Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
…
7 pages
1 file
This paper explores the Proto-Indo-European (PIE) verbal endings, focusing on their structural variations across different pronouns and tenses, particularly in the context of nominative and ergative case markings. The analysis discusses the evolution of these verbal endings, examining irregularities like depalatalizations and delabializations, and highlights the implications of these endings in comparative linguistics. The research contributes to understanding the historical development of verbal systems in Indo-European languages.
Proto-Indo-European (PIE) inherited two conjugational systems, usually referred to as the miconjugation and the hi-conjugation, after the endings of the respective first person present forms in Hittite.
Repanšek, Luka, Harald Bichlmeier & Velizar Sadovski (eds.), Vácām̐si miśrā krṇavāmahai. Proceedings of the international conference of the Society for Indo-European Studies and IWoBA XII, Ljubljana 4–7 June 2019. Hamburg: Baar-Verlag. 255-284., 2020
We argue that in PIE the so-called thematic present stems inflected in the same way as the so-called athematic ones. In the singular, the difference between the “primary” forms of the indicative mood and their “secondary” injunctive mood counterparts was marked solely by PIE *-i attached to the respective verb form. This means that this part of the PIE conjugation system was nearly identical with what is attested in the ancient Indo-Iranian languages such as Sanskrit. As for the deviating “primary” inflectional forms of thematic presents in the languages of Europe – such as 2sg. Gk φέρεις, Lith vedì, 3sg. Gk φέρει – , such forms can plausibly be explained within the individual history of the IE languages in question. Such explanations become available if one takes into consideration (a) the sound changes known to have once operated in these languages, (b) the possibility of a recent univerbation of finite verbs with sentence particles and other clitics. Since PIE, as any natural language, also had a prehistory, remaining anomalies in the inflection of thematic verbs may find an explanation within its phonological history.
Natalia Bolatti Guzzo and Piotr Taracha (eds.), “And I Knew Twelve Languages”: A Tribute to Massimo Poetto on the Occasion of His 70th Birthday. Warsaw: Agade, 2019
Based on the evidence of Anatolian and Tocharian, the 2pl. middle ending must be reconstructed for PIE not as *-dhwe, but as *-dhh2we.
This monograph attempts a new historical and comparative analysis of Old English (OE) preterite-present verbs. Preterite-present verbs show morphological peculiarities: their present singular typically exhibits the o-grade radical vocalism, to conform with the preterite singular of a strong verb, whilst their preterite is augmented with a dental suffix, which accords with the preterite formation of a weak verb. Traditionally, English and Germanic philologists have construed these characteristics as the result of an original o-grade perfect having been reinterpreted as the new present, along with the suppression of the original e-grade present, and of the Germanic (Gmc.) dental or weak preterite having been newly adopted for the preterite formation; this standpoint may be labelled the ‘strong verb origin’ theory. The present work calls this view into question by focusing on the difficulties inherent in this conventional approach. Authentic Indo-European comparative linguistic studies have considered that (the present tense formations of) the OE or Gmc. preterite-present verbs are reflexes of the archaic Proto-Indo-European (PIE) stative perfects, though the dental preterites are an independently Germanic innovation. Whilst this understanding, which may be dubbed the ‘stative perfect origin’ theory, seems to provide a far better explanation than the ‘strong verb origin’ theory, there also remain several significant issues to be resolved. First, how did the Gmc. preterite-present verbs lose their original reduplication if they go back to the PIE perfect? Second, does the Indo-European comparative evidence guarantee that all the preterite-present verbs unequivocally refer back to a PIE stative perfect alone? Third, how can the third person plural ending *-un in the present tense formation of the Gmc. preterite-present verb be explained, given that the third person plural termination of the PIE perfect should develop into *-ur in Germanic? Fourth, which PIE formation should the peculiar morphology of the infinitive of a Gmc. preterite-present verb reflect? This monograph claims that these important problems are not resolved if we merely assume that the PIE stative perfect continued into the Gmc. preterite-present by losing its original reduplication due to morphological haplology. These matters are interconnected to a remarkable extent, and a systematic account can be offered only if we recognize that the OE or Gmc. preterite-present verbs are in essence a historical product within the Germanic branch, resulting from the morphological conflation of the PIE stative perfect active and a PIE athematic present tense middle formation which can convey a present stative meaning; this perspective may be tagged as the ‘morphological conflation’ theory. This monograph adopts the ‘h2e-conjugation theory’ advocated recently by Jay H. Jasanoff and demonstrates that the same theory, remarkable in the very high level of explanatory power it achieves in treating the origin of the Anatolian ḫi-conjugation verbs, is also effective when attempting to give a historical account of the present tense formation of the OE or Gmc. preterite-present verbs. The core members of the preterite-present group have arisen from what is called a PIE stative-intransitive system within the framework of the h2e-conjugation theory, whilst there are also other preterite-present members which to some extent deviate from this pattern. In this way, the present work focuses on the historical and comparative analysis of the present tense formation of the preterite-present verb; accordingly, the origin and development of the Germanic dental preterite, another important issue concerning preterite-present morphology, is left open for future research.
The discourse potential of underspecified …, 2008
Linguistica
In this paper it is suggested that the original form of the Indo European third person plural verbal suffix was *-(e/o)N and that the nasal element which appears in this suffix was or ginally a deictic particle with 'there and then' signification.
In the talk it is argued that in PIE the so-called thematic present stems originally inflected in the same way as the so-called athematic ones. In the singular, the difference between the " primary " indicative mood inflectional forms and their " secondary " injunctive mood counterparts was marked solely by PIE *-i attached to the relevant verb form. This means that this part of the PIE conjugation system was nearly identical with what is attested in ancient Indo-Iranian. The deviating " primary " inflectional forms in the individual IE languages can be plausibly explained within the individual history of these languages. Such explanations become available as soon as one takes into consideration (a) the laws of final syllables relevant to these particular languages, (b) the possibility of a recent univerbation of finite verbs with sentence particles and other clitics.
Pre-PIE
(labializations) (delabializations) PIE 1 *-mu-átu *-m w ét w *-més w *-més (w) ~ *-mós 2 *-tu-átu *-t w ét w *-t w é *-té 1 *-mu-íku *-m w éx w *-m w éx *-wáh 2 2 *-tu-íku *-t w éx w *-t w éx *-táh 2
Note the irregular delabializations 2 which affected these forms.
When -i was added in the other forms to mark the present tense, *-mesi was no problem (we have it in Indo-Iranian), but the 2 pl. and 1/2 du. forms were apparently not capable of taking it 3 .
So when the non-Anatolian languages dropped the variants *-wén(i) 4 and *-tén(i) 5 , the resulting system may have looked temporarily like this:
There may have been pressure to distinguish primary and secondary endings in the 2 pl. and in the dual, and to distinguish the dual from the plural in the 3 rd person, but no obvious solution was available. However, when the perfect/aorist ending *-me was transferred to the 1 pl. imperfect (and, conversely, active *-té was transferred to the perfect/aorist), a pattern could be abstracted from the new opposition between past *-mé and present *-més 6 . In Italic, Celtic, Balto-Slavic and Armenian (possibly Germanic as well, but we cannot tell), a new 2pl. *-tés was created, and the new dual presents *-wh 2 ás, *-th 2 ás,*-tés 7 even spread to Indo-Iranian.
This gives a late PIE dialectal system (which does not include Anatolian, Tocharian, Greek or Indo-Iranian fully):
The former existence in Balto-Slavic of a contrast between secondary -w , -and primary -was, -tas is proven by the Lithuanian 1-2 pl. endings -and -, created analogically on the model of the dual (-w , -:: -was, -tas; -, -:: -mes, -tes), before the present endings were lost.
In Greek, and in part in Indo-Iranian and Tocharian 8 , the gaps in the system were either allowed to stand (Greek 1-2 pl. without primary/secondary distinction), or were filled instead with endings from the middle (marked in bold):
The stative endings
A completely different set of endings was employed in the "stative", i.e. the Hittite i-conjugation, the PIE perfect and the middle (medio-passive). Again there are two sets of endings, although here the "transitive" marker (*-s in the i-conjugation/perfect/aorist, *-t in the middle) appears in the 3 rd person only.
perfect -aorist 1 *-h 2 e *-h 2 e 2 *-th 2 e *-th 2 e 3 *-e *-s 1 *-me *-me 2 *-e *-e 3 *-ér *-ér-s
The similarities between the three paradigms are obvious. The only difficulties lie, as was the case in the active, in the 1-2 pl. endings.
In the 1 pl., Vedic -ma is (rarely) written -, and more often a long vowel is required by the metre. A reconstruction *-meH has sometimes been proposed. However, -also occurs in the active, and a metric long vowel is also found in the 1 sg. -a and 2 pl. -a. I would derive the long vowel ultimately from active 1 du. -, where the variation is to be expected: -vs. -vas and 1 pl. -ma vs. -mas leads to -va vs. -vas, or, as we also saw in Lith., to -vs. -mas. I see no reason, therefore, to reconstruct anything but *-me for the 1 pl. stative. As we saw above, this ending *-me was later transferred to the 1 pl. active as well (though not in Greek).
In the 2 pl., Vedic -a is weird enough that it must reflect the original ending. The *-te found elsewhere must have been secondarily imported from the active. This ending -a is really strange for a 2 pl., especially considering that the *-e in the stative endings is surely a secondary addition. It does not affect the stress or the Ablaut of the root, and seems to be simply an 'augment' to render the stative endings syllabic, which is of course the reason why it was not added to the already syllabic 3 pl. ending. This means that the 2 pl. stative ending was in fact *-ø, which makes no sense at all. The middle endings, so similar to the stative endings elsewhere, are strangely different in the 1-2 pl. The 1 pl. middle ending is sometimes reconstructed as *-medhh 2 (*-medhə 2 ), based on Gk. -μεθα and Ved. -mahi, which is of course phonetically possible, but does not really look like a plausible verbal ending. I would rather reconstruct *-medh-, with the vowel of the corresponding 1 sg. ending added in Greek (at a stage when the 1 sg. was still *-h 2 a c.q. *-a) and Indo-Iranian.
Communication Reports, 1990
War Books: A Study in Historical Criticism by Jean Norton Cru
Journal of High Energy Physics, 2011
Feminism in Literature: A Gale Critical Companion (6 volume set)Feminism in Literature: A Gale Critical Companion (6 volume set)
Optics Express, 2007
Revista de Estudios Políticos, MADRID, v. 74, n.74, p. 277-296, 1991, 1991
Proceedings of the 20th International Radiocarbon Conference, 2010
PDF FULL WILD by Adrienne Wilder
Ask and It Is Given: Learning to Manifest Your Desires
Voices on Central Asia, 2020
Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 2011
Wind Engineering, 2001
Geografiska Annaler. Series B, Human Geography, 1984
Journal of Immunology, 2022
Academic Psychiatry, 2021
African Journal of Oral Health, 2017
BMC Health Services Research, 2017
Educación Química, 2012
International Journal of Evaluation and Research in Education (IJERE), 2023