Five Minutes After Death:
A Study of Beliefs and Expectations
Allan Kellehear, Ph.D.
La Trobe University
Harvey Irwin, Ph.D.
University of New England
ABSTRACT"
This paper examines the beliefs and expectations that a sample
of 508 people hold about the first five minutes after death. A substantial
minority believed that they wilt experience the main elements of the neardeath experience (NDE). In general these elements were cited more frequently
t h a n were Biblical images. Six percent of the sample said that postmortem
survival for them will be a negative and disturbing experience. We discuss
these results in terms of their methodological implications for other survey
work and their theoretical contribution toward our understanding of negative
NDEs.
What, if anything, do people believe about experiences associated
with the first five minutes after their own death? This paper examines
the nature of popular beliefs and expectations about the initial moments of postmortem existence. Studying beliefs about personal survival of death is relevant to near-death research for two reasons.
First, these beliefs may reflect aspects of socialization and social
conditioning, which in turn may influence to some extent the content
and nature of the near-death experience (NDE). For example, Calvin
Schorer (1985-86) noted of native American NDE accounts that the
panoramic life review is absent but traditional images such as the
Dr. Kellehear is Lecturer in Sociology at La Trobe University, and Dr. Irwin is
Associate Professor of Psychology at the University of New England. Reprint requests
should be addressed to Dr. Kellehear at the Department of Sociology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, 3083, Australia.
Journal of Near-Death Studies, 9(2) Winter 1990
9 1990 H u m a n Sciences Press, Inc.
77
78
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATHSTUDIES
flathead snake, the "war eagle," deer, moose, and bows and arrows are
encountered. Dorothy Counts (1983) described an event within a
Melanesian NDE in which a deceased acquaintance cut the leg of the
NDEr and spat ginger on it. Counts noted that "spitting ginger on a
wound is a traditional healing p r a c t i c e . . , thought to have great healing power" (1983, p. 119). Satwant Pasricha and Ian Stevenson (1986)
noted several culture-specific features of their Indian accounts, including being "sent back" because of mistaken identity and the reading of
the record of a person's life. However, they cautioned against the
reductionist implication that these variations are purely a function of
prior beliefs and socialization.
We should remember, however, that if we survive death and live in an
afterlife realm, we should expect to find variations in that world, just
as we find them in the different parts of the familiar world of the
living. A traveller to Delhi encounters dark skinned immigration
officials, who in many respects behave differently from the lighter
skinned immigration officials another traveller may meet when arriving in London or New York. Yet we do not say that the descriptions
of the first traveller are "real" and those of the second "unreal."
(Pasricha & Stevenson, 1986, p. 169)
This caution notwithstanding, the observed cross-cultural variations
in NDE content do prompt further empirical scrutiny of the possibility
that the phenomenology of the experience is inspired to some degree by
processes of social conditioning.
Another reason for exploring beliefs with respect to the NDE is that
even if the relationship between social beliefs and the content of an
NDE is causally ambiguous, social beliefs will nonetheless be important to the interpretation of the NDE by NDErs and their social
network. For example, NDErs frequently depict elements of their NDE
in terms of some aspect of their religious belief. Kenneth Ring (1980)
reported cases where "the light" was described as a %ision of Jesus." Of
course, it is not necessarily the characteristics of the phenomenon that
are important for this identification but rather the NDErs' interpretation of what they believe they experience. Furthermore, not all NDErs
will necessarily interpret their experience as a brief glimpse of the
afterlife. The English philosopher Sir A.J. Ayers, in his much publicized account (1988) of his own NDE was happy to view that experience as a side effect of brain activity secondary to his cardiac arrest.
Although in his view survival is a philosophic question worth entertaining, Ayers wrote that persistence of brain activity is by far the
"most probable explanation." This is a similar interpretation to that
ALLANKELLEHEARAND HARVEYIRWIN
79
taken by some respondents in a Chinese survey (Kellehear, Heaven &
Gao 1990). Of the 26 respondents who claimed to have had an experience similar to the NDE described in that survey, only 6 chose to
construe it as evidence of life after death. It is within the context of
these issues and possibilities that the present study is set.
Background to the Study
In 1987 Harvey Irwin examined stereotypical images of heaven and
compared these with the common images and experiences reported in
NDEs. Past surveys had given little attention to beliefs about the
appearance of the afterlife environment and that confined the area to
speculation. In the Irwin survey questionnaires were distributed to 96
university students. They were asked to take an imaginary tour
through heaven and check off any statements on the survey form that
fairly represented their impressions. The survey covered four areas:
the physical appearance of heaven, beings that might be encountered,
the major mode of travel, and the sounds one might hear in that
environment.
There were several features in that survey that limit any subsequent
comparison with its findings and the NDE. First, a study of the beliefs
of the afterlife might not necessarily indicate beliefs about the first
five minutes of after-death experience. Indeed, theologian John Hick
(1976) rightly identified eschatology as the doctrine of last things or of
the ultimate state to which we are destined. He used the term pareschatology to designate the doctrine of next-to-last things or the
'r
future between the present life and Man's ultimate state"
(Hick 1976, p. 22). It is pareschatological beliefs about the very next
stage of existence beyond the present that we sought to examine here.
This is important because people who believe in heaven may have a
poorly developed idea of the experiential steps between this life and
paradise. Conversely, people with beliefs about the experience of death
itself may have poorly developed ideas about their ultimate destination and resting place in the '~next world."
Second, Irwin's study employed a checklist of items developed from a
pilot of 16 people. Checklists have the advantage of prompting poorly
motivated respondents but they also restrict the potential diversity of
images and experiences people may draw upon. In addition checklists,
including those with token provision for answers not covered by the
list, direct and channel respondents' thinking along set lines.
Finally, Irwin's study did not survey the emotional dimension.
80
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATH STUDIES
Sights, sounds, encounters, and travel were explored in the imaginary
trip through heaven, but feelings about being in heaven were not
addressed. This is an important omission since the NDE is typically
characterized by powerful positive or negative emotion.
The aims of this study, therefore, were to solicit beliefs and expectations about the first five minutes after death; to conduct this study by
using a semistructured instrument; and to document and examine
people's beliefs and expectations about their own emotional reactions
to this time.
The research was divided into two parts, which, for reasons of clarity
and organizationl are called Study 1 and Study 2. Study 1 documents
beliefs about postmortem existence insofar as respondents in this
study held them. Study 2 explores expectations about personal survival regardless of the beliefs held by those in this sample. The rationale behind Study 2 is the recognition that experiences might be
shaped not only by beliefs but also knowledge. In this respect, NDEs
also occur among those who do not believe in postmortem survival of
any kind. Directing respondents to construct images of the afterlife,
despite their personal beliefs, should tap this source of expectations.
Study 1
Sample
Two groups were surveyed for their beliefs about the first five minutes after their own deaths. The first group comprised 262 first year
undergraduate students at an Australian university. There were 41
males, 219 females and two whose gender was not specified. The mean
age was 21 and the majority of these students were studying social
science (101) or nursing (137).
After the student survey, volunteers were requested to distribute
questionnaires among the general population. This was the second
group for the study (N = 72). Student volunteer survey assistants were
asked to sample adults from the local community and to select from as
broad a range as possible in terms of age and socioeconomic status. The
sample was evenly divided between males (35) and females (35), with
two whose gender was not specified. The mean age for this sample was
38. One respondent had achieved primary education level, 30 had
achieved secondary level, 19 obtained secondary and further technical
training, 17 had some level of university or college education, and 5 did
ALLAN KELLEHEARAND HARVEY IRWIN
81
not specify educational level. The people in this sample came from a
wide variety of occupations, ranging in occupational prestige scores
from 2.4 to 6.6 with a possible range between 1.2 to 6.9 using Ann
Daniel's (1983) occupational prestige scale.
Questionnaire
Respondents were asked three questions. The first question was fixed
choice and the others were open ended. Question 1 enquired about
the respondent's belief in survival in the first five minutes after biological death. Response options were ~yes," ~'no," or '~unsure." ]f respondents answered ~yes" to this question they were directed to answer
question 2.
Question 2 was set in the context of their belief in surviving the first
five minutes after death. Respondents were asked to complete four
sentences. These were: "Within these very first moments, I believe I
will see . . . . "; "I believe I will meet . . . . "; "I believe I will hear . . . . ";
"Emotionally, I will feel . . . . "
Regardless of their answer to question 1, all respondents were asked
to answer question 3, which asked t h e m to identify the social influences they believed were important to the kind of answer they gave in
question 1. Students were also asked their sex, age, and field of study.
The community group were asked their sex, age, education, and
occupation.
Results
The distribution of responses to question 1, on the belief of postmortem existence, is presented in Table 1. The two samples differ in
their distribution here (Xz = 13.96, df = 2; p < .001). There is a trend
Table 1
Belief about surviving the first five minutes after d e a t h - S t u d y 1
will survive
will not survive
unsure
Students
(N = 262)
Community
(N = 72)
104 (40%)
34 (13%)
124 (47%)
33 (46%)
20 (28%)
19 (26%)
82
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATHSTUDIES
for a substantial proportion of the student group, who are younger
than the community group, to say they were "unsure" about postmortem experience. Perhaps as people grow older they take a more
definite stance on this issue.
The believers, who responded ~yes" to question I in the two samples
(104 students and 33 members of the community sample), were asked
what they believed they would see, meet, hear, and feel in the first five
minutes of their postmortem existence. "Don't know, no answer" responses for the student sample ranged from 12% for one category to
31% for two others. The response of "don't know" was somewhat lower
in the general sample, ranging from 3% for one category to 27% for
another. The full range of generic responses is summarized in Table 2.
The similarity between these responses and the usual elements of the
NDE is notable. What believers declare they will see, meet, hear, and
feel in the first five minutes after death tends to correspond closely to
the typical NDE.
Believers (137) and nonbelievers (54) were asked to identify the
likely sources of their beliefs. Their answers are summarized in Table
3. Although relatively few believers gave Biblical or heavenly depictions of the postmortem experience, a substantial proportion of believers cited the Bible and various forms of religious instruction as the
alleged source of their beliefs. The sources of belief in the existence of
an afterlife evidently are in some degree distinct from the sources of
belief in the nature of the afterlife. Accounts of NDEs nevertheless
represent another significant declared source of believers' understanding of postmortem experience. This is not surprising given that
an estimated 72-79% of Australians are familiar with the NDE
(Kellehear & H e a v e n 1989).
In both samples there is some tendency for nonbelievers to portray
themselves as independent thinkers, claiming to form their attitude to
an afterlife in terms of their own observations and logic rather than on
the basis of other people's suggestions and influence.
Study 2
Sample
A second group of first-year undergraduate Australian university
students was asked to complete a questionnaire concerning their expectations about postmortem existence, irrespective of their beliefs in
ALLAN KELLEHEARAND HARVEYIRWIN
83
Table 2
Incidence of reported beliefs about
postmortem experience-Studies 1 and 2
Study 1
Study 2
Students Community
Students
(N = 104)
(N = 33) (N = 174)
I will see:
the immediate setting of death
lights
Biblical scene, heaven, etc.
life review
pastoral scene, paradise
tunnel
darkness
don't know, no answer
27
23
10
9
8
6
6
22
(26%)
(22%)
(10%)
(9%)
(8%)
(6%)
(6%)
(21%)
I will meet:
deceased relatives, friends
God
unrecognized deceased people
angels
Jesus
deceased pets
don't know, no answer
35
21
20
6
5
3
32
(34%)
(20%)
(19%)
(6%)
(5%)
(3%)
(31%)
I will hear:
voices talking, singing
music
silence, no hearing
sounds of nature
don't know, no answer
25
17
14
4
32
(24%)
(16%)
(13%)
(4%)
(31%)
7 (21%)
3(9%)
8 (24%)
1 (3%)
3(9%)
I will feel:
calmness, contentment, peace
happiness, joy, love, relief
fear, anxiety, awe
sadness, anger
don't know, no answer
50
44
11
6
12
(48%)
(42%)
(11%)
(6%)
(12%)
22 (67%)
14 (42%)
1(3%)
2 (6%)
1(3%)
8 (24%)
8 (24%)
0 (0%)
1 (3%)
3(9%)
3 ( 9%)
0 ( 0%)
3 ( 9%)
9
13
4
0
0
0
9
(27%)
(39%)
(12%)
(0%)
(0%)
(0%)
(27%)
44 (25%)
44 (25%)
17 (10%)
17 (10%)
12(7%)
17 (10%)
17 (10%)
19 (11%)
45 (26%)
35 (20%)
49 (28%)
9 (5%)
5 (3%)
1(0.5%)
44 (25%)
28
30
47
7
30
(16%)
(17%)
(27%)
(4%)
(17%)
49 (28%)
59 (34%)
31 (18%)
17 (10%)
12(7%)
84
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATHSTUDIES
Table 3
Incidence of declared sources of belief-Study
Students
1
Community
Believers
( N = 104)
Nonbelievers
( N = 34)
Believers
( N = 33)
Nonbelievers
( N -- 20)
35 (34%)
2 (6%)
17 (52%)
2 (10%)
24 (23%)
1 (3%)
8 (24%)
1 (5%)
fictional
books,
movies
15 (14%)
5 (15%)
i (3%)
1 (5%)
nonfiction
books
family
15 (14%)
22 (21%)
6 (18%)
9 (26%)
5 (15%)
6 (18%)
1 (5%)
0 (0%)
friends,
groups
16 (15%)
5 (15%)
6 (18%)
1 (5%)
another's
death
5 (5%)
2 (6%)
2 (6%)
1 (5%)
own ideas,
logic
16 (15%)
8 (24%)
3 (9%)
10 (50%)
don't know,
no answer
20 (19%)
4 (12%)
3 (9%)
4 (20%)
Bible,
religious
education
NDE
accounts
such a possibility. Questionnaires were completed by 174 students.
This brings the total number of people participating in the project to
508, with 436 of these being students. Although the majority of the
respondents were students, the results nonetheless are relevant to a
general study of beliefs and their relationship to the NDE. Mary
Reardon Castles and Ruth Beckmann Murray (1979) argued that random selection has little meaning in the field of death and dying. The
important issue is to identify clearly the population being sampled. No
population should be less interesting or important than another where
questions of death and dying are concerned. For instance, since there is
no typical social profile of the NDEr, all populations should be equally
relevant to study.
ALLAN KELLEHEAR AND HARVEY IRWIN
85
There were 57 males and 113 females who participated in Study 2,
and 4 persons whose gender was not specified. The majority of these
students were studying social science (148). The mean age was 20.
Questionnaire
The format and design of the survey instrument was identical to that
described in Study 1 except for one detail. After question I concerning
belief in postmortem existence, all respondents were asked to answer
question 2 and 3 regardless of their beliefs. They were asked to imagine they actually do survive and to answer questions about what they
might actually experience in that eventuality.
Results
For this sample the distribution of responses is shown in Table 4.
Comparison of these data with those from the student sample in Study
i indicates a highly significant difference (X2 = 35.56 df = 2; p < .001).
There may be several explanations for this discrepancy. The two student samples may genuinely have differed; that is, rather different
student populations may have been tapped in Study I and Study 2. For
example, a unique feature of the sample in Study I was the large group
of nursing students. On the other hand, there may have been a factor
working against students' candidness in the earlier belief survey. In
Study 1, respondents who acknowledged postmortem belief then had to
give details about several aspects of that belief, whereas those who
were unsure about their belief were not required to do so. In Study 2,
on the other hand, all respondents were required to describe their
expectations for facets of postmortem experience. There may have been
a temptation for less motivated believers to claim to be unsure about
Table 4
Belief a b o u t s u r v i v i n g the first five m i n u t e s after d e a t h - S t u d y 2
Students (n = 174)
will survive
will not survive
unsure
112 (64%)
27 (16%)
35 (20%)
86
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATH STUDIES
their belief, thereby avoiding the time consuming task of answering
question 2.
In Study 2, some 25-45% of nonbelievers insisted that they did not
know what to expect of various facets of a postmortem existence. This
necessarily dilutes the incidence of specific expectations in comparison
to that of believers' views in Study 1. While the lack of response among
nonbelievers might be taken to signify an implacable determination
not to countenance the notion of an afterlife, it might also reflect some
nonbelievers' selective avoidance of, and lack of exposure to, social
input about postmortem existence. In other words, there m a y well be
good reason for their apparent ignorance of socially propagated expectations about the afterlife.
The expectations of postmortem experience within the complete sample for Study 2 are summarized in Table 2. Taking due account of many
nonbelievers' lack of contribution to the above data, the table again
documents the extent to which people's views of the first five minutes
of postmortem experience accord with the NDE.
There are no major discrepancies between the overall patterns in
these data and those of Study 1. Perhaps the emotional response of
calmness is rather lower in people's expectations than in people's
beliefs. Again, given the earlier consideration concerning the disparity
between the two student samples, the interpretation of this difference
is unclear.
The declared sources of expectations in Study 2 are summarized in
Table 5. These data suggest that whereas all categories of respondents
were able to use their religious education as a basis for generating
their expectations, NDE accounts were a source of expectations solely
for the believers. Perhaps because childhood religious education could
not generally be avoided, believers and nonbelievers alike were able to
draw on this information in conceptualizing a postmortem experience.
Accounts of NDEs, however, can be avoided to some extent. We can
decline to read the book about NDEs, we can turn off the TV documentary, and we can flip past the magazine article. Not only are nonbelievers able to avoid information on NDEs, they can selectively fail
to memorize such information to the extent that they are exposed to it.
Even if they sat through a segment of a television program on the
NDE, the program's content would be recognized as incompatible with
their beliefs and soon forgotten. In other words, because of nonbelievers' perception of the NDE as uninteresting, they genuinely may
be unable to recall this material when faced with a question about the
nature of a hypothetical afterlife.
ALLAN KELLEHEARAND HARVEYIRWIN
87
Table 5
I n c i d e n c e o f declared s o u r c e s o f e x p e c t a t i o n - S t u d y 2
Believers
( N = 112)
Bible, religious education
NDE accounts
fictional books, movies
nonfiction books
family
friends, groups
another's death
own ideas, logic
don't know, no answer
32
25
26
15
9
8
1
13
18
(29%)
(22%)
(23%)
(13%)
(8%)
(7%)
(1%)
(12%)
(16%)
Nonbelievers
( N = 27)
5
0
9
2
2
3
0
7
7
(19%)
(0%)
(33%)
(7%)
(7%)
(11%)
(0%)
(26%)
(26%)
Unsure
( N = 35)
8
1
20
1
4
5
1
4
4
(23%)
(3%)
(57%)
(3%)
(11%)
(14%)
(3%)
(11%)
(11%)
The inclination of nonbelievers to depict themselves as immune to
social influences is again evident in this sample.
Discussion
In both studies there is a substantial minority of people who believe
that they will experience elements of the NDE in the first five minutes
after death. Generally these elements are cited more frequently than
Biblical images. This is strong indication that accounts of NDEs have
made a powerful impact on the community in general and the young
student population in particular. This trend parallels those found by
Allan Kellehear and Patrick Heaven (1989) and Barbara Walker and
Robert Russell (1989) in their survey work. It cannot be concluded, of
course, that actual NDEs are inspired by the publicity given to previous cases. After all, NDEs are known to have occurred prior to the
wide and popular coverage of them. Our data nevertheless are by no
means incompatible with the hypothesis that the NDE itself could be
molded to some degree by socially propagated beliefs and expectations.
It is noteworthy also that the students who believed they will see a
Biblical or heavenly setting did not report a belief they would see the
immediate setting in which they die. These two beliefs form the basis
of two quite separate sub-groups of the sample ( V 2 = 3.84, df = 1;
p < .05; where V 2 is X2 corrected for sample size [Rhoades and Overall,
1982]). This supports the methodological importance of differentiating
88
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATHSTUDIES
between pareschatological beliefs and eschatological ones. Although
studies that focus only on beliefs about heaven or hell are useful in
exploring the imagery people may use, any such survey might also
artificially exaggerate the differences between popular belief about
postmortem existence and images of the NDE. Such observed differences might also merely reflect the fact that images of NDEs relate
specifically to a short period after death.
The belief in meeting deceased pets was uncommon but nonetheless
confined to the student sample. Possibly because the students were
young a pet may have been the only deceased entity known to some of
them. In this regard, it may be noted that pets are an (also infrequent)
element of children's rather than adults' NDEs (Irwin, 1989).
Of some interest too is the finding that a small proportion (approximately 6%) of the total sample of 508 respondents reported that they
believe they would be frightened or anxious five minutes after death.
For example, one student was unsure whether he would see God or
Satan, heaven or hell, but described only one way he might feel:
~'emotionally distressed." Another student described her first five minutes in ~'a barren, futile land" where she might meet '~another spirit or
alien." She would be ~'unnerved" and ~'disillusioned." A businessman
from the community sample described his belief about the first five
minutes after death as finding himself ~in a kaleidoscope of colour,
moving in random swirls like a mist." He would be unable to recognize
anyone and his experience would be "a confused picture, as in a
dream." There would be a "confusion of sound, voices, nothing
distinguishable-not musical." He would feel, understandably, ~insecure, disorientated." Other students in this group described their feelings as ~lost," ~afraid," ~frightened," ~confused," ~upset," and ~scared."
Two observations may be made about these responses, one methodological and the other theoretical.
Methodologically, these results show the importance of surveys of
belief to be semistructured and to explore the emotional dimension of
beliefs. George Gallup (1982), in his national U.S. survey, for example,
failed to establish that negative emotions are part of some people's
view of the afterlife. Although Gallup identified beliefs in hell, for
example, and found some respondents who believed they might be
bound for this place, a subsequent checklist did not canvass for beliefs
about the nature of hell nor feelings about this (Gallup, 1982). The 13%
of the sample who rejected all offered statements concerning their
beliefs about life after death may disguise the group who believe such a
prospect could be negative and frightening to them.
ALLAN KELLEHEAR AND HARVEY IRWIN
89
On a theoretical level, the reports of negative emotions from some of
our respondents have implications for the psychology of dying. We
cannot claim that the negative beliefs expressed by our respondents
are the fundamental cause of negative NDEs. Indeed, it remains to be
determined precisely how, if at all, the content of the NDE is socially
influenced. Nevertheless, while beliefs might well influence NDE content they most certainly do influence experiencers' interpretations of
their NDEs. In the latter respect, if people with negative emotional
beliefs about survival actually do find themselves conscious 'Tive minutes after death," their experiences might be viewed at the time or
subsequently as disturbing irrespective of their content. If NDEs are
commonly encountered by the dying, prior beliefs and expectations
could be important as to whether the experiences are interpreted in
comforting or disturbing ways. Perhaps this is part of the reason why
so many religious and cultural prescriptions for dying conduct emphasize the importance of positive attitude and equanimity in the face of
impending death.
The possible roles of social conditioning in the nature of and the
experiencer's interpretation of the NDE warrant further investigation.
There is a cogent case for such research to be undertaken in societies
that have been little exposed to the publicity over NDEs characteristic
of Western societies.
References
Ayer, A. J. (1988). Intimations of immortality: What I saw when I was dead. National
Review, No. 40, 38 40.
Castles, M. R., and Murray, R. B. (1979). Dying in an institution: Nurse-patient perspectives. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Counts, D. A. (1983). Near-death and out-of-body experiences in a Melanesian society.
Anabiosis: The Journal of Near-Death Studies, 3, 115-135.
Daniel, A. (1983). Power, privilege and prestige: Occupations in Australia. Melbourne,
Australia: Longman Cheshire.
Gallup, G., Jr. (1982). Adventures in immortality: A look beyond the threshold of death
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Hick, J. H. (1976). Death and eternal life. San Francisco, CA: Harper and Row.
Irwin, H. J. (1987). Images of Heaven. Parapsychology Review, 18(1), 1-4.
Irwin, H. J. (I989). The near-death experience in child.hood. Australian Parapsychological Review, 14, 7-11.
Kellehear, A., and Heaven, P. (1989). Community attitudes toward near-death experiences: An Australian study. Journal of Near-Death Studies, 7, 165-172.
Kellehear, A., Heaven, P., and Gao, J. (1990). Community attitudes toward near-death
experiences: A Chinese study. Journal of Near-Death Studies, 8, 163-173.
Pasricha, S., and Stevenson, L (1986). Near-death experiences in India: A preliminary
report. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 165-170.
90
JOURNAL OF NEAR-DEATH STUDIES
Rhoades, H. M., and Overall, J. E. (1982). A sample size correction for Pearson chi-square
in 2• contingency tables. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 418-423.
Ring, K. (1980). Life at death: A scientific investigation of the near-death experience. New
York, NY: Coward, McCann and Geoghegan.
Schorer, C. E. (1985-86). Two native American near-death experiences. Omega, 16, 111113.
Walker, B. A., and Russell, R. D. (1989). Assessing psychologists' knowledge and attitudes toward near-death phenomena. Journal of Near-Death Studies, 8, 103-110.