IDENTITY: AN INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF THEORY AND RESEARCH, 3(1), 29-52
Copyright © 2001 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
The New Color Complex:
Appearances and Biracial Identity
David L. Brunsma
Department of Sociology
University of Alabama
Kerry Ann Rockquemore
Department of XXX
University of Connecticut
Ethnic identity research has largely focused on the identity choices of White ethnics
(Alba, 1990; Ignatiev, 1995; Waters, 1990). One key factor in these choices is bodily
appearance. We extend this research to Black and White Biracial individuals and
examine the role that physical appearance plays in their “choices” of racial id entity.
We test Rockquemore's (1999) taxonomy of Biracial identity using survey data from
a sample of 177 Biracial respondents. The results indicate that Biracial individuals do
make choices within circumscribed cultural contexts and these understandings are
influenced not by skin color, but by an actor's assumption of how others perceive his
or her appearance.
Recent work on eth nic identity has focused on the construction and maintenance of
White ethnic identities an d the choic es made b y individuals with m ultiple W hite
Requests for reprints should be sent to David L. Brunsma, Department of Sociology, Morton Hall
344, University of Alabama, Huntsville, AL 35899. E-mail:
[email protected]
30
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
ethnic heritages (Alba, 1990 ; Ignatiev, 1995; W aters, 1990). As Am ericans’
ethnicities have become progressively intermixed, research has focused on why
individuals with multiple White ethnic backgrounds choose to emphasize one
ethnicity over others. Factors such as knowledge about an ethnicity, surname,
physical appearance, and the general p opularity of eth nic groups emerge as the m ost
salient factors influencin g individual p references. T his body o f research sup ports
the notion that W hite ethnicity is largely “symbolic” (Gans, 1979) and has no real
impact on an individual’s life chances o r everyday inte ractions. It is this symb olic
basis of White ethnicity that differentiates it significantly from race. Although
White ethnic identity remains an option, racial identities are neither situationally
based nor symbolic. For members of racial groups, options are either nonexistent
or function in a mo re tightly constrain ed manne r than for W hites because racial
categories are socially and culturally stratified. Due to cultural norms like the “one
drop rule” and the subseque nt racist social structure (Bonilla-Silva, 1997; Loveman,
1999), racial identity cuts across situations and im pacts individual life chances.
Studies of ethnic identity o ptions pro vide a rich fou ndation for r acial identity
research. The strength of this work is its fertile illumination of the complex link
between social context, culture , and identity. The literature highlights the ways that
individuals reflexively interpret their sense of self in specific social and cultural
contexts by using various symbols , cultural understandings, and relative
relationships between groups. In this article we focus not on W hite ethnic options,
but on Black and White Biracial individuals and the “choices” they make about
their racial identity. Because these Biracials (at least theoretically) straddle the color
line, their marginality makes them a unique group for the exploration of the
relationship between appearances and identity. Focusing on Biracial people enables
us to explore how culture and social context influence identity construction through
an understanding of how cognitive interpretations of differing racial categories
interact with significant symb ols to influence the way that social actors understand
themselves and their relationship to others. The most salient symbol representing
group membership is bodily appearance.
Appearances of various so rts (e.g., skin colo r, clothing, hairstyle, etc.) are
important to the proc ess of human interaction be cause we ar e unable to directly
observe the intentions and character of individuals (Goffman, 1959). It is through
appearances that we evaluate both others and ourselves, and through which we
present ourselves to others (Ichheiser, 1970). Appearances present our identities to
others and allow us to infer the identitie s of others. Appearances help to define a
situation and provide some cognitive context for all the individuals involved. In this
sense, app earances c an beco me a reality in and of themselve s (Stone, 19 62).
The empirical literature on the effects of skin color, the most salient of a ll bodily
appearance characteristics for discussion s of race, illumina tes its importanc e. Skin
color has been found to affect mate and friend selection (P orter, 199 1), a variety of
life chances and opportunities (for reviews, see Hughes & Hertel, 1990; Porter &
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
31
Washington, 1993), and mental illness (Boyd -Franklin, 19 89; Har vey, 1995; Neal
& W i lson, 1989). The phenotype literature, however, has neither addressed the
issue of how indiv iduals of colo r perceive th eir own racia l identity nor how
phenotype might play a ro le in the racial self-understandings of persons of color.
Biracial individuals’ appearances are often times ambiguous and they exist in a
variety of different social contexts of differing racial composition. According to the
common sense assumption that race is a biological, as oppo sed to a soc ially
constructed reality, individuals’ physical traits determine racial group membership.
Therefore, an individual’s phenotype should determine racial identity. The caveat
to this system is the one drop rule that manda tes the assumption that, above and
beyond phenotyp ic categoriza tions, anyone with a Black ancesto r is Bla ck
(regardless of appearance). Exploring the influence of appearances on identity for
Biracials allows us to join and extend the literature by understanding how
presentations of self and the reviews of others are reflexive and function within a
socially constructed set of parame ters.
Using Rockquemore’s (1999) taxonomy of the racial self-understandings of
Biracial individuals, we test how phenotype and social context affect the racial
identities of Black and White Biracials. We review the literature on the relation
between phenotyp e and racia l identity for multirac ial and Biracial individuals.
Unlike ethnic optio ns, appea rances play a counter-intuitive r ole in the way Biracial
people choose the ir racial identity. T hrough inve stigating the app earance– identity
link with an emerging demographic group, we can grasp the power of cultural
influences and social interaction over the process of constructing and understanding
racial identities.
Biraciality and Racial Identity
Since the late 1970s, research on the racial identity of Black and White Bir acials
has suggested that there exist two possi bilities for these individ uals’ racial selfunderstandings: Black or Biracial (Bowles, 1993; Brown, 1990; Cross, 1971; Field,
1996; Hall, 1980; Herring, 1995; Miller & Miller, 1990; Morten & A tkinson, 1983;
Porter & Washing ton, 1993; Porterfield, 1978; Poston, 1990; Poussaint, 1984;
Taylor-Gibbs, 1997). Early scholarship appears to have been based less on the
results of empirical research and more on the historical and cultural norm of the one
drop rule as the underlying assumption. This rule mandated that Biracial offspring
adopt a Black racial iden tity. M ost research in the 1970s and early 1980s was
interested in understand ing how these individuals d evelo ped a Black identity. By
the mid 1980s and throughout the 1990s, the extant pathologies associated with
Biracial individuals’ ma rginality drew th e attention of a new generation of
researchers who sough t to explain psycholog ically, clinically, and d evelopm entally
how these individuals developed a Biracial identity and how they could maintain
32
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
a healthy, integrated sense of their Biraciality (Bowles, 1993; Brown, 1990; Daniel,
1996; Field, 1996; Hall, 1980; Herring, 1995; Johnson, 1992; Kerwin & Ponterotto,
1993; Kich, 1992; Poston, 1990; Taylor-Gibbs, 1997; Wardle, 1989 , 1992). These
studies incorporated findings and discussions about the racial self-understandings
of Biracials fro m earlier rese arch and c ontinued the develop ment a dichotomous
typology of racial identity possibilities for Biracials: Black and Biracial—this time
with an overt preference for the deve lopment o f a Biracial ide ntity. Such research
set the parameters, within the scholarship, of the po ssibilities for racial identity
among Biracial people.
The 1990s brought yet another generation of scholars interested in the lives of
multiracial people. These investigators utilized new analytical tools and
incorporated interdisciplinary approaches. The work of several authors from this
group (Davis, 1991; Rockquemore, 1999; Root, 1990, 1992, 1996; Spickard, 1989)
began to illuminate for us that multiracial people had multiple understandings of
their racial identities. Davis challenged us to consider the question Who Is Black?
Root (1990) convinced us that these individuals had different negotiations and
answers to the question of “how to resolve the status of the ‘other.’” This new work
has set the tone for research on multiracial and Biracial individuals. Root’s (1990,
1992, 1996) work however, represented both the methodological limitations of the
existing research (i.e., the exclusive use of clinical studies and small self-selective
samples) while replicating the unidirectional, atheoretical, and ideologically biased
work of the 1980 s.
Rockquemore (1999) presented a comprehensive typology that integrates the
voices of Biracial individuals with a variety of classic and conte mporar y identity
theories (Cooley, 1902; Gergen, 1991; Giddens, 1991; Lifton, 1993; Mead, 1934;
Turkle, 1997). She suggested that there are four types of racial identity options for
biracial people:
a) singular identity (singular Black or White)
b) bord er identity (exclu sively Biracia l)
c) protean identity (sometimes Black, sometimes White, sometimes Biracial
d) transcen dent identity (no racial identity)
It is this typology of ra cial identification s of Biracials that we use to explore the
appearance–identity link using data from the Survey of Biracial Experience.
Research o n Pheno type and Ra cial Identity
Early research, built on the assu mption of the one drop rule, was inade quate in
identifying the complexities of racial identity among Biracials in the United States.
This being the case, the relation between phenotype and racial i dentity remained
unquestioned and was rarely the focus of empirical investigation. Methodological
problems in collecting data on Biracial individuals, coupled with samples revealing
little variation in phenotypes, made exploration of the relation between phenotype
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
33
and racial identity amon g Biracials d ifficult. The lack o f empirical stud y is
associated with the paucity of theoretical development of the appea rance–id entity
link. The research on Biracials in the 1980s never investigated the role of
appearance in identity formation and maintenance because people assumed that
regardless of your physical appearance, if you had a Black parent, you were Black.
Several researchers have given the appearance–identity question passing
comments that transcend the one drop assumption (Bowles, 1993; Brown, 1990;
Daniel, 1996; Field, 1996; Hall, 1980; Tizard & Phoenix, 1995). Brown stated that
appearance is an important factor in the emotional and cognitive attitudes toward
oneself and discussed the importance of physical appearance for Biracials. He
implied that the darke r the skin of the B iracial individu al, the more likely he or she
would be to adopt and develop an exclusively B lack identity. However, he also
theorized that factors such as the ratio of Blacks to Whites in given socialization
contexts could influen ce this relationsh ip, but he did not specify ho w. Similarly,
Daniel discussed how the intricate interrelationships between socialization
experiences and the broader social and cultural context of race relations can
influence the appearance–identity link; this was a step forward although he made
no clear hypotheses. Bowles described the treatment of her Biracial clients toward
the development of a healthy Biracial identity, found that women had a more
difficult time accepting their Black physical features than men; thus, implicating a
possible gender interaction with phenotype in the influence on identity. Tizard and
Phoenix found that White-appearing Biracials were unlikely to identify ex clusively
as Black. On the whole, although several scholars believe that physical appearance
is crucial to the developme nt and maintenance of a rac ial identity among Biracials,
the research has not rigorously investigated the appearance–identity link.
Furthermore, there is little theory to guide investigation.
The research on Biracial identity has often cited the reactions of W hites to
Biracial individuals; it was The Color Complex (Russell, Wilson, & Hall, 1992) that
really tackled the issue of the politics of skin color among African Americans. They
considered the Black community’s love–hate relationship with the one drop rule and
with skin color. T he argume nt is that a three-tiered society existed in the South with
the following hierarchy from highest to lowest status: Wh ites, Mulattos, and B lacks.
Mixed-race individuals often served as a buffer group between Whites and Blacks,
through which cross-color interactions and b usiness transactions c ould occur. T his
situation caused a preferential treatment of Mulattos by Whites and a generational
advantage for Mulatto s—the fo undation fo r a socia l and cultural system of color
classism within Black America was laid. The authors provided strong evidence that
those members of the community with the lightest skin color and the most
Caucasian-looking features have been allowed the greatest freedoms and achieved
at higher rates. The argument goes further to display the ways that darker-skinned
members of the Black comm unity discriminate against mixed-race individuals in the
workplace, how patterns of dating within the community are tangled up with
phenoty pe, how netwo rks are con structed or d ismantled o n the basis of co lor
34
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
classism, and how culturally, Blacks use unique cultural coding (tertiary clues such
as hair, first names, etc.) to distinguish between those who are Black and those who
are not. How ever, The Color Complex does not discuss the relation between color
and identity.
It is clear that there is only speculation as to the relationship between
appearance and racial identity among Biracial ind ividuals. Th ere has bee n little to
no theoretical development on this relationship. One important distinction
(Rockquemore, 1999; Root, 1990) is that color is both a personal and a social
characteristic; that is, one perc eives their skin co lor, but one a lso interprets the ir
appearance through the eyes of others within any given interactional sp here. Bo th
Root and Rockquemore highlighted the inherent problem that exists when there is
a mismatch b etween ap pearance and identity. In o ther words, it wo uld be extrem ely
difficult for a person to c hoose an exclusively B lack, or exclu sively White , identity
if their physical ap pearance s do not m atch their chosen identity. In the end, the
literature on the appearance–identity link is sparse and seriously underdeveloped.
We re medy this situatio n here.
Conceptual Hypotheses
Based on the previous discussions, we propose several hypotheses concerning the
link between ap pearance s and the rac ial identity choice s of Biracial individuals. The
literature seems to indicate that appeara nce is itself multifacete d. We conceptualize
appearance to be comprised of two components: self-perceived skin color (how
Biracial respondents place them selves on a skin color co ntinuum) and sociallymediated appeara nce (how B iracial respo ndents und erstand their own appearance
based on how others interpret their appearance). Although previous work assumes
that the one dro p rule would guide identity ch oices, w e feel, given the previous
appearance distinction, that self-perceived skin color will not directly affect racial
identity (H1). We expect this to be the case because a Biracial individu al’s
understanding of their own appearanc e seems to be rooted more in others’
perceptions and assum ptions of a B iracial individu al’s appear ance and the link with
identity (i.e., if others assume a p henotypica lly ambiguou s Biracial ind ividual is
Black, that Biracial will adopt a Black identity).
The variable we call “appearance” is distinctly more social than phenotype
because it is cr eated by the Biracial ind ividual’s unde rstanding of the ir skin color
as conditioned through the judgments of others in interactions. Thus, we expect that
it is appearance, not skin color, which will influence the racial identification of
Biracials, and that skin color works through one’s app earance to affect identity.
Therefore, our secon d propo sition is that an actor’s socially perceived appearance
will affect racial identity even after controlling for self-perceived skin color (H2).
Finally, we hypothe size that a variety of social factors, including the racial
composition of preadult social contexts, parental socioeconomic status, and
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
35
negative treatment by Whites or Blacks will alter the appearance–identity link. W e
are particularly interested in examining the effects of racial differences in social
network composition on the appearance– identity link because it is within these
social contexts that the interactional parameters for identity are established. From
the review of the literature that has touched on the appearance–identity link, we
expect that the strength of the link will fluctuate depending on Biracial individuals’
contextual experiences of race. Therefore, our final hypothesis is that the contextual
experiences of race will moderate the effect of appearance on racial identity (H3).
Method
In efforts to collect data on Biracial people, researchers have encountered numerous
unique challenges, including accurate identification of potential respondents and the
ability to attract e nough respondents to draw meaningful generalizations. Numerous
data sets have been collected in the pa st 5 years, however no published empirical
studies have exceeded samples of over 50 respondents and most studies have
included, within small university-based snowball samples, respondents with varying
combinations of racial backgrounds. The most oft-cited research anthology of
mixed-race people is Root’s (1996) The Multiracial Experience: Racial Borders as
the New Frontier. Although it is generally considered the most comprehensive
collection of research on the Biracial p opulation, the largest samp le used in any of
the studies include d in the antho logy consisted of 31 resp ondents. M ost of the
studies in this text generalize b roadly from anecdo tal, biograp hical, or small
interview studies with fewer than 20 respondents. Small samples, which include
large degrees of variance o n elements as fundamen tal as a racial–e thnic
combination of parents make it extremely difficult to draw any meaningful
generalizations whatsoever. To delve more deeply into the social processes that
govern the meaning of racial identity and factors influen cing racial ide ntity
construction for Biracia ls, we condu cted a surve y.
Respon dents for the Survey of Biracial Experience were taken from two
colleges in metropo litan Detroit, M ichigan: a priva te liberal arts college and a large
commu nity college. The Detroit Metropolitan Area was selected due to the large
African American population and the diversity of neighborhoods and social
contexts. Both the community college and the university dra w students from all
parts of the metropolitan area, allowing for respondents who live both inside and
outside the racially homogeneous city limits. Both institutions have students who
ranged in age, socioeconom ic status, and life experience, all of which were factors
examined in the process of the research project. In sampling the college student
population, we wanted to include respondents who were comparable along the
dimension of education level, but purposively stratified by socio econom ic
indicators, racial comp osition of their social networks, and other salient dimensions
of interest. Using ins titutional lists of students, we so licited 4,53 2 students
36
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
registered as “Black o r African Am erican”, “Other”, or those that left the race
question blank on the ir college’s ad mission form s. The ratio nale for this decision
was drawn from the sampling strategy used in Phase 1 of the research design where
e-mail solicitation of African American students at the University of Notre Dame
drew out the initial Biracial respondents. The pilot study reve aled that eve n students
who most m ilitantly stated they were Biracial (i.e., they were “not Black”)
indicated they had identified as Black or African American on their college
admission forms. This was done for two reasons: (a) the desire for social or
organizational inclusion and (b) perce ived individ ual gain. Students stated that they
checked the “Black” box instead of “other” (and writing in “Biracial” or
“mixed”) because they did not want to be excluded from activities that were
targeted for the Black student population, such as solicitations for Black student
organizations or announcements of special speakers. The second, and more salient
reason, was that students felt that it couldn’t hurt their opportunities for admission
or for financial opportunities that may be designated for minority students. Given
this consistent response, soliciting the entire Black student p opulation at each
institution seemed to be the most efficient way to draw a group of Biracial
respondents. We contac ted these individuals, inquiring whether they had one W hite
and one Black identifying parent and eventually we located and surveyed 250
individuals. After cleaning the data, identifying only Black and White Biracials, and
removing cases with missing data, we w ere left with 177 cases for the analysis.
Measu rements
Identity. To assess the racial identifications of the Biracial survey respondents, we
used a slight modification of Rockquemore’s (1999) typolo gy of the racial selfunderstandings of Biracial individuals. As mentioned previously, she found that
Black an d Wh ite Biracials un derstood themselves in four basic ways: (a) singular
identity, (b) border identity, (c) protean identity, and (d) transcendent. The
responses to the following question in the Survey of Biracial Experience were used
to measure id entity in this study: Which o f the following state ments best describes
your racial identity? Respondents chose from the following options: (a) I consider
myself exclusively Black (or African American ), (b) I some times consid er myself
Black, sometimes my other race, and sometimes Biracial depending on the
circumstances, (c) I consider myself Biracial, but I experience the world as a Black
person, (d) I consider myself exclusively as Biracial (neither Black nor White), (e)
I consider myself exclusively as my other race (not Black or biracial), (f) Ra ce is
meaningless, I do not believe in racial identities, or (g) Other (fill in the blank). W e
coded the singular Black identity as 1, the singular White identity as 5, the protean
identity as 2, and the transcendent identity as 6. Fundamental social-psychological
theory (e.g., Stone, 1962) highlights the important fact that identities, formulated
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
37
within interaction with o thers, can be validated o r unvalidated by others in
interaction. Therefo re, because these individu als in our samp le are all Biracials, we
modified the border identity within Rockquemore’s typology to include both a
validated border id entity and an unvalidated border identity. Hence, we coded the
validated border id entity as 3 and the unvalidate d borde r identity as 4. T his
distinction will become crucially impo rtant later. We do no t look at those
responding “Other” in this study—only 4.2% of the sample c hose to ide ntify this
way.
Phenotype (skin co lor). Defined as self-perceived skin color, the survey asked
respond ents to locate themselves on a scale from 0 to 12 that be st described their
skin color. The c ontinuum used the following terms and codes: 12 = black, 10 =
dark brown, 8 = medium brown, 6 = light brown, 4 = yellow, 2 = olive, and 0 =
white. This resulted in an ordin al level phenotype variable (odd numbers were
possible and indicate a phenotype between two categories). For the analytical
purposes of this study (and to reduce em pty cells) we co llapsed this or dinal variab le
into three categories, in which 1 was light skin (0 to 4), 2 was medium skin (5 to 7),
and 3 was dark skin (8 to 12).
Appearance. Although th e previous varia ble attemp ts to measure respond ents
self-perceived skin color, the appearance variable measures respondents’ estimates
of how other people categorize them based on th eir physical features. The
distinction here is between the respondents’ self-assessment and their understanding
of others’ assessments. We ask ed responden ts to sele ct which of the following
statements best described their physical appearance: (a) “I look Black and mo st
people assume that I am Black “; (b) “ M y physical feature s are ambig uous, peo ple
assume I am Black mixed with something else “; (c) “My physical features are
ambiguous, people do not assume that I am Black “; and (d) “I physically look
White, I could pa ss.” We r everse-c oded th is item so that highe r values reflect a
more Black categorization, or response from others , whereas low er values indic ate
a more White response from others in interaction.
Interactional Experience 1: Pread ult racial composition. Modifying Demo
and Hughes’s (1990) interracial contact variable to assess the racial composition of
respondents’ significant socialization contexts, individuals were asked to assess the
racial composition of their: (a) grammar or elementary school, (b) closest friends
in elementary school, (c) junior high schoo l, (d) high scho ol, (e) closest frien ds in
high school, and (f) neighborhood while growing up. The responses were cod ed in
the following way: 0 = most all W hite, 1 = mostly W hite, 2 = about half Black, 3 =
mostly Black, 4 = all Blacks. From these items we constructed a preadult scale of
38
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
social network composition (.93) ranging from 0 to 24. Fo r purpose s of the analysis
in this study, we collapsed this variable into three categories in which 1 was
predom inately Wh ite (0 to 4, 32.8% of ca ses), 2 was mixed B lack and White (5 to
10, 33.3% of cases), and 3 was predominately Black (11 to 24, 33.9% of cases).
Interactional Experience 2: Negative treatment from Whites and
Blacks. Another contextual experience of race that may alter the interactional
experiences of Biracial ind ividuals is whether they experience negative treatment
from Blacks or from W hites throughout their lives. Such negative interactions can
serve as “push factors” that may lead Biracials to adopt a particular racial
understanding of themselve s, and that ma y paramete rize the app earance– identity
link. To assess the way that Biracials may experience race, we aske d respon dents
whether they had ever experienced personal discrimination or hostility from either
Whites or Blacks because of their mixed-race. We coded this variable in the
following way in which 0 was no and 1 was yes.
Results
Phenotype , Appearances, and Identity
This sample of Biracial individuals was varied on the dimensions of most interest
to us in this study: phenotype, appearance, racial identity, and contextual experience
of race. Before repo rting the results of the analysis, it was importa nt to indicate the
degree of variation in this sample on the se key variables.
Within our sample, there existed wide variatio n in self-reporte d skin color. For
the original ordinal variable, which ranged from 0 to 12, the median score was 6,
and the variation is striking. For the analyses presented later, we collapsed the
original skin color va riable into thre e equal cate gories: light, (33.9%), medium
(39%), and dark (27.1%) skin. Respon dents, by their o wn estimation , assessed their
skin color as ranging across the phenotyp ic spectrum. T his variance w as critical to
our analysis because it allowed us to explore the influence of skin color on the
choices that o ur respond ents make a bout their rac ial identity.
In addition, respondents differed in their answer to the questio n of their socially
mediated appearance. Some variation in responses does exist. However, the
majority of respondents (56.2%), despite reporting wide variation in skin color,
described their appearance as “ambiguous though most people assume I am Blac k.”
Seventeen percent (17.2%) stated that they “appe ar Black, m ost peop le assume I
am Black,” whereas 16.6% stated that they were “ambiguous, most people do not
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
39
assume I am Blac k.” Finally, 10.1% of this sample o f Biracials said they “appear
White, I could pa ss as Wh ite.” An examinatio n of the desc riptive measu res reveals
two crucial facts that ar e importa nt to our forthco ming analysis: (a) The sam ple
includes variation on the two key measures of appearance set forth in our theoretical
model, and (b) the d ifferences between these two variables suggests that they are,
in fact, measuring two conceptua lly distinct elements.
Contrary to much of the literature on the racial identification of Biracials, the
individuals in this sample also vary in their racial self-understanding. Nearly two
thirds (64%) of the respo ndents con sider themse lves to have b order ide ntities.
Specifically, they understand themselves racially as neither Black and White, but
as exclusively B iracial. How ever, most o f these border Biracials say they
experience the world as a Black p erson; thus, their identity as exclusively Biracial
is unvalidated by others in interactions—that is, the category of border or biracial
simply is not recognized by their interaction partners. Fo rty percent o f the sample
have these unvalidated borde r identities, whereas 23.6% indicate a validated border
identity. Also contra ry to popu lar assumptio ns, only 13.7 % of our sample
understand themselves as singularly Black. The same percentage (13.7%)
understand themselves w ithout racial lab els, or with a transc endent ide ntity. Five
percent see themselves as shifting between Black, White, and Biracial depending
on the circumsta nces (i.e., pro tean identity), whe reas the rem aining 3.7 %
understand themselves a s singularly White. T here is significant va riation with
regards to racial identity among this samp le of Biracials.
There is also significant variation in the contextual experiences of race o f these
respondents. These Biracials came from a variety of prea dult socializatio n contexts
with varying racial compositions. The original variable, ranging from 0 to 24, has
a mean of 7.74 and a standard deviation of 5.90. We collapsed this variable into one
with roughly equal proportions: predominantly White (32.8%), mixed Black and
White (33.3 %), and predominantly Black (33.9%). Also, most of these Biracial
individuals had, throughout their lives reported experiencing negative treatment
from Blacks (60.5%) and even more from W hites (74.3%). 1
1
In addition to these dimensions of variation within the sample, it is important to note other vari ables
that show good degrees of variation in the Survey of Biracial Experience: For example, most of their
parents had at least a high school degree or some college, their parents held average occupations in
terms of prestige rankings, their parents were, on average, not very politically active. These respondents
range widely in age, but average in the mid-twenties.
40
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
The Relationship Between Phenotype, Appearance, and Racial
Identity Am ong B iracials
H1 stated that we did not expect skin color to affect racial identity choice among
Biracials. Table 1 presents the relationship between phenotype and racial
identification among our B iracial respondents. There is no association b etween skin
color and the way that Biracial individuals racially understand themselves, c 2(10,
N = 161) = 8.28, p = .602. This provides supportive evidence for our first
hypothesis.
In fact, we discussed the idea that others’ interpretations of our appearances are
more important in o ur self-understandings than our own perceptions of how we
look. H2 proposed that it is socially-mediated appearance, or the way you believe
others perceive yo urself, that we expected to influence racial identity among
Biracials. Table 2 explores H 2—the categorica l association between appearance
and racial identification among Black and White Biracials. There is, in fact, a strong
association between ap pearance and identity in this sample, c 2(15, N =159) = 35.80,
p = .002. The results can be most clearly understood by examining each of the
various identity types and their unique link with appearance. Appearance, measured
this way, operationalizes both the reviews of others and the racialized assumptions
others make about Biracial individuals in interactions—it is clear that these reviews
and assum ptions dee ply affect Birac ials’ self-understand ings of their own race.
Singular identity. By exam ining the appearance–identity link within each
separate identity choice, distinct patterns emerge. Biracials who adopt singular
Black identities most commonly report (95.5%) that others “assume they are
Black.” Even when they don’t “appear Black” and are ambiguous in appearance,
others assume they are Black and these assumptions have lead most of these
individuals to understand themselves as Black. In fact, none of the singular Blacks
say that they “appear White and could pass,” even though according to data from
Table 1, 27.3% of them have light skin color. The results for singular Whiteidentifying Biracial resp ondents are tenuous due to the low num ber of case s in this
category.
Protean identity. Some re sponde nts adopt a shifting, or pro tean, racial ide ntity,
in which they understand themselves as sometimes Black, sometimes White, and
sometimes Biracial. These respondents have a wide range of appearances as
understood through the reviews of others. Most (75%) report their appearance as
ambiguous (either “ambiguous, not assumed Black” or “ambiguous, assumed
Black “). We are cautious however, in o ur interpretatio n of the app earance id entity
TABLE 1
Relationship Between Phenotype and Racial Identifications Among Black and White Biracials
Validated
Unvalidated
Singular
Border ID
Border ID
Black ID
Protean ID
Phenotype
%
n
%
n
%
n
%
n
27.3
6
12.5
1
44.6
29
31.6
12
Light skina
Medium skinb
36.4
8
50.0
4
38.5
25
22.6
14
36.4
8
37.5
3
16.9
11
27.9
12
Dark skinc
Totals
13.7
22
5.0
8
40.4
65
23.6
38
Note. Percentages refer to those who identify as indicated who have that type of skin color. ID =
p=.602.
a
34.8%, n=56. b 38.5%, n=62. c 26.7%, n=43.
TABLE 2
Relationship Between Appearance and Racial Identifications Among Black and White Biracials
Singular
Unvalidated
Validated
Black ID
Protean ID
Border ID
Border ID
Appearance
%
n
%
n
%
n
%
n
Appears White:
00.0
0
12.5
1
4.7
3
15.8
6
could passa
4.5
1
25.0
2
10.9
7
18.4
7
Ambiguous: not
assumed Blackb
50.0
11
50.0
4
62.5
40
60.5
23
Ambiguous:
assumed Blackc
45.5
10
12.5
1
21.9
14
5.3
2
Appears Black:
assumed Blackd
Transcendent
Singular
ID
White ID
%
n
%
n
33.3
2
27.3
6
33.3
2
40.9
9
33.3
2
31.8
7
3.7
6
13.7
22
identity. c 2 (10, N=161) = 8.28,
Singular
White ID
%
n
20.0
1
40.0
2
22.7
5
40.0
2
45.5
10
00.0
0
4.5
1
Totals
13.8
22
5.0
8
40.3
64
23.9
38
3.1
5
Note. Percentages refer to those who identify as indicated because they have that type of appearance. ID = identity. c
35.80, p=.002.
a
10.7%, n=17. b 15.1%, n=24. c 56.6%, n=90. d 17.6%, n
41
Transcendent
ID
%
n
27.3
6
2
13.8
22
(15, N=159) =
42
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
link for this identity type because the number of respondents describing their racial
identity as pro tean is small.
Transcendent identity. A small number of our respondents described their racial
identity as transcendent, or with no racial self-understanding (13.7%). O f these
respondents, only 4.5% “a ppear B lack” and th e bulk of the transcendents (68.2%)
are “ambiguous” in their socially-mediated appearance. In othe r words, one does
not have the luxur y of being “race-free” if one’s phenotype is dark because for
dark-skinned individuals, race impinges itself on their day-to-day existence.
Border identity. The most substantial patterns highlighting the effects of
appearance on racia l identity are among the respondents we categorize as having
a border identity. These individuals see themselves racially as neither Black nor
W hite. Their racial self-understanding is something new, not a combination of
Black and White, but a new category of existence: Biracial. This identity, however,
does not exist in all of these individuals’ social networks. If it does exist as a
socially understood racial category then the likelihood of this identity option being
validated by others is increased. If the category of Biracial does not exist, then these
individuals’ racial identity is not validated by other people.
Over 60% o f the Biracials w hose bor der identity is validated (60.5%), and those
whose identity is not validated (62.5%) describe their appearance as “ambiguous
but people assume I am Black.” However, this interpretation of others regarding
these Biracials appearance is stronger for those choosing the unvalidated border
identity because they are more likely to be assumed B lack than those whose border
identity is validated by others. In other words, 34.2% of those choosing the
validated border identity do not appear Black, whereas only 15.6% of the
unvalidated borders’ a ppearan ces do. T he interpreta tions of Bira cial peoples’
appearance by others place serious para meters on the ir racial self-understandings.
The question remains, whether the appearance–identity link is solely a function of
the contextual experienc es of race thes e individuals e xperience or if it stands on its
own. Th ese findings sup port our hyp othesis that ap pearance does affect id entity.
The results indicate there is no association between self-perceived skin color
and identity, yet there is a strong association between socially-mediated appearance
and identity. H3 stated that appearances mediate the link between skin color and
identity—that skin color influences othe rs’ interpretation s of appea rance and this
ultimately influences the choice of identity. If this is true, we would e xpect skin
color to be assoc iated with app earance. T able 3 pre sents the results that explore this
idea. We find that there is a very strong association, within this sample, between
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
43
skin color and they way these individuals u nderstand their appea rance, c 2(6, N =
169) = 34.50, p = .000. Among those who appear White and “could pass,” 64.7%
of them have ligh t skin—the percentag e of respon dents with light skin c ontinuously
decreases as we move across the table from ambiguous to appearing Black. Mo st
(82.2%) of those who appear ambiguous and are no t assumed to be Black have light
to medium skin. Of those who say they “appear Black” and are assumed Black by
others, most (65.5%) perceive their skin color as dark. It does appear that skin color
is associated with appea rance. W hat is most intere sting is that this association is not
perfect. In other words, our respondent’s self-reported skin color does not
completely determine their previews or ho w they perceive the reviews of others.
What is less clear is whether these three primary associations of interest: skin color
and identity, appe arance and identity, and skin color and appearance, are affected
by contextual experie nces of race. Is the relationship between appearance and
identity the same for Biracials from predominantly Black preadult contexts? Is the
relationship between skin color and identity always insignificant, or does it become
important if the Biracial experiences negative treatment from Blacks, Whites, or
both? The se are but two questions exp lored in the following two sections.
Table 3
Relationship Between Phenotype and Appearance Among Black and White Biracials
Phenotype
Appear
White:
Could Pass
Ambiguous:
Not Assumed
Black
Ambiguous:
Assumed
Black
Appears
Black:
Assumed
Black
%
n
%
n
%
n
%
n
Light skina
64.7
11
42.9
12
32.6
31
6.9
2
Medium skinb
29.4
5
39.3
11
45.3
43
27.6
19
5.9
1
17.9
5
22.1
21
65.5
19
10.1
17
16.6
28
56.2
95
17.2
29
c
Dark skin
Totals
Note. Percentages refer to those who have the appearance indicated and have that
type of skin color. c 2 (6,N=169) = 34.50, p = .000.
a
33.1%, n = 56. b 39.6%, n = 67. c 27.2%, n = 46.
44
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
The Role of Phenotype and Appearance in Racial Identification
Under Differing Contextual Experiences of Race
An individual’s social context seems to be a crucial component in the relation
between phenotyp e and soc ially perceived appeara nce as well as in how these two
factors influence iden tity. Two important ways that these processes are
parameterized are due to the influence of the racial composition of significant social
networks throughou t Biracials’ soc ialization— particularly in the pread ult
years—and their experiences of negative treatment from Blacks, Whites, or both.
Is it possible that context influences Biracials’ very perceptions of skin color
and appearance? Our results (not shown here) indicate that there is an association
between Biracials perceptio ns of their own skin color and the racial composition of
their preadult s ocialization c ontexts, P 2(4, N = 177) = 10.38, p = .XXX . Those
Biracials who report having darker skin color came from contexts with more
Whites, whereas those who perceive the ir skin color as lig hter, came fro m pread ult
contexts of predominantly Blacks. This indicates that self-percep tion of one’s sk in
color is affected by the range of skin tones one is surrounded by in their social
networks. In addition, before lookin g at the associations of interest and how the
racial composition of contexts affects th ose, it is interesting to n ote that there is also
an association between the racial comp osition of these Biracials’ pr eadult con texts
and their socially mediated appeara nce, P 2(6, N = 169) = 19.17, p = .XXX. Three
findings are most in teresting in this regard: B iracials who sa y they appea r White
and could pass came from predominantly Black contexts, those who are ambiguous
and not assumed Black by others were more likely to come from predominantly
White contexts, and those who report that they look Black and are assumed to be
Black by others came from p redominantly Black so cial networks. These findings
should be kept in mind as we look at the influence of context on the associations of
interest, and they point to the fact that supports our expansion of Stone’s (1962)
model: Percep tions and self-un derstandin gs are heavily influenced by context and
interactional experiences.
The Role of Early Contextual Experiences of Race: Racial
Composition of Pre-Adult Social Networks
Biracial individuals exist in a variety of social contexts an d are socia lized by a
myriad of agents—some who are Black and some who are White (and some who
are Biracial them selves). In this section we report on how these different contextual
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
45
experiences of race influenc e the primar y processe s of interest in this study. Tab le
4 reports the tests of association (chi-square) for each of the processes under
investigation broken d own by the ra cial composition of these respo ndents’ pre-a dult
contexts.
To reiterate, for the entire sample of B iracial individu als, regardless of context,
there was no association between skin color and identity (see Table 1). This relation
remains insignificant for both predominantly White contexts and contexts that had
a mixture of Blacks and Whites. However, there is a significant relation between
skin color and racial identity among Biracials who grew up and interacte d in
predom inantly Black contexts. On closer look at the data, it appears tha t those with
darker skin are more likely to choose a singular Black identity (65%), whereas
61.3% of those with lighter skin and 57.9% of those with medium sk in are more
likely to adopt the unvalidated Border identity. This means that dark-skinned
Biracial individuals who grew up in predominantly Black contexts developed a
singular Black identity more than any other identity. Furthermore, those Biracial
individuals whose skin ra nged from light to medium and who w ere socialize d in
primarily Black co ntexts are more likely to say that they understand themselves as
Biracial, but they experience the world as a Black person. The fact that the
relationship between sk in color and identity is salient within individuals who grew
up in predominately Black preadult contexts provides further evidence that the
Black community sees shades of color whereas Whites see only two colors: Black
and White (see Russell et al., 1992).
The appearance–identity association was significant for the entire sample of
Biracials (see Table 2). If we look at the second row of results—those for the
association between appearance and identity—we see that, overall, the association
is significant regardless of the racial composition of preadult contexts of these
Biracials. Thus, it appears that the racial co mposition of Biracials’ e arly
interactional contexts does not mediate the relationship between appearance and
identity choice. However, our hypothesis was that context will moderate this
relationship — meaning that the relation will be somewhat stronger or weaker
depending on the context. This is in fact the case. T he appe arance–id entity link is
strongest in mixed W hite and B lack pread ult contexts, P 2(X, N = XXX) = 49.70,
p = .XXX. This seems to indicate that these contexts are more complex and more
full of a variety of pr eviews and reviews that nee d to be taken into consideration.
A closer look at the data indicate that in these mixed contexts: (a) those who
“appear White and could pass” developed a validated border identity (80%), (b)
many of those who are “ambiguous and assumed Black” developed an unvalidated
border identity (40.4%), and (c) most of those who “appear Black and are assumed
Black” develop ed a singular B lack identity (68.8%). The appearance– identity link
is weakest in predom inantly Black preadult co ntexts, P 2(X, N = XX) = 25.38 , p =
46
TABLE 4
Relationships Between Phenotype, Appearance, and Racial Identity by Racial Composition of Preadult Contexts
P
Skin Color x Identity
Appearance x Identity
*
p < .05. * * p < .01. * * * p < .001.
2
7.55
30.51**
Predominantly Black
Preadult Contexts
Mixed White and Black
Preadult Contexts
Predominately White
Preadult Contexts
Relationship/Association
df
n
P
10
56
15
55
2
df
n
12.44
10
72
49.70***
15
72
P
2
df
n
22.15*
10
70
25.38*
15
68
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
47
.XXX, primarily because the bord ers’ experiences and self-understand ings are less
likely to be validated in predom inantly Black social netwo rks (see Rus sell et al.,
1992) .
Finally, we demonstrated how the association between skin color and
appearance for the entire sample was significant (see Table 3). The relationship
between skin color an d appea rance app ears heavily m oderated by social co ntext.
Looking at the Row 3 of Table 4, we see that skin colo r is not associate d with
appearance in predominantly White contexts and the most strongly associated with
appearance in predominantly Black contexts. Again, it appears that the Black
commu nity can, and d oes, distinguish among the variation in skin tones among
Biracials, whereas, possibly the White community sees only two “colors”—Black
and White.
The Role of Push Factors in Identity: The Experience of Negative
Treatment from Blacks and Whites
Most of the Biracials have experienced negative treatment from both Blacks and
Whites. We wanted to investigate whether this particular contextual experience of
race moderates the three associations of interest. Table 5 summarizes the findings
regarding this issue. Experiencing a push factor of this kind from Blacks might lead
a Biracial of darker skin not to adopt a Black identity. On the other hand, a light
skinned Biracial who experiences negative treatment from W hites may cho ose to
adopt a Black identity. We also look at that group of Biracials who report
experiencing negative treatmen t from both groups.
TABLE 5
Relationship Between Phenotype, Appearance and Identity by Experiences of
Discrimination of Blacks and Whites
Experienced
Discrimination
from Blacks
Relationship/
Association
P
*
2
df
n
Experienced
Discrimination
from Whites
P
2
df
n
Experienced
Discrimination from
Whites and Blacks
P
2
df
n
Skin Color x
Identity
24.13**
10
121
23.57**
10
152
19.13*
10
105
Appearance
x Identity
34.73**
15
119
54.84***
15
149
31.75**
15
103
Skin Color x
Appearance
33.92***
6
125
51.05***
6
159
28.02***
6
109
p < .05. * * p < .01. * * * p < .001.
48
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
The association between skin color and identity for o ur sample of Biracials d id
not exist in previous analyses (see Table 1 ); however, this relationship does exist
for Biracials who have exp erienced n egative treatm ent from B lacks, P 2(X, N =
XXX) = 25.13 , p = .XXX; from W hites, P 2(X, N = XX X) = 23 .57, p = XXX; and
from both groups, P 2(X, N = XXX ) = 19.13 , p = XXX. A closer look at the data,
illustrates the push factors. Over thirty percent (31.4%) of dark-skinned Biracial
individuals who have experienced negative treatment from Blacks adopt a singular
Black identity, compared to 42.6% of similar Biracials who have experienced such
treatment from W hites. This kind of contextua l experienc e of race do es seem to
modera te the appearance– identity association in our data— because the association
is still significant (see Table 2), but stronger in the case of Biracials who have
experienced negative treatment from W hites. The same app lies to skin color and
appearance.
Although the extent to which social context influence racial identity formation
of Biracial respondents remains somewhat complicated, the fact that social context
does indeed influence racial identity formation is undeniable.
Discussion
When focusing on racial identity, the use of appearances as signifiers of group
membe rship is not always clear cut. T his relationship between appearances and
individual identity choice becomes even more complex when we examine Biracial
identities. We have presented the various racial self-understandings of a sample of
Biracial people to explore how the appearance–identity link may function. It is clear
that the basic processes of mutual identification and concomitant awareness help us
to better understand two ou tcomes. The first is how people with the same parental
background (one Black and one White parent) can make very different choices
about their identities. The second is to try to understand why one’s appearance does
not always predict that outcome—or why we find pe ople who are physically White
who identify as Black and those who are physically Black who say they are not
Black, but Biracial. Trying to interpret these cases becomes clearer if we focus on
the basic pro cesses that take place betw een individu als and within the individual (in
the proce ss of “self-ing “).
The first process, mutual identification, is critica l to both iden tity construction
and maintenance. If the individual actor (regardless of bodily char acteristics) exists
within a social context where Biracial has a meaningful existence, he or she may
cultivate a border identity. If this cultural category does not exist and one becomes
accustomed to and ade pt at switching from Black to White, they will cultivate a
protean identity. If their app earance is W hite, they may develop a transcendent
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
49
identity, but only if their social context does not demand categorization. If none of
these options are available to an individual, than the existing cultural no rms dictate
the racial identity above and beyond (and at times despite) their appearance.
The second process at work allows us to see the self-functioning as subject. How
does the self reflect on o ne’s own ide ntity-in-context? In some cases (the protean
identity), we saw the self as not only having the capacity for co ncomitant awareness,
but also utilizing that capacity in all interactions as a survival tool. The individual
was able to assess the anticipated interpretatio n from the app roaching o ther, to
modify the program accordingly, and to continually monitor for necessary
renegotiations or reformulations of the program. This intense activity requires an
ongoin g subjective awareness of the self’s activities and much bicultural (or
identity) capital (see Côté, 1996). We see this same process co ntinually at work in
the other ide ntity options, ho wever, it takes o n a slightly variant for m of activity.
A new body of literature focuses on the sp ace between the prev ious two
processes of mutual identification (a fundamentally social process) and concomitant
awareness (a process rooted in contextualized psychological phenomena) by
acknowledging that identity is a dynamic journey not a stagnant dest ination. Our
data indeed point in this direction as well. This growing collection of studies
highlight that identity constru ction and m aintenance in volves strateg ies (e.g.,
Berzo nsky, 1999c; Blanz, 1998), negotiations (e.g., Gilliam & Gilliam, 1999;
Swann, 1987), and tactics. Indee d, these authors approa ch this new area from
several directions. Some focus on the ways that individuals “do identity” on an
interaction by interaction basis through processe s of “identity work” (Storrs, 1999)
and uses of “identity capital” within a variety of salient “identity markets” (Côté,
1996, 1997). Other investigators concentrate on social-cognitive models of identity
formation where individuals strategize and make designed choic es as to whether
they will engage in o r avoid alto gether the co nstruction or r evision of their se lfidentities (see Berzonsky, 1989, 1992, 1994, 1999a, 1999b). Research on Biracial
and multiracial identity would also do well to understand the existence o f multiple
identities within one individual, the functions each plays, the contextual enactment
of these identitie s, and how th ey change o ver time (D eaux, 199 3; Deaux et al.,
1995; Ethier & Deaux 199 4). These emerging theoretical approaches to the study
of identity avoid a n over-soc ialized view o f the self and reflect the level of
complex ity illustrated by data such as ours. All of these theoretical schemas may
prove beneficial in our understanding of the appearance-identity link in particular
and the formation and maintenance of identity in general. Those researching
Biracial and multirac ial individuals w ould do well to ackno wledge th ese theories
because they cross disciplinary lines and p aradigms.
It remains to be seen how much negotiation and strategy is involved in racial
identity formation and maintenance among Black and W hite Biracials; however, it
is still the case that racial identities are subject to a degree of constraint that ethnic
identities are not. Specifically, racial identities are constrained by historical
stratification that is directly tied to bodily characteristics. It is these characteristics
50
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
that linger in our cultural symbols and traditional ways of thinking and being. The
existence, experience and voice of Biracial individuals is challenging the current
categorization of race, altering the meaning of racial identity, and, in the process,
altering the cultural toolkit (Swidler, 1986) of race in the United States. For
individuals who are caught in between existing cultural categories at this time of
uneven but emerging changes in those categories, appearances remain a significant
constraining factor in identity construction and maintenance.
REFERENCES
Alba, R. (1990). Ethnic identity: The transformation of White America. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Berzonsk y, M. (1989). The self as theorist: Individual differences in identity formation. International
Journal of Personal Construct Psychology, 2, 363–376.
Berzonsky, M. (1992). Identity style and coping strategies. Journal of Personality, 60, 771–788.
Berzonsk y, M. (1994). Individual differences in self-construction: The role of constructivist
epistemological assumptions. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 7, 263–281.
Berzonsk y, M. (1999a). Identity processing style and cognitive attributional strategies: Similarities and
difference across different contexts. European Journal of Personality, 13, 105–120.
Berzonsk y, M. (1999b). Identity styles and hypothesis-testing strategies. Journal of Social Psychology,
139, 784–789.
Berzonsk y, M. (1999c). Reevaluating the identity status paradigm: Still useful after 35 years.
Development Review, 19, 557–590.
Blanz, M., et al. (1998). Responding to negative socialidentity: A taxonomy of identity management
strategies.European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 697–729.
Bonilla-Silva, E. (1997). Rethinking racism: Toward a structural interpretation. American Sociological
Review, 62, 465–480.
Bowles, D. D. (1993). Bi-racial identity: Children born to African-American and White couples.
Clinical Social Work Journal, 21, 417–428.
Boyd-Franklin, N. (1989). Black families in therapy: A multisystems approach. New York: Guilford.
Brown, P. M. (1990). Biracial identity and social marginality. Child and Adolescent Social Work
Journal, 7, 319–337.
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribner’s.
Côté, J. E. (1996) . Sociological p erspectives on id entity formatio n: The culture-iden tity link and
identity capital. Journal of Adolescence, 19, 417–428.
Côté, J. E. (1997). An empirical test of the identit y capital model. Journal of Adolescence, 20,
577–597.
Cross, W. E. (1971). The Negro-to-Black conversion experience: Toward a psychology of Black
liberation. Black World, 20, 13–27.
Daniel, R. (1996). Black and W hite iden tity i n the new mi llenn ium: Unsev ering the ties that bind . In
M. Root (Ed.), The multiracial experience: Racial borders as the new frontier. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Davis, F. J. (1991). Who is Black? One nation’s definition. CITY: Pennsylvania State University Press.
Deaux, K. (1993). Reconstructing social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
4–12.
SKIN COLOR AND BIRACIAL IDENTITY
51
Deaux, K., et al. (1995). Parameters of social identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
68, 280–291.
Demo, D., & Hughes, M. (1990). Socialization an d racial identity among Black Americans. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 53, 364–374.
Ethier, K., & Deaux, K. (1994). Negotiating social identity when contexts change: Maintaining
identification and responding to threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
243–251.
Field, L. D. (1996). Piecing together the puzzle: Self-concept and group identity in biracialBlack/White
youth. In M. Root (Ed.), The multiracial experience: Racial borders as the new frontier. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Gans, H. J. (197 9). Symbo lic ethn icity: The future of ethnic groups and cultures in America. Ethnic and
Racial Studies, 2, 1–20.
Gergen, K. J. (1991). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in contemporary life. New York: Basic.
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Gilliam, A., & Gilliam, O. (1999). Odyssey: Negotiating the subjectivity of Mulatta i dentity in Brazil.
Latin American Perspectives, 26, 60–84.
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anc hor Doub leday.
Hall, C. (1980). The ethnic identity of racially mixed people: A study of Black-Japanese. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.
Harvey, A. R. (1995, January). The issue of skin color in psychotherapy with African Americans.
Families in Society: The Journal of Contemporary Human Services, 3–10.
Herring, R. D. (19 95). Devel oping bi racial et hnic id entity: A review of the increasing dilemma. Journal
of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 23, 29–38
Hughes, M., & Hertel, B. R. (1990). The significance of color remains: A study of life chances, mate
selection, and ethnic consciousness among Black Americans. Social Forces, 68, 1105–1120.
Ichheiser, G. (1970). Appearances and realities: Misunderstanding in human relations. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Ignatiev, N. (1995). How the Irish became white. New York: Routledge.
Johnson, D. J. (1992). Developmental pathways: Toward an ecological theoretical formulation of race
identity in Black–White Biracial children . In M. Root (Ed.), Racially mixed people in America.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Kerwin, C., Ponterotto, J. G., Jackson, B. L., & Harris, A. (1993). Racial identity in Biracial children:
A qualitative investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 221–231.
Kich, G. K. (1992). The developmental process of asserting a Biracial, Bicultural identity. In M. Root
(Ed.), Racially mixed people in America. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Lifton, R. (1993). The protean self: Human resilience in an age of fragmentation. New York: Basic.
Loveman, M. (1999). Is “race” essential?: Comment on Bonilla-Silva 1997. American Sociological
Review, 64, 891–898.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Miller, R., & Miller, B. (1990). Mothering the biracial child: Bridging the gaps between AfricanAmerican and White parenting styles. Women and Therapy, 10, 169–180.
Morten, G., & Atkinson, D. (1983). Minority identity development and preference for counselor race.
Journal of Negro Education, 52, 156–161.
Neal, A. M., & Wilson, M. L. (1989). The role of skin color and feat ures in t he Black communi ty:
Implications for Black women and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 323–333.
Porter, C. P. (1991). Social reasons for skin tone preferences of Black school-aged children. American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 61, 149–154.
Porter, J., & Washington, R. (1993). Minority identity and self-esteem. Annual Review of Sociology,
19, 139–161.
52
BRUNSMA AND ROCKQUEMORE
Porterfield, E. (1978). Black and White marriages: An ethnographic study of Black–White families.
Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Poston, W. C. (1990). The Biracial identity development model: A needed addition. Journal of
Counseling and Development, 69, 152–155.
Poussaint, A. (1984). Study of interracial children presents positive picture. Interracial Books for
Children, 15, 9–10.
Rockquemore, K. A. (1999). Between Black and White: Understanding the “Biracial” experience. Race
and Society, 2, 197–212.
Root, M. (1990). Resolving “other” status: Identity development of biracial individuals. Women and
Therapy, 9, 185–205.
Root, M. (Ed.). 1992. Racially mixed people in America. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Root, M. (Ed.). 1996. The multiracial experience: Racial borders as the new frontier. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Russel, K., Wilson, M., & Hall, R. (1992). The color complex: The politics of skin color among African
Americans. New York: Anchor.
Spickard, P. R. (1989). Mixed blood: Intermarriage and ethnic identity in twentieth-century America.
Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Stone, G. (1962). Appearance and the self. In A. M. Rose (Ed.), Human behavior and social processes.
Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Storrs, D. (1999). Whiteness as stigma: Essentialist identity work by mixed-race women. Symbolic
Interaction, 22, 187–212.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 53, 1038–1051.
Swidler, A. (1986). Culture in action: Symbols and strategies. American Sociological Review, 51,
273–286.
Taylor-Gibbs, J. (1997). Biracial adolescents. In J. T. Gibbs & L.-N. Huang (Eds.), Children of color:
Psychological interventions with culturally diverse youth. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Tizard, B., & Phoenix, A. (1995). The identity of mixed parentage adolescents. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1399–1410.
Turkle, S. (1997). Life on the screen: Identity in the age of the internet. New York: Touchston e.
Wardle, F. (1987). Are we sensi tive to interrac ial children’s special ident ity needs? Young Children,
43, 53–59.
Wardle, F. (1992). Biracial identity: An ecological and developmental model. CITY/STATE: Center
for the Study of Biracial Children.
Waters, M. (1990). Ethnic options: Choosing identities in America. Berkeley: University of California
Press.