TITLE: Drive: The surprising truth about
what motivates us
Speakers:
Dan Pink
Chaired by:
Date:
Venue:
RSA, 8 John Adam Street, London WC2N 6EZ
NB
This is an unedited transcript of the event. Whilst every effort is made to ensure accuracy there may be phonetic or
other errors depending on inevitable variations in recording quality. Please do contact us to point out any errors,
which we will endeavour to correct.
To reproduce any part of this transcript in any form please contact RSA Lectures Office at
[email protected] or
+44(0)20 7451 6868
The views expressed are not necessarily those of the RSA or its Trustees.
www.theRSA.org
RSA Animate | Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us
Page 1
Dan Pink: Out motivations are
unbelievably interesting, and I've been working
on this for a few years and I just find the topic
still so amazingly engaging and interesting, so I
want to tell you about that. The science is
really surprising; the science is a little bit freaky
okay? If we are not as endlessly manipulable
and as predictable as you would think. There's
a whole set of unbelievably interesting studies.
I want to give you two that call into question
this idea that if you reward something you get
more of the behaviour you want; if you punish
something you get less of it.
So let's go from London to the mean
streets of Cambridge, Massachusetts in the
North Eastern part of the United States. And
let's talk about a study done at MIT,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Here's
what they did; they took a whole group of
students and they gave them a set of
challenges, things like memorising strings of
digits, solving word puzzles, other kinds of
spatial puzzles even physical tasks like throwing
a ball through a hoop. They gave them these
challenges and they said to incentivise their
performance they gave them three levels of
rewards. So, if you did pretty well you got a
small monetary reward, if you did medium well
you got a medium monetary reward, and if you
did really well, if you were one of the top
performers you got a large cash prize. We've
seen this movie before, this is essentially a
typical motivation scheme within organisations;
we reward the very top performers, we ignore
the low performers and the folks kind of in the
middle, okay you get a little bit.
So what happens? They do the tests
they have these incentives, here's what they
found out: One, as long as the task involved
only mechanical skill, bonuses worked as they
would be expected, the higher the pay the
better their performance. Okay, that makes
sense. But here's what happens. But once the
task called for even rudimentary cognitive skill
a larger reward led to poorer performance.
Now this is strange - a larger reward led to
poorer performance - how can that possibly
be? Now what's interesting about this is that
these folks here who did this are all
economists; two at MIT, one at the University
of Chicago, one at Carnegie Mellon - top tier
of the economics profession. And they're
reaching this conclusion that seems contrary
to what a lot of us learned in economics,
which is that the higher the reward the
better the performance and they're saying
that once you get above rudimentary
cognitive skill it's the other way around,
which seems like this kind of... the idea that
these rewards don't work that way. It seems
vaguely left wing and socialist doesn't it? It's
this kind of weird socialist conspiracy.
For those of you who have these
conspiracy theories, I'm going to point out
the notoriously left wing socialist group that
financed the research, the Federal Reserve
Bank. So this is the mainstream of the
mainstream coming to a conclusion that's
quite surprising, seems to defy the laws of
behavioural physics. So this is strange a
strange finding. So what do they do? They say
this is freaky, let's go test it somewhere else.
Maybe that $50 or $60 prize isn't sufficiently
motivating for an MIT student. So let's go to
a place where $50 is actually more significant
relatively. So we'll take the experiment, we're
going to go to Mudarai India, rural India,
where $50/$60 whatever the number was is
actually a significant sum of money.
So they replicated the experiment in
India roughly as follows: small rewards
equivalent of two weeks' salary... I mean
sorry, small performance, low performance
two weeks' salary; medium performance
about a month's salary; high performance
about two months' salary. So those are real
good incentives so you're going to get a
different result here.
What happened though was that the
people offered the medium reward did no
better than the people offered the small
reward, but this time around the people
offered the top reward they did worst of all.
The higher incentives led to worst
performance. What's interesting about this is
that it actually isn't all that anomalous. This
has been replicated over and over and over
again by psychologists, by some extent by
sociologists and by economists over and over
RSA Animate | Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us
Page 2
and over again. For simple straightforward
tasks those kinds of incentives - if you do this
then you get that - they're great. For tasks that
are algorithmic, a set of rules where you have
to just follow along and get a right answer, if
then the rewards carrots and sticks outstanding. But when the task gets more
complicated when it requires some conceptual,
creative thinking those kind of motivator
demonstrably don't work. Fact: money is a
motivator at work but in a slightly strange way.
If you don't pay people enough they won't be
motivated. What's curious about this is there's
another paradox here which is that the best
use of money as a motivator is to pay people
enough to take the issue of money off the
table. Pay people enough so that they're not
thinking about money and they're thinking
about the work.
Now once you do that, it turns out
there are three factors that the science shows
lead to the better performance, not to
mention personal satisfaction: autonomy,
mastery and purpose. Autonomy is our desire
to be self directed, to direct our own lives.
Now in many ways traditional notions of
management run afoul of that. Management is
great if you want compliance, but if you want
engagement, which is what we want in the
workforce today as people are doing more
complicated sophisticated things, self-direction
is better. Let me give you some examples of
this almost radical forms of self-direction in the
workplace that leads to good results.
Let's start with this company right here,
Atlassian, an Australian company, it's a
software company and they do something
really cool. Once a quarter, on a Thursday
afternoon, they say to their developers, "For
the next 24 hours you can work on anything
you want. You can work on it the way you
want, you can work on it with whomever you
want, all we ask is that you show the results to
the company at the end of the hours 24 hours"
in this fun kind of meeting, not a star chamber
session but this fun meeting with beer and cake
and fun and other things like that. It turns out
that that one day of pure undiluted autonomy
has led to a whole array of fixes for existing
software, a whole array of ideas for new
products that otherwise had never emerged one day. Now this is not ((00:06:25?)) then
incentive, this is not the sort of thing that I
would have done three years ago before I
knew this research, I would have said "You
want people to be creative and innovative,
give them a fricken innovation bonus. If you
do something cool I'll give you twenty five
hundred dollars." They're not doing this at all.
They're essentially saying, "You probably
want to do something interesting, let me just
out of your way". One day of autonomy
produces things that had never emerged.
Now let's talk about mastery. Mastery
is our urge to get better at stuff; we like to
get better at stuff. This is why people play
musical instruments on the weekend. You
have all these people who are acting in ways
that seem irrational economically; they play
musical instruments on weekends - why? It's
not going to get them a mate, it's not going
to make them any money why are they doing
it? Because it's fun, because you get better at
it and that's satisfying.
Go back in time a little bit. I imagine
this if I went to my first economic professor,
a woman named Mary Alice Schuuman, and I
went to her in 1983 and said, "Professor
Schuuman, can I talk to you after class for a
moment?" "Yeah." "I've got this inkling, I've
got this idea for a business model, I just want
to run it past you. Here's how it would work.
You get a bunch of people around the world
who do highly skilled work, but they're willing
to do it for free and volunteer their time, 20
sometimes 30 hours a week." Now she's
looking at me somewhat sceptically there.
"Oh but I'm not done. And then what they
create they give it away rather than sell it. It's
going to be huge." She truly would have
thought I was insane. It seems to fly in the
face of so many things but what do you have?
You have Linex powering one our four
corporate servers in four to five hundred
companies. Apache powering more than the
majority of web servers. Wikipedia - what's
going on why are people doing this? Why are
these people, many of whom are technically
sophisticated, highly skilled people who have
jobs okay, they have jobs, they're working at
RSA Animate | Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us
Page 3
jobs for pay doing sophisticated technological
work, and yet during their limited discretionary
time they do equally, if not more, technically
sophisticated not for their employer but for
someone else for free, that's a strange
economic behaviour. Economists who looked
in it, "Why are they doing this?" It's
overwhelmingly clear - challenge and mastery
along with making a contribution - that's it.
look at the science I think we can actually
build organisations and work lives that make
us better off, but I also think they have the
promise to make our world just a little bit
better.
What you see more and more is a rise
of what you might call the purpose motive as if
more and more organisations want to have
some kind of a transcendent purpose: partly
because it makes coming to work better; partly
that's because that's the way to get better
talent. And what we're seeing now is in some
ways, when the profit motive becomes
unmoored from the purpose motive bad things
happen. Bad things ethically sometimes but also
bad things like not good stuff, like crappy
products, like lame services, like uninspiring
places to work that when the profit motive is
paramount, or when it becomes completely
unhitched from the purpose motive, it just...
people don't do great things. More and more
organisations are realising this and it's sort of
disturbing the categories between what's profit
and what's purpose.
And I think that actually heralds
something interesting. And I think that the
organisations that are flourishing whether
they're profit, for profit or somewhere in
between are animated by this purpose motive.
Let me give you a couple of examples. Here's
the founder of Skype, he says, "Our goal is to
be disruptive but in the cause of making the
world a better place" - pretty good purpose.
Here's Steve Jobs, "I want to put a ding in the
universe." All right that's the kind of thing that
might get you up in the morning racing to go
to work. So I think that we are purpose
maximisers not only profit maximisers, I think
the science shows that we care about mastery
very, very deeply, and the science shows that
we want to be self-directed. And I think that
the big takeaway here is that if we start
treating people like people and not assuming
that they're simply horses, you know, slower,
smaller, better smelling horses, if we get past
this kind of ideology of carrots and sticks and
RSA Animate | Drive: The surprising truth about what motivates us
Page 4