Research on
Gang-Related
Violence in the
21st Century
Edited by
Matthew Valasik and Shannon E. Reid
Printed Edition of the Special Issue Published in Social Sciences
www.mdpi.com/journal/socsci
Research on Gang-Related Violence in
the 21st Century
Research on Gang-Related Violence in
the 21st Century
Editors
Matthew Valasik
Shannon E. Reid
MDPI • Basel • Beijing • Wuhan • Barcelona • Belgrade • Manchester • Tokyo • Cluj • Tianjin
Editors
Matthew Valasik
Shannon E. Reid
Sociology
Criminology Criminal Justice
Louisiana State University
University of North Carolina,
Baton Rouge
Charlotte
United States
Charlotte
United States
Editorial Office
MDPI
St. Alban-Anlage 66
4052 Basel, Switzerland
This is a reprint of articles from the Special Issue published online in the open access
journal Social Sciences (ISSN 2076-0760) (available at: www.mdpi.com/journal/socsci/special issues/
Gang-Related Violence in the 21st Century).
For citation purposes, cite each article independently as indicated on the article page online and as
indicated below:
LastName, A.A.; LastName, B.B.; LastName, C.C. Article Title. Journal Name Year, Volume Number,
Page Range.
ISBN 978-3-0365-1534-2 (Hbk)
ISBN 978-3-0365-1533-5 (PDF)
© 2021 by the authors. Articles in this book are Open Access and distributed under the Creative
Commons Attribution (CC BY) license, which allows users to download, copy and build upon
published articles, as long as the author and publisher are properly credited, which ensures maximum
dissemination and a wider impact of our publications.
The book as a whole is distributed by MDPI under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons
license CC BY-NC-ND.
Contents
About the Editors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii
Matthew Valasik and Shannon E. Reid
“The More Things Change, the More They Stay the Same”: Research on Gang-Related Violence
in the 21st Century—Introduction to Special Issue
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2021, 10, 225, doi:10.3390/socsci10060225 . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
1
Dena Carson and Natalie Kroovand Hipple
Comparing Violent and Non-Violent Gang Incidents: An Exploration of Gang-Related Police
Incident Reports
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2020, 9, 199, doi:10.3390/socsci9110199 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
7
Matthew Valasik and Shannon E. Reid
East Side Story: Disaggregating Gang Homicides in East Los Angeles
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2021, 10, 48, doi:10.3390/socsci10020048 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
Gisela Bichler, Alexis Norris and Citlalik Ibarra
Evolving Patterns of Aggression: Investigating the Structure of Gang Violence during the Era
of Civil Gang Injunctions
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2020, 9, 203, doi:10.3390/socsci9110203 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
Marta-Marika Urbanik and Robert A. Roks
Making Sense of Murder: The Reality versus the Realness of Gang Homicides in Two Contexts
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2021, 10, 17, doi:10.3390/socsci10010017 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59
Alice Airola and Martin Bouchard
The Social Network Consequences of a Gang Murder Blowout
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2020, 9, 204, doi:10.3390/socsci9110204 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
Caterina G. Roman, Meagan Cahill and Lauren R. Mayes
Changes in Personal Social Networks across Individuals Leaving Their Street Gang: Just What
Are Youth Leaving Behind?
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2021, 10, 39, doi:10.3390/socsci10020039 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
Jordan M. Hyatt, James A. Densley and Caterina G. Roman
Social Media and the Variable Impact of Violence Reduction Interventions: Re-Examining
Focused Deterrence in Philadelphia
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2021, 10, 147, doi:10.3390/socsci10050147 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 115
Jaimee Mallion and Jane Wood
Street Gang Intervention: Review and Good Lives Extension
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2020, 9, 160, doi:10.3390/socsci9090160 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 133
Nicole J. Johnson, Caterina G. Roman, Alyssa K. Mendlein, Courtney Harding, Melissa
Francis and Laura Hendrick
Exploring the Influence of Drug Trafficking Gangs on Overdose Deaths in the Largest Narcotics
Market in the Eastern United States
Reprinted from: Social Sciences 2020, 9, 202, doi:10.3390/socsci9110202 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 157
v
About the Editors
Matthew Valasik
Matthew Valasik is an Associate Professor in the Department of Sociology at Louisiana State
University. His research interests include the socio-spatial dynamics of gang behavior and effective
strategies aimed at reducing neighborhood violence and discouraging gang activity. He is also
co-author of Alt-Right Gangs: A Hazy Shade of White, published by University of California Press
in September 2020, examining the rise of Alt-Right groups through the lens of street gang research.
Shannon E. Reid
Shannon Reid is an Associate Professor in the Department of Criminal Justice and Criminology
at the University of North Carolina—Charlotte. Her research interests include criminological theory,
gangs, GIS spatial analysis, juvenile delinquency, policing, quantitative methods, social network
analysis, and structural equation modeling. She is also co-author of Alt-Right Gangs: A Hazy Shade of
White, published by University of California Press in September 2020, examining the rise of Alt-Right
groups through the lens of street gang research.
vii
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Editorial
“The More Things Change, the More They Stay the Same”:
Research on Gang-Related Violence in the 21st
Century—Introduction to Special Issue
Matthew Valasik 1, *
and Shannon E. Reid 2, *
1
2
*
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Citation: Valasik, Matthew, and
Shannon E. Reid. 2021. “The More
Things Change, the More They Stay
the Same”: Research on Gang-Related
Violence in the 21st
Century—Introduction to Special
Issue. Social Sciences 10: 225.
https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci
10060225
Received: 9 June 2021
Accepted: 9 June 2021
Published: 11 June 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
Sociology Department, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Criminology & Criminal Justice, UNC Charlotte, 9201 University City Blvd, Charlotte, NC 28223, USA
Correspondence:
[email protected] (M.V.);
[email protected] (S.E.R.)
The goal of this Special Issue is to examine the diverse nature of gang-related violence
in modern life by providing insights into the growing complexities to better direct public
policy solutions in the 21st Century. Multiple perspectives and analytical techniques (e.g.,
quantitative, qualitative, or mixed-methods), across the United States and globally, are
necessary to unpack the dynamic nature of gang-related violence today. Work on this
Special Issue began just prior to COVID-19 upending the world and with it everyone’s
daily routines. We would like to thank first and foremost all of the contributors to this
Special Issue. Despite the exceptionally challenging life circumstances, each article in this
Special Issue highlights novel research methodologies to better understand gang violence
and potential interventions to reduce it.
As the patterns of daily life changed with COVID-19 (i.e., quarantines, social distancing, etc.), researchers were frantic to understand how these changes to routine activities
impacted all types of crime, from domestic violence (Nix and Richards 2021), to organized
crime (De la Miyar et al. 2021), to cybercrime (Hawdon et al. 2020), and everything in
between (Halford et al. 2020; Mohler et al. 2020; Rosenfeld and Lopez 2020). Studies generally show that crime patterns varied by the particular type of crime (e.g., theft, robbery,
domestic violence, homicide, etc.) suggesting that the changes in the mobility patterns
of offenders and victims directly contributes to these trends (Halford et al. 2020). Yet,
emerging studies reveal that gang-related violence either remained stable (Brantingham
et al. 2021) or increased (Kim and Phillips 2021) during the COVID-19 restrictions. It seems
that even a global pandemic is unable to disrupt the prevalence of gang-related violence
once it is entrenched in a community (see Valasik et al. 2017). That is, conflict, including
the threat or fear of potential violence, remains the principal driver in sustaining gang life.
Dena Carson and Natalie Hipple (Carson and Hipple 2020), in their contribution to
this Special Issue, “Comparing violent and non-violent gang incidents: An exploration
of gang-related police incident reports,” examine gang-related incident reports collected
over four years (2015–2019) from the Indianapolis Metropolitan Police Department. They
explore the reasons why incidents were attributed to gangs, and compare the characteristics
of violent, drug, and non-violent gang-related incidents. Their analyses focus on examining
the incident characteristics that influence a reporting officer’s categorization of an incident
as being gang-related and differentiating between violent, drug, and other non-violent
crimes. Carson and Hipple find that non-violent crimes make up the bulk of gang-related
incidents, followed by drug and then violent crimes. In fact, few incidents are labeled as
gang-related by police and the prevalence is decreasing annually. Furthermore, violent
crime incidents were more likely to be brought to the attention of the police through calls
for service, with it being rare for police to observe violent gang-related activity during
routine patrols. Overall, this study’s findings are valuable to policy makers, criminal
justice actors, and local agencies that work with gang members, since most (62%) of the
4.0/).
1
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 225
gang-related incident reports involve non-violent offenses, requiring programs and policies
to address more than just violence.
Matthew Valasik and Shannon Reid (Valasik and Reid 2021), in “East Side Story:
Disaggregating Gang Homicides in East Los Angeles”, argue that gang-related homicides
are not monolithic but have significantly distinct characteristics. Their study discusses
the variation in the circumstances, motives, setting, participant characteristics, and rivalry
relationship present in gang-related homicides to see how they vary from one another.
Using Latent Class Analysis (LCA) to classify cases into mutually exclusive types (classes),
Valasik and Reid examine gang-related homicides in the LAPD’s Hollenbeck Community
Policing Area between 1990 and 2012. The results reveal five mutually exclusive classes of
gang-related violence that are distinct from each other. Valasik and Reid’s study provides
evidence that patterned variation exists in gang-related homicides, arguing that disaggregation should be a regularly employed tool to understand the unique differences between
classes of gang-related homicides for policy, law enforcement response, and research.
Civil gang injunctions (CGIs) have been a crime control strategy used to target the most
violent street gangs by imposing behavioral restrictions on enjoined gang members (e.g.,
prohibitions against congregating in public) within a designated area (e.g., a gang’s claimed
turf) with the hopes of deterring gang violence. The City of Los Angeles, in particular, has
adopted this tactic. Gisela Bichler, Alexis Norris, and Citlalik Ibarra (Bichler et al. 2020),
in “Evolving Patterns of Aggression: Investigating the Structure of Gang Violence during
the Era of Civil Gang Injunctions”, examine the four social networks generated from
violent incidents occurring between the years 1998 and 2013 involving enjoined gang
members. The novel data were generated by linking defendants and victims named in 963
prosecutions involving street gangs enjoined with a CGI. Bichler and colleague’s goal is
ascertaining whether the substantive shifts in the structure of violence correspond with
phases of CGI adoption in Los Angeles. Exploring the structure of conflict through a
triad census, their findings reveal that across time periods, a substantial number of simple
structures reflect a domino pattern of aggression. That is, one group attacks another, who
then attacks a third group. Bichler and colleagues suggest that this structure of violence
indicates that not all gangs are equal, with some gangs being unable to retaliate and
instead prey upon weaker groups. Additionally, there was a substantial change after
CGIs were introduced, with many simple structures of violence shifting to more complex
patterns. The findings reveal that enjoined gangs were more likely to attack other enjoined
gangs, and excessively aggressive gangs were less likely to be victimized. This study
provides support for targeted enforcement strategies being able to facilitate change in gang
violence; however, as more and more injunctions were enacted the nature of gang conflict
became more complex. As such, the disruption of future gang violence may become more
challenging in the long-term.
Alice Airola and Martin Bouchard’s (Airola and Bouchard 2020) contribution, “The
Social Network Consequences of a Gang Murder Blowout,” also utilizes social network
analysis (SNA), but focuses on a sole gang, Red Scorpion, whose members were involved
in the Surrey Six Murder, one of the deadliest gang-related homicides in Canada. By
using SNA, Airola and Bouchard are able to examine the network consequences for the
organization and its members resulting from this gang-related homicide. The following
three types of ties are focused on in the network: trust ties (strong), business ties (weak),
and conflict ties (negative). Airola and Bouchard compare the different social ties and the
level of control during the conspiracy phase and the post-murder phase, revealing that
the fragmentation and network size increased post-murder, whereas the Red Scorpion’s
network density and centralization decreased. Similar to the terrorist group, Toronto 18,
Red Scorpion’s network showed signs of fragmentation after crisis as the role of the leader
was diminished after the murder. In addition, following the murder, the proportion of
trust ties increased and so did the number of positive and balanced cliques, which, as
Airola and Bouchard argue, suggests that such strong ties are effective at maintaining
the information flow and control of a group when facing a crisis. This unique case study
2
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 225
provides glimpses into how the composition of street gangs are not static structures but
change in response to stimuli, for instance a law enforcement murder investigation and
prosecution, and should be kept in mind when such groups are targeted with intervention
and/or suppression strategies.
Marta Urbanik and Robby Roks’ (Urbanik and Roks 2021) article, “Making Sense of
Murder: The Reality versus the Realness of Gang Homicides in Two Contexts,” employs a
multi-site (Canada and the Netherlands) ethnographic approach to illuminate how gang
members experience their associates’ murder(s). Particular attention focuses on how gang
members make sense of and respond to the fatalities of their peers. Gang members in both
Canada and the Netherlands made sense of and navigated a fellow member’s murder(s)
by conducting pseudo-homicide investigations, being hypervigilant, and attributing blame
to the victim. They discussed potential suspects and motives, reviewed eyewitness reports,
and analyzed crime scene photos published by the local media. Urbanik and Roks find
that following a gang-related murder, fellow gang members are more likely to carry a
weapon, pay extra attention to strangers, minimize their presence outdoors, and share
suspicions about who may be responsible. In the Netherlands, a gang member’s murder
planted doubts about the function and necessity of defending a gang’s claimed turf. While
in Canada, many gang members became desensitized and accustomed to the routineness
of fellow gang members being murdered, with these events breeding distrust between
group members. Urbanik and Roks’ approach is eye-opening, providing an alternative
data source that is not police generated and is better able to unpack the micro processes
associated with gang-related homicides.
In another multi-site study (two east coast cities in the United States), Caterina Roman,
Meagan Cahill, and Lauren R. Mayes’ (Roman et al. 2021) article, “Changes in Personal
Social Networks across Individuals Leaving Their Street Gang: Just What Are Youth
Leaving Behind?”, analyzes the changes in a gang member’s personal network composition
as it is associated with changes in a gang member’s membership stage. Using novel
survey data, Roman and colleagues observe notable differences between individuals who
reported leaving their gang and fully disengaging from their gang associates, and those
who report leaving but still interact with their gang friends. The results indicate that
the individuals who fully disengaged from their street gang acquired more prosocial
relationships and reduced some of their criminal behavior. For those who left their gang
but still interact with fellow gang members, however, there were limited changes for both
criminal behavior and network composition over time. Roman and colleagues contend
that a complete withdrawal from interaction with old gang members is likely followed by
large changes in the composition of social networks, particularly if prosocial relationships
can be established. This study supports the notion that crime desistance is more clearly
tied to full disengagement than simply de-identification as a gang member. These findings
suggest that gang intervention programs that use street outreach workers may be an
effective strategy to reduce violence and put high-risk individuals and gang members on
prosocial paths.
A gang intervention that has consistently been shown as an effective strategy at reducing gang-related violence is focused deterrence. Involving a mixture of strong enforcement
messages from law enforcement officials that stress the costs and consequences of gun
violence combined with the promise of additional social support and resources, focused
deterrence has been employed across many jurisdictions over the last thirty years. Focused
deterrence was implemented in Philadelphia between 2013 and 2016, resulting in a significant decrease in shootings; however, the effect on targeted gangs was not universal,
with some showing no change or an increase in gun-related activity. Jordan Hyatt, James
Densley, and Caterina Roman (Hyatt et al. 2021), in “Social Media and the Variable Impact
of Violence Reduction Interventions: Re-Examining Focused Deterrence in Philadelphia,”
study the extent to which social media may explain this differential. Specifically, does social
media’s use as a venue for communication diminish the robustness of a focused deterrence
message, which focuses on reinforcing a sense of collective accountability. Employing data
3
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 225
on group-level social media usage and content, Hyatt and colleagues reveal that all street
gangs have an online presence and promoted violence in almost one-third of their postings.
Furthermore, gangs with more shootings are younger with slightly larger memberships,
suggesting that these groups are likely to have a higher overall social media usage score
and a larger, more visible footprint on the internet. While the social media posts that
are considered to be an immediate threat did not correlate with an increase in shootings
during a focused deterrence period, instead the broader pattern of online engagement was
associated with an increased level of risk. These findings support the link between the
online activity of gang members and violence on the streets, especially shootings. As such,
this study reinforces the need to not ignore social media when developing harm-prevention
interventions, including focused deterrence, for gang-involved individuals.
A novel approach to street gang intervention is the Good Lives Model (GLM), a
strengths-based framework for offender rehabilitation. Mallion and Wood (2020), in “Street
Gang Intervention: Review and Good Lives Extension,” maintain that this public health
approach to gang membership assumes that criminal behavior occurs when individuals
are unable to attain their goals through prosocial means and will instead attempt to
achieve these goals through any means necessary, including antisocial behaviors. For
gang members in particular, gangs may provide a sense of protection and support, yet
there is also an increased risk of violent victimization and mental illness. Mallion and
Wood highlight that GLM interventions aim to develop an individual’s internal (i.e., skills
and values) and external capacities (i.e., resources, support, and opportunities). Current
prevention and intervention strategies are limited in their effectiveness, as the benefits of
belonging to a gang (e.g., protection, social and emotional support, sense of identity) extend
beyond the normal proceeds of crime (i.e., financial or material gain) and are generally not
adequately targeted in traditional interventions. GLM-consistent interventions provide a
relatively new framework that may increase client engagement and motivation to change.
Lastly, “Exploring the Influence of Drug Trafficking Gangs on Overdose Deaths
in the Largest Narcotics Market in the Eastern United States” by Nicole Johnson, Caterina Roman, Alyssa Mendlein, Courtney Harding, Melissa Francis, and Laura Hendrick
(Johnson et al. 2020) investigates whether deaths from accidental drug overdose are clustered around street corners controlled by drug trafficking gangs in a large neighborhood
in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. Using a concentration metric, the Rare Event Concentration Coefficient, to assess clustering of overdose deaths annually between 2015 and 2019,
the study reveals that overdose deaths became less clustered over time. Johnson and
colleagues find several socio-structural factors that are associated with a higher rate of
overdose deaths, including concentrated disadvantage and physical environmental factors
(e.g., dilapidated or deteriorated housing). Additionally, both the gang corner status and
the proximity to a street gang were significantly related to the rate of overdose incidents on
each street corner. The findings suggest that programs seeking to address overdose deaths
should be both mobile and be specifically targeted to risky places. Implications of this
study highlight the need for efforts to strategically coordinate law enforcement and social
service provisions and reduce deteriorated housing stock would be the most effective at
reducing drugs, other crime, and overdoses.
The contributions included in this Special Issue highlight the complex nature of
gang-related violence in the 21st Century. As much as policy makers, the media, and
even scholars like to simplify gang-related violence, all of the above studies highlight the
nuance and variation that exists. Furthermore, tried and true approaches (e.g., homicide
disaggregation) and interventions (e.g., focused deterrence, CGIs) should be reviewed and
brought into the 21st Century to address gang-related violence effectively and appropriately
in the present day. Through the use of novel data and methods, the studies presented in
this Special Issue reinforce this need. Gang-related violence is a persistent beast, difficult to
dislodge from communities and, as society begins to reopen from the COVID-19 pandemic
and return to our daily routines, it will be there waiting for us to reemerge.
4
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 225
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, M.V. and S.E.R.; writing—original draft preparation, M.V.;
writing—review and editing, M.V. and S.E.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Airola, Alice, and Martin Bouchard. 2020. The Social Network Consequences of a Gang Murder Blowout. Social Sciences 9: 204.
[CrossRef]
Bichler, Gisela, Alexis Norris, and Citlalik Ibarra. 2020. Evolving Patterns of Aggression: Investigating the Structure of Gang Violence
during the Era of Civil Gang Injunctions. Social Sciences 9: 203. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, P. Jeffrey, George E. Tita, and George Mohler. 2021. Gang-related crime in Los Angeles remained stable following
COVID-19 social distancing orders. Criminology & Public Policy. [CrossRef]
Carson, Dena, and Natalie K. Hipple. 2020. Comparing violent and non-violent gang incidents: An exploration of gang-related police
incident reports. Social Sciences 9: 199. [CrossRef]
De la Miyar, Jose Roberto Balmori, Lauren Hoehn-Velasco, and Adan Silverio-Murillo. 2021. Druglords don’t stay at home: COVID-19
pandemic and crime patterns in Mexico City. Journal of Criminal Justice 72: 101745. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Halford, Eric, Anthony Dixon, Graham Farrell, Nicolas Malleson, and Nick Tilley. 2020. Crime and coronavirus: Social distancing,
lockdown, and the mobility elasticity of crime. Crime Science 9: 1–12. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hawdon, James, Katalin Parti, and Thomas E. Dearden. 2020. Cybercrime in America amid COVID-19: The initial results from a
natural experiment. American Journal of Criminal Justice 45: 546–62. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hyatt, Jordan M., James A. Densley, and Caterina G. Roman. 2021. Social Media and the Variable Impact of Violence Reduction
Interventions: Re-Examining Focused Deterrence in Philadelphia. Social Sciences 10: 147. [CrossRef]
Johnson, Nicole J., Caterina G. Roman, Alyssa K. Mendlein, Courtney Harding, Melissa Francis, and Laura Hendrick. 2020. Exploring
the Influence of Drug Trafficking Gangs on Overdose Deaths in the Largest Narcotics Market in the Eastern United States. Social
Sciences 9: 202. [CrossRef]
Kim, Dae-Young, and Scott W. Phillips. 2021. When COVID-19 and guns meet: A rise in shootings. Journal of Criminal Justice 73: 101783.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mallion, Jaimee, and Jane Wood. 2020. Street Gang Intervention: Review and Good Lives Extension. Social Sciences 9: 160. [CrossRef]
Mohler, George, Andrea L. Bertozzi, Jeremy Carter, Martin B. Short, Daniel Sledge, George E. Tita, Craig D. Uchida, and P. Jeffrey
Brantingham. 2020. Impact of social distancing during COVID-19 pandemic on crime in Los Angeles and Indianapolis. Journal of
Criminal Justice 68: 101692. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nix, Justin, and Tara N. Richards. 2021. The immediate and long-term effects of COVID-19 stay-at-home orders on domestic violence
calls for service across six US jurisdictions. Police Practice and Research 22: 1443–51. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., Meagan Cahill, and Lauren R. Mayes. 2021. Changes in Personal Social Networks across Individuals Leaving
Their Street Gang: Just What Are Youth Leaving Behind? Social Sciences 10: 39. [CrossRef]
Rosenfeld, Richard, and Ernesto Lopez. 2020. Pandemic, Social Unrest, and Crime in US Cities. Washington, DC: Council on
Criminal Justice.
Urbanik, Marta-Marika, and Robert A. Roks. 2021. Making Sense of Murder: The Reality versus the Realness of Gang Homicides in
Two Contexts. Social Sciences 10: 17. [CrossRef]
Valasik, Matthew, and Shannon E. Reid. 2021. East Side Story: Disaggregating Gang Homicides in East Los Angeles. Social Sciences
10: 48. [CrossRef]
Valasik, Matthew, Michael S. Barton, Shannon E. Reid, and George E. Tita. 2017. Barriocide: Investigating the temporal and spatial
influence of neighborhood structural characteristics on gang and non-gang homicides in East Los Angeles. Homicide Studies 21:
287–311. [CrossRef]
5
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Comparing Violent and Non-Violent Gang Incidents:
An Exploration of Gang-Related Police
Incident Reports
Dena Carson 1, *
1
2
*
and Natalie Kroovand Hipple 2
Paul H. O’Neill School of Public and Environmental Affairs, Indiana University–Purdue University
Indianapolis, Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA
Department of Criminal Justice, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Received: 24 September 2020; Accepted: 31 October 2020; Published: 3 November 2020
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Abstract: Prior research has established a strong link between gangs and violence. Additionally, this
connection is demonstrated across multiple methodologies such as self-report surveys, qualitative
interviews, as well as official records. Officially recorded gang data can be increasingly hard to obtain
because data collection approaches differ by agency, county, city, state, and country. One method for
obtaining official gang data is through the analysis of police incident reports, which often rely on
police officers’ subjective classification of an incident as “gang-related.” In this study we examine
741 gang-related incident reports collected over four years from the Indianapolis Metropolitan Police
Department. This study will explore reasons why incidents were attributed to gangs as well as
compare the characteristics of violent, drug, and non-violent gang-related incidents. This work has
implications for understanding the complexities associated with gang incident reports as well as for
the commonality of violent gang crimes.
Keywords: gang; violence; incident reports; police data
1. Introduction
The link between gangs and violence is well-established in prior literature, which has resulted
in gang researchers naming violent behavior as one of the key features of gang life (Carson et al.
2017; Decker 1996; Irwin-Rogers et al. 2019; Pyrooz et al. 2016). This strong relationship between
gangs and violence persists across time, geographic location, and appears regardless of the research
methodology (e.g., ethnographies, survey data, official records). Early ethnographic gang researchers
identified themes surrounding violent behavior (Thrasher [1927] 1963; Yablonsky 1962) and more
recent ethnographic research discusses gang-related violence in the United States (U.S.) and other
countries (Andell 2019; Brenneman 2012; Decker and Winkle 1996; Densley 2013; Deuchar 2018; Garot
2010; Ward 2013; Weaver 2016). Individual-level survey data that compare violence among gang and
non-gang youth find that violent offenses are overwhelmingly committed by gang youth (Esbensen et
al. 2010; Melde and Esbensen 2013; Pyrooz et al. 2016; Thornberry et al. 2003). The link between gangs
and violence is also echoed in the analysis of police homicide data from several cities across the United
States (U.S.) (Adams and Pizarro 2014; Huebner et al. 2016; Papachristos et al. 2015; Papachristos et al.
2013; Pizarro and McGloin 2006; Pyrooz et al. 2010; Pyrooz et al. 2011; Rosenfeld et al. 1999).
While it is important to understand the violent nature of gangs, researchers often find that gangs
and gang members are involved in other types of non-violent offending. The “cafeteria-style” nature
of offending among gang members is largely supported in both qualitative (Decker and Winkle 1996;
Fleisher 1998; Lauger 2012; Miller 2001) and survey research (Esbensen and Carson 2012; Thornberry
7
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
1998; Thornberry et al. 2003; Weerman and Esbensen 2005). However, due to the emphasis on using
police data to understand gang-involved homicides, we know less about other gang-related crimes
that come to the attention of the police. This gap in the literature is partially due to law enforcement
practices that may limit the range of offenses that are labeled gang-related. Research by Decker and
Kempf-Leonard (1991) as well as Klein and Maxson (2006) suggest that law enforcement agencies
are restrictive in their definitions of gang activity and may fail to attribute non-violent crime to
gangs.. While the research shows that gang members may specialize in violence (Melde and Esbensen
2013; Pyrooz and Decker 2013) and that there is a benefit to understanding gang-motived homicides,
see (Rosenfeld et al. 1999), a narrow focus on violent gang incidents can reinforce the stereotype that
gangs are only involved in violence (Klein and Maxson 2006).
In addition to a heavy focus on violent gang acts, there is a high degree of variation across cities
and agencies in the identification of an incident as gang-related (Kennedy et al. 1997; Maxson and
Klein 1990; Pyrooz et al. 2011). Research on gang homicides demonstrates that some law enforcement
agencies label incidents as gang-motivated (i.e., those that result from gang operations such as turf wars
or gang rivalries), while other agencies use a much less restrictive definition of gang-related crimes
(i.e., those that involve a gang member) (Curry et al. 1996; Maxson et al. 2002; Maxson et al. 1985).
Other agencies may not have clear standards on what crimes should be or are labeled as gang-related.
These definitional discrepancies result in very different representations of gang crime (Maxson and
Klein 1990) and make it extremely difficult to generalize research findings or policy implications to
different cities and contexts.
A lack of definitional consistency and a failure to recognize the broad range of offenses that gang
members are involved in has major implications for criminal justice responses as well as the social
construction of gangs (Decker and Kempf-Leonard 1991; McCorkle and Miethe 1998). Additionally,
attributing a crime, especially a violent crime, to a gang or gang member has implications for the
prosecuting of these offenses (Pyrooz et al. 2011) and can activate gang enhancements in charging and
sentencing. These enhancements can drastically change the length of a prison sentence (Hall 2019).
Despite these serious implications, we have little empirical knowledge—especially for non-violent
crimes—about why crime incidents are attributed to gangs.
In an attempt to build knowledge in the area, we draw data from 741 police incident reports that
the reporting officer labeled as a gang-related incident. These incidents occurred in the American city
of Indianapolis, Indiana from 2015 to 2019. Indianapolis is a Midwestern city located in the “Crossroads
of America.” The city spans roughly 400 square miles. In 2019, the U.S. Census Bureau estimated the
city population to be roughly 886,000 making it the 17th most populous city in the U.S. In 2018, driven
by gun violence, Indianapolis experienced 1278 violent crimes per 100,000 people compared to the
national average of 369 per 100,000 people (Federal Bureau of Investigation 2018). The Indianapolis
Metropolitan Police Department (IMPD) is the largest law enforcement agency in Indiana employing
roughly 1700 sworn officers. IMPD is ranked consistently as one of the 30 largest police departments
in the U.S.1 Given these numbers, we believe that Indianapolis provides a suitable setting for our
research goals. Our first goal is to explore the reasons why reporting officers labeled an incident as
gang-related. Our second goal is to compare characteristics of violent, drug, and other non-violent
gang-related incidents.
2. The Validity of Police Perceptions of Crime
The empirical use of official police data and incident reports is common practice in criminology
and criminal justice literature. While use of these data are essential for improving our understanding
of crime, they were not intended for research purposes and scholars using these data have pointed to a
number of methodological limitations (Alison et al. 2001; Katz et al. 2012; Levitt 1998). These include
1
http://www.bjs.gov/index.cfm?ty=pbdetail&iid=6706.
8
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
variation in the amount of detail provided based on the reporting officer (Alison et al. 2001) as well
as a certain amount of reporting bias (Fisher 1993; Levitt 1998). Due in part to these limitations,
police records are viewed as having a certain amount of bias (Braga et al. 1994; Goldstein 1990).
While these flaws are troubling, other research suggests that police perceptions of crime and gangs in
their community are valid generally, as well as for gang research (Decker and Pyrooz 2010; Katz et al.
2000). Braga et al. (1994), for instance, argue that the experiences of law enforcement cause them to
develop a detailed sense of crime in certain neighborhoods and the city.
Of relevance to the current study is conceptions about who/what constitutes a gang as well as a
gang crime. Difficulties surrounding defining a gang and a gang member plague both academics and
practitioners alike (Curry and Decker 1997; Decker et al. 2014; Esbensen et al. 2001; Morash 1983; see,
also, Andell (2019) for a broad discussion in the context of the United Kingdom). Police knowledge
about gangs is often learned on the job (Decker and Kempf-Leonard 1991) and, therefore, likely to
improve with time and experience (Kennedy et al. 1997). Research exploring police perceptions
of gangs in their community find that law enforcement is quite knowledgeable about their local
gang situation (Kennedy et al. 1997). While law enforcement in some cities have a clear definition
of what constitutes gang crime (Maxson and Klein 1990), law enforcement agencies without clear
definitional standards may rely on an officer’s subjective classification of an incident as gang-related or
not. These perceptions, especially among newer officers, may be based on stereotypical, and often
inaccurate, depictions of gang-related crime presented by the media (Esbensen and Tusinski 2007;
Horowitz 1990). In Kennedy et al.’s (1997) analysis of gang violence in Boston, the authors reported
that police officers were quite knowledgeable about gang activity, but tended to believe that almost
all homicides committed by youth were perpetrated by gang members and that all youth homicide
victims were gang members. This finding indicates that law enforcement might attribute violent acts
to gang activity more easily.
Overall, the limitations of data provided by law enforcement underscore the importance of the
current work. The news media and policy makers lean heavily upon law enforcement perceptions
of gangs and gang crime; therefore, it is exceedingly important to understand the reasons behind
the classification of a crime as gang-related as well as variation across crime types. As Decker and
Kempf-Leonard (1991, p. 272) note, “the formulation of effective policy responses to gangs depends on
reliable and valid foundation of knowledge of the ‘gang problem.’”
3. Methodology and Data
Data for this study were initially collected as part of the Southern District of Indiana Project
Safe Neighborhoods2 project. The data come from the Indianapolis Metropolitan Police Department
(IMPD) incident records management system (RMS). The RMS is official police record and includes all
incidents where a police officer documents an illegal or potentially illegal event (i.e., a police report).
This system does not include incidents where the police were called to a scene and determined a crime
had not occurred (i.e., calls for police service). When initiating a police report, the authoring officer can
use a series of “check-boxes” to indicate if the report is gang-related, domestic violence-related, and/or
narcotics-related. The check boxes default to ‘no’ therefore the reporting officer must initiate a change
from ‘no’ to ‘yes.’ The sample includes all incident reports where the gang-related box was checked
(i.e., indicated yes) from 1 January 2015 through 31 May 2019.3 Indiana law (IC 35-45-9-1)4 defines a
“criminal gang” as a formal or informal group with at least three members that specifically:
2
3
4
https://www.justice.gov/psn.
IMPD changed their RMS in June 2019. The new RMS did not have a similar check-box system.
http://iga.in.gov/legislative/laws/2020/ic/titles/035#35-45-9.
9
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
(1)
Either:
(A)
(B)
(C)
(2)
Promotes, sponsors, or assists in;
Participates in; or
Has as one of its goals; or
Requires as a condition of membership or continued membership;
The commission of a felony, an act that would be a felony if committed by an adult, or the offense
of battery as included in IC 35-42-2.5
All law enforcement agencies in Indiana are bound by this gang definition for arrest and charging
purposes, however, we have no way of knowing if officers were guided by this definition when
checking the gang-related box. Similarly, there was no known formal training on the use of any of
the check-boxes.
Overall, incident reports designated as gang-related comprised a minute proportion of police
reports for IMPD over the project period (see Table 1). The proportion of cases that were designated
gang-related steadily decreases every year from 2015 to 2019. IMPD operated under two different
Indianapolis mayors and three different Chiefs of Police during the study period. Differing
administrative priorities leads to organizational changes which may be reflected by the decrease of
gang-related incident reports (Feeley 1973; Hagan 1999; Lipsky 1980).
Table 1. Annual police incident reports.
Incident
Reports
Year
Gang-Related
Reports
Percent
(of Total)
n
%
n
%
2015
2016
2017
2018
2019 *
127,397
128,770
124,725
119,728
45,961
23.3
23.6
22.8
21.9
8.4
266
175
152
89
59
35.9
23.6
20.5
12.0
8.0
0.05
0.03
0.03
0.02
0.01
Total
546,581
100.0
741
100.0
0.14
* Only includes incident reports through 31 May 2019. Source: IMPD Oversight, Audit, and Performance Division.
The majority of data collected from the reports was officer-coded at the time the report was
created, for example, incident location, age, race, and gender of any individuals involved, crime type,
and/or criminal charges. There is also a free text section called the “Incident Narrative.” In this section,
the officer provides a summary of the incident. There is no set format for this section and narratives can
vary greatly in length and detail. Police incident reports are not created for research (Alison et al. 2001)
therefore we recoded fields in an attempt to address our research questions. The following sections
discuss the variables used in the analyses as well as information on the coding techniques used for the
gang-related reasons variables. See Table 2 for the descriptive statistics for all variables.
5
IC 35-42-2: Battery and Related Offenses.
10
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for full sample and by dependent variable outcome.
Total
Violent Crime
Drug Crime
Other
Non-Violent
Crime
Variable (1 = Yes)
n
%
n
%
n
%
n
%
χ2
Crime Type
Named Gang
Self-Initiated
741
201
296
100
27.1
39.9
131
40
15
17.7
30.5
11.5
153
13
138
20.6
8.5
90.2
457
148
143
61.7
32.4
31.3
34.026 ***
219.658 ***
Reason
Gang Signs and Symbols
Self-identify
Associates with Gangs
Law Enforcement Intelligence
Unknown or Unclear
Firearm
100
61
161
227
261
327
13.5
8.2
21.7
30.6
35.2
44.4
5
17
38
11
68
58
3.8
13.0
29.0
8.4
51.9
44.3
2
1
4
109
39
82
1.3
0.7
2.6
71.2
25.5
53.6
93
43
119
107
154
187
20.4
9.4
26.0
23.4
33.7
40.9
48.375 ***
16.375 ***
41.944 ***
160.426 ***
22.802 ***
6.919 *
Number of Victims
Number of Suspects
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
M (SD)
F-Statistic
0.70 (0.86)
1.2 (1.3)
1.4 (1.1) b,c
2.1 (1.7) b,c
0.14 (0.40) a,c
1.0 (1.2) a
0.68 (0.76) a,b
0.94 (1.2) a
94.432 ***
38.855 ***
* p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001; a = significant difference from violent crime (p < 0.05); b = significant difference between
drug crime (p < 0.05); c = significant difference from non-violent crime (p < 0.05).
3.1. Dependent Variable
The dependent variable is a categorical measure of crime type (1= violent crime; 2 = drug crime;
3 = other non-violent crime). For each incident report, the reporting officer designates one or more
“incident offenses” that specify which state laws have been violated.6 Each offense designation includes
the corresponding Indiana Code.7 We grouped these into one of three Crime Types (1 = violent
crime; 2 = drug crime; 3 = other non-violent crime). In cases where the officer indicated more than
one crime type, we coded one crime type in order of severity (violent, drug, other non-violent).
‘Violent crimes’ included homicide, rape, robbery, aggravated assault, and sex crimes. ‘Drug crimes’
included possession of paraphernalia, possession, dealing, and cultivation of marijuana, possession
or dealing of cocaine, methamphetamine, or other controlled substance, and visiting or maintaining
a common nuisance. Any crime that did not fit into one of the first two categories was classified as
‘other non-violent crime.’ Of the incidents that were labeled as gang-related, the majority were other
non-violent crimes followed by drug crimes and violent crimes.
3.2. Explanatory Variables
We used the narrative portion of the incident report to try and determine the reason the reporting
officer indicated the incident was gang-related. Gang-related reasons were not determined a priori;
we instead used a iterative modified grounded theory approach (Glaser and Strauss 2009) looking
for themes to emerge and also with the understanding that each incident report could have more
than one reason for being considered gang-related. We finalized on four possible reasons that the
incident was gang-related. Each of the following reasons is a binary variable (0 = no; 1 = yes) and
gang-related reasons are not mutually exclusive. Incident reports could have more than one reason
for being labeled gang-related. Gang Signs and Symbols: The report writer indicted the presence of
gang signs and/or symbols which could include gang tattoos, graffiti, and the display of colors and/or
signs. Self Identifies: At least one individual listed in the police report self-identifies as a gang member.
Associates with Known Gang Members: At least one individual listed in the report associates with or is
related to a known gang member. Law Enforcement Intelligence: Law enforcement intelligence would
indicate the incident is gang-related. While we may not know the exact intelligence, the nature of the
incident including the units or outside agencies involved would indicates the incident is gang-related.
6
7
Incident offenses do not represent prosecutorial charging decisions.
See http://iga.in.gov/legislative/laws/2020/ic/titles/001.
11
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
We coded the reason as Unknown or Unclear if we were unable to determine the reason the incident
was gang-related. Law enforcement intelligence was the most common reason a report was labeled
gang-related—coded in 30% of incident reports (see Table 2). That said, there were a fair number of
reports, just more than one-third, for which we were not able to determine why the officer labeled the
incident gang-related. At least one reason was identified in 56% of reports. The remaining 10% of
reports had two or more reasons identified.
We read each report narrative to determine if the reporting officer recorded a specific gang name
(0 = no named gang; 1 = named gang). Just greater than 25% of incident reports included a Named
Gang. Report Initiation is the activity that prompted the police report. Report Initiation was categorized
according to whether the activity was self-initiated or not (0 = not self-initiated, 1 = self-initiated).
Reports that are the result of a ‘call for service’ (CFS) or reactive police activity can be inherently
different than a report that results from self-initiated police activity or proactive activity (Cordner 1979)
in that an officer can choose what self-initiated activity to document. Reports that result from a CFS are
influenced by the wants or needs of another individual (e.g., a community member) and therefore the
officer has less discretion about what is documented in the incident report. Incident reports resulting
from a community member’s call for assistance (call for service) or from the request of another agency
were classified as ‘not self-initiated.’ In these cases, a police officer in the field was responding to a
request for service and therefore has less control over documentation. Responding field officers may
not have the same level of working intelligence about the incident as an investigative officer who is
working an incident as part of an investigation or self-initiated activity. Self-initiated activity included
undercover operations or investigations, search warrant service, person warrant service, and activities
where the officer was not dispatched or requested to the location. The majority of police reports (60%)
were result of calls for service/not self-initiated.
The number of individual victims and suspects listed in the report were coded as continuous
variables. If the only victim listed was an organization and not a specific person, we coded that as
zero (i.e., no victim). Fifty-three percent of incidents included at least one victim however the average
number of victims per incident was less than one (mean = 0.70, SD = 0.86). More than one-half of
incident reports included at least one suspect (65%). The average number of suspects per incident
report was just greater than one (mean = 1.2, SD = 1.3). Firearms drive violence in Indianapolis as well
as in most urban cities across the United States. We coded ‘yes’ if the officer listed a firearm in the
property section of the report meaning at least one firearm was confiscated or taken into protective
custody at the incident scene. About 44% of incidents involved a firearm.
4. Results
The focus of this analysis is two-fold. We are interested in incident characteristics that (1) influence
the reporting officer’s categorization of that incident as gang-related and (2) differentiate between
violent, drug, and other non-violent crimes. Bivariate analyses revealed several differences in crime type
across the explanatory variables (see Table 2). In terms of the reasons why these crimes were labeled as
gang-related, violent crime incidents were significantly more likely to be labeled as gang-related due
to self-identification as a gang member, but it was also more likely that the reason for the gang-related
label was unclear. Non-violent crimes were more likely to include the presence of signs and symbols
for gang membership. Drug crimes were less likely to involve a named gang and be classified as
gang-related because of gang associations. However, drug crimes were significantly more likely to be
labeled as gang-related due to law enforcement intelligence. When looking at other characteristics
the data show that incidents involving violent crimes were the least likely to result from self-initiated
activity, violent crimes were significantly more likely to include multiple victims and offenders, and
officers were least likely to confiscate a weapon during other non-violent crime incidents.
Given the established difference in reactive versus proactive self-initiated police activity, it is
important to examine these results more closely. Within the non-violent crime incident reports, more
than two-thirds of these reports resulted from a call for service (i.e., self-initiated = no). The majority
12
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
of incidents categorized as violent crimes resulted from non-self-initiated officer activity, meaning the
officer was responding to a call for service from a community member or other law enforcement unit or
agency. Only a small proportion of violent crime incident reports resulted from officer-initiated activity.
In contrast, the majority (90%) of drug crime incidents were the result of self-initiated officer activity.
These differences are significant (χ2 = 219.657; p < 0.000). These findings may suggest several things.
First, when gang activity is violent, law enforcement is summoned; it is rare that law enforcement will
find violent gang-related activity on their own. Despite this finding, the majority of incidents where
officers are responding to a call for service are still non-violent, non-drug related incidents. These data
also demonstrate it is uncommon for an incident that was self-initiated by an officer to be a violent
incident, that is, gang-related violent incidents almost came to the attention of law enforcement via a
third party call for service.
Multivariate Analysis
Given our interest in crime type, we next performed a multinomial logistic regression to determine
if we could predict crime type using the explanatory variables. Multinomial regression is appropriate
due to the categorical nature of the dependent variable. Table 3 presents the comparison of violent
crimes and drug crimes with other non-violent crimes (reference category). The reference category was
changed to violent crime (see Table 4) in order to make comparisons between drug and violent crimes.
Table 3. Multinomial logistic regression for violent crime and drug crimes compared with other
non-violent crimes.
(n = 741)
Variable
Dependent
Independent (0 = No)
Violent Crime
Named Gang
Self-Initiated
Firearm
Number of Victims
Number of Suspects
Reason
Gang Signs and Symbols
Self-identify
Associates with Gangs
Law Enforcement Intelligence
Unknown or Unclear
Drug Crime
Named Gang
Self-Initiated
Firearm
Number of Victims
Number of Suspects
Reason
Gang Signs and Symbols
Self-identify
Associates with Gangs
Law Enforcement Intelligence
Unknown or Unclear
[Exp(b)]
95% Confidence Interval
β
SE
Sig
Odds Ratio
Lower
Upper
−0.039
0.909
0.496
0.737
0.468
0.306
0.345
0.246
0.147
0.078
0.898
0.008 *
0.044
0.000 ***
0.000 ***
0.962
2.483
1.642
2.089
1.597
0.528
1.263
1.013
1.565
1.37
1.752
4.883
2.662
2.788
1.863
1.372
−0.411
−0.332
0.087
−0.915
0.547
0.526
0.496
0.599
0.569
0.012 *
0.434
0.504
0.885
0.108
3.944
0.663
0.718
1.091
0.400
1.351
0.237
0.271
0.337
0.131
11.513
1.857
1.898
3.532
1.223
−0.159
−1.951
0.07
−0.639
0.197
0.419
0.331
0.235
0.258
0.102
0.704
0.000 ***
0.765
0.013 *
0.054 *
0.853
0.142
1.073
0.528
1.218
0.375
0.074
0.677
0.319
0.997
1.937
0.272
1.701
0.875
1.489
1.058
1.417
0.875
−1.445
−0.627
0.871
1.192
0.667
0.662
0.682
0.225
0.234
0.190
0.029
0.358
2.88
4.126
2.399
0.236
0.534
0.522
0.399
0.649
0.064
0.140
15.889
42.655
8.874
0.863
2.035
The reference category is Other Non-violent Crime. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
The full model fit was significantly improved with the addition of the predictors (χ2 (20) = 417.606,
p < 0.000) when compared to the intercept only model. Because we conducted a multinomial regression,
we use the odds ratios (ExpB) to examine the effect of the explanatory variables on the dependent
variable. We first examine the reasons the report was labeled gang-related. The presence of gang
signs and symbols increases the odds of the incident being a violent crime rather than a non-violent
crime by 3.9. No other gang-related reasons varied across crime type when controlling for other crime
characteristics. The number of victims and suspects documented in the incident report is also important
for crime type categorization. As the number of victims in the report increases by one, the odds of the
incident being a violent crime versus a non-violent crime increases by 2.1. Conversely, as the number
13
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
of victims in the report increases by one, the odds of the report being a drug crime versus a non-violent
crime decreases by 0.5. For suspects, as the number of suspects increases by one, the odds that the
incident report includes a violent crime versus a non-violent crime increases by 1.6. An increase in the
number of suspects increases the odds that the incident report includes a drug crime by 1.2.
Table 4. Multinomial logistic regression for drug crimes compared with violent crimes.
(n = 741)
Variable
Dependent
Independent (0 = No)
Drug Crime
Named Gang
Self-Initiated
Firearm
Number of Victims
Number of Suspects
Reason
Gang Signs and Symbols
Self-identify
Associates with Gangs
Law Enforcement Intelligence
Unknown or Unclear
[Exp(b)]
95% Confidence Interval
β
SE
Sig
Odds Ratio
Lower
Upper
−0.12
−2.861
−0.426
−1.375
−0.271
0.498
0.443
0.325
0.283
0.116
0.809
0.000 ***
0.190
0.000 ***
0.020 *
0.887
0.057
0.653
0.253
0.763
0.334
0.024
0.346
0.145
0.607
2.353
0.136
1.235
0.440
0.958
−0.314
1.829
1.207
−1.532
0.289
1.016
1.285
0.815
0.868
0.866
0.757
0.155
0.139
0.078
0.739
0.730
6.226
3.342
0.216
1.334
0.100
0.502
0.677
0.039
0.244
5.353
77.197
16.513
1.184
7.290
The reference category is Violent Crime. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.
Next, we explore differences in crime characteristics across violent and drug crimes when compared
with non-violent crimes. An officer responding to a call for service (i.e., not self-initiated) decreases the
odds of the incident involving a drug crime versus a violent crime by only a small margin (OR = 0.06).
Here again, the number of victims and suspects listed in the incident report is important to crime type
categorization. As the number of victims in the report increases by one, the odds of the report being a
drug crime versus a violent crime decreases by 0.2. For suspects, as the number of suspects increases
by one, the odds that the incident report includes a drug crime versus a violent crime decreases by 0.8.
5. Discussion
Gang members participate in more than their fair share of violent offending but are also involved
in other less serious criminal activities. This statement is supported by both qualitative and quantitative
research but has not been adequately explored through official records such as police incident reports.
Rather, prior work drawing on law enforcement data sources focuses heavily upon violent crime,
in particularly gang homicide. This gap in the literature may be due to law enforcement definitions
of gangs, gang members, and crimes that limit the range of offenses that are labeled gang-related.
Given that news media and policy makers rely upon law enforcement perceptions of these activities,
a focus on violence can lead to the misperception that gangs and gang members are only involved in
violent criminal behavior. This misperception can result in moral panic and the creation of highly
punitive policies targeted at gang members (e.g., gang enhancements and injunctions) Moreover, the
belief, whether accurate or not, that gangs drive urban violence can influence whether or not a law
enforcement agency maintains a gang unit despite the actual existence of documented gang violence
(Katz 2001). In this manuscript, we examined four years and five months worth, of violent, drug, and
non-violent gang related incidents from IMPD to determine why they were labeled as gang-related as
well as what characteristics differentiate incident types.
During these years, very few incident reports were labeled as gang-related and even fewer were
considered violent incidents. In fact, non-violent crimes made up the bulk of the gang-related incidents,
followed by drug and then violent crimes. These findings indicate that IMPD officers are not simply
choosing violent incidents to label as gang-related. Similarly, less than 50% of the incidents labeled
gang-related involved an officer confiscating a gun and the majority of those incidents were categorized
as non-violent. Only 60% of gang-related incident reports were the result of reactive police activity;
the remaining incident reports were the result of proactive police activity and were overwhelmingly
non-violent in nature.
14
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
Our work revealed that law enforcement intelligence is the primary reason incident reports were
labeled gang-related but beyond that, it was common for the reporting officer to not articulate a reason,
especially if the incident involved a violent crime. However, after controlling for other characteristics
of the incident, officers were more likely to document the presence of gang signs or symbols for
violent crime incidents than for non-violent crimes. This finding is consistent with prior literature
that indicates that officers rely upon the presence of gang signs and symbols when identifying gang
members (Densley and Pyrooz 2020; Scott 2020). Violent crimes were also distinguishable from drug
and non-violent crimes by the presence of multiple co-offenders/suspects as well as the presence of
multiple victims—a finding which is also consistent with prior research (Pyrooz et al. 2011). Our results
also indicate that violent crime incidents were more likely to be brought to attention of the police
through a call for service. This finding suggests that when gang activity is violent, law enforcement is
called; it is rare that law enforcement will find violent gang-related activity during routine patrol or
other unit specific activity.
Our findings indicate that drug crimes were likely to be labeled as gang-related due to law
enforcement intelligence and that they were likely to be self-initiated by officer. This finding is most
likely indicative of the routine activity of specialty units whose missions are highly focused and driven
by unit assignment. That is, we can make the assumption, for example, that the activity of the gang
unit is associated with gang-related crime without knowing the exact reason for the relationship.
While these findings contribute to the criminological literature on gangs and policing, there are
several limitations. First, police incident reports are not created for research which, therefore, limited
what variables we were able to code, how they were coded, as well as the analyses we were able to
conduct. For example, the reporting officer knows why he or she considered the incident gang-related
and our interpretation may or may not align with the reporting officer’s creating threats to internal
validity. We were also unable to determine a reason the incident was labeled gang-related for 35%
of the sample. Police incident reports are public record and law enforcement agencies must provide
access to these reports upon request (see Indiana Code 5-14-3). Investigatory records are excluded
from disclosure rules and, therefore, this type of information—which would provide more detail as to
why an incident is gang-related—is usually not found in police incident reports. We encourage future
researchers to engage with reporting officers to gather their perceptions on why incidents were labeled
as gang-related.
Second, we focus on one Midwestern, American law enforcement agency. Police incident reports
and how they are written are influenced by myriad factors that vary across time and space. The reports
used in this work are limited to information gathered by the reporting officer at the time of the
incident. While informative, these findings are only generalizable to Indianapolis during the study
period. We encourage similar work in other jurisdictions, states, and countries in order to build the
knowledge-base and allow for comparisons. Third, incidents were identified as gang-related through
the reporting officer’s use of “check-boxes” while filling out the incident report. We were not able to
determine what, if any, training officers received regarding when to check and when not to check the
box. There also may be error associated with officers who checked the boxes in error or unintentionally.
Moreover, the identification and labeling of the gang-related reasons was based on a thematic analysis
of the incident reports, not the officer’s perception of why he or she labeled an incident gang-related.
Future research would benefit from a more in-depth analysis of officers’ perceptions of these incidents.
Finally, we were unable to differentiate between violent acts that serve a functional or expressive role
in gang crime, (see Andell 2020 as well as Decker and Pyrooz 2015). Other research should compare
police incidents for different forms of violence.
Despite these limitations, our findings provide insight into gang incident reports and have
implications for gang research using official police records. While it is difficult to know exactly why
officers consider some incidents gang-related and others not, our findings indicate that the majority
(62%) of gang-related incident reports involve non-violent crimes. This finding is important for policy
15
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
makers and local agencies working with gang members in that it demonstrates programming should
address more than just violence.
6. Conclusions
While modest, these results are novel and have implications for research as well as policy.
Our research supports the idea that official records of gang-related crimes or gangs may not be
generalizable across cities, see (Maxson and Klein 1990) and, as our data indicate, may be dependent
on the type of law enforcement activity. The presence of a gang unit at the local level and/or other
state and federal units that focus on gang violence (e.g., Violent Gang Safe Streets Task Force)8
influences related law enforcement activity. More specifically, it influences self-initiated officer
activity. Documenting gang-related crimes is important for prevention, intervention, and suppression;
therefore, it is imperative that there are “best practices” for documenting these types of crime.
Consistent measurement of gang crimes across jurisdictions can only result in improved knowledge
and better policy.
The results show that despite an urban setting and frequent violent crime, very few incidents are
labeled as gang-related by law enforcement and that the prevalence is decreasing yearly. This fact
could be as a result of a movement away from a specialized gang unit as well as a deprioritization of
gang crime in Indianapolis. IMPD’s new records management system and coinciding removal of the
gang-related check box from incident reports may also indicate less emphasis on gang violence and
more emphasis on violence in general. The elimination of the gang-related label means that it may be
difficult for prosecutors to identify opportunities to use and apply Indiana gang enhancement code
as well as charge individuals with participating in criminal gang activity. In fact, these statutes are
invoked very infrequently in Indianapolis. We found only two instances of this charge (see Indiana
Code 35-45-9-3) in our entire multi-year sample of gang-related police incident reports and other
research indicates that gang enhancements are used infrequently in Indiana, especially in Marion
County where Indianapolis is located (Hall 2019). Additionally, a movement away from a focus on
gangs can result in a lack of guidance on how to work with and address gangs (Andell 2019) for a
discussion of this issue in the context of the United Kingdom).
Author Contributions: Writing—original draft, D.C. and N.K.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was partially funded by Bureau of Justice Assistance, grant number 2016-GP-BX-004.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Adams, Jennifer J., and Jesenia M. Pizarro. 2014. Patterns of specialization and escalation in the criminal careers of
gang and non-gang homicide offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior 41: 237–55. [CrossRef]
Alison, Laurence J., Brent Snook, and Kristin L. Stein. 2001. Unobtrusive measurement: Using police information
for forensic research. Qualitative Research 1: 241–54. [CrossRef]
Andell, Paul. 2019. Thinking Seriously About Gangs: Towards a Critical Realist Approach. Cham: Palgrave Macmillan.
Andell, Paul. 2020. Gangs, violence, and county lines. In The Handbook of Collective Violence: Current Developments
and Understanding. Edited by Carol A. Ireland, Michael Lewis, Anthony Lopez and Jane L. Ireland. New York:
Routledge, pp. 198–208.
Braga, Anthony A., Lorraine A. Green, David Weisburd, and Frank Gajewski. 1994. Police perceptions of
street-level narcotics activity: Evaluating drug buys as a research tool. American Journal of Police 13: 37–58.
Brenneman, Robert. 2012. Homies and Hermanos: Gods and Gangs in Central America. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
8
https://www.fbi.gov/investigate/violent-crime/gangs/violent-gang-task-forces.
16
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
Carson, Dena C., Stephanie A. Wiley, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2017. Differentiating between delinquent groups
and gangs: Moving beyond offending consequences. Journal of Crime and Justice 40: 297–315. [CrossRef]
Cordner, Gary W. 1979. Police patrol work load studies: A review and critique. Police Studies: The International
Review of Police Development 2: 50–60.
Curry, G. David, and Scott H. Decker. 1997. What’s in a name? A gang by any other name isn’t quite the same.
Valparaiso University Law Review 31: 501–14.
Curry, G. David, Richard A. Ball, and Scott H. Decker. 1996. Estimating the national scope of gang crime from law
enforcement data. In Gangs in America, 2nd ed. Edited by C. Ronald Huff. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 21–36.
Decker, Scott H. 1996. Collective and normative features of gang violence. Justice Quarterly 13: 245–64. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott, and Kimberly Kempf-Leonard. 1991. Constructing gangs: The social definition of youth activities.
Criminal Justice Policy Review 5: 271–91. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and David C. Pyrooz. 2010. On the validity and reliability of gang homicide: A comparison of
disparate sources. Homicide Studies 14: 359–76. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and David C. Pyrooz, eds. 2015. Street gangs, terrorists, drug smugglers, and organized crime:
What’s the difference? In The Handbook of Gangs. Hoboken: Wiley, pp. 294–308.
Decker, Scott H., and Barrick Van Winkle. 1996. Life in the Gang: Family, Friends, and Violence. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Decker, Scott H., David C. Pyrooz, Gary Sweeten, and Richard K. Moule Jr. 2014. Validating self-nomination in
gang research: Assessing differences in gang embeddedness across non-, current, and former gang members.
Journal of Quantitative Criminology 30: 577–98. [CrossRef]
Densley, James A. 2013. How Gangs Work: An Ethnography of Youth Violence. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Densley, James A., and David C. Pyrooz. 2020. The matrix in context: Taking stock of police gang databases in
London and beyond. Youth Justice 30: 577–98. [CrossRef]
Deuchar, Ross. 2018. Gangs and Spirituality: Global Perspectives. Cham: Palgrave Macmillan.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, and Dena C. Carson. 2012. Who are the gangsters? An examination of the age, race/ethnicity,
sex, and immigration status of self-reported gang members in a seven-city study of American youth. Journal
of Contemporary Criminal Justice 28: 462–78. [CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, and Karin Tusinski. 2007. Youth gangs in the print media. Criminal Justice and Popular
Culture 14: 21–38.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, L. Thomas Winfree Jr., Ni He Thomas, and Terrance J. Taylor. 2001. Youth gangs and
definitional issues: When is a gang a gang and why does it matter? Crime and Delinquency 47: 105–30.
[CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Dana Peterson, Terrance J. Taylor, and Adrienne Freng. 2010. Youth Violence: Sex and Race
Differences in Offending, Victimization, and Gang Membership. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Federal Bureau of Investigation. 2018. Crime in the United States. United States Department of Justice. Available
online: https://ucr.fbi.gov/crime-in-the-u.s/2018/crime-in-the-u.s.-2018/topic-pages/violent-crime (accessed
on 28 March 2020).
Feeley, Malcolm M. 1973. Two models of the criminal justice system: An organizational perspective. Law and
Society Review 7: 407–25. [CrossRef]
Fisher, Stanley Z. 1993. Just the facts, ma’am: Lying and the omission of exculpatory evidence in police reports.
New England Law Review 28: 1–62.
Fleisher, Mark. 1998. Dead End Kids: Gang Girls and the Boys They Know. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Garot, Robert. 2010. Who You Claim: Performing Gang Identity in Schools and on the Streets. New York: New York
University Press.
Glaser, Barney G., and Anselm L. Strauss. 2009. The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research.
New York: Transaction Publishers.
Goldstein, Herman. 1990. Problem-Oriented Policing. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.
Hagan, John. 1999. Why is there so little criminal justice theory? Neglected macro- and micro-level links between
organization and power. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 26: 116–35. [CrossRef]
Hall, Hannah. 2019. Analysis of Criminal Gang Enhancements. Working paper. Indianapolis, IN, USA: Indiana
University Purdue University.
Horowitz, Ruth. 1990. Sociological perspectives on gangs: Conflicting definitions and concepts. In Gangs in
America: Diffusion, Diversity, and Public Policy. Edited by C. Ronald Huff. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
17
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
Huebner, Beth M., Kimberly Martin, Richard K. Moule Jr., David Pyrooz, and Scott H. Decker. 2016. Dangerous
places: Gang members and neighborhood levels of gun assault. Justice Quarterly 33: 836–62. [CrossRef]
Irwin-Rogers, Kei, Scott H. Decker, Amir Rostami, Svetlana Stephenson, and Elke Van Hellemont. 2019. European
street gangs and urban violence. In Handbook of Global Urban Health. Edited by Igot Vojnovic, Amber
L. Pearson, Gershim Asiki, Geoffry DeVerteuil and Adriana Allen. New York: Routlege.
Katz, Charles M. 2001. The establishment of a police gang unit: An examination of organizational and environmental
factors. Criminology 39: 37–74. [CrossRef]
Katz, Charles M., Vincent J. Webb, and David R. Schaefer. 2000. The validity of police gang intelligence lists:
Examining differences in delinquency between documented gang members and nondocumented delinquent
youth. Police Quarterly 3: 413–37. [CrossRef]
Katz, Charles M., Andrew M. Fox, Chester L. Britt, and Phillip Stevenson. 2012. Understanding police gang data
at the aggregate level: An examination of relability of National Youth Gang Survey data (Research Note).
Justice Research and Policy 14: 103–28. [CrossRef]
Kennedy, David M., Anthony A. Braga, and Anne M. Piehl. 1997. The (un)known universe: Mapping gangs
and gang violence in Boston. In Crime Mapping and Crime Prevention. Edited by David Weisburd and
J. Tom McEwen. Monsey: Criminal Justice Press, pp. 219–62.
Klein, Malcolm, and Cheryl L. Maxson. 2006. Street Gang Patterns and Policies. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Lauger, Timothy R. 2012. Real Gangstas: Legitimacy, Reputation, and Violence in the Intergang Environment.
New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Levitt, Steven D. 1998. The Relationship between Crime Reporting and Police: Implications for the Use of Uniform
Crime Reports. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 14: 61–81. [CrossRef]
Lipsky, Michael. 1980. Street-Level Bureaucracy: Dilemmas of the Individual in Public Service. New York: Russell Sage.
Maxson, Cheryl L., and Malcolm W. Klein. 1990. Street gang violence: Twice as great or half as great. In Gangs in
America. Edited by C. Ronald Huff. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 71–100.
Maxson, Cheryl L., Margaret A. Gordon, and Malcolm W. Klein. 1985. Differences between gang and nongang
homicides. Criminology 23: 209–22. [CrossRef]
Maxson, Cheryl L., G. David Curry, and James C. Howell. 2002. Youth gang homicides in the United States in
the 1990s. In Responding to Gangs: Evaluation and Research. Edited by Winifred L. Reed and Scott H. Decker.
Washington: National Institute of Justice, pp. 107–37.
McCorkle, Richard C., and Terance D. Miethe. 1998. The political and organizational response to gangs:
An examination of a “moral panic” in Nevada. Justice Quarterly 15: 41–64. [CrossRef]
Melde, Chris, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2013. Gangs and violence: Disentangling the impact of gang membership
on the level and nature of offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 29: 143–66. [CrossRef]
Miller, Jody. 2001. One of the Guys: Girls, Gangs, and Gender. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Morash, Merry. 1983. Gangs, groups, and delinquency. British Journal of Criminology 23: 309–35. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., David M. Hureau, and Anthony A. Braga. 2013. The corner and the crew: The influence
of geography and social networks on gang violence. American Sociological Review 78: 417–47. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., Anthony A. Braga, Eric Piza, and Leigh S. Grossman. 2015. The Company You Keep?
The Spillover Effects of Gang Membership on Individual Gunshot Victimization in a Co-Offending Network.
Criminology 53: 624–49. [CrossRef]
Pizarro, Jesenia M., and Jean Marie McGloin. 2006. Explaining gang homicides in Newark, New Jersey: Collective
behavior or social disorganization. Journal of Criminal Justice 31: 195–207. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., and Scott H. Decker. 2013. Delinquent behavior, violence, and gang involvement in China.
Journal of Quantitative Criminology 29: 251–72. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Andrew M. Fox, and Scott H. Decker. 2010. Racial and ethnic heterogeneity, economic
disadvantage, and gangs: A macro-level study of gang membership in urban America. Justice Quarterly 27:
867–92. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Scott E. Wolfe, and Cassia Spohn. 2011. Gang-related homicide charging decisions: The
implementation of a specialized prosecution unit in Los Angeles. Criminal Justice Policy Review 22: 3–26.
[CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Jillian J. Turanovic, Scott H. Decker, and Jun Wu. 2016. Taking stock of the relationship between
gang membership and offending: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior 43: 365–97. [CrossRef]
18
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 199
Rosenfeld, Richard, Timothy M. Bray, and Arlen Egley. 1999. Facilitating violence: A comparison of gang-motivated,
gang-affiliated, and nongang youth homicides. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 15: 495–516. [CrossRef]
Scott, Daniel. 2020. Regional differences in gang member identification methods among law enforcement
jurisdictions in the United States. Policing: An International Journal. [CrossRef]
Thornberry, Terence P. 1998. Membership in youth gangs and involvement in serious and violent offending.
In Serious and Violent Juvenile Offenders: Risk Factors and Successful Interventions. Edited by Rolf Loeber and
David P. Farrington. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc., pp. 147–66.
Thornberry, Terence P., Marvin D. Krohn, Alan J. Lizotte, Carolyn A. Smith, and Kimberly Tobin. 2003. Gangs and
Delinquency in Developmental Perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thrasher, Frederic M. 1963. The Gang: A Study of One Thousand Three Hundred Thirteen Groups in Chicago. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press. First Published 1927.
Ward, Thomas W. 2013. Gangsters without Borders: An Ethnography of a Salvadorn Street Gang. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Weaver, Beth. 2016. Offending and Desistance: The Importance of Social Relations. New York: Routledge.
Weerman, Frank M., and Fine-Aage Esbensen. 2005. A cross-national comparison of gangs: The Netherlands and
the United States. In European Street Gangs and Troublesome Youth Groups. Edited by Scott H. Decker and
Frank M. Weerman. Lanham: Altamira Press, pp. 219–57.
Yablonsky, Lewis. 1962. The Violent Gang. New York: NY Macmillan.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional
affiliations.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
19
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
East Side Story: Disaggregating Gang Homicides in East
Los Angeles
Matthew Valasik 1, * and Shannon E. Reid 2
1
2
*
Sociology Department, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
Criminology & Criminal Justice, UNC Charlotte, 9201 University City Blvd, Charlotte, NC 28223, USA;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Abstract: This research extends the homicide literature by using latent class analysis methods to
examine the neighborhood structural and demographic characteristics of different categories of homicides in the Hollenbeck Community Policing Area of the Los Angeles Police Department (LAPD).
The Hollenbeck area itself is a 15 square-mile region with approximately 187,000 residents, the majority of whom are Latino (84 percent). Hollenbeck also has a protracted history of intergenerational
Latinx gangs with local neighborhood residents viewing them as a fundamental social problem.
Hollenbeck has over 30 active street gangs, each claiming a geographically defined territory, many of
which have remained stable during the study period. Over twenty years (1990–2012) of homicide
data collected from Hollenbeck’s Homicide Division are utilized to create an empirically rigorous
typology of homicide incidents and to test whether or not gang homicides are sufficiently distinct in
nature to be a unique category in the latent class analysis.
Keywords: homicide; homicide types; disaggregation; street gangs; latent class analysis
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Citation: Valasik, Matthew, and
Shannon E. Reid. 2021. East Side
1. Introduction
Story: Disaggregating Gang
Prior to the Covid-19 Pandemic, which disrupted crime trends (Campedelli et al. 2020;
Mohler et al. 2020; Rosenfeld and Lopez 2020), homicide rates across many jurisdictions
were at some of the lowest levels on record, yet this has not lessened policymakers and
police agencies’ desire to further reduce the number of homicides within a given jurisdiction. Despite these overall reductions in violence, gang prevalence continues to be a
widespread phenomenon throughout the United States, as witnessed by an increase of
over 20 percent in the number of jurisdictions reporting gang problems to the National
Gang Youth Survey between 2002 and 2009 (Howell et al. 2011). In fact, approximately
85 percent of gang-related homicides in the United States occur in large cities, populations over 100,000, or in proximate suburban counties (NGC 2017). Howell and Griffiths
(2018) investigated this trend by examining gang-related homicides from 1996 to 2012
in 248 large cities. Their findings indicate that in the majority of sampled cities (65.3%),
gang-related homicides contribute annually between 30 and 40 percent of all homicides
(Howell and Griffiths 2018). Valasik and colleagues (Valasik et al. 2017) have also shown
that in disadvantaged communities gang-related homicide remains stubbornly affixed over
decades. In contrast, non-gang homicide appears to be more responsive to interventions.
Overall, “street gang research has regularly shown a strong, positive relationship between
gangs and violence, existing across places and over time” (Valasik and Reid 2020, p. 273).
Homicides in East Los Angeles. Social
Sciences 10: 48. https://doi.org/
10.3390/socsci10020048
Academic Editor: Nigel Parton
Received: 16 December 2020
Accepted: 25 January 2021
Published: 1 February 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
21
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
From a legislative standpoint, the criminal justice system makes a concerted effort
to designate a criminal offense as gang-related1 if that criminal offense involves an individual who is associated with a gang. The NGC (2017) has identified forty-four states
and Washington D.C. as having legislation that explicitly defines a gang. The overall
majority of these states have also enacted some form of anti-gang legislation that allows
for enhancements to be added on to an accused gang member’s principal crime (Anderson
et al. 2009; Bjerregaard 2003, 2015; Geis 2002). For instance, the use of wide-reaching gang
enhancement laws known as STEP Acts, an acronym for Street Terrorism Enforcement and
Prevention, permit the felony prosecution of individuals who associate with a criminal
gang, assist gang members with their criminal actions, or just have prior knowledge of a
gang member’s engagement in criminal activity (Bjerregaard 2003, 2015; Geis 2002; Klein
and Maxson 2006). For instance, California’s STEP Act, penal code 186.22PC, mandates
that any gang member committing a felony (e.g., murder) will receive an additional prison
sentence consecutive to the penalty received for the original crime. In the case of a murder
conviction the STEP Act’s gang enhancement would result in an additional 15 years added
to an individual’s sentence. Prosecutors are then encouraged to aggressively seek justice,
which usually entails pursuing an enhancement for any gang-related homicide regardless
of the motivation driving the crime (Anderson et al. 2009; Rios 2011). As such, gang-related
homicides are frequently considered to be a distinct type of homicide different from other
forms of lethal violence. That is, homicides involving gang members are treated as something inherently distinct, from investigating (Katz and Webb 2006; Klein 2004; Leovy 2015;
Valasik et al. 2016) to prosecuting (Anderson et al. 2009; Capizzi et al. 1995; Caudill et al.
2017; Pyrooz et al. 2011) to sentencing (Anderson et al. 2009; McCorkle and Miethe 1998;
Miethe and McCorkle 1997). But what are the characteristics that make a gang-related
homicide so different from a non-gang homicide?
Prior research has disaggregated gang-related homicides from non-gang homicides to
answer this question, finding that a variety of micro-, meso-, and macrolevel characteristics
impact acts of gang-related violence differently than acts of non-gang violence (Bailey
and Unnithan 1994; Barton et al. 2020; Brantingham et al. 2020; Curry and Spergel 1988;
Decker and Curry 2002; Egley 2012; Mares 2010; Maxson 1999; Maxson et al. 1985; Maxson
and Klein 1990, 1996; Pizarro and McGloin 2006; Pyrooz 2012; Rosenfeld et al. 1999;
Smith 2014; Valasik et al. 2017). Despite the robust knowledge gained over the years
from these studies, they overlook one crucial element. These prior studies infer in their
analyses a level of homogeneity among gang-related homicides. That is, they treat all
gang-related homicides as being indistinguishable from one another. Yet, the variation
in motivations prompting gang members to participate in violence is wide-ranging, from
retaliation against a rival, to being a consequence of another criminal act (e.g., drug sales,
robbery), to arising from a domestic dispute. Important nuance exists in gang-related
homicides that is being lost in the straightforward analyses of prior research. As such, more
meaningful disaggregation must be examined to ascertain just how much variation exists
within gang-related homicides and acknowledging the complex nature of gang-related
violence.
The current paper addresses this gap in the literature by using the variation in the
circumstances, motive, setting, participant characteristics, and rivalry relationship present
in gang-related homicides to explore the diversity of gang-related homicides. Latent class
analysis (LCA) is utilized to look for hidden “classes” in data that are mutually exclusive to
each other. The goal of this study is to systematically disaggregate gang-related homicides
in a measured process and assess how the latent classes of gang-related homicides vary
from each other. The broader study objective, however, is to highlight that a more nuanced
1
Gang-related homicides, sometimes referred to as gang-affiliated or member-based gang homicides, are those events in which at least one gang
member is a participant (see Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996). Gang-motivated homicides are a subsample of gang-related events that result directly
from “gang behavior or relationships” and are prompted by some group incentive (e.g., reputation/status, revenge, initiation, etc.) (Rosenfeld
et al. 1999, p. 500). More discussion on the current study’s use of the more inclusive measure, gang-related homicides, is detailed below in the
data section.
22
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
understanding of gang-related homicide is required if interventions aimed at reducing
gang-related homicide are going to be implemented successfully (e.g., focused deterrence,
civil gang injunctions, etc.). The remainder of the paper begins with discussing the use
of homicide disaggregation in gang studies to highlight the disparities between gangrelated and non-gang violence. The prevalent theories guiding this disaggregation process
are highlighted along with persistent covariates that remain significant across the extant
literature. The unique dataset created out of homicide case files from the Homicide Unit of
the Los Angeles Police Department’s (LAPD) Hollenbeck Community Policing Area and
the LCA used in the current study are then discussed. Results are presented. A discussion
about the benefits and applications of disaggregating gang-related homicides concludes
the paper.
2. Background
2.1. Homicide Disaggregation and Gang Research
Land and colleagues (Land et al. 1990) indicate that homicide research needs to better investigate whether the associations between a study’s community covariates (i.e.,
population structure, deprivation, and percent divorced) and aggregated homicides are
generalizable to disaggregated types of homicide. Scholars have generally taken this to
mean that studies should examine if these covariates are similarly or differently associated
with distinct types of homicide (e.g., gang, drug, domestic, etc.) (see Corsaro et al. 2017;
Kubrin and Wadsworth 2003; Pizarro 2008; Tita and Griffiths 2005). Furthermore, Williams
and Flewelling (1988, p. 422) contend that homicide disaggregation “should be guided
by the theoretical focus of the research problem” and “into meaningful subtypes of homicide.” Homicide disaggregation, as Kubrin (2003) points out, is a valuable tool to better
understand how a neighborhood’s social structure relates to different types of homicide
and their frequency.
Much of the research on gang-related violence disaggregates the incidents into gang
and non-gang homicides (Bailey and Unnithan 1994; Barton et al. 2020; Brantingham
et al. 2020; Curry and Spergel 1988; Decker and Curry 2002; Egley 2012; Mares 2010;
Maxson 1999; Maxson et al. 1985; Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996; Pizarro and McGloin
2006; Pyrooz 2012; Smith 2014; Valasik et al. 2017), or even disaggregating into gangmotivated, gang-affiliated, and non-gang homicides2 (Rosenfeld et al. 1999) to examine
micro-level differences between these homicide subtypes. The results from these studies
have been remarkably consistent over time and place. Overall, these comparative studies
have highlighted how the characteristics of the participants, the setting/context, and the
neighborhood structure/environment are able to differentiate gang-related violence from
non-gang acts. The reason for pushing for gang homicides to be disaggregated similar to
broader homicides is that by grouping all gang homicides together, there is a limit to our
understanding of how multidimensional gang homicides can be. As Kubrin (2003) notes,
researchers need to expand on how a range of covariates are associated with different types
of homicides and to understand how invariances seen in broader homicide studies apply
to gang homicides.
2.2. Covariates of Gang Homicide: Prior Research and Ongoing Conceptual Issues
For over the last three decades there have been two consistent theoretical approaches
used to advance our understandings of gang-related homicide, the role of collective behavior (Decker 1996; Klein and Maxson 1989) or the influence of a community’s context,
principally through the lens of social disorganization theory (Bursik and Grasmick 1993;
Sampson and Groves 1989; Shaw and McKay 1942). The former, the role of collective
behavior argues that dynamic social processes (e.g., retaliation) are what drive the rates of
gang-related homicide (see Bichler et al. 2019; Brantingham et al. 2012, 2019; Brantingham
2
In the latter case, Rosenfeld and colleagues (Rosenfeld et al. 1999) categorized a homicide as non-gang when the participants involved were not
associated with a gang and the event was not the result of any known gang activity.
23
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
et al. 2020; Decker 1996; Klein and Maxson 1989; Lewis and Papachristos 2020; Nakamura
et al. 2020; Papachristos 2009; Papachristos et al. 2013; Pizarro and McGloin 2006). The
latter, the community context of gang-related homicide suggests that a neighborhood’s
social structure and correlates, including aspects of community social control, influence
the ebbs and flows of gang-related violence (see Barton et al. 2020; Curry and Spergel
1988; Kubrin and Wadsworth 2003; Mares 2010; Papachristos and Kirk 2006; Pizarro and
McGloin 2006; Pyrooz 2012; Radil et al. 2010; Smith 2014; Valasik 2018; Valasik and Tita
2018; Valasik et al. 2017).
Decker (1996) contends that gang-related violence, particularly sharp upticks in homicides, are driven by the role of collective behavior. Building from Short and Strodbeck’s
(1965) work, that gangs are more than the sum of their individual members but the notion
that group processes heavily influence that activities, Decker (1996, p. 244) stresses that the
function of threat, perceived or actual, “plays a role in the origin and growth of gangs, their
daily activities, and their belief systems.” Klein and Maxson (1989, p. 203) suggest that
violent activities can serve both a social and psychological function amongst a gang’s membership, which “may contribute to violence escalation” observed in street gangs. On the
basis of this point of view, the retaliatory nature of gang-related homicide can be thought
of as a series of “escalating” encounters of violence between gangs, catalyzed by an initial
act of violence. As Brantingham and colleagues (Brantingham et al. 2020, p. 14) astutely
surmise, “group-level processes amplify the dynamics of gang-related violence.” Such
patterns have been regularly observed in the existing gang literature (see Brantingham
et al. 2019, 2020; Lewis and Papachristos 2020; Nakamura et al. 2020; Papachristos et al.
2013; Tita et al. 2003).
To better unpack the group dynamics that make gang-related violence unique, studies
have evaluated the incident and participant characteristics of gang-related homicides
compared to acts of violence that do not involve gang members. Prior research examining
the incident characteristics of a gang-related homicides consistently finds that these acts
of violence involve a firearm; consist of multiple shots being fired at the victim; transpire
outside, in public, on the street; include multiple offenders and victims; and are prompted
by gang-related motivations (e.g., retaliation, defending turf, intra-gang conflict, etc.) and
statistically less likely to be driven by disputes that are domestic/romantic in nature,
and are more likely to involve a mobile offender seeking out the victim (Klein et al.
1991; Maxson 1999; Maxson et al. 1985; Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996; Curry and Spergel
1988; Rosenfeld et al. 1999; Decker and Curry 2002; Pizarro and McGloin 2006; Tita and
Griffiths 2005; Valasik 2014). When compared to non-gang violence, studies analyzing
the characteristics of the participants, offenders and victims, involved in a gang-related
homicide are statistically more likely to be a person of color (i.e., Latinx or Black); be male;
be younger in age; participants lack a clear relationship with each other (e.g., strangers);
and participants have a prior criminal history (Klein et al. 1991; Maxson 1999; Maxson
et al. 1985; Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996; Curry and Spergel 1988; Rosenfeld et al. 1999;
Decker and Curry 2002; Pizarro and McGloin 2006; Tita and Griffiths 2005; Valasik 2014).
The existing research has reliably shown that both the characteristics of the incident and
the participants are “clearly related to the group nature of” gang-related homicides making
them distinct from non-gang homicides (Maxson et al. 1985, p. 220).
Guided by the social disorganization framework, the community context of gangrelated homicide stresses that the spatial concentration of neighborhood-level characteristics are better able to account for the patterns in gang-related homicide (Bursik and
Grasmick 1993; Curry and Spergel 1988; Rosenfeld et al. 1999). That is, the social structure
and/or built environment of a neighborhood directly influences the trends in the violent
acts of gang members (see Barton et al. 2020; Pyrooz 2012; Smith 2014; Valasik 2018;
Valasik et al. 2017). Influenced by Short and Strodbeck’s (1965) research highlighting the
ecologically distinctness of gang homicides, being a localized community problem that
adheres to classical theories of poverty, Curry and Spergel (1988) explicitly operationalized
the framework of social disorganization to examine both gang-related delinquency and
24
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
homicide. Specifically, Curry and Spergel (1988) hypothesized that neighborhoods with
residential instability will likely have weak social controls, making these communities
more susceptible to gang violence. Conversely, they suspected that delinquency and crime
perpetrated by gang members would be more likely to transpire in neighborhoods that
are economically deprived (Curry and Spergel 1988). Using two different time periods to
analyze the patterns of gang homicide, Curry and Spergel (1988) found that gang-related
homicides are spatially concentrated in communities besieged with poverty and population churning, suggesting that social disorganization may be an important influence
contributing to the prevalence of gangs and their associated acts of violence. Expanding on
how a neighborhood’s structural conditions influence gang-related violence, Rosenfeld and
colleagues (Rosenfeld et al. 1999) compared and contrasted gang-motivated, gang-affiliated,
and non-gang homicides in St. Louis. Consistent with Curry and Spergel (1988), Rosenfeld
et al. (1999) find that all three homicide types are concentrated in unstable, disadvantaged neighborhoods that are racially isolated. Gang-motivated homicides were in fact
more likely to occur in racially segregated communities, and non-gang homicides were
more associated with disadvantaged neighborhoods, suggesting that a neighborhood’s
racial composition has a greater impact on the prevalence of gang homicides than socioeconomic disadvantage. Additionally, Rosenfeld and colleagues (Rosenfeld et al. 1999)
revealed that gang-affiliated homicides were more likely to resemble non-gang violence
than gang-motivated violence.
Further contributing to the limited research on the neighborhood-level correlates of
gang homicide, Pyrooz (2012) investigated the relationship between gang-related homicide
and the structural covariates of a neighborhood (e.g., resource deprivation, residential stability, racial composition, etc.) at the macro-level. Pyrooz (2012) finds that both population
density and socioeconomic deprivation impact gang-related homicides across America’s
88 largest cities (smallest city had 200,000 residents). A drawback to Pyrooz’s (2012) study
is that such a broad macro-level analysis can conceal “sub-area and neighborhood cycles
that cancel each other out in the aggregate” (Klein 1995, p. 223). More recently, Valasik and
colleagues (Valasik et al. 2017) addressed this issue by conducting a meso-level analysis,
examining longitudinal trends in gang homicide over a 35-year period in an area of East
Los Angeles. Valasik et al.’s (2017) findings reveal that gang-related homicides remain
spatially clustered and over-represented in socioeconomically disadvantaged neighborhoods, suggesting that intergenerational gangs and features of the neighborhood are able
to exert substantial influence on sustaining gang-related violence over the long term (see
also Barton et al. 2020). In fact, Brantingham and colleagues (Brantingham et al. 2020, p. 16)
point out that the majority of gang-related violence is not a “contagious offspring event”
(i.e., retaliation) but instead suggest that “structural environmental conditions” have a
greater influence on gang-related violence than the role of collective behavior.
3. Current Study
Disaggregating homicides between gang and non-gang incidents has produced a more
nuanced understanding of the micro-, meso-, and macro-level characteristics that influence
these acts of gang-related violence; however, this approach still assumes homogeneity
within gang-related homicides. For instance, the motivations that drive gang members to
engage in such criminal events can vary widely, from an escalated domestic dispute, to a
being the byproduct of a criminal act (e.g., robbery, drug sales). The current study uses
homicide case files from the Homicide Unit of LAPD’s Hollenbeck Community Policing
Area to examine if distinct classes of gang-related homicide actually exist. Utilizing a latent
class analysis (LCA), an underutilized, yet worthwhile semiparametric technique, attempts
to ascertain if hidden groups are present in data. This approach allows for the creation of
groups of “classes” that are mutually exclusive where observations (i.e., homicides) that
are similar to each other will be placed in the same class while observations that differ are
placed in separate classes (Collins and Lanza 2010; Eggleston et al. 2004; Oberski 2016;
Vaughn et al. 2009). This study’s goal is addressing the oversight of traditional examina25
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
tions of gang-related violence by acknowledging that variation exists in the circumstances,
motive, setting, participant characteristics, and rivalry relationship in gang-related homicides and assess in a systematic manner if different types of gang-related homicide are
present. By ascertaining how the latent classes of gang-related homicides differ will allow
for more appropriate interventions to be developed and applied to address gang-related
violence.
4. Methods
4.1. Data
The data include all 844 known gang-related homicides from 1978 through 2012. The
data were manually gathered from the individual homicide case files maintained at LAPD’s
Hollenbeck Community Policing Area (Barton et al. 2020; Brantingham et al. 2012, 2019;
Tita and Radil 2011; Valasik 2018; Valasik et al. 2017). The data include both open and closed
cases and contain a copious number of potential variables related to the participants involved
(e.g., age, gender, gang affiliation, residence, etc.) and the characteristics of the incident
(e.g., weapon, participants relationship, motivation, weapon used, etc.). Additionally, the
data include the street address of a homicide’s location. Griffiths and Tita (2009, p. 480)
point out that concerns about using official police data exist (i.e., reporting, recording,
etc.); however, “homicide is known to suffer from fewer of these limitations than other
offenses, is most likely to come to the attention of the police, and is the least biased
source of official crime data available” (see also Decker and Pyrooz 2010; Katz et al. 2000).
Directly culling the data from homicide detective’s case files allowed for gang-related
events to be coded as either member- or motive-based offenses3 . For a homicide to be
labeled gang-related under a motive-based definition requires the incident to be a direct
function of gang activity (e.g., recruitment, retaliation, territoriality, etc.). In contrast, a
member-based definition is a broader designation that includes any homicide in which
any participant, suspect(s) or victim, is affiliated with a gang. As such, the member-based
designation is more inclusive by capturing homicides that may be the result of an individual
member’s sole motivation, “after all, gang members can and do act of their own accord”
(Papachristos 2009, p. 86). Conversely, a motive-based definition errs by “sampling too
heavily on the dependent variable by capturing only those cases in which a group motive
was determined” (Papachristos 2009, p. 86). Motive-based gang homicides are a subsample
of member-based designated incidents, and artificially restricting a data sample could
discard potentially valuable information (Pyrooz 2012). Regardless of whether a memberor a motive-definition is used to designate a gang homicide, Maxson and Klein (1996, p. 10)
attest that for “all intents and purposes identical” results are produced with the same
variables being able to statistically differentiate a non-gang homicide from gang homicide.
Even though the definition of a “gang” homicide remains unsettled in the literature (see
Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996), the current study employs the more inclusive member-based
definition.
4.2. Research Site
A 15.2 square mile region, the Hollenbeck Community Policing Area, is just east
of the Los Angeles River and the downtown metro area. Over the current study’s time
period there have been approximately 170,000 residents living throughout Hollenbeck’s
eight communities: Boyle Heights, El Sereno, Hermon, Hillside Village, Lincoln Heights,
Montecito Heights, Monterey Hills, and University Hills (Valasik et al. 2017). The area
is over 80 percent Latino and remains a disadvantaged portion of the city with over
25 percent of residents living below the poverty line (Minnesota Population Center 2011).
Intergenerational gangs have a protracted history in Hollenbeck, and while the number of
3
The LAPD traditionally utilizes a member-based definition to demarcate gang-related homicides. The current Department Manual (Line Procedures
4/269.10) states that “any crime may constitute a gang-related crime when the suspect or victim is an active or affiliate gang member, or when
circumstances indicate that the crime is consistent with gang activity.” A near identical definition is reported by Maxson and Klein (1990) for how
LAPD designated such crimes in 1980, supporting the consistent reporting practices by the department during the current study’s time window.
26
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
active street gangs has varied, since the late 1990s there has been approximately 30 active
street gangs, each claiming a geographically demarcated territory (see Barton et al. 2020;
Brantingham et al. 2012, 2019; Moore 1991; Tita et al. 2003; Valasik 2018; Valasik et al.
2017; Vigil 2007). The quasi-institutional nature of Hollenbeck’s gangs has anchored them
to particular barrios (i.e., neighborhoods) greatly restricting the presence and activity
patterns of gang members in four of Hollenbeck’s communities (i.e., Hermon, Monterey
Hills, Hillside Village, and University Hills) (Valasik et al. 2017). While not impenetrable,
Hollenbeck’s jurisdictional boundaries greatly inhibit the local communities from the
adjacent neighborhoods’ activities. Tita and colleagues (Tita et al. 2003; Tita and Radil
2011) further indicate that the both the political boundaries along with the built and
natural environments buffer Hollenbeck’s gangs from interactions with outside groups in
proximate areas while also producing a setting in which gang rivalries in Hollenbeck are
self-contained, creating a natural field site.
4.3. Latent Class Analysis
The current study utilizes an analysis plan that is aimed at uncovering patterns in
gang-related homicides. Since this project is aimed at uncovering whether or not gangrelated homicides group together by specific characteristics, the most appropriate technique
is a Latent Class Analysis (LCA). LCA is a measurement model in which cases can be
classified into mutually exclusive and exhaustive types, or latent classes, based on their
pattern of answers on a set of categorical indicator variables. The LCA was conducted using
the Mplus software package (Muthén and Muthén 2012). The Mplus software package
allows for the statistical control of nonnormality and outliers through the use of robust
maximum likelihood estimation (Curran et al. 1996). In order to conduct tests of model
fit, the first step is to estimate the mixture model based on the latent profile indicators
with an increasing number of classes. LCA model fit was compared using log-likelihood,
Akaike information criteria (AIC), Bayes information criteria (BIC), and entropy, as is
recommended in evaluating these kinds of models (Grant et al. 2006). Smaller values
of log-likelihood, AIC, and BIC indicate better fit to the data or increased probability of
replication, and higher values of entropy reflect better distinctions between groups (Kline
2015). Since some evidence suggests that the BIC performs best of the information criterion
indices (Nylund et al. 2007), this index was prioritized in interpreting the current data.
4.4. Measures
The manual collection of the highly detailed data from individual homicide case files
allowed for a multitude of participant- and incident-level characteristics to be coded and
used in the subsequent analyses. The selection of variables was guided by the larger
literature on disaggregating homicides and key elements of gang-related violence (see
Klein and Maxson 2006; Kubrin 2003; Kubrin and Wadsworth 2003; Pizarro 2008; Skott 2019;
Tita and Griffiths 2005). All of the data culled from the individual case files were collected
and coded by a sole researcher. All of the personal identifiers (e.g., name, birthdate, etc.) in
the dataset were anonymized. Each measure used in the current study and the rationale
for how that measure was created and coded in the data is discussed below in the related
subsections (i.e., participant- or incident-level). Descriptive statistics for the measures are
listed in Table 1 below.
27
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for gang-related homicides, 1978–2012 (N = 844).
Characteristic
Participant-level
Victim age range
11–14
15–18
18–21
22–25
26–30
30+
Motivation
Crime
Drug
Gang
Dispute
Domestic
Other
Victim/Suspect Relationship
Stranger
Non-stranger
Gang Relationship
Rival
Non-rival
Intra-gang
None
Unknown
Incident-level
Location
Street
Inside a structure
Outside a structure
Public Housing Community
Gang Turf
Multiple victims
Drive-By shooting
Time of Day
Overnight
Work Hours
Early Evening
Obs
Percent
11
110
244
123
67
2
1.97%
19.75%
43.81%
22.08%
12.03%
0.36%
34
74
409
209
28
90
4.03%
8.77%
48.46%
24.76%
3.32%
10.66%
195
649
23.10%
76.90%
335
113
69
219
108
39.69%
13.39%
8.18%
26.05%
12.80%
567
67.90%
104
731
12.46%
87.54%
130
731
57
241
15.40%
86.61%
6.75%
28.55%
369
180
295
43.72%
21.33%
34.95%
4.4.1. Participant-Level Characteristics
Age of the victim is included and was organized into six age categories to capture
crime-prone age ranges4 . Race/ethnicity and gender were not included in the analysis as
Hollenbeck’s population is overwhelmingly Latinx (over 80 percent), including the local
intergenerational gangs. The lack of variation in gang violence, being concentrated among
Latino males, 96.0% of victims and 99.1% of suspects, prohibited the inclusion of these
variables as it substantially reduced the statistical power of the subsequent analyses. Prior
research (Griffiths and Tita 2009; Tita and Griffiths 2005) guided the creation of five mutually
exclusive dichotomous variables to capture the suspect’s primary motivation for the violent
act: gang, criminal, drug, dispute, domestic/romantic, and other. A homicide was only
coded as gang-motivated if the incident involved initiation practices, territorial disputes,
targeted attacks, inter-gang rivalries or feuds, or planned retaliations. That is, homicides
were only coded as gang-motivated if it was a decisive act that contributed to that gang
member maintaining his status in the group. Otherwise, a homicide was coded based upon
4
Due to missing data for the suspect (e.g., unknown individual), only the victim’s age was included in the analysis.
28
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
the participating gang member’s primary motive (e.g., dispute, domestic/romantic, etc.).
Any incident that was drug-related or substance-induced was coded as drug; the majority
of these incidents (74.3 percent) were centered around drug dealing, arguments between
participants, or dealer stickups. Likewise, homicides that resulted from a nondrug-related
crime (e.g., burglary, robbery, etc.) were coded as criminal. Homicides that involved
domestic disputes or romantic love interests (e.g., love triangles) were grouped together
and coded as domestic/romantic. Generally, these events involve family members or
intimates and tend to have a much different character than the other motive categories. A
dispute involves any type of argument or fight that escalates into a murder. Generally, these
are spontaneous actions or stem from an existing feud specifically between the participants
involved in the homicide and are not driven or planned out by the members’ respective
gangs. These events include physical altercations that evolve into lethal violence, the
redressing of an ad hominem insult or self-defense. The final category, other, includes
homicides that were accidental, business-related (nondrug-related), facilitated by mental
illness, or unknown.
The relationship between the participants, suspect and victim, is a dichotomous
variable indicating if they were strangers (1 = yes and 0 = no) or if they were non-strangers
(i.e., family members, friends, acquaintances). As Tita and Griffiths (2005, p. 283) argue,
“those who kill within the realm of gang motivated incidents or drug-market activities
“know” their victims, maybe not on a personal level but at least on an organizational/status
level.” To further tease apart the relationship between the participants involved in a gangrelated homicide, the gang affiliation of the suspect and victim was compared to establish
four mutually exclusive dichotomous variables. This categorization process is only possible
due to the robust investigation of Hollenbeck’s gangs over the course of three decades has
provided a rich history documenting the enduring, intergenerational feuds between gangs
in the community policing area (see Brantingham et al. 2012, 2019; Fremon 2008; Moore
1978, 1991; Tita et al. 2003; Tita and Radil 2011; Valasik 2014, 2018; Vigil 1988, 2007). Beyond
the detailed academic sources, detailed gang intelligence maintained by Gang Impact Team
(GIT) officers and gang detectives were also used in establishing this metric (see Valasik
et al. 2016). Rival (1 = yes and 0 = no) indicates that both of the participants involved in
a homicide were members of gangs that have an active rivalry with ongoing hostilities.
Events that involved participants from separate gangs without ongoing hostilities are
designated as non-rival (1 = yes and 0 = no). A homicide occurring where both the victim
and suspect were affiliated with the same gang is considered to be an intra-gang (1 = yes and
0 = no) event. The final category, none (1 = yes and 0 = no) involves one participant, either
suspect or victim, who was not affiliated with any known gang at the time of the homicide.
4.4.2. Incident-Level Characteristics
Prior research (Corsaro et al. 2017; Tita and Griffiths 2005) indicates that the location
of where a homicide occurs will differ between various types of homicides. Given gangrelated violence to transpire on the street, a variable was created to specifically capture this
phenomenon (1 = yes and 0 = no). Further, differentiating where a homicide takes place,
incidents are outside in open, public areas or inside a building or structure (1 = inside and
0 = outside). Gang turf (1 = yes and 0 = no) indicates if a homicide occurred within one
of the participant’s gang’s claimed territory or outside of those boundaries. Again, the
robust gang scholarship by Hollenbeck and gang intelligence allowed for this metric to be
created (see Brantingham et al. 2012, 2019; Radil et al. 2010; Tita et al. 2003; Valasik 2014).
Prior research (Griffiths and Tita 2009; Holloway and McNulty 2003; Popkin et al. 2000;
Venkatesh 1997; Vigil 2007; Weatherburn et al. 1999) has also suggested that public housing
communities experience dramatically higher levels of gang-related violence. Griffiths and
Tita (2009, p. 480) find that they are in fact “hotbeds of violence” where the participants
involved are more likely to local residents. Therefore, public housing (1 = yes and 0 = no)
is a measure specifically accounting for the influence of these disadvantaged areas by
designating if a homicide transpired within a public housing complex. It should be noted
29
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
that all of the public housing communities within Hollenbeck have a well-documented
history of entrenched gang activity and violence (see Barton et al. 2020; Fremon 2008;
Vigil 2007).
The literature on gang violence indicates that gang-related incidents are also more
likely to involve multiple victims (Maxson et al. 1985; Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996). A
dichotomous variable was used to capture this difference (1 = multiple individuals and
0 = a singular individual). Gang research has also indicated that gangs routinely employ
the drive-by as a technique to attack rival gangs (Bolden 2020; Klein 1971; Sanders 1994;
Huff 1996; Valdez et al. 2009; Vasquez et al. 2010). Moore and colleagues (Moore et al.
1983) further suggest that it is not uncommon for East Los Angeles gang members to reside
outside of their claimed turf and to routinely travel back to these locations to socialize (see
also Valasik and Tita 2018). Therefore, it is reasonable to suspect that if a vehicle is being
utilized by a gang member to return to their gang’s turf that it would also be accessible for
a directed attack on a rival if needed. This study defines a drive-by (1 = yes and 0 = no)
as an incident in which one gang member discharged a firearm towards another gang
member from a moving vehicle. Lastly, from a routine activities perspective, time of day
influences the activity patterns of gang members, thereby impacting gang-related violence.
Three dichotomous variables are constructed to capture the different times of day in which
a homicide could transpire: work hours, early evening, and overnight. Incidents were
coded based on when the homicide event transpired (1 = transpired in the time period and
0 = did not transpire in the time period), with work hours being from 7 a.m. to 6 p.m., early
evening being from 6 p.m. to 11 p.m., and overnight being from 11 p.m. to 7 a.m.
5. Results
On the basis of the analyses, there were five separate classes of gang-related homicides.
One of the key results is that stranger versus non-stranger homicides had to be separated
out since this distinction drove much of the variation in classes. Once it was realized
that the main distinguishing characteristic between the classes was whether or not the
participants, victim and suspect, knew each other or were strangers, the dataset was broken
into two separate LCAs. Overall there were five separate classes found in the homicide
data: three were non-strangers and two were strangers. In order to identify the best-fitting
number of profiles, latent class models containing one through four classes for the nonstranger data and one to three classes for the stranger data were fit to exhaust the available
models. To decide the final number of classes, we examined both fit statistics and whether
or not the added class provided additional nuance to our understanding of gang homicide.
Overall, improvements in fit (measured using AIC, BIC, and log-likelihood) occurred as
the number of classes increased up to three classes for non-stranger gang homicides and
two classes for stranger gang homicides.
For the non-stranger homicides there were three categories. Class 1, or Rival Drive-by
(n = 321), homicides were characterized by the participants being from rival gangs. These
homicides tend to employ a vehicle to facilitate a drive-by shooting. As such, the location
of the incident is outside. Rival Drive-by homicides are also more likely to take place
overnight, (i.e., very late at night or very early in the morning). Lastly, these homicides are
not precipitated by a known crime or dispute.
To make these findings more tangible, the above results were used to identify an
example of a “modal” Rival Drive-by homicide in our dataset.
April 2001: Around 1:50 a.m., two State Street gang members (a 36-year-old, Latino
male and a 17-year old, Latino male) were repairing a vehicle on a street alongside a curb
inside their gang’s claimed turf. Two rival Primera Flats gang members (a 21-year-old,
Latinx male and an unidentified Latinx male) proceeded to drive by and opened fire on
the victims, striking both of them multiple times. The suspects fled southbound in their
vehicle. The victims were transported to the LAC+USC Medical Center where they both
succumbed to their wounds.
30
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Note that the suspect and victims involved were from rival gangs, a drive-by was
used, the incident took place outside on the street, it transpired overnight, and was a
directed attack. That is, another crime or dispute did not facilitate the homicide.
Class 2, or Non-gang Involved Victim (n = 97), homicides are primarily characterized
by the victim not being associated with a documented gang. Usually, these homicides are
precipitated by another criminal act or drug-related activity. Non-gang Involved Victim
homicides are more likely to involve multiple victims. In addition, these homicides may
have the occasional drive-by, but they remain uncommon.
Selecting on the significant characteristics of this type, an incident from the case files
of a modal Non-gang Involved Victim homicide is presented.
January 2001: At approximately 6:30 a.m., the two victims (33-year-old, Latino male
and a 42-year-old, Latino male) were sitting in a vehicle when they were approached
by two Lincoln Heights gang members (25-year-old, Latino male and a 29-year old,
Latino male) who carjacked the vehicle with them inside. Two additional Lincoln Heights
gang members (34-year-old, Latino male and an unidentified Latina, female) followed in
another vehicle. The first victim was shot in the upper torso and was pushed out of the
vehicle while it drove away. The next day, in the neighboring LAPD police division, the
second victim was found executed with his hands tied behind his back. The murders were
in response to the victims stealing drugs from Lincoln Heights gang members.
Notice that the multiple victims involved were not associated with any gang, the
murders were in response to a drug rip-off, and while a vehicle was involved in crime,
there was not drive-by. Instead, one victim was shot and left at the scene while the other
was taken to a secure location to likely be interrogated in hopes that Lincoln Heights gang
members will be able to recover the stolen drugs.
Rival Confrontation (n = 231), or class 3, homicides involve both participants being
from rival gangs. These homicides often take place overnight (i.e., very late at night or very
early in the morning). They are also more likely to transpire within the boundaries of public
housing complexes. Rival Confrontation homicides are motivated by a dispute, either the
result of an unplanned encounter or being driven by an enduring feud. These homicides
seem to be more directed, resulting in a single victim as illustrated in the incident below.
July 1998: Around 5:30 a.m., two gang members, the suspects, from Cuatro Flats (25year-old, Latino male and a 13-year-old, Latino male) approached a rival ELA 13 Dukes
gang member (18-year-old, Latinx male) in the Aliso Village Public Housing Community.
The prior week a group of ELA 13 Dukes had intervened in a head to head fight between
the younger suspect, who was winning, and another ELA 13 Duke. The ELA 13 Dukes
beat up the younger Cuatro Flats gang member and he wanted to get even. As the
suspects approached the ELA 13 Dukes gang member they asked for some crack cocaine
as a distraction, before pulling out their guns and shooting the victim. The suspects then
fled the scene on foot.
The above example highlights that a prior altercation, in this case a fight, was what
facilitated the homicide, involved participants from rival gangs, the event transpired in a
public housing community where the suspects’ gang claims turf, and the event took place
in the early morning.
For stranger homicides there are two classes. Class 4, or Crime Prone Age (n = 134),
homicides are characterized by the victim being in the 14–22 years old age group. Additionally, there is no gang relationship between the participants, given that the victims do
not have any known associations with any Hollenbeck gangs. These homicides are also
likely to be the result of a drive-by shooting and are more likely to take place overnight
(i.e., very late at night or very early in the morning).
The case narrative presented below illustrates the characteristics which distinguish
Crime Prone Age homicides.
December 2010: Around 2 p.m., the victim (18-year-old, Latino male) was sitting on a
bench waiting for a bus. The two suspects, gang members from Cuatro Flats (18-year-old,
31
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Latino male and a 26-year-old, Latino male) were driving down the road when they saw
the victim sitting on the bench. The suspects quickly pulled over, exited the vehicle, and
fired multiple shots at the victim. LAPD was approaching the scene as the suspects were
about to flee, in which they abandoned their vehicle and ran away. Both suspects failed
to elude LAPD and were taken into custody shortly after committing the murder. The
detectives believe that the suspects mistook the victim for a Primera Flats gang member,
since he was in their territory and both gangs are rivals. The victim never associated with
any Hollenbeck gang and was only in the area to visit a friend.
This homicide highlights the fact that these incidents are likely to be the result of gang
members having greater levels of entitativity (Vasquez et al. 2015). That is, gang members
tend to consider any individual who is loitering within a rival gang’s territory as being
associated with that rival gang. As such, that individual becomes a potential target for
violence, with gang-related violence spilling over into the non-gang population. Thus,
Crime Prone Age homicides are likely to include a lot of cases in which a younger victim is
being mistakenly identified as a rival gang member by the suspect.
Lastly, class 5, or Older Dispute (n = 61), homicides feature a victim in an older
age category. The gang relationship between participants is that the victim and suspect
are members of gangs that are not rivals with each other. Older Dispute homicides are
preceded by some type of dispute that escalates to lethal violence. These homicides also
are more likely to transpire inside a building or residence and take place after work hours
in the early evening.
On the basis of the significant characteristics of this type, an incident from the case
files of a modal Older Dispute homicide is presented below.
May 2007: Just after 7:00 p.m., the victim, an Indiana Dukes gang member (26-year-old,
Latino male) was shopping with his girlfriend and their child at a Food 4 Less grocery
store. Two Laguna Park Vikings gang members (21-year-old, Latino male and a 17-yearold, Latino male) began verbally accosting the victim with a “Where you from?” The
victim called them for disrespecting him in front of his family. The suspects apologized,
but the victim said it was too late. Each party flashed knives at each other, and the
suspects said they would wait outside in the parking lot for the victim. As the victim
exited, he struck a suspect in the face and then was shot by the other suspect.
The above example illustrates that these incidents involve a suspect and victim who
are gang members, but whose gangs are not actively feuding or rivals. Instead, the violence
is sparked by some form of disrespect or affront to on the participants, culminating in
lethal violence. Additionally, the incident transpired in a neutral location, outside of either
participant’s gang’s turf.
6. Discussion and Conclusions
In building on the literature on homicide disaggregation, this study addresses an important gap in the literature: How does the variation in the circumstances, motive, setting,
participant characteristics, and rivalry relationship in gang-related homicides distinguish
one type of event from another? The objective was to systematically ascertain which participant and incident characteristics differentiate discrete subtypes or classes of gang-related
homicide using LCA. The results of the LCA clearly indicate that there are substantial
differences in gang-related homicides, supporting the premise that further disaggregation
is needed to fully understand that nature of these incidents of lethal violence. Specifically,
the LCA revealed that a five class solution (three classes for non-stranger and two classes
for stranger) was both appropriate and meaningful in terms of the theoretical focus in
understanding gang-related violence. The relationship between the participants, victim
and suspect, is an important characteristic driving the creation of the five subtypes/classes
of gang-related homicide detected in this study. There clearly exists distinct patterns in
gang-related homicides.
While the five classes of gang-related homicide tend to be quite distinct from one
another, in terms of the participant and incident characteristics, there does appear to be
32
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
similarities between class 1, Rival Drive-by, and class 4, Crime Prone Age. Gang violence
between rivals quickly becomes an intergenerational process with younger members being
provided with a well-known adversary to attack. The gang literature indicates that group
solidarity is a fundamental feature that drives gang-related violence with street gangs
adhering to a principle of collective responsibility (see Bolden 2020; Densley 2013). That is,
any member of gang acts as a representative for the entire group. Thus, if a gang member
is attacked by a rival gang member the act is considered to be an affront by the entire
rival gang. As such, gang members tend to have greater levels of entitativity, making “all
members of the offending group blameworthy” (Vasquez et al. 2015, p. 249). Additionally,
gangs tend to view any individual that resides in a rival gang’s territory and resembles the
demographics of the rival gang as being associated with that rival gang and a potential
target for retaliatory violence. It is not shocking when retaliatory gang-related violence
(e.g., Rival Drive-by homicides) spills over into the civilian population ensnaring victims
not associated with a street gang (e.g., Crime Prone Age homicides). Leovy (2015, p. 206)
documents this phenomenon in South Central Los Angeles affirming that “a black assailant
looking to kill a gang rival is looking before anything else, for another black male . . . a
presumed combatant, con-scripted into a dismal existence ‘outside the law’ whether he
wanted to be or not.” It seems likely that Crime Prone Age, class 4, homicides are essentially
defective class 1, Rival Drive-by, homicides.
The contributions of this study provide a more nuanced understanding of the variation
that exists in gang-related homicides; however, it is not without limitations that future
research could work to address. First, the focus is on a relatively small area within one
police jurisdiction (LAPD). As such, the results may be restricted to areas more similar to
Hollenbeck. Future research could remedy this by expanding from the division level out to
include other jurisdictions, and researchers will be better able to understand if these classes
maintain across place and improve generalizability. Second, Hollenbeck’s gangs are also
very homogenous. Demographically the gangs are predominately composed of members
of Mexican American descent. Structurally the gangs are considered to be “traditional”
in nature, with strong territorial dispositions and intergenerational linkages (Klein and
Maxson 2006). It is possible the findings from this study may be limited to communities
where only “traditional” gangs are dominant. Third, the dataset includes several years of
increased levels of gang violence in a highly active gang area (see Costanza and Helms 2012;
Howell et al. 2011; Howell and Griffiths 2018; Valasik et al. 2017). Additional replications
across a variety of jurisdictions will help validate how these classifications hold across
time periods. There may also be other variables captured in different databases that would
better capture the variations the exist within gang-related homicides.
Noting such limitations, the goal of this study was to test whether or not gang-related
homicides could (and should) be disaggregated in a manner similar to how researchers
currently disaggregate other homicide types. The purpose for disaggregating homicides is
to be better able to understand important differences between types of homicides for policy,
law enforcement response, and research. Since patterning is found in gang-related homicides, it does not make sense to continue to lump all gang homicides together in larger studies. Policy and practice should take this into consideration when targeting/investigating
gang homicides. By understanding variation in covariates of different homicide types,
this micro-analysis of gang-related homicides in a local setting is important to uncover
how this variation can be used to better understand non-structural characteristics of gangrelated homicide. Since this study is exploratory in nature, it is the first step for future
research to continue disaggregating gang-related homicides across time and place to see
how covariates vary, considering the type of gang-related homicide may impact a planned
intervention. For example, not all gang-related homicides will respond equally to the same
intervention (i.e., k- rails for drive-bys) (see Lasley 1998). Just as no two gangs are identical,
the same idiom applies to acts of gang-related violence.
33
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, M.V. and S.E.R.; methodology, S.E.R.; formal analysis,
S.E.R. writing—original draft preparation, M.V. and S.E.R.; writing—review and editing, M.V. All
authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: The data analyzed in this study were collected under AFSOR MURI grant [FA9550-10-10569]; ARO MURI grant [W911NF-11-1-0332]; ONR grant [ONRN00014-10-1-0221]; ONR grant [ONR
N00014-08-1-1015]; National Science Foundation FRG grant [DMS-0968309].
Institutional Review Board Statement: Not applicable.
Consent Statement: Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement: Not applicable.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Anderson, John, Mark Nye, Ron Freitas, and Jarrett Wolf. 2009. Gang Prosecution Manual; Washington: U.S. Department of Justice,
Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice & Delinquency Prevention.
Bailey, Gary W., and N. Prabha Unnithan. 1994. Gang homicides in California: A discriminant analysis. Journal of Criminal Justice 22:
267–75. [CrossRef]
Barton, Michael S., Matthew A. Valasik, Elizabeth Brault, and George E. Tita. 2020. “Gentefication” in the Barrio: Examining the
relationship between gentrification and homicide in East Los Angeles. Crime & Delinquency 66: 1888–913.
Bichler, Gisella, Alexis Norris, Jared R. Dmello, and Jasmin Randle. 2019. The impact of civil gang injunctions on networked violence
between the bloods and the crips. Crime & Delinquency 65: 875–915.
Bjerregaard, Beth E. 2003. Antigang legislation and its potential impact: The promises and the pitfalls. Criminal Justice Policy Review 14:
171–92. [CrossRef]
Bjerregaard, Beth E. 2015. Legislative approaches to addressing gangs and gang-related crime. In The Handbook of Gangs. Edited by
Scott H. Decker and David C. Pyrooz. Malden: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 345–68.
Bolden, Christian L. 2020. Out of the Red: My Life of Gangs, Prison, and Redemption. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Brantingham, P. Jeffrey, George E. Tita, Martin B. Short, and Shannon E. Reid. 2012. The ecology of gang territorial boundaries.
Criminology 50: 851–85. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, P. Jeffrey, Matthew Valasik, and George E. Tita. 2019. Competitive dominance, gang size and the directionality of gang
violence. Crime Science 8: 7. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, P. Jeffrey, Baichuan Yuan, and Denise Herz. 2020. Is Gang Violent Crime More Contagious than Non-Gang Violent
Crime? Journal of Quantitative Criminology. [CrossRef]
Bursik, Robert J., Jr., and Harold G. Grasmick. 1993. Neighborhoods and Crime: The Dimensions of Effective Community Control. New York:
Lexington Books.
Campedelli, Gian Maria, Alberto Aziani, and Serena Favarin. 2020. Exploring the Immediate Effects of COVID-19 Containment Policies
on Crime: An Empirical Analysis of the Short-Term Aftermath in Los Angeles. American Journal of Criminal Justice. [CrossRef]
Capizzi, Michael, James I. Cook, and M. Schumacher. 1995. The TARGET model: A new approach to the prosecution of gang cases. The
Prosecutor 29: 18–21.
Caudill, Jonathan W., Chad R. Trulson, James W. Marquart, and Matt DeLisi. 2017. On gang affiliation, gang databases, and
prosecutorial outcomes. Crime & Delinquency 63: 210–29.
Collins, Linda M., and Stephanie T. Lanza. 2010. Latent Class and Latent Transition Analysis: With Applications in the Social, Behavioral, and
Health Sciences. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Corsaro, Nicholas, Jesenia M. Pizarro, and Jillian Shafer. 2017. The Influence of Planned Aggression on the Journey to Homicide: An
Examination across Typology. Homicide Studies 21: 179–98. [CrossRef]
Costanza, S. E., and Ronald Helms. 2012. Street gangs and aggregate homicides: An analysis of effects during the 1990s violent crime
peak. Homicide Studies 16: 280–307. [CrossRef]
Curran, Patrick J., Stephen G. West, and John F. Finch. 1996. The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in
confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Methods 1: 16–29. [CrossRef]
Curry, G. David, and Irving A. Spergel. 1988. Gang homicide, delinquency, and community. Criminology 26: 381–406. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H. 1996. Collective and Normative Features of Gang Violence. Justice Quarterly 13: 243–64. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and G. David Curry. 2002. Gangs, gang homicides, and gang loyalty: Organized crimes or disorganized criminals.
Journal of Criminal Justice 30: 343–52. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and David C. Pyrooz. 2010. On the validity and reliability of gang homicide: A comparison of disparate sources.
Homicide Studies 14: 359–76. [CrossRef]
Densley, James. 2013. How Gangs Work. New York: NY, Palgrave Macmillan.
Eggleston, Elaine P., John H. Laub, and Robert J. Sampson. 2004. Methodological sensitivities to latent class analysis of long-term
criminal trajectories. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 20: 1–26. [CrossRef]
34
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Egley, Arlen, Jr. 2012. Gang Homicides—Five U.S. Cities, 2003–2008. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 61: 46–51.
Fremon, Celeste. 2008. G-Dog and the Homeboys: Father Greg Boyle and the Gangs of East Los Angeles. Albuquerque: University of New
Mexico Press.
Geis, Gilbert. 2002. Ganging Up on Gangs: Anti-Loitering and Public Nuisance Laws. In Gangs in America, 3rd ed. Edited by Huff C.
Ronald. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, pp. 257–70.
Grant, Julia D., Jeffrey F. Scherrer, Rosalind J. Neuman, Alexandre A. Todorov, Rumi K. Price, and Kathleen K. Bucholz. 2006. A
comparison of the latent class structure of cannabis problems among adult men and women who have used cannabis repeatedly.
Addiction 101: 1133–42. [CrossRef]
Griffiths, Elizabeth, and George Tita. 2009. Homicide In and Around Public Housing: Is Public Housing a Hotbed, a Magnet, or a
Generator of Violence for the Surrounding Community? Social Problems 56: 474–93. [CrossRef]
Holloway, Steven R., and Thomas L. McNulty. 2003. Contingent urban geographies of violent crime: Racial segregation and the impact
of public housing in Atlanta. Urban Geography 24: 187–211. [CrossRef]
Howell, James C., and Elizabeth Griffiths. 2018. Gangs in America’s Communities, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
Howell, James C., Arlen Egley Jr., George E. Tita, and Elizabeth Griffiths. 2011. US Gang Problem Trends and Seriousness, 1996–2009;
Washington: Bureau of Justice Assistance, US Department of Justice, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
Huff, C. Ron. 1996. The Criminal Behavior of Gang Members and Nongang At-Risk Youth. In Gangs in America. Edited by Huff C.
Ronald. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, pp. 75–102.
Katz, Charles M., and Vincent J. Webb. 2006. Policing Gangs in America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Katz, Charles M., Vincent J. Webb, and David R. Schaefer. 2000. The validity of police gang intelligence lists: Examining differences in
delinquency between documented gang members and nondocumented delinquent youth. Police Quarterly 3: 413–37. [CrossRef]
Klein, Malcolm W. 1971. Street Gangs and Street Workers. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Klein, Malcolm W. 1995. Street Gang Cycles. In Crime. Edited by James Q. Wilson and Joan Petersilia. San Francisco: Institute for
Contemporary Studies Press, 9 vols. pp. 217–36.
Klein, Malcolm W. 2004. Gang Cop: The Words and Ways of Officer Paco Domingo. Walnut Creek: AltaMira Press.
Klein, Malcolm W., and Cheryl L. Maxson. 1989. Street gang violence. In Violent Crime, Violent Criminals. Edited by Neil Alan Weiner
and Marvin. E. Wolfgang. Newbury Park: Sage Publications, pp. 198–234.
Klein, Malcolm W., and Cheryl L. Maxson. 2006. Street Gang Patterns and Policies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Klein, Malcolm W., Cheryl L. Maxson, and Lea C. Cunningham. 1991. “Crack,” street gangs, and violence. Criminology 29: 623–50.
[CrossRef]
Kline, Rex B. 2015. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th ed. New York: Guilford Publications.
Kubrin, Charis E. 2003. Structural covariates of homicide rates: Does type of homicide matter? Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency 40: 139–70. [CrossRef]
Kubrin, Charis E., and Tim Wadsworth. 2003. Identifying the structural correlates of African American killings: What can we learn
from data disaggregation? Homicide Studies 7: 3–35. [CrossRef]
Land, Kenneth C., Patricia L. McCall, and Lawrence E. Cohen. 1990. Structural covariates of homicide rates: Are there any invariances
across time and social space? American Journal of Sociology 95: 922–63. [CrossRef]
Lasley, James R. 1998. “Designing Out” Gang Homicides and Street Assaults; Washington: US Department of Justice, Office of Justice
Programs, National Institute of Justice.
Leovy, Jill. 2015. Ghettoside: A True Story of Murder in America. New York: Spiegel & Grau.
Lewis, Kevin, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2020. Rules of the Game: Exponential Random Graph Models of a Gang Homicide
Network. Social Forces 98: 1829–58. [CrossRef]
Mares, Dennis. 2010. Social disorganization and gang homicides in Chicago: A neighborhood level comparison of disaggregated
homicides. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice 8: 38–57. [CrossRef]
Maxson, Cheryl L. 1999. Gang Homicide: A review and extension of the literature. In Homicide: A Sourcebook of Social Research. Edited
by Smith M. Dwayne and Margaret A. Zahn. Newbury Park: Sage Publications, pp. 197–220.
Maxson, Cheryl L., and Malcolm W. Klein. 1990. Street gang Violence: Twice as Great, or Half as Great? In Gangs in America. Edited by
Huff C. Ronald. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, pp. 71–100.
Maxson, Cheryl L., and Malcolm W. Klein. 1996. Defining Gang Homicide: An Updated Look at Member and Motive Approaches. In
Gangs in America, 2nd ed. Edited by Huff C. Ronald. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, p. 320.
Maxson, Cheryl L., Margaret A. Gordon, and Malcolm W. Klein. 1985. Differences between Gang and Nongang Homicides. Criminology
23: 209–22. [CrossRef]
McCorkle, Richard C., and Terance D. Miethe. 1998. The political and organizational response to gangs: An examination of a “moral
panic” in Nevada. Justice Quarterly 15: 41–64. [CrossRef]
Miethe, Terance D., and Richard C. McCorkle. 1997. Gang membership and criminal processing: A test of the “master status” concept.
Justice Quarterly 14: 407–27. [CrossRef]
Minnesota Population Center. 2011. National Historical Geographic Information System (Version 2.0). Minneapolis: University of
Minnesota.
35
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Mohler, George, Andrea L. Bertozzi, Jeremy Carter, Martin B. Short, Daniel Sledge, George E. Tita, Craig D. Uchida, and P. Jeffrey
Brantingham. 2020. Impact of social distancing during COVID-19 pandemic on crime in Los Angeles and Indianapolis. Journal of
Criminal Justice 68: 101692. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Moore, Joan W. 1978. Homeboys: Gangs, Drugs, and Prison in the Barrios of Los Angeles. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Moore, Joan W. 1991. Going Down to the Barrio: Homeboys and Homegirls in Change. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Moore, Joan, Diego Vigil, and Robert Garcia. 1983. Residence and territoriality in Chicano gangs. Social Problems 31: 182–94. [CrossRef]
Muthén, Linda K., and Bengt O. Muthén. 2012. Mplus: Statistical Analyses with Latent Variables. User’s Guide. Los Angeles: Muthén &
Muthén.
Nakamura, Kiminori, George E. Tita, and David Krackhardt. 2020. Violence in the “balance”: A structural analysis of how rivals, allies,
and third-parties shape inter-gang violence. Global Crime 21: 3–27. [CrossRef]
NGC (National Gang Center). 2017. National Youth Gang Survey Analysis. March. Available online: http://www.nationalgangcenter.
gov/Survey-Analysis (accessed on 3 February 2019).
Nylund, Karen L., Tihomir Asparouhov, and Bengt O. Muthén. 2007. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and
growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 14: 535–69.
[CrossRef]
Oberski, Daniel L. 2016. Mixture models: Latent profile and latent class analysis. In Modern Statistical Methods for HCI. Edited by Judy
Robertson and Maurits Kaptein. Switzerland: Springer, pp. 275–87.
Papachristos, Andrew V. 2009. Murder by Structure: Dominance relations and the Social Structure of Gang Homicide. American Journal
of Sociology 115: 74–128. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., and David S. Kirk. 2006. Neighborhood effects on street gang behavior. In Studying Youth Gangs. Edited by
James F. Short and Lorine A. Hughes. Lanhma: AltaMira Press, pp. 63–84.
Papachristos, Andrew V., David M. Hureau, and Anthony A. Braga. 2013. The corner and the crew: The influence of geography and
social networks on gang violence. American Sociological Review 78: 417–47. [CrossRef]
Pizarro, Jesenia M. 2008. Reassessing the situational covariates of homicides: Is there a need to disaggregate? Homicide Studies 12:
323–49. [CrossRef]
Pizarro, Jesenia M., and Jean Marie McGloin. 2006. Explaining gang homicides in Newark, New Jersey: Collective behavior or social
disorganization? Journal of Criminal Justice 34: 195–207. [CrossRef]
Popkin, Susan J., Victoria E. Gwiasda, Lynn M. Olson, Dennis P. Rosenbaum, Larry Buron, and Rebecca M. Blank. 2000. The Hidden
War: Crime and the Tragedy of Public Housing in Chicago. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Pyrooz, David C. 2012. Structural Covariates of Gang Homicide in Large U.S. Cities. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 49:
489–518. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Scott E. Wolfe, and Cassia Spohn. 2011. Gang-related homicide charging decisions: The implementation of a
specialized prosecution unit in Los Angeles. Criminal Justice Policy Review 22: 3–26. [CrossRef]
Radil, Steven M., Colin Flint, and George E. Tita. 2010. Spatializing social networks: Using social network analysis to investigate
geographies of gang rivalry, territoriality, and violence in Los Angeles. Annals of the Association of American Geographers 100:
307–26. [CrossRef]
Rios, Victor M. 2011. Punished: Policing the Lives of Black and Latino Boys. New York: NYU Press.
Rosenfeld, Richard, and Ernesto Lopez. 2020. Pandemic, Social Unrest, and Crime in US Cities. Washington: Council on Criminal Justice.
Rosenfeld, Richard, Timothy M. Bray, and Arlen Egley. 1999. Facilitating Violence: A Comparison of Gang-Motivated, Gang- Affiliated,
and Nongang Youth Homicides. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 15: 495–516. [CrossRef]
Sampson, Robert J., and W. Byron Groves. 1989. Community structure and crime: Testing social-disorganization theory. American
Journal of Sociology 94: 774–802. [CrossRef]
Sanders, William B. 1994. Gangbangs and Drive-Bys: Grounded Culture and Juvenile Gang Violence. Piscataway: Transaction Publishers.
Shaw, Clifford R., and Henry D. McKay. 1942. Juvenile Delinquency in Urban Areas. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Short, James F., Jr., and Fred L. Strodbeck. 1965. Group Process and Gang Delinquency. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Skott, Sara. 2019. Disaggregating homicide: Changing trends in subtypes over time. Criminal Justice and Behavior 46: 1650–68. [CrossRef]
Smith, Chris M. 2014. The influence of gentrification on gang homicides in Chicago neighborhoods, 1994 to 2005. Crime & Delinquency
60: 569–91.
Tita, George E., and Elizabeth Griffiths. 2005. Traveling to violence: The case for a mobility-based spatial typology of homicide. Journal
of Research in Crime and Delinquency 42: 275–308. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., and Steven M. Radil. 2011. Spatializing the social networks of gangs to explore patterns of violence. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology 27: 521–45. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., K. Jack Riley, Greg Ridgeway, Clifford A. Grammich, Allan Abrahamse, and Peter Greenwood. 2003. Reducing Gun
Violence: Results from an Intervention in East Los Angeles. Santa Monica: Rand Corporation.
Valasik, Matthew A. 2014. “Saving the World, One Neighborhood at a Time”: The Role of Civil Gang Injunctions at Influencing Gang
Behavior. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Criminology, Law & Society, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA. Available
online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2065d17s (accessed on 11 December 2020).
Valasik, Matthew A. 2018. Gang Violence Predictability: Using Risk Terrain Modeling to study Gang Homicides and Gang Assaults in
East Los Angeles. Journal of Criminal Justice. 58: 10–21. [CrossRef]
36
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 48
Valasik, Matthew A., and Shannon E. Reid. 2020. Distinguishing between aggression in groups and in gangs: Are gangs always
violent? In The Handbook of Collective Violence: Current Developments and Understandings. Edited by Carol A. Ireland, Michael
Lewis, Anthony Lopez and Jane L. Ireland. New York: Routledge, pp. 273–90.
Valasik, Matthew A., and George E. Tita. 2018. Gangs and Space. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental Criminology. Edited by
Gerben J. N. Bruinsma, Gerben Bruinsma and Shane D. Johnson. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 843–71.
Valasik, Matthew A., Shannon E. Reid, and Matthew D. Phillips. 2016. CRASH and burn: Abatement of a specialised gang unit. Journal
of Criminological Research, Policy and Practice 2: 95–106. [CrossRef]
Valasik, Matthew A., Michael S. Barton, Shannon E. Reid, and George E. Tita. 2017. Barriocide: Investigating the temporal and spatial
influence of neighborhood structural characteristics on gang and non-gang homicides in East Los Angeles. Homicide Studies 21:
287–311. [CrossRef]
Valdez, Avelardo, Alice Cepeda, and Charles Kaplan. 2009. Homicidal events among Mexican American street gangs: A situational
analysis. Homicide Studies 13: 288–306. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vasquez, Eduardo A., Brian Lickel, and Karen Hennigan. 2010. Gangs, displaced, and group-based aggression. Aggression and Violent
Behavior 15: 130–40. [CrossRef]
Vasquez, Eduardo A., Lisa Wenborne, Madeline Peers, Emma Alleyne, and Kirsty Ellis. 2015. Any of them will do: In-group
identification, out-group entitativity, and gang membership as predictors of group-based retribution. Aggressive Behavior 41:
242–52. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vaughn, Michael G., Matt DeLisi, Kevin M. Beaver, and Matthew O. Howard. 2009. Multiple murder and criminal careers: A latent
class analysis of multiple homicide offenders. Forensic Science International 183: 67–73. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Venkatesh, Sudhir Alladi. 1997. The social organization of street gang activity in an urban ghetto. American Journal of Sociology 103:
82–111. [CrossRef]
Vigil, James Diego. 1988. Barrio Gangs: Street Life and Identity in Southern California. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Vigil, James Diego. 2007. The Projects: Gang and Non-Gang Families in East Los Angeles. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Weatherburn, Don, Bronwyn Lind, and Simon Ku. 1999. “Hotbeds of Crime”: Crime and Public Housing in Sydney. Crime &
Delinquency 45: 256–72.
Williams, Kirk R., and Robert L. Flewelling. 1988. The social production of criminal homicide: A comparative study of disaggregated
rates in American cities. American Sociological Review 53: 421–31. [CrossRef]
37
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Evolving Patterns of Aggression: Investigating the
Structure of Gang Violence during the Era of Civil
Gang Injunctions
Gisela Bichler 1, *, Alexis Norris 1 and Citlalik Ibarra 2
1
2
*
Criminal Justice Department, California State University San Bernardino, San Bernardino, CA 92407, USA;
[email protected]
Center for Criminal Justice Research, California State University San Bernardino,
San Bernardino, CA 92407, USA;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Received: 11 September 2020; Accepted: 6 November 2020; Published: 11 November 2020
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Abstract: Mapping the structural characteristics of attack behavior, this study explores how
violent conflict evolved with the implementation of civil gang injunctions (CGIs). Networks
were generated by linking defendants and victims named in 963 prosecutions involving street gangs
active in the City of Los Angeles (1998–2013). Aggregating directed ties to 318 groups associated
with the combatants, we compare four observations that correspond with distinct phases of CGI
implementation—development (1998–2001), assent (2002–2005), maturity (2006–2009), and saturation
(2010–2013). Using a triad census to calculate a ratio of simple patterns (retaliation, directed lines,
and out-stars) to complex three-way interactions, we observed that CGIs were associated with a
substantive thickening of conflict—greater complexity was found in conflict relations over time.
Dissecting the nature of change, stochastic actor-oriented models (SAOMs) show that enjoined gangs
are more likely to initiate transitive closure. The findings suggest that crime control efforts must make
regular adjustments in response to the evolving structure of gang interactions.
Keywords: street gang violence; civil gang injunctions; conflict network; social network analysis
1. Introduction
The harm generated by gang violence extends beyond members and their rivals, threatening entire
communities. The murder of Michael (20) and Timothy Bosch (21) illustrates this point. The brothers
were hanging out in Culver West Alexander Park on 27 September 2003 (Noonan 2008). A Culver City
Boys (CCB) gang member approached, and pointing a gun to Timothy’s head, declared his affiliation
and asked whether the victims belonged to a rival gang. Not believing the victims’ denials, the brothers
were shot. Bystanders are also caught in the crossfire. Melody Ross (16), a cheerleader at Wilson
High School in Long Beach had just left her homecoming football game in 2009 and was sitting with a
friend on a curb outside her school. Nearby stood two Rollin 20’s Crips gang members, both of whom
were not students. Melody did not know them. Two Insane Crips rival gang members approached,
exchanging gang slurs with the Rollin’ 20’s Crips. One of the Insane Crips shot in the Rollin 20’s
Crips direction. Both Rollin 20’s Crips were wounded: Melody Ross died (Vives and Bolch 2009).
As these cases show, gang violence puts all members of the community, gang and non-gang involved,
at great risk.
To stop the spread of violence, the City of Los Angeles adopted several crime control strategies, one of
which was to enact civil gang injunctions (CGIs) targeting the most violent groups. Across successive
administrations, three City Attorneys enacted a total of 46 civil gang injunctions targeting 72 gangs.
39
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
One of the aims behind the use of injunctions was to suppress the kinds of social interactions thought
to facilitate gang violence. A critical feature of most CGIs is a clause designed to restrict a gang’s ability
to exert a visible public presence in specific neighborhoods.
While research shows that focused crime-reduction interventions can reduce crime
(Braga and Weisburd 2012), there is still a need to understand how targeting the most problematic actors,
such as the most violent gangs by implementing a CGI, impacts the larger community. Why? Because
gang violence is an inherently social phenomenon (Lewis and Papachristos 2020)—embedded in a
community of combatants, targeting one gang is likely to generate ripple effects throughout the social
landscape that includes other groups with whom the target gang interacts. Targeting one, or a set of
highly aggressive gangs, stands to reshape the structure of violence across the conflict network.
By understanding how crime control efforts shape networked violence, we are in a better position
to develop interventions that minimize displaced aggression, reduce gang conflict, and improve
public safety. While the structure of gang violence has been investigated within a single gang (e.g.,
McCuish et al. 2015), within identifiable neighborhoods and large regions (e.g., Randle and Bichler 2017;
Tita and Radil 2011; Radil et al. 2010), and across cities, i.e., Boston (Papachristos et al. 2013),
Chicago (Lewis and Papachristos 2020; Papachristos 2009), Montreal (Descormiers and Morselli 2011),
and Newark (McGloin 2007), to the best of our knowledge, this study is among the first to investigate
shifting patterns in the structure of street gang violence associated with a protracted crime control
strategy such as CGIs. The present study extends network investigations of gang conflict by
comparing four violence networks generated from incidents occurring within a 16-year study period
(1 January 1998–31 December 2013). Our primary aim is to document whether there were substantive
shifts in the structure of violence that correspond with phases of CGI adoption in the City of Los Angeles.
This paper unfolds as such. Before we outline how we investigated gang violence networks,
we briefly describe CGIs as implemented in California and explore current thinking about violence
networks and the implications for gang control strategies. After describing the methodology used,
we report on two sets of analyses—triadic censuses and stochastic actor-oriented models—before
discussing the most salient implications of this investigation of gang-involved violence.
2. Background
2.1. CGIs and Focused Deterrence
CGIs are a crime control strategy designed to impose behavioral restrictions on gangs and/or
gang members within designated areas. The City of Los Angeles defines a gang as a group of allied
individuals working toward a common purpose who engage in violent, unlawful, or criminal activity
to achieve their aims. The group brands itself with symbols (e.g., tattoos and colors), often has
common demographic characteristics and may exert control over specific areas within neighborhoods
(Los Angeles Police Department 2020). CGIs fall under California Civil Code, sections 3479 and 3480,
which permit civil restrictions on activity found to be a public nuisance. Of interest to the present
study, CGIs impose restrictions on public behaviors within designated areas, known as “safety zones”.
Gang members can be subjected to enhanced penalties for engaging in illegal behavior in the safe
zone (e.g., selling drugs, vandalism, and threatening/intimidation). Other specifications may require
individuals to adhere to a curfew or avoid hanging out with other gang members in public (this includes
driving, walking, standing, or appearing together in the public’s view). Restrictions are also imposed on
the gang itself such as; no gathering in public areas, no lookouts or loitering, and no recruiting children.
CGIs can be framed as a focused-deterrent strategy directed at reducing gang-involved violence.
Focused deterrence is a problem-focused policing approach, which calls for targeting individuals or
groups that are driving crime in specific areas (Braga and Weisburd 2012). Those who violate CGIs may
face civil sanctions, such as financial penalties (up to $1000) or they may receive gang enhancements
on their sentences (up to 25 years). These sanctions are meant to send a clear message to targeted
individuals that the cost of engaging in the prohibited behaviors is high. By imposing behavioral
40
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
restrictions and increasing penalties for engaging in those behaviors, CGIs are intended to deter gang
violence in the community.
Implicit in the use of CGIs is the notion that social interactions trigger violence. For example,
violence may occur when gang members congregate in public space, particularly if the location is
known to be linked to a specific gang member (i.e., someone’s home) or controlled by the gang
(e.g., established turf or set space). Here, social interactions expose individuals to risk when rivals
pass by looking for conflict. Thus, some of the stipulations included within CGI conditions aim to
remove opportunities to become involved in social interactions that may lead to violent altercations,
i.e., do not drive, stand, sit, walk, gather or appear with other gang members in public view or anyplace
accessible within designated areas of the city (usually areas claimed as gang turf).
Most studies examining the effectiveness of civil gang injunctions explore the reduction in crime
within designated areas. Studies find that CGIs are associated with a decline in serious and violent
crime in areas with safe zones (e.g., Carr et al. 2017; Grogger 2002; Los Angeles County Civil Grand
Jury 2004; Ridgeway et al. 2019). While previous research has found most crime control effects
to be short lived (e.g., Maxson et al. 2005; O’Deane and Morreale 2011), a more recent study by
Ridgeway et al. (2019) examining quarterly crime reports from the Los Angeles Police Department
(LAPD) between 1988 and 2014 found a 5% short-term decline in crime, as well as a 18% long-term
decline in crime in targeted areas. Even though research examining the impact of CGIs on levels of crime
in focal neighborhoods have typically found positive effects, studies focusing on individuals targeted
by the CGIs have been less encouraging. For example, interviewing gang members subjected to CGI
restrictions, Swan and Kirstin A. (2017) discovered that individuals continued their gang activities
after CGIs were imposed on them; their activities shifted to neighborhoods without gangs or to rival
gang territory, which intensified existing conflict. Exploring the structure of post-CGI conflict among 23
Bloods and Crips gangs, (Bichler et al. [2017] 2019) discovered the most aggressive gangs became more
enmeshed in a web of violence and more centrally located in chains of violence post-injunction—CGIs
were associated with increased violence (Bichler et al. [2017] 2019).
Why would violence increase post-CGI? Because, as much as CGIs may help to remove
opportunities for conflict, they also contribute to reshaping the local social landscape,
which may displace, alter the nature of, or generate more violent conflict. Each gang is embedded in
a local social system wherein groups vary on their perceived social standing within the community
(e.g., dominance and street respect), control of resources (such as drug sales), and physical proximity
to other groups (Lewis and Papachristos 2020). The imposition of a CGI is a public announcement that
the group is under increased scrutiny and that their public behavior is restricted. As such, CGIs alter
the local social system, and may push gangs to other areas to remain competitive (e.g., expanding drug
markets by invading rival territories), leading to more aggression. It is also plausible that as enjoined
gangs refrain from public displays of dominance, their territorial control may faulter leading other
groups to attack. Thus, investigating how the social landscape of gang-related violence changes in
response to coordinated crime control interventions enriches our understanding of conflict dynamics
in a way that may support the development of more effective prevention measures.
2.2. Networked Violence
The dynamics of gang violence are complex and constantly shifting. Research in this area
has regularly focused on the behaviors of the gangs and/or individual gang members; often
using ethnographic and survey-based research, to understand changes in gang-on-gang violence.
Studies examined gang cohesion (Decker 1996; Hennigan and Sloane 2013; Klein and Maxson 2010;
Papachristos 2013), motivating factors for gang behavior such as turf disputes (Braga et al. 2006;
Papachristos et al. 2010), social influences (Hennigan and Spanovic 2012; Stafford and Warr 1993),
and interpersonal disputes (Papachristos and Kirk 2006); as well as, the amorphous nature of gang
membership (e.g., Decker 1996; Melde and Esbensen 2013) to understand shifts in violence. Contributing
to this body of work, we concur with recent arguments suggesting that there is a need to use structural
41
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
metrics to understand how violent social interactions among pairs of gangs shape gang violence at the
community level (e.g., Lewis and Papachristos 2020).
Violent encounters involving gang members do not occur in isolation. Rather, gang members are
embedded within an intricate web of social relations that aggregates to form a complex network of
interlinkages binding gangs within a larger community of violence. At the individual level, individuals
respond to what they learn or experience, and in turn, this reaction facilitates additional ripple effects,
often spreading in a hyperdyadic process toward new people (See: Christakis and Fowler 2009).
For instance, when a gang member suffers an injury or perceived harm to reputation or status, the
individual (or group acting on their behalf) will react in some fashion, often in an effort to reciprocate
harm (e.g., Papachristos et al. 2013, 2015). Notably, the individuals involved in the initial act of
violence may not be the actors who retaliate. Instead, other members of the group may initiate
violence, toward the original aggressor or someone else associated with the aggressor’s gang. Thus,
there are advantages to aggregating violent conflict to the group level when examining the pattern of
conflict—gang-on-gang attacking behavior may better capture the web of conflict.
While an initial act of violence can set a sequence of interactions into motion, fueling continued conflict,
transference or retaliation is not necessarily the most likely outcome (e.g., Randle and Bichler 2017).
Investigating the likelihood of direct retaliation (reciprocated violence) relative to other reactions,
Lewis and Papachristos (2020) also find evidence of generalized retaliation wherein gangs unable
to reciprocate directly against the group that murdered one of their own, launch attacks directed at
other gangs. Of critical importance in understanding how violent conflict ripples through communities
is the structure and topography of the local social neighborhood. Structural hierarchies are likely to
exist that reflect local patterns of social dominance. In network terms, the local social neighborhood
includes everyone a focal individual is directly connected to, referred to as alters, as well as all the
links among those alters. Local social neighborhoods are important because they influence what
information groups receive and how they react to events, providing a glimpse into the social context
within which a focal gang is embedded. These patterns may be indicative of competitive dominance
(Brantingham et al. 2019).
Figure 1 illustrates two sets of interaction patterns that may result from an initial violent event.
Circles represent gangs and the directed arrows originate at the aggressor and terminate at the victim.
The dashed arrows depict the reaction from an initial aggression (solid line). Looking at the transmission
of aggression, three simple structures are profiled. Direct retaliation by the aggrieved group may occur
when groups have equivalent stature within the community. Imbalanced patterns of violence may
indicate the groups have unequal social status. For instance, a knock on or domino effect representing a
directed line suggests that the victimized gang is unable to respond directly, instead they attack another
group of lesser status. When direct retaliation does not occur, the group can become emboldened,
reacting to their “success” by launching several attacks aimed at different groups (referred within
network analytic approaches as out-star structures) to improve their position of dominance.
Prior research using network analytics observe different hierarchical structures that may reflect
differential positions of competitive dominance. For instance, mapping conflict among 158 primarily
Blood and Crip gangs active in Los Angeles, Randle and Bichler (2017) discovered a high level of
internal conflict (within group violence), in-star and out-star structures (wherein a group was attacked
by multiple gangs, or a gang attacked many others), and directed lines (one gang attacks another
who then attacks a third group). More in tune with the present study, (Bichler et al. [2017] 2019)
investigate the structure of violence for 23 Bloods and Crips gangs under civil gang injunctions,
in the City of Los Angeles. While there is a tendency for the most violent groups to be victimized
the most, local hierarchies exist (e.g., directed lines); and attack networks change significantly over time.
Investigating murder in Chicago, Lewis and Papachristos (2020) significantly extend this line of inquiry
by testing the likelihood that different local structures shape the larger network of violence, discovering
that direct reciprocity differs by group attributes (e.g., race) and that other more complex structural
features, associated with generalized reciprocity, vary significantly over time when short observation
42
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
windows are used (e.g., two years). Of note, these authors also found that a few particularly aggressive
groups are central to spreading violence through the network (in network terms this is activity spread)
and that when two gangs are attacked by the same aggressor, they attack each other (reflecting the
network structure called popularity closure).
Figure 1. Structure of Violent Conflict.
Complex structures, like popularity closure, involve three-way relations of integrated conflict
among a set of actors A, B, and C: these structures may reflect a social hierarchy of dominance among
gangs (Papachristos 2009; Papachristos et al. 2013). When someone from gang A kills a member of
gang B, and a member of gang B responds by attacking a third party from gang C, a triadic structure
emerges that closes the loop: the loop closes when the third party to the violence, gang C, shoots a
member from gang A. To illustrate that there are many different complex structures in addition to
the scenario just described, the lines labeled with question marks in Figure 1 can be replaced with
directed arrows. Specifically, there are seven different configurations of interest: A→B←C, A→C;
→ ←
→
A←B←C, A→C; A←B→C, A←→C; A→B←C, A←→C; A→B→C, A←→C; A→B←→C, A←→C; and
← ←
→
← →
←→
→ ←
←→
→ →
←→
→ ←→
←→
A←→B←→C, A←→C.
←→ ←→
←→
Mapping the network of violence that emerges from local conflict, provides insight into the larger
community dynamics that may facilitate aggression. It is possible to support interdiction efforts
by observing change in these patterns. Where gang violence is characterized by simple structures,
and prolific aggressors dominate, crime control strategies may best target the main instigators of violence,
particularly when there is a small set of aggressors generating pockets of violence. Where the ratio of
simple to complex structures favors integrated patterns of conflict, a multi-faceted approach targeting
inter-related sets of gangs may yield greater violence reduction. Crime control strategies would stand
to be more effective if a set of combatants were targeted, rather than a single aggressor.
2.3. Current Study
The imposition of a civil gang injunction is, without doubt, a clear public admonition of a
group’s behavior. As such, it should trigger a shift in violent behavior, in either the frequency of
aggression, direction of attack, or selection of targets (Randle and Bichler 2017; Bichler et al. [2017] 2019).
While individual level changes in behavior are expected as police officers interact with specific gang
members, the sanction is directed toward the entire group. By aggregating individual-level interactions
to the gangs each combatant affiliates with, we can map out emergent gang-on-gang conflict patterns
(Lewis and Papachristos 2020). Joining the local social conflict neighborhoods of individual gangs will
reveal the emergent community structure of violent relations.
By examining an entire community of conflict, we extend prior research that investigated a
single gang (e.g., McCuish et al. 2015), a single neighborhood (e.g., Brantingham et al. 2019), or drew
from a subset of gangs sharing a characteristic, i.e., predominantly African American gangs, such as
43
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Bloods and Crips (Randle and Bichler 2017; Bichler et al. [2017] 2019). In addition, comparing across
successive waves of observations offers a way to explore the cumulative effect of multiple CGIs on the
structure of violence. As more gangs are enjoined, the effect of this crime control strategy may evolve.
To date, only one study has documented the long-term effect of the CGI experience in Los Angeles (see
Ridgeway et al. 2019): while this spatial investigation revealed neighborhood trends, it was unable to
expose changes in the social interactions among gang members. For instance, violence may decline in
affected neighborhoods if CGIs drive gang members away. However, as Swan and Kirstin A. (2017)
discovered through an ethnographic study involving interviews with gang members, criminal behavior
and interactions may shift to communities in other cities (not proximate displacement)—a network
approach is needed to investigate this possibility.
Our general expectation is that aggression levels change following the imposition of
CGIs, with targeted gangs becoming more deeply embroiled in complex patterns of violence
(Bichler et al. [2017] 2019; Lewis and Papachristos 2020).
Gang associations are dynamic
(Ouellet et al. 2019), and as individuals respond to perceived harms to address challenges to social
status (Papachristos 2009), conflict may erupt that involves unexpected combatants (Descormiers and
Morselli 2011), particularly given that the structure of violent relations is unstable, shifting substantially
between observations (Lewis and Papachristos 2020). The imposition of a CGI is a gang-specific
attack, and successive attacks on groups operating within a street gang community could generate a
cumulative effect that substantively alters the structural indicators of competitive dominance. With
little prior work documenting the nature of structural change to expect, we posit that while the
embeddedness of conflict is likely to be unstable, the overall tendency should be that complexity
will increase given that gangs may shift activities to new areas (Swan and Kirstin A. 2017). At the
community level, as more gangs are enjoined there may be a saturation effect, thus, when the CGI
adoption curve reaches the assent and maturity phases this should correspond to shifting ratios of
simple to complex patterns across successive observation periods, i.e., more popularity closure. At the
gang level, the most aggressive groups may exhibit a significant growth in dominance, meaning they
attack more following the imposition of a CGI.
3. Methods
3.1. Case Identification and Network Generation
A 2-step sampling method was used to identify cases of street gang violence (See: Figure 2).
The first step involved identifying cases associated with seed gangs. Seeds are the starting actors used
when sampling with a link-tracing method. In this study, seed gangs include all LA-based gangs (and
cliques) named in civil gang injunctions filed in the City of Los Angeles between 1 February 2000 and
24 September 2013. We used the advanced search parameters of Westlaw and LexisNexis to restrict
the hits returned to California court cases occurring within the designated observation period. Next,
all other gangs associated with named victims or co-offenders were searched. Formal names and
variations of gang names were used in this second step to ensure comprehensive case capture. The 2-step
sampling procedure generates complete egocentric networks for 76 seed gangs and 122 alters (groups
involved in conflict with the seed gangs). In general terms, this sample constitutes 198 case studies.
Egocentric networks include the focal actor (e.g., each seed gang) and all connections among those
actors directly connected to focal actors (alter gangs). Representing the local social world in which
actors are embedded, egocentric networks provide a glimpse into the social network as seen from the
actor’s perspective. The 120 additional groups identified in the second step (see the secondary alters
illustrated with white symbols in Figure 2) constitute the boundary of the network, as we do not have
complete information about the conflict patterns involving their local social neighborhoods.
44
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Figure 2. The 2-Step Sampling Process.
The sampling procedure generated 4610 cases. Four inclusion criteria were applied to focus the
investigation on gang violence originating from the City of Los Angeles:
1.
2.
3.
4.
The case involved at least one gang known to be based in the City of Los Angeles;
There was at least one charge/conviction for a violent crime (e.g., assault with a deadly weapon,
attempted homicide, or homicide);
At least one defendant was tried as an adult;
The crime occurred between 1 January 1997 and 31 December 2016 somewhere within the
five-county study region—Los Angeles, Orange, Ventura, Riverside and San Bernardino.
As illustrated in Figure 3, this screening protocol reduced the sample to 993 cases—35 additional
Mexican Mafia cases were identified but not included here as they did not involve a direct act of
violence perpetrated by this group.
Figure 3. Case Identification Protocol.
45
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Extracting information from the 993 cases found to satisfy all inclusion criteria, we identified
1771 defendants and 1944 victims1 . Exploring combatants’ age was challenging given a large
amount of missing information (27% of subjects); however, incidents regularly involved interactions
among adults and young people (only 35 cases were known to involve only juveniles or minors).
Exploring age further, approximately 20% of individuals (n = 3004 individuals with age reported)
involved in these violent conflicts were known to be under 21 years of age (16.6% were juveniles; 3.1%
were minors). From a case perspective, 34% of cases (n = 993) involved at least one minor or juvenile,
and from a group perspective, 55% of 307 street gangs observed in this sample were involved in at
least one conflict involving someone reported to be under 21.
Approximately 77% of cases (n = 993) involved murder or attempted murder, with the remainder
distributed across robberies (12%, including carjacking), assaults (9%) and other types of violence (2%).
Most incidents involved gun crime (91%). Investigating incident location, we discovered that 70%
occurred in the City of Los Angeles, and while the remaining 30% of cases transpired in 84 different
cities spanning from Oakland to San Diego, most occurred in cities within a one-hour drive (no traffic)
from Los Angeles. Within the City of Los Angeles, violent incidents occurred in 97 identifiable
neighborhoods or areas.2 Most cases involved a social context wherein offenders did not act alone,
such as parties or other social gatherings, however, 64.9% of cases list co-offenders and approximately
half of these incidents (31% of cases) describe 2 or more co-offenders. In approximately 51% of cases,
a single victim was named.
Valued, directed conflict networks were generated by linking each defendant and accomplice
named in the case to each identified victim. As such, directed ties (referred to as arcs) represent acts of
aggression. This means that if there were two co-offenders and one victim, two arcs were generated;
two co-offenders and two victims resulted in four directed acts of aggression; and one offender attacking
three victims resulted in three aggressions. Amplifying the amount of violence in this way permits
us to weight the network to reflect the dominance of gangs. When multiple gang members attack,
or a lone offender victimizes a group of people, community impacts are magnified as this level of
aggression stands to inflict greater street terrorism.
Associated gangs and cliques were recorded for each offender and victim. Due to the extensive
amount of missing clique information, we aggregated ties by the gang in order to investigate
gang-on-gang violence. Since some victims were not known to be affiliated with a gang, 11 additional
group categories were used—7 law enforcement and criminal justice agencies, and 4 community
groups (community, drug dealer, drug involved, and pimps).
Investigating the number of cases identified per year, we discovered censuring: few cases occurred
before 1998 or after 2013.3 As a result, we reduced the 20-year observation period to a 16 year period.
1
2
3
Inter-rater agreement was assessed on case inclusion criteria and identification of variables capturing defendant characteristics,
victim characteristics, witness characteristics, characteristics of other individuals involved in the case (e.g., gang experts and
responding officers), and situational elements of the case. Coders were assessed on a training sample of cases raging in
difficulty level (the most difficult cases involving multiple incidents spanning across different periods of time, each period
consisting of different incident elements). We observed a Cohen’s Kappa of 0.84, indicating substantial agreement between
the ten coders (Landis and Koch 1997). However, when just looking across defendant and victim characteristic the agreement
increased (k = 0.96). This indicates that in capturing the defendants and victims’ names, aliases, demographics, and which
gangs they belong to, there was almost perfect agreement. Subsequent random spot checks of coding confirmed reliable
retrieval of offenders, victims, and their gang affiliation.
The inclusion criterion specified that at least one individual associated with a case was known to be an active member of a
gang based in the City of Los Angeles, but the incident did not have to occur within the city boundaries. For instance, a
gang member from Los Angeles could travel to San Diego and become involved in a violent altercation with a gang local
to the San Diego region. Moreover, only one person involved in the incident had to have a Los Angeles affiliation, other
participants (accomplices and victims) were not required to be, and as such, the gang violence represented by this sample
was observed to spill out from the City of Los Angeles into proximate and distal locations. In addition, due to economic and
social conditions affecting housing availability and regional migration patterns associated with the 2008 economic crisis,
many LA-based gang members relocated from the city to suburban locations, such as Lancaster. Thus, regional migration
patterns may also contribute to the observed spread of incident locations.
Censuring resulted from two factors: (1) left-censoring corresponds with the origin of the development of digital case
retrieval systems, i.e., LexisNexis; and right-censoring corresponds to court processing timeframes.
46
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
As discussed shortly, this distribution better mirrors the trend in CGI enactments, and only results in a
3% loss of cases.
Applying final data cleaning protocol, we arrive at the sample used in this analysis. The final
sample is drawn from 963 cases and includes 318 groups with 3710 arcs (representing 625 unique
conflict dyads). The loss of 4.6% of arcs (179 offender/victim dyads) is the byproduct of missing case
details—23 ties were lost due to missing information about the year when the crime occurred and
the rest were lost due to missing gang affiliation (e.g., a victim or offender was described as a gang
member but the gang was not named). Despite finding a high level of connectivity—96.6% of groups
are linked in one large connected structure—the conflict network exhibits low cohesion. Of all the
possible conflict combinations, 3.4% of the groups were connected by at least one act of violence.
3.2. Analytic Framework
To investigate the cumulative impact of CGIs across 16 years, we used four observation
periods—development (15% of CGIs filed from 1998–2001), assent (40% filed 2002–2005), maturity
(35% filed 2006–2009), and saturation (10% filed 2010–2013). CGIs are inherently a prosecutorial crime
control mechanism aimed at addressing chronic community crime problems, thus, exploring the
change in cases generated is an appropriate analytic framework. We considered the social-legal context
of the adoption curve of what was at the time, an innovative crime control strategy, when developing
observation periods. The development period constitutes a baseline under the leadership of Los
Angeles City Attorney James K Hahn, during which this wave of CGIs began. This period includes
two years prior to the filing of the first CGI in order to capture the violent events that generated the
political and community impetus leading to the use of this gang control strategy. The next two periods
encapsulate growing use of this innovation, split between assent and maturity periods, both of which
span City Attorney Rockard J. Delgadillo’s term in office. The final observation captures the saturation
phase in the adoption curve of CGI implementation in Los Angeles; during this period, Carmen A.
Trutanich was the City Attorney of Los Angeles.
Since network structures are based on relational data, our analytic approach includes
two procedures, each designed to account for interdependence between observed relationships
(Krackhardt and Stern 1988). First, we use a triad census to catalogue the different classes of simple
and complex structures found in each phase of CGI adoption. Triad counts have long served as a
foundation upon which to generate theories about relational patterns, when studying associations
among sets of three people (See: Wasserman and Faust 1994). With few prior studies investigating in
detail, there is little evidence upon which to select specific local patterns of street gang violence that
may give rise to the overall network structure observed during each phase of CGI adoption (See for
example: Lewis and Papachristos 2020). If the overall complexity of conflict changes, as identified by
the triad counts, we can dissect the nature of change with stochastic actor-oriented models (SOAMs).
SOAMs are part of a class of longitudinal statistical modeling techniques (part of the exponential
family of random graph models, or ERGMs) used to test hypotheses about factors thought to be
conducive to change or evolution in the network. Several theoretical assumptions underly these
kinds of models, e.g., patterns reflect structural processes, and networks are dynamic and react
to multiple, simultaneous processes (Robins and Dean 2013, p. 10). Focused on the decisions of actors,
SOAMs assume that actors control their outgoing ties, making changes to meet their needs and
circumstances. These changes advance actor objectives. For instance, with regard to competitive
dominance, efforts to restore a gang’s reputation may lead a gang to attack the group who previously
victimized them (reciprocity) or to attack a group already victimized by other gangs (indegree
popularity). SOAMs differ from other ERGMs in that they do not seek to explain the emergent network
resulting from local connectivity, instead, the intent is to identify which factors explain changing
network structure across successive periods. Thus, if our triad census uncovers a shift in structural
complexity, these models can help dissect how the network evolved across successive phases of
CGI implementation.
47
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Using a method of moments maximum likelihood estimation process, these models run a
multi-variate logistic regression to explain change in ties (formation or dissolution). Applied to gang
violence, a tie forms when a new conflict occurs among pairs of gangs at T + 1, or T2 , and dissolves
when a prior attack (occurring in T1 ) is not repeated in T2 . In essence, this means that we can look at the
relative impact of different change elements and interaction effects (e.g., the imposition of an injunction
while controlling for the tendency of highly violent gangs to attack more over time), and we can do this
while modeling cumulative effects of multiple CGIs. We generated parameter estimates with an initial
value of gain set at 0.2, with deviation values calculated from 1000 iterations. Estimates are stable
if convergence occurs and t-ratios are near a value of 0.1: our final models achieved this threshold.
For an explanation of this application, see (Snijders 2011; Snijders et al. 2010; Ripley et al. 2020).
3.3. Network Descriptions
The conflict network observed for each period of CGI implementation varied in size and cohesion
(see Table 1) and there was a substantial drop in the percent of groups embroiled in internal conflict
during the maturity phase. Networks were characterized as having a low level of interconnectivity
(measured with density), meaning that the webs of conflict were sparse, and over time, there was a
slight decrease.4 Groups were also generally characterized as being situated in star-like networks:
this means that a gang may attack two other gangs, but those victimized gangs were not observed
to fight each other. Clustering coefficients confirm this attack pattern. Theoretically, the average
clustering coefficient ranges from 0, suggesting that the pattern of conflict ties linked to each gang
looks more like a star centered on the focal gang, to a 1, where there would be a thickly connected mass
of fighting.5 As reported in Table 1, the average clustering coefficients ranged between a 0.08 and 0.14.
This means that on average, gangs were not embroiled in tight dense clusters of fighting. [Note:
following established protocol, the statistics reported that describe overall network structure were
calculated on dichotomized networks. Ties in a dichotomized network are binary, meaning they are
scored a value of “1” if any conflict occurred between the pair and “0” if there was no observed conflict.]
Networked violence evolved with each phase of CGI use. Looking at the network structure
over time, the Jaccard Coefficient of similarity finds that between development and the assent phase
only 12% of the conflict relations involve the same pattern of violence, meaning that for 12% of conflicts,
the same aggressor and victim links exist.6 Between assent and maturity, we found the most similarity
in overall network connectivity, 16% of unique ties involved the same pair of groups in a consistent
role (aggressor or victim). The least similarity was found between the maturity and saturation phases.
Said another way, we can interpret these values to suggest that conflict patterns changed over time.
The Pearson correlation coefficient tells us that while the tie structure changed, the value associated with
ties (as used here this score reflects the number of aggressions) was somewhat consistent (the Pearson
4
5
6
Density is a measure of cohesion that calibrates how interconnected actors are within a network (Wasserman and Faust 1994, p. 101).
As used here, this metric tabulates the number conflicts observed among gangs in the network, relative to the number of
potential conflict relations that could exist if every gang was in combat with every other gang. High scores indicate that
gangs are well connected.
The average clustering coefficient is a measure of cohesion that is based on how many triplets (grouping of three actors)
are present in a network (Watts 1999, p. 498). As used here, this measure calculates the number of threesomes (triplets)
that are observed (sets of three gangs that are all in conflict with each other), relative to the all triplets that are possible (all
permutations of sets of three nodes) that could exist within the network. Lower scores highlight that potentially important
sub-groups exist within the network.
The Jaccard coefficient of similarity is a measure of association, based on how many shared ties are present between
actors when different observations of the network are compared. Networks must be binary and include the same actors
(Hanneman and Riddle 2005). As used here, this statistic measures the number of conflicts among gangs that are present
when observed at time 1 compared to a subsequent observation at time 2. The resulting score is the percentage of ties that
are the same in two observations of the network.
48
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
was moderately strong).7 Conflict relations with a lot of aggressions in one time period tend to also
exhibit a lot of aggressions in the subsequent time period.
Table 1. Network Description by Phase.
Variables
Network Size
Groups
Aggression (unique attack
arcs/total aggressions)
Internal conflicts (percent of
unique conflicts)
Cohesion
Number of components
(connected structures)
Percent of groups in the
largest component
Density
Average clustering coefficient
Structural Similarity
Jaccard coefficient of similarity
(with prior period)
Pearson correlation coefficient
(with prior period)
Development
(1998–2001)
Assent
(2002–2005)
Maturity
(2006–2009)
Saturation
(2010–2013)
113
173
197
124
152/599
247/1315
264/1242
145/554
16 (10.5%)
28 (11.3%)
22 (8.0%)
15 (10.3%)
10
8
15
9
78.8%
90.2%
82.7%
83.9%
4.4%
0.11
4.0%
0.12
3.0%
0.14
3.4%
0.08
–
12.0%
16.1%
10.1%
–
0.400
0.393
0.427
4. Results
4.1. General Structure of Violence—Simple vs. Complex
Exploring the structure of conflict through a triad census, we investigated the level of complexity
interweaving groups that were involved in violence. Selecting specific patterns of conflict and
tallying the number observed for each configuration provides an opportunity to calculate a ratio;
where simple structures dominate, violence suppression efforts could independently target select
aggressors, and where complex patterns emerge, actions require a coordinated approach focused on a
set of interlinked combatants. While it is conventional to count many lower order simple structures,
a shift in the ratio between types of structures over time can reveal important changes in the topography
of conflict.
Across periods, we found a substantial amount of simple structures reflecting a domino pattern of
aggression where one group attacked another, who in turn attacked a third group (see the percentages
reported in Table 2). This pattern has been interpreted to suggest that groups are not of equal
status or resources, and thus, groups are unable to retaliate for attacks. Instead they prey upon
groups perceived as weaker than themselves (e.g., Papachristos (2009)). Of course, without detailed
information about the specific groups involved, this interpretation is subjective. We also observed a
relatively high level of multi-target attack behavior where one gang victimizes two other groups.
7
The Pearson correlation coefficient is a measure of association, like Jaccard; however, networks must be valued
(Hanneman and Riddle 2005). This statistic calibrates the level of similarity of tie values, in this case, number of conflicts
among pairs of gangs across two observations.
49
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Table 2. Triad Census by Observation Period.
STRUCTURE
Retaliation 2
SIMPLE 1
Domino
Multiple targets
COMPLEX 3
3-way integrated conflict
RATIO OF SIMPLE
TO COMPLEX
DEVELOPMENT
(1998–2001)
ASSENT
(2002–2005)
MATURITY
(2006–2009)
SATURATION
(2010–2013)
661
(14 ties; 9.6%)
79
(54.5%)
52
(35.8%)
1262
(16 ties; 5.8%)
121
(43.8%)
139
(50.3%)
1271
(16 ties; 5.5%)
162
(55.9%)
112
(38.6%)
142
(4 ties; 4.3%)
47
(50.5%)
42
(45.2%)
16
33
44
12
50:1
46:1
35:1
19:1
1
Percentage distributions for simple structures are based on patterns of retaliatory conflict rather than permutations.
For instance, the denominator in the development phase was 145 (14 reciprocal arcs, 79 domino patterns, and 52
multi-target attacks). 2 Retaliation sets counted in a triad census include situations where actors A and B have a
mutual conflict, but no one attacks C. Internal conflict is ignored in this calculation. Since every permutation is
counted, the reciprocity scores do not reflect the true count of reciprocated violence. Investigating actual situations
where violence is reciprocated and is not linked to internal conflict, we count the following: 14 reciprocated ties
during the start-up period), 16 reciprocal ties in the building period, 16 reciprocal ties in the peak period, and 4
reciprocal ties in the decline period. 3 Complex ties include seven configurations: triad sets 9–10 and 12–16 as
listed by UCInet. Specifically, this includes A->B<-C, A->C; A<-B<-C, A->C; A<-B->C, A<->C; A->B<-C, A<->C;
A->B->C, A<->C; A->B<->C, A<->C; and A<->B<->C, A<->C.
A prominent result of this inquiry was the dramatic change in the ratio between simple and
complex structures. While the developmental period, when civil gang injunctions were first introduced,
exhibited many simple structures (50:1), the violence network observed during the assent period
exhibited a major structural change. As more gangs faced injunctions, the complexity of conflict
patterns changed as indicated by the ratio. In the final two observation periods we found ratios
decline precipitously. This suggests that gang violence in general became more integrated. The direct
implication is that as the CGI strategy took effect, new or additional coordinated actions were needed
to quell the conflict among sets of gangs.
A community level analysis offers insight into macro-level changes, but does not reveal if there
were differential effects on enjoined gangs compared to non-enjoined gangs? Table 3 reports on the
patterns of conflict observed for enjoined gangs compared to focal alters with no injunction. All groups
with egocentric networks containing at least two alters were selected for this analysis. Then, a triad
census was conducted for each phase. Since some gangs did not have sufficiently large egonets
for each phase, the sample size varies. Overall, simple structures were more prevalent irrespective
of injunction status. We found low levels of direct retaliation and a higher proportion of domino
patterns (directed chains), with one notable exception. During the assent phase (2002–2005), when CGIs
were being used more frequently, enjoined gangs were observed to shift to attacking multiple targets
(out-star patterns). Of note, the ratio of simple to complex structures declined a little for enjoined
gangs until the final observation, suggesting that there was a small increase in complex interactions as
more groups were sanctioned. The pattern was different for non-enjoined groups, although, by the
final phase there was no appreciable difference in ratios.
Table 3. Triad Census Comparing Egonet Structure of Enjoined Gangs to Focal Alter Gangs 1 .
SAMPLE
74 ENJOINED
GANGS 2
STRUCTURE
DEVELOPMENT
(1998–2001)
ASSENT
(2002–2005)
MATURITY
(2006–2009)
SATURATION
(2010–2013)
SIMPLE
Retaliation 4
Domino
Multiple targets
99
9 (9%)
57 (58%)
33 (33%)
218
12 (5%)
97 (45%)
109 (50%)
183
12 (7%)
101 (55%)
70 (38%)
56
3 (5%)
28 (50%)
25 (45%)
COMPLEX
(3-way integrated conflict)
23
51
61
16
RATIO
4:1
4:1
3:1
4:1
AVG. RATIO 5
5:1
(n = 43)
4:1
(n = 60)
3:1
(n = 56)
4:1
(n = 41)
50
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Table 3. Cont.
SAMPLE
74 FOCAL
ALTERS 3
DEVELOPMENT
(1998–2001)
STRUCTURE
ASSENT
(2002–2005)
MATURITY
(2006–2009)
SATURATION
(2010–2013)
SIMPLE
Retaliation 4
Domino
Multiple targets
44
2 (5%)
21 (48%)
21 (48%)
52
3 (6%)
25 (48%)
24 (46%)
95
7 (7%)
54 (57%)
34 (36%)
32
1 (3%)
19 (59%)
12 (38%)
COMPLEX
(3-way integrated conflict)
9
18
26
9
RATIO
5:1
3:1
4:1
4:1
AVG. RATIO 5
6:1
(n = 41)
3:1
(n = 55)
4:1
(n = 52)
4:1
(n = 43)
1
Values reported sum the number of structures observed for all egos. 2 Cliques named in injunctions are omitted
from this analysis. 3 To be included in this analysis, we selected all alters from the main file (consolidating cases
from 1998 to 2013) with egonetworks with a size of 2 or greater. 4 Egocentric networks will only be observed to
exhibit retaliations as counted in a triad census as A<->B, C if reciprocal ties exist among alters. For this reason,
we counted among alters and reciprocal conflict involving the ego manually. 5 The n varies because some groups
did not have sufficiently large egonetworks in each phase. To account for this variation, an average ratio was
calculated—the average ratio looks at the average number of simple patterns per group compared to the average
number of complex patterns.
4.2. Shifting Patterns of Violence
Table 4 reports several SOAMs disentangling how patterns of violence changed across phases of
CGI implementation. Several notable patterns are found. First, gangs may have a long memory as new
attacks are more likely to involve reciprocated violence. (Recall that each observation captures 4 years
of conflict: this means that a gang member’s murder in T1 could be reciprocated with a murderous
attack on the aggressor more than four years later). The baseline model also shows that tie changes are
not likely to form transitive triplets (significant negative effect for transitive triplets), except among
gangs with CGIs. This means that we observe a tendency among gangs with CGIs to attack in a manner
that generates a transitive triplet with another CGI restricted gang (the effect remains significant across
subsequent models). In other words, gangs with CGIs exhibit a tendency to form three-way conflicts
with other enjoined gangs. Further, although initially important, the probability that a new attack
generates balance (where gangs exhibit a tendency to attack others that they are structurally similar to,
meaning they also attack the same alters) weakens with the introduction of gang attributes. Meaning,
when we control for group characteristics differential social status emerges—some groups have more
competitive advantage. Interestingly, whether a focal gang or its combatant has a CGI does not account
for tie formation or dissolution, instead, popularity is the most significant factor. Gangs suffering a
lot of attacks in an initial observation will suffer more in subsequent observation. Gangs who attack
a lot, are less likely to be attacked in a subsequent observation (outdegree popularity), suggesting that
overt aggression may ward off attack. Notably, while change is significant across all models, the rate of
change from assent (T2 ) to maturity (T3 ) is the greatest.
Table 4. SAOM Investigation of Structural Complexity (* p < 0.05).
Factors
Baseline
β
Structural
Reciprocity
−1.849 *
Trans. triplets
−2.917 *
CGI Trans. triplets
2.212 *
Trans. mediated triplets
Trans. reciprocated triplets
3-cycles
Balance
Betweenness (control)
Transitivity
Dissection
Actor Attributes
Full Model
Parsimony
S.E.
β
S.E.
β
S.E.
β
S.E.
β
S.E.
0.761
0.819
0.872
−0.355
0.445
5.997 *
1.182
3.827 *
0.369
5.540 *
1.836
0.737
−0.94
1.682
1.120
0.488 *
−2.488 *
0.361
1.086
2.684
1.222
0.065
0.310
4.491 *
0.836
2.478 *
1.032
−0.798
2.346
0.214 *
−0.187
0.664
0.618
2.492
1.621
0.099
0.334
4.079 *
0.997
0.118
0.168
51
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Table 4. Cont.
Factors
Baseline
β
S.E.
Actor Attributes
Indegree-popularity
Outdegree-popularity
CGI alter
CGI ego
CGI similarity
Rate of Change
Period 1, T1 to T2
0.881 *
0.054
Period 2, T2 to T3
1.056 *
0.059
Period 3, T3 to T4
0.961 *
0.054
Estimate Performance
T Ratio (model
2 under 0.1
convergence)
Transitivity
Dissection
β
1.128 *
1.534 *
1.291 *
Actor Attributes
S.E.
0.075
0.105
0.089
all under 0.1
Full Model
Parsimony
β
S.E.
β
S.E.
β
S.E.
0.045 *
−4.734 *
−0.229
−0.5036
−0.7984
0.017
1.078
0.480
0.664
0.488
0.025 *
−2.839 *
0.009
0.328
0.045 *
−4.328 *
0.019
1.506
0.952 *
1.168 *
1.036 *
0.0605
0.0718
0.0628
0.945 *
1.183 *
1.036 *
0.058
0.071
0.066
0.955 *
1.176 *
1.043 *
0.061
0.077
0.064
all under 0.1
all under 0.1
all under 0.1
5. Discussion
5.1. Implications
Our results suggest that the structure of gang violence changed across successive observations.
While the implementation of CGIs covaried with the evolving structure of violence overall (global effect),
the impact was smaller when comparing enjoined gangs to alters. Dissecting how patterns of violence
changed we found that CGI gangs were more apt to attack other groups under an injunction, and that
excessively aggressive groups (measured with outdegree popularity) were less likely to be victimized at
a subsequent observation. These findings provide some support for the idea that targeted enforcement
strategies can facilitate change in gang violence—we found that over time, as more injunctions
were filed, the nature of gang conflict became more complex.
Integrating social network theory with crime opportunity theory, (Bichler 2019) argues that crime
opportunity flows through a network. It is an individual’s contacts and interactions with others
that exposes them to crime. If we consider Papachristos’ (Papachristos 2009, p. 75) conclusion to
be valid, that gang members “kill because they live in a structured set of social relations in which
violence works its way through a series of connected individuals”, then it can be argued that variable
criminal behavior, such as the use of violence, can be explained by differential positioning within
the network. Aggregating to the group level, this means that the topography of social relations may
explain intergroup violence, with some groups being “better” positioned to become embroiled in
conflict with other groups. Taken further, changing the social landscape should alter the opportunities
to fight, which should affect the level of violence observed. Applying this argument to the present
study, CGIs were intended to change how gang members interact in public settings. More specifically,
the stipulations included in most CGIs have the potential to reduce the visibility of enjoined gangs
(prohibitions against congregating in public) which should decrease their exposure to gang-on-gang
and gang-on-community interactions. As a result, violence should decline. However, this was
not found.
What the architects of the original CGIs failed to appreciate was just how important inter-gang
conflict is in shaping conflict networks. If opportunity has a network component, then changing
the behavior, and thus, social position of one group, will trigger a ripple effect through the network,
affecting other actors. To implement opportunity reducing strategies, the social network must be
considered as actors do not function in isolation. For instance, exploring the social processes associated
with risk of victimization, Green et al. (2017) show that gun violence spreads through a process of
social contagion (63% of 11,123 episodes occurring in Chicago, 2006 to 2014), transmitted through social
interactions, with alters being victimized on average 125 days after the victimization of their infector.
Investigating how local patterns shape violence at the network level, Lewis and Papachristos (2020)
show that complex transitive local patterns, actor characteristics, and group attributes (dominant
actors) shape violence networks. Contributing to this line of inquiry, our results suggest that continued
52
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
investigations of emerging and changing structure are needed, particularly those drawing from
different information sources. Comparing self-report and community observations with police records,
arrests, cases prosecuted, and convictions (and appeals), helps to uncover how criminal justice filtration
processes and social interactions (intimidation of witnesses) influence the nature of networks generated.
5.2. Reducing Gang Violence
Apart from (Bichler et al. [2017] 2019), the structure of conflict pre- and post-injunction has not
previously been investigated for a community of actors. The limitation of (Bichler et al. [2017] 2019)
is their focus on only Bloods and Crips. In the present study we sought to add to the literature
by extending the boundaries of the community. Though principally limited to capturing Hispanic
and African American street gangs operating in the City of Los Angeles, this study enriches our
understanding of the structure of intergroup conflict. Moving forward, subsequent research should
consider how gang attributes contribute to shaping the social landscape of gang relations. To bolster
the effect of focused deterrent strategies like CGIs, we need to incorporate control variables and
other rival causal factors to better account for shifting structure and the imbalance between groups
that may reflect positions of competitive dominance. Reviewing recent findings, three explanatory
variables are beginning to emerge: (1) group dynamics as reflected in membership or size of territory
controlled (Brantingham et al. 2019), internal cohesion (Ouellet et al. 2019), and race/ethnic homophily
(e.g., Gravel et al. 2018; Papachristos et al. 2013); (2) intersecting aspects of geographic and social connectivity
as evident in the spatial distribution of gang violence (Tita and Radil 2011); and (3) internet banging
that generates links between web-based provocations (posts that advance gang objectives, promote
reputation, and disrespect other gangs) and physical violence (e.g., Décary-Hétu and Morselli 2011;
Dmello and Bichler 2020; Moule et al. 2014). By understanding the explanatory power of these factors,
future research can continue to improve targeted crime control strategies.
5.3. Limitations
We acknowledge several potential limitations to this study. First, we must consider the data
source—this study drew from prosecuted cases generating appeals. Appeal cases typically involve the
most serious and violent incidents, which does not capture the full range of gang violence—recall that
77% of the cases investigated in this study involve murder or attempted murder. The LAPD reported
that 3390 gang-related homicides occurred during the study period, and that approximately 51%
were cleared with arrest, and not all cases went to trial (Los Angeles Police Department 2017, 2020;
Snibbe 2018). Comparing study cases to reported clearance rates, we estimate that the sample includes
at least 34% of cleared gang homicides. Though limited in scope, the types of incidents captured in
these cases are the forms of violence CGIs are meant to deter. Understanding the structure emerging
from these cases provides a glimpse into how CGIs are impacting behaviors stemming from the
most serious forms of gang violence. As CGIs are rooted in problem-based prosecutorial strategies,
compiling information from 198 case studies is a reasonable effort to generate direction for continued
exploration and development of court-based crime control strategies.
In addition, this study offers a point of comparison to Lewis and Papachristos (2020) who used
violence known to police—incidents known to police constitute a measure of crime situated at the
opposite end of the criminal justice information continuum to what we investigated. Comparing our
results to their study raises questions about which kinds of incidents filter out as cases move through
the system. For instance, are direct acts of retaliation less likely to result in a successful prosecution?
Further, to what extent does victim or witness cooperation impact case movement through the system?
To date, network science has yet to explore how criminal procedures and case characteristics filter cases,
affecting the nature of relations identified at the dyadic level, as well as the network structures that
emerge when conflict is mapped as a social network. The insight gained from such investigation could
inform prosecutorial efforts to enhance social justice.
53
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Second, gang identities were not always well documented in the data, thereby generating a
coding issue. For example, individuals may have been listed as gang members without identifying the
specific gang they belonged to. Further, naming conventions were not consistent across cases.
For instance, within the cases being coded as involving members of the 83 Gangster Crips,
gang affiliations were identified at trial by different names—Eight Tray Crips, Westside Eight Tray,
and 8 Tray Gangsters. This inconstancy in naming made it harder to identify which gang defendants
and victims belonged to. In addition, while individual association with the larger parent gang may
have been recorded, clique or subset information was missing. Large gangs are known to have
identifiable subgroups. These subgroups include people who co-offend together. Since some gangs
are reported to have upwards of a thousand members, understanding violent interactions involving
subgroups may result in more effective counter measures. The extensive, labor-intensive cleaning
protocol developed to deal with these issues lead us to strongly suggest that a greater effort should be
made to be consistent when describing gangs and gang associations during investigations and trials.
Meanwhile, these issues with naming conventions afflict all gang research, and thus, our results are
comparable to the current literature.
Finally, the directionality of conflict may be arbitrary in some cases. In cases where the victim is
an innocent bystander, directionality is clear (there is a clear victim and aggressor). However, when
gangs are being equally aggressive, directionality is not as straightforward. For example, in cases
where you cannot determine who the aggressor in the situation is, the survivor of a conflict is often
associated with being the defendant while and individual who is fatally wounded is associated with
being the victim. Yet, this designation does not necessarily capture the true nature of the conflict.
Subsequent analysis should consider non-directed intergang violence. By reconfiguring how relational
information is used to generate the conflict networks, we can conduct sensitivity analysis to test the
robustness of findings given described data limitations.
6. Conclusions
The fatal consequences of street gang violence extend beyond the identified combatants, spreading
into the fabric of a community by involving individuals with no known gang association. Adopting a
social network approach to this investigation, we describe the long-term effects that a dedicated
CGI program has on the structure of gang conflict originating from the City of Los Angeles.
While the prolonged use of CGIs by different city attorneys is associated with some pronounced,
albeit potentially short-term, reductions in crime, our findings suggest that while crime at the
community level may decline, the structure of conflict thickens, becoming more complex and embedded,
though more so for some gangs than others. Moreover, CGI implementation patterns have cumulative
effects. Continued effort is needed to develop strategies that will disentangle the web of violence that
continues to plague communities.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, G.B. and A.N.; methodology, G.B.; formal analysis, G.B.; data curation,
G.B. and C.I.; writing—original draft preparation, G.B., C.I., and A.N.; writing—review and editing, G.B. and A.N.;
visualization, G.B.; project administration, G.B. and A.N.; funding acquisition, G.B. and A.N. All authors have
read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was supported by a grant, no. 2017-JF-FX-0043, awarded by the Office of Juvenile Justice
and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department of Justice, to the California State
University San Bernardino. The opinions, findings, and conclusions and recommendations expressed are those of
the authors only.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
54
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
References
Bichler, Gisela. 2019. Understanding Criminal Networks: A Research Guide. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bichler, Gisela, Alexis Norris, Jared R. Dmello, and Jasmin Randle. 2019. The Impact of Civil Gang Injunctions on
Networked Violence between the Bloods and the Crips. Crime and Delinquency 65: 875–915. First published
2017. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony A., and David L. Weisburd. 2012. The Effects of Focused Deterrence Strategies on Crime:
A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Empirical Evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency 49: 323–58. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony A., Jack McDevitt, and Glenn L. Pierce. 2006. Understanding and preventing gang violence:
Problem analysis and response development in Lowell, Massachusetts. Police Quarterly 9: 20–46. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, P. Jeffrey, Matthew Valasik, and George E. Tita. 2019. Competitive dominance, gang size and the
directionality of gang violence. Crime Science 8: 1–20. [CrossRef]
Carr, Richard, Molly Slothower, and John Parkinson. 2017. Do gang injunctions reduce violent crime? Four tests
in Merseyside, UK. Cambridge Journal of Evidence-Based Policing 1: 195–210. [CrossRef]
Christakis, Nicholas A., and James H. Fowler. 2009. Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social Networks and
How They Shape Our Lives. New York: Little, Brown and Company.
Décary-Hétu, David, and Carlo Morselli. 2011. Gang presence in social network sites. International Journal of Cyber
Criminology 5: 876–90.
Decker, Scott H. 1996. Collective and normative features of gang violence. Justice Quarterly 13: 243–64. [CrossRef]
Descormiers, Karine, and Carlo Morselli. 2011. Alliances, Conflicts, and Contradictions in Montreal’s Street Gang
Landscape. International Criminal Justice Review 21: 297–314. [CrossRef]
Dmello, Jared R., and Gisela Bichler. 2020. Assessing the Impact of Civil Gang Injunctions on the Use of Online
Media by Criminal Street Gangs. International Journal of Cyber Criminology 14: 16–34.
Gravel, Jason, Blake Allison, Jenny West-Fagan, Michael McBride, and George E. Tita. 2018. Birds of a feather fight
together: Status-enhancing violence, social distance and the emergence of homogenous gangs. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology 34: 189–219. [CrossRef]
Green, Ben, Thibaut Horel, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2017. Modeling Contagion Through Social Networks
to Explain and Predict Gunshot Violence in Chicago, 2006 to 2014. JAMA Internal Medicine 177: 326–33.
[CrossRef]
Grogger, Jeffrey. 2002. The effects of civil gang injunctions on reported violent crime: Evidence from Los Angeles
county. Journal of Law and Economics 45: 69–90. [CrossRef]
Hanneman, Robert A., and Mark Riddle. 2005. Introduction to Social Network Methods. Riverside: University of
California. Available online: http://faculty.ucr.edu/~{}hanneman/ (accessed on 13 April 2020).
Hennigan, Karen M., and David Sloane. 2013. How implementation can affect gang dynamics, crime, and violence.
Criminology and Public Policy 12: 7–41. [CrossRef]
Hennigan, Karen, and Marija Spanovic. 2012. Gang dynamics through the lens of social identity theory. In Youth
Gangs in International Perspective. New York: Springer, pp. 127–49.
Klein, Malcolm W., and Cheryl L. Maxson. 2010. Street Gang Patterns and Policies. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Krackhardt, David, and Robert N. Stern. 1988. Informal networks and organizational crises: An experimental
simulation. Social Psychology Quarterly 51: 123–40. [CrossRef]
Los Angeles County Civil Grand Jury. 2004. A management review of civil gang injunctions. In Los Angeles County
Civil Grand Jury. Final Report 2003–2004. pp. 177–389. Available online: http://www.grandjury.co.la.ca.us/
gjury03-04/LACGJFR_03-04.pdf (accessed on 13 April 2020).
Landis, J. Richard, and Gary G. Koch. 1997. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.
Biometrics 33: 159–74. [CrossRef]
Los Angeles Police Department. 2017. Homicide Report 2017. Available online: http://assets.lapdonline.org/assets/
pdf/2017-homi-report-final.pdf (accessed on 13 April 2020).
Los Angeles Police Department. 2020. Gangs. Available online: http://www.lapdonline.org/get_informed/content_
basic_view/1396 (accessed on 13 April 2020).
Lewis, Kevin, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2020. Rules of the Game: Exponential Random Graph Models of a
Gang Homicide Network. Social Forces 98: 1829–58. [CrossRef]
55
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Maxson, Cheryl L., Karen M. Hennigan, and David C. Sloane. 2005. “It’s getting crazy out there”: Can a civil gang
injunction change a community? Criminology and Public Policy 4: 577–606. [CrossRef]
McCuish, Evan C., Martin Bouchard, and Raymond R. Corrado. 2015. The Search for Suitable Homicide
Co-Offenders among Gang Members. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 31: 319–36. [CrossRef]
McGloin, Jean Marie. 2007. The Continued Relevance of Gang Membership. Criminology and Public Policy 6: 231–40.
[CrossRef]
Melde, Chris, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2013. Gangs and violence: Disentangling the impact of gang membership
on the level and nature of offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 29: 143–66. [CrossRef]
Moule, Richard K., Jr., David C. Pyrooz, and Scott H. Decker. 2014. Internet adoption and online behaviour
among American street gangs: Integrating gangs and organizational theory. British Journal of Criminology
54: 1186–206. [CrossRef]
Noonan, Ari L. 2008. Gang-Tied Killer of Bosch Brothers Convicted 4 1/2 Years Later. The Front Page. January 31.
Available online: https://www.thefrontpageonline.com/news/gang-tied-killer-of-bosch-brothers-convicted4-12-years-later (accessed on 23 December 2019).
O’Deane, Matthew D., and Stephen A. Morreale. 2011. Evaluating the effectiveness of gang injunctions in
California. Journal of Criminal Justice Research 2: 1–32.
Ouellet, Marie, Martin Bouchard, and Yanick Charette. 2019. One gang dies, another gains? The network dynamics
of criminal group persistence. Criminology 57: 5–33. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V. 2009. Murder by Structure: Dominance Relations and the Social Structure of Gang
Homicide. American Journal of Sociology 115: 74–128. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V. 2013. The Importance of Cohesion for Gang Research, Policy, and Practice. Criminology
and Public Policy 12: 49–58. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., and David S. Kirk. 2006. Neighborhood effects on street gang behavior. Studying Youth
Gangs 12: 63–84.
Papachristos, Andrew V., David Hureau, and Anthony A. Braga. 2010. Conflict and the Corner: The Impact of
Intergroup Conflict and Geographic Turf on Gang Violence. Available online: https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/
papers.cfm?abstract_id=1722329 (accessed on 10 April 2020).
Papachristos, Andrew V., David M. Hureau, and Anthony A. Braga. 2013. The Corner and the Crew: The Influence
of Geography and Social Networks on Gang Violence. American Sociological Review 78: 1–31. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., Anthony A. Braga, Eric Piza, and Leigh S. Grossman. 2015. The Company You Keep?
The Spillover Effects of Gang Membership on Individual Gunshot Victimization in a Co-Offending Network.
Criminology 53: 624–49. [CrossRef]
Radil, Steven M., Colin Flint, and George E. Tita. 2010. Spatializing social networks: Using social network analysis
to investigate geographies of gang rivalry, territoriality, and violence in Los Angeles. Annals of the Association
of American Geographers 100: 307–26. [CrossRef]
Randle, Jasmin, and Gisela Bichler. 2017. Uncovering the Social Pecking Order in Gang Violence. In Crime
Prevention in the 21st Century. Edited by Benoit Leclerc and Ernesto Savona. New York: Springer, pp. 165–86.
Ridgeway, Greg, Jeffrey Grogger, Ruth A. Moyer, and John M. Macdonald. 2019. Effect of gang injunctions on
crime: A study of Los Angeles from 1988–2014. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 35: 517–41. [CrossRef]
Ripley, Ruth M., Tom A. B. Snijders, Zsófia Boda, András Vörös, and Paulina Preciado. 2020. Manual for RSiena.
Oxford: Department of Statistics and Nuffield College, University of Oxford.
Robins, Garry, and Lusher Dean. 2013. What are exponential random graph models? In Exponential Random
Graph Models for Social Networks. Edited by Dean Lusher, Koskinen Johan and Robins Garry. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, pp. 9–15.
Snibbe, Kurt. 2018. How many homicides go unsolved in California and the nation? Orange County Register.
April 29. Available online: https://www.ocregister.com/2018/04/29/how-many-homicides-go-unsolved-incalifornia-and-the-nation/ (accessed on 13 April 2020).
Snijders, Tom A. B. 2011. Network dynamics. In The SAGE Handbook of Social Network Analysis. Edited by John Scott
and Peter J. Carrington. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 501–13.
Snijders, Tom A. B., Gerhard G. Van de Bunt, and Christian E. G. Steglich. 2010. Introduction to stochastic
actor-based models for network dynamics. Social Networks 32: 44–60. [CrossRef]
Stafford, Mark C., and Mark Warr. 1993. A reconceptualization of general and specific deterrence. Journal of
Research in Crime and Delinquency 30: 123–35. [CrossRef]
56
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 203
Swan, Richelle S., and Bates Kirstin A. 2017. Loosening the ties that bind: The hidden alarms of civil gang
injunctions in San Diego county. Contemporary Justice Review 20: 132–53. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., and Steven M. Radil. 2011. Spatializing the social networks of gangs to explore patterns of violence.
Journal of Quantitative Criminology 27: 521–45. [CrossRef]
Vives, Ruben, and Ben Bolch. 2009. Student at Long Beach’s Wilson High Fatally Shot after Homecoming Game.
Los Angeles Times, November 1. Available online: https://www.latimes.com/local/la-me-wilson-shooting12009nov01-story.html (accessed on 23 December 2019).
Wasserman, Stanley, and Katherine Faust. 1994. Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Watts, Duncan J. 1999. Networks, dynamics, and the small world phenomenon. American Journal of Sociology
105: 493–592. [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional
affiliations.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
57
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Making Sense of Murder: The Reality versus the Realness of
Gang Homicides in Two Contexts
Marta-Marika Urbanik 1, * and Robert A. Roks 2
1
2
*
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Citation: Urbanik, Marta-Marika,
and Robert A. Roks. 2021. Making
Department of Sociology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB T6G 2H4, Canada
Erasmus School of Law, Erasmus University Rotterdam, 3062 PA Rotterdam, The Netherlands;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Abstract: Despite the proliferation of research examining gang violence, little is known about
how gang members experience, make sense of, and respond to peer fatalities. Drawing from
two ethnographies in the Netherlands and Canada, this paper interrogates how gang members
experience their affiliates’ murder in different street milieus. We describe how gang members
in both studies made sense of and navigated their affiliates’ murder(s) by conducting pseudohomicide investigations, being hypervigilant, and attributing blameworthiness to the victim. We
then demonstrate that while the Netherland’s milder street culture amplifies the significance of
homicide, signals the authenticity of gang life, and reaffirms or tests group commitment, frequent
and normalized gun violence in Canada has desensitized gang-involved men to murder, created a
communal and perpetual state of insecurity, and eroded group cohesion. Lastly, we compare the
‘realness’ of gang homicide in The Hague with the ‘reality’ of lethal violence in Toronto, drawing
attention to the importance of the ‘local’ in making sense of murder and contrasting participants’
narratives of interpretation.
Sense of Murder: The Reality versus
the Realness of Gang Homicides in
Two Contexts. Social Sciences 10: 17.
Keywords: gang homicide; comparative research; ethnography; gang violence
https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10010017
Received: 8 December 2020
Accepted: 6 January 2021
Published: 12 January 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
1. Introduction
Scholars have argued that violence is fundamental to gang life (Klein and Maxson 1989),
“so that membership may have a seductively glorious, rather than mundane, indifferent,
significance” (Katz 1988, p. 128). The epitome of gang violence is gang homicide, which
many academics, police officials, and policy markers consider a unique and distinct type
of murder1 (Maxson et al. 1985; Maxson and Klein 1996; Maxson 1999). Despite academic,
political, and public preoccupation with gang homicides, questions remain about how to
define, classify, measure, and study these murders. For example, while some classifications include homicides that are allegedly gang related (where the victim and/or offender
are gang members), others necessitate the homicide be motivated by gang functions (for
the explicit benefit of the group) (Maxson and Klein 1990, 1996). Scholars also disagree
about the necessary motivations for gang homicides, which can be further delineated by
expressive (signaling gang power), instrumental (i.e., protecting gang turf) motifs, or both
(Decker 1996; Decker and Pyrooz 2013). Importantly, data on ‘gangs’ and, by extension, on
‘gang homicides’ are predominantly police generated, and can therefore be riddled with
methodological limitations2 (see Esbensen et al. 2001; Skogan 1974). These distinctions and
data are consequential as they affect how we quantify gang homicides (and therefore fashion
1
Gang homicides are more likely to include multiple offenders, to occur in public settings, to have different spatial characteristics, to involve younger
persons, and to involve strangers than nongang incidents (Curry and Spergel 1988; Decker and Curry 2002; Howell 1999; Maxson et al. 1985;
Pizarro and McGloin 2006; Pyrooz 2012).
2
Criminologists disagree on this. For example, Decker and Pyrooz (2010, p. 370) argue “contrary to many claims, police reports of gang crime are not
fraught with measurement error so as to be unsuitable for meaningful analysis”.
59
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
policies/responses), how well our classifications resemble the realities of gang homicide
(including motivations), and how we understand the micro processes of gang homicide.
Though no robust predictors of gang homicide exist, several factors can propel its occurrence. Locality can be central to inciting and shaping gang violence, given many gangs’
commitments to specific blocks or territories (Aldridge et al. 2011; Brotherton and Barrios 2004;
Spergel 1984). Often, gang turf and territory is located within the neighbourhoods in
which gangs form and operate, with neighbourhoods providing the sole or primary
space for income generation, particularly in relation to drug trafficking (Hagedorn 1994;
Papachristos et al. 2013). Therefore, gang violence, including lethal violence, is often related
to ‘defending’ a neighbourhood, showing ‘love’ for the community, and competition over
territory, status, resources, and drug markets (Brotherton and Barrios 2004; Decker 1996;
Densley 2012; Hagedorn and Macon 1988; Maxson 1999; Rodgers 2002; Vargas 2014). Given
that drug trafficking is a central illegal enterprise for many gangs and drug trafficking
escalates risks of victimization and violent offending, gang homicides are often connected
to the drug trade (Adams and Pizarro 2014; Blumstein 1995; Curtis 2003; Densley 2014,
p. 52). Moreover, the illegality of drug trafficking prevents traffickers from relying upon
prosocial governance institutions to facilitate exchange and provide redress for disputes
(see Skarbek 2011), necessitating that they take matters into their own hands, sometimes
via lethal violence (Adams and Pizarro 2014).
Gang homicide—similar to gang violence more broadly—is not randomly distributed
across physical space, but often ensues in gang “hot spots,” frequently located in/near marginalized communities characterized by concentrated poverty and where formal social control actors
are absent or inadequate (Bursik and Grasmick 1993; Curry and Spergel 1988; Kubrin and
Wadsworth 2003; Mares 2010; Papachristos and Kirk 2006; Pizarro and McGloin 2006;
Rosenfeld et al. 1999). In addition to ecological and neighbourhood conditions, other factors
affect gang violence, including network processes such as a history of conflict and reciprocity of violence. It is therefore unsurprising that gang violence may be more pronounced
in areas where competing gangs share an adjacent turf (Papachristos et al. 2013). Further,
in such areas, victims/witnesses may be unwilling to cooperate with law enforcement due
to distrust in police and strained police–community relationships, an adherence to “street
codes” (Anderson 1999) which privilege street justice over police interventions, and/or due
to intimidation and silencing (Miethe and McCorkle 2002). This can mask the prevalence
and nature of gang violence from neighbourhood ‘outsiders’ (such as police) and can make
preventing, investigating, and prosecuting gang homicides exceptionally difficult. Gang
violence/homicide prosecutions can be further complicated by other elements, including
multiple victims/offenders, challenges in establishing gang membership in court, and
proving the violence was ‘gang motivated’. In response, some jurisdictions in the United
States have created dedicated gang units and specialized prosecutions to aid the capture
and prosecution of those suspected to be involved in gang violence (see Pyrooz et al. 2011;
Miethe and McCorkle 2002).
What is particularly notable about gang homicide is its often cyclical and reciprocal
nature. Much gang conflict is therefore both a consequence of and precursor to gang violence
(Decker 1996). Retaliatory homicides are often connected to and/or direct products of
operative “street codes” which, in addition to discouraging cooperation with law enforcement mandate that ‘disrespect’ is met with violence or it may propel additional victimization3 (Anderson 1999; Kubrin and Weitzer 2003, p. 158). As such, retaliatory violence
can be a response to competition, an effort to incite social control, to seek “street justice”,
and to hamper future victimization (Jacobs and Wright 2006; Kubrin and Weitzer 2003;
Maxson 1999). The extent to which gangs retaliate for affiliate homicides is unclear; one
study found that 37% of homicides in Chicago amongst organized street gangs were reciprocated (Papachristos 2009). However, some scholars posit gang retaliations are not always
3
See (Urbanik et al. 2017) for a discussion of how street codes can also limit violence.
60
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
targeted at the provoking group but may entail “generalized reciprocity,” where the performative
aspect of retaliation is directed against a different gang (Lewis and Papachristos 2020).
Recently, gang scholars have become particularly attuned to examining whether and
how technology affects gang violence and/or homicide. Though Miller (1975) seminal
work on gang violence examined how technological advances such as cars and handguns
may have driven the popularity of drive-by shootings, contemporary research has shifted
its gaze to the expansion of the Internet and social media. Scholars studying social media
and criminally-involved groups have begun to examine the role that social may play in
inciting or repelling gang violence and homicide (see Urbanik et al. 2020 for a review).
2. Current Study
The proliferation of studies on gang violence has illuminated many facets of this
phenomena. However, the bulk of research on gang homicides has examined the emergence
of gang violence and its consequences, including retaliation. Consequently, we know
little about the residual effects of gang homicide, particularly how they affect surviving
gang members and their respective groups. In this paper, we examine how gang murders
affect affiliates, focusing on how they experience, make sense of, and to respond to peer
fatality. By drawing from ethnographic research in two countries—the Netherlands and
Canada—we unmask how street cultures and milieu’s affect experiences of gang homicides
and respond to calls for comparative and multisite gang research (Klein 2005). We first
describe our field sites and the role of gang homicide in our respective studies.4 Second, we
highlight the commonalities in how gang members make sense of peer murder and discuss
the divergent residual effects of these instances. We then center ‘the local’ (Fine 2010) in
documenting how participants’ varying local contexts and lived realities impacted how
they perceived, experienced, and responded to peer fatality.
3. Study A: The Forgotten Village, The Hague (The Netherlands)
Between 2011 and 2013, Roks conducted fieldwork in a small neighbourhood—known
as the Forgotten Village—in the city of The Hague, Netherlands, which served as the
home base of the Dutch Crips since the late 1980s, who refer to it as their “h200d”
(Roks 2017b). Whilst Roks conducted semi-structured interviews, informal conversations,
and ethnographic observation with many local residents and stakeholders, he spent most of
his time with current and former members of the Dutch Crips. At the onset of the research,
the Dutch Rollin 200 Crips consisted of some 50 members (15–40 years old), predominantly
of Surinamese and Antillean background. Local and national media have heavily documented the Dutch Crips, including with a 90 min documentary, titled ‘Strapped ‘N Strong’
(Van der Valk 2009). In media interviews, Crips members have consistently referenced their
familiarity and experiences with violence and murder and their propensity to use violence
when necessary. For example, during one interview with the Dutch magazine Panorama,
Raymond—the gang’s leader—expressed: “If I want you dead tomorrow, then you’ll be dead
tomorrow. If I want you to die in one year, you’ll be dead in a year” (Viering 1994, p. 41).
Although violence was central to the Dutch Crips’ presentation of self in the media,
actual levels of (gang) violence in the Netherlands are low, consistent with many other
European nations. An analysis of gangs in different countries by Klein et al. (2006, p. 41)
indicates that both the patterns of violent behaviour and the levels of violence of European
gangs are less serious than in the United States. Klein et al. (2006) attribute these differences
to the nature of gangs in Europe, the lower prevalence of firearms, and lesser levels of gang
territoriality. In the Dutch context, Ganpat and Liem (2012, p. 329) show that between
1992 and 2009, 223 persons were murdered annually on average, usually precipitated by
arguments and domestic disputes. ‘Criminal’ homicides comprised 12% of all cases, with
incidents varying from “drug addicts killing one another, drug users who killed dealers,
and dealers who killed one another during a bad deal” (Ganpat and Liem 2012, p. 333),
4
For an extensive overview of both ethnographies, see (Urbanik and Roks 2020).
61
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
though none were classified as gang related. The Netherlands’ homicide rate has been
declining since 2009, with 119 murders in 2018 (CBS 2020), 34 of which were due to gun
violence, and an addition 577 incidents involved firearms (RTL Nieuws 2020).
During Roks’s fieldwork, one of his participants was murdered. In the late hours
of Sunday, August 19th, 2012, Quincy “Sin” Soetosenojo5 was shot several times at close
range in his hometown of Amsterdam. He succumbed to his injuries and passed away in
the hospital later that night. The murder remains unsolved, making it impossible to assess
whether this incident could be classified as a gang homicide. Sin’s murder was one of 157
murders in 2012, translating into a homicide rate of 0.94 per 100,000 inhabitants (CBS 2017).
4. Study B: Regent Park, Toronto (Canada)
Urbanik’s research is situated in a neighbourhood just east of Toronto’s downtown
core, Regent Park. Until its ongoing revitalization, Regent Park was Canada’s oldest and
largest social housing project, and the neighbourhood amassed a notorious reputation
as a space of concentrated, racialized poverty and violence. Between 2013 and 2018,
Urbanik conducted ethnographic observation and interviews in the neighbourhood. She
spent most of her time “deep hanging out” (Geertz 1998) with approximately 25 ganginvolved men, who loosely belonged to two neighbourhood gangs, The Rich Riderz and
The Young Soldiers (see Urbanik 2018).6 Despite engaging in several aspects of “gang life”
including repping, drug trafficking, turf wars, robberies, gun violence, and gang homicides,
these groups were more fluid and loosely organized than larger and more traditional
gangs, and had fewer expectations about group commitments. Gun and gang violence
are an unfortunate lived reality for many Regent Parkers, and for Urbanik’s participants
in particular, all of whom reported losing friends, affiliates and family members to gun
violence. During the study, many of Urbanik’s participants were shot (at) and several
were killed.
Similar to the Netherlands, Canada’s homicide rate pales in comparison to that of the
United States, in part due to stricter gun control. In 2018, Canada reported 651 homicides,
and a homicide rate of 1.76 per 100,000 inhabitants. Approximately one-quarter of homicides
were classified as “gang related,” 83% of which involved a firearm (Statistics Canada 2019).
Toronto—Canada’s most populous census metropolitan area (CMA)—reported the most
homicides of all CMAs, with 142 victims, a 53% increase7 from the year prior, and a
record number since data collection began (Statistics Canada 2019). Thirty-six of these
homicides were classified as “gang related”. Gun violence is also a growing concern, with
many Torontonians—including the former Police Chief—blaming gangs for the shootings
(Global News 2019) and news media characterizing the City’s gun violence as “civil war”
(Warmington 2020). Much of this violence is related to inter-neighbourhood ‘beefs’, which
are particularly common amongst Toronto’s social housing projects (see also Berardi 2018).
5. Making Sense of Murder
Despite notable differences in street and gang milieus and the frequency of gang
homicide across our field sites, our findings unmask several commonalities in how gang
members experienced and responded to peer fatality. We first describe how gang members
in both studies tried to make sense of and navigate their affiliates’ murder by conducting
pseudo-investigations, being hypervigilant, and attributing blameworthiness to the victim.
“What happened, and who did it?”: Pseudo-Investigations
Three days after Sin was shot and killed, Roks met with several Crips members who
spent most of the evening discussing and debating the circumstances surrounding his
murder. The men’s occupation with discussing Sin’s potential killers and their motives
5
Except for Quincy “Sin” Soetosenojo, all names are pseudonyms. Some details have been altered to protect participants’ identities.
6
Upon comparing field experiences with Roks, Urbanik returned to the field and conducted “problem-centered interviews” (Witzel 2000), specifically
focused on how participants experienced and navigated peer fatality.
7
Though 2018 was an unusual year given high-causality events, there was still an increase.
62
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
superseded traditional mourning rituals. Roks was struck by how meticulously the men
conducted their pseudo-investigation. Mirroring the methods used by law enforcement
officials, they spoke to Sin’s friends and acquaintances about whom he had spent time with
recently and whether he had any ongoing beefs or problems that they may be unaware
of. As both a member of the Crips’ chapter in Amsterdam and The Hague, and because
he recently joined a Dutch outlaw motorcycle gang, Sin moved in different circles. This
prompted the Crips to amass a long list of possible suspects and motives, deploying even
the slightest sliver of information to construct suspicions and allegations.
The Crips also collected and fastidiously reviewed eyewitness reports and crime scene
photos published by local media to reconstruct Sin’s murder and hopefully identify the
culprit(s). For example, they drew upon the arrangement of parked cars and the proximity
to Sin’s home to the crime street to determine that Sin likely knew his killer. The blood
stains in the crime scene photos suggested that Sin was walking away from his vehicle
when he was ambushed, reaffirming their hypothesis that an acquaintance must have
called him over. They also deliberated about eyewitness’ media descriptions that the
gunfire sounded like ‘rattling’ to deduce the murder weapon. Crips’ founder Raymond
was particularly fixated on this, asserting that identifying the gun and bullets could narrow
the suspect list and allow them to gauge if other Crips were in danger.
In an environment such as Regent Park, news about homicides and information about
suspected shooter(s) and potential motives travels with exceptional velocity. Similar to
StudyA, the men dedicated the immediate aftermath after someone was shot (fatally or otherwise) to trying to determine what happened and most importantly, who was responsible.
To illustrate, one summer afternoon in 2015, several of Urbanik’s participants were playing
cards on the boardwalk when they were ambushed by a drive-by shooter. Approximately
30 min later,8 Urbanik arrived at the scene to find police had taped off the area, and some of
her other participants9 and other local residents were already recounting what happened,
exchanging intel on the shooter and driver’s physical description, analyzing the car’s route,
and listing recent neighbourhood ‘beefs’ to determine possible motives, to determine the
shooter’s identity.10 Once they were ‘certain’ who the shooter was and his motive (less
than an hour later)11 , they called and text others to warn them and to elicit information on
his whereabouts.
The speed with which the men in Regent Park conduct pseudo-investigations postshootings and homicides and try to determine the possible culprit(s) even surprised them,
as Rehan highlighted: “Pretty quick! So quick, it’s crazy- though. Like, even I thought about it
a couple times. Like, how did you get this information-that’s going down? . . . Like, the same day.
Like, sometimes before it hits the news, you know?” One afternoon in 2018, a few weeks after
a prominent neighbourhood rapper and his affiliate were shot and killed, Matteo—who
has lost 12 close friends to gun violence—shared his internal monologue upon learning
another loved one was murdered: “Makes you think like, what the fuck? What happened? What
did he do? And where the fuck was he going and where did he end up?” Overhearing this, Ezekiel
chimed in: “And if it’s somebody close, the thought that comes through your head is "Fuck, I could
have been with that nigga. That could have been me!” When an affiliate was killed, Urbanik’s
participants’ proximity to the deceased (re)sensitized them to the fact that they may be
next. Consequently, meticulously gathering homicide details and any related knowledge is
“not for them to solve the crime or anything”—as Asad asserted, but is motivated by a need
to uncover what happened which can shape their response (and specifically, retaliation),
and can aid in deflecting subsequent victimization. Determining a peer’s last moments
and more importantly “who you was running with?” is an important survival tactic that can
8
Urbanik was in a local community center when the shooting occurred.
9
Those targeted immediately left the area to stay safe and avoid police interaction.
10
In instances where no eye-witnesses were available, Urbanik’s participants had to gather information through other sources, including: social media
posts, news media accounts, and details provided by friends and family.
11
While this strategy can result in fatal misunderstandings and errors, it is nevertheless a critical component of neighbourhood life post-shooting.
63
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
help protect members. However, since Urbanik’s participants, similar to the Dutch Crips,
associated with different groups, identifying the culprit could be challenging.
“Before you know it, you are shaking hands with his killer”: Hypervigilance
In the days following Sin’s murder, suspicion and distrust–signs of fear according to
van de Port (2001, pp. 109–19)–dominated Crips’ conversations. Concerns about whether
the killer walks among them even made it to the media; Raymond remarked to a journalist
who attended Sin’s wake: “Everyone is offering their condolences. Before you know it, you
are shaking hands with his killer” (Van Stapele 2012). When Roks spoke to Dre, a younger
Crips member, Dre was upset with his comrades for drinking alcohol and criticized their
alleged lack of vigilance during such a critical time. Though Dre noted that several officers
attended the wake in hopes of gathering intelligence, he nevertheless affirmed his own
commitment to protection: “But I had the heat on my balls!”, indicating he carried a gun to
the wake.
During this time, Roks’s participants were in a “hidden state of emergency” (Green 1994,
p. 228). Although many members prided themselves on being ‘strapped 24/7’, they appeared to increase their armament following the murder. Every night that following week,
they hid a weapon (e.g., baseball bat, firearm) nearby, something they never did during the
previous 20 months of fieldwork. It also seemed like the younger members of the Crips
were more vigilant in the h200d, paying extra attention to unknown others. They watched
passers-by closely, followed them for several blocks, and sometimes even demanded they
remove their hands from their pockets when walking through the neighbourhood.
One week after Sin’s death, Raymond summoned almost all the Crips to meet him
in the h200d before they travelled together to a nearby forest. Prior to leaving, Raymond
demanded they all turn off their cellphones and remove the battery to prevent police
monitoring. Once in the forest, the men gathered in a semi-circle with Raymond at the
center. He started the meeting with “a moment of silence for the dead homie” and following
the reflection, asked the others how they felt. When no one responded, he reiterated the
question, which implored the others to respond with: “Angry”, “Fucked up”, and “It’s dark
over here, cuzz”. Quincy, a younger member and Sin’s close friend, expressed his rage and
desire to retaliate. Raymond sympathized and admitted he also wanted revenge, though
he cautioned that they had to keep their emotions in check until they were certain of the
killer’s identity. Raymond then recounted the information he gathered during that week
and shared his suspicions about who may have more knowledge and who may have been
responsible.
A few days after the gathering, Sin was to be buried in Amsterdam. Before the funeral,
Raymond asked: “Roks, do you want to come? Just so you know, it is in the middle of enemy
territory”. The day of the funeral, temperatures were projected to reach over 30 ◦ C. When
Roks arrived in the h200d, he met with Marvin, a Crip member since the mid-1990s. After
exchanging a quick hug, Marvin asked Roks whether it was obvious he was carrying a gun
under his clothes. He pondered whether he should wear his coat, worried that donning a
jacket on such a hot day might betray to police or others at the funeral that he was armed.
Roks assured him that his oversized T-shirt concealed the gun well, which pleased Marvin.
This exchange made him more cognizant of how other Crips dressed for the funeral, and
he noted that most wore baggy and oversized clothing, including jackets.
In StudyB, murders initiated an almost identical series of events; news/information
travelled quickly, police saturated the neighbourhood, and Urbanik’s participants usually
retreated indoors to determine what happened, their own risk levels, and whether/how
they should respond. See how Leon described the group’s reactions upon finding out an
affiliate was killed:
“Anger! ‘Let’s go right now! like where’s he [killer] at, who did it?’ . . . Everything is
going through your head, you know? How you lost someone that’s really close to you,
right? . . . There’s too much anger, you want to do anything just, you know? So they
[killers] can know how you feel the pain, right? And you ain’t gonna heal nothing, it’s
just—you get at them [retaliate]”.
64
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
A few days after a young affiliate was shot and killed one summer, Urbanik and another
researcher12 pulled up to the apartment which served as her participants’ home base. The
area was eerily quiet which was unusual given its vivacious drug trade. Urbanik text
Booker—one of her key participants—that they arrived and he came downstairs with
Matteo shortly thereafter. Both men appeared uneasy, looking around frequently, studying
passing cars carefully, and staying close to the building entryway—atypical behaviours
since they were usually relaxed in that area. When Urbanik asked why it was so deserted,
Booker responded: “We’re all laying low at Ricky’s house. I told em you guys are here, they said
they might come down later. Things are hot right now” Urbanik then probed whether it was
related to the shooting, he responded: “Yea. This shit got us fucked up. Just trying to figure out
whose who and the next play [response], you know? I’d invite ya’ll up but trust me- you don’t
wanna be part of this right now”.
Following a peer’s murder, Urbanik’s participants operated in a state of hypervigilance
until they could identify the killer(s) and motive, wary of being outdoors and which group
members they spent time with. Matteo described their trepidation upon learning another
peer was killed:
“You think you’re next. Just cause it’s your community that they’re dropping close by,
right? . . . This shit is happening in your backyard. And to find out you don’t know who
the fuck the killer is? What if I’m chillin’ amongst the killer, and he’s just planning on
the next one? Like, that’s what gets me triggered!”
Concerns that the killer may be a close affiliate with intimate knowledge about the men’s
routines and potentially “planning on the next one” pushed them to withdraw from neighbourhood life and some group members until things settled. Frankie explained why they
rarely ventured outdoors until they had had more information: “I’m out numbered, I’m out
numbered, I can’t come outside . . . you don’t know who’s after who”. Once the men narrowed
down or identified possible culprits (usually within a week) and eliminated their affiliates
from the suspect list, they re-established their neighbourhood presence. However, the
possibility of future ambushes meant they usually only ventured outdoors in large groups
and only if at least one person was armed in the weeks following a homicide.
Whilst Urbanik’s participants considered strength in numbers a safety measure, they
acknowledged that they had to remain hyperaware of their surroundings. Booker described
the need to be exceptionally cautious following a peer’s murder: “You just gotta be on
your P’s and Q’s more. It makes you paranoid a little bit . . . got you looking around more
often, over your shoulders. You never know. Anything can be expected, right?” Being on your
“Ps and Qs” refers to being “on point”, a concept whereby streetwise residents must be
hypervigilant, recognizing and mitigating the dangers of their surroundings to thwart
violent victimization (Berardi 2018, pp. 120–23). Though Urbanik’s participants always
had to be circumspect within and beyond Regent Park, peer fatalities amplified their
attentiveness in the short term. For example, when discussing one of Urbanik’s key
participants’—Nathaniel’s—murder in 2016, Asther insisted that although he is “always
aware” given where he lives and his lifestyle, Nathaniel’s murder intensified his wariness:
“I’m saying that made me extra cautious, what happened to Nathanial, right?”
When a group member was killed (and especially after the men retaliated), Urbanik’s
participants were intensely committed to surveilling the neighbourhood to protect residents
and themselves from subsequent violence. To illustrate, consider Marcel’s response when
Urbanik asked how the group’s behaviours change when the “hood is hot”:
“People would see fishy vehicles, or, fishy people, you know what I mean, and from there,
you’d get that sense- like you’d know. We all know everyone from Regent Park. I know
everyone who has braids in Regent Park. Someone walks around with braids I don’t
know? I’ll be like ‘Look at this guy!’ And they would do the same thing . . . Altima,
tinted, moving funny, driving funny. And we’ll just stand on our toes.”
12
The researcher was a co-investigator on a separate ongoing study.
65
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
Urbanik’s participants deployed other safety protocols during times of heightened risk.
For example, they sometimes hung out in full-view of building security cameras in hopes
of deterring assailants, rarely veered far from building doors and ensured doors were
always open (sometimes breaking locks to ensure a speedy exit), and occasionally avoided
funerals/viewings. They also paid residents in easy-access apartments/townhomes to keep
their doors unlocked so they could run inside if necessary, increased group communication,
and had Urbanik check around adjacent buildings/corners, run neighbourhood errands
(e.g., trips to the convenience store), and drive them places.13 Similar to Roks’s study, peer
fatalities in Urbanik’s field site sparked a “hidden state of emergency” (Green 1994, p. 228),
despite their troubling frequency.
“They trusted someone they shouldn’t”: Attributing Blame to the Victim
In comparing StudyA’s and StudyB’s findings, a third common response to peer murder
emerges: how gang members ‘make sense of’ what occurred. Once the men in the Forgotten
Village and in Regent Park identified the probable killer(s), they shifted their attention to
the deceased’s actions preceding their murder.14
While the men in Roks’s study mourned Sin, Raymond drew upon the killing to
reiterate the informal rules of Dutch gang life:
“That’s why I always say: let me know your whereabouts. That shit can keep you alive.
Let me know where you are and let me know when you’ve made it home. I know it sounds
childish, but that shit can keep you alive. It’s fucked up, but this has to be a lesson for the
young homies. This is not a joke, this shit is serious. Fucked up that a homie like Sin has
to be the example.” (23 August 2012, excerpt from fieldnotes)
This comment offers a window into the daily practices of the Dutch Crips and how they
navigate street life. For their own safety, the men were expected to share their whereabouts
with other gang members. Raymond maintained that Sin may have prevented his murder
if he had adhered to this “code”. The ambiguities surrounding Sin’s death were obfuscated
by depicting Dutch gang life as guided by clear-cut conduct rules. However, instead of
seeing this specific ‘code’ as a concrete determinant of behaviour, the central argument put
forth by Copes et al. (2013) is that “telling the code” (Wieder 1974) illustrates how Dutch
gang members give meaning to the world around them, explaining their behaviour both to
themselves and to others.
Similarly, while the men in Regent Park had mutual concern for each other, they
ultimately regarded survival as an individual responsibility:
“It’s like, you already know these guys are all talking what they’re living . . . So, every
time I tell them, "Please keep your head up, please. I want to see you tomorrow, stay
safe." Everybody. Ask them. They say ‘Yeah’, but they’re not always keeping an eye on
their head. "Be safe, be safe." They don’t know. Tomorrow’s never promised. They be
walkin home, getting smoked. It’s crazy” (Asad)
“They slipped up, they trusted someone they shouldn’t, and guess what? Lights out!”
(Jefferson)
In this context, “keeping an eye on their head” refers to “staying on your Ps and Qs,” the
opposite of being “caught slipping” (see Berardi 2018, pp. 123–37). The men’s careful
dissection of the deceased’s alleged role in their own demise betrays that they perceive
and convey gang homicides are preventable, if potential victims operate accordingly. The
upshot here is that by being “caught slipping” and not successfully evading victimization
(including unprovoked, unanticipated violence), Urbanik’s participants regarded being
murdered as a choice: “They picked their own poison. They choose to go out [die] when the fuck
13
As a white woman, Urbanik was unlikely to be targeted in the neighbourhood.
14
Though this emphasis was often on the moments immediately before the killing (e.g., who they were with), this could also include earlier actions
(e.g., behaviors ‘inviting’ victimization, like filming a rap video on a rival block).
66
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
they chose to went out. If I told you to do something, and you went and did opposite and you end up
dying, I’ll feel like, ‘fucking dumb mofucker. You should have listened to me’”.
Though Booker’s reflection appears insensitive, it is rooted in his familiarity with death
and victimization as chronic exposure to neighbourhood violence can result in suppression
of sadness (see Fowler et al. 2009). When Urbanik asked whether the circumstances
surrounding a murder (e.g., wrong place at the wrong time, or provoked retaliation)
affected the extent to which the victim was considered responsible for their untimely death,
Booker insisted: “You still have to be on the P’s and Q’s about your own actions, right? So,
it doesn’t really matter on what they[rivals] did. It’s how they[victim] went about it and how
they got caught slipping . . . you were supposed to be more alert . . . ” The men also used these
expectations to disparage and police others’ behaviour, scolding those they believed were
too content. Frankie did this often, and he was firm in his position when he described
a peer being murdered because of their alleged slip in vigilance: “I’m gonna miss you,
yea. You’re my boy. But everybody has to use their head. You gotta get up and look, it’s like
crossing the street . . . I don’t mind you smoking and taking a nap but get up once in a while and
check [for rivals]”. Through monitoring and condemning each other’s actions, Roks’s and
Urbanik’s participants simultaneously expressed concern for their comrades and propelled
expectations that their affiliates were responsible for their own safety.15
6. Residual Effects: The “Realness” and “Reality” of Gang Homicide
In the previous section, we described commonalities in how gang members reacted
to and ‘made sense of’ peer fatalities. Although the men in both studies adopted similar
strategies, our data also reveal notable differences in responses to peer murder. We outline
these differences below and document how their varying street milieus produced these
differential effects.
“The homie is dead man, please keep it real!”: The Transformative Realness of
Sin’s murder
One evening about two weeks after Sin’s homicide, a couple of Crips members were
assigned to conduct “h200d patrol”—where members position themselves around neighbourhood entry points to ‘guard’ and ‘protect’ the h200d and senior gang members—from
potential enemies (Roks 2017a). Though Sin’s death initially heightened caution (sometimes
bordering on paranoia), members’ hypervigilance and increased safety concerns within
the h200d quickly dissipated. For example, when none of the members assigned to h200d
patrol reacted when a stranger on a scooter passed a pedestrian-only area, the gang leader,
annoyed by h200d patrol’s disregard, scoffed: “The homie is dead man, please keep it real!”
In this case, “keeping it real” referred to representing and defending Crips’ ‘turf.’ This
‘strip of reality’ (Appadurai 1996, p. 35)—since many gangs engage in defensive localism
(Adamson 2000)—forms a base ‘out of which scripts can be formed of imagined lives’. However, the transformative realness of Sin’s murder produced different interpretative schemes.
From the perspective of the gang leader and more established members—including longterm associates since the late 1980s—claiming a hood and defending their territory is
something ‘real gangstas’ do (Lauger 2012), particularly in the aftermath of peer fatality.
However, this was incongruent with how younger and new Crips perceived Sin’s murder
and ‘the need for’ h200d patrol. Since nothing ‘went down’ in the weeks after Sin’s death,
this signaled that the h200d was losing its ‘hood’ status and had become an unexciting
place. For these members, Sin’s murder did not reaffirm the ‘realness’ of Dutch gang life
but planted doubts about the function and necessity of defending a hood.
In his pioneering work on gangs, Thrasher [1927] (Thrasher [1927] 1964, p. 46) posits
conflicts with invisible or imagined adversaries can aid in gang integration: an “integration
through conflict”. After Sin’s death, a similar process occurred as the event amplified
several intra-group conflicts, mostly relating to a growing disillusion with Dutch gang
15
This was particularly true given broad distrust in police and perceptions of their ineffectiveness, with many participants attempting to protect
themselves in a milieu of police racism, brutality, and corruption.
67
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
life. Starting some months before Sin’s murder, the gang’s composition changed drastically; several previously-dedicated Crips lost interest in the group and left and younger
members were increasingly frustrated about their inadequate compensation for their work
for the Crips, which they saw as outweighing benefits of gang membership (Roks 2017b).
Dwindling membership dominated conversations, almost always against the backdrop of
Sin’s murder. Rick, one of the OGs, spoke for many of the older members when he made
sense of the Crips’ waning:
“After Sin was killed, the shit became too real for them. Then they couldn’t bang anymore,
because they suddenly had a job or something. But you know, the police also knows this.
That’s why they see us as the core members. But many have left, man.” (20 December
2012, conversation with Rick)
Sin’s murder was a defining moment that impacted all Crips, albeit in different ways.
For example, while older members claimed that they had lost close friends to violence
before and a few even asserted they “were used to it”, others openly shared that they cried
frequently and had trouble sleeping since the homicide. For some members, Sin’s murder
revealed who and what was “real”. In this sense, Sin’s death had a “transformative magic”
that brought “comic-book symbolism” to life (Katz 1988, pp. 129–31; Van Hellemont 2015,
pp. 191–224), (re)affirming the “realness” of the Rollin 200 Crips. For others however, the
murder ignited or cemented growing doubts about the reality—or realness—of belonging
to a Dutch gang. Members who left reported being drawn to the gang because of their
violent representations and street reputation; they had certain ideas about the realness of
Dutch gang life, in part inspired by media accounts of the Dutch Crips and influenced by
stereotypical representation of American gang life in movies, documentaries, and YouTube
(rap) videos. For them, beliefs about the realness of Dutch gang life were shattered by the
day-to-day realities, which usually consisted of spending long hours in the h200d doing
nothing.
Sin’s murder also had a transcendental significance for the Rollin 200 Crips. Annually,
multiple social media accounts dedicate posts to commemorating Sin. For example, on the
website of a recently established outlaw motorcycle gang that features prominent Dutch
Crips members (Roks and Densley 2020), a page is devoted to all the “cuzzos that we lost over
the years”, which maintains “They will never be forgotten”. The caption beneath Sin’s picture
reads “Triad in Peace Sin Locc”. These digital artifacts transmute Sin’s well-respected
status within the gang and simultaneously, as Conquergood (1994, pp. 51–52) analysis of
physical death murals for gang members attests, are “a generative source of strengthening
cohesion and commitment” and activate the group’s “cultural memory”. In addition to
these memorials, several Crip members have named their children (boys and girls) after Sin.
Through these communicative and mythmaking practices, the Dutch Crips have woven
Sin’s murder into their gang mythology.
“Out here everyone thinks they’re next”: The Reality of Gang Homicide in Regent Park
Similar to others living in impoverished communities characterized by stigmatization,
limited services, and neighbourhood violence (see Aspholm 2020, p. 217), peer murder was
an unfortunate lived reality for Urbanik’s participants and all considered it unavoidable.
However, while the men were heavily traumatized by losing their first peer to gun violence
(usually at 10–12 years old) they all reported becoming accustomed to affiliate murder,
referring to it as “normal”, “an everyday thing” and “just a part of life”. The normalcy and
near predictability of peer murder meant that even when Urbanik’s participants sat around
‘doing nothing’ like the men in The Forgotten Village, they needed to remain vigilant
and always be prepared to defend themselves, their crews, and their turf. Unlike Roks’s
participants who experienced peer fatality as signifying or demystifying the ‘realness’ of
gang life, the materiality and ‘realness’ of gang life in Regent Park was never in question.
Instead, Urbanik’s participants conveyed that the troubling routineness of peer fatality
both accustomed and benumbed them to losing loved ones. Booker succinctly described
68
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
this desensitization: “You just get over it [the murder] much faster now than before . . . You
lost people, after people, after people. It becomes like, you know, a common thing. When you get
used to something, it’s not as bad as the first time, right?” As Asther reflected upon his best
friend’s murder one afternoon, Urbanik asked whether subsequent losses affected him
similarly. He responded: “No, they don’t. Cause like, since that happened, it’s like [snaps fingers
to denote frequency] you get used to it . . . it’s easier for me this time”. Claims about becoming
habituated to murder are consistent with literature which has found youth exposed to
community violence may become emotionally desensitized to it as a form of pathological
adaptation and/or a coping mechanism (See Fowler et al. 2009 for a review).
This desensitization also meant that the ‘effects’ of peer homicide on group behaviours
often abated quickly:
“Like [when] someone dies, like yesterday, yeah-we all mourning them. Just give it like a
week later, people probably forget and people be all happy, laughing and doing their own
thing. But when it happens again, we’re back mourning them, then back to our normal
life. We lose so much people that it just, it’s like an everyday thing.” (Leon)
Similarly, Stefano described that while the group is “Edgy for a couple of days” after a
member’s homicide they “Have to get back to life . . . This is not the 1st time- this is not
gonna be the last time. It’s not the 3rd time, it’s the 100th time”. These descriptions align
with Urbanik’s field observations. While the men spent the initial weeks post-murder
openly mourning their loved ones and being hypervigilant, these behaviours largely
dwindled thereafter. This was not because Urbanik’s participants were unaffected by
their peer’s passing or questioned the ‘realness’ of gang life (like Roks’s participants).
They continued to commemorate them, engaging in several memorial processes including
“pouring some out for the dead homies,”16 producing commemoratory rap videos, and
honouring them on social media (Urbanik Forthcoming). However, they believed they
had to “get back to life” and “cool off” for survival; they needed to decompress quickly
in anticipation of and preparation for subsequent murders and/or their own potential
victimization.
Though the frequency of peer homicides necessitated that Urbanik’s participants
“get over” peer fatality quickly, their tragic regularity shaped group dynamics, creating a
communal and perpetual state of insecurity. This insecurity manifest itself via pervasive
beliefs members could be killed at any moment, eroding group cohesion, and (re)inciting
distrust among members. Unlike Roks’s participants who questioned the need to defend
their turf after Sin’s murder, Urbanik’s participants maintained that letting their guard
down even momentarily could be fatal and resigned themselves to the possibility they
could be murdered next. See Leon’s proclamation, for example: “It could happen anytime. It
could happen to us, you know? Me, just personally like, I just take it-it could happen to me at any
time, it could happen to anybody, right?” Marcel held a similar opinion, adding nuance based
on the neighbourhood’s ongoing revitalization which rendered violence less predictable
and avoidable (see also Urbanik et al. 2017: “At the end of the day, out here everyone thinks
they’re next, that’s what it is. It’s like, the fucking way they breaking the shit down, bodies are
dropping. The more buildings go down, the more bodies”.
Since neighbourhood violence was always imminent, Urbanik’s participants insisted
that even when the neighbourhood is “quiet” and hyperawareness is unnecessary they
must remain cautious and behave accordingly. As Ezekiel stressed: “You gotta play your
cards right. Life is a gamble, and they say it for a reason. You gotta roll the dice the right way”.
Yet, Ezekiel contradicted himself immediately: “You could just walk the street, look at someone
wrong, and they just shoot you. What part of the game is that? That’s not—that’s crazy”. Here, a
tension exists between the alleged safety provided by “playing your cards right”—not being
“caught slipping”—where one’s decisions can allegedly dictate survival or death, and life
as a “gamble”, where playing by street rules does not always shield against victimization.
These perspectives are incongruent; on the one hand victimization is ascribed to individual
16
A ritual of pouring alcohol out of freshly opened bottles on to the ground whilst reciting the names of murdered friends in a show of respect.
69
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
failures and on the other, it is credited to fatalism. This paradox likely reflects the men’s
attempts at feigning control in an environment where they have little (and sometimes zero)
control over safety.
The nature and frequency of peer fatality in Regent Park also bred distrust between
group members. While recounting his best friend’s murder several years earlier, Matteo
elucidated how this loss shattered his trust and reliance in his peers: “Don’t trust nobody.
Cause it was his own people that he trust that killed him. And no one knows that I know [culprit’s
identity]. I don’t trust a soul, I don’t bring no one to where I live. I rest my head [relax] nowhere.
I learned to distance . . . from the bullshit. I ain’t trying to go[die] like that”. Despite Matteo’s
recognition of these dangers and proclamation of pervasive distrust, he—like Urbanik’s
other participants—saw few possibilities of distancing “from the bullshit” and disengaging
from ‘the life.’ He remains a staple of Regent Park’s underground economy, spends his days
with other members, features in rap music videos, and engages in “hood politics”. Unlike
Roks’s participants who could and did disengage from gang life, Urbanik’s participants’
different street milieus and positionalities limited their ability to do the same. As such,
they continued to navigate their increasingly distrustful and tumultuous relationships
with group members, spending time together and operating as a cohesive unit all whilst
remaining suspicious of each other:
“He was on a block– that was supposed to be allies . . . He thought he was ok, you know?
The same allies hit him up [killed him]. So, you know . . . As much as people might be
your allies, you still can’t trust them, right?” (Leon)
Having lost many peers to gun violence and having been set up and shot, Leon was
chronically wary of his “allies,” explaining how this eroded his trust in other group
members: “I know how to move now. I watch my surroundings. I don’t chill with no one, I only
chill with who you see I’m here with every day. That’s it. I don’t need no new friends. Friends will
get you killed, they say . . . ” In Toronto’s street milieu, “the violent threat and militaristic
response exist in the same social circle” (Katz 1988, p. 218).
Many of Urbanik’s participants adhered to the “friends will get you killed” mantra,
echoing similar sentiments: “The streets talk. So, when you hear what happened, learn
how not to move, basically, you know? Usually the best way to stay is by yourself, to
yourself. Don’t have anyone watching your moves and stuff” (Booker). The men went
to great lengths to prevent even trusted peers from studying their habits. They kept
unpredictable schedules, seldom shared their whereabouts, and rarely committed to being
at a specific place at a specific time in fear that other members may set them up (see also
Goffman 2015). In this sense, ‘everyday’ community violence coupled with less common
but still too frequent gang homicides produced and exacerbated chronic suspicion of group
members, undermined reliance on group protection, and propelled additional violence
(see also Winton 2005). However, this disassociation did not push the men to become
disillusioned with “the life” like Roks’s participants, though they certainly questioned their
peers’ loyalty, by and large, they did not consider ‘leaving the life’, in part because they
believed they had few alternatives.
7. Discussion
In this paper, we explored how gang members make sense of peer murder(s) and
the residual effects of these violent events for gang members and their respective groups.
Despite the nuances in our respective studies, our data reveal notable commonalities in
how gang-involved men in two distinct contexts experience and respond to peer fatalities.
In both The Forgotten Village and Regent Park, gang-involved men initiate pseudo-murder
investigations, become hypervigilant in the immediate aftermath, and attribute blame
to the victim in attempts to ‘make sense’ of the violence. Below, we describe additional
commonalities in the how gang homicides affected our participants and their communities.
First, our data reveal that gang-involved men experience loss in complex, multidimensional ways. For the men in our respective studies, grief was a personal and communal
70
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
experience which produced individualized and collective effects, including trauma.17 Drawing attention to gang members’ lived experiences—particularly in relation to their exposure
to traumatic events, such as peer fatalities—is critical given societal and media narratives
which often pathologize gang members, portraying them as callous criminals. Whilst
our participants were offenders, they were also victims with extensive histories of violent
victimization by family members, friends, strangers, and rivals, usually commencing long
before they were old enough to join “the life”. Apart from their own victimization, the
men also experienced vicarious victimization and trauma. The common unilateral focus
on gang members as offenders obfuscates their experiences as simultaneously victims, dehumanizes them, masks their structural oppression, and de-contextualizes their decisions
and behaviours. As Pyrooz et al. (2014, p. 321) highlight: “This disjuncture has done a
disservice to criminology in general and gang research in particular for understanding the
linkages between these concepts”.
Second, gang homicides in both studies had immense collateral consequences that
extended beyond victims, perpetrators, and other gang members, deeply impacting families, loved ones, and communities. As evidenced, peer homicides had the potential to
(e.g., in The Forgotten Village) or did (e.g., in Regent Park) drive the cycle of victimization,
affecting inter-gang relations, retaliatory violence, and community safety. As such, gang
murders continue past the homicide; they can propel and are propelled by social contagion,
organizational memory, networks of competing groups jockeying for power, status, and
resources, which shape future gang behaviours, including homicide (Papachristos 2009,
p. 76). Future research should examine these collateral consequences in greater depth.
Third, how gang members made sense of, experienced, and responded to peer fatality
was intimately shaped by the specific street, social, economic, and political contexts in
which they were situated. Both of our studies involved marginalized and predominantly
racialized men socially excluded and ‘othered’ in their respective societies (albeit to varying degrees), including in the education system and labor market, because of race and
socioeconomic status. They were also—again, to varying degrees—harmed by and had to
navigate state violence most notably in the form of criminalization, overpolicing, and police
racism. For our participants, gang membership and its related activities (e.g., violence,
drugs and weapons trafficking, other criminal endeavors) was a form of “resistant identity”
(Castells 1997), a situated response and adaptation to their marginalization. Similar to other
marginalized, gang-involved men, our participants reported that gang membership afforded them with opportunities that they felt were less or unavailable elsewhere, including
economic benefits, independence, a sense of belonging, a (group) identity, and masculinity.
Fourth, in both studies, social media was central to how participants processed and
responded to peer fatality. Though a thorough examination of how the “digital street”
(Lane 2015, 2018) affects gang homicides is beyond the scope of this paper, our participants
relied upon social media to learn about others’ victimization, anticipate and hopefully
evade future violence, collect information on potential motives/suspects, determine rivals’
locations/movements, commemorate and grieve their murdered affiliates, try to save face
when disrespected, and threaten to avenge their loved ones’ homicide(s). Though much of
their online presentations were performative and sometimes departed from real life (see
Roks 2017b; Stuart 2020; Van Hellemont 2012), Urbanik’s participants engaged in digital
bravado, sometimes provoking suspected murderers and starting beefs with rivals and had
to simultaneously navigate the risks and dangers of doing so (Urbanik and Haggerty 2018;
Urbanik Forthcoming), which Roks’s participants did not. Though social media can incite
and propel gang violence in the real world, it is unclear which digital interactions can
produce offline violence (Stuart 2020) and how the street and online milieus in which gangs’
operate can affect this. Future research should examine the extent to which social media
affects on-the-ground processes, including inter- and intra-gang dynamics.
17
Whilst our participants spoke of how traumatic peer homicide is, they likely understated these effects given normative expectations about
masculinity and gang narratives emphasizing toughness.
71
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
Despite these commonalities, the men in The Forgotten Village and in Regent Park had
vastly different lived realities. While both our studies were based upon studying marginalized and street-involved men, the types and extent of our participants’ marginalization
differed, in part, due to their varying street contexts and positionality within their respective societies. We posit that these differences shaped how the men perceived, experienced,
and responded to peer homicide. As our data reveal, Dutch Crips members opted into gang
life because of glamorized ideals about what gang life entailed and opted out and pursued
alternate avenues (e.g., collecting unemployment benefits, finding regular, low-paying
jobs, or resorting back to street offending) when they became disillusioned (Roks 2017b).
Notably, feasible alternatives existed and could be pursued. Gang joining and gang exit were
distinct processes with few consequences, as violent victimization was rare even for the
most senior members and whilst they claimed membership provided them protection, the
broader social milieu rendered this alleged protection was largely unnecessary. By leaving
the gang, they could essentially escape risk.
Conversely, gang life was not something the men in Regent Park consciously opted
into or could essentially opt out of. Almost of all Urbanik’s participants were born or fell
into ‘the life’ because of their upbringing and neighbourhood context. The men reported
Old Heads—often brothers, cousins, fathers, uncles, neighbours—grooming them into
gang-related activities (e.g., drug running, stashing weapons, monitoring for police) during
their pre-teen years, and merely “going along with it” as they aged. As poor and racialized
men living in “the ghetto,” they saw few if any opportunities to support themselves and
their families outside of the informal economy, especially as they accrued lengthier criminal
records. Since most were unable to relocate, gang exit seemed both implausible and futile
as they could not easily sever their social ties and they considered violent victimization
largely inescapable.18
Our findings also uncover that variances in our participants’ respective positionalities and street cultures produced differences in how they experienced peer murder. In
accordance with Mares (2010, p. 41) observation that “the circumstances and settings of
gang violence are highly variable”, our findings indicate that the street milieus in which
gang violence and homicide occur can have a notable influence on how gang member’s
experience and respond to peer fatality. In Roks’s field site, gang violence and especially
murder, was rare and momentous. One peer homicide prompted members’ to contemplate
the realness of Dutch gang life. Contrary to StudyA, the tragic frequency of peer fatalities
in Regent Park diluted their impacts as murders did not have a “transformative magic”
(Katz 1988, p.129). Homicides did not signal the realness of gang life in Regent Park; the
frequency of peer murder and the incessant risks posed by merely living in Regent Park
meant these risks were largely imparted, inescapable, and had to be carefully mitigated.
Though additional deaths were unquestioningly tragic, their effects were relatively shortlived as the men recognized the need to “move on” quickly in preparation for the next
loss.
While peer homicide disintegrated group trust and amplified conflict in both field
sites, this occurred to varying degrees and in different contexts and therefore had different
consequences for gang dynamics. In Roks’s study, Sin’s murder played a notable role
in creating and exacerbating existing intra-gang disagreements and temporarily brewed
distrust between group members. These mounting tensions pushed some members to leave
the gang. Peer fatalities in Regent Park had similar effects, though they were amplified,
particularly in terms of mounting distrust in and fear of trusted affiliates. Unlike in
StudyA, this chronic wariness did not push the men to consider leaving gang life, as they
were already navigating an environment where gang violence and broad distrust was the
18
For many Black men/youth in Regent Park, “staying out of the life” does not necessarily protect them from violent victimization.
72
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
norm.19 Whilst peer fatalities amplified distrust in the short-term, homicides did not notably
change the gang’s fabric, as they did in StudyA.
Like other social realities and motivations for action, gang violence is often influenced
by an intersecting multiplicity of factors, and should be examined as a cultural, psychosocial, behavioural, and transactional manifestation occurring in a particular social setting
(Brotherton 2015, p. 163) with locally-specific consequences. As our data show, while
similarly disadvantaged gang-involved men in different gang, street, local, and national
contexts make sense of, experience, and respond to peer fatality similarly, their experiences
differ in notable ways due to their divergent social, economic, and political milieus. As
such, examinations of how affiliate murders affect gang members and gang dynamics
should be carefully situated within the broader milieus in which gang members operate.
Going forward, gang scholars should remain cognizant of the complexity and messiness of
gang violence and how its local context affects experiences of gang homicides.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, M.-M.U. and R.A.R.; methodology, M.-M.U. and R.A.R.;
formal analysis, M.-M.U. and R.A.R.; investigation, M.-M.U. and R.A.R.; writing—original draft
preparation, M.-M.U. and R.A.R.; writing—review and editing, M.-M.U. and R.A.R. All authors have
read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This work was partly supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research
(NWO) under the program ‘Conflict and Security’ (grant no. 432-08-089 to R.R.) and party supported
by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant number 767-2015-1881).
Institutional Review Board Statement: Urbanik’s study received Research Ethics Board Approval
from the University of Alberta (Pro00052729). Roks’ study did not require REB approval
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the
study.
Data Availability Statement: Data is contained within the article.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Adams, Jennifer J., and Jesenia M. Pizarro. 2014. Patterns of specialization and escalation in the criminal careers of gang and non-gang
homicide offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior 41: 237–55. [CrossRef]
Adamson, Christopher. 2000. ‘Defensive localism in white and black: A comparative history of European-American and AfricanAmerican youth gangs’. Ethnic and Racial Studies 23: 272–98. [CrossRef]
Aldridge, Judith, Robert Ralphs, and Juanjo Medina. 2011. Collateral damage: Territory and policing in an English gang city. In Youth
in Crisis. Edited by Barry Goldson. New York: Routledge, pp. 72–88.
Anderson, Elijah. 1999. Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral Life of the Inner City. New York: W.W. Norton and Co.
Appadurai, Arjun. 1996. Modernity at Large: Cultural dimensions of Globalization. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Aspholm, Roberto. 2020. Views from the Streets: The Transformation of Gangs and Violence on Chicago’s South Side. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Berardi, Luca. 2018. ‘Shots Fired: Experiences of Gun Violence and Victimization in Toronto Social Housing’. Ph.D. dissertation,
University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada.
Blumstein, Alfred. 1995. Youth violence, guns, and the illicit-drug industry. The Journal of Criminal law and Criminology 86: 10–36.
[CrossRef]
Brotherton, David C. 2015. Youth Street Gangs: A Critical Appraisal. New York: Routledge.
Brotherton, David C., and Luis Barrios. 2004. The Almighty Latin King and Queen Nation. New York: Columbia University Press.
Bursik, Robert, and Harold G. Grasmick. 1993. Economic deprivation and neighborhood crime rates, 1960–1980. Law & Society Review
27: 263–84.
Castells, Manuel. 1997. The Power of Identity. Oxford: Black-Well.
CBS. 2017. More Murder and Manslaughter Victims in 2017. Available online: https://www.cbs.nl/en-gb/news/2018/36/moremurder-and-manslaughter-victims-in-2017, (accessed on 9 January 2021).
CBS. 2020. Number of Homicides Halved over Two Decades. Available online: https://www.cbs.nl/en-gb/news/2020/42/numberof-homicides-halved-over-two-decades, (accessed on 9 January 2021).
19
Their distrust must also be understood in relation to their broader distrust across their lives, and particularly, in broader social institutions which
have often served as a source of institutional violence (see also Goffman 2015).
73
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
Conquergood, Dwight. 1994. Homeboys and Hoods: Gang Communication and Cultural Space. In Groups Communication in Context:
Studies of Natural Groups. Edited by Larry R. Frey. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 23–55.
Copes, Heith, Brookman Fiona, and Anastasia Brown. 2013. Accounting for violations of the convict code. Deviant Behavior 34: 841–58.
[CrossRef]
Curry, G. David, and Irving A. Spergel. 1988. Gang Homicide, Delinquency, and Community. Criminology 26: 381–407. [CrossRef]
Curtis, Ric. 2003. Crack, cocaine and heroin: Drug eras in Williamsburg, Brooklyn, 1960–2000. Addiction Research & Theory 11: 47–63.
Decker, Scott H. 1996. Collective and Normative Features of Gang Violence. Justice Quarterly 13: 243–64. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and G. David Curry. 2002. Gangs, Gang Homicide, and Gang Loyalty. Journal of Criminal Justice 30: 343–52. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and David C. Pyrooz. 2010. On the validity and reliability of gang homicide. Homicide Studies 14: 359–76. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott H., and David C. Pyrooz. 2013. "Gangs: Another Form of Organized Crime?". In Oxford Handbook of Organized Crime.
Edited by Letizia Paoli. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 270–87.
Densley, James A. 2012. Street gang recruitment: Signaling, screening, and selection. Social Problems 59: 301–21.
Densley, James A. 2014. It’s gang life, but not as we know it: The evolution of gang business. Crime & Delinquency 60: 517–46.
Esbensen, Finn Aage, Thomas Winfree Jr., Ni He, and Terrance J. Taylor. 2001. Youth gangs and definitional issues. Crime and
Delinquency 47: 105–30. [CrossRef]
Fine, Gary. A. 2010. The sociology of the local: Action and its publics. Sociological Theory 28: 356–76. [CrossRef]
Fowler, Patrick J., Carolyn J. Tompsett, Jordan M. Braciszewski, Angela J. Jacques-Tiura, and Boris B. Baltes. 2009. Community
violence: A meta-analysis on the effect of exposure and mental health outcomes of children and adolescents. Development and
Psychopathology 21: 227–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ganpat, Soenita M., and Marieke C. A. Liem. 2012. Homicide in the Netherlands. In Handbook of European Homicide Research. Edited by
Marieke C. A. Liem and William Alex Pridemore. New York: Springer, pp. 329–41.
Geertz, Clifford. 1998. Deep hanging out. New York Review of Books 45: 69–72.
Global News. 2019. Toronto Police Chief Says Recent String of Gun Violence in City Related to Street Gangs. August 9. Available
online: https://globalnews.ca/news/5746070/toronto-police-chief-mark-saunders-gun-violence/ (accessed on 9 October 2020).
Goffman, Alice. 2015. On the Run: Fugitive Life in an American City. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Green, Linda. 1994. Fear as a Way of Life. Cultural Anthropology 9: 227–56. [CrossRef]
Hagedorn, John M. 1994. Neighborhoods, markets, and gang drug organization. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 31: 264–94.
[CrossRef]
Hagedorn, John M., and Perry Macon. 1988. People and Folks: Gangs, Crime and the Underclass in a Rustbelt city. Chicago: Lake View
Press.
Howell, James C. 1999. Youth gang homicides: A literature review. Crime and Delinquency 45: 208–41. [CrossRef]
Jacobs, Bruce A., and Richard Wright. 2006. Street Justice: Retaliation in the Criminal Underworld. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Katz, Jack. 1988. The Seductions of Crime. New York: Basic Books.
Klein, Malcolm W. 2005. The Value of Comparisons in Street Gang Research. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 21: 135–52.
[CrossRef]
Klein, Malcolm. W., and Cheryl L. Maxson. 1989. Street Gang Violence. In Violent Crime, Violent Criminals. Edited by Neil Alan Weiner
and Marvin E. Wolfgang. Newbury Park: Sage.
Klein, Malcolm, Frank Weerman, and Terrance Thornberry. 2006. “Street gang violence in Europe”. European Journal of Criminology 3:
413–437. [CrossRef]
Kubrin, Charis E., and Tim Wadsworth. 2003. Identifying the Structural Correlates of African-American Killings. Homicide Studies 7:
3–35. [CrossRef]
Kubrin, Charis E., and Ronald Weitzer. 2003. Retaliatory homicide: Concentrated Disadvantage and Neighborhood Culture. Social
Problems 50: 157–80. [CrossRef]
Lane, Jeffrey. 2015. The digital street. American Behavioral Scientist 60: 43–58. [CrossRef]
Lane, Jeffrey. 2018. The Digital Street. New York: Oxford University.
Lauger, Timothy R. 2012. Real Gangstas: Legitimacy, Reputation, and Violence in the Intergang Environment. New Brunswick: Rutgers
University Press.
Lewis, Kevin, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2020. Rules of the game: Exponential random graph models of a gang homicide network.
Social Forces 98: 1829–58. [CrossRef]
Mares, Dennis. 2010. Social Disorganization and Gang Homicides in Chicago. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice 8: 38–57. [CrossRef]
Maxson, Cheryl L. 1999. Gang Homicide: A Review and Extension of the Literature. In Homicide: A Sourcebook of Social Research. Edited
by M. Dwayne Smith and Margaret A. Zahn. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 239–56.
Maxson, Cheryl L., and Malcolm. W. Klein. 1990. Street gang violence: Twice as great, or half as great? In Gangs in America. Edited by
C. Ronald Huff. Thousand Oaks: SAGE, pp. 71–100.
Maxson, Cheryl L., and Malcolm. W. Klein. 1996. Defining gang homicide. In Gangs in America, 2nd ed. Edited by C. Ronald Huff.
Thousand Oaks: SAGE, pp. 3–20.
Maxson, Cheryl L., Margaret A. Gordon, and Malcolm W. Klein. 1985. Differences between Gang and Nongang Homicides. Criminology
23: 209–12. [CrossRef]
74
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
Miethe, Terance D., and Richard C. McCorkle. 2002. Evaluating Nevada’s antigang legislation and gang prosecution units. In Responding
to Gangs: Evaluation and Research; Edited by Winifred L. Reed and Scott H. Decker. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice,
pp. 169–85.
Miller, Walter B. 1975. Violence by Youth Gangs and Youth Gangs as Crime Problem in Major American Cities; Washington, DC: SGPO, Office
of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, U.S. Department of Justice.
Papachristos, Andrew V. 2009. Murder by Structure: Dominance Relations and the Social Structure of Gang Homicide. American Journal
of Sociology 115: 74–128. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., and David S. Kirk. 2006. Neighborhood Effects on Street Gang Behavior. In Studying Youth Gangs. Edited by
James F. Short and Lorine Hughes. Lanham: Altamira, pp. 63–85.
Papachristos, Andrew V., David Hureau, and Anthony Braga. 2013. The corner and the crew. American Sociological Review 78: 417–47.
[CrossRef]
Pizarro, Jesenia M., and Jean Marie McGloin. 2006. Explaining gang homicides in Newark, New Jersey. Journal of Criminal Justice 34:
195–207. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C. 2012. Structural covariates of gang homicide in large U.S. cities. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 49:
489–518. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Scott E. Wolfe, and Cassia Spohn. 2011. Gang-related homicide charging decisions: The implementation of specialized
prosecution unit in Los Angeles. Criminal Justice Policy Review 22: 3–26. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Richard K. Moule, and Scott H. Decker. 2014. The contribution of gang membership to the victim–offender overlap.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 51: 315–48. [CrossRef]
Rodgers, D. 2002. We Live in a State of Siege: Violence, Crime, and Gangs in Post-Conflict Urban Nicaragua. DESTIN Working Paper No. 36.
London: Development Studies Institute (DESTIN), London School of Economics.
Roks, Robert A. 2017a. “Crip or die? Gang disengagement in the Netherlands”. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
Roks, Robert A. 2017b. “In the ‘h200d’: Crips and the intersection between space and identity in the Netherlands”. Crime, Media,
Culture. [CrossRef]
Roks, Robert A., and James A. Densley. 2020. From Breakers to Bikers: The Evolution of the Dutch Crips ‘Gang’. Deviant Behavior 41:
525–42. [CrossRef]
Rosenfeld, Richard, Timothy M. Bray, and Arlen Egley. 1999. Facilitating Violence: A Comparison of Gang-Motivated, Gang-Affiliated,
and Non-Gang Youth Homicides. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 15: 495–516. [CrossRef]
RTL Nieuws. 2020. Toename vuurwapengeweld in Nederland: vorig jaar 646 incidenten. Available online: https://www.rtlnieuws.nl/
nieuws/nederland/artikel/5095871/vuurwapen-wapens-illegaal-criminelen-toename-politie-2019-2018 (accessed on 9 January
2021).
Skarbek, David. 2011. Governance and prison gangs. American Political Science Review 105: 702–16. [CrossRef]
Skogan, Wesley. 1974. The Validity of Official Crime Statistics. Social Science Quarterly 55: 25–38.
Spergel, Irving A. 1984. Violent Gangs in Chicago. Social Service Review 5: 199–226. [CrossRef]
Statistics Canada. 2019. Homicide in Canada. 2018. Available online: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/pub/85-002-x/2019001/
article/00016-eng.htm (accessed on 4 October 2020).
Stuart, Forrest. 2020. Ballad of the Bullet: Gangs, Drill Music, and the Power of Online Infamy. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Thrasher, Frederic M. 1964. The Gang. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. First published 1927.
Urbanik, Marta-Marika. 2018. Drawing Boundaries or Drawing Weapons? Neighborhood Master Status as Suppressor of Gang
Violence. Qualitative Sociology 41: 497–519. [CrossRef]
Urbanik, Marta-Marika, and Kevin D. Haggerty. 2018. ‘#It’s Dangerous’: The Online World of Drug Dealers, Rappers and the Street
Code. The British Journal of Criminology 58: 1343–60.
Urbanik, Marta-Marika, and Robert A. Roks. 2020. GangstaLife: Fusing Urban Ethnography with Netnography in Gang Studies.
Qualitative Sociology 43: 213–33. [CrossRef]
Urbanik, Marta-Marika, Sara K. Thompson, and Sandra M. Bucerius. 2017. ‘Before There Was Danger but There Was Rules and Safety
in Those Rules’: Effects of Neighbourhood Redevelopment on Criminal Structures. The British Journal of Criminology 57: 422–40.
[CrossRef]
Urbanik, Marta-Marika, Roks Robert A, Storrod Michelle, and Densley James. 2020. "Emerging Issues in Gang Ethnography: The
Challenges and Opportunities of Eurogang Research in a Digital Age". In Gangs in the Era of Internet and Social Media. Edited by
Chris Melde and Frank Weerman. New York: Springer.
Urbanik, Marta-Marika. “Gangbangers are Gangbangers, Hustlers are Hustlers”: The Rap Game, Social Media, and Gang Violence in
Toronto. In International Handbook of Critical Gang Studies. Edited by David Brotherton and Rafael Gude. London: Routledge,
Forthcoming.
van de Port, Mattijs. 2001. Geliquideerd: Criminele Afrekeningen in Nederland. Amsterdam: Meulenhoff.
Van der Valk, Joost. 2009. Strapped ’n Strong. Available online: https://revolver.nl/film-tv/2120/crips-strapped-n-strong (accessed on
9 January 2021).
Van Hellemont, Elke. 2012. Gangland Online: Performing the Real Imaginary World of Gangstas and Ghettos in Brussels. European
Journal of Crime, Criminal Law and Criminal Justice 20: 159–73. [CrossRef]
75
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 17
Van Hellemont, Elke. 2015. The Gang Game: The Myth and Seduction of Gangs. Ph.D. dissertation, Koninklijke Universiteit van
Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.
Van Stapele, Saul. 2012. ‘Deze Crip sliep al jaren “heel lichtjes”’. NRC Handelsblad, August 21.
Vargas, Robert. 2014. Criminal group embeddedness and the adverse effects of arresting a gang’s leader. Criminology 52: 143–68.
[CrossRef]
Viering, Peter. 1994. Straatroof, inbraak, doodslag [Street Robbery, Burglary, and Manslaughter]. Panorama 3: 37–42.
Warmington, Joe. 2020. Wild Weekend of Shootings Like a Civil War. Toronto Sun. September 28. Available online: https:
//torontosun.com/news/local-news/warmington-wild-weekend-of-shootings-like-a-civil-war (accessed on 3 October 2020).
Wieder, D. Lawrence. 1974. Language and Social Reality: The Case of Telling the Convict Code. Den Haag: Mouton.
Winton, Ailsa. 2005. Youth, gangs and violence: Analysing the social and spatial mobility of young people in Guate mala City.
Children’s Geographies 3: 167–84. [CrossRef]
Witzel, Andreas. 2000. The Problem-centered Interview. Forum: Qualitative Social Research 1: 1–7.
76
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
The Social Network Consequences of a Gang
Murder Blowout
Alice Airola and Martin Bouchard *
School of Criminology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC V5A 1S6, Canada;
[email protected]
* Correspondence:
[email protected]
Received: 28 September 2020; Accepted: 7 November 2020; Published: 11 November 2020
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Abstract: An unexpected crisis in a criminal organization offers a rare opportunity to analyze whether
and how the configuration of business and trust relationships changes in response to external shocks.
The current study recreates the social network of the Red Scorpion gang members involved in the
Surrey Six Murder, one of the deadliest gang-related homicides to occur in Canada. The event,
which involved two bystanders and six victims in total, was the result of a poorly executed retaliation.
Our analyses focus on two phases of the network, the conspiracy phase and the post-murder phase.
In each phase, we examine the balance of business, trust, and conflictual ties. Results show that
the relative importance of key participants changed from the conspiracy to the post-murder phases,
whereby strong, trusted ties gained prominence over the mostly business-oriented network of the
conspiracy phase.
Keywords: gangs; social networks; crisis; organized crime; homicide; violence; retaliation
1. Introduction
Crime network scholars have sought to describe the inner workings of gangs and criminal
organizations for long enough, now, that we have a general understanding of their structure,
especially as it relates to specific activities such as drug trafficking (e.g., Bichler et al. 2017;
Bright and Delaney 2013; Calderoni 2012; Malm et al. 2017; Malm and Bichler 2011; Morselli 2009;
Natarajan 2006) and human smuggling (Bruinsma and Bernasco 2004; Campana 2020). More recently,
an increasing number of scholars have turned to network data to study conflicts among gangs
(Bichler et al. 2019; Descormiers and Morselli 2011; Lewis and Papachristos 2020; McCuish et al. 2015;
Papachristos 2009; Papachristos et al. 2013). Rarely, however, can we dive inside a specific gang to
examine how they manage relationships in trying times, such as when gang leaders are arrested,
when a new gang challenges one’s turf, or when the gang is under fire for having killed one or
multiple bystanders.
The current study proposes to take an inside look into a specific murder conspiracy gone wrong.
The conspiracy involves one of the most famous criminal organizations based in British Columbia (BC),
Canada, the Red Scorpions. In summer 2007, two criminal groups merged forces under the label of the
Red Scorpions (RS). The alliance expanded the organization, which now had two sides, the “Asian”
and the “White” side, labeled as such by the members themselves. The so-called Asian side was led by
Michael Le, the original founder of the Red Scorpions, while the White side was led by James Bacon,
the leader of another criminal group involved in drug trafficking in the same area. The purpose of the
merger was to improve the two groups’ power within the drug trade via cooperation, including an
improved ability to defend their turf against rivals when required.
With its loose hierarchy and emphasis on loyalty and symbolism, the Red Scorpions shared some
organizational features with some of the mature, business-oriented gangs found in the American
(e.g., Bichler et al. 2019; Papachristos 2009) or Canadian (e.g., Descormiers and Morselli 2011) literature.
77
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
Gang members could be identified by “RS” tattoos on their arms and necks, and the new members had
to pass a sort of probatory period before being accepted as part of the Red Scorpion family. The main
business of the Red Scorpions was running drug lines in the Lower Mainland, what locals labeled
as “dial-a-dope” operations—a text messaging drug delivery service. At the time of the merger,
approximately 30 to 40 members had the tattoos and were considered official members. Among them,
approximately 20 to 30 individuals regularly attended the Red Scorpions meetings.
Court documents revealed that a few months after the merger, “bad blood” developed between
James Bacon and a rival drug dealer, Corey Lal. Bacon threatened his rival’s life and decided to tax
him $100,000 as a way of resolving the dispute. But Lal never paid, so a conspiracy for his murder
took shape. Otherwise, the RS group would look weak and powerless. Three members of the Asian
Side, leader Michael Le, Matthew Johnston, and Cody Haevischer, along with four members of the
White side, leader James Bacon, Person X, Person Y, and Kevin Leclair, participated in the conspiracy.
The Surrey Six Murder took place on 19 October 2007, when Johnston, Haevischer, and a third unnamed
accomplice (“Person X”) broke into an apartment located in Surrey, BC, where Lal was used to carrying
out his activities related to the drug business. That day, Lal was not alone; another four people were
with him in the apartment, including one individual who was not involved in the drug trade. Another
person, not involved in the drug trade, was dragged into the apartment from the hallway. All six
people were shot to death in an attempt at eliminating any possible witnesses.
The Surrey Six Murder was the result of a series of unexpected external contingencies that thrust
the organization into a crisis. The concept of crisis here refers to the chaotic group response that
followed the gang homicide. The execution of six people was neither planned nor wanted by the
group; the organization was not ready to deal with such a major event a few months after the merger
and did not have a precise strategy to follow in case of unexpected contingencies. The aim of this
study is to examine the social network consequences of this event on a major criminal organization
like the Red Scorpions. Several studies have examined the effects of a crisis on legal organizations,
but few studied crises in criminal organizations. Does the network become more cohesive—a sort of
retrenchment phase—or does it instead break and fragment itself? We use social network analysis
(SNA) as an integrative framework to describe both the network consequences for the organization,
and the individuals within it.
2. Group Structure and Individual Centrality in Times of Crises
Organizational crises have been operationalized in different ways, including organizational death,
decline, retrenchment, and failure (Mellahi and Wilkinson 2004). All definitions share a common
feature: they underline that group crises have consequences on organizational structures and dynamics.
Sociologists have primarily focused on group dynamics and changes during crises in legitimate
organizations (i.e., Hamblin 1958; Fink et al. 1971; Mulder et al. 1971; Tutzauer 1985; Uddin et al. 2010;
Hossain et al. 2013). A number of studies have explored group dynamics during crises through the
lens of SNA (e.g., Tutzauer 1985; Uddin et al. 2010; Hossain et al. 2013). These studies highlighted
how a crisis within an organization impacts its internal structure or cohesion. Cohesion refers to the
degree of connectedness of nodes within a network: The more people who are connected to each other,
the more a network can be defined as cohesive. The inverse of cohesion is fragmentation, which refers
to the proportion of nodes within a network that cannot reach each other by any path (Borgatti 2006).
Network scholars have discussed two effects of crises: (1) network fragmentation increases,
creating multiple cliques (small, highly connected groups) (i.e., Uddin et al. 2010; Hossain et al. 2013);
(2) homophily increases (i.e., see Lanzetta 1955). Homophily and network fragmentation are related
concepts. The term “homophily” refers to the tendency of people to interact more with individuals
they perceive as similar (McPherson et al. 2001). Fragmentation may increase homophilic individuals’
tendencies to interact with similar others—and vice versa: a person’s tendency toward homophily may
itself lead to more fragmentation in times of crises, when the benefits of homophilous connections may
also increase. For instance, Hossain et al. (2013) examined the crisis that afflicted the Enron Corporation
78
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
in 2001. Enron was one of the most important American energy, commodities, and services companies
between 1985 and 2000. The authors analyzed Enron’s e-mail networks, deriving from the large set of
messages released by the US Federal Energy Regulatory Commission (FERC), to assess the changes
that occurred in the communication network structures during the year of the crisis in 2001. The results
showed a sharp increase in the number of cliques as the organization moved toward the peak of the
crisis. Network members faced the crisis by increasing communication within small groups of people
who felt closer to each other. Tutzauer (1985) claimed that, when two communication networks with
the same number of ties and nodes are compared, the network characterized by the higher number of
cliques is likely to be closer to dissolution. Yet, the presence of the cliques does not necessarily imply
fragmentation of the whole organization. Stogdill (1959) suggested that group integration is higher
when the subgroups are well coordinated and support the structure and the objectives of the larger
group. In this context, subgroups or cliques can represent an escape from the organizational pressure
and contribute to reinforcing the values and the identification of clique members with the larger group
structure (Stogdill 1959). However, too much independence may hinder survival in the long run.
It is unclear whether illegal organizations behave similarly when crises occur. Some indirect
results from studies examining the consequences of fragmentation have shown that an increase
in fragmentation within illegal organizations has often led to increased competition and violence
among newly born small groups (i.e., Massari and Martone 2019; Atuesta and Pérez-Dávila 2018;
Falcone and Padovani 1991; Vargas 2014). Massari and Martone (2019) argued that the high level
of fragmentation characterizing the Camorra is one of the explanatory factors used to understand
the extremely violent nature of this criminal organization. Atuesta and Pérez-Dávila (2018) showed
that the fragmentation within Mexican cartels led to a significant increase in intra-gang violence.
Falcone and Padovani (1991) explained how inter-clan conflicts made the Italian organized groups
more visible to the law enforcement, thus allowing the implementation of repressive actions that
weakened the power of the Sicilian Mafia. The impact of crises may depend on the structure of the
group. For example, Vargas (2014) showed that the arrest of two street gangs’ leaders in Chicago led to
increased inter-gang violence, but only within the group that lacked a solid organizational structure.
Few scholars have explored the effects of crises on criminal organizations from a network
perspective. Some studies have examined the changes in criminal networks through different
periods and have highlighted the flexibility that characterizes criminal networks when facing hard
or unstable times (e.g., Bright and Delaney 2013; Ouellet et al. 2017; Ouellet and Bouchard 2018).
Bright and Delaney (2013) examined the change and the evolution of a drug trafficking network across
time and found that networks are flexible and adaptive structures following a process of adaptation
similar to living organisms. Much on network adaptation can also be learned from crises occurring
in terrorist groups. After all, these groups also manage their social networks, in part, to avoid law
enforcement detection. Ouellet et al. (2017) studied the processes that drove collaboration between
offenders in the Al-Qaeda (AQ) network before and after 9/11 (war on terror period). They found that
although AQ leaders were still involved in planning activities after 9/11, they did so from an increased
social distance, in sparser networks. Crises may also be driven by internal forces. Dissension between
leader may, for instance, fragment the network, forcing the dissolution of many intragroup ties as
leaders pull away from each other (Ouellet and Bouchard 2018).
A few organized crime scholars have described retrenchment processes that are helpful in framing
our expectations toward the effects of crises on criminal organizations. Paoli (2007) described the
reaction of Cosa Nostra to a massive law enforcement activity that threatened the organization.
From a structural point of view, the solution of one of the most famous (and infamous) Italian Mafia
bosses in modern history, Bernardo Provenzano, to ensure the cohesion and avoid potential defectors,
was reducing the number of “men of honor” and creating a criminal elite to protect himself and the
most important criminal members from police actions (Paoli 2007). The same strategy was adopted by
Outlaw Motorcycle Clubs in the US in similar circumstances. According to Quinn (2001), during a
79
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
crisis, many of these clubs implemented a sort of “retrenchment” phase consisting of reducing the
group size and creating an elite group based on core members.
From a network perspective, the “retrenchment strategy” suggests that, when facing crises,
criminal organizations may adapt by decreasing the size of the organization, thus creating a smaller
cohesive group of core members. The retrenchment strategy mentioned by Paoli (2007) and Quinn (2001)
differs from the network fragmentation described by network communication scholars because of
the way in which it impacts network structure and size. For example, the Enron group did not face
the crisis by reducing network size, or by creating a single highly connected group of individuals
(Hossain et al. 2013). The retrenchment strategy implies a significant decrease in network size, and the
formation of one cohesive small group to protect the core members of the organization.
The effect of crises can also be analyzed from the point of view of individual group members. A few
actors may benefit from the crises, improving their position in the network as a direct consequence
of the events (Uddin et al. 2010). For instance, organizations may look to leaders for direction
(Lanzetta 1955), which may increase their influence during periods of crises (Hamblin 1958), especially
if they are counted on to control communications (Argote et al. 1989). The limited evidence for
changes in criminal leaders’ network positions is mixed (McCuish et al. 2015; Morselli and Petit 2007;
Ouellet et al. 2017). Ultimately, whether leaders emerge as stronger or weaker from a crisis may well
depend on the attribution of blame—was the crisis caused by the leaders in the first place? For instance,
Morselli and Petit (2007) examined a criminal organization that faced a crisis of confidence as the
police started seizing each of their drug shipments while refraining from arresting anyone over the
course of the 18-month investigation. This allowed them to monitor how the network reacted and
adapted to the crisis. Network members showed increased dissatisfaction and distrust with the initial
leaders, who eventually lost their central role in the network after new leaders emerged.
3. Shared Goals, Trust, and Control as Elements of Cohesion and Individual Centrality
The quality of the ties connecting people, and the level of control exercised by some group
members over others, may impact the way in which a criminal organization faces a crisis. In this
study, we differentiated between three types of ties: trust ties (i.e., strong), business ties (i.e., weak),
and conflict ties (negative). Different types of ties are linked to different kinds of social needs. Weak ties
allow for efficient information flow (Granovetter 1973), but strong ties that provide social support may
be most needed in times of uncertainty and crisis (Krackhardt 1992). We will examine this possibility
directly by comparing the balance of strong, weak, and negative ties, and after the murder.
Relational aspects such as shared goals and trust among group members may play a key role in
building strong group cohesion. Shared goals and trust are two key elements of criminal cooperation
(Morselli 2009; von Lampe and Johansen 2004). Criminal relationships based merely on business
interests, without the trust element, can be too weak to resist during times of crisis. According to
Paoli (2008b), the weakening of solidarity and trust bonds in the Sicilian Mafia in the mid-2000s has
caused a growth in the number of cooperating witnesses and a decrease in the criminal group’s
cohesion. Being surrounded by trustworthy offenders is even more important for those offenses
that imply a higher degree of risk because they face the most serious consequences (Tremblay 1993;
McCuish et al. 2015).
In this study, the level of control was articulated around (1) strategic network positioning of
individuals; (2) the presence within the network of triadic groups based on strong ties. First, some
individuals are more likely to exercise control over others by virtue of the strategic positions they occupy
within their networks, a concept that can be measured via betweenness centrality (Morselli 2009).
Betweenness centrality captures an individual’s capacity to connect others who would not be connected
otherwise. Higher betweenness values are associated with the ability to control the flow of information
and resources in a network (Freeman 1977). Second, Simmel (1989) argued that triadic relationships
based on strong ties have the power to reduce individualities, moderating conflicts and preserving
group survival by imposing a certain level of control on individuals (Krackhardt 1999). In other words,
80
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
triads based on strong ties are a source of both control and social support for their members. In our
study we identified as “strong ties” the relationships between individuals who share the same criminal
goals but who also trust each other.
4. The Current Study
The Surrey Six Murder represents an ideal case study to observe the impact of a crisis on network
structure. The available data allowed us to distinguish the conspiracy network connections that existed
before the murder, from those that emerged after the event. Our study is articulated in different levels
of analysis, focused on the effect of the crisis on individual centrality, but also on the network as
a whole.
We focused on three main research objectives:
(1)
(2)
(3)
To explore the impact of the crisis on network cohesion;
To investigate the impact of the crisis on leaders’ and other core members’ centrality within
the network;
To understand the effects of the crisis on the quality of the ties and the level of control.
5. Materials and Methods
5.1. Data Source
The study data were extracted from court documents associated with the Surrey Six Murder
Judgment. The transcript of the judgment was released in October 2014, and is available on the
Supreme Court of British Columbia website at https://www.bccourts.ca/supreme_court/. The judgment
referred to the trial of two members of the Red Scorpions group, two of the actual killers, Matthew
Johnston and Cody Haevischer. The judgment described the reasons behind the court’s decision
to charge Matthew Johnston and Cody Haevischer with first-degree murder. The court documents
provided us with detailed information about the relational connections between individuals involved
in the case, the Red Scorpions group, its story and the status of its members, and particulars about the
quality and the strength of the relationships connecting certain central members. The judgment also
contained personal information about the individuals involved in the conspiracy and in the murder
(e.g., name and surname, gender, nationality, and affiliation to a criminal organization). Only the first
names and family names of people directly involved in the murder were mentioned in the judgment,
while witnesses or individuals not directly involved in the trial were anonymized, as they are in
our study. A total of 18 individuals were identified as part of the Surrey Six Murder case from the
information presented in court documents.
The mixed-method approach that we applied included extracting cohesion measures and
individual centrality indices from the Surrey Six network and doing a content analysis to define
the quality of ties and the level of control within the network. The content analysis started with a
read-through of the 175-page long Surrey Six Judgment and other Surrey Six materials, seeking to
uncover the different types of relationships that connected the nodes, and situating the relationships as
occurring before or after the murder. We identified three main categories of relationships: business
ties, trust ties, and conflict ties. We then coded each social interaction as one of the three relationship
types. When the information about the relationships among the individuals involved in the Surrey Six
case was unclear, we searched for further details in the numerous newspaper articles related to the
case. Searches were conducted using the names (or surnames) of the most important Red Scorpion
affiliates involved in the murder (i.e., Michael Le, Matthew Johnston, Cody Haevischer, James Bacon).
The names or surnames were followed by the keywords “Surrey Six” (i.e., Michael Le Surrey Six;
James Bacon Surrey Six). We examined a body of 40 newspaper articles that provided us with further
information on the relationships linking the individuals involved in the murder, as well as a book on
the Bacon brothers written by an investigative journalist (Langton 2013).
81
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
5.2. Measures and Procedures
Our measures of the before and after Surrey Six network focused on six elements: group size,
cohesion, fragmentation, individual centrality indices, tie quality, and control. Most will be used to
describe the network and meso levels, while centrality indices will be used at the individual level.
5.2.1. Network and Meso-Level Measures
Group size: Group size refers to the number of nodes and the number of ties in the network.
Cohesion: At the network level, cohesion was measured employing three network metrics called
“density,” “average degree,” and “degree centralization.” Network density is the proportion of ties
existing among nodes in relation to the maximum number of potential connections that can exist in
the network if all nodes are reciprocally connected. Average degree refers to the average number of
connections per node, which has the advantage of being less impacted by network size (a drawback of
density). Finally, degree centralization assesses the extent to which the group’s cohesion is organized
around a particular node (Hanneman and Riddle 2005). Note that, because cohesion is normally
associated with a set of positive relationships, we removed any negative ties before calculating the
cohesion measures.
Level of fragmentation: Fragmentation was calculated through the total number of cliques, or the
maximum number of actors who have all possible ties among themselves. If the number of cliques
increases post-murder, it implies a higher level of fragmentation within the network.
Quality of ties: Tie quality has often been expressed by the concept “strength of ties” and has
been measured in different ways in prior studies. Some studies have based it on the frequency
of the interactions (Granovetter 1973), the recency of the contacts (Lin et al. 1978), the nature of
the relationships (i.e., Ericksen and Yancey 1980), or the presence of at least one mutual friend
(Shi et al. 2007). von Lampe and Johansen (2004) highlighted the importance of at least two relational
elements, trust and shared criminal goals, to consider a criminal tie strong and exploitable. We classified
the network ties in three categories: (1) trust ties, (2) business ties, and (3) conflict ties. The “trust
ties” (friendships, positive family and romantic connections) were the strongest ties in the network.
The term “business ties” refers to those relationships that were based only on shared business goals of
an illegal nature. We classified the “business ties” as “weak connections” because of the absence of
trust. Finally, the term “conflict ties” refers to the relationships that were based on shared business
goals, but that also involved some level of conflict (e.g., Red Scorpion affiliates who clearly stated
that they mistrusted other affiliates or had a conflictual relationship with them). The “conflict ties”
captured the negative relationships in the network. At the network level, the overall percentage of trust,
business, and conflict ties expressed the quality of the relationships the two networks were based on.
Level of control: At the meso-level, group control was calculated by integrating two theoretical
approaches: the Simmelian theory of social control (Simmel 1989) and Heider (1946) theory of cognitive
balance. Drawing from Simmel (1989) theory on triadic relationships, we identified positive triadic
groups as cliques that provide both social support and social control. By “positive cliques,” we referred
to groups of three people connected through ties based on both shared business goals and trust.
However, triadic relationships can be composed of different types of ties, such as trust, business,
and conflict ties. To establish the extent to which “mixed triads” could potentially become positive
triads, we used Heider (1946) theory of cognitive balance. Cognitive balance theory proposes that
when strong ties between A and B, and A and C exist, B and C are very likely develop a positive tie as
well. The search for cognitive balance would encourage B and C to align their feelings with those of
their common strong tie A.
Heider’s theory was subsequently translated into graphic–theoretic language by Cartwright and
Harary (1956). Signed graphs assigned positive or negative values to each tie composing the triad: an
odd number of negative signs made the graph unbalanced. We translated trust, shared business goals,
and conflict ties into signs: trust ties were positive (+), business ties were neutral, and conflict ties were
negative (−). Only those cliques composed of at least two signed ties (+ and −) were taken into account.
82
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
If the multiplication of the signed ties gave a positive result (i.e., +*+ = +; − * − = +), it meant that
the clique was balanced; thus, the group could potentially be, or become, a strong positive clique that
provided support and control. On the other hand, if the multiplication of signed ties gave a negative
result (i.e., +*− = −), the clique was unbalanced; thus, the triadic group was not likely to become a
strong positive clique. The unbalanced clique could be considered as a potential source of conflict.
5.2.2. Individual Level of Analysis
Betweeness centrality: We measured the extent to which a node occupied a strategic position in
the network using betweenness centrality = the extent to which a node connect nodes that would not
be connected otherwise. Occupying a strategic position within the network also means being able to
control the flow of information and resources within it (Freeman 1977).
Quality of ties: At the individual level, tie quality can influence the impact of individual positions
within the network. The quality of node relationships was examined descriptively by counting the
number of trust ties and conflict ties surrounding each node.
6. Results
Figure 1 represents the Surrey Six Murder network before and after the murder, respectively.
The squared nodes represent the individuals who took part in the conspiracy; in brown are the Asian
side’s members, while in orange, the White side’s members. The blue square in Figure 1a indicates
that Sophon Sek was present during the conspiracy but was not part of the Red Scorpions group.
The round nodes represent the individuals who were not directly involved in the conspiracy but who,
for some reason, played a role in the Surrey Six Murder story. The red round nodes and the gray nodes
in Figure 1b represent, respectively, the newcomers (nodes who were not present in the pre-murder
network) and the nodes who disappeared after the murder.
(a) Conspiracy network before the murder
Figure 1. Cont.
83
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
(b) Conspiracy network after the murder
Figure 1. The Surrey Six Murder network before (a) and after (b) the murder. Notes. The squared
nodes took part in the conspiracy, rounded nodes did not. The brown nodes represent the Asian side,
while the orange nodes represent the White side. The black lines represent business ties, the green lines
trust ties, and the red lines conflict ties. Leader names in bold. Node size by betweenness centrality.
(a) The blue squared node was involved in the conspiracy but was not part of the Red Scorpions.
(b) The gray nodes and lines stand for the nodes and ties that disappeared after the murder. The lines
in bold and the red rounded nodes represent the ties and the nodes that appeared after the murder.
Node size was determined by betweenness centrality values; the larger the node, the higher its
betweenness centrality score within the network. At first glance, we notice that the leader Michael
Le and the other members of the Asian side occupied a central position in both the pre- and the
post-murder network, while the members of the White side, led by James Bacon (in bold), seemed to
play a more marginal role, especially after the murder. The colors of the ties stand for the quality of
the relationships that bonded the nodes together. The black lines indicate that nodes were connected
through a business relationship, the green lines represent relationships based on both trust and shared
business goals, while the red lines represent the relationships characterized by shared business goals
and some level of conflict. In Figure 1b the gray lines represent the relationships that disappeared
after the murder, while the bold lines represent the new relationships that were not present before
the murder.
Looking at the green ties, it is possible to identify the strong positive cliques composed of three
trust ties. The clique that included Cody Haevischer, K.M., and Matthew Johnston, present in both the
pre- and post-murder networks, is an example of a strong positive triad. On the other hand, the cliques
with red, green, and black lines, such as the clique comprising Le, Haevischer, and Johnston in both
networks, represent an unbalanced triad. Finally, the balanced triads are characterized by two green
lines and one black line, such as the one including Jonathon Bacon, James Bacon and Haevischer in the
post-murder network.
6.1. Network Structures before and afetr the Murder
To start, we examine the structures, the quality of ties, and the level of control in the network
before and after the murder. The post-murder network represented the group during a period of
crisis. The study focused on a period of about one year. The pre-murder phase referred to the period
84
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
from the merger, which occurred in summer 2007, to the murder in October 2007. The post-murder
phase referred to the events that followed the murder until Spring 2008. Given that we were analyzing
the same group under a short time frame, we did not expect the network to show dramatic changes.
Yet we did expect the group to have made adjustments as they managed the aftermath of the event.
Table 1 presents a number of characteristics of the network before and after the murder. Overall,
the results show that the network evolved toward increased fragmentation, as would be predicted by
the literature on the impact of crises on social networks.
Table 1. Comparison of network structures before and after the Surrey Six Murder.
Before the Murder
After the Murder
Number of nodes
13
15
Number of ties
42
49
Density
0.269
0.233
Average degree
3.231
3.267
Degree centralization
0.765
0.637
6
8
Percentage of business ties
100%
100%
Percentage of trust ties
11.8%
16.3%
Percentage of conflict ties
4.8%
4.1%
Number of cliques
Note. Arrow up and down indicates increase/decrease after the murder, respectively; equal sign means no change.
First, the network changed only slightly in size, with two more individuals and seven additional
ties after the murder. Second, cohesion, which included both the density and the degree of centralization
of the network, declined after the murder, with the former decreasing from 0.269 to 0.233 and the
latter decreasing from 0.765 to 0.637. The decrease in centralization indicated that ties were spreading
out across the network, potentially making pre-murder hubs less central than before. This was also
consistent with the increased number of cliques (from six to eight) that we noticed post-murder.
Average degree remained stable, showing that people did not change the number of connections they
had; it was how these connections were spread out that differed.
Some changes to the post-murder network involved the quality of the ties. We observed that the
proportion of trust ties increased post-murder, from 11.8% to 16.3%. The proportion of business and
conflict ties remained similar.
The quantity of unbalanced triads did not vary after the murder. Both the pre- and post-murder
networks were characterized by three unbalanced triads. The unbalanced cliques mostly involved
core conspiracy members, such as Michael Le, Matthew Johnston, Cody Haevischer, and Person Y.
The only individual involved in the unbalanced cliques who was not directly involved in the murder
and who was not officially part of the Red Scorpions group was K.M—Haevischer’s girlfriend and the
only woman in the network.
Where things changed, post-murder, was with the balanced triads. Indeed, no balanced triad
was identifiable in the pre-murder network. Yet, three balanced triads formed after the murder.
The post-murder balanced cliques included two core conspiracy members, James Bacon and Cody
Haevischer, as well as three non-conspiracy members: K.M, Jonathon Bacon, and Justin Haevischer.
The addition of three family/romantic ties (two brothers and a girlfriend) increased balance in
the network.
The analysis of the dyadic connections characterizing the pre- and the post-murder networks
further clarified what is stated above. On the one hand, the individuals who were part of the RS group
and took part in the conspiracy were mostly linked to each other through business or conflict ties.
85
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
On the other hand, all the trust connections in the network linked core conspiracy members to nodes
who were external to the group.
6.2. Individual Level of Analysis
Table 2 shows the individual centrality measures and the individual tie quality both before and
after the murder. We assessed the nodes’ betweenness centrality by comparing the values related to
the pre- and post-murder networks; thus, only those individuals who were present both before and
after the murder are included in the table.
Table 2. Individual centrality measures and individual quality of ties before and after the murder.
Before
Betweenness
After
Betweenness
Before Trust
and Distrust
After Trust
and Distrust
“Asian Side”
Matthew Johnston
0.174
0.192
2T+1D
T
D
2T+1D
Michael Le
0.174
0.139
1T+1D
T
D
0T+1D
Cody Haevischer
0.059
0.119
2T+1D
T
D
4T+1D
“White Side”
James Bacon
0.033
0.030
0T+0D
T
D
1T+0D
Person X
0.019
0.002
00
T
D
00
Person Y
0.011
0.017
0T+1D
T
D
0T+1D
Kevin Leclair
0.003
0.002
00
T
D
00
Others
K.M.
0.011
0.093
2T+0D
T
D
3T+0D
Windsor Nguyen
0.000
0.066
00
T
D
1T+0D
Nam Hoang
0.000
0.000
00
T
D
00
In green: the nodes who experienced a significant decrease in betweenness centrality and who decreased the number
of trust connections, as well. In red: the nodes who represented a significant increase in betweenness centrality
and who increased the number of trust connections, as well. In bold: the leaders. Arrow up and down indicates
increase/decrease after the murder, respectively; equal sign means no change.
Before the murder, individuals from the so-called Asian side of the Red Scorpions were the
most prominent in terms of brokerage. All three of Johnston, Le, and Haevischer had the highest
betweenness centrality scores—both before and after the murder. We could have expected James
Bacon, the leader who gave rise to the dispute, to play a more important role in the Surrey Six Murder.
Johnston, Le, and Haevischer were also central in terms of trust relationships. Each of them had at least
one trust connection in the network. However, all three were also surrounded by a conflict tie that
weakened the overall quality of their relationships. As for the outsiders to the RS or to the conspiracy,
its worth noting that K.M occupied a unique position in the pre-murder network in terms of tie quality
(two trust ties), a position that she consolidated after the murder when she added a third trusted tie.
86
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
After the murder, some changes occurred. First, only some of the leaders improved their network
position. While Le experienced a slight decrease in betweenness, Johnston and Haevisher both
improved their pre-murder positions. None of the RS from the White side noticeably improved their
positions. Second, when focusing on tie quality, the node who experienced the greatest increase in
betweenness, Haevischer, was also the one who had the largest increase in trust connections, from two
to four. Leader Michael Le lost his sole trust tie post-murder—the only individual to lose a trust
connection. Third, two nodes who substantially increased their betweenness centrality after the murder,
K.M. and Windsor Nguyen, were not core conspiracy members. The increase in betweenness centrality
was particularly evident in the case of K.M., who became one of the most central individuals in the
network after the murder.
Finally, five new individuals appeared after the murder, three of whom were Red Scorpion
members’ relatives: Jonathon Bacon (James’ brother); Justin Haevischer (Cody’s brother); Mike
Nguyen (Windsor’s brother). The newcomers were not involved in the conspiracy and played
marginal roles in the network. However, they were rather central in terms of trust connections. Justin
Haevischer, for instance, was surrounded by three trust connections, two of which linked him to core
conspiracy members.
7. Discussion
The Surrey Six gang murder blowout case gave us a unique opportunity to explore the effects
of a period of crisis on a criminal organization. The comparison between the pre-murder and the
post-murder network helped us assess different hypotheses testing the cohesion of organizations and
the centrality of individuals during crises.
Our study results showed that the Surrey Six Murder network followed many of the patterns
found in legal organizations (see Tutzauer 1985; Uddin et al. 2010; Hossain et al. 2013). The level of
fragmentation and network size increased post-murder, while the network’s density and centralization
decreased. These results suggested that individuals sought to increase their connections, but these new
connections were not to the core conspiracy members.
We did observe network changes after the Surrey Six Murder, but the adjustments were different
from the retrenchment phases that occurred in major organizations like the Italian Mafia (Paoli 2007),
American biker gangs (Quinn 2001), or Al-Qaeda (Ouellet et al. 2017). Similar to what Ouellet and
colleagues observed for terrorist groups like the Toronto 18 (Ouellet and Bouchard 2018), the network
showed signs of fragmentation after the crisis. In addition, the role of the leaders (Le and Bacon) was
diminished after the murder, something that was also observed in prior studies (Morselli and Petit 2007).
This was also true of most other core conspiracy members who experienced slight decreases in
betweenness centrality. Cody Haevischer was the only core conspiracy member whose centrality
increased after the murder.
Analyses of tie quality and control provided insights on potential reasons for why the Surrey Six
Murder network did not experience a sort of retrenchment phase around core members. The pre-murder
network comprised a high percentage of business ties, but a low level of trust and control, especially
within the core conspiracy members group. After the murder, the proportion of trust ties increased
along with the number of positive and balanced cliques. These results supported prior research
that suggested that strong ties are particularly effective when a group faces uncertainty and crisis
(Krackhardt 1992), thus needing to reinforce obligations and social norms (Coleman 1988). In the
same way, intensifying the level of control over individuals and information flow is essential when the
group is threatened (Argote et al. 1989; Hossain et al. 2013), which is especially relevant in the case of
organized crime (Paoli 2002).
The increase in trust and control that characterized the post-murder network could be linked
to the increase in the network’s fragmentation and size. Paoli (2008a, 2008b) argued that criminal
organizations that implemented the retrenchment strategy were built on a high level of trust and
solidarity shared by all members. The meso-level analysis of dyadic relationships showed that the
87
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
Red Scorpion core conspiracy group was built mainly on business and conflict ties, while the trust
relationships in the pre-murder network linked mostly core members to nodes who were external to the
group. The positive cliques that did exist involved mostly nodes who were external to the conspiracy.
This network configuration was even clearer in the post-murder network, where we observed the
addition of new trust relationships that connected the core members to nodes who were not present
before the murder.
The most lasting and stable organized crime groups are typically founded on pre-existing trust
ties and collective shared identity (Paoli 2002, 2007). It could be that the low levels of trust found
in the Red Scorpions made room for family members and other trusted ties to join in, post-murder,
as a currency that was scarce yet needed in times of crisis. Criminal organizations need to balance
efficiency and security (Calderoni 2012; Morselli 2009), but they may not always search for that balance
unless circumstances force them to.
The study has some limitations that are necessary to discuss. One of the main problems is
missing data, as the study only includes those who were mentioned in the Surrey Six Judgment.
Some individuals who were either not involved in the law enforcement investigation or included in the
judicial decision may be missing. The missing data can impact all the network indices. For instance,
trust ties may be more present than the court documents show. A concern related to node centrality is
that the judgment is centered on the trial of two individuals, Cody Haevischer and Matthew Johnston.
Thus, Haevischer and Johnston’s high centrality scores may be due to the fact that they were central in
the judgment. Indeed, we examined the Surrey Six conspiracy network from the point of view of two
of its most central players, not the Red Scorpion network as a whole. The scientific literature on crises
within criminal organizations has often analyzed changes that occurred within the organization at
large, which could explain why our results sometimes diverged. As with all court data, the information
included in the judgment could lack objectivity because it was mostly based on witnesses’ declarations
and law enforcement’s recollection of the events. Thus, only the declarations that have been considered
reliable by the court were included in the analysis. A further factor that might have influenced the RS
network changes is the non-typical merger between the two gangs that occurred a few months before
the murder. Because of the merger, the organization was potentially more exposed to fragmentation
than longstanding, ethnically homogenous, and well-structured criminal organizations.
When dealing with court records and police investigations, Campana and Varese (2012) suggested
performing external validity checks by means, for instance, of interviews with key informants and
other open source records. Rostami and Mondani (2015) study showed that different data sources
related to the same study object have a fundamental impact on the network results. Although we were
able to find numerous written materials on the case, interviews with key participants would have
helped provide further context on specific relationships included in the network, including potentially
missing ones. Finally, the study of the Red Scorpions, as an organization, was limited to a very specific
time frame. We did not have access to specific data on the evolution of the group post-crisis, nor was it
the aim of the study. That said, there is some evidence to suggest that the organization suffered after
many of their leaders were arrested and charged in major police operations in the years following the
Surrey Six Murder. Yet more than 10 years after the post-murder phase we analyzed in this study,
the Red Scorpions was still an active gang in BC (e.g., Bolan 2019).
Despite these limitations, the Surrey Six Murder represented a unique opportunity to study
organized crime groups during crises from a network perspective. Rather than a retrenchment phase
taking place after the murder, the network expanded in size, leading to decreased cohesion. Leaders
became less central as trusted connections integrated the network. This sort of adjustment—reduced
importance of leaders—is not in and of itself a negative outcome for the group. When trust is not
in short supply, criminal leaders can afford to position themselves on the periphery of conspiracy
networks, as heavy involvement is simply not required –trust among participants removes much of
the need for control (Calderoni 2012). Trust was lacking prior to the Surrey Six murder, making it the
most pressing need to address post-crisis. To our knowledge, no studies have measured the impact
88
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
that a lack of trust and control can have on a criminal organization and its survival. The survival of
criminal organizations depends on a variety of factors that are not necessarily linear; small groups
survive longer when they forge alliances with outsiders, but larger groups benefit more when they
strive to keep alliances within (Ouellet et al. 2019). Achieving proper balance between efficiency (and
profit-making) and security, between waging wars over turf or sharing turf, are some of the most
consequential—yet understudied—decisions made by gang leaders.
Author Contributions: Study design/framing: M.B. and A.A.; Methodology: A.A. and M.B.; Analyses: A.A.;
Writing: A.A. and M.B. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Argote, Linda, Marlen E. Turner, and Mark Fichman. 1989. To centralize or not to centralize: The effects of
uncertainty and threat on group structure and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes 43: 58–74. [CrossRef]
Atuesta, Laura H., and Yocelyn Samantha Pérez-Dávila. 2018. Fragmentation and cooperation: The evolution of
organized crime in Mexico. Trends in Organized Crime 21: 235–61. [CrossRef]
Bichler, Gisela, Aili Malm, and Tristen Cooper. 2017. Drug supply networks: A systematic review of the
organizational structure of illicit drug trade. Crime Science 6: 1–23. [CrossRef]
Bichler, Gisela, Alexis Norris, Jared R. Dmello, and Jasmin Randle. 2019. The impact of civil gang injunctions on
networked violence between the bloods and the crips. Crime & Delinquency 65: 875–915.
Bolan, Kim. 2019. Red Scorpion Gangster Gets More Than 7 Years in Jail on Drug, Gun Charges. Vancouver Sun.
July 2. Available online: https://vancouversun.com/news/staff-blogs/real-scoop-red-scorpion-sentenced-indrug-and-firearms-case (accessed on 28 October 2020).
Borgatti, Stephen P. 2006. Identifying sets of key players in a social network. Computational and Mathematical
Organization Theory 12: 21–34. [CrossRef]
Bright, David A., and Jordan J. Delaney. 2013. Evolution of a drug trafficking network: Mapping changes in
network structure and function across time. Global Crime 14: 238–60. [CrossRef]
Bruinsma, Gerben, and Wim Bernasco. 2004. Criminal groups and transnational illegal markets. Crime, Law and
Social Change 41: 79–94. [CrossRef]
Calderoni, Francesco. 2012. The structure of drug trafficking mafias: The ’Ndrangheta and cocaine. Crime, Law
and Social Change 58: 321–49. [CrossRef]
Campana, Paolo. 2020. Human smuggling: Structure and mechanisms. In Organizing Crime: Mafias, Markets, and
Networks. Edited by Michael Tonry and Peter Reuter. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Campana, Paolo, and Federico Varese. 2012. Listening to the wire: Criteria and techniques for the quantitative
analysis of phone intercepts. Trends in Organized Crime 15: 13–30. [CrossRef]
Cartwright, Dorwin, and Frank Harary. 1956. Structural balance: A generalization of Heider’s theory.
The Psychological Review 63: 277–93. [CrossRef]
Coleman, James S. 1988. Social capital in the creation of human capital. The American Journal of Sociology
94: S95–S120. [CrossRef]
Descormiers, Karine, and Carlo Morselli. 2011. Alliances, conflicts, and contradictions in Montreal’s street gang
landscape. International Criminal Justice Review 21: 297–314. [CrossRef]
Ericksen, Eugene P., and William L. Yancey. 1980. Class, Sector and Income Determination, Temple University:
unpublished paper.
Falcone, Giovanni, and Marcelle Padovani. 1991. Cose di Cosa Nostra. Milano: Rizzoli.
Fink, Stephen L., Joel Beak, and Kenneth Taddeo. 1971. Organizational crisis and change. The Journal of Applied
Behavioral Science 7: 15–37. [CrossRef]
Freeman, Linton C. 1977. A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociometry 40: 35–41. [CrossRef]
Granovetter, Mark S. 1973. The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology 78: 1360–80. [CrossRef]
Hamblin, Robert L. 1958. Group integration during a crisis. Human Relations 11: 67–76. [CrossRef]
89
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
Hanneman, Robert A., and Mark Riddle. 2005. Introduction to Social Network Methods. Riverside: University of
California, Available online: http://faculty.ucr.edu/~{}hanneman/ (accessed on 28 October 2020).
Heider, Fritz. 1946. Attitudes and cognitive organization. The Journal of Psychology Interdisciplinary and Applied
21: 107–12. [CrossRef]
Hossain, Liaquat, Shahriar T. Murshed, and Shahadat Uddin. 2013. Communication network dynamics during
organizational crisis. Journal of Informetrics 7: 16–35. [CrossRef]
Krackhardt, David. 1992. The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations. Networks and
Organizations: Structure, Form and Action 5: 216–39. [CrossRef]
Krackhardt, David. 1999. The ties that torture: Simmelian tie analysis in organizations. Research in the Sociolology
of Organizations 16: 183–210. [CrossRef]
Langton, Jerry. 2013. The Notorious Bacon Brothers: Inside Gang Warfare on Vancouver Streets. Mississauga: Wiley.
Lanzetta, John T. 1955. Group behavior under stress. Human Relations 8: 29–52. [CrossRef]
Lewis, Kevin, and Andrea V. Papachristos. 2020. Rules of the game: Exponential random graph models of a gang
homicide network. Social Forces 98: 1829–58. [CrossRef]
Lin, Nan, Paul W. Dayton, and Peter Greenwald. 1978. Analyzing the instrumental use of relations in the context
of social structure. SociologicalMethods & Research 7: 149–66. [CrossRef]
Malm, Aili, and Gisela Bichler. 2011. Networks of collaborating criminals: Assessing the structural vulnerability
of drug markets. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 4: 271–97. [CrossRef]
Malm, Aili, Martin Bouchard, Tom Decorte, Marieke Vlaemynck, and Marije Wouters. 2017. More structural holes,
more risks? Network structure and risk perceptions among marijuana growers. Social Networks 51: 127–34.
[CrossRef]
Massari, Monica, and Vittorio Martone. 2019. Mafia violence: Political, Symbolic, and Economic Forms of Violence in
Camorra Clans. New York: Routledge. [CrossRef]
McCuish, Evan C., Martin Bouchard, and Raymond R. Corrado. 2015. The search for suitable homicide
co-Offenders among gang members. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 31: 319–36. [CrossRef]
McPherson, Miller, Linn Smith-Lovin, and James M. Cook. 2001. Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks.
Annual Review of Sociology 27: 415–44. [CrossRef]
Mellahi, Kamel, and Adrian Wilkinson. 2004. Organizational failure: A critique of recent research and a proposed
integrative framework. International Journal of Management Reviews 5–6: 21–41. [CrossRef]
Morselli, Carlo. 2009. Inside Criminal Networks. New York: Springer.
Morselli, Carlo, and Katia Petit. 2007. Law-enforcement disruption of a drug importation network. Global Crime 8:
109–30. [CrossRef]
Mulder, Mauk, Jan R. Ritsema Van Eck, and Rendel D. de Jong. 1971. An organization in crisis and non-crisis
situations. Human Relations 24: 19–41. [CrossRef]
Natarajan, Mangai. 2006. Understanding the structure of a large heroin distribution network: A quantitative
analysis of qualitative data. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 22: 171–92. [CrossRef]
Ouellet, Marie, and Martin Bouchard. 2018. The 40 members of the Toronto 18: Group boundaries and the analysis
of illicit networks. Deviant Behavior 39: 1467–82. [CrossRef]
Ouellet, Marie, Martin Bouchard, and Mackenzie Hart. 2017. Criminal collaboration and risk: The drivers of Al
Qaeda’s network structure before and after 9/11. Social Networks 51: 171–77. [CrossRef]
Ouellet, Marie, Martin Bouchard, and Yanic Charette. 2019. One gang dies, another gains? The network dynamics
of criminal group persistence. Criminology 57: 5–33. [CrossRef]
Paoli, Letizia. 2002. The paradoxes of organized crime. Crime, Law and Social Change 37: 51–97. [CrossRef]
Paoli, Letizia. 2007. Mafia and organised crime in Italy: The unacknowledged successes of law enforcement.
West European Politics 30: 854–80. [CrossRef]
Paoli, Letizia. 2008a. Mafia Brotherhoods: Organized Crime, Italian Style. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [CrossRef]
Paoli, Letizia. 2008b. The decline of the Italian mafia. In Organized Crime: Culture, Markets and Policies. Edited by
Siegel Dina and Nelen Hans. Studies in Organized Crime vol. 7. New York: Springer. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V. 2009. Murder by structure: Dominance relations and the social structure of gang homicide.
American Journal of Sociolology 115: 74–128. [CrossRef]
Papachristos, Andrew V., David M. Hureau, and Anthony A. Braga. 2013. The corner and the crew: The influence
of geography and social networks on gang violence. American Sociological Review 78: 417–47. [CrossRef]
90
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 204
Quinn, James F. 2001. Angels, bandidos, outlaws, and pagans: The evolution of organized crime among the big
four 1% motorcycle clubs. Deviant Behavior 22: 379–99. [CrossRef]
Rostami, Amir, and Hernan Mondani. 2015. The complexity of crime network data: A case study of its
consequences for crime control and the study of networks. PLoS ONE 10: e0119309. [CrossRef]
Shi, Xiaolin, Lada A Adamic, and Martin J. Strauss. 2007. Networks of strong ties. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics
and its Applications 378: 33–47. [CrossRef]
Simmel, Georg. 1989. Sociologia. Milano: Edizioni di Comunità.
Stogdill, Ralph M. 1959. Individual Behavior and Group Achievement: A Theory: The Experimental Evidence. Oxford:
Univer. Press.
Tremblay, Pierre. 1993. Searching for suitable co-offenders. In Routine Activity and Rational Choice: Advances
in Criminological Theory. Edited by Ronald V. Clarke and Marcus Felson. New Brunswick: Transaction,
pp. 17–36.
Tutzauer, Frank. 1985. Toward a theory of disintegration in communication networks. Social Networks 7: 263–85.
[CrossRef]
Uddin, Shahadat, Shahriar T. H. Murshed, and Liaquat Hossain. 2010. Towards a scale free network approach to
study organizational communication network. Paper presented at PACIS 2010—14th Pacific Asia Conference
on Information Systems, Taipei, Taiwan, July 9–12.
Vargas, Robert. 2014. Criminal group embeddedness and the adverse effects of arresting a gang’s leader:
A comparative case study. Criminology 52: 143–68. [CrossRef]
von Lampe, Klaus, and Per Ole Johansen. 2004. Organized crime and trust: On the conceptualization and
empirical relevance of trust in the context of criminal networks. Global Crime 6: 159–84. [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional
affiliations.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
91
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Changes in Personal Social Networks across Individuals
Leaving Their Street Gang: Just What Are Youth
Leaving Behind?
Caterina G. Roman 1, * , Meagan Cahill 2 and Lauren R. Mayes 3
1
2
3
*
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Citation: Roman, Caterina G.,
Meagan Cahill, and Lauren R. Mayes.
2021. Changes in Personal Social
Networks across Individuals Leaving
Department of Criminal Justice, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA
RAND Corporation, Arlington, VA 22202, USA;
[email protected]
Department of Criminology, Vancouver Island University, Nanaimo, BC V9R 5S5, Canada;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Abstract: Despite a small but growing literature on gang disengagement and desistance, little is
known about how social networks and changes in networks correspond to self-reported changes
in street gang membership over time. The current study describes the personal or “ego” network
composition of 228 street gang members in two east coast cities in the United States. The study
highlights changes in personal network composition associated with changes in gang membership
over two waves of survey data, describing notable differences between those who reported leaving
their gang and fully disengaging from their gang associates, and those who reported leaving but
still participate and hang out with their gang friends. Results show some positive changes (i.e.,
reductions) in criminal behavior and many changes toward an increase in prosocial relationships
for those who fully disengaged from their street gang, versus limited changes in both criminal
behavior and network composition over time for those who reported leaving but remained engaged
with their gang. The findings suggest that gang intervention programs that increase access to or
support building prosocial relationships may assist the gang disengagement process and ultimately
buoy desistance from crime. The study also has implications for theorizing about gang and crime
desistance, in that the role of social ties should take a more central role.
Their Street Gang: Just What Are
Youth Leaving Behind? Social Sciences
Keywords: desistance; social networks; street gangs; network composition; criminal behavior
10: 39. https://doi.org/10.3390/
socsci10020039
Received: 17 December 2020
1. Introduction
Accepted: 19 January 2021
Mounting evaluation research has shown few successes across the gang intervention
landscape. As scholars have grappled with this realization, they have begun to more closely
examine the characteristics and motivations related to individuals leaving a gang, and thus
attempted to be more precise in defining gang leaving. The term “disengagement” was
borrowed from (Bjorgo 2002; Bjorgo and Horgan 2009) on extremist groups to distinguish
the process of leaving a gang from that of desisting from crime after de-identifying as a
gang member. A former gang member might indicate that they have left the gang, but
continue to hang out with members of the group, or participate in some gang activities
(and hence, is not fully disengaged). This distinction has been deemed important because
research has shown that leaving a gang does not necessarily equate to leaving a life of
crime (Melde and Esbensen 2014; Sweeten et al. 2013). Additionally, studies indicate that
periods of active gang membership are relatively short, often under one year (Esbensen
and David 1993; Krohn and Thornberry 2008). Researchers stress that leaving the gang is a
process (i.e., a continually changing social activity, as opposed to a static activity or event),
that should be carefully studied, and in conjunction with crime desistance. Fortunately, the
literature examining these concomitant issues has grown in the last five years. However,
the research is still in its infancy.
Published: 26 January 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
93
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
The few studies that take a deeper dive to examine the process of leaving a gang
generally focus on the relationship between levels of disengagement and involvement
in and desistance from crime over time, with little description of the contextual, social
and interpersonal changes that may have accompanied changes in gang membership.
Scant research examines the nature of the social context of peer groups and other relations
that potentially provide support and both deviant and prosocial learning opportunities
across the life course, and how changes in relationships may support gang leaving. In
this paper, we provide a descriptive picture of gang disengagement across youth living
in two mid-eastern cities in the United States, with a specific focus on the social network
characteristics of youth while they are in gangs and during the process of leaving and
disengaging from the gang. We use a social network analysis framework, and specifically,
a personal network research design (PNRD) to study the composition of gang members’
extended personal networks as well as changes in those networks. Personal networks are
known as “ego networks” or “ego-centric networks” and are not whole networks in the
sense that they are not bounded by a group characteristic—such as a gang or organization1 .
Social network analysis refers to both a perspective for examining social relations
and a methodological technique for analyzing those relations. The technique has been
used in a wide range of academic disciplines dating back to the 1930s but is considered
relatively new within the study of criminal behavior (Bouchard and Malm 2016). Relations
between actors create patterns and, eventually, structures that in turn shape the behavior
of individuals (Marsden 1990; Wasserman and Faust 1994). Social network data describe
the contacts, ties, and attachments that one individual has to another. By examining these
data, and recreating the social networks of each individual, researchers can reconstruct
the patterns of interaction and social structures that influence individual behavior. Within
criminology, Krohn (1986) was among the first to suggest that a social network approach
was important for understanding delinquent and criminal behavior.
Although using a network analytical framework is relatively new to criminology,
relationships and the nature of those bonds at least played some role in theorizing about
crime and desistance, particularly with regard to youth delinquency. Social bonding theory
and social learning theory have a clear emphasis on relationships, but it was not until the
work of Warr (1996) and Sarnecki (2001) that scholars began to more clearly emphasize
the importance of bidirectional relations and the resultant interactions and formative
dimensions in creating and maintaining delinquent and criminal behavior. Furthermore,
understanding the attachments youth and young adults have to other relations outside of
peers began to capture more attention as life course and developmental theories emerged
(Roman et al. 2017). A PNRD allows a researcher to elucidate a wide array of social ties
for the sample’s respondents, potentially providing powerful measures of interpersonal
influence that may be strongly predictive of behavior (Valente 2010). The decision to study
ego networks, versus a whole network such as a specific gang, is a purposeful, theoretical
choice to focus on the local, or individual, rather than the global. A nuanced understanding
of the range and extent of social attachments of gang members is also important for policy
and practice; particular relationships or types of social ties may be possible leverage points
for both gang prevention and gang intervention. Furthermore, we know from extant
research that keeping youth from gangs and getting youth to leave gangs after they join are
worthwhile endeavors that can keep youth from increasing their level of involvement in
violence and can lower overall engagement in delinquent behavior (Melde and Esbensen
2014; Sweeten et al. 2013; Valasik et al. 2018; Weerman 2011).
1
In most cases with PNRD, only the ego is the respondent, and all of the information about the alters and their ties to other alters is obtained from the
ego. In contrast, in a survey-based whole network or sociocentric design, every node or actor in the network is a respondent, and information about
the ties between nodes (i.e., alters) is obtained from the alters themselves (although sociometric data can be analyzed with an ego-centric focus).
94
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
2. Background
2.1. Desistance
Researchers have spent more time theorizing about desistance from crime more
generally than about leaving a gang, specifically. Theoretical perspectives on desistance
from crime are relevant to leaving a gang, as work examining desistance from a number
of negative behaviors (e.g., crime, alcohol, drug use, and gangs) suggests that desistance
processes and the causal mechanisms associated with them are similar across different
behaviors. In the following paragraphs, we provide a short overview of the crime desistance
literature to set the stage for a discussion of gang desistance as it relates to the process of
leaving a gang.
In recent years, the application of developmental and life course frameworks has
facilitated an understanding of the dynamic processes related to crime desistance. Such
frameworks provide a context for situating individuals along dimensions of continuity
and change as they age and experience potential turning points. Within these frameworks,
life events are unpredictable, yet salient, features that modify or influence criminal careers.
The timing of these events in the life course and their relationship to other events or
contexts also play a crucial role in behavior. Essentially, a developmental framework
emphasizes non-random change in individuals’ offending behavior across various stages
of development (Loeber and Blanc 1990). The overarching reasons for crime desistance can
be internal (i.e., consistent with human agency) or external (i.e., consistent with structures
or events) to the individual, or provide influence through a complex interplay.
Depending on one’s specific theoretical lens regarding crime desistance, agency and
structure can take on more or less dominant roles. In the early years of theorizing about
desistance, theorists tended to fall on the extremes of the internal versus external forces
spectrum; today there is much less focus on the internal-external debate and more attention
paid to the timing of events and processes across the life course, though some theorists
may still emphasize the sides of the spectrum. Sampson and Laub’s (1990, 1993, 2003)
age-graded theory of crime takes the position that structure—external forces—are most
salient and asserts that crime occurs when bonds to society are weakened or broken. Social
ties are aspects of structural forces generally captured in aggregate form through bonds
to or interaction with age-graded institutions such as marriage. Significant life events or
socialization experiences in adulthood—called turning points—can modify trajectories
of crime in significant ways. These turning points are external—the result of macro-level
institutional processes and the resultant roles (Laub and Sampson 2001, 2003; Teruya and
Hser 2010)—and hence are largely contextual or situation based. Laub and Sampson
view desistance as a process where reductions in offending take place over time and
usually occur much earlier than one’s last criminal offense in one’s criminal career. Hence,
desistance is not simply the termination of one’s criminal career.
Theorists that focus less on structure and more on human agency and identity include
Bushway and Ray (2013), Maruna (2001, 2016), and Giordano et al. (2002, 2007, 2015). Their
perspectives give a more central role to cognitive shifts as internal reevaluations that give
voice to dissatisfaction with some aspects of a current lifestyle or can envision prospects
for an improved life. These reevaluations induce motivation to change. Such cognitive
shifts can be related to maturational processes and/or conscious decisions based on a
reappraisal of the costs and benefits of crime (see Ronald and Cornish 1985). Cognitive
shifts can lead to changes in the external environment or contexts that support or weaken
bonds. Individuals may take on more prosocial roles and relationships or be open to
participation in conventional institutions after initial cognitive shifts. Early work by
Giordano et al. (2002, 2007) gives particular voice to an individual’s own role in selectively
appropriating elements in the environment that act as “hooks for change”. Strengthened or
newly founded relationships can be hooks for change. However, the focus is first on agentic
moves because the emphasis is on how individuals respond to the structural obstacles
they encounter, not the objective social circumstances. Maruna’s (2001, 2004) work focuses
on the psychosocial factors that sustain abstinence from offending, and in particular, the
95
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
cognitive processes that support the identity of a pro-social self. For both Maruna and
Giordano, structural influences are very close behind and sometimes intertwined with
the shift in identity, but Bushway and Paternoster, in contrast, postulate that offenders
first desire to change and that desire to change is accompanied by a shift in identity that
precedes any shift to prosocial networks or prosocial behavior.
Some desistance theorists view changes in offending as more of a balanced interaction
between structural forces and human agency and do not necessarily impose a causal
ordering on the factors that create change (see, for instance, Bottoms et al. 2004; Farrall
2002; McNeill 2006, 2016; McNeill and Weaver 2010; Weaver 2012, 2016). Weaver (2012,
2016; Weaver and McNeill 2015), drawing on relational sociology, suggests that individuals
seek meaningful and consistent ways to refer to themselves (the creation of an identity)
and that this reflexive nature cannot be disentangled from the social context, where social
ties can take center stage. Weaver argues that past theories are restricted in their capacity to
reveal how agentic moves are variously enabled or constrained by the relational contexts
and that more focus should be put on the dynamics and properties of social relations.
2.2. Desistance in the Context of Gang Members
Interestingly, much of the scholarship on desistance does not include direct reference to populations who are gang members—when it is gang members who, while in a
gang, often exhibit active violent offender careers throughout youth and young adulthood.
Furthermore, some of the extant theories do not easily apply to the lives of gang members. For instance, some of the key turning points that have been described in the life
course literature are not readily generalizable to gangs because gang youth are younger
than marital age, tend not to marry, and do not secure the same prosocial opportunities
(e.g., post-secondary education, jobs, military service, etc.) as the average young person
(Carson and Vecchio 2015). Although some of these turning points may be relevant to
desistance from crime for gang members, there is likely even more complexity involved
in understanding crime desistance when one has a peer group or related social networks
that support and reinforce delinquent and criminal behavior. A few recent studies have
broadened the types of turning points to include adverse life events and experiences, such
as incarceration and violent victimization (Sampson and Laub 2016; Soyer 2014; Teruya
and Hser 2010).
It may be that the desistance literature does not have a related body of studies that
include direct reference to gang members because gang desistance has generally been
operationalized as leaving a gang and not necessarily associated with the termination of a
criminal career. As such, gang desistance historically has been viewed as a separate topic in
criminology but intersecting with crime desistance. Scholars began to delve deeper into the
meanings and measures around gang desistance in the early 2000s. Pyrooz and colleagues
suggested that gang de-identification—when an individual declares (or responds on a
survey) that they no longer belong to a gang—is an event to be distinguished from gang
desistance. As described in the introduction, gang desistance falls into the category of
being a process, and should be designated as “disengagement” in that disengagement is a
process that unfolds over time (Pyrooz et al. 2014; Pyrooz and Decker 2011; Sweeten et al.
2013). For some gang members, disengagement may equate to steep reductions in levels of
involvement with the group, but for others, it does not (Decker and Lauritsen 2002; Pyrooz
et al. 2014).
With regard to the gang disengagement process, Decker et al. (2014), in a mixedmethod study of 260 former gang members, drew on the four stages in Ebaugh’s (1988)
role exit theory and applied it to the process of gang disengagement. They described
the gang “exit” process as moving through stages: (1) first doubts, where gang members
contemplate the symbolic and instrumental value of their current role; (2) weighing alternative roles, where gang members engage in anticipatory socialization of new or different
roles; (3) turning points, which function as the crystallization of discontent to act on the
aforementioned considerations; and (4) post-exit certification, which works to validate new
96
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
roles while inoculating gang members from old ones. They indicated the process was not
linear for the gang members in their sample, and that gang members often relapsed to old
expectations and roles. Essentially, they stressed that gang members may see themselves
with many different identities and roles, both conventional and criminal. Gang members
may also vacillate between times of active participation in gang activity and more complete
withdrawal from gang ties even after reporting leaving the gang.
Studies conducted before Decker and colleagues’ study suggest that when perceptions
and objectives of a gang member begin to run opposite to his/her beliefs or priorities,
he/she will leave the gang (Spergel 1995; Vigil 1988). Events such as the victimization of
friends or an increasing commitment to family can lead a gang member to gradually reduce
time spent with other gang members. These earlier studies of the reasons or motivations
for leaving a gang led to the development of a framework to organize the different reasons
youth and adults may leave a gang. The push-pull categorization, originally conceptualized
by Decker and Winkle (1996) with regard to gang entry, divides the reasons for leaving
into those that relate to aspects of gang membership that are internal to the gang (pushes)
and those that are external to the gang (pulls). Negative occurrences related to the gang
are pushes—aspects of life or events that push the individual to begin to desire a more
prosocial life. Pushes can be internal aspects such as disillusionment or maturation. These
individuals reach a point when the costs of engaging in violence outweigh the benefits
(Decker and Lauritsen 2002; Decker and Winkle 1996).
Pull factors are factors that are external to the gang, serving to attract gang members
to prosocial others and institutions. These factors can include having a child, getting a job,
or other turning-point-like factors that help develop or strengthen bonds to conventional
society. Research has shown that push and pull factors often operate in concert (Decker et al.
2014), and one factor may not be sufficient for gang de-identification and/or disengagement.
Consistent with Decker and colleagues’ research mentioned above, recent research by
Roman et al. (2017) that examined reasons for leaving a gang across three large multi-site
datasets found that few respondents reported only push or pull factors, but as the average
age of the sample decreased, so did the number of reasons provided.
Although the push-pull categorization is not grounded in a particular theory or set of
theories, it is apparent that the pull factors closely align with prosocial opportunities and
experiences with new or strengthened social relations that offer opportunities and provide
support for or reinforcement of a new non-gang or non-offender identity. Similarly, social
relations play a salient role in at least two stages of Decker et al.’ (2014) disengagement
framework that utilizes role exit theory. At stage 2, where gang members are engaging
in anticipatory socialization of new or different roles—this “socialization” often involves
assessing and re-assessing their involvement with and attachment to other relations. Stage
4—post-exit validation—involves external reference groups, such as family or new friends
(p. 276). Indeed, it is possible that relations play a significant role across all gang exit
stages and is a key factor in long-term desistance from offending. This aligns with Bersani
and Doherty’s (2018) recent review of the crime desistance literature. When discussing
identity construction, they state: “whereas this divergence between external pressures and
internal progress may emerge from individual narratives, successful desistance may hinge
on external social networks” (2018, p. 321). Their conclusion includes a mention of research
by Giordano et al. (2007) which articulates that emotional maturation is related to the
expansion of social interactions during the developmental period of young adulthood and
particularly where criminal behavior no longer brings positive reinforcement. Similarly,
newer theorizing about crime desistance includes the work of Beth Weaver (2012, 2016), as
mentioned earlier, who draws on relational sociology to posit that individuals seek meaningful and consistent ways to refer to themselves (the creation of an identity) and that this
reflexive nature cannot be disentangled from the social context. Weaver emphasizes social
roles and discusses gang members in that social roles and relationships are particularly relevant for criminal offenders who are embedded in criminal groups/gangs. Her point here
is that these groups by definition, are social groups with roles and identities, and aspects
97
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
of group belonging—belonging to a group comprised of similar social relationships—are
highly relevant for understanding desistance.
2.3. Social Networks and Gangs
As summarized above, social structures or networks have a role in gang leaving
and disengagement but have not taken a prominent position among gang exit theorists.
Social networks can be thought of as representing the intersection between individual and
structural factors, in that it is social relations that tie individuals to their environments.
Relationships, or connections, are a fact of social life. The individuals, or actors, influence
one another and exchange resources. These relationships, and resultant resource exchanges,
are crucial in determining the life trajectory of a gang member pre-gang involvement,
during, and post-gang involvement. Social ties can reinforce gang identity or help nurture
and solidify emerging non-criminal/non-gang identities.
As mentioned in the introduction, in the current study, we are not focused on the gang
itself as a network, but instead on the personal social relations of those individuals who are
gang members. This “ego” centered approach is designed to determine the influence of each
network member (i.e., ego) on the respondent (McCarty 2002). Through an understanding
of how individual gang members are tied to their larger social network (which extends
beyond their gang friends), research can uncover the types of personal network associations
that provide influence on behavior, in this case, the likelihood of remaining in or a leaving
a gang, and the lifestyle associated with it. In our review of the literature, there are less
than a handful of studies that have examined the ego networks of gang members using
self-report methods. Fleisher’s (2002) seminal study of teenage and adult female gang
members in Champaign, Illinois, found that membership in a gang was more symbolic
than real, as many gang individuals had good friends across gang boundaries (i.e., with
individuals from different gangs) and exchanged important resources, such as childcare.
This was the first study to extensively and systematically measure the personal social
relations of gang members through self-report surveys and shed insight into the broader
social and cultural milieu of gang members. Surprisingly since then, the characteristics of
other social relations of the gang member—or delinquent youth even—have rarely been
studied using self-report measures in a network framework (Roman et al. 2016). The recent
scholarship on gang members and networks, for the most part, explores very limited ego
networks of gang members because the networks are defined by participation or linkage to
a criminal incident or event (e.g., arrest or police stop) and/or through administrative or
record data (Bouchard and Malm 2016). Administrative data typically only provide a few
demographic indicators, such as age, sex, race, and residential location, greatly limiting
information on network composition and social roles. For the most part, many of these
incident-based studies are built on socio-centric network methods, where the focus is on
patterns of the whole group or the organization delineated by the extent of the connections
or ties found. Network analysis of this type in the study of gangs as social groups has
proliferated, with studies analyzing the ties among gang members and ties across gangs
through networks of violence and conflict. That literature, because of the stark differences
in aims, is not reviewed here. (See Sierra-Arévalo and Papachristos 2017 for a review).
As stated earlier, the goal of this paper is to provide an in-depth description of the
social relations of gang members and how they change (or do not change) after members
report they have left their gang. More specifically, we describe the composition of the
networks at baseline and changes to network composition at wave 2 associated with
varying levels of gang disengagement. We aim to distinguish between gang-leavers who
remain attached to their gang peers through continued interaction with their gang peers
and those who say they are fully detached and no longer engage with their (ex-) gang peers.
In addition to examining the stated reasons for leaving a gang, which may include reasons
associated with social networks (e.g., “I made new friends”), the social network data allow
us to examine the characteristics of individuals dropped from networks over time and why
those network relations were dropped (Feld et al. 2007). We are particularly interested in
98
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
the presence and strength of prosocial network relations across the two different levels of
disengagement for those who reported leaving their gang between waves.
3. Methods
3.1. Sample and Survey Design
The current study uses survey data from a longitudinal survey and interview project
designed to obtain social network data from male and female youth and young adults
between the ages of 14 and 25 who were members of street gangs. The study was known as
the Connect Survey (Eidson et al. 2017). Data were collected in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
and the District of Columbia (DC). Participants were interviewed three times at least six
months apart beginning in mid-2013. Although the project consisted of three waves of data
collection, this paper focuses only on the first two waves. The average number of days
elapsed between the wave 1 and wave 2 surveys across all respondents was 8.8 months.
Respondents were recruited for Wave 1 by street outreach workers affiliated with
gang reduction programs in each city. Outreach workers, most of whom were ex-gang
members, were trained to recruit gang youth who they deemed were in a street gang, as
generally defined by the components of the Eurogang consensus definition (Weerman
et al. 2009), or, regularly hung out with people considered to be in a street gang. There
are four main elements to the Eurogang consensus definition: (a) durable: has been in
existence for at least several months, (b) street-oriented: spending group time outside of
locations that have adult supervision (and does not necessarily have to be on the street );
(c) youth: most of the group consists of individuals in their teens and early twenties; and
(d) illegal activity is part of its group identity in that behavior is deemed criminal, and
not simply bothersome, but is part of the group culture. The research team had a number
of conversations with the outreach workers to ascertain whether their definition of street
gangs was similar to the criteria to meet the Eurogang definition. Key to the recruitment of
study participants was the concept of illegal identity. In both cities, we knew from these
discussions that “gang” would not be a typical term used by street groups. Wave 1 survey
data revealed that the most common terms group members used for their peer group were
“clique” (34%), followed by “crew” (23%). Only 19% referred to their street group as a
gang. Other terms used by respondents included “organization” and “squad”. To enhance
the likelihood that the youth met our street gang definition before they sat down for the
survey, the research team used a screening tool to determine final sample inclusion. In
addition to the age requirement, the youth had to answer in the affirmative to having a
peer group that they currently hang out with and, at minimum, one of the following items:
ever involved in at least some type of illegal activity (individual or group activity), have
friends in a gang, or calls their peer group a “gang” or “crew”. The screening also helped
identify individuals who may have been eligible but were unwilling to honestly report
their behaviors on a survey—important to a study whose primary focus was examining the
process of leaving a gang, thereby necessitating the identification of those in street gangs.
Note that the research team did not mandate that the study participants met the official
self-reported Eurogang definition of a street gang.
Respondents were invited by outreach workers to meet the research team that day
or at a later date to complete a self-guided, computer-based survey. Participation in the
survey was voluntary. Parental consent and youth assent were obtained from youth under
18 and individual consent was obtained from those 18 or older. Respondents were paid
50 US dollars at each survey wave.
Recruitment for the study at baseline yielded 229 individuals across the two sites who
passed the screening criteria. For the second wave, we employed a variety of methods to
attempt to locate survey respondents after the first wave. Each site had at least three core
research team members working part-time to re-engage the sample. Efforts included meeting with the outreach workers to expand re-recruitment efforts and find out whether any
individuals who took the survey had moved away, were incarcerated, or were otherwise
99
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
unable to take the survey. All research protocols were approved by the RAND Corporation
human subjects review board.
The response rate for wave 2 reached just under 50% for the sample across both
cities; we obtained valid survey data for 112 respondents at wave 2. For the current
paper, however, we dropped one respondent who had a high level of missing data across
most variables, and as a result, the paper analyzes baseline data on 228 individuals and
111 respondents at wave 2. More details on the recruitment and re-contact/follow-up
strategy can be found in Eidson et al. (2017). That paper also includes details on the
attrition analyses conducted. The analyses, conducted to determine the relative risk of
being lost to follow-up given key baseline demographic characteristics, found the only
significant factor to be sex (males were less likely to return) and attachment to work or
school (those not attached to either institution were more likely to be lost to follow-up). In
addition, there were no significant differences in attrition by site.
Survey data were collected using EgoNet software (McCarty 2003) and Qualtrics
(Provo, UT). At each wave, the survey included three sections. The first section included
questions about the respondent (or “ego”). Questions were asked about demographic
characteristics, living arrangement and environment, work, school and leisure activities,
delinquency, group characteristics and involvement, and attitudes toward gangs and gang
joining. The second section, the alter section, asked about the respondent’s current social
network: at each administration, respondents were asked to identify 20 individuals (or
“alters”) that were important to the respondent and were at least 10 years of age. The
alter elicitation text was read as follows: Please list 20 people important to you and who are at
least 10 years old. Start by thinking of the people you hang out with or might see regularly in a
typical day. Then, think of the people you talk to or see the most. For example, you can name family
members, friends, neighbors, or even people you don’t like. Respondents were encouraged, but
not required, to provide 20 names. In total, 87% of the sample listed 20 names, with another
7% listing between 15 and 19 names. Following the identification of network members,
respondents were asked a series of questions about each alter. These questions, which
capture aspects of network composition, comprise a core component of measuring and
understanding personal networks (McCarty 2002). The alter question items are listed in
Table 1. In the third section of the survey protocol, respondents were asked about each
alter’s ties to one another, which results in the information about network structure (not
examined in this paper).
The wave 2 survey instrument included an additional component comprised of questions about changes in the lists of alters generated by the respondents between waves.
These questions provide important information about the reasons gang members break
away from their prior associates and other social relations. These questions allow us to
closely examine the reasons behind changing network composition for those who reported
leaving their gang and those who did not, as well as give us the opportunity to consider
the disengagement process and possible network-based drivers behind crime desistance
for individuals. Below we highlight key measures that will be examined in this paper.
100
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
Table 1. Connect Survey Question Items about Alters.
1. What are __________’s nicknames or other names that friends and family use to refer to
__________?
2. How old is __________?
3. What grade or year is __________?
4. Is ___________ male or female?
5. Can you name one thing to describe __________ so that we can tell the difference between this
__________ and another __________?
6. Who is __________? (relationship)
7. Does __________ live in your neighborhood?
8. Does __________ live with you?
9. How did you meet __________?
10. Is __________ of Hispanic, Latino, or Spanish origin or descent?
11. What country was __________ born in?
12. How much time do you spend each week hanging out with __________?
13. How much do you like __________?
14. If you needed some information or advice about something, is __________ someone you could
go to?
15. How likely is it that __________ carries a gun (including in his/her car)?
16. Has __________ ever sold illegal drugs such as marijuana, cocaine, or crack?
17. How likely is it that __________ has been in a gang fight over the last year?
18. How likely is it that __________ is currently in a gang?
19. How likely is it that you use drugs with __________?
20. How likely is it that you drink alcohol with __________?
21. When you are with __________, what do you do most often?
22. When you are with __________, what else do you do most often?
23. Have you ever in your life committed a crime with __________? Please think of any crime that
you know is against the law.
24. How supportive do you think __________ is of you being involved in a group of friends such
as a gang or crew that participates in illegal activities such as a gang or crew? If you are not in a
group like this or if __________ doesn’t know you are, how supportive do you think __________
would be if s/he knew you were in this kind of group?
3.2. Measures
3.2.1. Gang Leaving and Disengagement
The Connect Survey included items to assess whether a respondent left their peer
group that was a gang and whether they remained engaged with their group, even if they
reported leaving. Respondents were asked about their group that was deemed a gang
in wave 1 (their main peer group with whom they engage in illegal activity). At wave
2, we asked respondents to recall the group named at wave 1 (with prompts) and then
we asked: Since the last survey, have you left or quit the group that you described in the survey?
For those respondents who reported having left their wave 1 group, we asked them to
describe their current “level of involvement” with that group. There were five response
options ranging from “I never participate in anything the group does” to “I participate in
nearly everything that the group does”. Respondents who reported they never participate
in anything were classified as “disengaged”, and the other four response options were
collapsed and classified as “remaining engaged” in the analyses that follow. It is also
important to note that in the remaining narrative, we use the term “group” and “gang”
interchangeably.
The wave 2 survey instrument also included a set of 16 questions asking respondents
who reported leaving their gang or group why they left. The items are all binary yes/no
questions and respondents were directed to choose “all that apply”. The question items
included I left the group because I/my . . . : (1) found new interests, (2) was bored, (3)
something happened of which I wanted no part, (4) wasn’t what I thought it was going to
be, (5) was hurt, (6) family or friends hurt or killed, (7) got into trouble with the police, (8)
went to prison, (9) was forced out, (10) got a job, (11) had/am expecting a baby, (12) parents
101
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
made me, (13) partner made me, (14) had an adult encourage me to leave, (15) made new
friends, and (16) moved.
3.2.2. Other Group Characteristics
Although not used in analyses in the current study, we included a few measures on
group characteristics purely for baseline description purposes. These measures included:
(1) Does the group have a territory it claims as its own? (yes/no); (2) in the past six months,
has your group provided protection for each other? (yes/no); and (3) in the past six months,
has your group defended an area or place against other groups? (yes/no).
3.2.3. Delinquency and Crime
Delinquency and crime were measured at both the individual level and the group
level. We asked individuals about their own lifetime participation in crime and recent
(past six months) involvement in crime. The individual-level criminal behavior measures
included are: (1) sold illegal drugs, (2) motor vehicle theft, (3) participated in a gang fight,
(4) carried a weapon without a license, (5) used a weapon or force to steal or rob someone,
and (6) attacked someone with a weapon to hurt or kill them. We used these items to
construct two sets of binary measures across the crime types for “lifetime” and “recent”
involvement. Although not used in analyses, we also included baseline demographic
measures for “currently on probation or parole” (yes/no) and whether the respondent was
ever arrested for a violent crime (yes/no).
With regard to group involvement in illegal activity, we created a binary measure
for any recent group-based crime, which was created from seven binary survey items
asking the respondent whether, in the past six months, his/her group had done any of
the following: (1) been in fights with gangs/crews, (2) damaged or destroyed property, (3)
stolen things, (4) stolen cars or motorcycles, (5) robbed other people, (6) sold marijuana, (7)
and sold other illegal drugs.
3.2.4. Personal Network Composition
Composition: Alter Role Characteristics, Exposure to Prosocial Ties, and Exposure to
Anti-Social Behavior
Characteristics of the respondent’s personal networks with regard to roles and exposure to criminal behavior were operationalized from survey question items that asked
about each of the alters named. As listed in Table 1, there were a number of questions that
simply asked who the alter is and the type of relationship the alter has with the respondent
(how met, how long known, type of relation, age, race, sex, etc.) For the current paper,
we focus on three mutually exclusive roles defined from ego-alter ties: family, peers, and
mentors. We also create a measure representing tie dispersion across these three different
types of ties using Blau’s index of heterogeneity (“Blau’s H”) (Blau 1977). Its computational
formula is simply:
H = 1 − P1 2 − P2 2 − P3 2 − . . . . . . .. − Pr 2
where P1 is the proportion of ties in relation i members in r relation category. Values can
range from zero to a maximum of 1 − 1/r if each group has the same number of ties. In
our study, we measure three types of relations, hence the values range from 0 to 0.67. If all
ties are in one group, the value will be 0, versus higher values for more equal distribution
across the three types of relations.
To capture network exposure (Valente 2010) to criminal behavior, we utilized the four
alter questions about alters’ criminal behavior (carry gun, in a gang, co-offend with ego,
sell drugs). We used ordinal response categories: “not at all likely” (0); “somewhat likely”
(1); “very likely” (2); and “don’t know”. Response values 1 and 2 were recoded to “1”
indicating involvement in the behavior. “Don’t know” was recoded as “not at all likely”
(or a 0 value) to err on the conservative side. We also included a measure to capture the
likelihood that the alter supports the respondent’s gang lifestyle.
102
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
To create summary personal network measures for these alter variables (with exception
of tie dispersion), each binary variable was summed across all alters and divided by the
respondent’s number of alters, yielding proportional values ranging from 0.00 to 1.00. In
summary, these alter compositional and ego-alter tie measures include:
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Family in network is the proportion of alters who were listed as a parent/guardian,
sibling, cousin, aunt/uncle, or grandparents;
Peers in network is the proportion of alters who were listed as a “friend”;
Prosocial relations/ties was defined as ties listed as coaches, teachers, counselors, and
outreach workers. This variable was designated “mentorly relations” to distinguish it
substantively from other possible prosocial ties (e.g., parents, older siblings, etc.);
Tie dispersion across peer, family, and, mentorly relations;
Same household relations is the proportion of alters who live in the respondent’s household;
Same neighborhood relations is the proportion of alters who live in the same neighborhood;
Gun carrying alters is the proportion of alters where respondent indicated very likely
or somewhat likely to carry a gun;
Alters in a gang is the proportion of alters where respondent indicated very likely or
somewhat likely to currently be in a gang;
Co-offenders is the proportion of alters where respondent indicated “ever committed
crime with”. Note that the response options provided were yes/no;
Drug-selling alters is the proportion of alters where respondent indicated that alter
very likely or somewhat likely sells illegal drugs;
Supports gang lifestyle of respondent is the proportion of all alters who were somewhat
likely or very likely to support the gang lifestyle of the respondent, as reported by the
respondent. (Respondents were not provided a “don’t know” option).
Composition: Strength of Network Ties
Following research by Granovetter (1983) and other network scholars (Mathews
K. Michael et al. 1998; Wellman and Wortley 1990), we also included three measures
representing the strength of network ties. Granovetter asserts that tie strength generally
includes four properties: amount of time spent with someone; emotional intensity of the
relationship, intimacy (e.g., friend vs. best friend), and whether reciprocal services are
provided or the relationship itself is reciprocated. Other theorists and researchers have
suggested that plausible indicators of tie strength also include emotional support or advice
given and/or received. A measure of “high interaction alter” was created from the item
asking about how much time is spent with the alter. Response options ranged from every
day to “don’t really ever see this person”. A high-interaction relation was designated
where the respondent stated they see the alter “every day”. Alters were also designated an
“advice network relation” when a respondent stated they go to that individual for advice
(response options were “yes/no”). Last, a dichotomous measure for “dislikable relation”
was created for each alter when a respondent, when asked: How much do you like person
X? (a whole lot, some, not at all), reported they did not like an alter. This measure was
included because prior research by Fleisher (2002) has shown that not all gang members
like their gang peers and these adverse relationships may correlate to reasons for leaving a
gang.
3.2.5. Changes in Social Networks
To understand a range of possible differences between the social relations elicited at
each wave of the survey, at wave 2, respondents were first asked to name 20 alters, using
the same questions to elicit alters as used for wave 1. Respondents were shown their wave
1 alter list only after they had finished naming alters for wave 2. Respondents were then
asked to compare their wave 1 and wave 2 lists and identify the individuals that were
the same at each wave, those wave 1 alters who had been dropped by wave 2, and any
103
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
individuals who were new at wave 2. If respondents had dropped alters at wave 2, we
then asked individuals a series of questions about why they did not name that person.
3.2.6. Demographics
Basic demographic variables include age, sex, ethnicity (Latino = 1), marital/significant
relationship status, and site (DC = 1; Philadelphia = 0). Because the sample age range
was wide (14 to 25 years of age), to measure attachment to the traditional institutions of
school or work, we combined two items to form a dichotomous measure as to whether
the respondent was either in school or employed. If either, the variable was coded “1”.
We created a variable indicating whether a respondent lived with his/her parents and/or
other family members (e.g., siblings, cousins, aunts/uncles, and grandparents). We also
created binary variables indicating whether the respondent had children and if so, whether
they provided financial support for the children.
3.3. Analytic Strategy
In order to provide useful information about the personal relationships of those
respondents who reported leaving their group and the changes specific to their networks
over the elapsed time between waves 1 and 2 (roughly nine months), we first provide
descriptive information on the full baseline sample and those who reported leaving their
group, and then we examine any differences between those who reported leaving but
still hang out with members of their group (“engaged”) versus those who left but do not
associate with their old gang peers or participate in their activities (“disengaged”). We
provide tables to then highlight the reported reasons for leaving their group as well as
why some network members disappeared from the ego networks between waves and
focus on changes across the range of network composition variables. We do this by
making use of descriptive statistics (i.e., measures of frequency, central tendency, and
dispersion), and where appropriate, utilize t-tests to determine significant differences in
network compositional factors between waves. Where relevant, we examine differences
between self-reported group-leavers who remain engaged with their group versus those
reporting being fully disengaged. With regard to missing data, when a respondent had
missing values for key measures, we dropped the respondent(s) with missing data from
the particular statistical calculation and noted missing in the table. We chose not to conduct
imputation given that our analyses are descriptive and we wanted to provide an accurate
representation in the descriptive portrait.
Given the salience of social networks in the recent desistance literature as possible
“hooks for change” (Giordano et al. 2015; Weaver 2016), we expect to see a number of
network changes at wave 2 for those individuals who have completely disengaged. Specifically, we hypothesize that for those respondents who reported they left their group and no
longer participate in that group’s activities, there will be more network members dropped
between waves, and an increase in the proportion of network members considered prosocial (“mentorly”), as compared to those who remain engaged. For those who are fully
disengaged, we also expect a reduction in network members who are peers and reductions
in members who support the respondent’s “gang lifestyle”. We expect that any reduction
in peers translates to an increase in network members who are parents or considered
family because, in the short time between waves, it is not likely that respondents gain new
friendships that have solidified enough to be easily named at wave 2. We also examine the
reasons why some of our respondents’ relations were not named at wave 2 and whether
the reasons differed across disengaged group-leavers versus those who remain participants in group-based activities of the group they reported leaving. Here, we expect that
group-disengaged respondents will be more likely to report that they have new friends,
or their “old” friends did something they did not like or did not want to be a part of, or
hung out with people they do not like, than those who reported leaving their group, but
still participate in things the group does.
104
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
4. Results
4.1. Baseline Characteristics
Table 2 provides demographic characteristics of respondents at baseline (i.e., wave
1) including the individual-level offending and group-based characteristics of the sample.
With regard to individual-level offending, no more than half the sample reported ever
engaging in the following criminal activities: sold drugs (40%), stole a motor vehicle
(30%), robbery (47%), aggravated assault (“attacked someone to serious hurt/kill”—46%).
Roughly one-third of the sample had been arrested for a violent crime at some time in
their life, and one-third reported being on probation or parole at baseline. Nearly 70% of
respondents at baseline reported that they belonged to a group that committed, in the last
six months, at least one group-based illegal activity.
Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Sample at Baseline, n = 228.
Demographics
Philadelphia site (versus DC)
Average age
Male
African American
Hispanic/Latino(a)
Married or in serious relationship
Has child(ren)
Of those with children, supports them
Lives with parents and/or other family
In school or has job
Individual offending and group behavior
Sold drugs
Stole a motor vehicle
Carried a gun, last 6 months
Used force or weapon to rob
Attacked someone to seriously hurt, kill
Gang fight
Arrested for robbery or aggravated assault
On probation or parole
Group has committed any of 7 crimes, a last 6 months
Group claims territory
Group protected each other, last 6 months
Group defended an area against other groups, last 6 months
Wave 1
N
57.89%
19.35
65.07%
64.91%
19.74%
35.37%
31.0%
60.0%
83.41%
59.03%
228
228
228
228
227
228
228
228
228
227
40.27%
30.09%
34.65%
47.35%
46.02%
58.41%
32.02%
34.80%
69.91%
58.22%
76.44%
52.89%
226
226
227
226
226
226
227
227
225
225
225
225
Notes: a Group crimes include: gang fights, property damage, theft, auto theft, robbery, sold marijuana, and sold
other illegal drugs.
Table 3 provides descriptive information on the composition of the respondents’ ego
networks. The values aggregate the average proportion of alters in each respondent’s
network with those characteristics. The table also shows frequencies for respondents
reporting zero alters with the listed characteristics, as well as those reporting that all (100%)
of their alters possessed that characteristic. As discussed in the measures section, these
characteristics are those as described (i.e., reported on) by the ego; this study did not
collect information directly from alters. On average at wave 1, respondents’ networks
were majority male (0.67) and African American (0.76), reflecting the makeup of the
respondents themselves. One quarter of ego networks were, on average, comprised of
alters reported to be Latinx. In general, respondents reported networks that were roughly
half family; similarly, networks, on average, were half peers. A sum of 27 respondents
(12%) reported that none of their alters were peers. This roughly corresponds to the
number of alters who reported that their entire network was comprised of family members
(23 respondents). Respondents, on average, spent a lot of time with just over half of their
network members (0.59) and would go to roughly the same proportion for advice (0.60).
Only four respondents (roughly 2%) reported that there was no one in their network they
went to for advice. On average, ego networks were comprised of very few alters who were
disliked by respondents (0.11) and even lower proportion (0.01), on average, were listed as
105
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
coaches, teachers, counselors, or outreach workers—“mentorly”. A large majority reported
having no (0) mentorly alters (89%).
Table 3. Compositional Characteristics and Network Exposure at Wave 1, n = 227 a .
Respondents’ Alters Who . . .
Average
Proportion b
(S.D.)
Freq. Reporting No (0)
Alters with Characteristic
No. (%)
Freq. Reporting All Alters
with Characteristic
No. (%)
Are male
Are African American
Are Hispanic/Latino
Are respondent’s parents
Are family
Are peers
Are mentorly
Live with respondent
Carry a gun
Is in a gang
Commit crimes with respondent
Sell drugs
Are supportive gang lifestyle
You spend lots of time with
You go to for advice
You do not like
Lives in neighborhood
Tie dispersion c (family, peers, mentors)
0.67 (0.26)
0.76 (0.31)
0.27 (0.31)
0.06 (0.11)
0.45 (0.34)
0.46 (0.31)
0.01 (0.03)
0.17 (0.21)
0.39 (0.34)
0.28 (0.30)
0.27 (0.32)
0.29 (0.31)
0.62 (0.34)
0.59 (0.28)
0.60 (0.27)
0.11 (0.17)
0.49 (0.27)
0.29 (0.19)
5 (2.20%)
12 (5.29)
62 (27.31)
124 (54.63)
21 (9.25)
27 (11.89)
201 (88.55)
62 (27.31)
41 (18.06)
62 (27.31)
76 (33.48)
53 (23.35)
18 (7.93)
2 (0.90)
4 (1.79)
97 (42.73)
7 (3.08)
-
32 (14.10%)
67 (29.52)
7 (3.08%)
0
23 (10.13)
5 (2.20)
0
1 (0.44)
13 (5.73)
6 (2.64)
8 (3.52)
9 (3.96)
44 (19.38)
21 (9.42)
17 (7.62)
0
7 (3.08)
-
Notes: a Alter values missing for one respondent. b With the exception of tie dispersion, values are central
tendencies calculated as the average proportion of each characteristic across all respondents’ ego networks. S.D.,
standard deviation. c Tie dispersion is the mean value across all respondents, with values ranging from 0 to 0.67.
Although the proportion of mentorly alters was low on average across networks, ego
networks were not fully comprised of relations who would support a respondent’s gang
lifestyle. The average proportion of ego networks made up of relations who would support
their gang lifestyle was 0.62, indicating that at least a third, on average, do not support
the respondent’s gang lifestyle. Nonetheless, on average a quarter to one-third of one’s
network was likely to be engaged in some type of criminal behavior (carrying an illegal
gun, in a gang, committing crimes as a co-offender, or selling drugs). Eight respondents
reported that their entire network was comprised of individuals they commit crimes with.
4.2. Gang Leaving and Disengagement
Turning now to wave 2, Table 4 shows that just under 30% of respondents (n = 30)
self-reported leaving their group. Of those 30 who reported leaving, 13 (43%) reported that
they did not participate in anything that their wave 1 group did (“disengaged”), with 17
(57%) reporting they participate in some to all of the things their group does (“engaged”).
Table 4. Gang-leaving and Disengagement at Wave 2, n = 111.
Self-reported left W1 group
Self-reported left W1 group and never participates in anything group does
Self-reported left W1 group but continues to participate in things group does
Freq.
Percent
30
13
17
27.68%
43.33%
56.67%
Because an important substantive question among gang scholars has been whether levels of post-gang-leaving engagement signify changes in criminal activity, we first examine
differences in individual-level criminal behavior between waves for gang-leavers compared to non-gang-leavers (Table 5). The statistics reported here at both waves only include
those individuals who completed wave 2. The table then breaks down the gang-leavers
between those who disengaged from group activities versus those who remained tied to
their groups. When the disengaged versus engaged respondents are not disaggregated,
looking solely at group-leavers, there are no noticeable changes in participation in criminal
activity between waves 1 and 2. For those remaining in their groups (n = 80), there were a
number of changes at wave 2, with more individuals selling drugs, stealing cars, carrying
a gun, robbing and assaulting people in the time between wave 1 and wave 2 than in
106
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
the six months before wave 1. The lower half of the table shows a general withdrawal
from criminal activity by those reporting they were fully disengaged from their groups.
In contrast, across a number of crime types, those who reported leaving their groups but
not disentangling themselves from group activity reported more participation at wave 2
(compared to wave 1) for all crime types except robbery.
Table 5. Changes in Offending Between Wave 1 and Wave 2, Group-Leavers and Non-Leavers.
Group-Leavers (n = 30)
Individual offending behavior, last 6 months
Sold drugs
Stole a motor vehicle
Carried a gun, last 6 months
Used force or weapon to rob
Attacked someone to seriously hurt, kill
Gang fight
a
Missing data on one individual.
Gang-leavers, broken down by engagement: (last 6 months)
Sold drugs
Stole a motor vehicle
Carried a gun
Used force or weapon to rob
Attacked someone to seriously hurt, kill
Gang fight
Non-Leavers (n = 80) a
Wave 1
%
Wave 2
%
Wave 1
%
Wave 2
%
23.33
20.00
33.33
30.00
36.67
33.33
23.33
23.33
33.33
26.67
36.67
26.67
16.25
8.75
26.25
27.50
30.00
22.50
39.24
20.25
31.65
18.99
32.91
27.85
Disengaged (n = 13)
Still engaged (n = 17)
Wave 1
%
Wave 2
%
Wave 1
%
Wave 2
%
23.08
23.08
38.46
30.77
46.15
30.77
7.69
7.69
7.69
7.69
15.38
0
23.53
17.65
29.41
35.29
29.41
35.29
35.29
25.29
52.94
41.18
52.94
47.06
We examined further the differences between disengaged gang-leavers and engaged
gang-leavers by assessing their reported reasons for leaving the group. Recall from the
measures section that respondents were given a list of 16 possible reasons they could
have left their group and were asked to choose all that apply. The results, shown in
Table 6 highlight a number of differences between the disengaged group-leavers and those
that remain involved. Interestingly, all reasons except “an adult encouraged me” were
represented by a higher percentage of engaged group-leavers than disengaged. Engaged
leavers were also more likely to choose more reasons (as opposed to fewer) for leaving
than disengaged leavers.
Table 6. Reasons for Leaving a Self-Reported Group-Leavers, Disengaged vs. Engaged.
Push Reasons for Leaving b
Found new interests
Bored
It wasn’t what I thought
Something happened I didn’t like
Was hurt
Friends/family hurt
Police harassment/pressure
Went to prison/jail
Forced out by group
Pull Reasons for Leaving b
Got a job
Expecting a baby/had a baby
Made new friends
Moved (home or school)
Parent(s) made me
Significant other made me
Adult encouraged me to leave
Summary
Total pushes (mean)
Total pulls (mean)
% respondents listing pushes only
% respondents listing pulls only
a
Does Not Participate
n = 13
Remains a Participant
n = 17
53.85%
30.77
30.77
38.46
7.69
7.69
15.38
15.38
7.69
76.47%
47.06
52.94
47.06
41.18
58.82
41.18
41.18
23.53
23.08
38.46
38.46
7.69
15.38
23.08
46.15
64.71
52.94
52.94
35.29
25.29
52.94
29.41
2.08
1.92
7.69%
7.69%
4.29
3.24
0
0
Reasons for leaving group are not mutually exclusive; respondents could choose all that apply.
rows are based on valid responses.
107
b
Percentages in
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
4.3. Changes in Network Composition
Table 7 provides information on how ego networks changed across waves for those
group-leavers who fully disengaged from their group, compared to those who remained
involved. Not surprisingly, group leaving corresponded to a large percentage of alters
from wave 1 being dropped by wave 2. On average, disengaged group-leavers dropped
76% of their alters, whereas engaged group-leavers dropped a bit lower percentage at 64%.
However, some alters were dropped simply because respondents forgot about them or
their social networks may have comprised more than 20 relations and had not yet listed
that person who had been listed in wave 1 (the survey protocol capped alters at 20). After
“forgot to name that person”, for disengaged gang-leavers, the next most frequent reason
that an individual was dropped from a respondent’s network was “changed group of
friends” (15%), and alter “moved” (12%). Notably, fewer engaged group-leavers than
disengaged leavers reported they changed their group of friends (8%). Another prominent
difference between disengaged and engaged gang-leavers was the percentage of alters who
respondents reported they had completely severed ties with (i.e., the relationship is over):
39% for disengaged gang-leavers versus 23% for those leavers who remained engaged with
their gang peers.
Table 7. Reasons Why Respondents’ Dropped Alters at Wave 2, Group-leavers, Disengaged versus
Engaged, Averaged Across Ego Networks.
Avg. number of W1 alters dropped by W2
Avg. percent of W1 alters dropped by W2
At W2, I didn’t name that W1 person because . . .
. . . I forgot to name that person
. . . I already named 20 people
. . . that person did something I don’t like
. . . I changed my group of friends
. . . that person moved
. . . I don’t like that person
. . . that person hangs out with people I don’t like
. . . that person is an ex-boy/girlfriend
. . . we grew apart
. . . that person is in jail/prison
. . . that person is deceased
Relationship is over
a
a
Leavers,
Disengaged
n = 13
Leavers,
Still Engaged
n = 17
13.92
75.65%
12.71
63.92%
38.02%
6.00
4.64
15.07
12.19
2.14
7.83
0
1.56
1.71
0
39.25%
28.69%
14.36
4.93
7.88
11.12
2.28
2.73
2.45
1.88
2.65
0
23.17%
Respondents could select more than one reason for dropping an alter at wave 2.
The last set of analyses utilize t-tests to examine significant changes in the composition
of networks between the two waves. Significance tests were set up as single sample
tests to examine changes to personal network composition for respondents within each
category of gang-leaver over the two waves. We test the null hypothesis that change equals
zero. The results of the t-tests (Table 8) show that for disengaged group-leavers, there are
a number of significant differences pertaining to changes in network composition that
occurred between waves. At wave 2, compared to wave 1, disengaged group-leavers
were significantly more likely to name parents as social ties and people with mentorship
roles (i.e., coaches, teachers, outreach workers, and counselors). This increase in relations
designated parents and mentors by respondents was accompanied by a decrease in alters
designated as peers (though this decrease was not significant) and an overall significant
increase in tie dispersion across family, peer, and mentorly relations.
108
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
Table 8. Changes in Composition of Respondents’ Networks between Waves.
Average Change in Proportion a of
Respondents’ Alters Who . . .
Are respondent’s parents
Are family
Are peers
Are mentorly
Live with respondent
Carry a gun
Is in a gang
Commit crimes with respondent
Sell drugs
Are supportive gang lifestyle
You spend lots of time with
You go to for advice
You do not like
Lives in neighborhood
Tie dispersion (family, peers, mentors)
Leavers,
Disengaged
n = 13
Leavers,
Still Engaged
n = 17
0.13 *
−0.03
−0.14
0.40 ***
0.00
−0.20
−0.31 **
−0.16
−0.24 *
−0.16
−0.09
−0.03
−0.10
−0.08
0.13 *
−0.04
−0.12
0.29 †
0.28 ***
−0.01
0.05
0.05
0.06
0.03
0.08
−0.11
0.00
−0.02
−0.09
0.33 ***
Notes: a With the exception of the tie dispersion measure, values are the average change between waves in the
average proportion of characteristics across respondents’ ego networks. t-tests were calculated separately for
each group (as single sample t-tests for disengaged and engaged, respectively) testing the hypothesis that change
in means between waves was equal to 0. † p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Group-disengaged respondents were less likely to name alters who were in a gang,
co-offenders, sold drugs, and supported their gang lifestyle, though only alters in a gang
and alter sold drugs reached statistical significance. Very notably, for those who reported
leaving their group but were not disengaged, there were no decreases in the proportion of
social ties with any of the compositional attributes related to criminal behavior. There was
a significant increase in mentorly ties for those not fully disengaged, but this increase was
not as steep as the increase for those fully disengaged from their group. Furthermore, the
number of peers increased and these changes in relation types likely influenced the change
in tie dispersion, with a significant mean change in the dispersion of 0.33. It is not likely
that the peers added to the networks of the still-engaged gang-leavers in wave 2 were
positive role models, because the change statistic for the proportion of alters supportive of
gang lifestyle increased.
Overall, given the apparently reduced exposure to criminal behavior and large increase
in mentorly and parental ties for the fully disengaged respondents, compared to the
minimal positive changes for those who remained engaged, these results confirm our
hypotheses that a complete withdrawal from interaction with old gang members is the
status or state that is likely accompanied by large changes in the composition of social
networks. These changes are in the prosocial direction—toward shedding anti-social ties
and increasing the number or proportion of ties that can offer positive opportunities and
perhaps, life-changing opportunities.
5. Limitations
Before we discuss the findings, there are study limitations to mention. First, the
current study does not draw from a random sample of gang members; hence the findings
are not representative of the street gang population in the United States nor of street gangs
in Philadelphia or DC. Relatedly, with regard to generalizing, the findings of our study
are dependent on how network ties were elicited and defined. Our study specifically and
purposely asked respondents to name 20 social relations—broad and not role-based (i.e.,
parent, friend, relative, etc.—who are important to them. Though not all respondents
named 20, the vast majority did (88%), creating a larger ego network than is typical in
network studies from any social science field involving youth and young adults (with the
exception of studies about online relations). Second, we had roughly 50% attrition at wave 2,
109
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
but we are confident, given the attrition analyses (Eidson et al. 2017) and a deeper, informal,
qualitative assessment of who could not be reach for follow-up, that the attrition was not
biased in a way that would affect the analyses conducted. Third, alter characteristics were
operationalized through reports by the egos, not the alters themselves; some researchers
have questioned the utility of these types of perceptual measures in network studies and the
tendency for youth to overestimate their peers’ behaviors (Baerveldt et al. 2004). However,
a study examining this particular issue (Young and Weerman 2013) found that perceptions
are an important factor in peer and group-based behavior and that the network approach
eliciting perceptions about social ties remains a valid approach to social inquiry of peer
behaviors. The authors found that overestimation of friends’ deviant behavior may even
be a cause of one’s own deviant behavior.
Forth, the survey techniques used in the Connect Survey do not provide information
on the exact timing of when respondents de-identified as a part of the gang peer group
after wave 1. Hence, we cannot establish a temporal ordering in group leaving and
changes in ego networks. Future quantitative and qualitative network research on gang
disengagement could collect survey data using a monthly calendaring data collection tool
to help unpack disengagement and crime desistance processes and temporal ordering.
Last, our study only provides a short time span (under one year) from which to examine
the process of gang disengagement. We were unable to draw associations to aspects and
constructs discussed in Decker et al.’s (2014) study of role transitions and the stages of gang
exit (such as post-exit validation). Given that studies show youth can move back into gangs
(i.e., re-identify as gang members or join another gang) after periods of disengagement, it
is possible that even our study’s 13 fully-disengaged former group members re-identify as
group members at some future point in time.
6. Discussion
This two-site study was designed to describe the network composition of gang members and former gang members as they move through the process of disengagement and
crime desistance. Even with the limitations outlined above, we view this study as a step
toward advancing research on gang disengagement and desistance. Although the sample
was relatively small and purposive, we were able to successfully recruit and collect a range
of detailed social network data from the youth and young adults who typically are not
part of the longitudinal studies cited in criminology because they are not attached to social
institutions or are simply hard-to-reach through representative sampling frames.
Among the individuals who were retained at wave 2, 30 (28%) reported leaving
their gang by wave 2. But not all respondents stopped participating in their wave 1 peer
group’s activities—of the 30 individuals who left their group, 57% continued to be involved
with their wave 1 group, whereas 43% were fully disengaged. Our analyses revealed
stark differences in criminal behavior and changes in network composition at wave 2 by
engagement level. Furthermore, these differences between groups by level of engagement
were much greater than differences assessed by gang membership status. This finding
clearly reemphasizes the growing agreement that leaving the gang is an important process
to be studied and that, not surprisingly, crime desistance is more clearly tied to full
disengagement than de-identification as a member of a gang. This aligns with the markers
of identity change highlighted by Paternoster and Bushway (2009): (a) crystallization of
discontent, (b) changes in institutional/social relationships, and (c) a “break from the past”
in that the fully disengaged gang-leavers in our study clearly made a break from the past,
and this break was associated with significant shifts in social relationships.
These findings have implications for theorizing about crime desistance. As discussed
in the background literature section of this paper, the role of social relations in desistance
theories is largely relegated to a minor facet in theories or subsumed under the larger umbrella of social structure. Studies testing Weaver’s ideas incorporating relational sociology
(2016) could open avenues to more deeply examining how various ties and tie structure
engender changes in identity in relation to gang and crime desistance. The work of Warr
110
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
(1996) on peers and changes in peer relationships is also relevant here. He found that when
holding peer influence constant, the effect of age on crime and desistance for the most part
disappeared. This finding emphasizes relationships and suggests that decreasing exposure
to delinquent peers is important for reducing crime. Related to the current study, changes in
peer relationships may occur due to the increasing salience of adult role models in the lives
of youth—possibly those adults who encourage offenders and former offenders to leave a
life of crime, and in our case leave the gang. This line of research focused more squarely on
personal social relations, may be integral to understanding the criminal trajectories of gang
members. Indeed, a social network approach specifically capitalizes on the fact that each
network member does not contribute equally to the respondent’s behavior.
With regard to the various roles of social relations, significant others may have less
influence than other prosocial-oriented adults (Table 6), as the influence of significant others
was provided as a reason for leaving a gang by those who remained engaged after leaving,
but not nearly as often by those who had fully disengaged. Additionally and notably,
those who were fully disengaged provided fewer reasons for leaving—this is telling for
the relative importance of adult encouragement for those who fully disengaged from their
wave 1 group. It may be that, for our sample, recruited by street outreach workers who
are actively working to mentor youth and young adults and reduce youth engagement in
violence, that the large significant increase in the proportion of mentorly alters at wave
2 for those disengaged (Table 8) can be attributed to the work of those outreach workers.
These mentorly alters could be the adults who encouraged the respondents to leave their
group.
Not surprisingly, these findings related to increases in mentorly network ties imply
that programs that use street outreach workers, such as the Cure Violence Public Health
Model, may be effective strategies to reduce violence and turn high-risk individuals and
gang members onto prosocial paths. Cure Violence, which originated in Chicago, is a public
health-based gun violence reduction strategy that seeks to reduce community-levels of gun
violence through direct work with individuals (Butts et al. 2015). The model does not focus
on gang members per se, but the eligibility criteria focus on high-risk individuals who have
been involved in violence and the criminal justice system likely includes a large number
of street gang members. Butts and colleagues’ review of the evaluation research on Cure
Violence indicates that the model has been effective, for the most part, when implemented
with fidelity. Evaluation studies published since their review also show success (see for
example, Roman et al. 2018). The current study also has policy implications that would
support the importance of programs that buoy family structures, specifically strengthening
the relationship between youth and their parents/caregivers.
In conclusion, future gang studies should include longitudinal, broad survey methods that incorporate ego-network data collection with qualitative interviews and other
survey methods and techniques that will help inform the temporal ordering of gang deidentification, disengagement, and crime desistance alongside changes in both network
composition and structure. Although an expensive endeavor, longitudinal surveys that
enable a comprehensive set of data on a range of social ties would provide unlimited
opportunities to examine a host of potentially significant factors associated with gang
disengagement and crime desistance. Furthermore, an assessment of the structural aspects
of ego networks—such as density, number of components, centralization—factors not
examined in this study, would potentially advance theoretical work and provide additional
insight for gang intervention and violence reduction.
7. Materials and Methods
Survey protocols are available by request from the first author.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, funding acquisition, and project administration: C.G.R.
and M.C.; methodology, C.G.R. and M.C.; data cleaning and validation, M.C. and C.G.R.; final
analyses for tables, C.G.R.; writing: original draft preparation, C.G.R., M.C., and L.R.M. (literature
111
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
review); writing: review and editing, M.C., C.G.R., and L.R.M. All authors have read and agreed to
the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: The data analyzed in this study were collected under Grant #2011-JU-FX-0105 awarded by
the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department
of Justice. The opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication
are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the Department of Justice.
Institutional Review Board Statement: The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board
of the RAND Corporation.
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the
study.
Data Availability Statement: Not applicable.
Acknowledgments: The authors would like to thank Chris McCarty for all his advice and support
regarding the survey protocols and use of EgoNet software, as well as his general guidance in
developing a rigorous study involving ego networks. We thank Mark Fleisher for his substantive
guidance and advice on gang member relations. We also thank a number of individuals who assisted
with study recruitment, data collection and cleaning, including Jillian Eidson, Megan Forney, Doris
Weiland, Hannah Klein, and Samantha Lowry. A special thank you goes to the outreach workers who
assisted with recruitment (Cure Violence in Philadelphia and those from the Columbia Heights/Shaw
Family Support Collaborative in DC).
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design
of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or
in the decision to publish the results.
References
Baerveldt, Chris, Ronan van Rossem, Marjolein M. Vermande, and Frank M. Weerman. 2004. Students’ delinquency and correlates
with strong and weaker ties. A study of students’ network in Dutch high schools. Connections 26: 11–28.
Bersani, Bianca E., and Elaine Eggleston Doherty. 2018. Desistance from offending in the twenty-first century. Annual Review of
Criminology 1: 311–34. [CrossRef]
Bjorgo, Tore. 2002. Exit Neo-Nazism: Reducing Recruitment and Promoting Disengagement from Racist Groups. Paper 627. Olso: Norwegian
Institute of International Affairs.
Bjorgo, Tore, and John Horgan. 2009. Leaving Terrorism Behind: Individual and Collective Disengagement. New York: Routledge.
Blau, M. Peter. 1977. Inequality and Heterogeneity. New York: Free Press.
Bottoms, Anthony, Joanna Shapland, Andrew Costell, Deborah Holmes, and Grant Muir. 2004. Towards desistance: Theoretical
Underpinnings for an Empirical Study. The Howard Journal 43: 368–89. [CrossRef]
Bouchard, Martin, and Aili Malm. 2016. Social Network Analysis and Its Contribution to Research on Crime and Criminal Justice.
Oxford Handbooks Online. [CrossRef]
Bushway, Shawn D., and Paternoster Ray. 2013. Desistance from crime: A review and ideas for moving forward. In Handbook of
Life-Course Criminology. Edited by Chris L. Gibson and Marvin Krohn. New York: Springer, pp. 213–31.
Butts, Jeffrey A., Caterina Gouvis Roman, Lindsay Bostwick, and Jeremy R. Porter. 2015. Cure Violence: A public health model to
reduce gun violence. Annual Review of Public Health 36: 39–53. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Carson, Dena C., and J. Michael Vecchio. 2015. Leaving the gang: A review and thoughts on future research. In The Wiley Handbook of
Gangs. Edited by Scott H. Decker and David C. Pyrooz. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell.
Decker, Scott H., and Barrick Van Winkle. 1996. Life in the Gang: Family, Friends, and Violence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Decker, Scott H., and Janet Lauritsen. 2002. Leaving the gang. In Gangs in America, 3rd ed. Edited by Ronald Huff. Thousand Oaks:
Sage, pp. 51–67.
Decker, Scott H., David C. Pyrooz, and Richard K. Moule Jr. 2014. Disengagement from gangs as role transitions. Journal of Research on
Adolescence 24: 268–83. [CrossRef]
Ebaugh, Helen Rose F. 1988. Becoming an Ex: The Process of Role Exit. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Eidson, Jillian L., Caterina G. Roman, and Meagan Cahill. 2017. Successes and challenges in recruiting and retaining gang members
in longitudinal research: Lessons learned from a multisite social network study. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice 15: 396–418.
[CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Age, and Huizinga David. 1993. Gangs, drugs, and delinquency in a survey of urban youth. Criminology 31: 565–89.
[CrossRef]
Farrall, Stephen. 2002. Rethinking What Works with Offenders: Probation, Social Context, and Desistance from Crime. Devon: Willan
Publishing.
112
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
Feld, Scott L., J. Jill Suitor, and Jordana G. Hoegh. 2007. Describing Changes in Personal Networks over Time. Field Methods 19: 218–36.
[CrossRef]
Fleisher, Mark S. 2002. Women in Gangs: A Field Research Study. Final Report to the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention,
U.S. Department of Justice. Normal: Illinois State University.
Giordano, Peggy C., Stephen A. Cernkovich, and L. Jennifer Rudolph. 2002. Gender, crime, and desistance: Toward a theory of
cognitive transformation. American Journal of Sociology 107: 990–1064. [CrossRef]
Giordano, Peggy C., Stephen A. Cernkovich, and Ryan D. Schroeder. 2007. Emotions and crime over the life-course: A neo-median
perspective on criminal continuity and change. American Journal of Sociology 112: 1603–61. [CrossRef]
Giordano, Peggy C., Wendi L. Johnson, Wendy D. Manning, Monica A. Longmore, and Mallory D. Minter. 2015. Intimate partner
violence in young adulthood: Narratives of persistence and desistance. Criminology 53: 330–65. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Granovetter, Mark. 1983. The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Sociological Theory 1: 201–33. [CrossRef]
Krohn, D. Marvin. 1986. The Web of conformity: A network approach to the explanation of delinquent behavior. Social Problems 33:
S81–S93. [CrossRef]
Krohn, D. Marvin, and Terry P. Thornberry. 2008. Longitudinal perspectives on adolescent street gangs. In The Long View of Crime: A
Synthesis of Longitudinal Research. Edited by A. Liberman. New York: Springer, pp. 128–60.
Laub, John H., and Robert J. Sampson. 2001. Understanding the desistance from crime. In Crime & Justice: A Review of Research. Edited
by M. Tonry. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, vol. 28, pp. 1–69.
Laub, John H., and Robert J. Sampson. 2003. Shared Beginnings, Divergent Lives: Delinquent Boys to Age 70. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
Loeber, Rolf, and Marc Le Blanc. 1990. Toward a developmental criminology. In Crime and Justice. Edited by M. Tonry and N. Morris.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press, vol. 12, pp. 375–437.
Marsden, Peter V. 1990. Network data and measurement. Annual Review of Sociology 16: 435–63. [CrossRef]
Maruna, Shadd. 2001. Making Good. How Ex-Convicts Reform and Rebuild Their Lives. Washington: American Psychological Association.
Maruna, Shadd. 2004. Desistance from crime and explanatory style: A new direction in the psychology of reform. Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice 20: 184–200. [CrossRef]
Maruna, Shadd. 2016. Desistance and restorative justice: It’s now or never. Restorative Justice International Journal 4: 289–301. [CrossRef]
Mathews K. Michael, Michael C. White, Rebecca C. Long, Barlow Soper, and C. W. Von Bergen. 1998. Association of indicators and
predictors of tie strength. Psychological Reports 83: 1459–69. [CrossRef]
McCarty, Christopher. 2002. Measuring structure in personal networks. Journal of Social Structure 3: 1.
McCarty, Christopher. 2003. EgoNet. Personal Network Software. Available online: http://sourceforge.net/projects/egonet/ (accessed
on 10 April 2013).
McNeill, Fergus. 2006. A desistance paradigm for offender management. Criminology and Criminal Justice 6: 39–62. [CrossRef]
McNeill, Fergus. 2016. Desistance and Criminal Justice in Scotland. In Crime, Justice and Society in Scotland. Edited by H. Croall, G.
Mooney and M. Munro. London: Routledge.
McNeill, Fergus, and Beth Weaver. 2010. Changing Lives? Desistance Research and Offender Management. SCCJR Project Report
No.03/2010. Glasgow: Scottish Centre for Crime & Justice Research.
Melde, Chris, and Finn-Age Esbensen. 2014. The relative impact of gang status transitions: Identifying the mechanisms of change in
delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 51: 349–76. [CrossRef]
Paternoster, Ray, and Shawn Bushway. 2009. Desistance and ‘feared self’: Towards an identity theory of criminal desistance. Journal of
Criminal Law & Criminology 99: 1103–56.
Pyrooz, David C., and Scott H. Decker. 2011. Motives and methods for leaving the gang: Understanding the process of gang desistance.
Journal of Criminal Justice 39: 417–25. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David C., Scott H. Decker, and Vincent J. Webb. 2014. The ties that bind: Desistance from gangs. Crime and Delinquency 60:
491–516. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., Hannah J. Klein, and Kevin T. Wolff. 2018. Quasi-experimental designs for community-level public health violence
reduction interventions: A case study in the challenges of selecting the counterfactual. Journal of Experimental Criminology 14:
155–85. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., Meagan Cahill, and Jillian L. Eidson. 2016. Street Gang Definitions across Two US Cities: Eurogang Criteria,
Group Identity Characteristics, and Peer Group Involvement in Crime. In Gang Transitions and Transformations in an International
Context. Edited by Cheryl L. Maxson and Finn-Age Esbensen. New York: Springer, pp. 15–32.
Roman, Caterina G., Scott H. Decker, and David C. Pyrooz. 2017. Leveraging the pushes and pulls of gang disengagement to improve
gang intervention: Findings from three multi-site studies and a review of relevant gang programs. Journal of Criminal Justice 40:
316–36.
Ronald, Clarke V., and Derek B. Cornish. 1985. Modeling offenders’ decisions: A framework for research and policy. Crime and Justice 6:
147–85.
Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 1990. Crime and deviance over the life course: The salience of adult social bonds. American
Sociological Review 55: 609–27. [CrossRef]
Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 1993. Crime in the Making: Pathways and Turning Points through Life. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
113
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 39
Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 2003. Life-course desisters? Trajectories of crime among delinquent boys followed to age 70.
Criminology 41: 301–39. [CrossRef]
Sampson, Robert J., and John H. Laub. 2016. Turning points and the future of life-course criminology: Reflections on the 1986 criminal
careers report. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 53: 321–35. [CrossRef]
Sarnecki, Jerzy. 2001. Delinquent Networks: Youth Co-Offending in Stockholm. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sierra-Arévalo, Michael, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2017. Social Networks and Gang Violence Reduction. Annual Review of Law and
Social Science 13: 373–93. [CrossRef]
Soyer, Michaela. 2014. The Imagination of Desistance: A Juxtaposition of the Construction of Incarceration as a Turning Point and the
Reality of Recidivism. The British Journal of Criminology 54: 91–108. [CrossRef]
Spergel, Irving A. 1995. The Youth Gang Problem: A Community Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sweeten, Gary, David C. Pyrooz, and Alex R. Piquero. 2013. Disengaging from gangs and desistance from crime. Justice Quarterly 30:
469–500. [CrossRef]
Teruya, Cheryl, and Yih-Ing Hser. 2010. Turning points in the life course: Current findings and future directions in drug use research.
Current Drug Abuse Reviews 3: 189–95. [CrossRef]
Valasik, Matthew, Shannon E. Reid, Jenny S. West, and Jason Gravel. 2018. Gang activity regulation and the group nature of gang
violence. In International Handbook of Aggression: Current Issues and Perspectives. Edited by Jane Ireland, Carol A. Ireland and Philip
Birch. New York, NY: Routledge.
Valente, W. Thomas. 2010. Social Networks and Health: Models, Methods, and Applications. New York: Oxford University Press.
Vigil, James D. 1988. Barrio Gangs: Street Life and Identity in Southern California. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Warr, Mark. 1996. Organization and instigation in delinquent groups. Criminology 34: 11–37. [CrossRef]
Wasserman, Stanley, and Katherine Faust. 1994. Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Weaver, Beth. 2012. The relational context of desistance: Some implications and opportunities for social policy. Social Policy &
Administration 46: 395–412.
Weaver, Beth. 2016. Offending and Desistance: The Importance of Social Relations. New York: Routledge.
Weaver, Beth, and Fergus McNeill. 2015. Lifelines: Desistance, social relations, and reciprocity. Criminal Justice and Behavior 42: 95–107.
[CrossRef]
Weerman, Frank. M. 2011. Delinquent peers in context: A longitudinal network analysis of selection and influence effects. Criminology
49: 253–86. [CrossRef]
Weerman, Frank M., Cheryl L. Maxson, Finn-Age Esbensen, Judith Aldridge, Juanjo Medina, and Frank Van Gemert. 2009. Eurogang
Program (Manual Background n.d.)Manual Background, Development, and Use of the Eurogang Instruments in Multi-Site,
Multi-Method Comparative Research. Available online: http://www.umsl.edu/~{}ccj/eurogang/Eurogang_20Manual.pdf
(accessed on 15 December 2012).
Wellman, Barry, and Scot Wortley. 1990. Different strokes from different folks: Community ties and social support. American Journal of
Sociology 96: 558–88. [CrossRef]
Young, Jacob T. N., and Frank M. Weerman. 2013. Delinquency as a consequence of misperception: Overestimation of friends’
delinquent behavior and mechanisms of social influence. Social Problems 60: 334–56.
114
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Social Media and the Variable Impact of Violence Reduction
Interventions: Re-Examining Focused Deterrence in
Philadelphia
Jordan M. Hyatt 1, *, James A. Densley 2
1
2
3
*
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Citation: Hyatt, Jordan M., James A.
Densley, and Caterina G. Roman.
2021. Social Media and the Variable
Impact of Violence Reduction
Interventions: Re-Examining Focused
Deterrence in Philadelphia. Social
and Caterina G. Roman 3
Department of Criminology and Justice Studies, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA
School of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice, Metropolitan State University, St. Paul, MN 55445, USA;
[email protected]
Department of Criminal Justice, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Abstract: Focused deterrence is a gang violence reduction strategy that relies on a unique mix of
strong enforcement messages from law enforcement and judicial officials coupled with the promise
of additional services. At the heart of the intervention is a coordinated effort to communicate the
costs and consequences of gun violence to identified gang members during face-to-face meetings and
additional community messaging. In Philadelphia, focused deterrence was implemented between
2013 and 2016, and although an impact evaluation showed a significant decrease in shootings in
targeted areas relative to matched comparison neighborhoods, the effect on targeted gangs was not
universal, with some exhibiting no change or an increase in gun-related activity. Here, we employ
data on group-level social media usage and content to examine the correlations with gun violence.
We find that several factors, including the nature of social media activity by the gang (e.g., extent of
activity and who is engaging), are associated with increases in the average rate of gang-attributable
shootings during the evaluation period, while content-specific variables (e.g., direct threats towards
rivals and law enforcement) were not associated with increases in shootings. Implications for violence
reduction policy, including the implementation of focused deterrence, are discussed.
Sciences 10: 147. https://doi.org/
10.3390/socsci10050147
Keywords: gangs; violence; shootings; social media; focused deterrence; intervention
Academic Editors: Matthew Valasik
and Shannon E. Reid
1. Introduction
Received: 1 February 2021
Accepted: 16 April 2021
Published: 22 April 2021
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral
with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Copyright: © 2021 by the authors.
Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland.
This article is an open access article
distributed under the terms and
conditions of the Creative Commons
Attribution (CC BY) license (https://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/
4.0/).
Taking on a variety of expressive and pragmatic roles, gang violence, especially
involving guns, presents a persistent challenge for law enforcement and communities
worldwide (Decker et al. 2021). Violence is often directed at other gangs and is the result
of rivalries over activities, territory, or perceptions of respect (Nakamura et al. 2020).
Effective strategies to reduce violence are rare and difficult to generalize owing to the
unique characteristics of gangs and their environments (Braga et al. 2006). A growing body
of research has found that focused deterrence, more recently known as the Group Violence
Intervention, can be effective in reducing gang violence (Braga et al. 2018). However,
gangs subject to focused deterrence may adapt differently depending on certain factors
(Roman et al. 2019). The current study seeks to explore one group of factors that may be
associated with the differential responses to focused deterrence observed in Philadelphia,
USA: the influence of social media. Social media are increasingly recognized as potentially
playing a role in exacerbating gang violence (Patton et al. 2019), and there are unexplored
theoretical reasons to believe its use may impact the efficacy of an intervention liked
focused deterrence. To examine this relationship, we uniquely link real-world data on
gang-specific shooting rates to the nature and intensity of social media usage by those
same gangs. We begin by locating the current study in the existing literature on focused
deterrence, gang violence, and social media.
115
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
2. Focused Deterrence and the Philadelphia Experience
Focused deterrence, an approach in which many “levers” are “pulled” to discourage
gangs from engaging in violence, was developed in Boston in the late 1990s (See Kennedy
et al. 1996). In the initial model, called Operation Ceasefire (Kennedy et al. 2001), these
“levers” consisted of formal agency-level responses from prosecution, law enforcement,
and probation, among others, and of offering educational, employment, and social service
support. The risks of engaging in violence, and the benefits of abstaining, were communicated to key gang-involved individuals at “call in” meetings. Importantly, these individuals
were then instructed to share these messages with other members of their gang.
In Philadelphia, focused deterrence was first implemented in 2013 with “call in”
meetings and surveillance and enforcement actions (the unified response to non-compliant
participating gangs) running through 2016. Conceived as a multi-agency partnership
involving the Philadelphia District Attorney’s Office, the Philadelphia Police Department
(PPD), the Adult Probation and Parole Department, the First Judicial District (i.e., the
local court system), Juvenile Probation, and the Mayor’s Office of Criminal Justice, among
others, the program largely followed the original Boston model. This meant that the
“levers” included increased requests for high bail (by the prosecutor), adjustment of the
requirements of community supervision (by adult probation; see Roman et al. 2020 for more
context), and the execution of outstaying warrants (by various law enforcement partners).
Additionally, unique levers in Philadelphia encouraged rapid responses to utility theft,
accumulated non-payment for services (e.g., cable and internet), child support obligations,
and a review of public housing subsidies. Social services, coordinated by the Mayor’s Office,
were also offered to all gang members who were engaging with the program (see Roman
et al. 2019, 2020, for additional descriptions of the implementation processes). Throughout
the intervention, 14 gangs were the subject of the focused deterrence evaluation.
The “pulling levers” approach on which Philadelphia’s intervention was modeled has
been replicated and evaluated multiple times, including in Boston and other jurisdictions
across the United States and Western Europe. Research has shown that focused deterrence
can reduce crime. Recent studies have shown associated reductions in violence observed in
large (e.g., Los Angeles; Tita et al. 2003), medium (e.g., New Orleans; Corsaro and Engel 2015),
and smaller (e.g., Lowell (MA); Braga 2008) cities. Its adaptation in Glasgow, Scotland,
derived from a version of the model in Cincinnati, is credited with large reductions in
serious youth violence (Deuchar 2013; Graham 2016; Williams et al. 2014). One systematic
review aggregated the most rigorous of these jurisdiction-level studies (n = 10) and found
that 90% of them identified a statistically significant reduction in crime attributable to the
intervention (Braga and Weisburd 2012). A more recent review by the same authors found
smaller but significant reductions in violence, though variation in impact by program type,
goal, and outcomes was identifiable due to the larger sample size (n = 24) (Braga et al. 2018).
The evaluations of the sites that implemented programs focused specifically on gangs (as
opposed to individuals or drug markets) witnessed the largest effects.
Fewer studies have sought to examine how specific gangs responded to focused
deterrence. Braga et al. (2014), for example, employed a quasi-experimental approach
in Boston to compare a sample of comparison gangs that were similar to the targeted
gangs but that were not subject to the intervention themselves. A side-by-side comparison
showed that the shootings among the participating gangs decreased by 31%, a statistically
significant reduction. Similar results were identified in Chicago, where the gangs that
participated in a “call-in” demonstrated a 23% reduction in shootings (Papachristos and
Kirk 2015).
The evaluation in Philadelphia followed the model employed by Braga et al. (2018)
and sought to identify not only the community-wide impact of the strategy on gun violence
but also assess the impact at the gang level (Roman et al. 2019). In Philadelphia, all the
targeted gangs resided in the same region of the city. They employed a propensity scoring
and matching design to pair communities where the gangs were involved in focused
deterrence with similar neighborhoods, including in terms of baseline violence, gang
116
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
activity, and socio-demographics, where the gangs were not selected for participation.
The community-level difference-in-differences results showed a statistically significant
reduction in shootings in the 24 months after the implementation of focused deterrence
when compared to the matched comparison areas. The gang-specific analyses focused on
two metrics: the number of shootings in the geographic area(s) associated with the targeted
gangs as well as the number of shootings in which an identified member of a given gang
was identified as the perpetrator. Although shootings in the areas associated with the
targeted gangs decreased more than the comparison gangs’ areas (which also decreased,
to a lesser extent), these findings did not reach statistical significance. A descriptive
examination of the average change per quarter of the number of with a gang-identified
perpetrator highlights one potential challenge (Figure 1, below). Of the 14 gangs that
participated, the majority responded positively to focused deterrence, as was anticipated.
However, three gangs bucked that trend and were associated with more shootings after
efforts were made to prevent violence; the intervention backfired for those gangs. One
gang had neither a reduction in shootings nor an increase.
2
1.58
Change in Shootings
1.5
1
0.5
0.3
0.29
0
0
-0.1
-0.5
-0.2 -0.24
-0.53 -0.53 -0.56
-0.62
-1
Gang
-0.74 -0.79
-0.84
Figure 1. Average change per quarter in counts of gang-related (by known perpetrator) shootings
(n = 14). Note: Post period for each gang begins in the first quarter that the gang was “called in”.
In considering these findings, Roman and colleagues noted that even when targeted
with the same, generally effective strategy, gangs may not always respond the same way
(Roman et al. 2019). One possible source of this variable response may be the extent to
which gangs differentially engaged with social media, especially concerning threatening
rhetoric that could lead to intra-gang violence. In other words, gangs more likely to be
using social media as a venue for communication may be less likely to pay attention to the
deterrent message of focused deterrence—a lesson that centers on reinforcing a sense of
collective accountability. Similarly, high levels of this type of online activity could bring a
gang into virtual contact with other gangs or individuals engaged in illegal activity that
they would not otherwise encounter; these increased opportunities for confrontation could
translate into street-based violence if the gangs fight or if they collude to commit new
crimes. Alternatively, the online and physical worlds may not overlap in this space; social
media activity may function independently from face-to-face actions. In the current inquiry,
we seek to develop preliminary evidence on this dynamic, though we first consider the
theoretical and practical landscape.
117
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
3. Gang Violence on Social Media
Social media have become a dominant force in modern public discourse in just over
two decades (Dewing 2010; Zuboff 2018). Since their inception, social media sites have been
designed to encourage and facilitate interpersonal communication using videos, pictures, and
written messages; regulation of illegal content has been largely ineffective at preventing the
dissemination of violent rhetoric and, in some cases, actions (Bock 2012; Patton et al. 2013).
This has resulted in the creation of an online extension of the physical world where
friends and rivals can speak directly; street gangs have expanded their traditional means
of communication (Goldman et al. 2014) to adapt to this new reality. The impact of the
shift from the physical to the virtual world, especially within the context of social media,
can impact the psychological and social dynamics of communication, often requiring a
real-world response (see, e.g., Fedushko et al. 2021).
There has been an explosion of research examining gangs’ use of the internet in the last
decade (for reviews, see Densley 2020; Peterson and Densley 2017; Pyrooz and Moule 2019),
including a recent Eurogang edited volume titled Gangs in the Era of Internet and Social
Media (Melde and Weerman 2020). Examining the content and language used by gang
members on social media has taken a prominent role in these examinations. For example,
an examination of the Twitter profile of one murdered Chicago gang member demonstrated
that such communications are often used to promote gang affiliations, report on violent acts,
and to facilitate networking between geographically diverse gangs (Patton et al. 2017b).
Stuart (2020), drawing on ethnographic research in Chicago, argues that social media are
used to openly challenge the strength and masculinity of rival gangs, an action that may
engender violent retaliation in a certain subset of instances but not generally. Patton et al.
(2019) highlighted the interactive way gangs use Twitter and find that certain types of
messages (e.g., disses, call-outs, and threats) have a high potential to engineer a violent
response. The mechanisms by which social media may or may not directly facilitate gang
violence generally are not well specified nor measured. These limitations are drawn, in part,
from a lack of clear, empirical data on individual-level social media usage by gangs that
can be linked to changes in offline violence. However, the descriptive, critical examination
of social media by and about gang members has given rise to theoretically driven linkages
between online activity and “real world” violence.
Even if much of what gang members do online is the same as the activity of non-gang
members (Moule et al. 2013, 2014), what makes gang members unique is their use of the
internet to explicitly promote their criminal exploits and to insult and intimidate rivals
(Johnson and Schell-Busey 2016; Pawelz and Elvers 2018). However, a recent study of
gangs in London found there were “differential adaptations” to social media among gangs,
including gangs that occupied the same geographic spaces (Whittaker et al. 2020). The
authors attributed this to a “generation gap”. They argued that newer gangs and younger
gang members, especially those with tenuous “street capital” (Harding 2014), had more to
gain and less to lose from signaling their reputations online versus more durable gangs
and more senior gang members, who had street capital in the bank and could not afford
the extra scrutiny that social media attention provided (see also, Densley 2020).
The nature of the online forum itself may also influence the nature of the discourse for
gang members. Online disinhibition (anonymity) might mean youth act up more online
than they would in person, magnifying the exacerbating tensions between gangs which can
then later lead to violence in the streets (Patton et al. 2017b). Life online remains anchored
in the lived experience (Lane 2018; Roks et al. 2020), which is why references to physical
territories, such as street signs or zip codes (Densley 2013; Irwin-Rogers et al. 2018), are
a fixture of “internet banging” (Patton et al. 2013). Leverso and Hsiao (2020) learned
from digital trace data scraped from a public Facebook page about Chicago Latino gangs
(resulting in over 140,000 posts, likes, comments, and comment replies) and combined
with law enforcement data on the geographic locations of gangs, that fighting among gang
members in the online environment was conditional on the type of post displayed, but also
the geographic proximity of gang territory. They found gang members using social media
118
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
to interact with other gangs in faraway locales as well as individuals nearby, but the tone
and tenor of that communication often reflected the degree of physical distance. Gangs
living nearby, therefore, may have a much tighter (and rapid) relationship between online
communication and what happens on the streets.
At the same time, the social distance that social media create can help diffuse tensions
and de-escalate violence. In nature and many non-delinquent social engagements, one
method of self-protection is presenting as more intimidating, larger, or more dangerous than
is the case (Felson 2006; Howell 2007). Social media affords gangs the same illusion of size,
strength, and spread (Densley 2013). For gang members living in Chicago’s most dangerous
neighborhoods, urban ethnographer Forrest Stuart (2020) found that exaggerated virtual
identities created barriers to violence. By presenting themselves as scarier and more violent
than they were in real life, including posting photographs of themselves posing in crowds
of dangerous-looking people or holding borrowed guns, gang members could deter rival
predation. Authenticity still mattered, however, and if rivals called someone on their
bluff or caught them “lacking” (i.e., unwilling to take the bait), especially live on camera,
then violent retaliation could follow. These findings hold important implications for law
enforcement, who have been criticized (e.g., Lane et al. 2018; Patton et al. 2017a) for taking
online claims at face value and unduly criminalizing actions that everyone on social media
is guilty of—portraying their lives as more glamorous and exciting than they are, for
the sake of retweets or likes. However, it should be noted that, given the nature of the
ethnographic data and methods, the study findings cannot be broadly generalized and
have not yet been validated by other studies by examining gangs outside of Chicago or
with a larger sample of gangs.
4. Social Media and Focused Deterrence
There are a few reasons why social media could mediate, or even undermine, gang
interventions such as focused deterrence. For example, focused deterrence requires “community moral voices” such as pastors and parents to reinforce or “retail” the law enforcement message that violence is wrong and will incur consequences (Densley and Jones
2016; Kennedy et al. 2001). However, in the age of social media, these voices cannot reach
gang members with the same degree of constancy and invasiveness that social media
feeds can, as they are delivered to phones and computers constantly. Additionally, both
law enforcement and engaged community members are competing against persuasive
digital design techniques such as push notifications and the endless scroll of a newsfeed,
which capture the hearts and minds of users and create a feedback loop that keeps gang
members (and non-gang members) attached their devices. Technology companies such
as Facebook (which also owns Instagram), Google (which owns YouTube), and Twitter
profit from keeping users on their platforms because more human engagement means
more advertising dollars—their primary source of income in the absence of subscription
and usage fees (Zuboff 2018). They facilitate and often encourage discourse with limited
focus on the content to monetize their respective platforms.
Relatedly, social media platforms are designed to profit from “confirmation bias”
(Nickerson 1998), the natural human tendency to seek, “like,” and “share” (in social media
parlance) new information that aligns with strongly held preexisting beliefs. To keep
people online, they rely on adaptive algorithms that assess interests and flood users with
content that is similar to what they have liked previously. This self-reinforcing cycle
makes it difficult to change old habits such as violence and may bring together similar
messages from competing gangs. Someone subject to focused deterrence may want to
avoid gang content both in-person and online, but personalized media feeds based on past
click behavior and search history instead create “filter bubbles” that make encountering
aspects of active gang life unavoidable (Pariser 2011). The social media echo chamber
can also provide near-constant reaffirmation for the gang by silencing outside voices and
contradicting any intervention’s countervailing messaging (Eckberg et al. 2018). Social
media algorithms instead promote gang content that sparks outrage and which may
119
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
amplify biases. This need not be public or direct threats from rival gang members. Simply
being reminded constantly of friends killed in action might be enough. For example,
analyses of Twitter content have found that expressions of grief and loss predict increased
future aggression among gang-involved youth (Patton et al. 2018).
The noise from public and direct threats posted on the internet may well “drown out”
any focused deterrence messaging, especially those on social media that are perceived as
immediate and localized threats of violence or insults to gang identity. Actively posting
self-incriminating content that incites violence is so “costly” (Densley 2013) that it may
well be an indirect measure of a gang or gang member’s immunity to focused deterrence
messaging; their willingness to be violent and contempt for the law (Sandberg and Ugelvik
2016). Law enforcement has, with increasing frequency, sought to leverage these public
communications to prevent and prosecute criminal activity (Brayne 2017). At the same
time, it is difficult for law enforcement and social service providers to proactively monitor
the street for signs of impending violence when those high-risk individuals are not out in
the street (Patton et al. 2016), and without input from “domain experts” (i.e., people fluent
in gang content) such as ex-gang members, the cultural terms or coded language hidden in
memes and emojis that may provoke violence could go undetected (Frey et al. 2020; Patton
et al. 2019).
5. Materials and Methods
The Data
The data used in this study were derived from the administrative records and primary
data employed in the primary evaluation of focused deterrence in Philadelphia (see Roman
et al. 2019). These data describing general gang characteristics were constructed during a
series of large audit meetings held during the evaluation period and led by members of the
research team. In these retrospective, multi-agency meetings, law enforcement leadership,
front-line officers, and task force members met to aggregate information about each of
the gangs involved in the focused deterrence intervention. Information was provided
by various individuals with first-hand knowledge about the gangs, discussed among
all audit participants, and a consensus was reached (Roman et al. 2019). For each gang,
the audit participants worked through the names of every possible member, adding and
removing individuals as determined by group consensus. Prior studies have shown this
to be a valid measure of determining the nature and extent of gang activity (see, e.g.,
Gravel and Tita 2015). The result of the audit was a holistic picture of the size, activity
history, and activities of each gang (for a general discussion of this approach, see SierraArévalo and Papachristos 2015, 2017).
The current study employs three types of variables, two of which are drawn from
the gang audit procedures described above. The first type is gang variables, which focus
on describing the size and general nature of the gang. While these data are, by nature,
estimates, they represent the best available data on these gangs and their basic descriptive
statistics and activity level. Importantly, the data on each of these gangs were developed
by the same individuals and using the same procedures; though imprecision may be an
issue, between-gang comparisons are supportable.
In this set of gang variables, the number of rivals indicates the count of other gangs the
subject gang was actively feuding with at the time of the audit. Heat level is a measure of
how violent and/or serious of a threat a gang was perceived to be by local law enforcement
at the beginning of the initiative (in the second quarter of 2013) using a scale that ran from
inactive to highly active. Count of members is the number of core participants derived from
the audit; gang associates provide the same information, but for individuals less central to
the core activities and/or direction of the gang. Finally, average age reports the average age
of known members of the gang (not associates) based on administrative records (i.e., arrest
and court records).
The second set of gang variables are the social media variables, developed from a
different source, though using a largely similar process. A series of audits focusing only on
120
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
social media usage was held with the intelligence unit officers assigned to the gang task
force in the focused deterrence target area. In this role, these officers were responsible for
the collection, oversight, and synthesis of information on social media activity by the gangs
in their assigned region who were under investigation and/or surveillance. As with the
larger audits, data were obtained independently from each officer and then cross-validated.
This audit was conducted in early 2018 as interim impact analysis indicated that not all
gangs were responding to the focused deterrence messaging. At this time, the evaluation
window had concluded, but data collection for the study was still active. The variables
focused on estimates for and descriptions of specific online activities (e.g., feuds, threats
to law enforcement, and displays of violence and illegal items such as guns and drugs),
overall usage of various social media sites, estimates of how active social media usage was
within the gang, percent of high-visibility gang members using social media, and estimates
of the prevalence of violent content by gang members across all platforms.
In these data, the percent active on social media variable captures the percent of all
known gang members believed to use social media for gang-centric activities. Percent
impact indicates the number of high “impact” individuals from the gang, often leaders and
senior members, who were known to use social media for gang-related activities. The overall
social media usage variable captures the total amount of social media usage attributable to
the gang as compared to other gangs in the city. Illegal content (e.g., images or discussion of
guns, cash, and/or narcotics) and violent content convey similar information about these
subtypes of postings by the gang. Due to the difficulty in ascertaining precise levels of
social media activity, these data are captured in measures using categorical values (ranging
from no activity to high levels of activity of that type) with overall levels of gang activity
in the city at the time used as a reference. Similarly, variables regarding threats to law
enforcement, rival gangs, and other activity are constructed as binary variables reflecting
the presence or absence of the subtype of online activity during the focused deterrence
evaluation period. Finally, percent violent content is an estimate of what percentage of all
content posted by the gang and gang-involved individuals were estimated to be explicitly
violent (e.g., direct threats, boasts of past violence) in nature.
Finally, the gang shootings variable is the average quarterly change in gang-involved
shootings, comparing the period before the gang was first called in to a notification meeting
to the period after the gang’s first call-in meeting. The values for this variable were
derived from the regression models in the main impact analyses (Roman et al. 2019). For
these analyses, a gang-involved shooting is a shooting in which the PPD identified the
perpetrator as a member of a targeted gang. The research team had collected data on
every shooting in the target area between January 2009 and 1 April 2015. If there were any
shootings where the research team did not have coded shooting data from the PPD, the
intelligence analysts re-reviewed the spreadsheet to validate the data entry.
The auditing process employed for this study and related work (see Roman et al.
2019) was designed to quantify the often-subjective perceptions of law enforcement actors
regarding various aspects of gang activity in Philadelphia in a consistent manner. Modeled
off previous successfully implemented audits (see e.g., Kennedy et al. 1996; Papachristos
and Kirk 2015), the auditing process included multiple efforts to cross-validate the data
collected between several stakeholder groups and actors, both between gangs and over time,
to develop the most robust measures possible. This is important as the construction of gang
databases is an exercise fraught with challenges, especially in operationalizing definitions
of activity and membership (Densley and Pyrooz 2020; Kennedy 2009). In particular,
questions about the accuracy (e.g., are audit assessments reflective of the “real” world?),
reliability (e.g., are audit assessments consistent over time and repeated measurements?),
and validity (e.g., are audit assessments focusing on the correct measures of activity?)
persist. While difficult to authoritatively answer, the audit data, derived using the processes
above, represent the best and, in some cases, only data on gang-level activity available to
both the research team and local law enforcement. The audit process also includes several
procedural checks, including verification of all assessments using multiple types of data
121
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
and/or more than one reporting source, to limit the potential influences of individualor system-level biases. While potentially imperfect, social network analyses have shown
that, beyond the focused deterrence model, data derived from audits can successfully
guide interventional efforts in policing (Sierra-Arévalo and Papachristos 2015) and violence
reduction (Tita and Radil 2011).
6. Results
To examine the associations between social media and gang violence, we first consider
the descriptive statistics for the variables developed during the social media audits. These
data present a unique picture of the perceived online activities of the subset of gangs
that were included in the evaluation. Subsequently, we seek to examine the associations
between the factors detailed above and changes in the observed rates of violence. We do so
by calculating partial correlations between the shooting outcome for all fourteen gangs, the
average quarterly change in shootings over time, and the social media variables. In these
comparisons, we control for the gang variables to better identify the focal relationships.
We first consider the variables relating to gang characteristics and compare means,
ranges, and standard deviations for two aggregate groups. Table 1, below, reports average
values for the group of gangs that demonstrated an increase in shootings (n = 3) during
focused deterrence as compared to those reporting a decrease (n = 9). Note that, for
this set of analyses only, we omit the single gang for which no change in shooting rates
was reported as this outcome is not appropriately attributable to either the “increase” or
“decrease” groups and including a single gang as a distinct category could result in that
gang becoming identifiable.
Table 1. Gang-level descriptive statistics, by change in shootings.
Gangs That Increased Shootings during Focused Deterrence (n = 3)
N
Min
Max
Mean
Std.
Deviation
Number of known and
active rival gangs
3
3
5
4.00
1.00
“Heat Level”, as
assessed by law
enforcement
3
3
3
3.00
Number of known
gang members
3
22
123
Number of known
associates to the gang
3
5
Average age of gang
members
3
Average number of
times called in
during FD
3
Gangs That Decreased Shootings during Focused Deterrence (n = 9)
N
Min
Max
Mean
Std.
Deviation
Number of known and
active rival gangs
9
2
5
3.22
1.20
0.00
“Heat Level”, as
assessed by law
enforcement
9
2
3
2.89
0.33
69.33
50.79
Number of gang
members
9
7
122
49.56
37.70
34
15.67
15.94
Number of known
associates to the gang
9
0
33
12.00
12.15
23
25
24.000
1.00
Average age of gang
members
9
23
32
25.889
3.29
2
4
2.667
1.15
Average number of
times called in
during FD
8
3
4
3.875
0.3
NOTE: FD = focused deterrence.
There are slight observable differences between the gangs that responded positively
(n = 10, average change in shooting incidents per quarter: −0.51) to focused deterrence
and those that did not (n = 3, average change in shooting incidents per quarter +0.72) (see
Figure 1 above). On average, the gangs with more shootings during the implementation of
the strategy than before focused deterrence had more rivals with whom they were actively
feuding (4.0 v. 3.22 known rival gangs) and, not surprisingly, had been deemed “hotter”
about their observed levels of criminal activity (3.0 v. 2.89) at the start of the strategy
(e.g., 2013). They were also slightly larger, on average (85 total individuals v. 61.56 total
individuals), though the ratio of the more engaged member to affiliated associates (81.5%
v. 80.5%) is only slightly larger than that of the gangs whose violence decreased. Finally,
though most gang members were young, the more violent gangs had members who were
122
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
1.89 years younger than their peers in the less violent gangs, on average. Taken together,
these data suggest that there are some differences between the overall characteristics for
the gangs who maintained or increased their shootings post-implementation of focused
deterrence versus those that had fewer shootings. This is particularly true concerning
their size, but, overall, this variation is not overwhelmingly large, nor were any significant
outliers identified. This is unsurprising because the gangs that were included in the
intervention were all located in the same area of the city and met the common criteria for
inclusion in focused deterrence.
We next turn our attention away from the streets and towards the virtual world.
Detailed in Table 2 below, these data describe average levels and the nature of social media
activity attributable to the gangs, again disaggregated by their response to the intervention.
Table 2. Gang-level social media descriptive statistics by change in shootings.
Gangs That Decreased Shootings during Focused
Deterrence (n = 9)
Gangs That Increased Shootings during Focused Deterrence (n = 3)
N
Min
Max
Mean
Std. Deviation
N
Min
Max
Mean
Std. Deviation
Number of known and
active rival gangs
3
3
5
4.00
1.00
9
2
5
3.22
1.20
“Heat Level”, as assessed
by law enforcement
3
3
3
3.00
0.00
9
2
3
2.89
0.3
Number of known gang
members
3
22
123
69.33
50.79
9
7
122
49.56
37.70
Number of known
associates to the gang
3
5
34
15.67
15.94
9
0
33
12.00
12.15
Average age of gang
members
3
23
25
24.000
1.00
9
23
32
25.88
3.29
Average number of times
called in during FD
3
2
4
2.667
1.15
8
3
4
3.87
0.35
NOTE: FD = focused deterrence.
As was the case with the gang-level descriptive statistics, a visual examination of
Table 2 shows that there were small differences between the two groups’ online activities.
The gangs that did not respond to the intervention, for example, were overall more active on
social media (85% v. 68.8%), and a higher proportion of high visibility impact members were
engaged in these online activities (93.3% v. 78.33%). Unsurprisingly, this also translated to
a higher aggregate score of social media usage (2.67 out of 3 v. 2.11), as well as the scores
for violent (2.0 out of 3.0 v. 1.67) and general illegal (2.67 out of 3.0 v. 1.89) postings. Finally,
small differences (3.3%) in the number of overall messages that were violent can also be
observed. The differences in this area appear to be the most pronounced concerning the
pervasiveness of use among high-impact leaders and the general membership, though the
gangs that did not desist have higher average levels of violent and illegal rhetoric.
Finally, we calculated a series of partial correlations between our proxy for violence
during the evaluation period, the average change in the rate of shootings post engagement,
and the various social media variables described above. Data from all fourteen gangs who
were enrolled in the evaluation are used in this analysis. In calculating these statistics, we
control for the gang-level variables (heat level, number of reported rival gangs, average age,
size (members and associates), and the number of individuals on probation in the gang.
We also control for the number of times the gangs were “called-in” during the evaluation
period. As reported in Table 3 below, these associations provide some insight into both the
direction and potential strength of the relationship between virtual behaviors and violence
on the streets.
123
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
Table 3. Partial correlations between social media activity and shootings, after implementation of focused deterrence (n = 14).
Social Media Variables
Average change
per quarter in
counts of
gang-related
shootings
Illicit
Social
Media
Viol.
Social
Media
%
Violent
Posts
Feud
Online
Threat:
Rivals
Threat:
Law
Enforce
Other
Activity
%
Active
%
Impact
Overall
Social
Media
Correlation
Coef.
0.67
0.90
0.89
0.97
1.00
−0.45
0.35
0.39
0.51
0.63
Significance
(2-tailed)
0.33
0.10
0.10
0.03
0.00
0.55
0.65
0.61
0.49
0.37
**
**
*
*
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
2
df
2
2
NOTE: * p ≥ 0.05; ** p ≥ 0.1.
An examination of the correlation coefficients highlights a range of associations between social media usages among the fourteen gangs and the number of shootings in which
they were involved after becoming involved in the evaluation. The coefficient reported here
is one that must fall between −1 and +1, with larger positive numbers indicating a stronger
positive relationship, negative numbers indicating a stronger negative relationship, and
0 indicating no relationship is described by the data in the sample. It is illustrative to
consider these variables in two subgroups: first, measures of how active the gang was
online and, secondly, descriptions of the kinds of content that the gang posted online.
Overall, some of the variables that were associated with an increase in shootings
after the implementation of focused deterrence were those that described the general
presence of the gang on social media. For example, the overall percentage of “impact”
players, generally leaders and highly visible members, was significantly associated with
higher shooting rates (r = 0.89, p ≥ 0.1). The same is true about the overall level of social
media usage attributable to the gang (r = 0.88, p ≥ 0.1). While failing to reach statistical
significance, the overall percent of known gang members who were active on social media
demonstrated a similar pattern in the direction of the correlational relationship (r = 0.67,
ns) with more gangs with more identified shootings during focused deterrence.
A consideration of the type of content that the gang was seen as having posted to
social media paints a more complicated picture of the relationship between the internet
and the street. Three of the variables categorically describe the nature of the gang’s general
activity on social media. Illicit postings, those that reference illegal activities but are
not directly violent (e.g., post picturing or discussing guns, drugs, or ill-gotten cash),
are significantly and strongly associated with higher shooting rates (r = 0.97, p ≥ 0.05).
A similarly constructed variable capturing perceptions of overly violent content (e.g.,
threats and warnings) also reached statistical significance rates in this analysis (r = 0.99,
p ≥ 0.05). Similarly, the variable capturing the overall level of social media engagement
attributed to the gang was significant and positive (r = 0.88, p ≥ 0.1). However, the variables
that capture specific kinds of social media activity discussed as likely to spill over into
the streets had a different relationship with shootings in this analysis. Estimates of the
percentage of the number of a gang’s violent postings were negatively associated with
shooting (r = −0.45, ns), a surprising result. Other measures assessing specific kinds of
social media activity were also correlated with a positive change in the number of shootings
after their experience with focused deterrence began, though none reached statistical
significance: engagement in online feuds (r = 0.35), threats to rival gangs (r = 0.39), posting
of other, illicit types (r = 0.62). Finally, the correlation with our measure of the extent to
which the gang threatened law enforcement officers online was also positive, though not
statistically significant (r = 0.51).
124
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
7. Discussion
Violence, especially shootings, can be pervasive and deeply engrained into gangrelated actions (Decker 1996). This has presented a persistent challenge for both law
enforcement and public policymakers (see, e.g., Papachristos 2011). Focused deterrence is
one shooting reduction intervention that has been both widely adopted and enjoys a fairly
robust and supportive foundation within the evaluation literature (see Kennedy et al. 1996;
Kennedy 2019; Braga and Weisburd 2012; Braga et al. 2018). Social media, over the past ten
years, have taken on a central role in how many gangs develop and express their identity
(Storrod and Densley 2017), and it has been argued that they may contribute to subsequent
violence (Patton et al. 2019). Despite these parallel and contemporaneous trends, few
studies have sought to analytically examine the impact of social media usage by gangs
within the framework of a violence reduction intervention. There are many reasons for this,
including challenges in obtaining the relevant empirical data and rapid changes in social
media usage and platforms (Irwin-Rogers et al. 2018). The results of the current study,
which makes these preliminary connections, can inform our theoretical understanding of
the nexus between online activity and street violence, as well as provide evidence of new
avenues of emphasis for focused deterrence.
The descriptive statistics on gang-level social media usage paint a picture that is
largely consistent with the profile that was developed within ethnographic and qualitative
studies in this area. As multiple scholars have noted, gang communications made online can
be both expressive (e.g., Stuart 2020) and utilitarian (e.g., Johnson and Schell-Busey 2016).
Threats of violence that do not translate to action may be considered the former, while
discussion of criminal activity may be the latter; we find evidence of both. These data
also show that, on average, the gangs in this study promoted violence in almost one-third
of their postings; all gangs had a group-level online presence, and not a single gang was
scored as not using that platform to engage in activity online related to illegal activities.
These descriptive results reinforce the argument that social media may play a dominant role in general communication for gangs in the current moment. For the gangs in
this study, even a cursory examination of the descriptive data shows that not all social
media postings are equal, with some being used for expressive and arguably non-violence
purposes. For example, the percent of violent postings ranged from 15% to 60% across all
gangs in the sample. This range was largely the same within the gangs who were grouped
based on this response to focused deterrence, suggesting that while usage itself is pervasive,
gangs employ social media to deliver a wide range of rhetoric. The support of characteristics of communication common in the current literature strengthens the robustness of the
assumptions that underlie them as the current study relies on a different methodological
approach and investigates gangs in a previously unexamined jurisdiction.
A comparison of those gangs that had an increased rate of shootings with those that
had a decreased rate of shootings during focused deterrence shows some differentials
of note. We find, as others have (e.g., Harding 2014), that the gangs that increased their
shootings are younger by about two years on average. They are also slightly larger in
terms of the number of members. This difference is compounded by the fact that a higher
percentage of the gang, both overall and of the “impact” members, are engaging in these
forums. This, in turn, translates to a higher overall social media usage score and, in all
likelihood, a larger and more visible footprint for the gang on the internet. This reinforces
the commonly made assertion that gang intervention strategies have evolved to integrate
social media as an important venue for communication (Bock 2012; Densley 2020; Pyrooz
and Moule 2019).
Our results paint a more complex picture about the correlates between online activity and shootings among all of the gangs in this sample. On one hand, we find some
differentiation based on the types of social media content. Directly threatening rivals, as
well as being willing to openly challenge law enforcement, which can easily be assumed
to translate to the kinds of in-person confrontations that may lead to violence, did not
correlate with our outcome. The same was found concerning the development of online
125
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
feuds; online feuds did not appear to be associated with shootings on the street. Unlike
some of the results of recent examinations (Patton et al. 2019), we find that it is not the
explicitly violent rhetoric directed at identifiable parties that correlated with shootings.
Instead, the significant associations were linked to the overall tenor and tone of the gang’s
presence on social media. Here, for example, the overall measures of violent and illegal
content that were believed to characterize the online presence of the gang were found to be
significant.
It may be the case that threats on social media activity are more easily dismissed
as puffery and posturing (Densley 2013; Felson 2006) and not such a viable threat that
rivals feel compelled to respond in the immediate. Alternatively, this may be because
violent rhetoric may be rejected as more performative than similar activities by gangs,
especially in the more anonymous and less inhibited online space (Stuart 2020; but see
Patton et al. 2017b). A more concrete and ongoing discussion of illegal activity, which
here includes the display of cash and weapons without threats, could have been seen as
a more definitive indicator of viable criminality and less as online chatter. These types
of activities may be seen as openly and genuinely contemptuous of law enforcement as
well (Sandberg and Ugelvik 2016). When backed up with pictures, posts openly displaying
illegality may simply reflect more criminally active gangs (and so they are more likely
to find its members in an encounter or scenario that develops into a shooting), given the
clear ties between online conduct and lived experience (e.g., Roks et al. 2020). This may
be especially true when the more overly violent commentary comes from gangs that are
younger (though the differences here are small and could be practically insignificant).
Future studies should explore these preliminarily identified relationships more deeply, and,
where possible, within a causal framework.
The results of this study also provide support for some of the currently hypothesized
theoretical frameworks that may link online activity by gang members to violence on the
streets, especially shootings. From a Differential Association perspective, firstly, social
media might function to increase the frequency, duration, and intensity of face-to-face
gang communications (McCuddy and Esbensen 2020). It may also increase exposure to
unique or online-only gang associations, thus introducing members to individuals with
different constructions of definitions favorable toward crime, the acceptability of violence,
and the necessity of having firearms. At the very least, social media create the illusion of
proximity, connectivity, and having a large audience, both locally and internationally, that
far exceeds the opportunities available using offline communications. In the current data,
we see this reflected in the extent of overall usage of social media, as well as the number
of “impact” players who are present in these online forums. The usage of social media
is nearly universal among the gangs in this sample, suggesting that their audience and
influences may be both evolving in a difficult-to-predict direction.
When considering these results from a Routine Activities perspective, we also find
evidence for a relationship between online activity and violence. In this study that the
presence of gang members on social media is hardly benign: threats, overt criminal activity,
and other illegal behaviors are reported as being commonplace in this sample. Generally,
gang members report high rates of offending and victimization in online settings, including
harassment, intimidation, and violent threats (Pyrooz et al. 2015), an outcome supported
by these gang-level activity data. Given the extent of usage, in the social media age, it is
likely that individuals in these gangs are tasked with creating a continuous stream of gangrelated content for consumption as one of their duties of gang membership (Storrod and
Densley 2017). On the one hand, this might incapacitate gang members for a short period
by keeping them focused on their screens instead of on the street. However, as Lauger
and Densley (2018) observed in their content analysis of YouTube rap videos produced by
gangs in upstate New York, the internet is a natural extension of the street in part because
it meets the symbolic needs of gang members as a status enhancer. Short-term benefits
may be lost as the give-and-take between social media and the street becomes a basic
function of gang life. The fact that gang reputations can now be quantified by the number
126
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
of followers, likes, and retweets creates incentives to “do gang” (Lauger and Densley 2018)
and “perform” gang membership for status or to save face (Van Hellemont 2012). This
raises the prospect that gang members are “taken in by their own act” and find themselves
unable to break character on the street, no matter the invasiveness of, or potentially benefits
inherent in, engaging in a violence-reduction intervention (Goffman 1959). The impact on
street shootings for the gangs more deeply engrained in this way of thinking is possibly
reflected in the correlations observed here.
Lauger et al. (2020), using General Strain Theory as their guide, argue that threatening
or insulting online material becomes a tension that is more likely to incite violence when it is
seen as unjust, high in magnitude, is associated with low social control, and creates pressure
or incentives for criminal coping. For example, in The Digital Street, Lane (2018) argues
that social media has not only blurred the boundaries between the physical and virtual
worlds but has extended Anderson (1999) “code of the street” online (see also, Urbanik and
Haggerty 2018). Here, the correlations, albeit limited, between some measures of social
media usage and shootings support the robustness of this relationship. This relationship
goes in both directions; previous survey research has shown that gang members who are
more invested in the code of the street are also more likely to respond violently to online
threats, (Moule et al. 2017). In another study, gang-involved youth interpreted “dissing”
(content that humiliates and degrades), “calling out” (content that challenges or questions
someone’s reputation or social status), and “direct threats” as the most threatening forms of
communication on Twitter (Patton et al. 2019). In this case, the gangs with higher degrees
of “impact” players engaging online, perhaps a proxy for gang-level investment, was one
of the stronger correlates with an increased number of shootings during focused deterrence.
The resulting actions by gang members, unlike the messages themselves, may spill out
onto the streets as shootings as the result of these increased digital tensions
Finally, and perhaps most usefully, these results provide a foundation for a reconsideration of how focused deterrence specifically, and gang violence reduction policy more
generally, can take these issues into account. In many ways, the keystone of focused
deterrence is the messaging about the consequences to gang members if any member
commits a shooting (as well as the benefits of abstaining, including the services that are
typically made available). Practically, when certain members of the gang are “called in” to
hear these propositions from law enforcement and community leaders, it is assumed that
they will transmit the message to others in their gang and that this message will be heard.
Here, given the correlations found, the near-constant drone of social media chatter may be
“drowning out” that message for certain gangs.
The extent to which a gang actively engages with social media may, in itself, be a useful
proxy for the extent to which gangs are willing to disregard or are unable to internalize
the messaging in focused deterrence. Given the way law enforcement has responded to
internet activity, actively posting illegal content, especially postings designed to flaunt
criminal behaviors or taunt rivals, is risky (Densley 2013). Therefore, this could serve as
a signal that a gang’s desire to be seen as violent or a threat to rivals may supersede the
more practical, but distant, consequences communicated out in the focused deterrence
messaging. (Sandberg and Ugelvik 2016). Practically, during focused deterrence public
social media activity could be captured in near-real-time and examined at the gang, not the
individual level. In this way, these data may provide data-driven feedback on how certain
gangs are responding to the intervention. This may inform which gangs are “called in” for
meetings under the focused deterrence guidelines in that jurisdiction.
Within an existing researcher-practitioner partnership, social media analysis can
provide a useful opportunity to tailor a program or intervention to the realities of the
street (Sierra-Arévalo and Papachristos 2017). The formative feedback and hypothesis
development process, facilitated by independent researchers, can help better identify areas
of the strategy (e.g., messaging, forums, community partners engaged) that might not be
working, or that need differential focus to be more effective in reaching the unresponsive
target gangs. In Philadelphia, for example, there was a strong emphasis on fidelity to the
127
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
focused deterrence model, which had been collaboratively developed at the outset of the
project period. While an important aspect when replicating a national model, this may
have supported a focus on responding fully to each potential opportunity for a crackdown,
leading to missed opportunities to flexibly adapt to the shifting realities on the street.
Building in a process for the analysis and discussion of social media data may formalize
such an opportunity in future iterations of focused deterrence.
Finally, the results of this descriptive analysis should bring these issues to the forefront
for the justice system and community stakeholders seeking to understand how gangs
communicate and whether and how aspects of social media use have a street component.
Here, the types of social media posting that are most likely to be considered an immediate
threat, including threats of violence, did not correlate with an increase in shootings during
focused deterrence. Instead, it was a broader pattern of online engagement that best
reflected an increased risk. The findings here should be interpreted with the proper
caveats. There may be measurement errors or other unknown biases in the variables as
constructed. For example, the variable constructed as a percentage of the number of a gang’s
postings that were violent is dependent on the assumption that the social media information
collected by law enforcement appropriately represents the extent of a gang’s social postings.
Regardless, the study results underscore the potential value of holistically examining the
holistic picture of a gang’s internet activity as well as individual posts when seeking to
understand how a particular gang may act in the future. These results do not, however,
support the examination of social media usage at the individual level, as there are myriad
methodological and ethical issues inherent in that approach. While there is a significant
amount of research and policy development necessary to develop, examine, and evaluate
the nature of these relationships, the current findings provide an impetus and justification
for expanding the scope of this critical work.
8. Limitations
In addition to the limitation mentioned directly above on measurement error, there
are additional limitations inherent in the data available and the methods used in the
current analysis. First, it should be noted that this study includes only a small number of
gangs overall, and the implementation of focused deterrence in Philadelphia was unique
(see Roman et al. 2019, 2020). Additionally, the gangs studied were from one section of
Philadelphia that may have unique norms and culture. This constrains the generalizability
of the findings, both within the local context and to other cities. Secondly, and related to
measurement error, the data that were gathered during both auditing processes are the best
and, in some cases, the only data of this nature, but they have limitations. Audit data are
inherently retrospective, represent the perspective of a small number of law enforcement
officers and/or agency staff, are unverified outside of the auditing process (and could
be unverifiable using administrative data), and do not include the perspectives of the
justice-involved members that are described by these data. Additionally, the intrinsic
uncertainty regarding actual internet usage and behaviors meant that much of the social
media data were reduced to being estimated as categorical variables or using broad scales;
this limits the precision of the data and results. Finally, the methods employed here provide
only descriptive statistics or describe only correlations with shootings; these results are not
causal and should not be interpreted as such. Even with these limitations, the outcome
of this study sheds empirical light on a debated, but rarely measured, aspect of life for
gang-involved individuals and provides an opportunity to reconsider the results of an
effort to reduce shootings in Philadelphia.
9. Conclusions
Focused deterrence is a widely used intervention to reduce gang-related shootings.
When implemented in Philadelphia, a quasi-experimental evaluation found a reduction
in shootings across the community that comprised 14 targeted gangs. However, when
examining the change in shootings by gangs, it becomes clear that not all gangs responded
128
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
equivalently, with some demonstrating an increasing rate of known shootings during
the assessment. Here, we find descriptive evidence that there are differences between
those gangs that responded well (i.e., a decrease in gang shootings) and those that did
not regarding the nature and content of social media activity. When looking at all the
participating gangs, we find that their overall level of engagement with social media,
especially by high visibility gang members, was significantly correlated with a higher
level of shootings during the implementation of focused deterrence. The variables that
measured the posting of specific kinds of violent content, on the other hand, did not reach
significance. These findings provide preliminary evidence on the potentially mediating
role that public, social media content may have on efforts to reduce gang shootings. While
more robust and causal evidence is needed to further specify these relationships, the role
of social media should not be ignored when developing harm-prevention interventions,
including focused deterrence, for this population of gang-involved individuals.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization: J.M.H., J.A.D., C.G.R.; methodology: J.M.H., C.G.R.;
formal analysis: J.M.H., C.G.R.; investigation: J.M.H., C.G.R.; writing—original draft preparation:
J.M.H., J.A.D., C.G.R.; writing—review and editing: J.M.H., J.A.D., C.G.R.; project administration:
J.M.H., C.G.R.; funding acquisition: C.G.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version
of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was supported by grant number 2013-IJ-CX-0056 awarded to Temple University by the United States Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice. Opinions or points of
view expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies
of the Department of Justice or Temple University.
Institutional Review Board Statement: IRB protocol 21979 was approved (original version) by the
Temple University Review Board on 4 February 2015.
Acknowledgments: The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of their law enforcement
partners, with special thanks to Matt York and the members of the Philadelphia Police Department’s
(PPD) South Gang Task Force, Deputy Commissioner Joel Dales, the PPD Central Intelligence Unit,
and the PPD Research & Analysis Unit. In particular, Matt York and his colleagues participated in an
ongoing dialogue with the research team to examine the possible reasons behind the differences in
levels of shootings observed during focused deterrence.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Anderson, Elijah. 1999. Code of the Street. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.
Bock, Joseph G. 2012. The Technology of Nonviolence: Social Media and Violence Prevention. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Braga, Anthony. 2008. Pulling levers focused deterrence strategies and the prevention of gun homicide. Journal of Criminal Justice 36:
332–43. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony, and David L. Weisburd. 2012. The effects of focused deterrence strategies on crime: A systematic review and
meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 49: 323–58. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony, Jack McDevitt, and Glenn L. Pierce. 2006. Understanding and preventing gang violence: Problem analysis and
response development in Lowell, Massachusetts. Police Quarterly 9: 20–46. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony, David Hureau, and Andrew Papachristos. 2014. Deterring gang-involved gun violence: Measuring the impact of
Boston’s Operation Ceasefire on street gang behavior. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 30: 113–39. [CrossRef]
Braga, Anthony, David Weisburd, and Brandon Turchan. 2018. Focused deterrence strategies and crime control: An updated systematic
review and meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Criminology & Public Policy 17: 205–50.
Brayne, Sarah. 2017. Big data surveillance: The case of policing. American Sociological Review 82: 977–1008. [CrossRef]
Corsaro, Nicholas, and Robin S. Engel. 2015. Most challenging of contexts: Assessing the impact of focused deterrence on serious
violence in New Orleans. Criminology & Public Policy 14: 471–505.
Decker, Scott. 1996. Collective and normative features of gang violence. Justice Quarterly 13: 243–64. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott, David Pyrooz, and James Densley. 2021. On Gangs. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Densley, James. 2013. How Gangs Work. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Densley, James. 2020. Collective Violence Online: When Street Gangs Use Social Media. In The Handbook of Collective Violence: Current
Developments and Understanding. Edited by Carol Ireland, Michael Lewis, Anthony Lopez and Jane Ireland. London: Routledge,
pp. 305–16.
129
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
Densley, James, and David Jones. 2016. Pulling Levers on Gang Violence in London and St. Paul. In Gang Transitions and Transformations
in an International Context. Edited by Cheryl Maxson and Finn-Aage Esbensen. Cham: Springer, pp. 291–305.
Densley, James, and David Pyrooz. 2020. The Matrix In Context: Taking Stock Of Police Gang Databases In London and Beyond. Youth
Justice 20: 11–30. [CrossRef]
Deuchar, Ross. 2013. Policing Youth Violence: Transatlantic Connections. London: IOE Press.
Dewing, Michael. 2010. Social Media: An Introduction. Ottawa: Library of Parliament, vol. 1.
Eckberg, Deborah, James Densley, and Katrinna Dexter. 2018. When Legend Becomes Fact, Tweet the Legend: Information and
Misinformation in the Age of Social Media. Journal of Behavioral and Social Sciences 5: 148–56.
Fedushko, Solomiia, Tomáš Peráček, Yuriy Syerov, and Olha Trach. 2021. Development of Methods for the Strategic Management of
Web Projects. Sustainability 13: 742. [CrossRef]
Felson, Marcus. 2006. The Street Gang Strategy. In Crime and Nature. Edited by Marcus Felson. Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 305–24.
Frey, William R., Desmond U. Patton, Michael B. Gaskell, and Kyle A. McGregor. 2020. Artificial intelligence and inclusion: Formerly
gang-involved youth as domain experts for analyzing unstructured twitter data. Social Science Computer Review 38: 42–56.
[CrossRef]
Goffman, Erving. 1959. The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Anchor.
Goldman, Liran, Howard Giles, and Michael A. Hogg. 2014. Going to extremes: Social identity and communication processes associated
with gang membership. Available online: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1368430214524289 (accessed on
19 April 2021).
Graham, William. 2016. Global Concepts, Local Contexts: A Case Study of International Criminal Justice Policy Transfer in Violence
Reduction. Available online: http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?did=1&uin=uk.bl.ethos.726773 (accessed on 19 April 2021).
Gravel, Jason, and George E. Tita. 2015. With great methods come great responsibilities: Social network analysis in the implementation
and evaluation of gang programs. Criminology and Public Policy 14: 559. [CrossRef]
Harding, Simon. 2014. The Street Casino. Bristol: Policy Press.
Howell, James. 2007. Menacing or Mimicking? Realities of Youth Gangs. Juvenile and Family Court Journal 58: 39–50. [CrossRef]
Irwin-Rogers, Kier, James Densley, and Craig Pinkney. 2018. Gang Violence and Social Media. In The Routledge International Handbook of
Human Aggression. Edited by Jane Ireland, Philip Birch and Carol Ireland. Abingdon: Routledge, pp. 400–10.
Johnson, Joseph, and Natalie Schell-Busey. 2016. Old Message in a New Bottle: Taking Gang Rivalries Online Through Rap Battle
Music Videos on YouTube. Journal of Qualitative Criminal Justice and Criminology 4: 42–81.
Kennedy, David M. 2009. Gangs and public policy: Constructing and deconstructing gang databases. Criminology & Public Policy 8: 711.
Kennedy, David. 2019. Policing and the lessons of focused deterrence. In Police innovation: Contrasting Perspectives. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, pp. 205–26.
Kennedy, David, Anne Piehl, and Anthony Braga. 1996. Youth violence in Boston: Gun markets, serious youth offenders, and a
use-reduction strategy. Law and Contemporary Problems 59: 147–96. [CrossRef]
Kennedy, David, Anthony Braga, and Anne Piehl. 2001. Developing and Implementing Operation Ceasefire. In Reducing Gun Violence:
The Boston Gun Project’s Operation Ceasefire; Edited by U.S. Department of Justice Office of Justice Programs. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Justice, pp. 5–54.
Lane, Jeffrey. 2018. The Digital Street. New York: Oxford University Press.
Lane, Jeff, Fanny Ramirez, and Katy Pearce. 2018. Guilty by Visible Association: Socially Mediated Visibility in Gang Prosecutions.
Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication 23: 354–69. [CrossRef]
Lauger, Tim, and James Densley. 2018. Broadcasting Badness: Violence, Identity, and Performance in the Online Gang Rap Scene.
Justice Quarterly 35: 816–41. [CrossRef]
Lauger, Tim, James Densley, and Richard Moule. 2020. Social Media, Strain, and Technologically-Facilitated Gang Violence. In The
Palgrave Handbook of International Cybercrime and Cyberdeviance. Edited by Adam Bossler and Thomas Holt. New York: Palgrave
Macmillan, pp. 1375–95.
Leverso, John, and Yuan Hsiao. 2020. Gangbangin On The [Face]Book: Understanding Online Interactions of Chicago Latina/o Gangs.
Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. [CrossRef]
McCuddy, Timothy, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2020. The Role of Online Communication Among Gang and Non-gang Youth. In Gangs
in the Era of Internet and Social Media. Edited by Chris Melde and Frank Weerman. Cham: Springer, pp. 81–104.
Melde, Chris, and Frank Weerman. 2020. Gangs in the Era of Internet and Social Media. Cham: Springer.
Moule, Richard, David Pyrooz, and Scott Decker. 2013. From “What The F#@% Is A Facebook” To “Who Doesn’t Use Facebook?”: The
Role of Criminal Lifestyles in The Adoption Of The Use Of The Internet. Social Science Research 42: 1411–21. [PubMed]
Moule, Richard, David Pyrooz, and Scott Decker. 2014. Internet Adoption and Online Behaviour Among American Street Gangs:
Integrating Gangs And Organizational Theory. British Journal of Criminology 54: 1186–206. [CrossRef]
Moule, Richard, Scott Decker, and David Pyrooz. 2017. Technology and Conflict: Group Processes and Collective Violence in the
Internet Era. Crime, Law and Social Change 68: 47–73. [CrossRef]
Nakamura, Kiminori, George Tita, and David Krackhardt. 2020. Violence in the “balance”: A structural analysis of how rivals, allies,
and third-parties shape inter-gang violence. Global Crime 21: 3–27. [CrossRef]
Nickerson, Raymond. 1998. Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises. Review of General Psychology 2: 175–220.
[CrossRef]
130
Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 147
Papachristos, Andrew. 2011. Too big to fail: The science and politics of violence prevention. Criminology & Public Policy 10: 1053.
Papachristos, Andrew, and David S. Kirk. 2015. Changing the street dynamic: Evaluating Chicago’s group violence reduction strategy.
Criminology & Public Policy 14: 525–58.
Pariser, Eli. 2011. The Filter Bubble. New York: Penguin.
Patton, Desmond, Robert Eschmann, and Dirk Butler. 2013. Internet Banging: New Trends in Social Media, Gang Violence, Masculinity
and Hip Hop. Computers in Human Behavior 29: A54–A59. [CrossRef]
Patton, Desmond, Robert Eschmann, Caitlin Elsaesser, and Eddie Bocanegrad. 2016. Sticks, Stones and Facebook Accounts: What
Violence Outreach Workers Know About Social Media and Urban-Based Gang Violence in Chicago. Computers in Human Behavior
65: 591–600. [CrossRef]
Patton, Desmond, Douglas-Wade Brunton, Andrea Dixon, Reuben Miller, Patrick Leonard, and Rose Hackman. 2017a. Stop and Frisk
Online: Theorizing Everyday Racism in Digital Policing in The Use of Social Media for Identification of Criminal Conduct and
Associations. Social Media and Society 3: 1–10. [CrossRef]
Patton, Desmond, Jeffrey Lane, Patrick Leonard, Jamie Macbeth, and Jocelyn Smith Lee. 2017b. Gang Violence on the Digital Street:
Case Study of a South Side Chicago Gang Member’s Twitter Communication. New Media and Society 19: 1000–18. [CrossRef]
Patton, Desmond, Owen Rambow, Jonathan Auerbach, Kevin Li, and William Frey. 2018. Expressions of Loss Predict Aggressive
Comments on Twitter Among Gang-Involved Youth in Chicago. NPJ Digital Medicine 1: 11. [CrossRef]
Patton, Desmond, David Pyooz, Scott Decker, William Frey, and Patrick Leonard. 2019. When Twitter Fingers Turn to Trigger Fingers:
A Sociolinguistic Study of Internet-Mediated Gang Violence. International Journal of Bullying Prevention 1: 205–17. [CrossRef]
Pawelz, Janina, and Paul Elvers. 2018. The Digital Hood of Urban Violence: Exploring Functionalities of Social Media and Music
among Gangs. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 34: 442–59. [CrossRef]
Peterson, Jillian, and James Densley. 2017. Cyber Violence: What Do We Know and Where Do We Go From Here? Aggression and
Violent Behavior 34: 193–200. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David, and Richard Moule. 2019. Gangs and Social Media. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice.
[CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David, Scott Decker, and Richard Moule. 2015. Criminal and Routine Activities in Online Settings: Gangs, Offenders, and the
Internet. Justice Quarterly 32: 471–99. [CrossRef]
Roks, Robby, Rutger Leukfeldt, and James Densley. 2020. The Hybridization of Street Offending in the Netherlands. British Journal of
Criminology. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., Nathan W. Link, Jordan M. Hyatt, Avinash Bhati, and Megan Forney. 2019. Assessing the gang-level and
community-level effects of the Philadelphia Focused deterrence strategy. Journal of Experimental Criminology 15: 499–527.
[CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., Megan Forney, Jordan M. Hyatt, Hannah J. Klein, and Nathan W. Link. 2020. Law Enforcement Activities of
Philadelphia’s Group Violence Intervention: An Examination of Arrest, Case Processing, and Probation Levers. Police Quarterly
23: 232–61. [CrossRef]
Sandberg, Sveinung, and Thomas Ugelvik. 2016. Why Do Offenders Tape their Crimes? Crime and Punishment in the Age of the Selfie.
British Journal of Criminology 57: 1023–40. [CrossRef]
Sierra-Arévalo, Michael, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2015. Applying group audits to problem-oriented policing. In Disrupting
Criminal Networks: Network Analysis in Crime Prevention. Edited by Gisela Bichler and Aili E. Malm. Boulder: Lynne Rienner,
pp. 27–46.
Sierra-Arévalo, Michael, and Andrew V. Papachristos. 2017. Social networks and gang violence reduction. Annual Review of Law and
Social Science 13: 373–93. [CrossRef]
Storrod, Michelle, and James Densley. 2017. ‘Going Viral’ and ‘Going Country’: The Expressive and Instrumental Activities of Street
Gangs on Social Media. Journal of Youth Studies 20: 677–96. [CrossRef]
Stuart, Forrest. 2020. Code of the Tweet: Urban Gang Violence in the Social Media Age. Social Problems 67: 191–207. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., and Steven M. Radil. 2011. Spatializing the social networks of gangs to explore patterns of violence. Journal of
Quantitative Criminology 27: 521–45. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., K. Jack Riley, and Peter Greenwood. 2003. From Boston to Boyle Heights: The Process and Prospects of a ‘Pulling
Levers’ Strategy in a Los Angeles Barrio. In Policing Gangs and Youth Violence. Edited by Scott H. Decker. Belmont: Wadsworth
Publishing Company, pp. 102–30.
Urbanik, Marta, and Kevin Haggerty. 2018. #It’s Dangerous: The Online World of Drug Dealers, Rappers and the Street Code. The
British Journal of Criminology 58: 1343–60.
Van Hellemont, Elke. 2012. Gangland Online: Performing the Real Imaginary World of Gangstas and Ghettos in Brussels. European
Journal of Crime, Criminal Law and Criminal Justice 20: 159–73. [CrossRef]
Whittaker, Andrew, James Densley, and Karen Moser. 2020. No Two Gangs are Alike: The Digital Divide in Street Gangs’ Differential
Adaptations to Social Media. Computers in Human Behavior 110: 106403. [CrossRef]
Williams, Damien J., Dorothy Currie, Will Linden, and Peter D. Donnelly. 2014. Addressing gang-related violence in Glasgow: A
preliminary pragmatic quasi-experimental evaluation of the Community Initiative to Reduce Violence (CIRV). Aggression and
Violence Behavior 19: 686–91. [CrossRef]
Zuboff, Shoshana. 2018. The Age of Surveillance Capitalism. New York: Public Affairs.
131
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Review
Street Gang Intervention: Review and Good
Lives Extension
Jaimee Mallion 1, *
1
2
*
and Jane Wood 2
School of Psychology, London South Bank University, London SE1 6LN, UK
School of Psychology, University of Kent, Kent CT2 7NZ, UK;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Received: 7 August 2020; Accepted: 9 September 2020; Published: 15 September 2020
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Abstract: Tackling street gangs has recently been highlighted as a priority for public health. In this
paper, the four components of a public health approach were reviewed: (1) surveillance, (2) identifying
risk and protective factors, (3) developing and evaluating interventions at primary prevention,
secondary prevention, and tertiary intervention stages, and (4) implementation of evidence-based
programs. Findings regarding the effectiveness of prevention and intervention programs for street
gang members were mixed, with unclear goals/objectives, limited theoretical foundation, and a lack of
consistency in program implementation impeding effectiveness at reducing street gang involvement.
This paper proposes that the Good Lives Model (GLM), a strengths-based framework for offender
rehabilitation, provides an innovative approach to street gang intervention. Utilizing approach-goals,
the GLM assumes that improving an individual’s internal skills and external opportunities will
reduce the need to become involved in street gangs. Wrapping the GLM framework around current
evidence-based interventions (e.g., Functional Family Therapy) increases client engagement and
motivation to change, which is notably poor amongst those at risk of, or involved in, street gangs.
Keywords: street gangs; public health; Good Lives Model; intervention; prevention
1. Introduction
Street gangs are a growing problem internationally, with countries including the UK, USA, Sweden,
China, and the Netherlands reporting a marked increase in street gang membership (e.g., Chui and
Khiatani 2018; Roks and Densley 2020; Rostami 2017). In the UK alone, the number of street gang
affiliated youths has seen a dramatic increase over a five-year period. The Children’s Commissioner
(2017) approximated that in 2013/14, 46,000 young people were either directly gang-involved or
knew a street gang member. By 2019 this figure had increased to 27,000 full street gang members,
60,000 affiliates, and a further 313,000 youths who knew a street gang member (Children’s Commissioner
2019). Similar increases have been seen in the USA, with a 40.83% growth in the number of different street
gangs between 2002 and 2012 (National Gang Center 2020). As such, the World Health Organization
(World Health Organization 2020) has highlighted youth violence, including street gang membership,
as a global public health problem that requires an immediate international response.
Street gang membership is associated with increased perpetration of illegal activities, particularly
serious and violent offences (Pyrooz et al. 2016), with this relationship stable across time, place,
and definitions of street gangs (Dong and Krohn 2016). As such, street gangs are responsible
for causing heightened levels of fear and victimization amongst members of their community
(Howell 2007). In addition, street gang involvement has adverse health, welfare, and economic
consequences for individual members, which persist long after disengagement (Connolly and Jackson
2019; Petering 2016). For instance, longitudinal research identified that adults who belonged to a street
gang during adolescence experienced more mental and physical health issues than their non-gang
133
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
counterparts (Gilman et al. 2014). Adolescent street gang members also experience more economic
hardship during adulthood than their non-gang peers, with higher rates of unemployment and reliance
on welfare benefits or illicit income (Krohn et al. 2011). Furthermore, street gang involvement during
adolescence has a detrimental effect on the development of long-term stable family relationships,
with former members more likely to engage in intimate partner violence and child maltreatment
(Augustyn et al. 2014).
Considering these long-term and wide-ranging effects of street gang membership, it is unsurprising
that there has been a proliferation of prevention and intervention programs developed and implemented
world-wide. Although literature is beginning to emerge which suggests some of these are effective
programs at reducing street gang involvement, there remains a paucity of reliable evidence to date.
Highlighted by Wong et al. (2011). such programs often suffer from a lack of theoretical foundation
(McGloin and Decker 2010), clear goals and objectives (Klein and Maxson 2006), and methodologically
sound evaluation (Curry 2010). These factors are associated with an increased risk of harmful outcomes
for program participants (Welsh and Rocque 2014), including negative labeling and heightened rates
of recidivism (Petrosino et al. 2010). Thus, discovering “what works” in street gang prevention and
intervention is essential.
A public health approach to street gang membership has recently been suggested (Gebo 2016),
which could guide the development of effective prevention and intervention strategies. WHO
(Krug et al. 2002) suggests four key elements for a public health approach, including: (1) surveillance,
(2) identifying risk and protective factors, (3) developing and evaluating interventions, and (4)
implementation. See Figure 1 for an overview of each of these elements in relation to street gang
prevention and intervention. Using a public health approach, street gang intervention occurs across
three levels (Conaglen and Gallimore 2014): primary prevention (early intervention approaches prior to
initiation of street gang involvement), secondary prevention (interventions specifically for individuals
at-risk of street gang involvement), and tertiary prevention (long-term rehabilitation strategies for
those who have engaged in street gangs). In addition, public health interventions can be universally
implemented (aimed at the general population), selected (targeted towards those at-risk of street gang
involvement), or indicated (targeted specifically at street gang members).
Figure 1. WHO’s public health approach to violence prevention (Krug et al. 2002), adapted for street
gang intervention.
134
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Public health approaches have seen a number of successes in reducing behaviors related to street
gang membership (e.g., substance misuse, child maltreatment and youth violence; HM Government
2019; Pickering and Sanders 2015; Public Health England 2015). However, research is limited regarding
the effectiveness of interventions for street gang members (McDaniel et al. 2014). The aim of this paper
is to narratively summarize and evaluate existing street gang prevention and intervention programs,
within a public health approach. Aspects of the public health approach will be outlined in relation to
street gang membership, including: (1) surveillance (i.e., street gang definitions), (2) risk and protective
factors, (3) current street gang prevention and intervention programs (including primary, secondary,
and tertiary interventions). Furthermore, this paper will examine how a novel approach to offender
rehabilitation, termed the Good Lives Model (Ward and Fortune 2013), could be used as a framework
to guide street gang intervention.
2. Surveillance
Surveillance is a core aspect of a public health approach, which informs the development and
implementation of prevention and intervention programs (Richards et al. 2017). Surveillance involves
establishing clear definitions regarding the population of interest (i.e., street gang members), enabling
the identification of both those in need of intervention and the associated risk factors (Department of
Health 2012). By implementing surveillance measures, such as analyzing knife crime and criminal
convictions data, the extent of the problem in society on a local, national, and international scale can
be recognized (World Health Organization 2010). Ongoing monitoring enables any changes in the
patterns or frequencies of behavior to be quickly identified and disseminated to intervention providers,
informing the decision-making process (Public Health England 2017).
Street Gang Definition
The definition of a street gang member has been a matter of ongoing debate amongst academics,
policy-makers, and stakeholders for decades (e.g., Esbensen and Maxson 2012). To date, no single,
standardized definition of a street gang has been agreed. The ambiguity surrounding the definition of
a street gang has serious consequences for the development of effective prevention and intervention
strategies. As Melde (2016, p. 160) explains, “you cannot manage what you cannot measure”. Without a
reliable and valid definition, stakeholders are unable to accurately measure the rates of street gang
members and street gang-related offending. In addition, a lack of clear definitional criteria prevents an
assessment of the short- and long-term impact of prevention and intervention strategies on street gang
dynamics (Melde 2016).
To overcome this, stakeholders often devise their own street gang definition, which allows them
to undertake surveillance procedures and see the impact that prevention and intervention strategies
have on the local area. However, definitions of a street gang often vary widely from one region
to the next (Gilbertson and Malinksi 2005). For instance, each jurisdiction in the USA has its own
definition of a street gang and what constitutes a street gang-related offence (for a summary of
definitions, see National Gang Center 2016). Despite attempting to measure the same phenomenon,
by using different definitions a large disparity is likely to emerge in the estimates of street gang
members and rates of street gang-related offending between areas. Dependent on the definition used,
an over-identification (incorrectly identifying an issue as related to street gang membership, when it is
not) or under-identification (incorrectly identifying an issue as unrelated to street gangs, when it is) of
street gang members and street gang-related offending can occur (Joseph and Gunter 2011). As such,
prevention and intervention strategies for street gang members may be offered to too few or too many
in the local area. Furthermore, the differences in definitions used mean the generalizability of any
prevention and intervention strategies across areas is limited.
One method of identifying street gang members is through self-nomination, whereby stakeholders
simply ask individuals “are you currently in a gang?” (Esbensen et al. 2011). Past research has found
self-nomination to be a valid and effective method of identifying street gang members (e.g., Decker et al.
135
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
2014; Esbensen et al. 2001; Matsuda et al. 2012). In addition, self-nomination of street gang membership
is associated with heightened levels of violent crime (Melde et al. 2016), which is consistent with the
extensive research suggesting street gang members are more likely to commit serious and violent
offences than their non-gang counterparts (Melde and Esbensen 2013). However, self-nomination
relies on the individuals’ willingness to respond honestly, which could be reduced due to the negative
impact of disclosing street gang membership (e.g., risk of incarceration or retaliation from street
gang peers). Critically, self-nomination is dependent upon an individual’s subjective understanding
and interpretation of the term ‘gang’ (Tonks and Stephenson 2018). As public health surveillance
requires street gang members to be identified by an objective party, self-nomination methods would
not be appropriate.
The Eurogang Network, a group of the world’s leading street gang researchers, attempted to
establish a standardized definition of a street gang, which would allow cross-national comparative
research and surveillance (Klein and Maxson 2006). According to the Eurogang definition, a street gang
is a “durable, street-oriented youth group whose involvement in illegal activity is part of their group
identity” (Weerman et al. 2009, p. 20). Specifically, the group must: (1) include more than three people,
(2) last longer than three months, (3) be street-orientated, (4) be acceptive of illegal activities, and (5)
engage in illegal activities together (Matsuda et al. 2012). Critically, the Eurogang definition does not
require an individual to self-nominate in order to be classed as a street gang member. The Eurogang
Network avoids using the term ‘gang’ due to its emotive nature, instead preferring “troublesome
youth group” (Esbensen and Weerman 2005).
Although the Eurogang definition is increasingly adhered to in academic research, policy-makers
and stakeholders are resistant to its use. For instance, stakeholders have suggested that avoidance
of the term ‘gang’ reduces their ability to effectively distinguish between a street gang and a
group of individuals who happen to commit offences together (Centre for Social Justice 2009;
Pearce and Pitts 2011). Supporting this, researchers have found that the Eurogang definition leads
to an over-categorization of groups as street gangs (e.g., illegal ravers, peer groups who consume
drugs; Medina et al. 2013). Aldridge et al. (2012) suggests this is due to a lack of defining criteria
concerning street gang members engagement in violent crime. Despite typically being used in academia
as a self-report measure, the Eurogang criteria are observable (i.e., stakeholders can see whether a
young person is in a large street-based group, committing crimes), enabling surveillance measures
for identifying and monitoring street gangs (Melde 2016). To support consistency across surveillance
measures and intervention provision, it is recommended that the Eurogang definition is used to guide
a public health approach to street gangs.
3. Risk and Protective Factors
A public health approach involves developing an understanding of the causes of street gang
membership (Local Government Association 2018). This takes two forms, with the identification of
risk factors (increasing the likelihood of street gang involvement) and protective factors (reducing the
likelihood of street gang involvement). By establishing a framework of risk and protective factors,
this informs the development of prevention and intervention strategies aimed at reducing involvement
in street gangs. To date, focus has been placed on identifying the risk factors for street gang membership,
with a paucity of research on the protective factors (McDaniel 2012). This section will outline the risk
and protective factors for street gang membership that have been identified.
3.1. Risk Factors for Street Gang Membership
Past research has demonstrated that there are a wide range of risk factors robustly associated with
street gang membership. These span each of the five major risk factor domains: the individual, peers,
family, school, and community (O’Brien et al. 2013). The risk factors which have been related to street
gang membership across each of these domains are summarized in Table 1. Critically, Klein and Maxson
(2006) noted that a number of risk factors for street gang membership are supported by weak or
136
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
inconclusive evidence. However, it must be considered that the evidence-base for street gang-related
risk factors has rapidly grown since Klein and Maxson’s (2006) suggestions. Yet, to complicate matters
further, research has also suggested differences in risk factors within street gangs. Specifically, core street
gang members (i.e., those that self-identify as street gang members) are more likely than peripheral
members (i.e., those that engage in street gang crime, but do not self-identify as members) to have
early exposure to deviant peer groups, low impulse control, poor academic attainment, and endorse
antisocial attitudes (e.g., Alleyne and Wood 2010; Esbensen et al. 2001; Klein 1995; Melde et al. 2011).
This suggests peripheral and core street gang members have different needs that require targeting in
intervention programs.
The presence of a risk factor does not determine that an individual will join a street gang. Indeed,
many of the risk factors for street gang membership also predict other deviant behaviors (e.g., general
delinquency and violence; Decker et al. 2013). However, the more risk factors the individual experiences,
the higher the likelihood that they will engage in a street gang, beyond any other deviant behavior
(Melde et al. 2011). Supporting this accumulative effect, Esbensen et al. (2010) found 11 or more risk
factors were experienced by 52% of street gang members, compared with 36% of violent offenders.
Street gang members are also more likely to concurrently experience risk factors in each of the major
domains than their non-gang counterparts (Thornberry et al. 2003). This suggests that prevention and
intervention strategies need to address numerous risk factors across all domains (Howell 2010).
3.2. Protective Factors for Street Gang Membership
In areas with a high presence of street gangs, over 75% of young people successfully avoid
becoming members (Howell 2012). This is despite experiencing similar risk factors to those who
engage in street gangs, particularly across the school and community domains. As suggested above,
individuals who circumvent street gangs may not have accumulated as a high a number of risk factors
as those that do become members. Alternatively, these individuals may experience more protective
factors than those that do become affiliated with a street gang. In challenging environments, where it
may not be possible to remove or reduce all risk factors, focusing on adding protective factors could
decrease engagement in street gangs (Howell and Egley 2005).
However, with research predominantly focusing on the risk factors of street gang members,
the protective factors have been neglected. The protective factors that have been identified so far span
the individual, family, peer, and school domains (for a full summary, see Table 1). Regarding the
individual, protective factors for an at-risk young person include having effective coping strategies,
high emotional competence, and good social skills (Katz and Fox 2010; Lenzi et al. 2018; McDaniel 2012).
For the family domain, protective factors include strong parental monitoring, cohesiveness within the
family, and positive parental attachment (Li et al. 2002; Maxson et al. 1998). Interaction with prosocial
peer groups is a protective factor within the peer domain (Katz and Fox 2010). Positive child-teacher
relationships, clear familial expectations regarding schooling, and an individual’s commitment to
education are all protective factors in the school domain (Stoiber and Good 1998; Thornberry 2001).
Little is known regarding the protective factors for street gang membership in the community domain.
Future research examining protective factors is essential, particularly as strength-based approaches to
offender rehabilitation have suggested that focusing on these could improve prosocial behavior in
street gang members (O’Brien et al. 2013; Whitehead et al. 2007).
137
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Table 1. Examples of risk factors for street gang membership, according to domain.
Domain
Protective Factors
Individual
Offence supportive cognitions *, negative life experiences *, low self-esteem,
internalizing behaviors, externalizing behaviors *, impulsivity, lack of participation in
prosocial activities, mental health issues (e.g., Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder,
anxiety), negative attitudes towards the future, substance misuse, low empathy,
high callous-unemotional traits, low trait emotional intelligence,
moral disengagement, negative attitudes towards the police, hyperactivity,
poor interpersonal skills, and anger rumination.
Effective coping strategies, high emotional competence,
emotion regulation skills, resilient termperament, future orientation,
impulse control, low ADHD symptomology, high self-esteem,
intolerant attitude towards antisocial behavior, and belief in
moral order
Peers
Negative peer influence *, association with delinquent peer group, victim or
perpetrator of bullying, alienation from prosocial peers, strong emotional connection
to delinquent peers, prioritizing social identity, and peers’ substance misuse.
Interaction with prosocial peer groups, strong social skills, low peer
delinquency, and prosocial bonding
Family
Poor parental supervision * and monitoring *, lack of attachment to parents,
family involvement in street gangs, family involvement in crime, delinquent siblings,
hostile family environment, parental substance misuse, inconsistent discipline,
low familial socioeconomic status, single-parent households, childhood maltreatment,
and running away from home.
Strong parental monitoring, control and supervision, parental warmth,
cohesiveness within the family, positive parental attachment,
stable family structure, and low levels of parent-child conflict
School
Poor academic attainment, lack of commitment to education, lack of aspirations,
unsafe school environment, suspension/exclusion, truancy, inconsistent discipline,
victimization at school, inadequate teaching, negative relationships with staff,
and difficult transitions between schools.
Positive child-teacher relationships, clear familial expectations
regarding schooling, personal commitment to education, positive role
models, fair treatment from teachers, safe evironment, connectedness,
regular school participation, and academic achievement
Community
Disorganized neighborhood, high rates of crime, exposure to street gangs and
violence, availability of firearms, poverty, lack of community resources,
and experiencing unsafe environments.
Opportunities for prosocial involvement, positive community role
models, perceived neighborhood safety, and low economic deprivation
138
Risk Factors
Sources include: Home Office (2015), Lenzi et al. (2018), Mallion and Wood (2018), Melde et al. (2011), Merrin et al. (2015), O’Brien et al. (2013), Raby and Jones (2016), and Smith et al.
(2019). * Risk factors identified by Klein and Maxson (2006) as having a robust evidence-base.
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
4. Current Approaches to Street Gang Intervention
Street gang membership has typically been targeted through the criminal justice system, including
the imposition of street gang injunctions (behaviors or activities of the street gang member are
prohibited, such as going to certain areas; HM Government 2016). Whilst research has demonstrated
reductions in reoffending rates by recipients of street gang injunctions (Carr et al. 2017), long-term
negative effects have also been identified (e.g., reduced opportunities for education and employment,
and less access to prosocial networks; Swan and Bates 2017). However, there has been a recent growth
in prevention and intervention programs which are psychologically-informed (e.g., O’Connor and
Waddell 2015). These programs have more positive long-term outcomes, for both the individual and
the community, than criminal justice approaches (Howell 2010), and fit well within a public health
framework. This section will outline current approaches to street gang prevention and intervention,
across three levels (primary, secondary, and tertiary).
4.1. Primary Prevention
In a public health approach, it is assumed that given the right conditions, any young person
could be drawn towards joining a street gang (Gravel et al. 2013). As such, by using a universal
approach, primary prevention strategies attempt to protect all young people from engaging in adverse
behaviors (such as violence and street gang membership), by reducing risk and increasing protective
factors (Gebo 2016). Primary prevention strategies include the provision of services which aim to
reach and support a whole community. They are typically delivered via local schools, community
outreach, and faith-based organizations (Wyrick 2006). These include ensuring equal access to education,
employment, and housing, and improving the community space (i.e., cleaning communal areas and
better lighting). Wyrick (2006) suggests these primary prevention strategies enhance community
mobilization, which reduces engagement in street gangs.
Primary prevention strategies are commonly implemented in schools, as it is easy to reach a
large number of young people prior to the onset of any deviant or delinquent behavior. One of the
leading schools-based primary prevention programs for street gang membership is the Gang Resistance
and Education Training Program (G.R.E.A.T; Esbensen et al. 2001; Esbensen et al. 2002). G.R.E.A.T
is delivered by law enforcement officers to middle school pupils, aged 11–13 years, in the United
States. The original version of G.R.E.A.T targeted risk factors not specific to street gang membership,
including low self-esteem and unsafe schools (Klein and Maxson 2006). Despite program completers
having more pro-social peers, negative attitudes towards street gangs, and fewer risk-taking behaviors,
no difference was found between program recipients and non-recipients on levels of delinquency,
violence, or street gang involvement (Esbensen et al. 2001).
As such, G.R.E.A.T underwent substantial changes, with the new curriculum comprising
of 13 sessions targeting risk and protective factors specific to street gang membership.
The Revised-G.R.E.A.T program intended to inoculate young people against street gang membership,
through the development of skills (i.e., problem-solving, social and communication skills,
self-management, and personal responsibility) and creation of achievable goals (Esbensen 2015).
A Randomized Control Trial (RCT) evaluation of the Revised-G.R.E.A.T program found, compared to
controls, program recipients were 39% less likely to have become a street gang member at one-year
follow up (Esbensen et al. 2012), and 24% less likely at four-years follow up (Esbensen et al. 2013).
In addition, program recipients demonstrated less anger and expressed more positive attitudes towards
law enforcement (Esbensen et al. 2011).
Recently, Growing Against Gangs and Violence (GAGV) has been implemented as a primary
prevention measure in the UK, and is provided in areas prioritized in the Ending Gang and Youth
Violence initiative (HM Government 2011). Based on G.R.E.A.T, GAGV aims to build young people’s
resilience towards street gangs and is implemented universally to school year groups. Consistent
with the Revised-G.R.E.A.T program, GAGV promotes skill development, whilst also targeting the
‘push’ (e.g., fear of victimization and peer pressure) and ‘pull’ (e.g., protection, friendship, and money)
139
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
factors associated with street gang membership (see Densley 2018). However, its focus on raising
awareness of street gangs and the associated behaviors is closer to the original version of G.R.E.A.T
(Esbensen and Osgood 1999).
Outcomes from an RCT found recipients of the GAGV program had 2.72% lower odds of
joining a street gang than non-recipients, at a one-year follow-up. However, this did not reach the
criteria to be considered statistically significant, meaning findings should be interpreted with caution
(Densley et al. 2016). Critically, this may be due to poor retention and attrition rates at the one-year
follow-up. Alternatively, as Wong et al. (2011) suggest, primary prevention strategies, such as the
original G.R.E.A.T and GAGV, may not be effective at reducing street gang involvement as they are too
generic, often failing to target risk factors most strongly related to street gang membership. Despite this,
the focus on wellbeing and personal growth, rather than individual blame (Gebo 2016), means primary
prevention programs are perceived more positively by communities, schools, and policy-makers than
targeted prevention and intervention strategies (Tita and Papachristos 2010). As such, future research
needs to consider which risk and protective factors, specific to street gang members, should be targeted
in primary prevention strategies.
4.2. Secondary Prevention
Although primary prevention strategies should stop the majority of young people from joining
street gangs, for those that are not ‘immunized’ (as coined by the National Gang Center 2020) secondary
prevention measures represent the next level in anti-gang strategy. Esbensen (2000) suggests secondary
prevention efforts are needed which target young people who have displayed problematic behavior
and, as such, are at high risk of joining street gangs. As at-risk youths are most likely to face the
decision of whether to join a street gang, secondary prevention programs are often considered the most
important strategy in reducing street gang involvement (Howell 2010). Yet, systematic reviews and
meta-analyses have failed to find a strong evidence-base supporting the effectiveness of secondary
prevention strategies at reducing street gang involvement (Lipsey 2009; Wong et al. 2011).
As highlighted in the “Surveillance” section above, one of the key issues faced in secondary
prevention strategies is the accurate identification of young people at risk of street gang involvement.
Numerous attempts have been made at creating objective measures to identify youths at high risk of
joining a street gang (e.g., Hennigan et al. 2014). However, such instruments often suffer from a lack
of predictive validity (Gebo and Tobin 2012). As such, secondary prevention strategies are typically
targeted at young people who have had contact with law enforcement due to delinquent behavior or
those known to have family members or peers in street gangs (Gebo 2016). Such programs tend to be
delivered in areas with high rates of street gangs, as exposure to street gangs is a strong risk factor for
membership (Public Safety Canada 2007).
Wyrick (2006) suggests three key elements that any successful secondary prevention program
requires. Firstly, at-risk youths need access to alternatives to street gang membership, which are
appealing, engaging, and socially rewarding. For potential members, street gangs can be perceived as
a source of friendship, excitement, and income (e.g., Augustyn et al. 2019). By diverting at-risk youths’
attention onto prosocial alternatives, this will reduce their likelihood of engaging in a street gang.
Second, programs need to aid at-risk youths with developing effective support systems. Street gangs
offer a source of emotional and social support (Alleyne and Wood 2010). If this support is provided
through prosocial relationships, the need to become involved in a street gang will reduce. Finally,
Wyrick (2006) stresses that at-risk youths should be held accountable, with clear expectations for
appropriate behavior set. As street gang members tend to lack of parental monitoring and discipline
(Pedersen 2014), establishing appropriate behaviors in at-risk youths will reduce engagement in street
gangs. Due to the sheer number of secondary prevention programs available internationally, examples
included in this section are limited to those which have shown some success at preventing street gang
involvement, including Cure Violence, Montreal Prevention Treatment Program, Los Angeles Gang
140
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Reduction and Youth Development program, and Functional Family Therapy—Gangs (for an extensive
review of street gang prevention programs, see O’Connor and Waddell 2015; Wong et al. 2011).
Los Angeles Gang Reduction and Youth Development (GRYD) is a secondary prevention program
designed for young people aged 10–15 years, who are at high-risk of joining a street gang. To be eligible
for the GRYD program, young people must exhibit two or more of the following risk factors: antisocial
tendencies, weak parental supervision, critical life events, impulsive risk taking, guilt neutralization,
negative peer influence, peer delinquency, self-reported delinquency, and familial involvement in a
street gang (Brantingham et al. 2017). Using a strengths-based approach, the GRYD program aims to
increase resilience towards street gang membership by enhancing protective factors (e.g., support from
prosocial peers and family). Evaluation of the GRYD program has had positive results, with reduced
engagement in violent and street gang-related behavior at six-month follow-up (Cahill et al. 2015),
although this effect was stronger for younger and lower-risk participants, who may be less likely to
join a street gang anyway. Critically, evaluations conducted on GRYD failed to include a comparison
group of at-risk youths who did not participate in the program, meaning changes in behavior may not
be caused by GRYD.
A further secondary prevention program, Cure Violence (formerly CeaseFire), is based on the
view that violence is a contagious disease which can be prevented by targeting those most at-risk of
‘contracting violence’ (Skogan et al. 2009). By identifying and treating high-risk youths, intervening in
conflicts and changing community norms, it is assumed that this will reduce engagement in street gangs
and the associated violent behavior (McVey et al. 2014). Outcome evaluations of Cure Violence have
been mixed; a sixteen-year time series analysis found, after implementation of the program, shootings
reduced in five of the seven neighborhoods assessed (Slutkin et al. 2015). However, in one Baltimore
neighborhood, violence-related homicides increased by 2.7 times following the implementation of Cure
Violence (Webster et al. 2012). The inconsistency in findings may be due to problems with program
implementation across different neighborhoods (i.e., poor retainment of staff, lack of consistent funding,
communication breakdowns, and limited data sharing; Fox et al. 2015). Having been designed in the
USA, where rates of gun violence among street gangs are high, Cure Violence places an inordinate focus
on reducing gun-related offending (Butts et al. 2015). As such, Cure Violence lacks generalizability to
areas such as the UK, where gun-related violence is low (HM Government 2019).
Recently, researchers have explored whether Functional Family Therapy (FFT), an effective and
well-evidenced secondary prevention program typically used for adolescent behavioral and substance
misuse problems (Hartnett et al. 2016), could be adapted for young people at-risk of joining a street
gang (termed FFT-G). FFT involves treating the family as a whole; working towards establishing
better communication, family relationships, and minimizing conflict (Welsh et al. 2014). In FFT-G,
issues salient to street gang membership are also targeted (e.g., risk factors, retaliatory behavior,
and street gang myths). Outcome evaluations have found young people randomly assigned to receive
FFT-G had lower rates of recidivism at 18 months follow-up than the control group (Gottfredson et al.
2018), although, this depended on risk level, with program-recipients at highest-risk of street gang
involvement having lower recidivism rates than control, whilst lower-risk program-recipients showed
no difference in recidivism rates to the control group (Thornberry et al. 2018). This demonstrates that
young people who present with the most risk factors are more likely to benefit from FFT-G. Critically,
no research has yet been conducted to examine whether FFT-G is any more successful at reducing
street gang involvement than the original FFT program.
The Montreal Preventive Treatment Program (Tremblay et al. 1995) has the longest follow-up period
(19 years, with regular follow-ups throughout) of a secondary prevention program (Vitaro et al. 2013).
The Montreal Preventive Treatment Program is targeted at boys aged 7–9 years who have displayed
disruptive behavior. The program comprises a parental training component (e.g., effective behavioral
monitoring, crisis management, and positive reinforcement) and a social skills training component
for the child (e.g., self-control skills and building prosocial networks; Tremblay et al. 1991). Evidence
from RCTs found that program recipients were less likely to have joined a street gang at both 12 and
141
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
15 years-of-age than the control group (McCord et al. 1994; Tremblay et al. 1996). Furthermore,
at 24 years-of-age, program recipients were more likely to have graduated from high school and
less likely to have a criminal record than the control group (Boisjoli et al. 2007). This demonstrates
that secondary prevention programs provided when disruptive behavior first emerges can reduce
engagement in street gang membership.
4.3. Tertiary Prevention
In situations where primary and secondary prevention programs have not effectively prevented
an individual from joining a street gang, tertiary prevention programs can be provided. Tertiary
prevention programs target individuals who have already become a street gang member and are aimed
at helping them to leave the street gang or making participation in a street gang more challenging
(Mora 2020). Typically, tertiary prevention programs are provided to those who are incarcerated or
on probation, and have committed an offence related to their street gang membership. However,
the provision of tertiary prevention programs is inconsistent, with demand for services far outweighing
available resources (Lafontaine et al. 2005; Ruddell et al. 2006). For instance, in the United States
alone, it was estimated that 230,000 street gang members were incarcerated in 2011 (National Gang
Intelligence Center 2011), meaning the vast majority would not have been able to receive any form of
street gang intervention.
Despite this, attempts have been made internationally to develop and implement various tertiary
prevention programs for incarcerated street gang members. Typically, prison-based tertiary prevention
programs use suppression techniques, such as in-house or legal sanctions for street gang-related
behavior and separation from other street gang members. Suppression techniques used to tackle
street gang membership are beyond the scope of this paper; for a national analysis see Ruddell et al.
(2006). Whilst programs with a therapeutic basis (i.e., providing rehabilitation and support) are offered
to a lesser extent in prisons, these are an essential component of a public health approach to street
gang membership.
Di Placido et al. (2006) designed a tertiary prevention program for adult street gang members
incarcerated in a maximum-security, forensic mental health hospital, which utilized the Risk Need
Responsivity (RNR; Andrews et al. 1990) approach to offender rehabilitation. The RNR approach has
three key components: (1) risk (treatment intensity should match offenders’ risk of recidivism), (2) need
(treatment should target criminogenic needs, i.e., factors associated with offending behavior), and;
(3) responsivity (treatment style should utilize cognitive social learning methods that are appropriate
for each individual offender, accounting for their personal attributes and abilities). In addition,
Bonta and Andrews (2007) emphasize professional discretion, whereby clinical judgement can be
used to deviate from the previous principles, in exceptional circumstances. The RNR approach is
considered the “gold-standard” in offender rehabilitation (Fortune and Ward 2014), with RNR-consistent
interventions demonstrating considerable success at reducing recidivism (Andrews and Bonta 2010;
Hanson et al. 2009).
At a 24-month follow-up, treated street gang members were less likely to have reoffended violently
by 20% and non-violently by 11% than untreated matched controls. In addition, treated street gang
members committed fewer major institutional offences than controls. Whilst this program shows
promise, the extent to which street gang membership continued post-treatment was not examined;
meaning it is not possible to determine whether Di Placido et al.’s (2006) RNR approach is effective at
reducing street gang involvement. Furthermore, the RNR approach has been repeatedly criticized
for its demotivating nature and limited focus on non-criminogenic needs and therapeutic alliance
(Case and Haines 2015; Ward et al. 2007), which are critical factors for providing an effective street
gang intervention (Chu et al. 2011; Roman et al. 2017).
A new tertiary prevention program provided in the UK is Identity Matters (IM). Unlike
Di Placido et al.’s (2006) program, IM was designed for use in both prison and community settings.
IM is targeted at adults whose offending behavior is motivated by identification with a group or street
142
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
gang (Randhawa-Horne et al. 2019). Based on Tajfel and Turner’s (1986) Social Identity Theory, IM
assumes that offending behavior occurs as a result of “over-identification” with the group. Specifically,
individuals develop a collective sense of identity based on their group membership. The ingroup
is viewed more favorably than outgroups, with group members holding an “us” versus “them”
perspective. When social identity is salient, an individual’s behavior is guided by group norms
(Hogg and Giles 2012). For street gang members, group norms typically include aggressive and violent
behavior (Hennigan and Spanovic 2011).
IM consists of 19 structured and manualized sessions which aim to address participants’
offence-supportive cognitions, whilst strengthening their sense of personal identity. To date, only one
study has been conducted on IM, which consisted of a small-scale process study examining short-term
outcomes of a four-site pilot (Randhawa-Horne et al. 2019). Interviews with 20 program completers
(14 incarcerated offenders and 6 on probation) were generally positive regarding the content of
IM, with the majority recommending no changes. In particular, sessions which explored ‘push’
(i.e., community disorganization, poverty, unemployment) and ‘pull’ (i.e., financial gain, status,
and protection) factors, desistance, identity, and commitment to change were perceived as most
beneficial to participants.
IM was piloted in both a group and one-to-one format. One-to-one sessions were found to
be most successful, as participants were more engaged and the program could be tailored to the
individuals’ needs. However, as discussed previously, demand for IM is high and far outweighs the
staffing and time needed to provide the program. Despite this, the safety concerns regarding bringing
together members of opposing street gangs for a group-based intervention may overshadow the
benefits of increasing recipient numbers. Prison was perceived as the most suitable environment for
delivery of IM, with a lack of stability in the community, particularly surrounding accommodation and
employment, leading to difficulty in intervention delivery. Pre-post measures showed an increase in
participants’ understanding of the positive consequences of staying crime-free and negative outcomes
from engaging in crime. However, with a lack of control group and small sample size, it is not possible
to determine whether the observed changes occurred as a result of engaging in IM. Furthermore,
long-term outcome studies need to be conducted to examine whether any changes are maintained
post-intervention. Alike Di Placido et al.’s (2006) research, evaluations have not yet been conducted on
street gang engagement following receipt of IM; meaning it is not possible to deem this an effective
tertiary prevention program.
A number of limitations were highlighted concerning the implementation of IM. Firstly, both
facilitators and participants expressed difficulty surrounding the language used in IM. For instance,
using the terminology “group”, whilst avoiding the term “gang”, led to a lack of clarity surrounding
the purpose of the intervention. Second, participant motivation was identified as key to intervention
success. As street gang members have notoriously poor motivation to engage (Di Placido et al. 2006),
interventions should be personally meaningful, positively-oriented, and intrinsically motivating
(Fortune 2018). Therefore, the negative orientation of IM (i.e., focusing on harmful past behaviors) is
unlikely to improve participants’ motivation to engage in the intervention. Third, therapeutic alliance
deteriorated throughout the intervention, which is concerning considering past research has consistently
demonstrated that a good client-therapist relationship improves the effectiveness of interventions
(Gannon and Ward 2014). Fourth, IM is only accredited for use with adult offenders (Ministry of
Justice 2020). This is despite the majority of members joining street gangs during adolescence (Pyrooz
2014), which is a period characterized by an increased focus on peer relationships (Young et al. 2014),
and high salience of social identity (Tanti et al. 2011). Therefore, an intervention which targets social
identity, such as IM, may be more appropriate for young offenders.
Whilst the majority of tertiary prevention strategies are provided in prison settings, as demonstrated
in IM these can also be provided in the community. Multi-Systemic Therapy (MST; Henggeler et al.
1992) is a home-based intervention for adolescents, aged 12–17 years, that have engaged in offending
behavior (Mertens et al. 2017). According to MST, deviant behavior is a product of the proximal
143
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
systems (i.e., family, peer groups, school, and community) that the young person belongs to. As such,
MST focuses on risk factors within (e.g., parent-adolescent communication) and between (e.g., parent
communication with school) these systems (Henggeler and Schaeffer 2016). As completion of an MST
program has been associated with long-term reductions in recidivism (Sawyer and Borduin 2011) and
increased contact with prosocial peers (Asscher et al. 2014), it has been recommended as a tertiary
prevention program for street gang members (Madden 2013; O’Connor and Waddell 2015).
Findings regarding the effectiveness of MST for street gang members have been mixed. For instance,
Boxer et al. (2015) found treatment completion rates were lower for justice-involved youths who
self-identified as street gang members (38%), compared to their non-gang counterparts (78%).
In particular, street gang members were less engaged in the MST program and were more likely to be
removed from the program due to a new arrest (Boxer 2011). Success of MST is partially mediated by
reduced contact with delinquent peers (Huey et al. 2000). As ties to a street gang tend to be strong and
challenging to break (Decker et al. 2014), it is possible that MST therapists had difficulty reducing the
young person’s engagement in the street gang (Boxer et al. 2015); reducing overall program effectiveness.
Furthermore, street gangs provide access to social and emotional support (Alleyne and Wood 2010),
meaning members interpret the street gang as a positive peer network. As MST encourages the
formation of positive peer networks, street gang members may be reluctant to leave their street gang
(Boxer et al. 2015).
Despite limited support regarding the short-term effectiveness of MST for street gang members,
findings examining the longer-term effects have been more positive. Specifically, at one-year follow-up,
no difference was found between street gang members and non-gang youths on number of, or time to,
re-arrest (Boxer et al. 2017). This suggests that MST appears to have a ‘sleeper effect’, whereby it is
equally effective at reducing recidivism, over a longer time period, in street gang members as non-gang
youths. This may be because reducing engagement with a street gang takes time, so changes in
behavior will not be seen immediately. However, MST is a relatively novel tertiary prevention program
for street gang members, meaning further research is necessary to establish program effectiveness.
In general, this section has demonstrated that the evidence-base for tertiary prevention programs
is minimal. As such, there is currently no ‘gold-standard’ approach to intervening with street gang
members (Boxer and Goldstein 2012).
5. Good Lives Model as a Public Health Framework
The programs reviewed above represent just a small fraction of the wide range of street gang
interventions available. Whilst some interventions are emerging as being effective at preventing or
reducing street gang involvement, the vast majority suffer from a weak or limited evidence-base.
Critically, there is a lack of consistency in the provision of intervention programs for street gang members
across communities. Also, Wood (2019) suggests current prevention and intervention strategies are
limited by a number of therapeutic issues. Specifically, the benefits of belonging to a street gang
(e.g., protection, social and emotional support, sense of identity; Alleyne and Wood 2010) extend
beyond the typical proceeds of crime (i.e., financial and material gain), and are not adequately targeted
in interventions. In addition, street gang members’ mistrust and lack of motivation frequently hinder
intervention efforts (Di Placido et al. 2006). The Good Lives Model (GLM; Ward and Brown 2004),
a novel approach to offender rehabilitation, can provide a framework for street gang interventions
which overcomes these obstacles.
The GLM assumes offending behavior occurs when obstacles prevent the attainment of a
meaningful and fulfilling life through prosocial means (Yates et al. 2010). In order to achieve a
meaningful and fulfilling life, all humans are naturally predisposed to seek goals fundamental for
survival, social networking and reproducing (Laws and Ward 2011). Purvis (2010) proposed 11 universal
goals (termed primary goods) which contribute to an individual’s wellbeing, happiness, and sense
of fulfilment (Ward and Fortune 2013). For a summary of primary goods, see Table 2. Any means
necessary and available can be utilized in an effort to attain these primary goods, including both
144
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
prosocial and antisocial behaviors. For example, the primary good of Community can be fulfilled
through prosocial (e.g., volunteering in the local area) or antisocial methods (e.g., joining a street gang).
When antisocial methods are used, it is unlikely that an individual will have a truly meaningful and
fulfilling life, as the primary goods are under continuous threat. For instance, street gangs provide
members with a sense of safety, protection, and support (Hogg 2014), which are needed to fulfil the
primary good of Inner Peace. Yet, at best, Inner Peace will be fulfilled briefly, as street gang membership
increases an individual’s risk of violent victimization and mental illness (Taylor et al. 2008; Watkins
and Melde 2016).
Table 2. Eleven Primary Goods and Definitions (Yates et al. 2010).
Primary Good
Definition
1
Life
Incorporates basic needs for survival, healthy living, and physical functioning.
2
Knowledge
Aspiration to learn about and understand a topic of interest (including, but not
exclusively, oneself, others, or the wider environment).
3
Excellence in Work
Pursuing personally meaningful work that increases knowledge and skill
development (i.e., mastery experience).
4
Excellence in Play
Desire to pursue a leisure activity that gives a sense of achievement, enjoyment,
or skill development.
5
Excellence in Agency
Autonomy and independence to create own goals.
6
Community
A sense of belonging to a wider social group, who have shared interests and values.
7
Relatedness
Developing warm and affectionate connections with others (including intimate,
romantic, and family relationships and friendships).
8
Inner Peace
Feeling free of emotional distress, managing negative emotions effectively and
feeling comfortable with oneself.
9
Pleasure
Feeling happy and content in one’s current life.
10
Creativity
Using alternative, novel means to express oneself.
11
Spirituality
Having a sense of meaning and purpose in life.
Four obstacles have been identified which cause difficulty in obtaining primary goods
(Ward and Fortune 2013). Firstly, as discussed above, the use of inappropriate or antisocial means
leaves an individual feeling frustrated at their inability to fully secure the primary goods. Second,
the primary goods being sought can conflict, or lack coherence, with one another. For example,
the primary goods of Community and Excellence in Agency conflict when street gang members focus
on group norms, which contradict their personal goals. Third, a lack of scope occurs when primary
goods are neglected. For instance, street gang members neglect the primary good of Life (i.e., poor
sleep hygiene, lack of routine, reliance on takeaways), in order to spend time with the gang. Fourth,
external (e.g., poverty, lack of job opportunities, disorganized neighborhood) and internal obstacles
(e.g., impulsivity, low empathy, endorsement of moral disengagement strategies) result in prosocial
methods of attaining primary goods being inaccessible.
Critically, the GLM does not specify how to treat street gang members. Rather, it provides
a framework which guides the development and implementation of evidence-based interventions
(Ward et al. 2011). Specifically, any GLM-consistent intervention should begin by creating a Good Lives
Plan, which identifies an individual’s skills, the primary goods being sought, and any obstacles they face
(for an overview of GLM case formulation, see Fortune 2018). Aligned with a public health approach,
GLM-consistent interventions are framed in a manner that promotes well-being, by focusing on
achieving personally meaningful goals using prosocial methods (Ward and Fortune 2013). To support
the use of prosocial methods, GLM-consistent interventions aim to develop an individual’s internal
(i.e., skills and values) and external capacities (i.e., resources, support, and opportunities; Ward and
Maruna 2007). The GLM framework can guide primary, secondary, and tertiary programs (see Table 3).
145
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Table 3. Utilizing a Good Lives Model (GLM) framework for Primary, Secondary, and Tertiary Prevention Programs.
Stage of Intervention
Primary prevention
Secondary prevention
146
Tertiary intervention
Overview
GLM Framework
Universal prevention programs,
provided prior to the onset of street
gang membership.
Consistent with the GLM framework, primary prevention programs assist young people (regardless of
their risk for street gang involvement) to achieve their primary goods through prosocial means.
This involves developing the internal capacity skills necessary for primary good attainment.
For instance, school-based programs supporting the development of social skills, goal-making,
and emotional competencies can aid in the fulfilment of Relatedness, Excellence in Agency, and Inner
Peace. In addition, external obstacles that prevent attainment of primary goods need targeting.
For example, mobilizing communities, providing opportunities (e.g., youth groups and employment),
and reducing poverty will enable the fulfilment of primary goods through prosocial means.
Selected prevention programs,
targeting individuals who have been
identified as at greater risk of joining
a street gang.
Utilizing a one-to-one format, secondary prevention programs should begin with a GLM-consistent case
formulation. This involves identifying which primary goods are most important to the individual,
the means they have available to them, their personal strengths and skills, and any obstacles faced in the
pursuit of primary goods (Fortune 2018). This can guide the decision-making process regarding which
interventions are most suitable for the individual. For instance, FFT-G will be most appropriate for an
individual who is having difficulty attaining the primary good of Relatedness, due to family conflict.
Comparatively, an individual who is unable to achieve Inner Peace, because of mental health issues,
may respond better to a cognitive-behavioral intervention. As individuals at risk of street gang
membership are likely to face obstacles across many of the risk domains (i.e., individual, family, peer,
school, and community), a multidisciplinary approach will be necessary to ensure all internal and
external obstacles are targeted.
Indicated interventions, targeting
individuals who have already joined
a street gang.
For a street gang member, the perceived benefits of belonging to a street gang (e.g., financial gain,
protection, camaraderie), may outweigh the costs (e.g., risk of violent victimization and incarceration).
As such, it is important to identify, in case formulation, which primary goods an individual is trying to
attain through street gang membership. Again, this informs the selection of appropriate interventions.
Tertiary interventions should focus on providing alternative means of achieving the primary goods,
without needing to rely on street gang involvement. Similar to secondary prevention programs, this will
necessitate a multidisciplinary approach focusing on internal skill development and provision of
external resources. Critically, GLM-consistent tertiary interventions must be positively framed; focusing
on the strengths and goals of the individual, rather than their risk of returning to the street gang.
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
By utilizing a GLM framework, this can enhance existing evidence-based interventions for street
gang members. GLM-consistent interventions are strengths-based and goal-focused, which enhances
motivation and engagement with the program (Fortune 2018). In addition, as GLM-consistent
interventions are positively framed, therapists are encouraged to be empathic and respectful towards
clients (Barnao et al. 2015). This supports the development of a strong, trusting therapeutic alliance
(Ward and Brown 2004); overcoming issues of high drop-out rates, low therapeutic alliance, and poor
client engagement typically seen in street gang interventions. As the GLM has quickly become a
favored and widely applied framework for offender rehabilitation internationally (McGrath et al. 2010),
using a GLM framework could enable consistency in street gang interventions across communities.
However, as a relatively new framework, empirical evidence regarding GLM-consistent
interventions remains in its infancy (Mallion and Wood 2020a; Netto et al. 2014), and is primarily focused
on interventions for individuals who have sexually offended (Lindsay et al. 2007; Gannon et al. 2011).
Whilst the assumptions of the GLM have been theoretically applied to street gang members
(Mallion and Wood 2020b), to date, interventions that are GLM-consistent have not yet been
implemented with street gang members. Despite this, the GLM has been successfully applied
to young (e.g., Chu et al. 2015; Print 2013; Van Damme et al. 2016) and violent offenders
(Whitehead et al. 2007). As street gang members are typically young and engage in violent behavior
(Pyrooz 2014; Wood and Alleyne 2010), this supports the use of GLM-consistent interventions with
this population.
6. Conclusions and Future Directions
There has been a recent shift from viewing street gangs as a problem for law enforcement to
considering street gangs as a priority for public health (Catch22 2013). The public health approach
emphasizes the role of research in understanding the causes of street gang membership, with this
informing the development of primary prevention, secondary prevention, and tertiary intervention
programs (McDaniel et al. 2014). Whilst research regarding the risk factors for street gang membership
has rapidly grown over the past decade, the protective factors preventing involvement are still relatively
unknown (McDaniel 2012). As a large number of young people successfully avoid joining street gangs,
future research should focus on understanding protective factors which could guide street gang
prevention and intervention programs.
A key component of a public health approach involves conducting methodologically sound
evaluations of street gang prevention and intervention programs. Whilst this review has demonstrated
that some programs are beginning to show promise at reducing street gang involvement (e.g., G.R.E.A.T,
FFT-G), the majority of programs lack methodologically sound evaluation (i.e., no control group, reliance
on pre-post measures). Furthermore, the use of different definitions of street gang membership across
communities has impeded the consistent implementation of prevention and intervention strategies,
resulting in mixed findings regarding program effectiveness (e.g., Cure Violence). Thus, to support
consistency in the implementation of prevention and intervention programs, it is recommended that
the Eurogang definition is used to guide a public health approach to street gangs. Furthermore, in the
future, regular evaluations should be embedded into prevention and intervention programs to examine
their effectiveness at reducing street gang involvement.
Critically, prevention and intervention programs often suffer from a lack of theoretical foundation
and clear goals or objectives (Klein and Maxson 2006; McGloin and Decker 2010). This can be overcome
by using the GLM framework to guide evidence-based prevention and intervention strategies for
street gang members. The GLM assumes that improving an individual’s internal skills and external
opportunities will support them in attaining their primary goods through prosocial means. If these
primary goods are effectively secured, this will reduce the need for young people to engage with a
street gang. As the GLM is a model of healthy human functioning (Purvis et al. 2013), it can be utilized
across all stages of prevention and intervention. Whilst past research has theoretically applied the
147
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
GLM to street gang members (Mallion and Wood 2020b), future research is needed to empirically
examine the application of a GLM framework to street gang prevention and intervention programs.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, J.M. and J.W.; investigation, J.M.; resources, J.M.; data curation, J.M.;
writing—original draft preparation, J.M.; writing—review and editing, J.M.; visualization, J.M.; supervision, J.W.;
project administration, J.M.; funding acquisition, J.M. and J.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published
version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council, grant number ES/JS00148/1.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
Aldridge, Judith, Juanjo Medina-Ariz, and Robert Ralphs. 2012. Counting gangs: Conceptual and validity problems
with the Eurogang definition. In Youth Gangs in International Perspective. Edited by Finn-Aage Esbensen and
Cheryl Maxson. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 35–51.
Alleyne, Emma, and Jane Louise Wood. 2010. Gang involvement: Psychological and behavioral characteristics of
gang members, peripheral youth, and nongang youth. Aggressive Behavior 36: 423–36. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Andrews, Donald Andrews, and James Bonta. 2010. Rehabilitating criminal justice policy and practice. Psychology,
Public Policy, and Law 16: 39–55. [CrossRef]
Andrews, Donald Andrews, James Bonta, and Robert Hoge. 1990. Classification for effective rehabilitation.
Criminal Justice and Behavior 17: 19–52. [CrossRef]
Asscher, Jessica, Maja Deković, Willeke Manders, Peter van der Laan, Pier Prins, and Sander van Arum. 2014.
Sustainability of the effects of multisystemic therapy for juvenile delinquents in The Netherlands: Effects on
delinquency and recidivism. Journal of Experimental Criminology 10: 227–43. [CrossRef]
Augustyn, Megan Bears, Terrance Thornberry, and Marvin Krohn. 2014. Gang membership and pathways to
maladaptive parenting. Journal of Research on Adolescence 24: 252–67. [CrossRef]
Augustyn, Megan Bears, Jean Marie McGloin, and David Pyrooz. 2019. Does gang membership pay? Illegal and
legal earnings through emerging adulthood. Criminology 57: 452–80. [CrossRef]
Barnao, Mary, Tony Ward, and Peter Robertson. 2015. The Good Lives Model: A new paradigm for forensic
mental health. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 23: 288–301. [CrossRef]
Boisjoli, Rachel, Frank Vitaro, Éric Lacourse, Edward Barker, and Richard Tremblay. 2007. Impact and clinical
significance of a preventive intervention for disruptive boys. British Journal of Psychiatry 191: 415–19. [CrossRef]
Bonta, James, and Donald Andrews. 2007. Risk-Need-Responsivity Model for Offender Assessment and Rehabilitation;
Toronto: Public Safety Canada. Available online: https://www.publicsafety.gc.ca/cnt/rsrcs/pblctns/rsk-nd-rs
pnsvty/index-en.aspx (accessed on 2 July 2020).
Boxer, Paul. 2011. Negative peer involvement in multisystemic therapy for the treatment of youth problem
behavior: Exploring outcome and process variables in “real-world” practice. Journal of Clinical Child &
Adolescent Psychology 40: 848–54. [CrossRef]
Boxer, Paul, and Sara Goldstein. 2012. Treating juvenile offenders: Best practices and emerging critical issues.
In Handbook of Juvenile Forensic Psychology and Psychiatry. Edited by E. L. Grigorenko. Berlin and Heidelberg:
Springer, pp. 323–40.
Boxer, Paul, Joanna Kubik, Michael Ostermann, and Bonita Veysey. 2015. Gang involvement moderates the
effectiveness of evidence-based intervention for justice-involved youth. Children and Youth Services Review 52:
26–33. [CrossRef]
Boxer, Paul, Meagen Docherty, Michael Ostermann, Joanna Kubik, and Bonita Veysey. 2017. Effectiveness of
Multisystemic Therapy for gang-involved youth offenders: One year follow-up analysis of recidivism
outcomes. Children and Youth Services Review 73: 107–12. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, Jeffrey, Nick Sundback, Baichuan Yan, and Kristine Chan. 2017. GRYD Intervention Incident Response
& Gang Crime 2017 Evaluation Report.. Los Angeles: The City of Los Angeles Mayor’s Office of Gang
Reduction and Youth Development, Available online: https://www.lagryd.org/sites/default/files/reports/GRY
D%20IR%20and%20Gang%20Crime%20Report_2017_FINALv2_0.pdf (accessed on 4 July 2020).
Butts, Jeffrey, Catarina Roman, Lindsay Bostwick, and Jeremy Porter. 2015. Cure Violence: A public health model
to reduce gun violence. Annual Review of Public Health 36: 39–53. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
148
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Cahill, Meagan Jesse Jannetta, Emily Tiry, Samantha Lowry, Miriam Becker-Cohen, Ellen Paddock,
Maria Serakos, Loraine Park, and Karen Hennigan. 2015. Evaluation of the Los Angeles Gang
https:
Reduction and Youth Development Program. California: Urban Institute. Available online:
//www.urban.org/sites/default/files/publication/77956/2000622-Evaluation-of-the-Los-Angeles-Gan
g-Reduction-and-Youth-Development-Program-Year-4-Evaluation-Report.pdf (accessed on 10 July 2020).
Carr, Richard, Molly Slothower, and John Parkinson. 2017. Do gang injunctions reduce violent crime? Four tests
in Merseyside, UK. Cambridge Journal of Evidence-Based Policing 1: 195–210. [CrossRef]
Case, Stephen, and Kevin Haines. 2015. Children first, offenders second: The centrality of engagement in positive
youth justice. The Howard Journal of Criminal Justice 54: 157–75. [CrossRef]
Catch22. 2013. Violence Prevention, Health Promotion: A Public Health Approach to Tackling Youth Violence. London:
Author, Available online: http://yvcommission.com/wp-content/uploads/2017/08/Catch22-and-MHP-Health
-Violence-prevention-Health-promotion-A-public-health-approach-to-tackling-youth-violence-2013.pdf
(accessed on 3 July 2020).
Centre for Social Justice. 2009. Dying to Belong: An in-Depth Review of Street Gangs in Britain. London: Author,
Available online: https://www.centreforsocialjustice.org.uk/core/wp-content/uploads/2016/08/DyingtoBelon
gFullReport.pdf (accessed on 3 July 2020).
Children’s Commissioner. 2017. Estimating the Number of Vulnerable Children (29 Groups). London: Author, Available
online: https://www.basw.co.uk/system/files/resources/basw_84332-10_0.pdf (accessed on 6 July 2020).
Children’s Commissioner. 2019. Keeping Kids Safe: Improving Safeguarding Responses to Gang Violence and Criminal
Exploitation; London: Author. Available online: https://www.childrenscommissioner.gov.uk/wp-content/up
loads/2019/02/CCO-Gangs.pdf (accessed on 7 July 2020).
Chu, Chi Meng, Michael Daffern, Stuart Thomas, and Jia Ying Lim. 2011. Elucidating the treatment needs of
gang-affiliated youth offenders. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research 3: 129–40. [CrossRef]
Chu, Chi Meng, Li Lian Koh, Gerald Zeng, and Jennifer Teoh. 2015. Youth who sexual offended: Primary human
goods and offense pathways. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment 27: 151–72. [CrossRef]
Chui, Wing Hong, and Paul Vinod Khiatani. 2018. Delinquency among members of Hong Kong youth street
gangs: The role of the organizational structures of gangs and Triad affiliations. International Journal of Offender
Therapy and Comparative Criminology 62: 2527–47. [CrossRef]
Conaglen, Philip, and Annette Gallimore. 2014. Violence Prevention: A Public Health Priority. Glasgow: NHS
Scotland, Available online: https://www.scotphn.net/wp-content/uploads/2015/10/Report-Violence-Prevent
ion-A-Public-Health-Priority-December-2014.pdf (accessed on 12 July 2020).
Connolly, Eric, and Dylan Jackson. 2019. Adolescent gang membership and adverse behavioral, mental health,
and physical health outcomes in young adulthood: A within-family analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior 46:
1566–86. [CrossRef]
Curry, David. 2010. From knowledge to response and back again: Theory and evaluation in responding to
gangs. In Youth Gangs and Community Intervention: Research, Practice and Evidence. Edited by Robert Chaskin.
Columbia: Columbia University Press, pp. 109–26.
Decker, Scott, Chris Melde, and David Pyrooz. 2013. What do we know about gangs and gang members and
where do we go from here? Justice Quarterly 30: 369–402. [CrossRef]
Decker, Scott, David Pyrooz, and Ryan Moule. 2014. Disengagement from gangs as role transitions. Journal of
Research on Adolescence 24: 268–83. [CrossRef]
Densley, James. 2018. Gang joining. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice. Oxford:
Oxford University Press. [CrossRef]
Densley, James, Joanna Adler, Lijun Zhu, and Mackenzie Lambine. 2016. Growing against Gangs and Violence:
Findings from a process and outcome evaluation. Psychology of Violence 7: 242–52. [CrossRef]
Department of Health. 2012. Public Health Surveillance: Towards a Public Health Surveillance Strategy for England;
London: Author. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system
/uploads/attachment_data/file/213339/Towards-a-Public-Health-Surveillance-Strategy.pdf (accessed on
11 July 2020).
Di Placido, Chantal, Terri Simon, Treena Witte, Deqiang Gu, and Stephen Wong. 2006. Treatment of gang
members can reduce recidivism and institutional misconduct. Law and Human Behavior 30: 93–114. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
149
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Dong, Beidi, and Marvin Krohn. 2016. Escape from violence: What reduces the enduring consequences of
adolescent gang affiliation? Journal of Criminal Justice 47: 41–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage. 2000. Preventing Adolescent Gang Involvement; Washington, DC, USA: U.S. Department
of Justice, Office of Justice Programs. Available online: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/182210.pdf
(accessed on 2 July 2020).
Esbensen, Finn-Aage. 2015. The Gang Resistance Education and Training (G.R.E.A.T.) program: An evaluator’s
perspective. In The Handbook of Gangs. Edited by Scott Decker and David Pyrooz. Hoboken: John Wiley &
Sons, pp. 369–391.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, and Cheryl Maxson. 2012. The Eurogang program of research and multimethod comparative
gang research: Introduction. In Youth Gangs in International Perspective. Edited by Finn-Aage Esbensen and
Cheryl Maxson. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 1–14.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, and Wayne Osgood. 1999. Gang Resistance Education and Training (GREAT): Results from
the national evaluation. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 36: 194–225. [CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, and Frank Weerman. 2005. Youth gangs and troublesome youth groups in the United States
and the Netherlands: A cross-national comparison. European Journal of Criminology 2: 5–37. [CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Wayne Osgood, Terrance Taylor, Dana Peterson, and Adrienne Freng. 2001. How great
is GREAT? Results from a longitudinal quasi-experimental design. Criminology & Public Policy 1: 87–118.
[CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Adrienne Freng, Terrance Taylor, Dana Peterson, and Wayne Osgood. 2002. Putting research
into practice: The national evaluation of the Gang Resistance Education and Training (G.R.E.A.T.) program.
In Responding to Gangs: Evaluation and Research; Edited by Winnifred Reed and Scott Decker. Washington,
DC, USA: U.S. Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice, pp. 139–67.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Dana Peterson, Adrienne Freng, and Terrance Taylor. 2010. Youth Violence: Sex and Race
Differences in Offending, Victimization and Gang Membership. Pennsylvania: Temple University Press.
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Dana Peterson, Terrance Taylor, Adrienne Freng, Wayne Osgood, Dena Carson,
and Kristy Matsuda. 2011. Evaluation and evolution of the Gang Resistance Education and Training
(GREAT) program. Journal of School Violence 10: 53–70. [CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Dana Peterson, Terrance Taylor, and Wayne Osgood. 2012. Results from a multi-site
evaluation of the G.R.E.A.T program. Justice Quarterly 29: 125–51. [CrossRef]
Esbensen, Finn-Aage, Wayne Osgood, Dana Peterson, Terrance Taylor, and Dena Carson. 2013. Short- and longterm outcome results from a multisite evaluation of the G.R.E.A.T. program. Criminology and Public Policy 12:
375–411. [CrossRef]
Fortune, Clare-Ann. 2018. The Good Lives Model: A strength-based approach for youth offenders. Aggression and
Violent Behavior 38: 21–30. [CrossRef]
Fortune, Clare-Ann, and Tony Ward. 2014. Integrating strength-based practice with forensic CBT. In Forensic
CBT: A Handbook for Clinical Practice. Edited by Raymond Chip Tafrate and Daymon Mitchell. Sussex:
Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 436–55.
Fox, Andrew, Charles Katz, David Choate, and Eric Hedberg. 2015. Evaluation of the Phoenix TRUCE project:
A replication of Chicago CeaseFire. Justice Quarterly 32: 85–115. [CrossRef]
Gannon, Theresa, and Tony Ward. 2014. Where has all the psychology gone? A critical review of evidence-based
psychological practice in correctional settings. Aggression and Violent Behavior 19: 435–46. [CrossRef]
Gannon, Theresa, Tracy King, Helen Miles, Lona Lockerbie, and Gwenda Willis. 2011. Good Lives sexual offender
treatment for mentally disordered offenders. The British Journal of Forensic Practice 13: 153–68. [CrossRef]
Gebo, Erika. 2016. An integrated public health and criminal justice approach to gangs: What can research tell us?
Preventive Medicine Reports 4: 376–80. [CrossRef]
Gebo, Erika, and Kim Tobin. 2012. Creating and implementing a gang assessment instrument. In Looking beyond
Suppression: Community Strategies to Reduce Gang Violence. Edited by Erika Gebo and Brenda Bond. Lanham:
Lexington Books, pp. 61–82.
Gilbertson, Lee, and Seth Malinksi. 2005. Gangs in the law: A content analysis of statutory definitions for the term
“gang”. Journal of Gang Research 13: 1–15. Available online: https://ngcrc.com/journalofgangresearch/jgr.v13n
1.gilbertson.pdf (accessed on 8 July 2020).
Gilman, Amanda, Karl Hill, and David Hawkins. 2014. Long-term consequences of adolescent gang membership
for adult functioning. American Journal of Public Health 104: 938–45. [CrossRef]
150
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Gottfredson, Denise, Brook Kearley, Terence Thornberry, Molly Slothower, Deanna Devlin, and Jamie Fader. 2018.
Scaling-up evidence-based programs using a public funding stream: A randomized trial of Functional
Family Therapy for court-involved youth. Prevention Science 19: 939–53. [CrossRef]
Gravel, Jason, Martin Bouchard, Karine Descromiers, Jennifer Wongs, and Carlo Morselli. 2013. Keeping promises:
A systematic review and a new classification of gang control strategies. Journal of Criminal Justice 41: 228–42.
[CrossRef]
Hanson, Karl, Guy Bourgon, Leslie Helmus, and Shannon Hodgson. 2009. The principles of effective correctional
treatment also apply to sexual offenders: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior 36: 865–91. [CrossRef]
Hartnett, Dan Alan Carr, Elena Hamilton, and Gary O’Reilly. 2016. The effectiveness of Functional Family Therapy
for adolescent behavioral and substance misuse problems: A meta-analysis. Family Process 56: 607–19.
[CrossRef]
Henggeler, Scott, and Cindy Schaeffer. 2016. Multisystemic Therapy® : Clinical overview, outcomes,
and implementation research. Family Process 55: 514–28. [CrossRef]
Henggeler, Scott, Gary Melton, and Linda Smith. 1992. Family preservation using multisystemic therapy:
An effective alternative to incarcerating serious juvenile offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
60: 953–61. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hennigan, Karen, and Marija Spanovic. 2011. Gang dynamics through the lens of social identity theory. Youth Gangs
in International Perspective 1: 127–49. [CrossRef]
Hennigan, Karen, Cheryl Maxson, David Sloane, Kathy Kolnick, and Flor Vindel. 2014. Identifying high-risk
youth for secondary gang prevention. Journal of Crime and Justice 37: 104–28. [CrossRef]
HM Government. 2011. Ending Gang and Youth Violence: A Cross-Government Report Including Further Evidence and
Good Practice Case Studies; London: Author. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/gov
ernment/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/97862/gang-violence-detailreport.pdf (accessed on
12 July 2020).
HM Government. 2016. Statutory Guidance: Injunctions to Prevent Gang-Related Violence and Gang-Related Drug
Dealing; London: Author. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/injunctions-to-pr
event-gang-related-violence-and-drug-dealing (accessed on 10 July 2020).
HM Government. 2019. A Whole-System Multi-Agency Approach to Serious Violence Prevention: A Resource for Local
System Leaders in England; London: Author. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/governm
ent/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/862794/multi-agency_approach_to_serious_violence_preve
ntion.pdf (accessed on 8 July 2020).
Hogg, Michael. 2014. From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity processes. Current
Directions in Psychological Science 23: 338–42. [CrossRef]
Hogg, Michael, and Howard Giles. 2012. Norm talk and identity in intergroup communication. In The Handbook of
Intergroup Communication. Edited by Howard Giles. Oxfordshire: Routledge, pp. 373–88.
Home Office. 2015. Preventing Gang and Youth Violence: A Review of Risk and Protective Factors. London: Author,
Available online: https://www.eif.org.uk/report/preventing-gang-and-youth-violence-a-review-of-risk-andprotective-factors (accessed on 12 July 2020).
Howell, James. 2007. Menacing or mimicking? Realities of youth gangs. Juvenile and Family Court Journal 58: 39–50.
Available online: https://www.nationalgangcenter.gov/content/documents/menacing-or-mimicking.pdf
(accessed on 20 July 2020). [CrossRef]
Howell, James. 2010. Gang Prevention: An Overview of Research and Programs; Washington, DC, USA: U.S. Department
of Justice, Office of Justice Programs. Available online: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/ojjdp/231116.pdf
(accessed on 19 July 2020).
Howell, James. 2012. Gangs in America’s Communities. California: SAGE.
Howell, James, and Arlen Egley. 2005. Gangs in Small Towns and Rural Counties; Washington: Office of Juvenile
Justice and Delinquency Prevention. Available online: https://www.nationalgangcenter.gov/Content/Docum
ents/Gangs-in-Small-Towns-and-Rural-Counties.pdf (accessed on 18 July 2020).
Huey, Stanley, Scott Henggeler, Michael Brondino, and Susan Pickrel. 2000. Mechanisms of change in multisystemic
therapy: Reducing delinquent behavior through therapist adherence and improved family and peer
functioning. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68: 451–67. [CrossRef]
Joseph, Ian, and Anthony Gunter. 2011. Gangs Revisited: What’s a Gang and What’s Race Got to Do with It? London:
Runnymede Trust, Available online: http://oro.open.ac.uk/69952/1/69952.pdf (accessed on 15 July 2020).
151
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Katz, Charles, and Andrew Fox. 2010. Risk and protective factors associated with gang-involved youth in Trinidad
and Tobago. Revista Panamericana de Salud Pública 27: 187–202. Available online: https://www.scielosp.org/art
icle/rpsp/2010.v27n3/187-202/en/ (accessed on 16 July 2020). [CrossRef]
Klein, Malcolm. 1995. The American Street Gang. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Klein, Malcolm, and Cheryl Maxson. 2006. Street Gang Patterns and Policies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Krohn, Marvin, Jeffrey Ward, Terence Thornberry, Alan Lizotte, and Rebekah Chu. 2011. The cascading effects of
adolescent gang involvement across the life course. Criminology 49: 991–1028. [CrossRef]
Krug, Etienne, Linda Dahlberg, James Mercy, Anthony Zwi, and Rafael Lozano. 2002. World Report on Violence and
Health. Geneva: World Health Organization, Available online: https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/106
65/42495/9241545615_eng.pdf;jsessionid=8AD6F031E059843A6801FA8101412CD3?sequence=1 (accessed on
17 July 2020).
Lafontaine, Tania, Myles Ferguson, and Stephen Wormith. 2005. Street Gangs: A Review of the Empirical Literature
on Community and Corrections-Based Prevention, Intervention and Suppression Strategies. Canada: Saskatchewan
Corrections Public Safety and Policing, Available online: https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/fe4c/e3f7a5472f75
4458606b3b0d224148e3ed53.pdf (accessed on 11 July 2020).
Laws, Richard, and Tony Ward. 2011. Desistance from Sex Offender: Alternatives to Throwing Away the Keys. New York:
The Guildford Press.
Lenzi, Michela, Jill Sharkey, Allie Wroblewski, Michael Furlong, and Massimo Santinello. 2018. Protecting youth
from gang membership: Individual and school-level emotional competence. Journal of Community Psychology
47: 563–78. [CrossRef]
Li, Xiaoming, Bonita Stanton, Robert Pack, Carole Harris, Lesley Cottrell, and James Burns. 2002. Risk and
protective factors associated with gang involvement among urban African American adolescents. Youth &
Society 34: 172–94. [CrossRef]
Lindsay, William, Tony Ward, Tom Morgan, and Iris Wilson. 2007. Self-regulation of sex offending, future
pathways and the Good Lives Model: Applications and problems. Journal of Sexual Aggression 13: 37–50.
[CrossRef]
Lipsey, Mark. 2009. The primary factors that characterize effective interventions with juvenile offenders:
A meta-analytic overview. Victims and Offenders 4: 124–47. [CrossRef]
Local Government Association. 2018. Public Approaches to Reducing Violence; London: Author. Available
online: https://www.local.gov.uk/sites/default/files/documents/15.32%20-%20Reducing%20family%20viole
nce_03.pdf (accessed on 5 July 2020).
Madden, Vaishnavee. 2013. Understanding the Mental Health needs of Young People Involved in Gangs: A Tri-Borough
Public Health Report; Westminster: HM Government. Available online: https://committees.westminster.gov.u
k/documents/s5535/Mental%20Health%20and%20Gangs%20Report%202013.pdf (accessed on 7 July 2020).
Mallion, Jaimee Sheila, and Jane Wood. 2018. Emotional processes and gang membership: A narrative review.
Aggression and Violent Behavior 43: 56–63. [CrossRef]
Mallion, Jaimee Sheila, and Jane Wood. 2020a. Systematic Review of ‘Good Lives’ Assumptions and Interventions.
Aggression and Violent Behavior. in press.
Mallion, Jaimee Sheila, and Jane Wood. 2020b. Good lives model and street gang membership: A review and
application. Aggression and Violent Behavior 52: 1–11. [CrossRef]
Matsuda, Kristy, Finn-Aage Esbensen, and Dena Carson. 2012. Putting the "Gang" in “Eurogang”: Characteristics
of delinquent youth groups by different definitional approaches. In Youth Gangs in International Perspective:
Results from the Eurogang Program of Research. Edited by Finn-Aage Esbensen and Cheryl Maxson. Berlin:
Springer, pp. 17–33. [CrossRef]
Maxson, Cheryl, Monica Whitlock, and Malcolm Klein. 1998. Vulnerability to street gang membership: Implications
for practice. Social Service Review 72: 70–91. [CrossRef]
McCord, Joan, Richard Tremblay, Frank Vitaro, and Lyse Desmarais-Gervais. 1994. Boys’ disruptive behaviour,
school adjustment, and delinquency: The Montreal prevention experiment. International Journal of Behavioral
Development 17: 739–52. [CrossRef]
McDaniel, Dawn. 2012. Risk and protective factors associated with gang affiliation among high-risk youth:
A public health approach. Injury Prevention 18: 253–58. [CrossRef]
152
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
McDaniel, Dawn, Joseph Logan, and Janet Schneiderman. 2014. Supporting gang violence prevention efforts:
A public health approach for nurses. Online Journal of Issues in Nursing 19: 1–16. Available online: https:
//www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4703334/ (accessed on 8 July 2020).
McGloin, Jean, and Scott Decker. 2010. Theories of gang behaviour and public policy. In Criminology and Public
Policy: Putting Theory to Work. Edited by Hugh Barlow and Scott Decker. Pennsylvania: Temple University
Press, pp. 150–65.
McGrath, Robert, Georgia Cumming, Brenda Burchard, Steven Zeoli, and Lawrence Ellerby. 2010. Current
Practices and Emerging Trends in Sexual Abuser Management. Vermont: The Safer Society, Available online: http:
//www.robertmcgrath.us/files/6414/3204/5288/2009_Safer_Society_North_American_Survey.pdf (accessed on
1 July 2020).
McVey, Erin, Juan Duchesne, Siavash Sarlati, Michael O’Neal, Kelly Johnson, and Jennifer Avegno. 2014. Operation
CeaseFire–New Orleans: An infectious disease model for addressing community recidivism from penetrating
trauma. Journal of Trauma and Acute Care Surgery 77: 123–28. [CrossRef]
Medina, Juanjo, Judith Aldridge, Jon Shute, and Andy Ross. 2013. Measuring gang membership in England and
Wales: A latent class analysis with Eurogang survey questions. European Journal of Criminology 10: 591–605.
[CrossRef]
Melde, Chris. 2016. Gangs and gang crime. In The Handbook of Measurement Issues in Criminology and Criminal
Justice. Edited by Beth Huebner and Timothy Bynum. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 159–180.
Melde, Chris, and Finn-Aage Esbensen. 2013. Gangs and violence: Disentangling the impact of gang membership
on the level and nature of offending. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 29: 143–66. [CrossRef]
Melde, Chris, Stephen Gavazzi, Edmund McGarrell, and Timothy Bynum. 2011. On the efficacy of targeted gang
interventions: Can we identify those most at risk? Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice 9: 279–94. [CrossRef]
Melde, Chris, Finn-Aage Esbensen, and Dena Carson. 2016. Gang membership and involvement in violence
among US adolescents: A test of construct validity. In Gang Transitions and Transformations in an International
Context. Edited by Cheryl Maxson and Finn-Aage Esbensen. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 33–50.
Merrin, Gabriel, Jun Song Hong, and Dorothy Espelage. 2015. Are the risk and protective factors similar for
gang-involved, pressured-to-join, and non-gang-involved youth? A social-ecological analysis. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry 85: 522–35. [CrossRef]
Mertens, Esther, Maja Deković, Jessica Asscher, and Willeke Manders. 2017. Heterogeneity in Response during
Multisystemic Therapy: Exploring Subgroups and Predictors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 45:
1285–95. [CrossRef]
Ministry of Justice. 2020. Correctional Services Accreditation and Advice Panel (CSAAP): Currently Accredited
Programmes; London: Author. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/up
loads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/883024/descriptions-accredited-programmes.pdf (accessed on
9 July 2020).
Mora, Victor. 2020. Police response to juvenile gangs and gang violence. Oxford Research Encyclopedia, Criminology
and Criminal Justice 1: 1–29. [CrossRef]
National Gang Center. 2016. Brief Review of Federal and State Definitions of the Terms “Gang”, “Gang Crime”, and “Gang
Member”; Florida: Author. Available online: https://www.nationalgangcenter.gov/Content/Documents/Defin
itions.pdf (accessed on 8 July 2020).
National Gang Center. 2020. What We Do? Florida: Author. Available online: https://www.nationalgangcenter.g
ov/What-We-Do (accessed on 9 July 2020).
National Gang Intelligence Center. 2011. National Gang Threat Assessment—Emerging Trends; Washington: Author.
Available online: https://www.fbi.gov/file-repository/stats-services-publications-2011-national-gang-thre
at-assessment-2011%20national%20gang%20threat%20assessment%20%20emerging%20trends.pdf/view
(accessed on 7 July 2020).
Netto, Nicholas, James Carter, and Christopher Bonell. 2014. A systematic review of interventions that adopt the
“Good Lives” approach to offender rehabilitation. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation 53: 403–32. [CrossRef]
O’Brien, Kate, Michael Daffern, Chi Meng Chu, and Stuart Thomas. 2013. Youth gang affiliation, violence,
and criminal activities: A review of motivational, risk, and protective factors. Aggression and Violent Behavior
18: 417–25. [CrossRef]
153
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
O’Connor, Robyn, and Stephanie Waddell. 2015. What Works to Prevent Gang Involvement, Youth Violence and
Crime: A Rapid Review of Interventions Delivered in the UK and Abroad. London: Early Intervention
Foundation, Available online: https://www.eif.org.uk/report/what-works-to-prevent-gang-involvement-y
outh-violence-and-crime-a-rapid-review-of-interventions-delivered-in-the-uk-and-abroad (accessed on
10 July 2020).
Pearce, Jenny, and John Pitts. 2011. Youth Gangs, Sexual Violence and Sexual Exploitation. London: The Office of the
Children’s Commissioner for England, Available online: https://uobrep.openrepository.com/bitstream/hand
le/10547/315158/OCC_Uni-of-Beds-Literature-Review_FINAL.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y (accessed on
8 July 2020).
Pedersen, Maria. 2014. Gang joining in Denmark: Prevalence and correlates of street gang membership. Journal of
Scandinavian Studies in Criminology and Crime Prevention 15: 55–72. [CrossRef]
Petering, Robin. 2016. Sexual risk, substance use, mental health, and trauma experiences of gang-involved
homeless youth. Journal of Adolescence 48: 73–81. [CrossRef]
Petrosino, Anthony, Carolyn Turpin-Petrosino, and Sarah Guckenburg. 2010. Formal system processing of
juveniles: Effects on delinquency. Campbell Systematic Reviews 6: 1–88. [CrossRef]
Pickering, John, and Matthew Sanders. 2015. The Triple P-positive parenting program. Family Matters 96: 53–63.
Available online: https://aifs.gov.au/sites/default/files/fm96-jp.pdf (accessed on 6 July 2020).
Print, Bobby. 2013. The Good Lives Model for Adolescents Who Sexually Harm. Vermont: Safer Society Press.
Public Health England. 2015. The International Evidence on the Prevention of Drug and Alcohol
Use: Summary and Examples of Implementation in England; London: Author. Available online:
https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/7747
43/Preventing_drug_and_alcohol_misuse__international_evidence_and_implementation_examples.pdf
(accessed on 12 July 2020).
Public Health England. 2017. Public Health England Approach to Surveillance; London: Author. Available
online: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/public-health-england-approach-to-surveillance/pu
blic-health-england-approach-to-surveillance (accessed on 5 July 2020).
Public Safety Canada. 2007. Youth Gang Involvement: What Are the Risk Factors? Toronto: Author, Available online:
https://www.publicsafety.gc.ca/cnt/rsrcs/pblctns/yth-gng-nvlvmnt/yth-gng-nvlvmnt-eng.pdf (accessed on
8 July 2020).
Purvis, Mayumi. 2010. Seeking a Good Life: Human Goods and Sexual Offending. Saarbrucken: Lambert Academic
Press.
Purvis, Mayumi, Tony Ward, and Simone Shaw. 2013. Applying the Good Lives Model to the Case Management of
Sexual Offenders: A practical Guide for Probation Officers, Parole Officers, and Case Workers. Vermont: Safer
Security Press.
Pyrooz, David. 2014. “From your first cigarette to your last dyin’ day”: The patterning of gang membership in the
life-course. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 30: 349–32. [CrossRef]
Pyrooz, David, Jillian Turanovic, Scott Decker, and Jun Wu. 2016. Taking stock of the relationship between gang
membership and offending: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice and Behavior 43: 365–97. [CrossRef]
Raby, Carlotta, and Fergal Jones. 2016. Identifying risks for male street gang affiliation: A systematic review and
narrative synthesis. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology 27: 601–44. [CrossRef]
Randhawa-Horne, Kiran, Rachel Horan, and Phil Sutcliffe. 2019. Identity Matters Intervention for Group and Gang
Related Offenders in Custody and Community: Findings from a Small-Scale Process Study; London: Ministry of
Justice. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attac
hment_data/file/818633/identity-matters-intervention-for-group-and-gang-related-offenders.pdf (accessed
on 2 July 2020).
Richards, Chesley, Michael Iademarco, Delton Atkinson, Robert Pinner, Paula Yoon, Mac William Kenzie, Brian Lee,
Judith Qualters, and Thomas Frieden. 2017. Advances in public health surveillance and information
dissemination at the centers for disease control and prevention. Public Health Reports 132: 403–10. [CrossRef]
Roks, Robert, and James Densley. 2020. From breakers to bikers: The evolution of the Dutch crips ‘gang’.
Deviant Behavior 41: 525–42. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina, Scott Decker, and David Pyrooz. 2017. Leveraging the pushes and pulls of gang disengagement
to improve gang intervention: Findings from three multi-site studies and a review of relevant gang programs.
Journal of Crime and Justice 40: 316–36. [CrossRef]
154
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Rostami, Amir. 2017. Street-Gang Violence in Sweden Is a Growing Concern. Sociologisk Forskning 54: 365–68.
Available online: https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1168353/FULLTEXT01.pdf (accessed on
4 July 2020).
Ruddell, Rick, Scott Decker, and Arlen Egley. 2006. Gang interventions in jails: A national analysis. Criminal
Justice Review 31: 33–46. [CrossRef]
Sawyer, Aaron, and Charles Borduin. 2011. Effects of multisystemic therapy through midlife: A 21.9-year
follow-up to a randomized clinical trial with serious and violent juvenile offenders. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology 79: 643–52. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Skogan, Wesley, Susan Hartnett, Natalie Bump, and Jill Dubois. 2009. Evaluation of CeaseFire-Chicago. Available
online: https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/227181.pdf (accessed on 16 July 2020).
Slutkin, Gary, Ransford Charles, and Decker Brent. 2015. Cure Violence: Treating violence as a contagious disease.
In Envisioning Criminology: Researchers on Research as a Process of Discovery. Edited by Michael Maltz and
Stephen Rice. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 43–56.
Smith, Sven, Zenta Gomez Auyong, and Chris Ferguson. 2019. Social learning, social disorganization, and psychological
risk factors for criminal gangs in a British youth context. Deviant Behavior 40: 722–31. [CrossRef]
Stoiber, Karen, and Barbara Good. 1998. Risk and resilience factors linked to problem behavior among urban,
culturally diverse adolescents. School Psychology Review 27: 380–97. [CrossRef]
Swan, Richelle, and Kristin Bates. 2017. Loosening the ties that bind: The hidden harms of civil gang injunctions
in San Diego County. Contemporary Justice Review 20: 132–53. [CrossRef]
Tajfel, Henri, and John Turner. 1986. The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In Psychology of Intergroup
Relations. Edited by Stephen Worchel and William Austin. London: Nelson-Hall, pp. 7–24.
Tanti, Chris, Arthur Stukas, Michael Halloran, and Margaret Foddy. 2011. Social identity change: Shifts in social
identity during adolescence. Journal of Adolescence 34: 555–67. [CrossRef]
Taylor, Terrance, Adrienne Freng, Finn-Aage Esbensen, and Dana Peterson. 2008. Youth Gang Membership and
Serious Violent Victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 23: 1441–64. [CrossRef]
Thornberry, Terence. 2001. Risk factors for gang membership. In The Modern Gang Reader. Edited by Cheryl Maxson,
Jody Miller and Malcolm Klein. Pennsylvania: Roxbury, pp. 32–42.
Thornberry, Terence, Alan Lizotte, Marvin Krohn, Carolyn Smith, and Pamela Porter. 2003. Causes and
consequences of delinquency: Findings from the Rochester Youth Development Study. In Taking Stock of
Delinquency: An Overview of Findings from Contemporary Longitudinal Studies. Edited by Terence Thornberry
and Marvin Krohn. New York: Plenum, pp. 11–46.
Thornberry, Terence, Brook Kearley, Denise Gottfredson, Molly Slothower, Deanna Devlin, and Jamie Fader. 2018.
Reducing crime among youth at risk for gang involvement. Criminology & Public Policy 17: 953–89. [CrossRef]
Tita, George, and Andrew Papachristos. 2010. The evolution of gang policy: Balancing suppression and
intervention. In Youth Gangs and Community Intervention: Research, Practice and Evidence. Edited by
Robert Chaskin. Columbia: Columbia University Press, pp. 24–50.
Tonks, Sarah, and Zoe Stephenson. 2018. Disengagement from street gangs: A systematic review of the literature.
Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 26: 21–49. [CrossRef]
Tremblay, Richard, Joan McCord, Helene Boileau, Pierre Charlebois, Claude Gagnon, Marc Le Blanc, and Serge Larivée.
1991. Can disruptive boys be helped to become competent? Psychiatry 54: 148–61. [CrossRef]
Tremblay, Richard, Linda Pagani-Kurtz, Louise Mâsse, Frank Vitaro, and Robert Pihl. 1995. A bimodal preventive
intervention for disruptive kindergarten boys: Its impact through mid-adolescence. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology 63: 560–68. [CrossRef]
Tremblay, Richard, Louise Mâsse, Linda Pagani, and Frank Vitaro. 1996. From childhood physical aggression
to adolescent maladjustment: The Montreal prevention experiment. In Preventing Childhood Disorders,
Substance Abuse and Delinquency. Edited by Ray Peters and Robert McMahon. California: SAGE Publications,
pp. 268–98.
Van Damme, Lore, Machteld Hoeve, Robert Vermeiren, Wouter Vanderplasschen, and Olivier Colins. 2016. Quality
of life in relation to future mental health problems and offending: Testing the good lives model among
detained girls. Law and Human Behavior 40: 285–94. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vitaro, Frank, Mara Brendgen, Charles-Édouard Giguère, and Richard Tremblay. 2013. Early prevention of
life-course personal and property violence: A 19-year follow-up of the Montreal Longitudinal-Experimental
Study (MLES). Journal of Experimental Criminology 9: 411–27. [CrossRef]
155
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 160
Ward, Tony, and Mark Brown. 2004. The Good Lives Model and conceptual issues in offender rehabilitation.
Psychology, Crime & Law 10: 243–57. [CrossRef]
Ward, Tony, and Clare Ann Fortune. 2013. The Good Lives Model: Aligning risk reduction with promoting
offenders’ personal goals. European Journal of Probation 5: 29–46. [CrossRef]
Ward, Tony, and Shad Maruna. 2007. Rehabilitation: Beyond the Risk Paradigm. Abingdon: Routledge.
Ward, Tony, Ruth Mann, and Theresa Gannon. 2007. The Good Lives Model of offender rehabilitation: Clinical
implications. Aggression and Violent Behavior 12: 87–107. [CrossRef]
Ward, Tony, Pamela Yates, and Gwenda Willis. 2011. The Good Lives Model and the Risk Need Responsivity
Model: A response to Andrews, Bonta, and Wormith 2011. Criminal Justice and Behavior 39: 94–110. [CrossRef]
Watkins, Adam, and Chris Melde. 2016. Bad medicine. Criminal Justice and Behavior 43: 1107–26. [CrossRef]
Webster, Daniel, Jennifer Whitehill, Jon Vernick, and Elizabeth Parker. 2012. Evaluation of Baltimore’s Safe Streets
Program: Effects on Attitudes, Participants’ Experiences, and Gun Violence. Baltimore: John Hopkins Bloomberg
School of Public Health, Available online: https://www.jhsph.edu/research/centers-and-institutes/center-f
or-prevention-of-youth-violence/field_reports/2012_01_11.Executive%20SummaryofSafeStreetsEval.pdf
(accessed on 8 July 2020).
Weerman, Frank, Cheryl Maxson, Finn-Aage Esbensen, Judith Aldridge, Juanjo Medina, and Frank van Gemert.
2009. Eurogang Program Manual Background, Development, and Use of the Eurogang Instruments in
Multi-Site, Multi-Method Comparative Research. Available online: https://www.umsl.edu/ccj/Eurogang/Eu
rogangManual.pdf (accessed on 17 July 2020).
Welsh, Brandon, and Michael Rocque. 2014. When crime prevention harms: A review of systematic reviews.
Journal of Experimental Criminology 10: 245–66. [CrossRef]
Welsh, Brandon, Anthony Braga, and Christopher Sullivan. 2014. Serious youth violence and innovative
prevention: On the emerging link between public health and criminology. Justice Quarterly 31: 500–23.
[CrossRef]
Whitehead, Paul, Tony Ward, and Rachael Collie. 2007. Time for a change: Applying the good lives model
of rehabilitation to a high-risk violent offender. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology 51: 578–98. [CrossRef]
Wong, Jennifer, Jason Gravel, Martin Bouchard, Carlo Morselli, and Karine Descormiers. 2011. Effectiveness of
Street Gang Control Strategies: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Evaluation Studies. Toronto: Public
Safety Canada.
Wood, Jane. 2019. Confronting gang membership and youth violence: Intervention challenges and potential
futures. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 29: 69–73. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wood, Jane, and Emma Alleyne. 2010. Street gang theory and research: Where are we now and where do we go
from here? Aggression and Violent Behavior 15: 100–11. [CrossRef]
World Health Organization. 2010. European Report on Preventing Violence and Knife Crime among Young
People. Available online: https://www.euro.who.int/__data/assets/pdf_file/0012/121314/E94277.pdf (accessed
on 9 July 2020).
World Health Organization. 2020. Youth Violence. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/
detail/youth-violence (accessed on 12 July 2020).
Wyrick, Phelan. 2006. Gang Prevention: How to Make the “Front End” of Your Anti-Gang Effort Work. United
States Attorneys’ Bulletin 54: 52–60. Available online: https://www.nationalgangcenter.gov/Content/Documen
ts/Front-End.pdf (accessed on 14 July 2020).
Yates, Pamela, David Prescott, and Tony Ward. 2010. Applying the Good Lives and Self-Regulation Models to Sex
Offender Treatment: A Practical Guide for Clinicians. Vermont: Safer Society Press.
Young, Tara, Wendy Fitzgibbon, and Daniel Silverstone. 2014. The Role of the Family in Facilitating Gang
Membership, Criminality and Exit. Catch22. Available online: https://www.basw.co.uk/system/files/resourc
es/basw_24849-10_0.pdf (accessed on 9 July 2020).
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
156
$
social sciences
£ ¥€
Article
Exploring the Influence of Drug Trafficking Gangs on
Overdose Deaths in the Largest Narcotics Market in
the Eastern United States
Nicole J. Johnson 1, * , Caterina G. Roman 1 , Alyssa K. Mendlein 1 , Courtney Harding 2 ,
Melissa Francis 2 and Laura Hendrick 3
1
2
3
*
Department of Criminal Justice, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA 19122, USA;
[email protected] (C.G.R.);
[email protected] (A.K.M.)
Philadelphia Regional Office, Pennsylvania Office of the Attorney General, Philadelphia, PA 19153, USA;
[email protected] (C.H.);
[email protected] (M.F.)
Philadelphia Field Division, Drug Enforcement Administration, Philadelphia, PA 19106, USA;
[email protected]
Correspondence:
[email protected]
Received: 29 September 2020; Accepted: 4 November 2020; Published: 7 November 2020
!"#!$%&'(!
!"#$%&'
Abstract: Research has found that drug markets tend to cluster in space, potentially because of
the profit that can be made when customers are drawn to areas with multiple suppliers. But few
studies have examined how these clusters of drug markets—which have been termed “agglomeration
economies”—may be related to accidental overdose deaths, and in particular, the spatial distribution
of mortality from overdose. Focusing on a large neighborhood in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, known
for its open-air drug markets, this study examines whether deaths from accidental drug overdose
are clustered around street corners controlled by drug trafficking gangs. This study incorporates
theoretically-informed social and physical environmental characteristics of street corner units into the
models predicting overdose deaths. Given a number of environmental changes relevant to drug use
locations was taking place in the focal neighborhood during the analysis period, the authors first
employ a novel concentration metric—the Rare Event Concentration Coefficient—to assess clustering
of overdose deaths annually between 2015 and 2019. The results of these models reveal that overdose
deaths became less clustered over time and that the density was considerably lower after 2017. Hence,
the predictive models in this study are focused on the two-year period between 2018 and 2019.
Results from spatial econometric regression models find strong support for the association between
corner drug markets and accidental overdose deaths. In addition, a number of sociostructural factors,
such as concentrated disadvantage, and physical environmental factors, particularly blighted housing,
are associated with a higher rate of overdose deaths. Implications from this study highlight the need
for efforts that strategically coordinate law enforcement, social service provision and reductions in
housing blight targeted to particular geographies.
Keywords: drug markets; gangs; opioids; overdose; spatial concentration; generalized cross-entropy
1. Introduction
Urban areas in the United States, particularly those with high poverty levels, often experience
two significant public health concerns: high levels of drug overdose deaths (OD) and gun violence.
For some cities, these public health issues are intertwined and rise to epidemic proportions, greatly
diminishing quality of life for residents and incurring billions of dollars in economic losses related
to addiction treatment, criminal justice involvement, health care, and lost productivity. Particularly
with open-air drug markets, research has noted that violence may emanate from drug trafficking
157
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
gangs1 and groups that compete for territory and clients (Harocopos and Hough 2012; Johnson 2016;
Valasik and Tita 2018). In general, research has found that gang corners used for open-air drug
distribution have high rates of violence, and rates are even higher if multiple gangs have contiguous or
overlapping territories for drug distribution (Taniguchi et al. 2011).
Neighborhoods with many gang corners for drug distribution likely means more and easier
access to illegal drugs for clients. Rather than competition driving away customers and driving down
prices, research has shown that drug markets are another example of “agglomeration economies”,
which provide benefits by co-locating similar facilities (Taniguchi et al. 2009)—meaning more dealers,
more customers, and more profit in one area. Coupled with the exponential growth in recent years
of the illegal manufacturing and sale of fentanyl—a synthetic opioid 100 times more potent than
morphine—and strict limits on prescription opioids, drug gangs have no shortage of clients seeking
cheap options for opioids on the illegal market. Research confirms that increased availability of the
typical street drugs such as heroin and cocaine, now supplemented or cut with fentanyl and its analogs,
has led to increases in drug-related fatalities in many places, urban and rural alike (Armenian et al. 2018;
Han et al. 2019).
In some areas, accidental deaths from overdose may be co-located with outdoor drug markets.
Recent research suggests that many opioid misusers, in particular, use drugs near locations of drug
purchase rather than at their homes (Bates et al. 2019; Metraux et al. 2019). Law enforcement reports
and anecdotal evidence also suggest a contemporary link between drug market locations and deaths
from drug overdose given the powerful lure of inexpensive drugs and high-quality heroin in some
jurisdictions. Unsuspecting buyers may be purchasing more potent and dangerous substances,
and using the drugs on the street soon after purchase, leading to accidental overdose (DEA 2018;
Lieberman et al. 2020; Pardo et al. 2019). Research is needed that more closely examines the spatial
nature of deaths caused by overdose and how overdose fatalities are related to the location of gangs
that sell large quantities of drugs. There are potentially novel opportunities for coordinated policy
responses that can address both issues simultaneously.
The current paper examines the spatial relationship between the locations of gangs that control
drug trafficking and the locations of fatalities from accidental drug overdose. We apply the framework
of environmental criminology (Brantingham and Brantingham 1981) to assess this relationship.
We consider factors related to social disorganization theory (Shaw and McKay 1942) and the routine
activities perspective (Cohen and Felson 1979) to provide context and opportunity for drug use and
misuse tied to neighborhoods and places. We first explore the spatial dimensions of drug fatalities
over a number of years, along with potential changes to help inform an environmental analyses of
factors associated with OD locations.
We focus our study on a large area within the northeastern U.S. city of Philadelphia, Pennsylvania—
the neighborhoods of Fairhill and Kensington—where ODs and gun violence have been increasing
steadily over the last five years. These neighborhoods, not surprisingly, are characterized by years
of disinvestment, concentrated poverty, and poor physical conditions. The average income among
residents of Kensington-Fairhill was $12,669 for 2012 through 2016—approximately half of the average
income for residents of Philadelphia as a whole (Confair et al. 2019). In these neighborhoods, all open-air
drug markets are gang controlled, and tied to street corners. We seek to answer the following questions:
Are ODs clustered around these drug trafficking organization (DTO)-controlled corners? Does the
extent of clustering change over time or is it relatively stable? In addition to the DTO locations,
what socioeconomic factors and physical environmental features of the landscape are associated with
the location of ODs? We take into consideration how these DTO corners are nested within the larger
ecological context of neighborhoods and how small areas can provide their own opportunities to attract
1
We use the terms “drug gang” and “drug trafficking organization” interchangeably in this work, although law enforcement
agencies in Philadelphia more often use the term “drug trafficking organization” to describe the groups operating in this
drug market.
158
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
individuals seeking to purchase and use drugs. Overall, our intent is to advance the small, but growing
literature on risky environments for substance use and misuse (Mennis et al. 2016), while specifying
how particular constructs in environmental criminology can be integrated into that literature.
2. Background
2.1. Drug Overdose Deaths and Philadelphia
In the US, the number of ODs is four times higher today than it was in 1999, with overdoses
representing the leading cause of injury-related death among adults (CDC 2020). The use of opioids is
fueling the trend, which persists across all age groups, genders, and racial/ethnic groups (Olsen 2016).
In Philadelphia, the increasing trend mirrors that of the U.S. as a whole. Among the largest U.S. cities,
however, Philadelphia ranks at the top, with more ODs in 2019 than any big city, claiming 1150 victims
(Whelan 2020).
The Kensington-Fairhill area of Philadelphia has been referred to as the largest open-air heroin
market on the East Coast of the United States (Percy 2018) and data from the U.S. Federal Bureau
of Investigation (FBI) confirm that Philadelphia is a leading regional and multistate supplier for
high-grade heroin (Roselli 2018). The extensive reach of the Kensington-Fairhill area DTOs is evidenced
by the amount of drugs from Kensington-Fairhill found outside of this area of the city—a three-month
snapshot of victims of drug overdose from January through March of 2018 in the four counties
surrounding Philadelphia identified fifteen different heroin stamps linked directly to Kensington
suppliers (Roselli 2018).
Opiates such as heroin have increasingly been supplanted by synthetic opioids as a major driver
of overdoses in Philadelphia and elsewhere (Pardo et al. 2019). Beginning in 2013, the U.S. as a
whole has seen a large increase in ODs from synthetic opioids, largely illicitly-manufactured fentanyl.
The influence of fentanyl on ODs cannot be overstated. Between 2013 and 2014, the age-adjusted
rate of ODs from opiate pain relievers and heroin increased by 9% and 26% respectively, and the rate
of death from fentanyl has seen large increases as well (Rudd et al. 2016). In 2018, fentanyl was the
leading cause of opioid ODs in the U.S. over heroin and prescription painkillers (NIDA 2020). Similarly,
the number of drugs seized by law enforcement in the U.S. that tested positive for fentanyl increased
by 291% between 2015 and 2017 (DEA 2019). Because profit potential and demand for opiates is so
high in the U.S., it is not difficult to surmise that there are always heroin and fentanyl drug trafficking
organizations and gangs willing to fill voids in production or supply left by changing legislation or
enforcement efforts.
2.2. Spatial Distribution of Overdose Deaths
Although small-area geographic studies of crime and violence are common, studies examining
the geographic distribution of ODs are not. This might be because drug misuse is theorized to
be mostly attributed to individual-level and interpersonal factors, as opposed to environmental
factors (Connell et al. 2010; Mennis et al. 2016). There is reason to believe that drug overdoses are
not uniformly distributed throughout space, particularly given the inequitable distribution of what
(Mennis et al. 2016) term “risky substance use environments” (p. 3). Offering a profile of these
risky environments, the authors cite features of places that increase access to substances (such as
environments in close proximity to illicit drug markets), high in disadvantage and disorder, or low
in cohesion as facilitating drug use initiation or producing stressful conditions for which drug use is
used as a coping mechanism (Mennis et al. 2016). Much aligned with the environmental criminology
literature, they note that both detrimental and prosocial features of environments, such as density of
alcohol establishments and high crime (detrimental), and access to greenspace, libraries and other public
resources (prosocial), are inequitably distributed across communities, producing similar inequities in
substance use and abuse.
159
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
The rapidly-rising number of ODs associated with illegally-obtained opioids sold by drug
trafficking gangs in Philadelphia creates a unique opportunity to test whether ecological structures
and physical features of the environment help uncover spatial patterns that could have implications
for policies and practices by criminal justice stakeholders as well as public health officials and
practitioners. Furthermore, evidence of the concentration or co-location of two issues that have serious
consequences for the well-being of communities would call for collaboration across government and
community stakeholders.
We know from the criminological literature that open-air markets are associated with a variety
of highly-visible problems, such as quality-of-life issues and violence (Harocopos and Hough 2012;
Johnson 2016; Stevens and Bewley-Taylor 2009; Taniguchi et al. 2011). Studies conducted in Philadelphia
at the Census block group level found that factors related to the social disorganization of neighborhoods
(income level, vacant properties, female-headed households, residential instability, etc.) had more
salience with regard to the location of drug markets than factors representing the routine activities
perspective—such as the presence of attractors and generators of crime (McCord and Ratcliffe 2007).
Research by Weisburd and colleagues examining environmental factors related to high-crime street
segments found that street segments characterized by drug sales also experience high levels of social
disorganization (Weisburd et al. 2010).
As ODs are being connected to drug-dealing locations, and there is evidence that opioid deaths
cluster to a similar or even greater extent as compared to crime events (Carter et al. 2019), more research
is being done to understand the environmental context of overdose deaths. Studies using relatively
large units of analysis, such as the community or neighborhood, find clustering of ODs in relation to
particular contextual and environmental features. In a 2005 study of 59 community districts (CDs)
in New York City, Hembree and colleagues found a relationship between external and internal (e.g.,
windows, stairways, heating problems) environmental features of the neighborhood and accidental ODs.
CDs that had more dilapidated or deteriorating buildings, window or stairway problems, structural
fires, and housing with toilet, heating, or peeling paint issues were likely to have more ODs and those
with a higher percentage of acceptably clean streets were likely to have fewer (Hembree et al. 2005),
showing a potential facilitating role for the neighborhood-level physical environment in terms of ODs.
Recent research in Philadelphia by Johnson and Shreve (2020) examined the geographic distribution
of drug overdose mortality at the ZIP code level, and found that fatal drug overdose counts significantly
varied across ZIP codes. Testing constructs related to social disorganization, police surveillance,
and physical environmental features, they found that overdose mortality was consistently related to
neighborhood disadvantage and racial composition (percent White), and the overdose mortality of
surrounding neighborhoods. In addition, overdose mortality was also positively associated with police
activity for low-level crimes and with negative aspects of the built environment (unsafe and vacant
housing, demolitions, and older housing stock) (Johnson and Shreve 2020). Other research has focused
on units of analyses smaller than the neighborhood or ZIP code level, hypothesizing that place-based
studies at the Census block group level or smaller are better able to capture important variation in
places. A study by Li and colleagues (2019) used Census block groups to examine the relationship
between features of the built environment with heroin-related emergency calls in Cincinnati, OH from
2015 to 2019. The authors found that, in addition to certain sociodemographic and population features
(population age, gender, education, and median household income), heroin-related ODs were positively
related to the proportion of parks and commercial, manufacturing, and downtown development
zones within block groups. The distance to pharmacies also had a positive association with the
emergency medical service (EMS) calls for drug overdose, while number of fast-food restaurants,
distance to hospitals, and distance to opioid treatment programs were negatively associated with
overdose mortality (Li et al. 2019).
Research has also studied these features at more than one environmental level to determine how
spatial scales factored into overdose deaths. Headley Konkel and Hoffman (2020) examined the effects
of both the neighborhood (block-level) and immediate (parcel-level) context on fatal drug overdoses in
a non-urban Midwestern jurisdiction from 2014 to 2017. Using hierarchical linear modeling, they found
160
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
that income inequality, residences of gang members, and the absence of bars predicted all overdoses at
the block level, and sex offenses, drug arrests, and the presence of a drug house predicted ODs at the
parcel level. They noted the counterintuitive finding related to bars, suggesting that the guardianship
from bar management and the presence of other patrons may discourage illegal drug consumption.
In summary, the handful of studies seeking to uncover a relationship between overdose mortality
and aspects of the physical and socioeconomic environment has demonstrated a link to certain
environmental features, yet this research remains in its infancy, with studies using various units
of analyses. Building on the work of the previous studies, we continue to apply a theoretical lens
related to crime opportunity at places—in particular social disorganization theory and routine activity
theory—to understand the importance of environmental factors for the anti-social behavior of drug
misuse, with a focus on the location of drug trafficking gangs. Social disorganization theorists posit,
and research has confirmed, that disadvantaged neighborhoods lack the ability to foster informal social
control, thereby facilitating increased opportunities for crime (Bursik and Grasmick 1993). The routine
activities perspective focuses more on how the daily routines of persons linked to places influence
opportunity for crime. Daily patterns of life generate changes in the flow of potential victims and
offenders that can facilitate or inhibit the opportunity for crime (Felson 1987, 1994). With regard to
drug markets and accidental overdose, the victims are those purchasing and using drugs. In this
study, most victims are traveling to the study area from other neighborhoods (Friedman et al. 2019).
The perspective provides the framework to understand how facilities or block features can be attractors
or generators for the establishment and maintenance of drug markets at corners—in effect increasing
the likely convergence of dealers and purchasers/users (Taniguchi et al. 2009).
Federal and state law enforcement investigators in Philadelphia have noted the modus operandi of
drug consumers is to use the product shortly after purchase, regardless of whether they are a walk-up
or drive-up customer (Haigler and Francis 2020). The current study examines spatial relationships
at a very small geographic level—the Thiessen polygon—to capture the unique contribution of the
location of drug gangs to accidental ODs. The routine activities of DTOs would be closely tied to where
targets (i.e., users) are located, creating an environment that is beneficial to both sellers and users.
If these users consume dangerous drugs near their place of purchase, overdose mortality likely will be
higher in places closer to point of sales, and likely clustered within an area closest to the majority of
drug-selling gangs. The current study investigates these hypotheses.
3. Methods
3.1. Target Area
As described above, the focus of this work is on the neighborhood of Kensington-Fairhill, which
is part of the larger Kensington area, located in the northeastern portion of Philadelphia. The area
comprises approximately 1.4 square miles, with a population size of nearly 38,000 people2 , the majority
of whom are Puerto Rican3 (Friedman et al. 2019). In 2018, concern over rising opioid overdose
deaths was the mainspring for the mayor of Philadelphia’s executive order declaring a disaster in the
neighborhood (Exec. Order No. 3–18 2018).4
In addition to rising overdose deaths, as stated earlier, the area also experiences deep structural
disadvantage and high violent crime, with approximately double the population below the federal
poverty line than for the city as a whole.5 The area itself is marred by physical decay as a result
of deindustrialization over the past century, which (Friedman et al. 2019) note “represents a perfect
2
3
4
5
This figure is a weighted count using the 2014–2018 ACS 5 year average and areal interpolation procedures described further
in this section.
From the 2014–2018 ACS 5 year average, 72% of the total weighted population in the target area are ethnically Hispanic or
Latino, and of this group, 78% are Puerto Rican.
https://www.phila.gov/ExecutiveOrders/Executive%20Orders/eo99318.pdf.
According to the 2014–2018 American Community Survey, 54% of the population in Kensington were below the poverty
line, compared to 25% in the city as a whole.
161
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
environment for harboring difficult-to-police drug markets, sex work, drug consumption shooting
galleries, and homeless squats” (p. 7). The authors’ recent ethnographic work in the neighborhood
documents how structural disadvantage and the ethno-racial makeup of the community positions
it to be a hub for the illicit narcotics trade, and consequently, violent crime (Friedman et al. 2019).
Spending nearly half a decade in a 10-block area encompassing much of our target area, they found
that the ethnic makeup of this neighborhood provides a neutral meeting ground for black and white
users to purchase their product without drawing too much attention. Indeed, they found that most of
the buyers were not residents themselves, but users coming to the local drug markets from outside
the neighborhood.
3.2. Spatial Unit
As the focus of our research is on drug trafficking corners, we use Thiessen polygons as our
unit of analysis, a network of which is constructed around street corners in the target area of
Kensington-Fairhill. A Thiessen polygon is a geometric unit that contains all geographic space
closest to its centroid (street corners) than any other polygon’s centroid (Chainey and Ratcliffe 2005;
Taniguchi et al. 2011). A Thiessen polygon network was used for both theoretical and practical reasons.
From a theoretical standpoint, street corners are central hubs of activity for gangs in general and
open-air drug markets (Whyte 1955; Topalli et al. 2002; Hsu and Miller 2017). It has long been
acknowledged that gangs conduct their day-to-day operations at smaller geographic scales than
neighborhoods, Census tracts, or the full extent of their territory (Tita et al. 2005; Valasik and Tita 2018).
Commonly called their “set spaces”, gangs may choose the places (e.g., street corners) where they hang
out and conduct their daily activities, including selling drugs, based on a variety of factors related to
the built environment or the relative locations of other gangs’ set spaces (Tita et al. 2005; Valasik and
Tita 2018). This study echoes prior research that has used Thiessen Polygons as units approximating
gang-drug corners within larger set spaces (Taniguchi et al. 2011).
More practically, Thiessen polygons centered around street corners allow for aggregating point
data to the nearest corner, reducing potential bias resulting from coding errors (Weisburd et al. 1994).
Relatedly, Thiessen polygons are tessellated, ensuring observations are assigned to independent
units. Thiessen polygons have been used in previous studies of crime, e.g., (Ratcliffe et al. 2011;
Haberman 2017; Piza and Gilchrist 2018). The target area for this analysis was composed of 533
Thiessen polygons, representing approximately 2.5% of the street corners in the city. For the remainder
of the article, we use Thiessen polygons and street corner units interchangeably.
3.3. Data and Measures
3.3.1. Dependent Variable
The outcome measure of interest for the current study is accidental drug overdose deaths occurring
in each Thiessen polygon in the target area. We examined these data for the period between 1 January
2015 and 31 December 2019, with the multivariate analyses focused on the two-year period from 2018 to
2019. Accidental OD incidents were obtained deidentified from the Drug Enforcement Administration
(DEA). These data are not restricted to opioid-related deaths. Among other indicators, these data
included information on the address of the overdose event location, as well as the death location.
Because we are interested in where overdoses are taking place, we focus on the overdose event location,
and thus our discussion of OD incidents throughout the paper refers to accidental OD events that took
place within the city of Philadelphia, but these events all eventually resulted in a death. We used
ArcGIS Pro 2.6.0 software to geocode OD incidents occurring in Philadelphia, resulting in a citywide hit
rate of 95%, exceeding commonly-referenced standards (Ratcliffe 2004).6 All OD incidents occurring
within Kensington-Fairhill were subsequently aggregated to counts per individual Thiessen polygon.
6
Ratcliffe (2004) found that an 85% hit rate was an acceptable minimum standard when conducting spatial crime analysis,
although recent work has returned to this question of minimum acceptable hit rates. Briz-Redón et al. (2020) replicated
(Ratcliffe 2004) procedure using five crime types aggregated to five different areal units in Valencia. They found that
162
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
The distribution of OD counts per year from 2015 through 2019 across different race and ethnicity
groups is displayed in Table 1. Though accounting for 2.5% of the city’s street corners, the target area of
focus in this study experienced slightly more than 13% of the city’s overdoses between 2015 and 2019.
Citywide, overdose deaths increased more than 60% between 2015 and 2019, while the number of ODs
in Kensington-Fairhill increased by 50%. Drug overdose victims in Philadelphia are primarily white,
though the share of non-white victims appears to have grown, both in the city as a whole and in the
target area, during the same time period. Between 2015 and 2019, the share of overdose victims who
were non-white grew nearly 80% citywide, and by 375% in Kensington-Fairhill. The share of Hispanic
overdose victims grew more than 120% and 91% in the city and Kensington-Fairhill, respectively.
Victims who overdosed in Kensington-Fairhill who were from Philadelphia and for whom a home
address was known came from many different parts of the city, including neighborhoods in and
around the target area, and far north and south of the city. While we do not know the individual
socioeconomic status of the OD victims who make up our sample, we found by analyzing the census
tracts of their home address that most, but not all, of the decedents hailed from neighborhoods with
high levels of concentrated disadvantage7 . In 2019 alone, most of the victims in the target area for
whom a Philadelphia address was known lived in tracts more than 1 standard deviation above the city
average in concentrated disadvantage, with some home tracts as high as nearly 2.5 standard deviations
above the average.
Table 1. Number of ODs in the Target Area and Citywide, by Year.
Year
Demographics of
OD Victims
Total Phila. ODs
Race
White
Non-white
Unknown
Ethnicity
Hispanic
Non-Hispanic
Unknown
Target Area ODs *
Race
White
Non-white
Unknown
Ethnicity
Hispanic
Non-Hispanic
Unknown
2015
2016
2017
2018
2019
Total
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
N
%
666
100.0
832
100.0
1139
100.0
1021
100.0
1070
100.0
4728
100.0
445
220
1
66.8
33.0
0.2
565
267
0
67.9
32.1
0.0
817
321
1
71.7
28.2
0.1
682
339
0
66.8
33.2
0.0
677
393
0
63.3
36.7
0.0
3186
1540
2
67.4
32.6
0.0
77
585
4
100
11.6
87.8
0.6
15.0
103
721
8
105
12.4
86.7
1.0
12.6
156
970
13
160
13.7
85.2
1.1
14.0
137
875
9
115
13.4
85.7
0.9
11.3
172
858
40
150
16.1
80.2
3.7
14.0
645
4009
74
630
13.6
84.8
1.6
13.3
91
8
1
91.0
8.0
1.0
86
19
0
81.9
18.1
0.0
128
32
0
80.0
20.0
0.0
87
28
0
75.7
24.4
0.0
112
38
0
74.7
25.3
0.0
504
125
1
80.0
19.8
0.2
34
64
2
34.0
64.0
2.0
35
66
4
33.3
62.9
3.8
53
103
4
33.1
64.4
2.5
41
72
2
35.7
62.6
1.7
65
75
10
43.3
50.0
6.7
228
380
22
36.2
60.3
3.5
Notes. Total number of accidental overdose deaths occurring within Philadelphia between 2015 and 2019 that could
be successfully geocoded (i.e., no missing or unknown event address) or those that did not occur at the Philadelphia
International Airport, which was excluded from our citywide network of Thiessen polygons. Overdose data from
Drug Enforcement Administration. Phila. refers to Philadelphia. OD refers to Overdose deaths. * Percentages in
this row reflect the percentage of overdoses out of the total number of overdoses in the city.
Table 2 displays the distribution of ODs by Thiessen polygons in the study area. ODs are a
relatively rare occurrence for most Thiessen polygons in the study area. Nearly 34 of Thiessen polygons
experienced 0 or 1 ODs between 2015 and 2019.
7
the minimum acceptable hit rate varied by the crime type and areal unit under consideration. For all crime types,
when considering a spatial extent of 566 Thiessen polygons constructed around street intersections in Valencia, their analyses
yielded a minimum acceptable hit rate ranging from 72% to 83.7% (Briz-Redón et al. 2020).
As detailed in the measurements section, concentrated disadvantage is an index comprising the extent of unemployment,
poverty, households receiving public assistance, and female-headed households.
163
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Table 2. Number of ODs in Thiessen Polygons in the Study Area, 2015–2019.
N of ODs
N of TPs
%
Cumulative %
0
1
2
3
4
5
6–9
10+
Total
269
116
75
28
14
11
14
6
533
50.47
21.76
14.07
5.25
2.63
2.06
2.63
1.13
100
50.47
72.23
86.3
91.56
94.18
96.25
98.87
100
100
Notes. Overdose data from Drug Enforcement Administration. OD refers to overdose deaths. TP refers to
Thiessen polygons.
3.3.2. Independent Variables
The primary predictors of interest included indicators of DTO or drug corner status. A dichotomous
variable was created indicating whether the street corner was designated a Kensington-Fairhill “priority
corner.” Priority corners were determined in mid-2018 by a law enforcement working group that was
comprised of representatives from state, local and federal law enforcement agencies as those that
should be prioritized for collaborative long-term investigation efforts. Violent crime and drug seizure
data and street-level knowledge were used to define this list of corners based on each corner’s volume
of drug sales and level of violence (Roselli 2018).
In addition to the measure of priority corner status, a variable was also included to capture
the proximity of each street corner unit to priority corners. This variable was calculated from the
straight-line Euclidean distance between the centroid of each Thiessen polygon (the street corner)
and all priority corners. Each separate distance was then summed to represent the total distance
in miles between each Thiessen polygon and the priority corners. Lower values on this variable
indicate a street corner unit is closer to priority corners, whereas high values indicate the opposite.
Some studies have used a minimum distance measure between the centroid of a spatial unit and
key places such as hospitals or fire departments in predicting overdose deaths, e.g., (Li et al. 2019).
Similarly, studies examining the spatial exposure of risky facilities on spatial units have used inverse
distance calculations that weight places that are closer more and vice versa for those that are further
away, e.g., (Trangenstein et al. 2018). Rather than use an inverse distance calculation, which would
differentially weight gang corners based on their distance (Ratcliffe and Taniguchi 2008), we sum all
distances using equal weights, as we sought to capture the potential influence of all priority DTOs in
the target area on ODs as a measure of the agglomeration economy of drug markets. We also wanted
to be inclusive of all priority corners in the target area, as we anticipated a higher density of drug
purchasing opportunity would likely coincide with higher numbers of overdose deaths.
3.3.3. Additional Covariates
In addition to the priority corner indicators, we included a series of social and structural covariates
that we expected to have a theoretical relationship with the location of ODs. The typical contextual
factors related to social disorganization theory are the structural constraints that give rise to low social
control and cohesion (e.g., economic disadvantage, residential instability, foreign-born population).
Factors related to small-area places are informed by the routine activities perspective—those factors
that tend to attract and/or generate crime acting as places where motivated offenders come together
with potential targets and lack of capable guardians. These covariates are listed in turn below.
The sociodemographic context of the Thiessen polygons was measured by four variables
constructed from the 2014–2018 American Community Survey (ACS). We included the percent of the
population who were male, as prior research has linked this measure positively with heroin-related
emergency services calls (Li et al. 2019). We also included three variables capturing the social
164
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
disorganization of the street corners (Shaw and McKay 1942). These include an additive index
capturing the concentrated disadvantage of the corner. This variable was comprised of the sum of
the z scores for the percent of the population who are unemployed, below the poverty line, living in
female-headed households, and the percentage of households receiving public assistance divided by 4.
Residential instability is also measured as an index, comprised of the sum of the z scores of the percent
of renter-occupied housing units and percentage of households moving in after 2014 divided by 2.
To capture population heterogeneity, we created a proxy measure for foreign born, operationalized as
the percentage of the population who spoke a language at home other than English.8
In addition to the social context of street corner units, we include a series of measures related
to the environmental context of each unit, as prior research points to the importance of certain
environmental covariates in overdose deaths (Hembree et al. 2005; Li et al. 2019). Environmental
features of street corner units included the presence of commercial or recreational establishments
(Li et al. 2019; Hsu and Miller 2017), proximity of a street corner unit to a park (Groff and McCord 2012;
Hsu and Miller 2017; Li et al. 2019), as well as the number of street trees (Wheeler 2018) and presence
of bridges as measures of visibility. Commercial land-use data at the parcel level were obtained via the
City’s open data portal. The land-use data were available as a polygon shapefile that was imported into
ArcGIS Pro and overlaid with the Thiessen polygon network to calculate the percentage of commercial
or recreational establishments in each street corner unit. Park locations were also obtained via the
City’s data portal as a polygon shapefile that was overlaid with the Thiessen polygon network. If a
Thiessen polygon touched a park, it was given a value of one for the park variable, and a zero if it
did not. The number of street trees and presence of bridges were obtained as geocoded point data
from the same open data portal. The street trees were measured as part of a 2016 street tree inventory,
where each data point represents a tree. The number of trees was summed for each Thiessen polygon.
Considering bridge presence, a Thiessen polygon was said to contain a bridge if it contained a bridge
point after overlaying the point dataset with the Thiessen polygon network. We expected the measure
of whether the Thiessen polygon contained a bridge would be particularly relevant to ODs, since
Kensington has a recent history with homeless encampments situated beneath bridges and overpasses
(Metraux et al. 2019). Many of the inhabitants of these encampments are opioid users.
Regarding drug markets more generally, past research and the routine activities perspective
would suggest that environmental features that maximize retail accessibility and security confer more
success to drug dealing, or otherwise play into dealers’ decisions on where to do business (Eck 1995;
St. Jean 2007; Barnum et al. 2017; Bernasco and Jacques 2015). For instance, Valasik (2018) used Risk
Terrain Modelling (RTM) of 22 different environmental risk factors to predict gang assault and homicide.
He found that proximity to a Metro rail stop was one of the strongest predictors of gang assault.
In part, he notes this may be due to the routine activities of gang members themselves, who, along
with other community residents, often congregated around transit stops awaiting travel into the city.
Measures intending to capture the accessibility of street corners were thus included in this study, as we
hypothesized that corners conducive both to higher flows of people and drug sales would be associated
with more ODs. One measure included a dichotomous indicator of whether the Thiessen polygon
contained any transit stops (bus, train, trolley, or subway). Data on transit stop locations were obtained
via the Southeastern Pennsylvania Transit Authority’s (SEPTA) open data portal9 . The number of street
segments intersecting with each Thiessen polygon was also included. This variable was constructed by
overlaying a Philadelphia streets file with the Thiessen polygon layer in ArcGIS Pro, and capturing the
number of street segments that intersected with each polygon. Whether Thiessen polygons contained
any streets designated as “No-thru trucks streets” was included as another measure of accessibility,
given these would be smaller, local streets more conducive to pedestrian flow. Both street measures
were sourced from the city of Philadelphia’s open data web portal.
8
9
This American Community Survey item was not available at the Census block group level.
http://septaopendata-septa.opendata.arcgis.com/.
165
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Finally, we use 311 calls for service occurring between 1 January 2018 and 31 December 2019
as measures of physical incivilities and informal social control. We obtained geocoded data on five
categories of quality of life calls from the Philly 311 department. These data are also available via the
City’s open data web portal10 . The categories of calls included graffiti removal, streetlight outages,
vacant house calls, vacant lot cleanups, and calls regarding abandoned vehicles.
3.4. Allocation of Census Data to Thiessen Polygons
Thiessen polygons require an additional step compared to more traditional census geographies
when aggregating census data to each unit. Thiessen polygons may fall within multiple census units,
such as census block groups (CBGs). A simple method of assignment would be to assign each Thiessen
polygon the census attributes of the CBG that its centroid fell within. However, in this research,
we follow Taniguchi, Ratcliffe, and Taylor (2011) in proportionally allocating census attributes to
Thiessen polygons. This method calculates the proportion of area in CBGs that make up each Thiessen
polygon, and uses this proportion to allocate the count data to each Thiessen polygon. For instance,
if 25% of CBG 100 and 50% of CBG 101 fall within Thiessen polygon A, the weighted population for
Thiessen polygon A would be calculated as:
PopTPA = 0.25*PopCBG100 + 0.50*PopCBG101
(1)
All census attributes were proportionally allocated to Thiessen polygons using this method.
Census measures used in the analyses, including the percentage male, speaking a foreign language
at home, concentrated disadvantage and residential instability indices, were calculated from these
weighted counts. Table 3 presents descriptive statistics of all measures included in the analyses.11
Table 3. Descriptive Statistics.
Variable
Accidental overdose deaths, 2018–2019
Priority corner
Proximity to all priority corners
Concentrated disadvantage
Residential stability
Foreign language at home
Transit stops
No truck street
Number of street segments
Number of trees
Presence of a park
Presence of a bridge
Calls for abandoned vehicles
Calls for graffiti
Calls for broken street light
Calls for vacant houses
Calls for vacant lot
Male
Area (square ft)
Mean
SD
Min
Max
Skew
N
0.5
0.03
13.53
0.84
0.17
1
0.13
0.26
3.83
2.62
0.1
0.08
2.65
4.92
0.87
1.73
1.5
49.05
73,478.49
0.9
0.18
2.68
0.52
0.67
0.5
0.34
0.44
0.61
3.76
0.3
0.28
3.47
9.28
1.35
3.31
2.93
6.78
34,305.69
0
0
9.77
−0.55
−1.87
−0.15
0
0
2
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
27.93
20,439.41
6
1
22.18
2.97
2.02
4.74
1
1
6
33
1
1
27
87
9
27
24
82.52
312,922.9
2.47
5.16
0.74
0.42
0.24
2.05
2.16
1.07
0.21
2.9
2.68
2.99
2.71
4.62
2.48
3.23
3.71
0.04
2.37
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
533
Notes. SD refers to standard deviation, square ft refers to square feet.
10
11
https://www.opendataphilly.org/.
The land use variable measuring the percent commercial or recreational land use was dropped from all analyses due to its
correlation with transit stops, impeding model convergence. We retained the transit stops variable as we expected it would
vary across the spatial units in our study more than the land use variable, particularly due to the very small size of the units.
166
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
3.5. Spatial Concentration of ODs
Before multivariate analyses were conducted, we used a novel metric, the Rare Event Concentration
Coefficient (RECC), to determine the concentration of accidental ODs throughout the study area between
2015 and 2019 (Curiel and Bishop 2016; Curiel et al. 2018). We examined the RECC annually to provide
insight into the stability (or instability) of the concentration of ODs over time, which in turn, guided
which years of data were aggregated for the predictive analyses. The RECC was conceived as a
concentration metric designed for rare events (e.g., crime, volcano eruptions, and traffic accidents) that
typically follow a Poisson-type process. The rarity at which ODs occurred in the target area necessitated
this approach as most street corner units experienced 0 ODs across the five years of this study. Because
whether a place experiences 0 events in a year does not mean its probability of experiencing crime is
equal to 0 (Curiel et al. 2018), it is more desirable to consider observed counts as realizations of an
underlying Poisson process, rather than focus on the counts themselves (Curiel and Bishop 2016).
The calculation of the RECC proceeded in two main steps. The first was to create a distribution
of expected rates at which ODs occur. This was accomplished by estimating a mixture model of the
observed counts of ODs for all Thiessen polygons. We used the CAMAN package in R12 for this
analysis. This process identified groups of Thiessen polygons with the same rate of OD occurrence in
each year. A vector of the rates was then created, where the frequency at which each rate appeared
in the vector was proportional to the number of Thiessen polygons that fell into its respective group.
The second step in the calculation was to apply the formula for the Gini coefficient to the vector
of expected rates, which produced the RECC. The RECC takes on a value between 0 and 1, and is
interpreted in the same manner as the Gini coefficient, with values closer to 0 denoting more equality
(or more dispersion), and values closer to 1 indicating more concentration. We repeated this analysis
for ODs in Thiessen polygons for each year between 2015 and 2019. The resulting values are in Table 4.
The RECC dropped by nearly 20% between 2015 and 2019, reflecting that ODs are more dispersed—that
is, they are more evenly experienced by street corner units in Kensington—in the latter part of the
time period.
Table 4. Rare Event Concentration Coefficient (RECC) for Overdose Deaths in the Target Area, 2015–2019.
Year
RECC—Rates
RECC—Raw
2015
2016
2017
2018
2019
0.71
0.615
0.649
0.451
0.568
0.711
0.618
0.651
0.453
0.571
Notes. RECC refers to Rare Event Concentration Coefficient. Values in the “RECC—Rates” column were calculated
using the natural log of the area of the Thiessen polygons as an offset in the mixture model. The “RECC—Raw”
column does not use an offset in the mixture model; the estimated group rates are estimated counts per year.
3.6. Analytic Approach Predicting Counts of ODs in Thiessen Polygons
The RECC analyses revealed a shift in the spatial dispersion of ODs in 2018 and 2019, suggesting
ODs are less concentrated in more recent years. We therefore focused our multivariate analyses
predicting counts of ODs in street corner units on those overdose deaths occurring in 2018 and
2019. A global Moran’s I test of the 2018 and 2019 ODs also revealed significant positive spatial
autocorrelation among the target area Thiessen polygons (Moran’s I = 0.24, pseudo-p < 0.001)
(Anselin 1996). This motivated our use of a spatial econometric method using an information theoretic
approach to estimating ODs in Thiessen polygons. Specifically, we employed a series of generalized
cross-entropy (GCE) models, which are well suited for examining rare count outcomes with a correlated
12
https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/CAMAN/CAMAN.pdf.
167
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
spatial structure. GCE models are also adept at handling both over- and underdispersion and have
been demonstrated as more suitable (compared to negative binomial regression) to estimating spatially
autocorrelated and overdispersed count outcomes through simulations (Bhati 2008). The GCE modeling
approach has been used in previous place-based crime studies, including those examining homicides
in Chicago (Bhati 2008) and the effect of alcohol outlets on violence and disorder in Washington,
DC (Roman et al. 2008; Roman and Reid 2012). It is important to note that earlier studies comparing GCE
model outcomes to outcomes from the more typical negative binomial regression models incorporating
a spatial lag variable reveal the superiority of the GCE models for handling the spatial structure of the
data (Roman et al. 2008). We used a GCE macro written for the SAS platform to execute all regression
models (see Roman et al. 2008).
The general model building strategy we follow was nested according to different theoretical
blocks of interest. The first model predicted the number of ODs including only the key gang variables,
controlling for the percentage of the population who are male. The second model incorporated the
variables measuring the social structural features of the street corners, including measures of social
disorganization. Model three examined the effect of certain environmental features of street corners on
drug overdoses. Finally, model four explored the effect of the priority corner status on ODs, while
simultaneously considering the social and environmental features of the street corners. For each model
considered, the natural log of the area of each Thiessen polygon in feet was used as the offset variable.
4. Results
The GCE regression results from all four models are detailed in Table 5. Unlike OLS regression
models, the coefficients yielded from the GCE models do not allow for a direct substantive interpretation
(Bhati 2008). However, marginal effects can be calculated from the GCE coefficients, which can be
interpreted as the change in the expected rate of ODs per square foot given a one-unit change in the
predictor. In order to save space, the marginal effects are not shown in Table 5, but they are noted in
the text and available from the lead author upon request. In Model 1, both the gang corner status
and the proximity to gang measure were significantly related to the rate of OD incidents in each
street corner, controlling for the percentage male population. The priority corner status was positively
related to ODs, indicating that corners that were prioritized for drug gang enforcement efforts were
associated with higher rates of ODs than those that were not. The variable capturing each corner’s
proximity to all gangs was negatively related to ODs, meaning that the less distance between a street
corner and all gang corners in the target area, the higher the OD rate that corner is expected to have.
However, the pseudo-R2 for this model was −0.09, indicating a very poor fit to the data. Model 2
incorporated three measures of social disorganization of the street corners. The gang status of the
corner remained a positive and significant indicator of ODs, however the measure capturing the
proximity to all gang corners was no longer significant once considering the social disorganization of
the corners. Consistent with expectations, both concentrated disadvantage and residential instability
were significantly, and positively related to the rate of ODs. Yet, the measure of foreign language
at home was significantly and negatively related to overdoses. The Model 2 pseudo-R2 was 0.04,
indicating Model 2 was also a poor fit.
Model 3 incorporated aspects of the physical environment of the corners into the model, resulting
in an improved model pseudo-R2 of 0.12. Both priority corner predictors dropped out as significant
predictors of ODs when simultaneously controlling for social disorganization and certain environmental
characteristics of the corners. The coefficients for all three social disorganization factors remained
largely unchanged from Model 2, yet there were only two environmental characteristics—transit stops
and the number of street segments—that were significantly associated with ODs.
168
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Table 5. GCE Model Results.
Predicting Overdose Deaths
Variable
169
Constant
Drug Trafficking Organizations/Gangs
Priority corner
Proximity to all priority corners
Social Disorganization
Concentrated disadvantage
Residential stability
Foreign language at home
Physical Environment
Transit stops
No truck street
Number of street segments
Number of trees
Presence of a park
Presence of a bridge
Informal Social Control/Incivilities
Calls for abandoned vehicles
Calls for graffiti
Calls for broken street light
Calls for vacant houses
Calls for vacant lot
Control
Male
Pseudo R2
Rho
Overdispersion parameter
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Coeff.
S.E.
Coeff.
S.E.
Coeff.
S.E.
Coeff.
S.E.
4.103 ***
0.743
3.126 **
1.123
−0.279
0.344
−13.625 ***
2.815
0.406 *
−0.042 ***
0.184
0.011
0.446 **
−0.020
0.183
0.013
0.215
−0.031
0.214
0.019
0.479 *
−0.092 ***
0.233
0.03
-
-
0.254 ***
0.155 *
−0.448 ***
0.082
0.064
0.094
0.246 ***
0.158 *
−0.393 ***
0.085
0.07
0.103
0.397 ***
0.108
−0.463 ***
0.132
0.099
0.143
-
-
-
-
0.396 ***
0.009
0.257 ***
−0.029
−0.187
0.189
0.114
0.118
0.078
0.017
0.203
0.142
0.624 ***
0.038
0.190 *
0.015
−0.590 **
0.457 **
0.156
0.145
0.096
0.02
0.239
0.182
-
-
-
-
-
-
−0.047 *
−0.005
−0.056
0.055 ***
0.042 **
0.023
0.007
0.044
0.015
0.016
−0.013 *
0.007
−0.09
1.23 ***
0.80 ***
−0.003
0.007
0.04
1.20 ***
0.81 ***
−0.003
0.002
0.12
1.00 ***
0.75 ***
0.011
0.01
0.22
−0.08
0.57 ***
Notes. GCE refers to Generalized Cross Entropy. Coeff. refers to coefficient, S.E. refers to standard error. * p <= 0.05; ** p <= 0.01; *** p <= 0.001.
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
The full model (Model 4) included both priority corner variables, and social and environmental
features of the street corners in predicting rates of ODs. The pseudo-R2 for the main model increased
to 0.22, signifying an improvement in model fit. Taking into account the features of the social and built
environment, the variable capturing priority corner status significantly predicted overdose deaths,
in that street corners that are designated drug trafficking corners were associated with 0.42 more
expected overdoses per square foot. Similarly, the measure that captures the proximity to all priority
corners was significant and negative, meaning the closer a street corner is to all drug trafficking corners,
the higher the expected rate of ODs is in that corner, net of all other covariates. Stated differently,
each one-mile increase in the distance to all priority corners was associated with an expected 0.08 fewer
ODs per square foot.
With regard to the social disorganization factors, the final model revealed a significant relationship
between concentrated disadvantage and ODs (marginal effect = 0.34) and the percent speaking a
foreign language at home and ODs (marginal effect = −0.40). Additional contextual influences include
the positive and significant effects of transit stops and number of intersecting street segments, measures
intending to capture the accessibility of the street corner, on overdose deaths. Consistent with our
expectation, the results suggest street corners that generate more pedestrian flow were more likely
to have a higher expected rate of overdose deaths (transit stops: marginal effect = 0.54, number of
street segments: marginal effect = 0.16). Whether a street corner had a transit stop in particular was
associated with an expected increase of 0.54 ODs per square foot. However, the flag for whether a
street corner unit touched a park was significant and negatively related to overdose deaths (marginal
effect = −0.51). Finally, measures capturing the informal social control and incivilities of the area,
calls for vacant houses and for vacant lots, were significantly and positively associated with overdose
deaths (marginal effects = 0.05, 0.04, respectively). Calls for abandoned vehicles were also significantly
associated with more ODs, yet in the opposite direction (marginal effect = −0.04). Importantly, Rho,
the coefficient representing the spatial autocorrelation, was significant and positive for Models 1–3,
yet the full model had a non-significant Rho.
5. Limitations
Before we discuss the findings, there are several limitations to this study that require mention.
The statistical analyses we employed are inherently cross-sectional, and hence, our findings can only
comment on the correlational relationship between our predictors of interest and overdose deaths.
Future work could employ longitudinal methods to establish a causal link between presence of
gang-controlled drug corners and overdose deaths that might result. Second, our indicator for whether
a Thiessen polygon contained a priority drug trafficking corner status is not inclusive of all gang-related
corners in the target area (or even all drug-selling gang corners), though it captures the most violent
and/or high-volume drug trafficking corners as of 2018. Third, our data preclude our ability to know
whether or not ODs are affiliated with a given drug trafficking corner. The target area is relatively
small, with some drug-selling gang corners in relatively close proximity. If individuals use drugs
nearby after they purchase them, it is possible that they OD in another gang’s drug sales territory.
Finally, although the RECC analyses showed that the dispersion of overdose deaths changed over time,
we did not run analyses that might have given us clues as to why the concentration changed over
time. We could hypothesize that these changes had to do with macro-level forces such as the steep
increase in sales of fentanyl that occurred throughout the US (and Philadelphia) temporally around
late 2016 and 2017, or the closing of a large outdoor homeless encampment supporting users of heroin
around the same time, but we do not have the appropriate data to validly model these changes. These
limitations notwithstanding, this study helps shed light on the connection between drug trafficking
gangs and the spatial location of accidental overdose deaths.
170
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
6. Discussion and Conclusions
Using multiple methods, this work sought to answer whether deaths from accidental drug
overdose are located in close proximity to gang-controlled street corners in an area of a city known
for its agglomeration economy drug markets and provide insight into which features of places are
associated with the location of overdose deaths using theory as a guide. Findings from RECC analyses
indicated that patterns of overdose mortality clustering changed over time, providing guidance
that predictive analyses should focus only on a two-year period where the spatial patterns were
similar, as opposed to a longer time period. As stated in the “limitations section”, OD dispersion
could potentially be attributed to different factors changing the routine activities of drug users in
the neighborhood attributed to changes in access to public spaces such as the 2017 cleanup of a
large, open-air heroin camp at the Conrail train tracks in Kensington (Wolfram 2017)—potentially
displacing many homeless heroin users. It could be that this displacement shifted many users’ routine
activities and in consequence shifted the distribution of ODs across Kensington. It may also suggest
that risky environments move around and change with the habits of individuals. A state level policy
change may also have influenced the dispersal of overdose deaths in Kensington during the time
period examined. The governor of Pennsylvania issued a standing order in 2015 allowing for all
citizens—not just emergency services personnel—to obtain the overdose-reversal medication Naloxone
(Schwartz et al. 2020). This change allows for more people, including drug users and sellers, to carry
and administer the medication (Feldman 2020).
The GCE models revealed that street corners controlled by drug trafficking gangs or those that are
situated centrally for easy access to multiple drug corners were associated with more ODs, echoing
previous work finding a significant positive influence of gang member density at the block level and
the presence of a drug house at the parcel level on overdose deaths (Headley Konkel and Hoffman
2020). The results also suggest drug market dynamics where buyers use drugs right away or close to
purchase, consistent with anecdotal evidence from law enforcement. Unsurprisingly, street corners
that were designated as priority corners showed clear evidence of being affiliated with a higher rate
of ODs. Further, indeed, additional analyses (not shown) revealed that the average number of ODs
in Thiessen polygons designated as priority corners was double that of non-gang corners13 . A key
implication to also note from these findings is that the actual location and status of individual drug
corners are not the only measures predicting higher counts of overdose deaths. The agglomeration
economy of the Kensington-Fairhill drug market appears to contribute to its overall retail success,
but also its experience with overdose deaths. The significance of the variable capturing the proximity
to gang-controlled drug corners leads us to believe that even corners that are not gang-controlled may
experience more ODs because of where they are relatively located in space. There were 54 Thiessen
polygons in the top 90th percentile in proximity to drug corners, yet the average count of ODs for this
group was 8% higher than for the target area polygons as a whole. Geographically, these polygons
were clustered in the lower middle of the target area, several blocks from the Kensington Avenue
corridor. Our analyses reveal that it is important to capture not only the DTO status of corners in
predicting the spatial location of ODs, but also where each street corner is situated within the wider
network of open-air drug market corners.
We also found support for social disorganization theory with regard to the relevance of certain
macro structural constraints in predicting overdose deaths. Our findings revealed a significant effect of
concentrated disadvantage on ODs, which is somewhat inconsistent with previous work. For instance,
Martinez et al. (2008) and Li et al. (2019) both included structural deprivation measures in their models
predicting overdoses, yet the effects were non-significant. The differences in findings here could relate
13
There were 18 Thiessen polygons designated as priority corners, experiencing 19 ODs in 2018–2019. This resulted in a
mean of 1.06 ODs per Thiessen polygon. There were 515 Thiessen polygons that were not designated as priority corners,
experiencing 246 ODs in 2018-2019, with a mean of 0.48 ODs per Thiessen polygon.
171
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
to the different unit of analyses used or that there are regional differences (across the US) with regard
to how social disorganization influences drug misuse/overdose mortality. The negative effect of our
measure to capture foreign born—the percent speaking a foreign language at home—on ODs in our
model, however, echoes the (Martinez et al. 2008) finding that racial heterogeneity had a negative
relationship with drug overdoses. While it is impossible to know the precise mechanism responsible
for this relationship, it could be that our measure of foreign born reflects a type of protective factor that
has been associated with concentrations of immigrants and crime (Ousey and Kubrin 2018). For our
study, the Census-based measures represent the residents living in homes in the street corner areas,
not the characteristics of the overdose victims. Similar to Johnson and Shreve (2020), we are unable to
account for individual characteristics of the decedents due to the aggregate nature of our analyses and
idiosyncrasies of the data used here. Specifically, while the data often contained the home address
of decedents (some were labeled missing, unknown, or homeless), there may be some question as to
the reliability of this indicator in practice, particularly given the transience of much of the drug using
population in Kensington. This fact notwithstanding, we found that 34% of the fatal OD victims in the
target area for the period 2018–2019 were described as dying at their residence.14 From this we can
cautiously surmise that in at least 1/3 of the OD cases in the study area, for the social disorganization
measures, the corner attributes partially reflect the macro-level structural characteristics related to the
decedents’ themselves. This makeup agrees with recent qualitative work in Kensington that finds most
drug users in the neighborhood are coming in from other places (Friedman et al. 2019). Future research
could not only explore how the individual-level characteristics of those misusing drugs in micro-places
interact with characteristics of residents in those neighborhoods, but also the dynamics of drug use
and overdose among different populations—residents from inside and outside of the neighborhood
and the homeless population—in and around this drug market.
We also found that the accessibility of the street corners themselves translated to more ODs. This is
contrary to Li et al. (2019) finding that public transportation (measured as bus coverage) was not
significantly related to overdose deaths. The significant negative relationship between street corners
that touched a park and overdose deaths was also contradictory to Li et al. (2019) findings of a positive
relationship between these two constructs, though their measure of park presence differed from ours
in that it captured the proportion of park area in each census block group. For the measures capturing
the informal social control and incivilities of the area, two call types, those for vacant houses and
those for vacant lots, were significantly and positively associated with overdose deaths, while calls
for abandoned vehicles were significantly and negatively associated with ODs. We are unaware of
previous work examining these specific call types on overdose deaths, but research examining the
degradation of the built environment has found mixed results with regard to its effects on overdose
deaths. For example, (Hembree et al. 2005) found that the percentage of acceptably clean streets in New
York City was a negative predictor of drug overdose deaths, while (Cerdá et al. 2013), also looking
at New York City, found that acceptably clean sidewalks were only a significant predictor when
comparing analgesic-related ODs with heroin-related ODs, but not compared with non-overdose
accidental deaths. Furthermore, the authors found that dilapidated housing was not a significant
predictor for analgesic-related ODs for either comparison. In the context of our results, more calls
regarding vacant houses or lots could be related to the actual presence of such vacant lots and houses;
spaces that may be attractive for illicit drug use.
Ultimately, the results from our GCE models and RECC analyses have multiple implications
for policy related to community overdose mortality and gangs actively involved in selling heroin.
Our findings suggest programs seeking to address ODs should be mobile and targeted specifically to
risky places to enhance service reach. The emphasis here is on micro-places—prevention efforts can be
14
i.e., the death location was noted as “residence”, and the decedents’ home address, event address, and death address were
all the same.
172
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
pinpointed with precise targeting—down to blocks and corners. Mobile outreach initiatives, such as
those providing clean syringe exchange or treatment, and other flexible strategies would be a relevant
approach to reducing ODs, capable of adapting with the routine activities of users. Particularly in the
context of Kensington, where most users are not residents of the neighborhood (Friedman et al. 2019) and
therefore may not maintain a static presence or activity space (Brantingham and Brantingham 1995).
The effect of priority corner proximity, as well as of accessibility of street corners on ODs, bolsters
this idea that locating social service provision and outreach central to drug markets and high traffic
locations may prove beneficial.
The co-location of ODs and drug trafficking corners also alludes to the utility of coordinating
law enforcement and social services to address both challenges. It has long been acknowledged that
“focused, partnership-type law enforcement interventions are generally far more effective responses to
ongoing crime problems than are unfocused efforts relying entirely on law enforcement resources”
(Mazerolle et al. 2007, p. 116). Coordinated interventions have the potential to disrupt the harm caused
by gang members through large-scale drug distribution and gun violence, while attempting to reverse
some of the physical and social incivilities brought on certain neighborhood corners that affect residents’
quality of life. Indeed, in their systematic review of drug law enforcement, (Mazerolle et al. 2007)
found that proactive strategies involving partnerships between law enforcement and third parties or
community agencies were the most effective at reducing drug and other crime, as well as increasing
community residents’ reported quality of life (Mazerolle et al. 2007).
In some jurisdictions, the scale of the current opioid epidemic appears to be facilitating a shift
towards increased collaboration between federal, state, and local law enforcement agencies and their
public health and social service counterparts to reduce the harm affiliated with drug markets and
gangs (Police Executive Research Forum 2016). Collaborative partnerships that include permanent
supportive housing would be especially salient for neighborhoods such as Kensington with a recent
history of homeless and heroin encampments. So too would strategies that incorporate elimination of or
reduction in housing blight. In severely disadvantaged areas such as Kensington-Fairhill, collaborative
strategies could benefit from careful, data-driven targeting of micro-locations for clean-up and housing
investment following (or simultaneous to) law enforcement strategies for arrest and prosecution.
In short, a multipronged strategy of combating drug trafficking organizations while alleviating some of
the social and environmental obstacles faced by some areas may go a long way in lessening the impact
of individual drug corners and the larger market on overdose deaths. While the current research
focused on a single phenomenon, accidental overdose deaths, the scope of the harm brought by drug
trafficking organizations on the communities in which they operate is difficult to overstate. Researchers
and practitioners should continue to use data to better inform how drug trafficking organizations
shape public safety and public health in communities, and in turn, inform innovative solutions to
address both.
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, C.G.R., N.J.J., C.H., L.H.; methodology, C.G.R., N.J.J.; formal analysis,
C.G.R., N.J.J.; writing—original draft preparation, N.J.J, C.G.R., A.K.M., C.H..; writing—review and editing and
substantive additions, N.J.J, C.G.R., A.K.M., C.H.., M.F., L.H.; funding acquisition, C.G.R. All authors have read
and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was funded under the U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Assistance, grant
number 2019-AR-BX-0022. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily
reflect the official position/policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Acknowledgments: A number of people and agencies provided us with data or assisted when questions arose
about particular data elements. Those we thank include Evangelia Manos of Philadelphia’s Managing Director’s
Office, Vito Roselli, Federal Bureau of Investigation, Kendra Viner from the Philadelphia Department of Public
Health, Angela Ruffin and staff from the Philly311 office. We also express our gratitude to Nehemiah Haigler at
the OAG, and Avinash Bhati and Rafael Prieto Curiel for their statistical advice.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
173
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
References
Anselin, Luc. 1996. The Moran scatterplot as an ESDA tool to assess local instability in spatial. Spatial Analytical
4: 111.
Armenian, Patil, Kathy T. Vo, Jill Barr-Walker, and Kara L. Lynch. 2018. Fentanyl, fentanyl analogs and novel
synthetic opioids: A comprehensive review. Neuropharmacology 134: 121–32. [CrossRef]
Barnum, Jeremy D., Walter L. Campbell, Sarah Trocchio, Joel M. Caplan, and Leslie W. Kennedy. 2017. Examining
the environmental characteristics of drug dealing locations. Crime & Delinquency 63: 1731–56. [CrossRef]
Bates, Savannah, Vasiliy Leonenko, James Rineer, and Georgiy Bobashev. 2019. Using synthetic populations to
understand geospatial patterns in opioid related overdose and predicted opioid misuse. Computational and
Mathematical Organization Theory 25: 36–47. [CrossRef]
Bernasco, Wim, and Scott Jacques. 2015. Where do dealers solicit customers and sell them drugs? A micro-level
multiple method study. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice 31: 376–408. [CrossRef]
Bhati, Avinash S. 2008. A generalized cross-entropy approach for modeling spatially correlated counts. Econometric
Reviews 27: 574–95. [CrossRef]
Brantingham, Patricia L., and Paul J. Brantingham. 1981. Notes on the Geometry of Crime. In Environmental
Criminology. Edited by Paul J. Brantingham and Patricia L. Brantingham. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications,
pp. 27–54.
Brantingham, Patricia L., and Paul J. Brantingham. 1995. Criminality of place. European Journal on Criminal Policy
and Research 3: 5–26. [CrossRef]
Briz-Redón, Alvaro, Francisco Martinez-Ruiz, and Francisco Montes. 2020. Reestimating a minimum acceptable
geocoding hit rate for conducting a spatial analysis. International Journal of Geographical Information Science 34:
1283–305. [CrossRef]
Bursik, Robert J., and Harold G. Grasmick. 1993. Neighborhoods and Crime: The Dimensions of Effective Community
Control. New York: Macmillan.
Carter, Jeremy G., George Mohler, and Bradley Ray. 2019. Spatial concentration of opioid overdose deaths in
Indianapolis: An application of the law of crime concentration at place to a public health epidemic. Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice 35: 161–85. [CrossRef]
CDC (Centers for Disease Control, National Center for Health Statistics). 2020. Wide-Ranging Online Data for
Epidemiologic Research (WONDER). Available online: http://wonder.cdc.gov (accessed on 29 September
2020).
Cerdá, Magdalena, Yusuf Ransome, Katherine M. Keyes, Karestan C. Koenen, Kenneth Tardiff, David Vlahov,
and Sandro Galea. 2013. Revisiting the role of the urban environment in substance use: The case of analgesic
overdose fatalities. American Journal of Public Health 103: 2252–60. [CrossRef]
Chainey, Spencer, and Jerry H. Ratcliffe. 2005. GIS and Crime Mapping. Hoboken: John Wiley and Sons.
Cohen, Lawrence, and Marcus Felson. 1979. Social change and crime rate trends: A routine activity approach.
American Sociological Review 44: 588–608. [CrossRef]
Confair, Amy, Amy Carroll-Scott, Katherine Castro, Yuzhe Zhao, Steven Melly, Jennifer Kolker, Stephen Lankenau,
and Alexis Roth. 2019. Community Health Profile: Kensington. Philadelphia: Drexel University Urban Health
Collaborative.
Connell, Christian M., Tamika D. Gilreath, Will M. Aklin, and Robert A. Brex. 2010. Social-ecological influences on
patterns of substance use among non-metropolitan high school students. American Journal of Community
Psychology 45: 36–48. [CrossRef]
Curiel, Rafael P., and Steven Bishop. 2016. A measure of the concentration of rare events. Scientific Reports 6: 32369.
[CrossRef]
Curiel, Rafael P., Sofia C. Delmar, and Steven R. Bishop. 2018. Measuring the distribution of crime and its
concentration. Journal of Quantitative Criminology 34: 775–803. [CrossRef]
DEA (Drug Enforcement Administration & the University of Pittsburgh). 2018. The Opioid Threat in Pennsylvania.
Joint Intelligence Report. DEA-PHL-DIR-036–18. Available online: https://www.dea.gov/sites/default/files/2
018-10/Opioid%20threat%20in%20Pennsylvania%20FINAL.pdf (accessed on 29 September 2020).
DEA (Drug Enforcement Administration). 2019. National Drug Threat Assessment. DEA-DCT-DIR-007-20.
Available online: https://www.dea.gov/sites/default/files/2020-01/2019-NDTA-final-01-14-2020_Low_WebDIR-007-20_2019.pdf (accessed on 29 September 2020).
174
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Eck, John E. 1995. A general model of the geography of illicit retail marketplaces. Crime and Place 4: 67–93.
Exec. Order No. 3–18. 2018 October 3. Available online: https://www.phila.gov/ExecutiveOrders/Executive%20Or
ders/eo99318.pdf (accessed on 5 November 2020).
Feldman, Nina. 2020. One Woman’s Mission to Make Sure Everyone Carries Narcan—Including Drug Dealers.
WHYY. July 10. Available online: https://whyy.org/segments/one-womans-mission-to-make-sure-everyone
-carries-narcan-including-drug-dealers/ (accessed on 29 September 2020).
Felson, Marcus. 1987. Routine activities and crime prevention in the developing metropolis. Criminology 25:
911–31. [CrossRef]
Felson, Marcus. 1994. Crime and Everyday Life: Insight and Implications for Society. Thousand Oaks: Pine Forge Press.
Friedman, Joseph, George Karandinos, Laurie K. Hart, Fernando M. Castrillo, Nicholas Graetz, and Philippe Bourgois.
2019. Structural vulnerability to narcotics-driven firearm violence: An ethnographic and epidemiological
study of Philadelphia’s Puerto Rican inner-city. PLoS ONE 14: e0225376. [CrossRef]
Groff, Elizabeth, and Eric S. McCord. 2012. The role of neighborhood parks as crime generators. Security Journal
25: 1–24. [CrossRef]
Haberman, Cory P. 2017. Overlapping hot spots? Examination of the spatial heterogeneity of hot spots of different
crime types. Criminology & Public Policy 16: 633–60. [CrossRef]
Haigler, Nehemiah, and Melissa Francis. 2020. Personal communication.
Han, Ying, Wei Yan, Yongbo Zheng, Muhammad Z. Khan, Kai Yuan, and Lin Lu. 2019. The rising crisis of illicit
fentanyl use, overdose, and potential therapeutic strategies. Translational Psychiatry 9: 282. [CrossRef]
Harocopos, Alex, and Mike Hough. 2012. Drug Dealing in Open-Air Markets; Problem-Oriented Guides for
Police. Problem Specific Guides Series No. 31; Washington, DC: Department of Justice Office of Community
Oriented Policing Services.
Headley Konkel, Rebecca, and Chrystina Y. Hoffman. 2020. Immediate and neighborhood contextual effects
on intentional, accidental, and fatal drug overdoses in a non-urban jurisdiction. Deviant Behavior 1–18.
[CrossRef]
Hembree, C, Sandro Galea, Jennifer Ahern, Melissa Tracy, T. Markham Piper, J. Miller, David Vlahov, and Kenneth
J. Tardiff. 2005. The urban built environment and overdose mortality in New York City neighborhoods.
Health & Place 11: 147–56. [CrossRef]
Hsu, Ko-Hsin, and Joel Miller. 2017. Assessing the situational predictors of drug markets across street segments
and intersections. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 54: 902–29. [CrossRef]
Johnson, Lallen T. 2016. Drug markets, travel distance, and violence: Testing a typology. Crime & Delinquency 62:
1465–87. [CrossRef]
Johnson, Lallen T., and Tayler Shreve. 2020. The ecology of overdose mortality in Philadelphia. Health & Place 66:
102430. [CrossRef]
Li, Zehang R., Evaline Xie, Forrest W. Crawford, Joshua L. Warren, Kathryn McConnell, J. Tyler Copple,
Tyler Johnson, and Gregg S. Gonsalves. 2019. Suspected heroin-related overdoses incidents in Cincinnati,
Ohio: A spatiotemporal analysis. PLoS Medicine 16: 1–15. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lieberman, Dan, Sean Ryon, and Ed Ou. 2020. With Overdose Deaths up during the Pandemic, Philadelphia Fights
for a Legal Safe Injection site. NBC News. August 2. Available online: https://www.nbcnews.com/news/usnews/overdose-deaths-during-pandemic-philadelphia-fights-legal-safe-injection-site-n1235583 (accessed
on 29 September 2020).
Martinez, Ramiro, Jr., Richard Rosenfeld, and Dennis Mares. 2008. Social disorganization, drug market activity,
and neighborhood violent crime. Urban Affairs Review 43: 846–74. [CrossRef]
Mazerolle, Lorraine, David Soole, and Sacha Rombouts. 2007. Drug law enforcement: A review of the evaluation
literature. Police Quarterly 10: 115–53. [CrossRef]
McCord, Eric S., and Jerry H. Ratcliffe. 2007. A micro-spatial analysis of the demographic and criminogenic
environment of drug markets in Philadelphia. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology 40: 43–63.
[CrossRef]
Mennis, Jeremy, Gerald J. Stahler, and Michael J. Mason. 2016. Risky substance use environments and addiction:
A new frontier for environmental justice research. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public
Health 13: 607. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
175
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Metraux, Stephen, Meagan Cusack, Fritz Graham, David Metzger, and Dennis Culhane. 2019. An evaluation of
the City of Philadelphia’s Kensington Encampment Resolution Pilot. Available online: https://www.phila.go
v/media/20190312102914/Encampment-Resolution-Pilot-Report.pdf (accessed on 29 September 2020).
NIDA (National Institute on Drug Abuse). 2020. Pennsylvania: Opioid-Involved Deaths and Related Harms.
Available online: https://www.drugabuse.gov/drug-topics/opioids/opioid-summaries-by-state/pennsylvania
-opioid-involved-deaths-related-harms (accessed on 29 September 2020).
Olsen, Yngvild. 2016. The CDC guideline on opioid prescribing: Rising to the challenge. JAMA 315: 1577–79.
[CrossRef]
Ousey, Graham C., and Charis E. Kubrin. 2018. Immigration and crime: Assessing a contentious issue. Annual
Review of Criminology 1: 63–84. [CrossRef]
Pardo, Bryce, Jirka Taylor, Jonathan P. Caulkins, Beau Kilmer, Peter Reuter, and Bradley D. Stein. 2019.
The Future of Fentanyl and Other Synthetic Opioids. Santa Monica: RAND Corporation, Available online:
https://ntakd.lrv.lt/uploads/ntakd/documents/files/Published%20version.pdf (accessed on 29 September
2020).
Percy, Jennifer. 2018. Trapped by the ‘Walmart of Heroin’. The New York Times Magazine. October 10.
Available online: https://www.nytimes.com/2018/10/10/magazine/kensington-heroin-opioid-philadelphia.ht
ml (accessed on 29 September 2020).
Piza, Eric L., and Andrew M. Gilchrist. 2018. Measuring the effect heterogeneity of police enforcement actions
across spatial contexts. Journal of Criminal Justice 54: 76–87. [CrossRef]
Police Executive Research Forum. 2016. Building Successful Partnerships between Law Enforcement and Public Health
Agencies to Address Opioid Use; COPS Office Emerging Issues Forums. Washington: Office of Community
Oriented Policing Services.
Ratcliffe, Jerry H. 2004. Geocoding crime and a first estimate of a minimum acceptable hit rate. International Journal
of Geographical Information Science 18: 61–72. [CrossRef]
Ratcliffe, Jerry H., and Travis A. Taniguchi. 2008. Is crime higher around drug-gang street corners? Two spatial
approaches to the relationship between gang set spaces and local crime levels. Crime Patterns and Analysis 1:
17–39.
Ratcliffe, Jerry H., Travis Taniguchi, Elizabeth R. Groff, and Jennifer D. Wood. 2011. The Philadelphia foot
patrol experiment: A randomized controlled trial of police patrol effectiveness in violent crime hotspots.
Criminology 49: 795–831. [CrossRef]
Roman, Caterina G., and Shannon E. Reid. 2012. Assessing the relationship between alcohol outlets and domestic
violence: Routine activities and the neighborhood environment. Violence and Victims 27: 811–28. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
Roman, Caterina G., Shannon E. Reid, Avinash S. Bhati, and Bogdan Tereshchenko. 2008. Alcohol Outlets as
Attractors of Violence and Disorder: A Closer Look at the Neighborhood Environment. Washington, DC: Urban
Institute.
Roselli, Vito. 2018. The Violent Crime & Opioid Reduction Partnership (VCORP), Kensington; Philadelphia: FBI
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Office of the Attorney General, Philadelphia Police Department, Unpublished
Report (June).
Rudd, Rose A., Noah Aleshire, Jon E. Zibbell, and R. Matthew Gladden. 2016. Increases in drug and opioid
overdose deaths–United States, 2000–2014. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 64: 1378–82. [CrossRef]
Schwartz, David G., Janna Ataiants, Alexis Roth, Gabriela Marcu, Inbal Yahav, Benjamin Cocchiaro,
Michael Khalemsky, and Stephen Lankenau. 2020. Layperson reversal of opioid overdose supported
by smartphone alert: A prospective observational cohort study. EClinical Medicine 25: 100474. [CrossRef]
Shaw, Clifford R., and Henry D. McKay. 1942. Juvenile Delinquency and Urban Areas. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
St. Jean, Peter K.B. 2007. Pockets of Crime: Broken Windows, Collective Efficacy, and the Criminal Point of View. Chicago:
University of Chicago.
Stevens, Alex, and Dave Bewley-Taylor. 2009. Drug Markets and Urban Violence: Can Tackling one Reduce the Other?
Report 15. Oxford: The Beckley Foundation Drug Police Programme, pp. 1–15.
Taniguchi, Travis A., George F. Rengert, and Eric S. McCord. 2009. Where size matters: Agglomeration economies
of illegal drug markets in Philadelphia. Justice Quarterly 26: 670–94. [CrossRef]
176
Soc. Sci. 2020, 9, 202
Taniguchi, Travis A., Jerry H. Ratcliffe, and Ralph B. Taylor. 2011. Gang set space, drug markets, and crime around
drug corners in Camden. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency 48: 327–63. [CrossRef]
Tita, George E., Jacqueline Cohen, and John Engberg. 2005. An ecological study of the location of gang “set space”.
Social Problems 52: 272–99. [CrossRef]
Topalli, Volkan, Richard Wright, and Robert Fornango. 2002. Drug dealers, robbery and retaliation: Vulnerability,
deterrence and the contagion of violence. British Journal of Criminology 42: 337–51. [CrossRef]
Trangenstein, Pamela J., Frank C. Curriero, Daniel Webster, Jacky M. Jennings, Carl Latkin, Raimee Eck, and David
H. Jernigan. 2018. Outlet type, access to alcohol, and violent crime. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Research 42: 2234–45. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Valasik, Matthew. 2018. Gang violence predictability: Using risk terrain modeling to study gang homicides and
gang assaults in East Los Angeles. Journal of Criminal Justice 58: 10–21. [CrossRef]
Valasik, Matthew, and George Tita. 2018. Gangs and space. In The Oxford Handbook of Environmental Criminology.
Edited by Gerben Bruinsma and Shane Johnson. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [CrossRef]
Weisburd, David, Lorraine Green, Frank Gajewski, and Charlie Bellucci. 1994. Defining the street-level drug
market. In Drugs and Crime: Evaluating Public Policy Initiatives. Edited by D. L. MacKenzie and C. D. Uchida.
Thousand Oaks: Sage, pp. 61–76.
Weisburd, David, Elizabeth R. Groff, and Sue-Ming Yang. 2010. Understanding Developmental Crime Trajectories at
Places: Social Disorganization and Opportunity Perspectives at Micro Units of Geography; Washington: National
Institute of Justice.
Wheeler, Andrew P. 2018. The effect of 311 calls for service on crime in DC at microplaces. Crime & Delinquency 64:
1882–903. [CrossRef]
Whelan, Aubrey. 2020. Philly’s Overdose Deaths Rose again in 2019, Especially in Black and Latino Communities.
Philadelphia Inquirer. May 13. Available online: https://www.inquirer.com/health/opioid-addiction/philadelp
hia-overdose-deaths-2019-rise-20200513.html (accessed on 29 September 2020).
Whyte, William F. 1955. Street Corner Society: The Social Structure of an Italian Slum. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Wolfram, Joel. 2017. Philly Briefs Neighborhood on Cleanup of Conrail Heroin Encampment. WHYY, June 20.
Available online: https://whyy.org/articles/philly-briefs-neighborhood-on-cleanup-of-conrail-heroin-enca
mpment (accessed on 29 September 2020).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional
affiliations.
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
177
MDPI
St. Alban-Anlage 66
4052 Basel
Switzerland
Tel. +41 61 683 77 34
Fax +41 61 302 89 18
www.mdpi.com
Social Sciences Editorial Office
E-mail:
[email protected]
www.mdpi.com/journal/socsci
MDPI
St. Alban-Anlage 66
4052 Basel
Switzerland
Tel: +41 61 683 77 34
Fax: +41 61 302 89 18
www.mdpi.com
ISBN 978-3-0365-1533-5