Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice - Special Issue • Autumn • 3199-3209
©
2012 Educational Consultancy and Research Center
www.edam.com.tr/estp
Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in
Turkey*
a
Saban CEPIK
Hedayat SARANDI
Zirve University
Zirve University
Abstract
This study examines the interaction effect of age in L2 attainment. It explores whether success in foreign language learning at early childhood grades varies depending on age. It also addresses the beliefs of foreign language
teachers regarding the variables under review. Eighty-three 11 year-old language learners who started learning
English at different ages were placed into two groups. The initial exposure of the early starters was 5-6 and of
the late starters, 9-10. A set of language proficiency tests covering all four-language skills were given to the
participants to determine the possible differences in the proficiency of the two groups. Also, qualitative data was
collected from 6 teachers through a questionnaire that aimed to elicit their beliefs regarding the effect of age
on L2 attainment. The findings showed that the early starters did not perform significantly better than the late
starters in any of measures. The teachers, however, indicted that the early language learners had more positive
attitude towards English compared to the late starters. Findings underscore that language attainment may
involve a lot of variables and that early age may not take account of the whole issue.
Key Words
Early Childhood Language Instruction, Age Period, Length of Instruction, Early Starters, Late Starters.
he issue regarding the relationship between age
and language learning has both theoretical and
practical value. On theoretical ground, it relates to
*
This paper was revised after being presented at
the International Conference on Global Issues of
Early Childhood Education and Children’s Rights,
Gaziantep, Turkey, 27-29 April 2012.
a
Saban CEPIK, Ph.D., is an assistant professor and
director of the English Preparatory School and
Chair of the English Language Teacher Education
(ELTE) department at Zirve University in Gaziantep/Turkey. Dr. Cepik’s research interests include
teaching methodology, comparative study in language learning and teaching, pedagogical beliefs
of language teachers, curricula frameworks in
language teaching at schools and in higher education: Correspondence: Assist. Prof. Saban CEPİK,
Zirve University, Faculty of Education, Department
of English Language Teaching, Kızılhisar Campus
27260 Gaziantep/Turkey. E-mail: saban.cepik@
zirve.edu.tr Tel: +90 342 211 6830/6952.
one of the key issues in language acquisition: whether there is a diference between adult and child
language acquisition or if Universal Grammar
(UG) is still functional for adult second language
learners (Mayo & Lecumberri, 2003). Questions
like these are of the utmost importance for the
researcher of SLA (Long, 2007). On the practical
side, however, the age-related issue involves the
key decision of when to introduce second language
into the classroom settings (Mayo & Lecumberri,
2003), and whether or not it is possible to attain
native-like proficiency ater a certain age. Such questions fall into the interest range of a large number
of people including language planner, language
teachers, parents and language learners themselves. he issue has gained an added importance in
Turkey owing to Turkey’s relatively unsuccessful
history of English language teaching. Koru and
Akessson (2011), for example, in a study conducted by TEPAV (Economic Policy Research Foundation of Turkey) reported the results of the English
Proficiency Index in which Turkey ranks 43 among
44 countries. he researchers argued that the so-
EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE
called late introduction of English into language
classes is one of the main reasons, among others,
for the lack of success in language learning in Turkey. he present research study aims to explore this
issue hoping that its findings may cast light on the
long-running argument regarding the relationship
between age and language learning.
he Earlier, the Better?
he idea that an early introduction of a second language leads to a higher level of language proficiency appears to be accepted around the world (Pufahl, Rhodes, & Christian, 2000). It is assumed that
an early start of second language learning provides
the time that learners need for the daunting task
of learning a second language. Furthermore, early
starters are arguably endowed with the ability to
“sponge” the new language and become more proficient (Cenoz, 2003; Heighington, 1996). In fact as
Torras, Tragant, and García (1997, p. 142) put it,
“he younger they are, the more they are like sponges, the more they absorb, the more they retain.”
heoretically, the idea of the supremacy of young
learners over late learners is fueled by the Critical
Period Hypothesis (CPH). Originally proposed for
the first language, CPH claims that there is a certain time restriction outside which the learning of a
new language becomes dificult, if not impossible.
It is argued that the facility for the initial unconscious acquisition of a certain language through exposure disappears ater puberty and that learning a
foreign language becomes more of a conscious and
efortful endeavor aterwards that does not bring
the same results (Lennenberg, 1967). One reason
for this is the maturation changes, such as lateralization and plasticity-loss, that brain goes through
which in turn afects the processing of a new language. In lateralization, diferent hemispheres of the
brain take up separate responsibilities, allocating
language-related activities to the let hemisphere.
In terms of plasticity, a process called myelination is reduced resulting in the hardening of neural
cords and the transportation of neural impulses
(Marinova-Todd, Marshall, & Snow, 2000).
Maturational factors are only one of the reasons
that accounts for the superior performance of young language learners. Gass and Selinker (2008) offer social, psychological, cognitive and neurological
reasons to account for why adults are not as good
as children in language learning. In terms of social and psychological factors, adults’ willingness to
adhere to their identity and ego may prevent them
3200
from picking up a new accent and approaching
native-like mastery of the target language. Furthermore, adults have more developed cognitive skills
compared to children, and use analytical approaches to learning a second language, which are less
productive compared to children who rely more
on their language acquisition devices and implicit
learning. Finally, compared to adults, children have
access to more modified language, which results in
a better language input. Other reasons are ofered
for the low performance of adults. Herschensohn
(2008) attributes the dificulty of learning a second
language to the learners’ L1. She argues that it is the
perfection of the L1 language system that somehow
prevents the older learners accessing the internal
language learning mechanism. Bridsong (1999)
considers the loss of UG ater puberty, and changes
in memory capacity (and processing) as two reasons, among others, for the poor performance of
adults in terms of learning a second language.
he bulk of studies appear to show that in the ESL
context younger learners are better than older ones,
when considering the ultimate proficiency attainment. his is mainly so for phonology although
there is evidence for the success of young leaners
in other language areas as well (Gass & Selinker,
2008). In pronunciation, for example, several studies show that the age at which second language
learning started has a great bearing on the foreign accent (Yeni-Komshian, Feldge, & Liu, 2000;
Yeni-Komshian Robbin, & Flege 2001). Tahta,
Wood, and Loewenthal (1981) found that the ability of American children to imitate Armenian and
French intonational pattern reduced significantly
ater the age of 8. Moyer (1999) found that German
adult students, despite ample exposure to English
both in and out of classroom and having high motivation, failed to attain a native-like accent (see Gass
& Selinker).
Similar findings were reported for syntax. Patkowski (1980) found that out of 33 people who
started learning a second language before the age
of 14, all except one achieved native or near-native mastery of syntax whereas the majority of the
people who started their second language ater 14
remain at a comparatively lower proficiency level.
Likewise, Johnson and Newport’s study (1989)
examined the syntactic knowledge of a group of
Chinese and Japanese immigrants who arrived
at the US at diferent times and found that those
who arrived earlier achieved native-like mastery
in grammatical tests, whereas late arrivals varied
widely in this regard.
CEPIK, SARANDI / Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in Turkey
Voices of Concern!
While CPH is mainly accepted in first language
acquisition, its existence and role in L2 is still debated. For one thing, it is argued that the decline
in the acquisition of diferent language areas through age is not as abrupt as predicted by CPH. In
fact it is rather gradual. his has caused some to
use the term “sensitive” period rather than critical
(see Long 1990, for example). Second, puberty may
not be the right borderline as some language areas may start to deteriorate earlier. Foreign accent,
for example, may start to reveal itself at any time
ater the age of 6 (Yeni-Komshian et. al., 2000; Yeni-Komshian et. al., 2001). here are others who
argue that CPH fails to take full account of the issue. Singleton and Ryan (2004, p. 18), for example,
raises the point that age-related decline in learning
ability may be the result of multiple factors, and
that to attribute it solely to maturation and neurobiological phenomena as theorized by CPH is not
justified by the latest data. He concludes, “he idea
of a critical period for language development may
well have had its day.”
Perhaps the most severe criticism against CPH comes from the records of successful adult language
learners. Some researchers, drawing upon studies
whose findings revealed variations in the performance of late language learners, show that at least
some of the late language learners managed to reach a native or native-like proficiency level. hey
argue that if CPH holds true, then all late learners
should perform at a level lower than that of both
young learners and native speakers (MarinovaTodd, 2003). he results of research show, however,
that this is not necessarily the case. Marinova-Todd
reports a surprising study by Birdsong (1992),
where some late-learner near-native speakers performed better than native speakers. here are other
studies that show that native-like proficiency, at
least for some adults, is possible (see Juf & Harrington, 1995; White & Genesee, 1996 in syntax;
Bongarets, 1999 in pronunciation).
Another issue is that if the argument in favor of
CPH is valid in terms of final achievement, it is
not so in terms of the speed of acquisition. In fact,
with regard to the rate of acquisition, the opposite
holds true, and adults become better (faster) language learners, though young learners usually catch
up with them in the long run (Snow & HoefnagleHohle, 1978). here are of course other fallacies regarding the ineficiency of adult language learners.
Marinova-Todd et al. (2000, p. 27), for example,
argue that children learning a second language go
through a similarly efortful endeavor as adults.
hey also argue that it is premature to attribute the
diferences in language proficiency between adults
and children to neurobiological factors such as
lateralization and plasticity. Scovel (2000), voiced
similar concerns, making the point that biological
explanations such as lateralization can only account for the speech diferences between adults and
children, as speech has neuro-motor etiology that
can be afected by the loss of plasticity. However, it
is not yet clear how lack of plasticity in adults may
result in poor morphosyntactic knowledge. Furthermore, the researchers also contended that the
relatively lower achievement of adults compared to
children may not be due to CPH but to factors such
as lack of motivation, commitment and lack of support from the context in which the new language is
learned (Marinova-Todd et al.).
Several studies’ findings cast doubts on the assumption that late starters are not good language
learners. White and Genesee (1996) found that
some French adult English-language learners were
able to perform at a level not diferent from native
speakers on certain language structures that tap
UG access. he researcher concluded that even older language learners can achieve native-like competency, and that access to UG is not reduced through age, which they consider as evidence against
CPH. Bialystock (1997) examined two studies that
looked at the acquisition of French gender marking
and the acquisition of English syntax by non-native
speakers, and found that it was the length of residence, not the age of arrival, that accounted for the
success of second language learners leading the researcher to argue against the role of maturational
factors. Slavof and Johnson (1995) showed that
the idea of early starters eventually overtaking late
starters is not necessarily true. In their study, they
compared the grammatical performances of young
language learners who arrived at the US at diferent
ages (7-12) and found no meaningful diferences
between early and late starter. Even at the level of
pronunciation, the findings of several studies show
that some language learners who started their education late can attain native-like pronunciation (see
for example cited in Bongaerts, 1997; Bongaerts,
Mennen, & Van der Slik, 2000; Marinova-Todd,
2003; Palmen, Bongaerts, & Schils, 1997).
What these studies show is that late starters are not
necessarily doomed to failure. Overall, while the
bulk of the studies show that early starters are generally more successful, some variation in the performance of adult language learners is also observed.
3201
EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE
Such variations, while casting doubts on the strong
version of CPH (Nikolov & Mihaljevic-Djigunoic,
2006), do not refute it completely. he key question
is perhaps not whether or not age diferences exist,
but what could be the source of the discrepancy
between early and late language learning (Scovel,
2000). A lot of non-maturational factors could
work alongside the biological ones afecting the
learning abilities of language learners as they grow
up Nikolov & Mihaljevic-Djigunoic (2006).
EFL vs. ESL Settings
One of the important features of the studies that
have examined the relationship between age and
language learning is that they are mainly conducted in ESL settings. he findings of these studies
are later extrapolated to the EFL environment
though these two settings have striking diferences
(Muñoz, 2008, 2010). Muñoz (2008), for example,
criticizes the hasty generalization of studies carried
out in naturalistic learning contexts to formal learning contexts since each learning context has its
own unique features. he two contexts are diferent
on several grounds including quality and quantity
of the input that students receive, the objective of
language classes, and the proficiency level of language teachers (see Muñoz, 2008, 2010; Nikolov &
Mihaljevic-Djigunoic, 2006).
Several studies that compared late and early starters of English in EFL setting in Spain found that
in a great number of language measures older learners outperformed the younger ones (Cenoz, 2003;
Lecumberri & Gallardo, 2003; Mayo, 2003; Lasagabaster & Doiz, 2003; Muñoz, 2003). Cenoz, for
example, compared the performance of three groups of language learners on diferent language proficiency tests (oral production, writing, cloze test,
reading) and found that overall the general trend
observed ater 600 hours of exposure was that the
oldest group (fith year of secondary school, age
16 years) presents the highest level of proficiency
in English followed by the intermediate group (second year of secondary school, age 12). Of all three
groups, the lowest scores correspond to the youngest group (fith year of primary school, age 10).
García Mayo (2003) examined Basque/Spanish language learners learning English as a foreign language to find out if the length of exposure to English
has any efect on their performance in grammaticality judgment tests (GJT) and metalinguistic
awareness test. he findings showed that the length
of exposure resulted in the better performance of
3202
the participants in the grammaticality judgment
test. Moreover, once the length of exposure held
constant, it was the older learners who significantly
outperformed the younger group both on GJT and
metalinguistic tests. he researcher interpreted the
findings as evidence that early language introduction is efective only when instruction hours are used
in an eficient way.
Lasagabaster and Doiz (2003) explored whether
the written proficiency of Spanish students who
were learning English was related to their age. he
results showed that the writing scores of the participants increased with their age group. Of three
diferent age groups, the oldest group outperformed the other two groups and the youngest group
was significantly outperformed by both of the older
learners’ groups, leading the researchers to suggest
that cognitive development had a positive impact
on the writing performance of language learners.
Muñoz (2003) compared early and late starter Catalan-speaking Spanish students who were learning
English, in terms of listening comprehension performance and the comprehension and production
parts of oral interviews at two diferent times. he
results show that the older group statistically outperformed the younger one on both productive
and receptive measures of the interview but not in
the listening comprehension tests. he research attributed the significant performance of older learners in the interview to the active role and successful negotiation that they used during the interview,
while such an interactive role may not have been
possible in the listening comprehension where their role was more of input recipients.
here are also some studies that examined the effects of both age and length of instruction on the
performance of language learners in EFL settings.
Oller and Nagata (1974), at several stages, explored
the performance of Japanese students who were
exposed to English for diferent lengths of time,
using a cloze test. In their study, students in three
diferent grades were mixed. here were students
who had received early exposure to English and
students who had not been. heir findings showed that the diference between the early and late
starters, though significant at initial stages, became
insignificant in the upper levels.
Similar findings were reported in Burstall, Jamieson, Cohen, and Hargreaves (1974). In their study
English students learning French had diferent experiences in terms of prior exposure to the target
language. While some of them had received primary school instruction in French, others had not.
CEPIK, SARANDI / Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in Turkey
he results showed that the superior performance
of students with prior French knowledge diminished through time in all measures except for the
test of listening. Similar to Oller and Nagata (1974)
these findings suggest that the advantage that early
language learners enjoy at the beginning of language instruction may not last long.
A diferent result, however, emerged from a study
conducted recently. Domínguez and Pessoa (2005)
compared 32 English native speakers studying Spanish in the sixth grade. 27 of these students had
been receiving Spanish lessons since they were in
Kindergarten and 5 others had only been learning
Spanish for a year when the study was conducted.
he results showed that except for reading tasks,
the earlier starters outperformed the late starters
in oral communication (listening and speaking)
as well as writing. he early starters also proved
to be more confident in using Spanish compared
to the late starters. he researchers interpreted the
findings as a sign of the potential advantages of an
early start.
Overall, the bulk of the studies show that when the
length of instruction is constant, the late starters
are at an advantage. Not many studies, however,
examined the efect of diferent amount of instruction for the students who were in the same age
group, and the results gained from these studies
were inconclusive. While Burstall et al. (1974) and
Oller and Nagata (1974) found that the earlier positive efect of longer instruction disappears through time, Domínguez and Pessoa’s (2005) findings
contradicted this. Further studies may cast light in
this issue. Indeed, this is the objective of the present study.
It is also noteworthy that the recent history of English as a foreign language in Turkey began with the
Foreign Language Education and Teaching Act
of 1983 and the Primary Education Act of 1997,
which paved the way for radical changes in ELT
curriculum in Turkey. With respect to the foreign
language education in Turkey, the most significant
innovation brought by the above-mentioned laws
was the integration of foreign language education
into the national curriculum starting from the 6th
grade. he starting age was subsequently further
reduced to as low as the 4th grade, the aim being to
continually adjust and standardize English language teaching in line with the norms of the EU, later
the Common European Framework of Reference
for Languages (CEFR). Ever since, English as a foreign language has been introduced in grade four
in primary schools run by the state though some
private schools may start it earlier.
Method
Research Questions
In the lights of the issues raised above, the present
study was conducted to find answers to the following research questions
1. Do early starters perform better than late starters in diferent measures of language proficiency
in EFL context of Turkey?
he study also intends to triangulate the findings
of the research question mentioned above with the
insights and opinions of language teachers about
diferent types of language learners. More specifically, it intends to find the answer to the following
second research question.
2. What is the perception of language teachers regarding the early and late starter students? Which
groups of learners do language teachers perceive as
successful?
Research Model
Quantitative and qualitative research models were
used to collect data. Quantitative data were collected using quasi-experimental method where language learners with diferent amount of exposure to
English language were selected from intact groups
and their language proficiency was measured. In
this sense, the length of instruction was independent variable and the proficiency level of language
learners was dependent variable. Qualitative data
were collected using an open-ended questionnaire
through which teachers’ opinions about the success
of language learners with diferent starting age for a
foreign language were elicited.
Context and Participants
he participants of the study were 83 male and
female sixth grade students at a private elementary school in Gaziantep, Turkey. he ages of the
students at the time of testing were 11 and 12. he
study was carried out at the end of the first semester, academic year 2011-2012. Two groups of
the students were selected according to the age at
which they started to learn the English. he first
group represented the early beginners who had
started learning English in the first grade and in
kindergarten, and the second group represented
the late beginners who had started to learn English language when they were in the fourth grade.
he two groups were later mixed and both the late
and early starters received two years of English lan-
3203
EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE
guage instruction until they reached grade 6. he
English instruction hours at school were 6 hours
till the fourth grade and 7 hours for the fith grade
and above per week. he age and estimated amount
of instruction of the two diferent age groups was
shown in the Table 1 below. All the participants
were Turkish and started to learn English in Turkey. he data of those students who received foreign language instruction outside the school were
excluded from the data analysis.
Table 1.
Characteristics of the Students Participating in the Study
Age of first
exposure
Age at the
time of
testing
Estimated
amount of
instruction
Early age
group
5-6 years old
11-12yrs old
992 hrs
Late age
group
9-10 years
old
11-12 yrs old
406 hrs
Six language teachers who had the experience of teaching the late and early starters in the same school
were also selected to respond to a questionnaire
aiming to elicit the teachers view over the efectiveness of instruction and early language learning.
Data Collection
he data was collected in two diferent ways; first,
a questionnaire sent to 6 English language teachers
of the school, second, a full language test given to
the students. he interview posed the following
questions:
1. Are there any diferences between the students
who have had previous experience of learning
English and those who were exposed to English
in 4th grades (the late starters)? If yes, in which
language areas are the diferences more noticeable? Do the diferences create any problems in
terms of teaching? How do you usually deal with
these problems?
2. Does the diference between these students carry
over to the next levels (later classes)? If yes, how
is the existing proficiency gap filled, if at all?
Peer debriefing, avoiding researcher’s bias and negative case analysis were used to take care of the
validity issues of the qualitative data. First, diferent
stages of data collection and analyses were monitored by two researchers who had a considerable
experience in qualitative research. Second, since
the data for this part was collected on-line where the researchers had no direct contact with the
3204
teachers, it is believed that teachers could not be
afected by the possible bias of researchers. hird,
before drawing any conclusion in either direction,
the data was meticulously monitored for the counter evidence.
he second data was collected from a series of
tests administered to the students to measure their
language proficiency in all skills; first, they took a
computer-based test that consisted of a cloze test,
vocabulary, and reading tests. his was followed
by a writing test and speaking test. he computerbased test was taken from a web 2.0-based program called 7/24 for teaching and testing language
learners on line. A selection of elementary and
pre-intermediate level test material was made to
suit the level of the participants. he program included diferent sections mainly taping the students’
vocabulary, grammar and reading knowledge. he
cloze test included some multiple-choice grammar
questions, some gap-filling questions (asking prepositions, adjectives, and auxiliary verbs), picture-vocabulary matching (asking professions) and
daily conversations (asking hobbies, ages, families,
and parents’ profession). he vocabulary part included questions about colors, animals and some
daily life activities. he reading part consisted of
two short passages of beginning level followed by
some comprehension questions.
In the writing test, students were asked to write an
imaginary story to a family in England where they
were supposedly going to stay for a summer vacation, explaining about themselves, their daily life,
their hobbies, their interest etc. he task was completed in a classroom with no time limit allotted.
Two assessors graded each writing, and an average
grade was taken as the final score. To ensure the reliability of the scores, a holistic approach suggested
by Jacobs, Zinkgraf, Wormuth, Hartfiel, and Hughey (1981) was applied to grade students writing.
Scales were used to measure diferent aspects of
writing including content/ organization/ vocabulary/ language usage and mechanics. Following Lasagabaster and Doiz (2003), these criteria carried
the weight of 30, 20, 20, 25, 5 points respectively
culminating to a total score of 100. In cases where
more than 25 points discrepancy between scores
were observed a third assessor graded the paper. In
this case, the grade of the assessor who showed the
highest variance was removed and the average of
two closest grades was calculated to represent the
score of the participants. he Pearson correlation
coeficient between the final judgments of the raters was 0.80 (p= 0.000).
CEPIK, SARANDI / Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in Turkey
In the speaking test, pupils were asked to answer
several questions to elicit mainly the use of simple
present tense. he last question addressed the use of
simple past tense. hese questions included “what
is your name? where are you from? where do you
live? how many people are there in your family?
what does your father do? what are your hobbies?
what do you do in your free time? what did you do
last weekend? he performance of the participants
was graded based on luency, communication, accuracy, and vocabulary choice. Similar to writing
test, to guarantee reliability, two independent graders assessed the oral performance and an average
grade was calculated. A similar process to writing
was applied for the speaking once a 25 percent of
discrepancy was observed between the scores. he
Pearson correlation coeficient between the final
judgments of the raters was 0.85 (p= 0.000).
Results
Results of Interview with the school Language
Teachers
he irst item of questionnaire read as
1. Are there any diferences between the students
who have had previous experience of learning English and those who were exposed to English in 4th
grades (the late starters)? If yes, in which language
areas are the diferences more noticeable? Does the
diference create any problems in terms of teaching? How do you usually deal with it?
In terms of language teachers’ perception, the majority of language teachers believed that early language learners performed better in language classes. However, 5 out of 6 teachers mentioned that
the superior performance of early starters were in
oral communication, mainly pronunciation. Some
also mentioned that earlier starters were better in
terms of vocabulary. However, only two teachers
out of 6 mentioned, surprisingly, that late starters
were better at grammar and cognitively demanding
tasks and that they were very eager at language learning. Also, all of them agreed that the early starters have the self-confidence to express themselves
easily. For example, one of the language teachers
wrote:
I have experience working with second
and fourth grade students. I think early
starters are better than late starters. hey
are more interested in new languages. As a
child, they want to learn everything. here
are some diferences between early learners
and late learners in terms of their language
skills. For example; early learners are better
at listening and speaking. heir pronunciation is much better than late learners.
hey can pronounce the words better. Most
of them acquire the language because they
cannot write and read properly. hey memorize what they hear. heir brains are
like an empty disc. hey like repetition. On
the other hand, late learners are better at
grammar. Because they start to learn a new
language ater they learn their mother tongues’ grammatical rules. Knowing grammatical rules makes their job easier. hey
don’t want to speak in English because they
are afraid of making mistakes, but early
learners say what they want. hey are not
afraid of making mistakes.
he interview data suggested that the teachers
consistently argued that “the level system” through
which students are assigned to their appropriate levels should be applied, so as not to face the expected and predicted problems in the language classes
in the schools. Here, two teachers comment on the
diferences between the two groups of the students
and mention how they deal with the problems;
‘he students who start learning English
from the 1st grade use English better than
those who start later. hey practice English
more actively and willingly. hey have confidence and their pronunciation is better.
But those who start learning English at 4th
grade are more enthusiastic and dynamic.
hey are curious and have pleasure to use
English. And, of course, these diferences
create problems. But we deal with them by
putting students into diferent level and teaching them according to their knowledge.’
he second question asked how persistent the differences between students’ proficiency levels were.
It read:
2. Does the diference between these students carry
over to the next levels (later classes)? If yes, how is
the existing proficiency gap filled, if at all?
Generally teachers believe that the gap can be filled
if the students are well motivated and supported by
their teachers. But the most significant thing is to
group the students according to their level. Another language teacher expressed his ideas as follows:
As long as the English teacher who is in
charge of the new learners of English can
manage to motivate these students, the
3205
EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE
gap between the old comers and the new
comers close approximately in one educational year. But this may happen if the setting system is applied. If there is no setting
system and the students are mixed in one
class, then the new comers need to be given
extra courses to fill the gap.
Some teachers say that it is inevitable to experience
the problem at the later classes, but teacher support
may minimize the problem. Here is how an experienced teacher commented with this regard:
As far as I have experienced during my teaching life, students who have problem in
the early years of learning English carry
over all these problems to their further learning. But if the teacher gives more exposure to the late starters, they may solve the
problem.
Results of Proficiency Language Tests
Below are the descriptive statistics collected for
the diferent groups of the students in diferent
language measures.
Table 2.
Descriptive Statistic of Late Early Starters in Diferent Language Proficiency Tests
Speaking
Groups
N
Mean
Early starter
47
67
SD
19
Later starter
33
65
17
Groups
N
Mean
SD
Early starter
49
79
15
Later starter
34
78
14
Groups
N
Mean
SD
Early starter
19
15
23
Later starter
29
13
25
Groups
N
Mean
SD
Early starter
19
34
39
Later starter
29
39
43
Groups
N
Mean
SD
Early starter
19
61
40
Later starter
29
64
41
Writing
Reading
Cloze test
Vocabulary
In order to detect whether the diferent amount of
instruction that late and early starters had received
3206
resulted in diferent performance of the language
learners in diferent language measures independent T-tests (SPSS 16) were run on data. he results of data analysis revealed that in none of the
measures used in the study the diference between
two groups reached a significant level (Speaking:
F=0.33 p=0.855, Writing: F=0.104 p=0.748, Reading: F=0.30 p=0.864, Close test: F=0.238 p=0.628,
Vocabulary: F=0.002 p=0.965).
Discussion
he first research question asked if the students
who started language learning earlier had any advantages over the language learners who started
later. he findings show that early starters had no
advantage over late ones in any of the measures
used in the study. hus our findings confirm earlier studies of Burstall et al. (1974) and Oller and
Nagata (1974) and contradict those of Domínguez
and Pessoa (2005). It is also in line with the majority of studies conducted in EFL settings whose
findings did not reveal any advantage for an early
start (Cenoz, 2003; Lecumberri & Gallardo, 2003;
Mayo, 2003; Lasagabaster & Doiz, 2003; Muñoz,
2003).
he first reason to account for the findings is the
learning context in which the study was conducted.
As mentioned earlier, the exposure received in EFL
contexts both in terms of quality and quantity is
not significant compared to ESL settings (Muñoz,
2010). Hence, it could be argued that the amount
and intensity of the instruction that younger learners received may not have been suficient enough
to create a significant diference. he diference
between the early and late starters was about 586
hours. One may argue that this amount of extra
time may not to be enough to create any noticeable change in an EFL setting. Another possibility
is that the types of tasks that young learners had
dealt with may not be cognitively challenging, and
as such might have failed to stretch their inter-language beyond rudimentary levels (see Nikolov &
Mialjevic-Djigunoic, 2006 for a similar argument).
he efectiveness of instruction ofered to language
learners might also depend on the types of processing employed by the language learners. Young learners learn mainly implicitly while adults’ learning
is mainly explicit (DeKeyser, 2010). In this sense,
the mechanism used by older language learners fit
the formal instruction used in the great majority
of language classes in EFL settings (Muñoz, 2008)
including Turkey. In other words, the late starters’
learning mechanism might have allowed them to
CEPIK, SARANDI / Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in Turkey
benefit from form-focused language classes and
hence they managed to cover the gap.
Another reason for the present findings is that the
early and late starters were mixed in the classes. As
mentioned previously, early starters who had had 3
years of instruction beginning from the first grade
were mixed with the other language learners who
had no former instruction upon their arrival into
the new language setting. It could rightly be argued
that school system worked in such a way that the
preliminary higher proficiency of early starters was
leveled so that the instruction could be conducted
in a smooth manner (see Stern, 1976; Muñoz, 2008
for a similar argument). he following statement
from one of the teachers relects this point:
When I ask a question, early learners reply
it in few seconds. his situation went on a
few months, then, I started to give extra homework to the late learners. We memorized
new songs with them…. Now they are good
at English especially in grammar.
his statement shows that the instruction may not
have been distributed fairly between the diferent
age groups and while the earlier language learners
were not challenged, the other group received some
extra instruction.
Psychological variables may also afect the efectiveness of instruction. Moyer’s (2004) study on successful immigrants in Germany showed that age
factor and length of residence contributed little to
the final attainment in learning the German language. 74% of the variance in attainment was related
to psychological factors. Marinova-Todd (2003) argues that the right time for the introduction of high
quality instruction is when children are motivated,
not when they are necessarily young. In our case, it
is possible that at least some of the older students
were motivated to cover the existing gap and catch
up with the earlier language learners up with the
earlier language learners.
he second research question asked about theperception of language teachers in respect of the
efectiveness of early language learning. As the results of the study showed the majority of language
teachers believed that early language learners were
better at pronunciation and vocabulary. However,
the findings of our study showed no diference in
language proficiency of the two groups in terms
of any measures used in the study. One reason to
account for the seemingly contradicting results as
far as pronunciation was concerned was that in
speaking tests that we applied we did not include a
component to assess the pronunciation of the students. his was mainly so because the proficiency
level of the students was low and the diference
between the accents of the language learners did
not appear noticeable at the time of the testing. he
limited range of vocabulary chosen for the present
test compared to what students usually use in the
classroom might be another reason for the existing
discrepancy. he vocabulary used in the study was
limited to some colors, animals and daily life activity, which may not be broad enough to relect the
language areas that students encounter in their language classes. However, it is interesting to note that
the language teachers believed that the late starters
were good at grammar, and cognitively challenging
skills such as reading and writing. Obviously, these
skills and language areas match the brain maturation that these learners possessed at the time of
language testing.
Conclusion and Implications
hough the common belief is that early language
starters have some advantages over the later ones,
the results of the present study, as far as the students’ language proficiency is concerned, do not
indicate that this is necessarily the case. he early
language learners in our study failed to perform
better in any of language measures. he lack of
discernable proficiency diferences in the measures
applied implies that intensity and quality of programs used for the early starters may not be efective
enough to create a profound change in their linguistic competence. his underscores the need for the
reevaluation of the materials and syllabi used for
early starters so that the time and resources dedicated to this group are used in a more eficient way.
However, one should be careful not to misinterpret
the findings of the present study as evidence that
starting age has no efect on language learning.
For one thing, the testing instruments applied in
the present study did not allow us to measure the
possible pronunciation supremacy (and to a lesser
extend the vocabulary advantage) of the early starters, as suggested in the comments of language teachers. Furthermore, there was no measurement of
the willingness of early language learners to initiate
conversations and interactions in the classrooms
as the teachers ascribed to early starters. As Cenoz
(2003) points out, early language learners has more
positive attitude towards language learning, which
in the long-run may result in a better language development.
3207
EDUCATIONAL SCIENCES: THEORY & PRACTICE
Moreover, one may argue that early language instruction provides the opportunity to handle the difficult process of learning a new language in an extended time as opposed to an intensive one. In this
sense, the earlier starters have more time to process
the language implicitly, which may result in a more
profound and engrained learning, even though this
is not readily relected in their performance in the
language tests they take in their classes. It also introduces children to a new culture and receives the
positive attitudes of students (see also Lasagabaster
& Doiz, 2003). Overall, further studies are required
to examine if early starters become more successful
language learners in the later stages of their education, in the secondary school for example.
Finally, it should be born in mind that age is only
one of the factors involved in language learning.
here are other variables at work, alongside age,
such as the length of the instruction, methods of
teaching, students’ motivation, ability to simulate
a positive learning environment. that could afect
the final linguistic attainment of students. he introduction of the new language in the early ages can
be more efective if it is accompanied by professional teaching instructors, appropriate materials,
right methodology, suficient time, etc. Otherwise,
to expect that an early start on its own would lead
to ultimate success in foreign language learning is
too unrealistic.
References
Birdsong, D. (1992). Ultimate attainment in second language
acquisition. Language, 68 (4), 706-755.
Birdsong, D. (1999). Second language acquisition and the critical period hypothesis. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Bongaerts, T. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 pronunciation: he case of very advanced late L2 learners. In D. Birdsong
(Ed.), Second language acquisition and the critical period hypothesis (pp. 133-159). Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bongaerts, T., Mennen, S., & van der Slik, F. (2000). Authenticity of pronunciation in naturalistic second language acquisition: he case of very advanced late learners of Dutch as a second
language. Studia Linguistica, 54 (2), 298-308.
Burstall, C., & Jamieson, M., Cohen, S., & Hargreaves, M.
(1974). Primary French in the balance. NFER Slough.
Cenoz, J. (2003). he inluence of age on the acquisition of English: General proficiency, attitudes and code mixing. In M. P. G.
Mayo and M. L. G. Lecumberri (Eds.), Age and the acquisition
of English as a foreign language (pp. 77-93). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). he robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22 (4), 499-534.
3208
Domínguez, R., & Pessoa, S. (2005). Early versus late start in
foreign language education: Documenting achievements. Foreign Language Annals, 38 (4), 473-483.
Lecumberri, M. L. G., & Gallardo, F. (2003). English FL sounds
in school learners of diferent ages. In M. P. G. Mayo & M. L.
G. Lecumberri (Eds.), Age and the Acquisition of English as a
foreign language (pp. 115–35). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Mayo, M. P. G. (2003). Age, length of exposure and grammaticality judgments in the acquisition of English as a foreign
language. In M. P. G. Mayo & M. L. G. Lecumberri (Eds.), Age
and the acquisition of English as a foreign language (pp. 94-114).
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gass, S. M., & Selinker, L. (2008). Second language acquisition:
An introductory course. Taylor & Francis.
Heighington, S. (1996). Case study 5. Taking up the challenge.
In A. Hurrell & P. Satchwell (Eds.), Relections on Modern Languages in Primary Education (pp. 55-61). London: CILT.
Herschensohn, J. (2008). Developing I-language in L1 and L2.
In P. Masullo & C-H. Huang (Eds.), Romance Languages: Structure, interfaces, and microparametric variation (pp. 157-174).
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: J. Benjamins.
Jacobs, H. L., Zinkgraf, S. A., Wormuth, D. R., Hartfiel, V. F.,
& Hughey, J. B. (1981). Testing ESL composition. Newbury:
Rowley.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period efects in
second language learning: he inluence of maturational state
on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive
psychology, 21(1), 60-99.
Jufs, A., & Harrington, M. (1995). Parsing efects in second
language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17 (04), 483-516.
Koru, S., & Akesson, J. (2011). Turkey’s English deficit. Retrieved June 1, 2012 from http://www.tepav.org.tr/upload/
files/1324458212 1.Turkey_s_English_Deficit.pdf.
Lasagabaster, D., & Doiz, A. (2003). Maturational constraints
on foreign language written production. In M. P. M. Mayo &
L. G. Lecumberri (Eds.), Age and the acquisition of English as a
foreign language (p.136-160). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters,
Long, M. H. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12 (03),
251-285.
Long, M. H. (2007). Problems in SLA. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Marinova-Todd, S. H. (2003). Comprehensive analysis of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.
Marinova-Todd, S. H., Marshall, D. B., & Snow, C. E. (2000).
hree misconceptions about age and L2 learning. TESOL quarterly, 34 (1), 9-34.
Mayo, M. P. G., & Lecumberri, M. L. G. (2003). Age and the
Acquisition of English as a Foreign Language (4). Multilingual
Matters Ltd.
Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21 (1), 81-108.
CEPIK, SARANDI / Early and Late Language Start at Private Schools in Turkey
Moyer, A. (2004). Age, accent, and experience in second language acquisition: An integrated approach to critical period inquiry
(7). Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Muñoz, C. (2008). Symmetries and asymmetries of age efects
in naturalistic and instructed L2 learning. Applied Linguistics,
29 (4), 578-596.
Muñoz, C. (2010). On how age afects foreign language learning. Retrieved June 5, 2012, from http://www.enl.auth.gr/
gala/14th/Papers/Invited%20Speakers/Munoz.pdf.
Nikolov, M., & Mihaljevic-Djigunovic, J. (2006). Recent research on age, second language acquisition, and early foreign
language learning. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 26,
234-260.
Oller, J. W., & Nagato, N. (1974). he long-term efect of FLES:
An experiment. Modern Language Journal, 58, 15–19.
Palmen, M.-J., Bongaerts, T., & Schils, E. (1997). L’authenticité
de la prononciation dans l’acquisition
d’une
langage
étrangère au-delà de la période critique: Des
apprenants néerlandais parvenus à un niveau trés avancé en français.
Acquisition et interaction en langage etrangère, 9, 173–191.
Patkowski, M. S. (1980). he sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in a second language. Language Learning, 30 (2),
449-468.
Pufahl, I., Rhodes, N. C., & Christian, D. (2001). Foreign language teaching: What the United States can learn from other
countries. Foreign Language Annals, 34 (3), 281-283.
Scovel, T. (2000). A critical review of the critical period research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 20 (1), 213-223.
Singleton, D. M., & Ryan, L. (2004). Language acquisition: he
age factor (s). Multilingual Matters Ltd.
Snow, C., & Hoefnagle-Hohle, M. (1978). he critical age for
second language acquisition: Evidence from second language
learning. Child Development, 49, 1114–1128.
Stern, H. H. (1976). Optimal age: Myth or reality? Canadian
Modern Language Review, 32 (3), 283-294.
Tahta, S., Wood, M., & Loewenthal, K. (1981). Foreign accents:
Factors relating to transfer of accent from the first language to a
second language. Language and Speech, 24 (3), 265-272.
Torras, M. R., Tragant, E., García, M. L. (1997). Croyances populaires sur l’apprentissage précoce d’une langue étrangère. In
C. Muñoz, L. Nussbaum & M. Pujol (Eds.), Acquisition et interaction en langue étrangère 10 (pp. 127-158).
White, L., & Genesee, F. (1996). How native is near-native? he
issue of ultimate attainment in adult second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 12 (3), 233-265.
Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Flege, J. E., & Liu, S. (2000). Pronunciation proficiency in the first and second languages of KoreanEnglish bilinguals. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 3 (2),
131-150.
Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Robbins, M., & Flege, J. E. (2001). Effects of word class diference on L2 pronunciation accuracy.
Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 283–299.
3209