OPEN ACCESS
JOURNAL
& PSYCHOLOGY
Psychology & Neuroscience, 2008, 1, 1, 15 -20
DOI:10.3922/j.psns.2008.1.003
NEUROSCIENCE
Face perception in its neurobiological and social context
Wânia C. de Souza1, Maria Ângela G. Feitosa1, Satoshi Eifuku2, Ryoi Tamura2 and Taketoshi Ono2
1 Universidade de Brasília, Brazil
2 University of Toyama, Japan
Abstract
Significant advances in the understanding of processes involved in face perception have been achieved. This study aims to review
the literature of face perception in neurobiological and social contexts. The review focused on the mechanisms of mediation of
face perception by neural substrates, and discussed some of the social signals provided by faces. We showed that psychological,
neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that a dedicated neural system for face perception exists in primates,
which includes the fusiform face area (FFA), anterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) and anterior inferior temporal gyrus (ITG). But
it remains to be understood how the integration of face perception occurs in the neurobiological context and in the social context.
Keywords: face perception, neural substrates, social signals, prosopagnosia, face-responsive cells, face symmetry.
Received 24 April 2007; received in revised form 10 August 2007; accepted 15 August 2007. Available online 9 May 2008.
Introduction
Face perception may be one of the most developed
visual perceptual skills in humans. People probably spend
more time looking at faces than at any other single stimulus
(Morton & Johnson, 1991). The human face is important
for the identification of individual members of the human
species, a process that relies on the subtle differences and
variations that make every face unique (Bruce, 1982).
From an evolutionary view, the face has evolved in order
to allow other organs to function (the mouth for eating, for
instance); however, these organs are used for additional
purposes, as the signaling of emotion by movements of the
facial muscles (Darwin, 1872).
The early efforts at determining a clear theoretical
basis for the understanding of face processing date from
the 1970s and 1980s, through information-processing
models (Hay & Young 1982; Bruce & Young, 1986; Ellis,
1986), a computer-recognition model (Kohonen, Oja, &
Lehtio, 1981), a neurophysiological model (Baron, 1981),
and neuropsychological models (Damasio, Damasio, &
Wânia C. De Souza and Maria Ângela G. Feitosa Departamento de Processos Psicológicos Básicos, Instituto de
Psicologia da Universidade de Brasília, Brazil. Satoshi Eifuku
and Ryoi Tamura, Department of Integrative Neuroscience,
Graduate School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences,
University of Toyama, Japan. Taketoshi Ono, Department of
Molecular and Integrative Emotional Neuroscience, Graduate
School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of
Toyama, Japan. Correspondence regarding this articles should
be addressed to Dr. Wânia Cristina de Souza, Universidade de
Brasília, Instituto de Psicologia, Departamento de Processos
Psicológicos Básicos, Brasília – DF – Brazil, 70910-900, E-mail:
[email protected]
Hoesen, 1982; Ellis, 1983; Rhodes, 1985). Since then,
the increasing interest in the processes underlying the
perception and recognition of faces has been demonstrated
by the large number of studies in this field.
The purpose of this paper is to review the literature on
face perception in neurobiological and social contexts. The
review focuses on the mechanisms of mediation of face
perception by neural substrates, and discusses some of the
social signals provided by faces.
Prosopagnosia and the Early Studies on
Neural Substrates for Face Perception
The existence of a specialized system for face perception in
the human brain was first suggested by the observation of patients
with focal brain damage who had a selectively unimpaired ability
to recognize other objects. Most cases reported an upper left
quadrantanopia, some with defects in the upper right quadrant
as well. Therefore, critical substrates for face recognition were
localized in the right temporal cortex or bilateral temporal
cortices in most people (Meadows, 1974). This syndrome
was called prosopagnosia (Hecaen & Angelergues, 1962;
McNeil & Warrington, 1993). Prosopagnosia was further
associated with lesions in other areas, such as the ventral
occipitotemporal cortex (Damasio et al., 1982; Sergent
& Signoret, 1992), and right unilateral lesions
(De Renzi, 1986; Landis, Cummings, Christen, Bogen,
& Imhof, 1986).
The contribution of prosopagnosia to the study of face
perception is that prosopagnosia suggested the existence
of separate systems for the recognition of faces and for
the recognition of objects (McNeil & Warrington, 1993;
Damasio et al., 1982). Evidence of the independence of face
16
and object recognition has been shown. Farah, Lecvinson
and Klein, (1995) demonstrated that face perception does
not involve the same processes and representations as the
perception of objects. Moreover, Feinberg, Schindler and
Ochoa (1994) reported patients who showed dissociation
between object and face recognition, suggesting that
prosopagnosia is not simply a mild general visual agnosia,
because such an interpretation is inconsistent with the
possibility of relatively preserved face recognition in the
presence of object agnosia.
Concerning the two subtypes of prosopagnosia, it has
been suggested that associative prosopagnosia is caused
by damage to bilateral anterior temporal areas, whereas
apperceptive prosopagnosia is due to unilateral right
lesions of the fusiform and lingual gyri (Damasio, Tranel,
& Damasio, 1990; De Renzi, Perani, Carlesimo, Silveri,
& Fazio, 1994; Barton et al. , 2002). However, further
extensive neurophysiological and/or functional imaging
studies are required in order to clearly define the neural
substrates for each subtype of prosopagnosia.
Mediation of Face Perception by Neural
Substrates
A large number of studies have discussed distinct
physiological processing systems enrolled in face
perception, supporting the evidence for the existence of
neural substrates for face perception. These include evidence
of innate attentiveness to faces in newborn infants (Goren,
Sarty, & Wu, 1975; Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton,
1991), specialization of the right cerebral hemisphere for
face recognition (De Renzi, Perani, Carlesimo, Silveri, &
Fazio, 1994; Ellis, 1983), and neurophysiological evidence
of face-responsive cells in the temporal lobes of monkeys
(Desimone, 1991; Gross, 1992; Perrett, Rolls, & Caan,
1982; Perrett, Hietanen, Oram, & Benson, 1992; Eifuku,
De Souza, Tamura, Nishijo, & Ono, 2004; De Souza,
Eifuku, Tamura, Nishijo, & Ono 2005).
It is known that cells in two distinct regions of the
temporal lobe are preferentially activated by faces in
monkeys: one is located in the anterior superior temporal
sulcus (STS), and the other in the anterior inferior temporal
gyrus (ITG) (Rolls, 1992). Our group has recorded neuronal
activity from the temporal lobe of Japanese monkeys
(Macaca fuscata) to investigate whether cells in this region
respond specifically to faces. Our results indicated that the
anterior STS and the anterior ITG play different roles in the
process of identifying familiar faces (Eifuku et al., 2004,
De Souza et al., 2005). The population of neurons in the
anterior STS responded to faces with selectivity for viewing
angle, and these neurons are thought to be closely associated
with the analysis of incoming perceptual information from
faces, whereas the population of neurons in the anterior
ITG was essentially involved in the recognition of facial
identity. It appears, thus, that the functional roles of the
anterior STS and the anterior ITG differ, although these
roles might be complementary (Eifuku et al., 2004, De
Souza et al., 2005).
Souza et al
More recently, the technology of functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) has enabled considerable
advances in the understanding of face perception in
humans, partly because the spatial resolution of fMRI can
yield a much more precise picture of face-specific areas
compared to lesion studies. The fMRI studies indicate
that there are multiple neural systems responsible for face
perception distributed in both hemispheres (Haxby, 2000).
Imaging studies have also identified the importance of
the fusiform face area (FFA) during face perception tasks
and the apparent nonexclusive activation of this area by
faces (Gauthier, Skudlarski, Gore, & Anderson, 2000). The
FFA was reported to be engaged both in detecting faces
and in extracting the necessary perceptual information to
recognize them. It has also been demonstrated that the FFA
shows not only functional, but also structural specificity
(Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006). Furthermore, three bilateral
regions in the occipitotemporal visual extrastriate cortex
were found to compose the human neural system for face
perception: the region in the fusiform gyrus appears to be
more involved in the representation of identity (Hoffman
& Haxby, 2000; George, Dolan, Fink, Baylis, Russell, &
Driver, 1999), whereas the region in the superior temporal
sulcus appears to be more involved in the representation
of changeable aspects of faces (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000;
Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore, & McCarthy, 1998). The
region in the inferior occipital gyri may provide input to
the other two regions.
These results are consistent with the studies performed
with monkeys, which identified face-selective neurons in
the anterior STS and ITG (Perrett et al., 1992; Desimone,
1991; Eifuku et al., 2004; De Souza et al., 2005). These
results suggest that similar clusters of face-selective
neurons may exist in homologous regions in the human
brain. The exact location of these homologous regions in
the human brain, however, is not obvious.
Social Cognition Involving Face and Gaze
Recognition of faces, facial expressions and gaze
direction are important components of the non-verbal human
social behavior (Argyle & Cook, 1976; Grusser, 1984). The
face provides important social information related to sexual
attraction, emotional state, and individual identity. Animalbased studies of specialized skills and neural systems for
processing these social signals were recently reviewed by
Tate, Fischer, Leigh, & Kendrick (2006), and have shown that
these skills and systems evolved in a number of mammals
and are not exclusive to humans. Much of the cognitive and
neuroscience research in face perception has focused on
people’s ability to recognize individuals. The recognition of
identity is based on the perception of aspects of the facial
structure that are invariant across changes in expression
and other movements of the eye and mouth (Bruce, Burton,
Doyle, & Dench, 1989). According to Haxby (2000),
however, social communication that is facilitated by the face
may represent a more highly developed visual perceptual
skill than the recognition of identity.
Face perception in its neurobiological and social context
The amygdala has been described as a critical
component of the neural network underlying social
cognition (Rosvold, Mirsky, & Pibram, 1954). This
structure might play a role in guiding social behaviors
on the basis of socioenvironmental context, possibly by
modulating several areas of the visual and somatosensory
cortices that have been implicated in social cognition.
This structure might also help to direct overt visuospatial attention to face gaze (Adolphs & Spezio, 2006).
The relation of the amygdala and the anterior STS with
gaze recognition may explain why a patient with bilateral
amygdala damage fails to recognize expressions of fear.
In fact, the amygdala seems to be extensively involved in
the recognition of facial expressions of fear (Nagai, 2007).
Similarly, impairment in the ability to direct gaze towards,
and to use information from, the eyes in the face of others,
may explain the deficient recognition of basic emotions
and deficient social judgment seen in patients with damage
to the amygdala (Adolphs, 2007).
We have recently studied the modulation of face neuron
responses in the anterior STS of monkeys, according to the
direction of gaze in the observed face, since direction of gaze
is an important determinant of the biological significance of
an observed face. It should also be noted that in monkeys,
the anterior STS has abundant neural connections with
the amygdala (Amaral & Price, 1984). We found that
modulation was more evident in the rostral region of the
anterior STS. Some of the face neuron responses were
specific to the combination of a particular facial view and
a particular gaze direction, whereas others were associated
with the relative spatial relationship between facial view
and gaze direction (De Souza et al., 2005).
Researchers have hypothesized that the social
dysfunctions in social developmental disorders, such as
autism, impair the acquisition of normal face-processing
skills (Hefter, Manoach, & Barton, 2005). Indeed, Dawson,
Webb and McPartland (2005) have demonstrated that
individuals with autism have impaired face discrimination
and recognition, and use atypical strategies for processing
faces characterized by reduced attention to the eyes
and piecemeal rather than configural strategies. Face
perception in autistic persons was studied by Schultz et
al. (2000), who found a significantly smaller activation
of the middle aspect of the right fusiform gyrus (FG) in
14 high functioning individuals with autism or Asperger
syndrome, compared to controls. More recently, a study
with individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
concerning the activation of face identity-processing areas
in the so-called social brain found a widely distributed
network of brain areas related to face identity-processing,
which included the right amygdala, the inferior frontal
cortex (IFC), STS, and the face-related somatosensory and
premotor cortex (Hadjikhani, Joseph, Snyder, & TagerFlusberg, 2007). Furthermore, Kylliainen, Braeutigam,
Hietanen, Swithenby and Bailey, (2006) found subtle
differences in face and gaze processing among autistic
children as compared to children with normal development.
Averted eyes evoked a strong right lateralized component
17
in the normally developing children, but a weak response
in autistic children. By contrast, direct gaze evoked a left
lateralized component only in children with autism.
Face Symmetry/Asymmetry and its Positive
Relation to Attractiveness
In their analyses of the origins and nature of facial
expressions, Darwin (1872) and Ekman (1993) suggested
that facial expressiveness may be an important element
related to social interactions and sexual reproduction. In
this context, a general attractiveness for face symmetry,
among animals, would represent an important feature
for their survival (Kenrick, Montello, Gutierres, & Trost
1993). Evolutionary theorists have argued that symmetry
is a marker of good genes and resistance to disease that
can cause asymmetrical development (Riggio & Woll,
1984). Moreover, asymmetry may be a more salient
determinant of attractiveness in younger faces because
it is rarer (Walster, Aronson, Abrahams, & Rottman,
1966). Bashour (2006) has identified symmetry as the
most important factor of attractiveness when compared
to typicality, sexual dimorphism and youthfulness. In
contrast, Swaddle and Cuthill (1995) described a positive
relationship between asymmetry and facial attractiveness,
showing that it may be due to the fact that certain facial
features (including those contributing to attractiveness) in
fact show directional asymmetry or antisymmetry. Little
and Jones (2006) have suggested symmetry preferences
are driven by a mechanism that is independent of conscious
detection, and which may be the result of specific pressures
faced by human ancestors to select high-quality mates and
could support a modular view of mate choice. They showed
that symmetry preferences are influenced by inversion,
whereas symmetry detection is not, and that the ability
to detect facial symmetry is not related to preferences for
facial symmetry.
There have been, so far, only a few neurophysiological
studies regarding the neural substrates underlying the
recognition of face symmetry and/or asymmetry. We have
recently reported detailed characteristics of face-responsive
cells in the anterior STS of macaque monkeys (De Souza et al.,
2005). Interestingly, did not a small number of face neurons
in the caudal region of the anterior STS responded equally
to right- and left-angled faces, while on the other hand, face
neurons in the rostral region of the anterior STS responded
preferentially to faces in a unique (right or left) oblique view.
In other words, face-responsive cells in the caudal region of the
anterior STS tended to be insensitive to right-left symmetry
on the face. On the other hand, face neurons in the rostral
region of the anterior STS discriminated between right and
left-angled faces. Therefore, the rostral region of the anterior
STS may be important for the discrimination of right-left
differences in faces, i.e. face asymmetry. Taken together,
our results suggested a plausible functional hierarchy within
the anterior STS along the rostro-caudal axis. (De Souza et
al., 2005).
18
Souza et al
We have also reported an effect of familiarity on face
symmetry/asymmetry in humans. In a previous report
(Eifuku, De Souza, Tamura, Nishijo, & Ono, 2003),
we investigated brain representations of familiar and
unfamiliar faces during a reaction time (RT) task. This
study used a pair association paradigm using facial
stimuli. We found that RTs were influenced by previous
learning in the case of both unfamiliar and familiar
faces, but the shape of the RT curves differed markedly
between cases involving familiar and unfamiliar faces.
The RT curve for unfamiliar faces had two volleys,
which were right-left symmetrical to the midline,
whereas the RT curve for familiar faces had only a
single volley, i.e., those showing right-left asymmetry.
These results revealed a significant difference between
the mental representations of familiar and unfamiliar
faces. The results are also in agreement with those
obtained in our previous study on responsiveness of
face cells in the anterior STS of monkeys. The rightleft symmetry reflected in the RT curves for unfamiliar
faces might have been associated with the behavior
of face neuron responses frequently observed in the
caudal region of the anterior STS, where the majority
of face neuron responses had two peaks that were rightleft symmetrical to the midline (De Souza et al., 2005;
Eifuku et al., 2003). Unfortunately, research has not
yet linked symmetry or asymmetry in the human or
monkey face to social meanings such as mating value,
indicating that face symmetry (or asymmetry) might be
an attractive matter of study for the near future.
Conclusion
The face is a very powerful image. It is one of our
primary means of perception and communication as
humans. From all possible objects, the face is what attracts
us the most. For instance, when we meet someone, we
usually look first at their face, and then to their body. We
can recognize a familiar face, or distinguish one person
from another as soon as we see her/him, because of abilities
that are constructed in our brains.
The present review discussed important issues related
to face perception such as whether face perception
uses the same neural mechanisms as those used for the
recognition of objects. Evolutionary arguments support the
specialization for face processing and the existence of a
neural mechanism for face perception.
Paul Ekman (1982) found that people from distinct
cultures, even those from villages of New Guinea who had
not been exposed to the western culture, can recognize
certain emotional facial expressions. This indicates that
face expressions of basic emotions can be universally
recognized and, in a certain way that is biologically
programmed (Ekman et al., 1987). Despite this, people
often exert control on their facial expressions, adapting
them according to each circumstance. In other words,
the social context can influence the expression of
emotions.
p
We showed in this article that a number of studies –
psychological, neurophysiological and neuroimaging
– have demonstrated one neural substrate, the FFA,
which is specialized for face perception. But it remains
unclear how the integration of face perception occurs
in the neurobiological context and in the social context.
For instance, how do brain structures involved in face
perception and facial expression, such as the FFA and the
amygdala, integrate to each other in the moment we look
at one’s face?
The understanding of the neural mechanisms in
the recognition of facial expressions, gaze and face
perception is important to demonstrate how special
faces are in nonverbal communication. There is indeed
no doubt about the importance of face perception for
human communication but what still remains unclear is
the whole system of information processing that can be
provided by faces, such as age, emotional expression, and
attractiveness.
An intriguing challenge for researchers in the future
will be to link all of the information concerning neural
mechanisms and social context, as well as to evaluate
if or how these mechanisms change or influence face
perception. Studies in this field are expected to bring a
clearer comprehension of the interaction between neural
structures and social signals provided by faces.
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr. Eliane Shizuka Nakamura for comments
on early versions of this manuscript.
References
Adolphs, R. (2007). Looking at other people: mechanisms for social
perception revealed in subjects with focal amygdala damage.
Novartis Foundation Symposium., 278:146-159; discussion 160164, 216-221.
Adolphs, R., & Spezio, M. (2006). Role of the amygdala in
processing visual social stimuli. Progress in Brain Research,
156, 363-378.
Amaral, D.G., & Price, J. L. (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections
in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). Journal of Comparative
Neurology, 230, 465-496.
Argyle, M., & Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and mutual gaze. Cambridge
Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK.
Baron, R. J. (1981). Mechanisms of human facial recognition.
International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 15, 247-248.
Barton, J.J., Press D. Z., Keenan, J. P., & O’Connor, M. (2002).
Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial
configuration in prosopagnosia. Neurology, 8, 71-78.
Bashour, M. (2006). History and current concepts in the analysis of
facial attractiveness. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 118,
741-756.
Bruce, V. (1982). Changing faces: visual and non-visual coding
processes in face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 73,
105-116.
Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition.
British Journal of Psychology, 77, 305-327.
Bruce, V., Burton, M., Doyle, T., & Dench, N. (1989). Further
experiments on the perception of growth in three dimensions.
Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 528-536.
Face perception in its neurobiological and social context
Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Hoesen, G. W. (1982). Prosopagnosia:
anatomical basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neurology, 32, 331-341.
Damasio, A., Tranel, D., & Damásio, H. (1990). Face agnosia and the
neural substrates of memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13,
89–109.
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and
animals. John Murray. London.
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding
the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights
from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 27, 403-424.
De Renzi, E. (1986). Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT
scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere.
Neuropsychologia, 24, 385-389.
De Renzi E., Faglioni, P., Grossi, D., & Nichelli, P. (1991).
Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex,
27, 213–221.
De Renzi, E., Perani, D., Carlesimo, G.A., Silveri, M.C., & Fazio, F.
(1994). Prosopagnosia can be associated with damage confined
to the right hemisphere--an MRI and PET study and a review of
the literature. Neuropsychologia, 32, 893-902.
Desimone, R. (1991). Face-selective cells in the temporal cortex of
monkeys. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 1-8.
De Souza, W.C., Eifuku, S., Tamura, R., Nishijo, H., & Ono, T.
(2005). Differential characteristics of face neuron responses
within the anterior superior temporal sulcus of macaques. Journal
of Neurophysiology, 94, 1252-1266.
Eifuku, S., De Souza, W.C., Tamura, R., Nishijo, H., & Ono, T. (2003).
On the organization of memory. In: T. Ono., G. Matsumoto., R. R.
Llinás., A. Berthoz., A. Norgren, H. Nishijo & R. Tamura (Eds.).
Cognition and emotion in the brain. International Congress
Series 1250, 73-85. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
Eifuku, S., De Souza, W.C., Tamura, R., Nishijo, H., & Ono, T.
(2004). Neuronal correlates of face identification in the monkey
anterior temporal cortical areas. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91,
358-371.
Ekman, P. (1982). Emotion in the face. Cambridge University Press,
New York.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., Chan, A., DiacoyanniTarlatzis, I., Heider, K., Krause, R., Le Compte, W. A., Pitcairn,
T., & Ricci-Bitti, P. E. (1987). Universal and cultural differences
in the judgments of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712-717.
Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American
Psychologist, 48, 384-492.
Ellis, H.D. (1983). The role of right hemisphere in face perception.
In A. W. Young (ed.), Functions of the right hemisphere, (pp. 3364). London: Academic Press.
Ellis, H.D. (1986). Processes underlying face recognition. In: R.
Bruyer (ed.), The neuropsychology of face perception and facial
expression, (pp. 1-27). N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Hillsdale.
Farah, M. J., Levinson, K. L., & Klein, K. J. (1995). Face
perception and within-category discrimination in prosopagnosia.
Neuropsychology, 33, 661-674.
Feinberg, T. E., Schindler, R. J., & Ochoa, E. (1994). Associative
visual agnosia and alexia without prosopagnosia. Cortex, 30,
395-411.
Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J.C., & Anderson, A.W. (2000).
Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face
recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191-197.
George, N., Dolan, R.J., Fink, G.R., Baylis, G.C., Russell, C., &
Driver, J. (1999). Contrast polarity and face recognition in the
human fusiform gyrus. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 574-580.
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., & Wu, P. Y. (1975). Visual following and
pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants.
Pediatrics, 56(4), 544-549.
Gross, C. G. (1992). Representation of visual stimuli in inferior
temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society,
335(1273), 3-10.
Grusser, O. J. (1984). Face recognition within the reach of
neurobiology and beyond it. Human Neurobiology, 3(4),
183-190.
19
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., & Tager-Flusberg, H.
(2007) Abnormal activation of the social brain during face
perception in autism. Human Brain Mapping. , 28, 441-449.
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). The distributed
human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 4(6), 223-233.
Hay, D. C., & Young, A. W. (1982). The human face. In A. W. Ellis
(ed.), Normality and Pathology in cognitive functions, (pp. 173202). New York: Academic Press.
Hecaen, H., & Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces
(prosopagnosia). Archives of Neurology, 7, 24-32.
Hefter, R. L., Manoach, D. S., & Barton, J. J. (2005). Perception
of facial expression and facial identity in subjects with social
developmental disorders. Neurology, 65(10), 1620-1625.
Hoffman, E. A., & Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of
eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for
face perception. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 80-84.
Johnson M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991).
Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its
subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1-2), 1-19.
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: a cortical
region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, 361(1476), 2109-2128.
Kenrick, D. T., Montello, D. R., Gutierres, S. E., & Trost, M.
R. (1993). Effects of physical attractiveness on affect and
perceptual judgments: When social comparison overrides social
reinforcement. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19,
195-199.
Kohonen, T., Oja, E., & Lehtio, P. (1981). Storage and processing
of information in distributed associative memory systems. In G.
E. Hinton & J. A. Anderson (eds.), Parallel models of associate
memory. N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.
Kylliainen, A., Braeutigam, S., Hietanen, J. K., Swithenby, S. J., &
Bailey, A. J. (2006) Face- and gaze-sensitive neural responses
in children with autism: a magnetoencephalographic study.
European Journal of Neuroscence, 24(9), 2679-2690.
Landis, T., Cummings J. L., Christen, L., Bogen, J. E., & Imhof, H.
G. (1986). Are unilateral right posterior cerebral lesions sufficient
to cause prosopagnosia? Clinical and radiological findings in six
additional cases. Cortex, 22, 243-252.
Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2006). Attraction independent of
detection suggests special mechanisms for symmetry preferences
in human face perception. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London Series B Biological Sciences. 273(1605), 3093-3099.
McNeil, J., & Warrington, E. (1993). Prosopagnosia: a face-specific
disorder. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A,
1-10.
Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 37, 489-501.
Morton, J., & Johnson, M. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLEARN:
a two-process theory of infants face recognition. Psychological
Review, 98, 164-181.
Nagai, C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of facial recognition. No To
Shinkei, 59(1), 45-51.
Perrett, D. I., Rolls, E. T., & Caan, W. (1982). Visual neurones
responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex. Experimental
Brain Research, 47(3), 329-42.
Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K. Oram, M. W., & Benson, P. J. (1992).
Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the
temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society,
335(1273), 23-30.
Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore. J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1998).
Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth
movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(6), 2188-2199.
Rhodes, G. (1985). Lateralization processes in the face recognition.
British Journal of Psychology, 76, 249-271.
Riggio, R. E., & Woll, S. B. (1984). The role of nonverbal cues and
physical attractiveness in the selection of dating partners. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 1, 347-357.
Rolls, R. T. (1992). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face
processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 335, 11-20.
20
Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., & Pribram, K. H. (1954). Influence
of amygdalectomy on social behavior in monkeys. Journal of
Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 47, 173-178.
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A.
W., Volkmar, F., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D. J., & Gore,
J. C. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during
face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger
syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(4), 331-340.
Sergent, J., & Signoret, J. (1992). Varieties of functional deficits in
prosopagnosia. Cerebral Cortex, 2, 375-388.
Souza et al
Swaddle, J. P., & Cuthill, I. C. (1995) Asymmetry and human facial
attractiveness: symmetry may not always be beautiful. Proc. Biol.
Sci., 261(1360), 111-116.
Tate, A. J., Fischer, H., Leigh, A. E., & Kendrick, K. M. (2006).
Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence for face identity and
face emotion processing in animals. Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society, 361(1476), 2155-2172.
Walster, E., Aronson, V., Abrahams, D., & Rottman, L. (1966).
Importance of physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(5), 508-516.