Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
Acoustics and psychosocial environment in
intensive coronary care
V Blomkvist, C A Eriksen, T Theorell, R Ulrich and G Rasmanis
Occup. Environ. Med. 2005;62;1doi:10.1136/oem.2004.017632
Updated information and services can be found at:
http://oem.bmjjournals.com/cgi/content/full/62/3/e1
These include:
References
Rapid responses
Email alerting
service
Topic collections
This article cites 27 articles, 2 of which can be accessed free at:
http://oem.bmjjournals.com/cgi/content/full/62/3/e1#BIBL
You can respond to this article at:
http://oem.bmjjournals.com/cgi/eletter-submit/62/3/e1
Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article - sign up in the box at the
top right corner of the article
Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections
Occupational Health (1143 articles)
Critical Care / Intensive Care (272 articles)
Notes
To order reprints of this article go to:
http://www.bmjjournals.com/cgi/reprintform
To subscribe to Occupational and Environmental Medicine go to:
http://www.bmjjournals.com/subscriptions/
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
1 of 8
ELECTRONIC PAPER
Acoustics and psychosocial environment in intensive
coronary care
V Blomkvist, C A Eriksen, T Theorell, R Ulrich, G Rasmanis
...............................................................................................................................
Occup Environ Med 2005;62:e1 (http://www.occenvmed.com/cgi/content/full/62/3/e1). doi: 10.1136/oem.2004.017632
See end of article for
authors’ affiliations
.......................
Correspondence to:
Dr V Blomkvist,
Department of Public
Health Care, Uppsala
University, Sweden; vanja.
[email protected]
Accepted
19 November 2004
.......................
T
Background: Stress, strain, and fatigue at the workplace have previously not been studied in relation to
acoustic conditions.
Aims: To examine the influence of different acoustic conditions on the work environment and the staff in a
coronary critical care unit (CCU).
Method: Psychosocial work environment data from start and end of each individual shift were obtained
from three shifts (morning, afternoon, and night) for a one-week baseline period and for two four-week
periods during which either sound reflecting or sound absorbing tiles were installed.
Results: Reverberation times and speech intelligibility improved during the study period when the ceiling
tiles were changed from sound reflecting tiles to sound absorbing ones of identical appearance. Improved
acoustics positively affected the work environment; the afternoon shift staff experienced significantly lower
work demands and reported less pressure and strain.
Conclusions: Important gains in the psychosocial work environment of healthcare can be achieved by
improving room acoustics. The study points to the importance of further research on possible effects of
acoustics in healthcare on staff turnover, quality of patient care, and medical errors.
he potential for evidence based design of healthcare
physical environments to improve clinical and economic
outcomes has attracted increasing attention as healthcare providers internationally have faced strong pressures
to reduce costs yet increase care quality. Many studies show
that the physical environment of healthcare facilities
influences not only patient satisfaction but also outcomes
such as pain and infection occurrence.1–5 However, few
studies deal with effects of the physical design of healthcare on staff outcomes like job stress, work demands,
fatigue, and quality of patient care.6 One research area
illustrating this choice of focus is effects of noise in a hospital
setting.
The need for research to inform the creation of better
healthcare work spaces has never been greater or more time
urgent. Several countries, including the United States and
Britain, are embarking on vast programmes of healthcare
building construction impelled by the aging of the populations and the technological obsolescence of older hospitals.
The United States, for example, will spend more than $16
billion for hospital construction in 2004, and this will
increase to more than $20 billion annually by 2010.7 As a
step towards addressing the urgent need for research on
healthcare work spaces, the present study examined the
influences of environmental acoustics on the psychosocial
work environment in a coronary critical care unit (CCU) in a
Swedish hospital.
Guideline values for continuous background noise in
hospital wards are 30 dB LAeq, with night-time peaks at 40
dB LAmax. These guidelines further recommend that the
continuous sound pressure level should not exceed 35 dB
LAeq in patient rooms, including those in which patients are
being treated.8 Guidelines notwithstanding, several studies
have found that hospital background sound pressure levels
typically fall in much higher ranges—45–68 db(A)—with
peaks exceeding 85–90 db(A).9–11 Medical equipment and
staff voices frequently produce 70 db(A) sound levels
measured at the patient’s head, which approach decibel
levels in a busy restaurant or urban street.
Most findings suggest that higher db(A) levels detrimentally affect at least some patient outcomes, for example,
increasing heart rate and producing sleeplessness.10 12 13
Even when decibel levels are kept relatively low (27–58
db(A)), however, differences in acoustic properties with
respect to reverberation times or echo/liveliness characteristics may be linked with variations in sleep quality as
measured by EEG. In this regard, Berg14 found that
volunteers evidenced improved sleep quality (less sleep
fragmentation) when assigned to hospital patient rooms
with shorter reverberation times (produced by sound
absorbing ceiling tiles), in contrast to when assigned to
rooms with longer reverberation times (sound reflecting
tiles).
Studies concerning effects of noise on healthcare staff are
scarce. There is some evidence that staff perceive higher
sound levels in patient units as stressful.15 16 Also, nurses’
self-reports of noise induced stress correlate with selfreported emotional exhaustion.17 No previous research has
addressed the focus of the present study—that is, the
influence of differences in reverberation time on the
psychosocial work environment. Many studies performed in
laboratories and non-healthcare workplaces,18–20 however,
have contributed to our knowledge of the effects of noise
on stress and health, and have implications for research
on acoustics and the psychosocial work environment in
healthcare.
Noise is widely defined in the research literature as
‘‘unwanted sound’’, an interpretation that implies the
importance of controllability and predictability in understanding the effects of sound on people.18–20 The emphasis on
controllability means that whether or not sound is negative
or stressful is largely a matter of psychology. For instance,
detrimental psychological and physiological (heart rate,
blood pressure, skin conductance, respiration rate) effects
have been observed in laboratory studies when normal
subjects are exposed to transient, uncontrollable sounds
having intensity ranges comparable to those measured in
certain healthcare settings (peak ranges of 75–85 dB(A)).17
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
2 of 8
Main messages
N
N
N
N
N
N
It seems likely that improved acoustic conditions reduce
risks of conflicts and errors.
Reverberation times and speech intelligibility improved
during the study period when the ceiling tiles were
changed from sound reflecting tiles to sound absorbing
ones of identical appearance.
For each individual shift the delta score was computed,
indicating the accumulated effect of working conditions.
Improved acoustics positively affected the work environment.
Particularly during the afternoon shift the staff reported
lower work demands and decreased pressure and
strain.
Important gains in the psychosocial environment of
healthcare can be achieved by improving room
acoustics.
Other laboratory research indicates that uncontrollable
sounds that interfere with tasks or activities elicit negative
changes in emotions.21
The psychological effects of noise may trigger behavioural
aberration in healthy adults. Mental activities, such as
sustained attention to multiple cues or complex analysis,
are all directly sensitive to noise. Accidents may be indicators
of noise related effects on performance. Thus, one critical
effect of noise may be communication interference, including
interference with warning signals.8 22 23
If the work organisation is bad with excessive demands
combined with low decision latitude and poor support, sound
reflecting acoustics (for example, excessive noise levels and
long reverberation time) are likely to have more pronounced
negative effects.25 One of the most widely used theoretical
models in psychosocial environmental research is the
demand control support model.24 It has been used for
demonstrating that adverse combinations of high demands,
low decision latitude (possibility for employees to exert
control in the work situation) and poor social support from
superiors and workmates give rise to physiological arousal
reactions with energy mobilisation (resulting for instance
in blood pressure elevation). Energy mobilisation is also
followed by inhibition of regenerative activity in the body
reflected for instance in decreasing blood concentration of
anabolic hormones. If such physiological reactions are
pronounced and long lasting they may increase illness risks.
Demands, decision latitude, and social support are contingent
on organisational factors, although the individual’s perception also influences employee assessments. There are several
epidemiological studies indicating that exposure to a combination of high psychological demands and low decision
latitude increases the risk of developing myocardial infarction
in working age even when adjustment for conventional risk
factors has been made.26
Physiological correlates of job strain could be lowered as
well as raised morning cortisol, raised IgE and interleukin 6
(both indicators of raised activity in the immune system),
and disturbed blood pressure regulation.27 28 Studies have
found connections between physical reactions such as
musculoskeletal disorders and psychosocial environment,
both due to the organisation and the relation to patients.29
Experiences of stress in intensive care units create conflicts
for nurses, and the consequences may exceed the psychosocial and emotional resources of the staff.30
www.occenvmed.com
Electronic paper
Policy implications
N
N
N
There has been very little research on the influences of
acoustics on healthcare staff.
The study points at the importance of further research
on possible effects of acoustics in healthcare on staff
turnover, quality of care, and medical errors.
The findings imply that an approach for improving
healthcare acoustics will be inadequate if it focuses
narrowly on reducing sound pressure levels.
The possibility of medical staff communicating effectively
is potentially of great importance. When acoustic conditions
are characterised by longer in contrast to shorter reverberation times, echoes (reverberant sounds) will cause blending
and overlapping of sounds, thereby reducing speech intelligibility.31 Communication difficulties may also elicit irritation
in staff, who may become less empathic to patients.32
The aim of this study was to explore the influence of room
acoustics on the psychosocial work environment in coronary
care. It was expected that an environmental condition of
longer reverberation time would increase sound propagation
and sound levels in the CCU, reduce speech intelligibility, and
negatively affect the psychosocial environment. Conversely, it
was expected that improved acoustics could lead to an
improved perception of the work environment, with reduced
feeling of load.
STUDY GROUP AND METHODS
The medical setting
Huddinge University Hospital has a catchment area of 375
000 inhabitants. The Department of Cardiology has a full
service cardiac programme and serves as a referral clinic for
two hospitals regarding angioplasty, electrophysiology, and
thoracic surgery. The centre of the department is the coronary
care unit (CCU) with eight beds, which is where the study
took place (see fig 1). The comparatively small number of
beds consequently enhances the turnover of patients in the
unit; 6–7 patients with acute symptoms are daily admitted to
the unit and the average observations time is 17 hours.
Patients with unstable coronary heart disease are transferred
after the initial observation period, to a step-down ward
located beside the CCU. Patients with a less severe disease are
transferred to a general cardiac ward of the opposite side of
the CCU.
Both units are equipped with continuous ECG monitoring,
all of which is controlled on screens in the CCU. All patients
are monitored with a computerised system for ECG and/or
haemodynamics, with different automatic alarms for critical
values. Patients are continuously, when needed, transported
to the laboratories for angiography, electrophysiology, or CT
scanning. Laboratory testing is partly performed in a setting
in a corner of the central area. This and other logistics, such
as regular cleaning of the patient rooms, exchange of beds
and laundry among other things, give the unit a noisy and
somewhat turbulent atmosphere.
There were 31 patients (12 women) in the period of sound
reflective ceilings and 44 (13 women) in the period of sound
absorbing ceilings. Mean age in the sound reflective period
was 67.45 years and was 67.52 years in the sound absorbing
period. The patients’ diagnoses and level of severity were
comparable between the periods.
Study group
Nearly 50 nurses, all specially trained in cardiology, are
employed at the unit, scheduled either for morning,
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
Electronic paper
3 of 8
Outdoors
Room 1
1 patient
Room 2
1 patient
Room 3
1 patient
Room 5
1 patient
Room 4
1 patient
Corridor
Room 6
1 patient
Corridor
Central
Nurse's
Station
Room
not included
in study
(no windows)
0
1
Figure 1
2
3
4
5
10
Room 7
1 patient
Outdoor
area
20 m
Floor plan of the CCU.
afternoon, or night shifts. The 36 nurses working regularly at
the ward were asked to participate in the investigation of the
psychosocial environment and emotional states at the start
and end of each individual work shift. The members of the
staff were informed of the changes and the aim of the project
before the baseline study took place.
Twenty one nurses worked daytime and 15 worked nighttime shifts. Ages varied between 24 and 53 years (mean age
35 years). The daytime workers were scheduled either in a
morning shift or an afternoon shift.
Procedure
After an initial baseline period the acoustics were changed in
two steps in the patient rooms and the main work area of the
unit where most decisions are made and monitoring of the
patients takes place.
In the first step, remodelling of the ceiling took place,
creating a sound reflective ceiling surface in the entire ward.
In the second step, sound absorbing ceilings were installed
throughout the ward.
The baseline period lasted for three weeks in November
2001. This period should be regarded as a ‘‘feasibility period’’
during which procedures and questionnaires were tested. The
first part of the main study (sound reflecting period) was
carried out during 20 weekdays—from the beginning of
February until the beginning of March 2002. The second part
(sound absorbing period) lasted from March during 22
weekdays until the middle of April 2001. Data for the project
were collected during regular weekdays and corresponding
nights, but not during weekends since there were changes in
staffing and conditions during such periods. To promote
candour and validity when filling out the questionnaire, each
nurse received a numerical ID which was used instead of
their names.
Questionnaires with psychosocial items were distributed
and filled out on two occasions during the shift—at the
beginning of the work shift and at the very end. The
questions describing work situation were pace of work (high
or low tempo), quantity of work (great or small), decision
latitude (high or low), own competence (high or low), own
hard decisions (seldom or often), self-determination (a lot or
a little), information/education (a lot or a little), atmosphere
at work (calm or unsettling), quality of care (easy to prioritise or hard to prioritise), and social support at work (good
or poor). These questions correspond roughly to a condensed
visual analogue version of the demand-control-support
model, Swedish version.30 The questions describing mood
states were hastiness, calmness, irritation, anxiety, tension,
happiness, sadness, anger, depression, stress, and fatigue. A
longer version of this selection of adjectives has been used in
previous research.34 Analyses were performed with the
difference between ‘‘end’’ and ‘‘start’’ score (‘‘delta’’ values).
The rationale behind the analysis of the delta values was that
the accumulated effect of the exposure of working conditions
would be particularly visible in the comparison between the
start and the end levels. Another way of expressing this is
that the delta value shows the ‘‘netto’’ effect. A negative delta
score indicates a lower score at the end of the shift than at the
beginning.
All psychosocial questions were measured with visual
analogue scales.
A 1 decimeter horizontal line corresponded to each item.
The ends had verbal anchors (low and high extremes) and
there were no intermediate levels marked on the line. The
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
4 of 8
Electronic paper
participant was asked to make a cross on the line at the level
that corresponded to the current condition. The distance in
mm from 0 to the cross was measured. In the statistical
analysis the measures were used as scores from 0 to 10.
Statistically, the results obtained with the visual analogue
scales were comparable to those obtained with the
original questionnaires which were constructed with ordinal
scales.33–35
The participants were aware of the nature of the
manipulations. It would not have been possible to ‘‘blind’’
them to the purpose of the study.
Acoustic materials and methods
Acoustic measurements were done in the main work area
and in three patient rooms. The reverberation time was the
major acoustic variable defined as the time needed for the
sound pressure level to decrease by 60 dB after the sound
source has been switched off. A change in reverberation time
was expected to influence sound pressure level, sound
propagation, and speech intelligibility of the premises. The
reverberation time was measured for the two different
conditions36 in the main work area and in Room 4. Patient
rooms 1–7 were judged as similar in terms of room acoustic
character (size, height, and positioning of furniture and
equipment, etc) which meant that measurements were
limited to Room 4. The equivalent sound pressure level
(LAeq) was measured for one week during each study period
in Rooms 1, 4, and 7 (see fig 1) and in the main work area of
the ward. Sound propagation was determined by measuring
the attenuation of a reference sound of 88 db(A) pink noise
(positioned in the lower corner of the left corridor), which
was louder than the background noise level at all measured
positions. Measurements were done at predetermined
intervals from the source along the outer wall of the main
ward.37–39 The sound pressure level of the reference sound was
also measured in two patient rooms situated along the outer
wall. Speech intelligibility was measured according to the
RASTI (Rapid Speech Transmission Index) method,40 and
intended to measure the effects on speech caused by
background noise (mechanical installations and equipment)
and reverberant sound. Measurements were done in Room 4
(the speaker next to the bed and the receiver at the patient’s
head) as well as at two positions in the main work area. The
speaker was positioned at the lower left corner of the central
nursing station and the receiver was positioned either at the
upper right corner of the station or at the entrance to Room 5.
The RASTI method allows the user to transform the numeric
values to a qualitative interpretation, ranging from ‘‘bad’’,
‘‘poor’’, ‘‘fair’’, ‘‘good’’, to ‘‘excellent’’.
One of the patient rooms was not in use during the two
study periods and therefore no acoustic analysis was
performed.
Acoustic manipulation/experimentation
Reverberation time was manipulated during the two study
periods through the use of sound reflective and sound
absorbing ceilings. A sound reflective suspended ceiling
(13 mm solid painted plaster board tiles) was mounted in
the main work area. The original ceiling tiles (25 mm thick
resin bonded glass wool with a painted surface) in the main
work area were judged to correspond to absorption class
A.41 42 The patient rooms already contained a reflective ceiling
surface (solid painted plaster). During the fifth week the
plaster ceiling tiles in the main work area were substituted by
Table 1 Delta (end of shift minus start) sum scores based on principal component analysis
Baseline (B)
Morning shift
Demand
Control/supp
Distress
Pressure
Strain
Afternoon shift
Demand
Control/supp
Distress
Pressure
Strain
Night shift
Demand
Control/supp
Distress
Pressure
Strain
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
M/SD
95%CI
n = 13
21.15/2.91
22.74 to 0.43
20.67/1.55
21.51 to 0.17
n = 18
20.55/3.09
21.98 to 0.88
21.16/1.60
21.90 to 20.42
n = 46
1.52/2.52
0.79 to 2.25
0.18/1.46
20.24 to 0.60
Sound reflecting
(SR)
Sound absorbing
(SA)
n = 94
0.78/2.71
0.23 to 1.33
20.38/1.58
20.70 to 20.07
0.06/1.18
21.18 to 0.29
1.48/2.77
0.92 to 2.04
0.62/1.87
0.24 to 0.99
n = 70
0.65/2.26
0.12 to 1.18
20.82/1.51
21.17 to 20.46
0.03/0.91
20.19 to 0.24
1.34/2.83
0.68 to 2.01
0.25/1.34
20.06 to 0.57
n = 53
0.26/2.86
20.51 to 1.03
20.98/2.04
21.53 to 20.43
20.05/1.15
20.36 to 0.26
0.25/3.17
20.60 to 1.11
0.08/2.02
20.46 to 0.63
n = 36
22.16/2.30
22.91 to 21.41
20.89/1.82
21.49 to 20.30
20.39/0.88
20.68 to 20.10
21.73/2.29
22.48 to 20.98
20.85/1.88
21.47 to 20.24
n = 53
1.26/2.57
0.57 to 1.95
0.20/1.31
20.15 to 0.56
0.16/0.89
20.08 to 0.40
1.49/2.64
0.78 to 2.20
0.25/1.37
20.12 to 0.62
n = 37
1.23/2.36
0.47 to 1.99
0.29/0.81
0.03 to 0.55
0.17/1.63
20.35 to 0.70
1.32/2.44
0.53 to 2.11
0.04/1.98
20.60 to 0.67
F value
B-SR
Post hoc p
B-SA
Post hoc p
SR-SA
Post hoc p
3.31
0.011
0.021
0.739
1.60
0.527
0.761
0.078
0.71
0.868
9.42
0.758
2.58
0.171
8.47
0.282
0.043
0.0001
0.12
0.730
0.632
0.837
1.87
0.116
4.96
0.003
2.45
0.029
0.18
0.611
0.601
0.953
0.09
0.932
0.692
0.742
1.36
0.957
0.20
0.785
1.24
0.585
Results expressed as number of observations, means (M)/standard deviations (SD), 95% confidence limits of means (CI), and results of one way analyses of
variance. Post hoc p values for analyses with three conditions (Fisher’s PLSD).
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
Electronic paper
visually identical sound absorbing ceiling tiles. Additionally
the eight patient rooms were fitted with the same type of
sound absorbing ceiling tiles.
The sound absorbing Ecophon ceiling tile is made from a
40 mm thick high density resin bonded glass wool with a
painted surface. The ceiling tiles have high sound absorption,
absorption class A.41 42
Statistical methods
A principal component analysis (varimax, roots .1) highlighted a demand factor (high pace of work, high quantity of
work, bad atmosphere at work, high demand on quality of
care) as well as a control/support factor (autonomy, selfdetermination, own competence, social support at work),
which corresponds roughly to a combined decision latitude
and social support factor in the demand-control-support
model.11 The mood scales were also submitted to principal
component analysis. Three distinct factors were found,
namely distress (anxiety, sadness, depression), pressure
(stress, calmness (reversed), hastiness), and strain (irritation,
anger, tension). Five indices were computed by means of the
summation of item scores within each dimension (demand,
control/support, distress, pressure, strain). For each individual work shift the ‘‘delta’’ score (sum score at end minus
sum score at start of shift) was computed, indicating the
accumulated effect of working conditions. Means and 95%
confidence intervals of these means were calculated for all
the five sum delta scores during baseline, sound reflecting,
and sound absorbing conditions. The calculations were made
separately for the morning, afternoon, and night shifts. One
way analyses of variance were made separately for morning,
afternoon, and night shifts, with post hoc tests (Fisher’s
PLSD) comparing baseline–sound reflecting, baseline–sound
absorbing, and sound reflecting–sound absorbing conditions.
For the three mood scales complete information was not
available for the baseline period. For these scales, the only
possible comparison was between the sound reflecting and
sound absorbing periods.
Finally, five two-way analyses of variance (MANOVA)
were performed using shift and acoustic condition (sound
reflecting versus sound absorbing) as explanatory and each
one of the five delta sum scores as dependent variables.
This study was approved by the local research ethics
committee of Huddinge University Hospital, Stockholm,
Sweden.
RESULTS
Drop out and participation
In order to avoid measurement instability, we only asked
regularly—not temporarily—working staff to fill out the
questionnaires.
The sound reflecting period lasted for 20 days which
corresponded to a maximum of 300 possible shifts. Two
hundred of these shifts (67%) were covered by start and end
questionnaires. Accordingly the total percentage of lost shifts
was 33%. Sixteen per cent of the lost shifts were due to
planned education, 8% to vacation, and 8% to sick leave due
to own or child’s illness. Only three (1%) were ‘‘true’’ dropouts in the sense that there was no satisfactory explanation
of the loss.
During the sound absorbing observation period that lasted
for 22 days the maximum possible number of shifts was 330.
One hundred and forty two (43%) were covered by start and
end questionnaires. Accordingly the total percentage of lost
shifts was 57%. Thirty seven per cent of these were due to
planned education, vacation, and sick leave due to own or
child’s illness (with relative proportions as in the previous
period). The higher percentage of such drop-outs was mainly
5 of 8
due to a vacation week (Easter). In addition, along the same
lines as in the previous period, 20% were ‘‘true’’ drop-outs.
For the baseline period no corresponding calculations
could be performed since the data collection was not
continuous.
Psychosocial environment
Number of observations, means, standard deviations, and
95% confidence intervals for the three periods are presented
separately in table 1 together with results of one-way
analyses of variance.
There was a significant worsening of delta demands from
the baseline period to both experimental periods (regardless
of acoustic conditions) during the morning shift. During the
afternoon shift, delta demands were perceived as significantly improved (lowered) from the baseline to the sound
absorbing period. During the night shift there were no period
differences.
Prominent differences with regard to the delta values were
found in the afternoon shift when comparing the sound
reflecting and the sound absorbing condition. There was
a highly significant improvement in demand (‘‘delta’’)
(p = 0.0001). Separate item analyses showed that improvements in pace of work, quantity of work, atmosphere at work,
and demands on quality of work all contributed to this. There
were also significant improvements with regard to ‘‘pressure’’
(‘‘delta’’) (p = 0.0003) and ‘‘strain’’ (‘‘delta’’) (p = 0.029)
during the afternoon shift. Separate item analyses showed
that changes in ‘‘lack of calmness’’, ‘‘hastiness’’, and ‘‘stress’’
(‘‘delta’’) all contributed to the improvement in ‘‘pressure’’
(‘‘delta’’) and that changes in ‘‘tension’’ and ‘‘irritation’’
(‘‘delta’’) contributed to the improvement in ‘‘strain’’.
Two-way analyses of variance (table 2) showed significant
main effects of acoustic conditions for delta demand, delta
pressure, and delta strain. In this table the baseline condition
was not included. There were significant two-way interactions (shift and acoustics) for delta demand and delta
pressure. This means that the effects of the acoustics were
significantly different in the three shifts for these variables.
No interaction, however, was found for delta strain. This
means that the effects of the acoustics on delta strain were
similar in the three shifts.
The delta index demand was chosen to illustrate the
changes over time for the afternoon shifts (see fig 2).
Table 2 Two-way analysis of variance with reflecting/
absorbing acoustics and shift as explanatory and the five
delta sum variables as dependent variables
Delta demand
Sound reflecting/absorbing
Shift
Interaction
Control/supp
Sound reflecting/absorbing
Shift
Interaction
Distress
Sound reflecting/absorbing
Shift
Interaction
Pressure
Sound reflecting/absorbing
Shift
Interaction
Strain
Sound reflecting/absorbing
Shift
Interaction
F value
p
8.78
18.03
6.63
0.003
0.0001
0.002
0.23
13.43
1.14
0.630
0.0001
0.323
0.87
2.70
0.70
0.352
0.069
0.496
5.89
19.46
3.54
0.016
0.0001
0.030
6.43
6.19
1.07
0.012
0.002
0.344
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
6 of 8
Electronic paper
Figure 2 Changes over time for delta
index demands in the afternoon shift
High
workers.
demands
Evening shift
10
8
6
4
2
0
–2
–4
–6
–8
–10
Baseline study
Low
demands
Sound reflecting ceiling
Sound absorbing ceiling
The figure shows changes over time in delta demands from
the baseline study, period of sound reflecting ceilings, and
the period of sound absorbing ceilings. More ratings were
collected during the sound reflecting and sound absorbing
periods. However, the figure clearly illustrates differences
over time between the three different study periods.
Acoustic environment
The acoustic measurements (equivalent sound pressure level)
were continuous for one week during each study period
(table 3). Reverberation time improved from 0.8 to 0.4 seconds after the instalment of the sound absorbing ceiling in
the main working area, and from 0.9 to 0.4 seconds in one of
the patient rooms. The sound pressure level measured
directly over the main work area did not vary greatly during
the two weeks (57 v 56 db(A)). As the microphone was
positioned directly over the work area, mainly direct sound
was measured, and under such circumstances changes in
reverberation time have little impact on sound pressure
level.
The measurements confirm that the central area was the
noisiest area in the ward. The two patient rooms (4 and 7)
showed a drop in sound level of 5–6 dB, where ca 3 dB is due
to the shorter reverberation time in the actual room and 2–3
dB is due to a lower sound pressure level in the ward. This is
also confirmed by the sound propagation measurements (see
table 4).
The effect of the reference sound in the two rooms,
Rooms 1 and 4 respectively, was 75 and 64 db(A) during the
first period and 69 and 59 db(A) during the second period.
For the two periods, speech intelligibility (RASTI value),
improved from ‘‘good’’ to ‘‘excellent’’ in both measured
areas.
DISCUSSION
The findings indicate that the improved acoustics had
affected the psychosocial environment in such a way that
during the afternoon the staff experienced reduced demands,
and less pressure/strain. Such changes open up for an
increased capacity to care for the patients.
It seems likely that improved acoustic conditions in the
healthcare environment reduce risks of conflicts and errors.
During the study period with the sound absorbing acoustics
the staff reported that they also felt more relaxed and
irritability decreased. Although there were fewer patients
during the period with poor acoustics (longer reverberation
time), the staff experienced more strain, demands and
pressure at work. These negative effects occurred despite
the fact that the nurses were highly educated, well trained in
acute situations, and accustomed to coping with high
demands and making critical decisions.
The staff members were surprised by the improved speech
intelligibility after the installation of the new ceiling as well
as the perceived noise level. During the period of long
reverberation time speech intelligibility was thought to be
below what is needed in the CCU. It is possible that
alternative wording should be developed to better reflect
the demands on speech intelligibility in this type of work
environment. Although long term measurements have been
unable to fully confirm this, the findings together suggest
that the staff experienced a reduced noise load during the
working day when sound absorbing ceiling tiles were in
place. The most prominent positive effects of improved
acoustics on the psychosocial work environment were found
for the afternoon shift. Most of the effects were observed
during the afternoon shift. This was also the shift that was
subjected to the most noise due to family visits. In addition,
Table 3 Sound reflecting ceilings vs. sound absorbing ceilings; acoustic measurements in
three areas of the ward (reverberation time, sound pressure level, and speech
intelligibility)
Reverberation time (s)
Equivalent sound pressure
level (LAeq)
Speech intelligibility (RASTI value)
Area
Sound
reflecting
Sound
absorbing
Sound
reflecting
Sound
absorbing
Sound
reflecting
Sound
absorbing
Central area
Patient room 4
Patient room 7
0.8
0.9
–
0.4
0.4
–
57
56
56
56
50
51
0.72/0.68*
0.67
–
0.88/0.83*
0.87
–
–, not measured.
*Measurements performed in two different positions.
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
Electronic paper
7 of 8
Table 4 Sound propagation; attenuation of the reference sound (pink noise 88 db(A)
1 metre from the source) at predetermined intervals from the source
Distance from the source (m)
Sound reflecting LAeq (dB)
Sound absorbing LAeq (dB)
2
6
10
14
18
22
83
82
80
76
78
72
75
70
72
65
70
64
the morning shift overlapped with, or worked parallel to, the
afternoon shift during the initial part of the afternoon.
There was a significant worsening of delta demand
between the baseline period and the period with the sound
absorbing acoustics, perhaps partly due to some disarray still
present during the first weeks of installation of the sound
absorbing ceiling.
It is important to raise the question of a possible
Hawthorne effect. Almost any change or extra attention, or
the knowledge that a study is being conducted, can be
enough to cause subjects to change. In this study design,
however, the extra attention experienced by the staff was
constant throughout the periods of sound reflecting and
sound absorbing acoustics. Further, the patterns of improvement and deterioration observed were stable throughout the
two periods. A Hawthorne effect would have resulted in an
attenuation of the beneficial effect of sound absorption
(shorter reverberation time) during the studied one month
period, and perhaps in a progressive worsening of the
perceived work environment during the sound reflective
period (longer reverberation time). Control/support and
distress were not affected by the acoustic condition at all. It
is unlikely that the physical change and disarray associated
with modification of the CCU ceiling at the start could
explain the difference.
Testing of the possible statistical significance of differences
between the acoustic conditions was based on the following
considerations. A large number of observations were made.
The number of staff was relatively small, on the other hand.
‘‘Losses’’ of observations in individual participants were of
course very natural since every person could not work three
shifts every day. Natural losses of observations were made
because of illness episodes, periods of part time work, etc,
and in addition there were changes in shift work schedules.
Because of these non-systematic losses we decided to regard
every observation, regardless of individual, as an independent
observation. This decision was supported by the fact that
shifts in the coronary care unit could be very different; one
shift may have a large influx of severely ill newly admitted
patients while the next shift may have almost no patients,
etc. Shortage of staff, which has a pronounced influence on
perceived load, also occurs randomly.
It was therefore felt that differences in individual perception may play a relatively small role compared to the
situations in which the work environment is being studied—in which differences in real conditions may be more
important, particularly for load and demand measures. An
important condition for this decision is that there was a
comparable mix of individuals and observations between the
three study periods. This condition was fulfilled.
That every observation was regarded as an independent
one may have influenced the statistical significance levels in
some of the analyses. These effects are likely to be marginally
in the direction of ‘‘improved’’ p values. A cautionary procedure could accordingly be to regard all findings that are
significant on the 5% level as uncertain. In the comparison
between the sound reflecting and sound absorbing period
during the afternoon shift this pertains only to one of three
significant findings (p = 0.029) out of five tests altogether—
the remaining two findings in this column are significant at
least on the 1% level.
The decision to regard each observation as an independent
one was supported by an analysis of autocorrelations based
on the six subjects who had at least five observations
regarding change in demands (end minus start score in a
shift), in both the good and the bad acoustics condition
respectively. These autocorrelations were tested with different lags (one to four observations) and turned out to be zero
to modest and randomly distributed (mean 20.11, 15/24
being between 20.30 and 0.30 and the extremes being 20.57
to 0.41). Thus there was no strong linear within-subject
dependence for this main variable.
When the staff member is arriving to start a shift she will
immediately see how many patients there are in the ward
and what the atmosphere looks like. If the load and/or
intensity increases during the shift this will be reflected in
increased load scores. Mood states will be sensitive to
changes in the same way. Accumulated effects of increasing
load will be of particular importance. When the participant
starts working in the morning shift she may be tired and feel
gloomy because of the early morning hour. As she becomes
active the normal circadian variation in mood makes her feel
more active and perhaps happy. This introduces a possible
source of error in the work environment assessments and
may partly explain why the afternoon shifts (which may be
relatively unaffected by this particular factor) are the ones
that differ most clearly between the two acoustics periods.
It may also explain why there are differences between the
shifts.
There has been very little published research on the
influences of acoustics on healthcare staff, although there
are some previous examples. A recent study43 of staff in a
paediatric intensive ward showed correlations between sound
level and indicators of perceived stress. The low number of
previous research reports has hindered the development of
evidence based design and construction of guidelines or
standards for CCUs and other healthcare settings. Although
the sound reflecting ceiling condition (longer reverberation
time), compared to the sound absorbing condition, detrimentally affected the CCU work environment, the sound
reflecting ceiling nonetheless complied with healthcare
building requirements or standards in Sweden and many
other countries.
The study clearly raises the possibility that important gains
in the psychosocial work environment of healthcare can be
achieved by improving environmental acoustics. The findings
imply that an approach for improving healthcare acoustics
will be inadequate, however, if it focuses narrowly on
reducing sound pressure levels.
Rather, a more effective approach will additionally emphasise environmental design interventions that shorten reverberation time. The importance of improved environmental
acoustics for influencing speech intelligibility and perceived
work demands, point to the need for further research to
examine the possible role of acoustics in medical errors and
other aspects of patient safety.
www.occenvmed.com
Downloaded from oem.bmjjournals.com on 30 January 2006
8 of 8
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This study was partly funded by Ecophon under the supervision of
Jasper Cole, BSc and Sören Berg, MD, PhD. Acoustic measurements
have been performed by Ingemansson Technology AB, under the
supervision of Leif Åkerlöf (MSc), Gunilla Sundin (MSc), and Peter
Petterson (BSc). They have also given valuable input in the
formulation of the acoustic measurements as well as the final article.
A grant to Dr Theorell from the Swedish Council for Work
Environment Research (RALF) also contributed to the study.
.....................
Authors’ affiliations
V Blomkvist, C A Eriksen, T Theorell, National Institute for Psychosocial
Factors and Health, Stockholm, Sweden
R Ulrich, Center for Health Systems and Design, Texas A&M University,
College Station, Texas, USA
G Rasmanis, Department of Cardiology, Huddinge University Hospital,
Karolinska Institute, Sweden
Competing interests: none declared
REFERENCES
1 Horsburgh CR. Healing by design. N Engl J Med 1995;333:735–40.
2 Rubin HR, Owens AJ, Golden G. Status report: an investigation to determine
whether the built environment affects patients’ medical outcomes. Martinez,
CA: The Center for Health Design, 1998.
3 Ulrich RS. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery.
Science 1984;224:420–1.
4 Ulrich RS. Effects of health facility interior design on wellness: theory and
scientific research. Journal of Health Care Design 1991;3:97–109.
5 Ulrich RS, Zimring C. The role of the physical environment in the hospital of the
21st century, The Robert Wood Johnson Foundation and the Center for Health
Design.www.rwfj.org.
6 IOM (Institute of Medicine). Keeping patients safe: transforming the work
environment of nurses. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 2004.
7 Babwin D. Building boom. Hospitals & Health Networks 2002;76(3):48–54.
8 Berglund B, Lindvall T, Schwela DH. Guidelines for community noise.
Protection of the human environment. World Health Organisation, 1999.
9 McLaughlin A, McLaughlin B, Elliot J, et al. Noise levels in a cardiac surgical
intensive care unit: a preliminary study conducted in secret. Intensive Crit Care
Nurs 1996;12:226–30.
10 Hilton BA. Noise in acute patient care areas. Res Nurs Health
1985;8:283–91.
11 Redding JS, Hargest TS, Minsky SH. How noisy is intensive care? Crit Care
Med 1977;5:275–6.
12 Baker CF, Garvin BJ, Kennedy CW, et al. The effect of environmental sound
and communication on CCU patients’ heart rate and blood rate and blood
pressure. Res Nurs Health 1993;16:415–21.
13 Yinnon AM, Ilan Y, Tadmor B, et al. Quality of sleep in the medical
department. Br J Clin Pract 1992;46:88–91.
14 Berg S. Impact of reduced reverberation time on sound-induced arousals
during sleep. Sleep 2001;24:289–92.
15 Bayo MV, Garcia AM, Garcia A. Noise levels in an urban hospital and
workers’ subjective responses. Arch Environ Health 1995;50:247–51.
16 Norbeck J. Perceived job stress, job satisfaction, and psychological symptoms
in critical care nursing. Res Nurs Health 1985;8:97–122.
17 Topf M, Dillon E. Noise-induced stress as a predictor of burnout in critical care
nurses. Heart Lung 1988;17:567–573.
18 Evans GW, Cohen S. Environmental stress. In: Stokols D, Altman I, eds.
Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: John Wiley,
1987:571–610.
19 Parsons R, Hartig T. Environmental psychophysiology. In: Caccioppo JT,
Tassinary LG, Berntson G, eds. Handbook of psychophysiology. New York:
Cambridge University Press, 2000:815–46.
www.occenvmed.com
Electronic paper
20 Lundberg M, Frankenhaeuser M. Psychophysiological reactions to noise
as modified by personal control over noise intensity. Biol Psychol
1978;6:51–9.
21 Jones D, Broadbent DE. Side effects of interference with speech by noise.
Ergonomics 1979;22:1073–81.
22 McLaughlin A, McLaughlin B, Elliot J, et al. Noise levels in cardiac surgical
intensive care unit; a preliminary study conducted in secret. Intensive Crit Care
Nurs 1996;12:226–30.
23 Baker CF. Discomfort to environmental noise: heart rate responses of SICU
patients. Crit Care Nurs Q 1992;15(2):75–90.
24 Karasek RA, Theorell T. Healthy work: stress, productivity, and the
reconstruction of work life. New York: Basic Books, 1990.
25 Topf M. A framework for research on aversive physical aspects of the
environment. Res Nurs Health 1984;7:35–42.
26 Schnall P, Belkic K, Landbergis P, et al. The workplace and cardiovascular
disease. In: Occupational medicine. Hanley & Belfus, 2000:183–8.
27 Bongers PM, de Winter CR, Kompier MAJ, et al. Psychosocial factors
at work and musculoskeletal disease. Scand J Work Environ Health
1993;20:73–86.
28 Theorell T, Hasselhorn HM, Vingård E, Andersson B, the MUSIC-Norrtälje
group study group. Interleukin 6 and cortisol in acute musculoskeletal
disorders: results: results from a case-referent study in Sweden. Stress Med
2000;16:27–35.
29 Arnetz BB. Staff perception of the impact of health care transformation on
quality of care. Int J Qual Health Care 1999;11:345–51.
30 Cronqvist A, Theorell T, Burns T, et al. Dissonant imperatives in nursing: a
conceptualization of stress in intensive care in Sweden. Intensive Crit Care
Nurs 2001;17:228–36.
31 Nabelek AK, Nabelek I. Room acoustics and speech perception. In: Katz J,
eds. Handbook of clinical audiology, 4th edn. Baltimore: Williams and
Wilkins, 1994.
32 Topf M. Hospital noise pollution: and environmental stress model to guide
research and clinical interventions. J Adv Nurs 2000;31:520–8.
33 Karasek AK. Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain:
implications for job redesign. Adm Sci Q 1979;24:285–308.
34 Shimomitsu T, Theorell T. Intraindividual relationships between blood pressure
level and emotional state. Psychother Psychosom 1996;65:137–44.
35 Gillberg M, Kecklund G, Åkerstedt T. Relations between performance and
subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. Sleep
1994;17:236–41.
36 ISO. Acoustics–measurement of reverberation time in auditoria. International
Standard ISO 3382: 1975 3382, 1997. Geneva, Switzerland: International
Organisation for Standardisation, 1975, 1997.
37 ISO. Acoustics–recommended practice for the design of low-noise workplaces
containing machinery. Part 1: Noise control strategies. International Standard
ISO 11690-1: 1996, Geneva, Switzerland: International Organisation for
Standardisation, 1996.
38 ISO. Acoustics–recommended practice for the design of low-noise workplaces
containing machinery. Part 2: Noise control measures. International Standard
ISO 11690–2: 1996, Geneva, Switzerland: International Organisation for
Standardisation, 1996.
39 ISO. Acoustics–recommended practice for the design of low-noise workplaces
containing machinery. Part 3: Sound propagation and noise prediction in
workrooms. International Standard ISO 11690-3: 1996, Geneva,
Switzerland: International Organisation for Standardisation, 1996.
40 IEC. Sound system equipment. Part 16: Objective rating of speech intelligibility
by speech transmission index. International Standard IEC Publication 60268–
16: 1998, Geneva, Switzerland: International Electrotechnical Commission,
1998.
41 ISO. Acoustics–measurement of sound absorption in a reverberation room.
International Standard ISO 354: 1985, Geneva, Switzerland: International
Organisation for Standardisation, 1985.
42 ISO. Acoustics–sound absorbers for use in buildings. Rating of sound
absorption. International Standard ISO 11654: 1997, Geneva, Switzerland.
International Organisation for Standardisation, 1997.
43 Morrison WE, Haas EC, Shaffner DH, et al. Noise, stress, and annoyance in a
pediatric intensive care unit. Crit Care Med 2003;31:113–19.