ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 04 June 2019
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00568
Cortical Excitability Dynamics During
Fear Processing
Venkata C. Chirumamilla, Gabriel Gonzalez-Escamilla, Nabin Koirala, Tamara Bonertz,
Sarah von Grotthus, Muthuraman Muthuraman † and Sergiu Groppa* †
Section of Movement Disorders and Neurostimulation, Biomedical Statistics and Multimodal Signal Processing Unit,
Department of Neurology, Focus Program Translational Neurosciences, University Medical Center, Johannes Gutenberg
University Mainz, Mainz, Germany
Background: Little is known about the modulation of cortical excitability in the prefrontal
cortex during fear processing in humans. Here, we aimed to transiently modulate
and test the cortical excitability during fear processing using transcranial magnetic
stimulation (TMS) and brain oscillations in theta and alpha frequency bands with
electroencephalography (EEG).
Edited by:
Shapour Jaberzadeh,
Monash University, Australia
Reviewed by:
Xiaoli Li,
Beijing Normal University, China
Ali Yadollahpour,
Ahvaz Jundishapur University
of Medical Sciences, Iran
Matthias Grothe,
University of Greifswald, Germany
*Correspondence:
Sergiu Groppa
[email protected]
† These
authors have contributed
equally to this work
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Neural Technology,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Neuroscience
Received: 18 December 2018
Accepted: 17 May 2019
Published: 04 June 2019
Citation:
Chirumamilla VC,
Gonzalez-Escamilla G, Koirala N,
Bonertz T, von Grotthus S,
Muthuraman M and Groppa S (2019)
Cortical Excitability Dynamics During
Fear Processing.
Front. Neurosci. 13:568.
doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00568
Methods: We conducted two separate experiments (no-TMS and TMS). In the noTMS experiment, EEG recordings were performed during the instructed fear paradigm
in which a visual cue (CS+) was paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (electric
shock), while the other visual cue was unpaired (CS−). In the TMS experiment, in
addition the TMS was applied on the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC).
The participants also underwent structural MRI (magnetic resonance imaging) scanning
and were assigned pseudo-randomly to both experiments, such that age and gender
were matched. The cortical excitability was evaluated by time-frequency analysis and
functional connectivity with weighted phase lag index (WPLI). We further linked the
excitability patterns with markers of stress coping capability.
Results: After visual cue onset, we found increased theta power in the frontal lobe
and decreased alpha power in the occipital lobe during CS+ relative to CS− trials.
TMS of dmPFC increased theta power in the frontal lobe and reduced alpha power in
the occipital lobe during CS+. The TMS pulse increased the information flow from the
sensorimotor region to the prefrontal and occipital regions in the theta and alpha bands,
respectively during CS+ compared to CS−. Pre-stimulation frontal theta power (0.75–
1 s) predicted the magnitude of frontal theta power changes after stimulation (1–1.25 s).
Finally, the increased frontal theta power during CS+ compared to CS− was positively
correlated with stress coping behavior.
Conclusion: Our results show that TMS over dmPFC transiently modulated the regional
cortical excitability and the fronto-occipital information flows during fear processing,
while the pre-stimulation frontal theta power determined the strength of achieved effects.
The frontal theta power may serve as a biomarker for fear processing and stress-coping
responses in individuals and could be clinically tested in mental disorders.
Keywords: TMS, EEG, instructed fear paradigm, resilience, functional connectivity
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
1
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
INTRODUCTION
(TMS experiment) to determine the causal alterations during
the fear conditioning task. We then analyzed pre-stimulation
frontal theta power to predict the oscillatory activity in the
frontal cortex to show that the brain response to TMS is
state dependent. Moreover, we correlated individual transients
of modulated oscillatory activity to BRS ratings to reveal the
relationship between frontal theta power and individual stress
coping abilities.
Fear is an emotional response that is triggered in the brain
in anticipation of a potentially dangerous event (Garcia, 2017).
Instructed fear paradigms are commonly the experimental choice
to study the adaptive capacity of human brain processing during
threat. In such paradigms, the participants are explicitly informed
that a conditioned stimulus (CS+) will be repeatedly paired
with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), while a second
conditioned stimulus will always be safe (CS−) (Mechias et al.,
2010; Mertens et al., 2018). These fear responses are well
associated with subjective and peripheral psycho-physiological
measures, in terms of skin conductance, heart rate acceleration
and self-reported fear ratings (Gonzalez-Escamilla et al., 2018a).
Accumulating evidence indicates that instructions about the
CS+/US contingency evoke effects on the neural activity of
a distributed network of brain regions, namely the amygdala,
the cingulate cortex, the insula, hippocampus and prefrontal
cortices, among which the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex
(dmPFC) plays an important role in fear processing by
dynamically regulating excitability (Phelps et al., 2001; GonzalezEscamilla et al., 2018b). A recent electroencephalography
(EEG) study showed that increased theta power in frontal
regions together with decreased alpha power at occipital
locations are potential attributes of instructed fear responses
in humans (Chien et al., 2017). However, the modulation
of the neural oscillations during fear processing remains
unclear. Furthermore, the individual stress coping abilities as
measured by the brief resilience scale (BRS) are negatively
correlated with anxiety and depression (Chmitorz et al., 2018),
suggesting that this measure may be useful in searching for
behavioral markers of brain circuit responses during potential
threatening events. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)
is a non-invasive stimulation technique that offers targeted
modulation of cortical brain regions in humans (Bergmann
et al., 2016). The combined TMS-EEG technique can be used
to track the cortical excitability and functional connectivity
alterations of the stimulated brain region (Groppa et al., 2013;
Pellicciari et al., 2017).
In a previous study, we analyzed the TMS-evoked potentials
(TEP) measured with EEG and TMS over the right dmPFC
during an instructed fear paradigm (Gonzalez-Escamilla et al.,
2018a). We showed that TMS over dmPFC led to increased
evoked cortical excitability at a specific time window during
CS+ relative to CS−, measured by TMS-EEG potentials
amplitudes and latencies. Moreover, the enhanced responses were
further supported by the underlying structural integrity of the
brain. On the basis of these results, in the current study we
focused on characterizing the transient modulated oscillatory
activity and functional connectivity alterations following singlepulse TMS during an instructed fear paradigm. First, we
performed an instructed fear paradigm together with EEG
(no-TMS experiment) to determine the optimal time window
for modulatory effects of TMS in a subsequent experiment.
Based on evidence from the no-TMS experiment, we adapted
the instructed fear paradigm together with the application of
TMS over the right dmPFC in a second group of participants
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
SUBJECTS AND METHODS
Subjects
Thirty-eight healthy subjects (no-TMS experiment: n = 19, 9
males, mean age 27.4 ± 4.32 years, TMS experiment: n = 19,
10 males, mean age 28.6 ± 6.64 years) were included in our
study. All participants had two visits to the lab. During the
first visit, MRI data was acquired, whereas, at the second
visit, an instructed fear paradigm (no-TMS experiment) or an
instructed fear paradigm together with TMS (TMS experiment)
was performed. Participants were assigned to one of the two
experiments pseudo-randomly, such that age and gender were
matched. The TMS experiment was conducted after completing
the no-TMS experiment.
MRI Data Acquisition
Magnetic resonance images were acquired for the purpose of
neuronavigation using a 3 Tesla MRI scanner (Magnetom Tim
Trio, Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with
a 32-channel head coil at the Neuroimaging Center (NIC) in
Mainz, Germany. A magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo
(MP-RAGE) sequence was used (repetition time [TR] = 1900 ms;
echo time [TE] = 2.54 ms; inversion time [IT] = 900 ms;
pixel bandwidth = 180; acquisition matrix = 320 × 320;
flip angle = 9◦ ; pixel spacing = 0.8125 × 0.8125 mm; slice
thickness = 0.8 mm).
Experimental Procedure (Instructed Fear
Paradigm)
The instructed fear paradigm was developed with the Cogent
toolbox1 in Matlab 2006b (MathWorks). First, the participant
was asked to sit on a chair, and an electric shock was applied
to the dorsal part of the left hand using a surface electrode
that was connected to a DS7A electrical stimulator (Digitimer).
Then, the participant was requested to rate the amount of pain
perceived on a scale from 0 (no pain) to 10 (intense pain).
An electric shock intensity corresponding to a pain level of 7
was employed during the experiment (Meyer et al., 2015). The
experiment consisted of two visual cues, namely a circle and
a square, presented in a pseudo-randomized order on a screen
for 5 s with an inter-trial interval (ITI) jittered between 9 and
11 s (Figures 1A,B). Before the beginning of the experiment, all
participants were instructed that a CS+ (visual cue circle) would
be associated with a US (electric shock) with a probability of
33% during the time where the visual cue was present on the
1
2
http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_2000.php
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 1 | (A) Conditioned stimulus (CS+), unconditioned stimulus (US), and neutral stimulus (CS–) and their contingencies in an instructed fear paradigm. (B) Trial
sequence in the TMS experiment. Each trial consisted of the presentation of a stimulus (CS+ or CS–) followed by a fixation cross. The stimulus was presented on a
computer screen for 5 s followed by a fixation cross that jittered between 5 and 6 s. A single-pulse TMS was applied on the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex
(dmPFC) 1 s after the onset of each stimulus.
screen; and that the CS− (visual cue square) would never be
associated with a shock. The visual cues were counterbalanced
across subjects. A total of 90 visual cues (54 CS+ including 18
CS+/US, and 36 CS−) were used. During the whole duration of
the experiment EEG signals were recorded with a high-density
(256 electrodes) EEG system (Net Station 5.0, EGI, United States).
The caps were placed manually with the Cz electrode positioned
over a centralized location on the scalp, which was determined
as the simultaneous midpoint of the arc length for both nasioninion and preauricular arcs. The electrode impedances were kept
under 50 K throughout the experiment (Ferree et al., 2001),
and a sampling frequency of 250 Hz was applied. The time of
experiment across subjects was uniformly distributed throughout
the day between morning and evening.
x = 10, y = 12, z = 58) (Meyer et al., 2018). We used TMSNavigator (Localite, Sankt Augustin, Germany) based on a
coregistered individual T1-weighted MRI to navigate the TMS
coil and to maintain its exact location and orientation throughout
an experimental session. The TMS pulses were applied with a
stimulation intensity of 110% of RMT. All participants wore ear
plugs during the entire TMS experiment to reduce the auditory
click sound produced by TMS pulse.
EEG Data Processing
The processing steps of the analysis pipeline performed in
this study are shown in Figure 2. Pre-processing of the EEG
data was performed using a systematic procedure described
elsewhere (Herring et al., 2015). In brief, it included the following
steps: epoch creation, exclusion of TMS-related artifacts, and
physiological artifacts. The EEG data from both experiments
were preprocessed using the Fieldtrip toolbox2 and MATLAB
R2015b (The MathWorks). The EEG data was cut in epochs
of 7 s within the time interval of −2 to 5 s from the onset
of the visual cue (Bai et al., 2017). In the TMS experiment,
the 0.025 s of EEG signal containing the TMS pulse ringing
artifacts were deleted (−0.005 to 0.02 s around TMS pulse
onset). In both experiments, the original trials in which the
actual US was applied were discarded. Thus, only the conditionspecific (CS+ and CS−) trials were considered in further analysis.
Then, EEG data were re-referenced to a common grand average
of all electrodes. All trials were visually inspected, and the
Single-Pulse TMS
In the TMS experiment, single-pulse TMS was applied over the
right dmPFC after 1 s from each visual cue onset. The TMS was
delivered using a Magstim Super Rapid2 stimulator (Magstim,
United Kingdom) through a figure-of-eight coil with internal
wing diameter of 70 mm. First, the resting motor threshold
(RMT) was determined as the minimum stimulus intensity
required to elicit motor evoked potentials of amplitude 50 µV
in 5 out of 10 consecutive trials at rest (Groppa et al., 2012).
The TMS was targeted on the right dmPFC (MNI coordinate:
2
http://www.fieldtriptoolbox.org
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
3
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 2 | Electroencephalography data were acquired from the no-TMS and TMS experiments. The data were segmented into epochs and excluded the
TMS-related artifacts (ringing, decay, and muscle), and non-TMS artifacts (eye-blinks). Afterward, the global mean field power (GMFP) was calculated across all EEG
channels for both the experiments. Further, the power and connectivity were assessed by the multitaper method with Hanning window and weighted phase lag
index (WPLI), respectively. The significant differences in power and connectivity between the conditions were tested with non-parametric cluster-based statistics.
Finally, the neural oscillatory power was correlated with individual stress coping capabilities.
artefactual electrode data were interpolated using the spherical
spline interpolation method (Perrin et al., 1989). Independent
component analysis (ICA) was performed using the FastICA
method and the components reflecting eye-blinks, saccades and
decay artifacts (for the TMS experiment) were discarded (Bai
et al., 2016). The remaining components were transformed back
into electrode data representation. In the TMS experiment, the
remaining muscle artifact due to TMS pulse after ICA was
removed and interpolated using the pchip (Piecewise Cubic
Hermite Interpolating Polynomial) method (Herring et al.,
2015). Finally, we also implemented a 50 Hz notch filtering to
remove the line noise.
In this study, our main goal was to investigate the global
and local cortical excitability dynamics during fear processing.
Accordingly, we first computed the global mean field power
(GMFP) that is a measure of global cortical excitability.
Afterward, we assessed the local cortical excitability specifically
in the frontal and occipital lobes by estimating the power in theta
and alpha frequency bands, respectively. In addition, we also
investigated the direction of information flow in these frequency
bands with the weighted phase lag index (WPLI) method. The
WPLI works based on phase synchronization and is less sensitive
to uncorrelated noise sources and has increased statistical power
to detect the alterations in phase compared to other methods,
such as phase lag index (PLI) (Vinck et al., 2011). Furthermore,
the non-parametric tests were chosen due to the fact that they are
able to solve the multiple comparisons problem and are highly
sensitive to the expected effect (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007).
volume conduction is thought to be linear and instantaneous
and the sources of cardiac signals are not time-locked. Because
the sources of EEG activity are thought to reflect the activity
of cortical neurons (Milad et al., 2006), the ICA algorithm
can accurately identify the time courses of activation and the
scalp topographies of relatively large and temporally independent
sources from simulated scalp recordings, even in the presence of
a large number of low-level and temporally independent source
activities (Palazzo et al., 2008).
For EEG analysis, the EEG signals recorded at the 256
electrodes represent the rows of the sensor input matrix y for the
ICA component extraction, the rows of the output data matrix
v = Xy are time courses of activation of the ICA components,
and the columns of the inverse matrix, X −1 , give the projection
strengths of the respective components onto the scalp sensors.
In general, and unlike principal component analysis (PCA),
the component time courses of activation will be non-orthogonal.
Corrected EEG signals can then be derived as y′ = (X)−1 v′ ,
where v′ is the matrix of activation waveforms, v, with rows
representing cardiac artefactual sources which are then extracted
for further estimations from each participant. In total for the
no-TMS experiment, we concatenated the 36 CS+ trials to have
180 s and 36 CS− trials to have 180 s. For the TMS experiment,
we concatenated the 36 CS+ trials to have 180 s and 36 CS−
trials to have 180 s.
Global Mean Field Power (GMFP)
The GMFP is an index of distinctive global cortical excitability
and also reflects the synchronous activity across observations in
response to a specific stimulus (Romero Lauro et al., 2014; Varoli
et al., 2018), and was calculated for the time (−0.25 to 2 s) for
both CS+ and CS− conditions separately.
Fear Ratings and Heart Rate Estimation
At the end of the experiment, all the subjects were asked to
rate their perceived level of fear during the experiment, for
both CS+ and CS− independently, on a scale from 0% (not
fearful/safe) to 100% (very fearful). Heart rate was estimated
from the EEG signals using the extended version of the ICA
algorithm (Phelps and LeDoux, 2005) based on information
maximization (Chien et al., 2017). In the case of EEG signals,
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
Time-Frequency Analysis
Dynamic changes in neural oscillatory activity were assessed by
analyzing the time-frequency representations (TFR) of power.
The time-frequency analysis was performed using a multitaper
4
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
method (Herring et al., 2015). The EEG data were multiplied with
hanning tapered sliding window moving in steps of 0.02 s and
the length of the time window changed with frequency (T = 3
cycles = 3/f). The TFRs were computed for the time range of
−0.25 to 2 s for frequencies from 4 to 13 Hz. The time-frequency
grand averages across subjects were then computed for both CS+
and CS− conditions separately. The relative baseline correction
was applied by dividing the post-visual cue onset power with previsual cue onset power (−0.25 to 0 s). In this study, the theta and
alpha oscillations were investigated in the frontal and occipital
lobe, respectively. The electrodes corresponding to the frontal
and occipital lobes are shown (Supplementary Figure 1).
a categorical factor. To provide surface topography, we tested
the significant differences between stimulus conditions in power
and WPLI, using non-parametric cluster-based statistics with the
Monte-Carlo method in theta and alpha frequency bands (Maris
and Oostenveld, 2007). We applied 500 random permutations in
the Monte Carlo method to correct for multiple comparisons,
and a threshold of 2 channels to be considered a cluster. We
performed the subsequent regression and correlation analyses on
the data averaged across all subjects and significant electrodes
identified by cluster-based statistics. We conducted a linear
regression analysis to investigate the association of theta power
before and after the TMS pulse. The four temporal windows (0–
0.25, 0.25–0.5, 0.5–0.75, and 0.75–1 s) were added as predictor
variables and the window (1–1.25 s) as a dependent variable for
the power difference between stimulus conditions as a dependent
measure. To assess the relationship between theta power and
resilience, the Pearson correlation coefficient was computed
between individual theta power difference between conditions
and BRS ratings.
Functional Connectivity
The WPLI is a functional connectivity measure and evaluates
the distribution of phase angle differences between two signals
(Vinck et al., 2011). Specifically, if two signals are uncorrelated,
the angular difference will be evenly distributed giving a value of
zero, whereas if the signals are strongly coupled, the difference
will demonstrate an asymmetric distribution, approaching a
value of 1 or −1. For computation of the pairwise sensor
connectivity, the significant electrodes obtained by comparing
power distributions of CS+ and CS− using non-parametric
analysis were used as a reference. Then, the WPLI was computed
between the reference electrodes and the remaining EEG
electrodes at each the theta and alpha bands in both CS+ and
CS− conditions, respectively.
RESULTS
Fear Ratings and Heart Rate
The mean values of subjective fear ratings (+S.D.) for the CS+
and CS− conditions in the no-TMS experiment were 50.2 + 26.9
and 2.6 + 8 and in the TMS experiment 50.5 + 17.5 and
6.8 + 8.8, respectively. The reported fear ratings evidenced well
induced fear in the participants during the CS+ condition in
comparison to the CS− condition in both experiments (noTMS experiment: t(36) = 17.42; p < 0.001; TMS experiment:
t(36) = 18.47; p < 0.001), as shown in Figure 3A. The mean values
of the heart rate (+S.D.) in beat per minute (bpm) for the CS+
and CS− conditions in no-TMS experiment were 90 + 6.7 and
74 + 4.7 and in the TMS experiment were 91 + 6.8 and 72 + 4.6,
respectively. Accordingly, in both experiments, increased in heart
rate was detected during the CS+ trials relative to CS− (noTMS experiment: t(36) = 6.26; p < 0.001; TMS experiment:
t(36) = 5.98; p < 0.001) (Figure 3B).
Brief Resilience Scale
All subjects completed a BRS questionnaire (Park et al., 2018).
The BRS consists of six questions and is used to characterize the
ability of an individual to recover from stressful events (Smith
et al., 2008). The procedure for calculation of individual BRS
scores has been described elsewhere (Smith et al., 2008).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analysis of the data was performed using Statistica
software (version 13.13 ). Post hoc tests were performed if
the F-values were significant (p < 0.05) with the Bonferroni
correction method, unless otherwise explicitly specified. To study
the differences between the stimulus conditions (CS+ and CS−)
in behavioral fear ratings and heart rate, we conducted paired
t-tests. To examine the global cortical excitability dynamics
over time windows during fear processing, a one-way repeated
measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA) was conducted with
the factor Time (9 levels: −0.25–0, 0–0.25, 0.25–0.5, 0.5–0.75,
0.75–1, 1–1.25, 1.25–1.5, 1.5–1.75, and 1.75–2 s), and a dependent
measure of GMFP difference between stimulus conditions. To
study the differences between stimulus conditions and also across
temporal windows, a two-way rmANOVA was run including
two factors, Condition (2 levels: CS+ and CS−), and Time,
with the dependent measures for separate variables, power in
the theta and in the alpha band at the frontal and occipital lobe
electrodes (shown in Supplementary Figure 1), respectively. The
factor Experiment (2 levels: no-TMS and TMS) was added as
3
Global Mean Field Power
Figure 4A shows the difference in GMFP between CS+ and
CS− conditions across time windows in the no-TMS experiment.
The results of the one-way rmANOVA showed a significant
main effect of Time (F(8, 55) = 4015.7, p < 0.001). Post hoc
testing showed that the difference of GMFP increased in all the
time windows (0–2 s) compared to the baseline time window
(−0.25–0 s; all at p < 0.001). Furthermore, the GMFP difference
reduced in time windows (1–1.75 s) relative to (0.75–1 s)
window (all at p < 0.05). We repeated the same analysis for
TMS experiment (Figure 4B). One-way rmANOVA revealed a
significant main effect for the factor Time (F(8,55) = 7404.1,
p < 0.001). The post hoc comparisons showed that the GMFP
difference increased significantly in the time windows (0–0.25,
0.25–0.5, 0.5–0.75, 0.75–1, 1.25–1.5, 1.5–1.75, and 1.75–2 s)
compared to the (−0.25–0 s) time window (all at p < 0.05).
Moreover, the GMFP difference increased significantly in the
http://www.statistica.com
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
5
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 3 | For both experiments (no-TMS and TMS) the (A) subjective fear ratings and (B) heart rates. The light red bar represents CS+ and light green denotes
CS–. The values are mean ± standard deviation. The asterisk (∗ ) denotes significant differences after correcting for multiple comparisons (p < 0.001, Bonferroni
corrected).
FIGURE 4 | The difference of global mean field power (GMFP) between CS+ and CS– for (A) no-TMS experiment and (B) TMS experiment. The curve shadings are
mean ± standard error of the mean. The asterisk (∗ ) denotes significant differences after correcting for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected).
significantly lower during CS− in the time window (0.25–0.5 s)
compared to the baseline time window (−0.25–0 s; p < 0.05).
Furthermore, the theta power was reduced during CS+ in the
time windows (1–2 s) compared to the (0.75–1 s) time window.
And also, the frontal theta power was decreased for CS− in the
window (1–1.25 s) compared to the time window (0.75–1 s).
In the TMS experiment, frontal theta power was higher during
CS+ in all the temporal windows (0–2 s) relative to baseline
window (−0.25–0 s), while for CS− it was decreased in the time
window (0–0.25 s) compared to baseline window (all at p < 0.05)
(Figure 5B). As a result of single-pulse TMS, the theta power was
higher in frontal lobe during CS+ in the time windows (1–1.75 s)
relative to the time window (0.75–1 s) and decreased in the time
window (1–1.25 s) during CS− (all at p < 0.05).
In a similar manner, we investigated the alpha power dynamics
across occipital electrodes in the no-TMS experiment (Figure 5C)
and TMS experiment (Figure 5D). Two-way rmANOVA revealed
a significant main effect for the factor stimulus Condition (F(1,
18) = 4518.2, p < 0.001), significant main effect for the factor
time windows (1.25–1.5 and 1.75–2 s) compared to the time
window (0.75–1 s; all at p < 0.05) after single-pulse TMS.
Neural Oscillations During No-TMS and
TMS Experiments
The grand averaged theta power for the time (−0.25–2 s)
with respect to stimulus onset across frontal lobe electrodes
are presented separately for different stimuli (CS+ and CS−)
in the no-TMS experiment (Figure 5A) and TMS experiment
(Figure 5B). The results of the two-factorial rmANOVA revealed
a significant main effect for the stimulus Condition (F(1,
18) = 619.7, p < 0.001) and a significant main effect for the factor
Time (F(8, 144) = 240.1, p < 0.001). The interaction between the
factors was also significant (F(8, 144) = 1626.2, p < 0.001). The
post hoc comparisons revealed that in the no-TMS experiment,
the theta power was higher during CS+ in all the temporal
windows (0–2 s) compared to the baseline time window (−0.25–
0 s; all at p < 0.05) (Figure 5A). The frontal lobe theta power was
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
6
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 5 | Electroencephalography power averaged across frontal electrodes (shown in Supplementary Figure 1) in the theta band for (A) the no-TMS
experiment and (B) the TMS experiment. The power averaged across occipital electrodes (shown in Supplementary Figure 1) in the alpha band for (C) the no-TMS
experiment and (D) the TMS experiment. The values are mean ± standard error of the mean. The light red bar represents CS+ and light green denotes CS–. The
asterisk (∗ ) denotes significant differences after correcting for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected).
all at p < 0.05). Further to providing surface topography of the
oscillatory power changes, we performed comparisons between
stimulus conditions in the theta and alpha frequency bands. In
both experiments, statistical comparison of stimulus conditions
CS+ and CS− revealed an increase in theta power and a decrease
of alpha power in the latency range 0.15–0.45 s (all at t 18 > 2.2,
p < 0.05) (Figures 6A–D and Table 1).
Time (F(8, 144) = 2391.2, p < 0.001) and the interaction between
factors was also significant (F(8, 144) = 2391.2, p < 0.001). The
post hoc tests showed a significant decrease in occipital alpha
power during CS+ in the time window (0–1.25 s) but an increase
for CS− in the (0–2 s) compared to a baseline time window
(−0.25–0 s; all at p < 0.05) (Figure 5C). In the time window
(1.25–2 s) the occipital alpha power was higher for CS+, and
was lower for CS− relative to the (0.75–1 s) time window (all
at p < 0.05). In the TMS experiment, the occipital alpha power
was decreased for CS+ while increased for CS− in all the time
windows (0–2 s) compared to a baseline time window (−0.25–
0 s; all at p < 0.05) (Figure 5D). Due to single-pulse TMS, the
occipital alpha power was significantly reduced for CS+ in the
time windows (1–1.75 s), while increased for CS− in the time
windows (1.25–2 s) compared to the time window (0.75–1 s;
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
Functional Connectivity During No-TMS
and TMS Experiments
The pairwise cluster-based analysis revealed significant
differences in dynamic information flow at electrode level
between the CS+ and CS− in theta and alpha frequency bands.
In both the no-TMS and TMS experiments, we found increased
7
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 6 | Time-frequency plots of difference in power between CS+ and CS– averaged across channels in the theta band for (A) the no-TMS experiment and (B)
the TMS experiment. In the alpha band, the time-frequency plots of difference in power between CS+ and CS– averaged across channels for (C) no-TMS
experiment and (D) TMS experiment. The considered channels are indicated in each plot. These electrodes were identified with cluster-based statistics (p < 0.05).
information flow during CS+ relative to CS− from occipital
regions to prefrontal regions in the time windows (0–0.25 and
0.75–1 s), while the premotor regions received information from
prefrontal regions in the first temporal window (0–0.25 s) after
visual cue onset in the theta band (all at t 18 > 2.2, p < 0.05)
(Figures 7A,B and Table 2). In the same frequency band, the
TMS pulse on the right dmPFC increased the information flow
from the sensorimotor area and Supplementary Motor Area to
the prefrontal regions in the temporal window (1–1.25 s) during
CS+ compared to CS− (t 18 > 2.2, p < 0.05) (Figure 7B and
Table 2). In the alpha band, we found increased information flow
form parietal regions to the occipital regions and from there to
the prefrontal regions during CS+ relative to CS− in the time
windows (0–0.25 and 0.75–1 s) for both the experiments (all
at t 18 > 2.2, p < 0.05) (Figures 7C,D and Table 2). Moreover,
the single-pulse TMS increased the information flow from
sensorimotor area to occipital regions in the time window
(1–1.25 s) during CS+ compared to CS− in the alpha band
(t 18 > 2.2, p < 0.05) (Figure 7D and Table 2).
Neural Oscillatory Power Before TMS
Predicts TMS Response
A linear regression was performed to predict theta power increase
after TMS pulse delivery in the time window (1–1.25 s). The
results showed a high prediction power from pre-TMS pulse
activity (F(4,15) = 66.6, p < 0.001), explaining up to 93% of
the oscillatory activity increase after TMS stimulation. These
effects were only significant in the time window 0.75–1 s
(t = 13, p < 0.001). The linear regression showed no significant
relationship in alpha power between time windows before (0–1 s)
and after (1–1.25 s) TMS pulse delivery.
BRS Scale Correlations
In the no-TMS experiment, the correlation between theta power
(0.15–0.45 s) and BRS showed a positive statistical trend (r = 0.32,
p < 0.1). In the TMS experiment, theta power correlated with BRS
across all subjects in the time windows (0.15–0.45 and 1–1.25 s)
(Figure 8A: 0.15–0.45 s, n = 19, r = 0.53, p < 0.01; Figure 8B:
1–1.25 s, n = 19, r = 0.75, p < 0.001) suggesting that the increases
in the theta power during CS+ relative to CS− are related to the
individual coping abilities.
TABLE 1 | Significant clusters (p < 0.05) between conditions (CS+ and CS−) that
were compared regarding power in theta and alpha frequency bands using
cluster-based statistics.
Cluster
latency (s)
Type of
cluster
Number of
channels in
cluster
Theta
no-TMS experiment
0.15–0.45
positive
15
TMS experiment
0.15–0.45
positive
20
no-TMS experiment
0.15–0.45
negative
14
TMS experiment
0.15–0.45
negative
15
DISCUSSION
In this study, we tracked cortical excitability patterns during
fear processing and demonstrated that elevated excitability in the
theta generating system of dmPFC could be entrained through
Alpha
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
8
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
elevated fear ratings and increased heart rate during CS+
compared to CS−.
Excitability Patterns During Fear
Processing and Their Modulation
Previous studies showed increased cortical excitability to fear
stimuli after verbal instruction of the CS+/US contingency
(Bublatzky and Schupp, 2012; Weymar et al., 2013; Meyer
et al., 2015; Gonzalez-Escamilla et al., 2018a). The current
results from GMFP analyses also show increased cortical
excitability during processing of fearful stimuli compared to
neutral stimuli. The role of theta oscillations has already
been described in mice (Likhtik et al., 2014; Karalis et al.,
2016) and both theta and alpha oscillations have been
described in humans during fear processing (Meyer et al.,
2015; Chien et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2017). In mice, theta
oscillations are considered as a mechanism mediating prefrontalamygdala coupling related to fear expression (Karalis et al.,
2016). In humans, increased theta power at specific frontal
regions has been suggested as a mechanism of event-related
synchronization, whereas decreased alpha power at parietal
and occipital sites is related to event-related desynchronization
(Chien et al., 2017). In our study, both the experiments
evidenced increased theta power across the frontal lobe
and decreased alpha power in occipital lobe during fear
processing. Our results reproduce the generalizability of robust
oscillatory activity in the theta range during fear processing.
Furthermore, application of TMS over the right dmPFC induced
TABLE 2 | Significant clusters (p < 0.05) between conditions (CS+ and CS−) that
were compared regarding weighted phase lag index (WPLI) in theta and alpha
frequency bands using cluster-based statistics.
FIGURE 7 | The information flow at EEG electrode level during CS+
compared to CS– in the theta band for (A) the no-TMS experiment and (B)
the TMS experiment. In the alpha band, the information flow is shown for (C)
the no-TMS experiment and (D) the TMS experiment. The time windows are
labeled above the plot, while the direction is indicated below. The highlighted
electrodes were identified with cluster-based statistics (p < 0.05).
Cluster
latency (s)
Type of
cluster
Number of
channels in
cluster
Theta
single pulse TMS of this region. First, increased excitability of
the frontal regions was indicated by increased theta power, but
decreased alpha power was found in interconnected occipital
regions. These excitability variations were further represented
by modulation of functional connectivity in theta and alpha
frequency bands. Increased cortical excitability was achieved
by delivering single-pulse TMS over the right dmPFC, and
the patterns previous to the stimulation clearly determined the
modulation of cortical excitability after TMS. The excitability
patterns were further associated with a clear behavioral correlate,
the individual capability to cope with stressful events, as
measured by the BRS.
Previous studies have used subjective fear ratings as an
indicative measure of fear induction by the performed task
(Fredrikson et al., 1996; Meyer et al., 2015). Further, heart
rate acceleration has been proposed as an autonomic index of
fear stages (Steimer, 2002). In our study, successful induction
of fear in participants in both experiments was evidenced by
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
no-TMS experiment
0–0.25
positive
20
TMS experiment
0.75–1
negative
10
0–0.25
positive
25
0.75–1
positive
23
1–1.25
negative
31
positive
23
positive
42
Alpha
9
no-TMS experiment
0–0.25
positive
17
TMS experiment
0.75–1
negative
33
1–1.25
positive
9
0–0.25
negative
22
0.75–1
positive
11
1–1.25
negative
14
positive
8
negative
25
positive
14
negative
27
positive
15
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
FIGURE 8 | The increased theta power during fear processing (1: CS+ – CS–) correlated with Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) score in the time window (A) 0.15–0.45 s
and (B) 1–1.25 s in TMS experiment.
Predicting the Excitability Modulations
increased theta and lower alpha power in the same regions in
comparison to the pre-stimulation period. These results support
the hypothesis that single-pulse TMS modulates spontaneous
oscillations emerging from the stimulated region (Rosanova
et al., 2009). They therefore provide further evidence for
the dmPFC as a key region for the regulation of cortical
excitability and appropriate physiological responses to fear
processing. We also found evidence for differentiated fear
processing in specific temporal windows, highlighting a dynamic
process with clearly delimited spatial modulation in both theta
and alpha bands.
A recent study showed that pre-stimulation measures of
cortico-cortical evoked potentials (amplitude, latency, and
the distance between stimulation site and channel of interest)
predicted modulatory effects following 10 Hz prefrontal
repetitive stimulation (Keller et al., 2018). Similarly, our results
showed that frontal theta power on the pre-stimulation windows
explained the power changes after TMS stimulation (93%
of variance explained). This is likely to be of huge future
clinical relevance, as it provides evidence for the possibility of
accurately identifying target windows that are likely to produce
a maximum/optimal modulatory effect on the brain circuits, or
could serve as a biomarker in the therapeutic intervention of fear
or other mental health disorders. It could therefore be used in
future trials for the design of non-invasive treatment procedures.
Functional Connectivity Patterns During
Fear Processing and Their Modulation
The WPLI results showed that in the theta band, the
information flow increased from occipital to prefrontal
regions, which might represent a key element for the
appropriate processing of the threatening event, possibly
guiding connectivity patterns among the regions forming
the so-called fear network (Chien et al., 2017). In the alpha
band, the increased information flow was spatially limited
to occipital and parietal regions. The specific short-range
connectivity differences in the occipital lobe alpha oscillations
could be related to alterations due to the fear processing,
which have been shown in earlier studies (Tiitinen et al.,
1993; Ponjavic-Conte et al., 2013). After TMS stimulation, the
connectivity from sensorimotor regions to prefrontal regions
was increased in the theta band, while connectivity in the
alpha band occurred in the opposite direction (sensorimotor
area to occipital regions). These results suggest a specific
balance between excitability (Trenado et al., 2018) and
inhibitory mechanisms occurring at different brain areas,
as previously suggested (Piantadosi and Floresco, 2014; Lipp
et al., 2015), which temporally allow correct processing of
fearful stimuli. Such balance is likely to represent a physiological
marker for the existence of coping mechanisms, since an
impaired excitation and inhibition balance has been largely
associated with the development of neuropsychiatric disorders
(Selten et al., 2018).
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
Relationship Between Excitability and
BRS
We found that people with higher BRS scores showed higher
frontal theta power during fear processing. This result suggests
that resilient mechanisms play a key role in modulating fear
processing. We also showed that people with low BRS score
were less susceptible to the modulatory effects of dmPFC-TMS
stimulation, suggesting that preserved ability for coping with
aversive situations is directly related to specific patterns of cortical
excitation and communication within the regions forming fear
network. It will be of interest to evaluate this possibility in people
suffering with anxiety-related disorders in future studies.
Limitations
In this study, dmPFC localization was performed with a
neuronavigation system based on individual T1-weighted MRI
data and coordinates obtained from the previous study (Meyer
et al., 2018). Determining the coordinates in each individual
by implementation of instructed fear paradigm in fMRI which
has shown activation of dmPFC during threat compared to
safe (Meyer et al., 2018) might further improve the spatial
specificity of the TMS stimulation. In the future studies, it
would be interesting to investigate the effect of pre-processing
10
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
contributed to data discussion and manuscript writing. MM
and SG contributed to experimental design, data analysis, and
revision of the manuscript.
pipeline implemented in this study on functional connectivity
measures. Some researchers recommend playing the auditory
background noise through headphones to mask the auditory
interference caused by the TMS click, and future studies could
consider this approach.
FUNDING
CONCLUSION
This work was supported by a grant from the German Research
Council (DFG; CRC-TR-1193 project B05).
In summary, our results provide insight into the dynamics
of cortical excitability modulation and functional connectivity
during fear processing, while the patterns of pre-stimulation
frontal theta power determine the magnitude of effects induced
by TMS stimulation. Moreover, frontal theta power is clearly
related to an individual’s ability to cope with challenging
situations, leaving those individuals with low coping abilities
more vulnerable to functional failure in face of adversity.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank
proofreading the manuscript.
Gilchrist
for
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.
2019.00568/full#supplementary-material
VC contributed to data acquisition, data analysis, and manuscript
writing. NK, TB, and SvG contributed to data acquisition. GG-E
REFERENCES
cortex during sustained instructed fear responses: a TMS-EEG study. Sci. Rep.
8:14506. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32781-9
Groppa, S., Muthuraman, M., Otto, B., Deuschl, G., Siebner, H. R., and Raethjen,
J. (2013). Subcortical substrates of TMS induced modulation of the corticocortical connectivity. Brain Stimul. 6, 138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.014
Groppa, S., Oliviero, A., Eisen, A., Quartarone, A., Cohen, L. G., Mall, V., et al.
(2012). A practical guide to diagnostic transcranial magnetic stimulation: report
of an IFCN committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 858–882. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.
2012.01.010
Herring, J. D., Thut, G., Jensen, O., and Bergmann, T. O. (2015). Attention
modulates TMS-locked alpha oscillations in the visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 35,
14435–14447. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1833-15.2015
Karalis, N., Dejean, C., Chaudun, F., Khoder, S., Rozeske, R. R., Wurtz, H., et al.
(2016). 4-Hz oscillations synchronize prefrontal-amygdala circuits during fear
behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 605–612. doi: 10.1038/nn.4251
Keller, C. J., Huang, Y., Herrero, J. L., Fini, M. E., Du, V., Lado, F. A., et al.
(2018). Induction and quantification of excitability changes in human cortical
networks. J. Neurosci. 38, 5384–5398. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1088-17.2018
Likhtik, E., Stujenske, J. M., Topiwala, M. A., Harris, A. Z., and Gordon, J. A.
(2014). Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear
and innate anxiety. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 106–113. doi: 10.1038/nn.3582
Lipp, I., Evans, C. J., Lewis, C., Murphy, K., Wise, R. G., and Caseras, X. (2015). The
relationship between fearfulness, GABA+, and fear-related BOLD responses in
the insula. PLoS One 10:e0120101. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0120101
Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and
MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.
03.024
Mechias, M. L., Etkin, A., and Kalisch, R. (2010). A meta-analysis of instructed
fear studies: implications for conscious appraisal of threat. NeuroImage 49,
1760–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.040
Mertens, G., Boddez, Y., Sevenster, D., Engelhard, I. M., and De Houwer,
J. (2018). A review on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear
conditioning: empirical findings, theoretical considerations, and future
directions. Biol. Psychol. 137, 49–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.
07.002
Meyer, B., Yuen, K. S., Ertl, M., Polomac, N., Mulert, C., Buchel, C., et al. (2015).
Neural mechanisms of placebo anxiolysis. J. Neurosci. 35, 7365–7373. doi: 10.
1523/JNEUROSCI.4793-14.2015
Bai, Y., Wan, X., Zeng, K., Ni, Y., Qiu, L., and Li, X. (2016). Reduction
hybrid artifacts of EMG-EOG in electroencephalography evoked by prefrontal
transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neural Eng. 13:066016. doi: 10.1088/17412560/13/6/066016
Bai, Y., Xia, X., Kang, J., Yang, Y., He, J., and Li, X. (2017). TDCS modulates cortical
excitability in patients with disorders of consciousness. NeuroImage Clin. 15,
702–709. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.025
Bergmann, T. O., Karabanov, A., Hartwigsen, G., Thielscher, A., and Siebner,
H. R. (2016). Combining non-invasive transcranial brain stimulation
with neuroimaging and electrophysiology: current approaches and future
perspectives. NeuroImage 140, 4–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.012
Bublatzky, F., and Schupp, H. T. (2012). Pictures cueing threat: brain dynamics
in viewing explicitly instructed danger cues. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7,
611–622. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr032
Chien, J. H., Colloca, L., Korzeniewska, A., Cheng, J. J., Campbell, C. M., Hillis,
A. E., et al. (2017). Oscillatory EEG activity induced by conditioning stimuli
during fear conditioning reflects Salience and Valence of these stimuli more
than Expectancy. Neuroscience 346, 81–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.
12.047
Chmitorz, A., Wenzel, M., Stieglitz, R. D., Kunzler, A., Bagusat, C., Helmreich,
I., et al. (2018). Population-based validation of a German version of the brief
resilience scale. PLoS One 13:e0192761. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192761
Ferree, T. C., Luu, P., Russell, G. S., and Tucker, D. M. (2001). Scalp electrode
impedance, infection risk, and EEG data quality. Clinical neurophysiology 112,
536–544. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00533-2
Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., and Wik, G. (1996). Gender and age
differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 34,
33–39. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00048-3
Garcia, R. (2017). Neurobiology of fear and specific phobias. Learn. Mem. 24,
462–471. doi: 10.1101/lm.044115.116
Gonzalez-Escamilla, G., Muthuraman, M., Chirumamilla, V. C., Vogt, J., and
Groppa, S. (2018b). Brain networks reorganization during maturation and
healthy aging-emphases for resilience. Front. Psychiatry 9:601. doi: 10.3389/
fpsyt.2018.00601
Gonzalez-Escamilla, G., Chirumamilla, V. C., Meyer, B., Bonertz, T., von Grotthus,
S., Vogt, J., et al. (2018a). Excitability regulation in the dorsomedial prefrontal
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
Rosalind
11
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568
Chirumamilla et al.
Instructed Fear Paradigm TMS-EEG Study
Selten, M., van Bokhoven, H., and Nadif Kasri, N. (2018). Inhibitory control of
the excitatory/inhibitory balance in psychiatric disorders. F1000Res. 7:23. doi:
10.12688/f1000research.12155.1
Smith, B. W., Dalen, J., Wiggins, K., Tooley, E., Christopher, P., and Bernard, J.
(2008). The brief resilience scale: assessing the ability to bounce back. Int. J.
Behav. Med. 15, 194–200. doi: 10.1080/10705500802222972
Steimer, T. (2002). The biology of fear- and anxiety-related behaviors. Dialogues
Clin. Neurosci. 4, 231–249.
Tiitinen, H., Sinkkonen, J., Reinikainen, K., Alho, K., Lavikainen, J., and Naatanen,
R. (1993). Selective attention enhances the auditory 40-Hz transient response in
humans. Nature 364, 59–60. doi: 10.1038/364059a0
Trenado, C., Pedroarena-Leal, N., Cif, L., Nitsche, M., and Ruge, D. (2018). Neural
oscillatory correlates for conditioning and extinction of fear. Biomedicines
6:E49. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines6020049
Varoli, E., Pisoni, A., Mattavelli, G. C., Vergallito, A., Gallucci, A., Mauro,
L. D., et al. (2018). Tracking the effect of cathodal transcranial direct current
stimulation on cortical excitability and connectivity by means of TMS-EEG.
Front. Neurosci. 12:319. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00319
Vinck, M., Oostenveld, R., van Wingerden, M., Battaglia, F., and Pennartz, C. M.
(2011). An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological
data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias.
NeuroImage 55, 1548–1565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.055
Weymar, M., Bradley, M. M., Hamm, A. O., and Lang, P. J. (2013). When fear
forms memories: threat of shock and brain potentials during encoding and
recognition. Cortex 49, 819–826. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.012
Zheng, J., Anderson, K. L., Leal, S. L., Shestyuk, A., Gulsen, G.,
Mnatsakanyan, L., et al. (2017). Amygdala-hippocampal dynamics during
salient information processing. Nat. Commun. 8:14413. doi: 10.1038/
ncomms14413
Meyer, B., Yuen, K. S. L., Saase, V., and Kalisch, R. (2018). The functional role of
large-scale brain network coordination in placebo-induced anxiolysis. Cereb.
cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy188 [Epub ahead of print].
Milad, M. R., Rauch, S. L., Pitman, R. K., and Quirk, G. J. (2006). Fear extinction in
rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biol. Psychol.
73, 61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008
Palazzo, E., Fu, Y., Ji, G., Maione, S., and Neugebauer, V. (2008). Group III mGluR7
and mGluR8 in the amygdala differentially modulate nocifensive and affective
pain behaviors. Neuropharmacology 55, 537–545. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.
2008.05.007
Park, S. H., Naliboff, B. D., Shih, W., Presson, A. P., Videlock, E. J., Ju, T., et al.
(2018). Resilience is decreased in irritable bowel syndrome and associated
with symptoms and cortisol response. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 30:e13155.
doi: 10.1111/nmo.13155
Pellicciari, M. C., Veniero, D., and Miniussi, C. (2017). Characterizing the cortical
oscillatory response to TMS pulse. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 11:38. doi: 10.3389/
fncel.2017.00038
Perrin, F., Pernier, J., Bertrand, O., and Echallier, J. F. (1989). Spherical splines
for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalogr. Clin.
Neurophysiol. 72, 184–187. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(89)90180-6
Phelps, E. A., and LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion
processing: from animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48, 175–187. doi:
10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.025
Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., Grillon, C., and Davis, M.
(2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat.
Neurosci. 4, 437–441. doi: 10.1038/86110
Piantadosi, P. T., and Floresco, S. B. (2014). Prefrontal cortical GABA transmission
modulates discrimination and latent inhibition of conditioned fear: relevance
for schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2473–2484. doi: 10.1038/npp.
2014.99
Ponjavic-Conte, K. D., Hambrook, D. A., Pavlovic, S., and Tata, M. S. (2013).
Dynamics of distraction: competition among auditory streams modulates gain
and disrupts inter-trial phase coherence in the human electroencephalogram.
PLoS One 8:e53953. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053953
Romero Lauro, L. J., Rosanova, M., Mattavelli, G., Convento, S., Pisoni,
A., Opitz, A., et al. (2014). TDCS increases cortical excitability: direct
evidence from TMS-EEG. Cortex 58, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.
05.003
Rosanova, M., Casali, A., Bellina, V., Resta, F., Mariotti, M., and Massimini, M.
(2009). Natural frequencies of human corticothalamic circuits. J. Neurosci. 29,
7679–7685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0445-09.2009
Frontiers in Neuroscience | www.frontiersin.org
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2019 Chirumamilla, Gonzalez-Escamilla, Koirala, Bonertz,
von Grotthus, Muthuraman and Groppa. This is an open-access article distributed
under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use,
distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
12
June 2019 | Volume 13 | Article 568