Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
DOI 10.1007/s11199-006-9119-7
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Images of Old Men’s Masculinity: Still a Man?
Edward H. Thompson, Jr.
Published online: 6 December 2006
# Springer Science + Business Media, Inc. 2006
Abstract Relatively little attention has been paid to later
life masculinities. Separate studies were designed to
identify the commonly held images of old men and then
determine whether or not an old man is perceived as a man
or more in terms of a gender-neutral elderly person. The
first assessed the commonly held images of positive and
negative later life masculinities. A second study examined
if old men’s gendered character was perceived similarly in
comparison to a young man or an old woman. Findings
from the studies revealed age expectations and masculinity
norms jointly affected people’s images of old men. Old men
were favorably described more often than negatively
stereotyped, and there was evidence of distinct late life
masculinity expectations.
Keywords Old men . Later life masculinities . Stereotypes
Recent interest in the social worlds of old men and how
age-specific masculinities differentially structure men’s
experiences has been noticeable (Arber, Davidson, & Ginn,
2003; Calasanti, 2004; Emslie, Hunt, & O’Brien, 2004;
Kosberg & Kaye, 1997; Thompson, 1994). Yet very little is
known about what gender and age norms are most
applicable to old men’s lives, or how others perceive old
men. This information gap raises an intriguing question: To
what extent is an elder man recognized as a man? Do
people generally view him in terms of his gender as much
as his age? Or, does the perceptual salience of gender
recede from foreground to background to become less
E. H. Thompson, Jr. (*)
Department of Sociology & Anthropology,
College of the Holy Cross, 1 College Street,
Worcester, MA 01610-2395, USA
e-mail:
[email protected]
important in the eyes of others as a man ages, and thus is
the older man recognized more in terms of his age-status as
“an old man?”
I have argued elsewhere (Thompson, 1994) that old men
have remained relatively invisible in contemporary social
life. They are less visible in comparison to the attention
given to younger aged men and boys by both academic
researchers and the general public. Discussions of old men
and their masculinities within the burgeoning literature on
men and masculinities are infrequent. Instead, almost all of
the available research is in the gerontology literature and
emphasizes men’s aging and the difficulties that old men
face in later life. Any discourse on old men as men is very
peripheral to the predominant effort to understand men’s
aging bodies and health problems, in particular their
medical troubles. By principally framing old men’s lives
through a lens that medicalizes late life, the old man’s
masculinity is pushed into the background. Aging overshadows gender. This is the insidious effect of ageism
(Butler, 1969; Green, 1993). An old man is imagined as
“old” whether he is aged 67, fit, active, independent, and
employed, or age 90, frail and living in a nursing home.
Even these differently aged old men surely live gendered
lives. Are their masculinities and age-specific gender
performances recognized?
Despite the large body of research that has examined the
way that the public perceives older adults (e.g., Brewer,
Dull, & Lui, 1981; Hummert, Garstka, Shaner, & Strahm,
1994; Kite, Stockdale, Whitley, & Johnson, 2005), and the
equally established research tradition examining gender
stereotyping, very little is known about the way age and
gender constructions jointly influence conceptions of old
men. The research that has examined the dual effects of a
target’s age and gender on people’s descriptions of and
attitudes toward old men offers equivocal information about
how much or little masculinity expectations are evident.
634
A few studies (e.g., Braithwaite, 1986; Jackson &
Sullivan, 1988; Kite, Deaux, & Miele, 1991; Minnigerode
& Lee, 1978) report that the age-of-the-target influenced
people’s perceptions and descriptions more often than the
gender-of-the-target and thus old men’s age status as old
men was more influential in determining evaluations of the
old man. For example, Kite et al. (1991) found that when
respondents described all the qualities they thought characteristic of old men or old women, the attributes provided for
the two targets were highly similar (r=0.79) but quite
different characteristics were used to describe older and
younger men (r=0.12). Kite et al. also noted that the
descriptions of old men were less likely to include the
masculine traits, behaviors, and physical characteristics
ascribed to the younger men. Similarly, Levin (1988)
reported that when undergraduates evaluated photographs
of the same man at three ages, they saw age whittling away
at the attributes commonly associated with masculinity. By
comparison, several other studies (e.g., Canetto, Kaminski,
& Felicio, 1995; Deutsch, Zalenski, & Clark, 1986;
Zepelin, Sills, & Heath, 1987) observed that gender norms
remain potent in late life and the gender-of-the-target had
an important affect on the way men were perceived and
described. Canetto et al. (1995) found that old men were
evaluated more favorably in terms of intellectual competence and autonomy than old women; they were perceived
as more self-reliant and less passive and dependent.
Deutsch et al. (1986) reported that old men were regarded
as more attractive than old women, and Hummert, Garskta,
and Shaner (1997) noted that old men were less likely to be
described negatively than old women.
Although researchers have begun to untangle the ways
age and gender stereotyping frame how old men are
discussed and can be imaged, no study has directly
examined the content of the stereotypes about old men or
assessed the extent to which an old man’s masculinity is or
is not veiled by becoming old. This article details the
findings of one study that examined the content that makes
up the conceptions young adults have of old men and
another that examined the extent to which perceptions of an
old man’s trait-masculinity differ when he is compared to a
younger man or an old woman.
Conceptual Footing
It is a basic proposition of the social constructionist
perspective on masculinity that there are different kinds of
masculinities within society; it is also understood that lived
masculinities are negotiated performances that help maintain the gender scripts that are “out there” in the culture, in
institutions, and in relationships and reveal relations of
dominance and subordination (Connell, 1995; Martin,
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
2004; West & Zimmerman, 1987). The culturally idealized
form of masculinity may not be the lived form of
masculinity at all, but it remains a powerful, perhaps
dominant, script against which self and others are evaluated
across the life course (Morgan, 1992). Conversations about
age-related masculinities are not uncommon, and the
masculinity expectations for boys and adult men have been
systematically addressed (e.g., Connell, 2000; Kimmel,
1996). But research addressing later life masculinities is in
its infancy. Neither the general norms scripting later life
masculinity nor the behavioral norms older men institute
have been charted.
The blueprints of older men’s masculinities remain hazy.
But later life masculinities seem to be defined by norms
of sociability and reveal more vividly men seeking connections with others, instead of the acts of individuation. Older
men’s experiences become more centered on the emotion
work of relational concerns (Bleiszner, 1989; Kidder, 1993)
and reflection (Ryff, 1991) and less by the triumphs they
used to define themselves as younger men. A “busy ethic”
(Ekerdt, 1986) replaces the performance-oriented work ethic;
a search for coherence replaces the self-reliant, “give ‘em
hell” values of earlier masculinities (Erikson, Kivnick, &
Erikson, 1994).
Study 1
For several decades studies have documented the multidimensionality of stereotypes associated with older adults and
identified some of the content within the stereotypes.
Initially, Brewer et al. (1981; Brewer & Lui, 1984) established that when people are asked to describe an older
adult, rarely do they envision a general superordinate
category, rather they maintain images of specific kinds of
older adults. The presence of multiple stereotypes explains
why incompatible traits such as mellow and complaining
are used at the same time to describe an older adult. The
work of Brewer et al. also established that people’s
cognitive representations involved at least three recognizable stereotypes—grandmothers, elder statesman, and senior citizens—and distinct characteristics were associated
with each image. As is suggested by its name, the “elder
statesman” is an old man stereotype that exemplifies a
distinguished, conservative old man. Traits attributed to this
stereotype included some stereotypic masculine attributes
such as aggressive, strong-willed, and competitive. The
second stereotype associated with old men, the “senior
citizen,” emphasized the target’s age rather than gender and
represented an inactive, isolated, and old-fashioned “senior
citizen” who could be a woman as much as a man.
In an influential study of the content of stereotypes of an
old person, Schmidt and Boland (1986) asked informants to
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
generate traits by describing “the typical elderly adult;” 99
distinct traits were identified. The short phrases or single
words listed by the informants represented different content
domains, including physical characteristics, social characteristics, personality traits, and emotions. When the traits
were sorted by other participants, 12 middle-range stereotypes described “old people.” Four of the 12 were favorable
stereotypes—sage, John Wayne conservative, perfect
grandparent, and liberal patriarch/matriarch; twice as many
stereotypes were negative—nosy neighbor, shrew/curmudgeon, bag lady/vagrant, recluse, despondent, vulnerable,
mildly impaired, and severely impaired. Interestingly,
among the 99 traits, only ten physical traits (e.g., grayhaired, wrinkled skin, shaky hands) and a few social and
psychological traits (e.g., retired) applied exclusively to
older adults; the remainder were as descriptive of young
and middle-age adults as older adults (e.g., enjoys life,
curious, interesting, demanding). Hardly any trait was
gendered to be more descriptive of a man than a woman.
None of the 12 stereotypes was gendered in ways to describe
an old man any more than an old woman. The content of the
“perfect grandparent” stereotype entailed gender-neutral
traits such as generous, understanding, family-oriented,
wise, loving, and useful; even the content of the “John
Wayne conservative” stereotype was defined in genderneutral terms such as patriotic, wealthy, and Republican.
Hummert’s (1990; Hummert et al., 1994) studies of the
content of the stereotypes of older adults extended the work
of Schmidt and Boland. She demonstrated that there is, in
fact, little similarity in the content of the stereotypes that
describe older adults and younger adults, as well as
minimal agreement among people of different ages about
what stereotypes are associated with an older adult. Using
the 86 Schmidt and Boland traits that were not age-specific,
three positive and seven negative stereotypes were identified in the 1990 study for older adults. Only two of these ten
were analogous to informants’ images of young adults—the
“liberal matriarch/patriarch” was similar to “activist,” and
“severely impaired” older adult paralleled “invalid” young
adult. The 1994 study (Hummert et al., 1994) found fewer
stereotypes were generated by the young adults—eight
rather than the expected ten: three positive (Golden ager,
perfect grandparent, John Wayne conservative) and five
negative (shrew/ curmudgeon, despondent, vulnerable,
severely impaired, and recluse). Her middle-age informants
detailed 12 stereotypes, reintroducing “liberal matriarch/
patriarch” to the list of positive and adding “self-centered”
to the list of negative stereotypes. Her older informants
revealed even greater diversity in their images of older
adults: They identified five positive and seven negative
stereotypes from the same list of attributes.
As much as the earlier studies uncovered many distinct
images of older adults, the studies were not designed to
635
reveal public images of an old man. People were only asked
to generate a list of attributes and traits for a generalized
target—e.g., an older adult or the elderly, thus gender was
masked (Braithwaite, Gibson, & Holman, 1985–86). This
may account for why the content generated was overwhelmingly gender-neutral. As much as the informants in
the Schmidt and Boland study and Hummert’s studies may
have envisioned a “European American male” as the
default category and thereby gendered the old person they
were evaluating, which Kite et al. (2005) suggest is likely,
in fact by not specifying the target’s gender, researchers
have not knowingly determined if the gender of the target
affects the way informants characterize the target. The
present study was designed to reveal people’s images of an
“old man.” Remaining as one with the earlier studies, no
information about the target’s class or ethnic/cultural
background was provided. Only the gender and age of the
target was cued. I anticipated that the images people reported
would be as contradictory and multidimensional as prior
studies have detailed for the gender-neutral older adult.
Method
Participants Participants were 210 liberal arts undergraduates (age range=18–25, X=20.7, SD=1.78) recruited over a
four year period from the author’s introductory sociology
courses (n=7), which often had reverse registration priorities and enrolled fourth- and third-year students first. The
sample was made up of 45% males, and most students
(93%) were white. None were married, and nearly all lived
on-campus. At the time data were collected no participant
was majoring in sociology, and more participants reported a
major in the humanities or natural sciences than one of the
other social sciences. Data on the undergraduates’ socioeconomic status is not available; however, tuition at the
college is expensive, and more students come from uppermiddle class families than middle-class.
Procedure The content of the stereotypes for the target “an
old man” was generated with three sentence completions.
Sentence completions generally yield fuller descriptions of
a target compared to a word association exercise (cf.,
Schmidt & Boland, 1986). Similar to comparing photographs, the sentence completion method also provides an
opportunity to anchor the old man target to a comparison.
For the present study the sentence completion exercise was
designed to maximize consideration of the old man’s
masculinity and age. Two sentence stems anchored the
phrase an “old man” to a comparison—a young man or an
old woman, effectively producing a demand for informants
to pay attention to the age-of-the-target or gender-of-thetarget and hopefully priming rich descriptions of the old
man.
636
In the third week of the author’s introductory sociology
courses, students were asked to participate in an in-class
exercise at the beginning of the class. This was the second
in-class exercise in which students were asked to anonymously reveal a personal opinion on a 4×6 piece of paper,
submit their comments to the instructor, watch the papers
shuffled on a desk, then asked to take back one of their
peer’s comments and read aloud the comments to the class
as a trigger for class discussion. In this exercise students
were again informed that their participation was voluntary.
Only one of 211 students chose to not take part in the
exercise. The participants were asked to complete the
sentence “An old man...” on a 4×6 piece of paper. No
verb was provided in the sentence stem. No other verbal
instructions were provided, other than repeating the task
expectation, to please complete the sentence “An old
man...”
Once everyone completed the initial sentence, another
4×6 piece of paper was distributed, and the participants
were asked to complete the sentence stem “Compared to a
young man, an old man...” When everyone completed the
second sentence, a third 4×6 piece of paper was distributed
and the students were asked to complete the sentence
“Compared to an old woman, an old man...” The presentation order of the two comparative sentences varied so that
three classes detailed their old man/young man images
before they disclosed their old man/old woman images. The
priming for informants to attend to the age- or gender-ofthe-target in the final two sentence completions was
deliberate; a key objective was to assess if the content of
the stereotypes might differ when the old man target was or
was not anchored to a comparison other.
The attributes, behaviors, personality traits, physical
capabilities, physical appearance characteristics, and other
qualities disclosed in the informants’ sentence completions
provided the data for the study. The full range of attributes/
activities used to describe the “old man” target was
preserved, thus each unique expression within the sentence
completion was treated as a separate example of stereotype
content (e.g., “...is grandfatherly, considerate, full of
experience, and walks every morning” was coded as four
descriptions).
Analysis Strategy In the first step of the content analysis,
the qualities attributed to the “old man” target were coded
as (a) descriptors, attributes, or activities identifiable as
likely characteristic of older men, and (b) non-meaningful
sentence completions—e.g., An old man... “accosted my
younger sister, therefore I beat the hell out of him.” Coding
was completed by two independent judges; each descriptive
statement and attribute/activity reported by the informants
that could not be agreed upon by the judges was reviewed
by a third judge. Interrater reliability was assessed by
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
examining the assessments of two judges for the first 50
informants; the percentage of agreement was 0.93. Overall,
the third judge was needed in fewer than 10% of the
descriptive statements or attributes/activities disclosed in
the sentence completions; e.g., when the stimulus “An old
man...” was finished with completions such as “...is in
many works of art I studied last semester,” the information
was coded as a non-meaningful descriptor of old men by
both primary judges; but the sentence completion “...is on
the dollar bill” was arbitrated and eventually coded as a
non-meaningful descriptor rather than evidence of a wise,
worldly sage. This process identified 95 descriptive statements about the first unanchored old man target, 101
descriptors for the old man target when compared to a
young man, and 89 descriptors for the older man target
when compared to an old woman.
The next step in the analysis required a coding taxonomy
to identify the underlying representations. The same three
coders separately reviewed the entire list of descriptive
statements and generated possible thematic categories to
capture the meaning of the statements and/or qualities; e.g.,
wisdom, experienced, worldly, and ready to give advice
upon being asked were regarded as analogous and evidence
of an image that regarded the old man as a “sage.” After the
coders generated their lists of possible stereotype categories, lists were shared; similarities among the generated
categories were discussed, and after several cycles consensus was reached on the final stereotype categories and their
meta-themes.
Finally, with the taxonomy completed, the coders sorted
the descriptive statements into the categories to identify the
frequency for each stereotypes. Because an informant might
mention the same stereotype two or more times in the
sentence completion, each descriptor had been coded with
the informant’s ID number. One frequency measure,
absolute frequency, tallied the number of descriptors for
stereotype; the second tallied just the number of respondents who mentioned the stereotype in their sentence
completions. Interrater agreement among the three coders
on the first round was 86%. Descriptive statements that
were initially sorted into dissimilar categories were discussed and consensus agreement was reached. This process
was repeated for the sets of descriptive statements that
defined the old man in comparison to a young man and an
old woman.
Results
Overall, the initial characterizations of the old man target
provided different content and fewer images of an old man
than the descriptions linked to a “young man” and an “old
woman” (see Fig. 1). And generally speaking, as much as
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
637
Fig. 1 Stereotypes of an old man.
age-related descriptions of the old man recur, underneath
these images, evidence of later life masculinities was also
prevalent. The range of stereotypes that emerged from the
three sets of descriptive statements are summarized in
Fig. 1 and reviewed in detailed in this section.
There is evidence of four meta-themes involving 12
stereotypes within the initial descriptive statements, attributes, or activities linked to the sentence stem “An old
man....” Table 1 summarizes the meta-themes, the stereotypes, and their content. Representations ranged from
positive to negative images of later life masculinities and
from neutral to negative images of how aging likely affects
men. Images of old men’s masculinities involved the
positive stereotypes of the old man as a sage (n=40),
nurturing, sociable grandfather (n=49), and active retired
man (n=70). The sage and grandfather stereotypes are
analogous to Schmidt and Boland’s sage and perfect
grandparent stereotypes, and the active, retired man is a
bit similar to non-gendered “golden ager” of Hummert et al.
(1994).
Depicted significantly less often, t (209)=4.34, p<0.001,
later life masculinities are also evident in negative stereotypes—the curmudgeon (n=10), solitary old man (n=15),
failed old man (n=9), and dirty old man (n=4). Although
the curmudgeon stereotype was first identified by Schmidt
and Boland, the content of the old man curmudgeon in the
present study is only analogous to the trait cluster produced
by the elder informants in Hummert et al. (1994), not the
younger or middle-age informants in their study, nor the
student informants in Schmidt and Boland. Similarly, when
informants imaged a “solitary old man,” their descriptions
seem comparable to the “despondent” stereotype Hummert
(1990; Hummert et al., 1994) described, but informants’
sentence completions characterized the solitary man as
socially alone, not emotionally lonely. On its face, the
“failed old man” stereotype also appears similar to the
“vulnerable” and “vagrant” stereotypes of the old person
that younger informants described in Schmidt and Boland
and Hummert et al., but again the content of the stereotypes
markedly differ. Theirs emphasized a worried, fearful,
impoverished person, whereas the current failed old man
stereotype emphasizes the man’s failure as a (successful)
man and/or his victimization as an old man.
An equal number (n=178) of informants used descriptive statements, attributes, and activities that largely disregarded the gender of the old man and emphasized his age.
What emerged as an unexpected age-related description
was the association of an old man with death; in fact, 5% of
638
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
Table 1 Content of the “Old Man” stereotypes.
Positive later life masculinities
The sage—wise, experienced
Has wisdom
Has experience
Ready to give advice when asked
Is wise/knowledgeable/worldly
Listens intently and speaks when he has something important to say
Knows more than young give credit for
Should be well-respected/valued for his experience
Fun to talk to
The grandfather—nurturing, sociable
Gentle/kind
Considerate
Sweet & harmless
A (friendly) grandfather/reminds me of my grandfather(s)
Bought/gave me candy
Watches children play
Enjoyed the company of his grandchildren
Many stories to tell
Sitting with a little boy next to him, listening intently
The active retired man—engaged, busy
Enjoys life because of available time/more free time
Retires/receives Social Security
Walks/walked around neighborhood for exercise
Walked down/across the street/lane
Supervising the job (others are doing)
Still prefers to drive, not be driven
Drives an old car
Drove/went to the store
Plays bocce/tends to his garden
Fishes (with grandson/little boy)
Men growing old—neutral images of aging
The tranquil old man—at rest, philosophical
Has live a long life/ was once young
Spends time around the house
Sits on porch
Sitting in rocking chair
Sits smoking a pipe
Sat on park bench reading/feeding birds/talking
Relaxed on bench in park
Sat next to me in church
Sat home and watched television
Reflects on life/years past
The timeworn man—aging appearances
Been though a lot & that is shown in his face
Wrinkled
Gray/white hair
Balding
Wearing hat
Brown age spots
Old men—negative images of his aging
The diminished man—poorer well-being
Walks/shuffles slowly
Is bent over
Drives slow
Hearing/vision losses
Uses/caries a cane
Less physically active/athletic
Physically weak/weaker
Withered
Tired/less energy
Must listen carefully
May or may not be in good health and state of mind
The impaired man—restricted, curtailed
Suffering from chronic illness
Will not get much older
Cannot work
Feels a loss of power
Helpless
Feeble
Fragile
Senile/crazy
Dependent
Driveling
Considered as not responsible
The mortal old man
Died/is dead
Negative later life masculinities
The curmudgeon—ill-tempered
(Sometimes) grumpy
Cranky
Stubborn in his ways
More settled and set in ways
Sits around the house very restless
Longed for the days of his youth
Complaining
The solitary old man—dispirited, alone
Feels rejects
Discontented
Suffers from loneliness
Lonely man without wife and family
His time has past & with that his family
(Sometimes) lonely
Needs companionship
Lives by himself
Sat alone in corner of his room
Lives in a retirement home
The failed old man—singular
Slept on a park bench
Slumped on a park bench with a bottle
Standing on the corner beneath the street light
On the street came up and ask me for spare change
Got mugged by four teenagers
Came into shop asking for a cup of coffee
Suffers from many problems given to him by society
The dirty old man
Touched my girlfriend
Married a younger woman
Longed for sex
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
the informants finished the “An old man...” sentence stem
with “is dead” or “died” (n=13). The rest of the age
characterizations ranged from negative ageist stereotypes to
neutral descriptions of how old men may appear—as
timeworn and graying (n=32), tranquil (n=65), diminished
and coping with lesser capabilities (n=34), and restricted
and impaired (n=34). These characterizations are equivalent to the stereotypes reported by Schmidt and Boland
(1986) and Hummert (1990, Hummert et al., 1994) for an
old person whose gender was not specified. For example,
the content of the “timeworn man” is very similar to the list
of age-related physical traits reported in Schmidt and
Boland’s study; the content of the “diminished man”
stereotype is comparable to the content of the “mildly
impaired” old person that the middle-age and elder informants provided in the studies by Hummert et al.; and, the
“impaired man” is analogous to the “severely impaired”
older person in both Schmidt and Boland and Hummert et al.
Reviewing Table 1, it is evident that a considerable
proportion of the descriptions of the old man (41%) call
attention to the age-of-the-target. Two meta-themes and five
of the 12 stereotypes were strictly age-specific, as when an
informant’s narrative emphasized the experiences of growing older (e.g., “...been through a lot and that is shown in
his face”). Nearly all of these age-related descriptive
statements, attributes, and activities that make up the agespecific stereotypes were not gendered and could have
defined an old woman as much as an old man (e.g., drives
slow, reflects on life, is bent over).
Nonetheless, the old man target was more frequently
(59%) described in terms that recognize his later life
masculinity—mostly in the context of old men’s social
roles as a grandfather and former breadwinner, and to a
lesser extent the way his social isolation or his temperament
as a curmudgeon is noted. As a grandfather and/or former
breadwinner, the old man’s nurturing, sociable character
and active lifestyle were defined by his story telling and
side-by-side masculine companionship activities with boys
and other older men. As a sage, a concept that dictionaries
classify as masculine, the content of the stereotype underscores the long standing social construction of the old man
as venerated and able to advise, not as someone who offers
unsolicited advice. These positive characterizations of the
old man outnumbered the negative. Even for the negative
masculinity stereotypes, the descriptors disclosed traces of
agency and interpersonal involvement more often than just
an emotional state. For example, the old man “feels
rejected” or “suffers from loneliness” rather than “feels
alone,” or the old man “is discontented” rather than “sad”
or “melancholy,” suggests something about the man’s social
life as much as his emotional state or well-being.
When informants were asked to describe the old man
compared to a young man in the second sentence
639
completion, their characterizations again produced a pair
of themes about later life masculinities and about aging.
Not unexpectedly the same-sex/different-age comparison
enhanced the salience of the target’s age; very often
informants described the old man in terms of having
“more” or “less” of something than a young man. A total
of 13 stereotypes (Table 2) were disclosed. Eight of the
stereotypes were age-related, and their content comprised
64% of informants’ descriptive statements, a marked
increase over the age-related imagery disclosed for the
unanchored old man target. It is noteworthy that one-third
of these characterizations suggested a positive imaging of
aging and centered on three new stereotypes: the seasoned,
mature man (n=40), the successful, accomplished man (n=
25), and the principled gentleman (n=21). In each of these
stereotypes the descriptive statements about the old man
highlighted ways that growing older can yield integrity and
classiness, compared to a young man.
Consistent with the ageism in American culture, the
number of informants using negative descriptors was
greater than the number who mentioned positive images
about men’s aging, n=126 and 86, respectively, χ2 =7.55,
p<0.01. Many of these negative, age-specific characterizations were also explicitly or subtly gendered, such as
when informants produced the distinct, gendered negative
stereotype about men becoming old—the old man was
characterized as a former beast in bed (n=17). Similarly
informants emphasized the old man’s diminished status in
society more than his pitiful nature when they described
him as vulnerable.
Informants reestablished the images of an old man as
diminished and coping with lesser capabilities (n=76),
timeworn and graying (n=18), and, less often, as restricted
and impaired (n=7). When compared to a young man, these
images of the old man no longer contained neutral
characterizations. The timeworn man was more likely
defined as “worse looking” and “less attractive” rather than
described plainly in terms of his age-related wrinkles and
gray hair; the prevalent description of a tranquil old man
noted in the initial descriptions was replaced by an
emphasis on how aging likely diminishes a man’s sexual,
physical, and psychological capabilities and how men are
believed to lose agency with age. In all, the negative
characterizations accentuated the physiological aspects of
growing older and the imagined effects of the “rolelessness” that some young adults think men might experience
in later life.
Anchoring the old man to a young man also partly
reframed the way informants perceived later life masculinities. Altogether, the number of informants imaging later
life masculinity positively was far greater than the number
of informants defining it negatively (n = 74 and 47,
respectively), yet the number of discrete negative depictions
640
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
Table 2 Content of the “Old Man” stereotypes, compared to “Young
Man.”
Positive later life masculinities
The sage—wise, experienced
More knowledge/more wise about life (or ways of the world)
Is wise/has great wisdom/acts wisely
More experienced
Possesses air of worldliness/more worldly
A lot of character/by far more interesting
Greater foresight
More intellectually mature
Shares knowledge with young man, probably to keep the men
dominant
The Companion—sociable, considerate
More interested in you/more sincere
Much nicer/easier to talk with
More friendly/kindly
More understanding
More likely to tell you things about himself
More jovial/nicer personality
Old men—positive images of his aging
The mature man—seasoned, tempered
Becomes mellow/more at ease (to experience)
More mature and self-assured
Acts as he feels and speaks what he thinks
Sexier/more experienced in bed
More distinguished
More content, relax, at ease
More calm, stable, controlled & settled down
Less macho, competitive
Less threatening
Less reckless, impulsive
The gentleman—conventional, principled
More traditional, conservative, conventional
More settled
Better manners/a sense of chivalry
Worries more about (or more concerned with) his family
Still married to an old woman
The accomplished old man—successful, retired
More authoritative power
Doesn’t have to impress or prove himself to anyone
More independent
Less pretentious
Has established himself in working work
Less concerned with monetary success
More financially secure/wealthier
No longer earns much money
Works less/has fewer family responsibilities
Old men—negative images of his aging
The former beast in bed—lost sexuality
Much less a sex object
Fewer sexual desires, interests, fantasies
Less sexually active
More trouble in the sack/can get it up as long
Must fantasize about women more
The timeworn man—aging appearances
Less appealing/attractive
Looks different, uglier
Worse looking
Fatter
Grayer hair
Balding
Wrinkles
The diminished man—his poorer well-being
Not as quick, slower and more careful
Slowed down and has less energy
Not as capable or happy/more pessimistic
Appears less outgoing
Doesn’t have as much fun
Less enthusiastic
More physically restricted
Less athletic, physically active/fit
Tired, less vitality/stamina
Less agile, less able
Skinner, less muscle
Physically and mentally weaker
Not as sharp or alert
Poorer sight
More unhealthy/has more health problems
The impaired man—restricted, curtailed
Helpless
Senile
Dependent, not self-reliant
Isn’t capable of taking care of himself
Should be cared for
Has a minimal existence
Likely to die sooner than later
The vulnerable old man—alone, mistreated
Fewer life/job opportunities
Has a minimal existence
Sad figure in society
More depressed about place in society
Doesn’t have it/doesn’t stand a chance
Should be cared for
Negative later life masculinities
The curmudgeon—ill-tempered
Less calm
More demanding, stubborn more grouchy, cranky
Crabbier
Grumpier, less friendly
Tougher time accepting act of aging
Isn’t as much fun to be with
Frustrated that he is not young
The uncompromising old man—inflexible, self-centered
More set in ways (values/habits)
Less apt to adopt new methods of living
Not as open minded
Not as adventurous
Thinks he is always wiser/knows more about everything
Little alternative than to reminisce in past glories
The solitary old man—dispirited, alone
Feels less useful and productive
Pushed into retirement
Retirement yields helplessness
More withdrawn/less exciting
Lacks incentive/ambition
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
Table 2 continued.
Less determined
Lonely and isolated
Stripped of security
Considered less productive
Perceived as less important/influential
of later life masculinities was greater than the positive
behaviors and attributes mentioned. The old man again was
described in terms of the sage stereotype and its emphasis
on how aging yields wisdom (n = 62). He also was
stereotyped as an affable companion and someone who is
interpersonally engaging (n=12). This companion image
seems to be a variant of the stereotype of an old man as
grandfatherly. Informants’ descriptions also maintained the
negative stereotypes of later life masculinity that characterized an old man as alone and dispirited (n=19) and a
curmudgeon (n=14). Their descriptive imagery added a
stereotype of an uncompromising, inflexible old man (n=
14), which might be a blending of the “inflexible senior
citizen” and “self-centered” images Hummert (1990)
identified for “older adults.”
In the final sentence completion, informants’ characterizations of an old man compared to an old woman yielded
slightly fewer descriptive statements, yet 13 images of an
old man emerged from the data and revealed the same
meta-themes of how positive and negative images of later
life masculinities and aging underpin informants’ comments (see Table 3). Compared to an old woman, nearly
25% of the informants (n=46) made some reference to the
old man’s shorter life span. Otherwise, the aging double
standard evident in American culture cast a positive halo on
the way an old man is described in comparison to an old
woman. Thus unlike the less favorable imagery evident in
the descriptions of the old man when compared to a young
man, where only 36% of the content mentioned something
positive about an old man, description of the old man
compared to an old woman yielded a sizeable proportion of
the positive descriptive statements (56%), and the number
of informants reporting a positive characteristic exceeded
the number that mentioned a negative, n=129 and 114, respectively. Further, if the one set of descriptive statements
that mentioned men’s shorter life span was reclassified as a
description of a true sex difference rather than a negative
image of older men, then the number of informants reporting
a positive description of the old man would far exceed the
number that mentioned a negative, n=129 and 68.
Embedded within the imagery are five positive stereotypes about later life masculinities and two positive stereotypes of men’s aging. Not surprisingly nearly all of the
content of the seven stereotypes unmistakably acknowl-
641
edges the gender-of-the-target. Later life masculinities were
described positively in terms of an old man being a sage
(n=7), an amiable companion (n=19), and an active, retired
man (n=14). This is at least the second time these three
masculinity stereotypes were evident within informants’
sentence completions. Two new later life masculinity
stereotypes were introduced—the broadminded, philosophical man (n=7) who is perceived as more tolerant and
accepting than an old woman, and descriptions of an old
man that emphasized his continuing manliness, or being
“still a man” in character and lifestyle (n=22). The two
images that characterized an old man in positive aging
terms centered on the stereotype of a seasoned, mature man
who is perceived as more respected and aging gracefully
than an old woman (n=17) and a new stereotype of the old
man as handsome (n=43).
As formidable as the number of people positively
stereotyping the old man when compared to an old woman,
almost half of the unique content (46%) is again characterized in negative terms. Two underlying images continued to
characterize the old man’s later life masculinities in terms
of him being a curmudgeon (n=36) and a solitary, dispirited
man (n=15), and three stigmatizing images of his aging
again defined an old man in terms of his timeworn appearance (n=22), diminished agency (n=37), and impaired
healthy and lifestyle (n=8).
Discussion
Old men can be talked about and imaged through
conversations, as well as sentence completions. The range
of distinct images detected in this study strongly suggests
that people are, in fact, not of one mind when they think
about an old man. Images of the old man comprised
positive and negative depictions of aging as well as later
life masculinities. Most of the time informants’ characterizations of the old man included multiple later life
masculinities. Often enough the content within an individual’s sentence completion included contradictory descriptors (e.g., “...is valued for his experience yet he is at times
rejected because he is old”).
As important the findings are in beginning to describe
the ways old men are perceived, the findings are restricted.
On one hand, the “old man” target’s ethnic/cultural
background, health and employment statuses, living
arrangements, and so on were not identified, and surely
further information about the target could have affected the
way the old man was discussed. Whether or not the images
and stereotypes are dependent on the respondent’s gender is
also something that needs to be investigated. This study
consciously did not assess if young men defined the old
man differently than young women. The intent of the study
was to provide an initial description of a gendered, older
642
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
Table 3 Content of the “Old Man” stereotypes, compared to an “Old
Woman.”
Positive later life masculinities
The sage—wise, experienced
More experience in life
More wise/ more aware/worldly
More sexually competent
The companion—sociable, considerate
More fun to be around, less synthetic
More lively
Has more fun/enjoys life more/full of life
More freedom
Better sense of humor
More compassionate
Softer
Easier to get along with/talk more
Friendlier/likely to talk to you
The broadminded old man—philosophical
More tolerant
Accepts old age better/more at ease with his age
Gets older more gracefully
Accepts younger generations more readily
Less concerned about death/about dying
Greater pride in regard to his age
The active retired man—engaged, busy
Spends less time in the home
Hands out more with his buddies
More active/ plays more golf
Likes to walk
Likes younger women more than an old woman likes younger men
Thinks he stays young at heart longer
Likes to read/ talk
(Still) The man—conventional/unfeminine lifestyle
Can be a man’s man
Thinks about sex more
Tries to prove himself sometimes
Cooks less often/doesn’t shop or gossip
Not perceive to be nearly as weak
Less vulnerable/ less dependent
More self-sufficient
More adventurous
Less involved with family worries, more leisure
Still feels he has more financial responsibility for his family
Old men—positive images of his aging
The handsome old man—appearance, spunk
Ages/looks better, looks younger
Appears more distinguished
Less concerned about his physical aging
Ages more slowly
Not fat and ugly
More desirable/ more attractive
Less wrinkles
Doesn’t worry about facial wrinkles
Younger acting
Possesses more vigor/more vitality
The Mature man—seasoned, tempered
More respected
More established/financially secure
More independent
Less crabby
More easygoing/laid back
Old men—negative images of his aging
The timeworn man—aging appearances
Often looks older/uglier
Shows the effects of the tension/stress that was his life
Balding/less hair
More unkempt
More wrinkles
Isn’t expect to/can’t hide his age as easily
The diminished man—his poorer well-being
Less self-sufficient
Wants more done for him/needs more care and attention
Less interested/ curious
More sickly/not as healthy
Slower/ less active/ lazier
More depressed with old age
More unhappy/less happy with getting older
The impaired man—restricted, curtailed
Wears hearing aid
Older, usually more senile
Acts (as if) older
The mortal old man—compressed lifespan
Dies earlier/sooner
Dies younger than the old woman
Shorter life/does not live as long
Enjoys a briefer life span
Negative later life masculinities
The curmudgeon—ill-tempered
Not as pleasant
Not as much fun to be around
More stubborn, more (or very) demanding
More grouchy, cranky
Grumpier, fussier, set in ways
Not adaptive to times, more conservative
More restless/edgy
Harder to take care of
More negative complaining
Bad disposition
Much more particular
The solitary old man—dispirited, alone
Drinks more
More lonely
Often lonely with his wife
Has a hard time with getting old
Sat on a park bench
Sits around/does nothing
Sleeps more
Watches more television
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
adult target; the number of participants was too small to
reliably assess, in a post hoc study, whether or not a gender
difference exists in the way the young adults characterize
the old man target. Furthermore, studies need to go beyond
comparing men’s and women’s perceptions, and investigate
within-gender differences, determining whether men who
endorse particular masculinity values (e.g., self-reliance)
report more or less favorable images of old men.
Some of the images of the old man seem to be widely
held by the participants in this study, such as representations of an old man as an active, yet retired man, a sage,
and a curmudgeon. Other representations were less frequently mentioned and are reported after the salience of the
old man’s gender and age was primed with a contrasting
target, such as the old man being depicted as an
“accomplished man” and a “gentleman” when compared
to a younger man or as “handsome” and “(still) a man”
when compared to an old woman. Just six of the 22
representations were found in all three descriptions—the
sage, the timeworn man, the diminished man, the impaired
man, the curmudgeon, and the solitary man. It is noteworthy that all but one of the “common” images are
unfavorable; two call to mind negative late life masculinities and three evoked negative views of aging.
Ageist images of later life masculinities—from being a
dirty old man to a dispirited, solitary old man to a
curmudgeon—are representations presented frequently in
the mass media and literature, from Jack Nicholson in As
Good as it Gets to the Grumpy Old Men films to
Hemmingway’s Old Man at Sea. Finding the old man was
at times favorably visualized as grandfatherly, an affable
companion, and handsome is quite consistent with the way
older men are portrayed in film vis-à-vis older women
(Markson & Taylor, 2000). Thus, uncovering ageist (and
sexist) representations is not surprising, given the pervasiveness and persistence of ageist imagery of men and
women in our culture (Chasteen, Schwarz, & Park, 2002)
and the evaluatively-mixed nature of ageist stereotypes
(Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005)—e.g., old men equally
described as both a sage and as incompetent.
Principally, the images summarized in Fig. 1 and the
content of detailed in Tables 1, 2, 3 attest to the continued
importance of the target’s gender. Unlike the gender-neutral
findings reported by Schmidt and Boland (1986) and
affirmed by Hummert at al. (1990, 1994), even the findings
from the initial sentence completions make clear that
without priming informants’ sensitivity to the old man’s
gender with a comparison to the old woman, later life
masculinities were disclosed. A greater number of the
underlying images of an old man addressed a dimension of
later life masculinity, sometimes positively and other times
negatively, and less than half (41%) of the content
describing the old man was strictly age-specific.
643
When compared to a young man and an old woman,
other distinctive later life masculinities emerged. For
example, the image of an old man as a “former beast in
bed” unquestionably brings to the forefront the old man’s
age and the effects of aging on his sexuality. By addressing
his sexuality the informants were addressing a central
dimension of masculinity (Connell, 1995), one that has
gained greater public recognition with the recent pharmaceutical advertisements for erectile dysfunction (Calasanti
& King, 2005). Similarly, the positive images of how aging
could affect an old man—by being a “gentleman” and an
“accomplished old man”—certainly contain evidence of
later life masculinities and the continuities of expectations
for men to be “successful” in whatever they do as men.
Study 2
As much as the prior study reveals evidence of distinct later
life masculinities within discourses on old men, the extent
to which old men’s perceived masculinity differs from
younger men was not investigated. Since old men are talked
about and imaged in terms of masculine forms such of “the
gentleman” and “the sage,” what is the likelihood that their
perceived masculinity is diminished by their aging? Does
one type of gender performance—being a grandfather, a
sage, an active, retired man—replace earlier masculinity
performances—being a breadwinner, athlete—and maintain
an aura of respectability and masculinity? Is the old man still
perceived as masculine, and any less so than a younger man?
A second study was designed to extend the initial query
on whether an old man is thought to be masculine. It is
probable that an old man is perceived as less masculine
because he is imaged as less active and his appearance
timeworn; in the prior study he was often portrayed by the
unflattering depiction of a “diminished man.” His so-called
diminished masculinity may be especially salient when he
is paired with a young man. Does the context within which
an old man target is evaluated—when he is alone versus
with a younger man versus with a same aged woman—have
bearing on his evaluation? Smiler (2004) has noted that
investigations of how stereotyping is contextually specific
is an underdeveloped area of masculinity research.
Method
Participants Two hundred fifty six undergraduates participated in a questionnaire-based study of “men’s and
women’s beliefs about other people.” Participants were
recruited early in the semester from classes and asked to
take a short questionnaire with them upon leaving class.
Three hundred questionnaires were handed out. The
completed questionnaires were returned through campus
644
mail within the week, yielding an 85% response rate.
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 26 (mean=19.6, SD=
1.3). Women (66%) outnumbered men two to one, and very
nearly all students were white (97%). Their father’s age
ranged from 39 to 72 (mean=49.5, SD=5.6); 54% of the
students’ grandfathers were alive, and all but one student
had interacted with a grandfather in the past year.
Questionnaires Three versions of the questionnaire were
used, yet participants only filled out one version. One was
designed to have participants report their views about “an
old man,” another asked the participants to describe an old
man compared to “an old woman,” and the other asked the
participants to describe an old man in comparison to a
“young man.” Each participant rated the old man target either
without a comparison other or in comparison to an old woman
or a young man. The number of completed questionnaires for
each version was 86, 82, and 88, respectively.
Measures Participants were initially instructed to describe
the old man using a list of personality attributes (the Bem
Sex Role Inventory, BSRI; Bem, 1982). They revealed their
image of the old man for the 30 attributes in the short form
of the BSRI by indicating on a 7-point scale (ranging from
“never or almost never true” to “always or almost always
true”) the extent to which they saw the attribute describing
an old man. Among the listed cultural traits are ten socially
desirable, instrumental attributes (e.g., assertive, independent) that have been used in many gender studies to
describe “masculinity,” ten other attributes (e.g., sensitive
to others, compassionate) assessing traits more commonly
associated with women and “femininity,” and ten nongendered attributes (e.g., tactful, jealous) that are ignored.
To construct summary measures of the instrumental/
masculinity traits and expressive/femininity traits, ratings
on each set of ten attributes were algebraically summed.
Reliability estimates for the “masculinity” and “femininity”
trait measures reveal that they are internally consistent for
each version of the questionnaire (αs range from 0.80 to
0.85 for “masculinity” and 0.89 to 0.93 for “femininity”).
For further discussion of BSRI interpretation, see Bem
(1981) and Spence, Losoff, and Robbins (1991).
A modified from of the Osgood Semantic Differential
developed by Levin (1988) was also used to assess
participants’ descriptions of an old man. Numerous studies
have established the validity of the semantic differential as
a procedure for measuring meaning, and the method has
proven useful for assessing differences in attitudes and
conceptual understanding when perceptual targets are
compared (cf., Levin, 1988). By assessing attitudes toward
the target with opposing, or bi-polar, adjectives, the
semantic differential taps the connotative meaning participants assigned to the three old man targets.
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
The semantic differential consisted of 24 bipolar pairs of
adjectives, each rated on a seven-point scale. The items
were designed to measure attitudes about older persons’
level of instrumentality, competence, acceptability, and
integrity (cf., Intriri, von Eye, & Kelly, 1995). A factor
analysis (varimax orthogonal rotation) of the respondents’
ratings of the old man targets produced a two factor
solution. Numerous labels have been used by researchers to
characterize these two dimensions (e.g., task orientation vs.
social orientation), yet the closest to match to the actual
content of the factors are the terms instrumentality and
acceptability. The first scale evaluates the target’s instrumental/ autonomous character (α=0.80). It is an algebraic
mean of ratings on the 11 paired attributes that loaded highly
on the factor: active–passive, incompetent–competent, powerful–weak, healthy–sickly, confident–scared, creative–uncreative, slow–fast, energetic–lazy, wealthy–poor, educated–
uneducated, and lonely–supported. The item pair flexible–
rigid was discarded. The second scale examines an interpersonal side of the target’s perceived character (α=0.82) with
12 adjective pairs: calm–irritable, attractive–ugly, unpleasant–pleasant, unreliable–reliable, important–unimportant,
depressed–excited, involved–socially isolated, poor memory–good memory, generous–selfish, meaningless–meaningful, awkward–graceful, and loving–angry. For purposes of
scale construction, the item pairs were all coded for a
higher score to reflect greater endorsement of the positive
trait.
Analysis Strategy The research design involved three
distinct targets—two old man targets that explicitly involved comparison others, a young man and an old woman,
and one unanchored target. The means for the three sets of
evaluations of the BSRI traits and the semantic differential
ratings were compared using multivariant analyses of
covariance. To guard against the ways that routine
involvement with older men could differentially affect
some respondents’ evaluation of a target, the participant’s
father’s age and a dummy variable for whether or not the
respondent was interacting with a grandfather were entered
first as covariates.
Results
The two covariates explained little variance among respondents’ assessments. Respondents with older fathers
were less likely to evaluate older men in instrumental/
masculine terms, F (1,246)=4.01, p<0.05. One significant
main effect for the sex of the participants, F (1,246)=4.82,
p < 0.05, was also observed for the way old men’s
femininity was assessed. Women generally saw greater
expressiveness/ femininity in the old man target than men.
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
645
Table 4 Means for masculinity and femininity, instrumentality and
acceptability ratings for three old man stimulus targets.
Measure
Old
man
Old
man/
young
man
Old
man/
old
woman
F (2,246)
BSRI M scale
BSRI F scale
Instrumentality
Acceptability
4.47a
4.72b
4.46c
4.94
4.16
4.83b
4.07
4.60d
4.76a
4.14
4.62c
4.64d
12.02***
14.52***
12.02***
6.39***
Row means sharing a superscript do not significantly differ from one
another at p<0.05 (one-tailed); all other paired comparisons differ.
***p<0.001.
Neither respondents’ sex nor the covariates had any bearing
on the semantic differential assessments of instrumentality
and acceptability.
The MANCOVA examining the three assessments of the
old man’s perceived gender was significant, Wilks=0.834,
F (4,476)=11.30, p<0.001. As expected, the old man target
was differentially described in the three questionnaires in
terms of the “masculinity” scale, F (2,239)=10.58, p<
0.001, and “femininity” scale, F (2,239)=14.49, p<0.001.
Table 4 summarizes the group means and pairwise multiple
comparisons (Bonferroni) for the three old man targets.
Evaluations of the old man differ markedly when he was
judged in situations that highlighted either his age or gender
Fig. 2 Evaluations of old men’s
perceived gender.
(see Fig. 2). When gender and age were not explicitly cued
with a comparison target, the old man was perceived to
have greater feminine qualities than masculine. Similarly,
when attention was called to his age, respondents again
rated the feminine qualities as more characteristic of an old
man than masculine qualities. Not surprisingly, when
compared to an old woman he was perceived to be least
feminine and more masculine.
Ratings on the semantic differential scales were significantly swayed by the old man target evaluated, Wilks=
0.825, F (4,478)=12.04, p<0.001. His instrumentality was
assessed in patterns similar to evaluation of his trait
masculinity (see Table 4 and Fig. 2) and thus deemphasized
when the target was compared to a young man and accented
when he was compared to an old women, F=13.67 (2,240),
p<0.001. The old man’s acceptability was, however,
uniformly diminished when he was compared to another
person, which strongly suggests that an old man’s acceptability is affected by both age and gender norms, F=6.36
(2,240), p<0.01. Age norms make the old man less
acceptable than a young man, and gender norms make
him less tolerable than an old woman.
Discussion
The differential assessment of three old man targets
demonstrates that age and gender norms both underlie
646
young adults’ images of elderly men’s gendered lives. Not
unexpectedly, the old man was perceived as significantly
less instrumental/masculine when compared to a younger
man. Whether assessed with the BSRI or the semantic
differential scales, the instrumental attributes associated
with manhood in our culture—men being forceful, selfreliance, self-assured, strong and robust—were rated as less
characteristic of an old man. His perceived masculinity was
diminished when he was compared to a young man.
Viewing masculinity through the lens of age, the old man
does not measure up to the hegemonic standards of
masculinity typically associated with younger men’s lives
(Connell, 1995). But should he? Ironically, the BSRI
“masculinity” mean for the old man in the unanchored
target is congruent with the normative data Bem (1982)
reported for adult men’s self-evaluations. It was when the
target’s age was intentionally highlighted through direct
comparison to a young man that the respondents diminished his masculine character.
If these evaluations could be generalized to truly reflect
young adults’ beliefs about late life masculinities, as a man
ages he is no longer expected to evidence the characteristics
associated with younger men and manhood. Yet he is still
perceived as a man, especially when compared to an older
woman, when his instrumental qualities and masculine attributes are heavily accented and his feminine side becomes,
comparatively speaking, much less visible. Perhaps the
antifemininity dimension inherent in the standards of
manhood (Thompson & Pleck, 1995) swayed respondents’
evaluation to deemphasize the old man’s perceived femininity when attention was drawn to his gender by asking
informants to compare him to an old woman. Interestingly,
without an explicit comparison target, an old man’s masculine character is not devalued. The results uphold an
argument that gender norms do not fade away as men age
as well as an argument that age prejudices and gender
stereotypes cast old men as both old and men. Interestingly,
the gender of participants had minimal effect on perceptions; with the exception that women attributed greater
“femininity” to the old man than the men did, college-age
men and women imaged the old man similarly.
General Discussion and Conclusions
This article examined whether or not men’s later life likely
remains scripted by masculinity expectations and, more
directly, what types of age-based gender stereotyping is
ordinary for men in later life. According to the seminal
work of Neugarten (Neugarten, Moore, & Lowe, 1965), all
of us sustain an internal timetable about age-appropriate
behavior by which we measure our own and others’ march
across the life course. Age norms define the appropriateness
or inappropriateness of self-presentation, expressed emo-
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
tions, and other specific behaviors. Gender is also salient in
our judgments of elders’ age-appropriate behavior (cf.,
Plath & Ikeda, 1975). The two studies in this article reveal
that gender and age are both perceptually salient when old
men are thought of. People seem to hold clear stereotypic
beliefs that differentiated older and younger men, including
beliefs that highlight old men’s masculinities as well as
personal attributes. As Hearn (1995, p. 99) proposed, men
and masculinities “are talked about, pictured, imaged. To do
so invokes age and agedness. Men and masculinities,
whether in image, text, social practice, or social structure,
are necessarily about aging, sometimes explicitly so, often
implicitly. There is a recurring inseparability of ‘men’ and
age/aging.”
Further, this research underscores how important gender
is an inherent part of later life. Older men are perceived as
men, more so than simply as an elder. This finding is
instructive theoretically, because it is inconsistent with the
gender convergence assertion that in later life age status
outweighs gender and, by implication, an androgynous
standard defines late life (Gutmann, 1987; Sinnott, 1986).
The convergence hypothesis was built on clinical-based
opinion and research showing older men were less masculine/more feminine than young men when assessed with
M-F scales. As evidenced by the descriptions of the old
man compared to an old woman, aging does not seem to
result in the homogenizing of gender expectations.
This pattern of differential evaluation also lends empirical support to the argument that there are multiple
masculinities embedded in American culture, as has been
theorized by men’s studies scholars for the past decade (cf.,
Connell, 1995; Thompson & Whearty, 2004). Researchers
have persuasively demonstrated that men with physical
disabilities are viewed and treated from a different masculinity standard than the ones applicable to men without
disabilities (Gerschick & Miller, 1994), and that masculinities vary by class and race (cf., Duneier, 1992; Turner &
Turner, 1991). Logically, masculinities vary by age as well,
and the descriptions of old men observed in this research
strongly suggest the value of systematically outlining the
blueprints of masculinities that different old men encounter
in their everyday lives. As Smiler (2004) noted, the
relevance of masculinity theory is to reexamine another
construct, in this case “old,” through the lens of masculinity
and ask how manhood and gender influence being old.
Because the late life masculinity stereotypes of old men
seem to rest on different gender standards than applied to
men at earlier stages in the life course, much more
empirical research is needed which outlines the common
and unique dimensions of old men’s gender script (cf.,
Calasanti, 2004).
Also affirmed is that our understanding of old men’s
lives cannot be fully understood without an appreciation of
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
the multidimensionality of age-based gender expectations.
Research has demonstrated that people maintain multiple
stereotypes of the elderly rather than a single, global
stereotype. This line of research principally focused on
the superordinate category older adult (cf., Hummert et al.,
1994; Schmidt & Boland, 1986. Adding gender distinctions, as the present study did with its assessments of the
categories old man (and old woman), it is striking how
many ways the old man target was perceived.
One important limitation of the study is the likelihood
that some of the stereotypes underneath the four metathemes (Tables 1, 2, 3) may be only characteristic of how
young adults perceive old men. The research was restricted
to undergraduates, and no effort was made to identify the
ethnicity/racial background of the participants in this study.
The study of Hummert et al. (1994) revealed that different
age groups hold somewhat different images of a target. And
a study of three generations describing themselves at ages
20, 45, and 70 found the self-descriptions by the men (and
women) were structured by both age and gender norms
(Puglisi, 1983). In the study, young men’s projections into
later life anticipated that their personal characteristics
would reflect rising masculinity to mid-life, followed by
decreases in later life. Older men looking backward made
finer life stage discriminations regarding gender traits and
had richer, more complex cognitive schemas about gender.
Similar studies by McCreary (1990), in which elders rated
themselves at the present time and in two retrospective
points (work-entry and parenthood) and younger respondents reported their gender attributes at the present time and
in two prospective points (parenthood and retirement),
effectively demonstrated that informants’ presentation of
former, present, and future selves were sensitive to models
of aging that acknowledge age-based masculinities (and
femininities). The common denominator is that people
visualize their own changing identities as well as others’
lives in terms of age-based masculinities and femininities
and these need to be more thoroughly investigated with
other ages than young adults.
Being cautious in interpreting the strength of this
research is necessary. The first study tacitly yields the
warning that when participants are asked to describe an
older man target without a comparison, the images of the
old man almost involved as much age stereotyping as
gender stereotyping. The phrase “old man” might have
activated what Cole (1992) called bipolar ageism, which
vacillates between discredited negative stereotypes of old
age and positive constructions (cf., Cuddy et al., 2005;
McHugh, 2003). Thus, informants’ images and the content
they divulged about their images of an old man could be
unduly guided by the label “old man” as well as the
comparison labeling “young man” and “old woman.”
Braithwaite et al. (1985–86) and more recently the meta-
647
analytical review of Kite et al. (2005) have shown that
informants respond in more stereotypic manner when asked
to assess a general versus specific target. Given that the
present research was based on evaluations of a general target
word “old man,” it is possible that students responded to the
label in an exaggerated fashion. Whether such exaggeration
mars and/or masks their underlying beliefs about and
expectations of older men is also uncharted.
A second caution is linked to the findings of the second
study. I have interpreted the findings as affirmation of the
joint effects of age and gender expectations. Nonetheless,
the lack of a reliable difference in the one paired evaluation
of the old man and old woman on the semantic differential
acceptability scale could be evidence for the premise of Kite
et al. (1991) that age scripts and stereotyping outweigh
those of gender in late life. Nonetheless, to think of old men
as less like younger men and more similar to old women
seems to introduce a deficit model of how aging diminishes
men’s vitality (cf., Vaillant, 2002). It also suggests that there
is only one standard of masculinity in American culture.
Surely in old age the social worlds of men and women
might be more similar than they were earlier, but older men
and women bring into later life a gendered biography. This
study has shown that people also seem to anticipate distinct
gendered lives in late life. Standards of masculinity may not
remain stable across the life-span, but neither do they
disappear. Old men are still men after all, and “being a
man” is an expectation that extends into old age.
References
Arber, S., Davidson, K., & Ginn, J. (2003). Gender and ageing:
Changing roles and relationships. Maidenhead, England: Open
University Press.
Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sextyping. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.
Bem, S. L. (1982). Bem sex-role inventory: Professional manual. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Bleiszner, R. (1989). Developmental processes of friendship. In R. G.
Adams & R. Bleiszner (Eds.), Old adult friendship (pp. 108–
126). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Braithwaite, V. A. (1986). Old age stereotypes: Reconciling contradictions. Journal of Gerontology, 41, 353–360.
Braithwaite, V. A., Gibson, D., & Holman, J. (1985–86). Age
stereotyping: Are we oversimplifying the phenomenon? International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 22, 315–325.
Brewer, M. B., Dull, V., & Lui, L. (1981). Perceptions of the elderly:
Stereotypes as prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 41, 656–670.
Brewer, M. B., & Lui, L. (1984). Categorization of the elderly by the
elderly: Effects of perceiver’s category membership. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 585–595.
Butler, R. N. (1969). Age-ism: Another form of bigotry. The
Gerontologist, 9, 243–246.
648
Calasanti, T. M. (2004). Feminist gerontology and old men. Journal of
Gerontology: Social Sciences, 59B, S305–S314.
Calasanti, T. M., & King, N. (2005). Firming the floppy penis: Age,
class, and gender relations in the lives of old men. Men and
Masculinities, 8, 3–23.
Canetto, S. S., Kaminski, P. L., & Felicio, D. M. (1995). Typical and
optimal aging in women and men: Is there a double standard?
International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 40,
187–207.
Chasteen, A. L., Schwarz, N., & Park, D. C. (2002). The activation of
aging stereotypes in younger and older adults. Journal of
Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57B, P540–P547.
Cole, T. R. (1992). The journey of life: A cultural history of aging in
America. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Connell, R. W. (1995). Masculinities. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Connell, R. W. (2000). The men and the boys. Berkeley, CA:
University of California Press.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Norton, M., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). This old
stereotype: The pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly
stereotype. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 267–285.
Deutsch, F. M., Zalenski, C. M., & Clark, M. E. (1986). Is there a
double standard of aging? Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
16, 771–785.
Duneier, M. (1992). Slim’s table: Race, respectability, and masculinity.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Ekerdt, D. J. (1986). The busy ethic: Moral continuity between work
and retirement. The Gerontologist, 26, 239–244.
Emslie, C., Hunt, K., & O’Brien, R. (2004). Masculinities in older
men: A qualitative study in the west of Scotland. Journal of
Men’s Studies, 12, 207–226.
Erikson, E., Kivnick, H. Q., & Erikson, J. M. (1994). Vital
involvement in old age: The experience of old age in our time.
New York: Norton.
Gerschick, T. J., & Miller, A. S. (1994). Gender identities at the
crossroads of masculinity and physical disability. Masculinities,
2, 34–55.
Green, B. S. (1993). Gerontology and the construction of old age:
A study in discourse analysis. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de
Gruyter.
Gutmann, D. (1987). Reclaimed powers: Toward a new psychology of
men and women in late life. New York: Basic.
Hearn, J. (1995). Imaging the aging of men. In M. Featherstone & A.
Wernick (Eds.), Images of aging: Cultural representations of
later life (pp. 97–115). London, UK: Routledge.
Hummert, M. L. (1990). Multiple stereotypes of elderly and young
adults: A comparison of structure and evaluation. Psychology
and Aging, 5, 182–193.
Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., & Shaner, J. L. (1997). Stereotyping
of older adults: The role of target facial cues and perceived
characteristics. Psychology and Aging, 12, 107–114.
Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., Shaner, J. L., & Strahm, S. (1994).
Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle-age, and elderly
adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 49,
P240–P249.
Intriri, R. C., von Eye, A., & Kelly, J. A. (1995). The aging semantic
differential: A confirmatory factor analysis. The Gerontologist,
35, 616–621.
Jackson, L. A., & Sullivan, L. A. (1988). Age stereotype disconfirming information and evaluations of old people. Journal of Social
Psychology, 128, 721–729.
Kidder, T. (1993). Old friends. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Kimmel, M. S. (1996). Manhood in America: A cultural history. New
York: Free.
Sex Roles (2006) 55:633–648
Kite, M. E., Deaux, K., & Miele, M. (1991). Stereotypes of young and
old: Does age outweigh gender. Psychology and Aging, 6, 19–27.
Kite, M. E., Stockdale, G. D., Whitley, B. E., & Johnson, B. T. (2005).
Attitudes toward younger and older adults: An updated metaanalytic review. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 241–266.
Kosberg, J., & Kaye, L. (1997). Elderly men: Special problems and
professional challenges. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.
Levin, W. C. (1988). Age stereotyping: College student evaluations.
Research on Aging, 10, 134–148.
Markson, E. W., & Taylor, C. A. (2000). The mirror has two faces.
Ageing and Society, 20, 137–160.
Martin, P. Y. (2004). Gender as social institution. Social Forces, 84,
1249–1273.
McCreary, D. R. (1990). Self-perceptions of life-span gender-role
development. International Journal of Aging and Human
Development, 31, 135–146.
McHugh, K. E. (2003). Three faces of ageism: Society, image, and
place. Ageing and Society, 23, 165–185.
Minnigerode, F. A., & Lee, J. A. (1978). Young adults’ perceptions of
social sex roles across the life span. Sex Roles, 4, 563–569.
Morgan, D. H. (1992). Discovering men: Sociology and masculinities.
New York: Routledge.
Neugarten, B. L., Moore, J. W., & Lowe, J. C. (1965). Age norms,
age constraints, and adult socialization. American Journal of
Sociology, 70, 710–717.
Plath, D., & Ikeda, K. (1975). After coming of age: Adult awareness
of age norms. In T. Williams (Ed.), Socialization and communication in primary groups (pp. 107–123). Mouton, The Netherlands: The Hague.
Puglisi, J. T. (1983). Self-perceived age changes in sex role selfconcept. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 16, 183–191.
Ryff, C. D. (1991). Possible selves in adulthood and old age: A tale of
shifting horizons. Psychology and Aging, 6, 286–295.
Schmidt, D. F., & Boland, S. M. (1986). Structure of perceptions of
older adults: Evidence for multiple stereotypes. Psychology and
Aging, 1, 255–260.
Sinnott, J. D. (1986). Sex roles and aging: Theory and research from a
systems perspective. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.
Smiler, A. P. (2004). Thirty years after the discovery of gender:
Psychological concepts and measures of masculinity. Sex Roles,
50, 15–26.
Spence, J. T., Losoff, M., & Robbins, A. S. (1991). Sexually aggressive
tactics in dating relationships: Personality and attitudinal correlates. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 10, 289–304.
Thompson, E. H. (1994). Older men’s lives. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Thompson, E. H., & Pleck, J. H. (1995). Masculinity ideologies: A
review of research instrumentation on men and masculinities. In
R. Levant & W. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp.
129–163). New York: Basic.
Thompson, E. H., & Whearty, P. M. (2004). Old men’s social
participation: The importance of masculinity ideology. Journal of
Men’s Studies, 13, 5–24.
Turner, B. F., & Turner, C. B. (1991). Bem sex-role inventory stereotypes
for men and women varying in age and race among National
Register psychologists. Psychological Reports, 69, 931–944.
Vaillant, G. E. (2002). Aging well. New York: Little, Brown.
West, C., & Zimmerman, D. H. (1987). Doing gender. Gender and
Society,1, 125–151.
Zepelin, H., Sills, R. A., & Heath, M. W. (1987). Is age becoming
irrelevant? An exploratory study of perceived age norms.
International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 24,
241–256.