TONAL DEVELOPMENT
OF TAI LANGUAGES
LIAO HANBO
廖漢波
Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of
MASTER OF ARTS
IN
LINGUISTICS
PAYAP UNIVERSITY
May 2016
Title:
Tonal Development of Tai Languages
Researcher:
Liao Hanbo
Degree:
Master of Arts in Linguistics
Advisor:
Assistant Professor Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun, Ph.D.
Approval Date:
31 May 2016
Institution:
Payap University, Chiang Mai, Thailand
The members of the thesis examination committee:
1. _________________________________ Committee Chair
(Professor Somsonge Burusphat, Ph.D.)
2. _________________________________ Committee Member
(Assistant Professor Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun, Ph.D.)
3. _________________________________ Committee Member
(Robert Wyn Owen, Ph.D.)
Copyright © Liao Hanbo
Payap University 2016
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
First and foremost, I would like to extend my sincerest thanks to my thesis advisor,
Assistant Professor Dr. Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun, for her continuous support and
patience towards my research. She introduced the knowledge of literature to me
during my M.A. studies. She further guided me to do the fieldwork, and provided
critical feedback on my research skills, my views and my attitude towards the issue.
Her insightful comments sharpened my mind. It is she who enlightened me while I
pursued my academic career.
I would like to acknowledge the financial assistance from the Ford Foundation
International Fellowships Program for the postgraduate scholarship which supported
the first two years of my study in Payap University, Chiang Mai, Thailand.
Special thanks to Assistant Professor Dr. Tai Chung-pui for his steadfast support and
encouragement during my studies, to Dr. Robert Wyn Owen for providing useful
comments, to Professor Dr. Somsonge Burusphat for agreeing to be my thesis
committee chair and for her time of reading and giving me suggestions, to Mr. Louis
William Davis for helping with the proofreading of my thesis, to Researcher Alexis
Michaud for his kind and patient directions on tonology, and to Ms. Chen Yen-ling
for introducing me to the literature of my research area.
I must thank Ms. Suthinee Promkandorn, my M.A. classmate, who introduced me to
Thai language and culture when I was a new arrival to Thailand. Besides, she kindly
helped me to translate my thesis abstract into Thai. I also want to express my gratitude
to Mr. Suparak Techachareonrungrue who helped on the process of Thai translation of
the abstract.
I would also like to thank the scholars whom I met at various stages of my study.
They are Assistant Professor Kao Ya-ning, Dr. Margaret Milliken, Professor Li
Jinfang, Professor Stephen Matthews, Associate Professor Masayuki Yoshikawa, Dr.
Shen Ruiqing, Professor Pan Qixu, Professor Fan Honggui, Mr. Sattanan
i
Saengsrichan, Associate Professor Lu Xiaoqin, Mr. Liang Xianning, Mr. Lu Yungao,
Ms. Xu Xiaoming, and Mr. Lu Guangzhou.
I am grateful to all the Language Resource Persons (LRPs) whom I interviewed
during the fieldtrip. I was deeply moved by their enthusiastic help.
I dedicate this thesis to my family in particular to my mother, who passed away
during my first year of study in Payap. In the past few years, accompanied by my
father, she must have been gazing at me graciously in heaven. My sister-in-law Ms.
Yan Liyan has utilized all her personal connections to find the most suitable LRPs in
Guangxi for me, for which I am grateful. I could never express my gratitude by words
for their love.
Liao Hanbo
ii
Title:
Tonal Development of Tai Languages
Researcher:
Liao Hanbo
Degree:
Master of Arts in Linguistics
Advisor:
Assistant Professor Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun, Ph.D.
Approval Date:
31 May 2016
Institution:
Payap University, Chiang Mai, Thailand
Number of Pages:
225
Keywords:
Tai languages, Tonogenesis, Tonal split, Tone box, Tonology
ABSTRACT
This thesis aims to provide a full scheme of tonal development of Tai, from
tonogenesis in proto-Tai to different diachronic hierarchies of tonal splits in different
Tai groups and varieties, and further suggests a new viewpoint on Tai classification as
well as the revised Tai tone box based on tonology. The analysis is based on data of
tones from 42 Tai varieties, most of which were collected during my fieldworks in
China and Thailand. Data collection and analysis has been accomplished by a revised
version of Gedney’s tone box (Gedney 1989[1972], Liao & Shen 2012), which is
supposed to contain the basic patterns of tonal split and merger in Tai languages. Any
tone split which is not reflected in the tone box is emphasized to discuss.
The discussion of the diachronic processes of tonal development of Tai languages is
the main body of this thesis. Tonogenesis is treated as the very first step of the arising
of tones in proto-Tai. After tone arose, two groups of proto-initials (voiceless-voiced)
conditioned primary tonal splits whereas splits conditioned by other pharyngeal
features, such as aspiration and pre-glottalization represent stages of secondary
development. Some secondary tonal split patterns result in three-way register splits.
However, the bearing capacity of the tone numbers in a language often cause the third
register to merge into one of the other two to make two-way register splits always be
the mainstream. On the basis of the full tonal development scheme explored, the other
two hypotheses are attested as follows.
First, based on the analysis of tonal behaviors with supplementary evidence from
initial behaviors, vocalic behaviors, and exclusive lexical items, this thesis suggests
two levels of Tai division as follows. Primary, it agrees on the two-term system
iii
suggested by Haudricourt (1956) to divide proto-Tai first into Dioi or ʔɤaɓ vs. Tai
proper, by another two-term system, NT-YN (Northern Tai-Yongnan Zhuang) and
Southern Tai. Li’s Central Tai and Southwestern Tai (Li 1977) can be divided from
each other under the Southern Tai sub-group, in contrast Northern Tai and Yongnan
Zhuang differ from one another under the NT-YN sub-group.
Second, based on all the possible conditioning phonation types of initials at time of
tonal splits, an integrated tone box is provided. However, a unified Tai tone box have
been proved to be cumbersome if a Tai variety’s affiliation has been known. It is
because not all Tai varieties obtain all types of secondary tonal splits. For instance,
Northern Tai varieties lack of contrastive aspirated sounds, and unaspirated stops in
NT never go with glottalized initials to condition secondary tonal splits. Therefore, a
tone box designated with the divisions containing the rows of aspirated sounds and
unaspirated stops appear to be unnecessary for Northern Tai. For this reason, Tai tone
boxes designated with specific pertinence to different Tai periods and different Tai
sub-groups are first suggested if the diachronic period of a Tai variety or the
affiliation of a modern Tai variety are known. Nevertheless, the integrated Tai tone
box is finally suggested to be able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai variety.
iv
ชืไอรืไอง:
ผูຌวิจัย:
ชืไอปริญญา:
อาจารย์ทีไปรึกษาวิทยานิพนธ์:
วันทีไอนุมัติผลงาน:
สถาบัน:
จานวนหนຌา:
คาสาคัญ:
พัฒนาการของสียงวรรณยุกต์฿นภาษาตระกูลเท
ลีไยว หຌานป
ศิลปศาสตรมหาบัณฑิต ิภาษาศาสตร์ี
ผูຌชวยศาสตราจารย์ ดร. พิณรัตน์ อัครวัฒนากุล
31 พฤษภาคม 2559
มหาวิทยาลัยพายัพ จังหวัดชียง฿หม ประทศเทย
225
ภาษาตระกูลเท กานิดวรรณยุกต์ การยกสียงวรรณยุกต์
กลองวรรณยุกต์ การศึกษาวรรณยุกต์
บทคัดย่อ
วิทยานิพนธ์ฉบับนีๅมุงสนอรูปบบพัฒนาการของวรรณยุกต์฿นกลุมภาษาตระกูลเท ดยริไมจากการ
กานิดของวรรณยุกต์฿นภาษาเทดัๅงดิมเปสูลาดับขัๅนของการยกสียงวรรณยุกต์บบขຌามสมัย฿น
ภาษาตระกูลเทกลุมตาง โ ละวิธภาษาเทตาง โ นอกจากนัๅนยังสนอมุมมอง฿หม฿นการบงกลุม
ภาษาตระกูลเทละการปรับระบบกลองวรรณยุกต์ตามกณฑ์การศึกษาวรรณยุกต์ การวิคราะห์
ขຌอมูลอาศัยวรรณยุกต์จากวิธภาษาตระกูลเท ไโ ภาษา ซึไงสวน฿หญเดຌจากการกใบขຌอมูลภาคสนาม฿น
ประทศจีนละประทศเทย การกใบขຌอมูลละการวิคราะห์ขຌอมูลเดຌยึดนวคิดกลองวรรณยุกต์ของ
Gedney (แ้่้ [แ้็โ]ี ละ Liao & Shen (โเแโี ทีไสนอรูปบบพืๅนฐานของการยกละรวม
สียงวรรณยุกต์฿นกลุมภาษาตระกูลเท ดยการวิจัยครัๅงนีๅเดຌ฿หຌความสาคัญกับการยกสียงวรรณยุกต์
ทีไเมสอดคลຌองกับนวคิดกลองวรรณยุกต์พืไอนามาอภิปรายผลดຌวย
การอภิ ป รายพั ฒ นาการของวรรณยุ ก ต์ ฿ นภาษาตระกูล เทบบขຌ า มสมั ย ถื อ ป็ น ประดใ นหลั ก฿น
วิทยานิพนธ์ฉบับนีๅ การศึกษากานิดวรรณยุกต์ถือป็นขัๅนรกทีไนาเปสูการกิดขึๅนของวรรณยุกต์ภาษา
เทดัๅงดิม มืไอมีวรรณยุกต์กิดขึๅน สียงพยัญชนะตຌนดัๅงดิม โ กลุม ิพยัญชนะสียงเมกຌอง – พยัญชนะ
สี ย งกຌ อ งี ถื อ ป็ น งืไ อ นเขปฐมภูมิ ทีไ มีผ ลต อ การยกสี ย งวรรณยุก ต์ ดยขัๅ น ต อ มาการยกสียง
วรรณยุกต์ป็นผลมาจากลักษณ์ของสียง฿นชองคอ ชน การพนลมละการนาดຌวยการกักสຌนสียงซึไง
ป็นงืไอนเขขัๅนทุติยภูมิทีไมีอิทธิพลตอกระบวนการดังกลาว รูปบบการยกสียงวรรณยุกต์฿นขัๅน
ทุติยภูมิบางรูปบบสงผล฿หຌกิดการยกสียงวรรณยุกต์ออกป็น ใ ทาง อยางเรกใดี ความสามารถ฿น
การรองรับวรรณยุกต์฿นภาษาหนึไง โ มีผลทา฿หຌกิดการรวมสียงวรรณยุกต์ทีไสามกับหนึไง฿นสองของ
วรรณยุกต์ทีไหลือจึงทา฿หຌการยกสียงวรรณยุกต์บบ โ ทางซึไงถือป็นบบหลัก หลังจากศึกษา
รูปบบพัฒนาการของวรรณยุกต์อยางละอียดดังกลาว จึงมีการทดสอบสมมติฐานพิไมติม โ ประการ
ดังนีๅ
v
ประการรก จากการวิคราะห์พฤติกรรมของวรรณยุกต์ พรຌอมกับหลักฐานพิไมติมจากพฤติกรรมของ
สียงพยัญชนะตຌน พฤติกรรมของสียงสระ ละจากชุดคาศัพท์ฉพาะ วิทยานิพนธ์ฉบับนีๅเดຌนาสนอ
การบงภาษาตระกูลเทออกป็น โ ระดับ ฿นระดับรก ผลการวิจัยหในพຌองกับระบบการบงป็น โ
กลุมทีไนาสนอ฿นงานของ Haudricourt (1956) ฿นการบงภาษาเทดัๅงดิมป็น Dioi หรือ ʔyai กับ
ภาษาเททัไวเปตามระบบการบงป็น โ กลุม คือ NT-YN (ภาษาเทหนือ – จຌวง หยงหนานี ละ
ภาษาเท฿ตຌ สวนภาษาเทกลางละเทตะวันตกจากงานของ Li (1977) สามารถยกออกจากกันดยจัด
อยูภาย฿ตຌกลุมยอยของภาษาเท฿ตຌ ฿นทางตรงกันขຌาม ภาษาเทหนือละจຌวงหยงหนานกลับมีความ
ตกตางกันอยางสิๅนชิงดยจัดอยูภาย฿ตຌกลุม NT-YN
ประการทีไสอง จากลักษณะสียงพูดของพยัญชนะตຌนทีไป็นงืไอนเขเดຌทัๅงหมด฿นกระบวนการยกสียง
วรรณยุกต์ จึงมีการสนอกลองวรรณยุกต์บบผสมผสาน ทัๅงนีๅกลองวรรณยุกต์ภาษาตระกูลเท฿นรูป
บบดิมนัๅนสดง฿หຌหในถึงความยุงยากซับซຌอนกินเป หากพิจารณา฿นงของความกีไยวขຌองสัมพันธ์
กั น ของวรรณยุ ก ต์ ฿ นภาษาต า ง โ นืไ อ งจากเม ฿ ช ทุ ก ภาษาย อ ย฿นตระกู ล เททีไ มี ก ารยกตั ว ของ
วรรณยุกต์฿นขัๅนทุติยภูมิ ตัวอยางชน วิธภาษา฿นกลุมเทหนือเมมีการปรียบตางของสียงพนลม ละ
สียงกักบบเมพนลมจะเมปรากฏกับพยัญชนะตຌนทีไมีการกักสຌนสียงจึงทา฿หຌเมมีการยกตัวของ
วรรณยุกต์฿นขัๅนทุติยภูมิ ดังนัๅน กลองวรรณยุกต์ซึไงมีถวทีไประกอบเปดຌวย สียงพนลม ละสียงกัก
บบเมพนลมจึงเมจาป็นสาหรับภาษา฿นกลุมเทหนือ ดຌวยหตุนีๅ วิทยานิพนธ์ฉบับนีๅจึงมีขຌอสนอนะ
วา หากชวงวลาของภาษาเทหรือวิธภาษาเทนัๅน โ ป็นทีไทราบชัดจน ฿หຌนากลองวรรณยุกต์ทีไบงชีๅถึง
ความกีไยวขຌองกันระหวางภาษาเทจากหลากหลายชวงวลาละหลายกลุมมา฿ชຌพิจารณาป็นอันดับ
รก อย า งเรกใ ต าม กล อ งวรรณยุ ก ต์ บบผสมผสานนีๅส ามารถน าเป฿ชຌ สดงความตกต า งดຌาน
วรรณยุกต์ของวิธภาษาตาง โ ฿นตระกูลภาษาเทเดຌดຌวยชนกัน
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Acknowledgements ......................................................................................................... i
Abstract ........................................................................................................................ iii
บทคดยอ............................................................................................................................ v
List of Tables ................................................................................................................. x
List of Figures ............................................................................................................ xiii
List of Abbreviations and Symbols............................................................................. xiv
Glossary ...................................................................................................................... xvi
Introduction ................................................................................................... 1
1.1 Background .......................................................................................................... 1
1.1.1 Tai and related languages.............................................................................. 2
1.1.2 Tai classification ........................................................................................... 3
1.1.3 ISO 639-3 language code applied to Tai languages...................................... 4
1.1.4 Tai tones ........................................................................................................ 7
1.2 Objectives of the research .................................................................................... 9
1.2.1 Conventional descriptions of Tai tones....................................................... 10
1.2.2 More pending issues on Tai tones ............................................................... 16
1.2.3 Outstanding problems ................................................................................. 25
1.2.4 Three hypotheses for solving problems ...................................................... 30
1.3 Overview of the thesis ....................................................................................... 30
1.3.1 Scope and limitations .................................................................................. 30
1.3.2 Contribution of the thesis ............................................................................ 32
1.3.3 Chapters outline .......................................................................................... 33
Literature Review ........................................................................................ 34
2.1 Historical and comparative Tai .......................................................................... 34
2.1.1 Linguistic changes in Tai ............................................................................ 35
2.1.2 Comparative method applied to Tai ............................................................ 37
2.1.3 Language classification associated to Tai ................................................... 39
2.2 Tonology ............................................................................................................ 45
2.2.1 Tonogenesis ................................................................................................ 47
vii
2.2.2 Tonal splits and mergers ............................................................................. 59
2.2.3 The conditioning factors of tonal splits ...................................................... 69
2.2.4 Tai classification related to tonological issues............................................ 74
2.3 Summary ............................................................................................................ 79
Methodology, data and notation .................................................................. 80
3.1 General historical and comparative method ...................................................... 80
3.1.1 Linguistic change ........................................................................................ 80
3.1.2 Borrowing ................................................................................................... 81
3.1.3 Comparative method ................................................................................... 82
3.1.4 Linguistic classification .............................................................................. 83
3.1.5 Areal linguistics .......................................................................................... 84
3.2 Library research ................................................................................................. 85
3.3 Fieldwork ........................................................................................................... 85
3.3.1 Data collection and the LRPs ...................................................................... 86
3.3.2 Data management and PRAAT ................................................................... 88
3.4 Notation.............................................................................................................. 89
Diachronic hierarchies of Tai tonal development ....................................... 92
4.1 Tai Tonogenesis and earlier tonal behaviors revisited ....................................... 92
4.1.1 Principled clarifications of Tai tonogenesis ................................................ 93
4.1.2 A hypothesis of Tai tonogenesis based on tonal correspondence between
Tai and Sinitic languages ..................................................................................... 96
4.1.3 Speculation of specific phonetic shapes of PT tones .................................. 98
4.1.4 Conclusion about Tai tonogenesis ............................................................ 120
4.2 Primary tonal splits in Tai ................................................................................ 121
4.2.1 Phonation types of initial consonants at the PT level ............................... 121
4.2.2 The cause of the primary tonal split .......................................................... 124
4.2.3 Straightforward tonal split in Tai languages ............................................. 127
4.3 Secondary tonal splits in Tai ............................................................................ 131
4.3.1 Determinations of the diachronicity of secondary tonal splits.................. 131
4.3.2 Diachronic process of secondary tonal splits conditioned by syllable initial
phonations .......................................................................................................... 140
4.3.3 Secondary tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length ............................... 173
4.4 Summary .......................................................................................................... 182
Tai classification based on tonology ......................................................... 184
5.1 The tonological criteria of subgrouping Tai languages ................................... 184
5.2 NT-YN and ST: Tai classification based on the primary tonal split................ 187
viii
5.3 NT/YN + CT/SWT: Tai classification based on secondary tonal splits .......... 189
5.4 Supplementary evidence for supporting Tai division based on tonology ........ 192
5.4.1 Initial behaviors ........................................................................................ 192
5.4.2 Vocalic behaviors...................................................................................... 193
5.4.3 Exclusive lexical items ............................................................................. 195
5.5 Summary .......................................................................................................... 197
A new perspective of Tai tone box............................................................ 199
6.1 From carrying capacity to practicability .......................................................... 199
6.2 PT tone box: from tonogenesis to the primary tonal split ................................ 202
6.3 Tai tone boxes based on secondary tonal splits in ST ..................................... 204
6.4 Tai tone box based on secondary tonal splits in NT-YN ................................. 208
6.5 Integrated Tai tone box and its application ...................................................... 210
6.6 Limitations of Tai tone box.............................................................................. 215
6.7 Summary .......................................................................................................... 217
Conclusion................................................................................................. 218
7.1 An overview of this study ................................................................................ 218
7.2 Conclusions related to the goals of this study .................................................. 220
7.2.1 A diachronic scheme of Tai tonal development ....................................... 220
7.2.2 Implications of the diachronic process of Tai tonal development ............ 222
7.3 Suggestions for further study ........................................................................... 223
Bibliography .............................................................................................................. 226
Appendix A Questionnaire ........................................................................................ 244
Appendix B Checklist ................................................................................................ 246
Appendix C Language information and tone data analysis from 42 Tai varieties..... 257
Appendix D The maps of the Tai varieties investigated ............................................ 423
Resume....................................................................................................................... 425
ix
LIST OF TABLES
Table 1 Li Fang-Kuei’s Tai tone pattern (summarized from Li 1977) ........................ 11
Table 2 Tai Tone correspondence between the Sinitic and Western Circles ............... 12
Table 3 Examples from tonal numbering presenting the contradiction to the “tonal
category based principle” in the Sinitic circle ............................................................. 13
Table 4 Tai Tone box (adapted from Gedney 1989[1972]: 202-204) ......................... 14
Table 5 Gedney’s Tone box and Sinitic tone numbers together applied to Thai ......... 15
Table 6 Tonal correspondence among Li’s pattern, Sinitic method, and Gedney’s tone
box................................................................................................................................ 16
Table 7 A revised version of Gedney’s tone box (adapted from Liao & Shen 2012) . 18
Table 8 A tonal split in voiceless friction sounds in Tone A ....................................... 19
Table 9 Comparison of aspirations and voiced sounds among SWT, CT and NT in
Column A ..................................................................................................................... 21
Table 10 Comparison of aspirations and voiced sounds among SWT, CT and NT in
Column C ..................................................................................................................... 21
Table 11 Two new rows splitting from primary aspirations (adapted from Liao &
Shen 2012) ................................................................................................................... 23
Table 12 Comparison of unaspirated stops among Tai varieties in Column A (adapted
from Liao & Shen 2012) .............................................................................................. 24
Table 13 Comparison of unaspirated stops among Tai varieties in Column DL
(adapted from Liao & Shen 2012) ............................................................................... 24
Table 14 Two new rows splitting from Row 2 in Gedney’s tone box (adapted from
Liao & Shen 2012) ....................................................................................................... 25
Table 15 Some diagnostic lexical items of two groups in Tai (adapted from Zhang et
al. 1999: 9-10) .............................................................................................................. 42
Table 16 Vietnamese tonogenesis (summarized from Haudricourt 1954a)................. 47
Table 17 MC tones and their OC source (adapted from Norman 1988: 57)................ 51
Table 18 Correspondences between PT and MC tones (adapted from Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 240) ................................................................................................. 52
Table 19 Phonetic characteristics of PT tones (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 271) .... 56
Table 20 Vietnamese tones (adapted from Norman 1988: 55) .................................... 60
Table 21 The MC eight tonal categories (adapted from Norman 1988: 54) ................ 60
Table 22 Register tonal splits of Thai (adapted from Gedney 1989[1972]: 201) ........ 63
Table 23 Three-way register split in A tone in Kam (adapted from Haudricourt 1972:
68) ................................................................................................................................ 64
Table 24 Three-way register splits in Kam (adapted from Haudricourt 1972: 70) ..... 65
Table 25 Tonal correspondence of nasals between two groups of Kam-Sui languages
(adapted from Weera Ostapirat 1994: 79) ................................................................... 72
Table 26 Four groups of Kam-Sui nasals proposed by Weera Ostapirat (1994: 80) ... 73
Table 27 Voicing alternation among Cao Lan and NT and CT representative varieties
(Gregerson & Edmondson 1989: 160) ......................................................................... 77
Table 28 Voicing alternation among Nung An and the representative varieties of NT
and CT (adapted from Edmondson 2002: 59).............................................................. 77
Table 29 Disagreement on the phonetic characteristics of PT tones *B and *C ......... 93
x
Table 30 Tonal correspondence between Cantonese and Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
(DB) ............................................................................................................................. 97
Table 31 Phonetic shapes of tones and their voice qualities in Tai varieties
investigated .................................................................................................................. 99
Table 32 The correspondence among the voice qualities defined by different
proposals .................................................................................................................... 103
Table 33 Basic tonal patterns in Huishui Bouyei (NT) .............................................. 107
Table 34 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Dongling Zhuang (NT) ............................... 108
Table 35 Some shared vocabularies with the B tones (Tones 5 and 6) in Dai and with
syllable final -h in Wa (adapted from Liang & Zhang 1996: 816) ............................ 113
Table 36 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (CT) ........................... 118
Table 37 Phonetic characteristics of PT tones in the two diachronic stages ............. 120
Table 38 Different speculations of initial phonation types at the PT level ................ 122
Table 39 The speculated primary register tonal split on post-PT level ..................... 130
Table 40 Type 1 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from Zhang
et al. 1999: 243-244) .................................................................................................. 133
Table 41 Type 2 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from Zhang
et al. 1999: 244) ......................................................................................................... 134
Table 42 Type 3 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from Zhang
et al. 1999: 244-245) .................................................................................................. 135
Table 43 Type 4 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from Zhang
et al. 1999: 245) ......................................................................................................... 136
Table 44 Series 2/1 of voicing alternation in Tai ....................................................... 142
Table 45 Correspondence involving labial clusters by monosyllabic and sesquisyllabic
views (adapted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 48-49)........................................ 143
Table 46 Etyma with PT *C̬.t- (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 168) .......................... 145
Table 47 Etyma with PKS *h'-w- (Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1085) ............................. 146
Table 48 Etyma with the proposed PT *ɬ.w- which causing voicing alternation in Tai
.................................................................................................................................... 148
Table 49 Developing tracks of PT *ɬ.w- in modern Tai varieties .............................. 149
Table 50 Reflexes of *ʔd-, *ʔb-, and *ʔj- and their tones .......................................... 152
Table 51 PT monosyllabic onsets induced by aspiration in ST and YN ................... 155
Table 52 Reflexes of Li’s*kr- in some modern Tai varieties .................................... 158
Table 53 Etyma with PT *k.r- (adapted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 169, 345)
.................................................................................................................................... 160
Table 54 The reconstruction of PT ASO-CY ............................................................ 161
Table 55 Etyma with ASO-YN .................................................................................. 162
Table 56 Reflexes of aspiration involving in different modern Tai groups ............... 165
Table 57 Voicing alternation between Suogan of NT and other Tai varieties........... 166
Table 58 Voicing alternation in Tai and KS languages ............................................. 167
Table 59 Developing tracks of PT *C̥.N- in modern Tai varieties ............................ 168
Table 60 Basic tonal patterns in Daxin Baoxu Zuojiang Zhuang (cf. L23) ............... 170
Table 61 Basic tonal patterns in Huashan Min Zhuang (cf. L16) .............................. 172
Table 62 Tonal splits in checked syllables in Guangnan Nong Zhuang .................... 174
Table 63 Tonal splits in checked syllables in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang ............. 175
Table 64 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (cf. L9) .................... 178
Table 65 Basic tonal patterns in Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang (cf. L13) ................... 178
Table 66 Vowel-length alternation between ST and NT/YN .................................... 179
Table 67 The progressing vowel-length alternation between ST and NT/YN .......... 181
xi
Table 68 Different initial behaviors of the single r- segment among NT, YN, CT and
SWT ........................................................................................................................... 193
Table 69 Vowels involving Gedney’s Puzzles .......................................................... 194
Table 70 Some diagnostic items of NZ and SZ division (adapted from Edmondson
1994: 152-153) ........................................................................................................... 195
Table 71 Some diagnostic lexical items of ST and NT-YN (adapted from Zhang et al.
1999: 9-10, also cf. Table 15 in §2.1.3) ..................................................................... 196
Table 72 PT tone box: from tonal precursor to tones ................................................ 202
Table 73 Integrated PST & PNT tone box: from PT to PST (→) and PNT (→)....... 203
Table 74 PST tone box............................................................................................... 204
Table 75 SWT tone box (cf. Gedney 1989[1972]) .................................................... 205
Table 76 Ancillary box of the SWT tone box ............................................................ 205
Table 77 CT tone box ................................................................................................ 206
Table 78 Ancillary box of the CT tone box ............................................................... 206
Table 79 Different VASO-1/2 merging directions involving different tonal splits
among Tai varieties .................................................................................................... 207
Table 80 PNT tone box .............................................................................................. 208
Table 81 NT tone box ................................................................................................ 209
Table 82 Ancillary box of the NT tone box ............................................................... 209
Table 83 YN tone box ................................................................................................ 210
Table 84 Ancillary box of the YN tone box .............................................................. 210
Table 85 Integrated Tai tone box ............................................................................... 211
Table 86 Ancillary box of the integrated Tai tone box .............................................. 211
Table 87 Integrated Tai tone box applying to Debao Dalong (cf. Table 64) ............. 213
Table 88 The merging direction of particular series in Debao Dalong...................... 213
Table 89 Examples of merging of initials and vowels in Debao Dalong .................. 214
xii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1 Tai-Kadai language family tree (adapted from Diller 2008: 7) ....................... 2
Figure 2 Distribution of the Tai–Kadai language family (from wikipedia.org) ............ 3
Figure 3 Tripartite division of Tai languages (Li 1977) ................................................ 4
Figure 4 Two plus one taxonomy of Tai division (Gedney 1989b) ............................... 4
Figure 5 Dong-Tai (TK) family tree (adapted from Zhang et al. 1999: 3) .................. 39
Figure 6 Chao’s (1930) tone letter system ................................................................... 45
Figure 7 Voice qualities in PT tone summarized from Gedney (1989b) and Liang &
Zhang (1996) ................................................................................................................ 55
Figure 8 PT tone features suggested by Sagart (1988: 89) .......................................... 55
Figure 9 Chinese tonal development scheme (summarized from Norman 1988: 52 ff.)
...................................................................................................................................... 62
Figure 10 Hengxian checked tones (Bi 1982: 21 cited in Fu 1995: 83) ...................... 66
Figure 11 Type of tonal mergers (Pan 1982) ............................................................... 68
Figure 12 Classification of SWT dialects based on tonal splits (adapted from
Chamberlain 1975: 50) ................................................................................................ 75
Figure 13 Voice analysis diagram of ˀba:nC1ʔ ‘village’ in Debao Dalong by PRAAT
.................................................................................................................................... 104
Figure 14 Sound analysis diagram of tʰa:n332 ‘charcoal’ and pʰa:332 ‘to chop’ in Debao
Dalong Yang Zhuang (CT) by PRAAT ..................................................................... 115
Figure 15 Speculation of shared innovations on different diachronic orders for
subgrouping languages............................................................................................... 185
Figure 16 Tai diachronic division based on tonology ................................................ 198
Figure 17 Tai family tree on the primary and secondary levels ................................ 198
xiii
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AND SYMBOLS
1-A
Aspirated sounds in the high register
1-C
Continuant sounds in the high register
1-G
Glottal sounds in the high register
1-U
Unaspirated stops in the high register
1-UC
Voiceless unaspirated stop + *r clusters
2
Voiced sounds in the low register
1/2
Alternation of tonal series between Northern Tai and Southern Tai
AMO-ST
Aspirated induced by monosyllabic onsets in Southern Tai
AMO-SY
Aspirated induced by monosyllabic onsets in Southern Tai and
Yongnan
ASO-CY
Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in Central Tai and
Yongnan
ASO-SY
Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in Southern Tai and
Yongnan
ASO-ST
Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in Southern Tai
ASO-YN
Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in Yongnan
CT
Central Tai
GB
Guibei Zhuang
KS
Kam-Sui language branch
L
language
MC
Middle Chinese
MSEA
Mainland Southeast Asia
xiv
NT
Northern Tai
NZ
Northern Zhuang
OC
Old Chinese
PT
Proto-Tai
ST
Southern Tai
SWT
Southwestern Tai
SZ
Southern Zhuang
T
Tone
TK
Tai-Kadai language family
YN
Yongnan Zhuang
VASO-1/2
Series 1/2 of voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets
VASO-2/1
Series 2/1 of voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets
VASO-GB
Voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in Guibei
Zhuang
xv
GLOSSARY
Diachronic process
A linguistic process or evolution of a language considered to
have undergone the development through history. Historical
linguistics is typically a diachronic study (Saussure 1983).
Gedney’s tone box
The Tai tone box designed by Gedney (1989[1972]: 202)
consists of five tone categories (A, B, C, DL, DS) crossed
with four groups of initial consonants (Group 1=Voiceless
friction, Group 2=Voiceless unaspirated, Group 3=Glottal,
and Group 4=Voiced.) conditioning possible tone splits.
Macrolanguage
“Multiple, closely related individual languages that are
deemed in some usage contexts to be a single language.”
“In addition to defining three-letter codes for individual
languages, the ISO 639-3 standard also defines codes for
macrolanguages. Macrolanguages were introduced into the
standard in order to handle cases in which varieties would be
considered distinct languages by the criterion of nonintellgibility as described above, but had already been given a
code as a single language by the previously existing ISO 6392 standard. For instance, Arabic [ara] and Chinese [zho] were
already defined in ISO 639-2 on the basis of literature shared
across many spoken varieties (and a shared writing system in
the case of Chinese)”
(https://www.ethnologue.com/about/problem-languageidentification). In the case of Tai, Zhuang (ISO 639-3:zha) is
a macrolanguage including at least sixteen single languages
in China (https://www.ethnologue.com/language/zha).
xvi
Middle Chinese
From the language of the 7th century AD rhyme dictionary,
the Qìyùn, to the language of the Song dynasty rhyme tables
(the mid 12th-century Yùnjīng) (Norman 1988: 23-24).
Monosyllable
It includes syllables with single initial consonants or
tautosyllabic initial clusters (like pl-, tr-, and gr-)
(summarized from Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009).
Old Chinese
The language of the Shījīng (The Book of Poetry), c. 1000
BC (Norman 1988: 23).
Proto
“Existing or coming before other things of the same type”
(Pearson Education Asia Limited & Foreign Language
Teaching and Research Press 2009: 1834). It is normally seen
as a combining form meaning “first,” “foremost,” “earliest
form of” used in the formation of compound words. A protolanguage refers to an ancient language (for example, ProtoIndo-European) diversifies and develops to several daughter
languages (such as Proto-Germanic, Proto-Celtic and so on,
in the case of Indo-European) (Campbell 2004: 187). In the
case of Tai-Kadai language family, proto-Tai, proto-initials,
proto-vowels and proto-tones, specialized in Tai linguistics to
denote the first stage of the Tai parent language, and the
initials, vowels and tones presenting in the Tai parent
language.
Proto-Tai
Diller (1998:14) points out that most linguists working in the
field accept a time depth of less than two thousand years for
Li’s Proto-Tai.
Register
A register or pitch register is a prosodic feature of syllables in
certain languages, in which tone, vowel phonation,
glottalization, or similar features depend upon each other. In
tonology, register means the subdivisions of the tone space
(Fu 1995: 30-31).
xvii
Sesquisyllable
“Literally ‘one-and-a-half syllable’, was originally coined by
Matisoff (1993) to refer to words that consist of an unstressed
‘minor’ syllable followed by a fully stressed ‘major’ syllable.
Crucially, the minor syllable is characterized as lacking a
contrastive vowel, e.g. Palaung /k.tɛ/ ‘earth’, and /kr̩.taʔ/
‘tongue’ ” (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 41). In Pittayaporn’s
definition, sesquisyllable in Proto-Tai refers to “a prosodic
word that consist of two syllables, the first of which is
unstressed and lacks a phonological vowel”, and “the
consonants in a sequence at the beginning of a sesquisyllable
are not all linked to a single syllable node” (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 42).
Tautosyllable
Syllable structure presents as that the consonants in a
sequence at the beginning of a tautosyllable are all linked to a
single syllable node (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 42), such
as syllables with initial clusters pl-, tr-, and gr-.
Tonal development
A theory of how tones develop in a specific language, such as
tonogenesis, tonal splits, tonal mergers, and etc (summarized
from Fu 1995).
Tonal flip-flop
An explanation of the voiced-high phenomenon, which refers
to that in some specific tonal languages, the proto-voiced
initial consonant group conditions high pitch tones, while
proto-voiceless initial consonant group conditions low pitch
tones. It assumes that “all register-splits start as voiced-low,
and the voiced-high correlation is caused by a later
development whereby high tones become low and low tones
become high” (Fu 1995: 82).
Tonal split
A tone separated into different tones because of the effect of
secondary changes in initials or vowel lengths (Fu 1995: 69).
Tone category
The category or class of different tones in a language. Words
having the same tone category in a language have the same
xviii
tone value, but the same tone category in different varieties of
the same language family does not always have the same tone
value. This concept has been used in the tones of Chinese
poetry and dialectology (四聲), which have been called four
traditional tone categories of Chinese words for a long time
(You 2004: 44).
Tone value
The tone (or value) in a language is a way of assessing how
high or low or what contour it is (You 2004: 44).
Toneme
The smallest unit of tone which represents meaningful
contrast (Yip 2002).
Tonogenesis
A hypothesis of how tones emerged in the history, such as
tone arises as an effect of the loss or merger of final and
initial consonants (Matisoff 1973).
Tonology
The science of tones or of speech intonations, proceeding
historically and comparatively (Matisoff 1994).
xix
Introduction
This study aims to analyze the tonal development in three modern sub-branches of
Tai languages distributed in Southern China and some countries in Mainland
Southeast Asia, to apply insights from the analysis of tonal development to the
classification of the Tai languages, and to propose a revised tone box which may be
more applicable for analyzing maximal Tai varieties. The descriptions are primarily
based upon tone data from seven varieties from Southwestern Tai (hereafter SWT),
sixteen varieties from Central Tai (hereafter CT), fourteen varieties from Northern
Tai (hereafter NT), and five varieties of Yongnan Zhuang (hereafter YN) whose
classification as CT or NT is open to debate1. The following sections provide some
background information about the research topics and the rationale of the thesis, as
well as the motivation for the thesis research subsequently presented. Section 1.1
introduces and discusses the background of Tai languages, Tai classifications, the
definition of Tai languages and varieties, and Tai tones. Section 1.2 firstly discusses
the main descriptions and some puzzles of Tai tones, and then lays out the objectives
and goals of the research. Section 1.3 gives an overview of the study, including the
scope, the limitations, the benefits and a description of the chapters in this thesis.
Tai speaking groups are mainly found in the Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region of
China, Northern Vietnam, Laos, and Thailand in Mainland Southeast Asia (hereafter
MSEA), as far north as Guizhou Province of China, as far south as halfway down the
Malay Peninsula, as far east as Guangdong Province of China, and as far west as
Assam and the surrounding areas of India. With the most numerous groups, such as
the Thai of Thailand with a population of 55 million, the Zhuang of Guangxi and
Yunnan in China with 15 million, the Shan of Myanmar with 2.8 million, and the
Bouyei of Guizhou in China and Vietnam with 2.5 million, “the total number of Tai
speakers is greater than 80 million” (Edmondson & Solnit 1997: 1).
1
These Tai varieties and their numbering are arranged as in Appendix C. For example, the first
language is Bangkok Thai and is labeled as Language 1 with its abbreviated number L1. Others are
similarly hereinafter arranged as L2 (Songkhla Southern Thai) to L42 (Zhenning Bouyei).
1
Just as its name implies, Tai speaking groups generally speak Tai languages as their
first language. The Tai language branch, which is also called the Zhuang-Dai branch
(
傣語支) in China, is grouped along with other related language branches like
Kam-Sui (or Dong-Shui in the Sinitic circle), Hlai (or Li in the Sinitic circle), and Kra
(or Ge-Yang in the Sinitic circle) to form a language family termed Tai-Kadai
(hereafter TK) used by most Tai linguists, or other names, as Daic in Shafer (1940:
302-337), and as Kra-Dai in Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009a: 2-5). Some linguists
have different opinions of grouping branches of TK on different levels of the family
tree diagram. A typical example of the classification of TK language family proposed
by Diller (2008: 7) is shown in Figure 1. In this proposal, Kam-Sui and Tai are
grouped as sisters of Lakjia to form a branch named Kam-Tai of which the Hlai and
Kra branches are sisters, since Kam-Sui and Tai share much more genetic
characteristics on cognates, phonology, tonology and grammar. There is a
controversial hypothesis mainly in China that proposes all TK languages to form a
language group under Sino-Tibetan language family, while there is another
disputable opinion named Austro-Tai language family standing that TK is a subgroup
of which Austronesian is a sister proposed by Benedict (1942) prevailing in Western
circle (Zhang et al. 1999: 2-3). Despite different opinions of classifying the whole TK
language family, most linguists have an agreement that certain varieties are grouped
together to form a particular group as Tai, which is different from other groups.
Tai-Kadai Language Family
Kam-Tai (Zhuang-Dong)
Kam-Sui
Lakjia
Hlai (Li)
Kra (Geyang)
Tai (Zhuang-Tai)
Gelao
Be
Lachi
Kam
Laha
Sui
Buyang
…
NT
CT
SWT
…
Figure 1 Tai-Kadai language family tree (adapted from Diller 2008: 7)
The geographical distributions of TK languages are shown in the following Figure 2,
and Li’s (1977) three Tai sub-branches (Northern Tai in yellow, Central Tai in red,
and Southwestern Tai in orange) shows a big vertical Y shape upper left to Guangxi
2
and Guizhou provinces of China, upper right to Northern Burma and Northeastern
India, and down to Southern Thailand.
Figure 2 Distribution of the Tai–Kadai language family (from wikipedia.org)
Basically, there are two main positions about the primary classification of the Tai
branch. They are the two plus one taxonomy proposed by Haudricourt (1956) and
Gedney (1989) and the tripartite division of Li Fang-Kuei (1977). Li’s scheme of
three branches, SWT, CT, and NT, has long been accepted as the standard one in the
field of comparative Tai linguistics.
3
Tai
Southwestern Tai
Central Tai
Northern Tai
Figure 3 Tripartite division of Tai languages (Li 1977)
However, Haudricourt (1956) insisted on a two-term system, dividing the proto-Tai
(PT) first into Dioi or ʔyai (corresponding approximately but not exactly to NT) vs.
Tai proper. Gedney (1989b) also tends to support this hypothesis, and proposes that
Li’s CT sub-branch and SWT sub-branch can form a sub-group of which the NT subbranch is a sister. Gedney also admits that there are basic differences between the
CT and SWT. He has proposed to divide these two sub-branches by lexical and
phonological criteria, but proposes that the scale of these differences is different
from those between NT and other Tai languages, from the evidence of some
intermediate dialects between SWT and CT (Gedney 1989b: 65-66).
Tai
Northern Tai
Central Tai
Southwestern Tai
Figure 4 Two plus one taxonomy of Tai division (Gedney 1989b)
The argument of the classifications of the Tai languages is an important topic in the
discussions subsequently presented because it is related to one of the objectives of
this thesis. The analysis of tonal development is crucial to the explanation of the
insights to the classification of the Tai languages. The two main primary
classifications of the Tai branch aforementioned will be tested by the consequence of
this study.
There are many languages and dialects within three Tai sub-branches, such as
Central Thai, Northern Thai, Southern Thai and Northeastern Thai in SWT, Southern
Zhuang (hereafter SZ), Nung, and Tay in CT, and Northern Zhuang (hereafter NZ)
and Bouyei in NT discussed in previous research materials, but actually the criteria
of defining these “languages” and “dialects” are inconsistent and complicated. For
4
example, in Chinese publications, the word “the Zhuang language” is always
assumed to denote an individual language. This “language” is first divided into
Northern and Southern two big dialects, and NZ and SZ are furthermore divided into
eight sub-dialects and five sub-dialects respectively (Zhang et al. 1999: 12-13). But
just as in Edmondson (1994: 150), it has been pointed out that the SZ is included in
Li’s (1977) CT sub-branch, while NZ is included in Li’s NT sub-group, “thus, in some
sense the Zhuang speak at least two languages, one belonging to Northern Tai and
the other to Central Tai” (Edmondson 1994: 150). Despite Edmondson’s definition of
two languages of Zhuang, many vernaculars and varieties inside NZ and SZ are
actually not intelligible with each other at all, and they are treated as “dialects” or
“sub-dialects” of Zhuang, or even different “languages” of the Tai family in different
materials.
In this thesis, the definition of a “language” only follows the principles in part 3 of
ISO 639, which is a code that aims to define three-letter identifiers for all known
human languages. The standard of ISO 639-3 was published by ISO in 2007, and it
attempts to provide codes for the representation of names of languages “as complete
an enumeration of languages as possible, including living, extinct, ancient, and
constructed languages, whether major or minor, written or unwritten” (http://www01.sil.org/iso639-3/). As a result, ISO 639-3 deals with a very large number of
lesser-known languages. In this part of ISO 639, most identifiers are assumed to
denote distinct individual languages. For instance, ISO 639-3 contains 16 identifiers
designated as individual language identifiers for distinct varieties of Zhuang by
2013, while ISO 639-1 and ISO 639-2 each contain only one identifier for Zhuang
(including NZ and SZ which belong to NT and CT respectively), “za” and “zha”
respectively, which are designated as individual language identifiers in those parts
of ISO 639. It is assumed in ISO 639-3 that the single identifiers for Zhuang in parts
1 and 2 of ISO 639 correspond to the many identifiers collectively for distinct
varieties of Zhuang. Just as in the treatment of ISO 639-3, these varieties such as
Yang Zhuang, Nong Zhuang, and Central Hongshuihe Zhuang are treated as
individual languages in this thesis. On the other hand, Zhuang or NZ/SZ is treated as
a macrolanguage with ISO 639-3 code “zha” which contains at least sixteen
individual languages closely related (https://www.ethnologue.com/language/zha).
The necessity of using ISO 639-3 code for defining a language in this thesis can be
illustrated by the following aspects.
5
First, a language discussed in different sources may have different names, and it is
necessary for this thesis to use a representative name in a consistent way. For
example, “Central Thai” refers to the language including Tai varieties spoken in the
Central areas of Thailand, and “Standard Thai” is the standardized variety of this
language. This language has been more exclusively named “Siamese” in many
sources such as Li (1977). Some sources also follow what Thai people call this
language as Phasa Klang, which means “Central Language” in Thai. For uniformly
addressing this language which has served different names mentioned above, this
thesis follows its formal name in ISO 639-3 code to use only the name “Thai” when
addressing the entirety of this language, except when citing other names of it from
other sources. In a similar way, in this thesis “Northern Thai” represents the entirety
of the language having other names such as “Phasa Nuea”, “Lanna”, “Kam Mueang”,
and “Thai Yuan”, “Isan” represents the language having the other name
“Northeastern Thai”, and “Southern Thai” represents the language with the other
names “Pak Tai” (Thai: ภาษาปักษ์฿ตຌ) and “Dambro” (Thai: ภาษาตามพร)
(https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Southern_Thai_language).
Second, it is required to have a consistent definition of a language when discussing
“dialects”, and “sub-dialects” under a specific language. The linguistic varieties
denoted by each of the identifiers in ISO 639-3 are presumed to be distinct
languages and not dialects of other languages, even though for some purposes some
users may consider a variety listed in ISO 639-3 to be a “dialect” rather than a
“language”. If we follow the standard of ISO-639-3, the term “dialect” is used as in
the field of linguistics where it simply identifies any sub-variety of a language that
might be based on geographic region, age, gender, social class, time period, or the
like, which are all included within the denotation represented by the identifier for
that language. Thus, Yang Zhuang with the code “zyg” in ISO 639-3 represents the
complete range of all the spoken or written varieties of this language, including
Jingxi County Yang Zhuang, and Debao County Yang Zhuang, which are treated as
dialects of Yang Zhuang, and the Iang-Noengz orthography recommended to be a
unified writing system as proposed by Liao (2010) for all varieties of Yang Zhuang.
Additionally, Debao Urban, Debao Suburb, Debao Ma’ai, and Jingxi Hurun varieties
are treated as sub-dialects of the Debao County Yang Zhuang Dialect, as they share
more phonological similarities than those varieties of Jingxi County Yang Zhuang
Dialect, but differ from each other in tonal splits.
Also, consistently using terms like “languages” and “dialects” can guide readers to
understand what the term “variety” refers to in this thesis. Compared with the
6
specific scope of languages and dialects, there is not an obvious boundary to define a
variety of a language or a language family. As a general concept of a variation of a
language, a variety can address the entirety of a language under a language group or
family, a dialect of a language, a sub-dialect of a dialect, or a specific geographical
point investigated in a linguistic survey. For example, we can say Yang Zhuang is a
variety of CT, Debao County Yang Zhuang is a variety of Yang Zhuang, and Debao
Urban is a variety of Debao County Yang Zhuang. In this thesis, only “variety” but
not “language” or “dialect” is used to address all linguistic points investigated for
specifically illustrating the circumstances of tonal development. For example, when
illustrating the tonal development of Thai Language (ISO 639-3: THA), Bangkok is
chosen to be a specific “variety” to represent that language. For another example,
Debao Urban is registered to be one of the specific varieties of Debao County Yang
Zhuang.
In summary, languages and dialects are defined by following the standard of ISO
639-3 code in this thesis, and all specific linguistic points investigated or chosen for
analyzing tonal development are treated as varieties of Tai languages.
It is generally known that all Tai languages are tonal just like the majorities of the
languages in East Asia and MSEA. Tones in these languages are the use of pitch and
other laryngeal features to distinguish lexical or grammatical meaning. A distinct
tone normally has a fixed pitch, or has similar pitch on syllables with
complementary distribution. A tone in a language is a way of assessing how high or
low or what contour it is. In Chinese phonology, this kind of assessing the pitch of a
tone is called tone value or tonal value (調值). The same tonal value on different
syllables are normally classified to be of the same category, and this categorical
behavior of tones is called tone category or tonal category (調類). These two terms
are commonly used by linguistics working on tonal languages in the Sinitic circle.
For example, in the discussion of the tonal development of the Chinese languages by
Norman (1988: 52-57), there were four tonal categories píng, shǎng, qù, and rù in
Middle Chinese, and they presented different tonal values like ‘level and non-abrupt’
for the tone of píng recorded in the Chinese dictionary Qièyùn compiled in AD 601
by Lù Fǎyán (Norman 1988: 24).
Basically, the vast majority of Tai languages have five to six tones on live syllables
ending in a continuant (vowel, semivowel, or nasal), or smooth syllables used by
Gedney (1989[1972]: 192), and two to four tones on dead syllables ending with a
7
stop (-p, -t, -k, or -ʔ), or checked syllables used by Gedney (1989[1972]: 192).
Neither all five to six tones can be found in all the live syllables, nor two to four
tones can be found in all the dead syllables, since tonal behaviors are conditioned by
different features of initial consonants. That is to say, only a part of initials can
collocate with all tones. For example, there are five distinct tones in Thai (L1) with
different tonal values 33 (mid level), 21 (low-falling), 41ʔ (high-falling, with glottal
constriction), 453ʔ (high-rising-falling, with glottal constriction), 24 (low-rising),
and three of them (21, 41, and 453) can occur in dead syllables, while the tone 453
present as another similar tonal value 45 (high-rising) in this situation (Gedney
1989[1972]: 192)2. When a syllable begins with the initial m-, we can find all five
tones on live syllables and three tones on dead syllables, as ma:33 ‘to come’, ma:21 ‘to
soak’, ma:j4ʔ1 ‘widowed’, ma:453ʔ ‘horse’, ma:24 ‘dog’, ma:k21 ‘betel nut’, ma:k41 ‘very
much’, and mak45 ‘often’. But when a syllable begins with the initial d-, normally we
can only find three tones on live syllables, and one tone on dead syllables, as di:33
‘good’, da:m41 ‘handle’, da:21 ‘to scold’, and dɔ:k21 ‘flower’3 .
Different Tai varieties present different tonal behaviors and different types of tonal
splits. For example, in Thai (L1) syllables with aspirated stops can have all five
tones, while in Yang Zhuang varieties, aspirated stops can only have three tones of
all six distinct tones, as the following examples in Thai: kʰa:33 ‘to stick’, kʰa:21
‘galangal’, kʰa:41 ‘to kill’, kʰa:453 ‘trade’, and kʰa:24 ‘leg’ vs. the examples in the Debao
Urban variety of Yang Zhuang (L8): kʰa:353 ‘leg’, kʰa:24 ‘to kill’, and kʰa:33 ‘galangal’,
but words like kʰa:31, kʰa:213 , and kʰa:55 which have the other three tones of the all
six tones can be never found in this variety, except some modern loans from
Mandarin Chinese.
The limitation of the collocation between initials and tones originates from tonal
register splits triggered by initial voicing on post-PT level (cf. §4.2.2), such as the
voicing of proto-voiceless continuant initials (like *m̥ - > *m-, *n̥- > *n-, and *l ̥- >
The tonal values (in Chao’s 5-pitch scale) of Thai tones in Gedney’s original text are not provided,
instead the described diction like “low level”, “falling, with glotttal constriction” and etc. are provided.
There are some specific phonation types or phonetic shapes accompanying with some specific tones in
many Tai varieties (cf. §4.1.3.3). For example, tones 41ʔ and 453ʔ in Thai (L1) and 24ʔ and 213ʔ in
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (L8) have a final glottal constriction, and tones 51 and 11 in Yizhou
Suogan Guibei Zhuang (L36) have creakiness accompanying with these tones (cf. Appendix C).
Because these phonation types or phonetic shapes are not phonemic or tonemic, hereafter they will be
omitted if the discussion is not related to tonogenesis or other aspects which need to probe into these
phonation types or phonetic shapes.
3
Some exceptions can be found in some informal terms of Thai. For example, I have heard that
some Thai young people use the English word ‘dog food’ as d ɕ fu:t , whose the first syllable with
the initial d- which never has a high rising tone (45) on dead syllable in Thai words. This kind of
collocation is treated as later development and is not counted as an inherent tonal behavior in Thai.
2
8
*l-) and the devoicing of proto-voiced stops (like *b- > *p-, *d- > *t- and *g- > *k-)
which caused the PT four tonal categories to split into two series (original high and
low registers) for preserving semantic distinctions (Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997:
207). During the time of tonal splits, in some Tai varieties like Debao Urban Yang
Zhuang (L8) some of the series of initial consonants like aspirated plosives (pʰ-, tʰ-,
and kʰ-) only developed from proto-voiceless sounds which conditioned high register
tones at times of tonal split. This has made items with aspirated initials only
collocate with high register tones in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang. In contrast, in Thai
aspirated sounds developed from both proto-voiceless and proto-voiced sounds
which conditioned high and low register tones respectively, and this has made items
with aspirated initials collocate with both high and low register tones in Thai. This
is why all five tones can be found on items with aspirated initials in Thai but only
three of the all six tones can be found on items with aspirated initials in Debao
Urban Yang Zhuang.
The complicated situation of tones in Tai differs from variety to variety, from dialect
to dialect, and from group to group. This complexity is a result of different situations
of tonal development in different Tai daughter languages. For expressly probing into
the cause and the effect of this complexity, this thesis is going to study the objectives
involving Tai tonal development in the following section.
On the basis of the previous research of Tai tones and the Tai classification based on
Tai tones, this thesis aims at clarifying or solving the following issues.
1) To analyze the tonal development in three modern sub-branches of Tai
languages, especially those in Li’s CT and NT languages.
2) To apply insights from the analysis of tonal development to the classification of
the Tai languages.
3) To propose a revised tone box which may be more applicable for analyzing
maximal Tai varieties.
These three objectives are related to each other by their associated characteristic,
which is the study of Tai tones, or Tai tonology. There are a number of competing
descriptions of Tai tones. In this section, I first outline three most wildly adopted
system of describing Tai tones. They are Li Fang-Kuei’s pattern, the Sinitic pattern,
and Gedney’s tone box.
9
In Tai linguistics, the treatments of counting the number of tones are different
within different linguists in linguistic circles. For example, as in §1.1.4, normally
Western linguists follow the Thai traditional method to count that Standard Thai has
five distinct tones, which can all occur on live syllables. Those three tones in dead
syllables are treated as allotones of those tones having the same or similar pitch and
occurring with live syllables. That is why the tone in the word mak45 ‘often’ is seen
as an allotone of the tone in the word ma:453 ‘horse’, since they are similar in pitch
(of high tones) and have a complementary distribution – the former tone only occurs
on dead syllables and the latter tone only occurs on live syllables. However, scholars
in the Sinitic Tai linguistic circle, which include Liang and Zhang (1996) and Zhang
and colleagues (1999), count that there are ten tones in Thai, since they treat tones
on dead syllables as different tones from those on live syllables, and count tones
developed from different origin (proto-tones) as divided tones even though some of
them may have the same tone. For example, Liang and Zhang (1996: 962) list ten
tones in Thai as follows (with “T” as the abbreviation of a sorted tone): T1 (24), T2
(33), T3 = T6 (41), T4 (453), T5 (22), T7 (22), T8 (55), T9 (22), and T10 (41).
Within these tones, T1 – T6 are on live syllables, and T7 – T10 are on dead syllables.
T3 and T6 are separately counted even they are the same pitch (high-falling 41) and
are both on live syllables (and they are actually of the same tone counted in Thai
traditional method and in Western circle), since they developed from different prototones, such as in Thai the tones of the words ฆ kʰa:⁴¹ ‘to kill’ (T3 developing from
proto-tone C) and ค kʰa:⁴¹ ‘value’ (T6 developing from proto-tone B) are treated as
two tones even they are actually homophonous. T10 is also separately counted from
T3 and T6 although it has the same tonal value as T3 and T6, since it is on dead
syllables. Tones 5, 7 and 9 are listed with the same tonal value (low level), but are
separately counted since they are on live and dead syllables respectively, or
collocate with different vowel length even though two of them are both on dead
syllables. That is to say, scholars in the Sinitic circle normally define tone based on
the tonal category principle, in contrast scholars in the Western circle normally
define tone based on tonal value principle. In the situation of explicitly
distinguishing the concept of “tone” in the Sinitic and Western circles, we use
“contrasting tone” for referring to the later one.
To further illustrate the problem, it is necessary to compare the different styles of
numbering the tones of the Tai languages or dialects used by Li Fang-Kuei, scholars
10
in the Sinitic circle, and Gedney’s tone box. In this issue, the concepts of “prototone” and “tonal category” in Tai languages play an important role.
When illustrating tones in PT or comparing tones among modern Tai languages, Li
Fang-Kuei’s system of numbering the proto-Thai tones is most commonly used, and
that convention will be followed in this thesis as well. It is generally believed that
PT had a system of three tonal categories on smooth syllables, and a fourth tone on
checked syllables. These tones are designated by Li Fang-Kuei as Tones *A *B *C and
*D. Later these tones split into two series based on the voicing of the initial
consonants, and Li designated 1 for those from a proto-voiceless initial and 2 for
those from a proto-voiced initial. Furthermore *D was divided into DL for a dead
syllable with a long vowel or cluster of two vowels, and DS for a checked syllable
with short or simple vowel respectively, since vocalic length is crucial to explaining
the varying developments in different dialects (Li 1977: 25-28). Thus, in modern Tai
varieties, there are six tonal categories A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, and C2 on live syllables,
and four tonal categories DL1, DL2, DS1, and DS2 on dead syllables. Li’s tone pattern
which is shown in Table 1 has been widely used by linguists working on Tai
languages in Western circle.
Table 1 Li Fang-Kuei’s Tai tone pattern (summarized from Li 1977)
Proto-Tone
*A
*B
*C
Voiceless
A1
B1
Voiced
A2
B2
Proto-Initial
*D
(*DL)
(*DS)
C1
DL1
DS1
C2
DL2
DS2
In this tone pattern, normally the same tone category presents the same tonal value
in a specific Tai variety. For example, the ten tonal categories in Debao Urban
variety of Yang Zhuang (L8) present as the following tonal values: A1=453 (highrising-falling), A2=31 (mid-falling), B1/DL1/DS1=55 (high level), B2/DL2/DS2
=33 (mid level), C1=24ʔ (low- rising with final glottal constriction), and C2 =213ʔ
(low-falling-rising with final glottal constriction), as in the following examples:
ma:453 (A1) ‘dog’, ma:31 (A2) ‘to come’, ma:55 (B1) ‘to soak’, ma:33 (B2) ‘to wait (a
minute)’, ma:24ʔ (C1) ‘to grow up’, ma:213ˀ (C2) ‘horse’, ma:k55 (DL1) ‘fruit’, ma:k33
(DL2) ‘to be plenty’, mak55 (DS1) ‘beautiful’, and mak33 (DS2) ‘ink’. Some exceptions
also occur along with further tonal splits and mergers in some modern Tai varieties,
such as in Thai tonal categories B2 and C1 have merged to present a high-falling
pitch 41 to become a single contrasting tone.
11
In a similar way, in the Sinitic circle scholars have become accustomed to describing
tones of Kam-Tai languages using the same numbering system and tonal categories
that they use to describe the Chinese languages, the Miao-Yao languages and
Vietnamese. To that end, they employ both by cardinal numbers (1-10) and
traditional Chinese terms (píng for A, shǎng for C, qù for B, rù for D, yīn for voiceless,
and yáng for voiced) related to traditional Chinese phonology. So, viewed in Chinese
terms related to “tonal category based principle”, there are ten tones, which include
six tones occurring with live syllables and four tones occurring with dead syllables,
in Tai languages. This is in contrast to the system used by linguists mostly seen in
the Western circle, in which the tones that occur with dead syllables are actually
seen as allotones of tones that occur with live syllables, on account of “tonal value
based principle”. In all, when being viewed in the Western method, the Debao Urban
variety of Yang Zhuang has six contrasting tones which present the following tone
values: 453, 31, 24ʔ, 213ʔ, 55, and 33.
In spite of the different viewpoints of how many tones there are in a Tai variety,
there is a specific correspondence of Tai tonal categories between the Western circle
and the Sinitic circle which is demonstrated in Table 2.
Table 2 Tai Tone correspondence between the Sinitic and Western Circles
Chinese
yīn
yáng
yīn
yáng
yīn
yáng
yīn
yáng
yīn
yáng
Terms
píng
píng
shǎng
shǎng
qù
qù
rù
rù
rù
rù
Sinitic
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
A1
A2
C1
C2
B1
B2
DS1
DS2
DL1
DL2
33
41ʔ
453ʔ
21
41
21
45
21
41
31
24ʔ
213ʔ
55;
33
55
33
Pattern
Western
or Li’s
Pattern
Thai tone
24;
value
33
Debao
Urban
tone
453;
31
33
55;
33
33
value
In the tonal categories shown in Table 2, all the odd numbers correspond to those
tones with proto-voiceless initials and all the even numbers correspond to those with
proto-voiced initials, both in the Western and Sinitic circles. In the Sinitic circle a
tone with proto-voiceless initials is often called “odd tone” (單數調), while a tone
12
with proto-voiced initial is often called “even tone” (雙數調). It is in contrast to
some previous works out of these two systems numbering the tones of a Tai variety
by filling the cardinal numbers without a consistent order of odd or even. For
example, the traditional Thai teaching methodology numbers 1 – 5 tones in Thai to
correspond to Li’s tonal categories A2/A1, B1/DL1/DS1, B2/C1/DL2, C2/DS2, and
A1 respectively, and as Hanna (2012: xiv) lists 1 – 6 tones in Dai Lue with the order
corresponding to Li’s tonal categories A1, B1/DL1/DS1, C1, A2, B2/DL2/DS2, and
C2 respectively.
Despite the correspondence between the tonal description methods of the Western
and Sinitic circles, they do not always present the tones in the same way when
applied to different Tai varieties. That is, for presenting tonal splits in those Tai
languages and dialects which exhibit differently from most common situation, a
simple split conditioned by the voicing of the initial consonant giving, in many
cases, six contrasting tones instead of the earlier three, the description of the tonal
splits is more complicated and the consistent correspondence is lost. For example, in
Table 3 Tone A1 in Thai presents tonal splits to two tone values 24 and 33, and the
later one merges with the tonal value of Tone A2.
Table 3 Examples from tonal numbering presenting the contradiction to the
“tonal category based principle” in the Sinitic circle
Gloss
Li’s tonal
categories
Debao Ma’ai of Yang Zhuang
Thai
Tonal
Sinitic tonal
values
numbering
24
Tonal values
53
Sinitic tonal
numbering
dog
A1
ma:
ma:¹
ma:
ma:¹
leg
A1
kʰa:24
kʰa:1
kʰa:31
kʰa:²
year
A1
pi:33
pi:²
pej53
pej¹
leaf
A1
33
baj²
31
A2
kʰa:n²
ˀbɔj²
pole
kʰa:n33
ˀbɔj
ka:n31
ka:n²
have
A2
mi:33
mi:²
mej31
mej²
baj
This kind of tonal splits and mergers result in a problem of numbering the tones in
the Sinitic circle. This is when numbering two tones being in contrast and splitting
from the same proto-tonal category with proto-voiceless initial, if one of them merge
with an even tone’s tonal value, it is always numbered to be the same tone as that
even tone, even it does exactly not develop from a proto-tone with a proto-voiced
initial and should be categorized to an odd tone due to its proto-tonal category. In
Table 3, the examples of Thai and the Debao Ma’ai variety of Yang Zhuang are
13
adapted from Liang & Zhang (1996: 283, 461, 335, 244, 449, 308) and Zhang et al.
(1999: 150-151) respectively. Both in Thai and Debao Ma’ai variety of Yang Zhuang
tone category A1 splits into two contrasting tonal values, and one of them merges
with Tone A2. The tonal numbering of the words ‘year’ and ‘leaf’ in Thai, and the
words ‘leg’ and ‘leaf’ in Debao Ma’ai is T2, and is actually contradictory to the “tonal
category based principle” in the Sinitic circle since they all developed from prototones with voiceless initials.
To solve the problem, Gedney’s tone box (Gedney 1989[1972]) was used to explain
the complicated situation of tonal splits. It adapts Li’s tonal numbering of Proto-Tai
and further defined four groups of voiceless proto-initial consonants by their shared
phonetic features at the time of the tonal splits. These four groups are voiceless
friction, voiceless unaspirated, glottal, and voiced respectively, and condition
possible tone splits among different Tai languages. Within these four groups of
proto-initials, the former three consist of voiceless sounds in Li’s pattern. Gedney’s
tone box which is shown as in Table 4, supplemented by more lexical items, has
been widely used by linguists working on SWT languages.
Table 4 Tai Tone box (adapted from Gedney 1989[1972]: 202-204)
Proto-Tai Tones
Initials at time of
tonal splits
Voiceless friction
sounds,
*s-, *hm-, *ph-, etc.
Voiceless
unaspirated stops,
*p-, *t-, *k-, etc.
Glottal, *ʔ-, *ʔb-,
etc.
Voiced, *b-, *m-, *l-,
*z-, etc.
A
hu: ‘ear’;
B
C
DL
kʰa:t ‘torn’;
DS
kʰa: ‘leg’
pʰa: ‘to split’
kʰaj ‘egg’;
kʰa: ‘to kill’;
sɨa ‘shirt’
ŋɨak ‘gums’
pʰak ‘vegetable’
mat ‘flea’;
pi: ‘year’;
pa: ‘forest’;
hu: ‘ear’;
pɔ:t ‘lung’;
kop ‘frog’;
ta: ‘eye’
kaj ‘chicken’
tom ‘to boil’
tɔ:k ‘to pound’
bin ‘to fly’;
da: ‘to scold’;
ba: ‘shoulder’
ba: ‘crazy’;
ba:n ‘village’
dɛ:t ‘sunshine’;
bet ‘fishhook’;
pʰɔ: ‘father’;
na:m ‘water’;
mi:t ‘knife’;
nok ‘bird’;
dɛ:ŋ ‘red’
mɨ: ‘hand’;
na: ‘rice
field’
raj ‘dry field’
ma:j ‘wood’
ʔa:p ‘to bathe’
lɨat ‘blood’
tap ‘liver’
ʔok ‘chest’
lak ‘to steal’
The designation of Gedney’s tone box more reasonably presents tonal splits in those
Tai languages and dialects which exhibit the deviation of the most common
situation. In these Tai varieties, a single proto-tone has in many cases split into two
or even three tones on the basis of conditioning phonetic features of the initial
consonants other than the simple voiced-voiceless contrast. For example, Table 5
plots the tones in Thai using Gedney’s tone box and presents the tone numbers of the
14
Sinitic circle method in the box. In some cases, formerly contrastive tones have
merged into one tone, and some tones in dead syllables are actually allotones of
those in live syllables. In these cases, we use an equal sign “=” between two Sinitic
tone numbers for illustrating they are the same tone in the box. In all, Thai has five
contrasting tones.
Table 5 Gedney’s Tone box and Sinitic tone numbers together applied to Thai
Proto-Tai Tones
Initials at time of tonal
splits
A
B
C
DL
DS
1
5
3
9=5
7=5
2
5
3
9=5
7=5
Glottal
2
5
3
9=5
7=5
Voiced
2
6=3
4
10=3
8=4
Voiceless friction sounds
Voiceless unaspirated
stops
In Table 5 we see the tonal splits in Thai are irregular between Tone A and other
tone categories. The split in Tone A is between voiceless friction and other initial
groups, which cross the dividing line of voiceless and voiced proto-initials, while the
splits in Tones B, C, DL, and DS are between voiceless and voiced sounds. For the
tonal split in A in Thai, a scholar in the Sinitic circle might use the number 2, an
even number, which would normally correlate with a voiced initial (the fourth row),
to represent the tones found with voiceless unaspirated stops and glottal sounds (the
second and the third rows), since the tones on these two rows are the same as T2 in
the voiced row. This practice is already shown as the numbering of the Thai words
in Table 3. These tones, however, are actually in the rows, which correspond with
voiceless proto-initials. On the other hand, Li (1977: 28-29) describes the split in
Tone A in Thai with a more consistent explanation: A1 can be mid level (33) or lowrising (24), depends on the different initial features, and A2 is mid level (33). That is
to say, in Li's system, a tonal category can have more than one tone value depending
on the proto-initial.
Compared to Sinitic tone numbers and Li’s tone pattern, Gedney’s tone box explains
this kind of special case of tonal development more consistently. Its purpose is to
display “a maximum of possible tonal distinctions resulting from the various types of
tonal splits that have been described. In any given Tai dialect there will be a division
of each column of the chart into two or three tones, or in some cases no such
division at all in one column or another. Most dialects will also show coalescence or
15
syncretism between two or more boxes belonging to different columns” (Gedney
1989[1972]: 202). Furthermore, a curious feature of the tone box is that, when
using it to plot the tonal splits for any of the many Tai varieties, the set order of the
horizontal rows, from top to bottom, is always the same. This is true for all SWT
varieties that have been described, and “one never finds the same new tone
associated with, say, consonants of the first or second category, and also the fourth,
skipping the third”. This is called a single ‘contiguous’ tone box or a fixed order
phonological spectrum (Gedney 1985: 120). The tonal numbering correspondence
among Li’s pattern, Sinitic circle method, and Gedney’s tone box is demonstrated in
the following Table 6.
Table 6 Tonal correspondence among Li’s pattern, Sinitic method, and
Gedney’s tone box
Traditional
Chinese
terms
Li’s
pattern
Proto-Tai Tones
Initials at
time of tonal splits
A
B
C
DL
DS
1
5
3
9
7
2
6
4
10
8
píng
qù
shǎng
Voiceless friction sounds
yīn
1
Voiceless unaspirated stops
Glottal
yáng
2
Voiced
Traditional Chinese terms
rù
In practice, Gedney’s tone box (Gedney 1989[1972]) has been proved to be a good
way for solving the problems of complicated tonal splits in SWT varieties. However,
when applied to varieties of CT, such as Debao Zhuang (Yang Zhuang in ISO 639-3),
Wenma Zhuang (Dai Zhuang in ISO 639-3), and Bac Va Nung, and most NT
varieties, Gedney’s tone box has several shortcomings, the most noticeable being the
so-called voicing alternation between NT and CT/SWT, and the splits among the
voiceless friction sounds and the voiceless unaspirated stops. Some scholars,
including Gedney himself, have also found there are some problems with using the
tone box for explaining the tonal splits and mergers in some CT and NT varieties.
For example, Gedney (1989[1972]: 204) himself has alluded to this issue in his
seminal article detailing the famous tone box. He mentions that there are two
languages that split Box 1 of Column A into two series; a variety of Nung (a CT
language, which is called Bac Va as collected by Gedney (Hudak 2008: 37-39)) in
16
Northern Vietnam, and Saek in Thailand (a Northern Tai language). He further
states, “It is possible that further research may show that our four horizontal tiers
are not enough… When someday we fully understand the historical reasons for the
tonal irregularities found in languages of the Northern Branch of the Tai family, we
may find it necessary to make still further horizontal subdivisions in our chart”
Gedney (1989[1972]: 204). Although he believes that it is inefficient to further
refine the chart since the tone box is already more refined than necessary for the
vast majority of Tai languages (Gedney 1989[1972]: 204), it has been pointed out
that his tone box can only capture all the tonal distinctions in SWT languages, but
cannot deal with the problems of complex tonal correspondence among SWT, CT,
and NT varieties (Liao & Shen 2012).
As a suggestion for solving these problems, Liao and Shen (2012) have provided
below in Table 7 a revised and expanded version of the Tai tone box which can be
used beyond SWT. In their chart, every box in the proto-voiceless sounds category is
given a fixed name based on Li’s tone designations (A1, B1, C1, DL1, DS1, and etc.)
plus a letter corresponding to the first letter of the descriptive word accompanying
“voiceless” in the proto-voiceless rows. For example, A1-A would represent the tone
for a word which had a voiceless initial consonant in PT (1) and that voiceless initial
consonant was also Aspirated (A). A2, B2, C1, DL1, and DS2 are used for the protovoiced Row, and A1/2, B1/2, C1/2, DL1/2, and DS1/2 are used for the protobreathy Row4. This revised tone box is supplemented by more lexical items
commonly found in most languages from the three branches of Tai. Some etyma
have different semantic changes among different languages. For example, tʰa:B2
‘wharf’ in Bongkok Thai and ta:B2 ‘river’ in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang both develop
from PT *da:B, but have different and related meanings which are commonly found
in SWT and CT/NT/YN respectively. In such situation, an different semantic option
is bracketed after the main lexical definition which is more commonly found in
more Tai language varieties, such as ‘river (wharf)’ in Box B2 in Table 7.
4
For clearly illustrating the initial changes at time of tonal split, in Table 7 I add some of the
examples of common initial forms from modern Tai varieties into their box. Some forms of initial
change in some Tai varieties are shown in a blacket, such as pʰ- (f-) in Aspirations referring to the
aspirated initial consonant pʰ- in CT/YN varieties which normally presents as f- in SWT/NT varieties.
For instance, the word for ‘rain’ is pʰ :nA1 in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (CT), pʰonA1 in Nanning
Shuangding (YN), fonA1 in Bangkok Thai, and fɯnA1 in Wuming Shuangqiao Yongbei Zhuang (NT).
Examples of initials in their so-called proto-voiced aspirations & breathiness are separated by a slash
“/” to show different forms (normally aspirated in SWT/CT but unaspirated in NT) which cause
voicing alternation, like the intial pair pʰ-/p- in CT/SWT pʰowC1/pʰu:C1 vs. YN/NT powC2/pu:C2 ‘person
(CLF for person)’.
17
Table 7 A revised version of Gedney’s tone box (adapted from Liao & Shen
2012)
Initial
Tone categories
Groups at
Time of
A
B
C
DL
DS
Tonal Split
Aspirations:
A1-A
B1-A
C1-A
DL1-A
DS1-A
pʰ- (f-), tʰ-, kʰ-,
leg;
to chop;
cloth
torn; carry
vegetable;
mountain
egg;
(sheet); to
on poles;
hailstone; to
(cliff); to
charcoal
wait; to kill
astringent
shut
pʰj-, kʰj-, h-, ɕ-
…
add
Continuants:
A1-C
B1-C
C1-C
DL1-C
DS1-C
n-, m-, ŋ-, w-
dog; thick;
to soak;
face; grass;
fruits
flea; heavy;
three
tired; to
sour
(areca);
color (tattoo)
(ʋ-), l-, j-, s- (ɬ-)
Initials
from
ProtoVoiceless
…
Unaspirated
Stops:
p-, t-, k-, tɕ-,
send
A1-U
B1-U
deaf; pestle
C1-U
DL1-U
DS1-U
fish; door;
to blow;
aunt;
mouth; cliff
duck; liver;
crow
chicken;
seedling;
(waterfall);
frog
low
establish
mustard
pl- (pj-), kl(kj-) …
Unaspirated
Stop + *r
Clusters:
A1-UC
B1-UC
C1-UC
DL1-UC
DS1-UC
eye; die;
to hunt
near; CLF
to expose;
grasshopper;
for stone
broken;
to seed
put up
bamboo
*pr-, *tr-, *kr-
strips
Glottal Sounds:
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
ˀb- (b-, m-, w-),
to fly;
shoulder;
village; to
scar (time);
to pinch; to
star;
to scold; at
get;
hot
extinguish;
sugarcane
(boiling); to
one (above
want
tens digit)
ˀd- (d-, n-, l-),
ˀj- (j-, ð-), ˀw-
medicine
(w-, b-), ʔ-
(hungry)
Initials from Proto-voiced
Sounds:
p- (pʰ-), t- (tʰ-), k- (kʰ-),
tɕ- (tɕʰ-), pj- (pʰl-), kj- (kʰl-),
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
ricefield;
river
crutch; to
leech; rope;
tired (rest);
hand;
(wharf);
chew;
blood
bird; ant
buffalo
father
horse
(male);
n-, m-, ŋ-, l-, w-, j-, s- (ɬ-), f-, h-
dry
…
farmland
Initials from Proto-voiced
A1/2
B1/2
C1/2 rice;
DL1/2
DS1/2
Aspirations & Breathiness:
ear; to
bean; to
person
to hit the
to bite; ten;
arrive;
ride on; to
(CLF of
mark
cooked (ripe)
rightside
pull down
person);
(correct)
pʰ-/p-, f- (pʰ-)/p-, tʰ-/t-, kʰ-/k-,
kʰ-/h-, h- (kʰj-)/r-, s- (ɬ-, θ-)/
ɕ-…
bowl
18
For illustrating this revised Tai tone box, Liao and Shen (2012) separately enumerate
the further tonal splits found in CT and NT as follows. The following Table 8 firstly
displays a split inside Row 1 of Column A in several Central Tai varieties.
Table 8 A tonal split in voiceless friction sounds in Tone A5
Initial Type
Protoinitials
*f-
The 1st row
*th-
SWT
CT
Debao
Thai
Dalong
Wenma
Gloss
Bac Va
pʰɔ:n³¹
24
fon
tʰɛ:w24
tʰe:w³¹
in Gedney’s
*kh-
kʰa:
kʰa:
tone box:
*h-
ha:w24
ha:w³¹
voiceless
*hm-
ma:24
ma:53
4
53
24
31
rain
column
kʰɔ:⁴¹
1
kha:
leg
yawn
mɔ:²¹
ma:2
dog
friction
*hn-
na:²
sounds
*hŋ-
ŋa:j24
ŋa:j53
supine
*hl-
la:j24
la:j53
many
*s-
sɔ:ŋ
θo:ŋ
na:
24
thick
53
two
As in Table 8, one of the most noticeable problems with Row 1 in Gedney’s tone box
is a split among the voiceless friction sounds (Row 1) in several CT varieties, which
separates two series of initials with different phonetic features. These two series are
voiceless aspirated sounds (including proto-voiceless aspirated stops and *h-, *x-, *f-,
and etc.) in shaded, and voiceless continuants (*hm-, *hn-, *hɲ-, *hŋ-, *hw-, *hj-, *hl-,
and *s-).
Liao and Shen (2012) have pointed out the split among voiceless friction sounds in
Gedney’s tone box is clear as shown in Table 8, and have proposed a tone box with
four rows of the tonal categories associated with proto-voiceless initials instead of
three. They furthermore point out that actually Li (1966) has noted that there
should be four tonal categories associated with voiceless initials from Proto-Tai,
based on the evidence of tonal development in T’ien-pao (nowadays Debao Zhuang
or Debao variety of Yang Zhuang in ISO 639-3): 1) voiceless unaspirated stops, 2)
voiceless continuants, 3) glottalized consonants, and 4) voiceless aspirated stops. Li
once again emphasizes “the Proto-Tai initials may be roughly divided into five
groups according to the influence they may have on tone” (Li 1977: 43) and lists 1)
5
The sources of the tone data in the Table 8 are as follows, Thai and Debao Dalong variety of Yang
Zhuang are from my own research, Wenma which is of a Dai Zhuang (CT) variety in Yunnan is from
Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997), and Bac Va which is a CT variety in Vietnam is from Hudak (2008).
19
voiceless aspirated stops, 2) voiceless continuants, 3) voiceless unaspirated stops, 4)
glottalized consonants, and 5) voiced consonants. Gedney’s tone box does not follow
Li’s proposal of four groups of voiceless initials, but divides voiceless initials into
only three rows, merging voiceless aspirated stops and voiceless continuants into
‘voiceless friction’. In Table 8, we have seen the evidence supporting Li’s proposal of
four groups of voiceless initials over Gedney’s three. As in the following Table 8,
Liao and Shen (2012) separate the first row of Gedney’s tone box to two rows
naming ‘voiceless aspirations’ (new Row 1) and ‘voiceless continuants’ (new Row 2)
in their revised Tai tone box.
In addition to the split above in Row 1 of Gedney’s tone box, Liao and Shen (2012)
also point out there is another problem with this row. In their additional row
“voiceless aspirations” in CT and SWT varieties are actually lost the aspiration in NT
varieties (modern NT varieties normally do not have aspirated sounds), and this
series of initials also condition splits into two tonal series in NT varieties. One of
these two tonal series always merges with the tone with voiced initials which is of
the fourth row in Gedney’s tone box, in contrast this merger is never found in CT
and SWT varieties. This series of initial is called “voicing alternation” by some
linguists, such as Diller points out the so-called “voicing alternation” of proto-
voiceless and proto-voiced among different Tai languages (Diller 1998: 7), which is
a series of initials behaving an original low tone in NT languages but behaving an
original high tone in both SWT and CT languages. The following Tables 9 and 10 use
examples filled in Columns A and C of Gedney’s tone box from SWT, CT, and NT
varieties respectively to show that such tonal splits and mergers are only found in
NT varieties6. Note that in Tables 9 and Table 10, shaded areas refer to examples of
tonal categories with proto-voiced initials and those split from tonal categories with
proto-voiceless initials to merge with tonal values of voiced series.
6
The sources of the data in these two tables are as following: Siamese and Jingxi urban variety of
Yang Zhuang (Jingxi for short in the tables) from my own research, and Donglan from Zhang et al.
(1999). The proto-initials are adapted from Li (1977) and Liang & Zhang (1996).
20
Table 9 Comparison of aspirations and voiced sounds among SWT, CT and NT
in Column A
Examples of tonal splits and mergers
Column A
in Gedney’s tone box
Gloss
*x-
white
*kh-
to sell
Aspirations
*th-
in addition
in Gedney’s
*ɢɦ-
bitter
Row 1
*gɦ-
*dɦ-
son-in-law
SWT
CT
NT
Thai
Jingxi
Donglan
kʰa:w²⁴
kʰa:w⁵³
ha:w⁵³
kʰa:j²⁴
tʰɛ:m²⁴
kʰom²⁴
kʰə:j²⁴
reach
*bɦ-
tʰɨŋ²⁴
sharpen
fon²⁴
Voiced in
*b-
expensive
Gedney’s Row
pʰɛ:ŋ³³
*d-
paint
4
*g-
stuck
tʰa:³³
kʰa:j⁵³
tʰe:m⁵³
kʰam⁵³
ka:j⁵³
tem⁵³
ham²³¹
kʰy:⁵³
kɯ:j²³¹
pʰɔ:n⁵³
pan²³¹
pe:ŋ³¹
peŋ²³¹
ta:³¹
ta:²³¹
ka:³¹
ka:²³¹
tʰaŋ⁵³
kʰa:³³
taŋ²³¹
Table 10 Comparison of aspirations and voiced sounds among SWT, CT and NT
in Column C
Examples of tonal splits and mergers
Column C
in Gedney’s tone box
*xAspirations
in Gedney’s
Row 1
Voiced in
Gedney’s Row
4
Gloss
enter
*kh-
kill
*ph-
sweep
*ɣɦ-
*dɦ-
rice
bowl
*bɦ-
person
*b-
bride7
*d*g-
SWT
CT
Thai
Jingxi
kʰaw⁴¹
kʰa:⁴¹
pʰɛ:w⁴¹
kʰa:w⁴¹
tʰuaj⁴¹
NT
kʰaw
33
kʰa:
33
haw³³
ka:³³
pʰe:w33
pe:w³³
33
haw¹⁴
33
tu:j¹⁴
kʰaw
tʰu:j
pʰu:⁴¹
pʰow
stomach
tʰɔ:ŋ⁴⁵³
to chew
kʰiaw⁴⁵³
to:ŋ
(sa)pʰaj⁴⁵³
Donglan
33
pɔ:j33
33
33
ke:w
pu:¹⁴
paɰ¹⁴
tuŋ¹⁴
ke:w¹⁴
Liao and Shen (2012) point out that there is no split inside the aspirations in Thai
(SWT) and Jingxi (CT), while there is a split in Donglan (NT). Actually all described
NT varieties exhibit the same split as in Donglan. That is to say, in the vast NT
varieties, one of the two series of “proto-aspirated sounds” (Li 1977) always presents
with the same tone as that found in Gedney’s Row 4 (the voiced sounds). This series
of proto-initials is made up entirely of voiceless stops with a voiced aspiration (ɦ-) in
Li Fang-Kuei’s reconstruction (Li 1989), such as *kɦ-, *tɦ-, and *pɦ-, while it is made
7
In SWT varieties, this etymon normally refers to ‘daughter-in-law’.
21
up entirely of voiced stops with a voiced aspiration (ɦ-) in Liang and Zhang’s
reconstruction (1996), such as *ɢɦ-, *gɦ-, *dɦ-, and *bɦ-. The later one is adopted in
this thesis due to its more reasonable collocation of “voiced stop + voiced fricative”
than “voiceless stop + voiced fricative”. Obviously the tonal development of protovoiced aspirated sounds have gone along two different paths of development in the
tone box: in CT-SWT varieties, they maintain the aspiration but lost the voiced
feature, and merge into voiceless aspirated sounds (Row 1) in the tone box; while in
modern NT varieties, they keep the voiced feature but lost the aspiration, and merge
into voiced sounds (Gedney’s Row 4) in the tone box.
Liao and Shen (2012) furthermore indicate that Gedney had actually earlier
reconstructed “a series of six additional initial consonants for Proto-Tai, three stops
and three spirants, which would have to form a fifth category in the chart of initial
consonant types, contiguous with the fourth category, the voiced sounds, because in
Tai languages of the Northern branch they behaved like voiced initials, and also
with the first category, the voiceless friction sounds, because in all other Tai
languages they behaved like these, so that our chart becomes a closed loop, just as
in the color spectrum infra-red at one end meets ultra-violet at the other” (Gedney
1985: 120). And, furthermore, Gedney also demonstrates the evidence for this series
of voiced initial in Proto-Tai, as *B, *D, *G, *V, *Z and *ɣ- (Gedney 1989). However,
he does not make any addition to the tone box at the end of the discussion.
Though Gedney’s and Li’s reconstructions differ in the details, it is conceivable that
there is a series of special voiced sounds (voiced aspirated sounds in Li and another
series of voiced initials in Gedney), which condition the different tonal
developments between common aspirated sounds and common voiced sounds. In
this sense, the “tonal split” of aspirations reflecting in NT languages is actually not
an “split” anymore, but is a merger from another source – in NT varieties, it merges
with voiced sounds, but in CT/SWT varieties it merges with voiceless aspirations.
Liao and Shen’s (2012) solution for dealing with the two series of aspirated sounds
(the aspirated feature only reflects in CT and SWT) is to divide them into two rows.
They call these two rows “voiceless aspirations” (new Row 1 including proto-
voiceless aspirated stops *ph-, *phl/r-, *th-, *kh-, *khl/r-, *khw-, *tɕʰ-, etc. and other
spread sounds such as *h-, *x-, *f-, etc.), and “voiced aspirations and breathies (new
Row 7 including proto-initial sounds reconstructed by them as *kɦ-, *tɦ-, *pɦ-, *s̤-,
*f̤-, *ɦ-, and *r̤-), and sort the latter to be the last row – Row 7 under the row of
“voiced sounds” (new Row 6 in Liao & Shen 2012) in their revised Tai tone box as
22
the following Table 11. This order first makes the new Row 7 “voiced aspirations
and breathiness” associate with the new Row 6 “voiced sounds” reflecting in NT
varieties, and then also links the new Row 7 to the new Row 1 when the chart
becomes a “closed loop” called by Gedney (Gedney 1985: 120), for keeping the
single ‘contiguous’ tone box or a fixed order phonological spectrum (Gedney 1985:
120). This treatment is shown in Table 11.
Table 11 Two new rows splitting from primary aspirations (adapted from Liao
& Shen 2012)
Initials
Initial type
Initial feature
Proto-
1. Voiceless
voiceless
aspirations
Protobreathiness
Tonal category
A
B
C
DL
DS
A1
B1
C1
DL1
DS1
A1/A2
B1/B2
C1/C2
DL1/DL2
DS1/DS2
7. Voiced
aspirations &
breathies
In order to designate tone category, Liao and Shen (2012) follow Li’s pattern to use
1 and 2 only as indicators of proto-tone, since in different Tai varieties they exhibit
different vocalic features. Therefore, they cannot designate the voiced aspirations as
either proto-voiceless sounds or voiced sounds, but instead they designate a new
name “proto-breathy” for this special category, since the proto segment*ɦ-, common
to all of them, carries breathiness, and they have determined there were non-stop
breathy sounds (*s̤-, *f̤-, *ɦ-, and *r̤-) in the same row, too.
Besides the noticeable tonal splits or mergers in Row 1 in Gedney’s tone box, Liao
and Shen (2012) also discuss a split in Row 2 of Gedney’s tone box. They point out
that Johnson (2011: 30, 36-38) has proposed the split in Row 2 which is voiceless
unaspirated stops in Gedney’s tone box, by listing only two examples (‘to die’ and
‘eye’ only in A tone) from three Dai Zhuang (of CT) varieties to demonstrate this
split. Liao and Shen (2012) furthermore list more examples to illustrate the split, as
in the following Tables 12 and 138. The split demonstrated in Table 12 and Table 13
is dropped shadows. Row 2 of Gedney’s tone box refers to proto-unaspirated stops.
However, some items like the shaded items in Tables 12 and 13 which are put into
Gedney’s Row 2 indeed present as unaspirated stop initials in all modern varieties of
SWT and NT, but present as aspirated stop initials or h- in all modern CT varieties.
8
In Tables 12 and 13, the proto-initials *tr- and *pr- are adopted from Li (1977), and *p.t- and
*p.r- are adopted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009). Ma’ai is a Yang Zhuang variety.
23
Table 12 Comparison of unaspirated stops among Tai varieties in Column A
(adapted from Liao & Shen 2012)
Tone box categories
Unaspirated stops in Row 2
Examples of tones
Gloss
of Column A
CT
NT
Thai
Ma’ai
Po-ai
tʰa:³¹
ta:²⁴
to die
ta:j³³
eye
ta:³³
to go
paj³³
*t-
door
21
(praʔ ) tu:³³
*k-
I
ku:³³
*tr- or *p.tProto-initials
SWT
*p-
tʰa:j³¹
paj⁵³
tow⁵³
kaw⁵³
ta:j²⁴
paj²⁴
tu:²⁴
ku:²⁴
Table 13 Comparison of unaspirated stops among Tai varieties in Column DL
(adapted from Liao & Shen 2012)
Tone box categories
Unaspirated stops in Row 2
Examples of tones
Gloss
of Column DL1
CT
NT
Thai
Ma’ai
Po-ai
to dry
ta:k²¹
break
tɛ:k²¹
*p-
mouth
pa:k²¹
*t-
answer
tɔ:p²¹
*k-
to hug
*pr- or *p.rProto-initials
SWT
kɔ:t²¹
tʰa:k³³
tʰe:k³³
pa:k⁵⁵
ta:p⁵⁵
ko:t⁵⁵
ta:k²²
te:k²²
pa:k²²
ta:p²²
ko:t²²
The initials of this series of items has been reconstructed by Li (1977: 86-87, 118119, 225) as a cluster consisting of an unaspirated stop (p-, t-, k-) followed by -r-,
and in all CT varieties, the proto-unaspirated stop is aspirated on account of the
drop the following -r-. In contrast, in Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) which considers
that there were no aspirated stops in PT, this series of aspirated stops in CT varieties
develop from so-called “sesquisyllabic clusters” (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 5961), such as *p.t- and *p.r- in the two tables above. No matter whether there were
aspirated stops in PT or not (we will discuss this in §4.2.1 since it is crucial to the
tonal development of Tai languages), this aspiration in Gedney’s Row 2 causes its
tonal development to behave the same as other voiceless aspirated initials in Row 1.
This split is not easily perceived due to the relative rarity of examples of this split’s
conditioning factors, and only several CT varieties such as most Dai Zhuang varieties
and some varieties of Yang Zhuang always present this split – the tonal value of the
aspirated stops is different from those unaspirated stops. That is, other CT varieties
do not present this split because tones conditioned by voiceless aspirated stops and
voiceless unaspirated stops are the same.
24
From the discussion above, we see that some CT varieties need further refinement in
Gedney’s Row 2. Similar to the management of dividing Gedney’s Row 1 to new
Rows 1 and 2, and furthermore new Row 1 to new Row 7, Liao and Shen’s (2012)
solution for managing two series of voiceless unaspirated sounds is to divide it into
two new rows. They adapt Li’s (1977) reconstruction of proto-initials in this row and
call these two new rows – the new Row 3 being voiceless unaspirated stops
(including Li’s proto-voiceless unaspirated plosives and affricates), and the new Row
4 being “voiceless unaspirated stop + *r clusters” (*kr-, *pr-, and *tr-), which are
shown in the following Table 14.
Table 14 Two new rows splitting from Row 2 in Gedney’s tone box (adapted
from Liao & Shen 2012)
Tonal categories
Proto-initials
A
B
C
DL
DS
A1
B1
C1
DL1
DS1
3. Voiceless unaspirated stops
4. Voiceless unaspirated stops + *r
As the result of the illustration of tonal splits found in CT and NT varieties above,
the revised tone box has been suggested by Liao and Shen as shown in Table 7. On
my fieldwork research of this thesis, the practice has proved that this revised Tai
tone box provides a more suitable framework as a research tool when researching
the tones in CT and NT varieties. However, they have not been involved in the
classification of Tai languages by their discovery on CT and NT tones. Moreover,
there are also some exceptions of the cases of tonal irregularities lacking reasonable
explanation among the three sub-branches of the Tai or even among different
varieties of Tai languages, and the revised tone box still has limitations in its ability
to describe these phenomena, as which are shown in the last part of the discussion
of the following §1.2.3. All these unsolved issues are involved in the objectives of
the research in this thesis.
When centering on the objectives of this thesis and integrating them with the related
discussions in §1.2.1 and §1.2.2, there are several outstanding problems apparent in
the literature.
First of all, the tonal development needs a clearer frame to explain its primary and
secondary sequences in each Tai variety. Although Gedney’s tone box (1989[1972])
and the revised tone box found in Liao & Shen (2012) differ from the division of the
25
horizontal rows on account of their different initial grouping, the two boxes show
the same synchronic division of the initials at times of tonal splits. That is to say,
the two tone boxes do not describe the historical development of these tonal splits,
such as two-way or three-way splits, and which ones occur initially and which ones
are secondary splits. Liao & Shen’s revised tone box firstly divides proto-initials into
three groups – voiceless, voiced and breathy, and then divides proto-voiceless one
into five sub-groups based on the evidence of tonal splits in NT and CT varieties.
These two levels of grouping the proto-initials show a hint for the sequence of tonal
development especially those in voiceless sounds, but still do not provide a proper
diachronic scheme.
One of the most noticeable problems for grouping the proto-initials which
conditioned tonal splits in history is whether the aspirated sounds should be put into
that early stage of PT. As in the aforementioned discussion of the tonal split in
Gedney’s row of “unaspirated stops” accompanying aspiration in a number of CT
languages in §1.2.2, aspiration has been viewed as contrastive in PT in Li FangKuei’s reconstruction (Li 1977), which is widely accepted by Tai scholars. However,
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 88-89) argues that PT lacked contrastive aspirations
and the aspirated stops in modern CT and SWT languages developed mainly from
clusters with medial *-r- as post-PT innovations. He mentions that Haudricourt
(1963: 157) and Ferblus (1990) claim that aspirated sounds were of secondary
development in SWT and CT varieties, and Liang and Zhang (1993, 1996) believe
voiceless aspirated initials developed independently in different Tai varieties from
various initial consonant clusters, such as *pr-, *tl-, *tr-, *pw-, *xp-, xpl-, and *xt-.
Despite their different proposal on the specific origin of the rise and the
development of voiceless aspirated initials in Tai languages, if voiceless aspiration
truly secondarily developed in SWT and CT varieties, the treatment of the tonal split
conditioned by voiceless aspirations in those varieties should be adjusted to be a
post-PT development, but not a condition by “proto-initial”.
In a similar way, although there is no dispute that there were pre-glottalized initial
consonants in PT, Liang and Zhang (1996: 817-828) and Zhang and colleagues
(1999: 243-245) suggest the tonal splits conditioned by this series of initials were
secondary developments in a number of Tai varieties since they have various tonal
behaviors that differ from each other. In contrast, in the remaining Tai varieties this
series of initials uniformly present the same tonal behaviors with other voiceless
initials, and this is why Li Fang Kuei and some scholars treat pre-glottalized initials
as a sub-group among voiceless initials. The implication is if Liang and Zhang’s
26
proposal can be proved to be true, although pre-glottalized initial consonants may
exist in PT, they might have the same tonal behavior with voiceless initial
consonants at the earliest time of tonal splits, and then conditioned tonal re-split
later independently in different Tai varieties. The treatment of the tonal split
conditioned by pre-glottalized initial consonants in those Tai varieties should be
adjusted to be a post-PT development.
On the contrary, it appears that aforementioned “proto-breathy” sounds (Liao &
Shen 2012) which conditions the so-called “voicing alternation” (Gedney 1989a,
Diller 1998: 7, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 77) have conditioned primary tonal
splits at the earliest division of the chronological stages of post-PT. Although only
Liao and Shen (2012) explicitly provide a special initial group named “proto-
breathy” sounds, this series of initials was said to exhibit “voicing alternation” by
some linguists. For example, Diller points out the “voicing alternation” of proto-
voiceless and proto-voiced among different Tai languages (Diller 1998: 7), which is
a series of initials behaving like an original low tone in NT languages but behaving
like an original high tone in both SWT and CT languages. In Li’s hypothesis, the
reconstruction of this series of proto-initials is made up entirely of stops with a
voiced aspiration (ɦ), such as *pɦ-, *tɦ-, *kɦ- (Li 1989). Obviously the tonal
development of proto-voiced aspirated sounds has gone along two different paths of
development in the tone box: in CT-SWT varieties, they maintain the aspiration and
merge into voiceless aspirated sounds; while in modern NT varieties, they keep the
voiced feature and merge into voiced sounds. This is very similar to Gedney’s (1985:
120) hypothesis of “a series of six additional initial consonants for Proto-Tai, three
stops and three spirants” and furthermore his reconstruction of a series of voiced
initial in Proto-Tai, as *B, *D, *G, *V, *Z and *ɣ (Gedney 1989). Furthermore, Zhang
and colleagues (1999: 174) also modify this series of stops causing voicing
alternation as voiced aspirated stops *bɦ-, *dɦ-, and *gɦ-, which behave original low
tones (tones developing from proto-voiced initials) in NT but original high tones
(tones developing from proto-voiceless initials) in CT and SWT. Although Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009: 77-88) argues that so-called voicing alternation is not a unified
phenomenon in PT, but consists of post-PT borrowings from Chinese which must not
be reconstructed for PT, and positing so-called sesquisyllabic clusters. For
interpreting this series of voicing alternation, Liao and Shen (2012) propose there
was a series of proto-breathy initials out of the dichotomy of proto-voiced and protovoiceless initials, and have added a new row into the revised tone box for this series
of initials (Liao & Shen 2012: 9-12). That is to say, initials in PT may be divided into
a new trichotomy: proto-voiceless, proto-voiced, and proto-breathy. These “proto-
27
breathy” initials regularly condition tonal split going with original low tone with
those developed from proto-voiced initials in all NT dialects, but condition tonal
split going with original high tone with those developed from proto-voiceless initials
in the vast majority of CT and SWT. If this series of “proto-breathy sounds” really
existed in history, they should be treated as a group of sounds which should be put
into the early stage of PT.
The second outstanding question is whether the tonology of Tai languages can
become evidence to support any of the theories of Tai classification either at the
time of the formation of different Tai branches (or the early stage of post-PT) or at
the stage of modern Tai languages. The tonology criterion of Tai division is
complicated when being presented by different linguists. Some scholars such as
Chamberlain (1975), and Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997) have proposed that the
different tonal splits types can be seen as an important criterion of the classification
of Tai branches (cf. §2.2.4). There is one thing that has been clarified earlier in this
section is some tonal development conditioned by some types of initials in different
Tai varieties may occur much later than the time of tonal split in PT, such as preglottalization and aspiration might condition secondary tonal splits like original high
tones going with low tones observed by Edmondson (1994: 166-176). Then at least
these secondary tonal splits should be reliably treated as a basis of Tai classification
at variety level. If pre-glottalization and aspiration did condition secondary tonal
splits in some specific Tai varieties, these kinds of tonal development should not be
treated as a distinctive feature of classifying the main branches of Tai, since they
occur far later than the time of the formation of different Tai branches, which are
NT-CT/SWT being two plus one taxonomy (Gedney 1989) or NT-CT-SWT being the
tripartite division (Li 1977). That is to say, at least some tonological phenomena
such as tonal splits conditioned by pre-glottalized and aspirated initials should not
be treated as evidence to support the Tai primary classification at the early stage of
post-PT.
However, this clarification does not mean that all the tonological phenomena of Tai
varieties cannot be treated as distinctive features of the classification of the primary
Tai branches. Since the tonal split of aforementioned “voicing alternation”
conditioned by so-called “proto-breathy” sounds generally conditions different
direction of tonal split between NT and CT/SWT, and it is supposed to be treated as
a primary tonal development as in the aforementioned discussion, it should become
one of the distinctive features which can determine the primary Tai branches. In the
subsequent discussion, more phonological and lexical evidence will be referenced to
28
support this hypothesis. Since this hypothesis directly supports Gedney’s (1989b)
two plus one taxonomy division – Li’s CT sub-branch and SWT sub-branch forming a
sub-group of which the NT sub-branch is a sister, and Gedney (1989a: 231) also uses
the term Southern Tai (hereafter ST) to refer to all the non-NT languages, it is
proposed that before CT and SWT divided to two different groups, they were of the
same proto-CT/SWT group. I adapt Gedney’s terminology to call this group ProtoSouthern Tai (hereafter PST) based on its geographical distribution relatively to
Proto-NT (hereafter PNT). This division of PNT and PST is very similar to the twoterm system that Haudricourt (1956) insisted on, to divide the PT first into Dioi or
ʔyai (corresponding approximately to NT) vs. Tai proper (corresponding to ST). As a
matter of convenience, in this thesis North-South division is used to refer to this
possible historical Tai primary classification on the early post-PT stage.
The third outstanding problem is whether we can provide a more refined Tai tone
box to be a supplement to Gedney’s tone box and Liao and Shen’s revised tone box,
and to be able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties. As in the
discussion in §1.2.2, Gedney’s tone box can be treated as a suitable research tool
when studying SWT varieties, but when working with CT and NT varieties, it has
several shortcomings, and Liao and Shen (2012) has designed a revised tone box
which aims at providing a more suitable framework as a research tool when
studying Tai varieties out of SWT. However, they also admit the revised tone box
still has limitations in its ability to describe every Tai language since there are cases
of tones lacking regular correspondence prescribed by their tone box among
different Tai varieties. For example, the word for ‘become; to be’ presents with a
proto-voiceless initial in SWT (e.g. *penA1-U > pen³³ in Thai), but with a protovoiced initial in both CT and NT (e.g. *benA2 > pan³¹ in Debao Yang Zhuang and
*ben A2 > pan³¹ in Wuming Zhuang), raising questions about Column A (Liao &
Shen 2012). So far the conditions of such irregular tonal correspondence still lack
reasonable explanation. If there are more potential rows of initials conditioning
these seemingly irregular tonal behaviors, they should be added to Liao and Shen’s
revised tone box for being able to capture these kinds of tonal distinctions. Actually
Gedney (1989b: 25-33, 1989[1972]: 204) himself has alluded to this issue based on
the tonal split on Box 1 of Column A of some varieties of CT an NT, and has pointed
out that when the historical development of tones in NT is fully understood, more
horizontal rows may have to be drawn in his Tai tone box (cf. §1.2.2). From this
standpoint, to draw more lines or even columns to the tone box is not a problem if it
is necessary. Based on the study of tonal development of Tai varieties in this thesis, I
29
will test whether a more accurate Tai tone box can be designed to reasonably
explain these unsolved issues.
In relation to the objectives outlined in §1.2 and discussed in §1.2.3, the following
hypotheses are proposed:
1) After tone arose, three groups of proto-initials (voiceless-voiced-breathy)
conditioned primary tonal splits whereas splits conditioned by other pharyngeal
features, such as aspiration and pre-glottalization represent stages of secondary
development.
2) The “Two plus one taxonomy” of Tai classification is more reasonable than the
Tripartite Division at least in the early stage of Tai languages, because of
different tonal behaviors of “proto-breathy” initials in Proto-Northern and ProtoSouthern Tai languages.
3) The proposed tone box will be able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai
varieties.
In relation to the background and the objectives of this thesis, the following sections
are expanded for giving a brief overview to this study.
The selection of language varieties from SWT, CT and NT are based on my own
knowledge and my library research of tonal diversity of each Tai sub-group, for
striving to cover all types of tonal splits in Tai. Tone data from the following fortytwo Tai varieties are used to determine the specific tonal developments:
1) Seven SWT varieties: Bangkok Thai (L1), Songkhla Southern Thai (L2), Khon
Kaen Isan (L3), Chiang Rai Northern Thai (L4), Kho Lam Shan (L5), Rong Maet
Tai Lue (L6), and Muong Quan Son Tai (L7). All the data of these languages have
also been collected by me. Some published materials of the fully researched
varieties such as Thai, Southern Thai, and Northern Thai are also referenced to.
2) Seventeen CT varieties: Six Yang Zhuang varieties including Debao urban (L8),
Debao Dalong (L9), Debao Lüliu (L10), Jingxi Hurun (L11), Jingxi urban (L12),
30
and Jingxi Anning (L13); Debao Nalong Myang Zhuang (L14); Debao Tuoxin
Pyang Zhuang (L15); Jingxi Huashan Min Zhuang (L16); as well as four Zuojiang
Zhuang varieties including Tiandeng Xiangdu (L17), Daxin Naling (L18),
Lingding Zuozhou (L19), and Daxin Baoxu (L23) which are collected by myself,
but some of them are also compared with data of the same varieties in published
resources. Xiaoguangnan Nong Zhuang (L20), Wenshan Dazhai Dai Zhuang
(L21), Daxin Leiping Zuojiang Zhuang (L22), and Bac Va Nung (L24) are referred
to published resources.
3) Thirteen NT varieties: Debao Longsang (undescribed L31), Debao Dongling (of
Youjiang Zhuang, L32), Shanglin Yunling (of Central Hongshuihe Zhuang, L34),
Du’an Jiaren (of Central Hongshuihe Zhuang, L35), Yizhou Suogan (of Guibei
Zhuang, L36), Huishui Bouyei (L41), and Zhenning Bouyei (L42) are collected by
myself, some of them are also compared with the data of the same languages in
published resources. Wuming Shuangqiao (of Yongbei Zhuang, L30), Tianlin
Lizhou (of Guibian Zhuang, L33), Liujiang Baipeng (of Liujiang Zhuang, L37),
Donglan Urban (of Guibei Zhuang, L38), Qiubei Gehan (of Qiubei Zhuang, L39),
and Lianshan Xiaosanjiang (of Lianshan Zhuang, L40) are referred to published
resources.
4) Five varieties of YN whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate: Yongning
Baiji (of Sanxiang YN, L25), Nanning Shuangding (L26), and Jingxi Daqiu Nung
An (L28) are collected by myself, and Sanxiang and Nung An are also compared
with the data of the same languages in published resources; Long’an Xiaolin
(L27) and Yongning Xialeng (L29) are referred to published resources.
The language names are cross-referred in §3.3.1 and Appendix C, a chart of the
information of each language is also attached in Appendix C, and the map of the Tai
varieties investigated in this thesis is shown in Appendix D. The numbers of the Tai
varieties have the same ordering in this map.
The tone data selected are analyzed by Liao and Shen’s (2012) revised tone box
which is revised from Gedney’s (1989[1972]) tone box, and an overview of tones in
each Tai variety is given. More attention will be given to the analysis of the tonal
development in history and trends of tonal development in the current situation in
different Tai varieties. Language data from varieties from CT and NT languages have
been collected in Debao, Jingxi, Napo Counties, and Nanning City of the Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, and Guangzhou of Guangdong Province of China via
three times of fieldwork there. Transcription, translation and checking were done in
31
Guangxi and Chiang Mai. In addition, data of Thai, Northern Thai, Shan and Tai Lue
were collected in Thailand. Tone data from another SWT language spoken in Thanh
Hoa Province in Central Vietnam has been collected in the U.S. More language data
will be referenced from published materials. The detail of data collection is crossreferred in §3.3.1 and is also listed in Appendix D.
Sometimes the data collected from varieties selected are also limited because of
several reasons. First, the Tai language branch is distributed widely in South China
and MSEA, and has numerous varieties. It is impossible to work out a full-scale data
collection of tones in a short time. Some special issues related to the tonal
development may be hidden in those undescribed or unstudied varieties. Second,
some data selected from published sources may be imprecise, and may give the
study a negative result. This kind of issue has been found a lot in the previous
readings. This is unavoidable and needs further research to remedy in the future.
Furthermore, the wordlist itself also has limitations. The wordlist designated is only
limited to cognates including Old Chinese loans inherited from PT, but is excluded
to those modern loans from other languages in each variety, although such modern
loans like English loans in Thai and modern Mandarin loans in Yang Zhuang
varieties have been observed to cause some changes on tonal behavior. Besides, the
wordlist also conditions selected data to be limited to speech environments. That is,
I could only acquire the tone data from single words when the language resource
person (LRP) read and repeated them, but could not observe the tone expression
from the natural conversation. That means some tone changes (in natural speech)
such as tone sandhi might be missed. Since tone changes in natural conversation
especially tone sandhi may be accounted to be the newest part of tonal
development, the limitation of lacking tone data from this occasion is regrettable but
unmodifiable because of the limited fieldwork condition. For remedying these
regrets, in §6.6 I also analyze tonal behavior changes due to Mandarin loans in
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (L8) and Thai loans in Tai Lue (L6) as additional
examples of tone changes out of the tone expressing occasions limited by the
wordlist.
In relation to the hypotheses of the research in §1.2.4, the following contributions
are expected:
32
1. This study will contribute to Tai linguistics by providing more detailed research
on tonal diversities of Tai languages, especially those varieties beyond SWT.
2. By providing a more refined system of describing tone systems of Tai varieties, it
will provide for a more comprehensive inventory of tonal split patterns that can
be used as criteria for subdivision of the Tai language family.
3. A new tone box applied to the vast majority of Tai varieties will solve some of
the shortcomings of Gedney’s (1989[1972]) tone box and Liao & Shen’s (2012)
revised tone box.
The remainder of this thesis consists of eight chapters. Chapter 2 presents a brief
review of the literature relating to historical and comparative Tai and Tai tonology,
including less well-known Chinese materials of Tai tone research. Chapter 3
describes the theoretical foundations of methodology used in this thesis, the
fieldwork relating to the research methods, as well as notation for this thesis.
Chapters 4, 5 and 6 deal with the discussions for establishing the three hypotheses of
this thesis, namely diachronic hierarchies of Tai tonal development, Tai
classification based on tonology, and a comprehensive new Tai tone box
respectively. Chapter 7 as the last chapter provides the summarized conclusion of
this thesis, as well as suggestions for further studies of Tai tones.
33
Literature Review
This chapter presents a review of the literature topics relating to the subjects of the
thesis, namely tonology of Tai, Tai classification based on tonology, and Tai tone
boxes. In order to put these topics in the broader context, §2.1 and its subsections
give brief reviews of the literature relating to historical and comparative linguistics,
providing theoretical foundations to this study. §2.2 describes the previous works on
tonology of languages in China and MSEA by dividing the section into four subsections, namely tonogenesis, tonal splits and mergers, the conditioning factors of
tonal splits, and Tai classification related to tonological issues. Within these subsections, sub-sections §2.2.1 and §2.2.2 are both related to the processes of tonal
development; §2.2.3 reviews the previous works on the method of analyzing tonal
splits based on the conditional factors, since this will help us to explore a new Tai
tone box which will capture more tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties than the Tai
tone boxes proposed by Gedney (1989[1972]) and Liao and Shen (2012); §2.2.4
gives a brief review of Tai classification based on the issues related to tonology, and
this will give clues for testing our “Two plus one taxonomy” of Tai classification
which is proposed to be more reasonable than the Tripartite Division at least in the
early stage of Tai languages.
Because the topic of this thesis – tonal development of Tai languages is based on Tai
tonology, which is mainly due to the comparative method applying to Tai tones, the
theory of historical and comparative linguistics is bound to become the basis of the
research tool of this thesis. Historical linguistics including comparative methods has
been becoming an important linguistic subject for a long time since it first developed
in the 19th century for studying the historical linguistic changes among IndoEuropean languages, hence there are many precedents in historical linguistics like
Hoenigswald (1960), Arlotto (1972), Lass (1997), Crowley (1994), and Campbell
(2004), and to a large extent Jeffers & Lehiste (1982) and Hock (1991), and in other
areas as well, e.g. Harris (1960) and Lyons (1977). In the Sinitic Circle, Historical
34
Linguistics by Xu (1991) is a summative work with reference value. The following
respects of previous works on historical linguistics, comparative methods, and
historical and comparative Tai are referenced to in this thesis.
Historical and comparative method applying to Tai languages has been occupying a
significant part in the Tai linguistics. Within these achievements, Li Fang Kuei’s (Li
1977) articles on Tai linguistics and his remarkable work A handbook of Comparative
Tai, Gedney’s (1989[1972]) tone box and his other historical and comparative Tai
linguistics works, and other works by other Tai linguists have provided abundant
materials for subsequent research. In this section, the discussions will focus on some
Tai linguistics topics related to linguistic change, comparative method, language
classification, and areal linguistics except the content related to tonology which will
be discussed in the subsequent sub-sections of §2.2.
In previous studies, there are some specific cases related to linguistic changes in Tai
languages as in the following examples.
The discussions of the reconstruction of the Tai word ‘maternal grandmother’ have
given a characteristic example for sound changes in Tai languages. This Tai word
presents an irregularity on its modern forms ta:jB1 in most NT varieties, ta:jA1 in most
CT varieties, na:jA2 in most SWT varieties, and ja:jA2 in Thai (Li 1971). Either the
initial consonants or the tones of this word among three Tai sub-branches are
different. Li (1971) and Liao’s (2013) opposite reconstructions of this word in PT,
namely *na:jA2 and *ta:jA respectively, containing the contents of irregular sound
change especially analogical change and a specific factor of sound change –
“avoidance of pernicious homophony” – a term used by Campbell (2004: 322).
Different forms of the aberrant development of this word within Tai sub-branches
have been reflecting the dialectologists’ slogan ‘each word has its own history’9
attributed to Jules Gillíron (Campbell 2004: 212).
The dialectologists’ slogan reflects the thinking of Johannes Schmidt’s ‘wave theory’ (1872),
which deals with linguistic changes involving in language contact. Based on Schmidt’s wave model,
linguistic changes spread outward concentrically like waves, and farther to the central point of the
change the weaker influence of the change is presented. Therefore, there is no sharp boundaries
between neighboring dialects or languages. According to this theory, the dialectologists’ slogan
actually reflects that “a word’s history might be the result of various influences from various
directions, and these might be quite different from those involved in another word’s history, hence
each word has its own history”. (Campbell 2004: 213)
9
35
The historical linguistic change of a numeral word nɨŋB1 ‘one’ in Thai discussed by Li
(1987) is an illustrative example for indicating syntactic change in Tai languages.
Different from other numerals placed before the noun classifiers or measures, the
numeral nɨŋB1 ‘one’ may occupy both the pre-classifier and the post-classifier
positions in a noun phrase in Thai. He gives three hypotheses of the peculiar
behavior of this numeral: 1) the pre-classifier is the original position, 2) the postclassifier is the original position, and 3) both of them are original positions just as in
Thai today. He tries to find the evidence firstly from comparing cognate or
corresponding meaning word of this numeral in other Tai languages. For example,
the cognate of this numeral nɨŋB1 in Lao (most closely related language to Thai) and
nɨŋB1 in Lungchow (a CT language), can only occur as a post-classifier or occur after
the classifier. All other numerals in Lungchow are placed before the classifier as in
Thai. Other words having corresponding meaning of nɨŋB1 in other Tai dialects, such
as ʔdiaw in Wuming and ʔee in Po-ai (both of NT) also are placed after the classifier.
Li states, “Thus the numeral one, whether it is nɨŋB1 or some other word, has
definitely an association with the post-classifier position” (Li 1978: 142). From the
documentary evidence, Li also finds that the earlier documentary evidence in Thai
shows that nɨŋB1 was only used in the post-classifier position, while the other
numerals were in the pre-classifier position. Finally, Li proposes that nɨŋB1 in modern
Thai behaving in pre-classifier position was formed by being influenced by the usage
of other numerals. Its original meaning also changes as “a descriptive word” after
the syntactic change from the post-classifier position to the pre-classifier position.
Hock (1991: 560) talks about the cases of borrowing like the English/French/
German relationship which furnishes people with insights that “make it possible to
detect borrowing in other cases, where we do not have earlier, direct historical
evidence indicating a borrowing relationship” to interpret the borrowings from
Sinitic languages to TK languages by citing Greenberg’s (1957) argument of
“triangular relationship between Thai10, Sino-Tibetan, and Kadai/Malayo-
Polynesian11”, which concludes “the relationship between Thai and Sino-Tibetan
must be one of borrowing”, since the “specific resemblances” between Thai and
Chinese always “cluster in a few semantic spheres”, while Thai and Kadai/MalayoPolynesian may have genetic relations because those resemblances between Thai
“tend to recur throughout the family” (Hock 1991: 560-561).
In Hock’s discussion, “Thai” actually refers to the Tai language branch of TK.
In Hock’s citation from Greenberg (1957), Kadai and Malayo-Polynesian seem to be sub-grouped
together and to be different from the classification as TK in this thesis.
10
11
36
A similar point of view is also seen in the discussion by Xu (1991: 64) about the
relationship among Kam-Tai, Miao-Yao and Sinitic languages. Xu cites Benedict’s
(1984) discussion about the resemblances of the numerals in Kam-Tai, Miao-Yao,
and Sinitic languages to demonstrate that even basic vocabularies like numerals can
be borrowed, and calls a question about the traditional hypothesis of “Sino-TibetanTai-Miao/Yao genetic relationship” proposed by most scholars in the Sinitic circles.
In the field of applying the comparative method to Tai languages, Li (1977) has
done a most widely accepted PT reconstruction in his famous work A handbook of
comparative Tai. His reconstruction has become a basic source on the comparative
method of the Tai languages, has established ‘Tai’ in one particular sense, and has
delineated a particular family tree of Tai languages. Li firstly enumerates fourteen
Tai languages in the introduction section being the presentation of the basic
materials of the comparative Tai, and chooses three of them, Siamese, Lungchow,
and Po-ai, to be the representative of SWT, CT and NT languages respectively in the
following sections, to reconstruct the tones, the consonants, and the vowels of PT.
Data from other eleven Tai languages are also referenced to. The comparative
method applied to Tai languages in the book cogently demonstrates the process of
the reconstruction of PT morphemes. For example, when reconstructing the protoinitials, Li infers a proto-form by comparing the initials of cognates determined
among the sample languages of SWT, CT, and NT. The proto-initials inferred are not
always inherited in any of modern languages in these three Tai sub-branches. For
example, the proto *b- has developed to ph- in Siamese (SWT) and p- in both
Lungchow (CT) and Po-ai (NT). The reason that Li reconstructs the proto-initial as
*b- is because the tones of this series of cognates are proposed to be developing from
PT tones which always occurred with proto-voiced initials (Li 1977: 65-66). For the
proto-voiceless series, Li also indicates that “it is also assumed that the voiceless
nasals and liquids, written *hm-, *hn-, etc., remained voiceless, although all are
voiced in the modern dialects” (Li 1977: 254-255). In conclusion, Li gives four PT
tone categories *ABCD, sixty-seven initials, six final consonants, and thirty-three
vowels as the integrated reconstruction for PT12.
12
Li’s (1977) reconstruction of PT consonants and vowels are as follows. 1) Sixty-seven initials
include: 10 labials *p-, *pʰ-, *b-, *ʔb-, *m-, *hm-, *f-, *v-, *w- and *hw-; 9 labial clusters *pl-, *pr-,
*phl/r-, *bl- (*br-), *ʔbl/r-, *ml/r-, *fr-, *vr-, and *vl-; 6 dentals *t-, *tʰ-, *d-, *ʔd-, *n- and *hn-; 8
dental clusters *tl-, *tr-, *tʰl-, tʰr-, *dl-, *dr-, *ʔdl/r- and *nl/r-; 4 liquids *l-, *hl-, *r- and *hr-; 9
sibilants *s-, *z-, *t -, *t ʰ-, *dʑ-, *ɲ-, *ɒɲ-, *j- and *ʔɔ-; 7 velars *k-, *ɕʰ-, *g-, *ŋ-, *ɒŋ-, *x- and * -;
37
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) has also accomplished a comprehensive work on
reconstructing the phonological sound system of PT based on a systematic
application of the comparative method and an appreciation of the force of contact.
Opposite to the generally accepted assumption that Proto-Tai was monosyllabic, he
claims that PT was “a sesquisyllabic language that allowed both sesquisyllabic and
monosyllabic prosodic word” (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 2). As the argument in
the reconstruction of the PT prosodic word, he states that the monosyllabic view of
PT fails to account for the range of comparative data and suggests in favor of the
sesquisyllabic hypothesis (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 39-40). For example, he
gives an initial consonant with sesquisyllabic structure *t.n- for PT, for the set of
initials of the word list includes ‘star’, ‘to transplant’, and ‘earthworm’ which
developed into *d- in Siamese (da:wA1, damA1, dɯənA1), Lungchow (da:wA1, damA1,
dɤ:nA1), and Yay (da:wA1, damA1, dɯənA1), but into *tr- in Saek (tra:wA1, tramA1, truəlA1),
because Li’s (1977) reconstruction for this set cannot be accounted for since it is
limited to monosyllables (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 44-45). For the onset,
consonants can combine to form either tautosyllabic clusters (e.g. *kr-, *tr-, and *pr-)
or sesquisyllabic structures (e.g. *p.t-, *q.s-, and *k.r). For the reconstruction of
initials, in contrast to the previous reconstructions, he argues that PT lacked the
aspirated stop series (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 88-89), but only had the three
contrastive phonation types, namely plain voiceless, implosive, and voiced. As for
place of articulation, he adds a distinctive uvular series, in addition to the other five
series (labial, alveolar, palatal, velar, and glottal) previously reconstructed
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 75-76). For the rime, PT had seven vowel qualities
that contrasted in height, backness, rounding and length contrast for each quality
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 192-193). For the codas, PT had palatal and lateral as
final consonants in addition to the ones generally reconstructed (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 193). Finally, for the tones PT was a tonal language contained
four tone categories *ABCD contrasted both in terms of pitch and voice quality
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 285). He also points out that many of PT traits newly
reconstructed, such as, sesquisyllabic and uvular initial series, are not attested in
modern Tai languages because of the changs overridden by waves of convergent
trends that transformed PT (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: I-II).
8 velar clusters *kl-, *kr-, ɕʰl-, *ɕʰr-, *gl-, *gr-, *ŋl/r-, and *xr-; 6 labio-velars *kw-, *ɕʰw-, *gw-, *ŋw, *xw-, and * w-; 2 laryngeals *ʔ- and *h-. 2) Six final consonants include *-p, *-t, *-k, *-m, *-n, and
*-ŋ. 3) Thirty-three vowels include: 9 simple vowels *-i, *-ɨ, *-u, *-e, *-ə, *-o, *-ɛ, *-a, and *- ; 20
diphthongs *-ie, *-ɨe, *-ue, *-əɓ, *-ei, *-ɛɓ, *-ai, *-ɨai, *- ɓ, *-əɨ, *-eɨ/ɛɨ, *-oɨ, *-əu, *-eu, *-ou, *-ɓəu,
*-au, *-ɛu, *-iu and *-ɨɓ, and 4 triphthongs *iau, *-ɓəu, *uai, and *uəɓ.
38
In the Sinitic circles, Zhang and colleagues (1999: 168-246) have done an enormous
comparison of sound systems and vocabularies among many dialects of NZ (of NT)
and SZ (of CT) and some SWT languages. They choose cognates from 36 dialects (of
which 22 are from NZ and 14 are from SZ) as the comparative materials, to clarify
the correspondence of the sounds among Zhuang dialects. They state that they do
not aim to reconstruct all the proto-sounds of so-called proto-Zhuang since SZ and
NZ inherit from different specific ancestral groups (Luoyue and Xi’ou), and the
sounds and vocabularies of SZ are closer to Dai (Lue), Thai, Lao, Shan and etc., on
the contrary those of NZ are closer to Bouyei, but they do need to discuss some
specific proto-initials and proto-vowels when analyzing some correspondences. Some
proto-forms are adopted from Li’s (1977) reconstruction but some are modified by
themselves (Zhang et al. 1999: 169). For example, the voicing alternation of protovoiceless and proto-voiced stops among different Tai languages which lack
explanations in Li’s work (Li 1977), Zhang and colleagues (1999: 174) propose that
they are of a series of voiced aspirated stops **bɦ-, **dɦ-, and **gɦ-13, and developed
to be voiced stops with odd tones in pre-NT dialects but voiceless stops with even
tones in pre-SZ dialects (cf. §1.2.2, §1.2.3, §2.2.3, §2.2.4).
As the Tai language classification which has been briefly discussed in §1.1.2, there
are less debates of grouping the branches of Tai (Zhuang-Dai), Kam-Sui (Dong-Shui),
Hlai (Li) and Kra (Ge-Yang) together to form a language family named TK language
family, which is also called Zhuang-Dong (
侗) or Dong-Tai (侗台) in the Sinitic
circles. In this section, more theoretical principles of classifying Tai languages and
their genetic related languages are presented, except the Tai classification based on
tonology which will be reviewed in §2.2.4.
Tai
Dong-Tai
Family
Dong-Shui
Zhuang, Bouyei, Dai, Lingao, Thai, Lao, Shan, Black Tai,
White Tai, Khamti, Saek, Tho, Nung, Tay, Ahom
Dong, Mulao, Shui, Maonan, Mo, Yanghuang, Lakkja,
Biao
Li
Li, Cun
Ge-Yang
Gelao, Laji, Pubiao, Buyang, Laha
Figure 5 Dong-Tai (TK) family tree (adapted from Zhang et al. 1999: 3)
13
In Zhang et al. (1999), the single asterisk (*) is for labeling the forms in the node of Proto-Tai, the
double asterisk (**) is for labeling the forms in the node of Proto-Kam-Tai.
39
Similar to Diller’s (2008: 7) TK language family tree in Figure 1 in §1.1.1, Zhang and
colleagues (1999: 2-3) also provide an illustration of the family tree to the TK
languages, which they refer to Dong-Tai language group (侗台语族) as shown in
Figure 5. The illustration groups different TK branches, namely Tai, Dong-Shui
(Kam-Sui in the Western circles), Li (Hlai in the Western circles), and Ge-Yang (Kra
in the Western circles) together to form the Dong-Tai language family. In Figure 5
above, some language names follow the Chinese official items which are based on
the national ethnic identification. Some of the languages like Zhuang actually
consists of numerous “dialects” which are actually defined as single languages in ISO
639-3 language code (see §1.1.3).
When subgrouping the Tai branch of TK Family, Li’s (1977) scheme of classifying
the three sub-branches of Tai languages, namely SWT, CT, and NT, has long been
accepted as the standard one in the field of comparative Tai linguistics. His
definitions for the criteria of classifying the Tai languages are 1) phonological
features. For example, PT *tr- gives different modern form in the dialects of these
three branches, as in that it presents the initial of t- in the Southwestern dialects
(Siamese taa ‘eye’), th- or h- in the Central dialects (Lungchow ha: ‘eye’, Tho ta:
‘eye’), and t- or r- in the Northern dialects (Po-ai ta: ‘eye’, Wuming ra: ‘eye’);
2) certain lexical items showing phonological variation, such as, the word for ‘to be’
indicates an original voiceless initial in SWT varieties, but indicates an original
voiced initial in both CT and NT varieties (cf. §1.2.3); and 3) lexical items
containing, as ‘there are lexical items which are lost in one of the subgroups but
kept in the other two’ (Li 1977: xiii-xv).
Since then the argument of the classification of Tai languages is mainly revolved in
the relationship between Li’s CT and SWT sub-branches of this language family.
Even though Li’s (1977) proposal of a tripartite division of Tai has long been
accepted, Gedney (1989b: 62-66) and Chamberlain (1975) propose that the CT and
SWT can form a sub-group of which NT is a sister. Nevertheless, Gedney also admits
that there are basic differences between Li’s CT and SWT sub-branches, but argues
that the scale of these differences is different from those between NT and other Tai
languages, from the evidence of some intermediate dialects between SWT and CT
languages. He points out that “(t)here is general agreement that the Northern Tai
languages form a separate branch” (Gedney 1989: 62), supposes that “the Central
and Southwestern groups form a continuous dialect area, with only gradual
transition throughout and no real language boundary anywhere…” (Gedney 1989b:
40
66), and suggests using the cover term ST to refer to all the non-NT languages
(Gedney 1989a: 231).
As mentioned previously in this section, some “languages” in the Tai branch like
Zhuang consist of more than one single language, and this leads to a more complex
position of language subgrouping in Tai. The classification of Zhuang languages has
been shown to be a complicated situation associated with political national
identification (cf. Zhang et al. 1999). The population of the Zhuang is about
16,178,800 in 2000, making the Zhuang the biggest minority group in China (Office
of Population and Social Science Statistics et al. 2003). However, as Edmondson
(1994) points out that “(d)espite a large population, despite relatively accessible
settlement areas on flat land, and despite a sizable urban population, the Zhuang are
not well-known even among Tai specialists and other SE Asian cognoscenti”
(Edmondson 1994: 147), the Zhuang is disproportionally not well known out of
China when comparing with its large population. Besides, many scholars have found
that several languages are very closely related to Zhuang. For instance, Tay (1
million) and Nung (70,000) in northern Vietnam are connected with SZ, and the
language of 2.5 million Bouyei people in Guizhou province of China and
Giay/Yay/Bo-Y in northern Vietnam are very closely related to NZ (Edmondson
1994: 149).
Primarily, according to the classification established by Chinese scholars, there are
two main dialects of the Zhuang language—NZ and SZ. Approximately two-thirds of
the Zhuang people speak varieties of NZ, while the remaining one-third, about six
million people speak varieties of SZ (Jackson et al. 2011: 1). For solving the
contradiction between the linguistic classification and political ethnic identification
of the Zhuang and Bouyei languages in China, Zhang and colleagues (1999: 11-12)
first put SZ (linguistically of Li’s CT sub-branch) with Dai (Lue), Thai, Lao, and Shan
(all of Li’s SWT sub-branch) together, and NZ with Bouyei (both linguistically of Li’s
NT sub-branch) for clarifying the first stage of divergence time of PT. The evidence
for establishing these two historical branches are both phonology and vocabulary.
First, Thai, Dai, Lao, and Shan have aspirated initials shared with SZ, while NZ and
Bouyei do not have these aspirated initials. Second, NZ and Bouyei have a segment
r- initial occurring with both even and odd tones. Finally, there are also a number of
diagnostic lexical items separate NZ/Bouyei from SZ/SWT, and some of them are
listed in the following Table 15.
41
Table 15 Some diagnostic lexical items of two groups in Tai (adapted from
Zhang et al. 1999: 9-10)
Southern Tai Group
Southwestern Tai
Gloss
‘wing’
‘tiger’
‘cloth’
‘head’
‘above’
14
Thai
Lao
pi:k9
pi:k9
sɯə¹
sɯə¹
sɯə³
huə¹
nɯə¹
sɯə³
huə¹
nɯə¹
Dai
Northern Tai Group
Southern Zhuang
Northern Zhuang/Bouyei
Longzhou
Jingxi
Wuming
Liujiang
Bouyei
pi:k9
pik7
sə¹
ɬɯ:¹
pək7
fɯət8
fɯ:t10
vɯ:t8
kuk7
kuk7
ku:5
pu:6
pu:6
ɣaw³
pɯə6
kjaw³
(Lue)
sə³
ɬɯ:³
θy:¹
θy:³
ho¹
hu:¹
thu:¹
nə¹
nɯ:¹
ny:¹
kɯn²
tɕau³
kɯn²
kɯn²
kwak8
kuə6
fa:ŋ²
fa:ŋ²
6
‘to do’
--
het7
het8
hit7
hat7
‘ghost’
pʰi:¹
pʰi:¹
pʰi:¹
pʰi:¹
pʰej¹
ku: ,
kuək⁸
fa:ŋ²
According to Zhang et al. (1999), languages of SZ and SWT developed from the
same ancestor, Ancient Luoyue, and linguistically split further and further because
of the historical migration of SWT language groups from the homeland of Luoyue
(Guangxi, China) to Vietnam, Laos, Thailand, and Burma with the linguistic contact
with Mon-Khmer languages, and under the influence of Pali and other Indian
languages. On the other hand, SZ and NZ (which developed from Ancient Xi’ou)
become an ethnic union by sharing more and more areal features under the strong
influence from Sinitic languages in the late history. That is to say, if the political
approach is excluded, SZ (including Tay and Nung in Vietnam and approximately
equated with Li’s CT) are linguistically much more close to SWT, while NZ and
Bouyei group together to form a single group which almost equals to Li’s NT. The
treatment of their Zhuang and Bouyei classification is very analogous to Gedney’s
Tai internal classification.
Zhang and colleagues (1999: 12-13) finally distribute the Zhuang language into two
major dialects Northern and Southern, and further divide NZ into eight sub-dialects,
and SZ into five sub-dialects as follows. NZ includes Guibei, Liujiang, Hongshuihe,
Yongbei, Youjiang, Guibian, Qiubei, and Lianshan sub-dialects, while SZ contains
Yongnan, Zuojiang, Dejing, Yanguang, and Wenma sub-dialects. Nevertheless, just as
Tai languages applied by ISO 639-3 codes which we have discussed in §1.1.3,
Jackson and colleagues (2011: Ⅴ, 117-118) points out that varieties of the Dejing
14
The cognate for ‘above’ in Dai (Lue) and Southern Zhuang varieties means ‘north’ in Thai and
Lao.
42
dialect of SZ should be assigned to at least two distinct ISO 639-3 codes, and they
are [zyg] for Yang Zhuang and [zgm] for Min Zhuang. Therefore, it is perceived
further research may assign more distinct languages inside Zhang and colleague’s
(1999) eight sub-dialects of NZ and five sub-dialects of SZ in the future.
A recapitulative work on the comparative method applied to the classification of the
Tai language family has been provided by Diller (1998), as what he states, “(a)
central purpose of this thesis is to distinguish and clarify which types of linguistic
evidence have a core relevance to the operation of the comparative method in the
Tai case and which do not – a distinction which I believe has led to
misunderstanding in the past, including proposed questions of or reconstructions of
the Tai language family.” (Diller 1998: 1) Diller explains how the comparative
method has been applied to the cases of Tai languages, and how the Tai languages
are related to non-Tai languages in the TK family or out of this family. For the
relationships of the languages belonging to different families, such as Thai, Lao and
Khmer, Diller gives an example of the relationship between Swedish and Finnish,
which belong to Indo-European and Finno-Ugric families respectively but share
many similar cultural and linguistic areal features, to demonstrate the genetic
relationship among Thai/Lao and other Tai languages, and the cultural and
linguistic relationship formed with Khmers by contact with each other in the history.
He argues that the approach accepted of language families as valid reifications is
plausible since it is only based on the comparative method, but “does not imply
other claims about specifics of history, culture, race or the like” (Diller 1998: 3). He
also emphasizes that the comparative method provides nodes, rather than names for
nodes, such as, he suggests that Li (1977) provides a particular node of Tai language
tree, rather than names “Tai” to this family since other names such as “Zhuang-Dai”
also can label the same node (Diller 1998: 3-4). He explains that the linguistic
reconstruction is based on the regularity, such as in Li’s (1977) work of a very firm
hypothesis of PT established by showing the comparison of over 1,000 cognate
forms with regular sound correspondences. However, he also points out that “there
are a number of exceptions to regular sound changes evident or implicit in Li’s
work”, such as the voicing alternation of proto-voiceless and proto-voiced initials
among different Tai languages (Diller 1998: 7).
For positioning PT, he introduces some different principles of classifications of Tai
languages, such as Li’s (1977) three-way division, Luo’s (1997) possibility of a
separate Northwestern branch, Haudricourt’s (1948) suggestion of restricting the
term Tai to only those languages whose speakers typically refer to themselves as Tai,
43
etc. (Diller 1998: 9-10). In the contrast to some widespread Tai features, Diller
points out “the certain lexical categories that one might regard as basic do not show
good Pan-Tai cognates”, such as question words ‘who’, ‘what’, ‘why’, and ‘how’, and
many conjunctions, prepositions, preverbal auxiliaries and common verbs like
‘speak’, ‘look at’, ‘return’ and even ‘go’ and ‘be’ (Diller 1998: 14).
In Diller’s discussion, the Tai homeland problem cannot be ignored since the modern
Tai languages taken as a whole share a high degree of similarity of shared lexicons
and regular sound correspondences (Diller 1998: 14). Many scholars have made
useful contributions to the homeland problem and Diller summarizes, “(p)hilological
evidence such as how flora, fauna and agricultural practices are referred to would
not rule out the region encompassing parts of the watersheds of the Xi River system
in Guangxi extending to northern tributaries of the Red River in Vietnam” (Diller
1998: 15). This has surely eroded traditional support for the Altai Mountains origin
or the Nan Chao theory that Tai speakers are the descendants from Altai Mountains
or Yunnanese Nan Chao Kingdom (now Yunnan Province of China) in certain Thai
circles (Diller 1998: 15-16).
Diller’s (1998) discussion also contains other proper examples of areal traits among
Tai languages. In the discussion of more ultimate Tai relationships, Diller clarifies
the problems with typological comparison, such as languages from different
language families can share similar structures phonetically and syntactically, even
the shared vocabulary and regular sound correspondences cannot avoid facing the
problem how to determine which are inherited words and which are borrowed ones
since “regular sound correspondences can be the product of either borrowing or
inheritance” (Diller 1998: 17-18). In the many hypotheses of Tai related
classifications, Benedict’s Austro-Tai hypothesis is one of those resounding, since
some basic vocabularies are shared between PT and Proto-Austronesian, but it may
be disputed since Benedict (1975, 1990) presents many additional far less
convincing relationships (Diller 1998: 22). However, Weera Ostapirat (2013a)
defends the Austro-Tai hypothesis by “a sizable number of shared basic vocabulary
items, which are justified by regular sound correspondences”. In his basic
vocabulary test-list containing 24 basic vocabulary items, 14 words ‘eye’, ‘hand’,
‘nose’, ‘tongue’, ‘tooth’, ‘louse’, ‘fire’, ‘sun’, ‘water’, ‘I’, ‘thou’, ‘one’, ‘two’, and ‘die’
are determined to be cognate etyma between Austronesian and TK languages, and
this confirms the Austro-Tai hypothesis. Based upon the innovation on the mergers
of proto-Austro-Tai *-a: and *-ɤ: in Austronesian and the preservation of these two
44
vowels in TK, he furthermore points out that TK is a separate sister language rather
than an Austronesian daughter language group.
Before reviewing tonology as applied to Tai languages, it is necessary for this study
to look at the study of tones in any language with common usages and general
universality. The system of tone transcription employed in this thesis was introduced
in Chao (1930). It consists of three sets of symbols as illustrated in the following
Figure 6:
straight tones
circumflex tones
short tones
11
131
1
13
153
2
15
242
3
22
313
4
24
315
5
Figure 6 Chao’s (1930) tone letter system
In Chao’s tone letter system, the tone heights are separated into five levels, and the
numeral 1 stands for the lowest pitch, while 5 stands for the highest pitch. Straight
tones contain level and non-circumflex contour (falling or rising) tones. Contour
tones subsequently discussed only exclude level tones like 11, 22 and all short tones
in Chao’s system. This system of tonal transcription was sanctioned into
International Phonetic Association (IPA) in 1989.
Previously proposed studies of tones have captured some of the proposed systems of
tone features. A full scheme of these studies has been concluded by Fu (1995: 2) as
follows.
“i.
The number of tone heights: the maximum number of tone heights any
language can distinguish is five (Wang 1967; Woo 1969; Anderson 1978;
Maddieson 1978b; Edmondson 1992b).
ii. Register bipartition: historically, voiceless syllable initials induce a higher
pitch on the following vowel, while voiced syllable initials induce a lower
pitch. Such consonant effect on vowels may result in a bifurcation of the tone
space, known as register bipartition (Yip 1980; Clements 1981b; Hyman
1986; Bao 1990).
45
iii.
Register overlapping: the upper and lower register tones often overlap in
pitch (Duanmu 1990).
iv.
Tone-height compression: with the increase of the number of tone heights,
the overall tone space tends to stay the same with the space between each
tone height compressed (Pike 1948; Wang 1967).
v.
Tone-space expansion: a system with a larger number of tone heights tends
to have a wider tone space than a system with a smaller number of tone
heights (Maddieson 1970; 1978b).
vi.
The markedness of tone: the mid tone is the least marked (Maddieson 1970;
Hyman 1986; Pulleyblank 1986). ”
Besides, Fu (1995: 2-3) also provides “the construction of a new system of tone
features” which “have never been incorporated in any previous system of tone
features”, as the citation shown below:
“i. Register tripartition: in some Southeast Asian languages, tone space can split
into three registers (Haudricourt 1961; Edmondson 1990a).
ii.
Tonogenesis from syllable final consonants: not only can syllable-initial
consonants induce pitch changes on the following vowel, syllable-final
consonants can also give rise to pitch changes on the preceding vowel
(Haudricourt 1954a, b). In particular, -h gives rise to a falling pitch; -ʔ gives
rise to a rising pitch; sonorant ending give rise to a mid-pitch.
iii. Simultaneous tone-space expansion and tone-height compression: while tonespace tends to be larger in a system with a larger number of tone heights than
in a system with a small er number of tone heights, the distance between each
adjacent tone height tends to be smaller (Hombert 1978b).
iv....Tone merger: tone-height compression results in the reduction of the
perceptual distance between adjacent tones, which may ultimately lead to the
merger of originally distinct tones (Haudricourt 1961; Hombert 1978b).”
Fu’s conclusion above is based on the analyses of previous studies on tones, which
actually differ from each other due to different theoretical bases. Besides, many of
tone systems analyzed by Fu are mainly focused on phonetic features, such as tone
height, pitch, tone-height compression and tone-space expansion. However, since
Tai tonal development especially tonogenesis, tonal splits, tonal mergers, and tone
registers associated with consonantal elements and/or vowel length focused on in
this thesis are more historical and phonological, I am not going to display the detail
46
of these different theories and to estimate which ones are more superior. Some
phonetic tonal features will be only briefly concerned with when our discussion
needs some parts of them to prove some specific hypotheses. The following sections
present many of the previous studies on tones of languages in China and MSEA
including Tai languages to expand the discussion about the process of tonal
development, especially the topic that how tones have arisen in history and how
they have developed after their emergence thus far, as well as some previous studies
on Tai classification based on tonology which are the main subjects of the thesis.
Rejecting some inferences proposed by some early linguists like Henri Maspero
(1912) that tone is an inherent feature of tonal languages and cannot develop from
non-tonal elements, tonogenesis is a hypothesis proposing that tone is a secondary
development which induced by syllable finals, and this has been more and more
popular in modern linguistics. Fu (1995) points out that Matisoff (1973) has
originally referred tonogenesis to the processes “whereby tonal contrasts arise
through the decaying of the consonantal system”, and “has recently been extended
to include cases of tonal development from stress contrast and language contact” (Fu
1995: 61).
One of the earliest hypotheses of tonogenesis is presented by Haudricourt (1954b) as
shown in Table 16. He observes that Vietnamese words of category C (hỏi-ngã)
frequently correspond to cognate words ending in -h or -s, and words of category B
(sắc-nặng) to cognate words ending in a glottal stop -ʔ in other non-tonal MonKhmer languages.
Table 16 Vietnamese tonogenesis (summarized from Haudricourt 1954a)
Tonal Source
-#
-ʔ
-h
-p/ -t / -k
Tonal Category
A
B
C
D
Hence he argues that three tones on smooth syllables in Vietnamese were actually
derived from sonorant endings, -ʔ ending and -h ending respectively, and the latter
two endings dropped in history to give a rising tone and a falling tone
correspondingly, while the sonorant ending remained to give an unmarked mid tone.
Therefore, in history Vietnamese became a tonal language from a non-tonal
language.
47
After Haudricourt’s hypothesis of tonogenesis of Vietnamese in 1954, this scheme
was applied to Chinese in a number of tonology studies. Haudricourt (1954a)
himself has firstly proposed that the departing tone (qù) in Old Chinese (hereafter
OC) was like the Vietnamese counterpart hỏi-ngã tone (the falling tones), and was
marked by an -s. Pulleyblank (1962) posits a -ʔ in earlier source of rising tone
(shàng) in Middle Chinese (hereafter MC) based on analogy with Haudricourt’s
hypothesis of Vietnamese tonogenesis. He moreover contributes evidence for
validating Haudricourt’s *-s hypothesis, and proposes that the level tone (píng) in
MC arose due to the drop of syllable finals *-ɦ and *-ð in OC. In spite of his
inexplicable hypothesis of the arising of MC level tone, his MC rising tone has been
agreed with by Mei (1970), based on the evidence from some modern Sinitic dialects
from Wu and Min which preserve rising tones associating with syllable ending *-ʔ.
Mei (1970: 86) furthermore argues that syllable endings *-ʔ and *-h had dropped off
in the majority of Chinese dialects by the time of Early Middle Chinese (hereafter
EMC) which refers to the system of the Qìyùn, a rime dictionary first published in
601 A.D. and followed by several revised and expanded editions, and affirms that
tones arising due to the loss of these two syllable endings in EMC had manifested a
phonetic shape containing pitch, contour and length. That is, contrastive tones had
arisen in Chinese by the time of EMC. This conclusion is queried by Pulleyblank to
be “excessively cautious” (1978: 174). He argues that these two syllable endings
were preserved in the majority of Chinese dialects until the eighth century, and MC
shēng categories were not tonal but quasi-tonal at most. Contradicting to
Pulleyblank’s argument above, Mei (1982) convincingly provides more evidence for
proving that MC tones could not have associated with the final consonants since *-ʔ
must have been dropped from the northern Chinese dialects, the representative
Chinese language varieties at that time, no later than 200 A.D. Besides, he argues
that MC rising tone had remained stable no less than six centuries in Central Plain
(Zhō ngyuán) in Northern China area, at least nine hundred years in Loyang
(Luòyáng), and for more than one thousand years in Xi’an. This perspective
demonstrates that Chinese tonal system has remained stable for a very long time,
and challenges “the traditional point of view that tonal system is the most unstable
and changeable aspect of phonology due to “its great temporal changes and wide
regional variations” (Fu 1995: 63).
More evidence for supporting the syllable final *-ʔ can cause a rising tone by some
specific mechanism in Lahu is given by Matisoff (1970). Lahu is a language under
the Loloish branch of Lolo-Burmese group of the Tibeto-Burman family. Matisoff
points out that a glottal coarticulation in proto-Lolo-Burmese is the result of the
48
prefix*s- in proto-Tibeto-Burman fused with the initial consonant of the verb-root in
history. By the time proto-Lolo-Burmese had merged all final stops *-p, *-t, and *-k
into *-ʔ. Consequently, two ʔ’s had been created to the syllable’s initial and final
respectively by these two separated processes. This was more than the phonology
system could assume for Lahu, therefore a process “glottal dissimilation” called by
Matisoff caused to drop the final *-ʔ and gave the syllable a rising tone.
In addition, there are other cases opposing to the tonogenesis hypotheses above. For
example, glottal stop is also reported to cause a pitch-lowering effect on the
followed vowel in some languages like Hopi15 (Xu 1995: 64).
For the hypothesis of the direct precursor of the falling tone in MC, opposing to
Haudricourt’s (1954a) hypothesis (*-s > *-h > falling tone), Sagart (1986) suggests
the MC departing-tone was not derived from a syllable final *-h, but from a creaky
phonation. His argument is mainly based on the fact that Sanskrit words with a
syllable final laryngeal spirant *-h borrowed in MC were not transcribed by words
with departing tone which was expected to preserve a*-h in MC. Besides, there are
no modern Sinitic dialect that has ever been reported to preserve a syllable final *-h,
even though more and more evidence has suggested an earlier *-s that should have
been an earlier stage of the departing-tone. He argues that after the weakening of
syllable final *-s, the syllable was characterized by a high degree of glottal
constriction throughout the final in OC and then MC, and this feature was actually a
creaky phonation. Sagart (1988) furthermore expands this hypothesis to all the
tonogenesis of the falling tone that occurred in the history of Chinese and other
MSEA tonal languages. In his viewpoint, in this area languages preserving -h are all
non-tonal, while tonal languages have no -h final, therefore the final -h played no
tonogenetic role. He argues that the correspondence between Mon-Khmer cognates
with *-h in non-Vietnamese languages and those with an original falling tone in
Vietnamese cannot be taken as evidence for the origin of the falling tone. This
correspondence is actually due to two different processes from *-s to modern
languages. For Vietnamese it present as *-s > creaky phonation > falling tone,
while for non-tonal Mon-Khmer languages it present as *-s > -h. Since “the effective
mass of the vocal folds is increased, which results in a slowing down of the
vibrations, in other words, a pitch fall” (Sagart 1988: 84), the creaky phonation
should be the common origin of the falling tone in Chinese and MSEA languages.
This has directly challenged Haudricourt’s (1954a, 1954b) hypothesis of tonogenesis
15
Hopi is an Uto-Aztecan language spoken by the Hopi people (a Pueblo group) of northeastern
Arizona, USA.
49
of the falling tone. However, Fu (1995: 65-66) points out that Hopi, a language out
of the East Asia and MSEA, has been given data to support the *-h hypothesis.
A fuller comparison between Chinese and Vietnamese tonogenesis has been provided
by Norman (1988: 52-57) based on some previous tonogenesis studies. He firstly
points out that although the modern Chinese dialects are all tonal, this was not
necessarily always the case, and some studies especially Haudricourt’s Vietnamese
tonogenesis hypothesis have shown that some present-day tonal languages are
actually derived from non-tonal ancestral languages (Norman 1988: 52). Then he
raises a question of whether OC (about first millennium BC) had tone or not.
Although there were some scholars argued that tone was an inherent feature of tonal
languages like Chinese and could not be derived from non-tonal elements, as what is
reviewed above, Haudricourt (1954b) refutes this hypothesis and shows his evidence
from his hypothesis of tonogenesis of Vietnamese. This hypothesis suggests the
Vietnamese is derived from non-tonal proto-Mon-Khmer language and became a
tonal language by the influence from Chinese due to the abundant borrowings from
Chinese. Back to the question about the tonal feature in Old Chinese, Norman gives
the following discussion. Firstly, he states that “in the earliest layer of Chinese
loanwords in Vietnamese the Chinese departing tone corresponds regularly to the
Vietnamese hỏi-ngã (category C) tone, he (Haudricourt) proposed that the two tonal
categories had the same origin – they are both the result of the loss of an original
final -h, which in turn derives from a still earlier -s” (Norman 1988: 56). Secondly,
he also references the rising tone precursor hypothesis by Pulleyblank (1962)
reviewed above, and says that “the Old Chinese rising tone, like the corresponding
sắc-nặng tones of Vietnamese, might also have derived from a glottal stop (ʔ)”
(Norman 1988: 56). The píng (level) tone was the unmarked category consisting of
those syllables which ended in plain vowels, and the rù (entering) tone was
distinguished by its stop endings. Finally, he gives the answer as shown in the
following statement: “(i)f the rising tone can be derived from such a glottal segment
and the departing tone from a final -s, then Old Chinese was in effect a toneless
language” (Norman 1988: 56). The correspondence between MC tonal categories
and their OC tone precursors with segments pa-, pang-, and pan- as examples given
by Norman are shown in the following Table 17.
50
Table 17 MC tones and their OC source (adapted from Norman 1988: 57)
MC tonal categories píng (level) shǎng (rising) qù (departing) rù (entering)
OC tone precursors
pa[g]
pa-ʔ
pa-s
pak
pang
pang-ʔ
pang-s
kap
pan
pan-ʔ
pan-s
pat
The method of tonogenesis has also been applied to Tai language family without
exception. Some scholars like Li Fang Kuei (1977: 24) believe that “it is impossible
to recover the final consonants that are assumed to have been dropped” to give the
arising of tone in PT period, and it is reasonable to assume that tonal categories had
been done by the time of PT period, although he also mentions that tone system is
suggested to have evolved “from dropping of certain final consonants” in the
assumptions proposed by some ancient Chinese philologists and some modern
linguists, like Vietnamese tonogenesis by Haudricourt (1954b: 69 ff.). Li furthermore
suggests that if the proposal of tonogenesis is tenable, for Tai “it must refer to a
stage of the language prior to Proto-Tai” and the origin of PT tones may have been
in the “Pre-Tai” period (Li 1977: 24). However, in this thesis at least the process of
the arising of tones in Tai should be treated as the first step of tonal development,
and Tai tonogenesis has to be probed into in spite of the dispute of the period of
tone emergence.
The base of tonogenesis applying to Tai is that the Tai tone system shows a strong
resemblance to systems found in other surrounding language families such as
Chinese, Miao-Yao, Vietnamese, and etc. This has been discussed in a numerous of
literatures. It is generally believed that Tai tone system *ABCD is not an accident to
resemble those in Chinese (Karlgern 1964, Li 1980, Pulleyblank 1991, Baxter 1992,
Liang & Zhang 1996, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009), and other language groups in
MSEA (Haudricourt 1961), such as Vietnamese (Haudricourt 1954b), and Miao-Yao
(Chang 1975)16. The correspondence of tone system among these languages
historically formed from the undoubted intense language contact between those
non-Sinitic languages and Chinese. The connections between Chinese and Tai
languages are especially notable since their tones manifest a very robust
correspondence among their shared vocabularies, which present a Chinese source
16
Notice that the conventional tone descriptions of Tai languages and other non-Tai languages
above must be changed over between tone categories *B and *C, that is, Tai Tone *B corresponds to
Tone *C, and Tai Tone *C corresponds to Tone *B in Chinese, Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao languages.
51
borrowed into Tai (Wulff 1934; Prapin Manomaivibool 1975; Li 1976). Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009: 240-241) also summarizes this issue and sketches the
correspondence by showing the following Table 18.
Table 18 Correspondences between PT and MC tones (adapted from Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 240)
PT tones
PT reconstructions
MC glossaries
MC tonal
categories
*A
*so:ŋA ‘two’
ṣaŋ
̊ A ‘two’ (雙)
Even / A tone
*bwi: ‘fat’
bjwei ‘fat’ (肥)
píng (平)
*ha:nB ‘goose’
ŋanC ‘goose’ (雁)
Departing / C tone
A
*B
*ɣe:ŋ ‘shin’
B
*C
*ha:C ‘five’
ɣieŋ ‘shin’ (脛)
qù (去)
ŋuoB ‘five’ (五)
Rising / B tone
C
*ma: ‘horse’
ma ‘horse’ (馬)
shǎng (
*pe:tD ‘eight’
pǎtD ‘eight’ (八)
Entering / D tone
C
*D
A
*ŋɯək ‘crocodile’
D
B
ŋǎk ‘crocodile’ (鱷)
D
)
rù (入)
Since Tai tones and tones in Chinese, Vietnamese and Miao-Yao languages are
structurally identical and show such an evident correspondence, Tai tonogenesis
cannot avoid being explored after the tonogenesis hypothesis applying to these
languages. Here we also focus on the arising of PT Tones *B and *C which
correspond to the aforementioned Chinese/Vietnamese falling tone and rising tone
respectively. Although Tone *B and Tone *C in PT have generally been
characterized as plain syllables, in many modern Tai languages tone C’s (both C1
and/or C2) are actually characterized by glottalization or a creakiness addition to
pitch. Li (1977: 11) has pointed out that Tone C2 in Lungchow, which is a CT
dialect, is always accompanied by a glottal stop at the end of the syllable, and this is
considered a special feature of the tone rather than a final consonant. Gedney
(1989b, 1989c) also points out that there are a great number of CT and SWT
varieties having tones developed from PT Tone *C to be characterized by
glottalization. For example, in Western Nung, which is a CT dialect in Vietnam, both
C1 and C2 tones have been described as having glottalized tones (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 250). In some transcriptions of Debao Zhuang, either C1 and C2
tones are characterized by glottalization, or C2 is characterized by creakiness such
as lən¹²~ ‘tongue’ (Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997: 204). Since in these Tai varieties,
Tone C’s with voice quality in addition to pitch display a striking parallelism to
some modern dialects in Min and Wu of Sinitic languages and Vietnamese, this
phenomenon suggests a commonality in tonogenetic processes having been proposed
52
in Vietnamese and Chinese. Therefore, Tone *C has been speculated in PT as a
glottalized tone by Gedney (1989b: 89). As for PT Tone *B, although there are no
modern Tai dialects which keep a voice quality like aspiration, or in other words a
final *-h like the parallelism in tonogenetic processes proposed by Haudricourt
(1954a, 1954b) and Pulleyblank (1962, 1978) in Vietnamese and/or Chinese, in the
same papers Gedney (1989b: 91, 1989c: 208) speculates that it also had a final *-h
mainly based on its counterpart DL tone, and on the evidence that Thai treatment of
using tone B to borrow Indic loanwords ending in -ha, such as Sanskrit Loha > Thai
lo:B2 ‘shield’.
In the Sinitic circles, the hypothesis of Dong-Tai (Kam-Tai) tonogenesis proposed by
Liang and Zhang (1996: 810-816) agrees with Gedney’s Tai tonogenesis, although
they do not cite from Gedney in their discussion. Initially, they assume that the
emergence of tones in Dong-Tai languages was about three thousand years ago, and
before the split of Ge-Yang (Kra) and Li (Hlai) from Dong-Tai languages, there were
no tones in pre-Dong-Tai languages. Then, they propose that tonal categories *ABCD
in proto-Dong-Tai languages are derived from different syllable finals, namely
sonorants, glottal stop (*-ʔ), pharyngeal fricative (*-h), and oral stops (*-p, *-t, *-k)
respectively. It should be noticed that they follow the conventional Chinese tone
ordering píng (even) shǎng (rising) qù (departing) rù (entering) to sort the
corresponding *ABCD tones in Dong-Tai languages, therefore their Tones B and C
respectively amount to Tones C and B in Li and Gedney’s pattern which is accounted
as conventional tone descriptions in the Western circles. Moreover, they account the
precursor of Tone *B was *-ʔ based on the following evidence: 1) the later forms of
this tone characterized by glottalization or a creakiness in many modern Yongnan
and Zuojiang Zhuang varieties of Southern Zhuang (of CT). For example, in
Daqingshanxiang variety of Longzhou Zhuang the final glottal stop accompanying
with T4 (Liang & Zhang’s Tone B2, or Li and Gedney’s Tone C2) is especially clearer
than other dialects, so that syllables with T4 ending in finals -m, -n and -ŋ sound
very like -p, -t, and -k respectively due to the assimilation with tonal concomitant -ʔ.
The final -ʔ accompanying with T4 in other plain syllables with non-nasal finals are
a more acoustical glottal stop, such as te:ʔ ‘to bear’, tawʔ ‘crutch’, and majʔ
‘tree/wood’ (Liang & Zhang 1996: 814); 2) Some words with checked syllable
ending in a final -ʔ contrasting with -k in Lin’gao17 are cognates with words on Tone
Lin’gao is also known as Ong Be (native pronunciation: [ʔ ŋ˧ e˧]), Bê, or Vo Limgao (臨高,
Lin’gao in Chinese), and is a language spoken by 600,000 people in the counties of Lin’gao,
Qiongshan, Chengmai, and Zhan on the north-central coast of Hainan Island of China (Liang & Zhang
17
53
B (Li and Gedney’s Tone C) in other Dong-Tai languages, such as Lin’gao keʔ7 vs.
Zhuang ke3 ‘to untie, to undress’ (Liang & Zhang 1996: 814); 3) The orthography of
the old Dai Le (Tai Lue) script remains two alternative conventional writing styles of
Tone B (Li and Gedney’s Tone C), of which one is marked by a tone marker ้, and
the other one is labeled by a final -ʔ marker ะ. For example, some words with tone
marker
้ and with final -ʔ marker ะ are transcribed respectively into IPA with
conventional tonal category transcriptions in Chinese circles as follows: bɛ³~pɛʔ⁸
‘goat’, sɔ⁴~sɔʔ⁸ ‘to seek’, ja⁴~jaʔ⁸ ‘to separate’, lo⁴~loʔ⁸ ‘muddy’, ŋɤ⁴~ ŋɤʔ⁸ ‘silly’,
ŋa⁴~ ŋaʔ⁸ ‘sound of crying’ (Liang & Zhang 1996: 815); 4) Some shared vocabularies
between Dai (Tai) and Wa (Austroasiatic) in Yunnan Province of China have a B
tone (Li and Gedney’s Tone C) in Dai and a -ʔ final in Wa respectively, such as Dai
bɛ³/peʔ⁸ vs. Wa pèʔ ‘goat’, Dai la3 vs. Wa laɯʔ ‘girlfriend’, Dai sɤ3 vs. Wa s'bèʔ
‘clothes’, Dai lu⁴ vs. Wa làɯʔ ‘girlfriend’, Dai la:j4 vs. Wa kraiʔ ‘bad’, Dai fa4 vs. Wa
pràiʔ ‘sky’, Dai nɤ4 vs. Wa nèʔ ‘meat’, and Dai pɛ4 vs. Wa pɛ̀ʔ ‘to overcome’ (Liang &
Zhang 1996: 815).
For their PT tonal category *C (Li and Gedney’s PT Tone *B), Liang and Zhang
(1996: 815) also point out that although there are no modern Dong-Tai dialects
preserving a final -h which they suggest to accompany with this PT tone, the final –h
is suggested to exist in PT based on the evidence from some shared vocabularies
between Dai and Wa, just as their discussion of the fourth piece of evidence for the
final -ʔ to their Tone *B reviewed above. They list some of these words as follows:
Dai pha:w⁵ vs. Wa phruah ‘to broadcast (seed)’, Dai kɔj⁵ vs. Wa kɔih ‘to draw out, to
dig’, Dai thaj⁵ vs. Wa tauh ‘to redeem’, Dai bi:⁵ vs. Wa bih ‘to crack’, Dai xe:⁵ vs. Wa
rɛ̀h ‘to harrow’, Dai haw⁵ vs. Wa ràuh ‘to bark’, Dai lɛ:⁶ vs. Wa lɛ̀h ‘to cut, to split, to
peel’, and Dai vɛ:⁶ vs. Wa vɛ̀h ‘in passing, incidentally’ (1996: 816).
Therefore, Liang and Zhang’s proposal of Dong-Tai tonogenesis totally agrees with
Gedney’s (1989b: 91, 1989c: 208) speculation of the origin of four PT tonal
categories, and agrees with Haudricourt and Pulleyblank’s account of tonogenesis,
although their arguments are not always based on the same evidence. Their
agreement on PT tone’s voice qualities is shown in the following Figure 7.
1996: 8). The Lin’gao language is classified as a single language under Tai branch of Dong-Tai
languages by Liang & Zhang (1996), but is suggested to form a sub-branch of which the Tai and KamSui sub-branch is a sister under the Tai-Kadai family (Diller 2008: 7; Edmondson & Solnit 1997: 2),
as shown in Figure 1 in §1.1.1.
54
Proto-Tone
A
B
C
D
Proposed Reconstruction
Syllables ending in a vowels or sonorant.
Syllables ending in a glottal fricative *-h.
Syllables ending in a glottal stop -ʔ.
Syllables ending in an oral stop (-p / -t / -k).
Figure 7 Voice qualities in PT tone summarized
from Gedney (1989b) and Liang & Zhang (1996)
On the other hand, Sagart (1988: 89) argues another interpretation of Tai
tonogenesis containing the following reconstructions shown in Figure 8.
Proto-Tone
A
B
C
D
Proposed Reconstruction
Syllables ending in a sonorant, modal voice.
Syllables ending in a glottal stop, modal voice.
Syllables ending in a sonorant, creaky voice.
Syllables ending in an oral stop, modal voice.
Figure 8 PT tone features suggested by Sagart (1988: 89)
Notice that Sagart’s PT tone features are exactly opposite to his aforementioned
hypothesis to Chinese and Vietnamese tonogenesis in the same paper and in his
previous paper about Chinese tonogenesis (Sagart 1986). Once again, his argument
of the tone features in MC and Vietnamese contains a falling tone (corresponding to
PT Tone *B) with a creaky voice, and a rising tone (corresponding to PT Tone *C)
with a glottal stop, but for PT he has given an opposite ordering on glottal stop and
creaky voice to these two counterpart tones as shown in Figure 8. Sagart reasons
that PT Tone *B usually patterned with *D on tonal splits and mergers, and it is
often the case that these two tonal categories have the same contours and split
conditioning in modern Tai dialects. In other words, Tones *B and *D might be in
complementary distribution. This suggests that they must have ended on the same
kind of syllable finals, namely stops. When comparing with tonogenesis in
Vietnamese (Haudricourt 1954b), which includes that the tonal category sắc-nặng on
syllables ending in an oral or glottal stop, PT*B must have ended in a glottal stop *-ʔ
than a creaky voice. Hence, he argues that Thai treatment of using Tone B’s to
borrow Indic loans with a long vowel ending in a final *-ha was a result of
adaptation to using Tone B (with *-ʔ) to assign this final by dropping the -a, since
both -h and -ʔ are laryngeal consonants. He furthermore points out that in modern
Thai words ending in a glottal stop are all with a short vocalic nuclei and are all
borrowed from other languages, in particular Mon-Khmer languages. Therefore “Tai
may not have had syllables with short vocalic nuclei ending in a glottal stop” (Sagart
55
1988: 89) before this borrowing process, and it means the final -ʔ in modern Thai
has nothing to do with the *-ʔ in tone B in PT. As for PT *C, he accounts that it must
have ended in a creaky voice than a glottal stop *-ʔ by positing those final glottal
stop *-ʔ as a residual feature of the creaky voice quality of PT *C in some modern
Tai varieties.
Querying either Gedney or Sagart’s hypotheses of PT tone features, Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009) provides another possible scheme by proposing that PT Tone *A
was again a modal tone with mid level contour, *B was a creaky tone with a lowrising contour on a relatively long vowel duration, *C was a high-falling tone ending
in a glottal constriction, and *D was the same tone as *B in a checked syllable. The
scheme is as shown in the following Table 19.
Table 19 Phonetic characteristics of PT tones (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009:
271)
*A
*B
*C
*D
Pitch height
mid
low
high
low
Contour
level
rising
falling
rising
long
short
creaky
glottal
constriction
Vowel duration
Voice quality
modal
He shows that Sagart’s viewpoint that PT *B was ending in a glottal stop could not
have been the case by pointing out that a syllable with a glottal stop ending must be
expected to have contrasted long and short vowels as CV:ʔ vs. CVʔ, which is no
evidence for such contrast in PT (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 252). More evidence
for proving that posting a laryngeal coda for PT *B by the correspondence of
loanwords as proposed by Gedney and Sagart was untenable is found in modern
Thai loanwords from Indic or Khmer. That is, in Thai a -ʔ has to be inserted to an
open syllable to preserve the original short vowel in the borrowing language when
the source is with short vocalic nuclei in the original language, such as pʰraʔ ‘monk’
in Thai is actually borrowed from brah ‘honorific prefix’ in Khmer, and this is due to
dropping the -h first since Thai does not allow a final fricative (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 253).
By comparing the development of PT *ABCD tones in seven representative daughter
languages, Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) puts forward his insights as in the
56
following summaries: 1) He firstly proposes the PT tones concurrently associating
with pitch height, pitch contour, vowel duration, and voice quality. 2) As for the
phenomenon that Tone *D normally followed the same pattern of tonal splits and
mergers with Tone *B in the majorities of Tai languages, and Tone *D further split
into DL preserving the same patterns with *B and DS coming to be identified with *C
instead of *B, he posits vowel duration involving in conditioning tonal behaviors,
and assumes that vowel duration in Tone *B must be relatively longer than in Tone
*C. 3) Because the comparison of seven daughter languages reveals that Tone *C
had a higher pitch than *B, he proposes that the pitch of PT Tones *ABC was mid,
low, and high respectively, while *D had no distinctive phonetic properties since it
was solely on checked syllables, but occupied the lower part of the pitch range due
to its variant on non-checked syllable Tone *B. 4) The contours of PT Tones *ABCD
were level, rising, falling, and rising respectively due to his comparative data.
Although either level or falling contours could be reconstructed for both *A and *C
tones, the glottalization of *C indicates that it should have been a falling contour.
Therefore, *A must have been a level contour. The left contour rising is assigned to
*B because “languages that point to a rising contour is *B are spoken very far apart”
to make this contour for *B seem to be a case of retention, and “both experimental
typological studies have shown that vowels under rising tones tend to be longer than
vowels on level and falling tones”. Therefore, Tone *D is also reconstructed as rising
contour because it is on complementary distribution to *B (2009: 276). 5) As for the
voice quality of PT tones, he proposes that PT Tones *ABCD had modal, creaky, final
glottalized, and creaky voice qualities respectively. Since previous linguists’
hypotheses of straightforward of Tone *A are less argument, and glottalization of
Tone *C is supported by many of reflexes in modern Tai languages, Pittayawat
Pittayaporn’s discussion is focused on the voice quality of Tones *B and *D. His
argument of creakiness in *B is firstly based on relatively longer duration on Tone
*B than *C, and this is because a “final glottal constriction tends to end the syllable
abruptly resulting in a shorter vowel duration” (2009: 278), and a creakiness
extends “over a relatively long period of the duration of the vowel” (2009: 277).
Additional support for creakiness of *B is from some voice qualities accompanying
with Tone B’s in several modern SWT and CT varieties. Even though many of them
are described as glottalization, Pittayawat Pittayaporn believes that the voice
qualities may actually be creakiness since at least one of the descriptions Nung Fan
Slihng from Freiberger (1964) specifically mentions that the Tone B2 “ends with a
final glottal stop, but laryngealization may occur over all the voiced phonemes”.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn claims that this laryngealization is actually “a remnant of the
57
creaky voice characteristic of PT *B” (2009: 278-280). The last evidence is the
correspondence between PT *B and MC departing tone which is proposed to derived
from OC -s or -h finals and became creakiness during the MC period (2009: 280281). As for *D, he proposes that it had no voice quality of the tone itself, but the
vowel in syllables with Tone *D had a glottalization before final stops like what has
been discussed by Hombert and colleagues (1979), Mazaudon (1977), and Thurgood
(2002), and this “glottalization in *D resembled the creakiness characteristics of *B”
(2009: 281-282).
In addition, a hypothesis that Tai tonogenesis is related to the loss of the stress of
Austronesian languages has been provided by Ni (1990). He suggests that Dong-Tai
(TK) languages are genetically related to Austronesian languages. On this premise,
Dong-Tai languages’ tones A corresponds to stressed syllables in Austronesian
languages, like Tai / Zhuang ta1 vs. Indonesian mata ‘eye’, and Tai / Zhuang tu1 vs.
Indonesian pintu ‘door’; Dong-Tai language’s Tones BC correspond to unstressed
syllables like Dong-Tai ha:n5 vs. Indonesian gaŋsa ‘goose’, and Dong-Tai’s tone D
corresponds to the same check syllables in Austronesian languages (1990: 262). He
argues that Dong-Tai tones arose due to the language structure change from
multisyllable to monosyllable due to the language contact with Sinitic languages in
history, based on a typical example that five tones in an Austronesian language
Sanya Hui18 arose due to the strong influence from surrounding languages Chinese
and Hlai. This hypothesis of Tai tonogenesis is very different from other scholars’
that tone occurred due to the loss of final consonants or voice qualities.
A comprehensive summary of the tonogenesis hypothesis has been concluded by Fu
(1995: 67) as below:
“… there have been considerable disagreements in the literature regarding
the hypothesis of tonogenesis. For example, the time of tone emergence in
Chinese has been suggested to be as early as in the eleventh century B.C., or
as late as the ninth century A.D., with a difference of two thousand years.
The source of the Departing tone has been posited as -h in some studies, but
as creaky phonation in others. Glottal stop is known to have conditioned the
development of rising tone in Asian languages, but falling tone in American
languages. Creaky voice is found to correlate with high pitch in languages
Sanya Hui is also known as Tsat, Utsat, Utset, Huíhuī, or Hainan Cham, and is a language spoken
by 4,500 people in Yanglan and Huixin, two villages on the outskirts of near Sanya City in Hainan
Island, China by the Utsuls. Tsat is a member of the Malayo-Polynesian group within the Austronesian
language family, and is one of the Chamic languages originating on the coast of present-day Vietnam
(Edmondson 1993).
18
58
like Burmese, but with low pitch in languages like Mam. The falling pitch is
believed to have been conditioned by -h, -ɦ, -ʔ, creaky voice, voiceless
sonorants, semi-vowels, and breathy voice. But in spite of the disagreements
regarding the exact TIME and CONDITION of tonogenesis, the PATTERN of
Haudricourt’s original three-tone schema (1954a), i.e., rising, level and
falling, is left intact.”
After the emergence of tone, tonal splits associating with tonal mergers have been
acting as a main role on the stage of tonal development. Tonal splits have received
much more attention in the literature than tonal mergers, as what Fu (1995: 85)
points out that since the patterns of split are more clear and obvious due to its
conditions of register, and in contrast “both the conditions and the patterns of tone
merger are much more difficult to pinpoint”. Sometimes it is difficult to find out the
evidence to prove whether a seeming tonal merger is a real merger or lack of split.
But in spite of the difficulties, some patterns of tonal mergers have been explored. In
this sub-section the reviews are going to be focused on the patterns of tonal splits,
and sometimes also involving with the discussion of tonal mergers.
Tonal splits are usually treated as tonal registers. For the relationship between
register and tone feature, Fu (1995: 52) has pointed out that “(i)n terms of feature
definition, they are in a hierarchical relationship, register dominating tone” to
indicate this essence. Taking tonal splits after Vietnamese tonogenetic processes
proposed by Haudricourt (1954a) as an example, the three tones rising, level and
falling arose from different syllable finals to represent the tone space before register
split. The influence from different types of initials had given register distinctions on
the tone, that is, voiceless initial gave the upper register, while voiced initial gave
the lower register. Later, the merger of voiceless and voiced initial produced the two
contrasting registers to each of the original tonal categories. Therefore, the essence
of tonal splits is actually registers. Just as what Norman states, “Vietnamese tones
can be plotted using the same scheme as that used for Chinese. Tonal categories A, B
and C correspond to the traditional Chinese categories píng, shǎng and qù
respectively. Category D, like the Chinese rù tone, consists of all syllables which
ended in a stop” (1988: 55), the modern Vietnamese has eight tonal categories from
Chinese terms, and these tones correspond to MC tones which will be discussed
subsequently. This is shown in the following Table 20.
59
Table 20 Vietnamese tones (adapted from Norman 1988: 55)
Register
Tonal category
A
B
C
D
upper
1. ngang
3. sắc
5. hỏi
7. sắc
lower
2. huyền
4. nặng
6. ngã
8. nặng
Next concern is Chinese tonal splits. Since at least the time of the Qièyùn dictionary
(compiled by L̀ Fǎyán in AD 601), the source on which the reconstruction of MC is
based, “Middle Chinese possessed four tonal categories” (Norman 1988: 52), it is
necessary to examine the situation of how these four tonal categories developed into
LMC and Modern Chinese dialects. From a description of a Chinese dialect recorded
by a Japanese monk Annen in the ninth century, it is certain that at least by the late
Tang (AD 923-936), the original tonal categories of Middle Chinese had split into
two registers (Mei 1970), as shown in Table 21.
Table 21 The MC eight tonal categories (adapted from Norman 1988: 54)
Initial class
Voiceless
Voiced
Tonal category
pi ́ng
shǎng
qù
rù
1. yīn píng
3. yīn shǎng
5. yīn qù
7. yīn rù
‘upper level’
‘upper rising’
‘upper
‘upper
2. yáng píng
4. yáng shǎng
6. yáng qù
8. yáng rù
‘lower level’
‘lower rising’
‘lower
‘lower
departing’
departing’
entering’
entering’
From Qièyùn, the four tonal categories of the MC were defined as below: píng: ‘level
and non-abrupt’, shǎng: ‘rising and non-abrupt’, qù: ‘(probably) falling and non-
abrupt’, and rù: ‘abrupt (ending in a stop -p / -t / -k)’ (Norman 1988: 52). For the
development of tone from MC to Modern Chinese dialects, “(p)robably the single
most important factor in the development of the Qièyùn tonal categories to those of
modern dialects was the development of tonal register’ (Norman 1988: 53), when
the voiceless and voiced initials became merger. Basically, voiced initials condition a
lower pitch and voiceless initials condition a higher pitch.
At this stage, a new eight-term tonal system was created from the tonal splits from
four-term tonal categories of MC. However, the tonal split conditioned by sonorant
initials (nasals, laterals and semivowels) sometimes goes with voiced obstruents but
sometimes goes with voiceless obstruents, depending on the tonal category. The
60
more complicating factor is that today only a few modern Sinitic dialects preserve
this eight-term system intact, and most modern dialects “have merged one or more
of these categories, more rarely, one of the categories has split” (Norman 1988: 54).
For example, in Norman’s comparison of twelve Sinitic varieties from seven main
sub-groups of Chinese, eight of them have merged the most register splits and have
only preserved register distinction in the píng tonal category (1988: 182). Most of
Mandarin dialects have lost the final stops in MC, consequently many of them have
merged tone rù (entering, which have been accompanying with final stops) into
other tonal categories, or have developed it as a distinct tonal category without a
final stop, and several dialects merge all final stops into a single -ʔ to retain a
distinct tonal category. The Beijing (Peking in Norman’s description) dialect, the
basic dialect of Standard Mandarin, presents a tonal pattern as follows: it retains the
split of píng tone as yīn píng and yáng píng two registers due to the initial voicing in
MC; the shǎng tone shows no register distinction, and words with voiceless obstruent
initials and sonorant initials retain a single shǎng category, while those with voiced
obstruent initials of this tone in MC have merged into qù tone; the qù tone has
totally merged two registers in history, and the rù tone with final stops in MC has
dropped all the stops and has merged into other four tones (yīn píng, yáng píng,
shǎng, and qù) in a distribution with only partly predictable (1988: 193-195). As for
the Guǎngzhō u variety, which is treated as the standard form of Cantonese (Yuè
dialect of Chinese in Norman’s description), it preserves the MC tones as best since it
shows the most conservation of the MC final consonants with “the only merest
handful of exception”, and the upper register rù in Guǎngzhō u even splits “into two
subtypes conditioned by phonetic vowel length” to make this dialect have nine tonal
categories which is rarely found in other Sinitic languages (Norman 1988: 217-218).
A scheme of Chinese tonal development summarized from Norman (1988) can be
drawn out as shown in the following Figure 9, and it reasonably explains how
Chinese has developed from a non-tonal proto-language to well-developed tonal
daughter languages during at least three thousand years.
61
Stage 1: Old Chinese (first millennium BC): no tones but four phonetic syllable contrasts
vl initial
plain
vd initial
vowels
vowels-ʔ
vowels-s
vowels-p/-t/-k
Stage 2: Early Middle Chinese: phonemic four tones
vl initial
vd initial
píng
shǎng
qù
rù
Stage 3: Tonal splits to phonemic eight tones in Late Middle Chinese
Initial
voicing
mergers
yīn píng
yīn shǎng
yīn qù
yīn rù
yáng píng
yáng shǎng
yáng qù
yáng rù
Stage 4: Modern Chinese: tones preserved, or merged, or split from 8-tone system.
e.g. Guǎngzhō u (Standard Cantonese)
Preservation
& split of
MC tones
yīn píng
yīn shǎng
yīn qù
yáng píng
yáng shǎng
yáng qù
split
>
xià yīn rù
shàng yīn rù
yáng rù
e.g. Běijīng (Standard Mandarin):
Preservation
& mergers
of MC tones
yīn píng
yáng píng
Merger
Merger
> shǎng
>qù
Merger
> one of other
four tones
Figure 9 Chinese tonal development scheme
(summarized from Norman 1988: 52 ff.)
As for the tonal split in Tai languages, we have already reviewed the basic situation
of it in §1.2.1. Here we only have a briefly reminding. Just like what Li Fang Kuei
states that “from a comparative study of the tones of the modern dialects, it is
apparent that the opposition of voicing and voicelessness of the initial consonant
influences practically all the tones in all dialect” (Li 1977: 25), Tai tonal splits
shown in Table 1 in §1.2.1 and the remained discussion have presented a robust
comparability with the case of Chinese and Vietnamese tonal splits. With a detailed
description of the tonal systems of PT tones (*ABCD), Gedney (1989[1972]) points
out that after the period of PT unity, a wave of drastic sound changes swept this
entire area which includes various new locations groups of Tai speakers had
dispersed to. In Tai family, “these sound changes involved splits in the tonal system,
62
with the splits conditioned by the phonetic nature of initial consonants of the
syllable” (1989[1972]: 196). The details of these changes differed from place to
place, but in general “each of the original tones A, B, C, and D split into two or more
tones, with, in some dialects, various coalescences of the resulting tones with other
tones from the sources in the previous pattern” (1989[1972]: 196). For the checked
syllable, tonal splits are not only conditioned by initials but also conditioned by
vowel length. The Proto-Tai tones which were at time of split are described by Li
(1977) as Table 1 in §1.2.1.
Gedney gives an example of tonal splits and mergers in Thai for briefly indicating
the common situation of Tai case. Test words of Thai applying to this proto-tone box
are as below (1989[1972]: 197, 199): Column A: Box 1. pii ‘year’, naa ‘thick’; Box 2.
phii ‘fat’; naa ‘ricefield’; Column B: Box 3. pii ‘flute’, naa ‘arrow of crossbow’; Box 4.
phii ‘older sibling’ Column C: Box 5. naa ‘face’, maa ‘to increase’; Box 6. maa ‘horse’;
Column D-short: Box 7. lak ‘stake’, mat ‘flea’; Box 8. lak ‘to steal’, mat ‘to tie up’;
Column D-long: Box 9. maak ‘fruit’; Box 10. maak ‘classifier for tools’. For the tones
in smooth syllables (A, B, and C), as a result of tonal splits, six-tone systems are very
frequent throughout the modern Tai dialects speaking domain, while many modern
dialects shows irregular splits and mergers patterns. For example, tonal splits in Thai
are shown in Table 22.
Table 22 Register tonal splits of Thai (adapted from Gedney 1989[1972]: 201)
A
Initials
Friction
5th tone
at time
Voiceless
1st tone
B
C
D-short
D-long
2nd tone
3rd tone
2nd tone
2nd tone
3rd tone
4th tone
4th tone
3rd tone
of split
Voiced
In the table above, we see that in Thai there is an imbalanced tonal splits between
original Tone A and other tones, and lower register of Tone B and the upper register
of Tone C have merged with each other. Although the special split that Thai has
made in Column A is not common, many known dialects have made a split of this
sort in one or another of the other columns of the diagram, and this is why it
requires a test diagram which is to allow for a maximum of possibilities, include this
type of split (Gedney 1989[1972]: 201). As a result of comparison the tonal splits
and mergers of many modern Tai languages, the Tai tone box designed by Gedney
(1989[1972]: 202) consists of five tone categories (A, B, C, DS, DL) with four groups
of initial consonants conditioning possible tone splits, as what has been shown in
63
Table 4 in §1.2.1. As for some specific tonal splits in CT and NT languages, Liao and
Shen (2012) have provided a revised Tai tone box for solving the noticeable
shortcomings of Gedney’s original tone box when applying to Tai languages beyond
SWT, as what has been discussed in detail in §1.2.2, and has been shown in Table 7
in the same sub-section.
Just as what tonal splits presenting in Vietnamese, Chinese and Tai reviewed above,
two-way register split which refers to “split conditioned by the simple opposition
between voiced and voiceless initial consonants” (Fu 1995: 80), has been known for
a long time. However, later studies have shown three-way register split, a
tripartition of tonal split, can possibly occur in some languages even it is relatively
rare and less well-known. A more famous three-way register split among preglottalized vs. aspirated vs. voiced sounds in Kam has been presented by
Haudricourt (1961, 1972), and here we only cite the comparison in tonal category A
as shown in the following Table 23.
Table 23 Three-way register split in A tone in Kam (adapted from Haudricourt
1972: 68)19
Category A
Sui
Mak
Glottalized
ʔnaa
11
ʔbaaŋ
ʔdaai
Aspirated
11
11
hmaa11
naa
ʔbaaŋ
ʔdaai
24
24
hai
13
mya
mii
13
lee11
Raan11
hnai
11
Glossary
naa⁵⁵
‘thick’
laai⁵⁵
‘well’
maaŋ⁵⁵
maa³⁵
maa
11
Kam
nwaa³⁵
maa13
13
-Voiced
24
ai³⁵
‘thin’
‘dog’
‘come’
‘open’
mya
‘hand’
lee13
lee212
‘writing’
žaan13
yaan212
‘house’
212
Just like what Fu (1995: 80-81) points out that “the conditioning factor is not just
limited to the simple voiced: voiceless opposition”, tonal splits can also be
conditioned by other pairs of oppositional initial sounds after the tonal splits of the
original two-way register. For example, some Miao dialects present a secondary
tonal split between the pair of voiceless unaspirated vs. voiceless aspirated sounds,
and pre-nasalized stops/affricates vs. stops/affricates to make a three-way register
split among these pairs and the original voiced register (Li et al. 1959). Furthermore,
19
IPA symbols in this table are all adopted from Haudricourt’s (1972: 68). There may be some
imprecise symbols, such as R- in Raan ‘house’. The initial of ai35 ‘open’ in Kam may be ʔ- which may
have been omitted in Haudricourt’s transcription.
64
Pulleyblank (1978) has also argued a triple-split case from LMC to EMC. Wujiang
variety of the Wu dialect of Chinese has been found to have a typical three-way
register split by several scholars (Chao 1928; Yuan 1960; Ye 1983; Shi 1992).
As shown in Table 23, although Kam has lost the pre-glottalized element in other
sister-languages, and has merged voiceless nasals20 to voiced nasals, the tonal split
conditioned by these two contrastive original voiceless initial registers is clearly
presented. The Kam tonal split not only demonstrates that register splits in initial
consonants can be conditioned from the same features that have conditioned
tonogenesis, such as glottalization (-ʔ) and aspiration (-h), but also presents that
these features condition differently when they are in the final position and in the
initial position. Comparing with the voiceless aspiration in syllable-final position
giving a falling tone (lowest pitch) in the tonogenesis process, this feature in
syllable-initial position does not condition a lowest pitch in the tonal register split
process at least in Kam. A summary of Kam three-way split is provided by
Haudricourt (1961, 1972) as shown in the following Table 24.
Table 24 Three-way register splits in Kam (adapted from Haudricourt 1972:
70)21
Tone
Initial changes and retentions
A
B
C
ˀb- > m-, ˀm- > m-, ˀn- > n-, ˀd- > l-, ˀR- > y-, k-, t-, p-
55
53
323
hm- > m-, hn- > n-, hn̄- > ñy-, hṅ- > ø-, kh-, th-, ph-
35
453
13
m-, n-, ñ-, l-, b->p-, d->t-, g->k-, R->y-
212
33
31
Edmondson (1990) explains the Kam three-way register split in a different way. He
argues that the triple division of the tonal splits in Kam was actually not a
simultaneous tripartition, but two temporarily distinct bipartitions containing a
primary split preceding a secondary split. He furthermore suggests the cause of the
secondary tonal split was not aspiration but breathy voice. In spite of the real cause
of the secondary split being aspiration or breathy voice, the most notable
exploration is that three-way register split may be a tripartition resulting from two
successive double split.
20
In Kam, the tonal split of original voiceless nasal goes with aspirated sounds, and is treated as a
kind of aspirated sound in the tonal development process.
21
IPA symbols in this table are all adopted from Haudricourt’s (1972: 70). There may be some
imprecise symbols, such as hn̄-. The tonal values in Haudricourt’s text are 5-level pitch tone letters,
and here I transliterate to the Chao tone numerals based on the Chao 5-level pitch scale (Chao 1930).
65
In spite of the different views concerning the issue that three-way register split is a
simultaneous tripartite split or two successive double splits, Fu (1995: 82) states that
“tripartition as a current state-of-affairs has been confirmed in a number of tonal
systems”. One thing is clearly pointed out by him that “secondary split occurs only
in one of the register produced by the primary split” (1995: 82), such as in
Nanchang the lower register further splits into two but no further split occurs in the
upper register, but in Miao, Kam, and Songling-Tongli the opposite situation is
shown as that the secondary split only occurs in the upper register but is never
found in the lower register. That is to say, “no four-way split occurs” (1995: 83). Fu
furthermore points out the counterexample in tone on checked syllables, and this
time seeming four-way splits occur. He cites some previous studies on check tones in
some Cantonese dialects in Guangxi like Hengxian (Bi 1982), Yulin and Bobai (Yang
et al. 1985) to indicate this issue. The example of Hengxian is a typical example as
shown in the following Figure 10.
DIa
kəp
DIb
44
tɕət44
hək
44
‘quality’
‘black’
‘attack’
kik44
kuk
‘urgent’
44
‘valley’
DIIa
ka:p
33
tɕa:t33
ha:k
33
ke:k33
kɔ:k
33
‘first’
‘prick’
‘guest’
‘foot’
DIIb
tɕuk
11
tɕik11
muk
11
ləp11
‘country’ fət
11
‘yesterday’ tɕɔ:k22
‘muddy’
‘eye’
‘no’
‘straight’
‘stand’
‘Buddha’
tɕe:k22
mɔ:k
22
la:p22
fa:t
22
‘touch’
‘bacon’
‘punish’
Figure 10 Hengxian checked tones (Bi 1982: 21 cited in Fu 1995: 83)
In this language, the earlier two-way register split on checked syllable has split in
four ways due to the vocalic durations. The upper register (DI tone) and the lower
register (DII tone) co-occurs with short vowels are labelled as DIa and DIIa
respectively, while they are labelled as DIb and DIIb when co-occuring with long
vowels. Fu (1995: 83) cites a tentative explanation from Hirayama (1987: 19) to
suggest that the two different but pitch-closed tones splitting from the same register
due to the vowel duration are allotones to one another more than being two distinct
tones. He furthermore argues that this kind of “vowel-duration-induced tone split”
cannot constitute a counterexample to the proposition that “the maximum number
of register splits is three”, since this kind of tonal splits can be treated as splits
within a single register, and does not result in register split just like the “syllablefinal-induced tonogenesis” (1995: 84).
The last issue of tonal splits concerns the explanation of the “voiceless-low” and
“voiced-high” correlation, which refers to that initial voiceless consonants correlate
66
with low tones, while initial voiced consonants correlate with high tones, and
violates the normal situation of “voiceless-high” and “voiced-low” register split
reviewed above.
Fu (1995: 81) has cited Maddieson (1978a) to point out that the correlation of
“voiceless-low” and “voiced-high” is found in some languages and “is not unusual
cross-linguistically”. He furthermore cited Brown (1975) to give data to show that
the situation of “voiced-high” can be found in 75% of tone data from Thai, Northern
Thai, Lao, Shan, Phuan, and Phu Thai of SWT languages. Brown’s own explanation
for this phenomenon is based on the “different strategies listeners use to process
linguistic signals” (Fu 1995: 82). Brown argues that in proto-languages the syllables
with a voiced initial presented a lower pitch on initial consonants but higher pitch
on the rest following part, while syllables with a voiceless initial presented an
opposite situation that presented a higher pitch on initial but a lower pitch on
remaining part. This situation has allowed two opposite directions of tone
development based on the concentration being initial or median portions of the
syllables due to different fashions and prestige. Therefore, either “voiced-low” or
“voiced-high” languages were developed by different linguistic communities. For Tai
languages, Brown suggests that “voiced-high” splits antedated “voiced-low” splits by
400 years.
A more compellent explanation is that “voiced-high” phenomenon is tonal flip-flop
suggested by many scholars like Wang (1967, 1969), Matisoff (1973), Hashimoto
(1972), and Hashimoto (1986). Just like what Fu (1995: 85) states that “(i)t has
been pointed out by many scholars that tonal systems have a life of their own; once
established, they develop without regarding to their etymological values
(Haudricourt 1961; Hombert 1978), and there seem to be no guide-lines for
development after tone split, with different dialects changing in different directions
(Henderson 1982)”, at the time of register-splits tone pitch always started from
“voiced-low”, but the later development may change from high to low and low to
high after the establishment of tones. Edmondson (1994: 164) applies tonal flip-flop
to “voiced-high” phenomenon in SWT languages instead of the explanation from
Brown (1975) above. He turns over a theme about the different tonology among NT
(NZ), SWT, and CT (SZ). In this case, “voiced-low” principle, “which basically states
that original voiceless initial consonants conditioned low tones and original
voiceless initial consonants conditioned high tones” (Edmondson 1994: 164),
becomes a distinctive feature. In the vast majority of NZ and SZ, the historically
high tone set generally possesses a higher pitch than the historically low tone set,
67
while in SWT, “this situation is sometimes obscured by subsequent tonal flip-flop of
highs and lows”, and becomes a characteristic for SWT. Though there has no tonal
flip-flop in CT, there is a “slight tendency to tone reversal in the extreme
southwestern locations” of SZ area and this also becomes a distinguishing feature
from NZ (Edmondson 1994: 164).
The other three explanations for “voiced-high” correlation are also briefly
summarized by Fu (1995: 82) as follows. 1) “consonants had changed before register
split”, such as that the initial b- had changed to an implosive ɓ- before tonal split
suggested by Hombert and colleagues (1979). 2) “‘voiceless-low’ and ‘voiced-high’
are regarded as resulting from the effects of tone on consonant”, like what
Maddieson (1978a) shows that besides the initial consonants, syllable-final voicing
contrast, places or manners of articulation, and the insertion of consonantal
segments “may all be attributable to tonal effects”. 3) The last explanation is “by
posting more consonant types for the phonological system before register split”, and
Fu also cites proto-Min’s situation that a series of voiceless sonorants caused
sonorant initials in modern Min dialects to correlate with upper register tones
suggested by Norman (1973, 1974); this is quite similar to that the voiceless
sonorants have given upper register tones in Tai languages reviewed above.
As for tonal mergers, one thing need to be formatted is the pattern of merger. Fu
(1995: 85) cites the patterns of tonal merger from Pan (1982) and Lien (1986), and
they are shown in the following Figure 11. In Pan’s figure of tonal merger pattern,
Type A and Type B are called “vertical merger” and “horizontal merger” respectively
by Lien (1986), and they represent mergers between the upper and lower registers
as well as mergers across tonal categories within the same register respectively. Type
C refers to mergers across both tonal categories and upper-lower registers. Pan
points out that Types A and C are not found in languages where voicing distinction
in initial obstruents is still preserved, and Type B is not found in languages
preserving obstruent endings.
A
B
C
D
yin
IA
IB
IC
ID
yang
IIA
IIB
IIC
IID
Type A merger Type B merger
Type C merger
Figure 11 Type of tonal mergers (Pan 1982)
In addition, Fu (1995: 86) has reminded that “(a) true Type A merger must be
carefully distinguished from lack of split”, but a true Type A merger and lack of split
68
is hard to judge. However, Fu (1995: 88) also points out that “the more tones split,
the more likely merger would happen, especially mergers of Types B and C”. In the
Thai tonal patterns shown in Table 22 above, tones lower B and upper C which
should have been two distinct tones have merged as what Type C refers to.
The identification of the conditioning factors of register split is concerned when
talking about the tonal splits. In spite of some phenomenon of tonal flip-flop
mentioned in §2.2.2, the traditional “voiced-low” and “voiceless-high” principle has
been verified in a great number of cross-linguistic phonetic studies. However, just
like what we have reviewed in the three-way register splits and Gedney’s analysis of
Thai tonal splits in §2.2.2, more specific phonetic factors can also cause tonal splits.
Therefore, it is necessary to review the previous studies on the conditioning factors
of tonal splits, especially those factors beyond the simple voiceless-voiced
opposition. The following contents centre on several phonation types, namely the
aspiration, unaspiration, glottalization, and breathy voice, to expand the discussion.
The first issue concerns the traditional voiceless frictions, and we focus on the
situation in Tai languages since other languages such as Kam, Miao and Chinese
dialects have been discussed on this issue in §2.2.2. Voiceless frictions have been
further divided into several phonation types by Li (1977: 28-29). As the point of
penetration, his description of the splits in Tone A in Thai is with a consistent
explanation: A1 can be mid level (33) or low-rising (24), depends on the different
initial features, and A2 is mid level (33). Furthermore, an example of the
comparison among tonal development in different dialects is given, that is, A1 tone
(A upper register) in Thai, Lungchow and Po-ai shows three different types of tonal
development. Thai divides two tones between the line of aspiration and other types,
Po-ai divides two tones between the line of glottalization and other types, and
Lungchow does not divide tones in A1 (Li 1977: 29). Thus, voiceless initials at least
can be treated as three phonation types by the comparison among these three
languages: unaspiration, aspiration, and glottalization. In Li’s discussion of the
relationship between initials and tonal development, proto-initials can be divided
into three laryngeal types: voicing, glottalization, and aspiration (Li 1977: 26-27).
Altogether, Li finds “the Proto-Tai initials may be roughly divided into five groups
according to the influence they may have on tone” (Li 1977: 43) and lists 1)
aspirated voiceless stops, 2) voiceless continuants, 3) unaspirated voiceless stops,
4) glottalized consonants, and 5) voiced consonants. Li’s five groups of initial can
69
actually serve for wider range of Tai varieties than Gedney’s Tai tone box, which
only divides PT initials into four groups (cf. §1.2.1 and §1.2.2).
In the progress of tonal development of the Zhuang languages, Zhang (1980), Liang
and Zhang (1996: 818-828) and Zhang and colleagues (1999: 243 ff.) also assume
that several specific laryngeal types of initial consonants have conditioned secondary
tonal splits after the primary voiceless-voiced register split. This presents a temporal
sequence of tonal developing event. Register splits firstly conditioned by the initial
voicing gave eight tones from original four proto-tones. After that different laryngeal
features of voiceless initials caused different secondary tonal splits and mergers in
nearly half of Zhuang dialects. The processes of secondary tonal splits in different
dialects can be divided into four types, which will be enumerated in the discussions
of the determinations of the diachronic secondary tonal splits (cf. §4.3.1).
In addition, Liang and Zhang (1996: 826-828) also analyze similar secondary tonal
development conditioned by these specific phonation types of initials in other TK
languages like Yanghuang (Then), Lakjia, Lin’gao, and Biao. However, some
conditioning factors of secondary tonal development in these languages are different
from those in Tai languages. For example, in Yanghuang the continuants (nasals,
sonorants, and semivowels) development from proto-voiceless continuants have
conditioned some of the words from odd tone (upper register) to merge with even
tone (lower register), such as mjɛ:ŋ² ‘irrigation canals and ditches’, mje² ‘bear’, and
ra² ‘thick’ with a lower register tone corresponding to an upper register tone in other
sister-languages of TK family. In Lakjia, initials containing continuants like
semivowels, labialized stops (stops + -w-) or consonant clusters, as well as some
voiceless unaspirated stops also condition some words from an odd tone to an even
tone, like jau² ‘green’, jum² ‘amaranth’, com² ‘hair’, kou² ‘horn’, pliŋ² ‘leech’, lɔŋ²
‘griddle’, ka:m² ‘to ask’, wa:n ‘sowing’, hwai⁶ ‘new’, and ja² ‘rice field’, which
normally have an even tone in other sister-languages of the same family. The
conditioning factor which is a continuant (from a PT voiceless-continuant) causing a
secondary tonal split is not found in Tai languages at least in the previous studies.
In short, Zhang (1980), Liang and Zhang (1996), and Zhang and colleagues (1999)
also divide the initials which have influence on secondary tonal development into
four groups, namely glottalization, aspiration, unaspiration, and the remaining
continuants (from PT voiceless continuants) which is not found to condition
secondary tonal development. That is to say, the PT voiceless continuants tend to
mostly keep its tonal behaviors as in its primary tonal splits. This is somehow related
70
to but different from the suggestion from Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997) that “the
merging of proto-voiceless sonorants with proto-voiced sonorants is the cause of
primary tone split in Tho” (1997: 215) when she concludes her findings of tonal
splits of Dai Tho and Tai Tho discussed in the following §2.2.4. The division of
initial groups from these scholars above agrees with the four groups of PT voiceless
initials divided by Li (1970) reviewed above. However, the noticeable problem
mentioned in §1.2.3 that PT might not have aspirated sounds also reminds us to
carefully avoid using the term “proto-initial” when dividing these initial consonants
which are conditioning factors in the processes of tonal development. Besides,
aspiration in relation to pitch lowering in some historical and dialectal studies
(Haudricourt 1961, Brown 1975, Henderson 1982, Ye 1983) also presents a more
complicated effect on tones, although phonetic studies have not yet confirmed that
aspiration has a defining pitch-lower effect (Hombert 1976).
The next issue concerns the original voiced initials or lower register. Although both
plain and breathy voiced initials are assumed to give a pitch lowering effect, Fu
(1995: 81) has pointed out that “recent studies tend to attribute the real cause of
pitch lowering to breathy voice rather than plain voice”, such as the discussions in
Pulleyblank (1978), Shi (1981) and Edmondson (1990). This hypothesis is
conflictive with the suggestion that there was a contrast between breathy initials (or
voiced aspirated, as well as a series of special voiced initial) and plain voiced initials
in PT proposed by Gedney (1985), Li (1989), Liang and Zhang (1996), Zhang and
colleagues (1999) and Liao and Shen (2012). The breathy voice suggested here is
also commonly addressed as “voicing alternation”, which has been discussed in
§1.2.2 and §1.2.3 as a discrepancy generally described to have disagreement
between SWT/CT on one hand and NT on the other.
For the vast majority of TK languages, breathy voice is not found, and it is just
suggested to have had in history by the aforementioned scholars to explain the
“voicing alternation” or “register alternation” among different TK daughter
languages. We have focused on this hypothesis applying to Tai languages in §1.2.2
and §1.2.3, and this time Kam-Sui languages as another example to be given by
Weera Ostapirat (1994). He also discusses about a hypothesis that breathy sounds
(sounds with a ɦ-) in proto Kam-Sui (hereafter PKS) conditioned the different tone
developments into different modern Kam-Sui languages. Since there are no breathy
sounds in modern Kam-Sui languages and the data for reconstructing these kinds of
breathy sounds from modern Kam-Sui languages are still not enough, Weera
Ostapirat states “(t)he proposal here is not, however, a definite treatment but rather
71
a call for discussion” (Weera Ostapirat 1994: 79). The main evidence that proves
there were breathy sounds in Kam-Sui is the irregularity and non-straightforward
development of tones (sometimes including initial features) of PKS voiced nasal *N(N represents nasals in general). For example, Sui has nam³ while Kam has nam⁴
‘water’ (Weera Ostapirat 1994: 79). This kind of evidence distinguishes two groups
of modern Kam-Sui languages as shown in the following Table 25.
Table 25 Tonal correspondence of nasals between two groups of Kam-Sui
languages (adapted from Weera Ostapirat 1994: 79)
Group 1
Gloss
Kam
Mulam
Group 2
Then
Sui
Mak
Maonan
Maonan
(Xianan) (Shangnan)
‘bamboo
na:ŋ²
na:ŋ²
---
na:ŋ¹
na:ŋ¹
na:ŋ¹
n̥a:ŋ¹
‘water’
nam⁴
nəm⁴
nam⁴
nam³
nam³
nam³
---
na:m⁵
n̥am³
---
---
---
shoot’
‘mud’
‘hand’
na:m⁶
na:m⁶
mja²
nja²
mja²
mja¹
mii¹
---
---
‘ghost,
---
---
ma:ŋ²
ma:ŋ¹
---
ma:ŋ¹
m̥ a:ŋ¹
‘fog’
mun²
---
---
---
---
mu:n¹
m̥ u:n¹
spirit’
Because in most situations “the reflexes of PKS voiced nasals *N- are very regular
and straightforward, showing plain or voiced nasals with low tones in all daughter
languages” (Weera Ostapirat 1994: 79), the irregularities of the *N- in some
examples of two groups of PKS’s daughter languages in the table above show that
the initials of these examples may not belong to the common *N-, and Weera
Ostapirat proposes for these sounds the breathy nasals (*ɦN-)’ (Weera Ostapirat
1994: 80). Weera Ostapirat explains how *ɦN- develops into either N- or voiceless
hN- by the following splits: breathiness = voiced (vocal cords vibrating) +
aspirated (arytenoids apart), and it ‘then can be assumed to be either voiced (low) or
voiceless (high) as to assigning tones’ (Weera Ostapirat 1994:80). Thus, Weera
Ostapirat adds breathy nasals as an independent group of nasals into PKS, and
demonstrates four different groups of nasals as shown in the following Table 26.
72
Table 26 Four groups of Kam-Sui nasals proposed by Weera Ostapirat (1994:
80)
Sui
Maonan
Mak
Kam
Mulam
Then
*ØN-
N-31
N-231
N-31
N-11
N-121
N-35
*ɦN-
N-11
N-42
N-13
N-11
N-121
N-35
N̥ -11
N-42
N-13
N-35
N-13
ʔN-42
N̥ -42
N-24
N-55
N-42
N-13
*hN*ʔN-
ʔN-11
Furthermore, Weera Ostapirat points out that there are not only nasals but also
lateral and approximants have the same “tone alternation” between two groups
(Group 1 and Group 2 in Table 25) of the daughter languages of PKS, and the
conditional situations are the same. Therefore, he concludes a series of “PKS breathy
liquids” as *ɦN-, *ɦl-, and *ɦr-. Moreover, Weera Ostapirat points out that there is
also another kind of breathiness (PKS pre-nasalized stops with -r-) which evidences
the so-called “alternation of tonal series”, as in his statement that -r- “together with
the intervoiced environment, may have special influence in including breathiness
during the development of these pre-nasalized stops (e.g. *mpr- > mphr- > mbr-)
and bring about tone behavior similar to that of *ɦr-” (Weera Ostapirat 1994: 85).
Rejecting the hypothesis that breathy voice causes voicing alternation, Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009) has provided another explanation for this phenomenon. He
suggests that most of words with “voicing alternation” are loanwords from Old
Chinese during the post-PT period (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 77-87). For
example, he suggests the word for ‘bean’ in Old Chinese was a pre-nasalized form
*N-tho[k]-s citing from Sagart (1999: 185-187), and the CT/SWT dialects simply
dropped the *N- and borrowed this etymon as *tʰuəB1 while the NT dialects changed
the pre-nasalized stop into a plain stop, and borrowed this word as *duəB2
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 86)22. However, Pittayawat Pittayaporn also admits
some words with “voicing alternation” should be part of PT or Kra-Dai (TK)
cognates. He also finds that the voicing alternation items that may have been part of
PT cognates reflect proto-voiceless initials but those may have been borrowed from
Chinese at post-PT period reflect proto-voiced-initials in MC (Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 87). Finally, he argues that voicing alternation is not a unified phenomenon
but “consists of two distinct sets of data”. One set is a result of different
arrangements for post-PT borrowings between NT and CT/SWT languages, and the
In Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s original text this word has the B1 tone (2009: 86), but this should be
wrong since words developed from *d- should collocate with a lower register Tone B2, and all NT
languages have shown a B2 tone for this item in the previous transcriptions.
22
73
other set which may be from PT cognates is accounted to have sesquisyllabic clusters
onsets which have given different voicing effects between NT and CT/SWT
languages (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 87-88).
In summary, previous studies have proved that the conditioning factors of tonal
splits is not only limited to the voiceless: voiced opposition, but is also involved with
some other phonation features of initial consonant, like aspiration, de-aspiration,
continuants, glottalization, breathiness, and pre-nasalization.
Besides the aforementioned classification reviewed in §1.1.1, §1.1.2 and §2.1.3 of
the Tai languages, there are several opinions on classifying the whole branch or a
specific sub-group of the Tai languages by some principles established by tones.
Chamberlain (1975) uses different tonal splits based on Gedney’s (1989[1972]: 202)
tone box to classify SWT languages as shown in Figure 12. He suggests that the Tai
languages should be divided first into Northern and South-Central branches, and
that the South-Central branch should be divided into two groups, P and PH23.
Finally, the further classification under P and PH should follow the basis of the tonal
splits, that is to say, “certain types of tone system splitting and coalescing were
associated only with the PH group and certain others only with P group”
(Chamberlain 1975: 49). For example, in the P group tone system is *ABCD 123-424
with the possibility in some dialects of A12-34, while in the PH group two types of
tone splits are also shown as *A 1-23-4 / BCD 123-4 and *ABCD 1-23-4. However,
there are also rare exceptions in group P which show the splits like those in PH
group. For Li’s Central Tai dialects, the tone system variation correlates in precisely
the same way, A123-4 or A12-34 in P group and A 1-23-4 in the PH group. This
means that there is no reason for separating the CT and SWT. Chamberlain then lists
the hierarchy of criteria of classifying the Tai languages: 1) P/PH, 2) *A column,
23
Chamberlain (1975) uses P and PH to refer to the initial plosives developing from proto-voiced
plosives (conditioning the low register tones). In some SWT dialects like Northern Thai, Tai Lue, and
Shan, most of CT varieties, as well as all NT varieties, initials developing from proto-voiced plosives
are all unaspirated plosives (e.g. p-, t-, k-, and t -), which is represented by his Group P. In many SWT
dialects like Thai, Isan, and Southern Thai, as well as some CT varieties like Zuozhou, Leiping, and
Baoxu, initials developing from proto-voiced plosives are all aspirated plosives (e.g. pʰ-, tʰ-, ɕʰ-, and
t ʰ-), which is represented by his Group PH.
24
1, 2, 3, and 4 here refer to four initial groups allocated in the four horizontal rows of Gedney’s
tone box which has been introduced in Table 4 in §1.2.1. These four groups of initial consonants are
voiceless friction, voiceless unaspirated, glottal, and voiced respectively which condition possible tone
splits among different Tai languages.
74
3) *BCD columns, and 4) B-DL coalescence, and divides Li’s SWT languages into the
chart of four types (two under P and two under PH) as well.
PSWT
P
PH (*A 1-23-4)
*A1-23-4
ABCD 123-4; B=DL
*BCD 123-4; B=DL
BCD 1-23-4; B≠DL
Tse Fang,
Black Tai,
Siamese, Phu Tai,
Lao, Southern Thai.
Tai Mao,
Red Tai,
Neua, Phuan, etc.
Muang Ka.
White Tai,
Lue, Shan,
Yuan, Ahom, etc.
Figure 12 Classification of SWT dialects based on tonal splits (adapted from
Chamberlain 1975: 50)
Edmondson (1994) describes some different tonal behaviors among NT, CT, and SWT. In
his statement, the voiced-low principle, “which basically states that original voiced
initial consonants conditioned low tones and original voiceless initial consonants
conditioned high tones” (Edmondson 1994: 164), becomes a distinctive feature. In the
vast majority of NT and CT languages, the historically high tone set generally possesses a
higher pitch than the historically low tone set, while in SWT, “this situation is
sometimes obscured by subsequent tonal flip-flop of highs and lows”, and becomes a
characteristic for SWT just as what we have reviewed in previous §2.2.2. Though there is
no tonal flip-flop in CT, there is a “slight tendency to tone reversal in the extreme
southwestern locations” of SZ area (Edmondson 1994: 164). Furthermore, Edmondson
uses Gedney’s tone box to analyze the tonology of NZ and SZ, which are of NT and CT
languages respectively (Edmondson 1994: 165-169). The situation of tonal development
in SZ is generally much more complicated than in NZ. “If one can say that the theme of
tone splitting in NZ is voiced-low with variations of pre-glottalized B and C going with
the low, then the theme in SZ is voiced-low with additional changes in the A tone.” This
is also a distinguishing feature between the CT and the NT branches pointed out by
Gedney (1989c cited in Edmondson 1994: 169). From the standard system of pure
voiced-low splitting (such as Guangnan), to quadripartition of splitting in some columns
(such as Daxin), SZ shows all possibilities of splitting among all rows in Gedney’s tone
box. That is to say, in the SZ areas there is a pattern of tonal splitting involving
aspiration and involving pre-glottalization. Therefore, SZ is “a laboratory in which
tonogenetic affinities of various phonological features can be studied” (Edmondson
1994: 176).
75
Based on the previously discussed “voicing alternation” in Tai languages, both Li
(1977: 36-39) and Gedney (1989a) claim that there is a correlation between this
phenomenon and the classification of Tai languages, although their division of Tai
languages are different from each other. Li points out that the tonal behaviors
related to voicing alternation show regularity that CT languages agree in most
instances with SWT languages, but occasionally with NT languages. This supports
his three groups or sub-branches of Tai languages. Gedney also proposes that
reflexes as earlier voiceless onsets in ST (including Li’s SWT and CT) cognates and
those as earlier voiced onsets in NT counterparts support his NT-ST division of Tai
languages. In his point of view, the original phonetic feature of voicing alternation
was “voiced” due to those voicing alternation forms borrowed from Chinese voiced
initial forms. Therefore, these forms have undergone a process of initial devoicing to
become voiceless aspirations and have gone with the Row 1 of his tone box in ST
languages (1989a: 256). The exceptional examples that voicing alternation forms in
some ST varieties (limited to Li’s CT in particular) with “tones reflecting an original
voiced initial” agreeing with NT forms only indicate that “such cases the word failed
for some reason to undergo devoicing of the initial in that particular Southern
language” (1989a: 257).
Rejecting to both Li and Gedney’s claims above, Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 85)
cites items regarding voicing alternation from Luo (1996) to show that “many cases
in which some non-NT dialects also show reflexes of earlier voiced onsets” to
supplement his aforementioned explanation to voicing alternation (see §2.2.3).
However, this could not tenably deny Li and Gedney’s hypotheses since both Li and
Gedney have mentioned about this kind of exceptions and have given their own
explanation for it. Besides, Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s data cited from Luo (1996)
show some unvalued information. For example, tɤjB2 ‘thick, dense’ in Debao (CT)
must be a wrong transcription since it is experientially found that all varieties of
Debao Zhuang (of Yang Zhuang in ISO 639-3) have the form tʰejB1 for this word25,
and this corrected form agrees with the regular rule of voicing alternation among
Tai languages. Further discussions about voicing alternation applying to the
Debao Zhuang (or Debao County Yang Zhuang) is my mother tongue, and the word for ‘dense’ in
my mother tongue is tʰeɔ33 and is transcribed as tʰejB1 according to the notation of this thesis. I have
never found a form t ɔB2 for this word in any other Yang Zhuang varieties or even any other Zhuang
varieties in Debao County, Guangxi due to my personal experience and previous materials provided
by other linguists. For example, in Zhang et al. (1999: 775) the word for ‘thick, dense’ is tʰeɔ5 in Jingxi
(of Yang Zhuang), and tʰeɔ6 in Debao (of Yang Zhuang). Both of these two forms are transcribed as
tʰejB1 in this thesis due to their regular correspondence which will be discussed in the new tone box to
be provided in this thesis.
25
76
classification of Tai languages present as one of the phonological “puzzles” of Cao
Lan called by Gregerson and Edmondson (1998) and the similar tonal phenomenon
in Nung An (Edmondson 2002a) as shown in Table 27 and Table 28 respectively.
Table 27 Voicing alternation among Cao Lan and NT and CT representative
varieties (Gregerson & Edmondson 1989: 160)
Gloss
Cao Lan
Giay (NT)
Tay Cao Bang (CT)
‘excrement’
həi⁴
ʔe⁴
khi³
‘rice’
hau⁴
hau⁴
khau³
Table 28 Voicing alternation among Nung An and the representative varieties
of NT and CT (adapted from Edmondson 2002: 59)
Gloss
Nung An
Giay (NT)
Nung Giang (CT)
‘excrement’
həi⁴
ʔe⁴
khjai³
‘rice’
hau⁴
hau⁴
khau³
‘bowl’
tui⁴
tui⁴
thui³
‘bean’
tu⁶
--
thu⁵
‘bitter’
ham²
ham²
kham¹
‘son-in-law’
kɯi²
kɯi²
khi¹
Both Cao Lan and Nung An are spoken in Northern Vietnam, in which the majorities
being CT speaking groups live. Unlike most languages of NT without aspirated
initials, Cao Lan and Nung An have the same series of aspirated initials like
languages of CT does. Nung An is actually a migration from Long’an (in Guangxi)
(Edmondson 2002: 52), which is traditionally classified as one dialect of YN of CT.
However, unlike other CT languages showing typical Central Tai distinctive features,
Cao Lan and Nung An show more Northern-Like phonological and lexical forms, as
shown as what Gedney states that for a few Central languages having “sporadic
Northern-like forms” (Gedney 1989b: 63). One of the phonological puzzles of these
languages is a number of common items with voicing alternation behaving an
original low tone in Cao Lan, just like all NT languages do, but opposite to behaving
an original high tone in both SWT and CT languages (Gregerson & Edmondson 1998:
160).
In the comparison in the two tables above, both Cao Lan and Nung An clearly go
with Giay, which present the typical feature of “proto-breathy” initials going with
77
low tone in NT as sufficiently discussed in previous reviews. As for that whether Cao Lan
should be put into NT or CT groups, Gregerson and Edmondson (1998: 162) state that
“the Cao Lan people were once a NT people moving southward who stopped and
interfused with a CT speaking group at a time early enough for some late CT rules still to
operate”. As for Nung An, Edmondson however states that it “brought these NT features
with them when they immigrated from further north in an area located on the border
between NT and CT areas” (Edmondson 2002: 61). These two different but ambiguous
claims raise a question of the classification of NT-like purported CT languages (or in
particular YN varieties and Cao Lan), which will be analyzed and discussed in §4.3.2.1
and 5.2.
Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997) proposes that some other specific tonal split patterns can
be treated as an important criterion of the classification of Tai branches. She firstly
provides a research on tonal splits in Dai Tho (Dai Zhuang in ISO 639-3) and Tai Tho
(Debao County Yang Zhuang in ISO 639-3) of CT in Yunnan and Guangxi of China. A
special tonal behavior of these languages is that proto-voiceless sonorants did not go
with aspirated plosives like other Tai languages did, but went with voiceless unaspirated
plosives. This tonal behavior of Wenma (Dai Zhuang) and Debao has actually been
mentioned in the discussion of more pending issues on Tai tones in §1.2.2 and has been
partly shown in Table 7. However, her treatment is to put proto-voiceless sonorants into
Row 2 of Gedney’s tone box to be with voiceless unaspirated plosives, and to keep only
proto-aspirated plosives in Row 1 (Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997: 194). This is different
from Gedney’s original tone box which has Row 1 containing both voiceless aspirated
plosives and sonorants, and is different from Liao and Shen’s revised Tai tone box which
separates more horizontal rows to solve the problems of more tonal split patterns. Just
as mentioned in §2.2.3 that she argues that the main factor of primary tonal split is “the
merging of proto-voiceless sonorants with proto-voiced sonorants” in Tho (Theraphan LThongkum 1997: 215) which includes her Dai Tho and Tai Tho, the tonal behaviors of
these two languages show a specific pattern different from other Tai languages. Based on
this, she tends to support Li’s (1977) tripartite division of Tai since she believes that the
CT sub-branch has “its own interesting history of phonological development”, and “it
should be kept apart instead of being grouped together with the Southwestern branch”
(Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997: 215). In addition, Pranee Kullavanijaya and Theraphan
L-Thongkum (1998) propose a sub-classification of CT to divide “Nong-Tay” and “Budai”
by using tonal splits of PT tone A as the main factor, especially the tonal development
conditioned by the dental consonant clusters *tr- and *tʰr- in Li’s reconstruction (1977).
This time Dai Tho in Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997) is put into their Budai group of CT,
of which Nong-Tay which presents is a sister.
78
However, Johnson (2011: 48-55) calls both the specific group of “Tho” from
Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997) and the sub-classification of CT from Pranee
Kullavanijaya and Theraphan L-Thongkum (1998) in question. He points out that
although Dai Zhuang and Debao County Yang Zhuang which belong to Theraphan LThongkum’s Tho group seem to present a similar tonal split patterns to each other in
PT A tone, they have totally different tonal split patterns in other tonal categories.
The overall phonological systems of these two languages do not suggest that they
have more shared development between these two groups than between other CT
groups. He also points out that some “preliminary analysis of recent data from an
SIL International East Asia group and Guangxi Region Language Commission survey
of the Zhuang languages” has indicated linguistic grounds for grouping Debao
Zhuang together with the Yang Zhuang of Jingxi County, and this agrees with the
grouping of Debao together with Jingxi in previous studies on Zhuang dialects in the
Sinitic circles like Wei & Qin (1980) and Zhang et al. (1999). Therefore, he feels that
“the grouping of Debao and Tianbao, together with Dai Zhuang, is not a strong
argument” (2011: 54). That is to say, from Johnson’s viewpoint some tonal split
patterns conditioned by some specific conditioning factors like aspiration cannot
tenably suggest being a criterion for classifying a sub-group of Tai languages.
In this chapter, a review of the literature related to historical and comparative
method applied to Tai languages and Tai tonology is drawn on to provide an
overview of the theoretical framework for this study. Historical and comparative
method is the basis of tone data analysis from forty-two Tai varieties. There were
numerous previous studies on Tai tonology, and they are divided into four parts,
namely tonogenesis, tonal splits and mergers, the conditioning factors of tonal splits
and Tai classification related to tonological issues. The first three parts have close
correlation to Tai tonal development scheme, which is the first objectives and is the
main body of this thesis. In addition, the conditioning factors of tonal splits actually
constitute the theoretical basis of the design of Tai tone box, which is one of the
objectives of this thesis. Finally, Tai classification related to tonological issues is the
precursor of Tai classification based on tonology, one of the targets of this study.
Based on the previous studies, the following chapter describes the principles of
methodology, and three chapters after that successively probe into the three objects
of this studies, namely diachronic hierarchies of Tai tonal development, Tai
classification based on tonology, and a new perspective of Tai tone box.
79
Methodology, data and notation
Given the objectives outlined in §1.2, the problems discussed in §1.2.3, and the
hypotheses in §1.2.4, it is imperative to assess the assumptions about Tai tonology.
Therefore, this thesis approaches the reconstruction of tones in PT and the
diachronic developmental stages of tones in Tai languages by using a large set of
tone data from 42 Tai varieties. This section mainly presents a brief description of
the theoretical basis of the methods to test the hypotheses of this thesis, and falls
into four parts, viz. general comparative method, library research, fieldwork, and
notation.
As mentioned in §2.1, since Tai tonology is based on the study of tone changes
among different Tai varieties, this study has to be based on the theory of historical
linguistics which is first developed in the 19th century and mainly deals with
language change among Indo-European languages (Campbell 2004). Several aspects
of historical and comparative linguistics such as linguistic change, borrowing
loanwords, comparative method and linguistic reconstruction, linguistic
classification, and areal linguistics are emphasized in the discussion of this thesis.
When talking about linguistic change, we have to consider how to explain this
phenomenon, or what makes the change occur on sounds, meaning, lexicon, and
grammar. In this thesis, the main linguistic changes that we will be concerned with
are sound changes especially the changes of tones, such as non-tonal prosodic words
in PT generated tones, tonal value changes presenting like original high tone goes to
low tone in many varieties in SWT, or different tonal splits on those prosodic words
with specific initials. With respect to the discussion of semantic change, in this
thesis, cognates having derived different meanings among different modern Tai
varieties are involved in the discussions for interpreting the tone changes actually
occurred to the same prosodic word in PT. For example, the word for ‘river’ in CT
80
and NT varieties and the word for ‘wharf’ in SWT varieties both ascend to the same
proto-form *da:B in PT, and these “two words” are actually the results of semantic
changes from the same root and have to combine to be the same lexical item in the
research wordlist, for interpreting the sound changes especially the tone changes of
this prosodic word among different modern Tai varieties. In short, the discussions of
sound changes occupy the main domain of linguistic change in this thesis, while
semantic changes often supplement to sound change in related discussions.
The causes of linguistic change includes internal causes and external causes, just like
the discussion of the literature of linguistic change in §2.1.1. With respect to use of
internal factor, in this thesis, a sound change normally is considered to be due to an
internal factor, such as a tonal split is normally treated to be conditioned by a
specific group of initials in history. However, it does not mean that external factors
can be ignored even we have already known about that the factor was of internal on
the one hand since there might be external factor on the other hand. For example,
when talking about tonogenesis of Tai languages, many scholars have claimed there
were no tones in PT and Tai tones produced due to the avoidance of meaning
confusion because of the loss of final consonants which should be treated as an
internal factor, but we still cannot ignore that the loss of final consonants might be
as a result of being influenced by similar linguistic phenomenon on languages
outside of Tai such as Middle Chinese due to areal linguistic method, and this factor
is obviously external.
The role of borrowing or loanwords is also a factor that cannot be ignored in
applying the comparative method. Certain lexicons that are shared widely among
Tai varieties might be loans from Old Chinese, and many lexicons shared among CT
and NT varieties might also be loans from Middle Chinese and modern Chinese
dialects (Prapin Manomaivibool 1975, 1976; Zhang et al. 1999: 246-286). Old
Chinese loans give a hint for how tones developed in Tai. For example, except few
loans with aberrant tonal development, in Tai languages most of loanwords
borrowed from Old Chinese present a very regular correspondence between Tai
tonal categories *A, *B, *C, and *D and Chinese tonal categories yīn (level), qù
(departing), shǎng (rising), and rù (entering) respectively (Li 1976; Prapin
Manomaivibool 1975). Since some scholars such as Norman (1988: 52-57) have
tried to prove that tones qù and shǎng in Sinitic languages produced due to the loss
of the fricative and glottal codas in Old Chinese, it is speculated that when PT
81
borrowed words from Old Chinese, the sound correspondence has to be arranged
with the same or similar traits on coda, meaning in PT Tone B was with fricative
codas, and tonal category C was with glottal codas. The tonal genesis of PT must be
very similar to that in Chinese, meaning in the early stage of PT, before tonal splits
occurred the loss of fricative and glottal codas produced Tones B and C respectively.
The comparative method and the reconstruction of PT segments especially prototones is crucial to this study. Many linguists such as Li (1977), Gedney (1989[1972])
and Edmonson and Solnit (1997) have discussed in great detail the PT tonal system,
and have also provided a successful reconstruction of tonal categories in PT by
applying comparative methodology, as the discussion in §1.2.1. In this thesis, the
comparative method is mainly applied to the discussion of Tai tonal development.
For example, the “proto-breathy sounds” reconstructed by Liang and Zhang (1996)
and further discussed by Liao and Shen (2012) is based on the so-called voicing
alternation due to the comparison of different direction of tonal split between NT
and ST varieties. In the related discussion is in §1.2.3, we have known that these
“proto-breathy” initials regularly condition tonal split going with original low tone
with those developed from proto-voiced initials in all NT dialects, but condition
tonal split going with original high tone with those developed from proto-voiceless
initials in the vast majority of ST. This specific tonal developmental phenomenon led
to the addition of an extra horizontal row in Liao and Shen’s revised Tai tone box
(2012). On the other hand, when we compare more data from NT and ST varieties, a
contrastive tonal developmental phenomenon is observed, and that is some specific
cognates such as ‘man’ and ‘to blow’ having tonal split going with original low tone
with those developed from proto-voiced initials in all ST varieties (e.g. tɕa:jA2 ‘man’
and patDS2 ‘to blow’ in Jingxi County Yang Zhuang; tɕʰa:jA2 ‘man’ and pʰatDS2 ‘to
blow’ in Thai), but condition tonal split going with original high tone with those
developed from proto-voiceless initials in all NT varieties (e.g. θa:jA1 ‘man’ and patDS1
‘to blow’ in Wuming Zhuang). The specific condition of this kind of tonal
developmental phenomenon is still unstudied, and we need further discussion to
prove the causes of it by comparative method (cf. §4.3.2.1).
When discussing the comparative method, the regularity of correspondence among
data from different Tai varieties is emphasized. However, aberrant development
lacking regular tonal correspondence in some specific examples should not be
ignored. Liao (2013) proposes that the proto-Tai reconstruction of the word
82
‘maternal grandmother’ should be *ta:jA, which presenting a total irregularity in
modern NT, CT and SWT varieties to reflect proto-form *ta:jB, *ta:jA and *na:jA
respectively. Liao interprets that the irregularity is due to sound changes which
served to dissimilate this word from the otherwise homophonous word ‘to die’ in NT
and SWT varieties. This proto-form is supported by historical evidence and other
non-Tai languages in the TK family. Similar phenomena like tone changes by
analogy with words that are semantically similar should be also noted. For example,
in my checklist of lexicon for investigating tone data for this thesis, the word for
‘maternal grandfather’ is designated in the column of *A tone since this tone is
reflected by the vast majority of Tai varieties, while several CT varieties present this
word as B1 tone which is different from other Tai varieties. As what has mentioned
in §2.1.1, Liao (2013) has also discussed this phenomenon and has proposed that the
original Tone A1 of the word ta:B1 ‘maternal grandfather’ in these CT varieties has
been replaced with the tone of the word for ‘maternal grandmother’ ta:jB1 in these
varieties, and is a case of analogical change with words of paired semantic contents
in these CT languages, such as in Debao and Jingxi Yang Zhuang ta:B1 ta:jB1 ‘maternal
grandparents, wife’s parents’. In short, these phenomena give a clue that when we
apply to the comparative method, we should not ignore some external factors may
hide behind irregularities of sound correspondence among different daughter
languages.
Language classification especially how to sub-group Tai varieties based on tonology
is one of the main topics of this thesis. Campbell (2004: 188-191) gives a specific
determination of subgrouping methodology as being an internal classification which
determines the most closely related sister languages of the same language family,
and points out that “the only generally accepted criterion for subgrouping is shared
innovation” (Campbell 2004: 190), which is a linguistic change departing from a
specific trait of proto-language and is shared by the same subset of the daughter
languages. Because an innovation normally occurs in a single daughter language and
subsequently diversifies into daughter languages of its own, normally this innovation
is not shared by languages in other sub-group of the same family (Campbell 2004:
191).
When applying to Tai classification based on tonology, the criterion is also given to
determine which tonal innovations are shared and are not shared by which
languages, and then to subgroup different subsets of daughter languages. A typical
83
Tai classification based on tonology is displayed by Chamberlain (1975). He suggests
the Tai languages should be divided earlier into two branches, Northern and SouthCentral, and in the latter the South-Central branch was divided into two groups, P
and PH, and the further classification under P and PH should follow the basis of the
tonal splits to finally divide Li’s SWT into four types, as in the discussion in §2.2.4.
Whether Chamberlain’s Tai classification can stand scrutiny, the criterion of shared
innovation is indeed used to determine his viewpoint. Chamberlain’s claim will also
be tested in this thesis since one of the hypotheses of this thesis is to determine the
Tai classification based on tonology.
The method of areal linguistics which is related to borrowing is emphasized here by
the term ‘diffusion area’. It refers to a linguistic phenomenon which describes
languages within a certain geographical area coming to share certain structural
features including loanwords and “shared elements of phonological, morphological
or syntactic structure” due to language contact, such as borrowing and diffusion
(Campbell 2004: 330-331).
An example of areal linguistics applying to Tai languages can be analyzed in the
case of Gedney’s (1989b) viewpoint about the difference between Li’s (1977) CT and
SWT languages. Although Gedney suggests CT and SWT groups “form a continuous
dialect area, with only gradual transition throughout and no real language boundary
anywhere” (Gedney 1989b: 66), he still admits that there are basic differences
between CT and SWT when being determined by phonological and lexical criteria,
such as CT languages do share certain words with NT but not found in SWT, and CT
languages are conspicuous for tending to retain bizarre reflexes of original
consonant clusters like the words ‘eye’ and ‘to die’ with initial t- in all SWT and NT
languages but with such initials as tʰ-, h-, pʰ- or pʰj- in CT languages (Gedney 1989b:
64). If CT and SWT really form a continuous dialect area with no real language
boundary, the basic differences between these two sub-groups can be interpreted by
areal feature: Since CT and SWT languages have geographically separated from each
other for a long time, the “bizarre reflexes” of original consonant clusters retained in
CT languages can be supposed to be an areal trait due to independent development
within their own area for a long period, and that is why SWT languages do not share
this areal feature even though they are phonologically and lexically much more
closely related to CT languages than NT to CT languages; and since CT and NT
languages are still adjacent neighbors, CT languages may share certain inherited Tai
84
words with NT, but SWT languages might have dropped these inherited Tai words
due to lexical shift being an areal trait within their area, or CT languages just
directly borrowed these words from NT languages due to language contact, and no
matter which reason is true, they both can interpret that why we cannot find these
certain words in SWT languages.
Likewise, when we talk about different tonal behaviors in different sub-groups of
Tai, areal linguistics can explain many causes and reasons. For example, in the
subsequent discussions, we will see although the CT and SWT sub-groups show a
unified trait in the level of primary tonal development which is different from that
of NT, the CT sub-group shows a much more complicated secondary tonal
development while the SWT sub-group does not due to their own independent
development forming different areal feature.
Library research is necessary for completing the literature review for this study. The
literature review has been done before doing data collection. The literature review is
laid the emphasis on previous research of Tai tones including Chinese linguistics
materials. In the past Chinese materials of Tai tonogenesis, Tai tonal development
and other related research are easily neglected because of the language barrier.
Therefore, I placed particular emphasis on Chinese materials which acquire more CT
and NT linguistic information since most CT varieties and the vast majority of NT
Tai varieties are found in China. The details of this process have been listed in
Chapter 2.
For doing a comprehensive study of Tai tonal development as much as possible, it is
important to minimize gaps in our knowledge of tones in modern Tai varieties.
Therefore, in this thesis forty-two Tai varieties which are identified as
representatives of SWT (seven), CT (seventeen), YN (five) and NT (thirteen)
languages respectively are chosen for data analysis. These are listed in §3.3.1, and
are also filled in the chart of the information of Tai varieties selected attached in
Appendix D, as well as in the map of the Tai varieties investigated with the same
varieties numbering shown in Appendix D. Except some languages have been fully
researched, such as Thai, Northern Thai, and Wuming Zhuang in published
materials, the data of the other varieties is mainly based on my own fieldwork
research for this thesis.
85
The selection of the Tai varieties investigated is based on tonal diversity. Previous
studies have obtained much achievement on the research of tones in SWT. This
thesis relatively puts more attention to the tones of the less studied CT and NT
languages (including the YN group whose classification as CT or NT is open to
debate). Within the forty-two Tai varieties, the number of the CT languages is
seventeen to be most numerous. This is because when comparing with tonal
development in NT languages, the situation of tonal development in CT languages is
“generally far more diverse and complex” (Edmondson 1992a: 169). In the two
maps of the Tai varieties investigated in Appendix D, seventeen languages including
fifteen CT varieties and two NT varieties are concentrated in the narrow triangle
zone which is on the southwestern corner of Guangxi, since that area contains one of
the most tonal diversities in Tai languages.
The tone data from these forty-two Tai varieties have been collected by the wordlist
based on Liao & Shen’s (2012) revised Tai tone box (see §1.2.2), which is revised
from Gedney’s tone box (1989[1972]). For investigating these Tai varieties, the
wordlist with 195 words is designed as shown in Appendix B. The wordlist takes the
following two checklists as the original templates: Gedney’s checklist for his Tai tone
box (1989[1972]: 202-204) and the supplemental checklist made by Dr. Phinnarat
Akharawatthanakun for collecting tone data in Thai dialects in AL701 Field Methods
in Linguistics course, Linguistics Department, Payap University. Nevertheless, the
wordlist in this thesis has abundant supplements for the higher precision of attesting
the tonal splits and mergers. Certain words (normally five to seven words) are
designated for one box for insuring the confidence of establishing the tone for that
box. However, when there are not enough words found in one box, such as in B1UC, C1-UC, and DL1/2 boxes where I had only found 1-2 words for appropriately
investigating before going to the fieldwork, these boxes will be designated to have
the same blanks as other boxes (other tones) fully filled in with lexical items on the
same horizontal rows. For example, although B1-UC and C1-UC boxes have only two
lexical items (‘to hunt’ and ‘cage (box)’ for B1-UC as well as ‘near’ and ‘CLF for
stone’ for C1-UC) found respectively, both of them contain five blanks since in the
same horizontal level box of A tone (A1-UC box), there are plenty lexical items are
found, of which five items (‘eye’, ‘to die’, ‘gizzard’, ‘cucumber’, and ‘to put up’) are
selected to be put in the wordlist. These remainder blanks in boxes B1-UC and C1UC are reserved for filling in when I find that other appropriate words should be put
in them in the future.
86
The criterion for designing this wordlist is that all etyma selected should be
commonly found in all the three Tai branches. For example, some etyma commonly
found in SWT languages like tu:C1-U ‘cabinet’ and kʰuətDL1-A ‘bottle’ in Thai are never
found in CT, NT as well as the debatable affiliation YN varieties. Therefore, they are
not considered to be listed in the preferential items in the wordlist of this thesis,
even though they are commonly designated to be attested items in some checklist of
Gedney’s tone box in the fieldworks of SWT. Nevertheless, when an etymon is not
found in a specific Tai variety investigated, an optional substitute or a succedaneous
item which was provisionally designated at that time will be used, like wa:wB2 ‘kite’
(in Thai) is sometimes used for investigating SWT even though it is not found in CT
and NT.
The implement of data collection was accomplished during my own fieldwork
carried out on a number of Tai varieties from China, Thailand, and Vietnam.
Altogether I accomplished fieldwork on thirty Tai varieties. The data collection
periods are as below: During March 2012 – April 2012, I finished data collection
from five varieties of Napo County Yang Zhuang, Debao County Yang Zhuang, and
Min Zhuang around my home areas in Guangxi; In October 2012, I recorded five Tai
varieties of Thai, Northern Thai, Northeastern Thai, Southern Thai, and Shan in
Chiang Mai, Thailand; When I studied English course in the University of Arkansas,
the U.S. from January to March 2013, I got to know a fellow who is a Tai native
speaker from Central Vietnam, and asked for his permission to record his Tai
variety, which is called Tai Dam by himself and is of SWT sub-group; From April –
May 2013, I carried out the second time fieldwork to my home area Guangxi, China
and finished data collection from eighteen varieties of NT and CT; In June 2014, I
carried out fieldwork on the Tai Lue variety of Rong Maet Village, Jun District,
Phayao Province of Thailand; After thesis proposal defense, during the winter of
2013, I found that tone data collection for SWT varieties should also be done by
using the same wordlist with the one used for CT and NT varieties but not by the
previous wordlists with only 60-90 words used for SWT only, for more convincingly
interpreting the new revised tone box, I rerecorded tone data from Thai, Northern
Thai, Southern Thai, Isan, Shan, and Tai Lue varieties. At the end of August, 2014, I
carried out my last fieldwork for this thesis on the Daxin Baoxu variety of Zuojiang
Zhuang in Guangxi. The detailed information of languages investigated by myself is
also filled in the chart of the information of Tai varieties selected is attached in
Appendix D.
87
Certain tone data of some Tai varieties are also referenced to other research
materials, which are listed on the chart of the information of Tai varieties selected
attached in Appendix C. For those Tai varieties only referenced to other research
materials, I could only fill in the lexical items that are found in those materials, and
left the symbol of blank space “--” when that cognate was not recorded.
The transcriptions are all in IPA standard (International Phonetic Association 2015),
and tones are transcribed by using pitch level and contours based on the Chao 5level pitch scale (Chao 1930), which has been previewed in detail in §2.2.
Exceptionally, short tones in Chao’s system (tone letter 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5) are
transliterated as long level tones (11, 22, 33, 44, and 55) to avoid the superposition
of tonal categories (tones 1, 2, 3…10) in the Sinitic circles. In this situation, short
tones can only be identified according to the transcriptions of short vowels, such as
tɕat⁵⁵ ‘seven’ in Yang Zhuang varieties. For example, the six tones on live syllables in
the Urban variety of Debao County Yang Zhuang contains one falling (31), one
rising with glottalization (24ʔ), two levels (55 and 33), and two circumflexes (353
and 213ʔ). In addition, three level tones (including short tones) on dead syllables are
55, 33, and 11 respectively (for details see L8 in Appendix C).
For acquiring the accurate tone data from each language, the selection of LRPs is
limited to mother tongue speakers of the Tai varieties collected. I tried to select LRPs
all above 30 years old and at least three LRPs for each variety for acquiring stable
tone data, but sometimes failed to achieve this target because of limited time and
the complex geographical situation of some remote locations, even though I have
finished four times of fieldwork trips. When I met such a situation, I also guaranteed
the selection of 1-2 LRPs being at least above 20 years old, and having wholesome
vocal organs. The information of LRPs and each Tai variety is listed in the charts of
language information in Appendix C.
Data analysis has been done according to the general historical and comparative
linguistics methods, which are discussed from §3.1.1 to §3.1.5. The tonal
development has been analyzed mainly by comparing different tonal behaviors
among different Tai varieties. When a tonal split or merger occurs, the condition of
this tonal behavior such as initial feature, vowel length on dead syllable, or other
related factors need to be determined. If there are two tones separating according to
a conditional feature within one row, and that they are phonetically distinct in a
certain number of Tai varieties, an extra row of the tone box needs to be added.
88
Tone analysis in this thesis has mainly been done by perception of auditory sense;
however, acoustic analysis is used to analyze tone data when it is necessary. If the
tones are phonetically quite distinct, e.g. falling compared to rising, then that is
clear evidence that they are different tones. Acoustic analysis by using PRAAT
(Version 5.1.34) is only applied for analyzing tones from two different rows of tone
box that are almost identical and the LRPs claim there is a difference in
pronunciation between these two tones. If examples from X tone and Y tone always
show a fixed high-low frequency in Hz respectively in PRAAT analysis, they should
be separated into two tones even though they are phonetically quite similar to each
other, otherwise they should be treated as a merger. A typical example of tone
analysis by having recourse to PRAAT is the definitions among tones B1-U, B2, and
B1-G in the Jingxi Hurun variety of Yang Zhuang (cf. L11 in Appendix C).
In the charts of tonal split and merger patterns of each variety (cf. Appendix C),
some tones in smooth syllables and in dead syllables present different tonal values
but are treated as allotones of the same tone, such as B1 (242) and DL1 (24) in
Debao Dongling (L32, NT) in Table 34 (in 4.1.3.3.1, also cf. L32 in Appendix C),
based on their similarities of the pitch on the main part. When a tone in smooth
syllables is a complex-contour or so-called circumflex tones (including dipping e.g.
213, convex e.g. 453, delayed falling e.g. 553, and delayed rising e.g. 112), the
criterion of determining its allotone in the dead syllables is based on the
homologous pitch of the front half part of the circumflex on smooth syllables. This is
because the back half part of the circumflex is always dropped off in a non-final
position of a carrier sentence in normal speech, whereby this front part must be
treated as the main contour of the tone. It means, 242 in Debao Dongling Zhuang
actually presents 24 in a non-final position in normal speech, to be the same as its
allotone 24 in dead syllables. This criterion of determining allotones will be
throughout implemented.
Although this thesis mainly analyzes data collected and described by myself, certain
data is incorporated from diverse sources by different researchers. For facilitating
comparisons different Tai varieties, it is necessary to establish notational
conventions. All modern Tai varieties forms as well as reconstructed PT forms that
appear in this thesis except those cited have been re-transcribed according to the
IPA standard (International Phonetic Association 2015) as below.
89
1. IPA symbols are all in italic rather than in “[ ]”, e.g. Nong Zhuang tʃʰu:A1 ‘ear’
rather than [tʃʰu:A1].
2. The dash “-” is put before an IPA symbol to indicate a following segment, e.g. -a:
means a low-mid unrounded vowel following some consonant, and -m means a
bilabial nasal final consonant or coda; while it is put after an IPA symbol to
indicate a followed segment, e.g. m- means a bilabial nasal initial consonant
followed by some vowel, and a- means a low-mid unrounded vowel followed by
some coda.
3. The prepositive asterisk “*-” means its following form is a reconstructed form in
a proto-language, e.g. PT *pajA ‘to go’ rather than pajA.
4. Tone value markers for modern Tai varieties: tones are transcribed by using
superscripted pitch level and contours based on the Chao 5-level pitch scale
(Chao 1930), like the regulation of transcribing tone data in §3.3.1. The tone
markers must be tightly put next to the syllable transcribed by IPA. Creaky tones
are marked under the superscripted pitch value, while glottalized tones are
marked by a superscripted -ʔ next to the superscripted pitch values, e.g. Debao
Urban (L8) ma353 ‘dog’, ma:213ʔ ‘horse’, and Yizhou Suogan (L36) paj51̰ ‘to go’.
5. Tonal category markers for PT and modern Tai varieties: PT tone categories *A,
*B, *C, and *D and modern Tai tone categories A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, C2, DL1, DL2,
DS1 and DS2 are italic and keep the normal capital forms in the sentences of the
text, but are shrunk to the upper right corner when following the transcriptions
of lexical items. The tone markers must be tightly put next to the syllable
transcribed by IPA; when the PT tone markers are transcribed with consonant
and vowels, the prepositive asterisk should be tightly put before the initial
consonant, e.g. PT *da:B ‘river’; Debao ta:B1 ‘river’ rather than *da: B and ta: B1.
6. Superscripted symbols: aspiration is ⁻ʰ rather than a normal -h, and pre-
glottalization is ˀ⁻ rather than a normal ʔ- except the situation of PT, e.g. Jingxi
tʰa:A1 ‘eye’ rather than tha:A1, Nalong Zhongyi *ʰla:A1 ‘to seek’ rather than *hla:A1,
Debao ˀja:A1 ‘medicine’ rather than ʔja:A1, but PT *hna:A ‘thick’ and
*ʔjɯaA ‘medicine’.
7. The palatal semivowel is transcribed as j rather than y which actually represents
another sound – the high front rounded vowel, e.g. Debao ja:B2 ‘husband’s
mother’ rather than ya:B2.
90
8. Final semivowels of a syllable are treated as glides -j, -ɥ, -ɰ, and -w, e.g. Jingxi
kai45 ‘chicken’, mou53 ‘pig’, Wuming θaɯ41 ‘to buy’ (cf. Zhang et al. 1999) and
Debao ɬøy²¹³ ‘to buy’ (cf. Liao 2010) are re-transcribed as kaj45, mow53, θaɰ41, and
ɬøɥ²¹³ respectively.
9. Non-IPA symbols should be re-transcribed as IPA symbols: č- represent the
voiceless palatal affricate and re-transcribed as tɕ-; similarly, ǰ-, š- and ȵ- are re-
transcribed as dʑ-, ɕ- and ɲ- respectively.
10. Long vowels are indicated by “-:” rather than double vowels, e.g. Daxin kʰa:A1
‘leg’ rather than kʰaaA1.
Note that the notation related to the usage of IPA described above only applies to
forms in TK language family. When we reference to source from languages from
another family, the forms of IPA are only transcribed in their original notation.
Besides, in this thesis Chinese Pinyin which means Chinese Phonetic Alphabet
formulated by the Chinese government from 1955 is used generally for those names
of person and place, title of Chinese works in th literature, and some terminology
with a Sinitic linguistic source. But some names have been well-known by academic
circles for a long time, such as Li Fang-Kuei, and the Tai dialect of Lungchow
researched by Li (1947; 1977), I will only follow the original notation rather than
Chinese Pinyin which presents them as Li Fanggui and Longzhou respectively.
However, if I reference Zhang et al. (1999), one of the Southern Zhuang dialects will
be cited as Longzhou following the notation of Chinese Pinyin since the book is a
new publication in contemporary China. Last, although simplified Chinese
characters are officially and generally used in Mainland China, the Chinese
characters in this thesis are only transcribed in traditional characters, because many
discussions of Ancient Chinese loans in Tai varieties needs to regard etymology
linking to the original forms of Chinese character.
91
Diachronic hierarchies of Tai tonal development
This chapter discusses the process of Tai tonal development, from the arising of
tones, tonal splits including the conditioning factors of tonal splits, to the trends of
Tai tonal development in some specific modern Tai varieties.
Since tonogenesis or the arising of tones in Tai languages is viewed as the first step
of tonal development of these languages, it is necessary to give a full screening of
different arguments of Tai tonogenesis when we discuss the very first scales and
levels of Tai tonal development. Although all modern TK languages have welldeveloped tonal systems, Tai tonogenesis cannot avoid being explored after the
tonogenesis hypothesis applying to their surrounding languages, because Tai tones
and tones in Chinese, Vietnamese and Miao-Yao languages are structurally identical
and show an evident correspondence. This is because those tonal languages like
Vietnamese have genetically related non-tonal languages like Mon-Khmer languages,
as shown in Vietnamese tonogenesis presented by Haudricourt (1954b) in §2.2.1.
Moreover, in Sino-Tibetan Language Family, some languages especially the Sinitic
languages have well-developed tonal system, but more languages like most TibetoBurman languages have less-developed tonal system, and some languages like Amdo
Tibetan even have no tonal development at all (Dong 2014: 38).
Tai tonogenesis due to the loss of syllabic stress of original Austronesian
multisyllables proposed by Ni (1990) reviewed in §2.2.1 is rejected in this thesis.
This is because the relationship between TK languages and Austronesian languages
is still unclear, although recent studies such as Weera Ostapirat (2013a) also support
the Austro-Tai hypothesis proposed by Benedict (1942). Even though the Austro-Tai
hypothesis can be established, the arising of tones in PT should be on the stage of
monosyllabic or sesquisyllabic structures due to the previous studies on PT
reconstruction, and the time should be far later than the time when the language
structure changed from multisyllables to monosyllables or sesquisyllables.
Except Ni’s proposal, the following points are commonly agreed within the different
points of view of PT non-tonal elements or tonal features in the literature review in
92
§2.2.1) in a specific period PT had a tonal system containing four tonal categories
*A, *B, *C, and *D; 2) proto-tone *A was a modal tone without affiliated
consonantal endings or particular voice quality; 3) proto-tone *B and*C arose by
dropping some specific syllable finals or specific phonation voice; 4) proto-tone *D
was on checked syllables without tonal contrast as they are reflected in the vast
majority of the Tai daughter languages. In contrast, the phonetic characteristics
especially the voice qualities or affiliated consonantal endings of PT tones *B and *C
lack of a full agreement among different proposals. Table 29 summarizes some of
the disagreement.
Table 29 Disagreement on the phonetic characteristics of PT tones *B and *C
Proto-tones
*B
Sources
Pitch height Contour
*C
Vowel
Voice
Pitch
duration
quality
height
Contour
Vowel
Voice
duration
quality
Pittayawat
Pittayaporn
rising
long
creaky
high
falling
short
no
no
--
-h
no
no
--
-ʔ
no
no
--
-h
no
no
--
-ʔ
--
--
--
-ʔ
--
--
--
creaky
(2009: 271)
Liang & Zhang
(1996: 815)
Gedney
(1989b: 89; 91)
Sagart
(1988: 89)
glottal
low
constriction
Before the comparison of the four arguments about the Tai tonogenesis process and
the phonetic shapes of PT tones in the chart above, the following two clarifications
are preferentially enumerated.
First of all, the process of Tai tonogenesis should be treated as the very first step of
the arising of tones in PT. This clarification rejects the treatment that tone in Tai is
an inherent feature and cannot develop from non-tonal elements. The very first
proposal of non-tonal structure at a specific period of PT is provided by Matisoff
(1973), and it is supported by the tonogenesis hypothesis that all other tonal
languages surrounding TK language family have been commonly proposed to
develop from non-tonal languages, such as Vietnamese (Haudricourt 1954b; Matisoff
93
1973; Benedict 1972b, 1973), Chinese (Haudricourt 1954a; Pulleyblank 1962;
Benedict 1972b, 1973; Matisoff 1973; Norman 1989), and Miao-Yao (Matisoff 1973;
Benedict 1972b, 1973), and for the early period of PT this cannot be a single
exception. That is to say, typologically the treatment of tone in Tai as an inherent
feature and cannot develop from non-tonal elements is shown to be isolated.
Although Li (1977: 24) believes that “it is impossible to recover the final consonants
that are assumed to have been dropped” to give the arising of tone in PT period, he
has furthermore suggested that if the proposal of tonogenesis is tenable, for Tai “it
must refer to a stage of the language prior to Proto-Tai” and the origin of PT tones
may have been in the “Pre-Tai” period (Li 1977: 24). Here we still consider the
suspected non-tonal period to be of an early stage of PT instead of Li’s “Pre-Tai”
period, since we have reviewed that in most previous studies, PT tones are always
considered to arise by dropping the tonal-source finals -ʔ and -s/-h, or by reducing
specific phonations like creakiness or glottal constriction.
Second, at a later period of PT non-tonal structure of the earlier stage should have
changed to tonal structures due to the decaying or the weakening of the specific
consonantal finals, like -ʔ and -h. The typological evidence for this statement is
provided by the previous studies especially tonogenesis of Vietnamese, Chinese, and
Miao-Yao, although different studies have different points of view about the arising
of Tai tones and the tonal features in PT, like the weakening of marked phonations,
final glottal constriction and creakiness, suggested by Pittayawat Pittayaporn
(2009). We will discuss below to demonstrate that the earliest origin of tones should
have been some specific consonantal finals, which we treat as “cause” giving rise to
tones, instead of those marked phonations or voice qualities, which we treat as the
“effect” triggered by the cause. The most direct reason need to be emphasized is that
the method of tonogenesis suggested by Matisoff (1973) is based on a hypothesis
proposing that tone is a secondary development which induced by the decaying of
the consonantal system especially syllable finals, and this method has been applied
to Vietnamese, Chinese, and Miao-Yao, which have very clear tonal correspondence
to Tai. In this thesis we only focus on the evidence from the sound rules of historical
Chinese loanwords (OC and MC loans) in Tai languages. These Chinese loans present
a clear regular tonal correspondence to the source language, and this indicates that
to a certain extent, the non-tonal elements in the earlier period of PT and the
phonetic shape of tones in the later period of PT should have been similar to those
in OC and MC respectively.
94
The phonetic shapes of PT tones suggested by Liang and Zhang (1996) and Gedney
(1989b) have the same shape, but based on different evidence. Their common
agreement is that PT might not have been a tonal language, but might have a
structure with four types of syllable finals, namely plain, fricative, glottal, and nonglottal stops. This might be why they do not aim to reconstruct the non-consonantal
phonetic shapes of tones like pitch height, contour, and vowel duration. Another
reason is that most previous studies do not reconstruct the phonetic shapes of PT
tones because of the difficulty. Just as Haudricourt states that “once it is established,
the tonal system evolves without regard for its old etymological pitch levels”
(Haudricourt 1972: 63), Fu (1995: 85) has pointed out that tonal systems have a life
of their own, and “there seem to be no guide-lines for development after tone split,
with different dialects changing in different directions”. Although the four tonal
categories of PT have been reconstructed based on the regular tonal correspondence
among Tai daughter languages, the specific phonetic characteristics of PT tones
which include pitch height, contour and vowel length seems very difficult to
reconstruct by the comparative method because the absence of unity in phonetic
shape in Tai tones among different daughter languages faces a huge challenge of
reconstructing. Just as Li (1977: 24) points out the difficulty and only gives the four
tonal categories without the specific phonetic shape, most Tai linguists do not aim to
reconstruct the phonetic shape of PT tones.
As what has been reviewed in §2.2.1, in spite of the difficulty Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009) has provided a breakthrough on reconstructing the phonetic
shape of PT tones by internal reconstruction in specific daughter languages and
comparative method among those selected Tai varieties, and has given the shapes
“low, rising, long, and creaky” to PT Tone *B and “high, falling, short, and glottal
constriction” to PT Tone *C. However, we have indicated in the second clarification
above that in different periods PT must have had the similar non-tonal and similar
tonal shapes with their counterparts in OC and MC respectively. In other words,
unlike previous studies that treat phonetic shapes of tones in PT as a synchronic
feature and do not reach an agreement, in this thesis the tonogenesis in PT is seen to
undergo diachronic processes and is divided into at least two different periods. In
the earlier period it is speculated to have had non-tonal structure, and in the later
period it is proposed that tones arose to present as the four tonal categories *ABCD
under MC influence. Therefore, the hypothesis of phonation type (or voice quality),
vowel duration of Tai tones (or their pioneers), the pitch height and contour of PT
should be treated as applied to the later period of PT. The following sub-sections
will demonstrate the theme of the two principled clarifications above.
95
Combining with the previous studies on PT tones especially the tonal
correspondence among Tai, Chinese, Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao languages, the
evidence of tonal comparison among thirty Tai varieties in this thesis suggests a
result that PT tonal contrast arose by decaying of the syllable final consonantal
system, namely the weakening or dropping of *-h and *-ʔ. For keeping the semantic
distinction from the original plain syllable (without obstruent syllable finals), the
decaying of syllable final -h gave an original falling tone *B, and the weakening of
*-ʔ gave an original rising tone *C, to contrast to the least marked level tone *A on
smooth syllables. Since *D was on checked syllables with oral stop finals preserved,
it was not necessary for it to have any tonal feature to contrast to the *ABC tones.
Thus, on the basis of this hypothesis, the origin of PT tones is as the following
speculations: plain syllables with modal voice gave an unmarked level tone *A,
syllables with final aspiration -h decayed to give an high-falling tone *B on relatively
long vowel duration, syllables with final glottal stop -ʔ decayed to give a low-rising
tone *C on relatively short vowel duration, and syllables with final oral stops -p/-t/
-k were preserved to consist the fourth tonal category *D with a high-falling contour
like its counterpart tone *B.
The most direct evidence of this hypothesis is the external comparison among Tai
and those surrounding tonal languages. PT Tones *A, *B, *C, and *D correspond to
the above-mentioned Chinese/Vietnamese/Miao-Yao tones *A (level/ngang-huyền),
*C (falling or departing/hỏi-ngã), *B (rising/sắc-nặng), and *D (entering/sắc-nặng)
respectively, as shown in Table 20 in §2.2.2. The typological evidence for this
statement is provided by the previous studies although different studies have
different points of view about the arising of Tai tones and the tonal features in PT.
Here we firstly focus on additional evidence from the sound rules of historical
Chinese loanwords (OC and MC loans) in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (a CT variety)
as a representation of the Tai languages, as shown in Table 30. For comparing the
OC and MC loans in Yang Zhuang with their source from the borrowing language,
the Chinese words are demonstrated by Cantonese instead of Mandarin, because
Cantonese integrally preserves the tonal system from MC, in contrast Mandarin has
developed its new tonal system beyond clear recognition. Note that the difference
between OC and MC loans in Zhuang are ignored in the chart because they share
regular tonal correspondence, and there is not yet a full study on this.
96
Table 30 Tonal correspondence between Cantonese and Debao Urban Yang
Zhuang (DB)
Tonal category
A1
A2
B1
B2
C1
C2
DS1
DL1
DS2
DL2
Tai tone No. in
1
2
5
6
3
4
7
9
8
10
Chinese terms
yi ̄n
yáng
yi ̄n
yáng
DB tonal value
453
31
55
Cantonese value
55
21/11
33
Gloss Series 1
open
time
to
Cantonese
hɔi¹
ʃi²
kwɔ5
pun6
DB urban
kʰaj¹
ɬej²
Gloss Series 2
book
Cantonese
DB urban
Sinitic circle
pi ́ng
pi ́ng
yi ̄n yáng qù
qù
shàng xià yi ̄n
yáng rù
shǎng
shǎng
yi ̄n rù
rù
33
24ʔ
213ʔ
55
55
33
33
22
35
24
55
33
22
22
horse
seven
deliver
Buddha
wipe
kwɔŋ3
ma4
tʃʰat7
fat9
fat8
mat8
kwa:5 pu:n6
kwa:ŋ³
ma4
tɕat7
fa:t9
pat8
ma:t10
first
save
use
to fry morning north
eight
ten
candle
ʃy¹
tʰau²
kau5
jʊŋ6
tʃʰau³
tʃou4
pak7
pat9
ʃap8
lap8
ɬøɥ¹
taw² kjaw5
jʊŋ6
ɕa:w³
tɕaw4
pak7
pe:t9
tɕəp8
la:p10
pass
company wide
(October)
Just like in the chart above, the correspondence of tones between Yang Zhuang and
Cantonese are clearly regular. This regularity is not built on the similarity of
phonetic shape of their modern tones at all (since their tonal phonetic shapes are
quite different as shown in the chart), but on the same tonal roots from Ancient
Chinese, namely OC and MC. Actually in the chart all the Yang Zhuang glosses are of
OC and MC loanwords, which are actually not directly borrowed from Modern
Cantonese but from OC or MC via the ancient Pinghua in history according to the
previous studies of the ancient Chinese loans in the Zhuang languages, such as
Zhang (1982) and Lan (2001). It is assumed that at the time when PT borrowed the
earlier layer of Chinese loanwords, it adopted or adapted those loans with the
similar phonetic shapes of non-tonal elements in OC and tonal categories in MC. In
other words, it is proposed that the two corresponding counterparts in PT and
OC/MC had similar original non-tonal elements/tonal forms, so that their daughter
languages such as Cantonese developing from OC/MC and Yang Zhuang developing
from PT keep this correspondence of tones on shared vocabularies due to the tonal
development together in similar patterns as an areal feature.
97
This indicates that to a certain extent, in the earlier period of PT the phonation types
of the voice qualities, the non-tonal phonetic shapes of the later tonal categories,
should have been similar to their counterparts in OC and MC. For this reason, in
different periods PT must have had the similar non-tonal and similar tonal shapes
with their counterparts in OC and MC respectively, namely non-tonal structure in
the period coexisting with OC, and the four tonal categories *ABCD in the period
under MC influence. In the later period of PT, four tonal categories were established
by being influenced by MC tonal development, and Tone *A might be level or
unmarked like MC level tone píng, Tone*B might be falling like MC departing
(falling) tone qù, *C might be rising like MC rising tone shǎng, and*D should be with
final stops -p, -t, and -k like MC entering tone rù and as what the most majority of
Tai languages still preserve. For this reason, Tai tonogenesis should have been very
similar to that in OC and MC, namely the decaying of syllable final -h giving a
falling tone *B, and the weakening of *-ʔ giving a rising tone *C, as what will be
demonstrated in the following sections.
In this sub-section, the specific phonetic shapes of PT tones are discussed by
comparing the three arguments among Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009), Liang &
Zhang (1996), and Gedney (1989b) shown in Table 29, and by analyzing the tone
data from thirty Tai varieties recorded by me as shown in the following Table 3126.
26
Since marked phonation types or voice qualities found with tones is crucial to the reconstruction
of phonetic characteristics or shapes of PT tones, any specific phonation types or voice qualities found
accompanying with tones are labeled with tonal value scales in Table 31, like C2 tone in Bangkok
Thai is described as 453ʔ where the final -ʔ refers to a final glottal constriction. In Table 31, voice
qualities on tones like creakiness and final glottal constriction are mainly determined by auditory
identification. Some special issues are determined by PRAAT (Version 5.1.34), like the released final
glottal stop in Dalong (analyzed in Figure 13 in §4.1.3.3), and the final-aspiration-like soft offset in
Debao Urban (see Figure 14 in §4.1.3.3.2). In Table 31, -ʔ following the tonal numerical value
indicates final glottal constriction (like 453ʔ in Bangkok Thai), or even an audible unreleased or slight
released final glottal stop before pause or open juncture (like 24ʔ and 213ʔ in Debao Urban).
Creakiness is marked as ~ put right after the lowest number within the tonal value since creakiness
always occurs as of the characteristics of low pitch. Since almost all tones on checked syllables have
glottalizations (glottal constrictions and/or creakiness), the voice qualities on the D tones are omitted
in the chart. The shaded boxes in the chart refer to those tonal splits with “tonal flip-flop” or “voicehigh” phenomenon. Series 1 including all tones conditioned by proto-voiceless sounds (A1-A, A1-C,
A1-U, A1-UC, and A-G). Therefore in some Tai varieties Series 1 contains more than one tonal value,
such as A1 in Bangkok are 24 (A1-A and A1-C) and 33 (A1-U, A1-UC and A1-G) due to different
groups of initial.
98
Table 31 Phonetic shapes of tones and their voice qualities in Tai varieties
investigated
Language
SubGroup
SWT
CT
Variety Name
NT
*B
C*
D*
A2
B1
B2
C1
C2
DL1
DL2
DS1
DS2
Bangkok
24; 33
33
21
41ʔ
41ʔ
453ʔ
21
41
21
45
Songkhla
41; 453
31
41; 453
213
55; 44
22
55;44
213
55;45
21
Khon Kaen
24
35
33
33
41ʔ
21
41
23
45
Chiang Rai
13
33
22
31
41ʔ
453ʔ
22
31
24
35
Tai Yai
24
45
11̰
33ʔ
33ʔ
41ʔ
11
33
55
41
Tai Lue
55
31/335
35
33
13ʔ
11ʔ
35
33
55
33
Quan Son Tai
453
322
55
35ʔ
35ʔ
41ʔ
55
35
41
41
Debao Urban
453; 31
31
55; 33
33
24ʔ
21̰3ʔ 55; 33
33
55
33
Debao Dalong
53; 31
31
454; 332
332
24ʔ
33
45
21
Debao Lüliu
453; 31
31
55; 33
33
24ʔ
33
55
33
Jingxi Hurun
51̰; 31
31
334
223
45ʔ
21̰3ʔ 55; 33
22ʔ
33
22
55
13
Jingxi Urban
53
31
453
131
33ʔ
21̰3ʔ
45
21
33
21
Jingxi Anning
51̰
31
453
131
33ʔ
21̰3ʔ
45
13
33
21
Myang
55
42
35
33
24ʔ
31ʔ
35
33
55
42
Pyang
13
33
212
42
45ʔ
53ʔ
21
42
55
33
Min
24, 35
55
32
42
11ʔ
53ʔ
32
42
32; 55
55
Xiangdu
453, 31
31
33
11̰
24ʔ
24ʔ
33
11
33
11
453
31
33
11̰
24ʔ
11̰3ʔ
33
11
55
32
13ʔ,
34ʔ
11̰ʔ 34; 33
33
13; 34
11
Daxin Baoxu
NT?
*A
A1
Daxin Naling
CT?/
Tonal categories and their tonal values on smooth syllables
453, 53
Lingding
353,
Zuozhou
554, 11
21ʔ;
41ʔ
21̰3ʔ 45; 33
31
34, 33
33
11
33, 31
31
24ʔ
42ʔ 33; 31
31
55
11
Baiji Sanxiang
53
331
343
31
445ʔ
445ʔ
445
31
55
33
Shuangding
33
31
24
22
35ʔ
44ʔ
35
22
35
44
Nung An
554; 33
33
11̰2
31
24ʔ
42ʔ
11
31
55; 33
33
Longsang
35
55
31
53
213ʔ
33ʔ
21
33
55
33
Dongling
11̰
553
242
53
44ʔ
24
53
22; 44
44
Shanglin
35
221
33
53
55
31
33
53
55
32
Du’an
51̰
231
33
31
553ʔ
24ʔ
33
31
55
35
Yizhou Suogan
51̰
231
33
11̰
42
24ʔ
33
23
55
23
Huishui Bouyei
24ʔ
11̰3ʔ
55ʔ
41ʔ
33; 22
22
55
41
24
113
33
11
55ʔ
24ʔ
31
31
55
24
55
24
Zhenning
Bouyei
99
224ʔ;
44ʔ
Since PT Tones *A was undisputedly speculated to be unmarked on plain syllables
and *D was on checked syllables, the discussion of the PT tonal voice qualities of
these two tones will not be given unnecessary details. Therefore, the following
discussions will focus on the hypothesis of PT tonogenesis mainly surrounding the B
and C tones.
Concerning Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s hypothesis of the phonetic shapes of PT tones,
we first look at the pitch heights which are speculated by him as low for *B and high
for *C. The results of tonal development among different Tai daughter languages
present complex diversity with common “voiced-low” situation in the most NT and
CT varieties but “voiced-high” phenomenon in some dialects especially those in
SWT. The “voiced high” phenomenon is caused by “tonal flip-flop” suggested by
many linguists (see §2.2.2 and Glosarry), and it explains that at the time of registersplits tone pitch always started from “voiced-low”, but the later development may
change from high to low and low to high after establishment of tone. Since “voiced-
high” phenomenon is found in some languages and is not usual “cross-linguistically”
(Fu 1995: 81), this explanation is treated as the most plausible one within the many
explanations of “voiced-high” phenomenon reviewed in §2.2.2.
In Table 31, nine Tai varieties out of thirty have tonal flip-flop (cf. §2.2.2), and four
of them are found in the SWT areas which are treated as the areas of Tai emigration
in history. Tonal flip-flop found in the two CT and the three NT varieties is also in
the high lands at the western corner of Guangxi. These areas are historically treated
as of new territories of Tai emigrations from their Tai homeland in the east, which is
“the region encompassing parts of the watersheds of the Xi River system in Guangxi
extending to northern tributaries of the Red River in Vietnam” (Diller 1998: 15).
This suggests that tonal flip-flop phenomenon is of secondary development in the
new Tai territories. In addition, only four from these nine Tai varieties have a total
tonal flip-flop, the other five still retain some of the remnants of the original “voicelow” splits on some of the tonal categories, like the Tones A in Bangkok Thai (SWT)
as well as the Tones DS in Fuping (CT) and in Longsang (NT). Shanglin (NT) even
only has tonal flip-flop on the B column. This supports the idea that tonal flip-flop is
not usually “cross-linguistically” and is of secondary development.
Therefore, the internal reconstructions based on the phonetic shapes of tones in
modern Tai varieties, specifically pitch height which may be a result of “tonal flip-
flop” in some specific Tai varieties, may be untenable. The pitch height of PT Tones
100
B* and C* reconstructed by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) based on the comparison
among these internal reconstructions are considered to be false in this thesis.
Opposed to Pittayapon’s hypothesis, when PT tones initially arose in the later stage
of PT, the pitch height of Tones B* and C* should be reconsidered as high and low
respectively according to the following discussion in §4.1.3.2 and §4.1.3.3 about the
other aspects of phonetic shapes of these two tones, since pitch height may be
influenced by contours and phonation types.
The contour and the vowel duration of PT tones have to be discussed together since
in Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s reconstruction these two shapes are related with one
other. The reason that Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) gives the shapes “rising and
long” to PT Tone *B and “falling and short” to PT Tone *C is that Tone *D normally
followed the same pattern of tonal splits and mergers with Tone *B in the majorities
of Tai languages, and Tone *D further split into DL preserving the same patterns
with *B and DS coming to be identified with *C instead of *B. He posits vowel
duration being involved in conditioning tonal behaviors, and assumes that vowel
duration in Tone *B must be relatively longer than in Tone *C, and states that “both
experimental typological studies have shown that vowels under rising tones tend to
be longer than vowels on level and falling tones” (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009:
276).
Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s observation is a plausible explanation if we only focus on
the common collocation of vowel duration and pitch contour. However, if we
consider other aspects, some flaws can be perceived. First, his additional evidence
for Tone *C which is that although either level or falling contours could be
reconstructed for both *A and *C tones, the glottalization of *C indicates that it
should have been a falling contour. Second, the left contour rising is assigned to *B
because “languages that point to a rising contour in *B are spoken very far apart”
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 276) to make this contour for *B seem to be a case of
retention (cf. §2.2.1). These two points are treated to be arbitrary because we have
discussed the untenable reconstruction of phonetic shapes of proto-tones including
*A *B, and*C.
Above all, many scholars have provided previous studies on tonogenesis, which
suggests that glottal segments can cause a rising tone especially in Asian languages.
For example, in the early step of the process of tonogenesis in East and Southeast
Asian languages, a rising tone normally arose by dropping the syllable final -ʔ, such
101
as the sắc-nặng tone in Vietnamese (Haudricourt 1954a), shǎng ‘rising tone’ in
Chinese (Pulleyblank 1962, 1978; Mei 1970, 1982; Norman 1988), and the Lahu
rising tone caused by dropping the initial and the final -ʔ’s (Matisoff 1970) (cf.
§2.2.1). Therefore, PT tone *C which arose by dropping or weakening the final -ʔ or
a glottal constriction should not always be a falling tone even if its duration was
actual shorter than Tone *B in history.
For the second point, as we have discussed that tonal unpredictable development
could have occurred in history, the rising contour for tones developing from *B in
specific modern Tai varieties might not have been a retention, therefore the point of
view that *B was a retentive rising tone is also untenable.
Therefore, in this thesis PT proto-tones *B and *C are proposed to be falling and
rising respectively due to above-mentioned MC counterparts influencing as well as
the typological collocation in MSEA languages between tone pitch and phonation
voice (fricative final causing falling tones and -ʔ causing rising tones) during the
period that tones originally arose. The premise of this proposal is the voice quality
of these two tones, which is discussed below.
The rest of PT tonal phonetic shape is the phonation type or voice quality. Because
previous hypotheses of straightforward of Tones *A and *D are less argument, the
following discussion focuses on the voice quality of Tones *B and *C.
Before continuing discussing the voice qualities of the tones B and C in modern Tai
varieties and to prove the speculation of the phonetic characteristics of PT Tones *B
and *C, it is necessary to distinguish the terms glottal stop, glottal constriction, and
creaky voice, which are all found in tones in modern Tai varieties. Note that Gedney
uses the term “glottal constriction” loosely without distinguishing between
creakiness and final glottal constriction found in tones, and he even treats them as
the same phonetic phenomenon by the following statement: “… the tones (usually
two) that have developed from the earlier C tone are characterized by glottal
constriction, sometimes called ‘creaky voice’”, and this is “manifested by extreme
tension of the glottis throughout the vowel, increasing gradually, with closure in
glottal stop at the end of the syllable before pause of open juncture” (Gedney 1989c:
209-210). Referencing previous studies such as Gordon & Ladefoged (2001) and
Michaud (2004), Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 277) distinguishes creakiness from
final glottal constriction, by defining the former as an alternate name
102
‘laryngealization’, which refers to “the irregular, low-frequency vibration of the
vocal folds extended over a relatively long period of the duration of the vowel”, and
by defining the latter as “a complete or almost complete closure of vocal at the end
of rime”. On the basis of this distinction he has provided the aforementioned
hypothesis giving “creakiness” and “final glottal constriction” to PT Tones *B and *C
respectively (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 277-281). However, the types of voice
quality defined by Michaud (2004: 120) are actually three. He gives the three
definitions as follows: (1) ‘(Final) glottal stop’ is a gesture of vocal fold closure that
limitedly arouses specific voice quality on the syllable. (2) ‘Glottal constriction’
refers to “a tense gesture of adduction of the vocal folds that extends over the whole
of a syllable rhyme”. (3) ‘Laryngealization (creaky voice)’ is an “irregular vocal fold
vibration” and “is not tense in itself”. He furthermore points out that the term
‘glottalization’ covers ‘glottal constriction’ and ‘laryngealization’ (Michaud 2004:
120). This shows that Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s definition of ‘final glottal
constriction’ is used as a cover term for Michaud’s ‘final glottal stop’ and ‘glottal
constriction’. The relationships of the definitions above are concluded as in the
following Table 32.
Table 32 The correspondence among the voice qualities defined by different
proposals
Proposer
Correspondence (vertically)
Gedney (1989)
Pittayawat
Pittayaporn
(2009)
Michaud (2004)
Glottal constriction or glottalization
final glottal constriction
final glottal
stop
glottal
constriction
creakiness
laryngealization (creaky voice)
glottalization
Combining with the previous studies, in this thesis we synthesize the definitions
above to define as follows: ‘Final glottal constriction’, which is also called ‘glottal
constriction’ as its shortened form, refers to a tense gesture over the whole syllable
rhyme ending in a complete closure of the vocal folds. ‘Final glottal stop’, ‘creaky
voice/creakiness/laryngealization’, and ‘glottalization’ are in accordance with the
definitions provided by Michaud (2004: 120).
Final glottal stop -ʔ abundantly found in the C tones in modern Tai varieties
normally has coarticulatory effects on the preceding vowels to give a tense gesture
103
that extends over the whole of a syllable rhyme, and this tense gesture is called
‘glottal constriction’. This may be why Pittayawat Pittayaporn uses ‘final glottal
constriction’ to name this glottal phenomenon on Tai tones, and it is used in the
same way in this thesis. However, the glottalization mainly found in the C tones in
my fieldwork for this thesis has its own symbolic feature, which presents to always
end in a complete closure of vocal fold or rather a final glottal stop -ʔ, and this
glottal stop is even slightly released before pause or open juncture in some varieties
recorded from some LRPs, like in Debao Dalong (CT), Jingxi Nung An (CT/NT?), and
Debao Longsang (NT). Take the term ˀba:nʔ24 ‘village’ with the C1 tone in Debao
Dalong Yang Zhuang (CT) as an example shown in Figure 13.
Figure 13 Voice analysis diagram of ˀba:nC1ʔ ‘village’ in Debao Dalong by PRAAT
The selected part of the diagram indicates the audible released -ʔ even following a
nasal coda -n, and this shows a counterevidence of that syllable-final obstruents (like
-p, -t, -k and -ʔ) in tonal languages in East Asian and MSEA are known to be nonreleased, although the released segment actually disappears along with the final -ʔ at
a non-final position in a carrier sentence. There is no indication that medial
glottalization throughout the whole syllable can exist by itself without ending in a
final glottal stop before pause or open juncture. This is very like the common
presence of a glottal stop or a glottal constriction in association with a final oral stop
in languages in China and MSEA, like in Fujian (Iwata et al. 1979), Cantonese (Iwata
et al. 1981), Thai (Henderson 1964; Harris 2001), and Vietnamese (Thurgood 2002).
It indicates that the glottalization manifested by extreme tension of the glottis
104
throughout the vowel on the C tones may be an effect triggered by the final glottal
stop. Therefore, this thesis suggests the final glottal stop -ʔ to be the most symbolic
feature of historical residual from PT *C instead of the medial glottalization over the
whole of a syllable rhyme. In a similar way, creakiness found in some tones will be
proved to be an effect of a low pitch, a dipping tone, or a rapid falling tone, instead
of an inherent feature of some specific tones.
Since there is more obvious evidence for proving the syllable final -ʔ accompanying
with Tone *C than those evidence for proving the phonetic characteristics PT Tone
*B, we discuss firstly from Tone *C. The voice qualities found in modern Tai
varieties from my fieldwork are summarized as shown in Table 31 in §4.1.3.
Among the different hypotheses summarized in Table 29, Sagart’s argument is
totally different from the others on voice quality of Tones *B and *C, and this has
been rejected by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) (cf. §2.2.1). In this thesis, the points
suggested by Gedney (1989c), Liang and Zhang (1996), and Pittayawat Pittayaporn
(2009) that the phonetic characteristics of PT tone *C having a final -ʔ or being final
glottal constriction are agreed with, since the data personally selected and analyzed
by me in this thesis have shown that there are many modern Tai varieties that still
preserve robust residues of the glottalized features in the C tones as shown in Table
31. Within the thirty languages personally recorded by me, twenty-six have final
glottal constriction ending in a glottal stop on the C tones (C1 and/or C2) no matter
what the pitch level or contour of the respective tone. The phenomenon that this
glottal constriction with a glottal stop ending is mainly found on C tones has been
discovered by Gedney (1989c: 210-211). He points out that many Tai varieties from
Li’s CT and SWT such as Thai, Shan, Lao, White Tai, Lungchow, Lungming, and most
other CT/SWT languages researched by him, have this glottal constriction on the
tone that have developed from PT Tone *C, regardless of pitch level or contour such
as the two C tones in Thai (falling and high), in White Tai (low-rising and lowfalling), and in Lungming (mid level and low-falling-rising) (Gedney 1989c: 210).
The tonal correspondence among Tai, Sinitic languages, Miao-Yao, and Vietnamese
also supports that tone *C in Tai (which corresponds to Tone B in the others) used to
have a glottal final. This indicates that the final -ʔ should have accompanied with PT
Tone *C but not with Tone *B. Therefore, Sagart’s argument is not supported in this
thesis.
105
It is notable that Gedney also points out that the studied NT varieties except Saek,
which may be influenced by surrounding SWT languages, do not have this final
glottal constriction on C tones (Gedney 1989: 212-213). However, at least our data
shown in Table 31 have accounted for the more complicated situation on C tones in
NT varieties. At least four varieties within seven NT varieties and all the three
varieties whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate have a clear final glottal
constriction on C tones. Our LRP from Longsang Zhuang of NT even release the
glottal stop after the syllables on C tones when they repeat the terms alone. The
remaining three NT varieties which have no glottalization on C tones are Shanglin
Zhuang, Huishui Bouyei, and Zhenning Bouyei. Shanglin Zhuang may reflect the
situation pointed out by Gedney. The one LPR of this dialect produces none of the
tones with a consistent phonation type. However, in the two Bouyei dialects which
are spoken far to the north of the concentrated Tai areas, the situation shows
secondary tonal reversal on smooth syllables as follows. In Huishui Bouyei (L41),
items with A and B tones unexpectedly have a slight or clear glottal constriction, like
pja24ʔ (A1) ‘hill’, ma:24ʔ (A1) ‘dog’, kɨn24ʔ ‘to eat’, wa:j113ʔ (A2) ‘buffalo’, si:55ʔ ‘four’ (B1),
and ta:41ʔ (B2) ‘river’. In Zhenning Bouyei (L42), items with tones in the B column
normally have a slight final glottal constriction, like (ˀda:ŋ33) ka:j55ʔ ‘egg’, ɬej55ʔ ‘four’
(B1), and (ʔə33) ta:24ʔ (B2) ‘river’. These phenomena are very unique because final
glottal constriction is mostly found on C tones in many other Tai varieties, as what
mentioned above.
The problem is readily solved when comparing the tones in the whole tonal system
in these varieties. For Huishui Bouyei (L41), the final glottal constriction on A and B
tones may have been triggered by the mergers between Tones A and DS, as well as
between Tones B and DL (as shown in Table 33).
106
Table 33 Basic tonal patterns in Huishui Bouyei (NT)
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Proto-
Aspirations
A1-A: 24ʔ
B1-A: 55ʔ
C1-A: 33
B1-A: 55ʔ
A1-A: 24ʔ
voice-
Continuants
A1-C: 24ʔ
B1-C: 55ʔ
C1-C: 33
B1-C: 55ʔ
A1-C: 24ʔ
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24ʔ
B1-U: 55ʔ
C1-U: 33
B1-U: 55ʔ
A1-U: 24ʔ
A1-UC: 24ʔ
B1-UC: 55ʔ
B1-UC: 55ʔ
A1-UC: 24ʔ
A1-G: 24ʔ
B1-G: 55ʔ
C1-G: 22
B1-G: 55ʔ
A1-G: 24ʔ
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 11̰3ʔ
B2: 41ʔ
C2: 22
B2: 41ʔ
A2: 11̰3ʔ
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2: 11̰3ʔ
B1/2: 41ʔ
C1/2: 22
B1/2: 41ʔ
A1/2: 11̰3ʔ
less
Unaspirated stop +
*r clusters
Glottal sounds
breathy sounds
C1-UC:
33
The loss or weakening of the syllable final -k is commonly found in some NZ
varieties such as Nandan and Tian’e in northwestern Guangxi, and Qiubei in Yunnan
(Zhang et al. 1999: 240), as well as in their neighboring Bouyei languages, probably
because of the language contact with Southwestern Mandarin and the Loloish
languages (according to my fieldwork research)27. In Huishui Bouyei, *-k after a long
vowel in PT had been changed to a slight non-phonemic final glottal constriction to
merge into their counterpart Tones B (B1 and B2), like pa:55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘forehead’,
jia55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘guest’, pa:55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘mouth’, ma:55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘bean’ (semantic
change from ‘fruit’), ta:55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘to expose under the sun’, tiɑ55ˀ (DL1>B1)
‘cracking’, ˀjiə55ʔ (DL1>B1) ‘hungry’, and ða:41ʔ (DL2>B2) ‘root’; *-k after a short
vowel in PT has been changed to a clear non-phonemic glottal constriction to merge
into their counterpart Tones A (A1 and A2), like to:24ʔ (DS1>A1) ‘thin bamboo
stripes’, pja24ʔ (DS1>A1) ‘vegetable’, ðua:113ʔ (DS2>A2) ‘bird’, ða:113ʔ (DS1>A2) ‘to
steal’, and ɕja:113ʔ (DS2>A2) ‘cooked’; this may be why the tonal value of tones on
the vertical column of DL and DS has merged into the tonal value of their
counterpart Tones B and A; the reason why items with tones on the columns A and B
normally have a slight final glottal might be because in this dialect proto-final *-k
from Tones DL and DS might have brought about an effect, a slight glottal final -ʔ,
into Tones B and A.
For Zhenning Bouyei (L42), the glottal constriction on B tone may have been
triggered by the mergers between Tones B and DL, as the following analyses: on
original dead syllables, *-k after a long vowel in PT been changed to a slight nonphonemic glottal constriction to merge into their counterpart Tones B (B1 and B2),
27
However, most of these languages well preserve the finals -t and -p.
107
like (na:31) pa:55ʔ ‘forehead’, (pou31) ɕiə55ʔ ‘guest’, (ˀda:ŋ33) pa:55ʔ ‘mouth’, (ˀdam33)
ma:55ʔ ‘fruit’, ta:55ʔ ‘to expose under the sun’, piə55ʔ ‘cracking’, (tʊŋ31) ʔi:55ʔ ‘hungry’,
la:24ʔ (va:j31) ‘root’ and (tiaw33) tɕa:24ʔ ‘rope’; like Huishui Bouyei, proto-final *-k from
Tones DL might have brought about an effect, a slight glottal final -ʔ, into Tones B
(cf. Appendix C). Therefore, the original final glottal constriction on Tones C in
these two Bouyei varieties might have been dropped due to the extrusion of the later
development of the final glottal constriction on Tones A and/or B.
Furthermore, tonal splits and mergers on the C column in many NT languages
indicate the lack of glottalization on C tones may also be a natural quality of the
mergers of C1-G (the tonal category conditioned by initial glottal sounds) and C2
tones, which are very rarely found in CT and SWT varieties. The previous Table 33
and the following Table 34 show two examples of tonal mergers of C1-G and C2 in
the two NT varieties investigated.
Table 34 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Dongling Zhuang (NT)28
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Proto-
Aspirations
A1-A: 11̰
B1-A: 242
C1-A: 224ʔ
DL1-A: 24
DS1-A: 22
voice-
Continuants
A1-C: 11̰
B1-C: 242
C1-C: 224ʔ
DL1-C: 24
DS1-C: 22
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 11̰
B1-U: 242
C1-U: 224ʔ
DL1-U: 24
DS1-U: 22
less
Unaspirated stop +
*r clusters
Glottal sounds
A1-UC:
11̰
B1-UC: 242
C1-UC:
224ʔ
DS1-UC:
DL1-UC: 24
22
A1-G: 11̰
B1-G: 242
C1-G: 44ʔ
DL1-G: 24
DS1-G: 44
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 553
B2: 53
C2: 44ʔ
DL2: 53
DS2: 44
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2:
breathy sounds
553
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 44ʔ
DL1/2: 53
DS1/2: 44
In the tonal patterns of Huishui Bouyei (L41) shown in Table 33 and Debao
Dongling (L32) shown in Table 34, the same colors in the same chart indicate tonal
mergers crossing tonal categories or registers. In Debao Dongling Zhuang and
Huishui Bouyei, all the columns except the column C (and its counterpart checked
syllable column) show straightforward tonal development, which splits their
registered tones between original voiceless sounds (including glottal sounds) and
voiced initial sounds. On the other hand, the column C shows non-straightforward
28
The management that treats B1 (242) on smooth syllable and DL1 (24) on checked syllable as
well as C1 (224ʔ) and DS1 (22) to be allotones of the same toneme is explained in §3.3.2. Hereafter in
the similar situation in all other Tai varieties, this criterion of determining allotones of a specific tone
will be throughout implemented.
108
tonal development, which splits its registered tones between original voiceless
sounds (excluding glottal sounds) and glottal plus voiced sounds. C1-G going with
C2 is quite common in the western half part of NT areas.
In Kam-Tai languages, glottal or pre-glottalized initials often have a pitch-lowering
effect on the following syllable. Previous studies have found that CT and SWT
varieties often have tonal mergers between glottal register and original lower
register, namely A1-G and A2 as well as B1-G and B2 (and between their counterpart
checked syllable tones), but rarely have this merger on the category C (except in
Isan/Lao). The intensive final glottal constriction on the C tonal category in CT/SWT
varieties may be the factor preventing the tonal mergers between C1-G and C2. In
the early step of the process of tonogenesis in East and Southeast Asian languages, a
rising tone normally triggered by the syllable final -ʔ, such as the sắc-nặng tone in
Vietnamese (Haudricourt 1954a), shǎng ‘rising tone’ in Chinese (Pulleyblank 1962,
1978; Mei 1970, 1982; Norman 1988), and the Lahu rising tone caused by dropping
the initial and the final -ʔ’s (Matisoff 1970). This indicates that the original double
glottal segments (initial and final) on the *C tone in CT/SWT may condition a higher
pitch on tonal behaviors than the pitch conditioned by the single glottal segment
(initial) on the tonal categories *A and *B, therefore in most CT/SWT varieties C1-G
always keeps the merger into the high register to be with the tone conditioned by
other voiceless sounds, while A1-G and B1-G are often lowered or even merge into
A1 and B2 respectively, such as ˀdam31 (A2<A1-G) ‘black’ and ˀjow33 (B2<B1-G) ‘at’
in Debao Yang Zhuang varieties. However, glottal stop sometimes has a pitchlowering effect on the preceding vowel in some Uto-Aztecan languages, like in Hopi
(Fu 1995: 64). This is also reflected in NT varieties, which normally do not have the
tonal merger between glottal registers and original lower-register on tonal categories
A and B, but often have this merger on the C category which should have had been
given a pitch-lowering effect on the preceding vowel, perhaps due to the weakening
or dropping of the original final glottal stop on the C category. This may interpret
the disappearance of the final glottal constriction on C tones in the NT varieties
investigated by Gedney (1989c).
Hereto, the phonetic characteristics of Tone *C in the earlier stage of PT can be
concluded to have been very similar to its counterpart Chinese shǎng (rising) tone to
have an affiliated syllable final glottal stop, which is still preserved as a final glottal
constriction, sometimes even being a slight released final glottal stop before a pause
or an open juncture, in the vast majority of modern CT/SWT and those varieties
whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate, and in quite a few considerable
109
modern NT varieties. In the later stage of PT, this final glottal stop was weakened to
cause a low-rising contour on relatively short vowel durations due to its inherent
glottal constriction.
As described in §2.2.1 and §4.1, various authors have proposed different
characteristics for PT Tone *B. Gedney (1989c) and Liang and Zhang (1996) propose
that it had a final *-h; Sagart (1988) argues that it had a modal voice rime ending
with a glottal stop *-ʔ; Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) speculates that it was a lowrising tone with a creaky voice. However, the major problem with these proposals is
that modern Tai dialects do not have a consistent voice quality on the tones
developing from *B. This is apparent when being compared with the commonly
preserved glottal constriction on the tones from *C, as shown in Table 31 in §4.1.3.
In Table 31, the situation of the voice qualities of the non-C tones on smooth
syllables appears as an untidy distribution – some final glottal constriction and
creakiness irregularly accompanying one of the tones developing from *B in some
modern Tai varieties or from *A tones in others. Therefore, all these hypotheses
about PT Tone *B are quite speculative compared to the clear evidence in support of
the phonetic characteristics of PT Tone *C.
Even so, we have proposed in §4.1.2 that PT Tone *B might have been a falling tone
like its MC counterpart departing tone qù, and the falling contour is believed to have
been conditioned by the previous syllable finals -h (or -s) at the time29 in the
languages in China and MSEA (see §2.2.1), this thesis proposes that this tone should
have developed from a final fricative -h (or -s) which agrees with its OC counterpart
in an earlier stage of PT to a creaky phonation which was with its MC counterpart in
an later stage of PT. The earlier form of this tone agrees with the forms
reconstructed by Gedney (1989b) and Liang and Zhang (1996), and the later form
agrees with Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s (2009) hypothesis. However, the evidence for
supporting hypothesis in this thesis of Tone *B are different from Gedney, Liang &
Zhang, and Pittayawat Pittayaporn. In the following discussion, I will determine the
residue of the phonetic characteristics of PT Tone *B by a process of elimination.
The first proposal to be eliminated is final glottal constriction (cf. Sagart 1988).
Glottalization found in A and B tones in the Tai languages are scattered, as shown in
29
MC departing tone is proposed to derive from OC -s or -h finals and to become creakiness during
the MC period (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 280-281).
110
Table 31. In the thirty Tai varieties investigated during my fieldwork, only Thai, Tai
Yai, and Quan Son Tai of SWT, and Huishui Bouyei and Zhenning Bouyei of NT have
clear final glottal constriction on tones developing from *B. The final glottal
constriction of B1 and B2 tones in the two Bouyei varieties has been clarified above
as an effect by the mergers from original dead syllables. Moreover, the final
glottalized Tone B2 in Thai, Tai Yai, and Quan Son Tai is actually spread from the
original *C tone due to the coalescence or merger between C1 and B2 in these
varieties. This phenomenon has been pointed out by Gedney, and he further pointed
out the reverse that in some Tai varieties such as Khorat and the Yo dialect of Sakon
Nakhon glottalization has disappeared from both of B and C tones due to the tonal
coalescence involving the C tone (Gedney 1989: 214-215). However, Sagart (1988)
accounts that the final of Tone *B should be a glottal stop -ʔ instead of -h based on
that Tone *B patterns with Tone *D on tonal split and mergers, and the final of Tone
*B should be a complementary stop (-ʔ) since*D always ends on non-glottal stops.
Sagart’s argument is not supported in this thesis because there is more evidence
supporting that the final -ʔ should be the accompanied feature of Tone *C instead of
Tone *B. Moreover, his argument that glottalization in reflexes of Tone B in modern
Tai varieties is common is not substantiated. The data from my fieldwork show that
the glottalization in reflexes of the B tones is far less than in the C tones, and some
of the final glottal constriction on the B tones is actually spread from the C tones due
to tonal mergers as discussed above. Besides, the ‘glottalization’ regarded by him in
reflexes of the B tones might not have always been the same ‘final glottal
constriction’ like in reflexes of the C tones in our definition, but might be
‘creakiness’ which will be verified below. Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) also rejects
Sagart’s proposals of the phonation type of Tone *B. He suggests that Sagart’s
viewpoint that PT *B was ending in a glottal stop could not have been the case by
pointing out that a syllable with a glottal stop ending must be expected to have
contrasted long and short vowels as CV:ʔ vs. CVʔ, which is no evidence for such
contrast in PT30 (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 252). To sum up, PT Tone *B need
not have had glottal constriction even though this feature is found in the tones
developing from PT *B tone in some of the modern Tai varieties.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s clarification is neither supported in this thesis because final glottal stop
accompanying with Chinese shǎng tone or Vietnamese sắc-nặnɑ tone (which correspond to Tai tone
*C) has been suggested by many previous studies and the vowel length contrast is not always the case.
This final glottal stop might be actually more than a simple syllable final since it even occurs after a
syllable final nasal before pauses or open junctures in many modern Tai varieties like in Figure 13.
Besides, so-called ‘short vowel’ on open syllables in modern Thai is actually followed by a glottal
stop before pauses and open junctures, and these vowels are always short and do not have length
contrast.
30
111
The second clarification is that creakiness on B tones in some of the modern Tai
varieties should not be treated as the earliest origin of the tones themselves, or
should not be the phonetic characteristics of *B tone in the earlier stage of PT.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn suggests that creakiness found on *B in several modern SWT
and CT varieties is inherited, even though many of them are described as
glottalization (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 278). He believes the voice qualities
may actually be creakiness instead of final glottal constriction since at least one of
the descriptions Nung Fan Slihng from Freiberger (1964) specifically mentions that
the tone B2 “ends with a final glottal stop, but laryngealization may occur over all
the voiced phonemes”, and Pittayawat Pittayaporn claims that this laryngealization
is actually “a remnant of the creaky voice characteristic of PT *B” (2009: 278-280).
He even gives additional evidence – the correspondence between PT *B and MC
departing tone which is proposed to derive from OC -s or -h finals and became
creakiness during the MC period (2009: 280-281). However, creakiness is known as
a characteristic of low pitch in tonal languages due to the previous studies, such as
Northern Chinese dialects in Shanxi, Beijing, Hebei, Henan, Shandong, and Jiangsu
provinces commonly have creaky voice as the feature [+low] (Zhu & Yang 2010). A
typical example that indicates creaky voice as the feature [+low] in Tai languages is
provided by Mai (2011). He points out that creakiness can be found in the sixth tone
(B2) in Qingtang variety of NT from Zhongshan County, Guangxi. The sixth tonal
category in this NT variety do not have a stable pitch, but always accompanies with
a creaky voice variously presenting as follows: when the creaky voice appears at the
central of the syllable, the tone presents a falling-rising contour (31̰3 or 31̰4); when
it appears at the initial of the syllable, the tone presents a rising contour (1̰5 or 1̰55
and 3̰ 5 or 3̰55 due to different environment); and when it appears at the end the
syllable, or appears during all the duration of the syllable, the tone presents a midfalling or low level contour (31̰ or 11̰) (Mai 2011: 22-23). That is to say, the creaky
voice always accompanies with the low pitch or the so-called “shallow tone” (a tone
with falling first then rising contour) in this dialect. In Table 31, we can also see the
creakiness occurring ubiquitously on the low fundamental frequency of the pitch on
a syllable, no matter any tonal categories (including the C and D tones). For
example, in Yizhou Suogan Zhuang (NT), Tone A1 (51̰) has a strong creakiness
triggered by its rapid falling contour from the highest pitch 5 to the lowest pitch 1,
and B2 (11̰) also accompanies a weak creakiness due to the lowest pitch. Huishui
Bouyei Tone A2 (11̰3) normally has a creakiness triggered by its extremely low level
pitch 11 before rising to a higher pitch level 3. If we admit the method of
tonogenesis, we must accept that tonal languages have developed from non-tonal
112
proto-languages; consequently, an original pattern of manifestation of the voice
quality in a proto-language must not have been a fixed low pitch to automatically
have coarticulatory effects to produce a creaky voice. Therefore, the treatment that
the creakiness found on B tones is account for the genetic characteristics of the voice
quality of PT *B tone is like to put the incidental before the fundamental. Moreover,
the creakiness confirmed in my fieldwork are even more found in A and C tones
than in B tones as shown in Table 31, because low tones, dipping tones, and rapid
high falling tones are more concentrated in A and C tones than in B tones in the Tai
varieties investigated in my fieldwork. In short, creakiness may be an effect of low
pitches, dipping tones or rapid falling tones, and should not be treated as a cause of
tonogenesis. Therefore, it is indeed hardly to be proved to be a genetic feature of PT
*B tone.
The rest of the discussion comes back to the hypothesis of final -h for the origin of
PT tone *B. Although there are no modern Tai dialects which keep a voice quality
like final aspirations, or in other words a final *-h like the parallelism in tonogenetic
processes proposed by Haudricourt (1954a, 1954b) and Pulleyblank (1962, 1978) in
Vietnamese and/or Chinese, Gedney (1989b: 91, 1989c: 208) speculates that it also
had a final *-h mainly based on its counterpart DL tone, and on the evidence that
Thai treatment of using Tone *B to borrow Indic loanwords ending in -ha. Liang and
Zhang (1996) also propose a hypothesis totally agreeing with Gedney’s speculation,
although their arguments are not based on the same evidence, but on their own
evidence from the tonal correspondence of some shared vocabularies between Tai
Lue and Wa, as has been reviewed in §2.2.1. Their detailed evidence is partly shown
in Table 35.
Table 35 Some shared vocabularies with the B tones (Tones 5 and 6) in Dai
and with syllable final -h in Wa (adapted from Liang & Zhang 1996: 816)
Dai (Tai Lue)
Wa
Glosses
pha:w
phruah
kɔj
kɔih
‘to broadcast (seed)’
thaj
tauh
bi:
bih
xe:
rɛ̀h
haw
ràuh
lɛ:
lɛ̀h
vɛ:
‘to draw out, to dig’
‘to redeem’
‘to crack’
‘to harrow’
‘to bark’
‘to cut, to split, to peel’
‘in passing, incidentally’
vɛ̀h
113
However, Gedney’s hypothesis of -h for the origin of PT Tone *B has been refuted by
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) by being pointed out that the evidence of Indic
loanwords may have been processed by loanword adaptation. Loanword adaptation
in Thai can assign Indic loans ending in a final -ha to an open syllable with tone *B,
like Thai pʰa:B2 < Indic vāh(a) ‘bearer’ or Thai ʔutDS1 sa:B1 < Indic utsāh(a) ‘to make
an effort’. This is due to the same pattern as Indic loans ending in other obstruents
including the stops -p, -t, -c, and -k and the fricatives -s, -š, and -s were regularly
assigned to the tonal category *D which is the check-syllable counterpart of *B. In
those Indic loans with tonal category *B, the final -h in Indic source was in fact
dropped in the process of loanword adaptation because final fricatives are not
permissible in Thai (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 252-253). Pittayawat
Pittayaporn’s point of view is convictive since loanword adaptation is commonly
believed to cause loans in a borrowing language to change its original phonetic
shapes in the borrowed language to accommodate to the sound system of the
borrowing language. Therefore, Liang and Zhang’s argument is also not always
reliable, because the reliability of their proposal is queried to be unstable by the
loanword adaptation. The most crucial point is whether Liang and Zhang’s evidence
is Tai Lue loans in Wa or Wa loans in Tai Lue. Unfortunately, they only illustrate a
comparison of “corresponding words” shared between Dai and Wa, but do not
indicate the original source of these words, thus loanword adaptation seems to fall
into a confusion. If it is assumed that these words are Wa loans in Dai, the process
can still assign those words with final -h in Wa to tonal category *B on an open
syllable in Dai due to loanword adaptation just like Indic loans in Thai pointed out
by Pittayawat Pittayaporn. However, because at least the words tʰajB1 ‘to redeem’
and hauB1 ‘to bark’ in Dai are from typical Tai source, their counterparts tauh ‘to
redeem’ and ràuh ‘to bark’ in Wa should be treated as Dai loans. Therefore, the
direction of loans from Dai to Wa is plausible. The final -h of those counterparts in
Wa indicates that the source in Dai may have had a special laryngeal feature since -h
is a laryngeal fricative. Loanword adaptation can hardly explain how Wa has
adapted Tai Lue B tones (with a modal voice) by a syllable final -h. Therefore, tone
*B in a specific history of Tai Lue should have had at least a special laryngeal
feature since -h is a laryngeal fricative. So far, the final -h for the phonation type of
*B is the most probable hypothesis after eliminating the possibilities of glottalized
feature (final glottal constriction or final glottal stop) and creakiness, based on the
tonal correspondence among Tai, Chinese, Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao, as well as on
the loanword adaptation in Tai Lue-Wa shared vocabularies.
114
Therefore, even if at a specific period PT did have creakiness on its tonal category
*B like its counterpart MC q̀ (departing) tone which is proposed to derive from OC s or -h finals and became creakiness during the MC period (Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 280-281), this specific period of PT should be later than the exact tonogenesis
period due to the regional typological tonal correspondence among Tai, Sinitic,
Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao language families. The real cause of this proposed
“creakiness” on *B tone should be an original high-falling tone, which easily gave a
creakiness added to the pitch. This original falling tone typologically developed from
the decaying of syllable final -h.
To confirm the hypothesis of PT *B as from the decaying of syllable final -h, it is
necessary for us to discuss a series of tones with complex-contour like dipping (e.g.
213ʔ in Debao Urban), convex (e.g. 453 in Daxin Baoxu), delayed falling (e.g. 554 in
Zuozhou), and delayed rising (e.g. 112 in Nung An). When being individually read,
these kinds of complex tones always cause the duration of the syllable longer than a
simple tone namely level, falling and rising. In the Tai varieties investigated as
shown in Table 31, the complex-contour tones mainly found in the A and B
categories normally present as a modal voice over the whole syllable rhyme, but the
complex-contour tones found in the C categories normally accompany with a final
glottal constriction, like the C2 tone as 213ʔ in Debao Dalong. However, if we dig
into the complex tone found in the A and B categories, we still find that at the end
of the syllable before pause and open juncture they tend to have an aspiration-like
offset or ending. This offset actually consists of the final falling point or the final
rising point of the complex-contour, such as the final point (2) of a mid falling-rising
tone (332, the B1-A or B2 tone) in Debao Dalong, and the final point (3) of a highrising-falling tone (453, the A1 tone) in Daxin Baoxu. It is this kind of offset makes
the tone always being a waved contour. Figure 14 shows an example of complexcontour in B tone in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang.
Figure 14 Sound analysis diagram of tʰa:n332 ‘charcoal’ and pʰa:332 ‘to chop’ in
Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (CT) by PRAAT
115
In Figure 14, the voice quality of B1-A (332) tone in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang
presents as modal voice with a very slight aspiration-like final before pause or open
juncture, and I will call this “soft offset” instead of a final -h because it is too slight
to be audible, and is not phonemic or tonemic. The important point is that the soft
offset and the last falling point (2) of the tonal value (332) are actually integrated.
When the syllable is on a non-final position of a carrier sentence, the falling point
(2) and the soft offset (-ʰ) both disappear to make the tone be 33 instead of the full
form 332. It means the last falling point (2) and the soft offset of the full form are
actually the same thing, which presents as a falling point (2) continuing the previous
mid level contour (33) from the viewpoint of tonal value, and it presents as a soft
offset from the viewpoint of voice quality. Such kind of dipping, convex, delayedrising and delayed-falling contour tones are also found in the C tones in some Tai
varieties, like C2 (453ʔ) in Bangkok Thai. In the environment of being on a non-final
position of a carrier sentence, both the final glottal offset -ʔ and the falling point of
the contour (3) disappear since they are also integrated, or are the same thing
divided into two concepts due to the different points of views. This indicates that the
soft offset of A/B tones contrasts to the final glottal constriction presenting on the C
tones in many modern Tai varieties.
Many complex-contour tones found in the B and A tones in Tai varieties investigated
from my fieldwork are shown in Table 31. This final aspiration-like soft offset of a
syllable has not been found to be discussed in previous studies, and it needs more
discussion and demonstrations to be proved. Nevertheless, it deserves further
attention because this thesis considers it is crucial to the hypothesis of the phonetic
characteristics of PT Tone *B. A hypothesis built on the basis of the correspondence
among PT Tone *B, OC/MC tonal category qù (departing tone or original falling
tone), and Vietnamese tonal category C (hỏi-ngã) is supported by the following
mutual-correlative speculations involving complex-contour tones.
1) It can be speculated that in the period after tone arose, the original -h final
collapsed to cause a falling tone, but the duration of the syllable with original -h
final are lengthened to make up the decaying of the final -h, since syllables with
a final -h are phonetically longer than modal syllables (Tone *A), syllables with a
final glottal -ʔ (Tone *C), and syllables with an oral stop (Tone *D).
2) Later, the lengthened back part (a soft offset inherited from original -h) of the
duration are reflected on tonal contour to be a rising point, to make the tonal
value present a high-falling-rising contour, or in other words a dipping tone; the
116
important point of this process is that the first complex-contour tone in Tai is
speculated to appear on *B.
3) During the periods of the secondary development of tones in different Tai
daughter languages, the original contours of all tonal categories might be
changed to other contours, since “once it is established, the tonal system evolves
without regard for its old etymological pitch levels” (Haudricourt 1961);
consequently, in some Tai varieties, the original dipping tone (falling-rising)
developing from *B might have been changed to other complex-contours, like
convex (rising-falling), delayed rising (level-rising), and delayed falling (levelfalling) which have been discussed above and showed in Table 31, or even have
changed to other simple-contour such as level, rising and falling (also showed in
Table 31) during later development especially at the period of register splits
because tonal contours in a language are limited.
4) Any change in a tone may push another tone to change its original contour for
avoiding the tonal merger to keep the tonal distinctions; because the change
sometimes presents as a contour-exchange between two different tones, the
changes of the original complex contour *B tone in certain Tai varieties may
urge the complex-contour to spread to the other tonal categories, especially the
A and C tones which are also on smooth syllables.
5) However, C tones in most modern Tai varieties (especially in CT and SWT) still
robustly preserve the remnant or the residual of the phonetic characteristics of
PT tone *C, a final glottal constriction or even a clear accessary final glottal stop,
even though some of these C tones have been changed to the complex-contour
spread from original Tone *B, such as the C2 tone (213ʔ) which is a dippingcontour ending in a rising point (also a simultaneous glottal stop) in Debao
Dalong. This makes the C tones have the highest distinguishing degree to
recognize its original phonetic characteristics among all the smooth-syllable
tonal categories in most modern Tai varieties.
6) On the other hand, the A and B tones are more similar to each other on voice
quality (normally modal) especially after the complex-contour spread from *B to
the other tones, because the decaying of syllable final -h is more thorough.
Glottalizations (including final glottal constriction and creakiness) found in the A
and B tones as in the discussion above, are not inherited but are of one of the
following situations: a feature spread from *C due to tonal mergers, a feature
117
spread from the weakening of final oral stops from the D tones due to tonal
mergers, or an effect of a low pitch or a rapid falling contour.
However, we should be cautious to attribute a modal voice to *B based on the
elimination of glottalizations and on the similarity between the phonetic shapes of
the A tones and the B tones in modern Tai varieties. The correspondence among *B
in Tai and *C in Chinese/Vietnamese/Miao-Yao shows that *B in Tai should have
had a specific phonation type/voice quality which is phonetically similar to voice
quality of *C (a final aspiration like -s or -h) in Chinese/Vietnamese/Miao-Yao,
instead of a modal voice. In addition, in traditional poetry well preserved in many
Tai varieties (like Yang Zhuang and Tai Lue), the same syllable-rhyme with the tones
developing from *B and *C can rhyme with one another but cannot rhyme with the
same syllable-rhyme with tones developing from *A (Liao 2015). This also indicates
that *B and *C tones in PT constituted an associated pair on voice qualities, and
both of them should not have had a modal voice because they forbid being
companions with *A which is treated to have a modal voice without dispute in
previous studies. Tone *C has been frequently argued to have a syllable final -ʔ in
many previous studies and in this thesis, thus its paired companion tone *B should
also have had a counterpart syllable final -h, which has been favorably assumed in
the speculations above. Note that a final -s which is speculated to be the phonetic
shape of tone *C (qù or departing) in OC and to change to -h in MC in previous
studies. Here we only consider the final -h as of the phonetic shape of tone *B in PT
level, since it is also of lower laryngeal features to be paired with -ʔ in *C within the
meters of traditional Tai poetry.
Table 36 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (CT)
Phonation types
Protovoiceless
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 453
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 55
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 55
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 453
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 21̰3ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 33
A1/2: 453
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 55
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
118
Table 36 above shows the basic tonal patterns in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang, which
provides a typical example for demonstrating the speculations above. The discussion
will skip over the tones C and D since they straightforwardly reflect their original
phonetic characteristics in PT. The tones A and B afford much food for thought
although both of them have modal voice. It seems that they have interchanged their
earlier tonal contours because of the following two reasons: (1) Both high level (55)
and mid level (33) in the B tones reflect an original level tone, which should have
been the original tonal contour of PT tone *A due to most previous studies. (2) The
convex tone (453) and the mid-falling tone (31) in the A column should be
originally from the B column, since we have previously discussed about the tonal
developing processes of Tone *B after the decaying of -h: a falling tone (with long
duration ending in a soft offset) > a dipping tone (to make up the decaying of -h) >
other complex-contour tones like a convex tone (453). However, now we see that
the tonal contours of A1/A2 and B1/B2 are interchanged. This indicates that a
change in a tone can push one of the other tones to change to its original tonal
shape. In the case of this dialect, a possible process is that at a specific historical
period, a change of B1 from 453 to 55 causes A1 to switch from 55 to 453, as
playing a matching action their counterparts also switch to each other (B2 from 31
to 33, and A2 from 33 to 31), for keeping the tonal distinctions. The speculations
above together with evidence from the example analyzed indicate that complexcontours as a possible reflex of original syllable final -h is crucial to the hypothesis
of the phonetic characteristics of PT Tone *B, although complex-contours may have
also been spread to all the other tones in some modern Tai varieties.
Only if the speculation that complex-contour firstly occurred in PT tone *B after the
original falling tone arose is actually true, can the speculation that *B with a highfalling contour on a long vowel duration (cf. §4.1.3.2) be reasonably explained. The
original high-falling contour can also explain Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s proposal that
PT *B had creakiness like its counterpart qù in MC, and this creakiness is also found
on the B tones in many modern Tai varieties.
In addition, just like what Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) points that DL is the
counterpart of the B tones, PT tone *D should have been like PT tone *B on pitch
height, contour and voice quality after tone arose. My data (shown in Table 31) also
support this hypothesis. The voice quality of Tone *D may have been glottalized
since almost checked syllables found in modern Tai varieties always have glottal
constriction in association with the final oral stops. The situation that DS is the
119
counterpart of the C tones in many modern Tai varieties suggests that it should be a
secondary development due to their similar short vowel durations (cf. 4.3.3).
To sum up, from the evidence of the tonal correspondence among Tai, Sinitic
languages, Miao-Yao languages, and Vietnamese due to that all these non-Sinitic
languages may have been influenced by OC and MC in history, and the Tai internal
evidence demonstrated above, this thesis proposes that PT should be divided into
two stages: an earlier non-tonal period and a later tonal period. In the earlier stage
of PT, tonal categories *B and *C were on syllables with a final -h and a final -ʔ
respectively, like their counterparts in Chinese, Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao
languages. This indicates that the earlier phonetic characteristics of the *ABCD tonal
categories were respectively identical to the *ACBD tonal categories in Chinese,
Vietnamese, and Miao-Yao languages. Therefore, the argument of tonogenesis is
rationally supported in Tai languages in this thesis.
In the later stage of PT, under the influence of surrounding languages especially
Chinese which may have undergone its own tonogenesis process, syllables with final
-h and -ʔ in PT tended to decay. For keeping the semantic distinctions from the
original plain syllables without obstruent syllable finals, the decaying of syllable
final -h gave an original high-falling tone *B on relatively long vowel duration, and
the weakening of *-ʔ gave an original low-rising tone *C on relatively short vowel
duration, to contrast to each other and to the least marked level tone *A on smooth
syllables. Syllables with final oral stops (-p, -t, -k) were preserved to comprise the
fourth tonal category *D with a high-falling contour like its counterpart tone *B.
The diachronic process of Tai tonogenesis is proposed as in Table 37.
Table 37 Phonetic characteristics of PT tones in the two diachronic stages
Earlier stage of PT
plain
-h
-ʔ
-p, -t, -k
Tonal categories
*A
*B
*C
*D
Pitch height
mid
high
low
high
Contour
level
falling
rising
falling
long
short
creaky
glottal
constriction
non-tonal structures
Later
Stage
of PT
Vowel duration
Voice quality
modal
120
glottalization
After tone arose, tonal splits and mergers have been playing a main role on the stage
of tonal development. Just as tonogenesis initiated the four tonal categories at the
PT level, register tonal splits account for the more complex situation of tonal
categories in modern Tai varieties. As discussed in §1.2.1, the modern Tai tonal
categories are illustrated by Li (1977) and this led to a scheme of ten tonal
categories A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, C2, DL1, DL2, DS1, and DS2, which correspond to 1,
2, 5, 6, 3, 4, 9, 10, 7, and 8 tonal categories in the Sinitic circles. These schemes are
actually based on the view that modern Tai tones are residual evidence of segments
that have been lost. It indicates that the loss of the voicing contrast caused PT tones
*ABCD to split into two series. Series 1 or high frequency was conditioned from the
original voiceless initials, and series 2 or low frequency was usually conditioned by
the original voiced initials. The *D tone on checked syllables split further depending
on vowel length. This scheme is treated as the primary tonal split model as shown in
Table 1 in §1.2.1. On the other hand, Gedney’s scheme (cf. Table 5) and Liao &
Shen’s scheme (cf. Table 7) of Tai tonal categories indicate the more complex tonal
development including secondary development, which will be discussed in the
following sections.
Before discussing the primary Tai tonal splits, it is necessary to briefly review the
arguments of the different phonation types of initial consonants at the PT level,
since they are the conditioning factors of the subsequent tonal splits (cf. §1.2.1,
§1.2.2, §1.2.3 and §2.2.3). There are fewer disagreements about the following four
phonation types at the PT level: voiceless stops, voiceless continuants, glottal
sounds, and voiced sounds (including voiced stops and voiced continuants). The
main outstanding disagreement for grouping the initial consonants at the PT level is
whether there were aspirated stops at that stage.
Li treats voiceless aspiration as a contrastive group and lists it to be with the four
phonation groups above in his reconstruction of PT initials (Li 1977: 43). In spite of
the different influence on tone between aspirated voiceless stops and voiceless
continuants in some Tai dialects, Gedney puts them together under his definition
“voiceless frictions” in his Tai tone box (cf. Table 4 in §1.2.1). Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009) rejects Li’s reconstruction of aspiration at the PT level and
argues aspiration should be of post-PT innovation mainly developing from clusters
with medial *-r-. Liang and Zhang (1993, 1996) also suggest voiceless aspirated
121
stops developed on post-PT level from original initial clusters, like *pr-, *tl-, *tr-,
*pw-, *xp-, *xpl-, and *xt- (cf. §1.2.3). All these different points of view on phonation
types of PT initial consonants are summarized as in Table 38.
Table 38 Different speculations of initial phonation types at the PT level
Proposer
Li (1977)
Gedney
(1989[1972])
Liang & Zhang
(1993, 1996)
Pittayawat
Pittayaporn
(2009)
Phonation Types
voiceless
continuants
voiceless
aspirated
stops
voiceless
unaspirated
stops
glottal
sounds
voiced
sounds
voiceless
unaspirated
stops
glottal
sounds
voiced
sounds
×
voiceless
unaspirated
stops
glottal
sounds
voiced
sounds
×
voiceless
unaspirated
stops
glottal
sounds
voiced
sounds
voiceless frictions
voiceless
continuants
voiceless
continuants
Although voiceless aspiration has long been viewed as contrast in PT (Li 1977), this
thesis agrees with the viewpoint suggested by Liang and Zhang (1996) and
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) that voiceless aspirated stops are a post-PT
innovation, since only this can explain the irregularities of aspiration corresponding
within modern Tai varieties with fewest problems.
Liang and Zhang’s argument is based on the observation that the correspondence
between aspirated sounds in modern Kam-Tai languages are very uneven and
irregular. They point out that the vast majority of modern NT languages, some
Southern Thai varieties, and Northern Kam lack aspirated initials, whereas the other
Kam-Tai languages all have aspirated initials. However, the situations of these
aspirated initials are very complicated. In NT, aspirated initials only found in T3
(C1) in the third vernacular of Bouyei, but it is not contrastive. Within more than
one thousand cognates in their data, very few cognates all consistently have
aspirated initials within those Tai varieties which have aspirated initial sounds in
their sound system. For example, the initial of the word ‘arm’ is aspirated in CT and
SWT varieties which have well-developed aspirated initials, but in YN varieties (of
those whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate) which also have robust
aspirated initial sounds in their own sound system, this word is ke:n1 (A1) with an
unaspirated initial sound, agreeing with NT. In Kam-Sui languages which also have
122
robust aspirated initials, the initial of this word is also unaspirated. On the other
hand, some cognates have aspirated initials in Kam-Sui languages but have
unaspirated initials in Tai languages which have robust aspirated initials. Even
within Tai languages which have robust aspirated initials, the aspirated sounds in
some varieties do not always present as aspirated in the other varieties. Therefore,
they assert that aspirated initials are a secondary development after the split of the
Kam-Sui and Tai branches, and even after the splits of different dialects of Tai
varieties. They furthermore list the original segments of aspirated initials in modern
Kam-Tai languages, like that PT voiced stops become voiceless aspirated stops in
Thai, Lao, and Daxin Zhuang, voiced aspirations (in their own reconstruction)
become voiceless aspirated in CT and SWT, and voiceless unaspirated stops plus -w-,
-l-, or -r- clusters become aspirated initials in some of the modern Tai varieties.
However, they also mention that these segments do not always cause aspiration in
all the Kam-Tai languages which have robust aspirated initials (Liang & Zhang 1996:
75-77).
Pittayawat Pittayaporn also points out that the view that aspiration was an original
characteristic and NT lost it was problematic for the following reasons. First, the loss
of aspiration is not a common sound change, and it can hardly explain the situation
of NT since it has been heavily influenced by Chinese where aspiration is robust.
Second, many of Li’s simple aspirated stops actually develop from voicing
alternation which must be considered to be later loans or reflexes of complex onsets.
Third, etyma with invariable aspirated onset in Li’s list are mostly questionable or
irregular. Furthermore, the comparison of aspirations between Tai and Kam-Sui
show disagreements, and the crucial evidence is that aspirated initials has been
claimed not to be an inherited trait in Kam-Sui. Last, he mentions Liang & Zhang’s
viewpoint that modern aspirations have developed from PT clusters (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 88-89).
Data from my fieldwork also show some of the same evidence put forward by Liang
and Zhang as well as Pittayawat Pittayaporn. For example, unaspirated stops with
Tone C1 in the vast majority of NT varieties normally present aspirated in Zhenning
Bouyei, like kʰa:31 ‘to kill’, lok44 kʰa:31 ‘seedling’, tʰã31 ‘to establish’ and kʰou31 ‘nine’.
However, aspirated stops are not found in the other tonal categories in this dialect.
It is crucial that C1 and C2 tones have been merged to be one single tone in this
dialect, but the aspirated initial stops with original C1 tone contrast to unaspirated
initial stops with original C2 tone. This indicates that aspirated stops in this NT
variety are of secondary development in the C column for keeping the semantic
123
distinctions after the merger of the tones C1 and C2. Besides, some aspirated initials
in those varieties whose classification as CT or NT, Sanxiang, Shuangding, and Nung
An in my own fieldwork, present as unaspirated initials in the vast majority of
CT/SWT varieties, like kʰa:ŋ35 (C1-U) ‘fishbone’ in Shuangding YN contrasting to
ka:ŋ24 (C1-U) in Debao Yang Zhuang and ka:ŋ41 (C1-U) in Thai. This also supports
that some aspirations are of independent development. Furthermore, in some
specific varieties, some of the words still preserve unaspirated stops plus -r clusters
which have become simple aspirated initials in most CT/SWT varieties, such as pra:33
(A1-A) ‘stone mountain/hill’ in Shuangding YN contrasting to pʰja:453 ‘stone
mountain/hill’ in Debao Yang Zhuang and pʰa:24 ‘cliff’ in Thai. This also supports the
viewpoint that some of the origins of aspiration are voiceless unaspirated stops plus
-r clusters.
Therefore, this thesis admits the four phonation types, voiceless stops, voiceless
continuants, glottal sounds, and voiced sounds (including voiced stops and voiced
continuants) were at the PT level, but considers voiceless aspirated initials as of
post-PT innovation.
Just as in the previous studies reviewed, after tone arose in the vast majority of PT
voicing contrast was speculated to be lost. In this speculation, in the wave of the two
series of mergers of the PT initial consonants (mergers from voiceless into voiced
continuants due to the voicing process, as well as from voiced to voiceless stops due
to the devoicing process), PT tones felt into two series of primary registers – original
high tones and original low tones conditioned by original voiceless
stops/continuants and original voiced stops/continuants respectively. As has been
repeatedly mentioned, this process is treated as the primary register split, and
formed a system of eight tonal categories, namely tones A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, C2, D1,
and D231.
However, there are some disagreements on the direct cause of the primary tonal
split in some of the previous studies. The disagreement mainly lies in that the cause
was the devoicing of proto-voiced stops or the voicing of proto-voiceless
continuants. As Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997: 208) points out, previous studies
normally select the voiceless-voiced distinction in initial plosives/stops to illustrate
31
Tonal splits in dead syllables are also conditioned by vocalic length to result tonal categories
DL1, DL2, DS1 and DS2, which are treated as secondary development in this thesis. Therefore, they
will be discussed in the following §4.3.3.
124
the cause of tonal split, like in her citations Haudricourt (1961, 1972), Matisoff
(1973), Brown (1975), Mazaudon (1970), Li (1977), Henderson (1981, 1982), and
Gedney (1985). However, in some specific modern Tai varieties, initial consonants
developing from the PT voiceless/voiced continuants present as the only cause of
their primary tonal split. In Cao Bang Tho (of CT) which still preserves the voiced
stops, “the loss of aspiration in the sonorants” seems to be the only cause of tonal
split (Haudricourt 1972: 65). Dai Zhuang (Dai Tho) in which the voicing contrast
between stops is also still preserved, the merger of voiceless sonorants with voiced
sonorants from PT is suggested to be the cause of the primary tone split (Theraphan
L-Thongkum 1997: 215). These two CT dialects are very crucial to a hypothesis that
the cause of the primary tonal split in Tai languages is actually the voicing of protovoiceless continuants, because at least they very clearly indicate that while the
devoicing process of the proto-voiced stops has not yet started, the voicing process
of proto-voiceless continuants has long been completed and tonal register split has
been established since the early days. For further interpreting this phonetic
plausibility, Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997: 209) points out that the sound change
from *m̥ - (partially voiced) to m- (*m̥ -[m̥ m] > m-) takes shorter than that from *b(fully voiced) to p- (*b- > *b̥ - > p-) or to pʰ- (*b- > *b̤ - > *bh- > pʰ-) by citing the
point that voiceless sonorants are actually “partially voiced” when preceding vowels
(Ladefoged 1971: 11).
Haudricourt (1972: 65) and Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997: 212) cautiously indicate
the merger of proto-voiceless and proto-voiced sonorants to be the independent
cause of tonal split in Cao Bang Tho and Dai Zhuang respectively. However, because
voiceless continuants are actually not found in modern Tai varieties at all but voiced
plosives are completely or partially preserved in some specific Tai varieties, this
thesis suggests that the voicing of proto-voiceless continuants (including sonorants
and the sibilant *s-) was the only cause of the primary tonal split cover all Tai
varieties beyond the two varieties above. The following evidence give support to this
suggestion.
First, documentary evidence indicates that in specific Tai varieties that have
completely merged proto-voiced-voiceless plosives, the merging was later than the
tonal split period. Theraphan L-Thongkum cites Brown (1975) and Gedney (1991) to
point out that the low-register plosives (from proto-voiced plosives) in Thai
orthography are written as the original voiced sounds, even though they present
voiceless aspirated in modern Thai. This implies that the devoicing of proto-voiced
125
plosives are quite recent and may be far later than the tonal splits period
(Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997: 207-208).
Second, the merger of proto-voiced-voiceless plosives present as in progress in some
specific Tai varieties which have straightforward tonal split between earlier
voiceless and voiced registers. We have seen that voiced stops (from PT voiced or
low register) are still completely preserved in some modern Tai varieties, like Dai
Zhuang and Cao Bang Tay discussed above. However, there are some Tai varieties
that preserve incomplete voiced stops from PT voiced or low register. In the data
from my own fieldwork, some proto-voiced initial obstruents are preserved as voiced
in Yizhou Suogan Zhuang (Language 36 in my data), like (kaj33) ɣa:ŋ11 (B2 < B1/2)
‘young chicken’ and bəɰ24 (C2) ‘bride’, but some are merged into voiceless sounds,
like tɯŋ24 (C2) ‘crutch’. This implies that the merger of proto-voiced-voiceless
plosives is unfinished. In Yongning Baiji (L25), earlier voiced stops*b- and *d- have
been merged into b̤- and d̤- which developed from earlier *ʔb- and *ʔd- to
complementarily distribute on the higher register and the lower register
respectively. For example, d̤wa:331 (A2) ‘to spread on’, d̤wa:31 (B2) ‘river’, b̤ wa:k31
(DL2) ‘insane’, and b̤wak33 (DS2) ‘very tired’. However, unlike that b̤ - and d̤- on the
higher register sometimes present as p̤- and t ̤-, b̤ - and d̤- on the lower register always
present as breathy voiced sounds. In brief, breathy voice in this dialect is a signature
of the partially voiced in the process of devoicing earlier voiced and glottal plosives,
in contrast voicing of proto-voiceless continuants has much earlier completed to give
the straightforward register tonal split this dialect. These cases all suggest that
devoicing of voiced stops causing the merger of voiced-voiceless stops must be much
later than the voicing of voiceless continuants in Tai languages.
Furthermore, as a cross-linguistic phenomenon found in TK, Mon-Khmer, Miao-Yao,
and Tibeto-Burman languages, voiceless sonorants play an important role in the
process of pitch raising to cause tonal split (Theraphan L-Thongkum 1997: 209-212).
This confirms the plausibility that voicing of proto-voiceless continuants cause
primary tonal split in Tai languages.
The last piece of evidence is just like what has been reviewed in §2.2.3 that the PT
voiceless continuants tend to mostly keep their tonal behaviors as in their primary
tonal splits since they have not been found to have the case that other types of
initials, namely glottalization, aspiration, unaspiration all have influence on
secondary tonal development (Zhang 1980, Liang & Zhang 1996, Zhang et al. 1999).
126
This phenomenon will be furthermore discussed in the process of secondary tonal
splits in the following §4.3.1.
In brief, this thesis suggests that the devoicing of proto-voiced stops or the merger of
voiced-voiceless stops was not the first trigger of register tonal split. Instead, the
voicing of voiceless continuants which led the merger of voiceless-voiceless
continuants was the actual trigger of register tonal split in all Tai languages. This
clarification is considered to be very important in this thesis, since it reveals one
more diachronic tonal developing process in Tai tonal development soon after the
process of tonogenesis.
On the basis of the two clarifications involving the primary tonal split in §4.2.1 and
§4.2.2 above, we get a new structure of initial phonation types in the stage of the
primary tonal split in the post-PT level: voiceless unaspirated stops, glottal sounds,
voiced continuants and voiced stops. The primary register split caused by the
voicing of earlier voiceless continuants formed an eight tonal category system,
namely tones A1, B1, C1, and D1 on high register as well as A2, B2, C2, and D2 on
low register, as shown in Table-1 in §1.2.1. In this stage, it is remarkable that
phonation types of initial consonants coordinated with register split to provide a
regular collocation in the process of tonal development. The merger of voicelessvoiced continuants must lead a result that the remaining voiced continuants
collocated with high or low register tones respectively, at the same time the other
phonation types of initials must also have to choose to be preceding syllables with
either the high or the low register tones. Thus, the choice of high-low register tones
for non-voiced continuants in this stage is essential to the pattern of the primary
tonal split.
In the previous studies on Kam-Tai tones like Weera Ostapirat (1994), the pattern
that voiceless unaspirated stops and glottal sounds went with original voiceless
continuants to condition high register tones (A1, B1, C1, DL1, and DS1), and voiced
plosive went with original voiced continuant to condition low tones (A2, B2, C2,
DL2, and DS2) is called straightforward tonal development32. The choice that protoglottal sounds went with the original voiceless stops/continuants to condition their
32
Straightforward tonal developing pattern in the previous studies normally also includes that
voiceless aspirated stops go with original voiceless continuants, voiceless unaspirated stops, and
glottal sounds to have original high pitches. However, in §4.2.1 we have claimed that voiceless
aspirated stops were of secondary development in post-PT level, therefore they are omitted here and
will be discussed in the following §4.3.2.1 and §4.3.2.2.
127
syllables to have a tone going together with the original high register may also be
explained as that the glottal initial stop -ʔ or the pre-glottal segment are phonetically
voiceless (Li 1977: 44).
Although the straightforward tonal register split pattern it is not the most common
pattern in SWT varieties in Thailand, it is most common found in the other Tai
varieties out of Thailand, as some scholars in the Sinitic circles point out that it is
found in more than half of the Zhuang dialects (Zhang et al. 1999: 245), which are
linguistically of CT and NT respectively. Li Fang-Kuei also treats the straightforward
pattern as his most common Type I of initial-type and register-tone combinations
since it is found in all his three main sub-branches of Tai (Li 1977: 44-45). Within
the forty-two Tai varieties investigated for this thesis, nearly half of them (the
following twenty varieties) have the straightforward tonal register pattern: Kho Lam
Tai Yai, Rong Maet (Tai Lue), and Muong Quan Son Tai in SWT, Jingxi Urban (Yang
Zhuang), Jingxi Anning (Yang Zhuang), Nalong (Myang Zhuang), Xiaoguangnan
(Nong Zhuang), Tuoxin (Pyang Zhuang), and Daxin Naling (Zuojiang Zhuang) in CT,
Baiji (YN) and Shuangding (YN) in those whose classification as NT o CT is open to
debate, as well as Wuming Shuangqiao (Yongbei Zhuang), Debao Longsang
(undescribed), Du’an Jiaren (Central Hongshuihe Zhuang), Shanglin Yunling
(Hongshuihe Zhuang), Xiaosanjiang (Lianshan Zhuang), Yizhou Suogan (Guibei
Zhuang), Donglan (Guibei Zhuang), Baipeng (Liujiang Zhuang), and Zhenning
(Bouyei) in NT, as shown in Appendix C.
The straightforward pattern seems to reflect the primary tonal split triggered by the
voicing of proto-voiceless continuants after tonogenesis, since this pattern is
reflected in the majority of modern Tai varieties which do not reflect further
secondary tonal developing patterns. However, as the following two reasons point
out, the straightforward pattern cannot be treated as identical with the primary
register tonal split pattern in that early stage.
First, as Li points out that not all of this type of straightforward pattern has six
tones33, some of the two tonal categories within the six ones have been merged into
each other. There are five sub-types in this type, namely Ia (B2 = C1) represented
by Shan and Red Tai, Ib (A1 = B2) represented by Tay, Ic (A2 = B2) represented by
Nung, Id (C1 = C2) represented by Chenning Pu-i and Shuicheng Pu-i, as well as Ie
(A2 = B1 and B2 = C2) represented by Phu Thai, make the six tonal categories
33
In Li’s definition, tonal categories (DL1, DL2, DS1, DS2) on checked syllables are treated as
allotones of tones on smooth syllables. Thus, there are at most six register splitting tones (A1, A2, B1,
B2, C1 and C2) on smooth syllables in the straightforward type.
128
result a system with five (Ia, Ib, Ic, and Id) or four (Ie) distinct tones (Li 1977: 45).
In my data, except those having six distinct tones on smooth syllables, the following
Tai varieties also have a five-tone system with the mergers from some of the two
tonal categories. Tai Yai and Quan Son Tai belong to Li’s type Ia (B2 = C1), and
Baiji YN and Zhenning Bouyei belong to type Id (C1 = C2) (cf. Appendix C).
Therefore, at least the sub-types of this straightforward type do not reflect the
speculated pattern in the stage of the primary tonal split.
Second, tonal splits and mergers still lack of agreements on the diachronic process in
previous studies. In the Sinitic circles, scholars like Zhang (1980), Liang and Zhang
(1996: 816-817), and Zhang and colleagues (1999: 243-246) consider the primary
register tonal split was straightforward in all Tai varieties, and those Tai varieties
having non-straightforward tonal development patterns on some of the tonal
categories actually reflect secondary tonal development influenced by some specific
initial phonation types or laryngeal features, like aspiration, glottalization and preglottalization, and voiceless unaspirated stops. Li also doubts that tonal development
influenced by some of the laryngeal initial features like glottalization and aspiration
can be considered a PT phenomenon, and the tone 33 (author note: B1-A)
conditioned by aspiration like in tʰa:ʔ33 ‘to expose to the sun’ and tʰe:ʔ33 ‘to break,
crack’ in his T‘ien-pao (Debao Urban Yang Zhuang) is speculated to be secondary
development when compared with the original tone 42 (author note: B1-U) in such
words pa:ʔ42 ‘mouth’ and pe:t42 ‘eight’ (Li 1977: 26-27). However, some other
scholars have different viewpoints. In interpreting his designation of Tai tone box
(cf. Table 4 in §1.2.1), Gedney uses ‘initials at time of tonal splits’ to define the four
initial groups which condition possible tonal splits (Gedney 1989[1972]: 202). This
hints that different initial phonation types have chosen to collocate with one of the
two or three registers when the primary tonal split occurred. Thus in this viewpoint
even the primary tonal split cannot be straightforward in many Tai varieties which
do not reflect the straightforward pattern, like the other half of the forty-two Tai
varieties investigated in this thesis. Theraphan L-Thongkum (1997: 212) definitely
points out two possible types of primary tonal split in her Tho (author note: Dai
Zhuang and Debao Yang Zhuang) of CT. The first type basically equals to the
straightforward pattern because the split is between voiced sounds and all other
sounds, although the voiced plosives (from *b-, *d-, *g- and etc.) are still preserved
in Dai Zhuang. The second type has the split between voiceless plosives (aspirated
and unaspirated) and those developing from glottal sounds and voiced sounds in PT.
This division of primary tonal split indicates that the proposer treats the primary
tonal split not to be a unified phenomenon in history, but to be separated patterns in
129
individual Tai varieties. However, it seems that she does not account for the
diachronic process of tonal splits since her ‘primary tonal split’ has covered all the
modern tonal patterns in Dai Zhuang and Debao Yang Zhuang.
In spite of the different viewpoints on the diachronic process of register tonal splits,
this thesis somehow agrees with the primary-secondary division of tonal splits
suggested by Zhang (1980), Liang and Zhang (1996), Zhang and colleagues (1999)
and Li (1977). It considers the primary tonal split in Tai languages to be a common
phenomenon in the predecessors of modern Tai varieties in the post-PT period. It
further proposes that the voicing of PT voiceless continuants caused high-low two
series of primary register split to occur on the PT four tonal categories. The primary
register split is speculated to have been a straightforward pattern that grouped
voiceless unaspirated stops and glottal/pre-glottal sounds together to condition tones
in the high register from the voicing of proto-voiced continuants, and grouped
voiced stops to condition tones in the low register depressed by proto-voiced
continuants. Thus, an eight-tone system with Tones A1, B1, C1, and D1 on the high
register and Tones A2, B2, C2 and D2 on the low register tones has been established
in this stage, as shown in Table 39. In this table, the tone pitches and contours are
speculated by combining the speculation of the phonetic characteristics of PT tones
(cf. Table 37). Note that the primary split pattern is speculated to be an eight tonal
category pattern with fixed pitch heights and contours, and is different from the
straightforward split pattern on some of the tonal mergers in some specific modern
Tai varieties. In other words, the straightforward split pattern covers the primary
split pattern, but the latter does not conversely cover the former.
Table 39 The speculated primary register tonal split on post-PT level
Proto-Tone
Register
*A
*B
*C
D*
mid level
high-falling
low-rising
high-falling
A1
B1
C1
D1
high level
high-falling
high-rising
high-falling
A2
B2
C2
D2
low level
low-falling
low-rising
low-falling
High
*m̥ -, *l ̥-, *s- …
*p-, *t-, *k- …
*ʔ-, *ʔb-, *ʔd- …
Low
*m-, *l-, *z- …
*b-, *d-, *g- …
We will prove this hypothesis by the steps in the following sections about the
secondary tonal splits, which form the counterevidence to indicate that all the nonstraightforward tonal developing patterns in Tai languages are secondary.
130
Different from the unified pattern of the primary tonal split, secondary tonal splits in
Tai differ from dialect to dialect, or even from variety to variety within the same
language. The most typical example is the totally different secondary tonal split
patterns among different varieties in Debao Yang Zhuang and Jingxi Yang Zhuang,
which are under the same distinct language with “zyg” as the ISO 639-3 code (cf.
Languages 8-13 in the Appendix C). This phenomenon shows that secondary tonal
splits occurred after the formations of different hierarchies of languages and dialects
in Tai.
On account of interpreting the whole scheme of Tai tonal development, it is
necessary to outline the diachronic hierarchies of secondary tonal splits in different
groups and varieties of Tai languages. Within the following sub-sections of this
section, §4.3.1 aims to prove that the non-straightforward patterns of tonal splits in
Tai are all secondary development, §4.3.2 is separated into five sub-sections to
indicate the diachronic hierarchies of secondary tonal splits on smooth syllables, and
the last §4.3.3 discusses the secondary tonal split patterns in checked syllables in Tai
languages.
As pointed out in §4.2.2, initials developing from PT voiceless continuants tend to
mostly keep their tonal behaviors as in their primary tonal split since they are of the
only type that has only conditioned straightforward pattern of register split. That is,
non-straightforward tonal splits have not been found to be conditioned by initials
developing from proto-voiceless continuants. Moreover, it has been suggested that
initials developing from proto-voiceless continuants was the only cause of the
primary tonal split in Tai. Based on the reasoning above, it is apparent to receive a
motivation that non-straightforward tonal splits conditioned by all the other types of
initials, namely glottalized sounds, aspirated stops, and unaspirated stops are all
secondary in the history of Tai languages. Previous studies, like Zhang (1980), Liang
and Zhang (1996), and Zhang and colleagues (1999) have already provided
sufficient discussions to achieve this suggestion, which is agreed with in this thesis.
The reasons are summarized as follows.
First, the straightforward pattern of register tonal split is cross-dialect within the
Kam-Tai branch above the Tai group and the Kam-Sui (KS) group, while the non-
131
straightforward patterns are in an irregular distribution. The Zhuang dialects (of NT
and CT) from the most areas, Tai Lue (of SWT), Shan (of SWT), as well as Sui (of KS)
and Mulam (of KS) all reflect this pattern, and the tonal correspondence is basically
regularly agreed with each other among these Kam-Tai varieties (Liang & Zhang
1996: 817). However, some of the Zhuang dialects (of NT and CT) from the western
and northwestern parts of Guangxi, and from some counties around Nanning City,
the majority of the Bouyei dialects (of NT), the Dai dialects (of WT) from the
prefectures of Dehong and Honghe from Yunnan, Thai and Lao, as well as Kam,
Maonan, Ai-Cham, and Mak (which are all of KS) all have non-straightforward
patterns of tonal splits on the high register. The tonal correspondence among these
non-straightforward tonal splits is irregular. Besides, within these languages some
have splits on the high register on all the proto-tones, but some have splits only on
one to three proto-tones (Liang & Zhang 1996: 818). Only if that the straightforward
pattern reflects the primary split, and the non-straightforward patterns reflect
secondary splits, can these phenomena be reasonably explained.
Second, the causes of non-straightforward splits on the high register in Tai are found
to be restricted to glottalized sounds, aspirated stops, and unaspirated stops. These
non-straightforward patterns are foresightedly treated as secondary tonal splits by
Zhang and colleagues (1999: 243-245). On the basis of previous studies (Zhang
1980, Liang & Zhang 1996), they divided these secondary tonal splits into four types
as follows.
(1) Type 1: only glottal initials condition secondary tonal splits. These glottal initials
are ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-, ˀj- and those developing from proto-glottal initials such as j- (< *ʔj-),
m- (< *ʔb-), and n- (< *ʔd-). This type occurs in the three NZ languages Youjiang,
Guibian, and Qiubei, as well as in some varieties in some other Zhuang languages
such as Donglan, Tian’e, Nandan of NZ (of NT) and Tiandeng of SZ (of CT) (Zhang et
al. 1999: 243). As shown in the following glossary comparisons in Table 40, the first
four represent those having Type 1 of secondary tonal splits, and Wuming represents
those having the straightforward tonal split pattern.
132
Table 40 Type 1 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from
Zhang et al. 1999: 243-244)34
Examples
Modern
Tai
varieties
Situations
initials
of tone
causing
changes
secondary (secondary
‘good’
‘leaf’
‘cough’
‘get’
‘village’ ‘scold’
‘at’
‘out’
A1
A1
A1
C1
C1
B1
B1
DL1
ˀdi:1
ˀbaj1
ʔaj1
ˀdaj4
ˀba:n4
ˀda:5
ʔu5
ʔo:k9
ˀdi:1
ˀbaj1
ʔaj1
ˀdaj4
ˀba:n4
ˀda:6
ˀji:6
(6<10<9)
ˀdej1
ˀbaɰ1
ʔaj1
ˀdaj3
ˀba:n3
ˀda:6
ˀjow6
ʔo:k10
naj2
maɰ2
ʔaj2
naj3
ma:n3
na:5
jow5
ʔo:k9
ˀdaj1
ˀbaɰ1
ʔaj1
ˀdaj3
ˀba:n3
ˀda:5
ˀjaw5
ʔo:k9
tonal splits tonal splits)
Tian-dong ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-,
(NT)
Tian’e
(NT)
Donglan
(NT)
ˀj-
ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-,
ˀj-
ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-,
ˀj-
Tiandeng
ʔ-, m- (<
(CT)
*ʔb-), n- (<
3→4
3→4
5→6
9 → 10
5→6
9 → 10
ʔo:6
1→2
*ʔd-), j- (<
*ʔj-)
Wuming
(NT)
ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-,
ˀj-
Straightforward
Take the word ʔo:6 ‘out’ in Tian’e (NT) in Table 40 as an example to interpret Type 1
demonstrated. This word contains complicated tonal changes – secondary tonal split
associating with tonal merger due to the loss of final -k of the original checked
syllable. The original tonal category T9 (DL1) was changed to T10 (DL2) due to the
secondary tonal split conditioned by the initial ʔ-, and T10 (DL2) merged into T6
(B2) due to the loss of the syllable final -k (Zhang et al. 1999: 240).
Within the twenty-two languages that have non-straightforward tonal development
patterns from my fieldwork, Chiang Rai (L4) of SWT, Hurun Yang Zhuang (L11) and
Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang (L17) of CT, as well as Dongling Youjiang Zhuang (L32),
Lizhou Guibian Zhuang (L33), Gehan Qiubei Zhuang (L39), and Huishui Bouyei
(L41) of NT are of this type. Lingding Zuojiang Zhuang (L19) is partly of this type
but partly of Type 3 (see the detail in Type 3 below).
(2) Type 2: Only aspiration conditions secondary tonal splits. These initials are all
aspirated stops and this type is only found in Wenma Dialect (Dai Zhuang) of SZ
34
The numeral style of tones in Table 40 (similarly hereinafter) follows the style in the Sinitic
Circles. The correspondence among it and the other styles of tonal numerals has been explained in
§1.2.1. The shaded boxes refer to those cases being tone changes (secondary tonal splits) occurring.
133
(CT). The situations of secondary tonal splits present as in that T1 → T2, and T7 →
T2 (Zhang et al. 1999: 244). In the following glossary comparisons, Wenma
represents those having Type 2 of secondary tonal splits (in shaded), and Jingxi
represents those having the straightforward tonal split pattern as shown in Table 41.
Table 41 Type 2 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from
Zhang et al. 1999: 244)
Gloss
Wenma
(Dai Zhuang)
Jingxi
(Yang Zhuang)
‘dam’
‘eye’
‘leg’
‘warehouse’
‘vegetable’
‘six’
pʰ :2
tʰo:2
kʰo:2
tsʰɛ:2
pʰe:2
tsʰa:2
pʰa:j1
tʰa:1
kʰa:1
ɕa:ŋ1
pʰjak7
kʰjɔ:k7
Within the data from my fieldwork, only Dazhai Dai Zhuang (L21) of CT is directly
of this type. Huashan Min Zhuang (as shown in the tone box in L16 in Appendix C)
represent a special case of Type 2. The pitches in tones *A and *DS are depressed by
aspirated initials (both the rows 1-A, 1-UC, and 1/2-A) to form a secondary tonal
split. In Tone *A this split causes a three-way split pattern (1’-1-2), but in *DS, in
spite of this split all other rows (1-C, 1-U, 1-G, and 2) are merged together or lack of
split to keep the two-way split pattern. The situations of Bac Va Nung (as shown in
the tone box in L24 in Appendix C) is more complicated. In Bac Va, the secondary
splits in Tones *A and *DL have Type 2, in which the aspirated sounds (both the
rows 1-A and 1-UC) conditioned a secondary tonal split, but the secondary splits in
Tone *B have Type 3 (which is discussed below) with aspirated stops and glottal
sounds being the double conditioning causes. This dialect can be treated as having a
mixed Type 2/3.
(3) Type 3: Both aspirated stops and glottal sounds condition secondary tonal splits.
This type is found in the Zhuang varieties like Yongnan, Fubei, Long’an (all of those
whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate) and Debao Mai’ai (of Yang
Zhuang of CT) (Zhang et al. 1999: 244). As shown in the glossary comparisons in
Table 42, the first four represent those having Type 3 of secondary tonal splits, and
Longzhou represents those having the straightforward tonal split pattern.
134
Table 42 Type 3 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from
Zhang et al. 1999: 244-245)
Examples
Modern initials Situations of
Tai
causing
varieties secondary tonal
splits
Yongnan Glottal Sounds:
ʔ-, m- (<*ʔb ),
n- (< *ʔd), j- (<
*ʔj-);
Fubei
tone changes
(secondary
‘stone
‘sugar-
mountain’ cane’
‘spring’ ‘at’
‘carry
by pole’
‘one’
tonal splits)
A1
A1
A1
C1
C1
B1
B1
DL1
naj1'
ʔaw1'
pʰla:1'
ʔo:j5
mo:6
jow6 tʰa:p10
ʔət7
naj1'
ʔaw1’
pʰa:1’
ʔo:j3
mo:5
jəw5
tʰa:p9
ʔit7'
naj1
ʔaw1
pʰla:1
ʔɔ:j3
mo:5'
jow5' tʰa:p9'
ʔit8
ˀdɔ:j2
ʔaw2
pʰja:2
ʔo:j3
ˀbo:6
ˀjow6 tʰa:p10
ʔat7
ˀdaɰ1
ʔaw1
pʰja:1
ʔo:j3
ˀbo:5
1 →1'
3 →5
5 →6
Aspirated stops:
9 →10
pʰ-, tʰ-, kʰ-, ...
1 →1'
7 → 7'
Long’an
‘inside’ ‘take’
5 → 5'
7→8
9 → 9'
(Ma’ai)
Glotal Sounds: ʔ-,
ˀb-, ˀd-, ˀj-...;
Aspirated stops:
pʰ-, tʰ-, kʰ-, ...
Longzhou Glotal Sounds: ʔ-,
ˀb-, ˀd-, j- (<
*ʔj-) ...;
Aspirated stops:
pʰ-, tʰ-, kʰ-, ...
1→2
5→6
9 → 10
straightforward
Debao
ju:5
ha:p9
ʔit7
In Table 42, tonal category markers with an inverted comma like 1', 5', 7', and 9'
refer to the so-called “derived tones” (派生調) in the Sinitic circles working on KamTai languages. Such kind of derived tones actually refer to one of the splits of the socall “three-way split”. The details will be discussed in the following §4.3.2.5. In this
thesis, we also adapt this convention to mark “derived tones” when referencing to
the previous studies in the Sinitic circles. The correspondence among the style in the
Sinitic circles and the other styles of tonal numerals has been explained in §1.2.1.
The shaded boxes refer to those cases being secondary tonal splits occurring.
Within the data from my fieldwork, Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (L9), and Debao
Luliu Yang Zhuang (L10) of CT, as well as Long’an YN (L27), Daqiu YN (L28), and
Xialeng YN (L29) of those whose classification as CT or NT is open to debate are of
135
this type. The situation of Lingding Zuojiang Zhuang (L19) and Debao Urban Yang
Zhuang (L8) are complicated. In Lingding, the secondary splits in Tones *B and *DL
are only conditioned by glottal sounds to be of Type 1, but in Tone *A the splits are
conditioned by aspirated and glottalized initials to be of Type 3. However, the split
conditioned by aspirated is distinct from the split conditioned by glottalized initials.
The former (A1-1, A1-UC, A1/2-A) is depressed to be a derived tone 353 lower than
the original A1 tone 554, but the latter (A1-G) is depressed to merge into A2 to be
an extreme low tone 11. This results a three-way split in Tone *A. In Debao Urban,
the secondary splits have Type 1 in Tone *A, but have Type 3 in Tones *B and *DL.
These two dialects can be treated as having a mixed Type 1/3.
(4) Type 4: Both glottal sounds and unaspirated stops condition secondary tonal
splits. This type is only found in Houyi of Daxin Zuojiang Zhuang in SZ. The
situation of secondary tonal splits present as in that T1 → T1’, T5 → T6, and T7 long
(or 9) → T8 long (or 10) as shown in Table 43. The conditions of this type of tonal
splits is the same as Thai and Lao (Zhang et al. 1999: 245). In the following glossary
comparisons, Daxin Houyi (of Zuojiang Zhuang) represents this Type 2 of secondary
tonal splits (in shaded), and Longzhou Urban (of Zuojiang Zhuang) represents those
having the straightforward tonal split pattern.
Table 43 Type 4 of secondary tonal splits in the Zhuang dialects (adapted from
Zhang et al. 1999: 245)
Gloss
Daxin Houyi
(Zuojiang Zhuang)
Longzhou Urban
(Zuojiang Zhuang)
‘good’
‘year’
‘eat’
‘salt’
‘shoulder’
‘pass’
‘at’
‘out’
ˀdaj1'
pi:1'
kin1'
tsɯə1'
ˀba:6
kwa:6
ˀju:6
ʔo:k10
ˀdaj1
pi:1
kin1
kɯ:1
ˀba:5
kwa:5
ju:5
ʔo:k9
Within the data from my fieldwork, Bangkok Thai (L1), Songkhla Southern Thai
(L2), and Khon Kaen Isan (L3) of SWT, as well as Leiping Zuojiang Zhuang (L22) and
Baoxu Zuojiang Zhuang (L23) of CT are of this type.
Based upon to the presentations of the four types of non-straightforward tonal splits
above, we see that within the three phonation types that condition the claiming
secondary tonal splits, glottalized sounds and aspirated stops can condition nonstraightforward tonal splits alone (Type 1 and Type 2) or together with one another
(Type 3). However, unaspirated stops have not been found to condition nonstraightforward tonal splits by itself or together with aspirated stops, but only
136
condition the splits together with glottalized sounds (Type 4) (cf. Zhang 1980: 38,
Liang & Zhang 1996: 826), perhaps due to that unaspirated stops share the same
features [+voiceless, -contituant, -aspirated] with the segment ʔ- of glottalized
sounds. This adequately indicates that at least unaspirated stops must secondarily
condition the tonal split going after the split triggered by glottalized sounds. Since
voiceless aspirated stops is suggested to be secondary development on post-PT level
(cf. §4.2.1), the tonal splits on high register conditioned alone by aspirated stops
have to be secondary. Consequently, tonal splits conditioned together by aspirated
stops and glottalized sounds also have to be secondary.
Third, non-straightforward tonal splits in some specific Tai varieties give a
suggestion of the diachronic orders. The tonal splits in Southern Thai (cf. the tonal
pattern of L2 in Appendix C) provide a good example. Only if the following
diachronic process of secondary tonal splits in Southern Thai proposed by Zhang
(1980: 38) is true, can the tonal split patterns in this language be reasonable
explained. Zhang describes that after the primary tonal split, in Southern Thai Tone
5 (B1) with all proto-voiceless initials (voiceless stops, voiceless continuants, and
glottal sounds) merged into T1 (A1) before the non-straightforward tonal splits
occurred. After the secondary tonal splits conditioned by glottal sounds and
voiceless unaspirated stops (Type 4), T1 (A1 and those merged from B1) derives T1'
(A1-U/G as well as B1-A/G merging into A1-U/G), T3 (C1) derives T3' (C1-U/G),
and T7 (DS1-U/G) is merged into Tone 8 (DS2)35. Therefore, voiceless frictions
(including aspirated stops and continuants) only precede syllables with original T1
(including original T5 or B1 which merged into T1 or A1), T3, and T7, while
voiceless unaspirated stops and glottal sounds only precede syllables with T1', T3',
and T8. If the tonal splits conditioned by glottal sounds and voiceless unaspirated
stops (Type 4) occurred on the time of the primary tonal split before the merger
between original T1 (A1) and T5 (B1), at least the neat mergers between T1 (A1A/C) and T5 (B1-A/C) as well as T1' (B1-U/G) and T5' (B1-U/G) will not be
reasonably explained.
Zhang furthermore provides an example from Yongning Xialeng of YN to indicate
the diachronic order of secondary tonal splits. Xialeng has a non-straightforward
35
In Zhang’s data the Southern Thai tones are only transcribed in tonal categories like T1, T3, T5,
T7, T8, T1', and T3' with no tonal values. She does not give the specific location to her Southern Thai.
The secondary tonal splits conditioned by glottal sounds and voiceless unaspirated stops are identical
to my data of Hat Yai of Southern Thai (see L2 in Appendix C) except the merger from T7 (DS1-U
and DS1-G) to T8 (DS2), which presents as a derived T7' (DS1-U and DS1-G) with high-rising
contour (45) different from T8 (DS2) with low-falling contour (21).
137
tonal split pattern of Type 3, which presents the split condition being glottal sounds
together with aspirated stops (cf. the tonal pattern of L29 in Appendix C). Under this
condition, T1 (A1) derives T1' (A1-A/G), T3 (C1-A/G) changes to merge into T5 (B1C/U), T5 (B1-A/G) changes to merge into T6 (B2). The latter two changes both
involve in T5 (original B1). One explanation making sense is that the original T5
conditioned by aspirated and glottal initials depressed to change to merge into T6
firstly, and this process gave the original tonal space of T5 (B1-A/G) for Tone 3 (C1A/G) conditioned by aspirated and glottal initials to change itself to take over
(Zhang 1980: 38). Some aberrant examples in Xialeng give more hints for
interpreting this proposal. In this variety, the term haɰ6 ‘to give’ with the aspirated
initial (h- has the same tonal behaviors with aspirated stops in this dialect) should
have had T5 (=B1-C/U) being changed its original T3 (C1-A) when being
conditioned by aspirated initials (vs. the cognate reflecting *haɰC in the majorities of
Tai language) due to the regular rule. However, this term actually has T6 (merging
into B2). It is possible that when the process that B1-A/G changed to merge into B2
was not yet completely accomplished, the process that C1-A/G changed to occupy
the tonal space of original B1-A/G already started, and some single terms from
original C1-A/G like haɰ5<3 ‘to give’ felt into original tonal space of T5 (B1-A/G)
first and then felt into T6 (B1-A/G > B2) along with all other terms with T5
conditioned by A/G to give a result haɰ6. After the process that B1-A/G merging
into B2 was completely accomplished, most other terms with original T3 (A/G) were
afterward changed to original T5 but do not fall into B2 anymore (Zhang 1980: 38).
The proposal confirms that sound changes are gradually from some single words to
spread to all other words. That is to say, there were different diachronic orders even
within the non-straightforward tonal split patterns or the claiming secondary tonal
splits.
The last evidence is in the irregularities of tonal correspondence among some closely
related varieties within a distinct language with the same ISO 639-3 code like Yang
Zhuang (of CT). A survey of nineteen locations crossing the area of the language
varieties below found evidence “from intelligibility, from similarity of wordlists (as
determined by a string edit distance algorithm), and from speaker attitudes” has
assigned them to the distinct ISO 639-3 language codes [zyg] “Yang Zhuang”
(Jackson et al. 2011: V). In the data from my fieldwork, the tone boxes from six
Yang Zhuang varieties (L8 – L13 in Appendix C) present the patterns as follows.
When tones are conditioned by initials developing from proto-voiceless continuants
and unaspirated stops, all these Yang Zhuang varieties agree on the primary split
between voiceless-voiced registers. However, when tones are conditioned by
138
aspirated initials (including h- and ɕ-) and initials developing from proto-glottal
sounds, they all have different patterns of tonal splits: Jingxi Urban (L12) and Jingxi
Anning (L13) have straightforward patterns but are different in the tonal mergers on
checked syllables, Jingxi Hurun (L11) has Type 1 of non-straightforward patterns
(only conditioned by glottal initials), Debao Dalong (L9) and Debao Lüliu (L10) have
Type 3 of non-straightforward patterns (conditioned by both glottalized and
aspirated initials) but are all different in the details of tonal mergers in checked
syllables, and Debao Urban (L8) is of a mixed Type2/3. When being conditioned by
glottalized and aspirated initials, Debao Dalong and Debao Lüliu both merge their
high register tones into the tones in the counterpart low register in the tonal
categories A, B, and DL, but disagree on the DS2 tone – the former one merge its
DS2 into C2, while the latter one merge its DS2 into B2. For Debao Urban, the
situation is more complicated (just like in the discussion of Type 2 above). When
being conditioned by glottalized initials, it agrees with the other two ones to merge
its high tones into the low tones in Columns A, B, and DL; however, when being
conditioned by aspirated initials, it only merges its high tones into the low tones in
Columns B and DL, but keep the high tones in A. That is to say, even in the same
distinct language, the closed related Yang Zhuang varieties36 have different patterns
on non-straightforward tonal splits which are conditioned only by glottal sounds or
by aspirated sounds together with glottal sounds. This phenomenon indicates that
the non-straightforward tonal splits must be of quite late secondary development in
the Yang Zhuang varieties, since the split among these varieties cannot be early as
the time of primary tonal splits.
In conclusion, the non-straightforward tonal splits only conditioned by glottalized
sounds, aspirated stops, and/or unaspirated stops in Tai languages cannot be
primary. This directly denies that the primary tonal split can be conditioned by the
merger of glottal sounds and voiced implosives together with the mergers of
voiceless-voiced sonorants in Wenma Tho (Dai Zhuang) suggested by Theraphan LThongkum (1997: 209, 212).
36
These Yang Zhuang varieties are not only linguistically closed related to each other, but also
geographically closed to one other. For examples by straight-line distance, Debao Urban, Debao
Dalong and Debao Lüliu distribute as a triangle with about three kilometers from one another. Jingxi
Urban is about 35 kilometers from Debao Urban. Jingxi Anning is about 35 kilometers from Jingxi
Urban, and Jingxi Hurun is about 40 kilometers from Jingxi Urban. The distribution of these
languages is shown on the map in Appendix D.
139
As what has been discussed in §4.3.1 above, within the secondary tonal splits (on
smooth syllables), the periods of that conditioned by different phonation types of
initials must be different. That is, secondary tonal splits must be divided into
different diachronic sequences. In this section, the determination of the diachronic
sequences of secondary tonal splits is based on the degree of the dialectal uniformity
that secondary tonal splits occur. When determining these diachronic sequences, the
following discussions will show that some of the secondary tonal split patterns need
to be discussed under the three Tai sub-branches altogether, but the other patterns
are better to be divided into different language groups to discuss because most of
these secondary tonal patterns have individually developed in different languages,
dialects, and/or varieties.
As shown in the discussions in §4.3.1 above, glottalized sounds are found to
condition secondary tonal splits in Li’s all three Tai sub-branches, namely NT, CT,
and SWT. The other two groups of initials, voiceless aspirated stops and voiceless
unaspirated stops, are found to only condition secondary tonal splits in CT and in
CT/SWT respectively. This seemingly suggests that glottalized sounds earliest
condition secondary tonal splits within the three groups of initials. However,
secondary tonal splits conditioned by glottalized initials present quite different
features between NT and CT/SWT languages, as what has been partly discussed in
§4.1.3.3.1 that these splits are normally found in the *C tone and are sometimes
found in the *B tone in NT languages, but mostly found in the *A and *B tones in
CT/SWT languages. In other words, glottalized initials do not uniformly condition
tonal splits in the Tai languages like the merger of voiceless-voiced continuants
conditioning the primary tonal split. The tonal splits conditioned by glottalized
initials may have developed individually in different Tai sub-groups, just like
voiceless aspirated and unaspirated initials.
Aspirated initials, on the other hand, present to condition secondary tonal splits only
in CT and those classification as CT or NT is open to debate. Besides, aspirated
initials are treated to be of secondary development which still does not develop in
the most majority of NT. In this sense, secondary tonal splits conditioned by
aspirated initials could not arise earlier than those conditioned by glottalized
initials. However, aspirated initials involving voicing alternation has provided a
specific case that a series of tonal splits cover all the three Tai sub-branches. Based
140
upon their widest dialectal uniformity in the secondary tonal patterns, voicing
alternation is suggested to arise later than the primary tonal split (since the splits
follow the high-low registers), but earlier than the other secondary tonal splits
including those conditioned by glottalized initials.
In the following sub-sections of this section, voicing alternation uniformly covering
all the Tai varieties will be firstly discussed since it is treated as the earliest
secondary tonal developing pattern. It is impossible to determine the diachronic
order of the splits conditioned by glottalized initials and those conditioned by
aspirated initials, because they are all individual development in different
languages, dialects, and/or varieties. However, because splits conditioned by
glottalized initials are more commonly found in all three sub-branches of Tai, the
discussion of it as well as its continuation, splits conditioned by voiceless
unaspirated initials, is probed into right after the discussion of voicing alternation.
Subsequently, tonal splits conditioned by aspirated initials only found in CT and
voicing alternation on the low register only found in NT are successively discussed.
Finally, we will revisit the two-way split and three-way split patterns in some
specific Tai varieties.
In the previous discussions, we have defined that voicing alternation covering all the
Tai varieties has the following two distinctive features: 1) the conditioning initial
group presents as unaspirated initials in NT but mostly as aspirated (including h-) in
CT/SWT; 2) the tones conditioned by this conditioning group fall into the low
register in NT, but fall into the high register in CT/SWT (cf. §1.2.2, §1.2.3 and
§2.2.3). However, there is another series of voicing alternation attracting less
attentions in the previous studies, and the situation of tonal splits/mergers is
opposite to the aforementioned one. In this series of voicing alternation, tones fall
into the high register in NT, but fall into the low register in CT/SWT, as shown in
the following Table 44.
141
Table 44 Series 2/1 of voicing alternation in Tai
Gedney’s ST
Gloss
‘come’
‘man’
‘listen’
‘be
back’
‘blow’
Li’s SWT
CT/NT?
Li’s CT
NT
Bangkok
Tai
Jingxi
Daxin
Shuang-
Nung
Thai
Lue
Urban
Baoxu
ding
An
ma:
A2
ma:
A2
ma:
A2
ma:
A2
ma:
Wuming
Dongling
A1
ma:A1
ma:A1
ma:A1
θa:jA1
θa:jA1
ɬa:jA1
tɕʰa:jA2
tɕa:jA2
tɕa:jA2
ɕa:jA2
sa:jA1
-
-
təŋB2
tʰiŋB2
tʰəŋB1
tiŋB1
-
-
ta:wB2
tʰe:wB2
ta:wB1
ta:wB1
ta:wB1
ta:wB1
pʰatDS2
patDS2
patDS2
pʰatDS2
pʰatDS1
patDS1
patDS1
patDS1
tiŋB1
tiŋB1
Comparing with the common voicing alternation whose examples are plenty found,
this series of voicing alternation is very rare found, and do not provide a specific
hints of conditioning feature. For distinguishing these two opposite voicing
alternation, the former one is called Series 1/2 (following Liao & Shen 2012) and the
latter one is named Series 2/1 in this thesis.
We have reviewed that in the previous studies the conditioning causes of Series 1/2
of voicing alternation are considered to be early breathy sounds (Liao & Shen 2012),
proto-voiced aspiration (Li 1989, Liang & Zhang 1996, Zhang et al. 1999), a series of
voiced initials *B, *D, *G, *V, *Z and *ɣ- (Gedney 1989), as well as a series of
sesquisyllabic initial clusters (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009). Pittayawat Pittayaporn
considers that except those developing from proto-sesquisyllabic initial clusters,
many of the voicing alternation are actually of those borrowed from Chinese in
history and have nothing to do with Tai source. The first three ones share a common
agreement on that they all have voiced and aspirated features which are inherited in
NT and CT/SWT respectively. Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s viewpoint may be related to
Weera Ostapirat (2006), which firstly systematically suggests the “disyllabic” initial
cluster (consonantal onsets plus another consonantal medial) to indicate the cause of
voicing alternation in Proto-Kam-Sui (PKS). Although Pittayawat Pittayaporn and
Weera Ostapirat do not have an agreement on the sesquisyllabic structure and
disyllabic structure in PT and PKS respectively, their different structures agree on
that consonantal onsets plus another consonantal medial (different from
monosyllabic consonantal clusters) should have existed in these two related
language branches in their respective proto-level.
This thesis agrees on Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s viewpoint that sesquisyllabic and
monosyllabic structures coexisted at the PT level, since it “best accounts for the wide
142
range of sound correspondences observed among the daughter languages”
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 162). He points out that the growing consensus on
that monosyllabic structure has shortcomings on accounting the wide range of onset
correspondence within cognates from Proto-TK, on the other hand the sesquisyllabic
structure can straightforwardly capture these data (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 40,
44). He furthermore enumerates several sets of onset correspondence of Tai
languages to indicate that the monosyllabic view is problematic when interpreting
these sets of data, while sesquisyllabic view can account for these problems
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 44-52). For example, Li’s reconstructions *pʰr-, *tr-,
and *pr- (Li 1977) are challenged by Pittayawat Pittayaporn as in the following
Table 45.
Table 45 Correspondence involving labial clusters by monosyllabic and
sesquisyllabic views (adapted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 48-49)
Gloss
Li
SWT
(1977)
Pittayawat
Pittayaporn
Saek
piəkDS1
pʰja:jC1
pʰrakDS1
pja:jC1
-
pʰomA1
pʰjumA1
piəmA1
pʰramA1
ta:A1
ha:A1
ta:A1
pra:A1
ta:jA1
ha:jA1
ta:jA1
pra:jA1
‘grasshopper’37
takDS1
-
takDS1
-
‘to exposed’
ta:kDL1
pʰja:kDL1
ta:kDL1
pra:kDL1
te:kDL1
pre:kDL1
tawB1
-
‘to walk’
*pʰ-
*pʰr-
(2009)
pʰakDS1
*pr-
‘head hair’
‘eye’
‘to die’
2
‘to burst’
3
*t-
*t-
*tr-
*pr-
Thai
Lung-
Yay
‘vegetable’
1
Proto-
*p.t-
*p.r-
‘to hunt’
pʰa:jC1
tɛ:kDL1
-
chow
pʰjakDS1
pʰe:kDL1
pʰjawB1
Pittayawat Pittayaporn first points out that Li’s reconstruction on Set 2 and Set 3
cannot account for the same containing of labial consonant suggested by Saek. Set 2
of Saek cannot be treated as innovation because they are attested by the cognate
forms in some Tai or KS varieties like pʰja:A1 ‘eye’ in Bao Yen of CT and pla:A1 ‘eye’ in
Lakkja of KS38. Li’s set 1 is also not supported by the view that PT lacked of
aspirated sounds. As Li’s *pr- must be reconstructed by his another viewpoint on
aspiration of original PT clusters (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 93-101), he gives
two sesquisyllabic clusters *p.t- and *p.r- to replace Li’s *tr- and *pr- respectively. He
In Table 45, the full form of the word for ‘grasshopper’ in Thai is actually takDS1 ka-tɛ:nA1.
Quite a few NT varieties also support the labial consonant contained in Set 2 of Saek, like pja:A1
‘eye’ in Liujiang, Huanjiang, Rong’an, Longsheng, and Hechi (cf. Zhang et al. 1999: 640).
37
38
143
explains that *p.r- merged into *p.t- then later simplified to t- in Thai and Yai, on the
contrary *p.t- merged into *p.r- then later changed to pr- in Saek. In contrast,
Lungchow keeps the distinct by the modern reflexes h- and pʰj- (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 48-49). On the development from p.t- to h- and from p.r- to pʰj- in
Lungchow, he suggests the processes *p.t- > *p.r- > *ʰr- > h- (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 163) and *p.r- > *pr- > *pʰj- respectively (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 171). In short, his sesquisyllabic structure of PT can successfully
accounts for both labial reflexes on Set 2 and Set 3 without losing the distinction,
which cannot be explained by the monosyllabic view.
Besides, as we have suggested that there was no contrasting voiceless aspiration in
PT level (cf. §4.2.1), so do Liang and Zhang (1996: 106-107) and Zhang and
colleagues (1999: 211). This is crucial to query that the breathy sounds (cf. Liao &
Shen 2012) or voiced aspirated sounds (cf. Li 1989, Liang & Zhang 1996, Zhang et
al. 1999) which cause Series 1/2 of voicing alternation existed at the PT level since
it was uncommon that the aspiration isolatedly existed in the voiced group if there
was no contrasting aspiration existing in the voiceless group, thus Liang and Zhang
(1996) and Zhang and colleagues (1999) may have a self-contradiction in this point.
If we furthermore explore the more compellent suggestion for explaining voicing
alternation occurring in PKS (cf. Weera Ostapirat 2006), we will see Pittayawat
Pittayaporn’s sesquisyllabic onset can also account for the factors causing voicing
alternation via the later breathy or voiced-aspirated procedure. As what has been
reviewed in §2.2.3, Weera Ostapirat (1994) suggests that a series of breathy/voicedaspirated sounds were the actual factor for causing voicing alternation on sonorants
among KS languages. In his new scheme for the cause (disyllabic onsets) of the
voicing alternation in KS languages, he suggests that these sonorants were originally
medials, the non-initial segment of the disyllabic onset or Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s
sesquisyllabic onset, giving a byproduct breath/aspiration feature which finally give
a tone in tonal series 1 (the high register) in some KS languages (Weera Ostapirat
2006: 1113-1114). That is, the breathy/voiced aspiration which is proposed to
appropriately account for the voicing alternation between NT and CT/SWT may
have secondarily appeared in history following their predecessors – a specific series
of sesquisyllabic onsets. Thus, the main different theories of the conditioning cause
of voicing alternation, breathy/voiced aspirated sounds (Gedney 1989, Li 1989,
Liang & Zhang 1996, Zhang et al. 1999, Liao & Shen 2012) and a series of
sesquisyllabic onsets (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009), become consistent on different
diachronic sequence in the method from Weera Ostapirat’s explanation. We
therefore believe that there were no breathy or voiced aspirated sounds at the PT
144
level, instead sesquisyllabic onsets including voicing alternation between the later
NT and ST (the Proto-CT/SWT) did exist at the PT level.
This proposal can also explain that many items with voicing alternation were actual
OC and MC loans terms originally with pre-nasalized initials (cf. Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 86), since the process of dropping of pre-nasalized segment might
also cause voiced aspiration or breathiness. Those loans were borrowed as with
aspirated initials (conditioning a high register tone) into CT/SWT. On the other
hand, the voiced property in the breathiness of these loans became plain voiced
stops (later conditioning a low register tone) in NT which do not have aspirated
sounds.
Table 46 Etyma with PT *C̬.t- (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 168)
Gloss
PT
Thai
Lungchow
Yay
‘to arrive’
*C̬.tɤŋA
tʰɯŋA1
tʰɤŋA1
taŋA2
*C̬.tɯ:kD
tʰu:kDL1
tʰukDS1
tɯkDS2
*C̬.tɯəlB
tʰɯənB1
tʰɤ:nA1 (tone change)
tɯənB2
*C̬.tuəmB
tʰuəmB1
tʰu:mB1
tu:mB2
‘to come into contact’
‘wild’
‘to flood’ 39
The next concern is the determination of the phonetic characteristics of the two
series of sesquisyllabic onsets that caused voicing alternations or alternations of
tonal series. Previous studies have focused on Series 1/2 because this series have
plenty data for comparing. For example, the vocabularies in Table 46, show a set of
Series 1/2 of voicing alternation among Thai, Lungchow and Yay representing SWT,
CT and NT respectively. Pittayawat Pittayaporn speculates a “voiced obstruent +
voiceless stop” *C̬.C̥- for this series. Although he thinks that the initial voiced
consonants of *C̬.C̥- were unclear and might be different due to different etyma, he
The fourth gloss ‘to flood’ in Table 46 may be of data errors. This term is actually reflected as
tʰuəmC1 in Thai and tʰu:mC1 in Longchow (cf. Zhang et al. 1999: 700) instead of the two forms with
tone B1 in the chart. Its Thai orthographical form ทวม reflecting the B2 tone which is still different
from the B1 in the chart, and this may be an incorrect normalization in Thai due to the merger of B2
and C1 in modern Thai. The forms in other CT/SWT dialects like tʰuəmC1 in Chiang Rai and tʰo:mC1 in
Tai Lue (SWT), as well as tʰu:mC1 in Debao, Jingxi, and Daxin (CT), and tʰumC1 in Guangnan and
Yangshan (CT) all reflect *tʰuəmC for this term. In contrast, many NT forms all reflect a *dumB tone
for this term, like tumB2 in Wuming, Pingguo, Tianlin, and Lingle, as well as təmB2 in Qiubei (cf.
Zhang et al. 1999: 700). Although *tʰuəmC reflected in CT/SWT and *dumB reflected in NT have
similarities on the phonetic shape, their different tonal categories give a doubt to their cognate relation.
It is crucial that in Debao Yang Zhuang (CT), there are two terms tʰu:mC1 (reflecting *tʰuəmC) and
tʊmB2 (reflecting *dumB) for ‘to flood’, and the later one is normally used as idiomaticity like namC2
ɹʊmB2 tʊmB2 mɤ:ŋA2 ‘the Great Flood submerging the world’. If the two terms in this dialect are
actually from two different etyma, the formulation using them to give an example for voicing
alternation will be lost efficacy.
39
145
interprets that in NT the non-initial segment (voiceless *.C̥-) was assimilated to the
initial segment (voiced *C̬.-) and become voiced to condition tones into the low
register, on the other hand in CT/SWT the loss of the initial segment (voiced *C̬.-)
caused its following voiceless *.C̥- become aspirated to condition tones into the high
register (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 167-169).
As ‘to arrive’, one of the examples in the chart above, presents a diachronic
developing process as *C̬.tɤŋA > *dɤŋA > taŋA2 in Yay (NT), and *C̬.tɤŋA > *tʰɤŋA >
tʰɯŋA1 in Thai (SWT). Pittayawat Pittayaporn furthermore explains that his
reconstruction of this onset is based on that the etymon *C̬.tɤŋA may be related to
Proto-Austronesian *dateŋ ‘to arrive’ (cf. Thurgood 2007: 255) “which shows the
expected sequence” (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 168). Moreover, he gives some
examples with other *C̬ .C̥- onsets reconstructed like *C̬ .k- and *C̬ .q- (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 168-169) to indicate that they likewise cause Series 1/2 of
voicing alternation.
On the other hand, Weera Ostapirat gives another explanation on the origin of
voicing alternation. Although his reconstructions are for interpreting the situation of
voicing alternation in KS languages, we still treat them as efficient for those in Tai
because the situations in KS and in Tai are structurally similar, and some of them are
even the same since KS and Tai are closely related branches in the TK family. He
suggests that in some disyllabic onsets, the series with accented initial (like *k'-m-)
and another series with unaccented initial segment (like *k-m-) have given different
register splits in KS languages (Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1082). In the following Table
47, he gives some examples with the proposed accented initial disyllabic onsets to
interpret the voicing alternation in KS languages.
Table 47 Etyma with PKS *h'-w- (Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1085)40
Gloss
Then
Mak
Sui
Mulam
Kam
Tones
‘right (side)’
waa2
phaa1ʰ
faa1
faa1
waa1ʰ
*A
‘late’
wee2
--
fee1
fɛɛ1
wee1ʰ
*A
‘garden’
wjaan2
fiin1ʰ
fjaan1
fjen1
jaan1ʰ
*A
In his purpose, both accented and unaccented initials can give the following medial
an aspirated or a devoicing effect in some daughter languages, but just simply loses
40
The -h on the top right corner of the tonal maker 1 in this chart refers to the derived tone
conditioned by aspirated sound (including fricatives and voiceless sonorants) in Kam and Mak of the
KS languages, following the tonal labeling system of Weera Ostapirat (2006: 1078).
146
in some daughter languages. Therefore in the chart above, PKS *h'-w- has become
initial w- in Then with tonal Series 2 according to the Then voiced initial, has
become f- in Sui and Mulam with tonal Series 1 according to their voiceless initials,
has become pʰ- (and f- when following by -j-) in Mak with tonal series 1ʰ according
to Mak voiceless aspirated initials, and has become w- (and j- when following by -j-)
in Kam with tonal series 1ʰ indicating early Kam hw- due to the Kam tone
conditioned by voiceless aspirated initials. He furthermore points out that the terms
in this chart are related to some terms with proto-initial *s- in Tai, like Thai sa:jA1
‘late’ and suənA1 ‘garden’; and some of the Tai varieties have labio-velar features for
this set, namely Tho (CT) swa:jA1 ‘late’ and swa:A1 ‘right (side)’. He even suggests that
this extra-KS evidence indicates a source *s- to the accented *h'- as in PKS *h'-w- <
*s-w- (Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1085).
Gedney (1989a: 245) suggests the initial of the term *sa:A1 ‘right (side)’ reflected in
the majority of CT (agreeing with the Lungchow form in Table 48) to be
contaminated by the initial of the counterpart term *za:jC ‘left (side)’, e.g. Lungchow
ɬa:A1 ‘right (side)’ vs. ɬa:jC2 ‘left (side)’. Pittayawat Pittayaporn agrees on this point
and on that this term has a “clear Chinese origin” (右 jəuB) by citing from Thurgood
(2007) (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 82-83). For the other term ‘late (in the
morning)’, Pittayawat Pittayaporn agrees on Thurgood (2007: 246-248) that among
Thai, Lungchow and Po-ai it “shows irregularities in vowel length, and tone, as well
as onsets” and consider that the NT term which suggests earlier *gw- is not related to
the SWT/CT term (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 81), although in fact the vowel
length and the tone (reflected) do not show irregularities at all. If we compare more
data of other varieties from SWT, CT, and NT (like shown in Table 48) and the data
from extra-Tai evidence (in Table 47), the viewpoints provided by Gedney,
Pittayawat Pittayaporn as well as Thurgood above cannot be true.
By comparing the terms ‘right (side)’ and ‘late’ with labio-velar features in other Tai
varieties like Chiang Rai (Northern Thai), Rong Maet (Tai Lue), Debao Yang Zhuang
varieties (L8, L9, and L10) and Debao Ronghua (L14) as well as their cognates
presenting voicing alternation in NT, we will partly agree to Weera Ostapirat’s
explanation involving voicing alternation on this set, as shown in Table 48.
147
Table 48 Etyma with the proposed PT *ɬ.w- which causing voicing alternation
in Tai41
SWT
Gloss
Thai
(L1)
‘right
(side)’
‘late’
Chiang
Rai
(L4)
CT
Tai Lue
Lung-
(L6)
chow
Tho
NT
Ronghua
Debao
(L14)
(L8)
Po-ai
Wuming
(L30)
kʰwa:A1
kʰwa:A1
kʰwa:A1
ɬa:A1
swa:A1
θwa:A1
ɬɔ:A1
kwa:A2
kwa:A2
sa:jA1
kʰwa:jA1
kʰwa:jA1
ɬa:jA1
swa:jA1
θwa:jA1
ɬɔ:jA1
kwa:jA2
kwa:jA2
In Table 48, the additional SWT/CT Tai forms with labio-velar features (which are
all in shaded) are added to indicate that both of these two terms share the same
sesquisyllabic proto-onset which caused voicing alternation (of Series 1/2) between
NT and ST (CT/SWT), especially when being compared with their cognates with
labio-velar features in KS languages (shown in Table 47). All the forms reflect a PT
tone *A to indicate that the term ‘right (side)’ has nothing to do with the claimed
Chinese origin with a qù (departing) tone which corresponds to PT *B. The reason
that I reconstruct the initial *ɬ.w- for this set is as follows. (1) The method of
sesquisyllabic initial segment which cause voicing alternation in daughter languages
agrees on Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 167-169). Because Weera Ostapirat’s
method that the accented and unaccented initials affecting on the following medials
lacks of a consistent tonal behavior in a specific modern language, e.g. his PKS *h-p-,
*k'-p-, *h'-t-, *k'-t-, *p'-t-, *p'-k- and *t'-k- condition tonal series (high-1 and low-2) 2,
1, 1, 1, 1, 2 and 2 in Then respectively (cf. Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1086, 1088, 1091,
1103, 1104), we cannot judge whether the proposed accented or unaccented initial
could give the effect on the following medial consonant in Tai languages. Besides,
the situations of voicing alternation among Tai languages are quite straightforward
when being compared with those in KS languages, as both Series 1/2 and Series 2/1
occur between NT and CT/SWT (or Gedney’s ST). Therefore, Pittayawat
Pittayaporn’s structure like C̬ .C̥- can be more advisable for the situation of Tai. (2)
PT *s- commonly reconstructed (cf. Li 1977: 152, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 115)
always exhibits the same tonal behavior as voiceless sonorants (*hm-, *hn-, *hɲ-,
*hŋ-, and *hl-) and voiceless approximants (*hr- and *hw-) to form a unified group
proto-voiceless continuants but appears to be quite out of place in this series
phonetically. Since in many CT and NT varieties, such as Lungchow and Po-ai, the
41
Data in this chart are collected by me except those of Lungchow and Po-ai cited from Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009: 80, 82) and Tho cited from Weera Ostapirat (2006: 1085).
148
speculated *s- presents as a voiceless lateral fricative ɬ-, which contains a feature of
lateral shared with the sonorant l-, Liao and Shen (2012) has suggested the protoform of it may be *ɬ- instead of *s-. This accounts for its unusual phonetic
characteristics when compared with the rest of the voiceless sonorants and
approximants that it patterns with and provides a reasonable explanation for why
these sounds pattern together as they do. (3) The medial *.w- is reconstructed due to
the labio-velar features reflected in some Tai varieties and their related KS
languages. The following initial and tonal development speculated shown in Table
49 give the assurance for this reconstruction.
Table 49 Developing tracks of PT *ɬ.w- in modern Tai varieties42
PT
Developing process
Modern forms
*k.w- (POA
*gw- (voicing
assimilation)
assimilation)
most CT
*ɬw-
*ɬ-
Tho (CT)
*ɬw-
sw-
Ronghua (CT)
*ɬw-
θw-
NT
(Series 2)
ɬV[+rounded]
*ɬw-
*ɬwThai (SWT)
*k.w- (POA
most SWT
low
ɬ-/θ-/s-
*ɬ.wDebao (CT)
kw-
Tone
assimilation)
*ɬ
(MOA
high
assimilation)
(Series 1)
s-
*kʰw- (aspirated
due to the syllabic
kʰw-
structure change)
The situation of those CT varieties preserving labio-velar features indicates that after
voicing alternation process, the loss of the labio-velar segment in -w- of this set in
CT was quite late. The most crucial evidence is that in different Yang Zhuang
varieties which are of the same distinct language with the same ISO 639-3 code
(zyg), most of Debao varieties have ɬɔ:A1 and ɬɔ:j1, but all Jingxi varieties and some
Debao varieties have ɬa:A1 and ɬa:jA1 for these two term respectively. In contrast, the
situation in Thai present two different tracks with the two terms, and this perhaps
suggests the individual development of these two terms were quite early in this
dialect.
42
The terms POA and MOA in Table 49 are the abbreviations of the phonetic terms “place of
articulation” and “manner of articulation” respectively.
149
However, Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s C̬ .C̥ - which is suggested to cause voicing
alternation still cannot cover this set since the medial *.w- has to be a voiced
segment against to his suggesting *.C̥ -. Another possible reconstruction meeting his
criterion is *ɮ.p- which can be supposed to have different development, namely *ɮ.p> *ɮ.b- > *ɮ.w- > *g.w- > *gw- > kw- (with low register tone) in NT, *ɮ.p- >
*ɮ.b- > *ɮ.w- > *ɬ.w- > *ɬw- > ɬ- (with low register tone) in most CT, and *ɮ.p- >
*ɮ.b- > *ɮ.w- > *g.w- > kʰw- (with low register tone) in most SWT. But this
proposal contains more complicated sound changes and is left to further discuss in
the future since the reconstruction of initial forms is not the main target of this
thesis. Therefore, here we only give a concise and comprehensive proposal on the
reconstruction of the onsets causing Series 2/1 of voicing alternation (shown in
Table 44). Since all items in this series have an initial stop (including the nasal stop
m- and the affricate tɕ-/ tɕʰ- which phonologically belong to stops, as well as their
variations ɕ-/s-/θ-/ɬ-), I temporarily suppose their initials are of a sesquisyllabic
cluster *C̥.C̬- (voiceless stop + voiced obstruent), to be relevant to the situation (low
tone in CT/SWT and high tone in NT) which is opposite to Series 1/2. Hereafter we
will leave the arguments of the voicing alternation induced by onsets reconstructed,
but only focus on the result of alternation of tonal series, to name these two groups
of onsets “Series 1/2 of voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets” (VASO-
1/2) and “Series 2/1 of voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets” (VASO2/1) respectively.
In conclusion, after the primary tonal split, VASO-1/2 onsets became voiced stops to
condition the tone into the low register in NT, and became voiced aspiration or
breathy sound due to the loss of the previous initial segment (Weera Ostapirat’s
accented/unaccented segment and Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s voiced segment) and
then merge with the secondary voiceless aspiration to condition the tone into the
high register in SWT/CT. On the other hand, VASO-2/1 onset became voiced stops
to condition the tone into the low register in CT/SWT, and became voiceless
obstruents due to assimilation of the initial *C̥.- segment to condition the tone into
the high register in NT. Tonal behaviors involving voicing alternation between NT
and CT/SWT suggests that Li’s CT and SWT were of the same group with PNT as a
sister when splitting from PT, and this group is suggested to be ST by Gedney
(1989a: 231). This will be further discussed in Chapter 5.
150
Tonal splits secondarily conditioned by glottalized sounds has been pointed out to
be exist in Li’s all NT, CT, and SWT groups (cf. §4.3.1 and §4.3.2). In NT languages,
these splits are concentrated in the *C and/or *B tone, but are very rarely found in
the *A tone. In contrast, in CT/SWT languages the splits are normally found in the
*A and *B tones, but are rare in the *C tone. These phenomena may be related to
that the final glottal constriction on the C tones are very robust in CT/SWT, but are
reduced in NT (cf. §4.1.3.3.1). Tones secondarily conditioned by glottalized sounds
are normally depressed from the original high tone due to that glottal sounds are
known to have a pitch-lowering effect on the vowel (Fu 1995: 64). Actually all tones
of secondary splits are lower than the original one (Liang & Zhang 1996: 818),
except in those Tai varieties that have developed tonal flip-flop (cf. §4.1.3.1).
There is no lack of exceptions. Po-ai provides a single NT example that has a
secondary split in the *A tone conditioned by glottalized sounds in the previous
studies. The six tones A1, A2, B1, B2, C1, and C2 in smooth syllables in Po-ai are 24,
55, 22, 31, 44, and 33 respectively (Li 1977: 28). Both *B and *C present the
straightforward pattern splitting between voiceless-voiced registers. However, *A
has a three-way split pattern – initials developing from PT voiceless unaspirated
sounds and voiceless continuants condition the 24 tone, initials from PT glottalized
sounds condition the 31 tone (which is merged into B2), and initials from PT voiced
sounds condition the 55 tone (cf. Li 1977: 29). Isan/Lao of SWT also merge their C1G tone together with C1-U into C2 as shown in the tone box of Khon Kaen (L3) in
Appendix C, to indicate that secondary tonal splits conditioned by glottalized sounds
also can occur in the *C tone in SWT, although such phenomenon is rarely found. At
this point, secondary tonal splits conditioned by glottalized sounds are mostly
common found in all Tai languages. Thus, glottalized sounds may earliest cause
secondary tonal splits before aspirated stops and unaspirated stops.
Data from my fieldwork show that glottalized initials have undergone kinds of sound
changes in many modern Tai varieties except the glottal stop ʔ-. Pre-glottalized stops
ʔ
b- and ʔd- weakened to merge into plain voiced stops b- and d- are common in SWT.
In NT and CT varieties as well as Shan in SWT, pre-glottalized stops ʔb- and ʔd- are
merged into nasals m- and n-/l- (like in Shuangding YN, Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang
and Shan). In addition, the pre-glottalized glide ʔj- has been merged into j- in most
SWT and CT varieties, but is preserved in many NT varieties and Debao Yang
Zhuang varieties of CT. In Quan Son Tai (L7 in Appendixes C) of SWT, all preglottalized initial consonants have merged into their homorganic sonorants or
151
fricatives, and the processes are as *ʔb- > ʋ-, *ʔd- > l-, and *ʔj- > ð-, like in ʋa:nC1-G
‘village’, la:wA1-G ‘star’, and ða:A1-G ‘medicine’.
However, in most situation these initial changes are later development than the time
of tonal splits conditioned by their predecessors glottalized sounds. Po-ai of NT has
also merged their glottalized stops into nasals, like min31 ‘to fly’, ni:31 ‘good’, and
nɯ:n31 ‘month’, but their tone (A1-G) is still preserved to be the same as in ʔaw31 ‘to
take’, to be different from the tones of paj24 (A1-U) ‘to go’, ka:j24 (A1-A) ‘to sell’ and
na:55 (A2) ‘rice field’ (cf. Li 1977: 29). The phenomenon indicates that the glottalized
stops in Po-ai conditioned the secondary tonal split in Tone *A before they are
merged into nasals. The following Table 50 shows the similar situations.
Table 50 Reflexes of *ʔd-, *ʔb-, and *ʔj- and their tones43
PT initials
*hm-
*k-
*ʔd-
*ʔb-
*ʔj-
Gloss
‘dog’
‘crow’
‘star’
‘to fly’
‘medicine’
Tonal categories
A1-C
A1-U
ma:1
ka:1
la:w1
ʋin1
ða:1
na:2
xwa:j2
ma:1
ka:1
na:w1
man1
ja:1
na:2
ʋa:j2
ma:1
ka:1
na:w2
min2
ja:2
na:2
ʋa:j2
ma:1
ka:1
ˀda:w2
ˀban2
ja:1
na:2
ʋa:j2
Quan Son Tai
(L7)
Jingxi Urban
(L12)
Tiandeng Urban
Jingxi Hurun
(L11)
*n-
*ɣw-
‘rice
‘buffalo’
field’
A1-G
A2
In the chart above, the first two varieties have straightforward tonal splits patterns
in the tone *A, thus their tones conditioned by the original glottalized initials are
preserved as T1, even though the glottalized sounds have been merged into voiced
continuants in these two varieties. Tiandeng has a secondary tonal split conditioned
by the original glottalized initials, therefore the tones conditoned by these sounds
had split from T1 to merge into T2, before the glottalized sounds were merged into
voiced continuants.
Hurun presents an interesting counter-example. In this variety, pre-glottalized stops
are still preserved and condition the secondary tonal split to merge into T2, like in
the terms ‘star’ and ‘to fly’. However, pre-glottalized glide ʔj- has been merged into
43
For indicating the tonal splits and mergers, the tone markers in the Sinitic circles is followed to
use in this chart. Secondary tonal splits conditioned by original glottalized sounds are in shaded. Data
from Tiandeng Urban which is not in the list of the forty-two languages is collected by me.
152
j-, and this time the tone conditioned by this j- (< *ʔj-) is preserved the original T1
of the primary tonal split. This example accounts for the following speculations for
this dialect. (1) Before conditioning the secondary tonal split, all the glottalized
sounds did have the same tonal behaviors with the other voiceless groups to
condition tones into Series 1 (the high register). This can be an additional evidence
for indicating that tonal splits conditioned by glottal sounds are secondary. (2) Later,
pre-glottalized glide *ʔj- merged into voiced glide j- but kept its original tonal
behavior (Series 1). (3) After that, the preserved pre-glottalized stops conditioned
the secondary tonal split in the tone *A (1 > 2). The initial j- (< *ʔj-) does not play
a part in this process since its glottal feature has been lost.
In short, glottalized sounds are given their tonal behavior when they have the
glottalized feature. This tonal behavior can be secondarily changed (normally
depressed or pitch-lowering) due to their glottal feature. The tonal behavior
conditioned by original glottalized sounds, both primary or secondary, is preserved
even though the glottalized sounds are merged into other sounds in some modern
Tai varieties.
To make a passing remark, secondary tonal splits conditioned by voiceless
unaspirated stops all follow the patterns of those conditioned by glottalized sounds
due to their phonetic similarities, and are only found in some SWT and CT varieties
(cf. 4.3.1). They can be treated as the byproduct of secondary tonal splits
conditioned by glottalized sounds.
As in the previous discussions, aspirated sounds in CT/SWT and YN varieties (of
those whose classification of CT or NT is open to debate) are secondary in post-PT
level (cf. §4.2.1). So far, NT varieties still do not develop contrasting aspirated
sounds. Therefore, tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds on the original high
register have to be secondary. As what has been emphasized in the previous
discussions (cf. §4.3.1 and §4.3.2), although aspirated sounds are robust in SWT, CT
and YN, they are not found to condition secondary tonal splits in SWT, but only
found in CT and YN. This phenomenon suggests that on a certain level CT languages
have their own phonological development to separate them from SWT varieties (cf.
L-Thongkam 1997: 215). Note that YN varieties such as Yongning Baiji (L25),
Naning Shuangding (L26), Long’an (27), Nung An (28), and Yongning Xialeng (29)
153
investigated in this thesis are conventionally treated as dialects of SZ which is
traditionally grouped into CT, because these varieties also have plenty aspirated
initials which condition possible secondary tonal splits as well. But at the same time
they present more NT features on voicing alternation, vocalic behaviors, and some
of the initial behaviors. The secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds
in many of these dialects make their classification as CT or NT more complicated to
debate (cf. Chapter 5).
Secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds (on the high register) are
summarized as in Type 2 (aspiration condition splits alone) and Type 3 (aspiration
condition splits together with glottal sounds) by Zhang and colleagues (1999), and
we have also illustrated some of the mixed Type 2-3 and Type 1-3, such as Bac Va
Nung, Lingding Zuojiang Zhuang, and Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (cf. §4.3.1).
Within the forty-two Tai varieties investigated in this thesis, nine from CT and those
of YN confirm tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds. In other words, these
nine varieties have a split between voiceless aspirated sounds and voiceless
continuant, which are both included in Gedney’s voiceless frictions. The significant
inspiration of tonal splits due to aspirated sounds is to add an extra row (1-A) into
Gedney’s Tai tone box (cf. Liao & Shen 2012, §1.2.2, and Chapter 6) at least when
applying it to CT and YN.
However, it is necessary to distinguish different origins of aspirated initials in PT
since different groups of these aspiration induced by onsets do not always present as
aspirated in different groups of modern Tai languages. For example, VASO-1/2
normally present as aspirated stops (including h- and ɕ- in some specific Tai varieties
like Yang Zhuang) in both CT and SWT varieties, but never present as aspirated
initials in any varieties of YN even though YN is treated as a SZ dialect in the Sinitic
circles because they all have robust aspirations. Thus VASO-1/2 must be separated
from the 1-A group to be an extra row in the ideal tone box, otherwise the tone box
would not capture this tonal distinction between CT/SWT and YN/NT. The following
discussions in this sub-section will focus on different groups of aspirated initials
beyond VASO-1/2.
The most significant group of aspirated sounds is the 1-A group in Liao & Shen’s Tai
tone box (cf. Table 7 in §1.2.2), which is considered to commonly present as
aspiration in all CT/SWT and YN varieties. This group is treated as monosyllabic
onsets which includes those considered to be “plain stop + *-r-” clusters, *q-, *x-,
*χ-, *h-,*ʰw-, and *ʰr- in PT by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) as in Table 51.
154
*pʰr-
*tʰr-
*tʰl-
ST
(2009)
Pittayaporn
Pittayawat
Li (1977)
Table 51 PT monosyllabic onsets induced by aspiration in ST and YN
Gloss
‘vegetable’
*pr-
*tr-
*kʰr*kʰ-/
*x-
*kr-
*qr-
hawA1
‘tail’
‘plough’
‘to wait’
‘six’
*χ-
*f-
*ʰŋ-/
ʰr-
44
*h-
tʰawA1
tʰa:ŋA1
hinA1
tʰənA1
tʰajA1
‘white’
‘to cross’
‘fang’
pʰja:kDL1
pʰjamA1
tʰawA1
tʰɯ:ŋA1
Po-ai
Zhongyi
pʰlakDS1
Yay
pjakDS1
piəkDS1
pja:kDL1
pja:kDL1
pʰlamA1
pjɔmA1
piəmA1
tʰawA1
lawA1
rawA1
lɯ:ŋA1
rɯəŋA1
pʰla:kDL1
tʰu:ŋA1
hinA1
rinA1
tɕɔ:jA1
tɕajA1
tɕajA1
sajA1
sajA1
tʰa:C1
tʰa:C1
tɕa:C1
-
sa:C1
sa:C1
ha:A1
kʰja:A1
ʰɹa:A1
ʰla:A1
la:A1
ra:A1
lɔkDS1
rokDS1
ʰli:wA1
li:wA1
riəwA1
ʰlajB1
tɕajB1
tɕajB1
hokDS1
kʰjɔ:kDS1
kʰjajB1
‘torn’
Nalong-
Nung An
pʰjakDS1
NT
tʰənA1
kʰajB1
‘to sift’
YN
-
‘egg’
kʰɯŋA1
kʰa:C1
kʰa:tDL1
kʰɛ:nA1
kʰiəwA1
kʰa:wA1
kʰa:B1
kʰa:mC1
kʰiəwC1
kʰjaŋA1
ʰɹɔkDS1
ʰɹi:wA1
ʰɹajB1
ʰlɔkDS1
-
ʰlaŋA1
tɕaŋA1
-
ka:C1
ka:C1
ka:C1
ka:C1
ka:tDL1
ka:tDL1
ka:tDL1
ka:tDL1
kʰe:nA1
ke:nA1
ke:nA1
tɕe:nA1
tɕe:nA1
kʰi:wA1
he:wA1
je:wA1
he:wA1
hewA1
ha:wA1
ha:wA1
ha:wA1
ha:wA1
kʰa:B1
-
-
-
ha:B1
kʰa:mC1
ha:mC1
ha:mC1
ha:mC1
ha:mC1
he:wC1
he:wC1
he:wC1
hewC1
kʰa:C1
kʰa:tDL1
kʰa:wA1
kʰe:wC1
kʰiŋA1
kʰəŋA1
həŋA1
həŋA1
hiŋA1
hiŋA1
ha:C1
ha:C1
ha:C1
ha:C1
ha:C1
ha:C1
hajC1
hɔ:jC1
hajC1
hajC1
haɰC1
haɰC1
‘fragrant’
hɔ:mA1
ho:mA1
ho:mA1
ho:mA1
ho:mA1
homA
‘lid’
fa:A1
pʰa:A1
pʰa:A1
pʰa:A1
fa:A1
va:A1
fɯ:C1
-
A1
-
‘ginger’
‘to give’
*ʰw- ‘cloud’
*ʰr-
pʰjamA1
kʰu:A1
‘five’
*h
pʰja:kDL1
huəA1
‘galangal’
*x-
ha:ŋA1
pʰjakDS1
‘to laugh’
‘arm’
*x-
pʰakDS1
‘headlouse’
‘green’
*x-
Debao
pʰomA1
‘to kill’
*q-
Thai
‘head hair’
‘to seek’
*xr-
CT
‘forehead’44 pʰa:kDL1
‘stone’
*cr-
SWT
fa:C1
pʰa:C1
fa:j
‘to yawn’
ha:wA1
ha:wA1
-
ha:wA1
hawB1
hawB1
ʰɹ awB1
ʰlawB1
hiəwB1
he:wB1
‘to bark’
‘withered’
A1
pʰa:j
pʰy:C1
‘dam’
A1
pʰa:j
pʰɯ:C1
A1
ʰɹ e:wB1
The full form of the word ‘forehead’ in Thai is na:C1 pʰa:kDL1.
155
pʰa:j
A1
ʰle:wB1
fa:j
ŋa:wA1
ra:wA1
lawB1
rawB1
-
re:wB1
Li’s reconstructions in Table 5145 are rejected by the viewpoint that PT lacked of
aspirated sounds. In contrast, Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 93) supposes that some
etyma with aspirated onsets in CT and SWT must have had an earlier media *-rwhich caused PT voiceless plain stops to become aspirated in the first five sets in
Table 51. In his proposal, Debao has implemented the effect of the medial *-r- to
cause all the preceding stops to become aspirated, but the other three dialects do not
always carry out the aspirated process. On the sets *pr- and *cr- the medial *-r- had
an effect on the preceding stop to cause aspiration in Thai, but had just been simply
lost in Po-ai. On the sets *tr- and *kr-, the following processes may have occurred
respectively: *tr- > *tʰr- > *ʰr- > h- and *kr- > *kʰr- > *ʰr- > h- in Thai, *tr- >
*tʰr- > *ʰr- > r- as well as *tr- > *ʰr- > *r- > l- and *kr- > *ʰr- *r- > l- in Po-ai
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 94-95). We can also speculate *kr- > *kʰr- > ʰr- in
Nung An, and furthermore change into ʰl- in Nalong Zhongyi. An example illustrated
in §4.2.1 has directly supported Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s proposal. Shuangding
(L26) of YN partly preserves the cluster pr- which have become aspirated pʰj- or pʰin most CT/SWT varieties, but the neighbouring Nalong Zhongyi variety (from my
fieldwork) has changed the preceding plain stop into aspirated stops and has
changed the medial -r- to -l- to make the initial become pʰl- at the same time, such as
pra:33 (A1-A) contratsing to pʰla:453 ‘stone mountain/hill’ in Shuangding and Nalong
Zhongyi respectively. For this reason, this thesis also supports the viewpoint that the
earliest origin of aspiration in CT and SWT should be “voiceless unaspirated stops +
-r-” clusters, namely *pr-, *tr-, *cr-, and *kr- in Table 51.
Only if aspiration arose due to “voiceless unaspirated stops + -r-”, can other series
of initial such as rich dorsal series (as illustrated in the latter six sets in Table 51)
result the same phonation type in CT/SWT and YN. Note that h- in Debao as well as
h-, ʰr- or ʰl- in Nung An and Nalong Zhongyi are grouped into aspirated sounds (1-A)
because they always condition tones together with aspirated sounds due to the
[+spread] feature shared among them. Sets *cr- and *q- are noteworthy on the
45
In Table 51, data of Thai, Debao (the former four sets), and Yay are mainly adopted from
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 94, 104-105, 117-119, 133-135, 144-145). Po-ai Data are adopted from
Li (1977: 148, 208-209, 229, 250). Data of Debao (the latter seven sets), Nung An and Nalong
Zhongyi are collected by me. Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s original data of the first four sets are not
provided the tonal series (1 or 2) for those on smooth syllables, but all terms are actually with Series 1
since these sets are from PT voiceless initials. Set 1 to 4 of Debao in the chart should be of Yang
Zhuang due to the data provided, but some terms originally provided by Pittayaporn are also
problematic, like (1) tʰajA1 ‘plough’ is actually not found in any Yang Zhuang variety in Debao but is
found in Jingxi Yang Zhuang varieties, instead t :ɔA1 is found in Debao; (2) the vowel of tʰ nA1
‘stone’ is -ə in all other transcriptions (cf. Li 1970, Zhang et al. 1999, Liao 2010, Jackson et al. 2011);
and (3) the vowel of ɕʰɔoɕDS1 ‘six’ should be a long - : (this long vowel rather reflects a short *-o in
PT, vs. Debao -o: reflecting the long *-o: in PT) due to the Yang Zhuang sound system (cf. Zhang et
al. 1999, Liao 2010).
156
irregularity of induced by aspirated sounds. First, *cr- did not always merge into *tʰin Debao of CT, but changed to *c- and then tɕ- in the term ‘plough’; this PT initial
also merged into *c- and then tɕ- in Nung An and Shuangding, which are of YN.
Second, PT *q- has been merged into the aspirated kʰ- in SWT/CT (= Gedney’s ST),
but into the unaspirated k- in YN varieties. Thus these two initials have been
separated from the common “Aspirated induced by Monosyllabic Onsets in ST and
YN” (AMO-SY) to be a single sub-group named “Aspirated induced by Monosyllabic
Onsets in ST” (AMO-ST) in this thesis. In other words, Liao & Shen’s (2012) 1-A row
will be divided into at least two sub-rows AMO-SY and AMO-ST to designate when
dealing with the ideal Tai tone box (cf. Chapter 6).
There is another significant peculiarity on aspiration in Tai. This is a series of words
that have aspirated initial consonants in CT and YN, but have unaspirated stops in
SWT and NT. Gedney treats these certain words to be “conspicuous for tending to
retain bizarre reflexes of original consonant clusters” that distinguish Li’s CT from
SWT (Gedney 1989b: 64). These certain words are reconstructed to have a series of
“voiceless unaspirated stops + *r-” clusters, namely *pr-, *tr-, and *kr- by Li (1977:
86, 118, 225). As we have discussed in §1.2.2 (cf. Table 11), this series of initial has
been noticed by Johnson (2011: 30, 36-38) and has proposed a split in Row 2 of
Gedney’s tone box. For capture the tonal split conditioned by this series of aspirated
sounds in CT, Liao and Shen (2012) adopt this proposal and add this series as the
newly added row 1-UC (cf. Table 13 and Table 7).
However, there are different hypotheses on the reconstructions of this series of PT
initials that develop to unaspirated stops in SWT and NT but aspirated stops in CT
and YN. First, the split is not easily perceived due to the relative rarity of examples
of this split’s conditioning factors. Johnson (2011: 30-31) lists only two examples
(‘to die’ and ‘eye’) with the PT *tr- speculated by Li (1977: 118) to demonstrate this
split in Dai Zhuang. Liao and Shen (2012) list two more examples (‘to dry’ and
‘break’) with the PT *pr- speculated by Li (1977: 86) to confirm their new row 1-UC.
Li lists six terms under the other one *kr- in his PT reconstruction (Li 1977: 225). In
addition, the wordlist designated for collecting the examples from Tai varieties
investigated in this thesis contains five, two, two, three, and two examples in A1-UC,
B1-UC, C1-UC, DL-UC, and DS-UC respectively (cf. Appendix C).
Nevertheless, at least the term ‘cage’ reconstructed as having Li’s *kr- (cf. Li 1977:
225) and adopted into Row B1-UC of the wordlist (cf. Appendix B) in this thesis
should be a wrong design after comparing more data collected. Firstly, the Thai term
157
krɔ:ŋB1 46 listed in this set reflects a long vowel *-o: in PT, but forms in other dialects
like huŋB1 in Lungchow (CT), lɔŋB1 in Po-ai (NT), kʰjɔ:ŋB1 in all Yang Zhuang varieties
(CT), hʊŋB1 in Daxin Baoxu (CT), ɹɔŋB1 in Longsang (NT), hjɔŋB1 in Shanglin (NT) and
the cognate forms in all other CT and NT varieties all regularly reflects Li’s another
initial *xr- (cf. Li 1977: 233)47 and a short vowel *-o in PT. Besides, the Thai term
klɔ:ŋB1 actually does not mean ‘cage’ but ‘box’. In addition, in other SWT varieties
other forms with the meaning ‘cage’ and with regular sound correspondence to Li’s
*xroŋB (or Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s *kroŋB) reflected in CT and NT varieties are not
found. Therefore I suggests that the Thai term klɔ:ŋB1 ‘box’ may not be cognate with
this etymon. Furthermore, in Li’s the other five terms ‘sieve’, ‘near’, ‘husk of rice’, ‘to
polish, sooth’, and ‘to snore’ with his *kr- (cf. Li 1977: 225), I found that only ‘near’
actually reflects his reconstruction with an unaspirated initial stop in both SWT and
NT but an aspirated kʰj- or its variation ɕ- (<tɕʰ- < kʰj- < *kr- (Li 1977: 225) or
*k.r- (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 169) ) in CT varieties. The other four may
confirm to his other reconstructions *xr- and *kl- due to sound correspondence
among modern Tai varieties. Other terms that may be related to his *kr- may be ‘to
put up (like an umbrella)’ and ‘CLF of a stone’ which have been listed in the wordlist
designated (cf. Appendix B), as shown in Table 52.
Table 52 Reflexes of Li’s*kr- in some modern Tai varieties48
Thai (L1-
Debao Urban
Xiangdu
Shuangding (L26-
Du’an (L35-
SWT)
(L8-CT)
(L17-CT)
YN-CT/NT?)
NT)
‘near’
klajC1
kʰjɔ:jC1
ɕaɯC1
kʰjoɥC1 (Chongzuo)
kjaɯC1
‘to put up’
ka:ŋA1
kʰa:ŋA1
kʰa:ŋA1
kʰa:ŋA1
ka:ŋA1
kɔ:nC1
kʰo:nC1
kʰo:nC1
kʰo:nC1
ko:nC1
Gloss
‘CLF of a
stone’
In the chart above, the two terms ‘to put up (like an umbrella)’ and ‘CLF of a stone’
are still different from ‘near’ on that lacking of the glide (-l- or -j-) which is found in
‘near’ in modern forms. However, it is still possible that they share the same PT
initial and each developed into different paths due to their following vowels,
otherwise this PT initial is rather tentative because it is attested only by one
46
The Thai form is cited from Li (1977: 225), but there is not such a form with this meaning in
modern Thai. Li’s Thai form should be an accidental form of kl :ŋB1 ‘box’ in modern Thai.
47
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 142-143) reconstructs *kr- for Li’s *xr-, and k.r- (2009: 169, 345)
for Li’s *kr- (cf. the term ‘near’) respectively.
48
In Table 52, Shuangding lacks of the Tai cognate ‘near’, instead the Chinese loan kanB2 (< MC
*gjənC) is used. For replacing the borrowed term, the Tai term from Chongzuo which is also of
Yongnan Zhuang and is closely related to Shuangding is cited from Zhang et al. (1999: 777).
158
example ‘near’. Therefore, I presume that these three items share the same PT initial,
which is reconstructed as *kr- by Li (1977: 225) to be a partner of his *pr- and *tr-,
which likewise induce voiceless aspirated initials in CT and YN on one hand and
voiceless unaspirated initials in SWT and NT on the other hand.
As what Li’s reconstruction of this series of PT initial (*pr-, *tr-, and *kr-) presents a
consistent structure, namely “voiceless unaspirated stops + *-r-” clusters. This may
also consistently explain the regular sound changes in different Tai daughter
languages. However, we have discussed that PT lacked of contrasting aspirated
initials, and aspirated stops (on the high register) arising in CT and SWT have
several sources, such as VASO-1/2 (cf. §4.3.2.1) and PT clusters containing plain
stops plus a medial -*r-, namely *pr-, *tr-, *kr-, and *cr- (Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 93-94) to be rejective to Li’s reconstruction of this series. Note that Pittayawat
Pittayaporn’s proposal that aspiration in CT and SWT developed mainly from *pr-,
*tr-, *kr-, and *cr- has been shown in Table 51 and has been agreed with in this
thesis. Therefore, Li’s reconstructions *pr-, *tr-, and *kr- as another series of modern
aspiration only found in CT and YN must in fact be reconstructed differently.
For this series, we have actually partly discussed in §4.3.2.1 (cf. Table 45) as that
Pittayawat Pittayaporn gives proposals based on sesquisyllabic onsets. These onsets
are *p.r-, *p.t-, and *k.r- corresponding to Li’s *pr-, *tr-, and *kr- respectively.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s reconstructions are more proper if focusing on the
individual sound changes in different Tai varieties, because they avoid the
contradiction with his *pr-, *tr-, and *kr- which account for that PT lacked of
aspirations. For example, it can be explained that *p.r- merged into *p.t- then later
simplified to t- in Thai and Yai, on the contrary *p.t- merged into *p.r- then later
changed to pr- in Saek. In contrast, Lungchow keeps the distinct by h- and pʰj- (cf.
§4.3.2.1). Extending the explanation on the aspirated results in some other CT and
YN varieties such as Debao and Chongzuo, *p.r- > *p.t- > *ʰ.t- > tʰ- like in tʰa:kDL1
‘to expose’, *p.t- > *ʰ.t- > tʰ- like in tʰa:A1 ‘eye’, and *k.r- > *kʰr- > kʰj- like in
kʰjɔ:jC1 or kʰjoɥC1 ‘near’ can be processed. On the development from his *k.r- to
modern forms in Tai varieties, Pittayawat Pittayaporn suggests the following two
different paths as shown in Table 5349.
49
In Table 53, data of Tiandeng Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang (L17) and Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
(L8) in this chart are collected by me.
159
Table 53 Etyma with PT *k.r- (adapted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 169,
345)
Gloss
‘to imprison’
‘to drive away’
PT
*k.raŋA
SWT
CT
NT
Thai
Lungchow Xiangdu
Debao
Yay
kʰaŋA1
haŋA1
laŋA1
kʰjaŋA1
tɕa:ŋA1
-
lapDS1
kʰjapDS1
-
ha:ŋB1
la:ŋB1
kʰja:ŋB1
tɕa:ŋB1
hajC1
lajC1
kʰjajC1
tɕajC1
*k.rapD
kʰapDS1
‘top for spinning’ *k.ra:ŋB
kʰa:ŋB1
‘illness, fever’
*k.rajC
kʰajC1
‘to beg’
*k.ro:A
kʰɔ:A1
hɔ:A1
lo:A1
kʰjo:A1
-
klajC1
kʰjaɰC1
ɕaɰC1
kʰjɔ:jC1
tɕaɰC1
(2009: 169)
‘near’
*k.raɯC
(2009: 345)
These reconstructions are also problematic because of the following two reasons. (1)
They are structurally different on that *p.r- and *k.r- as “voiceless stop + medial” on
one hand and *p.t- as “voiceless stop + voiceless stop” on the other hand, and this
makes the explanation on the induced by process of aspiration in CT and YN become
less consistent than Li’s reconstruction. Nonetheless, this problem can still be solved
by Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s explanation on different diachronic hierarchies of initial
changes, like *p.t- > *p.r- or *p.r- > *p.t- depending on different languages. (2) The
explanations of the sound changes from PT *k.r- are inconsistent.
Although Pittayawat Pittayaporn claims that his PT *k.r- “either became a true
cluster *kr-, or simply lost the initial *k- to become *ʰr-, depending on the language”
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 170), thus *k.r- > *kr- > kʰ- in Thai, *k.r- > *ʰr- >
h- in Lungchow, *k.r- > *ʰr- > l- in Xiangdu, *k.r- > *kr- > kʰj- in Debao, and *k.r> *kr- > kj- > tɕ- in Yay can be drawn. However, data from Thai, Lungchow and
Xiangdu in Table 53 show that the term ‘near’ should have a different PT initials
from his *k.r-, otherwise the explanation cannot account for the different paths of
initial changes in Thai, Lungchow, and Xiangdu. Therefore, another possible PT *k.lwhich accounts for *k.l- > kl- in Thai, *k.l- > *k.r- > kʰr- > kʰj- in Lungchow and
Debao, *k.l- > *k.r- > kʰr- > kʰj- > tɕʰ- > ɕ- in Xiangdu, and *k.l >*kl- > kj- > tɕ-
in Yay is proposed in this thesis, for distinguishing *k.r- proposed by Pittayawat
Pittayaporn and for reasonably explaining the aspirated initial induced for this term
in CT varieties. Hence, we finally get a special series of PT sesquisyllabic onsets
consisting *p.r-, *p.t- and *k.l- on explaining the aspiration only found in CT and YN,
as shown in the following Table 54.
160
Table 54 The reconstruction of PT ASO-CY
Proposer
Li (1977: 86, 118, 225)
Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 170, 163,
169, 345)
Suggestions in this thesis
ASO-CY reconstructed
*pr-
*tr-
*kr-
*p.r-
*p.t
*k.r-
*p.r-
*p.t-
*k.l-
Example
Tai varieties
‘to expose’
‘eye’
‘near’
Thai (SWT)
ta:kDL1
ta:A1
klajC1
Debao Urban (L8, CT)
tʰa:kDL1
tʰa:A1
kʰjɔ:jC1
Lungchow (CT)
pʰja:kDL1
ha:A1
kʰjaɰC1
Chongzuo (YN, CT/NT?)
tʰa:kDL1
tʰa:A1
kʰjoɥC1
Yay (NT)
ta:kDL1
ta:A1
tɕaɰC1
This series of initials is called “Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in CT and
YN” (ASO-CY), for distinguishing the other series “Aspiration induced by
sesquisyllabic onsets in YN” (ASO-YN), in the following discussion. This ASO-CY will
replace the formulation 1-UC in the revised Tai tone box (cf. Liao & Shen 2012)
because they are not treated to be real “unaspirated stop + *-r-” in PT (cf. §6.5).
Different from ASO-CY induced by aspiration in both CT and YN, the other series of
onsets ASO-YN only induce aspiration in the most varieties of YN. In other words,
ASO-YN in CT and SWT which containing robust aspiration are not found to be
aspirated, as shown in Table 5550.
50
Data from Thai, Lungchow, Long’an and Yay are mainly adopted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn
(2009: 164, 171) except data of the term ‘to touch’ collected by me. Data from Debao Lüliu (L10) and
Shuangding (L26) are collected by me.
161
Table 55 Etyma with ASO-YN
Li
Pittayawat
(1977:
Pittayaporn
118, 121,
(2009: 164,
187)
171, 330)
SWT
Gloss
Debao
Lung-
Lüliu
chow
temA1
tamA1
totDS1
‘big leaf’ tɔ:ŋA1
NT
Shuang-
timA1
tʰimA1
rimA1
tatDS1
tɤtDS1
tʰɯtDS1
tʰəmA1
tʰɔtDS1
ratDS1
to:ŋA1
to:ŋA1
-
tʰo:ŋA1
roŋA1
tʊnB1
tɯ:nB1
tʰɤnB1
tʰənB1
rɯnB1
C1
‘to touch’ tɔ:ŋ
to:ŋC1
-
-
tʰo:ŋC1
-
‘fart’
*k.t-
YN
Long’an
Thai
‘full’
*tl-
CT
‘wake up’ tɯ:nB1
ding
Yay
*thr-
*t.r-
‘to break’ hakDS1
takDS1
takDS1
tʰakDS1
tʰakDS1
rakDS1
*k-
*k-
‘fishbone’ ka:ŋC1
ka:ŋC1
ka:ŋC1
kʰa:ŋC1
kʰa:ŋC1
ka:ŋC1
In the chart above, data from the first set are plenty to attest the phenomenon that
the proposed sesquisyllabic onset *k.t- causes aspiration only in the varieties of YN
whose classification as CT or NT is to debate, as drawn in the processes *k.t- > t- in
Thai, Debao and Lungchow, *k.t- > *t.r- > *ʰr- > r in Yay, but *k.t- > *t.r- > *tr> tʰ- in Long’an and Shuangding of YN. The different developing paths of PT *k.t-
indicates that NT and YN share closer innovation. The second and the third sets are
both only provided a single example, but the rule of sound correspondence attested
by them is still effective. Pittayawat Pittayaporn gives the first two sets
sesquisyllabic onsets, but leave the third one ‘fishbone’ under PT *k- in the wordlist
(Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 330), probably because the initials for this term in all
the Tai varieties investigated by him are all unaspirated. Li (1977: 187) also put this
term under his *k- due to the fact that there is no aspirated initial for this term in
any Tai dialect listed. However, at least Shuangding (L26) of YN from my fieldwork,
and Fusui of YN (cf. Zhang et al. 1999: 626) both have kʰa:ŋC1 for this term. In
Qinzhou of YN and Lianshan Zhuang (in Guangdong) which presents similar
aspirated rules with YN, the term for ‘(throat) stucked by fishbone’ is also the
aspirated kʰa:ŋC1 (cf. Zhang et al. 1999: 702), and this term has probably undergone
a semantic change from ‘fishbone’. Data of these YN varieties suggest that the PT
initial for ‘fishbone’ has to be in fact reconstructed differently. One may argue that
this term may be a post-PT loan from Chinese, namely *kaŋB (鯁) ‘fishbone’ in MC
(cf. Karlgren 1954). However, all the reconstructions for this term in MC agree on
the unaspirated initial stop *k-, and this hardly explain the aspirated stop *kʰreflected in the varieties in YN. Therefore, I temporarily reconstruct a sesquisyllabic
onset *C̥ .k- for this term, for the possibility that the initial voiceless segment might
162
give an aspirated effect on the following *.k- when it was lost in YN. Based upon the
adoption of Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s *k.t- and *t.r- for the first two sets, and my own
reconstruction *C̥ .k-, the AMO-YN row is established to be added into the
designation of the new revised of Tai tone box in this thesis (cf. Chapter 6), since
secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspiration are robust in many YN varieties.
Before finishing the discussion of secondary aspiration in this sub-section, we briefly
review the remaining sets of sesquisyllabic onsets proposed by Pittayawat
Pittayaporn (2009). For example, *p.q- which is commonly reflected as the aspirated
initial kʰ- in SWT and CT varieties, but is reflected as kw- in Saek like kwaA1 ‘leg’, and
the labio-velar feature -w- is the basis of reconstructed the initial segment *p.-.
Another similar one is *p.q- which is supposed to become *q- > kʰ- in SWT/CT, p- in
NT, but *ʰ.p- > pʰ- in YN varieties, like on the term ‘body air, feather’ as kʰonA1 in
Thai (SWT) and Lungchow (CT), punA1 in Yay (NT), but pʰunA1, pʰɤnA1, and pʰɯnA1 in
Qinzhou, South Yongning, and Fusui (all of YN) respectively (Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 164-165). All these proposed sesquisyllabic onsets are similar to AMO-SY to
induce aspirated initials in both ST (SWT/CT) and YN varieties, and they are named
as “Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic onsets in ST and YN” with the abbreviation
ASO-SY. Therefore, they should be grouped together under the 1-A row on the Tai
tone box designated by Liao and Shen (2012) when applying to the possible
secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds. In contrast, PT *c.r- is
proposed to have undergone the following processes *c.r- > *cr- > tʰ- in Thai (SWT)
and Lungchow (CT), *c.r- > *cr- > s- in Yay (NT), but *c.r- > *k.r- > *kr- > *kʰr> kʰj- in Debao and Jingxi and furthermore becomes tɕʰ- in Western Nung and
Guangnan Nung. Therefore, the word ‘to ask’ is tʰa:mA1 in Thai and Lungchow, sa:mA1
in Yay, kʰja:mA1 in Debao and Jingxi, as well as tɕʰa:mA1 in Western Nung and
Guangnan Nung (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 171). However, in YN varieties, this
PT initial developed into unaspirated sounds like in NT, like *c.r- > *cr- > tɕ- in
Shuangding. Another similar one is *t.h-, as in PT *t.hajC ‘to weep, to cry’ > hajC1 in
CT/SWT, but tajC1 in Yay (NT) (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 184-185) and YN
(agreeing with NT). One more example that confirms aspiration in CT/SWT but
unaspirated in YN/NT is PT *q.s-, which presents as in the single example *q.sepD
‘centipede’ > kʰepDS1 in Thai (SWT), kʰipDS1 in Lungchow (CT), and kʰjapDS1 in Jingxi
(CT), but lypDS1 in Qinzhou (YN) and θipDS1 in Wuming (NT) (Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 178). Therefore, a new series named “Aspiration induced by sesquisyllabic
onsets in ST” with the abbreviation ASO-ST is also established.
163
In conclusion, secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds has to be
divided into different diachronic hierarchies. The earliest hierarchy should be VASO1/2, since it indicates that the sesquisyllabic onsets “voiced obstruent + voiceless
stop” which induced by Series 1 of voicing alternation between NT/YN and CT/SWT
(cf. §4.3.2.1) were preserved at least at the time of CT/SWT splitting from PT. This
supports that CT and SWT should be grouped into the proposed ST group soon after
the post-PT period (cf. Chapter 5). Aspiration induced by “plain stop + *-r-” clusters
is commonly found in CT/SWT and YN, and this indicates that they may be earliest
factor that cause aspirated sounds to arise, resulting a new phonation group (AMOSY) in which other PT initials (like *q-, *x-, *χ-, *h-, *ʰw-, and *ʰr-) were allowed to
merge. After that, VASO-1/2 lost their sesquisyllabic structure to merge into
aspirated sounds to condition tones into the original high register in CT/SWT, but
this process is not found in YN. This reminds that VASO-1/2 has earlier lost its
sesquisyllabic structure to become plain stops in YN, just like what processed in NT.
This point is very crucial for grouping YN with NT together on a post-PT level before
they split from each other. Since aspirated sounds in YN should have secondarily
developed as an areal trait, they do not support to group YN with CT/SWT (cf.
Chapter 5). Other sesquisyllabic groups, namely ASO-CY, ASO-YN, ASO-SY have to
be later development after the arise of aspirated sounds which is induced by the
AMO-SY group. However, they have to be separated to rank into different diachronic
hierarchies in different language groups or varieties due to the different tonal
development as follows. (1) As an areal feature, ASO-SY were commonly merged
into aspirations in both ST and YN probably due to their adjacent distribution in
history. (2) After SWT split from ST due to the migration to MSEA from the Tai
homeland, ASO-CY as an areal feature were merged into aspirations in both CT and
YN. (3) Finally, AMO-YN independently developed in YN as presenting to be merged
into aspirations. VASO-1/2, AMO-SY, AMO-ST, ASO-SY, ASO-CY, AMO-YN, and
AMO-ST which result in aspirated sounds (1-A) have consistently condition tonal
splits in individual languages. In other words, in spite of the different sources,
aspirated sounds (1-A) in a single language always condition the same tonal
behavior (cf. Chapter 6). The situation of aspirated sounds developing from different
PT initial groups is summarized as in Table 56.
164
Table 56 Reflexes of aspiration involving in different modern Tai groups51
Initial groups
involving
Directions of merger in modern Tai groups
SWT
CT
YN
NT
VASO-1/2
1-A
1-A
2
2
AMO-SY
1-A
1-A
1-A
1-U
AMO-ST
1-A
1-A
1-U
1-U
ASO-SY
1-A
1-A
1-A
1-U
ASO-CY
1-U
1-A
1-A
1-U
ASO-YN
1-U
1-U
1-A
1-U
ASO-ST
1-A
1-A
1-U/1-C
1-U/1-C
aspiration
Different from secondary tonal splits amply found in the high register or tonal series
1, tonal splits have not been found in the low register or tonal series 2 until the data
from some varieties of Guibei Zhuang (of NT, hereafter GB) are collected. In the
wordlist of Suogan (see L36 in Appendix C) which is of GB (NT), several terms with
original voiced initials commonly reconstructed have an unexpected high register
tone (tonal series 1). In the lexical comparison in Table 57, a phenomenon of
voicing alternation between Suogan (NT) and all other Tai varieties is shown. The
shaded items which are expected to have PT voiced initials rather have a high
register tone to reflect having PT voiceless initials in Suogan. These items normally
have initial consonants developing from the proposed *mw- (vs. Li 1977: 74, Liang &
Zhang 1996: 97, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 154) like in məŋA1 ‘hand’ and majC1
‘tree’, *nr- (vs. Li 1977: 131, Liang & Zhang 1996: 97) or *C̬ .n (vs. Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 177) like in namC1 ‘water’ and nɔkDS1 ‘bird’, and *g- (cf. Li 1977:
199, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 359) or *gl- (Liang & Zhang 1996: 226) like hu:B1
‘pair’52. This phenomenon is also found in Huanjiang (also of GB) (cf. Zhang et al.
1999: 192), which is geographically closed to Suogan.
51
1-A, 1-U, 1-C and 2 in the chart are adopted from the tone box revised by Liao and Shen (2012)
(cf. §1.2.1), and refer to voiceless aspirated sounds, voiceless unaspirated sounds, initials developing
from proto-voiceless continuants and initials developing from proto-voiced sounds respectively.
52
The term hu:33 (B1) ‘pair’ has an unexpected initial (k- in NT and most CT/SWT but ɕʰ- in Thai
(SWT), Zuozhou (CT) and Baoxu (CT)) and tone (expected to have Tone B2 in NT), and it is not
certain that it is cognate with *gu:B ‘pair’ reflected in other Tai varieties. If it is indeed the cognate, the
changes of the initial and the tone need more counterpart data to be determined with.
165
Table 57 Voicing alternation between Suogan of NT and other Tai varieties
Initial reconstructed
Li
Pittayawat
(1977)
Pittayaporn
NT
CT
SWT
Suogan
Du’an
Wuming
Debao Thai
(L36)
(L35)
(L30)
(L8)
(L1)
‘hand’
məŋA1
fəŋA2
fɯŋA2
mʊŋA2
mɯ:A2
‘tree’
majC1
fajC2
majC2
majC2
ma:jC2
ma:C2
Gloss
(2009)
*mw-
*mw-
*m-
*m-
‘horse’
ma:C2
ma:C2
ma:C2
ma:C2
*d-
*d-
‘ant’
mɔtDS2
matDS2
motDS2
mɔ:tDS2 motDS2
*nr-
*C̬.n
‘water’
namC1
ɹam C2
ɣam C2
namC2
na:mC2
‘bird’
nɔkDS1
ɹɔk55
ɣok DS2
nɔ:kDS2
nokDS2
field’
na:A2
na:A2
na:A2
na:A2
na:A2
‘rot’
nawB2
nawB2
nawB2
nawB2
nawB2
‘pair’
hu:B1
kowB2
kawB2
kowB2
kʰu:B2
ga:A2
ka:A2
ka:A2
ka:A2
kʰa:A2
‘rice
*n-
*g-
*n-
*g-
‘get
stuck’
In this thesis, this phenomenon is temporarily proposed to be a split from the low
register in some specific NT varieties (like Suogan and Huanjiang) to merge into the
high register, since these terms all reflect a PT voiced initial in the vast majority of
the Tai languages. Therefore, the conditioning factors that cause the tones on these
terms to fall into the high register in these specific NT varieties afford for thought. It
is interesting that the first two sets *mw- and *nr- (or *C̬ .n-) present as the same to
m- and n- respectively in CT/SWT, but different from f- and r- respectively in the
vast majorities of NT, like fəŋA2 ‘hand’, fajC2 ‘tree’, ɹamC2 ‘water’ and ɹɔkDS2 ‘bird’ in
Du’an. This series of initials reconstructed by Li and Pittayawat Pittayaporn both can
hardly account for this voicing alternation between Suogan and other Tai varieties.
Therefore, these sets of PT initial must be differently reconstructed.
Although we do not see that this series of initials in the low register causes tonal
split in any other Tai varieties out of GB (NT), we do see that the same phenomenon
is commonly found in the KS languages. Geographically these GB varieties (like
Suogan and Huanjiang) distribute in the area of Tai and KS border, therefore this
voicing alternation may be an areal trait shared by these GB varieties and some of
166
the KS languages. For that reason, the determination of the reconstruction of this
series of initials may get help from the comparison between these GB varieties and
the KS languages, as shown in the following Table 58.
Table 58 Voicing alternation in Tai and KS languages53
Gloss
NT
KS
Suogan
Huanjiang
Wuming
Kam
Then
Mulam
Mak
Sui
‘hand’
məŋA1
mɯŋA1
fɯŋA2
mja:A2
mja:A2
nja:A2
mi:A1ʰ
mja:A1
‘tree’
majC1
majC1
majC2
majC2
majC2
majC2
majC2
majC2
‘ghost’
ma:ŋA1
ma:ŋA1
fa:ŋA2
-
ma:ŋA2
-
ma:ŋA1
ma:ŋA1
‘water’
namC1
nam C1
ɣam C2
namC2
namC2
nəmC2
namC1
namC1
‘bird’
nɔkDS1
nok55
ɣok DS2
mokDS2
nokDS2
nɔkDS2
nokDS2
nokDS2
In Table 58 above, all items with the proposed secondary tonal split from the
original low register are shaded. The voicing alternation present as that Suogan and
Huanjiang of NT as well as Mak and Sui of KS share similar tonal split patterns in
some items, in contrast Wuming (representing the vast majority of NT) as well as
Kam, Then, and Mulam of KS are supposed to preserve the original tonal behaviors
of these items. Just like what he reconstructs for other series of voicing alternation
among KS languages (cf. §4.3.2.1), Weera Ostapirat gives the explanation that the
accented initial (like *'-m-) was indeed the origin of voicing alternation of this series
of nasals in KS languages. In his explanation, the sonorant m- in this series was the
original medial of *'-m-, and the drop of the accented initial gave a byproduct,
namely a breath/aspiration feature which finally gave a tone in tonal series 1 (the
high register tone) in Mak and Sui, in contrast other KS languages just simply
dropped the accented initial and the medial -m- became the initial which gave a
tone in tonal series 2 (the low register tone) (Weera Ostapirat 2006: 1113-1114).
Therefore, we also simply speculate a counterpart *'-n- which has processed a similar
way in different KS languages for the set of ‘water’ in Table 58.
Because Li (1977), Liang and Zhang (1996), and Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) all
do not provide any reasonable reconstruction accounting the voicing alternation of
this series of nasals between Suogan/Huanjiang and other NT (as well as CT/SWT)
varieties, and the viewpoint of accented initial proposed by Weera Ostapirat is
limited to the application of PKS, here I gives a provisional series of sesquisyllabic
53
Data from Huanjiang are adopted from Zhang et al. (1999: 596, 615, 626, 632, 644), and data
from KS languages are adopted from Weera Ostapirat (2006: 1114) and Liang & Zhang (1996: 313,
314, 319).
167
onsets “voiceless stop + nasal” such as *C̥ .N- such as *C̥ .n- and *C̥ .m- in PT for
accounting of this series of voicing alternation. The initial segments of proposed
*C̥ .N- are reduced in all modern Tai varieties and may have given different effects
on the following nasal medial due to different languages as shown in the following
Table 59.
Table 59 Developing tracks of PT *C̥.N- in modern Tai varieties
PT
*N- (initial simply
ST
dropping)
*ʰN- (weakening/MOA
The vast
majority
NT
Developing process
*C̥.N-
changing)
Modern forms
Tone
n- and m-
low (Series 2)
r-/ɣ-/ɹ-/l- and f-
low (Series 2)
n- and m-
high (Series 1)
Suogan,
Huanjia
*N̥ - (devoicing)
ng, etc.
In ST (CT/SWT), it may have been just simply dropped off to make the following
voiced nasal medials become simple voiced nasal initials to condition the tones into
the low register, as in *C̥.n- > n-2 and *C̥.m- > m-2. In the vast majorities of NT, it
may have dropped off to give a weakening effect (with an pre-aspiration) first then
cause the following nasal medials to change their manner of articulation become rand f-, but the tonal behavior is preserved in the low register probably because it has
been earlier conditioned by the voiced nasal medials, as in *C̥.n- > ʰn- > r-2 and
*C̥.n- > ʰm- > f-2. In some GB varieties such as Suogan and Huanjiang which may
have been influenced by the neighboring KS languages, it may have been dropped
off and make the following voiced nasal medials become voiceless nasal initials to
condition the tones into the high register, as in *C̥.n- > *n̥- > n-1 and *C̥.m- > *m̥ > m-1.
The proposed tonal split within the low register in NT is then clarified to be a
special series of voicing alternation, due to another specific series of sesquisyllabic
onsets *C̥ .N-. This series is hereafter called “Voicing Alternation induced by
Sesquisyllabic Onsets in GB” (VASO-GB) to be put into the proposed ideal Tai tone
box in this thesis (cf. Chapter 6).
Tonal splits in tonal languages in China and MSEA are commonly known as two-way
register split, which refers to that voiceless initials give rise to a high pitch, and
168
voiced initials give rise to a lower pitch, as previously discussed (cf. §4.2). However,
in some languages including some Tai varieties, the tonal splits are not just in two
ways but in three ways, forming a tonal system with three registers, which is
relatively rare found and less-known.
Three-way splits are not single phenomenon in Tai. Lili and Jinxing, both of Wu
dialects spoken in Jiangsu Province of China, have respectively split their MC tones
*C/*D and MC tone *B into three tones (Chao 1928). Kam and Mak of KS have
three-way splits on tones ABC and A respectively (Haudricourt 1961: 68-69). Threeway splits are also reported to be found in some of the Miao languages (Li et al.
1959). Three-way tonal splits in Tai have actually involved in the discussions of the
types of secondary tonal splits in §4.3.1. In the Sinitic circles working on TK
languages, a specific notation “derived tone” (as the tone makers 1', 5', 7' and 9' in
Table-44)54 is used to name the third register which is considered to derived from
the original odd tones (= tonal series 1) conditioned by proto-voiceless initials.
Data from my fieldwork in this thesis show different schemes of three-way split in
the following Tai varieties investigated. Songkhla (L2, Southern Thai) of SWT has a
thorough three-way split pattern on all the ABCD tones. Jingxi Huashan (L16, Min
Zhuang), Jingxi Lingding (L19, Zuojiang Zhuang), and Daxin Leiping (L22, Zuojiang
Zhuang) of CT has it only in the A tone. Daxin Baoxu (L23, Zuojiang Zhuang) of CT
has it in A, C and DS. Bac Va Nung (L24) of CT has it in A, B, and DL. Long’an (L27)
of YN has it in B and DL. Xialeng (L29) of YN has it in A, C, and DL. Three-way
register split is rare found in NT varieties. The only NT variety having been reported
to have three-way register split may be Po-ai (cf. §4.3.2.2). An example of three-way
split pattern from Baoxu is provided as in the following Table 60.
Actually, the so-called “derived tones” (派生調) in the Sinitic circles refers to two different
phenomena of splitting tones: 1) Those secondary tonal splits (on the high register) which are
conditioned by one of the specific phonation types of initials (aforementioned glottalized sounds,
aspirated stops, or unaspirated stops) and do not merge into any other tones on smooth syllables, like
na:1 (A1-C) ‘thick’ with the “original tone” 55 vs. na:1’ (A1-G) ‘swaddling clothes’ with a “derived
tone” 33 vs. na:2 (A2) ‘rice field’ with another tone 31 in Xialeng Yongnan Zhuang. 2) Those
unexpected tone changes without any specific conditions on checked syllables. For example, in
Xialeng Yongnan Zhuang the original DL1-A (with aspirated initials) should be a high-falling tone 53,
but some items with aspirated initials have a different tone 35, and this is also called “derived tone”. In
the discussions in this thesis, the “derived tones” only refers to the first situation if without additional
notes. Note that Edmondson (1990) use “prime tone” which correspondences “derived tone” here in
his KS languages for adopting the system of tone lables in the Sinitic circles.
54
169
Table 60 Basic tonal patterns in Daxin Baoxu Zuojiang Zhuang (cf. L23)
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Protovoiceless
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 45
B1-A: 34
C1-A: 13ʔ
DL1-A: 34
DS1-A: 13
Continuants
A1-C: 45
B1-C: 34
C1-C: 13ʔ
DL1-C: 34
DS1-C: 13
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 34ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 34
A1-UC:45
B1-UC: 34
C1-UC: 13ʔ
DL1-UC: 34
DS1-UC: 13
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 34ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 34
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 11̰ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 11
A1/2: 45
B1/2: 34
C1/2: 13ʔ
DL1/2: 34
DS1/2: 13
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
As what we have discussed in §4.3.1, three-way splits must contain a secondary split
conditioned by aspirated and/or glottalized sounds (as well as unaspirated stops in
some varieties). This split leads a new tone to derive from the original odd tones (=
tonal series 1) which is conditioned by proto-voiceless initials. Therefore, three-way
split must be considered not to be of the primary splitting pattern. However, not all
secondary tonal split patterns result three-way registers. As in many other Tai
varieties which have secondary tonal splits, the register is still two-way. That is, the
split from the original high register has been merged into the low register, such as
the tonal patterns containing secondary splits in Thai (L1), Isan (L3), Northern Thai
(L4), the three Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (L8, L9, L10), Hurun (L11), Dazhai
(L21), Daqiu Nung An (L28), Dongling (L32), Lizhou (L32), Gehan (L39) and
Huishui Bouyei (L41). However, the following discussion will speculate a diachronic
sequence beginning with three-way split and ending in a two-way split due to the
limited capability of the number of tones in a language. That is to say, Tai varieties
with secondary splits resulting at a two-way register may have undergone a
diachronic process: primary two-way register split > secondary three-way split >
secondary two-way split. Just like in Baoxu in Table 60, although the A, C, and DS
columns preserve the three-way register split, the columns B and DL may have
merged their derived tone into the low register to end in a two-way register split.
The starting point of this hypothesis is that most secondary tonal splits conditioned
by glottalized and aspirated sounds have depressed the pitch of the syllable due to
170
the laryngeal tension (Zhang 1980: 38). This point is supported by that the derived
tones in Tai varieties with three-way splits are normally pitch-lower than the
original tone (except those varieties having tonal flip-flop). This suggestion is
inspired by the situation of three-way split in Kam. As what we have reviewed in
§2.2.2, three-way split in Kam (cf. Table 24 and Table 25 in §2.2.2) is treated as the
existence of register tripartition suggested by Haudricourt (1961), to demonstrate
that the third register split can result from syllable initial voiceless aspiration.
However, Edmondson (1990) argues that the three-way split pattern results from
two temporarily distinct bipartitions rather than one simultaneous tripartition
(Edmondson 1990: 188). On the basis of a proposed complex interaction among
three factors (breathy voice, aspiration, and deaspiration) in Kam, he furthermore
insists the real cause of secondary split to be breathy voice instead of aspiration,
since aspiration is unlike to be a candidate for lowering pitch of the syllable onset
because it is usually assumed to cause an increase in F0, on the other hand
breathiness is often treated as pitch depressor (Edmondson 1990: 191). Thus breathy
voice > aspiration > deaspiration is his process of the unmarking to form the two
splits on the high register. Note that the viewpoint that PT lacked of aspiration also
successfully applied to PKS (Liang & Zhang 1996). As we have supposed that
aspiration in Tai arose by being induced by by *C̥ r- clusters rather than any other
group of initials, breathy sounds which may be induced by *C̬ .C̥ - clusters (or VASO1/2) finally merged into aspiration through a possible breathy process (cf. §4.3.2.1
and §4.3.2.3). If Edmondson’s viewpoint that breathiness may be the real cause of
pitch-lowering in Kam is true, the secondary split conditioned by aspiration in many
CT and YN varieties may also have undergone a pitch-lowering process when the
breathy stops merging into aspirated stops. That is, the lower pitch of the derived
tone (or the prime tone by Edmondson) is brought into aspiration by breathiness
when it dropped the voiced feature off to be unmarked. In addition, glottalized
sounds is known as a pitch depressor, as it frequently occurring as the conditioning
factor of secondary tonal splits in many Tai varieties.
The three-way register split in Huashan Min Zhuang (CT) (shown in the following
Table 61) provides an example for indicating the speculation on the diachronic
process from three-way to two-way. In this dialect, the only three-way register split
is found in the A column. Aspirations (Rows 1-A, 1-UC, and 1/2) always condition a
derived tone with a lower pitch (242) than the original odd tone (353). This pitch
distinction is quite slight and is easily ignored if we do not observe that the same
tonal depress is also found on the same rows with aspirated initials (Rows 1-A, 1-UC,
171
and 1/2) in the DS column. Different from the slight split in Tone A, the pitchlowering in Tone DS is quite clear (a mid-falling 32 vs. a high level 55).
Table 61 Basic tonal patterns in Huashan Min Zhuang (cf. L16)
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Protovoiceless
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 242
B1-A: 32
C1-A: 11ʔ
DL1-A: 32
DS1-A: 32
Continuants
A1-C: 353
B1-C: 32
C1-C: 11ʔ
DL1-C: 32
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 353
B1-U: 32
C1-U: 11ʔ
DL1-U: 32
DS1-U: 55
Unaspirated stop
A1-UC:
B1-UC: 32
C1-UC: 11ʔ
DL1-UC: 32
DS1-UC: 32
+ *r clusters
242
Glottal sounds
A1-G: 353
B1-G: 32
C1-G: 11ʔ
DL1-G: 32
DS1-G: 55
A2: 55
B2: 42
C2: 53ʔ
DL2: 42
DS2: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A1/2-A:
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
242
A1/2-C:
DS1/2-A:
B1/2: 32
C1/2: 11ʔ
DL1/2: 32
32
DS1/2-C:
353
55
It is interesting that this dialect has undergone tonal flip-flops as the original low
tones are all pitch-higher than the original high tone. However, the derived tones
splitting from the odd tones are still lower than the lowered odd tone after tonal
flip-flop. This suggests that tonal flip-flop had established first, then secondary tonal
split conditioned by aspiration happened to cause a pitch-lowering derived tone. The
derived tone in the A column shows that the pitch-lowered effected by aspiration
may be too slight to cause a final split, and it may merge back to the original pitch if
the deriving process stops. However, the derived tone in the DS column may have
forced the original high tone to become higher to merge into the original low tone
which has long been high pitch since tonal flip-flop, to end in a two-way register
split: derived tone vs. original odd/even tone. All these suggest tonal split are
progressing.
We therefore can imagine that in those Tai varieties which secondary tonal splits
ending in a two-way register split may have undergone a similar process as that the
derived tone splitting from the high register form a three-way register first, and then
the pitch gets lower and lower, at last merge into the low register to end in a new
two-way register pattern.
172
In the previous sub-sections of §4.3, we have discussed different types of tonal splits
conditioned by different groups of PT initials. All these types of tonal splits are
focused on smooth syllables which develop from PT tonal categories *A, *B, and *C,
although they are also found on checked syllables developing from PT tonal
category *D. However, tonal patterns on checked syllables in modern Tai varieties
have to be divided into two tonal categories DL and DS due to vocalic length, as
frequently presenting in the majorities of Tai that Tone DL goes with the same
patterns of Tone B, and Tone DS goes with the same patterns of Tone C based on the
long vocalic duration of *B and the short vocalic duration of *C in PT level (cf.
§.4.1.3.2). In this sub-section, secondary tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length is
focused on. As previously mentioned, this type of secondary tonal splits is only
found in checked syllables in Tai languages (cf. §1.2.1 and §1.2.2). The following
observations are perceived during the library research, fieldwork and data analyses
of this thesis.
First, tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length (on checked syllable) present as
horizontally to contrast to those vertically conditioned by initial features. It means
that Tone D splits into DL conditioned by a long vowel or a cluster of two vowels
and DS conditioned by a short vowel, in contrast Tones A, B, C split into original
high Tones A1, B1, C1 conditioned by voiceless initials or into original low tones A2,
B2, C2 conditioned by voiced initials, or even into derived tones A1', B1', C1'
secondarily conditioned by a specific sub-group of voiceless initials. Gedney’s Tai
tone box (cf. Table 4 in §1.2.1) and Liao & Shen’s revised Tai tone box (cf. Table 7
in §1.2.2) has briefly and clearly drawn the outline of these two different directions
of tonal splits.
Second, the stability of tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length on checked syllable
support the reconstruction of the vocalic length contrast in PT. Previous studies have
argued on that whether there was vocalic length contrast in PT. Li (1977) posits that
PT lacked of vocalic length contrast and had rich diphthongs and triphthongs, based
on his argument that in many modern Tai dialects long and short vowels do not
contrast except the pair of *a: and *a (Li 1977: 259). Following his proposal, Luo
(1997) also considers that vocalic length contrast have secondarily developed in Tai
daughter languages. In contrast, Haudricourt (1948), Nishida (1954, 1955), Wang
(1966), Sarawit (1973), Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009), and Weera Ostapirat
(2013b) hold that PT contained vocalic length contrast. Sarawit’s argument which is
cited by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 196-198) is pertinent on that PT short *-e
173
and *-o were banned from open syllables due to that “only long vowels were
allowed in open syllable” (vs. that *-e: > -ɛ: and *-o: > -ɔ: in SWT can be in open
syllable) to indicate that Li’s proposal that Thai vowels *-e, *-ɛ:, *-o, *-ɔ: have PT
origins cannot account for the case (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 197-198). This
thesis supports the viewpoint that there was vocalic length contrast in PT due to the
stability of tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length on checked syllable. In some
Tai varieties, vocalic length contrast has been lost but the tonal splits conditioned by
the historical vocalic length are still preserved. In some other Tai varieties, some
vowels have been reversed the vocalic length but they still condition the horizontal
tonal splits on checked syllables together with their belonging original groups of
historical vocalic length. The following two tables illustrates these phenomena.
Table 62 Tonal splits in checked syllables in Guangnan Nong Zhuang 55
DL
Tonal values
Gloss
1-A=13
‘torn’
1-C=13
DS
Tonal values
Gloss
kʰat13
1-A=55
‘vegetable’
pʰak55
‘fruit’
mak13
1-C=55
‘heavy’
nak55
1-U=13
‘hug’
kɔt13
1-U=55
‘frog’
kɔp55
1-UC=13
‘to expose’
tʰak13
1-UC=55
‘grasshopper’
tak55
1-G=13
‘to love’
ˀdip13
1-G=55
‘raw’
ˀdip55
2=31
‘outside’
nɔk31
2=33
‘bird’
nɔk33
1/2=13
‘to hit/correct’
sok13
1/2=55
‘to bite’
kʰɔp55
Vocalic length contrast including *-a: vs. *-a has been totally lost in Nung Zhuang as
shown in Table 62 above. However, both horizontal splits and vertical splits are
straightforward to make a neat scheme of four tonal categories DL1 (13), DL2 (31),
DS1 (55) and DS2 (33) in this dialect. The vertical splits are obviously conditioned
by initials as what we have previously discussed. The horizontal splits lack of
conditioning factors if the synchronic vowel system without length contrast is only
considered. Only if the long vowels contrast to short vowels in history, can the
horizontal tonal splits on checked syllables in this dialect be reasonable explained.
55
Data from Guangnan Nong Zhuang are adopted from Zhang et al. (1999: 603, 615, 619, 621, 628,
693, 699, 701, 705, 711, 729, 737, 776, 782). The terms ‘to love’ in the DL column and ‘vegetable’ in
the DS column are from Yanshan Nong Zhuang, for they are able to more directly indicate the
situation of the loss of vocalic length contrast in Nung Zhuang. In the columns “tonal values”, tonal
categories (with abbreviated initial group names) and their tonal values are jointed by a “=” between
them.
174
The situation of vocalic length conditioning horizontal tonal splits in Debao Dalong
Yang Zhuang are more complicated as shown in Table 63.
Table 63 Tonal splits in checked syllables in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang 56
DL
Tonal values
Gloss
‘torn’
1-A=33
1-U=45
1-UC=33
1-G=33
‘fruit’
ma:k45
‘fog’
‘mouth’
‘to pound’
1-A=45
tʰʊk33
mo:k45
pa:k45
to:k45
‘to expose’
tʰa:k33
‘to love’
ˀdi:p33
1-C=45
1-U=45
‘bamboo stripe’
tʰo:k33
1-UC=45
‘flower’
ˀdo:k33
1-G=45
‘to wipe’
‘outside’
‘blood’
‘child’
‘surname’
‘knife’
‘to remove’
1/2=33
kʰa:t
ɕo:k33
‘bone’
2=33
Tonal values
33
‘measuring cup’
‘spinning’
1-C=45
DS
‘pale’
‘to hit/correct’
ˀdʊk33
‘chrysanths’
kʰjʊk45
‘six’
kʰjɔ:k45
‘heavy’
nak45
‘belly’
‘to stab’
‘to fall’
2=21
kjo:k33
mət33
1/2=45
pak45
tɔ:k45
tʰak45
‘raw’
ˀdəp45
‘to seed’
tʰɔ:k45
‘to suck’
ˀdɔ:t45
ˀdʊk45
mat21
nɔ:k21
‘poison’
tʊk21
‘motar’
kjɔ:k21
‘room’
ɹʊk21
‘to throw’
ʋət21
‘neat’
lət33
mɔ:k45
‘grasshopper’
‘bird’
lu:t33
tʰʊk33
pʰjak45
‘a grain’
no:k33
pʰa:k33
‘vegetable’
‘catfish’
ma:t33
lʊk33
Gloss
‘to bite’
‘cooked’
tɕət21
kʰap45
θʊk45
In Table 63 above, items on the same horizontal rows are supposed to have a
proposed PT long vowel (under the DL column) and its counterpart PT short vowel
(under the DS column) respectively. For example, the pair kʰa:t33 ‘torn’ vs. pʰjak45
‘vegetable’ indicates a vocalic length contrast *-a: vs. *-a in PT, and the pair ˀdip33 ‘to
love’ vs. ˀdəp45 ‘raw’ indicates a vocalic length contrast *-iə vs. *-i in PT, as the
vocalic length contrast is still preserved in these two pairs even some vowel shapes
have been changed (*-iə > -i: and *-i > -ə). However, the shaded items present an
opposite situation that all these items have unexpected vowel length as what
horizontal tonal splits they have conditioned. We first focus on -ɔ:- in the DS
56
This dialect is my mother tongue.
175
column. For example, nɔ:k21 ‘bird’ presents with a long vowel in Debao Dalong Yang
Zhuang (and also in all other Yang Zhuang varieties), but tonally it behaves as if it
belongs in the short vowel column (DS2). Actually the vowel -ɔ:- in Yang Zhuang
varieties always phonetically presents as a long vowel but corresponds to a short
vowel (-o- or -ɔ-) in the vast majority of Tai varieties, such as kɔ:n³¹ (Debao) vs.
kʰon³³ (Thai) ‘person’, kjɔ:n53 (Debao) vs. kron³³ (Thai) ‘to snore’, and tɔ:k45 (Debao)
vs. tok21 (Thai) ‘to fall’. The fact that ‘bird’ in the vast majority of Tai languages
presents as a short vowel confirms that the proto-vowel of it must have been the
short *-ok (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 230), thus *-o- > -ɔ:- (as well as their
counterpart *-o:- > -o:-) must have processed in Yang Zhuang varieties. However, in
Yang Zhuang tones on the items with this long -ɔ:- on checked syllables are
preserved to behave as in DS since they had been conditioned by its predecessor *-oin history. Therefore, kʰjɔ:k45 ‘six’, tʰɔ:k55 ‘to seed’, and ˀdɔ:t45 ‘to suck’ in the high
register, as well as kjɔ:k21 ‘motor’ and nɔ:k21 ‘bird’ in the low register do not have the
same tone as their DL counterpart 33 even they have the same vocalic length now.
Comparing this with the words tʰʊk33 ‘spinning’, ˀdʊk33 ‘bone’, lʊk33 ‘child’ and tʰʊk33
‘to hit/correct’ reflecting a short vowel -ʊ-, as well as mət33 ‘knife’ and lət33 ‘to
dismantle’ reflecting the other short vowel -ə-, which presents phonetically as a short
vowel but belongs to the DL column in this dialect, the proto-vowel sometimes have
been overturned their length in daughter languages. These two vowels always
contrast to their counterparts -ʊ- and -ə- in the DS column respectively on their
different ways of tonal behavior conditioning. This suggests -ʊ- and -ə- under the DL
column must have been long vowels in history. Cognates from SWT like Thai du:kDL1
‘bone’, lu:kDL2 ‘child’ and tʰu:kDL1/2 ‘to hit/correct’ and mi:tDL2 ‘knife’ all with a long
vowel conditioning their tones into DL clearly confirm this speculation. Therefore,
although they have been merged into their DS counterparts to lose the vowel length
contrast in Yang Zhuang, their collocating tones are still preserved to help us to
restore their historical shape.
Both the cases of Nong Zhuang and Yang Zhuang show us that in modern Tai
varieties, words do not always retain their proto-vowel length, and the loss of
vocalic length contrast must have been secondary.
Moreover, tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length must also be secondary
although the contrast of vocalic length was proposed to exist in PT. This is because
of the following two reasons. (1) As what previously pointed out that in the
majorities of Tai that tone DL frequently goes with the same patterns of tone B,
176
within the forty-two Tai varieties investigated in this thesis, thirty-six have the same
or similar tonal patterns between tones developing from *DL and *B. Only the
following six varieties have different tonal patterns between DL and B, as Songkhla
(L2, SWT) and Bac Va Nung (L24, CT) with DL going with A, as well as Khon Kaen
(L3, SWT), Longsang (L31, NT), Baipeng (L37, NT) and Gehan (39) with DL going
with C. The dominant tonal merger between DL and B tones supports that a
coordination between Tones *D and *B have existed at the PT level. For Tone DS,
the tonal mergers between smooth and checked syllables are scattered as in that
nine varieties have DS going with B, sixteen have DS going with C, and seventeen
have DS going with A. The situation that tonal mergers between DS and other tonal
categories lack of a dominant direction indicates that tonal splits conditioned by PT
short vowels were secondary. (2) Mergers between tones on checked syllables and
smooth syllables can be speculated to be on which diachronic orders due to
comparison.
Taking the tonal patterns of three Yang Zhuang varieties in Table 36 (in §4.1.3.3.2),
Table 64, and Table 65 (also cf. L8 to L13 in Appendix C) below as examples, this
problem will be readily solved. We first look at Table 36. Debao Urban (L8) have a
neat merger between DL and B, and an irregular merger between DS and B. If we
understand that secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspiration (Rows 1-A, 1-UC,
1/2) and glottal sounds (Row 1-G) have caused tones in these rows on the B and DL
columns to fall into the low register or to merge into Row 2, we will recognize that
both DL and DS had neat mergers into B in history. That is to say, short vowels on
checked syllables in Debao Urban do not condition horizontal tonal splits, and the
DS column is preserved to have the original straightforward tonal pattern while the
DL column has undergone vertical secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated
and glottalized initials. Debao Dalong in Table 64 present a horizontal secondary
tonal split in the DS2 box, going along with C2, but all the other boxes under the DS
column are preserved to have the original *D pattern going along with the original
patterns in B (although vertical secondary tonal splits in this column have
undergone like in Debao Urban). Jingxi Anning in Table 65 vertically represents a
straightforward primary tonal split pattern, but horizontally illustrates a full tonal
split conditioned by the short vowels, to cause DS to go along with C. Only if the
diachronic process above was true, can the historical changes of tone on dead
syllables in these three Yang Zhuang varieties be reasonably explained. It further
suggests that tones DL and DS which are still coordinated with B should be treated
as preserving the primary tonal pattern of *D, and Tones DL and DS which have
went along with Tones A and C are of later development.
177
Table 64 Basic tonal patterns in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (cf. L9)
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Protovoiceless
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 31
B1-A: 332
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 45
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 454
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 45
DS1-C: 45
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 454
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 45
DS1-U: 45
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
A1-UC: 31
B1-UC:
332
DS1-UC:
45
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 332
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 45
A2: 31
B2: 332
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 21
B1/2: 332
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 45
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A: 31
breathy sounds
A1/2-C: 53
Table 65 Basic tonal patterns in Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang (cf. L13)
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Protovoiceless
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 53
B1-A: 453
C1-A: 33ʔ
DL1-A: 45
DS1-A: 33
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 453
C1-C: 33ʔ
DL1-C: 45
DS1-C: 33
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 453
C1-U: 33ʔ
DL1-U: 45
DS1-U: 33
A1-UC: 53
B1-UC: 453
C1-UC: 33ʔ
DL1-UC: 45
DS1-UC: 33
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 453
C1-G: 33ʔ
DL1-G: 45
DS1-G: 33
A2: 31
B2: 131
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 13
DS2: 21
A1/2: 53
B1/2: 453
C1/2: 33ʔ
DL1/2: 45
DS1/2: 33
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
We finally probe into a series of “vowel-length alternation between ST and NT”
(DL/S), which is the last concern of tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length on
checked syllable. This series of vowels present to have short length in ST (SWT/CT)
on one hand and having long length in NT and YN on the other hand, as shown in
Table 66.
178
Table 66 Vowel-length alternation between ST and NT/YN
Tai group
ST
SWT
Debao
Gloss
NT
CT
Thai (L1) Urban
(L8)
Long-sang Dongling
(L31)
(L32)
YN
Du’an
Shuang-
Jiaren
ding
(L35)
(L26)
Daqiu
(L28)
Sanxiang
(L25)
‘gums’
ŋɯəkDL1
ŋy:kDL1
ŋɯ:kDL1
ŋəkDS1
həkDS1
-
-
-
‘deaf’
nuəkDL1
nu:kDL1
nukDS1
nokDS1
nokDS1
nokDS1
nukDS1
nokDS1
stripes’
tɔ:kDL1
tʰo:kDL1
tukDS1
tokDS1
tukDS1
tʰok DS1
tʰʊkDS1
tʰɔkDS1
‘sunshine’
dɛ:tDL1
ˀde:tDL1
ˀditDS1
ˀditDS1
ˀditDS1
nət DS1
ˀditDS1
ˀditDS1
tʰu:kDL1
tʰʊkDL1
təkDS2
təkDS2
təkDS2
tək DS2
tikDS2
-
‘child’
lu:kDL2
lʊkDL2
ləkDS2
ləkDS2
ləkDS2
ləkDS2
likDS2
likDS2
‘knife’
mi:tDL2
mjətDL2
mitDS2
mitDS2
mitDS2
-
-
-
‘bamboo
‘right/to
be’
Note that in Table 66 the phonetically short vowels of tʰʊkDL1 ‘right/to be’, lʊkDL1
‘child’ and mjətDL2 ‘knife’ in Debao actually reflect PT long vowels due to their tonal
behaviors (cf. Table 63 and its related discussions). Just like what is shown in Table
63, the items designated into the wordlist under the DL column due to SWT are
normally agreed on vocalic length by CT varieties, but show a regular vocalic
change along with the tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart rows in
the DS column in both NT and YN varieties. For example, in Longsang (L31), as
those in other NT varieties, the following terms expected to be under the DL column
show regular vocalic and tonal changes to coordinate with their counterpart DS
rows: nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’, ˀdit55 (DS1-G)
‘sunshine’, mit33 (DS2) ‘knife’, and tək33 (DS1/2-A) ‘to be’57. Shuangding (L26) of YN,
whose classification as CT and NT is open to debate, does not agree on the CT/SWT
varieties but agree on NT varieties to have terms nok35 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tʰok35 (DS1-UC)
‘thin bamboo stripes’, nət35 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, and tək44 (DS1/2) ‘correct’ which all
reflect short vowels. Therefore, an obvious boundary between ST and NT/YN on the
vocalic length reflected is outlined. This is crucial to our discussion because the
However, in Longsang the term ŋɯ:ɕ21 (DL1-C) ‘gums’ agreeing with that reflecting a long vowel
in the DL1-C column in all CT/SWT varieties. This may be influenced by its neighboring CT varieties
due to that it distributes on the NT and CT border.
57
179
supposed secondary horizontal tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length present
different splitting directions and results when meeting these kind of items. Besides,
any Tai tone box already designated cannot capture this tonal distinction by its DL
and DS columns.
On the basis of the hypothesis that PT had vocalic length contrast, this thesis agrees
on the reconstructions arguing that these items originally had long vowels from PT.
For example, the regular shortening of PT high long vowel *-i:-, *-ɯ:-, and *-u:preceding stop codas are frequently found in NT (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 215,
218-219, 220-221). The environments of vowel shortening may not be limited to
stop codas, such as *-ɨ- > -ɨ:- /___alveolar and *-ɨ:- > -u:-/___velar in Thai (SWT), but
*-ɨ:- > -ɨ-/___alveolar and *-ɨ:- > -u-/___velar in Lungchow (CT), as well as *-ɨ:- > -ɨ/___alveolar and *-ɨ:- > -ɨ-/___velar in Po-ai (NT), thus the terms ‘night’ and
‘firewood’, ‘child’ and ‘hit the mark’ which are assumed to have this PT vowel are
kʰɨ:nA2, fɨ:nA2, lu:kDL2 and tʰu:kDL1 in Thai, kʰɨnA2, fɨnA2, lukDS2 and tʰukDS1 in Lungchow,
as well as hɨnA2, fɨnA2, lɨkDS2 and tɨkDS2 in Po-ai respectively (Weera Ostapirat 2013b:
194)58. That is, in some specific environments original long vowels are speculated to
be shortened in some CT varieties and in NT (as well as YN) on both smooth and
checked syllables. Note that Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 222) reconstructs a short
*-uk for the rime of ‘bone’ which is also listed in Table 64. He explains that *-uk was
lengthened to *-u:k in all SWT and some CT varieties, was lengthened and lowered
to *-o:k in NT, and is preserved as a short -uk in some CT varieties. This explanation
can hardly account for the situation that it has a phonetically short vowel rime -ʊk
but reflects a historical long vowel by its tonal behavior in Debao Dalong Yang
Zhuang (cf. the discussions of Table 62). Therefore, the vowel of this term is still
considered to be originally long even though we will not furthermore reconstruct
every single proto-vowel in this thesis.
Such kind of “vowel-length alternation” may have accomplished in early history
after the split of CT/SWT and NT/YN due to the clear boundary between these two
groups (as in Table 63), but some of the items show that this process may still be
progressing. This can be confirmed by the comparison among SWT, NT and some
Yang Zhuang varieties that are very closely related, as in the following Table 67.
Note that items with both vocalic and tonal changes from DL to DS are shaded.
58
Weera Ostapirat (2013b) does not provide the tones for these items. Tones on these items are
replenished by me according to my own fieldwork or library research.
180
Table 67 The progressing vowel-length alternation between ST and NT/YN
Tai group
Gloss
ST
SWT
Thai (L1)
Yang Zhuang (CT)
Debao
Debao
Jingxi
Jingxi
Urban
Dalong
Urban
Anning
(L8)
(L9)
(L12)
(L13)
NT
YN
Dongling
Daqiu
(L32)
(L28)
‘gums’
ŋɯəkDL1
ŋy:kDL1
ŋy:kDL1
ŋi:kDL1
ŋi:kDL1
ŋəkDS1
-
‘deaf’
nuəkDL1
nu:kDL1
nu:kDL1
nu:kDL1
nu:kDL1
nokDS1
nukDS1
stripes’
tɔ:kDL1
tʰo:kDL1
tʰo:kDL1
tʰo:kDL1
tʰo:kDL1
tokDS1
tʰʊkDS1
‘sunshine’
dɛ:tDL1
ˀde:tDL1
ˀde:tDL1
ne:tDL1
ˀde:tDL1
ˀditDS1
ˀditDS1
tʰu:kDL1
tʰʊkDL1
tʰʊkDL1
tʰʊkDS1
tʰʊkDS1
təkDS2
tikDS2
‘child’
lu:kDL2
lʊkDL2
lʊkDL2
lʊkDS2
lʊkDS2
ləkDS2
likDS2
‘mucus’
mu:kDL2
mʊkDL2
mʊkDL2
mʊkDS2
mʊkDS2
mukDS2
mukDS2
‘knife’
mi:tDL2
mjətDL2
mjətDL2
mitDS2
mətDS2
mitDS2
-
‘bamboo
‘right/to
be’
In the chart above, four Yang Zhuang varieties present interesting progress on the
“vowel-length alternation”. They all agree with Thai (SWT) on the original vowel
length and the original tonal behaviors of the first four terms (from ‘gums’ to
‘sunshine’), and also agree with each other on the vowel shortening of the latter four
terms (from ‘right/to be’ to ‘knife’). However, different from the original tonal
behaviors are still preserved on the latter four terms in two Debao varieties, the
tones of these latter four terms in the two Jingxi varieties are also changed to be
coordinating with their shortened vowels, thus DL > DS agreeing with NT and YN
forms. The situation in the four Yang Zhuang varieties indicates that tonal behaviors
can be preserved even though the vowels are shortened, on the other hand they can
also be changed along with the vowel-length shortening. The different two groups of
the terms in the two Jingxi varieties also show that vowel-length shortening may be
progressing in the modern Thai varieties. This is crucial to the hypothesis of vowel
shortening in the Tai history.
In short, in contrast to secondary tonal splits conditioned by different groups of
initials to be vertical on both smooth and checked syllables (ABCD), secondary tonal
splits can be conditioned by vocalic length as horizontal only on checked syllables
181
(D). Tonal splits conditioned by vocalic length often present as that the original D
tone changes from the original behavior coordinating with tone B, to change to be
coordinating with tone C or A. Vowel-length alternation normally presents as long
vowel-length and/or its tonal behaviors preserved in SWT and CT, but vowel
shortening with tonal behavior changes in NT and YN. The phenomenon of vowellength alternation suggests that an extra column DL/S should be added into the
proposed ideal Tai tone box when being applied to all Tai varieties (cf. Chapter 6).
In this chapter, I have proposed the diachronic processes of tonal development in
Tai languages as briefly summarized as follows.
1) Tonogenesis: Four tonal categories arose after the dropping or weakening of the
syllable finals *-h and *-ʔ in the earlier stage of PT, for keeping the semantic
distinction among syllables with earlier plain voice (> *A), *-h (> *B) and *-ʔ
(> C*) finals respectively.
2) Primary tonal splits: Within the voiceless continuants, voiceless stops, glottal
sounds, voiced stops and voiced continuants in PT, the mergers between
voiceless and voiced continuants caused PT tones to fall into two series of
primary registers – original high tones (odd tones) and original low tones (even
tones). This process displayed original voiceless continuants, voiceless stops, and
glottal sounds on the high register, as well as original voiced continuants and
voiced stops on the low register. An eight-tone system, namely Tones A1, A2, B1,
B2, C1, C2, D1, and D2 was established.
3) Vertical secondary tonal splits: After ST split from PT, a series of “voiceless
plain stop + *r-” cluster in PT gave rise to aspirated sounds (AMO-SY) in both
ST and YN as an areal trait. VASO-1/2 then merged into this series in ST via a
breathy process to condition odd tones, in contrast in NT and YN VASO-1/2 had
earlier changed to voiced stops to condition even tones. VASO-2/1 also went to
opposite directions of tonal behaviors between ST and NT/YN. After that, ST
splitting from CT due to Tai migration from the Tai homeland to MSEA. ASO-CY
then developed to merge into AMO-SY in both CT and YN but excluding SWT. At
the same time, ASO-YN independently developed to merge into AMO-SY in YN
excluding CT. In both CT and YN, aspirated sounds separating from voiceless
continuant condition possible secondary tonal split. Glottalized stops commonly
condition secondary tonal splits in all Tai groups. However, it is found in tones C
182
and/or B but rarely A in NT, in contrast, it is found in tones A and/or B in ST.
Secondary tonal splits conditioned by unaspirated stops are only found in ST as
they are treated as the byproduct of secondary tonal splits conditioned by
glottalized sounds. In addition, VASO-GB induced voicing alternation between
the vast majority of NT and a small cluster of Guibei Zhuang of NT (including
Suogan and Huanjiang).
4) From three-way to two-way register: Derived tones splitting from the original
high register cause three-way splits in some CT/SWT varieties. But in more Tai
varieties, the bearing capacity of the tone numbers in a language often cause the
derived tone to merge into the original low tone to make two-way splits always
be the mainstream.
5) Horizontal secondary tonal splits: The tone *D on checked syllable secondarily
split due to different vocalic vowel length. Those D tones do not agree with the
tonal behavior as Tone*B should be treated as secondary development.
Going back to the first hypothesis of this thesis, the diachronic scheme of Tai tonal
development is outlined. The proposed three groups of proto-initials (voicelessvoiced-breathy) which conditioned primary tonal splits are attested to be of a
misapprehensive idea, since breathy sounds were even later development which
merged into aspiration only in ST (CT/SWT). However, the source of VASO-1/2
which contains a specific group of sesquisyllabic onsets must still be considered to
be on the PT level. The significance of it is still on the primary level, since two
different developing tracks between ST and CT/YN has been destined by it before
any types of secondary tonal splits occurred. The idea that splits conditioned by
other pharyngeal features, such as aspiration and pre-glottalization represent stages
of secondary development is verified to be attested. In addition, some other
diachronic stages and detailed issues involving Tai tonal development make the
scheme fuller and more accurate.
183
Tai classification based on tonology
This chapter suggests a scheme of Tai classification based on the diachronic
hierarchies of Tai tonal development. §5.1 provides a tonological criterion of Tai
classification. §5.2 suggests a Tai primary classification that NT and YN together
form a sub-branch of which the ST sub-branch (containing Li’s CT and SWT) is a
sister. §5.3 indicates that some of the secondary tonal splits can be applied to Tai
secondary classifications, but others can only be treated as criteria of Tai
classification on the levels of dialects or even varieties in the same dialect. §5.4 and
its sub-sections provide supplementary evidence from initial behaviors, vocalic
features, and exclusive lexical items to support the viewpoints of Tai classifications
based on primary and secondary tonal splits. §5.5 briefly summarizes the Tai
classification based on tonology.
Tai classification has been briefly discussed in §1.1.2, §2.1.3, and §2.2.4. Within the
three sub-sections, §2.2.4 especially focuses on the previous studies grouping Tai
varieties based on tonology. Tai classification based on tonology has never been
effectively suggested to subgroup Tai languages by itself, but has to be used for
accounting for subgrouping Tai varieties together with other criteria. For example,
in one of Li’s three criteria for grouping his NT, CT, and SWT, he points out that
certain lexical items show voicing alternation reflected in their tones, such as the
word ‘to be’ indicating an original voiced initial *b- in CT and NT, but an original
voiceless initial *p- in SWT (Li 1977: xiii, also cf. §2.1.3). Chamberlain’s Tai
classification suggested by the tonal splitting criterion together with the low register
initial p- vs. ph- criterion (cf. 2.2.4) is used for arguing on grouping the sub-types of
SWT as well as CT, but it seems that it is not able to be used for dividing the
primary Tai groups like NT, CT and SWT. Although Edmondson (1994: 164) points
out that tonal flip-flop is a characteristic of SWT (cf. 2.2.4), we have seen that there
is no tonal flip-flop in some SWT varieties such as Tai Lue (L6) and Quan Son Tai
(L7). On the other hand, in certain CT and NT varieties investigated here tonal flip-
184
flop does occur. These are Tuoxin Pyang Zhuang (L15) and Huashan Min Zhuang
(L16) in CT, as well as Longsang (L31), Dongling (L32) and Shanglin (L34) in NT (cf.
§4.1.3, §4.1.3.1, and Appendix C). Therefore, the tonal flip-flop criterion cannot be
individually used for grouping Tai varieties on any level except the variety level.
The only effective tonological criterion relating to Tai primary classification in
previous studies may be the so-called voicing alternation or the alternation of tonal
series (cf. §1.2.2, §1.2.3, §2.2.3, §2.2.4), although as previously pointed out, it has
been used for dividing Tai primary groups together with phonological and lexical
criteria. This tonal phenomenon has been commonly used to suggest Li’s NT to be
separate from his CT and SWT (or Gedney’s ST). However, the affiliation of the
varieties of YN which has the same tonal behaviors with NT on this tonal
phenomenon is still open to debate. This is because some previous studies arguing
that NT languages do not have aspirated stops rules out that varieties of YN are
considered of NT. However, just as Nicolson suggests that “other traditional
indicators of tone systems, segmental development and lexical items” can suggest
Nung An to be a NT member if these indicators can be applied to Nung An data
(Nicolson 2000: 293), YN varieties may possibly be of NT rather than CT. This will
be further discussed in the following §5.2. Before probing into the essential
discussion of Tai classification based on tonology, it is necessary for us to establish
the tonological criteria based on diachronic hierarchies as shown in Figure 15.
A
B (v, w, x…)
D (v, w-1, x-1…)
C (x, y, z…)
E (v, w-2, x-2…)
F (x-2, y, z-1…)
G (x-2, y, z-2…)
Figure 15 Speculation of shared innovations on different diachronic orders for
subgrouping languages
In spite of the different viewpoints on tonological criteria of Tai classifications, it is
very crucial that a language family must be subgrouped into different branches by
using the criterion of shared innovation (cf. §2.1.3) on diachronic orders. For
instance, in Figure 15 Group B and Group C splitting from the same proto-language
A must have developed their own innovations like ‘v’ and ‘w’ in B and ‘y’ and ‘z’ in
C, and both B and C share the same retention ‘x’. Note that these innovations are on
the primary level. As time moved on, Group B split into Groups D and E, and Group
C split into Groups F and G. The original innovations ‘v’ in B may be preserved in
185
both of the groups D and E, but ‘w’ and ‘x’ may have developed into w-1 and x-1 as
well as w-2 and x-2 in Group D and Group E respectively. On the other hand, the
original ‘y’ in C may be preserved in both of the groups F and G, but original ‘z’ may
have developed into z-1 and z-2 in Group F and Group G respectively. In contrast,
original ‘x’ in C may have developed into x-2 in both the groups F and G due to the
areal trait shared with Group E. However, the shared innovation ‘x-2’ among E, F,
and G cannot be a reason to suggest E, F and G to group together to form a subgroup, because this shared innovation is secondary when being compared with the
innovations ‘v’, ‘w’, ‘y’, and ‘z’ on the primary level (in the groups B and C). From
the diachronic logic, only the innovation ‘v’ shared between D and E as well as ‘w-1’
in D and ‘w-2’ in E reflected ‘w’ in B can become evidence for proving a closer
genetic relation between D and E. Similarly, only the innovation ‘y’ shared between
F and G as well as ‘z-1’ in F and ‘z-2’ in G reflecting ‘z’ in C can suggest a closer
genetic relation between F and G. In other words, even ‘x-1’ in D is different from ‘x-
2’ in E, F and G, but both ‘x-1’ and ‘x-2’ reflect ‘x’ which is a retention (from protoA) shared by both B and C. In short, it is not logical to suggest that E is closer to F
and G by the secondary changes ‘x-2’ shared among them.
Therefore, in this thesis the tonological criterion of Tai classification is based on the
diachronic hierarchies of Tai tonal development illustrated in Chapter 4. That is, any
Tai varieties classified with each other in the same group should be considered to
share more innovations complying with the diachronic orders. The more earlier
shared innovations the closer genetic relations should be considered to have
between/among the Tai groups. Some shared innovations between some groups fail
to agree on the diachronic orders, and they may be of areal traits (or even
coincidence of independent development) rather than the evidence for proving the
closer genetic relation. With this criterion, Tai classification based on tonology
become a consistent rule to attest the grouping of Tai varieties, and other criteria
such as initial and vocalic features as well as exclusive lexical items can be treated
as its supplementary evidence, since tones have arisen and have developed
conditioned by consonants and vowels (cf. Chapter 4). I will demonstrate this
argument by applying the criterion to the Tai classifications based on the primary
tonal split and secondary tonal splits in the following §5.2 and §5.3.
186
Because tonogenesis is consistently reflected in all Tai language groups (cf. §4.1),
tonogenesis which is treated as the very first step of tonal development in Tai is
invalid to be applied to Tai classification. Similarly, since the tonal scheme A1, A2,
B1, B2, C1, C2, D1, and D2 induced during the primary split period can be reflected
in all Tai language groups like Li’s CT, SWT, and NT as well as Gedney’s NT and ST
(cf. 4.2), Tai language subdivision cannot be advanced by the primary tonal split.
However, we have reviewed and discussed a couple of times voicing alternation or
the alternation of tonal series (cf. §1.2.2, §1.2.3, §2.2.3, §2.2.4) which indicates that
VASO-1/2 cause tones to fall into the low register in NT/YN and into the high
register in CT/SWT, and on the contrary VASO-2/1 cause opposite tonal directions
in the relevant language groups (§4.3.2.1). Because the sesquisyllabic onsets
containing the source of these two series of voicing alternation must have been on
the PT level, and VASO-1/2 and VASO-2/1 resulting in the different register merger
chosen by different Tai groups must have been on the primary tonal split level, Tai
varieties can be grouped into two primary branches on this level. This suggestion
can refer to the indication in Figure 15. If B represents the common ancestor of both
NT and YN, C then represents Gedney’s ST which is the common ancestor of Li’s CT
and SWT. Subsequently, D, E, F, and G represent NT, YN, CT, and SWT respectively.
As the linguistic features within these groups, ‘v’ can represent the low register tones
conditioned by VASO-1/2 or the high register tones conditioned by VASO-2/1 in
Group B, and ‘y’ can represent the high register tones conditioned by VASO-1/2 or
the low register tones conditioned by VASO-2/1 in Group C. This statement is built
on the primary tonal split level and rejects any diachronically later tonal innovations
(or any tonal coincidence of independent development) like ‘x-2’ (aspiration which
will be discussed in §5.3) shared between YN (E) and CT (F)/SWT (G) to suggest
another contradictory grouping, namely YN-CT or YN-CT-SWT constitute a subgroup of Tai. In short, according to the primary tonal split pattern involving VASO1/2 and VASO-2/1, Tai primary classification should be a two-term system which
suggests Li’s CT and SWT to form a primary sub-group ST agreeing with Gedney
(1989a: 231), and suggests Li’s NT and the debatable YN to constitute the other
primary sub-group of Tai.
It is noteworthy that both VASO-1/2 and VASO-2/1 are put under secondary tonal
splits in Chapter 4. It is because the diachronic stage of the merger from VASO-1/2
187
to aspirated sounds in ST must have been secondary since aspirated sounds arose
secondarily on post-PT level. However, the VASO-1/2 and VASO-2/1 themselves
must have been differentiated in PNT and PST on the primary sub-branch level,
otherwise their directions of merger would not be different like in the current
situation. There is another similar series of secondary tonal splits having been
differentiated on primary PNT and PST level, and it also can indicate the two-term
system on this primary Tai sub-branch level. It is the so-called “vowel-length
alternation” involving secondary tonal splits between NT-YN and ST (cf. §4.3.3).
Although the horizontal tonal splits conditioned by this “vowel-length alternation”
are actually of secondary level, the earlier forms of this series of “vowel-length
alternation” are neatly reflected as long in ST but as short in NT-YN (cf. §4.3.3).
That is, the vocalic differentiation of the earlier forms of “vowel-length alternation”
must have processed on the primary PNT and PST level.
As a result, YN varieties such as Cao Lan (cf. Gregerson & Edmondson 1998) and
Nung An (cf. Nicolson 2000; Edmondson 2002), as well as Yongning Baiji (L25),
Nanning Shuangding (L26), Long’an Xiaolin (L27), Jingxi Daqiu Nung An (L28) and
Yongning Xialeng (L29) investigated in this thesis should be all viewed as members
of the NT subgroup. This statement simply declares to solve the puzzle of Cao Lan
(Gregerson & Edmondson 1998), and to clarify that Nung An is neither an aberrant
son of CT nor a CT stepchild from NT quizzed by Nicolson (2000), but a virtual NT
member. In fact, Edmondson (2002) has pointed out that Nung An located in Cao
Bang Province of North Vietnam has brought “NT features with them when they
immigrated from further north in an area located on the border between NT and CT
areas” around Long’an of Guangxi (Edmondson 2002: 60-61). In this thesis I suggest
to follow the term YN to include these virtual NT varieties, instead of directly calling
them NT varieties because of the following three reasons. (1) In the Sinitic circles
linguists have been used to using YN (Yongnan Zhuang) which literally means “the
Zhuang in the areas south to Yongjiang River” to include these Tai varieties with
debatable features, and it is not necessary to break this tradition. (2) YN varieties
geographically distribute in the southeastern part of the Zhuang area (cf. Appendix
D), connected to the south and to the east of the usual line of division between NT
and CT languages, and so it is misleading to use the term “northern” for these
varieties. (3) The most crucial reason is that these virtual NT varieties are close to
each other by their own shared innovations like abundant aspirations and their
conditioning secondary tonal splits (cf. §5.3) which separate them from the common
NT varieties.
188
Based upon the naming scheme above, the aforementioned terms PNT and PST (cf.
1.2.3) are used to name the proto-languages of NT-YN and ST (CT-SWT)
respectively. That is, if D, E, F, and G in Figure 15 represent NT, YN, CT, and SWT
respectively, B and C on the primary level represent PNT (the common ancestor of
NT and YN) and PST (C the common ancestor of CT and SWT) respectively. In some
sense, this thesis agrees on the two-term system which is suggested by Haudricourt
(1956) and divides PT first into Dioi or ʔyai (corresponding to NT) vs. Tai proper on
the primary level, because the autonym “Tai” (<*dajA) commonly found in SWT and
CT varieties has not been found in languages developing from PNT, and the
autonym “Yay” (<*ˀjajC) (or Dioi in the French/Vietnamese circles) also has not
been found in languages developing from PST.
As discussed in §4.3 and its sub-sections, secondary tonal splits are found to be
conditioned by glottalized sounds, aspirated sounds (including their non-aspirated
variants like ɕ- (<tɕʰ- <kʰj- < *kr-), r- (< ʰr- < *kr-), or l- (< ʰl- < ʰr- < *kr-)),
and unaspirated stops in different Tai varieties. Different types of secondary tonal
splits are determined to be able to become indicators for subgrouping Tai languages
nor not as below.
A secondary split conditioned by glottalized sounds is normally found in the B and C
tones in NT varieties, but in the A and B tones in ST (cf. §4.3.2.2). However, this
cannot be a valid criterion to determine the affiliation of a Tai variety because
certain exceptions are found. For example, this split is found to exist in the A
column in Po-ai of NT (cf. §4.3.2.2), and in the C column in Isan (L3) and in Phuan
(cf. Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun 2010: 76) of SWT. Besides, secondary split
conditioned by glottal sounds in the C column is normally found in the western part
of NT areas (including Bouyei areas), but is not found in the eastern part of NT areas
and the areas of YN which has been grouped with NT on the primary level. The
distribution indicates an areal trait more than a genetic feature. Therefore, it is not
tenable to use secondary split conditioned by glottalized sounds to determine the Tai
affiliation on primary level.
A secondary split conditioned by unaspirated stops is only found in Gedney’s ST
(including CT and SWT) varieties, such as Bangkok Thai (L1), Songkhla Southern
Thai (L2), Khon Kaen Isan (L3), Leiping Zuojiang Zhuang (L22) and Baoxu Zuojiang
189
Zhuang (L23) (cf. §4.3.1). It seems to be an effective indicator for separating ST
from NT because NT varieties have not been reported to have this split. However,
the determination must be careful because unaspirated stops never condition tonal
splits by itself but always together with glottalized initials. A secondary tonal split
conditioned by unaspirated sounds should be a byproduct of those conditioned by
glottal sounds (cf. §4.3.1 and §4.3.2.2). In many SWT varieties and most CT
varieties, unaspirated stops do not condition secondary tonal splits at all. Therefore,
it is appropriate to treat it as a dialectal feature on variety level.
The last type of secondary tonal split is conditioned by aspirated sounds. In §4.3.2.3
the complicated situations of aspirated sounds in Tai languages are discussed. Note
that aspiration developing from VASO-1/2 in ST must be ignored here since it has
been proved to be on the primary level to separate ST and NT-YN from each other.
Because we have emphasized that aspirated sounds in Tai languages must be
secondary on post-PT level (§4.2.1), secondary tonal splits conditioned by these
aspirated sounds must be a subordinate indicator for subgrouping Tai languages
when being compared with VASO-1/2. Again taking symbols in Figure 15 to
illustrate the diachronic orders, ‘x-1’ of Group D can refer to the lack of aspirated
sounds in NT, and ‘x-2’ shared in E, F, and G can refer to the robust aspiration
shared in YN, CT, and SWT due to their close neighboring distribution in history.
Thus, that the lack of aspirated sounds in NT and the robust aspirations in YN, CT
and SWT cannot prove YN to be closer to CT/SWT and to be apart from NT, since ‘x1’ and ‘x-2’ on the secondary level cannot be used to refute the ‘v’ and ‘y’ on the
primary level.
However, the real situation of aspirated sounds in Tai languages are much more
complicated than in Figure 15 because the aspirated sounds develop from different
sources in PT, and that it is aspirated in one group does not mean that it must also
be aspirated in another group. Therefore, the segmental development involving
aspirations should be separated into different series to be discussed as to whether
they can be a valid indicator for Tai classification and on which level. These series
are AMO-SY, ASO-SY, AMO-ST, ASO-ST, ASO-CY, and ASO-YN (cf. §4.3.2.3) except
VASO-1/2. In Table 56 we have summarized different directions of merger involving
aspirations in different modern Tai groups.
Aspirations developing from AMO-SY and ASO-SY are shared between YN and ST
(CT-SWT), and this is very possibly of areal traits between YN and ST before the
ancestors of SWT migrated from the Tai homeland. This determination is very
190
crucial because aspirations developing from AMO-SY and ASO-SY cannot challenge
VASO-1/2 on the primary sub-group level to suggest YN-ST to compose a sub-group
under Tai. Tonal splits conditioned by these two series are suggested to be an
indicator not more than dialectal or variant level when being applied to ST (CTSWT), but to be the indicator of the YN sub-group when being applied to NT-YN.
Aspirations developing from AMO-ST and ASO-ST are shared between CT and SWT,
on the other hand YN and NT both reflect unaspirated sounds for these two series,
agreeing with the suggestion of VASO-1/2 on the primary level. This phenomenon
on the contrary indicates that secondary tonal splits conditioned by these two series
should be lifted to be with those conditioned by VASO-1/2, VASO-2/1 and “vowel-
length alternation” on the primary level. Nevertheless, they will be put in further
discussion in the future because the examples of these two series are still fewer than
other series.
The remaining two series ASO-CY and ASO-YN are important indicators on
secondary sub-group level. Aspirations developing from ASO-CY are only found in
CT and YN, but are never found in SWT (as well as NT). This indicates that this
series of sesquisyllabic onsets must have been preserved in PNT and PST, and must
have differentiated during the period that YN separating from NT-YN and SWT
separating from ST. The differentiation should be that it is simply merged into
unaspirated stops in SWT and NT due to individual development (they do not seem
to have shared areal features on post-PT level), but merged into aspirated sounds in
CT and YN as an areal trait after the migration of SWT to MSEA. Thus, ASO-CY as
well as secondary tonal splits conditioned by it should be treated as important
indicators of dividing SWT from CT, and YN from NT on secondary sub-group level.
The series ASO-YN only develops into aspirated in YN but into unaspirated stops in
SWT, CT, and NT. This individual development suggests that ASO-CY as well as
secondary tonal splits conditioned by it be treated as the exclusive indicators of YN
on secondary sub-group level.
To summarize, secondary tonal splits as well as their conditioning initials can be
indicators of grouping Tai languages on secondary sub-group, dialectal and variant
levels according to different groups, languages, and dialects. Comparing with Tai
classification based on the primary tonal split which consistently divides the neat
two term system NT-YN and ST, Tai classification based on secondary tonal splits are
dispersive, because secondary tonal splits are not always found in all varieties in a
specific group, a language, and even a dialect (cf. tonal patterns of different Yang
191
Zhuang varieties in Table 36 in §4.1.3.3.2 and Tables 64 and 65 in §4.3.3).
However, some specific series of aspirated sounds involving possible tonal splits can
be very important indicators to separate CT from SWT (by ASO-CY), as well as YN
from NT (by AMO-SY, ASO-SY, and ASO-YN) on secondary sub-group level. For
modern Tai languages, neither Li’s tripartite division (NT, CT, and SWT) (Li 1977)
nor Gedney’s two plus one taxonomy (CT/SWT + NT) can reasonably capture all
the initials and vowels involving tonal distinct within them. A more possible “two
plus two taxonomy” of modern Tai division, namely NT/YN + CT/SWT is led out as
a result of the discussion of secondary tonal splits and their conditioning initials.
“Two term system” of primary Tai sub-groups and “two plus two taxonomy” of
modern Tai division based on tonology can be supported by the non-tonal evidence
from initial and vocalic behaviors as well as exclusive lexical items as in the
following three sub-sections.
Some specific series of segments especially the so-called single r- segment in modern
NT Tai languages regularly present a reflex of *r- as the distinct feature separating
them from CT-SWT.
The single r- segment, which is often treated as a specific initial segment occurring
with both even and odd numeral tones in NT languages to distinguish NZ/Bouyei
(NT) from SZ/Tay-Nung (CT) in the Sinitic circles, is given to support the Tai
division based on tonology. The segment r- includes the merger or preservation of
some proto-initials listed by Gregerson and Edmondson (1998: 158) as *r-, *l- and
Li’s proto-clusters *dl-, *hr-, *nl/r-, *tʰr- in modern Tai languages. Traditionally this
r- segment is very often used to determine whether a Tai language belongs to NT,
because all Li’s proto-initials listed above have merged into this single r- or its
variants, like *ɣ- in Wuming Shuangqiao (L30) or *ð- in Giay (cf. Gregerson &
Edmondson 1998).
For example, as shown in Table 68, Giay, Cao Lan, Debao, and Bangkok Thai
represent NT, YN, CT, and SWT respectively. The seven proto-initials *dl-, *dr-, *hr-,
*hw-, *nl/r-, *tʰr- and *kʰr- have all merged to the r- segment (which presents as ðdeveloping from *r-) in Giay (NT). Five of them (*dl-, *dr-, *hr-, *hw-, and *kʰr-)
192
have merged to the r- segment (which presents as l- developing from *r-) but two
(*nl/r- and *tʰr-) are differently reflected in Cao Lan (YN), showing more similarities
with Giay (NT) and less similarities with Debao (CT) and Bangkok Thai (SWT). Only
*dr- goes with *r- in Thai, showing fewer similarities with Giay (NT) and Cao Lan
(YN). No other initials merge into ɹ- except *r- → ɹ- in Debao, showing the fewest
similarities with Giay (NT) and Cao Lan (YN), but the highest similarities with
Bangkok Thai (SWT).
Table 68 Different initial behaviors of the single r- segment among NT, YN, CT
and SWT59
Proto-initial
(Li 1977)
Gloss
NT
YN
CT
SWT
Giay
Cao Lan
Debao
Bangkok Thai
*r-
‘house’
ða:nA2
la:nA2
ɹu:nA2
rɯənA2
*dl-
‘wind’
ðumA2
lomA2
lamA2
lomA2
*dr-
‘boat’
ðuaA2
lu:A2
ly: A2
rɯəA2
*hr-
‘to bark’
ðawB1
lauB1
hawB1
hawB1
*hw-
‘comb’
ðojA1
lojA1
ʋejA1
wi:A1
*nl/r-
‘water’
ðamC2
nom4C
namC2
na:mC2
*tʰr-
‘to carry’
ða:pDL1
tʰa:pDL1
tʰa:pDL1
ha:pDL1
*kʰr-
‘to find’
ða:A1
la:A1
kʰja:A1
ha:A1
Obviously, in this case Giay presents a most typical NT feature, and Debao and
Bangkok Thai presents typical ST feature, while Cao Lan goes with NT rather than
ST. However, Cao Lan still keeps its own characteristics due to the two different sets
which do not merge into the r- segment. Debao and Bangkok Thai also disagree with
each other on the set *dr-. Initial behaviors clearly support the following two points.
(1) NT and YN on one hand and CT and SWT on the other hand should be grouped
into two primary sub-groups NT-YN and ST under Tai. (2) To some extent, YN and
NT differ from each other, and CT also differs from SWT on secondary sub-group
level under the primary groups NT-YN and ST respectively.
The irregular correspondence of vowels between NT-YN and ST is not limited to the
so-called “vowel-length alternation” in checked syllables (cf. §4.3.3 and §5.3), but is
59
Data from Giay and Cao Lan are adapted from Gregerson & Edmondson (1998: 158-159), and
data from Debao and Bangkok Thai are collected by me.
193
also found in other aspects. For example, in Lungchow (CT) both ŋu:A2 ‘snake’ and
mu:A1 ‘pig’ reflect to the same proto-vowel *u:, and so do other ST (both CT and
SWT). However, in NT such as Giay, ŋɯ:A2 ‘snake’ and mu:A1 ‘pig’ reflect two
different proto-vowels like *ɯ: and *u:. We also see the data of Nung An, Cao Lan,
and other varieties of YN go with NT in this case. More examples are summarized to
be compared as in Table 69, in which Yay, Long’an, Debao Urban, and Bangkok Thai
represent NT, YN, CT, and SWT respectively.
Table 69 Vowels involving Gedney’s Puzzles60
NT
Proto-vowel
(Pittayawat
Pittayaporn
Gloss
Yay
2009)
YN
CT
SWT
Long’an
Debao
Bangkok
(L27)
Urban (L8)
Thai (L1)
*-i:
‘year’
pi:A1
pɯjA1
pejA1
pi:A1
*-ɯ:
‘book’
θɯ:A1
ɬɯjA1
ɬøyA1
(naŋA1) sɯ:A1
*-u:
‘pig’
mu:A1
mowA1
mowA1
mu:A1
*-aj
‘egg’
rajB1
ʰlajB1
kʰjajB1
kʰajB1
*-aɰ
‘leaf’
baɰA1
majA1
ˀbɔ:jA1
bajA1
*-aw
‘old’
kawB1
kawB1
kawB1
kawB1
*-ɤj
‘fire’
fi:A2 (>*i:)
fɯjA2 (>*i:)
fajA2 (>*aj)
fajA2 (>*aj)
*-ɤɰ
‘breath’
sɯ:A1 (>*ɯ:)
ɕɯjA1 (>*ɯ:)
tɕɔ:jA1 (>*aɰ)
tɕajA1 (>*aɰ)
*-ɤw
‘empty’
pju:B1 (>*u:)
plowB1 (>*u:)
pjawB1 (>*aw)
pla:wB1 (>*aw)
*-ɯj
‘long’
rajA2 (>*aj)
ʰlajA2 (>*aj)
rejA2 (>*i:)
ri:A2 (>*i:)
*-ɯw
‘crab’
pawA1 (>*aw) pawA1 (>*aw) powA1 (>*u:)
pu:A1 (>*u:)
In this table, the first six sets refer to those items unequivocally reflecting PT vowels
*-i:, *-ɯ:, *-u:, *-aj, *-aɰ, and *-aw, and the later five sets (in shaded) refer to those
different reflexes of vowels in different Tai groups. The irregular vowel
correspondence in this five sets among Tai groups is called “Gedney’s Puzzle” and
has been discussed in detail by Gedney (1972). Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 198208) has cleared “Gedney’s Puzzle” up and has given a solution in the proto-vowels
reconstructed (in shaded). In his new reconstructions *-ɤj, *-ɤɰ, *-ɤw, *-ɯj, and
*-ɯw developed to merge into different PT vowels (the first six sets) in different
language groups. Again, Long’an (YN) goes with Yay (NT) on one hand, Debao (CT)
This chart is summarized from the solution for Gedney’s Puzzle (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009:
198-208). Data from Yay are adapted from Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 199-202). Data from
Long’an are from Zhang et al. (1999: 602, 605, 612, 622, 628, 683, 695, 769, 784). Data from Debao
and Thai are collected by me.
60
194
goes with Thai (SWT) on the other hand, to support the two-term (NT-YN + CTSWT) system of the primary Tai division.
Just as what Pittayawat Pittayaporn points out that YN varieties like Qinzhou,
Yongnan, Fusui, and Shangsi merged PT *-ɯ:# with *-i:# reflexed by their own
modern forms -uj, -ej, -ɯj, and -o:y respectively (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 216),
YN varieties have their own shared innovations in vocalic behaviors which separate
them from the common NT varieties. CT varieties such as Yang Zhuang varieties of
Debao (L8-10) and Jingxi (L11-13), as well as Debao Nalong Myang Zhuang (L14)
and Tiandeng Xiangdu (L17) also diphthongize those original high vowels *-i:#, *ɯ:#, and *-u:# in open rimes into -ej, -øy (or >ej)/-əɰ, and -ow respectively. These
phenomena are not found in SWT. All these also confirm the modern Tai “two plus
two taxonomy” division based on tonology.
Languages developing from the same root should share numerous cognate items.
However, some lexical items found in some daughter languages are not found in
other daughter languages due to lexical shifts, semantic changes and/or aberrant
development. This kind of lexical items may become diagnostic indicators to divide
daughter languages into different groups. Although Li (1977: xiv) points out that the
risks of using lexical items to classify dialects is that words often wander across
dialects line chiefly in the border dialects, the exclusive items still can be treated as
effective if a large numbers of lexical items in Tai varieties from the vast Tai areas
can be concluded. Edmondson ( 1994: 152-153) cites from Zhuangyu Jianzhi to
provide some of these diagnostic items separating NZ and SZ as in Table 70.
Table 70 Some diagnostic items of NZ and SZ division (adapted from
Edmondson 1994: 152-153)61
Gloss
Northern
Southern
‘sky’
bɯn¹
fa:⁴
na:m6
tum1
‘cow’
ɕɯ:2
mo:2
‘butterfly’
buŋ5 ba:3
kap7 fɯ:4
‘dry ground’
‘tiger’
kuk⁷
ɬɯ:¹
Edmondson’s original data should be wrong in the following items. The initial of ŋa:m6 ‘dry
ground’, the initial of ŋo:5 maj4 ‘bamboo shoot’, and the rime coda of pa:ŋ6 ‘flax’ are modified as in
Table 70.
61
195
Gloss
Northern
Southern
‘dragonfly’
pi:ŋ2 pej6
fi:4
‘wing’
pik⁷
fɯ:t⁸
‘black’
lap̞⁷
kaw1
‘horn’
‘bamboo shoot’
‘flax’
‘head’
‘cloth’
dam¹
ko:k7
ɣa:ŋ2
no:5 maj4
da:j3
pa:n5
kjaw³
hu:¹/baw³
paŋ
pʰa:j3
2
‘clothing’
‘black’
‘blanket’
pu:6
ɬɯ:3
lap7
dam1
teŋ2
fa:2
Similarly, Zhang and colleagues (1999) have also provided other testing items in
Table 7162.
Table 71 Some diagnostic lexical items of ST and NT-YN (adapted from Zhang
et al. 1999: 9-10, also cf. Table 15 in §2.1.3)
ST
Exclusive
NT-YN
SWT
items
CT
YN
NT
Long’an
Fusui
mɔ:n²
mɯn2
lo:n2
du:n¹
du:n¹
pa:n⁵
pa:n⁵
na:j3
na:j3
da:i³
da:i³
sɯə³
ɬɯ:³
θy:³
po:6
pu:6
pu:⁶
pɯə⁶
huə¹
huə¹
hu:¹
tʰu:¹
ʰlaw3
law3
kjaw³
tɕaw³
‘above’63
nɯə¹
nɯə¹
nɯ:¹
ny:¹
kɯn2
nɯ:1
kɯn²
kɯn²
‘below’
ta:j3
taj3
taɰ3
tɔ:j3
la:3
tɯj3
la:3
la:3
‘to do’
--
het⁷
hit⁷
hat⁷
kuk8
kuk10
kwak⁸
kuə⁶
‘ghost’
pʰi:¹
pʰi:¹
phi:¹
phej¹
ma:ŋ2
ma:ŋ2
fa:ŋ²
fa:ŋ²
‘he/she’
man²
--
min²
te:¹
te:5
ti:1'
te:¹
te:¹
Thai
Lao
Longzhou Jingxi
‘rounded’
mon²
mon²
mon²
‘ramie’
pa:n⁵
pa:n⁵
‘cloth’
sɯə³
‘head’
Liujiang Bouyei
The original chart in Zhang et al. (1999: 9-10) does not provide the data of Long’an and Fusui.
Data from these two YN varieties are adapted from the Appendix 2 in Zhang et al. (1999: 603, 632,
639, 664, 687, 739, 770, 800).
63
The cognate for ‘above’ in CT means ‘north’ in Thai and Lao.
62
196
Just like what is shown in Edmondson’s wordlist above, in Table 71 YN varieties
Long’an and Fusui clearly go together with NT varieties Liujiang and Bouyei on most
lexical items (in shaded). Only very rare testing items in Long’an and Fusui are of ST
terms which are probably of areal traits due to their shared areas of distribution. On
the other hand, CT and SWT varieties again go together with each other when being
applied by these exclusive lexical items, except very few NT items probably due to
the areal traits like te:1 ‘he/she’ in Jingxi64.
Lexical items can be easily borrowed from neighboring languages, as in Li’s warning
of the risk of using lexical items, and since some YN varieties like Cao Lan and Nung
An located in the areas dominated by the majority CT groups in Southwestern
Guangxi and Northern Vietnam, they may borrow more ST diagnostic lexical items
from these typical Central Tai languages. This does lead a risk that Cao Lan and
Nung An may use more ST diagnostic lexical items than other YN languages. As one
of the puzzles in Cao Lan, the mix of NT and ST diagnostic lexical items has been
discussed by Gregerson and Edmondson (1998: 161).
In conclusion, YN varieties should not be treated as a language in that is “half way”
between NT and CT because they do show more NT features rather than CT features
even in lexical items. After determining the exclusive lexical items, the primary NTYN and ST division based on tonology should be a reasonable establishment.
In conclusion, different from the hypothesis that “two plus one taxonomy” of Tai
classification is more reasonable than the Tripartite Division at least in the early
stage of Tai languages in Chapter 1, this thesis suggests two levels of Tai division as
follows. First, a two-term system, NT-YN (<PNT) and ST (<PST), based on the
primary tonal splits is suggested by tonal features of VASO-1/2, VASO-2/1, and
horizontal tonal splits conditioned by “vowel-length alternation”. It agrees on the
two-term system which is suggested by Haudricourt (1956) and divides PT first into
Dioi or ʔyai vs. Tai proper. Second, this thesis further suggests a new “Two plus two”
(NT/YN + CT/SWT) taxonomy of modern Tai division based on secondary tonal
splits, namely ASO-CY separating CT from SWT, as well as AMO-SY, ASO-SY, and
ASO-YN together separating YN from NT on secondary sub-group level. In other
This item may be of OC or MC loan, cf. Mandarin tʰaA ‘he/she’. Some other OC loans in Tai also
lost the original aspiration in OC since there might be no aspiration in PT during the borrowing period,
cf. Bangkok Thai t etDS1 ‘seven’ and Debao Yang Zhuang t atDS1 ‘seven’ vs. Cantonese tʃʰatD1 ‘seven’
and Mandarin t ʰi:A (<D) ‘seven’.
64
197
words, it proposes that Li’s CT and SWT can be divided from each other under the
ST sub-group, and NT and YN differ from one another under the NT-YN sub-group.
Finally, Tai classification based on tonology is diachronically and genealogically
illustrated as in the following Figure 16 and Figure 17 respectively.
PT
PNT
NT
PST
YN
CT
SWT
Figure 16 Tai diachronic division based on tonology
Tai
NT-YN
NT
ST
YN
CT
SWT
Figure 17 Tai family tree on the primary and secondary levels
198
A new perspective of Tai tone box
This chapter suggests a new opinion on Tai tone box based on the hypotheses of
diachronic tonal development in Tai. According to the diachronic process of Tai
tonal development in different Tai groups, a revised tone box which must reflect
diachronic sequences of Tai tonal splits is supposed to be more applicable for
analyzing maximal Tai varieties except those situations involving aberrant
development. However, it is not necessary to apply a multi-purpose Tai tone box to
all the Tai languages according to the actuality that there are not all types of tonal
splits in a specific Tai group. Therefore, as efficient research tools different tone
boxes are suggested to different diachronic periods and different Tai sub-groups. The
criterion of designating a Tai tone box will be established in §6.1. §6.2 provides a
Tai tone box which indicates the splits from PT to PNT and PST. §6.3 suggests three
Tai tone boxes which can be applied to the development from PST to SWT and CT
respectively. §6.4 suggests two Tai tone boxes which can be applied to the modern
varieties of NT and YN respectively. On the basis of these sections, §6.5 offers an
integrated Tai tone box which is supposed to be able to capture all tonal distinctions
in any Tai varieties. §6.6 points out the limitation of Tai tone boxes. §6.7 briefly
summarizes the method of Tai tone boxes.
When comparing Li’s primary tonal split pattern due to voicing mergers (Table 1),
Gedney’s tone box (Table 5), and the revised tone box suggested by Liao and Shen
(Table 7), the carrying capacity of capturing the tonal distinctions can be applied to
more and more Tai varieties (cf. §1.2.1, §1.2.2). It is obviously observed that Tai
varieties preserving the primary tonal split pattern can apply with Li’s simple tonal
split pattern in Table 1. These languages include the following Tai varieties
investigated in this thesis: Kho Lam Tai Yai (L5), Rong Maet Tai Lue (L6), and Quan
Son Tai (L7) of SWT, Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang (L12), Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang
(L13), Myang Zhuang (L14), Pyang Zhuang (L15), Daxin Naling Zuojiang Zhuang
(L18), and Xiaoguangnan Nong Zhuang (L20) of CT, Baiji (L25) and Shuangding
(L26) of YN, as well as Shuangqiao Yongbei Zhuang (L30), Longsang (L31), Shanglin
199
Hongshuihe Zhuang (L34), Du’an Hongshuihe Zhuang (L35), Baipeng Liujiang
Zhuang (L37), Donglan Guibei Zhuang (L38), Xiaosanjiang Lianshan Zhuang (L40)
and Zhenning Bouyei (L42) of NT. However, some ST languages such as Bangkok
Thai (L1), Songkhla Southern Thai (L2), Khon Kaen Isan (L3), and Chiang Rai
Northern Thai (L4) of SWT, as well as Jingxi Hurun Yang Zhuang (L11), Daxin
Leiping Zuojiang Zhuang (L22), and Baoxu Zuojiang Zhuang (L23) of CT have to
apply with Gedney’s tone box for indicating their secondary tonal splits due to the
four different initial groups at time of tonal splits, namely voiceless frictions,
voiceless unaspirated stops, glottalized sounds, and voiced sounds (cf. §1.2.1). The
following CT varieties investigated in this thesis cannot be applied with Gedney’s
tone box due to a tonal split between voiceless aspirated sounds and voiceless
continuants (both included in Gedney’s voiceless frictions). These are Debao Urban
Yang Zhuang (L8), Debao Dalong (L9), and Debao L̈liu (L10), Jingxi Huashan
(L16), Jingxi Lingding (L19), Dazhai Dai Zhuang (L21), and Bac Va Nung (24).
Besides, voicing alternations because of VASO-1/2 between ST and NT-YN are not
answerable for by Gedney’s tone box. For solving these problems, the revised tone
box suggested by Liao and Shen (2012) has been provided (cf. §1.2.2).
When dealing with more details of tonal splits involving more complicated initial
and vocalic changes, none of the schemes above including Gedney’s tone box and
Liao & Shen’s revised version can capture these tonal complexities. These
complicated tonal distinctions include but are not limited to the following series of
tonal conditioning factors: (1) The specific series of sesquisyllabic onset ASO-YN
which induces aspiration only in YN varieties but induces unaspirated initials in all
other Tai varieties (cf. §4.3.2.3); (2) The specific series of sesquisyllabic onset VASOGB which causes voicing alternation between GB and all other Tai varieties (cf.
§4.3.2.4); (3) The series of vowel-length alternation conditioning the alternation of
tonal Series DL and Series DS between NT/YN and ST varieties (cf. §4.3.3). That is
to say, when dealing with YN varieties which have secondary tonal splits involving
aspirated sounds like Long’an Xiaolin (L27), Jingxi Daqiu Nung An (L28) and
Yongning Xialeng (L29), as well as Yizhou Suogan (of NT, L36) which have voicing
alternation (caused by VASO-GB) with other Tai varieties, the available Tai tonal
schemes or Tai tone box have shortcomings for capturing their particular tonal
distinctions. To fix up these kinds of tonal puzzles by a multi-purpose Tai tone box
has become one of the motivations of exploring the Tai tonal development. This is
also a hypothesis of this thesis (cf. §1.2.4).
200
From the improvement solving relevant problems in previous studies, we can
speculate that it is possible to provide a complex Tai tone box which is able to
capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties if all types of tonal splits in Tai
languages are studied. However, as we have supported the argument that PT lacked
contrasting aspirated sounds which secondarily arose to condition secondary tonal
splits in some CT and YN varieties, a horizontal row in a Tai tone box must be
reconsidered to be designated from a specific synchronic initial group at time of
tonal splits rather than from a supposed diachronic PT initial group. The crucial
point is that secondary tonal splits must have been individually conditioned at a
post-PT period in different Tai varieties. Therefore, it is inappropriate that any
added row in a Tai tone box is supposed to refer to an initial group at the PT level.
The most proper formulation should be “initial groups at time of tonal splits”
suggested by Gedney (1989[1972]). If we consider the different situations of
secondary tonal splits in different Tai sub-groups, namely NT, YN, CT, and SWT, a
multi-purpose complex Tai tone box seems to become a burdensome research tool.
For example, secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated sounds and unaspirated
stops are never found in NT varieties due to the fact that NT varieties lack
contrastive aspirated sounds and that unaspirated stops never go with glottalized
initials to condition secondary tonal splits. In the case of NT, a tone box designated
with the divisions containing the rows of aspirated sounds and unaspirated stops
appear to be unnecessary. Similarly, secondary tonal splits conditioned by aspirated
stops are never found in SWT varieties even though they have robust aspirated
stops. This fact led Gedney’s to put aspirated stops and voiceless continuants
together to constitute the row “voiceless frictions” which condition possible tonal
splits in SWT, and putting voiceless unaspirated stops and ASO-CY (which merge
into voiceless unaspirated stops in SWT) together to be another single row. Gedney’s
Tai Tone box has been proved to be suitable for SWT varieties, although it has been
proved to be not able to capture all the tonal distinctions in CT and YN. Therefore,
in practice a well-suited Tai tone box should be designated for a specific sub-group
of Tai, rather than for all Tai varieties.
For that reason, ASO-CY developing into aspirated initials in CT and YN varieties
may be designated as being a component in Voiceless Aspirated Sounds in the tone
boxes of CT and YN, but ASO-CY developing into unaspirated stops in SWT varieties
may be designated as being a constituent part in Voiceless Unaspirated Stops in the
SWT tone box. All aspirated sounds, voiceless continuants and voiceless aspirated
stops in SWT, CT, and YN should be put into a single group Voiceless Sounds in the
201
NT tone box since there is no more secondary split from it. In addition, the row
“proto-breathy/voiced aspirations & breathy sounds” added in the Tai tone revised
by Liao and Shen (cf. Table 7) or VASO-1/2 suggested in this thesis will be simply
deleted from a specific Tai sub-group tone box. This is because different Tai tone
boxes will be separately designated for SWT, CT, NT, and YN, and voicing
alternations must be ignored when talking about a single sub-group of Tai.
In brief, although a Tai tone box can be designed as a multi-purpose research tool
applied to all types of tonal splits in any Tai varieties, it may become cumbersome
because the complete splitting divisions based on the most complicated tonal split
patterns in some of the Tai varieties will not be necessary for those having simple
tonal split patterns. It is not necessary to always use a multi-purpose Tai tone box if
the research scope does not cover varieties from all sub-groups of Tai. Tai tone boxes
designated with specific pertinence to different Tai periods and different Tai subgroups may make the tonal solution easier and more convenient.
Based on the principle discussed in §6.1 that Tai tone box can be designated
according to the actuality and practicality of different diachronic Tai periods and Tai
sub-groups, the first attempt of designing a PT tone box based on the tonogenesis
hypothesis, and a Tai tone box based on the primary tonal split is firstly discussed in
this section. Li’s primary tonal split pattern (Table 1) indicates that tones were
induced by voicing mergers at post-PT period, but does not provide the phonation
types of initial groups at the PT level and post-PT level. For illustrating this, a PT
tone box is designed as in Table 72 based on the determination of PT initial
phonation types (cf. §4.2.1).
Table 72 PT tone box: from tonal precursor to tones
Syllable Finals / Phonation Types
Monosyllabic &
Sesquisyllabic Initial Series
A
Primary
Subordinate
Proto-
Continuant
voiceless
Plosive
sounds
Glottalized
Proto-voiced
Plosive
sounds
Continuant
-#
B
C
-h
-ʔ
(→ -# / -ˀ)
(→ -ˀ/ -#)
202
D
-p /-t / -k
In this PT tone box, proto-voiceless sounds are divided into three phonation types,
namely continuant, plosive, and glottalized, and proto-voiced sounds are divided
into two phonation types, namely plosive and continuant respectively. The main
purpose of this PT tone box is to indicate the tonogenesis process from the non-tonal
structure on the earlier stage of PT to the tonal structure on the later stage of PT.
Note that tones on the later period of PT have not split to the voiceless-voiced
registers since the voicing of voiceless continuants and the devoicing of voiced
plosives have not yet taken place. Therefore, the two primary initial groups
(voiceless and voiced sounds) and the five subordinate initials groups only indicate
the potential effects on the tones at the coming register splitting period on the postPT level.
At one of the specific periods of post-PT, tonal register splits were triggered by the
merger of proto-voiceless continuants and proto-voiced continuants due to the
voicing of proto-voiceless continuants (cf. §4.2.2). This process has been repeatedly
mentioned as the primary tonal split, which presented as the straightforward register
pattern inherited in about half of modern Zhuang varieties (Zhang et al. 1999: 245),
and in twenty Tai varieties within the forty-two ones investigated in this study (cf.
4.2.3). As an extended version of Li’s two tonal series, an integrated PST and PNT
tone box designed based on this primary tonal split pattern is as shown in Table 73.
In this process, PT tones have split into two series 1 and 2 according to the high and
the low registers respectively.
Table 73 Integrated PST & PNT tone box: from PT to PST (→) and PNT (→)
Monosyllabic & Sesquisyllabic Initial Series
Tonal Categories
A
B
C
D
A1
B1
C1
D1
VASO-1/2
A1/2
B1/2
C1/2
D1/2
VASO-2/1
A2/1
B2/1
C2/1
D2/1
A2
B2
C2
D2
Primary
Subordinate
Continuant
Proto-voiceless
sounds
Plosive
Glottalized
Proto-voiced
Plosive
sounds
Continuant
The most remarkable tonal phenomenon in this Tai tone box is that two series of
sesquisyllabic onsets have parted their two different trends, which induced voicing
alternations between PST and PNT in the later period, especially after aspiration
203
arose in PST (cf. §4.3.2.1). VASO-1/2 resulted to merge into voiceless sounds in PST,
but into voiced sounds in PNT, in contrast VASO-2/1 became voiced sounds in PST,
but became voiceless sounds in PNT.
After the designations of the two Tai tone boxes on the PT and the post-PT levels,
the next concern becomes the tone boxes of the single Tai sub-groups after the postPT level. In this section the tone boxes of the ST group are designed. First, the
simple PST tone box extracted from the intergrated PST & PNT tone box (cf. Table
73) is shown in Table 74.
Table 74 PST tone box
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
secondary tonal splits
A
B
C
A1
B1
A2
B2
D
DL
DS
C1
DL1
DS1
C2
DL2
DL2
Frictions
Proto-voiceless/
VASO-1/2
Unaspirated stops
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced
sounds/VASO-2/1
Plosives + Continuants
In this PST tone box, series VASO-1/2 and VASO-2/1 have been put together with
voiceless and voiced sounds respectively since voicing alternation is not necessary to
take into account without PNT varieties. The subordinate initial groups in the high
register are treated to have the potential conditionings of secondary tonal splits in
the two daughter groups of PST, namely CT and SWT. Aspiration in the high register
may have arisen on the PST level due to the uniform aspirated data shared by its
daughter languages CT and SWT. But aspiration is put together with voiceless
continuants to be under voiceless frictions due to the fact that they need not be
separate to condition different tonal behaviors which is still not found in modern
SWT languages. Actually there is even no evidence for proving that secondary tonal
splits have occurred at the PST level. Therefore, the three subordinate initial groups
in the high register are still treated as of uniform proto-voiceless sounds to condition
tones into Series 1. Because tonal splits within the low register are never found in ST
languages, it is not necessary to divide proto-voiced sounds to plosives and
continuants anymore, even though there are also different phonation types in the
low register, like unaspirated plosives (in most Tai varieties), aspirated plosive in
204
some SWT varieties including Thai (L1) and Isan (L3), as well as some CT varieties
including Lingding (L19), Baoxu (L23), and Leiping (L22), and sonorants. Therefore,
Series 2 in Tai tone boxes hereafter refers to tones conditioned by initials developing
from proto-voiced plosives or continuants at time of tonal splits.
The very first practical Tai tone box serving for a modern Tai sub-group SWT is
Gedney’s tone box (cf. Table 4), which is adapted here and is revised by filling in the
terminology in this thesis as in Table 75. It is almost identical with the PST tone box
(cf. Table 74) except the horizontal divisions of the three sub-groups of voiceless
sounds, because they have been found to condition possible secondary tonal splits in
SWT languages.
Table 75 SWT tone box (cf. Gedney 1989[1972])
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
A
B
C
Frictions
A1-F
B1-F
Unaspirated stops
A1-U
Glottal sounds
Plosives + Continuants
secondary tonal splits
D
DL
DS
C1-F
DL1-F
DS1-F
B1-U
C1-U
DL1-U
DS1-U
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
Protovoiceless (1)
Proto-voiced
sounds (2)
For indicating the situations of initial mergers in SWT before the time of tonal splits,
an ancillary box of the SWT tone box is shown as in Table 76.
Table 76 Ancillary box of the SWT tone box
Initial
Directions of particular series of initial mergers in modern SWT varieties
groups
merged in
VASO-1/2
1-F
x
VASO-
AMO-
AMO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
2/1
SY
ST
SY
CY
YN
ST
x
x
x
1-U
2
x
x
x
x
This Ancillary box can help to clarify the different results of initial mergers which
may result in different horizontal rows to condition different possible tonal splits
when comparing with other modern Tai sub-groups. For example, ASO-CY becomes
aspirated sounds in CT and YN to condition a possible tonal split together with other
205
aspirated sounds from other sources, but becomes unaspirated stops in SWT to
condition a possible tonal split together with those common unaspirated stops
developing from proto-voiceless unaspirated stops. In the column of ASO-CY in the
ancillary box of the SWT tone box, the merger into the row of 1-U (voiceless
unaspirated stops) is selected by a “x”. Under this condition, items with proto-initial
of Series ASO-CY will be put into the 1-U row in the SWT tone box. Therefore, the
word ‘eye’ is ta:A1-U in SWT even though it is tʰa:A1-A, ha:A1-A or pʰja:A1-A in CT and
tʰa:A1-A in YN. Note that the 1-G row is not included in the ancillary box, since it is
not found that any specific series of initials would regularly merge into glottalized
sounds to condition possible tonal splits in any modern Tai varieties.
Similarly, the simple CT tone box and its ancillary box are designed in Tables 77 and
78 respectively.
Table 77 CT tone box
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
A
B
C
Aspirated
A1-A
B1-A
Continuant
A1-C
Unaspirated stops
secondary tonal splits
DL
DS
C1-A
DL1-A
DS1-A
B1-C
C1-C
DL1-C
DS1-C
A1-U
B1-U
C1-U
DL1-U
DS1-U
Glottal sounds
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
Plosives + Continuants
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
Frictions
Protovoiceless (1)
Proto-voiced
sounds (2)
D
Table 78 Ancillary box of the CT tone box
Initial
groups
Directions of particular series of initial mergers in modern CT varieties
VASO-
VASO-
AMO-
AMO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
merged in
1/2
2/1
SY
ST
SY
CY
YN
ST
1-A
x
x
x
x
x
1-C
x
1-U
2
x
x
x
The most noticeable point which differs the CT tone box from the SWT tone box is
the split between aspirations and continuants (cf. §1.2.2) when being compared with
206
the SWT tone box. It means that the situation of initial mergers and tonal splits in
CT varieties are more complicated than in SWT. Another different point between the
CT tone box and the SWT tone box is the direction of initial merger of the Series
ASO-CY, which merges into aspirated initials (1-A) in CT but merges into
unaspirated stops (1-U) in SWT, as shown in the CT ancillary box in Table 78. For
example, the VASO-1/2 column in the ancillary box is selected by a “x” both in the
rows 1-A and 1-C. It means that initials developing from voicing alternation Series
1/2 in CT are merged into aspirated sounds in some items, but are merged into
continuants in other items. This can be shown in the example in Debao Dalong Yang
Zhuang (L9) in where a tonal split between 1-A (voiceless aspirated sounds) and 1-C
(voiceless continuant) in the A, B, and DL columns as in Table 79.
Table 79 Different VASO-1/2 merging directions involving different tonal splits
among Tai varieties
Gloss
Bangkok Thai
Debao Dalong
Wuming (NT)
(SWT)
(CT)
‘ear’
hu:24 (A1-F)
kʰjow31 (A1-A)
ɣɯ:31 (A2)
‘bean’
tʰuə21 (B1-F)
tʰu:331 (B1-A)
tu:33 (B2)
‘CLF for person’
pʰu:41 (C1-F)
pʰow24 (C1-A)
pow41 (C2)
‘to hit the mark’
tʰu:k21 (DL1-F)
tʰʊk33 (DL1-A)
tɯk33 (DS2)
‘to bite’
kʰop21 (DS1-F)
kʰap55 (DS1-A)
hap33 (DS2)
‘right side’
kʰwa:24 (A1-F)
θɔ:53 (A1-C)
kwa:31 (A2)
‘late in morning’
sa:j24 (A1-F)
θɔ:j53 (A1-C)
kwa:j31 (A2)
‘ten’
sip21 (DS1-F)
θəp55 (DS1-C)
ɕip33 (DS2)
‘to mince’
sap21 (DS1-F)
θap55 (DS1-C)
θap33 (DS2)
Bangkok Thai (L1), Debao Dalong (L9) and Wuming (L30) represent SWT, CT and
NT respectively for indicating voicing alternation between ST and NT. The shaded
items of Debao Dalong all have a continuant initial θ-, contrasting with the other
items all with aspirated initial stops. In contrast, although Bangkok Thai also agrees
with Debao Dalong on aspirations and continuants in most items, it is not necessary
for it to separate the 1-A and 1-C rows from 1-F because a tonal split between
aspirated and continuant sounds is not found in SWT.
207
Having the diachronic Tai tone boxes from PST to SWT and CT for reference, tone
boxes designed from PNT to NT, YN, and GB are easily understood. In this section
the tone boxes of the NT group are described. First, the simple PNT tone box
extracted from the integrated PST & PNT tone box (cf. Table 73) is shown in Table
80. In this PNT tone box, series VASO-1/2 and VASO-2/1 have also been put
together with voiceless and voiced sounds respectively since voicing alternation is
not necessary to take into account without PST as a reference substance. Different
from the PST tone box, only two sub-groups of voiceless sounds are treated to have
the potential conditionings of secondary tonal splits in the daughter groups of PNT,
namely NT and YN. This indicates there is no tonal splits between voiceless plosives
and continuants in modern NT-YN varieties, except aspiration secondarily developed
in YN probably due to areal feature shared with ST.
Table 80 PNT tone box
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
secondary tonal splits
Proto-voiceless
Plosives + Continuants
sounds/VASO-2/1
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced
sounds/VASO-1/2
Plosives + Continuants
A
B
C
A1
B1
A2
B2
D
DL
DS
C1
DL1
DS1
C2
DL2
DS2
As the direct modern successor of PNT, modern NT languages can be applied with
the simple NT tone box which is the most concise Tai tone box as shown in Table
81. Only two sub-groups of voiceless sounds are designate. Common voiceless
sounds and glottalized sounds are labeled as 1 and 1-G respectively. Note that
contrasting aspirations are not found in NT. It is almost identical with the PNT tone
box (cf. Table 80) except the thorough horizontal divisions of the two sub-groups of
voiceless sounds, because they have been found to condition possible secondary
tonal splits in NT languages. Different from the situation of SWT and CT, Series
VASO-GB is added to be a new column in the ancillary boxes (shown in Table 82)
for the NT tone box. This is because of VASO-GB which is the voicing alternation
involving the low register between the common NT varieties and some of the BG
varieties (cf. §4.3.2.4).
208
Table 81 NT tone box
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
A
B
C
Plosives + Continuants
A1
B1
Glottal sounds
A1-G
Plosives + Continuants
A2
secondary tonal splits
Proto-
D
DL
DS
C1
DL1
DS1
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
voiceless
sounds (1)
Proto-voiced
sounds (2)
Table 82 Ancillary box of the NT tone box
Initial
Directions of particular series of initial mergers in modern NT varieties
groups
merged
VASO-
VASO-
AMO-
AMO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
VASO-
in
1/2
2/1
SY
ST
SY
CY
YN
ST
GB
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
1
2
x
x
In the ancillary box of the NT tone box, VASO-GB is checked in both tonal series 1
and 2. This is because in some GB varieties like Yizhou Suogan (L36), the merging
direction of VASO-GB is opposite to the vast majorities of NT (cf. §4.3.2.4). When
carrying a fieldwork research to the NT areas, if this series of initial is checked in
tonal series 1, the Tai variety should be of GB. If it is checked in tonal series 2, the
Tai variety should be treated as a common member of NT.
Nevertheless, the ancillary boxes of the NT tone box are simplest to show the least
initial merging directions (voiceless 1 and voiced 2). In the vast majorities of NT
varieties (including most GB varieties), all specific series of initial groups merge into
voiceless sounds to condition a high register tone except VASO-1/2 which merge
into voiced sounds to condition a low register tone. In a few GB varieties, such as
Yizhou Suogan (L36) and Huanjiang Urban (cf. Zhang et al. 1999: 192), all specific
series of initial groups have the same behaviors with all the other NT varieties,
except VASO-GB which merge into voiceless sounds to condition a high register
tone.
Comparing with the NT tone box, the YN tone box and its ancillary box shown in
Tables 83 and 84 respectively is more complicated because the secondary
aspirations conditioning possible secondary tonal splits in some YN varieties.
209
Table 83 YN tone box
Tonal Categories
Subordinate initial
Primary
groups conditioning
A
B
C
A1-A
B1-A
C1-A
secondary tonal splits
Aspirated
Proto-voiceless
Unaspirated stops +
(1)
Continuants
Proto-voiced
sounds (2)
D
DL
DS
DL1-A
DS1-A
A1-U/C B1- U/C C1- U/C DL1- U/C DS1- U/C
Glottal sounds
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
Plosives + Continuants
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
Table 84 Ancillary box of the YN tone box
Initial
Directions of particular series of initial mergers in modern YN varieties
groups
merged
VASO-
VASO-
AMO-
AMO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
ASO-
VAS
in
1/2
2/1
SY
ST
SY
CY
YN
ST
O-GB
x
x
x
1-A
x
1-U
2
x
x
x
x
x
The YN tone box is similar to the CT tone box (cf. Table 77) because of the shared
aspiration which may condition possible tonal split in both YN and CT. However, YN
lacks of split between voiceless continuants and voiceless unaspirated sounds. It is
not necessary for YN varieties to separate 1-C and 1-U like in the CT tone box.
Because aspirated and unaspirated sounds are a contrasting pair, in the YN tone box
1-U is used instead of 1-C to emphasize this contrast. For YN, the most remarkable
initial merging direction is ASO-YN resulting in aspiration, and this is not found in
CT and SWT even though they both have robust aspirated sounds (cf. §4.3.2.3).
In the previous sections, we have discussed all the possibilities of designing different
tone boxes for different Tai sub-groups. However, as we have predicted that an
integrated or unified Tai tone box can capture all tonal distinctions in any Tai
varieties, it is useful when the affiliation of the Tai variety investigated is unknown.
At all events, this integrated Tai tone box and its ancillary box are shown as in
Tables 85 and 86 on the basis of the different Tai tone boxes described in the
preceding sections and the previous discussions in Chapter 4.
210
Table 85 Integrated Tai tone box
Primary
Tonal Categories
Phonation types at
initial
groups
time of tonal splits
A
B
C
Aspirated sounds
A1-A
B1-A
Continuant sounds
A1-C
Unaspirated stops
DL
DS
C1-A
DL1-A
DS1-A
B1-C
C1-C
DL1-C
DS1-C
A1-U
B1-U
C1-U
DL1-U
DS1-U
Glottal sounds
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
Plosives + Continuants
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
Protovoiceless
sounds (1)
Proto-voiced
sounds (2)
D
Table 86 Ancillary box of the integrated Tai tone box
1-A
1
U/C
VASO-GB
ASO-ST
ASO-YN
ASO-CY
ASO-SY
1-A
1-F
1-
CT
AMO-ST
SWT
AMO-SY
YN
VASO-2/1
NT
Directions of mergers in any Tai variety
VASO-1/2
Initial groups merged in
1-C
1-U
2
Merging direction of DL/S in modern Tai varieties
Vowel-length
alternation
DL
DS
DL/S
The design principle of this integrated Tai tone box is that initials at the time of
tonal splits are maximally divided into five phonation groups in modern Tai
varieties, namely voiceless aspirated sounds (1-A), voiceless continuants (1-C),
voiceless unaspirated stops (1-U), glottalized sounds (1-G), and voiced sounds (2),
because tonal split have been found to be maximally conditioned by these five
phonation groups in Tai languages, diachronically and synchronically. The
integrated Tai tone box is almost identical to the CT tone box, because the most
complete tonal split patterns have been found in some CT varieties, like Debao Yang
Zhuang varieties, Baoxu, and Bac Va Nung. The integrated Tai tone box is also
similar to the revised Tai tone box suggested by Liao and Shen (2012), but is
different from it by deleting the unnecessary rows 1-UC and 1/2 in Liao & Shen’s
211
version. The reason is that particular series of initial groups are not limited to 1-UC
(ASO-CY) or 1/2, but are at least nine groups as shown in the ancillary box of the
intergrated Tai tone box in Table 86.
The supplementary ancillary box in Table 86 is much more facilitative than nine
rows of particular series of initials and one column of vowel-length alternation
(DL/S) added in the integrated tone box. This is because only the five initial
phonation types in the main box can condition possible tonal splits in Tai due to the
research of this thesis (cf. Chapter 4). If these nine initial groups as extra-rows and
the column of vowel length alternation on checked syllable are all added, the
integrated Tai tone box will become very unwieldy. Note that “1-G” is not shown in
the ancillary box because there are no any particular series of initials to merge into
glottal sounds to condition possible tonal split in any Tai variety. Before attesting
the tonal behaviors in a specific Tai variety, the ancillary box can first be used for
figuring out the merging directions of the specific series of initial groups in that
variety, by finally reducing them to the five groups conditioning. It can also deal
with the situation of vowel-length alternation on checked syllables (DL/S) between
ST and NT-YN (cf. §4.3.3). For example, in NT varieties like Debao Longsang (L31),
the following lexical items designated into the DL column in the wordlist for this
thesis show a regular vocalic change (shortened) to be coordinate with the
counterpart DS column: nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tuk55 (DS1-UC), ‘thin bamboo stripes’,
ˀdit55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, mit33 (DS2) ‘knife’, and tək33 (DS1/2-A) ‘to hit the mark’. For
testing the vowel-length changes of these items in a specific Tai variety, this series of
items can be listed under the ancillary box to investigate. As for Longsang, these
items would be ticked in the DS column. As for most SWT varieties, these items
would be simply ticked in the DL column since they all reflect a long vowel in SWT
varieties. In some CT varieties like Debao Dalong (L9), these items would also be
ticked in the DL column even though their vowels have been shortened, because the
tones are still preserved as those in the DL column in this variety (cf. §4.3.3).
A notable benefit of the design of the ancillary box is that we may find more
particular initial and/or vocalic changes which may condition secondary tonal splits
in some undescribed Tai varieties in further studies. These potential new findings
will be added into the ancillary boxes, but the main box will not need to be modified
because it should have reached its complete function by adding the maximal rows of
tonal-conditioning phonation types which condition possible tonal splits. For
illustrating the design of the Tai tone boxes, Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (L9) of CT
is taken here as an example for application, as shown in Tables 87, 88 and 89.
212
Table 87 Integrated Tai tone box applying to Debao Dalong (cf. Table 64)
Tonal Categories
Phonation types
Primary
at time of tonal
initial groups
A
B
C
Aspirated sounds
31
332
Proto-voiceless
Continuant sounds
53
sounds (1)
Unaspirated stops
Glottal sounds
splits
Proto-voiced
Plosives +
sounds (2)
Continuants
D
DL
DS
24
33
45
454
24
45
45
53
454
24
45
45
31
332
24
33
45
31
332
213
33
21
Table 88 The merging direction of particular series in Debao Dalong
1
1-U/C
x
x
1-U
2
VASO-GB
x
x
ASO-ST
1-C
x
ASO-YN
x
ASO-CY
1-A
ASO-SY
1-F
AMO-ST
1-A
CT
SWT
AMO-SY
YN
VASO-2/1
NT
Directions of mergers in any Tai variety
VASO-1/2
Initial groups merged in
x
x
x
x
Merging direction of DL/S in modern Tai varieties
Vowel-length alternation
DL
DL/S
x
DS
The basic tonal patterns in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang can be compared with the one
applied by Liao & Shen’s revised tone box (cf. Table 64 or tonal patterns of L9 in
Appendix C). The comparison shows that the tonal patterns in the integrated Tai tone
box (Table 87) are more concise than those in Liao & Shen’s version. This is because the
two extra rows in Liao & Shen’s version, 1-UC and 1/2 which correspond to ASO-CY and
VASO-1/2 respectively in the ancillary box, are actually merged into voiceless aspirated
sounds (and partially into voiceless continuants) to condition the same tonal split
pattern, and they are redundant to be shown when the investigation is only focused on
this variety. In addition, if the investigation is expanded to the comparison with other
Tai varieties, these two extra rows are not really enough. At least another seven rows of
particular initials and one column of vowel-length alternation on checked syllable are
needed to compare with since they potentially have different merging directions into the
five initial groups of phonation types which condition possible tonal splits.
213
Table 89 Examples of merging of initials and vowels in Debao Dalong
Checklist of particular series
kʰjow31 (>A1-A) ‘ear’
θɔ:53 (>A1-C) ‘right side’
VASO-1/2
tʰu:33 (>B1-A) ‘bean’
kʰaw24 (>C1-A) ‘rice’
tʰʊk33 (>DL1-A) ‘to hit the mark’
θəp45 (>DS1-C) ‘ten’
tɕa:j31 (>A2) ‘man’
VASO-2/1
ma: 31 (>A2) ‘to come’
təŋ33 (>B2) ‘to listen’
ta:w33 (>B2) ‘to overturn’
pʰja:31 (>A1-A) ‘cliff’
kʰjaj33 (>B1-A) ‘egg’
AMO-SY
pʰja:j24 (>C1-A) ‘to walk’
pʰja:k33 (>DL1-A) ‘forehead’
pʰjak45 (>DS1-A) ‘vegetable’
kʰe:n31 (>A1-A) ‘arm’
haw33 (>B1-A) ‘to bark’
AMO-ST
kʰa:24 (>C1-A) ‘to kill’
kʰa:t33 (>DL1-A) ‘torn’
tʰat45 (>DS1-A) ‘to jump’
kʰɔ:n31 (>A1-A) ‘body air’
ASO-SY
kʰa:j31 (>A1-A) ‘to sell’
kʰjɔ:j24 (>C1-A) ‘near’
tʰak45 (>DS1-A) ‘grasshopper’
tʰa:31 (>A1-A) ‘eye’
tʰaw33 (>B1-A) ‘to hunt’
ASO-CY
kʰjɔ:j24 (>C1-A) ‘near’
tʰa:k33 (>DL1-A) ‘to bask’
tʰak45 (>DS1-A) ‘grasshopper’
tam53 (>A1-U) ‘full’
tʊn55 (>B1-U) ‘to wake up’
ASO-YN
to:ŋ24 (>C1-U) ‘to touch’
tak33 (>DS1-U) ‘to break’
tat45 (>DS1-U) ‘fart’
kʰja:m31 (>DL1-A) ‘to ask’
ASO-ST
haj24 (>C1-A) ‘to cry’
kʰjap45 (>DL1-A) ‘centipede’
mʊŋ31 (>A2) ‘hand’
kow33 (>B2) ‘pair’
VASO-GB
maj213 (>C2) ‘tree’
nam213 (>C2) ‘water’
nɔ:k21 (>DS2) ‘bird’
214
Checklist of particular series
nu:k55 (>DL1-C) ‘deaf’
DL/S
tʰo:k33 (>DL1-A) ‘thin bamboo stripes’
ˀde:t55 (>DL1-G) ‘sunshine’
mət33 (>DL2) ‘knife’
tʰʊk33 (>DL1-A) ‘to hit the mark’
The checklist under the ancillary box (Table 89) provides some of the examples of
these particular series in Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang. In the practical fieldwork
research, the checklist of main tone box can be designated together with the
consideration of ancillary box if the research covers Tai varieties from different Tai
sub-groups, namely NT, YN, CT, and SWT.
Before entering the summary section to conclude the method of Tai tone box, it is
necessary to point out the limitations of the Tai tone box. The most noticeable
limitation is that aberrant tonal development can neither be resolved in a practical
Tai sub-group tone box, nor be explained in an integrated Tai tone box which can be
able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties. This kind of aberrant tonal
development normally includes tonal changes due to areal traits, phonological
contamination, and conditioning factors change before/after the time of tonal splits.
For example, different from the merger into DS2 in other NT varieties, ɬap55 ‘to
mince’ merges its tone with the rows of DS1 in Debao Longsang (L31, NT), to
present a ST behavior. This may be of an areal trait shared with the neighboring the
Debao Yang Zhuang varieties, which are of CT.
Another example is the term ˀdaŋ45 ‘to make fire’ in Daxin Baoxu (L23, CT).
Comparing with the cognate ˀdaŋC1-G in Debao Longsang (L31, NT) and Debao Urban
Yang Zhuang (L8, CT), it has an unexpected Tone A1-G (45), which is expected to be
C1-G (34) in the tone box designated. This term may have been undergone an
analogical change or phonological contamination by replacing its original *C tone to
*A tone from *vɛjA ‘fire’ (cf. Li 1977: 79, 286) or *wɤjA ‘fire’ (cf. Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 207) according to the frequently used compound word *ʔdaŋC
*vɛjA/*wɤjA ‘to make fire’. This indicates that the tonal category of this item in Daxin
Baoxu (L23) has been changed much earlier than the time of tonal splits.
Besides, some individual changes before/after the time of tonal splits also cannot be
captured by tone box. For example, we have previously discussed that in Jingxi
215
Hurun Yang Zhuang (L11), pre-glottalized stops condition a secondary tonal split
from the high register to merge into the low register in Tone A, like in ˀda:w31 (A2 <
A1) ‘star’ and ˀban31 (A2 < A1) ‘to fly’. However, pre-glottalized glide ʔj- has been
earlier merged into j-, and the tone conditioned by this j- (< *ʔj-) is preserved the
original tone A1 of the primary tonal split, like in ja:51 (A1) ‘medicine’. The initial j(< *ʔj-) does not condition a secondary tonal split since its glottal feature has been
lost before the time of secondary tonal splits (cf. §4.3.2.2).
Modern loanwords borrowed from foreign languages into Tai varieties often do not
follow the tonal patterns at time of tonal splits, and this determine that tone box
cannot apply to modern loanwords in Tai. For instance, Rong Maet Tai Lue (L6) has
borrowed abundant loanwords from the national language Bangkok Thai (L1).
Normally tones collocating with unaspirated initial stops in Thai all distribute in the
high register, which has a secondary tonal split together with the split conditioned
by glottalized initials from A1 to merge into A2 in the A column. Therefore, A1-U is
actually merged into A2 in Thai, in contrast A1-U is preserved as other high register
rows in Tai Lue, like Thai pla:33 (A2 < A1-U) vs. Tai Lue pa:55 (A1-U) ‘fish’.
According to this correspondence, the third syllable of the Thai word sin24 laʔ45 pin33
‘artist’ should have been borrowed as pin55 (A1-U) in Tai Lue. However, in Rong
Maet Tai Lue it unexpectedly has a A2 tone pin31, probably due to the
correspondence between Thai A2 (33) and Tai Lue A2 (31) because A1-U has been
merged into A2 in Thai.
For another example, the tonal patterns of Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (cf. Table 36)
determines that aspirated initials are never found in the low register (except B1-A >
B2 and DL1 > DL2) due to that voiced sounds never develop to aspirations, but lots
of modern Chinese loanwords in Yang Zhuang have low register tones, such as tʰa:j31
(=A2) ‘platform’ (< tʰa:i31 Southwestern Mandarin ), pʰa:j31 (=A2) ‘rank’ (< pʰa:i31
Southwestern Mandarin ), and tʰa:w213 (=C2) ‘cover’ (< tʰau21 Southwestern
Mandarin). B1-A has been merged into B2 (33) to be different from B1-U/C (55) due
to the regular rules, however many modern Chinese loanwords with aspirated
initials have 55 tone like in kʰa:55 (=B1-U/C) ‘card’ (< kʰa55 Southwestern
Mandarin), tʰa:n55 (=B1-U/C) ‘blanket’ (< tʰan55 Southwestern Mandarin), and
pʰa:w55 (=B1-U/C) ‘to run’ (< pʰau55 Southwestern Mandarin). In contrast, this 55
tone is never found in Tai inherited words in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang. This
phenomenon in both the cases of Tai Lue and Debao Urban Yang Zhuang indicates
that after the tonal structure formed, the collocation of initial and tones may become
flexible to break through the restrictions at time of tonal splits.
216
In short, any Tai tone box would never capture any kind of aberrant tonal
development and unconventional tonal behaviors in modern loanwords borrowed
from foreign languages.
A tone box has been proved to be a convenient research tool in the fieldworks of Tai
tones, especially Gedney’s tone box applied to SWT languages. On the basis of his
tone box and the revised tone box (Liao & Shen 2012), an attempt of improving the
designs for being able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai variety has been
hypothesized. However, an integrated Tai tone box may become redundant because
tonal development in Tai appears in a different diverse scene in different groups. Tai
tone boxes designed with specific pertinence to different Tai periods and different
Tai sub-groups have been suggested to use as practical research tools.
Nevertheless, Tai tone boxes suggested in this chapter are supposed to be able to
apply to different diachronic stages of Tai, specific sub-groups of Tai, and all Tai
varieties. From a practical point of view, if the modern Tai subgroup (namely SWT,
CT, NT, and YN) of a particular variety is known, a specific Tai tone box and its
ancillary box should be used for investigating the tones in that variety. If the
affiliation of that Tai variety is not known at all, the integrated Tai tone box and its
ancillary box should be applied, since it is supposed to be able to capture all tonal
distinct in any Tai varieties, except aberrant tonal development in some single items
and the unconventional tonal behaviors in modern loanwords borrowed from
foreign languages.
217
Conclusion
This chapter summarizes and discusses the research findings of this thesis, and
makes suggestions for related research which needs to be further studied. §7.1 gives
a brief overview of this study. §7.2 provides the conclusions of this study by being
divided into two sub-sections. The former §7.2.1 discusses the diachronic processes
of Tai tonal development which is the first objective of this thesis, and concludes
what the agreements and the disagreements between the hypothesis and the actual
findings of Tai tonal development are. The latter §7.2.2 concludes the other two
objectives, namely Tai classification and Tai tone box based on the findings in the
diachronic scheme of Tai tonal development, and also discusses the agreements and
the disagreements between these conclusions and the two relating hypotheses. The
last section §7.3 suggests further research of this study.
As the title suggests, Tonal Development of Tai languages, this thesis aims to provide
a full scheme of tonal development of Tai, from tonogenesis in PT to different
diachronic hierarchies of tonal splits in different Tai groups and varieties, and
further suggests new viewpoints on Tai classification and Tai tone box based on
tonology.
As a matter of my Tai ethnic background and my experience, I am very interested in
the language comparison among Tai languages, especially the tones. It is generally
known all Tai languages are tonal just like the majorities of the languages in China
and MSEA. My concern is that how tones in Tai languages have arisen, or in other
words how Tai languages have developed from a non-tonal proto-language to
become various tonal daughter languages. Driven by this interest, the main part of
library research has been accomplished before the hypotheses of this thesis were
proposed. My starting point is Gedney’s tone box (Gedney 1989[1972]). As a result
of comparing the tonal splits of many modern SWT languages, the tone box designed
by Gedney (1989[1972]: 202) consists of five tone categories (A, B, C, DL, DS) with
four groups of initial consonants (Group 1=Voiceless friction, Group 2=Voiceless
218
unaspirated, Group3=Glottal, and Group 4=Voiced.) conditioning possible tonal
splits. But when applied to varieties of CT and NT, Gedney’s tone box (1989[1972])
has several shortcomings. The most noticeable phenomena are tonal splits among
the voiceless friction sounds and the voiceless unaspirated stops. For this reason, my
colleague and I have suggested a revised version of Gedney’s tone box (Liao & Shen
2012). This background and the related literature review are discussed in Chapter 1
and Chapter 2 respectively.
However, the revised tone box was based on an elementary tonal comparison among
some of the Tai varieties, and it needs a more exhaustive tone data analysis to draw
out the full tonal development scheme of Tai for improving its incomplete method.
Therefore, based upon the wordlist designated in this revised tone box, this study
chooses forty-two Tai varieties from the four Tai sub-groups, namely SWT, CT, NT
and YN to collect tone data to analyze with, among them most are collected during
my fieldworks in China and Thailand, and some are referenced to published sources.
The criterion of selecting Tai varieties is based on the diversity of tonal patterns and
the amounts of internal varieties. Therefore, CT varieties which contain the most
complicated tonal split patterns are most numerous in the Tai varieties investigated.
Some undescribed Tai varieties, such as Quan Son Tai (L7) of SWT, Debao Nalong
Myang Zhuang (14), Debao Tuoxin Pyang Zhuang (L15) and Daxin Baoxu Zuojiang
Zhuang (L23) of CT, Debao Longsang (L31) and Debao Dongling (L32) of NT, as well
as Nanning Shuangding (L26) and Yongning Baiji (L25) of YN are particularly
selected to study with in case of careless omission of special tonal split patterns
(although finally most of them are proved to have common tonal split pattern except
Baoxu). Historical and comparative linguistics provides the main methodology for
this study. The details of the methodology are introduced in Chapter 3.
The hypotheses of this thesis are as follows. (1) Three groups of proto-initials
(voiceless-voiced-breathy) conditioned primary tonal splits whereas splits
conditioned by other pharyngeal features, such as aspiration and pre-glottalization
represent stages of secondary development. This is the basis of the other two
hypotheses because it points out the diachronic hierarchies and stages of tonal
development. (2) The “two plus one taxonomy” of Tai classification is more
reasonable than the Tripartite Division at least in the early stage of Tai languages,
because of different tonal behaviors of “proto-breathy” initials in Proto-Northern
and Proto-Southern Tai languages. This hypothesis is based on that in PNT these
initials conditioned the tones to merge with those tones conditioned by proto-voiced
initials, while in PST they conditioned the tones to merge those conditioned by
219
proto-voiceless initials. (3) Based on the fuller scheme of Tai tonal development, a
more comprehensive revised version of Gedney’s tone box will be able to capture the
tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties. These three hypotheses are discussed in the
three finding chapters, Chapter 4, Chapter 5, and Chapter 6 of this thesis
respectively.
After data analysis, the frame of the thesis has been fixed up. The conclusions which
aim to test the three hypotheses are as shown in the following sub-sections.
Before answering the first hypothesis, Tai tonogenesis is inevitably to discussed with
because it is the first step of tonal development. Just like the hypotheses of
tonogenesis which are applied to the majorities of languages in China and MSEA,
tones in Tai is a secondary development which induced by syllable finals. It was that
the decaying of syllable final -h gave an original high-falling tone *B on relatively
long vowel duration, and the weakening of *-ʔ gave an original low-rising tone *C
on relatively short vowel duration, to contrast to each other and to the least marked
level tone *A on smooth syllables, for keeping the semantic distinctions from the
original plain syllables without obstruent syllable finals. Syllables with final oral
stops -p / -t / -k were preserved to consist the fourth tonal category *D with a highfalling contour like its counterpart tone *B.
Tone data analysis suggests a full scheme of tonal splits as follows. After the four
tonal categories arose in PT, they further developed to fall into two series to form
eight tones due to the register splits conditioned by voiceless-voiced groups of
initials. The trigger must have been the voicing of proto-voiceless continuants
causing the merger of proto-voiceless-voiced continuants. In this earliest register
tonal split process, a straightforward tonal split pattern was presented as all initial
sounds with original voiceless features (voiceless stops and glottalized sounds) went
together with original voiceless continuants to condition an original high tone, in
contrast original voiced stops went together with original voiced continuants to
condition an original low tone. This is the primary tonal split on the post-PT level.
In all modern Tai sub-groups many varieties are found to preserve this primary tonal
split pattern. Secondary tonal splits in Tai languages must have been conditioned by
glottalized sounds, unaspirated stops, and aspirated sounds in the high register in
some of the modern Tai varieties far later than the period of the primary tonal split.,
220
because the crucial evidence that aspirated sounds are secondary development in
post-PT level in ST and YN. Glottalized sounds and aspirated sounds are found to be
able to condition secondary tonal split itself or together with one another, but
secondary tonal splits conditioned by unaspirated stops are only found to be a
byproduct of those conditioned by glottalized sounds. Secondary tonal splits
normally present as being depressed by the conditioning initials to become the third
register first, and in some varieties the third register is preserved to form a “three-
way split” pattern, but in more varieties the third register may go to merge into the
original low register or go back to merge into the original high register to finally
form the “two-way split” pattern. This is why the two-way split pattern is still the
main stream in Tai varieties.
For the first hypothesis of this thesis, the most remarkable change of the main idea
is the proto-breathy initials in the proposed three groups of proto-initials (voicelessvoiced-breathy) conditioning primary tonal splits (or in other words the voicing
alternation between NT and CT/SWT) was actually not an actual existence on the PT
level. After a fuller data analysis and a cautious comparison among the previous
studies, this thesis agrees with the hypothesis of the sesquisyllable and monosyllable
coexisting structure in PT (Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009), and with that voicing
alternation should have been induced by the different directions of initial mergers of
“initial segment + medial” onsets (Weera Ostapirat 2006). After realizing that
breathy sounds would have been induced as a transitional shape from sesquisyllable
onset to aspirated or continuants sounds in the modern ST varieties, the hypothesis
that proto-breathy at the PT level must be corrected to be VASO-1/2, which merged
into unaspirated sounds in PNT, but merged into aspirated sounds in PST. In
addition, voicing alternation between ST and NT-YN is not only limited to VASO1/2, but also suitable to VASO-2/1, which merge into voiced sounds in ST but into
voiceless sounds in NT-YN. However, the main significance of this hypothesis is not
changed, for that the result of voicing alternation between ST and NT-YN must have
been at the PT level. The most important point is that voicing alternation between
ST and NT-YN must have been on the earliest diachronic hierarchy of tonal split,
and this point is the basis of that Tai has to be first divided into two primary
branches ST and NT-YN, to attest the second hypothesis of this thesis.
221
On the basis of the full tonal development scheme suggested by the conclusion of
the first hypothesis above, the other two hypotheses are attested as follows.
First, the “Two plus one taxonomy” of Tai classification based on the primary tonal
splits must have been clarified to be somewhat incomplete. Based on the analysis of
tonal behaviors with supplementary evidence from initial behaviors, vocalic
behaviors, and exclusive lexical items, this thesis suggests two levels of Tai division
as follows. Primary, it agrees on the two-term system suggested by Haudricourt
(1956) to divide PT first into Dioi or ʔyai65 vs. Tai proper, by another two-term
system, NT-YN (<PNT) and ST (<PST), for avoiding confusing the conventional
naming of Tai sub-groups. The division on this level is based on the primary tonal
splits suggested by tonal behaviors of VASO-1/2, VASO-2/1, and horizontal tonal
splits conditioned by “vowel-length alternation” between Tai varieties from NT-YN
and ST respectively. From this level, YN whose classification as NT or CT is open to
debate has been clarified to be get together with NT rather than CT. Secondarily, it
further suggests a new “two plus two” (NT/YN + CT/SWT) taxonomy of modern Tai
division based on the conclusion of secondary tonal splits. For example, ASO-CY
shared by CT and YN as secondary areal feature can separate CT from SWT on
secondary sub-group level. AMO-SY, ASO-SY, and ASO-YN as secondary innovations
Dioi or ʔɔaɓ here is actually the autonym of many NT language members including many
Northern Zhuang languages and Bouyei. The second syllable of the term Bouyei “Yei” (as well as
“Yi” from Chinese Pinyin “Buyi”) is actually the same thing of Dioi or ʔɔaɓ (while the Bou/Bu is the
Tai etyma ‘person, people, CLF of person’, like pʰu:C1-A in Bangkok Thai (L1), pʰowC1-A in Debao
Urban Yang Zhuang (L8), and powC2 in Wuming Shuangqiao Yongbei Zhuang (L30)). I suppose that
all of these different forms (with different notations of spelling) must have developed from the same
PT etyma *ʔɔwaɔC (my reconstruction). According to my fieldwork research, most CT speakers, such
as Yang Zhuang in the counties Debao, Jingxi, and Napo in Southwestern corner of Guangxi, normally
call the speakers of their neighboring NT languages (e.g. Youjiang Zhuang, Guibian Zhuang and
Yongbei Zhuang) pʰowC1-A ˀɔuɔC1-G (literally “people + NT”) and call these NT languages ɕa:ŋC1-U
ʔ
jujC1-G (literally “speak + NT”). The segment ʔjujC1-G is actually the cognate of Dioi, ʔɔaɓ, Yei and Yi.
The reason that I reconstruct *ʔɔwaɔC for these different modern forms is because the proposed initial
*ʔɔw- can explain all the irregular vocalic correspondence among different Tai varieties, as illustrated
in the following examples. 1) The regular change *ʔɔw- > *ʔɔ- in many modern NZ and Bouyei
languages like Tianlin Lizhou Zhuang (L33) and Wangmo Bouyei (Standard Bouyei) led *ʔɔwaɔC >
ˀɔaɔC1-G ‘Bouyei’. 2) *ʔɔw- cause the following vowel to become rounded in all Yang Zhuang varieties,
so that *ʔɔwaɔC > ˀɔuɔC1-G ‘Northern Zhuang/Bouyei’ (the similar process can also explain the Dioi
form); 3) The same change as in Yang Zhuang may have processed in some extreme western NT
varieties, such as Zhenning Bouyei (L42) and Qiubei Gehan (L39). Note that in these varieties, -uj has
been merged into -i:, such as ti:C1/2 ‘bowl’ in Qiubei Gehan (vs. tʰuɔC1/2 in Debao Yang Zhuang (L810)). In Zhenning Bouyei (L42) and Qiubei Gehan (L39), *ʔɔw- > *ʔ- if the following vowel has the
feature [+high, +front], so that *ʔɔwaɔC > *ʔɔuɔC > ʔɓ:C1-G ‘Northern Zhuang/Bouyei’. I suppose that
only if the reconstruction *ʔɔwaɔC ‘NT (Northern Zhuang/Bouyei)’ is true, can all these irregular
modern Tai forms be reasonable explained.
65
222
which are shared between varieties of YN suggest that YN is separated from NT on
secondary sub-group level, too. Therefore, Li’s CT and SWT can be divided from
each other under the ST sub-group, in contrast NT and YN differ from one another
under the NT-YN sub-group.
Second, the proposed tone box which is suggested to be able to capture the tonal
distinctions in any Tai varieties can be obtained after all the possible conditioning
phonation types of initials at time of tonal splits are studied. However, an integrated
Tai tone box have been proved to be cumbersome if a Tai variety’s affiliation has
been known. It is because secondary tonal splits must have been individually
conditioned at a post-PT period in different Tai varieties. For instance, NT varieties
lack of contrastive aspirated sounds and that unaspirated stops never go with
glottalized initials to condition secondary tonal splits, therefore a tone box
designated with the divisions containing the rows of aspirated sounds and
unaspirated stops appear to be unnecessary for NT. For this reason, Tai tone boxes
designated with specific pertinence to different Tai periods and different Tai subgroups are first suggested if the diachronic period of a Tai variety or the affiliation
of a modern Tai variety are known. Different Tai tone boxes are designated based on
the different situations of secondary tonal splits in different Tai sub-groups, namely
NT, YN, CT, and SWT. Nevertheless, an integrated Tai tone box is further suggested
to be able to capture the tonal distinctions in any Tai varieties. In this sense, the first
two conclusions of the goals of this thesis are the two substantial basis of the
conclusion of Tai tone box.
This study has solved the three main knotty tonal problems as were presented
above. However, as has been pointed out in §1.3.1, there are still some limitations of
tonal research of this study. All these limitations should be put forward to further
studies together with other areas for further research suggested by this study. The
main suggestions for these areas are as follows.
First, secondary tonal splits are so far only found in the high register in Tai
languages, but are never found in the low register of Tai languages even though
there are also different phonation types (such as unaspirated stops, aspirated stops,
and continuants) on the low register. The reason deserves more attention. Further
study may give a reasonable explanation for this phenomenon.
223
Second, Tai tonal development in natural speech environments should be further
investigated. The limitation of lacking tone data from this occasion cannot be solved
by our wordlist. That is, tone data acquired from single words do not reflect tone
expressions from the natural conversation. Some tone changes in natural
conversation should be systematically researched, since these changes such as tone
sandhi or tone coarticulation may be found to be the newest part of tonal
development.
Furthermore, as discussed in §6.6, the phenomenon indicating that after the tonal
structure formed, the collocation of initial and tones may become flexible to break
through the restrictions at time of tonal splits like tones of modern loanwords in
different Tai varieties should be further studied. Some tentative research has
suggested that tones of loanwords in Tai varieties differ from different borrowing
periods, and from different varieties. For example, among different Yang Zhuang
varieties all OC and MC loans reflects regular correspondence of tonal categories,
but modern Chinese loans borrowed from Southwestern Mandarin and Standard
Mandarin all present very irregular correspondence of tonal categories. The factors
and the trends of these tonal phenomena need more attention from researchers.
Moreover, Tai classification based on tonology needs more investigation and
verification. In this thesis the two levels of Tai classification have been suggested by
the analysis of different tonal behaviors among different groups of Tai, and has been
supported from initial behaviors, vocalic behaviors, and exclusive lexical items. It
should be crucial to test some other recent Tai classifications based on newly
methodological findings. For instance, Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009: 298) has
provided a new complex Tai subgrouping diagram which suggests that “SWT is the
only one of Li’s three groups that is recognized as a valid subgroup” (Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 302). However, the primary and the secondary divisions
suggested by the tonal analysis of this thesis have called in questions that whether
Pittayawat Pittayaporn’s Tai division has complied with the diachronic orders of
shared innovations. This problem needs further comparisons and discussions.
Finally, the new perspective of Tai tone box suggested in this thesis needs further
extension to apply to other language groups which have internal tonal diversity, like
the more complicated tonal patterns in Kam-Sui languages and some of the Sinitic
languages (such as Cantonese dialects in Yulin, and some of the Southern Min
varieties). The design of the ancillary box of Tai tone box has been suggested to be a
flexible tool for collecting tone data from Tai varieties with the complicated tonal
224
correspondence based on complicated initial correspondence. The main tone box is
succinct because in any specific Tai variety conditioning phonation types of initial
groups cannot be more than five groups. When applying to Kam-Sui languages
which have extremely complex patterns of voicing alterations (cf. Weera Ostapirat
2006), a succinct tone box with a relatively complex ancillary box (in which the
source of voicing alternation will be able to be directly perceived) may be
applicable. The method of tone box will be proved to be able to solve all the
complicated tonal puzzles of languages with full-developing tones in further studies.
225
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Anderson, Stephen R. 1978. Tone features. In Victoria A. Fromkin (ed.), Tone: a
linguistic survey, 133-176. New York: Academic Press.
Anttila, Raimo. 1989. Historical and comparative linguistics, 2nd edn. Amsterdam &
Philadelphia: John Benjamin’s Publishing Company.
Arlotto, Anthony. 1972. Introduction to historical linguistics. Lanham, Maryland:
University Press of America.
Bao, Zhining. 1990. On the nature of tone. Massachusetts Avenue: MIT dissertation.
Baxter, William. 1992. A handbook of Old Chinese phonology. Berlin & New York:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Benedict, Paul K. 1942. Thai, Kadai and Indonesian: A new alignment in
Southeastern Asia. American Anthropologist 44. 576-601.
Benedict, Paul K. 1975. Austro-Thai: language and culture. New Haven, Conn: HRAF
Press.
Benedict, Paul K. 1990. Japanese-Austro-Thai. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
Bi, Kechao. 1982. 入
. [Entering tone]. Wuhan: Hubei People’s Publishing House.
Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Blust, Robert. 1987. Lexical reconstruction and semantic reconstruction: the case of
Austronesian ‘house’ words. Diachronica 4. 79-106.
Brown, J. Marvin. 1965. From ancient Thai to modern dialects. Bangkok: Social
Science Association Press.
Brown, J. Marvin. 1975. The great tone split: did it work in two opposite ways? In
Jimmy G. Harris and James R. Chamberlain (ed.), Studies in Tai linguistics in
226
honor of William J. Gedney, 33-48. Bangkok: Central Institute of English
Language, Office of State University.
Campbell, Lyle. 2004. Historical linguistics: an introduction, 2nd edn. Cambridge: The
MIT Press.
Chafe, W. L. 1959. Internal reconstruction in Seneca. Language 35. 477-495.
Chamberlain, James R. 1975. A new look at the history and classification of the Tai
Languages. In Jimmy G. Harris and James R. Chamberlain (ed.), Studies in
Tai linguistics in honor of William J. Gedney, 49–66. Bangkok: Central Institute
of English Language, Office of State University.
Chang, Kun. 1966. A comparative study of the Yao tone system. Language 42.2. 303310.
Chang, Kun. 1975. The reconstruction of Proto-Miao-Yao tones. Bulletin of the
Institute of History and Philology. Academia Sinica 44. 541-628.
Chao, Y. R. 1928. Studies in the modern Wu-dialects. Beijing: Tsing Hua College
Research Institute.
Chao, Y. R. 1930. A system of tone letters. Le Maître Phonétique 45. 24-27.
Clements, G. N. 1981a (ed.). Harvard Studies in Phonology 2. Bloomington: Indiana
University Linguistics Club.
Clements, G. N. 1981b. The hierarchical representation of tone features. In G. N.
Clements (ed.) Harvard Studies in Phonology 2, 108-177. Bloomington:
Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Crowley, Terry. 1994. An introduction to historical linguistics. Auckland: Oxford
University Press.
Diller, Anthony. 1998. The Tai language family and the comparative method. In
Somsonge Burusphat (ed.), The international conference on Tai studies, 1-32.
Bangkok: Institute of Language and Culture for Rural Development, Mahidol
University.
227
Diller, Anthony. 2008. Introduction. In Anthony V. N. Diller, Jerold A. Edmondson &
Yongxian Luo (ed.), The Tai-Kadai languages, 3-8. Routledge language family
series. London & New York: Routledge.
Dong, Hongyuan. 2014. A History of the Chinese Language. London & New York:
Routledge.
Downer, G. B. 1967. Tone-change and tone-shift in White Miao. Bulletin of the School
of Oriental and African Studies, University of London 30.3. 589-599.
Duanmu, San. 1990. A formal study of syllable, tone, stress, and domain in Chinese
languages. Massachusetts Avenue: MIT dissertation.
Dyan, Isidore. 1974. Lexical reconstruction. In Isidore Dyen & David F. Aberle (ed.),
Lexical reconstruction: the case of the Proto-Athapaskan kinship system, 5-22.
Cambridge University Press.
Edmondson, Jerold A. 1990a. Kam tone splits and the variation of breathiness. In
Jerold A. Edmondson, Crawford Feagin, & Peter Mühlhäusler (ed.),
Development and diversity: Language variation across time and space, 187-202.
Arlington: The University of Texas at Arlington and Summer Institute of
Linguistics.
Edmondson, Jerold A (ed.). 1990b. Kadai: discussions in Kadai and SE Asian
linguistics. Arlington: Department of Foreign Languages and Linguistics,
University of Texas at Arlington.
Edmondson, Jerold A. 1992a. A study of the tones and initials in Kam, Lakkja, and
Hlai. In Carol J. Compton & John F. Hartmann (ed.), Papers on Tai languages,
linguistics, and literatures, in honor of William J. Gedney on his 77th birthday,
77-102. Illinois: Northern Illinois University, Center for Southeast Asian
Studies.
Edmondson, Jerold A. 1992b. On five-level tone system. In Shin Ja J. Hwang and
William R. Merrifield (ed.), Language in context: Essays for Robert E. Longacre,
555-576. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics and the University of Texas
at Arlington.
Edmondson, Jerold A. 1993. Tonality in Austronesian languages. Hawaii: University of
Hawaii Press.
228
Edmondson, Jerold A. 1994. Change and variation in Zhuang. In Karen L. Adams &
Thomas John Hudak (ed.), Papers from the second Annual Meeting of the
Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, 147-185. Glendale: Arizona State
University, Program for Southeast Asian Studies.
Edmondson, Jerold A. 2002. Nung An: Origin of a species. In Robert S. Bauer (ed.),
Collected papers on Southeast Asian and Pacific languages, 51–62. Pacific
linguistics series 530. Canberra: Australian National University.
Edmondson, Jerold A. & David B. Solnit. (eds.) 1988. Comparative Kadai: Linguistic
studies beyond Tai. Summer Institute of Linguistics and the University of
Texas at Arlington Publications in Linguistics, 86. Dallas: Summer Institute of
Linguistics and the University of Texas at Arlington.
Edmondson, Jerold A. & David B. Solnit. 1997. Introduction. In Edmondson, Jerold
A. & David B. Solnit (ed.), Comparative Kadai: the Tai branch, 1-17. Summer
Institute of Linguistics and the University of Texas at Arlington Publications
in Linguistics, 124. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics and the University
of Texas at Arlington.
Enfield. N.J. 2005. Areal Linguistics and Mainland Southeast Asia. Annual Review of
Anthropology 2005. 34. 181-206.
Ferlus, Michel. 1990. Remarques sur le consonatisme de proto Thai-Yay (révision du
proto-Tai de Li Fangkuei). Paper circulated at the 23rd International Conference
on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics, University of Texas at Arlington.
Freiberger, Nancy. 1964. A phonemic description of Nong (Nung). Te Reo 7. 15-22.
Fu, Baoning. 1995. A system of tone features and its implications for the
representation of tone. Burnaby, PhD: Simon Fraser University dissertation.
Gedney, William J. 1972. A puzzle in comparative Tai phonology. In J. G. Harris &
R. B. Noss (ed.), Tai phonetics and phonology, 52-57. Bangkok: Central
Institute of English Language, Office of State Universities.
Gedney, William J. 1985. Confronting the unknown: total splits and the genealogy of
Tai-Kadai, Linguistics of the Sino-Tibetan Area: The State of the Art, Pacific
Linguistics Seriese-No.87. Canberra: Australian National University. 116-124.
229
Gedney, William J. 1989[1972]. A checklist for determining tones in Tai dialects. In
Robert J. Bickner, John Hartmann, Thomas John Hudak, and Patcharin
Peyasantiwong (ed.), Selected papers on comparative Tai studies, 191-206.
Michigan: Center for South and Southeast Asian studies 29. Michigan: the
University of Michigan.
Gedney, William J. 1989a. Evidence for another series of voiced initials in proto-Tai.
In Robert J. Bickner, John Hartmann, Thomas John Hudak, & Patcharin
Peyasantiwong (ed.), Selected papers on comparative Tai studies, 229-270.
Michigan Papers on South and Southeast Asian Studies 29. Michigan: The
University of Michigan.
Gedney, William J. 1989b. Future directions in comparative Tai linguistics. In
Robert J. Bickner, John Hartmann, Thomas John Hudak, & Patcharin
Peyasantiwong (ed.), Selected papers on comparative Tai studies, 7-116.
Michigan Papers on South and Southeast Asian Studies 29. Michigan: The
University of Michigan.
Gedney, William J. 1989c. Speculations on early Tai tones. In Robert J. Bickner,
John Hartmann, Thomas John Hudak, & Patcharin Peyasantiwong (ed.),
Selected papers on comparative Tai studies, 207-228. Michigan Papers on South
and Southeast Asian Studies 29. Michigan: The University of Michigan.
Gordon, Matthews, & Peter Ladefoged. 2001. Phonation types: a cross-linguistic
overview. Journal of Phonetics 29. 383-406.
Gregerson, Kenneth J. & Edmondson, Jerold A. 1998. Some puzzles in Cao Lan. n
Somsonge Burusphat (ed.), The international conference on Tai Studies, 151164. Bangkok: Institute of Language and Culture for Rural Development,
Mahidol University.
Haas, Mary R. 1969. Internal reconstruction of the Nootka-Nitinat pronominal
suffixes. International Journal of American Linguistics 35. 108-124.
Hanna, William J. 2012. Dai Lue-English dictionary. Chiang Mai: Silkworm Books.
Harris, Z. S. 1960. Structural linguistics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
230
Harris, J.G. 2001. States of the glottis of Thai voiceless stops and affricates. In M.R.
Kalaya Tingsabadh & A.S. Abramson (ed.), Essays in Tai linguistics, 3–11.
Bangkok: Chulalongkorn University Press.
Hashimoto, Mantaro J. 1972. The linguistic mechanism of flip flop. Unicorn 10. 119.
Hashimoto, Oi-kan Yue. 1986. Tonal flip-flop in Chinese dialects. Journal of Chinese
Linguistics 14(2). 161-183.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1948. Les occlusives velaires en thai. Bulletin de la
Société de Linguistique de Paris 48. 86-89.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1954a. Comment reconstruise le chinois archai ̈que.
Word 10. 351-364.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1954b. De l’origine des tons en viêtnamien. Journal
Asiatique 242. 161-183.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1956. De la restitution des intitiales dans les langues
monosyllabiques: le problème du thai commun. Bulletin de la Société de
Linguistique de Paris 52 (1). 307-322.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1961. Bipartition et tripartition des systèmes de tons
dans quelques langues d’Extrême-Orient. Bulletin de la Société de Linguistique
de Paris 56. 163-180.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1963. Remarques sur les initiales complexes de la
language Sek. Belletin de la Société de Linguistique de Paris 58 (1). 156-163.
Haudricourt, André-Georges. 1972. Two-way and three-way splitting of tonal
systems in some far eastern languages (translated by Christopher Court). In
J.G. Harris & R.B. Noss (ed.), A conference on Tai phonetics and phonology, 5886. Bangkok: Mahidol University.
Henderson, Eugéne J. A. 1964. Marginalia in Siamese phonetic studies. In D.
Abercrombie, D.B. Fry, P.A.D. MacCarthy, N.C. Scott & J.L.M. Trim (ed.), In
honour of Daniel Jones: papers contributed on the occasion of his eightieth
birthday 12 September 1961, 415–424. London: Longmans.
231
Henderson, Eugéne J. A. 1982. Tonogenesis: some recent speculations on the
development of tone. Transactions of the Philological Society 1982. 1-24.
Hirayama, Hisao. 1987. The record of tones of the Tang Dynasty in Shintanzoo by
the Japanesse monk Annen. Wang Li memorial volumes, Chinese volume.
Hoenigswald, H.M. 1960. Languages change and linguistic reconstruction. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Hock, Hans Henrich. 1991. Principles of historical linguistics, 2nd edn. Berlin: Mouton
de Gruyter.
Holm, David. 2010. Linguistic diversity along the China-Vietnam border. Linguistics
of the Tibeto-Burman Area 33(2). 1-62.
Hombert, Jean-Marie. 1976. The effect of aspiration on the fundamental frequency
of the following vowel. BLS 1976. 212-219.
Hombert, Jean-Marie. 1978. A model of tone systems. In Donna Jo. Napoli (ed.),
Elements of tone, stress, and intonation. Washington D. C.: Georgetown
University Press.
Hombert, Jean-Marie, John J. Ohala, & William G. Ewan. 1979. Phonetic
explanations for the development of tones. Languages 55(1). 37-58.
Huang, Yuanwei. 1997. The interaction between Zhuang and the Yue (Cantonese)
dialects. In Jerold A. Edmondson & David B. (ed.), Comparative Kadai: the Tai
branch, 57-76. Texas: The Summer Institute of Linguistics and The University
of Texas at Arlington.
Hudak, Thomas John. (ed.) 1995. William J. Gedney’s Central Tai Dialects: Glossaries,
Texts, and Translations. Michigan Papers on South and Southeast Asia, Center
for South and Southeast Asian Studies, The University of Michigan.
Hudak, Thomas John. (ed.) 1997. William J. Gedney’s Tai Dialects Studies: Glossaries,
Texts, and Translations. Michigan Papers on South and Southeast Asia, Center
for South and Southeast Asian Studies, The University of Michigan.
Hudak, Thomas John. 2008. William J. Gedney’s Comparative Tai Source Book.
Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
232
Hyman, Larry M. 1986. The representation of multiple tone heights. In K. Bogers, H.
Van der Hulst, & M. Mous (ed.), The phonological representation of
suprasegmentals, 109–152. Dordrecht: Foris.
International Phonetic Association (Ed.). 1999. Handbook of the International
Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic
Alphabet. Cambridge & New York: Cambridge University Press.
Iwata, R., M. Sawashima, & H. Hirose. 1981. Laryngeal adjustments for syllable-final
stops in Cantonese. Phoniat 15. 45–54.
Iwata, R., M. Sawashima, H. Hirose. & S. Niimi. 1979. Laryngeal adjustments of
Fukienese stops: initial plosives and final applosives. Phoniat 13. 61–81.
Jackson, Eric M., Emily H. S. Jackson, & Lau Shuh Huey. 2011. A sociolinguistic
survey of the Dejing Zhuang dialect area. SIL international, East Asia Group in
cooperation with the Guangxi Minorities Language and Scripts Work
Commission. https://mail.link77.net/
[email protected]/
20110629DRAFT -English-Dejing_survey_report-ESR.pdf (15 Dec., 2012.)
Jeffers, Robert J. & Lehiste, Ilse. 1982. Principles and methods for historical linguistics.
Cambridege, Massachusetts & London: The MIT Press.
Johnson, Eric C. 2011. A lexical and phonological comparison of the Central Taic
languages of Wenshan Prefecture, China. SIL Electronic Working Papers 2001005. http://www.sil.org/silewp/2011/silewp2011-005.pdf (17 Feb., 2012.)
Karlgren, Bernhard. 1964. Grammata serica recensa. Stockholm: Museum of Far
Eastern Antiquities.
Lan, Qingyuan. 1982.
语中古汉语借词及汉越语与平话的关系 [The relationship
among Middle Chinese loans in Zhuang, Sino-Vietnamese, and Pinghua].
Minority Languages of China 2001(3). 48-61.
Lass, Roger. 1997. Historical linguistics and language change. In S.R. Anderson, J.
Bresnan, B. Comrie, W. Dressler, C. Ewen, R. Huddleston, R. Lass, D.
Lightfoot, J. Lyons, R. H. Matthews, R. Posner, S. Romaine, N. V. Smith & N.
Nincent (ed.), Cambridge studies in linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
233
Li, Fang Kuei. 1940. The Tai dialect of Lungchow: Text, Translations, and Glossary.
Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica 16.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1943. The Hypothesis of a pre-glottalized series of consonants in
primitive Tai. Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica 11. 177188.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1944. 语喉塞音及带喉塞音
母对于剥隘
调系统之影响 [The
influence of the primitive Tai glottal stop and preglottalized consonants on
the tone systems of Po-ai]. The Journal of the Institute of Siniology of the
National University of Peking 4. 59-67.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1945. Some Old Chinese Loan Words in the Tai Languages. Harvard
Journal of Asiatic Studies 8. 333-342.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1948. The distribution of initials and tones in the Sui language.
Language 24. 160–167.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1949. Tones in the Riming System of the Sui Language. Word 5. 262267.
[Li, Fang Kuei. 1956. The Tai dialect of Wu-ming (Text, Translations, and Glossary).
Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica 19.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1959. Classification by vocabulary: Tai dialects. Anthropological
Linguistics 1.2. 15-21.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1960. A tentative classification of Tai dialects. In S. Diamond (ed.),
Culture in history: Essays in honor of Paul Radin, 951-959.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1965. The Tai and the Kam-Sui languages. Lingua 14. 148-179.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1966. The relationship between tones and initials in Tai. In Norman
H. Zide (ed.), Studies in comparative Austroasiatic Linguistics, 82-88.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1970a. Some tonal irregularities in the Tai languages. In Roman
Jakobson & Shigeo Kawamoto (ed.), Studies in General and oriental linguistics,
415-422. Tokyo: TEC Corporation for language and educational research.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1970b. 天保土歌-附音系 [The songs of T'ien-Pao: With a phonological
sketch]. Bulletin of the Institute of Ethnology, Academia Sinica 30. 1-21.
234
Li, Fang-Kuei. 1971. On Siamese jaai. Academia Sinica/Bulletin of the Institute of
History and Philology (Tapei) 42(3). 337-340.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1973. Some dental clusters in Tai. Bulletin of the School of Oriental and
African Studies 36.2. 334-339.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1976. Sino-Tai. Computational Analysis of Asian and African
Languages 3. 39-48.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1977. A handbook of comparative Tai. Honolulu: The University Press
of Hawaii.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1978. A syntactic change in Thai. In Mohamed Ali Jazayery, Edgar C.
Polome & Werner Winter (ed.), Linguistic and literary studies in honor of
Archioald A. Hill, volume 3: Historical and comparative linguistics, 141-145. The
Hague: Mouton Publishers.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1980.
古音研究. [Researches on Old Chinese phonology]. Beijing:
The Commerical Press.
Li, Fang Kuei. 1987. 广西僮语概况
中国地质学会专刊
8. 17-20. [The General
Situation of the Chuang Language of Kuang-his. Memoir of the Geological
Society of China 8. 17-20].
Li, Fang Kuei. 1989. Proto-Tai *kh and *x-. In J.H.C.S. Davidson (ed.), South-east
Asian Linguisitics: Essays in Honour of Eugénie J.A. Henderson, 143-146. School
of Oriental and African Studies, University of London.
Li, Yongsui, Chen Kejiong, & Chen Qiguang. 1959. Several issues concerning the
initials and tones of Miao. Yuyan Yanjiu (Language Study) 1959(4). 65-80.
Liang, Min & Zhang Junru. 1993. 侗台语族送气清塞音
母的产生和发展. [The rise
and development of voiceless aspirated initials in Kam-Tai languages]. Minzu
Yuwen 5. 10-15.
Liang, Min & Zhang Junru. 1996. 侗台语族概论. [An introduction to the Kam-Tai
Languages]. Beijing: China Social Sciences Publishing House.]
Liang, Min & Zhang Junru. 2004. 从汉台语言的数词是否同源词说起. [Discussion on
whether Chinese and Taic numerals are cognates]. Minzu Yuwen 2004(2).
52-56.
235
Liang, Yougang. 1986. 海南島文昌方言音系. [The phonology of the Wenchang
dialect, Guangdong Province]. Fangyan 1986(2). 123-132.
Liao, Hanbo. 2010. 台语支中部组佒侬语
德靖土语
音系概况及其拼音方案详解 [An
overview of the sound system of the Central Taic language Yang-Nong
(Dejing vernacular) and description of a phonetic spelling system]. In Li
Fuqiang and Gao Ya-ning (ed.), Chinese Zhuang Studies 4, 70-192. Beijing:
Publishing House of Minority Nationalities.
Liao, Hanbo. 2013. Proto-Tai reconstruction of ‘maternal grandmother’ revisited:
*na:j A or *ta:j A ?. Paper presented at The 23rd Annual Meeting of The
Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, Bangkok.
Liao, Hanbo. 2015. Yang Zhuang poetry. Paper presented at The 25th Annual
Meeting of The Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, Chiang Mai.
Liao, Hanbo & Shen, Ruiqing. 2012. Gedney’s tone box revisited: Evidence from
some varieties of Central and Northern Tai. Paper presented at The 45th
International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics,
Singapore.
Lien, Chinfa. 1986. Tone merger in the dialects of northern Chinese. Journal of
Chinese Linguistics 14(1). 243-291.
Luo, Yongxian. 1994. Some auxiliary verbs in Zhuang. In Hajime Kitamura, Nishida
Tatsuo, & Nagano Yasuhiko (ed.), Current issues in Sino-Tibetan linguistics,
876-882. Osaka: Organizing Committee of The 26th International Conference
on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics.
Luo, Yongxian. 1996. Tonal irregularities in Tai revisited. Mon-Khmer studies 25. 69102.
Luo, Yongxian. 1997. The subgroup structure of the Tai languages: A historical –
comparative study. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, monograph series number
12.
Lyons, J. 1977. Semantics, 2 vols. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
M.R. Kalaya Tingsabadh. 2001. Thai tone geography. In M.R. Kalaya Tingsabadh &
Arthur S. Abramson (ed.), Essays in Tai Linguistics, 205-228. Bangkok:
Chulalongkorn University Press.
236
Maddieson, Ian. 1970. The inventory of features required for handing tone in
transformational phonology. Research Notes of Department of Linguistics and
Nigerian Languages 3.2-3. 3-18. University of Ibadan.
Maddieson, Ian. 1978a. Tone effects on consonants. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 6.
327-344.
Maddieson, Ian. 1978b. Universals of tone. In Joseph H. Greenberg (ed.), Universals
of human language: phonology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Mai, Yun. 2011. 从广西钟山清塘
语第六调看嘎裂
[Creakiness: A case of the sixth
tone in Qingtang Zhuang from Zhongshan, Guangxi]. Minority Languages of
China 2011(1). 20-26.
Maspero, Henri. 1912. Etudes sur la phonétique historique de la langue annamite.
Bulletin de l’Ecole Française de l’Extrême Orient 12. 1-127.
Matisoff, James A. 1970. Glottal dissimilation and the Lahu high-rising tone: a
tonogenetic case-study. Journal of the American Oriental Society 90 (1). 13-44.
Matisoff, James A. 1973. Tonogenesis in Southeast Asia. In Larry M. Hyman (ed.),
Southern California occasional papers in linguistics 1, 72-95. LA: University of
Southern California.
Mazaudon, Martine. 1977. Tibeto-Burman tonogenetics. Linguistics of the TibetoBurman area 3(2). 1-123.
Mei, Tsu-lin. 1970. Tones and prosody in Middle Chinese and the origin of the rising
tone. Harvard Journal of Asiatic Studies 30. 86-110.
Mei, Tsu-lin. 1982. On the rising tone. Tsing Hua Journal of Chinese Studies new
series XIV (1&2). 233-241.
McMahon, April M.S. 1994. Understanding language change. Cambridge University
Press.
Michaud, Alexis. 2004. Final consonants and glottalization: new perspectives from
Hanoi Vietnamese. Phonetica 61(2-3). 119-146.
Ni, Dabai. 1990. 侗台语概论. [An introduction to Dong-Tai languages]. Beijing: The
Press of Minzu Universiy of China.
237
Nicolson, Beth. 2000. The Nung An language of Vietnam: stepchild or aberrant son?
In The Fifth International Symposium on Languages and Linguistics, 266-295. Ho
Chi Minh City: Vietnam National University, Ho Chi Minh City University of
Social Sciences and Humanities.
Nishida, Tatsuo. 1954. トンネイムによるタイ諸語比較言語学的研究 [Comparative
linguistic studies of the Tai languages based on tonemes]. Gengo Kenkyū 25.
19-46.
Nishida, Tatsuo. 1955. マック・スイ語と共通語比較言語学的研究 [Mak-Sui and
Common Tai]. Gengo Kenkyū 28. 30-62.
Norman, Jerry. 1988. Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Owen, Robert Wyn. 2008. Language use, literacy, and phonological variation in Khuen.
Chiang Mai: Payap University MA thesis.
Owen, Robert Wyn. 2008. A tonal analysis of contemporary Tai Khuen varieties.
Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Societ 5: 12-31.
Pan, Wuyun. 1982. Concerning some questions in the development of Chinese tones.
Journal of Chinese Linguistics 10. 361-385.
Pearson Education Asia Limited & Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press.
2009. Longman Dictionary of Contemporary English. Beijing: Foreign
Language Teaching and Research Press.
Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun. 2010. Phonological variation in Phuan. MANUSYA:
Journal of Humanities 13.1. 50-87.
Pike, Kenneth L. 1948. Tone languages. Michigan: University of Michigan Press.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn. 2007a. A chronological-approach to subgrouping: The case
of Southwestern Tai. Paper presented at Leipzig Students’ Conference in
Linguistics. Max-Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig,
Germany. March 25-26.
Pittayawat Pittayaporn. 2007b. Directionality of tone change. In Jürgen Trouvain
and William J. Barry (ed.), Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of
Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS XVI), 1421-24. Saarbrücken, Germany: Saarland
University.
238
Pittayawat Pittayaporn. 2009. The phonology of Proto-Tai. New York: Cornell
University dissertation.
Prapin Manomaivibool. 1975. Study of Sino-Tai lexical correspondences. Seattle:
University of Washington dissertation.
Prapin Manomaivibool. 1976. Layers of Chinese loanwords in Thai. In W. Gething J.
G. Harris & P. Kullavanijaya (ed.), Tai linguistics in honor of Fang-Kuei Li, 179184. Bangkok: Central Institute of English Language, Office of State
University.
Pranee Kullavanijaya. 1990. Undoing Homonymy: Cases in Debao Zhuang and Thai.
In Proceedings of The 4th International Conference on Thai Studies, 78-92.
Pranee Kullavanijaya. 1991. Dictionary of Southern Zhuang-Thai. Bangkok:
Chulalongkorn University Press.
Pranee Kullavanijaya & Theraphan L-Thongkum. 1998. Linguistic criteria for
determining Tai ethnic groups: case studies on Central and South-western
Tais. In Proceedings of The International Conference On Tai Studies, July 29-31,
1998, Bangkok.
Pranee Kullavanijaya et al. 1984. ศพท์
ท 6 ภ ษ . [The vocabulary of six Tai
languages]. Bangkok: Faculty of Arts, Chulalongkorn University.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1986. Tone in lexical phonology. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Pulleyblank, E. G. 1962. The consonantal system of Old Chinese. Asia Major 9. 58114 and 206-65.
Pulleyblank, E. G. 1978. The nature of the Middle Chinese tones and their
development to early Mandarin. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 6(2). 173-203.
Pulleyblank, E. G. 1991. Lexicon of reconstructed pronunciation in Early Middle
Chinese, Late Middle Chinese, and Early Mandarin. Vancouver: Unversity of
British Columbia Press.
Ren, Min. 1999. 粤方言区的
调问题. [Issues on tones of Cantonese dialectal areas].
Journal of Foshan Univeristy (Social Science Edition) 17(2). 61-65.
239
Rose, Phil. 2006. Zooming-in on Oujiang Wu: tonal homogeneity and acoustic
reconstruction in a small subgroup of Chinese dialects. Australian
International Conference on Speech Science and Technology 2006. 58-63.
Sagart, Laurent. 1986. On the departing tone. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 14 (1).
90-113.
Sagart, Laurent. 1988. Glottalized tones in China and South-East Asia. In D. Bradley,
E. J. Henderson & M. Mazaudon (ed.), Prosodic analysis and Asian linguistics:
to honour R. K. Sprigg, 83-93. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.
Saussure, Ferdinand de. 1983. Course in General Linguistics. Eds. Charles Bally and
Albert Sechehaye. Trans. Roy Harris. La Salle, Illinois: Open Court.
Sarawit, Mary E. 1973. The Proto-Tai vowel system. Michigan: The University of
Michigan dissertation.
Sarawit, Mary E. 1975. Some changes in the final component of the Tai syllable. In
J. G. Harris & J. R. Chamberlain (ed.), Studies in Tai linguistics in honor of
William J. Gedney, 316-328. Central Institute of English Language.
Sears, Andrew & Julie A. Jacko. 2007. The human-computer interaction handbook:
fundamentals, evolving technologies, and emerging applications, 2nd edition.
New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Group.
Schmidt, Johannes. 1872. Die Verwandtschaftsverhältnisse De Indogermanischen
Sprachen. Weimar: Böhlau.
Shafer, Robert. 1940. The vocalism of Sino-Tibetan. Journal of the American Oriental
Society 60(3). 302-337.
Shafer, Robert. 1944. Problems in Sino-Tibetan phonetics. Journal of the American
Oriental Society 64(3). 137-143.
Shen, Ruiqing & Liao Hanbo. 2012. Acoustic-tonetic study of Pyang Zhuang: An
undescribed Central Tai variety. Paper presented at The 45th International
Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics, Singapore.
Shi, Cunzhi. 1981. An outline of the history of the Chinese speech sounds. Beijing:
The Commerical Press.
240
Shi, Lin. 1992. Distinctive features of tone for the Dong language. Minority Languages
of China 1991(5). 26-34.
Somsonge Burusphat. 2011. Variation of initial consonants in Thai Song. Language
and Culture 29(2). 81-113.
Somsonge Burusphat. 2012. Tones of Thai Song varieties. Journal of the Southeast
Asian Linguistics Societ 5. 32-48.
Strecker, D. 2009. Tai languages. In B. Comrie (ed.), The world’s major languages,
653-659. Abingdon: Routledge.
Tai Chung-pui. 2006. 宋代嶺南文獻中的侗台語詞 [Kam-Dai words in Lingnan
manuscripts of the Song Dynasty]. Minority Languages of China 2006(3). 2125.
Theraphan L-Thongkum. 1997. Implications of the retention of proto-voiced plosives
and fricatives in the Dai Tho language of Yunnan Province for a theory of
tonal development and Tai language classification. In Jerold A. Edmondson &
David B. (ed.), Comparative Kadai: the Tai branch, 191-220. Texas: The
Summer Institute of Linguistics and The University of Texas at Arlington.
Thurgood, Graham. 2002. Vietnamese and tonogenesis: revising the model and the
analysis. Diachronica 19(2). 333-363.
Wang, Stephen S. 1966. Phonology of Chinese loanwords in a northern Tai dialect.
Seattle: University of Washington dissertation.
Wang, William S-Y. 1967. Phonological features of tone. IJAL 33. 93-105.
Wang, William S-Y. 1969. Competing changes as a cause of residue. LG 45(1). 9-25.
Weera Ostapirat. 1994. Speculations on proto Kam-Sui breathy sounds. Kadai:
Discussions in Kadai & Southeast Asian Linguistics 4. 79-87.
Weera Ostapirat. 1999. Proto-Kra. Berkeley: Unviversity of California. dissertation.
Weera Ostapirat. 2006. Alternation of tonal series and the reconstruction of protoKam-Sui. In Dah-An Ho, Samuel Cheung, Wuyun Pan, & Fuxiang Wu (ed.),
Linguistic studies in Chinese and neighboring languages: festschrift in honor of
Professor Pang-hsin Ting on his 70th birthday, 1077-1121. Language and
241
linguistics monograph series, W-6. Taipei: Institute of Linguistics, Academia
Sinica.
Weera Ostapirat. 2013a. Austro-Tai revisited. Paper presented at SEAL23, Bangkok.
Weera Ostapirat. 2013b. The rime system of proto-Tai. Bulletin of Chinese Linguistics
7(1). 189–227.
Wei, Qingwen & Qin Guosheng. 1980. 壮语简志 [Brief overview of the Zhuang
language]. Chinese minority languages brief overview series. Beijing: The
People’s Publishing House.
Woo, Nancy. 1969. Prosody and phonology. Bloomington: Indiana University
dissertation.
Wulff, K. 1934. Chinesisch und Tai: sprachvergleichende Untersuchungen. København
[Copenhagen]: Levin und Munksgaard, Ejnar Munksgaard.
Xu, Tongqiang. 1991. 历史语言学 (第一版) [Historical linguistics, first edition].
Beijing: The Commercial Press.]
Yang, Huandian, Liang Zhenshi, Li Puying & Liu Cunhan. 1985. The distribution of
Chinese dialect in Guangi. Fangyan 1985(3).
Ye, Xiangling. 1983. The tones of the Wujiang dialect. Fangyan 1983 (1).
Yip, Moira. 1980. The tonal phonology of Chinese. Massachusetts Avenue: MIT
dissertation.
Yip, Moira. 2002. Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Yuan, Jiahua. 1960. An outline of Chinese dialects. Beijing: Wenzi Gaige Chubanshe.
Zhang, Jie. 2002. The effects of duration and sonority on contour tone distribution.
New York; London: Routledge.
Zhang, Junru. 1980. 原始台语
母类别探索 [An exploration of the classification of
initial consonants of proto-Tai]. Minority Languages of China 1980(2). 31-40.
Zhang, Junru. 1982. 广西中南部地区
语中的老借词源于汉语古“平话”考 [A
verification that the old Sinitic loans in the Zhuang languages in Central-
242
Southern Guangxi are from ancient Chinese Pinghua]. The Study of
Languages 1982(1). 197-219.
Zhang, Junru, Liang Min, Ouyang Jueya, Zheng Yiqing, Li Xulian & Xie Jianyou.
1999.
语方言研究. [Zhuang dialect research]. (Chinese minority language
dialect research series.) Chengdu: Sichuan Ethnic Publishing House.
Zheng, Yiqing. 1996. 靖西
语研究. [Jingxi Zhuang language research]. Beijing:
Chinese Academy of Social Science, Nationalities Research Institute.
Zhu, Xiaonong & Yang Jianfen. 2010. 嘎裂聲作為低調特徵:河
省方言的聲調考察
[Creaky voice as a feature of [+low]: A tone survey of Hebei dialects]. Hong
Kong University of Science and Technology 2010(7). 134-147.
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Tai_languages (8 May, 2012.)
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Southern_Thai_language (11 June, 2016.)
http://www-01.sil.org/iso639-3/ (17 July, 2013.)
http://www-01.sil.org/iso639-3/scope.asp#I (3 July, 2013.)
https://www.ethnologue.com/about/problem-language-identification (24 April,
2016.)
https://www.ethnologue.com/language/zha (24 April, 2016.)
243
APPENDIX A
QUESTIONNAIRE
Information collecting of Language and Language Resource Person66
Dialect/Language:
Dialect/Language Name
Sub-district
Place of data elicitation
Date
Village
District
Province
Month
Year
LRP’s Name-Last name
Age
Gender
Birth Place: Village
Sub-district
District
Places lived:
1. Village
Province
2. Village
Province
3. Village
Province
4. Village
Province
Sub-district
Period of time
Sub-district
Period of time
Sub-district
Period of time
Sub-district
Period of time
District
Time period of living at the present residence
year(s)
District
year(s)
District
year(s)
District
year(s)
year(s)
Occupation
Education
Dialects/Languages spoken: LRP can speak
1st dialect/language
3rd dialect/language
Birth place of LRP’s father: Village
District
Province
dialects/languages
dialect/language
4th dialect/language
2nd
Sub-district
Province
LRP’s father’s spoken language(s): LRP’s father can speak
dialects/languages
66
This questionnaire is made by Dr. Phinnarat Akharawatthanakun for collecting tone data in Thai
dialects in AL701 Field Methods in Linguistics course, Linguistics Department, Payap University.
244
1st dialect/language
3rd dialect/language
2nd dialect/language
4th dialect/language
Birth place of LRP’s mother: Village
Sub-district
District
Province
LRP’s mother’s spoken language(s): LRP’s mother can speak
1st dialect/language
2nd dialect/language
3rd dialect/language
4th dialect/language
Parents’ place lived:
District
Village
dialects/languages
Sub-district
Province
Spouse 1:
Village
Sub-district
District
Province
Status: living together divorced
passed away
Language spoken: 3
1st dialect/language
2nd dialect/language
3rd dialect/language
4th dialect/language
Spouse 2:
Village
Sub-district
District
Province
Status: living together divorced
passed away
Language spoken:
1st dialect/language
2nd dialect/language
3rd dialect/language
4th dialect/language
Note:
245
APPENDIX B
CHECKLIST
A revised version of Gedney’s tone box (Liao & Shen 2012)
attaching a checklist with examples from three representative
varieties from SWT, CT, and NT respectively
Proto-Initial/Tone Categories
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Voiceless Aspirations
A1-A
B1-A
C1-A
DL1-A
DS1-A
Voiceless Continuants
A1-C
B1-C
C1-C
DL1-C
DS1-C
Voiceless Unaspirated Stops
A1-U
B1-U
C1-U
DL1-U
DS1-U
A1-UC
B1-UC
C1-UC
DL1-UC
DS1-UC
Glottal Sounds
A1-G
B1-G
C1-G
DL1-G
DS1-G
Voiced Sounds
A2
B2
C2
DL2
DS2
A1/2
B1/2
C1/2
DL1/2
DS1/2
Voiceless Unaspirated Stop +
*r Clusters
Proto-Voiced
Proto-Breathy
Voiced Aspirations &
Breathiness
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
* In the following charts of checklist, Bangkok Thai (L1), Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
(L8), and Wuming Shuangqiao Yongbei Zhuang (L30) are varieties representing
Southwestern Tai, Central Tai, and Northern Thai respectively. In the charts, these
three representative languages are abbreviated as Thai, Debao-1, and Wuming
respectively. The tonal values of the lexical items are omitted in the checklist, but
can be consulted in the correcponding languages in Appendix C. There are some
limitation of lexical items in some cases. For example, words are not plenty found as
A tones in the boxes of B1-UC, B1/2, and so on.
246
Checklist of test words in Tai languages:
Column A
Language
A1-A
A1-C
A1-U
A1-UC
A1-G
A2
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
kʰa:
kʰa:
ka:
leg 脚
huə
tʰu:
--
head 头
pʰa:
pʰja:
pla:
cliff (SWT), hill (CT, NT)石山
ha:
kʰja:
ɣa:
to seek
fa:
pʰa:
fa:
lid, cover, wall
ha:ŋ
tʰa:ŋ
ɣiəŋ
tail
tʰɛ:m
tʰe:m
te:m
to add on, in addition添加
ma:
ma:
ma:
dog 狗
na:
na:
na:
thick
ŋa:j
ŋa:j
--
turn face up
--
ɬa:
θa:
paper (CT, NT)
sa:m
ɬa:m
θa:m
three
saj
ɬɔj
θaɰ
clean
净
ka:
ka:
ka:
crow
乌鸦
kin
kin
kɯn
to eat
吃
tu:
tow
tow
door
ta:
ta: (B1-U)
ta:
maternal grandfather
pla:
pja:
pla:
fish
paj
paj
paj
to go 去
ta:
tʰa:
ɣa:
eye
ta:j
tʰa:j
ɣa:j
to die
taj
tʰɔj
taɰ
gizzard 胗 鸡~
tɛ:ŋ
tʰe:ŋ
tiəŋ
cucumber
ka:ŋ
kʰa:ŋ
ka:ŋ
put up, spread out
--
ˀda:
ˀda:
baby sling (CT, NT)
ba:n
ˀba:n
--
bloom
da:w
ˀda:w
ˀda:w
star
ʔa:
ʔa:
ʔa:
father’s younger sister
ja:
ˀja:
ˀjɯ:
medicine
bin
ban
bin
to fly
tʰa:
ta:
ta:
spread on; paint; daub
kʰa:
ka:
ka:
get stuck; stick
247
山崖
找
盖子
墙
尾巴
等
再
厚
仰望
抬头
纸
门
外公
鱼
眼睛
死
黄瓜
~雨伞
婴儿背带
开 花~
星星
药
飞
涂~色
~漆
Column A
Language
A1/2
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
--
pa:
pa:
wife
kʰwa:j
ʋa:j
wa:j
buffalo 水牛
na:
na:
na:
rice field
ma:
ma:
--
to come
mɨ:
mʊŋ
fɯŋ
hand 手
kʰwa:
ɬɔ:
kwa:
right side
hu:
kʰjow
ɣɯ:
ear
fon
pʰɔn
pan
to sharpen; to grind
kʰom
kʰam
ham
bitter
tʰɨŋ
tʰaŋ
taŋ
to arrive
到 ~达
想~
tʰiəŋ
tʰi:ŋ
tiəŋ
field pavilion 亭
棚子
tɛ:n
tʰe:n
tin
hornet; wasp
248
妻子
水田
稻田
来
右
耳朵
磨~刀石
苦 味道~
黄蜂
~尖
Column B
Language
B1-A
B1-C
B1-U
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
pʰa:
pʰa:
pa:
chop, split
kʰa:
kʰa:
--
galangal
kʰaj
kʰjaj
kjaj
egg 蛋
kʰaw
kʰaw
ho:
knee
ha:ŋ
ha:ŋ
ha:ŋ
few and far between 稀疏
hom
ham
hom
to cover up
tʰa:n
tʰa:n
ta:n
charcoal
ma:
ma:
--
soak
nɨəj
nu:j
na:j
tired
wa:n
ʋa:n
wa:n
to snow, to scatter
~米
saŋ
ɬaŋ
θaŋ
to blow one’s nose
擤~鼻涕
soŋ
ɬɔ:ŋ
θoŋ
to send, conduct
si:
ɬej
θej
four
pa:
pa:
--
forest, trees
kaj
kaj
kaj
chicken 鸡
taw
taw
--
turtle , tortoise
tam
tam
tam
low
kɛ:
ke:
ke:
old 老
paw
paw
po:
to blow
吹
--
tʰaw
taw
to hunt
打猎
klɔŋ
kʰjɔ:ŋ
ɣoŋ
cage, box 笼子
ba:
ˀba:
ˀba:
shoulder
da:
ˀda:
ˀda:
to scold 骂
ba:w
ˀba:w
ˀba:w
young man 男青
da:ŋ
ˀdaŋ
ˀdaŋ
potash, lye, alkaline salt
ju:
ˀjow
ˀjaw
to be exist, on, at, in 在
ʔɔ:n
ʔo:n
ʔo:n
tender, soft
tʰa:
ta:
ta:
river (NT, CT), wharf (SWT)
pʰɔ:
po:
po:
father, male
kʰu:
kow
kow
开
南姜
良姜
膝盖
盖~被子
碳
~米
累
衰弱
抛~秧
送
四
森林
龟
矮
B1-UC
B1-G
B2
249
肩膀
帅
软
父亲
性
pair (chopsticks, shoes)
双 ~鞋
对
碱水
~筷子
河
Column B
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
raj
ɹ̥aj
ɣej
farm, dry farmland, upland field
旱田
la:ŋ
B1/2
la:ŋ
ɣiəŋ
旱地
under (SWT), the ground floor
牛栏
老房子的第
容易
层
ŋa:j
ŋa:j
ŋa:j
easy
naw
naw
naw
rot
tʰua
tʰu:
tu:
bean 豆
tʰi:
tʰej
tej
thick, dense
--
tʰow
taɰ
chopsticks
kʰi:
kʰwej
kɯj
to ride on (horse, bicycle)
kʰɨəŋ
kʰy:ŋ
ha:ŋ
young (chicken) 鸡膥
tʰuəŋ
tʰu:ŋ
tuəŋ
pull down 往
tʰɨən
tʰɨ:n (LC)
tɯ:n
wild 野外的、野性的
250
腐烂
浓密
频繁
筷子
扯
骑
小母鸡
Column C
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
pʰa:
pʰa:
--
Gloss
cloth, a sheet
云
kʰa:
C1-A
C1-C
kʰa:
ka:
to kill
tʰa:
ɕa:
to wait, if 等候
ha:
ha:
ha:
five
kʰaj
kʰjaj
--
sick, fever
tʰa:
病怏怏
发烧
to walk
走
haj
hɔj
haɰ
to give
给
na:
na:
na:
face
--
ma:
ma:
to grow up, to branch out生长
ja:
ŋja:
ɳɯ:
grass
--
to swim
游泳
θaj
intestine
肠子
ʋa:j
wa:j
ɬaj
som
ɬam
pa:
pa:
θom
行
脸
sour
草
酸
aunt (parents’ older sister)
pa:
kla:
kja:
kja:
姨
父或母的
seedling 秧苗
ka:w
kaw
kow
nine
ka:ŋ
ka:ŋ
ka:ŋ
fishbone
九
鱼刺
鱼骨头
taŋ
taŋ
to erect, establish
ta:j
tɔj
--
south, below
klaj
kʰjɔj
kjaɰ
near
kɔ:n
kʰo:n
kon(Du’an)
ˀwa:
ʔa:
ʔa:
ˀba:n
ba:n
ˀdaj
da:j
ʔɔ:j
ʔo:j
ˀba:
面
Piece (CLF for stone)
crazy
ʔa:
竖起来
近
石头的量词
ba:
67
五
pla:j
C1-UC
C2
杀
pʰja:j
taŋ
C1-G
~布条
--
saj
C1-U
~云
片状的量词
~石头
疯
to open 张开 ~嘴巴
ˀba:n
ˀdaj
village
村
to get
得到
ʔo:j
sugarcane
ˀdaŋ
to make fire, build fire
甘蔗
daŋ (Isan)
ˀdaŋ
sa-pʰaj
pɔj
paɰ
bride, daughter-in-law新娘
tʰa:w
taw
tɯŋ
crutch
67
(Longsang)
tʰa:wC2 in Thai means ‘foot’.
251
生火
拐杖
媳
Column C
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
kʰiəw
ke:w
ke:w
to chew
ma:
ma:
ma:
horse
马
na:m
nam
ram
water
水
ma:j
maj
faj
tree, wood
ra:j
ɹa:j
ɣa:j
bad, very much
haw
rice
kʰej
haj
excrement
pʰow
pow
kʰa:w
kʰi:
pʰu:
kʰaw
米
树
木头
烂,厉害,非常
饭
大便
person (CLF for person)
人或
C1/2
嚼,咀嚼
个群体的量词
~人 ~族
tʰuəj
tʰu:j
tɯ:i (Xilin)
bowl
su:
ɬow
ɕow
to fight, to be against (SWT); to load,
碗
to carry (CT, NT) 装,盛,放
252
Column DL
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
kʰa:t
kʰa:t
ka:t
Gloss
to be broken, worn out
断线~
DL1-A
烂衣服~
to carry at both ends of a pole
ha:p
tʰa:p
ɣa:p
(na:C1) pʰa:k
pʰja:k
pla:k
挑
forehead
--
to entrust
寄,交付
fɨət (Tianlin)
astringent
苦涩
kʰe:k
he:k
guest
kʰu:p
ho:p
fa:k
fa:t
kʰɛ:k
kʰuəp
pʰa:k
pʰa:t
头
客人
circumference, anniversary
~期
周~
ma:k
ma:k
ma:k
fruits
果
ŋɨək
ŋy:k
hɯ:k
gums
齿龈
nu:k
nuk
deaf 聋
听不见
ɬa:p
θa:p
cockroach
蟑螂
sa:k
pestle
to smart, pierchingly painful
nuək
DL1-C
DL1-U
sa:p
sa:k
ɬa:k
sɛ:p
ɬe:p
θe:p
pa:k
pa:k
pa:k
辣痛,刺痛
mouth 嘴
kɔ:t
ko:t
ko:t
to hug 搂抱
pi:k
pək
--
wing 翅膀
tɔ:k
to:k
to:k
to pound, to hammer in 捶打
ta:t
ta:t
ta:t
cliff, waterfall 悬崖
ka:t
ka:t
ka:t
mustard plant
ta:k
tʰa:k
ta:k
tɛ:k
tʰe:k
te:k
to be broken 碎裂
芥菜
阳
tʰo:k
--
thin bamboo-stripes竹篾,竹条
ba:t
ˀba:t
ˀba:t
scar, time
dɔ:k
ʔa:p
dɛ:t
ˀdu:t
ˀdo:k
ʔa:p
ˀde:t
ˀda:t
hot
--
flower
花
ʔa:p
to bath
洗澡,
ˀde:t
热
sunshine
阳光
水,游泳
晴
to want, to be hungry
饿 肚~
ta:k
ta:k
dry blood sucker
蟥
tɕʰɨək
tɕy:k
ɕa:k
rope
--
pa:k
pa:k
crazy
疯,傻
lɨət
lu:t
lɯət
blood
血
tʰa:k
ˀja:k
次数
ʔiək
ja:k
DL2
瀑布
tɔ:k
dɨət
DL1-G
木杵
to expose under the sun
晒~
DL1-UC
寄付
253
绳
旱
Column DL
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
ra:k
la:k
ra:k
root
ma:k
ma:k
ma:k
full, plenty
mi:t
mət
mit
knife
tʰu:k
tʰʊk
tɯk
pʰɨək
pʰa:k
pu:k
DL1/2
254
根
饱满
匕首
to hit the mark, right, cheap
对 正确,击中
white 白色,苍白
Column DS
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
pʰak
pʰjak
plak
vegetable
pʰat
--
hot, pungent
kʰɔ:t
kut
pʰet
kʰot
DS1-A
DS1-C
DS1-U
DS1-UC
辣
卷曲
偏差 口音~
het
hat
--
to do 做
hap
hap
hap
to shut
(tɕa-) kʰep
(kaj-) kʰjap
θip
centipede
蜈
--
hailstone
冰雹
关闭
hep
tʰap
mat
mat
mat
flea 跳
nak
nak
nak
heavy
ŋok
ŋak
ŋak
nod, shaking, shaky
lap
lap
lap
重
点头
to be late, get to sleep
wat
wat
--
黑 闭眼
have a cold 感冒
sak
ɬak
θak
color
kop
kap
kop
frog 青蛙
kat
kat
kat
to bite, to be very cold
tap
tap
tap
liver
tak
tak
tak
色
田鸡
肝脏
舀~饭
打~饭
刺,插,熬煮
pak
pak
to stab
pet
pat
pit
duck
tak
tʰak
tak
鸭
grasshopper, cricket
tok
tʰɔ:k
tok
蚱蜢 蝗虫
to seed, to sow
dap
ˀdap
ˀdap
to extinguish
ʔok
dip
ʔet
ˀbat
ʔak
ˀdəp
ˀdak
ʔak
ˀdip
ˀdak
fishhook
chest
播种
灭
鱼钩
胸部
unripe, raw
deep night
生~肉
睡着
深夜
ʔit
one
pak
pak
very tired, rest
kʰrok
kjɔ:k
--
mortar
rap
ɹ̥ap
--
to receive, to welcome
me
mat
nat
pʰak
ʔat
ˀbit
十
的“ ”
累
臼 石~
a grain (CLF)
粒
255
啃咬
scoop (rice, alcohol)
pak
dɨk
DS2
蔬菜
to coil, coiled up, twisted
弯曲
bet
DS1-G
菜
颗
量词
~米
接
Column DS
Language
Thai
Debao-1
Wuming
Gloss
mot
mɔ:t
mot
ant
lak
lak
ɕak
to steal
nok
nɔ:k
kʰop
kʰap
sip
suk
DS1/2
sap
ɬəp
ɬʊk
ɬap
ɣok
bird
hap
to bite
ɕip
ten 十
偷
鸟
咬
ɕuk
cooked, ripe
熟~肉
sap
mince
砍碎
256
剁碎
APPENDIX C
LANGUAGE INFORMATION AND TONE DATA
ANALYSIS FROM 42 TAI VARIETIES
The naming principle of Tai varieties investigated follows “location name + language
name”, such as Bangkok Thai (L1) and Khon Kaen Isan (L3). The language names are all
individual language names in the list of ISO 639-3, or a name given by me in the case
that the language has not been listed in ISO 639-3 such as Muong Quan Son Tai (L7,
“Muong” as a location and “Quan Son Tai” as a language name). For distinguishing some
language varieties of the same individual language in the same region, the segment of
location can be separated into two parts, a upper level and a subordinate level, such as
Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (L9, “Debao” as a location name at the county level,
“Dalong” as a village in Debao County, and “Yang Zhuang” as an individual language
name listed in ISO 639-3) and Tiandeng Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang (17, “Tiandeng” as a
county level location, “Xiangdu” as a township in Tiandeng County, and “Zuojiang
Zhuang” as an individual language name listed in ISO 639-3). In the text of this thesis,
the language names can be abbreviated as the individual language names such as Thai
(L1), and the location names such as Debao Dalong (L9). Tai varieties investigated in
this thesis and their numbering are as follows.
1. Seven from SWT (L1-7):
1. Bangkok (Thai, Language 1, abbreviated as L1 and similarly hereinafter)
2. Songkhla (Southern Thai, L2)
3. Khon Kaen (Isan, L3)
4. Chiang Rai (Northern Thai, L4)
5. Kho Lam (Shan, L5)
6. Rong Maet (Tai Lue, L6)
7. Muong (Quan Son Tai, L7)
All the data of these languages are collected by me.
2. Seventeen from CT (L8-L24):
8. Debao Urban (Yang Zhuang, L8)
9. Debao Dalong (Yang Zhuang, L9)
257
10. Debao Lüliu (Yang Zhuang, L10)
11. Jingxi Hurun (Yang Zhuang, L11)
12. Jingxi Urban (Yang Zhuang, L12)
13. Jingxi Anning (Yang Zhuang, L13)
14. Debao Nalong (Myang Zhuang, L14)
15. Debao Tuoxin (Pyang Zhuang, L15)
16. Jingxi Huashan (Min Zhuang, L16)
17. Tiandeng Xiangdu (Zuojiang Zhuang, L17)
18. Daxin Naling (Zuojiang Zhuang, L18)
19. Jingxi Lingding (Zuojiang Zhuang, L19)
20. Xiaoguangnan (Nong Zhuang, L20) (Zhang et al. 1999)
21. Wenshan Dazhai (Dai Zhuang, L21) (Zhang et al. 1999)
22. Daxin Leiping (Zuojiang Zhuang, L22) (Hudak 2008, Zhang et al. 1999)
23. Daxin Baoxu (Zuojiang Zhuang L20)
24. Bac Va (Nung, L24) (Hudak 1997)
Most of them are collected by myself except those with reference, but some of them are
also compared with data of the same languages in published resources. Nong Zhuang,
Dai Zhuang, Leiping, and Bac Vac Nung are referred to published resources.
3. Five from YN (L25-29):
25. Yongning Baiji (L25)
26. Nanning Shuangding (L26)
27. Long’an Xiaolin (L27) (Zhang et al. 1999)
28. Jingxi Daqiu (Nung An, L28)
29. Yongning Xialeng (L29) (Zhang et al. 1999)
YN varieties in this paper are conventionally treated as dialects of Southern Zhuang (SZ)
which is traditionally grouped into CT, because they have plenty aspirated initials which
condition possible secondary tonal splits as the same to those in CT. But at the same
time they present more NT features on voicing alternation, vocalic behaviors, and some
of the initial behaviors. Baiji, Shuangding and Nung An are collected by me; Long’an
Yongnan Zhuang and Xialeng Yongnan Zhuang are referred to published resources.
4. Thirteen from NT (L30-42):
30. Wuming Shuangqiao (Yongbei Zhuang, L30)
31. Debao Longsang (undescribed, L31)
32. Debao Dongling (Youjiang Zhuang, L32)
33. Tianlin Lizhou (Guibian Zhuang, L33) (Zhang et al. 1999)
34. Shanglin Yunling (Central Hongshuihe Zhuang, L34)
35. Du’an Jiaren (Central Hongshuihe Zhuang, L35)
36. Yizhou Suogan (Guibei Zhuang, L36)
258
37. Liujiang Baipeng (Liujiang Zhuang, L37) (Zhang et al. 1999)
38. Donglan Urban (Guibei Zhuang, L38) (Zhang et al. 1999)
39. Qiubei Gehan (Qiubei Zhuang, L39) (Zhang et al. 1999)
40. Lianshan Xiaosanjiang (Lianshan Zhuang, L40) (Zhang et al. 1999)
41. Huishui (Bouyei, L41)
42. Zhenning (Bouyei, L42)
Most of them are collected by me; some of them are also compared with the data of the
same languages in published resources. Tianlin, Donglan, Liujiang, Qiubei, and Lianshan
are referred to published resources.
The following contents concludes the language information obtained from LRPs and the
results of tone data analysis. The first chart under the third item (in all languages
recorded by me) or under the second item (in all languages referenced to published
sources) “Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect” demonstrates the basic
patterns of tonal splits and tonal mergers of that variety. For example, the first chart
under ‘1.3’ of L1 and 4.3 of L4 demonstrate the tone boxes or the basic patterns of tonal
splits and tonal mergers of Bangkok Thai and Chiang Rai Northern Thai respectively. In
those charts, boxes shaded by the same color indicates that they are of the same toneme
or tones in complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Basically, in the same vertical column, register tones with higher pitch are in warm
colors (progressive increasing by yellow, orange, red, crimson and dark red from
Columns A - DS respectively), while register tones with lower pitch are in cool colors
(cyan, green, blue, mazarine, and dark blue from Columns A to DS respectively).
However, because there must be tonal mergers between different tones in a Tai variety,
all tonemes of the same tone must be in the same color with their allotone in the very
first former box from higher rows to lower row and from left to right columns.
Therefore, the dark red and dark blue are actually never found in any of the tone boxes
because all tones on dead syllables must be an allotone of a tone on smooth syllables.
For example, in 1.3 (L1 or Bangkok Thai), the orange color has been used to mark the
B2 tone first in Column B, and its following allotones C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
and DL2 have to be marked in the same orange color, in spite of that they should be
marked in blue and mazarine respectively when being compared with the other register
tones in the same columns.
Note: I got permission from all the LRPs to open their biodata in my thesis which will be
able to be downloaded online.
259
1. Language 1 Bangkok, Thai, SWT
1.1 Language information
Language
Thai
ISO 639-3 Code
tha
Autonym
Thai
Exonym
Central Thai; Standard Thai; Siamese
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Bangkok, Thailand.
1.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
Place of data elicitation
Betta House, Nimmanhaemin 5, Chiang Mai, Thailand.
Wirawit
Given Name
November 24, 2013
Family Name
วรวชญ์
Gender
Phonwattanakaisert
พรวฒน กร ลศ
male
Age
27
Birth Place
Sathon District, Bangkok, Thailand.
Places living
Before he moved to Chiang Mai, he had been living in Bangkok from his birth.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
physician
3 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Bangkok Thai
Mother Tongue
English
Intermediate
bachelor
Language Remarks
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Bangkok. He speaks Bangkok Thai and Teochew.
LRP’s mother was born in Bangkok. She can speak Bangkok Thai, Teochew and English.
LRP’s parents are living together in Bangkok now.
Remarks
Although the LRP was born in a Chinese Teochew family, Bangkok Thai is his only mother tongue. In
Thailand, particularly in Bangkok, Teochew is still spoken among older ethnic Chinese Thai citizens;
however, the younger generation tends to learn Standard Chinese as a third language after Thai and
English, and Teochew seems to has become an endangered language due to the inheriting gap.
260
1.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones except tone A.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Proto-
Aspirations
A1-A: 24
B1-A: 21
C1-A: 41ʔ
DL1-A: 21
DS1-A: 21
voice-
Continuants
A1-C: 24
B1-C: 21
C1-C: 41ʔ
DL1-C: 21
DS1-C: 21
less
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 33
B1-U: 21
C1-U: 41ʔ
DL1-U: 21
DS1-U: 21
A1-UC: 33
B1-UC: 21
C1-UC: 41ʔ
DL1-UC: 21
DS1-UC: 21
A1-G: 33
B1-G: 21
C1-G: 41ʔ
DL1-G: 21
DS1-G: 21
A2: 33
B2: 41ʔ
C2: 453ʔ
DL2: 41
DS2: 45/55
A1/2: 24
B1/2: 21
C1/2: 41ʔ
DL1/2: 21
DS1/2: 21
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are summarized as in the following two charts.
Vertically: Two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The nonstraightforward column is A, which has a split between A1-C and A1-U.
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
A[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C,
1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[2] = C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[2]
C[2] = DS[2]
261
1.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2].
Tone 3:
Low-falling 21 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], as well as in DL[1A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-falling 41 occurs in C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2] and in DL[2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in C[2], and high-rising 45 or high level 55
occurs in DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
1.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect:
Tones C1 (as well as B2 which merges into C1) and C2 have glottal constrictions,
like tʰa:41ʔ ‘wharf’, ha:41ˀ ‘five’, fa:j41ˀ ‘cotton’, sɨə41ˀ ‘shirt’, ka:ŋ41ˀ ‘fishbone’, tʰa:w453ˀ
‘foot’, and na:m453ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
1.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) The term ja:33 ‘medicine’ is reflected as having a low register consonant j- (< *j-)
by the Thai orthography ยา, but it should have had a high register consonant j(< *ʔj-) in Row 1-G due to the data comparison with other Tai varieties.
2) The term kʰa:41ʔ ‘to kill’ is reflected as having a low register consonant kʰ- (< *g) by the Thai orthography ฆา, but it should have had a high register consonant
kʰ- (< *q- (cf. Pittayaporn 2009: 105, 357)) in Row 1-G due to the data
comparison with other Tai varieties.
262
2. Language 2 Songkhla, Southern Thai, SWT
2.1 Language information
Language
Southern Thai
ISO 639-3 Code
sou
Autonym
Pak Tai
Exonym
Dambro
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Muang District, Songkhla Province, Thailand.
2.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
December 23, 2013
Place of data
Betta House Café, Nimmanhaemin Soi 5, Chiang Mai Province, Thailand.
elicitation
Given Name
non³³ tʰaˀ³³ wat45 นนทวัช
Family Name
kə:t21 tʰip45 เกิดทิพย์
Gender
male
Age
27
Birth Place
Muang District, Songkhla Province, Thailand.
Places lived
When he was 22 years, he moved to Chiang Mai from his hometown.
Time period of living at the present residence
5 years in Chiang Mai
Occupation
Personal business
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Southern Thai
Mother Tongue
Standard Thai
Fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
English
Intermediate
Mandarin
A little
This language is used for his personal business when
he makes contact with his Chinese customers.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language
background of LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Muang District of Songkhla Province. His mother tongue is Songkhla
Southern Thai. He also speaks Standard Thai.
LRP’s mother was born in Nakhon Si Thammarat Province. Her mother tongue is Nakhon Si
Thammarat Southern Thai. She also speaks Standard Thai.
263
2.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Phonation types
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 41
B1-A: 41
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 55
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 41
B1-C: 41
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 454
B1-U: 454
C1-U: 44
DL1-U: 44
DS1-U: 45
A1-UC: 454
B1-UC: 454
C1-UC: 44
DL1-UC: 44
DS1-UC: 45
A1-G: 454
B1-G: 454
C1-G: 44
DL1-G: 44
DS1-G: 45
A2: 31
B2: 213
C2: 22
DL2: 213
DS2: 22
A1/2: 41
B1/2: 41
C1/2: 55
DL1/2: 55
DS1/2: 55
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: three way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Although the
splits between voiceless and voiced sounds are straightforward, but there is another secondary split
between voiceless frictions (aspiration and continuants) and voicelss unaspiration (unaspirated
stops, unaspriated stop + *r, and glottal sounds) to make the third register split in all the five tonal
categories.
Horizontally: one individuals and six mergers
Individual
A[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] = B[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
A[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = B[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS [1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2] = DL[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] = DS [1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2] = DS[2]
264
2.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 41 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] and B [1-A, 1-C, 1/2].
Tone 2:
High-rising-falling 454 occurs in A[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and B[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], as
well as high-rising 45 in DS [1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2].
Tone 4:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in B[2] and DL[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1/2], as well as in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] and
DS[1-A, 1-C, 1/2], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
High level 44 occurs in C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DL[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 7:
Low level 22 occurs in C[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
2.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect: There are no
phonation types of voice qualities found in tones in this variety.
265
3. Language 3 Khon Kaen, Isan, SWT
3.1 Language information
Language
Isan
ISO 639-3 Code
tts
Autonym
Lao
Exonym
Isan; Northeastern Thai; Lao
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Chun Phae, Khon Kaen Province, Thailand.
3.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
October 12, 2013
Place of data elicitation
tʰuŋ⁴¹ luəŋ³³ tʰɔ:ŋ³³ village, Chun District, Phayao Province, Thailand.
Given Name
na:j³³ tʰaj³³
Family Name
kʰiəw²⁴ mi:³³
Gender
male
Age
61
Birth Place
Muang District, Pitsanulok Province, Thailand.
Places lived
When he was 3 months, he moved to Chum Phae District, Khon Kaen with his mother,
and lived there for 10 years until 1962 (2505). After that he left to Rong Maet Village
(next to the village of the present residence) in Chun District of Phayao Province
which was a district of Chiang Rai Province at that time for four years. After that he
moved to the present residence until now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
51 years
Education
First year of senior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Remarks
Isan
Mother Tongue
Northern Thai
Fluent
This language is the lingua franca in the LRP’s area.
Standard Thai
Fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Thai Lue
familiar
LRP is familiar with Tai Lue since it is spoken in the next
village Rong Maet.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in That village, Phu Khiaw District, Chai Ya Phum Province. He probably can
speak Isan and Thai, and dead before he left Khon Kaen.
LRP’s mother was born in Sam Phan village, Khua Liang Khet District, Chum Phae District, Khon Kaen
Province. She can speak Isan and Thai.
LRP’s mother is living with his younger brother and younger sister in Chaiya Phuek Village, Don Sila
Sub-district, Wiang Chai District, Chiang Rai Province now.
Remarks
Although the LRP lives in Chun District of Phayao Province where local Isan/Lao speakers live, he insists
his Isan variety is of Chum Phae, Khon Kaen Province.
266
3.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 23
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 21ʔ
DL1-A: 21
DS1-A: 23
Continuants
A1-C: 23
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 21ʔ
DL1-C: 21
DS1-C: 23
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 23
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 42ʔ
DL1-U: 21
DS1-U: 23
A1-UC: 23
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 42ʔ
DL1-UC: 21
DS1-UC: 23
A1-G: 23
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 42ʔ
DL1-G: 21
DS1-G: 23
A2: 45
B2: 33
C2: 42ʔ
DL2: 42
DS2: 45/55
A1/2: 23
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 21ʔ
DL1/2: 21
DS1/2: 23
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Checked Syllable
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The nonstraightforward columns are B and C. The column B has no spilts at all. The column C have a unique
two wait split: voiceless aspirations, voicelss continuants and voicing alternation serials on one register,
and voiceless unaspirations, voicelss unaspirated stops + *r-, golttal sound, and voiced register on the
other register. The merger that golttal sound goes with voiced sounds on C tone is often found in NT
varieties, but is very rare found in Tai varieties developing from ST (CT/SWT). Northeastern Thai as
well as its most closed Lao varieties represent this unique pattern of tonal split and merger.
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[2] = DS[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2] = DL[2]
267
3.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 23 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] as well as in DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
High-rising 45 occurs in A[2] and high-rising 45 or high level 55 in DS[2]
to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
Tone 4:
Low-falling 21 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2] and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 42 occurs in C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2], and in DL[2], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
3.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect: Tones C1 and C2
have glottal constrictions, like ha:21ˀ ‘five’, fa:j21ˀ ‘cotton’, siə²¹ˀ ‘shirt’, ka:ŋ41ˀ
‘fishbone’, da:m41ˀ ‘handle’, and nam41ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions are not
phonemic.
3.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) The term muan42 ‘to gargle’ has an unexpected initial consonant m-, which is
expected to be b- of the glottal sounds row (C1-G) in the tone box designated, vs.
buan C1-G ‘to gargle’ in Thai and ʔunC1-G ‘to gargle’ in Debao Yang Zhuang. However,
its tone is kept in the C1-G row.
3.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) The term ba:w
33
‘kite’ has an unexpected initial consonant b-, which is expected
to be w- of the voiced row (B2) in the tone box designated.
268
4. Language 4 Chiang Rai, Northern Thai, SWT
4. 1 Language information
Language
Northern Thai
ISO 639-3 Code
nod
Autonym
kamA2 mɨəŋA2
Exonym
Tai Yuan; Lanna
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Chiang Mai, Thailand.
4.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
December 12, 2013
Mae Tam Tai Village, Mae Tam Sub-district, Phaya Meng Rai District,
Chiang Rai Province.
Given Name
Nawatchai นวตรชย
Family Name
Kabchan ก บจนทร์
Gender
male
Age
33
Birth Place
Mae Tam Tai Village, Mae Tam Sub-district, Phaya Meng Rai District, Chiang Rai
Province. บ น มต๋ ต ต. มต๋ อ.พญ ม็งร ย ชยงร ย
Places lived
He moved from Mae Tam Tai Village to the main town of Phaya Meng Rai District
when he was 13. He went to Bangkok to study in Ramkhamhaeng University for 4
years. After graduating, he went back to his hometown to live with his parents until
now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
20 years
Personal business
Languages spoken
Level
Northern Thai
Mother Tongue
Standard Thai
Near Native
Education
bachelor
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
English
Intermediate
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were both born in Mae Tam Tai Village, Mae Tam Sub-district, Phaya Meng Rai District,
Chiang Rai Province. His parents speak Northern Thai as their mother tongue and speaks Standard Thai
to outlanders. They both live together with the LRP now.
269
4.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 24
B1-A: 22
C1-A: 44ʔ
DL1-A: 22
DS1-A: 24
Continuants
A1-C: 24
B1-C: 22
C1-C: 44ʔ
DL1-C: 22
DS1-C: 24
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24
B1-U: 22
C1-U: 44ʔ
DL1-U: 22
DS1-U: 24
A1-UC: 24
B1-UC: 22
C1-UC: 44ʔ
DL1-UC: 22
DS1-UC: 24
A1-G: 335
B1-G: 22
C1-G: 44ʔ
DL1-G: 22
DS1-G: 24
A2: 335
B2: 31
C2: 53ʔ
DL2: 31
DS2: 35
A1/2: 24
B1/2: 22
C1/2: 44ˀ
DL1/2: 22
DS1/2: 23
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-G, 2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The nonstraightforward column is A, which has a split between A1-UC and A1-G.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[G, 2] = DS[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
270
4.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2] as well as in DS [1-A, 1-C,
1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid level-rising 335 occurs in A[1-G, 2] and mid-rising 35 in DS[2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Low level 22 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U,
1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2] and DL[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 44 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
High-falling 53 occurs in C[2].
4.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have glottal constrictions, like ha:55ˀ ‘five’, fa:j55ˀ ‘cotton’, sɨə55ˀ
‘shirt’, ka:ŋ55ˀ ‘fishbone’, paj53ˀ ‘bride’, and nam53ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions
are not phonemic.
271
5. Language 5 Kho Lam, Tai Yai, SWT
5.1 Language information
Language
Shan
ISO 639-3 Code
shn
Autonym
Kam Tai
Exonym
Shan; Tai Yai
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Kho Lam Village, Taunggyi City, Shan State, Myanmar
5.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
October 12, 2013
Place of data elicitation
Betta House, Nimmanhaemin 5, Chiang Mai, Thailand.
Given Name
Lungta
Family Name
Lungkham
Gender
male
Age
50
Birth Place
kʰo:24 lam24 ( ขวด๋ in Thai) Village, Taunggyi District, Shan State, Myanmar.
Places lived
Before he moved to Chiang Mai, Thailand when he was 42, he only stayed in his
village.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
Construction
8 years
Education
He has never gone to a school.
builder
Languages spoken
Level
Shan
Mother Tongue
Thai
Fluent
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of Thailand, in
where the LRP is working.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same village of LRP’s. They spoke Shan and Thai. Both of them passed
away.
Remarks
There are many Shan migrant workers in Thailand, especially in Chiang Mai which is the largest city in
Northern Thailand and is relatively closed to their hometowns in Shan State of Myanmar. Most of them
grow up in their villages, and come to Thailand for their migrant work when they are in teen-age
youths. Although Shan is their mother tongue, most of them also speak Standard Thai as their main
occupational language. Most of them have a chance to learn Thai in the vocational schools built by their
communities in some cities in Thailand.
It is not clear that Kho Lam is of what level of the administration of Myanmar. The LRP claimed that
Kho Lam was a single village, and he did not know the relationship of administrative subordination
272
between Kho Lam and other cities and districts in Shan State of Myanmar. However, the map of
Myanmar shows that Kho Lam is a transportation junction within Taunggyi District. According to
Wikipedia, states and divisions in Myanmar are divided into districts (ခရု င;် kha yaing or khayaing, IPA:
[kʰəjàɪɴ]). These districts consist of townships (မ န
့ ယ်; myo-ne, IPA: [mjo̰nɛ̀]) that include towns (မ ့;
myo, IPA: [mjo̰]), wards (ရပ်ကွက်; yatkwet, IPA: [jaʔ kwɛʔ])) and village-tracts (ကက းရွ ာအုပ်စု; kyayywa
oksu, IPA: [tɕ́ jwà ʔoʊʔ sṵ]). Village-tracts are groups of adjacent villages (ကက းရွ ာ; kyayywa, IPA: [tɕ́
jwà]). Nevertheless, the LRP did not provide any clear information that what level of these divisions Kho
Lam should be of. It reflects that the common Shan people are not entitled to a regular or favorable
education system, which would have given them the necessary knowledge of the administrative
subordination of their own regions.
5.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 24
B1-A: 11
C1-A: 33ʔ
DL1-A: 11
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 24
B1-C: 11
C1-C: 33ʔ
DL1-C: 11
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24
B1-U: 11
C1-U: 33ʔ
DL1-U: 11
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 24
B1-UC: 11
C1-UC: 33ʔ
DL1-UC: 11
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 24
B1-G: 11
C1-G: 33ʔ
DL1-G: 11
DS1-G: 55
A2: 45
B2: 33ʔ
C2: 41ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 41
A1/2: 24
B1/2: 11
C1/2: 33ʔ
DL1/2: 11
DS1/2: 33
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
273
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
A[2] =DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]= DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[2] = C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[2]
C[2] = DS[2]
5.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 2:
High-rising 45 occurs in A[2] and High level 55 in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Low level 11 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DL [1-A,
1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[2], C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], and
DL[2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 41 occurs in C[2] and in DS[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
5.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 (as well as B2 which merges into C1) and C2 have glottal constrictions,
like pɔ:33ʔ ‘father’, ha:33ˀ ‘five’, fa:j33ˀ ‘cotton’, ka:ŋ33ˀ ‘fishbone’, paɰ33ˀ ‘bride’, and
nam41ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
5.6 Linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) As in Tai Lue and Quan Son Tai which are of Group P of SWT (cf. Chamberlain
1975), this Shan variety reflect unaspirated stops in the low register (A2, B2, C2,
DL2, DS2), to contrast to Group PH (including Thai, Southern Thai and Isan) (cf.
§2.2.4). Comparing with Northern Thai, although both Shan and Northern Thai
reflect the same unaspirated initials in B2, C2 and DL2, but different in A2, which
allow aspirated in Northern Thai, e.g. Northern Thai kʰwa:jA2 ‘buffalo’, kʰonA2
‘person’ and tʰajA2 ‘Thai’ vs. Shan kwa:jA2 ‘buffalo’, konA2 ‘person’ and tajA2
‘Tai/Thai’.
2) PT *ʔb- and *ʔd- becomes w-/m- and l- in Shan in most cases, such as winA1 ‘to fly’
and liaŋA1 ‘red’.
274
6. Language 6 Rong Maet, Tai Lue, SWT
6.1 Language information
Language
Tai Lü
ISO 639-3 Code
khb
Autonym
tajA2 lɯ:C2
Exonym
Thai Lue; Tai Le; Dai Le
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Rong Maet, Chun District, Phayao Province, Thailand.
6.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
Place of data elicitation
Rong Maet (rɔ:ŋ⁴¹ mɛt⁴¹) village, Chun District, Phayao Province
Given Name
sa:²⁴ taʔ²¹ nan³³
Family Name
sɛŋ²⁴ si:²⁴ tɕan³³
Gender
male
Age
33
Birth Place
Rong Maet village, Chun District, Phayao Province
Places lived
Muang District of Lam Pang Province (junior college 2 years).
April 18, 2013
Bangkok (4 years)
Seoul of South Korea (6 years).
Chiang Mai (2 years)
Time period of living at the present residence
2 years
Occupation
freelancer
Education
Junior College
Languages spoken
Level
Tai Lü
mother tongue
Northern Thai
native
This language is the lingua franca in the LRP’s area.
Standard Thai
near native
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Isan
familiar
LRP is familiar with Isan since it is spoken in the same village.
Korean
intermediate
LRP is familiar with Korean since he used to live in South Korea
Language Remarks
for six years. He can also read and write Korean.
English
a little
He can only read some written English.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. He can speak Tai Lue, Northern Thai, and Thai.
LRP’s mother was born in the same village. She can speak Tai Lue, Northern Thai, and Thai.
LRP’s parents live together in the same village now.
Remarks
LRP’s village Rong Maet is a multilingual village. Northern Thai, Tai Lue, and Isan are spoken in Moo 7,
Moo, 4 and Moo 9 which are three different sub-villages of this village respectively. Northern and Lao
speakers also live together in Moo 11 of the same village. Northern Thai is the LWC of this village, and is
used in the conversation among speakers of the different aforementioned languages.
275
6.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation
Checked Syllable
types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 55
B1-A: 35
C1-A: 13ʔ
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 45/55
Proto-
Continuants
A1-C: 55
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 13ʔ
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 45/55
Voiceless
Unaspirated
A1-U: 55
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 13ʔ
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 45/55
A1-UC: 55
B1-UC: 35
C1-UC: 13ʔ
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 45/55
A1-G: 55
B1-G: 35
C1-G: 13ʔ
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 45/55
B2: 33
C2: 11ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 33
B1/2: 35
C1/2: 13ʔ
DL1/2: 35
DS1/2: 45/55
stops
Sounds
Unaspirated
stop + *r
clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
A2:
31/335
A1/2: 55
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: three individuals and four mergers
A[2]
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2] = DS[2]
276
6.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High level [55] occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], as well
as high level [55] or high-rising [45] in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1G, 1/2], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid-falling [31] or its free variation mid level-rising [335]
(probably due to Northern Thai influence) occurs in A[2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising [35] occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], and in
DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid level [33] occurs in B[2], as well as in DL[2] and DS[2] to form
a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising [13] occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Low level [11] occurs in C[2].
6.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have glottal constrictions, like ha:13ˀ ‘five’, fa:j13ˀ ‘cotton’, siə13ˀ
‘shirt’, ka:ŋ13ˀ ‘fishbone’, da:m13ˀ ‘handle’, and nam11ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
6.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
When the item with A2 tone pronounced individually, the tone has a mid-falling
[31] contour, but when the item is on an utterance, it can preserve mid-falling [31],
or change to a mid-level-rising contour [335]. The environment of this tone
coarticulation needs more data to study with.
6.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
In this language, stops developed from earlier voiced stops mostly present as
unaspirated, like ka:A2 ‘to stuck’ and ta:A2 ‘to spread on’. However, some items with
expected unaspirated initial stops unexpectedly have an aspirated stop, like kʰunA2
‘person’ provided by the LRP. The reason may be that some of his pronunciations are
influenced by Standard Thai or Northern Thai, which have aspirated stops developed
from earlier voiced stops, like kʰonA2 ‘person’ in Thai and Northern Thai.
277
7. Language 7 Quan Son Tai, SWT
7.1 Language information
Language
Quan Son Tai
ISO 639-3 Code
unknown
Autonym
tajA2
Exonym
Tai Lam, Tai Dam, Black Tai, Phớ Tày, Kun Tày,
Phú tày
Data Source
1 LRPs
Variety Name
Muong (meaning: mango) Village, Quan Son District, Thanh Hoa Province, Vietnam.
7.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
November 18, 2015
Place of data elicitation
Quan Son Town, Quan Son District, Thanh Hoa Province, Vietnam
Given Name
Anh Tuan
Family Name
Pham
Gender
male
Age
32
Birth Place
Muong village, Quan Son District, Thanh Hoa Province, Vietnam
Places lived
The Muong, Lam and Na Meo villages and Quan Son Town of Quan Son district, Thanh
Hoa Province. Ha Noi (3 years). Hue city, Thua Thien Hue Province. Long Xuyen City,
An Giang Province. Ho Chi Minh City .
Bangkok Thailand (3 years)
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
District Financial staff
9 years
Education
Master
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Quan Son Tai
mother tongue
Another Tày đắm
Near native
This language is his wife’s mother tongue.
Vietnamese
near native
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Lao
familiar
LRP is familiar with Lao since his hometown is closed to the
variety in Quan Son
Vietnam-Laos border, and his ethnic group is also similar to
Lao.
Thai
familiar
LRP is familiar with Thai since he used to study for his
postgraduation in Bangkok Thailand for two years.
English
intermediate
He used to study his master program in English when he
studied in Thailand.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Muong village, Quan Son District, Thanh Hoa Province, Vietnam. He can speak
Quan Son Tai and Vietnamese.
LRP’s mother was born in Muong village, Quan Son District, Thanh Hoa Province, Vietnam. She can
speak Quan Son Tai, Vietnamese and Lao (Her mother is a Laotian).
278
Remarks
Together with most other SWT language varieties, this language is officially classified as Thai in
Vietnam. Therefore, non-Tai ethnic group like Kinh and Muong usually call them Thái. The Thai
speaking groups make up 83% of the population of this district, and the others are Kinh, Muong, and
Hmong. Among the 13 Thai communities in that area, the sounds and vocabularies are little different.
Autonyms Phớ Tày, Kun Tày, and Phú tày are commonly used. However, speakers of this language claim
they are of Tày đắm (black Tai) since other Tai groups around them like Red Tai and White Tai (Thái đỏ,
Thái trắng) call them in this way. In Thanh Hoa province Red Thai live in Lang Chanh district, next to
this group. White Tai live in Nhu Thanh district, about 100 km from this group.
7.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 453
B1-A: 55
C1-A: 35ʔ
DL1-A: 55
DS1-A: 41
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 55
C1-C: 35ʔ
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 41
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 55
C1-U: 35ʔ
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 41
A1-UC: 453
B1-UC: 55
C1-UC: 35ʔ
DL1-UC: 55
DS1-UC: 41
A1-G: 453
B1-G: 55
C1-G: 35ʔ
DL1-G: 55
DS1-G: 41
A2: 312
B2: 35ʔ
C2: 41ʔ
DL2: 35
DS2: 41
A1/2: 453
B1/2: 55
C1/2: 35ʔ
DL1/2: 55
DS1/2: 41
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development with a register merger on
DS tone.
279
Horizontally: two individuals and three mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
A[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[2] = C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[2]
C[2] = DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
7.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling-rising 312 occurs in A[2].
Tone 3:
High level 55 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], to form a complementary distribution in smooth
and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[2], and in C1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 41 occurs in C[2], and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2],
to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
7.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 (including B2 which merges into C1) and C2 have final glottal
constrictions, like ta:35ʔ (B2) ‘wharf’, ha:35ʔ (C1) ‘five’, saj35ʔ (C1) ‘intestine’, maj41ʔ
(C2) ‘tree’, and nam41ʔ (C2) ‘water’. When being pronounced individually, terms with
C2 tone have a clear the glottal final stop which is slightly released even after a
nasal coda. All the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
7.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The term ʔa:55 ‘father’s younger sister’ has an unexpected tone B1-G, which is
expected to be A1-G in the tone box designated, vs. ʔa:A1-G ‘father’s younger sister’
in Thai and in Debao Yang Zhuang. This tone may have undergone an analogical
change according to the frequency of phonological contamination on Tai kinship
terms.
2) The term la:35ʔ ‘to scold’ has an unexpected tone B2, which is expected to be B1-G
in the tone box designated, vs. da: B1-G ‘to scold’ in Thai and ˀda:B1-G ‘to scold’ in
Debao Yang Zhuang. The cause of the tone change needs more data to determine
with.
280
3) The term la:ŋ55 ‘ground floor’ has an unexpected tone B1, which is expected to be
B2 in the tone box designated, vs. la:ŋB2 ‘under, below’ in Thai and la:ŋB2 ‘ground
floor’ in Debao Yang Zhuang. The cause of the tone change needs more data to
determine with.
4) The term laŋ453 ‘to make fire’ has an unexpected tone A1-G, which is expected to
be C1-G in the tone box designated, vs. ˀdaŋC1-G in Longsang (NT) and Debao Yang
Zhuang (CT). This term may have undergone an analogical change or
phonological contamination by replacing its original *C tone to *A tone from *vɛjA
‘fire’ (cf. Li 1977: 79, 286) or *wɤjA ‘fire’ (fl. Pittayaporn 2009: 207) according to
the frequently used compound word *ʔdaŋC *vɛjA/*wɤjA ‘to make fire’.
5) The term fu:453 ‘CLF of person’ has an unexpected tone A1/2, which is expected to
be C1/2 in the tone box designated, vs. pʰu:C1/2 in Thai (SWT), pʰowC1/2 in Debao
Yang Zhuang (CT), and pu:C1/2 in Dongling Youjiang Zhuang (NT). This term may
have been undergoing an analogical change or phonological contamination by
replacing its original *C tone to *A tone from *ɣu̯ɨnA ‘person, human being’ (cf. Li
1977: 215, 272) or *ɢwɯnA ‘person, human being’ (cf. Pittayaporn 2009: 283)
since it has frequently been being used to classify *ɣu̯ɨnA or*ɢwɯnA ‘person, human
being’, cf. Debao Yang Zhuang (CT) θa:mA1 pʰowC1/2 kɔ:nA2 ‘three people’ and
Dongling Youjiang Zhuang (NT) ɬa:mA1 pu:C1/2 hunA2 ‘three people’.
6) The term tʰɯ:k35 (DL1/2) ‘to hit the mark, correct’ has an unexpected tone 35
which is merged into DL2, to have the same merging pattern as NT varieties. This
is different from other SWT and CT varieties, which have the tone merging into
DL1 for this cognate, such as tʰu:k35 (DL1/2>DL1) ‘to hit the mark, correct’ in Thai
(SWT) and tʰʊk33 (DL1/2>DL1) ‘to hit the mark’ in Debao County Yang Zhuang
(CT). The cause of the tone change needs more data to determine with.
7) The term ʔaŋ453 (A1-G) ‘chest’ has an unexpected tone A1-G which is expected to
be DS1-G and unexpected syllable final -ŋ which is expected to be -k according to
the cognate in other Tai varieties, cf. ʔokDL1-G ‘chest’ in Thai (SWT), ʔakDL1-G ‘chest’
in Debao Yang Zhuang (CT), and ʔakDL1-G ‘chest’ in Du’an Hongshuihe Zhuang
(NT). The term in Quan Son Tai may not be cognate with the counterpart terms in
other Tai varieties, or has been underwent a process of aberrant development
which need more data to determine with.
7.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) All pre-glottalized initial consonants in this dialect have merged into their
homorganic sonorants or fricatives, and the processes are as ʔb- > ʋ-, *ʔd- > l-,
and *ʔj- > ð-, like in ʋa:nC1-G ‘village’, la:wA1-G ‘star’, and ða:A1-G ‘medicine’.
281
2) The meaning of the term ʋa:j35ʔ (C1-C) ‘to float’ seems to have processed a
semantic change from ‘to swim’, because the cognate in all other Tai varieties
investigated has the meaning ‘to swim’. Examples with this term are as ʋa:j35ʔ (C1C) hɯə322 (A2) ‘to float a boat’ and ʋa:j35ʔ (C1-C) pɛ:31 (A2) ‘to float a raft’.
3) The term he:35ʔ (C1-UC) ‘near’ has an unexpected initial consonant h-, which is
expected to be k- according to the vast majorities of SWT. However, when
comparing to CT and NT varieties like kʰjɔj24ʔ ‘near’ in Debao Yang Zhuang (CT),
ɕaɰ24ʔ ‘near’ in Tiandeng Zuojiang Zhuang (CT), tɕaɰ213ʔ ‘near’ in Longsang Zhuang
(NT), and kjaɰ55 ‘near’ in Wuming Shuangqiao Yongbei Zhuang (NT), as well as to
the reconstructed forms *krəɰC by Li (1977: 225, 289) or *k.raɰC by Pittayaporn
(2009: 452), the modern initial consonant h- in this dialect is not a single evidence
and presents a unique historical change developing from *kr- or *k.r- within SWT
varieties.
4) The term kə- ʋa:35ʔ (C1-G) ‘crazy’ has an unexpected prefix kə-, which makes the
initial consonant sound very like a sesquisyllabic consonant cluster k.ʋ-. The LRP
confirmed that this prefix-like kə- or k- cannot be taken out from this term, or it
would be homophonous with ʋa:35ʔ (B2) ‘to talk’, since tone C1 and B2 are merged
into each other in this dialect. The LRP also claimed that this prefix-like kə- or khas no any other meaning and there is no any other term in his language has this
segment kə- or k-. This is very unique since all other SWT varieties investigated in
this paper have a consonant b- or ˀb- reflecting a PT *ʔb- or *ɓ- for this term, such
as ba:41ˀ (C1-G) ‘crazy’ in Thai, ba:55ˀ (C1-G) ‘crazy’ in Northern Thai and in ˀba:13ʔ
(C1-G) ‘crazy’ Tai Lue. But when comparing with the cognate in some CT varieties
like ˀwa:24ʔ (C1-G) ‘crazy’ in Debao Yang Zhuang and ʋa:33ʔ (C1-G) ‘crazy’ in Jingxi
Yang Zhuang68, the reconstruction of the initial consonant of this term should be
reconsidered to be *ʔ.w- instead of *ʔb- or *ɓ-. This is because the form kə-ʋa:35ʔ
(C1-G) ‘crazy’ in Quan Son Tai accounts for a direction on initial consonant *ʔ.w-
>*k.w- > kə.ʋ- or k.ʋ- since the change from a glottal stop segment ʔ- to a velar
stop segment k- is possible, and only the PT form *ʔ.w- reflected by kə.ʋ- or k.ʋ- in
Quan Son Tai and ˀw- in Debao Yang Zhuang is able to explain all the following
directions: *ʔ.w- >*ʔw- > *ʔb- > b-/ˀb- in SWT, *ʔ.w- >*k.w- > kə.ʋ- in Quan
68
The initial consonant form ʋ- in Jingxi Yang Zhuang presents a merging direction from
ˀw- to ʋ- when comparing with the form in Debao Yang. Other paralleled phenomena in these
two Yang Zhuang varieties also suggest this merging process, like ʋa:nA1 (Debao) vs. ʋa:nA1
(Jingxi) ‘sweet’, ʋɔ:nA2 (Debao) vs. ʋanA2 (Jingxi) ‘day’, and ʋa: C2 (Debao) vs. ʋa:C2 (Jingxi)
‘and’, however ru:ŋA2 ˀwa:A1 (Debao) vs. lu:ŋ A2 ʋa:A1 (Jingxi) ‘rainbow’, ˀwe:B1 (Debao) vs. ʋe:B1
(Jingxi) ‘to turn round’, and ˀwi:nB1 (Debao) vs. ʋi:nB1 (Jingxi) ‘to blame’.
282
Son Tai, *ʔ.w- >*ʔw- > ˀw- in Debao Yang Zhuang, and *ʔ.w- >*ʔw- > *w- > ʋin Jingxi Yang Zhuang. Nevertheless, our wordlist is only designated to collect the
tone data and is limited to the whole sound system of the languages investigated.
It is hardly to believe there is only a single example with this structure in a
language, therefore the property of kə- (sesquisyllable segment) or k(sesquisyllabic cluster segment) needs more paralleled phenomena to confirm with
in the future. If it is confirmed to be a sesquisyllabic cluster segment, Quan Son
Tai will be a very rare modern Tai variety to inherit some of the sysquisyllabic
clusters from PT.
5) The initial of the term ʋɔ:k55 (DL1-G) ‘flower’ indicates that the trace of the
development of PT *ʔbl/r- (cf. Li 1977: 91) or *ɓl- (cf. Pittayaporn 2009: 157) is
*ʔbl/r- or*ɓl- > *ʔb/ɓ- > *w-> ʋ- in this language. This is different from that
*ʔbl/r- or*ɓl- > *ʔd/ɗ- > d/ˀd- in the vast majorities of SWT, which have dɔ:k DL1-G
or ˀdɔ:k DL1-G for ‘flower’.
6) The initial of the term ʔet41 (DS1-A) ‘to do’ presents an aberrant development that
is expected to be h- according to all other Tai varieties which have this cognate,
like hetDS1-A in Isan (SWT), hatDS1-A in Yang Zhuang varieties (CT), and hetDS1-A in
Zuozhou Zhuang (CT).
283
8. Language 8 Debao Urban, Yang Zhuang, CT
8.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
ˀja:ŋA1-G; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Fu; Nong Fu; T’ienpao/ Tianbao;
ka:ŋC1-U ka:jA1-U; ka:ŋC1-U ŋajB1-C
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang, which is spoken in Chengguan Town, which is the main
town of Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
8.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 17, 2013
LRP’s home, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Place of data elicitation
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Lìyàn (丽雁)
Family Name
Ýn (言)
Gender
female
Age
37
Birth Place
Duolang (tejB2 la:ŋA2 in local Yang Zhuang) Village (多郎屯), Najia Township, Debao
County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Chengguan Town, the main town of Debao County.
Liuzhou City (study for 2 years).
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
self-employment
29 years
Education
technical secondary school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Debao Duolang Yang
mother tongue
Spoken in Duolang village, Najia Township, Debao County,
Guangxi, China
Debao Urban Yang
native
LRP has been speaking this language to her friends around
her in the main town from childhood.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Debao Duolang Yang Zhuang as native, he can
communicate in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, Standard Mandarin, and Baihua (a Cantonese variety).
284
LRP’s mother was born in Shangwang (pʰi:ŋA1-A ˀja:ŋB1-G in local Yang Zhuang) village, Najia Township,
Debao County. Besides her mother tongue Debao Shangwang Yang, she can speak Guiliu Southwestern
Mandarin, and Putonghua.
Remarks
Many Debao Urban Yang Zhuang speakers speak both their parents’ mother tongue, one of the Debao
rural Yang Zhuang varieties, and the urban variety of Chengguan Town (the main town of Debao
County) as their corporate mother tongues, since they are born at the town or came to the main town
from Childhood. Both the two Yang Zhuang varieties are the same native level for them.
8.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 453
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 55
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 55
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 453
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 33
A1/2: 453
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 55
Unaspirated stop +
*r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1G, 2]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal
splits conditioned by glottalized sounds and aspirated sounds are found in Columns A, B, and DL.
Horizontally: four individuals and two mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
A[1-G, 2]
285
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] =DS[2]
Merger
B[1-C, 1-U] = DL[1-C, 1-U] = DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
8.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[1-G, 2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], as well as in DL[1-A, 1UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High level 55 occurs in B[1-C, 1-U], as well as in DL[1-C, 1-U] and DS[1A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth
and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[2].
8.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:24ʔ (C1) ‘five’, ɬaj24ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions
are not phonemic.
8.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Secondary tonal split conditioned by aspirated sounds (1-A, 1-UC, 1/2) along
with glottalized sounds (1-G) are found in Columns B and DL, but not in Column
A, which only has a secondary tonal split conditioned by glottalized sounds. Such
kind of situation is quite rare found, because any kind of secondary tonal split
found only in Column A or in both Columns A and B is more common than only in
Column B in ST (CT and SWT) varieties.
8.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial consonant ɬ- (<*s- (cf. Li 1977: 152) or <*ɬ- (cf. Liao & Shen 2012))
is sometimes pronounced as a free variation θ-, such as ɬɔ:jA1-C = θɔ:jA1-C ‘clear’.
2) Glottalized sounds are robust. Besides the common ones ʔ-, ˀb-, and ˀd-, the other
two ones ˀj- and ˀw- are also well preserved. This is quite different from the
Jingxi Yang Zhuang varieties, which have normally merged ˀj- and ˀw- into j- and
ʋ- respectively. For example, Debao Urban Yang Zhuang ˀjowB1-G ‘at’ vs. Jingxi
286
Urban Yang Zhuang jowB1-G ‘at’, as well as Debao Urban Yang Zhuang ˀwaC1-G
‘crazy’ vs. Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang ʋaC1-G ‘crazy’.
3) Yodicization of the PT liquids in an initial consonant cluster refers to the original
liquid -l- and/or -r- eventually becomes -j- (sometime transcribed as -i- or -̂- by
different authors) (cf. Li 1977: 83) are commonly found in this variety and in all
the following CT and NT varieties. For example, the following items in this
variety contains yodicization of PT medial liquids: pja:453 ‘fish’, pʰjak55 ‘vegetable’,
kjɔ:k33 ‘mortar’ and kʰja:435 ‘to seek’ . This is very different from the changes of PT
medial liquids in SWT, in which some languages partly preserve the liquids such
as pla:33 ‘fish’ in Thai, and some languages have simply dropped the liquids such
as pa:55 ‘fish’ in Tai Lue.
4) PT short *-o- is changed to a long -ɔ:- (in most situation) and to a short -a(before -m and -p) in this variety, like pʰɔ:n453 (A1/2) ‘to sharpen’, kʰam453 (A1/2)
‘bitter’, ham33 (B1-A) ‘to cover up’, θɔ:ŋ55 (B1-C) ‘to send’, kʰɔ:t55 (DS1) ‘twisted’,
kap55 (DS1-U) ‘frog’, nɔ:k33 (DS2) ‘bird’, and mɔ:t33 ‘ant’. This situation is also
found in other Yang Zhuang varieties (L9-10, L12-13) except the more
complicated situation in Jingxi Hurun Yang Zhuang (L11).
5) The word developing from PT Etymon *da:B refers to ‘wharf’ in SWT varieties
(L1-7). However, ta:B2 developing from *da:B in this variety refers to ‘river’.
Actually, hereafter all CT and YN, and NT varieties have the same meaning
‘river’ for this etymon.
6) The word developing from PT Etymon *pa:B refers to ‘forest’ in SWT varieties
(L1-7). However, pa:B1 developing from *pa:B in this variety refers to ‘a clump of
tree’ or ‘bush’. Actually, hereafter CT and NT varieties which remain this etymon
all have the meanings ‘a clump of tree’, ‘bush’ or ‘grove’ more than ‘forest’.
287
9. Language 9 Dalong, Debao Ma’ai, Yang Zhuang, CT
9.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
ˀja:ŋA1-G; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Fu; Nong Fu; T’ienpao/ Tianbao; ka:ŋC1-U ˀba:nC1-G
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Dalong (village name) of Debao Ma’ai Yang Zhuang, which is spoken in the
Northwestern Part of Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
9.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Date
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Dānlú (丹
Gender
male
Birth Place
May 18, 2013
)
Family Name
Huáng (黄)
Age
56
Dalong (ka:B2 ɹaŋA2 in local Yang Zhuang) Village (大龙屯), Chengguan Township,
Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He lives in Dalong Village, but goes to the main town Debao County which is about
four km from his village every day to deal in his trade. He has never left Debao
County to other place to live more than three months since he was born.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer, petty dealer
56 years
Education
junior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Debao Dalong Yang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the northwestern
part of Guangxi, China. It has been used as the language of
instruction and education in the western part of Guangxi
before Standard Mandarin was popularized from 1980s.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang, he can communicate in
Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin and Cantonese with Yang Zhuang accents.
LRP’s mother was born in the same village. She was monolingual in Yang Zhuang.
Remarks
Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang is actually often treated as a variety of Debao Ma’ai Yang Zhuang in local
concept. The only difference between Dalong and Ma’ai may be the different tonal adaptation of
288
Mandarin Chinese loans. For example, the modern Mandarin loanword ‘Guangxi’ is borrowed as kwa:ŋB1
θi:B1 into Dalong but kwa:ŋA1 θi:B1 into Ma’ai.
9.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 31
B1-A: 332
C1-A: 24ˀ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 45
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 454
C1-C: 24ˀ
DL1-C: 45
DS1-C: 45
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 454
C1-U: 24ˀ
DL1-U: 45
DS1-U: 45
A1-UC: 31
B1-UC: 332
C1-UC: 24ˀ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 45
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 332
C1-G: 24ˀ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 45
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 31
B2: 332
C2: 213ˀ
DL2: 33
DS2: 21
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A: 31
breathy sounds
B1/2: 332
C1/2: 24ˀ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 45
A1/2-C: 53
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1G, 2, 1/2-A]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1G, 2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal splits
conditioned by glottalized sounds and aspirated sounds are found in Columns A, B, and DL.
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C]
Individual
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2-A]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
Merger
B[1-C, 1-U] = DL[1-C, 1-U] = DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2] =DS[2]
289
9.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2-A].
Tone 3:
Mid level-falling 332 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1-UC,
1-G, 2, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-rising-falling 454 occurs in B[1-C, 1-U], as well as High-rising 45 occurs
in DL[1-C, 1-U] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[2], as well as low-falling 21 occurs in
DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
9.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:24ʔ (C1) ‘five’, θaj24ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. The final glottal constriction is
quite clear, and the LRP almost releases it as a glottal final stop every time when
reading word with these two tones. However, all the glottal constrictions are not
phonemic.
9.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Different from the Urban variety that has different tonal split patterns between
Column A and Columns B/DL, secondary tonal split conditioned by aspirated
sounds (1-A, 1-UC, 1/2) along with glottalized sounds (1-G) are neatly found in
Columns A, B and DL.
2) In Columns A, B and DL, only initials developing from voiceless continuants (1-C)
and voiceless unaspirated stops (1-U) tend to condition the original high register
tones, other initial sounds in the high register (1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2) all condition a
secondary tonal split to depress the original high register tone to merge into the
low register tone.
3) Because in Row 1/2 (or VASO-1/2) there are two different initial sounds,
aspirated sounds and continuants, two different register tones are found in this
row. For example, kʰjow31 (A1/2-A) ‘ear’ vs. θɔ:53 (A1/2-C) ‘right side’ are put into
the same Row 1/2 (because they are of the voicing alternation initial group), but
have different register tones conditioned by aspirated and continuant sounds
290
respectively. This suggests that the designation of Tai tone box should be based on
modern phonation types (aspirated and continuant in this case) rather than on
historical sources, which may develop to merge into different modern phonation
groups.
9.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial consonant ɬ- (<*s- (cf. Li 1977: 152) or <*ɬ- (cf. Liao & Shen 2012))
in the Urban variety is pronounced as θ- in this variety, such as θɔ:jA1-C ‘clear’ and
θamC1-C ‘sour’.
2) Just as the Urban variety, glottalized sounds are also robust. Besides the common
ones ʔ-, ˀb-, and ˀd-, the other two ones ˀj- and ˀw- are also well preserved.
3) Some of the PT long vowels on dead syllables are shortened in this variety, but
the tonal behaviors of these shortened vowels are preserved to be in the longvowel column (DL). For example, tʰʊk33 (DL1/2 > DL1-A) ‘to hit the mark’, lʊk33
(DL2) ‘child’, and mjət33 ‘knife’ all have short vowels corresponding to long vowels
reflected in SWT varieties, but the tonal behaviors are all preserved as in Column
DL, which has different tones in Column DS in this variety.
291
10. Language 10 Lüliu, Debao Suburb, Yang Zhuang, CT
10.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
ˀja:ŋA1-G; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Fu; Nong Fu; T’ienpao/ Tianbao;
ka:ŋC1-U ŋajB1-C ˀba:nC1-G
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Lüliu (village name) of Debao Suburb Yang Zhuang, which is spoken at the rural
areas surrounding the main town of Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
10.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Date
May 18, 2013
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Q́xí (奇席)
Family Name
Ľ (李)
Gender
male
Age
34
Birth Place
Lüliu (lu:kDL2 nawB2 in local Yang Zhuang) Village (绿柳屯), Chengguan Township,
Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He was born in Lüliu Village of Chengguan Township, Debao County. When he was
23 years old, he went to Zhongshan City of Guangdong Province for his migrate work
for about 10 years. He just came back to his village from Guangdong 1 year ago.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
23 years
Education
junior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Debao Dalong Yang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Cantonese
fluent
The LRP learned this language during the time he worked in
Guangdong Province, where Cantonese is a lingua franca.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the northwestern
part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Debao L̈liu Yang as native, he could communicate
in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin with outlander. LRP’s mother was born in the same village. She is
monolingual in Yang Zhuang. She lives in the same village with the LRP now.
Remarks
Yang Zhuang community members from the rural areas normally go to the neighboring Guangdong
Province (where is one of the most economically developing regions in China) for their migrant works
after junior high school, if their families are not able to support them to continue their studies.
292
10.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 31
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 55
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 55
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 31
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 33
A1/2: 31
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 55
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1G, 2, 1/2]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1G, 2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal splits
conditioned by glottalized sounds and aspirated sounds are found in Columns A, B, and DL.
Horizontally: four individuals and two mergers
A[1-C, 1-U]
Individual
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]= DS[2]
B[1-C, 1-U] = DL[1-C, 1-U] = DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
293
10.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2,
1/2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High level 55 occurs in B[1-C, 1-U], as well as DL[1-C, 1-U] and DS[1-A,
1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth
and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[2].
10.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:24ʔ (C1) ‘five’, θaj24ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
10.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) All tonal values of the six tones are the same with the Urban variety (L8), but the
basic tonal patterns are almost identical with the Dalong variety (L9) except that
DS2 does not merge into C2 like Dalong does, but merges into B2 like the Urban
variety (L8) does.
2) Just like the Dalong variety, there are two different initial sounds, aspirated
sounds and continuants, are found in Row 1/2 (or VASO-1/2). For example,
kʰjow31 (A1/2-A) ‘ear’ vs. θɔ:453 (A1/2-C) ‘right side’ are put into the same Row 1/2
(because they are of the voicing alternation initial group), but have different
register tones conditioned by aspirated and continuant sounds respectively. This
supports that the designation of Tai tone box should be based on modern
phonation types (aspirated and continuant in this case) rather than on historical
sources, which may develop to merge into different modern phonation groups.
10.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial consonant ɬ- (<*s- (cf. Li 1977: 152) or <*ɬ- (cf. Liao & Shen 2012))
in the Urban variety is pronounced as θ- in this variety, such as θɔ:jA1-C ‘clear’ and
θamC1-C ‘sour’.
294
2) Unlike the Urban variety (L8) and the Dalong variety (L9), there are only four
initial consonants ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd- and ˀj- in the glottalized sound group. The one ˀw- in
the Urban and Dalong varieties has been merged into ˀb- in this variety. For
example, Lüliu ˀba:24ʔ ‘crazy’ vs. Dalong / Urban ˀwa:24ʔ ‘crazy’.
295
11. Language 11 Jingxi Hurun Yang Zhuang, CT
11.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
nʊŋA2; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
ka:ŋC1-U nɔŋA2; huA1-A jʊnC2
Data Source
3 LRPs
Variety Name
Hurun (township name) Nong of Yang Zhuang. This variety is spoken in the urban
and rural areas of Hurun Township, a town near Chinese-Vietnam border in Jingxi
County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Hurun Nong of Yang Zhuang is an awkward name for this variety. From
intelligibility, from similarity of wordlist and from speaker attitudes, this variety
is of Yang Zhuang (ISO 639: zyg) beyond doubt. However, different from other
Yang Zhuang varieties in both Jingxi and Debao counties (where Yang Zhuang
varieties are the dominant language), speakers of this variety never call
themselves Yang but Nong. Note that they are not of Nong Zhuang (ISO 639-3:
zhn) distributing in the neighboring Yunnan Province of China. All speakers
investigated are obviously unwilling to be called Yang during my fieldwork
research on this variety. Nevertheless, I have to temporarily treat them as a
variety of Yang Zhuang in this thesis due to the ISO 639-3 principle of the
individual language naming in §1.1.3.
11.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
May 16, 2013
The 4th community, Hurun Town, Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Yù hóng (玉紅)
Family Name
Húng (黄)
Gender
female
Age
48
Birth Place
Hurun (hu:A2 jʊnC1 in local Yang Zhuang) Township (湖潤鎮), Jingxi County, Baise
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
She has been living in Hurun from her birth for her whole life.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
48 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Hurun Nong of Yang
mother tongue
junior high school
Language Remarks
Zhuang
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Baihua
intermediate
This is a Cantonese variety in Guangxi.
296
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Shangbu Community ( 卜屯 ˀba:nC1 pʊkDS2 ni:A1 in local Zhuang), Xinqun
Village (新群村), Hurun Township, Jingxi County. Besides Yang Zhuang as native, he could communicate
in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, Standard Mandarin, and Cantonese with Yang Zhuang accents. LRP’s
mother was born in the same village of her father’s. She was monolingual in local Yang Zhuang.
Remarks
Hurun is the east end of Jingxi County, and borders on Daxin County, where the main town Taocheng
Town has Baihua which is Cantonese variety spoken in Guangxi to be a lingua franca. Therefore, the
local Zhuang people in Hurun Township are normally familiar with Baihua.
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP2
Date
May 16, 2013
Hurun (hu:A2 jʊnC1 in local Yang Zhuang) Township (湖潤鎮), Jingxi
County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Hóngḿi (紅梅)
Family Name
Sū (蘇)
Gender
male
Age
47
Birth Place
Hurun (hu:A2 jʊnC1 in local Yang Zhuang) Township (湖潤鎮), Jingxi County, Baise
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Xinjing Town, Jingxi County (10 years).
She is now living in Hurun Township, Jingxi County.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
30 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Hurun Nong of Yang Zhuang
mother tongue
Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang
fluent
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca of Jingxi County.
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Baihua
a little
This is a Cantonese variety in Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in Hurun Township. Besides local Hurun Nong of Yang Zhuang, they both can
communicate in Baihua and Standard Mandarin. Both of them passed away.
297
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP3
Date
May 16, 2013
Hurun (hu:A2 jʊnC1 in local Yang Zhuang) Township (湖潤鎮), Jingxi County,
Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Gù ihuā (桂花)
Family Name
Ńng (農)
Gender
female
Age
48
Hurun (hu:A2 jʊnC1 in local Yang Zhuang) Township (湖潤鎮), Jingxi County, Baise
Birth Place
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
She has been living in Hurun from her birth for her whole life.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
48 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Hurun Nong of Yang Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
Junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
Baihua
intermediate
This is a Cantonese variety in Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in Hurun Township. Besides local Hurun Nong of Yang Zhuang, they both can
communicate in Baihua and Standard Mandarin. Both of them live with the LRP now.
11.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop n this dialect.
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 51
B1-A: 445
C1-A: 45ʔ
DL1-A: 44
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 51
B1-C: 445
C1-C: 45ʔ
DL1-C: 44
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 51
B1-U: 445
C1-U: 45ʔ
DL1-U: 44
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 51
B1-UC: 445
C1-UC: 45ʔ
DL1-UC: 44
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 445
C1-G: 45ʔ
DL1-G: 44
DS1-G: 55
A2: 31
B2: 334
C2: 13ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 13
A1/2: 51
B1/2: 445
C1/2: 45ʔ
DL1/2: 44
DS1/2: 55
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
298
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1G, 2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal splits
conditioned by glottalized sounds are found in Column A.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
A[1G, 2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[2] = DL[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] =DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2] = DS[2]
11.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 51 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[1-G, 2].
Tone 3:
High-level-rising 445 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and mid level 44
occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-level-rising 334 occurs in B[2] and mid level 33 occurs in DL[2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-rising 45 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and high level 55 occurs
in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-rising 13 occurs in C[2] and in DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
299
11.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:45ʔ (C1) ‘five’, ɬaj45ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj13ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam13ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
11.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The two register tones in Column B in Hurun Yang Zhuang are almost identical
to be a relatively high level and rising tone. However, all the LRPs claimed that
they can certainly distinguish minimal pairs with tones B1 and B2. For testing
the pitch difference of the two register tones, I have used PRAAT (5.1.34) to
define these two register tones as 445 and 334 respectively. For example, three
words with these two register tones are as ta:445 (B1-U) ‘grandfather’, ˀda:445 (B1G) ‘to scold’, and ta:334 (B2) ‘river’ in the following figure.
2) Basically, glottalized sounds (ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-) in Column A (A1-G) condition a
secondary tonal split to depress the original high register tone (51) to merge it
into the low register tone A2 (31), such as in ˀbən31 ‘to fly’, ˀda:w31 ‘star’, and
ʔaw31 ‘to get’. However, the initial ˀj- (<*ʔj-) preserved in the Yang Zhuang
varieties in Debao County has been merged into j- in this variety, and the loss of
the pre-glottalized feature does not affect the condition of this secondary tonal
split, therefore the tone of ja:51 ‘medicine’ and ja:ŋ51 ‘Yang Zhuang people /
language’ is preserved as the original high register pitch 51.
3) The term kaw13 ‘nine’ has a low-rising tone which is of C2. This is unexpected
because all other Tai varieties all reflect C1-U, which is presented as a high
rising tone 45 in this variety.
300
11.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Unlike the three Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (L8-10), only three glottalized
sounds ʔ-, ˀb-, and ˀd- are preserved. The ˀj- and ˀw- has been merged into j- and
ʋ- respectively in this variety. For example, Hurun Yang jowB1-G ‘at’ vs. Debao
Urban Yang ˀjowB1-G ‘at’, and Hurun Yang ʋaC1-G ‘crazy’ vs. Debao Urban Yang
ˀwaC1-G ‘crazy’.
2) Different from most other Yang Zhuang varieties (including L8, L9, L10, L12,
and L13) which reflect that PT *-o- has become a long -ɔ:- (in most situation) and
a short -a- (before -m and -p), like pʰɔ:n53 (A1/2) ‘to sharpen’, kʰam53 (A1/2)
‘bitter’, ham353 (B1-A) ‘to cover up’, θɔ:ŋ353 (B1-C) ‘to send’, kʰɔ:t33 (DS1) ‘twisted’,
kap33 (DS1-U) ‘frog’, nɔ:k21 (DS2) ‘bird’, and mɔ:t21 ‘ant’ in Jingxi Urban Yang
(L12), in this variety the development of PT short *-o- is more complicated. It
has been changed to a short -ʊ before -ŋ, to -ə before -m and -n, to -ɔ:- (but
sometimes -a-) before -k and -t, to -a- before -p. Therefore, the items above are
pʰən51 (A1/2) ‘to sharpen’, kʰəm51 (A1/2) ‘bitter’, həm445 (B1-A) ‘to cover up’,
θʊŋ445 (B1-C) ‘to send’, kʰɔ:t55 (DS1) ‘twisted’, nɔ:k13 (DS2) ‘bird’, kap55 (DS1-U)
‘frog’, and mɔ:t13/mat13 ‘ant’ in this variety.
301
12. Language 12 Jingxi Urban, Yang Zhuang, CT
12.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
ja:ŋA1-G; tʰo:C1-A; ɬəŋA2
Exonym
Jingxi Zhuang; Nong Shun; Nung Giang
Data Source
2 LRPs
Variety
Jingxi Urban of Yang Zhuang, which is spoken at the main town of Jingxi County,
Name
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
12.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 16, 2013
No.768 Chengdong Road, Xinjing Town, Jingxi County, Baise
Place of data elicitation
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Cáish́o (才韶)
Family Name
Ẃng (王)
Gender
male
Age
33
Birth Place
Xinjing Town, Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region,
China.
Places lived
Chengguan Town, Debao County (high school study, 1 year)
Shunde City of Guangdong Province (1 year).
Nanning (1 year)
He is now living in the Jingxi main town.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
unemployed
Languages spoken
Level
Jingxi Urban Yang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
30 years
Education
junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Debao Urban Yang
familiar
He used to learn in Debao main town for half a
year, and has many local friends from Debao.
Cantonese
intermediate
He used to work in Guangdong Province, where
Cantonese is a lingua franca.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Zumei Community (足梅屯 ˀba:nC1 pja:C1 tʰa:ŋA1 in local Yang Zhuang), Shangba
Village (
壩村), Longbang Township (龍邦鎮), Jingxi County. Besides local Yang Zhuang, he speaks
Southwestern to communicate with outlanders. The LRP’s mother was born in Gongtun Community
(供屯), Wuping Village (
村), Wuping Township (
鎮), Jingxi County. Her mother tongue is local
Yang Zhuang. She also speaks Standard Mandarin and Baihua. Both of them live with the LRP now.
302
Remarks
The main town of Jingxi County, Xinjing Town, is the largest city in the southwestern corner of the
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region of China. Probably due to the language contact with Han Chinese
immigrations since the recent history, the Jingxi Urban variety has the most simplified sound system
(like the complete loss of pre-glottalization) in Yang Zhuang.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Date
August 26, 2014
Tianyu Studio, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region,
Place of data elicitation
China.
Given Name
B̄nbīn (彬彬)
Family Name
Liú (
Gender
female
Age
28
Birth Place
)
Xinjing Town, Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region,
China.
Places lived
Xinjing Twon, Jingxi (16 years); Baise City (high school study, 3 years); Guilin City
(university, 4 years); Yizhou City (3 years). She is living in Nanning City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
teacher
Languages spoken
Level
Jingxi Urban Yang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
3 years
Education
bachelor
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Southwestern Mandarin
familiar
She used to work in Yizhou City, where
Southwestern Mandarin is a dominant language.
Baihua
a little
She is now work in Nanning City, where Cantonese
variety Baihua is a lingua franca.
English
a little
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Ronglao Township (榮勞鄉) of Jingxi County. LRP’s mother was born in the
main town of Jingxi County. They both speak their own Yang Zhuang varieties as mother tongue, and
communicate with outlanders in Standard Mandarin. LRP’s father can speak Guiliu
303
12.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 53
B1-A: 353
C1-A: 33ʔ
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 33
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 353
C1-C: 33ʔ
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 33
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 353
C1-U: 33ʔ
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 33
A1-UC: 53
B1-UC: 353
C1-UC: 33ʔ
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 33
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 353
C1-G: 33ʔ
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 33
A2: 31
B2: 131
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 21
DS2: 21
A1/2: 53
B1/2: 353
C1/2: 33ʔ
DL1/2: 35
DS1/2: 33
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
3) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1G, 1/2]
Individual
A[2]
B[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] =DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2] = DL[2] = DS[2]
304
12.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising-falling 353 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and midrising 35 occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-rising-falling 131 occurs in B[2].
Tone 5:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2] and DS [1-A, 1-C, 1U, 1-UC, G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[2] as well as low-falling 21 occurs in
DL[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
12.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:33ʔ (C1) ‘five’, ɬaj33ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
12.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Different from the Yang Zhuang varieties in Debao County and the Jingxi Hurun
Nong variety of Yang Zhuang which all have non-straightforward tonal
development on tonal splits, Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang has neat tonal splits
between proto-voiceless and proto-voiced sounds.
2) According to the previous study of Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang in Zheng (1996),
DL2 and DS2 in this variety are distinct tones, which are merged into B2 and C2
respectively just like the tonal merging pattern of Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang
(L13). However, in my data from the two LRPs, these two tones tent to be
merged into C2. This may be a recent tone change, because Jingxi Urban data
provided by Zheng (1996) are based on her personal linguistic background. Note
that she was born in Jingxi main town in 1936, and her mother tongue accent
may be of the old style, which preserves the original tonal pattern.
3) Initials in the groups of glottalized sounds are all merged into continuants (ˀd>n-, ˀb- > m-, ˀj- > j-, ˀw- > ʋ-) except ʔ-. However, tones conditioned by this
initial groups are neatly preserved as in the high register. This indicates that the
loss of pre-glottalization are much later than that glottalized sounds conditoned
tones in the high register in this variety, since the neighboring Yang Zhuang
305
varieties (including those in Debao County and those in the rural areas of Jingxi
County) still have robust pre-glotallized sounds. For example, ˀban31 ‘to fly’,
ˀda:w31 ‘star’, ʔaw31 ‘to get’, ˀjow33 ‘at’, and ˀwa24ʔ ‘crazy’ in Debao Urban Yang
Zhuang are man53, na:w53, ʔaw53, jow353, and ʋa33ʔ respectively in Jingxi Urban
Yang Zhuang.
12.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Unlike the three Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (L8-10) and Jingxi Hurun Yang
Zhuang, pre-glottalized sounds are least robustly preserved in this variety.
2) Like -ɔ: on dead syllables in other Yang Zhuang varieties, items with this long
vowel always show a regular short tone (DS), like kʰɔ:t33 (DS1) ‘twisted’, nɔ:k21
(DS2) ‘bird’, and mɔ:t21 ‘ant’, to correspond to cognate with short *-o in other Tai
varieties.
3) The vowels of tʰʊk33 (DL1/2 > DS1/2) ‘to hit the mark’ and mit21 ‘knife’ (DL2 >
DS2) are shortened.
306
13. Language 13 Jingxi Anning, Yang Zhuang, CT
13.1 Language information
Language
Yang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyg
Autonym
ja:ŋA1-G; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Jingxi Zhuang; Nong Shun; Nung Giang
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Anning Jiaotun of Jingxi Yang Zhuang, which is spoken at the rural areas of Anning
Township of Jingxi County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
13.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
August 27, 2014
Jingxi High School, Xinjing Town, Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Cháowěi (朝偉)
Family Name
Ľ (李)
Gender
male
Age
42
Birth Place
Jiaotun Community (叫屯, ˀba:nC1 ke:wB1 in local Zhuang) , Zuhuai Village (逐懷村),
Anning Township (安寧鄉), Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Anning Township, Jingxi County (16 years); Baise City (high school + working 10
years). Nanning City (undergraduate study, 4 years); Pingguo County (13 years). He is
now living in Baise City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
civil servants
Languages spoken
Level
Jingxi Anning Yang
mother tongue
7 years
Education
on-job postgraduates
Language Remarks
Zhuang
Jingxi Urban Yang
fluent
Zhuang
Debao Ma’ai Yang
fluent
Zhuang
Spoken in most rural areas of Debao County. This
variety is treated as the representative accent of Debao
Yang Zhuang. The LRP has many friends from Debao
County, and he likes speaking Debao Ma’ai Yang
Zhuang to his Debao friends.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
307
Baihua
familiar
English
a little
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were both born in the same village. Both of them are monolingual in local Yang Zhuang.
13.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 53
B1-A: 453
C1-A: 44ʔ
DL1-A: 45
DS1-A: 44
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 453
C1-C: 44ʔ
DL1-C: 45
DS1-C: 44
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 453
C1-U: 44ʔ
DL1-U: 45
DS1-U: 44
A1-UC: 53
B1-UC: 453
C1-UC: 44ʔ
DL1-UC: 45
DS1-UC: 44
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 453
C1-G: 44ʔ
DL1-G: 45
DS1-G: 44
A2: 31
B2: 131
C2: 213ʔ
DL2: 13
DS2: 21
A1/2: 53
B1/2: 453
C1/2: 44ʔ
DL1/2: 45
DS1/2: 44
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
A[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
B[2] = DL[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2] = DS[2]
308
13.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2].
Tone 3:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and high-rising
45 occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-rising-falling 131 occurs in B[2], and low-rising 13 occurs in DL[2] to form
a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 44 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, G, 1/2] and DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[2] as well as low-falling 21 occurs in DS[2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
13.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:44ʔ (C1) ‘five’, ɬaj44ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
13.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Just like Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang, this variety has neat tonal splits between
proto-voiceless and proto-voiced sounds.
2) Unlike Jingxi Urban merging DL2 and DS2 together to C2, this variety merges
DL2 to B2, but DS2 to C2. This pattern seems to be more common, because
Columns DL and B are of long vowel-duration pair, and Columns DS and C are of
short vowel-duration pair (cf. §4.1.3.2). This makes the whole tonal pattern of
this variety to be identical with the Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang variety in the
previous study Jingxi Zhuang Language Research (Zheng 1996) (cf. the point 2) of
12.6 in Appendix C).
3) Unlike Jingxi Urban Yang Zhuang which merges all pre-glottalized sounds into
continuants (ˀd- >n-, ˀb- > m-, ˀj- > j-, ˀw- > ʋ-), this variety preserves ˀd- and
ˀb- but merges ˀj- > j- and ˀw- > ʋ-. However, tones conditioned by this initial
groups are neatly preserved as in the high register. Therefore, ˀban31 ‘to fly’,
ˀda:w31 ‘star’, ˀjow33 ‘at’, and ˀwa24ʔ ‘crazy’ in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang are
ˀban53, ˀda:w53, jow453, and ʋa44ʔ respectively in this variety.
309
13.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Unlike the three Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (L8-10) which most robustly
preserve glottalized sounds, and unlike Jingxi Hurun Yang Zhuang (L12) which
least robustly preserves glottalized sounds, this variety is like Jingxi Hurun (L11)
to preserve the pre-glottalized stops (ˀb- and ˀd-) but to lose the pre-glottalization
of minivowels (ˀj- > j- and ˀw- > ʋ-).
2) Like -ɔ: on dead syllables in other Yang Zhuang varieties, items with this long
vowel always show a regular short tone (DS), like kʰɔ:t33 (DS1) ‘twisted’, nɔ:k21
(DS2) ‘bird’, and mɔ:t21 ‘ant’, to correspond to cognate with short *-o in other Tai
varieties.
3) The vowels of tʰʊk33 (DL1/2 > DS1/2) ‘to hit the mark’ and mit21 ‘knife’ (DL2 >
DS2) are shortened and the tone of this item is also changed to be in Column DS.
This is unlike Debao Yang Zhuang varieties, in where the same item has been
shortened the vowel but is preserved its tonal behavior conditioned by long
vowels, like tʰʊk33 (DL1/2) in Debao Ma’ai.
310
14. Language 14 mja:ŋA2, Debao Ronghua Nalong, CT
14.1 Language information
Language
Myang
ISO 639-3 Code
undescribed
Autonym
mja:ŋA2; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Debao Ronghua
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Nalong (village name) of Myang, which is spoken at the most areas of the Ronghua
township, Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
As far as I known, varieties of Myang Zhuang are only found in Ronghua Township in
Debao County. The LRP provided interesting information on their own autonym and
some exonyms to other Zhuang varieties around them. They call their native language
mja:ŋA2, which is labled as Myang Zhuang here. Members of Myang Zhuang call all
Yang Zhuang varieties nʊŋA2 instead of ja:ŋA1-G which is the autonym of many Yang
Zhuang variety. This is because the exonym ja:ŋA1-G called by Myang Zhuang members
refers to Debao Longsang (L31), which is an undescribed NT variety. We will see the
corresponding information in the following Language 31, Debao Longsang, which is
called ja:ŋA1-G as an autonym by their own members. In fact, as far as I know, many NT
varieties in the western part of Guangxi call some NT varieties distributing on the CTNT border ja:ŋA1-G. In other words, ja:ŋA1-G never refers to the Yang Zhuang varieties
(CT) (which is put into ISO 639-3 code as “zyg”) by these NT varieties. Instead, they
call Yang Zhuang varieties of Debao and Jingxi ɲaŋA2 or noŋA2. The term noŋA2 is the
common autonym or exonym of CT varieties. The term ɲaŋA2 is actually adapted from
the exclusive Southern Zhuang (CT) lexical item ‘still’ which is not found in NT
varieties, to refer to the Yang Zhuang varieties (which robustly use this item) by NT
members (Liao 2010: 77). In short, the exonyms of Debao Longsang and Yang Zhuang
called by the Myang Zhuang members indicate the awkward situation of the naming
Yang Zhuang again, to respond the situation that the members of Hurun Nong of Yang
Zhuang reject the exonym Yang but prefer the autonym Nong (cf. the language
information of the LRP1 of L10). The information indicates that the naming of Yang
Zhuang in ISO 639-3 system needs more review and discussions.
14.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
May 15, 2013
Xinlong South Six-Road 13, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Fèngrén (奉仁)
Family Name
M̌ng (蒙)
Gender
female
Age
60
Birth Place
Nalong Community (那陇屯na:A2 lʊŋB2 in local Myang and Yang Zhuang), Nalong
Village (那陇村), Ronghua Township, Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
311
Places lived
She was born in Nalong Village of Ronghua Township, Debao County. When she was
18 years old, she moved to Chengguan Town to live until now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
retired worker
42 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Myang Zhuang
mother tongue
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
familiar
primary school
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca in Debao
County, Guangxi.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in
the northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
Baihua
intermediate
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. He was monolingual in local Myang Zhuang.
LRP’s mother was born in Maomei Village (tʰu:A1-A ˀdɔ:ŋA1-G in local Yang Zhuang), Ronghua Township,
Debao County. She was also monolingual in local Myang Zhuang.
14.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 55
B1-A: 35
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 55
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 55
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 55
B1-UC: 35
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 55
B1-G: 35
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 42
B2: 33
C2: 31ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 42
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2: 55
B1/2: 35
C1/2: 24ˀ
DL1/2: 35
DS1/2: 55
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
breathy sounds
312
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[2] = DS[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
14.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High level 55 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U,
1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
High-falling 42 occurs in A[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 33 occurs in B[2] and DL[2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in C[2].
14.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:24ʔ (C1) ‘five’, ɬaj24ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj213ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam213ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
313
14.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Just like the Jingxi Urban and Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang varieties, this variety
has neat tonal splits between proto-voiceless and proto-voiced sounds.
2) The term ŋa:j42 ‘turn face up’ has an unexpected A2 tone, which is expected to be
A1 tone in the tone box designated, vs. ŋa:j24 (A1-C) in Thai and ŋa:j453 (A1-C) in
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang.
14.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Like the three Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (L8-10) which most robustly
preserve pre-glottalized sounds, ʔ-, ˀb-, ˀd-, and ˀj- are all found in this Myang
Zhuang variety, and they all condition a high register tone. Because of the
limited data, we do not know whether it preserves ˀw- or not.
2) Labialized sounds are robuster than the neighboring Yang Zhuang. Not only kw-,
kʰw-, and ŋw- (which are also found in Debao County Yang Zhuang) are found,
but also ɬw- and tw- (which are not found in Debao County Yang Zhuang) are
found due to my data, like ɬwa:55 (A1/2) ‘right side’ and twa: (A2) ‘to spread on’.
3) The term tʰjap55(DS1-A) ‘hailstone’ has an unexpected initial consonant tʰj-, which
is expected to be tʰ- according to other Tai varieties.
314
15. Language 15 Tuoxin, Pyang Zhuang, CT
15.1 Language information
Language
Pyang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
undescribed
Autonym
pja:ŋB2; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
ˀjujC1-G; Rui
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Tuoxin (驮信村 ˀba:n55 te:31 θin21 in local Pyang Zhuang), Debao Fuping Pyang
Zhuang, which is spoken at the rural areas surrounding the main town of Debao
County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
15.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 17, 2012
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise
Place of data elicitation
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Chángchéng (長城)
Family Name
Ćn (岑)
Gender
male
Age
34
Birth Place
Tuoxin Community (驮信村 ˀba:n55 te:31 θin21 in local Pyang Zhuang), Fuping Village,
Jingde Township, Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Region, China.
Places lived
Tuoxin (5 years); Tongkuang (Copper Mine Refinery) in Yandong Township, Debao
County (10 years)y. Chengguan Town, Debao County (Senior high school 3 years +
working 7 years). Nanning City (Undergraduate study 4 years + working 6 years).
He is now living in Chengguan Town, Debao County.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
instrument technician
7 years
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Tuoxin Pyang Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the northwestern
part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides mother tongue Pyang, he spoke Debao Ma’ai Yang
Zhuang which is the lingua franca of Debao County, Southwestern Mandarin and Standard Mandarin.
LRP’s mother was born in an unknown village at where Nung An (of Yongnan Zhuang) was spoken.
When she was 5, she moved to the LRP’s village. Her mother tongue is Nung An, but she states that her
Pyang Zhuang is much better than Nung An because she speaks Pyang Zhuang in her family after she
moved to Tuoxin Village. She also speaks Debao Ma’ai Yang Zhuang and Min Zhuang which is another
315
CT language spoken in the near villages in Fuping area. Besides, she can communicate in Standard
Mandarin and Southwestern Mandarin in her Zhuang accent.
Remarks
This language is also called ˀjujC1 by the surrounding Yang Zhuang members, and is sometimes
transcribed as Rui (瑞) in Chinese character. As far as I known, varieties of Pyang Zhuang are only
found in Ronghua Township in Debao County and the neighboring Kuixu Township in Jingxi County. It
may be closely related to Nong Zhuang (ISO 639-3: zhn) varieties in Yunnan Province of China due to
some of their shared innovations, e.g. PT *xr- (Li 1977: 233) or *kr- (Pittayaporn 2009: 143) is reflectd
as tɕʰ- both in Pyang Zhuang and Nong Zhuang (vs. kʰj- in Yang Zhuang, and h- in SWT varieites), such
as tɕʰa:A1-A ‘to seek’ and tɕʰɔkDS1-A ‘six’, as well as some specific words like hɔkDS1-A ‘to do’ only found in
Pyang Zhuang and Nong Zhuang (vs. hatDS1-A ‘to do’ in Yang Zhuang, hetDS1-A ‘to do’ in Zuojiang Zhuang
and Isan). Besides, Gedney’s western Nung (cf. Hudak 2008) may be closed related to Nong Zhuang and
Pyang Zhuang.
15.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are tonal flip-flops on columns B and DL.
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 24
B1-A: 212
C1-A: 45/55ʔ
DL1-A: 21
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 24
B1-C: 212
C1-C: 45/55ʔ
DL1-C: 21
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24
B1-U: 212
C1-U: 45/55ʔ
DL1-U: 21
DS1-U: 55
B1-UC:
C1-UC:
DL1-UC:
DS1-UC:
212
45/55ʔ
21
55
A1-G: 24
B1-G: 212
C1-G: 45/55ʔˀ
DL1-G: 21
DS1-G: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 33
B2: 42
C2: 53ʔ
DL2: 42
DS2: 33
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1-U: 24
B1/2: 212
C1/2: 45/55ʔ
DL1/2: 21
DS1/2: 55
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
A1-UC: 24
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
316
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[2] = DS[2]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
15.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Low-falling-rising 212 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and low-falling
21 occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-falling 42 occurs in B[2] and DL[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-rising 45 or high level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and
high level 55 occurs in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
High-falling 53 occurs in C[2].
15.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have final glottal constrictions, like ha:24ʔ (C1) ‘five’, θaj24ʔ (C1)
‘intestine’, maj53ʔ (C2) ‘tree’, and nam53ʔ (C2) ‘water’. However, all the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
15.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Just like other CT varieties investigated, the vowel and the tone of the term pik55
‘wing’ is a short vowel which conditions the tone to be of DS1-U (55). This is
different from the cognate of SWT varieties, which have this term with a long
vowel and a DL1-U tone in the tone box designated, vs. pi:k 21 (DL1-U) ‘wing’ in
Thai.
15.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) In this Pyang Zhuang variety, yodicizaion of PT liquid is also found, such as pja24
and pʰjak55 ‘vegetable’. However, the yodicization kj- and kʰj- which are
317
commonly found in the neighboring Yang Zhuang varieties has been changed to
tɕ- and tɕʰ- due to palatalization in this variety, such as tɕɔk33 ‘mortar’, tɕʰu:24 ‘ear
‘and tɕʰɔk33 ‘six’.
2) Some specific words like hɔkDS1-A ‘to do’ is found in this variety. This is different
from the common ST cognate hatDS1-A ‘to do’ in Yang Zhuang, hetDS1-A ‘to do’ in
Zuojiang Zhuang and hetDS1-A ‘to do’ in Isan).
318
Language 16 Jingxi Huashan, Min Zhuang, CT
16.1 Language information
Language
Min Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zgm
Autonym
ɕu:ŋB1; minA1; tʰo:C1
Exonym
Zhong; Min; Black Cloth Zhuang
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Hushan(華山屯, mo:A2 ɹ̥ənC2 in local Min Zhuang) of Jingxi Sanhe Min Zhuang,
which is spoken at the rural areas of Sanhe Township, Jingxi County, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
16.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 20, 2012
Shangjie 65, Ande Township, Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture,
Place of data elicitation
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Čipíng (彩萍)
Family Name
X̌ (許)
Gender
female
Age
42
Birth Place
A2
Huashan (華山屯, mo:
Township (
ɹ̥ən
C2
in local Min Zhuang), Sanxi Village (
西村), Sanhe
合鄉), Baise Prefecture, Jingxi County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Region, China.
Places lived
Hushan Village (22 years); Dajie Community (大節屯, ˀba:nC1 ˀdaɰA1 tɕe:tDL1), Dale
Village (大樂村), Sanhe Township (
合鄉) (16 years) . Guangdong Province (6 years).
Hainan Province(1 year). Nanning City (1 year).
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
Languages spoken
Level
Huashan Min Yang
mother tongue
Jingxi Yang Zhuang
familiar
Standard Mandarin
fluent
Cantonese
intermediate
16 years
Education
The fourth year of primary school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Min Zhuang, he also spoke Yang Zhuang which is the
lingua franca of Jingxi County, as well as Mandarin and Cantonese for communicating with outlanders.
LRP’s mother was born in Dale Village (大樂村 ˀdaɯA1 lɔ:kDL1), Ande Township, Jingxi County. Besides
Min Zhuang, she also spoke Yang Zhuang which is the lingua franca of Jingxi County.
Remarks
Min Zhuang is a second largest language in the Dejing area of Guangxi, next to Yang Zhuang (Jackson et
al. 2011). The main body of Min Zhuang region includes the rural areas of all the townships of western
part of Jingxi County, the most areas of Napo County, and the southwestern corner of Debao County.
319
They are well-known as Black Cloth Zhuang in Chinese media recently, due to their unique traditional
dress.
Although the LPR claims that she can speak Yang Zhuang, she insisted to communicate in her Min
Zhuang with me even though I spoke my Debao County Yang Zhuang to her during the whole
investigation. This does not mean that Yang Zhuang and Min Zhuang are intelligible with one another at
a functional level (that is, can understand based on knowledge of their own variety without needing to
learn the other variety). For most Min Zhuang speakers (as well as other non-Yang Zhuang languages) in
Jingxi County, Yang Zhuang is a second language which can be acquired when they go to a school where
Yang Zhuang children are the dominant group. For a Yang Zhuang mother tongue speaker, to understand
Min Zhuang (as well as other non-Yang Zhuang languages in Jingxi County) at a functional level without
learning Min Zhuang is quite difficult. However, most of the Yang Zhuang mother tongue speakers in
Ande, Sanhe, and Longlin Townships can understand Min Zhuang very well because Min Zhuang is the
largest Zhuang group in these townships. It is very commonly seen that a local Yang Zhuang and a local
Min Zhuang speaker communicate with each other in their own languages with inherent understanding.
16.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Proto-
Aspirations
A1-A: 242
B1-A: 32
C1-A: 11ʔ
DL1-A: 32
DS1-A: 32
Voice-
Continuants
A1-C: 353
B1-C: 32
C1-C: 11ʔ
DL1-C: 32
DS1-C: 55
less
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 353
B1-U: 32
C1-U: 11ʔ
DL1-U: 32
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 242
B1-UC: 32
C1-UC: 11ʔ
DL1-UC: 32
DS1-UC: 32
A1-G: 353
B1-G: 32
C1-G: 11ʔ
DL1-G: 32
DS1-G: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 55
B2: 42
C2: 53ʔ
DL2: 42
DS2: 55
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A: 242
breathy sounds
A1/2-C: 353
B1/2: 32
C1/2: 11ʔ
DL1/2: 32
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
DS1/2-A: 32
DS1/2-C: 55
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: three-way splits on A and two-way splits on the others
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1ʼ
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 2, 1/2-C]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The nonstraightforward columns are A and DS. The splits on column A presents as three-way, with voiceless
320
aspirated stops (1-A, 1-UC, and 1/2-A) on one register, voiceless non-aspirated sounds (1-C, 1-U, 1-G,
1/2-C) on one register, and voiced on the other register. The others are all two-way, but the column
DS has very unique and meaningful two-way splits: voiceless aspirated stops (1-A, 1-UC, and 1/2-A)
on one register and other sounds including voiced sounds on the other register. The unique and
important points are as: 1) the merger that voiced sounds go with all other voiceless sound except
aspirated stops is never found in any other Tai varieties, and this may indicate a possible process
accomplishing on the column DS: three-way splits [1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]-[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]-[2] like
in the column A occured first, and later the two registers [1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C] and [2] merged into
each other since they are all high tones, therefore a special two-way split on this column is
established; 2) the split between 1-A and 1-C on column DS confirms that voicelss frictions must be
divided into 1-A and 1-C, since previous studies have only found the split between 1-A and 1-C limited
to the A, B, and DL columns in Debao Yang Zhuang varieties and Dai Zhuang.
Horizontally: four individuals and three mergers
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
Individual
A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[2] = DS[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 2, 1/2-C]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-UC,
1/2-A]
B2[2] = DL[2]
16.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising-falling 242 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A].
Tone 2:
Mid-rising-falling 353 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C].
Tone 3:
High level 55 occurs in A[2].
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 32 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G, 1/2], as well as DS[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 42 occurs in B[2] and DL[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low level 11 in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 7:
High-falling 53 occurs in C[2].
321
16.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect:
Tones C1 and C2 have glottal constrictions, like ha:55ˀ ‘five’, θɯ:11 ‘shirt’, ka:ŋ11ˀ
‘fishbone’, maj53ˀ ‘tree’, te:53ʔ ‘carry on the back’, and ma:53ʔ ‘horse’. However, all the
glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
16.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) Like some other CT varieties such as Jingxi Urban Yang, the items pik55 ‘wing’ in
DL1 and mit55 ‘knife’ in DL2 designed to expect having a long vowel have merged
their tone into DS1(44/55) and DS2 (44/55) respectively, probably triggered by the
shortening of their vowels.
16.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) The terms (na:11) pʰjak 32(DL1-A) ‘forehead’ and pʰjak 32 (DL1-UC) ‘to expose under
the sun’ both have an unexpected short vowel -a, which is expected to be a long
vowel -a: according to the cognate in other Tai varieties. Therefore, they have
become homophonous with the term pʰjak 33 (DS1-A) ‘vegetable’.
2) Like many other CT varieties, the term pik 55 ‘wing’ has a short vowel -i, which is
expected to be a long vowel -i: in DL1-U designated. Therefore, its tone has been
also changed to DS1-U.
3) The vowel of te:k42 ‘to measure’ is unexpected, vs. ta:kDL2 in all Yang Zhuang
varieties and in NT varieties as well as tʰa:kDL2 in Jingxi Lingding and Daxin Baoxu.
4) The vowel of pʰo:32 ‘to chop’ is unexpected, vs. pʰa:B1-A in other CT and SWT
varieties.
322
Language 17 Xiangdu, Tiandeng, Zuojiang Zhuang, CT
17.1 Language information
Language
Zuojiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zzj
Autonym
tʰɔ:C1-A
Exonym
Tiandeng Tuhua; ka:ŋC1 həɰA1 na:jC1
Data Source
3 LRPs
Variety Name
Xiangdu Township (向都鎮, həɰA1 na:jC1 in local Xiangdu Zhuang) of Tiandeng County
Zuojiang Zhuang, which is spoken at the town of Xiangdu Township, Tiandeng
County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Actually, varieties of Zuojiang Zhuang and varieties of Yang Zhuang can more or less
understand with each other in a certain extent. However, they are divided into two
different individual languages based on intelligibility and similarity of wordlist.
Although this Zhuang variety is divided into Zuojiang Zhuang, speakers of this variety
have inherent understanding of Debao County Yang Zhuang at a functional level, that
is Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang and Debao County Yang Zhuang can understand each
other based on knowledge of their own variety without needing to learn the other
variety. This indicates that Tiandeng County Zuojiang Zhuang including this variety
may be a transitional dialect between Zuojiang Zhuang and Yang Zhuang.
17.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
May 18, 2013
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Guótuán (國團)
Family Name
Wáng (王)
Gender
Male
Age
28
Birth Place
Xiangdu Township (向都鎮, həɰA1 na:jC1 in local Xiangdu Zhuang), Tiandeng County,
Chongzuo Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Xiangdu Township (23 years); The main town of Tiandeng County (3 years). Beijing (1
year). Shenzhen, Guangdong Province (1 year). Shanghai (2 years). He is living in
Xiangdu now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
23 years
personal business
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Tiandeng Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of
the LRP’s country.
Baihua
intermediate
323
A Cantonese variety spoken in Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same town. Besides their Xiangdu Tiandeng Zuojiang Zhuang, both of
them can speak Standard Mandarin and Baihua.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Date
May 18, 2013
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Place of data elicitation
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Jiàn (劍)
Family Name
Ćn (岑)
Gender
male
Age
35
Birth Place
Xiangdu Township (向都鎮, həɰA1 na:jC1 in local Xiangdu Zhuang), Tiandeng County,
Chongzuo Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Xiangdu Township (15 years). The main town of Tiandeng County (4 years). Nanning
City (4 years). He is living in the main town of Debao County.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
social service
Languages spoken
Level
Tiandeng Xiangdu Zhuang
mother tongue
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
familiar
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
12 years
Education
bachelor
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in
the northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
Baihua
intermediate
A Cantonese variety spoken in Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same town. Besides his Xiangdu Tiandeng Zuojiang Zhuang, his father
could can speak Baihua. His mother was monolingual in local Zhuang. Both of them passed away.
Remarks
The LRP2 speaks his own Tiandeng Xiangdu Zuojiang Zhuang to Debao Yang Zhuang speakers in his
daily life. I observed that he sometimes tried to use some Debao Yang Zhuang pronunciations and lexical
items which are different from his mother tongue since he has been living in Debao for a long time.
However, his Debao Yang was still with strong Tiandeng Xiangdu accent.
324
Serial number of LRP
LRP3
May 18, 2013
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Baise Prefecture,
Place of data elicitation
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Tīnf́ (
Gender
male
Birth Place
Date
福)
Xiangdu Township (向都鎮, həɰ
A1
C1
na:j
Family Name
D̀ (杜)
Age
28
in local Xiangdu Zhuang), Tiandeng County,
Chongzuo Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Xiangdu Township (23 years). Guangzhou City of Guangdong Province (1 year).
Dongguan City of Guangdong Province (2 years). Other cities in Guangdong Province
(several years).
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
23 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Tiandeng Xiangdu Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in
the northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
Cantonese
intermediate
He learned this language when he was in
Guangdong Province.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same town. Besides his Xiangdu Tiandeng Zuojiang Zhuang, both of his
parents could can speak Baihua and Standard Mandarin. Both of them live with the LRP now.
17.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect are as below.
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 453
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 33
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 33
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 33
A1-UC: 453
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 33
A1-G: 31
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 33
A2: 31
B2: 11̰
C2: 24ʔ
DL2:11
DS2: 11
A1/2: 453
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 33
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
325
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Column A has a
split between 1-UC and 1-G. The column C has no spilts at all.
Horizontally: three individuals and two mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Individual
A[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
B1[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] =
Merger
DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2] = DS[2]
17.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-rising-falling 453 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2].
Tone 2:
Low-falling 31 occurs in A[1-G, 2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], DL [1-A, 1-C, 1-U,
1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 11 occurs in B[2], DL[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
17.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tone C has glottal constrictions, like ha:24ˀ ‘five’, pʰja:j24ˀ ‘to walk’, ɬɯ:24ˀ ‘shirt’,
ka:ŋ24ˀ ‘fishbone’, maj24ˀ ‘tree’, and nam24ˀ ‘water’. Tone B2 has creakiness triggered
by its extreme low pitch. All the voice qualities are not phonemic.
326
17.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Like in some CT varieties such as all Yang Zhuang varieties and Baoxu, the term
ta:33 ‘maternal grandfather’ has an unexpected B1-U tone, which is expected to be
A1-U in the tone box designated, vs. ta:33 (A1-U) in Thai (SWT) and ta:51 (A1-U) in
Du’an Hongshuihe Zhuang (NT). The tone change is speculated as a tonal
contamination by the tone of the term ta:jB1-U ‘maternal grandmother’ in these CT
varieties (Liao 2016, in press).
2) The tone of the term ha:ŋ11 ‘young hen’ has an unexpected B2 tone, which is
expected to be B1(33)<B1/2 in the tone box designated. Because the initial of
this term is also unexpected to be h- which is expected to be kʰ- due to the
correspondence among Xiangdu and other CT/SWT varieties (according to the
comparison among kʰʊmA1<A1/2 ‘bitter’~kʰawC1<C1/2 ‘rice’~ha:ŋB2<B1/2 ‘young hen’ in
This variety, kʰamA1<A1/2 ‘bitter’~kʰaw C1<C1/2 ‘rice’~kʰy:ŋB1<B1/2 ‘young hen’ in
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang (CT), and kʰomA1<A1/2 ‘bitter’~kʰa:w C1<C1/2
‘rice’~kʰɨəŋB1<B1/2 ‘young hen’ in Thai (SWT)), this term should be a loan word
from the neighboring NT varieties, which have an initial reflecting *ɦ- and a tone
B2 merged from B1/2 for this term, like ha:ŋB2<B1/2 in Wuming, and ɣa:ŋB2<B1/2 in
Huishui Bouyei.
17.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial consonant developing from PT *xr- (cf. Li 1977: 233) or *kr(Pittayaporn 2009: 143-144) presents as l- in this dialect, like in la453 (A1-A) ‘to
seek’, lou453 (A1/2) ‘ear’, and laj33 (B1-A) ‘egg’. However, the tonal behavior of this
initial keeps in the A1-A Row as other aspirated sounds. The process of the initial
development might have been like *xr- or *kr- > *kʰr- >*ʰr- > *r- > l-.
2) Short -a- preceding -k has been changed into a long -a:-, as in pʰja:kDS1 ‘vegetable’
= ‘forehead’, pa:kDS1 ‘to stab’ = ‘mouth’, pa:kDS2 ‘tired’ = ‘insane’, and la:kDS2 ‘to
steal’ = ‘root’.
327
Language 18 Naling, Daxin, Zuojiang Zhuang, CT
18.1 Language information
Language
Zuojiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zzj
Autonym
tʰɔ:C1-A
Exonym
Daxin Tuhua
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Dongli (village name, tʊŋB2 lɛ:B2 in local Zhuang) of Daxin Naling Zuojiang Zhuang.
18.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
31 August, 2014
#4101 Dushi Jimei Residential Quarter, Jianshe Road, Nanning
Place of data elicitation
City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Yùb̄ (月
Gender
female
Birth Place
Date
)
Family Name
Zhào (趙)
Age
66
Dongli Community (洞利屯, tʊŋB2 lɛ:B2 in local Zhuang), Nalian Village (那廉村),
Naling Township (那岭鄉, fa͏ɰB2 lɛ:ŋC2 in local Zhuang), Daxin County, Chongzuo
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
She was born in 1948. She grew up and married in her hometown. She moved to
Taocheng Town, the main town of Daxin County, in 1977 with her husband, who
worked as a teacher in Daxin High School. She lived in Taocheng Town from 18771999. From 2000 up to now, she moved to Nanning to live with her daughters.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
school worker
Languages spoken
Level
Daxin Naling Zuojiang
mother tongue
15 years
Education
primary school
Language Remarks
Zhuang
Daxin Encheng Zuojiang
familiar
Zhuang
This language is another variety of Daxin Zuojiang
Zhuang.
Baihua
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of Daxin County.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. He could speak all the languages that the LRP can. LRP’s
mother was born in Balan Village (巴兰村, faɰB2 ˀda:nB1-G in local Zhuang) of Naling Township, Daxin
328
County. Besides her mother tongue Daxin Balan Zuojiang Zhunag, she could speak Daxin Naling
Zhuang, Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, and Baihua.
Remarks
From the simplest tonal patterns in Daxin Naling to the very complicated tonal patterns in Daxin Baoxu
(L23), Zuojiang Zhuang varieties in Daxin County have complicated diversities.
18.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 453
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55/45
Continuants
A1-C: 453
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 55/45
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 453
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 55/45
A1-UC: 453
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC:
DL1-UC:
DS1-UC:
24ʔ
33
55/45
A1-G: 453
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55/45
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 31
B2: 11
C2: 113ʔ
DL2: 11
DS2: 31
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2: 453ʰ
B1/2: 33ʰ
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 55/45
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[2] = DS[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
329
18.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising-falling 453 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], as well
as high-rising 45 or high level 55 in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2] and in DS[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1-C,
1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 11 occurs in B[2] and in DL[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Low-level-rising occurs in C[2].
18.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tone C has glottal constrictions, like ha:24ˀ ‘five’, pʰja:j24ˀ ‘to walk’, ɬɯ:24ˀ ‘shirt’,
ka:ŋ24ˀ ‘fishbone’, maj113ˀ ‘tree’, and nam113ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions are
not phonemic.
330
Language 19 Lingding, Jingxi Zuozhou, Zuojiang Zhuang, CT
19.1 Language information
Language
Zuojiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zzj
Autonym
ɕu:ŋB2; tɕa:B1 tɕowA1;
Exonym
Zazhou; Zuozhou
tʰo:
C1
Data Source
2 LRPs
Variety Name
Lingding (凌
屯, θaŋA2 tɛ:ŋB1 in local Zhuang) Zuozhou of Zuojiang Zhuang,
19.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
Lingding Community (凌
Place of data elicitation
Village (峒
May 16, 2013
屯, θaŋA2 tɛ:ŋB1 in local Zhuang), Dongping
村), Xinjia Township (新甲鄉), Jingxi County, Baise
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Míngchū n (明春)
Family Name
Zh̄u (周)
Gender
male
Age
67
Birth Place
Lingding Community (凌
屯, θaŋA2 tɛ:ŋB1 in local Zhuang), Dongping Village (峒
村),
Xinjia Township (新甲鄉), Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Region, China.
Places lived
He lives in Lingding village during all his life.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
67 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Lingding Zuozhou Zuojiang Zhuang
mother tongue
Jingxi County Yang Zhuang
intermediate
primary school
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca of Jingxi
County.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in
the northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
Standard Mandarin
a little
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents born in the same village. Besides his mother tongue Lingding Zuozhou, LRP’s father can
communicate in Jingxi County Yang Zhuang and Jingxi Min Zhuang. LRP’s mother was monolingual in
Zuozhou, but can understand Jingxi Yang Zhuang.
Remarks
Zuozhou is a specific group of Zuojiang Zhuang. In Jackson et al. (2011), Zuozhou is not classified in any
studied individual Zhuang languages listed in ISO 639-3 because their data is limited. However,
331
according to my personal fieldwork research, Zuozhou is an immigration group migrating from Zuozhou
Township, Jiangzhou District, Chongzuo Prefecture of Guangxi. The name Zuozhou is actually their
ancestral home. Because the Zhuang dialect in Zuozhou Township is put under Zuojiang Zhuang in
previous studies in China, I also treat this group as a variety of Zuojiang Zhuang, which is listed as “zzj”
in ISO 639-3. However, Zuozhou is a specific group containing their own internal characteristics.
Varieties of Zuozhou may consist of an individual language with its own ISO 639 code from
intelligibility, from similarity of wordlist and from speaker attitudes in further studies. For convenience,
in this thesis I use Zuozhou to refer to this specific group rather than Zuojiang Zhuang in my
formulation. Therefore, Jingxi Lingding Zuozhou is used instead of Jingxi Lingding Zuojiang Zhuang.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Date
Lingding Community (凌
Village (峒
Place of data elicitation
May 16, 2013
屯, θaŋA2 tɛ:ŋB1 in local Zhuang), Dongping
村), Xinjia Township (新甲鄉), Jingxi County, Baise
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Guó’ān (國安)
Family Name
Ĺng (凌)
Gender
male
Age
61
Birth Place
Lingding Community (凌
(峒
屯, θaŋA2 tɛ:ŋB1 in local Zhuang), Dongping Village
村), Xinjia Township (新甲鄉), Jingxi County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He was born, grew up, studied, and worked in the same village during all his life. The
Dongping High School is where he studied, and Dongping Primary School is where he
studied and taught.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
retired teacher
61 years
Education
senior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Zuozhou
mother tongue
Jingxi County Yang Zhuang
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of Jingxi County.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
familiar
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. He spoke Zuozhou and Jingxi County Yang Zhuang. He passed
away. LRP’s mother was born in Dazhong Community (大種屯), Daci Village (大慈村), Ronglao Township
(榮勞鄉), Jingxi County. Besides her mother tongue Dazhong Zuozhou, she can speak Jingxi County Yang
Zhuang. She lives with LRP now.
332
19.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops except tones C in this
dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 353
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Proto-
Continuants
A1-C: 554
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 55
Voiceless
Unaspirated
Sounds
stops
A1-U: 554
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 353
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 21
B1-G: 31
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 31
DS1-G: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 21
B2: 31
C2: 42ʔ
DL2: 31
DS2: 21
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A: 353
breathy sounds
A1/2-C: 554
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 55
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: three-way splits and two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
[1-G, 2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-G, 2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-G, 2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development.
Horizontally: three individuals and four mergers
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[C2]
A[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[1-G, 2] = DS[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]= DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2]
B[1-G, 2] = DL[1-G, 2]
333
19.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones
Tone 1:
High level-falling 554 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U, 1/2-C] (continuants and
unaspirated stops).
Tone 2:
Mid-rising-falling 353 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A] (aspirated sounds), and
high level 55 occurs in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Low-falling 21 occurs in A[1-G, 2] and DS[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1/2], and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[1-G, 2], and in DL[1-G, 2], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 7:
High-falling 42 occurs in C[2].
19.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect: Tones C1 and C2
have glottal constrictions, like ha:24ˀ ‘five’, θɯ:24ˀ ‘shirt’, ka:ŋ24ˀ ‘fishbone’, maj42ˀ
‘tree’, and nam42ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
19.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The three-way split in Column A is similar to Huashan Min Zhuang (L16).
Aspiration (1-A, 1-UC, 1/2) depress the tone pitch from the original high register
(1) to change to a lower pitch to form a third register (1'), which is not merged
into the low register (2) like in Debao Yang varieties.
2) Like many other CT varieties, the term pik33 ‘wing’ has a short vowel -i, which is
expected to be a long vowel -i: in DL1-U designated. However, its tone (33) is
preserved to be in DL1-U. This is very similar to Debao Dalong Yang Zhuang (L9).
3) Unlike other CT/SWT varieties, the terms nuk55 ‘deaf’ and hɯk55 ‘gums’ which are
expected to be long vowels in Column DL1 designated have short vowels like NT
varieties, and their tones have also been changed from Columns DL1 to DS1.
19.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The word (japDS1) nemA1 ‘in addition’ has an unexpected initial n-, vs. hɛ:mA1 ‘more’
in Rong Maet Tai Lue, (ʔejC1) he:mA1 in Debao Urban Yang Zhuang, and tʰe:m ‘in
addition’ in Jingxi Anning Yang Zhuang. It may have undergone an initial change
due to phonological contamination of the initial of the first segment of this item,
namely the initial of nemA1 may have been changed from tʰ- (stop) to n-
(continuant) due to the analogy of j- (continuant).
334
Language 20 Xiaoguangnan, Nong Zhuang, CT
20.1 Language information
Language
Nong Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zhn
Autonym
nɔŋA2
Exonym
Nong Dao
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 47-48, 157-159, 595-808; Johnson 2011: 20-28
Data Info.
The language data was collected from Xiaoguangnan Village, Xiaoguangnan
Community, Liancheng Township, Guangnan County, Yunnan Province, China.
20.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flops runs through B, C, and DL tones.
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 35
B1-A: 12
C1-A: 22
DL1-A: 13
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 35
B1-C: 12
C1-C: 22
DL1-C: 13
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 35
B1-U: 12
C1-U: 22
DL1-U: 13
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 35
B1-UC: 12
C1-UC: 22
DL1-UC: 13
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 35
B1-G: 12
C1-G: 22
DL1-G: 13
DS1-G: 55
A2: 33
B2: 31
C2: 53
DL2: 31
DS2: 33
A1/2: 35
B1/2: 12
C1/2: 22
DL1/2: 13
DS1/2: 55
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
335
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[2] = DS[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
20.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], as well as
high-level in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[2] and DS[2], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid-level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
Tone 4:
Low-falling 21 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2], and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 41 occurs in C[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2], and in DL[2], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
20.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect.
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
336
Language 21 Dazhai, Dai Zhuang, CT
21.1 Language information
Language
Dai Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zhg
Autonym
dajA2
Exonym
Tulao; Tuzu; Dai
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 48-50, 164-165, 595-808
Reference
Johnson 2011:28-38
Data Info.
According to the original material, this language is commonly called Dai in local
Zhuang, or Tulao in the local Chinese dialect in that area. The language data
was collected from Dazhai Village, Heimo Township, Wenshan County, Yunnan
Province, China.
21.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, only a tonal flip-flop runs through Tone A in this
dialect.
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 31
B1-A: 53
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 53
DS1-A: 31
Continuants
A1-C: 112
B1-C: 53
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 53
DS1-C: 33
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 112
B1-U: 53
C1-U: 55
DL1-U: 53
DS1-U: 33
A1-UC: 31
B1-UC: 53
C1-UC: 55
DL1-UC: 53
DS1-UC: 31
A1-G: 112
B1-G: 53
C1-G: 55
DL1-G: 53
DS1-G: 33
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 31
B2: 42
C2: 33
DL2: 42
DS2: 31
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A:31
breathy sounds
A1/2-C:112
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 55
DL1/2: 53
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
DS1/2-A: 31
DS1/2-C:33
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: two-way split
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2-A]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2-A]
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal split
patterns are found in Column A and DS.
337
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
A[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2-A] = DS[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2-A]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
B[2] = DL[2]
C[2] = DS[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
21.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low level-rising 112 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2-A] and DS[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2A] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 3:
High-falling 53 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DL[1-A, 1C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth
and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-falling 42 occurs in B[2], and in DL[2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 6:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[2] and DS [1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively
21.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
338
Language 22 Lei Ping, Zuojiang Zhuang, CT
22.1 Language information
Language
Zuojiang Zhuang
Autonym
po:n
C1-U h B2
Data Source
Hudak 2008: 27-30
Reference
Zhang et al. 1999:44-45, 146-149, 595-808
Data Info.
According to the original material, Gedney collected the Pei Ping data from Mr. Liang
t i:
ʋa:
B2
ISO 639-3 Code
zzj
Exonym
tʰo:C1-U
Shao-lu (62-year-old) in 1966, in Hong Kong. The informant called the language as
po:nC1-U thi:B2 ʋa:B2, which means ‘the native language’ in this language (Hudak 2008:
27). This language is spoken in Leiping Township (in nowadays Daxin County) in
southwest Guangxi. The Zhuang dialect Daxin Houyi described in Zhang et al. (1999) is
almost identical with Leiping investigated by Gedney according to the data
comparison. Because Houyi is a village name in nowadays Leiping Township, it can be
supposed that Houyi and Lei Ping are of the same variety even though the tonal values
between Houyi and Lei Ping are a little bit different (probably due to different auditory
sense between the researchers). As for tonal categories, the only difference in the
descriptions between Lei Ping and Houyi is that there is no tonal split in Column C in
Lei Ping (Hudak 2008: 28), but in Zhang et al. (1999: 146) Houyi contains some single
items containing Tone 4 (C2) separating from Tone 3 (C1), such as naj44 ‘this’.
Nevertheless, in Houyi the original Tone 4 (C2) has been merged into Tone 3 (C1) in
most situation just like what Gedney’s observation. It can be assumed that Gedney did
not observe the remnant of the original C2 tone in such as naj44 ‘this’ because of the
data limitation. Here I adopt Gedney’s description of Lei Ping to draw the tone box, but
data from Houyi in Zhang et al. (1999) are important supplement to Gedney’s Lei Ping
because Houyi’s vocabularies are much more plenty.
22.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops except Tones C in this
dialect.
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 454
B1-A: 44ʔ
C1-A: 13ʔ
DL1-A: 44
DS1-A: 44
Continuants
A1-C: 454
B1-C: 44ʔ
C1-C: 13ʔ
DL1-C: 44
DS1-C: 44
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 44ʔ
B1-U: 11
C1-U: 13ʔ
DL1-U: 44
DS1-U: 11
A1-UC: 454
B1-UC: 44ʔ
C1-UC: 13ʔ
DL1-UC: 44
DS1-UC: 44
A1-G: 44ʔ
B1-G: 11
C1-G: 13ʔ
DL1-G: 44
DS1-G: 11
A2: 21
B2: 11
C2: 13ʔ
DL2: 21/11
DS2: 11
A1/2-A: 454
B1/2: 44ʔ
C1/2: 13ʔ
DL1/2: 44
DS1/2: 44
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
339
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: three-way split, two way split and no split
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-G]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-G, 2]
[1-U, 1-G, 2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. There are secondary
tonal splits found in Columns A, B, DS, and Column C has merged the two registers.
Horizontally: three individuals and two mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
Individual
A[2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
A[1-U, 1-G] = B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] =
Merger
DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
B[1-U, 1-G, 2] = DL[2] = DS[1-U, 1-G, 2]
22.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-rising-falling 454 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1/2-A] (aspirated sounds).
Tone 2:
High level 44 occurs in A[1-U, 1-G] as well as B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2], DL[1-A,
1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid-falling 21 occurs in A[2].
Tone 4:
Low level 11 occurs B[1-U, 1-G, 2] as well as in DL[2] and DS[1-U, 1-G, 2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 13 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
22.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
The tone on Column C is glottalized. The tone with tonal value 44 (A[1-U, 1-G]) and
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]) has a final drop before pause (Hudak 2008: 28).
340
Language 23 Baoxu, Zuojiang Zhuang, CT
23.1 Language information
Language
Zuojiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zzj
Autonym
tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Nung Fan Sling
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Naya Community (那
屯, ˀba:nC1-G na:A2 jɯ:A1-G in local Zhuang), Shangyi Village
(尚益村), Baoxu Township (寶墟鄉, faɯB2 ɕɔ:kDL2 in local Zhuang), Daxin County,
Chongzuo Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
23.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Room 403, Nanning Gucheng Road Branch, City Express Inn
Given Name
Hǎihuá ( 海華)
Family Name
Nóng (農)
Gender
male
Age
29
Birth Place
Naya Community (那
(
Date
August 31, 2014
屯, ˀba:nC1-G na:A2 jɯ:A1-G in local Zhuang), Shangyi Village
益村), Baoxu Township, Daxin County, Chongzuo Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He grew up in the village in where he was born in 1985. When he was 15, he went
to Taocheng Town, the main town of Daxin County, to study in high school for 6
years. From 2003 to 2010 he had his undergraduate and postgraduate studies in
Nanning City. After postgraduate he has been working in Nanning City for four
years.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
executive secretary
9 years
Education
M.A.
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Daxin Baoxu Zuojiang Zhuang
mother tongue
Baihua
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of Daxin County.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
English
intermediate
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides his Daxin Baoxu Zhuang as native, he can
communicate in Baihua and Guiliu Standard Mandarin with his Zhuang accents.
LRP’s mother was born in Banzhou Community (板 屯, ˀba:nC1-G ɕowC2 in local Zhuang) village,
Jingyang Village (景陽村), Baoxu Township, Daxin County. Besides her mother tongue Daxin Baoxu
341
Zuojiang Zhuang, she can communicate in Baihua and Guiliu Standard Mandarin with his Zhuang
accents.
Remarks
Together with Lei Ping and other Zuojiang Zhuang varieties in Daxin County, Baoxu has complicated
tonal split and merger patterns. The most noticeable point is the three-way split pattern found in
Columns A, C, and DS, which has a secondary tonal split conditioned together by glottalized sounds (AG) and unaspirated stops (A-U). This kind of tonal split conditioning factor is commonly found in SWT
varieties (like Bangkok Thai and Isan), but is very rare found in CT varieties.
23.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 45
B1-A: 34
C1-A: 13ʔ
DL1-A: 34
DS1-A: 13
Continuants
A1-C: 45
B1-C: 34
C1-C: 13ʔ
DL1-C: 34
DS1-C: 13
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 34ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 34
A1-UC:45
B1-UC: 34
C1-UC: 13ʔ
DL1-UC: 34
DS1-UC: 13
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 34ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 34
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 11̰ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 11
A1/2: 45
B1/2: 34
C1/2: 13ʔ
DL1/2: 34
DS1/2: 13
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: three-way splits and two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-G]
[2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
[1-U, 1-G, 2]
[1-U, 1-G]
[1-U, 1-G, 2]
[1-U, 1-G]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
342
[2]
[2]
Horizontally: three individuals and four mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
Individual
A[[1-U, 1-G]
A[2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] = C[1-U, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
Merger
B[1-U, 1-G, 2] = DL[1-U, 1-G, 2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]
C[2] = DS[2]
23.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones.
Tonal value 34 can be found in the following boxes: B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2], C[1-U,
1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]. Column DL on checked syllables can be ignored
when discussing the difference between the two columns in smooth syllables. It is
very interesting that this tone 34 is not glottalized in Column B (1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2)
but has clear glottal constriction in Column C (1-U, 1-G). It indicates that before
tonal merger, B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] and C[1-U, 1-G] were two different tones, and
this tone in Column C still preserves the glottal constriction even its tonal value has
been merged with B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2]. However, because its tonal value 34
complementarily distributes in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] (conditioned by voiceless
frictions) and C[1-U, 1-G] (conditioned by voiceless unaspirated stops and glottal
sounds) with different groups of initials, the glottalized feature of C[1-U, 1-G]
cannot account for as phonemic or tonemic. Therefore, B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] and
C[1-U, 1-G] has to be treated as allotones of the same toneme.
Tone 1:
High-rising 45 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2].
Tone 2:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-U, 1-G].
Tone 3:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2].
Tone 4:
Mid-rising 34 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] and C[1-U, 1-G], as well as in
DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-U, 1-G, 2] and in DL[1-U, 1-G, 2]
to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-rising 13 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC, 1/2] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-UC,
1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 7:
Low level [11] occurs in C[2] = DS[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
343
23.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect: All the three
tones in Column C have glottal constrictions, like ha:13ˀ ‘five’, ɕa:j13ˀ ‘to walk’, ɬɯ:13ˀ
‘shirt’, ka:ŋ34ˀ ‘fishbone’, ˀba:n34ˀ ‘village’, and nam11̰ˀ ‘water’. Tone C2 has a
creakiness triggered from its extremely low pitch. All the glottal constrictions are
not phonemic.
23.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) PT *pʰl/r- (cf. Li 1977: 87-88) or *pr- (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 140-141,
230) is changed to be ɕ- in this dialect, like in the term ɕa:45 (A1-A) ‘hill; cliff’, and
ɕa:j24ʔ ‘to walk’. PT *khr- (cf. Li 1977: 228-229) or *qr- (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009: 144-145, 330) is also changed to be ɕ- in this dialect like in ɕaj34 (B1-A)
‘egg’. That is to say these two PT initials have been merged into one another.
However, the tonal behavior of ɕ- in this dialect is preserved to be with aspirated
sounds in the 1-A row, which is different from the tonal behaviors of the 1-U and
1-G rows. This indicates the process of the initial development of ɕ- may have
processed a period of aspiration, as *pʰl/r- or*pr- > *pʰr- > *pʰj- > *kʰj- > *tɕʰ-
> ɕ- and khr- or*qr- > *kʰr- > *kʰj- > *tɕʰ- > ɕ- due to the neighboring CT
varieties which have pʰja:A1-A ‘hill; cliff’ (in all Yang Zhuang varieties) or kʰja:A1-A
‘hill; cliff’ (in some Zuojiang Zhuang varieties like Tiandeng Bahe which has
merged PT *pr- and *kr- into kʰj-), pʰja:jC1-A ‘to walk’ as well as kʰjajB1-A ‘egg’ for
these terms. Since PT *pl- (cf. Li 1977: 84-85; Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 147148) in this dialect has been merged into *kl- as a modern tɕ- via a possible
process *pl- > *pj- > *kj- > tɕ- like in the term tɕa:53 (A1-U) ‘fish’, the
counterpart PT *pr- is suggested to process the transitional succession *pʰj- >
*kʰj- > *tɕʰ- > ɕ-. However, Daxin Naling (Language 18), which is another Daxin
Zuojiang Zhuang variety in the same county, has two optional variations pʰja:A1-A
and ɕa:A1-A for ‘hill; cliff’. This also suggests that the initial development of this
consonant in Baoxu may have been directly changed from pʰj- into ɕ-.
2) Initial consonants in the supposed 1-UC row which is to follow Li’s unaspirated
stop + *r- clusters or ASC (Aspiration induced by Sesquisyllabic Clusters) row
which is to be suggested as a series of sesquisyllable cluster following the
hypothesis provided by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) normally regularly present
as aspirated sounds or a sound with spread feature (h-), like in ha:45 (A1-UC) ‘eye’,
ha:j45 (A1-UC) ‘die’, pʰe:ŋ45 (A1-UC) ‘cucumber’, and kʰa:ŋ45 (A1-UC) ‘to put up’ like
other CT and Yongnan Zhuang varieties. However, the item taɰ53 (A1-U<A1-UC)
which is expected to be in this row due to the cognate in other CT varieties (like
tʰɔjA1-UC in Yang Zhuang varieties and tʰaɰA1-UC in Tiandeng) has a unexpected
unaspirated initial stop t- and a tone corresponding to be in the A1-U row. The
344
term ‘gizzard’ with an unaspirated initial stop t- is also found in some other Tai
varieties like Yongnan Zhuang Varieties which have an additional tonal split due
to the proposed ASC row. The irregularity involved in this term needs further
investigation.
3) The term ˀdaŋ353 ‘to make fire’ has an unexpected Tone A1-G, which is expected to
be C1-G in the tone box designated, vs. ˀdaŋC1-G in Longsang (NT) and Debao Yang
Zhuang (CT). This term may have been undergoing an analogical change or
phonological contamination by replacing its original *C tone to *A tone from *vɛjA
‘fire’ (cf. Li 1977: 79, 286) or *wɤjA ‘fire’ (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 207)
according to the frequently used compound word *ʔdaŋC *vɛjA/*wɤjA ‘to make fire’.
4) The collocation between tones and vowels on boxes DS1-A, DS1-C, DS1-UC, and
DS1/2 is interesting as follows: boxes DS1-A, DS1-C, DS-UC, and DS1/2 have a
low-rising tonal value 13, and since a short vowel hardly fulfils a low rising tonal
value, items with these groups of tones have been lengthened their original short
vowels for coordinating to this tonal value, like ɕa:k13 (DS1-A) ‘vegetable’, he:t13
(DS1-A) ‘to do’, ha:p13 (DS1-A) ‘to shut’, ma:t13 (DS1-C) ‘flea’, na:k13 (DS1-A) ‘heavy’,
wa:t13 (DS1-A) ‘have a cold’, ɬe:p31 (DS1/2-C > DS2) ‘ten’, and ɬo:k13 (DS1/2-C>
DS2) ‘cooked’, while other items like het11 (DS1-A > DS2) ‘centipede’, wak11 (DS1C > DS2) ‘to nod’, lap11 (DS1-C > DS2) ‘to close (eyes)’, and kʰɔp11 (DS1/2-A >
DS2) ‘to bite’ have been changed their original tones to a low-level tone for
keeping their vocalic length.
5) The item ma:t13 (DS1-C) ‘flea’ has a free variation mat11 (DS1-C > DS2). The item
mat11 (DS2) ‘grain’ also has a free variation mat34 (DS1ʼ) ‘grain’. These phenomena
may have internal relationship. The term mat11 (DS2) ‘grain’ may be changed its
original DS2 tone to the derived DS1' tone (tonal value 34) for avoiding being
homophonous with the item mat11 (DS1-C > DS2) ‘flea’ after being changed from
DS1-A/C to DS2 as a free variation. That is to say, mat11 (DS1-C > DS2) ‘flea’
which is the free variation of ma:t13 (DS1-C) ‘flea’ might have urged the tone of
mat11 ‘grain’ to change from DS2 (11) to DS1' (34) to form its free variation.
However, this process is not completed; therefore, both mat11 and mat34 stand for
‘grain’.
23.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect:
1) Sound changes on the term ham45 (A1-A) ‘in addition’ are as follows: like some
other CT and SWT varieties such as Debao Urban and Northern Tai, the initial of
this term has been irregularly changed from *tʰ- to h- due to its grammaticalized
process (to add (v.) > in addition (conj.)); probably due to the same
345
grammaticalized process, the term has an unexpected vowel -a, which is expected
to be -e according to other Tai varieties, like he:m453 in Debao Urban, hɛ:m13 in
Chiang Mai, and tʰɛ:m24 in Bangkok.
2) The term ma:j45 ‘to face up’ has an unexpected initial m-, which is expected to be
ŋ- in the tone box designated according to the correspondence to the cognate in
other Tai varieties, like ŋa:jA1-C in Thai and in Yang Zhuang varieties.
3) PT *-e (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 223) is diphthongized as -ej like in kej33
(B1-U) ‘old’. This is very rare since PT *-e is generally preserved as a mid-vowel in
most CT and NT varieties whereas is lowered as -ɛ in some CT and most SWT
varieties.
4) PT *-o (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 228) is diphthongized as -ow like in
pʰow33 (B2) ‘male; father’. This is very rare since PT *-o is generally preserved as a
mid-vowel in most CT and NT varieties whereas is lowered as -ɔ in some CT and
most SWT varieties.
5) PT *-ʰɲ- (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 228) is yodicized as nj- in this dialect
like in nja:24ʔ (C1-C) ‘grass’. This is very rare since PT *-ʰɲ- is generally preserved
as ɲ- or is changed to a yod j- in the vast majority of Tai varieties.
6) PT *r- has been merged into *s- (cf. Li 1977:152, Pittayawat Pittayaporn
2009:115) or *ɬ- (cf. Liao & Shen 2012) to be a modern ɬ- in this language, like in
the terms ɬaj33 (B2) ‘dryfarmland’ and ɬa:j11 (C2) ‘bad’.
346
Language 24 Bac Va, Nung, CT
24.1 Language information
Language
Bac Va
ISO 639-3 Code
nut
Autonym
Nung
Exonym
Nung Fan Sling
Data Source
Hudak 1997: 1147-1186
Data Info.
The data of Bac Va are adopted from Gedney’s study on the Tai dialect of Bac Va (Hudak
1997). According to the original material, Gedney collected the data from Mr. Tran Phuc
Ky on July 19, 1964, in Dalat, Vietnam. This language is commonly called Nung, and is
spoken in northeastern Vietnam near Phu Lang Thuong (Bac Giang).
24.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, a tonal flip-flops occurs on C in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 14
B1-A: 21ʔ
C1-A: 11ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 14
Continuants
A1-C: 45
B1-C: 14
C1-C: 11
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 14
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 45
B1-U: 14
C1-U: 11ʔ
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 14
A1-UC: 14
B1-UC: 21ʔ
C1-UC: 11ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 14
A1-G: 45
B1-G: 21ʔ
C1-G: 11
DL1-G: 55
DS1-G: 14
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 33
B2: 32
C2: 55ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 32
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2-A: 14
breathy sounds
A1/2-C: 45
B1/2: 21ʔ
C1/2: 11ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 14
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: three-way splits and two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A]
[2]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
DL
[1-C, 1-U, 1-G]
[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
[2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal
split patterns are found in Columns A, B, and DL.
347
Horizontally: three individuals and four mergers
A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C]
Individual
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A] = B[1-C, 1-U]= DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]
Merger
A[2] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2]
B[2] = DS[2]
C[2] = DL[1-C, 1-U, 1-G]
24.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones.
In the original material, Gedney determines eight tones to the open syllables.
However, Tone 6 (occurring on Liao & Shen’s C1-A, C1-U, C1-UC, C1/2 tonal
categories) and Tone 7 (occurring on Liao & Shen’s on C1-C and C1-G) are both low
level 11, and the only difference is that there is a the glottalized feature on Tone 6.
Just like Tones B1 and C1' in Baoxu, since Tones 6 and 7 are in a complementary
distribution with different groups of initial consonants, the glottalized cannot be
treated as phonemic or tonemic. Here I treat them to be the same tone, and reduce
the tone numbers of this dialect from eight to seven.
Tone 1:
Low-rising 14 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1/2-A], as well as in B[1-C, 1-U] and DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
High-rising 45 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U, 1-G, 1/2-C].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[2] and DL[1-A, 1-UC, 2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-falling 21 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 5:
Mid-falling 32 occurs in B[2] and DS[2] to form a complementary distribution
in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low level 11 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2].
Tone 7:
High level 55 occurs in C[2] and DL[1-C, 1-U, 1-G] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
24.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
According to Gedney’s original text, Tone 4 (B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]), Tone 7 (C2)
and Tone 6 (C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2]) all have glottalized feature (Hudak
1997: 1147). We do not know which type of glottalization, final glottal constriction
or creakiness, these tones are of since they are not mentioned in the original text. All
the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
348
Language 25 Yongning Baiji, Yongnan Zhuang, CT/NT?
25.1 Language information
Language
Sanxiang YN
ISO 639-3 Code
zyn
Autonym
ɬa:mA1 jiəŋA1; tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Sanxianghua ( 鄉話); jajC1-G
Data Source
2 LRPs
Reference
Zhang et al. 1999: 42-44, 119-127, 595-808.
Variety Name
Baiji of Sanxiang Yongnan Zhuang (YN). This variety is spoken in the rural and urban
areas of Baiji Township (百濟鄉), Yongning District (邕
區), Nanning City, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
According to my own field trip, this language is commonly known as Sanxiang (ɬa:mA1
jiəŋA1), which literally means ‘three townships’ in Chinese and in Zhuang. The main
areas of this language are the three townships Baiji (百濟), Nalou (那樓) and Zhonghe
(中和) in Yongning District of Nanning City. Although Sanxiang is a language under
Yongnan Zhuang (ISO 639-3: zyn), it is not intelligible with other YN varieties like
Shuangding, Long’an and Nung An. Besides, speakers of Sanxiang have strong sense of
internal identity towards to speakers of any varieties of Sanxiang, but lack of this
identity with speakers of other YN varieties and other Zhuang languages. Varieties of
Sanxiang may put together to consist a new individual language with ISO 639-3 code
in further research and surveys.
25.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Date
Railway Hotel, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Q̀ngtáng (慶唐)
Gender
male
Birth Place
Tunsu Community (屯蘇坡, ˀbua:n
(屯林村, tʰuən lum tɕʰɔ:n
A1
District (邕
Places lived
May 13, 2013
A2
A1
C1
tʰuən ɬɔ:
A1
Family Name
Xì (謝)
Age
36
A1
in local Zhuang), Tunlin Village
in local Zhuang), Baiji Township (百濟鄉), Yongning
區), Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Tunsu Community (16 years). Pumiao Town (蒲廟鎮), the main town of Yongning
County (nowadays Yongning District) (3 years). The main town of Daxin County
(1 year). He is now in Nanning City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
self-employed entrepreneurs
Languages spoken
Level
Baiji Sanxiang YN
mother
16 years
Education
Language Remarks
tongue
349
junior high school
Baihua
fluent
This language is a Cantonese variety in Guangxi and is a lingua
franca in the main town of Yongning District and in Nanning
City.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Pinghua
intermediate
A Sinitic Language spoken in the Central part of Guangxi, China.
It is closed to Cantonese, and is supposed to be the official
language in Guangxi in history before the Guiliu Southwestern
Mandarin came to Guangxi.
Naknai
intermediate
A Cantonese variety spoken in Qinzhou City of Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Baiji Sanxiang as native, he can communicate in
Baihua. LRP’s mother was born in Nafeng Village (那豐村, nuəA2 fʊŋA1 in local Yang Zhuang), Naxiang
Township (那鄉鎮), Qinzhou City, Guangxi. She is monolingual in her Sanxiang YN. She lives with LRP
in Nanning City now.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Place of data
Hengyang Xilu Nantie Er Jie 1, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
elicitation
Region, China.
Given Name
Yù ngāo (運高)
Gender
male
Birth Place
Date
Tanhuai Community (壇槐坡, tʰəm
tʰuən lum tɕʰɔ:n
A1
A2
A1
A2
ma:j
A2
May 19, 2013
Family Name
Lù (陸)
Age
39
in local Zhuang), Tunlin Village (屯林村,
in local Zhuang), Baiji Township (百濟鄉), Yongning District (邕
區), Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Tanhuai Community (15 years). Hengyang City, Hunan Province (4 years). Pingguo
County (2.5 years). Baise City (10 years). He is now living in Nanning.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
engineering supervision
Languages spoken
Level
Baiji Sanxiang YN
mother tongue
Baihua
fluent
6 years
Education
on-the-job undergraduate
Language Remarks
This language is a Cantonese variety in Guangxi and is a
lingua franca in the main town of Yongning District and in
Nanning City.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu
intermediate
A Sinitic Language spoken in the Central part of Guangxi,
China. It is closed to Cantonese, and is supposed to be the
official language in Guangxi in history before the Guiliu
Southwestern Mandarin came to Guangxi.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Baiji Sanxiang as native, he can communicate in
Baihua and a little Standard Mandarin. LRP’s mother was born in Sanlexia Community (
350
樂
坡),
Shitang Village (石塘村), Lingli Township (伶俐鎮), Qingxiu District (青秀區), Naning City, Guangxi. Her
mother tongue is of Yongbei Zhuang (a NT language). Besides, she can speak Baiji Sanxiang YN and
Standard Mandarin, and a little Baihua. She lives with LRP in Nanning City now.
25.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
Aspirations
A1-A: 53
B1-A: 343
C1-A: 445ʔ
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 343
C1-C: 445ʔ
Unaspirated stop
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 343
C1-U: 445ʔ
DL
DS
DL1-A: 34
DL1’-A: 445
DS1-A: 55
DL1-C: 34
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
DL1’-C: 445
DS1-C: 55
DL1-U: 34
DS1-U: 55
DL1'-U: 445
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
A1-UC:
B1-UC:
C1-UC:
53
343
445ʔ
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 343
C1-G: 445ʔ
DL1-UC: 34
DS1-UC: 55
DL1-G: 34
Glottal Sounds
DS1-G: 55
DL1'-G: 445
DL2: 31
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 331
B2: 31
C2: 445ʔ
DS2: 33
DL2': 445
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
A1/2:
331
B1/2: 31
C1/2: 445ʔ
DL1/2: 31
DS1/2: 33
2) The tonal complexity in the C and DL columns (the unexpected tones are all in
dark-orange) does not suggest the tonal flip-flop or the non-straightforward
development. In this dialect, most items with expected C2 tone have merged
their original C2 tone into C1, a high-level-rising tone 445, to be treated as the
merged C2. The derived tone DL1’ on DL1-A and DLA-C has been found from the
wordlist, like tʰa:k445 (DL1'-A) ‘to prop’ and ɬa:k445 (DL1’-C) ‘pestle’. According to a
supplementary investigation to this dialect by confirming with LRP2, I verified
that this high-level-rising (DL1') are not only found on DL1-U and DL1-G, but
also on found on DL2 as a derived tone of DL2. According to the examples
provided by LRP2, the derived tones DL1’ and DL2’ are both a high-level-rising
445 (vs. DL1=223 and DL2=31), and are mainly used for Middle Chinese loans,
like lwak445 (DL2’) ‘happy’ (<樂 vs. lɤ51 Mandarin, lɔk22 Cantonese),ŋwa:k445
351
(DL2’) ‘prison’ (<獄 vs. y51 Mandarin, jʊk22 Cantonese), hwa:k445 (DL2’) ‘to learn’
(<學 vs. ɕyɛ35 Mandarin, hɔk22 Cantonese), lwa:p445 (DL2’) ‘dried meat’ (<臘 vs.
la51 Mandarin, la:p22 Cantonese), tʰwa:p445 (DL2’) ‘to tread’ (<踏 vs. tʰa53
Mandarin, ta:p22 Cantonese), lo:p445 (DL2’) ‘green’ (<綠 vs. ly51 Mandarin, lʊk22
Cantonese), me:k445 (DL2’) ‘wheat’ (<麥 vs. mai53 Mandarin, mak22 Cantonese),
and etc. These merged or derived tones are of secondary aberrant development
due to MC loans and are out of the evaluation criteria of the conditioning of
tonal splits. Therefore, they cannot be a reference substance of tonal flip-flops or
the directionality of tonal development. Actually this kind of complicated tonal
splits on dead syllable due to MC loans are found in all the YN varieties
investigated in this thesis (L25-29). They are omitted in this study because they
are beyond the tonal conditioning factors like initials or vowel-length.
3) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two-way splits and no-split
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
[1'-A, 1'-C, 1'-U, 1'-UC, 1'-G]
[2']
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development. Similar to the previous
point 1), the derived tones on DL1-A and DL1-C are not conditioned by any homogeneous initial
groups. Many of them are used for Middle Chinese loans. They are of secondary aberrant
development and are out of the evaluation criteria of the conditioning of tonal splits. Therefore,
they cannot be a reference substance of the directionality of tonal development.
Horizontally: no individual and five mergers; there are five tones on checked syllables, and each of
them is on the complementary distribution with those five tones on smooth syllable.
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[1-A', 1-C', 2']
352
25.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones:
Tone 1:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], as well as high-level 55
in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid level-falling 331 occurs in A[2, 1/2], and mid level 33 in DS[2, 1/2], to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 3:
Low-rising-falling 343 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], and mid rising
34 in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and in DL[2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-level-rising 445 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2], and in the
derived DL[1'-A, 1'-C, 1'-U, 1'-G, 2'], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
25.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
The tone on the C column has a glottal constriction, like ha:445ʔ ‘five’, pʰa:j445ˀ ‘to
walk’, ɬom445ˀ ‘sour’, taŋ445ˀ ‘to establish’, b̤wa:n445ˀ ‘village’, and nam445ˀ ‘water’.
Syllables with B1 and B2 Tones have a slight breathy final -ɦ. All tones on the lower
register except those on the C column always accompany a slight breathy voice. All
the glottal constrictions are not phonemic.
25.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Labialization is very common seen before -a: and -a with initials on earlier voiced
and glottal rows in this dialect, like nwa:331 (A2) ‘rice field’, dwa:331 (A2) ‘to spread
on’, kʰwa:331 (A2) ‘to stuck’, d̤wa:31 (B2) ‘river’, and b̤ wa:tDL1-G ‘time’. Sometimes the
labialized segment -w- presents as an unrounded -ɰ-, like in t ̤ɰak55 (DS1-G) ‘deep
night, to fall in sleep’. The term lwa:31 (B2<A1) ‘to seek’ been has changed its tone
from A1-A to B2 probably due to its labialized initial, which is commonly seen
before -a: and -a on lower register tones69. On the contrary, the term ma:k34
(DL1<DL2) ‘plenty’ been has changed its tone from DL2 to DL1 probably due to
69
However, a change from A1 to A2 is more regular than from A1 to B2. The reason why
the tone of lwa:31 (B2<A1) ‘to seek’ is not from A1 to A2 is not clear. Another possiblitiy is
that the term lwa:31 (B2<A1) ‘to seek’ is actually not cognate with*xraA (cf. Li 1977: 233)
or*kraA (cf. Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 353) reflected in other Tai varieties.
353
the loss of its labialization, which is commonly seen on lower register tones.
Therefore, it becomes homophonous with ma:k34 (DL1-C) ‘fruits’. In brief,
labialization before -a: and -a in this dialect is one of the signatures of the earlier
voiced and glottal registers.
2) Pre-glottalized stops ˀb- and ˀd- developed from proto-glottal sounds in other Tai
varieties have been changed to breathy stops in this dialect, and the voicing of
these stops are not phonemic since sometimes they are p̤- and t ̤- but sometimes
they are b̤ - and d̤-, cf. the sound files of t ̤wa:343 (B1-G) vs. d̤wa:343 (B1-G) ‘to scold’.
Here the forms b̤ - and d̤- are treated as the phonemes because they have
developed from earlier glottalized voiced stops. The breathy voice is their
phonemic feature to make them contrast with the plain voiceless stops p- and t- in
the 1-U row. For exmaple, d̤wam53 (A1-G) ‘black’, b̤wa:343 (B1-G) ‘shoulder’,
b̤ wa:n445 (C1-G) ‘village’, b̤ wa:t334 (DL1-G) ‘time’ and d̤wak44 (DS1-G) ‘deep night’.
Moreover, earlier voiced stops*b- and*d- have been merged into b̤ - and d̤- which
developed from earlier *ʔb- and *ʔd- to complementarily distribute on the higher
register and the lower register respectively. For example, d̤wa:331 (A2) ‘to spread
on’, d̤wa:31 (B2) ‘river’, b̤ wa:k31 (DL2) ‘insane’, and b̤wak33 (DS2) ‘very tired’.
However, unlike that b̤- and d̤- on the higher register sometimes present as p̤- and
t ̤-, b̤ - and d̤- on the lower register always present as voiced sounds. In brief,
breathy voice (including voiced aspiration) in this dialect is another signature of
the earlier voiced and glottal registers.
3) The tone of paj331 (A2) ‘to go’ is expected to be A1, but it is actually A2 in this
dialect. Similarly, in LRP1’s dialect the tone of mɛ:343 (B1) ‘mother’ is expected to
be B2, but it is actually B1 in this dialect. These tone changes need more data to
determine with.
4) The tone of kʱɰej331 (A2) ‘to ride on’ is expected to be B2 (<B1/2) due to the
reflection in the vast majority of Tai varieties, but it is actually A2 in this variety.
The reason may be that this etymon is actually a Middle Chinese loan (vs. tɕʰi35
(A2) Mandarin, kʰei21 (A2) Cantonese), and the tonal category is also adopted from
its Sinitic source.
5) Unlike other CT/SWT varieties, the terms nok31 (DL2<DL1-C) ‘deaf’ and tʰɔk31
(DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’ which are expected to have a long vowel in DL1
designated have a short vowel like NT varieties. However, the tones of these two
terms do not merge into their counterpart DS1 tones (55) like other Yongnan
354
Zhuang varieties and all NT varieties, but merge into DL2 tone (31). In addition,
their tones are slightly different from the common DL2 because of the difference
between their vocalic lengths. Since items with expected DL2 tone normally have
a long vowel, their tones present a full mid-falling 31. But the DL2 tone on these
two items lower their pitch to be like 21 because their short vowel. The terms lit31
(DL2) ‘blood’, fet31 (DL2) ‘wing’ and met31 (DL2) ‘knife’ which are expected to have
a DL2 tone are similar to these two terms to have a lower pitch on their tone to be
like 21, because their vowels are also shorten. Nevertheless, this lower pitch is
treated as a conditioned variation of DL2 and is not an independent tone.
Therefore, it is transcribed as 31 as the common DL tone.
6) The original tonal value of DL1 tones is mid-rising 34 (=B1) like tʰa:p34 (DL1) ‘to
carry’ and ɬa:p34 (DL1) ‘cockroach’, but some of the items has a derived tone DL1’
whose tonal value is high level-rising 445 (=C1), like tʰa:k445 (DL1’-A) ‘to prop’
and ɬa:k445 (DL1’-C) ‘pestle’.
7) The initial and the tone of the term tʰap55 (DS1-A) ‘to receive’ are unexpected.
One may say that this term might not be cognate with *rapD ‘to receive’ reflected
in other Tai varieties. However, if we think over this problem with deep
consideration, we may see that this term may have been changed its tone from
DS2 to DS1 probably due to the loss of its labialization, which commonly precedes
-a: and -a on lower register tones. And then the initial may have been changed
from *r- to the same POA tʰ- because there are no modern initial consonant
developing from *r- on the higher register, and tʰ- is actually a common initial on
the higher register.
25.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect.
1) All aspirated initial stops and fricatives on the lower register like tʱ-, pʱ-, kʱ-, and
tɕʱ- have the voiced aspiration, which is phonetically breathy. For example, kʱwa:331
(A2) ‘to stuck’, kʱwa:331 (A2<A1/2) ‘right side’, tʱiaŋ331 (A2<A1/2) ‘pavilion’,
pʱun331 (A2<A1/2) ‘to sharpen’, pʱu:31 (B2) ‘male’, tʱu:31 (B2<B1/2) ‘bean’, tʱi:31
(B2<B1/2) ‘dense’, kʱɰej331 (A2) ‘to ride on’, and pʱu:35 (C2<C1/2) ‘CLF (of a
person)’.
2) The initial of ʔa:jA1-G ‘to turn face up’ is unexpected since it is designated into A1C.
355
3) The term kʰɔ:445 (C1-UC) ‘CLF (of stones)’ is expected to have a final consonant -n
(vs. kɔ:n41ˀ in Thai of SWT, kʰo:n24ˀ in Debao Yang Zhuang of CT, and ko:n213ˀ in
Longsang of NT), but it might have lost this final consonant.
4) As those in other NT varieties, the following terms put in the wordlist for the
revised tone box (Liao & Shen 2012) has different etyma, like lomA1-A ‘mortar’,
tawC1 ‘to come’, fe:t31 (DL2) ‘wing’, θe:pDL1 ‘fishhook’, and hatDS1-A ‘morning’.
5) The vowel of tʰɔ:kDL1-UC ‘cracking’ is unexpected. This sound change needs more
data to determine with.
6) The meaning of the term ʔa:pDL1-G ‘to apply (medical ointment, powder or etc.), to
spread on’ might be a result of semantic change from ‘to bath’.
7) The initial consonant of ɬak55 (DS1-UC) ‘grasshopper’ is unexpected. It reflects tin SWT and NT, and tʰ- in most CT varieties.
356
Language 26 Nanning Shuangding, Yongnan Zhuang, CT/NT?
26.1 Language information
Language
Yongnan Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyn
Autonym
tʰo:C1
Exonym
ja:ŋA1;Tuhua
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety
Shuangding (township name) Urban of YN is spoken at the urban area of Shuangding
Name
Township, Xixiangtang District, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region,
China. All Shuangding varieites including Urban have an exonym ja:ŋA1-G called by other
Zhuang groups surrounding them. This is interesting because this exonym is found in
Yang Zhuang and some NT varieties like Debao Longsang (L31), which are far away from
Shuangding YN. This phenomenon further suggests that the definition of Yang Zhuang in
ISO 639-3 has problems (cf. Language Information in L11 and L14, and L31).
26.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 5, 2013
Boluo Community (菠萝坡), Xingping Village (興平村), Shuangding
Place of data elicitation
Township (雙
鎮), Xixiangtang District (西鄉塘區), Nanning City,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Xiángjiā (祥嘉)
Family Name
Liáng (梁)
Gender
male
Age
50
Birth Place
Boluo Community (菠萝坡) or Jiujie Street (舊街, literally Old Street), Xingping
Village (興平村), Shuangding Township (雙
鎮), Xixiangtang District (西鄉塘區),
Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He lives in Boluo Community over all his life.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
farmer
Languages spoken
Level
Shuangding YN
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
Baihua
intermediate
Pinghua
a little
50 years
Education
junior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Longshan Village (龍山村, pla:A1 plukDL2), Shuangding Township (雙
鎮),
Xixiangtang District (西鄉塘區), Nanning City. Besides his Shuangding YN as native, he could
communicate in Baihua. LRP’s mother was born in Jiujie Street (舊街), Shuangding Township (雙
357
鎮),
Xixiangtang District (西鄉塘區), Nanning City. Besides her mother tongue Pinghua (of a Sinitic
language), she could speak Shuangding YN.
Remarks
Boluo Community (菠萝坡) of Xingping Village (興平村) is actually the old street of Shuangding
Township. It is contiguous with the new street of Shuangding Township. Local people treat the Zhuang
variety in Boluo Community and in the urban area of Shuangding Township the same urban variety of
Shuangding YN. In my investigation in Shuangding Township, three LRPs accepted my interview. I
found that the other two LRPs are from other two villages Licun of Shuangding Township and Zhongyi
of Nalong Township. Although they insist that they speak in the same language with Shuangding
Urban, the sound systems of their own varieties are different. For example, both the varieties from
Licun and Zhongyi has aspiration to correspondence to Shuangding Urban’s unaspirated stop + -l-
clusters, like pʰa:33 vs. pla:33 ‘stone mountains’ in Licun/Zhongyi and Shuangding Urban respectively.
Besides, PT *ɯa has been changed to a rounded long -y: in Zhongyi (like in Debao Urban and Dalong)
but has been merged into the unrounded -i: in Shuangding Urban and Licun, such as my:B1-G vs. mi:B1-G
‘boring’ in Zhongyi and in Shuangding Urban/Licun respectively. Therefore, I finally decide to reduce
the number of LRPs of Shuangding Urban from 3 to 1 because data from a single identical variety is
needed to analyze.
26.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
Aspirations
A1-A: 33
B1-A: 24
C1-A: 35ʔ
Continuants
A1-C: 33
B1-C: 24
C1-C: 35ʔ
Unaspirated stop
A1-U: 33
B1-U: 24
C1-U: 35ʔ
A1-UC: 33
B1-UC: 24
C1-UC: 35ʔ
A1-G: 33
B1-G: 24
C1-G: 35ʔ
A2: 31
B2: 22
C2: 44ʔ
A1/2: 31
B1/2: 22
C1/2: 44ʔ
DL
DS
DL1-A: 24
DL1’-A: 35
DS1-A: 35
DL1-C: 24
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
DL1’-C: 35
DS1-C: 35
DL1-U: 24
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
DL1’-U: 35
DL1-UC: 24
DS1-U: 35
DS1-UC: 35
DL1-G: 24
Glottal Sounds
DL1’-G: 35
DS1-G: 35
DL2: 22
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
358
DL2’: 44
DL1/2: 22
DS2: 44
DS1/2: 44
2) The tonal complexity on the DL column (in dark-orange and blue) does not
suggest the tonal flip-flop or the non-straightforward development. The derived
tone DL1’ on DL1-A and DLA-C has been found from the wordlist, like he:k35
(DL1’-A) ‘to prop’ and ɬa:k445 (DL1’-C) ‘pestle’. According to a supplementary
investigation to this dialect by confirming with LRP1, I verified the mid-rising 35
tone on DL[1'-A, 1'-C, 1'-U, 1'-G] (in dark-orange) as a derived tone of DL1, and a
high-level 44 tone on DL2' (in blue) as a derived tone of DL2. According to LRP1,
both the derived tones DL1’ (vs. DL1=24) and DL2’ (vs. DL2=22) are mainly
used for Middle Chinese loans, like tʰi:t35 (DL1’-A) ‘iron’ (<鐵 vs. tʰiɛ214
Mandarin, tʰit33 Cantonese),tʰa:p35 (DL1’-A) ‘pagoda’ (<塔 vs. tʰa:214 Mandarin,
tʰa:p33 Cantonese), si:t35 (DL1’-C) ‘snow’ (<雪 vs. ɕyɛ214 Mandarin, syt33
Cantonese), wa:t35 (DL1’-C) ‘to dig’ (<挖 vs. wa55 Mandarin, wa:t33 Cantonese),
ta:p35 (DL1’-U) ‘to answer’ (<答 vs. ta35 Mandarin, ta:p33 Cantonese), ʔa:p35 (DL1’-
G) ‘duck’ (<鴨 vs. ia55 Mandarin, ʔa:p33 Cantonese), ʔa:k35 (DL1’-G) ‘ferocity’ (<
惡 vs. ɤ51 Mandarin, ʔɔk33 Cantonese), la:p44 ‘dried meat’ (DL2’) (<臘 vs. la51
Mandarin, la:p22 Cantonese), ŋi:t44 ‘month’ (DL2’) (<月 vs. yɛ51 Mandarin, jyt22
Cantonese) and etc. These derived tones are of secondary aberrant development
and are out of the evaluation criteria of the conditioning of tonal splits.
Therefore, they cannot be a reference substance of tonal flip-flops or the
directionality of tonal development.
3) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: two-way split
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
[1'-A, 1'-C, 1'-U, 1'-UC, 1'-G]
[2']
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
DS
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development. The complicated
tonal split patterns are ignored here because they are aberrant development due to MC loans (cf.
L25).
359
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
26.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Low-rising 24 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 22 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and in DL[2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-rising 35 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] as well as in DL[1'-A,
1'-C, 1'-U, 1'-UC, 1'-G] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
High-level 44 occurs in C[2, 1/2] as well as in DL [2'] and DS[2, 1/2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
26.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect.
Tones C1 and C2 have a final glottal constriction, and the glottalization on C2 are
clearer like to nearly assimilate the previous nasals to become stops, like ha:35ˀ ‘five’,
na:j35ˀ ‘face’, pla:j35ˀ ‘to walk’, ʔɔ:j35ˀ ‘sugarcane’, luj44ˀ ‘bad’, and namʔ44 ‘water’. Tones
B1 and B2 have slight breathy finals. However, all these voice qualities are not
phonemic.
26.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect.
1) The tone of the term lu:ŋ24ʔ (B1) ‘cattle pen (<ground floor)’ is expected to be B2,
but it is actually B1 in this dialect. The reason needs more data to determine with.
2) Unlike other CT/SWT varieties, in this dialect the terms nok35 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tʰok35
(DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’, nət35 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, and tək44 (DS1/2) ‘correct’
360
which are expected to be long vowels in DLs designated have short vowels and
have been merged their tones into their counterpart DS boxes like NT varieties.
3) The tone of the term ta:k24 (DL1) ‘dry blood sucker’ is expected to be DL2 (22),
but in this dialect it is an unexpected DL1. The reason is still unknown.
26.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect.
1) All pre-glottal sounds in PT have been changed to sonorants in this dialect, the
rules are *ʔb- > m-, *ʔd- > n-, and *ʔj- > j-.
2) The initial consonant of the term kʰa:ŋ35 (C1) ‘fishbone’ is expected to be an
unaspirated stop k-, but in this dialect it is an aspirated stop kʰ-.
3) The initial consonant of the term law35 (C1) ‘head’ is expected to be an
unaspirated stop k-, but in this dialect it is a lateral l-.
361
Language 27 Long’an Xiaolin, Yongnan Zhuang, CT/NT?
27.1 Language information
Language
Yongnan Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyn
Autonym
tʰo:C1-A
Exonym
Long’an Zhuang
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999:42-44, 127-130, 595-808.
Data Info.
Long’an Xiaolin of YN. This variety is spoken in Xiaolin Township, Long’an
County, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
27.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 34
B1-A: 213
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 213
DS1-A: 21
Proto-
Continuants
A1-C: 34
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 55
Voiceless
Unaspirated
Sounds
stops
A1-U: 34
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 55
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC:34
B1-UC: 213
C1-UC: 55
DL1-UC: 213
DS1-UC: 21
A1-G: 34
B1-G: 213
C1-G: 55
DL1-G: 213
DS1-G: 21
A2: 22
B2: 21
C2: 33
DL2: 33
DS2: 21
A1/2: 22
B1/2: 21
C1/2: 33
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 21
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: three-way splits and two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-C, 1-U]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G]
Register 2
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
362
Horizontally: two individuals and five mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-C, 1-U] = DL[1-C, 1-U]
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B2[2, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-C, 1-U]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
27.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid-rising 34 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Low level 22 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[1-C, 1-U] and DL[1-C, 1-U] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-falling 21 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DS[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DS[1-C, 1-U] to form
a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 7:
Low level 33 occurs in C[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
27.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect.
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
363
Language 28 Daqiu, Nung An, Yongnan Zhuang, CT/NT?
28.1 Language information
Language
Yongnan Zhuang
A2
ʔa:n
A1-G
ISO 639-3 Code
zyn
Exonym
Nung An
Autonym
nɔŋ
Data Source
1 LRP
Reference
Edmondson (2002); Zhang et al. (1999: 42-44, 127-130, 595-808)
Variety Name
Daqiu (Chinese) or te:B2 kʰjouB1-A (local Zhuang) (village name) of Nung An,
which dispersed distributes at the rural areas of Jingxi County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
28.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
May 16, 2013
Qiulu Village (球路村), Xinjing Town, Jingxi County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Q̀ngyù (慶育)
Family Name
Lù (陸)
Gender
female
Age
62
Birth Place
Daqiu (te:C1 kʰjouB1-A in local Zhuang) Village (大秋屯), Longman Community (龙满
村), Longlin Township (龙临镇), Jingxi County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Region, China.
Places lived
Daqiu Village of Longlin Township, Jingxi County (22 years).
Da’en (大恩, or teC1 ŋjanC1 in local Zhuang) of the same township (6 years).
Xinjing Town, the main town of Jingxi County (33 years).
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
housewife
33 years
Education
junior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Nung An
mother tongue
Daqiu Variety
Jingxi Yang
fluent
This language is the wider communication
language of Jingxi County.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Nung An as native, he can communicate in Jingxi
Yang, which is the WCL of Jingxi County.
LRP’s mother was born in Daman (大满 or te:C1 mo:nC2 in local Yang Zhuang) village, Longlin Township,
Jingx County. She could only speak her mother tongue Jingxi Shangjia Yang.
Remarks
According to Edmondson (2002), Nung An may be immigrant language from Long’an County of
Guangxi.
364
28.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 33
B1-A: 11̰2
C1-A: 24ʔ
DL1-A: 11
DS1-A: 33
Proto-
Continuants
A1-C: 554
B1-C: 11̰2
C1-C: 24ʔ
DL1-C: 11
DS1-C: 55
Voiceless
Unaspirated
Sounds
stops
A1-U: 554
B1-U: 11̰2
C1-U: 24ʔ
DL1-U: 11
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC:33
B1-UC: 11̰2
C1-UC: 24ʔ
DL1-UC: 11
DS1-UC: 33
A1-G: 33
B1-G: 11̰2
C1-G: 24ʔ
DL1-G: 11
DS1-G: 33
A2: 33
B2: 31
C2: 42ʔ
DL2: 31
DS2: 33
A1/2: 33
B1/2: 31
C1/2: 42ʔ
DL1/2: 31
DS1/2: 33
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal split
patterns are found in Columns A and DS.
Horizontally: one individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2]
A[1-C, 1-U] = DS[1-C, 1-U]
Merger
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
365
28.5 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High level-falling 554 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U] and DS [1-C, 1-U] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and DS[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2,
1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 3:
Low level-rising 112 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and low level 11
occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 6:
High-falling 42 occurs in C[2, 1/2].
28.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones B1 have creakiness triggered by its extreme low pitch, like ʰɹaj11̰2 ‘egg’ and
ˀba:11̰2 ‘shoulder’. Tones C1 and C2 have glottal constrictions, like ha:24ˀ ‘five’, pʰja:j24ˀ
‘cotton’, ʔɔ:j24ˀ ‘sugarcane’, maj42ˀ ‘tree’ and nam42ˀ ‘water’. All the glottal
constrictions are not phonemic.
28.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Initial consonants in the supposed 1-UC row which is to follow Li’s unaspirated
stop + *r- clusters or ASC (Aspiration induced by Sesquisyllabic Clusters) row
which is to be suggested as a series of sesquisyllable cluster following the
hypothesis provided by Pittayawat Pittayaporn (2009) normally regularly present
as aspirated sounds or a sound with spread feature (h-), like in tʰa:33 (A1-UC) ‘eye’,
tʰa: j33 (A1-UC) ‘die’, and tʰe: ŋ33 (A1-UC) ‘cucumber’ like other CT and Yongnan
Zhuang varieties. However, the item taɰ554 (A1-U<A1-UC) and tɕa:ŋ554 (A1U<A1-UC) ‘to put up’ which are expected to be in this row due to the cognate in
other CT varieties (like tʰɔjA1-UC and kʰa:ŋA1-UC in Yang Zhuang varieties as well as
kʰa:ŋA1-UC in Tiandeng) has unexpected unaspirated initial stops t- and tɕ- as well as
a tone corresponding to be in the A1-U row. The term ‘gizzard’ with an
unaspirated initial stop t- is also found in other Yongnan Zhuang Tai varieties and
Daxin Baoxu (CT, Language 23) which have an additional tonal split due to the
366
proposed ASC row. The irregularity involved in this term needs further
investigation.
2) The tone of lɯŋ112 (B1<B2) ‘grand floor’ is expected to be B2 due to the reflection
in the vast majority of Tai varieties, but it is unexpected to be B1 in this variety.
3) Unlike other CT/SWT varieties, the terms nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tʰʊk33 (DS1-UC)
‘thin bamboo stripes’ and tik33 (DS1/2) ‘to hit the mark’ which are expected to
have a long vowel in DL1 designated have a short vowel and are merged into their
counterpart DS1 rows like NT varieties.
4) Just like other CT varieties, the term mit33 (DS2) ‘knife’ which is designated into
DL2 due to its vocalic length in SWT has a short vowel. Its tone is also changed to
DS2 (mid level).
28.6 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect.
1) The vowel of the term mowA2 ‘hand’ is unexpected.
2) The final consonant of kʰɔ:ŋ24 (C1-UC) ‘CLF of a stone’ is expected to be -n, but it
is actually -ŋ in this dialect.
3) The final stops on checked syllables in Tai languages normally are not released,
but the LRP often tends to release some of the final stops, like in the terms ma:k11
‘fruits’, pa:k11 ‘mouth’, and kɔt11 ‘to hug’. However, the released stops are not
phonemic since she also repeated some of those items with non-released final
stops.
4) As those in other NT varieties, the following terms put in the wordlist for the
revised tone box (Liao & Shen 2012) has different etymological sources, like
ɹ̥umA1-A ‘mortar’, tawC1 ‘to come’, la:C1 ‘below’, fɯ:tDL2 ‘wing’, θe:pDL1 ‘fishhook’, and
hatDS1-A ‘morning’.
367
Language 29 Xialeng, Yongnan Zhuang, CT/NT?
29.1 Language information
Language
Yongnan Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
Autonym
tʰo:
Exonym
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 42-44, 121-127, 595-808
Data Info.
Xialeng (township name) of YN. This variety is spoken at Xialeng Township,
C1-A
zyn
Yongning District, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
29.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Aspirations
Proto-
Continuants
Voiceless
Unaspirated
Sounds
stops
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
A1-A: 33
B1-A: 53
C1-A: 13
A1-C: 55
B1-C: 13
C1-C: 35
A1-U: 55
B1-U: 13
C1-U: 35
A1-UC:33
B1-UC: 53
C1-UC: 13
A1-G: 33
B1-G: 53
C1-G: 13
A2: 31
B2: 53
C2: 11ʔ
A1/2: 31
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 11ʔ
DL
DL1-A: 53
DL1’-A: 35
DL1-C: 13
DL1-U: 13
DL1’-U: 35
DL1-UC: 53
DL1-G: 53
DL1’-G: 35
DL2: 53
DL2’: 11
DL1/2: 53
DS
DS1-A: 55
DS1-C: 55
DS1-U: 55
DS1-UC: 55
DS1-G: 55
DS2: 33
DS1/2: 33
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: three-way splits and two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 1'
Register 2
A
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-C, 1-U]
C
[1-C, 1-U]
DL
DS
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
[1-C, 1-U]
[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
[1'-A, 1'-U, 1'-G]
[2']
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal
split patterns are found in all columns except Column DS.
368
Horizontally: three individuals and four mergers
A[2, 1/2]
Individual
C[1-C, 1-U]
C[2, 1/2]
A[1-C, 1-U] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[2, 1/2]
B[1-C, 1-U] = C[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] =DL[1-C, 1-U]
B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
29.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: seven distinct tones.
Just like in L25, L26, and L27, The complexity of tonal splits on dead Column DL are
of secondary aberrant development due to MC loans and are out of the evaluation
criteria of the conditioning of tonal splits. They are omitted in this study because
they are beyond the tonal conditioning factors like initials or vowel-length.
Tone 1:
High level 55 occurs in A[1-C, 1-U] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to form
a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G] and DS[2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 4:
Low-rising 13 occurs in B[1-C, 1-U] and C[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G], as well as in DL
[1-C, 1-U] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 53 occurs in B[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2] and in DL[1-A, 1-UC, 1-G,
2, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in C[1-C, 1-U] and in DL[1'-A, 1'-U, 1'-G] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 7:
Low-level 11 occurs in C[2, 1/2] and in DL[2'] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively..
29.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
According to the original materials, Tones C2 (including C1/2 which is merged into
C2) have clear glottal constrictions, like maj11ʔ ‘tree’, nam11? ‘water’, and na:m11ʔ
‘abdomen’ (Zhang et al. 1999: 122). This glottal constriction is not phonemic.
369
29.5 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect.
1) Like in NT varieties, the terms nuk55 (DS1) ‘deaf’ which is expected to have a long
vowel in DL1 designated have short vowels.
2) There are many “derived tones” in this language. In the Sinitic circles, linguists
use the specific punctuation ʼ to mark these “derived tones”, like na1 (A1-C) ‘thick’
vs. na1’ (A1-G) ‘swaddling clothes’ in Xialeng. In this thesis, we also adapt this
convention to mark “derived tones”, like naA1 (A1-C) ‘thick’ vs. naA1’ (A1-G)
‘swaddling clothes’.
29.6 Notes on dead syllables
According to the original material (Zhang et al. 1999: 122), tones on the DS
column are regularly splits as “Tone 7” (DS1, high-level 55) and “Tone 8” (DS2 and
DS1/2, mid-level 33) on earlier voiceless-voiced initials sounds as shown in the
following Series 1. However, tonal splits in DL column are especially complex and
present as follows: The tonal values of “Tone 9” (DL1) and “Tone 10” (DL2 and
DL1/2) are low-rising 13 and high-falling 53 respectively, as shown in the
following Series 2; however, aspirated initial stop (including aspirated initial
clusters) and those initial consonants developing from proto-glottal sounds like ʔ-,
m- (<*ʔb-), n- (<*ʔd-), j- (<*ʔj-), and w- (<*ʔw-) condition the Tone 9 to change
to Tone 10, and it means that these two groups of initials condition tones DL1-A
(as well as DL1-UC which also has aspirated initials) and DL1-G to merge into DL2
in our words in this thesis. The remained tones of DL1-C and DL1-U preserve the
original Tone 9 (that is, original DL1 tone), as shown in the following Series 3.
Nevertheless, this situation only indicates the regular rules of tonal splits and
merger on the DL column. Some words in DL1-A, DL1-UC and DL2 unexpectedly
have “Tone 9” (low-rising 13), which is the same tone as in DLC and DLU, as
shown in the following Series 4. In addition, there are two other derived tones on
the DL column, and they are Tone 9’ (DL1’, a mid-rising tone 35) and 10’ (DL2’, a
low-level tone 11) respectively. These two tones normally occur on terms with
Sinitic etymologic source, and the derived tone DL2’ is normally used for the cìzhuó
(次浊) or ‘secondarily muddy’ initials (which refers to voiced sonorants in the
terminology of the traditional Chinese phonology) of Middle-Chinese loans, as
shown in the following Series 5.
370
Series 1: Original tones on the DS column (in the order of DL1-A, DL1-C, DL1-U,
DL1-UC, DL1-G, DL2, and DL1/2)
hat55 (DS1-A) ‘morning’
tʰat55 (DS1-A) ‘to run’
tʰot55 (DS1-A??) ‘fart’
hlət55 (DS1-A) ‘scabies’
me:t55 (DS1-C) ‘end’
nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’ (vocalic length agreeing with NT)
lap55 (DS1-C) ‘dark’
lap55 (DS1-C) ‘tired’
tap55 (DS1-U) ‘liver’
tok55 (DS1-U) ‘to fall’
kok55 (DS1-U) ‘root’
tak55 (DS1-U) ‘to scoop’
tət55 (DS1-U) ‘to fillip (by fingers)’
kut55 (DS1-U) ‘fern’
klik55 (DS1-U) ‘lazy’
kuk55 (DS1-U) ‘tiger’
tip55 (DS1-U) ‘petal’
tsəp55 (DS1-U) ‘to kiss’
tʰuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘bamboo skin’ (vocalic length agreeing with NT but disagreeing
with CT/SWT)
nip55 (DS1-G) ‘raw’
ʔak55 (DS1-G) ‘chest’
ʔəp55 (DS1-G) ‘to close (with the door left unlocked or unlatched)’
ʔut55 (DS1-G) ‘to bend (a bamboo)’
ʔot55 (DS1-G) ‘to plug’
(ɬək55)ʔek55 (DS1-G) ‘to hiccup’
ʔuk55 (DS1-G) ‘brains’
nət55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’ (vocalic length agreeing with NT but disagreeing with
CT/SWT)
mok55 (DS1-G) ‘dry up’ (vs. Debao ˀbɔ:k55 (DS1-G) ‘dry up’ and Thai bok21 (DS1-G)
‘land’)
ʔik55 (DS1-G) ‘benefit’ (<益 vs. i53 Mandarin, jɪk55 Cantonese)
371
lik33 (DS2) ‘child’ (vocalic length agreeing with NT but disagreeing with CT/SWT)
jip33 (DS2) ‘to sew’
kap33 (DS2) ‘to catch’
kət33 (DS2) ‘indignant’
mat33 (DS2) ‘CLF (of a grain)’
mot33 (DS2) ‘ant’
nok33 (DS2) ‘bird’
ləp33 (DS2) ‘to stroke’
hot33 (DS2) ‘to stir’
kut33 (DS2) ‘thick (liquid)’
təp33 (DS2) ‘to smash’
hap33 (DS1/2) ‘to bite’
nak33 (DS1/2) ‘to press on’
tsʰip33 (DS1/2) ‘ten’
tik33 (DS1/2) ‘to be’ (vocalic length agreeing with NT but disagreeing with
CT/SWT)
Series 2: Original tones on the DL column (in the order of DL1-C, DL1-U, DL2, and
DL1/2)
ɬe:t13 (DL1-C) ‘to finish’
mo:k13 (DL1-C) ‘fog’
ɬe:p13 (DL1-C) ‘fishhook’
pe:t13 (DL1-U) ‘eight’
pa:t13 (DL1-U) ‘basin’
kwe:t13 (DL1-U) ‘to scrape’
ko:p13 (DL1-U) ‘hold in both hands’
kle:p13 (DL1-U) ‘husk (of rice)’
ko:t13 (DL1-U) ‘to hug’
ko:k13 (DL1-U) ‘corner’
te:p53 (DL2) ‘husk (of rice)’
ta:k53 (DL2) ‘to measure’
to:k53 (DL2) ‘to read’ (<讀 vs. tu35 Mandarin, tʊk35 Cantonese)
ma:k53 (DL2) ‘CLF (of a knife)’
mo:t53 (DL2) ‘rice weevil’
372
hlo:t53 (DL2) ‘outside’
na:k53 (DL2) ‘otter’
to:t53 (DL2) ‘to protrude’
ku:k53 (DL2) ‘to do’
pu:t53 (DL2) ‘to dial’ (<撥 vs. po55 Mandarin, put22 Cantonese)
ho:p53 (DL2) ‘wound healing’
mo:p53 (DL2) ‘mouth-muffle (of an ox)’
nu:k53 (DL2) ‘infect’
(ɬow11) ɬə:k53 (DL2) ‘whacked’
te:p53 (DL2) ‘dish’ (<碟 vs. tiɛ35 Mandarin, tip22 Cantonese)
pu:k53 (DL1/2) ‘white’ (tonal behavior agreeing with NT but disagreeing with
CT/SWT)
Series 3: expected tone changes (DL1 > DL2 conditioned by aspiration and glottal
sounds)
tsʰa:t53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘brush’ (<刷 vs. ʂua55 Mandarin, tsʰa:t35 Cantonese)
pʰla:k53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘forehead’
pʰlu:k53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘taro’
tʰu:k53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘oil dipper’
hle:k53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘wok’
ha:p53 (DL2<DL1-A) ‘box’ (<盒 vs. xɤ35 Mandarin, hap11 Cantonese)
tʰe:k53 (DL2<DL1-UC) ‘cracking’
ʔi:p53 (DL2<DL1-G) ‘to salt’
ʔi:t53 (DL2<DL1-G) ‘to stretch’
ʔe:k53 (DL2<DL1-G) ‘yoke (of an ox)’
na:t53 (DL2<DL1-G) ‘hot’
ma:t53 (DL2<DL1-G) ‘times’
Series 4: unexpected tonal preservation on DL1-A and DL1-G
he:k13 (DL1-C/U<DL1-A) ‘guest’ (<客 vs. kʰɤ51 Mandarin, ha:k33 Cantonese)
tʰa:p13 (=DL1-C/U<DL1-A) ‘pagoda’ (<塔 vs. tʰa214 Mandarin, tʰa:p33 Cantonese)
kʰwi:t13 (=DL1-C/U<DL1-A) ‘lack’ (<缺 vs. tɕʰuɛ55 Mandarin, kʰwit33 Cantonese)
tʰa:t13 (=DL1-C/U<DL1-A) ‘to excoriate (skin)’
ʔe:p13 (=DL1-C/U<DL1-G) ‘to force’
373
ʔa:k13 (=DL1-C/U<DL1-G) ‘fierce’ (<惡 vs. ɤ51 Mandarin, ʔɔk33 Cantonese)
Series 5: unexpected derived tones on the DL column
hlo:p35 (DL1’< DL1-A) ‘quaff (drinks)’
tsʰo:t35 (DL1’< DL1-A) ‘end’
ki:p35 (DL1’< DL1-U) ‘tough’
ku:t35 (DL1’< DL1-U) ‘uncover’ (<揭 vs. tɕiɛ55 Mandarin, kʰit33 Cantonese)
tsi:t35 (DL1’< DL1-U) ‘festival’ (<節 vs. tɕiɛ35 Mandarin, tsi:t33 Cantonese)
ka:p35 (DL1’< DL1-U) ‘to unite’ (<合 vs. hɤ35 Mandarin, hap33 Cantonese)
ʔu:t35 (DL1’< DL1-G) ‘fool’
li:p11 (DL2’< DL2) ‘to hunt’ (<獵 vs. liɛ51 Mandarin, li:p11 Cantonese)
la:p11 (DL2’< DL2) ‘dried meat’ (<腊 vs. la51 Mandarin, la:p11 Cantonese)
374
Language 30 Shuangqiao, Wuming, Yongbei Zhuang, NT
30.1 Language information
Language
Yongbei Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyb
Autonym
to:C1-U
Exonym
ˀba:nC1-G
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 35-36, 50-55, 595-808.
Data Info.
According to the original material, This language is commonly called to:C1-U ‘local,
native’, and is spoken in Shuangqiao Township, Wuming County, Nanning City,
Guangxi, China.
30.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops except tones C in this
dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 24
B1-A: 35
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 24
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 55
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 24
B1-UC: 35
C1-UC: 55
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 24
B1-G: 35
C1-G: 55
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 55
A2: 31
B2: 33
C2: 42
DL2: 42
DS2: 33
A1/2: 31
B1/2: 33
C1/2: 42
DL1/2: 42
DS1/2: 33
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
375
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
30.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC,
1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and in DS[2, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 6:
High-falling 42 occurs in C[2, 1/2] and in DL[2, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
30.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
376
Language 31 Longsang, NT
31.1 Language information
Language
Longsang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
undescribed
Autonym
ja:ŋ
Exonym
ˀju:jC1-G
Data Source
3 LRP
Variety Name
Debao Longsang. This is a undescribed NT language spoken at the urban and rural
A1-G
C1
; to:
areas of Longsang Township, Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
This language’s autonym and exonym are both ja:ŋA1-G, which is contradictory to the
definition of Yang Zhuang (L8-13) (cf. Language Information of L11, L14, and L 26).
31.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
Place of data elicitation
LRP1
Date
May 6, 2013
Xinlong Road 96, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Xìcǎo (秀草)
Gender
female
33
Family Name
Lù (陸)
Age
72
31
Birth
Sanhe Community (
合屯, fa:k kaw in local Zhuang), Qiaotou Village (橋頭村),
Place
Longsang Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places
She lived in her own village until she was 37. After that she came to follow her husband to
lived
live in the main town of Debao County until now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
retired worker
Languages spoken
Level
Debao Longsang Zhuang
mother tongue
Debao Urban Yang
fluent
Zhuang
34 years
Education
primary school
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca of Debao County.
However, as a native Yang Zhuang speaker, I could
cognize that her Yang Zhuang had clear NT accent. For
example, aspirated sounds in Yang Zhuang were always
pronounced as unaspirated counterpart sounds when
she spoke Yang Zhuang to me.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Longsang Zhuang as native, he can communicate in
Debao County Yang Zhuang. LRP’s mother was born in Qiaonan Community (橋南屯 tɕawC1 kɔ:ŋC1 in
377
local Zhuang) village, Qiaotou Village (橋頭村), Longsang Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County. She was
monolingual in Longsang Zhuang.
Remarks
The LRP1 is the LRP2’s mother. Their pronunciations are obviously different in some tones (like tones
A1 are 24 by LRP1 but 242 by LRP2), probably because the son has grown up in the main town of
Debao County, where is far away from his mother’s village. The basic tonal descriptions are based on
LRP1’s pronunciation because it is treated as the original pattern due to the fact that LRP1 is LRP2’s
mother, and she grew up in the central area of this language. Data from the LRP3 also support this
speculation.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Date
May 15, 2013
Xinlong Road 6, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang
Place of data elicitation
Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Wèiquán (衛權)
Family Name
Lù (陸)
Gender
male
Age
40
Birth Place
Sanhe Community (
合屯, fa:k33 kaw31 in local Zhuang), Qiaotou Village (橋頭村),
Longsang Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
He lived in her own village until he was 6. After that he came to follow his parents to live
in the main town of Debao County until now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
deliveryman
34 years
Education
junior high school
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Debao Longsang Zhuang
mother tongue
Although he left his village to live in the main town of
Debao from 6 years old, he always speaks his mother
tongue with his family until now.
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
native
This language is the lingua franca of Debao County. He
speaks Yang Zhuang almost as fluently as native speakers
because he started to speak this language from his
childhood.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in Daji Village (大吉村), Longsang Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County. Besides his
mother tongues Longsang Zhuang, he also speaks Debao Urban Yang Zhuang, Standard Mandarin, and
Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin.
LRP’s mother was born in the same village as the LRP’s. She also speaks Debao Urban Yang Zhuang,
Standard Mandarin, and Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin. She is now living with the LRP.
378
Serial number of LRP
LRP3
Date
May 15, 2013
Xinlong Road 96, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Guangxi
Place of data elicitation
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Jí (傑)
Family Name
Lù (陸)
Gender
male
Age
60
Birth Place
Longtan Community (龍壇屯, luŋ31 ta:n55 in local Zhuang), Longtan Village (龍壇
村), Longsang Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Region, China.
Places lived
Longtan Village (16 years). Tianlin County, Baise Prefecture (6 years). Baise City
(2 years). He is now living in the Debao main town.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
retired cadre
35 years
Education
junior college
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Debao Longsang Zhuang
mother tongue
Although he left his village to live in the main town of
Debao from 6 years old, he always speaks his mother
tongue with his family until now.
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of Debao County. He
speaks Yang Zhuang with NT accent.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu Dialect
fluent
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
Baihua
intermediate
This Cantonese variety is the lingua franca of the urban
area of Baise City.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides his mother tongues Longsang Zhuang, he also speaks
Debao Urban Yang Zhuang, Standard Mandarin, Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, and Baihua.
LRP’s mother was born in Longyuan Community (龍苑屯), Longtan Village (龍壇村), Longsang
Township (隆桑鎮), Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China. Besides her mother
tongues Longsang Zhuang, she also speaks Debao Urban Yang Zhuang. The LRP’s parents are now
living with the LRP.
379
31.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, tonal flip-flop runs through all tones except the
column of DS, which preserves a higher pitch on tones DS1 and a lower pitch on
tones DS2 and DS12.
Smooth Syllable
Phonation types
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 35
B1-A: 31
C1-A: 213ʔ
DL1-A: 21
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 35
B1-C: 31
C1-C: 213ʔ
DL1-C: 21
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 35
B1-U: 31
C1-U: 213ʔ
DL1-U: 21
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 35
B1-UC: 31
C1-UC: 213ʔ
DL1-UC:
DS1-UC:
21
55
A1-G: 35
B1-G: 31
C1-G: 213ʔ
DL1-G: 21
DS1-G: 55
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 55
B2: 53
C2: 33ʔ
DL2: 33
DS2: 33
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2: 55
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 33ʔ
DL1/2: 33
DS1/2: 33
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development with voiceless-voiced
register splits.
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Individual
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2]
A[2, 1/2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[ 2, 1/2] = DS[ 2, 1/2]
380
31.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone
A1
A2
B1
B2
C1
C2
DL1
DL2
DS1
DS2
LRP1, 3
24
55
31
53
213ʔ
33ʔ
21
33
55
33
LRP2
242/24
45/55
31
53
213ʔ
33ʔ
22
33
55
33
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 (LRP1, LRP3) or Low-rising-falling 242 (LRP2) occurs in A[1-A, 1-C,
1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]; some items with this tone from LRP2’s recording also present as
low-rising [24], like ta:35 ‘eye’, ta:j35 ‘to die’, and taɰ35 ‘gizzard’ but they are
treated as the free variation of [242], due to the fact that the LRP2 insists that
‘eye’ (recording ta:24) and ‘maternal grandmother’ (recording ta:242) are
homophonous.
Tone 2:
High-level 55 (LRP1, LRP3) or high rising 45 (LRP2) occurs in A[2, 1/2], and high
level 55 occurs in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively; some items with this
tone on A[2, 1/2] from LRP2’s recording also present as high level 55 which is the
free variation of 45, such as na:55 ‘rice field’, ham55 ‘bitter’, and kwa:55 ‘right side’.
Tone 3:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 4:
High-falling 53 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2].
Tone 5:
Low-falling-rising 213 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], and low-falling 21
(LRP1) or low level 22 (LRP2) in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[2, 1/2], DL[2, 1/2], and DS[2, 1/2].
31.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
Tones C1 and C2 have clear glottal constrictions, like fɯ:213ˀ ‘cloud’, tɕa:j213ˀ ‘to
walk’, ʔa: 213ˀ ‘to open’, ma:33ˀ ‘horse’, nam33ˀ ‘water’, and maj33ˀ ‘tree’. All these voice
qualities are not phonemic.
31.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial of the term ʔa:24 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
2) Different from the merger into DS2 in other NT varieties, ɬap55 ‘to mince’ merges
its tone with the rows of DS1. This may be of an areal trait shared with the
neighboring the Debao Yang Zhuang varieties, which are of CT.
381
3) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, tuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’, ˀdit55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’,
mit33 (DS2) ‘knife’, and tək33 (DS12-A) ‘to hit the mark’. However, the term ŋɯ:k21
(DL1-C) ‘gums’ agreeing with that reflecting a long vowel in the DL1-C column in
all CT/SWT varieties.
4) Not only the tones DL2 and DS2 are totally merged, but all the long vowels in the
DL2 column are also shortened to merge into short vowels, like that nɔk33 (DL2)
‘outside’ and nɔk33 (DS2) ‘bird’ are homophonous, and the vowels of the following
terms ɕakDL2 ‘rope’, pakDL2 ‘insane’, lakDL2 ‘root’ and makDL2 ‘plenty’ are all shortened.
31.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) As those in other NT varieties, the following terms put in the wordlist for the
revised tone box (Liao & Shen 2012) has different etymological sources, like tawC1
‘to come’, te:B1 ‘to wait’, la:C1 ‘below’, fɯ:tDL2 ‘wing’, θe:pDL1 ‘fishhook’, ɹʊmA1
‘mortar’, and to:nC1 ‘to receive’.
2)The term te:m24 ‘in addition’ has a compound form ʔej213 te:m24, the optional form
ˀjem24 for this item should be a liaison from this compound form.
3) The term ta:55 only refers to ‘to pump’ in this dialect, while in CT/SWT varieties,
the cognate developing from *da:A refers to both ‘to spread on’ and ‘to pump’.
4) The vowel of faɰ55 (A2) is different from that reflecting *-ɯŋ in other NT
varieties, but is the same to that reflecting *-ɯ: in the vast majorities of CT/SWT
varieties.
5) The initial of nam33ˀ ‘water’, nɔk33 ‘bird’, and nɔk33 ‘outside’ is n- which does not
agree with that reflecting *r- in the vast majorities of NT varieties, but agrees with
that reflecting *n- in all CT/SWT varieties. In a similar way, the initial of maj33ˀ
‘tree’ is m- which doesn’t agree with that reflecting *v- in the vast majorities of NT
varieties, but agrees with that reflecting *m- in all CT/SWT varieties.
6) The item tɯkDL1 only refers to ‘touch’ or ‘contact’, while in other Tai varieties, its
cognate has several other meanings besides ‘touch’, like ‘to be’, ‘right’ or ‘to hit the
mark’.
7) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of θipDS1-A ‘centipede’ which is
designated into DS1-A reflects *s- or *ɬ- which should be considered as DS1-C.
382
Language 32 Dongling, Youjiang Zhuang, NT
32.1 Language information
Language
Youjiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zyj
Autonym
to:C1-U; ja:ŋA1-G
Exonym
ˀju:jC1-G
Data Source
2 LRPs
Variety Name
Dongling (township name) of Youjiang Zhuang. This variety is spoken at the plain
paddy areas of Dongling Township, Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China. Dongling is an intermountain basin township. Dongling
Youjiang Zhuang is spoken in the central plain paddy part of this basin, while Yang
Zhuang varieties are spoken in the surrounding mountainous areas. The Yang Zhuang
population is much more than Dongling Youjiang Zhuang. Besides, Yang Zhuang is the
lingua franca of the whole Debao County. Therefore, many Dongling Youjiang Zhuang
speakers are bilingual in their mother tongue and Debao County Yang Zhuang.
The autonym ja:ŋA1-G provided by LRP2 again indicates the problem of the definition of
Yang Zhuang (cf. Language Information in L11, L14, L26, and L31).
32.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Date
May 17, 2013
Xinlong Road 115, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Ľi (磊)
Family Name
Húng (黄)
Gender
male
Age
31
Birth Place
Xiadong Community (
垌屯, tɔŋB2 la:C1 in local Zhuang), Dongling Village (東陵村),
Dongling Township (東陵鎮), Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Xiadong Community (18 years). Wuzhishan City in Hainan Province (2 years).
Shandong Province (0.5 years). He lives in the main town of Debao County now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
worker
5 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Dongling Youjiang Zhuang
mother tongue
Debao County Yang Zhuang
fluent
senior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca of Debao
County.
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
Baihua
intermediate
A Cantonese variety spoken in Guangxi,
China.
383
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Dongling Youjiang Zhuang as native, he can
communicate in Debao County Yang Zhuang, Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, Standard Mandarin
(Putonghua hereafter), and Baihua.
LRP’s mother was born in the same village. Besides her mother tongue Dongling Zhuang, she can speak
Standard Mandarin.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Place of data elicitation
May 18, 2013
Xinlong Road 57, Chengguan Town, Debao County, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Chángcāo (常操)
Gender
male
Birth Place
Date
Suoda Community (索達屯, θo:k
DL1
Family Name
Lú (羅)
Age
57
B2
ta: in local Zhuang), Dongling Village (東陵村),
Dongling Township(東陵鎮), Debao County, Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang
Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Suoda Community (18 years). Baise City (3 years). Dongling Township (17 years).
Now he lives in the main town of Debao County.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
administrative leader
Languages spoken
Level
Dongling Youjiang Zhuang
mother tongue
Debao County Yang Zhuang
fluent
Standard Mandarin
fluent
19 years
Education
junior college
Language Remarks
This language is the lingua franca of Debao County.
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Guiliu
fluent
This language is a Southwestern Mandarin variety
spoken in the western part of Guangxi.
Baihua
intermediate
A Cantonese variety spoken in Guangxi, China.
Yao
intermediate
The LRP often went to support the poor in the
country side in some Yao communities in the
mountain areas in Dongling Township, Debao County
when he worked in Dongling Central Primary School.
This language is called jiwA2 in Debao County Yang
Zhuang. It is not clear that which Yao variety it is.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Dongling Youjiang Zhuang as native, he can
communicate in Debao County Yang Zhuang, Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin, Standard Mandarin
(Putonghua hereafter), Baihua, and Yao.
LRP’s mother was born in the Qinke Community (琴克屯), Dongling Village (東陵村), Dongling
Township(東陵鎮), Debao County. Besides her mother tongue Dongling Zhuang, she can speak Standard
Mandarin, Debao County Yang Zhuang, and Nung An.
384
32.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, tonal flip-flop runs through all the tones in this
dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 11̰
B1-A: 242
C1-A: 224ʔ
DL1-A: 24
DS1-A: 22
Continuants
A1-C: 11̰
B1-C: 242
C1-C: 224ʔ
DL1-C: 24
DS1-C: 22
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 11̰
B1-U: 242
C1-U: 224ʔ
DL1-U: 24
DS1-U: 22
A1-UC: 11̰
B1-UC: 242
C1-UC: 224ʔ
DL1-UC: 24
Glottal Sounds
A1-G: 11̰
B1-G: 242
C1-G: 44ʔ
DL1-G: 24
DS1-G: 44
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 553
B2: 53
C2: 44ʔ
DL2: 53
DS2: 44
A1/2: 553
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 44ʔ
DL1/2: 53
DS1/2: 44
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
[1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
[1-G, 2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The nonstraightforward columns are C and DS, with a spilt being voiceless register on one side and
golttal/voiced register on the other side.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
C[1-G, 2, 1/2] = DS[1-G, 2, 1/2]
385
DS1-UC:
22
32.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-level 11 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
High level-falling 553 (LRP1) occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Low-rising-falling 242 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], and low-rising 24
occurs in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-falling 53 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Low level-rising 224 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC], and mid level 22 in DS[1A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
High-level 44 occurs in C[1-G, 2, 1/2] and DS[1-G, 2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
32.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
A1 tone (11) has a creakiness triggered by its lowest pitch 1; Tones C1 and C2 have
glottal constrictions, like fiə224ˀ ‘cloud’, pja:j224ˀ ‘to walk’, ʔa: 224ˀ ‘to open’, ma:44ˀ
‘horse’, lam44ˀ ‘water’, and maj44ˀ ‘tree’. The glottal constriction on Tone C1 is clearer
than on Tone C2. All these voice qualities are not phonemic.
32.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial of the term ʔa:11 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
2) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
nok22 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, ŋək22 (DS1-C) ‘gums’, tok22 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’,
ˀdit22 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, mit44 (DS2) ‘knife’, and tək44 (DS12-A) ‘to hit the mark’.
3) LRP2 claimed that the term for ‘to pull down’ has two optional tones, tuəŋ53
(merging into B2) and tuəŋ242 (merging into B1). The former one agrees with NT
varieties. The later one agrees with CT/SWT varieties, and should be an areal trait
shared with its neighboring Debao Yang Zhuang varieties (of CT).
4) LRP2 provided ta:k24 ‘dry blood sucker’ which has DL1 tone agreeing with CT
varieties but different from DL2 tone in other NT varieties. However, when I asked
him to pronounce ‘to measure’ which should be the homophone of ‘dry blood
sucker’, he provided two optional tones ta:k24 (DL1) and ta:k53 (DL2). The later one
386
should be the original form since it agrees with all other NT varieties, while the
former one should be an areal trait shared with the neighboring Debao Yang
Zhuang varieties (of CT).
32.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) As those in other NT varieties, the following terms put in the wordlist for the
revised tone box (Liao & Shen 2012) has different etymological sources, like tawC1
‘to come’, te:B1 ‘to wait’, la:C1 ‘below’, fiətDL2 ‘wing’, and θe:tDL1 ‘fishhook’. However,
the term lom55 ‘mortar’ has a C2 tone, which does not agree with its cognate
reflecting *trumA or *krumA in other NT varieties.
2) The term ˀje:m53 ‘in addition’ provided by LRP1 has been changed not only the
initial but also the tone (from A1 to C2), and this indicates a typical sound change
triggered by grammaticalization. Its initial consonant change can be conferred to
its cognate ˀjem24 in Longsang (Language 27), which has a compound form ʔej213
te:m24 being simplified to ˀjem24.
3) The term for ‘to put up’ is ka:ŋ11(A1) provided by LRP1, but is ha:ŋ11̰(A1) provided
by LRP2.
4) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of θipDS1-A ‘centipede’ which is
designated into DS1-A reflects *s- or *ɬ- which should be considered as DS1-C.
5) The vowel of kuəB2 ‘pair’ provided by LRP1 is unexpected (it is expected to be
ku:B2 due to the sound correspondence, and LRP2 provides the expected form).
This aberrance change might be influenced by the compound word kuəB2 tɯəB12-A ‘a
pair of chopsticks’ provided by LPR1. However, the vowel of tɯ:B12-A ‘chopsticks’ is
a long ɯ: when it is pronounced independently.
6) In the vast majority of Youjiang Zhuang varieties, the initial pj- has been merged
into tɕ-. But in this variety, the process of this merger is not completed. For
example, LRP1 only provides pja:jC1 for ‘to walk’, but LRP2 provides two variations
pja:jC1 and tɕa:jC1. LRP2 prefers the later one, and he only read the form tɕa:jC1 for
‘to walk’ in the actual recording. In a similar way, LRP1 only provides pjakDS1for
‘vegetable’, and LRP2 provides pjakDS1 and tɕakDS1. For this term LRP2 read the
both forms for the actual recording.
7) The term ɬe:t DL1 ‘fishhook’ has a different syllable final -t from -p shared by other
NT varieties.
387
8) LRP2 unexpectedly provided lap55 for ‘to receive’, which is shared with CT/SWT
varieties, because all other NT varieties do not share this etymon.
9) LRP2 provided ɕakDS2 (agreeing with NT) and lakDS2 (agreeing with CT/SWT) for
‘to steal’. But he claimed the meanings of these two terms are slightly different.
388
Language 33 Lizhou, Tianlin, Guibian Zhuang, NT
33.1 Language information
Language
Guibian Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zgn
Autonym
ˀjajC1-G
Exonym
ˀjajC1-G; ˀjo:jC1-G
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 37-38, 65-67, 595-808.
Data Info.
This language is spoken in Lizhou Township (利周鎮), Tianlin County (田林縣),
Baise Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
33.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is tonal flip-flops in Columns A and C in this
dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 13
B1-A: 24
C1-A: 45
DL1-A: 24
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 13
B1-C: 24
C1-C: 45
DL1-C: 24
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 13
B1-U: 24
C1-U: 45
DL1-U: 24
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 13
B1-UC: 24
C1-UC: 45
DL1-UC: 24
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 13
B1-G: 24
C1-G: 33
DL1-G: 24
DS1-G: 55
A2: 52
B2: 31
C2: 33
DL2: 22
DS2: 22
A1/2: 52
B1/2: 31
C1/2: 33
DL1/2: 22
DS1/2: 22
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-Voiced Aspirations &
Breathy Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
[1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal
split conditioned by glottalized sounds is found in Column C.
389
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Individual
A[2, 1/2]
B[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
33.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones.
Tone 1:
Low-rising 13 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
High-falling 52 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Low-rising 24 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2, 1/2].
Tone 5:
High-rising 45 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC,
1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 6:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[1-G, 2, 1/2], as well as in DL[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2]
to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
33.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
390
Language 34 Shanglin Yunling, Central Hongshuihe Zhuang,
NT
34.1 Language information
Language
Central Hongshuihe Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zch
Autonym
ɕuəŋB2
Exonym
Shanglin Zhuang
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Yunling (village name) of Central Hongshuihe Zhuang. It is spoken at Xiyan
Town, Shanglin County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
34.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 20, 2013
Chuangyi Art School, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Place of data elicitation
Region, China.
Given Name
Ýng (瑩)
Family Name
Sū (蘇)
Gender
female
Age
26
Birth Place
Yunling Village (雲靈村), Xiyan Township (西燕鎮), Shanglin County (
林縣),
Nanning Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
When she was 8 years old, she moved to Nanning City with her family. She lives in
Nanning City until now.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
dancing teacher
18 years
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Shanglin Yunling Central
mother tongue
Although she left her village to live in the
Hongshuihe Zhuang
Nanning from 8 years old, she always speaks
his mother tongue with his family until now.
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the
LRP’s country.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Yunling Central Hongshuihe Zhuang as native, he can
communicate in Standard Mandarin and Baihua.
LRP’s mother was born in Hongtao Community village, Yunling Village (雲靈村), Xiyan Township (西燕
鎮), Shanglin County (
林縣). Hongtao is a Yao community. Besides her mother tongue Yao, she can
speak Yunling Central Hongshuihe Zhuang, Standard Mandarin, and Baihua.
LRP’s parents live together with the LRP in Nanning City now.
391
34.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is a tonal flip-flop on tones B, and no tonal
flip-flop in other tones.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 35
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 35
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 35
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 55
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 35
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 55
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 35
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 55
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 221
B2: 53
C2: 31
DL2: 53
DS2: 32
A1/2: 221
B1/2: 53
C1/2: 31
DL1/2: 53
DS1/2: 32
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-Voiced Aspirations &
Breathy Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as follows.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development with voiceless-voiced
register splits.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[ 2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DS[ 2, 1/2]
392
34.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Low level-falling 221 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC,
1-G].
Tone 4:
High-falling 53 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2].
Tone 5:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G].
Tone 6:
A mid-falling with a wider decreasing amplitude 31 occurs in C[2, 1/2] and
another mid-falling with a narrower decreasing amplitude 32 occurs in DS[2,
1/2], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
34.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
According to the sound recording of the only one LRP, there are no phonation types
or voice qualities of tones in this dialect.
34.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The term hja31 ‘to seek’ has an unexpected C2 tone, while its cognate in other Tai
varieties is always of A1 tone.
2) The initial of the term ʔa:35 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
3) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
nok55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, hək55 ‘gums’ (DS1-A), tuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’,
ˀdit55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, and mit32 (DS2) ‘knife’.
34.7 Other sound changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The vowel of ke:33 ‘egg’ is unexpected. The sound change needs more data to
determine with.
2) The initial of kom33 ‘to cover up’ is unexpected. The sound change needs more
data to determine with.
3) The term ŋɔn33 ‘soft’ has been changed the initial from glottal -ʔ to nasal ŋ-, while
its tonal category is contained to be at the B1-G column.
393
4) The initial of ɕəɯ53 ‘chopsticks’ is unexpected because all other NT varieties show
an initial t-, but the tonal category of this term is contained to be at the B12-A
column. The sound change needs more data to determine with.
5) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of θipDS1-A ‘centipede’ which is
designated into DS1-A reflects *s- or *ɬ- which should be considered as DS1-C.
394
35. Language 35 Jiaren, Du’an Yong’an, Central Hongshuihe
Zhuang, NT
35.1 Language information
Language
Central Hongshuihe Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zch
Autonym
to:C1; ɕuəŋB2
Exonym
Du’an Zhuang
Data Source
1 LRP
Jiaren (village name) of Du’an Hongshuihe Zhuang, which is spoken at Yong’an
Variety Name
Township, Du’an County, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
35.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 20, 2013
Chuangyi Art School, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous
Place of data elicitation
Region, China.
Given Name
Chū nyáng (春陽)
Family Name
Mó (磨)
Gender
male
Age
42
Birth Place
Jiaren Community (加仁屯, ka:B1 ɲanB2 in local Zhuang), Yong’an Village (永安村),
Yong’an Township (永安鄉), Du’an County (都安縣), Hechi Prefecture, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Jiaren Community (16 years). Nandan County, Hechi Prefecture (2 years). The
main town of Du’an County (2 years). He is now living in Nanning City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
vice-president of school
22 years
Education
on-the-job junior college
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Du’an Central
mother
Hongshuihe Zhuang
tongue
Standard Mandarin
intermediate
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
intermediate
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background
of LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Du’an Central Hongshuihe Zhuang as native, he
can communicate in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin.
LRP’s mother was born in Banke Community (板客屯), Anhua Village (安
安鄉), Du’an County (都安縣). She is monolingual in her Du’an Zhuang.
395
村), Yong’an Township (永
35.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 51̰
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 553ʔ
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 51̰
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 553ʔ
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 51̰
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 553ʔ
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 51̰
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 553ʔ
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 51̰
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 553ʔ
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 231
B2: 31
C2: 24ʔ
DL2: 31
DS2: 35
A1/2: 231
B1/2: 31
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 31
DS1/2: 35
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-Voiced Aspirations &
Breathy Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as follows:
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development with voiceless-voiced
register splits.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]=DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2]=DS[2, 1/2]
396
35.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 51 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Mid rising-falling 231 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2].
Tone 5:
High level-falling 553 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], and High-level 55
occurs in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in
smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-rising [24] occurs in C[2, 1/2] and Mid-rising [35] occurs in DS[2, 1/2], to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
35.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
A1 tone has a creakiness triggered by its rapid falling contour from the highest pitch
5 to the lowest pitch 1; Tones C1 and C2 have a glottal constriction, like hu:553ˀ
‘cloud’, pja:j553ˀ ‘to walk’, ʔa:553ˀ ‘to open’, ma:24ˀ ‘horse’, ɹam24ˀ ‘water’, and faj24ˀ ‘tree’.
All these voice qualities are not phonemic.
35.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial of the term ʔa:51 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
2) The tone of the term ma:51 ‘to come’ is of A1 tone, to present another type of
voicing alternation which reflects a proto-voiceless initial in NT and voiced in
CT/SWT.
3) The tone of the term kon33 ‘CLF of stone’ is unexpectedly of B1-UC tone, while this
word is expected to be of C1-UC tone in the tone box designed.
4) The tone of the term ˀdaŋ33 ‘to make fire’ is unexpectedly of B1-G tone, while this
word is expected to be of C1-G tone in the tone box designed.
5) The tone of the term tuaj33 ‘bowl’ is unexpectedly of B1-U tone, while this word is
expected to be of C1/2-A tone in the tone box designed. However, the LRP also
claims that tuaj13ˀ ‘bowl’ which is of C12-A tone is also spoken in other villages
around his area.
6) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
397
nok55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, hək55 ‘gums’ (DS1-A), tuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’,
ˀdit55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’, mit35 (DS2) ‘knife’, and tək35(DS12-A) ‘to hit the mark’.
35.7 Other sound changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The vowel ɛ: in ˀdɛ:w51 ‘star’ is an aberrant development when being compared
with a long a: reflected by its cognate (<*ˀda:wA) in other Tai varieties.
2) The term ŋɔn44 ‘soft’ has been changed the initial from glottal -ʔ to nasal ŋ-, while
its tonal category is contained to be at the B1-G column.
3) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of θipDS1-A ‘centipede’ which is
designated into DS1-A reflects *s- or *ɬ- which should be considered as DS1-C.
398
Language 36 Yizhou Suogan, Guibei Zhuang, NT
36.1 Language information
Language
Guibei
ISO 639-3 Code
zyb
Autonym
ɕuəŋB2
Exonym
Man
Data Source
2 LRPs
Variety Name
Suogan (village name) of Guibei Zhuang. This variety is spoken at some villages
in the rural areas in Ma’an Township, Yizhou City, Hechi Prefecture, Guangxi
Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
From the innovation voicing alternation induced by sesquisyllabic onsets between
some Guibei Zhuang and all the other Tai varieties (VASO-GB) shared between
this variety and Huanjiang Chengguan which distributes in the neighboring
Huanjiang County, it can be speculated that some Guibei Zhuang varieties around
that area may be able to be put under an individual language with a new ISO
639-3 code in further studies and surveys.
36.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Qixing Road 13, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
F́zhèng (福正)
Family Name
Wéi (韋)
Gender
male
Age
73
Birth Place
Date
May 19, 2013
Liangqiao Community (良橋屯, li:ŋA2 kiəwA2 in local Zhuang) Suogan Village (索敢村,
ʰɹo:kDL1-A ka:mC1 in local Zhuang), Anma Township (安馬鄉), Yizhou City, Hechi Prefecture,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Liangqiao Community (15 years). Yizhou City (12 years). Guilin City (4 years). Tiandong
County, Baise Prefecture (1 year). Jinchengjiang City (3 years). He is now living in
Nanning City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
retired cadre
39 years
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Suogan Guibei Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
fluent
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China. This language is the
LRP’s hometown Yizhou City’s lingua franca.
Russia
intermediate
His undergraduate major was Russian Language.
399
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Suogan Guibei Zhuang as native, he can communicate
in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin.
LRP’s mother was born in Daping Community (大平屯), Suogan Village (索敢村), Anma Township (安馬
鄉), Yizhou City, Hechi Prefecture, Guangxi. She was monolingual in Suogan Guibei Zhuang.
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Place of data elicitation
Changfu Road 35, Nanning City, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Given Name
Hù idǒu (慧斗)
Family Name
Q́n (覃)
Gender
male
Age
31
Birth Place
Date
May 12, 2013
Liangqiao Community (良橋屯, li:ŋA2 kiəwA2 in local Zhuang) Suogan Village (索敢村,
ʰɹo:kDL1-A ka:mC1 in local Zhuang), Anma Township ( 安 馬 鄉 ), Yizhou City, Hechi
Prefecture, Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
Places lived
Liangqiao Community (12 years). Yizhou City (8 years). He is currently living in
Nanning City.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
engineering management
10 years
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Suogan Guibei Zhuang
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s country.
Guiliu Dialect
fluent
A Southwestern Mandarin variety spoken in the
northwestern part of Guangxi, China. This language is the
LRP’s hometown Yizhou City’s lingua franca.
English
a little
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village. Besides Suogan Guibei Zhuang as native, he can communicate
in Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin and standard Mandarin. LRP’s mother was born in Xiadong Community
(
洞屯, ʰɹʊŋB2 la:C1 in local Zhuang), Dacai Township (大才鄉), Huanjiang County (環江縣), Hechi
Prefecture, Guangxi. Her mother tongue is very closed to Suogan Guibei Zhuang. Besides, she also speaks
a little Guiliu Southwestern Mandarin.
400
36.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flop in this dialect.
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 51̰
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 42
DL1-A: 33
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 51̰
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 42
DL1-C: 33
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 51̰
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 42
DL1-U: 33
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 51̰
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 42
DL1-UC: 33
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 51̰
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 42
DL1-G: 33
DS1-G: 55
A2: 51̰
B2: 33
C2: 42
DL2: 33
DS2: 55
A2: 231
B2: 11̰
C2: 24ʔ
DL2: 23
DS2: 23
A1/2: 231
B1/2: 11̰
C1/2: 24ʔ
DL1/2: 23
DS1/2: 23
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as follows:
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] [2]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] [2]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] [2]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] [2]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] [2]
[2, 1/2]
Some of the items designated into the voiced sounds row ([2]) have the same tone as the one in higher
register ([1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]), while most others in this row keep distinct tone in the lower
register. This tonal split is unique because tonal splits at the row of voiced sounds are not found in the
vast majorities of Tai. However, a similar tonal split pattern is found in the neighboring KS languages.
Tonal split in the row of voiced sounds makes this dialect cannot share the common NT tone box with
other NT varieties. A specific Guibei Zhuang tone box (see Chapter 6) derived from the common NT
tone box is designated based on this dialect. Except this unique tonal split, tones in this dialect are of
straightforward tonal development with voiceless-voiced register splits.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DS[ 2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]=DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2]
C[2, 1/2]=DL[ 2, 1/2]
401
36.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 51 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in tones of some items
designated into A2 (like məŋ51 ‘hand’), as well as a higher level 55 occurs in DS[1A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in tones of some items designated into DS2 (like nɔk55
‘bird’), to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 2:
Low-rising-falling 231 occurs in A [2, 1/2], as well as low-rising [23] occurs in
DL[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
High level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in tones of some items
designated into B2 (like hu33 ‘pair’) as well as in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2], to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-level 11 occurs in B[2, 1/2],
Tone 5:
High-falling 42 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in tones of some items
designated into C2 (like nam42 ‘water’ and maj42 ‘tree’).
Tone 6:
Low-rising 24 occurs in C[2, 1/2].
36.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
A1 (51) has a creakiness triggered by its rapid falling contour from the highest pitch
5 to the lowest pitch 1; B2 (11) also accompanies a weak creakiness due to the
lowest pitch; C2 has a glottal constriction, like bə:24ˀ ‘bride’, təŋ24ˀ ‘crutch’, and ma:24ˀ
‘horse’. All these voice qualities are not phonemic.
36.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) In the wordlist, the items which are designated into the voiced sounds row ([2])
but have the same tone as the one in the high register ([1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G])
are as follows: məŋ51 (A2 > A1) ‘hand’, hu:33 (B2 > B1)‘pair’, nam42 (C2 > C1)
‘water’, maj42 (C2 > C1) ‘tree’, and nɔk55(DS2 > DS1) ‘bird’. These items normally
have initial consonants developing from *mw- (vs. Li 1977: 74, Liang & Zhang
1996: 97, Pittayawat Pittayaporn 2009: 154) like in məŋ51 ‘hand’ and maj42 ‘tree’,
*nr- (vs. Li 1977: 131, Liang & Zhang 1996: 97) or *C̬ .n (vs. Pittayawat
Pittayaporn 2009: 177) like in nam42 ‘water’ and nɔk55 ‘bird’, and an undetermined
*g- (vs. Li 1977: 198, Pittayaporn 2009: 113) or *gl- (Liang & Zhang:1996: 226)
402
like hu:33 ‘pair’70. It is interesting that these initials (except *g- > h- in hu:33 ‘pair’)
present as the same to those in CT/SWT varieties (like mʊŋA2 ‘hand’, majC2 ‘tree’,
namC2 ‘water’ and nɔ:kDS2 ‘bird’ in the Yang Zhuang varieties), but different from
those in the vast majorities of NT, which have f- and r- for these items
respectively, like fəŋA2 ‘hand’, fajC2 ‘tree’, ramC2 ‘water’ and rɔkDS2 ‘bird’ in Du’an.
This phenomenon is also found in Huanjiang (also of Guibei Zhuang) (vs. Zhang et
al. 1999: 192, 198). The items presenting a special voicing alternation between
Suogan/Huanjiang and the vast majority of Tai varieties make the originally
simple NT tone box not able to capture all the tonal splits.
2) The initial of the term ʔa:51 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
3) The tone of the term ma:51 ‘to come’ (provided by LRP2) is of A1 tone, to present
another series of voicing alternation which reflects a proto-voiceless initial in NT
and voiced in CT/SWT.
4) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
nuk55 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, hɯk55 ‘gums’ (DS1-A), tuk55 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’,
and ˀdit55 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’. Since both DL2 and DS2 have merged into one
another to have a low-rising tone (23), it is not clear that mit23 ‘knife’ and tɯk23 ‘to
hit the mark’ which are designated into DL columns but are normally coordinate
with DS columns in other NT varieties have tones DS12-A and DS2 respectively or
not.
36.7 Other sound changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) s-/θ-/ɬ- reflecting *s- (vs. Li 1977) or *ɬ- (vs. Liao & Shen 2012) in other Tai
varieties has been changed to a voiceless approximant ɹ̥- (or sometimes a free
variation r̥-) in this dialect, like r̥a:m51 ‘three’, ɹ̥i:33 ‘four’, and ɹ̥aj42 ‘intestine’.
2) Some proto-voiced initial obstruents are preserved as voiced in this dialect, like
(kaj33) ɣa:ŋ11 (B1/2) ‘young chicken’ and bəɰ24 (C2) ‘bride’, but some are merged
into voiceless sounds, like tɯŋ24 (C2) ‘crutch’.
The term hu:33 (B1) ‘pair’ has an unexpected initial (k- in NT and most CT/SWT but ɕʰ- in Thai
(SWT), Zuozhou (CT) and Baoxu (CT)) and tone (expected to have tone B2 in NT), and it is not certain
that it is cognate with *gu:B ‘pair’ reflected in other Tai varieties. If it is indeed the cognate, the changes
of the initial and the tone need more counterpart data to be determined with.
70
403
3) The term wun33 ‘soft’ has been changed the initial from a glottal -ʔ to an
approximant w-, while its tonal category is contained to be at the B1-G column.
4) The term ŋa:42 ‘to open’ has been changed the initial from a glottal -ʔ to a nasal
ŋ-, while its tonal category is contained to be at the C1-G column.
5) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of ɹ̥ipDS1-A (gɯ:A2) which is designated into
DS1-A reflects *s- (vs. Li 1977) or *ɬ- (vs. Liao & Shen 2012) which should be
considered as DS1-C.
6) The final consonant -k has a free variation -ʔ in this dialect, like pja:ʔDL1
‘forehead’, pjaʔDS1 ‘vegetable’ and ʔaʔDS1 ‘chest’.
7) For the item ‘guest’‚ LRP1 provided hekDL1 but LRP2 provided kekDL1.
8) The initial ɣ- has a free variation g-, like ɣaw24 / gaw24 ‘rice’.
404
Language 37 Baipeng, Liujiang Zhuang, NT
37.1 Language information
Language
Liujiang Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zlj
Autonym
tsu:ŋB2
Exonym
tsu:ŋB2; su:ŋB2
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 33-34, 75-81, 595-808.
Data Info.
This language is spoken in Baipeng Township (百朋鎮), Liujiang Township (柳江縣),
Liuzhou City (柳州市), Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
37.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there is no tonal flip-flops.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 42
B1-A: 33
C1-A: 53
DL1-A: 24
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 42
B1-C: 33
C1-C: 53
DL1-C: 24
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 42
B1-U: 33
C1-U: 53
DL1-U: 24
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 42
B1-UC: 33
C1-UC: 53
DL1-UC: 24
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 42
B1-G: 33
C1-G: 53
DL1-G: 24
DS1-G: 55
A2: 231
B2: 22
C2: 24
DL2: 22
DS2: 33
A1/2: 231
B1/2: 22
C1/2: 24
DL1/2: 22
DS1/2: 33
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-Voiced Sounds
Proto-Voiced Aspirations &
Breathy Sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
405
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[2, 1/2]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
37.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 42 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Low-rising-falling 231 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid level 33 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DS[2, 1/2] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 22 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and in DL[2, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
High-falling 53 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U,
1-UC, 1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
37.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect.
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
406
Language 38 Donglan, Guibei Zhuang, NT
38.1 Language information
Language
Guibei Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zgb
Autonym
ˀjaiC1-G
Exonym
Donglan Zhuang
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 32-33, 99-101, 595-808.
Data Info.
This language is spoken in the main town of Donglan County, Hechi Prefecture,
Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, China.
38.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there is no tonal flip-flops except tones C in this
dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 53
B1-A: 35
C1-A: 33
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 53
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 33
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 53
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 33
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 53
B1-UC: 35
C1-UC: 33
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 53
B1-G: 35
C1-G: 33
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 55
A2: 231
B2: 11
C2: 14
DL2: 11
DS2: 11
A1/2: 231
B1/2: 11
C1/2: 14
DL1/2: 11
DS1/2: 11
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
407
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[2, 1/2]
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
38.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] as well as high level 55
occurs in DS [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 1/2] to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Low-rising-falling 231 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low level 11 occurs in B[2, 1/2], as well as in DL[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 6:
Low rising 14 occurs in C[2, 1/2].
38.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
408
Language 39 Gehan, Qiubei Zhuang, NT
39.1 Language information
Language
Guibian Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zgn
Autonym
ˀi:C1-G
Exonym
ˀjo:jC1-G; Shahua
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 38-40, 72-75, 595-808.
Data Info.
The data of Gehan (township name) are adopted from Zhang et al. (1999). This
language is spoken in Gehan Township (革寒鎮), Qiubei County (丘
縣),
Wenshan Zhuang and Miao Autonomous Prefecture, Yunnan Province, China.
39.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there are tonal flip-flops on A and B tones, but are
not on C, DL, and DS tones in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 32
B1-A: 12
C1-A: 35
DL1-A: 55
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 32
B1-C: 12
C1-C: 35
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 32
B1-U: 12
C1-U: 35
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 32
B1-UC: 12
C1-UC: 35
DL1-UC: 55
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 32
B1-G: 12
C1-G: 33
DL1-G: 55
DS1-G: 55
A2: 53
B2: 31
C2: 33
DL2: 22
DS2: 22
A1/2: 53
B1/2: 31
C1/2: 33
DL1/2: 22
DS1/2: 22
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clusters
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts:
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
[1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. Secondary tonal split
conditioned by glottalized sounds is found in Column C.
409
Horizontally: four individuals and two mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Individual
A[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2]
Merger
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
C[1-G, 2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
39.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid-falling 32 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
High-falling 53 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Low-rising 13 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 4:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in B[2, 1/2].
Tone 5:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC], as well as high level 55 occurs
in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 6
Mid level 33 occurs in C[1-G, 2, 1/2], as well as low level 22 occurs in DL[2,
1/2] and in DS[2, 1/2] to form a complementary distribution in smooth and
checked syllables respectively.
39.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
410
40. Language 40 Xiaosanjiang, Lianshan Zhuang, CT
40.1 Language information
Language
Lianshan Zhuang
ISO 639-3 Code
zln
Autonym
tshy:ŋB2
Exonym
--
Data Source
Zhang et al. 1999: 40-42, 114-119, 595-808.
Data Info.
This variety is spoken in Xiaosanjiang Township (小
江鎮), Lianshan County (連
山縣), Qingyuan Prefecture, Guangdong Province. Lianshan Zhuang is the only
Zhuang language distributing in Guangdong Province.
40.2 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 42
B1-A: 35
C1-A: 55
DL1-A: 35
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 42
B1-C: 35
C1-C: 55
DL1-C: 35
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 42
B1-U: 35
C1-U: 55
DL1-U: 35
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 42
B1-UC: 35
C1-UC: 55
DL1-UC: 35
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 42
B1-G: 35
C1-G: 55
DL1-G: 35
DS1-G: 55
A2: 132
B2: 13
C2: 214
DL2: 13
DS2: 13
A1/2: 132
B1/2: 13
C1/2: 214
DL1/2: 13
DS1/2: 13
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clustersz
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development.
411
Horizontally: three individuals and three mergers
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
A[2, 1/2]
Individual
C[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]= DS[2, 1/2]
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
40.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
High-falling 42 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Low-rising-falling 132 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
Mid-rising 35 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G].
Tone 4:
Low-rising 13 occurs in B[2, 1/2], as well as in DL[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
High level 55 occurs in C[1-A, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and in DS[1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 6:
Low-falling-rising 214 occurs in C[2, 1/2]
40.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
We do not know the phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect since
they are not mentioned in the original text.
412
Language 41 Huishui, Bouyei, NT
41. Language information
Language
Bouyei
ISO 639-3 Code
pcc
Autonym
ˀjʊjC1-G
Exonym
Bouyei
Data Source
2 LRPs
Variety Name
This variety is of the Central Qian Group of Bouyei. This variety is spoken at the
rural areas around Dima Township, Huishui County, Qiannan Bouyei and Miao
Autonomous Prefecture, Guizhou Province, China.
4.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Place of data elicitation
Room 606, Panyu Hotel, Panyu City, Guangdong Province, China.
Given Name
Cháokuān (朝寬)
Family Name
Lú (羅)
Gender
male
Age
49
Birth Place
Dazhai Village (大
Date
May 3, 2013
, ˀba:nC1-G wʊŋA1 in local Bouyei), Dima Township (抵麻鄉),
Huishui County (惠水縣), Qiannan Bouyei and Miao Autonomous Prefecture,
Guizhou Province, China
Places lived
Dazhai Village (10.5 years). Huaxi District of Guiyang City (the capitcal of Guizhou
Province) (9 years). The main town of Huishui County (4 years). He is now living in
Panyu City of Guangdong Province.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
teacher
25 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Huishui Bouyei
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
bachelor
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Southwestern Mandarin
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of the whole Guizhou
Province.
Cantonese
Intermediate
This language is the lingua franca of the cities in Pearl
River Delta in Guangdong Province.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same village as the LRP’s. Besides Huishui Bouyei as native, both of
them can communicate in Southwestern Mandarin and Standard Mandarin.
413
Serial number of LRP
LRP2
Place of data elicitation
Room 606, Panyu Hotel, Panyu City, Guangdong Province, China.
Given Name
Cháoyǔ (朝宇)
Gender
male
Birth Place
Dazhai Village (大
, ˀba:n
Date
C1-G
May 3, 2013
Family Name
Lú (羅)
Age
43
A1
wʊŋ in local Bouyei), Dima Township (抵麻鄉),
Huishui County (惠水縣), Qiannan Bouyei and Miao Autonomous Prefecture,
Guizhou Province, China
Places lived
Dazhai Village (11 years). Huaxi District of Guiyang City (the capitcal of Guizhou
Province) (10 years). Xiamen City of Fujian Province. Shenzhen of Guangdong
Province. He is now living in Panyu City of Guangdong Province.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
20 years
company employee
Education
bachelor
Languages spoken
Level
Language Remarks
Huishui Bouyei
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Southwestern Mandarin
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of the whole Guizhou
Province.
Cantonese
Intermediate
This language is the lingua franca of the cities in Pearl
River Delta in Guangdong Province.
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background of
LRP’s parents
LRP’s parents were born in the same village as the LRP’s. Besides Huishui Bouyei as native, both of
them can communicate in Southwestern Mandarin and Standard Mandarin.
Remarks
LRP2 is the younger brother of LRP1.
41.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart below, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Phonation types
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 24ʔ
B1-A: 55ʔ
C1-A: 33
DL1-A: 55
DS1-A: 24
Continuants
A1-C: 24ʔ
B1-C: 55ʔ
C1-C: 33
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 24
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 24ʔ
B1-U: 55ʔ
C1-U: 33
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 24
A1-UC: 24ʔ
B1-UC: 55ʔ
C1-UC: 33
DL1-UC: 55
DS1-UC: 24
A1-G: 24ʔ
B1-G: 55ʔ
C1-G: 22
DL1-G: 55
DS1-G: 24
Proto-voiced sounds
A2: 11̰3ʔ
B2: 41ʔ
C2: 22
DL2: 41
DS2: 113
Proto-voiced aspirations &
A1/2: 11̰3ʔ
B1/2: 41ʔ
C1/2: 22
DL1/2: 41
DS1/2: 113
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clustersz
Glottal Sounds
breathy sounds
414
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
[1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of non-straightforward tonal development. The only nonstraightforward column is C, with a spilt being voiceless register on one side and golttal/voiced
register on the other side.
Horizontally: two individuals and four mergers
Individual
C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC]
C[1-G, 2, 1/2]
A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Merger
A[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2]
41.4 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: six distinct tones
Tone 1:
Low-rising 24 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 2:
Low level-rising 113 occurs in A[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2], to form a
complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 3:
High level 55 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], and in DL[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1UC, 1-G], to form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked
syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
High-falling 41 occurs in B[2, 1/2] and DL[2, 1/2], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 5:
Mid level 33 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC].
Tone 6:
Low level 22 occurs in C[1-G, 2, 1/2].
415
41.5 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
In this dialect, C tones do not accompany with a glottal constriction, while items
with all other tones unexpectedly have a slight or clear glottal constriction, like
pja24ʔ (A1) ‘hill’, ma:24ʔ (A1) ‘dog’, kɨn24ʔ ‘to eat’, wa:j113ʔ (A2) ‘buffalo’, si:55ˀ ‘four’ (B1),
and ta:41ˀ (B2) ‘river’. This phenomenon is very unique because glottal constrictions
are mostly found on C tones in many other Tai varieties. This may be triggered by
the mergers between Tones A and DS, as well as between Tones B and DS, as the
following analyses: on original dead syllables, *-k after a long vowel in PT been
changed to a slight non-phonemic glottal constriction to merge into their
counterpart tones B (B1 and B2), like pa:55ˀ ‘forehead’, jia55ˀ ‘guest’, pa:55ˀ ‘mouth’,
ma:55ˀ ‘bean’ (semantic change from ‘fruit’), ta:55ˀ ‘to expose under the sun’, tiɑ55ˀ
‘cracking’, ˀjiə55ˀ ‘hungry’, and ða:41ˀ‘root’; *-k after a short vowel in PT has been
changed to a clear non-phonemic glottal constriction to merge into their counterpart
tones A (A1 and A2), like to:24ˀ ‘thin bamboo stripes’, pja24ˀ ‘vegetable’, ðua:113ˀ ‘bird’,
ða:113ˀ ‘to steal’, and ɕɥa:113ˀ ‘cooked’; this may be why the tonal value of tones on the
vertical column of DL and DS has merged into the tonal value of their counterpart
tones B and A; the reason why items with tones on the columns A and B normally
have a slight final glottal might be because in this dialect proto-final *-k from tones
DL and DS might have brought about an effect, a slight glottal final -ˀ, into tones B
and A; the glottalization on tones A is clearer than on tones B, probably because *-k
after a short vowel in PT tends to be changed to a clearer glottalization than *-k
after a long vowel does, and the former one has merged into tones on the A column,
and the later one has merged into tones on the B column. The original glottalization
on tones C might have been dropped due to the extrusion of the later development
of glottalization on Tones A and B. In addition, Tone A2 (113) normally has a
creakiness triggered by its extremely low pitch 11 before rising to a higher pitch 3.
All these voice qualities in this dialect are not phonemic.
41.6 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The initial of the term ʔa:11 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
2) The tone of ˀdaŋ55 ‘to make fire’ is unexpectedly of B1-G tone, while this word is
expected to be of C1-G tone in the tone box designed.
416
3) As those in other NT varieties, the following items designated into DL columns
show a regular tone change to be coordinate with the counterpart DS columns:
no:24 (DS1-C) ‘deaf’, to:24 (DS1-UC) ‘thin bamboo stripes’, ˀdit24 (DS1-G) ‘sunshine’,
ˀdat55 ‘hot’ and miat11 (DS2) ‘knife’.
41.7 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) ʔa:j24 ‘turn face up’ is a glottal sound, while in most Tai dialects it is a nasal ŋ-.
The final consonant of tiŋ113ˀ ‘hornet’ may have been changed from -n to -ŋ.
2) As those in other NT varieties, the initial of θipDS1-A ‘centipede’ which is
designated into DS1-A reflects *s- or *ɬ- which should be considered as DS1-C.
3) Sound changes on dead syllables present as follows: (1) *-k after a long vowel in
PT is completely lost, like pa:55 ‘forehead’, jia55 ‘guest’, pa:55 ‘mouth’, ma:55 ‘bean’
(semantic change from ‘fruit’), ta:55 ‘to expose under the sun’, tiɑ55 ‘cracking’, ˀjiə55
‘hungry’, and ða:41‘root’; *-k after a short vowel in PT has been changed to a slight
non-phonemic glottal final to merge with tones A (A1 and A2), and this means
that tonal value of tones on the vertical column of DS has merged into the tonal
value of their counterpart tones A, like to:24ˀ ‘thin bamboo stripes’, pja24ˀ
‘vegetable’, ðua:113ˀ ‘bird’, ða:113ˀ ‘to steal’, and ɕya:113ˀ ‘cooked’; the reason why
items with Tone A always have a slight final glottal which we have previously
pointed out might be because in this dialect proto-final *-k from tones DS might
have brought about an effect, a slight glottal final -ˀ, into tones A; (2) *-t and *-p
are preserved; however, all items with DL1 tones and *-t /*-p finals, proto-long
vowels have been changed to short vowels, like wat55 ‘astringent’, ɣap55 ‘to carry’,
ɣop55 ‘circumference’, kat55 ‘mustard plant’, and kwat55 ‘to hug’.
417
Language 42 Shitouzhai, Zhenning Bouyei, NT
42.1 Language information
Language
Bouyei
ISO 639-3 Code
pcc
Autonym
pow24 ʔi:31
Exonym
Bouyei
Data Source
1 LRP
Variety Name
Shitouzhai, Western Qian Group (or Zhenning Bouyei), Bouyei. This variety
is spoken in Shitouzhai Village (石頭
), Huangguoshu Township (黃果樹
鎮), Anshun City (安順市), Qiannan Bouyei and Miao Autonomous
Prefecture, Guizhou Province, China.
Although this language is put under the individual language Bouyei with ISO
639-3 code “pcc”, it is clear that varieties of this language are not intelligible
with varieties of the other two Bouyei groups, namely Central Qian of Bouyei
and Southern Qian of Bouyei. Its unique features like aspirated sounds,
diphthongization, and the loss or weakening of final stops, together with
speaker attitudes suggest that it should be an individual language with its
own ISO 639-3 code.
42.2 Information of data source
Serial number of LRP
LRP1
Date
May 2, 2013
Qifu Shijie (祈福食街), Zhongcun Village, Shiguang Road, Panyu
Place of data elicitation
City, Guangdong Province, China.
Given Name
Dēngyàn (登艷)
Family Name
Wǔ (伍)
Gender
male
Age
38
Birth Place
Shitouzhai Village (石頭
), Huangguoshu Township (黃果樹鎮), Anshun City (安
順市), Qiannan Bouyei and Miao Autonomous Prefecture, Guizhou Province,
China.
Places lived
Shitouzhai Village (15 years). Huangguoshu Township (3 years). Anshun City (3
years). Beijing (5-6 years). From 2003 up to now, he has been living in Panyu
City, Guangdong Province.
Time period of living at the present residence
Occupation
worker
10 years
Education
Languages spoken
Level
Huishui Bouyei
mother tongue
Standard Mandarin
fluent
senior high school
Language Remarks
This language is the national language of the LRP’s
country.
Southwestern Mandarin
fluent
This language is the lingua franca of the whole
Guizhou Province.
Cantonese
Intermediate
This language is the lingua franca of the cities in Pearl
River Delta in Guangdong Province.
418
The main factors of the process of LRP’s language cultivating behavior like the language background
of LRP’s parents
LRP’s father was born in the same village as the LRP’s. Besides Zhenning Bouyei as native, both of
them can communicate in Southwestern Mandarin and Standard Mandarin.
LRP’s mother was born in Zhedou Village, Guanling County, Anshun City (安順市), Qiannan Bouyei
and Miao Autonomous Prefecture, Guizhou Province. Her mother tongue is also of Zhenning Bouyei
and is identical with the LRP’s one. She also speaks Southwestern Mandarin and Standard Mandarin.
Remarks
Because of the many mergers of phonemes, most nouns in this dialect are disyllable, like ti:55 ka:33
‘leg’, ti:55 ʋoŋ11 ‘hand’, ʔə:33puə33 ‘hill’, and pe44 ɕi:ŋ44 ‘tail’, to distinguish the meaning of many
homophones on single syllables. Many natural monosyllable nouns in other Tai varieties are added a
prepositive classifier (often with aberrant sound change) in this dialect. For example, the LRP insists
that the word for ‘dog’ must be tu:31 ma:33 but not ma:33, or people would not understand its accurate
meaning. Besides the disyllable nouns, sometimes the LRP also added another segment to a verb or a
classifier to make the item become a compound word. For example, the LRP provided haŋ55 fa:11 for
‘to cover’, but actually the segment fa:11 is actually ‘quilt’, and haŋ55 fa:11actually refers to ‘to cover up
a quilt’. In a similar way, the LRP provides kow24 o:33 for ‘pair’, but the segment o:33 is actually a
phonetic change of ‘one’. Therefore, he did not follow my expected scheme to pronounce all the
monosyllable items during the whole recording. This is very important because some disyllabic words
distinguish meanings from other words containing the homophonous segments of its nuclear syllable.
For example, on the contrast ˀda:ŋ33 ka:j53 ‘egg’ vs. tu:11 ka:j53 ‘chicken’, the first syllables are CLF of
substance and animal respectively, and they play important role of distinguishing the meaning of the
following nuclear syllables which are homophonous with one another. Another example is (ˀdan33) taʔ
‘liver’ vs. taʔ55(ʔo31) ‘to scoop (rice)’. Some tone sandhi or tone coarticulation is found in the recording
of disyllable items, but the specific situation need more data to research with. In the data analyses,
only the syllables being cognate with those in other Tai varieties are employed.
42.3 Basic patterns of tonal development of this dialect
1) As shown in the chart above, there are no tonal flip-flops in this dialect.
Smooth Syllable
Checked Syllable
Phonation types
A
B
C
DL
DS
Aspirations
A1-A: 33
B1-A: 55ʔ
C1-A: 31
DL1-A: 55
DS1-A: 55
Continuants
A1-C: 33
B1-C: 55ʔ
C1-C: 31
DL1-C: 55
DS1-C: 55
Unaspirated stops
A1-U: 33
B1-U: 55ʔ
C1-U: 31
DL1-U: 55
DS1-U: 55
A1-UC: 33
B1-UC: 55ʔ
C1-UC: 31
DL1-UC: 55
DS1-UC: 55
A1-G: 33
B1-G: 55ʔ
C1-G: 31
DL1-G: 55
DS1-G: 55
A2: 11
B2: 24ʔ
C2: 31
DL2: 24
DS2: 24
A1/2: 11
B1/2: 24ʔ
C1/2: 31
DL1/2: 24
DS1/2: 24
ProtoVoiceless
Sounds
Unaspirated stop
+ *r clustersz
Glottal Sounds
Proto-voiced sounds
Proto-voiced aspirations &
breathy sounds
419
2) Patterns of tonal split and merger are as in the following two charts.
Vertically: two way splits and no splits
Tonal category
Register 1
Register 2
A
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
B
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
C
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
DL
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
DS
[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
[2, 1/2]
In conclusion, tones in this dialect are of straightforward tonal development. The only specific
column is C with no spilts.
Horizontally: three individuals and two mergers
A1[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
Individual
A2[1-G, 2, 1/2]
C [1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2]
B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] = DL[1-A,
Merger
1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G]
B[2, 1/2] = DL[2, 1/2] = DS[2, 1/2]
42.3 Tonal characteristics & tone occurrences: five distinct tones
Tone 1:
Mid level 33 occurs in A[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G].
Tone 2:
Low level 11 occurs in A[2, 1/2].
Tone 3:
High level 55 occurs in B[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], as well as in DL[1-A, 1-C,
1-U, 1-UC, 1-G] and DS[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G], to form a complementary
distribution in smooth and checked syllables respectively.
Tone 4:
Low-rising 24 occurs in B[2, 1/2], as well as in DL[2, 1/2] and DS[2, 1/2], to
form a complementary distribution in smooth and checked syllables
respectively.
Tone 5:
Mid-falling 31 occurs in C[1-A, 1-C, 1-U, 1-UC, 1-G, 2, 1/2].
42.4 Phonation types or voice qualities of tones in this dialect
In this dialect, C tones do not accompany with a glottal constriction, while items
with tones in the B column normally have a slight glottal constriction, like (ˀda:ŋ33)
ka:j55ˀ ‘egg’, ɬej55ˀ ‘four’ (B1), and (ʔə33) ta:24ˀ (B2) ‘river’. This may be triggered by the
mergers between tones B and DL, as the following analyses: on original dead
syllables, *-k after a long vowel in PT been changed to a slight non-phonemic glottal
constriction to merge into their counterpart tones B (B1 and B2), like (na:31) pa:55ˀ
420
‘forehead’, (pou31) ɕiə55ˀ ‘guest’, (ˀda:ŋ33) pa:55ˀ ‘mouth’, (ˀdam33) ma:55ˀ ‘fruit’, ta:55ˀ ‘to
expose under the sun’, piə55ˀ ‘cracking’, (tʊŋ31) ʔi:55ˀ ‘hungry’, la:24ʔ (va:j31) ‘root’ and
(tiaw33) tɕa:24ˀ‘rope’; the reason why items with tones on the B column normally have
a slight final glottal might be because in this dialect proto-final *-k from tones DL
might have brought about an effect, a slight glottal final -ˀ, into tones B; the original
glottalization on tones C might have been dropped due to the extrusion of the later
development of glottalization on tones B. The glottal constriction in this dialect is
not phonemic.
42.5 Sound changes involving tones reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) The A1 tones sometimes tend to have a higher pitch 44 than its original pitch 33
when it is pronounced individually. But when it precedes or follows a B1 tone (55,
sometimes a free variation 53), it clearly presents a mid-level 33 for being easily
distinguished from the high level 55 of B1 tone.
2) The tonal value of B1 has two free variations, high level (55) and high-falling (53
when being deliberately pronounced), such as (ˀda:ŋ33) ka:j53 and (ˀda:ŋ33) ka:j55
‘egg’. Another example is that the first syllable in kɔŋ55 ˀda:33‘swaddling clothe’ has
a B1 tone, and it was clearly pronounced as high level and high falling
respectively.
3) The initial of the term ʔa:11 ‘crow’ is of the initial group of glottal sounds, as that
in other NT varieties.
4) The second syllable of the item ʑa:24 nja:33 ‘medicine’ should be cognate with
*ʔjɯəA reflected in other Tai varieties. Its initial consonant has been changed from
a pre-glottalized sound to a yodicized nasal nj-. The environment of the sound
change needs more data to be studied with.
5) The tone of kʰa:33 ‘get stuck’ has an A1 tone (while other Tai varieties have A2
tone for this item), to leave a question open.
6) The tone of ti:55 ʋoŋ11 ‘hand’ is clearly of A2, but in the item ʋoŋ33 kwa:11 ‘right
side’, the first syllable refers to ‘hand’. The tone change from A2 to A1 is suspected
to be a tone sandhi, but need more data to confirm it.
42.6 Other linguistic changes reflected in the wordlist of this dialect
1) Short -aj in other Tai varieties becomes long a:j, like pa:j33 ‘to go’; long -a:j in
other Tai varieties becomes -e:, like te:33 ‘to die’ and ʋe:11 ‘buffalo’. The second
421
syllable of the item kʊŋ44 te:53 ‘maternal grandfather’ should be cognate with
‘maternal grandmother’ in other Tai varieties due to its vowel (-a:j > -e:) and its
B1 tone (the vast majorities of NT reflect *ta:jB for ‘maternal grandmother’). In the
investigation, the LRP also told that the word for ‘maternal grandmother’ is na:j53
te:53. The prefixes kʊŋ44 and na:j53 are actually Chinese loans ‘grandfather’ (<公
kʊŋ55) and ‘grandmother’ (<奶 na:i214) respectively, and they must be added to the
etymon te:53 (<*ta:jB ‘maternal grandmother’) to distinguish the meanings.
Therefore, this item is ruled out in this dialect because it is not the cognate with
*ta:A ‘maternal grandfather’ reflected in other Tai varieties.
2) -a (open front unrounded vowel) contrasts with -ɑ (open back unrounded vowel),
like kʰa31 ‘to kill’ vs. kʰɑ31 ‘near’.
3) Unaspirated stops with Tone C1 in other NT varieties normally present aspirated
in this dialect. For example, kʰa:31 ‘to kill’, lok44 kʰa:31 ‘seedling’, tʰã ‘to establish’
and kʰou31 ‘nine’. The aspiration on C1 contrasts with unaspiration on C2.
4) Sound changes on nasals: -m in other Tai varieties has been changed to -ŋ in this
dialect, like ɬaŋ33 ‘three’ and za:ŋ31 ‘water’; -ŋ in other Tai varieties has been
changed to -n in this dialect, like lək24 tin33 ‚‘cucumber’, but some are nasalized,
like tã ‘to arrive’. The change environments need more data to confirm with. The n in other Tai varieties seems not to be changed,like tu11 tən11 ‘hornet’ and kən33
‘to eat’
5) Proto-Tai *ɣ- is changed to the glottal initial ʔ- like in ʔo:31 ‘rice’ and ʔiə31
‘excrement’.
6) Proto-Tai *tr- change to ɕ- in this dialect, like ɕən33 (<*trin A) ‘stone’ and ɕəʔ55
(<*trap DL) ‘to carry’ .
7) The sound files of (lok44) kʰa:31 ‘seedling’, (ˀbən33) ləʔ55 ‘get dark’, and (tu:11) taʔ55
‘grasshopper’ are followed by modal final particle word ma31 or ɑ31 to be
emphasized by the LRP.
8) Sound changes on dead syllables present as follows: 1) *-k after a long vowel is
completely lost, like (na:31) pa:55 ‘forehead’, (ˀda:ŋ33) pa:55 ‘mouth’, (ˀda:m33) ma:55
‘fruit’, ta:55 ‘to expose under the sun’, and za:24 (va:j31) ‘root’; *-k after a short
vowel is preserved or is changed to -ʔ, like (zi:11) nok55 ‘deaf’, pak55 or paʔ55
‘vegetable’, and naʔ55 ‘heavy’.
422
APPENDIX D
THE MAPS OF THE TAI VARIETIES INVESTIGATED
See the amplified
detail in Map 2
MAP 1. Tai varieties investigated in South China and MSEA (cf. Map 2)
*This map is produced based on the Google Map. The language names and
numberings with ISO 639-3 codes are as in the chart below (also cf. Appendix C).
423
MAP 2. Investigated Central Tai and Northern Tai varieties
concentrating in Guangxi and its neighboring areas (cf. Map 1)
*This map is produced based on the Baidu Map. The language names and
numberings with ISO 639-3 codes are as in the chart below (also cf. Appendix C).
Seven from SWT (L1-7)
15. Debao Tuoxin (unknown)
Thirteen from NT (L30-42)
1. Bangkok Thai (tha)
16. Jingxi Huashan (zgm)
30. Wuming Shuangqiao (zyn)
2. Songkhla Southern Thai (sou)
17. Tiandeng Xiangdu (zzj)
31. Debao Longsang (unknown)
3. Khon Kaen Isan (tts)
18. Daxin Naling (zzj)
32. Debao Dongling (zyj)
4. Chiang Rai Northern Thai (nod)
19. Jingxi Lingding (zzj)
33. Tianlin Lizhou (zgn)
5. Kho Lam Shan (shn)
20. Xiaoguangnan (zhn)
34. Shanglin Yunling (zch)
6. Rong Maet Tai Lue (khb)
21. Wenshan Dazhai (zhg)
35. Du’an Jiaren (zch)
7. Muong Quan Son Tai (unknown)
22. Daxin Leiping (zzj)
36. Yizhou Suogan (zgb)
23. Daxin Baoxu (zzj)
37. Liujiang Baipeng (zlj)
24. Bac Va (nut)
38. Donglan Urban (zgb)
Seventeen from CT (L8-L24)
8. Debao Urban (zyg)
39. Qiubei Gehan (zqe)
9. Debao Dalong (zyg)
Five from YN (L25-29)
40. Lianshan Xiaosanjiang (zln)
10. Debao Lüliu (zyg)
25. Yongning Baiji (zyn)
41. Huishui Bouyei (pcc)
11. Jingxi Hurun (zyg)
26. Nanning Shuangding (zyn)
42. Zhenning Bouyei (pcc)
12. Jingxi Urban (zyg)
27. Long’an Xiaolin (zyn)
13. Jingxi Anning (zyg)
28. Jingxi Daqiu (zyn)
14. Debao Nalong (unknown)
29. Yongning Xialeng (zyn)
424
RESUME
Name:
Liao Hanbo
Date of Birth:
25 July 1977
Place of Birth:
Chengguan Township, Debao County, Guangxi, China
Institutions Attended:
1996-2000, Bachelor of Arts in Japanese Language
(Tourism), Xi’an Foreign Languages University
(西安外国语学
).
2010-2011, Postgraduate Foreign Research Student
(The phonological study of Zhuang), The University of
Tokyo (東京大学).
2011-2016, Master of Arts in Linguistics, Payap
University (มหาวิทยาลัยพายัพ).
Academic Publications:
Liao Hanbo. 2010. 台语支中部组佒侬语 德靖土语
音系概况及其拼音方案详解 [An
overview of the sound system of the Central Taic language Yang-Nong
(Dejing vernacular) and description of a phonetic spelling system]. In Li
Fuqiang and Gao Ya-ning (ed.), Chinese Zhuang Studies 4:70-192. Beijing:
Publishing House of Minority Nationalities.
Liao, Hanbo. 2016a. Diachronic Hierarchies of Tai Tonal Development. Payap
University Journal, 26(2), to appear.
Liao, Hanbo. 2016b. Proto-Tai reconstruction of ‘maternal grandmother’ revisited:
*na:jA, *ta:jA or *ta:jB? Language and Linguistics, to appear.
425
Academic Papers Presented:
Liao, Hanbo. 2013. Proto-Tai reconstruction of ‘maternal grandmother’ revisited:
*na:jA or *ta:jA? SEALS23, Bangkok, 29–31 May, 2013.
Liao, Hanbo. 2015. Yang Zhuang poetry. SEALS25, Chiang Mai, 27–29 May, 2015.
Liao, Hanbo & Shen, Ruiqing. 2012. Gedney’s tone box revisited: Evidence from
some varieties of Central and Northern Tai. ICSTLL45, Singapore, 26–28
October, 2012.
Shen, Ruiqing & Liao, Hanbo. 2012. Acoustic-tonetic Study of Pjang Zhuang: An
undescribed Central Tai Variety. ICSTLL45, Singapore, 26–28 October, 2012.
426