlling if the statistic test is inflated and
shows that we can accept the conclusion provided by the initial test.
We also need to control the impact of decision dependence over time. For that purpose, we
will ignore consecutive sells. Whenever we find more than one sells within a week (five
18
consecutive trading days) on the same stock, only the first sell will be considered. In a similar
way, we only consider the first sell within the week to calculate potential gains and losses.
The test is done, once again, by calculating the PGR and the PLR for each investor and then
the average for all investors. Table 4 reports the results. We conclude that after controlling for
time dependence, the disposition effect is still evident and seems even stronger. The
difference between PGR and PLR is slightly higher than that observed in the previous test.
This procedure does not guarantee that decisions are independent over time. When investors
hold loser stocks the decision can be kept for a period longer than 5 days. Especially in longer
bear periods, one can expect that investors hold their loser investments for longer periods,
expecting to recover their losses in order to achieve the break-even. As we saw previously,
the satisfaction provided for this possibility is higher than the pain imposed by additional
losses. This is one of the reasons why investors accept risky bets. And in a difficult situation,
it is always preferable to hold the status quo, once a non-decision is not as painful as a wrong
decision.
Table 4 – Disposition Effect – Alternative Measure to Control Accounts and Time
Dependence
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) over the period from 1-1-1999 to 3112-2002. The number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across
days for each investor.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR)
σ(PGR)
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-estatistic
Entire Year
11.07
8.50
12.69
34.66
December
3.39
1.90
4.43
9.08
0.263
0.660
-0.397
2.510
0.22092
0.26941
0.00527
-75.369
0.247
0.583
-0.336
2.363
0.30918
0.32713
0.01872
-17.953
Up to the moment PGR and PLR are calculated on the basis of the number of sells and
potential sells. Even though, to measure the impact of investors’ dimension is pertinent to
calculate these proportions on the basis of volume and value of the traded stock.
19
When the number of sells and potential sells is taken as basis, all transactions are considered
equally important and they are used with the same weight when calculating our instrumental
variables. However, it is pertinent to ask whether the disposition effect is still persistent when
we take into consideration the number of stocks sold and the number of stocks that could have
been sold. Table 5 reports the results for PGR and PLR calculated on the basis of stocks
instead of singular sells. The proportions as well as its difference are now lower.
Nevertheless, the difference is still significant (t-statistic of 89). The lower PGR and PLR can
be explained in line with the explanations presented for the alternative tests. Investors with
higher transaction volume (and more frequent trades) are now strongly weighted and these
investors exhibit lower PGR and lower PLR as well as a lower difference between these
proportions. This means they are less prone to the disposition effect.
Table 5 – Disposition Effect Based on Volume
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) over the period from 1-1-1999 to 3112-2002. The volume of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across
accounts and days. The t-statistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
Entire Year
16 764 347
20 927 061
58 433 585
233 432 824
December
1 581 558
1 626 243
6 391 875
20 006 371
Jan.-Nov.
15 182 789
19 300 818
52 041 710
213 426 453
0.082
0.223
-0.141
2.710
0.00158
-88.835
0.075
0.198
-0.123
2.639
0.00499
-24.661
0.083
0.226
-0.143
2.723
0.00167
-85.551
Hypothetically, investors may realize gains and losses of significantly different amounts.
Therefore, we also calculated the PGR and PLR based on the value of gains and losses. This
is critical, because more import than a gain or loss should be its magnitude. If realized small
gains are frequent but big losses are seldom, the previous conclusions could be challenged.
Table 6 shows the results for value weighted PGR and PLR and the conclusions are similar to
previous, based on trading volumes: the PLR is 0.06; the PGR is 0.18 and its difference has a
t-statistical of 81.5. Considering the value of gains and losses we get lower figures for the
proportions. This is likely because investors with low amounts in transactions are more prone
to the disposition effect. Nevertheless, we keep concluding for the disposition effect for the
total data set and for every month, including December. Brown, Chappel, Rosa and Walter
20
(2002), when considering values of transaction, also get the same conclusions for individual
investors, although for other investors’ classes disposition effects disappears on this
calculation. This indicates that, when pondering strongly the larger investments, investors
with more experience/sophistication are more weighed and these are not so eager for gains
realization (the reduction in disposition effect comes specially from the reduction of PGR,
when compared with volume).
Attending the monthly progress of the ratio PGR/PLR
calculated on the basis of volume and value, we keep concluding that the preference for
selling winners is always, at least, twice than the preference for selling losers.
Table 6 – Disposition Effect Based on Value
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) over the period from 1-1-1999 to 3112-2002. The value of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across
accounts and days. The t-statistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
t-statistic
Entire Year
December
Jan.-Nov.
8 156 844
15 678 820
36 982 974
236 451 301
686 055
844 335
4 085 564
15 379 937
7 470 789
14 834 485
32 897 410
221 071 364
0.062
0.181
-0.119
2.906
-81.562
0.052
0.144
-0.092
2.763
-21.047
0.063
0.185
-0.122
2.943
-79.348
As a conclusion, we may state that individual investors in the Portuguese market exhibit the
disposition effect for the entire year, including December. The preference to realize winners
instead of losers is stronger in our study than in other studies, probably because Portuguese
individual investors are less sophisticated than in other more developed markets. Opposed to
other markets studied, the disposition effect is neither inverted nor reduced in the end of the
fiscal year.
5.2. The disposition effect in bull and bear markets
In bull periods, there are more opportunities for realising gains, while in bear periods there are
more opportunities for realising losses. However, the measure of the disposition effect under
use already considers this potential trap. Realised gains and losses are computed in
21
comparison to the existent opportunities due to the market trend. Therefore, if the measure
used to compute the disposition effect is not affected by the market trend the differences that
may arise if any are the result of investors’ preferences under different market conditions.
We split the period of analysis into a bull market period (from 1st January 1999 to 3rd March
2000) a bear market period (from 4th March 2000 to 31st December 2002) and repeated the
previous tests. Table 7 shows the PGR and PLR for each sub-period, calculated on the basis
of the number of sells. We found strong evidence of the disposition effect both in bull and in
bear markets, but stronger in bull markets. The difference found between the disposition
effect in bull and bear markets is statistically significant.
Table 7 – Disposition effect in bull and bear markets
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) over the bull period (from 1st
January 1999 to 3rd March 2000), the bear period (from 4th March 2000 to 31st December 2002) and its difference. The number of Realized
Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across days. The t-statistics calculation
assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions over time.
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
[(PLR-PGR)Bull – (PLR-PGR)Bear]
t-statistic
Bull
13 890
6 494
28 270
62 065
0.095
0.329
-0.235
3.478
0.0025
-92.138
-0.046
-17.909
Bear
12 001
15 135
27 283
114 910
0.116
0.305
-0.189
2.625
0.0025
-76.000
For illustration purposes and deeper detail when studying the differences between bull and
bear markets, the graph 2 exhibits the monthly progress of the PGR/PLR ratio and the graph 3
shows the PGR, PLR and its difference for each month. Considering the graph 2, it shows
clearly that, in the bull period, the preference for realising gains is, at least, three times higher
than the preference for realising losses, while in the bear period, the ratio falls (even though,
the tendency for realizing gains is still at least twice than the tendency for realizing losses,
except in February 2001, when the ratio falls to an unprecedented value of 1.77). In the graph
3, it is shown that the change between bull and bear markets results, essentially, from the
decreasing of the PGR, while the PLR exhibits more stables values for the entire period.
22
Graph 2 – Evolution of PGR/PLR by Months
This graph reports the ratio Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR)/Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) by month from 1-1-1999 to 31-122002. The number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated across
accounts and days.
4,500
4,000
3,500
3,000
2,500
2,000
1,500
1,000
0,500
Jan-99
Feb-99
Mar-99
Apr-99
May-99
Jun-99
Jul-99
Aug-99
Sep-99
Oct-99
Nov-99
Dec-99
Jan-00
Feb-00
Mar-00
Apr-00
May-00
Jun-00
Jul-00
Aug-00
Sep-00
Oct-00
Nov-00
Dec-00
Jan-01
Feb-01
Mar-01
Apr-01
May-01
Jun-01
Jul-01
Aug-01
Sep-01
Oct-01
Nov-01
Dec-01
Jan-02
Feb-02
Mar-02
Apr-02
May-02
Jun-02
Jul-02
Aug-02
Sep-02
Oct-02
Nov-02
Dec-02
0,000
Graph 3 – Evolution of PGR, PLR and its difference by Months
This graph reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR), Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) and its difference by month from 1-1-1999
to 31-12-2002. The number of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are aggregated
across accounts and days.
PLR
0,45
PGR
0,40
PGR-PLR
0,35
0,30
0,25
0,20
0,15
0,10
0,05
Jan-99
Feb-99
Mar-99
Apr-99
May-99
Jun-99
Jul-99
Aug-99
Sep-99
Oct-99
Nov-99
Dec-99
Jan-00
Feb-00
Mar-00
Apr-00
May-00
Jun-00
Jul-00
Aug-00
Sep-00
Oct-00
Nov-00
Dec-00
Jan-01
Feb-01
Mar-01
Apr-01
May-01
Jun-01
Jul-01
Aug-01
Sep-01
Oct-01
Nov-01
Dec-01
Jan-02
Feb-02
Mar-02
Apr-02
May-02
Jun-02
Jul-02
Aug-02
Sep-02
Oct-02
Nov-02
Dec-02
0,00
These results, we believe, can only be explained by behavioural reasons. Rationally, one
would expect the opposite: momentum strategies in bull periods and contrary strategies in
bear periods (contrary strategies conduct to disposition effect). The stronger preference for
realising gains and holding losers in bull markets is related to the prospect theory value
function, mechanisms of mental accounting and loss aversion. In bull periods, the realization
of gains is “easier”: gains are “easily” available and investors will have the desire to realize
them because, according to the prospect theory, the satisfaction of a new gain, in terms of
value function, is decreasing. This explains why PGR is higher in bull periods. On the
23
contrary, the realization of losses implies the assumption of a wrong decision when the
market is going up which is, psychologically, very difficult to accept. As Gervais and Odean
(2001) refer, investors learn to be overconfident in bull markets. Then, with reinforced
overconfidence investors want to feel the pride of gains (selling winners) and avoid the regret
of losses more painful in bull markets (holding their losses and hoping for recovery). Then,
investors prefer to hold the stock and avoid the regret of a bad decision.
In bear periods, investors are still averse to losses. However, it is easier to accept losses
because they can input them to external factors. We found a smooth increase of PLR in the
bear period, by comparison to the bull, (from 0.095 to 0.116). When market is decreasing
sharply, as it was the case, it becomes easier to accept a loss because investors show a
decreasing sensitivity to additional losses and also because the break-even appears very
difficult to achieve. Even tough, the increase in PLR is small. As the market is falling down,
potential losses increased considerably and, in absolute terms, realised losses increased
significantly as well. Simultaneously, the PGR decreases from 0.329 in the bull period to
0.305 in the bear period. When the market is falling, winning stocks are seen as good choices
which reinforce the investors’ confidence to keep their decision unchanged. However,
according to mental accounting, unrealized gains aren’t considered real gains and, as a result,
investors will wait to realise them. Therefore disposition effect still holds.
Concluding, we found disposition effect in the bull and bear periods, even tough it is
significantly stronger in the bull period.
5.3. The disposition effect and the investors’ sophistication
In order to study the disposition effect in relation to the investors’ sophistication, we segment
them following three criteria: the trade frequency; the trading volume and the account value.
Once it is difficult to select a frontier for deciding whether an investor is a sophisticated one
in terms of number of trades, trading volume or account value we divided the sample of
investors accounts into two groups, using for frontiers the percentiles 50%, 75%, 90% and
95%. The main purpose is to identify if the disposition effect exists in each group and if there
are significant differences between these groups.
24
Tables 8, 9 and 10 exhibit the results for each sophistication criteria: the number of trades; the
trading volume and the account value. We found disposition effect for each group and we also
found significant differences among these groups: those assumed less sophisticated groups of
investors shown significantly stronger disposition effect than the more sophisticated ones.
Odean (1998) also found that less frequent traders have stronger disposition effect (Odean
only tests the division using the percentile 90%), but the differences we found are much
stronger. We also found that, with the increase of the frontier percentile, the disposition effect
decreases for the frequent traders group. This means that more active investors are less prone
to disposition effect. Consistently, when the division percentile considered is higher the less
sophisticated group also exhibits lower disposition effect because we are moving investors
from the frequent traders group to the less frequent one. Taking into account the trading
volume the conclusions are similar: all groups evidence disposition affect and the groups with
lower trading volume show significantly stronger disposition effect.
Table 8 – Disposition Effect and Investors’ Trade Frequency
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) from 1-1-1999 to 31-12-2002 for
each group of investors. The value of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are
aggregated across accounts and days. The t-statistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
50
inferior
superior
RG
1 331
24 560
RL
727
20 902
PG
1 000
54 553
PL
2 836
174 139
PLR
0.204
0.107
PGR
0.571
0.310
PLR-PGR
-0.367
-0.203
PGR/PLR
2.798
2.897
σ(PLR-PGR)
0.01227
0.00179
t-statistic
-29.895 -113.698
(PLR-PGR)inf –(PLR-PGR)sup
-0.164
t-statistic
-13.335
Percentil
75
inferior
superior
3 951
21 940
2 644
18 985
3 830
51 723
12 106
164 869
0.179
0.103
0.508
0.298
-0.329
-0.195
2.833
2.884
0.00649
0.00183
-50.634 -106.427
-0.134
-20.644
90
inferior
superior
8 436
17 455
6 267
15 362
9 395
46 158
33 031
143 944
0,159
0,096
0,473
0,274
-0,314
-0,178
2,967
2,845
0.00417
0.00192
-75.208
-92.809
-0.136
-32.533
95
inferior
superior
11 902
13 989
9 123
12 506
16 224
39 329
52 828
124 147
0,147
0,092
0,423
0,262
-0,276
-0,171
2,874
2,867
0.00327
0.00206
-84.324
-82.991
-0.105
-32.107
25
Table 9 – Disposition Effect and Investors’ Trading Volume
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) from 1-1-1999 to 31-12-2002 for
each group of investors. The value of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are
aggregated across accounts and days. The t-statistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
Percentil
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
(PLR-PGR)inf –(PLR-PGR)sup
t-statistic
50
inferior
2 091
1 193
2 134
5 919
0.168
0.495
-0.327
2.950
0.008877
-36.857
superior
23 800
20 436
53 419
171 056
0.107
0.308
-0.201
2.888
0.001805
-111.614
-0.126
-14.158
75
inferior
superior
5 513
20 378
3 774
17 855
6 185
49 368
19 782
157 193
0.160
0.102
0.471
0.292
-0.311
-0.190
2.942
2.864
0.005197 0.001868
-59.850 -101.821
-0.121
-23.260
90
inferior
superior
11 321
14 570
8 397
13 232
15 419
40 134
51 628
125 347
0.140
0.095
0.423
0.266
-0.283
-0.171
3.026
2.789
0.003337 0.002048
-84.956
-83.418
-0.113
-33.752
95
inferior
superior
15 317
10 574
11 684
9 945
26 780
28 773
85 218
91 757
0.121
0.098
0.364
0.269
-0.243
-0.171
3.018
2.748
0.002568 0.002421
-94.748
-70.608
-0.072
-28.168
The other criterion used to classify investors’ sophistication, and probably the most accurate
one, is the selection based on the account (portfolio) value. Investors with high account value
are expected to base their decision making processes on more complex criteria and
sophisticated models. Consequently, we should expect them to be less influenced by
psychological and behavioural factors. The 5% group of investors with higher account value
exhibit a difference between PGR and PLR of 0.14 (with a t-statistic of 51), while the 50%
group of investor with lower account value evidence a difference between PGR and PLR of
0.308 (with a t-statistic of 48). This means that the intensity of disposition effect for the lower
group is more than twice than the intensity of disposition effect shown by the upper group.
Even tough, the disposition effect holds for every group of investors. We conclude that all
individual investors seem to prefer realizing gains to losses but the more sophisticated ones
evidence a significantly lower disposition effect. Brown, Chappel, Rosa and Walter (2002)
also test inventors’ sophistication and disposition effect on the basis of the value of investors’
trades and get similar conclusions.
26
Table 10 – Disposition Effect and Investors’ Account Value
This table reports the Proportion of Gains Realized (PGR) and the Proportion of Losses Realized (PLR) from 1-1-1999 to 31-12-2001 for
each group of investors. The value of Realized Gains (RG), Realized Losses (RL), Potential Gains (PG) and Potential Losses (PL) are
aggregated across accounts and days. The t-statistics calculation assumes that GR, LR, PG and PL result from independent decisions.
Percentil
RG
RL
PG
PL
PLR
PGR
PLR-PGR
PGR/PLR
σ(PLR-PGR)
t-statistic
(PLR-PGR)inf –(PLR-PGR)sup
t-statistic
50
inferior
4 168
3 485
3 904
13 221
0.209
0.516
-0.308
2.475
0.006389
-48.167
superior
21 723
18 144
51 649
163 754
0.100
0.296
-0.196
2.968
0.001826
-107.515
-0.111
-17.440
75
inferior
superior
8 666
17 225
6 932
14 697
10 296
45 257
34 206
142 769
0.169
0.093
0.457
0.276
-0.289
-0.182
2.712
2.954
0.004061 0.001932
-71.043
-94.374
-0.106
-26.143
90
inferior
superior
14 859
11 032
12 110
9 519
21 972
33 581
77 495
99 480
0.135
0.087
0.403
0.247
-0.268
-0.160
2.985
2.832
0.002799 0.002214
-95.823
-72.233
-0.108
-38.694
95
inferior
superior
19 422
6 469
15 207
6 422
33 883
21 670
112 244
64 731
0.119
0.090
0.364
0.230
-0.245
-0.140
3.054
2.547
0.002274 0.002728
-107.776
-51.174
-0.105
-46.359
One could say that investors tend to sell winners and hold losers because they expect future
reverse movements on prices. But, evidence shows that, on average, this expectation is
misleading. This preference for realising winners and holding losers is much more associated
with past prices than with expectation about future prices.
27
Conclusions
This paper addresses the disposition effect, firstly raised by Shefrin and Statman (1985).
According to this behavioural finding, investors tend to less (realising) winner stocks faster
and while holding loser stocks longer. The behavioural finance helps to explain such findings,
basing investors’ decisions on the apperceived value that investors assign to gains and losses.
We use a unique database for the Portuguese market composed by 1496 individual investors’
accounts and 159 406 trades.
We find a strong preference for investors selling winners and holding losers and this tendency
holds whether the basis of measurement is the number of trades, the trading volume or the
turnover. This tendency is far stronger than in other markets previously studied: the
Proportion of Gains Realized is 20% higher than the Proportion of Losses Realized. We
believe that the main reason for this finding relates with the low level of sophistication of the
individual investors.
The preference for realizing winners and holding losers holds for each month of the year. As
opposed to the evidence found in other markets, the so-called fiscal effect has not enough
impact to significantly reduce or invert the disposition effect (in spite of the existence of fiscal
incentives under the Portuguese jurisdiction). This specific situation may be due to nonhomogeneous legislation applicable to investors in the sample. This non-homogeneity was
also true when we compare subperiods within the entire sampling period. Given our results,
we conclude that even some investors decide to realize capital losses for fiscal reasons, there
are other investors which are not motivated by fiscal aspects that prefer to realize capital gains
at the end of the year (e.g.: to evidence improved performance), balancing each other. This
means that mental accounting mechanisms tend to consider as gains those already realized.
Therefore, the fiscal effect and the disposition effect counterbalance each other and the
pattern of realized gains and losses is not significantly modified on December.
Furthermore, we also studied differences in the disposition effect considering market trends
(bull/bear). One could expect that, in bull periods, there are more opportunities for realising
gains. However, the measure of the disposition effect used considers the realisation of gains
and losses compared to the opportunities which exists due to the market trend. We found that,
in bull markets, the disposition effect is stronger than in bear markets, even though it is
28
significant for both periods. Rationally, one would expect the opposite: momentum strategies
in bull periods and contrary strategies in bear periods. We believe that these results may be
explained on the basis of behavioural factors. In bull periods the realization of gains is
“easier”: gains are “easily” achievable that investor will desire to realize them because,
according to the prospect theory, the satisfaction of a new gain, in terms of value function, is
decreasing. On the contrary, the realization of losses implies to assume a wrong decision
when the market is going up which is, psychologically, very difficult to accept. Then,
investors prefer to hold the stock and avoid the regret of a wrong decision. In bear periods,
investors are still averse to losses, but it is easier to accept losses because they can input them
to external factors.
Finally, we also studied the disposition effect in relation to the investors’ sophistication. In
order to identify differences, we divided investors into groups stratified by the following
sophistication criteria: number of trades; trading volume and account value. We found that
less sophisticated investors exhibit stronger disposition effect, although it is significant in
every group of individual investors. Although the Portuguese stock market is no more
considered as an emergent market, it may be surprising that it still exhibits some
characteristics of those markets and, so, investors tend to reveal low sophisticated investment
behaviour, particularly individual investors.
29
Bibliography
Brown, P., N. Chappel, R. d. S. Rosa e T. Walter (2002). The Reach of the Disposition Effect: Large Sample
Evidence across Investor Classes. European Finance Association Conference, Berlin.
Case, K. e R. Shiller (1988). "The Behavior of Home Buyers in Boom and Post-Boom Markets." New England
Economic Review: 29-46.
Chui, P. (2001). "An Experimental Study of Disposition Effect: Evidence from Macau." Journal of Psychology
and Financial Markets 2 (4): 216-222.
CMVM (2003). Relatório de Síntese Estatística Sobre a Actividade de Recepção de Ordens por Conta de Outrem
por Canal de Recepção, Tipo de Investidor e Mercado de Execução das Ordens. Comissão do Mercado de
Valores Mobiliários - Estudos e Documentos. Lisboa.
Dhar, R. and N. Zhu (2006). "Up Close and Personal: An Individual Level Analysis of the Disposition Effect."
Management Science forthcoming.
Dyl, E. (1977). "Capital Gains Taxation and the Year-End Stock Market Behavior." Journal of Finance 32 (1):
165-175.
Ferris, S., R. Haugen e A. Makhija (1988). "Predicting Contemporary Volume with Historic Volume at
Differential Price Levels: Evidence Supporting the Disposition Effect." Journal of Finance 43 (3): 677-697.
Genesove, D. and C. Mayer (2001). "Loss Aversion and Seller Behavior: Evidence from the Housing Market."
The Quarterly Journal of Economics 116(4): 1233-1260.
Gervais, S. and T. Odean (2001). "Learning to Be Overconfident." The Review of Financial Studies 14(1): 1-27.
Grinblatt, M. e M. Keloharju (2000). "The Investment Behavior and Performance of Various Investor Types: A
Study of Finland's Unique Data Set." Journal of Financial Economics 55 (1): 43-67.
Kahneman, D. e A. Tversky (1979). "Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk." Econometrica 47
(2): 263-292.
Kaustia, M. (2000). Can Loss Aversion Move the Market? Evidence from IPO Trading Volume. SSRN Working
Paper Series.
Kim, K. e J. Nofsinger (2002). The Behavior and Performance of Individual Investors in Japan. Review of
Financial Studies Conference, Mannheim.
Lakonishok, J. e S. Smidt (1986). "Volume for Winners and Losers: Taxation and Other Motives for Stock
Trading." Journal of Finance 41: 951-974.
Locke, P. e S. Mann (2000). Do Professional Traders Exhibit Loss Realization Aversion? SSRN Working Paper
Series.
Odean, T. (1998). "Are Investors Reluctant to Realize Their Losses?" Journal of Finance 53 (5): 1775-1798.
Oehler, A., K. Heilmann, V. Lager e M. Oberlander (2002). Dying out or Dying Hard? Disposition Investors in
Stock Markets. European Finance Association Conference, Berlin.
Schlarbaum, G., W. Lewellen e R. Lease (1978). "Realized Returns on Common Stock Investments: The
Experience of Individual Investors." Journal of Business 51 (2): 299-325.
Shapira, Z. e I. Venezia (2001). "Patterns of Behavior of Professionally Managed and Independent Investors."
Journal of Banking and Finance 25 (8): 1573-1587.
Shefrin, H. e M. Statman (1985). "The Disposition to Sell Winners Too Early and Ride Losses Too Long:
Theory and Evidence." Journal of Finance 40 (3): 777-790.
Thaler, R. (1985). "Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice." Marketing Science 4 (3): 199-214.
Weber, M. e C. Camerer (1998). "The Disposition Effect in Securities Trading: An Experimental Analysis."
Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 33 (2): 167-184.
30
View publication stats