Intelligence 35 (2007) 115 – 121
National differences in intelligence and educational attainment
Richard Lynn a,⁎, Jaan Mikk b
a
University of Ulster, Coleraine, Ireland
b
Šiauliai University, Lithuania
Received 9 March 2006; received in revised form 7 June 2006; accepted 7 June 2006
Available online 7 July 2006
Abstract
We examine the correlations between the national IQs of Lynn and Vanhanen (Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the
wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. Westport, CT: Praeger, Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality.
Athens, GA: Washington Summit Books.) and educational attainment scores in math and science for 10- and 14-year olds in 25
countries and 46 countries (respectively) given in the TIMSS 2003 reports. It was found that national IQs had (attenuation
corrected) correlations of between 0.92 and 1.00 with scores in math and science. The results are interpreted as a validation of the
national IQs. They suggest that national differences in educational attainment may be attributable to differences in IQ, or
alternatively that national IQs and in educational attainment are both indicators of the mental ability of national populations. It is
also shown that national IQs are positively associated with national per capita income (r = .61). It is proposed that these have a
reciprocal positive feedback relationship such that each augments the other.
© 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Keywords: National IQs; Educational attainment; TIMSS
1. Introduction
This paper reports high across country correlations
between the national IQs calculated by Lynn and
Vanhanen (2002, 2006) and national differences in
educational attainment in mathematics and science. The
paper has three objectives. First, to show that these
correlations provide some validation of the national IQs.
Second, to discuss the mechanisms underlying these
correlations. And third, to examine the relationships
between national differences in IQ and educational
attainment and a number of educational, economic and
demographic variables.
⁎ Corresponding author. University of Ulster, Coleraine, Ireland.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (R. Lynn).
0160-2896/$ - see front matter © 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.06.001
The first objective is addressed because of the
controversy concerning the validity of the national IQs.
Among those who have accepted the national IQs as
valid, it has been argued that they are a significant
correlate of economic growth (Weede & Kämpf, 2002;
McDaniel & Whetzel, 2004; Dickerson, 2006; Jones &
Schneider, 2006; Whetzel & McDaniel, in press),
measures of education (Barber, 2005), rates of suicide
(Voracek, 2004), and skin color and winter temperature
(Templer & Arikawa, 2006).
Others, however, have contended that the national
IQs presented by Lynn and Vanhanen (2002) have no
validity. Among these, Ervik (2003, p.406) has written:
“the authors fail to establish the reliability …of
intelligence (IQ) test scores” (a measure that has no
reliability must necessarily have no validity). Volken
116
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
(2003, p.411) criticises the national IQs on the grounds
of the “highly deficient data…in the type of IQ test used”.
Barnett and Williams (2004, p.392) criticise the IQs on
the grounds that the samples “are, in many cases, not
representative of the countries from which they are
derived” and assert that the cross-country “comparisons
are virtually meaningless”. Hunt and Sternberg (2006,
pp. 133,136) also assert that “the estimates of national IQ
are technically inadequate” and “the concept of national
IQ is meaningless”.
The classical method of establishing the validity of
intelligence tests is to show that they are correlated with
educational attainment: “thousands of studies have been
published, in numerous languages throughout the world,
attempting to demonstrate the validity of intelligence
tests against academic performance in school” (Matarazzo, 1972, p.281). Here we apply the same method to
establish the validity of national IQs by showing that
they are highly correlated with scores on tests of mathematics and science.
The second objective of the present paper is to discuss
the mechanisms underlying the correlations between
national IQs and educational attainment in mathematics
and science. National differences in educational attainment in mathematics and science have been reported since
the 1960s in a series of reports of the International
Mathematics and Science Studies (Husen, 1967; Comber
& Keeves, 1973; Baker & Jones, 1993; Beaton et al.,
1996a,b; Beaton et al., 1996a,b). The national differences
in average scores in these studies have been quite
consistent. In general, the Chinese, Japanese and Koreans
have achieved the highest scores, followed by the
Europeans, while a number of developing countries
have had the lowest scores. It has not proved possible to
find a full explanation for these differences. Several
hypotheses have been examined, including those that the
differences may be partly determined by expenditure on
schools, class size, and the qualifications of teachers, but
none of these have produced substantial associations with
educational attainment.
Although it is forty years since the publication by
Husen (1967) of the first study of national differences in
educational attainment in mathematics, the possibility that
these might be associated with differences in intelligence,
or even partly caused by differences in intelligence, has
never been considered. The reason for this is that there
have been no data on national differences in intelligence
that could be used to examine these possibilities. This
situation has changed with the publication by Lynn and
Vanhanen (2002, 2006) of IQs for all nations in the world.
This makes it possible to test the hypothesis, and this is the
second objective of the present paper.
The hypothesis that the national differences in educational attainment are associated with differences in
intelligence is derived from the numerous studies in
many countries showing that intelligence is associated
with educational attainment at a magnitude of a correlation of around .5 to .7. The results of nine studies
showing this are summarized in Table 1. This is not a
complete list of such reports, but we believe it is typical.
The first column gives the country, the second column
gives the number in the sample, the third gives the age at
which intelligence was measured, the fourth gives the
age at which educational attainment was measured, and
the fifth gives the educational subjects assessed by tests.
The correlations range between .45 and .74 with a
Table 1
Correlations between Intelligence and Educational Attainment
Country
Number
Age
IQ
Age
ed. att.
Subject
r
Reference
1
2
Canada
England
208
85
13
5
13
16
Several
English
.55
.61
3
England
85
5
16
Math
.72
4
5
Britain
N. Ireland
20,000
701
11
16
16
16
GCSE
GCSE
.74
.65
6
USA
–
–
–
Several
.71
7
USA
455
13
13
Reading
.68
8
USA
–
18
18
Math
.66
9
Switzerland
82
11
11
Math
.45
Gagne and St. Pere, 2002
Yule et al.,
1982
Yule et al.,
1982
Deary, 2004
Lynn et al.,
1984
Walberg,
1984
Lloyd and
Barenblatt,
1984
Lubinski and Humphreys,
1996
Tewes, 2003
117
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
median of .66. It makes little difference to the size of the
correlations whether intelligence is measured early in
childhood or in adolescence. One of the highest
correlations (.72) is between intelligence measured at
the age of 5 years and educational attainment in
mathematics at the age of 16 years.
2. Method
In their first study Lynn and Vanhanen (2002)
published IQs for 81 countries obtained from studies in
which intelligence tests have been administered to
samples of the populations. In their second study the
number of studies in which intelligence tests have been
administered to samples of the populations and for which
IQs were calculated was increased to 113 countries
(Lynn & Vanhanen, 2006). For most of these countries
IQs were measured by the Progressive Matrices, a nonverbal reasoning test. In other cases IQs were measured
by a variety of other non-verbal tests including the
Cattell Culture Fair and the Goodenough Draw-a-Person
test. National IQs were calculated in relation to a mean
IQ of 100 in Britain and standard deviation of 15. The
increases in intelligence known as the Flynn effect were
taken into account in these calculations.
They also estimated IQs for a further 79 countries for
which there is no evidence from intelligence tests. IQs
for these 79 countries were estimated from the measured
IQs of similar and geographically adjacent countries. For
example, the IQ in Afghanistan was estimated at 84 on
the basis of the measured IQ of 84 in neighboring Iran
and Pakistan. By the use of this method they provided
IQs for all the 192 nations of the world with populations
over 40,000. Some of the national IQs reported in Lynn
and Vanhanen's second study differ slightly from those
reported in their first study. The explanation for this is
that they had collected more studies in the second study,
but the differences are very small.
The data for counties' educational achievement in
science and mathematics in were taken from the TIMSS
2003 study. This study was carried out by testing
representative samples of school students from each
country. The student samples were obtained by random
sampling. At the first stage of the sampling procedure,
150 public and private schools were selected in each
country. At the second stage, random sampling was used
to find one class in every school that had two or more
classes of students of the required age. The percentage
of excluded students was less than five percent from the
desired sample. The average number of students tested
in a country was 4498 for the grade 4 study and 4777
students for the grade 8 study. The smallest number of
Table 2
National IQs and TIMSS scores on math and science
Countries
IQ
Math
Science
Math
Science
Armenia
Australia
Bahrain
Belgium
Botswana
Bulgaria
Chile
Cyprus
Egypt
England
Estonia
Ghana
Hong Kong
Hungary
Indonesia
Iran
Israel
Italy
Japan
Jordan
Korea, Rep. of
Latvia
Lebanon
Lithuania
Macedonia
Malaysia
Moldova
Morocco
Netherlands
New Zealand
Norway
Palestine
Philippines
Romania
Russia
Saudi Arabia
Scotland
Serbia
Singapore
Slovakia
Slovenia
South Africa
Sweden
Taiwan
Tunisia
United States
94⁎
98
83⁎
99
70⁎
93
90
91⁎
81
100
99
71
108
98
87
84
95
102
105
84
106
98⁎
82
91
91
92
96
84
100
99
100
86
86
94
97
84⁎
97
89
108
96
96
72
99
105
83
98
Grade 4
449
507
–
544
–
–
–
511
–
534
–
–
565
526
–
395
–
504
566
–
–
531
–
529
–
–
502
356
536
502
462
–
362
–
527
–
499
–
580
–
486
–
–
559
330
521
Grade 4
438
520
–
518
–
–
–
483
–
538
–
–
540
529
–
424
–
517
541
–
–
532
–
510
–
–
499
306
518
519
470
–
332
–
527
–
502
–
558
–
492
–
–
551
317
534
Grade 8
470
506
406
534
369
475
394
459
406
502
531
282
581
526
413
414
492
483
569
428
585
507
432
500
435
505
457
390
535
497
465
395
380
472
505
339
500
473
596
505
492
278
499
580
409
502
Grade 8
455
525
442
515
366
479
421
444
423
540
548
258
550
539
426
458
489
493
546
476
550
512
396
518
451
510
471
399
532
518
496
439
378
472
512
398
510
468
569
517
520
257
522
565
412
526
students tested in a country was 2890 and the largest
number was 9829 (Martin, Mullis, Gonzales, &
Chrostowski, 2004, pp. 369–375). The students of
grade 4 were 10 years old and the students of the grade
8 were 14 years old. The same students were involved in
the science and in the mathematics study in both grades.
The TIMSS tests included the same tasks in all the
participating countries. The tests represented the curricula
118
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
Table 3
Correlation coefficients (corrected for attenuation in brackets) between
TIMSS 2003 results and IQ
Subject
Grade
All countries
Measured IQ countries
Mathematics
Science
Mathematics
Science
Fourth grade
Fourth grade
Eighth grade
Eighth grade
0.87 (.95)
0.85 (.92)
0.92 (1.0)
0.91 (.99)
0.89 (.97)
0.86 (.93)
0.93 (1.0)
0.91 (.99)
in the eighth grade. The reliability of the measures was
0.84 for both grades (Martin et al., 2004, pp. 356–386).
The TIMSS 2003 mathematics test was composed and
administered analogously to the TIMSS science tests. The
mathematics test included items on number, algebra,
measurement, geometry, and data. The items were in four
cognitive domains: knowing facts and procedures, using
concepts, solving routine problems, and reasoning
(Mullis, Martin, Gonzales, & Chrostowski, 2004,
p.343). About 40% of the items were in multiple-choice
format. The international average scale sore was 495 in the
fourth grade and 467 in the eighth grade. The reliability
was 0.87 for the fourth grade test and 0.89 for the eighth
grade mathematics test (Mullis et al., 2004, p.367).
In addition, questionnaires were given to students,
teachers and school head-teachers. The answers give
information about the learning and teaching background
in schools and homes.
The publication of the 2003 Trends International
Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) makes it
possible to examine the relation between the national
scores in mathematics and science and the national IQs
calculated by Lynn and Vanhanen in their more recent
of the countries as well as possible (Martin et al., 2004,
p.359). The science tests included items on life science,
chemistry, physics, earth science, and environmental
science. The test items were on three cognitive domains
— factual knowledge, conceptual understanding, and
reasoning and analysis (Martin et al., 2004, p.356).
Altogether 152 items in the test for the fourth grade level
and 189 test items were used in the test for the eighth
grade. The items in the tests were in multiple-choice
format. 1200–2000 students answered every test item.
Every correct answer was awarded one or two points
depending on the complexity of the item. The international average scores were 489 in the fourth grade and 474
Table 4
The correlation coefficients between TIMSS test results in grade 8 science and school characteristics (the labels are explained below of the table)
Mean
TIMSS
Mean
TIMSS
IQ Lynn
Textbook
Class size
Teach Univ+
Sch CL
Low
Stud safety
Life
expectancy
>50% EDH
GDP USD
Computer
Study desk
Parent Univ
IQ
Lynn
Textbook
Class
size
Teach
Univ +
Sch Cl
Low
1,00
Stud
safety
Life
expectancy
>50%
EDH
GDP
USD
1.00
−0.63
−0.74
−0.74
−0.51
1.00
0.83
0.47
0.43
Computer
Study
desk
1.00
0.91
0.34
− 0.46
0.45
− 0.49
1.00
0.31
− 0.38
0.43
− 0.51
1.00
− 0.15
0.16
− 0.28
1.00
− 0.15
0.19
1.00
− 0.31
0.68
0.72
0.66
0.75
0.39
0.22
− 0.66
− 0.37
0.22
0.45
− 0.31
− 0.65
1.00
0.62
1.00
− 0.78
0.55
0.66
0.73
0.44
− 0.73
0.61
0.70
0.69
0.43
− 0.29
0.01
0.12
0.22
0.27
0.56
− 0.34
− 0.38
− 0.43
− 0.45
− 0.41
0.18
0.25
0.28
0.49
0.54
− 0.42
− 0.59
− 0.46
− 0.37
− 0.58
0.38
0.41
0.58
0.55
−0.57
0.52
0.64
0.52
0.40
1.00
0.64
0.36
1.00
0.50
TIMSS — the arithmetical average of the TIMSS grade 8 science sub-tests.
IQ — the national values of IQ from Lynn and Vanhanen (2006).
Textbook — percentage of students taught by teachers who report textbooks as primary bases for lessons.
Teach Univ — percentage of students whose teachers have university degrees or equivalent.
Sch Cl Low — percentage of students whose principals assess school climate as low.
Stud safety — percentage of students who assess high their being safe in schools.
>50% EDH — the percentage of students in schools with more than 50% economically disadvantaged students according to school principals'
reports.
GDP USD — Gross domestic product (from The World Factbook, 2005).
Computer — percent of students who have computer in the home.
Study desk — percent of students who have a study desk/table in the home.
Parent Univ — percentage of students whose parents finished university or equivalent or higher.
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
(Lynn and Vanhanen, 2006) publication. The 2003
International Mathematics and Science Study gives
scores for grade 4 school children for 25 countries and
for grade 8 school children for 46 countries in
mathematics (Mullis et al., 2004) and science (Martin
et al., 2004). These reports also give sub-test scores for
different areas of mathematics and science.
3. Results
The average sub-test scores for both subjects and both
grades are given in Table 2, together with the national
IQs calculated by Lynn and Vanhanen (2006). Six of the
IQs are asterisked to show that they are estimated IQ. The
remaining 40 are measured IQs. The data for Belgium are
for the Flemish-speaking part of the country; the data for
Palestine are for the Palestinian National Authority. The
IQ of 86 for Palestine is given in Lynn (2006) and the IQ
of 97 for Scotland is given in Lynn (1979).
The Pearson correlations between the TIMSS 2003
results and IQ are given in Table 3. The correlations are
given for all countries (column 3) and for measured IQ
countries (column 4). The correlations for all countries
and for measured IQ countries are virtually identical,
suggesting that the estimated IQs must be accurate. The
correlations are also given corrected for attenuation for
all countries and for measured IQ countries. The
reliability coefficients for calculating the corrections
for attenuation were derived as follows. The reliability of
the national IQs is given by Lynn and Vanhanen (2006,
p.62) as 0.92, derived as the correlation between pairs of
IQs obtained from the same country and based on 71
countries for which there are two IQ measures. The
reliability of the national mathematics scores is derived
as the correlation between the scores of the grade 4 and
the grade 8 children (r = 0.92), and the reliability of the
national science scores is derived in the same way
(r = 0.92). The correlations corrected for attenuation are
given in brackets in Table 3.
We have considered whether any other variables could
explain the national differences in scores in mathematics
and science and examined the possible effects of a number
of school and demographic variables including teacher
training, school climate, economic development. The data
were taken from the TIMSS international Science report
for grade eight (Martin et al., 2004). The correlations
between these and scores in science for the 8 grade
students are shown in Table 4. It will be seen that many of
the correlations are high but the correlation between
TIMSS science scores and IQ is the highest. Regression
analysis for the eighth grade science results revealed only
one statistically significant predictor — IQ. Coefficients
119
of partial correlation revealed that no characteristic of
schooling had a statistically significant relationship with
TIMSS results when IQ was partialed out.
4. Discussion
The first question addressed in this paper is whether
the validity of the national IQs can be demonstrated by
showing that they are highly correlated with national
scores on tests of mathematics and science. We have
examined the relationships between the national IQs and
the results of the four TIMMS studies of international
achievement in mathematics and science of grade 4 and
grade 8 school students published in 2003. The study has
found very high correlations between national IQs and
the scores obtained in mathematics and science. These
correlations range between .85 and .93; and, corrected
for attenuation, between .93 and 1.0. We believe that
these high correlations establish beyond reasonable
dispute that the national IQs have a high degree of
validity.
The second question addressed in this paper is how to
explain the relationships responsible for the national
differences in IQ and attainment on tests of mathematics
and science. It has been noted by Luo, Thompson, and
Detterman (2003) that there are three theories to explain
the correlations between IQs and educational attainment
at the level of individuals. These are (1) intelligence is a
cause of educational attainment “because g is commonly
acknowledged as more pervasive in intellectual tasks,
and appears to be more biologically rooted than school
achievement” (Jensen, 1998, p.68); (2) intellectual abilities are partly a product of education (Ceci, 1991); (3)
intelligence, measured by intelligence tests, and scholastic performance are both partly determined by “basic
cognitive processes, which are measured using tasks
such as simple reaction time, inspection time, and
memory recall and recognition tasks” (Luo et al., 2003,
p. 67). These authors produce evidence supporting the
last of these theories and conclude that “individual differences in mental speed are a main causal factor underlying the observed correlation between general
intelligence and scholastic performance in children
between the ages of 6 and 13.”
These theories can be applied to explain the crosscountry correlations between IQs and the mathematics
and science attainment: (1) we can follow Jensen and
regard national differences in intelligence (operationalized by IQs) as virtually entirely responsible for
differences in mathematics and science attainment; (2)
we can follow Ceci (1991) and posit that differences in
education have had an effect on both national IQs and
120
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
educational attainment in math and science. There is little
doubt that education affects IQs at the level of individuals,
as Ceci (1991, p.703) has concluded: “Schooling fosters
the development of cognitive processes that underpin
performance on most IQ tests”. More recently the positive
effect of education on IQ has been confirmed by Whaley,
Fox, Deary, and Starr (2005) who have found that the
level of education is an independent predictor of
intelligence at age 64 after controlling for childhood IQ
and other predictors. Further evidence has been provided
by Blair, Gamson, Thorne, and Baker (2005).
(3) We can follow Luo et al. (2003) and regard national
IQs and educational attainment in math and science as
measures of the same latent construct that they describe as
“basic cognitive processes”. The cross-country correlations between the IQs and mathematics and science
attainment scores are so high (at between .93 and 1.0,
corrected for attenuation) that this appears a plausible
theory. This conclusion is supported by genetic studies
that have found that the relationship between academic
attainment and IQ is largely due to common genetic
influences (Wainwright, Wright, Geffen, Luciano, &
Martin, 2005; Kovas, Harlaar, Petrill, & Plomin, 2005).
The third objective of the paper has been to explore
further the relationships between national IQs and
mathematics and science attainment and a number of
educational, economic and demographic variables. The
correlation matrix showing the inter-relationships between these is given in Table 4. It will be seen that a
number of these correlations are moderately high.
Educational attainment and IQ are correlated with the
use of textbooks as primary bases for lessons (r = .34,
.31), the percentage of teachers with university degrees
(r = .45, .43), the percentage of students whose principals
assess the school climate as low (r = − .49,− .51), the
percentage of students who assess their schools as safe
(r = .68, .66), the percentage of students in schools with
more than 50% economically disadvantaged students
according to school principals' reports (r = − .78,− .73),
the percentage of students who have a computer in the
home (r = .66, .70), the percentage of students who have
a study desk or table in the home (r = .73, .69), and the
percentage of students whose parents had university
degrees or equivalent (r =. 44, .43). There are also
substantial correlations between national IQs and
mathematics and science attainment and gross national
per capita domestic product (r = .55, .61) and life
expectancy (r = .72, .75).
We suggest that these correlations arise from a
complex network of relationships. We propose that the
two most fundamental are national IQs and per capita
GDP (a measure of per capita income) that are correlated
at 0.61. We suggest that these two variables have a
reciprocal positive feedback relationship such that each
has a causal effect on the other, i.e. national IQs have a
positive effect on per capita income and per capita income
has a positive effect on national IQs. National IQs have a
positive effect on per capita income because a nation
whose population has a high IQ can earn more, just as
individuals with high IQs can earn more than those with
low IQs. Conversely, national per capita income has a
positive effect on national IQs because a nation whose
population has a high per capita income can spend more
on education, nutrition and health care, and these have a
beneficial effect on intelligence. The other correlations
given in Table 4 appear to arise largely as effects of per
capita income on educational variables. For instance,
national populations with high per capita incomes can
afford to buy their children desks (r = .47) and computers
(r = .83), and can afford to have smaller classes (r = −.34).
National populations with high per capita incomes have
high life expectancy (r = .52) probably largely because
they can provide a high standard of nutrition and health
care and also because intelligence is associated with
longevity, as shown by Whaley and Deary (2001).
References
Baker, D. B., & Jones, D. P. (1993). Creating gender equality: Crossnational gender stratification and mathematical performance. Sociology of Education, 66, 91−103.
Barber, N. (2005). Educational and ecological correlates of IQ: A
cross-national investigation. Intelligence, 33, 273−284.
Barnett, S. M., & Williams, W. (2004). National intelligence and the
emperor's new clothes: A review of IQ and the Wealth of Nations.
Contemporary Psychology, 49, 389−396.
Beaton, A. E., Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzales, E. J., Kelly, D. L., &
Smith, T. A. (1996). Mathematics achievement in the middle school
years. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College, TIMSS International Study
Center.
Beaton, A. E., Martin, M. O., Mullis, I. V. S., Gonzales, E. J., Smith, T. A., &
Kelly, D. L. (1996). Science achievement in the middle school years.
Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College, TIMSS International Study Center.
Blair, C., Gamson, D., Thorne, S., & Baker, D. (2005). Rising mean IQ:
Cognitive demand of mathematics education for young children,
population exposure to formal schooling, and the neurobiology of
the prefrontal cortex. Intelligence, 33(93), 106.
Ceci, S. J. (1991). How much does schooling influence general
intelligence and its cognitive components? Developmental Psychology, 27, 703−722.
Comber, L. C., & Keeves, J. (1973). Science achievement in nineteen
countries. New York: John Wiley.
Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence differences. In R. L. Gregory (Ed.), The
Oxford companion to the mind Oxford, UK: Oxford University
Press.
Dickerson, R. E. (2006). Exponential correlation of IQ and the wealth
of nations. Intelligence, 34, 292−295.
Ervik, A. O. (2003). Review of IQ and the wealth of nations. The
Economic Journal, 113, 406−408.
R. Lynn, J. Mikk / Intelligence 35 (2007) 115–121
Gagne, F., & St. Pere, F. (2002). When IQ is controlled, does
motivation still predict achievement? Intelligence, 30, 71−100.
Hunt, E., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Sorry, wrong numbers: An
analysis of a study of a correlation between skin color and IQ.
Intelligence, 34, 121−139.
Husen, T. (1967). International achievement in mathematics: A
comparison of twelve countries. New York: John Wiley.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability.
Westport: Praeger Publishers.
Jones, G., & Schneider, W. J. (2006). Intelligence, human capital, and
economic growth: A Beysian averaging of classical estimates
(BACE) approach. Journal of Economic Growth, 11, 71−93.
Kovas, Y. N., Harlaar, S. A., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (2005). Generalist
genes and mathematics in 7-year-old twins. Intelligence, 33, 473−489.
Lloyd, J., & Barenblatt, L. (1984). Intrinsic intellectuality: Its relation to
social class, intelligence, and achievement. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 46, 646−654.
Lubinski, D., & Humphreys, L. G. (1996). Seeing the forest from the
tree: When predicting the behavior or status of groups, correlate
means. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 2, 363−376.
Luo, D., Thompson, L. A., & Detterman, D. (2003). The causal factor
underlying the correlation between psychometric g and scholastic
performance. Intelligence, 31, 67−83.
Lynn, R. (1979). The social ecology of intelligence in the British Isles.
British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 1−12.
Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: An evolutionary
analysis. Athens, GA: Washington Summit Books.
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations.
Westport, CT: Praeger.
Lynn, R., & Vanhanen, T. (2006). IQ and global inequality. Athens,
GA: Washington Summit Books.
Lynn, R., Hampson, S., & Magee, M. (1984). Home background,
intelligence, personality and education as predictors of unemployment in young people. Personality and Individual Differences, 5,
549−558.
Martin, M. O., Mullis, I. V. S., Gonzales, E. J., & Chrostowski, S. J.
(2004). TIMSS 2003 international science report. : IEA, Boston
College.
Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler's measurement and appraisal of
intelligence. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
121
McDaniel, & Whetzel (2004). IQ and wealth of nations: Prediction of
GDP not dependent on precise g estimates. New Orleans:
Conference of the International Society for Intelligence Research.
Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Gonzales, E. J., & Chrostowski, S. J.
(2004). TIMSS 2003 international mathematics report Boston:
IEA, Boston College.
Templer, D. I., & Arikawa, H. (2006). Temperature, skin color, per
capita income, and IQ: An international perspective. Intelligence,
34, 121−139.
Tewes, U. (2003). Germany. In J. Georgas, L. G. Weiss, F. van der
Vijver, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), Culture and children's
intelligence Amsterdam: Academic Press.
Volken, T. (2003). IQ and the wealth of nations: A critique of Richard
Lynn and Tatu Vanhanen's recent book. European Sociological
Review, 19, 411−412.
Voracek, M. (2004). National intelligence and suicide rate: An
ecological study of 85 countries. Personality and Individual
Differences, 37, 543−553.
Wainwright, M. A., Wright, M. J., Geffen, G. M., Luciano, M., &
Martin, N. G. (2005). The genetic basis of academic achievement
on the Queensland Core Skill Test and its shared genetic variance
with IQ. Behaviour Genetics, 35(2), 133−145.
Walberg, H. J. (1984). Improving the productivity of America's
schools. Educational Leadership, 41, 19−27.
Weede, E., & Kämpf, S. (2002). The impact of intelligence and
institutional improvements on economic growth. Kyklos, 55,
361−380.
Whaley, L. J., Fox, H. C., Deary, I. J., & Starr, J. M. (2005). Childhood
IQ, smoking, and cognitive change from age 11 to 64 years. Addictive Behaviors, 30(1), 77−88.
Whaley, L. J., & Deary, I. J. (2001). Longitudinal cohort study of
childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal,
322, 1−5.
Whetzel, D.L. and McDaniel, M.A. (in press). Prediction of national
wealth. Intelligence.
The World Factbook (2005). Retrieved June, 2005 http://www.cia.gov/
cia/publications/factbook/
Yule, W., Gold, R. D., & Busch, C. (1982). Long term predictive
validity of the WPPSI: An 11 year follow-up study. Personality
and Individual Differences, 3, 65−71.