Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15 – 27
www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho
Symptom occurrence in persons with chronic
fatigue syndrome
L.A. Jason *, S.R. Torres-Harding, A.W. Carrico, R.R. Taylor
DePaul Uni6ersity, Center for Community Research, 990 West Fullerton Road, Chicago, IL 60614, USA
Received 19 April 2001; accepted 3 July 2001
Abstract
This investigation compared differences in the occurrence of symptoms in participants with
CFS, melancholic depression, and no fatigue (controls). The following Fukuda et al. [Ann.
Intern. Med. 121 (1994) 953] criteria symptoms differentiated the CFS group from controls,
but did not differentiate the melancholic depression group from controls: headaches, lymph
node pain, sore throat, joint pain, and muscle pain. In addition, participants with CFS
uniquely differed from controls in the occurrence of muscle weakness at multiple sites as well
as in the occurrence of various cardiopulmonary, neurological, and other symptoms not
currently included in the current case definition. Implications of these findings are discussed.
© 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: CFS; Symptoms; Diagnostic criteria
1. Introduction
Chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) remains a poorly understood and controversial
disease, because the exact causal agents are unknown, physical signs and symptoms
are variant, and diagnostic laboratory tests have poor sensitivity and specificity
(Holmes, 1991; Jason et al., 1995). In the absence of laboratory tests or other
objective indicators, case identification of CFS relies upon the clinical assessment of
a constellation of symptoms that have been present for 6 or more months since the
onset of the fatiguing illness (Fukuda et al., 1994). Since its emergence as a new
* Corresponding author. Tel.: + 1-773-325-2018; fax: + 1-773-325-4923.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (L.A. Jason).
0301-0511/02/$ - see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S0301-0511(01)00120-X
16
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
disease category in the 1980s, four definitions of CFS have been proposed, but
none have been empirically derived (Jason et al., 1997).
The current US case definition of CFS (Fukuda et al., 1994) requires that the
following criteria be met for diagnosis: (a) 6 or more months of persistent or
relapsing chronic fatigue of a new or definite onset that is neither the result of
ongoing exertion nor alleviated by rest, which results in substantial reductions in
previous levels of occupational, educational, social, or personal activities; and (b)
the concurrent occurrence of at least four of eight symptoms (postexertional
malaise, unrefreshing sleep, memory and concentration difficulties, new
headaches, sore throat, lymph node pain, muscle pain, and joint pain) that
persist or reoccur during 6 or more months of the illness and do not predate the
fatigue.
Researchers have sought to validate the criteria for CFS established by the
CDC using factor analytic methods. Nisenbaum et al. (1998) found that three
correlated factors (fatigue-mood-cognition symptoms, flu-type symptoms, and visual impairment symptoms) explained a set of additional correlations between
fatigue lasting for 6 or more months and 14 inter-related symptoms. No factor
explained observed correlations among fatigue lasting for 1–5 months and other
symptoms, indicating that only fatigue lasting 6 or more months (with selected
symptoms) overlaps with published criteria to define CFS. In another study,
Friedberg et al. (2000) examined symptoms of patients with CFS who had an
illness duration of 10 or more years and found three factors: cognitive problems,
flu-like symptoms, and neurologic symptoms.
Other research has focused on classifying persons with CFS based on symptom profiles. Using latent class analysis, Hadzi-Pavlovic et al. (2000) determined
that patients with CFS could be grouped into three classes: those with multiple
severe symptoms, those with lower rates of cognitive symptoms and higher rates
of pain; and those with a less severe form of multiple symptoms. Participants
with a less severe form of multiple symptoms tended to be younger and with
shorter illness duration. Jason and Taylor (2002) performed a cluster analysis of
persons in a community-based sample of persons with chronic fatigue (fatigue
lasting 6 or more months) to define a typology of chronic fatigue symptomatology. Among the participants with CFS, findings suggested that a majority of
individuals with moderate to severe symptoms could be classified into two important subgroups: one distinguished by severe postexertional malaise with fatigue that was partially alleviated by rest; and one distinguished by severe overall
symptomatology, severe postexertional malaise, and fatigue that was not alleviated by rest.
Researchers have also examined the occurrence of specific symptoms reported
by persons with chronic fatigue and CFS (Hartz et al., 1998; Komaroff et al.,
1996). Komaroff et al. (1996) examined the occurrence of minor symptoms
(Holmes et al., 1988), as well as respiratory, gastrointestinal, neurologic, rheumatologic, cardiac, and miscellaneous objective and subjective symptoms that were
not included in the 1988 case definition. The occurrence of these symptoms were
compared among persons with severe, disabling fatigue lasting for 6 or more
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
17
months, persons with multiple sclerosis, persons with major depression, and
healthy controls. Komaroff et al. (1996) concluded that rheumatologic and gastrointestinal symptoms were found more frequently in patients meeting the major
criteria. Based upon these findings, researchers recommended adding anorexia
and nausea as well as eliminating the symptoms of muscle weakness, arthralgias,
and sleep disturbance to strengthen the case definition. Finally, Hartz et al.
(1998) examined the association between the number and severity of symptoms
of CFS in persons with idiopathic chronic fatigue and determined that persons
with fatigue could be classified by the degree to which they match the case
definition of CFS (Fukuda et al., 1994). In addition, Hartz et al. (1998) suggested including symptoms such as frequent fever and chills, muscle weakness,
and sensitivity to alcohol in the current US case definition.
The occurrence of neurally mediated hypotension (NMH) has also been investigated in persons with CFS. NMH is defined as a 30 mmHg drop in systolic (or
a 15 mmHg drop in diastolic) blood pressure in response to an orthostatic
challenge such as standing upright (Rowe and Calkins, 1998). This precipitous
drop in blood pressure is thought to be due to low blood volume (Streeten and
Bell, 1998) or excessive venous pooling in the extremities (Stewart et al., 1999;
Stewart and Weldon, 2000). Symptoms of NMH include but are not limited to:
lightheadedness, dizziness when standing, nausea, fatigue, tremors, breathing or
swallowing difficulties, headaches, visual disturbances, and pallor (Streeten et al.,
2000). While the frequency of NMH in persons with CFS has not been consistently reported across investigations, cardiopulmonary and neurological abnormalities are heterogeneous and common (Wilke et al., 1998).
The findings reviewed herein suggest that other symptoms in addition to the
eight symptoms listed as part of the definitional criteria may be important and
occur frequently in persons with CFS. The present investigation examined the
occurrence of symptoms in the Fukuda et al. (1994) case definition of CFS to
determine whether these symptoms uniquely differentiated those with CFS from
controls. In addition, other fatigue/weakness related, sleep related, neuropsychiatric, infectious, rheumatological, cardiopulmonary, gastrointestinal, neurological,
and reproductive symptoms not specified in the current US case definition were
examined. The occurrence of these additional symptoms was examined to determine whether other symptoms occur with greater frequency in persons with CFS
when compared to controls, as well as what symptoms occurred with greater
frequency in the melancholic depression group compared to controls.
2. Methods
2.1. Procedure
The data are derived from a larger community-based study of the prevalence
of chronic fatigue syndrome (for more details of this study see Jason et al.,
1999). This larger study was carried out in three stages. Stage 1 involved admin-
18
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
istering an initial telephone screening questionnaire in order to identify the
symptoms of chronic fatigue syndrome. Stage 2 involved administering a semistructured psychiatric interview. In stage 3, participants underwent a complete
physical examination. Following the completion of the medical evaluation, four
physicians and a psychiatrist were responsible for making a final diagnosis with
two physicians independently rating each case using the current US case definition of CFS (Fukuda et al., 1994). Where disagreement occurred, a third physician rater was used.
2.2. Sample
Procedures developed by Kish (1965) were used to select one adult from each
household. Birth dates for each adult were gathered, and the person with the
most recent birthday was selected for interview. A stratified random sample of
several neighborhoods in Chicago was utilized (see Jason et al., 1999 for more
details). In stage 1, 28,673 residential/working telephone numbers were contacted,
and 18,675 adults completed the initial screening interview (65.1% completion
rate).
The stage 1 screen revealed that of the 18,765 participants who were interviewed, 780 (4.2%) had chronic fatigue. Of these, 408 had chronic fatigue and
the concurrent occurrence of four or more symptoms. These participants were
defined as CFS-like (the suffix ‘like’ was used to clarify that individuals in this
group only met the Fukuda et al. (1994) criteria by self-report, and did not
necessarily qualify as having a final diagnosis of CFS rendered by a physician).
One hundred and sixty-six of the 408 CFS-like participants agreed to complete
a structured psychiatric interview and a comprehensive physical examination.
There were no significant differences on sociodemographic (i.e. gender, ethnic
identification, age, occupation, education, and marital status) or fatigue scores
between these 166 screened positive (CFS-like) participants and the 242 screened
positive (CFS-like) non-participants. The control group was composed of 199
individuals selected randomly from the remaining 18,260 screened negatives
(seven cases were excluded due to missing data). Of these 199 individuals, 47
completed medical evaluations. There were no sociodemographic differences (i.e.
gender, ethnic identification, age, occupation, education, and marital status) or
fatigue scores between the 152 screened negative non-participants and 47
screened negative participants. Participants were then classified by independent
physician consensus.
2.3. Participants
The present investigation examined the occurrence of symptoms in three
groups of participants. The first group consisted of 32 persons from the larger
group of 166 persons with CFS-like symptoms who were diagnosed with CFS by
the independent physician review panel (CFS group). The second group consisted
of 19 individuals with melancholic depression, who were taken from a larger
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
19
group of 33 CFS-like persons with a psychiatric explanation for their chronic
fatigue illness (Melancholic Depression group). Melancholic Depression was diagnosed using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID) (Spitzer et
al., 1995), which is a valid and reliable semi-structured interview guide that
approximates a traditional psychiatric interview. Melancholic Depression is
defined by either the absence of pleasure in almost all activities or lack of
reactivity to usually pleasurable stimuli. In addition, there would need to be
three or more other symptoms, such as excessive guilt, significant weight loss,
and marked psychomotor retardation or agitation. The control group consisted
of 47 randomly selected individuals who screened negative for having a CFS-like
illness (control group). Three participants who initially screened negative for a
CFS-like illness were excluded from the control group following examination by
the study physicians who determined that they had idiopathic chronic fatigue or
chronic fatigue explained by a psychiatric condition.
2.4. Measures
2.4.1. Medical questionnaire
As part of a detailed medical questionnaire, participants were asked if they
were experiencing or had experienced each of the eight Fukuda et al. (1994)
symptoms of CFS in the past 6 months. Additional symptoms examined included other medical symptoms and neuropsychiatric symptoms that incorporated questions from a measure used by Komaroff et al. (1996). The definitional
symptoms and the additional symptoms were then classified into the following
categories: weakness/fatigue, disturbed sleep, neuropsychiatric, infectious,
rheumatological, cardiopulmonary, neurological, and reproductive abnormalities.
We examined the occurrence of Fukuda et al. (1994) symptoms in the past 6
months because it was most comparable to available data on the occurrence of
other symptoms examined in the medical questionnaire. Also, examining the
occurrence of Fukuda et al. (1994) defined symptoms in the past 6 months
allows results of the present investigation to be compared to those of Komaroff
et al. (1996).
2.5. Statistical analyses
First, the sociodemographic variables of gender, age, ethnicity, marital status,
parental status, work status, and socioeconomic status were examined, using
chi-squares across the three groups: CFS, melancholic depression, and controls.
The occurrence of symptoms was compared between the CFS and control
groups, and between the melancholic depression and control groups. Using binomial logistic regression, sociodemographic variables that were found to be significant between the groups were entered as predictors in the logistic regression
model to control for the effects of these variables on the occurrence of symptoms. We first made the CFS group the referent group, and have compared it to
the controls and the depression group. We next made the control group as a
20
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
referent group to compare it with the depressed group. Due to the numerous
comparisons made, the overall statistical significance was set at P B0.01 for all
analyses in order to minimize the likelihood of type I error.
3. Results
3.1. Sociodemographic 6ariables
Analyses indicated that there were significantly more men in the control group
than in the Melancholic Depression group [x2 (2,95) =11.297, P B0.01]. Furthermore, participants with CFS were significantly more likely to have children than
controls [x2 (2,95)= 9.431, P B0.05]. Thus, gender and parental status were entered as predictors in the subsequent binomial logistic regression analyses of
symptom occurrence.
3.2. Symptoms
Table 1 presents symptoms of Fatigue/Weakness, Disturbed Sleep, Neuropsychiatric, Infectious, and Rheumatological symptoms, and these are the symptom
categories in which are located the current Fukuda et al. (1994) CFS symptom
criteria. Table 2 lists other categories (Cardiopulmonary, Gastrointestinal, Neurological, and Reproductive) that consist of symptoms that are not found within
the Fukuda et al. (1994) criteria.
3.2.1. Fatigue/weakness
Participants with CFS differed significantly from controls in the occurrence of
generalized muscle weakness, and more specific weakness in their legs, arms,
neck, and back. Weak legs was the most frequently reported form of weakness
for the CFS group. Also, participants with CFS and those with melancholic
depression both reported significantly more postexertional malaise than controls.
3.2.2. Disturbed sleep
The occurrence of unrefreshing sleep was reported with significantly greater
frequency in both the CFS and melancholic depression groups compared to
controls. Those in the CFS group had significantly more insomnia than the
controls.
3.2.3. Neuropsychiatric
Participants with CFS reported the occurrence of new headaches with significantly greater frequency than controls. Both the CFS and melancholic depression
groups reported the occurrence of memory and concentration difficulties, depression, and irritability with significantly greater frequency than controls.
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
21
Table 1
Occurrence of symptoms in Fukuda Symptom Categories
CFS
No
Fatigue
(N = 44)
%
Significance
(N= 32)
%
Melancholic
Depression
(N = 19)
%
Fatigue/weakness
General muscle weakness
Postexertional malaise
Legs weak
Arms weak
Neck weak
Shoulders weak
Back weak
Head weak
Abdomen weak
Buttocks weak
Other weak muscles
75.0a
68.8a
62.5a
53.1a
40.6a
40.6
40.6a
25.0
9.4
3.1
3.1
47.4
47.4b
26.3
26.3
21.1
21.1
15.8
5.3
5.3
0.0
15.8
18.2
13.6
9.1
11.4
4.5
6.8
9.1
2.3
0.0
0.0
0.0
**
**
**
**
**
Disturbed sleep
Unrefreshing sleep
Insomnia
Early morning awakening
Trouble staying asleep
Hypersomnia
90.6a
59.4a
59.4
43.8
31.3
84.2b
36.8
47.4
21.1
42.1
25.0
15.9
25.0
11.4
22.7
**
**
Neuropsychiatric
Memory and concentration
New headaches
Depression
Irritability
Disturbances in eyesight
87.5a
75.0a
56.3a
50.0a
46.9
63.2b
68.4
73.7b
42.1b
26.3
20.5
31.8
13.6
11.4
11.4
**
**
**
**
Infectious
Sore throat
Lymph pain
Fever/chills
Oral herpes
Shingles
Oral thrush
Genital herpes
62.5a
40.6a
21.9
6.3
3.1
3.1
0.0
42.1
21.1
15.8
21.1
0.0
5.3
15.8
29.5
9.1
9.1
9.1
4.5
0.0
0.0
**
**
Rheumatological
Muscle pain
Joint pain
Morning stiffness
Stiff after sitting
Hay fever
Puffy face
Dry mouth
Night sweats
Sinus infection
Earaches
Dry eyes
Eye pain
Jaw pain
84.4a
56.3a
56.3a
56.3
46.9a
40.6
37.5
34.4
31.3
25.0
21.9
21.9
18.8
52.6
26.3
36.8
31.6
42.1
21.1
57.9b
15.8
21.1
31.6
15.8
5.3
10.5
22.7
20.5
15.9
25.0
13.6
11.4
11.4
0.0
9.1
6.8
9.1
9.1
0.0
**
**
**
**
**
**
Italics indicate symptoms that are part of the current CFS case definition (Fukuda et al., 1994).
a
Statistically significant difference between CFS and No Fatigue groups.
b
Statistically significant difference between Melancholic Depression and No Fatigue groups.
** Using binomial logistic regression analyses, difference is statistically significant at the PB0.01 level.
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
22
Table 2
Occurrence of symptoms in Non-Fukuda Symptom Categories
CFS
No
fatigue
(N = 44)
%
Significance
(N = 32)
%
Melancholic
depression
(N = 19)
%
Cardiopulmonary
Shortness of breath
Chest pains
Rapid heartbeat
Cough
Heartbeat in ears
Raynaud’ phenomenon
65.6a
40.6a
34.4
28.1a
18.8
9.4
26.3c
10.5
15.8
26.3
21.1
10.5
22.7
6.8
9.1
2.3
2.3
2.3
**
**
Gastrointestinal
Bloating
Lower abdominal pain
Upper abdominal pain
Anorexia
Nausea
Diarrhea
Black bowel movement
Blood in bowel movement
40.6
37.5
31.3
28.1
25.0
12.5
9.4
9.4
26.3
21.1
21.1
15.8
21.1
5.3
5.3
15.8
9.1
9.1
9.1
9.1
0.0
6.8
0.0
2.3
Neurological
Dizzy after standing
Skin sensations
General dizziness
Alcohol intolerance
Dizzy moving head
Unsteady upright
Ringing in ears
Tingling sensations
Paralysis
Temporary blindness
46.9a
46.9a
43.8a
43.8a
37.5a
28.1
21.9
15.6
3.1
6.3
21.1
21.1
36.8
15.8
15.8
21.1
31.6
5.3
0.0
0.0
9.1
9.1
2.3
6.8
6.8
6.8
11.4
0.0
0.0
0.0
**
**
**
**
**
Reproducti6e
Decreased sexual interest
Impairment of sexual functioning
Nocturia
Irregular periodsd
Incontinence
Menopaused
Vaginal discharged
Excessive menstrual bleedingd
Recurrent urinary infections
Recurrent vaginal infectionsd
Dysuria
Nipple discharged
Positive pap smeard
Blood in urine
50.0a
50.0a
43.8
37.5
21.9
20.8
20.8
16.7
12.5
6.3
6.3
4.2
0.0
0.0
15.8c
36.8b
31.6
52.9
10.5
11.8
35.3
17.6
5.3
10.5
0.0
5.9
0.0
0.0
6.8
2.3
13.6
61.9
6.8
23.8
19.0
0.0
2.3
2.3
0.0
0.0
4.8
0.0
**
**
a
**
Statistically significant difference between CFS and No Fatigue groups.
Statistically significant difference between Melancholic Depression and No Fatigue groups.
c
Statistically significant difference between the CFS and Melancholic depression groups.
d
Women only.
** Using binomial logistic regression analyses, difference is statistically significant at the PB0.01 level.
b
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
23
3.2.4. Infectious
The occurrence of sore throats and lymph node pain was significantly greater in
the CFS group when compared to controls. There were no significant differences
between the melancholic depression and control groups in the occurrence of other
infectious symptoms.
3.2.5. Rheumatological
The CFS group reported muscle pain, joint pain, morning stiffness, and hay
fever with significantly greater frequency than controls. Participants with melancholic depression only reported the occurrence of dry mouth with significantly
greater frequency than controls.
3.2.6. Cardiopulmonary
The CFS group reported the occurrence of chest pain and cough with significantly greater frequency than controls. The CFS group and melancholic depression group significantly differed on shortness of breath.
3.2.7. Gastrointestinal
Neither the CFS nor the melancholic depression groups reported gastrointestinal symptoms with significantly greater frequency than controls.
3.2.8. Neurological
The CFS group reported dizziness after standing, skin sensations, general
dizziness, alcohol intolerance and dizzy moving head with significantly greater
frequency than controls.
3.2.9. Reproducti6e
Participants with CFS reported the occurrence of decreased sexual interest
with significantly greater frequency than controls and those with melancholic
depression. The occurrence of impairment of sexual functioning was reported
with significantly greater frequency in both the CFS and melancholic depression
groups when compared to controls.
4. Discussion
Results of this investigation lend support to the importance of the CFS diagnostic criteria (Fukuda et al., 1994). Participants with CFS more frequently
experienced the Fukuda et al. (1994) CFS symptoms when compared to other
symptoms in their respective categories, with the exception of postexertional
malaise. These findings were anticipated, given the selection criteria of four of
eight symptoms to receive a diagnosis of CFS. In addition, the CFS group in
comparison to controls reported significantly higher frequencies of all eight
24
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
Fukuda et al. (1994) definitional symptoms. Furthermore, the occurrence of postexertional malaise, cognitive and memory difficulties and unrefreshing sleep did
not uniquely discriminate between the CFS and control groups, as individuals in
the melancholic depression group also experienced these symptoms with significantly greater frequency than controls.
Interpreting these findings, it is important to note that the present investigation examined only the occurrence of symptoms. The CFS group may also
uniquely differ from controls in symptom duration and severity. For example,
Jason et al. (2000) found that examining symptoms utilizing severity criteria has
proven useful in uniquely differentiating CFS from fatigue explained by a psychiatric disorder (Jason et al., 2000). Also, the frequency of the occurrence of the
Fukuda et al. (1994) defined symptoms in the melancholic depression group may
have been elevated due to the screening process. Only chronically fatigued participants initially reporting four or more symptoms specified in the current US case
definition of CFS were classified as ‘CFS-like’ and subsequently received a medical examination.
Cardiopulmonary and neurological abnormalities have been investigated as
they relate to neurally mediated hypotension (NMH) in persons with CFS (Rowe
and Calkins, 1998; Streeten et al., 2000; Wilke et al., 1998). NMH occurs when
the central nervous system misinterprets the body’s needs when it is in an upright position, then sends a message to the heart to slow down and lower the
blood pressure, responses that are directly opposite to the responses the body‘s
needs. Several cardiopulmonary and neurological symptoms in the present investigation occurred with higher frequency and uniquely differentiated the CFS
group from controls. Shortness of breath, chest pain, dizziness after standing,
skin sensations, general dizziness, dizzy moving the head, and alcohol intolerance
uniquely differentiate those with CFS from controls. It is possible that examining
these cardiopulmonary and neurological abnormalities not currently assessed by
the current case definition (Fukuda et al., 1994) could provide greater diagnostic
reliability.
The CFS and melancholic depression group only significantly differed for two
symptoms: decreased sexual interest and shortness of breath. Decreased sexual
interest might very well be a result of low levels of cortisol. A deficit of cortisol
has been found in patients with CFS, and is linked to lethargy and fatigue, and
this deficit might be contributing to an overactive immune system (Scott and
Dinan, 1999). Shortness of breath as well as other neurological symptoms might
reflect a finding that has been confirmed by other investigators, who have not
found neurally mediated hypotension to play a major role in CFS (Poole et al.,
2000).
Komaroff et al. (1996) have suggested that eliminating the symptoms of muscle weakness, arthralgias, and sleep disturbance would provide greater sensitivity
and specificity in CFS diagnosis. In contrast, the present investigation found that
muscle weakness and arthralgias were reported in over half of participants with
CFS and uniquely differentiated this group from controls. Regarding sleep dis-
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
25
turbance, results of the present investigation did not support the ability of any
symptoms in this category to uniquely discriminate between CFS and control
groups. Komaroff et al. (1996) also suggested adding anorexia and nausea as
minor symptoms in the CFS case definition. However, in the present study, both
occurred with relatively low frequency and neither uniquely differentiated those
with CFS from controls.
Hartz et al. (1998) also investigated the occurrence of symptoms in persons
with fatigue, and recommended the inclusion of fever and chills, muscle weakness, and sensitivity to alcohol as CFS case definition symptoms. Results of the
current investigation also indicated that muscle weakness and sensitivity to alcohol uniquely differentiated the CFS group from controls, but neither the CFS
nor the melancholic depression groups significantly differed from controls in the
occurrence of fever and chills. Furthermore, it appeared that muscle weakness in
the CFS group occurred at multiple sites, with weak legs being the most frequently reported form of weakness. These findings concur with those of Hartz et
al. (1998), and therefore provide further support for the inclusion of muscle
weakness in the case definition of CFS.
Differences between the present investigation and previous work could be a
result of the sampling methods employed. Other research has relied on predominantly clinic-based samples that may report greater occurrence and severity of
symptoms, especially those of a viral or infectious nature (Wessely, 1998). Persons with CFS found in clinic-based samples may represent a more severely ill
population. Current findings should be interpreted within the context of limitations on statistical power imposed by a small sample size. Small sample sizes
might have precluded the detection of significant differences between groups, and
this may be especially true for all comparisons with the depressed group because
they have the smallest sample size and because they appear to be mid-way on
most measures between the CFS group and controls. Because some differences
between groups may have not been detected, more research with larger samples
is necessary to replicate these results.
In summary, results from this investigation in a community-based sample support the use of the symptoms included in the current CFS case definition
(Fukuda, et al., 1994). Furthermore, this investigation found that weakness and
various neurological, cardiopulmonary, neuropsychiatric, and rheumatological
symptoms uniquely differentiated persons with CFS from the control group. In
contrast, relatively few of these symptoms significantly differentiated the group
of individuals with melancholic depression from the control group.
Acknowledgements
Financial support for this study was provided by NIAID grant number
AI36295. Requests for reprints should be sent to Leonard Jason, Ph.D., Center
for Community Research, DePaul University, 990 W. Fullerton Ave. Chicago,
IL 60614.
26
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
References
Friedberg, F., Dechene, L., McKenzie, M.J.I., Fontanetta, R., 2000. Symptoms patterns in long-duration chronic fatigue syndrome. J. Psychosom. Res. 38, 383 – 392.
Fukuda, K., Straus, S.E., Hickie, I., Sharpe, M.C., Dobbins, J.G., Komaroff, A., 1994. The chronic
fatigue syndrome: A comprehensive approach to its definition and study. Ann. Intern. Med. 121,
953 – 959.
Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Hickie, I.B., Wilson, A.J., Davenport, T.A., Lloyd, A.R., Wakefield, D., 2000.
Screening for prolonged fatigue syndromes: Validation of the SOFA scale. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. 35 (10), 471 – 479.
Hartz, A.J., Kuhn, E.M., Levine, P.H., London, R., 1998. Characteristics of fatigued persons associated with features of chronic fatigue syndrome. J. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome 4 (3), 71 – 97.
Holmes, G.P., 1991. Defining chronic fatigue syndrome. Rev. Infect. Dis. 13, S53 – 55.
Holmes, G.P., Kaplan, J.E., Gantz, N.M., Komaroff, A.L., Schonberger, L.B., Strauss, S.S., et al.,
1988. Chronic fatigue syndrome: A working case definition. Ann. Intern. Med. 108, 387 – 389.
Jason, L.A., King, C.P., Taylor, R.R., Kennedy, C., 2000. Defining chronic fatigue syndrome:
Methodological challenges. J. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome 7 (3), 17 – 32.
Jason, L.A., Richman, J.A., Friedberg, F., Wagner, L., Taylor, R., Jordan, K.M., 1997. Politics,
science, and the emergence of a new disease: The case of chronic fatigue syndrome. Am. Psychol.
52, S60 – S67.
Jason, L.A., Richman, J.A., Rademaker, A.W., Jordan, K.M., Plioplys, A.V., Taylor, R.R., et al.,
1999. Arch. Intern. Med. 159 (18), 2129 –2137.
Jason, L.A., Taylor, R.R., 2002. Applying cluster analysis to define a typology of chronic fatigue
syndrome in a medically-evaluated random community sample. Psychol and Health (in press).
Jason, L.A., Wagner, L., Taylor, R., Ropacki, M.T., Shlaes, J., Ferrari, J.R., et al., 1995. Chronic
fatigue syndrome: A new challenge for health care professionals. J. Commun. Psychol. 23, 143 –
164.
Kish, L., 1965. Survey Sampling. Wiley, NY.
Komaroff, A.L., Fagioli, L.R., Geiger, A.M., Doolitle, T.H., Lee, J., Kornish, J., et al., 1996. An
examination of the working case definition of chronic fatigue syndrome. Am. J. Med. 100, 56 – 64.
Nisenbaum, R., Reyes, M., Mawle, A.C., Reeves, W.C., 1998. Factor analysis of unexplained severe
fatigue and interrelated symptoms: Overlap with the criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome. Am. J.
Epidemiol. 148 (1), 72 – 77.
Poole, J., Herrell, R., Ashton, S., Goldberg, J., Buchwald, D., 2000. Results of isoproterenol tilt
table testing in monozygotic twins discordant for chronic fatigue syndrome. Arch. Intern. Med.
160, 3461 – 3468.
Rowe, P.C., Calkins, H., 1998. Neurally mediated hypotension and chronic fatigue syndrome. Am. J.
Med. 105 (3A), 15S –21.
Scott, L.V., Dinan, T.G., 1999. The neuroendocrinology of chronic fatigue syndrome: Focus on the
hypothalamic– pituitary – adrenal axis. Funct. Neurol. 14 (1), 3 – 11.
Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B.W., Gibbon, M., First, M.B., 1995. Structured Clinical Interview for
DSM-IV. SCID-NP, Version 2.0, Non-Patient edition. American Psychiatric Press, Washington
DC.
Stewart, J.M., Gewitz, M.H., Weldon, A, Munoz, J., 1999. Patterns of orthostatic intolerance: The
orthostatic tachycardia syndrome and adolescent chronic fatigue. J. Pediatr. 135 (2), 218 –225.
Stewart, J.M., Weldon, A., 2000. Vascular perturbations in the chronic orthostatic intolerance of the
postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome. J. Appl. Physiol. 89 (4), 1502 – 1505.
Streeten, D.H., Bell, D.S., 1998. Circulating blood volume in chronic fatigue syndrome. J. Chronic
Fatigue Syndrome 4 (3), 3 –11.
Streeten, D.H.P., Thomas, D., Bell, D.S., 2000. The roles of orthostatic intolerance, orthostatic
tachycardia, and subnormal erythrocyte volume in the pathogenesis of chronic fatigue syndrome.
Am. J. Med. Sci. 320 (1), 1 – 8.
L.A. Jason et al. / Biological Psychology 59 (2002) 15–27
27
Wessely, S., 1998. The eipdemiology of chronic fatigue syndrome. Epidemiol. Psyichiatria Soc. 7 (1),
10 – 24.
Wilke, W.S., Fouad-Tarazi, F.M., Cash, J.M., Calabrese, L.H., 1998. The connections between
chronic fatigue syndrome and neurally mediated hypotension. Cleveland Clin. J. 65 (5), 261 – 266.