A dependency-based typology of nasalisation
and voicing phenomena
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
University of Leiden/LUCL / University of Amsterdam/ACLC
1.
Introduction
In earlier work (see e.g. Botma 2004, Botma & Smith 2006) we have argued that
there are good grounds to assume that distinctive voicing and nasalisation are
in complementary distribution; the former is a property of obstruents, the latter
of sonorants. In our dependency-based approach we formalise this in terms of a
dependent element |L|, whose interpretation depends on whether it is linked to an
obstruent or sonorant manner component. Dependent |L| is interpreted as voicing
in obstruents, and as nasalisation in sonorants.
In this paper we evaluate this hypothesis against the behaviour of voicing and
nasalisation in domains like the syllable and the word. he behaviour of voicing
and nasalisation in these domains appears at irst sight to be problematic for our
approach, since cross-linguistic evidence indicates that they display asymmetric
behaviour; while there are languages with long-distance nasal harmony, there
do not seem to be any languages with long-distance voicing harmony (by ‘longdistance’ we mean that the targets are not necessarily adjacent on the segmental
level). Rather, long-distance voicing relationships appear to be dissimilatory in
nature. Our research shows that this asymmetry is part of a general asymmetry
between sonorants and obstruents: long-distance laryngeal assimilation targets
sonorants and leaves obstruents unafected; long-distance laryngeal dissimilation targets obstruents and leaves sonorants unafected. Our interpretation of this
asymmetry is motivated by the idea that long-distance assimilation is regulated
by syllabic heads, i.e. projections of nuclear positions.1 Given that nuclei contain
sonorants, and given that dependent |L| in sonorants denotes nasalisation, we
predict the possibility of long-distance nasalisation, and rule out the existence of
long-distance voicing. herefore, the hypothesis that voicing and nasalisation are
represented by a single element, viz. |L|, can be maintained.
his paper is organised as follows. First, in §2, we provide a brief outline of
the theoretical background. Next, in §3, we ofer a number of arguments for the
Linguistics in the Netherlands 2007, 36–48.
issn 0929–7332 / e-issn 1569–9919 © Algemene Vereniging voor Taalwetenschap
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
dual representation of voicing and nasalisation in terms of dependent |L|. In §4 we
discuss the asymmetric behaviour of voicing and nasalisation in assimilation and
dissimilation phenomena. §5 concludes our paper.
2. Representational assumptions
We assume that subsyllabic constituents have the general structure in (1), where
‘O’, ‘N’ and ‘C’ are short for onset, nucleus and coda, and ‘x’ represents the skeletal
level. Subsyllabic constituents have a maximum of two x-positions, a ‘head’ and
a ‘dependent’. In this paper we restrict our attention to constituents with a single
x-position.
(1)
{O, N, C}
x
|
manner
{|‘,H,L|}
|
place
{|A,I,U|}
phonation
{|‘,H,L|}
In (1) the manner and place components together form what may be termed the
segmental ‘core’. he phonation component forms a dependent of this core. his is
in line with the observation that it is unmarked for a segment to be speciied for
manner and place, but marked for a segment to be speciied for phonation.
For the purposes of this paper, two aspects of (1) require further comment.
he irst is the location of the phonation component, which, following Kehrein
(2002) and Kehrein & Golston (2004), we take to be a dependent of a subsyllabic constituent (rather than of individual segments, as is traditionally assumed).
his predicts that subsyllabic constituents can have at most one laryngeal contrast.
he phonetic implementation of this contrast is language-speciic. For instance, a
contrastively aspirated /p/ can be realised as pre- or postaspirated. However, (1)
predicts that no language contrasts /hp/ with /ph/, at least not in the same syllabic
position. We refer the reader to the sources cited for discussion of this and related
issues. What is relevant for our purposes is that laryngeal assimilation minimally
applies between subsyllabic constituents, as there are no phonologically relevant
laryngeal distinctions below this level.
37
38
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
he second aspect concerns the interpretation of the elements |‘|, |H|, and |L|,
which represent both the manner and the laryngeal properties of segments. he
articulatory and acoustic interpretations of |‘,H,L| are given in (2).
(2)
Articulatory interpretation
‘ : complete closure
H : close approximation
L : open approximation
Acoustic interpretation
energy reduction
aperiodicity
periodicity
he corresponding manner and laryngeal interpretations are given in (3).
(3)
Manner interpretation
‘ : plosive
H : sibilant/fricative
L : vowel
Laryngeal interpretation
glottal constriction
glottal widening
voice/nasalisation
he interpretation of |‘,H,L| thus depends on their position in the phonological
structure. A case in point is the element |L|. If |L| occurs as a manner element,
i.e. as (part of) the head, the segment is identiied as a sonorant. If |L| occurs as a
phonation element, i.e. as the dependent, its interpretation is variable; |L| denotes
nasalisation if there is also an |L| in the head, and voicing if there is no |L| in the
head. Phonologically, nasalisation is therefore a laryngeal property.2 Consider the
examples in (4).
(4) a.
N
|
x
|
L
|
U
/u/
b.
N
x
|
L
|
U
/ũ/
c.
L
O
|
x
|
‘
|
U
/p/
d.
O
x
|
‘
|
U
L
/b/
(4a) represents the high-back vowel /u/. he manner component of /u/ consists of
|L|, which is dominated by the nucleus and itself dominates the place element |U|.
In (4b), nasalised /ũ/ has an additional dependent |L|; since there is also an |L| in
the manner component, this dependent |L| denotes nasalisation. (4c) represents a
‘plain’ labial plosive /p/. (4d), which represents /b/, has an additional dependent |L|
which denotes voicing, since there is no |L| present in the manner component.
he context-sensitive interpretation of |L| embodies the claim that voicing and
nasalisation are in complementary distribution. We discuss this claim in more detail in §3.
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
3. he dual interpretation of dependent |L|
here are a number of arguments in favour of the dual interpretation of dependent
|L| as voicing and nasalisation. One is that it obviates the need for cooccurrence
restrictions between sonorancy and voicing, and obstruency and nasalisation.3
Cross-linguistic evidence shows that languages neither have distinctively voiced
sonorants nor distinctively nasalised obstruents.4
he dual interpretation of dependent |L| also permits a natural interpretation
of postnasal voicing. Postnasal voicing is a cross-linguistically common process
in which a voiceless stop is realised as voiced under the inluence of a preceding
nasal. In our approach this can be represented as spreading of dependent |L| from
the nasal (in our view, a ‘nasalised sonorant stop’; see Botma & Smith 2006) to the
dependent position of the stop. his is shown in (5), where the nasal ‘N’ and the
voiceless stop ‘C® ’ form a coda–onset sequence.
(5)
C
x
|
L
|
‘
O
L
x
|
‘
/NC® / → [NC̈]
he mirror image of this process, i.e. ‘stop-induced nasalisation’, is unattested. Apparently, dependent |L| can spread from a sonorant to an obstruent but not vice
versa. At present, we have no explanation for this asymmetry.
Perhaps the most compelling support for the dual interpretation of dependent
|L| comes from processes which trigger either voicing or nasalisation, depending
on whether the target is an obstruent or a sonorant. One such process is found in
Navajo. As Rice (1993) observes, the Navajo perfective is signalled by voicing of
stem-inal fricatives (6a) and by nasalisation of stem-inal vowels (6b).
(6) a.
Imperf
-‘aaS
-‘aaw
-lóós
Perf
-‘aal
-‘aaŠ
-lóóz
‘chew, eat’
‘few go’
‘lead’
b. Imperf
-bí
-‘á
-ka
Perf
-bı½
‘swim’
-‘a½
‘cl. small object’
-kã
‘cl. contained object’
hese surface manifestations can be accounted for if the perfective morpheme is
analyzed as |L|, which links to the dependent position of the stem-inal segment.
Botma (2004) discusses a number of similar processes from Irish, Maukakã and
Sambú.
39
40
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
he evidence reviewed above suggests that there are good grounds to assume
that distinctive voicing and nasalisation are represented by a single element, viz.
dependent |L|. In the remainder of this paper we will evaluate this hypothesis
against the asymmetric behaviour of voicing and nasalisation in domains such as
the syllable and the word.
4. Voicing and nasalisation: Typological observations
In this section we ofer an overview of assimilation and dissimilation processes
that involve nasalisation and voicing. Before doing this, it is important to make
explicit a number of terms. We interpret assimilation in standard autosegmental
terms, i.e. as spreading of a dependent element from some trigger to some target
(as is exempliied in (5), for instance). We assume that all spreading is local, in
the sense that it takes place between adjacent constituents (see also Walker 2000
and references there). his implies that a spreading analysis of long-distance assimilation, where segmental trigger and target are apparently not adjacent, can be
maintained only if we assume that spreading takes place at some higher level in
the phonological structure. In §4.2 we will see that this is the case in long-distance
nasalisation, which, we argue, is local on the level of syllable heads.
We assume that laryngeal dissimilation involves the deletion of a dependent
element, forced by the presence of an identical dependent element in the same
domain. A hypothetical input to dissimilation is given in (7), which shows a word
with two voiced obstruent stops in onset position. If a language does not tolerate
such sequences, dissimilation may apply to remove one of the two ofending |L|s.
(7) [… O
…
O
x
|
‘
|
…
L
x
|
‘
|
…
…]WORD
L
Note, incidentally, that a representation in which the two voiced stops are linked to
a single dependent |L| would violate locality, and is therefore impossible.
With this background, we now turn to an overview of voicing and nasalisation
processes. In §4.1 we consider processes that are local on the subsyllabic level; we
term these ‘adjacent’ processes. In §4.2 and §4.3 we focus on processes that are not
local on the subsyllabic level; we term these ‘long-distance’ processes.
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
4.1 Adjacent nasalisation and voicing
As regards adjacent assimilation, we ind cases of both voicing and nasalisation
in coda–onset clusters. (Nuclei are never targeted by voicing assimilation, since
nuclei contain sonorants, where dependent |L| denotes nasalisation.) In (8ab) we
give two examples of coda-induced assimilation (in both examples the following
onsets are preix-initial).
(8)
Process
a. Voicing
b. Nasalisation
Language
Oromo
Yakut
Example
/b-t/ → [bd]
/m-l/ → [mn]
Source
Lloret (1995)
Stachowski & Menz (1998)
We analyze Yakut /l/ in (8b) as a sonorant stop, i.e. as a segment with a manner
component that consists of |L| and |‘|. his /l/ is targeted by the dependent |L| of
the preceding nasal, turning it into a nasalised sonorant stop, i.e. [n], as in (9).
(9)
C
x
|
L
|
‘
|
U
O
L
x
|
L
|
‘
|
I
/m-l/ → [mn]
We will see in §4.3 that sonorant stops are the universally unmarked nasalisation
target.
Conversely, there do not appear to be any adjacent dissimilation processes that
involve nasalisation or voicing. Note that there are languages which denasalise the
second nasal in a nasal cluster (i.e. /NN/→[NC̈]), such as Fore (Scott 1978). However, this process involves dissimilation of manner rather than of phonation; the
change from a nasal to a voiced (obstruent) stop involves deletion of |L| from the
manner component of the afected nasal.
4.2 Long-distance voicing
he asymmetry between voicing and nasalisation becomes apparent when we
consider long-distance assimilation. Long-distance nasalisation is well-attested,
but the status of long-distance voicing is dubious. Some researchers accept it as
41
42
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
a possibility (see e.g. Walker 2000), while others explicitly reject it (see e.g. Gafos
1998). In any case, it would appear as though all reported cases of long-distance
voicing assimilation are also open to alternative interpretations.
In recent work on voicing agreement, two general patterns of long-distance
voicing agreement are recognized (see e.g. Walker 2000, Rose & Walker 2004,
Hansson 2001, 2004). In the irst, voicing agreement is part of a more general
identity requirement on consonants. his may involve other phonation types, as
in Aymara and Chaha, but also supralaryngeal aspects such as place (see Rose &
Walker 2004 for a typological overview). his type of consonant agreement is at
best infrequent. Walker (2000:533) claims that it is found in Ngbaka, which has
roots of the kind in (10ab), but not (10c).
(10) a.
tita
pεpu
babã
‘grandparent’
‘vent’
‘companion
b. bata
duka
tolo
‘three’
‘shoulder’
‘strike’
c.
*tida
*dita
*pεbu
he data cited by Walker relects only a subset of the cooccurrence restrictions in
Ngbaka. Wescott (1965:346) notes that a disyllabic word with a voiced obstruent
cannot also contain its prenasalised counterpart, and that a disyllabic word with
a prenasalised obstruent cannot also contain its ‘plain’ nasal counterpart. Hence,
it would seem as though Ngbaka imposes a general ban on segments that are too
much alike. his suggests that an OCP-based approach to the Ngbaka restrictions
is more appropriate (as in, for instance, Mester 1986 and van de Weijer 1996).
he second pattern of long-distance agreement involves an inter-dependence
between voicing and low tone. Hansson (2001, 2004) argues that in such cases
voicing agreement can be seen as a by-product of low-tone spreading and toneinduced voicing. his is arguably also the case in Kera, the only language where
voicing agreement –apparently– results in overt alternations. Pearce (2006) has
recently argued that the Kera voicing facts result from spreading of low tone, and
that the language in fact lacks distinctive voicing altogether.5
We conclude from these observations that voicing generally does not display
long-distance assimilation. Rather, all long-distance voicing efects that we are
aware of involve dissimilation. Examples include Lyman’s Law in Japanese (Itô &
Mester 1986), Dahl’s Law in a number of East African Bantu languages (Davy &
Nurse 1982) and hurneysen’s Law in Gothic (Mossé 1956). We will not ofer an
analysis of any speciic voicing dissimilation process here. Such processes can in
principle be handled by a ban on multiple occurrences of dependent |L| in obstruents in a particular domain, as was outlined in (6) above.
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
4.3 Long-distance nasalisation
Long-distance nasalisation is well-attested, and forms part of a phenomenon that is
referred to as ‘nasal harmony’. One type of long-distance nasalisation is found in a
number of Amazonian languages, including Southern Barasano. In Southern Barasano nasalisation is an underlying property of some vowels. From these it spreads
rightwards to the end of the word, skipping voiceless obstruents. Any consonant preceding a nasalised vowel also surfaces as nasalised, except when that consonant is a
voiceless obstruent. Consider the forms in (11), taken from Smith & Smith (1971).
(11) kãmõkã ‘rattle’
mãsã
‘people’
w̃ãtĩ
rimã
‘demon’
‘poison’
hati-ãmĩ
hiamãkõnõ
‘he sneezes’
‘ten’
In Southern Barasano (and in many other Amazonian languages) the voiced stops
[b,d,g] are in complementary distribution with the nasals [m,n,ŋ]; the former
precede oral vowels, the latter nasalised vowels. his leads Botma (2004) to treat
/b,d,g/ as sonorant stops, which, like other harmonic targets, surface as nasalised
through association with dependent |L|. he advantage of this account is that it
limits the set of nasalizable segments in Southern Barasano to sonorants.
Botma (2004) further assumes that, underlyingly, nasalisation is a property of
the syllable, rather than of the vowel that this syllable dominates (see Piggott & van
der Hulst 1997 and Nasukawa 2005 for similar approaches). Speciically, his claim
is that |L| is a dependent of the syllable node N1, itself a projection of the nucleus
N0. his makes it possible to analyze rightward spreading of nasalisation as local
on the syllabic level, as is illustrated in (12) for the word [w̃ãtĩ].
N2
(12)
N1
N2
N1
L
O
|
x
|
L
|
U
N0
|
x
|
L
|
A
O
|
x
|
‘
|
I
N0
|
x
|
L
|
I
w̃
ã
t
ĩ
Since Southern Barasano does not allow codas, we assume that it lacks a nuclear
projection equivalent to the level of the rhyme. N1 is equivalent to the syllable
level; the projection N2 incorporates the harmonic element |L|.
43
44
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
Our account of Southern Barasano nasalisation essentially extends Kehrein’s
(2002) prosodic theory of laryngeal contrasts to include the level of the syllable.
Indeed, there is reason to believe that |L| can also occur as a dependent in even
larger domains. For instance, Piggott (1996) argues that nasalisation in Kikongo
is bound by the foot, while in Amazonian languages such as Tuyuca and Yuhup
nasalisation is a property of entire words (see e.g. Botma 2004). In each of these
domains |L| can be viewed as an underlying dependent of a particular syllable
head (and therefore ultimately as a dependent of a sonorant manner component),
and in each of these domains spreading of |L| can be viewed as applying at the level
of syllable heads. his ensures, then, that dependent |L| will always be interpreted
as nasalisation at the syllabic level. hus, the asymmetric behaviour of voicing and
nasalisation in long-distance phenomena does not force us to reject the hypothesis
that both are represented by |L|. Indeed, aside from being restrictive, an account in
terms of a context-sensitive element |L| explains why we ind long-distance nasalisation, but no long-distance voicing. Compare this to a traditional feature-based
approach, in which it must be stipulated that some features, e.g. [+nasal], can be
harmonic, while others, e.g. [+voice], cannot.
A comment is in order regarding the phonetic implementation of dependent
|L| in languages like Southern Barasano. As the data in (11) shows, nasalisation
targets all sonorants in the harmonic domain. his suggests that in nasal surface
forms, all |L|-headed segment types contain dependent |L|.6 But since dependent
|L| can in principle also be linked to obstruents, we must restrict its association in
Southern Barasano to the set of sonorants. Adopting a term from Piggott & van
der Hulst (1997), Botma (2004:140) proposes that the dependency relation of a
harmonic element in a prosodic domain must be ‘consistent’. We formalise this in
terms of the principle in (13).
(13) Principle of Consistent Dependency Relations
In a harmonic domain Nn, where Nn is a projection of N0, and Nn is speciied
for a dependent element X, X can be implemented only on heads of the same
type as N0.
(13) predicts that long-distance nasalisation never triggers voicing of obstruents.
Processes which involve both voicing and nasalisation, such as postnasal voicing
and Navajo perfective formation, always take place at the subsyllabic level.
To conclude this section, we briely consider another type of long-distance
nasalisation. his concerns the consonant nasalisation process that is found in a
number of Bantu languages. he basic pattern is displayed by Lamba. As Odden
(1994:325) observes, Lamba has oral and nasal suix allomorphs, depending on
whether the preceding stem-inal consonant is oral (14a) or nasal (14b). (14c)
shows that the nasalisation process may have more than one target.
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
(14) a.
pat-ile
mas-ile
b. uum-ine
nw(a)-ine
c. mas-ulul-a
min-unun-a
‘scold-perf’
‘plaster-perf’
‘dry-perf’
‘drink-perf’
‘un-plaster’
‘un-swallow’
(*pat-ine)
(*mas-ine)
(*uum-ile)
(*nw-ile)
(*mas-unun-a, *mas-ulun-a, *mas-unul-a)
(*min-ulul-a, *min-ulun-a, *min-unul-a)
We analyze the target of nasalisation, i.e. /l/, as a sonorant stop, similar to what we
proposed for Yakut in (8b).
he Lamba facts appear to be problematic for the account suggested above. If,
as we claim, long-distance nasalisation involves spreading at some projection of
N0, then how can we explain that harmonic nasalisation is not implemented at N0
itself? In other words, if dependent |L| in Lamba is an underlying property of the
stem-inal syllable, then why is |L| realised only on the suix consonant, and not
on the preceding suix vowel?
We suggest that the answer to this question lies in the relative markedness of
nasalisation targets. Typological research has shown that there is an implicational
hierarchy in the compatibility of segments with nasalisation (see Walker 1998 and
references there). Walker (1998:34) proposes the following harmony scale.7
(15) sonorant stop > vowel > glide > liquid > fricative > obstruent stop
his hierarchy is implicational in the sense that if, say, in some language glides are
targets for nasalisation, then vowels and sonorant stops will also be targets. Hence,
(15) relects the fact that of all segment types, sonorant stops are best compatible
with nasalisation. his is corroborated by the observation, made by Maddieson
(1984), that a language which has contrastively nasalised segments minimally has
nasals — i.e., nasalised sonorant stops. Notice also that the relative unmarkedness
of nasals seems feasible on phonetic grounds, since nasalisation is perceptually
most salient in nasals.
In Walker’s Optimality-heoretic approach, the scale in (15) is formalised as a
set of intrinsically ranked markedness constraints, as in (16).
(16) *NasObsStop >> *NasFric >> *NasLiq >> *NasGli >> *NasVow >>
*NasSonStop
Walker formalises spreading of nasalisation in terms of NasSpread constraints.
his generates a typology of nasal harmony processes, including one in which the
harmonic targets are restricted to just sonorant stops (the appropriate ranking has
the NasSpread constraint ranked between *NasVow and *NasSonStop). his,
we suggest, is the type of nasal harmony that operates in Bantu languages such as
Lamba. he phonetic realisation of harmonic forms in Lamba creates the impression that the nasal spreading is non-local. However, this is only apparent, since the
process operates at the level of syllable heads, where it is local.
45
46
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
Reasons of space preclude a detailed analysis of Bantu consonant nasalisation, but the contours of such an analysis will by now be clear.8 In Lamba, as in all
languages with long-distance nasalisation, dependent |L| is underlyingly linked
to a syllable head. he surface manifestation of nasalisation is regulated by the
interaction between spreading and markedness constraints. As to the latter, we
will have to reformulate the constraints in (16) in terms of our dependency-based
representations. For instance, in our model it seems to be the case that dependent
|L| is best compatible with manner structures containing the element |‘|, either in
isolation (in which case dependent |L| denotes voicing), or in combination with |L|
(in which case dependent |L| denotes nasalisation). Dependent |L| is less compatible with manner structures containing |H|, which relects the markedness of such
segment types as nasalised liquids and glides. We leave the formalisation of these
constraints for future research.
5. Conclusion
In this paper we have shown that there is an asymmetry between long-distance
voicing and nasalisation: the former is dissimilatory, the latter is assimilatory. We
have argued that this asymmetry receives a principled answer if (1) voicing and
nasalisation are represented by dependent |L|, and (2) long-distance nasalisation
is regulated by prosodic heads, i.e. projections of nuclei. he fact that dependent
|L|, when linked to a syllable head, is interpreted as nasalisation explains why we
ind long-distance nasal harmony, but no long-distance voicing harmony. Hence,
the asymmetry of voicing and nasalisation in long-distance phenomena does not
undermine an approach in which they are represented by the same element, viz.
dependent |L|.
One question that is raised by our account is to what extent the behaviour of
dependent |L| is representative of laryngeal contrasts in general. As we intimated
above, we suspect that the asymmetry between voicing and nasalisation relects a
general asymmetry between obstruents and sonorants. All the cases of laryngeal
dissimilation known to us target obstruents, and leave sonorants unafected. An
uninteresting reason for this asymmetry would be that laryngeal contrasts are on
the whole much less frequent in sonorants. In that case, laryngeal dissimilation of
sonorants would be unattested simply because laryngeal contrasts in sonorants
are very rare to begin with. However, a more interesting reason would be that
only sonorants are capable of projecting prosodic heads, so that, as a result, only
sonorants can take part in long-distance assimilations. he asymmetry between
obstruents and sonorants could then be attributed to the fact that their laryngeal
dependents occupy diferent structural positions, and therefore display diferent
A dependency-based typology of nasalisation and voicing phenomena
behaviour. Some support for the latter position comes from Spokane, where we
ind a process of long-distance glottalisation which targets sonorants and skips
intervening obstruents (see Carlson 1980). We hope to address the Spokane facts
in a future paper.
Notes
1. For a similar approach to vowel harmony, see van der Hulst & van de Weijer (1995).
2. he Government Phonology model of Nasukawa (2005) also assumes a single element representing voicing and nasality.
3. In feature theories [voice] is usually underspeciied in sonorants. For an approach in which
[nasal] is limited to segments speciied for SV (i.e. sonorants), see Rice (1993).
4. his implies that pre- and postnasalised stops, which are potentially contrastive, cannot be
analyzed as single nasalised stops but must be treated as NC clusters. See Downing (2005) for a
recent account along these lines.
5. In our account there is a natural relation between voicing and low tone, in that both are expressed by the dependent low-tone element |L|. We leave the speciic representation of low tone,
and its interaction with voicing, for further research, however.
6. We assume that all sonorants in the harmonic domain ‘inherit’ an |L|-speciication from the
syllable-level speciication.
7. Contrary to what we claim, Walker assumes that obstruents are potential nasalisation targets.
his does not afect the point at issue, however.
8. It should be noted that Bantu languages difer in the kind of NasSpread constraints that are
active. Compare Hyman’s (1995) description of Yaka, for instance.
References
Botma, Bert 2004. Phonological aspects of nasality: an element-based dependency approach. Ph.D.
diss., University of Amsterdam (published by LOT, Utrecht).
Botma, Bert & Norval Smith. 2006. “A dependency account of the fortis–lenis contrast in Cama”.
Linguistics in the Netherlands 2006 ed. by Jeroen van de Weijer & Bettelou Los, 15–27. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Carlson, Barry. 1980. “Spokane –e–”.International Journal of American Linguistics 46.78–84.
Davy, John & Derek Nurse. 1982. “Synchronic versions of Dahl’s Law: the multiple applications of a phonological dissimilation rule”. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics
4.157–195.
Downing, Laura. 2005. “On the ambiguous segmental status of nasals in homorganic NC sequences”. he internal organization of phonological segments ed. by Marc van Oostendorp &
Jeroen van de Weijer, 183–216. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
47
48
Bert Botma and Norval Smith
Gafos, Adamantios. 1998. “Eliminating long-distance consonantal spreading”. Natural Language
and Linguistic heory 16.223–278. New York: Garland.
Hansson, Gunnar. 2001. heoretical and typological issues in consonant harmony. Ph.D. diss.,
University of California, Berkeley.
Hansson, Gunnar. 2004. “Long-distance voicing agreement: an evolutionary perspective.” Proceedings of the 30th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society ed. by Marc Ettlinger
et al., 130–141. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Hulst, Harry van der & Jeroen van de Weijer. 1995. “Vowel harmony”. he handbook of phonological theory ed. by John Goldsmith, 495–534. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hyman, Larry. 1995. “Nasal consonant harmony at a distance: the case of Yaka”. Studies in African Linguistics 24.5–30.
Itô, Junko & Armin Mester. 1986. “he phonology of voicing in Japanese: theoretical consequences for morphological accessibility”. Linguistic Inquiry 17.49–73.
Kehrein, Wolfgang. 2002. Phonological representation and phonetic phasing: africates and laryngeals. Ph.D. diss., University of Marburg.
Kehrein, Wolfgang & Chris Golston. 2004. “A prosodic theory of laryngeal contrasts”. Phonology
21.325–357.
Lloret, Maria-Rosa. 1995. “he representation of glottals in Oromo”. Phonology 12.257–280.
Maddieson, Ian. 1984. Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mester, Armin. 1986. Studies in tier structure. Ph.D. diss., University of Massachusetts.
Mossé, Fernand. 1956. Manuel de la langue gotique: grammaire, textes, notes, glossaire. Paris:
Aubier, Éditions Montaigne.
Nasukawa, Kuniya. 2005. A uniied approach to nasality and voicing. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Pearce, Mary. 2006. Evidence against voicing spread in Kera. Paper presented at the 14th Manchester Phonology Meeting, May 2006, Manchester.
Rice, Keren. 1993. “A re-examination of the feature [sonorant]: the status of ‘sonorant obstruents’”. Language 69.308–344.
Rose, Sharon & Rachel Walker. 2004. “A typology of consonant agreement as correspondence”.
Language 80.475–531.
Odden, David. 1994. “Adjacency parameters in phonology”. Language 70.289–330.
Piggott, Glyne. 1996. “Implications of consonant nasalization for a theory of harmony”. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 41.141–174.
Piggott, Glyne & Harry van der Hulst. 1997. “Locality and the nature of nasal harmony”. Lingua
103.85–112.
Scott, Graham. 1978. he Fore language of Papua New Guinea. (Paciic Linguistics B–47). Canberra: Australian National University.
Smith, Richard & Connie Smith. 1971. “Southern Barasano phonemics”. Linguistics 78. 80–85.
Stachowski, Marek & Astrid Menz. 1988. “Yakut”. he Turkic languages ed. by Lars Johanson &
Éva Csató, 417–433. London: Routledge.
Walker, Rachel. 1998. Nasalization, neutral segments, and opacity efects. Ph.D. diss., University
of California at Santa Cruz.
Walker, Rachel. 2000. “Long-distance consonantal identity efects”. WCCFL 19.532–545.
Weijer, Jeroen van de. 1996. Segmental structure and complex segments. Tübingen: Niemeyer.
Wescott, Roger. 1965. “Review of J.M.C. homas (1963) Le parler Ngbaka de Bokanga — phonologie, morphology, syntax”. Language 41.346–347.