OIKOS 99: 552– 570, 2002
How does landscape structure influence landscape connectivity?
Brett J. Goodwin and Lenore Fahrig
Goodwin, B. J. and Fahrig, L. 2002. How does landscape structure influence
landscape connectivity? – Oikos 99: 552– 570.
We investigated the impact of landscape structure on landscape connectivity using a
combination of simulation and empirical experiments. In a previous study we
documented the movement behaviour of a specialized goldenrod beetle (Trirhabda
borealis Blake) in three kinds of patches: habitat (goldenrod) patches and two types
of matrix patch (cut vegetation and cut vegetation containing camouflage netting as
an impediment to movement). In the current study, we used this information to
construct simulation and experimental landscapes consisting of mosaics of these three
patch types, to study the effect of landscape structure on landscape connectivity,
using the T. borealis beetle as a model system. In the simulation studies, landscape
connectivity was based on movements of individual beetles, and was measured in six
different ways. The simulations revealed that the six measures of landscape connectivity were influenced by different aspects of landscape structure, suggesting that:
(1) landscape connectivity is a poorly defined concept, and (2) the same landscape
may have different landscape connectivity values when different measures of landscape connectivity are used. There were two general predictions that held over all
measures of landscape connectivity: (1) increasing interpatch distance significantly
decreased landscape connectivity and (2) the influence of matrix elements on landscape connectivity was small in comparison to the influence of habitat elements.
Empirical mark-release-resight experiments using Trirhabda beetles in experimental
landscapes supported the simulation results.
B. J. Goodwin and L. Fahrig, Ottawa-Carleton Inst. of Biology, Carleton Uni6., 1125
Colonel By Dri6e, Ottawa, Ontario, CANADA K1S 5B6 (present address of BJG, Inst.
of Ecosystem Studies, Box AB (65 Sharon Turnpike), Millbrook, NY 12545 -0129, USA
[
[email protected]]).
Rates of anthropogenic landscape change have increased markedly in the last century (Wilcove et al.
1986, Groom and Schumaker 1993). Persistence of
wildlife populations in the face of these changes depends, at least in part, on their ability to move through
modified landscapes. Such movements allow individuals
to forage over multiple habitat patches (Kozakiewicz
1995), rescue local populations from extinction (Brown
and Kodric-Brown 1977), or recolonize local populations after extinction (Henderson et al. 1985, McCauley
1989, Thomas 1994). The interaction between animal
movement (set by physiology and behaviour) and landscape structure (set by landscape composition and
configuration) will determine the ability of an animal to
move through a landscape. Merriam (1984) referred to
the landscape property resulting from this interaction
as ‘‘connectivity’’. Landscape connectivity was later
defined as ‘‘the degree to which the landscape facilitates
or impedes movement among resource patches’’ (Taylor
et al. 1993). Understanding the impact of landscape
change on landscape connectivity is essential for predicting the impact of landscape change on a species.
Generally, landscape change negatively affects a species when its habitat is lost due to conversion into other
patch types (e.g. forest into agriculture). Habitat loss
tends to increase habitat interpatch distances and decrease habitat patch sizes (Turner and Ruscher 1988,
Saunders et al. 1993). Both effects will tend to decrease
landscape connectivity, as greater interpatch distances
are harder to cross (Laan and Verboom 1990, Sjögren
Accepted 10 June 2002
Copyright © OIKOS 2002
ISSN 0030-1299
552
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
1991, Vos and Stumpel 1995, Matter 1996) and smaller
habitat patches are harder to find (Kareiva 1985, Capman et al. 1990, Matter 1996). Additionally, the novel
patch types generated during habitat loss (matrix patch
types) can influence movement behaviour (Baars 1979,
Crist et al. 1992, Johnson et al. 1992, Matthysen et al.
1995, Charrier et al. 1997, Pither and Taylor 1998,
Jonsen and Taylor 2000, Goodwin and Fahrig 2002)
and movement risk (Charrier et al. 1997, Sakai and
Noon 1997, St Clair et al. 1998, Zollner and Lima 1999,
Hanski et al. 2000). The effect of the matrix on landscape connectivity will depend on the composition and
configuration of the matrix patches. Landscapes dominated by matrix patches that facilitate movement will
have high connectivity while landscapes dominated by
matrix patches that impede movement will have low
connectivity. Similarly, certain configurations of matrix
patches might reduce landscape connectivity (e.g. when
impassable patches encircle all habitat patches) or increase landscape connectivity (e.g. when impassable
patches are clumped and far from habitat).
General relationships between landscape connectivity
and landscape structure, necessary for predicting the
impact of landscape change on landscape connectivity,
are lacking. This is, in part, due to the use of many
connectivity metrics (Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000a, b),
some focused on habitat structure (e.g. Green 1994,
With et al. 1997, Metzger and Décamps 1997, Keitt et
al. 1997, Tiebout and Anderson 1997) and others focused on organism movement (Doak et al. 1992, Demers et al. 1995, Gustafson and Gardner 1996,
Schumaker 1996, Schippers et al. 1996, Ruckelshaus et
al. 1997, Pither and Taylor 1998). Structural measures
of connectivity often have no link to movement behaviour (Green 1994, With et al. 1997, Metzger and
Décamps 1997, Collinge 2000) and as such merely
describe landscape pattern and not connectivity (see
definition in Taylor et al. 1993). Measures of connectivity based upon organism movements provide for the
interaction between movement behaviour and landscape structure. Such measures of connectivity can be
divided into two broad categories based on the approach to measuring animal movement: direct measures
of individual movement such as search time, displacement distances, path tortuosity, or searching success
based on tracking individuals (e.g. radio or GPS
telemetry) and indirect measures of movement such as
immigration rates based on mark-recapture/resight
data. The nature of the system under study will dictate
which approach is feasible. Finally, simulation and
empirical approaches tend to use different connectivity
metrics. For example, connectivity based on organism
movements has been measured as mean probability of
moving between pairs of patches (referred to as emigration success by Gustafson and Gardner 1996), dispersal
success (Gustafson and Gardner 1996, Schumaker
1996, Schippers et al. 1996, Ruckelshaus et al. 1997,
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000a, Tischendorf 2001),
search time (Doak et al. 1992, Tischendorf and Fahrig
2000a, Tischendorf 2001), and cell immigration (Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000a, Tischendorf 2001) in simulation studies, and as dispersal success (Andreassen et
al. 1996) and re-observation after displacement (Pither
and Taylor 1998) in empirical work. Determining the
relationships between different connectivity metrics will
allow the results from different studies of landscape
connectivity to be compared, which in turn should help
generate general theories of landscape connectivity.
Our first goal in this study was to assess the influence
of different aspects of landscape structure on landscape
connectivity. Specifically, we used simulations to test
how the spatial structure of habitat (amount of habitat,
number of patches, patch size distribution, distance
between patches, and patch shape) and matrix (relative
coverage of different matrix patch types, matrix patch
dispersion, matrix patch size distribution, matrix patch
edge to area ratio) influence landscape connectivity.
The simulations also allowed us to test the relative
importance of various aspects of landscape structure on
landscape connectivity. (i.e. Is habitat amount more
important than arrangement? Are matrix patches more
or less important than habitat patches?)
Simulations are always an abstraction and simulation
predictions should be empirically tested. This is rarely
done with landscape connectivity simulations (but see
Fahrig and Merriam 1985, With and Crist 1995,
Brooker et al. 1999, With et al. 1999). Accordingly, our
second goal was to test the simulation predictions in the
field. The field system consisted of a goldenrod specialist beetle, Trirhabda borealis (Blake), in mosaic landscapes of goldenrod patches (habitat), patches of cut
vegetation (matrix), and cut patches containing
camouflage netting to impede movement (also matrix).
Our third goal was to compare the response of
multiple landscape connectivity metrics to variation in
landscape structure. Work in landscape connectivity
usually focuses on a single metric and different metrics
are rarely compared (but see Tischendorf and Fahrig
2000a). However, due to the variety of landscape connectivity metrics in the literature, comparisons between
simulation and empirical approaches or among results
from different empirical systems will depend on the
relationships between the different landscape connectivity metrics. Since our field system constrained us to
estimate beetle movements in the landscapes through
mark-resight methods, we focused on metrics of connectivity that could be used in such a situation in this
paper. A comparison of metrics more appropriate for
telemetry type studies is beyond the scope of this paper.
We measured connectivity in six different ways, transition probabilities between habitat patches, transition
probabilities between habitat cells, mean number of
habitat patches visited per individual, mean number of
habitat cells visited per individual, habitat patch immi553
gration, and habitat cell immigration (Table 1). We
measured landscape connectivity at both the cell and
patch level for all our metrics of connectivity because
earlier modeling work had suggested that cell immigration may avoid an artefactual increase connectivity with
increasing habitat fragmentation that can occur with
patch level metrics (Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000a, b).
Immigration rates and the related measure, dispersal
success, are the most common functional measures of
connectivity (Goodwin 2000). Both patch and cell immigration should increase in landscapes with higher
connectivity. Mean immigration rates may be sensitive
to particular patches that are either easy or difficult for
dispersing organisms to reach. An alternative approach
is to average dispersal success across individuals instead
of across habitat patches or cells, that is to average the
number of habitat patches or cells visited per individual
as they move through the landscape. The number of
patches or cells visited by an individual should increase
as landscape connectivity increases. Finally, though
transition probabilities have rarely been used to measure connectivity (Gustafson and Gardner 1996, Hof
and Flather 1996, Hof and Raphael 1997) they represent the most direct measure of connectivity as defined
by Merriam (1984) and Taylor et al. (1993). Increases in
the mean chance of moving between pairs of habitat
patches or cells should indicate increased landscape
connectivity.
Methods
We assessed the influence of landscape structure on
landscape connectivity using three experiments. The
first was a simulation experiment assessing the influence
of habitat amount and configuration on landscape connectivity (Fig. 1). The second was a simulation experiment primarily assessing the influence of matrix
composition and configuration on landscape connectivity, but also including aspects of habitat structure the
first experiment deemed important (Fig. 2). It was
necessary to examine the influence of habitat and matrix structure with two experiments because the model
runs would have taken too long if habitat and matrix
variables were varied together. The third experiment
was a combination of simulation and empirical experiments that tested the main predictions from the first
two experiments (Fig. 3). Since we could not test as
many aspects of landscape structure in the field as we
could using simulations, we identified the most important aspects of landscape structure from the first two
experiments, performed simulations focused on the
identified landscape attributes, and then tested the hypotheses generated by that set of simulations with
mark-release-resight experiments in the field.
All three sets of experiments involved two stages: the
generation of landscapes and the tracking of individuals
to determine connectivity within the landscapes. All
Table 1. Landscape connectivity metrics investigated. np, nc, and nb are number of patches, cells, or beetles in the landscape,
respectively. p pij is the probability of moving from patch i to patch j and is estimated by dividing the number of beetles moving
from patch i to patch j by the number available to move. p cij is the probability of moving from cell i to cell j and is calculated
similarly to p pij. 6 pi and 6 ci are the number of patches or cells visited by beetle i, respectively. m pi and m ci are the number of
immigrants into patch i or cell i, respectively.
Landscape connectivity metric
Description
Patch transition probability
Mean patch transition probability,
averaged across all pairs of patches in the landscape
Computation
np
i=1 j=1
np (np−1)
nc
Cell transition probabilitiy
Mean cell transition probability,
averaged across all pairs of cells in the landscape
np
% % p pij
, i" j
nc
% % p cij
i=1 j=1
nc (nc−1)
, i"j
nb
Patch visits
Mean number of visits to a new patch per individual
% 6 pi
i=1
nb
nb
% 6 ci
Cell visits
Mean number of visits to a new cell per individual
i=1
nb
np
% m pi
Patch immigration
Mean number of immigrants per patch
i=1
np
nc
% m ci
Cell immigration
554
Mean number of immigrants per cell
i=1
nc
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Fig. 1. Variation in habitat
spatial structure in the
landscapes for simulation
experiment 1: habitat. Each
panel shows how one aspect
of landscape structure was
varied. The panel for amount
of habitat is missing examples
for 12, 20 and 28%. In
simulations landscapes with
all combinations of variables
were created. The different
patch types are = habitat
and = cut.
landscapes (both in the simulations and in the field)
were 5 ×5 m and were divided into a 10 by 10 cell grid.
Each cell in the grid was assigned to one of three patch
types: 1) goldenrod (habitat); 2) cut (matrix) where all
vegetation was cut to 2 cm; and 3) netting (matrix)
where camouflage netting was suspended in cut patches
to emulate vegetation and thereby impede movement.
Landscapes were created in the field by cutting natural
vegetation to a height of 2 cm and leaving goldenrod
patches uncut. Netting patches were created in the cut
areas by stringing camouflage netting within each netting cell, such that the top of the netting was suspended
approximately 50 cm above the ground. As the netting
was 1 to 1.3 m in width this created a vertical panel of
suspended netting (emulating the structure of standing
vegetation) with the rest of the netting piled in the cell
(emulating the structure of plant litter and groundcover vegetation). Previous work (Goodwin and Fahrig
2002) demonstrated that these patch types influence T.
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
borealis movement behaviour and the empirically determined distributions describing those behaviours (Table
2) were used in the simulations.
Simulation experiment 1. Habitat
The virtual landscapes for the habitat simulation experiment were created as follows (parameters and algorithms are described in Table 3). 1) The amount of
habitat was set. 2) The number of habitat patches was
set. 3) The size distribution of the habitat patches was
determined. 4) The location for the first cell of the first
habitat patch was chosen randomly. 5) The habitat
patch was placed on the landscape by choosing cells
using a self-avoiding random walk, which could only
enter empty cells. The shape of the habitat patch was
determined during this process by a) the number of
steps in the random walk (set by the patch size) and
b) the chance of the random walk turning, which was
555
varied to give more or less edge by having less or more
turning, respectively. 6) To ensure that habitat patches
did not coalesce, a 1-cell buffer inaccessible to the
random walk was placed around the completed patch.
7) After the habitat patch was placed, the random walk
jumped a set distance, determined by a distance
parameter and distance variance parameter, in a random direction, to select the first cell of the next patch.
If a habitat patch could not be placed at this location
then the program systematically searched for another
suitable location at the appropriate distance. Steps 5)
through 7) were repeated, jumping from completed
patch to the next new patch, until all the habitat
patches were placed in the landscape. If at any time a
habitat patch could not be fit into the landscape the
program would backtrack (removing habitat patches)
until all of the patches fit. 8) Any part of the landscape
not filled with habitat patches was filled with cut
patches. This approach to generating landscapes produced slightly different values for indices of landscape
structure (e.g. coefficient of variation [CV] for patch
sizes, edge to area ratios, mean interpatch distance, CV
of interpatch distance) even given the same algorithm
parameters, so landscape structure indices were measured and these measures of landscape structure were
used in subsequent analyses.
Fig. 2. Variation in habitat
and matrix spatial structure in
the landscapes for simulation
experiment 2: matrix. Each
panel shows how one aspect
of landscape structure was
varied. The panel for
proportion of matrix in net is
missing examples for 20, 40,
60 and 80%. In simulations
landscapes with all
combinations of variables
were created. The different
landscape elements are
= habitat, b = camouflage
netting, and = cut.
556
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Fig. 3. Design of landscapes
for experiment 3
( = habitat, b = camouflage
netting, = cut).
To determine landscape connectivity one hundred
virtual beetles were randomly placed in the goldenrod
patches. Movements were modeled using empirically
determined, patch-type specific, movement parameter
distributions (see Table 2 for a summary) in a vectorbased, stochastic movement model. The movement
model kept track of movement state (moving or not)
and kept an individual in that state for a duration
drawn from the empirically determined distribution. If
the individual was moving, a turning angle and step-
length were randomly chosen from the appropriate
distribution. Movements were modeled using a 30 second time step. Each model run lasted 5800 time steps,
approximately equivalent to four 12-hour days in the
empirical system. During movements each individual
was tracked and data for calculating the six landscape
connectivity indices were tabulated (see Table 1 for
computational details). Landscape edges were tiled, so
the surrounding landscape was like the focal landscape,
and individuals were allowed to move beyond and
Table 2. Summary of T. borealis movement behaviour in the three patch types (see Goodwin and Fahrig 2002 for details).
Patch type
Goldenrod
Cut
Netting
Movement parameter
Probability of moving1
Step length2
Turning angle3
Short bouts of mobility (0.5, 0.5, 3.0)
Long bouts of immobility (0.5, 2.0, 49.0)
Long bouts of mobility (0.5, 0.5, 9.0)
Short bouts of immobility (0.5, 1.0, 27.5)
Short bouts of mobility (0.5, 0.5, 2.5)
Intermediate bouts of immobility (0.5, 1.0, 42.0)
Short (1, 8, 28)
Uniform distribution (K=0)
Short (1, 6, 30)
Forward concentration (K= 0.4)
Long (1, 8, 63)
Uniform distribution (K= 0)
1
Duration of bouts of mobility or immobility in minutes; minimum, median, and maximum values are presented.
Step lengths in centimetres per 30 second time step; minimum, median, and maximum values are presented.
Shape of turning angle distribution as described by a von Mises distribution (Batschelet 1981); concentration parameter K is
provided.
2
3
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
557
Table 3. Description of the landscape variables for the habitat simulations. The generating algorithm is described as well as the
minimum and maximum values attained. Units of measure are indicated.
Landscape variable
Description Generating Algorithm; (Min., Max.)
Amount of habitat
Amount of habitat in the landscape (proportion of landscape)
Set levels: 0.08, 0.12, 0.16, 0.20, 0.24, 0.28, 0.32; (0.08, 0.32)
Number of habitat patches
Number of habitat patches
Set levels: 2, amount/4, amount/2, 3*amount/4; (2, 24)
Coefficient of variation (CV) of the habitat patch sizes
Every patch gets one cell of habitat, start putting the remaining habitat in the first
patch, probability of moving on to a new patch set at 4 levels: 0, 0.1, 0.25, 1; (0.00,
1.98)
Ratio of total edge to total area for all habitat patches
Patches are placed as self-avoiding random walks with 4 levels for the probability
of turning: 0.75, 0.5, 0.25, 0.05; (0.88, 3.75)
Mean distance between habitat patch edges (cells)
The base step distance from one patch to the next as they are placed in the
landscape is set at 4 levels: 2, 4, 6, 8 cells; (0.00, 7.07)
CV of distances between habitat patch edges
Two levels of variance: first level: no change in step distance; second level:
randomly changed the step distance from −2 to +2 cells; (0.00, 1.41)
Mean distance between habitat cell centres (cells)
Set by placing patches; (1.84, 7.49)
CV of habitat cell distances
Set by placing patches; (0.37, 0.95)
Habitat patch size distribution
Habitat edge to area ratio
Interpatch distance
Variance in interpatch distance
Intercell distance
Variance in intercell distance
possibly back into the focal landscape. Only movements in the focal landscape were counted in landscape
connectivity measures.
Simulation experiment 2. Matrix
Based on their relative importance in the first simulation experiment (see results below) we included amount
of habitat, number of habitat patches, and interpatch
distance in the matrix simulation experiments. Habitat
edge to area ratio was also a candidate but habitat
amount had to be relatively low to allow for manipulation of matrix structure, precluding any reasonable
manipulation of habitat edge to area ratios. For these
landscapes habitat patches were placed in predetermined patterns (Fig. 2) and then patches of the least
common matrix patch type were placed in a similar
manner as the habitat patches were in the first simulation experiment (parameters and algorithms are described in Table 4), but distances between matrix
patches were random (as opposed to the set distances in
Table 4. Description of the landscape indices for the matrix simulations. The generating algorithm is described as well as the
minimum and maximum values attained. Units of measure are indicated.
Descriptor
Description Generating Algorithm; (Min., Max.)
Amount of habitat
Amount of habitat in the landscape (proportion of landscape)
Set levels: 0.04, 0.16; (0.04, 0.16)
Number of habitat patches
Set levels: 2, 4; (2, 4)
Mean distance between habitat patch edges (cells)
Set levels: near, far; (2, 10)
Proportion of the non-habitat landscape taken up by the netting
Set levels: 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9; (0.1, 0.9)
Number of matrix patches
Set levels based on the number of cells of the least common matrix element (LCM): 2,
LCM/4, LCM/2, 3*LCM/4; (2, 28)
Coefficient of variation (CV) of the least common matrix type patch sizes
Every patch gets one cell of the least common matrix type, start putting the
remaining matrix in the first patch, probability of moving on to a new patch set at
4 levels: 0, 0.1, 0.25, 1; (0.00, 0.22)
Ratio of total edge to total area for all patches of least common matrix type
Patches are placed as self-avoiding random walks with 4 levels for the probability
of turning: 0.75, 0.5, 0.25, 0.05; (1.0, 3.6)
Number of habitat patches
Habitat interpatch distance
Proportion of matrix as net
Matrix dispersion
Matrix patch size distribution
Matrix edge to area ratio
558
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
the habitat simulations). After all of the least common
matrix patches were placed the rest of the landscape
was filled with the more common matrix patch type.
Modeling of movement and calculation of landscape
connectivity indices were performed exactly as in the
first simulation experiment.
Simulation experiment 3 and field test
For the third simulation experiment amount of habitat,
number of habitat patches, placement of habitat
patches, and matrix composition were all set (Fig. 3)
and matrix cells were randomly assigned to patch type.
All of the area outside the focal landscape was the cut
patch-type to emulate the buffer strips in the field tests
(see below). Modeling of movement was performed
exactly as in the first two simulation experiments.
Landscape connectivity in these simulations was calculated in a manner that matched the limitations faced in
the field (see below).
We ran the model a second time to determine both
the average displacement from release point over time
and confidence intervals around that average. This
second run was required because we needed more runs
to construct the confidence envelope around the average displacement. In this second run the model was set
up the same as it was for the runs that determined
landscape connectivity. Displacement was calculated as
the distance from the centre of the cell the individual
was released in to the centre of the cell the individual
was observed in for both the simulations and the field
releases. For the simulations, a 90% confidence envelope was constructed around the mean displacement by
determining the 5th and 95th percentiles from 1000
simulation runs.
For the field tests 4 landscapes were constructed at
each of 4 sites in an old field south of Ottawa, Canada.
Over the course of the field season (July 28 to September 9, 1997) each unique type of landscape (Fig. 3) was
replicated 4 times (n =64). To do this we ran 2 sequential replicates, 16 landscapes per replicate, at 16% goldenrod and then cut the goldenrod back to 4% and ran
two more replicates. At each site all the natural vegetation between landscapes (minimum 1 m separation) and
a buffer around the 4 landscapes (minimum 1 m width)
was also cut. Between releases of beetles, any re-growth
of vegetation in the cut or netting patches was trimmed
back. When T. borealis populations in the surrounding
fields had finished pre-ovipositional long-distance dispersal (Messina 1982) and were only engaged in shortdistance, ‘‘trivial’’ movement, individuals were captured
and marked by dusting with fluorescent powder.
Twenty-five of these marked beetles were released into
a single goldenrod patch in each of the experimental
landscapes. We used a different colour of powder for
each landscape at a site, so immigration from a neighOIKOS 99:3 (2002)
bouring landscape could be detected and not influence
measures of movement within a particular landscape.
The 4 sites were separated by a minimum of 50 m of
old field vegetation, so the chance of beetles moving
between sites was extremely low. Beetles were released
in a small paper bag and allowed to find their own way
out to allow any effect due to handling to have a
minimal influence on their movements. Any beetles that
had not left the paper bag by the first night of searching
were removed at night when they were sluggish and
unresponsive. Movements of the marked beetles were
monitored for four nights following release by searching the goldenrod patches using a hand-held UV lamp.
The numbers of beetles in each patch or cell were
recorded.
Since we could not obtain the information necessary
to calculate all landscape connectivity metrics in the
field (e.g. differentiate between individual beetles to
determine the number of patches visited), we were
restricted to calculating landscape connectivity as either
cell or patch immigration rates. For both simulations
and field tests there was an initial release followed by
four subsequent observations of patch or cell occupancy, spaced a day apart. These observations allowed
us to estimate immigration rates (either into cells or
patches). Since the beetles could potentially visit multiple cells or even patches in a day this approach will
consistently detect fewer immigration events than occurred in the first two simulation experiments where we
knew the locations of all individuals all the time.
Results
Simulation experiment 1. Habitat
Based on multiple regressions, landscape connectivity
was strongly influenced by overall landscape structure
(R 2 ranged from 0.85 to 0.94 for all landscape connectivity measures, except for patch immigration where
R 2 =0.58; Table 5). However, ranking landscape variables by partial correlation indicate that the landscape
connectivity metrics respond to different aspects of
landscape structure as four different landscape variables were ranked highest for at least one landscape
connectivity metric (amount of habitat, number of
habitat patches, distance, habitat edge to area ratio;
Table 5). Furthermore, landscape variable rankings
could vary dramatically among landscape connectivity
metrics (e.g. number of habitat patches was ranked first
for patch visits and sixth for patch immigration). Relationships among landscape connectivity metrics were
not clear-cut, ranging from no relationship (Fig. 4a) to
linear (Fig. 4b) and curvilinear (Fig. 4c) relationships
that depended on the state of the landscape (e.g.
amount of habitat, number of habitat patches).
559
(3)
(4)
(6)
(2)
(5)
(1)
0.9393
−0.6217
0.4659
0.0041
−0.6898
0.2504
−0.7182
(4)
(6)
(5)
(1)
(2)
(3)
0.5802
0.2782
−0.1238
−0.1971
−0.6485
0.3322
0.2891
Average interpatch distance for patch based measures and average intercell distance for cell based measures.
a
0.9365
0.5714
0.0021
0.2955
−0.2934
−0.1751
−0.6657
R
Amount of habitat
Number of habitat patches
Habitat patch size distribution
Distancea
Variance in distance
Habitat edge to area ratio
0.8680
0.2107
−0.3013
0.0507
−0.7255
−0.6379
0.1808
(4)
(3)
(6)
(1)
(2)
(5)
0.8545
−0.6046
−0.0706
−0.2431
−0.3407
0.2427
0.1739
(1)
(6)
(3)
(2)
(4)
(5)
(5)
(1)
(4)
(2)
(3)
(6)
0.9418
0.0902
0.8161
−0.2890
−0.5446
0.3699
0.0682
(2)
(6)
(3)
(4)
(5)
(1)
Cell immigration
Patch immigration
Cell visits
Patch transition probability
Cell transition probability
Patch visits
2
Table 5. Multiple regression results for simulation experiment 1: habitat. R 2 values for the overall model and partial correlations for each of the landscape variables are presented
for six regression models, one for each landscape connectivity metric (n = 3584 for each model). Additionally, the partial correlations are ranked within each model (column) and
significant terms in the model (a= 0.05) are indicated with the ranking in bold.
560
Distance was the only landscape variable that behaved consistently across all landscape connectivity
measures, strongly and negatively influencing landscape
connectivity (though the effect was stronger for patch
level measures of landscape connectivity). The strength
and direction of the relationships between the remaining landscape variables and landscape connectivity
varied between connectivity metrics. Amount of habitat
was relatively important for all the cell level metrics
(magnitude of partial r’s of 0.57 to 0.62, Table 5) but
not for patch level connectivity metrics (magnitude of
partial r’s B0.28, Table 5). However, landscapes with
more habitat had more cell visits per individual but a
lower cell transition probability and lower cell immigration. The number of habitat patches in the landscape
had a strong, positive effect on patch visits but was
relatively unimportant for the remaining landscape connectivity metrics (and effects were both positive and
negative). Habitat edge to area ratio had strong, negative effects on the number of cell visits and cell immigration rate but weak, positive effects on the remaining
landscape connectivity metrics. Variance in interpatch
distance had a strong, negative effect on patch transition probability but had weaker, mixed effects on the
remaining landscape connectivity metrics. Finally, habitat patch size distribution had a consistently low partial
correlation across all landscape connectivity metrics
though direction of the effect varied among metrics.
Simulation experiment 2. Matrix
Matrix configuration (matrix dispersion, matrix patch
size distribution, and matrix edge to area ratio) had no
impact on any of the six measures of landscape connectivity (magnitude of all partial r’sB 0.02, Table 6).
Matrix composition (proportion of matrix as net) was
the only aspect of the matrix that had even a minimal
impact on landscape connectivity. For all six measures
of landscape connectivity increasing the amount of
netting in the matrix decreased landscape connectivity,
but this effect was weak in all cases (magnitude of all
partial r’s B0.2, Table 6), and with the exception of
patch immigration was always less important than the
habitat variables.
Habitat amount often affected landscape connectivity
in a direction opposite to its effect in simulation experiment 1. For cell visits and cell immigration the number
of habitat patches also had an opposite effect to their
effect in simulation experiment 1. For the matrix simulations increasing the amount of habitat decreased the
habitat edge to area ratio (r = − 0.91, n =9216) while
increasing the number of habitat patches increased the
edge to area ratio (r =0.39, n = 9216). This allowed the
effect of edge to area ratios to reverse the anticipated
effect of habitat amount or number of patches. In fact,
this hidden effect of habitat edge to area ratio was
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Fig. 4. Examples of relationships
between different connectivity
metrics for simulation
experiment 1: habitat.
a) Connectivity measured as cell
immigration vs connectivity
measured as patch immigration,
b) connectivity measured as cell
visits vs connectivity measured
as cell immigration, and
c) connectivity measured as
patch transition probability vs
connectivity measured as patch
immigration.
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
561
(2)
(1)
(3)
(4)
(6)
(5)
(7)
0.8727
0.8333
−0.8747
−0.5577
−0.1265
0.0045
−0.0062
−0.0026
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(7)
(6)
(5)
0.8789
−0.3014
−0.1229
−0.9369
−0.1962
0.0124
−0.0039
−0.0044
(2)
(4)
(1)
(3)
(5)
(7)
(6)
Simulation experiment 3 and field test
a
Average interpatch distance for patch based measures and average intercell distance for cell based measures.
0.9352
0.9613
−0.7519
−0.4880
−0.1346
0.0012
−0.0010
0.0016
(3)
(2)
(1)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
0.7910
−0.2153
0.5877
−0.8634
−0.1425
0.0112
−0.0049
−0.0044
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
(6)
(7)
(5)
0.7341
−0.8163
−0.5915
−0.2646
−0.0324
0.0057
0.0003
−0.0074
(3)
(2)
(1)
(4)
(5)
(6)
(7)
0.8563
−0.2313
−0.7312
−0.9163
−0.1306
0.0035
0.0012
0.0003
R2
Amount of habitat
Number of habitat patches
Distancea
Proportion of matrix as net
Matrix dispersion
Matrix patch size distribution
Matrix edge to area ratio
Cell immigration
Patch immigration
Cell visits
Patch visits
Cell transition probability
Patch transition probability
Table 6. Multiple regression results for simulation experiment 2: matrix. R 2 values for the overall model and partial correlations for each of landscape variables are presented for
six regression models, one for each landscape connectivity metric (n = 9216 for each model). Additionally, the partial correlations are ranked within each model (column) and
significant terms in the model (a= 0.05) are indicated with the ranking in bold.
562
strongest in the two landscape connectivity metrics
most sensitive to habitat edge to area ratios (cell visits
and cell immigration).
With 16% habitat, the model had a slight positive bias
when predicting average beetle displacement over time,
though most observed displacements in the field fell
within a 90% confidence envelope for the model predictions (Fig. 5). Cases where the observed mean displacement fell outside the 90% envelope all had low
resighting success, allowing for a few individuals that
had either not moved very far from the release point or
moved a long way from the release point to skew the
average displacement. Similar patterns held for 4%
habitat, though there were more cases of only a few
individuals being resighted. Model predictions for patch
immigration were significantly positively biased (x2 =
896.7, df= 63, p B0.0001). However, standardizing predicted and observed patch immigration by the predicted
or observed mean patch immigration, respectively, removed this bias (x2 = 31.05, df= 63, p = 0.998) suggesting that the model was correctly predicting the
direction of the response of connectivity to landscape
structure but not the absolute magnitude of the effect.
Furthermore, patch immigration responded in the same
direction in both the field and simulation experiment 3
to all four of the landscape variables (Fig. 6). A similar
pattern was observed for cell immigration. This suggests that though detection of immigration in the field
was somewhat limited this did not alter the observed
pattern of response to landscape structure. Additionally, patch immigration in these simulations responded
similarly to the landscape variables as in the other
simulations (compare Table 7 with Tables 5 and 6).
Together, this evidence suggests that the model represented the response of interpatch movements in T.
borealis to landscape structure well, while overestimating the absolute magnitude of patch immigration. Alternatively,
field
measurements
might
have
underestimated patch immigration.
Along with the independent effects of landscape variables on patch immigration, significant interaction
terms in the simulation analysis suggest that patch
immigration in landscapes with more habitat should be
more strongly influenced by the number of habitat
patches and more weakly influenced by interpatch distance while patch immigration in landscapes with more
habitat patches should be more weakly influenced by
interpatch distance (Table 7). For the field tests we first
removed the effects of two significant covariates, site
location and within-site landscape location, since all the
southerly sites and all the landscapes that were
southerly within their site had higher patch immigration. The ensuing ANOVA revealed the only significant
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Fig. 5. Observed average
beetle displacement from
point of release (symbols) and
expected average displacement
(solid line) along with 90%
confidence envelop (dashed
lines). Expected values are
based on 1000 model runs.
Results have been grouped
according to each unique
landscape arrangement (inset
figure, corresponding to Fig.
3). Unique symbols
differentiate between replicate
landscapes. Note different
scales on the displacement
axes.
aspects of landscape structure to be distance between
habitat patches and number of habitat patches. However,
with the exception of number of patches, all of the
landscape variables influenced patch immigration in the
same direction as they did in the simulation (Table 7).
Only the interaction between habitat amount and number
of patches acted in a different direction in the field tests
than the simulation.
Similarly, along with the independent effects of landscape variables on cell immigration, significant interaction terms suggest that cell immigration in landscapes
with more habitat should be less strongly influenced by
intercell distance and number of habitat patches. As with
patch immigration, in the empirical analysis we removed
the effects of the significant covariates, site location and
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
within-site location, since the southerly sites had higher
cell immigration and the southwest landscape at each site
had higher cell immigration. The ensuing ANOVA
revealed that number of habitat patches, distance between
patches, amount of netting in the matrix, the interaction
between amount of habitat and number of patches, and
the interaction between amount of habitat and distance
were significant and all of the landscape variables influenced cell immigration in the same direction as they did
in the simulations (Table 8). The only discrepancy
between the field tests and the simulations was a significant, negative effect of amount of netting on estimated
cell immigration in the field, which was not significant in
the simulations. However, in both cases netting had a
negative effect on estimated cell immigration.
563
Fig. 6. Box-whisker plots of estimated patch immigration for the simulation results (left panel of each pair, n = 320) and field
tests (right panel of each pair, n = 64). Plots have been classified by the four landscape variables: a) amount of habitat in the
landscape, b) whether the matrix was predominately cut or netting, c) number of habitat patches, and d) distance between
habitat patches. Whiskers represent the range, the boxes represent the interquartile range and the horizontal lines indicate the
median value.
Table 7. Results of ANOVA (single factors and all 2-way interactions) for the influence of landscape structure (Fig. 3) on
estimated patch immigration in simulated landscapes (n = 320) and field tests (n = 64). Effects for significant factors from either
the simulation or the empirical results (a = 0.05) are described.
Variable
Amount of habitat (PG)
Number of patches (NP)
Distance (DIST)
PG×NP
PG×DIST
NP×DIST
PN×DIST
Amount of netting (PN)
PG×PN
PN×NP
a
Field testsa
Simulations
F
p-level
408
311
126
83.4
19.6
18.8
1.54
1.14
0.80
0.02
0.0000
0.0000
0.0000
0.0000
0.0000
0.0000
0.22
0.29
0.37
0.88
Effect
PG
NP ¡
DIST
PG
PG ¡
NP ¡
immigration
immigration
¡ immigration
effect of NP
effect of DIST
effect of DIST
F
p-level
1.77
4.26
15.5
0.14
0.23
2.96
0.08
1.77
1.77
0.144
0.19
0.045
0.0003
0.71
0.64
0.092
0.78
0.19
0.19
0.71
Effect
PG
NP ¡
DIST
PG ¡
PG ¡
NP ¡
immigration
immigration
¡ immigration
effect of NP
effect of DIST
effect of DIST
After the removal of 2 significant covariates, site location and landscape location within site.
Discussion
The influence of spatial patterning of habitat on
landscape connectivity
Interpatch distance had the most consistent and, for
patch-level measures, strongest influence on landscape
connectivity. This is not a surprising finding, as other
researchers have noted that interpatch distance has a
strong effect on functional measures of connectivity
564
(Doak et al. 1992, Schippers et al. 1996, With and King
1999, Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000a). For the matrix
simulations, amount of habitat, number of habitat
patches (fragmentation), and interpatch distance (isolation) were varied essentially independently. Interpatch
distance always had the strongest, negative effect on
landscape connectivity; habitat amount and fragmentation were not always important and could affect landscape connectivity negatively or positively depending
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Table 8. Results of ANOVA (single factors and all 2-way interactions) for the influence of landscape structure (Fig. 3) on
estimated cell immigration in simulated landscapes (n = 320) and field tests (n =64). Effects for significant factors from either
the simulation or the empirical results (a = 0.05) are described.
Variable
Distance (DIST)
Number of patches (NP)
PG×DIST
PG×NP
PN×DIST
Amount of habitat (PG)
Amount of netting (PN)
PN×NP
NP×DIST
PG×PN
a
Field testsa
Simulations
F
p-level
110
75.6
6.63
2.67
2.65
2.26
2.20
0.20
0.07
0.03
0.0000
0.0000
0.011
0.10
0.10
0.13
0.14
0.66
0.79
0.87
Effect
DIST
NP ¡
PG ¡
PG ¡
PN
¡ immigration
immigration
effect of DIST
effect of NP
¡ immigration
F
p-level
3.42
8.98
10.8
21.1
0.14
1.46
8.89
1.50
1.38
2.39
0.071
0.004
0.002
0.0000
0.708
0.234
0.004
0.280
0.246
0.129
Effect
DIST
NP ¡
PG ¡
PG ¡
PN
¡ immigration
immigration
effect of DIST
effect of NP
¡ immigration
After the removal of 2 significant covariates, site location and landscape location within site.
on the connectivity metric. Ruckelshaus et al. (1997)
found that dispersal success increased as habitat increased and decreased as fragmentation increased
though both effects were weak. For search time and cell
immigration, Tischendorf and Fahrig (2000a) found
stronger correlations with mean nearest neighbour distance than the number of habitat patches, though they
found the opposite for dispersal success.
The terms habitat loss (reducing the amount of habitat) and fragmentation (increasing the number of
patches) have been used interchangeably in the literature, and many authors think of the two as being a
single process (Wilcox and Murphy 1985, Wilcove et al.
1986, Herben et al. 1991, Robinson et al. 1992, Perry
and Gonzalez-Andujar 1993, Diffendorfer et al. 1995,
Holt et al. 1995, Schumaker 1996, Donovan and
Flather 2002). It has been argued that the two should
be separated (Fahrig 1997, 1998, McGarigal and Cushman 2002, Schmigelow and Mönkkönen 2002). Trzcinski et al. (1999) and McGarigal and McComb (1995)
measured the independent effects of habitat (forest)
fragmentation and loss in actual landscapes and both
found that habitat loss had a much stronger effect than
fragmentation on bird species presence or abundance.
Using an individual-based, spatially explicit simulation
model, which separated the effects of habitat amount
and fragmentation, Fahrig (1997) found amount of
habitat had a greater effect than fragmentation on
extinction probability. Similarly, the landscape connectivity measures that we investigated responded differently to habitat loss and fragmentation, with most
influenced more strongly by amount of habitat than
habitat fragmentation, suggesting that the distinction
between amount and fragmentation of habitat can be
important.
Habitat fragmentation has two potential effects on
connectivity. Fragmentation will reduce average patch
size and increase the edge to area ratio. Smaller patches
may be more difficult for moving organisms to find
(Kareiva 1985, Capman et al. 1990, Matter 1996).
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Alternatively, the increased amount of edge may make
patches easier to find (Bowman et al. 2002). Additionally, fragmenting a fixed amount of habitat will tend to
decrease interpatch distances, since the small fragments
would be spread throughout the landscape, which in
turn would increase connectivity. Using a simulation
model Schumaker (1996) found that fragmentation
measured as the number of patches in a landscape was
only weakly correlated with dispersal success. After
accounting for effects due to isolation, in our simulations most measures of landscape connectivity decreased as the number of patches increased, suggesting
that for most measures of connectivity the response to
habitat fragmentation was more sensitive to patch finding success than gap crossing ability. But patch visits
and cell immigration increased with fragmentation, suggesting that these two measures of landscape connectivity were more sensitive to gap crossing ability than
patch finding success. With and King (1999) found a
similar effect where dispersal success was strongly
linked to landscape lacunarity, a measure of the gaps in
the landscape (Plotnick et al. 1993). This importance of
gap crossing was particularly evident with patch visits,
as number of patches was the landscape variable with
the strongest effect on landscape connectivity. Patch
visits increased with fragmentation because more
patches offer more opportunities for individuals to visit
new patches; fragmenting habitat therefore increases
the mean number of patches visited per individual.
Landscapes with more habitat should have higher
landscape connectivity because it will be easier for
moving individuals to find the greater amount of habitat in the landscape. This is the inherent assumption in
structural connectivity measures based on percolation
theory (Gardner et al. 1987, Gardner and O’Neill 1991,
Green 1994). Paradoxically, landscape connectivity
metrics based on between-patch movements should decline at high habitat amount because habitat will tend
to congeal into a single large patch precluding betweenpatch movements (Tischendorf and Fahrig 2000b).
565
Measuring landscape connectivity at the cell-level, particularly cell immigration, has been suggested as a
potential solution to this paradox (Tischendorf and
Fahrig 2000b). In our habitat simulations, cell immigration and cell transition probability strongly declined as
habitat amount increased, for two reasons. First, cell
immigration per habitat cell declined in landscapes with
more habitat because the same total number of immigration events were spread across more habitat cells,
therefore depressing the landscape-wide average. This is
confirmed by the fact that the mean number of cell
visits, which was not averaged over cells but individuals, increased as habitat amount increases. Second, in
our simulations based on T. borealis behaviour, individuals in habitat patches moved infrequently in short
bursts of slow movement producing tortuous paths
(Table 1, Goodwin and Fahrig 2002). Landscapes with
more habitat have more of such slow movements further reducing the ability of individuals to move between
habitat cells. In the habitat simulation experiment, individuals in landscapes with more habitat actually spent
less time moving (r = −0.96, n= 9216).
Changes to habitat configuration can also occur via
changes to patch size distributions (O’Neill et al. 1988,
Riitters et al. 1995, Gustafson 1998). Patch-based measures of landscape connectivity did not seem to respond
to habitat patch size variability, but cell-based measures
did. Number of cell visits and cell immigration rates
declined as patches became more uniform. In these
cases, when there were a few large patches individuals
could move more easily among many cells in that large
patch than they could move between habitat patches,
despite being slowed by their movement behaviour in
goldenrod. These within-patch movements increased
both the average number of habitat cells visited per
individual and the average number of immigrants per
habitat cell, for a given amount of habitat. This suggests that maintaining heterogeneity in the landscape,
especially regarding patch sizes, should increase landscape connectivity. In contrast, cell transition probability increased as patches became more uniform in size
and shape. This difference is most likely due to the cell
transition probability weighting each possible habitat
cell to habitat cell movement equally, such that any
isolated, single-cell habitat patch will contribute extremely low probabilities of exchanging individuals with
all the other habitat cells. When there are many single
cell habitat patches in the landscape they all contribute
low probabilities to the mean habitat cell transition
probability, thereby depressing the mean cell transition
probability over the entire landscape. Higher cell transition probabilities in the few large habitat patches will
not offset the effect of the single cell habitat patches as
only the transition probabilities for pairs of habitat
cells in the patch will be increased.
Anthropogenic habitat loss both decreases habitat
amount and increases habitat isolation. Both of these
566
landscape changes decrease landscape connectivity. It
has been suggested that the arrangement of habitat
could offset detrimental effects of habitat loss (Kareiva
and Wennergren 1995). The consistent negative response of all measures of landscape connectivity to
interpatch distance suggests that measures decreasing
isolation such as corridors (Merriam 1991, Noss 1993,
Rosenberg et al. 1998), stepping-stones (Potter 1990,
Arnold et al. 1993), relocation programs (McCullough
et al. 1996) or habitat restoration (Holland et al. 1991,
Fedorowick 1993), all have the potential to increase
landscape connectivity. However, the relatively weak
responses of the landscape connectivity measures to the
remaining landscape variables suggest that other aspects of habitat arrangement (e.g. patch shape, degree
of fragmentation) could not mitigate against detrimental effects of habitat loss. Furthermore, the fact that
many of the landscape connectivity measures responded
differently to some of the landscape variables suggests
that a particular arrangement of habitat patches might
have high landscape connectivity for some measures
while having low landscape connectivity for others
measures. This suggests that managing habitat arrangement to increase connectivity will not be straightforward and most likely will not be successful. This argues
primarily for the preservation of habitat and secondarily for managing habitat loss to minimize patch
isolation.
The influence of spatial patterning of matrix
elements on connectivity
To date there has been little work on the influence of
matrix composition and, particularly, matrix configuration on movements among habitat patches. There are
suggestions that matrix characteristics can be important
since different matrix elements have been shown to
function as barriers to movement (Mader 1984, Merriam et al. 1989, Baur 1990, Mader 1990, Kozakiewicz
1993), influence movement behaviour (Baars 1979, Crist
et al. 1992, Johnson et al. 1992, Matthysen et al. 1995,
Charrier et al. 1997, Pither and Taylor 1998, Jonsen
and Taylor 2000, Goodwin and Fahrig 2002) and influence movement risk (Krohne and Burgin 1987, Fahrig
et al. 1995, Schippers et al. 1996, Taylor and Merriam
1996, Charrier et al. 1997, Sakai and Noon 1997). It
would seem reasonable to expect landscape matrix pattern to affect landscape connectivity. At a minimum, as
the matrix becomes more difficult to move through, due
to increasing proportions of impervious matrix elements, landscape connectivity should decline (Knaapen
et al. 1992). In fact, theoretical formulations of the idea
of landscape connectivity seem to focus almost exclusively on the influence of the matrix (Merriam 1984,
Taylor et al. 1993). However, our simulations and field
studies found little effect of matrix composition and
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
no effect of matrix configuration on landscape
connectivity.
The simulations and field tests may have failed to
find much effect of matrix structure on landscape connectivity because matrix structure might influence connectivity through means other than movement
behaviour within matrix elements (Wiens et al. 1985).
Matrix structure may influence landscape connectivity
through its effects on edge crossing behaviour and/or
mortality, components that were not present in the
simulations or the T. borealis field test (earlier experiments found no response to edges; Goodwin and
Fahrig 2002). The way an organism responds to edges
influences patch residence times (Kareiva 1985, Turchin
1986, Buechner 1987, Stamps et al. 1987) and movement through corridors (Soulé and Gilpin 1991, Tischendorf and Wissel 1997), so one might expect that the
pattern of matrix patches that influence edge crossing
behaviour might affect landscape connectivity. Similarly, different risks of mortality in different matrix
elements should change landscape connectivity, since
movements will terminate with mortality events. While
not investigating landscape connectivity per se, using a
simulation experiment Fahrig (2001) found that varying
mortality risk in the matrix had a stronger influence on
extinction risk than habitat fragmentation, suggesting
that the importance of the matrix can be increased by
considering mortality. Further investigation of the necessary conditions for the landscape matrix to influence
connectivity, such as differential matrix mortality and
behavioural responses to patch edges, would be fruitful.
How should landscape connectivity be measured?
Our simulation and empirical experiments demonstrated that different potential measures of landscape
connectivity respond to different aspects of landscape
structure, and in some cases respond differently to the
same aspect of landscape structure. Currently there is
no broadly accepted metric of landscape connectivity
and many different metrics have been used in the
literature. Based on the fact that the six measures of
landscape connectivity we investigated responded to
different aspects of landscape structure, were weakly
correlated, and showed complex interactions, it is
highly unlikely that all the connectivity results in the
literature are directly comparable. The addition of more
metrics of connectivity to our analysis would not
change this conclusion.
This also suggests that landscape connectivity is a
poorly defined concept. If the investigator is constrained to mark-recapture or mark-resight approaches,
patch and cell immigration are the easiest to measure in
the field. However, patch immigration increases as the
number of patches increases. This increase is a reflection of how the metric is calculated and does not
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
indicate more movement in more fragmented landscapes. Also, the R 2 value for patch immigration was
the lowest, suggesting that patch immigration does not
respond as strongly to landscape structure as the other
measures of connectivity. Therefore, we recommend use
of cell immigration as the most practical and consistent
measure of landscape connectivity when using mark-recapture or mark-resight approaches. When using direct
measures of movement such as telemetry other metrics
of connectivity may be more appropriate.
It is worth mentioning, though, that in many cases
researchers are constrained to mark-recapture/resight
approaches. Many telemetry approaches are infeasible
for smaller taxa (but see Mascanzoni and Wallin 1986).
When they are feasible, most telemetry approaches are
so effort intensive that only a few individuals can be
followed. These restrictions make comparisons between
landscapes with different structure difficult. Additionally, many of the connectivity metrics that might be
applied to telemetry data have potential problems as
well. For example, the relationship between distance
travelled and landscape connectivity is not clear; short
distances could either indicate high connectivity due to
short traverses between habitat patches or poor connectivity due to individuals being unable to leave habitat
patches. It would be interesting to see how potential
connectivity metrics based on telemetry approaches
would change as landscape structure changes and how
such metrics would relate to the metrics we investigated
in this paper.
Conclusions
These results suggest three general points about the
relationship between landscape structure and the measures of landscape connectivity we investigated. First,
given that matrix patches differ only in their influence
on movement behaviour as in our system, the influence
of habitat spatial structure seems much more important
than the influence of matrix spatial structure on landscape connectivity. Second, all measures of landscape
connectivity declined when habitat patches were farther
apart. This suggests that mitigation of the effects of
landscape change on connectivity requires reduced interpatch distances. Overall, the general relationships
between landscape structure and landscape connectivity
warn that habitat loss should be avoided. Third, different measures of landscape connectivity respond differently to landscape structure. Based on what is easily
measured in the field we recommend cell immigration
as a good measure of landscape connectivity.
Acknowledgements – We would like to thank Darren Bender
and Dave Omond for help in the field and Naomi Cappuccino,
David Currie, Eric Gustafson and Phil Taylor for commenting
on and improving earlier versions of the manuscript. Funding
was provided by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research
567
Council of Canada (NSERC) scholarship and an Ontario
Graduate Scholarship to BJG and a NSERC research grant to
LF.
References
Andreassen, H. P., Halle, S. and Ims, R. A. 1996. Optimal
width of movement corridors for root voles: not too narrow and not too wide. – J. Appl. Ecol. 33: 63 – 70.
Arnold, G. W., Steven, D. E., Weeldenburg, J. R. and Smith,
E. A. 1993. Influences of remnant size, spacing pattern and
connectivity on population boundaries and demography in
Euros Macropus robustus living in a fragmented landscape.
– Biol. Conserv. 64: 219 – 230.
Baars, M. A. 1979. Patterns of movement of radioactive
carabid beetles. – Oecologia 44: 125 – 140.
Batschelet, E. 1981. Circular statistics in biology. – Academic
Press.
Baur, B. 1990. Are roads barriers to dispersal in the land snail?
– Can. J. Zool. 68: 613 – 617.
Bowman, J., Cappuccino, N. and Fahrig, L. 2002. Patch size
and population density: the effect of immigration behavior.
– Conserv. Ecol. 6: 9. [online] URL: http://
www.consecol.org/vol6/iss1/art9.
Brooker, L., Brooker, M. and Cale, P. 1999. Animal dispersal
in fragmented habitat: measuring habitat connectivity, corridor use, and dispersal mortality. – Conserv. Ecol. 3: 4.
Available
from
the
Internet.
URL:
http://
www.consecol.org/vol3/iss1/art4.
Brown, J. H. and Kodric-Brown, A. 1977. Turnover rates in
insular biogeography: effect of immigration on extinction.
– Ecology 58: 445 – 449.
Buechner, M. 1987. Conservation in insular parks: simulation
models of factors affecting the movements of animals
across park boundaries. – Biol. Conserv. 41: 57 – 76.
Capman, W. C., Batzli, G. O. and Simms, L. E. 1990. Responses of the sooty wing skipper to patches of host plants.
– Ecology 71: 1430 – 1440.
Charrier, S., Petit, S. and Burel, F. 1997. Movements of Abax
parallelepipedus (Coleoptera, Carabidae) in woody habitats
of a hedgerow network landscape-a radio-tracing study. –
Agric. Ecosys. Environ. 61: 133 – 144.
Collinge, S. K. 2000. Effects of grassland fragmentation on
insect species loss, colonization, and movement patterns. –
Ecology 81: 2211 – 2226.
Crist, T. O., Guertin, D. S., Wiens, J. A. and Milne, B. T.
1992. Animal movement in heterogenous landscapes: an
experiment with Eleodes beetles in shortgrass prairie. –
Funct. Ecol. 6: 536 – 544.
Demers, M. N., Simpson, J. W., Boerner, R. E. J. et al. 1995.
Fencerows, edges, and implications of changing connectivity illustrated by two contiguous Ohio landscapes. – Conserv. Biol. 9: 1159 – 1168.
Diffendorfer, J. E., Gaines, M. S. and Holt, R. D. 1995.
Habitat fragmentation and movements of three small
mammals (Sigmodon, Microtus, and Peromyscus). – Ecology 76: 827 – 839.
Doak, D. F., Marino, P. C. and Kareiva, P. M. 1992. Spatial
scale mediates the influence of habitat fragmentation on
dispersal success: implications for conservation. – Theor.
Popul. Biol. 41: 315 – 336.
Donovan, T. M. and Flather, C. H. 2002. Relationships
among North American songbird trends, habitat fragmentation, and landscape occupancy. – Ecol. Appl. 12: 364 –
374.
Fahrig, L. 1997. Relative effects of habitat loss and fragmentation on population extinction. – J. Wildl. Manag. 61:
603 – 610.
Fahrig, L. 1998. When does fragmentation of breeding habitat
affect population survival? – Ecol. Model. 105: 273 – 292.
568
Fahrig, L. 2001. How much habitat is enough? – Biol. Conserv. 100: 65 – 74.
Fahrig, L. and Merriam, G. 1985. Habitat patch connectivity
and population survival. – Ecology 66: 1762 –1768.
Fahrig, L., Pedlar, J. H., Pope, S. E. et al. 1995. Effect of road
traffic on amphibian density. – Biol. Conserv. 73: 177 –
182.
Fedorowick, J. L. 1993. A landscape restoration framework
for wildlife and agriculture in the rural landscape. – Landscape Urban Planning 27: 7 – 17.
Gardner, R. H. and O’Neill, R. V. 1991. Pattern, process and
predictability: the use of neutral models for landscape
analysis. – In: Turner, M. G. and Gardner, R. H. (eds),
Quantitative methods in landscape ecology. Springer, pp.
289 – 307.
Gardner, R. H., Milne, B. T., Turner, M. G. and O’Neill, R.
V. 1987. Neutral models for the analysis of broad-scale
landscape pattern. – Landscape Ecol. 1: 19 – 28.
Goodwin, B. J. 2000. Landscape connectivity: The interaction
between insect movements and landscape spatial structure.
– Ph.D. thesis, Carleton Univ.
Goodwin, B. J. and Fahrig, L. 2002. Effect of landscape
structure on the movement behaviour of a specialized
goldenrod beetle, Trirhabda borealis. – Can. J. Zool. 80:
25 – 34.
Green, D. G. 1994. Connectivity and complexity in landscapes
and ecosystems. – Pacific Conserv. Biol. 1: 194 – 200.
Groom, M. J. and Schumaker, N. 1993. Evaluating landscape
change: patterns of worldwide deforestation and local fragmentation. – In: Kareiva, P. M., Kingsolver, J. G. and
Huey, R. B. (eds), Biotic interactions and global change.
Sinauer Associates Inc., pp. 24 – 44.
Gustafson, E. J. 1998. Quantifying landscape spatial pattern:
what is state of the art? – Ecosystems 1: 143 – 156.
Gustafson, E. J. and Gardner, R. H. 1996. The effect of
landscape heterogeneity on the probability of patch colonization. – Ecology 77: 94 – 107.
Hanski, I., Ahlo, J. and Moilanen, A. 2000. Estimating the
parameters of survival and migration of individuals in
metapopulations. – Ecology 81: 239 – 251.
Henderson, M. T., Merriam, G. and Wegner, J. 1985. Patchy
environments and species survival: chipmunks in an agricultural mosaic. – Biol. Conserv. 31: 95 – 105.
Herben, T., Rydin, H. and Soderstrom, L. 1991. Spore establishment probability and the persistence of the fugitive
invading moss, Orthodontium lineare: a spatial simulation
model. – Oikos 60: 215 – 221.
Hof, J. and Flather, C. H. 1996. Accounting for connectivity
and spatial correlation in the optimal placement of wildlife
habitat. – Ecol. Model. 88: 143 – 155.
Hof, J. and Raphael, M. G. 1997. Optimization of habitat
placement – a case study of the northern spotted owl in
the Olympic Peninsula. – Ecol. Appl. 7: 1160 – 1169.
Holland, M. M., Risser, P. G. and Naiman, R. J. (eds) 1991.
Ecotones: the role of landscape boundaries on the management and restoration of changing environments. – Chapman & Hall.
Holt, R. D., Robinson, G. R. and Gaines, M. S. 1995.
Vegetation dynamics in an experimentally fragmented
landscape. – Ecology 76: 1610 – 1624.
Johnson, A. R., Milne, B. T. and Wiens, J. A. 1992. Diffusion
in fractal landscapes: simulations and experimental studies
of tenebrionid beetle movements. – Ecology 73: 1968 –
1993.
Jonsen, I. D. and Taylor, P. D. 2000. Fine-scale movement
behaviors of calopterygid damselflies are influenced by
landscape structure: an experimental manipulation. –
Oikos 88: 553 – 562.
Kareiva, P. 1985. Finding and losing host plants by Phyllotreta: patch size and surrounding habitat. – Ecology 66:
1809 – 1816.
Kareiva, P. and Wennergren, V. 1995. Connecting landscape
patterns to ecosystem and population process. – Nature
373: 299 – 302.
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Keitt, T. H., Urban, D. L. and Milne, B. T. 1997. Detecting
critical scales in fragmented landscapes. – Conserv. Ecol. 1:
4. Available from the Internet. URL: http://
www.consecol.org/vol1/iss1/art4.
Knaapen, J. P., Scheffer, M. and Harms, B. 1992. Estimating
habitat isolation in landscape planning. – Landscape Urban
Planning 23: 1 – 16.
Kozakiewicz, M. 1993. Habitat isolation and ecological barriers
– the effect on small mammal populations and communities.
– Acta Therrologica 38: 1 – 30.
Kozakiewicz, M. 1995. Resource tracking in space and time. –
In: Hansson, L., Fahrig, L. and Merriam, G. (eds), Mosaic
landscapes and ecological processes. Chapman & Hall, pp.
136 – 148.
Krohne, D. T. and Burgin, A. B. 1987. Relative success of
residents and immigrants in Peromyscus leucopus. – Holarct. Ecol. 10: 196 – 200.
Laan, R. and Verboom, B. 1990. Effects of pool size and
isolation on amphibian communities. – Biol. Conserv. 54:
251 – 262.
Mader, H. 1984. Animal habitat isolation by roads and agricultural fields. – Biol. Conserv. 29: 81 – 96.
Mader, H. J. 1990. Linear barriers to arthropod movement in
the landscape. – Biol. Conserv. 54: 209 – 222.
Mascanzoni, D. and Wallin, H. 1986. The harmonic radar: a
new method of tracing insects. – Ecol. Entomol. 11: 387 –
390.
Matter, S. F. 1996. Interpatch movement of the red milkweed
beetle, Tetraopes tetraophthalmus: individual responses to
patch size and isolation. – Oecologia 105: 447 –453.
Matthysen, E., Adriaensen, F. and Dhondt, A. A. 1995.
Dispersal distances of nuthatches, Sitta europea, in a highly
fragmented forest habitat. – Oikos 72: 375 –381.
McCauley, D. E. 1989. Extinction, colonization, and population structure: a study of milkweed beetles. – Am. Nat. 134:
365 – 376.
McCullough, D. R., Fischer, J. K. and Ballou, J. D. 1996. From
bottleneck to metapopulation: recovery of the tule elk in
California. – In: McCullough, D. R. (ed.), Metapopulations
and wildlife conservation. Island Press, Washington, pp.
375 – 403.
McGarigal, K. and Cushman, S. A. 2002. Comparative evaluation of experimental approaches to the study of habitat
fragmentation effects. – Ecol. Appl. 12: 335 – 345.
McGarigal, K. and McComb, W. C. 1995. Relationships
between landscape structure and breeding birds in the
Oregon coast range. – Ecol. Monogr. 65: 235 – 260.
Merriam, G. 1984. Connectivity: a fundamental ecological
characteristic of landscape pattern. – In: Brandt, J. and
Agger, P. (eds), Proc. First Int. Sem. Methodology in
Landscape Ecol. Res. and Planning. Vol. I. Roskilde Universitessforlag GeoRue, Roskilde, Denmark, pp. 5 –15.
Merriam, G. 1991. Corridors and connectivity: animal populations in heterogeneous environments. – In: Saunders, D. A.
and Hobbs, R. J. (eds), Nature conservation 2: The Role of
Corridors. Surrey Beatty & Sons, Chipping Norton, NSW,
pp. 133–142.
Merriam, G., Kozakiewicz, M., Tsuchiya, E. and Hawley, K.
1989. Barriers as boundaries for metapopulations and
demes of Peromyscus leucopus in farm landscapes. – Landscape Ecol. 2: 227 – 235.
Messina, F. J. 1982. Timing of dispersal and ovarian development in goldenrod leaf beetles Trirhabda 6irgata and T.
borealis. – Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 74: 78– 83.
Metzger, J.-P. and Décamps, H. 1997. The structural connectivity threshold: an hypothesis in conservation biology at the
landscape scale. – Acta Oecol. 18: 1 – 12.
Noss, R. F. 1993. Wildlife corridors. – In: Smith, D. S. and
Hellmund, P. C. (eds), Ecology of greenways: design and
function of linear conservation areas. Univ. of Minnesota
Press, pp. 43 –68.
O’Neill, R. V., Krummel, J. R., Gardner, R. H. et al. 1988.
Indices of landscape pattern. – Landscape Ecol. 1: 153 – 162.
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)
Perry, J. N. and Gonzalez-Andujar, J. L. 1993. Dispersal in a
metapopulation neighbourhood model of an annual plant
with a seedbank. – J. Ecol. 81: 453 – 463.
Pither, J. and Taylor, P. D. 1998. An experimental assessment
of landscape connectivity. – Oikos 83: 166 – 174.
Plotnick, R. E., Gardner, R. H. and O’Neill, R. V. 1993.
Lacunarity indices as measures of landscape texture. –
Landscape Ecol. 8: 201 –211.
Potter, M. A. 1990. Movement of North Island brown kiwi
(Apteryx australis mantelli ) between forest remnants. – N.
Z. J. Ecol. 14: 17 – 24.
Riitters, K. H., O’Neill, R. V., Hunsaker, C. T. et al. 1995. A
factor analysis of landscape pattern and structure metrics.
– Landscape Ecol. 10: 23 – 39.
Robinson, G. R., Holt, R. D., Gaines, M. S. et al. 1992. Diverse
and contrasting effects of habitat fragmentation. – Science
257: 524 – 526.
Rosenberg, D. K., Noon, B. R., Megahan, J. W. and Meslow,
E. C. 1998. Compensatory behaviour of Ensatina eschscholtzii in biological corridors: a field experiment. – Can.
J. Zool. 76: 117 – 133.
Ruckelshaus, M., Hartway, C. and Kareiva, P. M. 1997.
Assessing the data requirements of spatially explicit dispersal models. – Conserv. Biol. 11: 1298 –1306.
Sakai, H. F. and Noon, B. R. 1997. Between-habitat movement
of dusky-footed woodrats and vulnerability to predation. –
J. Wildl. Manag. 61: 343 – 350.
Saunders, D. A., Hobbs, R. J. and Arnold, G. W. 1993. The
Kellerberrin project on fragmented landscapes: a review of
current information. – Biol. Conserv. 64: 185 –192.
Schippers, P., Verboom, J., Knappen, J. P. and van Apeldorn,
R. C. 1996. Dispersal and habitat connectivity in complex
heterogeneous landscapes: an analysis with GIS-based random walk model. – Ecography 19: 97 – 106.
Schmigelow, F. K. A. and Mönkkönen, M. 2002. Habitat loss
and fragmentation in dynamic landscapes: avian perspectives from the boreal forest. – Ecol. Appl. 12: 375 – 389.
Schumaker, N. 1996. Using landscape indices to predict habitat
connectivity. – Ecology 77: 1210 –1225.
Sjögren, P. 1991. Extinction and isolation gradients in metapopulations: the case of the pool frog (Rana lessonae). – Biol.
J. Linn. Soc. 42: 135 – 147.
Soulé, M. E. and Gilpin, M. E. 1991. The theory of wildlife
corridor capability. – In: Saunders, D. A. and Hobbs, R.
J. (eds), Nature conservation 2: the role of corridors. Surrey
Beatty & Sons, Chipping Norton, NSW, pp. 3 – 8.
St Clair, C. C., Belisle, M., Desrochers, A. and Hannon, S.
1998. Winter responses of forest birds to habitat corridors
and gaps. – Conserv. Ecol. 2: 13. Available from the
internet. URL: http://www.consecol.org/vol2/iss2/art13.
Stamps, J. A., Buechner, M. and Krishnan, V. V. 1987. The
effects of edge permeability and habitat geometry on emigration from patches of habitat. – Am. Nat. 129: 533 – 552.
Taylor, P. D. and Merriam, G. 1996. Habitat fragmentation
and parasitism of a forest damselfly. – Landscape Ecol. 11:
181 – 189.
Taylor, P. D., Fahrig, L., Henein, K. and Merriam, G. 1993.
Connectivity is a vital element of landscape structure. –
Oikos 68: 571 – 573.
Thomas, C. D. 1994. Extinction, colonization, and metapopulations: environmental tracking by rare species. – Conserv.
Biol. 8: 373 – 378.
Tiebout, H. M. and Anderson, R. A. 1997. A comparison of
corridors and intrinsic connectivity to promote dispersal in
transient successional landscapes. – Conserv. Biol. 11:
620 – 627.
Tischendorf, L. 2001. Can landscape indices predict ecological
processes consistently? – Landscape Ecol. 16: 235 – 254.
Tischendorf, L. and Fahrig, L. 2000a. How should we measure
landscape connectivity? – Landscape Ecol. 15: 633 – 641.
Tischendorf, L. and Fahrig, L. 2000b. On the usage and
measurement of landscape connectivity. – Oikos 90: 7 –19.
569
Tischendorf, L. and Wissel, C. 1997. Corridors as conduits for
small animals: attainable distances depending on movement
pattern, boundary reaction and corridor width. – Oikos 79:
603 – 611.
Trzcinski, M. K., Fahrig, L. and Merriam, G. 1999. Independent effects of forest cover and fragmentation on the
distribution of forest breeding birds. – Ecol. Appl. 9:
586 – 593.
Turchin, P. 1986. Modelling the effect of host patch size on
Mexican bean beetle emigration. – Ecology 67: 124 – 132.
Turner, M. G. and Ruscher, C. L. 1988. Changes in landscape
patterns in Georgia, USA. – Landscape Ecol. 1: 241 – 251.
Vos, C. C. and Stumpel, H. P. 1995. Comparison of habitat-isolation parameters in relation to fragmented distribution
patterns in the tree frog (Hyla arborea). – Landscape Ecol.
11: 203 – 214.
Wiens, J. A., Crawford, C. S. and Gosz, J. R. 1985. Boundary
dynamics: a conceptual framework for studying landscape
ecosystems. – Oikos 45: 421 – 427.
Wilcove, D. S., McLellan, C. H. and Dobson, A. P. 1986.
Habitat fragmentation in the temperate zone. – In: Soulé,
570
M. E. (ed.), Conservation biology: the science of scarcity
and diversity. Sinauer Associates, Inc., pp. 237 – 256.
Wilcox, B. A. and Murphy, D. D. 1985. Conservation strategy:
the effects of fragmentation on extinction. – Am. Nat. 125:
879 – 887.
With, K. A. and Crist, T. O. 1995. Critical thresholds in species’
responses to landscape structure. – Ecology 76: 2446 –
2459.
With, K. A. and King, A. W. 1999. Dispersal success on fractal
landscapes: a consequence of lacunarity thresholds. – Landscape Ecol. 14: 73 – 82.
With, K. A., Gardner, R. H. and Turner, M. G. 1997.
Landscape connectivity and population distributions in
heterogeneous environments. – Oikos 78: 151 – 169.
With, K. A., Cadaret, S. J. and Davis, C. 1999. Movement
responses to patch structure in experimental fractal landscapes. – Ecology 80: 1340 – 1353.
Zollner, P. A. and Lima, S. L. 1999. Search strategies for
landscape-level interpatch movements. – Ecology 80: 1019 –
1030.
OIKOS 99:3 (2002)