Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
Alexander Kozintsev
Museum of Anthropology and Ethnography, Saint-Petersburg
and Saint-Petersburg State University, Russia
This study collates linguistic, genetic, and archaeological data
relevant to the problem of the IE homeland and proto-IE (PIE)
migrations. The idea of a proto-Anatolian (PA) migration from
the steppe to Anatolia via the Balkans is refuted by linguistic,
archaeological, and genetic facts, whereas the alternative
scenario, postulating the Indo-Uralic homeland in the area east of
the Caspian Sea, is the most plausible. The divergence between
proto-Uralians and PIEs is mirrored by the cultural dichotomy
between Kelteminar and the early farming societies in southern
Turkmenia and northern Iran. From their first homeland the
early PIEs moved to their second homeland in the Near East,
where early PIE split into PA and late PIE. Three migration
routes from the Near East to the steppe across the Caucasus can
be tentatively reconstructed — two early (Khvalynsk and
Darkveti-Meshoko), and one later (Maykop). The early eastern
route (Khvalynsk), supported mostly by genetic data, may have
been taken by Indo-Hittites. The western and the central routes
(Darkveti-Meshoko and Maykop), while agreeing with
archaeological and linguistic evidence, suggest that late PIE
could have been adopted by the steppe people without biological
admixture. After that, the steppe became the third and last PIE
homeland, from whence all filial IE dialects except Anatolian
spread in various directions, one of them being to the Balkans
and eventually to Anatolia and the southern Caucasus, thus
closing the circle of counterclockwise IE migrations around the
Black Sea.
1. Introduction
The growth of knowledge about the IE homeland and the
earliest stage of IE history has been quite rapid in recent years.
This mostly concerns genetic data, but the body of linguistic
and archaeological information has substantially increased as
well. Until recently, it appeared evident to many that the steppe
homeland theory had been overwhelmingly supported by the
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
294
Alexander Kozintsev
available data, especially those concerning paleogenomics and
the Yamnaya expansion (Allentoft et al. 2015; Haak et al. 2015;
see Anthony & Ringe 2015 and Anthony & Brown 2017 for
reviews), although serious doubts were expressed (see, e.g.
Heggarty 2018).
New facts on which this study focuses disagree with an
important part of the steppe scenario — one which concerns
early PIEs (Sturtevant’s Indo-Hittites) and Anatolians. The part
concerning the late PIEs and the filial IE groups remains largely
valid but the previously reconstructed picture is now seen as a
fragment of a much broader panorama, in which the steppe is
the last rather than the first setting of what might be termed
the IE prologue. This study addresses only this prologue,
namely PIE history from its beginning — the split of the
hypothetical Indo-Uralic macrofamily to the moment when the
steppe became the homeland of the late PIEs. In short, I will try
to reconstruct the events preceding the first act of the IE
prehistory proper, i.e., those before the Yamnaya expansion. In
so doing, I will sometimes have to proceed in a time machine
fashion, from later (and better documented) stages to earlier
ones.
2. Linguistics
2.1. Lexicostatistics: cognates versus loanwords
2.1.1. The position of IE among other families
In a previous publication (Kozintsev 2018) I used the
pairwise similarity matrix (percentages of matching items in
the preliminary 50-item wordlist) between 12 IE and 29 non-IE
languages representing eight families in the STARLING
lexicostatistical database compiled by George Starostin et al.
(http://starling.rinet.ru/new100/trees.htm, accessed September
18, 2018) to explore the external ties of the Indo-European
family, to examine the affinities within the Indo-Uralic
macrofamily, and to test the theories concerning the non-IE
1
adstrate in PIE.
1
I take this opportunity to once again thank George Starostin, Alexei Kassian,
and Mikhail Zhivlov for permission to use their unpublished preliminary data.
Kassian’s comments were very useful.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
295
In accordance with many preceding studies, the cluster
analysis showed the IE family to be a member of both the
Nostratic macrofamily (Illi-Svity 1971; Dolgopolsky 2008;
Bomhard 2018) and of the Eurasiatic macrofamily (Greenberg
2000, 2002). Apart from the IE family, the resulting cluster
included Uralic, Altaic, Eskaleut, and Chukotko-Kamchatkan.
“Southern” families, which are sometimes included in Nostratic,
such as Kartvelian and Semitic, fall outside this cluster or, if its
boundaries are extended, take a peripheral position within it. In
this regard, the cluster conforms to Eurasiatic as described by
Greenberg rather than to Nostratic as delimited by Illi-Svity
or Dolgopolsky (see Pagel et al. 2013, for a similar result).
Moreover, another statistical technique, based on the
quasi-spatial rather than genealogical approach — the
nonmetric multidimensional scaling and the minimum
spanning tree finding the shortest path between points in space
— shows IE to be markedly intermediate. It is directly
connected with four other families whereas none of the
remaining families is linked with more than two. The most
intriguing connection is that between IE and Eskimo. This
2
strange link was noted long ago (Uhlenbeck 1937). Like the
membership of the Eurasiatic macrofamily, it highlights the
eastern roots of PIE — a caveat against placing the earliest IE
homeland too far west of the Urals, as, e.g., in Anatolia, in the
Balkans, or on the Danube. The second family with which IE is
connected is Uralic. The cluster analysis links these two
families into a single macrofamily within Eurasiatic, in line
with the widespread view (see Kassian et al. 2015 with
discussion, for the recent assessment of the plausibility of IndoUralic).
The remaining two connections of IE, revealed by the
minimum spanning tree, are with “southern” families which do
not belong to Eurasiatic and take a peripheral position within
Nostratic — Kartvelian and Semitic. The latter connection is
especially noteworthy because on the quasi-spatial graph it is a
link in a nearly straight chain-like succession connecting Altaic
with Uralic, Uralic with IE, and IE with Semitic — a sequence
2
Dolgopolsky (2008: 4, 7) and S. Starostin (1999), too, believed that Eskaleut
and Chukchi-Kamchatkan likely belong to the Nostratic macrofamily.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
296
Alexander Kozintsev
that makes sense in terms of geography and the wave model of
3
language change (see Dyen et al. 1992 for a similar model
applied to dialects, languages, and subfamilies within the IE
family).
A similar but more detailed analysis of affinities within
the presumed Indo-Uralic macrofamily demonstrates that
neither the reconstructed Baltic-Finnic language nor the FinnoPermic subfamily in general, whose ties with IE languages have
been especially close due to geographic proximity, are
connected with the IE family. Instead, the shortest edge of the
minimum spanning tree connecting the two families passes
between the reconstructed Ob Ugric and Hittite. The relevance
of this fact to the IE homeland problem will be discussed in
section 5.3.
2.1.2. The Semitic adstrate in PIE
As a next step, I used four quasi-spatial models —
Northwest Caucasian, Northeast Caucasian, Kartvelian, and
Semitic — to test the hypothesis that IE is an Eurasiatic
language, probably closest to Uralic and, unlike the latter,
influenced by a “southern” substrate or adstrate (Uhlenbeck
1937; S. Starostin 1988, 2000; Kortlandt 1990, 2018, 2019;
4
Bomhard 2018: 717; 2019a,b) . IE, Uralic, and a “southern”
family were represented by a triangle in a space generated by
the principal coordinate analysis. The test was based on the
assumption that two sister groups (in our case, IE and Uralic)
should be approximately equally distant from the outgroup
unless one of the sister groups was evolutionarily more
3
No such arrangement is seen in the case of Kartvelian, so in this case, strictly
speaking, IE cannot be regarded as intermediate — a conclusion that is
supported by lexicostatistical modeling (see below). The mean percentage of
matching items with IE is 13.7 for Uralic, 10.2 for Cukchi-Kamchatkan, 8.8 for
Eskaleut, 7.9 for Altaic, 6.6 for Semitic, 6.0 for Kartvelian, 3.4 for Western
Caucasian, and 0.3 for Eastern Caucasian. With regard to the 50-item
wordlist, this corresponds to approximately 7, 5, 4-5, 4, 3-4, 3, 1-2, and 0-1
matching items, respectively.
4
Nikolai Trubezkoy (1939) claimed that proto-IE originally belonged to the
“Mediterranean” Sprachbund, which included North Caucasian, Kartvelian,
and Semitic, but owing to its geographically intermediate position converged
toward the Uralic and Altaic dialects — a view that has been rejected by
virtually all linguists.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
297
conservative than the other or horizontal transfer had taken
place. The null hypothesis would be rejected if the side
connecting IE with a “southern” family was significantly
shorter than that connecting this family with Uralic
(significance was assessed by comparing the variation between
and within the families; see Kozintsev 2018, for details).
In three models out of four (Northwest Caucasian,
Northeast Caucasian, and Kartvelian), the sides differed
5
insignificantly. In the Semitic model, the result was highly
significant: the side connecting IE with Semitic was 5.6 %
shorter than that connecting Uralic and Semitic. The same
result can be obtained by directly comparing frequencies of
lexical matches by means of the non-parametric Mann-Whitney
test (Table 1).
Semitic (n=2)
Northwest Caucasian (n=1)
Northeast Caucasian (n=8)
Kartvelian (n=2)
IE (n=12)
6.6
3.4
0.3
6.0
p
Uralic (n=8) U
3.8
7 0.0004
3.8
38.5 0.46
0.2
34
0.36
7.6
25
0.07
Note: numbers of languages or reconstructions are indicated in
parentheses.
Table 1. Frequencies of lexical matches (%), Mann-Whitney U values,
and probabilities
Because the supposed evolutionary conservatism of IE
compared to Uralic did not show up in other models, the
alternative explanation — horizontal transfer within a
Sprachbund — was accepted, although the 50-item list
5
As shown by Nikolai Trubetzkoy (1939) and confirmed by Ranko Matasovi
(2012), the “North Caucasian” areal tendency of IE manifests itself primarily
in phonological and morphologival features rather than in lexical borrowings
(but see Bomhard 2018, 2019). The “Kartvelian” tendency could be expected to
manifest itself in all characteristics (Starostin 2007/2000: 817, 819), but my
analysis based on the STARLING 50-item wordlist failed to reveal that, as in
the case of both North Caucasian models. Is it because this short list includes
only unborrowable lexemes and is therefore unsuitable for areal studies in
principle? Results relating to the Semitic model cast doubt on this tenet.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
298
Alexander Kozintsev
presumably includes only words from the basic vocabulary,
supposed to be the most resistant to borrowing.
Vladislav Illi-Svity (1964) pointed to a group of IE–
Semitic lexical parallels which concerned cultural terms and
therefore could not be cognates. He believed them to be
borrowings from proto-Semitic to PIE. This idea was supported
by Gamkrelidze & Ivanov (1995: 769–773), Dolgopolsky (1987)
and Shevoroshkin (1987); see also Blaek 2013a. However, in
the view of Sergei Starostin (2007/2000; 2007/2001–2002), the
principal direction of borrowing was the opposite — from early
IE to proto-Semitic. Gamkrelidze and Ivanov (2013) eventually
agreed with that.
Igor Diakonoff (1985) rejected most of the IE–Semitic
6
parallels cited by Gamkrelidze and Ivanov (see Gamkrelidze &
Ivanov 1995: 862, for a response). Vitaly Shevoroshkin (1987),
who intervened on their behalf, claimed that the neighbors of
PIEs were Semites but not Kartvelians or North Caucasians.
Indeed, as the lexicostatistical modeling suggests, in the case of
Semites the borrowings comprised not only culture items but
basic vocabulary as well. The direction of supposed borrowing
cannot be assessed on the basis of these data.
According to Sergei Starostin, judging from the
phonology, the borrowing from early IE to proto-Semitic
occurred very early — before the loss of laryngeals. In his view,
these words had been borrowed either from PA or from an
extinct branch of Indo-Hittite but hardly from common IndoHittite (otherwise, one might expect a comparable number of
Semitic loanwords in IE dialects, which is not the case). S.
Starostin (2007/1999; 2007/2000; 2007/2001–2002) found no
evidence of contact between PIE and Afroasiatic languages
7
other than Semitic and concluded that in the early fifth
millennium BC a branch of Indo-Hittites migrated from their
homeland, situated north of the Fertile Crescent, southwards,
where it met the Semites; another branch migrated in the
northern and northeastern direction. The latter migration gave
6
Kassian (2010: 426), too, calls the proto-IE – proto-Semitic isoglosses a
“mirage”, subscribing to Diakonoff’s view that the IE homeland was located in
the Carpatho-Balkan region.
7
Disputing this, George Starostin (2007) points out that much has yet to be
done to arrive at a satisfactory Afroasiatic reconstruction.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
299
rise to all IE dialects except Anatolian (S. Starostin 2007/2001–
2002).
According to the STARLING database, Hittite is somewhat
closer to Semitic (average percentage of matching items is 9.0,
i.e., 4-5 words in the 50-item list) than are other IE languages
(the respective percentage is 6.4, that is 3, less often 4 words).
This may be because Hittite is an extinct language where more
cognates with Semitic are preserved. If, on the other hand, we
deal with loanwords rather than cognates, the result may be in
line with the view of Sergei Starostin (see above). None of this,
however, accounts for the “Semitic” shift of the entire IE family
(6.6 % of matching items, i.e., also 3–4 words) with regard to
Uralic (3.8 % of matches, i.e., two words at most). Does this
imply that contacts between PIE and proto-Semitic occurred
right at the moment when Anatolian began to split off from
early PIE (Indo-Hittite)? Or perhaps (more likely given the
geographic proximity) Anatolian received the Semitic adstrate
entirely or partly independently from the late PIE?
Apart from Hittite, Semitic loanwords are sometimes said
to be especially numerous in Greek (Watson 2013). However,
judging by the STARLING database, the frequency of lexical
matches between Semitic and Greek is 8.5 %, which is the same
as between Semitic and Sanskrit (8.5 %) and roughly the same
as between Semitic and Iranic, Germanic, and Latin (8.0 % in
each case). If we deal with cognates, the result is selfexplanatory. But if some of the items are loanwords, then the
variation could be low because borrowing concerned proto-IE
8
rather than filial IE languages.
At first sight, even the difference of 2.8 %, or 1-2 words in
the 50-item list, is minute and, accordingly, the “Semitic”
tendency of IE relative to Uralic, too, may appear random.
8
Anthony (2007: 304–305) claims that because the western Yamnaya groups
lived next to the Tripolye people, who, in his views, might have spoken an
Afroasiatic language, western IE languages retain more Afroasiatic
borrowings than eastern IE languages do. According to the STARLING
database, as far as Semitic languages are concerned, the relationship is the
opposite: the mean share of Semiti isoglosses in western IE languages (Baltic,
Slavic, Germanic, Latin, and Celtic) is 6.4 % whereas the occurrence in
Sanskrit and Iranic is 8.3 %. Again, the result is relevant only if we deal with
loanwords and not with cognates, as expected.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
300
Alexander Kozintsev
Moreover, members of the Starostin team believe that only one
of these words, denoting ‘horn’ (IE *ker-, *kr-no- / Semitic
*arn-), may be a loanword, whereas the second parallel,
referring to the nominative singular pronoun (IE *egoH vs.
phonetically quite dissimilar Akkadian anku, Northwest
Semitic *anVkV, Egyptian jnk, Berber *nakk-), if not
accidental, may be due only to a very remote common origin
(see discussion at https://www.academia.edu/s/d8be18e175/
proto-ie-meets-semitic-more-details-2019pdf?source=work,
accessed March 26, 2019). Even so, why do two of these four
lexemes concern the northernmost Afroasiatic languages, i.e.,
those geographically closest to PIE, wherever it was spoken?
Can’t this be an example of what Eugene Helimski (1982: 24–
25) termed “areal-genetic ties”, whereby cognates are preserved
in adjacent dialects?
Of course, the evidence of areal ties between IE and
Semitic in this analysis must be meager by definition because
the wordlist contains only basic (i.e. supposedly unborrowable)
lexemes. However, this extremely weak signal can be amplified
by comparing entire families rather than separate languages or
reconstructions. IE is represented by 12 languages, Uralic by 8,
and Semitic by two. Each “triangle” generated by the principal
coordinate analysis, then, has a swarm of points around each
vertex. Because the variation within families is incomparably
lower than between them, the coordinates of the vertices have
very small statistical errors, so even the difference of 5.6 %
between side lengths is highly significant. This, incidentally,
justifies the use of IE and Uralic as sister taxa at least in the
9
relative sense: IE is 20.8 % closer to Uralic than to Northwest
Caucasian; 18.4 % closer to Uralic than to Eastern Caucasian;
13.7 % closer to Uralic than to Kartvelian; and 16.9 % closer to
Uralic than to Semitic. Needless to say, all these differences are
highly significant.
9
This is not tantamount to regarding Indo-Uralic as a strictly monophyletic
taxon, i.e., a clade, although this does follow from the Markov-chain
lexicostatistical analysis carried out by Pagel et al. (2013) on the basis of the
200-item wordlist in the same database.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
301
2.1.3. Lexicostatistics: general conclusions
What is the relevance of all this to the problem of the IE
homeland? Sergei Starostin (2007/2000) argued that the ties of
PIE with proto-Semitic were mostly areal whereas those with
proto-Uralic indicate solely kinship (see Kortlandt 1990; Oswalt
1998; Ringe 1998; Helimski 2001; Pagel et al. 2013; Kassian et al.
2015; and Bomhard 2018: 294 for arguments in favor of a
genetic relationship between IE and Uralic). The position of the
opponents of this relationship is evidently much weaker, see,
e.g., Koivulehto 2001; see also Helimski’s (2001) scathing
criticism of his ideas about the alleged PIE loans in protoUralic. But given the plausibility of Indo-Uralic, neither
Anatolia nor the Balkans can be the original home of IE, in
contrast to what Illi-Svity (1971), Diakonoff (1985), Renfrew
(1989), and Kassian (2010) claimed. Predominantly or
exclusively Siberian affinities of IE, which had not yet been
detected in the 1970s and 1980s, make such hypotheses
implausible.
The glottochronological estimate of the separation of
Anatolian is 4670 BC according to Sergei Starostin and 4340 BC
according to George Starostin (quoted by Blaek 2013b). The
former estimate coincides with that proposed by Yanovich et al.
(2015) — 4660 BC — and is somewhat earlier than the average
across the 31 analyses by Chang et al. (2015) — 4244 BC. All
these estimates fall within the fifth millennium BC and are
much later than the date proposed by Bouckaert et al. even in
the corrected version (Bouckaert et al. 2013) — 5580 BC, let
alone the one from the previous study by the Auckland team —
6700 (Gray et al. 2011). .
But even the fifth millennium BC, where the most recent
glottochronological studies place the separation of the
Anatolian branch of IE, is sufficiently early to indicate a very
special evolutionary trajectory of this branch. An independent
linguistic fact supports this conclusion: both words denoting
the wheel in other IE dialects and derived from late PIE (but not
Indo-Hittite), *kwé-kwl-o-s and *Hrot-ó-s, are absent in
Anatolian whereas in Tocharian A and B the former word
denoted the carriage (Gamkrelidze, Ivanov 1995: 622–623;
Anthony, Ringe, 2015). Instead of them, a different IE word
with the stem *h2werg- was used in both Hittite and Tocharian
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
302
Alexander Kozintsev
to denote the wheel. According to Darden (2001) and Anthony
& Ringe (2015), this suggests that the Anatolian branch had
separated from the Indo-Hittite stem before wheel transport
was invented.
In sum, linguistics reveals three seemingly contradictory
facts: (1) the genetic affinities of PIE point to Siberia; (2) its
areal connections point to the Near East; (3) the first group to
have branched off from PIE in the fifth millennium BC is
likewise Near Eastern — Anatolian.
2.2. Nostratic, Indo-Uralic, and PIE: linguistic evidence relevant to
homelands
Frederik Kortlandt (2002), basing his conclusions on both
linguistic and archaeological evidence, located the Indo-Uralic
homeland south of the Ural Mountains in the seventh
millennium BC. Sergei Yakhontov (1991), utilizing words
10
referring to snow, ice, frost, and the northern forest biota,
placed the Nostratic homeland in the same or an adjacent place
like the Volga drainage or Western Siberia, several millennia
earlier, i.e., in the Pleistocene. Yakhontov's estimate
(“somewhat earlier than 15 thousand years ago”) matches the
recent estimates made by Pagel et al. (2013) — 14.5 thousand
years if their Eurasiatic tree, which includes IE, Uralic, Altaic,
Chukchi-Kamchatkan, and Eskimo as principal members, and
Dravidian and Kartvelian as peripheral members, is rooted with
proto-Dravidian and 15.6 thousand years if it is rooted with
proto-Kartvelian.
On the other hand, the analysis of reconstructed PIE terms
for topographical features and of the related attributes and
beliefs suggests that at some stage of their history, the IndoHittites lived in a mountainous terrain, moreover, that the
mountain was perceived as a “mighty cliff reaching to the sky”
(Gamkrelidze, Ivanov 1995: 574–577; Dybo 2013). As the
authors conclude, this eliminates the steppe as a possible
10
As Yakhontov admits, most of the reconstructed Nostratic words referring
to the northern forest biota are rather unreliable with regard to the homeland
location, and only the retention of the generic term for ‘berry’ (*marja),
reconstructed by Illi-Svity (1976: 43–45), unambiguously suggests, in
Yakhontov’s view, that Nostratic speakers had lived in the northern forest
zone.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
303
homeland. The proponents of the Steppe Theory describe the
PIE word *h4ékmn ‘stone’ with reflexes rendering ‘sky’ or
‘heaven’ in the daughter IE languages as “certainly, one of the
most troublesome words” (Mallory, Adams 2006: 121–122). PIE
words for mountains and swift rivers, they admit, “may suggest
broken topography” (ibid.: 130; Gamkrelidze, Ivanov 1995: 578).
In addition, the PIEs knew a sea or a large lake (Gamkrelidze,
Ivanov 1995: 581; Mallory, Adams 2006: 127, 130).
The skepticism that Salmons (2019) expresses in this
regard is unwarranted. Citing the case of Algonquian
languages, he claims that terms for landscape features refer to
relative rather than absolute categories. If this were true, all
languages would describe one and the same average mental
landscape, where everything is measured on a local scale. What
then accounts for the sharp contrast between the respective
terminologies in PIE and proto-Altaic (Dybo 2013)? In the
former case there are numerous terms for high cliffs, rocks,
canyons, precipices, swift rivers and a sea or large lake; in the
latter case, the reconstructed landscape is steppe with hillocks
and large rivers with seasonal floods, but no sea.
3. Archaeology
3.1. The Balkan corridor
After the incredibly early date of the first split in the IE
tree, i.e., of the separation of the Anatolian branch, initially
estimated at the early seventh millennium BC by the Auckland
team, had been shifted to the mid-sixth millennium BC by the
same authors and then to the fifth millennium BC by other
experts (see 2.1.3), it was widely believed that the Early
Farming Theory had been finally discredited whereas the
Steppe Theory had received crucial support (see, e.g., Anthony,
Ringe 2015, for non-glottochronological arguments against the
early split).
As for the Early Farming Theory, the decisive blow to it
has been inflicted not so much by the linguistic as by the
genetic data (see 4.2). But claiming that the demise of this
hypothesis automatically results in the acceptance of the Steppe
Theory is a non sequitur, and in the following I will try to
explain why.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
304
Alexander Kozintsev
The best option under the Steppe Theory is to identify the
steppe people such as those associated with the
Suvorovo/Novodanilovka/Skelya culture, who migrated from
the steppe to the lower Danube in the late fifth millennium BC,
with PAs (Mallory 1989: 233–243; Darden 2001; Carpelan,
Parpola 2001; Anthony 2007: 249–262; Dergachev 2007;
Anthony & Ringe 2015). As Anthony (2007: 262) puts it, “PreAnatolian languages probably were introduced to the lower
Danube valley and perhaps to the Balkans about 4200–4000 BC
by the Suvorovo migrants. We do not know when their
descendants moved into Anatolia. Perhaps pre-Anatolian
speakers founded Troy I in northwestern Anatolia around 3000
BC.”
However, when is not the only question we are unable to
answer. In fact, it may not even be a proper question to ask
because actually we don’t know either what language the
Suvorovo people had spoken or whether their descendants had
ever moved to Anatolia. To be sure, southward migration(s)
from the steppe to Anatolia did occur during the fourth
millennium — but when exactly and how? And who were the
migrants? There’s little doubt that they belonged to filial IE
groups, but it is not at all evident that any of them spoke PA.
Therefore, instead of applying, as Marija Gimbutas (1977) did,
sweeping expressions such as “Kurgan thrusts”, “first wave”,
“second wave”, “hybrid cultures”, “turmoil of the fourth
millennium BC”, etc., to the entire region from the western
steppe to Troy and to the entire epoch spanning more than a
millennium, one might look first at the chronology of the
purported waves and then at the separate and the most reliable
indicators of migration.
“A fine-grained sociolinguistic explanation of language
shift,” Anthony & Ringe (2015) argue, “should be linked to the
local conditions affecting each of these movements: the first
that carried Pre-Anatolian into southeastern Europe about
4200–4000 BC, coinciding with the Suvorovo-to-Cernavod I
migration and the sudden end of the tell cultures of Old
Europe,” etc. The same claim is repeated in Anthony’s recent
article (2019).
Now for whichever reason the tell cultures collapsed, the
first link in the supposed chain turns out to be broken.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
305
Suvorovo/Novodanilovka/Skelya is indeed contemporaneous
with Cucuteni A/Tripolye BI, i.e., it ended no later than 4100
BC (Rassamakin 2012; Manzura 2013), but Cernavod I began
hardly earlier than 3700 BC and was broadly contemporaneous
with cultures such as Baden, Ezero, and Sitagroi, which
Gimbutas (1977) believed to have resulted from the second
migration wave (Wild et al. 2001; Stadler et al. 2001; Horváth et
al. 2008; Furholt 2008; Ivanova 2013: 177; Manzura 2013: 138;
Krauss et al. 2016; Bulatovi & Vander Linden 2017).
Radiocarbon dates suggest that Baden had originated in Central
Europe, whence it spread in the southeastern direction (Stadler
et al. 2001; Wild et al. 2001; Horváth et al. 2008). Interestingly,
Lev Klejn (2010: 85–88) — one of the last and most eloquent
proponents of the regrettably untenable theory placing the IE
homeland in Central Europe — related Baden to PAs, whose
migration from the Danube to Troy was marked by
intermediate cultures such as Ezero and Sitagroi. Whatever one
might think of these reconstructions, the relative chronology of
migration events in this specific case is correct.
In short, the idea of the “Suvorovo-to-Cernavod I
migration” does not stand up to scrutiny. These cultures are
separated by a hiatus of nearly half a millennium. This fact
alone makes the entire “first movement” along the western
Black Sea coast rather problematic. But there’s more. Let’s see
how separate elements of the steppe culture moved southwards
along the Balkan route. Owing to Aleksandar Bulatovi (2014),
the situation has become much clearer than it was before. At
present, the movement can hardly be described as either a
“thrust” or a “wave” or even a succession of two waves; in fact,
even demic diffusion is questionable.
One of the principal features marking the early expansion
of steppe cultures is the use of crushed shell for tempering
ceramics. This practice appeared in the steppes already in the
Neolithic — in the early sixth millennium BC (Kotova 2015: 58,
63). Experts in the Cucuteni-Tripolye culture call the shelltempered pottery the “Cucuteni type” (Schmidt 1932: 42;
Mova 1961; Palaguta 1998; Manzura 2000). It was introduced to
Tripolye by people of the Novodanilovka/Skelya culture
(similar to Suvorovo), who migrated there from the Lower
Dnieper at the Cucuteni A3-A4/Tripolye BI stage or even
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
306
Alexander Kozintsev
earlier, at the end of the pre-Cucuteni/Tripolye A stage, in the
second half of the fifth millennium BC, with a ceramic tradition
that was entirely different from and more primitive than the
local one (Palaguta 1998). Much later, after 3700 BC, shelltempered ceramics appeared on the Lower Danube in the
Cernavod I culture and spread to the Balkans, perhaps not
directly via Cernavod I, in the second half of the fourth
millennium BC (Bulatovi 2014).
Another important marker of IE migrations is pottery
decorated with cord imprints. The tradition probably originated
in the Chalcolithic Dereivka culture of Ukraine (late fifth –
early fourth millennia BC) (Kotova 2013: 103–104). From there
it rapidly diffused to the Cucuteni–Tripolye area and further
south. The early variety of the cord decoration — the so-called
Wickelschnur design — occurs on vessels of the Cucuteni
type. At Balkan sites of the Bubanj–Slcua–Krivodol (BSK)
cultural complex the cord decoration appears at about the same
time as in the supposed area of origin, i.e., in the early
Eneolithic, before 3500 BC and possibly even before 4000 BC,
but only sporadically and on vessels of local types, becoming
common only after 3500 BC (Bulatovi 2014).
The third indicator of migrations from the steppes are
zoomorphic stone maceheads, sometimes called scepters, dating
to the second half of the fifth millennium BC and scattered
across a vast territory from the southern Ural to Macedonia.
Valentin Dergachev (2007: 145–148; and David Anthony (2007:
234), following Marija Gimbutas (1977) and Valentin Danilenko
(1974: 92–106), believed that these symbols of power had spread
from the east to the west, i.e., from Khvalynsk to Tripolye BI,
then to Cernavod I and ultimately to the Carpatho-Balkan area
with the first migration wave. However, the dates of the
Khvalynsk cemeteries based on animal bones, i.e., without the
reservoir effect, fall within the mid-fifth millennium (Shishlina
at al. 2009), coinciding with the average of Tripolye BI dates
based on charcoal, animal bones, and grain samples and
spanning the entire fifth millennium (Rassamakin 2012).
No wonder these chronological ambiguities give rise to
diametrically opposite views. For instance, Igor Manzura (2000)
claims that all cultural features which the followers of
Danilenko and Gimbutas believe to be indicative of the
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
307
westward expansion of steppe herders actually mirror the
eastward migration of the Tripolye farmers. Similar ideas are
expressed by Blagoje Govedarica, who believes that the scepter
owners, having moved from the Cucuteni-Tripolye agricultural
area to the steppe, became the pastoralist elite (Govedarica &
Manzura 2011: 47). In the view of Yuri Rassamakin (1999: 102),
these artifacts belonged to traders related to the
Novodanilovka/Skelya culture, who were intermediaries
between the steppe and the agricultural groups. In line with
this view, Sergei Korenevsky (2008) and Aleksandar Bulatovi
(2014) regard scepters as “transcultural” artifacts associated
11
with various steppe societies.
What matters for us, however, is the southward spread of
the scepters. The evidence in favor of the “Kurgan thrust” in
this case is as meager as in the case of other indicators.
According to Bulatovi (2014: 110), “(t)he presence of scepters
in the central and southern Balkans is sporadic, as are the finds
of corded ware, so it is not realistic to presume the presence of
steppe communities on this territory solely on the basis of these
isolated finds, even more so since elementary steppe
characteristics are lacking, like typical ceramic shapes, crushed
shell temper in the pottery and steppe funerary rites.” Again,
what we observe instead of “thrusts” or “waves” is but a trickle.
Archaeological facts, then, suggest that filial IE groups
entered Anatolia from the Balkans no earlier than 3000 BC, i.e.,
long after the wheeled vehicles had become common both west
and east of the Black Sea. If PA speakers were indeed among
these groups, why are both words denoting the wheel in other
IE dialects missing in Hittite (see 2.1.3)? It does appear, in line
with the consensus view, that PA speakers had been isolated
from other Indo-Europeans since the fifth millennium BC. This
would agree with a single, distant and rapid migration rather
than a slow and gradual diffusion, as in the Balkans. Also, the
Balkan scenario is at odds with linguistic facts indicating
contacts between PIE and proto-Semitic groups.
11
One might recall the much later but likewise transcultural Seima-Turbino
phenomenon, also caused by tumultuous events in Eurasian prehistory,
distant migrations, and a chain-like transfer of tradition (Chernykh 2013: 269287).
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
308
Alexander Kozintsev
In short, if an early IE “thrust”, or “first wave” such as the
one envisaged by Marija Gimbutas had actually occurred, the
Balkans are not the right place to look for it. Where then
should we look without leaving either Anatolia or the steppe —
areas inseparably linked by their common IE past — out of
sight? If the Balkans were the western corridor connecting
these two regions, then we should turn to the alternative,
eastern corridor — the Caucasus.
3.2. The Caucasian corridor: Pre-Maykop and Maykop
Compared to the Balkans, the role of the Caucasian route
is minor in both rival theories of IE origin, Steppe and Early
Farming (the latter theory can be considered non-viable for all
practical purposes). When advocates of the steppe origin
mention the Caucasus, they discuss the possible movement of
Indo-Europeans from the steppe to the southern Caucasus — an
idea supported by Gimbutas (1963) and rejected by Mallory
(1989: 231–233) — or the penetration of alleged North
Caucasian speakers to the steppe (Anthony 2007: 297; 2019;
Kortlandt 2018; Bomhard 2019a,b).
Amazingly, rather than exploring the possibility of a
direct northward expansion of late PIEs from their presumed
eastern Anatolian homeland, Gamkrelidze & Ivanov (1995: 831–
844) entertained a bizarre idea that they had split already in the
Near East and that their filial groups migrated in various
directions, some westward, some eastward, and some taking a
roundabout route to Eastern Europe via Central Asia. Only one
branch of Indo-Aryans, after its incredibly early separation, was
allowed to migrate directly northward via the Caucasus, and
that’s where Maykop came into play (ibid.: 811–814). This
strange scenario, where Maykop’s role was minor, resulted
from the idea that PIE correlates with Halaf (ibid.: 787–788),
then dated to the fifth and fourth millennia but later moved to
6100–5100 BC. Because both “southern” theories of an IE
homeland, Renfrew’s Western Anatolian and Gamkrelidze &
Ivanov’s Eastern Anatolian, posited a very early disintegration
of PIE, both postulates — a southern homeland and an early
split — have come to be inseparable, and disproving an early
split is often believed to be tantamount to upholding the Steppe
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
309
Theory (see, e.g., Chang et al. 2015). This, as noted above, is a
non sequitur.
Regardless of its relevance to the IE issue, the role of the
Caucasus as a corridor connecting the Near East with the
steppe is evidenced by numerous archaeological facts, and the
principal migration vector was northward. The movement of
the Near Eastern groups in that direction along the eastern
coast of the Black Sea began in the early Neolithic, possibly
even earlier (Formozov 1965: 65–68; Kotova 2015: 59; Gorelik et
al. 2016).
Viktor Trifonov (2009) writes about two migration routes
taken by the Near Eastern populations in the Chalcolithic. One
of them led from western Iran to the eastern Caucasus (the
Shulaveri–Shomutepe culture) and further west to the Black
Sea coast (the Odishi culture); another, from eastern Anatolia to
the northwestern Caucasus (the Darkveti-Meshoko culture, see
Trifonov 2001a). Which, if any, of these routes may be relevant
to the IE issue, is not clear, but in any case, the migration
vectors across the Caucasus at that time were mostly westward
and northward.
Of particular relevance in this respect are the pre-Maykop
(late fifth – early fourth millennia BC) Chalcolithic fortified
sites in the southern Kuban drainage, representing the
Darkveti-Meshoko culture. Especially important is the
eponymous highland fortress Meshoko, situated on a
promontory, whose inland side was protected by a cyclopean
stone wall that was 150 m long, 3-4 m thick, and has been
preserved to the height of 2 m (Formozov 1965: 69–73; Lyonnet
2007; Stolyar et al. 2009; Cherlenok 2013: 41–43; Sagona 2018:
12
137–143). The site has a detailed stratigraphy. Its lower layers,
representing the culture of people who had constructed the
14
fortress and dating to 4200–4000 BC by C (Korenevsky 2012:
63), contained high-quality Near Eastern type burnished
13
ceramics tempered with finely ground calcite and suggestive
12
In Sagona’s book, regrettably, there is some confusion between pre-Maykop
and Maykop materials such as those reproduced on p. 140 (fig. 4.4).
13
Calcite temper was typical of pottery from Ochazhny Grot (Grotto of
Hearths) in the Vorontsov Cave, Abkhazia (Solovyev : 1958: 143;
Kozintsev 2017) and from Hammam et-Turkman VB, Syria (van Loon 1988:
292–293), dating to 4360–3800 BC. Also, similarities are observed with the
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
310
Alexander Kozintsev
of migration from the south, and a scarce and inexpressive
lithic industry. In the upper horizons, the combination of
cultural components is the reverse: the pottery, decorated with
interior-punched nodes, is coarse, tempered with amorphous
limestone and grit, and apparently rooted in the local tradition
whereas the lithic industry, likewise having Neolithic roots, is
rich and diverse — a true Renaissance after the degradation
evidenced by the earlier industry (Stolyar et al. 2009; Kozintsev
2017).
In short, the later culture of Meshoko is paradoxically
more archaic, a few metal tools notwithstanding. Despite the
striking contrast between the two cultural components,
immigrant and autochthonous, the changes of both ceramics
and lithics, which occurred strictly in parallel, were gradual and
no abrupt boundary between the horizons can be traced
(Kozintsev 2017). At the late stage the habitation layers spread
beyond the fortification walls, evidently following the downfall
of the citadel (Stolyar et al. 2009: 136–167). The top layers show
some reversal of the trend — the material culture, assessed by
both ceramics and lithics, becomes a little closer to that of the
early stage, possibly attesting to symbiosis (Kozintsev 2017).
In the middle layers of Meshoko, several potsherds
tempered with crushed shell were found. They are decorated
with comb imprints, less often with interior-punched nodes,
and resemble the Cucuteni ware. In Tripolye, such a
decoration appears on the Cucuteni vessels at the BII stage in
the first half of the fourth millennium (Movsha 1961). At the
pre-Maykop fortress Svobodnoe, which is somewhat earlier
than Meshoko and, unlike the latter, is situated on the
boundary with the steppe, shell-tempered ceramics
Chalcolithic pottery from Areni, Armenia (Areshian et al. 2012). The southern
origin of people who constructed the Meshoko fortress is evidenced by this
indicator as well. Yet another evidence thereof may be the obsidian found at
Meshoko. The pieces analyzed by Formozov & Chernykh (1964) had been
made of obsidian from Transcaucasian mines situated 600-650 km away from
Meshoko whereas that from earlier (Upper Paleolithic and Mesolithic) sites in
the area had been procured from north-central Caucasian mines situated only
250-300 km away. The problem is that obsidian pieces were found in both
lower and upper layers of the site, and we don’t know if the sources were the
same or different (Sergei Ostashinsky’s personal communication).
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
311
predominate. Svobodnoe hardly belongs to the DarkvetiMeshoko culture as its affinities are mostly with the steppe,
Tripolye, and even Central Europe (Nekhajev 1992). Ties
between pre-Maykop and Skelya sites had already been noted
on the basis of ceramics (Nekhajev 1992; Rassamakin 1999: 78–
80; Kotova 2006). Likewise indicative of such ties are an
unfinished cruciform stone mace-head (unfinished means not
imported) from the upper layers of Meshoko (Stolyar et al.
2009: 138, 161; cf.: Govedarica 2005–2009) and a fragment of a
zoomorphic stone scepter from another pre-Maykop fortress —
Yasenova Polyana (Korenevsky 2008: 147).
The gradual transition from a culture apparently
introduced from the southern Caucasus to a more archaic one
indicates a clash of two traditions — southern and
autochthonous. The third (northern) component evidences ties
with the steppe tribes. Importantly, all the pre-Maykop
fortresses emerged in the northwestern Caucasus next to the
steppe in the late fifth millennium BC (Korenevsky 2012: 63).
They are half a millennium earlier than the allegedly PA
Cernavod I culture and were constructed soon after the
Anatolian branch had split off from the Indo-Hittite stem.
To the extent that cultural contrast points to linguistic
heterogeneity, the possibility that people speaking at least three
very different languages met in the Northwest Caucasus in the
late fifth millennium must be considered. Judging by the
cyclopean fortification walls, they clashed, and judging by the
steppe-type artifacts and by the gradual nature of cultural
changes, they interacted. Whereas the Balkan scenario
precludes contacts between Indo-Europeans and Semites, the
Caucasian scenario can easily incorporate such contacts
provided the Indo-Europeans were migrants from the south —
those who constructed the fortresses. We know neither the
exact source of their migration nor whether the travel was fast
or slow. What we do know is that they had reached the
borderland with the steppe in the fifth millennium BC — 1000odd years before Troy I was founded by people whom some
believe to be Anatolians (Luwians?) but whose ethnicity is
actually not in the least more understandable than that of the
Meshoko people (Yakubovich 2010: 128–129).
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
312
Alexander Kozintsev
The southern tendency, evidenced by the early culture of
Meshoko, reappeared even more strikingly in the Maykop
culture (Munchaev 1994; Korenevsky 2004; Kohl 2007: 72–86;
Lyonnet 2007; Ivanova 2012; 2013: 50–118; Sagona 2018: 143–
182), which emerged shortly after 4000 BC (Trifonov 2001b;
Chernykh & Orlovskaya 2008; Korenevsky 2012 101; Kohl &
Trifonov 2014). Rauf Munchaev (1975: 328–329) and Marina
Andreyeva (1977) pointed to Syro-Anatolian (Amuq F) and
northern Mesopotamian (Gawra XII–VIII, especially XIA)
parallels to Maykop, and the same tradition was later registered
at Arslantepe VII in eastern Anatolia (Frangipane 1993).
However, Amuq F, marked by the so-called “Chaff-Faced Ware”
14
(CFW), is no earlier than 3700 BC (Marro 2010; Greenberg,
Palumbi 2015; Sagona 2018: 184–187) and is therefore coeval
with early Maykop; the same applies to Arslantepe VII
(Helwing 2000); but Gawra XIA is earlier, correlating with the
Ubaid to Uruk transition of the late fifth millennium (Peasnall,
Rothman 2003; Marro 2010). Catherine Marro (2010) believes
that Maykop is closest to Hammam et-Turkman V in northern
15
Syria, which is somewhat earlier than Maykop.
Unlike their predecessors and possible ancestors in the
northwestern Caucasus, the Maykop people did not build
fortifications, apparently because their relations with both the
natives and the steppe tribes were more peaceful. Moreover,
unlike the pre-Maykop people, those of Maykop were both
willing and able to colonize the steppe (Gej 1989; Korenevsky
2004: 93–96; Shishlina 2013; Trifonov 2014).
3.3. The Near Eastern roots of Maykop and Novosvobodnaya
Maykop was broadly contemporaneous with the CFW
tradition of the southern Caucasus and Iran, called the Leilatepe
culture in Azerbaijan (Akhundov 2007; Marro 2007, 2010;
Lyonnet 2007; Museibli 2014: 48; Ivanova 2012; 2013: 118–121;
Sagona 2018: 153, 182–199). This tradition, in turn, correlates
with Amuq F, Hammam et-Turkman V, etc. (Marro 2010). In
14
Most of the Maykop pottery, however, is not chaff-faced although the
resemblance of vessel forms is indeed striking.
15
The Hammam VB ware, dating to 4360–3800 BC (Akkermans 1988), is
tempered with calcite (van Loon 1988: 292–293) — a feature paralleled by the
contemporaneous ware from the lower layers of Meshoko (see 3.2).
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
313
northwestern Iran, it dates to 4200–3700 BC, the same as in the
southern Caucasus and northern Mesopotamia (Abedi et al.
2015).
Some researchers view early Maykop as a northern
extension of Leilatepe (Munchaev & Amirov 2012; Museibli
2014: 46; Rezepkin 2017), and some speak of a migration from
Mesopotamia to the Caucasus — a pre-Uruk expansion
(Andreyeva 1977; Akhundov 2007; Lyonnet 2007; Pitskhelauri
2012; Kohl, Trifonov 2014).
Catherine Marro (2010) argues that the CFW tradition
originated, not in Mesopotamia, but “somewhere in the
highlands between the Upper Euphrates and the Kura Rivers.”
Specifically, its early manifestation, dating to the late fifth
millennium BC, was found at Ovçular Tepesi, Nakhichevan
(Marro et al. 2009, 2011, 2014). In Marro’s opinion, Ovçular
Tepesi may be ancestral to both Amuq F and Leilatepe (and
thereby, one might add, indirectly to Maykop). Later, CFW was
discovered at another site, broadly contemporaneous with
Ovçular Tepesi — Noruntepe on the Upper Euphrates (Gülçur
& Marro 2012) and at certain other early sites (Palumbi 2011).
Gülçur & Marro (2012: 325) call this tradition the “standardized
ware oikumene” and conclude that “the technical roots of CFW
should rather be sought in the east Anatolian/Caucasian
highlands.”
Whereas the southern roots of Maykop are beyond doubt,
the origin of the Late Chalcolithic (early fourth millennium BC)
kurgan tradition south of the Caucasus range is a contentious
matter (Poulmar’h & Le Mort 2016). Barrows have been
discovered in Azerbaijan (Museibli 2014: 6–48; Lyonnet et al.
2008; Sagona 2018: 199–202), Georgia (Makharadze 2007),
Armenia (Muradyan 2014), and Iran (Muscarella 2013: 88–94;
Ivanova 2012; 2013: 124–128). They correlate, though not
intrinsically, with the Leilatepe culture. When Maykop-type
tumuli were first excavated at Sé Girdan, Jean Deshayes (1973),
in line with Gimbutas’ ideas, regarded this as evidence of an
Indo-European migration from the northern Caucasus. Alexey
Rezepkin (2000: 20–21), agreeing with him, wrote that PIEs,
who, in his view, were associated with the Novosvobodnaya
culture and used wheeled transport, migrated to Iran via
Azerbaijan, introducing the kurgan tradition to those areas.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
314
Alexander Kozintsev
Without explicitly mentioning Indo-Europeans, but probably
alluding to the Kurgan theory, Munchaev & Amirov (2012)
regarded the spread of the kurgan tradition, which they
erroneously believed to be later than Leilatepe, as evidence of
the first migration wave of pastoralists from the steppes to the
Near East. Anthony (2007: 294–295), too, writes about “the
migration southward of a Klady-type chief” but in his view, the
Novosvobodnaya/Klady people, like those of Maykop proper,
spoke a North Caucasian dialect (ibid.: 98, 297). A similar view
has been expressed by Alexei Kassian (2010: 417, 421–422, 428),
who likewise regards Novosvobodnaya as just a late stage of
Maykop and associates the Maykop culture in toto with North
Caucasian speakers.
Now whatever language the Novosvobodnaya/Klady
people spoke, they couldn’t have introduced the kurgan
tradition to the southern Caucasus. First, absolute dates of
Novosvobodnaya/Klady fall in the 3600–2800 BC range
(Trifonov et al. 2017; Wang et al. 2019, Supplementary data 1).
Maykop proper, on the other hand, is earlier and so are the
Transcaucasian kurgans. Two dates for Soyuq Bulaq are in the
3900–3600 BC interval and thus no later than those for early
Maykop, let alone Klady (Lyonnet et al. 2008). While no
absolute dates are available for Sé Girdan, this cemetery is
hardly later because the stone scepter with a feline head found
there is stylistically quite similar to the one with an equine
head from Soyuq Bulaq; also, most beads from both tombs are
similar in shape and material (Lyonnet et al. 2008).
Second, a child burial in a jar at Klady (Rezepkin 2000 fig.
12: 12, 13) parallels earlier ones, correlating with Late
Chalcolithic contexts at several sites in the southern Caucasus
(Museyibli 2014: 49–59). This early agricultural practice spread
from Anatolia to the Balkans in the Neolithic as part of the
Neolithization package. By the Early Bronze Age it became
dominant in Anatolia and the Levant, but virtually disappeared
in Europe (Bacvarov 2008).
Third, in the case of Klady, genetics indicates a migration
from the southern to the northern Caucasus, not vice versa (see
4.2.1). And still, all these facts, important as they are, do not
cancel the possibility of a southward spread of the kurgan
tradition in the first half of the fourth millennium BC, from
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
315
early Maykop to Sé Girdan (see, e.g. Kohl 2007: 85). Here,
indirect evidence must be examined.
3.4. Who were the Maykop people?
Sergei Korenevsky (2012: 62, 68, 107–108, 114) claims that
the earliest kurgans appeared on the northern Caucasian
16
steppes in the fifth millennium BC and may in some respects
be regarded as prototypes of the Maykop tumuli. Korenevsky
(2012: 109), however, takes a cautious stance, writing that
Maykop and Leilatepe kurgans emerged simultaneously in the
early fourth millennium BC, largely independently of the
steppe tradition. Viktor Trifonov (2000), describing the Sé
Girdan kurgans and calling them “Maykop type tumuli”,
refrained from assessing the direction in which the tradition
had spread.
Najaf Museibli (2014: 72), on the other hand, attributes Sé
Girdan to the Leilatepe culture, claiming that the kurgan
tradition, having originated near Lake Urmia, spread to the
northern Caucasus. Philip Kohl and Viktor Trifonov (2014), too,
assert that kurgans had been constructed in the southern
Caucasus “well before” their appearance on the Eurasian
steppes.
A detailed elaboration of such ideas with regard not only
to the kurgan tradition, but to the entire material culture, has
been proposed by Mariya Ivanova (2012; 2013: 108–118; 121–
129). Her most provocative conclusion is that the key foreign
elements in the fourth millennium BC cultures of the northern
Caucasus point neither to the upper Euphrates and Tigris nor
to the alluvial plains of Mesopotamia, but to High Asia — the
Iranian Plateau and the mountainous regions of southwestern
Central Asia. These elements include colorful ornamental
stones such as lapis lazuli and turquoise, which at that time
were rare in southwest Asia but had been abundant in Iran and
Central Asia at least since the sixth millennium BC. Other
indicators, unique but pointing, as Ivanova argues, in the same
16
A Chalcolithic kurgan at Tipki in the Stavropol steppe has a very early
14
calibrated C date of 4891–4692 BC (Korenevsky 2012: 64) whereas others
allegedly date to the Tripolye BI and BI-BII stages (second half of the fifth
millennium). Igor Manzura (2017) believes that most if not all these cases are
doubtful.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
316
Alexander Kozintsev
direction, are precious fabric and exotic red pigment from
Klady, an unusual bone pin with a flat triangular head from
Ust-Dzheguta, and a carnelian cylinder seal from
Krasnogvardeyskoe, very similar to that from Tepe Sialk IV
(see, however, Kohl 2007: 75, for Mesopotamian and Anatolian
parallels to these artifacts).
According to Mariya Ivanova (2013: 113), the only area
where all the animals portrayed on the Maykop silver vessels
were native in the fourth millennium is northwestern Iran,
Armenia, and Azerbaijan. Especially noteworthy are stylistic
parallels between these vessels and gold ones from Fullol,
Afghanistan (Ivanova 2013: 110–114), some of which she dates
to the fourth millennium BC. Moreover, the geometric motifs of
those vessels closely parallel the Geoksyur-type painted pottery
of southwestern Central Asia. Vadim Masson (2006: 58–59), too,
regarded these motifs as direct replicas of the Late Chalcolithic
Kara-depe and Geoksyur prototypes, though attempts were
th
made to link the Fullol style to the 13 century BC Assyrian
glyptics (see, e.g., Olijdam 2000).
Also suggestive are late fifth and fourth millennia BC
Iranian and Turkmenian rather than Syro-Anatolian prototypes
of Maykop copper tools (Ivanova 2016). Based on all these facts,
Ivanova (2012; 2013: 123–124) points to the well-trodden path
coinciding with the Great Khorasan Road of later epochs and
connecting Khorasan with Iranian Azerbaijan. A northern
branch of the route leads along the western Caspian coast to
the Kura valley and further north, to the passes in the Caucasus
Range.
As to the CFW, Ivanova notes that its distribution area
included northwestern Iran and the Plain of Urmia, where the
Sé Girdan tumuli are located. But the distribution area of tumuli
is smaller than that of the CFW. Fourth millennium BC mounds
resembling those at Sé Girdan have been excavated in
Transcaucasia but not in Anatolia or Mesopotamia. Ivanova
(2013: 129) concludes: “Thus, the funerary evidence adds
further credibility to the hypothesis that the foreign elements
in the northern Caucasus originated from the Iranian plateau
and its borderlands, and not from Greater Mesopotamia or the
Anatolian highland, two regions which lie far outside the area
of distribution of early tumuli.”
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
317
Indeed, there are indications that the kurgan tradition in
Transcaucasia is more ancient than Leilatepe. The calibrated
date based on a human tooth from kurgan 5 at Aknalich,
Armenia, is 4340–4050 BC, suggesting that the burial is
contemporaneous with the Middle Chalcolithic Sioni horizon
(Muradyan 2014). While the reservoir effect is possible, the
date, according to Firdus Muradyan, is substantiated by
typological parallels between Aknalich and Sioni sites in
Armenia.
Before Ivanova, Leonid Sverkov (2011: 172) argued than
Maykop had originated in the southeastern Caspian region,
whence it migrated first to the area near Lake Urmia and then
to the northern Caucasus. He pointed to the close similarity
between the asymmetrical flint arrowheads in the Maykop and
Kelteminar cultures (ibid.: 74–75; see 5.4).
According to Gimbutas (1963), the Maykop people were
PAs; according to Kristiansen (2012), they were early PIEs
(Indo-Hittites) — common ancestors of all IE groups including
PAs; whereas Heggarty (2018) claims that Maykop was
associated with a minority of late PIE branches, marked by the
high amount of the CHG component (see 4.2), primarily
Germanic and Balto-Slavic. The problem with the first
hypothesis is that the Maykop people used wheeled vehicles
(Trifonov 2004), which had apparently been introduced from
the Near East (Izbitser 2013), whereas, as noted above, Hittite
had neither of the two common IE words for ‘wheel’. The
problem with the second hypothesis is that the Maykop culture,
like Leilatape, emerged hardly before 3900–3800 BC (Trifonov
2001b; Chernykh & Orlovskaya 2008; Shishlina et al. 2009;
Korenevsky 2012: 101; Museibli 2014: 48) — several centuries
after the Anatolian branch had split off from the Indo-Hittite
stem. The third hypothesis, postulating a dispersal of most IE
groups directly from the Near East, is implausible given the
facts pointing to the steppe origin of the second earliest branch
of IE — Tocharian (see 5.6), let alone the later branches.
What language did the pre-Maykop people speak? We
don’t know that, of course. What we do know are three things:
(a) they had migrated from the south (see 3.2); (b) they reached
the northwestern Caucasus shortly after the separation of the
Anatolian branch; and (c) there are no indications of the use of
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
318
Alexander Kozintsev
wheeled vehicles at that time. Therefore the pre-Maykop
people could have spoken either PA or late PIE (the language
ancestral to all IE branches except Anatolian). The former
possibility is more likely because late PIE was, to all
appearances, spoken by the Maykop people, who, like those of
Leilatepe, used both wheeled transport and the pottery wheel
17
(Museibli 2014: 67). Theoretically, the pre-Maykop people
could be their ancestors, but this disagrees with the fact that
they did not build kurgans. So rather than being
autochthonous, the Maykop people could represent the second
wave of migrants from the south — those who introduced the
kurgan tradition to the northern Caucasus.
3.5. Khvalynsk: southern roots?
Igor Gorauk (2003) claims that the blade industries of
the Khvalynsk sites in the Middle and Lower Volga drainage,
Kair-Shak VI, Kara-Khuduk, and Gundorovskoe, are identical to
that of the Shebir sites on the Mangyshlak Peninsula. The
latter, in turn, reveal parallels with early farming cultures of
southern Turkmenia and Iran, implying a northward migration
route along the eastern Caspian coast. Moreover, Gorauk
points to Near Eastern parallels of the Khvalynsk stone maces
and bracelets.
Sergei Bogdanov, on the other hand, uses the same
parallels, which he supplements by those relating to the preMaykop culture of Northern Caucasus, to hypothesize a
Caucasian route of the IE migration from the Near East to the
Volga (Bogdanov 2004: 231, 235).
David Anthony (2019), too, believes that Khvalynsk
ancestors migrated from northwestern Iran or Azerbaijan along
the western Caspian coast sometime before 5000 BC. Because
his conclusion is based mostly on genetic facts, I will discuss it
in section 4.2.4.
One must admit that the cultural evidence of the southern
roots of Khvalynsk is much weaker than that demonstrating
the southern origin of pre-Maykop, let alone Maykop. This is
not only because archaeological facts in the former case are
17
On the basis of her experimental studies, Galina Poplevko (2017), however,
rejects the idea that Maykop pottery was wheel thrown.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
319
scantier, but also because the route from the Near East in the
latter case is shorter.
4. Genetics
4.1. The Balkan corridor, yet again
Before the emergence of paleogenetics, physical
anthropology was sometimes employed to tackle the IE
problem (Gimbutas 1977; Menk 1980; Schwidetzky 1980;
Gamkrelidze & Ivanov 1995: 847–849; Day 2001; Kozintsev
2008, 2009). The results of some of these publications,
specifically those by Menk and Kozintsev, foreshadowed later
genetic studies, but most of the work concerned comparatively
late stages of IE history, beginning with the Yamnaya
expansion. Human remains from territories relevant to PIE
history are, with few exceptions, too scarce to warrant
statistical treatment, which was often substituted by obsolete
racial labels attached to individual crania.
Early attempts to use phenotypic genetic markers such as
blood groups, plasma proteins, enzymes, etc., for solving the IE
problem were rather disappointing (Sokal et al. 1992), but later
studies, using synthetic maps based on the integration of gene
frequencies across various systems were very illustrative,
showing both the “early farming cline” and the later “steppe
expansion” (Piazza et al. 1995; Cavalli-Sforza 2001: 117–119,
161–163). Certain important conclusions made by CavalliSforza and his team, specifically that the cline connecting the
steppe with the Near East is quite pronounced in the Caucasus
and eastern Anatolia while attenuating toward the Balkans and
western Anatolia (Piazza et al. 1995: 5839, fig. 3), are supported
both by archaeological data (see 3.1 and 3.2) and by recent
genomic studies.
The birth of genomics and especially paleogenomics
marked a new era. Not only were Cavalli-Sforza’s findings
upheld, but so were those of the recent archaeological research
cited above. As to the Balkan route, the conclusion of a recent
study (Mathieson et al. 2018: 5) is unequivocal: “No steppe
migration to Anatolia via southeast Europe.” The authors go
on: “Although we find sporadic steppe-related ancestry in
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
320
Alexander Kozintsev
18
Balkan Copper and Bronze Age individuals, this ancestry is
rare until the Late Bronze Age.” But even in genomes of the
Mycenaean Greeks, the share of the steppe ancestry is
estimated at only 4–16 %, and the predominant Y-chromosome
haplogroup is the “southern” J, not subclades of R1 that
supposedly mark the steppe IEs (Lazaridis et al. 2017).
In other words, both the first and even the second wave of
the alleged Indo-European advance toward Anatolia via the
Balkans turn out to be illusory not only in archaeological terms,
but in terms of genetics as well. Why the tell cultures collapsed
is a different question, but whatever the reason might be, the
facts unambiguously suggests that PA speakers did not migrate
to Anatolia from the steppe along the Balkan route. Had they
ever lived on the steppe, after all?
4.2. CHG, AF, and Y-chromosome haplogroups: their role in
tracing PIE migrations
4.2.1. Pre-Maykop, Maykop, and Novosvobodnaya
The feature that has gained greatest importance for the IE
problem in the recent years is the autosomal component CHG
(Caucasus Hunter-Gatherers). It is especially marked in Upper
Paleolithic and Mesolithic bone samples from Georgia (Jones et
al. 2015) and in Neolithic and, to a lesser extent, in Chalcolithic
samples from northern Iran (Lazaridis et al. 2016). However,
CHG was not detected in the Chalcolithic (Namazga III, ~3300
BC) sample from Turkmenia despite the presence of a Neolithic
Iranian ancestry in that population (Damgaard et al. 2018).
Having been introduced to the northern Caucasus from
the south, CHG was first registered in the pre-Maykop people
of the southern Kuban drainage, in the Chalcolithic people of
the north-central Caucasian steppes and Ukraine, and in the
Khvalynsk group (Damgaard et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2019).
Later, it became very prominent (over 40 %) in the Yamnaya
people and, after their westward expansion, also in those of the
Corded Ware culture and in other supposedly Indo-European
18
Two individuals with steppe ancestry buried at mid-fifth millennium
cemeteries Varna I and Smyadovo in Bulgaria hardly suffice to speak of a
“wave”.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
321
groups of Central and Western Europe (Haak 2015; Allentoft
2015).
The pre-Maykop samples were taken from bones of three
children buried in Unakozovskaya Cave, Adygei, in the midfifth millennium BC. Their southern ancestry is demonstrated
by two components that are predominant in their autosomal
genomes — western Anatolian, introduced to Europe by the
early farmers (AF) and CHG, as well as by the “southern” Ychromosome haplogroup J (Wang et al. 2019). The latter is
coupled with the CHG component in the Upper Paleolithic and
Mesolithic people of the western Caucasus (Jones et al. 2015); it
was also detected in a Mesolithic man from Hotu, Iran
(Lazaridis et al. 2016) and in one of the two Kura-Araxes (Early
Transcaucasian Culture) males (Wang et al. 2008).The gracility
of the male cranium from Unakozovskaya points in the same
direction. Inna Potekhina (1995) attributed it to the Eastern
Mediterranean type. The Maykop cranial series, too, shows a
southern (Mediterranean) tendency (Kazarnitsky 2012: 36).
The Maykop genomes contain mostly the same two
southern autosomal components as the genomes of their
predecessors and possibly ancestors — western (AF) and
eastern (CHG). As in the pre-Maykop and Kura-Araxes
genomes, the CHG to AF ratio is about 6:4 (Wang et al. 2019: 4,
fig. 2a). Most Maykop Y-chromosome haplogroups (four out of
six) are markedly “southern” — L and J. The L1a subclade was
found in a Chalcolithic sample from Arpa, Azerbaijan (Lazaridis
et al. 2016; see above on the J haplogroup).
Apart from the two autosomal constituents present in
Maykop people proper, the Novosvobodnaya people buried at
Klady are marked by the third component, also southern,
19
specific to the Chalcolithic of Iran. In this respect, they
resemble the Kura-Araxes people. The share of this component
is 37 % in Novosvobodnaya and 25 % in Kura-Araxes (Wang et
al. 2019: 6, fig. 4b). The Y-chromosome haplogroups of the three
19
Whether or not this has anything to do with archaeological parallels
between Maykop or Novosvobodnaya and the Sé Girdan tumuli near Lake
Urmia (see 3.3) is hard to say because Sé Girdan has no absolute date and
Leilatepe genomics is a blank spot. Nevertheless, genetic data suggest that the
migration vector was directed northwards, from Iran to the northern
Caucasus.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
322
Alexander Kozintsev
Novosvobodnaya males (J2a1 and G2a2a) are likewise
“southern”. The G2 subclade was found in a very early (eighth
millennium BC) Neolithic sample from Central Zagros, Iran
(Broushaki et al. 2016) and in one half of the Neolithic (seventh
millennium BC) samples from northwestern Anatolia — the
place whence this haplogroup spread to the Balkans with the
“Neolithization package” including AF (Mathieson et al. 2015).
G2 is also present in a Kura-Araxes male whereas the second
one carried the J haplogroup (Wang et al. 2019).
In sum, genetic data lend no support whatever to the idea
that Novosvobodnaya is “less southern” or “more European”
than Maykop. On the contrary, ties with Iran and Kura-Araxes
are even more salient here. Despite certain Central European
cultural traits mentioned by proponents of the cultural
distinctness of Novosvobodnaya (Nikolaeva, Safronov 1974;
Rezepkin 2000; Klejn 2010: 308–322), these people are
genetically quite far from those associated with Central
European cultures such as Baalberge and Globular Amphorae
(Wang et al. 2019: 4, fig. 2c). The principal difference consists in
the virtually complete absence of CHG in Central Europe
20
before the Yamnaya migration.
4.2.2. CHG versus AF
CHG was likewise absent in Neolithic Anatolia, where
only the AF component was present; but from the Chalcolithic
onward, Anatolian autosomal genomes consist of AF and CHG
in an approximately equal proportion (Damgaard et al. 2018: 16,
fig. 3). The influx of CHG to central and western Anatolia and
eventually to southeastern Europe — areas with the highest
concentration of AF — began in the Late Neolithic or
20
One might add that the Baden samples are virtually indistinguishable from
those relating to the Neolithic cultures of Hungary such as LBK and Lengyel
(Gamba et al. 2014; Lipson et al. 2017) and likewise include no CHG
component (Jones and al. 2015), implying that Baden had nothing to do with
the Indo-Europeanization of Anatolia, its striking archaeological parallels
with Troy notwithstanding. Were it otherwise, the high content of CHG in
Hittites, the Yamnaya people, and Central and Western European groups after
the Yamnaya expansion would be hard to interpret. In other words, genetic
facts are at odds not only with those parts of the Kurgan theory that concern
PAs, but also with the Central European homeland theory.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
323
Chalcolithic, however not from the steppe (because it is
unaccompanied by the steppe component EHG) but from
eastern Anatolia, the southern Caucasus or Iran, where its
share was very high (Hofmanová et al. 2016; Lazaridis et al.
2017; Mathieson et al. 2018; Raveane et al. 2018). The Late
Chalcolithic (fourth millennium BC) sample from Arslantepe,
evidently correlating with the Amuq F (CFW) horizon, differs
from other Anatolian samples by having a higher amount of
CHG (Skourtanioti & Selim 2018). The Late Chalcolithic (4500–
3900 BC) population from Israel differs from Neolithic
populations of the same area by the presence of an Iranian
autosomal component (close to CHG), whose amount is
estimated at 17 % (Harney et al. 2018).
Interestingly, the frequency of the allele associated with
blue eye color is quite high (49 %) in this population (ibid.),
although the source of gene flow was Iran. In modern Iranians,
the blue-eyed phenotype is usually attributed to a Late Bronze
Age (Andronovo) migration from the steppe. Had the Late
Chalcolithic people from the Levant (and, respectively, from
Iran) retained the genetic memory of a much earlier migration
by the same route?
The western part of the southern ancestry, AF, which had
been predominant in entral, Western and Southeastern
Europe including Tripolye before the Yamnaya people migrated
westwards, was introduced there by the early Neolithic farmers
from western Anatolia (Lazaridis et al. 2014; Haak et al. 2015;
Mathieson et al. 2015, 2018; Hofmanová et al. 2016; Lipson et al.
2017). AF is abundant in the Kura-Araxes, pre-Maykop, and
Maykop people but generally rare in the steppe, even in the
steppe Maykop (Wang et al. 2019, fig. 2a; see
http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2019/02/the-steppe-maykopenigma.html, accessed June 3, 2019). One striking exception is
th
an early 5 millennium BC male from Dereivka on the Dnieper,
whose ancestry is entirely composed of AF (Mathieson et al.
2018, fig. 1d), evidencing ties with one of the farming cultures
21
of Southeastern or Central Europe. The other 29 Neolithic
21
His Y-chromosome haplogroup is the “southern” H1 rather than a subclade
of U4 and U5 as in other Neolithic Dereivka males and in those from
Khvalynsk (https://amtdb.org/records/, accessed April 13, 2019).
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
324
Alexander Kozintsev
individuals from Ukraine show a predominantly EHG ancestry
with a small share of WHG (Western Hunter-Gatherers), but
none has any CHG.
The four Eneolithic (Sredni Stog and later) individuals
th
from Ukraine (Dereivka and Aleksandriya, 4 – early 3d
millennia BC) are genetically quite different, showing a mixture
of CHG, EHG, WHG, and AF; in two of these males, the Ychromosome haplogroups belong to subclades of R1a and R1b
(Mathieson et al. 2018, fig. 1d; Wang et al. 2019, fig. 2c;
Anthony 2019; https://amtdb.org/records/, accessed April 13,
2019).
If Central European Neolithic or Chalcolithic groups such
as that associated with the Globular Amphorae culture rather
than the standard AF samples from Anatolia are used as proxies
in admixture models, the fit is better, and the amount of AF in
22
Yamnaya groups rises to 13–17 % (Wang et al. 2019). The
share of AF is even higher in Middle to Late Bronze Age steppe
populations suh as Sintashta, Andronovo, and Srubnaya
(Mathieson et al. 2015), evidently marking the presence of a
Central European component in those cultures. In other words,
in the steppe belt, AF appears to mark migrations from the
west, not from the south. These migrations occurred mostly at
the later stages of the IE dispersal, not at the PIE stage.
Can the eastern part of the southern ancestry — CHG —
provide a gauge of PIE dispersal? To some extent, yes, but the
connection is not intrinsic: having emerged no later than the
Upper Paleolithic, CHG is way older than PIE. They became
coupled only by migration. But non-PIE migrants from the
southern Caucasus, too, might have carried this component. By
22
In one of Davidski’s analyses, the best surrogate donor of the AF component
was a Neolithic sample from Blätterhöhle, Germany, dating to the fourth
millennium BC (http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2018/01/another-look-atgenetic-structure-of.html, accessed September 18, 2018). In this regard I
should mention very close craniometric parallels between a Funnel Beaker
(specifically Tiefstichkeramik) sample from Ostorf, Germany, and those from
a Yamnaya cemetery on the Ingulets, Ukraine, and, amazingly, from an
Okunevo (Chaa-Khol’) cemetery at Aimyrlyg, Tuva (Kozintsev 2008, 2009).
Interesting as they are, such parallels in no way support the obsolete theory
locating the IE homeland in Central Europe because in that case the massive
influx of CHG to Europe from the steppe following the Yamnaya expansion
would remain unexplained.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
325
collating genetic and archaeological data we may hope to
disentangle the evidence.
This is how David Reich interprets the facts: “Ancient
DNA available from this time in Anatolia shows no evidence of
steppe ancestry similar to that in the Yamnaya (although the
evidence here is circumstantial as no ancient DNA from the
Hittites themselves has yet been published). This suggests to
me that the most likely location of the population that first
spoke an Indo-European language was south of the Caucasus
Mountains, perhaps in present-day Iran or Armenia, because
ancient DNA from people who lived there matches what we
would expect for a source population both for the Yamnaya and
for ancient Anatolians. If this scenario is right, the population
sent one branch up into the steppe — mixing with steppe
hunter-gatherers in a one-to-one ratio (Reich implies two
autosomal components — local steppe EHG, from Eastern
Hunter-Gatherers, and CHG — A.K.) to become the Yamnaya as
described earlier — and another to Anatolia to found the
ancestors of people there who spoke languages such as Hittite”
(Reich 2018: 120; see also Mathieson et al. 2018). This is
precisely what I had written before these two publications
appeared (Kozintsev 2016).
Moreover, the evidence about the Hittites is no longer
circumstantial. After Reich’s book had been published, Eske
Willerslev’s team made us familiar with the results of their
recent study (Damgaard et al. 2018). They have demonstrated
that the autosomal genomes of five individuals from central
Anatolian cemeteries dating to the Old Kingdom period (those
buried were apparently Hittites) contained no steppe
component (EHG). Instead, like the Maykop and pre-Maykop
genomes, they revealed a mix of CHG and AF in an
approximately one-to-one proportion.
But while Hittite (and Balkan) genetics is the thorn in the
flesh for those advocating the Steppe Theory, their opponents
have to deal with their own stumbling block — the absence of
the southern variety of the AF component in Yamnaya.
4.2.3. What was the source of CHG in the Yamnaya gene pool?
The idea that CHG had been carried from the Caucasus to
the steppe by PIEs met severe criticism from David Wesolowski
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
326
Alexander Kozintsev
alias Davidski, who uncompromisingly disagrees with how
Reich and Willerslev interpret their findings. Based on his own
calculations, he claims that southern groups such as Maykop
and pre-Maykop had not contributed to the Yamnaya gene pool
whereas the steppe ancestry in Anatolians is possible (see, e.g..
https://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2018/11/big-deal-of-2018yamnaya-not-related-to.html; accessed November 5, 2018;
http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2019/02/on-maykop-ancestry-inyamnaya.html, assessed May 29, 2019). In his words, “Yamnaya
gets its CHG from an earlier population still, that was living in
the steppe during the Meshoko period and probably much
earlier. It does look like the CHG in Yamnaya is native to the
steppe, and was present there since the Mesolithic”
(Wesolowski, pers. comm.).
Another, though milder, critic of Reich and Willerslev is
David Anthony (2019), who points to the fact that CHG is
heavily admixed with the Anatolian version of AF in all
Caucasian samples discussed above, in strong contrast to
Yamnaya, where only the European variety of AF is found. The
sole exception are three individuals from the Eneolithic
kurgans in the north-central Caucasian piedmont, dating to the
second half of the fifth millennium — two males from Progress
2, situated between Pyatigorsk and Nalik, and a female from
Vonyuchka 1 near Pyatigorsk (Korenevsky 2012: 43; Greski &
Berezina 2012). All three show a mixture of CHG and EHG in
an approximately 7:3 proportion, but no AF, at least not in its
Anatolian version (Wang et al. 2019, fig.2a). In this respect they
are similar to the Yamnaya samples. The Y-chromosome
haplogroups of the males belong to the R1b haplogroup, typical
of Yamnaya, but to its extremely rare subtype, namely R-V1636,
also registered in a Kura-Araxes male from Armenia (in
Davidski’s view, this may be a signal of proto-Hittites
migrating southwards from the steppe to Anatolia via
Caucasus, see http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2018/, accessed
May 28, 2019).
An anonymous comment in the Supplementary
Information to Wang et al. 2019:36 reads, “Complementary to
the southern Eneolithic component, a northern component
started to expand between 4300 and 4100 calBCE manifested in
low burial mounds with inhumations densely packed in bright
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
327
red ochre. Burial sites of this type, like the investigated sites of
Progress and Vonyuchka, are found in the Don-Caspian steppe,
but they are related to a much larger supra-regional network
linking elites of the steppe zone between the Balkans and the
Caspian Sea. These groups introduced the so-called kurgan, a
specific type of burial monument, which soon spread across the
entire steppe zone.” Given the very early appearance of the
kurgan tradition in Transcaucasia, specifically in Armenia (see
3.4), and the fact that very little is known about the culture of
Progress and Vonyuchka, caution must be applied.
Notably, very little AF is found in the genomes of the
steppe Maykop people. Amazingly, according to Davidski, their
makeup can be modeled as a nearly equal mix of two
components — Piedmont Eneolithic (Progress and Vonyuchka)
and Western Siberian Upper Paleolithic such as Ust’-Ishim —
plus a small share of a Southwestern Asian Eneolithic
component
akin
to
Geoksyur
and
Sarazm
(http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2019/02/the-steppe-maykopenigma.html, accessed June 6, 2019). Unlike Maykop proper,
then, the steppe Maykop reveals a distinctly eastern rather than
southern ancestry (see http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2018/10/
steppe-maykop-buffer-zone.html, accessed June 6, 2019). It
could hardly be the source of CHG in the Yamnaya gene pool.
4.2.4. Khvalynsk comes into play
Another sample showing a blend of EHG with CHG with
little or no AF consists of three males from Khvalynsk, whose
Y-chromosome haplogroups are the “Indo-European” R1a and
R1b as well as U4, as in Neolithic Dereivka (Wang et al. 2019,
fig. 2c). Regrettably, the genetic makeup of these three
individuals is not clear because in another study by Reich’s
team, Narasimhan et al. (2018, Supplementary Online Materials:
144) write: “On the ADMIXTURE plot, we note that the
Khvalynsk samples show additional components that are
maximized in Anatolian and Iranian agriculturalists” (see ibid.:
23
145, fig. S3.21, right). Whether present or not, the share of AF
23
The discrepancy between the two studies from the same lab apparently
stems from the fact that Wang et al. assess the proportion of CHG whereas
Narasimhan et al. speak of a component related to the “Iranian
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
328
Alexander Kozintsev
in Khvalynsk is much smaller than in Maykop or DarkvetiMeshoko.
In the words of David Anthony (2019), “Without any
doubt, a CHG population that was not admixed with Anatolian
Farmers mated with EHG populations in the Volga steppes and
in the North Caucasus steppes before 4500 BC. We can refer to
this admixture as pre-Yamnaya, because it makes the best
currently known genetic ancestor for EHG/CHG R1b Yamnaya
genomes.” As Anthony believes, the admixture could have
begun as early as 6200 BC, when the first hunter-fisher camps
appeared on the Lower Volga. Davidski, too, believes that
Khvalynsk
is
ancestral
to
Yamnaya
(https://eurogenes.blogspot.com/search?q=Khvalynsk, accessed
June 5, 2019). Unlike him, however, Anthony doesn’t claim that
CHG was native to the steppe (indeed, its distribution range
prevents us from believing this because Khvalynsk is situated
on the remote periphery of that range). Instead, in Anthony’s
opinion, CHG was introduced to the steppe by migrants from
northwestern Iran or Azerbaijan, and the migration route
passed along the western Caspian coast.
According to Anthony, the pre-Maykop (DarkvetiMeshoko) people spoke Northwest Caucasian languages and
did not mix with the steppe population whereas those who
introduced CHG to Khvalynsk spoke “very archaic eastern
Caucasian languages.” Alan Bomhard (2019b) subscribed to this
view although it is inconsistent with the idea that the North
Caucasian component is a substrate, as he claims following
Uhlenbeck and Kortlandt. If Anthony’s scenario holds, then the
substrate is the Eurasiatic (“pre-Uralic”, as he calls it)
component associated with EHG whereas the southern
component, associated with CHG, is a superstrate.
After these very important concessions, and despite the
fact that Anthony, the mounting evidence to the contrary
notwithstanding, still insists on the Balkan route of the proto24
Anatolian migration from the steppe, the Steppe Theory is no
agriculturalists from the Zagros”. Components evaluated that way are not the
same. Note added in proof: In the final version (Science 365 (6457), 2019), only
the Iranian component is mentioned, but not AF.
24
So does Davidski (https://eurogenes.blogspot.com/search?q=Anatolians,
accessed May 30, 2019).
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
329
longer what it used to be: now it incorporates the Caucasian
prologue.
Nonetheless, problems immediately arise. First, the
Caucasian component is supposed to be Northwest Caucasian,
not Northeast Caucasian. Second, none of them seemed to exist
at that time — judging by S. Starostin’s glottochronological
estimate, common North Caucasian split only in the mid-fourth
millennium BC. And third, most importantly, it is logical to
assume that the EHG/CHG ratio mirrors the proportion of
Eurasiati-speaking
natives
versus
Caucasian-speaking
immigrants. Now the share of CHG in Yamnaya samples nearly
equals that of EHG, which Anthony attributes to “pre-Uralians”
(Wang et al. 2019, fig. 4b). Should one conclude that migrants
from the south, who evidently introduced pastoralism to the
region, adopted the language of the local foragers en masse?
This is hard to believe. But if not, then why has the IE family
remained an undeniable member of the Eurasiatic stock rather
then becoming a hybrid situated halfway between the
Eurasiatic and the North Caucasian macrofamilies or even a
member of the Caucasian macrofamily with some Eurasiatic
tendency (see the two-dimensional projection of relationships
between families in Kozintsev 2018, fig. 11, showing nothing of
the kind)? Pace Trubetzkoy, such views can hardly be revived
at present. In sum, the PIE attribution of people who carried
CHG is much more probable, and the Western Caucasian
substrate couldn’t have been received that way.
4.2.5. Autosomal components versus Y-chromosome haplogroups
One must agree with the critics of Reich, who dispute a
direct link between Maykop and Yamnaya. Oleg Balanovsky
(2019), without mentioning the CHG/AF controversy, writes
that the link was indirect — the recipient was, not Yamnaya,
but some earlier steppe population. He believes that this
th
component was introduced in the mid-5 millennium BC or
somewhat earlier by late PIEs, who had migrated to
northwestern Caucasus along the eastern coast of the Black
Sea. If so, pre-Maykop would, theoretically, be the best
candidate as a donor. The recipient remains unknown, and the
mysterious disappearance of AF remains unexplained unless we
invoke some bizarre explanation such as natural selection
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
330
Alexander Kozintsev
against the Anatolian variety of AF in the steppe. The
Eneolithic sample from Ukraine, which does contain AF (see
4.2.2), may be a candidate (Anthony 2019), but the provenance
of AF in this group is unknown.
To this one must add that Y-chromosome data contradict
those relating to the autosomal genome. Whereas the latter
unambiguously suggest that the “southern” CHG component
was introduced to the steppe from the Caucasus on a large
scale, none of the likewise “southern” Y-haplogroups J and L,
registered in the pre-Maykop and Maykop people (Wang et al.
2018), followed suit — most haplogroups of the later IEs of the
steppe belonged to the R1 subclades.
Which of the R1 variants present in Khvalynsk, whether
R1a or R1b, showing very different distribution patterns and
very dissimilar evolutionary histories, should be regarded as
the IE marker, is not clear. R1a was thought to be preferable for
tracing IE migrations (Zerjal et al. 1999). If so, why is it rare in
Western Europe? And why are the Yamnaya people, whose
expansion is said to have been crucial for the formation of the
Bronze Age gene pool of Europe, and their close relatives, the
Afanasievo people of eastern Central Asia, marked by R1b
rather than R1a (Haak et al. 2015; Allentoft et al. 2015)?
The situation with the Y-chromosome haplogroups in Iran
is disappointing for advocates of the R1a primacy since the
most frequent haplogroup there is J2a. As to R1a, “in spite of
the recent dissection of this haplogroup, none of the identified
sub-branches support a patrilineal gene flow from western
Eurasia through southern Asia ascribable to the diffusion of
Indo-European languages. Accordingly, the present analysis of
the Iranian R1a Y-chromosomes does not provide useful
information to disentangle this issue” (Grugni et al. 2012: 11).
Recently it was claimed that R1a is a reliable indicator of the IE
migration to India (Silva et al. 2017). But the estimated age of
the South Asian subclades of this haplogroup is relatively
young and indicates Andronovo migration rather than PIE
25
dispersal (ibid.; see also Narasimhan et al. 2018).
25
However small the amount of steppe ancestry in Iran and India may be, the
close linguistic affinity of Indo-Iranian with other filial branches of late PIE
disproves the idea that proto-Aryans migrated eastwards directly from the
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
331
Should one conclude that R1b, carried by the Yamnaya
and Afanasievo males, was an inherently PIE trait? Its
predominance in the Bell Beaker people (Olalde et al. 2018)
might suggest that, but what role should be ascribed to R1a in
that case? Also, variants of R1b in Central and Western Europe
differ from those in Yamnaya (Kivisild 2017; Balanovsky 2019).
More importantly, regarding any of the R1 clades as a PIE
marker is tantamount to postulating that PIE originated in the
steppe — an idea contradicted by all the available evidence. The
distribution of these clades in modern Europe does not warrant
the use of any of them as an IE marker (Balanovsky 2015: 71–
72, 80–94, 107). According to Balanovsky et al. (2013: 30) “the
‘Indo-European marker’ does not exist, simply because the first
population to speak Proto-Indo-European must have possessed
a spectrum of haplogroups which were shared (or identical)
with its sister and neighbor populations that spoke other
languages.”
5. Putting bits and pieces together
5.1. The Balkan corridor: the final assessment
Facts from three independent disciplines speak decisively
against the Balkan scenario envisaged by Marija Gimbutas and
her followers at least in its strong version (migration of PAs).
What about the weak version (diffusion of PA)? Why not
assume that this language was a remote forerunner of Greek,
which spread to the Balkans from the north with minor
population replacement (Lazaridis et al. 2017), or Armenian,
26
which was transmitted virtually without gene flow?
primary IE homeland in the Near East, as Gamkrelidze and Ivanov believed
(see also Heggarty 2018). If this were true, Indo-Iranian would have split from
th
other descendants of late (steppe) PIE already in the early 4 millennium BC,
which is quite unlikely, especially in view of the facts indicating the steppe
origin of an earlier branch — Tocharian (see 5.6). Contra Heggarty (2018), the
presence of “farming” cognates in Indo-Iranian and Tocharian (Mallory 2018;
Peyrot 2018) does not rule out the steppe as the secondary (late PIE)
homeland. Rather, it points to an earlier homeland in or adjacent to the
agricultural zone of the Near East.
26
To all apperances, Armenian was adopted mostly by Hurrians of the Upper
Euphrates after 1200 BC from speakers of a Paleo-Balkan IE language
belonging to the Thraco-Phrygian branch, who had migrated to Anatolia from
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
332
Alexander Kozintsev
The answer is that, firstly, not only the genetic but also
the archaeological evidence of the southward spread via the
Balkans is virtually lacking until the Late Bronze Age.
Secondly, the lexicon referring to cliffs, rocks, precipices, swift
rivers, etc. (see 2.2) could emerge neither on the steppe nor on
the western Black Sea coast. And finally, Indo-Hittite was likely
spoken by people who, at some stage of their history, were
neighbors of Semites. Such people could have lived neither on
the steppe nor on the Danube nor in the Balkans unless we
invent ad hoc possibilities such as, for instance, that Tripolye
people spoke an Afroasiatic language (Anthony 2007: 304–395)
or that borrowing occurred through intermediaries like preKartvelian or North Caucasian (ibid.: 98) or that contacts
occurred along distant trade routes, some of them by sea (S.
Starostin 2007 (2000): 819). Or, by way of brushing these
isoglosses aside, one may stigmatize them as chance
coincidences, relics of the common Nostratic–Afroasiatic stage
or Wanderwörter (Kassian 2010: 426). Such explanations appear
problematic because it was statistically demonstrated using the
latest version of the STARLING database, from which all the
false coincidences had been purged, that proto-IE had
maintained contact primarily with Semitic rather than with any
of the three Caucasian families (Kozintsev 2018). Clearly, this in
no way disproves the Western Caucasian substrate hypothesis,
which is based on entirely different data. In my view, the most
natural explanation of my own results is that at least one of the
two groups, either the PIEs or the Semites, took a direct route
from the Near East to the northern Caucasus. Which of the two
did that?
5.2. PIEs, Semites, North Caucasians
According to Nadezhda Nikolaeva and Vladimir Safronov
(1974, 1982), the migrants were Semites, whom they associated
with the Maykop people proper. The Novosvobodnaya people,
in their view, were PIEs, who had moved to the Caucasus from
their presumed Central European homeland. There are two
grains of truth in this scenario. One is that the Maykop culture
the Balkans (Diakonoff 1984). According to Haber et al. (2016), no population
mixture occurred in Armenia after 1200 BC.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
333
is so manifestly “southern” that even if it wasn’t related to
Semites, then, perhaps, to their neighbors. The second is that
Novosvobodnaya, as Nikolaeva and Safronov have convincingly
demonstrated, cannot be regarded as merely the late stage of
Maykop. Rather, these are two distinct cultures differing by
origin. Genetic facts suggest, however, that Novosvobodnaya
had likewise southern, albeit somewhat different roots (see
4.2.1).
If the genetic specificity of Novosvobodnaya vis-à-vis
Maykop and its affinity with Kura-Araxes receive support (see,
e.g. Lyonnet 2007: 149, and Courcier 2014, for archaeological
parallels), it would be tempting to explore the possibility that
the Novosvobodnaya people, unlike those of Maykop, spoke a
Northwest Caucasian language. Interestingly, the applied
bosses, decorating the Novosvobodnaya ceramics, resemble the
interior-punched nodes, with which the coarse pottery, typical
of the upper strata of the Kuban fortresses and apparently
evidencing the local component (see 3.2), was decorated
(Lyonnet 2007: 149). Could this attest to continuity between
proto-North Caucasian and Northwest Caucasian speakers?
Novosvobodnaya is too late (no earlier than 3600 BC and
thus mostly contemporaneous with Kura-Araxes) to be
attributed to PIEs. The Hurro-Urartian, resp. East Caucasian
(Diakonoff & Starostin 1986) attribution of Kura-Araxes
(Burney 1978: 132; Diakonoff 1990: 62–63; Kassian 2010: 423) is
plausible given its expansion to Daghestan (Kohl &
Magomedov 2014). According to Sergei Starostin (2007/1995:
546), the glottochronological estimate of the disintegration of
common North Caucasian is mid-fourth millennium BC —
27
precisely when Novosvobodnaya and Kura-Araxes originated.
27
Marro et al. (2014), however, claim that Kura-Araxes is earlier than
previously thought. Indeed, at Ovçular Tepesi, the Kura-Araxes ware was
found in the same late fifth millennium BC context as the CFW, and the same
is true of the early fourth millennium strata at Berikldeebi. These findings, as
they believe, indicate a prolonged coexistence of two culturally distinct
populations. The coexistence, however, is questioned by Palumbi &
Chataigner (2015), who adhere to the traditional idea that Kura-Araxes
originated around 3500 BC. In any case, the cultural distinctness of KuraAraxes vis-à-vis CFW is no less evident than that of Novosvobodnaya vis-àvis Maykop proper.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
334
Alexander Kozintsev
Anthony (2007: 98) proposed that the Kura-Araxes people
had spoken pre-proto-Kartvelian, and that their contact with
the presumed PIEs of the steppe was maintained through the
North Caucasian speaking Maykop people including those of
Novosvobodnaya. This complex construction, based on the
remark by Johanna Nichols and postulating at least two non-IE
intermediaries between PIE and proto-Semitic, viz., Maykop
(including Novosvobodnaya) and Kura-Araxes, appears to have
been invented solely to avoid a simple but inconvenient
possibility of a direct contact between PIEs and Semites. My
lexicostatistical analysis has not revealed the slightest trace of a
proto-Kartvelian influence on PIE.
According to Anthony (2019), both the Darkveti-Meshoko
and the Maykop people spoke Northwest Caucasian languages
(see Kassian 2010 for the same view). But if the DarkvetiMeshoko people, who had migrated from the south and built
the fortresses like Meshoko south of the Kuban were Northwest
Caucasians, then who were those against whom these
fortresses were meant to protect and who lived there during the
late stages (Stolyar et al. 2009; Kozintsev 2017)? They were not
steppe dwellers because the third component, traced in the
middle layers of the fortresses, is reliably associated with the
steppe cultures like Sredny Stog and Skelya/Novodanilovka
(ibid.).
If, on the other hand, Northwest Caucasian speakers were
the Novosvobodnya people (likely descendants of those who
manufactured the punched-node pottery at the late stages of
Darkveti-Meshoko) whereas Maykop proper is late PIE, the
respective substrate in PIE (if real) would have a much more
realistic explanation than that suggested by Kortland (2018),
Bomhard (2019) and Anthony (2019), who link the CHG
autosomal component exclusively with North Caucasian
speakers. As mentioned above, the proportion of CHG in
Yamnaya, indisputably associated with late PIEs, is much too
high to ascribe it to Caucasian speakers, whether eastern or
western.
What about the possible association between CHG and
PIEs? Judging by archaeological data, the pre-Maykop
population, from which a yet unknown Chalcolithic steppe
group might theoretically have received CHG (Balanovsky
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
335
28
2019), did not spread to the steppe on a large scale. Are the
cultural parallels with the steppe at pre-Maykop fortresses a
sufficient testimony of incipient contacts? The MaykopYamnaya link is doubtful on genetic grounds (see 4.2). Were the
ties between Maykop and the steppe people largely cultural
(and possibly linguistic) rather than genetic (Anthony 2019)?
Should we focus on ties between northwestern Iran and
Khvalynsk, as Anthony and his predecessors suggest? Hardly
so. Language spread can occur without gene flow, and cultural
evidence of contact is no less informative than genetic
evidence.
5.3. The PIE migrations: tracking the routes
As I see it, the simplest and the most natural option is to
assume that the migrants to the northern Caucasus were PIEs,
whose routes passed through the Near East whereas the
primary source of migration was located somewhere closer to
the place where proto-Uralic was spoken in the fifth
millennium BC immediately before its disintegration (Hajdú
1964; Helimski 2001; Napolskikh 1997: 135; Janhunen 2003;
Blaek 2013b). While the Indo-Uralic macrofamily cannot yet be
considered a clade in the strict taxonomic sense, though the
lexicostatistical data do suggest that, the predominantly
Siberian ties of PIE can hardly be doubted (see 2.1.1). If so, the
source of migration should be located in some intermediate
place — southern Ural or eastern Caspian.
The lexicostatistical analysis using the minimum spanning
tree method has revealed an amazing fact — the shortest path
between IE and Uralic connects Hittite with Ugric (Kozintsev
2018). According to a nearly consensus view, Hittite is the most
archaic IE language (see, e.g. Ringe et al. 2002). The Ob Ugric
languages, on the other hand, are closest to the center of the
Uralic distribution area and, therefore, to the presumed Uralic
28
This possibility was discussed in Davidski’s Eurogenes Blog with regard to
my
article
about
Meshoko
(Kozintsev
2017)
(http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2018/01/the-case-of-chalcolithic-fortressesin.html, accessed 18 September 2018).
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
336
Alexander Kozintsev
29
and possibly Indo-Uralic homeland (see Kloekhorst 2008 on
the supposedly Indo-Uralic aspects of Hittite).
Yakhontov (1991), based on climatic and biotic terms,
concluded that the Nostratic homeland could be located
anywhere from the Volga drainage to Western Siberia
“somewhat earlier than 15 thousand years ago”. Pagel et al.
(2013) arrived at broadly the same estimate (see 2.2). Kortlandt
(2002) placed the Indo-Uralic homeland south of the Ural
Mountains in the seventh millennium BC.
On the other hand, the analysis of reconstructed PIE terms
for topographical features and related attributes and beliefs
suggests that at some stage of their history, the Indo-Hittites
lived in a mountainous terrain, where the mountain was a
“mighty cliff reaching to the sky”, and there were precipices,
swift rivers and a sea or large lake nearby (Gamkrelidze, Ivanov
1995: 574–577; Dybo 2013), excluding the steppe as a possible
homeland (see 2.2).
Provided that all these geographic features must be found
in broadly the same area, this leaves us, in essence, with two
possibilities. We must discard the southern Urals, where the
most salient mountain, Yamantau, 1640 m high, is situated 900
km north of the Caspian Sea. One of the remaining options is
the area with three large lakes, Sevan, Van, and Urmia, two of
which are saline and thus similar to seas, and Mount Ararat,
5137 m high, towering 120 km southwest of Lake Sevan
(freshwater) and at about the same distance northeast of Lake
Van (saline). But an even better option is northern Iran, where
the majestic dome of Damavand, 5610 m high — the second
highest dormant volcano in Asia — can be viewed directly from
30
the Caspian shore, and tumultuous rivers, most notably
Safidrud, break through the Elburz range. It appears likely,
then, that the Indo-Hittites migrated to the southern Caucasus
and further west along the Elburz and Zagros ranges by the
29
This conclusion is supported by the evidence of physical anthropology. The
combination of highly diagnostic cranial traits distinguishing Uralic groups
from all others is expressed most strongly in Ob Ugrians (Kozintsev 2004)
30
The Caspian Sea meets not only the weak condition of just “body of water”
but also the strong, though possibly not sufficiently convincing condition of
“heaving surf, raging watery element” (Gamkrelidze, Ivanov 1995: 581). Lake
Sevan and Lake Van, however, can be stormy too.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
337
route that, several millennia later, was known as the Great
Khorasan Road (Majidzadeh 1982; Ivanova 2012; 2013: 121–129).
Interestingly, Vyacheslav Ivanov did not always advocate
the Near Eastern theory of IE origin. In the 1960s he claimed
tht certain Anatolian texts retain reminiscences of a migration
from the north. This, as he believed, is exemplified by Hittite
and Palaian prayers to the sun god Sius, who is said to rise from
the sea or large lake, aruna- (Ivanov 2001/1963: 10–11). In his
words, such texts might refer to the western coasts of the Black
Sea, of the Caspian Sea, or of Lake Van. He notes that in Hittite
annals of the New Kingdom period, the word aruna- evidently
referred to a large lake situated east of the original territory of
the Hittites and possibly of the Anatolians in general (because
the same myth is found in Palaic). In a later study, Gamkrelidze
& Ivanov (1995: 792) did not mention the Black Sea but added
Lake Urmia. Anthony (2007: 262), on the contrary, mentions
only the Black Sea — the sole possibility reconcilable with the
Steppe Theory except that the highest mountain of the Balkans,
Musala, just 2925 m high, is 350 km away from the Black Sea
coast.
5.4. The first homeland
Johanna Nichols (1997, 1998) suggested that the IE
homeland was situated east of the Caspian Sea, from whence
the PIE language spread in the western direction in two streams
— northern (toward the western steppe) and southern (toward
Anatolia). If so, then the southwestern mountainous coast of
the Caspian Sea is one of the places which the PAs passed on
their way to Asia Minor. This hypothesis accounts for the early
separation of the Anatolian branch but not for the “Semitic”
tendency of the entire IE family, revealed by the
lexicostatistical analysis. Nor does it explain why common IE
words referring to high mountains and swift rivers are found in
languages originating from the presumed northern (steppe)
stream.
Nichols eventually retracted her theory. In her words, she
did that with much regret because “(i)t's a beautiful theory that
accounts elegantly for a great deal of the dynamic and
linguistic geography of the IE spread, but it conflicts with
essential
archaeological
and
etymological
facts”
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
338
Alexander Kozintsev
(https://www.academia.edu/28869625/The_epicenter_of_the_In
do-European_linguistic_spread, accessed September 18, 2018;
see also Nichols 2019). The reason for this “partial retraction”
was the article by Bill Darden (2001), who had advocated his
version of the Kurgan theory — the hypothesis of a PA
migration from the steppe along the western Black Sea coast.
However, as seen today, it is precisely Nichols’s initial
idea of the east Caspian center and, respectively, of the eastern
origin of PA that appears worth being preserved. Other parts of
the theory, stemming from the questionable postulate that the
number of migrations accounting for the language spread must
be minimized, are hardly tenable. This concerns, for instance,
the idea that “(t)he PIE ecological and cultural world (…)
included the forested mountains southeast of the Kazakh
steppe, the dry eastern steppes, the Central Asian deserts, the
urbanized oases of southern Turkmenistan and Bactria–
Sogdiana, the eastern extension of the urban Near East, the rich
grasslands of the Black Sea steppe, the southern edge zone and
the Siberian taiga, fresh-water lakes, and salt seas (the Aral and
31
Caspian)” (Nichols 1998: 233). Beautiful or otherwise, this is
but a fantasy engendered in a vacuum and supported by a
strange claim that language spread should not correlate with
specific archaeological cultures.
To understand why the PIE lexicon had absorbed so many
vastly incompatible geographic and cultural realities, we
needn’t imagine a boundless and infinitely diverse territory
inhabited by numerous groups speaking one and the same
31
Commenting on the draft of my next article, Allan Bomhard wrote,
“Perhaps we (all of us, not just those involved in this session) have been too
narrow-minded. What if the Indo-European tribes were more widely
dispersed than previously thought?…. What if the original homeland of PIE,
or better, pre-PIE ("Proto-Indo-Hittite") was actually "circum Pontic"? And,
what if there were cultural, linguistic, and genetic interactions between these
various Indo-Hittite tribes themselves and between neighboring peoples —
North
Caucasian,
Hurrian-Urartean,
Semitic,
etc.?”
(https://www.academia.edu/s/d8be18e175/proto-ie-meets-semitic-moredetails-2019pdf?source=work, accessed March 24, 2019). Similar ideas about
small PIE groups widely scattered across the “Circum-Pontic province” were
long ago voiced by Evgeny Chernykh (1987). The tacit assumption of such
explanations is that for some inconceivable reason such a dispersal pattern
did not result in the breakup of PIE.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
339
proto-language. How could PIE possibly remain undivided, as it
did, under such conditions? If, however, the demand to
minimize the number of migrations is abandoned in favor of
32
the idea of multiple PIE homelands, then the connection
between the primary (Indo-Uralic) homeland and the secondary
PIE locus in northern Iran becomes a necessity — the first link
in the reconstructed chain.
Can we try and project this theoretical construct onto
archaeological reality? To do that, we must examine the area
between the southern Ural and Iran, i.e., the Transcaspian
region. The reason why we focus on the eastern rather than the
western Caspian coast is that during the period spanning the
Mesolithic and the Chalcolithic, the Urals had stronger cultural
ties with western Central Asia than with the Caucasus. Also,
the eastern route is shorter and straighter than the western one.
It can become even shorter if the sources and directions of
migrations are reconstructed from archaeological data.
One of the most important Transcaspian cultures is
Kelteminar — a Neolithic community southeast of the Aral Sea
with a foraging economy that was eventually supplemented by
animal breeding. The early stage of Kelteminar dates to 6200–
5400 BC, its middle stage, to 4000–3000 BC, and its late stage to
3000–2100 BC (Szymczak & Khudzhanazarov 2011; see also
Vinogradov 1981: 132; Korobkova 1996: 108). The hiatus
between the early and the middle stage was supposedly caused
by aridification.
The discoverer of Kelteminar, Sergei Tolstov (1941; 1948:
64–65) pointed to striking similarities between the Kelteminar
artifacts, especially pottery, and those of the Neolithic cultures
of the southern Urals and Western Siberia. The ties reach as far
as the Lower Ob, and the data suggest that Kelteminar was the
donor whereas Uralian and Siberian cultures were the
recipients of cultural innovations.
Tolstov’s conclusions were elaborated by Valery
Chernetsov (1953, 1960, 1968), who suggested that the
Kelteminar people were the ancestors of Finno-Ugrians or even
32
Obviously, the acceptance of this idea, manifested in the title of Vladimir
Safronov’s 1989 book, does not force us to accept the specific migration
trajectory he proposed.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
340
Alexander Kozintsev
undifferentiated Uralians. Chernetsov’s ideas or rather their
overstatements by his supporters were challenged by
33
Alexander Formozov (1972),
but several Russian
archaeologists including Vadim Masson (1999) agreed with
them (see Kovaleva & Zyrianova 2009, for a recent, positive
assessment of the archaeological evidence, suggesting that
contacts with the eastern Ural began during the early stage of
Kelteminar).
On the other hand, Tolstov (1941; 1948: 66) mentioned
certain traits linking Kelteminar with the cultures of southern
Turkmenia and northern Iran. This concerns, for instance, thinwalled well-fired bowls, on some of which traces of red paint
are preserved. The same applies to vessels with spouts. Tolstov
regarded those traits as evidence of ties with Anau.
Southern ties of the early Kelteminar were discussed in
more detail by Alexander Vinogradov (1957), who noted the
parallels between the decorative motifs of Kelteminar ceramics
and those of the western group of painted pottery sites of the
Namazga I horizon (fifth millennium BC). Ties are also
observed with more advanced early farming cultures of
southern Turkmenia such as Anau IA (fifth millennium) and
even late Jeitun (sixth millennium). Similar decorative motifs
include parallel wavy lines, “ladders”, zigzags, and hatched
figures. These motifs are common in the early Neolithic Kozlov
culture of the eastern Urals and western Siberia as well
(Kovaleva & Zyrianova 2009), the obvious difference being that
the pottery decoration in early Kelteminar and more northerly
cultures was incised whereas in southern Turkmenia and Iran it
was painted. At the middle Kelteminar stage, contemporaneous
with the Namazga II horizon, parallels with the south are no
longer traceable (Parzinger 2006: 151–152).
Pointing to even more southerly parallels, Tolstov (1948:
65–66) subscribed to the old hypothesis of affinities between
Dravidian and Uralic languages and regarded Kelteminar as a
33
Without touching upon ethnogenetic issues, Formozov conceded that the
impact of Kelteminar on the Uralian and Siberian Neolithic cultures was a fact
that could not be brushed aside. Alfred Khalikov (1969: 384–385), who
likewise disagreed with Tolstov and Chernetsov, erroneously believed that
parallels between Kelteminar and the eastern Urals were late and thus
relevant only to Ugrian origins.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
341
possible proof of such affinities. This idea, too, was supported
by Masson (1981: 115–118), who cited the views of the linguist
34
Mikhail Andronov (1965: 99–104). Masson’s key argument
was the discovery of two Harappan-type seals in the Namazga
V (late third – early second millennia BC) context at AltynDepe. On one of them, there were two signs matching those of
the Indus script (later, a Harappan seal was found at
another Bronze Age site in Turkmenia -- Gonur Depe).
Pointing to connections between Dravidian and Elamite,
Masson asked if Brahui — a Dravidian isolate in
Balochistan — could be a remnant of an ancient population
35
associated with the painted pottery culture of that region.
However, even if we agree with Andronov and others that
Uralic had had areal relationships with Dravidian, and even if
Yuri Knorozov and Asko Parpola were right when they claimed
that the Indus script was related to a Dravidian language, even
so, three late and possibly imported seals are not enough to
convince us that Dravidian had always been the native
language of the early farmers of southern Turkmenia. In fact,
the sharp discontinuity between the cultures of the Central
Plateau of Iran, specifically between those closest to AltynDepe — Sialk III and IV, indicating an intrusion of a ProtoElamite culture around 3400 BC (Seresti & Tashvigh 2016),
prevents us from regarding all earlier painted pottery cultures
as an Elamo-Dravidian legacy. Could some of them be
associated with PIEs, as Colin Renfrew (1987: 192; 1991) and
Peter Bellwood (2012) have suggested?
The positive answer to this question follows from much of
the evidence outlined above — primarily from the fact that the
IE homeland must be located south of the Uralic homeland, but
34
See Pudas Marlow 1974, for a later version of this hypothesis.
35
George Debetz (1980: 263), who carried out an anthropometric study of
numerous groups in Afghanistan, was certain that the Brahui were a remnant
of a pre-IE population. Having a somewhat darker pigmentation
compared to the neighboring IE groups of Balochistan, the Brahui are
indistinguishable from them both anthropometrically (ibid.) and
genetically (Pagani et al. 2017). While certain linguists regarded
Brahui as the earliest branch of the Dravidian family (Andronov
2003:23), others claimed that the Brahui had migrated to Balochistan from
central India less than 1500 years ago (Elfenbein 1987).
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
342
Alexander Kozintsev
also from the Iranian and southern Turkmenian parallels to
36
Maykop (Ivanova (2012; 2013: 108–118; 121–129). Despite the
apparent contrast between Maykop and the painted pottery
cultures of Turkmenia and Iran, the important fact is that the
transition between one of the painted pottery cultures in
northwestern Iran — –the Late Chalcolithic 1 (LC1) Pisdeli
culture and the CFW culture of LC2 and LC3 (the likely
forerunner of Maykop) — occurs without interruption around
4200 BC, as in certain areas of Mesopotamia (Abedi et al. 2015),
prompting certain experts to look for economic rather than
ethnic factors behind this change (Palumbi 2011; Marro 2012).
Undocumented and undated as it is, the Fullol vessel (see 3.4)
provides yet another link between Maykop and the painted
pottery cultures of southern Turkmenia.
Leonid Sverkov (2011: 171) has attributed all eastern
Caspian Neolithic cultures such as Jeitun, Kelteminar, and
related Aidabol to filial IE groups, implying a much too early
disintegration of PIE. But given the northern Siberian affinities
of Kelteminar, one should agree with Tolstov and Chernetsov,
who related it to the Uralians. If so, then Jeitun, situated south
of Kelteminar and contemporaneous with its early stage, and
the fifth millennium BC horizons in the same area — Anau IA
and Namazga I — can be associated with the Indo-Hittites.
Interestingly, Davidski, who has detected a component relating
to the early Southwestern Central Asian farmers (Geoksyur–
Sarazm) in the steppe Maykop gene pool, suggested that the
Western Siberian component, which is also present there, may
stem from Kelteminar, whose genomics is yet unknown
36
Mariya Ivanova (2013: 123–124) writes about the westward extension of the
Central Asian network into north and west Iran during the Namazga II-III
period. This, in her view, marked the beginning of the route that led to the
Urmia Plain and from there, along the Kura basin, to northern Caucasus. The
idea is appealing in principle, but one must keep in mind that the radiocarbon
dates for Namazga II and III used by Western experts (Ivanova 2013: XVII,
Table 2) are 300–400 years earlier than those generated at the SaintPetersburg laboratory (Alekshin et al. 2005: 530, 539). If the former are
correct, then Namazga II (but not III) can theoretically be indirectly ancestral
to Maykop. If the latter are correct, then one should address underlying
horizons such as Namazga I, which undeniably dates to the fifth millennium.
Given the cultural continuity in southern Turkmenia, the issue isn’t critical.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
343
(http://eurogenes.blogspot.com/2019/02/the-steppe-maykopenigma.html, accessed June 6, 2019).
Vadim Masson (1971: 62–64) and Galina Korobkova (1996:
97) pointed to the affinity between the lithic industry of Jeitun
and the southern and southeastern Caspian Mesolithic
industries such as Jebel 6-5 and Kamarband 11-10. Kelteminar,
on the other hand, may have originated from the local
Mesolithic cultures with microlithic industries (Korobkova
1996: 110). It follows that certain Mesolithic cultures of western
Central Asia could be associated with Indo-Uralic speakers.
Before the relatively recent desertification, the climate of this
region was much more humid, and large areas of what are now
the Karakum and Kyzylkum deserts were occupied by steppes.
The 550-km-long and currently dry channel of the Uzboy (the
western extension of Amu-Darya) was a wide and deep river.
Judging by the number of sites, dozens and possibly hundreds
of human populations thrived on its banks (Korobkova 1996).
How can we reconcile this unexpectedly southern location
of the presumed Indo-Uralic homeland and an even more
southerly placement of the PIE locus with the exclusively
Siberian ties of PIE within Greenberg’s Eurasiatic macrofamily?
Do these Siberian ties imply that the Nostratic homeland must
be sought near the southern Urals, as Yakhontov (1991)
proposed? Not necessarily! Yakhontov pointed to common
Nostratic lexemes denoting snow and ice. But even the present
climate of Turkmenia is extremely continental with winter
temperatures dropping below –20 º. In the mountains of
northern Iran, too, the winters are cold and snowy. In addition,
common Nostratic was spoken during the final Pleistocene,
when the climate was considerably colder. In the early
Holocene, judging by the similarity of microlithic industries,
periods of aridification forced the Mesolithic inhabitants of
southwestern Turkmenia and northern Iran to migrate
northwards, and these migrations produced a profound impact
on the cultures of the Urals (Matyushin 2003).
Aharon Dolgopolsky (1994: 2838) placed the Nostratic
homeland “somewhere in southwest Asia” whereas Allan
Bomhard & John Kerns (1994: 155) pointed to the area “in or
near the Fertile Crescent,” which disagrees with the
environmental realities mentioned by Yakhontov. Colin
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
344
Alexander Kozintsev
Renfrew (1991) wrote of the area, “from the Levant northwards
to Kurdistan and eastern Anatolia, and then south to the
southern Zagros.”
Because the boundary separating two climatic zones,
western (Mediterranean, with mild winters) and eastern
(continental, with cold winters) passes through northern Iran, I
would look for the Nostratic homeland, provided it ever existed,
immediately east of this border, near the presumed filial
homelands — Indo-Uralic, Uralic, and IE. This placement would
agree with most of the available facts — linguistic,
archaeological, genetic, and climatic. If so, then not only the
Uralic homeland would be very close to the Nostratic homeland
(in agreement with Yakhontov), but so would the IE homeland.
Their ethno-linguistic divergence was caused not so much by
the geographic distance as by the economic gap: the farming
cultures of southern Turkmenia were a peripheral part of the
Near Eastern proto-civilization, whereas Kelteminar was
outside it.
In northern Iran, the Indo-Hittite concepts relating to the
mountains, swift rivers, and the sea originated and were
embodied in respective lexemes. Apart from the archaeological
facts pointing to this area (Ivanova 2012, 2013), genetics, too,
seems to provide a clue since the autosomal CHG component is
predominant in the Neolithic and Chalcolithic samples from
northern Iran. It is also present in the Khvalynsk samples; and
the Khvalynsk people may have received CHG, possibly
through an intermediary, from a Caucasian population
(Anthony 2019). Southern archaeological parallels to
Khvalynsk, meager as they are, have prompted certain experts
to look for its Near Eastern roots, suggesting that the
northward migration passed either along the eastern Caspian
coast or through the Caucasus during the pre-Maykop period
(Bogdanov 2004: 231, 235; Anthony 2019). Given the lexicon
related to geographic features, the Caucasian route should be
preferred to the Transcaspian. However, the archaeological
evidence documenting either route is tenuous, and neither
scenario accounts for the Semitic adstrate in PIE.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
345
5.5. The second homeland
After the appearance of genetic facts indicating the
transfer of CHG from northwestern Iran or Azerbaijan to the
Volga along the eastern Caucasian route (Anthony 2019), and
after Mariya Ivanova adduced arguments in favor of the Iranian
homeland of cultures such as CFW/Leilatepe and Maykop, it is
hardly possible to doubt that not only PAs but all Indo-Hittites
had migrated from southwestern Central Asia along the
southern Caspian mountainous route postulated by Nichols —
the future Great Khorasan Road.
If so, where and when did the paths taken by PAs and by
the late PIEs diverge? Any theory addressing these questions
must account for the Semitic adstrate in both Hittite and late
PIE. It appears that one must consider the possibility of three
migration waves from the Near East to the steppe. For
geographical reasons, the eastern wave, that had supposedly
brought the CHG component to Khvalynsk, could hardly be
involved in the spread of the Semitic adstrate. Was this adstrate
absorbed before or after the dissociation of Indo-Hittite? In
other words, did PA and late PIE receive this adstrate
independently (or maybe additionally in the case of the
former)?
Attempting to link linguistic facts with those furnished by
archaeology, we must focus on two things: (a) the Amuq F
(CFW) tradition and (b) the early kurgan tradition. In her
important publications mentioned above, Catherine Marro
(2010, 2012) argues that the CFW oikumene, which existed from
~4300 BC onward and included Upper Mesopotamia, eastern
Anatolia, the southern Caucasus, and possibly Maykop as a
remote and aberrant northern member, cannot be regarded as a
result of migrations from the Fertile Crescent. Instead, as the
Ovçular Tepesi and Noruntepe materials demonstrate, the
CFW tradition might have originated in the highlands of
eastern Anatolia and Transcaucasia.
On the other hand, the Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of
Semitic languages has shown that proto-Semitic was spoken in
the northeast Levant in the fifth millennium BC, and that the
earliest split occurred when Akkadian branched off by way of
an eastward migration from the Levant to Mesopotamia in the
early fourth millennium BC (Kitchen et al. 2009). In my bird’sVolume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
346
Alexander Kozintsev
eye lexicostatistical analysis dealing with virtually unrelated
families, only two Semitic languages were used — Akkadian
and a Northwest Semitic reconstruction. Both turned out to be
indistinguishable at that level, so the adstrate in PIE could have
been received either from proto-Semites in the late fifth
millennium or from Akkadians in the early fourth millennium.
In any case, the core area of Semitic dispersal, specifically of
the proto-Akkadian migration to Mesopotamia, was close to the
western part of the CFW distribution range. This could mean
that the two oppositely directed dispersal waves, one eastward,
carrying the Semitic speech to Mesopotamia, the other
37
westward, carrying the CFW tradition to the Levant, mixed,
resulting in what Marro (2010: 52) describes as a “dimorphic
economic system” marked by the coexistence of two
contrasting communities — the sedentary agriculturalists of the
Upper Euphrates Valley and Upper Mesopotamia, some of
whom evidently spoke Semitic, on the one hand, and the
nomadic herders of the Transcaucasian and eastern Anatolian
highlands, who possibly spoke PIE, on the other. The westward
spread of CFW was evidently paralleled by the westward
diffusion of the CHG autosomal component (see 4.2.2).
Although the direct connection between the two has yet to be
ascertained, genetic data suggest that the population movement
in the Late Chalcolithic spanned the regions as widely
separated as Iran and the Levant, and that the gene flow was
directed westwards (Harney et al. 2018).
The transition from the elaborately painted Ubaid
ceramics to the standardized low-cost CFW could have been
caused by economic factors such as the urge to cut costs
(Palumbi 2011). But if the CFW tradition originated in the
highlands, as Marro claims, then the ultimate reason behind
this urge could be the nomadic lifestyle. Why the farmers
should have adopted a cultural tradition introduced by the
nomads is harder to grasp than why the latter could have
borrowed certain words from the former; see Zvelebil (1995)
and Darden (2001) on the relationships between nomadic
pastoralists and farmers with reference to PIEs.
37
It should be reminded that Amuq F — the horizon eponymous to the CFW
tradition, is comparatively late.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
347
Moreover, even in the eastern highlands, the situation was
that of “cultural duality and probably even multiculturality”
(Marro 2010). Was cultural duality accompanied by linguistic
diversity? Could at least some groups involved in the westward
diffusion of the CFW tradition have spoken early PIE or PA? If
so, this might account for the Semitic adstrate in Indo-Hittite
and especially in the first branch to have separated from it —
PA.
Initially, PAs were but one of dozens of Near Eastern
groups speaking unrelated languages such as Semitic,
Sumerian, Hattic, Hurro-Urartian, and many others of which
nothing is known. The initial “invisibility” of Anatolian
speakers (but see Whittaker 2012), traditionally referred to by
advocates of the idea of their late appearance in the Near East,
is easy to understand in the context of this ethnic mosaic. For
that matter, they were likewise “invisible” not only in the
Balkans, but even in the Troad.
According to Ilya Yakubovich (2010: 129), “(n)ow that
various views on the ethnicity of the Trojans have been clearly
stated and their limitations have been made obvious, the lesson
to be learned from this discussion is probably that of
Wittgenstein: whereof one cannot speak, thereof one must be
silent.” Even the staunchest proponents of the Steppe Theory
concede that the first appearance of IEs in Anatolia is shrouded
in mystery: “Nothing definitive can be said about the route by
which the Indo-European entry took place” (Melchert 2011:
713). Linguistic, archaeological, and genetic facts known today
prompt us to take a more optimistic stance.
5.6. The third homeland and the end of the prologue
What happened after PA had branched off? One of the
likely cultural correlates of the first split of PIE is the kurgan
tradition, whose advance toward the northern Caucasus can be
traced from Lake Urmia via the Kura valley to the passes in the
Caucasus Range and eventually to Maykop. There is every
reason to view the people related to this tradition as late PIEs.
Before them, however, two earlier group of migrants — the
proto-Khvalynsk people (apparently, undifferentiated IndoHittites) and the Darkveti-Meshoko people, who probably
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
348
Alexander Kozintsev
spoke PA — took the eastern and the western coastal routes to
the northern Caucasus, respectively.
How was PIE adopted by the steppe people? How many of
them adopted it? How did PIE spread across the steppe? Studies
addressing the southern roots of Khvalynsk, the Khvalynsk
roots of Yamnaya and the ecology of the Maykop dispersal to
the steppe (see, e.g., Shishlina 2013) offer many helpful clues.
Thus far, it appears that only in the case of Khvalynsk was
language spread accompanied by biological admixture
(Anthony 2019), but, ironically, this migration is poorly
documented by archaeological data. In the case of the two more
westerly migrations, pre-Maykop and Maykop, on the contrary,
the archaeological evidence is much weightier but given the
genetic data, admixture, at least with known steppe groups,
appears unlikely.
The easiest way of brushing these difficulties aside is to
assume that the first and only homeland of PIEs was a huge
territory of the steppe from the Ural Mountains to the Dniester
and that they expanded in an amoeba-like fashion by stretching
out pseudopodia in various directions (see, e.g., Anthony &
Ringe 2015: 209, fig. 2). Then the problem of the first split in PIE
would be easily solved by drawing a pseudopodium stretching
towards the Balkans and Anatolia. It is hardly necessary to
enumerate once again all the difficulties this simplistic solution
entails.
If, however, we admit that the steppe was not the first but
the last PIE homeland, then it can be seen as the setting of the
first act of the play, which had a prologue with an entirely
different setting — a prologue that has been overlooked by
proponents of all the past theories, possibly except Anthony
(2019). From the first act onward, the amoeba model is
generally acceptable because the daughter IE languages indeed
spread from the steppe in all directions, one of them being to
the Balkans and eventually to Anatolia. Unlike the imaginary
“first wave” of the IE expansion in the fifth millennium BC,
38
supported by few if any facts , later migrations of proto38
The idea of the Anatolian substrate in the Balkans is largely rejected (see,
e.g., Otkupikov 1988: 52-53), and those who accepted it proceeded, like
Diakonoff (1985), from the assumption that the Balkans were the IE homeland
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
349
Greeks and Paleo-Balkan peoples along that route are
reconstructed with greater reliability. Some of these groups,
such as Thraco-Phrygians and others entered Asia Minor
(Gindin 1993) and some, including Proto-Armenians,
transmitted their language to the natives of the southern
Caucasus (Diakonoff 1984). As a result of this
“counterclockwise” motion around the Black Sea, IE came full
circle and returned to the place from which some of its
speakers had moved northwards to the steppe 2500 or even
3000 years earlier.
The movement around the Caspian Sea, on the other
hand, was “clockwise” because in the Late Bronze Age the
Iranian languages, spoken by the Srubnaya and Andronovo
people, returned from the steppe to Iran, from where, if the
theory holds, the Indo-Hittites had moved to the southern
Caucasus — the starting point of the late PIE migration to the
steppe. Although the genetic evidence of the migration from
the steppe to Iran is weak (Grugni et al. 2012), archaeological
facts speak in its favor (Kuz’mina 2007).
One of the most difficult questions concerns the
Tocharians. As Leonid Sverkov (2011: 162) aptly remarked,
“both hypotheses, Anatolian and Ponto-Caspian (like all others)
are quite logical and consistent, but each of them would be
even better if the Tocharian languages had never existed.”
While from the geographical standpoint it is indeed tempting to
postulate a direct and relatively short route from the first PIE
homeland in western Central Asia, indirect evidence suggests
otherwise. Males buried at Xiaohe, one of the early cemeteries
in the Tarim Basin, dating to the early second millennium BC,
were marked by the Y-chromosome haplogoup R1a1a (M17),
the same as in the Andronovo males (Li 2010).
Craniometrically, the series from another early second
millennium BC cemetery in the same region, Gumugou, is close
to the Andronovo groups from eastern Kazakhstan and Rudny
or at least part of it, like the northern Pontic region (Gindin 1993: 165).
Earlier, however, Gindin (1967: 169) believed that Anatolians had migrated
from the Caucasus to Asia Minor and then to the Aegean, hence becoming the
earliest Indo-European stratum there. At present, Kristiansen, too, claims that
the Hittites had migrated from the Caucasus (see 3.4). None of the two
versions is supported by the available facts.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
350
Alexander Kozintsev
Altai (Kozintsev 2009). The third craniometrric parallel of
Gumugou is with a later series from Dashti-Qazi in the
Zerafshan Valley, Uzbekistan, dating to 1200–1000 BC and
associated with the intrusive Bronze Age culture from the
steppe (Kozintsev 2012). If at least one of these five groups
represents proto-Tocharians, this would mean that the source
of migration was the steppe, not southwestern Central Asia.
We don’t know what language(s) the steppe people had
spoken before adopting late PIE. Disappointing as it may sound,
the situation is much the same as with the Uralic languages in
the forest belt of Eastern Europe. In the past they appeared as
autochthonous there as the IE languages have until recently
appeared to be in the steppe belt. At present it can hardly be
doubted that the Uralic dialects were introduced to
northeastern Europe from western Siberia much later than the
steppe dialect of late PIE had emerged (Napolskikh 1997: 135,
137; Janhunen 2003; Saag et al. 2019) whereas before that,
northeastern Europe was inhabited by speakers of “PaleoEuropean languages”, about which as little is known as about
the predecessors of the late PIE in the steppe.
5.7. The horse, the ass, and language
The final question concerns the horse. What role did it
play in the IE prologue? The answer is simple: none whatever
until the setting of the play moved to the steppe. Attempts to
regard single economic factors such as agriculture or horse
breeding as key drivers of early IE dispersal have proved futile.
Migrations from highland Iran to the southern Caucasus and
then to Anatolia and the northern Caucasus were apparently
caused by a shift from a sedentary to a nomadic or
seminomadic lifestyle, which required pack animals. But
donkeys would serve this purpose no worse than horses.
Judging by the iconographic evidence, donkeys were
domesticated in Iran no later than the first half of the fifth
millennium BC, and their remains are found at fourth
millennium BC sites in highland Iran (Potts 2011). Modest as it
is, this animal rather than its celebrated congener might have
been vital for the earliest PIE migrations.
Admittedly no PIE term for donkey is known (Beekes
1995: 37); however, the Armenian word for ‘donkey’, , is a
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
351
reflex of the reconstructed proto-IE word for horse, *h1ekwo-,
implying that the latter word could refer to the onager rather
than the horse (Pereltsvaig & Lewis 2015: 177), and it has no
convincing IE etymologies. Vaclav Blaek (1998) agrees with
Pierre Naert (1958: 137–138), who pointed to the parallel with
the Yeniseian compound word ‘male’+’horse’=‘stallion’ (protoYeniseian *i-ku’s, according to Starostin; see also
Gamkrelidze & Ivanov 1995: 479, fn. 22; 2013) and suggests that
the word was borrowed from a Yeniseian language.
Gamkrelidze & Ivanov (2013) hypothesize that such a language
was spoken by the Botai people of northern Kazakhstan. J. P.
Mallory (1982) noted that PIEs used only one name for ‘horse’
making no distinction between the domesticated and the wild
horse. In Blaek’s view, the word *h1ekw o- referred to the
domesticated horse, suggesting that the IE homeland must be
localized in a place where the wild horse did not live. Clearly,
such a place is easier to find in the Near East, where the
domestic horse was evidently introduced from the steppe, than
in the steppe itself (Shev 2016). All this lends support to
Anthony’s (2019) idea of a migration from northwestern Iran or
Azerbaijan to the Volga, the obvious correction being that, to
all appearances, the migrants were PIEs rather than Northeast
Caucasian speakers. After this migration, the Yeniseian term
for ‘horse’ could have been borrowed by PIEs — the late
Khvalynsk or early Yamnaya people in the eastern part of their
39
distribution range.
The problem with this explanation is that, as Don Ringe
tells us, the PA word for horse, *áwos, cannot be a late
borrowing. It was either inherited from Indo-Hittite or
borrowed “into or out of pre-PA during the relatively short
time when pre-PA was still in contact with related languages—
and that time must have been some centuries before 3500 BCE”
(https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=994, accessed June 4,
2019). If so, there are two possibilities. If the Yeniseian version
holds, then the Darkveti-Meshoko people could have borrowed
the word from a late (steppe) PIE dialect and passed it on to
their linguistic relatives in Anatolia. After all, this is a shorter
39
The proportion of equids in the livestock of both the pre-Maykop and the
Maykop people was negligible.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
352
Alexander Kozintsev
route than the one connecting Ural with Anatolia via the
Balkans; also, it has been reliably traced in one direction and
doesn’t presuppose a migration. If, on the other hand, the PA
word has nothing to do with Yeniseian and was inherited from
Indo-Hittite, then, as the Armenian reflex suggests, it might
have initially denoted the donkey.
6. Conclusions
As every specialist in PIE reconstruction is well aware, PA
is the cornerstone, on which the entire reconstruction is based.
But to everybody’s chagrin, precisely this element of the
building, being the earliest, is the most problematic. Its
position, more than that of any other block, depends on our
theoretical views regarding the factors behind language
dispersal. If one ascribes the critical role to farming, the
cornerstone is inevitably placed in Neolithic Anatolia. If horse
breeding is in the foreground, the preferred locus shifts to the
steppe Eneolithic.
I began this study without any preconceptions regarding
the PIE economy. The idea that the economic gap between
Jeitun and Kelteminar might be equally, if not more relevant to
the disintegration of Indo-Uralic than the geographic distance
between them was an ad hoc invention. It occurred to me at the
final stage of the study, and none of my preceding conclusions
were based on it. The same is true of nomadism or seminomadism. Here, too, the reasoning was a posteriori, not a
priori. To be sure, in reality PIEs moved from one place to
another without splitting into filial groups because they were a
relatively small group of nomadic or semi-nomadic herders. In
the reconstruction, however, it was the other way around: PIEs
must have been a small group of nomadic or semi-nomadic
herders because they appear to have undertaken a long journey
without splitting into filial groups.
There is mounting evidence that the most probable
location of both the IE and the Uralic homelands is the area east
of the Caspian Sea. The former can be associated with the early
farming cultures of southern Turkmenia and northern Iran, the
latter, with the Kelteminar culture. The Mesolithic inhabitants
of the Transcaspian area, therefore, probably spoke Indo-Uralic.
While the placement of the IE homeland is in line with the
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
353
theory of Johanna Nichols, her idea of two parallel westward
streams of PIE dispersal disagrees with available facts. Judging
by the Iranian and southern Caucasian autosomal component
(CHG) and by the lexicon relating to high mountains and swift
rivers, the southern route along the Elburz range was taken by
all Indo-Hittites. Their earliest branch headed north toward the
Volga forest-steppe and eventually gave rise to the Khvalynsk
culture.
Other PIEs moved on toward Anatolia and on their way
absorbed a small Semitic adstrate. The northern branch of PAs
may correlate with the Darkveti-Meshoko culture. The late
PIEs’ migration to the northern Caucasus, marked by the
kurgan tradition, culminated in the Maykop culture, which had
retained its ties with southern Turkmenia and northern Iran.
The expansion of Maykop led to the further IndoEuropeanization of the steppe, apparently with little or no gene
flow. The transmitter of the Semitic adstrate to the late (steppe)
PIE could be either Darkveti-Meshoko or Maykop.
Figure1. The reconstructed dispersal of proto-Uralians (Tolstov 1941,
1948; Chernetsov 1953, 1960, 1968), proto-Semites (Kitchen et al.
2009), and proto-IEs. Numbers show the order of proto-IE migrations.
The amount of CHG, which the steppe people had
received from the Caucasus (apparently during the earliest PIE
migration from northern Iran or Azerbaijan to the Volga) and
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
354
Alexander Kozintsev
which the Yamnaya people later passed on to Central and
Western European populations, is much too high to be
associated with North Caucasian speakers. Northwest and
Northeast Caucasian were evidently spoken by the
Novosvobodnya and the Kura-Araxes people, respectively. The
time when these cultures emerged matches the
glottochronological estimate of the split of common North
Caucasian. The probable ancestors of the Novosvobodnaya
people were those who lived in the Kuban fortresses at their
late stage. Contacts between the Maykop and Novosvobodnaya
people may account for the Northwest Caucasian adstrate in
PIE.
The principal conclusions of this study are illustrated by
the map (Figure 1).
***
An important part of this study was based on the
preliminary lexicostatistical distance matrix from the
STARLING database, generously shared by George Starostin,
Alexei Kassian, and Mikhail Zhivlov. I am thankful to Alexei
Kassian, Mikhail Zhivlov, Svetlana Burlak, Allan Bomhard,
Alexander Militarev, Leo Klejn, Nadezhda Nikolaeva, Yuri
Berezkin, Arnaud Fournet, Juho Pystynen, and David
Wesolowski for their helpful comments. I thank Richard P.
Martin, who has checked my English.
References
Abedi, Akbar, Behrooz Omrani & Azam Karimifar
2015
Fifth and fourth millennium BC in north-western Iran: Dalma and
Pisdeli revisited. Documenta Praehistorica 42: 321–338.
Akhundov, Tufan
2007
Sites des migrants venus du Proche–Orient en Transcaucasie. In
B. Lyonnet (ed.). Les cultures du Caucase (VIe-IIIe millénaires avant
notre ère). Leurs relations avec le Proche-Orient, 77–94. Paris: CNRS
Éditions.
Akkermans, Peter M.M.G.
1988
An updated chronology for the northern Ubaid and Late
Chalcolithic periods in Syria: New evidence from Tell Hammam
et-Turkman. Iraq 50: 109–145.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
355
Alekshin, Vadim A., Lyubov B. Kircho & Sergei G. Popov (eds.).
2005
Xronologija epoxi pozdnego eneolita – srednej bronzy Srednej Azii
(pogrebenija Altyn-Depe) [The Late Eneolithic – Middle Bronze
Age Chronology of Southwestern Central Asia: The Burials of
Altyn-Depe]. Saint-Petersburg: Nestor-Istorija.
Allentoft, Morton N., Martin Sikora, Karl-Göran Sjögren et al.
2015
Population genomics of Bronze Age Eurasia. Nature 522 (7555):
167-172.
Andreyeva, Marina V.
1977
K voprosu o junyx svjazjax majkopskoj kul’tury. Sovetskaja
arxeologija 1: 39–56.
Andronov, Mikhail S.
1965
Dravidijskie jazyki [Dravidian languages]. Moscow: Nauka.
2003
A Comparative Grammar of the Dravidian Languages.
Wiesbaden: Otto Harrassowitz.
Anthony, David W.
2007
The Horse, the Wheel, and Language: How Bronze Age Riders From
the Eurasian Steppes Shaped the Modern World. Princeton/Oxford:
Princeton University Press
2019
Archaeology, genetics, and language in the steppes: A comment
on Bomhard. Journal of Indo-European Studies 47 (1-2): 173-196.
Anthony, David W. & Dorcas R. Brown
2017
Molecular biology and Indo-European linguistics: Impressions
from new data. In: B.S.S. Hansen, A. Hyllested, A.R. Jørgensen et
al. (eds.). Usque ad Radices: Indo-European Studies in Honour of
Birgit Anette Olsen, 25–54. Copenhagen: Museum Tusculanum
Press.
Anthony, David W. & Don Ringe
2015
The Indo-European homeland from linguistic and archaeological
perspectives. Annual Review of Linguistics 1: 199-219.
Areshian, Gregory E., Boris Gasparyan, Pavel S.Avetisyan et al.
2012
The Chalcolithic of the Near East and south-eastern Europe:
Discoveries and new perspectives from the cave complex Areni-1,
Armenia. Antiquity 86 (331): 115–130.
Astafyev, Andrey E.
2006
Poselenie Kuskuduk I – pamjatnik final’nogo etapa ojuklinskoj
kul’tury Vostonogo Prikaspija [Kuskuduk I — a late Oyukly
village on the eastern Caspian coast]. Voprosy arxeologii Povolja
4: 161–185.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
356
Alexander Kozintsev
Bacvarov, Krum
2008
A long way to the west: Earliest jar burials in Southeast Europe
and the Near East. In K. Bacvarov (ed.). Babies Reborn: Infant/child
burials in pre- and protohistory. BAR International Series 1832, 61–
70. Oxford: BAR Publishers.
Balanovsky, Oleg
Genofond Evropy [The Gene Pool of Europe].
2015
Moscow:Tovariestvo naunyx izdanij KMK.
2019
Comments on the Caucasian substrate hypothesis. Journal of
Indo-European Studies 47 (1-2): 164-172.
Balanovsky, Oleg, Olga Utevska & Elena Balanovska
2013
Genetics of Indo-European populations: The past, the future.
Journal of Language Relationship 9: 23–35.
Beekes, Robert S.P.
2011
Comparative Indo-European Linguistics. Amsterdam and
Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Bellwood, Peter
2012
How and when did agriculture spread? In P. Gepts, T.R. Famula,
R.L. Bettinger et al. (eds.). Biodiversity in Agriculture:
Domestication, Evolution, and Sustainability, 160–189. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Blaek, Václav
1998
Is Indo-European *H 1ekwo- really of Indo-European origin?
Studia Indogermanica Lodziensia 2: 21–32.
2013a
Indo-European zoonyms in Afroasiatic perspective. Journal of
Language Relationship 9: 37–54.
2013b
Northern Europe and Russia: Uralic linguistic history. In I. Ness
(ed.). The Encyclopedia of Global Human Migration, 178–183.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Bogdanov, Sergei V.
2004
Epoxa medi stepnogo Priuralja [The Copper Age of the Uralian
Steppes]. Ekaterinburg: UrO RAN.
Bomhard, Allan R.
2018
A Comprehensive Introduction to Nostratic Comparative
d
Linguistics. With Special Reference to Indo-European, 3 ed., vol.1.
Florence, S.C.
2019a
The origins of Proto-Indo-European: The Caucasian substrate
hypothesis. Journal of Indo-European Studies 47 (1-2): 9-124.
2019b
Response to the comments. Journal of Indo-European Studies 47
(1-2): 197-210.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
357
Bomhard, Allan R. & John C. Kerns
1994
The Nostratic Macrofamily. A Study in Distant Linguistic
Relationship. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
Bouckaert, Remco, Philippe Lemey, Michael Dunn et al.
2013
Correction to ‘Mapping the origins and expansion of the IndoEuropean language family.’ Science 342 (6165): 1446.
Broushaki, Farnaz, Mark G. Thomas, Vivian Link et al.
2016
Early Neolithic genomes from the eastern Fertile Crescent.
Science 353 (6298): 499–503.
Bulatovi, Aleksandar
2014
Corded ware in the Central and Southern Balkans: A
consequence of cultural interaction or an indication of ethnic
change? Journal of Indo-European Studies 42 (1-2): 101-143.
Bulatovi, Aleksandar & Marc Vander Linden
2017
Absolute dating of Copper and Early Bronze Age levels at the
eponymous archaeological site Bubanj (southeastern Serbia).
Radiocarbon 59 (4): 1047–1065.
Burney, Charles
1978
From Village to Empire: An Introduction to Near Eastern
Archaeology. Oxford: Phaidon.
Carpelan, Christian & Asko Parpola
2001
On the emergence, contacts and dispersal of Proto-IndoEuropean, Proto-Uralic and Proto-Aryan in an archaeological
perspective. In C. Carpelan, A. Parpola & P. Koskikalio (eds.).
Early Contacts between Uralic and Indo-European: Linguistic and
Archaeological Considerations. Suomalais-ugrilaisen seuran
toimituksia (Mémoires de la Société Finno-Ougrienne) 242: 55–130.
Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi L.
2001
Genes, Peoples and Languages. London: Penguin Books.
Chang, Will, Chundra Cathcart, David Hall & Andrew Garrett
2015
Ancestry-constrained phylogenetic analysis supports the IndoEuropean steppe hypothesis. Language 91 (1): 144–244.
Cherlenok, Evgeny A.
2013
Arxeologija Kavkaza (mezolit, neolit, eneolit) [Archaeology of the
Caucasus: Mesolithic, Neolithic, Eneolithic)]. Saint-Petersburg:
Saint-Petersburg State University Press.
Chernetsov, Valery N.
1953
Drevnjaja istorija Ninego Priobja [The ancient history of the
Lower Ob]. Materialy i issledovanija po arxeologii SSSR 35: 7–71.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
358
Alexander Kozintsev
1960
Drevnejie periody istorii narodov ural’skoj (finno-ugrosamodijskoj) obnosti [The earliest stages in the history of
peoples of the Uralian (Finno-Ugro-Samojed) community]. In
Naunaja konferencija po istorii Sibiri i Dal’nego Vostoka, 53–59.
Irkutsk: Irkutsk State University Press.
1968
K voprosu o sloenii ural’skogo neolita [On the origin of the
Uralian Neolithic]. In Istorija, arxeologija i etnografija Srednej Azii.
K 60-letiju S.P. Tolstova, 41–52. Moscow: Nauka.
Chernykh, Evgeny N.
1987
Protoindoevropejcy v sisteme Circumpontijskoj provincii [ProtoIndo-Europeans in the system of the Circum-Pontic Province]. In
L.A. Gindin (ed.). Antichnaja balkanistika, 136–147. Moscow:
Nauka.
2013
Kul'tury nomadov v megastrukture evrazijskogo mira [Nomadic
Cultures in the Megastructure of the Eurasian World]. Vol. 1.
Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskoj kul’tury.
Chernykh, Evgeny N. & Lyubov B. Orlovskaya
2008
Fenomen majkopskoj obnosti i ee radiouglerodnaja xronologija
[The Majkop phenomenon and its radiocarbon dating]. In N.Y.
Merpert, S.N. Korenevsky (eds.). Arxeologija Kavkaza i Blinego
Vostoka. Sbornik k 80-letiju R.M. Munaeva, 259–275. Moscow:
Taus.
Courcier, Antoine
2014
Ancient metallurgy in the Caucasus from the sixth to the third
millennium BCE. In B.W. Roberts & C.P. Thornton (eds.).
Archaeometallurgy in Global Perspective, 579–664. New York:
Springer.
Damgaard Peter B., Rui Martiniano, Jack Kamm et al.
2018
The first horse herders and the impact of Early Bronze Age
steppe expansions into Asia. Science 360 (6396). DOI:
10.1126/science.aar7711.
Danilenko, Valentin N.
1974
Eneolit Ukrainy: Etnoistorieskoe issledovanie [The Chalcolithic of
Ukraine: An Ethno-Historical Study]. Kiev: Naukova dumka.
Darden, Bill J.
2001
On the question of the Anatolian origin of Indo-Hittite. In R.
Drews (ed.). Greater Anatolia and the Indo-Hittite Language
Family. Papers Presented at a Colloquium Hosted by the University
of Richmond, March 18–19, 2000, 184–228. Washington: Institute
for the Study of Man (Journal of Indo-European Studies Monograph
Series 38).
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
359
Day, John V.
2001
Indo-European Origins: The Anthropological Evidence. Washington:
Institute for the Study of Man (Journal of Indo-European Studies
Monograph Series 2).
Debetz, George F.
1980
Antropologieskie issledovanija v Afganistane [Anthropological
studies in Afghanistan]. In M.G. Abdushelishvili, K.C. Malhotra
(eds.). Novye dannye k antropologii Severnoj Indii. Rezul’taty
sovetsko-indijskix issledovanij 1971 g., 241–308. Moscow: Nauka.
Dergachev, Valentin A.
2007
O skipetrax, o loadjax, o vojne. Etjudy v zaitu migracionnoj
koncepcii M. Gimbutas [On Scepters, Horses, and War: Essays in
Support of the Migrationist Theory of M. Gimbutas]. SaintPetersburg: Nestor–Istorija.
Deshayes, Jean
1973
La date de tumuli de Sé Girdan. Iran 11: 176–178.
Diakonoff, Igor M.
1984.
The Pre-History of the Armenian People. New York: Caravan
Books.
1985
On the original home of the speakers of Indo-European. Journal
of Indo-European Studies 13 (1–2), 92–174.
1990
Language contacts in the Caucasus and the Near East. In T.L.
Markey & J.A.C. Greppin (eds.). When Worlds Collide: IndoEuropeans and Pre-Indo-Europeans. The Bellagio Papers, 53–65.
Ann Arbor: Karoma Publishers.
Diakonoff, Igor M. & Sergei A. Starostin
1986
Hurro-Urartian as an Eastern Caucasian Language. Münchner
Studien zur Sprachwissenschaft, Beiheft 12, Neue Folge. Munich:
R. Kitzinger.
Dolgopolsky, Aharon B.
The Indo-European homeland and lexical contacts of proto-Indo1987
European with other languages. Mediterranean Language Review
3: 7–31.
1994
Nostratic. In R.E. Asher (ed.). The Encyclopedia of Language and
Linguistics, vol. 5, 2838. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Nostratic Dictionary. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for
2008
Archaeological Research.
Dybo, Anna V.
2013.
Language and archaeology: some methodological problems. 1.
Indo-European and Altaic landscapes. Journal of Language
Relationship 9: 69-92.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
360
Alexander Kozintsev
Dyen, Isidore, Joseph B. Kruskal & Paul Black
1992
An Indo-European classification: A lexicostatistical experiment.
Transactions of the American Philosophical Society 82 (5): 1-132.
Elfenbein, Josef
1987
A periplus of the ‘Braui problem.’ Studia Iranica 16 (2). 215-233.
Formozov, Alexander A.
1965
Kamennyj vek i eneolit Prikubanja [The Stone Age and Eneolithic
of the Kuban Drainage]. Moscow: Nauka.
1972
O roli zakaspijskogo i priaral’skogo mezolita i neolita v istorii
Evropy i Azii [On the role of the Transcaspian and Aralian
Mesolithic and Neolithic in the history of Europe and Asia].
Sovetskaja arxeologija 1: 22–35.
Formozov, Alexander A. & Evgeny N. Chernyhh
1964
Novye poselenija majkopskoj kul’tury v Prikubanje [New
Maykop sites in the Kuban drainage]. Kratkie soobenija Instituta
arxeologii 101: 102–110.
Forster, Peter & Colin Renfrew
2011
Mother tongue and Y chromosomes. Science 333 (6048): 1390–
1391.
Frangipane, Marcella
1993
Local components in the development of centralized societies in
Syro-Anatolian Regions. In M. Frangipane, H. Hauptmann, M.
Liverani et al. (eds.), Between the Rivers and over the Mountains.
Archaeologica Anatolica et Mesopotamica Alba Palmieri Dedicata,
133-161. Roma: Università di Roma “La Sapienza”.
Furholt, Martin
2008
Pottery, cultures, people? The European Baden material
reexamined. Antiquity 82 (317): 617–628.
Gamba, Cristina, Eppie R. Jones, Matthew D. Teasdale et al.
2014
Genome flux and stasis in a five millennium transect of European
prehistory. Nature Communications 5:5257. DOI:
10.1038/ncomms6257.
Gamkrelidze, Thomas V. & Vjaeslav V. Ivanov
1995
Indo-European and the Indo-Europeans. A Reconstruction and
Historical Analysis of a Proto-Language and a Proto-Culture.
English version by Johanna Nichols. Berlin and New York:
Mouton de Gruyter.
2013
Indoevropejskaja prarodina i rasselenie indoevropejcev: polveka
issledovanij i obsudenij [Indo-European homeland and IndoEuropean dispersals: half a century of studies and discussions].
Journal of Language Relationship 9: 109–136.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
361
Gej, Alexander N.
1989
Xozjajstvennye struktury i etnieskie processy u plemen eneolita
– bronzovogo veka Severo-Zapadnogo Kavkaza (k postanovke
problemy) [Economic and ethnic pocesses in Eneolithic and
Bronze Age tribes of northwestern Caucasus: An outline of a
problem]. In Pervaja Kubanskaja arxeologieskaja konferencija.
Tezisy dokladov, 27–29. Krasnodar: Kuban State University
Publishers.
Gimbutas, Marija
1963 The Indo-Europeans: Archaeological problems. American
Anthropologist 65 (4): 815–836.
1970
Proto-Indo-European culture: The Kurgan culture during the
fifth, fourth, and third millennia B.C. In G. Cardona, H.M.
Hoenigswald & A. Senn (eds.). Indo-European and Indo-Europeans,
155–197. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
1977
The first wave of Eurasian steppe pastoralists into Copper Age
Europe. Journal of Indo-European Studies 5 (4): 277–338.
Gindin, Leonid A.
1967
Jazyk drevnejego naselenija juga Balkanskogo poluostrova.
Fragment indoevropejskoj onomastiki [The Language of the
Earliest Populations of the Balkan Peninsula: A Fragment of the
Indo-European Onomastics]. Moscow: Nauka.
1993
Naselenie gomerovskoj Troi: Istoriko-filologieskie issledovanija po
etnologii drevnej Anatolii [The People of Homeric Troy. Historical
and Philological Studies of the Population of Ancient Anatolia].
Moscow: Nauka.
Gorauk, Igor V.
2003
Texnologija izgotovlenija kamennyx orudij na stojankax
xvalynskoj kul’tury. [The technology of manufacturing stone
tools at sites of the Khvalynsk culture]. Voprosy arxeologii
Povolja 3: 118–133.
Gorelik, Alexander, Andrej Tsybrij & Viktor Tsybrij
2016
‘Neolithization’ in the NE Sea of Azov region: one step forward,
two steps back? Documenta Praehistorica 43: 139–160.
Govedarica, Blagoje
2005–2009 Kamennye krestovidnye bulavy mednogo veka na territorii
Jugo-vostonoj i Vostonoj Evropy [Cruciform stone mace-heads
in the Chalcolithic of Southeastern and Eastern Europe]. Stratum
Plus 2: 419–437.
Govedarica, Blagoje & Igor Manzura
2011
Grundzüge einer Kulturgeschichte des nordwestlichen
Schwarzmeergebietes im 5. und 4. Jahrtausend v. Chr. In E. Sava,
B. Govedarica & B. Hänsel (Hrsg.). Der Schwarzmeerraum vom
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
362
Alexander Kozintsev
Äneolithikum bis in die Früheisenzeit (5000–500 v. Chr.). Bd. 2, 41–
61. Rahden/Westf.: Verlag Marie Leidorf.
Gray, Russell D., Quentin D. Atkinson & Simon J. Greenhill
2011
Language evolution and human history: what a difference a date
makes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366
(1567): 1090–1100.
Greenberg, Joseph H.
2000, 2002 Indo-European and Its Closest Relatives. The Eurasiatic Language
Family, vols. 1–2. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Greenberg, Raphael & Giulio Palumbi
2015
Corridors and colonies: Comparing fourth–third millennia BC
interactions in southeast Anatolia and the Levant. In A.B. Knapp
& P. van Dommelen. The Cambridge Prehistory of the Bronze and
Iron Age Mediterranean, 111–138. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Gresky Julia & Natalia Y. Berezina.
2012.
Two cases of trepanation in Eneolithic burials from Progress 2
and Vonjuchka 1. The latest archaeological discoveries in the
North Caucasus: Researches & Interpretations. The XXVIIth
Krupnov’s Readings. Proceedings of the International Scientific
Conference. Makhachkala, April 23–28, 2012, 44–45. Makhachkala:
Mavraev.
Grugni, Viola, Vincenza Battaglia, Baharak H. Kashani et al.
2012
Ancient migratory events in the Middle East: New clues from the
Y-chromosome variation of modern Iranians. PloS ONE 7 (7):
e41252.
Gülçur, Sevil & Catherine Marro
2012
The view from the North: Comparative analysis of the
Chalcolithic pottery assemblages from Noruntepe and Ovçular
Tepesi. In C. Marro (ed.). After the Ubaid: Interpreting Change
from the Caucasus to Mesopotamia at the Dawn of Urban
Civilization (4500–3500 BC). Papers from ‘The Post-Ubaid Horizon
in the Fertile Crescent and Beyond.’ International Workshop Held at
th
st
Fosseuse, 29 June – 1 July 2009, 305–352. Paris: De Boccard
Édition.
Haak, Wolfgang, Iosif Lazaridis, Nick Patterson et al.
2015
Massive migration from the steppe was a source for IndoEuropean languages in Europe. Nature 522 (7555): 207-211.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
363
Haber, Marc, Massimo Mezzavilla, Yali Xue et al.
2016
Genetic evidence for an origin of the Armenians from Bronze
Age mixing of multiple populations. European Journal of Human
Genetics 24 (6): 931–936.
Hajdú, Péter
1964
Über die alten Siedlungsräume der uralischen Sprachfamilie. Acta
Linguistica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae 14: 147–183.
Harney, Éadaoin, Hila May, Dina Shalem et al.
2018
Ancient DNA from Chalcolithic Israel reveals the role of
population mixture in cultural transformation. Nature
Communications 9:3336. DOI: 10.1038/s41467-018-05649-9.
Heggarty, Paul
2018
Indo-European and the ancient DNA revolution. In G. Kroonen,
J.P. Mallory, B. Comrie (eds.). Talking Neolithics: Proceedings of the
Workshop on Indo-European Origins Held at the Max Planck
Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, December 2–3,
2013. Journal of Indo-European Studies Monograph Series 65: 120–
173.
Helimski, Eugene A.
1982
Drevneiie vengersko-samodijskie jazykovye kontakty [The earliest
Hungarian-Samoyed language contacts]. Moscow: Nauka.
2001
Early Indo-Uralic linguistic relationships: Real kinship or
imagined contacts? In C. Carpelan, A. Parpola & P. Koskikalio
(eds.). Early Contacts between Uralic and Indo-European: Linguistic
and Archaeological Considerations. Suomalais-ugrilaisen seuran
toimituksia (Mémoires de la Société Finno-Ougrienne) 242: 187–205.
Helwing, Barbara
2000
Regional variation in the composition of Late Chalcolithic pottery
assemblages. In C. Marro & H. Hauptmann (eds.). Chronologies de
e
e
pays du Caucase et de l’Euphrate aux IV et III millénaires. Actes
du Congres d’Istanbul, 16–19 décembre 1998, 145–164. Istanbul:
Institut français d’études anatoliennes.
Hofmanová, Zuzana, Susanne Kreutzer, Garrett Hellenthal et al.
2016
Early farmers from across Europe directly descended from
Neolithic Aegeans. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the USA 113 (25): 6886–6891.
Horváth, Tünde, S. Éva Svingor & Mihály Molnár
2008
New radiocarbon dates for the Baden Culture. Radiocarbon 50 (3):
447–458.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
364
Alexander Kozintsev
Illi-Svity, Vladislav M.
1964
Drevneiie indoevropejsko-semitskie jazykovye kontakty [The
earliest IE–Semitic language contacts]. In V.N. Toporov (ed.).
Problemy indoevropejskogo jazykoznanija, 3–12. Moscow: Nauka.
1971
Opyt sravnenija nostratieskix jazykov (semitoxamitskij,
kartvel’skij, indoevropejskij, ural’skij, dravidskij, altajskij) [An
Attempt at a Comparison of the Nostratic Languages (HamitoSemitic, Kartvelian, Indo-European, Uralic, Dravidian, Altaic)],
vol. 1. Moscow: Nauka.
1976
Opyt sravnenija nostratieskix jazykov (semitoxamitskij,
kartvel’skij, indoevropejskij, ural’skij, dravidskij, altajskij) [An
Attempt at a Comparison of the Nostratic Languages (HamitoSemitic, Kartvelian, Indo-European, Uralic, Dravidian, Altaic)],
vol. 2. Moscow: Nauka.
Ivanov, Vyacheslav Vs.
2001/1963 Hettskij jazyk [The Hittite Language]. Moscow: URSS.
Ivanova, Mariya
2012
Kaukasus und Orient: Die Enstehung des “Maikop-Phänomens”
im 4. Jahrtausend v. Chr. Praehistorische Zeitschrift 87 (1): 1–28.
2013
The Black Sea and the Early Civilizations of Europe, the Near East
and Asia. New York: Cambridge University Press.
2016
Stop and go: Die Ausbreitung kaukasischer Metallformen in
Osteuropa in der ersten Hälfte des 3. Jt. v. Chr. In V. Nikolov &
W. Schier (Hrsg.). Der Schwarzmeerraum vom Neolithikum bis in
die Früheisenzeit (6000–600 v. Chr.). Kulturelle Interferenzen in der
Zirkumpontischen Zone und Kontakte mit ihren Nachbargebieten,
403–415. Rahden/Westf.: Verlag Marie Leidorf.
Izbitser, Elena V.
2013
Ee raz k voprosu o meste proisxodenija kolesnogo transporta i
ego pojavlenii na Severnom Kavkaze [The origin of the wheeled
transport and its appearance in the northern Caucasus revisited].
In Materialy po izueniju istoriko-kul’turnogo nasledija Severnogo
Kavkaza 11, 133–140. Moscow: Pamjatniki istorieskoj mysli.
Janhunen, Juha
2003.
Proto-Uralic — what, where, and when? Suomalais-ugrilaisen
seuran toimituksia (Mémoires de la Société Finno-Ougrienne) 258:
156-167.
Jones, Eppie R., Gloria Gonzales-Fortes, Sarah Connell et al.
2015
Upper Paleolithic genomes reveal deep roots of modern
Eurasians. Nature Communications 6:8912. DOI:
10.1038/ncomms9912.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
365
Kassian, Alexei S.
2010
Hattic as a Sino-Caucasian language. Ugarit-Forschungen 41: 309–
447.
Kassian, Alexei S., Mikhail A. Zhivlov & George S. Starostin
2015
Proto-Indo-European-Uralic comparison from the probabilistic
point of view. Journal of Indo-European Studies 43 (3-4): 301-347.
Kazarnitsky, Alexei A.
2012
Naselenie azovo-kaspijskix stepej v epoxu bronzy (antropologieskij
oerk) [The Population of the Azov–Caspian steppes in the
Bronze Age: An Anthropological Sketch]. Saint-Petersburg:
Nauka
Khalikov, Alfred H.
1969
Drevnjaja istorija srednego Povolzhja [Ancient history of the
Middle Volga drainage]. Moscow: Nauka.
Kitchen, Andrew, Christopher Ehret, Shiferaw Assefa et al.
2009
Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of Semitic languages identifies an
Early Bronze Age origin of Semitic in the Near East. Proceedings
of the Royal Society B 276 (1668): 2703–2710.
Kivisild, Toomas
2017
The study of human Y chromosome variation through ancient
DNA. Human Genetics 136 (5): 529–546.
Klejn, Lev S.
2010
Vremja kentavrov. Stepnaja prarodina grekov i ariev [The Time of
the Centaurs. The Steppe Homeland of Greeks and Aryans].
Saint-Petersburg: Evrazija.
Kloekhorst, Alwin
2008
Some Indo-Uralic aspects of Hittite. Journal of Indo-European
Studies 36 (1-2): 88–95.
Kohl, Philip L.
2007
The Making of Bronze Age Eurasia. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Kohl, Philip L. & Rabadan G. Magomedov
2014
Early Bronze developments on the West Caspian coastal plain.
Paléorient 40 (2): 93–114.
Kohl, Philip L. & Viktor A. Trifonov
2014
The prehistory of the Caucasus: Internal developments and
external interactions. In C. Renfrew, P. Bahn (eds.). The
Cambridge World Prehistory, vol. 3: West and Central Asia and
Europe, 1571–1595. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
366
Alexander Kozintsev
Koivulehto, Jorma
2001
The earliest contacts between Indo-European and Uralic speakers
in the light of lexical loans. In C. Carpelan, A. Parpola & P.
Koskikalio (eds.). Early Contacts between Uralic and IndoEuropean: Linguistic and Archaeological Considerations. Suomalaisugrilaisen seuran toimituksia (Mémoires de la Société FinnoOugrienne) 242: 235–264.
Korenevsky, Sergei N.
2004
Drevnejie zemledel’cy i skotovody Predkavkazja. Majkopskonovosvobodnenskaja obnost’. Problemy vnutrennej tipologii [The
Earliest Farmers and Herders of the Northern Caucasus: The
Maykop–Novosvobodnaya Community. Problems of Inner
Typology]. Moscow: Nauka.
2008
Simvolika atributov duxovnoj vlasti epoxi eneolita Vostonoj
Evropy i Predkavkaz’ja — kamennyx zoomorfnyx skipetrov [The
symbolism of ideological power attributes of the Eneolithic of
Eastern Europe and northern Caucasus — the stone zoomorphic
scepters]. Arxeologija vostono-evropejskoj stepi 6, 135–156.
Saratov: SGU.
2012
Rodenie kurgana (pogrebal’nye pamjatniki eneolitieskogo vremeni
Predkavkazja i volgo-donskogo medureja) [The Birth of the
Kurgan: Eneolithic Funerary Monuments of the Northern
Caucasus and the Volga-Don Watershed]. Moscow: Taus.
Korobkova, Galina F.
1996
Srednjaja Azija i Kazaxstan [Western Central Asia and
Kazakhstan]. In S.V. Oshibkina (ed.). Neolit Severnoj Evrazii, 87–
133. Moscow: Nauka.
Kortlandt, Frederik H. H.
1990
The spread of the Indo-Europeans. Journal of Indo-European
Studies 18 (1-2): 131-140.
2002
The Indo-Uralic verb. In Finno-Ugrians and Indo-Europeans:
Linguistic and Literary Contacts. Proceedings of the Symposium at
the University of Groningen, November 22-24, 2001, 217–227.
Maastricht: Shaker.
2018
The expansion of Indo-European languages. Journal of IndoEuropean Studies 46 (1-2): 219–231.
2019
Indo-European and its neighbors. Journal of Indo-European
Studies 47 (1-2): 142-146.
Kotova, Nadezhda S.
2006
O naale kontaktov stepnogo i predkavkazskogo naselenija v
epoxu rannego eneolita [On the earliest ties between the steppe
and the northern Caucasian populations in the early Eneolithic].
Voprosy arxeologii Povolja 6: 147–153.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
2013
2015
367
Dereivskaja kul’tura i pamjatniki ninemixajlovskogo tipa [The
Dereivka culture and the Lower Mikhailovka sites].
Kiev/Kharkov: Majdan.
Drevnejaja keramika Ukrainy [The Earliest Pottery of Ukraine].
Kiev/Kharkov: Majdan.
Kovaleva, Valentina T. & Svetlana Y. Zyrianova
2009
O roli kel’teminarskoj kul’tury v neolitizacii srednego Zauralja i
Zapadnoj Sibiri [On the role of the Kelteminar culture in the
Neolithization of the mid-eastern Ural and western Siberia]. In S.
Vasiliev (ed.). Vzaimodejstvie i xronologija kul’tur mezolita i
neolita Vostonoj Evropy. Materialy medunarodnoj konferencii,
posvjaennoj 100-letiju N.N. Gurinoj, 173–175. Saint-Petersburg:
IIMK RAN.
Kozintsev, Alexander G.
2004
Kety, ural’cy, “amerikanoidy”: Integracija kraniometrieskix i
kranioskopieskix priznakov [Kets, Uralians, “Americanoids”: An
integration of metric and nonmetric cranial traits]. In A.G.
Kozintsev (ed.). Paleoantropologija, etnieskaja antropologija,
etnogenez. K 75-liju Ilji Iosifovia Gokhmana, 172–185. SaintPetersburg: Museum of Anthropology and Ethnography Press.
2008
The “Mediterraneans” of Southern Siberia and Kazakhstan, IndoEuropean migrations, and the origin of the Scythians: A
multivariate craniometric analysis. Archaeology, Ethnology and
Anthropology of Eurasia 36 (4): 140–144.
2009
Craniometric evidence of the early Caucasoid migrations to
Siberia and Eastern Central Asia, with reference to the IndoEuropean problem. Archaeology, Ethnology and Anthropology of
Eurasia 37 (4): 125–136.
2012
Iz stepi v pustynju: Rannie evropeoidy Vostonogo Turkestana po
dannym genetiki i antropologii [From the steppe into the desert:
The early Caucasoids of Xinjiang, based on the evidence of
genetics and physical anthropology]. In Kul’tury stepnoj Evrazii i
ix vzaimodejstvie s drevnimi civilizacijami, vol.1, 122–126. SaintPetersburg: IIMK RAN Press.
2016
Naal’nyi etap indo-evropejskoj istorii: svidetel’stva lingvistiki,
paleogenetiki i arxeologii [The earliest stage of Indo-European
history: The evidence of linguistics, paleogenetics, and
archaeology]. Vestnik Tomskogo Gosudarstvennogo universiteta.
Istorija 5 (43): 152-156.
2017
A generalized assessment of cultural changes at stratified sites:
The case of Chalcolithic fortresses in the northwestern Caucasus.
Archaeology, Ethnology and Anthropology of Eurasia 45 (1): 62–75.
2018
On certain aspects of distance-based models of language
relationships, with reference to the position of Indo-European
among other language families. Journal of Indo-European Studies
46 (1-2): 173–205.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
368
Alexander Kozintsev
Krauss, Raiko, Clemens Schmid, Dan Ciobotaru & Vladimir Slavchev
2016
Varna und die Folgen – Überlegungen zu den Ockergräbern
zwischen Karpatenbecken und der nördlichen Ägäis. In M.
Bartelheim, B. Herejs & R. Krauss (eds.). Von Baden bis Troia.
Ressourcennutzung, Metallurgie und Wissenstransfer. Ein
Jubiläumsschrift für Ernst Pernicka, 273–315. Rahden/Westf.:
Verlag Marie Leidorf.
Kristiansen, Kristian
2012
The Bronze Age expansion of Indo-European languages: an
archaeological model In C. Prescott & H. Glørstad (eds.).
Becoming European: The Transformation of Third Millennium
Northern and Western Europe, 165–181. Oxford: Oxbow Books.
Kuz’mina, Elena E.
2007
The Origin of the Indo-Iranians. Edited by J.P. Mallory.
Leiden/Boston: Brill.
Lazaridis, Iosif, Alissa Mittnik, Nick Patterson et al.
2017
Genetic origins of the Minoans and Mycenaeans. Nature 548
(7666): 214-218.
Lazaridis, Iosif, Dani Nadel, Gary Rollefson et al.
2016
Genomic insights into the origin of farming in the ancient Near
East. Nature 536 (7617): 419-424.
Lazaridis, Iosif, Nick Patterson, Alissa Mittnik et al.
2014
Ancient human genomes suggest three ancestral populations for
present-day Europeans. Nature 513 (7518): 409–413.
Li, Chungxiang, Hongjie Li, Yinqiu Cui et al.
2010
Evidence that a West-East admixed population lived in the Tarim
Basin as early as the Early Bronze Age. BMC Biology 8:15.
Lipson, Mark, Anna Szécsényi-Nagy, Swapan Mallick et al.
2017
Parallel palaeogenomic transects reveal complex genetic history
of early European farmers. Nature 551 (7680): 368-372.
Lyonnet, Bertille
2007
La culture de Maïkop, la Transcaucasie, l’Anatolie orientale et le
Proche-Orient: Relations et chronologie. In B. Lyonnet (ed.). Les
cultures du Caucase (VIe-IIIe millénaires avant notre ère). Leurs
relations avec le Proche-Orient, 133–161. Paris: CNRS Éditions.
2009
Périphérie de la Mésopotamie à la période d’Uruk (4e millénaire):
le cas des régions du Caucase. In J.-M. Durand & A. Jacquet (eds.).
Centre et périphérie: approches nouvelles des Orientalists. Actes du
colloque organisè par l’Institut du Proche-Orient Ancien du Collège
de France, la Société Asiatique et le CNRS (UMR 7192) les 31 Mai et
er
1 Juin 2006, 1–28. Paris: Collège de France.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
369
Lyonnet, Bertille, Tufan Akhundov, Xaqani Almamedov et al.
2008
Late Chalcolithic kurgans in Transcaucasia: The cemetery of
Soyuq Bulaq (Azerbaijan). Archäologische Mitteilungen aus Iran
und Turan 40: 27–44.
Majidzadeh, Yousef
1982
Lapis lazuli and the Great Khorasan Road. Paléorient 8 (1): 59–69.
Makharadze, Zurab
2007
Nouvelles données sur le Chalcolithique en Géorgie orientale. In
B. Lyonnet (ed.). Les cultures du Caucase (VIe-IIIe millénaires avant
notre ère). Leurs relations avec le Proche-Orient, 123–131. Paris:
CNRS Éditions.
Mallory, J. P.
1982
Indo-European and Kurgan fauna. I: Wild animals. Journal of
Indo-European Studies 10: 193–222.
1989
In Search of the Indo-Europeans: Language, Archaeology and Myth.
London: Thames and Hudson.
2018
The Indo-Europeans and agriculture. In G. Kroonen, J.P. Mallory,
B. Comrie (eds.). Talking Neolithics: Proceedings of the Workshop
on Indo-European Origins Held at the Max Planck Institute for
Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, December 2–3, 2013. Journal of
Indo-European Studies Monograph Series 65: 196–241.
Mallory, J. P. & Douglas Q. Adams
2006
The Oxford Introduction to Proto-Indo-European and the Proto-IndoEuropean World. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.
Manzura, Igor V.
2000
Vladejuie skipetrami [The scepter owners]. Stratum Plus 2: 237–
295.
2013
Kul’tury stepnogo eneolita [Cultures of the steppe Eneolithic]. In
I.V. Bruyako & T.L. Samoilova (eds.). Drevnie kul’tury severozapadnogo Priernomorja, 115–160. Odessa: SMIL.
2017
Vostonaja Evropa na zare kurgannoj tradicii [Eastern Europe at
the dawn of the kurgan tradition]. In L.B. Vishnyatsky (ed.). Ex
Ungue Leonem. Sbornik statej k 90-letiju L’va Samuilovia Klejna,
107–129. Saint-Petersburg: Nestor-Istorija.
Marro, Catherine
2007
Upper Mesopotamia and Transcaucasia in the Late Chalcolithic
Period (4000–3500 BC). In B. Lyonnet (ed.). Les cultures du
Caucase (VIe-IIIe millénaires avant notre ère). Leurs relations avec
le Proche-Orient, 77–94. Paris: CNRS Éditions.
2010
Where did Late Chalcolithic Chaff-Faced Ware originate?
Cultural dynamics in Anatolia and Transcaucasia at the dawn of
urban civilization (ca 4500-3500 BC). Paléorient 36 (2): 35–55.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
370
2012
Alexander Kozintsev
Is there a post-Ubaid culture? Reflections on the transition from
the Ubaid to the Uruk periods along the Fertile Crescent and
beyond. In C. Marro (ed.). After the Ubaid: Interpreting Change
from the Caucasus to Mesopotamia at the Dawn of Urban
Civilization (4500–3500 BC). Papers from ‘The Post-Ubaid Horizon
in the Fertile Crescent and Beyond.’ International Workshop Held at
th
st
Fosseuse, 29 June – 1 July 2009, 13–38. Paris: De Boccard
Édition.
Marro, Catherine, Veli Bakhshaliyev & Safar Ashurov
2009, 2011 Excavations at Ovçular Tepesi (Nakhchivan, Azerbaijan).
Anatolia Antiqua 17: 31–87; 19: 53–100.
Marro, Catherine, Veli Bakhshaliev & Rémi Berthon
2014
On the genesis of the Kura-Araxes phenomenon: New evidence
from Nakhichevan (Azerbaijan). Paléorient 40 (2): 131–154.
Masson, Vadim M.
Poselenie Jeitun (Problema stanovlenija proizvodjaej ekonomiki)
1971
[The Settlement of Jeitun (The Problem of Origin of the
Producing Economy)]. Leningrad: Nauka.
1981
Altyn-Depe. Leningrad: Nauka.
Drevnie civilizacii Vostoka i stepnye plemena v svete dannyx
1999
arxeologii [Ancient civilizations of the Near East and steppe
tribes in the light of archaeological data]. Stratum Plus 2: 265–285.
Kul’turogenez drevnej Central’noj Azii [Cultural Genesis of
2006
Ancient Central Asia]. Saint-Petersburg: Saint Petersburg State
University Press.
Matasovi, Ranko
2012
Areal typology of proto-Indo-European: The case for Caucasian
connections. Transactions of the Philological Society 110 (2): 283–
310.
Mathieson, Iain, Songül Alpaslan-Roodenberg, Cosimo Posth et al.
2018
The genomic history of Southeastern Europe. Nature 555 (7695):
197-203.
Mathieson, Iain, Iosif Lazaridis, Nadin Rohland et al.
2015
Genome-wide patterns of selection in 230 ancient Eurasians.
Nature 528 (7583): 499–503.
Matyushin, Gerald N.
2003
Problems of inhabiting central Eurasia: Mesolithic – Eneolithic
exploitation of the central Eurasian steppes. In M. Levine, C.
Renfrew & K. Boyle (eds.). Prehistoric Steppe Adaptation and the
Horse, 367–393. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for
Archaeological Research.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
371
Melchert, H. Craig
2011
Indo-Europeans. In S.R. Steadman & G. McMahon (eds.). The
Oxford Handbook of Ancient Anatolia, 10,000–323 B.C.E., 704–716.
Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.
Menk, Roland
1980
A synopsis of the physical anthropology of the Corded Ware
complex on the background of the expansion of the Kurgan
culture. Journal of Indo-European Studies 8 (3-4): 361–392.
Militarev, Alexander Y.
2002
The prehistory of a dispersal: The Proto-Afrasian (Afroasiatic)
farming lexicon. In P. Bellwood & C. Renfrew (eds.). Examining
the Farming/Language Dispersal Hypothesis, 135–150. Cambridge:
McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
Mova, Tamara G.
1961
O sviazjax plemen tripol’skoj kul’tury so stepnymi plemenami
mednogo veka [On the ties between the Tripolye tribes and the
steppe tribes of the Chalcolithic]. Sovetskaja arxeologija 2: 186–
199.
Munchaev, Rauf M.
1975
Kavkaz na zare bronzovogo veka. Neolit, eneolit, rannjaja bronza
[Caucasus at the Dawn of the Bronze Age. Neolithic, Chalcolithic,
Early Bronze Age]. Moscow: Nauka.
1994
Majkopskaja kul’tura [The Maykop Culture]. In K.C. Kushnareva
& V.I. Markovin (eds.). Epoxa bronzy Kavkaza i Srednej Azii.
Rannjaja i srednjaja bronza Kavkaza, 158–225. Moscow: Nauka.
Munchaev, Rauf M. & Shahmardan N. Amirov
2012
Ee raz o mesopotamsko-kavkazskix svjazjax [Ties between
Mesopotamia and Caucasus revisited]. Rossijskaja arxeologija 4:
37–46.
Muradyan, Firdus
2014
Discovery of the first Chalcolithic burial mounds in the Republic
of Armenia. In B. Gasparyan & M. Arimura (eds.). Stone Age of
Armenia: A Guide-book to the Stone Age Archaeology in the
Republic of Armenia, 339–364. Kanazawa: Center for Cultural
Studies, Kanazawa University.
Muscarella, Oscar W.
2013
Archaeology, Artifacts and Antiquities of the Ancient Near East.
Sites, Cultures, and Proveniences. Leiden/Boston: Brill.
Museibli, Najaf
2014
The Grave Monuments and Burial Customs of the Leilatepe Culture.
Baku: Nafta-Press.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
372
2016
Alexander Kozintsev
Potter’s marks on Leilatepe culture pottery: Eastern Anatolian
Chalcolithic traditions in the Caucasus. Mediterranean
Archaeology and Archaeometry 16 (1): 283–294.
Naert, Pierre
1958
La situation linguistique de l’Aïnou. 1. Aïnou et indoeuropéen.
Lund: C. W. K. Gleerup.
Napolskikh, Vladimir V.
1997
Vvedenie v istorieskuju uralistiku [Introduction to Historical
Uralistics]. Ievsk: Udmurtskij Institut jazyka, istorii i literatury
URO RAN.
Narasimhan, Vagheesh M., Nick Patterson, Priya Moorjani et al.
The genomic formation of South and Central Asia. bioRxiv 31
2018
March. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1101/292581.
Nekhajev, Alexej A.
1992
Domajkopskaja kul’tura Severnogo Kavkaza [The pre-Maykop
culture of the northern Caucasus]. Arxeologieskie vesti 1: 76–96.
Nichols, Johanna
1997
The epicenter of the Indo-European linguistic spread. In R.
Blench & M. Spriggs (eds.). Archaeology and Language, vol. 1.
Theoretical and Methodological Orientations, 122–148.
London/New York: Routledge.
1998
The Eurasian spread zone and the Indo-European dispersal. In R.
Blench & M. Spriggs (eds.). Archaeology and Language, vol. 2.
Archaeological Data and Linguistic Hypotheses, 220–266.
London/New York: Routledge.
2019
Comments on Allan Bomhard, “The Origins of Proto-IndoEuropean: The Cauasian Substrate Hypothesis”. Journal of IndoEuropean Studies 47 (1-2): 147-154.
Nikolaeva, Nadezhda A. & Vladimir A. Safronov
1974
Proisxodenie dol’mennoj kul’tury severo-zapadnogo Kavkaza
[The origin of the Dolmen culture of the northwestern Caucasus].
In Voprosy oxrany, klassifikacii i ispol’zovanija arxeologieskix
pamjatnikov, 174–306. Moscow: Znanie.
1982
Xronologija i proisxodenie majkopskogo iskusstva [The
chronology and origin of Maykop art]. In T.B. Turgiev (ed.).
Xronologija pamjatnikov bronzovogo veka Severnogo Kavkaza, 28–
63. Ordonikidze: Izdatel’stvo Severo-osetinskogo
gosudarstevnnogo universiteta.
Olalde, Iñigo, Selina Brace, Morten E. Allentoft et al.
2018
The Beaker phenomenon and the genomic transformation of
northwest Europe. Nature 555 (7695): 190–196.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
373
Olijdam, Eric
2000
Additional evidence of a late second millennium lapis lazuli route:
The Fullol hoard (Afghanistan). In M. Taddei (ed.). South Asian
th
Archaeology 1997. Proceedings of the 14 International Conference
of the European Association of South Asian Archaeologists, vol. 1,
397–407. Rome: Istituto Italiano per l’Africa e l’ Oriente.
Oswalt, Robert L.
1998
A probabilistic evaluation of North Eurasiatic Nostratic. In J.C.
Salmons & B.D. Joseph (eds.). Nostratic: Sifting the Evidence, 199–
216. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Otkupikov Yuri V.
1988.
Dogreeskij substrat. U istokov evropejskoj civilizacii [The PreGreek Substrate: At the Sources of the European Civilization].
Leningrad: Leningrad State University Press.
Pagani, Luca, Vincenza Colonna, Chris Tyler-Smith & Qasim Ayub
2017
An ethnolinguistic and genetic perspective on the origins of the
Dravidian-speaking Brahui in Pakistan. Man in India 97 (1): 267–
278.
Pagel, Mark, Quentin D. Atkinson, Andrea S. Calude & Andrew Meade
2013
Ultraconserved words point to deep language ancestry across
Eurasia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of U.S.A.
110 (21): 8471–8476.
Palaguta, Ilya V.
1998
K probleme svjazej Tripolja-Cucuteni s kul’turami eneolita
stepnoj zony Severnogo Priernomorja [The problem of ties
between Tripolye-Cucuteni and the Eneolithic cultures of the
steppe zone of the North Pontic region]. Rossijskaja arxeologija
1:5-14.
Palumbi, Giulio
2011
The Chalcolithic of Eastern Anatolia. In S.R. Steadman & G.
McMahon (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Ancient Anatolia,
10,000–323 B.C.E., 205–226. Oxford/New York: Oxford University
Press.
Palumbi, Giulio & Christine Chataigner
2015
Answer to C. Marro, V. Bakhshaliev & R. Berthon. Paléorient 41
(2): 163–166.
Parzinger, Hermann
2006
Die frühen Völker Eurasiens vom Neolithikum bis zum Mittelalter.
München: C.H. Beck.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
374
Alexander Kozintsev
Peasnall, Brian & Mitchell S. Rothman
2003
One of Iraq’s earliest towns. Expedition 45 (3): 34–39.
Pereltsvaig, Asya & Martin W. Lewis.
2015
The Indo-European Controversy. Facts and Fallacies in Historical
Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Piazza, Alberto, Sabina Rendine, Eric Minch et al.
1995
Genetics and the origin of European languages. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the USA 92 (13): 5836–5840.
Pitskhelauri, Konstantine
2012
Uruk migrants in the Caucasus. Bulletin of the Georgian National
Academy of Sciences 6 (2): 153–161.
Poplevko, Galina N.
2017
Nekotorye priemy formovki majkopskoj keramiki [Certain
techniques of molding Maykop ceramics]. In N. Museibli (ed.).
Problemy arxeologii Kavkaza i Perednej Azii. Neolit – pozdnjaja
bronza. K 90-letihu I.G. Narimanova, 176–199. Baku: privately
published.
Potekhina, Inna D.
1995
Antropologieskie materialy iz pogrebenij rannego mednogo
veka v Unakozovskoj peere [Human skeletal remains from the
Chalcolithic burials in Unakozovskaja Cave]. P.A. Dietler (ed.).
Arxeologija Adygei, 30–35. Maykop: Meoty.
Potts, Daniel T.
2011
Equus asinus in highland Iran: Evidence old and new. In N.J.
Conard, P. Drechsler & A. Morales (eds.). Between Sand and Sea:
The Archaeology and Human Ecology of Southwestern Asia.
Festschrift in Honor of Hans-Peter Uerpmann, 167–175. Tübingen:
Kerns Verlag.
Poulmarc’h, Modwene & Françoise Le Mort
2016
Diversification of the funerary practices in the southern Caucasus
from the Neolithic to the Chalcolithic. Quaternary International
395: 184–193.
Pudas Marlow, Elli J.
1974
More on the Uralo-Dravidian Relationship: A Comparison of Uralic
and Dravidian Etymological Vocabularies. Ph. D. Thesis.
University of Michigan. Ann Arbor.
Rassamakin, Yuri J.
1999
The Eneolithic of the Black Sea steppe: Dynamics of cultural and
economic development 4500–2300 BC. In M. Levine, Y.
Rassamakin, A. Kislenko & N. Tatarintseva (eds.). Late Prehistoric
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
2012
375
Exploitation of the Eurasian Steppe, 59-182. Cambridge: McDonald
Institute for Archaeological Research.
Absolute chronology of Ukrainian Tripolian settlements. In F.
Menotti & A.G. Korvin-Piotrovskiy (eds.). The Tripolye Culture
Giant-Settlements in Ukraine: Formation, Development and Decline,
19–69. Oxford: Oxbow Books.
Raveane, Alessandro, Serena Aneli, Francesco Montinaro et al.
2018
Population structure of modern-day Italians reveals patterns of
ancient and archaic ancestries in Southern Europe bioRxiv 13
December. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/494898
Reich, David
2018
Who We Are and How We Got Here: Ancient DNA and the New
Science of the Human Past. N.Y.: Pantheon Books.
Renfrew, Colin
1988
Archaeology and Language. The Puzzle of Indo-European Origins.
London: Penguin Books.
1991
Before Babel: Speculations on the origins of linguistic diversity.
Cambridge Archaeological Journal 1 (1): 3–23.
Rezepkin, Alexey D.
2000
Das frühbronzezeitliche Gräberfeld von Klady und die MajkopKultur in Nordwestkaukasien. Rahden/Westf.: Verlag Marie
Leidorf.
2017
Majkopskaja i leilatepinskaja kul'tury: erty obego i razlinogo
[Maykop and Leilatepe cultures: similarities and differences]. In:
N. Museibli (ed.). Problemy arxeologii Kavkaza i Perednej Azii.
Neolit – pozdnjaja bronza K 90-letihu I.G. Narimanova, 200–210.
Baku: privately published.
Ringe, Don
1998
A probabilistic evaluation of Indo-Uralic. In J.C. Salmons & B.D.
Joseph (eds.). Nostratic: Sifting the Evidence, 153–197.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Ringe, Don, Tandy Warnow & Ann Taylor
2002
Indo-European and computational cladistics. Transactions of the
Philological Society 100 (1): 59–129.
Saag, Lehti, Margot Laneman, Liivi Varul et al.
2019
The arrival of Siberian ancestry connecting the Eastern Baltic to
Uralic speakers further east. Current Biology 29: 1–11. DOI:
10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.026.
Safronov,Vladimir A.
1989
Indoevropejskie prarodiny [Indo-European Homelands]. Gorky:
Volga-Vyatka Press.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
376
Alexander Kozintsev
Sagona, Antonio
2018
The Archaeology of the Caucasus. From Earliest Settlements to the
Iron Age. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Salmons, Joseph
2018
A methodological challenge for Neolithic linguistics: The search
for substrate vocabulary. In G. Kroonen, J.P. Mallory, B. Comrie
(eds.). Talking Neolithics: Proceedings of the Workshop on IndoEuropean Origins Held at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary
Anthropology, Leipzig, December 2–3, 2013. Journal of IndoEuropean Studies Monograph Series 65: 315–335.
Schmidt, Hubert
1932
Cucuteni in der oberen Moldau, Rumänien. Die befestigte Siedlung
mit bemalter Keramik von der Steinkupferzeit bis in die
vollentwickelte Bronzezeit. Berlin/Leipzig: Walter de Gruyter.
Schwidetzky, Ilse
1980
The influence of the steppe people based on the physical
anthropological data in special connection to the Corded Ware
Battle Axe culture. Journal of Indo-European Studies 8 (3-4): 345–
360.
Seresti, Rahmat A. & Somayeh A. Tashvigh
2016
Central Plateau of Iran: The transition from Sialk III6-7 to Sialk
IV1. International Journal of the Society of Iranian Archaeologists 2
(4): 15–25.
Shevoroshkin, Vitaly V.
1987
Indo-European homeland and migrations. Folia Linguistica
Historica 7 (2): 227–250.
Shishlina, Natalia I.
2013
The steppe and the Caucasus during the Bronze Age: Mutual
relationships and mutual enrichments. In S. Bergerbrant & S.
Sabatini (eds.). Counterpoint: Essays in Archaeology and Heritage
Studies in Honour of Professor Kristian Kristiansen, 53–69. Oxford:
Publishers of British Archaeological Reports (BAR International
Series, No. 2508).
Shishlina, Natalia I., Elya P. Zazovskaya, Johannes van der Plicht et al.
14
2009
Paleoecology, subsistence, and C chronology of the Eurasian
Caspian steppe Bronze Age. Radiocarbon 51 (2): 481–499.
Silva, Marina, Marisa Oliveira, Daniel Vieira et al.
2017
A genetic chronology for the Indian Subcontinent points to
heavily sex-biased dispersals. Evolutionary Biology !7: 88.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
377
Skourtanioti, Eirini & Erdal Y. Selim
2018
Palaeogenetic and anthropological perspectives on Late
th
Chalcolithic and Early Bronze Age Arslantepe. In The 11
International Congress on the Archaeology of the Ancient Near East,
03–07 April 2018. Abstracts, 293. Munich: Ludwig-MaximiliansUniversität.
Sokal, Robert R., Neal L. Oden & Barbara A. Thomson
1992
Origins of the Indo-Europeans: Genetic evidence. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 89 (16): 7669–7673.
Solovyev, Lev N.
1958
Novyi pamjatnik kul'turnyx svjazej kavkazskogo Priernomorja v
epoxu neolita i bronzy – stojanki Voroncovskoj peery [New
evidence of cultural ties of the Caucasian Black Sea coast in the
Neolithic and Bronze Age — the Vorontsov Cave sites]. Trudy
Abxazskogo Instituta Jazyka, Literatury i Istorii 29: 135–184.
Stadler, Peter, Suzanne Draxler, Hervig Friesinger et al.
2001
Absolute chronology for early civilizations in Austria and Central
14
Europe using C dating with accelerator mass spectrometry with
special results for the Baden culture. In P. Roman (ed.). Cernavod
III – Boleráz. Ein vorgeschichtliches Phänomen zwischen dem
Oberrhein und der unteren Donau, 541–554. Bucureti: Institutul
Român de Tracologie.
Starostin, George S.
2007
A comment on “Indo-European glottochronology and homeland”
by Sergei A. Starostin. In S.A. Starostin. Trudy po jazykoznaniju,
895. Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskix kul’tur.
Starostin, Sergei A.
1988
Indoevropejsko-severnokavkazskie izoglossy [IE – North
Caucasian isoglosses]. In G.M. Bongard-Levin & V.G. Ardzinba
(eds.). Drevnij Vostok: Etnokul’turnye svjazi, 112-163. Moscow:
Nauka.
1999
Subgrouping of Nostratic: Comments on A. Dolgopolsky’s
“Nostratic Macrofamily and Linguistic Paleontology.” In C.
Renfrew & D. Nettle (eds.). Nostratic: Examining a Linguistic
Macrofamily (Papers in the Prehistory of Language), 137–156.
Cambridge: Institute of Archaeological Research.
2007/1995 Problema genetieskogo rodstva i klassifikacii kavkazskix
jazykov s toki zrenija bazisnoj leksiki [The problem of the
genetic affinities and classification of the Caucasian languages
from the standpoint of basic vocabulary]. In S.A. Starostin. Trudy
po jazykoznaniju, 529–546. Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskix kul’tur.
2007/2000 Indo-European among other language families: Problems of
dating, contacts and genetic relationships. In S.A. Starostin. Trudy
po jazykoznaniju, 806–820. Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskix kul’tur.
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
378
Alexander Kozintsev
2007/2001–2002 Indo-European glottochronology and homeland. In S.A.
Starostin. Trudy po jazykoznaniju, 821–826. Moscow: Jazyki
slavjanskix kul’tur.
Stolyar, Abram D., Alexander A. Formozov & Sergei M. Ostainsky (eds.)
Meoko – drevnejaja krepost’ Predkavkazja [Meshoko — the
2009
Earliest Fortress in the Caucasian Piedmont]. Saint-Petersburg:
The State Hermitage Press.
Sverkov, Leonid M.
2011
Toxary: drevnie indoevropejcy v Central’noj Azii [Tocharians:
Early Indo-Europeans in Central Asia]. Tashkent: SMI-ASIA.
Szymczak, Karol & Mukhiddin Khudzhanazarov
2011
The milky white chalcedonite/opal distribution in the Neolithic
Kelteminar culture of the Kyzyl-Kums, Uzbekistan. wiatowit
8/49: 35–40.
Tolstov, Sergei P.
1941
Drevnosti verxnego Xorezma (osnovnye itogi rabot Xorezmskoj
ekspedicii) [Antiquities of the Upper Khorezm (principal findings
of the Khorezm Expedition)]. Vestnik drevnej istorii 1: 155–184.
1948
Drevnij Xorezm. Opyt istoriko-arxeologieskogo issledovanija
[Ancient Khorezm: A historical and archaeological study].
Moscow: Moscow State University Press.
Trifonov, Viktor A.
2000
Kurgany majkopskogo tipa v severo-zapadnom Irane [Maykop
type tumuli in northwestern Iran]. In N.G. Gorbunova, N.K.
Kachalova & Y.Y. Piotrovsky (eds.). Sud’ba uenogo: K 100-letiju so
dnja rodenija Borisa Aleksandrovia Latynina, 244–264. SaintPetersburg: The State Hermitage Publishers.
2001a
Darkveti-Meshokovskaja kul’tura [The Darkveti-Meshoko
culture]. In Tretja Kubanskaja arxeologieskaja konferencija. Tezisy
dokladov medunarodnoj arxeologieskoj konferencii, 190–194.
Krasnodar–Anapa.
2001b
Popravki k absoljutnoj xronologii kul’tur epoxi eneolita – srednej
bronzy Kavkaza, stepnoj i lesostepnoj zon Vostonoj Evropy (po
dannym radiouglerodnogo datirovanija) [Corrections to the
absolute chronology of Eneolithic to Middle Bronze Age cultures
of the Caucasus, the steppe and forest-steppe zones of Eastern
Europe (based on radiocarbon dating)]. In Bronzovyj vek Vostonoj
Evropy: xarakteristika kul’tur, xronologija i periodizacija. Materialy
medunarodnoj naunoj konferencii “K stoletiju periodizacii V.A.
Gorodcova bronzovogo veka junoj poloviny Vostonoj Evropy” 23–
28 aprelja 2001 g., 71–82: Samara: Nauno-texnieskij centr.
2004
Die Majkop-Kultur und die ersten Wagen in der südrussischen
Steppe. In M. Fansa & S. Burmeister (eds.). Rad und Wagen. Der
The Journal of Indo-European Studies
Proto-Indo-Europeans: The Prologue
379
Ursprung einer Innovation. Wagen im Vorderen Orient und Europa,
167–176. Mainz am Rhein: Philipp von Zabern.
2009
Suestvoval li na Severo-Zapadnom Kavkaze neolit? [Did the
Neolithic exist in the northwestern Caucasus?] In Adaptacija
kul’tur paleolita – eneolita k izmenenijam prirodnoj sredy na
Severo-Zapadnom Kavkaze, 84–93. Saint-Petersburg: Teza.
2014
Zapadnye predely rasprostrannenija majkopskoj kul’tury [The
western boundary of the Maykop culture distribution area].
Izvestija Samarskogo naunogo centra Rossijskoj Akademii nauk 16
(3): 276–284.
Trifonov, Viktor A., Natalia I. Shishlina, Johannes van der Plicht et al.
2017
Radiouglerodnaja xronologija dol’menov epoxi rannej bronzy u
st. Carskaja, severo-zapadnyj Kavkaz, 1898 g. [The radiocarbon
chronology of the Early Bronze Age dolmens near the Cossack
village of Tsarskaya, northwestern Caucasus (1898)]. In A.P.
Derevianko (ed.). V (XXI) Vserossijskij arxeologieskij sjezd.
Sbornik naunyx trudov, 1042–1043. Barnaul: Altai State
University Press.
Trubetzkoy, Nikolai S.
1939
Gedanken über das Indogermanenproblem. Acta linguistica 1 (2).
81-89.
Uhlenbeck, Christianus C.
1937 Indogermanic mother language and mother tribes complex. American
Anthropologist 37 (3): 385–393.
Van Loon, Maurits N. (ed.)
1988
Hammam et-Turkman I. Report on the University of Amsterdam’s
1981-84 Excavations in Syria. Istanbul: Nederlands HistorischArchaeologisch Instituut.
Vinogradov Alexander V.
1957
K voprosu o junyx svjazjax kel’teminarskoj kul’tury [On
southern ties of the Kelteminar culture]. Sovetskaja etnografija 1:
25–45.
1981
Drevnie oxotniki i rybolovy Sredneaziatskogo medureja [Ancient
Hunters and Fishers of the Western Central Asian Interfluve].
Moscow: Nauka.
Wang, Chuan-Chao, Sabine Reinhold, Alexey Kalmykov et al.
2019
Ancient human genome-wide data from a 3000-year interval in
the Caucasus corresponds with eco-geographic regions. Nature
Communiications 10:590. DOI: 10.1038/s41467-018-08220-8.
Watson, Wilfried G.E.
2013
Indo-European and Semitic: Two-way traffic. In J.P. MonferrerSala & W.G.E. Watson (eds.). Archaism and Innovation in the
Volume 47, Number 3 & 4, Fall/Winter 2019
380
Alexander Kozintsev
Semitic Languages: Selected Papers, 163–193. Cordoba: Cordoba
Near Eastern Research Unit.
Whittaker, Gordon
2012
Euphratic: A phonological sketch. In B.N. Whitehead, T. Olander,
B.A. Olsen & J.E. Rasmussen (eds.). The Sound of Indo-European.
Phonetics, Phonemics, and Morphophonemics, 577–606.
Copenhagen:Museum Tusculanum Press.
Wild, Eva M., Peter Stadler, Mária Bondár et al.
2001
New chronological frame for the Young Neolithic Baden Culture
th
in Central Europe (4 millennium BC). Radiocarbon 43 (2B):
1057–1064.
Yakhontov, Sergei E.
1991
Prarodina nostratieskix jazykov [The homeland of the Nostratic
languages] In R.V. Bulatova, G.I. Zamjatina & S.L. Nikolaev (eds.).
Slavistika. Indoevropeistika. Nostratika. K 60-letiju so dnja
rozhdenija V.A. Dybo, 13–17. oscow: Institute for Slavic and
Balkan Studies.
Yakubovich. Ilya
2010
Sociolinguistics of the Luvian Language. Leiden/Boston: Brill.
Yanovich, Igor, Armin Buch, Johannes Dellert et al.
Archaeological temporal constraints make phylogenetic methods
2015
support the steppe homeland theory of Indo-European. In
Linguistics, Archaeology and Genetics: Integrating New Evidence for
st
the Origin and Spread of the Indo-European Languages. 1
International Conference/Workshop, Jena. Book of Abstracts, 17–18.
Zerjal, Tatiana, Arpita Pandya, Fabricio R. Santos et al.
1999
The use of Y-chromosomal DNA variation to investigate
population history. In S.S. Papiha, R. Deca, R. Chakraborty (eds.).
Genomic Diversity: Applications in Human Population Genetics,
91–101. New York: Plenum Publishers.
Zvelebil, Marek
At the interface of archaeology, linguistics and genetics: Indo1995
European dispersals and the agricultural transition in Europe.
Journal of European Archaeology 3 (1): 33–70.
The Journal of Indo-European Studies